Skip to main content

Full text of "Apprasing And Recording Student Progress"

See other formats


373  P964  v«3      60-11109 

Smith 

*  * ^Appraising  and  recording 

student  progress 


3  1148  00721    2384 


PROGRESSIVE  EDUCATION  ASSOCIATION  PUBLICATIONS 
Commission  on  the  Relation  of  School  and  College 

<<<^^^ 


ADVENTURE 

IN 
AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Volume  III 
Appraising  and  Recording  Student  Progress 


ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

<«•(«•  C«-  («•«<-«<•  «(• «(•  CCC  <«•  C^«fr  («•  («•  ^f^f^C-  CC<-  <«~ 

Volume  I 
The  Story  of  the  Eight-Year  Study 

by 
Wilford  M.  Aikin 

Volume  II 

Exploring  the  Curriculum 

The  Work  of  the  Thirty  Schools 

from  the  Viewpoint  of  Curriculum  Consultants 

by 
H.  H.  Giles,  S.  P.  McCutchen,  and  A.  N.  Zechiel 

Volume  III 

Appraising  and  Recording  Student  Progress 

Evaluation,  Records  and  Reports 

in  the  Thirty  Schools 

by 

Eugene  R.  Smith,  Ralph  W.  Tyler 
and  the  Evaluation  Staff 

Volume  IV 
Did  They  Succeed  in  College? 

The  Follow-up  Study  of  the 
Graduates  of  the  Thirty  Schools 

by 

Dean  Chamberlin,  Enid  Straw  Chamberlin 

Neal  E.  Drought  and  William  E.  Scott 

Preface  by  Max  McConn 

Volume  V 

Thirty  Schools  Tell  Their  Story 

Each  School  Writes  of  Its  Participation 

in  the  Eight-Year  Study 


The  Commission  on  the  Relation  of  School  and  College 

of 
The  Progressive  Education  Association 

*&HKtr*&<S^^  ^  CCC  ^<^<<<<<<<^^^^^^^^^ 

MEMBERS  OF  THE  COMMISSION 

Walter  Raymond  Agard  John  A.  Lester 

Wilford  M.  Aikin,  Chairman  Max  McConn,  Secretary 

Willard  Beatty  Clyde  R.  Miller 

Bruce  Bliven  *Jesse  H.  Newlon 

C.  S.  Boucher  W.  Carson  Ryan 

*A.  J.  Burton  Harold  Rugg 

Flora  S.  Cooke  *Ann  Shumaker 

Harold  A.  Ferguson  Eugene  R.  Smith 

Burton  P.  Fowler  Perry  Dunlap  Smith 

Josephine  Gleason  Katharine  Taylor 

Thomas  Hopkins  Vivian  T.  Thayer 

Leonard  V.  Koos  Goodwin  Watson 

W.  S.  Learned  Raymond  Walters 

Robert  D.  Leigh  Ben  D.  Wood 

After  originating  and  organizing  the  Eight- Year  Study,  the  Commis- 
sion in  1933  gave  full  responsibility  and  authority  for  the  supervision 
of  the  Study  to  the  Directing  Committee. 

DIRECTING  COMMITTEE   OF   THE   COMMISSION 

Wilford  M.  Aikin,  Chairman,  Director  of  the  Study 

Willard  Beatty  Robert  D.  Leigh 

Boyd  H.  Bode  Max  McConn,  Secretary 

Burton  P.  Fowler  *  Jesse  H.  Newlon 

Carl  Brigham  Marion  Park 

Will  French  Eugene  R.  Smith 

Herbert  E.  Hawkes  J.  E.  Stonecipher 

John  A.  Lester  1John  B.  Johnson 
Elizabeth  M.  Steel,  Secretary  to  the  Director 

*  Deceased. 
1  Resigned. 


APPRAISING    AND    RECORDING    STUDENT  PROGRESS 

Copyright,  1942,  by  Harper  6-  Brothers 
Printed  in  the  United  States  of  America 

All  rights  in  this  book  are  reserved. 
No  part  of  the  book  may  be  reproduced  in  any 
manner  whatsoever  without  written  permission 
except  in  the  case  of  brief  quotations  embodied 
in  critical  articles  and  reviews.  For  information 
address  Harper  6-  Brothers 


The  Progressive  Education  Association 

the  Commission  and  the  Schools 
gratefully  acknowledge  their  indebtedness 

to 
CARNEGIE  CORPORATION"  OF  NEW  YORK 

and  to  the 

GENERAJL  EDUCATION  BOARD 
for  the  funds  which  made  this  Study  possible 


During  the  first  year  the  Commission  had  no  funds  except  $800 
contributed  in  equal  amounts  by  the  Francis  W.  Parker,  John  Bur- 
roughs, Lincoln,  and  Tower  Hill  Schools.  From  the  beginning  of 
1932,  generous  subventions  from  Carnegie  Corporation  of  New 
York  supported  the  work,  except  that  in  evaluation,  through  1936. 
Much  larger  grants  from  the  General  Education  Board  financed 
the  work  of  the  Evaluation  Staff,  the  Curriculum  Associates  and, 
since  1936,  all  the  activities  of  the  Commission. 

Neither  Carnegie  Corporation  of  New  "York  nor  the  General 
Education  Board  is  author,  owner,  publisher,  or  proprietor  of 
this  publication.  They  are  not  to  be  understood  as  approving  by 
virtue  of  their  grants  any  of  the  statements  made  or  views  ex- 
pressed therein. 


Commission  on  the  Relation  of  School  and  College 

frCCK<<-<<<<<<-^^<Cfr^^ 

COMMITTEE  ON  EVALUATION  AND  RECORDING 

Helen  M.  Atkinson  *  Frances  Knapp 

Frederick  H.  Bair  Robert  D.  Leigh 

E.  Gordon  Bill  Max  McConn 

Burton  P.  Fowler  Eugene  R,  Smith,  Chairman 

Ben  D.  Wood 


SUB-COMMITTEES1    ON   RECORDS   AND  REPORTS 

Committee  on  Behavior  Description 

Helen  M.  Atkinson  Anna  Rose  Hawkes  Rollo  G.  Reynolds 

E.  Gordon  Bill  "Frances  Knapp  Eugene  R.  Smith 

Carl  Brigham  Robert  D.  Leigh  John  Tildsley 

Oscar  K.  Euros  W.  S.  Learned  Ben  D.  Wood 

Cecile  Flemming  John  A.  Lester  Stanley  R.  Yarnall 

John  W.  M.  Rothney 


Committee  on  Teachers"  Reports  and  Reports  to  the  Home 

Helen  M.  Atkinson  Rosamond  Cross  Elrnina  R.  Lucke 

Derwood  Baker  Burton  P.  Fowler  Eugene  R.  Smith 

Genevieve  L.  Coy  I.  R.  Kraybill  John  W.  M.  Rothney 


Committee  on  Form  for  Transfer  from  School  to  College 

Victor  L.  Butterfield  Ruth  W.  Crawford  Eugene  R.  Smith 

Genevieve  L.  Coy  Burton  P.  Fowler  Herbert  W.  Smith 

Albert  B.  Crawford  Elmina  R.  Lucke  Arthur  E.  Traxler 

John  W.  M.  Rothney 


General  Committee  on  Study  of  the  Development 
of  Pupils  in  Subject  Fields 

Helen  M,  Atkinson  Edith  M.  Penney 

Genevieve  L.  Coy  Eugene  R.  Smith 

Harry  Herron  Arthur  E.  Traxler 

G.  H.  V.  Melone  John  W.  M.  Rothney 

*  Deceased. 

1In  addition  to  those  who  were  continuing  committee  members,  at  least 
400  others  from  the  schools  and  other  institutions  cooperated. 


Commission  on  the  Relation  of  School  and  College 


EVALUATION  STAFF 
Ralph  W.  Tyler,  Research  Director 
Associate  Director  Associates 

Oscar  K.  Euros,  1934-35  Bruno  Bettelheim        Louis  M.  Heil 

Louis  E.  Raths,  1935-38  Paul  B.  Diederich        George  Shevialcov 

Maurice  L.  Hartung,  1938-42  Wilfred  Eberhart         Hilda  Taba 

Harold  Trimble 


Assistants 

Herbert  J.  Abraham          Paul  R.  Grim  Carleton  C.  Jones 
D wight  L.  Arnold            Chester  William  Harris    W.  Harold  Lauritsen 

Jean  Friedberg  Block       John  H.  Herrick  Christine  McGuire 

Charles  L.  Boye  Clark  W.  Horton  Harold  G.  McMullen 

Fred  P.  Frutchey  Walter  Howe  Donald  H.  McNassor 

Secretaries:  CeceHa  K,  Wasserstrom,  Kay  D.  Watson 


CURBICULUM  ASSOCIATES 

H.  H.  Giles  S.  P.  McCutchen 

A.  N.  Zechiel 

The  following  served  as  special  curriculum  consultants  at  various  times: 
Harold  B.  Alberty  Henry  Harap 

Paul  B.  Diederich  Walter  V.  Kaulfers 

John  A,  Lester 


COLLEGE  FOLLOW-UP  STAFF 

John  L.  Bergstresser  Neal  E.  Drought 

Dean  Chamberlin  William  E.  Scott 

Enid  Straw  Chamberlm        Harold  Threlkeld 


EDITORIAL  COMMITTEE 

Harold  B.  Alberty  Burton  P.  Fowler 

C.  L.  Cushman  Max  McConn 

Thomas  C.  Pollock 


Commission  on  the  Relation  of  School  and  College 


THE  PARTICIPATING  SCHOOLS 


School 
Altoona  Senior  High  School,  Altoona, 

Pa. 
Baldwin  School,  Bryn  Mawr,  Pa. 

Beaver  Country  Day  School,  Chest- 
nut Hill,  Mass. 

Broaxville  High  School,  Bronxville, 
N.  Y. 

Cheltenham  Township  High  School, 
Elkins  Park,  Pa. 

Dalton  Schools,  New  York,  N.  Y. 

Denver  Senior  and  Junior  High 
Schools,  Denver,  Colo. 

Des  Moines  Senior  and  Junior 
Schools,  Des  Moines,  Iowa 

Eagle  Rock  High  School,  Los  An- 
geles, Cal. 

Fieldston  School,  New  York,  N.  Y. 

Francis  W.  Parker  School,  Chicago, 

111. 
Friends'  Central  School,  Overbrook, 

Pa. 

George  School,  George  School,  Pa. 
Germantown   Friends   School,    Ger- 

mantown,  Pa. 

Horace  Mann  School,  New  York,N.  Y. 
John  Burroughs  School,  Clayton,  Mo. 

Lincoln  School  of  Teachers  College, 

New  York,  N.  Y. 
Milton  Academy,  Milton,  Mass. 
New  Trier  Township  High  School, 

Winnetka,  III 
North   Shore  Country  Day  School, 

Winnetka,  III 
Radnor  High  School,  Wayne,  Pa. 


Head1 
(Levi  Gilbert)  Joseph  N.  Maddocks 

(Miss  Elizabeth  Johnson)  Miss  Ros- 
amond Cross 
Eugene  R.  Smith 

Miss  Edith  M.  Penney 
I.  R.  Kraybill 

Miss  Helen  Parkhurst 

(Charles  Greene)  (C.  L.  Cushman) 

John  J.  Cory. 
(*R.  C.  Cook)  J.  E.  Stonecipher 

Miss  Helen  Babson 

(Herbert     W.     Smith)      (Derwood 

Baker)  Luther  Tate 
(Miss  Flora  Cooke)  (Raymond  Os- 

borne)  Herbert  W.  Smith 
Barclay  L.  Jones 

George  A.  Walton 

(Stanley    R.    Yarnall)     Burton    P. 

Fowler 

(Rollo  G.  Reynolds)  Will  French 
(Wilford    M.    Aikin)    Leonard    D, 

Haertter 
(* Jesse  H.  Newlon)    (Lester  Dix) 

Will  French 
W.  L.  W.  Field 
Matthew  P.  Gaffney 

Perry  Dunlap  Smith 

Sydney  V.  Rowland,  T,  Bayard  Beatty 


TMany  changes  in  administration  occurred  in  the  schools  during  the 
period  of  the  Study.  Such  cases  are  indicated  by  names  in  parentheses  given 
in  chronological  order  of  service. 

*  Deceased 


Commission  on  the  Relation  of  School  and  College 


Sc/zooZ 
Shaker  High  School,  Shaker  Heights, 

Ohio 
Tower  Hill  School,  Wilmington,  Del. 

Tulsa  Senior  and  Junior  High 
Schools,  Tulsa,  Okla. 

University  of  Chicago  High  School, 
Chicago,  111. 

University  High  School,  Oakland, 
Cal. 

University  School  of  Ohio  State  Uni- 
versity, Columbus,  Ohio 

Winsor  School,  Boston,  Mass. 

Wisconsin  High  School,  Madison, 
Wise. 


Head 


R.  B.  Patin 


(Burton  P.  Fowler)  James  S.  Guern- 
sey 

(Will  French)  (Eli  C,  Foster)  H. 
W.  Gowans 

(Arthur  K.  Loomis)  P.  B.  Jacobson 

( George  Rice )  Paul  T.  Fleming 

(Rudolph  Lindquist)  (Harold  B. 
Alberty)  Robert  S.  Gilchrist 

(Miss  Katharine  Lord)  Miss  Frances 

Dugan 
(H.  H.  Ryan)    ([ Stephen  M.  Corey) 

( Gordon  Mackenzie )  Glen  G.  Eye 


CONTENTS 


FOREWORD  xvii 

PREFACE  xxi 

Part  1  —  Development  and  Use  of  Evaluation  Instruments 

I.  PURPOSES  AND  PROCEDURES  OF  THE  EVALUATION  STAFF, 

Ralph  W.  Tyler  3 

How  the  Evaluation  Staff  Came  into  Existence  3 

Significance  of  the  Evaluation  Project  5 

Major  Purposes  of  Evaluation  7 

Basic  Assumptions  11 
General  Procedures  in  Developing  the  Evaluation 

Program  15 

Division  of  Labor  in  tibe  Evaluation  Program  28 

Summary  29 

II.  ASPECTS  OF  THINKING,  Maurice  L.  Hartung,  Leah 
Weisman,   Harold   G.   McMullen,   Harold   C. 

Trimble  35 

I.  Interpretation  of  Data  —  Analysis  of  the  Objec- 

tive 38 

The  Development  of  Evaluation  Instruments  43 

Other  Instruments  to  Measure  This  Objective  60 

Validity  of  the  Interpretation  of  Data  Tests  65 

Reliability  of  the  Interpretation  of  Data  Tests  74 

II.  Application  of  Principles  of  Science  —  Analysis 

of  the  Objective  77 

The  Development  of  Evaluation  Instruments  80 

III.  Application  of  Principles  of  Logical  Reason- 

ing —  Analysis  of  the  Objective  111 

The  Development  of  Evaluation  Instruments  114 

Validity  and  Reliability  of  Form  5.12  124 

IV.  The  Nature  of  Proof  —  Analysis  of  the  Objec- 

tive 126 


xiv  CONTENTS 

The  Development  of  Evaluation  Instruments  130 

Validity  and  Reliability  of  Test  Form  5.22  143 

A  Related  Instrument  148 

Conclusion  154 

III.  EVALUATION    OF    SOCIAL    SENSITIVITY,    Hilda    Taba> 

Christine  McGuire  157 
Informal  Methods  of  Getting  Evidence  on  Social 

Sensitivity  162 
Evaluation  of  the  Ability  to  Apply  Social  Facts  and 

Generalizations  168 

Analysis  of  the  Objective  168 
Construction  of  the  Test  on  the  Ability  to  Apply 

Social  Values  175 
Validity  and  Reliability  190 
Applying  Social  Facts  and  Generalizations  to  So- 
cial Problems  (Form  1.5)  197 
Evaluation  of  Social  Attitudes — Analysis  of  the  Ob- 
jective 203 
Evaluation  of  Beliefs  on  Social  Issues  209 
Description  of  the  Test  on  Beliefs  on  Social  Issues 

(Form  4.21-4.31)  ,  215 

Beliefs  about  School  Life  229 

Beliefs  on  Economic  Issues  234 

Uses  of  These  Tests  239 

IV.  ASPECTS  OF  APPRECIATION,  Chester  William  Harris, 

Bruno  Bettelheim,  Paul  B.  Diederich  245 

Appreciation  of  Literature  246 

The  Evaluation  of  the  Appreciation  of  Art  276 

Development  of  the  Instrument  283 

Description  of  the  Test  289 

The  Test  Interpretation  292 

How  to  Administer  the  Test  299 

Reliability  and  Validity  300 

Future  Use  of  the  Test  306 

Other  Instruments  307 

V.  EVALUATION  OF  INTEBESTS,  Chester  William  Harris, 

Wilfred  Eberhart,  Jean  Friedberg  Block  313 


CONTENTS  xv 

Analysis  of  the  Objective  313 

The  Interest  Index  8.2a  338 

VI.  EVALUATION  OF  PERSONAL  AND  SOCIAL  ADJUSTMENT, 

George  V.  Sheviakov,  Jean  Friedberg  Block  349 

Discussion  of  the  Objective  349 
Discussion  of  the  Technique  of  Appraisal  of  the 

Objective  354 
Description  of  the  Questionnaire  361 
Interpretation  of  the  Responses  to  the  Question- 
naires 372 
Sample  Analysis  of  Response  of  One  Student  377 
Reliability  384 
Validity  385 
Possible  Uses  of  the  Questionnaires  396 
Summary  400 

VII.  INTERPRETATION   AND   USES   OF   EVALUATION   DATA, 

Hilda  Taba  403 

Methods  of  Interpreting  and  Using  Evaluation  Data  430 

VIII.  PLANNING  AND  ADMINISTERING  THE  EVALUATION  PRO- 
GRAM, Hilda  Taba  441 

Part  II — Recording  for  Guidance  and  Transfer 
Eugene  R.  Smith 

IX.  PHILOSOPHY  AND  OBJECTIVES  463 

General  Purposes  and  Philosophy  of  Recording  464 

Working  Objectives  for  Records  and  Reports  467 

X.  BEHAVIOR  DESCRIPTION  470 

Use  of  Record  Cards  474 

Summary  of  Advantages  486 

XI.  TEACHERS'  REPORTS  AND  REPORTS  TO  THE  HOME  488 

XII.  FORM  FOR  TRANSFER  FROM  SCHOOL  TO  COLLEGE  494 
Confidential  Report  to  the  Committee  on  Admission  494 

The  "Junior  Year"  Blank  498 

XIII.  STUDY  OF  THE  DEVELOPMENT  OF  PUPILS  IN  SUBJECT 

FIELDS  499 

APPENDICES  507 


FOREWORD 
ccc  c<g-«g-cgc-«c-c 


It  is  an  amazing  fact  that  our  schools  and  colleges  know 
little  of  the  results  of  their  work.  It  is  even  more  amazing 
that  they  seldom  attempt  seriously  to  find  out  what  changes 
schooling  brings  about  in  students.  Ask  any  school  what  its 
objectives  are  and  you  will  be  told  that  it  seeks  to  develop 
character,  ability  to  think  clearly,  social  responsibility,  good 
health  habits,  readiness  for  earning  a  living.,  knowledge  of 
certain  facts  and  mastery  of  certain  skills.  Ask  whether  the 
school  succeeds  in  doing  these  things,  the  answer  is,  "We 
know  only  in  part."  Half  of  the  boys  and  girls  who  begin  the 
work  of  the  secondary  school  drop  out  before  completing  it. 
Schools  usually  do  not  know  why  these  students  leave  or 
what  becomes  of  them  immediately  afterward.  Few  schools 
know  even  what  their  graduates  are  doing,  what  problems 
they  are  facing,  or  how  well  prepared  they  are  to  solve 
them. 

How  can  this  lack  of  knowledge  and  concern  be  ex- 
plained? There  are  doubtless  many  causes,  but  one  of  the 
most  obvious  is  the  universal  emphasis  upon  the  accumula- 
tion of  credits  for  promotion,  graduation,  and  admission  to 
college.  To  secure  a  credit  or  unit  the  student  must  "pass" 
a  course.  To  pass  a  course  he  must  remember  certain  facts 
and  show  proficiency  in  certain  skills.  Therefore,  remember- 
ing knowledge  and  practicing  techniques  for  examinations 
become  the  purposes  of  education  for  pupils  and  teachers 
alike.  What  goes  on  the  school  record  becomes  the  real 
objective  of  the  student,  no  matter  what  the  school  says  its 
purposes  are.  If  the  pupil  secures  the  required  credits,  he  is 
graduated.  The  job  is  done.  Concentration  on  these  worthy 
but  limited  goals  seems  to  make  teachers  and  students  for- 
get the  larger,  long-range  purposes  of  education. 


xviii  FOREWORD 

One  of  the  major  reasons  for  over-emphasis  upon  these 
limited  objectives  is  that  results  in  these  fields  are  more 
easily  measured  than  in  other  less  tangible  areas.  There  are 
many  instruments  of  evaluation  applicable  to  the  conven- 
tional subjects  of  the  curriculum.  Much  of  the  work  of  such 
organizations  as  the  Educational  Records  Bureau,  the  Co- 
operative Test  Service,  and  the  College  Entrance  Examina- 
tion Board  is  of  great  value  to  schools  and  colleges.  But 
most  tests  available  when  this  Study  began  were  measures 
chiefly  of  accretions  of  knowledge  and  proficiency  in  the 
use  of  skills.  Because  such  tests  are  at  hand  the  teacher  uses 
them.  Because  instruments  of  appraisal  in  other  areas  have 
not  been  available,  the  teacher  tends  to  neglect  other  objec- 
tives and  to  strive  only  for  results  that  can  be  ascertained 
with  relative  ease  and  objectivity. 

It  follows,  then,  that  comprehensive  appraising,  record- 
ing, and  reporting  of  results  are  matters  of  vital  concern  to 
those  who  seek  improvement  in  the  work  of  our  schools  and 
colleges.  The  Eight- Year  Study  has  recognized  die  impor- 
tance of  these  aspects  of  school  work.  To  assist  the  Thirty 
Schools  in  developing  adequate  programs  of  evaluation  and 
reporting,  committees  and  technical  staffs  were  organized 
shortly  after  the  Study  began.  The  Commission  was  fortu- 
nate in  securing  the  services  of  Eugene  R.  Smith  and  Ralph 
W.  Tyler  as  leaders  in  this  work.  This  volume  reports  in 
detail  the  steps  that  were  taken  to  help  the  schools  to  dis- 
cover, record,  and  report  the  progress  of  students  toward  the 
whole  range  of  desired  goals. 

The  work  reported  here  rests  upon  three  basic  convic- 
tions: first,  that  evaluation  and  recording  should  always  be 
directly  related  to  each  school's  purposes;  second,  that  any 
school's  evaluation  program  should  be  comprehensive,  in- 
cluding appraisal  of  progress  toward  all  the  school's  major 
objectives;  third,  that  teachers  should  participate  in  the  con- 
struction of  all  instruments  of  evaluation  and  forms  for 
records  and  reports. 


FOREWORD  xix 

It  is  Impossible  to  estimate  the  wastage  of  material  and 
human  resources  which  results  from  education's  ignorance 
of  the  consequences  of  its  efforts.  Until  schools  and  colleges 
develop  adequate,  comprehensive  appraising  and  recording 
programs,  that  waste  will  continue.  Although  no  one  con- 
nected with  the  Eight-Year  Study  would  claim  that  its  work 
in  these  fields  is  complete  or  entirely  satisfactory,  it  is  clear 
that  what  is  reported  in  this  volume  points  the  way  to  fuller 
knowledge,  more  complete  understanding,  and  wiser  guid- 
ance of  youth. 

WDLFORB  M.  Anoosr 


PREFACE 

«<•  C«- «««-<«• («-<«««• KC- CCC- C<C- «<•  CCO  CCC  CCC' CCC  CCC* «<•  <£««•  <«•  <«•  <«-<«- 

When  the  Directing  Committee  of  the  Commission  on  the 
Relation  of  School  and  College  appointed  a  Committee  on 
Records  and  Reports,  it  assigned  to  this  new  committee  the 
general  task  of  recommending  methods  of  obtaining  and 
recording  information  about  the  pupils.  The  immediate  rea- 
son for  this  assignment  was  the  need  of  supplying  to  the 
colleges  data  upon  which  they  could  decide  about  the  ac- 
ceptability of  candidates  who  did  not  present  the  traditional 
pattern  of  subjects  for  entrance  or  had  not  submitted  the 
usual  entrance  information  in  terms  of  marks  and  examina- 
tions. A  second  important  reason  was  the  desire  of  schools 
for  help  in  their  guidance  programs. 

The  instructions  given  this  committee  specified  as  its  first 
task  the  devising  of  methods  of  obtaining  and  recording  in- 
formation about  personality.  It  was  necessary,  however, 
from  the  beginning  to  try  to  find  ways  of  testing  that  would 
neither  determine  nor  depend  upon  the  content  of  the 
courses  given  in  the  various  schools,  yet  would  be  reason- 
ably comparable  and  objective  measures  of  knowledge  and 
power. 

The  committee  met  with  some  frequency  for  periods  of 
two  or  three  days  at  a  time.  It  soon  announced  to  the 
schools  a  list  of  comparable  tests  that  seemed  to  have  value 
for  estimating  the  degree  of  mastery  attained  by  pupils  in 
various  subject  fields.  Many  of  the  schools  tried  these  tests, 
and  some  added  others  from  quite  a  wide  selection  of  those 
of  an  objective  type.  It  became  apparent,  however,  that 
even  these  tests  were  too  much  influenced  by  the  content 
studied  to  be  acceptable  to  all  of  the  schools.  The  reason 
was  that  the  schools  were  anxious  to  use  the  utmost  flex- 

xx  i 


XX11 


PREFACE 


ibility  in  meeting  the  needs  of  their  pupils  even  when  that 
meant  departing  markedly  from  traditional  subjects  or  their 
content.  A  period  of  experimentation  followed,  during 
which  other  work  was  accomplished.  When  it  was  recog- 
nized that  no  matter  how  valuable  existing  methods  and 
material  for  testing  might  be  for  various  purposes,  never- 
theless they  did  not  fit  the  need  of  the  cooperating  schools 
for  testing  that  would  measure  the  power  attained,  irre- 
spective of  the  way  in  which  it  had  been  reached,  the  Di- 
recting Committee  obtained  further  funds  and  enlarged  the 
branch  responsible  for  testing,  recording,  and  reporting. 

The  final  organization  of  this  department  was  headed  by 
an  over-all  committee  called  the  Committee  on  Evaluation 
and  Recording.  It  had  responsibility  for  determining  pol- 
icies, considering  reports  on  work  accomplished  and  giving 
direction  about  the  next  steps  to  be  undertaken.  Dr.  Ralph 
W.  Tyler  was  engaged  as  Research  Director  for  this  part  of 
the  Eight- Year  Study,  and  was  given  as  his  particular  assign- 
ment charge  of  the  work  on  evaluation.  This  assignment 
included  direction  of  the  follow-up  study  of  graduates  of 
the  cooperating  schools  who  were  attending  college,  as  well 
as  of  the  study  of  objectives  and  of  the  testing  and  other 
evaluation  carried  on  in  the  schools.  Under  Dr.  Tyler's  su- 
pervision the  Evaluation  Staff  and  a  large  number  of  com- 
mittees assisted  in  this  part  of  the  work.  A  detailed  account 
is  given  in  Part  I  of  this  volume. 

The  chairman  of  the  Committee  on  Evaluation  and  Re- 
cording was  given  charge  of  the  production  of  recording 
forms,  and  of  methods  of  reporting  to  the  colleges  and  to 
the  homes.  As  a  part  of  this  work  the  original  Committee  on 
Records  and  Reports,  which  had  in  the  meantime  published 
two  editions  of  the  "Behavior  Description/*  described  in 
Part  II,  was  assigned  the  continued  study  of  personal  char- 
acteristics and  their  recording  and  reporting. 

Other  committees  whose  members  were  chosen  not  only 


PREFACE  xxiii 

from  the  cooperating  schools  but  also  from  colleges  and 
from  schools  and  other  groups  not  definitely  concerned  with 
the  Eight- Year  Study,  worked  on  the  various  problems  con- 
cerned with  records  and  reports  and  were  responsible  for 
the  forms  devised.  Of  much  importance  also  was  the  help 
given  by  the  various  members  of  the  staff.  The  assistance  of 
the  Director  of  the  Study,  the  Research  Director,  the  Cur- 
riculum Assistants  and  the  Members  of  the  Evaluation  Staff 
was  available  both  indirectly,  through  the  results  of  their 
studies,  and  directly  by  means  of  conferences  and  attend- 
ance at  group  meetings.  Dr.  John  W.  M.  Rothney  deserves 
special  mention  since  he  has  been  Research  Assistant  to  all 
of  these  committees  since  the  change  in  organization. 

While  it  is  not  possible  to  list  the  large  number  of  those 
"who  took  part  in  the  work  on  evaluation  and  that  on  record- 
ing, the  committee  in  charge  of  these  activities  wishes  to 
express  its  appreciation  of  the  contributions  made  by  those 
who  assisted.  Without  their  self-sacrificing  cooperation,  little 
could  have  been  accomplished. 

EUGENE  R.  SMITH, 

Chairman 


PART  I 

DEVELOPMENT  AND  USE  OF  EVALUATION 

INSTRUMENTS 

<<K<<^^<<(-<<<-<<<<<<^^^^ 


Chapter  I 

PURPOSES  AND  PROCEDURES  OF  THE 
EVALUATION  STAFF 

4&*&^K&^^ 

How  THE  EVALUATION  STAFF  CAME  INTO  EXISTENCE 

The  plan  of  the  Eight-Year  Study,  as  Dr.  Smith  explained 
in  the  Preface,  placed  upon  the  cooperating  schools  the  re- 
sponsibility for  reporting  in  some  detail  the  characteristics 
and  achievements  of  students  who  were  recommended  for 
admission  to  college.  Furthermore,  the  Directing  Committee 
of  the  Study  expected  the  schools  not  only  to  record  the 
steps  taken  to  develop  new  educational  programs,  but  also 
to  appraise  the  effectiveness  of  these  programs,  so  that  other 
schools  might  benefit  from  their  experience. 

After  the  first  year  it  became  clear  that  these  tasks  were 
too  great  for  them  both  to  be  assumed  by  the  Committee  on 
Records  and  Reports.  The  magnitude  of  the  work  had  be- 
come evident  when  the  Committee  on  Records  and  Reports 
reviewed  the  available  tests,  examinations,  and  other  devices 
for  appraising  student  achievement.  Most  of  the  achieve- 
ment tests  then  on  the  market  measured  only  the  amount  of 
information  which  students  remembered,  or  some  of  the 
more  specific  subject  skills  like  those  in  algebra  and  the 
foreign  languages.  The  new  courses  developed  in  the  Thirty 
Schools  attempted  to  help  students  achieve  several  addi- 
tional qualities,  such  as  more  effective  study  skills,  more 
careful  ways  of  thinking,  a  wider  range  of  significant  inter- 
ests, social  rather  than  selfish  attitudes.  Hence,  the  available 
achievement  tests  did  not  provide  measures  of  many  of  the 
more  important  achievements  anticipated  from  these  new 


4  ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

courses.  Furthermore,  the  content  of  most  significance  in  the 
new  courses  was  frequently  different  from  that  which  had 
been  included  before.  Hence,  the  available  tests  of  informa- 
tion did  not  really  measure  the  information  which  students 
would  be  obtaining  in  the  new  courses.  A  comprehensive 
appraisal  of  the  new  educational  programs  could  not  be  car- 
ried on  unless  new  means  of  evaluating  achievement  were 
developed. 

The  Directing  Committee  obtained  a  preliminary  subsidy 
from  the  General  Education  Board  to  explore  the  possibility 
of  constructing  devices  which  could  be  used  in  appraising 
the  outcomes  of  the  new  work.  During  the  autumn  of  1934, 
the  Thirty  Schools  were  visited,  inter-school  committees 
were  formed,  and  preliminary  steps  taken  to  construct 
needed  instruments  of  evaluation.  By  the  winter  of  1935  it 
seemed  apparent  that  new  instruments  could  be  devised  and 
that  a  more  comprehensive  program  of  appraisal  could  be 
conducted.  Hence,  a  generous  subsidy  for  the  services  of  an 
evaluation  staff1  was  provided  by  the  General  Education 
Board,  and  the  work  was  continued  until  the  close  of  the 

1  During  the  exploratory  period,  Oscar  K.  Euros,  of  Rutgers  University, 
served  as  Associate  Director.  After  helping  to  get  the  plan  outlined,  Mr. 
Euros  resigned  as  Associate  Director  of  the  Evaluation  Staff  and  returned 
to  Rutgers  University.  From  July,  1935,  until  September,  1938,  Mr.  Louis 
E.  Ratns  served  as  Associate  Director.  The  Staff  was  then  housed  at  the 
Ohio  State  University.  When  Mr.  Tyler,  the  Director,  moved  to  the  Uni- 
versity of  Chicago  in  September,  1938,  Mr.  Maurice  L.  Hartung  was  made 
Associate  Director.  Others  who  served  as  members  of  the  staff  at  least 
part  time  for  one  or  more  years  were:  Herbert  J.  Abraham,  Dwight  L. 
Arnold,  Bruno  Bettelheim,  Jean  Friedberg  Block,  Charles  L.  Boye,  Paul 
E.  Diederich,  Wilfred  Eberhart,  Fred  P.  Frutchey,  Paul  R.  Grim,  Chester 
William  Harris,  Louis  M.  Heil,  John  H.  Herrick,  Clark  W,  Horton,  Walter 
Howe,  Carleton  C.  Jones,  W.  Harold  Lauritsen,  Christine  McGuire,  Harold 
G.  McMullen,  Donald  H.  McNassor,  George  V.  Sheviakov,  Hilda  Taba, 
Harold  Trimble,  Cecelia  K.  Wasserstrom,  Kay  D.  Watson,  Leah  Wcisman. 

Throughout  the  years  these  persons  have  worked  together  as  a  unified 
staff.  Although  authorship  of  chapters  is  indicated  in  the  table  of  contents, 
in  a  very  real  sense  this  report  is  a  staff  document,  the  product  of  all 
members  of  the  staff.  Each  chapter  was  criticized  and  revised  several  times 
by  all  those  who  were  members  of  the  staff  at  the  time  the  report  was 
written. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  5 

Study.  The  Evaluation  Staff  was  primarily  concerned  with 
developing  means  by  which  the  achievement  of  the  students 
in  the  schools  could  be  appraised,  and  the  strengths  and 
weaknesses  of  the  school  programs  could  be  identified. 

In  1936  the  first  class  enrolled  in  these  new  programs 
graduated  from  the  Thirty  Schools,  and  most  of  them  en- 
tered college  in  the  fall.  This  provided  an  opportunity  to 
appraise  the  school  programs  in  terms  of  the  success  of  their 
graduates  in  college.  Through  the  generosity  of  the  General 
Education  Board,  funds  were  provided  for  this  study  and  a 
second  division  of  the  Evaluation  Staff2  was  established. 
The  report  of  the  study  of  college  success  appears  in  an- 
other volume.  The  present  volume  is  devoted  to  the  discus- 
sion of  evaluation  in  the  Schools  and  methods  of  recording 
and  reporting. 

SIGNIFICANCE  OF  THE  EVALUATION  PROJECT 

The  term  "evaluation"  was  used  to  describe  the  staff  and 
the  project  rather  than  the  term  "measurement/'  "test/"  or 
"examination"  because  the  term  "evaluation"  implies  a  proc- 
ess by  which  the  values  of  an  enterprise  are  ascertained.  To 
help  provide  means  by  which  the  Thirty  Schools  could  as- 
certain the  values  of  their  new  programs  was  the  basic  pur- 
pose of  the  evaluation  project.  The  project  has  significance 
not  only  for  the  Thirty  Schools  but  for  schools  and  colleges 
generally.  Adequate  appraisal  of  the  educational  program 
of  a  school  or  college  is  rarely  made.  Yet  an  appraisal  of  an 
educational  institution  is  fundamentally  only  the  process  by 
which  we  find  out  how  far  the  objectives  of  the  institution 
are  being  realized.  This  seems  a  simple  and  straightforward 
task,  and  the  efforts  at  evaluation  of  certain  social  institu- 
tions are  not  very  complex.  For  example,  in  the  case  of  a 
retail  business  enterprise  the  most  commonly  recognized  ob~ 

2  Composed  of  John  L.  Bergstresser,  Dean  Chamberlin,  Enid  Straw 
Chamberlin,  Neal  Drought,  William  E.  Scott,  Harold  Threlkeld. 


6  ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

jectives  are  two:  namely,  the  distribution  of  large  quantities 
of  goods  and  the  making  of  profit  from  the  sale  of  these 
goods.  The  methods  for  determining  the  quantities  of  goods 
sold  and  the  profits  are  tangible  and  not  very  difficult  to 
apply.  Hence,  the  problem  of  evaluation  is  not  usually  con- 
sidered a  perplexing  one,  and  although  the  business  enter- 
prise devotes  a  portion  of  its  time  and  energy  to  appropriate 
accounting  procedures,  so  as  to  make  a  periodical  evaluation 
of  its  activities,  we  do  not  find  a  high  degree  of  uncertainty 
about  the  methods  of  evaluation. 

In  education,  however,  the  problem  of  evaluation  is  more 
complex  for  several  reasons.  In  the  first  place,  since  schools 
generally  have  not  agreed  upon  their  fundamental  objec- 
tives, there  is  doubt  as  to  what  values  schools  expect  to 
attain  and  therefore  what  results  to  look  for  in  the  process 
of  evaluation.  Even  when  the  objectives  of  a  school  are 
agreed  upon  and  stated,  they  are  frequently  vague  and 
require  clarification  in  order  to  be  understood.  Furthermore, 
the  methods  of  obtaining  evidence  about  the  attainment  of 
some  of  these  educational  objectives  are  more  difficult  and 
less  direct  processes  than  those  used  in  appraising  a  busi- 
ness. It  is  easy  to  see  how  to  measure  the  amount  of  profit  in 
a  retail  store;  it  is  not  so  easy  to  devise  ways  for  measuring 
the  educational  changes  taking  place  in  students  in  the 
school.  Finally,  the  task  of  summarizing  and  interpreting  the 
results  of  an  evaluation  of  the  school  is  complicated.  Sum- 
maries of  educational  evaluation  are  needed  for  several  dif- 
ferent groups,  that  is,  for  students,  teachers,  administrators, 
parents,  and  patrons.  Each  of  these  groups  may  need  some- 
what different  information,  or  at  least  it  will  be  necessary  to 
present  the  data  in  different  terms.  It  is  easy  to  see,  then, 
that  educational  evaluation  requires  more  intensive  study 
than  evaluation  of  many  other  institutions.  The  work  of  the 
Evaluation  Staff  should  help  to  demonstrate  pi'ocedures  by 
which  the  process  of  evaluation  may  be  carried  on  and  to 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  7 

provide  instruments  and  devices  that  may  be  used  in  evalua- 
tion or  that  may  suggest  ideas  for  the  construction  of  other 
instruments. 

MAJOK  PUBPOSES  OF  EVALUATION 

In  perceiving  the  appropriate  place  of  evaluation  in  mod- 
ern education,  consideration  must  be  given  to  the  purposes 
which  a  program  of  evaluation  may  serve.  At  present  the 
purposes  most  commonly  emphasized  in  schools  and  colleges 
are  the  grading  of  students,  their  grouping  and  promotion, 
reports  to  parents,  and  financial  reports  to  the  board  of  edu- 
cation or  to  the  board  of  trustees.  A  comprehensive  program 
of  evaluation  should  serve  a  broader  range  of  purposes  than 
these. 

One  important  purpose  of  evaluation  is  to  make  a  periodic 
check  on  the  effectiveness  of  the  educational  institution,  and 
thus  to  indicate  the  points  at  which  improvements  in  the 
program  are  necessary.  In  a  business  enterprise  the  monthly 
balance  sheet  serves  to  identify  those  departments  in  which 
profits  have  been  low  and  those  products  which  have  not 
sold  well.  This  serves  as  a  stimulus  to  a  re-examination  and 
a  revision  of  practices  in  the  retail  establishment.  In  a  sim- 
ilar fashion,  a  periodic  evaluation  of  the  school  or  college,  if 
comprehensively  undertaken,  should  reveal  points  of  strength 
which  ought  to  be  continued  and  points  where  practices 
need  modification.  This  is  helpful  to  all  schools,  not  just  to 
schools  which  are  experimenting. 

A  very  important  purpose  of  evaluation  which  is  fre- 
quently not  recognized  is  to  validate  the  hypotheses  upon 
which  the  educational  institution  operates.  A  school,  whether 
called  "traditional"  or  "progressive,"  organizes  its  curricu- 
lum on  the  basis  of  a  plan  which  seems  to  the  staff  to  be 
satisfactory,  but  in  reality  not  enough  is  yet  known  about 
curriculum  construction  to  be  sure  that  a  given  plan  will 
work  satisfactorily  in  a  particular  community.  On  that  ac- 


8  ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

count,  the  curriculum  of  every  school  is  based  upon  hypoth- 
eses, that  is,  the  best  judgments  the  staff  can  make  on  the 
basis  of  available  information.  In  some  cases  these  hypoth- 
eses are  not  valid,  and  the  educational  institution  may  con- 
tinue for  years  utilizing  a  poorly  organized  curriculum  be- 
cause no  careful  evaluation  has  been  made  to  check  the 
validity  of  its  hypotheses.  For  example,  many  high  schools 
and  colleges  have  constructed  the  curriculum  on  the  hypoth- 
esis that  students  would  develop  writing  habits  and  skills 
appropriate  to  all  their  needs  if  this  responsibility  were  left 
entirely  to  the  English  classes.  Careful  appraisal  has  shown 
that  this  hypothesis  is  rarely,  if  ever,  valid.  Similarly,  in  a 
program  of  guidance  the  effort  to  care  for  personal  and 
social  maladjustments  among  students  in  a  large  school  is 
sometimes  based  on  the  hypothesis  that  the  provision  of  a 
well-trained  guidance  officer  for  the  school  will  eliminate 
maladjustments.  Systematic  evaluation  has  generally  shown 
that  one  officer  has  little  effect  unless  a  great  deal  of  sup- 
plementary effort  is  devoted  to  educating  teachers  in  child 
development  and  to  revising  the  curriculum  at  those  points 
where  it  promotes  maladjustments.  In  the  same  way,  many 
of  our  administrative  policies  and  practices  are  based  upon 
judgments  which  in  a  particular  case  may  not  be  sound. 
Every  educational  institution  has  the  responsibility  of  test- 
ing the  major  hypotheses  upon  which  it  operates  and  of 
adding  to  the  fund  of  tested  principles  upon  which  schools 
may  better  operate  in  the  future. 

A  third  important  purpose  of  evaluation  is  to  provide  in- 
formation basic  to  effective  guidance  of  individual  students. 
Only  as  we  appraise  the  students  achievement  and  as  we 
get  a  comprehensive  description  of  his  growth  and  develop- 
ment are  we  in  a  position  to  give  him  sound  guidance.  This 
implies  evaluation  sufficiently  comprehensive  to  appraise 
all  significant  aspects  of  the  student's  accomplishments. 
Merely  the  judgment  that  he  is  doing  average  work  in  a 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  9 

particular  course  is  not  enough.  We  need  to  find  out  more 
accurately  where  he  is  progressing  and  where  he  is  having 
difficulties. 

A  fourth  purpose  of  evaluation  is  to  provide  a  certain 
psychological  security  to  the  school  staff,  to  the  students, 
and  to  the  parents.  The  responsibilities  of  an  educational 
institution  are  broad  and  involve  aspects  which  seem  quite 
intangible  to  the  casual  observer.  Frequently  the  staff  be- 
comes a  bit  worried  and  is  in  doubt  as  to  whether  it  is 
really  accomplishing  its  major  objectives.  This  uncertainty 
may  be  a  good  thing  if  it  leads  to  a  careful  appraisal  and 
constructive  measures  for  improvement  of  the  program;  but 
without  systematic  evaluation  the  tendency  is  for  the  staff 
to  become  less  secure  and  sometimes  to  retreat  to  activities 
which  give  tangible  results  although  they  may  be  less  im- 
portant. Often  we  seek  security  through  emphasizing  pro- 
cedures which  are  extraneous  and  sometimes  harmful  to  the 
best  educational  work  of  the  school.  Thus,  high  school  teach- 
ers may  devote  an  undue  amount  of  energy *to  coaching  for 
scholarship  tests  or  college  entrance  examinations  because 
the  success  of  students  on  these  examinations  serves  as  a 
tangible  evidence  that  something  has  been  accomplished. 
However,  since  these  examinations  may  be  appropriate  for 
only  a  portion  of  the  high  school  student  body,  concentra- 
tion of  attention  upon  them  may  actually  hinder  the  total 
educational  program  of  the  high  school.  For  such  teachers 
a  comprehensive  evaluation  which  gives  a  careful  check  on 
all  aspects  of  the  program  would  provide  the  kind  of  secur- 
ity that  is  necessary  for  their  continued  growth  and  self- 
confidence.  This  need  is  particularly  acute  in  the  case  of 
teachers  who  are  developing  and  conducting  a  new  educa- 
tional program.  The  uncertainty  of  their  pioneering  efforts 
breeds  insecurity.  They  view  with  dismay  or  resentment 
efforts  to  appraise  their  work  in  terms  of  devices  appropriate 
only  to  the  older,  previously  established  curriculum.  They 


io          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

recognize  that  the  effectiveness  of  the  new  work  can  be 
fairly  appraised  only  in  terms  of  its  objectives,  which  in  cer- 
tain respects  differ  from  the  purposes  of  the  older  program. 
Students  and  parents  are  also  subject  to  this  feeling  of  in- 
security and  in  many  cases  desire  some  kind  of  tangible 
evidence  that  the  educational  program  is  effective.  If  this  is 
not  provided  by  a  comprehensive  plan  of  evaluation,  then 
students  and  parents  are  likely  to  turn  to  tangible  but  ex- 
traneous factors  for  their  security. 

A  fifth  purpose  of  evaluation  which  should  be  emphasized 
is  to  provide  a  sound  basis  for  public  relations.  No  factor  is 
as  important  in  establishing  constructive  and  cooperative 
relations  with  the  community  as  an  understanding  on  the 
part  of  the  community  of  the  effectiveness  of  its  educational 
institutions.  A  careful  and  comprehensive  evaluation  should 
provide  evidence  that  can  be  widely  publicized  and  used  to 
inform  the  community  about  the  value  of  the  school  or  col- 
lege program.  Many  of  the  criticisms  expressed  by  patrons 
and  parents  can  be  met  and  turned  to  constructive  coopera- 
tion if  concrete  evidence  is  available  regarding  the  accom- 
plishments of  the  school. 

Evaluation  can  contribute  to  these  five  purposes.  It  can 
provide  a  periodic  check  which  gives  ^direction  to  tber*  con- 
tfnued  improvement  of  the  program  of  the  school;  it  can 
help  to  validate  some  of  the  important  hypotheses  upon 
which  the  program  operates;  it  can  furnish  data  about  in- 
dividual students  essential  to  wise  guidance;  it  can  give  a 
more  satisfactoiy  foundation  for  the  psychological  security 
of  the  staff,  of  parents,  and  of  students;  and  it  can  supply  a 
sound  basis  for  public  relations.  These  purposes  were  basic 
to"  the  Thirty  Schools  but  sthey  are  also  important  to  all 
schools.  For  these  purposes  to  be  achieved,  however,  they 
must  be  kept  continually  in  mind  in  planning  and  in  devel- 
oping the  program  of  evaluation.  The  Evaluation  Staff  real- 
ized that  the  decision  as  to  what  is  to  be  evaluated,  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  n 

techniques  for  appraisal,  and  the  summary  and  interpreta- 
tion of  results  should  all  be  worked  out  in  terms  of  these 
important  purposes. 

BASIC  ASSUMPTIONS 

In  developing  the  program,  the  Evaluation  Staff  accepted 
certain  basic  assumptions.  -Eight  of  them  were  of  particular 
importance.  In  the  first  place,  it  was  assumed  that  educa- 
tion is  a  process  which  seeks  to  change  the  behavior  pat- 
terns jof  human  beings.  It  is  obvious  that  we  expect  students 
to  change  in  some  respects  as  they  go  through  an  educa- 
tional program.  An  educated  man  is  different  from  one  who 
has  no  education,  and  presumably  this  difference  is  due  to 
the  educational  experience.  It  is  also  generally  recognized 
that  these  changes  brought  about  by  education  are  modifica- 
tions in  the  ways  in  which  the  educated  man  reacts,  that  is, 
changes  in  his  ways  of  behaving.  Generally,  as  a  result  of 
education  we  expect  students  to  recall  and  to  use  ideas 
which  they  did  not  have  before,  to  develop  various  skills, 
as  in  reading  and  writing,  which  they  did  not  previously 
possess,  to  improve  their  ways  of  thinking,  to  modify  their 
reactions  to  esthetic  experiences  as  in  the  arts,  and  so  on.  It 
seems*  safe  to  say  on  the  basis  of  our  present  conception  of 
learning,  that  education,  when  it  is  effective,  changes  the 
behavior  patterns  of  human  beings. 

A  second  basic  assumption  was  that  the  kinds  of  changes 
in  behavior  patterns  in  human  beings  which  the  school 
seeks  to  bring  about  are  its  educational  objectives.  The  fun- 
damental purpose  of  an  education  is  to  effect  changes  in 
the  behavior  of  the  student,  that  is,  in  the  way  he  thinks, 
and  feels,  and  acts.  The  aims  of  any  educational  program 
cannot  well  be  stated  in  terms  of  the  content  of  the  program 
or  in  terms  of  the  methods  and  procedures  followed  by  the 
teachers,  for  these  are  only  means  to  other  ends.  Basically, 
the  goals  of  education  represent  these  changes  in  human 


12         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

beings  which  we  hope  to  bring  about  through  education, 
The  kinds  of  ideas  which  we  expect  students  to  get  and  to 
use,  the  kinds  of  skills  which  we  hope  they  will  develop, 
the  techniques  of  thinking  which  we  hope  they  will  acquire, 
the  ways  in  which  we  hope  they  will  learn  to  react  to 
esthetic  experiences — these  are  illustrations  of  educational 
objectives. 

A  third  basic  assumption  was  referred  to  at  the  opening 
of  the  chapter.  An  educational  program  is  appraised  by  find- 
ing out  how  far  the  objectives  of  the  program  are  actually 
being  realized.  Since  the  program  seeks  to  bring  about  cer- 
tain changes  in  the  behavior  of  students,  and  since  these  are 
the  fundamental  educational  objectives,  then  it  follows  that 
an  evaluation  of  the  educational  program  is  a  process  for 
finding  out  to  v&atj3ggrg£  these  changes  in  the  students  are 
actually  taking  place. 

The  fourth  basic  assumption  was  that  human  behavior  is 
ordinarily  so  complex  that  it  cannot  be  adequately  described 
or  measured  by  a  single  term  or  a  single  dimension.  Several 
aspects  or  dimensions  are  usually  necessary  to  describe  or 
measure  a  particular  phase  of  human  behavior.  Hence,  we 
did  not  conceive  that  a  single  score,  a  single  category,  or  a 
single  grade  would  serve  to  summarize  the  evaluation  of 
any  phase  of  the  student's  achievement.  Rather,  it  was  antic- 
ipated that  multiple  scores,  categories,  or  descriptions  would 
need  to  be  developed. 

The  fifth  assumption  was  a  companion  to  the  fourth.  It 
was  assumed  that  the  way  in  which  the  student  organizes 
his  behavior  patterns  is  an  important  aspect  to  be  appraised. 
There  is  always  the  danger  that  the  identification  of  these 
various  types  of  objectives  will  result  in  their  treatment  as 
isolated  bits  of  behavior.  Thus,  the  recognition  that  an  edu- 
cational program  seeks  to  change  the  student's  information, 
skills,  ways  of  thinking,  attitudes,  and  interests,  may  result 
in  an  evaluation  program  which  appraises  the  development 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  13 

of  each  of  these  aspects  of  behavior  separately,  and  makes 
:no  effort  to  relate  them.  We  must  not  forget  that  the  human 
being  reacts  in  a  fairly  unified  fashion;  hence,  in  any  given 
situation  information  is  not  usually  separated  from  skills, 
from  ways  of  thinking,  or  from  attitudes,  interests,  and  ap- 
preciations. For  example,  a  student  who  encounters  an  im- 
portant social-civic  problem  is  expected  to  draw  upon  his 
information,  to  use  such  skill  as  he  has  in  locating  addi- 
tional facts,  to  think  through  the  problem  critically,  to  make 
choices  of  courses  of  action  in  terms  of  fundamental  values 
and  attitudes,  and  to  be  continually  interested  in  better  solu- 
tions to  such  problems.  This  clearly  involves  the  relation- 
ship of  various  behavior  patterns  and  their  better  integra- 
tion. The  way  the  student  grows  in  his  ability  to  relate  his 
various  reactions  is  an  important  aspect  of  his  development 
and  an  important  part  of  any  evaluation  of  his  educational 
achievement. 

A  sixth  basic  assumption  was  that  the  methods  of  evalua- 
tion are  not  limited  to  the  giving  of  paper  and  pencil  tests; 
any  device  which  provides  valid  evidence  regarding  the 
progress  of  students  toward  educational  objectives  is  appro- 
priate. As  a  matter  of  practice,  most  programs  of  appraisal 
have  been  limited  to  written  examinations  or  paper  and 
pencil  tests  of  some  type.  Perhaps  this  has  been  due  to  the 
long  tradition  associated  with  written  examinations  or  per- 
haps to  the  greater  ease  with  which  written  examinations 
may  be  given  and  the  results  summarized.  However,  a  con- 
sideration of  the  kinds  of  objectives  formulated  for  general 
education  makes  clear  that  written  examinations  are  not 
likely  to  provide  an  adequate  appraisal  for  all  of  these  ob- 
jectives. A  written  test  may  be  a  valid  measure  of  informa- 
tion recalled  and  ideas  remembered.  In  many  cases,  too,  the 
student's  skill  in  writing  and  in  mathematics  may  be  shown 
by  written  tests,  and  it  is  also  true  that  various  techniques 
of  thinking  may  be  evidenced  through  more  novel  types  of 


i4          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

written  test  materials.  On  the  other  hand,  evidence  regard- 
ing the  improvement  of  health  practices,  personal-social  ad- 
justment, interests,  and  attitudes  may  require  a  much  wider 
repertoire  of  appraisal  techniques.  This  assumption  empha- 
sizes the  wider  range  of  techniques  which  may  be  used  in 
evaluation,  such  as  observational  records,  anecdotal  records, 
questionnaires,  interviews,  check  lists,  records  of  activities, 
products  made,  and  the  like.  The  selection  of  evaluation 
techniques  should  be  made  in  terms  of  the  appropriateness 
of  these  techniques  for  the  kind  of  behavior  to  be  appraised. 

A  seventh  basic  assumption  was  that  the  nature  of  the 
appraisal  influences  teaching  and  learning.  If  students  are 
periodically  examined  on  certain  content,  the  tendency  will 
be  for  them  to  concentrate  their  study  on  this  material,  even 
though  this  content  is  given  little  or  no  emphasis  in  the 
course  of  study.  Teachers,  too,  are  frequently  influenced  by 
their  conception  of  the  achievement  tests  used.  If  these  tests 
are  thought  to  emphasize  certain  points,  these  points  will  be 
emphasized  in  teaching  even  though  they  are  not  included 
in  the  plan  of  the  course.  This  influence  of  appraisal  upon 
teaching  and  learning  led  the  Evaluation  Staff  to  try  to  de- 
velop evaluation  instruments  and  methods  in  harmony  with 
the  new  curricula  and,  as  far  as  possible,  of  a  non«restrictiv§ 
nature.,  That  is,  major  attention  was  given  to  appraisal  de- 
vices appropriate  to  a  wide  range  of  curriculum  content  and 
to  varied  organizations  of  courses.  Much  less  effort  was  de- 
voted to  the  development  of  subject-matter  tests  since  these 
assumed  certain  common  informational  material  in  the  cur- 
riculum. 

The  eighth  basic  assumptiqn  was  that  the  responsibility 
for  evaluating  the  school  program  belonged  to  the  staff  and 
clientele  of  the  school.  It  was  not  the  duty  of  the  Evaluation 
Staff  to  appraise  the  school  but  rather  to  help  develop  the 
means  of  appraisal  and  the  methods  of  interpretation. 
Hence,  this  volume  does  not  contain  an  appraisal  of  the  work 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  15 

of  the  Thirty  Schools  or  the  results  obtained  by  the  use  of 
the  evaluation  instruments  in  the  schools.  This  volume  is  a 
report  of  the  development  of  techniques  for  evaluation. 

The  evaluation  program  utilized  other  assumptions  but 
these  eight  were  of  particular  importance  because  they 
guided  the  general  procedure  by  which  the  evaluation  pro- 
gram was  developed.  They  showed  the  necessity  for  basing 
an  evaluation  program  upon  educational  objectives,  and  they 
indicated  that  educational  objectives  for  purposes  of  evalu- 
ation must  be  stated  in  terms  of  changes  in  behavior  of  stu- 
dents; they  emphasized  the  multiple  aspects  of  behavior, 
and  the  importance  of  the  relation  of  these  various  aspects 
of  behavior  rather  than  treatment  of  them  in  isolation;  and 
they  made  clear  the  possibility  of  a  wide  range  of  evaluation 
techniques. 

GENERAL  PROCEDURES  IN  DEVELOPING  THE 
EVALUATION  PROGRAM 

The  general  procedure  followed  in  developing  the  evalu- 
ation program  involved  seven  major  steps.  Since  the  pro- 
gram was  a  cooperative  one,  including  both  the  Schools  and 
the  Evaluation  Staff,  it  should  be  clear  that  although  the 
report  was  prepared  by  the  staff,  the  work  was  done  by  a 
large  number  of  persons.  No  one  of  the  instruments  devel- 
oped is  the  product  of  a  single  author.  All  have  required  the 
efforts  of  various  members  of  the  school  staffs  and  the  Evalu- 
ation Staff. 

i.  Formulating  Objectives 

As  the  first  step,  each  school  faculty  was  asked  to  formu- 
late a  statement  of  its  educational  objectives.  Since  the 
schools  were  in  the  process  of  curriculum  revision,  several 
of  them  had  already  taken  this  step.  This  is  not  just  an  evalu- 
ation activity,  for  it  is  usually  considered  one  of  the  impor- 
tant steps  in  curriculum  construction.  It  is  not  necessary 


16          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

liere  to  point  out  that  the  selection  of  the  educational  objec- 
tives of  a  school  and  their  validation  require  studies  of  sev- 
eral sorts.  Valid  educational  objectives  are  not  arrived  at  as 
a  compromise  among  the  various  whims  or  preferences  of 
individual  faculty  members  but  are  reached  on  the  basis  of 
considered  judgment  utilizing  evidence  regarding  the  de- 
mands of  society,  the  characteristics  of  students,  the  poten- 
tial contributions  which  various  fields  of  learning  may  make, 
the  social  and  educational  philosophy  of  the  school  or  col- 
lege, and  what  we  know  from  the  psychology  of  learning  as 
to  the  attainability  of  various  types  of  objectives.  Hence, 
many  of  the  schools  spent  a  great  deal  of  time  on  this  step 
and  arranged  to  re-examine  their  objectives  periodically. 

2.  Classification  of  Objectives 

As  a  second  step,  these  statements  of  objectives  from  the 
Thirty  Schools  were  combined  into  one  comprehensive  list 
and  classified  into  major  types.  Before  classification,  the 
objectives  were  of  various  levels  of  generality  and  specificity 
and  too  numerous  for  practicable  treatment.  Furthermore, 
it  was  anticipated  that  the  classification  would  be  useful  in 
guiding  further  curriculum  development,  because  if  prop- 
erly made  it  would  suggest  types  of  learning  experiences 
likely  to  be  useful  in  helping  to  attain  the  objectives.  A 
classification  is  of  particular  importance  for  evaluation  be- 
cause the  types  of  objectives  indicate  the  kinds  of  evalua- 
tion techniques  essential  to  an  adequate  appraisal.  The 
problem  of  classification  is  illustrated  by  the  following  par- 
tial list  of  objectives  formulated  by  one  school: 

1.  Acquiring  information  about  various  important  as- 
pects of  nutrition 

2.  Becoming  familiar  with  dependable  sources  of  in- 
formation relating  to  nutrition 

3.  Developing  the  ability  to  deal  effectively  with  nutri- 
tion problems  arising  in  later  life 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  17 

4.  Acquiring  information  about  major  natural  resources 

5.  Becoming  familiar  with  sources  of  information  re- 
garding natural  resources 

6.  Acquiring  the  ability  to  utilize  and  to  interpret  maps 

7.  Developing  attitudes  favoring  conservation  and  bet- 
ter utilization  of  natural  resources 

8.  Becoming  familiar  with  a  range  of  types  of  literature 

9.  Acquiring  facility  in  interpreting  literary  materials 

10.  Developing  broad  and  mature  reading  interests 

11.  Developing  appreciation  of  literature 

12.  Acquiring  information  about  important  aspects   of 
our  scientific  world 

13.  Developing  understanding  of  some  of  the  basic  scien- 
tific concepts  which  help  to  interpret  the  world  of 
science 

14.  Improving  ability  to  draw  reasonable  generalizations 
from  scientific  data 

15.  Improving  ability  to  apply  principles  of  science  to 
problems  arising  in  daily  life 

16.  Developing  better  personal-social  adjustment 

17.  Constructing  a  consistent  philosophy  of  life 

These  sample  statements  of  objectives  are  of  different 
levels  of  specificity  and  might  well  be  grouped  together 
under  a  smaller  number  of  major  headings.  Thus,  for  pur- 
poses of  evaluation,  the  several  objectives  having  to  do  with 
the  acquisition  of  information  in  various  fields  could  be 
classified  under  one  heading,  since  the  methods  of  apprais- 
ing the  acquisition  of  information  are  somewhat  similar  in 
the  various  fields.  Similarly,  various  objectives  having  to  do 
with  techniques  of  thinking,  such  as  drawing  reasonable 
inferences  from  data  and  die  application  of  principles  to 
new  problems,  could  be  classified  under  the  general  heading 
of  development  of  effective  methods  of  thinking,  because 
the  means  of  appraisal  for  these  objectives  are  somewhat 


i8          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

similar.  Furthermore,  the  methods  of  instruction  appropriate 
for  these  techniques  of  thinking  have  similarities  even  though 
the  content  differs  widely.  Eventually,  the  following  classi- 
fication was  used  in  general  by  the  Staff: 

MAJOR  TYPES  OF  OBJECTIVES 

1.  The  development  of  effective  methods  of  thinking 

2.  The  cultivation  of  useful  work  habits  and  study  skills 

3.  The  inculcation  of  social  attitudes 

4.  The  acquisition  of  a  wide  range  of  significant  inter- 
ests 

5.  The  development  of  increased  appreciation  of  music, 
art,  literature,  and  other  esthetic  experiences 

6.  The  development  of  social  sensitivity 

7.  The  development  of  better  personal-social  adjust- 
ment 

8.  The  acquisition  of  important  information 

9.  The  development  of  physical  health 

10.  The  development  of  a  consistent  philosophy  of  life 

This  classification  is  not  ideal  but  it  served  a  useful  pur- 
pose by  focusing  attention  upon  ten  areas  in  which  evalua- 
tion instruments  were  needed.3  It  also  helped  to  suggest 
emphases  important  in  the  curricular  development  of  the 
Eight- Year  Study.  The  classification  of  objectives  will  be  im- 
proved as  evidence  accumulates  regarding  the  social  signifi- 
cance of  different  behavior  patterns  and  regarding  the  cor- 
relation and  consistency  among  the  various  specific  reactions 
classified  under  each  type  of  behavior.  Until  such  research 
has  been  carried  farther,  each  school  or  college  will  find 
useful  some  classification  which  serves  the  two  purposes 
suggested. 

3  The  appraisal  of  the  development  of  physical  health,  requiring,  as  it 
does,  technical  medical  training,  was  not  worked  upon  by  the  Evaluation 
Staff. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  19 

3.  Defining  Objectives  in  Terms  of  Behavior 

The  third  step  was  to  define  each  of  these  types  of  objec- 
tives in  terms  of  behavior.  This  step  is  always  necessary  be- 
cause in  any  list  some  objectives  are  stated  in  terms  so  vague 
and  nebulous  that  the  kind  of  behavior  they  imply  is  not 
clear.  Thus,  a  type  of  objective  such  as  the  development  of 
effective  methods  of  thinking  may  mean  different  things  to 
different  people.  Only  as  "effective  methods  of  thinking"  is 
defined  in  terms  of  the  range  of  reactions  expected  of  stu- 
dents can  we  be  sure  what  is  to  be  evaluated  under  this 
classification.  In  similar  fashion,  such  a  classification  as 
"useful  work  habits  and  study  skills"  needs  to  be  defined  by 
listing  the  work  habits  the  student  is  expected  to  develop 
and  the  study  skills  which  he  may  be  expected  to  acquire. 

In  defining  each  of  these  classes  of  objectives,  committees 
were  formed  composed  of  representatives  from  the  Schools 
and  from  the  Evaluation  Staff.  Usually,  a  committee  was 
formed  for  each  major  type  of  objective.  Since  each  com- 
mittee included  teachers  from  schools  that  had  emphasized 
this  type  of  objective.,  it  was  possible  to  clarify  the  meaning 
of  the  objective  not  in  terms  of  a  dictionary  definition  but 
rather  in  terms  of  descriptions  of  behavior  teachers  had  in 
mind  when  this  objective  was  emphasized.  The  committee 
procedure  in  defining  an  objective  was  to  shuttle  back  and 
forth  between  general  and  specific  objectives,  the  general 
helping  to  give  wider  implication  to  the  specific,  and  the 
specific  helping  to  clarify  the  general. 

The  resulting  definitions  will  be  found  in  subsequent 
chapters;  however,  a  brief  illustration  may  be  appropriate 
here.  The  committee  on  the  evaluation  of  effective  methods 
of  thinking  identified  various  kinds  of  behavior  which  the 
Schools  were  seeking  to  develop  as  aspects  of  effective 
thinking.  Three  types  of  behavior  patterns  were  considered 
important  by  all  the  Schools,  These  were:  (1)  the  ability 
to  formulate  reasonable  generalizations  from  specific  data; 


20          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

(2)  the  ability  to  apply  principles  to  new  situations;  and 

(3)  the  ability  to  evaluate  material  purporting  to  be  argu- 
ment, that  is,  to  judge  the  logic  of  the  argument.  When  the 
committee  proceeded  to  define  the  kinds  of  data  which  they 
expected  students  to  use  in  drawing  generalizations,  the 
principles  which  they  expected  students  to  be  able  to  apply, 
and  the  kinds  of  situations  in  which  they  expected  students 
to  apply  such  principles,  and  when  they  had  identified  the 
types  of  arguments  which  they  expected  students  to  ap- 
praise critically,  a  clear  enough  definition  was  available  to 
serve  as  a  guide  in  the  further  development  of  an  evaluation 
program  for  this  class  of  objectives.  This  process  of  defini- 
tion had  to  be  carried  through  in  connection  with  each  of 
the  types  of  objectives  for  which  an  appraisal  program  was 
developed. 

4.  Suggesting  Situations  in  Which  the  Achievement 
of  Objectives  Will  Be  Shown 

The  next  problem  was  for  each  committee  to  identify 
situations  in  which  students  could  be  expected  to  display 
these  types  of  behavior  so  that  we  could  know  where  to  go 
to  obtain  evidence  regarding  this  objective.  When  each  ob- 
jective has  been  clearly  defined,  this  fourth  step  is  not 
difficult.  For  example,  one  aspect  of  thinking  defined  in  the 
third  step  was  the  ability  to  draw  reasonable  generalizations 
from  specific  data.  An  opportunity  to  exhibit  such  behavior 
would  be  provided  when  typical  sets  of  data  were  presented 
to  students  and  they  were  asked  to  formulate  the  generaliza- 
tions which  seemed  reasonable  to  them. 

Another  aspect  of  thinking  defined  in  the  third  step  was 
the  ability  to  apply  specified  principles,  such  as  principles  of 
nutrition,  to  specified  types  of  problems,  such  as  those  relat- 
ing to  diet.  Hence,  it  seemed  obvious  that  at  least  two  kinds 
of  situations  would  give  evidence  of  such  abilities.  One 
would  be  a  situation  in  which  the  student  was  presented 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  21 

with  these  problems,  for  example,  dietary  problems,  and 
asked  to  work  out  solutions  utilizing  appropriate  principles 
of  nutrition.  Another  kind  of  situation  would  be  one  in  which 
the  students  were  given  descriptions  of  certain  nutritional 
conditions  together  with  a  statement  regarding  the  diet  of 
the  people  involved,  and  the  students  were  asked  to  explain 
how  these  nutritional  conditions  could  have  come  about, 
using  appropriate  nutritional  principles  in  their  explanations. 

As  a  third  illustration,  the  definition  of  objectives  identi- 
fied as  one  educational  goal  the  ability  to  locate  dependable 
information  relating  to  specified  types  of  problems.  It  seemed 
obvious  that  a  situation  which  would  give  students  a  chance 
to  show  this  ability  would  be  one  in  which  they  were  asked 
to  find  information  relating  to  these  specified  problems. 

One  value  of  this  fourth  step  was  to  suggest  a  much  wider 
range  of  situations  which  might  be  used  in  evaluation  than 
have  commonly  been  utilized.  By  the  time  the  fourth  step 
was  completed,  there  were  listed  a  considerable  number  of 
types  of  situations  which  gave  students  a  chance  to  indicate 
tibe  sort  of  behavior  patterns  they  had  developed.  These 
were  potential  "test  situations/' 

5.  Selecting  and  Trying  Promising 
Evaluation  Methods 

The  fifth  step  in  the  evaluation  procedure  involved  the 
selection  and  trial  of  promising  methods  for  obtaining  evi- 
dence regarding  each  type  of  objective.  Before  attempting 
to  construct  new  evaluation  instruments,  each  committee 
examined  tests  and  other  instruments  already  developed  to 
see  whether  they  would  serve  as  satisfactory  means  for  ap- 
praising the  objective.  Only  limited  test  bibliographies  were 
then  available.4  In  addition  to  examining  bibliographies,  the 

4  Now,  any  group  working  on  an  evaluation  program  will  find  useful  a 
more  complete  bibliography  of  evaluation  instruments,  such  as  the  Buros 
Mental  Measurements  Yearbook.  This  bibliography  not  only  lists  tests  and 
other  appraisal  instruments  which  are  commercially  available,  but  also  in- 


22          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

committees  obtained  copies  of  those  instruments  which 
seemed  to  have  some  relation  to  their  objectives.  In  exam- 
ining an  instrument  the  committee  members  tried  to  judge 
whether  the  student  taking  the  test  could  be  expected  to 
carry  out  the  kind  of  behavior  indicated  in  the  committee's 
definition  of  this  objective.  Then,  too,  the  situations  used  in 
the  instruments  were  compared  with  those  suggested  in  the 
fourth  step  as  to  their  likelihood  of  evoking  the  behavior  to 
be  measured.  The  committees  recognized  that  they  might 
be  misled  by  undue  optimism  in  the  name  or  the  description 
of  the  test,  and  sought  to  guard  against  it.  Even  though  a 
test  was  called  a  general  culture  test,  or  a  world  history 
test,  or  a  general  mathematics  test,  it  was  generally  found 
that  it  measured  only  one  or  two  of  the  objectives  which 
teachers  of  these  fields  considered  important.  In  order  to 
estimate  what  the  test  did  measure,  it  was  necessary  to 
examine  the  test  situations  to  judge  what  kind  of  reaction 
must  be  made  by  the  student  in  seeking  to  answer  the  ques- 
tions. It  also  proved  useful  to  examine  any  evidence  reported 
which  helped  to  indicate  the  kind  of  behavior  the  test  was 
actually  measuring. 

At  this  point  most  of  the  committees  found  that  no  tests 
were  available  to  measure  certain  major  aspects  of  the  impor- 
tant objectives.  In  such  cases,  it  was  necessary  to  construct 
additional  new  instruments  in  order  to  make  a  really  com- 
prehensive appraisal  of  the  educational  program  in  the 
Thirty  Schools.  The  nature  of  the  instruments  to  be  built 
varied  with  the  types  of  objectives  for  which  no  available 
instruments  were  found.  Every  committee,  however,  found 
it  helpful  in  constructing  these  instruments  to  set  up  some 
of  the  situations  suggested  in  step  four  and  actually  to  try 
them  out  with  students  to  see  how  far  they  could  be  used  as 

eludes  several  critical  reviews  of  these  tests  written  by  teachers,  curriculum 
constructors,  and  test  makers.  These  reviews  help  in  selecting  from  avail- 
able instruments  those  which  might  be  worth  a  trial. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  23 

test  situations.  By  the  time  the  fifth  step  had  been  carried 
through,  certain  available  tests  were  selected  and  tried  out 
and  certain  new  appraisal  instruments  were  constructed  and 
given  tentative  trial 

6.  Developing  and  Improving  Appraisal  Methods 

The  sixth  major  step  was  to  select  on  the  basis  of  this 
greliminary  trial  the  more  promising  appraisal  methods  for 
further  development  and  improvement.  This  further  devel- 
opment and  improvement  was  largely  the  responsibility  of 
the  Evaluation  Staff.  The  committees  met  from  time  to  time 
to  review  the  work  of  the  Staff,  and  many  teachers  were 
asked  to  criticize  and  make  suggestions  for  improvement. 
Obviously,  however,  the  detailed  work  had  to  be  done  by  the 
Staff. 

The  basis  for  selecting  devices  for  further  development 
included  the  degree  to  which  the  appraisal  method  was 
found  to  give  results  consistent  with  other  evidences  regard- 
ing the  student's  attainment  of  this  objective  and  the  extent 
to  which  the  appraisal  method  could  be  practicably,  used 
under  the  conditions  prevailing  in  the  Schools.  The  refine- 
ment and  improvement  consisted  in  working  out  directions 
which  were  unambiguous,  modifying  exercises  which  were 
found  not  to  give  discriminating  results,  eliminating  exer- 
cises which  were  found  to  be  almost  exact  duplicates  of  other 
exercises  in  terms  of  the  type  of  reaction  elicited  from  the 
student,  developing  practicable  and  easily  interpretable  rec- 
ords of  the  student's  behavior,  and  making  other  revisions 
which  gave  more  clear-cut  measures,  which  provided  a  more 
representative  and  adequate  sample  of  the  student's  reac- 
tion, and  which  improved  the  ease  with  which  the  instru- 
ment could  be  used. 

An  important  problem  in  the  refinement  and  improve- 
ment of  an  evaluation  instrument  proved  to  be  the  determina- 
tion of  the  aspects  of  student  behavior  to  be  summarized 


24          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

and  the  decision  regarding  the  units  or  terms  in  which  each 
aspect  was  to  be  summarized.  For  example,  consider  a  test 
constructed  to  appraise  the  ability  of  students  to  formulate 
reasonable  generalizations  from  data  new  to  them.  An  ob- 
vious type  of  test  situation  would  be  one  in  which  sets  of 
data  new  to  the  student  were  presented  to  him  and  he  was 
asked  to  examine  the  data  and  to  formulate  generalizations 
which  seemed  reasonable  to  him.  When  we  approach  the 
question  of  summarizing  his  behavior  in  some  form  which 
provides  a  measurement  or  appraisal,  we  are  faced  with  the 
problem  of  identifying  aspects,  that  is,  dimensions  of  the 
behavior  to  measure,  and  of  deciding  upon  units  of  measure- 
ment to  use.  One  aspect  which  is  important  in  judging  the 
value  of  the  generalization  formulated  is  its  relevance.  Gen- 
eralizations which  have  no  relevance  to  the  data  are  ob- 
viously not  satisfactory.  If  this  aspect  is  to  be  measured, 
there  are  several  possible  units  of  measurement  which  might 
be  used.  For  example,  we  could  set  up  a  subjective  scale  for 
degree  of  relevance  and  have  judges  apply  this  scale  to  each 
generalization,  rating  it  at  some  point  on  this  scale.  Another 
unit  of  measurement  could  be  used  by  classifying  each  gen- 
eralization as  relevant  to  the  data  or  irrelevant  to  the  data, 
thus  measuring  the  relevance  in  terms  of  the  number  of  the 
student's  generalizations  which  are  classified  as  relevant.  On 
the  other  hand,  since  students  may  differ  markedly  in  the 
total  number  of  generalizations  formulated,  a  better  unit  of 
measure  for  the  degree  of  relevance  might  be  the  per  cent  of 
the  student's  generalizations  which  are  classified  as  relevant. 
A  second  aspect  which  has  some  importance  in  appraising 
generalizations  of  this  type  would  be  the  degree  to  which 
relevant  generalizations  are  carefully  formulated  and  in- 
volve no  overgeneralizations,  that  is,  generalizations  more 
sweeping  than  the  data  would  justify.  If  this  aspect  were 
chosen  as  part  of  the  appraisal,  several  possible  units  could 
be  used  in  the  measurement.  One  possible  unit  might  be  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  25 

judgment  of  the  reader  of  the  paper  as  to  the  degree  to 
which  each  generalization  was  carefully  or  incautiously  for- 
mulated. This  kind  of  unit  involves  a  considerable  degree  of 
subjective  judgment  so  that  many  might  prefer  the  simple 
categorization  of  each  relevant  generalization  as  either  going 
beyond  the  data  or  not  going  beyond  the  data.  In  this  case, 
a  unit  of  measurement  might  be  the  per  cent  of  relevant 
generalizations  not  going  beyond  the  data.  Perhaps  these 
illustrations  are  sufficient  to  show  that  it  is  always  necessary 
in  the  development  of  new  evaluation  instruments  or  in  the 
use  of  those  which  have  been  developed  by  others  to  decide 
on  the  aspects  of  the  behavior  to  be  described  or  measured 
and  the  terms  or  units  which  will  be  used  in  describing  or 
measuring  this  behavior. 

7.  Interpreting  Results 

The  seventh  and  final  step  in  the  procedure  of  evaluation 
was  to  devise  means  for  interpreting  and  using  the  results 
of  the  various  instruments  of  evaluation.  The  previous  steps 
resulted  in  the  selection  or  the  development  of  a  range  of 
procedures  which  could  be  used  periodically  in  appraising 
the  degree  to  which  students  were  acquiring  the  objectives 
considered  important  in  a  given  school.  These  instruments 
provided  a  series  of  scores  and  descriptions  which  served  to 
measure  various  aspects  of  the  behavior  patterns  of  the  stu- 
dents. As  these  instruments  were  used,  a  great  number  of 
scores  or  verbal  summaries  became  available  at  each  ap- 
praisal period.  Each  of  these  scores  or  verbal  summaries 
measured  an  aspect  of  behavior  considered  important  and 
represented  a  phase  of  the  objectives  of  the  school.  The  Staff 
then  conducted  comparability  studies  for  certain  of  the  in- 
struments so  that  the  scores  or  verbal  summaries  could  be 
compared  with  scores  or  verbal  summaries  previously  ob- 
tained; by  this  comparison  some  estimate  of  the  degree  of 
change  or  growth  of  students  could  be  made.  However,  the 


26          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

meaning  of  these  scores  became  fuller  through  various  addi- 
tional studies. 

One  type  of  study  involved  the  identification  of  scores 
typically  made  by  students  in  similar  classes,  in  similar  in- 
stitutions, or  with  other  similar  characteristics.  Another  help- 
ful study  involved  a  summary  and  analysis  of  the  typical 
growth  or  changes  made  in  these  scores  from  year  to  year. 
A  tHird  type  involved  studies  of  the  interrelationship  of  sev- 
eral scores  to  identify  patterns.  These  patterns  are  not  only 
useful  when  obtained  among  several  scores  dealing  with  the 
behavior  relating  to  one  objective,  but  are  also  useful  in 
seeing  more  clearly  the  relation  among  the  objectives.  It 
was  pointed  out  in  the  introductory  section  of  this  chapter 
that  human  behavior  is  to  a  large  degree  unified  and  that 
efforts  to  analyze  behavior  into  different  types  of  objectives 
are  useful  but  may  do  some  harm  if  the  essential  interrela- 
tionships of  various  aspects  of  behavior  are  forgotten.  It  was 
found  important  in  this  seventh  step  to  examine  the  progress 
students  were  making  toward  each  of  the  several  objectives 
in  order  to  get  more  clearly  the  pattern  of  development  of 
each  student  and  of  the  group  as  a  whole  and  also  to  obtain 
hypotheses  helpful  in  explaining  the  types  of  development 
taking  place.  Thus,  for  example,  the  evaluation  results  in 
one  school  showed  that  students  were  making  marked  prog- 
ress in  the  acquisition  of  specific  information  and  were  also 
shifting  markedly  in  their  attitudes  toward  specific  social 
issues,  but  at  the  same  time  they  showed  a  high  degree  of 
inconsistency  among  their  various  social  attitudes,  and  were 
making  little  progress  in  applying  the  facts  and  principles 
learned.  These  results  suggested  the  hypothesis  for  further 
study  that  the  students  were  being  exposed  to  too  large  an 
amount  of  new  material  and  were  not  being  given  adequate 
opportunity  to  apply  these  materials,  to  interpret  them  thor- 
oughly, and  to  build  them  into  their  previous  ideas  and  be- 
liefs. A  test  of  this  hypothesis  was  made  by  modifying  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  27 

course  so  as  to  provide  for  a  smaller  amount  of  new  mate- 
rial, the  introduction  of  more  opportunities  for  application, 
and  the  emphasis  upon  thoroughness  of  interpretation  and 
reorganization.  This  revision  in  the  course  resulted  in  corre- 
sponding improvements  in  the  pattern  of  student  achieve- 
ment. If  this  revision  had  not  resulted  in  corresponding 
improvements,  other  hypotheses  which  might  explain  the  re- 
sults would  have  been  considered.  This  procedure  illustrates 
a  useful  means  of  interpreting  the  results  of  several  evalua- 
tion instruments.  It  was  found  that  each  school  needed 
methods  for  interpreting  and  using  the  results  of  appraisal 
so  as  to  improve  the  educational  program  and  to  guide  in- 
dividual students  more  wisely. 

The  usefulness  of  the  evaluation  program  depends  very 
largely  upon  the  degree  to  which  the  results  are  intelligently 
interpreted  and  applied  by  the  teachers  and  school  officers. 
The  Evaluation  Staff,  however,  had  some  responsibility  in 
developing  methods  for  interpreting  the  results  intelligently 
and  in  helping  teachers  and  school  officers  to  use  them  most 
helpfully.  Hence,  in  addition  to  making  these  studies  of  the 
instruments,  members  of  the  Evaluation  Staff  visited  a  num- 
ber of  the  Schools  and  went  over  the  results  with  the  school 
staffs,  suggesting  possible  interpretations  and  indicating 
methods  by  which  these  interpretations  could  be  more  ade- 
quately verified  and  used.  As  a  result  of  these  preliminary 
visits,  certain  methods  of  interpretation  were  developed.  At 
this  point  members  of  the  school  staffs  who  were  participat- 
ing in  si^mmer  workshops  were  asked  to  try  these  methods 
of  interpretation  and  to  criticize  them.  Then,  for  a  period  of 
two  years,  opportunity  was  provided  for  at  least  one  repre- 
sentative from  each  school  to  spend  a  considerable  period 
of  time  in  the  staff  headquarters  to  gain  further  familiarity 
with  the  evaluation  instruments,  with  their  interpretation, 
and  with  their  use.  These  school  representatives  received 
the  training  on  the  assumption  that  they  would  have  oppor- 


28          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

tunity  for  giving  leadership  to  the  evaluation  program  in 
their  respective  schools.  As  a  result  of  this  experience,  the 
staff  believes  that  a  program  of  testing  or  evaluation  can 
reach  greater  fruition  when  a  systematic  attempt  is  made  to 
provide  for  the  training  of  teachers  and  school  officers  in 
the  interpretation  and  use  of  evaluation  results. 

DIVISION  OF  LABOR  IN  THE  EVALUATION  PROGRAM 

The  previous  description  of  the  development  of  the  evalu- 
ation program  explained  that  it  involved  the  cooperation  of 
the  school  personnel  and  the  Evaluation  Staff.  This  does  not 
imply  that  teachers,  school  officers,  and  Evaluation  Staff 
members  were  all  performing  the  same  functions.  Although 
there  was  some  overlapping  of  functions,  there  was  also  a 
general  plan  for  division  of  labor.  One  major  division  of 
labor  was  based  on  the  principle  that  the  school's  duty  is 
to  evaluate  its  program,  while  the  technician's  function  is  to 
help  develop  means  of  evaluation.  Furthermore,  in  follow- 
ing through  the  steps  of  evaluation,  there  was  some  division 
of  duties.  Every  faculty  member  and  school  officer  bore 
some  responsibility  for  the  formulation  of  the  objectives  of 
his  school.  The  classification  of  objectives  into  major  types 
of  behavior  was  largely  a  function  of  the  Evaluation  Staff 
because  the  primary  purpose  of  this  classification  was  to 
place  in  the  same  group  those  objectives  which  involved 
similar  types  of  student  reactions,  and  which  might  con- 
ceivably involve  somewhat  similar  techniques  of  appraisal. 

The  further  definition  and  clarification  of  each  class  of 
objectives  was  the  task  of  an  interschool  committee  com- 
posed of  teachers,  school  officers,  and  members  of  the  Eval- 
uation Staff.  The  staff  members  raised  questions  and  sug- 
gested directions  for  discussion  which  would  help  to  define 
or  clarify  the  given  type  of  objective,  but  most  of  the  defin- 
ing was  done  by  the  representatives  of  the  schools  which 
had  emphasized  this  type  of  objective. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  29 

The  interscliool  committee  also  suggested  situations  in 
which  the  desired  behavior  might  be  shown  by  students. 
The  school  representatives  then  assumed  responsibility  for 
trying  out  these  situations  to  see  if  they  would  serve  as 
means  of  evaluation.  The  review  of  these  trials,  their  criti- 
cism, and  plans  for  improving  the  methods  of  evaluation 
were  carried  on  by  the  entire  committee.  From  this  point  on, 
the  refining  of  the  evaluation  instrument  and  its  develop- 
ment for  constructive  use  was  largely  the  task  of  members 
of  the  Evaluation  Staff.  However,  teachers  and  school  offi- 
cers gave  helpful  criticisms  and  suggestions  and  eventually 
determined  whether  an  instrument  was  worth  using  and 
could  practicably  be  used  in  a  given  school.  Finally,  the 
school  staff  was  expected  to  assume  responsibility  for  obtain- 
ing evidence  of  growth  and  studying  these  results. 

This  plan  has  wide  applicability.  It  provides  a  way  in 
which  technicians  in  testing  and  evaluation  may  work  con- 
structively with  teachers  and  school  officers  to  develop  an 
evaluation  program.  It  avoids  the  danger  on  the  one  hand 
of  having  instruments  constructed  by  technicians  who  are 
not  clear  about  the  curriculum  and  guidance  program  of 
the  school,  and  on  the  other  hand  the  formulation  of  an 
evaluation  program  by  persons  who  are  relatively  unfamiliar 
with  methods  of  describing  and  measuring  human  behavior. 

SUMMARY 

This  brief  description  of  the  steps  followed  in  developing 
the  evaluation  program  should  have  indicated  that  the  proc- 
ess of  evaluation  was  conceived  as  an  integral  part  of  the 
educational  process.  It  was  not  thought  of  as  simply  the 
giving  of  a  few  ready-made  tests  and  the  tabulations  of 
resulting  scores.  It  was  believed  to  be  a  recurring  process 
involving  the  formulation  of  objectives,  their  clearer  defini- 
tion, plans  to  study  students'  reactions  in  the  light  of  these 
objectives,  and  continued  efforts  to  interpret  the  results  of 


30          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

such  appraisals  in  terms  which  throw  helpful  light  on  the 
educational  program  and  on  the  individual  student.  This 
sort  of  procedure  goes  on  as  a  continuing  cycle.  Studying 
the  results  of  evaluation  often  leads  to  a  reformulation  and 
improvement  in  the  conception  of  the  objectives  to  be  ob- 
tained. The  results  of  evaluation  and  any  reformulation  of 
objectives  will  suggest  desirable  modifications  in  teaching 
and  in  the  educational  program  itself.  Modifications  in  the 
objectives  and  in  the  educational  program  will  result  in  cor- 
responding modifications  in  the  program  of  evaluation.  So 
the  cycle  goes  on. 

As  the  evaluation  committees  carried  on  their  work,  it 
became  clear  that  an  evaluation  program  is  also  a  potent 
method  of  continued  teacher  education.  The  recurring  de- 
mand for  the  formulation  and  clarification  of  objectives,  the 
continuing  study  of  the  reactions  of  students  in  terms  of 
these  objectives,  and  the  persistent  attempt  to  relate  the 
results  obtained  from  various  sorts  of  measurement  are  all 
means  for  focusing  the  interests  and  efforts  of  teachers 
upon  the  most  vital  parts  of  the  educational  process.  The 
results  in  several  schools  indicate  that  evaluation  provides 
a  means  for  the  continued  improvement  of  the  educational 
program,  for  an  ever  deepening  understanding  of  students 
with  a  consequent  increase  in  the  effectiveness  of  the  school. 

The  subsequent  chapters  describe  in  more  detail  the  de- 
velopment of  evaluation  instruments  for  certain  types  of  ob- 
jectives. Space  does  not  permit  the  description  of  all  the 
evaluation  instruments  developed.  Tests  of  effective  methods 
of  thinking  are  described  because  this  objective  was  of  con- 
cern to  all  the  schools,  and  few  instruments  of  this  sort  had 
previously  been  developed.  On  the  other  hand,  although 
work  habits  and  study  skills  were  emphasized  in  most  of 
the  schools,  the  description  of  the  instruments  developed  is 
not  included  in  this  report.  The  committee  identified  the  fol- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  31 

lowing  work  habits  and  study  skills  for  which  methods  of 
appraisal  were  needed: 

Range  of  Work  Habits  and  Study  Skills 

1.1  Effective  Use  of  Study  Time 

1.11  Habit  of  using  large  blocks  of  free  time  effectively 

1.12  Habit  of  budgeting  his  time 

1.13  Habit  of  sustained  application  rather  than  working 

sporadically 

1.14  Habit  of  meeting  promptly  study  obligations 

1.15  Habit  of  carrying  work  through  to  completion 

1.2  Conditions  for  Effective  Study 

1.21  Knowledge  of  proper  working  conditions 

1.22  Habit  of  providing  proper  working  conditions  for  him- 

self 

1.23  Habit  of  working   independently,   that  is,  working 

under  his  own  direction  and  initiative 

1.3  Effective  Planning  of  Study 

1.31  Habit  of  planning  in  advance 

1.32  Habit  of  choosing  problems  for  investigation  which 

have  significance  for  him 

1.33  Ability  to  define  a  problem 

1.34  Habit  of  analyzing  a  problem  so  as  to  sense  its  impli- 

cations 

1.35  Ability  to  determine  data  needed  in  an  investigation 

1.4  Selection  of  Sources 

1.41  Awareness  of  kinds  of  information  which  may  be  ob- 

tained from  various  sources 

1.42  Awareness  of  the  limitations  of  the  various  sources  of 

data 

1.43  Habit  of  using  appropriate  sources  of  information,  in- 

cluding printed  materials,  lectures,  interviews,  ob- 
servations, and  so  on 

1.5  Effective  Use  of  Various  Sources  of  Data 
1.51     Use  of  library 

1.511    Knowledge  of  important  library  tools 


32,          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

1.512    Ability  to  use  the  card  catalogue  in  a  library 

1.52  Use  of  books 

1.521  Ability  to  use  the  dictionary 

1.522  Habit  of  using  the  helps  (such  as  the  Index) 

in  books 

1.523  Ability  to  use  maps,  charts  and  diagrams 

1.53  Reading 

1.531  Ability  to  read  a  variety  of  materials  for  a 

variety  of  purposes  using  a  variety  of  read- 
ing techniques 

1.532  Power  to  read  with  discrimination 

1.533  Ability  to  read  rapidly 

1.534  Development  of  a  more  effective  reading  vo- 

cabulary 

1.54  Ability  to  get  helpful  information  from  other  persons 

1.541  Ability  to  understand  material  presented  orally 

1.542  Facility  in  the  techniques  of  discussion,  par- 

ticularly discussions  which  clarify  the  issues 
in  controversial  questions 

1.543  Ability  to  obtain  information  from  interviews 

with  people 

1.55  Ability  to  obtain  helpful  information  from  field  trips 

and  other  excursions 

1.56  Ability  to  obtain  information  from  laboratory  experi- 

ments 

1.57  Habit  of  obtaining  needed  information  from  observa- 

tions 

1.6  Determining  Relevancy  of  Data 

1.61    Ability  to  determine  whether  the  data  found  are  rel- 
evant to  the  particular  problem 

1.7  Recording  and  Organizing  Data 

1.71  Habit  of  taking  useful  notes  for  various  purposes  from 

observations,  lectures,  interviews,  and  reading 

1.72  Ability  to  outline  material  for  various  purposes 

1.73  Ability  to  make  an  effective  organization  so  that  the 

material  may  be  readily  recalled,  as  in  notetaking 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  33 

1.74  Ability  to  make  an  effective  organization  for  written 

presentation  of  a  topic 

1.75  Ability  to  make  an  effective  organization  for  oral 

presentation  of  a  topic 

1.76  Ability  to  write  effective  summaries 

1.8  Presentation  of  the  Results  of  Study 

1.81  Ability  to  make  an  effective  written  presentation  of 

the  results  of  study 

1.811  Habit  of  differentiating  quoted  material  from 

summarized  material  in  writing  reports 

1.812  Facility  in  handwriting  or  in  typing 

1.82  Ability  to  make  an  effective  oral  presentation  of  the 

results  of  study 

1.9  Habit  of  Evaluating  Each  Step  in  an  Investigation 

1.91  Habit  of  considering  the  dependability  of  the  data 

obtained  from  various  sources 

1.92  Habit  of  considering  the  relative  importance  of  the 

various  ideas  obtained  from  various  sources 

1.93  Habit  of  refraining  from  generalization  until  data  are 

adequate 

1.94  Habit  of  testing  his  own  generalizations 

1.95  Habit  of  criticizing  his  own  investigations 

A  number  of  preliminary  instruments  were  constructed 
for  this  extensive  list  of  habits  and  skills.5  Most  of  these 
have  not  been  sufficiently  refined  to  justify  inclusion  in  this 
volume. 

Instruments  for  appraising  social  attitudes  are  treated  in 
the  chapter  on  the  evaluation  of  social  sensitivity.  Because 
so  many  tests  of  information  were  already  available,  and 
because  techniques  for  measuring  the  recall  and  use  of  in- 
formation were  well  understood  by  teachers,  the  committees 
did  not  devote  major  attention  to  developing  further  instru- 

5  A  monograph,  "Study  Skills  and  Work  Habits:  Some  Selected  Mate- 
rials," was  prepared  by  a  committee  headed  by  Cecile  White  Flemming 
of  the  Horace  Mann  School  for  Girls,  and  was  circulated  in  mimeographed 
form  in  1935.  It  is  now  out  of  print. 


34  ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

ments  of  this  type.  A  few  were  constructed  for  specific  pur- 
poses, but  these  are  not  reported  here. 

The  appraisal  of  the  philosophy  of  life  developed  by  the 
students  involves  the  use  of  evidence  from  many  of  the  other 
areas,  such  as  thinking,  social  attitudes,  interests,  apprecia- 
tions, and  social  sensitivity.  Hence,  methods  for  evaluating 
the  student's  philosophy  of  life  are  primarily  methods  of 
combining  and  interpreting  the  results  of  other  measure- 
ments. Methods  of  interpretation  are  discussed  in  Chapter 
VII.  Finally,  the  planning  of  a  comprehensive  evaluation 
program  and  the  problems  of  recording  are  considered. 

It  is  obvious  that  there  are  other  areas  and  other  problems 
in  the  construction  and  use  of  evaluation  instruments  still 
untouched.  The  Evaluation  Staff  hopes,  however,  that  its 
experience  will  be  useful  in  guiding  further  endeavor  so 
that  ultimately  schools  may  be  able  to  evaluate  their  work 
with  a  high  degree  of  comprehensiveness. 


Chapter  II 
ASPECTS  OF  THINKING 


INTRODUCTION 

The  responsibility  of  secondary  schools  for  training  citizens 
who  can  think  clearly  has  been  so  long  and  so  frequently 
acknowledged  that  it  is  now  almost  taken  for  granted.  The 
educational  objectives  classifiable  under  the  generic  heading 
"clear  thinking"  are  numerous  and  varied  as  to  statement, 
but  there  can  be  little  doubt  concerning  their  fundamental 
importance.  Although  in  recent  years  there  has  been  increas- 
ing recognition  of  other  responsibilities  and  purposes,  there 
has  been  little  accompanying  tendency  to  demote  clear  think- 
ing to  a  minor  role  as  an  educational  objective.  It  was  there- 
fore not  surprising  to  find  considerable  emphasis  upon  this 
objective  in  the  statements  of  purposes  submitted  to  the 
Evaluation  Staff  by  the  schools  participating  in  the  Eight- 
Year  Study. 

The  fact  that  an  objective  has  been  stated  frequently  or 
with  emphasis  does  not  insure  that  its  meaning  and  implica- 
tions are  sufficiently  clear  to  guide  effective  teaching  or  to 
serve  as  a  basis  for  the  evaluation  of  achievement.  In  this 
respect  the  "clear  thinking"  objectives  as  originally  stated  by 
the  schools  were  no  different  from  other  even  more  "in- 
tangible" objectives.  An  examination  of  the  pertinent  educa- 
tional literature,  moreover,  revealed  that  most  of  the  available 
analyses  of  these  objectives  were  unsatisfactory  for  the  pur- 
pose of  evaluation.  It  therefore  proved  necessary  to  devote 
considerable  time  to  clarification  of  the  objectives  and  to 
analysis  of  the  behaviors  which  would  reveal  that  students 

35 


36          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

were  achieving  them.  In  the  course  of  the  analysis  it  was 
convenient  to  break  up  the  general  objective  into  a  limited 
number  of  component  parts,  and  then  to  analyze  each  of 
these  in  some  detail.  The  aspects  of  clear  or  "critical"  think- 
ing which  were  selected  dealt  with  the  ability  to  interpret 
data,  with  the  ability  to  apply  principles  of  science,  of  the 
social  studies,  and  of  logical  reasoning  in  general,  and  finally, 
with  certain  abilities  associated  with  an  understanding  of 
the  nature  of  proof.  This  chapter  will  be  devoted  chiefly  to 
the  description  of  each  of  these  aspects  as  they  were  even- 
tually analyzed,  and  to  a  description  of  some  of  the  evalua- 
tion instruments  which  were  developed  to  evaluate  the  asso- 
ciated abilities. 

It  may  be  well  to  note  at  the  outset  that  the  abilities 
involved  in  the  aspects  of  thinking  listed  above  are  over- 
lapping. Although  the  abilities  called  into  action  in  a  sup- 
cessful  interpretation  of  a  set  of  data  seem  to  be  primarily 
inductive,  and  those  utilized  in  the  other  aspects  are  more 
deductive  in  nature,  it  is  neither  necessary  nor  desirable  ^:o 
emphasize  such  distinctions.  In  connection  with  any  gnfpn 
problem,  the  process  of  reflective  thinking,  as  defined  f|y 
Dewey  and  others,  is  likely  to  call  upon  a  number  of  tiie 
abilities  to  be  described  in  connection  with  each  major  aspect 
of  thinking  mentioned  above.  It  should  also  be  noted  that 
other  important  aspects  of  thinking — for  example,  the  ability 
to  formulate  hypotheses — are  only  implicitly  included  in  the 
above  list  and  receive  only  cursory  attention  in  the  following 
discussion.  The  separation  of  clear  thinking  into  these  and 
other  aspects  is  a  product  of  the  analysis  and  is  not  to  be 
considered  as  inherent  in  the  process  of  clear  thinking.  It  was 
convenient  because  it  facilitated  the  exploration  of  the  larger 
objective  and  the  development  of  practicable  means  of  eval- 
uation. A  satisfactory  evaluation  of  the  thinking  abilities  of 
students  involves  a  synthesis  of  the  data  obtained  from  vari- 
ous instruments. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  37 

The  four  major  aspects  of  clear  thinking  listed  above  not 
only  overlap  among  themselves,  but  they  also  overlap  with 
other  educational  objectives.  The  attitudes  and  the  emotions 
of  students  may  influence  their  ability  to  think  clearly  in  cer- 
tain situations.  This  has  been  explicitly  recognized  in  the 
analyses  of  these  objectives  and  in  the  construction  of  the 
evaluation  instruments  to  be  described  in  this  chapter.  At 
the  moment,  it  is  necessary  to  mention  only  that  evaluation 
of  the  disposition  to  think  critically  has  not  been  extensively 
worked  upon  and  is  not  discussed  in  the  following  pages.  In 
the  opinion  of  the  Evaluation  Stajff ,  the  best  available  means 
is  some  sort  of  observational  record,  and  this  method  de- 
mands only  the  simplest  of  techniques  supported  by  alert 
sensitivity  and  perseverance  on  the  part  of  die  observer.  Evi- 
dence of  the  disposition  to  think  critically  collected  by  this 
method  would,  however,  be  a  valuable  addition  to  other  evi- 
dence relevant  to  clear  thinking  of  the  sort  to  be  described 
later. 

The  scope  of  this  phase  of  the  evaluation  project  made  it 
necessary  to  omit  many  details  in  the  discussion  of  some  of 
the  instruments.  For  purposes  of  illustration,  certain  pro- 
cedures are  explained  at  length  in  relation  to  a  selected  in- 
strument, and  are  condensed  or  omitted  elsewhere.  The 
analysis  of  the  application  of  principles  in  the  field  of  social 
science  is  treated  somewhat  differently  from  that  for  the 
natural  sciences,  and  will  consequently  be  found  in  Chapter 
III  on  "Social  Sensitivity/*  The  following  sections  include 
the  analyses  which  were  made  of  the  ability  to  interpret  data, 
of  the  application  of  principles  of  science  and  of  logical 
reasoning,  and  of  abilities  associated  with  an  understanding 
of  the  nature  of  proof.  The  instruments  to  measure  achieve- 
ment that  were  developed  and  some  of  their  technical  char- 
acteristics and  uses  will  also  be  described.  No  account  is 
given  of  similar  instruments  developed  by  individual  teachers. 


38          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

I.  INTERPKETATION  OF  DATA 

ANALYSIS  OF  THE  OBJECTIVE 

The  Committee  on  the  Interpretation  of  Data,  composed  of 
representatives  from  each  school  interested  in  this  objective 
and  members  of  the  Evaluation  Staff,  began  with  two  major 
questions:  What  do  students  do  when  they  interpret  data 
well?  What  kinds  of  data  should  they  be  able  to  interpret? 

Behaviors  Involved  in  Interpretation  of  Data 

Some  conceived  of  interpretation  as  a  complex  behavior 
which  included  the  ability  to  judge  the  accuracy  and  rele- 
vance of  data,  to  perceive  relationships  in  data,  to  recognize 
the  limitations  of  data,  and  to  formulate  hypotheses  on  the 
basis  of  data.  From  the  wide  range  of  behaviors  which  were 
suggested,  the  committee  selected  two  which  seemed  to  them 
to  be  of  paramount  importance:  (1)  the  ability  to  perceive 
relationships  in  data,  and  (2)  the  ability  to  recognize  the 
limitations  of  data. 

The  first  of  these  involves  the  ability  to  make  comparisons, 
to  see  elements  common  to  several  items  of  the  data,  and  to 
recognize  prevailing  tendencies  or  trends  in  the  data.  These 
behaviors  are  dependent  on  the  ability  to  read  the  given  data, 
to  make  simple  computations,  and  to  understand  the  sym- 
bolism used.  It  became  apparent  that  these  operations  vary 
for  different  types  of  data.  Thus  in  the  case  of  graphic  presen- 
tation the  student  must  be  able  to  locate  specific  points  on 
the  graph,  relate  these  to  the  base  lines,  recognize  variations 
in  length  of  bars  or  slope  of  graph  line,  and  so  on.  In  many 
cases,  students  must  understand  simple  statistical  terms  (e.g., 
"average"),  the  units  used,  and  the  conventional  methods  of 
presentation  of  different  forms  of  data. 

A  second  type  of  behavior  which  the  teachers  expect  of 
students  is  the  ability  to  recognize  the  limitations  of  given 
data  even  when  the  items  are  assumed  to  be  dependable.  A 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  39 

student  who  develops  this  ability  recognizes  what  other  in- 
formation, in  addition  to  that  given,  he  must  have  in  order 
to  be  reasonably  sure  of  certain  types  of  interpretations.  He 
refrains  from  making  judgments  relative  to  implied  causes, 
effects,  or  purposes  until  he  has  necessary  facts  at  hand.  He 
recognizes  the  error  in  allowing  his  emotions  to  carry  him 
beyond  the  given  facts  when  he  judges  conclusions  that 
affect  him  personally.  If  he  holds  rigidly  to  what  is  estab- 
lished by  the  data,  the  kinds  of  generalizations  that  he  can 
make  without  qualifications  are  limited.  He  recognizes  that 
many  interpretations  must  be  regarded  as  almost  completely 
uncertain  because  the  facts  given  are  insufficient  to  support 
such  interpretations  even  with  appropriately  stated  quali- 
fications. 

These  behaviors  do  not  preclude  the  possibility  of  making 
qualified  inferences  when  the  situation  warrants.  This  type 
of  interpretation  can  be  made,  for  example,  when  the  data 
reveal  definite  trends.  By  qualifying  the  statement  with 
words  such  as  "probably"  a  student  may  then  extrapolate, 
that  is,  make  interpretations  which  are  somewhat  beyond 
the  facts  but  in  agreement  with  a  definitely  established  trend. 
Or  a  student  may  interpolate.,  in  other  words,  make  a  quali- 
fied inference  concerning  an  omitted  point  between  observed 
points  in  a  set  of  data  which  reveal  an  established  trend.  In 
another  case,  a  student  may  risk  a  qualified  prediction  rela- 
tive to  similar  sets  of  data  applying  to  similar  conditions. 
Even  when  the  inferences  are  qualified,  the  student  must  be 
careful  not  to  allow  his  statements  to  go  far  beyond  the  ob- 
served facts.  These  inferences  are  necessarily  confined  to  a 
rather  narrow  range  whose  extent  depends  somewhat  on  the 
subject  to  which  the  data  apply.  Fundamentally,  the  objec- 
tive involves  making  a  distinction  between  what  is  estab- 
lished by  the  data  alone,  and  what  is  being  read  into  the  data 
by  the  interpreter. 

During  the  analysis  of  the  objective  it  was  also  recognized 


40          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

that  the  ability  to  make  original  interpretations  and  the 
ability  to  judge  critically  interpretations  made  by  others 
might  not  be  closely  related.  When  judging  a  stated  inter- 
pretation one  may  derive  a  clue  that  directs  attention  to 
specific  relationships  in  the  data.  An  original  interpretation 
usually  involves  the  ability  to  perceive  these  relationships 
without  the  aid  of  suggestions  or  directions.  In  the  discus- 
sion of  this  point  it  was  noted,  on  the  one  hand,  that  rela- 
tively few  individuals  have  occasion  to  collect  data  and  make 
original  interpretations,  since  most  of  the  data  encountered 
in  life  are  already  wholly  or  partially  interpreted.  Critical 
judgment  of  these  interpretations  is,  however,  very  impor- 
tant. On  the  other  hand,  it  was  noted  that  some  individuals 
do  have  frequent  need  to  collect  data  and  formulate  original 
interpretations,  and  almost  everyone  has  some  need  of  the 
abilities  involved.  A  decision  was  made  to  concentrate  pri- 
marily upon  evaluation  of  the  ability  to  judge  interpretations 
made  by  others,  and  to  study  the  relationship  between  this 
and  the  ability  to  make  original  interpretations. 

Several  other  behaviors  were  recognized  as  ones  which 
may  be  considered  important  in  connection  with  the  inter- 
pretation of  data.  One  of  these  is  the  ability  to  evaluate  the 
dependability  of  data;  another  is  the  ability  to  formulate 
hypotheses.  In  evaluating  the  dependability  of  data,  a  stu- 
dent might  question  the  competence,  bias,  or  integrity  of  the 
person  who  presents  the  data;  he  might  attempt  to  determine 
the  adequacy  and  appropriateness  of  the  methods,  tech- 
niques, and  controls  used  in  obtaining  the  data;  he  might 
question  the  adequacy  and  the  appropriateness  of  the 
methods  of  summarizing  the  data.  In  formulating  hypotheses 
on  the  basis  of  given  data,  the  student  might  infer  probable 
causes  or  he  might  predict  probable  effects.  Information 
other  than  that  given  in  the  data  may  be  required  in  order 
to  make  a  satisfactory  evaluation  or  to  formulate  a  reasonable 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  41 

hypothesis.  Thus  recall  of  information  might  also  be  re- 
garded as  an  ability  involved  in  the  interpretation  of  data. 

Although  the  importance  of  all  these  aspects  of  interpre- 
tation of  data  was  fully  recognized,  the  teachers  selected  for 
more  intensive  study  those  behaviors  on  which  they  proposed 
to  give  the  greatest  emphasis  in  their  respective  schools. 
Whether  a  student  is  making  original  interpretations  or  judg- 
ing interpretations  made  by  others,  the  teachers  expect  the 
student  who  has  achieved  the  objective  to  perceive  relation- 
ships in  data  and  to  recognize  the  limitations  of  data.  These 
two  important  behaviors  were  therefore  selected  for  par- 
ticular attention  in  developing  evaluation  instruments. 

Kind  of  Data 

The  second  major  question  which  had  to  be  answered  in 
analyzing  the  objective  dealt  with  the  kinds  of  data  that 
students  should  be  able  to  interpret.  The  committee  recog- 
nized several  different  ways  of  classifying  data.  Among  these 
were  the  following:  (1)  according  to  the  form  of  presenta- 
tion, (2)  according  to  the  subject-matter  fields  from  which 
the  data  are  drawn,  (3)  according  to  problems  or  areas  of 
living  with  which  the  data  deal,  (4)  according  to  types  of 
relationships  inherent  in  the  data,  (5)  according  to  the  pur- 
pose the  data  are  intended  to  serve,  (6)  according  to  various 
levels  of  generality,  (7)  according  to  die  degree  to  which  the 
possibility  of  making  meaningful  interpretations  depends 
upon  the  knowledge  of  other  facts. 

The  form  of  presentation  of  data  may  vary.  For  example, 
data  may  be  presented  in  graphical  form.  Pictures,  maps, 
cartoons,  and  various  types  of  graphs,  such  as  line  or  bar 
graphs,  are  familiar  examples.  Data  also  are  often  presented 
in  tabular  form.  Such  tables  are  frequently  found  in  reports 
of  experiments,  election  returns,  scores  of  baseball  games, 
and  so  on.  Sometimes  data  are  not  set  off  from  the  prose 
form  of  reading  matter  but  are  incorporated  in  the  context. 


42          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

This  method  of  presentation  is  often  used  in  editorials, 
printed  speeches,  and  news  items.  Sometimes  the  same  data 
are  presented  in  several  forms;  this  situation  is  commonly 
found  in  advertisements,  for  example. 

Data  may  be  drawn  from  various  subject  fields.  Data  from 
the  fields  of  economics  and  sociology  commonly  appear  in 
newspapers,  magazines,  and  current  books.  Data  from  the 
fields  of  physics,  chemistry,  biology,  and  other  sciences  are 
presented  in  many  publications  which  are  commonly  read; 
advertisements,  for  example,  often  incorporate  data  from 
these  fields. 

The  classification  of  data  in  terms  of  areas  of  living  or 
problems  would  probably  make  use  of  categories  such  as 
vocation,  health,  government,  transportation,  family  relation- 
ships, and  others  of  similar  type.  Classification  according  to 
types  of  relationship  would  emphasize  categories  such  as 
chronological  trends,  relationship  of  parts  to  a  whole,  and 
the  like.  If  data  are  differentiated  in  terms  of  the  purposes 
which  they  are  intended  to  serve,  distinctions  may  be  made, 
for  example,  between  what  purports  to  be  an  impartial 
presentation  of  facts  and  a  presentation  intended  to  sell  a 
particular  idea  or  defend  a  special  interest.  Different  levels 
of  generality  are  illustrated  by  data  showing  unemployment 
in  a  single  city  in  contrast  to  data  on  unemployment  in  an 
entire  state  or  country.  If  the  latter  are  available,  often  more 
meaningful  interpretations  could  be  made  concerning  the 
situation  in  the  single  city,  and  hence  this  same  illustration 
indicates  how  additional  information  may  influence  the  in- 
terpretation, and  how  the  amount  of  such  information  needed 
may  form  a  basis  of  classification. 

Although  other  classifications  are  possible  and  were  con- 
sidered, for  purposes  of  evaluation  the  teachers  chose  the 
following  criteria  for  the  selection  of  the  data  to  be  presented 
to  students  for  interpretation:  (1)  data  presented  in  various 
forms;  (2)  data  relating  to  various  subject  fields;  (3)  data 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  43 

relating  to  major  problem  areas;  (4)  data  including  various 
types  of  relationships.  As  is  often  the  case,  these  criteria  are 
not  independent,  and  a  given  set  of  data  will  satisfy  several 
criteria  simultaneously. 

In  order  that  the  interpretation  may  not  be  made  from 
memory,  it  is  necessary  that  the  data  be  "new"  to  the  student 
in  the  sense  that  this  particular  organization  of  the  facts  has 
not  previously  been  interpreted  for  the  student  by  someone 
else.  If  he  has  heard  or  read  an  interpretation  of  this  or- 
ganization of  facts,  his  response  may  represent  recall  of  an 
interpretation  made  by  another  and  not  give  a  measure  of 
his  own  ability  to  interpret. 

The  analysis  of  the  objective  thus  resulted  not  only  in  a 
description  of  the  behaviors  which  might  be  included  under 
the  phrase  "interpretation  of  data,"  but  also  in  a  conscious 
restriction  of  the  scope  of  the  eventual  evaluation.  This  re- 
striction applied  to  the  types  of  behavior  which  were  to  be 
emphasized,  and  to  the  criteria  for  the  selection  of  data 
which  were  to  be  presented  to  students. 

THE  DEVELOPMENT  OF  EVALUATION  INSTRUMENTS 

Preliminary  Investigations 

Observations  of  a  student's  many  overt  behaviors  in  re- 
sponding to  data  of  various  kinds  is  one  way  in  which  evi- 
dence of  his  ability  to  interpret  data  may  be  obtained.  This 
type  of  evidence  can  probably  be  best  secured  by  observa- 
tional records  kept  by  teachers  or  other  persons  trained  to 
observe  and  record  these  behaviors.  Under  certain  condi- 
tions a  student's  written  materials,  such  as  laboratory  note- 
books, papers,  etc.,  may  be  a  fruitful  source  of  evidence. 
However,  the  time  consumed  and  the  possible  lack  of  ob- 
jectivity of  scores  present  serious  difficulties  in  the  use  of 
these  techniques.  Since  these  methods  usually  involved  more 
or  less  uncontrolled  situations,  teachers  were  interested  in 
devising  a  method  that  would  better  stabilize  some  of  the 


44          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

variable  factors.  The  method  which  was  selected  makes  use 
of  pencil  and  paper  tests  in  which  the  student  reacts  in  writ- 
ing to  written  data.  Many  methods  of  obtaining  this  type  of 
evidence  have  been  experimented  with  in  the  Study.  A  few 
will  be  discussed  to  present  some  of  the  approaches  used 
and  some  of  the  difficulties  the  Evaluation  Staff  has  encoun- 
tered in  measuring  the  abilities  involved.  One  of  the  most 
direct  methods  used  was  to  present  the  student  with  sets  of 
written  data,  ask  him  to  write  true  statements  concerning 
the  data,  and  to  appraise  the  interpretations  which  he  wrote. 
However,  such  a  free-response  essay-form  presents  several 
difficulties  in  evaluation.  It  was  found  that  even  when  the 
number  of  interpretations  to  be  made  is  specified  in  the  di- 
rections, individual  students  tend  to  use  a  narrow  range  of 
relationships  in  their  responses.  Thus,  the  responses  do  not 
adequately  sample  the  types  of  interpretations  which  the 
students  are  capable  of  making  when  their  attention  is  fo- 
cussed  on  data  relating  to  their  own  particular  problems  or 
concerns,  or  when  breadth  of  treatment  is  encouraged  by 
more  specific  directions  in  the  test.  Moreover,  great  difficulty 
is  experienced  in  scoring  such  a  test,  for  it  is  often  impossible 
to  be  reasonably  sure  what  the  student  means  by  his  written 
statements.  This  perplexity  may  arise  from  ambiguity  or  in- 
completeness of  student's  statements  or  from  peculiarities  in 
his  style.  It  is  possible  to  attain  high  objectivity  for  such  a 
test,  but  only  after  elaborate  criteria  for  scoring  have  been 
carefully  set  up.  Even  with  such  a  device,  it  is  a  time-con- 
suming method.  In  one  case,  for  example,  it  required  ap- 
proximately 90  hours  for  each  of  the  trained  markers  to  score 
193  papers  of  ten  exercises  calling  for  responses  of  this  type. 
Because  of  these  difficulties,  this  method  of  getting  evidence 
of  a  student's  ability  to  interpret  data  is  impractical  for  most 
teachers. 

In  order  to  determine  the  types  of  interpretations  students 
should  be  expected  to  judge  critically  and  the  kinds  of  errors 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  45 

commonly  made  in  interpreting  data,  a  study  was  made  of 
interpretations  commonly  found  in  editorials,  advertisements, 
news  items,  reports  of  scientific  experiments,  and  similar 
materials.  For  instance,  the  conclusions  of  many  reports  of 
experiments  were  critically  studied  in  relation  to  the  data  on 
which  they  were  based.  In  this  and  other  such  studies  it  was 
possible  to  discover  the  kinds  of  relationships  involved  in 
the  interpretations,  the  kind  of  assumptions  that  were  made, 
the  accuracy  and  adequacy  of  the  inferences  made  from  the 
data.  When  students'  essay  responses  were  also  critically 
studied  in  the  same  way  and  comparisons  made,  it  became 
apparent  that  the  interpretations  from  both  these  sources 
were  susceptible  to  virtually  the  same  types  of  classifications. 
One  classification  that  could  be  made  was  in  terms  of  the 
kind  of  relationships  involved.  For  convenience  of  reference, 
these  types  are  denoted  by  various  words  or  phrases,  such  as 
"extrapolation,"  "comparison  of  points,"  or  "cause."  They  are 
as  follows:1 

1.  Reading  Points.  This  type  of  statement  is  usually 
merely  a  restatement  of  the  data. 

2.  Comparison  of  Points.  The  statement  is  a  comparison 
of  two  or  more  items  or  "points"  in  the  data. 

3.  Cause.  The  statement  presents  a  cause  of  conditions 
presented  in  the  data. 

4.  Effect.  The  statement  formulates  a  prediction  of  a 
probable  effect  of  the  conditions  described. 

5.  Value  Judgment.  The  statement  presents  a  recom- 
mended course  of  action  suggested  by  the  data,  or  an 
opinion  of  what  ought  to  be  or  ought  not  to  be. 

6.  Recognition  of  Trend.  The  statement  describes  a  pre- 
vailing tendency  or  trend  in  the  data. 

7.  Comparison  of  Trends.  The  statement  presents   a 

1  For  examples  of  statements  of  these  types,  see  the  sample  problem  on 
page  52. 


46          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

comparison  of  two  or  more  prevailing  tendencies  or 
trends  in  the  data. 

8.  Extrapolation.  The  statement  formulates  a  prediction 
of  a  point  or  item  or  fact  which  is  not  given  in  the 
data  and  lies  beyond  points  or  items  or  facts  which 
are  given  in  the  data. 

9.  Interpolation.  The  statement  formulates  a  prediction 
of  a  point  or  item  or  fact  of  data  which  lies  between 
points  or  facts  which  are  given  in  the  data. 

10.  Sampling.  The  statements  concern  ( a )  only  a  part  of 
the  group  described  in  the  data,  or  (b)  a  larger  group 
containing  as  a  part  of  itself  the  group  described  in 
the  data. 

11.  Purpose.  The  statement  presents  a  judgment  of  pur- 
pose of  the  given  data. 

These  types  of  interpretations  may  be  also  arranged  into 
a  concise  and  meaningful  classification  which  emphasizes  the 
difference  in  degree  of  accuracy  with  which  they  are  used 
by  students.  Thus,  students'  responses  may  include  the  fol- 
lowing: 

1.  Interpretations  which  are  accurate.  These  interpreta- 
tions may  formulate  comparisons,  trends,  and  specific 
facts  which  are  established  by  the  data  as  true  or 
false  and  are  correctly  stated  without  qualification. 
Other  interpretations  under  this  classification  may  be 
concerned  with  sampling,  extrapolation,  or  interpola- 
tion. They  are  not  fully  supported  by  the  given  data, 
but  are  probably  true  or  probably  false  on  the  basis 
of  the  trends  established  in  the  data,  and  are  stated 
by  the  student  with  sufficient  qualification. 

2.  Interpretations  which  are  overgeneralizations — that 
is,  interpretations  containing  unqualified  or  unwar- 
ranted statements  involving  interpolation,  extrapola- 
tion, and  sampling,  or  statements  of  cause,  purpose, 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  47 

effect  which  cannot  be  established  by  the  given  data 
^even  in  qualified  form.  This  type  of  error  may  be  re- 
ferred to  as  "going  beyond  the  data." 

3.  Interpretations  which  are  undergeneralizations — that 
is,  which  involve  unnecessarily  qualified  statements 
concerning  specific  facts,  trends,  and  comparisons 
which  are  established  in  the  data.  Such  departures 
from  accuracy  may  be  referred  to  as  "overcaution."2 

4.  Interpretations  which  involve  "crude  errors'";  for  ex- 
ample, the  student  errs  by  misreading  the  points  or 
trends  in  the  data,  by  failing  to  understand  meanings 
of  terms,  such  as  "average"  and  "per  cent,"  or  by 
failing  to  relate  properly  the  data  of  a  graph  to  the 
base  lines. 

Such  analyses  provided  a  basis  for  construction  of  a  short- 
answer  type  of  test  exercise.  This  type  of  test  does  not  pre- 
sent the  difficulties  in  scoring  inherent  in  the  essay  form  and 
makes  it  possible  for  a  student  to  react  to  many  types  of  data 
in  a  limited  time.  During  its  development,  the  short-answer 
test  has  passed  through  several  transitional  forms.  Analysis 
and  statistical  study  of  early  forms  suggested  changes  which 
were  incorporated  in  subsequent  forms.  For  the  sake  of  sim- 
plicity of  explanation,  only  the  latest  form  of  the  interpreta- 
tion of  data  test  (Form  2.52)  will  be  described  in  detail. 

Structure  of  Interpretation  of  Data  Test,  Form  2.52 

The  test  to  be  described  is  intended  primarily  for  the 
senior  high  school  level^  It  contains  ten  sets  of  data  selected 
to  satisfy  the  criteria  set  up  by  the  committee  interested  in 
the  objective.  These  data  are  presented  in  various  forms, 
including  tables,  prose,  charts,  and  different  kinds  of  graphs. 
The  problems  are  selected  from  several  fields  ( such  as  medi- 

2  Overcaution  is  not  considered  an  error  by  everyone.  Some  consider  it 
evidence  of  a  tendency  to  suspend  judgment  until  further  evidence  is 
available. 


48          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

cine,  home  economics,  sociology,  genetics )  and  contain  data 
pertinent  to  such  topics  as  technological  unemployment, 
heredity,  crop  rotation,  immigration,  government  expendi- 
tures, and  health. 

Each  set  of  data  is  followed  by  15  statements  which  pur- 
port to  be  interpretations.  The  student  is  asked  to  indicate 
his  judgment  of  each  of  the  statements  by  placing  it  in  one 
of  five  categories  as  indicated  by  the  short  code  given  at  the 
top  of  the  sample  exercise  on  page  52.  In  the  sample,  the 
list  of  responses  accepted  as  correct  by  a  jury  of  competent 
persons  is  given  in  the  margin  before  each  interpretation.  A 
word  or  phrase  describing  the  main  kind  of  relationship  in- 
volved follows  each  interpretation. 

A  study  of  the  sample  exercise  in  relation  to  the  following 
summary  of  the  procedure  used  in  constructing  the  test  will 
indicate  how  the  analyses  described  previously  were  utilized. 
It  may  also  serve  as  a  guide  for  teachers  who  wish  to  con- 
struct similar  tests  suited  for  use  with  their  own  students. 

1.  The  data  were  selected  according  to  the  criteria  set 
up  by  the  committee. 

2.  Fifteen  interpretative  statements  were  made  from 
each  set  of  data.  The  types  of  statements  included 
were  based  on  an  analysis  of  types  of  interpretations 
which  were  found  in  current  literature,  the  judgment 
of  teachers  who  were  concerned  with  the  objective, 
and  the  analysis  of  responses  of  students  who  were 
asked  to  write  original  interpretations.  This  approach 
was  used  both  to  give  the  students  an  opportunity  to 
judge  statements  including  typical  errors  made  in 
interpretations,  and  to  insure  the  inclusion  in  the  test 
of  types  of  interpretations  which  students  encounter 
and  are  capable  of  recognizing.  These  interpretations 
involve  the  following  types  of  behaviors:  comparisons 
of  points  of  data,  recognition   and  comparison  of 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  49 

trends,  judgments  of  cause,  effect,  purpose,  value, 
analogy,3  extrapolation,  interpolation,  and  sampling. 
3.  The  types  of  relationship  involved  in  the  interpreta- 
tions which  the  students  are  asked  to  judge  were  dis- 
tributed among  the  five  response  categories  as  fol- 
lows: 

a.  Interpretations  adequately  supported  by  the  data, 
and  so  worded  that  they  are  meant  to  be  judged 
by  the  students  as  true.  These  statements  require 
the  student  to  judge  interpretations  that  involve: 
comparison  of  points  in  die  data;  recognition  of 
trends;  and  comparison  of  trends.  Ten  per  cent  of 
the  total  number  of  statements  in  the  test  are  in 
this  category.4 

b.  Interpretations   inadequately   supported  by   the 
data,  so  worded  that  they  are  meant  to  be  judged 
probably  true.  These  statements  require  the  stu- 
dents   to   judge    interpretations    that   involve    a 
knowledge  of  the  principles  of  prudent  extrapola- 
tion, interpolation,  and  sampling  as  previously  de- 
fined. They  include  inferences  that  go  beyond  the 
data  but  are  suggested  by  the  data  and  are  based 
on  trends  or  facts  in  the  data.  They  also  include 
some  conclusions  that  would  be  popularly  inter- 
preted as  true.  They  are  intended  to  contribute 
information  concerning  the  ability  of  students  to 
recognize  the  necessity  for  qualification  in  inter- 
pretation. About  20  per  cent  of  the  total  number  of 
statements  are  iji  this  category. 

c.  Interpretations   inadequately   supported   by   the 

3  Although  in  this  Study  analogy  was  not  found  to  be  used  to  any  great 
extent  in  student-written  interpretations  of  data,  this  type  of  interpretation 
is  encountered  extensively  in  advertising,  newspaper  articles,  etc.   It  was 
also  the  thought  of  the  Evaluation  Staff  that  analogy  is  one  aspect  of 
scientific  thinking  which  they  desired  to  measure  in  several  different  con- 
texts. It  appears  also  in  the  Application  of  Principles  of  Science  tests. 

4  This  distribution  was  based  upon  studies  of  reliabilities  of  early  forms. 


50          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

data,  so  worded  that  they  are  meant  to  be  judged 
as  based  upon  insufficient  data.  They  give  oppor- 
tunity for  the  student  to  make  judgments  concern- 
ing statements  of  analogies  relating  to  the  data, 
concerning  statements  referring  to  a  cause  or  an 
effect  of  the  situation  revealed  by  the  data,  con- 
cerning the  purpose  the  data  are  supposed  to 
serve,  and  concerning  a  recommended  course  of 
action  supposedly  desirable  on  the  basis  of  the 
data.  Also  included  are  some  statements  depend- 
ing upon  an  injudicious  use  of  interpolation.,  extra- 
polation, and  sampling.  About  40  per  cent  of  the 
total  number  of  statements  are  in  this  category. 

d.  Interpretations   inadequately   supported   by   the 
data,  so  worded  that  they  are  meant  to  be  judged 
probably  false.  These  include  inferences  which  are 
suggested  by  the  data  but  which  are  contrary  to  the 
trends  of  facts  in  the  data,  and  conclusions  which 
would  be  popularly  interpreted  as  false.  The  same 
types  of  interpretations  are  used  here  as  in  b. 
Twenty  per  cent  of  the  total  number  of  statements 
are  in  this  category. 

e.  Interpretations   which   are   contradicted   by   the 
data,  so  worded  that  they  are  meant  to  be  judged 
as  false.  These  statements  involve  the  same  types 
of  interpretations  as  are  listed  in  a  above.  Ten  per 
cent  of  the  total  number  of  statements  are  in  this 
category. 

4.  Within  each  test  exercise  the  interpretations  were  ar- 
ranged in  random  order.  Directions  to  the  students 
were  formulated.  These  directions  asked  students  to 
place  each  statement  in  one  of  the  five  different 
categories. 

Before  the  test  was  considered  ready  for  use,  an  analysis 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  51 

of  student  responses  was  made.  In  each  case  where  the  judg- 
ment of  a  large  number  of  students  conflicted  with  the  key, 
there  was  an  attempt  to  analyze  the  student's  thinking  to  see 
if  the  conflict  in  judgment  was  due  to  confusion  in  the  test 
or  to  an  erroneous  concept  held  by  the  students.  Ambiguous 
statements  were  revised,  and  a  final  key  was  drawn  up.  The 
scores  made  by  students  are,  therefore,  to  be  considered  as 
a  means  of  comparison  of  their  thinking  with  the  judgments 
of  the  jury. 

Summarization  of  Scores 

For  purposes  of  exposition,  the  manner  in  which  the  an- 
swer sheets  from  a  class  are  scored  may  be  described  as  fol- 
lows. By  tabulating  a  student's  response  for  each  item  in. 
relation  to  the  jury's  key  for  that  item  in  the  proper  cell  of 
the  following  chart,  a  teacher  can  describe  student's  achieve- 
ment both  as  to  accuracy  and  as  to  errors.5 

As  indicated  by  the  chart,  student  responses  can  be  de- 
scribed in, the  following  terms:  general  accuracy,  caution, 
beyond  data,  and  crude  errors.  This  terminology  may  be  de- 
fined as  follows:  General  accuracy  means  the  extent  to  which 
the  student  agrees  with  the  jury — that  is,  recognizes  true 
statements  as  true,  probably  true  as  probably  true,  etc.  The 
total  number  of  statements  which  a  student  judged  accu- 
rately may  be  found  by  counting  all  of  the  tally  marks  in  the 
cells  labeled  a,  g,  m,  s,  and  y.  This  number  may  be  expressed 
as  a  per  cent  of  the  maximum  possible  number  of  correct  re- 
sponses (150). 

Since  the  judgment  of  the  accuracy  of  the  statements  in- 
volves different  levels  of  discrimination,  depending  on 
whether  or  not  the  interpretation  needs  to  be  qualified,  it 
was  found  helpful  to  derive  the  following  subscores  on  ac- 
curacy: (a)  accuracy  with  probably  true  and  probably  false 

5  In  practice,  the  scoring  may  be  done  on  the  electric  scoring  machine, 
or  if  one  is  not  available,  by  use  of  punched  key  stencils. 


SAMPLE  EXERCISE  FROM  FORM  2.52 

(1)  are  sufficient  to  make  the  statement  true. 

(2)  are  sufficient  to  indicate  that  the  statement  is 
probably  true. 

These    (3 )  are  not  sufficient  to  indicate  whether  there  is  any 
Data  degree  of  truth  or  falsity  in  the  statement. 

Alone    (4)  are  sufficient  to  indicate  that  the  statement  is 

probably  false. 
(5)  are  sufficient  to  make  the  statement  false. 

PROBLEM  i.  This  chart  shows  production.,  population,  and  em- 
ployment on  farms  in  the  United  States  for  each 
fifth  year  between  1900  and  1925. 


oJ-OU 
en 

T-< 

03 
£ 

o!40 

0 

Il20 

<L) 
OS 

•g 

O) 

I1001 

/ 

Volume  of  Farm 
Production 

Farm  Population  of 
Employable  Age 

Number  of  Farm 
Workers  Employed 
25 

^ 

r^ 

•Z^~- 

^-  — 

.,  •  

-  —  —  —  - 

900        1905          1910           1915          1920           19 

Statements 

1.  The  ratio  of  agricultural  production  to  the  number  of 
farm  workers  increased  every  five  years  between  1900 
and  1925. 

2.  The  increase  in  agricultural  production  between  1910 
and  1925  was  due  to  more  widespread  use  of  farm  ma- 
chinery. 


3.  The  average  number  of  farm  workers  employed  during 
the  period  1920  to  1925  was  higher  than  during  the 
period  1915  to  1920. 

4.  The  government  should  give  relief  to  farm  workers  who 
are  unemployed. 

5.  Between  1900  and  1925,  the  amount  of  fruit  produced  on 
farms  in  the  United  States  increased  about  fifty  per  cent. 

6.  During  the  entire  period  between  1905  and  1925  there 
was  an  excess  of  farm  population  of  employable  age  over 
the  number  of  people  needed  to  operate  farms. 

7.  Wages  paid  farm  workers  in  1925  were  low  because  there 
were  more  laborers  than  could  be  employed. 

8.  More  workers  were  employed  on  farms  in  1925  than  in 
1900. 

9.  Since  1900,  there  has  been  an  increase  in  production  per 
worker  in  manufacturing  similar  to  the  increase  in  agri- 
culture. 

10.  Between  1900  and  1925,  the  volume  of  farm  production 
increased  over  fifty  per  cent. 

11.  Farmers  increased  production  after  1910  in  order  to  take 
advantage  of  rapidly  rising  prices. 

12.  The  average  amount  of  farm  production  was  higher  in 
the  period  1925  to  1930  than  in  the  period  1920  to  1925. 

13.  Between  1900  and  1925,  there  was  an  increase  in  the 
farm  population  of  employable  age  in  the  Middle  West, 
the  largest  farming  area  in  the  United  States. 

14.  Farm  population  of  employable  age  was  lower  in  1930 
than  in  1900. 

15.  The  production  of  wheat,  the  largest  agricultural  crop  in 
the  United  States,  was  as  great  in  1915  as  in  1925. 


54          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

CHART  SHOWING   HOW  SCORES   ARE   DERIVED 


\v      Key 

Student\ 
Responses   \ 

True 

Probably 
True 

Insufficient 
Data 

Probably 
False 

False 

True 

Accurate 
a 

Beyond 
Data 
b 

Beyond 
Data 
c 

Crude 
Error 
d 

Crude 
Error 
e 

Probably 
True 

Caution 
f 

Accurate 

g  ' 

Beyond 
Data 
h 

Crude 
Error 
i 

Crude 
Error 
j 

Insufficient 
Data 

Caution 
k 

Caution 
1 

Accurate 
m 

Caution 
n 

Caution 

0 

Probably 
False 

Crude 
Error 

P 

Crude 
Error 

q 

Beyond 
Data 
r 

Accurate 
s 

Caution 
t 

False 

Crude 
Error 
u 

Crude 
Error 

V 

Beyond 
Data 
w 

Beyond 
Data 
x 

Accurate 

y 

statements,  (b)  accuracy  with  insufficient  data  statements, 
and  (c)  accuracy  with  true  and  false  statements.  They  indi- 
cate the  extent  to  which  the  student  agrees  with  the  jury  in 
judging  these  three  types  of  statements  taken  separately. 

The  first  of  these  subscores  may  be  computed  by  counting 
the  tallies  in  cells  g  and  s,  and  expressing  this  number  as  a 
per  cent  of  the  maximum  possible  number  of  such  responses 
(59  in  the  case  of  the  test  under  discussion).  The  second 
subscore  mentioned  above  is  derived  from  the  number  of 
tallies  in  cell  m  (expressed  as  a  per  cent  of  61).  The  third 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  55 

subscore  is  derived  from  the  number  of  tallies  in  cells  a  and 
y  (expressed  as  a  per  cent  of  30). 

The  going  beyond  the  data  score  indicates  the  extent  to 
which  the  student  marks  statements  keyed  probably  true  as 
true,  statements  keyed  insufficient  data  as  probably  true  or 
probably  false,  and  statements  keyed  probably  false  as  false. 
The  student  is  then  granting  the  interpretation  greater  cer- 
tainty than  is  warranted  by  the  data. 

In  order  to  determine  how  frequently  a  student  has  "gone 
beyond  the  data/'  one  may  count  the  tallies  in  the  cells 
labeled  b,  c,  h,  r,  w,  x.  There  are  120  opportunities  for  the 
student  to  react  in  this  way,  and  the  per  cent  of  such  re- 
sponses may  easily  be  calculated. 

The  caution  score  indicates  the  extent  to  which  the  student 
marks  statements  keyed  true  as  probably  true,  statements 
keyed  probably  true  as  based  upon  insufficient  data,  state- 
ments keyed  false  as  probably  false,  and  statements  keyed 
probably  false  as  based  upon  insufficient  data.  The  student 
is  then  refusing  to  attribute  to  the  interpretations  as  much 
certainty  as  the  jury  was  willing  to  do. 

The  crude  errors  score  indicates  the  extent  to  which  the 
student  marks  true  or  probably  true  statements  as  false  or 
probably  false,  or  marks  false  or  probably  false  statements  as 
true  or  probably  true.  This  type  of  error  is  often  due  to  care- 
lessness in  reading  the  data  or  interpretations,  or  to  a  mis- 
understanding of  some  terms  involved  in  the  data.  Both  of 
the  last  two  scores  may  be  computed  in  the  manner  pre- 
scribed for  previous  scores. 

Omissions  are  scored  in  order  to  determine  the  actual 
number  of  opportunities  the  student  had  to  score  in  other 
columns. 

A  form  of  data  sheet  on  which  scores  from  this  test  are 
conveniently  summarized  is  presented  on  page  57.  The 
scores  made  by  seven  students  in  the  twelfth  grade  were 
selected  for  purposes  of  illustration.  At  the  bottom  of  the 


56          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

sheet  the  maximum  possible  score,  the  highest  score,  the 
lowest  score,  and  the  group  median  and  the  mean  are  re- 
corded for  each  column. 

Interpretations  of  Scores 

The  achievement  of  a  student  as  revealed  by  the  test  may 
be  analyzed  in  terms  of  two  related  questions.  The  first  of 
these  questions  is;  To  what  extent  does  the  student  recog- 
nize the  limitations  of  the  data?  In  general,  one  may  secure 
some  answers  to  this  question  chiefly  on  the  basis  of  the 
scores  on  general  accuracy  (column  1),  caution  (column  6), 
and  beyond  data  (column  7).6  Column  1  gives  the  per  cent 
of  statements  in  which  the  student  agreed  with  the  jury's 
key,  that  is,  the  student  judged  as  true  those  statements 
keyed  as  true,  etc.  This  is  probably  the  best  single  summariz- 
ing score,  although  it  is  of  limited  value  by  itself.  Columns 
6,  7,  and  8  reveal  the  types  of  judgments  that  the  student 
made  when  he  failed  to  be  accurate.  Thus,  column  6  gives 
the  per  cent  of  statements  in  which  the  student  tended  to 
require  more  qualifications  than  the  jury.  This  score  gives 
some  measure  of  the  student's  tendency  to  call  true  state- 
ments probably  true,  etc.  Column  7  gives  the  per  cent  of 
statements  in  which  the  student  tended  to  ascribe  more 
truth  or  falsity  to  the  interpretation  than  the  data  justify.  A 
high  score  here  is  usually  considered  undesirable,  since  it 
indicates  the  tendency  of  the  student  to  go  beyond  the  limits 
of  the  given  data,  making  definite  judgments  about  state- 
ments for  which  the  given  data  yields  insufficient  informa- 
tion for  such  judgments.  For  example,  on  the  sample  data 
sheet  it  may  be  seen  that  Peggy's  score  on  general  accuracy 
is  low  in  relation  to  her  class.7  In  those  judgments  in  which 

6  The  column  numbers  used  in  the  following  paragraphs  refer  to  the  data 
sheet  ( see  p,  57 )  on  which  scores  are  recorded. 

7  This  discussion  of  interpretations  of  test  scores  is  quite  informal.  For  a 
more  rigorous  interpretation  of  a  "relatively**  high  or  low  score,  the  stand- 
ard error  of  measurement  of  each  category  for  the  particular  population 
under  consideration  is  useful.  Tables  in  the  Appendix  give  the  data  needed 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS 

SAMPLE  DATA  SHEET 


57 


School          A 
Grade  12 


Summary  for  test     2.52 
Interpretation  of  Data 
Date  Test  Given     6-2-39 


Seven  Students  Selected  from  a  Group  of  69 


Accuracy 

Gen- 

Students 

eral 
Ac- 

P. T. 

Insuffi- 

True- 

Omit 

Cau- 
tion 

Be- 
yond 
Data 

Crude 
Errors 

curacy 

P.  F. 

cient 
Data 

False 

Students 

1 

2 

3 

4 

5 

6 

7 

8 

1.  Peggy 

34 

20 

30 

52 

0 

13 

60 

32 

2.  Joseph 

71 

66 

69 

71 

0 

20 

23 

7 

3.  William 

64 

65 

54 

68 

0 

7 

38 

14 

4.  Homer 

51 

18 

74 

52 

0 

53 

22 

10 

5.  Andrew 

71 

74 

60 

78 

0 

6 

33 

8 

6.  George 

47 

11 

60 

75 

0 

41 

38 

8 

7.  Faye 

57 

46 

53 

75 

0 

21 

37 

11 

Maximum 

possible 

100  p 

er  cent 

in  all 

column 

s 

Lowest  Score 

21 

11 

11 

11 

0 

6 

22 

5 

Highest  Score 

71 

74 

74 

78 

24 

53 

64 

32 

Group  Median 

51 

43 

45 

65 

0 

22 

42 

13 

Group  Mean 

50.0 

42.2 

45.0 

60.0 

1.3 

22.2 

43.4 

13.9 

Scores  in  all  columns  are  per  cents. 

she  failed  to  recognize  the  limitations  of  the  given  data  and 
had  made  no  crude  errors,  she  was  overcautious  less  often 
and  went  beyond  the  data  more  often  than  was  average  for 

for  computing  this  statistic  for  the  populations  used  in  obtaining  the  re- 
liability coefficients. 


58          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

her  class.  In  the  case  of  the  student  called  Homer,  the  pat- 
tern of  scores  Indicates  that  he  recognized  the  limits  of  the 
given  data  with  an  accuracy  about  equal  to  the  average  for 
his  class.  When  he  failed  to  judge  accurately  the  limitations 
of  the  given  data,  Homer  was  overcautious  in  more  judg- 
ments and  went  beyond  the  data  in  fewer  judgments  than 
was  average  for  his  class. 

The  second  question  that  the  test  scores  should  answer  is: 
How  accurately  does  the  student  perceive  various  types  of 
relationships  in  the  data? 

By  examining  the  scores  in  columns  2,  37  4,  and  8,  some 
tentative  answers  to  this  question  may  be  obtained.  As  stated 
above,  the  score  in  column  1  gives  the  per  cent  of  accuracy 
with  which  the  student  is  able  to  judge  limitations  of  inter- 
pretations dealing  with  all  of  the  types  of  relationships  in  the 
test.  Scores  in  columns  2,  3,  and  4  are  subscores  of  the  gen- 
eral accuracy  score.  Each  subscore  refers  to  the  accuracy 
with  which  the  student  judges  certain  of  the  relationships  in- 
volved in  the  interpretation.  For  example,  column  2  gives 
the  per  cent  of  accuracy  of  a  student  in  recognizing  those 
statements  which  are  probably  true  or  probaUij  false.  A  high 
score  here  indicates  that  the  student  persistently  applies  with 
success  the  principles  of  prudent  extrapolation,  interpola- 
tion, and  sampling.  Column  3  gives  the  per  cent  of  accuracy 
in  judging  statements  which  cannot  be  justified  without  the 
use  of  information  from  other  sources.  These  statements  in- 
clude relationships  such  as  cause,  effect,  purpose,  analogy, 
as  well  as  some  statements  of  extrapolation,  interpolation, 
and  sampling.  Column  4  gives  the  per  cent  of  accuracy  of 
a  student  in  recognizing  those  statements  which  are  true  or 
false.  A  high  score  indicates  that  the  student  is  able  to  judge 
accurately  statements  that  involve  comparisons  of  points  in 
the  data,  and  recognition  or  comparison  of  trends.  The  per 
cent  of  crude  errors  (column  8)  indicates  errors  in  which  the 
student  marked  interpretations  true  that  the  jury  considered 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  59 

false  or  probably  false,  and  vice  versa.  Such  errors  may  be 
due  to  vocabulary  or  reading  difficulties,  carelessness,  or  in- 
ability to  identify  the  relationship  involved. 

The  following  examples  may  help  to  clarify  this  explana- 
tion. Peggy's  score  in  column  2  indicates  that  she  stands  low 
in  relation  to  her  group  in  the  ability  to  make  the  finer  dis- 
criminations necessary  to  judge  accurately  those  extrapola- 
tion, interpolation,  and  sampling  statements  which  are  based 
on  trends  in  the  data.  She  is  relatively  poor  in  the  accuracy 
with  which  she  judges  statements  based  on  insufficient  evi- 
dence, cause,  effect,  or  purpose,  as  well  as  those  extrapola- 
tion, interpolation,  and  sampling  items  that  fall  in  this  cate- 
gory. The  score  on  accuracy  with  true  and  false  statements 
(column  4)  seems  to  indicate  an  ability  approaching  the 
average  for  her  class  in  recognizing  trends  and  comparisons 
of  trends  or  of  points  in  the  data.  However,  this  can  be  deter- 
mined only  after  studying  the  entire  pattern  of  scores.8  In 
view  of  Peggy's  evident  tendency  to  "go  beyond  the  data/* 
the  higher  score  in  column  4  may  be  a  result  of  her  tendency 
to  be  "gullible"  and  to  mark  many  statements  as  true  or  false. 

Homer's  scores  in  columns  2,  3,  and  4  seem  to  indicate  a 
greater  accuracy  in  his  judgment  of  statements  based  on  in- 
sufficient data  than  on  the  statements  classified  in  the  other 
two  categories.  However,  it  is  necessary  again  to  consider 
the  entire  pattern  of  scores  to  make  a  justifiable  inference. 
Homer's  relatively  high  score  on  caution  and  low  score  on 
beyond  data  imply  that  he  tends  to  refuse  to  make  judgments 

8  Intercorrelations  have  been  computed  to  investigate  the  extent  to  which 
scores  described  above  are  statistically  independent.  See  Appendix.  Al- 
though positive  correlation  exists  between  each  of  the  subscores  on  general 
accuracy,  the  intercorrelation  is  not  sufficiently  high  to  permit  the  predic- 
tion of  one  score  from  another.  However,  a  high  negative  correlation  exists 
between  the  score  on  beyond  data  and  insufficient  data,  and  between  gen- 
eral accuracy  and  crude  errors.  From  a  statistical  standpoint  it  is  possible 
in  both  these  cases  to  predict  one  of  these  scores  from  the  other  without 
appreciable  loss  of  information  about  the  student,  but  teachers  find  it  less 
difficult  to  interpret  the  individual  scores  when  all  these  scores  are 
retained. 


60          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

of  probability  and  classifies  statements  that  are  not  well  justi- 
fied by  the  data  as  of  the  insufficient  data  type. 

An  examination  of  scores  made  by  Joseph  and  Andrew 
shows  that,  although  both  boys  receive  the  same  score  in 
general  accuracy,  for  those  judgments  in  which  they  fail  to 
be  accurate  Andrew  tends  to  go  beyond  the  data  more  often 
than  Joseph. 

It  is  usually  inadvisable  to  interpret  scores  on  this  test  in 
terms  of  national  norms,  since  opportunities  to  develop  these 
abilities  vary  markedly  from  group  to  group.  Data  on  means 
and  standard  deviations  for  certain  groups  are  given  in  tables 
in  the  Appendix.  If  a  group  is  known  to  be  comparable  to 
these  groups,  these  statistics  may  be  helpful  as  a  background 
of  comparison. 

OTHER  INSTRUMENTS  TO  MEASURE  THIS  OBJECTIVE 

During  the  period  of  the  Eight- Year  Study  a  number  of 
instruments  were  developed  for  exploration  of  the  ability  to 
interpret  data.  Responses  on  some  of  these  were  useful  in 
pointing  out  a  need  for  further  clarification  of  the  objective. 
Statistical  studies  of  responses  led  to  changes  which  were  in- 
corporated in  subsequent  forms.  In  some  forms  of  the  test, 
modifications  were  introduced  to  meet  the  particular  needs 
of  different  teachers.  The  purpose  of  the  discussion  that  fol- 
lows is  to  give  a  brief  survey  of  the  changes  that  have  taken 
place  in  the  test  and  the  reasons  for  them. 

One  of  the  earliest  tests  that  explored  certain  aspects  of 
this  objective  was  constructed  to  measure  "the  ability  to 
infer."9  One  short-answer  form  of  this  test  required  the  stu- 
dents to  judge  the  best  of  five  given  inferences.  A  study  of 
the  responses  on  this  test  and  a  corresponding  essay  form 
yielded  many  clues  concerning  the  types  of  inferences  that 
students  make.  A  higher  validity  coefficient  was  secured 

9R.  W.  Tyler,  "Measuring  the  Ability  to  Infer,"  Educational  Research 
Bulletin,  IX  (Nov.  19,  1930),  p.  475. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  61 

when  the  students  were  required  to  judge  both  best  and 
worst  inferences  than  when  they  judged  only  the  best 

Results  of  exploratory  tests  using  a  three-response  form 
and  others  using  a  five-response  form  yielded  valuable  in- 
formation concerning  the  objective.  In  one  of  the  earliest  of 
the  five-response  forms  of  the  test,  the  student  was  presented 
with  different  types  of  data  and  asked  to  judge  interpreta- 
tions made  from  them.  The  directions  were  as  follows: 

Consider  carefully  each  of  the  following  statements,  and  indi- 
cate in  the  columns  to  the  right  whether  you  believe: 

1.  the  data  alone  justify  the  statement. 

2.  the  data  alone  do  not  justify  the  statement. 

3.  the  data  together  with  your  information  suggest  that  the 
statement  is  probably  true. 

4.  the  data  together  with  your  information  suggest  that  the 
statement  is  probably  false. 

5.  the  data  together  with  your  information  are  insufficient  to 
make  a  decision  concerning  the  statement 

This  form  was  used  in  an  attempt  to  get  evidence  of  two 
kinds  of  behavior  in  interpretation  of  data,  namely,  ability  to 
adhere  rigidly  to  the  data  and  reject  interpretations  that  go 
beyond  or  are  contradicted  by  the  data;  and  the  ability  to 
draw  meaningful  inferences  from  those  interpretations  which 
go  beyond  the  data  but  which  appear  highly  probable  or 
improbable  in  the  light  of  other  information  known  to  stu- 
dents. Difficulty  was  encountered  in  interpreting  these  scores, 
since  there  was  no  way  of  setting  up  controls  or  standards 
for  judging  the  amount  or  quality  of  outside  information  a 
student  was  using  in  judging  the  inferences  presented.  As 
will  be  recalled,  the  definition  of  the  objective  accepted  by 
the  committee  emphasizes  the  ability  of  the  students  to 
recognize  what  the  given  data  reveal,  and  to  distinguish  ac- 
ceptable inferences  from  those  that  cannot  be  justified  with- 
out using  information  or  principles  from  other  sources.  This 


62          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

restriction  led  to  a  reformulation  of  the  directions,  and  there- 
after they  remained  virtually  the  same  in  subsequent  forms 
of  the  test. 

Teachers  of  several  subject  fields  were  interested  in  this 
objective.  To  meet  their  request  some  of  the  first  forms  in 
which  the  revised  directions  were  used  restricted  the  field 
from  which  the  data  were  drawn  to  the  natural  sciences  or 
the  social  sciences.10  Since  it  was  believed  that  the  behaviors 
involved  in  these  forms  are  not  essentially  different,  it  was 
deemed  advisable  to  reduce  the  time  required  in  measuring 
this  objective  by  measuring  in  one  instrument  the  achieve- 
ment relative  to  several  fields.  Thus  subsequent  forms  in- 
cluded in  the  same  booklet  data  drawn  from  both  fields.11 
Statistical  considerations  (e.g.,  studies  of  reliability)  indicate 
that  this  has  not  changed  the  homogeneity  of  the  behavior  to 
any  great  extent. 

The  summarization  of  scores  has  remained,  with  one  ex- 
ception, very  much  as  it  is  found  on  the  sample  data  sheet 
given  above  for  Form  2.52.  In  early  forms  (2.2,  2.3,  2.4)  the 
beyond  data  scores  had  subscores  which  indicated  the  tend- 
ency of  the  student  to  go  beyond  the  data  in  the  direction  of 
greater  truth  or  in  the  direction  of  greater  falsity  than  the 
data  warranted.  From  an  analysis  of  responses  it  was  found 
that  in  general  most  students  showed  much  greater  tendency 
to  go  beyond  the  data  in  the  direction  of  judging  the  printed 
statement  as  true  than  in  judging  it  as  false.  Because  of  this 
fact  these  subscores  on  "going  beyond  the  data"  did  not 
greatly  aid  the  interpretation  of  scores  and  were  omitted 
from  subsequent  forms  of  the  test.  A  caution  score  that  was 
found  to  be  more  meaningful  in  describing  the  behavior  of 
students  was  added. 

A  statistical  study  of  student  responses  to  Form  2.5  sug- 

10  Form  2.2,  Interpretation  of  Data  (Natural  Sciences),  and  Form  2.3, 
Interpretation  of  Data  (Social  Sciences). 

11  Forms  2.4,  2.5,  2.51,  2.52,  Interpretation  of  Data. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  63 

gested  that  greater  reliability  of  certain  scores  could  be  ob- 
tained by  increasing  the  number  of  statements  of  each  type 
used  in  the  test.  These  suggestions  were  used  in  building 
Form  2.51  by  including  in  each  of  the  ten  exercises  15  state- 
ments which  constituted  a  definite  pattern  of  types  of  inter- 
pretations and  types  of  responses  expected.  An  effort  was 
made  to  include  in  each  exercise  at  least  one  statement  in- 
volving each  type  of  relationship  used  in  the  test,  but  state- 
ments including  extrapolation,  interpolation,  and  sampling 
were  used  in  greater  number.  The  entire  test  was  thus 
lengthened  from  119  statements  in  Form  2.5  to  150  state- 
ments in  Form  2.51  and  the  probably  true  or  probably  false 
response  was  expected  in  40  per  cent  of  the  statements. 

The  latest  form  of  Interpretation  of  Data  test  (Form  2.52) 
was  intended  to  be  comparable  to  Form  2.51.  An  effort  was 
made  to  match  the  form  of  presentation,  types  of  interpreta- 
tions, topics  with  which  the  data  deal,  and  types  of  response 
expected.  Each  of  the  two  forms  was  administered  within  a 
week  to  105  students  of  the  tenth  grade,  133  students  of  the 
eleventh  grade,  and  99  students  of  the  twelfth  grade  of  two 
large  high  schools.  The  coefficient  of  correlation  between  the 
two  f orms  of  the  test  for  each  category  was  computed  by  the 
product-moment  method.  These  coefficients,  together  with 
means  and  standard  deviations  on  each  category  for  both 
tests,  are  given  in  Table  1  below. 

Although  these  correlations  are  fairly  high,  the  fact  that 
they  are  no  higher  may  be  partially  explained  by  the  ob- 
servation that  more  rigorous  standards  were  used  in  keying 
Form  2.52  and  that  some  sources  of  ambiguity  found  to  be 
present  in  Form  2.51  were  eliminated. 

Since  some  teachers  were  interested  in  measuring  the  abili- 
ties of  junior  high  school  students  in  interpreting  data,  a 
form  was  developed  for  students  of  this  grade  level.  The 
criteria  for  the  selection  of  data  were  similar  to  those  used 
in  Form  2.52,  and  the  advice  of  junior  high  school  teachers 


64 


ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


and  librarians  was  sought  in  checking  the  appropriateness  of 
the  data  and  the  interpretations  for  students  of  this  level.  As 
a  result  of  this  advice,  an  attempt  was  made  to  simplify  this 

TABLE    1 

Means  and  Standard  Deviations  for  Tests  2.57  and  2.52;  Product- Moment  Correla- 
tions between  Forms  2.51  and  2,52. 


Gen- 

Be- 

Crude 

Category 

eral 
Accu- 

PT 
PF 

Insuf. 
Data 

TF 

Cau- 
tion 

yond 
Data 

Er- 
rors 

racy 

x/r                        2.51 
Means                 2^ 

40.1 
45.2 

26.5 
24.4 

38.6 
53.7 

52.3 
70.6 

27.8 
26.4 

51.4 
37.5 

16.9 
13.8 

Standard             2.51 

10  4 

14.0 

15.2 

16  2 

11.0 

12.4 

5  97 

Deviations           2  .  52 

11  4 

14.4 

18.4 

16.2 

12.7 

13.4 

6  30 

F2  51,  2  52 

.85 

.84 

.83 

.74 

.85 

.81 

.65 

instrument,  in  comparison  with  Form  2.52,  in  vocabulary,  in 
types  of  responses  expected,  in  number  of  interpretations 
used,  and  in  problem  areas  or  concepts  involved.  A  prelim- 
inary form  (2.7)  was  constructed  and  administered,  and  after 
a  statistical  study  of  the  responses,  the  suggested  improve- 
ments were  incorporated  in  the  present  test,  Form  2.71. 

This  test  contains  ten  sets  of  data,  each  of  which  is  fol- 
lowed by  ten  interpretations.  The  data  deal  with  problems 
of  safety,  budgeting,  sports,  choice  of  vocation,  cost  of  living, 
etc.  The  student  is  required  to  make  three  distinctions  in 
judging  these  interpretations.  These  are  given  in  the  direc- 
tions of  the  test  as  follows: 

A.  Enough  information, is  given  to  make  the  statement  true. 

B.  Not  enough  information  is  given  to  decide. 

C.  Enough  information  is  given  to  make  the  statement  -false. 

The  student  responses  are  summarized  in  terms  of  scores 
briefly  denoted  by  the  following  phrases:  general  accuracy, 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  65 

caution,  beyond  data,  crude  errors,  accuracy  with  true — 
false,  and  accuracy  with  insufficient  data.  Reliability  coeffi- 
cients were  computed  by  the  Kuder-Richardson  formula  for 
five  populations  drawn  from  each  of  grades  seven,  eight, 
and  nine.12  For  these  15  populations  the  reliability  coefficients 
of  the  beyond  data  and  insufficient  data  scores  are  of  the 
same  order  of  magnitude  as  are  those  of  the  general  accuracy 
score.  The  reliability  of  the  other  scores  analogous  to  those 
of  Form  2.52  are  a  little  lower  with  the  exception  of  those  for 
crude  errors  which,  as  one  might  expect,  are  erratic  and 
tend  to  be  rather  low.  This  same  general  pattern  is  found 
for  each  grade. 

VALIDITY  OF  THE  INTERPRETATION  OF  DATA  TESTS 

Two  main  aspects  of  the  validity  of  the  interpretation  of 
data  tests  will  be  considered:  (1)  the  validity  of  the  tests 
as  a  measure  of  the  students'  ability  to  judge  interpretations 
formulated  by  others,  and  (2)  the  validity  of  the  tests  as  an 
index  of  students'  ability  to  write  original  interpretations. 

Ability  to  Judge  Interpretations  Made  by  Others 

The  validity  of  this  test  as  a  measure  of  the  ability  to  judge 
interpretations  made  by  others  depends  upon  several  factors: 
(a)  the  correspondence  between  the  behaviors  demanded  of 
students  in  the  test  and  the  behaviors  defined  in  the  state- 
ment of  the  objective,  (b)  the  adequacy  of  sampling  relative 
to  form  of  presentation,  to  problem  areas  with  which  the 
data  are  associated,  and  to  types  of  interpretations,  (c)  the 
appropriateness  of  the  test  as  to  difficulty  for  the  high  school 
level. 

In  considering  the  first  point,  it  should  be  recalled  that  the 
test  is  so  constructed  as  to  afford  the  student  an  opportunity 

12  G.  F.  Kuder  and  M.  W.  Richardson,  "The  Theory  of  the  Estimation 
of  Test  Reliability"  Psychometrika,  Vol.  2,  No.  3  (Sept.,  1937),  pp.  151- 
160.  Throughout  this  report,  wherever  the  Kuder-Richardson  Method  is  in- 
dicated, case  III  of  this  method  was  used.  These  and  other  data  on  Form 
2,71  will  be  found  in  the  Appendix. 


66          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

to  demonstrate  the  two  main  behaviors  defined  in  the  objec- 
tive, namely,  the  ability  to  perceive  relationships  in  the  data 
and  the  ability  to  recognize  the  limitations  of  the  data.  To 
verify  this,  it  will  be  necessary  to  review  briefly  the  method 
of  construction  of  the  test.  Incorporated  in  the  interpretations 
which  the  student  is  asked  to  judge  are  the  various  types  of 
relationships,  such  as  trends,  comparisons,  etc.,  that  he  is 
expected  to  perceive,  expressed  in  such  a  way  as  to  have 
varying  degrees  of  substantiation  from  the  given  data.  Thus 
some  statements  are  intended  to  be  fully  established  or  con- 
tradicted by  the  data  alone,  some  statements  if  properly 
qualified  are  partially  established  or  contradicted  by  the 
data,  and  others  are  unjustified  without  the  use  of  informa- 
tion from  other  sources.  The  five-point  response  by  which 
the  student  indicates  his  judgment  of  the  interpretations 
forces  a  response  by  the  student  from  which  the  extent  of 
his  recognition  of  the  limitations  of  the  data  and  his  percep- 
tion of  relationships  may  be  inferred. 

It  should  also  be  recalled  that  the  criteria  for  selection  of 
data  were  determined  by  the  judgment  of  members  of  the 
committee.  Their  knowledge  of  types  of  materials  that  stu- 
dents read  and  an  analysis  of  the  types  of  data  commonly 
found  in  curricular  and  other  reading  materials  form  the 
basis  of  their  judgment  of  the  adequacy  of  the  sampling  of 
forms,  of  presentation,  of  problem  areas,  and  of  types  of  in- 
terpretations. The  analysis  made  by  E.  W.  Hellmich  of  text- 
books for  social  studies  in  the  junior  and  senior  high  school 
and  in  elementary  college  courses  indicates  that  the  subject 
matter  and  types  of  presentation  of  the  data  used  in  Test 
2.52  are  those  which  students  encounter.13 

13  Eugene  W.  Hellmich,  Mathematics  in  Certain  Elementary  Social  Studies 
in  Secondary  Schools  and  Colleges,  Teachers  College,  Columbia  University, 
Contributions  to  Education,  No.  706,  1937.  Studies  in  other  fields  report 
similar  results:  for  example,  Robert  C.  Scarf,  Mathematics  Necessary  for 
the  Reading  of  Popular  Science,  Master's  Thesis,  The  University  of  Chicago, 
Department  of  Education,  1925. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  67 

The  appropriateness  of  the  test  for  the  high  school  level 
can  be  considered  in  terms  of  two  sources  of  evidence.  First, 
the  interpretations  represented  by  the  statements  in  the  test 
are  of  the  types  students  are  found  to  use  when  they  make 
their  own  interpretations  of  data.  Secondly,  study  of  the  dis- 
tribution of  scores  made  by  students  who  have  taken  the  test 
shows  that  no  student  from  the  ninth  grade  to  the  junior  col- 
lege level  has  received  the  maximum  score  possible,  nor  is 
there  concentration  of  scores  at  the  lower  end  of  the  range. 
The  distribution  of  scores  is  symmetrical  with  concentration 
of  scores  at  the  mean,  and,  in  general,  the  means  tend  to 
increase  with  grade  level. 

Ability  to  Make  Original  Interpretations 

Although  teachers  are  interested  in  appraising  students' 
ability  to  judge  interpretations  made  by  others,  many  teachers 
wish  also  to  measure  the  students'  ability  to  make  their  own 
interpretations.  In  order  to  use  scores  on  the  interpretation 
of  data  test  as  an  index  of  the  latter  ability,  there  must  be 
evidence  of  a  high  correlation  between  scores  on  the  test 
and  judgments  of  the  students'  ability  to  make  original  in- 
terpretations. To  obtain  such  evidence,  attempts  were  made 
in  earlier  studies  to  validate  the  interpretation  of  data  test  by 
using  free  essay  responses  of  students  as  a  criterion.  For  ex- 
ample, in  a  study  conducted  in  a  large  public  junior  high 
school  in  which  193  students  of  seventh,  eighth,  and  ninth 
grades  participated,  the  students  were  given  the  sets  of  data 
taken  from  an  Interpretation  of  Data  test  for  the  junior  high 
school  level  (Form  2.71)  and  were  asked  to  make  free  essay 
responses  following  such  general  directions  as:  "Write  five 
statements  that  you  are  sure  are  true  according  to  the  facts 
given  in  these  data,"  and  "Write  three  statements  based  on 
the  data  which  you  are  not  quite  sure  are  true  according  to 
these  data." 

The  objectivity  secured  in  grading  this  essay  form  is  indi- 


68 


ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


cated  by  the  values  of  the  product-moment  coefficients  of 
correlation  among  the  three  judges  who  marked  the  papers. 
These  values  ranged  from  0.92  to  0.96.  Table  2  below  gives 
the  values  of  the  product-moment  coefficient  of  correlation 
between  Form  2.71  and  the  essay  form,  and  the  reliabilities 
of  each  form  of  the  test. 

TABLE    2 

Statistics  for  General  Accuracy  Score  of  Test  2.71 


Grade 

N 

Product-Moment 
Correlation 
between  Test  2.71 
and  Essay  Form 

Reliability  Coeffi- 
cient of  Essay 
Form  by  Split- 
Halves  Method 
with  Spearman- 
Brown  Correction 

Reliability  Coeffi- 
cient of  Test  2.71 
by  Kuder- 
Richardson 
Method 

7 

68 

0.69 

0.88 

0  80 

8 

60 

0.58 

0  73 

0.87 

9 

65 

0.44 

0  79 

0.91 

The  correlations  between  the  two  forms  were  positive  and 
sufficiently  large  to  warrant  a  further  investigation  of  the  re- 
lationship between  the  behaviors  involved. 

Although  a  wide  range  of  relationships,  such  as  compari- 
sons and  recognition  of  trends,  was  found  in  the  statements 
made  by  students,  as  a  rule  the  free  responses  made  by  any 
one  student  involved  a  narrow  range  of  relationships,  and 
did  not  sample  adequately  his  ability  to  make  various  types 
of  interpretations.  In  the  next  study,  directions  on  the  essay 
form  of  the  test  were  changed  in  an  effort  to  encourage  the 
student  to  include  a  wider  range  of  relationships  in  his  inter- 
pretations. The  new  directions  posed  a  series  of  questions 
designed  to  direct  the  attention  of  the  student  to  the  various 
types  of  relationships  found  in  the  interpretations  given  in 
Form  2.52.  For  example,  after  each  of  the  following  inter- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  69 

pretations  is  the  question  which  corresponded  to  it  in  the 
essay  form: 

la.  The  ratio  of  agricultural  production  to  the  number 
of  farm  workers  increased  every  five  years  between 
1900  and  1925.  (Comparison  of  trends) 

Ib.  In  terms  of  these  data  alone,  what  do  you  believe 
you  can  say  concerning  (a)  the  change  in  number 
of  farm  workers  employed  compared  to  (b)  the 
change  in  volume  of  farm  production  throughout 
the  period  recorded  in  the  chart? 

2a.  The  increase  in  agricultural  production  between 
1910  and  1925  was  due  to  more  widespread  use  of 
farm  machinery.  ( Cause ) 

2b.  In  terms  of  these  data  alone,  what  do  you  believe 
you  can  say  about  the  cause  of  the  increase  in 
volume  of  farm  production  between  1910  and  1925? 

3a.  The  average  amount  of  farm  production  was  higher 
in  the  period  1925  to  1930  than  in  the  period  1920 
to  1925.  (Extrapolation) 

3b.  In  terms  of  these  data  alone,  what  do  you  believe 
you  can  say  about  the  volume  of  farm  production 
during  the  period  from  1925  to  1930? 

This  study  was  made  with  two  populations  of  ninth,  tenth, 
eleventh,  and  twelfth  grade  students.  One  group  consisted  of 
119  students  from  a  large  public  high  school  and  the  other 
was  made  up  of  99  students  from  a  smaller  private  high 
school.  The  essay  form  was  administered  first,  followed 
within  a  week  by  the  regular  form  of  Form  2.52. 

The  essay  responses  were  scored  and  summarized  so  that 
statements  involving  each  type  of  relationship  could  be  clas- 
sified as  accurate,  beyond  the  data,  cautious,  involving  a 
crude  error,  or  unable  to  see  the  relationship.  In  scoring,  it 
was  possible  by  the  use  of  a  simple  set  of  rules  to  score  papers 
so  objectively  that  correlations  of  the  scores  given  inde- 


7o    •      ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

pendently  by  three  markers  ranged  from  .94  to  .96.  The  addi- 
tional time  required  to  answer  the  essay  form  made  it  neces- 
sary to  sample  the  types  of  relationships  and  the  types  of 
data  used  in  Form  2.52.  Seven  questions  were  formulated  for 
each  of  six  of  the  ten  exercises  in  Form  2.52;  each  of  the  42 
questions  thus  formulated  corresponded  in  subject  matter 
and  type  of  relationship  to  a  statement  used  in  that  test.  Only 
39  answers  were  scored  in  the  essay  form  because  three  ques- 
tions were  later  found  to  be  ambiguous.  These  were  a  fair 
sample  of  the  whole  test,  since  a  product-moment  correlation 
coefficient  of  .85  (uncorrected  for  overlapping)  was  obtained 
between  the  "general  accuracy"  score  on  these  39  items  and 
on  the  entire  150  items  of  Form  2.52.  Since  the  correlation 
between  the  part  and  the  total  test  was  desired  as  a  measure 
of  the  adequacy  of  the  sampling,  no  correction  for  overlap- 
ping was  made.  There  was  also  a  product-moment  correla- 
tion coefficient  of  .96  between  the  general  accuracy  scores  of 
the  entire  ten  exercises  of  Form  2.52  and  the  six  exercises 
from  which  these  39  items  were  taken.  However,  there  does 
appear  to  be  some  difference  in  the  difficulty  of  the  39  items 
and  of  the  total  test.  The  mean  general  accuracy  score  for  the 
39  items  was  definitely  higher  than  that  for  the  total  150 
items  for  each  of  the  two  different  populations  of  approxi- 
mately 100  high  school  students.  In  spite  of  this  difference, 
however,  the  sample  appeared  to  be  sufficiently  representa- 
tive for  use  in  this  validity  study. 

The  scores  on  the  essay  form  were  correlated  by  the 
product-moment  method  with  scores  on  similar  categories  for 
Form  2,52.  The  results  are  given  in  Table  3  below. 

The  reliabilities  of  the  essay  form  for  these  populations 
were  computed  by  die  Kuder-Richardson  formula  and  are 
found  in  Table  4.  Reliabilities  for  Form  2.52  will  be  found  in 
Table  5  under  the  discussion  of  reliability. 

Since  the  correlation  coefficient  is  to  be  used  as  a  measure 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS 


TABLE   3 

Correlations  for  Each  Category  between  Essay  Form  and  Form  2.52 


Score 

General 
Accu- 
racy 

Beyond 
Data 

Caution 

Crude 
Error 

True- 
False 

Insuffi- 
cient 
Data 

Probably 
True 
Probably 
False 

Statistic 

N 

corr 

corr 

Small  Private 

"• 

School 

99 

72 

80 

60 

65 

50 

55 

22 

56 

37 

47 

64 

71 

.53 

.63 

Large  Public 

School 

119 

.74 

.83 

.47 

.52 

.51 

57 

08 

.12 

58 

77 

58 

65 

55 

.66 

rcorr.  refers  to  reliability  coefficient  corrected  for  attenuation  due  to  the  unreliability  of  the 
criterion. 


TABLE   4 

Reliabilities  by  Kuder-Richardson  Formula 
for  Two  Populations  on  Essay  Form 


. 

Prob- 

Score 

Gen- 
eral 
Ac- 
curacy 

Be- 
yond 
Data 

Cau- 
tion 

Crude 
Error 

True- 
False 

Insuffi- 
cient 
Data 

ably 
True- 
Prob- 
ably 

False 

Small  Private  School 

.81 

.85 

.82 

.15 

.61 

.82 

.70 

Large  Public  School 

.80 

.82 

.81 

.43 

.57 

.80 

.70 

of  validity  (that  is,  of  the  degree  to  which  the  ability  to 
make  original  interpretations  of  data  can  be  predicted  from 
a  score  011  Form  2.52),  it  does  not  seem  legitimate  to  correct 
for  attenuation  due  to  the  unreliability  of  Form  2.52.  The 
relation  between  the  theoretical  ability  to  judge  interpreta- 
tion and  the  theoretical  ability  to  make  original  interpreta- 
tions is  not  at  issue,  but  rather  how  well  Form  2.52  predicts 
the  latter  ability.  Hence,  it  seems  defensible  to  correct  for 


72          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

the  unreliability  of  the  criterion  but  not  for  that  of  Form 
2.52.  As  seen  in  Table  35  such  correction  yielded  validity 
coefficients  of  .80  and  .83  for  the  general  accuracy  score,  and 
lower  values  for  the  other  categories.  A  validity  coefficient  of 
.80  is  sufficiently  high  for  group  predictions  and  is  of  some 
value  for  study  of  individual  students.  Thus  Form  2.52  can 
be  used  as  an  index  of  the  general  accuracy  with  which  a 
group  can  make  original  interpretations  of  data.  For  the  pop- 
ulations used  in  this  study,  its  validity  as  an  index  of  the 
types  of  errors  into  which  students  fall  in  making  original 
interpretations  was  not  high. 

Some  differences  in  the  two  forms  of  the  test  are  apparent. 
In  the  essay  form  the  student  could  respond  with  more  than 
one  statement  or  could  make  an  irrelevant  statement — that 
is,  a  statement  in  which  he  failed  to  involve  the  relationship 
to  which  the  question  was  intended  to  direct  his  attention. 
There  was  no  opportunity  in  Form  2.52  to  react  in  either  of 
these  ways.  However,  since  the  relevant  responses  to  each 
question  on  the  essay  form  were  scored  as  a  whole  on  the 
basis  of  the  main  thought  expressed,  the  number  of  extra 
statements  did  not  affect  the  score.  The  irrelevant  statements 
affected  the  score  on  general  accuracy  in  the  same  way  that 
an  omitted  item  would  have  affected  this  score  on  either  form. 
A  study  was  made  to  determine  whether  the  opportunity  in 
the  essay  form  to  respond  with  irrelevant  statements  might 
be  an  important  factor  affecting  the  correlation  between  the 
two  instruments.  The  correlation  coefficient  between  the 
general  accuracy  score  of  the  essay  form  and  all  of  the  corre- 
sponding 39  items  of  Form  2.52  for  the  group  of  99  students 
was  .68.  A  general  accuracy  score  on  Form  2.52  was  derived 
for  only  that  part  of  the  39  items  to  which  the  student  had 
made  relevant  responses  on  the  essay  form.  The  product- 
moment  correlation  coefficient  between  the  general  accuracy 
score  on  the  essay  form  and  this  part  score  was  found  to  be 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  73 

.78,  This  seems  to  indicate  that  the  opportunity  to  make 
irrelevant  responses  on  the  essay  form  may  be  one  of  the 
factors  that  limits  the  correlation. 

The  comparison  of  patterns  of  responses  for  the  same  indi- 
viduals on  the  two  test  forms  suggests  another  likely  hy- 
pothesis to  account  for  the  differences  in  results.  Many  stu- 
dents apparently  employed  somewhat  different  standards  in 
making  original  interpretations  than  they  used  when  judging 
interpretations  of  data  made  by  others.  Students'  behavior  in 
this  respect  may  be  classified  into  the  following  patterns: 

a.  The  student  reacts  similarly  on  corresponding  items 
of  the  two  forms. 

b.  The  student  is  overcautious  on  an  item  in  judging  in- 
terpretations made  by  others  but  goes  beyond  the 
data  on  the  corresponding  item  in  making  his  own 
interpretations.  The  reverse  pattern  also  appears. 

c.  The  student  is  either  very  cautious  or  goes  beyond 
the  data  in  judging  interpretations  made  by  others, 
but  is  accurate  when  making  his  own  interpretations. 
Here  also  the  reverse  pattern  appears. 

Of  these  patterns,  the  first  appeared  most  frequently,  as  might 
be  expected  from  the  high  validity  coefficients.  Extreme  dis- 
crepancies between  reactions  on  corresponding  items  of  the 
two  tests  (as  described  in  pattern  b)  appeared  very  infre- 
quently. In  pattern  c,  students  tend  to  go  beyond  the  data 
more  in  making  their  own  interpretations  of  data  than  in 
judging  interpretations  made  by  others. 

While  other  factors  may  be  present,  the  differences  be- 
tween the  essay  form  and  Form  2.52  may  in  part  be  at- 
tributed to  the  opportunity  in  the  essay  form  to  make  irrel- 
evant statements,  and  to  the  tendency  of  some  students  to 
use  different  standards  in  reacting  to  corresponding  items  of 
the  two  forms. 


74          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 
RELIABILITY  OF  THE  INTERPRETATION  OF  DATA  TESTS 

The  most  comprehensive  study  of  reliability  of  Form  2.52 
was  made  by  the  use  of  the  Kuder-Richardson  formula  with 
19  populations  from  grades  nine,  ten,  eleven,  and  twelve  in 
seven  schools.  The  reliabilities  for  the  two  populations  used 
in  the  validity  study  were  of  special  interest  and  are  given  in 
Table  5  below.  The  means  and  standard  deviations  for  these 
two  populations  are  listed  in  Table  6  below. 

TABLE    5 

Reliabilities  by  Kuder-Richardson  Formula 
on  Form  2.52 for  Two  Populations 


Prob- 

Score 

N 

Gen- 
eral 
Ac- 
curacy 

Be- 
yond 
Data 

Cau- 
tion 

Crude 
Error 

True- 
False 

Insuffi- 
cient 
Data 

ably 
True-- 
Prob- 
ably 

False 

Small  Private 

School 

Grades  9,  10, 

11,  12 

99 

0.93 

0.91 

0.91 

0.75 

0.78 

0.92 

0.88 

Large  Public 

School 

Grades  9,  10, 

11,  12 

119 

0.95 

0.93 

0.87 

0.81 

0.84 

0.90 

0.88 

It  will  be  noted  that  the  reliability  coefficients  in  all  cate- 
gories except  crude  error  and  true-false  cluster  around  .90 
for  both  of  these  populations  and  that  the  general  accuracy 
score  has  the  highest  reliability.  The  coefficients  tend  to 
form  the  same  definite  pattern  from  category  to  category  for 
both  populations,  and  the  difference  between  the  coefficients 
for  the  two  populations  on  any  single  category  is  slight. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS 

TABLE   6 

Means  and  Standard  Deviations  of  Per  Cent  Scores 
on  Form  2.52  for  Two  Populations 


75 


Score 

General 
Accu- 
racy 

Beyond 
Data 

Caution 

Crude 
Error 

True- 
False 

Insuffi- 
cient 
Data 

Probably 
True- 
Probably 
False 

Statistic 

N 

M 

<r 

M 

<r 

M 

tr 

M 

er 

M 

er 

M 

tr 

M 

a 

Small  Private 

School 

99 

56.3 

10.9 

19.6 

11.2 

36.1 

13.5 

7.8 

5.3 

78.3 

15.0 

76.8 

16.7 

24.3 

14.1 

Large  Public 

School 

119 

45.9 

13.7 

47.6 

13.8 

24.5 

10.3 

13.2 

7.0 

62.0 

17.3 

41.3 

17.5 

34.1 

16.1 

When  the  means  and  standard  deviations  for  the  two  sam- 
ples are  considered,  it  will  be  noticed  that  the  group  from 
the  small  private  school  is  in  general  a  superior  group  as 
measured  by  Form  2.52.  It  is  also  a  more  cautious  group  as 
measured  by  the  high  mean  score  on  caution  and  by  the  low 
mean  score  on  accuracy  with  probably  true — probaby  false. 
Yet  in  spite  of  the  difference  in  these  two  groups,  the  relia- 
bilities computed  from  them  are  very  similar.  Table  1  in  the 
Appendix  gives  the  reliability  coefficients  for  all  nineteen 
populations.  It  will  be  noted  again  that  for  these  populations 
the  reliability  coefficients  of  all  scores  except  crude  errors 
and  accuracy  with  true  and  false  statements  are  sufficiently 
high  for  group  interpretation. 

Before  Form  2.52  was  made,  the  split-half  method  was 
used  in  deriving  the  reliability  of  Form  2.51.  An  effort  was 
made  to  split  the  test  into  "equivalent"  halves  by  pairing 
items  according  to  definite  criteria,  such  as  the  response  ex- 
pected of  the  student,  the  types  of  interpretation  involved, 
the  topic  with  which  the  data  dealt,  and  the  form  of  presen- 
tation of  the  data.  An  analysis  of  the  responses  of  88  students 
was  used  in  an  attempt  to  include  in  each  half  items  which 
presented  these  students  with  the  same  type  of  difficulty,  but 


76 


ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


it  was  not  always  possible  to  make  an  accurate  match.  The 
correlation  between  "equivalent"  halves  of  Form  2.51  was 
computed  from  the  scores  of  another  population  of  284  stu- 
dents in  the  three  upper  grades  of  two  high  schools.  By 
means  of  the  Spearman-Brown  formula  it  was  possible  to 
predict  the  correlation  for  a  test  doubled  in  length.  Table  7 
contains  these  corrected  correlations. 

The  coefficients  obtained  from  the  comparability  study 
discussed  previously  may  be  considered  another  measure  of 
reliability  of  the  interpretation  of  data  test  and  are  also  given 
in  Table  7  below.  However,  the  lower  values  of  these  coeffi- 
cients are  attributable  more  to  the  difference  between  the 
two  tests  than  to  the  unreliability  of  either  of  the  tests. 

TABLE    7 

Reliability  Coefficients  for  Interpretation  of  Data  Tests 


Prob- 

Method 

Population 

N 

Gen- 
eral 

Accu- 

Be- 
yond 
Data 

Cau- 
tion 

Crude 
Error 

True- 
False 

Insuf- 
ficient 
Data 

ably 
True- 
Prob- 

racy 

ably 

False 

Kuder-Richardson 

Grades  9,  10, 

Form  2.  52 

11,  12 

119 

0.95 

0  93 

0  87 

0  81 

0  84 

0.90 

0  88 

Comparability 

Grades  10, 

Forms  2.  5  1-2.  5  2 

11,  12 

337 

0.85 

0.81 

0.85 

0.65 

0.74 

0  83 

0.84 

Split-halves 

Grades  10, 

Form  2.51 

11,  12 

284 

0  92 

0.91 

0.91 

0.82 

0  86 

0.92 

0.87 

When  the  reliabilities  obtained  by  the  three  methods  are 
compared>  it  will  be  noted  that  the  coefficients  computed  by 
the  Kuder-Richardson  formula  and  by  the  split-halves 
method  are  approximately  the  same  and  that,  as  would  be 
expected,  the  coefficients  computed  from  scores  on  "com- 
parable" forms  are  smaller  for  all  categories.  These  reliabili- 
ties were  considered  rather  high  in  view  of  the  complexity  of 
the  behaviors  involved. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  77 

II.  APPLICATION  OF  PRINCIPLES  OF  SCIENCE 
ANALYSIS  OF  THE  OBJECTIVE 

Teachers  of  science  in  schools  of  the  Study  believed  that 
students  should  learn  to  apply  knowledge  obtained  in  the 
science  classroom  and  laboratory  to  the  solution  of  problems 
as  they  arise  in  daily  living.  This  aspect  of  critical  thinking 
was  frequently  mentioned  in  the  list  of  objectives  submitted 
to  the  Evaluation  Staff.  A  study  of  the  prevailing  curriculum 
materials  for  science  instruction  confirmed  the  importance  of 
this  objective,  and  therefore  a  committee  was  formed  for  the 
purpose  of  clarifying  it  and  of  aiding  in  the  development  of 
evaluation  instruments  for  appraising  growth  in  the  ability 
to  apply  science  information.  Although  this  objective  had 
previously  been  explored  to  some  extent  at  the  college  level 
by  Tyler14  and  others,  and  these  explorations  had  served  to 
show  that  certain  techniques  for  the  measurement  of  the 
objective  were  feasible,  it  could  not  be  assumed  that  the 
available  analyses  and  methods  were  immediately  applicable 
at  the  secondary  school  level.  This  committee  of  teachers  in 
the  schools  therefore  aided  the  Evaluation  Staff  in  clarifying 
the  objective  to  be  appraised  and  also  in  finding  situations 
which  would  give  students  an  opportunity  to  show  the  de- 
gree to  which  the  objective  had  been  attained.  In  the  present 
instance,  clarifying  the  objective  necessitated  an  analysis  of 
the  behaviors  involved  in  application  and  a  selection  of  the 
principles  to  be  used. 

Behaviors  Involved  in  Application 

The  analysis  of  the  behaviors  involved  in  application  sep- 
arated the  process  of  applying  principles  into  two  steps: 
(1)  the  student  studies  a  situation  and  makes  a  decision 
about  the  probable  explanation  or  prediction  which  is  ap- 

14  Ralph  W.  Tyler,  Constructing  Achievement  Tests,  Bureau  o£  Educa- 
tional Research,  Ohio  State  University. 


78          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

plicable  to  this  situation;  (2)  lie  justifies  through  the  use  of 
science  principles  and  sound  reasoning  the  explanation  or 
prediction  that  he  made  in  the  first  step.  In  the  first  step  he 
acts  in  the  role  of  an  authority  who  is  presented  with  a 
problem  and  asked  for  a  solution.  In  the  second  step,  he  is 
asked  to  explain  or  justify  that  proposed  solution  by  means 
of  his  previous  knowledge  of  what  has  occurred  in  similar 
situations. 

The  kind  of  deductive  thinking  needed  for  the  solution  of 
these  problems  consists  of  the  search  for  an  explanation  of 
the  fact  or  facts  described  in  the  problem  situation  by  means 
of  some  general  rule  which  asserts  a  highly  probable  con- 
nection between  facts  of  the  kind  described  in  the  problem 
and  other  facts  the  student  knows  to  be  applicable  to  similar 
problems.  The  question  he  attempts  to  answer  is:  Does  the 
general  rule  which  is  suggested  by  the  given  facts  as  an  hy- 
pothesis for  explaining  what  has  happened  (or  what  will 
happen)  actually  apply  to  this  specific  problem?  The  answer 
to  this  question  comes,  of  course,  from  experimentation  or 
direct  observation.  However,  if  observations  have  been  made 
in  several  situations  which  can  be  shown  to  be  similar  to  that 
one  which  is  described  in  the  test,  then  without  obtaining 
the  empirical  evidence  one  may  nevertheless  predict  with 
considerable  confidence  that  the  same  conclusion  is  also  true 
in  this  case.  It  was  for  the  measurement  of  such  behavior  that 
the  instruments  to  be  described  later  were  constructed.  The 
teachers  felt  they  needed  the  most  help  in  evaluating  the 
ability  of  students  to  apply  principles  in  new  situations,  and 
consequently  the  remembering  of  applications  which  had 
been  made  was  not  included  as  a  behavior  to  be  directly 
appraised. 

Selection  of  the  Principles 

In  the  discussions  that  were  held  to  clarify  the  meaning 
of  the  term  principle  it  was  found  that  some  teachers  were 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  79 

inclined  to  accept  certain  statements  as  representing  "prin- 
ciples" whereas  others  wanted  to  regard  them  as  statements 
of  "facts."  The  difficulty  was  resolved  by  obtaining  an  agree- 
ment which  permitted,  for  the  purpose  of  testing  application, 
the  use  of  any  science  information,  fact,  generalization,  un- 
derstanding, concept,  or  "law"  which  proves  to  be  useful 
(alone  or  in  connection  with  other  information)  for  predic- 
tive or  explanatory  purposes.  Although  more  inclusive  than 
the  definition  of  principle  that  is  frequently  used  by  science 
teachers,  this  agreement  seemed  satisfactory  for  the  measure- 
ment of  the  objective  as  this  committee  conceived  it.  After 
the  committee  had  accepted  this  agreement  as  to  the  "prin- 
ciples" which  were  to  be  used  in  the  construction  of  the  in- 
struments, teachers  were  asked  to  submit  statements  of  those 
principles  which  were  considered  important  in  their  courses 
and  which  had  received  the  greatest  emphasis  in  their  teach- 
ing. These  lists  included  the  principles  with  which  their  stu- 
dents had  had  the  greatest  opportunity  to  become  familiar 
through  reading,  discussion,  and  experimentation. 

The  original  lists  from  individual  teachers  included  princi- 
ples from  the  fields  of  chemistry,  physics,  and  biology,  as 
well  as  some  that  were  common  to  all  three  fields.  After  the 
principles  submitted  had  been  classified  into  subject-matter 
areas,  the  complete  list  was  sent  to  a  number  of  teachers  in 
the  Thirty  Schools.  These  teachers  were  asked  to: 

1.  Select  those  statements  that  they  would  expect  their 
students  to  apply  in  making  predictions  or  explana- 
tions in  new  situations. 

2.  Select  those  statements  that  they  would  expect  their 
students  to  know  in  a  general  way,  but  not  to  the  ex- 
tent of  being  able  to  use  them  to  make  predictions  in 
new  situations. 

Only  those  principles  which  were  included  in  the  first 
category  by  at  least  three-fourths  of  the  teachers  were  con- 


8o          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

sidered  for  use  in  the  tests.  Two  additional  criteria  were 
established  to  aid  in  the  selection: 

3.  The  principle  should  have  a  wide  range  of  applica- 
bility to  commonly  occurring  natural  phenomena. 

4.  The  principle,  with  examples  of  its  application  to 
commonly  occurring  phenomena,  should  be  found  in 
all  of  the  science  textbooks  commonly  used  in  these 
schools. 

The  teachers  were  also  asked  to  judge  the  relevance  of  each 
principle  to  the  areas  of  general  science,  biology,  chemistry, 
or  physics,  or  to  all  of  these  areas. 

THE  DEVELOPMENT  OF  EVALUATION  INSTRUMENTS 

During  the  period  of  the  Eight- Year  Study  a  number  of 
instruments  were  developed  for  evaluating  the  ability  to 
apply  principles.  Several  of  these  instruments  included  prin- 
ciples drawn  from  the  subject-matter  area  of  general  science; 
others  were  restricted  to  principles  drawn  from  physics, 
chemistry,  or  biology.  Because  the  instruments  which  in- 
cluded principles  from  general  science  were  used  more  ex- 
tensively than  the  others  and  because  they  were  the  ones 
experimented  with  in  attempting  to  arrive  at  a  satisfactory 
pattern  for  the  test,  they  will  be  used  to  illustrate  the  con 
struction  of  tests  of  application  of  principles. 

Preliminary  Investigations 

In  preparing  a  test  of  Application  of  Principles,  the  first 
step  after  the  principles  had  been  selected  was  to  obtain 
problem  situations  to  which  the  student  might  react.  Teachers 
were  asked  to  submit  to  the  committee  problem  situations 
which: 

1.  were  new  to  the  students  (i.e.,  they  were  not  ordi- 
narily discussed  in  the  classroom  or  used  in  the  text- 
books ) ; 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  81 

2.  occur  rather  commonly  in  actual  life; 

3.  could  be   explained  by   the  principles  which  the 
teachers  had  selected  as  important  for  their  students 
to  apply. 

Attempts  to  phrase  the  problem  situations  revealed  that  they 
might  be  so  described  as  to  demand  several  different  types 
of  response  from  the  student.  Four  types  of  response  were 
used;  namely,  making  a  prediction,  offering  an  explanation 
for  an  observed  phenomenon,  choosing  a  course  of  action, 
and  criticizing  a  prediction  or  explanation  made  by  others. 
An  illustrative  situation  of  each  type  follows: 

1.  A  farmer  grafted  a  Jonathan  apple  twig  on  a  small 
Baldwin  apple  tree  from  which  he  had  first  removed 
all  the  branches.  The  graft  was  successful.  If  a  new 
branch  develops  from  a  bud  below  the  point  of  the 
graft  and  produces  apples,  what  kind  of  apple  will  it 
be?  Here  the  student  is  asked  to  make  a  prediction 
about  a  situation  in  which  presumably  he  has  had  no 
actual  experience.  It  is  presumed  that  if  he  under- 
stands certain  laws  of  heredity,  he  will  be  able  to 
make  a  valid  prediction. 

2.  All  of  the  leaves  of  a  growing  green  plant  were  ob- 
served to  be  facing  in  the  same  direction.  Under  what 
conditions  of  lighting  was  the  plant  probably  grown? 
This  example  requires  that  the  student  offer  an  ex- 
planation of  an  observed  phenomenon.  Some  knowl- 
edge of  the  principles  of  photosynthesis,  growth,  and 
tropistic  responses  of  plants  would  be  required  for  the 
solution  of  this  problem. 

3.  The  rear  of  an  automobile  on  a  wet  pavement  is  skid- 
ding   toward    a    ditch.    If    you    were    the    driver 
of  {he  car,  what  would  you  do  to  bring  the  car  out  of 
the  skid?  This  problem  requires  the  student  to  choose 
a  course  of  action.  A  knowledge  of  the  principles  of 


82          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

centrifugal  force  and  Newton's  laws  of  motion  would 
enable  die  student  to  choose  a  satisfactory  course  of 
action. 

4.  It  was  reported  in  a  newspaper  that  in  order  to  tow 
down  a  river  a  huge  oil  drum  filled  with  air,  the  work- 
men found  it  necessary  to  fill  the  drum  with  com- 
pressed air  to  increase  its  buoyancy.  Do  you  believe 
that  this  would  increase  the  buoyancy  of  the  oil 
drum?  This  problem  asks  the  student  to  criticize  an 
explanation  which  has  been  given.  Knowledge  of  the 
fact  that  air  has  weight  and  of  the  principles  of 
buoyancy  are  required  for  a  satisfactory  solution  in 
this  problem. 

In  none  of  these  problems  were  the  answers  expected  to 
be  in  exact  quantitative  terms;  rather  a  qualitative  under- 
standing of  the  general  outcome  was  required.  It  was  thought 
that  the  kind  of  activity  shown  by  students  in  making  a  pre- 
diction of  this  kind  was  of  more  importance  for  general 
education  than  one  which  required  exact  substitutions  of 
numerical  data  in  a  formula  or  similar  activities  frequently 
used  in  the  laboratory.  One  often  encounters  problems  in 
which  a  principle  is  used  to  explain  what  happens  in  general 
when  certain  factors  are  varied  in  the  situation,  while  the 
need  for  numerical  solutions  of  problems  occurs  relatively 
infrequently  for  most  people.  Although  the  above  problem 
situations  are  stated  in  such  a  way  that  the  student  is  ex- 
pected to  react  somewhat  differently  in  each,  it  is  not  likely 
that  he  will  react  intelligently  to  any  of  these  situations  un- 
less he  has  a  knowledge  of  the  principles  operating  and  has 
recognized  their  application  to  the  problem.  Whether  he 
criticizes  a  prediction  made  by  someone  else  or  makes  the 
prediction  himself,  he  must  base  his  answer  upon  the  knowl- 
edge which  he  feels  is  applicable  to  the  situation. 

The  next  step  in  constructing  the  test  was  to  determine  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  83 

reasons  which  might  justify  the  response  to  the  problem 
situation,  and  to  find  a  means  of  appraising  the  reasons  cited 
by  the  student.  Science  teachers  were  in  rather  general  agree- 
ment that  the  most  valid  of  all  the  reasons  a  student  might 
use  for  justifying  his  conclusions  would  be  those  that  cited 
established  scientific  facts,  principles,  and  generalizations. 
However,  in  addition  to  these,  it  was  agreed  that  the  student 
might  cite  from  his  experience,  from  authoritative  materials 
he  had  read,  or  he  might  use  analogous  situations  familiar  to 
the  person  to  whom  he  was  explaining  his  decision,  provided 
these  experiences,  authorities,  or  analogies  were  pertinent  to 
the  situation  he  was  attempting  to  explain. 

In  order  to  determine  whether  or  not  students  did  use 
these  kinds  of  reasons,  they  were  asked  to  write  out  both 
their  own  predictions,  choice  of  action  or  responses  to  the 
situation,  and  all  of  the  reasons  that  they  believed  would 
support  the  decision  they  had  made.  When  these  papers  were 
analyzed  by  the  teachers  and  the  Evaluation  Staff,  the  types 
of  acceptable  reasons  which  had  been  anticipated  were 
found  in  the  students'  responses.  However,  in  addition  to  the 
reasons  which  were  agreed  upon  as  being  acceptable,  certain 
types  of  errors  were  also  found  to  occur  rather  consistently 
in  the  written  responses  of  the  students.  It  was  found  that 
students  frequently  used  teleological  explanations  and 
analogies  not  closely  correspondent  to  the  situation  de- 
scribed in  the  problem.  They  cited  authorities  that  were  ques- 
tionable, ridiculed  positions  other  than  their  own,  stated  as 
facts  certain  misconceptions  or  superstitions,  merely  restated 
either  the  facts  given  or  their  own  prediction,  and  made  less 
frequently  a  variety  of  other  types  of  errors.  They  also  used, 
in  addition  to  the  principles  and  facts  judged  to  be  accept- 
able and  necessary  to  the  explanation  of  the  problem,  other 
facts  and  principles  that  were  irrelevant  to  the  solution  of 
the  problem.  The  frequency  with  which  each  of  these  types 
of  reasons  was  used  was  not  constant,  but  varied  from  class 


84          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

to  class  and  from  problem  to  problem.  In  examinations  of 
sufficient  length  given  to  a  large  number  of  students,  how- 
ever, these  types  of  errors  were  found  to  be  most  prevalent. 
In  general,  it  was  possible  to  infer  that  the  errors  were 
made  because: 

1.  The  student  did  not  know  the  principles. 

2.  He  did  not  see  that  a  principle  he  knew  applied  to 
the  situation. 

3.  He  knew  the  principle  and  knew  that  it  applied  to 
the  situation,  but  he  was  unable  to  explain  adroitly 
how  or  why  it  applied. 

4.  He  used  teleology,  poor  analogy,  or  poor  authority, 
rather  than   (or  in  addition  to)   correct  facts  and 
principles. 

5.  Although  his  explanation  was  correct  as  far  as  it  was 
given,  he  cited  facts  and  principles  which  were  in- 
adequate for  a  convincing  proof  for  a  given  selected 
conclusion  or  course  of  action. 

6.  He  confused  closely  related  principles,  only  one  of 
which  was  applicable  to  the  problem. 

7.  He  used  irrelevant  material. 

8.  He  neglected  to  study  the  description  of  the  situation 
carefully  enough  to  note  all  of  the  limiting  factors  in 
the  description. 

This  list  does  not  include  all  of  the  reasons  why  students 
made  errors  but  it  does  help  to  show  why  it  was  difficult  to 
score  the  written  responses. 

Construction  of  Early  Short-Answer  Forms 

The  same  problems  of  objectivity  of  scoring  and  of  ade- 
quate sampling  that  are  found  in  any  essay-type  test  were 
inherent  in  these  written  responses.  The  teachers  found  that 
it  was  difficult  to  differentiate  among  those  acceptable  uses 
of  generalizations,  facts  and  principles  which  were  relevant 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  85 

to  the  problem,  and  the  logical  errors,  obscured  as  they  some- 
times were  by  illegibility  o£  handwriting  and  by  awkward 
literary  style.  It  was  also  difficult  to  decide  when  a  student 
had  cited  enough  evidence  to  support  his  choice  of  answer. 
A  second  criticism  of  this  form  of  test  was  that  it  limited  the 
number  of  principles  which  could  be  sampled  because  of 
the  time  required  by  the  student  to  write  out  the  answers. 
Because  of  these  difficulties,  a  more  objective  means  of  test- 
ing this  same  ability  was  sought. 

Following  a  study  of  the  responses  written  out  by  students, 
the  first  of  a  series  of  objective  test  forms  in  this  area  was 
made.  The  objective  form  of  the  test  asked  the  student  to 
select  from  a  list  of  predictions  for  each  problem  situation 
the  one  which  he  thought  was  most  likely  to  be  true,  and 
then  to  select  from  a  list  of  reasons  those  which  would  be 
necessary  to  establish  the  validity  of  his  choice.  The  predic- 
tions and  reasons  used  in  the  test  paralleled  those  which 
had  been  used  frequently  by  the  students  when  they  wrote 
essay-type  responses.  When  experimental  groups  were  given 
an  examination  which  required  them  to  write  out  their  pre- 
dictions and  reasons  for  the  first  half  of  the  testing  period, 
and  an  examination  in  which  they  were  required  to  select 
the  correct  prediction  and  the  reasons  which  supported  it 
from  a  given  list  during  the  latter  half  of  the  period,  it  was 
found  that  the  results  on  the  two  types  of  examinations  were 
quite  similar.  The  coefficient  of  correlation  was  in  all  cases 
above  0.80.15  The  advantages  of  more  objective  scoring  and 
the  possibilities  for  more  extensive  sampling  of  problem 
situations  led  to  the  adoption  of  the  objective  form. 

15  Ralph  W.  Tyler,  Constructing  Achievement  Tests,  Bureau  of  Educa- 
tional Research,  Ohio  State  University;  Fred  P.  Frutchey,  "Evaluating 
Chemistry  Instruction,"  Educational  Research  Bulletin,  XVI  (Jan.  13, 
1937);  Louis  E.  Raths,  "Techniques  of  Test  Construction,"  Educational 
Research  Bulletin,  XVII  (April  13,  1938);  Louis  M.  Heil,  "Evaluation  of 
Student  Achievement  in  the  Physical  Sciences — The  Application  of  Laws 
and  Principles/*  The  American  Physics  Teacher,  VI  (April,  1938). 


86          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

The  procedures  used  in  preparing  the  early  form  of  ob- 
jective tests  in  this  area  were  as  follows: 

1.  The  principles  to  be  used  in  the  test  were  selected 
in  accordance  with  the  criteria  formulated  by  the 
teachers  interested  in  this  objective. 

2.  Problem  situations  in  which  these  selected  principles 
would  apply  were  chosen  with  the  following  criteria 
in  mind: 

2.1  They  were  to  be  new  in  the  sense  that  they  had 
not  been  used  in  the  classroom  or  laboratory. 

2.2  The  situation  should  approximate  a  rather  com- 
monly occurring  life  situation. 

2.3  The  problem  should  be  significant  to  students  in 
that  its  solution  might  help  them  to  solve  similar 
problems  which  occur  in  their  everyday  living. 

2.4  The  vocabulary  used  should  be  at  an  appropriate 
level  for  the  students  taking  the  test.  They  should 
be  able  to  understand  the  description  of  the 
situation. 

3.  Several  (usually  three  or  more)  plausible  answers  for 
the  problem  were  formulated.  These  might  be  in  the 
form  of  predictions,  courses  of  action  to  be  taken, 
causes  to  be  stated,  or  an  evaluation  of  one  of  these 
when  it  was  given.  Actually,  when  possible  answers 
were  suggested  by  listing  them  in  the  test,  the  proce- 
dure in  every  case  would  be  one  of  evaluation  through 
the  selection  of  what  the  student  thought  was  the 
most  desirable,  whether  it  was  a  prediction,  course 
of  action  or  explanation  for  the  phenomena  which 
had  been  described  in  the  problem. 

4.  Finally,  reasons  of  the  sort  used  by  students  were 
listed,  including  for  each  situation  those  common 
types  of  errors  which  students  made  when  they  wrote 
out  their  reasons.  In  addition  to  correct  statements 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  87 

of  scientific  principles  needed  for  a  satisfactory  ex- 
planation, the  following  types  of  statements  were 
formulated: 

4.1  False  statements  purporting  to  be  facts  or  prin- 
ciples. These,  if  accepted  as  true,  would  support 
one  of  the  alternative  conclusions.  For  example, 
if  the  correct  principle  stated  that  a  direct  rela- 
tionship existed  between  two  phenomena,   one 
might  word  a  false  statement  in  such  a  way  as  to 
indicate  that  there  was  no  relationship  or  that 
the  relationship  was  an  inverse  one.  To  remain 
consistent  in  his  reasoning,  the  student  can  use 
such  a  statement  only  to  support  a  conclusion 
other  than  the  acceptable  one. 

4.2  Irrelevant  reasons.  These  statements  are  true,  but 
either  they  have  no  relationship  to  the  phenom- 
enon described  in  the  problem  or  they  are  quite 
unnecessary  in  the  explanation  of  the  phenom- 
enon. 

4.3  False  analogies.  These  stated  directly  or  inferred 
that  the  phenomenon  described  in  the  problem 
was  identical  with,  or  very  much  like,  some  other 
known  phenomenon  when  it  actually  had  little 
or  nothing  in  common  with  it;  therefore,  an  ex- 
planation  for   one   phenomenon   would   not  be 
acceptable  for  explaining  the  other.  Metaphors 
were  sometimes  included  as  an  example  of  a  more 
subtle  use  of  analogy,  in  that  the  analogy  was 
implied  by  the  use  of  words  but  not  definitely 
expressed. 

4.4  Popular  misconceptions.  These  included  the  more 
common  beliefs  based  upon  unreliable  evidence 
or  false  assumptions.  Frequently  they  were  state- 
ments of  rather  common  practices  based  upon 
accepted     but     unreliable     evidence.     Common 


88          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

cliches  or  superstitions  would  also  be  included  in 
this  type  of  statements. 

4.5  The  citing  of  unreliable  authorities.  Statements  in- 
troduced by  phrases  such  as  "Science  says  .  .  .    " 
or  "People  say  .  .  .  /*  or  "It  is  reported  in  pop- 
ular magazines  that  .  .  ."  were  used.  Here  a  dis- 
tinction must  be  made  between  such  very  gen- 
eral or  unreliable  sources  and  those  which  might 
be  used  with  considerable  assurance.  However, 
in  any  case  the  mere  citation  of  authority  did  not 
in  any  sense  explain  why  a  particular  point  of 
view  was  correct;  one  would  need  in  addition 
to  give  the  evidence  used  by  this  authority  to 
establish  his  position   on  the  outcome   of  the 
problem. 

4.6  Ridicule.  This  rather  common  device  of  students 
in  their  explanations  suggested  that  any  position 
contrary  to  their  own  could  only  be  held  by  some- 
one who  did  not  know  the  facts. 

4.7  Assuming  the  conclusion.  These  statements  as- 
sumed what  was  to  be  proved.  This  was  most 
frequently  represented  in  these  tests  by  essen- 
tially repeating  the  conclusion  by  rewording  it 
without  changing  its  meaning. 

4.8  Teleology.  These  statements  assume  that  plants, 
animals,   or   inanimate   objects   are   rational   or 
purposive. 

An  example  of  the  wording  of  the  directions  for  one  of  the 
tests  and  a  sample  problem  taken  from  the  test  follow. 

Form  1.3 

APPLICATION  OF  PKINCIPLES 

Directions:  In  each  of  the  following  exercises  a  problem  is  given. 
Below  each  problem  are  two  lists  of  statements.  The  first  list  con- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  89 

tains  statements  which  can  be  used  to  answer  the  problem.  Place 
a  check  mark  (V)  i*1  the  parentheses  after  the  statement  or 
statements  which  answer  the  problem.  The  second  list  contains 
statements  which  can  be  used  to  explain  the  right  answers.  Place 
a  check  mark  (\/}  in  the  parentheses  after  the  statement  or 
statements  which  give  the  reasons  for  the  right  answers.  Some  of 
the  other  statements  are  true  but  do  not  explain  the  right  an- 
swers; do  not  check  these.  In  doing  these  exercises  then,  you  are 
to  place  a  check  mark  (V)  i*1  tne  parentheses  after  the  state- 
ments which  answer  the  problem  and  which  give  the  reasons  for 
the  EIGHT  answers. 

In  warm  weather  people  who  do  not  have  refrigerators  some- 
times wrap  a  bottle  of  milk  in  a  wet  towel  and  place  it  where 
there  is  a  good  circulation  of  air.  Would  a  bottle  of  milk  so 
treated  stay  sweet  as  long  as  a  similar  bottle  of  milk  without  a 
wet  towel? 
A  bottle  wrapped  with  the  wet  towel  would  stay  sweet 

a.  longer  than  without  the  wet  towel . .  (  )  a. 

b.  not  as  long  as  without  the  wet  towel.  (  )  b. 

c.  the   same  length  of  time — the  wet 
towel  would  make  no  difference . . . .  (  )  c. 

Check  the  statements  below  which  give  the  reason  or  reasons 
for  your  explanation  above. 

Superstition  d.  Thunderstorms  hasten  the  souring  of 

milk (  )  d. 

Right  Principle  e.  The  souring  of  milk  is  the  result  of 
the  growth  and  life  processes  of  bac- 
teria   (  )  e. 

Wrong  f.  Wrapping  the  bottle  prevents  bac- 
teria from  getting  into  the  milk (  )  f. 

Wrong  g.  A  wet  towel  could  not  interfere  with 

the  growth  of  bacteria  in  the  milk . .  (  )  g. 

Wrong  h.  Wrapping  keeps  out  the  air  and  hin- 
ders bacterial  growth. (  )  h. 

Right  Principle  i  Evaporation  is  accompanied  by  an 

absorption  of  heat (  )  i. 


90  ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Authority  j.  Milkmen  often  advise  housewives  to 

wrap  bottles  in  wet  towels (  )  j. 

Unacceptable       k.  Just  as  many  foods  are  wrapped  in 

Analogy  cellophane  to  keep  in  moisture,  so  is 

milk  kept  sweet  by  wrapping  a  wet 

towel  around  the  bottle  to  keep  the 

moisture  in (      )  k. 

Right  Principle  1.  Bacteria  do  not  grow  so  rapidly 

when  temperatures  are  kept  low. . . .  (  )  L 

In  formulating  statements  for  these  earlier  test  forms,  no 
consistent  pattern  was  followed.  A  study  of  the  results  ob- 
tained by  giving  Form  1.3  to  many  science  students  sug- 
gested the  desirability  of  using  in  each  of  the  testing  situa- 
tions a  pattern  of  reasons  which  would  remain  constant 
throughout  the  test.  It  was  believed  that  this  would  tend  to 
give  a  greater  reliability  to  the  subscores  used  in  interpreta- 
tion and  thus  make  the  interpretations  more  meaningful.  The 
pattern  of  reasons  to  be  included  was  determined  through 
discussions  with  teachers  who  had  used  Form  1.3.  They  were 
asked  to  indicate  the  types  of  items  in  the  test  which  seemed 
to  be  most  useful  in  diagnosing  students*  difficulties.  Using 
their  suggestions,  tests  employing  a  pattern  of  responses 
were  constructed  by  following  through  these  steps:  Situa- 
tions were  selected  using  the  criteria  described  for  Form  1.3 
but  with  greater  emphasis  upon  problems  of  social  signifi- 
cance. These  situations  were  worded  in  a  way  that  would 
require  an  explanation,  prediction,  choice  of  course  of  action, 
or  an  evaluation  of  any  one  of  these.  Three  conclusions  were 
then  formulated,  one  being  defensible  through  the  use  of 
science  principles  as  preferable  to  the  other  two.  In  every 
case  the  other  two  conclusions  would  not  be  nonsensical, 
absurd,  or  preposterous. 

The  reasons  used  in  the  test  were  arrived  at  by  first  sup- 
porting the  correct  conclusion  by  formulating  three  state- 
ments of  facts  or  principles  which  support  it  and  by  implica- 
tion eliminate  the  other  two  conclusions.  Four  wrong  reasons 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  91 

which,  if  accepted  as  true,  would  support  the  other  conclu- 
sions were  next  formulated.  Two  of  these  would  tend  to 
support  one  of  the  wrong  conclusions  and  two  the  other. 
They  would  all  tend  by  implication  to  eliminate  the  right 
conclusion.  One  statement  was  formulated  so  as  to  be  true 
but  irrelevant  to  the  explanation  of  the  problem.  One  each 
of  the  following  kinds  of  reasons  completed  the  pattern — 
a  teleological  statement,  ridicule  statement,  assuming  the 
conclusion,  unacceptable  analogy,  unacceptable  authority, 
and  unacceptable  common  practice.  Each  of  these  was 
worded  to  appear  to  be  consistent  with  the  conclusion  keyed 
as  right.  Tests  following  this  general  procedure  were  con- 
structed for  the  areas  of  chemistry  (Form  1.31),  physics 
(Form  1.32),  biology  (Form  1.33),  and  general  science 
(Forml.Sa).16 

A  sample  problem  taken  from  Form  1.3a  is  given  with  the 
directions  and  key. 

PROBLEM 

The  water  supply  for  a  certain  big  city  is  obtained  from  a  large 
lake,  and  sewage  is  disposed  of  in  a  river  flowing  from  the  lake. 
This  river  at  one  time  flowed  into  the  lake,  but  during  the  glacial 
period  its  direction  of  flow  was  reversed.  Occasionally,  during 
heavy  rains  in  the  spring,  water  from  the  river  backs  up  into  the 
lake.  What  should  be  done  to  safeguard  effectively  and  econom- 
ically the  health  of  the  people  living  in  this  city? 

Directions:  Choose  the  conclusion  which  you  believe  is  most  con- 
sistent with  the  facts  given  above  and  most  reasonable  in  the 
light  of  whatever  knowledge  you  may  have,  and  mark  the  appro- 
priate space  on  the  Answer  Sheet  under  Problem 

Conclusions: 

V    A.  During  the  spring  season  the  amount  of  chemicals  used 

in  purifying  the  water  should  be  increased.  (Supported 

by  3,  7,  10,  12) 
B.  A  permanent  system  of  treating  the  sewage  before  it  is 

16  A  junior  high  school  test,  Form  l.Sj,  which  uses  a  somewhat  different 
and  less  complex  technique  was  also  constructed. 


92          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

dumped  into  the  river  should  be  provided.   (Consistent 
with  5,  8,  12) 

C.  During  the  spring  season  water  should  be  taken  from  the 
lake  at  a  point  some  distance  from  the  origin  of  the 
river.  (Consistent  with  12,  14) 

Directions:  Choose  the  reasons  you  would  use  to  explain  or  sup- 
port your  conclusion  and  fill  in  the  appropriate  spaces  on  your 
Answer  Sheet.  Be  sure  that  your  marks  are  in  one  column  only — 
the  same  column  in  which  you  marked  the  conclusion. 

Reasons: 

False  1.  In  the  light  of  the  fact  that  bacteria  cannot 

analogy  survive  in  salted  meat,  we  may  say  that  they 

cannot  survive  in  chlorinated  water. 
Irrelevant  2.  Many  bacteria  in  sewage  are  not  harmful  to 

man. 

Right  3.  Chlorination  of  water  is  one  of  the  least  ex- 

Principle  pensive  methods  of  eliminating  harmful  bac- 

teria from  a  water  supply. 

Ridicule  4.  An  enlightened  individual  would  know  that 

the  best  way  to  kill  bacteria  is  to  use  chlorine. 
Wrong  5.  A  sewage  treatment  system  is  cheaper  than 

Supporting  B  the  use  of  chlorine. 

Authority  6.  Bacteriologists  say  that  bacteria  can  be  best 

controlled  with  chlorine. 

Right  7.  As  the  number  of  micro-organisms  increases 

in  a  given  amount  of  water,  the  quantity  of 
chlorine  necessary  to  kill  the  organisms  must 
be  increased. 

Wrong  8.  A  sewage  treatment  system  is  the  only  means 

Supporting  B  known  by  which  water  can  be  made  abso- 

lutely safe. 

Assuming  9.  By  increasing  the  amount  of  chlorine  in  the 

Conclusion  water  supply,  the  health  of  the  people  in  this 

city  will  be  protected. 

Right  10.  Harmful  bacteria  in  water  are  killed  when  a 

small  amount  of  chlorine  is  placed  in  the 
water. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  93 

Tele-  11.  When  bacteria  come  in  contact  with  chlorine 

ology  they  move  out  of  the  chlorinated  area  in 

order  to  survive. 

Right  12.  Untreated  sewage  contains  vast  numbers  of 

Supporting  bacteria,  many  of  which  may  cause  disease 

ABC  in  man. 

Prac-  13.  In  most  cities  it  is  customary  to  use  chlorine 

tice  to  control  harmful  bacteria  in  the  water  sup- 

ply. 

Wrong  14.  Sewage  deposited  in  a  lake  tends  to  remain 

Supporting  C  in  an  area  close  to  the  point  of  entry. 

An  examination  of  the  complete  test  would  show  that  the 
problem  situations  included  in  this  form  of  the  test  deal  with 
personal  health,  public  health,  eugenics,  conservation,  and 
the  like,  and  many  of  them  involve  questions  of  opinion  as 
well  as  of  the  operation  of  science  principles.  The  desirabil- 
ity of  using  these  types  of  problem  situations  was  mentioned 
by  many  of  the  science  teachers  who  had  used  the  earlier 
form  of  the  test;  however,  after  such  problems  were  form- 
ulated it  was  discovered  that  very  little  agreement  could  be 
secured  among  these  teachers  as  to  the  most  defensible  con- 
clusions for  such  problems.  This  difficulty  is  illustrated  by 
the  above  problem  on  water  supply.  Several  science  prin- 
ciples might  be  cited  in  proposing  a  solution  to  the  problem 
of  securing  for  this  city  a  supply  of  water  free  from  patho- 
genic bacteria;  but  whether  or  not  a  supply  of  water  free 
from  pathogenic  bacteria  constitutes  an  "effective"  safeguard 
of  the  health  of  these  people  and  whether  or  not  any  pro- 
posed method  of  securing  such  a  supply  of  water  will  be 
"economical"  cannot  be  determined  by  science  principles 
alone. 

In  choosing  any  one  of  the  three  conclusions  given  with 
this  problem,  it  is  necessary  for  the  student  to  interpret  the 
terms  effectively  and  economically.  If  the  student  regards 
reasonable  safety,  such  as  might  be  secured  by  the  adminis- 


94          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

tration  of  additional  chemicals  to  the  water  supply,  as  an 
effective  safeguard,  and  if  he  regards  the  use  of  chemicals 
as  an  economical  practice,  then  he  might  defend  conclusion 
A.  However,  another  student  might  wish  to  defend  conclu- 
sion B  by  pointing  out  that  the  use  of  chemicals  assures  only 
a  reasonable  safety  under  ordinary  conditions  and  may  fail 
under  unusual  circumstances,  such  as  the  sudden  reversal  of 
flow  of  the  river,  and  that  this  practice  cannot  be  considered 
economical  in  the  long  run  when  all  the  benefits  of  a  sewage 
disposal  system  are  considered.  Still  another  student  might 
defend  conclusion  C  as  representing  a  more  effective  safe- 
guard than  that  of  A  and  a  more  economical  practice  than 
that  of  B. 

The  difficulty  of  keying  any  of  these  responses  by  students 
as  the  correct  one,  unless  one  knows  all  of  the  evidence  and 
values  which  the  student  would  use  to  support  his  point  of 
view,  is  obvious.  Insofar  as  the  student  considers  the  prob- 
able effects  of  these  practices  upon  the  people  living  in  the 
city,  upon  the  people  in  nearby  regions  or  in  towns  lying 
along  the  river,  upon  the  future  as  well  as  the  present  citi- 
zens of  this  region,  and  upon  the  biological  life  in  the  waters 
of  this  region,  he  may  interpret  the  terms  effectively  and 
economically  so  as  "to  justify  any  of  these  three  conclusions. 
The  pertinent  science  principles  can  only  aid  a  person  in 
predicting  the  effects  of  each  of  these  practices;  they  cannot 
determine  whether  or  not  these  effects  are  to  be  desired. 
Other  students  might  wish  to  remain  uncertain  about  which 
conclusion  to  choose  until  further  evidence  had  been  ob- 
tained about  the  problem.  Such  evidence  might  reveal  that 
it  would  be  better  to  put  into  practice  all  three  of  the  sug- 
gested conclusions,  i.e.,  purify  the  sewage  by  a  permanent 
system  of  treatment  before  it  is  dumped  into  the  river,  take 
the  water  from  the  lake  at  a  greater  distance  from  the  shore, 
and  finally  add  chlorine  to  the  water  before  it  is  put  into 
the  water  mains.  It  should  be  clear  from  this  discussion  that 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  95 

die  effort  to  construct  a  test  form  which  involved  social 
values  as  well  as  scientific  principles  led  to  situations  which 
were  well  suited  for  generating  a  desirable  type  of  thinking, 
but  which  at  the  same  time  created  considerable  technical 
difficulty  for  the  test  constructors.  In  the  discussion  of  the 
next  test  in  this  series  a  method  for  solving  these  difficulties, 
at  least  partially,  will  be  discussed. 

Structure  of  Form  i.jb 

In  developing  Form  1.3b  two  changes  were  made:  (1) 
the  adoption  of  a  different  form  of  conclusion  and  the  con- 
sequent inclusion  of  reasons  to  be  used  if  the  student  were 
uncertain  about  the  conclusion;  (2)  addition  of  acceptable 
analogy  and  acceptable  authority  to  the  reasons  to  be  used 
to  support  or  refute  the  conclusion.  A  keyed  sample  prob- 
lem from  Form  1.3b  is  reprinted  here  to  illustrate  these 
changes: 

PROBLEM  I 

A  motorist  driving  a  new  car  at  night  at  the  rate  of  30  miles  per 
hour  saw  a  warning  sign  beside  the  road  indicating  a  "through 
highway"  intersection  200  feet  ahead.  He  applied  his  brakes 
when  he  was  opposite  the  sign  and  brought  his  car  to  a  stop  65 
feet  beyond  the  sign.  Suppose  this  motorist  had  been  traveling 
at  the  rate  of  60  miles  per  hour  and  had  applied  his  brakes  ex- 
actly as  he  did  before.  He  would  have  been  unable  to  stop  his 
car  before  reaching  the  "through  highway"  intersection. 

Directions: 

A.  If  you  are  uncertain  about  the  truth  or  falsity  of  the  under- 
lined statement,  place  a  mark  in  the  box  on  the  answer  sheet 
under  A. 

B.  If  you  think  that  the  underlined  statement  is  quite  likely  to  be 
true,  place  a  mark  in  the  box  on  the  answer  sheet  under  B. 

C.  If  you  disagree  with  the  underlined  statement,  place  a  mark 
in  the  box  on  the  answer  sheet  under  C. 


96 


ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


Directions  for  Reasons: 

If  you  placed  a  mark  under  A,  select  -from  the  first  ten  reasons 
given  below  all  those  which  help  you  to  explain  thoroughly  why 
you  were  uncertain  and  place  a  mark  in  Column  A  opposite  each 
of  the  reasons  you  decide  to  use. 

If  you  placed  a  mark  under  B,  select  from  reasons  11  through  24 
all  those  which  help  you  to  explain  thoroughly  why  you  agreed 
with  the  underlined  statement  and  place  a  mark  in  Column  B 
opposite  each  of  the  reasons  you  decide  to  use. 
If  you  placed  a  mark  under  C,  select  from  reasons  11  through  24 
all  those  which  help  you  to  explain  thoroughly  why  you  dis- 
agreed with  the  underlined  statement  and  place  a  mark  in  Col- 
umn C  opposite  each  of  the  reasons  you  decide  to  use. 

Reasons  to  be  used  if  you  are  uncertain: 

Lack  of  1.  I  have  never  driven  an  automobile  at  60  miles 

Experience         per  hour  and  don't  know  how  far  an  automobile 

will  travel  after  the  brakes  are  applied. 

Irrelevant  2.  The  distance  required  to  bring  a  car  to  a  stop 
"Control"  depends  upon  the  condition  of  the  road  surface. 

Irrelevant  3.  The  reaction  time  of  the  driver  is  an  important 
"Control"  factor  in  determining  the  distance  a  car  will 

travel  before  it  stops. 

Irrelevant  4,  The  mechanical  efficiency  of  the  brakes  will  af- 
"Control"  feet  the  distances  required  for  stopping  a  car. 

Irrelevant  5.  Whether  the  brakes  are  of  the  mechanical  or  hy- 
"Control"  draulic  type  would  make  a  difference  in  the 

stopping  distance. 

Irrelevant  6.  There  are  too  many  variable  conditions  in  the 
"Control"  situation  to  enable  one  to  be  sure  about  the  stop- 

ping distance. 

Lack  of  7.  I  do  not  know  which  mathematical  formula  to 

Knowledge         apply  in  this  problem. 

Irrelevant  8.  The  distance  required  to  bring  a  car  to  a  stop 
"Control"  depends  upon  the  mass  of  the  car  as  well  as  the 

speed. 

Irrelevant  9.  Whether  he  stopped  the  car  or  not  before  enter- 
"Control"  ing  the  intersection  would  depend  upon  how 

good  a  driver  he  was. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  97 

Irrelevant     10.  The  condition  of  the  tires  would  be  a  factor  to 
"Control"  consider  in  determining  the  stopping  distance  for 

the  automobile. 

The  description  of  this  problem  includes  an  underlined 
conclusion  which  the  student  is  asked  to  judge.  The  student 
may  agree,  disagree,  or  be  uncertain  about  the  conclusion. 
In  the  earlier  tests  he  had  been  asked  to  select  from  a  list  of 
conclusions  the  one  he  thought  most  appropriately  answered 
the  question  asked  in  the  description  of  the  science  situa- 
tion. The  use  of  this  form  of  the  problem  was  adopted  in 
order  to  score  the  student  on  his  ability  to  distinguish  be- 
tween problems  in  which  sufficient  information  was  given  to 
enable  him  to  be  reasonably  sure  of  his  answer,  and  others 
about  which  he  should  remain  uncertain  because  necessary 
information  was  not  included  in  the  description  of  the  prob- 
lem. This  form  of  the  problem  also  enables  the  teacher  to 
discover  those  students  who  have  become  "over-critical/7  i.e., 
who  challenge  problems  by  choosing  the  uncertain  response 
when,  in  the  judgment  of  the  teachers,  these  problems  are 
so  stated  that  one  can  either  agree  or  disagree  with  the 
conclusion. 

An  investigation  was  undertaken  to  discover  what  effect 
the  changed  form  of  presenting  the  conclusion  might  have 
upon  the  results.  It  was  found  that  it  made  little  difference 
in  which  form  the  conclusion  was  given.  Ninety-one  students 
were  given  a  test  especially  prepared  for  this  investigation 
in  which  they  were  asked  to  select  from  a  list  of  four  con- 
clusions the  one  that  they  believed  was  most  appropriate. 
This  was  followed  in  the  same  testing  period  by  a  second 
prepared  test  in  which  they  were  asked  to  make  a  judgment 
about  a  single  conclusion.  Two  sample  items  are  given  here 
to  illustrate  how  the  problems  were  paired  in  the  two  tests. 

TEST  I,  PROBLEM  1 

A  motorist  had  his  tires  filled  to  35  pounds  of  pressure  when  the 
temperature  was  110°  F.  The  temperature  dropped  to  80°  the 


98          ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

next  day.  What  probably  happened  to  the  pressure  of  the  air  in 
the  tires?  (Assume  that  no  air  is  lost  from  the  tires. ) 

(     )  A.  The  pressure  would  be  greater  than  35  pounds. 
(      )  B,  The  pressure  would  be  less  than  35  pounds. 
(      )  C.  The  pressure  would  not  change. 
(      )  D.  The  pressure  may  be  the  same,  greater,  or  less — 
one  cannot  tell. 

TEST  II,  PROBLEM  I 

A  motorist  on  a  trip  to  the  West  had  his  tires  checked  to  35 
pounds  on  the  edge  of  Death  Valley  Desert  at  about  4:00  P.M. 
That  night  he  stayed  at  a  nearby  tourists'  camp  where  the  tem- 
perature always  dropped  several  degrees  during  the  night.  In 
order  to  be  sure  that  the  old  tires  on  his  car  would  not  blow  out 
during  the  night,  he  should  let  some  of  the  air  out  of  the  tires. 

(     )  Agree  (     )  Disagree  (     )  Uncertain 

Twenty-two  such  paired  problems  were  included  in  the 
two  tests.  A  correlation  between  the  number  of  right  re- 
sponses made  on  the  two  tests  was  found  to  be  .83.  The  two 
tests  were  found  to  be  about  equally  reliable  (.53  and  .55). 
The  mean  of  test  I  was  slightly  higher  (10.91)  than  the 
mean  of  test  II  (10.02)  indicating  that  it  was  slightly  less 
difficult.  The  responses  of  the  individual  students  to  the 
paired  problems  on  the  two  tests  were  found  to  be  consistent 
in  75  per  cent  of  the  cases.  From  this  study  it  seems  likely 
that  a  score  obtained  from  a  test  in  which  the  student  is 
asked  to  select  a  conclusion  for  a  stated  problem  will  be  a 
good  index  of  his  score  on  a  test  in  which  he  is  asked  to 
judge  a  given  conclusion.  Because  the  student  is  required 
to  do  less  reading  and  consequently  can  react  to  more  prob- 
lems in  a  given  unit  of  time,  the  type  of  problem  requiring 
a  judgment  about  a  single  conclusion  was  adopted. 

The  introduction  of  the  "uncertain"  response  required  a 
new  list  of  reasons  to  be  included  (reasons  1  to  10).  These 
ten  reasons  enable  the  student  who  chooses  the  uncertain 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  99 

response  to  explain  why  he  is  unable  to  agree  or  disagree 
with  the  conclusion.  Most  of  these  reasons  are  statements  of 
additional  factors  which  one  might  want  to  know  before 
making  a  decisive  judgment  about  the  conclusion.  They  have 
been  called  "control"  statements  in  the  problems  where  un- 
certainty is  considered  the  acceptable  response  to  the  con- 
clusion, and  "irrelevant  controls"  in  those  problems  where 
either  agreement  or  disagreement  with  the  underlined  con- 
clusion is  considered  the  acceptable  response.  It  was  also 
recognized  that  one  might  be  unable  to  agree  or  disagree 
with  the  conclusion  because  of  insufficient  knowledge  about 
the  problem.  To  provide  for  this,  statements  which  enable 
the  student  to  say  that  he  is  unable  to  make  a  decision  be- 
cause of  lack  of  knowledge  about,  or  experience  with,  this 
sort  of  situation  are  included  in  the  first  ten  reasons. 

The  student  who  chooses  the  uncertain  response  to  the 
problem  marks  only  those  of  the  first  ten  reasons  which  he 
selects  to  explain  his  uncertainty  and  then  proceeds  to  the 
next  problem.  The  student  who  agrees  or  disagrees  with  the 
underlined  conclusion  disregards  the  first  ten  reasons  and 
selects  his  supporting  statements  from  reasons  11  to  24.  The 
pattern  of  reasons  included  for  supporting  or  refuting  the 
conclusion  is  similar  to  that  described  for  Test  1.3a,  with 
two  exceptions.  These  are  the  inclusion  of  an  "acceptable" 
analogy  and  an  "acceptable"  authority  statement  in  each 
problem. 

Continuation  of  PROBLEM  I  (p.  95} 

Reasons  to  "be  used  if  you  agree  or  disagree: 

Tele-  11.  The  increasing  difficulty  of  stopping  objects 

ology  at  higher  speeds  is  a  part  of  nature's  plan  to 

keep  people  from  driving  too  fast. 

Wrong  12.  The  distance  required  to  bring  a  car  to  a 

Principle  stop  is  directly  proportional  to  the  speed  of 

the  car,  (Inconsistent  with  B) 


ioo        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Acceptable          13.  Many  drivers  have  learned  from  experience 
Practice  that  the  distance  required  to  bring  a  car  to  a 

stop  is  more  than  doubled  when  the  speed  is 
doubled.  ( Inconsistent  with  C ) 

Unacceptable      14.  Just  as  the  centrifugal  force  acting  on  a  car 
Analogy  gomg  around  a  curve  is  increased  four  times 

when  the  speed  is  doubled,  so  will  the  dis- 
tance required  to  stop  a  car  be  increased  four 
times  when  the  speed  is  doubled.  (Incon- 
sistent with  C ) 

Right  15.  When  brakes  are  applied  with  constant  pres- 

Principle  sure  there  is  constant  deceleration  of  the  car. 

Ridicule  16.  Any  student  of  physics  ought  to  know  that 

the  distance  required  to  stop  a  car  when  it  is 

traveling  at  60  miles  per  hour  is  more  than 

200  feet.  (Inconsistent  with  C) 

Assuming  17.  It  would  require  more  than  200  feet  for  the 

Conclusion  motorist  to  bring  his  car  to  a  stop  traveling 

60  m.p.h.  (Inconsistent  with  C) 

Wrong  18.  As  the  speed  of  a  car  increases,  the  mechan- 

Principle  ical  efficiency  of  the  brakes  decreases  consid- 

erably. ( Inconsistent  with  B ) 

Right  19.  When  the  speed  of  a  car  is  doubled,  the  dis- 

Principle  tance  required  to  bring  it  to  rest  is  increased 

four  times.  (Inconsistent  with  C) 

Unacceptable     20.  Automobile  mechanics  report  that  cars  trav- 
Authority  eling  at  60  miles  per  hour  cannot  be  brought 

to  a  stop  within  200  feet.  (Inconsistent  with 
C) 

Right  21.  The  distance  moved  while  coming  to  rest  by 

Principle  an  object  undergoing  constant  deceleration 

is  proportional  to  the  square  of  the  velocity. 
( Inconsistent  with  C ) 

Wrong  22.  When*  the  velocity  of  a  car  is  doubled,  the 

Principle  distance  required  to  bring  it  to  a  stop  may  be 

quickly  calculated  by  multiplying  the  veloc- 
ity by  four.  ( Inconsistent  with  C ) 

Right  23.  The  kinetic  energy  of  a  car  traveling  at  60 

Principle  miles  per  hour  is  four  times  that  of  the  same 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  101 

car  traveling  30  miles  an  hour.  (Inconsistent 
with  C) 

Acceptable          24.  Just  as  the  penetrating  distance  of  a  bullet  is 
Analogy  increased  four  times  when  its  velocity  is  dou- 

bled, so  is  the  stopping  distance  of  an  auto- 
mobile jncreased  four  times  when  its  speed  is 
doubled.  ( Inconsistent  with  C ) 

In  the  earlier  forms  of  the  test  all  analogy  statements  were 
formulated  as  unacceptable  reasons.  In  this  form  two  analogy 
statements  are  used  in  each  problem,  one  acceptable  as  a 
reason  for  supporting  the  conclusion,  the  other  unaccept- 
able. The  inclusion  of  acceptable  analogy  statements  makes 
it  possible  to  score  a  student  on  his  ability  to  distinguish  be- 
tween those  statements  of  situations  which  are  closely  analo- 
gous to  the  original  problem  and  those  which  seem  to  be 
but  actually  are  not  explainable  by  means  of  the  same  under- 
lying principles.  The  use  of  authority  and  practice  had  also 
been  restricted  in  earlier  test  forms  to  the  unacceptable  use 
of  such  reasons.  Because  in  life  students  are  often  forced 
through  exigencies  of  time  and  circumstance  to  use  author- 
ity, it  was  thought  desirable  to  include  in  this  test  two  such 
statements  in  each  problem,  one  of  which  was  judged  to 
be  acceptable  and  the  other  unacceptable.  If  students  then 
used  such  statements  in  justifying  their  reaction  to  the  con- 
clusion, one  would  be  able  to  distinguish  those  students  who 
used  authorities  discriminatingly  from  those  who  either  did 
not  cite  authorities  or  who  were  unable  to  distinguish  be- 
tween authorities  judged  acceptable  and  those  judged  un- 
acceptable. The  inclusion  of  these  statements  gives  students 
an  opportunity  to  reveal  whether  or  not  they  can  distinguish 
between  authorities — either  persons  or  institutions — which, 
because  of  training,  study,  experience,  etc.,  should  be  in  a 
position  to  give  reliable  information  about  the  problem,  and 
those  which  involve  the  use  of  false  credentials,  or  transfer 
of  prestige  from  one  field  to  another,  and  in  reality  offer  little 
reliable  evidence  about  the  problem. 


I 


I 


I     >s 

$3    U 

o  c 

*S 

00 
CN 

!   OO  CO  CO  OO  O 
CN        CN  CN 

o 
o 

0 

!  o 

i"1 

Tf 

O    (U 

53 

eo 

O* 
£ 

1> 
(N 

T-l                         T-« 

r- 
c\ 

0 

S 

r-i 

Jfffji     £ 
S'sf  gl 

^ 

un 

CN 

O  cO  cO  O  O  r-t  O 
T-.^        CNCN^^ 

0 

o 

0 

0 

CO 

CO 

sja'3| 

6 
£ 

"fr 

CN 

CO  CO  r-<  CN  ^  CO  CO 

s 

o 

<N 

cO 

1  fr|  • 

P4 

CN 
CN 

CN  t—  o  in  CN  CN  m 

c\ 

0 

!> 

CN 

3-s  8 

<  & 

0 

K 

CN 

CN  CN  O  CN  Tf  CN  00 

r-H 

SO 

o 

0, 

CO 

% 

'£ 

ti 

Cs 

rf  r-.  m  -5t  o  "=!•  m 

CO 

0 

r- 

01 

•a 
1 

1 

00 

\o  <N  in  oo  T-I  so  o 

TH                                              T-M 

\0 

T-" 

o 

cO 

CO 

w 

"o 

V-i 

rf 

SO 

m  \o  «n  \o  CN  Tt-  o 

m 

t—  i 

0 

CO 

T_( 

CM 

a 

o 

K 

lO 

vH 

m  \o  tn  co  \o  m  CN 

o 

so 

o 

V3 
CO 

tn 

§3 

ti 
^ 

cO 

•T—  1 

o  co  vo  o  o  co  r*- 
o  oo  c\  oo  o  oo  m 

o 

0 

0 

O 

o 

•<—  1 

^0 
00 

a 
*o 

tf 

CNI 

tn  T—  i  co  so  co  o  o 

-ri  CN  CM  r-4          TH  v-i 

^f- 

CO 

o 

•sf- 

CM 

00 

* 

o 

K 

r-i 

m  -«sf-  -^-  o  m  CN  cc 

v-<  CN  CN  CM        r-i  CN 

sO 

m 

0 

c> 

CN] 

o 

T-< 

S 

ti 
^ 

C^ 

•xf-  \o  -3-  TH  m  ^t  r- 
co  so  c\  m  co  iS*  co 

o 
o 

CN 
CN 

O 
O 

0 

m 

o 

ri 

00 

SO  O  CN  T->  t>  O  sO 
CN  «t  CO  CO        CN  <N 

in 

so 

CO 

CO 
CO 

•^t- 
t—i 

ti 

0 

K 

r- 

,_  ^  ^-  ^  o  r--  o 

CO  O  cO  so  CN  CN  f- 

so 

o 

xf 

0 

r-- 

VD 

CN 

Wt; 

experi- 
ence 

m 

T-< 

O 
CN 

O 

r-i 

-=1- 

86-9 

CN 

o 

Conclusions 

HQ2 

sis 

H;«2 

*_» 

DH  <D 

83 

U  ed 

< 

CN 

O  O  O  CM  ^  CM  CN 

oo  r~-  1>  ^  CN  M-  co 

sO 
CO 

o 

r-i 

VO 

C30 

CN 

-d- 

Column 
Numbers 

<pqOQ«feO 
+-* 
PJ 

u 

T3 

3 
& 

Maximum 
Possible 

i 

CO 

I 

8 

o 

GO 

rC 

bo 

2 

§ 
eft 

i* 

APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  103 

Summarization  and  Interpretation  of  the 
Scores  on  Form  1.36 

The  form  of  the  data  sheet  on  which  the  several  scores 
are  tabulated  and  summarized  is  presented  on  page  102.  A 
description  of  how  these  scores  are  obtained  from  the  test 
results  and  some  of  the  possible  interpretations  is  also  given 
below.  Some  of  die  experimental  procedures  used  for  arriv- 
ing at  this  form  of  summary  will  also  be  described. 

An  experimental  form  of  Form  l.Sb  was  given  to  415  stu- 
dents who  were  in  the  eleventh  and  twelfth  grades  of  two 
large  public  high  schools  (161  juniors  and  254  seniors).  The 
results  were  studied  in  an  attempt  to  discover  a  convenient 
and  meaningful  method  for  reporting  achievement.  An  item 
analysis  or  record  of  the  responses  of  students  to  each  item 
in  the  test  was  prepared.  This  was  studied  to  reveal  items 
which  seemed  to  need  revision  either  because  they  were  too 
difficult,  because  they  were  ambiguous,  or  for  some  other 
reason  did  not  elicit  the  expected  student  response.  A  score 
indicating  the  number  of  student  responses  on  each  separate 
kind  of  item  was  then  put  on  a  tentative  data  sheet.  Twenty- 
seven  scores  were  used  for  each  student  on  this  original  data 
sheet,  and  several  others  were  computed  from  these  in  an 
effort  to  find  those  which  gave  the  most  meaning  to  the 
results. 

The  interrelationships  of  the  scores  were  also  studied. 
From  these  preliminary  studies  the  final  form  was  made  and 
given  to  a  new  group  of  283  students  from  two  schools  in 
the  Eight-Year  Study.  These  students  included  127  from  the 
tenth  grade,  166  from  the  eleventh  grade,  and  40  from  the 
twelfth  grade.  These  results  were  used  for  the  statistical  data 
which  will  be  found  in  Table  4  of  Appendix  II. 

The  final  form  for  reporting  scores  determined  by  these 
means  contains  20  scores  for  each  student.  These  20  scores 
seem  to  give  all  of  the  essential  information  necessary  to 


io4        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

describe  the  differences  in  the  students'  ability  to  apply  prin- 
ciples in  the  manner  defined  and  measured  by  this  test.  An 
examination  of  the  data  sheet  (p.  102)  will  show  how  these 
scores  were  finally  recorded. 

The  scores  made  by  seven  students  in  the  eleventh  grade 
were  selected  for  purposes  of  illustration.  At  the  bottom  of 
the  sheet  the  maximum  possible  score,  highest  score,  lowest 
score,  and  group  median  is  recorded  for  each  column.  These 
were  computed  from  the  class  from  which  these  seven  stu- 
dents were  selected.  Some  of  the  scores  represent  actual 
number  of  responses,  while  others  are  computed  in  per  cent 
by  using  certain  of  the  scores  from  other  columns  as  bases. 

The  achievement  of  the  student  as  revealed  by  the  test 
may  be  analyzed  in  terms  of  five  related  questions.  The  first 
of  the^e  questions  is:  To  what  extent  can  the  student  reach 
valid  conclusions  involving  the  application  of  selected  prin- 
ciples of  science,  which  he  presumably  knows,  to  new  situa- 
tions? 

Columns1'7  Column  1  gives  the  number  of  conclusions  out  of  a 
1,2,3  possible  eight  which  the  student  marked  correctly. 
The  eight  correct  responses  were  distributed  among 
agreement  with  the  stated  conclusion  in  three  prob- 
lems, disagreement  with  the  stated  conclusion  in  three 
problems,  and  uncertainty  about  the  stated  conclu- 
sion in  the  remaining  two  problems.  Column  2  (too 
uncertain)  gives  the  number  of  conclusions  which 
the  student  marked  uncertain  when  the  correct  re- 
sponse was  either  "agree"  or  "disagree."  Column  3 
(too  certain)  gives  the  number  of  conclusions  which 
the  student  marked  either  agree  or  disagree  when  the 
correct  response  was  "uncertain."  When  his  scores  in 
columns  1,  2,  and  3  do  not  total  to  eight,  either  the 
student  marked  some  conclusions  agree  which  should 
have  been  marked  disagree,  or  he  marked  some  con- 

17  The  column  numbers  used  in  the  following  paragraphs  refer  to  the 
summary  sheet  (p.  102)  on  which  the  scores  are  recorded. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS 


105 


elusions  disagree  which  should  have  been  marked 
agree,  or  else  he  omitted  some  of  the  conclusions.  If 
we  denote  an  interchange  of  the  agree  and  disagree 
responses  by  the  term  "error  in  fact,"  the  following 
table  may  be  used  to  describe  the  complete  scoring  of 
the  student's  conclusions. 


\ 

\Key 
Student     \ 

Agree 

Uncertain 

Disagree 

Agree 

Acceptable 

Too  certain 

Error  in  fact 

Uncertain 

Too  uncertain 

Acceptable 

Too  uncertain 

Disagree 

Error  in  fact 

Too  certain 

Acceptable 

Thus  on  the  sample  data  sheet  student  A  marked  all  eight 
of  the  conclusions  in  agreement  with  the  key.  Student  D 
agreed  with  the  key  four  times,  marked  two  of  the  conclu- 
sions as  uncertain  when  he  should  either  have  agreed  or  dis- 
agreed with  them  according  to  the  key.  He  also  marked  one 
of  the  conclusions  which  was  keyed  as  uncertain  as  agree 
or  disagree.  Further  he  either  made  an  "error  in  fact"  by 
marking  an  agree  conclusion  as  disagree  or  a  disagree  con- 
clusion as  agree,  or  he  omitted  one  problem.  This  is  shown 
by  the  fact  that  his  score  on  conclusions  totals  seven  rather 
than  eight.  One  wrould  have  to  examine  his  paper  to  deter- 
mine whether  he  had  omitted  a  problem  or  made  an  "error 
in  fact,"  for  no  score  for  problems  omitted  is  recorded  on 
the  data  sheet. 

The  second  question  is:  How  does  the  student  explain  his 
uncertainty  when  he  marks  the  stated  conclusion  "uncer- 
tain"? 

Columns    Column  5  gives  the  number  of  statements  which  the 
5, 15, 16     student  used  to  express  either  a  lack  of  knowledge 


io6  ,       ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

about,  or  experience  with,  the  situation  described  in 
the  problem.  These  explain  why  he  marked  one  or 
more  of  the  stated  conclusions  "uncertain."  These 
statements  are  considered  neither  "right"  nor  "wrong" 
in  scoring  the  test.  Column  15  gives  the  number  of 
statements  which  express  a  desire  for  control  (see  the 
test  items  themselves  to  clarify  the  intended  meaning 
of  "Control").  They  also  are  used  by  the  student  to 
explain  why  he  marked  one  or  more  of  the  stated 
conclusions  "uncertain."  In  two  of  the  eight  problems 
there  is  actually  a  need  for  further  clarification  or  con- 
trol of  certain  factors  involved  in  the  problems.  Col- 
umn 16  gives  the  number  of  statements,  used  by  the 
student  in  these  two  uncertain  problems,  describing 
"controls"  which  are  considered  to  be  essential  addi- 
tional information  necessary  for  the  solution  of  the 
problem,  and  hence  are  valid  reasons  for  marking  the 
conclusion  uncertain.  In  the  remaining  six  problems, 
the  controls  are  considered  to  be  unnecessary  for  the 
solution  of  the  problem.  The  difference  between  the 
scores  in  columns  15  and  16  gives  the  number  of  un- 
necessary controls  marked  by  the  student.  It  should  be 
borne  in  mind  that  a  student  has  an  opportunity  to 
score  in  columns  5  and  15  when  he  marks  a  conclusion 
"uncertain,"  but  has  an  opportunity  to  score  in  column 
16  only  when  he  marks  the  conclusion  "uncertain"  in 
one  of  the  two  problems  where  the  uncertain  response 
is  regarded  as  the  correct  one. 

Student  D,  as  shown  in  column  5,  used  five  statements 
which  expressed  either  a  lack  of  knowledge  about,  or  ex- 
perience with,  those  problems  which  he  marked  as  uncer- 
tain. Generally  speaking,  a  high  score  in  column  5  will  be 
associated  with  a  low  score  in  column  1.  The  correlation 
between  column  1  and  column  5  is  — .34.  The  fact  that  he 
has  a  score  of  one  in  column  3  indicates  that  he  marked  one 
of  the  problems  which  was  keyed  as  uncertain  in  agreement 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  107 

with  the  key,  while  the  score  of  six  in  column  16  indicates 
that  he  must  have  judged  correctly  the  other  uncertain  prob- 
lem. His  score  of  two  in  column  2  would  account  for  the 
seven  unacceptable  control  statements  which  were  used  ( dif- 
ference between  columns  15  and  16)  for  in  these  two  prob-. 
lems  he  was  attempting  to  justify  an  uncertainty  through  the 
use  of  "control"  statements  when  according  to  the  key  he 
should  have  either  agreed  or  disagreed  with  the  conclusion. 
In  summary,  student  D  marked  four  of  the  conclusions  in 
agreement  with  the  key.  He  was  too  uncertain  in  two  of  the 
problems  and  too  certain  in  one.  He  either  omitted  one  prob- 
lem or  made  an  "error  in  f  act"  by  marking  an  agree  conclu- 
sion disagree  or  a  disagree  conclusion  as  agree.  He  used  five 
statements  to  indicate  that  he  did  not  understand  some  of 
the  problems  where  he  was  uncertain  about  the  conclusion, 
and  thirteen  statements  of  "controls/'  six  of  which  were  con- 
sidered to  be  acceptable. 

The  third  question  is:  To  what  extent  can  the  student  jus- 
tify logically  his  agreement  with,  his  uncertainty  about,  or 
his  disagreement  with  the  stated  conclusions? 

Columns  Column  7  gives  the  total  number  of  reasons  used  by 
7,  8,  9,  the  student  to  explain  his  decisions  about  the  stated 
27, 28  conclusions  ( excepting  those  which  express  a  lack  of 
knowledge  about,  or  experience  with,  the  situation 
described  in  the  problem  scored  in  column  5).  Stu- 
dents vary  a  great  deal  in  their  comprehensiveness, 
that  is,  in  the  extent  to  which  they  use  a  large  num- 
ber of  reasons  to  explain  their  decisions  about  the 
stated  conclusions.  The  meanings  of  every  subscore 
on  reasons  for  a  chosen  student  must  be  interpreted 
in  the  light  of  the  score  which  he  received  in  column 
7.  Column  8  gives  the  number  of  correct  or  acceptable 
reasons  used  by  the  student.  Column  9  gives  the  per 
cent  accuracy  of  the  student  in  supporting  his  decisions 
about  the  stated  conclusions  with  acceptable  reasons. 


io8        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Thus  the  score  in  column  9  is  computed  by  dividing 
the  score  in  column  8  by  the  score  in  column  7  and 
expressing  the  result  in  per  cent  This  score  helps  to 
"smooth  out"  differences  due  to  one  student's  using 
more  reasons  than  another. 

Column  27  gives  the  number  of  reasons  selected  by  the 
student  which  were  inconsistent  with  his  decisions 
about  the  stated  conclusions.  This  means  that  these 
reasons  actually  supported  responses  to  the  stated 
conclusions  which  were  contradictory  to  the  responses 
which  the  student  made.  Column  28  gives  the  per  cent 
of  the  student's  reasons  which  were  inconsistent  with 
his  decisions  about  the  stated  conclusions.  Thus  the 
score  in  column  28  is  computed  by  dividing  the  score 
in  column  27  by  the  score  in  column  7,  and  expressing 
the  result  in  per  cent. 

Student  B  used  61  reasons  to  explain  the  eight  conclu- 
sions which  he  marked,  while  student  G  used  74  ( column  7 
plus  column  5).  Both  of  these  students  used  a  great  many 
more  reasons  than  the  average  for  their  class.  Of  the  61  rea- 
sons used  by  student  B,  40,  or  66  per  cent,  were  keyed  as 
acceptable;  while  for  student  G  only  26,  or  37  per  cent,  of 
the  70  reasons  he  used  were  keyed  as  acceptable.  (The  scores 
in  column  5  are  considered  as  neither  right  nor  wrong,  and 
are  not  used  in  this  computation — they  are  only  used  to 
make  a  judgment  about  how  aware  the  student  was  of  his 
lack  of  knowledge.)  Student  B  used  only  reasons  which 
were  consistent  with  the  conclusions  he  had  chosen.  How- 
ever, 14,  or  20  per  cent,  of  the  70  reasons  used  by  student  G 
were  contradictory  to  the  conclusions  he  had  chosen.  This 
shows  that  student  G  was  not  as  discriminating  in  his  choice 
of  supporting  reasons  as  was  student  B. 

The  fourth  question  is:  What  kinds  of  reasons  does  the 
student  select  to  explain  his  decisions  about  the  stated  con- 
clusions? 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  109 

Columns  The  total  number  of  reasons  selected  by  the  student  to 
11, 15,  explain  the  conclusion  he  has  selected  (column  7)  is 
18,21,  broken  down  into  the  number  of  science  principles 
24,25  (column  II),  the  number  of  controls  (column  15), 
the  number  of  analogies  (column  IS),  the  number  of 
appeals  to  authority  or  common  practice  (column  21), 
and  the  number  of  times  ridicule,  teleology,  assuming 
the  conclusion  were  used  (column  24).  The  score  in 
column  15  has  been  discussed  above  in  connection 
with  the  second  question.  From  one  point  of  view  it 
may  be  desirable  to  rely  entirely  upon  the  use  of 
science  principles  to  explain  one's  agreements  or  dis- 
agreements with  the  stated  conclusions.  However,  in 
this  test,  the  test  directions  permit  the  discriminating 
use  of  "sound"  analogies,  "good"  authorities,  and  "de- 
pendable" common  practices  in  explaining  agreement 
or  disagreement  with  the  conclusions.  The  use  of  ridi- 
cule, assuming  the  conclusion,  or  teleology  is  unac- 
ceptable. Column  25  gives  the  per  cent  of  the  stu- 
dent's responses  which  could  be  classified  as  calling 
upon  ridicule,  assuming  the  conclusion,  or  teleology  to 
explain  his  agreement  or  disagreement  with  the  stated 
conclusions.  Thus  the  score  in  column  25  is  computed 
by  dividing  the  score  in  column  24  by  the  score  in 
column  7  and  expressing  the  result  in  per  cent. 

The  -fifth  question  is:  To  what  extent  does  the  student  dis- 
criminate between  acceptable  and  unacceptable  reasons  in 
the  various  categories? 

Columns    Column  12  gives  the  number  of  correct  statements  of 

12, 13,        science  principles  which  the  student  used  to  explain 

16, 19,        his  responses  to  the  stated  conclusions.  The  difference 

22  between  the  scores  in  columns  11  and  12  gives  the 

number  of  incorrect  or  technically  false  statements  of 

science  principles  used  by  the  student.  Column  13 

gives  the  per  cent  accuracy  of  the  student  in  his  use  of 

science  principles.  Thus  the  score  in  column  13  is  com- 


no        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

puted  by  dividing  the  score  in  column  12  by  the  score 
in  column  11  and  expressing  the  result  in  per  cent. 
The  scores  in  column  16  were  discussed  above  in  con- 
nection with  the  second  question.  Column  19  gives  the 
number  of  "sound"  analogies  used  by  the  student.  The 
difference  between  the  scores  in  columns  19  and  18 
gives  the  number  of  unacceptable  or  false  analogies 
selected  by  the  student.  Column  22  gives  the  number 
of  acceptable  appeals  to  authority  or  common  practice 
which  the  student  used  in  explaining  his  decisions 
about  the  stated  conclusions.  The  difference  between 
the  scores  in  columns  22  and  21  gives  the  number  of 
unacceptable  appeals  to  authority  or  common  practice 
selected  by  the  student. 

Student  C  used  a  total  of  34  reasons  to  justify  the  eight 
conclusions  he  selected.  Twenty-four  of  these  were  restricted 
to  principles,  of  which  23,  or  96  per  cent,  were  keyed  as  ac- 
ceptable. He  also  used  five  acceptable  analogies,  and  only 
one  statement  which  was  classified  as  unacceptable  because 
it  was  a  ridicule,  teleological,  or  assuming  the  conclusion 
type  of  reason.  He  did  not  use  authority  or  common  practice 
to  explain  his  choice  of  conclusions. 

In  making  interpretations  of  a  student's  scores,  all  of  his 
scores  on  reasons  should  be  judged  in  relation  to  his  score  in 
column  7.  Per  cent  scores  should  be  judged  in  relation  to  the 
number  of  items  on  which  the  per  cent  is  based.  That  is,  one 
out  of  two  may  have  quite  a  different  meaning  than  10  out 
of  20.  Reference  to  the  "maximum  possible,"  the  "lowest 
score"  and  "highest  score,"  and  the  group  median  ( all  given 
at  the  bottom  of  the  summary  sheet)  will  provide  a  frame  of 
reference  for  judging  the  student  with  respect  to  the  mem- 
bers of  his  own  class. 

Statistical  data,  including  the  reliability  of  each  score,  the 
intercorrelations  of  various  scores,  means,  and  standard  de- 
viations for  several  populations  will  be  found  in  the  Appen- 
dix II,  Tables  4  and  5. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  in 

If  students  have  been  placed  in  situations  in  the  classroom 
and  laboratory  where  resourcefulness,  adaptability,  and  se- 
lective thinking  have  been  essential  for  the  solution  of  prob- 
lems, and  if  the  emphasis  given  to  teaching  science  prin- 
ciples has  been  upon  their  applications  to  the  solution  of 
problems  involving  commonly  occurring  natural  phenomena 
rather  than  on  the  mastery  of  science  information  as  an  end 
in  itself,  then  students  should  have  little  difficulty  in  behav- 
ing in  the  manner  anticipated  by  this  test.  Such  students 
would  have  had  many  opportunities  to  apply  the  principles 
of  science  as  they  learned  them  to  a  number  of  situations  in 
the  laboratory  and  classroom,  and  would  have  been  encour- 
aged to  be  alert  for  similar  opportunities  for  application  as 
they  occur  outside  the  classroom. 

Experience  of  teachers  with  this  objective  seems  to  indi- 
cate that  the  objective  is  not  attained  through  any  one  par- 
ticular teaching  unit.  Rather  it  is  the  outcome  of  the  way  in 
which  emphasis  has  been  given  to  the  objective  with  all  the 
science  materials  taught  in  the  classroom  and  laboratory. 
Consequently,  teachers  may  wish  to  use  from  time  to  time 
during  the  semester  or  year  classroom  exercises  which  can 
be  used  for  checking  on  these  abilities  and  giving  a  tenta- 
tive appraisal  of  progress.  A  considerable  number  of  such 
exercises,  much  simpler  in  form  than  the  tests  of  Application 
of  Principles,  have  been  constructed  by  classroom  teachers 
in  summer  workshops. 

III.  APPLICATION  OF  PRINCIPLES  OF  LOGICAL  REASONING 
ANALYSIS  OF  THE  OBJECTIVE 

The* phrase  "logical  reasoning"  is  currently  used  to  de- 
scribe a  wide  variety  of  behaviors.  The  whole  process  of 
thinking  about  problems  in  an  orderly  scientific  fashion  is 
sometimes  called  logical  reasoning.  In  what  follows  the 
phrase  'logical  thinking"  will  be  restricted  to  mean  distin- 


ii2        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

guishing  between  conclusions  which  follow  logically  from 
given  assumptions  and  conclusions  which  do  not  follow  log- 
ically from  the  given  assumptions. 

The  intended  meaning  of  the  term  "principles  of  logical 
reasoning*'  may  be  illustrated  by  means  of  the  following  ex- 
amples of  such  principles: 

A.  Definitions:  Crucial  words  and  phrases  must  be  precisely  de- 
fined, and  a  changed  definition  may  produce  a  changed  con- 
clusion although  the  argument  from  each  definition  is  logical. 

B.  Indirect  Argument:  The  validity  of  an  indirect  argument  de- 
pends upon  whether  all  of  the  possibilities  have  been  con- 
sidered. If  there  are  three  and  only  three  possibilities  and 
one  of  them  must  happen,  then  if  two  of  the  possibilities  are 
shown  to  be  in  fact  impossible,  the  third  must  happen.  The 
conditions  necessary  for  the  logical  use  of  indirect  argument 
are  seldom  fulfilled  in  practice. 

C.  Argumentum  ad  Hominem:  An  attack  upon  certain  aspects  of 
a  person  or  institution,  even  though  justified,  is  not  sufficient 
to  prove  the  lack  of  all  merit  in  that  person  or  institution. 
This  covers  the  common  use  of  ridicule,  attack  on  motives, 
etc. 

D.  If-Then:  If  one  accepts  certain  premises,  then  one  must  ac- 
cept the  conclusions  which  follow  from  these  premises.  The 
if-then  principle  is  a  necessary  part  of  our  method  of  criticiz- 
ing generalizations,  questioning  assumptions,  etc. 

The  belief  that  the  study  of  certain  secondary  school  sub- 
jects develops  a  faculty  for  logical  reasoning  is  no  longer 
considered  tenable.  It  is,  however,  quite  different  to  claim 
that  properly  guided  contact  with  the  subject  matter  of  the 
secondary  school  curriculum  may  provide  experiences  which 
will  promote  logical  thinking  in  dealing  with  life  situations. 
Many  secondary  school  teachers  are  endeavoring  to  have 
their  students  recognize  patterns  for  logical  thinking  in  the 
organization  of  certain  bodies  of  subject  matter.  Sometimes 
the  teachers  make  a  conscious  effort  to  have  their  students 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  113 

apply  these  patterns  for  thinking  to  problems  which  arise 
in  connection  with  their  daily  experiences.  It  is  found  that 
principles  of  logical  thinking  may  be  stated  and  applied  to 
widely  different  kinds  of  situations.  In  the  light  of  the  fore- 
going explanation,  the  objective  under  consideration  may  be 
stated  in  general  terms  as  follows:  Students  in  secondary 
schools  should  acquire  the  ability  and  the  disposition  to 
apply  principles  of  logical  reasoning  in  dealing  with  their 
everyday  experiences. 

Several  more  specific  behaviors  which  might  be  chosen 
to  characterize  progress  toward  the  achievement  of  the  ob- 
jective are  listed  below: 

a.  Disposition  to  examine  the  logical  structure  of  the  argu- 
ments and  to  apply  principles  of  logical  reasoning  in  the 
study  of  these  arguments. 

b.  Ability  to  distinguish  between  conclusions  which  do  and 
ones  which  do  not  follow  logically  from  a  given  set  of 
assumptions. 

c.  Ability  to  isolate  the  significant  elements  in  the  logical 
structure  of  an  argument  as  shown  by  distinguishing  be- 
tween statements  of  ideas  which  are  relevant  and  state- 
ments of  ideas  which  are  irrelevant  for  explaining  why  a 
conclusion  follows  logically  from  given  assumptions. 

d.  Ability  to  recognize  the  application  of  a  logical  principle, 
whether  stated  in  general  terms  or  specifically  referred  to 
the  situation  in  question,  to  explain  why  a  conclusion  fol- 
lows logically  from  given  assumptions. 

No  effort  to  prepare  objective  tests  to  measure  the  disposi- 
tion of  students  to  apply  logical  principles  in  dealing  with 
their  everyday  experiences  was  made  by  the  Evaluation 
Staff.  A  test  devised  for  this  purpose  would  present  serious 
problems  of  validation.  The  difficulties  attendant  upon  the 
construction  and  administration  of  such  a  test  would  prob- 
ably be  greater  than  the  difficulties  of  observing  the  stu- 
dents directly.  Hence  the  efforts  to  measure  behaviors  re- 


ii4        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

lated  to  die  objective  have  been  directed  toward  measuring 
the  abilities  connected  with  applying  logical  principles 
rather  than  toward  measuring  the  disposition  to  apply  log- 
ical principles. 

The  following  discussion  deals  with  the  evaluation  of  the 
ability  to  judge  the  logical  structure  of  arguments  presented 
in  written  form.  This  ability  will  have  much  in  common  with 
the  ability  to  judge  the  logical  structure  of  arguments  pre- 
sented verbally,  pictorially,  or  otherwise.  Some  students  will 
have  occasion  in  later  life  to  write  essays,  prepare  speeches, 
and  the  like.  For  these  students  an  emphasis  upon  the  pro- 
ducer aspect  of  applying  logical  principles  is  easily  justified. 
Almost  all  students,  however,  will  read  editorials  and  adver- 
tisements, listen  to  political  speeches,  and  the  like.  Hence 
this  consumer  aspect  of  applying  logical  principles  (for  ex- 
ample, taking  note  of  the  need  for  definition  of  terms )  may 
be  considered  an  objective  of  general  education. 

THE  DEVELOPMENT  OF  EVALUATION  INSTRUMENTS 

Preliminary  Investigations 

The  first  step  toward  the  construction  of  a  test  for  this  ob- 
jective was  the  preparation  of  a  list  of  logical  principles 
which  secondary  school  students  might  be  expected  to  apply. 
A  few  principles  were  found  explicitly  stated  in  secondary 
school  textbooks  (particularly  of  geometry)  and  the  list  was 
extended  by  reference  to  books  on  logic.  From  this  list  the 
four  stated  above  were  selected.  Teachers  of  mathematics 
were  particularly  concerned  with  the  objective,  and  their  in- 
terests largely  determined  the  choice  which  was  made.  The 
principles  stated  relative  to  definitions,  indirect  (or  reduc- 
tio  ad  dbsurdum]  argument,  and  "if-then"  reasoning  play  an 
important  role  in  the  teaching  of  geometry.  The  fallacy  of 
argumentum  ad  hominem  was  included  because  the  claim 
has  so  frequently  been  made  that  the  study  of  geometry, 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  115 

which  as  usually  taught  offers  little  opportunity  for  this  sort 
of  error,  provides  a  standard  of  comparison  for  reasoning  in 
other  situations.  Consequently  if  the  acquaintance  with  this 
standard  is  functional,  it  should  enable  the  student  to  recog- 
nize the  fallacy. 

The  second  step  toward  the  construction  of  a  test  consisted 
of  a  search  of  current  newspapers,  magazines,  and  legal  case- 
books for  suitable  reasoning  situations.  These  sources  were 
chosen  because  of  the  emphasis  being  given  in  several  of  the 
schools  upon  reasoning  in  life  situations.  The  legal  cases 
which  formed  the  basis  of  several  test  problems  were  typical 
of  those  reported  almost  daily  by  the  press,  but  were  be- 
lieved to  be  of  greater  interest  to  students. 

Construction  of  Early  Short-Answer  Forms 

The  first  test  which  was  constructed  (Form  5.1)  described 
12  different  reasoning  situations  or  problems18  each  followed 
by  several  possible  conclusions.  The  student  was  asked  to 
select  one  of  the  conclusions  and  to  defend  it  by  selecting 
reasons  from  a  list  which  followed.  Each  logical  principle 
could  be  correctly  used  to  defend  a  conclusion  in  three  dif- 
ferent problems.  Included  in  each  list  of  reasons  were  state- 
ments of  several  of  the  principles  listed  above,  and  additional 
statements  which  were  irrelevant  or  otherwise  unsatisfactory 
as  reasons.  The  occurrence  of  several  of  the  principles  in 
each  list  of  reasons  required  the  student  to  discriminate 
among  them  even  if  the  relatively  abstract  form  of  state- 
ment helped  him  to  identify  them. 

In  order  to  discover  what  sort  of  statements  other  than 
principles  should  be  included  among  the  reasons,  a  form  was 
prepared  which  contained  only  the  situations  and  the  sev- 
eral alternative  conclusions.  Four  classes  of  tenth  and  elev- 
enth grade  students  took  this  test  and  wrote  out  their  rea- 
sons in  essay  form.  Many  of  the  reasons  ultimately  used  in 

18  For  a  similar  problem  taken  from  a  later  form,  see  p.  119. 


u6        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

the  short-answer  form  were  taken  with  practically  no  changes 
from  student  papers.  This  preliminary  investigation  also 
served  to  suggest  revisions  in  the  statement  of  the  situations 
and  the  conclusions. 

The  scoring  plan  finally  adopted  for  Form  5.1  allowed  two 
points  for  each  correct  conclusion,  one  point  for  each  correct 
reason,  and  deducted  one  point  for  each  incorrect  reason. 
A  score  was  given  indicating  achievement  relative  to  each 
principle  separately,  and  also  a  total  score. 

The  next  form  (5.11)  of  the  Application  of  Principles  of 
Logical  Reasoning  test  incorporated  several  changes.  It  was 
noted  that  the  statements  of  logical  principles  in  Form  5.1 
were  of  two  kinds.  Some  of  the  statements  referred  directly 
to  the  situation  under  consideration  and  others  were  general 
statements  of  logical  principles.  A  pattern  of  statements  was 
built  into  Form  5.11  with  a  view  to  securing  separate  scores 
on  ability  to  recognize  the  application  of  principles  which 
were  stated  specifically  and  principles  which  were  stated 
generally.  In  each  problem  there  were  four  specific  state- 
ments of  principles,  four  general  statements  of  principles, 
and  two  extraneous  statements  including  in  the  test  as  a 
whole  statements  of  personal  opinion,  prejudice,  reliance 
upon  authority,  and  the  like.  Of  the  four  specific  and  four 
general  statements  in  each  problem,  one  of  the  specific  and 
one  of  the  general  statements  were  relevant  in  the  sense  that 
they  explained  why  the  correct  conclusion  followed  logically 
from  the  given  assumptions.  In  a  sense  the  cards  were  stacked 
against  the  student  by  providing  three  opportunities  to  use 
an  irrelevant  statement  of  a  principle  and  one  opportunity  to 
use  an  extraneous  statement  for  each  opportunity  to  use  a 
relevant  statement  of  a  principle. 

The  four  principles  (definition  of  terms,  indirect  argu- 
ment, argumentum  ad  hominem,  and  if-then)  tested  in  Form 
5.1  were  again  tested  in  Form  5.11.  In  addition,  a  principle 
relative  to  sampling  ("A  sample  does  not  necessarily  repre- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  117 

sent  the  population  from  which  it  was  drawn" )  was  included 
in  Form  5.11.  Three  problems  on  each  principle  were  given, 
or  15  problems  in  all. 

When  the  test  results  were  summarized,  an  attempt  was 
made  to  score  the  number  of  correct  conclusions  (out  of  a 
possible  three )  on  each  principle  and  the  number  of  correct 
(out  of  a  possible  six)  and  incorrect  (out  of  a  possible 
eighteen)  uses  of  statements  of  each  of  the  given  principles. 
These  scores  were  found  to  be  too  unreliable  to  be  useful 
in  practice.  Moreover,  the  attempt  to  summarize  separately 
the  right  and  wrong  uses  of  specific  and  of  general  state- 
ments of  principles  did  not  yield  results  of  practical  signifi- 
cance. It  was  found  that  the  scores  on  specific  statements 
were  highly  correlated  with  the  scores  on  general  statements. 

In  the  final  analysis  the  scoring  of  Form  5.11  yielded  six 
useful  scores.  These  were  scores  on  numbers  of  right  and 
wrong  conclusions,  right  and  wrong  total  reasons,  extrane- 
ous reasons,  and  general  accuracy.  The  general  accuracy 
score  was  computed  as  twice  the  total  number  of  right  re- 
sponses (conclusions  and  reasons)  minus  the  total  number 
of  wrong  responses  (conclusions  and  reasons).  This  score 
was  highly  correlated  with  each  of  the  other  scores,  and  a 
reliability  coefficient  of  .94  was  obtained  for  a  population  of 
216  students. 

A  consideration  of  the  desirable  improvements  to  be  made 
in  revising  this  test  led  to  several  suggestions.  Form  5.11  was 
a  long  test  and  was  made  inefficient  by  the  large  proportion 
of  wrong  statements.  The  student  who  responded  correctly 
to  the  test  problems  made  an  explicit  response  to  only  one 
statement  in  five.  The  assumption  that  by  refraining  from 
marking  a  statement  a  student  was  making  an  explicit  re- 
sponse (e.g.,  "the  statement  is  irrelevant")  was  not  thought 
to  be  tenable.  Thus  the  student  who  refrained  from  marking 
a  statement  might  have  done  so  because  he  did  not  under- 
stand the  statement  or  did  not  take  time  to  consider  it  fully 


u8        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Hence  a  tentative  revision  of  Form  5.11  was  made  and  given 
to  60  students.  In  this  form,  S.lla,  the  students  were  asked 
to  respond  to  every  statement  and  to  decide  whether  it  was 
(1)  specific  and  relevant,  (2)  specific  and  irrelevant,  (3) 
general  and  relevant,  (4)  general  and  irrelevant.  This  at- 
tempt to  get  at  possible  differences  in  the  ability  of  students 
to  deal  with  specific  and  general  statements  of  logical  prin- 
ciples was  again  not  successful.  No  very  meaningful  inter- 
pretations of  difference  between  the  ability  to  deal  with 
specific  and  the  ability  to  deal  with  general  statements  could 
be  made.  However,  when  scored  in  terms  of  relevance  alone, 
for  example,  total  number  of  irrelevant  statements  classified 
under  (2)  or  (4)  above,  Form  S.lla  yielded  very  promising 
results.  With  only  eight  problems  based  on  four  principles, 
it  was  possible  to  secure  a  number  of  diagnostic  scores  in- 
cluding scores  on  each  of  the  principles  separately.  For  this 
latter  purpose  the  method  formerly  used  for  scoring  the 
separate  principles  on  Form  5.11  was  changed.  Rather  than 
counting  the  number  of  correct  and  incorrect  uses  of  each 
principle  throughout  the  test,  the  plan  was  now  adopted  of 
scoring  two  intact  problems  both  directed  at  the  definition 
principle  to  secure  a  score  on  accuracy  with  definition,  and 
similarly  with  the  other  principles.  This  plan  was  later  used 
in  summarizing  the  results  on  the  final  test,  Form  5.12.  The 
scoring  of  this  test  will  be  discussed  in  some  detail  in  what 
follows. 

Structure  of  the  Application  of 

Principles  of  Logical  Reasoning  Test,  Form  5.12 

It  has  been  found  that  Form  5.12  of  the  Logical  Reason- 
ing test  provides  a  better  analysis  of  the  students*  abilities 
in  relation  to  the  objective  than  did  previous  forms.  More- 
over, with  the  exception  of  the  orginal  Form  5.1,  this  form 
is  considerably  shorter  than  previous  forms  and  somewhat 
simpler  from  the  standpoint  of  the  directions  to  the  student. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  119 

A  study  of  the  following  explanation  of  the  structure  of  the 
test  problems  in  comparison  with  the  sample  test  problem 
presented  below  will  serve  to  clarify  the  objective  further 
and  to  indicate  the  extent  to  which  it  is  measured  by  the 
test.  A  list  of  the  responses  accepted  as  correct  by  a  jury  of 
competent  persons  (i.e.,  a  test  key)  is  given  in  the  margin. 

Problem  TV 

In  January,  1940,  Commissioner  K.  M.  Landis  submitted  a  plan 
to  give  financial  aid  to  minor  league  baseball  teams  to  restore 
fair  competition  by  preventing  certain  major  league  teams  from 
controlling  the  supply  of  players.  Several  leaders  in  the  baseball 
world  objected  to  this  plan;  some  declared  that  Landis  should 
enforce  the  rules  governing  the  operation  of  baseball  teams,  but 
should  not  make  interpretations  which  would  change  the  in- 
tended meaning  of  the  rules  set  up  by  the  proper  committees. 

Larry  MacPhail,  president  of  the  Brooklyn  Dodgers,  speaking  at 
a  dinner  in  Boston,  expressed  grave  concern  over  the  situation. 
The  following  statements  are  quoted  from  his  remarks:  "In  the 
matter  of  Landis  versus  the  present  system,  he  sits  as  prosecutor, 
judge,  and  jury,  and  there  is  no  appeal.  If  baseball  is  to  be  dom- 
inated by  any  selfish  group,  it  won't  be  long  before  professional 
football  or  some  other  sport  will  replace  baseball  as  the  great 
national  game,  and  none  of  us  want  that." 

Directions:  Examine  the  conclusions  given  below.  If  by  "us"  Mr. 
MacPhail  means  all  persons  at  the  dinner,  and  if  they  accept  his 
remarks  as  true,  which  one  of  the  conclusions  do  you  think  is 
justified? 

Conclusions 

A.  Logical  persons  at  the  dinner  will  conclude  that  »they  do 
not  want  baseball  to  be  dominated  by  a  selfish  group. 

B.  Logical  persons  at  the  dinner  will  conclude  that,  if  the 
domination  of  baseball  by  a  selfish  group  is  prevented, 
baseball  will  not  be  replaced  as  the  great  national  game. 

C.  It  is  impossible  to  say  what  a  logical  person  at  the  dinner 
will  conclude. 


i2o        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

A:  Statements  which  explain  why  your  conclu- 
sion is  logical. 
Mark  in  column    B:  Statements  which  do  not  explain  why  your 

conclusion  is  logical. 

C:  Statements  about  which  you  are  unable  to 
decide. 

Statements 

A  1.  Since  we  assumed  that  Mr.  MacPhail  referred  to  all  per- 
sons present  at  the  dinner  when  he  said  "none  of  us,"  and 
that  those  present  accepted  his  statements  as  true,  the 
conclusion  which  we  reached  follows  logically. 

B  2.  Logical  persons  at  the  dinner  may  agree  or  disagree  with 
Mr.  MacPhail. 

B  3.  Without  knowing  the  assumptions  of  logical  persons,  we 
cannot  predict  their  conclusions. 

A  4.  If  no  person  at  the  dinner  wants  professional  football  or 
some  other  game  to  replace  baseball  as  the  great  national 
game,  then  the  logical  ones  cannot  want  baseball  to  be 
dominated  by  a  selfish  group. 

A  5.  If  we  accept  the  assumptions  on  which  an  argument  is 
based,  then,  to  be  logical,  we  must  accept  the  conclu- 
sions which  follow  from  them. 

B  6.  Sometimes  the  meaning  of  a  word  or  phrase  used  in  an 
argument  must  be  carefully  defined  before  any  logical 
conclusion  can  be  reached. 

B  7.  A  changed  definition  may  lead  to  a  changed  conclusion 
even  though  the  argument  from  each  definition  is  logical. 

B  8.  If  the  domination  of  baseball  by  a  selfish  group  results 
in  some  other  sport  replacing  baseball,  then,  if  such 
selfish  domination  is  prevented,  baseball  will  not  be  re- 
placed. 

B  9.  Mr.  MacPhail  considered  every  possibility — either  base- 
ball will  or  will  not  be  replaced  as  the  great  national 
game — and  thus  made  a  sound  indirect  argument. 

A  10,  If  a  conclusion  follows  logically  from  certain  assump- 
tions, then  one  must  accept  the  conclusion  or  reject  the 
assumptions. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  121 

B  11.  If  one  removes  the  fundamental  cause  for  other  games 
replacing  baseball,  baseball  will  not  be  replaced  as  the 
great  national  game. 

B  12.  The  soundness  of  an  indirect  argument  depends  upon 
whether  all  of  the  possibilities  have  been  considered. 

In  each  problem  the  student  is  given  a  paragraph,  three 
conclusions,  and  twelve  statements.  He  is  directed  to  read 
the  paragraph  carefully  and  to  choose  the  one  of  the  three 
conclusions  which  he  thinks  is  justified  by  the  paragraph. 
In  the  test  as  a  whole  the  student  judges  the  logical  ap- 
propriateness of  the  conclusions  drawn  in  eight  different 
situations.  In  two  of  these  the  definition  principle  operates; 
in  two  others  the  indirect  argument  principle  operates;  in 
two  others  the  argumentum  ad  hominem  principle  operates; 
and  in  the  remaining  two  the  if-then  principle  operates.  It 
should  be  noted  that  the  number  of  possible  correct  conclu- 
sions is  small,  especially  if  considered  with  respect  to  the 
opportunity  to  use  the  correct  principles  separately.  Conse- 
quently the  major  emphasis  is  placed  upon  the  students'  re- 
actions to  the  statements  which  follow  the  conclusions  in 
each  test  problem. 

The  statements  offered  to  the  students  are  of  several  kinds, 
including: 

a.  General  or  abstract  statements  of  the  logical  principle 
involved  in  that  particular  test  situation. 

b.  Specific  statements  of  the  logical  principle  involved 
in  the  particular  test  situation. 

c.  General  or  specific  statements  of  logical  principles 
not  pertinent  to  the  particular  test  situation,  state- 
ments which  appeal  to  authority,  statements  of  per- 
sonal opinion,  or  statements  which   are  otherwise 
irrelevant. 

The  student  is  directed  to  mark  each  statement  in  one  of 
three  ways  according  as  it  is: 


122        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

a.  Relevant  for  explaining  why  his  conclusion  is  logical. 

b.  Irrelevant  for  explaining  why  his  conclusion  is  log- 
ical. 

c.  Not  sufficiently  meaningful  to  him  to  permit  a  deci- 

sion. 

In  the  test  as  a  whole  the  student  judges  the  relevance  of 
96  statements,  and  is  given  the  opportunity  to  reveal  his  lack 
of  understanding  of  any  of  these  statements.  The  variety  of 
the  statements  including  specific  and  general  statements  of 
the  principles,  statements  of  authority,  personal  opinion, 
prejudice  and  the  like  provides  an  opportunity  to  make 
many  of  the  common  logical  errors.  The  sample  of  state- 
ments in  the  test  includes  36  relevant  and  60  irrelevant  state- 
ments. Of  the  36  relevant  statements,  16  are  specific  and  20 
are  general.  Of  the  60  irrelevant  statements,  20  are  general 
statements  of  the  four  principles  of  the  test,  19  are  specific 
statements  of  these  principles,  and  21  are  specific  statements 
of  the  other  kinds  mentioned  above. 

Summarization  and  Interpretation  of  the  Scores  on  Form  5.12 

During  the  experimental  stages  of  Form  5.12,  the  test  re- 
sults for  a  sample  population  of  351  students  were  studied 
intensively  in  an  attempt  to  discover  the  most  convenient 
and  most  meaningful  form  for  reporting  the  results.  An  item 
analysis  or  record  of  the  responses  of  all  students  to  each 
item  on  the  test  was  prepared.  The  individual  student  papers 
were  scored  by  entering  the  number  of  responses  of  each 
separate  kind  on  a  tentative  data  sheet.  Fourteen  scores  were 
summarized  for  each  student,  and  more  than  eight  additional 
scores  were  considered  during  the  study.  Certain  important 
scores  were  selected  and  studied  with  reference  to  the  item 
analysis  in  an  effort  to  see  more  clearly  the  relationships  be- 
tween each  of  these  scores  and  the  responses  of  students  to 
individual  test  items. 
The  351  students  comprised  12  separate  classes  in  four 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  123 

public  schools.  Certain  facts  about  the  backgrounds  of  these- 
different  classes  were  known.  The  responses  of  each  class  to 
the  individual  test  items  (taken  from  the  item  analysis),  and 
the  median  scores  of  each  class  (taken  from  the  data  sheets), 
were  studied  in  an  attempt  to  discover  the  degree  of  agree- 
ment or  disagreement  of  these  results  with  the  known  facts 
about  the  various  classes  of  students.  The  results  of  this  study 
indicated  that  the  students  who  secured  good  total  scores 
were  also  the  students  who  did  well  with  the  individual  test 
items.  Moreover,  it  was  found  that  the  classes  which  had  had 
most  contact  in  school  with  the  logical  reasoning  objective 
tended  to  secure  the  highest  scores  on  the  test. 

Certain  correlation  coefficients  between  the  scores  which 
had  been  summarized  were  computed.  It  was  found  possible 
to  reduce  the  number  of  scores  on  the  data  sheet  to  11  with- 
out an  appreciable  loss  of  information.  It  was  again  found 
that  separating  the  responses  to  specific  statements  of  prin- 
ciples from  the  responses  to  general  statements  of  principles 
did  not  yield  results  of  practical  significance.  Several  at- 
tempts were  made  to  secure  a  general  accuracy  score  which 
would  serve  as  a  good  over-all  index  of  behaviors  involved 
in  the  application  of  principles  of  logical  reasoning.  For  ex- 
ample, the  total  number  of  correct  responses  to  statements 
on  the  test,  and  twice  the  number  of  relevant  statements 
recognized  as  such,  less  the  number  of  irrelevant  statements 
judged  to  be  relevant,  were  tried.  It  was  found  that  all  of 
these  indices  were  highly  correlated  with  one  or  none  of 
the  simpler  scores  obtained  Ly  counting  the  numbers  of  re- 
sponses of  a  certain  kind,  and  that  the  indices  were  no  more 
reliable  than  the  simpler  scores.  Hence  no  score  in  general 
accuracy  was  retained.  Because  the  number  of  irrelevant 
statements  on  the  test  is  larger  than  the  number  of  relevant 
statements  (60  as  compared  with  36),  the  score  on  irrele- 
vant statements  recognized  as  such  is  more  reliable  than  the 
score  on  relevant  statements  recognized  as  such  ( .88  as  com- 


i24        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

pared  with  .72).  The  correlation  studies  indicated  that  if  a 
single  index  for  the  abilities  measured  by  this  test  is  desired, 
the  score  on  the  number  of  irrelevant  statements  judged  to 
be  irrelevant  is  perhaps  the  best  such  index  among  the  11 
scores  summarized  on  the  data  sheet  which  was  finally 
adopted.19 

Scores  on  this  test  may  be  interpreted  in  terms  of  the  an- 
swers to  the  following  three  questions: 

1.  To  what  extent  can  the  pupil  reach  logical  conclu- 
sions in  situations  which  may  involve  his  attitudes 
and  prejudices? 

2.  To  what  extent  can  the  pupil  justify  his  conclusions 
in  terms  of  certain  principles  of  logical  reasoning? 

3.  How  well  can  the  pupil  apply  each  of  the  four  prin- 
ciples of  logical  reasoning? 

By  study  of  the  various  scores  reported  on  the  data  sheet, 
the  teacher  may  obtain  evidence  relative  to  each  of  these 
questions.  Different  patterns  of  behavior  analogous  to  those 
described  for  the  test  on  Interpretation  of  Data  are  identi- 
fiable in  terms  of  the  relation  of  the  separate  scores  to  the 
group  averages. 

VALIDITY  AND  RELIABILITY  OF  FORM  5.12 

The  construction  of  Form  5.12  of  the  Logical  Reasoning 
test  was  undertaken  in  the  light  of  two  kinds  of  previous 
experience.  The  previous  forms  of  the  test  had  been  given 
to  selected  groups  of  students  and  the  test  results  carefully 
studied.  The  criticisms  of  certain  teachers  who  were  endeav- 
oring to  promote  the  logical  reasoning  objective  were  avail- 
able. Sometimes  these  teachers  based  their  criticisms  upon 

19  This  data  sheet  is  similar  to  those  presented  above  for  the  tests  on 
Interpretation  of  Data  and  Application  of  Principles  of  Science.  For  a 
sample  copy  and  detailed  description  of  the  interpretation  of  scores  from 
this  test  the  reader  is  referred  to  the  manual,  obtainable  from  the  Progres- 
sive Education  Association. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  125 

their  experiences  in  administering  the  tests  and  interpreting 
the  test  results.  Sometimes  these  teachers  had  met  in  groups 
for  die  purpose  of  studying  and  criticizing  the  tests.  Both 
the  studies  of  test  results  and  the  suggestions  made  by  teach- 
ers as  individuals  or  as  discussion  groups  helped  the  test 
makers  with  the  construction  of  test  Form  5.12.  In  par- 
ticular, the  problem  situations  were  chosen  with  regard  for 
the  interests  of  secondary  school  students.  Most  of  the  prob- 
lem situations  in  this  test  form  are  taken  directly  from  state- 
ments found  in  the  feature  articles  and  in  the  editorial  pages 
in  newspapers.  These  quotations  were  edited  to  some  extent 
to  avoid  the  introduction  of  extraneous  factors  such  as  un- 
necessary vocabulary  difficulties,  lack  of  clear  antecedents 
for  pronouns,  and  the  like.  The  statements  regarding  the 
logical  structure  of  the  paragraphs  which  set  forth  the  prob- 
lem situations  were  carefully  chosen  in  an  effort  to  make 
them  typical  of  the  kinds  of  statements  which  students  com- 
monly make  when  they  are  discussing  the  logical  structure 
of  such  paragraphs.  Several  readers  went  over  each  test 
problem  carefully  in  an  attempt  to  discover  loopholes  in  its 
logical  structure.  Although  it  is  probably  quite  impossible 
to  construct  a  lifelike  argument  to  illustrate  just  one  prin- 
ciple of  logical  reasoning,  and  express  this  argument  with- 
out ambiguity  in  words,  an  effort  was  made  to  approach 
this  ideal  in  the  test  situations  included  in  Form  5.12  of  the 
logical  reasoning  test. 

The  studies  upon  which  the  scoring  of  Form  5.12  of  the 
Logical  Reasoning  test  was  based  were  described  above.  It 
is  important  to  note  that  even  a  carefully  constructed  test, 
which  actually  provides  opportunities  for  the  behaviors  in 
terms  of  which  the  objective  is  defined,  may  become  invalid 
if  the  system  of  scoring  adopted  does  not  yield  scores  which 
present  a  true  picture  of  the  significant  behaviors  called  forth 
by  the  test.  Hence  it  should  be  noted  that  careful  attention 
was  given  to  the  mode  of  scoring  of  Form  5.12  of  the  logical 


126        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

reasoning  test.  When  conditions  o£  administration  are  ap- 
propriate and  when  the  results  are  interpreted  by  a  person 
who  is  familiar  with  the  objective  and  the  structure  o£  the 
test,  Form  5.12  provides  a  measure  of  a  range  of  significant 
behaviors  related  to  the  logical  reasoning  objective. 

For  the  purpose  of  statistical  analysis,  the  scores  of  351 
students,  of  whom  292  were  finishing  grade  ten,  28  were  in 
grade  eleven,  and  31  in  grade  twelve,  were  used.  These 
students  were  all  attending  public  high  schools  when  tested 
and  composed  nine  classes  in  grade  ten,  one  class  in  grade 
eleven,  and  one  class  in  grade  twelve.  The  statistical  data 
presented  in  Appendix  II  on  reliability,  intercorrelations  of 
scores,  and  so  forth,  Table  6,  are  based  upon  a  study  of 
these  351  students.  Within  certain  definite  limitations  these 
data  would  apply  to  other  groups  of  students  in  the  tenth, 
eleventh,  and  twelfth  grades. 

The  statistical  constants  presented  will  provide  enough 
basic  information  to  enable  the  teacher  trained  in  statistics 
to  study  the  significance  of  changes  in  the  mean  scores  of 
a  class  or  in  the  scores  of  an  individual  student. 

Form  5.12  of  the  test  on  the  Application  of  Certain  Prin- 
ciples of  Logical  Reasoning  is  recommended  only  for  classes 
where  conscious  attention  has  been  directed  toward  logical 
reasoning.  Otherwise,  the  students  are  apt  to  wonder  why 
they  should  attempt  to  reach  logical  conclusions  which  are 
sometimes  contrary  to  their  "better  judgments."  The  judg- 
ment of  the  teacher  as  to  the  readiness  of  his  class  for  prob- 
lems of  the  type  included  in  the  test  is  for  this  reason  very 
important. 

IV.  THE  NATURE  OF  PROOF 

ANALYSIS  OF  THE  OBJECTIVE 

In  the  past,  teachers  of  several  of  the  subject  fields  in 
the  secondary  school  curriculum  have  been  concerned  with 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  127 

particular  aspects  of  "proof."  For  example,  one  of  the  objec- 
tives for  courses  in  demonstrative  geometry  is  to  develop  an 
understanding  of  the  meaning  of  proof,  and  students  in  such 
courses  have  been  expected  to  learn  to  prove  theorems  of 
geometry.  Teachers  of  courses  in  which  oral  and  written  ex- 
pression is  emphasized  have  also  been  concerned  with  cer- 
tain aspects  of  proof.  Logical  organization  has  been  sought 
in  themes  and  speeches.  Courses  in  science  have  relied  heav- 
ily upon  laboratory  experiments  to  "prove"  certain  laws,  and 
students  have  been  expected  to  learn  to  cite  experimental 
evidence  for  their  conclusions.  Similarly,  teachers  of  other 
subject-matter  fields  have  objectives  related  to  the  concept 
of  proof,  in  each  case  with  connotations  rather  specific  to 
their  own  field.  The  following  paragraphs  present  a  gener- 
alized definition  of  an  objective  which  has  come  to  be  called 
"the  nature  of  proof." 

Both  children  and  adults  in  our  society  are  constantly  bom- 
barded with  "proofs";  i.e.,  by  arguments  designed  to  con- 
vince them  that  they  should  act  in  certain  ways  or  should 
believe  in  certain  things.  The  whole  field  of  advertising 
directs  its  efforts  toward  convincing  people  to  act  in  cer- 
tain ways.  Children  of  elementary  school  age  are  persuaded 
by  a  radio  announcer  to  ask  mother  to  buy  a  certain  brand 
of  breakfast  food.  Newspapers  and  magazines  contain  car- 
toons which  set  forth  the  dramatic  stories  of  lives  set  right 
by  buying  and  using  a  different  brand  of  soap.  The  editorial 
pages  encourage  readers  to  adopt  one  of  several  possible 
courses  of  action.  Even  the  news  articles  in  the  daily  papers 
are  likely  to  reflect  the  policy  and  convictions  of  the  man- 
agement, and  hence  may  be  said  to  be  one  of  the  kinds  of 
"proofs"  with  which  people  are  bombarded.  The  books  and 
magazines  they  read,  the  plays  and  movies  they  see,  the  lec- 
tures and  radio  talks  they  hear,  and  the  conversations  they 
have  with  their  associates,  all  play  a  part  in  forming  the 
convictions  upon  which  the  actions  of  people  are  based. 


128        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

In  particular,  students  in  secondary  schools  react  to  the 
proofs  which  they  meet  in  their  daily  experiences.  Author- 
ities on  the  secondary  school  curriculum  and  classroom 
teachers  have  expressed  concern  with  the  problem  of  guid- 
ing the  reactions  of  the  students  to  these  proofs.  This  concern 
has  led  many  teachers  to  attempt  to  have  students  be- 
come critical  of  proofs  and  to  have  students  acquire  the  abil- 
ities needed  for  analyzing  proofs.  It  would  be  ineffective  to 
have  students  become  critical  of  the  proofs  which  they  en- 
counter unless  the  students  also  acquired  some  of  the  abil- 
ities needed  in  analyzing  proofs.  On  the  other  hand,  the 
ability  to  analyze  proofs  is  not  likely  to  function  unless  there 
is  a  disposition  to  analyze  proofs  when  the  need  for  such 
analysis  arises.  Hence  the  nature  of  proof  objective  should 
include  the  ability  to  judge  proofs,  and  also  the  disposition 
to  apply  this  ability  on  appropriate  occasions. 

It  should  be  noted  explicitly  that  any  of  the  physical 
senses  may  be  the  medium  for  arriving  at  proofs.  Touch, 
taste,  or  smell  may  be  the  basis  for  simple  proofs.  The  ques- 
tion, "Are  the  potatoes  salty?"  is  easily  answered;  the  method 
is  to  taste  them.  Sometimes  visual  impressions  also  provide 
simple  and  direct  proofs,  but  often  these  impressions  involve 
more  subtle  factors.  The  hand  may  be  quicker  than  the  eye; 
the  story  told  by  the  moving  picture  may  create  certain  im- 
pressions which  lead  up  to  an  intended  conclusion  through 
a  series  of  inferences.  Verbal  presentations  such  as  speeches 
and  debates  are  also  common  vehicles  for  proof.  The  writ- 
ten "proofs"  which  are  so  frequently  met  in  daily  life  have 
much  in  common  with  proofs  in  the  other  forms.  It  is  with 
arguments  or  proofs  presented  in  written  form  that  we  shall 
be  chiefly  concerned  in  this  chapter. 

One  of  the  important  characteristics  of  proofs  should  be 
noted  immediately.  Some  proofs  proceed  mostly  from  stated 
opinions  or  convictions.  Other  proofs  are  based  in  part  upon 
data  derived  from  experiments  or  investigations.  Both  of 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  129 

these  kinds  of  proofs  will  involve  certain  basic  assumptions 
which  may  be  more  or  less  tenable.  Whatever  the  subject 
matter  with  which  a  proof  deals,  and  whatever  the  form  of 
presentation  in  which  the  proof  appears,  the  location  and 
appraisal  of  the  basic  assumptions  upon  which  the  sound- 
ness of  the  proof  depends  becomes  a  fundamental  ability 
connected  with  analyzing  proofs. 

In  the  light  of  the  preceding  remarks,  some  of  the  be- 
haviors which  might  be  chosen  to  characterize  progress  to- 
ward the  achievement  of  the  nature  of  proof  objective  are 
listed  below: 

a.  Disposition  to  analyze  proofs  critically. 

b.  Ability  to  recognize  the  basic  assumptions  upon  which  a 
conclusion  depends,  and  to  see  the  logical  relationships 
between  these  assumptions  and  the  conclusion. 

c.  Recognition  of  the  need  for  further  data  to  confirm,  qual- 
ify, or  negate  the  available  evidence. 

d.  Ability  to  distinguish  between  assumptions  whose  ten- 
ability  could  be  checked  by  collecting  further  data  and 
assumptions  whose  tenability  could  not  be  checked  in 
this  way.  Examples  of  assumptions  of  the  latter  sort  are 
value  judgments,  statements  of  preference,  and  definitions 
of  terms. 

e.  Recognition  of  the  possible  ways  for  studying  a  problem 
further,  and  ability  to  distinguish  between  fruitful  and 
unfruitful  methods  of  further  study. 

f.  Willingness  to  accept  or  reject  assumptions  tentatively, 
and  to  test  the  conclusions  which  follow  from  these  as- 
sumptions by  acting  upon  them. 

g.  Recognition  that  new  evidence  upon  the  soundness  of 
one  or  more  of  the  assumptions  may  make  it  desirable 
to  reconsider  the  argument  and  perhaps  to  qualify  the 
conclusion  tentatively  reached. 

The  efforts  of  the  Evaluation  Staff  to  measure  behaviors 
relative  to  the  Nature  of  Proof  objective  were  directed  to- 


i3o        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

ward  measuring  the  abilities  connected  with  analyzing  writ- 
ten arguments  rather  than  toward  the  disposition  to  analyze 
arguments  critically.  Even  when  the  problem  was  reduced 
to  measuring  the  skills  involved  in  the  critical  analysis  of 
arguments,  it  was  found  to  be  an  extremely  complex  prob- 
lem. Groups  of  teachers  in  the  Eight- Year  Study  were  en- 
thusiastic in  their  approval  of  the  objective,  and  they  sug- 
gested many  behaviors  which  seemed  to  them  significant. 
The  task  of  clarification  and  simplification  was  much  greater 
than  was  originally  anticipated.  The  early  forms  of  the  test 
used  experimentally  in  an  attempt  to  secure  insight  into  the 
nature  of  proof  objective  were  too  complicated  for  prac- 
tical purposes.  The  persons  who  worked  on  this  problem 
were,  however,  convinced  that  the  objective  is  very  sig- 
nificant for  general  education  at  the  secondary  level  and 
that  a  continuing  effort  to  overcome  the  obstacles  set  up  by 
its  complexity  is  worthwhile. 

THE  DEVELOPMENT  OF  EVALUATION  INSTRUMENTS 

The  first  nature  of  proof  tests  which  were  constructed  pre- 
sented the  student  with  a  described  situation  which  presum- 
ably led  to  a  conclusion,  and  he  was  asked  to  write  down 
the  assumptions  which  seemed  to  him  to  underlie  the  argu- 
ment.20 An  analysis  of  the  responses  indicated  that  for  the 
most  part  they  could  be  classified  into  a  few  types.  For  ex- 
ample, a  uniqueness  assumption  is  often  needed  to  clinch 
an  argument — an  assumption  which  states  that  a  product 
advertised,  or  a  chemical  used  in  an  experiment,  etc.,  is  the 
only  one  which  has  a  given  property. 

The  student  responses  and  the  results  of  the  analysis  were 
utilized  in  the  construction  of  a  short-answer  form.  A  list  of 
statements  relative  to  a  problem  situation  was  given,  includ- 

20  Cf .  H.  P.  Fawcett,  The  Nature  of  Proof,  Thirteenth  Yearbook  of  the 
National  Council  of  Teachers  of  Mathematics  (New  York,  Bureau  of  Pub- 
lications, Teachers  College,  Columbia  University,  1938),  Appendix,  Part  L 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  131 

ing  some  which  purported  to  represent  facts  and  others 
which  were  assumptions.  Students  were  asked  to  distinguish 
facts  from  assumptions,  to  reconstruct  the  argument  by  using 
statements  from  the  list,  and  to  indicate  whether  they  would 
accept  or  reject  the  conclusion  of  the  reconstructed  argu- 
ment. 

The  results  from  the  first  short-answer  form  threw  a  good 
deal  of  light  on  the  thinking  of  the  students.  Difficulties  fre- 
quently arose,  however,  with  respect  to  the  use  of  the  terms 
"fact"  and  "assumption/'  and  the  first  part  of  the  test  did 
not  discriminate  well  among  students.  The  scoring  of  the 
reconstructed  arguments  also  caused  difficulty.  The  test  was 
therefore  revised  several  times,  but  limitations  of  space  pre- 
vent a  discussion  of  the  resulting  experience.  Only  the  forms 
which  the  test  had  taken  toward  the  end  of  the  Study  can 
be  described  here. 

Form  5.21  of  the  Nature  of  Proof  test  incorporated  sev- 
eral major  changes.  An  attempt  was  made  to  have  the  stu- 
dents locate  the  basic  assumptions  underlying  the  argument, 
but  the  term  assumptions  was  not  used  in  the  directions  to 
the  student.  In  each  problem  a  paragraph  which  presum- 
ably justified  a  conclusion  stated  at  the  close  of  the  para- 
graph was  presented.  There  followed  a  list  of  statements. 
Some  of  these  statements  were  relevant,  in  the  sense  that 
they  described  assumptions  underlying  the  argument,  and 
some  of  them  were  irrelevant.  The  students  were  asked  to 
pick  out  the  relevant  statements  and  to  decide  which  of 
these  might  logically  be  used  to  support  the  stated  conclu- 
sion. In  this  way  the  students  were  given  an  opportunity 
to  locate  basic  assumptions,  and  to  recognize  the  function 
of  these  assumptions  in  an  argument,  although  the  word  as- 
sumption was  not  used  in  the  test  directions. 

One  of  the  problems  taken  from  Form  5.21  of  the  Nature 
of  Proof  test  is  given  below.  The  directions,  in  a  shortened 


i32        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

form,  are  presented  along  with  the  problem.21  A  list  of  re- 
sponses accepted  as  correct  in  scoring  the  test,  i.e.,  a  test 
key,  is  given  in  the  margin.  It  should  be  noted  that  the  test 
key  adopted  by  a  committee  of  competent  persons  before 
the  test  was  given  to  students  was  changed  to  some  extent 
when  the  test  results  for  a  sample  group  of  students  were 
studied.  It  became  apparent  that  the  "C"  step  in  the  direc- 
tions was  interpreted  differently  by  the  students  than  by 
the  committee.  There  were  also  apparent  differences  in  the 
interpretation  given  to  the  "C"  step  by  students.  It  should 
also  be  noted  that  there  was  no  decision  as  to  a  "correct" 
response  to  the  conclusion. 

Read  the  problem  and  then: 

A.  Select  the  statements  which  either  support  or  contradict  the 
underlined  conclusion. 

B.  Select  the  statements  marked  under  A  which  support  the 
underlined  conclusion. 

C.  Select  the  statements  marked  under  B  which  you  do  not  con- 
sider satisfactorily  established  by  whatever  general  knowl- 
edge you  may  Jiave,  but  which  must  be  included  in  the 
argument  if  the  conclusion  is  to  be  completely  justified. 

Conclusion,  According  to  what  seems  most  consistent  with  your 
analysis  thus  far,  decide  whether  you: 

A.  Are  inclined  to  B.  Are  very  uncer-  C.  Are  inclined  to 
accept  the  con-  tain  about  the  reject  the  con- 
clusion, conclusion.  elusion. 

Reasons.  Select  the  statements  marked  under  C  which  might 
cause  you  to  reconsider  your  decision  about  the  under- 
lined conclusion  if  more  information  were  made  avail- 
able to  you.  Mark  these  under  D. 

21  The  use  of  A,  B,  C,  D  in  the  directions  below  is  clarified  by  the  com- 
plete directions,  by  the  form  of  the  special  answer  sheet  on  which  the 
student  makes  his  responses,  and  also  by  a  sample  exercise  explained  in  the 
general  directions.  In  the  marginal  keys  below,  these  letters  refer  to  the 
columns  in  which  a  statement  should  be  marked  on  the  answer  sheet. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  133 

PROBLEM  IX 

In  a  radio  broadcast  the  following  story  was  told:  "The  people 
in  a  little  mining  town  in  Pennsylvania  get  all  their  water  with- 
out purification  from  a  clear,  swift-running  mountain  stream. 
In  a  cabin  on  the  bank  of  the  stream  about  a  half  a  mile  above 
the  town  a  worker  was  very  sick  with  typhoid  fever  during  the 
first  part  of  December.  During  his  illness  his  waste  materials 
were  thrown  on  the  snow.  About  the  middle  of  March  the  snow 
melted  rapidly  and  ran  into  the  stream.  Approximately  two  weeks 
later  typhoid  fever  struck  the  town.  Many  of  the  people  became 
sick  and  114  died."  The  speaker  then  said  that  this  story  showed 
how  the  sickness  of  this  man  caused  widespread  illness,  and  the 
death  of  over  one  hundred  people. 

Statements: 

ABCD  1.  Typhoid  fever  organisms  can  survive  for  at  least 
three  months  at  temperatures  near  the  freezing 
point. 

Irrele-        2.  Good  doctors  should  be  available  when  an  epi- 

vant  demic  hits  a  small  town. 

ABCD  3.  Typhoid  fever  germs  are  active  after  being  carried 
for  about  half  a  mile  in  clear,  swift-running  water. 

A  4.  There  may  have  been  other  sources  of  contamina- 

tion by  waste  materials  containing  typhoid  fever 
germs  along  the  stream  or  at  some  other  point  in 
titie  water  supply  of  the  town. 

AB  5.  The  waste  materials  of  a  person  who  has  a  severe 

case  of  typhoid  fever  contain  active  typhoid  organ- 
isms. 

AB  6.  Typhoid  fever  is  contracted  by  taking  the  typhoid 

organisms  into  the  body  by  way  of  the  mouth. 

Irrele-        7.  Only  a  few  people  in  this  town  had  developed  an 

vant  immunity  to  typhoid  fever. 

A  8.  Typhoid  organisms  are  usually  killed  if  subjected 

to  temperature  near  the  freezing  point  for  a  period 
of  several  months. 


134        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Irrele-        9.  Sickness  and  death  usually  result  in  a  great  eco- 

vant  nornic  loss  to  a  small  town. 

ABCD  10.  The  only  typhoid  organisms  with  which  the  peo- 
ple in  the  town  came  in  contact  were  in  the  water 
supply. 

Irrele-       11.  Vaccination  should  be  compulsory  in  communities 

vant  which  have  no  means  of  purifying  their  water 

supply. 

ABCD  12.  The  worker's  waste  materials  were  the  only  source 
of  contamination  along  the  stream. 

A  13.  There  may  have  been  other  sources  of  typhoid 

fever  germs  in  the  town  such  as  milk  or  food  con- 
taminated by  some  other  person. 

AB  14.  The  symptoms  of  typhoid  fever  usually  appear 

about  two  weeks  after  contact  with  typhoid  germs. 

Several  further  comments  on  the  structure  of  this  sample 
problem  might  be  added  to  those  made  above.  When  the 
student  has  chosen  the  statements  which  he  thinks  support 
the  stated  conclusion,  he  is  asked  to  decide  which  of  these 
are  essential  assumptions  whose  truth  he  would  question. 
On  the  basis  of  his  analysis  of  the  problem,  the  student  is 
then  asked  to  indicate  the  degree  of  his  acceptance  of  the 
stated  conclusion.  Finally  the  student  is  asked  to  decide 
which  of  the  essential  assumptions  might,  in  the  light  of 
further  evidence,  make  it  necessary  to  reconsider  his  deci- 
sion about  the  stated  conclusion. 

The  relationship  between  the  activities  which  the  students 
were  directed  to  perform  and  the  definition  of  the  objective 
in  terms  of  behavior  will  be  apparent  to  the  reader.  Under 
ideal  conditions  the  activities  which  the  student  performs 
might  be  expected  to  yield  evidence  on  the  students'  ability 
to  recognize  the  basic  assumptions  in  an  argument,  the 
standard  of  proof  which  the  student  demands,  the  student's 
recognition  of  the  tentative  nature  of  the  conclusions  which 
are  based  upon  arguments,  and  the  role  of  reexamining  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  135 

underlying  assumptions  in  order  to  qualify  the  conclusions 
which  one  reaches.  In  practice,  the  results  do  not  yield  valid 
evidence  on  achievement  relative  to  all  of  these  behaviors. 
For  example,  students  vary  a  great  deal  in  the  number  of 
statements  which  they  recognize  as  supporting  the  stated 
conclusions.  This  makes  the  number  of  opportunities  to  chal- 
lenge assumptions  different  for  different  students.  A  still 
more  serious  consideration  is  the  possibility  for  variation  in 
the  interpretation  of  the  test  directions  from  student  to  stu- 
dent. Such  variation  was  noted  particularly  in  connection 
with  the  directions  for  challenging  the  truth  of  the  state- 
ments which  had  been  marked  as  supporting  the  stated  con- 
clusions. Moreover,  the  fact  that  the  various  activities  which 
the  students  are  requested  to  carry  out  are  interrelated,  so 
that  failure  to  perform  one  step  seriously  interferes  with  per- 
forming the  next  step,  presents  a  difficulty  in  interpreting 
the  results.  In  this  connection  the  number  and  complexity 
of  the  related  activities  which  the  students  were  asked  to 
carry  through  proved  discouraging  to  many  students. 

In  the  next  section  a  description  of  the  structure  of  Form 
5.22  of  the  Nature  of  Proof  test  in  which  the  attempt  is  made 
to  avoid  some  of  these  difficulties,  is  presented. 

Structure  of  the  Nature  of  Proof  Test,  Form  5.22 

The  progress  toward  Form  5.22  has  involved  an  attempt 
to  simplify  both  the  procedures  which  students  are  asked  to 
carry  out  and  the  directions  for  carrying  out  these  proce- 
dures. At  the  same  time  there  has  been  an  attempt  to  retain 
many  of  the  aspects  of  thinking  commonly  associated  with 
problem-solving  and  scientific  method. 

A  study  of  the  following  explanation  of  the  structure  of 
the  test  problems  in  comparison  with  the  sample  test  prob- 
lem presented  below  will  serve  to  clarify  the  reasons  for  the 
inclusion  of  each  part  of  the  test.  A  list  of  the  responses  ac- 


136        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

cepted  as  correct  by  a  jury  of  competent  persons,  I.e.,  a  test 
key,  is  given  in  the  margin. 

PROBLEM  III 

A  science  class  was  studying  methods  of  caring  for  the  skin.  The 
teacher  described  the  following  experiment  and  stated  the  con- 
clusion which  had  been  drawn  from  it.  "A  large  bottle  of  each 
of  the  five  leading  brands  of  hand  lotion  was  purchased  from 
a  drug  store.  The  lotion  in  each  bottle  was  thoroughly  mixed  by 
shaking  the  bottle  for  three  minutes.  Five  exactly  similar  water 
glasses,  one  for  each  lotion,  were  set  in  a  row  on  a  table,  and  a 
piece  of  filter  paper  was  placed  over  the  open  top  of  each  glass. 
Each  brand  of  lotion  was  tested  by  pouring  a  half  teaspoonful 
of  it  on  the  piece  of  filter  paper.  For  the  first  brand  of  hand 
lotion,  drops  appeared  in  the  water  glass  within  thirty  seconds. 
The  other  four  brands  all  took  longer  than  one  minute,  and  two 
brands  failed  to  filter  through  at  all."  This  experiment  shows  that 
the  first  brand  of  lotion  is  absorbed  by  the  skin  more  readily 
than  any  of  the  others. 

L  Directions:  In  this  part,  you  are  to  do  two  things: 
Select  all  statements  which  could  logically  be  used  to  support 
the  underlined  conclusion.  Blacken  the  space  under  A  opposite 
the  number  of  each  such  statement. 

At  the  same  time,  select  all  statements  which  might  make  the 
underlined  conclusion  less  acceptable.  Blacken  the  space  under 
B  opposite  the  number  of  each  such  statement. 

In  this  part  of  the  test,  your  decision  about  a  statement  should 
not  be  influenced  by  whether  you  believe  the  idea  expressed 
to  be  true  or  false. 

Statements  for  I  and  II: 

AC  1.  The  contents  of  one  large  bottle  of  a  certain  brand 

of  hand  lotion  are  exactly  like  the  contents  of  any 
ather  large  bottle  of  the  same  brand  of  hand  lotion. 

Irrele-        2.  The  liquid  which  is  absorbed  most  readily  by  the 

vant  skin  is  the  most  effective  in  softening  the  hands. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  137 

B  3.  To  be  absorbed  by  the  skin  a  hand  lotion  need 

not  pass  through  the  skin. 

Irrele-        4,  Hand  lotions  are  of  doubtful  value. 

vant 

AC  5.  The  faster  a  liquid  drips  through  filter  paper  the 

faster  it  will  be  absorbed  by  the  human  skin. 

AC  6.  The  pores  of  the  skin  are  quite  similar  to  the  little 

holes  between  the  fibers  of  filter  paper. 

A  7.  Since  each  bottle  was  given  a  thorough  shaking, 

the  results  for  each  lotion  were  typical  of  the  per- 
formance of  the  lotion  in  that  bottle. 

B  8.  The  "pores"  in  filter  paper  are  constructed  quite 

differently  from  the  "pores"  in  the  human  skin. 

Irrele-        9.  The  experiment  was  probably  intended  to  make 

vant  sales  for  some  cosmetics  manufacturer. 

B  10.  Although  drops  of  a  liquid  appeared  in  the  water 

glass,  certain  ingredients  of  the  first  lotion  may  have 
been  retained  by  the  filter  paper. 

Irrele-      11.  The  speed  with  which  a  lotion  drips  through  filter 

vant  paper  is  no  indication  of  its  effectiveness  in  soften- 

ing the  skin. 

B  12.  Water  will  penetrate  filter  paper  but  is  not  absorbed 

by  the  skin. 

Irrele-      13.  The  obvious  way  to  test  the  five  lotions  is  to  try 

vant  them  on  the  hands  of  a  large  group  of  people. 

A  14.  The  amounts  of  lotion  placed  on  each  piece  of  filter 

paper  were  very  nearly  the  same. 

II.  Directions:  Select  from  the  statements  already  marked  under 
A  (the  supporting  statements)  those  which  you  would  chal- 
lenge because  you  are  not  convinced  they  are  true  enough 
to  be  used  in  supporting  the  underlined  conclusion.  Blacken 
the  space  under  C  opposite  the  number  of  each  such  state- 
ment. 

III.  Directions:  Conclusions  A,  B,  and  C  are  stated  below.  Choose 
the  one  which  seems  to  you  to  be  most  consistent  with  your 
analysis  of  the  situation  described  in  the  problem.  In  the 


138        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

block  at  the  top  of  the  answer  sheet,  blacken  the  space  A, 
B,  or  C  to  indicate  the  conclusion  which  you  choose. 

Conclusions: 

\/A.  This  experiment  does  not  help  in  deciding  which,  one  of 
the  hand  lotions  would  be  most  readily  absorbed  by  the 
skin. 

B.  The  experiment  suggests  that  the  first  brand  of  hand  lotion 
is  absorbed  by  the  skin  more  readily  than  any  of  the  others, 
but  the  experiment  would  have  to  be  repeated  several 
times. 

C.  The  experiment  shows  that  the  first  brand  of  hand  lotion 
is  absorbed  by  the  skin  more   readily  than  any   of  the 
others. 

IV.  Directions:  Hand  lotions  are  commonly  used  to  replace  the 
oils  in  the  outer  layers  of  the  skin  which  are  lost  through 
excessive  exposure,  washing,  and  other  causes.  Hence  it  may 
be  less  important  to  study  the  extent  to  which  a  lotion  pene- 
trates the  layers  of  the  skin  than  to  study  its  effect  upon  the 
surface  of  the  skin.  The  statements  presented  below  describe 
some  activities  which  have  been  suggested  to  study  the  ef- 
fectiveness of  a  hand  lotion  in  keeping  the  skin  soft  in  the 
absence  of  an  adequate  supply  of  natural  skin  oils. 

Select  all  statements  that  describe  activities  which  you  think 
would  help  in  studying  this  effect  of  a  hand  lotion  upon  the 
skin.  Blacken  the  space  under  A  opposite  the  number  of 
each  such  statement. 

In  this  part  of  the  test.,  your  decision  about  a  statement 
should  not  be  influenced  by  whether  you  believe  the  activity 
described  could  actually  be  carried  out. 

Statements  for  TV  and  V: 

A  B          15.  Secure  a  description  of  the  structure  of  the  human 

skin. 

Irrele-      16.  Find  out  the  names  of  the  companies  which  manu- 
vant  facture  each  of  the  brands  of  hand  lotion  used  in 

the  experiment. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  139 

A  17.  Make  a  precise  laboratory  analysis  of  each  of  sev- 

eral brands  of  hand  lotion  to  find  out  the  amounts 
and  properties  of  its  principal  ingredients,  such  as 
vegetable  oils,  water,  etc. 

Irrele-    :  18.  Repeat  the  experiment  several  times  with  the  same 

vant  five  lotions  and  under  exactly  the  same  conditions. 

A  B  19.  Set  up  an  experiment  in  which  ten  boys  and  ten 
girls  apply  a  hand  lotion  to  one  hand  and  no  hand 
lotion  to  the  other  hand  once  each  day  for  a  month 
and  compare  the  results. 

Irrele-      20.  Send  out  a  questionnaire  to  a  large  number  of 

vant  users  of  hand  lotion  to  find  out  which  brand  is 

most  popular. 

A  B  21.  Use  hand  lotions  regularly  on  several  parts  of  the 
body  and  compare  the  results. 

A  22.  Set  up  an  experiment  to  compare  the  natural  skin 

oils  to  the  oils  contained  in  hand  lotions. 

Irrele-      23.  Compare  the  absorbing  power  of  filter  paper  and 

vant  human  skin. 

A  B  24.  Look  for  published  information  about  some  of  the 
good  and  bad  effects  of  using  different  brands  of 
hand  lotion. 

V.  Directions:  Select  from  the  statements  already  marked  under 
A  only  things  which  you  think  you  or  your  class  in  high  school 
could  actually  carry  out.  Blacken  the  space  under  B  opposite 
the  number  of  each  such  statement. 

In  each  problem  the  student  is  given  a  paragraph  which 
presumably  justifies  an  underlined  conclusion  stated  at  the 
close  of  the  paragraph.  This  is  followed  by  14  statements. 
Some  of  these  statements  are  relevant  in  the  sense  that  they 
describe  assumptions  underlying  the  argument,  and  some  of 
them  are  irrelevant.  Some  of  the  relevant  statements  might 
be  used  to  support  the  underlined  conclusion  and  the  re- 
mainder of  them  might  be  used  to  contradict  it.  In  the  first 
part  of  the  test  the  student  is  asked  to  decide  which  of  these 
statements  are  relevant  and  to  mark  them  as  either  support- 


i4o        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

ing  or  contradicting.  In  making  these  judgments,  the  stu- 
dent is  to  disregard  the  degree  of  truth  or  falsity  which  he 
may  ascribe  to  the  statements  in  the  paragraph  or  to  the 
statements  listed  below  the  paragraph.  He  is  to  judge  the 
relevance  of  a  given  statement  solely  in  terms  of  the  con- 
text of  the  argument  and  to  decide  whether  each  relevant 
statement  supports  or  contradicts  the  underlined  conclusion. 

In  the  second  part  of  the  test  the  student's  attention  is 
directed  toward  those  particular  statements  which  he  marked 
as  supporting  statements.  He  is  asked  to  indicate  the  ones 
which  he  would  challenge  because  he  is  not  convinced  that 
they  are  true  enough  to  be  used  in  supporting  the  underlined 
conclusion.  Since  the  relevant  statements  describe  assump- 
tions necessary  in  order  to  establish  the  underlined  conclu- 
sion, in  a  sense  the  student  is  asked  in  the  first  two  parts  of 
the  test  to  decide  which  statements  are  necessary  assumptions 
in  the  argument,  and  of  these,  to  choose  the  ones  about 
which  he  is  uncertain  or  is  in  doubt. 

In  the  third  part  of  the  test  the  student  is  asked  to  choose 
one  of  three  stated  conclusions.  One  of  these  conclusions  ex- 
presses an  acceptance,  another,  a  qualified  acceptance,  and 
the  third,  a  rejection  of  the  underlined  conclusion.  In  each 
problem  the  student  is  asked  to  choose  the  conclusion  which 
seems  to  him  to  be  most  consistent  with  his  analysis  of  the 
problem.  In  order  to  agree  with  the  test  key,  the  student 
should  in  two  problems  choose  acceptance,  in  four  problems 
choose  qualified  acceptance,  and  in  two  problems  choose  re- 
jections of  the  underlined  conclusions. 

Parts  I,  II,  and  III  of  the  test  can  be  given  and  scored  in- 
dependently of  the  remainder  of  the  test,  and  for  some  pur- 
poses may  be  considered  sufficient.  However,  besides  being 
able  to  test  a  stated  conclusion  (as  in  parts  one  and  two) 
by  an  examination  of  the  assumptions  underlying  the  argu- 
ments which  purport  to  establish  this  conclusion,  it  is  also 
important  to  be  able  to  recognize  fruitful  lines  of  further 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  141 

investigation  and  to  distinguish  between  types  of  activities 
which  are  relevant  to  testing  the  conclusion  and  those  which 
are  not.  It  may  also  be  considered  important  for  students  to 
leam  to  judge  the  practicability  of  a  proposed  line  of  inves- 
tigation. Parts  IV  and  V  of  the  test  were  designed  to  secure 
evidence  on  the  ability  of  students  to  appraise  the  relevance 
and  practicability  of  proposals  for  the  further  study  of  a 
problem. 

In  the  fourth  part  of  the  test  a  significant  problem  which 
involves  further  study  of  the  issues  raised  in  Parts  I,  II,  and 
III  is  stated.  The  student  is  asked  to  select  from  a  list  of 
statements  those  that  describe  activities  which  would  help 
him  to  solve  this  problem.  In  making  his  judgment,,  the 
student  is  not  to  be  influenced  by  whether  he  believes  the 
activity  described  could  be  carried  out  in  a  practical  sense. 

In  the  fifth  part  of  the  test  the  student's  attention  is 
directed  toward  those  particular  statements  which  he  se- 
lected in  Part  IV.  He  is  asked  to  indicate  the  ones  which 
he  thinks  he  or  his  class  in  high  school  could  actually  carry 
out. 

The  scores  given  to  students  reflect  their  success  or  fail- 
ure in  carrying  out  the  procedures  in  each  part  of  the  test. 
The  interpretation  of  the  results  depends  upon  the  inter- 
preter's understanding  of  the  structure  of  the  test  problems. 
The  usefulness  of  the  test  results  is  in  direct  proportion  to 
the  extent  of  the  interpreter's  concern  with  the  objective  and 
his  confidence  that  significant  behaviors  involved  in  the  ob- 
jective are  actually  sampled  in  the  different  parts  of  the  test. 

Summarization  and  Interpretation  of  the  Scores  on  Form  5.22 

During  the  experimental  stages  of  Form  5.22,  the  test  re- 
sults for  a  sample  population  of  307  students  were  studied 
intensively  in  an  attempt  to  discover  the  most  convenient 
and  most  meaningful  form  for  reporting  the  results.  These 
students  comprised  12  separate  classes  divided  among  the 


142        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

tenth,  eleventh,  and  twelfth  grades.  The  procedure  de- 
scribed previously  in  connection  with  the  test  on  Applica- 
tion of  Principles  of  Logical  Reasoning  was  also  used  in 
this  case.22  Twenty-two  scores  were  summarized  for  each 
student,  and  several  additional  scores  were  computed  from 
these  during  the  study.  Certain  important  scores  were  se- 
lected and  studied  with  reference  to  the  item  analysis  in  an 
effort  to  see  more  clearly  the  relationships  between  each  of 
these  scores  and  the  responses  of  students  to  individual  test 
items.  Certain  correlations  between  the  various  scores  whicH 
had  been  summarized  were  run.  It  was  found  possible  to 
reduce  the  number  of  scores  on  the  data  sheet  from  22  to  13 
without  an  appreciable  loss  of  information.  Scores  on  per 
cent  accuracy,  computed  as  number  of  responses  marked  in 
agreement  with  the  test  key  divided  by  total  number  of  re- 
sponses of  that  kind,  were  tried  and  abandoned  because 
they  were  somewhat  unreliable  and  apt  to  be  misleading. 
Moreover  an  examination  of  the  scores  on  various  kinds  of 
errors  which  were  also  summarized  yielded  the  desired  in- 
formation in  a  slightly  different  form.  A  score  on  the  per 
cent  of  the  statements  keyed  as  supporting  and  marked  by 
students  as  supporting  which  the  students  also  marked  as 
critical  was  tried  in  an  effort  to  secure  an  index  of  the  "criti- 
calness"  of  a  student.  This  score  was  found  to  correlate 
highly  with  a  score  on  critical  statements  marked  by  stu- 
dents as  critical  statements.  Hence  a  score  on  critical  state- 
ments marked  as  critical  was  used  as  an  index  of  the  tend- 
ency of  a  student  who  had  marked  a  statement  as  supporting 
to  challenge  its  truth.  This  score  when  used  as  an  index  is 
not  subject  to  the  criticism  that  it  depends  upon  the  number 
of  supporting  statements  which  the  student  marked  as  sup- 
porting since  the  effect  of  this  dependence  was  considered 
and  found  to  be  insignificant. 
22  See  pp.  122-123. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  143 

The  scores  on  this  test  can  be  interpreted  in  terms  of  the 

answers  to  five  questions: 

1.  To  what  extent  does  the  student  recognize  relevant 
phases  of  an  argument,  and  distinguish  between  con- 
siderations which  support  and  ones  which  contradict 
a  stated  hypothesis  or  conclusion? 

2.  To  what  extent  does  the  student  challenge  the  as- 
sumptions underlying  an  argument,  and  distinguish 
between  assumptions  which,  from  the  point  of  view 
of  a  committee  of  adults,  should  and  should  not  be 
challenged? 

3.  How  do  the  conclusions  reached  by  the  student  com- 
pare with  those  reached  by  the  committee  who  made 
the  test? 

4.  To  what  extent  does  the  student  recognize  the  rele- 
vance of  proposals  for  the  further  study  of  a  problem? 

5.  To  what  extent  does  the  student  judge  the  relevant 
activities  as  practicable,  i.e.,  distinguish  between  ac- 
tivities which,  from  the  point  of  view  of  a  committee 
of  adults,  are  and  are  not  practicable? 

By  a  study  of  the  various  scores  reported  on  the  data  sheet 
the  teacher  may  obtain  evidence  relative  to  each  of  these 
questions.  It  is  particularly  true  of  this  test  that  the  number 
of  patterns  of  behavior  revealed  by  the  test  scores  is  almost 
as  great  as  the  number  of  students  who  take  the  test.  Each 
pattern  should  be  considered  as  a  unique  situation  to  be 
interpreted. 

VALIDITY  AND  RELIABILITY  OF  TEST  FORM  5.22 

The  construction  of  Form  5.22  of  the  nature  of  proof  test 
was  undertaken  in  the  light  of  a  good  deal  of  negative  and 
some  positive  evidence  on  the  behaviors  of  secondary  school 
students  relative  to  the  nature  of  proof  objective.  Certain 
"don'ts"  were  clearly  indicated  by  experience  with  previous 


i44        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

forms  of  the  test.  For  example,  in  Form  5.21  the  dependence 
of  each  step  upon  preceding  steps  made  the  interpretation  of 
the  test  results  difficult.  At  the  same  time,  a  number  of  "do's" 
were  indicated.  For  example,  the  realization  that  the  basic 
assumptions  upon  which  a  conclusion  depends  may  be  ex- 
pressed in  the  form  of  statements  which  either  support  or 
tend  to  contradict  the  conclusion  (as  opposed  to  statements 
which  are  irrelevant)  made  it  possible  to  get  at  the  concept 
of  assumptions  in  operational  terms. 

In  approaching  the  construction  of  Form  5.22  of  the  nature 
of  proof  test,  a  need  was  felt  for  another  check  upon  the 
direct  responses  of  students.  The  students  in  a  geometry  class 
of  a  large  public  high  school  not  participating  in  the  Eight- 
Year  Study  were  selected  for  this  purpose.  The  teacher  of 
this  class  was  known  to  be  working  actively  to  improve  the 
achievement  of  this  objective.  For  purposes  of  illustration, 
one  of  the  four  test  exercises  which  were  given  is  reprinted 
below  together  with  the  responses  which  one  student  made 
to  the  questions. 

'Exercise  II 

Read  the  paragraph  and  then  answer  the  questions  which  follow. 
Speed  is  not  at  all  important.  You  should  take  enough  time  to 
organize  your  ideas  and  to  state  them  precisely. 
In  an  agriculture  class  the  teacher  was  discussing  the  importance 
of  the  use  of  fertilizer.  He  described  the  following  experiment: 
"Some  wheat  seeds  were  planted  in  two  large  pots  of  earth.  The 
seeds  were  of  the  same  variety,  and  the  soil  used  had  been  thor- 
oughly mixed  and  then  divided  into  two  parts,  one  for  each  pot. 
Fertilizer  was  added  to  one  and  not  to  the  other.  The  pots  were 
then  placed  side  by  side  in  a  greenhouse  and  both  regularly  and 
equally  watered.  At  the  end  of  three  months  the  wheat  plants  in 
the  fertilized  pot  weighed  twenty-five  per  cent  more  than  those 
in  the  unfertilized  pot." 
The  class  came  to  the  following  conclusion:  "Farmers  who  use 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  145 

this  fertilizer  on  land  on  which  they  raise  wheat  will  get  larger 
yields  of  grain" 

1.  Indicate  your  reaction  to  the  underlined  conclusion  by  a  check 
mark  (V)  in  one  of  the  three  spaces  provided. 

After  a  consideration  of  this  experiment  I  feel  that  the  under- 
lined conclusion  is: 

Probably  true  \/>  Completely  uncertain  ,  Probably  false 

Explain  your  answer  in  some  detail,  that  is,  tell  why  you  felt 
that  the  underlined  conclusion  was  probably  true,  completely 
uncertain,  or  probably  false. 

"I  felt  that  the  underlined  conclusion  was  probably  true,  because 
if  the  fertilizer  had  been  placed  in  the  pot  where  the  wheat  seed 
grew  the  faster,  then  that  would  prove  it. 

"Especially  if  the  soil  had  been  mixed  thoroughly  and  the  pots 
watered  equally  each  day." 

2.  What  things  does  the  class  have  to  assume  (take  for  granted) 
if  the  underlined  conclusion  is  to  be  considered  true?  You  may 
include  statements  of  ideas  reported  in  the  above  experiment 
and  also  statements  of  ideas  not  actually  mentioned.  Make  a 
separate  statement  for  each  assumption  which  you  wish  to 
point  out,  and  number  these  statements  1,  2,  3, 

"1.  The  wheat  seeds  were  the  same. 

2.  The  soil  was  thoroughly  mixed. 

3.  The  two  plants  were  regularly  and  equally  watered. 
\/4.  All  wheat,  even  from  the  same  crop*  grows  the  same  as 

the  rest." 

3.  Now  go  back  to  the  statements  which  you  listed  under  point 
2  above.  You  may  feel  that  some  of  these  statements  should 
not  be  assumed  or  taken  for  granted.  Place  a  check  mark  (V) 
beside  the  number  of  each  statement  which  you  feel  should 
not  be  taken  for  granted. 

4.  What  things  occur  to  you  which,  if  true,  would  contradict  the 
underlined  conclusion?  Make  a  separate  statement  for  each 

.  contradictory  idea  and  number  these  statements  1,  2,  3, 


146        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

"1.  Two  wheat  seeds,  even  from  the  same  crop,  would  most 
likely  not  grow  the  same,  even  under  the  same  condi- 
tions." 

5.  In  what  ways  could  the  above  experiment  be  improved?  Make 
a  separate  statement  for  each  suggested  improvement  and 
number  these  statements  1,  2,  3, 

"1.  Take  more  than  two  pots  and  then  let  them  grow  under 
the  same  conditions,  because  the  more  seeds  you  use, 
the  more  perfect  will  be  your  conclusion. 

"2.  Take  soil  from  same  general  location  and  mix,  putting 
one  with  fertilizer  and  one  without.  Use  water  equiva- 
lent to  general  rainfall  in  location  from  which  soil  is 
taken  from,  and  at  approximately  the  same  intervals. 

"3.  Run  tests  over  a  greater  period  of  time." 

Several  significant  observations  were  made  from  this  in- 
vestigation. The  rather  weak  responses  which  the  student 
quoted  made  to  question  1  (the  general  direction  was  "ex- 
plain your  answer  in  some  detail")  are  typical  of  this  sample 
of  students.  In  response  to  question  2  some  of  the  students 
wrote  out  basic  assumptions  which,  went  beyond  a  mere 
repetition  of  the  statements  made  in  the  paragraph.  Other 
students  found  even  more  difficulty  at  this  point  than  did 
the  student  whose  responses  are  presented  above.  The  re- 
sponses to  question  3  are  dependent  upon  the  responses  to 
question  2  and  as  a  result  were  significant  only  for  students 
whose  performance  on  question  2  was  satisfactory.  The  re- 
sponses to  question  4  seldom  yielded  new  ideas  not  pre- 
viously expressed  in  the  answers  to  questions  1  and  2.  An 
appreciable  number  of  the  students  introduced  several  new 
ideas  in  their  responses  to  question  5.  The  student  whose 
responses  are  presented  above  is  an  example.  In  summary, 
the  results  were  as  follows:  (1)  The  general  direction  "Ex- 
plain your  answer  in  some  detail/'  does  not  elicit  detailed, 
comprehensive  answers.  (2)  There  is  a  considerable  differ- 
ence in  the  minds  of  some  students  between  locating  as- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  147 

sumptions  upon  which  a  conclusion  depends  and  suggesting 
ways  for  improving  the  argument  upon  which  a  conclusion 
depends.  In  the  light  of  the  first  point,  we  would  expect  dif- 
ficulties if  we  attempted  to  compare  the  written  responses  of 
students  to  the  general  direction  "Explain  your  answer  in 
some  detail"  to  their  responses  on  an  objective  test.  In  the 
light  of  the  second  point,  it  may  be  worthwhile  to  include  in 
an  objective  test  two  logically  equivalent  forms  of  questions 
relative  to  underlying  assumptions:  (a)  pick  out  the  state- 
ments of  underlying  assumptions,  (b)  pick  out  the  state- 
ments of  activities  relevant  to  improving  the  argument.  The 
reader  will  recall  from  his  study  of  the  simple  problem  that 
an  attempt  was  made  in  constructing  Form  5.22  of  the  Nature 
of  Proof  test  to  include  questions  of  these  two  kinds. 

The  construction  of  Form  5.22  of  the  Nature  of  Proof  test 
was  undertaken  by  a  committee  of  five  persons  with  the  as- 
sistance at  certain  stages  of  several  other  persons.  The  test 
situations  and  test  directions  were  viewed  critically  in  the 
light  of  all  of  the  available  evidence  from  previous  forms  of 
the  test.  An  analysis  of  the  statements  made  by  various  stu- 
dents provided  helpful  suggestions  for  the  construction  of 
statements  to  be  included  in  the  objective  form  of  the  test. 
The  kinds  of  irrelevant  statements  which  the  students  made 
were  especially  helpful  in  building  irrelevant  statements 
which  would  be  used  as  relevant  by  an  appreciable  number 
of  students.  The  results  of  the  statistical  study  which  is  de- 
scribed below  indicate  that  the  directions  to  the  students  are 
unambiguous,  and  that  several  distinct  behaviors  are  meas- 
ured by  the  test.  The  evidence  available  to  date  strongly  in- 
dicates that,  under  certain  conditions  of  administration, 
Form  5.22  of  the  Nature  of  Proof  test  provides  a  valid  meas- 
ure of  a  certain  range  of  behavior  relative  to  the  nature  of 
proof  objective. 

For  the  purpose  of  statistical  analysis,  307  students — 115 
finishing  grade  ten,  96  in  grade  eleven,  and  96  in  grade 


148        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

twelve — were  selected.  These  students  were  all  attending 
public  high  schools  when  tested  and  composed  five  classes 
in  grade  ten,  three  classes  in  grade  eleven,  and  four  classes 
in  grade  twelve.  The  five  classes  in  grade  ten,  and  one  of 
the  classes  in  grade  twelve,  were  then  completing  a  course 
which  emphasized  the  nature  of  proof  objective.23  In  the  re- 
maining groups  there  was  an  awareness  of  this  objective,  but 
less  specific  attention  to  it.  The  results  of  the  study  seem  to 
indicate  that  at  the  present  there  would  be  little  advantage 
in  computing  grade  norms,  since  the  emphasis  given  to  the 
objective  has  more  influence  on  the  scores  than  does  the 
grade  placement  of  the  students  from  the  tenth  to  the  twelfth 
grades. 

The  statistical  data  presented  in  Appendix  II,  Table  7, 
are  based  on  this  population  of  307  students.  Within  limita- 
tions these  data  would  apply  to  other  groups  of  students  in 
the  tenth,  eleventh,  and  twelfth  grades.  If  a  chosen  group  is 
comparable  to  the  sample  group,  the  statistical  constants  pre- 
sented in  Appendix  II,  Table  7,  will  provide  enough  basic 
information  to  enable  the  teacher  trained  in  statistics  to 
study  the  significance  of  changes  in  the  mean  scores  of  a 
class  or  in  the  scores  of  an  individual  student.  The  reliabil- 
ities of  the  various  scores  are  in  general  not  as  high  as  have 
been  obtained  in  other  tests  of  thinking  abilities.  A  number 
of  the  scores  are,  however,  fairly  reliable,  and  it  is  a  reason- 
able hypothesis  that  the  interpretations  drawn  on  the  basis 
of  a  careful  examination  of  the  patterns  of  scores  are  more 
trustworthy  than  the  reliability  of  the  separate  scores  would 
suggest. 

A  RELATED  INSTRUMENT 

A  group  of  objectives  which  are  closely  related  to  those 
discussed  in  connection  with  the  discussion  of  Logical  Rea- 

23  This  course  followed  somewhat  the  pattern  outlined  by  Fawcett,  loo. 
cit. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  149 

soning  and  the  Nature  of  Proof  relate  to  what  is  popularly 
known  as  "propaganda  analysis."  During  the  Eight- Year 
Study  some  attention  was  given  to  evaluation  with  respect 
to  these  objectives.  This  section  will  give  a  brief  account  of 
this  project. 

The  definition  of  propaganda  which  was  adopted  is  as  fol- 
lows: Propaganda  represents  any  use  of  the  spoken  or  writ- 
ten word,  or  other  forms  of  symbolization  (pictures,  movies, 
plays)  designed  to  convince  people  to  hold  certain  opin- 
ions, to  give  allegiance  to  a  particular  group  or  cause,  or  to 
pursue  some  land  of  social  action  predetermined  by  the 
source  of  the  propaganda.  As  used  in  this  sense,  propaganda 
has  no  unpleasant  or  tcbad"  overtones.  Our  concern  with  it  is 
to  better  understand  which  groups  are  selling  what  kind  of 
propaganda;  the  possible  social  consequences  and  implica- 
tions of  this;  the  symbol  appeals  which  are  used  and  their 
relation  to  behavior  dynamics  of  individuals;  the  relation  of 
susceptibility  to  propaganda  to  social  conditions;  etc. 

Propaganda  also  is  used  to  characterize  forms  of  argument 
which  are  untenable  in  terms  of  certain  intellectual  or  logical 
criteria  such  as:  documenting  evidence,  presenting  several 
sides  of  a  problem,  drawing  conclusions  which  follow  logi- 
cally from  the  data,  minimizing  the  use  of  slogans  and  "emo- 
tional" terms,  etc.  Used  in  this  sense  propaganda  does  have 
unpleasant  overtones  and  our  problem  is  to  teach  pupils  to 
react  critically  to  it  by  applying  criteria  of  good  argument. 
The  scope  of  this  report  takes  both  of  these  definitions  into 
consideration. 

Among  the  behaviors  which  were  listed  as  important  ob- 
jectives of  education  related  to  propaganda  analysis  were  the 
following: 

a.  Recognition  of  the  purposes  of  authors  of  propaganda — 
that  is,  ability  to  make  more  discriminating  judgments  as 
to  the  points  of  view  which  it  is  intended  the  consumer 


150        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

should  accept  or  reject.  (In  the  broad  sense,  this  refers 
to  the  generally  accepted  concept  of  "reading  compre- 
hension.") 

b.  Identification  of  the  forms  of  argument  used  in  selected 
statements  of  propaganda.  (This  refers  to  reading  com- 
prehension in  a  different  sense.) 

c.  Recognition  of  forms  of  argument  which  are  considered 
intellectually  acceptable  and  which  are  not  employed  in 
certain  statements. 

d.  Critical  reaction  to  the  forms  of  argument  which  repre- 
sent typical  devices  employed  in  propaganda. 

e.  Ability  to  analyze  argument  in  terms  of  principles  of  the 
nature  of  proof. 

£  Recognition  of  the  relation  of  propaganda  to  the  social 

forces  which  breed  it. 
g.  Knowledge  of  the  psychological  mechanisms  involved  in 

the  susceptibility  of  people  to  certain  language  symbols. 

The  evaluation  instrument  entitled  Analysis  of  Contro- 
versial Writing  (Form  5.31)  was  developed  to  obtain  evi- 
dence concerning  the  achievement  of  the  first  four  behaviors 
listed  above.  Item  e  in  the  list  has  been  discussed  at  some 
length  above.  The  others,  although  they  were  considered 
important  and  some  preliminary  analyses  of  them  were 
made,  were  not  explored  during  the  study.  The  test  contains 
ten  samples  of  writing  on  controversial  issues  selected  from 
magazines  and  newspapers.  The  choices  were  made  on  the 
basis  of  the  following  criteria:  (1)  the  selection  should 
focus  upon  a  controversial  issue;  (2)  liberal  and  conserva- 
tive sources  were  represented  on  each  issue;  (3)  the  group 
of  selections  should  make  use  of  a  variety  of  propaganda 
devices;  (4)  the  issues  involved  should  represent  areas  of 
tension  for  pupils. 

In  each  problem  the  pupils  were  first  directed  to  read  the 
quotation  carefully,  and  then  in  Part  I  to  mark  them  so  as  to 
indicate  statements  where  there  is: 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  151 

A.  evidence  that  the  author  of  the  quotation  wants  you  to 
agree  with  or  accept  the  idea  in  the  statement. 

B.  evidence  that  the  author  wants  you  to  disagree  with  or 
reject  the  idea  in  the  statement. 

C.  no  evidence  as  to  whether  the  author  wants  you  to  agree 
or  disagree  with  the  idea  in  the  statement. 

Twelve  statements  follow  these  directions.  The  examples 
below  are  taken  from  Problem  I,  based  on  a  selection  whose 
tenor  may  be  judged  from  the  closing  sentence  in  one  para- 
graph: "The  American  system  of  private  industry  and  busi- 
ness has  distributed  more  income  to  more  people  than  any 
other  system  in  the  history  of  the  world." 

1.  The  present  purchasing  power  of  workers  is  possible  only 
under  a  system  of  private  ownership  of  industry. 

2.  Workers  should  receive  higher  wages  than  they  receive 
at  present. 

3.  The  present  system  of  private  ownership  is  superior  to 
any  other  way  of  organizing  industry. 

4.  Industry  still  has  far  to  go  in  distributing  wealth  more 
evenly  between  the  workers  and  the  owners. 

5.  The  profits  of  corporations  should  be  turned  over  to  the 
workers  rather  than  to  stockholders. 

In  Part  II,  the  student  was  to  decide:24 

first,  which  of  the  following  statements  represent  forms  of  argu- 
ments used  by  the  author  in  this  situation,  and  second,  which 
ones  represent  desirable  forms  of  argument  whether  used  by  the 
author  or  not. 

1.  Assumes  that  the  point  of  view  expressed  in  the  article  is 
that  which  is  held  by  the  majority  of  Americans. 

2.  Gives  facts  in  such  a  way  that  the  reader  can  check  their 
source  to  see  whether  they  have  been  reported  accurately. 

3.  Uses  statistics  for  industries  in  which  wages  are  among 
the  highest  to  illustrate  the  rise  in  wages. 

24  The  following  quotation  is  an  excerpt  from  the  directions. 


152        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

4.  Presents  some  of  the  major  advantages  and  disadvantages 
of  our  system  of  private  ownership  of  industry. 

5.  Indicates  that  there  will  be  undesirable  consequences  to 
industry  if  our  present  industrial  system  is  changed. 

6.  Tries   to   make   us   feel   sympathetic   toward   ID  dus  trial 
owners. 

Ten  statements  of  this  general  sort  were  used  in  Part  II  of 
each  Problem.  In  both  parts  the  various  statements  were  so 
chosen  that  a  student  responding  according  to  the  direc- 
tions could  reveal  evidence  of  his  status  with  respect  to  the 
first  four  behaviors  listed  above. 

The  scores  of  die  pupils  in  Part  I  are  tabulated  in  the  fol- 
lowing descriptive  categories:25 

General  Objectivity.  Scores  in  this  category  represent  the  per 
cent  of  total  correct  responses  and  show  the  relative  objectiv- 
ity with  which  the  pupil  interprets  highly  biased  material. 

Non-Recognition  of  conflicting  points  of  view.  Pupils  who  have 
difficulty  in  recognizing  ideas  which  are  contradicted  by  the 
author's  data  can  be  identified  through  scores  in  this  category. 

Misconception  of  authors  purposes.  Scores  in  this  category  indi- 
cate a  pupil's  tendency  to  attribute  conservative  ideas  to  liberal 
articles  and  liberal  ideas  to  conservative  articles.  Such  scores 
indicate  a  kind  of  gross  error  in  judgment  and,  if  relatively 
large,  suggest  inability  of  the  pupil  to  comprehend  the  general 
ideas  which  the  authors  are  trying  to  sell  to  the  reader. 

Suggestibility.  Scores  in  this  category  indicate  the  extent  to  which 
the  pupil  indiscriminately  attributes  conservative  ideas  to  con- 
servative articles  and  liberal  ideas  to  the  liberal  articles.  (A 
score  of  this  kind  means  that  the  pupil  says  that  the  author 
wants  him  to  "accept"  an  idea  which  is  keyed  "insufficient  evi- 
dence." The  items  keyed  "insufficient  evidence'*  reflect  the 
general  point  of  view  in  the  articles. ) 

Except  for  the  category  "general  objectivity/'  the  scores 
in  Part  I  categories  are  separated  into  "liberal"  and  "conserv- 

25  A  more  detailed  description  of  how  these  categories  are  derived  from 
the  test  scores  and  how  they  are  to  be  interpreted  can  be  found  in  the 
"Explanation  Sheet  and  Interpretation  Guide"  for  Form  5.31. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  153 

alive/*  Thus  in  the  "suggestibility"  category  each  pupil  has 
two  scores,  one  showing  his  suggestibility  in  interpreting  the 
conservative  articles  and  one  showing  suggestibility  toward 
the  liberal  articles. 

The  scores  on  Part  II  are  tabulated  according  to  the  fol- 
lowing categories: 

Identification  of  propaganda  techniques  used  in  the  articles.  This 
category  indicates  the  degree  to  which  the  pupil  can  recognize 
the  use  of  the  forms  of  argument  keyed  as  "propaganda  tech- 
niques." 

Confusion  of  propaganda  techniques  used  and  not  used.  This 
category  shows  the  extent  to  which  the  pupil  indicates  that 
the  techniques  keyed  as  "not  used"  were  used  in  the  articles. 

Uncritical  toward  the  use  of  propaganda  techniques.  The  tend- 
ency of  the  pupil  to  approve  the  use  of  propaganda  techniques 
is  indicated  under  this  heading. 

Recognition  of  acceptable  nature  of  certain  forms  of  argument. 
Recorded  in  this  category  are  scores  showing  whether  the  pupil 
approves  of  the  use  of  the  acceptable  forms  of  argument. 

Gullibility.  Scores  in  gullibility  show  the  tendency  of  the  pupil 
to  indicate  that  the  acceptable  forms  of  argument  keyed  as 
"not  used"  are  used  in  the  articles.  Due  to  the  nature  of  the 
test  items,  gullibility  means  attributing  "fairness,"  "impartial- 
ity," "open  mindedness"  to  the  authors  of  the  articles. 

In  constructing  Part  I  of  the  test,  the  basic  hypothesis  was 
that  pupils  whose  attitudes  toward  the  five  social  issues  in- 
cluded in  the  test  were  strongly  liberal  would  tend  to  be 
more  "suggestible'*  toward  the  conservative  articles  than  to- 
ward the  liberal  articles.  This  was  based  on  the  notion  that 
the  liberal  pupil  would  more  willingly  exaggerate  the  ideas 
of  conservative  authors  than  those  of  liberal  authors.  Simi- 
larly it  was  believed  that  the  scores  of  such  pupils  in  the 
other  columns  of  Part  I  would  tend  to  differ  as  between  the 
sub-categories  "liberal"  and  "conservative."  To  check  this 
hypothesis  an  attitude  scale  consisting  of  items  used  in  the 
test  was  given  to  approximately  one  hundred  pupils.  These 


154        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

same  pupils  took  Form  5.31  and  their  attitudes  were  com- 
pared  with  scores  in  the  "suggestibility"  category  in  the  test. 
This  study  showed  that  "liberal"  pupils  were  no  more  sug- 
gestible toward  conservative  articles  than  conservative  pu- 
pils, and  vice  versa.  Furthermore,  a  study  of  test  scores  has 
shown  that  most  pupils  tend  to  be  equally  suggestible  toward 
conservative  and  liberal  articles.  This  same  tendency  is  char* 
acteristic  of  the  other  categories  in  Part  I. 

The  conclusion  justified  from  these  findings  is  that  the  test 
does  not  discriminate  sharply  between  the  reactions  of  lib- 
eral and  conservative  pupils  in  their  interpretation  of  the 
purposes  of  the  propaganda  articles.  Sharper  differences  are 
discovered  when  scores  on  individual  articles  are  compared, 
for  example,  scores  on  the  liberal  and  conservative  articles 
dealing  with  the  issue  of  socialized  medicine.  This  procedure 
Is  cumbersome,  however,  and  would  be  impractical  for  use 
with  large  classes.  Other  hypotheses  underlying  the  test  seem 
to  be  reasonably  valid.  As  one  phase  of  a  validity  study,  50 
essays  by  pupils  who  analyzed  a  subtle  piece  of  propaganda 
as  part  of  a  unit  of  work  on  this  subject  were  compared  with 
the  test  results.  The  studies  of  validity  and  reliability  are 
not  complete,  however.  The  instrument  has  been  described 
because  it  illustrates  an  approach  to  this  problem  which  is 
somewhat  unique  and  which  warrants  further  study. 

CONCLUSION 

The  two  principal  uses  for  these  types  of  instruments  are: 
( 1 )  the  diagnosis  and  description  of  the  strengths  and  weak- 
nesses of  individual  students  and  of  groups  of  students  in 
relation  to  the  objectives  as  they  have  been  operationally 
defined  in  the  tests;  (2)  the  measurement  of  growth  in  the 
abilities  required  for  successful  achievement.  The  scores  on 
the  data  sheets  will  yield  significant  descriptions  of  students 
with  respect  to  the  objectives.  The  interpreter  must,  how- 
ever, clearly  understand  the  structure  of  the  test  problems 
and  the  relationship  of  this  structure  to  the  problem-solving 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  155 

process.  For  certain  students  the  interpreter  may  desire  even 
more  detailed  evidence  from  the  test  results  than  that  which 
appears  on  the  data  sheet.  An  examination  of  the  responses 
of  a  particular  student  to  certain  items  on  a  test  may  yield 
such  evidence.  More  often  the  suggestions  raised  by  an  ex- 
amination of  the  data  sheet  will  lead  the  teacher  to  seek 
evidence  from  other  sources  to  confirm  or  deny  these  sug- 
gestions. For  example,  a  student  may  reveal  a  tendency  to 
use  many  reasons  on  the  nature  of  proof  test  but  fail  to 
discriminate  between  relevant  and  irrelevant  reasons.  This 
tendency  may  or  may  not  be  confirmed  by  the  teacher's  ex- 
perience with  the  student  in  daily  classroom  activities. 

The  uses  of  these  instruments  are  not  fundamentally  dif- 
ferent from  those  of  many  other  types  of  tests.  Thus  after 
studying  the  test  results  the  teacher  may  wish  to  provide 
curriculum  experiences  designed  to  overcome  obvious  weak- 
nesses of  a  group  as  a  whole,  or  of  individuals  within  the 
group.  This  may  lead  to  a  special  unit  of  work  for  the  whole 
class;  special  assignments  undertaken  by  a  particular  student 
with  the  advice  of  the  teacher;  special  attention  by  the 
teacher  to  certain  details  of  the  written  work  handed  in  by 
one  or  more  of  the  students;  and  the  like.  In  other  cases, 
growth  toward  this  objective  might  be  one  of  the  desired 
outcomes  of  the  work  of  a  class  over  a  longer  period  of  time. 
For  example,  every  activity  of  a  class  over  a  period  of  a  year 
might  be  designed  to  make  some  contribution  to  the  students* 
concept  of  proof. 

In  this  connection  it  will  be  useful  to  measure  the  growth 
of  individuals  and  of  classes  toward  the  objective.  Although 
the  students  may  remember  the  general  nature  of  these  tests 
for  several  months,  they  can  scarcely  be  expected  to  re- 
member the  anwers  to  specific  items  on  the  test.  Hence  the 
practice  effect  of  taking  the  tests  once  will  probably  not  be 
a  serious  factor  influencing  the  scores  on  a  second  administra- 
tion of  a  test  several  months  later.  If  such  studies  of  growth 
are  desired,  it  is  especially  important,  of  course,  that  the 


1 56        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

specific  exercises  in  the  tests  should  not  be  "taken  up  in 
class/'  It  is  also  important  to  keep  in  mind  the  effect  of  the 
total  testing  situation  upon  the  test  results.  This  total  situa- 
tion involves  more  than  a  careful  explanation  of  the  test 
directions  to  students,  and  the  provision  of  adequate  time  for 
the  completion  of  the  test.  In  the  case  of  many  tests,  and 
particularly  those  which  have  been  described,  it  involves 
also  the  "readiness"  of  the  class  for  the  test,  their  attitude 
toward  the  test  as  a  diagnostic  instrument  rather  than  as  a 
marking  device,  and  the  like.  Ideally,  the  class  should  look 
upon  these  tests  as  an  opportunity  to  demonstrate  their  abil- 
ity to  do  clear  thinking  rather  than  as  a  burden  and  a  threat. 
The  chief  feature  of  all  of  these  tests  is  the  extent  to  which 
they  make  possible  a  description  of  a  student's  thinking  abil- 
ity in  terms  of  at  least  tentative  answers  to  a  series  of  ques- 
tions which  are  quite  general  and  comprehensive.  Success- 
ful performance  depends  relatively  little,  compared  with  the 
usual  achievement  test,  upon  knowledge  of  particular  bodies 
of  subject-matter  content,  and  relatively  much  upon  broad 
principles  of  science  and  of  scientific  thinking.  The  objec- 
tives demanded  tests  to  probe  among  the  higher  mental 
processes  applied  not  to  materials  of  the  sort  commonly  used 
in  psychological  investigations,  but  rather  to  those  commonly 
found  in  reading  of  newspapers  and  magazines,  or  elsewhere 
in  daily  life.  This  approach  is  fundamentally  different  from 
one  which  seeks  to  synthesize  a  description  of  a  student's 
thinking  abilities  from  data  on  many  simpler  but  more  read- 
ily controllable  psychological  reactions.  The  experience  of 
the  Evaluation  Staff  has  been  that  this  endeavor  has  led  to 
increasing  complexity  in  the  test  instruments  in  spite  of  the 
demands  of  practicality  for  greater  simplicity.  This  increas- 
ing complexity  was  tolerated  in  order  to  maintain  close  cor- 
respondence between  the  stated  objectives  and  the  behavior 
demanded  of  the  student,  and  in  the  hope  that  the  instru- 
ments of  this  sort  may  eventually  be  simplified. 


Chapter  III 

EVALUATION  OF  SOCIAL  SENSITIVITY 
<<<<<<<<<<<<<<*  <&<<<<<<<<<<<^^ 

INTRODUCTION 

Origin  and  Scope  of  the  Objectives  Related  to  the 
Development  of  Social  Sensitivity 

In  any  social  situation,  an  individual  is  aware  of,  and  re- 
sponds to,  certain 'factors  and  lets  others  go  unnoticed.  Thus, 
on  observing  an  old  man  selling  apples  on  the  street  comer, 
one  individual  may  be  aware  only  of  the  convenience  of 
having  apples  easily  available  to  him,  or  be  annoyed  at  hav- 
ing the  man  clutter  up  the  street  comer.  The  awareness  and 
attendant  feelings  in  this  case  are  self -centered;  there  is  little 
consideration  for  the  apple  man.  Another  person  may  "see" 
primarily  an  old  man  trying  to  make  a  living.  He  may  in 
addition  feel  sympathy  for  a  man  who  has  to  make  a  living 
in  such  a  precarious  way,  or  feel  that  this  way  of  earning  a 
living  is  the  man's  just  due,  determined  by  his  ability.  Atten- 
tion in  this  case  is  centered  on  the  apple  man  as  a  human 
being.  To  a  third  person  this  experience  may  suggest  the 
problem  of  security  in  old  age.  He  may  wonder  why  there  is 
not  a  more  satisfactory  provision  for  old  people  to  make  their 
living.  Awareness  and  sympathy  in  this  case  center  not  only 
on  the  apple  man.  He  becomes  a  symbol  for  a  whole  group 
of  people,  or  for  an  issue,  and  sympathy  for  him  is  likely  to 
evoke  concern  for  the  problem  or  issue  which  he  symbolizes. 
Depending  on  the  type  of  response,  various  impulses  to  ac- 
tion may  also  suggest  themselves.  Annoyance  with  the  apple 
man  may  suggest  activity  leading  to  his  removal.  Sympathy 
toward  him  may  lead  to  consideration  of  ways  of  helping 

157 


158        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

"him.  Concern  about  injustice  in  the  social  order  tends  to  sug- 
gest the  need  for  correcting  them. 

Several  different  behaviors  are  involved  in  these  responses. 
Personal  sympathies  and  aversions  largely  determine  the  pat- 
tern of  initial  awareness.  The  knowledge  one  possesses,  and 
the  attitudes  and  viewpoints  one  has,  determine  how  one 
interprets  the  experience.  On*,  "s  ability  and  inclination  to 
relate  and  reorganize  ideas  gained  from  previous  experiences 
and  to  apply  them  to  the  new  situation  add  insight.  The 
inclination  and  ability  to  relate  the  feelings  evoked  and  posi- 
tions taken  in  specific  situations  to  more  general  and  abstract 
ideas  add  to  both  the  coherence  and  the  depth  of  one's  in- 
sights in  a  given  case.  All  of  these  behaviors,  although  capa- 
ble of  analytic  distinction,  are  related  to  each  other  in  any 
given  experience. 

The  term  "social  sensitivity"  has  been  used  to  refer  to  this 
complex  of  responses.  In  the  common  usage  of  the  term  the 
emotional  factors — such  as  the  feelings  of  sympathy  or  aver- 
sion, attitudes  of  approval  or  disapproval — have  been  em- 
phasized. However,  this  term  can  also  be  used  to  connote  the 
intellectual  responses — such  as  the  range  and  quality  of  the 
elements  perceived  in  a  given  experience  or  the  significance 
of  the  ideas  associated  with  it. 

In  the  first  statements  of  objectives  submitted  by  the 
schools  in  the  Eight- Year  Study  the  term  "social"  was  used 
in  connection  with  many  types  of  behavior  somewhat  similar 
to  the  ones  described  above.  Frequent  among  the  statements 
were  terms  such  as  social  consciousness,  social  awareness, 
social  concern,  social  attitudes,  social  integration,  sense  of 
social  responsibility,  social  understanding,  social  intelligence. 
Thus  many  schools  seemed  interested  in  promoting  a  greater 
awareness  of  social  aspects  of  the  immediate  scene  as  well  as 
of  the  issues  underlying  current  social  problems.  At  the  same 
time  concern  was  expressed  that  unless  students  achieve 
clarification  of  their  personal  patterns  of  social  values  and 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  159 

beliefs,  intelligent  social  thinking  would  remain  an  elusive 
object  of  educational  effort.  The  apparent  blocking  of  ra- 
tional thought  by  personal  prejudices  and  biases,  by  a 
warped  sense  of  values,  or  by  the  tendency  to  react  in  terms 
of  social  stereotypes,  was  recognized,  and  many  statements 
of  objectives  emphasized  the  importance  of  a  clearer,  more 
consistent,  and  more  objective  pattern  of  social  values  and 
beliefs.  A  good  deal  of  attention  was  also  devoted  to  the 
problem  of  helping  students  apply  the  values,  loyalties,  and 
beliefs  they  developed  to  an  increasing  range  of  life  prob- 
lems. The  term  "social  sensitivity"  was  adopted  to  serve  as  a 
consolidating  focus  for  this  apparently  heterogeneous  yet 
highly  related  complex  of  objectives. 

In  order  to  see  more  clearly  what  was  implied  in  these 
statements  of  objectives  from  the  schools,  two  committees 
were  established.  These  committees  undertook  to  make  a 
coherent  analysis  of  social  sensitivity  as  a  total  objective  and 
to  clarify  and  specify  some  of  the  more  crucial  aspects  of  it 
sufficiently  to  lay  a  foundation  for  the  development  of  eval- 
uation instruments.  Some  of  the  significant  aspects  of  social 
sensitivity  which  were  emphasized  in  the  course  of  the 
analysis  are  described  in  the  following  section. 

Significant  Aspects  of  Social  Sensitivity 

The  first  exploratory  meetings  of  the  committees  revealed 
a  diversity  of  concepts  regarding  social  sensitivity.  In  the 
course  of  the  discussion  sensitivity  was  defined,  by  implica- 
tion, as  awareness,  ways  of  thinking,  interest,  attitude,  and 
knowledge.  A  whole  range  of  problems  representing  signifi- 
cant areas  of  social  sensitivity  was  also  mentioned.  These 
ranged  from  such  "immediate"  social  patterns  as  relations 
with  other  people  to  such  general  social  issues  as  unemploy- 
ment, effective  democracy,  and  social  justice. 

To  get  a  clearer  and  a  more  concrete  picture  of  the  specific 
behavior  involved  in  this  objective,  the  committee  under- 


160        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

took  to  collect  anecdotal  recordings  of  behavior  incidents 
illustrating  any  aspect  of  social  sensitivity  which  teachers  in 
the  Thirty  Schools  thought  important.  This  material  was 
carefully  analyzed  and  the  various  types  of  specific  behavior 
were  listed.  Altogether,  74  types  of  behavior  were  indicated 
or  implied  by  the  anecdotes  submitted  by  committee  mem- 
bers and  other  teachers.  The  list  below  gives  a  few  illustra- 
tions: 

1.  The  student  frequently  expresses  concern  about  so- 
cial problems,  issues,  and  events  in  conversation,  free 
writing,  creative  expression,  class  discussion. 

2.  The  student  is  fairly  well  informed  on  social  topics; 
he  has  a  reasonable  background  and  perspective,  and 
would  not  often  be  misled  by  misstatements. 

3.  When  facing  a  new  situation,  problem  or  idea,  he 
is  eager  for  more  information,  seeks  to  identify  sig- 
nificant factors  in  the  situation,  carries  thought  be- 
yond the  immediate  data. 

4.  He  is  critical  about  expressed  attitudes  and  opinions 
and  does  not  accept  them  unquestioningly;  distin- 
guishes statements  of  fact  from  opinion  or  rumors, 
discerns  motives  and  prejudices. 

5.  He  is  able  to  discern  relevant  issues  and  relationships 
in  problems,  ideas,  and  data.  He  relates  ideas  widely 
and  significantly  and  discriminates  among  issues. 

6.  He  judges  problems  and  issues  in  terms  of  situations, 
issues,  purposes,  and  consequences  involved  rather 
than  in  terms  of  fixed,  dogmatic  precepts,  or  emo- 
tionally wishful  thinking. 

7.  He  reads  newspapers,  magazines,  and  books  on  social 
topics. 

8.  He  is  able  to  formulate  a  personal  point  of  view;  he 
applies  it  to  an  increasingly  broader  range  of  issues 
and  problems. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  161 

9.  He  is  increasingly  consistent  in  his  point  of  view. 
10.  He  participates  effectively  in  groups  concerned  with 
social  action. 

A  classification  of  these  behaviors  resulted  in  the  following 
list  of  major  aspects  of  social  sensitivity  of  concern  to  teachers 

in  the  Thirty  Schools: 

1.  Social  thinking;  e.g.,  the  ability  (a)  to  get  significant 
meaning  from  social  facts,  (b)  to  apply  social  facts 
and  generalizations  to  new  problems,  (c)  to  respond 
critically  and  discriminatingly  to   ideas   and   argu- 
ments. (Statements  4  and  5  above,  for  example,  would 
fall  into  this  classification. ) 

2.  Social  attitudes,  beliefs,  and  values;  e.g.,  the  basic 
personal   positions,   feelings,   and   concerns   toward 
social  phenomena,  institutions,  and  issues.    (State- 
ments 8  and  9. ) 

3.  Social  awareness;  that  is,  the  range  and  quality  of 
factors  or  elements  perceived  in  a  situation.  (State- 
ments 1  and  6.) 

4.  Social  interests  as  revealed  by  liking  to  engage  in 
socially  significant  activities.  (Statements  3,  7,  and 
10.) 

5.  Social  information;  that  is,  familiarity  with  facts  and 
generalizations  relevant  to  significant  social  problems. 
(Statements  2  and  3.) 

6.  Skill  in  social  action,  involving  familiarity  with  the 
techniques  of  social  action  as  well  as  ability  to  use 
them.  (Statement  10.) 

The  committee  on  social  sensitivity  took  the  responsibility 
for  developing  instruments  for  evaluating  three  of  these  as- 
pects; namely,  the  ability  to  apply  social  generalizations  and 
facts  to  social  problems,  social  attitudes,  and  social  aware- 
ness. The  present  chapter  is  chiefly  devoted  to  a  description 


i6a        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

of  the  instruments  pertaining  to  these  aspects.  Instruments 
dealing  with  other  phases  of  social  thinking — such  as  the 
interpretation  of  social  data,  and  critical  reactions  to  argu- 
ments and  propaganda — have  been  discussed  in  the  chapter 
on  Aspects  of  Thinking,  The  appraisal  of  social  interests 
is  discussed  in  the  chapter  on  Interests.  No  new  instruments 
were  developed  to  evaluate  the  acquisition  of  social  informa- 
tion, primarily  because  published  tests  were  already  avail- 
able and  because  teachers  felt  relatively  little  need  of  assist- 
ance in  this  task.  As  far  as  securing  evidence  of  skill  in  social 
action  is  concerned,  observational  records  seemed  to  be  the 
most  effective  method.  These  are  discussed  briefly  in  the 
following  section. 

INFORMAL  METHODS  OF  GETTING  EVIDENCE  ON  SOCIAL 
SENSITIVITY 

An  objective  which  involves  as  diverse  types  of  behavior 
as  those  described  in  the  preceding  section  obviously  neces- 
sitates the  use  of  several  approaches  and  several  techniques 
for  its  appraisal.  These  will  include  paper-and-pencil  tests  as 
well  as  observational  techniques,  each  being  employed  ac- 
cording to  its  appropriateness  to  the  behavior  that  is  being 
evaluated.  Thus  the  ability  to  think  through  social  problems 
can  be  adequately  appraised  by  using  paper-and-pencil  tests. 
For  the  evaluation  of  some  other  aspects  of  social  sensitivity, 
such  as  the  identification  of  social  beliefs,  paper-and-pencil 
tests  are  recommended  chiefly  because  they  are  economical 
and  because  these  behaviors  are  rather  difficult  to  observe 
directly  and  objectively.  Still  other  types  of  behavior,  such  as 
the  disposition  to  act  on  one's  beliefs,  or  the  degree  of  par- 
ticipation in  social  action  and  in  discussion  of  social  prob- 
lems, require  direct  observation  of  overt  behavior.  Many  of 
these  observational  and  informal  techniques  involve  only  a 
more  effective  use  of  procedures  employed  and  materials 
secured  in  the  course  of  normal  teaching  procedures. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  163 

Anecdotal  records  are  an  effective  way  of  securing  con- 
crete descriptions  of  significant  behavior  of  individuals  or 
groups.  Since  they  are  a  way  of  recording  direct  observa- 
tions, anecdotal  records  are  appropriate  for  securing  evi- 
dence on  all  types  of  overt  behavior.  However,  since  such 
a  descriptive  record  is  highly  time-consuming,  the  function 
of  anecdotal  records  in  a  comprehensive  evaluation  program 
is  usually  supplementary:  to  give  vivid,  intimate,  concrete 
material  to  help  make  more  meaningful  other  more  sys- 
tematic but  less  colorful  types  of  evidence.  The  nature  and 
role  of  anecdotes  and  the  criteria  for  selecting  and  writing 
them  have  been  described  elsewhere.1  Here  it  may  suffice  to 
give  a  few  illustrations  of  anecdotes  pertaining  to  social 
sensitivity. 

A  disposition  on  the  part  of  a  group  to  consider  the  effects 
of  one's  actions  upon  the  welfare  of  others,  and  to  apply 
ethical  principles  in  making  decisions,  is  illustrated  by  the 
following  incident: 

The  school  newspaper  had  been  supported  by  the  income  from 
advertising  solicited  from  small  neighborhood  stores  which  the 
students  did  not  patronize.  A  student  questioned  the  ethics  of 
such  a  procedure  in  the  student  government  assembly.  Others  in 
charge  of  the  business  management  of  the  paper  defended  the 
method  on  the  grounds  that  it  was  a  general  practice  with  school 
papers  and  there  was  no  other  way  of  supporting  a  printed  pub- 
lication. Another  group  proposed  other  ways  of  earning  money, 
involving  more  work  on  the  part  of  the  student  body.  The  latter 
suggestion  was  accepted. 

Class  discussion  often  reveals  the  degree  to  which  students 

1  The  Commission  on  Secondary  School  Curriculum,  The  Social  Studies 
in  General  Education,  D.  Appleton-Century  Company  (New  York,  1940), 
pp.  347-50. 

L.  L.  Jarvie  and  Mark  Ellingson,  A  Handbook  on  the  Anecdotal  Behavior 
Journal,  University  of  Chicago  Press  (Chicago,  1940). 

Arthur  E.  Traxler,  The  Nature  and  Use  of  Anecdotal  Records,  Educa- 
tional Records  Bureau  (New  York,  1939).  Mimeographed. 


164        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

are  capable  of  using  present  events  to  speculate  about  their 
consequences. 

In  connection  with  a  report  of  a  demonstration  by  members  of 
the  League  for  Industrial  Democracy  protesting  against  the  "Rex" 
sailing  with  munitions  for  abroad,  speculation  was  aroused  re- 
garding the  consequences  of  an  embargo.  How  effective  would 
government  control  of  the  sale  of  munitions  be?  What  devious 
ways,  such  as  selling  to  a  neutral  country,  would  be  devised? 
(This  discussion  occurred  during  the  Italian  conquest  of 
Ethiopia. ) 

Personal  attitudes  toward  social  issues  are  often  reflected  in 
the  daily  incidents  in  the  school,  as  in  the  following: 

Gene  came  into  my  room,  explaining  that  she  had  had  an  argu- 
ment with  some  members  of  her  group  over  their  attitudes  dur- 
ing trips  they  had  made  to  Harlem  and  the  East  Side  of  New 
York,  Jane  had  told  her  that  she  could  not  see  how  anybody  could 
like  slumming.  Gene  had  objected  to  such  an  attitude,  since  the 
purpose  of  the  trip  was  to  study  the  living  conditions  of  people 
in  an  unfortunate  situation.  To  her,  she  said,  those  trips,  together 
with  the  study  of  housing  and  income,  had  been  one  of  the  most 
meaningful  experiences.  She  wants  to  write  on  that  problem.2 

Students'  writing  presents  other  opportunities  for  securing 
evidence  on  social  sensitivity.  Much  writing  contains  some 
expression  of  social  attitudes  and  of  social  values  held  by  the 
author,  provided  its  content  is  analyzed  from  that  standpoint. 
Often  only  a  listing  of  the  topics  chosen  for  creative  writing 
over  a  period  of  time  or  for  free  choice  "research"  reveals 
trends  in  social  sensitivity.  Thus,  frequent  choice  of  social 
problems  to  write  about  or  frequent  empjiasis  on  social  con- 
text and  social  implications  is  an  indication  of  real  interest 
in  social  matters.  Free  choice  writing,  however,  provides 

2  It  is  possible  to  interpret  the  incidents  given  above  in  several  different 
ways.  A  single  incident  does  not  necessarily  prove  anything  about  the 
behavior  of  an  individual  and  a  number  of  anecdotes  covering  a  period  of 
time  must  be  collected  before  any  generalization  is  attempted. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  165 

only  sporadic  evidence,  and  not  necessarily  on  the  particular 
aspects  of  behavior  a  teacher  may  wish  to  explore.  To  secure 
more  systematic  evidence,  controlled  assignments  in  which 
all  students  respond  to  the  same  general  problem,  issue,  or 
experience,  are  often  employed.  Below  is  a  sample  of  written 
responses  to  the  following  paragraph  assigned  as  a  topic  to 
the  whole  class:  "Nothing  can  be  done  about  poverty.  There 
have  been  and  always  will  be  poor  people,  incapable  people, 

unambitious  people,  dirty  work  to  do,  survival  of  the  fit- 

•     t          " 
test  .  .  . 

Roy:  I  think  something  could  be  done  about  poverty.  They  could 
be  taught  many  things  they  have  no  chance  to  learn  today.  They 
should  be  housed  in  a  healthy  environment.  I  think  there  will 
always  be  poor  people,  unambitious  people,  incapable  people, 
and  dirty  work  to  do,  but  I  do  not  think  that  a  very  great  per- 
centage of  the  poor  today  are  poor  because  of  these  reasons. 
They  don't  have  a  chance.  I  don't  think  that  42  per  cent  of  the 
Americans  today  fall  into  that  lazy  and  unambitious  class,  yet 
42  per  cent  of  Americans  are  poor.  There  must  be  something 
wrong  with  our  system  today. 

John:  I  can  find  little  pity  for  white  and  colored  trash  who  have 
never  amounted  to  anything.  ...  I  think  that  the  smarter  man 
should  make  more  money  and  that  it  would  wreck  any  advance- 
ment of  civilization  so  to  restrict  initiative  as  to  pay  the  man 
who  carries  twice  the  load  as  much  as  the  mass  below  him  gets. 
Mary:  Very  few  people  would  at  any  time  ...  be  willing  to 
give  their  money  away.  Of  course,  they  can  be  made  to  give  it 
to  the  government,  but  it  seems  to  me  to  be  a  shame  if  people 
are  taxed  so  heavy  to  aid  all  the  poor.  Surely  I  agree  something 
could  be  done,  but  I  can  imagine  my  own  feelings  if  the  majority 
of  the  voters,  who  are  middle  class  and  poor,  should  vote  for  a 
tax  that  would  take  away  a  large  part  of  the  money  and  savings 
I  had  worked  for  and  made. 

Even  this  limited  sampling  reveals  the  possibilities  of  this 
method  of  learning  about  the  social  viewpoints  of  the  stu- 


i66        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

dents.  These  excerpts  reveal  an  interesting  variety  of  views 
regarding  causes  and  cure  of  poverty  and  unemployment. 
Different  positions  are  taken  toward  taxation.  Personal  sym- 
pathy for  people  in  different  economic  circumstances  or  lack 
of  it  is  shown.  One  can  even  gain  some  idea  of  the  nature 
and  degree  of  awareness  of  social  conditions  in  each  student* 

Records  of  free  choice  activities  of  all  sorts  often  yield  sur- 
prisingly useful  information.  Thus  records  of  free  reading 
may  give  clues  regarding  students'  social  interests,  level  of 
social  awareness,  and  maturity  and  direction  of  social  out- 
look. Records  of  activities  of  all  sorts,  in-school  and  out-of- 
school,  such  as  participation  in  school  government,  vacation 
activities,  attendance  at  motion  pictures,  lectures,  and  con- 
certs, and  other  leisure-time  activities  are  also  useful,  par- 
ticularly when  the  nature  of  the  activity  is  recorded  in  addi- 
tion to  its  frequency.3  Although  these  records  serve  primarily 
as  evidence  of  interests,  analysis  of  their  content  also  serves 
for  evidence  of  social  sensitivity. 

Free  response  tests  employing  a  form  akin  to  projective 
techniques  are  also  useful  devices  for  getting  at  personal  re- 
sponses to  social  issues.  Their  advantage  lies  in  their  indi- 
rection. The  individual  is  not  asked  directly  to  reveal  his 
social  values.  He  is  provided  an  apparently  innocent  object 
of  attention  to  which  he  can  respond  freely  and  personally. 
The  object  of  attention  is  so  chosen  as  to  draw  out  revela- 
tions of  his  pattern  of  social  sensitivity.  In  a  completely  free 
response  test,  only  a  brief  statement  is  given,  and  students 
are  asked  to  list  all  of  die  thoughts  that  occur  to  them  in  con- 
nection with  that  subject. 

Problem.  The  following  quotation  from  a  local  produce  market 
appeared  in  a  daily  paper: 

"Cooking  onions — 30  cents  per  bu." 
Directions:  List  all  of  your  thoughts  about  this  quotation  which 

3  For  further  discussion  of  the  use  of  reading  records  and  activities  records, 
see  The  Social  Studies  in  General  Education,  pp.  345-46. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  167 

might  be  of  social  importance.  Number  your  ideas  1,  2,  3,  4, 
etc.4 

Certain  ideas  about  students'  understanding  of,  and  atti- 
tudes evoked  by,  the  problem  can  be  gained  from  mere 
examination  of  each  student's  responses.  However,  clearer 
descriptions  of  each  student,  as  well  as  of  groups  of  students, 
are  possible  when  the  responses  are  summarized  in  terms  of 
certain  general  criteria.  Thus,  the  responses  to  the  exercise 
above  could  be  summarized  in  terms  of  the  frequency  of 
purely  personal  association  (such  as,  "I  don't  like  onions"); 
in  terms  of  frequency  of  responses  showing  awareness  of  the 
implications  of  this  situation  to  immediate  personal-social 
values,  like  the  family  budget  or  diet  ( such  as,  "If  onions  are 
so  cheap,  they  could  be  used  more  frequently  in  family 
menus'');  or,  finally,  in  terms  of  how  frequently  the  wider 
social  implications  are  mentioned  (such  as,  "If  onions  are  so 
cheap,  what  about  the  income  and  the  standard  of  living  of 
those  who  work  in  onion  fields").  A  summary  could  also  be 
made  in  terms  of  how  frequently  each  student  mentioned 
important  considerations  and  how  relevant  his  remarks  are 
to  the  problem. 

More  controlled  forms  of  essay  tests  were  used  by  the 
evaluation  committee  in  explorations  preliminary  to  the  draft- 
ing of  objective  test  forms.  Students  were  given  a  problem 
situation,  with  several  courses  of  action  listed,  and  were 
asked  to  choose  the  course  of  action  they  thought  most  de- 
sirable. They  were  then  asked  to  indicate  the  reasons  they 
would  use  in  supporting  their  choice.5  All  such  free  exercises 
are,  of  course,  fraught  with  certain  difficulties.  To  score  the 
responses  objectively  is  a  time-consuming  process.  The  fact 
that  each  student  expresses  his  thoughts  in  a  somewhat  per- 

4  This  exercise  was  used  in  Ohio  and  Michigan  at  the  time  when  there 
were  strikes  in  onion  fields,  and  reports  of  them  appeared  in  the  daily 
newspapers. 

5  An  exercise  of  this  type  is  discussed  on  p.  178. 


168        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

sonal  way  interferes  with  the  possibility  of  assigning  his  re- 
sponses a  precise  and  fully  objective  meaning.  However, 
when  teachers  are  able  to  develop  valid  exercises  of  this  sort 
and  take  the  care  and  the  time  necessary  for  a  diagnostic 
analysis  of  the  responses,  tests  of  this  sort  have  a  real  role  to 
play,  particularly  since  they  can  be  made  more  readily  an 
integral  part  of  teaching  than  is  the  case  with  more  formal 
tests. 

EVALUATION  OF  THE  ABILITY  TO  APPLY  SOCIAL  FACTS 

AND  GENERALIZATIONS 

The  teachers  in  the  Thirty  Schools  were  much  concerned 
that  students  develop  a  willingness  and  ability  to  use  social 
facts  and  generalizations,  gained  through  their  study,  in  un- 
derstanding and  explaining  social  phenomena  around  them. 
They  recognized  the  futility  of  the  mastery  of  a  background 
of  facts  without  growing  in  ability  to  apply  them  to  an  in- 
creasing rarge  of  social  issues  met  in  daily  life.  In  many 
schools  a  serious  attempt  was  made  to  give  students  an  op- 
portunity to  think  through  new  problems  in  the  light  of  their 
previous  knowledge.  For  this  reason  interest  was  expressed 
in  developing  some  instruments  to  appraise  students'  growth 
in  ability  to  apply  social  facts  and  generalizations. 

ANALYSIS  OF  THE  OBJECTIVE 

Prior  to  the  development  of  instruments  several  explora- 
tions seemed  necessary.  First,  it  seemed  important  to  iden- 
tify the  generalizations  which  were  considered  fundamental 
to  the  understanding  of  social  problems  and  which,  there- 
fore, the  students  were  expected  to  know  and  to  apply  in 
their  thinking.  It  seemed  also  necessary  to  analyze  and  de- 
scribe the  kinds  of  behavior  involved  in  applying  social  gen- 
eralizations and  facts.  Finally,  some  exploration  of  the  areas 
of  problems  and  issues  which  the  students  may  be  expected 
to  be  able  to  think  through  was  also  needed.  In  order  to  get 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  169 

some  appropriate  criteria  by  which  to  appraise  this  aspect 
of  thinking,  it  seemed  important  to  identify  some  of  the  de- 
sirable characteristics  or  qualities  of  the  process  of  applying 
social  generalizations  as  well  as  the  difficulties  encountered 

by  the  students  in  achieving  these  qualities.  The  following 
sections  will  discuss  these  questions  in  turn  and  indicate  the 
decisions  which  were  made. 

Generalizations  and  the  Processes 
Involved  in  Applying  Them 

Students  are  often  expected  to  decide  whether  certain  ac- 
tions— proposed  or  accomplished — are  justifiable,  desirable, 
or  reasonable.  Such  decisions  as  whether  an  article  attacking 
democracy  submitted  to  a  school  paper  should  be  printed,  or 
whether  a  certain  law  should  be  passed  in  the  legislature,  are 
examples.  Decisions  are  presumably  made  more  intelligently 
when  the  student  understands  some  of  the  generalizations 
which  are  applicable  and  has  the  pertinent  facts  available. 
Students  may  also  be  expected  to  explain  certain  events  or 
to  predict  the  probable  consequences.  Thus,  in  predicting 
the  probable  effects  of  a  certain  type  of  sales  tax,  it  is  impor- 
tant to  consider  both  what  Is  known  about  the  effects  of 
different  forms  of  taxation  on  various  groups  in  society  and 
certain  general  principles  of  taxation.  In  determining  the 
desirability  of  the  measure  in  a  democratic  society,  the  con- 
sideration of  certain  basic  democratic  values,  such  as  the 
welfare  of  all  groups  and  individuals,  and  securing  equality 
of  sacrifice  as  far  as  possible,  is  also  necessary.  In  much  the 
same  way,  facts  and  generalizations  are  needed  in  judging 
the  soundness  of  conclusions  drawn  or  decisions  made  by 
other  people. 

An  effort  was  made  by  the  committee  to  assemble  a  rep- 
resentative list  of  the  generalizations  taught  in  social  sci- 
ences. An  initial  list  was  drafted  by  members  of  the  com- 
mittee. This  list  was  circulated  among  the  social  science 


170        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

teachers  in  schools  participating  in  the  Study  for  additions 
and  criticism.  Other  sources  such  as  Billings'  list  of  social 
science  generalizations  and  typical  textbooks  and  references 
were  also  examined.6  The  final  list  was  again  checked  by 
teachers  to  indicate  which  of  the  generalizations  they  con- 
sidered fundamental  in  understanding  social  phenomena, 
which  of  them  were  emphasized  in  their  teaching,  and  which 
of  them  were  touched  upon  but  not  emphasized. 

The  analysis  of  this  list  of  generalizations  raised  several 
questions  about  the  nature  of  social  science  generalizations. 
In  the  first  place,  the  line  of  demarcation  between  a  social 
fact  and  a  social  generalization  was  not  clear.  Many  of  the 
generalizations  listed  as  major  understandings  seemed  little 
more  than  generalized  facts  and  as  such  had  a  limited  utility 
in  explaining  social  phenomena  other  than  the  ones  which 
they  directly  summarized.  Thus,  the  generalization  that  a 
variety  of  taxes  is  levied  in  the  United  States  adds  but  little 
to  the  understanding  of  the  issues  of  taxation. 

The  question  of  the  dependability  or  the  "truth"  of  many 
of  the  generalizations  was  also  raised.  Many  of  these  gen- 
eralizations seemed  to  apply  only  to  a  limited  range  of  situa- 
tions, and  lacked  the  universality  commonly  attributed  to  a 
"principle,"  as  the  term  is  used  in  the  natural  sciences.  Often 
these  generalizations  seemed  little  more  than  hypotheses, 
useful  in  exploring  ways  of  explaining  events,  but  question- 
able for  exact  prediction.  Still  other  generalizations  seemed 
to  have  little  validity  independently  of  a  particular  social 
philosophy  or  theory.  Some  generalizations  seemed  to  be  di- 
rect expressions  of  the  social  beliefs  held  by  individual 
teachers,  and  the  validity  of  these  beliefs  was  often  ques- 
tioned by  other  teachers  holding  different  beliefs.  It  seemed 
clear  that  the  majority  of  useful  and  significant  social  science 

6  Neal  Billings,  A  Determination  of  Generalizations  Basic  to  the  Social 
Studies  Curriculum  (Baltimore,  Warwick  and  York,  1929). 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  171 

generalizations  were  not  verifiable  in  the  same  sense  as  are 
the  majority  of  scientific  principles. 

It  seemed  advisable,  therefore,  to  think  of  social  science 
generalizations  primarily  as  tools  for  further  thinking,  for 
formulating  tentative  explanations,  solutions  and  conclusions, 
rather  than  as  bases  for  precise  predictions,  as  infallible 
guides  for  action,  or  as  indisputable  expressions  of  "truth." 
It  was  finally  agreed  that  the  term  "social  science  generaliza- 
tion or  principle"  would  be  used  to  describe  any  generaliza- 
tion which  could  be  applied  to  a  range  of  specific  situations 
for  the  purpose  of  explanation  or  prediction,  whether  or  not 
this  generalization  was  applicable  over  an  indefinitely  wide 
range  of  such  situations  or  was  universally  true,  precise,  or 
verifiable.7 

It  was  clear  also  that  the  different  types  of  generalizations 
suggested  involved  differences  in  the  ways  in  which  they 
could  be  used  in  the  thinking  process.  On  the  basis  of  these 
differences  the  principles  were  classified  into  three  types, 
each  type  perhaps  implying  a  different  technique  for  eval- 
uating its  use.  One  group  included  descriptive  generaliza- 
tions, serving  merely  to  summarize  a  body  of  discrete  facts. 
Thus,  a  body  of  facts  about  income  might  be  summarized  by 
the  generalization,  "people  earn  their  incomes  through  a  di- 
versified range  of  activities."  Another  type  of  generalization 
served  to  indicate  cause-and-effect  relationships  and  to  ex- 
plain social  phenomena.  Thus,  a  body  of  data  relating  to 
economic  penetration  into  undeveloped  countries  might  be 
summarized  by  some  such  generalization  as  "economic  pene- 
tration of  an  undeveloped  country  frequently  results  in  mili- 
tary and  political  domination."  A  third  type  had  to  do  with 
expressions  of  value  judgments,  opinions,  or  beliefs.  Thus, 
the  body  of  facts  regarding  freedom  of  speech  might  be 

7  For  a  discussion  on  usage  of  the  term  "principle"  in  curriculum  build- 
ing see  HolHs  L.  Gas  well  and  Doak  S.  Campbell,  Curriculum  Development 
(New  York,  American  Book  Co.,  1935),  pp.  87-90. 


172        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

summarized  in  the  principle,  "freedom  of  speech  is  essential 
to  the  preservation  of  democracy."  This  sort  of  statement  ex- 
presses a  viewpoint  or  value  judgment  which  is  incapable  of 
verification  in  the  usual  sense  of  the  term. 

The  effect  of  the  compilation  of  such  a  sample  list  of  gener- 
alizations upon  teaching  was  also  considered.  Some  teachers 
feared  that  the  list  would  suggest  a  minimum  set  of  generali- 
zations to  be  adopted  by  all  teachers  and  to  be  taught  for 
memorization.  It  was  agreed  the  preparation  of  the  list  should 
not  be  taken  to  imply  that  these  generalizations  had  been  or 
should  be  taught  as  statements  to  be  learned,  but  rather  that 
through  the  best  learning  procedures  the  students  would  be 
brought  to  understand  certain  generalizations.,  and  that  they 
would  be  given  opportunity  to  apply  some  of  these  in  their 
school  work.  The  list  was  to  be  used  as  an  illustrative  sample 
of  generalizations  for  the  sole  purpose  of  exploring  the  possi- 
bility of  evaluating  students'  ability  to  apply  them. 

Analysis  of  Behavior 

In  the  course  of  the  above  discussion  some  of  the  behaviors 
involved  in  applying  facts  and  principles  to  social  problems 
have  already  been  indicated. 

As  was  described  above,  application  of  principles  and  facts 
usually  takes  place  when  people  are  called  upon  to  do  any  of 
the  following:  (a)  explain  certain  ideas  or  phenomena,  (b) 
predict  consequences  of  events,  (c)  decide  on  a  course  of 
action,  or  (d)  judge  predictions,  conclusions,  or  decisions 
made  by  other  people.  In  any  of  these  situations,  provided 
they  are  new  to  the  students,  it  is  necessary  to  be  aware  of 
the  major  issues  in  the  problem.  A  more  reasonable  judgment 
usually  results  when  appropriate  use  is  made  of  whatever 
facts  and  generalizations  are  pertinent  to  the  problem.  In  the 
process  of  making  judgments  of  this  type  the  following  be- 
haviors are  involved: 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  173 

1.  Relating  previously  learned  facts  and  generalizations 
to  each  other  and  to  the  given  problem. 

2.  Discriminating   between   facts   and   generalizations 
which  are  relevant  to  a  given  problem  and  those 
which  are  not. 

3.  Discerning  the  logical  relationship  between  a  par- 
ticular conclusion,  decision,  or  a  course  of  action  and 
a  generalization  or  a  fact. 

4.  Organizing  facts  and  principles  learned  in  different 
contexts  in  such  a  way  that  they  can  be  helpfully  used 
in  ^analyzing  the  problem  or  in  arriving  at  the  con- 
clusion. 

One  of  the  important  points  brought  out  in  analyzing  the 
objective  was  that  the  most  fruitful  use  of  important  facts 
and  generalizations  takes  place  when  these  are  applied  to 
problems  new  to  the  students.  Although  knowing  die  facts 
and  generalizations  themselves  was  regarded  as  basic  to  the 
ability  to  use  them,  teachers  were  primarily  concerned  in  this 
connection  with  having  students  develop  the  ability  to  or- 
ganize the  facts  and  principles  and  relate  them  to  each  other 
in  new  ways.  Hence,  the  recall  of  applications  made  by  other 
people  was  not  considered  a  behavior  to  be  diagnosed  by  the 
prospective  instruments. 

Criteria  for  Appraising  the  Process  of 
Applying  Facts  and  Generalizations 

An  analysis  of  the  specific  behaviors  in  this  type  of  think- 
ing is  helpful,  but  it  is  not  sufficient  for  evaluating  that  be- 
havior. It  is  also  necessary  to  indicate  certain  criteria  by 
which  to  appraise  that  behavior.  Therefore,  an  attempt  was 
also  made  to  outline  the  characteristics,  both  positive  and 
negative,  of  the  process  of  applying  social  science  principles, 
which  it  seemed  important  and  useful  to  diagnose. 

The  following  characteristics  were  suggested  as  important 
by  the  committee: 


174        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Relevance:  Is  the  student  discriminating  in  his  use  of 
generalizations  and  facts?  Are  the  generalizations 
which  he  uses  relevant  to  the  situation? 

Comprehensiveness:  To  what  extent  does  the  student 
see  the  implications  of  generalizations  and  facts? 
What  range  of  important  generalizations  does  he  con- 
sider? Has  he  failed  to  use  some  of  the  important 
generalizations  ? 

Consistency:  In  the  use  of  value  or  attitudinal  principles 
does  he  show  consistency  in  the  point  of  view  which 
he  accepts?  Does  he  use  some  principles  which  are 
conflicting  either  with  each  other  or  with  the  course 
of  action  or  solution  under  consideration? 

Objectivity  and  Tenability:  Does  the  student  rely  pri- 
marily on  generalizations  which  can  be  substantiated 
by  fact,  or  does  he  use  slogans,  emotional  phrases, 
and  cliches?  Are  the  statements  of  facts  and  generali- 
zations used  tenable  in  the  sense  that  they  do  not 
contradict  commonly  known  information? 

Selection  of  Problems 

The  kinds  of  problems  in  which  students  may  be  expected 
to  apply  facts  and  generalizations  which  they  have  learned 
were  also  explored.  Again,  teachers  were  asked  to  submit  a 
list  of  problem  areas  dealt  with  in  their  classes.  A  list  of  52 
problem  areas  was  thus  assembled.  A  considerable  range  of 
types  of  problems  was  suggested.  Some  teachers  emphasized 
problems  of  personal-social  relations;  others  were  concerned 
principally  with  so-called  large  social  issues.  The  most  fre- 
quently mentioned  among  the  latter  were:  consumer  educa- 
tion and  advertising,  capitalism,  distribution  of  wealth,  civil 
liberties,  theories  and  forms  of  government,  international  re- 
lations, labor,  natural  resources,  racial  issues,  profit  system, 
public  health,  relief,  taxes,  housing,  war  and  peace,  unem- 
ployment, public  opinion. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  175 

CONSTRUCTION  OF  THE  TEST  ON  THE  ABILITY  TO  APPLY 
SOCIAL  VALUES 

The  explorations  described  above  determined  in  several 
ways  the  nature  of  the  instruments  that  were  developed.  In 
the  first  place  the  analysis  of  the  nature  of  generalizations 
indicated  that  there  was  a  sufficient  difference  between  the 
processes  involved  in  the  application  of  social  values  and 
those  involved  in  the  application  of  non-value  generaliza- 
tions and  facts  to  warrant  the  use  of  different  evaluation 
techniques.  Accordingly,  two  instruments  were  developed: 
one  to  deal  with  the  application  of  value  principles  or  demo- 
cratic tenets,  the  other  to  appraise  the  application  of  facts 
and  explanatory  generalizations.  The  first  of  these  instru- 
ments, Social  Problems  (Form  1.41  and  Form  1.42),  was 
developed  and  studied  more  extensively  and  is,  therefore, 
reported  more  completely  in  this  chapter.  The  second,  Ap- 
plication of  Social  Facts  and  Generalizations  (Form  1.5),  is 
reported  briefly. 

Several  suggestions  regarding  techniques  for  the  construc- 
tion of  instruments  were  also  derived  both  from  the  analysis 
of  the  generalizations  and  of  the  behavior  processes  involved 
in  their  use.  Thus,  it  seemed  to  be  out  of  the  question  to  con- 
struct exercises  requiring  students  to  respond  to  social  gen- 
eralizations, particularly  to  value  principles,  as  true  and  false, 
or  right  and  wrong.  It  seemed  more  appropriate  to  require 
students  to  determine  the  logical  relationships  between  con- 
clusions, courses  of  action,  and  certain  generalizations  and 
facts.  The  very  nature  of  the  thinking  process  in  this  area 
indicated  that  the  exercises  should  take  die  form  of  respond- 
ing to  social  values  in  the  context  of  certain  problems  and 
issues,  and  not  in  isolation.  Similarly,  the  criteria  for  apprais- 
ing the  process  of  applying  social  generalizations,  such  as 
relevance,  consistency,  comprehensiveness,  and  pattern  of 
values,  determined,  in  a  general  way,  the  selection  of  the 


176        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

issues  to  be  included  in  the  test,  and  the  sampling  of  the 
specific  items  in  the  exercises.  Thus,  in  order  to  appraise 
the  consistency  of  value  pattern  it  was  necessary  to  include 
conflicting  value  principles  in  each  of  the  exercises.  Broadly 
speaking,  then,  the  categories  for  the  subsequent  keying  of 
the  test  items  were  determined  by  a  jury  of  teachers. 

Naturally  the  analysis  of  the  committee  suggested  only  the 
main  structure  of  the  instrument.  Additional  criteria  for  the 
choice  and  formulation  of  the  items  in  the  test  as  well  as  for 
the  choice  of  summary  categories  were  developed  according 
to  what  was  revealed  in  the  study  of  the  results  from  the 
tentative  forms  of  the  instrument. 

The  Choice  of  the  Elements  in  the  Test 

In  the  main  it  seemed  necessary  to  provide  a  testing  situa- 
tion in  which  the  students  would  have  an  opportunity  to  take 
positions  or  to  make  decisions  about  some  significant  social 
issues  and  to  support  these  decisions  by  using  value  princi- 
ples. Consequently  the  following  structure  for  the  test  was 
eventually  adopted: 

1.  A  problem  situation  describing  an  important  issue 
was  presented. 

2.  Three  courses  of  action  representing  three  different 
positions  toward  the  issue  were  formulated.  The  stu- 
dents were  to  choose  the  one  or  ones  which  they 
thought  most  desirable. 

3.  A  list  of  "reasons"  consisting  of  value  principles  was 
given  from  which  students  could  choose  the  ones  they 
would  use  to  support  the  course  of  action  chosen. 
( See  illustrative  exercise,  pp.  180-182. ) 

As  suggested  in  the  analysis  of  this  objective,  certain  cri- 
teria were  set  up  for  the  choice  of  the  content  in  each  of  the 
three  parts  of  the  test  mentioned  above.  Thus,  in  order  to  be 
sure  of  providing  opportunity  for  applying  value  principles 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  177 

and  not  just  remembering  them,  the  problems  were  to  be 
new  to  the  students.  Since  application  to  life  problems  was 
of  concern  to  teachers,  significant  contemporary  problems 
were  chosen  whenever  possible;  actual  problems  reported  in 
newspapers  or  magazines  were  used.  The  fact  that  there  are 
differences  of  opinion  about  the  value  generalizations  sug- 
gested problems  of  controversial  nature  permitting  several 
solutions  or  conclusions.  In  order  to  engage  the  effort  of  stu- 
dents, it  seemed  necessary  to  select  problems  which  had 
some  significance  and  meaning  to  them.  Therefore,  the  tenta- 
tive formulations  of  the  problems  were  submitted  to  students 
for  their  criticism  and  suggestions. 

Since  solutions  to  social  problems  could  not  be  considered 
as  "right"  or  "wrong"  in  themselves,  the  courses  of  action 
outlined  in  the  exercises  represented  different  positions  and 
were  not  to  be  marked  as  "right"  or  "wrong."  In  order  to  pro- 
vide for  a  diagnosis  of  different  value  patterns,  it  seemed 
necessary  for  die  courses  of  action  to  incorporate  the  posi- 
tions currently  taken  toward  the  issues  described  in  the 
problem. 

The  kind  of  diagnosis  that  teachers  were  interested  in 
making,  expressed  as  criteria  for  evaluating  this  type  of  think- 
ing, suggested  the  main  types  of  reasons  to  be  included. 
Thus,  in  order  to  discover  dominant  value  patterns,  it  seemed 
obvious  that  statements  of  contrasting  beliefs  and  values 
were  needed.  In  order  to  provide  opportunities  for  students 
to  engage  in  desirable,  as  well  as  undesirable,  forms  of  rea- 
soning, it  seemed  necessary  to  include  reasons  which  logi- 
cally supported  each  course  of  action,  as  well  as  those  which 
were  contradictory,  irrelevant,  or  untenable. 

Preliminary  Explorations  of  Test  Forms 

In  order  to  be  sure  that  the  proposed  test,  in  addition  to 
incorporating  the  desired  diagnostic  features,  would  be  on 
a  level  appropriate  to  the  students  who  were  to  take  it — that 


178        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

is  would  use  terms  they  could  understand  and  include  the 
kinds  of  values  they  were  familiar  with,  the  types  of  unde- 
sirable reasoning  they  indulged  in,  and  the  kinds  of  value 
conflicts  current  among  them — several  tentative  drafts  of  the 
test  were  tried  out. 

Ten  "direct  form"  exercises  were  drafted.  Each  contained  a 
statement  of  a  problem,  and  three  courses  of  action.  Students 
were  asked  to  choose  from  these  alternative  courses  of  action 
or  conclusions  those  which  they  approved  and  to  write  out 
their  own  reasons  to  support  their  choices. 

SAMPLE  EXERCISE: 

Cotton  Picker.  Cotton  has  been  picked  by  hand,  which  is  a  slow 
and  expensive  process.  Recently,  the  Rust  brothers  invented  a 
machine  to  do  this  work.  It  would  pick  in  7/2  hours  as  much  cot- 
ton as  one  hand  picker  could  pick  over  a  whole  season  of  eleven 
weeks.  The  cost  of  production  of  cotton  could  be  reduced  from 
$14.52  to  $3.00  per  bale.  To  date,  this  machine  has  not  been 
placed  on  the  market.  What  should  be  done  with  this  machine? 

Solutions:  (Check  one  or  more  which  you  think  are  desirable.) 

A.  The  machine  should  be  placed  on  the  commercial  market 

for  immediate  manufacture  and  sale. 

B.  The  machine  should  be  made  available  under  some  form 

of  public  control  and  provisions  made  for  establishing  in 
other  jobs  the  cotton  pickers  who  are  thrown  out  of  work. 

C.  The  machine  should  not  be  put  to  use  at  the  present  time. 

Directions:  Write  in  the  space  below  the  reasons  which  you 
would  use  to  support  the  solution  or  solutions  you 
have  checked.  Re  sure  to  write  all  of  the  reasons  you 
can  think  of. 

Below  is  a  sample  of  the  reasons  used  by  the  students  check- 
ing the  course  of  action  A: 

1.  The  normal  trend  of  business  would  reemploy  the  re- 
placed workers  gradually. 

2.  The  cotton  workers  could  always  go  on  temporary  relief. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  179 

3.  When  a  good  invention  like  this  has  to  be  withheld  from 
the  market  because  of  the  problem  of  what  to  do  with 
the  unemployed,  it  is  a  little  doubtful  whether  our  pres- 
ent economic  system  is  really  serviceable. 

4.  Society  should  not  be  deprived  of  anything  that  might 
improve  work  and  the  products  it  uses. 

5.  Economic  statistics  prove  that  there  is  no  such  thing  as 
technological  unemployment. 

These  student  responses  were  used  in  several  ways  in 
drafting  the  instrument.  In  the  first  place,  it  was  possible  to 
check  the  usefulness  and  the  validity  of  the  criteria  for  sum- 
marizing and  evaluating  the  responses  suggested  by  the  com- 
mittee. It  was  found  that  most  of  them — comprehensiveness, 
consistency,  relevance,  tenability,  and  patterns  of  values — 
were  useful  in  classifying  and  summarizing  student  re- 
sponses. Thus  variations  were  found  in  the  range  of  implica- 
tions seen  (comprehensiveness).  Often  the  reasons  chosen 
by  the  students  were  in  conflict  with  the  courses  of  action 
they  had  marked  (inconsistencies).  Many  students  used  rea- 
sons contrary  to  facts  (untenable)  or  which  did  not  apply  to 
the  courses  of  action  they  had  chosen  (irrelevant).  Different 
value  patterns  were  also  expressed.  These  value  patterns 
were  at  first  summarized  under  the  following  headings:  pro- 
tection of  human  values,  consideration  of  general  public 
welfare,  democratic  tenets,  desire  for  justice,  approval  of 
change,  protection  of  the  economic  interests  of  property 
owners,  protection  of  the  interests  of  privileged  groups,  eco- 
nomic individualism,  safeguarding  of  present  institutions, 
laws,  and  customs.  Because  of  the  limitation  in  the  length  of 
the  test,  later  it  was  necessary  to  reduce  this  classification  to 
the  following  one:  democratic  values,  undemocratic  values, 
and  rationalizations.  In  the  second  place,  student  responses 
also  suggested  the  content  for  each  variety  of  reasons.  Thus, 
the  types  of  untenable  and  irrelevant  reasons  to  be  used,  the 
kinds  of  inconsistencies,  and  the  kinds  of  democratic  and 


180        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

undemocratic  values  to  be  included  were  largely  determined 
by  analysis  of  these  free  responses.  Suggestions  were  also 
found  regarding  terminology  suitable  for  use  in  the  test  state- 
ments. The  final  form  of  the  test  included  many  statements 
made  by  the  students.  In  other  cases  the  phrasing  as  well  as 
the  content  was  patterned  closely  to  the  statements  made  by 
the  students. 

Description  of  tlie  Final  Test 

A  sample  of  the  final  test  exercise  with  an  example  of  some 
of  the  reasons  is  given  below.  The  key  is  inscribed  on  the 
margin. 

PROBLEM  IV.  "WORKING  CONDITIONS" 
Each  year  many  workers  have  to  stop  working  either  temporarily 
or  permanently  because  they  develop  poor  lung  conditions,  ar- 
thritis, rheumatism,  or  just  general  ill  health.  It  is  known  that 
such  factors  as  dust,  dampness,  and  unregulated  temperature 
greatly  contribute  to  these  ailments,  though  it  is  impossible  to 
determine  in  many  individual  cases  to  what  extent  the  illness  was 
caused  by  these  conditions. 

Since  it  would  involve  costly  improvements  to  eliminate  these 
conditions,  many  mines  and  factories  have  done  little  about  them 
and  oppose  further  regulation.  With  the  exception  of  a  few  states 
which  have  adequate  health  regulations,  at  present  only  such 
things  as  hours  of  work  and  conditions  leading  to  accidents  are 
regulated  by  the  government. 

What  should  be  done  about  such  problems? 

Directions:  Choose  the  most  acceptable  course  (or  courses)  of 
action  and  fill  in  the  appropriate  spaces  on  the  answer  sheet 
under  Problem  IV. 

Courses  of  Action: 

(Undemocratic)  A.  It  should  be  left  to  the  individual  mine  and 
factory  owners  to  determine  what  is 
needed  and  what  they  can  afford. 

(Democratic)          B.  Minimum  standards  for  general  working 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS 


181 


conditions,  including  all  factors  injurious 
to  health,  should  be  set  by  the  government 
and  all  industries  should  be  required  to 
meet  these  standards. 

(Compromise)  C.  In  industries  where  such  conditions  are 
likely  to  prevail,  improvements  should  be 
made  on  the  basis  of  suggestions  from  joint 
committees  of  workers  and  employers. 

What  reasons  would  you  use  to  support  your  course  ( or  courses ) 
of  action? 

Directions:  Choose  the  reasons  which  are  in  harmony  with  what 
you  believe  and  which  you  would  use  to  support  your  course  (or 
courses)  of  action  and  fill  the  spaces  on  the  answer  sheet  in  the 
column  under  the  course  of  action  you  marked  at  the  top.  If  you 
have  chosen  more  than  one  course  of  action,  and  a  reason  sup- 
ports both,  mark  it  in  both  columns. 

Reasons: 


Key 

Supports  A  and  C 
Inconsistent  with  B 
Rationalization 
Supports  A  and  C 
Inconsistent  with  B 
Undemocratic  Value 
Supports  C 
Inconsistent  with 
A  and  B.  Democratic 
Value 
Supports  B 
Inconsistent  with  A 
Irrelevant  to  C 
Democratic  Value 
Supports  A  and  C 
Inconsistent  with  B 
Undemocratic  Value 


1.  It  would  be  unfair  to  require  factories 
to     introduce    costly    improvements 
which  they  feel  they  cannot  afford. 

2.  Without  regulation,  business  can  be 
depended  upon  to  make  necessary  im- 
provements. 

6.  If  workers  participate  successfully  in 
solving  this  problem,  there  is  likely  to 
be  further  cooperation  between  em- 
ployers and  employees. 

8.  Human  welfare  should  be  protected 
regardless  of  the  cost  to  industry. 


10.  Since  employers  have  to  bear  the  ex- 
pense of  making  improvements  in 
working  conditions,  they  should  have 


1 82        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Key 

a  voice  in  deciding  what  changes 
should  be  made. 

Untenable  12.  Most    industries    today    provide    as 

healthy  working  conditions  as  they  can 
afford  without  undue  strain  on  their 
finances. 

Supports  A  15.  If  a  worker  is  willing  to  accept  em- 

Inconsistent  with  ployment  in  an  industry,  he  should 

B  and  C.  Undemo-  be  willing  to  work  under  the  condi- 

cratic  Value  tions  prevailing  in  that  industry. 

Supports  A  and  C         16.  Even  though  it  is  important  to  im- 
Inconsistent  with  B  prove  working  conditions,  it   is   un- 

Rationalization  democratic  to  accomplish  this  through 

dictation  by  the  government. 

Untenable  20.  In  the  past  improvements  in  working 

conditions  have  come  only  under  gov- 
ernment compulsion. 

A  word  of  explanation  may  be  necessary  regarding  the 
method  of  arriving  at  the  key  for  this  instrument.  The  anal- 
ysis made  by  the  committee  suggested  the  classification  of  all 
items,  except  the  specific  diagnosis  of  the  value  pattern.  This 
was  developed  by  an  analysis  of  responses  and  was  checked 
by  teachers.  The  items  were  keyed  by  a  jury  composed  of 
members  of  the  Evaluation  Staff  and  some  teachers  of  social 
sciences. 

On  the  assumption  that  value  preferences  and  logical  judg- 
ments both  enter  into  and  influence  each  other  in  the  normal 
life  response  to  controversial  social  issues,  evaluation  proc- 
esses should  not  isolate  these  behaviors  and  treat  them  as  if 
they  occurred  independently  of  each  other.  Hence,  the  test 
is  not  made  up  of  parts  corresponding  to  each  of  the  aspects 
of  behavior  measured  by  the  test. 

Only  one  process  of  marking  the  test  is  employed:  the  stu- 
dents mark  the  reasons  which  they  would  use  to  support  the 
courses  of  action  they  chose.  The  students  use  each  reason 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  183 

only  once  with  each  course  of  action.  But  each  reason  is 
keyed  in  several  different  ways.  Thus,  reason  1  in  the  above 
exercise  supports  courses  of  action  A  and  C  and  is  incon- 
sistent with  B.  Depending  on  the  course  of  action  with  which 
it  is  used,  response  to  this  reason  is  scored  under  the  accu- 
rate reasons  contributing  to  comprehensiveness,  or  under 
inconsistency.  In  addition,  each  exercise  contains  two  or  three 
reasons  which  are  contrary  to  commonly  known  facts,  i.e., 
are  untenable  (reasons  12,  20).  These  reasons  are  not  keyed 
to  any  particular  course  of  action,  but  are  so  sampled  that 
for  each  position  there  is  one  untenable  reason  which  has 
some  logical  relationship  to  it.  They  are  scored  as  untenable 
no  matter  with  which  course  of  action  they  are  used. 

Most  of  the  reasons  are  also  keyed  to  represent  value  posi- 
tions, as  are  all  courses  of  action.  The  value  patterns  are 
grouped  into  three  categories:  (1)  democratic*  representing 
defense  of  the  interests  of  the  general  public  or  general  wel- 
fare, of  such  democratic  rights  as  freedom  of  speech,  equality 
of  opportunity,  and  a  decent  standard  of  living,  of  rights  of 
minorities  and  other  underprivileged  groups  (course  of  ac- 
tion B,  reasons  2,  6,  8);  (2)  undemocratic,  representing  pro- 
tection of  special  privilege,  supremacy  of  efficiency  and  eco- 
nomic gain  over  human  needs  and  values,  undemocratic 
procedures,  or  discrimination  ( course  of  action  A,  reasons  10, 
15);  (3)  compromise,  representing  essentially  an  effort  to 
reconcile  these  two  types  of  values  (only  courses  of  action, 
e.g.  C,  are  used).  Rationalizations  (reasons  1,  16),  repre- 
senting undemocratic  values  stated  as  democratic  slogans, 
are  keyed  and  scored  as  a  separate  value  category,  but  they 
can  be  used  to  support  either  the  undemocratic  or  com- 
promise courses  of  action.  At  least  six  supporting  reasons  are 
available  for  each  course  of  action.  The  logically  sound  sup- 

8  The  meaning  of  the  terms  "democratic"  and  "undemocratic"  as  used  in 
this  test  have  thus  a  special  definition,  somewhat  more  encompassing  than 
the  common  usage  of  these  terms. 


184        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

port  for  the  democratic  course  of  action  is  composed  exclu- 
sively of  democratic  values.  Those  supporting  the  undemo- 
cratic course  of  action  are  all  undemocratic  values.  About 
half  of  the  supporting  reasons  for  the  compromise  course  of 
action  are  democratic  and  half  are  undemocratic  values.  No 
matter  with  which  course  of  action  the  reason  is  used,  it  is 
keyed  to  the  same  value.  Thus  reason  1  is  keyed  as  a  demo- 
cratic value  and  reason  2  as  an  undemocratic  value,  inde- 
pendently of  the  course  of  action  with  which  they  are  used. 
In  the  entire  test  there  are  eight  of  these  exercises,  cover- 
ing such  problems  as  conservation  of  national  resources,  free 
speech,  unemployment,  protection  of  health,  distribution  of 
wealth,  collective  bargaining,  and  socialized  medicine.  The 
pattern  of  reasons  described  above  is  the  same  in  all  eight 
exercises. 

Summarizing  and  Interpreting  the  Results 

On  the  sample  form  of  a  data  sheet  shown  on  page  185  the 
scores  for  four  students  are  presented  for  purposes  of  illustra- 
tion. At  the  bottom  of  the  data  sheet  the  maximum  possible 
score,  the  highest  score,  lowest  score,  and  the  group  median 
are  recorded  for  each  column.  All  of  these  are  computed  for 
the  class  of  53  students  from  which  these  four  were  drawn. 

Scores  on  this  test  can  be  interpreted  in  terms  of  answers 
to  three  questions.  The  first  of  these  questions  is:  How 
broadly  does  the  pupil  relate  principles  or  value  generaliza- 
tions to  chosen  courses  of  action? 

Comprehensiveness  (columns  1,  2,  3,  4).  The  most  impor- 
tant score  here  is  found  in  the  column  headed  Ratio  ( column 
4).  This  score  is  the  average  number  of  logically  accurate 
reasons  the  student  has  marked  for  each  course  of  action.  A 
high  score  here  usually  indicates  the  ability  to  see  the  impli- 
cations of  social  values  in  concrete  social  problems  broadly 
and  fully.  Thus  Student  A  has  one  of  the  highest  scores  in 
the  whole  class  on  comprehensiveness  (6.1),  while  Student 


&«c 

CS     ^-, 

S   .2 

I   8 

S     CO 

co    ^ 


I 


II 


6,.    tn 
0    0 

*rsl 

P  ^> 

S 

r-»  CM  CM  O 
00  CM  Tf  O 

0 

o 

° 

O 
0 

T-l 

00 

in 

i  "3    i    2 

2-1-31 

co 

00  OO  CO  O 

" 

VD 
CM 

o 

in 

r- 

w 

o 

i  §*S 

co  o  m  -«^- 

CO 

3 

C3 
O 

5^2 

•*-• 

m 

m 

CM 

13 

P4 

nj   o 

> 

o  « 

cs 

S  •£ 

T-t 

CO  CO  O  O 

o 

o 

m 

^f 

03 

p  ^ 

0 
P 

0 

|g| 

o 

^  in  \o  CM 

CO 

0 

•o 

CO 

a 

o 

%_, 

i     O.S 

0 

J5  S  t3 

^b 

O 

OCM^O 

CO 

0 

VO 

0 

0 

o  w 

0 

l§ 

oo 

in  T-I  -r-i  \O 

00 

0 

CO 

^ 

•3  | 

S_i     R3 

SO  ^  NO  i-l 

CO  CN  CO 

0 

0 

CO 

§ 

M    0 

QPO 

i| 

i       i     CJ 

511 

0 

-^  f-  00  CM 

*ss 

- 

- 

« 

1—*    o 

W  i— 

1 

p 

t—  1 

tn 

CO  CO  O  O 

o 

- 

in 

w 

.2 

- 

T-t  "3-  oo  co 

\O  CM  T  CM 

VD 

ON 

0 

m" 

G 

SB  ^ 

CO 

m  CN  oo  co 

co  co  m 

r- 

^ 

CN 

in  t-i  co  T-I 

m  m  m 

co 

•s 

<  UP^ 

PPO 

1 

Sss 
Cu 

o  <u 

CM 

TH    T-H    C\T-H 

i>»  x^  in  CM 

m 

CM 

00 

un 

a 

HP^ 

T-l 

1 

S      s 

jjj  «4_,  2 

T—  * 

CN  CO  CO  CO 

00 

in 

CM 

00 

§  °  o 

r-i 

>  k. 

O 

0 

U 

O 

1 

Column  _ 
Number 

Student:  A 
B 
C 

r 

Maximum 
Possible 

eg 

1 

CO 

^ 

4 

£ 

Median 

1 86        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

D  has  used  on  the  average  only  2.3  reasons  for  each  course 
of  action  that  he  chose — a  ratio  score  which  is  considerably 
below  the  median.  This  suggests  that  Student  A  has  a  much 
broader  vision  of  the  implications  of  social  values  than  does 
Student  D.  The  scores  on  total  reasons  (column  2)  and  ac- 
curate reasons  (column  3)  are  for  purposes  of  reference  only. 
Thus  occasionally  it  is  important  to  see  whether  a  student 
has  marked  many  reasons  in  excess  of  those  needed  to  sup- 
port his  position.  This  would  suggest  that  the  student  is  con- 
fused or  lacks  discrimination,  which,  for  instance,  is  the  case 
with  Students  B  and  C.  Each  has  used  over  20  reasons  which, 
do  not  support  the  courses  of  action  he  chose.  In  the  case  of 
Student  B,  these  constitute  over  half  of  the  total  reasons 
marked. 

The  second  question  is:  To  what  extent  does  the  pupil 
show  lack  of  logical  discrimination  in  the  use  of  reasons  to 
support  the  courses  of  action  which  he  chooses? 

Undesirable  Reasons  (columns  5,  6,  7). 

Per  Cent  Inconsistency  (column  5).  This  score  gives  the 
per  cent  of  the  total  number  of  reasons  checked  by  the  stu- 
dent which  are  inconsistent  with  the  course  of  action  chosen. 
A  high  score  here  indicates  inability  to  see  clearly  the  logical 
relations  between  value  principles  and  social  issues.  As  such, 
it  is  an  index  either  of  lack  of  ability  to  deal  with  abstract 
principles  or  else  of  a  confused  value  pattern  which  makes 
it  impossible  to  see  their  implications  clearly.  Student  D  has 
avoided  all  inconsistencies,  while  28  per  cent  of  the  reasons 
marked  by  Student  B  were  inconsistent  with  the  courses  of 
action  chosen,  the  median  for  the  class  for  inconsistency 
being  5. 

Untenable  Reasons  (column  6).  This  score  gives  die  num- 
ber of  reasons  checked  by  the  student  which  are  contrary  to 
commonly  known  facts.  A  high  score  here  indicates  either  a 
tendency  to  use  questionable  evidence  to  support  one's  posi- 
tion, or  it  expresses  idealistic  naivete  and  goodwill  toward 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  187 

social  conditions  and  a  lack  of  awareness  of  the  real  condi- 
tions. Student  C  uses  eight  such  reasons,  while  Student  D 
uses  only  two.  It  must  be  observed,  however,  that  the  range 
for  this  score  is  small. 

Irrelevant  Reasons  (column  7).  This  score  gives  the  num- 
ber of  reasons  checked  by  the  student  which  do  not  apply  to 
the  particular  course  of  action  chosen.  A  high  score  here  sug- 
gests lack  of  discrimination  between  reasons  that  are  relevant 
and  those  which  do  not  apply  to  a  given  course  of  action. 
Students  A  and  C  show  higher  than  average  tendency  to  fail 
to  discriminate  between  the  relevant  and  irrelevant  reasons, 
while  Student  D  has  marked  only  one  irrelevant  reason. 

The  third  of  these  questions  is:  What  values  are  dominant 
among  the  courses  of  action  and  reasons  chosen  by  the  stu- 
dent? 

While  the  choices  of  courses  of  action  as  well  as  of  reasons 
yield  information  on  patterns  of  value,  the  former  are  used 
only  in  a  subsidiary  fashion  to  determine  the  consistency  of 
the  pattern.  The  scores  on  reasons  (columns  11  to  14)  are  of 
primary  importance  here.  Those  on  courses  of  action  can  be 
used  only  as  supplementary  evidence.  The  main  score  on 
dominant  values  is  the  per  cent  democratic  values  (column 
14).  A  high  score  here  indicates  a  clear-cut  and  exclusive 
acceptance  of  the  democratic  values  as  defined  above  (p. 
183) .  One  hundred  per  cent  of  the  values  used  by  Student  D 
are  democratic,  while  only  22  per  cent  of  the  value  reasons 
used  by  Student  B  fall  in  this  category. 

Columns  11,  12,  and  13  represent  a  more  specific  analysis 
of  the  distribution  of  value  scores. 

Democratic.  Scores  in  column  11  report  the  number  of 
times  the  student  has  used  reasons  expressing  the  values  of 
general  welfare  and  democratic  rights.  Student  A  uses  a  large 
number  (43)  of  values  of  this  type,  while  Student  B  has  a 
score  of  6,  which  is  at  the  bottom  of  distribution  for  the  class. 

Undemocratic.  Scores  in  column  12  give  the  number  of 


i88        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

reasons  which  express  defense  of  the  interests  of  special 
privilege  of  all  sorts.  A  high  score  here  indicates  a  predom- 
inant acceptance  of  undemocratic  viewpoints  on  social  issues, 
as  defined  above  (p.  183).  Student  B  has  used  13  of  this  type 
of  value  statements.  This  is  not  only  considerably  above  the 
median  but  also  this  type  of  value  composes  the  largest  part 
of  the  total  value  reasons  used  by  him. 

Rationalization.  ( Scores  in  column  13 ) .  Included  under  this 
heading  are  reasons  which  attempt  to  rationalize  an  essen- 
tially undemocratic  viewpoint  by  couching  it  in  democratic 
terminology.  High  scores  here  indicate  a  tendency  of  gulli- 
bility to  slogans  and  an  inclination  to  pay  lip  service  to  demo- 
cratic generalities.  Student  C  shows  such  an  inclination, 
having  used  more  than  the  average  of  these  reasons. 

Sometimes  it  is  worth  while  to  compare  the  values  ex- 
pressed in  choices  of  courses  of  action  with  the  value  pattern 
revealed  in  reasons.  Often  these  two  aspects  of  reasoning  are 
not  consistent  with  each  other.  Thus  if  the  majority  of  the 
reasons  checked  by  the  student  are  democratic  values  but 
several  undemocratic  or  compromise  courses  of  action  are 
chosen  at  the  same  time,  one  may  infer  that  the  student  does 
not  fully  see  the  implications  of  the  values  he  accepts  ver- 
bally. Such  seems  to  be  the  case  with  Student  D.  He  has 
chosen  two  compromise  courses  of  action,  which  normally 
call  for  part  democratic,  part  undemocratic  support,  yet  he 
has  used  no  undemocratic  values  among  his  supporting 
reasons. 

In  the  foregoing  explanation  each  of  the  scores  was  con- 
sidered independently.  This  is  normally  the  first  step  in  in- 
terpretation. Since  each  of  the  single  scores  describes  only 
one  part  of  a  pattern,  it  is  not  justifiable  to  draw  conclusions 
about  an  individual  without  considering  the  whole  pattern 
of  scores.  In  such  a  pattern,  a  score  often  assumes  a  meaning 
which  differs  from  the  one  gained  from  considering  it  by  it- 
self. In  attempting  a  pattern  interpretation  it  is  useful  to 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  189 

consider  these  scores  In  two  groups:  one  representing  the 
logical  aspects  (comprehensiveness,  consistency,  tenability, 
relevance;  columns  1  to  7),  the  other  representing  the  pat- 
tern of  values  (democratic,  undemocratic,  rationalization; 
columns  8  to  14).  However,  in  addition  to  examining  the 
scores  in  each  major  group  in  relation  to  each  other,  it  is  also 
necessary  to  consider  the  logical  aspects  in  the  light  of  the 
value  pattern  and  vice  versa. 

Student  A,  for  instance,  tends  to  be  comprehensive  in  his 
use  of  reasons.  At  the  same  time  he  is  somewhat  lacking  in 
logical  discrimination,  as  shown  by  his  tendency  to  accept 
inconsistent  and  irrelevant  statements  in  supporting  the 
courses  of  action.  Since  his  dominant  value  pattern  is  demo- 
cratic in  a  clear-cut  way,  one  is  led  to  infer  that  his  main 
difficulty  is  weakness  in  logical  discrimination. 

Student  C  shows  confusion  both  in  the  logical  aspects 
(relatively  high  inconsistency)  and  in  his  value  pattern  as 
shown  by  his  frequent  choice  of  compromise  courses  of  ac- 
tion and  of  rationalizations.  One  might  infer  from  this  that 
his  difficulties  with  logical  aspects  of  applying  values  stem 
from  the  confusion  of  the  values  he  accepts.  His  scores  on 
democratic  and  undemocratic  values  are  rather  evenly  di- 
vided and  a  high  score  on  rationalizations  suggests  gullibility 
to  democratic  slogans.  When  one  considers  in  addition  the 
fact  that  he  uses  only  a  few  supporting  reasons,  one  is  forced 
to  describe  the  whole  picture  as  that  of  a  lack  of  awareness 
of,  and  confusion  about,  social  issues. 

A  high  degree  of  inconsistency  is  one  of  the  major  facts 
about  Student  B.  But  because  his  value  pattern  tends  rather 
clearly  toward  the  undemocratic  one,  one  is  forced  to  con- 
clude that  his  main  difficulty  is  that  of  misapprehension  of 
logical  relationships  between  reasons  and  courses  of  action. 

The  patterns  of  reasoning  illustrated  above  are  found  re- 
currently among  students.  Some  students  may  be  broad  in 
their  reasoning  and  at  the  same  time  consistent,  discrirainat- 


190        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

ing,  and  have  a  clear  value  pattern.  Others  may  be  broad, 
but  inconsistent  and  ambivalent  in  value  pattern.  Some  are 
narrow,  clear,  and  have  a  democratic  value  pattern.  Others 
may  be  ambivalent  in  their  values,  but  not  inconsistent.  This 
usually  happens  when  they  take  different  positions  regarding 
the  different  issues  included  in  the  test  but  are  not  confused 
as  far  as  the  same  issue  is  concerned.  For  teachers  interested 
in  diagnosis  of  the  kinds  of  thinking  students  do  and  of  the 
ways  their  value  patterns  either  help  or  hinder  that  thinking, 
this  is  useful  information. 

VALIDITY  AND  RELIABILITY 

The  usefulness  of  this  instrument,  as  of  any  instrument,  is 
determined  by  ( 1 )  how  adequately  it  measures  what  it  sets 
out  to  measure  (validity)  and  (2)  how  reliable  a  particular 
set  of  the  students*  responses  is  likely  to  be.  The  problem 
of  validity  is  a  complex  one  and  includes  the  consideration 
of  the  validity  of  the  instrument  itself,  as  well  as  of  the  con- 
ditions under  which  the  test  is  given  and  taken.  In  this  sec- 
tion attention  is  devoted  to  the  discussion  of  the  validity  of 
the  instrument  itself.  The  conditions  under  which  valid  re- 
sults are  possible  in  a  given  situation  will  be  discussed  in  the 
section  on  uses. 

The  validity  of  the  results  from  a  test  of  this  type  is  deter- 
mined by  several  factors.  In  the  first  place,  there  may  be  a 
difference  between  the  behavior  specified  in  analysis  and  the 
behavior  actually  measured  by  the  test.  Any  test  situation  is 
an  artificial  situation  and  may  introduce  difficulties  irrelevant 
to  its  purpose.  Hence,  it  is  important  to  see  what  correspond- 
ence there  is  between  the  evidence  from  the  test  and  that 
obtained  from  freer  and  more  natural  situations. 

Each  test  also  employs  a  certain  method  of  scoring  and 
summarizing.  This  method  may  not  give  the  most  adequate 
picture  of  the  responses  to  the  test  and  therefore  it  is  neces- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  191 

saiy  to  determine  how  effective  the  method  of  scoring  and 
summarizing  is. 

Finally,  there  is  always  the  question  of  the  degree  to  which 
general  ability  affects  success  with  a  given  test.  This  test  does 
not  purport  to  be  a  measure  of  general  intelligence.  There- 
fore, some  evidence  is  needed  to  determine  the  relation  of 
this  factor  to  the  responses  to  this  instrument. 

Some  evidence  was  secured  on  all  of  these  points  in  the 
course  of  the  study.  Serious  effort  was  made  in  the  process  of 
constructing  the  test  to  assure  as  great  a  degree  of  validity  as 
possible.  Throughout  the  process  of  construction  steps  were 
taken  to  make  sure  that  the  test  appraised  the  behaviors  it 
was  intended  to  appraise.  As  was  indicated  in  the  description 
of  the  preliminary  analysis  and  of  the  exploratory  studies, 
care  was  taken  to  see  to  it  that  the  behavior  measured  as 
well  as  the  content  of  the  exercises  was  appropriate  to  the 
students  who  were  to  be  tested  and  consistent  with  the  ob- 
jectives and  curriculum  emphasis  of  the  schools.  The  prob- 
lems and  generalizations  included  in  the  test  were  chosen 
according  to  what  was  found  to  be  most  widely  emphasized 
in  the  schools  intending  to  use  the  test.  Student  responses  to 
essay  forms  were  examined  to  secure  reasons  representing 
the  types  of  values  and  patterns  of  reasoning  current  among 
the  students.  In  addition,  tentative  drafts  of  the  more  objec- 
tive forms  were  tried  out  and  revisions  were  made  on  the 
basis  of  the  responses. 

Similar  explorations  were  conducted  to  develop  the  most 
useful  categories  of  summary  and  methods  of  scoring.  The 
initial  choice  of  the  summary  categories  was  made  according 
to  the  suggestions  made  by  the  committee.  These  were  tried 
out  experimentally,  and  revisions  and  additions  were  made 
according  to  the  dependability  and  usefulness  of  the  par- 
ticular scores  as  shown  by  experimental  use  of  the  test.  Thus, 
for  instance,  some  of  the  rather  fine  classifications  of  values 
attempted  at  first  proved  impracticable  because  the  test 


192        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

could  not  be  made  long  enough  to  get  high  reliability  on 
these  scores. 

The  validity  of  the  diagnostic  descriptions  of  students 
made  from  the  test  scores  was  also  checked  informally 
throughout  the  Study.  In  each  school  where  the  test  was 
given  conferences  were  held  with  the  faculty.  Students  se- 
lected by  the  faculty  were  described  on  the  basis  of  the  test 
scores  and  these  descriptions  were  submitted  to  the  collec- 
tive judgment  of  the  faculty.  Usually  students  who  were 
known  by  most  teachers,  and  intimately  known  by  some, 
were  chosen  for  this  purpose.  This  was  done  in  about  25  of 
the  Thirty  Schools,  and  descriptions  of  several  hundred  stu- 
dents were  thus  examined  and  checked  in  the  course  of  two 
years.  Outright  disagreements  on  major  points  were  rare. 
These  occurred  mostly  in  cases  where  the  observations  of 
different  teachers  varied  considerably. 

Certain  difficulties  were  experienced  in  the  use  of  the  usual 
statistical  techniques  for  estimating  validity  and  reliability. 
The  scores  describing  the  logical  aspects  and  those  describing 
the  value  judgments  are  both  derived  from  a  single  process 
of  marking  by  the  student.  Each  aspect  influences  the  other, 
however,  and  interpretation  must  account  for  this  interrela- 
tionship. Thus  a  high  score  on  comprehensiveness  combined 
with  high  consistency  means  one  thing.  The  same  score  on 
comprehensiveness  combined  with  high  inconsistency  means 
something  different. 

However,  statistical  techniques  which  are  simple  enough 
for  practical  purposes  in  an  exploratory  study  such  as  this 
one  do  not  permit  the  treatment  of  the  validity  and  relia- 
bility data  in  terms  of  a  pattern  of  scores.  They  usually  are 
predicated  on  the  assumption  that  each  score  is  a  separate 
entity.  Hence  it  is  felt  that  the  quantitative  evidence  pre- 
sented in  substantiating  the  claims  for  a  certain  degree  of 
validity  and  reliability  of  the  instrument  do  not  do  full  justice 
to  it. 

Validity  was  investigated  by  the  following  three  methods: 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS 


193 


(1)  comparison  of  teacher  observations  with  test  scores,  (2) 
comparison  of  interviews  with  students  with  the  test  mate- 
rials, (3)  correlation  of  the  scores  on  this  test  with  scores  on 
psychological  tests. 

The  comparison  of  teacher  observations  with  the  test  re- 
sults was  employed  with  the  full  recognition  of  the  fact  that 
the  opportunities  for  teachers  to  observe  these  particular 
characteristics  were  apt  to  be  deficient  and  hence  not  fully 
reliable.  In  three  schools  a  selected  group  of  teachers  was 
asked  to  rate  a  group  of  senior  students  separately  on  the 
three  major  characteristics  diagnosed  in  the  test:  comprehen- 
siveness in  seeing  implications  of  social  values,  consistency 
of  their  social  reasoning,  and  the  pattern  of  social  values. 
Altogether,  132  students  from  three  schools  were  thus  rated. 
From  five  to  eight  teachers  in  each  school  participated,  with 
an  average  of  four  teachers  rating  each  student.  A  three- 
point  scale  (1 — high,  2 — average,  3 — low)  was  used  for  each 
of  the  characteristics.  These  ratings  were  then  compared  with 
the  corresponding  test  ratings.  The  results  are  presented  in 
the  table  below. 

MEAN   SQUARE    CONTINGENCY   CORRELATIONS 
OF   TEACHER   RATINGS   AND    TEST   RATINGS 


Compre- 
hensiveness 

Consistency 

Democratic 
Values 

School  I 

.49 

.63 

.38 

School  II 

.50 

.66 

.64 

School  III 

.60 

.41 

.58 

One  teacher  in  School  II 

.78 

.79 

.88 

These  data  suggest  that,  on  the  whole,  there  is  a  general 
agreement  between  teachers'  ratings  and  test  ratings.  All  cor- 
relations are  positive  and  with  three  exceptions  are  .50  or 
higher.  The  highest  relationships  were  found  in  School  II,  in 
which  the  teachers  participating  in  the  rating  had  the  best 


194        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

opportunities  to  observe  their  students.  The  ratings  of  the 
student  adviser  in  the  same  school  have  the  highest  corre- 
spondence with  test  scores.  Thus  the  relationship  between 
the  test  and  the  teacher  ratings  seems  to  increase  as  the  con- 
ditions necessary  for  reliable  teacher  rating  improve.  This 
would  suggest  that  the  reliability  of  teacher  ratings  is  a 
strong  factor  in  limiting  the  correspondence.  It  should  also 
be  remembered  that  while  in  the  normal  process  of  inter- 
preting the  results  of  this  test  the  meaning  of  a  single  score 
is  often  altered  in  the  light  of  the  whole  pattern  of  scores, 
single  scores  were  used  in  the  statistical  processes  of  com- 
puting the  correlations.  Hence,  the  coefficients  expressing 
the  correspondence  are  apt  to  be  lower  than  would  have 
been  the  case  had  it  been  possible  to  use  all  scores  in  rela- 
tionship to  each  other.  However,  in  spite  of  these  difficulties, 
these  data  suggest  that  when  thoughtful  judgments  are  made 
by  teachers  who  have  had  adequate  opportunity  to  observe 
students'  social  thinking,  a  rather  close  agreement  is  likely  to 
occur,  These  data  are  also  in  accord  with  the  hypothesis  that 
under  usual  classroom  conditions  teachers  would  be  able  to 
identify  most  of  the  extreme  cases  without  the  test,  but  that 
close  agreement  throughout  between  the  test  and  teacher 
rating  would  not  be  found,  since  teachers  ordinarily  do  not 
have  a  very  adequate  basis  for  observing  these  particular 
qualities  and  hence  for  rating  them  very  precisely. 

Another  method  used  was  that  of  interviewing  the  stu- 
dents. Forty-five  students,  15  from  each  of  three  schools, 
were  interviewed.  Their  specific  responses  to  the  test  items 
were  first  analyzed  and  summarized  in  a  written  statement. 
The  students  were  then  interviewed  regarding  their  view- 
points on  social  issues  included  in  the  test.  Through  a  series 
of  questions,  the  students  were  led  to  comment  on  the  kinds 
of  solutions  they  approved  and  the  reasons  why  they  thought 
these  solutions  were  appropriate.  Verbatim  records  of  these 
interviews  were  taken.  The  itemized  analysis  of  the  test  re- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  195 

sponses  and  interview  records  were  then  submitted  to  four 
judges,  all  of  whom  were  familiar  with  what  the  test  was 
attempting  to  measure.  These  judges  were  first  asked  to  indi- 
cate the  extent  of  agreement  between  what  the  students  said 
in  the  interview  and  how  they  had  marked  each  exercise  in 
the  test.  This  agreement  was  rated  on  a  three-point  scale:  1 — 
good,  2 — fair,  3 — poor.  An  average  rating  for  the  degree  of 
agreement  for  each  student  throughout  the  test  was  com- 
pounded by  adding  the  values  of  all  judges'  ratings  on  all 
exercises  and  by  dividing  this  total  by  the  number  of  ratings. 

In  most  cases  the  agreement  was  found  to  be  high.  Thus, 
the  mean  rating  on  all  students  on  all  problems  was  1.29, 
indicating  only  slightly  less  than  "good"  correspondence  in 
the  majority  of  cases.  The  lowest  average  rating  on  any  stu- 
dent was  slightly  better  than  "fair"  (1.78).  The  number  of 
"good"  ratings  represented  75  per  cent  of  the  total  number 
of  ratings,  while  the  number  of  cases  of  poor  correspondence 
represented  3  per  cent  of  the  total  ratings.  Thus  it  is  apparent 
that  these  judges  considered  the  interview  materials  to  be 
highly  consistent  with  the  test  responses.  This  is  particularly 
gratifying  in  view  of  the  fact  that  several  students  confessed 
a  change  of  viewpoint  between  the  taking  of  the  test  and 
the  interview. 

Three  of  the  judges  were  then  asked  to  consider  the  inter- 
view materials  alone  and  to  rate  each  student  on  three  as- 
pects measured  in  the  test:  comprehensiveness,  consistency, 
and  pattern  of  values,  on  a  three-point  scale  (high,  average, 
low),  in  order  to  get  some  evidence  of  the  adequacy  of  the 
summarization  and  scoring.  These  ratings  were  correlated 
with  the  test  ratings  on  the  corresponding  scores,  with  the 
following  results  (expressed  as  product-moment  correla- 
tions): comprehensiveness  .59,  consistency  .51,  democratic 
value  .66.  Considering  the  meagerness  of  the  interview  ma- 
terials for  rating  purposes  and  the  fact  that  the  interviews 
were  conducted  on  topics  similar  to  but  not  the  same  as  the 


196        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

test  exercises,  and  taking  account  of  the  difficulty  involved 
in  treating  the  test  scores  in  isolation  from  each  other,  it  is 
justifiable  to  assume  that  the  method  of  scoring  and  sum- 
marizing represents  student  responses  to  the  test  fairly  ade- 
quately. 

In  order  to  see  to  what  degree  general  intelligence  is  re- 
lated to  the  results  on  this  test,  the  scores  on  the  American 
Council  Psychological  Examination  for  45  students  were 
correlated  with  the  three  main  scores  on  this  test.  The  rela- 
tionship was  found  to  be  low  on  all  three;  namely,  compre- 
hensiveness .27,  consistency  .35,  democratic  values  .04.  The 
number  of  students  is  too  small  to  afford  conclusive  evidence, 
but  there  is  a  fair  indication  that  the  performance  on  this  test 
is  relatively  independent  of  the  abilities  measured  by  the 
psychological  examination. 

Several  checks  were  also  made  of  various  aspects  of  relia- 
bility. The  stability  of  scores  was  tested  by  several  methods 
of  estimating  reliability.  The  split-half  method  was  used  on 
scores  which  permitted  such  treatment.  The  Kuder-Richard- 
son  formula  was  used  wherever  the  split-half  method  did  not 
apply.9  The  estimated  reliability  for  the  score  on  per  cent 
democratic  values  was  obtained  by  correlating  Forms  1.41 
and  1.42  of  the  test.  The  coefficients  of  correlation  secured 
from  a  sample  of  600  students  in  tenth,  eleventh,  and  twelfth 
grades  range  from  .50  (untenable)  to  .91  (democratic 
values )  .10 

On  the  chief  scores  used  in  interpreting  the  results  ( com- 
prehensiveness ratio,  per  cent  inconsistency,  number  demo- 
cratic values,  number  undemocratic  values,  per  cent  demo- 
cratic values),  the  reliabilities  range  from  .70  to  .91,  which 
may  be  considered  fairly  high  for  a  test  of  this  type,  par- 
ticularly since  the  final  judgment  of  the  students'  behavior  is 
based  on  a  pattern  of  scores  and  does  not  depend  exclusively 

9  Loc.  cit. 

10  See  Appendix  for  a  complete  table  of  reliability  coefficients  by  grades. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  197 

on  any  one  single  score.  Low  reliabilities  were  found  on  the 
scores  on  untenable  reasons  (.50)  and  rationalizations  (.67). 
These  data  seem  to  indicate  that  this  test  has  sufficient 
validity  and  reliability  to  be  a  useful  instrument  for  diag- 
nosis. It  must  be  remembered  that  the  behavior  measured  in 
this  test  is  highly  complex,  affected  by  variability  in  the  in- 
terpretation of  test  statements  and  by  emotionalized  re- 
sponses. Hence,  objective  tests  in  this  area  probably  cannot 
be  judged  by  the  same  criteria  as  are  applied,  for  instance, 
to  tests  measuring  achievement  in  acquiring  information.  It 
is  also  likely  that  under  optimum  conditions,  where  teachers 
have  worked  seriously  on  this  objective,  and  students  are 
familiar  with  the  type  of  reasoning  and  the  kind  of  content 
involved,  both  the  reliability  and  validity  estimates  might  be 
higher. 

APPLYING  SOCIAL  FACTS  AND  GENERALIZATIONS  TO  SOCIAL 
PROBLEMS  (FORM  1.5) 

As  was  pointed  out  above,  teachers  of  the  social  studies 
were  concerned  with  students'  ability  to  apply  not  only  value 
judgments  but  also  relevant  and  accurate  information  in  their 
analysis  of  social  problems.  An  instrument  developed  to  ^get 
evidence  of  the  latter  ability  will  be  described  briefly,  since 
the  processes  involved  in  its  construction  were  analogous  to 
those  reported  at  length  in  the  preceding  section. 

Analysis  of  the  Objective 

The  analysis  of  the  objective  resulted  in  the  following  list 
of  important  types  of  behavior  to  be  evaluated:  (1)  The 
ability  to  see  the  logical  relations  between  general  principles 
and  specific  information  on  the  one  hand  and  the  issues  in- 
volved in  a  given  social  problem  on  the  other;  i.e.,  to  see 
whether  a  statement  supports,  contradicts,  or  is  irrelevant  to 
a  conclusion.  (2)  The  ability  to  evaluate  arguments  pre- 
sented in  discussing  a  specific  social  problem,  and  in  par- 


198        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

ticular,  to  discriminate  between  statements  of  verifiable  fact, 
statements  of  opinion  and  common  misconceptions.  (3)  The 
ability  to  judge  the  consistency  of  social  policies  with  social 
goals;  i.e.,  to  judge  the  appropriateness  of  certain  social  poli- 
cies for  achieving  certain  social  aims. 

There  are  two  major  types  of  situations  in  which  individ- 
uals make  use  of  these  abilities:  (1)  when  one  evaluates  a 
proposed  solution  of  any  social  problem,  and  (2)  when  one 
proposes  a  solution  and  tries  to  support  it.  The  test  described 
below  is  based  upon  the  first  type.  These  situations  occur  in 
the  consideration  of  a  wide  variety  of  problems,  involving 
many  types  of  generalizations  and  of  factual  information. 
Before  any  instruments  could  be  developed  in  this  field,  it 
was  necessary  to  make  a  choice  of  problem  areas  and  types 
of  generalizations  to  be  sampled.  The  list  of  social  science 
generalizations  and  of  significant  problem  areas  submitted 
by  the  teachers  and  discussed  above  was  used  as  the  primary 
source  of  issues  upon  which  to  build  the  test.11  These  were 
checked  further  with  respect  to  the  frequency  with  which 
they  occurred  in  high  school  courses  on  social  problems.  The 
following  problem  areas  were  selected:  consumer  buying, 
health,  unemployment,  housing,  soil  conservation,  civil  liber- 
ties, international  relations,  taxation,  and  civil  service. 

Description  of  the  Instrument 

Exercises  were  constructed  for  each  of  the  problem  areas 
listed  above.  Each  exercise  is  a  complete  test  in  itself  and  can 
be  used  independently  of  the  others.  An  exercise  is  composed 
of  several  parts,  constructed  in  such  a  way  as  to  give  evi- 
dence of  the  three  abilities  listed  in  the  analysis  of  the  ob- 
jective. In  the  first  part  of  the  exercise  a  social  problem  is 
described,  and  one  of  the  frequently  suggested  solutions  is 
indicated.  Various  statements  (some  supporting,  some  con- 
tradicting, and  some  irrelevant)  concerning  the  solution  are 
11  See  p.  170. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  199 

presented.  The  student  is  asked  to  indicate  whether  each 
statement  supports,  contradicts,  or  is  irrelevant  to  the  sug- 
gested solution.  A  student's  reactions  to  this  part  of  the  test 
are  summarized  in  terms  of  the  number  of  accurate  responses 
he  makes,  the  number  of  times  he  confuses  supporting  and 
contradictory  statements,  and  the  number  of  times  he  fails  to 
see  the  relevance  of  a  statement  to  the  conclusion.  The  state- 
ments include  basic  assumptions,  general  principles,  accurate 
information,  and  common  misconceptions.  In  the  second  part 
of  the  test  the  student  is  asked  to  indicate  whether  each  of 
the  statements  can  be  proved  to  be  either  true  or  false.  The 
student's  reactions  to  this  section  are  summarized  in  terms 
of  the  number  of  times  he  discriminates  between  statements 
of  fact  and  assumptions,  the  number  of  times  he  marks  value 
judgments  as  verifiable,  the  number  of  times  he  marks  state- 
ments of  fact  as  not  verifiable,  and  the  number  of  times  he 
discriminates  accurately  between  true  statements  and  com- 
mon misconceptions.  An  excerpt  from  one  exercise  is  given 
below.  The  key  is  indicated  at  the  left  of  each  statement. 

HOUSING12 

Application  of  Form  I 

Principles  1.5  (Tentative  Draft) 

Problem: 

Housing  is  one  of  the  problems  of  concern  today.  Many 
schemes  have  been  suggested  as  a  means  of  improving  housing 
conditions.  In  general,  there  are  two  major  ways  in  which  govern- 
ment can  aid  in  solving  this  problem:  (1)  by  setting  standards 
for  and  regulating  the  construction  of  private  housing,  and  (2) 
by  building  houses  at  public  expense,  contributing  either  part  or 
all  of  the  funds  necessary.  Each  method  has  certain  advantages 
and  disadvantages.  Nevertheless,  many  people  believe  that  the 
government  should  build  houses  at  public  expense  to  rent  to  those 
sections  of  the  population  with  the  lowest  incomes. 

12  In  all  cases  where  the  phrase  "decent  house"  or  its  equivalent  is  used, 
it  is  to  be  defined  as  a  separate  house  or  apartment  for  each  family  with 
running  water,  inside  bath,  fire  protection,  and  enough  room  for  privacy. 


200        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

I.  Directions:  For  each  of  the  following  statements,  place  a  check 
mark  (V)  in  one  °f  ^ne  columns  labeled  Part  I.  Place  the 
check  mark  (V)  opposite  the  number  which  corresponds  to 
the  number  of  the  statement  in: 

Column  A  if  the  statement  may  logically  be  used  to  support 
the  underlined  conclusion. 

Column  B  if  the  statement  may  logically  "be  used  to  contradict 

the  underlined  conclusion. 

Column  C  if  the  statement  neither  supports  nor  contradicts 

the  underlined  conclusion. 

Check  each  item  in  only  one  column.  In  case  of  doubt,  give  the 

answer  which  seems  most  nearly  right. 

In  this  part  of  the  exercise,  assume  that  each  statement  is  true. 

Supports  1.  Whenever  houses  are  not  available  to  the 

Assumption  public,  society  should  assume  the  responsi- 

bility for  making  it  possible  for  everyone  to 
have  a  decent  place  to  live. 

Contradicts  3,  Government-built  houses  are  more  expensive 

Misconception          to  construct  than  comparable  houses  built  by 

private  companies. 

Supports  11.  It  has  been  demonstrated  that  the  federal 

Misconception          government  can  build  adequate  houses  for 

the  lowest  income  group  cheaply  enough  so 

that  they  can  be  paid  for  out  of  income  from 

rent. 

Contradicts         14.  Individuals  who  have  heavy  investments  in 
Accurate  slum  property  would  probably  suffer  heavy 

losses  if  a  broad  program  of  federal  housing 
went  into  effect. 

Contradicts         17.  The  system  of  private  initiative  in  business 
Assumption  should  not  be  jeopardized  by  the  socialization 

of  any  of  the  fundamental  industries. 

Supports  20.  Under  present  conditions,  at  least  50  per  cent 

Accurate  of  the  people  cannot  easily  afford  to  own  a 

decent  home;  at  least  one-third  of  the  popula- 
tion cannot  afford  to  rent  decent  homes. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  201 

Irrelevant  22.  Comparable  houses  can  frequently  be  rented 

Accurate  in  the  suburbs  for  somewhat  lower  rentals 

than  in  the  city. 

II.  Directions:   Go  back  over  the  statements.  In  the  columns 
labeled  Part  II  place  a  check  mark  (\/)  opposite  the  number 
which  corresponds  to  the  number  of  the  statement  in: 

Column  D  if  you  believe  that  the  statement  can  be  proved  to 
be  true. 

Column  E  if  you  believe  that  the  statement  can  be  proved  to 
be  false. 

Column  F  if  you  believe  the  statement  cannot  be  proved  to  be 
either  true  or  false. 

Check  each  item  in  only  one  column.  In  case  of  doubt,  give 
what  seems  to  you  to  be  the  one  best  answer. 

When  you  have  finished  Part  II,  go  on  to  Part  III. 

A  student  may  be  able  to  make  the  logical  analysis  and  to 
evaluate  the  argument  very  accurately  and  yet  may  not  be 
able  to  judge  whether  or  not  a  given  social  policy  is  likely  to 
achieve  a  given  social  objective.  Therefore,  in  the  third  part 
of  the  test  the  student  is  given  opportunity  to  make  this  type 
of  judgment.  This  part  of  the  test  consists  of  a  statement  of 
a  particular  social  objective  ( such  as  the  improvement  of  the 
housing  conditions  of  the  third  of  the  population  with  the 
lowest  income),  and  several  proposals,  some  appropriate, 
some  inappropriate,  for  achieving  this  objective.  The  student 
is  asked  to  indicate  which  proposals  he  thinks  would  be  ef- 
fective in  achieving  the  objective.  His  reactions  to  this  sec- 
tion of  the  test  are  summarized  in  terms  of  the  number  of 
times  he  chooses  policies  which  are  helpful  in  achieving  the 
stated  objective. 

An  illustration  of  this  part  of  the  test  is  given  below. 

III.  Directions:  In  the  column  labeled  Part  III  opposite  the  num- 
ber which  corresponds  to  the  number  of  the  statement,  write: 


202        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

A  plus  sign  (+)  if  it  expresses  a  type  of  action  which  you 
think  would  improve  the  housing  conditions  of  that  third  of 
the  population  with  the  lowest  incomes. 

A  zero  sign  (0)  if  it  does  not  express  a  type  of  action  which 
you  think  would  improve  the  housing  conditions  of  that  third 
of  the  population  with  the  lowest  incomes. 
+    1.  New  buildings  should  be  required  to  measure  up  to  higher 

minimum  standards  for  construction. 
+    2.  Credit  for  housing  should  be  supplied  in  larger  quantities 

and  at  lower  rates  of  interest. 
0      3.  All  city  land  should  be  reassessed. 
0      4.  Laws  should  be  passed  requiring  the  destruction  of  all 

slum  areas. 

-f-    5.  The  government  should  subsidize  housing  for  lower  in- 
come groups. 

Accurate  response  to  each  of  the  first  three  steps  involves 
the  use  of  certain  general  information.  In  case  the  student 
makes  a  large  number  of  inaccurate  responses,  it  is  impor- 
tant to  know  whether  it  is  because  he  does  not  have  the  in- 
formation or  whether  he  knows  the  facts  of  the  situation  but 
cannot  apply  them.  Therefore,  in  the  last  section  of  the  test 
the  student  is  asked  to  judge  the  truth  or  falsity  of  a  series  of 
statements  which  sample  the  information  that  is  assumed  in 
the  preceding  sections  of  the  test. 

A  sample  of  the  factual  statements  in  this  section  of  the 
test  which  correspond  to  the  arguments  used  in  the  illustra- 
tion of  Part  I  is  given  below: 

Directions:  Form  II.  The  following  items  refer  to  the  problem  of 
housing.  In  the  columns  labeled  Form  II  place  a  check  mark 
(V)  opposite  the  number  which  corresponds  to  the  number  of 
the  statement,  in: 

Column  X  if  you  believe  the  statement  to  be  true. 
Column  Y  if  you  believe  the  statement  to  be  false. 

Column  Z  if  you  are  uncertain  whether  the  statement  is  true  or 
false. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  203 

True  1.  At  present  various  estimates  agree  that  at  least  one- 
third  of  the  population  lives  in  unsanitary  or  un- 
healthy homes. 

False  3.  On  the  average,  the  cost  of  federal  housing  has  been 
approximately  $1?000  more  per  unit  than  the  cost  of 
comparable  private  construction. 

False  11.  To  date  the  income  from  rent  on  housing  projects 
has  been  large  enough  to  pay  for  the  original  cost  of 
the  investment  in  a  relatively  short  time. 

False  14.  Government  competition  in  the  construction  of  low- 
cost  housing  would  probably  not  affect  the  value  of 
slum  property. 

True  17.  In  the  past,  housing  has  been  one  of  the  largest  pri- 
vate industries  in  the  United  States. 

True  20.  More  than  50  per  cent  of  the  families  in  the  United 
States  have  an  annual  income  of  $1,800  or  less;  while 
at  the  same  time  over  three-fourths  of  the  houses 
built  in  the  last  five  years  were  built  to  be  sold  for 
over  $4,000. 

False  22.  Statistical  studies  show  that  cost  of  living  is  as  high 
in  suburban  areas  as  in  the  metropolitan  districts. 

Reactions  to  these  statements  are  summarized  in  terms  of 
the  number  of  accurate,  inaccurate,  and  uncertain  responses. 
These  scores  are  used  primarily  for  aiding  the  interpretation 
of  scores  on  the  first  two  sections  of  the  test. 

EVALUATION  OF  SOCIAL  ATTITUDES 
ANALYSIS  OF  THE  OBJECTIVE 

The  study  of  social  attitudes  has  been  of  concern  to  Amer- 
ican psychologists  and  sociologists  for  a  long  time.  The  litera- 
ture on  this  subject,  however,  reveals  a  great  diversity  of 
opinion  regarding  the  proper  delimitation  of  the  behaviors 
to  be  called  "attitudes"  and  the  terminology  to  be  used  in 
denoting  that  behavior.  Similar  diversities  also  prevail  in  the 
conceptions  of  die  important  characteristics  of  "attitudes" 


2o4        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

and  in  the  techniques  employed  in  measuring  these  charac- 
teristics. 

The  difficulties  with  the  definition  and  classification  of  at- 
titudes soon  became  apparent  as  the  schools  began  apprais- 
ing social  attitudes.  While  die  development  of  social  attitudes 
was  one  of  the  most  widely  emphasized  objectives  among 
the  schools  in  the  Eight- Year  Study,  there  seemed  to  be  little 
clarity  regarding  the  kind  of  behavior  this  objective  involved 
and  the  significant  areas  in  which  it  was  important  to  develop 
and  appraise  social  attitudes. 

Analysis  of  Behavior 

The  initial  statements  from  the  schools  revealed  that  many 
diverse  types  of  behavior  were  considered  to  be  social  atti- 
tudes. Thus,  some  mathematics  teachers  submitted  the  ability 
to  see  quantitative  relationships  as  an  illustration  of  an  atti- 
tude. Willingness  to  make  an  effort  to  express  oneself  clearly 
was  one  of  the  attitudes  suggested  by  English  teachers.  Often 
objectives  which  seemed  more  closely  related  to  interests 
and  appreciations  were  included  in  this  classification.  Such 
personal  qualities  as  resourcefulness,  initiative  in  school  work, 
and  open-mindedness  about  the  ideas  of  other  people,  along 
with  beliefs  about  a  wide  range  of  social  issues,  were  sug- 
gested in  the  statements  of  objectives  submitted  by  the 
schools. 

Recognizing  the  difficulties  arising  from  the  lack  of  clarity 
as  to  what  kinds  of  behavior  could  be  classified  as  attitudes 
and  the  diversity  of  objects  toward  which  the  suggested  atti- 
tudes were  directed,  the  committee  on  social  attitudes  pro- 
ceeded along  two  major  lines  of  analysis.  It  attempted  (1) 
to  describe  the  nature  of  social  attitudes  sufficiently  to  dis- 
tinguish them  from  other  school  objectives,  such  as  interests 
and  appreciations,  and  (2)  to  delineate  the  major  areas  of 
social  issues  toward  which  social  attitudes  developed  in 
school  were  usually  directed.  In  doing  this  the  committee 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  205 

recognized  that  it  could  not  solve  the  problem  of  defining 
and  classifying  attitudes  in  a  comprehensive  fashion.  Since 
the  committee  was  concerned  with  evaluation,  it  tried  to 
identify  only  those  aspects  of  social  attitudes  which  consti- 
tuted important  objectives  of  the  schools. 

From  this  viewpoint  the  following  distinguishing  charac- 
teristics of  attitudes  were  identified: 

1.  An  attitude  may  involve  a  feelingtone  of  acceptance  or 
rejection.  This  feelingtone  may  be  evoked  by  an  idea,  a  per- 
son, a  way  of  behaving,  or  a  mode  of  doing  things.  Thus  one 
may  like  or  dislike  a  person;  reject  or  accept  authoritarian 
methods;  be  afraid  of  or  feel  at  home  with  members  of  the 
other  sex,  strange  manners,  or  novel  experiences.  Attitudes 
of  this  sort  are  rather  directly  expressed  in  immediate  be- 
havior and  the  possession  of  "an  attitude"  may  not  neces- 
sarily be  consciously  recognized  by  the  person  concerned. 

2.  To  have  a  belief  about,  or  an  opinion  about,  or  to  take 
position  toward  an  issue,  value,  or  institution  may  be  con- 
sidered another  type  of  attitude.  Thus  one  may  approve  of 
equality  for  Negroes,  be  for  or  against  religion,  disapprove 
of  government  control,  believe  in  the  efficacy  of  democratic 
processes,  or  be  opposed  to  war.  Though  beliefs  of  this  sort 
are  not  always  arrived  at  by  rational  processes,  they  usually 
involve  a  conscious  intellectual  recognition  that  a  position  is 
being  taken. 

3.  Often  attitudes  represent  a  latent  tendency  to  act,  such 
as  the  disposition  to  be  kindly  and  considerate  toward  aliens, 
to  defend  the  rights  of  minorities,  or  to  proceed  democrat- 
ically in  managing  student  government.  Presumably  these 
tendencies  prevail  as  a  result  of  conscious  beliefs.  However, 
this  does  not  mean  that  there  is  of  necessity  a  consistent  rela- 
tionship between  what  one  believes  and  the  character  of 
overt  action.  Overt  behavior  may  often  be  inconsistent  with 
one's  conscious  beliefs,  or  it  may  express  or  imply  value  posi- 
tions not  consciously  recognized  as  such  by  the  individual. 


206        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Thus  one  may  express  prejudices  toward  certain  ideas  and 
values  in  one's  daily  behavior  without  reflecting  upon  the 
implications  of  these  actions  or  without  recognizing  the  be- 
liefs which  may  have  motivated  them. 

The  problem  of  distinguishing  the  ways  in  which  attitudes 
and  social  beliefs  could  be  expressed  was  of  major  impor- 
tance for  purposes  of  evaluation,  since  these  distinctions 
would  largely  determine  the  techniques  to  be  used  in  ap- 
praisal. For  this  reason  the  relationship  between  "beliefs 
about"  or  "feeling  toward"  and  overt  action  was  discussed  at 
length  by  the  committee.  Considered  from  the  standpoint  of 
the  techniques  to  be  used  in  appraisal  of  attitudes,  the  lists 
of  specific  attitudes  submitted  by  the  teachers  suggested 
three  groupings.  Some  of  these  objectives  referred  to  atti- 
tudes pertaining  to  immediate  social  relations,  such  as  co- 
operation and  respect  for  others.  The  schools  were  concerned 
with  attitudes  of  this  sort  primarily  as  expressed  in  some  form 
of  overt  action.  This  type  of  attitude  could  therefore  be  ap- 
praised best  by  means  of  anecdotal  recordings,  behavior 
records,  and  observational  checklists  to  be  devised  by  each 
school  for  its  own  use.13 

Another  series  of  attitudes  also  permits  expression  in  overt 
behavior,  but  social  conventions  and  personal  inhibitions 
tend  to  suppress  that  expression.  Attitudes  toward  the  other 
sex,  toward  family  relations,  toward  certain  aspects  of  one's 
own  personality,  and  so  on,  are  of  this  sort.  Indirect  methods 
of  appraising  these  attitudes  are  necessary.  A  method  of  this 
type  is  described  in  the  chapter  on  Personal  and  Social  Ad- 
justment. 

13  Several  such  devices  were  developed.  Behavior  records  developed 
under  the  leadership  of  Eugene  R.  Smith  will  be  discussed  in  Part  II  of  this 
book.  The  Francis  Parker  School  developed  a  checklist,  "Record  for  De- 
scribing Attitudes  and  Behavior  in  High  School"  covering:  I,  Cooperation; 
II,  Responsibility;  and  III,  Attitude  toward  School  Work.  A  somewhat 
similar  scheme  for  collecting  anecdotal  records  was  adopted  in  the  Tower 
Hill  School. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  207 

A  third  group  dealt  with  such  social  issues  as  international 
relations,  unemployment,  freedom  of  speech,  and  democracy 
in  school.  While  measurable  consequences  in  overt  behavior 
attend  some  of  these  attitudes,  their  expression  is  largely 
confined  to  a  theoretical  or  verbal  level.  Even  adults  as  indi- 
viduals have  only  limited  opportunities  for  expressing  their 
beliefs  through  overt  action.  Thus,  for  example,  belief  in  the 
desirability  of  government  aid  to  agriculture  would  in  the 
case  of  most  people  be  expressed  in  verbal  arguments,  in 
taking  sides  on  ideas  presented  in  print,  or  in  writing  about 
these  issues.  Only  such  "token  overt  action"  as  writing  to 
one's  Senator  or  casting  a  vote  on  certain  measures  affecting 
the  issue  seemed  to  be  open  to  the  majority  of  people  on  a 
great  many  social  issues.  On  the  other  hand,  in  a  democracy 
die  beliefs  held  by  people  influence  social  action  by  groups, 
and  consequently  a  great  deal  of  effort  is  directed  toward 
clarifying  beliefs  and  opinions  on  controversial  issues.  It  was 
therefore  thought  important  to  appraise  the  development  of 
these  beliefs  even  though  the  appraisal  would  have  to  be 
confined  to  verbal  expression  of  beliefs.  Scales  of  beliefs  in- 
viting reactions  toward  statements  of  opinion  on  significant 
social  issues  seemed  the  most  economical  and  appropriate 
method  for  appraising  attitudes  of  this  sort. 

Areas  of  Social  Beliefs 

One  of  the  first  tasks  in  developing  an  instrument  to  eval- 
uate social  beliefs  was  to  secure  suggestions  regarding  the 
major  areas  of  social  beliefs  to  be  covered  in  the  appraisal. 
Obviously,  it  is  possible  to  have  a  belief  about  almost  any- 
thing, and  almost  anything  can  be  covered  by  the  term 
"social."  It  was  clear  also  that  certain  of  the  possible  areas 
of  social  beliefs  were  of  more  concern  to  schools  than  were 
others.  The  schools  were,  therefore,  asked  to  suggest  the 
areas  of  social  beliefs  in  which  they  were  interested.  In  sev- 
eral cases  both  students  and  parents  as  well  as  teachers  par- 


208        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

ticipated  in  this  exploration.  The  rating  scales  and  attitude 
tests  already  in  use  in  schools  were  also  examined.  Samples 
of  student  writing  were  analyzed,  as  were  their  choices  of 
"research"  topics  and  free  reading.  In  some  classes  daily  logs 
of  topics  of  discussion  were  kept. 

When  compiled,  these  suggestions  included  the  following 
areas  of  social  issues:  democracy — political  and  economic, 
the  role  of  the  machine  and  invention  in  contemporary  civ- 
ilization, consumer  problems,  use  of  natural  resources,  labor, 
unemployment,  housing,  nationalism  and  internationalism, 
war  and  peace,  school  life,  religion,  and  family.  Some  of 
these  were  mentioned  by  all  schools  and  others  by  only  a  few. 

In  order  to  provide  means  of  appraisal  of  so  varied  a  range 
of  social  beliefs,  a  series  of  instruments  was  developed.  With 
the  exception  of  one  instrument  devoted  to  appraisal  of  be- 
liefs on  issues  of  school  life,  all  of  them  deal  with  large  social 
issues.  The  following  list  indicates  the  scope  of  this  project. 

1.  Beliefs  on  Social  Issues  (Form  4.21-4.31),  an  instru- 
ment covering  general  social  issues.  Two  forms  were 
developed,  one  for  the  senior  high  school  level,  an- 
other for  the  junior  high  school.14 

2.  Beliefs  on  School  Life   (Form  4.6),  an  instrument 
covering  issues  in  the  area  of  school  relationships. 

These  two  instruments  included  issues  which  were  sug- 
gested by  a  large  number  of  schools  and  were  designed  for 
general  use.  IA  addition,  several  instruments  were  developed 
for  more  specific  purposes.  These  included: 

3.  Beliefs  on  Economic  Issues.  This  was  made  for  a 
school  particularly  interested  in  developing  economic 
attitudes  through  the  study  of  selected  short  stories 
and  poems. 

14  Another  form  (4.9-4.10)  included  religion  and  family  life  in  addition 
to  the  areas  covered  in  Form  4.21-4.31. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  209 

4.  A  series  of  instruments  sampling  in  detail  beliefs  on 
such  issues  as  Men  and  Machines,  Distribution  of 

Wealth,  Consumer  Problems,  and  Use  of  National 
Resources,  designed  for  a  school  emphasizing  these 

particular  problems. 

5.  Beliefs   on  Housing  in  your  Community,   for  two 
schools  conducting  an  intensive  study  of  housing. 

Of  these,  the  development  of  the  instrument  Beliefs  on 
Social  Issues  is  discussed  in  detail  in  this  chapter.  Brief  ac- 
counts are  given  of  the  Beliefs  on  School  Life  and  Economic 
Beliefs. 

EVALUATION  OF  BELIEFS  ON  SOCIAL  ISSUES 

Before  an  instrument  suitable  for  appraising  beliefs  on 
social  issues  could  be  developed,  it  was  necessary  to  (1) 
select  the  areas  of  issues  to  include,  (2)  determine  the  types 
of  sub-issues  to  sample  in  each  area,  ( 3 )  decide  on  the  level 
of  intensity  at  which  each  of  the  statements  in  the  test  should 
be  formulated,  (4)  designate  the  characteristics  of  beliefs 
which  were  to  be  measured,  and  (5)  choose  a  technique 
appropriate  for  securing  and  summarizing  the  responses  of 
students.  This  section  summarizes  the  preliminary  investiga- 
tions which  influenced  the  final  decision  on  these  problems. 

Sampling  of  Issues  and  Formulation  of  Statements 

From  the  list  submitted  by  the  teachers,  six  areas  of  inter- 
est to  many  schools  were  chosen  by  the  committee.  These 
were:  democracy,  economic  relations,  labor  and  unemploy- 
ment, race,  nationalism,  and  militarism.  The  problem  of 
determining  the  specific  issues  to  be  sampled  in  each  area 
and  their  specific  direction  was  a  more  difficult  one.  To  have 
a  discriminating  instrument,  it  is  not  only  necessary  to  sam- 
ple the  significant  aspects  of  an  issue  but  also  to  sample  the 
major  variations  in  beliefs  about  these  aspects.  Each  one  of 


210        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

the  major  areas  chosen  was  broad  enough  to  involve  a  host 
of  more  specific  aspects.  Thus  the  issue  of  equality  of  races 
involves  such  specifics  as  equality  of  work  opportunity,  of 
education,  of  political  and  civic  rights,  of  social  relations, 
and  so  on.  A  quite  different  set  of  sub-issues  appears  when 
the  causes  and  consequences  of  racial  equality  or  inequality 
are  considered.  The  positions  taken  toward  each  of  these 
aspects  of  racial  equality  may  differ  considerably  in  the  case 
of  the  same  individual,  as  well  as  from  individual  to  indi- 
vidual Thus  those  who  believe  that  Negroes  should  have 
educational  opportunities  equal  to  those  of  whites  may  not 
believe  that  both  groups  should  have  equal  opportunities  for 
every  kind  of  work. 

For  an  effective  appraisal  of  beliefs  it  is  also  important 
to  determine  a  reasonable  threshold  for  each  statement.  A 
statement  of  a  position  toward  any  social  issue  can  be 
phrased  with  any  degree  of  intensity.  It  can  be  phrased  so 
strongly  that  very  few  people  can  agree  with  it,  or  so  mildly 
that  most  people  responding  to  it  can  agree  with  it.  Thus,  a 
statement  expressing  opposition  to  equality  for  Negroes  could 
be  phrased  to  deny  any  form  of  equality  or  permit  only  cer- 
tain kinds  of  equality.  A  statement  implying  low  standards 
of  morality  or  lack  of  intellectual  ability  could  be  applied 
to  all  Negroes,  or  only  to  Negroes  of  certain  social  status, 
and  so  on.  Effective  statements  for  the  purpose  of  the  meas- 
uring instrument  are  ones  which  elicit  a  reasonable  amount 
of  both  agreement  and  disagreement  from  the  students. 

Interwoven  with  this  problem  of  threshold  is  the  question 
of  the  use  of  language  in  the  statement  of  beliefs.  Because 
of  the  general  nature  of  the  issues,  a  certain  degree  of  ab- 
stractness  in  stating  them  seemed  unavoidable.  Abstract 
terms,  however,  are  often  subject  to  different  interpretations 
by  different  people.  Statements  of  opinion  frequently  necessi- 
tate the  use  of  emotionally  colored  words,  the  interpretation 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  211 

of  which  varies  from  person  to  person.  Care  was  therefore  nec- 
essary to  avoid  words  likely  to  be  ambiguous  to  the  students 
or  likely  to  create  emotional  reactions  causing  an  interpreta- 
tion irrelevant  to  the  intended  meaning  of  the  statement. 

To  get  suggestions  on  how  to  deal  with  these  problems, 
students  in  several  schools  were  asked  to  submit  statements 
of  opinion  on  issues  in  each  of  the  six  areas  chosen.  Several 
hundred  statements  of  opinion  were  collected  in  this  way. 
A  selection  of  these  chosen  from  each  area  was  resubmitted 
to  the  students.  They  were  asked  to  indicate  their  agreement 
or  disagreement  with  each  of  the  statements  and  then  to 
arrange  all  of  the  statements  in  ten  groups,  ranging  from  the 
ones  they  thought  stated  strong  opposition  to  ones  stating 
strong  approval  of  the  central  issue  in  each  area. 

The  results  from  these  studies  were  used  in  several  ways. 
By  a  priori  analysis,  lists  of  important  issues  to  be  sampled 
in  each  area  had  been  drawn  up.  These  lists  were  checked 
against  the  items  suggested  by  the  students  to  eliminate  any 
issues  of  which  students  did  not  seem  to  be  aware.  The  re- 
duced lists  of  issues  then  served  as  a  basis  for  formulating 
statements  for  the  test.  In  the  area  of  democracy,  for  exam- 
ple, the  statements  sampled  the  following  issues: 

1.  Civil  liberties,  such  as  freedom  of  speech,  the  right  to 
trial  by  a  jury,  and  the  right  to  vote. 

2.  Equality  of  opportunity  and  responsibility  in  a  democ- 
racy, such  as  equality  in  economic  and  educational  op- 
portunities, and  equality  of  responsibility  in  carrying  the 
financial  burden  of  the  government. 

3.  Manner  of  appointing  and  electing  government  officials 
and  representatives. 

4.  Functions  and  responsibilities  of  democratic  government 
in  promoting  general  welfare,  such  as  providing  medical 
care  and  social  security  for  all. 

5.  Freedoms  and  responsibilities  of  citizens  in  a  democracy. 

6.  Influences  of  social  and  economic  classes  on  democracy. 


ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCAIiON 

From  the  students'  responses  It  was  also  possible  to  deter- 
mine the  kinds  of  statements  which  were  so  extreme  as  to 
elicit  either  a  unanimous  agreement  or  a  unanimous  dis- 
agreement. Usually  only  the  items  on  which  there  was  a 
reasonable  division  of  opinion  were  chosen.  In  a  few  in- 
stances, however,  items  were  retained  because  they  were 
considered  important  and  because  there  was  reason  to  be- 
lieve that  unanimity  of  opinion  was  caused  by  some  special 
factor  in  the  background  of  these  students  rather  than  by 
the  fact  that  the  issue  was  not  in  general  a  debatable  or  a 
significant  one.  Whenever  possible,  the  terms  used  by  stu- 
dents were  employed.  All  statements  were  scrutinized  by  a 
jury  of  12- persons  for  possible  ambiguity,  or  other  verbal 
difficulties,  and  for  their  relevance  to  the  major  issue. 

Characteristics  to  Be  Diagnosed 

In  considering  the  characteristics  of  beliefs,  three  were 
found  to  be  of  importance  to  schools.  In  the  first  place,  the 
teachers  wanted  to  see  whether  increased  understanding  of 
social  problems  brought  about  an  ability  and  willingness  to 
take  personal  positions  upon  an  increasing  range  of  social 
issues.  One  of  the  main  criticisms  of  social  education  in 
schools  had  been  centered  on  the  failure  to  develop  in  stu- 
dents personal  viewpoints  toward  important  social  issues. 
It  was  therefore  decided  that  the  prospective  instrument 
should  be  so  set  up  as  to  diagnose  the  extent  to  which 
students  are  able  and  willing  to  take  a  definite  stand  on 
social  issues. 

Teachers  were  also  interested  in  learning  about  the  direc- 
tion of  positions  taken  by  the  students.  Thus  they  wanted 
to  know  whether  on  the  whole  students  accepted  or  rejected 
the  principle  of  universal  freedom  of  speech,  whether  stu- 
dents were  for  or  against  certain  measures  to  alleviate  pov- 
erty and  unemployment,  and  so  on.  This  interest  in  the  type 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  2,13 

of  positions  taken  did  not  imply  a  decision  regarding  the 
desirability  of  any  one  specific  position,  however.  While 
there  was  a  fairly  close  agreement  among  the  teachers  on 
the  desirability  of  developing  acceptance  of  democratic  proc- 
esses and  of  racial  tolerance,  it  seemed  both  impossible  and 
undesirable  to  classify  the  positions  on  many  other  issues  as 
desirable  or  undesirable.  At  the  same  time,  it  seemed  neces- 
sary to  adopt  some  scheme  of  distinguishing  and  classifying 
the  positions  taken  toward  the  statements  of  opinion  in- 
cluded in  the  test.  Unfortunately,  most  of  the  terms  used  to 
refer  to  the  direction  of  attitudes  suggest  an  idea  of  right- 
ness  or  wrongness,  approval  or  condemnation  of  a  given 
position.  The  members  of  the  committee  wished  to  avoid 
such  terms  for  summarizing  the  test  results,  but  found  it 
impossible  to  locate  any  terms  which  did  not  have  such  con- 
notations. The  terms  liberal  and  conservative  were  finally 
adopted  as  a  convenient  way  of  describing  two  opposite 
directions  on  issues  selected  for^  the  test.  The  meanings 
adopted  for  these  terms  will  be  discussed  later  in  connection 
with  the  description  of  the  scoring  and  summarizing  of  the 
responses. 

The  consistency  of  students'  beliefs  was  a  third  character- 
istic teachers  wished  to  diagnose.  Teachers  regarded  con- 
sistency as  a  desirable  characteristic  of  social  beliefs,  no 
matter  which  position  was  taken.  The  committee  recognized 
at  least  two  levels  of  consistency.  Generalizing  a  multitude 
of  specific  beliefs  in  different  areas  into  a  coherent  and  con- 
sistent viewpoint  represented  one  level.  Inconsistency  in  this 
case  would  reveal  itself  by  a  shift  of  viewpoint  from  area 
to  area.  The  other  and  more  specific  level  involves  the  con- 
sistency of  beliefs  toward  the  same  issue.  Inconsistency  in 
this  case  means  agreement  with  expressions  of  opposite  view- 
points on  the  same  issue.  It  seemed  possible  to  diagnose  con- 
sistency of  the  first  type  by  examining  the  direction  of  be- 


2i4        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

liefs  in  each  of  the  areas.  To  get  evidence  on  consistency  of 
the  second  type,  two  statements  expressing  opposite  view- 
points on  each  issue  were  included  in  the  instrument. 

Techniques  of  Constructing  the  Scale 

There  are  several  possible  techniques  of  securing  and 
summarizing  the  responses  of  students  to  statements  of  is- 
sues. Thurstone  regards  the  intensity  of  a  feeling  or  position 
as  the  most  significant  characteristic  of  attitudes,  and  has 
developed  a  series  of  scales  measuring  the  intensity  of  the 
favorable  and  unfavorable  positions  toward  a  single  issue, 
such  as  war  and  peace.15  Each  statement  in  a  scale  contain- 
ing 20  or  more  represents  a  position  toward  a  given  issue, 
these  positions  ranging  from  intense  opposition  to  intense 
approval,  with  a  neutral  zone  in  the  middle.  A  quantitative 
"scale  value"  is  assigned  to  each  statement  and  the  student's 
score  is  expressed  as  the  median  of  the  scale  values  of  the 
statements  he  endorses.  Low  scores  indicate  opposition  and 
high  scores  indicate  approval.  Another  approach  is  used  by 
Neumann.16  He  attempts  to  combine  a  survey  of  various  in- 
ternational issues  with  a  measure  of  the  intensity  of  reac- 
tion toward  each  one.  He  accomplishes  this  by  including 
statements  on  a  series  of  issues  and  by  directing  students  to 
mark  each  statement  by  indicating  five  degrees  of  reaction 
ranging  from  strong  approval  to  strong  disapproval. 

Although  several  schools  in  the  Study  used  Thurstone's 
scale  for  measuring  Attitude  Toward  War,  and  tried  out 
experimentally  a  modified  form  of  Neumann's  Attitude  In- 
dicator, the  committee  decided  that  a  still  different  tech- 
nique would  be  more  useful  in  serving  the  purposes  of  these 

15  L.  L.  Thurstone  and  E.  J.  Chase,  The  Measurement  of  Attitude  (Chi- 
cago, University  of  Chicago  Press,  1929),  pp.  10-12. 

16  George  B.  Neumann,  A  Study  of  International  Attitudes  of  High  School 
Students  (New  York,  Teachers  College,  Bureau  of  Publications,  Columbia 
University,  1926). 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  215 

particular  schools.  It  was  believed  that  separate  scales,  each 
of  which  focuses  on  a  single  major  issue  (e.g.,  war  or  reli- 
gion), make  it  relatively  easy  for  a  student  to  decide  what 
is  likely  to  be  the  "acceptable"  position  and  to  respond  ac- 
cordingly, thus  raising  questions  as  to  the  validity  of  the  in- 
strument as  an  indicator  of  the  student's  "real"  attitude.  This 
aspect  of  validity  might  be  at  least  partially  protected  by 
mixing  statements  on  a  variety  of  issues  in  the  same  instru- 
ment and  avoiding  the  use  of  titles  which  would  reveal  the 
major  issues  included.  Moreover,  it  seemed  more  important 
to  the  schools  to  appraise  the  positions  on  a  range  of  sub- 
issues  under  each  major  area  of  issues  than  to  scale  in  detail 
the  intensity  of  each  position.  To  attempt  to  do  both  would 
probably  result  in  an  instrument  too  long  for  practical  use. 
All  of  these  considerations  influenced  the  technique  which 
was  eventually  chosen  and  which  will  be  described  in  the 
next  section. 

DESCRIPTION  OF  THE  TEST  ON  BELIEFS  ON  SOCIAL  ISSUES 
(FORM  4.21-4.31) 

After  the  above-mentioned  problems  had  been  considered, 
a  plan  emerged  for  a  new  instrument  to  measure  Beliefs  on 
Social  Issues.  In  the  present  form  it  consists  of  200  statements, 
classified  under  the  following  areas  of  issues:  democracy, 
economic  relations,  labor  and  unemployment,  race,  nation- 
alism, and  militarism.  Students  respond  to  each  statement 
by  indicating  agreement,  disagreement,  or  uncertainty.  The 
statements  are  arranged  in  random  order  and  are  presented 
to  the  students  in  two  sections  given  at  different  times.  For 
each  statement  in  the  first  section  there  is  a  statement  in  the 
second  section  representing  the  opposite  point  of  view. 

A  sample  of  the  statements  is  given  below.  The  items 
from  the  two  sections  of  the  test  are  shown  together.  The 
key  is  inserted  after  each  statement. 


216        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Democracy 

4.21    ?      1.  Complete  freedom  of  speech  should  be  given  to  all 
*  groups  and  all  individuals  regardless  of  how  radical 

their  political  views  are. 

(A,  Liberal;  D,  Conservative;  U,  Uncertain.) 
4.31    I  111.  Freedom  of  speech  should  be  denied  all  those  groups 

and  individuals  that  are  working  against  democratic 

forms  of  government. 

(D,  Liberal;  A,  Conservative;  U,  Uncertain.) 


4.21 


4.31 


4.21 


4.31 


4.21 


4.31 


Economic  Relations 

20.  Since  the  welfare  of  a  whole  nation  depends  on  its 
natural  resources,  their  use  should  be  subject  to  pub- 
lic control. 

(A,  Liberal;  D,  Conservative;  U,  Uncertain.) 
125.  Those  who  own  oil  wells,  coal  mines,  and  other  nat- 
ural resources  should  be  allowed  to  operate  them  as 
they  think  best. 
(D,  Liberal;  A,  Conservative;  U,  Uncertain.) 

Labor  and  Unemployment 

14.  Most  workers  who  are  unable  to  provide  for  them- 
selves during  a  period  of  unemployment  have  been 
too  shiftless  to  save. 

(D,  Liberal;  A,  Conservative;  U,  Uncertain.) 
104.  The  wages  of  most  workers  are  so  low  that  it  is  im- 
possible for  them  to  save  enough  money  to  support 
themselves  during  periods  of  unemployment. 
(A,  Liberal;  D,  Conservative;  U,  Uncertain.) 

Race 

97.  It  is  all  right  for  Negroes  to  be  paid  lower  wages 
than  whites  for  similar  kinds  of  work. 
(D,  Liberal;  A,  Conservative;  U,  Uncertain.) 
192.  The  same  wages  should  be  paid  to  Negroes  as  to 
whites  for  work  which  requires  the  same  ability  and 
training. 
(A,  Liberal;  D,  Conservative;  U,  Uncertain.) 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  2,17 

"Nationalism 

4.21        79.  Our  government  ought  to  protect  American  business 
interests  in  foreign  countries  even  if  it  involves  using 

our  army  and  navy. 

(D,  Liberal;  A,  Conservative;  U,  Uncertain.) 
4.31       189.  Our  government  should  not  risk  a  war  to  protect 
American  business  interests  in  foreign  countries. 
(A,  Liberal;  D,  Conservative;  U,  Uncertain.) 

Militarism 

4.21    I    35.  The  amount  of  profit  made  from  the  sale  of  war 
materials  should  be  strictly  limited. 
(A,  Liberal;  D?  Conservative;  U,  Uncertain.) 

4.31    |  132.  Men  should  be  allowed  to  make  profits  out  of  muni- 
tion making  just  as  they  are  allowed  to  make  profits 
from  other  business  enterprises. 
(D,  Liberal;  A,  Conservative;  U,  Uncertain.) 

Scoring  and  Summarizing  the  Results 

The  responses  to  the  whole  test  as  well  as  to  each  of  the 
areas  are  summarized  under  four  main  headings:  liberalism, 
conservatism,  uncertainty,  and  consistency.  No  attempt  was 
made  to  arrive  at  a  categorical  definition  of  the  terms  liberal 
or  conservative.  These  terms  were  adopted  for  convenience 
only  and  carry  a  somewhat  different  connotation  with  refer- 
ence to  each  area.  The  liberal  point  of  view  in  the  area  of 
democracy,  for  instance,  tends  to  endorse  freedom  of  speech; 
democratic  processes  in  government;  responsibility  of  the 
government  for  promoting  the  welfare  of  all  groups  in  soci- 
ety with  respect  to  health,  security  for  old  age?  and  the  pro- 
tection of  consumers;  and  reinterpretation  of  the  Constitu- 
tion and  other  basic  laws  in  keeping  with  present-day  social 
and  economic  demands.  The  conservative  position  tends  to 
approve  restrictions  on  freedom  of  speech,  to  limit  the  re- 
sponsibility of  the  government  for  social  welfare,  and  to 
favor  a  strict  interpretation  of  the  Constitution. 

In  the  area  of  economic  relations,  the  liberal  position  tends 


ai8        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

to  endorse  government  regulation  of  public  utilities,  natural 
resources,  wage  levels,  insurance,  and  to  approve  of  moving 
in  the  direction  of  production  for  use  rather  than  for  profit. 
The  conservative  position  represents  the  policy  of  economic 
individualism,  the  policy  of  laissez  faire,  and  the  preserva- 
tion of  the  profit  system  in  unrestricted  form. 

With  respect  to  labor  and  unemployment,  the  liberal  posi- 
tion tends  to  favor  collective  bargaining;  to  approve  of  social 
legislation  providing  for  minimum  wage  levels,  health  insur- 
ance, and  unemployment  relief;  and  to  maintain  that  unem- 
ployment is  caused  by  social  conditions  beyond  the  control  of 
individuals,  and  hence  that  its  consequences  should  be  borne 
by  society  rather  than  by  the  individuals  who  happen  to  be 
affected  by  it.  The  conservative  position  tends  to  oppose  the 
organization  of  labor  for  collective  bargaining;  to  oppose 
labor  legislation  or  expenditure  of  government  funds  for  re- 
lief of  unemployment;  and  to  maintain  that  unemployment 
is  caused  by  some  deficiency  of  the  individuals,  and  hence 
that  the  consequences  should  be  borne  by  those  who  happen 
to  be  unemployed. 

In  the  area  of  race,  the  liberal  position  tends  to  endorse 
the  equality  of  all  races  as  far  as  social,  economic,  and  educa- 
tional opportunities  are  concerned,  and  to  deny  that  racial 
inequality  is  inherent  or  inborn.  The  social,  economic,  and 
educational  status  of  Negroes  as  a  group  is  Attributed  to  en- 
vironmental conditions  rather  than  to  hereditary  causes.  The 
conservative  position  accepts  the  inherent  supremacy  of  the 
white  race  and  indorses  racial  discrimination  of  all  sorts. 

A  pacifistic  viewpoint  represents  liberalism  in  the  area 
of  Militarism:  that  is,  the  tendency  to  favor  arms  limitation, 
arbitration,  and  condemnation  of  war  as  a  way  of  settling  in- 
ternational troubles.  Belief  in  the  inevitability  of  war,  in 
armed  preparedness,  in  the  use  of  armed  force,  and  in  the 
benefits  of  military  training  for  character  development  illus- 
trates the  conservative  position. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  219 

In  the  area  of  nationalism,  a  liberal  viewpoint  is  ascribed 
to  those  who  are  internationally-minded,  who  recognize  the 
worth  and  the  contributions  of  other  nations,  and  who  deny 
that  there  is  need  for  protecting  a  nation's  imperialistic  eco- 
nomic enterprises  abroad  with  armed  forces.  A  conservative 
viewpoint  is  associated  with  emphasis  on  national  glory  and 
honor,  and  the  belief  that  American  ways  would  be  best  for 
other  peoples;  it  tends  to  defend  the  notion  of  the  supremacy 
of  America  and  of  things  American  in  all  respects  and  to 
insist  on  the  use  of  American  standards  in  judging  the  con- 
tributions of  other  nations. 

In  all  areas  the  uncertain  response  is  taken  to  mean  either 
that  the  student  does  not  understand  the  statement  or  that 
he  is  unable  to  take  a  position  regarding  the  issue  because 
of  conflicting  ideas  about  it.  It  was  also  anticipated  that  a 
relatively  high  degree  of  uncertainty  might  characterize  the 
position  of  the  more  thoughtful  students.  Consistency  indi- 
cates the  extent  to  which  students  take  a  similar  position 
twice  on  the  same  issue;  i.e.,  do  not  agree  with  both  of  two 
contradictory  statements.  The  tendency  to  take  a  similar  posi- 
tion on  a  range  of  issues  in  one  area  or  in  different  areas  is 
indicated  by  the  percentage  of  liberal  and  conservative  re- 
sponses in  each  area. 

As  can  be  seen  from  the  data  sheet,  these  four  headings 
(liberalism,  conservatism,  uncertainty,  and  consistency)  are 
used  to  summarize  both  the  total  scores  and  the  subscores 
for  each  of  the  six  areas.  No  such  headings  are  used  in  the 
instrument  itself,  and  the  student  is  not  aware  that  his  re- 
sponses are  to  be  classified  in  this  way.  Moreover,  it  cannot 
be  emphasized  too  strongly  that,  as  far  as  the  instrument  is 
concerned,  there  is  no  implication  that  either  the  liberal  or 
the  conservative  position  is  to  be  preferred.  The  instrument 
is  designed  to  measure  the  status  or  change  of  beliefs;  the 
problem  of  determining  the  desirability  of  the  direction  that 


220        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


beliefs  of  students  should  take  is  a  responsibility  of  the 
schools. 

Explanation  of  the  Data  Sheet 

The  scores  on  this  scale  can  be  interpreted  in  terms  of 
three  questions  centering  about  the  direction,  uncertainty, 
and  consistency  of  the  viewpoints  shown.  The  first  question 
is:  What  is  the  direction  of  the  pattern  of  beliefs  and  how 
is  it  distributed? 

The  scores  on  liberalism  (columns  25  and  1-6)  indicate 
the  per  cent  of  the  statements  to  which  the  student  re- 
sponded in  the  liberal  direction.17  The  scores  on  conserva- 
tism (columns  26  and  7-12)  give  the  per  cent  of  responses 
made  in  the  direction  described  as  conservative.  High  scores 
in  either  direction,  uniformly  distributed,  would  mean  a 
fairly  well-thought-out  position.  Student  A,  for  example,  has 
responded  in  the  liberal  direction  to  90  per  cent  of  all  items 
in  the  test  (column  25).  His  scores,  furthermore,  are  distrib- 
uted evenly  in  all  of  the  six  areas  (columns  1-6).  Student  D 
achieves  a  similarly  high  and  fairly  even  distribution  of 
scores  in  the  conservative  direction  (columns  7-12  ).18  Stu- 
dent R  is  near  the  median  of  the  class  as  far  as  his  total  score 
on  liberalism  is  concerned,  but  there  is  a  good  deal  of  fluc- 
tuation of  his  liberal  responses  from  area  to  area.  He  is,  for 
instance,  inclined  to  an  international  viewpoint  (N,  80)  and 
pacifism  (M,  78),  but  is  at  the  same  time  inclined  to  reject 
collective  bargaining  and  social  measures  to  combat  unem- 
ployment (LU,  liberalism,  44,  conservatism,  50).  The  same 
type  of  fluctuation  can  be  observed  in  the  scores  on  liberal- 
ism of  Student  G.  In  the  area  of  economic  relations  (ER,  7) 

17  The  terms  "liberal"  and  "conservative"  are  used  throughout  this  sec- 
tion in  place  of  making  the  more  lengthy  references  to  their  meaning  in 
each  of  the  areas. 

18  The  usual  ratio  of  liberal  to  conservative  scores  in  the  schools  of  the 
Eight-  Year  Study  was  about  2:1.  Hence  scores  on  conservatism  which  are 
as  large  as,  or  larger  than,  scores  on  liberalism  were  interpreted  as  a  high 
degree  of  conservative  beliefs. 


*-» 

"^ 

a 

=0 

CM 

^«^0t^ 

oc  no  to  o 

CO 

O 

0s 

5 

^T 

^ 

£ 

£ 

^0-^ 

O 

CO 

CM 
CM 

^ 

1 

0 

CJ 

C^ 

\O  OC  -O  O 

CM  —  »  r»- 

CM 

OO 
CO 

0 

H 

Ss 

o 

H-5 

UO 

CM 

§S3^ 

% 

o 

3 

<2 

'o 

PH 

i 

S 

CM 

^o  oS 

10 
CM 

OO 

§ 

53 

& 

^ 

?3 

00^0 
t^   O   "*  to 

rM 

-t» 
OO 

CM 

a 

"^ 

a 

c 

^ 

CM 

0  0  10  0 

co  oc  *--  -t* 

8 

£ 

03 

- 

O 

p 

CM 

rH  O  O  CM 

5 

CM 

O\ 

vO 

^ 

w 

0 
CM 

vo  -tn  <-M  0 
ON   ^  co  OC 

CM 

o 
o 

CO 

CO 

fl 

CN 

T*H     0     TfH     0 

00   0  O  JO 

CM 

co 

% 

0 

CO 

s 

oo 

003.0 

O 

s 

o 

CM 

>, 

Jz; 

^ 

CM   O   '-H  o-« 

»•* 

IT) 

CM 

H     ^ 

"3 

p< 

S 

0  0  O  O 
CM           "~* 

o 

o 

to 

OO 

S   S 

s  s 

1 

ID 

P 

to 

<*O   O   vO  O 

o 

10 

CM 
CM 

<  ^ 
5    " 

S 

3 

O  O  O  0 
"-«  vo 

o 

O 

vo 

O 
CM 

ft      rt 

M           0 

Q 

2 

^HVOC^ 

o 

SO 

vO 

^     1 

S 

2 

OO    CM   CM   O 
CM            CO 

o 

O 
OO 

CM 

<    2 

W            4-» 

S 

!zi 

£ 

i-*            CO 

CO 

OO 

0 

1 

& 

o 

CO   vO    CM  OO 

o 

OO 

OO 

S 

o 

S 

c\ 

ro  O  O  "-• 
to   CO  VO 

0 

VO 

3 

fe 

O 

w 

00 

co  co  i"~- 

o 

s 

JC-- 

Q 

*- 

10Q   «a«   -*OH   IO 
^    CM   ^  VO 

0 

to 

2 

S 

VO 

OO  *>•  vO  o—  t 

S 

£ 

CO 
ON 

vO 

X 

to 

C*   O  co  vo 

CO    OO    10    o-l 

I 

5 

g 

CO 
«o 

~£ 

X 

* 

CM   OO    OO   CM 
CA   10    OO   CO 

1 

CM 

8 

00 
CO 

•S 

& 

g 

CO 

ON    Tf    <—i    CO 

.S 

S! 

f- 

c\ 

VO 

1 

T-- 

& 

S 

CM 

Ov  10           CM 

o 

a 

*- 

o 
o 

CO 

10 

t3 

O 

- 

CO  r—  xr<  co 

§ 

CO 

vO 
ON 

O 

'o 

1 

03 

0 
T3 

O 

th 

J| 

"S 

GO 

Maximum 
Possible 

o  8 

h-3  CO 

11 

1 

SJ 

222        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

and  labor  and  unemployment  (LU,  14)  lie  makes  few  liberal 
responses,  while  his  score  on  toleration  of  racial  equality 
(R,  88)  is  very  high.  In  his  case,  however,  the  absence  of 
liberal  responses  cannot  be  interpreted  as  a  rejection  of  this 
position.  His  scores  on  uncertainty  in  these  two  areas  (un- 
certainty: ER,  60,  LU,  58)  indicate  that  in  these  areas  he 
has  difficulty  in  taking  a  position.  In  the  few  instances  he 
does  so,  the  responses  in  the  conservative  direction  prevail 
(ER:  liberal,  7,  conservative,  33;  LU:  liberalism,  14,  con- 
servatism, 30). 

The  second  question  is:  To  what  extent  are  the  students 
willing  (or  able)  to  take  definite  positions  on  these  social 
issues? 

The  uncertainty  (columns  27  and  13-18)  scores  give  the 
per  cent  of  responses  in  which  a  student  neither  agrees  nor 
disagrees  with  the  statements.  High  uncertainty  might  mean 
desirable  caution,  inability  to  understand  the  statements, 
lack  of  information,  or  lack  of  conviction.  In  most  cases  this 
response  seems  to  mean  "I  don't  know  or  I  can't  decide/7 
for  socially  conscious  and  active  students  usually  have  low 
"uncertain"  scores.  Thus,  Student  C  is  very  uncertain  of  his 
position  on  all  of  the  issues  with  the  exception  of  those  per- 
taining to  race.  He  scores  far  above  the  median  for  the  class 
on  total  uncertainty  (column  27),  and  in  five  of  the  areas. 
Students  A  and  D  indicate  little  uncertainty  as  to  their  posi- 
tions. Neither  extreme  certainty  nor  extreme  uncertainty  in 
themselves  are  desirable.  Whether  or  not  either  can  be  con- 
sidered desirable  depends  on  the  total  pattern  of  scores. 
Thus,  certainty  combined  with  high  consistency  is  more  ac- 
ceptable than  high  certainty  combined  with  low  consistency 
because  flexibility  is  important  as  long  as  there  is  confusion. 
Experience  with  test  data  has  shown  also  that  certainty  com- 
bined with  high  conservatism  is  not  as  desirable  from  the 
standpoint  of  growth  as  is  high  certainty  combined  with 
high  liberalism.  This  conclusion  was  drawn  because  it  was 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  223 

found  that  conservative  beliefs  were  more  frequently  bor- 
rowed beliefs,  while  liberal  beliefs  were  more  often  arrived 
at  through  personal  thought  and  consideration.  In  interpret- 
ing the  meaning  of  high  or  low  uncertainty,  however,  the 
developmental  trend  of  the  student  needs  to  be  considered. 
Thus  one  would  expect  an  increase  in  uncertainty  whenever 
an  individual  is  in  a  state  of  transition  from  one  type  of 
social  viewpoint  to  another. 

The  third  question  is:  To  what  extent  are  the  students 
consistent  in  the  positions  they  take? 

The  consistency  (columns  28  and  19-24)  scores  give  the 
per  cent  of  consistent  responses  on  the  total  test  and  in  the 
areas  listed  above.  High  scores  in  these  columns  indicate 
clarity  of  outlook,  whether  it  is  liberal  or  conservative  in  its 
direction.  Low  consistency  may  occur  for  at  least  two  rea- 
sons. Students  may  be  inconsistent  because  of  inability  to 
think  through  their  beliefs  or  because  they  are  actually 
embracing  conflicting  positions.  In  the  first  case,  there  is 
likely  to  be  an  even  distribution  of  inconsistency  scores  in 
all  areas.  In  the  other  case  there  is  more  likely  to  be  high 
consistency  in  some  areas  and  low  consistency  in  other  areas. 
While  high  consistency  can  be  generally  regarded  as  a  de- 
sirable characteristic,  one  must  be  aware  that  often  incon- 
sistency is  a  by-product  of  transition  from  one  pattern  of 
beliefs  to  another.  In  the  latter  case,  low  consistency  may 
be  an  index  of  change  and  may  be  temporary.  Whether  this 
is  true  or  not  can  be  determined  if  the  test  is  readministered 
after  an  appropriate  interval  of  time  and  a  description  of  the 
kinds  of  changes  taking  place  in  students  is  secured. 

Student  A  is  the  most  consistent  of  the  four  students  whose 
scores  are  given  on  the  data  sheet.  Student  B  shows  a  vari- 
able pattern  of  consistency.  On  racial  issues  he  is  rather  con- 
sistent (consistency:  R,  80),  but  on  issues  of  labor  and  unem- 
ployment he  is  the  least  consistent  student  in  his  entire  group 
(consistency:  U,  40).  Similar  fluctuation  in  consistency  from 


224        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

area  to  area  is  shown  by  Students  C  and  D.  Student  D  is 
rather  consistent  on  issues  in  economic  relations  (consist- 
ency: ER,  80)  and  relatively  inconsistent  on  racial  issues 
(consistency:  R,  40). 

The  scores  on  liberalism,  conservatism,  and  uncertainty 
are  interdependent  and  must  be  viewed  in  relation  to  each 
other.  This  can  be  illustrated  by  comparing  the  scores  on 
economic  relations  for  Students  C  and  D.  Both  of  these  stu- 
dents have  low  scores  on  liberalism  in  this  area,  but  while 
Student  C  is  rather  highly  uncertain,  Student  D  is  highly 
conservative.  Thus  scores  on  liberalism  alone  tell  only  part 
of  the  story.  One  can  infer  that  the  low  score  on  liberalism 
in  the  case  of  Student  C  results  from  the  fact  that  he  has  not 
made  up  his  mind  on  many  of  the  issues.  Student  D,  how- 
ever, seems  to  have  definite  convictions  about  economic 
relationships.  For  this  reason  the  interpreter  must,  in  addition 
to  studying  each  score  independently,  consider  the  whole 
pattern  of  scores  before  arriving  at  a  final  judgment  about 
a  student  or  groups  of  individuals. 

Several  other  general  considerations  apply  in  interpreting 
different  combinations  of  score  patterns.  Thus,  when  the 
score  on  uncertainty  is  unusually  high,  the  scores  on  both 
liberalism  and  conservatism  are  of  necessity  low.  In  such 
cases  one  can  interpret  these  scores  better  by  comparing 
them  with  each  other  than  by  comparing  each  with  the 
median.  Thus,  in  the  case  of  Student  C  one  might  say  that 
whenever  he  makes  up  his  mind  on  economic  relations  his 
position  will  be  predominantly  in  the  conservative  direction, 
because  33  per  cent  of  the  items  are  marked  in  the  conserva- 
tive direction  while  only  7  per  cent  of  the  items  are  marked 
in  the  liberal  direction.  High  scores  on  uncertainty,  coupled 
with  high  scores  on  consistency,  are  more  likely  to  be  an 
indication  of  intelligent  doubt  than  of  mere  confusion  and 
inability  to  see  the  issues  clearly.  Conversely,  lack  of  un- 
certainty where  inconsistency  is  high  would  indicate  a  pre- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  225 

mature  feeling  of  security  about  beliefs  which  in  reality  are 
confused.  Decisions  such  as  these  concerning  the  relative  de- 
sirability of  high  or  low  scores  on  liberalism  are  left  for  the 
teacher  to  make. 

Although  in  the  course  of  the  above  discussion  comments 
have  been  made  concerning  the  scores  of  four  students,  no 
attempt  has  here  been  made  to  present  a  complete  and  co- 
herent account  of  the  beliefs  of  these  students.  The  data  on 
each  student  and  each  group  of  students  made  available  by 
this  instrument  are  too  extensive  to  permit  the  presentation 
within  the  limits  of  this  chapter  of  a  complete  treatment  of 
the  possibilities  of  interpretation. 

Validity  and  Reliability 

Several  factors  influence  the  validity  of  this  instrument.  In 
the  first  place,  there  is  the  problem  of  the  role  of  language 
in  expressing  feelings  and  viewpoints.  In  statements  of  is- 
sues terms  which  have  different  meanings  for  different  indi- 
viduals are  apt  to  be  used.  The  expressions  of  attitudinal 
positions  also  require  the  use  of  some  words  or  ideas  to 
which  strong  emotional  reactions  are  attached  and  these  re- 
actions usually  are  not  the  same  from  individual  to  individ- 
ual. Certain  words  may  evoke  responses  somewhat  inde- 
pendent of,  or  irrelevant  to,  the  meaning  and  intent  of  the 
whole  statement.  Also  involved  is  the  fact  that  many  indi- 
viduals are  not  clear  about  their  own  beliefs.  Those  who  tend 
to  be  confused  or  uncertain  about  their  own  positions  are 
apt  to  respond  more  or  less  automatically  to  familiar  ter- 
minology in  place  of  attempting  to  decide  what  their  own 
beliefs  are.  Moreover,  it  is  likely  that  individuals  with  no 
definite  beliefs  on  a  given  issue  may  be  induced  to  give 
definite  responses  merely  because  familiar  verbal  stereo- 
types are  presented  to  them. 

Secondly,  there  is  the  problem  of  securing  honesty  of  re- 
sponse. Social  beliefs  are  somewhat  in  the  realm  of  the  pri- 


226        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

vate  life  of  an  individual  and  he  is  not  always  willing  to 
reveal  them.  There  are  either  general  social  pressures  or  pres- 
sures in  a  given  group  toward  the  "right  way  of  believing/' 
and  individuals  whose  personal  beliefs  differ  from  the  pre- 
dominant ones  may  feel  threatened  in  disclosing  them.  Thus, 
often  in  a  school  where  the  majority  of  students  are  liberal 
in  a  certain  respect,  those  who  do  not  share  the  liberal  view- 
point are  put  on  the  defensive.  This  applies  also  to  teacher- 
pupil  relations.  Even  in  responding  to  an  instrument  of  this 
sort  which  is  not,  properly  speaking,  a  "test,"  students  are 
apt  to  try  to  live  up  to  the  expectations  of  teachers  who  are 
known  to  favor  certain  viewpoints  rather  than  to  express 
their  own  viewpoints.  It  is  for  reasons  like  these  that  the 
question  of  validity  is  peculiarly  complex  in  the  measure- 
ment of  social  beliefs. 

An  additional  difficulty  lies  in  the  fact  that  the  social  be- 
liefs of  individuals  are  rarely  so  generalized  that  the  subjects 
mentioned  in  the  statements  do  not  affect  the  response. 
Thus,  in  securing  opinions  upon  the  issue  of  government 
control  vs.  economic  individualism,  it  may  make  a  consider- 
able difference  whether  the  issue  is  stated  with  reference  to 
public  utilities  or  to  railroads,  whether  the  object  of  con- 
trol is  profits  or  wages,  and  so  on.  Ideally,  the  specific  issues 
used  in  the  test  should  include  all  of  these  variations.  Since 
this  ideal  cannot  be  achieved  in  a  test  of  this  sort,  one  is 
faced  with  the  problem  of  sampling  and  of  the  reliability  of 
the  sample. 

The  efforts  made  in  the  process  of  construction  to  assure 
high  validity  for  the  test  were  described  above.  Summarized 
briefly,  these  consisted  of  securing  a  clear  delimitation  by 
the  committee  of  the  behavior  to  be  measured,  and  of  utiliz- 
ing statements  from  students  in  deciding  which  specific  is- 
sues to  include,  in  determining  the  level  of  intensity  at 
which  statements  should  be  formulated,  and  in  phrasing  the 
statements.  Finally,  the  instrument  was  revised  several  times 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  22.7 

on  the  basis  of  analyses  of  student  responses  to  tentative 
forms. 

In  addition  to  the  above  precautions,  several  studies  of 
the  validity  were  conducted.19  In  the  first  study  the  instru- 
ment was  given  to  65  junior  and  senior  classes  studying 
American  history  and  sociology  in  a  large  public  high  school 
Verbal  descriptions  of  the  beliefs  of  these  students  based  on 
their  numerical  scores  were  made  and  these  were  discussed 
with  the  cooperating  teacher.  The  validity  of  the  scores  in 
each  area  in  the  scale  was  considered  separately.  The  teach- 
er's judgments  of  the  social  attitudes  of  the  students  as  re- 
vealed by  his  observations  in  the  classroom  coincided  with 
the  interpretations  of  the  scores  from  the  test  in  90  per  cent 
of  the  cases. 

Thirty  of  these  students  were  interviewed.  They  were 
chosen  on  the  basis  of  the  test  scores  so  that  they  repre- 
sented the  ten  most  conservative,  the  ten  most  liberal,  and 
the  ten  most  inconsistent  and  uncertain  students  in  the  entire 
group.  The  questions  asked  in  these  interviews  paralleled 
die  statements  of  the  test.  Some  of  the  students  were  ques- 
tioned regarding  their  points  of  view  within  a  single  area; 
others  were  interrogated  with  respect  to  two,  three,  or  even 
all  six  areas.  When  the  information  obtained  in  this  way  was 
compared  with  the  test  results,  the  two  sets  of  data  were 
found  to  be  fairly  consistent;  that  is,  the  direction  of  points 
of  view,  the  certainty,  and  the  consistency  of  the  students 
as  revealed  by  the  test  were  very  closely  related  to  those 
indicated  by  their  verbally  expressed  opinions. 

A  second  study  of  the  validity  of  the  instrument  was  car- 
ried out  in  a  ninth  grade  social  science  class  composed  of  18 

19  These  validity  studies  were  conducted  by  Paul  R.  Grim  and  the  dis- 
cussion here  summarizes  his  findings  described  in  more  detail  in  "A  Tech- 
nique for  the  Evaluation  of  Attitudes  in  the  Social  Studies,"  a  dissertation 
submitted  to  the  Ohio  State  University  in  1939.  Dr.  Grim's  study  was  made 
in  connection  with  the  Form  4.2-4.3.  Only  slight  revisions  were  made  in 
the  Form  4.21-4.31. 


228        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

students.  Written  descriptions  of  their  social  beliefs  as  re- 
vealed by  test  scores  were  made.  Apprentice  teachers  col- 
lected hundreds  of  anecdotes  pertaining  to  expressions  of, 
and  behavior  relative  to,  the  social  viewpoints  of  the  stu- 
dents, and  also  examined  these  students'  written  work.  They 
then  summarized  their  findings  by  rating  these  students  on 
a  five-point  scale  for  liberalism  and  for  consistency  in  each 
of  the  six  areas.  It  was  found  that  over  90  per  cent  of  the 
judgments  of  the  teachers  coincided  with  the  test  ratings. 
The  students  in  this  group  were  also  interviewed.  In  17  out 
of  the  18  cases,  the  opinions  expressed  in  the  conferences 
conformed  closely  with  the  responses  to  the  test. 

In  one  study  of  reliability,  coefficients20  for  this  test  based 
on  a  total  population  of  600  students  selected  from  14  schools 
and  representing  grades  nine  through  twelve  were  com- 
puted. The  results  were  as  follows:  On  liberalism  they 
ranged  from  .79  to  .86  for  the  different  areas;  for  the  total 
score  on  liberalism  the  coefficient  was  .95.  On  conservatism 
they  ranged  from  .72  to  .81  in  different  areas;  the  reliability 
coefficient  for  the  total  score  on  conservatism  was  .93.  On 
uncertainty  the  range  of  reliability  coefficients  was  from  .79 
to  .85,  and  a  coefficient  of  .96  was  obtained  for  the  total 
score.  On  consistency  the  reliability  coefficients  ranged  from 
.45  to  .61,  with  a  coefficient  of  .85  for  the  total  score.21  These 
data  check  rather  closely  with  those  obtained  in  other  studies 
from  other  populations  and  by  other  methods.  The  scores  in 
the  test  are  stable  enough  so  that,  within  appropriate  statis- 
tical limits,  they  may  be  used  for  diagnosis  of  individual  as 
well  as  group  differences. 

As  can  be  seen  from  these  data,  the  stability  of  the  scores 
by  areas  is  a  good  deal  lower  than  the  stability  of  the  total 
scores.  The  scores  on  consistency  by  areas  have  particularly 

20  Estimated  by  the  Kuder-Richardson  formula.  More  complete  data  on 
reliability  and  other  statistics  are  given  in  the  Appendix. 

21  Since  pairs  of  items  are  scored  to  determine  consistency,  the  test  is 
in  effect  only  half  as  long  for  this  purpose. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  229 

low  stability  and  can  be  used  only  to  designate  the  extremes. 
All  other  scores  used  within  the  content  of  the  whole  pat- 
tern of  scores  and  within  appropriate  statistical  limits,  can 
be  used  for  helpful  diagnostic  judgments  regarding  the 
nature  of  social  beliefs. 

BELIEFS  ABOUT  SCHOOL  LIFE 

Another  scale  of  social  beliefs  (Beliefs  about  School  Life, 
Form  4.6),  was  devoted  to  the  area  of  school  life. 

Appraisal  of  the  beliefs  regarding  various  aspects  of  school 
life  was  considered  important  for  several  reasons.  In  the 
first  place,  students7  points  of  view  on  such  matters  as  grades 
and  awards,  methods  of  teaching,  and  ways  of  conducting 
the  school  government,  determine  to  a  considerable  extent 
the  type  and  the  effectiveness  of  their  adjustment  to  school. 
The  beliefs  prevailing  among  students  on  these  matters  also 
influence  the  organization  and  functioning  of  the  school  since 
students'  beliefs  play  an  important  part  in  motivating  their 
behavior  in  specific  situations.  Finally,  certain  of  these  be- 
liefs represent  aspects  of  "democracy  in  school"  and  as  such 
are  considered  in  many  schools  as  desirable  ends  in  them- 
selves. Awareness  of  the  nature  of  these  beliefs  on  the  part 
of  both  students  and  teachers  is  helpful  in  accomplishing 
desirable  changes  in  the  school  environment  or  in  an  indi- 
vidual student's  reactions  to  that  environment.  For  these 
reasons  a  means  of  obtaining  systematic  evidence  on  beliefs 
toward  a  range  of  issues  about  school  life  was  thought  to  be 
a  desirable  addition  to  observations  of  overt  behavior. 

Analysis  of  the  Objective 

In  order  to  be  sure  that  the  test  sampled  opinions  on  issues 
of  concern  relative  to  school  life,  two  investigations  were 
conducted.  First,  some  students  were  asked  to  write  brief 
essays  on  "Democracy  in  My  School."  Their  essays  discussed 
many  kinds  of  problems,  from  rules  regarding  the  use  of  lip- 


230        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

stick  to  criticism  of  the  course  of  study  which  they  were 
following.  Secondly,  a  list  of  the  major  areas  of  school  life 
and  illustrative  statements  of  issues  in  each  area  was  sent  to 
teachers  in  several  schools.  They  were  asked  to  criticize  the 
choice  of  issues  and  the  tentative  list  of  specific  statements, 
and  to  make  additions  to  either  if  they  thought  there  were 
important  omissions.  In  analyzing  the  material  obtained 
from  teachers  and  students,  it  was  found  that  the  most  fre- 
quently mentioned  issues  could  be  classified  in  six  major 
areas:  school  government,  curriculum,  grades  and  awards, 
school  spirit,  pupil-teacher  relations,  and  group  life.  These 
became  categories  of  summary  for  the  instrument  which  was 
developed.  This  instrument  is  similar  in  form  to  the  one  de- 
scribed in  the  preceding  section  except  for  the  difference  in 
content  and  the  fact  that  no  attempts  were  made  to  meas- 
ure consistency.  It  consists  of  a  series  of  118  statements  of 
opinion,  and  students  respond  by  indicating  either  agree- 
ment or  disagreement  with  them,  or  uncertainty  about  them. 
In  the  following  paragraphs  a  brief  description  of  the  cate- 
gories and  some  illustrative  statements  from  the  instrument 
are  given. 

Description  of  the  Test 

The  area  of  school  government  samples  such  issues  as 
appropriate  bases  for  electing  students  to  school  offices,  treat- 
ment of  minority  groups,  appropriate  degree  of  student 
responsibility  for  the  conduct  of  school  affairs.  Student  re- 
sponses to  these  items  are  classified  as  democratic  and  un- 
democratic. For  example,  agreement  with  each  of  the  fol- 
lowing statements  is  scored  as  a  "democratic"  response,  and 
disagreement  with  these  statements  is  scored  as  an  "undemo- 
cratic" response: 

19.  Criticisms  of  the  school  government  made  by  first  year 
pupils  should  be  considered  just  as  carefully  as  criticisms 
which  juniors  and  seniors  make. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  231 

20.  The  teachers  and  principal  should  have  pupils  help  in 
deciding  what  books  to  buy  for  the  school  library. 

The  area  of  group  life  involves  issues  of  the  status  of  vari- 
ous school  groups  and  their  relations  to  each  other  and  to 
school.  The  following  problems  are  included:  the  extension 
of  special  privileges  of  various  sorts  only  to  members  of  cer- 
tain groups.,  the  maintenance  of  class  distinctions  in  terms  of 
these  groups,  and  the  desirability  of  characterizing  students 
as  members  of  certain  groups  or  cliques  rather  fhan  as  indi- 
viduals. Responses  to  these  items  are  summarized  in  terms 
of  the  number  of  responses  indicating  a  "social  attitude," 
meaning  approval  of  equal  treatment  of  all  groups,  and  a 
"class"  attitude,  indicating  a  disposition  to  approve  all  kinds 
of  distinctions  and  cliques.  For  example,  agreement  with  the 
following  statements  indicates  a  "class"  attitude,  whereas 
disagreement  indicates  a  "social"  attitude: 

6.  Pupils  from  the  wealthier  families  in  a  community  and 
pupils  from  the  poorer  families  should  not  be  put  in  the 
same  homeroom  together, 

99.  In  most  cases,  it  is  undesirable  to  have  slow  and  bright 
pupils  working  together  in  the  same  class. 

The  area  of  pupil-teacher  relations  involves  problems  of 
sharing  responsibility  between  teachers  and  pupils,  and  of 
the  methods  by  which  the  allocation  of  responsibility  should 
be  made.  The  following  issues  are  sampled:  the  appropriate 
degree  of  pupil-planning  of  various  school  activities,  methods 
of  making  decisions,  types  of  problems  which  teachers  alone 
should  solve.  Reactions  to  this  group  of  items  are  summa- 
rized in  terms  of  the  number  of  responses  indicating  approval 
of  cooperative  relations,  and  the  number  indicating  approval 
of  authoritarian  relations.  Following  are  two  illustrations  of 
items  in  this  area  in  which  disagreement  with  the  item  in- 
dicates approval  of  cooperative  methods  and  agreement  in- 
dicates approval  of  authoritarian  methods: 


2,32        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

2.  It  is  better  for  a  teacher  to  decide  what  the  pupils  are  to 
study  in  a  class  than  to  let  the  pupils  plan  their  work  by 
themselves. 

17.  Too  much  time  is  wasted  when  pupils  take  part  in  the 
discussion  of  plans  for  a  unit  of  study. 

The  area  of  curriculum  involves  issues  of  educational  phi- 
losophy and  practice.  Responses  to  these  issues  are  summa- 
rized in  terms  of  liberal  and  conventional  attitudes.  A  "lib- 
eral" attitude  is  indicated  by  an  experimental  point  of  view: 
that  is,  a  belief  in  the  integration  of  school  subjects,  pupil- 
teacher  planning,  flexibility  in  planning  units  of  study,  and 
in  utilizing  community  resources.  A  "conventional"  attitude 
is  indicated  by  a  disposition  to  maintain  rigid  subject  mat- 
ter divisions,  to  prefer  teacher-planned  courses  of  study,  and 
to  emphasize  the  acquisition  of  facts  and  information.  The 
following  statements  are  taken  from  this  area: 

11.  It  would  be  a  good  idea  for  several  teachers  of  different 

school  subjects  to  take  part  in  a  class  discussion  with  a 

group  of  pupils. 
56.  Trips  outside  of  the  school  building  should  not  be  taken 

at  a  time  when  they  interfere  with  the  regular  class 

schedule. 

In  the  above  illustration,  agreement  with  the  first  statement 
indicates  a  "liberal"  attitude,  whereas  agreement  with  the 
second  indicates  a  "conventional"  attitude  toward  school 
problems. 

The  area  of  grades  and  awards  samples  issues  concerning 
the  appropriate  use  of  grades  and  awards,  and  the  types  of 
grades  and  awards  which  are  desirable.  For  example,  such 
statements  as  the  following  are  made: 

18.  If  a  pupil  receives  failing  grades  most  of  the  time,  it  shows 
that  he  is  not  learning  anything  in  school. 

50.  If  grades  were  done  away  with,  pupils  would  have  no 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  233 

way  of  knowing  whether  they  were  making  progress  in 
their  studies. 

Responses  to  such  issues  are  summarized  as  non-traditional 
or  traditional.  "Non-traditional"  attitudes  are  indicated  by 
questioning  the  desirability  of  using  grades  and  awards  as 
incentives,  as  means  of  determining  participation  in  school 
activities,  and  as  providing  the  exclusive  measure  of  the 
value  derived  from  school  life.  The  "traditional"  point  of 
view  is  indicated  by  an  acceptance  of  grades  and  awards  for 
such  purposes. 

The  area  of  school  spirit  is  sampled  by  issues  concerning 
the  extent  of  school  loyalty  which  is  desirable,  and  the  types 
of  expressions  of  school  loyalty  which  are  appropriate.  For 
example,  the  following  statements  are  offered  for  considera- 
tion: 

40.  We  would  get  some  helpful  ideas  for  improving  our  school 

by  visiting  other  schools  to  see  how  they  do  things. 
102.  One  of  the  best  ways  for  a  pupil  to  show  that  he  is  a  good 
school  citizen  is  always  to  defend  his  school  when  others 
criticize  it. 

Agreement  with  the  first  statement  is  classified  as  a  "cos- 
mopolitan" point  of  view,  agreement  with  the  second  as 
a  "provincial"  attitude.  A  "cosmopolitan"  viewpoint  is  indi- 
cated by  a  disposition  to  recognize  certain  weaknesses  in 
one's  own  school,  a  disposition  to  view  the  school  as  a  chang- 
ing rather  than  as  an  inflexible  institution,  and  a  tendency 
toward  "worldliness"  in  one's  relations  with  students  from 
other  schools.  A  "provincial"  viewpoint  is  indicated  by  ex- 
pressing intense  loyalty  to  one's  immediate  group  to  the  ex- 
tent of  excluding  cooperative  relations  with  other  groups. 
In  addition  to  the  descriptive  categories,  the  number  of  un- 
certain responses  in  each  area  is  given. 

As  is  indicated  by  the  method  of  summarizing  student  re- 
sponses, the  test  may  be  useful  in  identifying  points  of  view 


234        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

on  the  part  of  an  individual  student  which  are  likely  to  be 
hampering  his  adjustment  to,  and  active  participation  in, 
school  life.  It  must  be  noted,  however,  that  the  test  has  not 
been  studied  sufficiently  to  warrant  a  recommendation  that 
it  be  used  for  precise  individual  diagnosis.  Its  primary  use- 
fulness is  for  studying  groups.  Only  students  who  deviate 
markedly  from  the  group  pattern  can  be  identified  with  as- 
surance as  being  significantly  different  from  others  in  the 
group. 

A  teacher  who  wishes  to  use  the  test  should  examine  it 
with  respect  to  her  own  school  situation  in  terms  of  the  fol- 
lowing criteria:  (1)  Does  it  sample  problems  and  conflicts 
which  pupils  in  this  school  must  deal  with  in  order  to  make 
a  better  adjustment  to  school  life?  (2)  Are  the  beliefs  to- 
ward school  life  which  are  sampled  likely  to  affect  participa- 
tion in"  social  movements  and  processes  outside  school?  ( 3 ) 
Does  it  involve  issues  regarding  educational  philosophy 
which  are  really  controversial  issues  within  this  school? 
(4)  Does  it  sample  beliefs  which  may  provide  clues  con- 
cerning the  behavior  of  individual  pupils  in  a  variety  of 
situations  in  this  school? 

BELIEFS  ON  ECONOMIC  ISSUES 

Frequently  the  Evaluation  Staff  received  requests  for  spe- 
cialized instruments  to  evaluate  certain  unique  features  of  a 
particular  school  program.  One  such  request  was  for  the  de- 
velopment of  means  of  appraising  the  effects  on  social  aware- 
ness of  the  reading  of  fiction  dealing  with  social  problems. 
The  literature  used  in  this  program  described  social  and  eco- 
nomic problems,  offered  explanations  of  the  causes  and  ef- 
fects of  these  conditions,  and  suggested  (in  certain  cases) 
types  of  solutions  for  the  problems. 

Analysis  of  the  Objective 

In  analyzing  the  effects  of  such  a  program,  it  was  appar- 
ent that  they  might  be  classified  as  follows:  (1)  increasing 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  235 

student  awareness  of  existing  social  and  economic  condi- 
tions; (2)  stimulating  the  development  of  a  consistent  social 
philosophy,  and  (3)  aiding  students  to  see  tlie  implications 
of  their  personal  social  philosophy  for  concrete  action  in 
specific  problem  situations. 

Two  characteristics  were  thought  important  in  describing 
awareness  or  recognition  of  social  and  economic  conditions. 
First,  there  is  the  extent  of  the  awareness  or  lack  of  it.  The 
extent  of  awareness  may  be  characterized  either  by  the  range 
of  problems  of  which  an  individual  is  aware  or  by  the  depth 
of  understanding  about  any  particular  problem.  It  was  de- 
cided that  in  this  instance  the  range  of  problems  to  which 
an  individual  responds  was  more  significant  than  the  depth 
of  his  understanding  of  any  one  problem.  The  lack  of  aware- 
ness may  be  expressed  in  several  ways.  Students  may  believe 
that  conditions  are  worse  than  facts  indicate,  that  they  are 
better  than  the  facts  indicate,  or  they  may  feel  uncertain 
about  either  the  existence  or  non-existence  of  these  condi- 
tions. The  second  characteristic  of  awareness  is  consistency. 
An  individual  who  has  a  clear  impression  of  actual  social  and 
economic  conditions  will  not  agree  with  both  of  two  plausi- 
ble statements  describing  exactly  opposite  conditions.  An 
instrument  designed  to  measure  awareness  of  social  and  eco- 
nomic conditions  should  yield  evidence  on  each  of  these  char- 
acteristics of  awareness. 

An  individual's  social  philosophy  may  also  be  described  in 
terms  of  several  characteristics.  First,  there  is  the  question 
of  its  general  direction:  Is  it  highly  individualistic?  Is  it  based 
on  humanitarian  values  and  considerations  of  general  wel- 
fare? Is  it  dominated  by  the  acceptance  of  the  status  quo1? 
Does  it  indicate  a  willingness  to  change  contemporary  social 
and  economic  conditions?  Second,  the  degree  of  certainty 
with  which  an  individual  holds  a  particular  point  of  view  is 
of  interest  in  appraising  his  social  philosophy.  Certainty  may 
be  defined  either  with  respect  to  one's  degree  of  conviction 
about  any  single  issue  or  with  respect  to  the  range  of  issues 


236        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

toward  which  one  indicates  a  positive  point  of  view.  For  the 
purposes  of  this  particular  appraisal,  certainty  in  the  latter 
sense  was  considered  more  significant.  The  third  important 
characteristic  of  a  social  philosophy  is  the  degree  of  its  in- 
ternal consistency. 

An  individual's  ability  to  see  the  implications  of  his  social 
philosophy  for  concrete  social  action  may  be  described  first 
with  respect  to  the  predominant  type  of  social  action  he  gen- 
erally approves  or  disapproves  in  specific  problem  situations, 
and  in  terms  of  the  variety  or  comprehensiveness  of  things 
which  he  agrees  should  be  done.  Second,  the  type  of  social 
action  about  which  he  is  frequently  uncertain  can  be  de- 
scribed. Third,  the  types  of  problem  situations  in  which  he 
approves  an  extensive  and  far-reaching  social  action,  those 
in  which  he  approves  little  or  no  social  action,  and  those  in 
which  he  is  primarily  uncertain,  may  be  indicated. 

Description  of  the  Test 

On  the  basis  of  the  analysis  of  (a)  the  types  of  issues 
sampled  in  the  literature  and  of  (b)  the  nature  and  charac- 
teristics of  the  behavior  to  be  measured,  a  test  called  Scale 
of  Beliefs  on  Economic  Issues  was  constructed.  This  test  is 
made  up  of  three  parts. 

The  first  part  of  the  test  consists  of  statements  that  cer- 
tain conditions  do  or  do  not  exist  in  the  United  States.  The 
statements  are  made  in  pairs  so  that  while  one  statement  in- 
dicates the  existence  of  a  given  condition,  the  other  state- 
ment in  the  pair  indicates  the  existence  of  exactly  the  oppo- 
site condition.  The  student  reacts  by  indicating  that  he 
agrees,  disagrees,  or  is  uncertain  about  each  statement  pur- 
porting to  describe  existing  conditions.  In  order  to  get  an 
index  of  the  consistency  of  his  responses  the  two  scales  con- 
taining opposite  statements  are  given  on  different  days.  Re- 
sponses to  this  part  of  the  test  are  summarized  in  terms  of 
the  number  of  answers  wl^ch  indicate  awareness  of  social 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  237 

and  economic  conditions,  lack  of  awareness  of  these  condi- 
tions, uncertainty  about  them,  and  consistency  of  belief  about 
them. 

The  second  part  of  the  test  consists  of  statements  sam- 
pling various  points  of  view  regarding  the  types  of  condi- 
tions which  are  desirable.  These  statements  are  also  made 
in  pairs  in  order  to  obtain  evidence  on  the  consistency  of 
the  student's  social  philosophy.  One  set  of  conditions,  if  con- 
sidered desirable,  implies  approval  of  the  status  quo;  whereas 
the  other,  if  followed  to  its  logical  implications  would  in- 
volve changes  in  the  present  scheme  of  things.  The  issues 
sampled  in  this  section  of  the  test  parallel  those  sampled 
previously.  That  is,  in  the  first  section  there  is  a  statement 
as  to  the  extent  to  which  people  achieve  economic  security 
today,  in  the  second  section,  a  statement  concerning  the 
degree  to  which  people  ought  to  have  economic  security. 
The  student  reacts  by  indicating  agreement,  disagreement, 
or  uncertainty  about  each  statement.  A  student's  responses  to 
this  section  of  the  test  are  summarized  in  terms  of  the  degree 
to  which  he  accepts  and  approves  the  status  quo,  the  degree 
to  which  he  accepts  a  social  philosophy  which  implies  change 
in  the  present  order,  the  degree  to  which  he  is  uncertain 
about  his  social  philosophy,  and  the  degree  to  which  his 
social  philosophy  is  internally  consistent. 

The  third  part  of  the  test  is  made  up  of  a  number  of  prob- 
lem situations  describing  some  specific  instances  of  the  con- 
ditions described  in  the  first  section  of  the  test.  The  descrip- 
tion of  the  problem  is  followed  by  five  courses  of  action  that 
represent  different  points  of  view  about  what  should  be  done 
about  such  specific  problems.  The  types  of  points  of  view 
sampled  in  the  courses  of  action  have  been  labeled  futile, 
conservative,  compromise,  liberal,  and  radical.  These  terms 
are  not  be  understood  as  meaning  anything  other  than  con- 
venient summaries  of  various  points  of  a  scale  ranging  from 
the  attitude  of  "do  nothing"  to  the  attitude  of  "change  the 


238        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

whole  system."  The  student  is  asked  to  indicate  whether  he 
agrees?  disagrees,  or  is  uncertain  about  each  course  of  action. 
His  responses  are  summarized  in  such  a  way  as  to  indicate 
the  extent  to  which  he  agrees,  disagrees,  or  is  uncertain  about 
each  type  of  social  action. 

USES  OF  THESE  TESTS 

The  fact  that  a  test  is  valid  "in  general"  does  not  assure 
that  valid  results  are  necessarily  obtained  in  a  given  school 
or  with  a  given  group  of  students.  There  are  many  condi- 
tions which  must  be  fulfilled  if  these  tests  are  to  be  useful. 
The  most  obvious  one  is  that  the  teacher  should  be  interested 
in  developing  the  kinds  of  behavior  diagnosed  in  the  test. 
Thus  the  tests  dealing  with  social  values  and  beliefs  should 
be  considered  only  if  the  development  of  social  beliefs  and 
the  ability  to  analyze  social  problems  in  terms  of  a  personal 
pattern  of  social  values  is  of  concern  to  the  school. 

A  certain  minimum  background  on  the  part  of  the  students 
is  also  assumed  in  several  of  these  tests.  For  instance,  to  ob- 
tain valid  results  from  the  test  on  Social  Problems  (Form 
1.42),  it  is  necessary  for  students  to  have  had  some  oppor- 
tunity to  discuss  controversial  problems,  to  develop  view- 
points with  reference  to  them,  and  to  acquire  familiarity  with 
basic  democratic  values.  Otherwise  their  responses  will  be 
conditioned  by  factors  other  than  their  ability  to  apply  value 
principles,  such  as  lack  of  familiarity  with  these  principles. 
Similarly  the  test  Application  of  Social  Facts  and  Generaliza- 
tions (Form  1.5)  is  explicitly  designed  for  use  with  students 
who  have  had  opportunity  to  study  issues  similar  to  the  ones 
used  in  the  test  and  have  acquired  some  general  informa- 
tion about  them.  Occasionally  a  teacher  may  want  to  use  an 
exercise  from  this  test  as  a  pre-test,  before  undertaking  a 
specific  unit  of  study.  This  is  appropriate  when  the  stu- 
dents have  had  some  general  experience  with  the  problem 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  239 

and  the  teacher  is  anxious  to  find  out  at  which  level  to  attack 
the  problem  with  them. 

It  is  also  important  for  the  teacher  to  decide  whether  the 
content  and  vocabulary  of  these  tests  are  appropriate  for  his 
group.22  Too  often  in  selecting  a  test,  consideration  is  given 
only  to  its  appropriateness  for  a  given  grade  level.  Pupils  who 
do  not  respond  sensitively  to  the  connotations  of  the  words 
used  in  these  tests  will  not  give  an  accurate  picture  of  their 
social  beliefs  and  values.  The  absence  of  a  time  limit  helps, 
but  not  sufficiently  for  many  groups. 

The  attitudes  and  expectations  of  students  at  the  time  of 
taking  the  test  regarding  the  purpose  of  the  test  and  the  use 
of  the  results  are  extremely  important  in  all  tests  in  which 
students  are  expected  to  express  their  own  viewpoints.  If 
the  students  expect  to  be  graded  on  such  tests,  or  if  for  some 
reason  they  think  that  they  should  please  the  teacher,  they 
are  likely  to  mark  the  test  according  to  their  best  guess  of 
what  is  expected  of  them.  Certain  precautions  have  been 
taken  in  the  tests  themselves  to  prevent  dishonest  marking. 
Thus  in  the  Scales  of  Beliefs  the  items  pertaining  to  a  range 
of  issues  are  in  random  order  to  make  it  more  difficult  for 
the  students  to  see  what  the  "acceptable"  responses  might 
be.  In  the  Social  Problems  test  the  directions  for  marking 
the  test  do  not  reveal  the  kind  of  analysis  to  be  made  of  the 
responses.  No  such  precautions,  however,  can  take  the  place 
of  a  classroom  in  which  the  pupils  and  the  teacher  trust  one 
another. 

Provided,  then,  that  the  qualities  diagnosed  in  the  test  are 
of  concern  to  the  teacher,  that  the  content  and  vocabulary 
of  the  tests  are  appropriate  to  the  level  of  student  develop- 
ment, and  that  students  feel  free  to  express  their  own  views, 
several  fruitful  uses  of  the  results  are  possible.  In  the  first 

22  With  the  exception  of  the  test,  Beliefs  on  School  Life,  which  can  be 
used  in  grades  seven  to  twelve,  none  of  these  tests  is  appropriate  for  non- 
verbal students,  nor  should  they  be  given  below  the  tenth  grade  except  in 
unusual  cases. 


240        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

place,  the  teacher  may  want  to  diagnose  the  strengths  and 
weaknesses  of  the  individuals  in  his  class,  in  order  that  he 
may  give  each  one  the  kind  of  help  he  needs.  In  the  case  of 
the  application  of  social  values,  the  difficulty  of  some  stu- 
dents may  be  in  their  lack  of  social  awareness,  while  others 
are  blocked  by  their  inability  to  see  the  implications  of  social 
values  in  concrete  social  problems.  Conflicting  or  confused 
values  prevent  clear  thinking  for  some  students,  while  gul- 
libility to  slogans  may  be  the  main  difficulty  with  others. 
Each 'needs  a  different  kind  of  help.  Experiences  necessary 
for  broadening  awareness  do  not  necessarily  contribute  to 
greater  consistency.  The  methods  employed  to  clarify  values 
and  beliefs  and  to  eliminate  prejudices  differ  from  the 
methods  of  building  up  a  more  realistic  understanding  of 
social  phenomena.  Students  whose  difficulty  is  the  absence 
of  any  personal  viewpoint  are  not  helped  by  the  kinds  of 
experiences  needed  by  those  handicapped  with  entrenched 
biases  and  prejudices.  The  results  of  the  test  on  Social  Prob- 
lems (Form  1.41  or  1.42)  throw  some  light  on  the  needs  of 
individuals  in  these  respects. 

If  the  teacher  is  interested  in  the  development  of  social 
beliefs,  he  may  want  to  know  in  which  areas  students  tend 
to  be  confused,  to  embrace  conflicting  viewpoints,  or  have 
unfounded  prejudices.  Information  of  this  type  may  also 
serve  as  a  background  for  understanding  difficulties  in  think- 
ing logically.  For  example,  students  who  reveal  strong  preju- 
dices in  the  area  of  economic  relations  in  the  Scale  of  Beliefs 
test  often  make  mistakes  in  reasoning  in  this  area  in  the 
Social  Problems  test.  The  barrier  is  emotional,  not  neces- 
sarily intellectual. 

Through  the  use  of  these  tests,  the  teacher  can  also  check 
the  effectiveness  of  his  curriculum.  For  example,  the  study 
of  current  social  problems  was  introduced  in  many  schools 
in  the  hope  of  engendering  social  awareness  and  a  greater 
ability  and  inclination  to  use  scientific  methods  in  dealing 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  241 

with  social  phenomena.  First-hand  exploration  of  the  com- 
munity and  use  of  literary  materials  to  illustrate  social  prob- 
lems became  a  part  of  most  programs.  Democratic  processes 
in  administering  school  affairs  were  introduced  in  the  hope 
that  personal  democratic  attitudes  might  be  developed. 
These  hypotheses  need  to  be  checked  by  evidence  of  changes 
taking  place  in  students.  Furthermore,  curriculum  experi- 
ences effective  in  one  respect  sometimes  produce  unexpected 
and  undesirable  results  in  some  other  respect.  Thus,  courses 
dealing  with  modern  problems,  introduced  to  enlarge  social 
awareness,  sometimes  increase  inconsistency  and  enhance 
ambivalence  and  confusion  of  social  values.  An  emphasis  on 
democratic  processes  in  school  may  develop  loyalty  to  certain 
values  in  this  situation,  but  without  proper  reference  to 
larger  social  problems,  a  double  standard  of  democratic 
values  may  result. 

There  are  many  points  at  which  an  objective  check  is  par- 
ticularly needed.  One  of  the  most  common  difficulties  in 
social  education  is  that  students  tend  to  master  generalized 
concepts  without  seeing  concretely  enough  how  these  con- 
cepts apply  in  a  variety  of  life  problems.  Thus,  students  tend 
to  remember  and  accept  such  democratic  tenets  as  equality 
of  opportunity  or  freedom  of  speech,  without  recognizing  in 
life  the  problems  in  which  these  values  are  involved  and 
the  ways  in  which  they  are  violated.  The  use  of  the  Scale  of 
Beliefs  in  conjunction  with  the  test  on  Social  Problems  shows 
in  what  degree  these  difficulties  are  present  among  students. 

A  teacher  may  also  want  to  see  whether  his  students  are 
achieving  an  increasingly  consistent  social  viewpoint.  Most 
individuals  tend  to  accept  values  which  are  in  conflict  with 
others  which  they  hold  at  the  same  time.  While  one  would 
not  expect  anyone  to  be  wholly  free  of  these  conflicts,  one 
would  hope  that  with  increasing  maturity  and  with  increas- 
ing understanding  these  conflicts  would  tend  to  be  elimi- 
nated. Often,  however,  school  programs  tend  to  increase 


242        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

these  conflicts  rather  than  to  eliminate  them.  This  is  particu- 
larly the  case  when  the  community  or  the  family  has  a  differ- 
ent philosophy  from  the  one  emphasized  in  the  school. 

A  similar  effect  is  produced  when  students  are  exposed  to 
many  new  experiences  creating  new  beliefs  and  values  with- 
out sufficient  time  to  reconsider  the  values  they  have  already 
developed  in  their  previous  experiences.  Conflicts  are  particu- 
larly apt  to  appear  between  general  beliefs  and  their  specific 
implications.  Thus,  it  is  not  uncommon  to  see  students  ap- 
prove of  a  more  equitable  distribution  of  wealth  in  general 
and  at  the  same  time  be  violently  opposed  to  such  practical 
measures  to  achieve  it  as  the  graduated  income  tax  or  mini- 
mum wage  law.  As  long  as  the  school  programs  tend  to  em- 
phasize generalities,  while  experiences  at  home  and  in  the 
community  contribute  to  the  development  of  specific  values 
and  loyalties,  such  conflicting  viewpoints  are  unavoidable. 
An  increasing  ambivalence  and  conflict  rather  than  increas- 
ing clarification  and  integration  of  social  outlook  result  unless 
teachers  are  continually  aware  of  points  at  which  individuals 
need  help  in  integrating  or  clarifying  their  value  concepts 
and  beliefs.  The  examination  of  the  distribution  of  the  scores 
on  values  in  the  social  problems  test  and  of  the  scores  on 
scales  of  belief  would  reveal  to  what  degree  and  at  which 
points  individuals  and  groups  are  embracing  contradictory 
values  and  beliefs. 

In  addition  to  diagnosing  the  strengths  and  weaknesses  of 
individuals  at  a  given  time,  teachers  may  also  be  interested 
in  changes  occurring  over  a  period  of  time.  The  diagnosis  of 
growth  is  particularly  important  in  connection  with  the  as- 
pects of  social  sensitivity  dealt  with  in  this  chapter.  Changes 
in  fundamental  value  patterns,  methods  of  applying  values, 
and  using  information  to  gain  deeper  insight  into  complex 
social  problems  do  not  take  place  overnight.  The  results  of 
experiences  at  a  given  time  may  not  show  up  until  a  good 
deal  later.  Moreover,  these  are  objectives  which  cannot  be 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  243 

finally  established  during  the  high  school  years.  At  best,  one 
can  hope  to  establish  certain  tendencies  and  predispositions 
and  to  initiate  certain  techniques  of  analysis  and  inquiry. 
This  means  that  it  is  important  to  get  evidence  of  the  direc- 
tion of  changes  taking  place  in  students.  Administering  tests 
of  this  sort  over  a  period  of  time  would  help  determine  such 
long-term  changes.23 

Generally  it  is  not  advisable  to  use  any  of  these  tests  less 
than  a  year  apart.  They  are  too  general  in  content,  in  the  first 
place,  to  reveal  minor  changes.  Secondly,  the  scores  are  not 
reliable  enough  to  detect  small  amounts  of  change.  However, 
the  exercises  in  the  test  Application  of  Social  Facts  and  Gen- 
eralizations (Form  1.5)  can  be  used  as  a  pre-test  and  as  an 
end-test  in  evaluating  the  effectiveness  of  a  given  unit  of 
study,  within  an  interval  of  a  few  weeks.  The  use  of  these 
exercises  as  a  pre-test  would  serve  two  ends:  (1)  to  diagnose 
the  background  of  the  students  in  order  to  attack  the  prob- 
lem at  an  appropriate  level,  and  (2)  to  give  direction  and 
impetus  to  the  study.  The  end-test  would  show  how  well 
students  had  mastered  the  ideas  and  techniques  for  under- 
standing a  given  problem. 

It  must  be  pointed  out  here  that  while  each  of  these  tests 
was  designed  as  an  independent  unit,  better  information 
about  the  students  and  the  effectiveness  of  the  curriculum  is 
secured  when  several  of  them  are  given  and  interpreted  to- 
gether. This  is  particularly  true  of  the  Scale  of  Beliefs 
(Form  4.21-4.31)  and  of  the  Social  Problems  test  (Forms 
1.41  and  1.42).  These  two  tests  were  planned  as  companion 
instruments — one  to  give  an  overview  of  general  beliefs,  and 
the  other  to  diagnose  their  application  in  concrete  situa- 
tions. In  most  cases  the  data  from  a  single  instrument  must 

23  The  tests  of  beliefs,  such  as  the  Scale  of  Beliefs  on  Social  Issues  ( Form 
4.21-4.31)  can  be  administered  several  times.  Two  forms  of  the  test  on 
Social  Problems  (Forms  1.41  and  1.42)  have  been  made  available.  These 
forms  are  sufficiently  similar  to  enable  teachers  to  compare  scores  on  one 
form  with  those  on  the  other  form. 


244        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

be  supplemented  with  other  evidence  before  safe  inferences 
can  be  drawn.  This  is  particularly  the  case  when  it  is  neces- 
sary to  carry  the  diagnosis  to  the  point  of  locating  the  causes 
of  difficulty.  Thus,  ambivalence  of  value  pattern  may  be  the 
result  of  lack  of  acquaintance  with  the  issues  involved,  lack 
of  ability  to  see  logical  relations,  sheer  inability  to  read  and 
to  understand  this  test,  or  a  genuine  division  of  viewpoint. 
These  possibilities  have  to  be  checked  against  other  evi- 
dence, such  as  reading  scores,  scores  on  psychological  tests, 
tests  on  logical  thinking,  or  daily  observations  of  students' 
behavior  in  the  classroom.  Only  after  such  checking  can  the 
teacher  be  safe  in  planning  the  experiences  necessary  to 
eliminate  the  difficulties. 

In  still  other  cases,  the  interpreter  needs  to  resort  to  a  more 
detailed  analysis  of  student  responses  than  is  possible  by 
examining  the  score  sheet.  In  the  case  of  the  Social  Problems 
test,  some  students  may  have  difficulties  in  connection  with 
certain  problems  and  issues  and  not  with  others.  Whenever 
there  is  reason  to  believe  that  the  scores  on  the  data  sheet 
have  covered  up  important  information,  it  is  profitable  to 
examine  the  answer  sheets  themselves. 


Chapter  IV 

ASPECTS  OF  APPRECIATION 


«<•  CCC-  C<C  «fr  CCC-  C<C-  C<(-  C<C-  CCC-  C«-  C<C- 


INTBODUCTION 

All  of  the  lists  of  objectives  submitted  by  schools  in  the 
Eight-  Year  Study  mentioned  the  development  of  a  wide 
range,  an  increasing  depth,  and  a  personal  selection  of  inter- 
ests and  appreciations.  Accordingly,  an  interschool  Commit- 
tee on  the  Evaluation  of  Interests  and  Appreciations  was 
formed  early  in  the  Study  and  met  frequently  to  analyze 
this  area  of  objectives.  One  of  its  first  conclusions  was  that, 
although  interests  and  appreciations  are  so  closely  related 
that  it  is  often  impossible  to  distinguish  them  in  specific  in- 
stances, techniques  for  evaluating  them  would  be  sufficiently 
different  to  justify  a  division  of  labor.  The  committee  was 
therefore  divided  into  sub-groups  after  arriving  at  a  common 
understanding  of  the  objectives  to  be  considered.  Many 
subtle  distinctions  were  drawn  between  interests  and  appre- 
ciations, but  their  common  purport  seemed  to  be  that  inter- 
ests emphasize  'liking"  an  activity,  while  appreciations  in- 
clude "liking"  but  emphasize  "insight"  into  the  activity: 
understanding  it,  realizing  its  true  values,  distinguishing  the 
better  from  the  worse,  and  the  like.  The  sub-committees  on 
appreciations  developed  instruments  chiefly  in  the  fields  of 
literature  and  the  arts,  which  are  reported  in  this  chapter. 
The  work  of  the  Committee  on  Interests  is  reported  in  Chap- 
ter V. 


245 


246        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

APPRECIATION  OF  LITERATUBE 

Since  there  are  somewhat  different  points  of  view  as  to 
what  is  meant  by  the  objective  "Appreciation  of  Literature/'1 
it  is  important  to  recognize  at  the  outset  that  the  analysis 
which  will  be  described  here  is  restricted  to  an  analysis  of 
certain  types  of  students'  reactions  to  reading.  This  restric- 
tion should  not  be  taken  to  imply  that  other  behaviors  might 
not  be  included  under  the  heading  "Appreciation  of  Litera- 
ture'*; a  number  of  articles  and  studies  might  be  cited  to 
illustrate  the  range  of  behaviors  which  have,  at  various 
times,  been  identified  with  appreciation.  Carroll,2  for  ex- 
ample, mentions  information,  sensitivity  to  style,  understand- 
ing of  "deeper  meanings,"  and  emotional  response  as  in- 
cluded in  appreciation.  In  developing  his  tests  of  prose 
appreciation  Carroll  chose  to  measure  students'  ability  to 
differentiate  the  good  from  the  less  good  and  the  less  good 
from  the  very  bad.3  This  ability  has  been  regarded  by  many 
as  an  important  element  in,  or  index  of,  appreciation.  Logasa 
and  Wright,  to  cite  a  second  example,  have  made  a  rather 
extensive  analysis  of  appreciation4  and  have  published  tests 
of  the  following  behaviors;  discovery  of  theme,  reader  par- 
ticipation, reaction  to  sensory  images,  discrimination  be- 
tween good  and  poor  comparisons,  recognition  of  rhythm, 
and  appreciation  of  fresh  expressions  as  opposed  to  triteness. 
Instead,  the  restriction  mentioned  above  merely  implies  a 
selection,  on  the  part  of  the  committee,  of  behaviors  which 
( 1 )  were  regarded  by  them  as  important  aspects  of  appre- 
ciation, and  (2)  were  not  being  adequately  appraised  by  the 
available  instruments.  A  major  question  which  the  committee 

1  Cf.  Broom,  M.  E.,  "Literature  and  Aesthetics/'  The  High  School  Teacher, 
VIII  (October,  1932),  pp.  293-294. 

2  Carroll,  Herbert,  "A  Method  of  Measuring  Prose  Appreciation,"  English 
Journal  XXII  (March,  1933),  p.  184. 

3  Op.  cit.,  p.  185. 

4 See  "Tests  for  Measuring  Appreciation,"  School  Review,  XXXIII  (Sep- 
tember, 1925),  pp.  491-492. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  247 

wished  to  be  able  to  answer  Is:  <<rHow  do  students  react  to 
their  reading?7'  For  convenience,  certain  of  these  reactions 
to  reading  have  been  designated  as  "Aspects  of  Apprecia- 
tion." 

The  Committee's  Analysis  of  Students' 
Reactions  to  Reading 

The  Committee  on  the  Evaluation  of  Reading  was  organ- 
ized in  the  fall  of  1935.  In  selecting  members  for  this 
committee  the  schools  recognized  that  teachers  other  than 
teachers  of  literature  are  often  responsible  for  guiding  the 
reading  of  students  and  hence  should  participate  in  the  eval- 
uation of  reading  outcomes.  For  this  reason,  in  addition  to 
the  field  of  English,  other  areas,  such  as  social  studies,  the 
core  program,  the  school  library,  and  school  administration, 
were  represented  by  various  members  of  the  committee.  Be- 
cause of  the  wide  geographical  distribution  of  the  schools  in 
the  Eight- Year  Study,  this  committee  was  divided  into  two 
sub-committees,  one  of  which  met  in  New  York  City  and  the 
other  in  Chicago.  During  the  school  years  1935-36,  1936-37, 
and  1937-38  a  number  of  committee  meetings  were  held  in 
these  two  cities.  The  meetings  held  in  New  York  City  were 
attended  by  representatives  of  16  eastern  schools;  meetings 
in  Chicago  were  attended  by  representatives  of  eight  schools 
in  the  Middle  West.  Members  of  the  Evaluation  Staff  also 
attended  these  meetings  and  coordinated  the  work  of  the 
two  sub-committees. 

The  Committee  on  the  Evaluation  of  Reading  undertook, 
as  its  first  task  in  developing  instruments  for  appraising  stu- 
dents' reactions  to  their  reading,  to  clarify  what  was  meant 
by  "reactions  to  reading/'  A  preliminary  analysis  of  students' 
reactions  to  reading  was  made,  at  the  request  o£  the  commit- 
tee, by  Carleton  Jones  of  the  Evaluation  Staff  and  was  sub- 
mitted to  them  for  revision.  After  some  discussion,  the  com- 
mittee selected  from  the  preliminary  analysis  seven  behaviors 


248        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

or  reactions  to  reading  which  seemed  to  them  to  be  of  con- 
siderable importance.  These  are: 

1.  Satisfaction  in  the  thing  appreciated 

Appreciation  manifests  itself  in  a  feeling,  on  the  part  of 
the  individual,  of  keen  satisfaction  in  and  enthusiam  for 
the  thing  appreciated.  The  person  who  appreciates  a  given 
piece  of  literature  finds  in  it  an  immediate,  persistent,  and 
easily-renewable  enjoyment  of  extraordinary  intensity. 

2.  Desire  for  more  of  the  thing  appreciated 
Appreciation  manifests  itself  in  an  active  desire  on  the 
part  of  the  individual  for  more  of  the  thing  appreciated. 
The  person  who  appreciates  a  given  piece  of  literature  is 
desirous  of  prolonging,  extending,  supplementing,  renew- 
ing his  first  favorable  response  toward  it. 

3.  Desire  to  know  more  about  the  thing  appreciated 
Appreciation  manifests  itself  in  an  active  desire  on  the 
part  of  the  individual  to  know  more  about  the  thing  ap- 
preciated. The  person  who  appreciates  a  given  piece  of 
literature  is  desirous  of  understanding  as  fully  as  possible 
the  significant  meanings  which  it  aims  to  express  and  of 
knowing  something  about  its  genesis,  its  history,  its  locale, 
its  sociological  background,  its  author,  etc. 

4.  Desire  to  express  one's  self  creatively 

Appreciation  manifests  itself  in  an  active  desire  on  the 
part  of  an  individual  to  go  beyond  the  thing  appreciated: 
to  give  creative  expression  to  ideas  and  feelings  of  his 
own  which  the  thing  appreciated  has  chiefly  engendered. 
The  person  who  appreciates  a  given  piece  of  literature  is 
desirous  of  doing  for  himself,  either  in  the  same  or  in  a 
different  medium,  something  of  what  the  author  has  done 
in  the  medium  of  literature. 

5.  Identification  of  one's  self  with  the  thing  appreciated 
Appreciation  manifests  itself  in  the  individual's  active 
identification  of  himself  with  the  thing  appreciated.  The 
person  who  appreciates  a  given  piece  of  literature  re- 
sponds to  it  very  much  as  If  he  were  actually  participat- 
ing in  the  life  situations  which  it  represents. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  249 

6.  Desire  to  clarify  ones  own  thinking  with  regard  to  the 
life  problems  raised  by  the  thing  appreciated 

Appreciation  manifests  itself  in  an  active  desire  on  the 
part  of  the  individual  to  clarify  his  own  thinking  with  re- 
gard to  specific  life  problems  raised  by  the  thing  appre- 
ciated. The  person  who  appreciates  a  given  piece  of  litera- 
ture is  stimulated  by  it  to  re-think  his  own  point  of  view 
toward  certain  of  the  life  problems  with  which  it  deals 
and  perhaps  subsequently  to  modify  his  own  practical 
behavior  in  meeting  those  problems. 

7.  Desire  to  evaluate  the  thing  appreciated 
Appreciation  manifests  itself  in  a  conscious  effort  on  the 
part  of  the  individual  to  evaluate  the  thing  appreciated  in 
terms  of  such  standards  of  merit  as  he  himself,  at  the 
moment,  tends  to  subscribe  to.  The  person  who  appreci- 
ates a  given  piece  of  literature  is  desirous  of  discovering 
and  describing  for  himself  the  particular  values  which  it 
seems  to  hold  for  him. 

An  example  may  aid  in  clarifying  each  of  these  seven 
behaviors.  Let  us  suppose  that  a  student  has  read  a  particular 
novel,  such  as  Dickens'  Tale  of  Two  Cities,  and  that  during 
the  reading  of  this  book  he  has  read  attentively  and  with 
absorption  ( 1 ) .  Let  us  also  suppose  that  he  has  derived  such 
satisfaction  from  the  book  that  he  plans  to  read  it  again  and 
to  read  other  novels  by  Dickens  (2).  Perhaps  his  curiosity 
about  Dickens  as  an  author,  about  the  literary  currents  of 
the  middle  nineteenth  century,  about  the  historical  novel  as 
a  type,  or  about  the  French  Revolution  has  been  aroused  by 
his  reading  (3).  He  might  want  to  sketch  Carton  riding  to 
the  guillotine  or  try  to  conceive  in  words  some  scene  or 
character  which  grows  out  of  his  reading  (4).  While  reading 
he  might  "lose  himself  in  the  events  of  the  book,  he  might, 
like  Booth  Tarkington's  Willie  Baxter,  become  one  with  Car- 
ton and  feel  that  "It  is  a  far,  far  better  thing  that  I  do  .  .  ." 
(5).  Many  problems  might  be  suggested  or  raised  again 
for  him  by  his  reading;  he  might  want  to  think  through  what 


250        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

friendship  or  love  implies,  what  the  proper  ends  o£  life  are, 
what  terror  and  force  effect  in  the  world  (6).  Finally,  he 
might  want  to  compare  this  novel  with  others  by  Dickens 
and  others  of  its  type,  compare  his  judgments  of  it  with 
those  of  other  persons,  seek  out  its  values  and  its  limita- 
tions (7). 

This  statement  of  important  reactions  to  reading  is  a  selec- 
tive one  and  should  be  regarded  as  such.  A  number  of  other 
reactions  or  responses  to  reading  might  be  identified  and 
judged  to  be  of  importance  by  other  teachers  or  test  makers. 
Pooley,5  for  example,  has  made  a  rather  detailed  analysis  of 
"fundamental"  and  "secondary"  responses  to  prose  and 
poetry  which  differs  somewhat  from  the  analysis  accepted 
by  the  committee.  Since  our  purpose  is  to  report  what  was 
done  by  these  committees  and  the  Evaluation  Staff  during 
the  period  of  the  Eight-Year  Study,  a  comprehensive  discus- 
sion of  the  many  definitions  of  appreciation  or  of  the  many 
possible  analyses  of  responses  to  reading  cannot  be  given. 
Consequently,  the  omission  of  a  careful  consideration  of  the 
many  studies  and  tests  of  literary  appreciation  which  have 
been  made  by  others  should  not  be  regarded  either  as  an 
oversight  or  as  evidence  of  a  belief  that  the  work  reported 
here  exhausts  the  topic  "The  Evaluation  of  Appreciation  of 
Literature/7 

Instruments  Which  Were  Developed  to  Appraise 
Students'  Reactions  to  Their  Reading 

A  number  of  instruments  were  developed  for  the  evalua- 
tion of  students'  reactions  to  their  reading.  Three  of  these 
instruments  make  use  of  a  questionnaire  technique  which 
consists  essentially  of  asking  students  to  observe  themselves, 
in  retrospect,  and  to  record  these  observations.  This  tech- 
nique was  arrived  at  in  the  following  manner.  The  commit- 
tee first  discussed  ways  in  which  the  seven  types  of  reaction 

5  Pooley,  Robert,  "Measuring  the  Appreciation  of  Literature,"  English 
Journal  (High  School  Edition),  XXIV  (October,  1935),  pp.  627-633. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  251 

to  reading  might  be  manifested  in  readily  observable  student 
behavior  and  prepared  a  list  of  overt  acts  and  verbal  re- 
sponses which,  they  judged,  would  in  certain  situations  re- 
veal the  presence  or  absence  of  each  of  these  seven  types  of 
behavior.  A  few  of  the  overt  acts  and  verbal  responses  which 
were  included  in  this  list  are: 

1.  Satisfaction  in  the  thing  appreciated 

1.1  He  reads   aloud  to  others,  or  simply  to   himself, 
passages  which  he  finds  unusually  interesting. 

1.2  He  reads  straight  through  without  stopping,  or  with 
a  minimum  of  interruption. 

1.3  He  reads  for  considerable  periods  of  time. 

2.  Desire  for  more  of  the  thing  appreciated 

2.1  He  asks  other  people  to  recommend  reading  which 
is  more  or  less  similar  to  the  thing  appreciated. 

2.2  He  commences  this  reading  of  similar  things  as  soon 
after  reading  the  first  as  possible. 

2.3  He  reads  subsequently  several  books,  plays,  or  poems 
by  the  same  author. 

3.  Desire  to  know  more  about  the  thing  appreciated 

3.1  He  asks  other  people  for  information  or  sources  of 
information  about  what  he  has  read. 

3.2  He  reads  supplementary  materials,  such  as  biogra- 
phy, history,  criticism,  etc. 

3.3  He  attends  literary  meetings  devoted  to  reviews, 
criticisms,  discussions,  etc. 

4.  Desire  to  express  one's  self  creatively 

4.1  He  produces,  or  at  least  undertakes  to  produce,  a 
creative  product  more  or  less  after  the  manner  of 
the  thing  appreciated. 

4.2  He  writes  critical  appreciations. 

4.3  He  illustrates  what  he  has  read  in  some  one  of  the 
graphic,  spatial,  musical,  or  dramatic  arts. 

5.  Identification  of  one's  self  with  the  thing  appreciated 

5.1    He  accepts,  at  least  while  he  is  reading,  the  persons, 
places,  situations,  events,  etc.,  as  real. 


252        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

5.2  He  dramatizes,  formally  or  informally,  various  pas- 
sages. 

5.3  He    imitates,    consciously    and    unconsciously,    the 
speech  and  actions  of  various  characters  in  the  story. 

6.  Desire  to  clarify  one's  own  thinking  with  regard  to  the  life 
problems  raised  by  the  thing  appreciated 

6.1  He  attempts  to  state,  either  orally  or  in  writing,  his 
own  ideas,  feelings,  or  information  concerning  the 
life  problems  with  which  his  reading  deals. 

6.2  He   examines   other   sources   for   more   information 
about  these  problems. 

6.3  He  reads  other  works  dealing  with  similar  problems. 

7.  Desire  to  evaluate  the  thing  appreciated 

7.1  He  points  out,  both  orally  and  in  writing,  the  ele- 
ments which  in  his  opinion  make  it  good  literature. 

7.2  He  explains  how  certain  unacceptable  elements  (if 
any )  could  be  improved. 

7.3  He  consults  published  criticisms. 

The  committee  next  suggested  that  one  method  of  securing 
evidence  of  these  seven  types  of  response  in  secondary 
schools  would  be  to  ask  students  to  report  on  these  be- 
haviors themselves.  The  advantage  of  asking  students  to 
observe  themselves  and  to  record  these  observations,  as  com- 
pared with  the  collection  of  anecdotal  records  or  the  use  of 
interviews,  is  primarily  one  of  practicability.  The  committee 
also  recognized  that  the  use  of  a  questionnaire  technique 
demands  that  certain  assumptions  be  fulfilled  if  the  method 
is  to  give  valid  evidence.  Most  important  among  these  as- 
sumptions are:  (1 )  that  the  overt  behaviors  and  their  accom- 
panying situations  specified  in  the  items  are  significant  evi- 
dence of  the  seven  types  of  behavior;  (2)  that  the  students 
are  capable  of  observing  these  overt  behaviors,  of  remember- 
ing them,  and  of  recording  them;  (3)  that  the  students  are 
honest  in  their  responses  to  each  item.  The  extent  to  which 
these  assumptions  actually  are  fulfilled  will  depend  upon 
both  the  characteristics  of  the  questionnaire  itself  and  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  253 

situation  in  which  the  student  is  asked  to  respond  to  the 
questionnaire.  First,  let  us  review  the  construction  of  one  of 
these  three  questionnaires,  pointing  out  the  criteria  in  its 
construction  which  were  made  necessary  by  these  assump- 
tions; later  we  shall  consider  the  administration  of  such  an 
instrument  and  the  conditions  under  which  its  use  is  most  apt 
to  give  valid  evidence. 

Questionnaire  on  Voluntary  Reading 

Of  the  three  appreciation  questionnaires — The  Novel 
Questionnaire,  The  Drama  Questionnaire,  and  The  Question- 
naire on  Voluntary  Reading — which  were  developed  during 
the  period  of  the  Eight- Year  Study,  The  Questionnaire  on 
Voluntary  Reading  was  used  and  studied  most  extensively; 
for  this  reason  it  will  be  chosen  to  illustrate  the  construction 
of  an  instrument  to  measure  students*  responses  to  their 
reading.  This  questionnaire  was  designed  to  measure  the  ex- 
tent to  which  students  exhibit  the  seven  types  of  response 
to  their  "free"  or  voluntary  reading  of  books.  The  directions 
to  the  student  on  the  questionnaire  read  in  part  as  follows: 

QUESTIONNAIRE  ON   VOLUNTARY  READING 

Directions  to  the  Student 

The  purpose  of  this  questionnaire  is  to  discover  what  you  really 
think  about  the  reading  which  you  do  in  your  leisure  time.  Alto- 
gether there  are  one  hundred  questions.  Consider  each  question 
carefully  and  answer  it  as  honestly  and  as  frankly  as  you  pos- 
sibly can.  There  are  no  "right"  answers  as  such.  It  is  not  expected 
that  your  own  thoughts  or  feelings  or  activities  relating  to  books 
should  be  like  those  of  anyone  else. 

The  numbers  on  your  Answer  Sheet  correspond  to  the  numbers 
of  the  questions  on  the  questionnaire.  There  are  three  ways  to 
mark  the  Answer  Sheet: 

A — means  that  your  answer  to  the  question  is  Yes. 

U — means  that  your  answer  to  the  question  is  Uncertain. 

D — means  that  your  answer  to  the  question  is  No. 


254        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

If  it  is  at  all  possible,  answer  the  questions  by  Yes  or  No.  You 
should  mark  a  question  Uncertain  only  if  you  are  unable  to  an- 
swer either  Yes  or  No. 

Please  answer  every  question 

One  hundred  questions  which  the  student  is  asked  to  an- 
swer make  up  the  items  of  the  questionnaire.  An  illustrative 
set  of  items,  grouped  under  the  seven  types  of  response,6 
follows: 

"Derives  satisfaction  from  reading" 

1.  Is  it  unusual  for  you,  of  your  own  accord,  to  spend  a 
whole  afternoon  or  evening  reading  a  book? 

2.  Do  you  ever  read  plays,  apart  from  school  requirements? 
'Wants  to  read  more" 

1.  Do  you  have  in  mind  one  or  two  books  which  you  would 
like  to  read  sometime  soon? 

2.  Do  you  wish  that  you  had  more  time  to  devote  to  reading? 
"Identifies  himself  with  his  reading'' 

L  Have  you  ever  tried  to  become  in  some  respects  like  a 
character  whom  you  have  read  about  and  admired? 

2.  Is  it  very  unusual  for  you  to  become  sad  or  depressed 

over  the  fate  of  a  character? 
"Becomes  curious  about  his  reading" 

1.  Do  you  read  the  book  review  sections  of  magazines  or 
newspapers  fairly  regularly? 

2.  Do  you  ever  read,  apart  from  school  requirements,  books 
or  articles  about  English  or  American  literature? 

"Expresses  himself  creatively" 

L  Have  you  ever  wanted  to  act  out  a  scene  from  a  book 

which  you  have  read? 
2.  Has  your  reading  of  books  ever  stimulated  you  to  attempt 

any  original  writing  of  your  own? 
"Evaluates  his  reading" 

1.  Do  you  ordinarily  read  a  book  without  giving  much 
thought  to  the  quality  of  its  style? 

6  In  the  questionnaire  itself,  the  items  are  ungrouped;  they  are,  how- 
ever, readily  classified  by  use  of  the  scoring  key. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  255 

2.  Do  you  ever  consult  published  criticisms  of  any  of  the 

books  which  you  read? 
"Relates  his  reading  to  life*7 

1.  Has  your  attitude  toward  war  or  patriotism  been  changed 
by  books  which  you  have  read? 

2.  Is  it  very  unusual  for  you  to  gain  from  your  reading  of 
books  a  better  understanding  of  some  of  the  problems 
which  people  face  in  their  everyday  living? 

It  will  be  observed  that  this  statement  of  the  seven  types  of 
behavior  differs  somewhat  from  that  given  on  pages  251  and 
252.  The  major  purpose  of  this  rewording  was  to  place  the 
emphasis,  for  several  of  these  types  of  behavior,  on  what 
students  actually  do  rather  than  on  what  they  desire  to  do. 

The  first  criterion  that  the  items  included  in  the  question- 
naire had  to  satisfy  was  that  they  must  deal  with  behaviors 
which  were  judged  by' teachers  who  prepared  and  used  the 
questionnaire  to  be  significant  evidence  of  the  seven  types 
of  response  to  reading.  In  a  sense,  then,  the  items  constitute 
a  definition,  in  terms  of  what  students  do  and  say,  of  what 
these  teachers  meant  by  "Derives  satisfaction  from  reading/' 
"Wants  to  read  more,"  etc.  In  order  to  insure  that  this  cri- 
terion was  satisfied,  the  items  were  drawn  originally  from 
the  list  of  overt  acts  and  verbal  responses  which  the  com- 
mittee judged  to  be  significant  evidences  of  the  seven  types 
of  response.  Then,  as  use  of  the  questionnaire  in  a  number 
of  schools  gave  opportunity  to  secure  from  teachers  addi- 
tional judgments  of  the  significance  of  these  items,  the  ques- 
tions were  revised. 

In  selecting  and  phrasing  items  it  was  necessary  to  con- 
sider several  additional  criteria.  The  assumption  that  stu- 
dents are  capable  of  observing  these  overt  behaviors  in 
themselves,  of  remembering,  and  of  recording  them  de- 
mands first  of  all  that  each  item  deal  only  with  those  be- 
haviors which  secondary  school  students  are  apt  to  exhibit 
and  only  with  situations  in  which  students  are  apt  to  find 


2,56        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

themselves.  This  is  almost  an  obvious  criterion,  for  if  we 
expect  the  student  to  report  on  his  behavior  we  must  ask 
him  questions  about  things  he  actually  has  an  opportunity 
to  do.  The  committee,  in  preparing  the  list  of  overt  acts  and 
verbal  responses,  and  teachers,  in  judging  the  significance  of 
items  included  in  the  early  forms  of  the  questionnaire,  were 
asked  to  consider  whether  or  not  each  of  the  specific  acts  or 
verbal  responses  is  something  which  secondary  school  stu- 
dents are  apt  to  do  or  say.  It  was  possible  later,  by  studying 
the  responses  of  students  to  each  item  on  the  questionnaire, 
to  check  these  judgments  of  teachers  to  some  extent.  Second, 
this  assumption  demands  that  each  item  deal  with  behavior 
and  situations  which  the  student  is  apt  to  remember.  This 
criterion  immediately  rules  out  certain  types  of  questions.  In 
general,  we  would  not  expect  students  to  remember,  for 
example,  exactly  how  many  books  they  had  read  during  the 
summer;  yet  we  might  expect  them  to  remember  whether  or 
not  they  had  read  a  book  during  the  preceding  week.  In 
general,  we  would  not  expect  them  to  remember  the  details 
of  an  argument  with  a  friend  about  the  merits  of  a  particular 
book;  yet  we  might  expect  them  to  remember  having  tried 
to  defend  their  judgment  of  a  book.  Third,  this  assumption 
demands  that  any  judgments  or  generalizations  which  the 
student  is  asked  to  formulate  be  relatively  simple  ones.  An 
item  which  calls  for  an  extensive  introspection,  for  the  rating 
of  one's  self  on  an  abstract  and  undefined  quality,  for  mak- 
ing fine  distinctions  between  causes  or  effects,  etc.,  thus 
would  be  ruled  out.  Fourth,  this  assumption  demands  that 
each  question  be  so  phrased  that  it  is  readily  understood  by 
the  student  and  can  be  answered  with  a  minimum  of  writing. 
That  the  question  must  be  understood  if  he  is  to  answer  it 
intelligently  is  obvious.  That  his  ability  to  express  himself  in 
writing  may  become  a  factor  which,  for  this  test,  may  inap- 
propriately condition  the  evidence  and  the  judgments  made 
from  the  evidence,  was  also  recognized.  The  selection  of 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  257 

Jes,  No,  and  Uncertain  as  the  particular  pattern  of  "con- 
trolled response"  for  the  questionnaires  eliminated  the  neces- 
sity of  the  student's  writing  out  his  answers,  but  made  it 
necessary  that  each  question  be  so  phrased  that  it  could  be 
answered  with  one  of  the  three  responses  provided. 

The  assumption  that  students  are  honest  in  their  responses 
also  suggests  criteria  which  each  item  must  meet.  Certain  ac- 
tivities and  certain  situations  may  have  such  a  "prestige" 
value  that  questions  dealing  with  them  would  tempt  the 
student  to  say  that  he  took  part  in  them?  whether  he  actually 
did  or  not.  Questions  dealing  with  any  activity  which  is  ordi- 
narily participated  in  because  of  its  "social"  value  thus  were 
ruled  out,  as  were  all  questions  dealing  with  activities  in 
which  participation  might  be  dependent  primarily  upon  an 
economic  factor.  Likewise,  items  which  deal  with  activities 
or  situations,  the  disclosure  of  which  might  threaten  the 
student's  sense  of  security,  may  tempt  him  to  disavow  actual 
participation  in  these  activities  or  situations.  Questions  which 
asked  students  to  admit  the  reading  of  certain  kinds  of  ma- 
terials which  are  commonly  frowned  upon,  such  as  comic 
magazines,  or  to  disclose  any  of  his  more  intimate  feelings  or 
relationships  with  other  persons  also  were  ruled  out.  The  final 
criterion  for  the  selection  of  the  items,  then,  is  that  they  deal 
only  with  overt  acts  and  verbal  responses  which  the  student 
might  be  expected  to  report  honestly. 

Summarizing  and  Scoring  the  Questionnaire 
on  Voluntary  Reading 

Several  forms  of  the  Questionnaire  on  Voluntary  Reading 
were  prepared  during  the  period  of  the  Eight-Year  Study; 
comparison  of  these  several  forms  reveals  that  ( 1 )  the  items 
included  in  Form  3.32  probably  best  meet  the  criteria  out- 
lined above,  (2)  the  length  of  Form  3.32  probably  is  an 
optimum  for  both  practicability  and  reliability,7  (3)  the 

7  Statistical  data  on  reliability  are  presented  in  the  Appendix. 


258        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

method  of  summarizing  Form  3.32  is  statistically  preferable. 
For  these  reasons,  the  form  of  the  Questionnaire  on  Volun- 
tary Reading  which  is  recommended  for  use  is  Form  3.32. 

Form  3.32  is  made  up  of  the  set  of  directions  reprinted  on 
page  253  and  a  list  of  100  questions  which  students  are  asked 
to  answer  with  one  of  three  responses:  Yes,  No,  or  Uncertain. 
The  responses  to  each  of  these  100  items  are  summarized 
under  six  categories:  (1)  Likes  to  read,  (2)  Identifies  him- 
self with  reading,  (3)  Becomes  curious  about  reading,  (4) 
Expresses  himself  creatively,  (5)  Evaluates  his  reading, 
(6)  Relates  his  reading  to  life.  Originally,  seven  categories 
were  used  for  summary  of  the  scores  on  the  questionnaire, 
but  study  of  the  students'  responses  revealed  that  scores  on 
the  categories  "Derives  satisfaction  from  reading"  and  "Wants 
to  read  more"  are  so  closely  related  statistically  as  to  warrant 
their  being  consolidated  under  one  heading,  "Likes  to  read." 
On  page  259  there  is  presented  a  sample  of  the  data  sheet 
on  which  the  scores  made  by  individual  students  on  Form 
3.32  are  reported.  The  scores  of  five  students  are  presented 
for  purposes  of  illustration.  At  the  bottom  of  the  data  sheet 
appear  the  maximum  possible  score  for  each  column,  and 
the  highest,  the  lowest,  and  the  median  score  for  each  column 
computed  for  the  class  from  which  these  five  students  were 
selected.  All  the  scores  on  the  data  sheet  are  expressed  as  per 
cents;  for  example,  the  scores  in  column  one  are  per  cents  of 
the  35  responses  which  are  grouped  under  the  heading 
"Likes  to  read." 

Three  scores  are  available  for  each  of  the  categories:  an 
"Appreciation"  score,  a  "Non-appreciation"  score,  and  an 
"Uncertain"  score.  For  each  category  the  "Appreciation" 
score  summarizes  the  responses  which  indicate  that  the  stu- 
dent engages  in  those  behaviors  which  are  regarded  as  sig- 
nificant evidence  of  that  type  of  behavior;  the  "Non-apprecia- 
tion" score  summarizes  the  responses  which  indicate  that  the 
student  does  not  engage  in  those  behaviors;  and  the  "Uncer- 


Total  Score 

a  i     N 
t>  1     « 

t^  wl  ^  ^  O 
~*  ~4           ff 

O 

"* 

•* 

c 

o 

co 

\o  «  ri  to  ^< 

CM  *^  r*5  vo 

OJ 

<O        M 

< 

O    [  f-  fO  "0  r}»  O 
<XJ    \   t*-  <5  00  CM  fO 

Tt< 
CM 

00 

ON 

s 

0 

H  *J 

nsjj 

rt  "3  M 
w  ^ 

5 

l*» 

CM 

r\j  «p#  O  CM  O 
fO             10 

O 

CM 
V) 

•* 

o          vo 
£    1      « 

<*  N  O  (N  ^ 

fo  oj  fo  o 

o 

CO 

o 

CM 
fO 

< 

l/> 

CM 

•^  -f  O  \0  *O 

\O  V3  00  ~*  ^ 

\0 

8 

0 

o 

H 

H-d 

tJ  o 

£H 

I 

<*5 

01 

CM  r«j  O  t^  O 

Cv]  ^   r-i   Tl* 

O 

N. 

"* 

<# 

1 

CM 
CM 

O  00  O  O  «5 

*H   Oi  rH   ^  1>- 

O 

IO 

0 

«? 

ft 

CM 

00  O  C  fO  10 

*o  ^o  oc  *-i  cs 

[*> 

o 

o 

PO 

<? 

Part  IID 
"Evaluates" 

I 

O\ 

o  o  o  o  o 

^              10 

o 

o 

vO 

0 

o 

£ 

00 

00  O  00 

cs  ^  ^  TH  co 

o 

O 

00 

o 

CM 

ft 

< 

*«• 

O  O  O  O  O 
^*  ON  O\  ^  cs 

o 

o 

o 

o 

CO 

Part  IIC 
"Expresses" 

!§ 

10 

O  O  O  O  O 

r-l  iH  T-(  J> 

o 

o 

r- 

0 
CM 

1 

^ 

O  O  O  O  O 
C4  •*  fi  fO  00 

o 

o 

CO 

O 

^n 

ft 

fO 

o  o  o  o  o 

r*  »O  vO       d 

o 

o 
o 

o 
«o 

Part  IIB 
"Curious" 

ti 

- 

O  O  O  O  O 
CN  iH  1-1  M 

o 

s 

o 

1 

o 

OOO  0  0 
ro       00  CX 

o 

o 

en 

o 
^ 

ft 

O 

o  o  o  o  o 

00  O  C\       ^ 

0 

o 

o 

0 

10 

Part  IIA 
"Identifies*1 

fl 

^ 

o  o  o  o  o 

N  ro  CN  1/5 

100  per  cent  in  all  columns 

o 

0 
10 

o 

fO 

c 

0 

£ 

\0 

OOO  0  O 

tO           1H   10 

o 

o 
10 

o 

ft 

»0 

OOOO  0 
00  Tji  00  •*  lO 

§ 

o 

o 

o 

CO 

Part  I 
"Likes  to 
Read" 

1 

CO 

O  r}(  O  O  O 

T-t                N 

o 

VO 
CM 

<# 

1 

r* 

N  O  CM  CM  (N 

cs  *^  <*>  ^ 

<N 

<N 

VD 

VD 
CM 

a 

- 

00  VO  00  N  00 

Ov  M3  00  ^  «O 

0 
fl 

00 
0\ 

CM 
t- 

Column  Number  >- 

<wuft« 

4J 

1 

w 

Maximum  Possible 

Low  Score 

4) 

8 

CO 

1 

w 

1 

a 

260        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

tain"  score  gives  the  proportion  of  items  which  he  was  un- 
able to  answer  with  either  Jes  or  No.  In  addition  to  these 
scores  for  each  of  the  six  categories,  total  "Appreciation," 
total  "Non-appreciation,"  and  total  "Uncertain"  scores  may 
be  computed.  These  total  scores  summarize  the  responses  to 
all  the  100  items  of  the  questionnaire  and  are  analogous  to 
the  "single  score"  given  by  many  tests. 

An  explanation  of  the  scores  made  by  these  five  students 
follows: 

Part  I.  Likes  to  Read 

Columns  Column  1  gives  the  per  cent  of  responses  which  reveal 
1, 2, 3  that  the  student  likes  to  read.  Column  2  gives  the  per 
cent  of  responses  which  reveal  that  he  does  not  like  to 
read.  Column  3  gives  the  per  cent  of  uncertain  re- 
sponses. A  high  score  in  column  1,  Accompanied  by 
low  scores  in  columns  2  and  3,  indicates  that  the  stu- 
dent likes  to  read  to  a  great  extent.  Student  A,  for 
example,  has  such  a  score.  Low  scores  in  columns  1 
and  3,  accompanied  by  a  high  score  in  column  2,  indi- 
cate that  the  student  dislikes  reading.  Among  these 
five  students,  Student  E  has  the  highest  score  in  column 
2;  however,  reference  to  the  line  marked  "High  Score" 
reveals  that  his  score  in  column  2  is  not  the  highest  in 
this  class.  A  high  score  in  column  3,  such  as  that  of 
Student  D,  indicates  that  the  student  was  somewhat 
uncertain  in  answering  the  questions  grouped  under 
this  heading. 

Part  IIA.  Identifies 

Columns  These  scores  indicate  the  extent  to  which  the  student 
5, 6, 7  identifies  himself  with  his  reading.  Among  these  five 
students,  Students  A  and  C  have  relatively  high  "Ap- 
preciation" scores  on  this  category  (column  5)  and 
zero  "Non-appreciation"  scores  (column  6).  Such 
scores  indicate  that  the  student  identifies  himself  with 
his  reading  to  a  considerable  extent.  Student  E  has  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  261 

highest  "Non -appreciation"  score  on  this  category, 
both  among  these  students  and  among  the  class  as  a 
whole.  Student  D  has  a  high  "Uncertain'*  score  (col- 
umn 7). 

Part  IIB.  Curious 

Columns  These  scores  indicate  the  extent  to  which  students  are 
93 10, 11  curious  about  their  reading.  Students  A  and  C  have 
high  "Appreciation"  scores  (column  9)  and  low  "Non- 
appreciation"  scores  (column  10).  This  pattern  indi- 
cates that  these  students  respond  to  their  voluntary 
reading  by  wanting  to  ^know  more  about  authors, 
books,  literary  periods,  etc.  Students  D  and  E  prob- 
ably do  not  respond  in  this  fashion,  for  they  have  low 
scores  in  column  9  and  very  high  scores  in  column  10. 
Column  11  gives  the  per  cent  of  responses  marked 
"Uncertain." 

Part  IIC.  Expresses 

Columns  These  scores  indicate  the  extent  to  which  the  student 
13, 14, 15  expresses  himself  creatively  as  a  response  to  his  read- 
ing. The  highest  "Appreciation"  score  (column  13)  in 
this  class  is  100;  none  of  these  five  students  has  such 
a  high  score  in  column  13;  Students  A  and  C  are  some- 
what above  the  median  of  the  class  (50),  and  Student 
B  is  at  the  median.  Probably  none  of  these  five  stu- 
dents expresses  himself  creatively  to  a  very  great  ex- 
tent. Student  E,  with  his  high  "Non-appreciation" 
score  (column  14),  probably  rarely  engages  in  such 
activities  as  creative  writing,  painting,  dramatizing, 
etc.  Student  D  is  characterized  by  a  very  high  "Uncer- 
tain" score  (column  15). 

Part  IID.  Evaluates 

Columns    These  scores  indicate  the  extent  to  which  the  student 
17, 18, 19   evaluates  or  judges  his  reading.  Students  B  and  C  have 
high  "Appreciation"  scores  (column  17)  and  low  "Non- 
appreciation"  scores  (column  18);  this  pattern  indi- 


262        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

cates  that  they  tend  to  evaluate  their  reading  to  a  very 
great  extent.  Student  A  has  a  low  score  in  column  17, 
as  compared  with  the  median,  and  his  "Uncertain" 
score  (column  19)  is  rather  high.  This  pattern  differs 
considerably  from  the  pattern  of  his  scores  on  the  pre- 
ceding categories,  and  it  suggests  as  an  hypothesis 
that  his  greatest  weakness  may  be  a  failure  to  engage 
in  such  activities  as  reading  reviews  and  criticisms, 
attempting  to  make  judgments  about  what  he  reads, 
etc. 

Part  II.  Total 

Columns  These  three  scores  represent  the  totals  of  the  scores  in 
21, 22, 23  the  four  preceding  categories  and  are  reported  pri- 
marily to  provide  measures  whose  reliabilities  are 
comparable  to  those  of  the  scores  on  Parts  I  and  III. 
For  the  group  of  responses  included  in  Part  II,  student 
C  has  a  relatively  high  "Appreciation"  score  (column 

21)  and  relatively  low  "Non-appreciation"    (column 

22)  and  "Uncertain"  (column  23)  scores.  In  diagnos- 
ing the  specific  differences  between  him  and  Student 
A,  for  example,  it  is  necessary  to  refer  to  the  four  pre- 
ceding categories.  Student  D  has  the  lowest  "Apprecia- 
tion" score  and  the  highest  "Uncertain"  score  on  Part 
II;  Student  E  has  the  highest  "Non-appreciation"  score. 

Part  III.  Relates  to  Life 

Columns  These  scores  indicate  the  extent  to  which  the  student 
25, 26, 27  relates  his  reading  to  his  life  and  to  the  problems  which 
he  recognizes  as  existing.  A  high  "Appreciation"  score 
(column  25),  such  as  that  of  Student  C,  indicates  that 
he  relates  his  reading  to  life,  as  he  knows  it,  to  a  con- 
siderable extent.  Student  E  has  a  high  "Non-apprecia- 
tion" score  (column  26),  in  fact  almost  the  highest  in 
the  class.  Probably  he  does  not  relate  his  reading  to 
life  to  any  great  extent.  Students  A  and  D  have  rather 
high  "Uncertain"  scores  (column  27). 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  263 

Total  Score 

Columns  These  scores  are  convenient  for  making  a  summarizing 
30, 31, 32  judgment  of  a  student's  responses  to  the  test;  however, 
they  necessarily  obscure  some  of  the  differences  among 
students  on  various  categories.  The  "Appreciation" 
score  (column  30)  gives  the  number  of  the  student's 
responses  to  the  one  hundred  items  of  the  test  which 
reveal  these  seven  reactions  to  reading;  the  "Non- 
appreciation"  score  ( column  31 )  gives  the  number  of 
his  responses  which  reveal  that  he  does  not  react  to 
reading  in  these  seven  ways,  and  the  "Uncertain"  score 
(column  32)  gives  the  number  of  his  uncertain  re- 
sponses. 

Several  rather  commonly  occurring  patterns  are  revealed 
by  the  scores  of  these  students.  A  set  of  scores  which  reveals 
that  the  student  responds  to  his  reading  to  a  considerable 
extent  in  these  seven  ways  is  illustrated  by  that  of  Student  C. 
Nearly  all  his  "Appreciation"  scores  are  relatively  high  and 
his  "Non-appreciation"  and  "Uncertain"  scores  relatively  low. 
Almost  the  opposite  pattern  is  revealed  by  the  scores  of  Stu- 
dent E:  relatively  low  "Appreciation"  scores  and  relatively 
high  "Non-appreciation"  scores.  The  relatively  high  "Uncer- 
tain" scores  of  Student  D  reveal  that,  despite  the  instruc- 
tions to  answer  the  questions  with  ~Yes  or  No  if  it  were  at  all 
possible,  he  answered  a  large  number  of  the  questions  with 
Uncertain.  Several  hypotheses  might  be  advanced  to  account 
for  this:  He  may  have  been  quite  indifferent  to  the  test  and 
have  marked  almost  at  random;  he  may  have  been  extremely 
"overcautious"  or  scrupulous  in  attempting  to  answer  the 
questions;  he  may  have  been  unable  to -answer  many  of  these 
questions  because  he  had  failed  previously  to  observe  such 
behaviors  in  himself.  Further  study  of  other  data  about  this 
student  would  be  necessary  to  confirm  or  deny  these  hy- 
potheses and  to  arrive  at  a  satisfactory  interpretation  of  such 


264        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

a  pattern  of  scores.  The  scores  of  Student  A  indicate  a  stu- 
dent who  likes  to  read  very  much  yet  does  not  evaluate  his 
reading  to  any  great  extent.  His  relatively  high  "Uncertain" 
scores  on  Part  IID  and  Part  III  should  be  used  as  a  starting 
point  for  hypotheses  as  to  why  he  responded  in  this  fashion 
only  to  these  two  categories. 

Other  Instruments 

Two  questionnaires,  similar  in  structure  to  the  Question- 
naire on  Voluntary  Reading,  were  developed  for  the  purpose 
of  measuring  students'  responses  to  a  particular  novel  or  a 
particular  drama  which  they  have  read.  The  Novel  Question- 
naire (Test  3.22)  includes  65  items,  the  responses  to  which 
are  summarized  under  the  same  six  categories  as  are  the  re- 
sponses to  Form  3.32.  Similar  scores  are  computed  for  each 
of  the  six  categories,  and  for  the  total  of  65  items.  The  Drama 
Questionnaire  (Test  3.21)  includes  80  questions,  the  re- 
sponses to  which  are  summarized  under  the  six  headings 
mentioned  above  plus  an  additional  heading:  "Feels  that  he 
understands  the  play."  This  category  was  added  to  the  Drama 
Questionnaire  in  order  to  aid  in  the  interpretation  of  scores 
on  the  six  categories.  It  was  believed  that  the  extent  to  which 
a  student  feels  that  he  understands  the  play  he  has  read  may 
demand  differing  interpretations  of  his  other  responses.  For 
example,  a  pattern  of  scores  which  indicates  that  a  student 
derived  no  satisfaction  from  reading  the  play  yet  felt  that  he 
understood  it  perfectly  probably  would  demand  a  different 
interpretation  from  one  which  indicates  that  the  student  de- 
rived no  satisfaction  from  reading  the  play  and  felt  that  he 
did  not  understand  it.  A  similar  category  has  not  been  added 
to  the  Novel  Questionnaire;  it  is  possible  that  teachers  using 
the  Novel  Questionnaire  would  find  such  an  addition  helpful. 

Each  of  the  three  questionnaires  described  includes,  as 
has  been  indicated,  a  set  of  items  the  responses  to  which  are 
summarized  under  the  heading,  "Evaluates  his  reading."  The 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  265 

purpose  of  this  category  is  to  discover  to  what  extent  stu- 
dents actually  engage  in  such  activities  as  comparing  the 
merits  of  one  book  with  those  of  another,  discovering  what 
critics  have  said  about  books  they  have  read,  comparing 
their  judgments  of  books  with  those  made  by  others,  etc. 
Scores  on  this  category  obviously  do  not  furnish  information 
about  the  quality  of  the  judgments  which  the  student  makes 
of  books,  just  as  scores  on  the  category  "Likes  to  read"  do 
not  furnish  information  about  the  quality  of  the  books  which 
he  actually  reads.  Because  a  number  of  teachers  wished  to 
have  some  objective  means  of  appraising  the  quality  of  stu- 
dents' judgments,  this  evaluation  problem  was  explored. 
Three  experimental  instruments  were  developed;  these  are: 
An  Interpretation  of  Literature  (Test  3.1),  Critical-Minded- 
ness  in  the  Reading  of  Fiction  (Test  3.7),  Judging  the 
Effectiveness  of  Written  Composition  (Test  3.8).  Because 
these  instruments  have  not  been  used  extensively  or  studied 
sufficiently,  they  are  not  as  yet  to  be  recommended  for  wide- 
spread use.  However,  they  might  serve  as  useful  classroom 
exercises  and  they  might  suggest  techniques  for  appraising 
students'  judgments  which  others  would  want  to  utilize. 

These  three  tests  use  short  stories  as  their  content  or 
subject-matter.  In  brief,  they  were  constructed  by  first  ask- 
ing a  group  of  students  to  write  out  any  judgments  of  the 
story  which  they  could  or  would  care  to  make.  After  these 
judgments  had  been  sorted  and  the  duplicating  ones  dis- 
carded, they  were  submitted  to  a  jury  of  teachers.  The  jury 
grouped  them  and  marked  each  as  a  "good"  or  a  "poor"  judg- 
ment. The  test  was  then  made  up,  including  the  story  and 
the  list  of  students7  judgments,  and  those  who  took  the  test 
were  directed  to  read  the  story  and  respond  to  each  of  the 
judgments  listed  by  agreeing  with  it,  disagreeing  with  it,  or 
stating  that  they  could  neither  agree  nor  disagree.  The  eval- 
uation of  each  judgment  made  by  the  jury  is  used  as  a  test 
key.  Scores  are  given  in  terms  of  the  extent  to  which  the 


266        ADVENTURE  IX  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

student  evaluated  these  judgments  as  did  the  jury.  It  should 
be  pointed  out  that  this  is  only  one  method  of  scoring  re- 
sponses on  such  a  test.  Other  methods  might  be  devised 
which  would  better  suit  the  purposes  of  particular  schools  or 
teachers. 

Test  3.1,  An  Interpretation  of  Literature,  is  based  on 

0.  Henry's  story  "A  Municipal  Report."  The  student  is  asked, 
after  reading  the  story,  to  respond  to  statements  which  are 
grouped  under  such  headings  as: 

1.  What  is  your  interpretation  of  the  story? 

2.  What  was  O.  Henry's  point  of  view? 
8.  What  was  O.  Henry's  philosophy? 

4.  What  was  the  character's  motive? 

5.  Which  is  the  most  logical  ending  for  the  story? 

Scores  for  each  of  these  parts  may  be  computed. 

Test  3.7,  Critical-Mindedness  in  the  Reading  of  Fiction, 
makes  use  of  two  short-short  stories  reprinted  from  a  popu- 
lar magazine.  The  statements  which  follow  each  of  these 
stories  deal  with  the  extent  to  which  the  actions  and  speech 
of  these  characters,  the  description  given  by  the  authors,  the 
outcomes  of  the  stories,  etc.,  are  "true  to  life."  For  example, 
these  statements  follow  the  story  "First  Acquaintance"  by 

1.  A.  R.  Wylie: 

1.  The  general  atmosphere — the  smells,  the  signs  on  the 
door,  the  moving  nurses,  etc. — is  depicted  accurately  in 
this  story. 

2.  It  seems  scarcely  likely  that  a  young  man  would  wonder 
about  the  "No  visitors"  sign,  the  oxygen  tank,  and  the  sick 
mother  and  daughter  as  the  youth  in  this  story  did. 

3.  Under  the  circumstances  it  seems  natural  for  the  youth  to 
say  "Gosh"  and  "That's  tough"  several  times. 

4.  No  nurse,  even  a  young  one,  would  volunteer  as  much 
information  about  patients  to  a  stranger  as  the  nurse  in 
this  story  does. 

5.  The  youth's  sudden  realization  of  what  death  means  and 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  267 

his  thoughts  about  his  own  mother  seem  real  and  natural. 

6.  The  suggestion  that  the  youth  was  crying  when  he  left 
the  hospital  Is  difficult  to  believe. 

7.  The  emphasis  upon  the  fact  that  the  mother  and  daughter 
were  alone  in  the  world  seems  exaggerated  and  over  done. 

OC3 

8.  Under  the  circumstances  it  seems  natural  for  the  young 
man,  on  his  return  to  the  hospital  the  next  morning,  to  be 
more  concerned  to  find  out  about  the  condition  of  the 
sick  girl's  mother  than  of  that  of  his  sister. 

9.  The  action  of  the  young  man  in  going  Into  the  girl's  room 
to  tell  her  that  she  had  not  been  left  completely  alone  is  in 
accordance  with  what  the  reader  has  previously  found  out 
about  his  character. 

10.  The  sick  girl's  response  to  his  sympathy  does  not  seem 
true  to  life. 

Four  scores  are  given  on  this  test:  (1)  "Judicious/*  i.e.,  the 
extent  to  which  the  student's  responses  agree  with  the  jury's 
judgment;  (2)  "Hypercritical/7  i.e.,  the  extent  to  which  the 
student  judges  situations  which  the  jury  believes  are  true  to 
life  to  be  not  true  to  life;  (3)  "Uncritical/'  i.e.,  the  extent  to 
which  the  student  judges  situations  which  the  jury  believes 
are  not  true  to  life  to  be  true  to  life;  (4)  "Uncertain/*  i.e., 
the  extent  to  which  the  student  was  unable  to  agree  or  dis- 
agree with  these  statements. 

Test  3.8,  Judging  the  Effectiveness  of  Written  Composi- 
tion, makes  use  of  a  short-short  story  written  by  a  high 
school  student.  This  story  is  followed  by  28  statements  about 
the  narrative  quality,  the  style,  the  characterization,  etc.,  of 
this  story.  For  example,  these  statements  are  included: 

1.  The  writer  should  not  have  included  so  many  different 
episodes  in  one  brief  story. 

8.  The  writer  shows  considerable  skill  in  depicting  the  hu- 
morous aspects  of  situations. 

4.  The  dialog  in  the  story  is,  in  general,  handled  ably. 

5.  Esmond's  stammering,  hesitant  way  of  speaking  in  trying 


268        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

situations  helps  the  reader  to  see  him  as  an  individualized 
character. 

6.  The  concluding  episode  provides  a  very  effective  climax 
for  the  story. 

7,  Esmond  is  a  good  name  for  the  chief  character  in  the 
story. 

This  test  is  also  scored  by  comparing  the  student's  responses 
with  those  provided  by  a  jury  of  adults. 

Validity  of  the  Questionnaires 

In  order  to  assess  the  value  of  the  instruments  designed  to 
measure  students'  responses  to  their  reading  it  will  be  neces- 
sary to  consider  their  validity,  their  reliability,  and  the  uses 
which  classroom  teachers  may  make  of  them.  It  was  pointed 
out  earlier  that  the  validity  of  the  questionnaire  technique 
for  measuring  students'  responses  to  their  reading  is  pri- 
marily dependent  upon  the  extent  to  which  three  major  as- 
sumptions are  fulfilled;  it  was  also  pointed  out  that  whether 
or  not  these  assumptions  are  fulfilled  will  depend  upon  both 
the  nature  of  the  instrument  and  the  conditions  under  which 
it  is  administered.  The  construction  of  one  of  the  question- 
naires has  been  described  in  some  detail  in  order  to  illustrate 
how  certain  criteria  which  were  demanded  by  these  three 
assumptions  were  applied.  If  these  criteria  are  judged  to  be 
adequate  and  the  items  of  the  questionnaire  meet  the  cri- 
teria, then  the  instrument  is  one  which  is  so  constructed  as 
to  make  possible  the  collection  of  valid  evidence  of  the  seven 
types  of  response  to  reading. 

Valid  evidence  of  these  types  of  response,  however,  may 
not  be  given  by  the  questionnaire  even  though  its  construc- 
tion is  judged  to  be  satisfactory.  Obviously,  if  such  an  instru- 
ment as  Form  3.32  were  administered  as  a  "final  examina- 
tion" and  the  students  informed  that  their  grades  or  credits 
would  be  determined  by  their  scores,  we  would  not  expect 
it  to  yield  valid  evidence  of  those  students'  responses  to  their 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  269 

voluntary  reading.  The  conditions  which  should  attend  the 
administration  of  one  of  these  questionnaires  are  as  follows: 
First,  the  teacher  should  understand  the  kinds  of  evidence 
the  questionnaire  is  designed  to  give  and  should  desire  to 
secure  this  evidence.  Second,  the  teacher  should  have  a  cur- 
riculum program  which  might  be  expected  to  bring  about 
the  development  of  the  seven  types  of  response.  Third,  the 
teacher  should  have  developed  a  rapport  with  the  students 
which  will  enable  and  encourage  them  to  respond  honestly 
to  the  questions.  Fourth,  the  students  should  understand  and 
accept  the  purpose  of  the  administration  of  the  questionnaire 
and  the  uses  which  are  to  be  made  of  the  results.  This  is 
merely  to  say  that  an  evaluation  instrument  must  be  under- 
stood, must  be  relevant  to  the  objectives  and  the  curriculum, 
and  must  be  accepted  by  the  students  as  an  opportunity  to 
appraise  themselves,  if  its  use  is  to  be  of  greatest  value. 

The  assumption  that  students  will  respond  honestly  is  a 
crucial  one  in  these  questionnaires,  and  unless  it  is  fulfilled 
we  cannot  hope  for  valid  evidence.  In  the  construction  of  the 
questionnaire  an  attempt  was  made  to  select  items  which 
would  not  tempt  students  to  be  dishonest  in  their  responses, 
and  the  directions  were  so  phrased  as  to  emphasize  the  de- 
sirability of  answering  as  frankly  and  as  honestly  as  possible. 
These  were  efforts  to  aid  in  securing  honest  responses.  How- 
ever, these  efforts  cannot  be  expected  to  make  certain  that 
the  assumption  will  be  fulfilled.  The  degree  of  rapport  be- 
tween teacher  and  students,  students'  previous  experiences 
with  "tests"  and  with  the  uses  of  test  results,  and  students7 
concepts  of  the  purposes  of  education  and  of  the  place  of 
evaluation  in  education  may  determine  to  what  extent  the 
responses  will  be  honest  ones. 

The  questionnaire  technique  which  is  used  in  these  instru- 
ments differs  from  the  method  of  direct  observation  of  stu- 
dents by  a  teacher  only  in  that  the  student  is  both  subject 
and  observer  rather  than  being  merely  the  subject.  One 


270        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

method,  then,  of  checking  the  honesty  of  a  student's  re- 
sponses to  the  questionnaire  would  be  to  compare  his  re- 
sponses with  observations  made  by  one  or* more  adults  of 
what  he  actually  does  and  says.  It  should  be  possible  for  one 
familiar  with  the  overt  acts  and  verbal  responses  included  in 
the  questionnaire  to  compare  his  observations  of  some  of 
these  behaviors  with  student's  responses.  For  example,  a 
teacher  might  provide  periods  for  "free-reading"  and  during 
those  periods  determine  to  what  extent  the  student  welcomes 
interruptions  of  his  reading,  reads  various  types  of  fiction 
and  nonfiction,  reads  attentively,  etc.  Also,  in  conversation 
with  a  student,  a  teacher  could  secure  evidence  which  would 
help  her  judge  to  what  extent  certain  wishes  and  feelings 
expressed  in  his  responses  to  the  questionnaire  were  genuine. 
This  is  one  method  of  validating  responses  to  the  question- 
naire. 

A  somewhat  different  method  which  might  be  used  would 
be  to  interview  a  student  about  his  reading  behaviors  and  in 
addition  to  asking  him  what  he  does,  ask  him  for  illustrations 
or  examples  of  these  behaviors.  For  example,  a  teacher  who 
wished  to  know  whether  or  not  a  student  reads  book  reviews 
in  current  publications  rather  regularly  probably  could  dis- 
cover this  without  attempting  to  observe  such  reading  di- 
rectly. By  asking  him  whether  or  not  he  ever  read  book 
reviews  and,  if  his  reply  were  yes,  following  this  by  asking 
in  what  publications  he  read  them  and  what  reviews  he  had 
read  recently,  and  by  giving  him  an  opportunity  to  discuss 
some  of  these  reviews,  she  could  be  reasonably  certain  of 
whether  or  not  he  actually  did  such  reading.  Such  a  pro- 
cedure, of  course,  need  not  be  an  inquisition  nor  need  it 
result  in  only  an  answer  to  the  teacher's  question.  Reading 
guidance  might  be  given  as  well  as  reading  behaviors  ap- 
praised in  the  same  conversation. 

Recognition  of  this  method  as  a  means  of  achieving  rea- 
sonable certainty  about  what  students  actually  do  and  say 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  271 

leads  to  the  possibility  of  constructing  a  paper  and  pencil 
instrument  which  would  achieve  a  similar  result.  The  stu- 
dent might  be  asked  to  respond  on  paper  to  questions  about 
his  reading  behavior  and  then  write  out  an  illustration  or  an 
example  of  each  behavior.  The  nature  of  the  illustration  or 
example  presumably  would  be  evidence  which  would  tend 
to  substantiate  or  refute  his  contention  that  he  engaged  in 
such  behaviors.  Let  us  for  convenience  call  this  a  "direct 
form"  of  the  questionnaire.  The  first  page  of  such  a  direct 
form  is  reprinted  below. 

Name Age Sex 

Grade Instructor 


This  is  not  a  "test"  but  an  attempt  to  discover  more  about  your 
reading  interests.  Obviously,  no  two  persons  have  exactly  the 
same  reading  interests;  consequently  there  are  no  "right"  or 
"wrong"  answers.,  as  such,  to  these  questions. 

Please  answer  each  question  as  carefully  and  as  honestly  as  you 
can.  Mark  your  answer  to  each  question  by  checking  the  space 
under  Yes,  No,  or  Uncertain  at  the  right  of  the  sheet.  If  your 
answer  to  a  question  is  "Yes,  please  give  the  additional  information 
asked  for  in  the  question.  If  your  answer  is  No  or  Uncertain,  go 
on  to  the  next  question. 

Uncer- 
Yes        No       tain 

1.  Do  you  have  in  mind  one  or  two  books 

which  you  would  like  to  read? 

If  you  do,  please  give  the  author  and 
title  of  one: 

2.  Do  you  ever  read  adventure  novels  in 
your  spare  time? 

If  you  do,  please  give  the  author  and 
title  of  one  which  you  have  read: 

3.  Do  you  ever  read   essays,   apart  from 
school  requirements? 


272        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

If  you  do,  please  give  the  author  and 
titie  of  one  which  you  have  read: 

4.  Is  there  any  author  whom  you  like  so 
well  that  you  would  like  to  read  any  new 
book  he  might  write? 

If  there  is,  please  give  his  name  and  the 
title  of  one  of  his  books  which  you  have 
read: 

5.  Do  you  ever  of  your  own  accord  read 
humorous  stories  or  books  of  satire? .... 
If  you  do,  please  give  the  author  and 
title  of  one  which  you  have  read: 

6.  Do  you  ever  read  biography,  apart  from 

school  requirements? 

If  you  do,  please  give  the  author  and 
title  of  one  which  you  have  read: 

Such  "direct  forms"  of  the  questionnaire  have  been  used 
in  studying  the  functioning  of  the  Questionnaire  on  Volun- 
tary Reading.  The  methods  and  the  results  of  these  studies 
will  be  reported  in  full  in  a  forthcoming  monograph.  In  brief, 
we  find,  for  some  classes,  a  relatively  high  relationship  be- 
tween responses  on  the  Questionnaire  on  Voluntary  Reading 
and  on  a  direct  form.  These  relationships,  expressed  as 
product-moment  correlation  coefficients,  range  from  .38  to 
.79.s  Other  types  of  studies  which  make  use  of  interview 
techniques  and  of  comparison  of  teachers'  ratings  of  students 
with  test  scores  will  also  be  reported  in  the  monograph. 
Similar  studies  of  students'  responses  to  the  Novel  and  Drama 
Questionnaires  have  not  been  made;  the  presumption  would 

8  Fourteen  such  coefficients  derived  from  a  study  of  Form  3.32  are  dis- 
tributed as  follows:  .35  to  .40,  one;  .45  to  .50,  one;  .60  to  .65,  two;  .65 
to  .70,  three;  .70  to  .75,  three;  .75  to  .80,  four.  The  median  of  this  distribu- 
tion is  .695. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  273 

be,  since  the  basic  technique  is  similar  to  that  of  Form  3.32, 
that  such  studies  would  yield  results  much  like  these.  Tests 
3.1,  3.7,  and  3.8  were  described  as  experimental  instruments 
and  the  fact  that  they  have  not  been  studied  has  been  men- 
tioned. 

Uses  of  the  Instruments 

Two  major  uses  of  the  instruments  described  in  this  section 
may  be  pointed  out:  (1)  To  provide  information  about 
students  which  will  aid  in  planning  the  school  program  and 
in  guiding  students;  (2)  To  provide  evidence  on  which  can 
be  based  an  appraisal  of  the  progress  of  students  and  of  the 
effectiveness  of  the  school  program.  Before  instruments  such 
as  the  questionnaires  described  here  are  used,  however,  it  is 
important  for  the  teacher  to  examine  the  instruments  care- 
fully and  to  satisfy  herself  that  they  deal  with  behaviors 
which  she  regards  as  important.  When  such  instruments  are 
used,  it  is  also  important  to  recognize  the  limitations  in- 
herent in  them  and  to  supplement  the  evidence  given  by 
them  with  evidence  gained  from  classroom  observation  and 
from  other  instruments.  In  interpreting  scores  on  these  in- 
struments, it  is  important  to  consider  the  reliability  data 
which  are  furnished  in  the  Appendix  and  to  use  caution  in 
making  judgments  based  on  differences  in  scores,  either  be- 
tween individuals  or  groups. 

The  kinds  of  information  given  by  these  instruments  have 
been  described  above.  Such  information  as  that  given  by  the 
Questionnaire  on  Voluntary  Reading  should  be  of  use  to  a 
teacher  early  in  the  school  year  to  aid  her  in  becoming  ac- 
quainted with  some  of  the  reading  behaviors  of  her  students. 
For  example,  a  teacher  might  profitably  make  use  of  the  in- 
formation that  certain  students  or  certain  groups  of  students 
make  very  low  "Appreciation"  scores  on  the  category  "Likes 
to  read."  Assuming  that  a  favorable  attitude  toward  the 
reading  of  books  is  of  some  importance,  either  as  an  end  in 


274         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

itself  or  as  a  means  to  other  ends,  the  teacher  might  plan 
special  classroom  experiences  which  would  help  these  stu- 
dents to  overcome  the  unfavorable  attitude  and  to  develop 
a  favorable  attitude  toward  books.  In  planning  these  experi- 
ences the  question  of  why  these  students  do  not  seem  to  like 
to  read  would  necessarily  be  raised.  In  order  to  answer  this 
question  a  number  of  hypotheses  would  have  to  be  explored. 
Here  the  teacher  would  want  to  make  use  of  evidence  from 
other  tests,  such  as  tests  of  reading  comprehension,  from 
classroom  observations  made  by  other  teachers,  and  from  the 
school  and  home  records  of  these  students. 

Such  exploration  of  hypotheses  might  lead  the  teacher  to 
give  special  attention  to  the  reading  behaviors  of  certain  stu- 
dents as  well  as  of  the  class  as  a  whole.  In  planning  reading 
experiences  for  individual  students  she  also  might  find  scores 
on  the  questionnaire  helpful.  For  example,  discovery  of  a 
student  with  a  high  "Appreciation"  score  on  the  category 
"Likes  to  read"  but  with  relatively  low  "Appreciation"  scores 
on  the  other  categories  might  prompt  the  teacher  to  help  the 
student  discover  and  participate  in  such  reactions  as  evaluat- 
ing reading  or  relating  it  to  life.  Teachers  have  found  that 
a  conference  early  in  the  year  with  individual  students  which 
begins  with  the  consideration  of  test  scores  may  lead  to  an 
enthusiastic  planning  of  individual  programs  of  reading  and 
other  activities  by  the  students  themselves.  In  such  confer- 
ences, of  course,  test  scores  should  not  be  regarded  as  "marks'* 
or  judgments  but  instead  as  evidence  which  should  be  con- 
sidered in  planning  the  work  of  the  year. 

The  second  use  is  that  of  providing  evidence  on  which 
appraisals  may  be  based.  Evidence  of  change  from  year  to 
year  in  the  status  of  individual  students  in  their  reactions  to 
voluntary  reading  should  be  given  by  such  an  instrument  as 
the  Questionnaire  on  Voluntary  Reading.  This  evidence 
should  be  useful  to  the  student  who  wishes  to  make  an  ap- 
praisal of  his  achievement,  to  parents  who  wish  to  appraise 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  275 

the  progress  of  their  children  toward  goals  such  as  develop- 
ing a  favorable  attitude  toward  voluntary  reading,  and  to 
teachers  who  wish  to  appraise  the  success  of  their  guidance 
and  instruction  in  aiding  students  to  cultivate  some  of  these 
responses  to  reading.  The  appraisal  of  their  own  achievement 
by  students  is  probably  a  necessary  concomitant  in  any  plan 
of  promoting  student  as  well  as  teacher  planning  of  the  edu- 
cational program.  Such  appraisal,  in  turn,  should  stimulate 
further  planning  by  both  teacher  and  student.  When  the  in- 
terest of  parents  in  the  success  of  their  children  demands 
more  than  a  summarizing  mark,  a  description  of  change  in 
status  as  revealed  by  test  scores  should  provide  useful  evi- 
dence to  supplement  anecdotal  records  or  comments  of  the 
teacher.  It  is  important,  of  course,  for  those  who  interpret 
these  scores  to  others  to  make  sure  that  changes  in  test  scores 
are  not  mere  chance  fluctuations,  but  are  "significant''  dif- 
ferences, before  interpreting  them  as  such. 

The  role  of  other  instruments  in  aiding  the  teacher  in  plan- 
ning or  in  appraising  her  program  should  not  be  overlooked. 
Let  us  recall  the  three  questions  which  members  of  the 
Committee  on  the  Evaluation  of  Reading  wished  to  be  able 
to  answer;  namely,  (1)  How  well  does  the  student  read? 
(2)  What  does  the  student  read?  and  (3)  How  does  the 
student  react  to  his  reading?  An  answer  to  the  first  question 
may  be  needed  to  help  explain  why  a  student  does  not  read, 
of  his  own  accord,  or  does  not  like  to  read.  An  answer  to  the 
second  question  may  be  needed  to  help  explain  why  a  stu- 
dent does  not  relate  his  reading  to  life.  Thus  in  establishing 
hypotheses  about  the  causes  of  certain  students'  difficulties 
in  responding  to  reading  it  may  be  necessary  to  make  use  of 
several  instruments  which  were  designed  to  measure  some- 
what different  behaviors.  On  the  basis  of  such  hypotheses, 
educational  programs  which  are  relevant  to  the  particular 
needs  of  the  student  or  group  of  students  may  be  planned. 
In  appraising  the  program  it  may  be  desirable  to  make  use 


276        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

of  several  instruments  again  in  order  to  determine  to  what 
extent  each  of  these  behaviors  has  been  modified.  Conse- 
quently the  use  of  such  an  instrument  as  the  Questionnaire 
on  Voluntary  Reading  may  not  be  a  sufficient  evaluation 
procedure  in  itself.  Those  who  wish  to  develop  a  more  com- 
prehensive plan  of  evaluation  of  reading  behaviors  should 
find  the  description  of  the  instruments  designed  to  help  de- 
termine how  a  student  reads  and  what  he  reads  pertinent  to 
their  needs.  These  descriptions  appear  on  pages  319  to 
337. 

THE  EVALUATION  OF  THE  APPRECIATION  OF  ART 

The  Committee  on  Evaluation  in  the  Arts,  composed  of 
art  teachers  in  the  schools  of  the  Eight- Year  Study,  listed  as 
purposes  of  art  teaching  the  following:  (1)  objectives  per- 
taining to  the  development  of  sensitivity  to  art  values,  com- 
monly called  appreciation;  (2)  objectives  related  to  the 
development  of  the  ability  to  express  certain  types  of  experi- 
ences creatively;  and  (3)  objectives  related  to  emotional 
adjustment  resulting  from  the  release  afforded  by  creative 
experience. 

The  evaluation  of  the  first  of  these  objectives — the  devel- 
opment of  sensitivity  to  art  values — is  the  one  with  which 
the  staff  has  been  primarily  concerned.  Emotional  adjust- 
ment can  be  fostered  by  means  of  well  directed  creative 
experience  in  the  arts  but  the  question  of  which  are  the 
particular  types  of  emotional  problems  that  can  be  solved, 
as  well  as  the  question  of  which  kinds  of  creative  experience 
offer  a  remedy  for  a  particular  emotional  problem,  is  as  yet 
not  definitely  answered.9  So  it  was  felt  that  the  primary  con- 
sideration was  the  evaluation  of  sensitivity  to  art  values  and, 
although  some  attention  was  devoted  to  the  emotional  con- 
notations, the  results  are  not  as  yet  sufficiently  established  to 

9  The  more  important  literature  concerning  this  problem  is  cited  in  Levey, 
Harry,  "A  Theory  Concerning  Free  Creation  in  the.  Inventive  Arts,"  Psychi- 
atry, III  (May,  1940),  p.  229  ff. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  277 

warrant  extensive  discussion.  Furthermore,  the  area  of  per- 
sonal and  social  adjustment  was  being  explored  separately 
( cf .  Chapter  VI ) ;  consequently  only  casual  remarks  on  this 
aspect  of  the  objective  will  be  made  in  the  following  pages. 
.The  problem  of  evaluating  sensitivity  to  art  values  was 
further  narrowed  to  include  only  the  field  of  the  visual  arts. 
Here  again  it  seemed  unnecessary  to  duplicate  work  done  in 
other  areas.  The  evaluation  of  the  appreciation  of  literature 
is  discussed  in  the  preceding  section;  other  instruments  of 
evaluation  of  appreciation  in  the  field  of  the  arts  will  be  dis- 
cussed on  page  307.  Thus  the  task  became  one  of  developing 
evaluation  instruments  which  would  appraise  the  students* 
sensitivity  to  art  values  in  the  field  of  the  visual  arts. 

Ways  of  Getting  Evidence  and  Exploration  of 
Possible  Criteria  for  a  New  Instrument 

The  first  step  in  the  study  of  the  problem  was  to  survey 
currently  used  methods  of  getting  evidence  regarding  art 
experiences  and  art  appreciation  of  students.  Some  of  the 
methods  which  have  been  used  to  discover  the  development 
of  the  subject's  knowledge  regarding  art — his  intellectual 
understanding  of  art — include  art  questionnaires,  art  vocab- 
ulary tests,  and  similar  instruments.  These  tests  have  at- 
tempted to  appraise  primarily  the  extent  to  which  the  student 
is  familiar  with  art  history  and  art  techniques.  Other  tests 
have  attempted  to  obtain  an  appraisal  of  the  extent  to  which 
the  student  is  able  to  apply  certain  rules  of  color-combination, 
balance,  etc.,  in  dealing  with  art  objects.  The  success  of  the 
student  on  all  of  these  tests  seems  to  be  chiefly  dependent 
upon  the  extent  to  which  he  has  mastered  a  body  of  factual 
knowledge  which  may  be  helpful  in  bringing  about  an 
esthetic  experience. 

Another  approach  to  evaluation  in  the  arts  is  through  tests 
which  attempt  to  measure  the  extent  of  the  subject's  interest 
in  art  and  to  discover  in  which  sub-fields  he  has  a  special 


278        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

interest.  Still  another  method  of  gathering  evidence  regard- 
ing art  experience  has  been  to  rely  on  a  student's  opinion 
about  these  experiences.  His  opinions  may  be  stated  in  essay 
form  or  they  may  be  expressed  as  responses  to  a  checklist. 
More  informal  methods  frequently  employed  by  teachers  in- 
clude anecdotal  records  about  student  behavior,  collections, 
descriptions,  or  photographs  of  creative  work,  and  checklists 
filled  out  by  teachers.  The  advantages  and  disadvantages  of 
all  these  methods  were  reviewed  in  an  attempt  to  set  up 
criteria  for  an  instrument  designed  to  appraise  responses  to 
art  values. 

First  of  all,  it  was  thought  that  tests  of  intellectual  under- 
standing, of  mastery  of  specific  areas  of  information,  while 
useful  where  information  is  a  part  of  the  objective,  would  not 
necessarily  contribute  to  an  appraisal  of  the  art  sensitivity  of 
the  subject,  It  was  recognized  that  a  student  may  be  sensitive 
to  art  values  even  though  he  has  not  mastered  a  body  of 
specific  information  or  rules.  The  converse  seems  also  to  be 
true;  that  is,  a  student  may  be  familiar  with  the  meaning  of 
technical  terms,  die  facts  of  art  history,  and  so  on,  without 
being  responsive  to  artistic  values.  It  seemed  desirable, 
therefore,  that  an  instrument  of  appraisal  should  be  so  con- 
structed that  it  would  depend  as  little  as  possible  upon  the 
student's  previously  amassed  information  regarding  art.  The 
fact  that  it  would  be  extremely  difficult  to  eliminate  this 
element  entirely  was  also  recognized. 

Even  though  written  statements  about  art  experiences 
have  the  advantage  of  being  highly  personal  and,  therefore, 
may  give  insight  into  the  nature  of  the  individual's  reaction, 
they  too  have  one  important  disadvantage — they  are  fre- 
quently unfair  to  the  student  who  is  relatively  lacking  in  the 
ability  to  state  his  reaction  in  words.  It  should  be  recognized 
that  not  all  students  who  are  capable  of  genuine  and  deep 
art  experience  have  correspondingly  well  developed  verbal 
abilities.  It  is  very  likely,  for  instance,  that  some  students 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  279 

who  have  very  little  verbal  facility  find  a  means  of  expression 
in  art.10  Finally,  there  seem  to  be  certain  immediately  visible 
qualities  in  an  art  object  which  are  extremely  difficult  to 
translate  into  words,  even  for  the  verbally  gifted  person. 
Painting  and  prose  are  seldom  mutually  interchangeable  as  a 
means  of  expression.  For  these  reasons  it  was  thought  desir- 
able to  have  the  instrument  depend  as  little  as  possible  upon 
verbal  expression  of  subjective  reactions.  Since  it  was  recog- 
nized that  it  would  not  be  possible  to  eliminate  the  verbal 
element  entirely,  the  aim  W7as  to  reduce  it  to  a  minimum. 

Records  of  behavior,  anecdotal  records,  and  collections  of 
creative  work,  wrhereas  they  have  the  advantage  of  yielding 
evidence  about  the  personal  art  experience  of  the  individual, 
also  have  disadvantages.  For  instance,  they  do  not  provide  a 
uniform  basis  for  comparisons  between  students;  also  they 
apply  only  to  the  students  who  are  productive  in  the  studio; 
they  fail  if  a  student  does  not  attend  art  classes. 

In  summary  it  might  be  said  that  there  seemed  to  be  a 
need  for  a  new  instrument  which,  as  far  as  possible,  would 
be  constructed  in  such  a  way  as  to  satisfy  the  following  cri- 
teria: (1)  that  the  results  should  not  depend  primarily  upon 
a  body  of  factual  knowledge;  (2)  that  the  results  should  not 
depend  upon  the  ability  to  express  art  experience  verbally; 
(3)  that  the  responses  should  permit  a  comparison  of  differ- 
ent students  on  a  uniform  basis;  and  (4)  that  the  instrument 
should  permit  the  evaluation  of  the  responses  both  of  stu- 
dents who  are  known  to  be  artistically  creative  and  of  those 
who  have  not  as  yet  exhibited  such  talents. 

It  was  thought  further  that  the  instrument  should  attempt 
to  get  at  the  person's  reaction  to  a  work  of  art  as  a  unit  or  as 
a  whole,  rather  than  at  reactions  to  specific,  separate  ele- 
ments of  an  object  of  art.  It  is  doubtful  whether  one  can  get 

10  Moreover,  it  seems  as  if  adolescents  especially  are  reluctant  to  state 
their  problems  openly  and  verbally.  To  them  the  less  obvious  way  of  ex- 
pression by  means  of  creation  and  participation  in  the  arts  is  one  of  the 
main  ways  of  dealing  with  these  problems. 


280        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

a  valid  indication  of  the  capacity  for  esthetic  experience 
evoked  by  an  art  object  and  what  this  object  conveys,  by 
asking  a  person  to  react  separately  to  line,  spatial  arrange- 
ment, or  color.  Although  this  seems  true  for  the  evaluation 
of  the  esthetic  experience  as  a  whole,  for  the  evaluation  of 
certain  aspects  of  esthetic  capability  a  person's  response  to 
certain  specifics  of  an  art  object  is  also  needed.  This  is  par- 
ticularly true  if  die  teacher  wants  to  know  at  what  particular 
stage  of  development  the  student's  reactions  to  certain  known 
features  of  art  may  be.  Two  additional  criteria,  then,  seemed 
necessary.  First,  the  instrument  should  allow  the  student  to 
react  to  the  art  object  in  an  esthetic  way  and  permit  a  re- 
sponse to  the  work  of  art  as  a  whole;  that  is,  to  have  as  com- 
plete an  art  experience  as  possible.  Second,  the  instrument 
should  contain  a  variety  of  elements  and  evoke  specific  re- 
sponses so  that  the  examination  of  these  reactions  of  the 
student  would  permit  an  evaluation  of  his  esthetic  develop- 
ment with  reference  to  these  known  elements. 

Some  Remarks  on  the  Psychology  of  Art  Appreciation 

Before  discussing  in  detail  the  specific  assumptions  under- 
lying the  development  of  the  instrument,  some  further  re- 
marks concerning  "art  appreciation"  should  be  made.  Un- 
fortunately the  connotations  of  this  term  vary  in  different 
contexts  and  no  definition  is  generally  accepted.  Sometimes 
the  term  is  used  in  a  rather  narrow  sense,  covering  only  a 
passive  act  on  the  part  of  the  beholder  who  in  this  context 
is  compared  with  a  piece  of  wax  that  bears  the  impression  of 
a  seal.  A  recent  theory  recognizes  a  great  deal  more  activity 
on  the  part  of  the  beholder  who  is  supposed  in  the  act  of 
"empathy"  to  neglect  his  own  personality  and  to  live  in  the 
world  of  the  work  of  art  for  the  span  of  time  during  which  he 
is  in  "empathy."  A  still  more  recent  theory  is  that  offered  by 
"Gestalt"  psychology.  In  dealing  with  these  problems,  from 
the  point  of  view  of  this  psychology,  art  appreciation  is  con- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  281 

sidered  as  a  field  phenomenon/1  the  field  consisting  of  the 
beholder  and  the  work  of  art.  The  act  of  art  experience  can 
take  place — the  field  can  be  established — only  if  the  spec- 
tator is  willing  to  undergo  the  art  experience.  This  willing- 
ness is  a  deliberate  act  on  the  part  of  the  spectator,  and  art 
appreciation  becomes  an  active  rather  than  a  passive  reac- 
tion. In  this  connection  it  may  be  mentioned  that  for  other 
and  more  elaborate  reasons  John  Dewey12  suggests  that  the 
term  "art  appreciation"  may  be  discarded  for  the  term  "art 
experience/'  and  the  latter  term  implies  activity  on  the  part 
of  the  beholder. 

If  art  experience  is  conceived  of  as  a  field  phenomenon, 
then  the  field  will  be  strongly  conditioned  by  the  difference 
in  the  degree  to  which  any  one  of  the  main  elements  con- 
stituting the  field  governs  it.  One  extreme  would  be  a  situa- 
tion in  which  the  work  of  art  dominates  the  field,  a  situation 
close  to  the  one  mentioned  above  in  the  example  of  the  seal 
on  wax.  Fortunately  this  situation  never  occurs  because  even 
the  most  passive  spectator  is  still  a  personality  with  a  par- 
ticular background,  particular  education,  particular  opinions 
and  feelings  about  art,  which,  even  though  he  may  be  un- 
aware of  them,  will  influence  the  field.  The  other  extreme 
would  be  a  situation  in  which  the  spectator  dominates  the 
field  and  is  not  touched  at  all  by  the  work  of  art.  It  might  be 
said  that  he  is  in  a  situation  in  which  he  is  confronted  with 
a  work  of  art  which  he  sees  but  does  not  experience.  The 
ideal  situation  is  a  playing  back  and  forth  within  the  realm 
of  the  field,  the  spectator  becoming  more  and  more  incited 
to  bring  new  facets  of  his  personality  into  play,  and  in  turn 
becoming  more  aware  of  new  facets  of  the  work  of  art.  Spec- 
tator and  work  of  art  may  be  said  to  be  communicating  with 
one  another,  a  communication  which  is  strongly  conditioned 

11  See  Koffka,  "Psychology  of  Art,"  Bryn  Mawr  Symposium  on  Art,  p. 

224  ff. 

12  See,  for  example,  John  Deweys  recent  volume,  Art  as  Experience. 


2,82        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

by  the  nature  of  both  of  them*  The  importance  of  the  per- 
sonality of  the  spectator,  his  experience,  and  his  emotional 
predispositions,  may  be  corroborated  by  the  well-known  fact 
that  at  different  times  in  life  we  experience  works  of  art  in 
different  ways. 

It  thus  becomes  apparent  that  when  one  learns  which 
aspects  of  a  work  of  art  are  important  for  the  art  experience 
of  an  individual.,  access  has  been  gained  not  only  to  his  par- 
ticular way  of  experiencing  art,  but  also  to  his  personality,  It 
is  even  more  important  to  ascertain  which  works  of  art  in- 
duce a  spectator  to  have  this  personal  experience — to  learn 
which  works  of  art  incite  him  to  establish  this  field  phenom- 
enon called  art  experience.  Moreover  it  is  of  interest  to  find 
out  which  works  of  art  "leave  him  cold,"  because  they  are  to 
him  void  of  meaning,  or  because  they  seem  too  unimportant 
to  him  to  induce  the  amount  of  interest  necessary  for  ex- 
periencing them.  Again  this  will  shed  some  light  not  only  on 
the  character  of  the  spectator's  art  experience  but  also  on  his 
personality.  If  something  about  the  personality  of  a  student 
can  be  learned  by  studying  the  environment  which  he  cre- 
ates for  himself,  by  exploring  the  kinds  of  persons  he  prefers 
to  be  with,  or  the  kinds  of  persons  that  he  avoids,  then  the 
type  of  pictures  with  which  a  person  does  or  does  not  "com- 
municate" may  be  indicative  not  only  of  his  art  experience, 
but  also  of  his  personality.  Finally,  one  wants  to  learn 
whether  or  not  a  person  actually  prefers  the  works  of  art  with 
which  he  is  able  to  communicate. 

The  possible  bearings  of  art  experience  on  creativity  in  the 
field  of  art  deserve  comment.  Obviously  only  the  person  who 
is  able  to  experience  in  an  esthetic  way  objects  and  events 
of  the  outer  world,  art  objects  as  well  as  others,  is  able  to 
express  these  esthetic  experiences  creatively.  It  was  assumed 
that  artists  perhaps  more  than  others  are  capable  of  having 
esthetic  experiences  with  objects  not  yet  molded  into  esthetic 
wholes.  Moreover,  during  the  process  of  expression  or  creation 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  283 

the  emerging  product  has  to  be  evaluated  by  the  artist  in 
terms  of  his  esthetic  perception.,  in  terms  of  the  evolving 
product's  suitability  to  induce  or  evoke  art  experiences  in  an 
ideal  beholder.13  Therefore  it  is  to  be  expected  that  the  art 
experience  of  the  artist  would  not  be  essentially  different, 
but  only  more  highly  and  more  intricately  developed  when 
compared  with  the  art  experience  of  the  non-artist.  It  might 
also  be  expected  that  persons  whose  art  experience  is  highly 
developed  need  not  be  or  become  artists,  either  because  of 
lack  of  skills  or  because  of  other  reasons.  On  the  other  hand, 
one  would  expect  the  artist's  art  experience  to  be  of  the  high- 
est quality.  Moreover,  the  person  who  demonstrates  a  high 
degree  of  esthetic  sensitivity  in  relation  to  the  extent  of  his 
art  experience  may  be  a  latent  or  future  artist. 

Although  the  above  remarks  are  not  adequate  for  covering 
the  topic  with  which  they  deal,  it  seemed  desirable  to  clarifv 
to  a  certain  extent  the  theoretical  framework  underlying  the 
assumptions  on  which  the  development  of  the  new  instru- 
ment was  based.  These  assumptions  will  now  be  discussed. 

DEVELOPMENT  OF  THE  INSTRUMENT 
Basic  Assumptions 

The  basic  assumption  of  the  new  instrument  to  be  de- 
scribed in  the  following  pages  is  that  it  is  possible  to  under- 
stand the  nature  of  and  degree  to  which  the  art  experience 
of  an  individual  is  developed  by  ascertaining  the  degree  to 
which  he  is  able  to  see  and  appreciate  significant  similarities 
and  differences  in  art  objects.  "The  reaction  of  the  artist  is 
colored  by  all  sorts  of  ...  associations  and  feeling,  of  which 
he  is  naturally  unaware,  but  which  affect  profoundly  the 
form  taken  by  the  work  of  art  and  which  have  the  power  to 
stir  up  corresponding  .  .  .  feelings  in  the  spectator.  It  is  the 

13  It  is  not  implied  that  the  artist  tries  to  "please"  the  general  public, 
but  that  his  efforts  are  concentrated  on  organizing  his  creation  in  such  a 
way  that  it  may  be  suitable  for  conveying  his  esthetic  message. 


284        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

fact  that  the  works  of  art  act  as  a  transmitting  medium  be- 
tween the  artist's  .  .  .  nature  and  our  own  that  gives  it  its 
peculiar,  and  as  we  may  say  'magic'  power  over  us.  It  is 
"magic"  because  the  effect  on  our  feelings  often  transcends 
what  we  can  explain  by  our  conscious  experience."14  If  the 
reactions  of  the  artist,  colored  and  conditioned  by  his  per- 
sonal associations  and  feelings  and  embodied  in  his  work  of 
art,  have  stirred  the  spectator  to  corresponding — even  though 
not  necessarily  identical — feelings,  the  artist  (or  actually  the 
work  of  art)  and  the  spectator  may  be  said  to  be  communi- 
cating with  one  another.  This  communication  is  possible  if 
the  spectator  has  been  able  to  establish  an  esthetic  field  in- 
cluding himself  and  the  art  object.  When  this  happens,  we 
may  say  that  he  really  is  able  to  "appreciate"  the  work  of  art, 
that  he  is  "sensitive"  to  its  artistic  qualities. 

The  deeper  the  art  experience  of  the  subject  is,  the  more 
he  responds  to  the  personality  of  the  artist  as  revealed  in  the 
work  of  art,  the  specific  way  in  which  the  artist  rendered  his 
subject  matter,  the  cultural  background  of  the  work  of  art, 
the  importance  of  the  media  chosen,  the  particular  way  they 
are  used,  etc.  The  quality  of  his  art  experience  is  developed 
to  an  even  higher  degree  if  he  is  responsive  in  this  way  to 
different  works  by  the  same  artist,  though  the  subject  mat- 
ters and  other  more  superficial  qualities  ( such  as  the  size  of 
a  picture)  may  differ  from  one  work  to  the  next. 

A  first  assumption,  then,  may  be  that  art  sensitivity  is  re- 
vealed by  the  degree  to  which  a  student  responds  to  the 
visible  similarities  existing  in  certain  works  of  art  created  by 
the  same  artist.  As  a  matter  of  fact,  the  degree  to  which  these 
similarities  can  be  seen  and  the  degree  to  which  a  subject 
can  reasonably  be  expected  to  respond  to  the  affinity  existing 
between  the  objects  created  by  one  artist  will  depend  on 
many  factors.  Some  of  these  factors,  such  as  the  particular 

14  Fry,  Roger,  Art  History  as  an  Academic  Study,  p.  13  in  his  "Last 
Lectures." 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  285 

selection  of  works  of  art  being  viewed  and  the  context  or 
conditions  under  which  these  works  are  seen,  are  of  out- 
standing importance.  Unless  these  are  properly  controlled, 
the  assumption  may  become  invalid. 

A  second  assumption  is  that  the  nature  of  a  student's  art 
experience  may  be  revealed  by  the  kinds  of  similarities  to 
which  he  is  or  is  not  responsive.  He  may  be  responsive  to  the 
similarities  existing  between  works  of  art  seen  as  wholes,  to 
the  affinity  mentioned  before,  or  he  may  be  responsive  only, 
or  chiefly,  to  similarities  in  color,  mood,  or  spatial  arrange- 
ment. If  enough  opportunities  are  given  to  a  student  to  select 
similarities,  his  pattern  of  reaction  may  be  open  to  examina- 
tion. This  may  also  be  said  to  be  true  in  a  negative  sense; 
that  is,  it  may  be  characteristic  of  a  student  not  to  see,  or  to 
be  unresponsive  to  certain  kinds  of  similarities. 

A  third  assumption  is  that  a  student  whose  appreciation  is 
weU  developed  will  have  a  certain  definite  emotional  reac- 
tion to  art  objects.  He  will  like  works  of  art  which  make  use 
of  the  qualities  he  is  responsive  to;  he  will  dislike  art  objects 
which  make  use  of  qualities  that  do  not  appeal  to  him.  He 
will  neither  like  nor  dislike  art  objects  which  "leave  him 
cold/'  which  "do  not  convey  any  meaning,"  i.e.,  art  objects 
which  seem  uninteresting  either  way. 

Construction  of  the  Instrument 

The  construction  of  the  instrument,  ""Finding  Pairs  of  Pic- 
tures," was  based  largely  upon  the  three  assumptions  dis- 
cussed above.  The  instrument  had  to  provide  evidence  as  to 
the  degree  to  which,  and  the  way  in  which,  students  respond 
to  the  affinities  existing  between  works  of  art;  it  had  to  pro- 
vide evidence  concerning  the  kinds  of  similarities  to  which 
they  are  responsive  or  unresponsive;  and  it  had  to  reveal  the 
art  objects,  or  qualities  of  art  objects,  to  which  they  have  a 
definite  emotional  reaction. 

According  to  the  first  and  second  assumption,  it  is  possible 


286        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

to  understand  the  nature  and  degree  of  art  experience  of  an 
individual  by  ascertaining  the  degree  to  which  he  is  able  to 
see  and  appreciate  important  similarities  and  differences  in 
art  objects.  It  was  thought  that  this  might  be  tested  most 
appropriately  by  presenting  students  with  examples  of  art 
objects  and  asking  them  to  pair  them,  and  then  examining 
the  results  to  see  what  inferences  might  be  drawn.  The  third 
assumption — that  students  will  have  an  emotional  reaction  to 
art  objects  which  make  use  of  qualities  to  which  they  are 
responsive — could  be  tested  by  asking  the  students  to  select 
certain  examples  which  they  liked  or  disliked  for  certain 
reasons,  and  examining  these  choices  to  see  whether  or  not 
they  corroborated  hypotheses  raised  by  the  examination  of 
the  pairings. 

In  constructing  the  instrument  it  was  impossible  to  present 
a  great  variety  of  art  objects  at  one  time  and  hence  for  prac- 
tical reasons  a  restriction  to  one  field  of  the  visual  arts  was 
necessary.  A  decision  was  made  to  begin  with  the  construc- 
tion of  a  test  covering  the  field  of  painting.  This  field  was 
selected  for  two  reasons:  (1)  it  is  more  complex  than  some 
of  the  minor  arts,  and  (2)  students  are  usually  more  familiar 
with  it  than  with  sculpture,  architecture,  or  with  the  minor 
arts.  There  is  also  the  possibility  that  the  response  to  certain 
subtle  values  in  paintings  may  be  a  valid  indication  of 
esthetic  response  to  the  same  values  when  they  appear  in 
other  fields  of  the  visual  arts.  For  instance,  one  would  expect 
a  person  whose  response  to  color  combinations  in  paintings 
is  well  developed  to  be  able  to  apply  the  same  discrimination 
in  dealing  with  textiles,  etc.  This  will  have  to  be  tested  in 
future  studies,  however.15 

The  next  problem  after  limiting  the  field  to  that  of  paint- 

15  It  is  realized  that  ideally  an  evaluation  of  art  experiences  should  cover 
all  the  fields  of  the  visual  arts,  and  it  is  thought  that  tests  based  on  similar 
principles  but  covering  other  areas,  such  as  sculpture,  architecture,  and  the 
minor  arts,  can  and  should  be  developed. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  287 

ing  was  that  of  setting  up  criteria  for  the  selection  of  the 
paintings  to  be  used  for  the  pairing. 

In  the  first  place,  the  pictures  had  to  be  selected  in  such  a 
way  that  there  would  be  an  optimum  chance  for  creating  an 
esthetic  mood.  It  was  desirable  that  everything  endangering 
this  mood  should  be  avoided  as  far  as  possible.  It  was  neces- 
sary to  exclude  pictures  evoking  too  strong  effects  and  pic- 
tures evoking  extra-esthetic  deliberations,  if  not  very  special 
reasons  recommended  using  them.  Thus,  it  was  decided  that 
certain  subject-matter  fields  could  not  be  used  because  they 
dominated  the  students'  interest  too  strongly.  For  instance,  a 
picture  such  as  "Washington  Crossing  the  "Delaware"  could 
not  be  used  because  primarily  it  evokes  patriotic  feelings  or 
historical  deliberations,  rather  than  "purely  esthetic"  "feel- 
ings. Because  it  was  found  in  preliminary  studies  that  some 
students  have  difficulty  in  pairing  pictures  from  widely  dif- 
ferent subject-matter  fields,  it  was  thought  desirable  to  limit 
the  subject-matter  somewhat  in  order  to  provide  a  maximum 
opportunity  for  pairing. 

It  was  also  felt  that  students  brought  up  in  the  tradition 
of  appreciation  for  the  old  masters  and  students  whose  main 
interest  is  concentrated  on  modern  art  should,  in  taking  the 
test,  have  about  the  same  opportunities  to  reveal  sensitivity 
to  art  values.  Therefore,  it  was  necessary  to  exercise  care  in 
order  that  the  selection  not  be  dominated  by  one  group  or 
the  other. 

Most  important  of  all,  however,  was  the  selection  of  ex- 
amples which  could  be  legitimately  paired;  that  is,  examples 
containing  affinities  which  can  be  recognized  by  students.  It 
was  realized  that  the  similarities  between  the  paintings  of  a 
single  artist  may  not  always  be  greater  than  the  similarity  of 
certain  elements  of  one  of  his  paintings  to  the  same  elements 
in  a  painting  by  another  artist  Care  had  to  be  exercised  to 
remove  as  many  of  these  potential  sources  of  confusion  as 
possible.  To  assure  this  point,  it  was  decided  that  the  selec- 


288        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

tion  of  paintings  to  be  paired  should  be  made  on  a  strictly 
empirical  basis.  In  line  with  this,  a  series  of  experiments  was 
made  with  a  group  of  60  high  school  students  who  were 
chiefly  in  the  ninth,  tenth,  and  eleventh  grades.  Several  hun- 
dred reproductions  of  paintings  were  presented  to  them  in 
groups  of  about  40  paintings,  and  a  careful  record  of  their 
responses  was  kept.  Pictures  which  were  not  used  at  all  by 
these  students  for  pairing  were  discarded  at  once.  Pictures 
which  were  paired  with  pictures  by  other  artists  in  more  than 
25  per  cent  of  the  total  number  of  times  they  were  used  were 
also  excluded  from  further  experiments.  The  remainder  of 
the  pictures  which  had  been  paired  with  those  by  another 
artist  were  dealt  with  in  a  manner  which  can  be  best  de- 
scribed by  giving  an  example  of  what  actually  happened. 

One  group  of  paintings  presented  to  the  students  of  the 
experimental  group  contained  among  other  pictures  two 
paintings  by  Picasso,  "The  Absinth-drinker"  and  "The  Gui- 
tarist"; several  paintings  by  El  Greco,  among  them  the  "View 
of  Toledo";  and  several  paintings  by  Corot,  among  them 
"Paysage/* 

In  more  than  25  per  cent  of  the  times  any  one  of  the  two 
paintings  by  Picasso  was  used  for  the  purpose  of  pairing,  it 
was  paired  with  the  other  painting  by  Picasso.  Therefore, 
the  experiments  with  these  two  Picassos  were  continued. 
Suppose  one  student  paired  "The  Absinth-drinker"  by  Picasso 
with  the  "View  of  Toledo/'  while  another  student  paired  the 
same  picture  with  the  "Paysage"  by  Corot.  This  suggested 
that  in  a  complicated  situation,  when  many  elements  from 
which  to  choose  are  offered,  it  is  difficult  for  some  students 
to  respond  to  the  affinity  existing  between  these  two  Picassos. 
Therefore,  a  less  complicated  experimental  situation  was  set 
up.  To  students  four  pictures  were  presented,  the  two 
Picassos,  the  Corot,  and  the  El  Greco,  and  they  were  asked 
to  find  the  picture  closest  to  "The  Absinth-drinker."  In  other 
words,  this  time  they  did  not  have  to  select  one  out  of  39 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  289 

pictures,  but  one  out  of  three.  Unless  at  least  90  per  cent  of 
the  students  of  the  group  selected  the  other  painting  by 
Picasso  as  the  best  choice,  the  painting  "The  Absinth-drinker" 
would  have  been  excluded  from  future  experiments. 

This  procedure  was  followed  with  all  paintings  for  which 
some  doubt  existed  about  whether  or  not  they  ought  to  be 
excluded  from  the  test.  The  purpose  of  this  procedure  was  to 
make  sure  that  the  pre-supposed  affinity  existing  between 
paintings  by  the  same  artist  actually  exists  for  students  of  this 
age  level  and  cultural  background.  By  selecting  the  sample 
in  this  way  it  was  hoped  that  as  far  as  possible  no  standards 
would  be  imposed  on  the  students  which  might  be  outside  of 
their  particular  experience  or  alien  to  the  orbit  of  tiieir 
esthetic  perception. 

In  selecting  the  material  for  the  instrument,  then,  the  sam- 
ples were  restricted  to  the  field  of  painting;  pictures  were 
selected  in  such  a  way  as  to  provide  an  optimum  chance  for 
creating  an  esthetic  mood;  pictures  which  might  prove  too 
distracting  were  avoided;  examples  were  restricted  to  a  few 
subject-matter  fields;  care  was  taken  to  provide  examples  of 
the  works  of  old  and  modern  masters;  and  as  far  as  possible 
only  those  pictures  by  any  one  artist  were  chosen  which,  ac- 
cording to  preliminary  experiments,  had  similarities  which 
students  are  able  to  recognize  as  such. 

DESCRIPTION  OF  THE  TEST   . 

As  finally  developed,  the  instrument  consists  of  a  picture 
sheet,  a  set  of  instructions  to  the  student,  and  an  answer 
sheet. 

The  Picture  Sheet 

The  picture  sheet  consists  of  a  piece  of  cardboard,  approxi- 
mately 24 "  x  44"  in  size,  on  which  40  colored  postcards  are 
mounted.  These  are  copies  of  more  or  less  well-known  paint- 
ings ranging  in  periods  represented  from  the  Italian  and 


290         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

German  Renaissance  to  modem  and  contemporary  art. 
Dutch,  Spanish  XVIIth  century,  and  French  XlXth  century 
paintings  are  included.  Portraits,  landscapes,  and  still-lifes 

are  represented.  The  copies  used  are  of  the  best  available 
quality,  chiefly  Jaffe  prints,  and  they  have  been  arranged  on 
the  cardboard  in  such  a  way  that  the  whole  set  makes  in 
general  a  pleasant  appeal.  Particular  effort  has  been  made 
to  avoid  having  one  painting  interfere  with  the  appreciation 
of  another  next  to  it.  No  titles  or  names  of  artists  are  given, 
but  each  painting  is  marked  with  a  number  for  identifica- 
tion.16 

The  Instructions 

The  instructions  presented  to  the  students  are  so  stated  as 
to  reassure  them  that  the  test  is  not  based  on  any  particular 
notions  about  art  or  painting,  periods  or  painters.  They  are 
told  that  it  is  not  expected  that  the  art  appreciation  of  an 
individual  ought  to  conform  to  any  fixed  standards.  Efforts 
are  made  to  convince  them  that  art  appreciation  is  some- 
thing very  personal,  different  from  one  person  to  the  next. 
Therefore  it  is  carefully  pointed  out  that  there  are  no  "right" 
or  "wrong"  ways  of  going  about  taking  the  test. 

Deliberate  efforts  are  made  to  avoid  as  far  as  possible  re- 
strictions which  might  limit  the  response,  or  create  an  at- 
mosphere of  examination.  Thus  students  are  told  that  no  time 
limit  is  set,  and,  even  though  according  to  experience  the 
student's  ability  to  find  pairs  is  usually  exhausted  after  about 
45  minutes,  it  is  recommended  that  teachers  allow  students 
to  use  as  much  time  as  they  wish  in  taking  the  test. 

Other  limitations  of  the  response  would  be  to  ask  the  stu- 
dents to  use  every  picture,  or  to  find  a  prescribed  number  of 
pairs.  In  order  to  avoid  this  type  of  restriction  it  is  pointed 
out  to  the  students  that  they  are  not  required  to  use  every 
one  of  the  pictures,  that  they  may  use  one  picture  several 

16  For  the  list  of  paintings  used  see  the  Appendix. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  291 

times  for  the  purpose  of  pairing,  another  one  not  at  all.  A 
certain  freedom  is  given  to  the  students  in  determining  the 
number  of  responses  they  wish  to  make.  During  the  prelim- 
inary experiments  the  students  were  not  told  to  select  any 
particular  number  of  pairs;  nevertheless  the  great  majority 
selected  between  20  and  30  pairs.  Experience  to  date  has 
shown  that  nearly  all  students  are  able  to  find  about  20  pairs 
and  that  after  about  23  pairs  most  of  the  students  stop  work- 
ing. On  the  basis  of  this  experience,  the  students  are  asked  to 
find,  if  possible,  at  least  20  pairs,  but  not  more  than  30  pairs. 
The  instructions  suggest  the  selection  of  pairs  of  pictures 
which  have  important  artistic  features  in  common.  As  exam- 
ples of  such  features  style  of  painting,  use  of  colors,  design, 
mood,  the  way  in  which  objects  are  painted,  are  mentioned. 
Since  experience  demonstrated  that  most  students  show  a 
tendency  to  rely  too  strongly  in  their  pairing  on  the  similarity 
of  subject  matter,  they  are  warned  that:  "If  a  subject  matter 
in  two  pictures  is  the  same  (such  as  flowers),  but  if  each  of 
them  is  painted  in  a  different  way,  then  this  similarity  of 
subject  matter  does  not  seem  to  be  an  important  reason  for 
pairing  them.  It  might  be  better  to  put  one  of  these  paintings 
of  flowers  together  with  a  portrait,  or  a  landscape  in  which 
the  colors  and  the  design,  the  style  and  the  mood  are  very 
much  like  those  used  in  the  painting  of  flowers." 

The  Answer  Sheet 

The  students  are  asked  to  indicate  their  choices  of  pairs 
and  their  preferences  and  dislikes  of  pictures  on  an  answer 
sheet  prepared  for  this  purpose.  The  answer  sheet  consists 
of  two  parts  and  contains  in  its  first  part,  in  addition  to  the 
usual  identifying  data,  spaces  in  which  the  students  can  indi- 
cate their  selections  of  pairs  by  writing  the  numbers  of  the 
two  paintings  which  according  to  their  opinion  have  impor- 
tant artistic  features  in  common.  This  part  is  arranged  as 
follows: 


292        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

1.  No. and  No. make  a  pair.  2.    No. and 

No. make  a  pair,  and  so  on  up  to  30. 

The  second  part  of  the  answer  sheet  is  arranged  as  fol- 
lows: 

Now  that  you  have  studied  all  of  the  pictures,  give  some 
general  information  as  to  your  personal  preferences  and 
dislikes. 

1.  Please  give  the  numbers  of  1,  2,  or  3  pictures  which  you 
like  best: 

The  numbers  of  these  pictures  are 

I  like  these  pictures  best  because 

2.  The  picture  I  like  best  for  the  mood  is  picture  num- 
ber     _ — __ 

3.  The  picture  I  like  best  for  the  colors  is  picture  num- 
ber      , 

4  Please  give  the  numbers  of  1,  2,  or  3  pictures  which  you 
like  least: 

The  numbers  of  these  pictures  are 

I  like  these  pictures  least  because  

5.  The  picture  I  like  least  for  the  mood  is  picture  num- 

ber     

6.  The  picture  I  like  least  for  the  colors  is  picture  num- 

ber     

THE  TEST  INTERPRETATION 
Tfie  Scoring 

The  basis  for  the  scoring  is  the  number  of  pairs  of  pictures 
painted  by  the  same  artist  which  a  student  is  able  to  find. 
Pairs  of  pictures  painted  by  the  same  artist  will,  for  con- 
venience, be  called  "S"  pairs.  The  pictures  used  permit  the 
selection  of  as  many  as  43  different  "S"  pairs. 

One  of  the  "S"  pairs  consists,  for  instance,  of  the  pictures 
No.  1  and  No.  24  (see  list  of  paintings  in  Appendix).  Both 
are  paintings  by  Picasso,  painted  in  his  so-called  "blue" 
period.  The  color  scheme  used  in  both  paintings  is  very  sim- 
ilar, and  no  other  painting  is  included  in  the  test  which  has 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  293 

an  analogous  color  scheme.  Both  paintings  are  representative 
of  a  certain  period  of  painting.  The  particular  flow  of  lines, 
the  sad  mood  expressed  in  them,  the  way  in  which  the  sub- 
ject is  rendered,  and  many  other  features  can  be  found  only 
in  these  two  paintings  in  the  present  set.  If  a  student  selects 
this  pair  we  may  assume  that  he  is  responsive  to  several  of 
the  artistic  similarities  these  two  paintings  have  in  common, 
and,  moreover,  that  he  probably  is  responsive  to  the  affinity 
existing  between  the  two  pictures  as  a  whole. 

A  copy  of  a  score  sheet  is  reproduced  in  the  Appendix. 
Three  scores  are  given  in  per  cents — the  "S"  pairs  a  student 
was  able  to  find;  the  ratio  of  the  number  of  "S*?  pairs  to  the 
number  of  attempts;  and  the  number  of  artists,  expressed  as  a 
per  cent  of  the  total  number  of  artists,  whose  paintings  the 
student  was  able  to  pair  in  an  "S"  way. 

Of  these  three  elements  the  most  important  and  most  in- 
formative is  the  second.  The  first  score  obviously  is  condi- 
tioned by  the  willingness  of  a  student  to  select  many  pairs; 
by  pure  chance  a  student  who  selects  30  pairs  ought  to  find 
more  "S"  pairs  than  one  who  selects  only  20  pairs.  Therefore, 
the  per  cent  of  "S"  pairs  has  to  be  interpreted  in  the  light  of 
the  number  of  attempts  the  student  made;  this  is  facilitated 
by  the  second  score.  The  score  on  number  of  "S"  pairs  is 
recorded  because  if  two  students,  for  instance,  have  about 
the  same  score  in  "Ratio,"  the  one  with  the  higher  score  in 
"S"  pairs  obviously  has  given  a  better  performance. 

The  score  on  "Number  of  artists"  is  mainly  of  descriptive 
character  and  may  be  used  for  the  purpose  of  ranking  stu- 
dents only  if  the  score  on  "Ratio"  as  well  as  on  "S"  pairs  is 
nearly  the  same  for  two  students.  Actually  this  score  is  sep- 
arated into  subscores  and  the  record  on  the  right  side  of  the 
score  sheet  indicates  those  artists  whose  paintings  a  student 
was  able  to  pair  in  an  "S"  way. 

If  a  student  paired  only  or  primarily  old  masters  in  an  "S" 
way,  one  may  infer  that  this  is  the  realm  of  his  main  interests. 


294        ADVENTURE  IX  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

If  a  student  found  a  great  many  "S"  pairs  by  using  only 
paintings  by  one  or  two  masters  it  might  be  that  he  is  for 
one  reason  or  another  very  well  acquainted  with  just  these 
paintings,  and  it  may  be  inferred  that  the  range  of  his  under- 
standing is  smaller  than  is  indicated  by  the  score  on  "S"  pairs 
and  "Ratio/5  The  statements  regarding  preferences  and  dis- 
likes are  not  recorded  on  the  score  sheet  because  so  far  no 
way  of  treating  them  numerically  has  been  found. 

The  scores  indicate  to  what  degree  the  student's  apprecia- 
tion of  the  40  paintings  included  in  the  test  is  developed  as 
compared  with  other  members  of  his  group.  They  indicate 
roughly  whether  his  appreciation  of  modem  or  old  masters, 
of  portraits  or  still-lifes,  is  developed  to  about  the  same  de- 
gree, or  is  unevenly  developed  in  any  one  of  these  areas.  By 
means  of  the  scores  alone  it  is  not  possible  to  ascertain 
whether  a  student  has  native  artistic  ability  or  only  an  intel- 
lectual understanding  of  the  field.  A  high  score  may  be  due 
to  native  ability  or  it  may  be  due  to  the  special  background 
of  the  student.  Familiarity  with  art,  frequent  visits  to  mu- 
seums, and  the  like,  influence  the  score  in  the  same  way  as 
creative  work  in  the  arts  or  native  abilities  influence  them. 
Nevertheless,  the  rough  score  seems  to  indicate  fairly  ac- 
curately where  a  student  stands  within  his  group  with  respect 
to  the  degree  to  which  his  art  experience  is  developed.  If  one 
wishes  to  know  more  about  a  student,  his  individual  re- 
sponses must  be  examined,  since  the  answer  sheet  furnishes 
information  which  is  not  reported  on  the  score  sheet.  The 
method  of  obtaining  this  is  to  make  an  interpretation  of  the 
data  recorded  on  the  answer  sheet. 

The  main  assumption  underlying  this  interpretation  is: 
everything  that  the  subject  does  is  important  and  he  does  not 
do  anything  without  valid  reasons.  The  basis  for  a  given  re- 
action of  a  student  may  or  may  not  be  a  genuine  esthetic 
response  to  an  art  experience;  nevertheless,  in  interpreting 
the  results  of  the  test,  one  ought  to  be  able  to  answer  certain 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  295 

questions.  For  example:  What  were  the  main  artistic  features 
to  which  the  student  responded?  What  are  the  artistic  fea- 
tures to  which  he  seems  to  be  unresponsive?  What  might 
have  been  the  reasons  preventing  him  from  making  an 
esthetic  response  to  the  art  objects  presented  to  him?  Or, 
approaching  it  in  another  way,  one  might  ask  what  might  be 
the  reasons  within  a  student's  personality  which  made  him 
respond  to  certain  works  of  art,  or  particular  art  elements, 
and  not  to  others.  To  answer  these  questions,  the  study  of 
pairs  consisting  of  two  pictures  painted  by  different  artists  is 
as  important  as  the  study  of  the  so-called  "S"  pairs.  The 
former  pairs  may  be  called  "D"  pairs. 

A  TD"  pair  which  is  occasionally  selected  by  some  students 
consists  of  No.  1  and  No.  35.  Both  paintings  make  use  of 
greenish  colors,  but  their  use,  the  way  they  are  blended,  and 
their  meaning  within  the  context  of  the  painting  is  quite  dif- 
ferent in  these  two  paintings.  The  mood  expressed  in  both 
paintings  is  of  a  more  or  less  introspective  quality,  enforced 
by  the  cold  colors  in  which  both  are  painted.  The  quality  of 
this  introspectivity  is  different,  however.  The  mood  of  No.  1 
may  be  described  as  being  sad  and  withdrawn,  whereas  the 
mood  of  No.  35  is  one  of  religious  exaltedness.  The  style  in 
which  these  pictures  are  painted  is  different,  but  there  may 
still  be  recognized  in  both  a  common  "Spanish"  element. 
The  selection  of  this  "D"  pair  may  be  accepted  as  indicating 
that  the  subject  who  selected  it  was  responsive  to  the  general 
color  used  in  these  paintings,  even  though  he  was  not  respon- 
sive to  the  different  ways  in  which  these  greenish  colors  are 
blended.  He  probably  was  responsive  to  the  general  mood  of 
introspectivity  permeating  both  pictures,  without  being  re- 
sponsive to  the  important  difference  in  mood  which  can  be 
recognized.  The  student  may  have  been  reponsive  to  the 
"Spanish"  element  common  to  Nos.  1  and  35  without  being 
responsive  to  the  difference  in  the  style. 

Many  more  inferences  pertinent  to  the  student's  art  experi- 


296        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

ence  and  in  this  way  pertinent  to  his  response  to  art  as  well 
as  to  his  personality  might  be  drawn  from  the  fact  that  he 
selected  this  particular  pair.  Great  caution  has  to  be  exercised 
not  to  consider  valid  an  inference  based  on  the  study  of  any 
one  pair.  The  selection  of  any  particular  pair  can  have  a 
quite  different  meaning  when  occurring  in  different  con- 
texts. Any  one  response  to  this  instrument  has  to  be  inter- 
preted in  the  light  of  the  possible  meaning  of  all  other  evi- 
dence which  can  be  obtained  through  a  study  of  all  of  the 
responses  of  the  subject  to  the  test.  In  this  connection,  as  has 
been  mentioned  before,  not  only  wrhat  a  student  does  is  of 
importance,  but  also  what  he  avoided,  or  missed  doing,  has 
significance.  Pairs  he  selected  not  only  have  to  be  studied  in 
the  context  of  all  other  pairs,  but  they  have  to  be  studied  in 
their  sequence,  and  in  the  light  of  the  pairs  the  student  failed 
to  select.  First  we  have  to  consider  which  are  the  pictures  he 
likes  and  dislikes;  these  data  in  turn  will  shed  light  on  the 
pairs  selected  because  students  tend,  in  their  pairings,  to 
make  different  uses  of  preferred  and  of  disliked  pictures. 

When  the  present  study  of  this  instrument  is  concluded, 
all  pairs  which  have  been  used  to  a  considerable  extent  and 
which  seem  to  be  significant  either  for  the  art  experience  or 
the  personality  of  a  student,  will  be  listed,  each  with  the  in- 
ferences which  suggest  themselves  in  connection  with  the 
use  or  non-use  of  the  pair.  Once  this  list  is  available  the 
interpreter  will  have  to  integrate  into  a  consistent  picture 
the  meaning  of  the  pairs  which  a  student  has  selected  plus 
the  meaning  of  the  non-use  by  this  student  of  pairs  com- 
monly used.  This  integration  will  have  to  be  achieved 
through  considerations  of  the  meaning  of  the  preference  or 
the  dislike  of  any  one  of  the  40  pictures. 

This  task  will  be  less  difficult  than  it  appears,  because  we 
can  restrict  the  investigation  of  the  student's  responses  to  the 
areas  in  which  he  differs  from  the  group.  The  "Ratio"  score 
which  a  student  obtains  places  him  in  a  certain  section  of  his 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  297 

group.  The  importance  for  the  interpretation  of  his  selection 
of  any  one  pair  depends  upon  the  extent  to  which  it  is  similar 
in  difficulty  to  the  other  pairs  which  he  has  used.  An  example 
might  clarify  this  somewhat.  If,  for  instance,  a  student  who 
is  in  the  lowest  quarter  of  his  class  in  his  "Ratio"  score  selects 
an  "S"  pair  which  has  been  found  by  only  one  or  two  other 
students  who  are  among  those  receiving  the  highest  scores 
on  "Ratio,"  this  pair  becomes  very  significant  for  the  inter- 
pretation. It  becomes  significant  because  one  would  expect  a 
student  with  a  low  "Ratio"  score  to  be  able  to  find  only  the 
most  obvious  pairs,  that  is,  only  the  pairs  which  have  also 
been  selected  by  a  large  portion  of  the  group.  The  opposite 
is  also  true — if  a  student  is  in  the  highest  quarter  of  the  class 
in  his  score  on  "Ratio,"  and  one  finds  that  there  are  pairs 
selected  by  a  large  portion  of  the  group  which  he  has  missed 
or  avoided  using,  these  pairs  become  significant  for  the  inter- 
pretation. 

It  is  evident  that  a  student's  responses  must  always  be 
examined  against  the  background  of  the  group  and  the  way 
in  which  the  members  of  the  group  have  reacted  to  the  test 
problems.  This  is  not  only  true  of  the  particular  group  in 
which  the  student  is  working  but  it  is  also  true  of  large  age, 
sex,  and  cultural  groups.  The  study  of  these  larger  group 
differences  will  provide  important  material  for  future  inves- 
tigations. 

As  a  basis  for  the  test  interpretation,  the  following  informa- 
tion is  therefore  needed: 

1.  An  analysis  of  how  often  any  pair  has  been  used  by 
the  other  members  of  the  group.  This  analysis  will 
make  possible  a  decision  as  to  the  degree  of  signifi- 
cance which  might  be  attached  to  the  selection  of  a 
pair.  The  kind  of  inferences  which  can  be  drawn  if 
a  pair  has  been  selected  has  been  indicated  on  pages 
293-296.  Here  we  may  add  some  of  the  inferences 


298        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

which  might  be  drawn  If  a  student  does  not  select  a 
particular  pair.  The  "D"  pair  mentioned  above  con- 
sisting of  Nos.  1  and  35  may  again  be  used  as  an 
example.  Assuming  that  this  "D"  pair  has  been  com- 
monly used  by  the  other  members  of  the  group,  some 
of  the  reasons  for  the  avoidance  of  this  pair  might 
then  be:  a  better  developed  discrimination  for  the 
importance  of  color  shades,  for  differences  in  style, 
and  a  lesser  degree  of  responsiveness  to  introspec- 
tivity. 

2.  Knowledge  of  the  average  number  of  times  any  single 
one  of  the  pictures  has  been  used.  Continuing  die 
example,  we  should  like  to  know  whether  the  student 
used  pictures  expressing  an  introspective  mood  less 
often  than  the  average.  If  that  is  true,  then  the  avoid- 
ance of  Pair  1-35  might  not  be  due  to  a  higher  dis- 
crimination, but  may  be  due  to  a  lack  of  interest  in 
paintings  expressing  an  introspective  mood.  In  this 
connection  it  may  be  added  that  the  use  of  a  pic- 
ture more  often  than  the  group  average  usually  indi- 
cates that  the  student's  interest  centers  around  this 
picture.  This  interest  need  not  always  be  of  a  positive 
nature.  A  repetition  of  a  pair  may  also  indicate  a  con- 
centration of  interest. 

8.  A  comparison  of  the  preferences  or  dislikes  of  a  stu- 
dent with  the  preferences  and  dislikes  of  his  fellow 
students.  In  continuation  of  the  example  mentioned 
above,  we  may  say  that  if  this  student  states  that  he 
prefers  introspective  pictures,  or  pictures  making  use 
of  dark,  greenish,  or  cold  colors,  we  can  be  reason- 
ably sure  that  he  avoided  the  selection  of  Pair  1-35 
for  esthetic  reasons.  On  the  other  hand,  if  he  dis- 
likes this  type  of  painting,  or  is  not  at  all  interested 
in  it,  the  avoidance  of  Pair  1-35  becomes  less  impor- 
tant as  far  as  the  evaluation  of  his  discrimination  for 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  299 

artistic  values  is  concerned.  As  another  indication  of 
his  avoidance  of  introspective  tendencies  it  will  still 
be  important  for  evaluating  his  personality. 
4.  A  study  of  the  sequence  of  pairs.  Study  of  the  mean- 
ing of  the  sequence  of  pairings  has  been  very  fruit- 
ful. For  purposes  of  illustration  of  the  kinds  of  in- 
sights this  permits,  the  following  illustration  may  be 
given.  Certain  very  obvious  pairs  tend  to  appear  in 
the  very  beginning  of  the  test.  A  student  who  begins 
with  seldom  used  pairs  seems  to  be  one  whose  art 
experience  is  different  from  that  of  others  in  the 
group.  To  begin  by  indicating  pairs  consisting  of 
portraits  is  usual.  To  begin  with  a  pair  consisting  of 
still-lifes  suggests  either  a  person  very  much  inter- 
ested in  this  subject  matter  or  a  student  who  is  re- 
served at  first  in  establishing  positive  relations  with 
his  fellow  men,  or  both. 

It  can  be  seen  that  the  interpretations  would  be  greatly  fa- 
cilitated if  they  could  be  made  on  the  basis  of  a  fairly  large 
collection  of  data  on  the  way  in  which  members  of  different 
groups  respond — the  ways  in  which  they  pair  the  pictures; 
the  pictures  they  like  and  dislike;  and  the  sequences  in 
which  pictures  are  used.  Thus  far  it  has  not  been  found  pos- 
sible to  achieve  this. 

How  TO  ADMINISTER  THE  TEST 

In  accordance  with  our  general  conception  of  art  experi- 
ence, it  is  important  that  a  spirit  of  freedom  prevail  during 
the  time  the  test  is  taken  in  order  that  an  esthetic  mood  may 
be  created  and  maintained.  It  is  best  to  have  every  student 
work  with  a  separate  picture  sheet.  However,  two  or  three 
students  may  work  together  on  one  picture  sheet.  Although 
care  should  be  taken  that  they  do  not  unduly  influence  one 
another,  nevertheless  explicit  prohibitions  not  to  discuss  the 


300        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

test  should  be  avoided.  Some  free  discussion  makes  the  se- 
lection of  pairs  much  more  interesting.  Anything  that  can 
contribute  to  the  students7  feeling  at  ease  should  be  done. 
Thus,  they  should  be  allowed  to  stand  up  and  move  around 
so  that  they  may  see  the  pictures  better,  etc. 

RELIABILITY  AND  VALIDITY 

Reliability 

On  a  priori  grounds  it  seems  reasonable  to  believe  that  it 
is  more  difficult  to  secure  an  adequate  sample  of  pictorial 
art  ( a  field  in  which  reactions  may  be  strongly  influenced  by 
the  emotions )  than  it  is  to  achieve  an  adequate  sampling  of 
information  within  a  restricted  subject-matter  area.  Because 
sampling  affects  reliability,  a  reliability  coefficient  which 
would  not  be  considered  very  high  for  an  information  test 
may  be  the  highest  reliability  coefficient  which  can  be  ex- 
pected on  an  art  test  of  the  type  described. 

Meier  and  Seashore,  for  example,  state  that  "with  tests 
based  upon  concrete  learning  accomplishment  a  higher  reli- 
ability is  expected  than  one  testing  complex  mental  functions. 
With  the  latter  kind,  a  coefficient  of  reliability  of  .80  is 
regarded  as  about  as  high  as  can  reasonably  be  expected, 
because  of  the  uncertainty  of  knowing  exactly  what  factors 
operate  in  the  person's  total  reaction.  With  a  test  of  capacity 
the  opportunity  for  chance  factors  to  control  the  final  result 
is  increased,  hence  a  somewhat  greater  allowance  must  be 
made  for  them."17 

Two  reliability  studies  of  the  instrument  under  discussion 
here  were  made,  the  first  based  on  the  split-half  method,  the 
second  on  a  comparable  test  form.  The  reliability  coefficients 
estimated  by  correlating  the  halves  of  the  test  and  applying 
the  Spearman-Brown  prophecy  formula,  based  on  the  test 
results  of  145  twelfth-grade  high  school  girls  and  boys,  are 
as  follows:  for  the  scores  on  "S"  pairs,  the  coefficient  is  0.57; 

17  Art  Judgment  Test,  Examiners  Manual,  p.  21. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  301 

for  "Number  of  artists,"  it  is  0.58.  Since  the  ratio  score  for 
each  half  cannot  be  added  to  get  the  "Ratio"  score  for  the 
entire  test,  we  cannot  give  a  statistical  estimation  of  the 
reliability  of  the  ratio  score  based  on  the  split-half  method. 
Therefore,  a  somewhat  comparable  form  consisting  of  49 
other  paintings  in  place  of  the  original  40  paintings  was 
developed.  These  49  paintings  are  not  as  well  known  as  the 
ones  used  in  the  original  form,  but  they  cover  the  same 
periods.18  The  students  who  took  both  tests  were  not 
homogeneous  in  their  art  experience.  The  group  consisted  of 
27  senior  high  school  students  and  38  college  students.  It 
may  be  expected  that  the  results  will  be  somewhat  better  if 
the  experiment  is  repeated  with  a  larger  group.  The  second 
form  was  taken  shortly  after  the  first  test  was  taken,  either 
after  a  lapse  of  several  hours,  or  within  one  or  two  days  fol- 
lowing. The  reliability  coefficients  based  on  the  intercorrela- 
tions  of  the  two  forms  are  0.58  for  "S"  pairs,  0.77  for  "Ratio," 
0.54  for  "Number  of  Artists." 

As  has  been  mentioned  before,  the  most  important  score 
is  the  one  on  "Ratio."  According  to  the  directions  of  the  test, 
which  give  great  liberty  to  the  students  in  selecting  many 
or  few  pairs,  and  in  using  the  paintings  of  many  or  few 
artists,  it  was  not  to  be  expected  that  the  reliability  coefficient 
of  the  scores  on  "S"  pairs  and  on  "Number  of  Artists"  would 
be  very  high. 

Validity 

Validity  studies  are  still  in  progress.  Such  evidence  of 
validity  as  has  been  collected  will  be  presented  here,  with 
the  reservations  which  must  accompany  data  which  are  in- 
complete. It  was  thought  that  the  validity  of  this  test  might 

18  For  a  list  of  these  paintings  use  the  Appendix.  Some  of  the  pictures 
in  the  comparable  form  furnished  such  interesting  and  important  informa- 
tion that  they  ought  to  be  included  in  a  future  form  of  the  test  in  place 
of  some  of  the  pictures  originally  used  which  were  less  successful  in  yield- 
ing information. 


3o2        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

be  established  If  the  following  assumptions  can  be  substan- 
tiated by  the  evidence: 

1.  The  test  measures  some  ability  which  is  not  a  func- 
tion of  the  particular  pictures  used  in  the  test.  The 
correlation  between  two  tests  which  use  quite  differ- 
ent pictures  would  seem  to  indicate  that  the  test  is 
measuring  an  ability  which  is  not  dependent  upon 
the  particular  pictures  which  make  up  the  test,  but  an 
ability  which  does  operate  within  a  wide  variety  of 
pictures. 

2.  Subjects  are  responsive  to  one  or  more  of  the  basic 
qualities  of  art,  but  are  responsive  in  different  de- 
grees. This  would  seem  to  be  supported  by  the  fact 
that  the  lowest  score  on  number  of  "S"  pairs  which 
any  student  made  is  higher  than  a  chance  score  would 
be,  and  there  is  a  considerable  range — from  17  per 
cent  to  100  per  cent — in  the  scores  of  the  subjects.19 

8.  The  development  of  visual  sensitivity  or  of  art  abili- 
ties need  not  correspond  to  the  development  of  intel- 
lectual abilities  as  measured  by  the  usual  intelligence 
tests.  In  the  case  of  one  school,  the  results  of  this  test 
were  compared  with  the  results  of  intelligence  tests 
giving  a  correlation  coefficient  of  approximately  zero. 

4.  Since  art  is  something  which  can  be  taught,  at  least 
up  to  a  certain  degree,  the  general  level  of  a  group 
of  art  students  ought  to  be  higher  than  the  level  of 
a  comparable  group  without  art  training.  The  median 
score  of  a  group  of  art  students  has  been  found  to  be 
higher  than  the  median  score  of  any  unselected  group. 
The  groups  are  small,  however,  and  no  controlled  ex- 
periments have  been  set  up  to  indicate  whether  or 
not  a  further  selective  factor  of  ability  or  interest  has 

19  See  table  on  p.  304.  By  mere  chance  a  student  might  be  expected  to 
get  a  score  of  5  per  cent  or  less. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  303 

been  operating  to  produce  the  results  which  we  have 
at  present.  It  would  be  desirable  to  repeat  this  study 
with  larger  groups  of  students  and  to  compare  the  re- 
sults with  the  results  of  control  groups.  (See  table  on 
page  304,) 

5.  Students  with  native  ability  should  give  a  better  per- 
formance on  the  test  than  students  without  native 
ability.  Within  a  group  without  art  training,  there- 
fore, there  should  be  some  students  who,  due  to  their 
native  ability,  perform  as  well  as  students  with  art 
training.  This  would  seem  to  be  corroborated  by  the 
fact  that  in  the  eighth  grade  the  highest  score  made 
by  any  student  is  as  high  as  the  lowest  score  made 
by  any  student  in  the  master  class  in  painting  in  an 
art  academy.  This  latter  group  is  composed  of  stu- 
dents who  intend  to  become  professional  artists.  As 
can  be  seen  in  the  table  on  the  next  page  there  is 
considerable  overlapping  in  the  ranges  of  scores  of 
different  groups.  The  weight  which  each  factor,  abil- 
ity and  training,  contributes  to  the  scores  will  have 
to  be  determined  by  a  controlled  experiment. 

6.  The  student  reveals  the  nature  of  his  appreciation  of 
art  and  some  elements  of  his  personality  structure  by 
his  choices  of  pairs  and  by  his  preferences  for  pic- 
tures. Evidence  for  this  assumption  is  encouraging 
though  not  conclusive.  Unfortunately,  many  of  the 
evaluations  of  the  interpretations  have  been  made  in 
verbal  rather  than  numerical  form.  It  is  impossible 
at  this  point  to  print  them  in  full,  or  to  ascribe 
numerical   values    to    these    evaluations.20    In    four 
schools,  however,  teachers  were  asked  to  select  a 
number   of  students   with  whom   they   were   very 
familiar.  The  test  results  of  these  students  were  in- 

20  A  morosprapb  in  preparation  will  include  more  extensive  discussion  of 
similar  studies. 


0 

o 

.. 

* 

tn 

^ 

«„ 

o 

I         ! 

J 

m 

,                   ' 

oo                                           ; 

i 

'.  ' 

oc 

i 

3           j 

m 

/ 

o 

,< 

j 

{>,                                               o 

tn                             o 

r    "S       ° 

0     < 

3 

< 

^            o 

<^             o 

i 

£* 

m 

j 

^      j        m 

i 

o                      < 

^      '               r<                     O 

<    i      .5 

m 

i 

} 

"i     '          SJ              tn 

* 

* 

S       !           U                  "^t" 

*  *     "  * 

&     ''         J 

0 

in    I 

m 

. 

S 

a    • 

o 

< 

m 

, 

. 

- 

fs! 

'o 

o 

*• 

rj 

03 

Z 

o 

C 

5/5 

in 

i 

s 

o 

o 

1 

-* 

bo 

RANGE  OF 

Type  of  School 

bo         bo         0               ^ 

S       G      -fi          ^ 
?g      ^3      ^  ^o      j| 

0           0           £  *9          C/} 

P5       PQ        >  (^         o 

1         1         £    E         5    ; 
J3        ^H         M   £        e£ 
"bb      ^b      i:  to       ^   ; 
q        S        o    «         g  ^ 

1  1   M  JH 

JZJ        J2J        J2J  ^        ^  " 

r\i  i  r\i:ciuciiiy 

Mid-  Western  Teachers 

Art  Students  in  Mid- 
Teachers  College 
tudents  in  Fine  Arts  in  an 
University 
f  a  Master  Class  in  Paintin 
•t  Academy 

<D             QJ^            dlP    <D^            CJ^ 

l-al-sll-sl 

0  J20JJOO  J2G_ 

q*atu-oSc/2fl°< 

ygs^^sfc^fl 

11=3  i|Sa|  | 
ggu^^-gw|.s 

=H  fe       ^      o       ^ 

OO          <NJ           (SI   CSl           CM 

6  <*-  i  ** 

I*"*           CD           \O   C7N           ^~~*   vD  in 

Ti- 

T—  4 

!>- 

^  °  &  -§ 

TH                   T-»                   T-*      \O                    CO      <N     I-* 

^ 

•«—  1 

APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  305 

terpreted  and  the  teachers  rated  these  interpretations 
on  a  five-point  scale  ranging  from  "very  good'7  to 
"poor."  The  intermediate  ratings  were  classified  as 
"generally  accurate,"  "possibly  accurate,  but  insig- 
nificant/' and  "of  doubtful  value/* 

In  one  school  the  teachers  selected  17  students.  Of  the 
interpretations  of  the  test  results  of  these  students,  nine  re- 
ceived the  highest  rating — "very  good."  Four  descriptions 
received  the  next  rating — "generally  accurate/'  and  four  the 
middle  rating — "possibly  accurate,  but  insignificant/'  No 
cases  were  placed  in  the  "of  doubtful  value"  or  "poor"  col- 
umns. The  teachers  were  also  asked  to  indicate  any  "gross 
inconsistencies  or  errors."  They  found  none,  and  stated  fur- 
ther that  in  no  instance  was  there  failure  to  designate  at 
least  one  important  characteristic  of  the  student.21 

In  another  school  the  descriptions  were  not  only  rated  on 
the  five-point  scale.  For  some  descriptions  the  teachers  used 
ratings  composed  of  two  of  the  five  points  of  the  scale,  in- 
dicating in  this  way  that  one  part  of  the  description  seemed 
to  deserve  one  rating,  another  part  of  it  another  rating. 
Thirty-three  students  were  described;  of  these  descriptions 
16  were  rated  as  "very  good/*  four  as  partly  "very  good"  and 
partly  "generally  accurate4."  Two  were  rated  as  "generally  ac- 
curate/* one  as  partly  "very  good/'  partly  "possibly  accurate, 
but  insignificant."  Three  were  rated  as  partly  "very  good'* 
and  partly  "poor/'  two  as  "of  doubtful  value/*  none  as  "poor." 
Five  descriptions  were  rated  with  different  combinations  of 
the  five  values  of  the  rating  scale.22 

The  test  results  of  27  students  of  an  art  academy  were  in- 
terpreted and  the  faculty  of  the  department  of  painting  was 
asked  to  rate  these  interpretations  on  the  same  five-point 

21  This  study  was  conducted  at  George  School,  Bucks   County,   Penn- 
sylvania. 

22  This    study   was    conducted   at    the    Cambridge    School,    Cambridge, 
Massachusetts. 


3o6        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

scale.  Eighteen  cases  received  the  highest  rating,  six.  the  next, 
none  the  middle  rating,  one  was  rated  of  doubtful  value,  and 
two  received  the  lowest  rating.23 

Finally,  81  students  of  a  teacher-training  institution  were 
tested  and  the  results  interpreted.  The  descriptions  of  these 
students  were  rated  by  the  art  faculty,  and  22  descriptions 
were  rated  as  "very  good,"  six  as  "generally  accurate/'  two 
as  "possibly  accurate  but  insignificant,"  one  as  "of  doubtful 
value,"  none  received  the  lowest  rating.  It  was  added  that 
"almost  without  exception  the  essential  qualities  of  the  stu- 
dents" were  "clearly"  mentioned  in  the  descriptions.24 

The  validity  studies  conducted  at  these  four  institutions 

j 

are  summarized  in  the  table  on  page  534.  According  to  this 
table,  approximately  60  per  cent  of  the  descriptions  were 
rated  "very  good,"  and  approximately  81  per  cent  of  the 
descriptions  were  considered  as  being  satisfactory  (either 
very  good,  or  generally  accurate,  or  of  an  intermediate  value 
between  these  two).  Approximately  10  per  cent  of  the  de- 
scriptions were  considered  as  being  unsatisfactory  (either 
of  doubtful  value,  or  poor,  or  intermediate  values  between 
these  two ) .  Only  2  per  cent  of  the  descriptions  were  rated 
as  being  definitely  of  poor  quality.  It  is  hoped  that  this  dis- 
cussion will  indicate  the  direction  of  the  work  on  validity, 
both  past  and  future,  and  the  extent  to  which  the  evidence, 
however  meager,  supports  the  original  assumptions. 

FUTUBE  USE  OF  THE  TEST 

The  study  of  this  test  has  not  matured  to  a  point  where 
it  is  possible  to  present  scientifically  dependable  conclusions 
about  how  such  an  instrument  can  be  used  most  efficiently. 
However,  it  does  seem  that  the  instrument  may  be  used  for 
the  purpose  of  counseling  in  so  far  as  it  may  be  possible  to 

23  This  study  was  conducted  at  Cranbrook  Academy  of  Art,  Bloomfield 
Hills,  Michigan. 

24  This    study   was    conducted   at   State   Teachers    College,    Milwaukee, 
Wisconsin. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  307 

decide  where  a  particular  student  stands  when  compared 
with  his  peers  as  far  as  his  response  to  paintings  are  con- 
cerned. Moreover,  it  may  be  possible  to  use  this  instrument 
to  ascertain  changes  in  the  performances  of  students,  or 
groups  of  students,  after  they  have  taken  art  courses.  For 
these  two  purposes  the  scores  seem  to  furnish  valuable 
evidence. 

This  instrument  can  be  used  much  more  efficiently  if  in- 
dividual interpretations  are  made.  When  this  is  done,  it 
seems  possible,  by  means  of  the  instrument,  to  get  evidence 
about  the  specific  art  abilities  of  a  student  as  well  as  about 
some  of  the  features  of  his  personality.  It  will  be  possible  to 
discover  some  of  the  areas  where  he  needs  special  help.  By 
repetition  of  the  test,  it  will  be  possible  to  discover  the  areas 
in  which  he  has  changed  and  those  in  which  he  remained 
on  the  same  level  as  before. 

Finally,  even  at  the  present  stage  of  development,  the  test 
furnishes  some  insights  regarding  the  way  in  which  art  ex- 
perience is  tied  up  with  personality  structure.  More  extended 
studies  will  enlarge  our  understanding  of  important  art- 
psychological  questions,  such  as  the  ways  in  which  art  ex- 
perience varies  with  different  age  and  sex  groups,  different 
cultural  groups,  and  groups  from  different  socio-economic 
levels.  Information  as  to  the  particular  way  in  which  the  in- 
dividual experiences  are  combined  with  information  about 
the  differences  in  the  reactions  of  different  groups  should 
have  implications  for  the  teaching  of  art. 

OTHER  INSTRUMENTS 

Several  other  instruments  to  reveal  the  ways  in  which 
students  respond  to  art  experiences  were  developed  experi- 
mentally but  were  not  studied  as  carefully  as  the  one  just 
described.  One  of  these  was  called  Seven  Modern  Paintings 
(Form  3.9).  A  committee  of  art  teachers  selected  seven  ex- 


308        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

cellent  large  framed  reproductions  in  color  of  modem  paint- 
ings, not  too  well  known  to  students  (a  Cezanne,  a  Van 
Gogh,  a  Picasso,  a  George  Grosz,  a  Eugene  Speicher,  a 
Maurice  Sterne,  and  an  Alexander  Brook).  These  were  hung 
for  at  least  a  week  at  a  time  in  six  schools,  without  com- 
ment by  teachers,  allowing  time  for  all  interested  students 
to  become  thoroughly  familiar  with  the  paintings.  Then  the 
art  teachers  in  these  schools  asked  all  students,  or  a  repre- 
sentative cross-section  of  students,  to  write  any  comments 
they  cared  to  make  about  any  or  all  of  the  paintings.  The 
students  were  asked  not  to  sign  their  names,  but  only  to 
indicate  their  sex  and  grade  in  school,  with  the  understand- 
ing that  no  attempt  would  be  made  to  identify  any  comment. 
No  directions  were  given  except  that  they  were  not  expected 
to  write  anything  very  profound  or  very  clever,  but  to  tell 
simply  and  honestly  what  they  thought  and  felt  about  the 
paintings.  In  a  few  classes  some  of  the  more  provocative 
comments  were  later  read  aloud,  and  more  comments  were 
collected  during  the  ensuing  discussion.  About  12,000  com- 
ments were  collected  from  about  1,000  students  in  grades 
five  through  twelve. 

These  comments  were  sorted  until  the  following  widely 
prevalent  modes  of  response  were  discovered: 

1.  Liking  or  disliking  the  paintings 

2.  Liking  or  disliking  the  subject  of  the  paintings 
8.  Demands  for  photographic  realism 

4.  Far-fetched  interpretations  of  what  the  subject  repre- 
sented or  was  doing:  e.g.,  "The  artist  is  trying  to  show 
how  the  wilderness  is  creeping  in  on  the  little  house/* 

5.  Fixed,  dogmatic  rules  applied  uncritically:  e.g.,  "A  por- 
trait should  always  have  a  dull,  neutral  background." 

6.  Interpretations  of  the  mood  of  the  paintings:  e.g.,  "The 
position  of  the  body  and  the  drab  colors  suggest  sorrow 
and  resignation." 

7.  A  feeling  of  understanding,  or  not  understanding,  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  309 

artist's  intention:  e.g.,  "I  don't  see  what  he  was  driving 
at/' 

8.  Comments  indicating  special  sensitivity  or  insensitivity  to 

color 

9.  Comments  indicating  special  sensitivity  or  insensitivity  to 
design  qualities  other  than  color 

A  few  comments  on  each  type  about  each  painting  were 
selected  and  mimeographed.  Thereafter  students  were  asked 
to  indicate,  while  looking  at  these  same  reproductions, 
whether  they  agreed,  disagreed,  or  were  "neutral"  with  re- 
spect to  each  comment.  An  answer  sheet  adapted  for  ma- 
chine scoring  was  used.  The  directions  also  indicated  that 
if  a  comment  wrere  true,  but  stupid  and  irrelevant,  one  should 
mark  it  "disagree";  and  if  it  were  neither  true  nor  false,  or 
partly  true  and  partly  false,  or  meaningless,  one  should  mark 
it  "neutral."  The  way  in  which  the  test  was  set  up  made  pos- 
sible two  more  categories  of  responses  which  were  helpful 
in  interpreting  other  scores: 

10.  Tendency  to  approve  (to  agree  with  favorable  statements 
and  to  disagree  with  unfavorable  statements) 

11.  Tendency  to  be  "neutral"  (the  percentage  of  all  state- 
ments marked  "neutral'7) 

No  judgments  by  a  jury  were  thus  far  involved  except  in 
classifying  the  statements  as  truly  representing  one  category 
or  another.  For  example,  the  statement  "I  don't  know  whether 
it  is  a  successful  portrait  because  I  can't  see  enough  of  the 
subject's  face"  was  selected  by  the  jury  as  representing  a  de- 
mand for  photographic  realism.  No  judgment  at  this  point 
was  involved  as  to  whether  the  comment  was  good  or  bad: 
only  whether  it  was  an  authentic  demand  for  photographic 
realism.  No  comments  were  included  on  which  100  per  cent 
of  the  jury  of  artist-teachers  could  not  agree.  This  was  pos- 
sible because  there  were  12,000  comments  to  choose  from 


3io        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

and  only  105  comments  were  used  in  the  test  (15  about 
each  painting). 

In  selecting  comments  revealing  special  sensitivity  or  in- 
sensitivity  to  color  and  to  design,  it  was  necessary  to  decide 
which  comments  showed  sensitivity  and  which  did  not.  In 
the  "interpretations  of  the  mood  of  the  painting,"  also,  it  was 
necessary  to  select  comments  which  were  obviously  within, 
or  very  far  beyond,  the  range  of  commonly  acceptable  in- 
terpretations. These  judgments,  however,  were  relatively 
easy  to  make,  and  100  per  cent  agreement  was  secured. 

Although  the  committee  originally  intended  to  get  away 
from  the  criterion  of  agreement  with  an  adult  jury  as  much 
as  possible,  it  came  to  feel  that  it  would  be  interesting  to 
have  the  jury  mark  the  comments,  and  to  see  to  what  extent 
children  of  various  ages  approached  the  jury's  judgment. 
The  jury  was  composed  of  practicing  artists  who  were  also 
teachers — people  who  were  presumably  sensitive  to  art 
qualities  and  getting  a  great  deal  of  enjoyment  and  stimula- 
tion from  good  painting.  It  was  felt  that  if  children  ap- 
proached the  jury's  way  of  thinking  and  feeling  about  these 
objects  as  they  grew  older,  the  chances  were  favorable  that 
they  were  headed  in  the  direction  of  greater  "appreciation." 
The  committee  had  become  diffident  about  using  the  term 
"appreciation,"  however,  so  they  did  not  apply  it  to  the  per- 
centage of  agreement  with  the  jury.  They  were  not  sure  that 
the  jury  was  "right,"  but  believed  it  was  reasonably  mature 
as  to  judgment.  They  therefore  called  this  score  "general 
maturity  of  response."  This  score  is  not  to  be  taken  too  seri- 
ously. For  example,  100  per  cent  agreement  with  the  jury 
would  probably  be  undesirable,  since  it  would  eliminate  that 
individual  idiosyncrasy  of  judgment  which  seems  to  be  char- 
acteristic of  people  who  enjoy  painting.  It  was  felt,  how- 
ever, that  a  gain  from  about  50  per  cent  agreement  to  75  per 
cent  agreement  as  the  child  grew  older  would  probably  be 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  311 

desirable.  Within  these  limits,  therefore,  another  category  of 
responses  was  created: 

12.  General  maturity  of  responses  (agreement  with  the  jury) 

The  jury  agreed  almost  unanimously  in  marking  all  of  the 
statements  except  in  the  categories  of  'liking  the  paintings" 
and  'liking  the  subjects  of  the  paintings/'  so  these  categories 
were  eliminated  from  consideration  in  arriving  at  the  "gen- 
eral maturity"  score.  It  was  apparent  that  two  equally  sensi- 
tive people  could  look  at  the  same  painting,  and  both  appre- 
ciate it  deeply,  while  one  liked  it  and  the  other  did  not. 
Liking  paintings  was  essential  to  appreciation,  but  liking  any 
given  painting  was  not.  The  same  reasoning  would  hold  with 
even  greater  force  with  respect  to  the  subjects  of  the  paint- 
ings. These  two  categories  were  included  chiefly  to  discover 
how  they  would  affect  other  scores. 

Many  of  these  categories  of  responses  are  desirable  in  one 
period  of  artistic  development  and  undesirable  in  another. 
"Demands  for  photographic  realism/'  for  example,  would 
have  been  accepted  as  desirable — as  making  for  artistic 
progress — in  the  early  Renaissance,  and  perhaps  they  may 
still  be  considered  desirable  at  certain  stages  of  adolescence. 
To  make  scores  easier  to  interpret,  however,  it  was  conceded 
that  art  teachers  of  this  generation  generally  regard  demands 
for  photographic  realism  as  undesirable,  so  this  category  was 
stated  negatively  in  the  summary  sheets  as  "Avoids  evaluat- 
ing in  terms  of  photographic  realism."  Thus  a  high  score 
always  calls  attention  to  what  most  art  teachers  would  re- 
gard as  strength,  and  a  low  score  to  a  weakness. 

This  test  has  not  yet  been  scientifically  validated,  since  it 
was  developed  only  recently  and  has  not  yet  been  given  to 
enough  students  to  justify  a  statistical  report  on  validity  and 
reliability.  Early  returns,  however,  are  very  promising;  at 
least  promising  enough  to  justify  further  research  along 
these  lines.  The  test  requires  some  sensitivity  to  the  mean- 


ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

ing  of  words,  but  verbal  difficulties  are  minimized  in  two 
ways.  First,  students  do  not  have  to  verbalize  their  responses 
for  themselves,  but  only  to  indicate  whether  they  agree  or 
disagree  with  a  comment  which  has  already  been  phrased 
for  them.  Second,  the  comments  are  in  the  language  of  other 
students  who  have  been  able  to  put  their  thoughts  and  feel- 
ings into  words,  so  the  student  is  not  confronted  with  adult 
concepts  in  adult  terminology.  Comments  were  edited  only 
enough  to  remove  ambiguities.  Nevertheless,  low  scores 
made  by  students  who  are  known  to  be  nonverbal  should 
be  taken  with  a  grain  of  salt. 


Chapter  V 
EVALUATION  OF  INTERESTS 


INTRODUCTION 

The  introduction  to  Chapter  IV  mentioned  the  close  con- 
nection of  interests  with  appreciations  and  the  difficulty  of 
distinguishing  them  in  specific  instances.  Work  in  both  areas 
was  initiated  by  a  Committee  on  Interests  and  Appreciations, 
which  was  later  divided  into  sub-groups  when  it  became 
apparent  that  techniques  for  evaluating  interests  and  appre- 
ciations would  be  sufficiently  different  to  justify  a  division 
of  labor.  The  sub-committees  on  appreciations  developed  in- 
struments, which  were  described  in  Chapter  IV,  to  discover 
the  ways  in  which  students  responded  to  literature  and  the 
arts.  The  sub-committees  on  interests  developed  instru- 
ments to  discover  and  appraise  interests  revealed  by  choices 
of  books,  magazines,  newspapers,  radio,  and  motion  pic- 
tures, and  interests  fostered  by  the  various  fields  of  study 
in  school. 

ANALYSIS  OF  THE  OBJECTIVE 

One  of  the  first  conclusions  of  the  Committee  on  the  Eval- 
uation of  Interests  was  that  interests  may  be  regarded  both 
as  means  and  as  ends.  When  they  are  regarded  as  means, 
teachers  try  to  discover  activities  in  which  pupils  are  already 
interested,  and  to  utilize  such  activities  in  teaching  pupils 
whatever  they  have  to  learn.  They  justify  certain  activities  in 
the  school  program  on  the  ground  that  they  are  similar  or  re- 
lated to  activities  in  which  pupils  have  expressed  an  inter- 
est. They  guide  pupils  who  have  such  interests  into  these 

313 


314        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

activities,  and  direct  other  pupils  elsewhere.  They  try  to  in- 
clude in  the  program  more  activities  in  which  pupils  have 
manifested  a  lively  interest.  If  little  or  no  interest  is  ex- 
pressed in  a  given  activity,  it  is  regarded  as  not  likely  to 
promote  learning. 

When  interests  are  regarded  as  ends  or  objectives,  how- 
ever, a  different  approach  is  indicated.  Teachers  have  to  de- 
cide in  what  areas  of  activity  pupils  need  to  develop  inter- 
ests, and  the  character  and  direction  of  interests  in  these 
areas  which  promise  most  for  individual  happiness  and  the 
common  welfare.  They  must  then  examine  the  evidence  of 
interests  already  developed  as  critically  as  test  scores  in  other 
areas  of  objectives,  noting  strengths  and  weaknesses,  and 
changing  the  school  program  to  build  upon  the  strengths  and 
remedy  the  weaknesses.  For  example,  it  is  generally  assumed 
that  pupils  should  develop  interests  in  one  or  more  wisely 
selected  fields  of  service  to  society,  since  a  man  who  is  in- 
terested in  his  work  is  usually  a  happier  and  better  citizen 
than  one  who  is  not.  If  pupils,  then,  shortly  before  gradua- 
tion from  high  school,  have  not  developed  such  interests,  or 
if  their  interests  lie  in  a  few  fields  which  are  inappropriate 
to  their  talents  and  opportunities,  the  school  has  failed  in 
one  of  its  obligations  toward  them.  The  character  and  direc- 
tion of  these  vocational  interests  may  also  be  examined. 
Pupils  may  be  interested  in  a  career  primarily  as  an  oppor- 
tunity to  get  rich  at  the  expense  of  other  people,  to  "get  to 
the  top'*  against  ruthless  competition,  and  to  enjoy  a  Holly- 
wood conception  of  "success."  Or  they  may  be  interested  in 
a  career  primarily  as  a  job  that  needs  doing — as  a  part  of  a 
great  cooperative  endeavor  to  provide  adequately  for  our 
common  needs.  The  latter  promises  so  much  more  for  indi- 
vidual happiness  and  the  common  welfare  than  the  former 
that  it  may  be  regarded  as  one  of  many  criteria  for  judging 
vocational  interests.  In  this  same  fashion  all  other  areas  of 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  315 

desirable  Interests  may  be  examined  for  evidence  of  growth 
in  the  kinds  of  interests  which  the  school  is  trying  to  foster. 

The  Committee  on  the  Evaluation  of  Interests  accepted 
both  ways  of  regarding  interests  as  legitimate  and  necessary, 
but  conceived  its  own  primary  function  to  be  that  of  helping 
to  evaluate  interests  as  objectives — as  outcomes  rather  than 
as  starting-points  of  the  educative  process.  One  reason  for 
this  decision  was  that  in  the  agreement  with  the  colleges  co- 
operating in  this  Study,  the  schools  promised  to  provide  only 
three  types  of  evidence  as  a  basis  for  admission  to  college, 
and  one  of  them  was  "evidence  of  well-defined,  serious  in- 
terests and  purposes."  Another  reason  was  that  relatively 
little  work  had  been  done  in  evaluating  interests  as  objec- 
tives. Most  of  the  standardized  techniques  as  well  as  in- 
formal school  practices  attempted  to  discover  interests  as 
starting-points  or  clues  in  attaining  other  objectives;  they  did 
not  evaluate  the  effectiveness  of  a  school  program  in  devel- 
oping interests  which  were  important  for  adolescent  devel- 
opment and  social  progress. 

In  the  course  of  its  work,  the  committee  had  to  discover 
and  overcome  three  difficulties  which  commonly  deter  the 
evaluation  of  interests  as  objectives,  and  which  may  ham- 
per the  work  of  similar  committees  in  the  future.  One  was 
the  unconscious  assumption  that  little  can  be  done  about 
interests,  that  any  interest  is  as  good  as  any  other  if  it  is  not 
obviously  criminal,  and  that  having  no  interests  in  impor- 
tant areas  of  activity  is  at  most  a  misfortune,  not  a  serious 
handicap  which  should  be  remedied  by  the  school.  The  com- 
mittee came  to  regard  these  assumptions  as  completely  false. 
No  one  ever  had  an  interest  which  was  not  learned,  or  picked 
up  in  one  way  or  another  from  the  environment.  Even  if 
something  in  the  organism  generates  the  interest,  such  as  an 
interest  in  food,  the  character  and  direction  of  the  interest 
are  obviously  a  product  of  the  environment.  The  Eskimo  is 


316        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

said  to  enjoy  seal  blubber  and  tallow  candles,  while  we  pre- 
fer beefsteak  and  potatoes.  If  all  our  present  interests  were 
acquired  and  are  continually  changing,  new  interests  can 
also  be  acquired,  and  less  promising  interests  can  be  changed 
for  the  better.  A  school  program  may  be  judged  in  part  by 
the  character,  direction,  and  importance  of  the  interests 
which  it  generates. 

A  second  factor  deterring  the  evaluation  of  interests  as 
objectives  among  progressive  teachers  was  the  common  as- 
sociation of  evaluation  with  penalties  and  failure.  It  is  espe- 
cially obvious  in  this  area  that  if  pupils  are  given  low  marks 
or  are  penalized  in  any  other  way  for  not  having  interests 
which  they  ought  to  have,  they  will  subsequently  "fake"  an 
interest  in  these  areas,  thus  invalidating  the  tests  without 
affecting  their  real  interests.  This  consideration  only  points 
to  the  way  in  which  almost  all  evaluation  data  should  be 
used,  but  especially  the  data  on  interests.  If  serious  defi- 
ciencies are  revealed,  the  program  should  be  changed  to 
remedy  them.  It  will  do  no  good  whatever  to  flunk  die  pupils 
who  are  deficient  in  these  respects,  nor  even  to  criticize  them. 
They  need  not  even  be  told  the  judgment  of  the  school  in 
regard  to  their  interests.  That  is  primarily  a  matter  to  be  dis- 
cussed in  faculty  meetings  devoted  to  curriculum  revision, 
and  in  case  conferences  devoted  to  planning  the  program 
of  individual  students, 

A  third  factor  deterring  the  evaluation  of  interests  as  ob- 
jectives was  the  suspicion  that  people  who  set  out  to  implant 
interests  in  the  young  have  in  mind  only  adult  interests.  This 
danger  was  recognized  and  guarded  against  in  devising  in- 
struments to  discover  interests  which  are  desirable  at  the 
adolescent  level.  These  may  include  some  interests  which 
would  be  inappropriate  for  adults;  they  may  not  include 
some  interests  which  are  indispensable  for  adults;  and  they 
may  translate  other  adult  interests  into  adolescent  terms, 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  317 

just  as  little  children  transform  the  adult  interest  in  children 
into  an  interest  in  dolls.  None  of  these  considerations  denies 
that  there  are  areas  and  directions  in  which  adolescents 
should  develop  interests.  If  they  do  not,  then  the  school 
should  do  something  to  help  them. 

This  view  of  interests  as  school  objectives,  which  gradu- 
ally evolved  during  the  Eight- Year  Study,  rests  upon  three 
basic  assumptions,  all  matters  of  common  observation.  The 
first  is  that  people  who  have  desirable  interests  in  the  major 
areas  of  life  activities  are  obviously  happier  and  better  off 
than  those  who  do  not.  If  a  man  is  not  interested  in  his 
work,  or  if  he  is  little  interested  in  his  home  and  family,  he 
is  so  plainly  miserable  that  the  matter  does  not  admit  any 
philosophic  uncertainty.  Second,  interests  are  the  mainspring 
of  the  educational  process.  They  practically  determine  what 
can  be  effectively  learned.  If  schools,  therefore,  wish  to  de- 
velop competence  in  the  major  areas  of  living,  they  must  first 
develop  interests  in  those  areas.  Third,  the  common  welfare 
depends  upon  the  character  and  direction  of  the  interests  of 
all  citizens.  If  these  are  narrow  and  selfish,  or  morbid  and 
cruel,  as  in  the  later  days  of  the  Roman  Empire,  the  quality 
of  the  civilization  obviously  declines.  These  three  assump- 
tions leave  no  choice  but  to  find  out  what  interests  are  de- 
sirable, to  foster  them  by  every  means  consistent  with  our 
democratic  traditions,  and  to  ascertain  at  regular  intervals 
which  of  them  are  developing  satisfactorily,  and  which  of 
them  need  renewed  attention. 

The  first  principle  which  the  committee  followed  in  locat- 
ing desirable  interests  was  that  some  interests  should  be  de- 
veloped in  each  major  area  of  living.  These  may  be  classified 
broadly  as  economic  interests,  civic  interests,  interests  cen- 
tering in  the  home,  and  recreational  interests.  The  first  three 
areas  wTere  sampled  chiefly  in  the  Interest  Index  which  is 
described  on  pages  338-348,  although  many  inferences  as  to 


3i 8        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

interests  in  these  areas  can  be  drawn  from  other  instruments 
described  in  this  report.  Lack  of  civic  interests,  for  example, 
was  found  to  be  reflected  frequently  in  high  scores  on  un- 
certainty and  inconsistency  on  the  Scale  of  Beliefs,  and  in 
great  confusion  of  implications  and  values  on  the  Social 
Problems  test.  These  areas  were  also  studied  through  many 
informal  instruments  devised  for  particular  courses  or  situa- 
tions and  not  reported  here,  and  through  standardized  tests 
available  from  other  sources.  Vocational  interests,  for  exam- 
ple, were  frequently  sampled  by  the  Strong  Vocational  In- 
terest Blank,  by  papers  written  in  various  courses,  and  by 
counseling  conferences.  Interests  in  these  areas  were  also  re- 
vealed by  the  instruments  developed  in  the  area  broadly  clas- 
sified as  recreational  interests.  An  interest  in  books,  for  ex- 
ample, wyould  be  classified  as  a  recreational  interest,  but  if 
a  student  read  an  unusual  number  of  rather  technical  books 
about  architecture,  and  if  in  the  arts  and  crafts  (also  clas- 
sified as  recreational)  he  devoted  himself  to  drafting,  to  in- 
terior decoration,  and  to  making  models  of  houses,  buildings, 
and  communities,  one  might  safely  infer  a  vocational  interest 
in  architecture.  Thus,  all  of  the  instruments  on  interests  cut 
across  the  areas  of  activity  in  terms  of  which  they  are  first 
classified. 

In  the  area  broadly  classified  as  recreational  interests,  the 
committee  distinguished  five  sub-areas  in  which  interests 
should  be  developed:  interests  in  people,  in  sports  and  games, 
and  in  the  arts  and  crafts  (including  fine  and  industrial  arts, 
music,  dancing,  drama,  movies,  and  radio  programs ) ,  in  read- 
ing, and  in  science  or  scholarship — at  this  level,  interests  in 
the  various  school  subjects.  Interests  in  people  were  such  an 
important  element  in  personal  and  social  adjustment  that  an 
instrument  revealing  these  interests  among  others  will  be 
described  in  Chapter  VI.  The  other  "recreational'"  interests 
were  sampled  by  the  instruments  now  to  be  described. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  319 

The  Reading  Record 

The  character  and  direction  of  interests  in  reading  which 
the  committee  regarded  as  most  promising  were  the  follow- 
ing: 

1.  The  reading  should  be  abundant. 

2.  The  reading  should  be  varied  as  to  type  and  content.  It 
should  include,  for  example,  both  fiction  and  non-fiction; 
it  should  reflect  a  wide  range  of  human  experience,  and 
deal  with  many  subjects. 

3.  The  reading  should  be  selective,  showing  some  concen- 
tration of  interest  upon  subjects  or  types  of  reading  suited 
to  the  reader. 

4.  The  reading  should  be  increasingly  mature,  gradually  in- 
creasing in  difficulty,  complexity,  and  depth  of  insight 

It  was  agreed  that  evidence  of  progress  in  these  directions 
could  be  secured  through  a  record  of  reading  kept  by  stu- 
dents and  summarized  periodically  in  these  terms.  The  com- 
mittee first  tried  out  a  very  long  and  elaborate  record  of  all 
reading  done  over  a  period  of  two  weeks.  This  included  as- 
signed and  unassigned  reading  in  books,  pamphlets,  maga- 
zines, and  newspapers,  and  asked  all  questions  about  it 
which  any  member  of  the  committee  thought  would  be 
helpful.  Over  1,000  students  entered  their  reading  on  this 
record  every  morning  for  two  weeks.  When  the  results  were 
analyzed,  it  was  agreed  that  in  the  future: 

1.  The  record  should  involve  an  irreducible  minimum  of  time 
and  effort  lest  distaste  for  reading  should  be  engendered. 

2.  The  record  should  be  filled  out  at  stated  intervals,  usually 
once  a  week,  in  English  classes.  Leaving  it  to  pupils  to  fill 
out  at  their  convenience  usually  resulted  in  incomplete 
records. 

8.  Only  voluntary  reading  should  be  recorded.  Students  oc- 
casionally had  difficulty  in  distinguishing  voluntary  from 
required  reading,  especially  when  books  were  strongly 
suggested  by  teachers,  or  when  supplementary  reading 


320        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

was  required  but  not  in  specified  books  or  amounts. 
Teachers  were  to  decide  what  reading  might  be  regarded 
as  voluntary,  or  as  indicating  individual  preferences. 
4.  The  record  of  books  read  voluntarily  should  be  kept 
throughout  the  academic  year  in  order  to  get  a  large 
enough  sample  to  provide  safe  inferences  as  to  the  direc- 
tion of  reading  habits  and  tastes.  A  reliable  sample  of 
magazine  and  newspaper  reading,  however,  could  be  ob- 
tained through  a  check  list  or  questionnaire,  administered 
annually  or  semi-annually. 

The  minimum  record  of  books  read  voluntarily  consisted, 
in  most  of  the  Thirty  Schools,  of  notebook  pages  with  spaces 
to  record  the  author  and  title  of  each  book,  the  date  on 
which  it  was  finished,  and  a  few  comments.  Some  teachers 
asked  also  for  the  number  of  pages  in  order  to  secure  a  more 
precise  measure  of  "abundant"  reading  than  the  number  of 
books.  A  few  teachers  provided  a  list  of  types  of  books, 
breaking  up  "fiction"  (which  constituted  about  90  per  cent 
of  all  voluntary  reading)  into  a  number  of  smaller  categories 
such  as  school  stories,  adventure,  mystery,  love  and  romance, 
etc.,  and  asked  pupils  to  classify  each  book  in  terms  of  this 
list.  Other  teachers,  who  were  especially  interested  in  widen- 
ing horizons  through  reading,  asked  pupils  to  classify  each 
book  by  the  nationality  and  period  of  the  author,  and  by  the 
period  and  country  with  which  the  book  dealt.  This  was 
done  in  very  broad  categories.  Since  most  of  the  authors 
read  were  American  or  English,  and  most  of  the  books  re- 
flected an  American  or  English  setting,  both  authors  and  set- 
tings were  classified  as  "American/7  "English/'  and  "Other/' 
The  periods  of  both  were  classified  as  B.C.,  A.D.-15QO,  1500- 
1800,  1800-now,  and  "Other"  (when  the  period  dealt  with 
was  not  specified,  or  in  the  future ) .  Most  of  the  tallies  ac- 
cumulated in  the  spaces  marked  "American"  and  "English" 
from  1800  to  the  present,  and  served  to  remind  pupils  of  the 
vast  expanse  of  space  and  time  which  they  had  not  yet  ex- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  321 

plored  in  their  reading.  Finally,,  some  teachers  asked  pupils 
to  indicate  how  well  they  liked  each  book  on  a  rough  scale 
from  0,  not  at  all,  to  4,  signifying  boundless  approbation. 

Since  most  of  these  items  could  be  recorded  by  number, 
referring  to  an  item  in  the  summary  sheet,  some  teachers  used 
the  following  form,  mimeographed  on  notebook  pages  or  on 
index  cards: 

.  Author 


TitlA 

AUTHOR:  Place 

Tfpnfi 

Tvpfi  of  |-»nnV 

SETTING:  Place 

Tim  ft 

Comments: 

These  teachers  asked  pupils  to  keep  their  own  summary 
sheet  up  to  date  as  they  read.  This  was  often  set  up  in  some- 
what the  fashion  shown  on  page  322. 

When  this  sort  of  summary  was  kept  by  pupils,  as  soon 
as  they  entered  a  book  in  their  reading  record,  they  put  a 
tally  on  the  summary  sheet  opposite  the  type  of  book  and 
under  the  degree  of  their  enjoyment.  As  these  tallies  accu- 
mulated, they  presented  a  graphic  summary  of  the  pupil's 
reading  development  in  at  least  three  of  the  four  directions 
which  the  committee  regarded  as  important.  The  total  num- 
ber of  tallies  indicated  abundance  of  reading;  their  dispersal 
represented  variety  by  types,  periods,  and  places;  and 
concentration  at  particular  points  on  the  first  gridiron,  accom- 
panied by  high  ratings  on  "enjoyment,"  represented  selectiv- 
ity, which  then  had  to  be  considered  in  terms  of  its  appro- 
priateness to  the  reader.  The  first  gridiron  also  gave  a  rough 
indication  of  increasing  maturity  of  reading,  for  the  types 
of  fiction  listed  there  ranged  from  juvenile  to  adult,  and  the 
amount  of  non-fiction  read  proved  also  to  be  a  crude  meas- 
ure of  maturity,  since  so  little  of  it  was  read  by  the  younger 
pupils.  In  the  second  gridiron,  almost  any  tallies  outside  the 
spaces  reserved  for  American  and  English  authors  from  1800 


TYPES 


ENJOYMENT 


Fiction 


0 


1.  Children's  stories 

2.  Animal  stories 

3.  School  stories 

4.  Sports  stories 

5.  Adventure — Western 1 

6.  Sea  stories    ' 

7.  Success  stories 

8.  Humorous  stories ' 

9.  Detective-mystery-horror... 

10.  Love  and  romance 

11.  Historical  novels 

12.  Novels  on  social  problems. . 

i  13.  Tragic  novels 

j  14.  All  other  novels 

i  15.  Books  of  short  stories 

j         Fiction  Totals 

i 

i  Non-Fiction 

I  16.  Biography,  autobiography. . 

J  17.  Books  of  plays 

i  18.  Books  of  poems 

1  19.  Books  of  essays 

20.  Books  of  information 

21.  Philosophy,  religion 

22.  Books  on  music  and  the  arts . 

23.  Hobbies,  practical  arts 

24.  Science,  natural  history .... 

25.  Social  problems 

26.  History 

27.  Travel  and  exploration .... 

28.  Sports  and  games 

29.  Vocations 

30.  All  other  non-fiction 

Non-Fiction  Totals 

GRAND   TOTALS 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  323 

to  the  present  represented  a  gain  in  maturity.  These  meas- 
ures of  maturity,  however,  were  too  crude  for  the  purposes 
of  English  teachers  who  wished  to  measure  the  effects  of 
various  experimental  programs,  so  a  more  refined  measure 
was  developed.  This  measure  takes  a  good  deal  of  time  and 
some  practice  to  use,  so  that  it  will  probably  be  used  chiely 
in  connection  with  experimental  programs. 

This  measure  of  maturity  was  based  upon  a  study  by  Jean- 
ette  H.  Foster  of  the  reading  of  15,000  adults.1  Her  analysis 
showed  that  the  250  authors  of  fiction  most  frequently  read 
could  be  objectively  classified  in  six  different  levels  of  matur- 
ity in  terms  of  the  average  age,  education,  occupational  level, 
and  general  reading  habits  of  their  readers.  Her  placement 
of  these  authors  on  the  various  levels  of  maturity  coincided 
with  the  judgment  of  the  committee,  looking  at  the  list  from 
the  standpoint  of  the  sort  of  maturity  in  reading  which  they 
wanted  to  develop.  They  therefore  extended  her  list  to  in- 
clude approximately  1,000  authors  of  fiction  most  frequently 
read  by  their  pupils,  matching  each  author  with  the  authors 
whose  maturity  level  had  been  determined  objectively.2 

At  the  same  time  they  made  a  detailed  classification  of 
types  of  fiction  and  classified  the  works  of  each  author  in 
terms  of  this  list.  Authors  typical  of  each  of  the  six  levels  of 
maturity,  from  1  (very  easy  reading)  to  6  (very  difficult 
reading),  and  of  various  types  of  fiction  may  be  found  in 
the  following  sample: 

1  Jeanette  H.  Foster,  "An  Approach  to  Fiction  through  the  Characteris- 
tics of  Its  Readers,"  Library  Quarterly  (April,  1936),  pp.  124-174. 

2  The  committee  responsible  for  the  extension  was  composed  of  Harold 
Anderson,  University  of  Chicago  High  School;  Irvin  C.  Poley,  Germantown 
Friends  School;  B.  "j.  R.  Stolper,  Lincoln  School;  Ruth  M.  Ersted,  Super- 
visor of  School  Libraries  in  Minnesota;   Jennie  Flexner,  New  York  City 
Public   Library;   Jeanette   Foster,   Holh*ns    College,   Hollins,   Virginia;    and 
Douglas  Waples,  Graduate  Library  School,  University  of  Chicago.  Douglas 
Waples  served  as  a  consultant  on  research  in  reading  to  the  Committee  on 
the  Evaluation  of  Reading  Interests  throughout  its  work  and  took  major 
responsibility  for  the  development  of  the  maturity  scale. 


324        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Author  Type  Maturity  Level 

Altsheler,  Joseph  A.  Setting  1 

Austen,  Jane  Character  6 

Bacheller,  Irving  Historical  2 

Barrie,  James  Character,  Romance  4 

Bennett,  Arnold  Character  5 

Boyd,  James  Historical  4 

Brush,  Katharine  Character  3 

Connolly,  J.  B.  Adventure  3 

Conrad,  Joseph  Adventure,  Psychological  6 

Curwood,  James  O.  Adventure  1 

Dell,  Ethel  M.  Romance  1 

Douglas,  Lloyd  Philosophical  2 

This  list  provided  at  least  a  standard,  uniform,  agreed- 
upon  classification  of  fiction  by  type  and  maturity  so  that 
teachers  in  different  schools  could  compare  the  results  of 
their  reading  programs.  These  were  summarized  by  teachers 
in  a  new  gridiron,  with  types  of  fiction  at  the  left  and  col- 
umns for  the  six  maturity  levels,  unclassified,  and  totals  for 
each  type.  Until  the  list  became  familiar,  each  book  recorded 
by  a  pupil  had  to  be  found  in  the  list  and  tallied  in  accord- 
ance with  the  type  and  maturity  level  there  assigned  to  it. 
Some  teachers  avoided  this  labor  by  securing  enough  copies 
of  the  list  of  authors  to  enable  each  pupil  to  tally  his  own 
books  on  his  summary  sheet.  It  was  feared  that  this  expedi- 
ent might  lead  pupils  to  attach  undue  importance  to  reading 
books  at  the  higher  levels  of  maturity,  but  when  it  was 
clearly  understood  that  the  maturity  figure  was  largely  an 
index  of  difficulty,  and  that  there  was  no  virtue  in  reading 
books  that  one  could  not  understand,  this  fear  proved  to  be 
unfounded. 

The  list  enabled  teachers  to  classify  about  75  per  cent  of 
the  fiction  read  by  senior  high  school  pupils.  Other  authors 
were  classified  by  matching  them  with  classified  authors,  or 
were  tallied  as  "unclassified."  If  even  75  per  cent  of  the  fie- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  325 

tion  read  by  a  pupil  were  classified,  this  was  sufficient  in 
most  cases  for  an  individual  diagnosis  of  the  direction  of 
reading  habits  and  tastes  in  fiction."  The  list  does  not  include 
enough  authors  commonly  read  by  pupils  in  grades  below  the 
ninth  to  be  discriminating  beyond  this  point. 

No  classification  of  non-fiction  by  maturity  was  attempted 
for  several  reasons.  It  comprised  only  about  10  per  cent  of 
pupils'  voluntary  reading  in  most  schools.  It  was  too  scat- 
tered to  be  easily  classified.  Thousands  of  different  authors 
were  read,  but  only  a  few  by  .more  than  a  handful  of  pupils. 
Frequently  only  parts  of  books  were  read,  such  as  single 
poems,  plays,  essays,  or  chapters  about  a  particular  subject. 
Since  so  little  non-fiction  was  read  by  the  younger  pupils, 
the  mere  number  of  books  or  of  pages  of  non-fiction  read 
proved  to  be  a  sufficient  index  of  maturity  for  the  purposes 
of  the  teachers  involved.  Any  refinement  of  this  simple 
measure  would  have  cost  more  in  time  and  effort  than  it 
was  worth. 

The  Magazine  Checklist 

The  record  of  two  weeks'  reading,  referred  to  above, 
proved  that  a  continuous  record  of  magazine  reading  would 
be  more  burdensome  than  the  result  would  justify.  It  also 
seemed  to  indicate  that  the  titles  of  magazines  read  would 
be  sufficient  for  purposes  of  evaluation,  without  a  list  of  the 
authors  and  titles  of  stories  and  articles  in  them.  While  some 
magazines  included  a  wide  range  of  types  of  material  and 
maturity  levels,  most  magazines  were  fairly  homogeneous  in 
both  respects.  Furthermore,  pupils  read  magazines  rather  in- 
discriminately, so  that  no  safe  inferences  could  be  drawn 
from  their  choices  of  particular  authors. 

When  it  was  decided  to  sample  magazine  reading  only 
once  or  twice  a  year,  it  was  found  that  pupils  tended  to  f or- 

3  For  a  detailed  presentation  of  the  reading  summary  for  one  student 
see  Wilfred  Eberhart,  "Evaluating  the  Leisure  Reading  of  High-School 
Pupils,"  The  School  Review,  XLVII  (April,  1939),  pp.  257-69. 


326        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

get  many  o£  the  magazines  which  they  were  known  to  have 
read  during  that  period  unless  they  were  reminded  by  a 
checklist.  In  his  Cooperative  Study  of  Secondary  School 
Standards,  Eells  had  found  that  108  magazines  accounted 
for  about  94  per  cent  of  all  the  magazine  reading  done  by 
17,338  representative  high  school  pupils.4  These  magazines 
were  listed  under  the  following  headings; 

1.  Popular  weeklies 

2.  Popular  monthlies 

3.  Picture  magazines 

4.  "Elite"  magazines 

5.  Non-fiction  weeklies 

6.  Monthly  reviews 

7.  Classroom  magazines 

8.  Popular  science 

9.  Sports 

10.  Special  interests 

11.  Youth  magazines 

12.  Detective,  adventure,  and  true-story  magazines 

13.  Motion  picture  and  radio  magazines 

14.  Farm  magazines 

Students  were  asked  to  check  each  magazine  they  had 
read  in  three  columns:  one  indicating  whether  they  read  it 
seldom,  occasionally,  or  regularly;  another  indicating  whether 
they  usually  skimmed  it,  read  parts  of  it,  or  read  it  in  full; 
and  a  third  indicating  whether  they  obtained  the  magazine 
in  school,  at  home,  from  a  friend,  a  public  library,  a  news- 
stand, or  elsewhere.  The  last  check  had  little  significance  for 
evaluation,  but  interested  some  teachers  for  other  reasons 
and  took  almost  no  additional  time,  so  that  it  was  included 
for  their  sake.  At  the  end  of  the  checklist  pupils  were  asked 

4  Walter  Crosby  Eells,  "What  Periodicals  Do  School  Pupils  Prefer?"  Wil- 
son Bulletin  for  Librarians  (December,  1937).  Reprinted  in  Evaluation  of 
Secondary  Schools:  Supplementary  Reprints.  Cooperative  Study  of  Second- 
ary School  Standards,  744  Jackson  Place,  Washington,  D.  C. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  327 

to  state  what  magazine  they  liked  best,  what  magazines  were 
received  regularly  at  home,  what  magazines  they  had  begun 
to  read  as  a  result  of  consideration  given  them  in  school, 
and  what  magazines  they  would  like  to  have  added  to  the 
school  library. 

The  maturity  level  of  39  of  the  magazines  in  this  checklist 
was  determined  objectively  by  Wert  by  finding  the  average 
intelligence  percentile,  English  placement  score,  and  score 
on  the  Cooperative  Contemporary  Affairs  Test  of  readers  of 
each  magazine  among  4,763  students  at  Ohio  State  Univer- 
sity, the  University  of  Minnesota,  and  five  smaller  colleges 
in  the  Midwest.5  He  converted  these  data  into  an  Index 
figure  for  each  magazine  by  dividing  the  average  score  of 
Its  readers  on  each  test  by  the  average  score  of  readers  of 
the  Saturday  Evening  Post.  The  unweighted  average  of  the 
three  quotients  thus  obtained  yielded  an  index  of  maturity 
or  "quality"  for  each  magazine,  ranging  from  about  40  for 
most  of  the  "pulp"  magazines  to  about  200  for  The  Nation 
and  The  New  Republic.  Abundance,  variety,  and  concen- 
tration of  magazine  reading  were  studied  as  In  the  case  of 
books.  Although  it  was  feared  in  the  beginning  that  maga- 
zine reading  would  not  be  a  significant  index  of  reading  In- 
terests, since  pupils  would  tend  to  read  whatever  magazines 
were  received  at  home  or  in  school,  the  variety  of  magazines 
read  and  Its  coincidence  with  other  measures  of  reading  de- 
velopment soon  dispelled  this  fear. 

Newspaper  Questionnaire 

In  appraising  students'  reading  of  newspapers  It  seemed 
important  to  determine  (1)  what  papers  they  read  regu- 
larly or  occasionally,  (2)  the  amount  of  time  devoted  to 
newspaper  reading,  and  (3)  the  sections  of  the  paper  which 
they  read  regularly.  Since  the  newspapers  read  by  students 

5  James  E.  Wert,  "A  Technique  for  Determining  Levels  of  Group  Read- 
ing," Educational  Research  Bulletin,  XVI,  4  (May  19,  1937),  pp.  113-121, 
136. 


328        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

were  those  published  in  their  communities,  no  attempt  was 
made  to  prepare  a  checklist  which  sampled  the  titles  of 
newspapers.  Instead,  a  newspaper  questionnaire  was  devel- 
oped which  provided  spaces  for  the  student  to  enter  the 
names  of  the  newspapers  which  he  read  and  asked  him  to 
check  the  sections  which  he  read  regularly.  Headings  such 
as  editorial,  financial  news,  comics,  book  reviews,  etc.,  wTere 
listed  for  him  to  check.  The  student  was  also  asked  to  esti- 
mate the  amount  of  time  he  spent  each  week  in  reading 
newspapers,  and  to  indicate  the  editorial  policy  of  each  paper 
as  "liberal,"  "conservative,"  "Republican,"  or  "Democratic." 
Few  students  were  able  to  do  the  latter  accurately. 

Radio  and  Motion  Picture  Checklists 

The  experience  of  the  Thirty  Schools  indicates  that  a 
checklist  is  a  feasible  device  for  gathering  evidence  of  inter- 
ests revealed  by  choices  of  radio  programs  and  motion  pic- 
tures. A  list  of  the  two  or  three  hundred  motion  pictures 
which  have  appeared  during  a  three-month  period  may  be 
given  to  students  with  the  request  that  they  check  each  pic- 
ture which  they  have  seen  and  indicate  their  degree  of  lik- 
ing for  it.  In  one  such  checklist  used  in  the  Eight- Year 
Study,6  recent  motion  pictures  were  listed  alphabetically 
under  the  following  headings:  comedy,  romance,  historical 
musical,  sports,  documentary,  Western,  adventure,  and  mys- 
tery. Including  the  names  of  the  principal  actors  in  each  pic- 
ture proved  to  be  helpful  in  refreshing  the  student's  memory, 
since  titles  often  had  little  relation  to  the  film.  Students  were 
asked  to  check  each  film  which  they  had  seen  and  to  judge 
its  quality.  Through  the  use  of  such  a  checklist,  data  can  be 
secured  concerning  (1)  the  number  of  films  seen,  (2)  the 
types  of  films  seen,  and  (3)  the  opinions  of  students  con- 
cerning the  quality  of  the  films.  In  addition,  the  level  of 

6  The  motion  picture  checklists  used  in  the  Eight-Year  Study  were  pre- 
pared with  the  assistance  of  Edgar  Dale,  Bureau  of  Educational  Research,. 
Ohio  State  University. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  329 

quality,  as  judged  by  critics  writing  motion  picture  reviews 
in  selected  periodicals,  can  be  determined  for  each  film  seen 
and  a  median  quality  level  computed  for  the  films  seen  by 
a  student. 

Similar  checklists  are  useful  as  a  measure  of  the  extent  and 
character  of  the  radio  listening  in  which  students  engage. 
One  checklist  used  in  the  present  study7  lists  the  popular 
programs  heard  over  national  networks  between  four  and 
ten  p.m.  and  all  day  Saturday  and  Sunday  under  such  head- 
ings as  variety  shows,  comedians,  serials,  religious  programs, 
classical  music,  dance  music,  news  commentators,  sports 
broadcasts,  and  discussion  programs.  It  requests  the  pupil  to 
check  each  program  which  he  has  heard  in  columns  indicat- 
ing whether  he  likes  it  very  much  and  listens  to  it  whenever 
he  can,  likes  it  fairly  well  but  does  not  go  out  of  his  way  to 
listen  to  it,  or  dislikes  and  avoids  it,  As  with  the  movie 
checklist,  a  tabulation  of  responses  reveals  the  programs  of 
various  types  listened  to  frequently  and  enjoyed  most.  Since 
both  motion  picture  and  radio  checklists  go  out  of  date 
quickly,  their  usefulness  depends  upon  their  continuous 
revision. 

The  radio  checklist  is  obviously  more  than  a  measure  of 
interest  in  radio  programs.  For  the  first  time  in  history  some 
of  the  world's  best  music  and  a  great  deal  of  the  world's 
worst  music  are  equally  available  to  everyone,  with  a  per- 
fectly free  choice  between  them.  The  level  of  musical  taste 
revealed  by  choices  of  radio  programs  is  based  upon  a  very 
extensive  sample  of  voluntary  behavior  in  a  natural  situa- 
tion. Studies  in  this  field  indicate  that  high  school  students 
.are  at  least  within  earshot  of  a  radio  for  an  average  of  two 
hours  daily.  They  listen  to  the  radio  far  more  than  they  read. 
Hence,  radio  preferences  are  one  of  the  most  valid,  reliable, 

T  The  radio  checklists  used  in  the  Eight-Year  Study  were  prepared  with 
the  assistance  of  I.  Keith  Tyler,  Director,  Evaluation  of  School  Broadcasts, 
Ohio  State  University,  and  Luella  Hoskins  of  the  Radio  Division  of  the 
Chicago  Board  of  Education. 


330         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

and  sensitive  indices  now  available  of  interests  not  only  in 
music  but  in  drama,  current  affairs,  social  problems,  and  the 
like.  The  radio  is  also  unique  among  the  instruments  com- 

monlv  used  bv  schools  to  discover  interests  in  that  it  so 

j  ^ 

readily  brings  to  light  undesirable  interests,  or  interests  that 
are  at  least  unpromising  and  a  waste  of  time.  The  possibili- 
ties of  this  medium  of  evaluation  have  only  begun  to  be 
explored.8 

Validity  and  Reliability 

The  problem  of  determining  the  validity  and  reliability  of 
activity  records  differs  from  the  case  of  paper-and-pencil 
tests.  A  test  score  is  regarded  only  as  an  indication  of  how 
students  would  respond  in  an  actual  situation  calling  for  the 
ability  measured  by  the  test.  It  therefore  has  to  be  demon- 
strated that  the  way  in  which  students  respond  to  the  test 
is  the  way  in  which  they  habitually  respond  to  appropriate 
life  situations.  The  test  maker  ideally  tries  to  get  an  accurate 
record  of  how  students  respond  to  such  situations  and  com- 
putes the  correlation  of  their  test  scores  with  these  responses. 
Often  this  is  not  possible,  so  some  other  indirect  measure, 
such  as  marks  in  courses,  has  to  be  used  instead,  but  an  ac- 
tivity record  is  commonly  accepted  as  the  best  criterion 
against  which  to  validate  a  test.  If  the  activity  recorded  is 
the  objective,  the  only  question  of  validity  in  the  record  of 
that  activity  is  whether  it  is  accurate.  The  only  question  of 
reliability  is  whether  the  record  includes  a  large  enough 
sample  of  the  behavior  in  question  to  make  sure  that  it  is 
typical.  If  all  the  behavior  relevant  to  a  given  objective  were 
recorded,  then  there  would  be  no  question  of  reliability  at 
all.  Only  when  a  small  sample  of  behavior  is  taken  do  we 
need  assurance  that  it  fairly  represents  the  habitual  behavior 
of  a  given  student. 

In  the  case  of  interest  in  reading,  the  behavior  which 

8  Many  promising  instruments  have  been  developed  by  the  Radio  Division 
of  the  Bureau  of  Educational  Research,  Ohio  State  University. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  331 

teachers  were  trying  to  develop  was  voluntary  reading  in 
books,  magazines,  and  newspapers  that  was  abundant,  varied, 
selective,  and  increasingly  mature.  A  record  of  such  activity 
was  secured.  If  the  record  was  accurate  and  complete,  it  was 
a  valid  measure  of  progress  toward  the  objective,  by  the  very 
definition  of  validity.  The  behavior  recorded  was  the  objec- 
tive itself — not  an  associated  behavior  which  might  or  might 
not  reflect  the  desired  behavior  accurately. 

To  find  out  whether  the  record  was  accurate  and  com- 
plete, during  1940  a  member  of  the  Evaluation  Staff  inter- 
viewed 51  students  in  the  tenth,  eleventh,  and  twelfth  grades 
of  a  private,  urban  secondary  school,  who  had  been  keep- 
ing rather  extensive  activity  records  as  a  part  of  their  school 
program.  These  records  included  reading  in  books,  maga- 
zines, and  newspapers,  attendance  at  plays,  operas,  and  con- 
certs, and  choices  of  radio  programs.  The  staff  member  ex- 
plained that  his  interest  was  only  in  finding  the  facts  about 
their  records  and  that  he  had  no  academic  connection  with 
their  school  or  with  any  college.  He  then  talked  informally 
with  these  students,  asking  them  whether  or  not  activities  in 
wilich  they  had  not  engaged  ever  were  recorded,  and 
whether  or  not  they  recorded  all  the  activities  in  which  they 
engaged. 

All  of  the  51  students  interviewed  said  that  books  which 
they  had  not  read  were  never  entered  in  the  record.  In  most 
schools  in  the  Eight- Year  Study,  this  was  no  more  than 
prudent,  for  nothing  was  to  be  gained  by  padding  the  list, 
and  the  books  recorded  as  read  were  discussed  in  confer- 
ences. Of  the  ten  tenth-grade  students  interviewed,  all  said 
that  all  the  books  which  they  read  wTere  consistently  entered. 
Of  the  22  eleventh-grade  students  interviewed,  ten  said  that 
not  all  their  reading  was  recorded.  Of  the  19  twelfth-grade 
students  interviewed,  three  said  that  not  all  their  reading 
was  recorded.  The  students  who  said  that  not  all  their  read- 
ing was  recorded  explained  that  "trashy"  books  sometimes 


332        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

were  not  entered.  These  "trash}7"  books,  they  said,  were 
chiefly  mystery  or  detective  stories.  Also  they  explained  that 
parts  o£  books,  such  as  single  plays,  poems,  essays,  or  stories 
from  a  collection  often  were  not  entered. 

When  the  students  were  asked  about  the  recording  of 
motion  pictures,  their  responses  indicated  that  for  many  of 
them  the  motion  picture  record  was  quite  incomplete.  A  few 
students  who  seldom  went  to  motion  pictures  said  their  rec- 
ord was  complete.  However,  most  of  the  students  said  that 
not  all  the  motion  pictures  which  they  saw  were  recorded. 
Some  students  said  they  consistently  omitted  recording  the 
"poor"  movies  which  they  saw;  some  said  they  omitted  re- 
cording the  second  feature,  that  is,  the  one  they  did  not  go 
to  see,  of  a  double  feature  program;  some  said  that  they 
often  neglected  to  enter  all  the  motion  pictures  which  they 
saw,  or  forgot  them  and  were  unable  to  enter  them. 

All  51  of  these  students  said  that  their  record  of  plays, 
operas,  concerts,  etc.,  attended  was  complete  and  accurate. 
Such  activities  as  attending  plays  and  concerts,  they  ex- 
plained, were  important  experiences  and  easily  remembered; 
consequently  all  these  were  consistently  recorded. 

These  interviews  led  to  the  conclusion  that  for  these 
students  the  record  of  books  read  was  accurate  in  what  it 
contained  but  that  it  was  incomplete.  This  finding  would  de- 
mand caution  in  interpreting  the  summaries  of  some  stu- 
dents* records  of  books  read.  The  quantity  of  reading  repre- 
sented in  these  summaries  would  have  to  be  regarded  as  a 
minimum;  the  median  maturity  level  of  the  fiction  read 
would  have  to  be  considered  in  error,  probably  in  that  it 
would  be  too  high.  A  second  conclusion  was  that  these  stu- 
dents' difficulties  in  keeping  a  continuous  record  of  motion 
pictures  attended  were  so  great  as  to  make  the  use  of  a 
checklist  technique  a  more  desirable  procedure.  A  third 
conclusion  was  that  for  these  students  a  record  of  plays, 
operas,  concerts,  etc.,  attended  could  be  kept  easily  and  ac- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  333 

curately  and  represents  a  satisfactory  method  of  securing 
evidence  of  participation  in  such  activities. 

Three  observations  need  to  be  made.  One  is  that  under 
certain  conditions  the  technique  of  asking  students  to  re- 
cord information  about  their  participation  in  certain  activi- 
ties can  yield  valid  and  reliable  data  for  the  appraisal  of 
interests.  The  interviews  cited  above  revealed  that  for  most 
of  the  students  it  was  reasonably  certain  that  their  record 
of  books  read  was  both  accurate  and  complete.  Second,  it 
must  be  observed  that  the  student's  attitude  toward  his  rec- 
ord may  be  a  crucial  factor  in  determining  the  validity  of 
the  data.  Recognizing  this,  the  teacher  should  help  students 
to  understand  and  accept  the  purposes  of  this  type  of  evalua- 
tion and  to  remove  as  far  as  possible  all  academic  or  social 
pressure  which  would  tempt  students  to  falsify  their  records. 
Third,  it  is  important  to  remember  that  the  interpretation  of 
data  derived  in  this  fashion  should  attempt  to  take  into  ac- 
count the  conditions  under  which  they  wrere  gathered. 

The  validity  of  the  evidence  secured  by  means  of  check- 
lists is  dependent  upon  many  of  the  same  factors  as  is  the 
validity  of  the  evidence  secured  by  means  of  continuous  rec- 
ords. A  checklist  requires  that  a  student  recognize,  rather 
than  recall,  those  activities  in  which  he  has  participated; 
thus  it  demands  a  less  difficult  task  of  the  student.  A  check- 
list, however,  often  must  present  only  a  sample  of  the  many 
possible  activities  or  materials  and  thus  is  dependent  upon 
the  adequacy  of  the  sampling.  The  Checklist  of  One  Hun- 
dred Magazines,  for  example,  presents  to  the  student  only  a 
fraction  of  the  total  number  of  magazines  which  are  pub- 
lished. There  is  evidence,  however,  that  this  sample  is  ade- 
quate for  determining  the  magazine  reading  interests  of 
secondary  school  students.  Students,  of  course,  may  be  dis- 
honest in  responding  to  a  checklist.  Again  it  must  be  pointed 
out  that  the  total  situation  must  be  considered  in  guarding 
against  such  dishonesty.  There  are  no  devices  and  no  format 


334        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

of  a  checklist  which  will  compensate  for  a  lack  of  rapport 
between  teachers  and  students,  for  failure  to  prepare  for  the 
administration  of  such  evaluation  instruments,  or  for  short- 
sighted use  of  data  gathered  in  this  fashion. 

Uses  of  the  Instruments 

In  making  use  of  data  gathered  by  means  of  activity  rec- 
ords, one  of  the  problems  which  teachers  face  is  that  of 
summarizing  the  data  in  such  a  fashion  as  to  obtain  a  reason- 
ably precise,  yet  brief,  description  of  the  interests  revealed. 
Summaries  of  certain  activity  records  for  two  students  will 
be  presented  in  order  to  illustrate  the  kinds  of  information 
about  students  which  they  make  available. 

Elizabeth 

Elizabeth  read  15  books  during  the  year.  Fiction  included 
Mary  Johnston's  To  Have  and  To  Hold,  Churchill's  The  Crisis, 
The  Prince  and  the  Pauper,  Bertita  Harding's  Farewell  'Toinette, 
and  Let  the  Hurricane  Roar;  two  college  stories,  Iron  Duke  and 
College  in  Crinoline;  one  dog  story;  The  Count  of  Monte  Cristo; 
The  Girl  of  the  Limberlost,  Anne  of  Green  Gables.  Non-fiction 
included  The  Boys  Life  of  Will  Rogers,  Life  with  Mother,  Men 
Are  Like  Street  Cars,  and  Daily  Except  Sundays.  Eight  of  these 
books  were  read  during  the  summer  and  seven  during  the  school 
year.  The  class  of  students  of  which  Elizabeth  is  a  member  read 
an  average  of  12  books  during  the  summer  and  24  books  during 
the  school  year.  She  did  not  read  books  of  as  great  difficulty  and 
maturity  as  did  the  group  as  a  whole.  The  fiction  she  read  is  dis- 
tributed over  Levels  III  (e.g.,  The  Crisis},  II  (e.g.,  Jock  the 
Scot),  and  I  (e.g.,  Girl  of  the  Limberlost);  whereas  the  median 
maturity  level  of  the  fiction  read  by  the  group  as  a  whole  is  IV. 

In  October,  1938,  Elizabeth  checked  New  Yorker  as  the  only 
magazine  she  read  regularly;  in  March,  1939,  Life.  In  October, 
she  was  reading  no  magazine  completely;  in  March,  two — Life 
and  Look.  She  was  below  the  class  median  in  the  number  of 
magazines  read  regularly  and  the  number  read  completely.  This 
evidence,  together  with  the  number  of  books  which  she  read, 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  335 

suggests  that  she  does  not  like  to  read  to  an  extent  comparable 
with  other  students  in  her  group. 

Elizabeth  far  exceeded  most  of  the  members  of  her  class  in  the 
number  of  motion  pictures  which  she  attended.  She  recorded 
seeing  39  during  the  summer  and  86  during  the  school  year.  The 
median  number  of  motion  pictures  attended  by  students  of  her 
class  during  the  school  year  was  27;  the  range,  0  to  99.  Also,  she 
saw  many  of  these  86  different  motion  pictures  more  than  once. 
Evidently,  then,  a  large  amount  of  her  leisure  time  was  spent  in 
viewing  motion  pictures.  During  the  year,  Elizabeth  saw  two 
plays:  The  Boijs  from  Syracuse  and  Abe  Lincoln  in  Illinois,  and 
attended  a  performance  of  The  Mikado.  The  median  number  of 
plays,  operas,  and  concerts  attended  by  students  in  her  class, 
however,  was  five. 

Elizabeth's  five  favorite  radio  programs  in  December,  1938, 
were  Benny  Goodman,  Bob  Crosby,  Kay  Kyser,  Make  Believe 
Ballroom,  and  Tommy  Dorsey.  Of  the  19  programs  which  she 
checked  as  the  ones  she  listened  to  regularly,  seven  were  dance 
orchestras  such  as  the  ones  listed  as  favorites.  In  addition  to 
dance  music,  she  listened  regularly  to  five  variety  programs, 
three  question  and  answer  programs,  two  dramatic  programs — 
Big  Town  and  Lux  Radio  Theatre,  and  to  Walter  Winchell  and 
Jimmie  Fiddler.  Elizabeth  was  approximately  at  the  median  of 
her  class  in  the  number  of  programs  she  heard  regularly. 

Claire 

Claire  read  ten  books  during  the  summer  and  35  during  the 
school  year.  Five  of  these  books  read  during  the  school  year  were 
collections  of  plays,  such  as  The  Theatre  Guild  Anthology,  two 
were  volumes  of  poetry;  two  were  discussions  of  political  and 
social  problems;  and  four  were  books  about  journalism  and  the 
writing  of  short  stories.  The  fiction  she  read  during  the  school 
year  included  two  volumes  of  short  stories  and  such  novels  as 
Drums  Along  the  Mohawk,  My  Antonia,  House  of  Seven  Gables, 
House  of  Exile,  Mary  Roberts  Rinehart's  The  Doctor,  and  Gone 
With  the  Wind.  More  than  half  of  Claire's  reading  was  devoted 
to  non-fiction,  whereas  for  her  class  as  a  whole  approximately 
25  per  cent  of  the  titles  were  non-fiction.  Also  she  read  more  than 


336        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

the  average  number  of  books  during  the  school  year.  The  fiction 
which  she  read  was  of  Levels  III,  IV,  and  V;  this  indicates  that 
she  was  reading  books  of  approximately  the  same  maturity  as  was 
the  group  as  a  whole. 

Claire  checked  eight  magazines  as  those  which  she  read  regu- 
larly in  October,  1938;  and  ten  in  March,  1939.  These  numbers 
are"  considerably  above  the  group  medians.  In  October  she 
checked  six  magazines  as  the  ones  which  she  read  completely;  in 
March,  five.  Again,  these  numbers  are  above  the  group  medians. 
The  magazines  which  she  read  were  American  Home,  Better 
English^Life,  New  York  Times  Magazine,  Readers  Digest,  Rider 
and  Driver,  Quiz  Digest,  and  Time. 

During  the  school  year  Claire  saw  18  different  motion  pictures; 
one  of  these,  Grand  Illusion,  she  saw  twice.  Some  of  these  pic- 
tures which  she  liked  very  much  were  Grand  Illusion,  Four 
Daughters,  Joung  Doctor  Kildare,  A  Man  to  Remember,  The 
Sisters,  Brother  Rat,  Scarf  ace,  Gunga  Din,  Stage  Coach,  Made  for 
Each  Other,  and  Irene  and  Vernon  Castle.  Her  comments  about 
the  motion  pictures  which  she  saw  and  the  list  of  pictures  which 
she  liked  suggest  that  she  chooses  her  motion  picture  entertain- 
ment with  some  care. 

In  addition  to  these  motion  pictures,  Claire  attended  three 
plays,  Abe  Lincoln  in  Illinois,  American  Landscape,  Outward 
Bound;  and  three  musical  performances,  The  Boys  from  Syra- 
cuse, Ballet  Russe,  and  The  Hot  Mikado.  This  is  slightly  above 
the  class  median  of  five.  Her  activity  record  also  records  visits  to 
several  museums  and  art  galleries. 

In  December,  1938,  Claire  checked  eight  radio  programs  as 
those  which  she  listened  to  regularly.  These  included  the  Colum- 
bia Workshop,  three  programs  of  classical  music,  Information 
Please,  two  news  commentators,  and  talks  on  politics.  This  num- 
ber is  much  smaller  than  the  median  number  of  programs  heard 
regularly  by  the  group  as  a  whole. 

The  leisure-time  activities  of  these  two  students  present 
two  quite  different  pictures.  One  has  its  chief  emphasis  on 
activities  such  as  attending  motion  pictures  and  listening  to 
the  radio  with  very  little  emphasis  on  reading  experiences; 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  33' 

the  other  presents  quite  a  different  pattern.  The  one  reveals 
interests  which  might  be  characterized  as  the  more  "popular" 
ones,  while  the  other  reveals  interests  which  might  be  char- 
acterized as  much  more  intellectual. 

Data  such  as  those  presented  in  these  illustrations  should 
be  of  use  to  teachers  who  are  concerned  about  the  pattern 
of  interests  which  students  are  developing.  In  order  to  use 
such  data  most  effectively,  it  is  important  for  the  teacher 
to  determine  what  kinds  of  interests  he  considers  desirable 
for  the  student  or  the  group  of  students,  to  exercise  care  in 
gathering  the  evidence,  and  to  summarize  this  evidence  in 
a  convenient  fashion.  Cumulative  summaries  have  several 
advantages.  One  is  that  changes  wThich  take  place  over  a 
longer  period  of  time  may  become  evident.  Another  is  that 
such  summaries  may  be  passed  on  from  teacher  to  teacher 
as  the  student  moves  through  school.  Such  summaries  prob- 
ably should  not  be  as  lengthy  as  the  illustrations  given  here. 
However,  data  in  tabular  form  similar  to  that  suggested  for 
books  can  be  recorded  and  cumulated  by  students.  Summary 
comments  about  the  pattern  of  interests  revealed,  changes 
observed,  and  the  directions  in  which  future  changes  should 
take  place  might  then  be  added  by  the  teacher  with  rela- 
tively little  effort. 

One  further  suggestion  about  the  use  of  such  data  seems 
warranted.  Whenever  possible,  other  evidence  should  be 
combined  with  the  evidence  supplied  by  such  summaries  in 
order  to  provide  a  more  comprehensive  description  of  the 

student's  interests.  The  observations  made  bv  teachers  both 

•> 

in  and  out  of  the  classroom,  evidence  from  other  instruments 
such  as  the  Interest  Questionnaire  described  in  this  chapter, 
and  the  like,  should  prove  useful  either  in  corroborating 
hypotheses  or  in  revealing  inconsistencies  which  need  care- 
ful study  in  order  to  arrive  at  a  clearer  understanding  of  the 
student. 


338        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

THE  INTEREST  INDEX  8.2A 

In  addition  to  records  of  activities,  the  questionnaire  has 
also  been  found  useful  as  a  method  of  studying  students'  in- 
terests. In  order  to  investigate  the  possibilities  of  this  tech- 
nique, a  questionnaire  was  developed  which  listed  three 
hundred  activities  which  students  were  asked  to  mark  "Like/' 
"Indifferent,"  or  "Dislike."  The  questionnaire  sampled  activ- 
ities which  were  expected  to  reveal  interests  fostered  by 
school  subjects  as  well  as  interests  in  certain  types  of  rela- 
tionships with  other  people. 

Method  of  Selecting  Items  for  the  Questionnaire 

The  list  of  activities  in  the  questionnaire  was  prepared  by 
staff  members  who  were  concerned  with  evaluation  instru- 
ments in  the  various  academic  fields.  Each  staff  member  ex- 
amined current  textbooks  and  analyzed  classroom  activities 
in  order  to  identify  activities  which  might  indicate  an  inter- 
est developed  by  his  field.  Each  activity  submitted  was  ex- 
amined critically  by  the  entire  staff  to  make  sure  that  it 
fairly  represented  the  interests  developed  by  these  fields 
and  that  it  was  actually  carried  on  by  students.  All  activities 
in  which  a  student  was  apt  to  engage  as  a  part  or  result  of 
his  work  in  several  subjects  were  either  eliminated  or  so 
sharpened  that  they  became  more  clearly  related  to  one  field 
only.  An  attempt  was  also  made  to  include  items  indicative 
of  varying  degrees  or  different  depths  of  interest  in  a  field: 
from  easy  and  attractive  activities  to  those  involving  con- 
siderable effort,  hours  of  study,  a  high  degree  of  proficiency, 
etc. 

The  items  thus  selected  were  arranged  in  random  order 
in  an  inventory  which  was  used  experimentally  in  several 
grades  in  20  of  the  schools  participating  in  the  Study.  On 
the  basis  of  the  experience  of  staff  members  who  interpreted 
the  findings  to  the  faculties  of  these  schools  and  in  the  light 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  339 

of  criticisms  of  teachers  who  felt  that  some  of  the  areas  had 
not  been  adequately  sampled  or  that  the  vocabulary  of  some 
of  the  items  was  confusing,  the  questionnaire  was  revised. 
This  revision  was  also  based  upon  an  item  analysis  and  re- 
liability studies  of  the  responses  of  250  boys  and  250  girls 
in  typical  high  schools. 

The  Revised  Form:  Interest  Index  8.22. 

The  revised  form  of  the  questionnaire  consists  of  only  200 
items  and  thus  can  be  given  in  one  study  period  in  a  junior 
or  senior  high  school  The  areas  selected  for  this  question- 
naire are:  social  studies,  biology,  physical  science,  English., 
foreign  languages,  mathematics,  business,  home  economics, 
industrial  arts,  fine  arts,  music,  and  sports.  In  addition  to  these 
areas,  two  larger  categories  which  cut  across  most  of  them 
were  included:  reading  and  manipulative.  These  two  cate- 
gories are  composed  of  items  which  appear  in  the  above  12 
categories  and  involve  either  reading  or  handwork.  Thus, 
for  instance,  "To  make  and  classify  a  collection  of  insects'*  is 
classified  under  biology  and  also  under  the  manipulative 
category.  The  item:  "To  read  such  books  as  The  Life  of 
Pasteur,  Microbe  Hunters,  Arrowsmith,  etc."  is  classified 
under  biology  and  also  under  reading.  There  are  16  activ- 
ities in  each  of  11  of  the  above  categories,  24  in  social 
studies,  35  in  reading,  and  38  in  manipulative.  The  sort  of 
items  included  is  indicated  by  the  following  sample.  The 
parenthesis  after  each  item  indicates  how  it  is  classified  in 
scoring. 

1.  To  write  stories.  (English) 

3.  To  go  on  trips  with  a  class  to  find  out  about  conditions 
such  as  housing,  unemployment,  etc.,  in  various  parts  of 
your  community.  (Social  Studies) 

5.  To  visit  stores,  factories,  offices,  and  other  places  of  busi- 
ness to  find  out  how  their  work  is  carried  on.  ( Business ) 


340        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

6.  To  correspond  in  a  foreign  language  with  a  student  In 
another  country.  (Foreign  Language) 

7.  To  play  baseball  (either  hard  or  soft  ball).  (Sports)9 

14.  To  learn  how  to  cook  well  ( in  camp  or  at  home ) .  ( Home 
Economics )   ( Manipulative ) 

15.  To  sing  in  a  glee  club,  chorus,  or  choir.  ( Music ) 

16.  To  put  eggs  into  an  incubator  and  open  one  every  day  to 
see  how  the  chick  develops.  (Biology)  (Manipulative) 

17.  To  sketch  or  paint.  ( Fine  Arts )  ( Manipulative ) 

21.  To    make    chemical    compounds.     (Physical    Sciences) 
( Manipulative ) 

22.  To  make  things  of  wood,  metal,  etc.   (Industrial  Arts) 
( Manipulative ) 

23.  To  do  the  arithmetic  necessary  in  planning  trips  or  parties 
for  the  class.  ( Mathematics ) 

Interpretation  of  the  Questionnaire 

As  indicated  on  the  data  sheet  on  page  341,  the  scores  give 
the  per  cent  of  each  student's  "likes"  and  "dislikes"  in  each 
of  the  categories  and  the  per  cent  of  his  "likes"  and  "dislikes" 
for  the  whole  questionnaire:  i.e.,  for  the  200  items.  The  per 
cent  of  items  marked  "Indifferent"  is  not  recorded  but  may 
be  obtained  by  subtracting  the  sum  of  the  "likes"  and  "dis- 
likes" in  each  category  from  100.  The  Data  Sheet  also  gives 
the  lowest  and  highest  scores  and  the  group  median  for 
"likes"  and  "dislikes"  in  each  category. 

This  instrument  is  so  simple  in  construction  that  it  has 
been  found  that  teachers  learn  to  interpret  it  in  a  short  time. 
As  with  most  instruments,  persons  with  greater  experience 
may  get  more  from  it  than  persons  with  limited  experience. 
As  long  as  the  interpreter  confines  himself  to  what  he  may 
learn  about  the  general  direction  of  a  student's  interests,  the 
interpretation  is  simple  and  rather  reliable.  If,  however,  a 
person  attempts  to  find  what  effect  a  given  course  offered 

9  Sports  were  not  classified  as  "Manipulative"  because  they  were  so  nearly 
universal  interests  that  they  did  not  identify  students  whose  interests  were 
predominantly  manipulative. 


H     H 


^  i' 

«     I 
w    i- 


1                M 

^  01 

o  o 
in  01      i 

T3   ^ 

1 

£4'3 

OX              X               'O 

*t  Ol 

m  T-t 

f*  "r>   rj 

IS  "t        *•<  O        CO  CO        X  O 

CO    CO              OJ                        "t    TH              is. 

^  o 

n-  oj 

5J: 

X 

0  O       C\O       'Or-        CrO 
o          <-<  in      m  to      m  »-< 

Oj  O 

^t    TH 

o 

OOC        "f  O        "J-  0        GO 

00              0               C\              C 

?E5 

TH    SO 

0 

£  £ 

2    t* 

s< 

01  to      co          co  oi      in 

TH  m 

CO  Ol 

m  xj- 
in 

*-  « 

CNO      in  o      oo      coo 

T-I  TH      oa          so          x 

s* 

m  o 

o    . 

soo      coo      com      c\c\ 

to  01 

VD 

;   li 

rtm         T-HO         cOsO         *<tr  O 
TO!         CO              T-I  iO         Cs 

vO 

VO  Cv 
0 

IS* 

sOO        OO        »^C%        t-*O 

m      un          co  r-*      co 

?s 

CO  0 

oj  m 

ill 

co      co          r*-          T-I  t-( 

2? 

CS    TH 

c  !s 
w- 

•^  O        OCO        insO        rj-  to 

\o  m 

CO  01 

in  TH 

fl£ 

sOCN        V2O        OO        ^"O 

CO  \O 

so 

m  TH 

•1? 

OO        COO        r-*Q        OO 

m      co          co          o 

o  m 

CO  cO 

•si 

J'-4  T3 
1 

COCO         —  i  O         COCO         T«00 

m      oj          m  r-«      r- 

N  m 

CO  r-f 

CO  vO 

C3      *      " 

s  ;  * 

rf    . 

'  o    '    "        '    ' 

T3     *     " 

h          :.S     •     •          •     *T3     •     . 

o        -2    •    -j3    '    •  j-i    •    . 

%      -U  :  •  a  :  •  g  :  • 

U(4Q  wh-lQ  «h4Q  n^Q 

00                   0                  M 
H-J               (-5                K 

o    ;    • 
>»»-3  Q 

w 

•3JO 
b 

342.        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

in  the  school  had  upon  the  change  of  interests  of  a  group 
of  students,  certain  complications  arise,  and  rather  advanced 
statistical  treatment  of  the  data  becomes  a  necessary  condi- 
tion for  arriving  at  valid  conclusions.  In  the  following  pres- 
entation of  the  method  of  interpreting  results,  the  relatively 
simple  methods  will  be  described. 

Each  student's  scores  are  interpreted  in  relation  to  the 
group  median  and  group  range  and  in  the  light  of  his  own 
scores  on  other  categories,  e.g.,  his  own  pattern  of  scores. 
The  examination  of  scores  of  a  student  in  relation  to  the 
group  median  and  the  range  for  each  of  the  categories  of 
summary  will  indicate  in  which  areas  the  student  has  high 
or  low  Kkes  or  dislikes,  thus  establishing  tentatively  the  de- 
viate points  in  his  preferences  or  dislikes.  Thus,  comparing 
Chester's  scores  with  the  group  medians,  one  notices  high 
dislikes  in  many  areas  and  high  likes  only  in  three,  whereas 
Howard  has  high  likes  in  most  areas  and  few  dislikes  in  any 
of  them. 

One  may  further  note  the  relative  frequency  of  the  sig- 
nificant likes  and  dislikes  and  the  areas  in  which  they  occur. 
At  this  point  it  is  helpful  to  examine  the  scores  in  terms  of 
certain  broad  common  elements  in  the  pattern  of  likes  and 
dislikes  to  locate  the  significant  tendencies  and  character- 
istics of  the  student's  pattern  of  interest.  Thus  a  frequency 
of  high  likes  in  English,  social  studies,  foreign  language,  and 
reading  indicates  high  preference  for  verbal  activities.  High 
likes  in  biology,  physical  sciences,  mathematics,  and  indus- 
trial arts  indicate  interest  in  activities  involving  things  and 
precision  manipulation.  An  artistic  pattern  is  suggested  by 
high  likes  in  music,  fine  arts,  industrial  arts,  and  home  eco- 
nomics. High  likes  in  sports,  business,  industrial  arts,  home 
economics,  and  manipulative  activities  would  suggest  an  in- 
clination toward  practical  activities.  If  likes  in  one  pattern 
are  accompanied  with  dislikes  in  a  contrasting  one,  a  further 
reinforcement  of  a  personal  selection  of  activities  is  indi- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  343 

cated.  Thus,  If  fairly  high  likes  in  English,  social  studies, 
foreign  language,  and  reading  are  accompanied  by  dislikes 
In  biology,  physical  sciences,  and  mathematics,  a  fairly  strong 
case  of  verbal  Interests  is  indicated. 

It  must  be  noted,  however,  that  these  general  patterns  are 
nothing  more  than  suggestions  for  exploring  general  tend- 
encies. The  areas  liked  and  disliked  group  themselves  in  in- 
numerably diversified  ways  In  any  Individual  case,  and  it 
Is  therefore  neither  possible  to  describe  all  of  the  possibili- 
ties, nor  wise  to  attempt  to  define  any  one  pattern  precisely 
or  to  follow  its  implications  In  any  one  individual  case 
slavishly. 

Applying  this  method  to  the  scores  given  above,  one  may 
note  that  Chester  has  a  negative  reaction  to  all  academic 
activities,  verbal  and  scientific  alike.  Music  Is  the  only  area 
of  high  positive  interest  to  him.  In  contrast,  Joseph  has  a 
high  interest  in  academic  activities  of  all  types,  but  shows 
high  dislikes  in  such  practical  areas  as  home  economics  and 
business,  and  sports.  Josephine's  preferences  ran  predomi- 
nantly in  the  direction  of  verbal  activities,  with  an  additional 
interest  in  music  and  business,  with  no  dislikes  in  any  area 
but  sports.  Howard's  Interest  pattern  is  so  catholic  as  to 
arouse  a  suspicion  of  lack  of  discrimination. 

In  addition  to  examining  the  scores  of  a  student  in  rela- 
tion to  those  of  other  students  in  his  group  (i.e.,  examining 
them  on  the  background  of  the  group's  scale),  one  must  also 
examine  these  scores  In  terms  of  the  student's  own  scale.  Some 
students  have  high  likes  in  many  categories,  others  have  low 
likes  in  most  categories,  or  generally  high  dislikes.  The  total 
score  on  'likes"  and  "dislikes"  is  indicative  of  the  general 
tendency  of  the  student  in  terms  of  which  his  scores  have 
to  be  examined.  For  instance,  a  student  may  be  one  of  the 
highest  in  the  group  in  liking  music;  if,  however,  all  of  his 
likes  are  high,  and  on  his  scale  music  is  one  of  the  lowest, 


344        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

a  different  meaning  is  attributed  to  his  score  than  if  we  con- 
sider it  only  with  reference  to  the  group  score. 

Thus  in  the  case  of  Josephine,  the  score  of  50  on  disliking 
sports  assumes  great  significance,  because  of  the  general  ab- 
sence of  dislike  reactions.  Similarly,  Chester's  high  dislike 
of  mathematics,  being  part  of  a  pattern  of  disliking  all  aca- 
demic activities.,  needs  to  be  viewed  as  a  part  of  this  total 
negative  reaction,  rather  than  as  a  specific  reaction  to  mathe- 
matics. The  fact  that  Howard's  likes  are  uniformly  high  re- 
quires an  investigation  to  see  whether  these  are  genuine  in- 
terests or  whether  some  such  extraneous  factor  as  lack  of 
discrimination  combined  with  a  benevolent  disposition  is  not 
playing  a  part. 

One  thing  to  be  remembered  in  interpreting  these  scores 
is  that  interests  are  personal,  and  therefore  a  certain  degree 
of  uniqueness  is  both  to  be  expected  and  desired.  Therefore 
both  the  range  and  the  pattern  of  interests  should  be  judged 
in  personal  terms  rather  than  by  general  norms.  Thus,  while 
a  certain  breadth  of  interests  usually  is  desirable,  it  would 
be  a  mistake  to  assume  that  high  likes  in  all  areas  indicated 
in  the  questionnaire  is  to  be  expected  or  is  even  desirable. 
Similarly,  while  negative  reactions  on  the  whole  may  be 
considered  undesirable,  one  should  expect  individuals  with 
selective  interests  to  react  negatively  to  some  activities,  while 
showing  high  positive  reactions  to  others. 

In  examining  group  patterns,  similar  methods  need  to  be 
applied.  Thus  one  may  note  the  areas  in  which  there  are 
tendencies  toward  positive  or  negative  reactions.  This  can 
be  observed  by  comparing  the  medians  with  the  medians 
of  other  groups  or  by  noting  the  frequency  of  high  likes  and 
high  dislikes  in  any  given  area.  By  this  method  one  may 
note  the  prevalence  of  preferences  in  such  verbal  areas  as 
social  sciences,  English,  and  the  like,  or  negative  reactions 
to  areas  of  artistic  activities.  There  also  it  is  important  to 
bear  in  mind  that  a  valid  interpretation  cannot  be  secured 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  345 

by  simply  noting  the  areas  of  high  likes  or  high  dislikes. 
These  observations  must  be  scrutinized  in  terms  of  the  total 
pattern  as  well  as  in  terms  of  other  data  on  the  same  group. 
Thus  a  relatively  high  preference  for  physical  sciences  has 
one  meaning  when  this  is  the  only  area  of  high  preference., 
and  a  different  one  when  it  is  one  of  many.  High  preference 
for  foreign  language  in  a  group  with  no  organized  experience 
in  this  field  and  no  special  aptitude  in  this  direction  usually 
suggests  wishful  thinking  while  the  same  pattern  for  a  group 
with  verbal  ability  and  experience  in  this  area  can  be  taken 
to  mean  a  thoughtful  and  actual  interest. 

Value  of  the  Questionnaire  to  the  Counselor  or  Teacher 

The  counselor  will  be  interested  chiefly  in  the  configura- 
tion of  the  student's  per  cent  of  likes,  indifferences,  and  dis- 
likes in  the  various  categories.  The  important  point  to  note 
here  is  whether  the  picture  is  consistent  with  what  is  known 
about  the  student's  inclinations  and  interests,  and  if  some 
inconsistency  is  discovered,  this  lead  should  be  investigated. 
When  considered  in  connection  with  other  information  avail- 
able, it  should  be  helpful  in  academic  or  vocational  guidance. 
Thus  the  preference  pattern  of  the  student  suggests  the 
areas  which  can  be  utilized  for  his  further  development.  If 
it  seems  broad  enough,  and  sensible  enough  for  a  given  stu- 
dent, it  suggests  the  line  of  activities  for  him  to  carry  on 
and  by  which  he  will  be  enriched.  If  an  undue  narrowness 
is  indicated,  the  spots  of  positive  reactions  can  be  mobilized 
as  a  springboard  for  expansion  of  interests.  Thus  high  inter- 
est in  physical  sciences  would  suggest  that  reading  in  that 
area  could  be  used  to  develop  interest  in  reading,  should 
that  be  lacking.  Similarly,  the  pattern  of  negative  responses 
should  suggest  to  teachers  the  areas  in  which  remedial  action 
may  be  needed  or  in  which  direct  pressure  should  not  be 
applied.  Thus  it  would  be  futile  to  try  to  develop  good  work 
habits  in  English  in  the  case  of  an  individual  with  negative 


346        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

responses  to  this  area  until  a  more  positive  reaction  is  devel- 
oped. Other  types  of  activities  should  be  used  to  this  end. 

Since  motivation  is  an  important  factor,  the  evidence  in 
interests  is  also  useful  in  explaining  other  facts  about  the 
students,  such  as  high  or  low  achievement  in  various  areas,, 
behavior  in  class,  or  activities  in  thinking. 

The  classroom  teacher  may  be  interested  also  in  the  kinds 
of  activities  which  a  given  student  likes  or  dislikes  or  to 
which  he  Is  indifferent,  within  particular  subject-matter 
fields.  Specific  responses  to  Individual  items  may  be  exam- 
ined for  this  purpose  and  new  or  more  subtle  patterns  than 
those  revealed  in  the  category  scores  may  become  evident. 
It  should  be  noted  that  the  emphasis  in  this  type  of  exam- 
ination of  responses  is  not  on  the  amount  of  interest  which 
a  student  may  have,  but  on  the  nature  of  that  interest.  One 
may  find,  for  instance,  on  examining  the  scores  that  a  stu- 
dent is  at  the  group  median  in  liking  biology;  on  his  own 
scale  biology  is  neither  particularly  high  nor  low;  but  when 
his  specific  responses  in  this  category  are  examined,  one 
may  find  that  his  liking  is  centered  on  Items  which  have  to 
do  with  people,  human  physiology,  health,  etc.  This  knowl- 
edge should  be  of  value  to  the  teacher. 

The  classroom  teacher  may  also  make  a  similar  use  of  the 
responses  of  the  group.  The  evidence  on  prevailing  prefer- 
ences is  helpful  in  planning  classroom  activities,  areas  to  be 
studied  or  the  approach  to  be  taken.  Thus  exploration  of 
printed  material  may  be  a  very  good  way  of  studying  a 
given  topic  for  one  group,  while  other  sources  must  be  used 
with  groups  who  have  a  high  negative  reaction  to  verbal 
activities.  Diagnosis  of  group  preferences  and  dislikes  also 
points  to  gaps  in  the  curriculum  to  be  filled,  or  unwise  em- 
phases in  the  present  curriculum.  Thus  in  one  school  an  ex- 
tremely high  negative  preference  was  shown  for  art  activ- 
ities. The  examination  of  their  curriculum  revealed  that  this 
group  had  no  opportunity  in  this  field  and  could  well  profit 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  347 

from  it.  In  another  case,  an  unusually  high  negative  reaction 
to  writing  was  traced  to  a  large  amount  of  required  writing 
resulting  from  separate  assignments  by  several  teachers, 
each  of  whom  was  unaware  of  the  total  load  on  the  students. 
As  in  case  of  the  individuals,  the  hypotheses  regarding 
constructive  action  to  be  taken  cannot  be  formulated  validly 
by  using  the  data  from  this  questionnaire  alone.  These  data 
are  descriptive  and  as  such  are  helpful  only  in  suggesting 
hunches  regarding  the  causes  of  preferences  or  of  dislikes, 
yet  for  a  remedial  or  constructive  program  it  is  necessary  to 
have  a  fairly  good  idea  of  the  cause  of  the  interest  pattern 
shown.  Therefore  it  is  imperative  to  consider  these  data  in 
context  of  other  evidence  before  decisions  are  made  regard- 
ing what  to  do  about  an  individual  or  a  group. 

Factors  Influencing  Accuracy  of  Results 

The  usefulness  and  accuracy  of  results  of  this  instrument 
depend  on  at  least  two  factors:  the  degree  to  which  the  items 
sample  activities  which  are  affected  by  the  curriculum  in 
the  school  in  which  the  instrument  is  used,  and  the  sincerity 
of  the  response  made  by  the  students. 

The  first  of  these  may  be  determined  by  a  careful  exam- 
ination of  the  specific  items  by  the  teachers  who  expect  to 
use  the  instrument.  If  it  is  found  that  the  items  do  not  sam- 
ple activities  which  reveal  interests  that  they  are  trying  to 
develop,  or  activities  to  which  they  would  like  to  know  their 
students*  reactions,  a  similar  instrument  can  easily  be  con- 
structed which  includes  both. 

The  responses  of  the  students  will  be  most  sincere  if  the 
instrument  is  not  regarded  as  a  "test"  in  which  high  scores 
are  desirable.  If  the  students  recognize  that  the  information 
which  they  convey  through  the  questionnaire  may  be  helpful 
in  planning  class  work,  their  cooperation  can  be  readily 
enlisted. 

In  making  interpretations  it  should  be  remembered  that 


348        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

in  this  instrument  the  student  is  asked  to  tell  how  he  feels 
about  certain  activities:  whether  he  likes  them,  is  indifferent 
to  them,  or  dislikes  them.  These  feelings  are  not  necessarily 
an  index  of  his  performance  in  any  of  the  areas  sampled. 
A  student  may  do  poor  work  in  class  and  still  like  many  of 
the  activities  listed.  Likewise  a  student  may  do  very  well  in 
class  and  dislike  many  of  the  items.  The  reasons  for  this 
seeming  discrepancy  may  be  worth  exploring. 

For  certain  types  of  interpretations  it  is  advisable  to  com- 
pute averages  for  boys  and  for  girls  separately,  although  this 
greatly  extends  the  scope  of  the  statistics  which  are  needed. 
The  mean,  standard  deviation,  and  coefficient  of  reliability 
of  each  category  for  the  "like"  scores  from  one  sample  popu- 
lation of  542  eleventh  grade  students  are  given  in  Appendix 
V.  Reliability  coefficients  computed  by  the  Kuder-Richardson 
formula  for  this  sample  range  from  .79  to  .92.  The  median 
coefficient  is  .89?  and  only  three  categories  are  below  .85. 

A  more  thorough  discussion  of  the  interpretation  and  pos- 
sible uses  of  this  technique  will  be  found  in  the  next  chapter. 
It  will  also  be  seen  there  that  the  study  of  interests  can  be 
used  for  a  different  purpose,  namely  the  evaluation  of  per- 
sonal and  social  adjustment.  The  validity  of  the  instrument 
will  be  treated  in  this  connection. 


Chapter  VI 

EVALUATION  OF  PERSONAL  AND  SOCIAL 
ADJUSTMENT 

4$&gfr4&^^ 

DISCUSSION  OF  THE  OBJECTIVE 

History  of  the  Objective 

One  of  the  concerns  voiced  by  the  schools  cooperating  in 
the  Eight- Year  Study  was  that  of  promoting  the  personal 
and  social  adjustment  of  their  students.  In  an  effort  to  clarify 
the  meaning  of  these  terms  and  to  devise  ways  in  which  at 
least  a  few  of  the  aspects  of  personal  and  social  adjustment 
might  be  appraised,  groups  of  teachers  and  of  specialists  in 
various  pertinent  fields  met  together.  The  Committee  on  the 
Study  of  Adolescents  of  the  Commission  on  Secondary 
School  Cuiriculum  of  the  Progressive  Education  Associa- 
tion, for  example,  provided  special  help  in  attempting  to 
clarify  the  meaning  of  this  objective.  The  study  of  the  ways 
in  which  the  schools  were  gathering  and  recording  evidence 
of  students'  adjustment  revealed  that  many  techniques  of 
appraising  personality  and  social  adjustment,  though  they 
suffered  from  one  shortcoming  or  another,  were  of  promise. 
The  work  of  the  regional  committees  on  anecdotal  records 
was  especiaEy  helpful  in  pointing  to  ways  in  which  teachers 
might  collect  evidence  which  would  give  some  insight  into 
the  personality  problems  of  students. 

Urged  by  the  cooperating  schools  to  devise  more  prac- 
ticable means  of  appraising  personal  and  social  adjustment, 
the  Evaluation  Staff  began  an  extensive  study  of  this  problem 
of  appraisal  early  in  1938.  Before  the  results  of  this  study  are 
presented,  however,  it  will  be  necessary  to  attempt  to  dis- 

349 


350         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

tinguish  between  personal  and  social  adjustment,  to  clarify 
the  concepts  of  adjustment,  and  to  attempt  to  set  up  a  list  of 
criteria  for  a  method  of  appraisal. 

Differentiation  between  Personal  and  Social  Adjustment 

Personal  adjustment  is  thought  of  broadly  as  including  the 
subjective  feelings  of  the  individual,  such  as  feelings  of  ade- 
quacy and  inadequacy,  personal  happiness  and  unhappiness, 
the  adjustive  reactions  of  the  individual,  the  presence  or 
absence  of  inner  conflicting  tendencies.  Social  adjustment  is 
thought  of  as  being  directed  toward  the  adequacy  and  effec- 
tiveness of  a  person's  interaction  with  other  people  in  face- 
to-face  situations.  Relationships  with  age-mates,  older  and 
younger  people,  with  the  opposite  sex,  etc.,  are  included 
under  this  heading.  It  also  includes  the  person's  attitudes  to 
the  mores  and  standards  of  the  group  of  which  he  is  a 
member.  It  is  recognized  that  the  division  between  personal 
and  social  adjustment  is,  in  some  respects,  an  artificial  one 
and  that  they  should  be  thought  of  as  being  intimately  con- 
nected and  interrelated  and  as  representing  two  aspects  of 
die  emotional  adjustment  of  a  person  to  his  environment. 

Discussion  of  "Adjustment" 

There  appears  to  be  considerable  difference  of  opinion 
about  what  constitutes  adjustment.  Because  this  term  lacks 
clarity  and  may  have  different  meanings  to  different  persons, 
it  is  necessary  to  attempt  to  clarify  the  particular  concept  of 
adjustment  which  underlies  the  study  to  be  reported  in  this 
chapter. 

Broadly  speaking,  the  investigators  regard  personality  as  a 
dynamic  structure,  which  must  be  viewed  as  a  whole,  rather 
than  as  a  collection  of  parts.  Since  personality  is  viewed  as  a 
product  of  the  interaction  of  forces  within  the  individual 
and  the  interaction  between  the  individual  and  his  surround- 
ings, it  must  be  seen  in  the  light  of  his  past  history  and 
against  the  background  of  his  present  environment. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  351 

The  point  of  view  underlying  this  investigation  may  be 
clarified  somewhat  by  indicating  how  it  differs  from  the  ap- 
proach which  has  governed  certain  other  attempts  in  this 
field.  The  idea  that  certain  behaviors,  in  and  by  themselves, 
are  indicative  of  "good"  or  "poor"  adjustment  seems  to  be 
rather  widely  accepted.  This  point  of  view  has  been  made 
the  basis  of  a  number  of  attempts  to  appraise  students'  ad- 
justment. The  procedure  involves  the  construction  of  a  be- 
havior scale  which  lists  sample  statements  of  both  "good" 
and  "bad"  behaviors.  The  mere  counting  of  these  behaviors 
is  expected  to  give  an  adjustment  score  or  index  for  the 
student1 

Such  classification  of  behaviors  as  "good"  or  "bad"  in 
themselves  is  a  relatively  simple  attack  upon  the  problem. 
It  leaves  out  important  factors  wrhich  need  to  be  considered 
prior  to  arriving  at  a  judgment  regarding  the  person's  adjust- 
ment or  maladjustment.  Two  major  criticisms  may  be  made 
of  this  concept  of  adjustment. 

It  is  an  oversimplification  which  omits  consideration  of 
the  individual,  his  motivation,  surrounding  temporal  and 
environmental  conditions,  etc.  The  courts,  for  example,  do 
not  hold  that  certain  acts  constitute  a  crime  everywhere  and 
under  all  circumstances.  Before  evaluating  an  act,  a  careful 
study  is  made  of  the  motivation  of  the  indicted  person,  con- 
sideration is  given  to  the  extenuating  circumstances,  etc.  The 
final  judgment  is  also  made  in  the  light  of  the  history  of  the 
behavior  of  the  person.  Likewise,  when  parents  or  teachers 
judge  the  behavior  of  children,  they  are  aware  of  the  neces- 
sity of  attempting  to  determine  not  only  what  was  done  but 
also  why  it  was  done,  under  what  circumstances  the  behavior 
occurred,  and  the  like. 

Furthermore,  such  a  classification  of  behaviors  as  "good" 

1  For  a  discussion  of  the  present  status  of  personality  measurement  and 
of  the  difficulties  involved,  the  reader  is  referred  to  Chapters  I  and  II  of 
Fulcra  of  Conflict,  Douglas  Spencer  (New  York,  World  Book  Co.,  Yonkers- 
on-Hudson,  1939). 


35^      ^  ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

and  "bad"  in  themselves  suffers  from  another  oversimplifica- 
tion— that  of  not  discriminating  between  the  condition  and 
the  sijmptom  of  the  condition.  This  may  be  clarified  by  the 
following  analogy:  an  infection  may  be  said  to  be  a  condition 
or  a  state  of  an  organism,  whereas  the  high  fever  which  is 
apt  to  accompany  the  infection,  is  an  outcome  or  symptom 
of  the  infection.  Although  the  fever  is  indicative  of  an  infec- 
tion and  therefore  represents  something  undesirable,  never- 
theless in  itself  and  under  the  circumstances  it  is  believed  to 
be  a  desirable  adjustive  reaction  of  the  organism  to  the  in- 
fection. In  making  lists  of  undesirable  behaviors  there  is  a 
tendency  to  use  both  kinds  of  behaviors — those  which  may 
be  thought  of  as  "conditions"  as  well  as  those  which  may  be 
thought  of  as  "symptoms" — and  to  neglect  the  fact  that  they 
are  phenomena  of  an  entirely  different  order  and  that  they 
have  to  be  evaluated  differently. 

Thus,  there  appear  to  be  cogent  reasons  against  beginning 
a  program  of  appraisal  of  adjustment  with  the  focus  of  the 
inquiry  centering  on  an  attempt  to  determine  whether  the 
adjustment  of  the  individual  is  desirable  or  not.  Determina- 
tion of  what  specific  behaviors  may  constitute  "desirable 
adjustment"  for  a  given  individual  is  legitimate  only  at  the 
end  of  a  study  of  a  personality,  when  the  judgment  can  be 
based  on  a  great  many  considerations.  Even  then  it  is  apt  to 
be  a  value  judgment.  Obtaining  a  picture  revealing  how  the 
individual  functions,  what  adjustive  devices  he  employs, 
seems  to  be  of  greater  value. 

Another  rather  commonly  accepted  point  of  view  is  that 
adjustment  consists  largely  in  conformity  to  social  standards 
and  demands.  This  point  of  view  neglects  the  importance  of 
adjustment  in  terms  of  oneself,  i.e.,  the  importance  of  being 
able  to  handle  satisfactorily  one's  own  impulses  and  strivings, 
the  importance  of  being  consistent  with  oneself.  It  must  be 
borne  in  mind  that  the  lack  of  this  type  of  adjustment  ex- 
presses itself  frequently  in  a  variety  of  serious  overt  or  veiled 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  353 

emotional  disturbances.2  In  this  connection  the  following 
may  be  said  regarding  what  must  be  included  in  thinking 
about  adjustment.  On  the  one  hand,  we  have  the  individual 
with  his  native  needs,  impulses,  and  drives  which  seek  satis- 
faction, and  which  undergo  certain  changes  with  age.  On 
the  other  hand,  we  have  society  which  has  its  needs  and 
which  makes  certain  demands  on  the  individual.  These  de- 
mands on  the  individual  van*  in  different  cultures  and  de- 
pend on  the  age  and  sex  of  the  individual,  social  status  of 
the  family,  and  similar  factors.  Maladjustment  of  the  indi- 
vidual thus  may  be,  broadly  speaking,  one  of  two  kinds.  In 
one  instance  die  individual  may  comply  to  such  a  high  de- 
gree to  the  demands  of  society  that  his  native  drives  become 
thwarted,  cramped,  and  distorted.  In  such  cases  the  indi- 
vidual's behaviors  with  regard  to  society  are  acceptable  to 
society,  but  he  pays  too  high  a  price  for  them  himself.  In 
such  an  event  some  neurotic  condition,  accompanied  by  a 
good  deal  of  anxiety  and  considerable  personal  unhappiness, 
may  be  found  in  him.  In  the  second  type  of  maladjustment 
the  individual  rebels  against  society,  its  demands  and  re- 
strictions. In  extreme  cases  such  a  person  may  suffer  from 
society's  ostracism  or  other  types  of  punishment,  but  his  diffi- 
culty, nevertheless,  will  be  largely  one  of  social  adjustment. 
This  is,  of  course,  an  oversimplification  of  the  picture,  yet 
for  a  broad  frame  of  reference  it  is  sufficiently  correct.  It 
permits  us  to  see  that  in  general  optimum  adjustment  may 
be  thought  of  as  a  compromise  between  the  individual  and 
the  group  to  which  he  belongs,  in  which  each  party  adjusts 
to  the  other  to  a  certain  extent  in  order  to  avoid  conflicts 

2  The  fact  that  educators  are  prone  to  regard  as  the  most  serious  prob- 
lems those  of  non-conformity,  and  to  underestimate  the  importance  o£  prob- 
lems which  are  not  brought  to  light  through  anti-social  behavior,  has  been 
demonstrated  in  a  number  of  studies.  The  best  known  of  these  is  E.  K. 
Wickman,  "Children's  Behavior  and  Teachers'  Attitudes,"  The  Common- 
wealth Fund/  1928. 


354        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

within  the  individual  or  clashes  between  the  individual  and 
the  social  group. 

Desirable  adjustment  for  the  individual  may  then  be 
thought  of  as  a  process  of  maturation  and  adaptation  during 
which  he  is  able  to  integrate  successfully  (i.e.,  without  neu- 
rotic compromises  or  anti-social  acts)  his  native  impulses 
and  drives  with  those  expectations  or  demands  which  are 
imposed  upon  him  (with  reference  to  his  age,  sex,  social 
status,  race,  etc. )  by  the  group  to  which  he  belongs. 

The  above  discussion  leads  to  the  formulation  of  the  fol- 
lowing point  of  view: 

1.  The  adjustment  of  the  individual  must  be  conceived  as 
a  complex  of  feelings  and  behaviors  which  are  meaningful 
only  when  seen  in  relationship  to  each  other,  rather  than  as 
a  series  of  discrete  behaviors  regarded  as  meaningful  in 
themselves. 

2.  This  complex  of  feelings  and  behaviors  must  be  evalu- 
ated in  terms  of  the  status  of  the  individual  (i.e.,  his  age, 
sex,  position  in  society,  etc.).  The  same  behavior  may  be 
evaluated  differently  when  observed  in  the  case  of  a  six-year- 
old  and  a  sixteen-year-old,  in  a  boy  or  In  a  girl. 

3.  The  adjustment  of  the  individual  must  be  considered 
in  terms  of  the  relationships  between  his  own  strivings,  pur- 
poses, and  past  conditionings,  and  also  in  terms  of  the  rela- 
tion of  these  to  the  demands  or  expectations  of  society.  His 
adjustment  must  be  viewed  as  a  process  rather  than  a  state. 

DISCUSSION  OF  THE  TECHNIQUE  OF  APPRAISAL  OF  THE 
OBJECTIVE 

Desirable  Characteristics  of  an  Instrument  for 
Appraising  Personal  and  Social  Adjustment 

Being  well  aware  of  the  impossibility  of  evolving  any 
single  device  for  appraising  all  of  the  pertinent  factors  which 
need  to  be  considered  in  the  evaluation  of  the  life  adjust- 
ment of  an  individual,  the  staff  set  out  to  explore  feasible 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  355 

ways  of  appraising  at  least  a  few  of  these  factors.  During  this 
process  of  exploration  an  effort  was  made  to  define  the  gen- 
eral characteristics  which  were  felt  to  be  desirable  in  an 
evaluation  Instrument  for  this  purpose. 

1.  If  should  be  a  technique  applicable  to  a  large  number 
of  students  at  one  time. 

Since  the  paper-and-pencil  technique  is  much  more  eco- 
nomical, as  far  as  the  examiner's  time  Is  concerned,  than 
the  interview,  anecdotal  record,  etc.,  and  thus  permits  testing 
a  larger  number  of  students  at  the  same  time,  and  since  it 
rules  out  one  of  the  possible  subjective  factors — the  biases 
of  the  observer — this  technique  was  thought  to  be  preferable. 

2.  The  evidence  obtained  from  different  individuals 
should  be  comparable. 

It  was  felt  that  the  form  in  which  the  data  were  to  be  col- 
lected should  be  such  that  there  would  be  an  opportunity 
for  comparison  of  results.  To  the  extent  that  the  response- 
pattern  of  one  Individual  can  be  compared  with  that  of  an- 
other or  that  of  a  group,  it  should  be  possible  to  discover 
those  ways  in  which  he  is  similar  or  dissimilar  and  thus  gain 
further  insight  into  how  his  personality  Is  organized.  Com- 
parability of  results  might  also  lead  to  investigation  of  group 
phenomena. 

3.  The  technique  should  be  indirect. 

In  devising  an  appraisal  Instrument  It  was  considered  very 
important  that  the  approach  be  relatively  indirect.  One  diffi- 
culty which  Is  Implicit  in  inventories  which  attempt  to  get 
at  the  individual's  private  and  intimate  feelings  is  the  fear 
and  anxiety  which  most  people  experience  when  they  feel 
that  they  are  being  "tested"  or  evaluated  personally.  Whereas 
they  frequently  seem  able  to  consider  certain  abilities  as 
actually  extraneous  to  themselves  and  are,  therefore,  not 


356         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

threatened  when  an  attempt  is  made  to  measure  these  abil- 
ities, they  usually  feel  defensive  about  obvious  attempts  to 
get  at  their  private  feelings.  The  anxiety  aroused  may  be  so 
great  as  to  completely  inhibit  or  invalidate  the  response. 
Thus,  it  was  felt  that  the  instrument  should  not  be  obviously 
a  "Personality  Test"  but  rather  should  attempt  to  appraise 
personal  and  social  adjustment  in  a  more  indirect  manner. 

4.  The  subject  should  be  called  on  to  express  himself 
rather  than  to  appraise  himself. 

In  addition  to  the  fact  that  a  great  deal  of  anxiety  is 
aroused  by  the  demand  for  self -appraisal,  it  is  also  a  matter 
of  general  psychological  knowledge  that  few  persons  are 
capable  of  objective  self -evaluation  with  regard  to  their  emo- 
tions and  personalities.  Attempts  to  make  a  subject  evaluate 
himself  and  his  own  emotional  reactions  presume  a  knowl- 
edge of  self  which  is  lacking  in  most  individuals.  With  this 
consideration  in  mind,  it  was  decided  that  asking  the  subject 
to  appraise  himself  should  be  avoided;  instead,  he  should 
be  given  an  opportunity  to  express  himself  in  a  number  of 
different  ways. 

5.  The  instrument  of  appraisal  should  provide  a  varied 
response — a  field  upon  which  the  subject  can  express 
himself. 

This  method  of  appraisal  differs  somewhat  from  one  of 
the  common  conceptions  of  a  test.  In  many  tests  the  subject 
is  given  a  problem  which  is  presumably  comparable  to  a 
life  situation  and  his  performance  in  attaining  the  solution 
of  the  problem  is  interpreted  as  a  measure  of  his  ability  to 
cope  with  an  analogous  situation  in  life.  In  an  instrument 
which  attempts  to  appraise  personal  and  social  adjustment, 
however,  it  was  felt  that  it  might  be  undesirable  that  the 
problems  be  thus  limited  by  the  examiner  rather  than  re- 
vealed by  the  individual.  It  seemed  that  the  most  desirable 
technique  to  use  would  be  that  of  presenting  a  large  variety 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  357 

of  stimuli  to  which  each  individual  might  react  emotionally 
in  a  variety  o£  ways,  thus  providing  a  field,  so  to  speak,  upon 
which  the  individual  might  draw  his  own  design.  This 
means,  also,  that  there  should  be  opportunity  for  an  ex- 
tremely large  number  of  configurations  of  response,  in  order 
that  each  individual  might  have  the  maximum  practicable 
opportunity  to  project  his  personality.  Single  responses,  then, 
would  have  meaning  chiefly  as  they  became  a  part  of  a 
larger  pattern.  Each  response  could  be  interpreted  in  the 
light  of  every  other  response.  Whereas  it  is  not  possible  to 
"provide  a  field  so  large  that  an  individual  can  express  his 
whole  personality,  even  a  limited  field  in  which  the  inter- 
relationships are  traceable  is  apt  to  provide  a  great  deal  of 
useful  material. 

6.  It  should  give  the  individual  pattern  of  the  person- 
ality of  the  subject. 

In  order  to  get  at  the  more  detailed  picture  of  the  per- 
sonality, one  has  to  guard  against  the  use  of  too  broad  classi- 
fications, such  as  "sociable"  and  "a-sociable."  Such  classifica- 
tions tend  to  obliterate  individual  differences  and  to  be  useful 
only  in  very  extreme  cases.  It  was  thought  desirable  that  an 
appraisal  instrument  give  a  description  aiming  at  something 
more  than  a  rough  categorization  of  the  personality.  This 
description,  if  it  is  to  be  useful  to  educators,  should  go  be- 
yond what  is  readily  observable  in  a  classroom  situation.  It 
should  lead  to  deeper  insights  into  the  individual,  his  motiva- 
tion, his  system  of  subjective  meanings  attached  to  things, 
his  values,  etc.  Understanding  another  person  is  an  under- 
standing of  this  person's  acts  in  terms  of  his  feelings  and  not 
in  terms  of  the  feelings  of  an  outsider. 

7.  It  should  be  open  to  interpretation  at  different  levels. 
It  was  felt  that  to  demand  from  the  interpreter  a  certain 

degree  of  psychological  understanding  is  legitimate.  On  the 
other  hand,  it  was  felt  that  the  instrument  should  not  be  so 


358        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

complicated  that  only  a  person  with  specialized  training 
could  interpret  it.  Ideally  such  an  instrument  should  give  re- 
sults which  would  permit  deep  interpretation  by  persons 
with  a  good  deal  of  training  and  experience  and  still  yield 
some  useful  material  to  persons  with  limited  training. 

Exploratory  Studies 

1.  Use  of  the  Interest  Questionnaire 

While  the  above  criteria  for  a  technique  of  appraisal  of  a 
personality  were  being  considered,  several  exploratory 
studies  were  conducted  with  tests  devised  by  the  Evaluation 
Staff  for  other  purposes.  It  was  thought  that  since  personal 
and  social  adjustment  was  intimately  related  to  these  other 
areas,  a  great  economy  would  be  achieved  if  it  were  found 
possible  to  draw  inferences  for  the  present  objective  from  the 
results  of  other  tests.  Moreover,  such  an  approach  would  be 
ideal  from  the  standpoint  of  indirection. 

Of  all  the  tests  examined  from  this  angle,  the  first  Interest 
Questionnaire,  Form  8.2,  gave  the  best  results.  This  ques- 
tionnaire provided  data  on  the  students'  feeling  reactions  to 
300  activities  commonly  carried  on  in  school.3  The  students 
responded  to  the  items  in  terms  of  like,  indifferent,  dislike. 
In  an  exploratory  study  an  attempt  was  made  to  discover 
what  kinds  of  things  and  how  many  one  might  say  about  the 
personal  and  social  adjustment  of  33  college  students,  using 
the  data  from  this  questionnaire.  The  students  selected  for 
study  were  attending  an  institution  which  was  known  to  have 
elaborate  and  detailed  records  on  its  students. 

The  descriptions  written  from  the  questionnaire  results 
were  compared  with  teachers'  ratings  of  these  students  on  a 
Descriptive  Trait  Profile*  a  rather  flexible  personality  rating 
scale  devised  for  the  purpose  of  validation  of  this  study. 

3  This  questionnaire  has  since  been  revised.  The  revised  form,  Interest 
Index  8.2a,  is  described  in  the  chapter  on  Interests. 

4P.E.A.  2968  (mimeographed),  University  of  Chicago,  Chicago,  111. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  359 

Each  student  was  rated  by  four  teachers.  Although  valida- 
tion through  a  comparison  of  descriptions  of  personalities 
presents  certain  difficulties  of  a  purely  semantic  nature,  those 
who  examined  the  data  felt  that  quite  similar  portraits  of 
students  were  presented  by  the  teachers  and  by  the  inter- 
preters of  the  questionnaire.  Specifically,  it  was  estimated 
that  the  personality  sketches  of  27  of  the  33  students  bore  a 
remarkable  similarity  to  the  teachers'  descriptions.  In  some 
cases  the  questionnaire  revealed  traits  which  would  seem  to 
be  completely  unrelated  to  interests  as  usually  conceived. 
These  results  were  sufficiently  encouraging  to  justify  using 
the  interest  questionnaire  approach  and  exploring  it  further 
as  a  possible  means  of  appraising  personal  and  social  ad- 
justment. 

2.  Significance  of  interests 

The  approach  taken  was  directly  dependent  upon  the  point 
of  view  held  as  to  the  significance  of  interests.  This  point 
of  view  differed  somewhat  from  earlier  and  other  current 
concepts  of  interests. 

In  the  present  study  interests  were  approached  from  the 
point  of  view  of  the  relationship  between  the  individual  and 
the  reaction  or  interest.  It  was  thought  that  unless  we  are  to 
consider  interests  to  be  merely  chance  reactions,  arbitrary 
and  capricious,  psychological  fungi  as  it  were,  playing  no 
part  in  the  fundamental  body  of  the  individual's  character, 
we  must  assume  that  they  are  a  result  of  the  interaction  of 
deeper  desires  with  environmental  forces.  Interest  then  takes 
on  the  significance  of  an  index  of  emotional  tendencies  and 
of  the  personality  pattern  of  the  individual.  It  becomes  the 
expression  of  the  aims  of  the  individual,  conscious  and  ex- 
pressed, or  unconscious  and  to  be  inferred.  Liking  and  dis- 
liking, accepting  and  rejecting  activities,  become  significant 
as  expressions  of  some  of  the  basic  elements  and  drives 
within  the  individual.  For  the  purposes  of  this  study  specific 


360        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

interests  in  themselves  become  rather  insignificant;  the  em- 
phasis is  no  longer  on  the  desirability  of  interest  within  a 
certain  field,  but  rather  on  the  significance  of  interest  for  the 
inference  of  underlying  urges  and  aims.  Furthermore,  in- 
terests were  not  thought  of,  in  relation  to  this  problem,  as 
discrete,  separable  entities,  but  as  interrelated  and  inter- 
acting. 

Those  who  can  accept  this  point  of  view  about  the  signifi- 
cance of  interests  can  readily  see  how  an  interest  inventory 
can  be  used  as  a  projectile  technique,  as  "a  means  of  dis- 
covering the  way  in  which  an  individual  personality  or- 
ganizes experience,  in  order  to  disclose  or  at  least  gain  insight 
into  the  individual's  private  world  of  meanings,  significances, 
patterns,  and  feelings."5  The  Interest  Questionnaire  offers  to 
the  individual  the  opportunity  to  reveal  his  way  of  organiz- 
ing experience  by  presenting  him  with  a  large  number  of 
activities  from  different  areas  to  which  he  reacts  emotion- 
ally, in  terms  of  like,  dislike,  and  indifferent. 

3.  Discussion  of  the  Significance  of  Like,  Indifferenty 
and  Dislike  Responses 

The  exploratory  study  and  interviews  with  students 
showed  that  certain  inferences  may  be  drawn  from  the  types 
of  responses  which  the  student  gives  to  the  questionnaire.  It 
was  possible  to  do  this  partly  on  theoretical  grounds,  and 
partly  because  the  examiners  of  the  students'  responses 
trained  themselves  to  seek  in  the  data  every  possible  clue  to 
the  emotional  state  of  the  subjects.  Thus,  it  was  found  that 
"like,"  "indifferent/'  and  "dislike,"  may  not  be  taken  as  mean- 
ing "just"  like,  indifferent,  dislike,  but  may  be  thought  of  as 
having  much  more  affective  significance.  "Like"  may  mean, 
for  instance,  "Is  strongly  attracted  by  it,  loves."  "Indifferent" 
may  mean  either  no  affect,  or  withdrawal  or  repression  of 

5L.  K.  Frank,  "Protective  Methods  for  the  Study  of  Personality,"  The 
Journal  of  Psychology,  1939,  p.  402. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  361 

affect,  or  an  avoidance  of  expressing  an  affect.  "Dislike"  may 
express  active  antagonism,  fear,  resentment.  Thus,  for  in- 
stance, it  seemed  reasonable  to  assume  that  a  student  who 
expresses  a  "dislike"  response  to  a  great  many  school  activi- 
ties does  not  "just  happen"  not  to  enjoy  a  large  number  of 
the  listed  activities  but,  perhaps,  reveals  an  undercurrent  of 
general  antagonism  to  school. 

DESCRIPTION  OF  THE  QUESTIONNAIRE 

The  preliminary  considerations  and  the  results  of  the  ex- 
ploratory studies  suggested  as  a  next  step  the  extension  and 
elaboration  of  the  interest  inventory  technique.  This  led  to 
the  construction  of  three  inventories:  Interest  Index  8.2a,  de- 
scribed in  Chapter  V,  and  Interests  and  Activities  8.2b  and 
8.2c.  Each  of  these  inventories  consists  of  200  items  to  which 
students  respond  by:  like,  indifferent,  or  dislike.  Interest 
Index  8.2a  consists  of  items  relating  to  school  studies  and 
school  subjects,  whereas  Interests  and  Activities  8.2b  and 
8.2c  consist  of  items  dealing  with  non-academic  activities.  It 
was  thought  that  three  questionnaires  dealing  with  the  intel- 
lectual, esthetic,  social,  and  inner  mental  and  emotional 
areas  of  functioning;  ought  to  give  a  rather  comprehensive 
picture  of  the  organization  of  the  energies  of  the  individual. 
It  was  further  assumed  that  the  above  areas  are  intimately 
interrelated  and  that  if  attention  is  focussed  on  the  inter- 
action among  them  rather  than  on  the  examination  of  them 
as  separable  units,  one  ought  to  be  able  to  infer  a  great  deal 
regarding  the  functioning  of  the  individual. 

Method  of  Gathering  Material  for  the  Questionnaires 

In  order  to  make  certain  that  the  questionnaires  contained 
material  taken  from  life  situations  of  the  students,  leads  for 
the  choice  of  the  items  were  obtained  from  children.  A  class 
of  junior  high  school  students,  known  rather  well  by  one  of 
the  investigators,  was  told  that  information  on  children's 


362,        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

interests  would  be  of  use  to  educators,  writers  of  radio  pro- 
grams, publishers  of  children's  books,  etc.  They  were  asked 
how  they  would  go  about  discovering  such  interests.  After  a 
study  of  the  problem,  the  class  arrived  at  the  following 
methods  of  studying  children's  interests:  (1)  a  carefully 
drawn  up  but  informally  administered  questionnaire;  (2) 
diary  records,  which  were  to  include  all  activities  engaged 
in  by  the  members  of  the  group,  with  comments  as  to  how 
they  had  felt  about  them;  and  (3)  a  survey  of  the  group  as 
to  what  things  its  members  wanted  most  to  do  or  to  have. 
The  questionnaire  contained  such  questions  as:  "What  things 
do  you  like  to  do  most  when  you  are  alone?"  "What  things 
do  you  like  to  do  with  others?"  "What  do  you  like  pretend- 
ing?" "What  do  you  like  to  do  when  you  feel  happy?"  "What 
do  you  like  to  do  when  you  feel  sad?"  etc.  The  questionnaire, 
diary,  and  survey  yielded  a  large  variety  of  activities  which 
formed  the  basis  for  the  choice  of  items.  As  far  as  possible, 
the  original  phraseology  of  the  children's  statements  was 
kept.  Later  a  similar  study  was  conducted  in  another  city 
with  a  group  of  high  school  students;  the  resemblance  be- 
tween the  two  activity  lists  was  striking. 

Criteria  for  Selection  of  Items 

In  selecting  items  for  the  questionnaires,  three  criteria 
were  kept  in  mind:  (1)  that  the  item  represent  a  fairly  char- 
acteristic or  common  activity  of  children,  (2)  that  the  ac- 
tivity seem  to  belong  to  one  of  the  clusters  or  categories  of 
activities  which  were  thought  to  be  related  to  personal  and 
social  adjustment,  and  (3)  that  the  activity  listed  be  not  too 
threatening.  In  general,  there  was  no  effort  to  find  single 
crucial  items  which  would  be  diagnostic  in  and  by  them- 
selves. Doing  so  would  be  contrary  to  the  whole  philosophy 
of  study  of  personality  as  it  has  been  outlined  in  die  preced- 
ing discussion.  In  a  sense,  each  item  in  a  category  may  be 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  363 

said  to  be  significant  only  as  it  is  viewed  as  a  part  of  the  total 
configuration  of  responses. 

Discussion  of  Categories  in  8.2b  and  8.2C8 

Since  there  seems  to  be  no  generally  accepted  frame  of 
reference  in  terms  of  which  a  personality  should  be  studied, 
the  selection  of  categories  was  made  in  terms  of  the  thinking 
of  the  investigators  regarding  some  of  the  more  important 
factors  which  need  to  be  considered  in  a  study  of  a  person's 
adjustments.  Since  a  possible  approach  toward  the  evaluation 
of  adjustment  was  thought  of  as  a  systematic  study  of  the 
individual's  ways  of  making  adjustments,  rather  than  as  an 
appraisal  of  whether  or  not  he  is  "well  adjusted,"  no  cate- 
gories were  designed  to  be  indicative  of  "good"  or  "poor" 
adjustment  in  and  by  themselves.  Each  category  was  thought 
of  in  the  light  of  the  possible  meaning  it  might  have  when 
examined  in  relation  to  other  categories.  This  must  be  borne 
in  mind  when  examining  the  categories. 

An  effort  was  made  to  choose  categories  which  so  far  as 
possible  would  yield  information  relative  to  the  various  kinds 
of  adjustments  the  individual  has  to  make.  It  should  be  noted 
that  all  of  the  information  necessary  for  the  description  of 
an  individual's  adjustment  cannot  be  obtained  from  the 
questionnaire.  Information  as  to  the  environmental  factors, 
the  individual's  past  history,  and  so  forth,  must  be  obtained 
in  some  other  way.  The  present  technique  aims  largely  at 
tracing  some  of  the  subjective  feelings  of  an  individual  and 
at  making  inferences  from  these  regarding  the  organization 
of  his  personality. 

It  will  be  seen  later  from  the  discussion  of  interpretation 
and  from  the  sample  case  analysis  that  each  student,  without 
knowing  that  he  is  doing  so,  determines  himself  the  organiza- 
tion of  the  categories  by  means  of  his  reactions  to  the  items. 

6  The  activities  listed  in  the  questionnaires  are  not  grouped  by  categories; 
the  keyed  list  of  items  can  be  obtained  in  mimeographed  form  from  Pro- 
gressive Education  Association,  University  of  Chicago,  Chicago,  111. 


364        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Depending  on  his  responses,  any  of  the  categories  may  come 
into  a  dominant  position  in  the  interpretation  or  may  come 
to  be  regarded  as  of  minor  importance  in  his  particular  case. 
Thus,  interpretations  take  their  lead  from  the  student  and  his 
way  of  responding. 

Nevertheless,  in  order  to  facilitate  the  exposition  of  the 
thinking  of  the  investigators,  in  the  following  presentation 
the  categories  are  grouped  into  three  major  areas:  (1)  "Or- 
ganization of  impulses  and  drives"  encompasses  categories 
which  shed  light  predominantly  on  the  way  in  which  an 
individual  handles  some  of  his  impulses;  (2)  "Human  rela- 
tionships" lists  categories  which  are  meant  to  tap  predom- 
inantly the  feelings  of  the  student  regarding  social  interac- 
tion of  various  types;  (3)  "Fantasy  life"  contains  categories 
which  are  meant  to  reveal  predominantly  the  extent  and  type 
of  fantasies  in  which  a  student  engages  or  which  he  avoids. 

It  should  be  emphasized  that  the  above  three  areas  are  not 
thought  of  as  discrete  and  separate  entities.  This  classifica- 
tion is  merely  a  method  of  organizing  certain  emotional  dis- 
positions which  are  in  constant  interaction.  It  should  also  be 
remembered  that  depending  on  the  configuration,  the  same 
category  may  have  different  meanings.  Furthermore,  any 
one  meaning  attached  to  one  of  the  categories  is  apt  to  influ- 
ence the  significance  of  some  of  the  other  categories. 

1.  "Organization  of  Impulses  and  Drives" 

a  and  b.  Acceptance  of  Own  Impulses  and  Severity 
with  Oneself 

Those  working  on  the  construction  of  this  instrument  felt 
that  one  of  the  most  fundamental  problems  with  which  every 
growing  child  has  to  cope  is  the  reconciliation  of  his  primi- 
tive drives  and  impulses  with  the  restrictions  which  social 
living  and  social  mores  impose  on  him.  As  has  been  stated 
earlier  in  the  formulation  of  the  definition  of  adjustment,  the 
desirable  pattern  was  thought  of  as  a  certain  balance  be- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  365 

tvveen  acceptance  of  the  primitive  impulses  on  the  one  hand 
and,  on  the  other  hand,  considerations  of  social  expedience 
and  actual  incorporation  into  the  individual's  personality  of 
some  of  the  standards  and  restricting  concepts  of  the  social 
milieu.  Difficulties  in  achieving  such  a  balance  are  very  com- 
mon. These  difficulties  may  be  said  to  fall  into  two  broad 
categories.  The  first  evidences  itself  in  a  personality  which 
continues  to  operate  primarily  on  the  basis  of  its  primitive 
impulses  and  urges,  and  disregards  or  fails  to  incorporate 
the  social  standards  and  taboos.  The  second  type  of  difficulty 
may  express  itself  in  a  too  rigorous  repression  of  the  im- 
pulses and  their  gratification  and  may  result  in  a  truly  in- 
hibited, extremely  self-censoring  and  "over-restricted"  per- 
sonality. 

Categories  entitled  "Acceptance  of  Own  Impulses"  and 
"Severity  with  Oneself"  attempt  to  bring  to  light  the  stu- 
dent's status  among  his  classmates  with  reference  to  the 
above  areas"  of  adjustment.  In  a  sense,  both  of  these  cate- 
gories aim  to  appraise  the  same  area  of  adjustment,  but  ap- 
proach it  from  two  opposite  poles.  Thus,  a  very  high  score 
on  "Severity"  would  tend  to  indicate  that  at  least  in  certain 
respects  the  student's  "Acceptance  of  Own  Impulses"  is 
under  actual  or  potential  censorship.  A  very  low  score  on 
"Severity"  would  tend  to  suggest  that  "Acceptance  of  Own 
Impulses"  functions  with  considerable  freedom. 

Examples  from  the  category  "Acceptance  of  Own  Im- 
pulses" are:  being  a  little  sick  and  staying  in  bed  all  day; 
eating  so  much  I  can't  take  another  bite;  saying  whatever 
comes  into  my  head. 

Examples  from  the  category  "Severity  with  Oneself"  are: 
setting  myself  tasks  to  strengthen  my  will  power;  working 
on  myself,  improving  myself  in  some  way;  taking  a  cold 
shower  on  a  winter  morning. 


366         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

c.  Preoccupation  with  Cleanliness 

Early  training  in  cleanliness  usually  represents  the  first 
demand  which  the  social  mores  make  upon  the  child  to  regu- 
late his  impulses.  This  training  is  often  accomplished  by 
building  up  strong  feelings  of  shame  or  guilt  about  bodily 
functions  and  the  body  itself.  Various  feelings  of  shame  and 
guilt,  conscious  or  unconscious,  may  result  in  undue  preoc- 
cupation with  cleanliness,  purity,  fear  of  contamination,  fear 
of  germs,  etc.  This  type  of  anxiety  seems  to  be  particularly 
common  in  our  society.  This  category  is  designed  to  furnish 
indications  as  to  the  extent  to  which,  and  the  way  in  which, 
the  individual  has  accepted  and  incorporated  into  himself 
this  early  experience.  Thus,  very  low  likes  and  high  dislikes 
in  this  area  might  indicate  a  lack  of  acceptance  of  these 
demands  of  society,  whereas,  on  the  other  hand,  very  high 
likes  and  low  dislikes  might  be  symptomatic  of  other  ten- 
sions in  this  area. 

d.  Methodical 

The  child's  attempts  to  master  his  impulses  may  result  in 
a  certain  rigidity  of  personality  with  a  tendency  to  compul- 
sive behaviors.  Most  of  the  activities  in  the  methodical  cate- 
gory are  quite  common  behaviors,  behaviors  which  are  usu- 
ally even  encouraged  by  educators.  They  are  activities  which 
are  characteristically  rigidly  patterned  and  repetitive;  they 
also  are  activities  which  involve  collecting,  arranging,  classi- 
fying, etc.  Examples  of  the  activities  listed  in  this  category 
are:  copying  papers  to  make  them  neat;  keeping  a  calendar 
or  notebook  of  the  things  I  plan  to  do;  making  up  catalogs 
and  card  files. 

e.  Aggression 

Making  the  large  number  of  adjustments  which  every 
child  has  to  make,  enduring  frustrations,  having  to  inhibit 
his  impulses,  invariably  and  quite  normally  produces  and 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  367 

contributes  to  the  reservoir  of  stored  hostility  within  the 
child.  The  expression  of  this  hostility  may  take  the  form  of 
overtly  a-social  acts;  more  frequently,  however,  it  takes  a 
more  or  less  socially  acceptable  form,  which  serves  as  an 
outlet  for  the  hostile  feelings,  without  seriously  imperiling 
the  person.  Categories  entitled  Aggression  in  8.2b  and  in 
8.2c  are  composed  of  activities  through  which  hostile  im- 
pulses frequently  find  an  outlet.  Some  of  these  involve  overt 
acts,  such  as:  hitting  someone  who  has  annoyed  me  very 
much,  always  telling  people  the  truth  even  when  it  might 
hurt  their  feelings,  picking  someone's  argument  to  pieces; 
others  involve  thinking:  thinking  of  what  111  do  when  I  grow 
up  to  people  who  have  been  mean  to  me,  looking  at  pictures 
of  death  and  destruction. 

2.  "Human  relationships'* 

f .  Relationship  with  Family 

Items  dealing  with  activities  commonly  carried  on  in  and 
with  the  family  were  selected  for  the  drawing  of  inferences 
about  the  extent  to  which  the  student  enjoys,  is  indifferent 
to,  or  does  not  enjoy  his  home  life.  An  effort  was  made  to 
have  a  wide  spread  of  activities,  ranging  from  such  activities 
as  having  a  good  argument  or  serious  discussion  with  the 
family  to  cleaning  up  after  meals,  washing  or  drying  dishes. 

g.  Relationship  with  the  Same  Sex 

This  category  is  composed  of  activities  in  which  usually 
only  students  of  the  same  sex  participate.  It  was  thought 
that  liking  or  disliking  such  activities  as  belonging  to  a  boys* 
club  or  girls'  club,  staying  overnight  at  -a  friend's  house,  etc., 
might  be  indicative  of  a  student's  feelings,  particularly  when 
reactions  to  these  activities  are  seen  as  part  of  a  whole  set  of 
reactions  in  the  area  of  human  relationships. 

h.  Relationship  with  the  Opposite  Sex 
The  items  in  this  category  were  so  selected  that  a  high 


368        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

score  In  liking  them  would  indicate  a  person  who  attaches  a 
value  to  activities  requiring  the  participation  of  both  sexes. 
This  category  may  be  broken  down  into; 

1.  Ordinary  activities  with  the  opposite  sex,  such 
as  parties,  dancing,  etc. 

2.  Activities  implying  a  stronger  interest  in  the  op- 
posite sex  than  the  above — making  oneself  at- 
tractive, courtship,  etc. 

3.  Activities  indicating  a  less  openly  displayed  or 
perhaps  vicarious  interest  in  the  opposite  sex — 
such  as  reading  love  novels,  watching  others 
who  are  in  love,  day-dreaming  about  it,  etc. 

i.   Identification  with  Others 

The  purpose  of  this  category  is  to  investigate  the  extent  to 
which  a  student  likes,  or  likes  to  think  of  himself  as  liking, 
activities  which  involve  a  strong  personal  interest  in  other 
people,  close,  intimate  friendships,  sympathetic  taking  care 
of  others,  defending  the  molested,  etc.  Many  of  these  items 
are  concerned  with  imagining  things  about  other  people,  or 
about  one's  relationship  with  other  people,  rather  than  with 
actually  doing  things.  Thus,  it  is  possible  that  a  student  who 
has  not  yet  actually  established  successful  social  relations 
may  still  like  these  activities.  This  category  is  designed,  then, 
to  show  the  extent  to  which  the  student  has  a  value  for  such 
relationships.  Characteristic  items  are:  having  a  lot  of  close 
friends  with  whom  I  can  talk  about  anything;  trying  to  find 
out  what  a  quiet  shy  person  is  really  like;  discussing  with 
younger  boys  or  girls  what  they  like  to  do  and  how  they  feel 
about  things. 

j.  School  Activities 

This  category  is  designed  to  reveal  the  student's  attitudes 
toward  student  organizations,  the  school,  school  life,  etc.  It  is 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  369 

composed  of  activities  commonly  carried  on  in  school,  such 
as:  being  an  active  member  of  a  school  club,  being  on  class 
committees,  going  to  school  dances,  etc. 

k.  Out-of-School  Activities 

This  category7  summarizes  all  the  activities  which  might 
reveal  participation  and  interest  in  social  life  outside  of  the 
school  situation.  When  considered  in  relation  to  the  category 
school  activities,  it  may  reveal  whether  the  student  is  gen- 
erally sociable  and  enjoys  all  types  of  social  situations,  is 
generally  a-sociable,  or  sociable  in  school  situations  but  not 
in  out-of -school  situations  or  vice  versa. 

1.  Solitary 

This  category  is  composed  of  activities  in  which  one  usu- 
ally engages  alone,  such  as  keeping  a  diary,  playing  solitaire, 
etc.  It  also  lists  some  activities  which  are  usually  sociable 
but  are  designated  as  solitary,  such  as:  eating  alone,  going 
swimming,  skating,  bike-riding  alone,  etc. 

m.  Impressing  Others 

This  category  is  composed  of  activities  which  involve  pre- 
occupation with  personal  appearance,  desire  to  be  unique, 
outstanding,  in  the  limelight.  The  following  items  are  repre- 
sentative: making  my  handwriting  unusual  and  decorative; 
having  the  reputation  of  being  different  or  unusual;  starting 
a  fashion  or  a  fad. 

n.  Leadership 

Activities  which  involve  organizing  others  into  groups,  di- 
recting groups,  debating,  arguing,  etc.,  are  sampled  in  this 
category.  Examples  of  these  activities  are:  organizing  com- 
mittees to  plan  various  school  affairs;  being  in  public  speak- 
ing or  debating  contests;  etc. 


370        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 
o.  Reactions  to  Authority 

Activities  listed  in  this  category  involve  either  submission 
to  or  rebellion  against  authority.  Statements  are  so  coded 
that  a  high  score  in  likes  is  indicative  of  a  submissive  attitude 
toward  authority,  whereas  a  high  score  in  dislikes  is  indica- 
tive o£  a  rebellious  or  antagonistic  attitude.  Typical  items 
are:  writing  papers  on  definite,  assigned  topics  rather  than 
having  a  free  choice;  being  in  a  group  where  one  person 
takes  the  responsibility  and  decides  what  people  should  or 
should  not  do. 

3.  "Fantasy  life" 

p.  Birth— Life— Death 

Activities  in  this  category  involve  wondering  about  the 
meaning  of  life  and  death,  thoughts  about  the  origin  and  end 
of  things,  the  meaning  of  eternity,  and  the  stability  and 
permanence  of  the  universe.  Preoccupation  here  might  indi- 
cate the  need  to  externalize  personal  anxieties  and  put  them 
on  a  cosmic  scale.  Conflicts  one  cannot  face  near  at  hand  are 
often  projected  into  the  cosmos,  and  dealt  with  in  a  philo- 
sophical way.  Examples  of  items  are:  finding  out  how  tilings 
got  started;  thinking  about  what  might  be  the  end  of  the 
world;  imagining  what  would  happen  if  gravity  ceased  to 
exist. 

q.  Fantasy 

Although  it  is  important  to  recognize  that  fantasy  can  play 
a  part  as  an  adjustment  mechanism  and  therefore  in  itself  is 
not  an  indication  of  maladjustment,  it  is  also  true  that  indi- 
viduals who  have  difficulties  in  coping  with  reality  may  use 
this  mechanism  as  a  substitute  for  action  and  as  an  escape 
from  actualities.  This  category  lists  a  number  of  fantasy  ac- 
tivities in  which  most  youngsters  engage  at  one  time  or  an- 
other. Examples  are:  carrying  on  imaginary  conversations 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  371 

with  someone  I  like  or  admire;  imagining  how  it  would  feel 

to  be  rich  and  famous. 

r.  Mystery 

A  child  who  has  been  unduly  sheltered  and  kept  away 
from  the  realities  of  the  life  around  him  may  develop  not 
only  distorted  notions  regarding  his  environment,  but  also 
great  curiosity  and  preoccupation  with  "the  secrets  of  adults" 
and  other  mysteries.  The  items  in  this  category  attempt  to 
sample  the  different  "mystery-interests'*  of  children  and 
adolescents.  Such  statements  as  the  following  are  found  in 
the  questionnaire:  having  people  "forget  themselves"  and 
talk  freely;  listening  to  other  people's  phone  conversations. 

s.  Magic 

Every  child,  at  least  in  part  because  of  his  relative  incom- 
petence as  compared  with  adults,  in  his  efforts  to  deal  with 
his  environment  tends  to  resort  to  magical  means,  such  as 
good  luck  charms,  avoidance  of  symbols  of  bad  luck,  etc. 
Great  dependence  upon  these  symbols  may  reveal  a  feeling 
of  incompetence  and  a  need  to  resort  to  "superior  powers" 
for  help.  This  category  lists  some  of  the  activities  which  in- 
volve using  magic,  such  as:  carrying  a  good  luck  charm; 
making  up  little  games  or  schemes  which  will  bring  luck  if 
they  come  out  right;  seeing  if  a  hoped  for  thing  comes  true 
if  I  concentrate  on  it. 

t.  Dramatics 

This  category  is  composed  of  theater  arts  activities — those 
involving  writing  and  production  of  plays,  and  those  in- 
volving taking  specific  roles.  It  can  be  interpreted  both  as 
revealing  interest  or  lack  of  interest  in  the  theater  arts  per 
se,  and  it  can  also  be  interpreted  as  revealing  the  wish  or 
fantasy  life  of  the  individual.  In  this  connection  an  examina- 
tion of  the  types  of  roles  which  are  preferred  is  particularly 


372        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

interesting.  Examples  of  items  are:  thinking  up  plots  for 
plays;  taking  the  part  of  a  wicked  or  dangerous  person  in  a 
play. 

u.  Humor 

This  category  is  composed  of  activities  which  have  to  do 
with  the  appreciation  or  expression  of  humor.*-  Humor  may 
be  thought  of  as  a  way  of  relieving  tension.  It  also  is  fre- 
quently an  accepted,  subtle  way  of  expressing  hostility.  This 
is  particularly  clear  in  playing  practical  jokes  and  other  such 
forms  of  humor.  The  items  in  this  category  also  serve  to  make 
the  whole  questionnaire  lighter  in  tone  and  more  entertain- 
ing. Examples  are:  drawing  cartoons;  seeing  plays  which  are 
"take-offs"  on  dignified  people  or  institutions;  reading  or 
writing  funny  poems  or  limericks. 

INTERPRETATION  OF  THE  RESPONSES  TO  THE  QUESTIONNAIRES 

The  questionnaires  are  scored  in  terms  of  the  per  cent  of 
the  items  in  each  category  to  which  the  student  responds 
with  like,  and  the  per  cent  to  which  he  responds  with  dislike. 
The  per  cent  of  indifferent  responses  may  be  readily  calcu- 
lated by  subtracting  the  sum  of  the  above  two  scores  from 
100.  As  will  be  seen  presently,  the  interpretations  may  be 
made  on  two  levels.  For  a  quick  overview  of  the  student's 
interests  and  adjustive  trends,  one  may  examine  his  tabulated 
per  cent  scores  in  the  various  categories  on  the  Summary 
Sheet.  This  takes  little  time  and  gives  a  fair  but  rather  gen- 
eral picture.  A  much  more  detailed  study  of  the  student  may 
be  made  from  the  examination  of  his  specific  responses  to 
individual  items  in  the  questionnaires. 

Interpretation  of  the  Scores  on  the  Summary  Sheet 

A  student's  score  on  a  category  acquires  meaning  in  two 
ways:  when  viewed  in  reference  to  the  group  median,  and 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  373 

when  viewed  in  relation  to  the  student's  scores  on  other 
categories. 

No  scores  are  considered  high  or  low  per  se.  The  student's 
scores  are  always  examined  in  the  light  of  the  scores  of  other 
students  in  the  group  in  which  he  is  living  and  working.  It 
is  possible,  however,  to  single  out  categories  in  which  he 
ranks  high  or  low  in  his  group  in  likes  or  dislikes.  From  exam- 
ining these  categories  it  is  possible  to  draw  certain  inferences 
about  the  student.  For  instance,  it  frequently  happens  that 
a  student  has  low  likes  on  the  academic  interest  question- 
naire (8.2a)?  but  has  high  likes  on  all  the  sociable  categories 
in  the  non-academic  interest  questionnaires,  or  vice  versa.  A 
student's  likes  and  dislikes  may  group  themselves  not  only 
in  this  broad  manner  but  may  also  group  themselves  in 
greater  specificity.  One  may  find,  for  instance,  a  student  who 
is  high  in  likes  in  categories  involving  precision  in  work,  such 
as  physical  science,  mathematics,  industrial  arts,  and  method- 
ical, whereas  he  may  be  low  in  likes  in  categories  involving 
greater  freedom  of  action  and  self-expression,  such  as  fine 
arts  and  dramatics.  Again  a  student  might  be  low  in  liking 
such  -sociable  activities  as  are  listed  in  the  categories  same 
sex,  opposite  sex,  sociable  activities  in  school,  and  sociable 
activities  out  of  school,  and  at  the  same  time  be  high  in  lik- 
ing fantasy,  mystery,  magic,  etc.  Many  different  configura- 
tions are  thus  possible. 

The  final  picture  is  derived  from  the  way  in  which  the 
individual  student  reacts  to  a  great  many  fields  of  activity: 
academic  interests,  sociable  activities,  and  activities  \*[hich 
indicate  his  attitude  toward  himself.  In  linking  these  seem- 
ingly quite  different  fields,  the  interpreter  attempts  to  discover 
the  common  elements  which  make  the  student's  response  to 
academic  situations  understandable  in  terms  of  the  way  in 
which  his  personality  is  organized.  The  fact  that  the  mean- 
ing of  a  given  score  in  a  category  may  change  with  the  re- 
sponse of  the  student  to  other  categories  is  an  important 


374        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

consideration.  For  instance,  if  a  student  responds  to  leader- 
ship by  liking  80  per  cent  of  the  items  and  also  comes  out 
high  on  fantasy,  but  comes  out  low  on  most  of  the  categories 
dealing  with  sociable  activities,  one  is  justified  in  raising  the 
question  as  to  whether  or  not  the  high  liking  of  leadership 
indicates  wishful  thinking.  Careful  study  of  results  thus  far 
has  indicated  that  if  one  watches  for  the  inner  consistency  of 
the  picture  presented  by  a  student,  one  learns  to  discover 
facts  about  his  fantasy  life  and  learns  to  single  out  his  wish- 
ful responses.  The  fact  that  with  some  students  the  question- 
naires are  apt  to  reflect  their  wishes  rather  than  represent 
their  actual  behaviors  is  an  important  one  and  should  not  be 
regarded  as  something  which  makes  this  technique  invalid. 
On  the  contrary,  is  not  this  gaming  of  insight  into  the  inner 
mental  life  of  the  child  the  most  difficult  but  important  part 
of  the  problem? 

It  frequently  happens  that  a  student's  category  scores  are 
generally  high  or  low  in  likes,  indifference,  or  dislikes;  i.e., 
one  finds  students  who  are  "high  likers,"  'low  likers,"  "highly 
indifferent" — high  or  low,  that  is,  in  relation  to  the  group 
medians.  When  there  is  a  general  tendency  to  respond  in  a 
certain  way,  deviations  from  this  tendency  become  impor- 
tant, even  though  the  deviations  may  not  be  apparent  at 
first.  If,  for  instance,  a  student  is  below  the  group  median  in 
likes  in  all  categories,  but  near  the  median  in  some  cate- 
gories, and  at  the  same  time  is  one  of  the  lowest  in  the  class 
in  his  scores  in  likes  on  other  categories  it  becomes  evident 
that  his  scale  has  a  smaller  area,  but  that  there  still  is  a  dif- 
ferentiation in  his  response. 

In  each  case  it  is  necessary  to  examine  all  three  scores,  like, 
indifferent,  and  dislike.  A  student  may  have  an  equal  like 
score  on  two  categories,  but  the  fact  that  he  feels  differently 
about  the  activities  in  each  may  be  evidenced  by  a  strong 
dissimilarity  in  his  dislike  scores. 

Generally,  the  process  of  interpreting  the  summary  sheet 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  375 

is  as  follows:  The  interpreter  first  picks  out  the  highest  likes 
in  relation  to  the  student's  other  scores,  and  attempts  to  seek 
common  elements  in  these  categories.  The  same  is  done  for 
the  high  dislikes  and  high  indifferences.  This  examination 
includes  also  a  consideration  of  the  categories  which  the  stu- 
dent likes  or  dislikes  least. 

Interpretation  of  Responses  to  Individual  Items 

Although  this  approach  to  personality  study  attempts  to 
procure  quantitative  data  on  emotional  tendencies  and  dis- 
positions and  seems  to  do  so  rather  successfully,  for  a  deeper 
understanding  of  a  student  a  more  detailed  analysis  is  neces- 
sary. This  is  done  by  an  examination  of  his  responses  to  indi- 
vidual items  and  is  a  procedure  which  is  particularly  impor- 
tant for  gaining  an  understanding  of  the  dynamics  of  the 
student's  behavior.  Here  again  the  same  main  principle  of 
interpretation  as  is  used  with  the  category  scores  is  applied. 
First  the  likes,  then  the  dislikes,  and  then  the  indifferences 
for  individual  items  in  each  category  are  taken  and  each 
time  an  attempt  is  made  to  single  out  the  common  elements 
which  characterize  or  run  through  the  given  group  of  activi- 
ties. This  examination  frequently  reveals  new  categorizations 
peculiar  to  the  individual  whose  responses  are  being  exam- 
ined. For  instance,  two  students  may  have  very  similar  scores 
in  the  total  number  of  likes  on  the  category  opposite  sex;  one 
may  like  only  the  items  concerned  with  actual  sociable  ac- 
tivities; the  other,  however,  may  like  only  those  items  show- 
ing a  vicarious  interest,  those  involving  fantasying,  reading 
romantic  novels,  etc.,  and  be  indifferent  to  or  dislike  the 
actual  sociable  activities.  In  the  same  manner,  one  may  ob- 
serve that  a  student  may  consistently  like  or  dislike  all  the 
items  involving  speaking  before  a  group,  regardless  of 
whether  the  activity  appears  in  a  foreign  language  class, 
mathematics,  or  in  a  social  situation.  Many  such  individual 
categorizations  have  been  traced. 


376        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

With  regard  to  the  study  of  specific  responses  within  a 
category,  it  must  be  borne  in  mind  that  the  meaning  of  any 
given  response  to  any  item  must  be  again  examined  in  a 
twofold  way.  First  it  must  be  examined  from  the  point  of 
view  of  the  particular  pattern  which  it  reveals  for  the  stu- 
dent; i.e.,  from  the  point  of  view  of  the  types  of  activities 
within  a  given  category  or  in  different  categories  that  the 
student  likes,  dislikes,  and  is  indifferent  to.  Second,  these 
specific  responses  must  be  examined  against  the  background 
of  the  responses  of  the  same  age  and  sex  group.  To  make 
such  a  comparison  possible  the  staff  is  preparing  a  table  of 
responses  of  students  to  every  item  in  the  questionnaires. 
These  tables  are  based  on  a  study  of  responses  of  a  large 
number  of  students  and  will  show  how  the  boys  and  girls  of 
different  school  grades  have  distributed  their  likes,  indiffer- 
ences, and  dislikes.  Thus,  for  the  evaluation  of  the  meaning 
of  a  specific  response  it  is  important  to  know  that  less  than 
10  per  cent  of  both  boys  and  girls  of  all  grades  from  seven 
to  twelve  mark  "dislike"  the  item:  "Talking  in  halls  and 
locker  rooms. "  The  significance  of  a  student's  specific  re- 
sponses obviously  changes  depending  on  how  the  majority 
of  his  age  mates  respond  to  the  item. 

Discussion  of  Total  Scores  on  8.22,  b?  and  c7 

We  note  that  whereas  the  bulk  of  Lyle's  interests  on  the 
academic  interest  questionnaire  (8.2a)  are  in  the  upper  quar- 
ter, in  the  non-academic  questionnaires  (8.2b  and  c)  none 
of  his  likes  on  any  of  the  categories  is  high  enough  to  place 
him  in  the  upper  quarter  of  his  class.  On  all  except  one 
category  in  8.2a  he  shows  zero  dislikes.  The  only  category  in 
which  he  has  any  dislikes  is  sports.  It  begins  to  look  as  if 
sports  were  differentiated  from  the  other  interests  on  8.2a  by 
Lyle,  possibly  because  this  area  of  activity  involves  dealing 

7  This  description  was  made  from  the  material  obtained  from  the  ques- 
tionnaires above.  The  teachers'  descriptions  were  made  and  held  "by  them 
until  the  completion  of  the  interpretation. 


SAMPLE  ANALYSIS  OF  RESPONSES  OF  ONE  STUDENT 


TABLE    I 


Scores  of  One  Student  and  Medians  of  his  Class  on  Three  Interest  Questionnaires 


Lyle  0.,  Age  12  years,  6  months 
Mid-Western  Private  School 
7th  Grade,  Class  of  22  boys 


Case  No.  13 


Category 

Likes 

Dislikes 

Indifferent 

Rank 
in 
Group 

Per 

Cent 
Scores 

Me- 
dian 

Rank 
in 
Group 

Per 
Cent 
Scores 

Me- 
dian 

Per 
Cent 
Scores 

Me- 
dian 

8.2at  20  Boys 

Upper  Quarter 
Fine  Arts 

1 
1 

0 

¥ 

1 

3-4 
3-4 
3-4 

4 

6 
6 

12-14 
16-17 

6-7 
6-8 
9-11 
9-12 
10 
10-12 

11-12 
13-15 

14-15 

14-17 
15-16 
15-17 

16 
16-18 
17 
17-18 
17-18 
17-19 
18 

18 
19-20 
20 
20-21 

22 

88 
75 
76 
94 
75 
69 
63 
63 
63 
54 
88 

63 

46 

19 
31 

63 

66 
25 
49 
22 
38 

36 

24 

34 
19 
22 
30 

18 
26 
IS 
31 
14 
13 
28 

18 
8 
0 
22 

8 

38 
14 
50 
75 
20 
31 
36 
26 
22 
33 
48 

30 
23 

26 
50 

52 
58 
25 
49 
17 
38 

37 
30 

37 
31 
31 
38 

32 
39 

48 
56 
42 
60 
39 

32 

44 
43 
38 

34 

17-20 
18-20 
17-20 
18-20 
18-20 
15-20 
20 
19-20 
18-20 
20 
17-20 

16-20 
18-20 

18-20 
9-11 

12-15 
10-11 
13-15 
17 
18-19 
14-17 

21 
15 

12-13 
19-20 
18 
18-19 

15 
18 
4 
13-15 
6-S 
5-7 
21 

9-12 
15 
9-16 
9-12 

19 

0 
0 
0 
0 
0 
0 

1 

0 
0 
0 
0 

0 
0 

0 
19 

0 

14 
19 
6 
22 
6 

8 
32 

26 
19 
12 
13 

20 
12 
50 
0 
36 
19 
21 

25 
0 
10 
28 

16 

25 
58 
20 
4 
38 
23 
28 
42 
37 
22 
14 

23 
24 

26 
17 

19 

12 
27 
22 
52 
25 

35 
38 

31 
38 
24 
24 

30 
16 
22 
19 

27 
13 
31 

25 
15 
10 

27 

38 

36 

81 
50 

37 

56 

45 
56 
56 

56 

44 

40 
62 
66 

57 

62 
62 

69 
50 

58 
51 

57 
92 
90 
50 

76 

36 

48 
33 

29 

48 
29 
31 
37 

28 
32 

32 
31 
45 
38 

38 

45 

25 
31 
27 
30 

43 

41 
47 
35 

28 

Music. 
Manipulative 

Industrial  Arts  . 
Mathematics  
Business     .    .  . 
Total  (a)   .  . 
English 
Foreign  Languages  . 
Reading.             .  .    . 
Physical  Science  ,  . 
2nd  Quarter 
Biology   .... 

Social  Studies  .... 
3rd  Quarter 
Home  Economics.   . 
Lower  Quarter 
Sports  

82b,  20  Boys 
8.2c,  22  Boys 

2nd  Quarter 
Fantasy 

Humor 

Mag^c  .  .         ..... 

Family        

Authority 

Dramatics   
3rd  Quarter 
Methodical  ... 
Severity     . 
Acceptance  of  Own 
Impulses  .... 
Impressing  Others 
Opposite  Sex  .  . 
Total  (b) 

Lower  Quarter 
Birth-Life-Death   .. 
School  Activities.  . 
Aggression  (b)  .  . 
Mystery  
Aggression  (c)  
Leadership  . 

Total  (c)  .      . 
Identification  with 
Others    

Out-of-School  

Same  Sex.. 

Solitary 

Preoccupation  with 
Cleanliness  

Figures  falling  in  the  upper  quarter  in  the  indifferent  column  are  italicized. 


3?8        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

with  other  people  and  dealing  with  certain  environmental 
realities.  (Incidentally,  having  zero  dislikes  for  any  of  the 
academic  activities  is  most  unusual. ) 

Whereas  Lyle  is  above  the  median  in  indifference  in  prac- 
tically every  category  on  8.2b  and  c,  he  is  above  the  median 
in  indifference  in  only  two  categories  on  8.2a — home  eco- 
nomics and  sports.  This  seems  to  point  to  some  very  impor- 
tant differentiations  in  the  organization  of  his  energies. 
Probably  his  indifferences  in  8.2a  should  be  examined 
separately  as  they  may  be  equivalent  to  dislikes  in  his  case. 
Evidently,  for  some  reason  which  we  do  not  know,  Lyle  has 
a  value  for  the  "academic" — and  either  accepts  or  feels  he 
should  accept  everything  which  seems  to  fall  into  this  classi- 
fication. 

On  8.2b  and  8.2c,  it  is  interesting  to  note  that  on  his  scale 
of  interests  fantasy  is  highest,  whereas  all  of  the  categories 
involving  interaction  with  other  people  (with  the  sole  excep- 
tion of  family)  fall  below  the  median.  It  would  seem,  again, 
that  he  distinguishes  in  some  way  between  activities  with 
other  people  and  the  things  that  go  on  in  his  mind. 

Lyle  has  only  three  high  dislikes  on  8.2b  and  8.2c:  aggres- 
sion (b),  and  aggression  (c)  and  leadership.  We  see  in  this 
a  strong  avoidance  of  asserting  himself,  openly,  with  other 
people.  Furthermore  his  high  indifference  in  the  category 
authority,  coupled  with  the  very  low  dislike  and  what  is,  on 
his  scale,  a  fairly  high  like  of  it,  make  us  feel  that  he  is  a  boy 
who  has  accepted  a  certain  set  of  adult  standards  and  avoids 
expressing  any  criticism  or  questioning  of  it.  In  a  sense  he 
seems  to  be  a  boy  who  is  pretty  thoroughly  subjugated  by 
the  world  of  adults.  It  is  startling  to  note  that  Lyle  has  zero 
likes  in  the  category  dealing  with  activities  with  the  same 
sex,  and  has  only  8  per  cent  likes  in  the  category  dealing 
with  sociable  activities  out-of-school.  This  is  very  unusual 
for  a  seventh  grade  boy  or  any  boy  for  that  matter.  Actually, 
he  shows  on  these  questionnaires  a  slightly  higher  interest  in 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  379 

the  opposite  sex  than  in  the  same  sex.  Usually  this  is  re- 
versed among  seventh  grade  boys.  However,  his  interest  in 
the  opposite  sex  is  not  high  enough  so  that  it  could  be  called 
an  outlet  for  his  sociable  feelings.  It  would  rather  seem  that 
he  does  not  avoid  it  to  the  extent  that  he  does  the  same  sex. 
(An  examination  of  Lyle's  specific  reactions  in  this  category 
reveals  that  he  likes  only  three  items.  These  items  are  only 
remotely  connected  with  this  category  and  do  not  involve 
any  activities  with  the  opposite  sex — they  deal  rather  with 
learning  facts  and  with  daydreaming.) 

Lyle's  low  likes  in  the  category  solitary  seem  contradictory 
to  the  picture  we  have  been  getting  of  him.  In  his  case,  how- 
ever, we  tend  to  think  that  this  low  score  is  an  indication  of 
a  tendency  in  him  to  avoid  admitting  to  himself  (or  to 
others )  that  he  does  not  have  a  normal  play-life  with  other 
boys  and  girls.  If  this  hunch  is  correct  then  we  may  say  that 
Lyle  may,  in  himself,  have  a  value  for  or  feel  a  lack  of  satis- 
faction in  the  sociable  area,  but  that  the  full  realization  of 
the  fact  that  he  misses  something  in  life  is  too  painful  for  him 
and  he  attempts  to  convince  himself  that  he  is  really  indif- 
ferent to  it. 

Discussion  of  Reactions  to  Specific  Items  on  8.22 

Since  Lyle  has  dislikes  only  in  the  category  sports  a  de- 
tailed examination  of  his  responses  in  this  category  may  be 
fruitful.  Such  an  examination  reveals  that  he  dislikes:  to  play 
baseball,  to  play,  basketball,  and  to  do  setting-up  exercises. 
The  strength  of  these  dislikes  is  particularly  impressive  when 
we  recall  that  they  are  the  only  items  which  he  so  marked 
on  the  whole  questionnaire.  We  notice  further  that  he  is  in- 
different to  all  the  team  games.  He  likes  only  such  highly 
individualized  sports  as:  to  play  horseshoes,  to  shoot  with 
bow  and  arrow,  to  play  golf,  etc. 

In  social  studies  we  notice  that  Lyle  is  indifferent  to  all 
"social  action"  items,  such  as:  taking  part  in  a  campaign 


380        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

against  countries  or  business  firms  which  treat  people  un- 
justly; attending  public  meetings  to  protest  against  some- 
thing you  regard  as  unfair;  getting  people  to  vote  for  certain 
candidates,  etc.  On  the  other  hand,  he  likes  those  items 
which  deal  with  study,  reading,  and  history.  His  interest  in 
social  studies  seems  to  be  largely  an  academic  one. 

Discussion  of  Responses  to  Individual  Items  on  8.2b  and  8.20 

We  may  take  first  categories  on  which  Lyle  expresses  high 
dislikes  (for  him). 

Leadership.  In  this  category  Lyle  is  indifferent  to  almost 
all  the  items  except  that  he  likes  to  speak  at  a  club  or  class 
meeting,  and  likes  organizing  a  hobby  club.  Both  of  these 
are  explainable  in  terms  of  his  interest  in  academic  pursuits. 
He  dislikes:  organizing  groups  to  vote  in  a  certain  way  in 
school  elections,  organizing  a  protest  meeting  in  or  out  of 
school  ( cf .,  social  studies ) ,  and  being  captain  of  an  athletic 
team.  This  latter  item  is  disliked  by  only  two  other  boys  in 
the  whole  class. 

Aggression.  In  this  category  Lyle  dislikes  such  aggression 
as:  throwing  spit  balls,  throwing  things  when  I  am  mad, 
playing  a  joke  on  a  teacher  (disliked  by  only  three  other 
boys),  picking  a  fight  when  I  am  in  the  mood,  and  telling 
someone  what  I  think  of  him.  He  has  only  five  likes  out  of  a 
total  of  33  items  in  this  category,  and  these  likes  are  distin- 
guished by  the  fact  that  again  they  are  not  open  expressions; 
in  fact,  they  seem  to  represent  what  may  be  called  f antasying 
about  his  aggressions.  He  likes:  thinking  of  what  111  do  when 
I  grow  up  to  people  who  have  been  mean  to  me,  checking 
up  on  things  that  teachers  say  in  order  to  find  out  if  they  are 
true  or  not,  reading  about  real  crimes  and  how  criminals  get 
caught,  and  thinking  about  how  to  become  the  cleverest, 
richest,  hardest  financial  genius  in  the  world. 

Authority.  The  striking  thing  here  is  that  Lyle  is  very  in- 
different to  authority.  His  very  indifference  seems  to  indicate 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  381 

a  certain  submlsslveness.  We  notice  that  lie  likes:  having  a 
teacher  lead  and  supervise  a  free-time  activity,  having  a 
teacher  outline  in  detail  what  should  be  studied  and  how  to 
go  about  it,  and  being  on  a  committee  where  the  chairman 
makes  the  decisions  instead  of  allowing  a  lot  of  discussion 
(he  is  the  only  boy  in  the  group  who  likes  this  item).  We 
draw  from  this  the  inference  that  Lyle  is  happiest  in  a 
teacher-controlled  situation,  and  that  for  some  reason  or  other 
pupil-controlled  or  pupil-dominated  situations  contain  some 
sort  of  threat  to  him. 

The  avoidance  of  asserting  himself  in  leadership  and  ag- 
gression and  his  apparent  liking  of  following  adult  authority 
and  avoidance  of  interaction  with  other  youngsters,  makes 
us  think  that  the  hostility  which  he  must  have  toward  his 
group  must  be  expressed  through  isolation  from  the  group 
rather  than  through  open  conflict,  except  perhaps  in  a  very 
spotty  and  spasmodic  way.  This  isolation  from  the  group  is 
probably  expressed  in  his  fantasy  activities  and  also  by  using 
his  intellectual  interests  as  a  way  of  achieving  superiority  ( in 
his  own  mind)  over  other  youngsters.  We  consider  that  he 
has  adopted  too  early  the  adult-approved  pattern,  without 
having  gone  through  the  necessary  stages  of  really  arriving 
at  it.  This,  we  tend  to  believe,  has  fixated  him  on  an  emo- 
tionally immature  level  of  development.  It  is  interesting  to 
note  that  he  likes:  having  people  take  me  for  older  than  I 
am,  discussing  things  with  older  people,  etc.  The  world  of 
adults  seems  to  threaten  "him  much  less  than  the  world  of 
other  youngsters. 

This  interest  in  older  people  is  in  striking  contrast  to  his 
seeming  lack  of  warm,  intimate,  friendly  interest  in  his  own 
age-group.  We  notice  for  instance,  that  Lyle  is  the  only  one 
in  his  group  who  dislikes:  trying  to  find  out  what  a  quiet, 
shy  person  is  really  like,  standing  up  against  a  group  and 
defending  a  person  who  has  been  picked  on,  etc.  Such  re- 
sponses make  us  think  that  he  is  probably  essentially  very 


382,        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

shy  himself.  We  tend  to  feel  that  while  his  constellation  of 
academic  interests  may  seem  "mature/'  there  is  a  great  de- 
pendence upon  adults.  Thus  he  seems  to  fear  those  situations 
in  which  he  is  unprotected.  We  notice,  for  instance,  that  he 
dislikes:  talking  to  strangers,  taking  a  long  trip  all  alone, 
having  my  parents  go  off  on  a  long  trip,  etc.  This  again  seems 
to  point  to  that  odd  combination  of  adultish  and  infantile 
qualities  in  Lyle  upon  which  we  have  remarked  before. 

In  connection  with  this  we  note  that  Lyle  is  in  every  in- 
stance indifferent  to  items  which  are  concerned  with  per- 
sonal appearance.  There  are  only  two  categories  to  which  he 
is  more  indifferent  than  he  is  to  preoccupation  with  cleanli- 
ness— out-of-school  activities,  and  same  sex. 

Some  general  comment  should  be  made  about  the  possible 
meaning  of  Lyle's  indifferences.  We  are  inclined  to  interpret 
them  in  two  ways:  in  part,  they  seem  to  represent  a  with- 
drawal of  his  energies  from  the  sociable  areas  and  throwing 
them  into  the  academic  area;  in  part,  they  may  be  an  escape 
or  protection  from  the  reality  situation.  The  very  great  in- 
difference (over  60  per  cent)  in  such  categories  as  same  sex, 
out-of-school  activities,  school  activities,  and  opposite  sex  is 
really  very  striking.  We  do  not  interpret  this  as  meaning  that 
Lyle  does  not  have  or  never  had  any  desire  for  social  inter- 
action, but  rather  we  interpret  it  as  meaning  that  for  some 
reason,  and  in  some  way,  he  finds  such  interaction  difficult 
and  disturbing.  We  tend  to  think  that  he  would  like  to  be 
able  to  get  along  with  other  people.  He  likes,  for  instance: 
carrying  on  imaginary  conversations  with  someone  whom  I 
like  or  admire,  imagining  situations  in  which  I  might  be  a 
hero,  planning  long  adventurous  journeys,  etc.  (In  connec- 
tion with  this  we  note  that  he  does  not  like  the  reality  ver- 
sions of  these  statements — i.e.,  he  does  not  like:  trying  to 
describe  my  innermost  feelings  to  a  friend,  standing  up  for 
someone  who  has  been  picked  on,  taking  a  long  trip  all  alone, 
etc.)  Thus  we  see  an  important  discrepancy  between  his 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  383 

fantasy  life  and  his  attitude  toward  his  real  life.  We  also 
notice  a  tendency  to  project  a  great  many  of  these  wishes  or 
unsatisfied  desires  into  the  future — he  likes  for  instance: 
planning  my  future  family.,  daydreaming  about  the  future, 
listening  to  fantastic  plays  about  the  future,  and,  on  the  other 
hand,  imagining  what  I  would  do  if  I  could  live  my  life  over 
again. 

In  conclusion,  one  may  say  that  Lyle  probably  does  not 
get  into  open  clashes  with  adults  and  is  very  likely  to  be 
academically  a  good  student.  His  age-mates  may  elect  him 
to  class  offices,  but  probably  few  of  diem,  if  any,  accept  him 
as  a  real  member  of  the  group.  A  number  of  youngsters  are 
apt  to  be  annoyed  by  him  and  make  him  the  butt  of  their 
jokes.  Lyle's  main  difficulties  seem  to  be  that  although  or 
because  he  has  accepted  prematurely  the  standards  and 
values  of  a  certain  group  of  adults — his  own  emotional 
development  has  been  warped  and  arrested. 

Statements  checked  and  written  in  by  teachers  who  filled  out  the 
Descriptive  Trait  Profile 

Shy,  retiring,  academic  minded  hoy.  Likes  science  especially.  Re- 
treats from  all  social  functions.  Adult  in  thinking  and  associa- 
tions. Brother  so  much  older.  Father  and  mother  very  brilliant 
Lyle  suffers  from  asthma  and  many  allergies  and  heart  weakness. 
Fear  of  death  is  strong. 

Observable  propelling  drives?  For  perfection  and  truth  in  scien- 
tific approach.  Strong  questioning  mind — extremely  modest — 
introvert. 

Vital,  active,  efficient,  well-organized  and  concentrated  in  his  at- 
tack on  school  work. 

In  thinking  through  a  problem  tries  within  the  range  of  his  ability 
to  obtain  a  wide  range  of  facts  and  considers  and  weighs  them 
impartially  before  arriving  at  a  conclusion. 

Outstanding  interests:  Science — impersonal  scientific  research. 
Anything  but  people. 


384        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Thought  of  as  being  only  moderately  boyish  in  dress,  activities 
and  interests,  and  physique. 

Average  looking.  Timid  soul  type.  Not  physically  strong.  Pleasant 
boy,  however. 

Too  secure  with  parents — and  himself — not  enough  with  boys  his 
age — adultish  in  standards. 

Holds  rigid  standards  for  himself — very  self-critical. 
Follower — and  yet  respected  because  he  knows  his  stuff. 

Tendency  toward  daydreaming,  fantasy — Lyle  is  an  introvert — 
but  in  the  scientific  sense. 

Ordinarily  contented,  satisfied,  serene.  Tends  to  make  the  best  of 
situations  even  when  they  are  unpleasant. 

Calm,  composed,  even,  level-headed,  well-balanced.  Expresses 
his  emotions  freely  and  is  not  either  uncontrolled  or  over- 
restrained. 

Generally  flexible  and  adaptable;  adjusts  readily  to  new  situa- 
tions, to  changes  in  routine,  etc. 

Self-confident  in  a  calm  way,  estimates  self  fairly  correctly,  ac- 
cepts own  assets  and  liabilities  fairly  realistically;  is  not  over- 
modest  nor  has  the  need  to  brag. 

Is  fairly  well-poised. 

Shies  away  from  students  of  the  same  sex. 

Is  respected  though  not  a  prominent  member  of  the  group.  His 
friendship  is  sought  and  he  enjoys  popularity  and  attracts  stu- 
dents. 

May  not  have  any  strong  individual  attachments,  yet  responds  in 
a  moderately  friendly  and  interested  way  to  the  opposite  sex. 

RELIABILITY 

The  reliability  of  each  category  of  scores  on  the  two  ques- 
tionnaires was  computed  by  the  Kuder-Richardson  formula 
for  a  sample  population  of  1?000  students,  divided  evenly 
between  boys  and  girls  and  among  grades  seven  to  twelve  in 
several  representative  schools.  The  results,  along  with  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  385 

range  of  scores,  the  mean,  and  the  standard  deviation  on 
likes  and  dislikes  in  each  category,  are  given  in  Tables  in 
Appendix  VI.  In  general,  the  coefficients  of  reliability  range 
from  .53  to  .86,  the  median  coefficient  for  likes  being  .78 
and  for  dislikes  .75.  Only  three  categories  of  likes  and  six  of 
dislikes  have  a  reliability  coefficient  lower  than  .70.  While  a 
higher  degree  of  reliability  would  be  desirable,  considering 
the  intrinsic  variability  of  behavor  in  this  area,  the  reliability 
of  other  tests  in  this  field,  and  the  way  in  which  one  score 
is  continually  checked  against  another,  the  obtained  relia- 
bilities were  considered  sufficiently  high  for  the  purposes  of 
these  tests  and  for  the  manner  in  which  they  were  inter- 
preted. 

VALIDITY 

The  problem  of  validity  of  a  technique  of  appraisal  is  one 
of  paramount  importance.  It  is  a  complex  problem,  however. 
On  the  long  road  at  the  start  of  which  are  the  assumptions 
which  underlie  the  technique  and  at  the  end  of  which  are 
the  final  interpretations  or  descriptions  of  a  subject,  there 
are  many  points  at  which  validity  should  be  questioned  and 
scrutinized.  As  it  has  been  stated  above,  the  degree  of  effec- 
tiveness of  the  present  method  of  study  of  personality  was 
checked  upon  at  the  very  beginning  of  the  study  when  33 
college  students  were  described  and  these  descriptions  com- 
pared with  the  school  records  of  these  students.  Similar 
informal  studies  have  been  conducted  as  work  progressed. 
These  studies  helped  in  guiding  the  staff  in  its  experimenta- 
tion with  untried  methods  and  suggested  the  abandonment 
of  certain  ones  which  were  not  found  fruitful.  The  following 
is  a  presentation  of  some  of  the  findings  on  validity  to  date. 

Discussion  of  the  Evidence  on  the 
Validity  of  the  Questionnaires 

Broadly  speaking,  validity  may  be  broken  down  into  two 
parts:  (a)  validity  of  the  instrument  as  such,  and  (b)  validity 
of  the  interpretation  of  the  results. 


386         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Genuineness  of  response.  One  important  element  involved 
in  the  validity  of  any  instrument  of  appraisal  is  the  so-called 
genuineness  of  the  response  of  the  subject.  By  genuineness 
of  response,  in  this  instance,  is  meant  the  extent  to  which 
the  response  represents  the  real  feelings  of  the  individual. 
If?  as  may  be  the  case  in  such  an  instrument,  the  response 
represents  wishful  thinking,  it  is  nevertheless  genuine,  for 
the  wishful  thinking  is  an  important  part  of  the  individual's 
feelings.  It  is  possible  to  have  genuineness  of  response  with- 
out making  valid  interpretations  of  these  responses,  although 
it  is  difficult  to  see  how  the  contrary  might  be  true. 

One  would  naturally  expect  some  fluctuation  in  category 
scores  from  year  to  year  because  of  growth  factors.  If  the 
response  were  not  genuine  one  would  expect  marked  and 
unpredictable  fluctuations  in  category  scores  from  year  to 
year.  One  would  be  dealing  with  chance  or  random  reac- 
tions. If,  however,  after  having  made  allowance  for  the 
growth  factor,  there  still  is  a  fairly  high  relationship  between 
the  category  scores  one  year,  and  the  scores  on  a  retest  a  year 
later,  one  might  be  justified  in  concluding  that  there  is  con- 
stancy, and  therefore  genuineness  of  response.  The  following 
table  shows  the  results  obtained  when  correlations  were  run 
between  the  category  scores  of  48  boys  and  56  girls  who 
responded  to  the  questionnaires  in  the  seventh,  eighth,  or 
ninth  grades  one  year,  and  in  the  eighth,  ninth,  or  tenth  the 
next. 

These  data  seem  to  indicate  that  having  made  allowances 
for  the  growth  factor  there  is  still  a  high  degree  of  con- 
sistency of  response,  and  therefore  of  predictability.  It  would 
seem  justifiable  to  assume  that  genuineness  of  response  was 
a  contributor  to  this  constancy  factor. 

In  preparing  the  questionnaires  it  was  felt  important  to 
learn  how  students  feel  about  this  approach.  In  an  attempt 
to  determine  this,  toward  the  end  of  the  third  questionnaire 
was  placed  the  item:  "Answering  questionnaires  like  this," 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS 


387 


TABLE    2 

Product- Moment  Correlations  of  Scores  Obtained  One  Tear  Apart 


Category 


Leadership .78 

Fantasy .77 

Life-Death .76 

Identification  with  Others .74 

Aggression  (c) .72 

Total  (c) 70 

Self-acceptance .70 

Total  (b) 68 

Humor .68 

Cleanliness .68 

Mystery .66 

Methodical .65 

Out-of-School .62 

Aggression  (b) .61 

Dramatics .58 

Non-Identification .58 

Magic .58 

Severity .57 

Family 55 

Opposite  Sex .53 

Authority .46 

Same  Sex 40 

Solitary .44 

School  Activities .34 


48  Boys 


56  Girls 


.48 
.77 
.77 
.70 
.65 
.81 
.70 
.75 
.74 
.49 
.60 
.59 
.57 
.61 
.69 
.67 
.70 
.63 
.68 
.64 
.20 
.78 
.55 
.68 


It  may  be  seen  from  the  following  tabulation  of  responses  to 
this  item  that  girls  in  all  grades  enjoy  the  questionnaires 
more  than  the  boys,  that  students  in  the  lower  grades  like 
them  more  than  the  older  students,  that  in  most  grades  more 
students  marked  this  item  like  than  dislike  and  that  only  in 
the  case  of  the  tenth  grade  boys  did  as  many  as  41  per  cent 
of  them  mark  this  item  dislike. 

Discussion  of  the  Evidence  of 
Validity  of  Interpretations 

1.  Validation  through  information  from  the  school.  During 
the  course  of  the  present  study  the  questionnaires  were  ad- 


388        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

TABLE   3 

Per  Cent  of  Students  Responding  Like,  Indifferent,  and  Dislike  to  "Answering 
Questionnaires  Like  This" 


Number 

Per  Cent  of  Boys 
Responding 

Per  Cent  of  Girls 
Responding 

Grade         

Boys 

Girls 

L           I 

D 

L 

I 

D 

7 

78 

91 

42 

32 

26 

66 

26 

8 

8 

60 

50 

47 

30 

23 

78 

8 

14 

9 

164 

177 

41 

30 

29 

57 

28 

15 

10 

97 

176 

32 

27 

41 

49 

28 

23 

11 

114 

200 

42 

24 

34 

43 

23 

34 

12 

126 

95 

30 

32 

38 

35 

30 

35 

i 

ministered  widely  in  a  number  of  schools  and  in  several  of 
these  schools  the  Evaluation  Staff  agreed  to  furnish  written 
descriptions  of  some  of  the  students'  personalities  in  order  to 
check  on  the  correctness  of  the  interpretations  derived  from 
the  questionnaires.  The  faculties  in  the  schools  selected  the 
students  for  this  study  before  the  questionnaires  had  been 
administered.  The  only  information  on  these  selected  stu- 
dents which  the  staff  had  was  the  name,  age,  grade,  and  sex 
of  the  student  and  the  responses  to  the  questionnaires;  on 
the  basis  of  this  information  a  rather  detailed  description  of 
the  personality  of  each  student  was  prepared.8 

While  the  written  descriptions  of  the  students  were  being 
prepared  by  members  of  the  Evaluation  Staff,  teachers  who 
knew  these  students  best  rated  them  on  the  Descriptive  Trait 
Profile.  The  Profiles  were  held  by  the  school  until  the  school 
received  the  interpreters*  descriptions  of  students.  As  an  ad- 
ditional check  on  the  descriptions  derived  from  the  question- 
naires, the  teachers  who  had  rated  these  students  were  asked 

8  The  case  which  was  presented  in  the  preceding  section  is  one  of  these 
studies.  It  was  selected  for  presentation  because  it  was  shorter  than  most. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  389 

to  read  these  descriptions  carefully  and  to  make  marginal 
notes,  especially  in  instances  when  they  disagreed  with  the 
picture  presented.  By  this  method  16  case  studies  were  made. 
2.  Method  of  appraising  the  extent  of  agreement  and  dis- 
agreement with  the  material  submitted  by  the  schools.  Since 
the  present  approach  to  personality  study  is  thought  of  essen- 
tially as  a  technique  which  aims  to  bring  out  some  of  the 
outstanding  features  of  a  personality,  different  patterns  of 
organization  of  energies  of  individuals,  it  was  felt  that  the 
final  validation  should  employ  methods  suitable  for  such  ma- 
terial. This  made  it  impossible  to  attempt  to  arrive  at  some 
single  index  or  coefficient  which  would  represent  the  degree 
of  validity  of  the  interpretations.  It  was  thought  further  that 
the  problem  of  validation  of  descriptions  of  students  derived 
from  the  interest  questionnaires  involves  the  examination  of 
the  cases  from  three  angles.  First,  there  must  be  an  appraisal 
of  the  comprehensiveness  of  the  description  of  the  students, 
the  extent  to  which  the  analysis  brings  out  a  number  of  sig- 
nificant facts  about  the  student  (significant  from  the  point  of 
view  of  the  counselor  and  classroom  teacher).  Second,  there 
must  be  an  appraisal  of  the  degree  of  consistency  or  incon- 
sistency between  the  interpretation  of  the  questionnaire  re- 
sults and  the  material  presented  by  the  school  on  the  same 
students.  Third,  since  the  descriptions  derived  from  the 
questionnaires  at  times  attempted  to  go  beyond  what  the 
classroom  teacher  might  know  about  the  student,  a  judgment 
had  to  be  made  regarding  the  reasonableness  or  probability 
that  these  inferences  were  valid  in  the  light  of  all  the  informa- 
tion available  on  the  student.  The  same  judgment  had  to  be 
made  in  cases  when  there  was  an  actual  disagreement  be- 
tween the  two  descriptions;  the  teacher's  judgment  could  not 
be  accepted  as  necessarily  infallible  any  more  than  could 
that  of  the  interpreters. 

Because  none  of  the  simpler  statistical  methods  could  be 
used  to  measure  the  degree  to  which  two  pictures  of  a  per- 


390         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

sonality  coincide  or  differ,  or  to  determine  which  picture  is 
more  likely  to  be  psychologically  correct,  it  was  thought  that 
the  opinions  of  a  number  of  competent  judges  would  form 
the  best  evaluation  of  this  study.  In  other  words,  the  criterion 
of  enlightened  common  sense  seemed  to  be  the  most  feasible 
method  of  appraising  the  validity  of  the  interpretation. 

Sixteen  judges  were  selected  and  they  were  asked  to  guide 
themselves  by  the  following  questions  in  making  their  judg- 
ments: (1)  Would  most  reasonably  competent  people  tend 
to  agree  or  disagree  that  the  same  tendency  or  characteristic 
of  the  student  was  commented  upon  by  the  interpreters  and 
by  the  teachers,  even  though  they  may  have  described  this 
characteristic  in  different  words  and  in  a  different  context? 
(2)  From  my  experience  with  children  and  adults,  from  my 
observations  of  human  behavior  and  motivation  and  from  all 
facts  presented  in  this  case,  which  of  the  two  statements 
about  the  student  seems  more  likely  to  be  correct — the  one 
made  by  the  interpreters  or  the  one  submitted  by  the  school? 

The  judges  were  asked  to  use  the  following  procedure  in 
making  their  evaluation  of  this  material: 

1.  Read  through  the  interpretations  of  the  interest  question- 
naires carefully. 

2.  Read  the  comments  of  the  teachers,  marginal  and  other- 
wise, including  the  information  from  the  Descriptive  Trait 
Profile. 

3.  Make  a  statement  regarding  (a)  the  degree  of  compre- 
hensiveness of  the  picture  of  the  student,  (b)  the  degree 
of  agreement  between  the  interpretation  and  the  data 
from  the  school,  and  finally,  (c)  in  cases  of  disagreement, 
a  judgment  regarding  which  of  the  two  pictures  seems 
most  reasonable  or  valid  in  the  light  of  all  die  information 
gathered  on  the  student. 

A  list  of  statements  was  prepared  for  each  of  the  three 
questions  (a,  b,  and  c)  on  which  judgment  was  sought.  The 
judges  were  instructed  to  check  the  appropriate  statement 
but  to  regard  these  statements  as  merely  suggestive  and  to 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  391 

feel  free  to  make  their  own  statements.  The  tabulation  of 
statements  checked  or  written  in  by  the  judges  will  be  found 
on  the  following  pages  for  all  16  cases.  Since  each  case  was 
judged  by  four  judges  the  total  number  of  judgments  for 
each  of  the  three  questions  should  normally  be  16  X  4  =  64. 
Because  some  judges  checked  more  than  one  statement,  the 
actual  number  of  statements  is  often  above  64. 

LIST  OF  JUDGES 

Peter  Bios,  Institute  for  the  Study  of  Personality  Development, 
New  York  City. 

J.  F.  Brown,  Professor  of  Psychology,  University  of  Kansas, 
Lawrence,  Kansas. 

P.  S.  de  Q.  Cabot,  Director,  Cambridge-Somerville  Youth  Study, 
Cambridge,  Massachusetts. 

Frank  S.  Freeman,  Professor  of  Education,  Cornell  University, 
Ithaca,  New  York. 

Robert  J.  Havighurst,  Professor  of  Education  and  Secretary  of 
the  Committee  on  Human  Development,  The  University  of 
Chicago,  Chicago,  Illinois. 

Josephine  R.  Hilgard,  M.D.,  Fellow  in  Psychiatry,  Institute  for 
Juvenile  Research,  Chicago,  Illinois. 

L.  L.  Jarvie,  Director  of  Guidance  and  Curriculum,  Rochester 
Athenaeum  and  Mechanics  Institute,  Rochester,  New  York. 

Harold  E.  Jones,  Director,  Institute  of  Child  Welfare,  University 
of  California,  Berkeley,  California. 

Jean  W.  Macfarlane,  Director  of  Child  Guidance  Study,  Institute 
of  Child  Welfare,  University  of  California,  Berkeley,  Cali- 
fornia. 

George  J.  Mohr,  M.D.,  Clinical  Staff,  The  Institute  of  Psycho- 
analysis, Chicago,  Illinois;  Associate  Professor  of  Criminol- 
ogy, University  of  Illinois  Medical  School,  Urbana,  Illinois. 

Willard  C.  Olson,  Director  of  Research  in  Child  Development; 
Professor  of  Education,  University  of  Michigan,  Ann  Arbor, 
Michigan. 

Daniel  Prescott,  Professor  of  Education,  The  University  of  Chi- 
cago, Chicago,  Illinois. 

Fritz  Redl,  Professor  of  Psychology,  Wayne  University,  Detroit, 


ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Michigan;  Division  on  Child  Development  and  Teacher  Per- 
sonnel, Commission  on  Teacher  Education,  The  University 
of  Chicago,  Chicago,  Illinois. 

Helen  Ross,  Research  Associate,  The  Institute  for  Psychoanalysis, 
Chicago,  Illinois. 

Verner  M.  Sims,  Professor  of  Psychology,  University  of  Alabama. 

Herbert  R.  Stolz,  M.D.,  Assistant  Superintendent  in  Charge  of 
Individual  Guidance,  Oakland  Public  Schools,  Oakland,  Cali- 
fornia. 

TABLE  4 

Judgment  as  to  the  comprehensiveness  of  picture  of  stu- 
dent, the  usefulness  of  this  information  to  the  counselor 
or  teacher. 

Statement 

No.  of  times 
checked 

1.  The  description  of  the  personality  of  the  stu- 
dent  is   very   clear    and    comprehensive;    it 
should  be  of  real  value  to  a  counselor.  15 

2.  The  analysis  seems  to  have  come  very  close 
to  several  of  the   central   difficulties  of  the 
youngster;  it  should  be  of  help  to  the  coun- 
selor. 29 

3.  Although  the  interest  questionnaire  did  not 
obtain  a  consistent  and  clear-cut  picture  of 
the  student,  the  study  unearthed  some  impor- 
tant hypotheses  about  him.  12 

4.  The  description  from  the  interest  question- 
naires is  too  vague  and  equivocal  to  make  a 
judgment.  2 

5.  The   statements   in  the   interpretation  could 
apply    to    anyone — there    is    nothing    which 
seems  to  apply  to  this  youngster  specifically 

and  alone.  1 

6.  Many  dominant  characteristics  mentioned  by 
the  school  are  missed  completely  in  the  inter- 
pretation, 8 

Total        67 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  393 

TABLES 

Judgment  as  to  the  degree  of  agreement  between  inter- 
pretation and  data  from  school. 

Statement 

No.  of  times 
checked 

1.  The  picture  presented  is  highly  consistent  with 

the  material  submitted  by  the  school.  17 

2.  There  Is  agreement  on  important  aspects  of 
personality,  disagreement  on  the  less  impor- 
tant. 9 

3.  There  is  general  agreement  between  the  re- 
port of  school  and  the  interpretation,  but  the 
interpretation  seems  to  over-emphasize  or  ex- 
aggerate certain  aspects.  8 

4.  There  is  agreement  in  part,  but  there  is  a  lack 

of  verification  by  the  school  on  details.  7 

5.  There  is  excellent  agreement  in  some  parts, 
whereas  in  other  parts  there  is  marked  dis- 
agreement. 8 

6.  The  school  gives  a  "surface"  picture  of  be- 
havior, whereas  questionnaire  results  describe 
"central"    or    "underlying"    behaviors.     This 
makes  a  comparison  difficult.  1 

7.  There  is  marked  disagreement  in  most  areas; 

only  in  minor  points  is  there  agreement.  2 

8.  There  is  little  agreement  between  the  inter- 
preters' analysis  of  the  major  outline  of  per- 
sonality  and   the   version   presented   by  the 
school.  8 

9.  Neither  of  the  reports  gives  a  clear-cut  pic- 
ture; therefore,  a  comparison  is  difficult.  1 

10.  Insufficient  data  from   school  for  making  a 
judgment.  2 

11.  There  seems  to  be  no  relationship  between 
the  interpretation   and  the   description  pre- 
sented by  the  school.  1 

Total        64 


394        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

TABLE   6 

Judgment  as  to  which  picture  seems  most  reasonable, 
or  valid  in  the  light  of  all  the  information  gathered  on 
the  student.  (In  cases  of  disagreement,  or  in  cases  in 
which  the  interpretation  goes  beyond  the  material  pre- 
sented by  the  school. ) 

Statement 

No.  of  times 
checked 

1.  The  interpretations  which  go  beyond  the  ma- 
terial submitted  by  the   school  are  psycho- 
logically very  consistent  with  the  total  picture.         19 

2.  The   description    derived   from   the   interest 
questionnaires  seems  more  convincing.  I  tend 
to  accept  it  as  being  more  likely  to  be  psycho- 
logically correct.  18 

3.  Even  though  the  school's  description  of  the 
youngster's  behavior  and  the  interpretation  of 
his  feelings  as  revealed  through  the  question- 
naires do  not  seem  to  coincide,  it  is  very  prob- 
able that  each  is  valid  at  its  own  level.  9 

4.  Analysis  seems  to  have  hit  upon  the  central 
themes  of  conflict,  a  fact  which  renders  it  espe- 
cially valuable  for  the  counselor.  2 

5.  The  questionnaire  results  help  to  get  at  some 
of  the  causes  of  the  picture  of  maladjustment 
painted  by  the  teachers.  1 

6.  The  conclusions  of  the  analysis  give  perspec- 
tive and  psychological  meaning  to  teachers* 
statements.  1 

7.  The  questionnaire  results  and  the  school  re- 
port supplement  each  other,  though  I  regard 
the  questionnaire  as  the  more  valuable  psy- 
chologically. 1 

8.  The  questionnaire  interpretation  is  more  pene- 
trating than  the  school  material.  The  school 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  395 

description,  while  helpful,  has  a  few  incon- 
sistencies; and  it  is  more  of  a  surface  descrip- 
tion. 1 
9.  The  description  presented  by  the  school  and 
the  description  derived  from  the  interest  ques- 
tionnaires supplement  each  other  to  form  a 
consistent  picture  of  the  student.  19 

10.  There  are  too  many  contradictory  statements 

from  the  school  to  make  a  Judgment.  1 

11.  Insufficient  data  from  school  to  make  a  judg- 
ment. 1 

12.  There  are  too  many  contradictions  in  the  ma- 
terial to  make  a  judgment.  2 

13.  The  description  presented  by  the  school  seems 
more  convincing  or  plausible.  I  tend  to  accept 
it  as  being  more  likely  to  be  psychologically 
correct.  9 

Total        84 

These  three  tables  indicate  a  preponderance  of  opinion  in 
favor  of  the  inferences  about  students  drawn  from  the  ques- 
tionnaires. Of  194  judgments  which  may  be  classified  as 
favorable  or  unfavorable,  157  favor  the  questionnaires,  while 
37  express  some  criticism  or  indicate  a  preference  for  the 
materials  presented  by  the  school.  Of  the  latter,  31  express 
only  the  following  criticisms:  many  dominant  characteristics 
mentioned  by  the  school  are  missed  in  the  interpretation 
(7),  the  interpretation  seems  to  over-emphasize  or  exag- 
gerate certain  aspects  (6),  there  is  little  agreement  between 
the  interpretation  and  the  school's  version  (8),  and  the 
school's  description  seems  more  plausible  ( 9 ) .  Some  of  these 
were  not  intended  as  criticisms  for  they  were  frequently  ex- 
pressed by  judges  who  preferred  the  version  given  by  the 
interpretation.  When  it  is  recalled  that  the  material  pre- 
sented by  the  school  was  the  result  of  several  years  of  close 
association  with  and  study  of  students,  while  the  interpreta- 
tion was  based  on  three  short  tests  by  investigators  who  had 


396        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

never  seen  these  students  and  knew  nothing  else  about  them, 
the  preponderance  of  critical  opinion  in  favor  of  the  ques- 
tionnaires is  encouraging. 

POSSIBLE  USES  OF  THE  QUESTIONNAIRES 

It  may  be  well  to  indicate  at  this  point  that  paper  and 
pencil  interest  questionnaires  do  not  necessarily  constitute 
the  best  method  of  studying  interests.  It  is  possible  that  skill- 
fully conducted  interviews,  direct  observation,  etc.,  may  yield 
much  richer,  more  dependable  material.  On  the  other  hand,  it 
may  be  that  one  of  the  advantages  of  a  questionnaire  is  the 
fact  that  a  mass  of  comparable  data  are  secured  on  a  large 
number  of  students  at  one  time.  This  material  can  be  used 
for  studies  of  individuals  or  for  studies  of  groups  or  for 
studies  of  shifts  of  interests  occurring  with  age  in  boys  and 
girls. 

Value  to  the  Counselor 

1.  It  is  expected  that  persons  who  work  out  a  few  of  the 
individual  interpretations  and  who  begin  to  see  the  intimate 
relationship  between  the  so-called  "academic"  interests  and 
the  emotional  dispositions  of  the  individual,  will  begin  to 
view  the  in-school  behavior  of  youngsters  quite  differently. 

2.  The  questionnaires  afford  the  opportunity  to  look  at  a 
student  from  a  new  angle — the  expression  of  his  likes  and 
dislikes  in  a  great  many  areas.  These  one  examines  in  terms 
of  the  individual  and  in  terms  of  how  he  compares  with  the 
other  members  of  the  group. 

3.  The  questionnaire  results  suggest  a  number  of  hypoth- 
eses about  the  student — point  to  directions  which  ought  to 
be  investigated.  The  questionnaires  are  expected  to  serve 
the  function  of  a  time-saving  device  since  they  point  out 
specific  areas  which  have  to  be  investigated  first.  Such  in- 
vestigations are  not  blind  trial-and-error  searches  for  infor- 
mation, since  they  are  based  on  an  hypothesis  and  since  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  397 

area  investigated  is  naturally  connected  with  some  aspect  of 
the  student  which  is  of  importance  to  the  educator. 

4.  On  the  basis  of  the  information  derived  from  the  pic- 
ture of  the  interests  and  on  the  basis  of  the  information  ob- 
tained from  other  sources,  it  is  expected  that  courses  of  ac- 
tion will  suggest  themselves.  These  remedial  steps  will  be 
based  on  a  knowledge  of  the  student's  abilities,  on  a  knowl- 
edge of  his  academic  interests,  and  on  some  facts  regarding 
his  personal  and  social  adjustment. 

The  question  of  the  extent  to  which  it  is  legitimate  to 
discuss  with  students  their  scores  is  being  asked  repeatedly. 
Some  teachers  even  feel  that  a  description  of  a  student  de- 
rived from  the  questionnaires  should  be  read  to  the  young- 
ster. Those  who  have  worked  with  the  questionnaires  take  a 
very  definite  stand  on  this  point.  It  is  felt  very  strongly  about 
8.2b  and  8.2c  that  the  scores  should  never  be  shown  to  a 
youngster,  just  as  the  youngster  is  never  shown  his  Intelli- 
gence Quotient.  There  are  two  main  reasons  for  taking  this 
stand. 

In  the  first  place,  by  making  the  students  self-conscious 
about  the  questionnaire,  by  revealing  to  them  the  nature  of 
the  categories  on  which  they  expressed  themselves,  one 
would  spoil  the  chances  for  administering  the  questionnaires 
again.  The  next  time  the  answers  would  be  apt  to  be  much 
less  spontaneous;  the  student  would  tend  either  to  give  the 
teacher  what  he  thinks  the  teacher  wants  him  to  give,  or 
give  whatever  ideas  he  has  regarding  his  liking  for  a  given 
category  as  such.  It  would  be  very  similar  to  giving  the  stu- 
dents the  key  to  questionnaires  and  asking  them  to  respond 
to  items  as  they  are  arranged  under  the  various  categories 
instead  of  having  the  statements  in  a  random  order.  This 
consideration  applies  to  8.2a  as  well  as  to  8.2b  and  8.2c. 

The  second  reason  which  makes  letting  the  students  see 
their  own  scores  seem  undesirable  is  the  injury  which  this 
may  do  to  them.  When  one  constantly  sees  adults  who  take 


398         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

numerical  scores,  medians,  etc,,  as  if  they  were  absolute  and 
infallible  realities,  one  can  easily  imagine  the  damage  which 
may  be  done  to  a  youngster  who  would  suddenly  be  con- 
fronted by  the  fact  that  he  scored  way  below  the  median  of 
the  class  in  liking  his  family  or  that  he  came  out  highest  in 
the  class  in  disliking  it.  Even  if  the  scores  were  absolutely 
correct  representations  of  youngsters'  feelings,  pointing  them 
out  to  the  student  would  not  alter  these  feelings,  but  would 
be  apt  to  increase  the  self-consciousness  and,  therefore,  the 
conflict  about  these  feelings.  There  seems  to  be  a  very  com- 
mon misconception  in  the  minds  of  many  people  that  the 
mere  pointing  out  of  a  fact  to  a  person  has  therapeutic  effects. 
This  misconception  may  be  due  to  two  things.  In  the  first 
place,  it  is  true  that  in  relatively  simple  matters,  pointing  out 
a  fact  to  a  person  often  makes  this  person  watch  himself  in 
this  respect  or  makes  him  actually  change  his  behavior.  For 
instance,  when  a  student  consistently  misspells  a  word  or 
has  difficulty  in  constructing  a  sentence,  pointing  out  his 
shortcoming  to  him  may  have  beneficial  effects.  In  the  area 
of  feelings  or  emotions,  however,  the  pointing  out  of  a  ten- 
sion or  conflict  or  the  pointing  out  of  a  symptom  of  a  tension 
often  tends  to  aggravate  the  situation. 

In  the  second  place,  this  misconception  may  be  due  to  an 
incorrect  understanding  of  the  word  "insight,"  which  is  fre- 
quently found  in  psychological  literature.  Contrary  to  the 
popular  notion,  an  effective  guidance  worker,  psychologist, 
or  psychiatrist  does  not  give  insight  to  his  client,  but,  when 
this  is  indicated,  so  works  with  the  client  that  the  latter  gains 
insight  into  himself.  Giving  insight,  instead  of  allowing  the 
person  to  develop  insight,  often  only  strengthens  the  block 
which  prevents  the  person  from  understanding  what  is 
really  operating  in  him.  To  help  a  student  gain  insight  re- 
quires a  great  deal  of  skill  and  considerable  experience.  The 
classroom  teacher  who  may  have  some  qualms  about  an  un- 
dertaking of  this  sort,  will  nevertheless  be  able  to  gain  cer- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  399 

tain  insights  which  will  assist  him  or  her  in  manipulating  the 
environment  of  the  youngster  as  a  means  of  making  it  easier 
for  the  student  to  make  the  necessary  adjustments. 

It  is  somewhat  less  dangerous  to  let  students  see  their 
scores  on  8.2a.  In  certain  situations  this  may  be  permissible, 
much  depending  on  the  type  of  youngster  one  is  dealing  with 
and  much  depending  on  the  relationship  between  the  stu- 
dent and  the  interpreter  of  the  questionnaire.  One  should  be 
always  cognizant  of  the  fact,  however,  that  such  a  discussion 
is  almost  certain  to  make  it  impossible  to  give  the  same  ques- 
tionnaire again.  Moreover,  the  student  is  apt  to  take  his  score, 
as  compared  with  the  median  of  the  class,  as  evidence  of  a 
permanent  characteristic  of  himself,  perhaps  as  evidence  of 
an  inherent  lack  of  interest  in  the  subject,  perhaps  even  as 
evidence  of  his  inability  to  do  well  in  this  area.  Trying  to 
correct  this  by  telling  a  student:  "Now  just  snap  out  of  it, 
John,  you  can  be  interested  in  this  as  much  as  anyone  else!" 
can  hardly  be  expected  to  stimulate  a  real  interest. 

In  cases  of  students  who  are  really  eager  to  learn  more 
about  themselves  and  their  performances  on  the  question- 
naire, it  is  suggested  that,  without  showing  them  their  actual 
scores  and  the  median  of  the  class,  one  could  pick  out  the 
highest  interests  of  the  individual,  mentioning  to  him  that 
they  seemed  to  be  his  highest  interests  and  pointing  the  dis- 
cussion in  the  direction  of  what  this  student  actually  enjoys 
doing,  what  he  actually  enjoys  at  school,  etc.  The  areas  of 
low  interests,  as  revealed  by  the  scores,  do  not  have  to  be 
discussed  with  reference  to  the  questionnaire  but  may  come 
up  for  discussion  naturally,  as  the  outcome  of  the  whole  con- 
versation. The  above  approach  in  which  one  starts  with  the 
area  of  outgoing  feelings  and  interests  of  the  student  is 
thought  to  be  much  more  positive.  This  positive  approach 
is  apt  to  make  the  whole  discussion  a  pleasant  and  spon- 
taneous one  and  is  apt  to  cement  the  relationship  between 
the  counselor  and  counselee  rather  than  create  a  breach. 


400        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

The  Administration  of  the  Questionnaires 

Questions  relative  to  the  method  of  administration  have 
been  brought  up  by  a  number  of  teachers.  Some  seem  to  feel 
that  the  situation  under  which  the  questionnaires  are  admin- 
istered has  a  great  deal  to  do  with  the  results. 

It  is  thought  best  to  present  the  questionnaires  rather  cas- 
ually, perhaps  as  part  of  a  survey  of  the  school  or  as  part  of 
a  study  of  pupils'  interests.  Certainly  the  validity  of  the  re- 
sults is  considerably  reduced  if  one  tells  the  students  that  the 
school  wants  to  find  out  "everything  about  their  personali- 
ties" or  if  one  singles  out  a  troublesome  student  and  lets  him 
take  the  questionnaires  by  himself  or  under  the  immediate 
supervision  of  some  stern  adult.  Preferably  the  question- 
naires should  not  be  given  at  a  time  when  they  draw  the 
students  from  an  activity  which  they  particularly  enjoy. 
Their  resentment  will  probably  reflect  itself  in  their  re- 
sponses. The  traditional  "test"  situation  should  be  avoided  as 
much  as  possible  and  every  effort  should  be  made  to  make 
it  a  pleasurable  experience. 

The  fact  that  most  of  the  items  in  the  questionnaires  were 
furnished  by  youngsters  indicates  that  frank  statements  can 
be  obtained  from  diem.  The  fact  that  such  responses  can  be 
obtained  only  by  a  person  in  whom  the  children  have  com- 
plete confidence,  because  of  this  person's  tact  in  dealing  with 
their  feelings,  must  also  be  borne  in  mind. 

SUMMARY 

In  concluding  this  chapter  it  may  be  well  to  point  out  some 
of  the  main  features  of  the  present  technique  of  study  of 
personal  and  social  adjustment.  These  features  may  be  sum- 
marized as  follows: 

1.  Indirection.  It  is  felt  that  the  questionnaires  do  not  ap- 
pear to  the  students  to  be  obviously  a  "personality  test/'  and 
that  therefore  they  do  not  arouse  the  anxieties  which  many 
such  tests  evoke.  They  have  been  found  to  be  actually  en- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  401 

joyed  by  a  great  many  children.  Most  of  the  items  in  the 
questionnaires  have  been  obtained  from  children's  diary 
records  of  their  daily  activities.  Whenever  possible,  young- 
sters' language  was  preserved  in  the  inventories. 

2.  Flexibility.  The  inventories  do  not  attempt  to  discover 
whether  the  student  does  or  does  not  fall  into  one  of  a  group 
of  patterns  prearranged  by  the  investigator.  Rather  they  at- 
tempt to  provide  a  field  upon  which,  with  certain  limitations, 
the  student  may  trace  his  own  pattern  or  profile.  The  sub- 
jects are  thought  to  reveal  their  various  affective  trends 
through  the  configuration  and  the  interrelation  of  their  re- 
sponses. 

3.  Aims  at  a  dynamic  instead  of  a  static  picture.  This 
method  attempts  to  reveal  how  a  student  operates  or  func- 
tions, what  adjustive  devices  he  employs,  how  he  feels  about 
various  activities.  This  aspect  of  the  method  is  expected  to  be 
of  particular  practical  usefulness. 

4.  Aims  at  gaining  insigfot  into  students  motivation.  In- 
sofar as  it  is  possible  through  the  examination  of  specific 
responses  to  discern  common  elements  in  new  groupings  of 
likes  and  dislikes,  one  is  frequently  able  to  see  what  lies 
behind  these  feelings.  This  gives  useful  clues  as  to  how  to 
motivate  the  student's  interest  in  some  other  activities. 

5.  Tends  to  make  a  student's  academic  likes  and  dislikes 
understandable  in  terms  of  the  organization  of  his  person- 
ality. It  is  felt  that  only  too  frequently  there  is  a  dichotomy 
in  our  concept  of  a  personality.  The  thinking  life  of  a  stu- 
dent is  thought  of  as  a  discrete,  separate  unit  determined  by 
his  I.Q.  and  "special  abilities"  and  unrelated  to  his  needs, 
drives,  and  goals.  The  approach  outlined  above  aims  to  bring 
to  light  certain  common  trends  in  the  individual  which  evi- 
dence themselves  both  through  his  academic  interests  and 
other  activities.  Should  it  be  possible  to  give  a  classroom 
teacher  an  instrument  which  will  enable  her  to  relate  the 
strivings  and  the  goals  of  a  student  and  the  possible  satisfac- 


402         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

tion  of  these  goals  to  work  on  certain  academic  problems,  the 
opportunity  to  make  education  meaningful  to  children  would 
be  increased  greatly. 

6.  Final  results  are  descriptive  rather  than  definitive.  In- 
stead of  having  the  final  picture  a  score  or  series  of  scores,  it 
is  a  brief  personality  sketch  or  study.  This  sketch  is  derived 
from  the  way  in  which  the  individual  student  reacts  to  a 
great  many  fields  of  activity:  academic  interests,  sociable  ac- 
tivities, and  activities  which  indicate  his  attitudes  toward 
himself. 

7.  Questionnaire  results  are  inferential.  The  present  ap- 
proach should  not  be  thought  of  as  a  "test"  or  as  an  instru- 
ment which  is  meant  to  give  conclusive  evidence  regarding 
a  student's  personality.  The  results  are  inferential.  The  inter- 
pretations should  always  be  regarded  as  hypotheses  which, 
when  combined  with  other  information  on  the  student,  might 
prove  useful  to  the  counselor. 


Chapter  VII 

INTERPRETATION  AND  USES  OF 
EVALUATION  DATA 


The  preceding  chapters  have  explained  the  development  of 
evaluation  instruments  in  several  major  areas  of  objectives. 
References  to  methods  of  interpretation  and  uses  of  these 
instruments  were  confined  to  single  instruments  or  pairs  of 
instruments.  Other  problems  of  interpretation  and  uses  were 
encountered  when  a  whole  program  of  evaluation  was  de- 
veloping. The  present  chapter  is  devoted  to  these  problems. 
Methods  of  interpretation  and  uses  of  evaluation  data  were 
determined  largely  by  two  factors.  One  was  the  conception 
of  the  functions  which  interpretation  was  to  serve;  the  other 
was  the  character  of  the  data  and  the  assumptions  on  which 
they  were  based. 

Functions  of  Interpretation 

Since  the  main  purpose  of  evaluation  was  to  help  teachers 
improve  their  curriculum  and  guidance,  the  first  function  of 
interpretation  was  to  translate  the  evidence  from  columns  of 
figures  into  descriptions  of.  behavior  which  were  intelligible 
and  useful  to  teachers  for  this  purpose.  Such  translation  oc- 
curred on  three  levels:  single  scores  or  bits  of  evidence, 
whole  instruments,  and  batteries  of  instruments. 

At  the  first  level,  even  a  single  score  on  a  test  usually  car- 
ried no  self-evident  meaning.  What,  for  example,  did  a  score 
of  11  per  cent  on  crude  errors  in  the  test  on  interpretation  of 
data  mean?  It  seemed  to  be  low  (desirable);  it  was  actually 
high  (undesirable)  as  such  scores  went;  but  in  a  group  which 
had  had  little  training  in  this  ability,  it  might  be  below 

403 


404        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

the  median,  and  better  than  was  to  be  expected  from  this  stu- 
dent. Thus  each  score  had  to  be  translated,  at  least  in  the 
mind  of  the  interpreter,  in  terms  of  the  behavior  which  it 
represented. 

Each  score,  however,  was  only  a  part  of  the  larger  pattern 
of  behavior  revealed  by  a  given  instrument.  At  the  second 
level  of  translation,  therefore,  each  score  had  to  be  inter- 
preted in  the  light  of  the  other  scores  on  the  same  instru- 
ment, in  order  to  see  the  larger  tendencies  in  behavior  in  this 
area  and  their  dependence  on  one  another. 

This  process  was  continued  with  scores  from  a  battery  of 
instruments  at  the  third  level  of  translation.  Thus,  scores  in- 
dicating inability  to  get  accurate  meaning  from  quantitative 
data,  combined  with  evidence  of  general  ability  in  logical 
discrimination  and  skill  in  quantitative  techniques,  might  in- 
dicate that  the  difficulty  lay  only  in  failure  to  devote  the 
necessary  attention  and  persistence. 

This  level  of  translation  made  possible  the  second  func- 
tion of  interpretation:  to  suggest  hypotheses  regarding  the 
possible  causes  of  the  strengths  or  weaknesses  of  individuals 
and  groups.  To  locate  such  causes,  it  was  necessary  to  con- 
sider not  only  all  available  evidence  of  present  status  but 
also  the  history  of  development  up  to  this  point,  and  the 
relevant  factors  in  experience  in  and  out  of  school.  This  was 
entirely  possible  when  the  data  accumulated  gradually,  and 
when  teachers  had  known  their  students  for  a  long  time. 

Finally,  it  was  the  function  of  interpretation  to  suggest 
hypotheses  regarding  constructive  measures  to  remedy  the 
situation.  This  was  a  step  requiring  thoughtful  judgment,  not 
a  decision  that  could  be  made  automatically.  Usually  it  was 
necessary  to  consider  the  objectives  of  the  school,  the  pattern 
of  goals  of  the  individual,  as  well  as  the  demands  made  on 
him  by  life  or  school  activities  in  order  to  decide  which  short- 
comings needed  to  be  remedied.  A  wise  judgment  regarding 
the  methods  of  remedy  required,  in  addition,  insight  into 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  405 

human  behavior  and  the  methods  by  which  that  behavior 
could  be  controlled  and  changed. 

The  Nature  of  the  Data  and  the  Assumptions  Underlying  Them 
The  process  of  evaluation  was  composed  of  two  elements 
which  on  the  surface  seemed  contradictory,  and  which  tradi- 
tionally had  been  held  to  be  contradictory.  In  the  first  place, 
any  form  of  appraisal  is  essentially  an  analytic  process.  To 
see  each  individual  clearly  and  accurately  and  to  observe  the 
differences  among  individuals  more  precisely,  it  was  neces- 
sary to  break  up  larger  complexes  of  behavior  into  their  com- 
ponent parts  and  to  get  as  accurate  measures  of  each  as 
possible. 

Thus,  in  the  course  of  the  Eight- Year  Study,  reference  was 
often  made  to  "breaking  up"  objectives.  Separate  instru- 
ments were  constructed  to  appraise  each  area  of  objectives, 
and  in  many  cases  each  aspect  of  specific  objectives.  This 
type  of  approach  could  easily  be  identified  with  "atomism," 
that  is,  with  an  assumption  that  human  behavior  is  composed 
of  isolated  reactions,  each  of  which  can  be  understood,  ex- 
plained and  appraised  as  a  separate  entity. 

However,  evaluation  in  the  Eight- Year  Study  has  also  ad- 
hered to  the  second,  synthesizing  function  of  appraisal.  One 
of  the  most  influential  psychological  principles  guiding  the 
work  has  been  the  assumption  that  the  essential  character- 
istic of  human  behavior  is  its  organic  unity,  and  that  various 
aspects  of  it  function  in  close  relationship  with  each  other. 
It  was  clear  that  no  single  aspect  of  human  behavior  would 
be  understood  without  reference  to  the  total  pattern  of  be- 
havior. Similarly,  it  was  clear  that  usually  no  single  type  of 
growth  could  be  fully  achieved  without  some  progress  in  all 
others.  While  an  uneven  development  was  expected  toward 
certain  objectives,  such  as  thinking,  attitudes,  interests,  social 
adjustment,  and  so  on,  no  one  aspect  should  be  developed  too 
far  without  some  growth  in  other  important  aspects  of  de- 


406        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

velopment  taking  place  at  the  same  time.  Thus,  if  logical 
thinking  were  cultivated  without  much  attention  to  emo- 
tional and  social  maturation,  not  only  would  the  development 
of  thinking  be  handicapped;  personality  maladjustments 
might  also  appear  as  a  result  of  too  uneven  a  rhythm  of 
growth.  Similarly,  the  possibility  of  rational  and  objective 
social  attitudes  was  greatly  limited  unless  a  certain  degree  of 
maturation  took  place  in  social  interests. 

This  basic  assumption  found  expression  at  several  points 
in  the  development  of  the  evaluation  program.  One  of  these 
was  the  conception  underlying  the  comprehensive  set  of  ob- 
jectives. The  areas  of  objectives  described  in  the  first  chapter 
were  not  chosen  arbitrarily  or  accidentally.  In  formulating 
objectives  and  in  classifying  them,  an  effort  was  made  to 
include  such  a  range  of  the  significant  aspects  of  human 
growth  that,  taken  together  as  goals  of  development,  the 
areas  of  objectives  would  represent  a  unified  and  related  de- 
velopment of  the  whole  person.  Thus  the  term  "comprehen- 
sive" used  in  conjunction  with  objectives  referred  primarily 
to  the  range  of  aspects  of  human  growth  viewed  as  an  or- 
ganic unit. 

The  idea  of  relatedness  of  behavior  was  also  expressed  in 
the  structure  of  the  instruments  developed  as  well  as  in  plan- 
ning the  series  of  instruments.  Thus,  each  instrument  at- 
tempted to  diagnose  a  pattern  of  closely  related  behavior 
aspects  rather  than  isolated  behaviors.  For  example,  in  de- 
veloping the  test  to  measure  the  ability  to  apply  social  values 
to  controversial  problems,  an  analysis  was  made  of  the  be- 
haviors involved  in  this  process.  The  ability  to  see  implica- 
tions of  social  values  broadly  or  comprehensively  was  con- 
sidered to  be  one  of  them.  At  the  same  time,  it  was  evident 
that  some  people,  while  seeing  issues  broadly,  also  indulged 
in  inconsistent  and  irrelevant  reasoning.  While  their  scores 
on  comprehensiveness  might  be  quantitatively  the  same,  the 
meanings  of  these  scores  differed  depending  on  what  logical 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  407 

qualities  were  shown  at  the  same  time.  Further,  the  question 
of  the  nature  of  their  values  entered.  A  broad  and  compre- 
hensive awareness  of  values  and  their  implications  might 
involve  a  consistent  or  inconsistent,  homogeneous  or 
ambivalent  pattern  of  those  values.  This  pattern  might  be 
what  is  commonly  called  "democratic,"  or  "undemocratic/' 
Recognizing  the  relationship  of  these  three  types  of  reactions, 
namely  comprehensiveness,  logic,  and  values,  it  was  neces- 
sary to  construct  a  test  permitting  the  diagnosis  of  each  of 
these  behaviors  in  a  context  involving  the  others.  The  test 
provided  for  each  type  of  reaction  and  permitted  a  descrip- 
tion of  them  in  their  relationship  to  each  other. 

While  each  instrument  was  constructed  to  appraise  specific 
behavior  related  to  specific  objectives,  the  relationship  of 
these  behaviors  to  the  total  behavior  pattern  of  an  individual 
was  not  forgotten.  In  many  cases  instruments  were  frankly 
devised  as  "mates"  to  each  other,  because  it  was  clear  that 
the  behaviors  measured  by  them  were  strongly  influenced  by 
each  other,  or  because  it  was  recognized  that  certain  kinds 
of  behavior  needed  to  be  checked  in  different  content.  Thus 
the  instruments  measuring  general  social  beliefs  were  supple- 
mented with  others  appraising  the  application  of  these  be- 
liefs in  concrete  situations  and  the  logical  thinking  involved 
in  such  a  process.  The  evaluation  of  free  reading  was  con- 
ducted hand  in  hand  with  the  evaluation  of  responses  made 
to  that  reading.  Information  and  application  of  information 
were  found  to  be  importantly  related  and  some  instruments 
appraised  both  with  reference  to  the  same  content.  Similarly, 
recognition  of  the  strong  relationship  between  interests  and 
thinking  made  it  necessary  to  secure  evidence  on  interests  in 
all  areas  in  which  logical  thinking  was  appraised,  so  as  to  be 
able  to  diagnose  weaknesses  in  thinking  in  relation  to  in- 
terests in  the  same  areas. 

Often  an  effort  was  made  to  secure  supplementary  evi- 
dence from  a  series  of  instruments  on  certain  characteristics 


408         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

appraised  directly  in  one  instrument.  Thus  the  tendency  to 
go  beyond  data  or  to  be  overcautious  was  directly  measured 
in  the  test  on  interpretation  of  data.  Supplementary  evidence 
on  the  same  tendencies  could  be  gained  from  other  tests  also. 
For  this  reason  some  scores  were  retained  even  though  their 
statistical  reliability  as  separate  scores  was  low,  for  the  reli- 
ability of  the  conclusions  increased  as  the  same  tendency 
was  shown  in  many  different  instruments. 

Thus,  in  a  sense,  the  series  of  major  instruments  composed 
a  related  hattenj.  Each  instrument  was  a  part  of  a  compre- 
hensive plan  for  evaluation,  designed  to  correspond  to  re- 
lated behaviors  within  a  unified  pattern  of  development. 
Thus  the  synthesizing  function  of  evaluation  was  expressed 
in  the  structure  of  the  instruments  as  well  as  in  the  relation- 
ship of  the  instruments  to  one  another. 

As  a  result,  what  the  interpreter  found  was  not  a  series  of 
isolated  data,  but  a  series  of  data  which  fitted  into  a  pattern 
of  behavior  relationship.  His  job  was  facilitated  because  the 
required  synthesis  was  not  to  be  brought  about  from  a  plan- 
less series  of  isolated  bits  of  evidence.  Certain  generalized 
relationships  were  inherent  in  the  very  nature  of  the  data. 
His  task  was  to  detect  the  variations  of  individual  and  group 
patterns  within  this  general  framework. 

Illustrative  Case  Study 

To  illustrate  the  problems  encountered  and  methods  of 
reasoning  and  inference  fruitful  in  synthesizing  a  range  of 
data,  a  case  study  is  presented  on  p.  409.  An  effort  was  made 
to  use  the  types  of  data  actually  securable  in  a  public  school 
and  to  analyze  them  as  they  were  analyzed  by  the  school 
staff.  A  deviation  from  the  school's  procedure  was  necessary, 
however,  in  the  order  of  presentation. 

Usually  a  case  study  of  test  data  is  made  when  a  decision 
is  necessary  regarding  some  problem  of  an  individual  or 
group.  The  occasion  may  be  that  of  choosing  a  program  of 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  409 

studies,  a  difficulty  observed  by  some  teacher,  a  behavior 
problem  requiring  explanation,  or  some  inconsistency  ob- 
served in  the  data  themselves.  The  nature  of  the  problem 
usually  determines  at  which  point  the  analysis  of  informa- 
tion begins  and  what  sequence  the  consideration  assumes. 
The  case  of  Jane  came  to  the  attention  of  counselors  and 
teachers  when  they  surveyed  the  data  from  a  battery  of  in- 
struments prepared  by  the  Evaluation  Staff  and  found  that 
the  impressions  of  Jane  secured  from  these  data  differed 
from  the  ones  prevailing  among  the  school  staff.  For  this 
reason  the  investigation  proceeded  first  to  locate  some  of 
the  outstanding  conflicting  impressions  and  then  to  examine 
data  relevant  to  explaining  them.  However,  the  data  are 
here  presented  not  in  the  order  in  which  they  were  secured 
or  analyzed  in  the  school,  but  in  the  order  of  their  explana- 
tory value  for  the  subsequent  data. 

Background  Data 

Jane  is  a  senior  in  a  large  public  high  school  and  has  come 
to  it  through  a  junior  high  school  on  the  same  campus.  Sev- 
eral teachers  have  thus  known  her  for  some  time.  She  is 
considered  an  average,  normal  child,  so  much  so  that,  ac- 
cording to  the  counselor,  she  has  scarcely  been  noticed.  She 
has  never  created  any  trouble,  has  done  her  work  fairly  well 
and,  except  for  occasional  difficulty  with  her  Latin  teacher, 
has  behaved  as  a  "good"  student.  Her  I.Q.  is  120  (Terman 
group)  which  is  in  the  middle  of  the  range  of  her  group. 

Standardized  Achievement  Test  Scores 

Her  percentile  scores  on  standardized  achievement  tests 
taken  over  the  preceding  two  years  were  as  follows: 

Year  I  Year  II 

Algebra 55         English  Usage 87 

English 84         Spelling 64 

French 85         Vocabulary 98 

Latin 100         Literary  Comprehension ....  92 

Medieval  History. 99         Reading  Rate 85 

Literary  Acquaintance 98 


4io        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Apparently  Jane  has  a  high  level  o£  achievement  in  the 
usual  subject  matter  skills  and  information.  With  the  excep- 
tion of  algebra  and  spelling,  her  scores  are  at  or  above  the 
84th  percentiles. 

Two  questions  suggest  themselves  at  this  point.  First,  one 
notices  that  her  scores  on  mathematics  and  spelling  are  con- 
spicuously lower  than  the  others  and  one  wonders  what  may 
be  the  cause  for  that.  Secondly,  one  is  curious  about  how 
Jane's  standing  in  the  class  on  achievement  scores  compares 
with  her  abilities  as  measured  by  intelligence  test  scores. 

Examination  of  the  range  of  scores  for  the  group  revealed 
that  Jane  tends  to  stand  higher  on  achievement  tests  than 
on  the  intelligence  test  scores.  One  notices  also  that  the  areas 
of  her  high  achievement  are  areas  of  high  verbal  content 
which  suggests  a  special  proficiency  with  words  and  possibly 
difficulties  with  areas  and  processes  requiring  the  use  of 
other  techniques  and  symbols. 

Teacher  Reports 

A  look  at  the  teachers'  reports  to  her  parents  reveals  the 
following: 

Algebra — Teacher  has  little  to  say,  except  that  Jane  has  diffi- 
culty with  learning  mathematics,  especially  when  it  comes 
to  application  of  quantitative  concepts  to  practical  problems. 

English — In  general,  Jane  understands  what  she  reads.  Some 
of  the  modern  poetry  presents  difficulty.  She  needs  to  in- 
crease her  speed  of  reading.  As  far  as  free  reading  is  con- 
cerned, she  shows  "appreciation,  acquaintance,  and  scope  in 
her  reading/*  Her  literary  background  is  satisfactory,  espe- 
cially with  reference  to  literary  criticism.  When  in  a  hurry, 
Jane  makes  unreasonable  mistakes  in  spelling.  "Jan^  knows 
better."  Organization  of  materials  is  excellent  and  presenta- 
tion acceptable.  Excellent  work  habits. 

French — Reads  with  comprehension,  speed,  and  accuracy. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  411 

Has  good  memory  for  words.  Understands  and  remembers 
grammatical  principles.  Reads  smoothly  and  knows  rules  of 
pronunciation.  Responds  orally  in  fluent  speech.  Written 
work  could  show  improvement  in  application.  Is  much  inter- 
ested in  foreign  people  and  their  contribution  to  civilization. 
Does  individual  research  work  in  music  for  her  own  pleas- 
ure. Work  habits  excellent,  though  lack  of  preparation  was 
evident  in  the  last  two  tests  during  the  two  weeks  preceding 
the  report.  Has  intellectual  interests  in  Romance  languages 
and  their  development.  Is  studying  Spanish  in  her  leisure 
time  and  corresponds  with  a  foreigner  in  that  language. 

Latin — Has  keen  power  to  get  thought  from  foreign  lan- 
guage without  translation.  Vocabulary  is  very  good;  gram- 
mar and  pronunciation  good.  In  applying  fundamentals, 
written  work  is  better  than  oral  work.  For  the  past  six  weeks 
has  made  no  effort  to  do  more  in  silent  reading  than  the 
minimum  requirement.  Is  unique  on  occasion  in  applying 
historic-cultural  materials,  but  frequently  fails  to  come 
through.  Work  habits  are  bad.  Does  not  pretend  to  do  things 
on  time.  Intellectual  interests  sometimes  very  high,  some- 
times very  low. 

Social  Studies — Good  mastery  of  such  skills  as  reading,  map 
work,  use  of  graphs  and  charts,  library  books.  Knows  a  satis- 
factory number  of  historical  facts.  Reads  more  than  average, 
though  mostly  nature  books.  Work  habits  are  steady  and 
persistent.  Has  intellectual  interests  in  cultures  different  from 
her  own. 

A  few  things  stand  out  in  these  reports.  First,  with  the  ex- 
ception of  reports  from  the  teacher  of  Latin,  teachers'  re- 
ports are  consistent  with  the  results  of  the  standardized  tests. 
The  mathematics  teacher  reports  difficulty  with  algebra,  and 
the  English  teacher  comments  on  Jane's  "unreasonable" 
spelling.  One  wonders  whether  the  teachers'  reports  were 


ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

based  on  or  influenced  by  the  achievement  tests,  but  the  re- 
ports were  written  before  the  tests  were  given.  The  fact 
that  the  Latin  teacher  reports  difficulty  with  Jane's  work 
habits,  while  her  achievement  score  in  Latin  is  very  high, 
suggests  several  possibilities.  First,  the  Latin  classes  may 
emphasize  objectives  not  measured  by  the  achievement  test. 
The  Latin  teachej  may  have  been  unduly  influenced  by 
Jane's  slump  during  the  last  six  weeks,  and  may  be  apply- 
ing pressure  to  get  her  out  of  it.  Jane  may  also  have  had 
some  special  difficulty  with  the  teacher  which  may  have  in- 
fluenced the  teacher's  observations.  Finally,  Jane's  profi- 
ciency with  words  may  have  caused  her  to  be  bored  by  the 
class  work,  which  she  mastered  all  too  easily.  Each  one  of 
these  points  can  be  checked  easily  enough  in  the  school 
situation.  According  to  the  counselor,  the  Latin  teacher  was 
the  only  one  who  insisted  that  Jane  develop  a  modicum  of 
precision  and  care  with  details.  Others  seem  to  have  been 
satisfied  with  more  general  accomplishments. 

Behavior  Descriptions  by  Teachers1 

The  descriptions  by  teachers  of  several  of  Jane's  behavior 
traits  are  rather  diverse  and  on  the  whole  non-conclusive. 
On  the  15  traits  there  described,  usually  the  teachers  of 
French,  social  studies,  and  occasionally  English,  place  her 
higher  on  any  given  trait  than  do  the  teachers  of  mathe- 
matics and  Latin,  particularly  the  latter.  Thus,  in  assessing 
her  imagination,  the  French  teacher  describes  her  as  "gen- 
erally imaginative,"  the  social  studies  teacher  as  "specifically 
imaginative/'  mathematics  teacher  as  "imitative"  and  Latin 
teacher  as  "unimaginative."  Similarly,  according  to  the 
French  teacher,  she  is  highly  analytical,  but  according  to 
the  mathematics  and  Latin  teachers,  limited  in  her  power 
of  analysis.  In  most  of  the  15  characteristics,  she  gets  the 

1The  forms   developed  by  the   committee  headed  by   Mr.   Eugene   R. 
Smith  were  used.  These  forms  are  described  in  Part  II  of  this  volume. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  413 

highest  as  well  as  the  lowest  ratings.  This  suggests  several 
possibilities.  First,  the  teachers  may  have  had  insufficient 
opportunity  to  observe  Jane  on  all  characteristics,  and  there- 
fore may  have  given  somewhat  invalid  reports.  The  teachers 
may  also  have  rated  Jane  according  to  her  achievement  in 
the  class,  thus  being  influenced  by  what  is  called  a  "halo 
effect."  It  may  also  be  that  Jane's  difficulties  in  academic 
achievement  influenced  her -personal  relations  with  each  of 
the  teachers  concerned  and  hence  affected  her  actual  be- 
havior in  class. 

Summary  of  Counselors'  Interviews  over  Two  Years 

Due  to  the  loss  of  her  parents,  Jane  lives  some  distance  in 
the  country  with  her  grandmother  and  aunt.  She  has  con- 
sequently had  little  companionship  with  other  children  and 
is  thrown  a  great  deal  with  older  people.  Moreover,  the 
grandmother  and  the  aunt  do  not  get  along  well,  and  Jane 
feels  that  she  often  has  to  take  the  brunt  of  their  differences 
with  each  other.  Jane  feels  that  her  ideas  are  "foreign"  to 
those  of  her  grandmother  and  aunt,  and  she  suppresses  them 
at  home,  "for  the  sake  of  peace."  When  the  difficulty  with 
her  work  habits  in  Latin  was  pointed  out  to  her,  Jane  said 
she  was  in  the  habit  of  leaving  work  to  the  last  minute  and 
rushing  through  with  it,  a  habit  indulged  in  by  many  "bright 
students."  Since  she  got  good  grades,  "why  bother?"  As  to 
her  difficulty  with  Latin,  she  felt  that  she  could  get  more 
out  of  the  language  by  herself. 

Concerning  her  personal  life,  Jane  confesses  that  she  can- 
not work  with  other  people,  because  of  her  unwillingness  to 
accept  suggestions.  She  also  talked  about  having  temper  tan- 
trums and  throwing  things  around  in  her  room.  These  tan- 
trums were  referred  to  in  both  interviews,  a  year  apart.  She 
has  only  a  few  friends.  One  of  them,  a  Jewish  girl,  whom 
she  admired  very  much,  she  was  forced  to  desert  on  the  in- 
sistence of  her  other  friends. 


414        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Her  vocational  plans  are  undecided.  In  the  tenth  grade 
she  expressed  interest  in  history  and  archeology,  and  the 
next  year  in  languages.  She  wants  to  go  to  Stanford  Univer- 
sity, however,  because  "the  climate  suits  her  health  and  the 
architecture  her  temperament."  This  is  contrary  to  the  wishes 
of  her  family,  who  want  her  to  enter  Bryn  Mawr.  She  has 
had  no  vocational  experiences. 

Summer  vacation  activities  include  a  trip  to  Mexico  ( sub- 
sequent interest  in  Spanish),  summer  high  school  work  in 
Spanish,  and  the  study  of  Italian  by  herself. 

Recreational  and  club  activities  are  limited  in  number  and 
are  mostly  solitary  in  nature.  Orchestra  is  the  only  club  ac- 
tivity in  school,  which  is  less  than  average  for  high  school 
students.  Athletic  experiences  include  riding,  swimming, 
cycling,  and  walking.  She  hates  and  fears  "gym."  She  listens 
to  the  radio,  reads,  and  attends  a  few  movies,  and  confesses 
she  does  not  know  how  to  play.  She  reports  that  her  health 
is  good. 

This  record  reveals  several  adjustment  problems  and  their 
probable  sources.  There  is  a  tendency  to  withdrawal  and  a 
certain  degree  of  difficulty  in  adjusting  to  other  people,  both 
adults  and  those  of  her  own  age.  These  difficulties  appar- 
ently have  not  been  noticed  by  the  classroom  teachers.  Her 
choices  of  free  activities,  which  do  not  include  many  usually 
chosen  by  girls  of  her  age,  concentrate  exclusively  on  soli- 
tary activities.  She  has  few  friends,  and  her  relations  with 
them  are  somewhat  complicated.  Immaturity  is  shown  in  her 
vocational  plans  and  experiences.  Her  reasons  for  choosing 
a  college  seem  far-fetched  and  affected.  Part  of  the  sources 
of  her  difficulties  lie  in  her  home  life.  At  least  the  fact  that 
she  lives  out  of  town,  in  a  household  composed  of  elderly 
adults,  may  be  sufficient  cause  for  her  lack  of  contact  with 
people  her  own  age,  and  hence  a  cause  for  her  apparent  ad- 
justment difficulties. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS 
Summary  of  the  P.E.A.  Test  Data 

INTEREST   INDEX,    TEST    8.2a2 


4*5 


Category 

Jar 

ie's 

Class  I 

Median 

Likes 

Dislikes 

Likes 

Dislikes 

Social  Studies  

38 

0 

51 

13 

Biolosrv   

19 

0 

56 

13 

Physical  Science 

25 

0 

56 

13 

English. 

75 

0 

63 

13 

Foreign  Languages  

100 

0 

63 

6 

Mathematics  

0 

25 

43 

25 

Business  

0 

0 

56 

6 

Home  Economics 

13 

6 

44 

19 

Industrial  Arts          .           .    . 

31 

0 

44 

18 

Fine  Arts 

88 

0 

38 

12 

Mhisic  

76 

6 

56 

12 

Sports  

12 

38 

56 

18 

^Manipulative 

37 

3 

44 

21 

Reading  

54 

0 

58 

14 

Total  ,  

39 

6 

52 

21 

In  the  twelfth  grade  as  well  as  during  two  previous  years, 
Jane's  interest  pattern  Is  highly  selective.  Strong  preferences 
are  shown  in  four  areas:  English,  fine  arts,  foreign  languages, 
and  music — foreign  languages  being  the  highest.  These 
choices  reveal  two  types  of  basic  preferences:  verbal  activ- 
ities and  creative  activities.  The  areas  having  to  do  with 
life  realities,  practical  activities,  and  precise  thinking  are 
conspicuously  lacking  in  her  pattern  of  likes.  The  general 
tone  of  her  interests  in  areas  other  than  the  ones  mentioned 
above  is  that  of  indifference.  Thus,  the  activities  classified 
as  biology,  physical  sciences,  home  arts,  business  and  sports 
are  a  matter  of  indifference  to  her.  Her  total  "dislikes"  com- 
prise only  6  per  cent  of  all  of  the  items. 

2  For  a  detailed  description  of  this  test  and  of  the  meaning  of  the  summary 
categories  see  p.  338,  Chap.  V. 


416        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

In  the  area  of  sports,  however,  she  shows  marked  negative 
responses.  Her  dislikes  here  are  in  the  highest  quarter  in  the 
class.  This  is  significant,  because  Jane  has  few  dislike  re- 
sponses. Her  remark  to  the  counselor  about  her  fear  of  gym 
corroborates  this  evidence  but  offers  no  explanation.  Con- 
sidering the  fact  that  her  choice  of  free  recreational  sports 
activities  is  limited  to  solitary  activities,  and  also  the  fact 
that  there  is  no  evidence  of  a  physical  handicap  or  lack  of 
physical  skill,  one  is  inclined  to  suggest  that  her  negative 
reaction  to  sports  occurs  at  the  points  of  group  or  team  ac- 
tivities. There  is  also  other  evidence  suggesting  that  she  dis- 
likes and  avoids  activities  involving  social  or  competitive 
contact.  Thus,  on  a  previous  questionnaire  she  showed  very- 
high  dislikes  on  items  concerning  leadership  and  sociable  ac- 
tivities. One  is  also  reminded  of  her  remarks  to  the  counselor 
to  the  effect  that  she  could  not  work  or  play  with  other 
people. 

From  these  facts  one  develops  an  hypothesis  of  a  solitary 
girl  with  a  rather  concentrated  and  somewhat  narrow  range 
of  interests,  which  deviate  in  many  aspects  from  the  average 
pattern  for  girls  of  her  age.  An  interesting  inconsistency  is 
apparent  in  one  spot.  Her  score  on  interest  in  art  is  high. 
Yet  her  activity  record  shows  no  participation  in  art  activ- 
ities. Her  lack  of  participation  in  art  activities  in  the  school 
might  be  due  to  the  fact  that  her  school  schedule  did  not 
permit  it,  but  she  chose  a  second  foreign  language  rather 
than  art  as  an  elective,  and  a  study  period  rather  than  an 
art  club.  Neither  is  there  any  hint  of  artistic  expression 
among  her  summer  activities.  On  another  questionnaire  she 
shows  no  special  preferences  except  in  instrumental  music. 
As  will  be  seen  later,  her  responses  to  free  reading  do  not 
include  a  tendency  to  translate  impressions  gained  from 
reading  into  art  expression.  It  may  be  that  her  "art"  interest 
is  entirely  passive,  or  that  this  interest  is  "spurious"  in  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  417 

sense  of  being  a  symbolic  expression  of  some  other  difficulty 
or  problem. 

Free  Reading  and  Cultural  Activities 

Another  series  of  data  on  her  interests  and  preferences 
comes  from  her  free  choice  activities  and  reading  record. 
She  reads  the  local  daily  paper  and  occasionally  the  New 
Jork  Times.  This  latter,  she  says,  is  her  favorite  paper  be- 
cause of  the  book,  art,  and  music  notes.  She  is,  however, 
unaware  of  the  political  theory  favored  by  the  papers  she 
reads.  (This  is  rather  common,  though,  among  high  school 
students.)  She  spends  an  average  amount  of  time  (four 
hours  per  week)  reading  newspapers.  Interesting,  however, 
are  the  items  she  remembers  from  her  reading  during  one 
month.  These  deal  mostly  with  music  (death  of  Chaliapin, 
opening  of  Robin  Hood  Dell)  and  international  news  (quake 
in  Mexico,  Hungarian  countess  married,  taking  over  of  Amer- 
ican oil  interests  by  Mexican  government,  Seiiora  Cardenas 
and  her  friends  giving  their  jewels  to  help  United  States  oil 
interests,  former  Ethiopian  ruler  paying  back  dues  to  League 
of  Nations).  There  are  no  items  of  national  importance 
among  the  list  of  items  she  remembers,  nor  does  she  pay 
any  attention  to  the  editorials. 

Her  free  reading  during  one  sample  period  of  a  month 
(May  6  to  June  6)  included  the  following  books:  Wilder, 
Bridge  of  San  Luis  Rey;  Wallace,  Fair  God;  Lewis,  Charles 
"of  Europe;  Sabbatini,  Stalking  Horse;  Ellis,  The  Soul  of 
Spain.  These  are  books  about  countries  other  than  the  United 
States,  or  by  foreign  authors.  Her  reading  over  a  period  of 
a  year  is  twice  as  voluminous  as  the  average  for  the  class. 
Her  magazine  reading  is  rather  average  in  quantity  and 
character.  Thus  the  Ladies''  Home  Journal^  Saturday  Eve- 
ning  Post,  Time,  and  Woman  s  Home  Companion  are  read 
regularly,  mainly  because  they  are  received  at  home.  The 
only  deviation  from  the  usual  pattern  is  the  reading  of  the 


418        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

National  Geographic  regularly  and  in  full,  and  the  omission 
of  the  Readers9  Digest.  National  Geographic  was  subscribed 
for  at  her  request.  At  no  time  has  she  made  use  of  the  period- 
icals in  the  school  library. 

She  attends  no  concerts,  which  is  surprising  in  view  of  her 
apparent  interest  in  music,  of  her  proximity  to  a  major  or- 
chestra, and  the  tradition  in  the  region  of  attending  concerts. 
She  has  attended  no  plays.  She  spends  a  lot  of  time,  though, 
listening  to  music  over  the  radio,  her  favorite  programs  being 
Charlie  McCarthy,  Ford  Sunday  Evening  Hour,  RCA  Magic 
Key,  Radio  City  Music  Hall,  and  La  Rosa.  Archery  is  her 
only  other  recreational  activity. 

All  of  this  is  rather  consistent  with  what  was  suggested 
by  previously  given  facts  about  her  interests  and  personality 
pattern.  The  impression  of  her  preoccupation  with  the  far- 
away and  the  esoteric  is  reinforced  by  her  reading  selec- 
tions. Her  failure  to  face  the  "here  and  now"  is  again  em- 
phasized. 

APPRECIATION   OF   LITERATURE,   TEST   3.33 


Category 

Jane's  Scores 

Class  Median 

Likes  Reading       .          

100 

62 

Wants  More  

60 

75 

Curious  

100 

55 

Expresses  Other  Media        .......... 

35 

25 

Identifies  Self  

50 

60 

Relates  To  Life  

100 

70 

Evaluates  Reading  

100 

70 

Totals 
Appreciation     

84 

65 

Non-appreciation  

15 

40 

Undecided  

1 

1 

With  the  information  about  her  reading  interests  at  hand, 

3  For  a  detailed  description  of  this  test  and  the  meaning  of  the  sum- 
mary categories,  see  p.  253,  Chap.  IV. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS 


419 


it  Is  interesting  to  look  into  her  responses  to  free  reading. 
The  results  from  this  test  conflict  at  several  points  with  the 
impressions  from  data  up  to  this  point.  She  apparently  likes 
reading  very  much  and  is  also  curious  about  the  background 
of  authors  and  of  the  settings  of  literary  works.  This  is  con- 
sistent enough  with  her  voluminous  reading.  She  shows  a 
much  higher  than  usual  tendency  to  relate  what  she  reads 
to  life  and  to  evaluate  reading,  which  is  surprising  in  view 
of  her  apparent  lack  of  interest  in  matters  concerning  life 
realities.  As  will  be  seen  later,  however,  she  shows  little 
ability  to  discriminate  between  what  is  true  to  life  and  what 
is  not.  It  has  already  been  noted  that  while  she  has  indicated 
high  interest  in  the  arts,  she  does  not  show  any  strong  inclina- 
tion to  translate  her  impressions  from  reading  into  other  art 
forms.  People  who  are  withdrawn  and  rely  much  on  read- 
ing to  secure  experience  with  life  are  usually  inclined  to 
respond  to  reading  with  a  high  degree  of  self -identification. 
This  is  not  the  case  with  Jane.  Her  score  on  identifying  her- 
self with  what  she  reads  is  below  the  median  of  the  group 
and  also  below  the  usual  scores  in  the  same  grade.  This  may, 
however,  be  a  mark  of  sophistication  in  reading. 

CRITIC AL-MINDEDNESS   IN   THE    READING    OF  FICTION,   TEST   3.7* 


Judicious 

Hypercritical 

Uncritical 

Uncertain 

Jane's  Score     

40 

36 

33 

25 

Class  Median  

70 

18 

22 

5 

According  to  the  results  from  this  test,  Jane  is  not  very 
successful  in  distinguishing  realistic  life  situations  from  the 
dramatic  or  melodramatic  ones.  Her  recognition  of  lifelike 
situations  (judicious  decisions)  is  the  lowest  in  her  group. 

4  For  the  description  of  this  test,  see  p.  266,  Chap.  IV. 


420         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

She  also  has  a  strong  tendency  to  be  hypercritical:  to  judge 
situations  and  behaviors  which  are  usually  considered  true- 
to-life  as  the  opposite.  These  data  support  the  impression  of 
her  lack  of  experience  with  life  realities,  and  her  immaturity 
in  dealing  with  them.  At  many  points  she  finds  it  impossible 
to  make  up  her  mind.  This  test  is  not  good  enough  to  be 
conclusive  on  this  point,  but  it  gives  rise  to  some  doubt  about 
her  literary  judgment,  in  spite  of  her  voluminous  reading  and 
her  high  score  on  disposition  to  evaluate  reading. 

INTERPRETATION    OF   DATA,    TEST    2.515 


Category 

Jane's  Scores 

Class  Median 

General  Accuracy       

54 

57 

Accuracy  with  Probably  True  and 
Probably  False                    .      ... 

35 

38 

Accuracy  with  Insufficient  Data  .... 
Accuracy  with  True  and  False 

51 
76 

58 
73 

Overcaution 

48 

21 

Going  Beyond  Data   

43 

36 

Crude  Errors  

11 

8 

In  techniques  of  getting  meaning  from  quantitative  data 
requiring  precise  thinking,  Jane  is  near  the  average  for  her 
class.  Her  scores  on  accuracy  are  slightly  below  the  median. 
This  indicates  inability  to  recognize  the  limitations  of  data. 
An  examination  of  types  of  errors  shows  a  greater  than  aver- 
age tendency  to  go  beyond  the  data,  or  accept  generalities 
ignoring  the  limitations  of  the  data.  Not  only  is  this  score 
among  the  highest  in  the  class  (significant,  since  most  of 
her  scores  are  close  to  the  median),  but  the  proportion  of 
errors  in  this  direction  in  comparison  to  those  in  the  direc- 
tion of  overcaution  is  also  larger  than  that  of  the  class  (Be- 
yond Data:  Overcaution  =  43:18,  Class  =  36:21).  Her 
score  on  crude  errors  is  one  of  the  highest  in  the  class. 

5  For  a  detailed  description  of  these  summary  categories,  see  p.  51, 
Chap.  II. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS 


421 


In  view  of  her  fairly  high  accuracy  in  determining  the 
absolute  truth  or  falseness  of  inferences,  her  inaccuracies  in 
judging  trends  and  probabilities  may  have  been  the  result 
of  somewhat  careless  reading,  particularly  in  view  of  the  pre- 
vious hints  of  difficulty  with  details  requiring  precise  work 
and  application,  such  as  low  scores  on  mathematics  and  dif- 
ficulties in  areas  where  detailed  application  and  precision 
was  demanded.  However,  there  is  strong  enough  evidence 
that  Jane  does  not  have  the  techniques  necessary  for  precise 
manipulation  and  judgment  of  trends.  There  is  also  sufficient 
evidence  that  in  instances  where  she  does  not  get  accurate 
meaning  from  the  data,  her  tendency  is  to  overgeneralize 
rather  than  to  undergeneralize.  The  possibility  of  lack  of 

ABILITY  TO   APPLY  PRINCIPLES   OF  LOGIC,   TEST    5. 16 


Jane's  Scores 

Class  Median 

Definitions 
Right  Conclusions 

6 

4 

Right  Reasons  

2 

2 

Total  

8 

7 

Indirect  Argument 
Right  Conclusions  

0 

4 

Right  Reasons 

0 

0 

Total         

0 

4 

Ridicule 
Right  Conclusions 

6 

6 

Right  Reasons         

5 

3 

Total  

11 

9 

If-Then 
Right  Conclusions       .              .... 

4 

2 

Right  Reasons  

0 

0 

Total 

4 

2 

Total 
Right  Conclusions  

16 

18 

Right  Reasons  

7 

5 

Total 

23 

22 

6  For  the  description  of  this  test,  see  p.  115,  Chap.  II. 


42,2,         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

experience  is  ruled  out  on  the  grounds  that  while  the  class 
improved  over  the  period  of  one  year,  Jane's  pattern  of 
scores  showed  practically  no  change.  Apparently  the  experi- 
ences provided  for  the  class  did  not  "take"  with  Jane. 

Apparently  Jane's  ability  to  apply  principles  of  logic,  such 
as  the  importance  of  definitions  in  arriving  at  conclusions, 
the  recognition  of  the  limitations  of  indirect  argument,  the 
fallacy  of  trying  to  disprove  by  attacking  the  opponent,  and 
the  logical  necessity  of  accepting  conclusions  flowing  from 
the  assumptions  one  has  accepted,  is  approximately  at  the 
average  for  the  class.  Her  highest  score  is  on  recognizing  the 
futility  of  ridiculing  the  opponent  as  a  method  of  argument. 
Her  lowest  score  is  in  recognizing  the  limitations  of  indirect 
argument  in  proof.  She  seems  to  use  "common  sense"  logic 
but  is  not  particularly  conscious  of  the  principles  she  applies 
and  has  not  developed  finer  techniques  of  reasoning.  Since 
the  class  had  devoted  a  good  deal  of  attention  to  applying 
principles  of  logic  of  this  sort,  the  cause  must  be  sought  not 
in  lack  of  experience  but  in  lack  of  interest  or  ability.  Appar- 
ently the  ability  to  abstract  from  the  concrete  situation  which 
is  required  in  this  test  and  to  draw  refined  logical  distinc- 
tions is  not  the  strong  point  in  Jane's  intellectual  make-up. 

Jane's  ability  to  recognize  the  logical  relationships  in  argu- 
ments and  to  discriminate  between  relevant  facts  and  as- 
sumptions and  irrelevant  ones  is  at  the  average  for  her  group. 
However,  since  in  each  of  the  categories — relevance,  sup- 
port, criticalness — the  number  of  reasons  she  attempts  is 
considerably  higher  than  the  number  of  reasons  she  gets 
right,  a  tendency  to  a  broad  and  somewhat  indiscriminate 
reasoning  is  suggested.  ( The  same  tendency  was  manifested 
in  her  methods  of  interpreting  data. )  Thus  while  the  actual 
score  of  "rights"  in  each  case  is  at  the  median,  she  uses  a 
large  number  of  irrelevant  considerations,  avoiding  the  out- 
right inconsistent  and  non-critical  considerations.  Thus,  gen- 
eral common  sense  combined  with  the  lack  of  precise  tech- 
niques and  cautiousness  is  again  indicated. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS 


423 


NATURE    OF   PROOF,    TEST    5.2 17 


General  Accuracy 129 

Relevancy 

No.  Marked 96 

Relevant 70 

Irrelevant 16 

Support 

No.  Marked 66 

Support 42 

Contradict 8 

Irrelevant 16 

Criticalness 

No.  Marked 30 

Critical 20 

Non-Critical 3 

Irrelevant 7 

Conclusions 

Accepts 5 

Uncertain 3 

Rejects 1 

Qualifications 

No.  Marked 16 

Accurate 10 


Jane's  Scores 


Class  Median 


128 

76 

69 

6 

48 

42 

2 

3 

22 

19 

3 

2 

6 
2 
1 

17 
11 


Apparently  Jane's  logical  abilities  are  not  very  high.  She 
seems  to  fall  short  on  precise  techniques  in  both  inductive 
and  deductive  thinking.  Her  confession  of  depending  on  her 
quick  grasp  and  on  a  last  minute  rush  to  complete  her  assign- 
ments suggests  that  throughout  her  career  in  school  Jane 
may  not  have  taken  the  opportunity  to  cultivate  precise 
methods  of  thinking  and  handling  facts.  The  concentration 
of  her  interests  in  the  direction  of  die  arts,  requiring  imagina- 
tion, and  languages,  requiring  memory,  may  have  in  addition 
militated  against  cultivating  these  processes. 


7  For  the  description  of  this  test,  see  p.  131,  Chap.  II. 


424        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

APPLICATION    OF   PRINCIPLES   IN   SCIENCE,    TEST    1.38 


Jane's  Scores 

Class  Median 

General  Accuracy  

_9 

18 

Conclusions 
Attempted           .        

12 

13 

Right  

2 

7 

Reasons 
Attempted        

12 

18 

Right  

3 

10 

Unacceptable  Reasons 
Technically  False        

7 

1 

Irrelevant  

0 

2 

False  Analogy                 .        

2 

1 

Common  Misconception  

2 

2 

Assuming  Conclusion   

1 

1 

False  Authority 

0 

0 

Ridicule  .  .        

0 

0 

Jane  is  extremely  weak  in  the  knowledge  and  use  of 
science  principles.  On  this  test  requiring  application  of  scien- 
tific principles  to  everyday  problems,  Jane's  general  accuracy 
is  the  lowest  in  the  group.  Although  she  attempted  a  total 
of  12  conclusions,  only  two  were  right  while  ten  were  wrong. 
Both  of  these  scores  are  the  poorest  in  the  group.  Similar 
behavior  is  shown  in  her  use  of  reasons.  Since  the  score  on 
false  principles  is  the  highest  among  her  unacceptable  rea- 
sons, her  chief  weakness  is  ignorance  of  these  principles,  but 
this  does  not  explain  her  failure  to  recognize  her  own  limita- 
tions, and  to  avoid  marking  reasons  which  she  did  not  under- 
stand. Lack  of  experience  in  science  would  ordinarily  ex- 
plain part  of  the  difficulty,  but  the  school  record  shows  that 
Jane  took  general  science  in  the  tenth  grade,  which  is  usu- 
ally sufficient  to  permit  a  better  record  on  this  test.  One 
could  therefore  conclude  that  it  is  Jane's  own  aversion  to 
or  inability  in  this  area  of  thinking  that  is  at  the  bottom  of 

8  For  the  description  of  this  test,  see  p.  84  ff.,  Chap.  II. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS 


425 


her  weakness.  The  school  record  shows  that  Jane  was  sched- 
uled for  a  special  course  in  general  science  in  her  senior  year 
to  give  her  more  experience  in  techniques  of  precise  think- 


SCALE    OF    BELIEFS,    TEST   4.2I9 


Jane's  Scores 

Class  Median 

%  Liberalism 
D  emocracy 

73 

69 

Economic  Relationships 

84 

38 

Labor  and  Unemployment 

76 

74 

Race  

94 

70 

Nationalism          .    . 

96 

70 

Militarism    

87 

70 

%  Conservatism 
Democracy 

12 

17 

Economic  Relationships 

0 

20 

Labor  and  Unemployment  

18 

10 

Race  

0 

6 

Na  ti  on  alism 

4 

12 

Militarism  

3 

12 

%  Uncertainty 
Democracy 

15 

12 

Economic  Relationships  

16 

28 

Labor  and  Unemployment  

6 

12 

Race 

6 

10 

Nationalism  .    . 

0 

15 

Militarism  

10 

13 

%  Consistency 
Democracy  

65 

75 

Economic  Relationships 

85 

80 

Labor  and  Unemployment  

76 

88 

Race  

90 

80 

Nationalism 

90 

77 

Militarism        .... 

76 

80 

Totals 
Liberalism  

83 

65 

Conservatism  .    . 

7 

15 

Uncertainty  

10 

16 

Consistency  

77 

77 

9  For  the  description  of  this  test,  see  p.  215,  Chap.  III. 


426        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

ing — a  special  concession  and  departure  from  general  policy. 
However,  there  is  no  report  of  Jane's  achievement  in  that 
course  nor  are  science  information  tests  included  among  the 
standardized  tests  given,  Thus,  the  reasons  for  Jane's  diffi- 
culties with  scientific  reasoning  remain  obscure. 

A  glance  at  the  picture  of  Jane's  performance  on  various 
aspects  of  thinking  in  comparison  with  her  achievement  on 
information  tests  opens  up  an  interesting  hypothesis.  As  a 
student  of  high  verbal  ability  and  good  memory,  has  Jane 
been  permitted  to  exploit  these  two  qualities  without  a  suf- 
ficient challenge  to  other  intellectual  processes? 

Two  tests  give  data  on  Jane's  social  attitudes.  One  of  these 
attempts  to  diagnose  generalized  social  beliefs.  Jane  appar- 
ently has  a  clearly  thought  out  pattern  of  social  beliefs.  Her 
scores  on  liberalism  are  high  and  evenly  distributed  over  all 
of  the  six  areas  included  in  the  test.  Thus,  she  tends  to  ap- 
prove government  control  on  behalf  of  the  general  welfare, 
and  to  reject  economic  individualism.  She  accepts  equality 
for  Negroes  and  thinks  they  have  the  same  qualities  as  white 
people.  She  favors  the  international  viewpoint,  a  logical 
counterpart  of  her  interest  in  foreign  cultures.  There  are 
very  few  items  to  which  she  has  responded  in  a  conserva- 
tive direction.  She  also  seems  to  be  rather  certain  about  her 
beliefs.  Her  responses  are  highly  consistent  in  all  areas, 
though  in  one  of  them,  democracy,  she  falls  in  the  lowest 
quarter  for  the  class,  because  the  class  has  an  unusually  high 
level  of  consistency. 

There  is  also  a  marked  growth  in  her  social  beliefs  from 
the  previous  year.  At  that  time  she  was  highly  uncertain 
and  inconsistent  in  all  areas  except  in  the  area  of  national- 
ism. Social  attitudes  seem  to  be  the  only  area  in  which  Jane 
has  made  a  greater  growth  than  the  group  of  which  she  is  a 
member.  One  would  judge,  then,  that  Jane's  social  beliefs 
are  mature  and  clear  and  probably  arrived  at  by  her  own 
efforts. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS 

SOCIAL   PROBLEMS,    TEST    1.4110 


427 


Jane's  Scores 

Class  Median 

Comprehensiveness 
Total  Courses  of  Action  

6 

6 

Total  Reasons  

48 

46 

Accuracy  in  Reasons  .         .  . 

31 

33 

Ratio  

5.2 

5.1 

Confusion  of  Implications 
Number  Inconsistent 

1 

4 

®/Q  Inconsistent      

3 

9 

Undesirable  Reasons 
Untenable  

9 

9 

Irrelevant 

7 

7 

Dominant  Values  in  Courses  of  Action 
Democratic      

4 

5 

Undemocratic 

0 

0 

Compromise 

2 

2 

Dominant  Values  in  Reasons 
Undemocratic    

3 

5 

Democratic  

24 

26 

In  test  1.41  the  task  is  to  apply  social  values  to  controver- 
sial social  problems.  Here,  also,  Jane  shows  a  preponderantly 
democratic  outlook.  Sixty-two  per  cent  of  the  tenable  reasons 
she  has  used  to  support  the  courses  of  action  she  chose  are 
what  are  defined  as  democratic  values.  She  applies  them 
consistently,  only  3  per  cent  of  her  responses  being  contra- 
dictory to  the  courses  of  action  she  chose.  She  also  shows  a 
higher  degree  of  cautiousness  here  than  on  any  other  test. 
Thus,  a  larger  than  average  fraction  of  the  reasons  she  at- 
tempts are  applicable  to  the  courses  of  action  she  chose. 
The  range  of  die  implications  that  Jane  sees  is  average  for 
the  group. 

Apparently  Jane  does  much  better  with  forms  of  reason- 
ing involving  broad  generalizations  and  general  logical  dis- 


3  For  a  description  of  this  test,  see  p.  180,  Chap.  III. 


428        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

tinctions.  One  is  also  impressed  and  surprised  by  the  coher- 
ence of  her  social  outlook  in  comparison  to  the  apparent 
immaturity  of  her  personal  philosophy  and  her  personal 
goals. 

SKILL   IN   USING   LIBRARIES   AND   BOOKS,    TEST    7.2 


Jane's  Score 


Class  Median 


References 

Right 12 

Wrong 9 

Score 15 

Library  Classification 

Right 6 

Wrong 4 

Score 

Card  Catalog 

Right 9 

Wrong 1 

Score 17 

Reader's  Guide 

Right 1 

Wrong 2 

Score 0 

Index  Information 

Right 

Wrong 0 

Score    16 

Parts  of  Book 

Right 7 

Wrong 3 

Score 11 

Information 

Right 6 

Wrong 4 

Score. .  .  . 

Total  Score 75 


24 


14 


17 


13 


16 


9 

99 


In  skills  in  the  use  of  libraries  and  books  Jane  shows 
marked  weaknesses.  Except  for  her  knowledge  of  the  card 
catalog  and  the  use  of  index  information,  in  which  she  is  at 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  429 

the  median  for  the  group,  she  shows  marked  deficiencies, 
particularly  in  knowledge  of  the  use  of  the  Reader's  Guide. 
Her  total  score  is  the  lowest  in  the  class.  Again  a  deficiency 
with  techniques  of  work  is  indicated. 

By  way  of  general  summary,  one  may  point  out  that  Jane 
has  good  general  ability,  particularly  verbal  ability.  She  has 
a  measure  of  success  in  logical  thinking,  but  falls  down  in 
all  areas  requiring  precise  knowledge,  precise  processes  of 
thinking  or  precise  skills.  In  some  respects  her  techniques  of 
work  seem  quite  deficient.  Her  social  attitudes  are  mature 
and  liberal.  Her  interests  are  highly  selective  and  concen- 
trated on  esthetic  pursuits,  with  preference  for  passive  rather 
than  productive  activities. 

Deficiencies  and  difficulties  seem  to  be  greatest  in  the  area 
of  adjustment  to  other  people,  both  adults  and  age-mates. 
She  seems  immature  in  her  attitudes  toward  herself,  other 
people,  and  work.  Her  personal  goals  and  ambitions  are 
fanciful  and  show  little  thoughtfulness. 

Apparently  she  has  had  altogether  too  meager  an  experi- 
ence in  challenging,  concentrated  work,  and  has  cultivated  a 
tendency  to  take  her  work  and  to  approach  her  interests 
somewhat  lightly. 

It  is  difficult  to  tell  what  would  have  happened  had  the 
faculty  become  cognizant  of  her  difficulties  sooner.  The 
faculty  made  several  efforts  to  meet  her  needs  during  her 
last  year  at  school.  Arrangements  were  made  to  send  her 
to  college  away  from  home  (neither  Stanford  nor  Bryn 
Mawr)  with  the  proviso  that  she  live  in  the  dormitory. 
Special  science  work  was  arranged  in  an  effort  to  give  her 
training  in  precise  thinking.  To  prevent  her  being  lost  in  a 
large  crowd,  she  was  shifted  from  a  large  orchestra  to  a  small 
string  ensemble,  and  from  mass  hockey  into  a  smaller  arch- 
ery group,  in  which  she  "made  the  team."  Her  further  prog- 
ress can  only  be  traced  in  reports  on  her  work  in  college. 


430        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

METHODS  OF  INTERPRETING  AND  USING  EVALUATION  DATA 

For  Guidance  of  Individual  Students 

As  was  described  in  Chapter  I,  one  important  purpose  of 
evaluation  is  the  guidance  of  individual  students.  The  tech- 
niques of  interpretation  illustrated  by  the  case  study  were 
especially  relevant  to  this  purpose.  First,  the  meaning  of 
the  separate  scores  had  to  be  clearly  understood.  The  names 
given  to  these  scores,  such  as  "comprehensiveness,"  might 
be  misleading  unless  related  to  the  behavior  required  by  the 
test.  The  meaning  of  these  scores  was  further  determined  by 
their  deviation  from  the  group  average  as  well  as  the  level 
of  expectancy  for  a  given  student. 

Second,  scores  on  any  test  were  examined  in  relation  to  one 
another  to  arrive  at  a  central  pattern  of  behavior.  In  several 
instruments  the  scores  were  so  dependent  upon  one  another 
that  the  meaning  of  any  one  of  them  was  not  clear  until  the 
others  are  examined.  For  example,  in  the  Scale  of  Beliefs  two 
students  might  both  have  a  score  of  50  on  liberalism,  and  one 
might  say  at  first  that  they  were  equally  liberal.  But  if  the 
first  had  a  score  of  40  on  conservatism  and  10  on  uncertainty, 
while  the  second  had  a  score  of  10  on  conservatism  and  40 
on  uncertainty,  it  is  apparent  that  they  were  not  equally 
liberal.  The  first  had  made  up  his  mind  on  90  per  cent  of  the 
issues  presented  in  the  test  and  divided  his  opinions  almost 
equally  between  the  liberal  and  conservative  viewpoints. 
The  second  had  made  up  his  mind  about  only  60  per  cent  of 
the  issues,  but  his  liberal  responses  predominated  in  the  ratio 
of  five  to  one.  He  was  thus  far  more  liberal  than  the  first  stu- 
dent, although  his  score  on  liberalism  was  the  same. 

There  were  even  occasions  when  the  interpreter  had  to  be 
aware  of  the  possibility  of  a  considerable  shift  in  the  original 
meaning  of  the  score,  when  that  score  appeared  in  certain 
combinations  with  other  scores.  Thus  a  high  score  on  crude 
errors  in  interpreting  data  (marking  true  statements  as  false 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  431 

and  false  as  true )  usually  indicated  a  lack  of  even  rudimen- 
tary skill  in  drawing  inferences  from  data.  If,  however,  the 
scores  on  accuracy  were  high  and  scores  on  other  types  of 
errors  low,  this  score  indicated  careless  reading  of  qualifying 
phrases  in  the  test  statements,  rather  than  a  deficiency  in 
techniques  of  interpreting  data. 

Interpreting  a  comprehensive  set  of  data  from  a  battery  of 
tests  and  other  instruments  presented  a  still  more  complex 
task  of  relating  variables  and  revising  the  meaning  of  each 
aspect  of  behavior  in  terms  of  the  larger  pattern.  Thus,  since 
interests  and  social  attitudes  were  known  to  influence  think- 
ing, data  on  thinking  needed  to  be  examined  in  the  light  of 
evidence  on  interests  and  attitudes.  Formulation  of  tentative 
hypotheses  of  explanation  usually  helped  sharpen  the  exami- 
nation of  evidence  that  might  be  thus  related.  In  formulating 
these  hypotheses  the  interpreter  was  first  assisted  by  the 
structure  of  the  instruments  presented  in  this  report,  for  they 
were  designed  to  reveal  relationships  between  different  types 
of  behavior  as  well  as  possible  causes  of  deviant  behavior. 
Thus  the  tests  of  clear  thinking  provided  some  neutral,  scien- 
tific problems  and  other  problems  in  areas  involving  per- 
sonal values  and  beliefs.  If  errors  in  reasoning  were  con- 
centrated in  the  latter,  the  tests  of  attitudes  and  interests 
might  show  that  the  difficulty  lay  in  lack  of  interest  or  in 
prejudice  rather  than  in  techniques  of  thinking. 

Familiarity  with  common  patterns  of  behavior  in  the  school 
threw  further  light  on  the  behavior  of  individual  students. 
An  ambivalent  pattern  of  social  beliefs  might  be  only  the  re- 
sult of  conflict  between  the  values  emphasized  by  the  school 
and  those  held  by  the  community,  and  therefore  might  not 
be  very  significant  in  individual  cases.  If,  however,  the  usual 
pattern  of  social  beliefs  in  the  school  lay  in  one  direction 
while  an  individual's  pattern  lay  in  another,  this  is  significant 
for  individual  guidance.  Similarly,  if  dislike  of  writing  was 
prevalent  throughout  the  school  due  to  overemphasis  on 


432-        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

written  assignments  in  all  classes,  even  a  moderate  exception 
to  this  general  rule  assumed  significance. 

This  sort  of  interpretation  was  essentially  a  process  of 
postulating  several  alternative  hypotheses  to  explain  deviant 
behavior,  and  of  checking  each  hypothesis  against  other 
data  to  see  which  one  was  most  likely  to  be  correct.  Once  the 
most  probable  causes  of  important  weaknesses  were  located, 
it  was  a  problem  for  the  counselor  and  the  school  staff  to  de- 
cide how  serious  the  difficulty  was  for  a  given  individual 
and  what,  if  anything,  needed  to  be  done  about  it.  Illustrative 
guidance  procedures  have  been  suggested  in  connection 
with  each  instrument  as  well  as  in  the  case  study.  Individual 
variations  were  too  great  to  permit  a  comprehensive  account 
of  all  possible  constructive  methods.  The  results  of  a  con- 
sistent program  of  evaluation  over  a  period  of  years  suggested 
that  certain  methods  work  better  than  others  in  similar  cases. 
However,  it  must  be  remembered  that  evaluation  data  alone 
could  not  solve  the  problems  of  teaching  or  guidance.  They 
only  provided  a  more  adequate  basis  for  solving  them.  Teach- 
ers were  sometimes  annoyed  when  a  program  of  evaluation 
revealed  certain  weaknesses  in  their  program  or  in  some  of 
their  students  without  indicating  precisely  what  was  to  be 
done  about  those  weaknesses.  They  sometimes  concluded 
that  the  tests  were  useless.  This  is  like  saying  that  a  ther- 
mometer is  no  good  because  it  does  not  tell  what  to  do  about 
the  weather.  Tests  could  not  be  expected  to  solve  all  the 
problems  of  education,  but  they  could  and  did  call  attention 
to  many  of  the  problems  to  be  solved. 

For  -Checking  the  Effectiveness  of 
Curriculum  in  Achieving  Ma/or  Objectives 

Another  important  purpose  of  evaluation  was  to  discover 
whether  the  school  was  achieving  its  major  objectives.  Most 
schools  wanted  to  develop  citizens  who  could  think  clearly, 
who  had  democratic  social  attitudes,  who  were  well  adjusted, 
and  the  like.  Evaluation  data  indicated  the  degree  to  which 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  433 

changes  of  this  sort  were  taking  place.  For  this  purpose  in- 
terpretation of  group  data  was  necessary. 

In  the  main,  the  processes  employed  in  interpreting  group 
data  were  similar  to  those  employed  in  examining  data  on 
individuals.  In  each  case  it  was  necessary  to  determine  the 
meaning  of  individual  scores  by  reference  to  a  more  general 
pattern.  In  both  cases  hypotheses  formulated  at  any  point 
were  checked  against  further  evidence. 

The  usual  method  employed  in  locating  strengths  and 
weaknesses  of  a  whole  group — namely,  considering  the  aver- 
ages and  the  distributions  of  scores — was  used  with  these 
data.  By  this  method  it  was  possible  to  determine  the  status 
of  the  group  in  the  separate  aspects  of  behavior  measured  by 
each  instrument,  such  as  the  ability  to  distinguish  facts  from 
assumptions,  or  the  tendency  to  mistake  popular  misconcep- 
tions for  sound  scientific  principles.  Frequently,  however,  it 
was  necessary  to  determine  also  which  combinations  of  be- 
havior were  common  to  many  students  in  a  group,  thus 
requiring  a  common  treatment. 

Thus  in  the  case  of  interests,  the  recurrence  of  a  combina- 
tion of  high  interest  in  music  and  art,  or  a  combination  of 
negative  responses  to  English,  reading,  and  foreign  lan- 
guages by  many  students  were  important  kinds  of  evidence 
for  diagnosing  the  group.  Group  medians  and  distributions  of 
scores  in  each  of  the  separate  categories  did  not  yield  evi- 
dence of  this  type.  A  comparison  of  the  patterns  of  interests 
of  all  individuals  in  the  same  group  was  needed. 

Three  types  of  processes  were  usually  involved  in  estimat- 
ing the  progress  of  a  group:  A  comparison  of  the  scores  by 
groups  in  the  same  grade  or  by  successive  grades  in  the  same 
school,  a  comparison  of  scores  made  by  groups  in  other 
schools  with  a  comparable  curriculum,  and  a  comparison  of 
student  achievement  with  the  behaviors  specified  in  the 
statements  of  the  objectives. 

While  the  only  satisfactory  measure  of  growth  was  the 


434        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

record  of  tlie  same  class  over  a  period  of  years,  a  rough  indi- 
cation of  the  success  of  a  school  program  was  secured  at  once 
by  comparing  scores  on  the  same  test  for  successive  grades. 
In  some  areas  of  objectives,  the  median  of  each  grade  was 
considerably  higher  than  the  median  of  the  preceding  grade, 
while  in  other  areas,  there  was  no  significant  difference  in  the 
grade  medians.  While  the  latter  might  be  the  general  picture, 
particular  classes  taught  by  one  or  two  teachers  made  sig- 
nificant progress.  It  then  became  the  duty  of  the  school  to 
discover  the  factors  which  could  account  for  the  difference. 

The  most  convenient  method  of  comparing  these  scores 
with  scores  made  by  comparable  groups  in  other  schools 
might  have  been  with  reference  to  national  norms.  Thus, 
while  progress  might  be  shown  from  grade  to  grade  on  the 
test  of  interpretation  of  data,  the  median  of  each  grade  might 
stand  in  the  lowest  quarter  of  scores  made  by  all  other  pupils 
of  this  grade  who  took  the  test.  Unless  some  special  factor 
was  at  work,  such  as  very  low  reading  test  scores  for  the 
school  population,  this  might  indicate  at  once  that  still  fur- 
ther progress  must  be  made  before  the  school's  record  could 
be  considered  satisfactory. 

This  method,  however,  was  avoided  as  much  as  possible 
in  the  Eight-Year  Study  for  several  reasons.  In  the  first  place, 
it  was  recognized  that  as  long  as  there  were  important  differ- 
ences in  objectives  and  curriculum  practices  among  schools, 
it  would  be  inappropriate  to  measure  progress  by  the  same 
standards,  particularly  if  these  standards  represented  nothing 
more  than  an  average  of  the  performance  of  different  groups 
under  varying  circumstances.  The  pattern  of  interests  in  a 
school  for  foreign  students  in  New  York  City  could  not 
necessarily  be  considered  appropriate  as  a  "norm"  or  desir- 
able pattern  of  interests  for  a  suburban  school  in  the  Middle 
West,  and  the  average  of  the  two  patterns  might  not  be  desir- 
able for  either  school.  Similarly,  one  would  not  expect  stu- 
dents in  a  school  which  was  barely  beginning  to  explore  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  435 

methods  of  developing  critical  thinking  to  be  judged  by  the 
same  criteria  as  were  students  who  have  had  long  and  care- 
ful training. 

Difficulties  were  also  encountered  because  of  the  methods 
of  using  norms  to  which  teachers  had  been  accustomed.  The 
national  average  had  been  invested  with  almost  magical  sig- 
nificance, so  that  many  teachers  werej  too  easily  satisfied  if 
their  groups  came  up  to  it,  even  when  they  might  have 
greatly  exceeded  it,  and  too  easily  discouraged  if  their  groups 
fell  below  it,  even  though  their  progress  was  all  that  could 
be  expected.  For  this  reason,  only  tables  of  medians  of  com- 
parable groups  in  other  schools  were  made  available  to  the 
evaluation  representatives  of  schools  in  the  Eight-Year  Study, 
who  were  trained  to  interpret  them.  These  gave  a  rough  and 
admittedly  cumbersome  method  of  estimating  the  relative 
progress  of  comparable  groups.,  but  it  was  hoped  that  by  this 
very  fact  a  more  thoughtful  use  of  norms  would  be  stimulated. 

A  third  possible  method  of  interpreting  scores  to  indicate 
the  success  of  a  program  in  reaching  its  objectives  was  a 
comparison  of  the  level  of  ability  revealed  by  the  tests  with 
the  level  of  ability  required  in  life  situations.  Thus,  if  the 
use  of  the  correct  scientific  principles  in  life  problems  were 
the  objective  of  the  school,  and  the  tests  revealed  that  stu- 
dents accepted  a  variety  of  popular  misconceptions  as  scien- 
tific principles,  then  the  school  had  not  done  enough  in  this 
direction,  even  though  all  other  schools  showed  a  similar 
weakness. 

This  sort  of  interpretation,  however,  had  always  to  be  made 
cautiously,  because  the  level  of  accomplishment  demanded 
by  life  situations  was  often  a  matter  of  vague  conjecture.  It 
was  thus  easy  to  expect  too  much  or  too  little  of  students. 
The  present  level  of  achieving  these  newer  intangible  objec- 
tives may  be  too  much  determined  by  inadequate  methods 
of  helping  students  achieve  them.  Nevertheless,  some  com- 
parison of  pupils'  performance  with  life  demands  was  in- 


436        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

escapable  if  we  were  not  always  to  rest  content  with  wliat 
other  schools  were  doing.  Perhaps  none  of  them  was  doing 
enough. 

For  Checking  Hypotheses  Underlying  the  Program 

A  third  important  purpose  of  evaluation  was  to  check  the 
hypotheses  underlying  the  school  program.  Often  new  prac- 
tices were  introduced  in  the  hope  of  producing  certain  desir- 
able changes  in  students.  These  changes  might  not  come 
about,  or  they  might  be  accompanied  by  other  changes  which 
were  less  desirable.  One  public  school  introduced  a  core  pro- 
gram with  several  purposes  in  mind,  one  of  which  was  to  de- 
velop better  social  attitudes.  A  comprehensive  testing  pro- 
gram revealed  that  while  the  social  attitudes  developed  were 
clearer,  more  consistent,  and  more  liberal  than  in  most 
schools  in  the  Study,  the  students  had  serious  difficulties 
with  techniques  of  precise  thinking.  In  drawing  inferences 
from  data,  they  exhibited  little  caution  and  showed  a  tend- 
ency to  go  beyond  the  data.  In  applying  facts  and  principles 
they  failed  to  discriminate  those  which  were  valid  and  rel- 
evant from  their  opposites.  Apparently  in  emphasizing  social 
values  the  school  relied  too  much  on  generalizations  and  too 
little  upon  the  careful  analysis  of  factual  data. 

In  another  school  the  evaluation  of  reading  revealed  that 
one  group  specializing  in  science  and  mathematics  showed 
a  more  limited  appreciation  than  all  others,  including  those 
in  other  grades  specializing  in  the  same  field.  They  found 
little  enjoyment  in  reading;  they  did  not  identify  themselves 
with  their  reading  or  relate  their  reading  to  life  problems. 
Since  this  was  a  marked  deviation  from  the  type  of  responses 
prevailing  in  the  school,  the  problem  was  considered  by  the 
faculty.  It  developed  that  a  special  course  in  literature  was 
offered  to  this  group.  On  the  hypothesis  that  science  students 
are  interested  in  scientists,  this  course  concentrated  on  biog- 
raphies of  scientists  and  mathematicians.  Since  it  was  not  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  437 

intention  of  the  staff  to  narrow  the  reading  interests  of  these 
students,  a  broader  program  was  agreed  upon. 

Still  another  school  had  hoped  to  develop  democratic  at- 
titudes by  means  of  a  program  of  extra-curricular  activities 
organized  by  the  student  council,  while  conducting  its  aca- 
demic curriculum  in  the  usual  manner.  The  results  of  the  test 
on  Beliefs  about  School  Life  revealed  that  a  large  majority 
of  these  students  preferred  authoritarian  methods  of  class- 
room management,  approved  of  social  distinction  of  all  sorts, 
and  in  general  had  tendencies  toward  undemocratic  atti- 
tudes. These  results  called  into  question  the  efficacy  of  this 
program  of  student  activities  for  the  purpose  of  democratiz- 
ing school  life.  In  the  course  of  an  investigation  by  a  group 
of  students  and  faculty  members,  it  was  discovered  that  the 
student  council  was  run  by  an  inner  clique.  Many  of  the 
student  activities  tended  to  be  exclusive  and  to  have  other 
undemocratic  characteristics.  The  active  participation  was 
limited  largely  to  students  in  the  upper  grades.  In  the  light 
of  the  facts  brought  out  by  this  study,  a  reorganization  of 
student  activities  was  undertaken,  involving  a  closer  relation- 
ship between  curricular  and  extracurricular  activities. 

Such  instances  indicated  that  special  care  had  to  be  exer- 
cised when  changes  were  introduced  into  the  program  to  find 
out  not  only  whether  the  intended  results  were  produced  but 
also  whether  undesirable  features  did  not  accompany  them. 
Even  if  no  major  changes  had  been  made,  the  hypotheses  on 
which  the  school  had  always  operated  might  be  faulty. 
Hence,  evaluation  data  needed  to  be  examined  with  special 
reference  to  the  issues  underlying  the  program. 

Possibility  of  Interpretation 

The  foregoing  discussion  may  have  left  the  impression 
that  interpretation  of  evaluation  data  required  very  unusual 
insight  and  patience,  and  too  extensive  knowledge  of  evalua- 
tion for  the  classroom  teacher  to  master.  There  is  no  getting 


438         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

around  the  fact  that  a  thoughtful  interpretation  of  the  evi- 
dence on  students'  progress  and  the  effectiveness  of  curricu- 
lum practices  is  complex,  and  that  it  can  be  learned  only  by 
long  practice  supplemented  by  careful  explanation.  Yet  there 
is  no  reason  to  believe  that  further  progress  in  getting  a  more 
adequate  picture  of  pupil  growth  will  ever  return  to  the 
primitive  simplicity  of  school  marks.  Reducing  the  amount  of 
data  secured  is  no  solution,  for  a  few  scattered  data  can  only 
raise  questions,  not  answer  them.  A  rich  and  full  program 
of  evaluation  can  suggest  answers  to  a  great  many  questions, 
but  only  by  thoughtful  interpretation  and  not  by  chance. 
Teachers  must  learn  to  get  meaning  from  the  extensive  and 
well-integrated  sets  of  data  now  available.  Unless  somebody 
knows  what  the  scores  mean  and  takes  them  into  account  in 
his  teaching,  it  is  obvious  that  there  is  no  point  in  getting 
them. 

On  the  other  hand,  the  process  of  interpretation  is  not  so 
difficult  for  busy  teachers  in  a  large  public  school  as  the 
foregoing  may  suggest.  When  teachers  know  the  pupils  con- 
cerned, hypotheses  to  account  for  their  test  scores  readily 
occur  to  them.  Then,  too,  if  evaluation  is  carried  on  con- 
tinuously, the  evidence  accumulates  gradually,  and  only  a 
few  data  need  be  interpreted  at  any  one  time,  and  fitted  into 
what  one  already  knows  about  students.  Also,  the  processes 
which  appear  elaborate,  when  written  down  and  explained 
verbally,  easily  become  part  and  parcel  of  the  common  sense 
thinking  of  thoughtful  teachers.  Finally,  when  evaluation  is 
undertaken  as  a  common  task  for  the  school,  with  the  whole 
faculty  cooperating  in  interpreting  the  results,  the  task  for 
any  one  individual  is  reduced. 


Chapter  VIII 

PLANNING  AND  ADMINISTERING  THE 
EVALUATION  PROGRAM 


The  preceding  chapters  have  already  dealt  with  many  of  the 
basic  problems  in  planning  and  administering  an  evaluation 
program.  They  have  discussed  the  purposes  of  evaluation,  its 
basic  assumptions,  and  the  steps  which  must  be  followed  in 
developing  appraisal  instruments.  They  have  indicated  an 
appropriate  division  of  labor  among  teachers,  school  officers, 
and  experts  in  evaluation.  They  have  suggested  a  possible 
classification  of  school  objectives  by  types  of  behavior,  each 
of  which  requires  a  different  technique  of  appraisal.  They 
have  described  instruments  and  techniques  for  the  study  of 
growth  toward  objectives  usually  regarded  as  "intangible," 
such  as  certain  aspects  of  thinking,  social  sensitivity,  appre- 
ciations, interests,  and  personal  and  social  adjustment.  They 
have  reported  in  great  detail  the  method  of  construction  of 
these  instruments  so  that  teachers  might  develop  others. 
They  have  indicated,  at  least  by  implication,  certain  charac- 
teristics which  are  desirable  in  evaluation  instruments  de- 
veloped or  selected  by  a  school  staff.  In  addition  to  those 
usually  discussed,  such  as  validity,  reliability,  objectivity,  ap- 
propriateness to  age  levels,  and  the  like,  the  characteristics 
given  special  emphasis  in  this  report  were  the  diagnostic 
value  of  the  multiple  scores  yielded  by  these  instruments, 
and  the  interrelationships  of  these  instruments,  so  that  each 
score  was  supported  and  explained  by  other  scores  on  the 
same  or  other  instruments.  Finally,  the  previous  chapter  dealt 
with  methods  of  interpreting  and  using  evaluation  data. 
All  of  these  considerations  are  pertinent  to  the  problem  of 

439 


440        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

planning  and  administering  an  evaluation  program.  In  addi- 
tion, certain  administrative  procedures  are  essential  to  assure 
the  comprehensiveness  of  the  appraisal,  to  secure  the  co- 
operation of  the  entire  staff  of  the  school,  and  to  increase 
the  practicability  of  the  program.  When  testing  is  left  to  each 
individual  teacher,  there  is  likely  to  be  incoordination,  and 
the  most  important  objectives — those  to  which  the  whole 
school  program  is  dedicated — are  frequently  overlooked,  es- 
pecially since  they  are  usually  the  hardest  to  evaluate.  Ob- 
jectives which  are  easiest  to  evaluate  may  be  emphasized  out 
of  all  proportion  to  their  importance  and,  as  a  result,  attention 
may  be  drawn  away  from  other  equally  important  objectives. 
No  data  may  be  secured  relevant  to  the  hypotheses  on  which 
the  school  is  operating.  Pupils  may  be  overburdened  with 
tests  in  certain  departments  or  at  certain  times. 

If,  on  the  other  hand,  the  actual  conduct  of  the  appraisal 
is  left  to  an  evaluation  specialist,  there  is  the  danger  that 
pertinent  data  will  not  reach  the  teachers  who  should  act 
upon  them.  The  results  may  be  reported  in  a  form  which 
teachers  cannot  readily  understand,  and  recorded  in  ways 
which  involve  undue  clerical  labor.  A  most  common  defect  is 
that  all  available  time  and  effort  are  spent  in  gathering  data, 
with  none  left  over  to  interpret  or  use  them  for  individual 
guidance  or  curriculum  improvement. 

It  is  the  intention  of  this  chapter  to  discuss  certain  prin- 
ciples and  procedures  of  planning  and  administering  an  eval- 
uation program  which  have  helped  to  make  it  effective  in 
some  of  the  schools  participating  in  the  Eight-Year  Study. 
For  the  sake  of  brevity,  no  account  will  be  given  of  the 
gradual  development  of  these  practices,  and  only  occasional 
references  will  be  made  to  the  diversity  of  practice  on  these 
points  now  prevailing  among  the  cooperating  schools.  The 
chapter  will  attempt  to  describe  a  few  illustrative  practices 
in  planning  the  program,  recording  the  data,  and  providing 
for  their  effective  use. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  441 

Planning  the  Scope  and  Emphasis  of  the  Program 

Early  in  the  Study  it  was  found  that  a  comprehensive  eval- 
uation program  required  careful,  cooperative  planning  by 
the  staff  of  the  school.  The  data  necessary  for  a  well  rounded 
picture  of  individual  development,  of  the  progress  of  the 
group,  and  of  the  effectiveness  of  the  curriculum  would  not 
be  secured  if  the  task  was  left  to  individuals.  It  was  quite 
evident  that  the  staff  as  a  whole  must  decide  what  to  evaluate, 
what  kinds  of  evidence  to  secure,  and  how  to  go  about  secur- 
ing evidence  and  using  it.  As  the  first  step  in  evaluation  in- 
volves the  formulation  of  the  school's  objectives,  this  coopera- 
tive planning  of  evaluation  began  with  this  step.  In  order 
to  secure  a  statement  of  objectives  which  was  representative 
of  the  work  done  in  the  school  and  thus  to  make  sure  that 
no  phase  of  growth  really  emphasized  in  the  school  was  neg- 
lected, the  whole  staff  participated  in  the  process  of  formu- 
lating -the  basic  platform  of  objectives.  Each  teacher  or  de- 
partmental group  of  teachers  submitted  a  list  of  objectives. 
These  lists  were  then  considered  by  committees  and  by  the 
whole  faculty  in  order  to  clarify  them  further  and  to  discover 
where  there  were  common  emphases  and  where  unique  types 
of  development  were  indicated. 

If  there  was  any  conflict  between  the  appraisal  of  the 
school- wide  objectives  and  those  held  by  individual  teachers, 
it  was  rather  commonly  assumed  that  the  first  responsibility 
of  the  school  was  to  its  general  objectives.  While  the  principle 
was  never  abandoned  that  the  school  as  well  as  individual 
teachers  should  do  all  they  could  to  study  growth  toward  the 
objectives  unique  to  the  specific  courses,  the  larger  principle 
usually  prevailed  that  the  study  of  the  most  important  aspects 
of  human  development  as  expressed  in  the  general  objectives 
should  be  the  major  concern  of  a  school.  The  nature  and  ex- 
tent of  the  appraisal  of  the  specific  objectives  was  to  be 
planned  so  that  it  was  consistently  related  to  this  general 
program  and  helped  to  support  and  clarify  it. 


442.        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Fortunately,  the  areas  of  objectives  of  general  concern 
were  usually  limited  in  number  and  thus  did  not  constitute 
too  heavy  a  burden  either  on  the  resources  of  the  school  or 
the  time  and  tolerance  of  the  students.  For  example,  most 
schools  were  concerned  with  one  or  more  phases  of  critical 
thinking,  social  attitudes,  certain  work  habits  and  study  skills, 
interests  and  appreciations,  social  adjustments,  and  certain 
types  of  functional  information.  Hence,  in  most  schools  there 
was  sufficient  opportunity  to  carry  on  additional  specific  in- 
vestigations of  student  growth. 

A  second  major  principle  governing  the  planning  was  that 
appraisal  was  to  be  continuous.  The  adoption  of  this  policy 
meant  that  the  schools  had  to  consider  the  time  and  effort 
needed  for  a  continuous  check  before  decisions  were  made 
regarding  what  range  of  objectives  would  be  appraised, 
or  how  detailed  the  check  was  to  be.  As  can  be  seen  later, 
this  consideration  also  determined  the  calendar  adopted  for 
the  administration  of  the  evaluation  instruments. 

It  was  also  clearly  understood  that  it  was  the  program  of 
the  school  and  its  effects  on  student  growth  and  not  the  in- 
dividual teacher  or  pupil  that  was  being  appraised.  The 
effectiveness  of  evaluation  is  likely  to  be  impaired  if  the 
evaluation  program  is  conceived  by  the  teachers  either  as  an 
extension  of  the  usual  examinations  and  marks  in  courses  or 
as  a  means  of  judging  their  competence.  With  the  first  mis- 
conception, teachers  may  try  to  find  the  strengths  and  weak- 
nesses of  their  pupils  with  the  idea  of  rewarding  the  strengths 
and  penalizing  the  weaknesses,  accompanied  by  some  exhor- 
tation to  do  better,  but  without  making  any  significant  change 
in  their  courses,  or  still  less  in  the  whole  school  program. 
With  the  second  misconception,  teachers  may  try  to  justify 
the  present  situation  rather  than  to  seek  fully  and  frankly  for 
points  needing  improvement.  For  these  reasons  the  schools 
favored  instruments  and  devices  which  yielded  descriptive 
diagnoses  of  students  and  which,  because  of  this  character- 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  443 

istic,  could  not  be  easily  converted  into  grades  and  marks. 
Most  of  the  evaluation  instruments  used  also  diagnosed  the 
kinds  o£  behavior  capable  of  development  only  through  con- 
certed and  cooperative  efforts  of  many  teachers  over  a  period 
of  time,  and  not  by  the  work  of  one  teacher  in  one  course  or 
unit  of  work. 

Finally,  it  was  understood  that  the  evaluation  program 
was  to  serve  the  local  needs  and  purposes  of  each  school. 
The  particular  emphasis  as  well  as  the  extent  of  the  program 
was  largely  determined  by  what  each  school  needed  data 
for.  Thus  many  schools  had  set  up  an  experimental  program 
on  some  central  hypothesis.  Checking  that  particular  hypoth- 
esis often  required  a  detailed  appraisal  of  certain  specified 
types  of  growth,  such  as  in  critical  thinking,  in  range  and 
maturity  of  interests,  in  social  sensitivity.  In  these  cases  the 
evaluation  program  was  planned  to  give  most  detailed  evi- 
dence on  these  points.  Local  conditions  also  influenced  the 
plans.  For  example,  some  schools  drawing  students  from 
widely  scattered  places  had  to  concentrate  the  evaluation  in 
the  earlier  grades  on  the  diagnosis  of  interests,  abilities,  and 
basic  skills.  Still  other  schools  had  differentiated  sequences  of 
programs,  calling  for  evidence  necessary  for  the  placement 
of  the  students  in  these  sequences  as  well  as  for  determining 
the  relative  effectiveness  of  these  programs.  Often  special 
effort  was  needed  to  appraise  the  acquisition  of  common  skills 
in  order  to  answer  the  questions  of  parents  and  the  commu- 
nity who  feared  that  the  new  curriculum  might  neglect  these 
outcomes. 

Certain  practical  considerations  also  limited  the  plans. 
While  most  schools  made  an  effort  to  plan  the  scope  and  the 
nature  of  their  evaluation  programs  according  to  what  they 
thought  to  be  important  objectives  or  crucial  needs  of  their 
programs  rather  than  in  terms  of  economy,  immediate  avail- 
ability of  instruments  and  techniques,  or  the  ease  of  their  ad- 
ministration, it  was  natural  that  the  cloth  had  to  be  cut  ac- 


444        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

cording  to  the  resources  of  the  school.  Thus,  financial  ex- 
penses were  involved  in  administering  the  testing  program 
even  though  much  of  the  scoring  was  done  at  the  evaluation 
headquarters.  Someone's  time  and  effort  was  required  for 
handling  the  data,  since  there  was  no  point  in  collecting  more 
data  than  could  be  properly  recorded,  interpreted,  and  used. 

In  determining  how  to  adjust  the  scope  of  the  program  to 
the  limitations  of  resources,  the  general  principle  followed 
was  to  plan  to  appraise  at  least  in  limited  fashion  each  of  the 
major  areas  of  objectives  before  planning  a  more  detailed 
evaluation  of  a  single  area.  This  seemed  wise  first  because  it 
was  recognized  that  evidence  covering  a  fairly  broad  range 
of  behavior  is  needed  for  proper  appraisal  of  the  program  of 
a  school.  The  schools  also  realized  that  teachers  tend  to  em- 
phasize the  areas  of  development  the  results  of  which  they 
can  see  more  clearly.  An  even  distribution  of  efforts  of  ap- 
praisal over  the  significant  objectives  was  thus  expected  to 
produce  a  more  even  distribution  of  emphasis  in  teaching. 
Finally,  since  detailed  appraisal  was  usually  given  to  areas 
of  objectives  which  were  easiest  to  appraise  or  in  which  in- 
struments were  readily  available,  it  seemed  wise  to  make 
sure  that"  some  of  the  important  intangible  objectives  for 
which  no  refined  techniques  or  instruments  were  as  yet  avail- 
able would  not  be  overlooked. 

Generally  speaking,  then,  while  the  schools  attempted  to 
evaluate  as  broad  a  range  of  objectives  as  possible,  the  actual 
program  rested  on  decisions  representing  a  combination  of 
the  ideal  possibilities  and  the  practical  limitations  of  the 
school  situation. 

Collecting  Data. 

Once  the  staff  agreed  on  the  general  scope  of  the  program, 
it  considered  the  methods  for  securing  the  needed  evidence. 
This  required  a  preliminary  survey  of  the  data  already  avail- 
able in  the  school.  Only  when  the  faculty  had  explored  the 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  445 

possible  relationships  to  school  objectives  of  the  data  which 
was  already  collected  was  it  in  a  position  to  decide  what  fur- 
ther data  were  needed.  In  the  process  of  clarifying  the  school 
objectives  it  was  usually  discovered  that  the  faculty  was  al- 
ready collecting  many  types  of  data  on  these  aspects  of  de- 
velopment. Thus,  many  schools  had  a  testing  program  in- 
cluding aptitude  tests,  reading  tests,  and  information  tests. 
Most  schools  also  had  an  abundance  of  less  formal  types  of 
data  collected  in  the  normal  process  of  teaching  and  adminis- 
tering the  school.  In  most  cases  these  data  were  put  to  only  a 
limited  use,  partly  because  they  were  scattered,  partly  be- 
cause of  the  tendency  to  consider  only  the  scores  on  objective 
tests  as  appropriate  evidence,  but  mainly  because  their  bear- 
ing on  the  objectives  of  the  school  was  not  evident. 

When,  however,  the  objectives  were  clarified  to  the  point 
where  teachers  could  clearly  see  the  concrete  behaviors  in- 
volved, the  bearing  on  the  broader  objectives  of  some  data 
which  teachers  were  collecting  for  specific  purposes  became 
apparent.  Thus  the  English  teachers  found  that  student  writ- 
ing could  be  examined  for  evidences  of  interests,  social  ad- 
justment, and  social  attitudes  as  well  as  of  the  ability  to  spell 
and  write  correctly.  Records  of  free  reading  were  found  to 
yield  evidence  on  maturity  of  tastes  as  well  as  of  quantity  of 
reading.  Even  such  simple  data  as  the  records  of  activities 
and  subjects  taken  assumed  significance  when  considered  in 
the  context  of  other  facts  about  the  students. 

This  examination  of  the  data  already  available  usually  in- 
dicated certain  gaps,  that  is,  objectives  on  which  little  evi- 
dence was  being  obtained.  Hence,  the  next  step  was  to  plan 
the  ways  and  means  of  securing  the  additional  data  needed. 
Usually  at  this  point  there  was  a  tendency  to  consider  only 
paper-and-pencil  tests.  However,  a  careful  analysis  of  the 
methods  of  securing  evidence  most  appropriate  to  each  objec- 
tive revealed  that  the  classroom  situations  provided  a  far 
greater  source  for  securing  data  on  students  than  had  usually 


446        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

been  assumed.  For  the  appraisal  of  some  objectives,  such  as 
the  ability  to  plan  the  attack  on  research  problems,  or  to  use 
laboratory  techniques  and  tools,  the  observation  and  record- 
ing of  student  behavior  in  normal  classroom  situations  was 
the  best  if  not  the  only  adequate  source.  Thus,  one  school 
secured  data  on  student  growth  in  planning  research  by  the 
simple  device  of  providing  students  with  pads  on  which  to 
record  in  duplicate  the  successive  outlines  of  the  plans  they 
made.  At  other  points  semi-controlled  classroom  situations, 
suitable  for  both  learning  and  evaluation  purposes,  could  well 
be  used  in  place  of  formal  tests.  Thus  the  difficulties  encoun- 
tered in  getting  information  from  libraries  and  books  could 
be  diagnosed,  and  in  many  schools  were  diagnosed,  by  giving 
students  assignments  requiring  the  use  of  the  library  and  by 
observing  the  methods  they  used  in  obtaining  the  necessary 
information. 

These  uses  of  sources  of  data  in  processes  integral  to  teach- 
ing were  found  to  be  particularly  helpful  because  when  teach- 
ers were  directly  responsible  for  collecting  evidence  they 
more  often  used  the  results  than  when  only  the  summary  of 
data  came  to  them.  However,  collaboration  and  systematic 
allocation  of  responsibilities  on  a  school-wide  basis  are  neces- 
sary to  prevent  this  method  from  being  too  time  consuming. 
In  economizing  effort  it  was  found  that  certain  departments 
or  teachers  of  certain  areas  were  in  especially  strategic  posi- 
tions to  collect  one  kind  of  evidence,  while  others  had  greater 
opportunity  to  obtain  information  of  a  different  sort.  By  sys- 
tematizing the  use  of  such  informal  devices  and  by  making 
the  results  generally  available,  many  schools  found  that  they 
could  extend  the  scope  of  their  evaluation  through  the  use 
of  opportunities  already  existing  in  the  classroom. 

Having  agreed  upon  the  informal  methods  to  be  used  in 
obtaining  evidence,  the  next  step  was  to  plan  the  use  of  more 
formal  devices.  Usually  paper  and  pencil  tests  were  reserved 
for  points  where  information  was  lacking  altogether,  or 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  447 

where  the  available  information  was  inadequate,  or  where 
the  use  of  informal  methods  entailed  too  much  time  and 
effort.  Thus,  most  schools  had  considerable  evidence  on 
information  and  skills,  but  little  or  no  evidence  on  the 
growth  of  students  in  various  phases  of  thinking.  The  infor- 
mation on  social  attitudes  secured  or  securable  through  anec- 
dotal records,  classroom  observation,  or  from  student  papers 
was  found  to  be  too  scattered  and  meager  to  give  an  adequate 
picture  of  social  beliefs  over  a  range  of  social  issues  of  im- 
portance. At  many  points,  then,  it  was  necessary  to  use  addi- 
tional paper  and  pencil  tests,  either  because  they  represented 
the  only  appropriate  method  of  getting  the  evidence  or  be- 
cause they  were  most  economical. 

Drawing  Up  a  Schedule  for  Testing 

In  setting  a  calendar  for  the  testing  program,  it  was  neces- 
sary to  consider  several  factors.  In  die  first  place,  the  total 
time  devoted  to  testing  could  not  be  so  great  that  students 
and  faculty  thought  themselves  overburdened  with  tests. 
To  avoid  this  difficulty,  careful  estimates  were  made  of  the 
total  time  needed  for  taking  all  tests  which  were  tentatively 
proposed  for  the  program.  Some  schools  even  went  so  far  as 
to  set  up  a  time  limit  and  to  eliminate  certain  instruments  if 
the  proposed  schedule  exceeded  that  limit. 

In  the  second  place,  the  schedule  had  to  be  drawn  so  that 
there  was  no  undue  concentration  of  formal  tests  toward  the 
end  of  the  year,  and  particularly  toward  the  end  of  the 
twelfth  grade,  since  such  a  congestion  of  schedule  subjected 
students  to  unnecessary  tension,  and  did  not  provide  evi- 
dence at  times  when  the  results  could  most  effectively  be 
used.  Generally,  congestion  was  prevented  by  devising  a  ten- 
tative calendar  for  the  testing  program  covering  all  the  grades 
of  the  school.  Such  a  calendar  included  the  repeated  use  of 
certain  instruments  to  check  on  growth  as  well  as  the  giving 
of  certain  tests  which  needed  to  be  used  only  once.  Tests 


448        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

yielding  information  basic  to  understanding  new  students 
and  for  the  initial  planning  of  teaching  were  usually  placed 
early  while  others  were  distributed  over  successive  years. 

The  schedule  also  provided  for  a  fair  distribution  of  time 
among  the  several  subject  fields  so  that  the  testing  did  not 
take  an  undue  amount  of  time  from  any  one  class.  This  was 
done  by  allocating  different  tests  to  different  departments 
in  the  school  or  by  staggering  the  successive  periods  of  the 
day  used  for  giving  tests. 

The  methods  of  organizing  for  this  cooperative  job  varied 
greatly  from  school  to  school,  depending  on  the  size  of  the 
school  and  the  make:up  of  their  faculties.  In  some  cases,  par- 
ticularly in  smaller  schools,  the  school  psychologist  or  coun- 
selor took  the  major  responsibility  for  drafting  the  tentative 
plans  and  for  arranging  the  practical  details.  In  such  cases 
much  of  the  participation  of  the  faculty  was  achieved  through 
informal  contacts  and  personal  conferences. 

In  other  schools  evaluation  committees  were  established, 
whose  responsibility  it  was  to  get  the  necessary  information 
and  advice  from  the  rest  of  the  faculty,  to  draw  up  a  plan, 
and  to  care  for  the  routines.  Often  members  of  such  commit- 
tees took  special  responsibility  for  giving  certain  instruments 
or  series  of  instruments  as  well  as  for  collecting  certain  ma- 
terials from  other  teachers. 

In  still  other  schools  the  responsibilities  were  divided 
among  the  staff  according  to  the  types  of  evidence  to  be  col- 
lected. Thus  a  psychologist  became  responsible  for  giving  the 
psychological  tests  and  reading  tests.  An  evaluation  represen- 
tative supervised  the  use  of  the  special  tests  developed  by  the 
Evaluation  Staff,  while  individual  teachers  were  responsible 
for  information  and  skill  tests  in  their  respective  areas.  What- 
ever the  particular  scheme,  it  was  found  necessary  to  make 
careful,  coordinated  plans  for  the  entire  program  of  evalua- 
tion. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  449 

Summarizing  and  Circulating  the  Results 

Since  the  evidence  of  student  development  was  obtained 
from  records  already  existing  in  the  school,  from  collecting 
data  easily  obtained  as  part  of  the  class  work,  and  from  espe- 
cially selected  tests  and  appraisal  devices,  the  problem  of 
organizing  and  summarizing  these  varied  types  of  informa- 
tion was  an  important  one.  Part  of  the  task  of  organization 
was  accomplished  by  using  a  folder  for  each  student,  and 
placing  all  records  relating  to  this  student  in  this  folder.  The 
student  folder  became  a  file  of  information  to  which  addi- 
tions were  made  as  the  evidence  accumulated. 

However,  the  varied  forms  of  evidence  made  it  necessary 
to  utilize  additional  techniques  of  organization.  The  test 
scores  were  already  organized  into  patterns  devised  by  the 
evaluation  committees.  In  the  case  of  data  recorded  by  stu- 
dents or  parents,  such  as  entrance  information,  reading  rec- 
ords, and  written  papers,  the  administrative  problem  was  to 
organize  the  record  keeping  in  such  a  way  that  a  consistent 
and  cumulative  record  became  available.  Thus,  in  case  of  the 
reading  records,  a  certain  time  each  week  was  allotted  to  stu- 
dents to  write  down  the  books  they  had  read  during  the  pre- 
ceding week.  Copies  of  written  work  were  assembled  in  the 
student  folder. 

To  obtain  satisfactory  records  from  observations  made  by 
teachers  or  other  persons  in  a  position  to  observe  students  in- 
volved several  other  administrative  problems.  Chief  among 
these  was  that  of  obtaining  observed  facts  on  behavior,  in 
place  of  ratings  drawn  largely  from  memory.  Some  organiza- 
tion was  also  needed  to  obtain  a  sufficiently  representative 
sampling  of  the  observations  from  different  teachers,  sup- 
posedly in  a  position  to  see  the  student  in  different  situations. 
Staff  conferences  devoted  to  clarifying  the  behavior  to  be  ob- 
served and  the  techniques  of  obtaining  the  record  most 
economically,  and  then  to  periodic  discussion  of  records  sub- 


450        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

mitted  were  usually  the  most  effective  means  of  improving 
the  content  and  the  representativeness  of  such  records. 

Another  problem  was  that  of  time.  Teachers  often  felt  them- 
selves too  pressed  to  report  in  writing  the  significant  obser- 
vations they  made.  Since  this  type  of  information  had  an  im- 
portant place  in  the  evaluation  program  and  since  the 
availability  of  each  teacher's  observations  to  all  others  was 
necessary,  various  devices  were  adopted  to  effect  economy  of 
time  without  losing  the  descriptive  quality  of  this  evidence. 
One  method  adopted  was  to  combine  checking  and  anecdotal 
descriptions,  particularly  when  frequency  of  a  given  type  of 
behavior  constituted  an  important  phase  of  evidence.1 

Another  method  of  economizing  time  was  to  identify  the 
points  at  which  the  observational  records  were  of  particular 
significance,  and  then  to  limit  the  writing  to  descriptions  of 
these  types  of  behavior  only.  Furthermore,  in  many  schools, 
where  extended  use  was  made  of  observational  records,  time 
was  allotted  in  the  teaching  schedule  for  making  anecdotal 
records. 

Organization  of  these  varied  items  of  data  involved  not 
only  that  they  be  brought  together  at  one  point  or  in  one 
folder,  but  also  that  they  be  interpreted  by  someone  and 
their  implications  passed  through  the  mind  of  one  who  knew 
the  student  and  had  a  responsibility  for  him.  Too  often,  the 
school  psychologist  retained  the  data  on  psychological  tests; 
information  about  home  background  and  previous  experience 
was  to  be  found  only  in  the  principal's  office;  records  of 
achievement  tests  and  other  information  pertaining  to 
achievement  in  subject  areas  were  to  be  found  only  in  the  files 
of  department  offices  or  of  the  individual  teachers.  This  de- 
centralized method  of  information  keeping  proved  to  be  a 
serious  obstacle  to  adequate  summary,  interpretation,  and 

1 A  sample  of  a  record  of  this  type  is  described  in  the  Twelfth  Yearbook 
of  the  National  Council  for  the  Social  Studies,  1941,  p.  222  ff.  Miss  Dor- 
othy Van  Alstyne,  psychologist  of  the  Francis  W.  Parker  School,  developed 
a  form  for  this  purpose  which  was  used  by  several  schools. 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  451 

use  of  the  data.  Even  those  who  seriously  and  sincerely  tried 
to  learn  something  about  the  students  were  discouraged  when 
much  time  had  to  be  spent  in  locating  the  evidence.  When 
records  were  not  easily  available,  nor  easily  summarized,  they 
were  treated  as  something  to  be  filed  away  and  not  as  some- 
thing to  be  used  for  teaching,  guidance,  and  curriculum  mak- 
ing. Thus,  in  schools  where  data  about  the  reading  ability 
of  students  had  been  obtained,  teachers  often  made  reading 
assignments  unaware  of  the  differences  in  the  reading  abili- 
ties of  their  students,  except  those  they  had  observed  directly. 

It  seemed  clear  that  the  basic  data  had  to  be  made  avail- 
able in  at  least  tvyo  senses.  The  information  in  the  record  it- 
self needed  to  be  made  accessible  to  the  teachers  concerned 
with  the  students.  But  since  the  process  of  getting  the  perti- 
nent facts  and  ideas  from  a  bulky  record  was  too  time  con- 
suming a  task  to  be  done  by  all  teachers  who  needed  the 
information  over  and  over  again,  some  kind  of  summary  of 
that  record  was  needed,  so  that  people  using  these  data  for 
different  purposes  could  without  difficulty  locate  what  they 
needed. 

Part  II  of  this  volume  describes  in  detail  the  work  of  the 
various  committees  on  records  and  reports  and  presents 
samples  of  the  forms  they  devised.  While  the  procedures  used 
varied  from  school  to  school,  and  while  no  one  school  in  the 
study  developed  a  fully  adequate  method  to  solve  the  prob- 
lem of  summarizing  and  circulating  data,  procedures  some- 
what like  the  following  were  adopted. 

The  teachers  most  concerned  with  a  given  objective  or 
most  immediately  involved  in  securing  the  evidence  usually 
were  responsible  for  analyzing  and  summarizing  these  re- 
sults. For  example,  the  English  teachers  usually  secured  data 
on  language  skills  and  collected  the  records  of  free  reading. 
It  was  their  first  responsibility  to  use  these  data  in  their  plan- 
ning of  the  English  program,  in  their  teaching,  and  in  their 
work  with  individual  pupils.  Hence,  it  was  logical  for  them 


ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

to  assume  the  task  of  summarizing  this  evidence  and  of  pass- 
ing these  summaries  along.  Furthermore,  they  were  expected 
to  be  mo'st  familiar  with  the  tests  relating  to  their  objectives, 
hence  they  were  usually  expected  to  give  these  tests  and  to 
summarize  the  most  pertinent  points  revealed  in  the  test 
scores.  If  some  other  members  of  the  staff,  such  as  the  psy- 
chologist, the  counselor,  or  the  evaluation  representative, 
were  responsible  for  parts  of  the  testing,  they  assumed  the 
responsibility  for  summarizing  the  results  of  the  tests  they 
gave. 

These  summaries  of  various  items  of  data  about  a  student 
were  then  brought  together  by  the  person  mainly  responsible 
for  his  guidance,  usually  his  homeroom  teacher  or  counselor. 
This  person  was  responsible  for  making  an  over-all  interpre- 
tation of  the  data,  indicating  the  outstanding  strengths  and 
weaknesses,  pointing  out  some  factors  contributing  to  these, 
and  making  some  tentative  suggestions  regarding  what 
needed  to  be  done.  Until  this  step  was  taken,  one  teacher 
knew  about  his  language  skills,  another  about  his  social  atti- 
tudes, another  about  his  techniques  of  thinking,  another 
about  his  interests,  but  no  one  had  a  coherent  picture  of  his 
development.  Hence,  few  teachers  were  in  a  position  to  make 
comprehensive  suggestions  regarding  what  the  student 
needed,  or  able  to  take  constructive  action. 

While  the  summaries  of  specific  data  were  usually  made 
at  the  time  when  the  evidence  was  secured  and  when  the  cir- 
cumstances of  securing  it  and  its  implications  were  fresh,  the 
over-all  interpretations  were  made  only  at  certain  regular  in- 
tervals or  at  times  when  such  information  was  most  needed. 
That  is,  these  interpretations  were  usually  made  at  the  times 
when  reports  to  parents  were  being  prepared  and  when  par- 
ticular curriculum  plans  were  being  made.  From  time  to  time 
the  case  of  an  individual  student  might  require  a  special  in- 
terpretation of  his  record.  The  members  of  the  staff  who 
made  these  over-all  interpretations  usually  had  some  insight 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  453 

into  the  psychological  implications  of  behavior,  some  train- 
ing in  the  interpretation  of  these  types  of  data,  and  some  per- 
sonal contact  with  the  students.  In  order  that  the  data  be 
actually  used,  it  was  found  to  be  extremely  important  that 
all  data  on  the  growth  of  a  student  pass  through  the  mind  of 
a  person  who  knew  him  and  had  a  responsible  relation  to  his 
all-round  development. 

The  schools  found  it  necessary  to  develop  plans  for  circu- 
lating information  as  well  as  summarizing  it.  Usually  the 
basic  data  collected  by  each  teacher  remained  in  his  posses- 
sion as  long  as  he  needed  it.  The  summaries  were,  however, 
circulated  as  soon  as  they  became  available.  This  was  done 
either  by  exchange  of  notes  or  by  frequent  meetings  of  small 
groups  of  teachers  and  advisers  of  each  group  of  students. 
The  latter  method  was  most  commonly  adopted  by  schools 
where  some  form  of  core  or  unified  curriculum  was  in  force, 
in  which  case  a  small  group  of  teachers  was  responsible  for  a 
major  portion  of  the  school  experiences  for  a  given  group  of 
students. 

To  facilitate  still  further  the  circulation  of  information,  the 
basic  files  were  placed  in  spots  accessible  to  teachers  and 
counselors.  If  there  was  a  school  counselors'  office,  the  files 
were  placed  there.  If  teachers  acted  as  counselors,  their  re- 
spective classrooms  or  offices  contained  those  files.  The  main 
principle  was  to  keep  the  records  of  students  where  they  were 
most  frequently  used.  Several  copies  were  made  of  data 
which  were  needed  in  different  places  or  by  different  people 
at  the  same  time.  Thus,  often  the  basic  entrance  data  were 
available  in  teachers'  or  counselors'  folders  as  well  as  in  the 
principal's  office. 

A  somewhat  different  problem  was  involved  in  handling 
group  data.  It  must  be  recalled  that  all  data  pertinent  to  in- 
dividual growth  could  also  be  summarized  so  as  to  give  evi- 
dence about  the  strengths  and  weaknesses  common  to  groups 
of  students.  These  group  summaries  were  particularly  useful 


454        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

in  appraising  the  effectiveness  of  the  curriculum.  Since  the 
summarizing  of  group  data  requires  a  certain  degree  of  statis- 
tical competence  and  since,  furthermore,  the  analysis  of  these 
data  involves  comparative  study  of  data  on  all  groups  in  the 
school,  these  tasks  were  usually  in  the  charge  of  a  person  or 
a  committee  responsible  for  coordinating  the  curriculum 
program  in  the  school.  It  was  the  responsibility  of  this  person 
or  committee  to  analyze  and  summarize  the  data  and  to  re- 
port periodically  to  the  faculty  on  the  effectiveness  of  the 
school  program  in  achieving  its  major  objectives. 

The  processes  involved  in  interpreting  group  data  have 
been  described  in  the  previous  chapter.  The  chief  administra- 
tive arrangement  required  was  to  provide  time  for  the  staff 
to  meet  together  regularly  to  study  the  data,  bringing  to  bear 
upon  it  the  specialized  competence  and  points  of  view  of  a 
representative  sample  of  all  departments  in  the  school,  and 
for  cooperative  planning  of  teaching.  This  time  was  usually 
secured  by  a  more  careful  rearrangement  of  schedules  and 
teacher  responsibilities.  A  few  schools  reduced  the  total 
teaching  period  of  the  day  by  having  students  come  half  an 
hour  later.  In  a  great  many  cases  teacher  time  was  saved  by 
teaching  students  to  work  independently  and  thus  dispensing 
with  teacher  supervision  at  some  points  of  their  work. 

I/sing  Evidence  for  Improving  Teaching  and  Curriculum 

Availability  of  evidence  alone,  no  matter  how  well  or- 
ganized and  summarized,  did  not  assure  its  effective  use.  The 
implications  of  the  individual  and  group  data  to  daily  pro- 
cedures in  guidance,  teaching,  and  curriculum  making  had  to 
be  intelligently  digested  by  every  teacher  before  the  greatest 
value  of  the  evidence  was  attained.  It  was  necessary  to  make 
special  provisions  for  teachers  to  develop  the  insight  and 
techniques  needed  to  translate  into  practice  what  was  learned 
about  the  students. 
Definitely  scheduled  opportunities  to  study  the  data  was 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  455 

one  of  these  special  provisions.  To  make  maximum  use  of 
evaluation  evidence  in  teaching  and  guiding  students  and  in 
curriculum  improvement  was  found  to  require  continuous 
study  and  collective  thinking  by  the  whole  staff.  Occasional 
reports  to  the  staff  about  the  results  of  the  evaluation  pro- 
gram proved  inadequate  for  this  purpose.  At  best  these  occa- 
sional reports  served  only  to  acquaint  the  staff  with  the  fact 
that  something  could  be  learned  from  the  evaluation  pro- 
gram. Similar  limitations  were  found  with  occasional  case 
study  conferences  regarding  individual  students.  The  occa- 
sional conferences  introduced  the  staff  to  the  techniques  of 
analyzing  evidence  about  individuals  and  suggested  some 
possible  implications,  but  they  did  not  provide  adequate 
opportunity  for  the  staff  to  explore  multiple  explanations  and 
to  consider  various  constructive  modifications  in  daily  prac- 
tices which  were  implied  by  the  evidence. 

A  second  provision  was  to  see  that  the  staff  explored  the 
evidence  and  its  implications  at  those  points  where  decisions 
were  to  be  made  and  actions  to  be  taken.  When  discussions 
of  evaluation  data  took  place  apart  from  any  need  for  action, 
they  were  often  received  by  the  staff  with  the  passivity 
usually  accorded  to  academic  discussions  and  often  regarded 
merely  as  an  interesting  theory.  In  many  cases,  what  the  staff 
seemed  most  to  need  was  a  clear  demonstration  of  the  help- 
fulness of  the  information  to  the  teachers*  ongoing  activities. 
It  was  found  to  make  an  enormous  difference  in  the  attitude 
of  the  faculty  toward  evaluation  data  whether  the  data  on 
a  given  student  were  just  "studied"  or  whether  they  were 
introduced  at  a  time  when  the  staff  was  concerned  with  such 
questions  as  what  to  do  about  certain  students'  lack  of  suc- 
cess in  academic  work  or  apparent  failure  to  adjust  to  the 
life  of  the  school.  Similarly,  when  such  questions  as  the  use- 
fulness of  Greek  history  for  the  non-academic  students  or 
the  advisability  of  social  mathematics  for  those  failing  in 
regular  mathematics  were  raised,  the  evidence  on  the  success 


456        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

or  failure  of  these  groups  in  achieving  various  objectives 
assumed  a  greater  significance.  Not  only  were  the  implica- 
tions of  available  evidence  scrutinized  more  carefully,  but 
the  possibilities  of  constructive  action  were  also  considered 
more  thoughtfully  when  the  attack  was  made  in  terms  of 
definite  problems  to  be  solved. 

There  were  several  occasions  in  the  typical  school  pro- 
cedure which  proved  to  be  particularly  appropriate  for 
demonstrating  the  usefulness  of  evaluation  data  and  for  in- 
itiating teachers  into  the  habit  of  basing  their  decisions  and 
practices  on  whatever  evidence  was  available.  Making  out 
programs  for  the  students  for  the  year  was  one  such  occasion. 
Often,  student  programs  were  decided  on  the  basis  of  such 
factors  as:  convenience  of  the  time,  college  requirements, 
previous  success  or  failure  in  various  subjects,  and  the  stu- 
dent's own  wishes.  When  a  fairly  comprehensive  set  of  evalu- 
ation data  became  available  an  attempt  was  made  to  reach 
these  decisions  in  the  light  of  all  available  data  about  the 
student.  Frequently,  also,  the  program  making  was  done 
cooperatively  by  a  faculty  group  in  charge  of  a  group  of  stu- 
dents. Such  conferences  served  not  only  as  a  means  of  ac- 
quainting the  teachers  with  what  was  in  the  "records,"  but 
also  to  clarify  and  unify  the  guidance  policies  of  the  school 
and  as  a  means  of  initiating  a  habit  of  making  decisions  of  all 
sorts  in  terms  of  evidence  rather  than  in  terms  of  previous 
practice  or  of  unconsidered  personal  preferences. 

Reports  to  parents  offered  another  occasion  to  study  the 
growth  of  students,  to  consider  their  needs,  and  to  initiate 
the  habit  of  making  judgments  in  terms  of  evidence.  Many 
teachers  had  felt  at  a  loss  in  finding  a  sufficient  number  of 
valid  things  to  say  to  each  parent  about  the  students.  Exami- 
nation of  objective  evidence  proved  to  be  very  welcome  at 
such  times. 

Most  of  the  schools  also  had  to  consider  from  time  to  time 
certain  changes  in  the  curriculum.  This  afforded  another 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  457 

occasion  for  studying  evaluation  results.  These  suggested 
changes  ranged  from  the  proposal  to  add  new  courses  to  the 
possible  reorganization  of  the  whole  structure  of  curriculum 
offerings.  These  occasions  were  an  opportune  time  to  survey 
the  effectiveness  of  the  curriculum  in  terms  of  available  evi- 
dence. Several  of  the  Thirty  Schools  began  with  occasional 
staff  meetings  considering  such  problems.  They  proved  so 
useful  that  curriculum  planning  sessions  held  regularly 
through  the  year  became  a  frequent  practice.  Many  schools 
held  prolonged  sessions  either  in  the  spring  after  the  school 
was  out  or  in  the  fall  before  the  year's  program  was  begun. 
At  this  time  the  information  about  the  growth  of  students 
toward  all  objectives  of  the  school  was  carefully  examined 
by  the  whole  staff,  and  the  curriculum  plans  as  well  as  plans 
for  teaching  and  special  activities  to  be  promoted  were  made 
in  terms  of  that  evidence.  Weekly  conferences  throughout  the 
school  by  smaller  groups  of  teachers  dealing  with  the  same 
group  of  students  were  also  a  frequent  practice. 

A  third  provision  was  to  involve  the  entire  school  staff  in 
the  study  of  the  results  of  evaluation.  Often  consideration  of 
the  implications  of  the  evaluation  evidence  suggested  changes 
in  practices  which  were  not  under  the  direct  control  of  any 
one  member  nor  any  small  portion  of  the  staff.  For  example, 
in  many  cases  the  sources  of  difficulties  in  achieving  con- 
sistent democratic  attitudes  appeared  to  be  in  the  whole  or- 
ganization of  the  school,  the  weaknesses  in  clear  thinking 
were  apparently  produced  by  an  inconsistent  approach 
among  the  different  teachers,  and  adjustment  problems  could 
largely  be  traced  to  the  way  in  which  the  program  of  student 
activities  was  organized.  To  uncover  difficulties  of  this  sort 
and  to  plan  constructive  remedies,  it  was  necessary  to  take 
the  whole  staff  into  partnership  in  considering  and  formulat- 
ing school  policies  and  in  examining  evidence  helpful  in 
making  wiser  decisions. 

As  the  evaluation  program  proceeded,  it  became  increas- 


458        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

ingly  clear  that  to  be  effective  it  must  involve  extensive  par- 
ticipation by  the  entire  faculty.  Teachers  had  to  formulate 
objectives  and  to  agree  on  the  common  objectives  of  the 
school.  They  had  to  select  certain  manifestations  of  growth 
toward  these  objectives  which  could  be  tested,  observed,  or 
recorded.  While  in  the  choice  of  instruments  technical  ad- 
vice was  needed,  the  final  decision  regarding  their  appropri- 
ateness rested  with  the  teachers.  Similarly,  the  final  decisions 
regarding  what  was  significant  in  the  evaluation  data  and 
how  they  could  be  used  in  improving  school  practices  could 
wisely  be  made  only  by  those  who  were  carrying  on  the  job. 
When  judgments  and  decisions  of  this  sort  were  made  by 
"experts"  and  passed  on  to  the  teachers,  the  results  were  less 
fruitful.  . 

A  program  which  involved  wide  participation  naturally 
raised  the  question  of  the  competence  of  the  rank  and  file 
of  teachers  in  such  matters.  Thus,  for  instance,  the  ability  of 
teachers  to  interpret  properly  evaluation  data,  particularly 
those  requiring  psychological  insight  or  technical  manipula- 
tion, was  questioned.  The  usual  assumption,  for  example,  was 
that  only  statistically  trained  people  could  be  trusted  to  deal 
with  test  scores.  The  experience  in  the  Thirty  Schools  was 
that  on  the  whole  teachers  made  better  interpreters  than 
persons  statistically  qualified  but  whose  personal  contact 
with  students  was  limited.  Moreover,  since  it  seemed  evident 
that  unless  teachers  were  trained  to  interpret  evaluation  data 
for  themselves,  their  ability  and  insight  in  using  the  results 
as  well  as  their  willingness  to  do  so  would  remain  limited, 
the  schools  in  cooperation  with  the  Evaluation  Staff  em- 
barked on  the  job  of  training  the  teachers  for  this  work. 
Periodic  conferences  on  interpretation  were  held  in  each 
school.  To  provide  for  more  continued  help,  in  each  school 
one  person  was  chosen  to  act  as  an  evaluation  representative 
and  as  an  evaluation  adviser  to  the  rest  of  the  school.  This 
person  spent  somd  time  receiving  training  in  interpretation 


APPRAISING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  459 

either  in  workshops  during  the  summer  or  with  the  Evalua- 
tion Staff  during  the  school  year. 

Similarly,  the  use  of  evaluation  data  in  shaping  an  im- 
proved school  program  could  not  be  left  to  accidental  or 
amateurish  efforts.  Some  training  and  guidance  of  teachers 
was  needed.  This  did  not  mean  that  all  teachers  were  packed 
off  to  summer  school  to  receive  such  training.  Participating  in 
planning  and  administering  the  evaluation  program  and  in 
the  study  and  application  of  its  results  in  itself  provided  an 
opportunity  for  training  hardly  exceeded  by  any  other  de- 
vice, provided  there  were  opportunities  in  the  school  for  the 
staff  to  think  together  on  these  matters  and  to  make  coopera- 
tively decisions  which  had  previously  been  made  by  in- 
dividuals. 

This  brief  report  on  the  planning  and  administration  of  an 
evaluation  program  provides  a  further  illustration  of  the 
ways  in  which  the  evaluation  project  was  an  integral  part 
of  the  processes  of  teaching,  of  curriculum  making,  of  guid- 
ance, and  of  teacher  education  in  many  of  the  Thirty  Schools. 
As  a  result  of  its  work  with  the  schools,  the  Evaluation  Staff  is 
convinced  that  a  program  of  evaluation  can  achieve  its  maxi- 
mum usefulness  only  when  it  is  an  integral  part  of  the  major 
tasks  of  the  school.  Deriving  its  direction  from  the  major 
objectives  of  the  school,  the  evaluation  program  helps  to 
clarify  these  objectives  into  clearly  apprehended  goals  and 
purposes  which  are  more  effective  guides  to  teaching  and 
counseling.  Exploring  each  major  objective  to  identify  types 
of  behavior  manifestations  which  will  serve  to  reveal  the 
progress  of  students  toward  this  objective  helps  to  focus  at- 
tention upon  the  learner  and  the  meaning  of  the  educative 
process  to  him.  Studying  the  results  of  evaluation  serves  to 
identify  strengths  and  weaknesses  of  teaching  and  inade- 
quacies in  the  school  program.  Effective  participation  in 
these  several  phases  of  evaluation  serves  as  a  stimulating  ex- 
perience for  teachers  in  their  own  continuing  education. 


PART  II 

RECORDING  FOR  GUIDANCE  AND  TRANSFER 


The  work  of  the  Committees  on  Records 

and  Reports,  and  the  Forms  produced 

by  them. 


Chapter  IX 
PHILOSOPHY  AND  OBJECTIVES 

XXX— XXX.  /XA,  XXXL  fS_f./j'S-f/'f.fSifm_f/S~f/fm-fsr_fm_f/'fm /x/'  xxx  xx/-  /vx  XXX-.  xxx.  XXXr  xxx1  xxx.  xxx  xxx  xxx 
Wv^vxx  xxV  xvv  vw  >xx~ wv  wv  wvxxv"  wv~ vxx~  vwv wvxw  wv  v\x "Vxx Vx\ *  xv\xx> ,  xxx rxxxxxx 

The  Foreword  and  the  Preface  explain  the  relation  of  this 
work  to  the  general  undertaking,  including  the  original  or- 
ganization of  this  department  and  the  way  in  which  it  later 
became  a  part  of  the  work  carried  on  under  the  direction  of 
the  Committee  on  Evaluation  and  Recording.  In  addition  to 
the  Committee  on  Behavior  Description,  there  were  organ- 
ized working  committees  for  the  preparation  of  progress 
forms  in  each  of  several  subject  fields,  forms  for  use  in 
transfer  from  school  to  college,  and  forms  to  be  used  in 
reporting  to  the  home.  Because  the  American  Council  on 
Education  had  a  cumulative  record  card  that  was  soon  to 
be  revised,  no  committee  was  appointed  to  work  on  this 
type  of  form,  although  it  was  needed  to  complete  the  set. 
The  revision  has  now  been  made  and  the  new  form  is  de- 
scribed in  this  report. 

Of  special  significance  in  consideration  of  the  material  in 
this  book  is  the  community  of  interest  and  acceptance  of 
common  philosophic  bases  for  work  that  characterized  the 
different  groups  that  are  responsible  for  it.  As  a  matter  of 
fact,  there  was  throughout  the  study  a  considerable  amount 
of  overlapping  membership,  so  that  not  only  members  of 
the  staff  but  other  individuals  worked  on  committees  for 
evaluation  and  ones  for  recording,  or  on  committees  devising 
record  forms  for  two  different,  although  related,  purposes. 
This  common  membership  helped  the  effort  that  was  made 
to  avoid  unnecessary  duplication  or  conflict  between  those 
responsible  for  evaluation  and  those  working  on  recording. 
Some  problems  were,  of  necessity,  attacked  from  both  angles, 
but  with  advantage  rather  than  waste  of  time.  Various 

463 


464        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

groups,  for  example,  studied  the  objectives  of  teachers  and 
schools,  but  always  in  relation  to  particular  problems,  and 
always  with  the  results  obtained  by  other  groups  available 
for  comparison  and  use.  The  list  of  objectives  prepared  by 
the  Evaluation  Staff  was  particularly  helpful  to  all  the  com- 
mittees on  recording  and  reporting. 

All  record  forms  that  can  do  so  provide  space  for  informa- 
tion of  the  kinds  obtained  by  the  Evaluation  Staff,  so  that 
this  can  be  related  to  the  other  data  and  so  can  help  to 
make  a  more  complete  description  of  the  pupil. 

Although  it  will  be  said  again  in  relation  to  various  forms 
and  their  use,  it  must  be  emphasized  here  that  no  single 
result  of  evaluation  procedures  or  of  observations  recorded 
on  the  forms  is  considered  to  be  independent  of  other  in- 
formation about  a  pupil.  All  the  information  obtained,  as 
would  be  true  if  he  were  studied  by  a  psvchologist  or 
psycho-analyst,  contributes  to  the  more  complete  understand- 
ing of  him  that  becomes  the  basis  for  the  school's  dealing 
with  him. 

Philosophy  and  Objectives 

The  original  Committee  on  Reports  and  Records  consid- 
ered with  great  care  former  methods  of  recording  facts  about 
personal  characteristics  or  traits,  and  the  words  used  in  de- 
scribing and  reporting  about  them. 

Out  of  this  study  and  the  discussion  of  the  problems  fac- 
ing the  committee  came  the  philosophy  and  objectives  that 
governed  the  later  work.  The  list  of  objectives  in  explicit  or 
implicit  form  was  reexamined  by  the  other  committees,  and 
was  generally  accepted  as  a  guide,  though  it  was  realized 
that  some  of  it  applied  most  completely  to  the  study  of  per- 
sonal characteristics. 

GENERAL  PURPOSES  AND  PHILOSOPHY  OF  RECORDING 

1.  (a)  The  purpose  of  recording  is  not  primarily  that  of 
bookkeeping.  Instead  the  fundamental  reason  for  records  is 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  465 

their  value  as  a  basis  for  more  intelligent  dealing  with 
human  beings. 

The  first  purpose  of  records  is  therefore  that  of  form- 
ing a  basis  for  understanding  individuals  so  that  effective 
guidance  can  be  given. 

(b)  Since  the  educational  process  is  a  continuous  one  that 
should  not  be  set  back  at  certain  transfer  points,  it  becomes 
necessary  that  guidance  shall  continue  across  such  points 
in  such  a  way  as  to  increase  the  probability  of  continuity 
in  dealing  with  the  person. 

An  extended  purpose  of  records  hence  becomes  that  of 
furnishing  transferable  information  for  guidance. 

(c)  Because  of  the  need  of  cooperative  and  consistent 
dealing  with  a  boy  or  girl  by  home  and  school,  as  well  as 
the  right  of  the  home  to  information  as  complete  and  reli- 
able as  possible  about  progress  and  development,  records 
should  furnish  the  material  on  which  reports  can  be  founded, 
and  reports  should  be  considered  an  essential  and  consistent 
part  of  the  recording  system. 

A  third  purpose  of  record  keeping  is  therefore  to  provide 
the  information  needed  for  reports  to  the  home,  and  to  add 
effective  ways  of  giving  such  information. 

(d)  Information  is  needed  at  all  stages  of  education,  and 
particularly  at  points  of  transfer  from  one  institution  to  an- 
other, or  from  an  institution  to  employment,  in  order  that 
qualifications  of  the  individual  for  the  new  experience  can 
be  fairly  judged. 

A  fourth  purpose  of  record  keeping  is  therefore  to  pro- 
vide information,  and  methods  of  transferring  it  to  others, 
that  will  give  evidence  regarding  a  pupil's  readiness  for  suc- 
ceeding experiences.  This  would  apply  to  fitness  for  a  par- 
ticular college  or  other  institution. 

2.  What  might  be  considered  an  indirect  but  nevertheless 
important  purpose  of  records  is  that  of  stimulating  teachers 
to  consider  and  decide  upon  their  objectives,  judge  some- 


466        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

thing  of  the  relative  importance  of  their  aims,  and  estimate 
their  own  work  and  the  progress  of  their  pupils  in  relation 
to  the  objectives  chosen. 

Many  teachers  think  almost  entirely  in  terms  of  the  most 
obvious  objectives  concerned  with  the  learning  of  subject 
matter  and  evaluate  their  results  only  in  terms  of  such  aims. 
They  give  little  or  no  consideration  to  the  changes  in  their 
pupils  that  should  come  about  as  a  result  of  the  experiences 
undergone,  and  so  they  fail  to  bring  about  the  development 
that  is  possible.  Through  well  planned  records  they  can  be 
helped  to  a  wider  vision  and  a  more  constructive  influence. 

It  is  evident  that  the  most  valuable  and  complete  record 
that  could  be  made  by  observation  of  an  individual  would 
consist  of  a  record  of  his  behavior  throughout  life,  or  that 
portion  of  it  under  observation.  It  is  believed  that  any  ob- 
servational technique  that  has  value  must  consist  in  using 
the  parts  of  such  a  record  that  can  be  collected  and  arranged 
in  the  time  at  a  teacher's  disposal.  This  can  be  done  by  re- 
cording significant  incidents  of  behavior  and  interpretations 
of  them  (the  "anecdotal"  method),  by  characterizing  in  one 
way  or  another  the  kinds  of  behavior  observed  (sometimes 
called  "behavior  description"),  or  by  a  combination  of  char- 
acterization and  of  supplementary  analysis  in  paragraph 
form. 

Where  a  teacher  deals  with  a  small  number  of  pupils,  or 
carries  a  light  schedule,  the  recording  of  extensive  anecdotal 
material  seems  possible  and  highly  valuable.  Some  institu- 
tions and  teachers  use  such  a  method  even  when  the  written 
material  cannot  be  extensive.  The  more  the  demands  on  the 
teacher  through  appointments  or  pupil  load,  the  less  is  it 
possible  to  write  voluminously,  and  the  more  does  it  seem 
necessary  for  each  instructor  to  digest  his  observations  into 
quickly  recorded  (but  not  too  quickly  arrived  at)  judgments 
about  the  typical  behavior  of  the  pupils.  No  "checking"  sys- 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  467 

tern,  however,  can  fit  all  of  the  significant  differences  among 
people,  no  matter  how  well  it  is  devised,  so  such  a  system 
must  allow  for  supplementary  notes  that  modify  or  add  com- 
pleteness to  a  description. 

As  this  committee  was  trying  to  devise  a  method  and 
blanks  for  recording  facts  about  a  pupil  in  abbreviated  form, 
it  was  necessary  to  agree  upon  working  objectives  for  pro- 
ducing the  kind  of  forms  that  would  serve  the  purposes  de- 
sired. The  following  objectives  were  used. 

WORKING  OBJECTIVES  FOR  RECORDS  AND  REPORTS 

1.  Any  form  devised  should  be  based  on  the  objectives  of 
teachers  and  schools  so  that  a  continuing  study  of  a  pupil 
by  its  use  will  throw  light  on  his  successive  stages  of  devel- 
opment in  powers  or  characteristics  believed  to  be  important. 

2.  The  forms  dealing  with  personal  characteristics  should 
be  descriptive  rather  than  of  the  nature  of  a  scale.  Therefore 
"marks"  of  any  kind,  or  placement,  as  on  a  straight  line 
representing  a  scale  from  highest  to  lowest,  should  not  be 
used. 

3.  Every  effort  should  be  made  to  reach  agreement  about 
the  meaning  of  trait  names  used,  and  to  make  their  signifi- 
cance in  terms  of  the  behavior  of  a  pupil  understood  by  those 
reading  the  record. 

4.  Wherever  possible  a  characterization  of  a  person  should 
be  by  description  of  typical  behavior  rather  than  by  a  word 
or  phrase  that  could  have  widely  different  meanings  to  dif- 
ferent people. 

5.  The  forms  should  be  flexible  enough  to  allow  choice 
of  headings  under  which  studies  of  pupils  can  be  made,  thus 
allowing  a  school,  department,  or  teacher  to  use  the  objec- 
tives considered  important  in  the  particular  situation,  or  for 
the  particular  pupil. 

6.  Characteristics  studied  should  be  such  that  teachers  will 


468        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

be  likely  to  have  opportunities  to  observe  behavior  that  gives 
evidence  about  them.  It  is  not  expected,  however,  that  all 
teachers  will  have  evidence  about  all  characteristics. 

7.  Forms  should  be  so  devised  and  related  that  any  school 
will  be  likely  to  be  able  to  use  them  without  an  overwhelm- 
ing addition  to  the  work  of  teachers  or  secretaries. 

8.  Characteristics  studied  should  be  regarded  not  as  inde- 
pendent entities  but  rather  as  facets  of  behavior  shown  by 
a  living  human  being  in  his  relations  with  his  environment. 

This  last  objective  is  a  fundamental  one.  It  has  been  ob- 
served in  the  work  on  both  evaluation  and  recording,  and 
must  be  kept  in  mind  in  considering  whatever  has  been  pro- 
duced. The  one  great  danger  in  the  use  of  any  forms  that 
offer  opportunity  for  recording  facts  about  people  is  that 
those  who  use  them  may  revert  to  the  idea  of  "marking," 
using  the  material  on  the  forms  as  a  scale  for  rating,  instead 
of  as  an  abbreviated  basis  for  description  of  the  person's  be- 
havior in  some  area  or  under  some  conditions.  The  various 
record  forms  too  should  be  considered  as  supplementing 
each  other  so  as  to  give  a  more  complete  description  of  the 
individual  than  a  single  form  could  present. 

It  should  be  emphasized  that  no  form  produced  in  this 
study  is  believed  to  be  final,  or  to  be  the  only  kind  of  form 
for  its  purpose.  Because  of  the  generosity  of  the  contribut- 
ing foundations  and  the  willingness  of  the  committee  mem- 
bers to  give  their  time  and  effort,  a  more  extensive  and  in- 
tensive study  of  recording  has  been  made  than  had  been 
possible  before.  There  is  reason  to  hope,  therefore,  that  these 
forms  may  prove  suitable  for  many  institutions,  particularly 
in  view  of  their  wide  flexibility.  For  other  institutions  they 
may  need  modification,  for  still  others  they  may  prove  sug- 
gestive in  detail  or  principles.  In  any  case  the  committees 
concerned  hope  the  objectives  and  the  material  developed 
will  prove  worthy  of  study  and  trial,  though  the  members 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  469 

are  far  from  being  dogmatic  about  the  form  or  content  of 
what  is  now  offered. 

While  that  which  has  been  done  by  these  committees 
represents  the  most  organized  work  accomplished  in  record- 
ing and  reporting,  since  it  involves  the  cooperation  of  those 
in  many  colleges  and  schools,  the  achievements  of  various 
of  the  cooperating  schools  working  individually  in  devising 
forms  to  fit  their  own  particular  needs  also  deserves  mention. 
Committees  of  faculty  members  studied  the  conditions  and 
needs  of  their  institutions  and  arrived  at  interesting  and 
valuable  methods  of  collecting  and  recording  information 
about  their  pupils. 

It  is  obviously  impossible  to  reproduce  and  discuss  the 
forms  produced  by  such  efforts,  but  other  schools  may  profit 
by  consultation  with  cooperating  schools  whose  problems 
seem  similar  to  their  own. 


Chapter  X 

BEHAVIOR  DESCRIPTION 


Much  of  the  foregoing  philosophy  was  developed  while  the 
Committee  on  Records  and  Reports1  was  making  a  prelimi- 
nary study  of  its  first  record-making  assignment,  which  con- 
cerned the  study  of  personal  characteristics.  This  study 
began  with  exploration  of  what  had  previously  been  done 
in  this  field.  The  committee  found  many  attempts  to  clarify 
and  organize  the  study  of  human  beings,  with  little  agree- 
ment on  the  terms  used  or  the  methods  employed.  From  the 
great  number  of  people-describing  words  in  the  language, 
however,  certain  ones  had  attained  somewhat  common 
usage.  The  first  survey  of  terms  used  by  various  agencies  to 
describe  people  produced  over  150  terms,  and  a  later  study 
made  by  Dr.  Rothney  listed  over  260  trait  names. 

All  of  these  words  were  considered  and  compared.  It  was 
found  that  they  fell  into  sets,  each  set  composed  of  words 
having  somewhat  the  same  meaning,  so  that  the  number  of 
markedly  distinct  characteristics  was  only  a  fraction  of  the 
number  of  names  of  traits.  Each  set  was  considered  by  itself 

1  COMMITTEE  ON  BEHAVIOR  DESCRIPTION.  (Members  and  those  added 
during  the  work.  Institutional  affiliations  are  those  for  the  time  of  appoint- 
ment.) Miss  Helen  M.  Atkinson,  Horace  Mann  School  for  Girls;  E.  Gordon 
Bill,  Dartmouth  College;  Carl  Brigham,  Princeton  University;  Oscar  K. 
Euros,  Research  Assistant  1933-35,  Rutgers  College;  Mrs.  Cecile  Fleming, 
Horace  Mann  School;  Mrs.  Anne  Rose  Hawkes,  The  Carnegie  Foundation; 
Miss  Frances  Knapp  (deceased),  Wellesley  College;  Robert  D.  Leigh, 
Bennington  College;  William  S.  Learned,  The  Carnegie  Foundation;  John 
Lester,  The  Hill  School;  Rollo  Reynolds,  Horace  Mann  School  for  Girls; 
Eugene  R.  Smith,  Chairman,  The  Beaver  Country  Day  School;  John  Tild- 
sley,  Associate  Superintendent  of  Schools,  New  York  City;  Ben  Wood,  Co- 
operative Test  Service;  Stanley  R.  Yarnall,  Germantown  Friends  School; 
John  W.  M.  Rothney,  Research  Assistant,  Secretary,  Harvard  University. 

470 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  471 

until  the  committee  members  agreed  on  the  term  or  terms 
that  best  expressed  its  fundamental  meaning.  From  the  re- 
sulting list  of  key  words  the  first  group  of  characteristics  was 
chosen  for  further  study. 

The  criteria  for  choosing  the  characteristics  to  be  used 
were: 

1.  Importance.  The  ones  chosen  should  be  worth  observ- 
ing because  they  throw  light  on  the  person  being  studied. 

2.  Observability.  They  should  be  such  that  some  at  least 
of  a  pupil's  teachers  will  have  opportunity  to  observe  sig- 
nificant behavior  in  relation  to  them. 

3.  Completeness.  Taken  together  they  should  give  a  rea- 
sonably complete  picture  of  the  person  as  seen  by  the  adults 
dealing  with  him. 

4.  Differentness.  They  should  be  sufficiently  independent 
so  that  teachers  can  distinguish  between  them  and  so  that 
intercorrelations  will  not  be  too  high. 

From  the  beginning,  the  members  of  the  committee  were 
agreed  that  the  evidence  from  research  did  not  justify  a 
method  of  rating,  or  any  type  of  scale  for  judging  personal 
characteristics,  such,  for  example,  as  one  constructed  along 
a  straight  line,  or  one  composed  of  named  points  with  sup- 
posedly equal  intervals  between  them.  It  questioned  the  use 
of  undefined  terms  for  designating  degrees  of  excellence  or 
lack  of  it,  and  believed  that  it  was  unlikely  that  intervals  on 
a  line  or  other  scale  had  any  accuracy  in  terms  of  their  rela- 
tive size  or  importance. 

Furthermore  the  committee  was  not  much  interested  in 
a  scale  even  if  it  could  have  been  constructed.  It  hoped 
rather  for  something  that  would  encourage  and  help  teachers 
to  observe  and  analyze  behavior  and  from  the  evidence  ob- 
tained to  reach  a  better  understanding  of  their  pupils  as  liv- 
ing functioning  human  beings. 

The  members,  as  has  been  shown,  were  definite  in  their 
desire  to  eliminate  comparisons  except  as  they  were  implicit 


472.        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

in  any  descriptive  material.  They  therefore  set  as  their  goal 
a  form  that: 

1.  at  the  time  of  a  single  use  of  it,  would,  through  de- 
scriptions of  behavior,  present  a  picture  of  a  person 
not  only  in  terms  of  his  commonest  (modal)   be- 
havior, but  also  in  terms  of  the  range  and  variety 
of  his  behavior  under  different  conditions; 

2.  over  a  period  of  time  would,  through  a  series  of 
studies  and  recordings,  constitute  a  record  of  devel- 
opment in  significant  characteristics. 

It  would  be  difficult  for  any  one  who  had  not  worked  on 
such  an  undertaking  to  realize  the  difficulties  encountered. 
The  members  of  the  committee  covered  a  wide  range  of  ex- 
perience and  specialization  that  naturally  influenced  their 
ideas  of  the  work  of  the  committee  and  their  conceptions  of 
the  use  and  meanings  of  certain  terms.  Some,  at  the  begin- 
ning, were  even  skeptical  about  the  possibility  of  working 
out  anything  of  value.  Frequently  hours  had  to  be  spent  in 
the  discussion  of  the  meanings  of  a  few  words  whose  use 
seemed  necessary.  Little  by  little,  however,  techniques  of 
work  developed  and  language  difficulties  became  fewer.  The 
final  form  includes  only  material  on  which  the  committee 
reached  substantial  agreement. 

The  committee's  first  achievement  was  the  choice  of  a 
group  of  characteristics  and  the  development  of  blanks  for 
recording  behavior  in  terms  of  them.  A  manual  for  teachers 
was  also  written.  The  cooperating  schools  were  asked  to 
study  groups  of  pupils  by  the  use  of  the  forms  and  manual, 
to  send  the  completed  blanks  to  the  committee,  and  to  make 
suggestions  for  revisions.  Mr.  Oscar  K.  Euros  of  the  research 
staff  and  an  assistant  studied  the  results  and  from  their 
analysis  made  suggestions  for  changes.  A  blank  and  a  man- 
ual incorporating  the  revisions  decided  on  was  next  pre- 
pared. After  rather  a  large  number  of  pupils  had  been  studied 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  473 

the  intercorrelations  among  descriptions  were  worked  out  at 
Columbia  University  under  the  direction  of  Dr.  Ben  Wood. 
The  figures  showed  that  either  some  names  of  traits  con- 
veyed too  nearly  the  same  meaning  to  teachers  despite  the 
committee's  attempt  to  differentiate  their  meanings,  or  else 
certain  characteristics  were  so  closely  related  that  they 
tended  to  appear  in  similar  ways  in  many  situations.  In  either 
case  the  aims  for  the  undertaking  were  not  being  achieved. 

The  committee  made  the  changes  indicated.  It  also  added 
to  the  scope  of  the  information  asked  for,  since  some  valu- 
able facts  seemed  to  be  omitted,  and  rewrote  and  enlarged 
the  manual.  Further  trial,  experimentation  and  testing  re- 
sulted in  still  further  changes,  though  with  less  radical  al- 
terations in  later  steps.  Eventually  a  considerable  body  of 
material  was  again  submitted  to  correlation  study,  this  time 
at  Harvard  University  by  Dr.  Rothney.  This  study  found 
that  the  characteristics  were  sufficiently  different  and  the 
judgments  of  the  teachers  sufficiently  well  made  so  that  the 
reports  were  significant  descriptions  of  the  pupils. 

Even  after  this  the  committee  again  called  for  criticisms 
and  suggestions  from  the  schools  and  tried  to  refine  its  work. 
It  is  hoped  that  it  is  now  in  such  form  that  it  will  have  value 
to  schools  in  general,  and  perhaps  to  more  advanced  institu- 
tions.2 

It  will  be  clear  from  the  material  itself  that  the  method 
of  studying  pupils  devised  by  the  committee  depends  on 
the  supplying  of  descriptions  of  the  different  kinds  of  be- 
havior that  are  likely  to  be  observed  in  relation  to  the  char- 
acteristics chosen.  The  descriptions  made  by  the  committee 

2  The  form,  modified  in  its  text  material  only  by  the  addition  of  two 
characteristics  and  two  additional  questions  was  used  by  Dartmouth  Col- 
lege in  "The  Dartmouth  Visual  Survey  of  The  Dartmouth  Eye  Institute." 
It  is  said  to  have  served  the  purpose  of  the  study  successfully.  The  cards 
have  also  been  used  to  study  the  students  in  a  college  dormitory.  It  is 
likely  that  a  form  planned  especially  for  college  use  will  eventually  be 
published. 


474        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

are  designed  to  define  what  might  be  called  types  or  classifi- 
cations of  behavior  in  terms  of  each  characteristic.  The  use 
of  carefully  worded  standard  definitions  in  place  of  teachers' 
own  wordings  is  intended  to  bring  about  a  more  nearly  com- 
mon understanding  of  the  characteristics  themselves  and  of 
the  persons  described.  The  form  resulting  also  decreases 
greatly  the  time  required  for  recording  and  for  using  the 
record  for  purposes  of  interview  or  transfer. 

In  general,  all  teachers  having  opportunity  to  know  a 
pupil  would  be  expected  to  describe  him  by  the  use  of  this 
material.  The  combined  reports,  which  would  appear  on  the 
Behavior  Description  card,  would  show  the  pupil's  most 
common  behavior,  as  well  as  the  range  of  behavior  under 
different  conditions. 

It  is  recommended  that  the  descriptions  be  recorded  twice 
a  year  through  the  six  years  of  junior  and  senior  high  school. 
To  the  degree  that  the  information  covers  this  period  the 
card  becomes  a  record  not  only  of  what  a  pupil  is  like  at 
any  one  time  but  of  his  many-sided  development  through 
this  period  of  his  growth. 

USE  OF  RECORD  CARDS 

To  show  the  manner  in  which  the  classifications  are  con- 
sidered and  used  in  school  practice  the  entire  section  on 
"Creativeness  and  Imagination"  is  quoted  here  from  the 
manual. 

CREATIVENESS  AND  IMAGINATION 

NOTE:  The  question  whether  what  is  created  has  been  created 
before  does  not  enter  into  this  discussion.  Newness  to  the  person 
in  question,  and  the  extent  of  the  contribution  he  himself  makes, 
determine  the  amount  of  creativeness  shown.  Creation  includes 
not  only  originating  entirely,  but  also  recombining  old  elements 
and  seeing  new  relationships.  Some  characteristics  that  tend 
toward  creativeness  are: 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  475 

the  desire  and  habit  of  trying  new  things,  of  putting  things 
together  in  new  combinations  (experimentation), 

the  ability  to  think  new  things,  an  art  form,  a  melody,  a  new 
concept,  a  new  situation  (imagination), 

the  ability  to  organize,  direct  or  control  new  combinations 
of  people  or  things  (executive  manipulation). 

TYPE  IA.  General:  those  who  approach  whatever  they  do  with 
active  imagination  and  originality,  so  tJiat  they  contribute  some- 
thing that  is  their  own. 

TYPE  IB.  Specific:  those  who  make  distinctly  original  and  signifi- 
cant contributions  in  one  or  more  fields. 

Discussion:  For  secondary  school  pupils  this  might  occur  in 
writing,  the  fine  or  applied  arts,  music,  drama,  or  research  in 
scientific  or  other  fields. 

Examples:  One  may  show  the  possession  of  this  trait  by: 

1.  Expressing  one's   emotions   and  thoughts  through  such 
media  as  language,  arts  and  crafts,  music,   or  drama. 
This  might  result  in  the  writing  of  poems,  stories  or  essays, 
in  the  conception  and  execution  of  pictures,  statues,  cos- 
tumes, or  stage  sets,  or  in  one  or  more  of  various  other 
such  expressions. 

2.  So  expressing  an  old  idea  that  it  is  reinterpreted  through 
a  new  viewpoint  or  a  different  organization  of  material. 

3.  Using  logical  processes  with  such  imagination  that  he  sees 
implications  and  relationships  that  open  new  fields  of 
thought  or  throw  light  on  old  ones. 

4.  Bringing  to  the  planning  and  activities  of  the  day  think- 
ing and  action  that  result  in  improved  procedures.  This 
might  appear  in  the  formulation  and  carrying  out  of  a 
procedure  for  study  investigation,  the  accomplishment  of 
a  task,  or  the  manipulation  of  a  group. 

5.  So  completely  projecting  oneself  into  a  situation  that  it 
becomes  his  own.  One  can  listen  creatively  to  a  sym- 
phony, or  can  interpret  with  originality  the  one  whose 
part  he  plays  in  dramatization. 


476        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

6.  Combining  elements  (as  in  an  invention)  to  produce  a 
new  result  or  improve  a  procedure. 

TYPE  n.  Promising:  those  who  show  a  degree  of  creativeness  that 
indicates  the  likelihood  of  valuable  original  contribution  in  some 
field,  although  the  contributions  already  made  have  not  proved 
to  be  particularly  significant. 

Discussion:  This  includes  those  who  show  imagination  and  ap- 
proach their  problems  creatively,  although — perhaps  because  of 
lack  of  experience  or  of  opportunity  in  the  fields  in  which  they 
will  eventually  contribute — they  have  as  yet  shown  indications 
rather  than  demonstrated  accomplishments. 
TYPE  ra.  Limited:  those  whose  general  attitude  shows  the  desire 
to  contribute  their  own  thinking  and  expression  to  situations,  but 
whose  degree  of  imagination  and  originality  is  not  in  general  high 
enough  to  have  much  influence  on  their  accomplishments. 

Discussion:  A  person  of  this  type  may  make  occasional  con- 
tributions of  some  general  value  where  particular  experience  or 
other  favorable  influences  make  this  possible,  or  may  from  time 
to  time  show  originality  in  details  rather  than  in  general  situa- 
tions. 

TYPE  iv.  Imitative:  those  who,  while  they  make  little  or  no  crea- 
tive contributions  themselves,  yet  show  sufficient  imagination  to 
see  the  implications  in  the  creation  of  others  and  to  make  use  of 
their  ideas  or  accomplishments. 

TYPE  v.  Unimaginative:  those  who  have  given  practically  no  evi- 
dence of  originality  or  creativeness  in  imagination  or  action. 

The  "Type"  numbers  are  used  for  convenience  in  referring 
to  the  descriptions  of  different  kinds  of  behavior,  and  the 
words  "General,"  "Specific,"  and  so  forth  are  key  words  de- 
fining each  type  of  behavior  as  well  as  one  or  two  words 
could  be  found  to  do  it. 

Under  some  characteristics  two  types  keep  the  same  num- 
ber but  with  letters  added,  as  in  IA  and  IB  in  this  case. 
This  occurs  where  the  committee  wishes  to  indicate  two  re- 
lated types  of  behavior  that  differ  only  in  the  way  in  which 
the  individual  uses,  or  is  limited  in  the  use  of,  the  character- 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  477 

istic  in  question.  Both  IA  and  IB  in  this  example  indicate  a 
highly  creative  approach  to  problems  on  the  part  of  those 
they  describe,  but  of  two  listed  under  these  definitions  the 
one  under  IA  might  be  thought  of  as  applying  his  creative 
ability  more  extensively,  while  the  one  described  by  IB 
would  respond  less  generally,  but  quite  possibly  with  equal 
or  greater  intensity,  to  the  particular  stimuli  that  do  arouse 
his  creativeness. 

The  Behavior  Description  card,  because  of  its  size,  which 
is  that  of  a  filing  envelope  for  an  8/2"  by  11"  file,  cannot 
easily  be  shown  in  this  volume.  It  is  possible  however  to  de- 
scribe what  is  most  significant  about  it.  It  consists  of: 

1.  A  listing  of  characteristics  and  the  descriptive  clas- 
sifications under  them. 

2.  Spaces  opposite  the  classifications  that  make  it  pos- 
sible to  include  on  the  card  the  study  of  a  pupil 
over  the  six  years  of  junior  high  school  and  senior 
high  school,  or  over  the  seventh  and  eighth  grades 
and  the  four  year  secondary  school. 

3.  A  key  system  for  use  in  recording  the  judgments  of 
teachers.  This  will  be  illustrated  later  under  "Respon- 
sibility-Dependability." 

4  A  considerable  space  for  "General  Comment." 

The  entire  list  of  characteristics  that  use  defined  descrip- 
tions of  types  of  behavior  follows  as  it  appears  on  the  filing 
card. 

RESPONSIBILITY-DEPENDABILITY 

Type 

RESPONSIBLE  AND  RESOURCEFUL:  Carries  through  whatever  is 
undertaken,  and  also  shows  initiative  and  versatility  in 
accomplishing  and  enlarging  upon  undertakings.  1 

CONSCIENTIOUS:  Completes  without  external  compulsion 
whatever  is  assigned  but  is  unlikely  to  enlarge  the  scope 
of  assignments.  2 


478        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Type 

GENERALLY  DEPENDABLE:  Usually  carries  through  undertak- 
ings, self -assumed  or  assigned  by  others,  requiring  only 
occasional  reminder  or  compulsion.  3 A 

SELECTIVELY  DEPENDABLE:  Shows  high  persistence  in  under- 
takings in  which  there  is  particular  interest,  but  is  less 
likely  to  carry  through  other  assignments.  3B 

UNRELIABLE:  Can  be  relied  upon  to  complete  undertakings 
only  when  they  are  of  moderate  duration  or  difficulty 
and  then  only  with  much  prodding  and  supervision.  4 

IRRESPONSIBLE:  Cannot  be  relied  upon  to  complete  any 
undertaking  even  when  constantly  prodded  and  guided.  5 

CREATTVENESS  AND  IMAGINATION 

GENERAL:  Approaches  whatever  he  does  with  active  imag- 
ination and  originality,  so  that  he  contributes  some- 
thing that  is  his  own.  1A 

SPECIFIC:  Makes  distinctly  original  and  significant  contribu- 
tions in  one  or  more  fields.  IB 

PROMISING:  Shows  a  degree  of  creativeness  that  indicates  the 
likelihood  of  valuable  original  contribution  in  some 
field,  although  the  contributions  already  made  have  not 
proved  to  be  particularly  significant.  2 

LIMITED:  Shows  the  desire  to  contribute  his  own  thinking 
and  expression  to  situations,  but  his  degree  of  imagina- 
tion and  originality  is  not  in  general  high  enough  to 
have  much  influence  on  his  accomplishments.  3 

IMITATIVE:  Makes  little  or  no  creative  contributions,  yet 
shows  sufficient  imagination  to  see  the  implications  in 
the  creation  of  others  and  to  make  use  of  their  ideas  or 
accomplishments.  4 

UNIMAGINATIVE:  Has  given  practically  no  evidence  of  orig- 
inality or  creativeness  in  imagination  or  action.  5 

INFLUENCE 

CONTROLLING:  His  influence  habitually  shapes  the  opinions, 

activities,  or  ideals  of  his  associates.  1 

CONTRIBUTING  INFLUENCE:  His  influence,  while  not  control- 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  479 

Type 

ing,  strongly  affects  the  opinions,  activities,  or  ideals  of 
his  associates.  2 

VARYING:  His  influence  varies,  having  force  when  particular 
ability,  skill,  experience,  or  circumstance  gives  it  op- 
portunity or  value.  3 

COOPERATING:  Has  no  very  definite  influence  on  his  associ- 
ates, but  contributes  to  group  thinking  and  action  be- 
cause of  some  discrimination  in  regard  to  ideas  and 
leaders.  4 

PASSIVE:  Has  no  definite  influence  on  his  associates,  being 
carried  along  by  the  nearest  or  strongest  influence.  5 

INQUIRING  MIND 

GENERAL:  Responds  with  consistent,  active,  and  deep  interest 
to  any  intellectual  stimulus  and  uses  to  good  advantage 
various  sources  of  information.  1 

SPECIFIC:  Responds  with  consistent,  active,  and  deep  interest 
only  to  stimuli  arising  in  specific  fields  or  problems. 
Uses  effectively  the  sources  available  for  such  purposes.  2 

LIMITED:  Somewhat  sensitive  to  stimuli  arising  from  limited 
fields,  but  engages  in  exploration  and  investigation  only 
when  a  general  plan  of  attacking  the  problem  is  indi- 
cated to  him.  3 

DIRECTED:  Responds  to  stimuli  in  a  limited  field  of  interests 
but  is  impelled  to  act  only  when  both  the  plan  and  the 
details  of  procedure  are  definitely  outlined  for  him.  4 

UNRESPONSIVE:  Rarely  seems  to  be  sensitive  to  any  intellec- 
tual stimulus  and  shows  little  or  no  ability  to  use  the 
tools  and  methodology  of  exploration  and  investigation.  5 

OPEN-MlNDEDNESS 

DISCRIMINATING:  Welcomes  new  ideas  but  habitually  sus- 
pends judgment  until  all  the  available  evidence  is  ob- 
tained. 1 

TOLERANT:  Does  not  readily  appreciate  or  respond  to  oppos- 
ing viewpoints  and  new  ideas,  although  he  is  tolerant 
of  them  and  consciously  tries  to  suspend  judgment  re- 
garding them.  2 


480        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Type 

PASSIVE:  Tolerance  of  the  new  or  different  is  passive,  arising 
from  lack  of  interest  or  conviction.  Welcomes,  or  is  in- 
different to,  change,  because  of  lack  of  understanding 
or  appreciation  of  the  new  or  of  that  which  it  replaces.  3 
RIGID:  Preconceived  ideas  and  prejudices  so  govern  his  think- 
ing that  he  usually  ends  a  discussion  or  an  investigation 
without  change  of  opinion.  4 

INTOLERANT:  Is  actively  intolerant;  resents  any  interference 
with  his  habitual  beliefs,  ideas,  and  procedures.  5 

THE  POWER  AND  HABIT  OF  ANALYSIS;  THE  HABIT  OF 

REACHING  CONCLUSIONS  ON  THE  BASIS  OF 

VALID  EVIDENCE 

HIGHLY  ANALYTICAL:  Habitually  makes  an  analytical  ap- 
proach to  his  problems,  assembling  the  facts,  showing 
a  clear  perception  of  their  relationships  and  implica- 
tions, and  thinking  through  the  situation  to  well  founded 
conclusions.  1 

^COMPLETE:  Makes  an  intelligently  analytical  approach  to 
his  problems  but  is  more  limited  in  ability  to  assemble 
the  facts  completely,  and  to  see  their  relationships  or 
their  implications.  2 A 

IRREGULAR:  On  occasion  shows  unusual  analytical  power  but 

does  not  do  so  habitually.  2B 

UNDEVELOPED:  Shows  signs  of  analytical  power,  but  because 
of  fears,  the  domination  of  others,  or  some  other  inhibit- 
ing agency,  has  not  yet  developed  it  to  any  high  degree.  3 A 

LIMITED:  Is  able  to  pursue  reasoning  processes  if  aided  by 

some  guidance  and  direction.  SB 

PASSIVE:  His  approach  to  a  problem  is  not  an  analytical  one, 
though  he  may  be  able  to  appreciate  a  train  of  reason- 
ing or  to  follow  one  laid  out  by  some  one  else.  4 

UNREASONING:  Seems  unable  to  analyze  even  a  fairly  simple 
situation,  tending  rather  to  rely  on  memory  as  a  substi- 
tute for  logic.  Accepts  statements  and  results  without 
attempting  to  reason  about  them.  5 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  481 

SOCIAL  CONCEKN  Type 

GENERALLY  CONCERNED:  Shows  an  altruistic  and  general  social 
concern  and  interprets  this  in  action  to  the  extent  of  his 
abilities  and  opportunities.  1 

SELECTIVELY  CONCERNED:  Shows  concern  by  attitude  and  ac- 
tion about  certain  social  conditions  but  seems  unable  to 
appreciate  the  importance  of  other  such  problems.  2 

PERSONAL:  Is  not  strongly  concerned  about  the  welfare  of 
others  and  responds  to  social  problems  only  when  he 
recognizes  some  intimate  personal  relationship  to  the 
problem  or  group  in  question.  3 

INACTIVE:  Seems  aware  of  social  problems,  and  may  profess 
concern  about  them,  but  does  nothing.  4 

UNCONCERNED:  Does  not  show  any  genuine  concern  for  the 
common  good.  5 

EMOTIONAL  RESPONSIVENESS 

TO  IDEAS:  Is  emotionally  stirred  by  becoming  aware  of  chal- 
lenging ideas.  1 

TO  DIFFICULTY:  Responds  emotionally  to  a  situation  or  prob- 
lem challenging  to  him  because  of  the  possibility  of 
overcoming  difficulties.  2 

TO  IDEALS:  Responds  emotionally  to  what  is  characterized 
primarily  by  its  personal  or  social  idealism.  3 

TO  BEAUTY:  Responds  emotionally  to  beauty  as  fgund  in 
nature  and  the  arts.  4 

TO  ORDER:  Responds  emotionally  to  perfection  of  function- 
ing as  it  is  seen  in  organization,  mechanical  operation, 
or  logical  completeness.  5 

SERIOUS  PURPOSE 

PURPOSEFUL:  Has  definite  purpose  and  plans  and  carries 
through  to  the  best  of  his  ability  undertakings  consist- 
ent with  this  purpose.  1 

LIMITED:  Makes  plans  and  shows  determination  in  attack- 
ing short-time  projects  that  interest  him,  but  has  not  yet 
thought  out  goals  for  himself.  2 


482        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

Type 

POTENTIAL:  Takes  things  as  they  come,  meeting  situations 
somewhat  on  the  spur  of  the  moment,  yet  may  be  capa- 
ble of  serious  purpose  if  once  aroused.  3 
UNRELIABLE:  Makes  plans  that  are  fairly  definite,  but  cannot 
be  counted  on  for  the  determination  to  carry  them 
through.  4 
VAGUE:  Is  likely  to  drift  without  the  decision  and  persistence 
that  will  enable  him  to  carry  out  his  vaguely  conceived 
plans.  5 

SOCIAL  ADJUSTABILITY 

SECURE:  Appears  to  feel  secure  in  his  social  relationships  and 
is  accepted  by  the  groups  of  which  he  is  a  part.  1 

UNCERTAIN:  Appears  to  have  some  anxiety  about  his  social 
relationships  although  he  is  accepted  by  the  groups  of 
which  he  is  a  part.  2 

NEUTRAL:  Shows  the  desire  to  have  an  established  place  in 
the  group,  but  is,  in  general,  treated  with  indifference.  3 

WITHDRAWN:  Withdraws  from  others  to  an  extent  that  pre- 
vents his  being  a  fully  accepted  member  of  his  groups.  4 

NOT  ACCEPTED:  Has  characteristics  of  person  or  behavior  that 
prevent  his  being  an  accepted  member  of  his  group.  5 

WORK  HABITS 

HIGHLY  EFFECTIVE:  A  pupil  having  highly  effective  work 
habits  would  be  likely  to  reach  the  maximum  accom- 
plishment for  one  of  his  ability.  1 

ADEQUATE:  A  pupil  having  adequate  work  habits  would  ac- 
complish all  that  would  commonly  be  expected  of  one 
of  his  ability.  2 

PROMISING:  While  his  habits  are  not  yet  adequate,  they 
show  promise  of  becoming  so.  3 

LIMITED:  Has  work  habits  that  are  adequate  only  for  simple 
situations,  or  are  limited  by  the  lack  of  development  of 
some  elements  that  make  for  efficiency.  4 

INEFFECTIVE:  Has  not  developed  his  work  habits  to  the  point 
where  he  can  work  efficiently.  5 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  483 

It  will  be  seen  that  the  subheads  under  "Emotional  Re- 
sponsiveness" are  not  exclusive,  since  a  pupil  might  respond 
to  any  number  of  them.  In  this  respect  the  treatment  of  this 
characteristic  differs  from  that  of  die  others. 

The  key  for  recording  teachers'  judgments,  which  a  school 
can  extend  as  it  seems  necessary,  lists  abbreviations  that 
show  the  type  of  opportunity  a  teacher  has  had  for  observ- 
ing the  pupil  being  described. 

The  following  example  will  show  how  this  is  used. 

Under  "Responsibility-Dependability"  six  types  of  behav- 
ior are  defined.  They  will  be  listed  by  their  numbers  and 
key  words,  and  the  judgments  of  nine  teachers  about  a  pupil 
will  be  shown  as  they  would  appear  on  a  filing  card: 

1  Responsible M — HR 

2  Conscientious N.S. — S.S. — E. — F. 

3A  Generally  Dependable A — Mu 

3B  Selectively  Dependable 

4  Unreliable P 

5  Irresponsible 

This  indicates  that  the  teacher  of  mathematics  and  the  home- 
room teacher  believe  the  boy  fits  the  definition  of  Type  1, 
that  teachers  of  natural  science,  social  science,  English,  and 
French  place  him  as  Type  2,  that  art  and  music  teachers 
would  describe  his  behavior  as  of  Type  3A,  while  the  one 
in  charge  of  physical  education  would  place  him  under 
Type  4. 

The  total  picture  of  this  boy's  behavior  (but  only  in  re- 
spect to  his  responsibilities)  shows  him  to  be  highly  con- 
scientious in  meeting  the  demands  of  academic  work  and 
of  the  group  (home-room)  with  which  he  is  closely  con- 
nected. It  also  shows  that  for  some  reason  he  is  not  so  highly 
dependable  in  the  arts,  and  that  he  is  failing  to  meet  with 
any  consistency  the  obligations  that  are  related  to  physical 
education.  It  is  not,  of  course,  safe  to  make  positive  judg- 


484        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

ments  about  the  arts  and  the  physical  education  from  this 
information  alone.  Evidence  about  the  other  characteristics 
may  throw  light  on  what  is  shown  here,  and  personal  rela- 
tionships, home  obligations,  or  other  factors  may  enter  into 
the  situation. 

It  is  evident  from  this  example  that  a  principal,  super- 
visor, or  guidance  officer  can  not  only  obtain  information 
from  the  numerical  distribution  of  judgments  and  the  situa- 
tions in  which  extremes  of  behavior  occur,  but  also  can  take 
into  account  what  he  knows  about  teachers  and  courses,  in 
this  way  reaching  a  more  accurate  understanding  of  the 
pupil  than  would  otherwise  be  possible. 

While  one  outside  an  institution  cannot  obtain  so  com- 
plete an  understanding  as  this,  information  from  this  card 
and  the  comment  of  a  supervisor,  recorded  on  such  a  form  as 
that  used  for  transfer  to  college  ( Chapter  XII ) ,  can  give  a 
very  accurate  description  for  the  use  of  a  college  admissions 
officer  or  a  prospective  employer. 

The  fact  that  the  classifications  under  any  heading  on  the 
card  were  not  intended  to  constitute  a  rating  scale  cannot 
be  too  strongly  emphasized.  The  committee  was  also  agreed 
in  the  belief  that  the  classifications  obtained  could  not  even 
be  said  to  define  orders  of  excellence,  since  there  was  no 
certainty  that  some  earlier  classes  were  better  than  others 
that  were  later  in  the  lists,  nor  that  behavior  of  a  certain  type 
was  best  for  all  kinds  of  people  under  all  kinds  of  conditions. 
It  is  true  that  the  first  classifications  generally  describe  be- 
havior that  would  be  considered  highly  desirable,  that  the 
last  are,  in  general,  not  indicative  of  such  favorable  traits, 
and  that  there  is  a  general  decrease  of  desirability  through 
most  of  the  classes.  It  cannot  be  assumed,  however,  that  each 
class  is  below  the  preceding  one  or  above  the  following  one 
in  desirability.  Neither  can  it  be  taken  for  granted  that  where 
there  is  evident  decrease  in  desirability  the  intervals  are 
equal,  or  in  any  fixed  relationship  to  one  another. 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  485 

The  classifications  are  therefore  simply  items  of  the  de- 
scription of  a  person  in  terms  of  his  behavior  under  various 
conditions,  as  judged  by  a  number  of  practiced  and  suppos- 
edly impartial  observers.  It  is  of  course  true  that  the  limited 
number  of  descriptions  cannot  exactly  describe  all  possible 
kinds  of  behavior.  It  is  believed,  however,  that  the  definitions 
will  usually  fit  closely  enough  for  practical  purposes,  par- 
ticularly since  when  necessary  they  can  be  modified  by.  fur- 
ther comment. 

In  addition  to  the  characteristics  so  far  listed  there  are 
four  on  the  card  about  which  the  only  judgment  asked  for 
each  is  whether  it  is  present  or  absent  to  a  marked  degree. 
The  four,  which  are  defined  on  the  blank,  are  PHYSICAL 

ENERGY,  ASSURANCE,  SELF  RELIANCE,  and  EMOTIONAL  CONTROL. 

Two  other  details  are  worthy  of  notice.  At  the  end  of  the 
printed  material  there  is  a  place  for  indicating  the  judgment 
of  the  faculty  in  regard  to  the  success  of  the  pupil  in  four 
broad  fields  of  thought  and  activity.  These  are  "abstract 
ideas  and  symbols,"  "people,"  "planning  and  management/* 
and  "things  and  manipulation."  It  is  thought  that  where 
there  are  marked  differences  in  success  in  these  areas  the 
evidence  may  prove  valuable  in  guiding  a  pupil  toward 
suitable  after-school  experiences.  The  information  may  help 
to  decide  whether  or  not  the  pupil  should  go  to  college,  and 
if  so  to  what  kind  of  a  college,  whether  or  not  he  should 
undertake  some  form  of  specialization,  what  kind  of  a  job 
he  should  try  to  obtain. 

The  other  detail  is  the  large  space  left  for  "comment." 
This  is  useful  for  the  recording  of  information  that  explains, 
amplifies  or  brings  into  relationship  the  description  on  other 
parts  of  the  card. 

Successful  use  of  the  behavior  description  material  re- 
quires study  of  the  manual  and  careful  following  of  its  direc- 
tions. At  first  this  may  seem  to  require  more  time  than  a 
teacher  is  able  to  give.  However,  the  time  needed  for  re- 


486        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

cording  will  grow  rapidly  less  as  one  becomes  familiar  with 
the  method  used,  particularly  if  a  teacher  is  already  observ- 
ing and  analyzing  the  behavior  of  his  students  to  the  extent 
any  good  teacher  should.  It  is  the  conviction  of  the  commit- 
tee that  time  spent  in  better  understanding  of  a  pupil  does, 
in  any  case,  justify  itself  in  better  relationships  and  more 
effective  work. 

It  is  interesting  to  know  in  this  connection  that  one  pub- 
lic school  system  has  adopted  this  form  for  the  study  of 
12,000  pupils  in  junior  and  senior  high  school  and  expects 
soon  to  extend  it  to  another  6,000  pupils.  Some  colleges,  as 
has  been  said,  have  found  the  card  valuable  in  obtaining 
and  recording  facts  about  behavior,  and  many  types  of 
schools  are*  experimenting  with  the  material.  Samples  have 
gone  to  other  countries,  even  to  Russia  and  South  Africa, 
as  well  as  to  most  sections  of  the  United  States. 

StlMMABY  OF  ADVANTAGES 

This  form  replaces  "rating"  as  a  basis  for  studying  indi- 
viduals by  description  of  behavior  as  observed  by  adults 
having  a  variety  of  associations  with  the  one  studied. 

In  general  it  shows,  for  any  characteristic,  a  pupil's  most 
common  behavior  and  range  of  behavior.  Where  no  mode 
appears,  the  judgments  being  so  scattered  as  to  have  no 
modal  point,  that  fact  in  itself  has  significance,  the  particular 
implications  depending  on  the  pattern  of  judgments  and  the 
characteristics  in  question. 

Taken  as  a  whole,  the  card  when  filled  in  gives  a  reason- 
ably complete  picture  of  the  person's  behavior  because  the 
characteristics,  each  of  which  emphasizes  one  facet  of  behav- 
ior, combine  to  form  quite  a  comprehensive  description  of 
him. 

The  material  is  in  such  form  that  it  can  very  quickly  be 
transferred  to  a  cumulative  record  card  or  a  college  entrance 
blank,  or  be  used  as  a  basis  for  an  interview  with  parents. 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  487 

On  a  college  entrance  blank  the  information  can  show  the 
pupil's  most  common  behavior  and  the  number  of  reporters 
who  observed  it,  and  can  indicate  the  range  and  under  what 
conditions  extremes  occur.  The  form  in  Chapter  XII  shows 
such  a  transfer  from  this  card. 


Chapter  XI 

TEACHERS'  REPORTS  AND  REPORTS  TO 
THE  HOME 

«<-  C«-  «C-  C«"  («:  C«-  «C-  «0  C«-  C«"  ^gC-  <«'  <«•  «««•  C«-<«-  «C-  «<-  C«-C<C«C-  <«•»<«- 

During  the  Study  various  schools  wrote  to  the  chairman  of 
the  Committee  on  Evaluation  and  Recording  asking  about 
tendencies  in  reports  to  parents  and  expressing  dissatisfac- 
tion with  existing  forms.  A  sub-committee1  was  therefore  ap- 
pointed to  investigate  the  practices  of  schools,  to  analyze 
tendencies  in  reporting,  and  to  make  recommendations  of 
forms  for  teachers'  use  and  for  sending  reports  to  the  home. 
This  committee's  first  step  was  to  collect  report  forms  from 
schools  of  various  kinds,  and  to  ask  the  schools  to  say  how 
and  why  present  practices  were  unsatisfactory  and  to  com- 
ment on  what  reports  should  be.  The  report  cards  obtained 
were  carefully  studied,  and  the  criticisms  and  suggestions 
sent  in  by  the  schools  were  analyzed.  Quite  a  number  of 
schools,  however,  sent  no  forms,  saying  that  they  had  noth- 
ing that  would  be  of  any  help  in  the  undertaking.  It  became 
clear  at  once  that  the  most  general  demand  was  for  some- 
thing that  would  replace  numerical  or  letter  marks,  and 
would  give  more  usable  information  about  a  pupil's  strengths 
and  weaknesses. 

Many  schools  were  convinced  that  the  single  mark  in  a 
subject  hid  the  facts  instead  of  showing  them  clearly.  The 
mark  was,  in  effect,  an  average  of  judgments  about  various 
elements  in  a  pupil's  progress  that  lost  their  meaning  and 

1  The  members  of  the  committee  were:  Helen  M.  Atkinson,  Derwood 
Baker,  Genevieve  Coy,  Rosamond  Cross,  Burton  P.  Fowler,  I.  R.  Kraybill, 
Elvina  Lucke,  Eugene  R.  Smith,  Chairman,  John  W.  M.  Rothney,  Research 
Assistant, 

488 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  489 

their  value  when  thus  combined.  The  schools  believed  that 
the  value  of  a  judgment  concerning  the  work  done  by  a 
pupil  in  any  school  course  or  activity  depended  on  the 
degree  to  which  that  judgment  was  expressed  in  a  form 
that  showed  his  strengths  and  his  weaknesses  and  therefore 
presented  an  analyzed  picture  of  his  achievement  that  would 
be  a  safe  basis  for  guidance. 

There  was  also  a  feeling  that  marks  had  become  competi- 
tive to  a  degree  that  was  harmful  to  both  the  less  able  and 
the  more  able,  and  that  they  were  increasingly  directing  the 
attention  of  pupils,  parents,  and  even  teachers,  away  from 
the  real  purposes  of  education  toward  the  symbols  that 
represented  success  but  did  not  emphasize  its  elements  or 
its  meaning. 

The  commonest  method  of  replacing  marks  proved  to  be 
that  of  writing  paragraphs  analyzing  a  pupil's  growth  as 
seen  by  each  teacher.  This  method  is  an  excellent  one,  since 
good  descriptions  by  a  number  of  teachers  combine  to  give 
a  reasonably  complete  picture  of  development  in  relation 
to  the  objectives  discussed.  On  the  other  hand,  a  report  in 
this  form  is  very  time-consuming  for  teachers  and  office,  as 
well  as  difficult  to  summarize  in  form  for  use  in  transfer  and 
guidance.  The  committee  decided  on  a  compromise  that 
would  make  place  for  giving  definite  information  about  im- 
portant objectives  in  an  abbreviated  form  and  would  allow 
for  supplementing  this  with  written  material  needed  to  mod- 
ify or  complete  the  information. 

To  find  the  objectives,  the  list  collected  by  the  Evalua- 
tion Staff  and  the  forms  worked  out  by  the  committees  for 
the  various  subject  fields  (Chapter  XIII)  were  studied.  It 
was  discovered  that  there  were  five  objectives  that  were 
common  to  all  fields  and  experiences,  and  about  which 
knowledge  would  be  particularly  valuable  to  parents  as  well 
as  to  pupils.  These  five  objectives  were  therefore  chosen  as 
headings  to  be  reported  on  by  all  teachers  and  to  be  used 


490        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

in  reports  to  the  home.  The  wording  adopted  for  them  is 
not,  however,  identical  with  the  wordings  on  the  forms  used 
in  subject  fields.  The  reason  is  that  this  committee  had  to 
draw  from  the  large  amount  of  information  asked  for  on 
the  subject  forms  that  which  could  be  condensed  into  sim- 
ple phrases  that  would  have  meaning  and  importance  on  a 
report  to  the  home.  The  headings  follow: 

Success  in  Achieving  the  Specific  Purposes  of  the  Course 
Progress  in  Learning  How  to  Think 
Effectiveness  in  Communicating  Ideas: 

Oral 

Written 

Active  Concern  for  the  Welfare  of  the  Group 
General  Habits  of  Work 

The  question  of  classifications  to  indicate  degrees  of  suc- 
cess or  growth  in  relation  to  these  objectives  proved  a  diffi- 
cult one.  After  much  discussion  and  experimentation  it  was 
decided  to  take  as  a  point  of  departure  the  usual  expecta- 
tion for  one  of  the  age  group  and  the  background  of  the 
pupil  in  question.  Two  classifications  above  and  two  below 
are  used.  They  are  defined  as  follows: 

is  OUTSTAYING:  The  pupil  has  reached  an  outstanding  stage  of 
development  in  the  characteristic  and  field  indicated:  that 
is,  a  stage  distinctly  above  that  usual  for  pupils  of  the  same 
age  and  similar  opportunities. 

is  ABOVE  USUAL:  The  pupil  has  reached  a  stage  of  development 
somewhat  higher  than  usual,  perhaps  with  promise  of  even- 
tually reaching  a  superior  level. 

is  AT  USUAL  STAGE:  The  pupil  is  at  approximately  the  usual  stage 
of  development  for  age  and  opportunity. 

is  BELOW  USUAL:  The  pupil  is  sufficiently  below  the  usual  stage 
in  this  field  to  need  particular  help  from  the  home  and 
school  or  greater  effort  on  the  part  of  the  pupil. 

is  SERIOUSLY  BELOW:  The  pupil  is  seriously  below  an  acceptable 
standard  in  the  field  indicated. 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  491 

In  this  particular  these  forms  depart  somewhat  from  the 
descriptive  method  that  is  emphasized  in  the  work  of  all  the 
committees,  though  taken  as  a  whole  these  blanks  are  still 
highly  descriptive.  This  departure,  however,  should  not  be 
thought  of  as  too  inconsistent,  since  the  purpose  of  these 
forms  affected  to  some  extent  the  method  to  be  used.  It 
seems  likely  that  the  time  will  come  when  each  pupil  is 
judged  primarily  in  accordance  with  his  ability  and  his  op- 
portunities, rather  than  in  comparison  with  others.  There  is 
still  demand,  however,  for  information  that  will  tell  parents 
with  some  definiteness  where  their  children  are  showing 
strengths  or  weaknesses  as  judged  by  normal  expectations. 
These  forms  try  to  meet  that  demand  and  at  the  same  time 
to  describe  the  pupil's  progress  in  a  way  analytical  enough 
to  give  helpful  guidance. 

In  addition  to  the  section  that  tells  the  degree  of  success 
a  pupil  is  achieving  in  the  five  objectives  listed,  there  are 
three  other  sections  of  the  report.  The  first  gives  opportunity 
for  the  teachers  to  point  out  weaknesses  a  pupil  should  par- 
ticularly try  to  eradicate.  There  are  eight  of  these  listed,  and 
the  subjects  in  which  the  weaknesses  are  evident  are  shown 
on  the  home  report: 

Accuracy  in  following  directions 
Efficient  use  of  time  and  energy 
Neatness  and  orderliness 
Self-reliance 

Persistence  in  completing  work 
Thoughtful  participation  in  discussion 
Conscientiousness  of  effort 
Reading 

There  is  also  opportunity  for  the  teachers  to  report  on 
the  pupils'  likelihood  of  success  in  continuing  to  work  in 
their  fields,  both  in  later  years  in  school  and  in  advanced 
institutions. 


ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

A  section  for  "General  Comment"  appears  on  the  teacher's 
report,  and  011  the  report  to  the  home.  Some  schools  copy  the 
most  valuable  of  the  teachers'  comments  upon  the  home  re- 
port form.  Others  summarize  criticisms  and  suggestions  in 
this  space.  Occasionally  so  much  of  value  should  be  sent 
that  an  attached  sheet  must  be  used,  but  in  general  the  space 
for  comment  seems  to  be  sufficient. 

In  all  the  details  that  have  been  mentioned  the  teachers' 
report  and  the  home  report  are  identical,  although  they  dif- 
fer in  arrangement,  since  the  home  report  is  designed  to 
combine  the  reports  of  all  the  teachers  into  a  single  form 
that  can  be  read  easily. 

There  are  two  forms  of  the  report  to  the  home.  They  in- 
clude the  same  material  but  differ  in  arrangement  in  a  way 
that  produces  somewhat  different  emphases.  Form  A  tends 
to  emphasize  the  objectives  in  which  a  pupil  is  strong  or 
weak,  while  Form  B  goes  further  in  showing  a  pupil's  degree 
of  success  in  individual  subjects.  A  school  can  choose  either 
form  or  can  do  as  a  school  represented  on  the  committee  has 
done.  This  school  liked  the  completeness  of  the  teachers' 
reports  so  well  that  it  decided  to  send  copies  of  all  of  them 
to  the  parents  instead  of  using  the  combined  report  form. 

While  one  of  the  greatest  values  of  these  forms  is  the  way 
in  which  they  provide  for  guidance  by  analyzing  a  student's 
progress  instead  of  trying  to  express  several  factors  in  one 
"mark,"  the  form  has  other  advantages. 

An  important  one  is  the  degree  to  which  it  directs  the 
minds  of  pupils,  parents,  and  teachers  away  from  marks  to- 
ward the  fundamental  objectives  with  which  pupils  should 
be  concerned.  Incidentally,  in  this  procedure  it  is  not  easy 
to  compare  two  reports  in  a  way  to  make  the  less  able  pupil 
feel  inferior  or  the  more  able  one  become  smug,  for  in  such 
an  analysis  even  the  poorest  student  is  likely  to  find  some 
appreciation,  while  the  best  student  is  likely  to  discover  some 
weaknesses  to  be  corrected. 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  493 

It  hardly  seems  necessary  to  point  out  the  fact  that  this 
form,  like  the  "Behavior  Description/'  attempts  to  describe 
somewhat  fully  a  phase  of  the  behavior  of  a  person.  In  this 
case,  it  is  principally  the  pupil  as  one  who  is  learning  and 
developing  mental  power  that  is  observed.  As  in  the  other 
form,  the  pupil  is  studied  by  a  number  of  teachers,  and  the 
mode  and  distribution  of  response  in  different  environments 
is  recorded.  The  comment  appearing  on  the  form  sent  to 
the  parents  becomes  an  analysis  of  what  is  shown  under  the 
various  headings,  and  a  recommendation  of  ways  in  which 
the  pupil  can  be  helped  to  overcome  his  weaknesses  and  use 
his  ability  more  effectively. 

A  word  of  warning  about  the  introduction  of  such  report 
forms  may  not  be  amiss.  Pupils  and  parents  should  receive 
some  explanation  of  the  meaning  of  the  information  given 
so  that  they  will  not  be  confused  by  the  very  completeness 
of  what  is  said  and  will  not  be  antagonized  by  the  unfamiliar 
material. 


Chapter  XII 

FORM  FOR  TRANSFER  FROM  SCHOOL 
TO  COLLEGE 

«<-«<-  «C-  C«-  CCfr  C<C'«C-«<-<«'C«-«C-  «<•  C«-  C«-  «fr  <«•<«-  <««  C«-  «<•  «<•  C«-  <£f«£- 

CONFIDENTIAL  REPORT  TO  THE  COMMITTEE  ON  ADMISSION 

The  need  for  a  new  transfer  form  has  been  widely  recog- 
nized. Schools  everywhere  wish  a  uniform  blank,  since  the 
present  waste  of  the  time  of  school  officers,  because  of  the 
wide  variety  of  forms  used  by  different  colleges,  has  reached 
serious  proportions. 

Recognition  of  the  extent  to  which  marks  and  "units"  are 
preventing  schools  and  colleges  from  giving  their  best  serv- 
ice to  individual  students,  and  are  interfering  with  educa- 
tional progress,  also  becomes  daily  more  widespread.  The 
reasons  for  replacing  marks  by  analyses  were  discussed  in 
relation  to  reports  to  the  home.  Units,  too,  become  the  ob- 
jectives for  which  pupils  strive,  sometimes  with  little  con- 
sideration of  the  methods  by  which  they  are  obtained.  In 
many  schools,  also,  reorganized  courses,  activity  programs, 
and  long  time  researches  (though  on  a  secondary  school 
level)  have  so  changed  the  schedule  that  the  definition  of  a 
unit  no  longer  has  meaning.1  A  college  entrance  form  with 
less  emphasis  on  marks  and  units  can  help  greatly  toward 
overcoming  the  abuses  that  are  of  so  much  concern  to  the 
schools.  Then,  too,  it  is  increasingly  recognized  that  educa- 
tion should  have  a  degree  of  continuity  that  has  not  yet 
existed,  and  that  information  useful  for  guidance  should  be 

1  The  Carnegie  Foundation  for  the  Advancement  of  Teaching  has  been 
considered  responsible  for  the  adoption  of  units  as  a  measure  of  work  ac- 
complished in  school.  Various  officers  of  the  Foundation  have  now,  in 
speeches  and  writing,  said  that  units  no  longer  have  value. 

494 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  495 

provided  by  the  schools  for  use  in  college.  The  entrance 
blank  seems  a  natural  place  for  such  information. 

As  an  example  of  this  general  movement,  the  Committee 
on  School  and  College  Relations  of  the  Educational  Records 
Bureau,  which  is  composed  of  school  and  college  represen- 
tatives, has  sent  bulletins  to  the  colleges  emphasizing  needed 
changes  in  information  required  at  entrance,  and  has  pub- 
lished2 the  answers  of  the  colleges,  which  show  quite  gen- 
eral willingness  to  cooperate  in  making  the  changes.  Another 
bulletin  has  recently  been  sent  to  the  colleges,  and  the  an- 
swers will  soon  be  published.  A  striking  example  of  the  inter- 
est taken  by  educators  in  the  various  needs  being  discussed 
is  the  fact  that  the  Educational  Records  Bureau  Committee 
has  given  the  Committees  on  Records  and  Reports  of  the 
Progressive  Education  Association  standing  as  sub-commit- 
tees of  its  own  in  order  to  keep  in  touch  with  their  work, 
and  to  lend  its  support  to  whatever  promises  progress  in 
better  school  and  college  relations. 

This  dissatisfaction  with  entrance  blanks  was  focussed  by 
the  necessity,  under  the  Eight- Year  Plan,  of  developing  an 
entrance  form  that  would  accomplish  two  objectives: 

1.  Have  such  a  range  of  flexibility  and  such  carefully 
chosen  items  that  it  would  not  restrict  any  school's 
curriculum  or  methods. 

2.  Provide  for  information  complete  enough  to  replace 
effectively  the  data  that  was  omitted  under  the  spe- 
cial plan  for  the  cooperating  schools,  and  significant 
enough  to  assist  in  the  guidance  programs  of  the 
colleges. 

The  Committee  on  Evaluation  and  Recording  appointed  a 
sub-committee3  to  work  on  this  problem.  This  committee, 

2  Published  by  the  Educational  Records  Bureau,  437  West  59th  Street, 
New  York  City. 

8  The  members  oi  this  committee  were:  Victor  L.  Butterfield,  Genevieve 
L.  Coy,  Albert  B.  Crawford,  Ruth  W.  Crawford,  Burton  P.  Fowler,  Elvina 
Luclce,  Herbert  W.  Smith,  Eugene  R.  Smith,  Chairman,  Arthur  E.  Traxler, 
John  W.  M.  Rothney. 


496        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

after  studying  previous  reports  on  the  subject,  explored  the 
forms  in  use,  especially  those  prepared  by  groups  of  colleges. 
All  forms  that  had  wide  use  were  analyzed,  and  their  items 
were  listed  with  ratings  of  their  prevalence  in  present  blanks. 

The  committee  also  asked  schools  for  their  criticisms  of 
entrance  blanks  and  their  suggestions  for  improvement,  and 
on  the  basis  of  the  two  surveys  a  new  blank  was  devised  and 
has  been  in  use  by  the  cooperating  schools  with  the  very 
large  number  of  colleges  to  which  they  send  students. 

The  first  page  of  the  form4  is  given  over  very  largely  to  a 
tabular  history  of  the  courses  the  pupil  has  taken  in  school, 
and  a  combined  recommendation  and  prediction  for  work 
in  college.  This  table  allows  a  school  that  wishes  to  do  so  to 
record  only  traditional  marks  and  units,  but  it  also  allows  for 
courses  not  easily  expressed  in  units  and  not  recorded  by 
marks,  since  it  has  space  for  final  recommendations  in  the 
major  departments  most  likely  to  be  presented  for  entrance 
or  followed  in  college,  and  provides  blank  spaces  for  addi- 
tions. If  this  form  were  being  prepared  now  it  would  prob- 
ably have  no  column  for  units,  but  when  it  was  being  devised 
the  movement  for  omission  of  unit  equivalents  in  Statements 
of  Credit  had  not  reached  the  point  it  has  since  attained. 

The  second  page  is  given  to  test  records  and  includes  a 
blank  space  for  "Summary  Interpretation''  of  tests  whose 
results  are  not  easily  expressed  in  numerical  forms.  Such  tests 
include  ones  described  in  the  "Evaluation"  section  of  this 
report,  as  well  as  tests  of  primary  abilities  and  others  that 
have  important  sub-heads. 

The  particular  contribution  of  the  third  page  is  the  tabular 
form  for  the  description  of  a  pupil's  behavior,  and  a  resulting 
characterization  of  him.  The  table  is  based  on  definitions  of 
the  characteristics  and  the  sub-heads  under  them  as  they 
are  given  in  the  "Manual  of  Behavior  Description,"  and  is 
supposed  to  be  used  with  those  definitions.  ( See  Chap.  X. ) 

4  The  form  is  between  pp.  469-497. 


4-»            I 
1      C 

s: 

:l      i   i 

QCD       ?a  d 

CDC 

2       *H  C       J 

<S  J  y 

Q^Ci          OJ  4-1*      • 

Pu     J^C 

>       4-3  0        , 

U! 

So 

34-5    03          f-i  4-3> 

^J  CO  4-5  -r 

•i         ?H  O         i 

Q 

sis 

;j        ^5   ^   0  C 

4J        JL 

•i  <D  Cj               , 

Q 

Ig* 

fq-i   O  -H    Oi  CD 

ca  C5-jQ 

j)  <r~|  fX4-3 

•i 

u 

S 

".O  «H    CD    g    S 

<D  CD  O  £ 

H-P       C      ! 

Iii 

gl       ^  ^  *H   C 

zl>)  CC*4->          O 

^4S«*n<i 

*  CO  4-3   CQ 

a. 

•P  OC     •  JH^-J   CD 

CQ 

C 

5^0 

<3          rd    ?-»  -H 

H  0  >,  CD  ^  SL,   • 

40 

°  K  ^ 

>4->  CD  c  q  > 

CQ 

J-i 

<n 

^J-B 

J 

r^j 

>4^>  O  O     «        E 

<?s 

SLU 

!l 

S        -P                 CD 

C 

ori-p  txo  OCD 

i- 

"*  "   Q 

i 

>Ci3          <D   CD 

•^ 

"d«r|  CQ 

«  4-3|—|  j 

1 

M 

1 

"Z. 

s  1  ^S 

gli| 

S 
i' 

^-j    O  TCJ    CO 
>CQ    0    C-P- 
§0j          CD          C 

0 

£» 

b- 

C-H> 
P3X!  SH 
C  O    *  OJ 

^  c'o 

CD  O  JH 

• 

Ul     ?-4 

CD 

h£»    CQ  »H    O    Cd 

^J 

OSVi  5s  ?n 

XI-H&,; 

r~? 

K     O 

•    »Q 

®  £  X  ^ 

J     ,  Q<0  -P  6 

O 

0       CD  0 

4^4^ 

O 

O  **~^ 

«H 

s  §  «  S 

OC3 

3  -H  oa 

aJ 

^eCSp 

P^r^j  ' 

0 

< 

r._1 

> 

^  ^  5  £ 

( 

^C 

>,rj       ca  «M 

,0 

+4       <& 

<5-l  CDC 

.£! 

o  Js> 

I 

to 

5  3  S  s3 

5f 

:  CQ  <u  c  o 

ri  0         CD 

O  4-3  OJ 

O 

<    ^ 

J>- 

C 

c  c  S  5 

^  c  o  o 

C 

CWS-r-P 

40 

w  tfj 

j  J2J 

4-3 

> 

2  g  £  § 

"V1 

J|  O        -H  CO 

cd 

hJDCC;  0  C 

1  C 

1     . 

f-\ 
HJ 

z 

3 

1113 

Sc 
fer 

>  ^j   01  4^>  4^ 

)    4J>  r^j     Ctf    O 

P 

HS      o 

CQ  •    >»O 

O  !k  O 

'T   O 

M   "^ 

f"H 

0 

S  S  o  5 

u 

•   C^  H    0   CD 

O 

4-3  *£$ 

3    CQ 

J^J 

CD 

5  ^ 

*  r-i   CD  -H   CX 

$L, 

U  c5  jzJ  TZ 

QH-P 

.§ 

5 

•H 

i 

2C 
& 

:    CD  -H  r-!   CO 

3  ^<*H  ft  as 

J§ 

P4^>  cn  cd 

JM  O*£J 

0 

O 

C 

0 

u 

X             *» 

1 

^ 

& 

fill 

C 

2 

5         > 

' 

cd 

D  (fl 

S 

j 

j 

«<^> 

rH 

W  u 

p 

j 

r-l 

M 

2 

. 

C 
o 

< 

""  < 

|| 

2 

i 

3 

1 

1 

1 

} 

^ 

<>5 

^H 

tn 

to    . 

til  -1 

—. 

x  > 

Z 

J 

] 

J 

I 

i 

1 

LU 

j 

J 

S 

O 

3  0 
0  g) 

=  z 

jl 

o 

1 

\ 

J 

] 

i 

i 

'      0 

U 
C 

in 

LU 

H° 

is 

. 

\ 

\ 

} 

3 

1 

s 

i 

'     C 

o 

z  O 

oa 

og 

i| 

1     o5 

0    ^, 

o  y 

(/)  W 

"•' 

I     r-1 
•     P« 

1 

Q     C 

1 

LJ   £ 
<  K 

r» 

:  S 

5I 

0. 

j-  u; 

wE 
5& 

5 

g 

;  $ 

3    CD 

g  5 

Sz 

«1 

1 

»•  4J> 

5 

5  ~ 

— 

o.g. 

4^ 

O 

3      • 

<'« 

M 

o 

S     * 

(<•£} 

5s 

c   ^ 

Q  D 

cc, 

g 

3    £ 

Cif 

A, 

LL! 

It.  2 

5» 

g     • 

J  •  £ 

•H 
w 

s 

si 

LU 

5 

1 

tf 

en 

88 

CD 

d 

§ 

1 

gS 

s! 

U>  (A 
til  Ul 

8 

His 

0 

0 

c    CD 

o    C? 

ffl  Q 

M 

^  ^ 

^g 
0  0 

Q 

Q 

1 

U 

r) 

^ 

0 

H 

o 

Ld 
CO 

(A  t/J 

i 

H 

U 

tJ 

d 

cc 

!r      t 

or 

iii  Hi 

H 

H 

>    «r~i 

O 

8 

INDICA' 
1NDJ  CA- 

= 

i  1 

£i 

) 

C 

I 

JL 

?: 

C 

04 

SH 

t> 

h 

i,,,c: 

•is 

, 

LU 

4J 

4J 

o; 

t/) 

o 

I  4^» 

K   W 

i 

0) 

z 
0 

1 

to 
u 

a: 

E 

LJj 

•r- 

O 

43 

LU 

Q 

cc! 
CQ 

4^ 
C 

H 

u.    CO 

0     K, 

a 

< 

— 

as 

SUJ 
— 

t/> 

4-5 

H2 

—  i 

CD 

< 

_J 

fe 

UJ 

cd 

o 

CD 
*±3 

- 

Ul 

ca 

f— 

CO 

<D 

E 
C 

(— 
CO 

C 
oj 

O 

^ 

H 

i 

5  a 

1 

a: 

Q 

13    U. 

JH 
O 

C 

o 

-H 
CQ 

g 

LTJ 
<r 
o 

u. 

'c 

C 

C 

X 
1— 
< 

£• 

a 
tx 

d 
i  ec 

•C 
r~ 

" 

cd 

<L 
C 

o 

of" 

CO 

> 

O 

*> 

1 

<L 
P 

'  ,t 
CC 

^ 

J3 

JH 

cc 

r— 

r- 

q 

CD 

£• 

E 

S 

fo 

<I 

P^ 

ca 

O 

^ 

< 

! 

. 

tn 

en 
CD 

r  .1 

; 

x 

3 

HH 

to 

CD 

j 

i 

ixj 

r-j 

JLj 

1 

j- 

! 

Q 

LU 

ce 
O 

3 
C3 

CD 
W 

j 

Z 

& 

$ 

t 

2 

CD 

iii 

— 

a 

H 

ID 
C2 

££ 

2 

IP 

0 

o 

»^ 
w 

1 

E 
< 

tf 

<U 

I* 

^ 

C 

rH 

§  — 

U. 

«-tw 

0 

4-2 

§1^ 

s 

CD 

|i 

s 

55 

0 

e  1 

03 

<u 

I  *" 

vt 

pd 

p 

|  > 

I 

* 

£z 

A-i 
4^ 

•s 

8  •* 

w 

10 

vO 

LO 

O 

&v 

4-3 

CD 

CD 

Q 

1 

^ 

0 

40 

cc 
o 

O 

1 

K 

4-3 

0 

o: 

K 
O 

? 

< 

f- 

& 

H 

LU 

0 

i 

H 

LU 

5 

S. 

"Z, 

o 

w 

c 

in 

u 

I 

"2L 

u. 

Z 

~ 

0 

. 

1 

1 

to 

cd 

> 

w 

o 

3 

Jxl 

0 

i 

5 

5 

Q 

OJ 

cc 

« 

u, 

t; 

w 

o 

Ul 
O 

CX 

§ 

CO 

to 

S  JH 
3   0) 

0 
w 

2 

i 

PS 

/•*- 

0 

5 

•H   C 

C 

h 

t 

Q 

,  i 

Wl 

OF  TEST 

O  -H 

o 

CO 

a 

0 

r- 

2 

!  \ 

1 

*  ,C 
CD   > 

1 

S 

CO 


H 
O 


a 

CQ 


•ri 
O 


CO 
O 


s 

CD 


43 

S 


CD 

•H 

OiH 


ES 


CO 
CD 

-H 
.p 


<J>  -r-S 
0-P 

A  o 

•H   C(S 

CO 


H  0 


o 
o 

CO 

o 

"p 

3   CQ 

•P     P* 

•H  ^S 
CO    O 


•d 

<D  Ci> 
O   K 


H«H 
0)  <D 
CQ  0) 


Ord 

CD     O   (D 

<D  -P       I   43  ' 

>  aS    «HI  Ck 

T-|  p«   H  Q> 

CD  O 

CQ  O 


O 

•H 


O    CQ 


.P   Pk 

^S  O 
O-P 


CD     ^ 
S  co 

O  H 
CO   SU 


^H  <U     bQ 
1 
S>> 

4^: 


|xj   CD 

.  W 

"3Q 


'Z    O       CO    CQ 

r  &Q          CD 
3   OS      <D  ^H 


-P 
(D  O 

SI  a$ 


-  ^   O  o 

fri       --H 

y  CQ 


i          i  P, 
=  tu     < 


Q  m  0 

ggi 
|12 

|8| 


- 

MS* 


fci 

&0o. 


s 


SEASONING 

Z 

3 

!!! 

Ul 

§*" 

p 

< 

id 

z 

> 

rSj 

S 

5 

1 

, 

u 

i 

ca 

1 

1 

r*~ 

rCj 

T* 

A* 

w 

Ul 

§ 

kt 
D 

£4 

ABLE 

3* 

JZ 

CQ 
>- 

1 

S 

3AI5NC 

1 

O 

1 

g 

jsl 

3 

I 

z 

1 

CO 

A* 

d 

C 

CO 

xf 

IMITA1 

I 
O 

«H 
CQ 

a. 

CO 

> 

E 

D 

1 

•H 

3 

CO 

£H 

i 

CO 

rd 

g 

•". 

5 

3 

j 

Q 

1 
J 

o 

-H 
CQ 
3 

NRELfABLE 

1 

LIMITED 

2 

i 

P 

o 

•H 
CQ 

DIRECTED 

CO 

> 

S 
tu 

INACTIVE  j 

ITHDRAWN  | 

ZZ 

3 

u>Q 

o 

Jgj 

£1 

3 

5 

As 

0 

o 

fc*C 

PROMISING 

10 

POTENTIAL 

•H 
CO 

1 

•P 

GENERALLY 
DEPENDABL1 

CQ 
-P 

& 

PROMISING 

VARYING 

f 

o 

to 

LIMITED 

1 

IRREGULAR 

J 

PERSONAL 

o 

Cfl 

j 
z 

s 

J 
3 

> 

\ 

j 

i 

<jj 

ctf 

3 

I 

fp*! 

0 

O 

t 

to 

CO 

ti 

ca 

UI 
f- 

to 

s 

to 

! 

g 

»s 

F 

0 

§ 

1 

s! 

-p 

ctf 

1 

z 

1 
Q 

1 

i 

tk 

1 

s. 

P 

jl 

1 

C 

s 

1 

to 

1 

< 

0 

4: 

4J> 

o 

4J 

S 

,£ 
-p 

s 

3 

^ 

0 

Fi 

cc 

a 

1 

0 

j 

S 

> 

Q 

O 

< 

I 

h 

J 

LE  AN 
EFUL 

f£ 

<D 

J 

0 

z 

J 
< 

^ 

1 

dl 

U 

5 

i 

0 

I 

3 

§1 

sS 

1 

Q. 

I 

CONTROL 

GENER 

p 

a 

JHLY  ANA 

2£ 

I! 

bi 

D 

r 

I 

K 

•P 

V 

0 

(3 

0 

z 

cd 

1 

U 

w 

5E 

I 

111    ^1 

Z 

H 

II 

SERIOUS 

PURPOSE 

RESPONSIBILITY 
DEPENDABILITY 

CREATIVENESS 
AND  IMAGINATIO 

I 

INQUIRING 
MIND 

POWER  AND  HABI 
OF  ANALYSIS 

CONCERN 
FOR  OTHERS 

PERSONAL 
ADJUSTMENT 

SELF 
RELIANCE 

AESTHETIC 
APPRECIATION 

F  «Q| 

S      Q3> 


H       gj 

S       E 

<      S 

M 

*      w 

•t 

Ul 

a: 

i 


I  £ 
i  g 


M          O 

^  i 

z    2 

Ul         T 

h, 

!!! 
iEi 

m       Z       h 

Z      C      < 

3       UJ        * 
Z       O       5 

<H 


, 

i 


g 


P-i 


< 


NT 


8 

< 


G 


tttl 


<  S  vu. 
O:  00 


HP>> 

&=-5 
11£§ 

fcQt:2 


s!5;^ 

l§ih 
gfefs 

H^Z? 

w  |s<i 

5   pi 

;ss§ 

zw2< 

i£M 

J  UJ       < 
J  QQ    . 


R 

FO 
CLU 
AN 
E 


ENE 

THE 
IN 
ED, 
NE 


E 
CL 
,  A 
E 


•       m 
QW  J 

a  =  < 


tss: 


CD   O 

a48 

4J 
«t^    O 

cx5  o« 
X 

>s  CD 
-P 
•H    J>j 

§,S 

si 
a 
o 
o 


C 
o 


H  -P 

CD    O    ctf 

^  o  o 

*  O  T3 
<HI  CQ  CD 
O 

0  (D 
co^  bO 
M  -P  CD 


e  -P  o 

CD  *H   O 

H  £ 

CO 
ijt    CQ  — 

O  C  £ 
O   O  ^ 

bJOnH    O 

-Ph) 


OS    CD    O 

CD  -P 

O  JH  CQ 

Q  -H  O 

CD  O 


CQ 


CD 


«r3       -P 

C   CD  CD 

05    >  CD 

•H  E 
*  4-5 

^    Gj  O 

*"*    CD 
ft  CD 
OiH 
OP 
O    Cj 


0  &>       C 

^1  c      o 

4^>  -H         -H 

yj    (D  4J> 

4^  c3W  cd 

CU  -CD          fj 

JH     *  05 
ca  a  ,M  ft    « 

CO    G    p4    <E>   CD 

CD  'H  O   5^  C 
G  _    >   ftO 


rH    O  n_     .   .    _. 
•H    C   CD  .O   O 

•P  __,   O 

CD  co  aJ  frf  bfi 

JH  -H  HI   CD 

CD          <D '  31    CtJ 
>    CD   f^    O        . 

CD  &         <~\    <P 
CQ          qj  H  »Q 

•p  a  o 
cd  ^  cd«*-i  o 

,0  -P 

t>>         CO    >> 
,Q      ^Oi-J   C 

CD  ^  ,Q  as 
«rj  to  4^  aJ  (H 

CD   &    CtJ,P    PU 
p,  O    <D    Js   CQ 

aS  o  ^J 

o       -p 
-HH  erf 


o 

CI  O 


o . 

CQ 


4-3    CD 
I  JH   > 

I    CtJ   CD 


ctj  aJ  CD 


o  6  b  H 

CQ    O  «H         « 


0   CQ    ^  -H    CSS 

CD       aj 

CD  CQ  CUCQ    * 


3 


J-»  CD  «rl' 

<i-S  CD  >»  ?-* 
O '55  <D 

O  O   <D 


,    _         -P«H 
>  ^H  tiD       tiD 

C  -H    CD   CD 

tiO  O   CO   U 
CD^J^j   O 

ft   CO   j£  ^ 


cS   i 

•C?  O   53 

O 

•H    ?^4    J-4 
CQ   CD   (U 

S  oS 

<D8   • 

S 

co       £4 
nrf  *d  O 

•rl   O  «M 


cd  cti 


«d  o  CD 
C^5  O 

cd  ^  o 

^  CD  CO 


n  ^  CD 

O  fctfl 
-H  d  <D 
-P  -OH 

CtJ         rH 

3  co  o 

4^  -H  O 

-H 
CQ    CD   £i 

»cj  o 

<D        1-1 

O  &  CD 
CD  O  CO 

c:  is 

•H         O 

o  ^ 
>>o  p. 


S4  4-* 
CQ   CD  *H 

*H40 

d  CD  >> 

fl  t>0  O 
OOP 

>^)^p 

H  CtJ 

CD  cr( 


4-*   03   CO 

•H  CQ 

>  P«a> 


0 

H 


|  o  o 

8  *  & 

o  s^ 

ill    CD  (D 

N    S-p 


£g 


IU         "        S    (D 

CC      -          °  <» 

°.d 

(I) 

Trt 

Li 

r- 

C  ft 

£ 

c 

fij  Cti 

o 

CO 

O 

C$J  TJ 

z 

a 

"  <D  § 

«v 

c 

O 

Q- 

a* 

<D 

<D 

z     2 

0  ft 

S 

r 

j[l* 

<J 

S 

fe    J    SH 
ui   S  H 

UD 

o 

u    °  ^ 
-   O  "W 

H 

<  a>  ca 

1 

0 

RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  497 

The  method  of  recording,  which  reports  the  judgments  of 
all  the  teachers  dealing  with  a  pupil,  gives  two  very  important 
facts  about  his  behavior  in  respect  to  any  one  of  the  charac- 
teristics : 

1.  His  most  common  type  of  behavior. 

2.  The  range  of  behavior  on  one  or  both  sides  of  the 
modal  heading. 

For  example: 

WORK     Highly  effective       Adequate      Promising      Ineffective       Limited 
HABITS          English  M-5  Math.  Sci. 

This  would  indicate: 

a.  that  the  pupil's  work  habits  had  been  judged  by  eight 
people,  of  whom  five  thought  they  accorded  best  with  the 
definition  of  "Promising"; 

b.  that  in  English,  because  of  response  to  the  subject,  the 
influence  of  the  teacher,  or  some  other  reason,  his  habits 
seemed  "Highly  Effective"; 

c.  that  in  mathematics  and  science  his  habits  were  as  de- 
fined under  "Limited." 

These  facts  might  have  great  significance  both  for  con- 
sideration of  a  candidate  for  college,  and  for  guidance  if  he 
was  accepted. 

A  school  that  did  not  wish  to  use  any  tabular  method  of 
description  might  omit  the  use  of  this  table  and  describe  the 
candidate  in  paragraph  form  on  the  next  page. 

The  fourth  page  is  left  for  the  school's  comments.  It  may 
replace  the  table  on  page  three  but  in  any  case  it  gives  the 
opportunity  to  supplement,  modify,  and  summarize  the  rest 
of  the  blank.  It  ends  with  a  place  for  the  definite  recommen- 
dation of  the  school  head,  an  item  that  all  colleges  seem  to 
value. 

Other  items  on  the  blank  are  self-explanatory  and  differ 
only  slightly  from  commonly  used  headings. 

All  the  items  most  commonly  asked  for  by  the  colleges,  and 


498         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

possible  for  the  schools  to  furnish,  are  included  on  the  blank, 
while  those  that  have  been  found  to  have  little  importance  in 
actual  use  have  been  omitted.  An  occasional  college  asks  for 
one  or  two  additional  facts,  which  can  usually  be  given  under 
"Comment"  if  no  other  place  seems  more  suitable  for  them. 

This  form  has  been  in  successful  use  for  four  years,  and 
its  use  is  spreading  to  schools  outside  of  the  Study,  sometimes 
through  initiation  by  a  school,  sometimes  through  its  adop- 
tion by  a  college.  It  is  hoped  that  in  its  present,  or  a  modified, 
form  it  will  show  the  way  to  a  uniform  blank  for  the  schools 
and  colleges  of  the  country.5 

A  reproduction  of  the  blank,  filled  in,  follows.  The  use  of 
"C"  to  show  predicted  success  if  a  subject  is  "continued," 
and  of  "TJ"  to  show  ability  to  "use"  it  in  other  fields  if  it  is 
not  continued  in  college  should  be  noted.  "U"  is  not  entered 
unless  the  prediction  for  continuance  is  not  high.  - 

THE  "JUNIOR  YEAR"  BLANK 

An  increasing  number  of  colleges  are  interested  in  obtain- 
ing information  about  candidates  when  they  are  in  the  elev- 
enth grade.  Information  at  that  time  need  not  be  so  complete 
as  in  the  twelfth  grade,  but  it  should  follow  much  the  same 
lines. 

To  supply  this  need  a  preliminary  report  form  was  also  pre- 
pared and  is  in  use  by  the  schools. 

5  An  important  contribution  in  this  respect  has  recently  been  made  by 
the  publication  of  a  blank  prepared  by  a  committee  representing  a  number 
of  associations.  See  Appendix,  p.  508. 


Chapter  XIII 

STUDY  OF  THE  DEVELOPMENT  OF  PUPILS 
IN  SUBJECT  FIELDS 


4&*&r4&&&<^^  C<C-  <«-  «C-  C<C- 

Departments  in  the  various  subject  fields  studied  their  ob- 
jectives more  intensively  during  the  early  years  of  the  Eight- 
Year  Plan  than  the  teachers  concerned,  or  perhaps  any  group 
of  teachers,  had  ever  done  before.  It  became  evident  in  this 
study  of  objectives  that  teachers  in  general,  even  excellent 
ones,  were  not  fully  aware  of  any  but  the  most  general,  and 
therefore  vague,  purposes  for  which  they  were  supposed  to 
be  working,  and  that  they  often  had  little  appreciation  of  the 
importance  of  the  changes  that  were  brought  about  in  their 
pupils  by  the  experiences  of  school  and  out-of-school  life. 
As  a  matter  of  fact  many  an  instructor  is  teaching  in  his  par- 
ticular subject  field  (or  is  teaching  at  all)  only  because  he 
found  that  subject  easy  and  so  made  a  good  record  in  it  him- 
self. He  assigns  a  lesson  or  presents  material  to  his  classes, 
expecting  a  certain  success  in  learning,  but  he  never  looks 
deeply  into  his  pupils'  emotional  responses  and  thought  proc- 
esses or  analyzes  the  developmental  stages  through  which 
they  pass,,  and  the  reasons  for  them. 

Because  of  increased  realization  of  the  need  for  a  more 
analytical  approach  to  the  problems  of  teaching,  a  demand 
arose  for  help  in  making  and  keeping  teachers  aware  of  the 
aims  for  which  they  should  strive.  A  committee1  was  there- 
fore appointed  to  investigate  methods  of  recording  that  might 
serve  such  a  purpose. 

1  The  members  of  this  committee  were:  Helen  M.  Atkinson,  Genevieve 
L.  Coy,  Harry  Herron,  G.  H.  B.  Melone,  Edith  M.  Penney,  Eugene  R. 
Smith,  Chairman,  Arthur  Traxler,  John  W.  M.  Rothney.  They  were  assisted 
by  a  very  large  number  of  school  and  college  teachers  who  contributed 
greatly  to  the  undertaking. 

499- 


500        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

The  original  committee  included  specialists  in  various 
fields,  as  well  as  executives.  Its  first  conclusion,  resulting  from 
a  comparison  of  objectives  of  large  numbers  of  teachers,  was 
that,  while  it  did  not  seem  possible  to  make  one  form  that 
would  be  suitable  for  use  in  all  the  fields  of  knowledge  and 
activity,  it  would  be  possible  to  develop  separate  forms  for 
those  fields  that  would  not  only  be  consistent,  but  would 
parallel  each  other  in  many  respects. 

Further  experimentation  convinced  the  group  that  the 
work  should  be  done  largely  by  specialists  in  the  various 
fields,  assisted  by  some  members  of  the  general  group  who 
had  studied  recording  intensively. 

The  first  detailed  attack  on  the  problem  was  made  by  di- 
viding the  original  committee,  according  to  its  subject  inter- 
ests, into  those  who  would  work  in  English,  social  studies, 
mathematics,  and  science,  and  by  inviting  other  school  and 
college  representatives  to  join  these  groups.  Meetings  usually 
started  with  a  discussion  of  the  questions  involved  in  the 
general  problem,  after  which  the  four  groups  met  separately, 
coming  together  again  to  report  progress  at  the  end  of  the 
second  day. 

A  very  significant  development  was  the  increase  in  breadth 
of  thinking  that  came  to  all  of  the  groups,  the  growth  in  rec- 
ognition of  the  similarity  of  purposes  in  different  fields,  and 
an  appreciation  of  the  importance  of  common  and  correlated 
effort  to  achieve  such  purposes.  Not  only  did  the  groups  in 
mathematics  and  science  spend  much  time  working  together, 
but  the  mathematics  group  asked  the  teachers  of  social  stud- 
ies to  consider  a  question  with  them,  or  some  other  combina- 
tion attacked  a  problem  together.  After  preliminary  forms 
were  made,  other  teachers  and  schools  were  asked  to  criti- 
cize them,  and  eventually  through  really  grueling  work  car- 
ried on  with  considerable  sacrifice  by  some  of  the  workers, 
four  forms  were  arrived  at. 

When  this  stage  was  reached,  others  were  invited  to  join 


RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  501 

the  committee  and  forms  were  added  for  foreign  languages, 
art,  music,  physical  education,  and  homemaking. 

It  was  expected  that  two  forms  might  be  needed  for  for- 
eign languages,  one  for  the  modern  and  the  other  for  the  clas- 
sical languages,  but  as  the  work  went  on  it  seemed  likely  that 
one  form  could  well  cover  the  objectives  for  both  divisions. 

Two  comments  have  special  significance  regarding  all  the 
forms.  The  first  is  that  it  proved  impossible  in  any  field  to 
limit  the  objectives  to  a  number  that  teachers  in  general 
would  be  able  to  use.  The  main  headings  under  which  judg- 
ments can  be  made  are  reasonably  few,  but  the  sub-heads 
considered  important  by  the  committees  increase  the  possible 
number  of  judgments  to  a  point  where  few  teachers  would 
have  the  time  to  make  so  complete  a  study  of  their  pupils. 
This  may  be  a  strength  instead  of  a  weakness,  for  it  brings 
in  enough  flexibility  to  enable  any  school  or  teacher  to  choose 
the  objectives  that  fit  the  aims  of  the  institution  or  the  teacher, 
and  to  concentrate  on  the  study  of  their  degree  of  attainment. 
The  record  is,  then,  just  as  simple,  or  as  extended,  as  one 
chooses  to  make  it.  It  depends  absolutely  on  one's  judgment 
as  to  which  objectives  are  important  enough  to  justify  careful 
study  of  each  pupil's  development  in  respect  to  them. 

The  second  comment  concerns  the  "Behavior  Description" 
section  on  the  back  of  each  card.  Each  committee  that  ana- 
lyzed and  stated  the  aims  of  its  department  included  develop- 
ment in  respect  to  most  of  the  characteristics  in  the  "Be- 
havior Description"  list.  Each  group  eventually  realized  that 
these  characteristics  had  already  been  exhaustively  studied 
by  a  very  competent  committee,  and  that  there  would  be  no 
advantage  in  duplicating  that  work,  even  if  it  were  possible 
to  do  so.  Accordingly,  the  committees  made  places  for  the 
"Behavior  Description''  in  abbreviated  form  on  their  prog- 
ress cards.  It  must  be  understood,  however,  that  this  part 
of  the  cards  can  be  applied  with  full  effect  only  through  use 


502        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

of  the  definitions  of  characteristics  and  classifications  ex- 
plained in  the  Behavior  Description  section  of  this  report. 

A  valuable  feature  of  most  of  the  cards  is  their  inclusion  of 
a  prediction  of  future  success  in  the  field  in  question.  This  is 
meant  to  be  a  basis  for  the  prediction  on  the  "Confidential 
Report  to  the  Committee  on  Admissions/*  Information  under 
"Significant  Interests"  and  the  headings  following  that  one 
are  also  valuable  for  transfer  as  well  as  for  guidance. 

The  committees  endeavored  to  make  these  cards  as  nearly 
self-explanatory  as  possible,  both  in  the  listing  of  objectives 
and  the  explanation  of  methods  of  recording.  Here  too,  how- 
ever, it  must  be  emphasized  that  in  recording  the  pupil  as 
high,  modal,  or  low  in  regard  to  any  objective,  the  teacher  is 
indicating  the  kind  of  growth  the  pupil  is  making  rather  than 
giving  him  a  mark.  The  pattern  of  judgments  about  the  ob- 
jectives considered  should  show  where  the  pupil  is  develop- 
ing well,  and  where  poorly,  and  should  thus  provide  data  for 
helping  him. 

Unfortunately  the  committees  were  unable  to  prepare  such 
cards  for  all  the  purposes  that  might  have  proved  useful.  It 
is  likely  that  the  most  important  omission  concerns  "core" 
courses  that  either  include  two  or  more  fields,  such  as  English 
and  social  studies,  or  are  concerned  primarily  with  the  life 
needs  of  the  pupils.  It  seems  possible,  however,  that  objec- 
tives not  much  different  from  those  that  would  have  been 
chosen  for  such  a  course  can  be  found  on  the  card  for  "Social 
Studies,"  and  that  this  card  can  therefore  be  used  without 
serious  disadvantage.  There  have  been  requests  for  cards  for 
drama  and  for  instrumental  music  also,  and  such  cards  may 
yet  be  devised. 

Perhaps  in  no  kind  of  recording  is  a  teacher  likely  to  be 
so  critical  as  in  that  to  be  used  in  his  own  subject,  and  the 
less  one  has  studied  the  detailed  objectives  in  a  field,  the 
more  likely  he  is  to  overlook  the  implications  in  such  lists  as 
are  on  these  forms.  The  committees,  though  they  make  no 


OF  THE  Of 


IN 


CHOOSE  THE  OBJECTIVES    FOR  WHICH  YOU  WfSH  TO  RECORD  JUDGMENTS^  AMD  INDICATE  WHETHER  THE  PUPIL  IS  Hl6H(H^  MOPAL  OR  USUAL  FOR  AGE  (M) 

SY  CHECKING  IN  THE  APPROPRIATE  COLUMNS.      USE  OILY    HEADINGS  CONCERNING  WHICH   YOU  HAVE  EVIDENCE    OR  AT  t€ ACT  A  FAIRLY  INFINITE  OPIMI 

MAIW  HEADINGS    MAYBE  USED  WITH   OR  WITHOUT    THEIR   SUBHEADS.      AH    X  HAY  BE  USED   IN  THE    L  C0LUMW   TO  INDICATE  A  SERfOOS    LACK. 


OBJECTIVES 

TCACHCR'S   I^ITIAIUS 

CRAOES  AMD  V£APt 

M   0  T  ETS 

GR.       19 

GR.        19 

GR.          19 

GR.        19 

H 

M 

L. 

H 

M 

L, 

H 

M 

L. 

H 

M 

L. 

WORK  HABITS  AND  STUDY 

SKILLS 

PERSISTENCE 

EFF&CT/1S&  C/SE  OF   T/ME 

3M/LL  /N  O8TA/M/A/G  /MFQJ?MAT/O/VOTtf£ffT#M/ffiOMB8®fa 

TECHNIQUES    AND    SKILLS 

L/&XARY    &X/£*S 

\        f  A  /ft 

^/ytLstA't 

~XX->AX 

AS/LfTY  TO  EVALUATE    MATERfAL 

AB/aTf  TO  ORGAN/ZE  MA  TER/A  L 

AB/L/TY   TO  PRESENT  /DEAS    OF  ANOTHER  THROUGH 
/>/t£C/S  AND   PARAGRAPH 

COMMUNICATION 

COMMUN/CATES  OW/V  THOUGHT 
CLEAR  L  Y  AND    ELECTIVE  L  Y 

ORAL 
WRITTEM 

C/SE  OF  l/AR/Oi/S  READJMGiJrECHN/QUES 

At/RAL    CQMPAEHENS/OM 

MECrtANfCS  OF  SPEECH          JVOT£  $E#/O(/$ 

WEAKNESS 

MECHAN/CS  OEWAfT/NG 

/FANY 

MASTERY  OF  PROCESSES  OF  REFLECTIVE 

THINKING 

ff£COGN/ZES  AND   DEMN£$    PROBLEMS 

MAKES  AND  TESTS  #YPOTHE$£ 

rs 

MAKES  GEA/EMLJZAT/ONS  AND  APPL/ES 

PAST  EXP&&/EMCE 

XEACKES   CONCLVS/Q/VS  BY  LOG/CAL    STfPS 

CREATIVE    EXPRESSION 

PRAWS  ON  MS  OW/V  EXPERIENCE  FOR  MATEX/AL 

AMOV/VT  OF  Wff/T/NG  PONE 

CREAT/VE  QUALITY  OF  THE  WK/T/NG 

//VP/CATE  VAME7YOrFO#MSV$E0-VE#$E.  ESSAY.  STOP  Yf  ETC. 

APPRECIATIONS  AND  UNDERSTANDINGS 

DEVELOPMEA/T  OF  PERSONAL   STANDARDS 

DEVELOPMENT  OF  C&/T/CAL    A&/L/T/ES 

3ENS/T/V/TY  TO  FORM.    RHYTHM,    SQl/MD  OF  WORDS,  SAJAGEfW 

JNS/GHT  /NTO  WOT/VES  AND  OTHEft 

/MPL/CAT/ONS 

FSNDS    CLAR/F/CAT/ON  OF  OWN  EXPER/EMCESML/TERATt/RE 

-SEES    iN  L/TERATURE  AN  INTERPRETATION  OF 

L/FE 

DEVELOPING  INTEREST  IN  THE  FIELD 

DEVELOPMENT  TOWARD  A  FUNCTIONING  PHILOSOPHY  OF  UF£ 

GRADE:               YEAR 

GRADE                      YEAR                      GRADE                      YEAR                       GRADE                       YEAR 

»>ST») 

*      VttRV    < 

*   weu. 

iHfWT 
ABLY 

QAftCLY    f 

Atlr 
MG 

IST1MC 

v  X  TH  * 

.HFDftv 

0AKKC 
PA36W 

if     rAllr     vsWTH       VE"I*Y      CACIW>     BAfUTLY     ^AIL-     wnr**        VBRY     CRC(M>>  6A6CI.Y 

MASTERY  OF  ESSENTIALS   OF 

THE 

COURSE 

PREDICTION    OF   FUTURE     PROGRESS 

REAPING  RECORD             «*AD*" 

UMOKHL 

1  O 

1  1 

12. 

COMMENT 

READING    RECORD               cftAoc^*^1        to        M         12             COMMENT 

&OOKS  OF  F/CT/ON   READ 

MAGAZ/MES  READ    REGt/LA&^Y 

TYPE  Of  F/CT/ON  #EAP 

MAGAX/NES  #EA0  OCCAS/ONALLY 

MED/AM  LEVELS  OFMATUX/TY 

MOV/NG   P/CTVRES  PEff  MGA/T/i 

BOCA'S  OF  MOM-&CT/ON    /tEAD 

AVERAGE  PLAYS    PER    YEA/9 

rYfE  OF  /VON-^/CT/OM  #£A0 

RECORDING  STUDENT  PROGRESS  503 

extravagant  claims  for  their  product,  hope  that  anyone  inter- 
ested in  such  forms  will  take  time  for  careful  consideration 
before  deciding  that  the  cards  do  not  quite  adequately  serve 
the  purposes  for  which  they  were  designed.  It  should  be 
noted,  for  example,  that  "conscientiousness/'  which  most 
teachers  would  expect  to  find  in  the  list,  is  not  on  the  front 
of  the  card  because  it  is  included  under  "Responsibility- 
Dependability"  in  the  Behavior  Description  on  the  back  of 
the  card.  Some  headings  that  at  first  thought  seem  essential 
appear  in  less  general  form,  or  are  included  in  more  gen- 
eral statements.  On  the  English  card,  for  example,  "Skill  in 
obtaining  information  other  than  from  books,"  is  included, 
while  the  more  common  and  important  (in  this  field)  pur- 
pose of  obtaining  information  from  books  is  omitted.  It  is 
omitted  because  it  is  too  important  and  so  must  appear  in 
more  analyzed  form.  It  will  be  found  in  such  headings  as 
those  under  "Techniques  and  Skills"  in  "Use  of  Various 
Reading  Techniques,"  and  in  the  "Reading  Record."  It  is  of 
course  included  in  "Mastery  of  Essentials  of  the  Course." 

To  show  the  method  and  organization  used  for  these  cards 
the  front  of  the  English  card  is  reproduced  here. 

The  back  of  the  card  includes,  as  has  been  said,  the 
Behavior  Description  (Chapter  X)  but  uses  only  the  key 
words,  the  definitions  being  omitted.  It  also  has  spaces  for 
recording  the  results  of  comparable  tests,  and  for  making 
notes  about: 

Significant  Interests,  Activities,  and  Accomplishments 
Special  Abilities 
Significant  Limitations 
General  Comment 

The  cards  in  the  other  subjects  follow  the  same  general 
plan  as  the  English  card,  but  they  differ  in  details  in  ac- 
cordance with  the  particular  purposes  of  the  various  courses. 
These  differences  are  not  listed  because  that  would  require 


504        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

what  would  approximate  a  reproduction  of  all  the  cards, 
and  in  a  rather  confusing  arrangement.  It  seems  much  better 
for  one  interested  in  a  particular  field  to  obtain  a  sample 
card  for  that  field,  in  order  to  study  it  as  a  whole. 

These  cards  differ  from  the  ones  described  in  other  chap- 
ters because  while  the  others  are  primarily  office  forms,  these 
are  just  as  definitely  teachers*  forms,  planned  to  help  the 
teachers  in  their  study  of  their  pupils,  and  to  serve  as  source 
material  for  the  other  records.  From  them  can  be  taken  the 
teachers*  judgments  for  entering  on  the  "Behavior  Descrip- 
tion," and  much  that  goes  on  the  "Form  for  Transfer  from 
School  to  College.**  They  serve  as  a  basis  for  the  teachers'  re- 
ports that  become  reports  to  the  home.  If  a  cumulative  record 
form  is  kept,  much  of  the  information  on  it  must  come  from 
the  teachers'  cards.  It  seems,  therefore,  that  these  cards, 
except  when  data  is  being  taken  from  them,  might  well 
remain  in  the  hands  of  the  teachers,  serving  as  reminders  of 
objectives  and  offering  the  opportunity  to  record  information 
whenever  it  seems  timely. 


APPENDICES 


Appendix  I 
^ 


CUMULATIVE  RECORD  FORM 

(Prepared  by  a  Committee  of  the 
American  Council  on  Education1) 

As  was  said  in  Chapter  IX,  no  work  was  done  by  the  Com- 
mittee on  Evaluation  and  Recording  on  a  cumulative  record 
form  for  the  use  of  school  offices  because  the  American  Council 
on  Education  was  planning  to  revise  the  form  that  had  been 
used  so  widely  since  its  publication  in  1930.  The  revision  for 
secondary  schools  has  now  been  completed  and  the  card  can  be 
obtained  from  the  Council's  office  in  Washington.  It  accords  with 
the  principles  and  methods  of  the  other  forms  described  in  this 
volume,  and  so  fits  well  into  the  set  from  which  a  school  can 
choose  its  equipment  for  recording. 

The  cumulative  record  form  is  a  double  sheet  of  tagboard  that 
fits  an  8%"  by  11"  file.  It  furnishes  space  for  all  the  commonly 
recorded  facts  about  a  pupil  and  his  family,  and  for  a  six-year 
history  of  his  school  career. 

One  of  the  largest  spaces  on  the  card  is  given  to  the  history 
and  analysis  of  the  pupil's  progress  in  subject  fields.  This  allows 
opportunity  for  whatever  type  of  reporting  a  school  uses,  though 
the  directions  suggest  some  form  of  analysis  such  as  is  described 
in  Chapter  XI.  Alternative  forms  provide  for  recording  test 
results  in  tabular  or  graphic  form,  and  there  is  also  provision  for 
interpreting  the  test  record  in  relation  to  the  pupil's  academic 
achievement. 

The  "Description  of  Behavior"  section  uses  material  from  the 
card  and  manual  described  in  Chapter  X,  and  adds  spaces  for 
advice  by  guidance  officers,  and  for  follow-up  after  the  pupil 
leaves  school. 

1  Richard  D.  Allen,  Associate  Superintendent,  Providence,  R.  I.;  Millard 
E.  Gladfelter,  Temple  University;  William  S.  Learned,  Carnegie  Foundation 

507 


5o8       ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

UNIFORM  COLLEGE  ENTRANCE  BLANK 

In  1941  under  the  joint  auspices  of  The  American  Council  on 
Education  and  The  National  Association  of  Secondary  School 
Principals  a  committee  was  appointed  representing  these  asso- 
ciations, and  the  New  England  Association  of  Colleges  and 
Secondary  Schools;  the  Middle  States  Association  of  Colleges 
and  Secondary  Schools;  the  North  Central  Association  of  Colleges 
and  Secondary  Schools;  the  Southern  Association  of  Colleges  and 
Secondary  Schools;  the  Progressive  Education  Association;  the 
American  Association  of  Collegiate  Registrars,  for  the  purpose  of 
considering  the  demand  for  an  improved  and  uniform  college  en- 
trance blank.  The  chairman  and  secretary  of  the  Committee  on 
Evaluation  and  Recording  were  members. 

This  committee  considered  blanks  already  prepared  by  various 
groups  and  agreed  upon  a  form  which  has  now  been  published 
by  the  National  Association  of  Secondary  School  Principals  and 
can  be  obtained  from  its  office  in  Washington,  D.  C. 

While  this  form  is  much  more  condensed  than  that  prepared 
for  the  Eight-Year  Study,  having  in  particular  a  limited  space 
for  free  comment  about  the  candidate,  it  has  much  in  common 
with  that  form  and  recognizes  much  the  same  educational  prin- 
ciples. It  offers  opportunity  for  the  use  of  analyses  or  predictions 
instead  of  marks  if  a  school  prefers  them,  omits  any  reference  to 
units,  provides  space  for  annual  tests,  and  gives  emphasis  to  the 
description  of  behavior. 

This  form  shows  marked  progress  toward  present-day  objec- 
tives and  promises  to  influence  school  and  college  relations  con- 
structively. 

for  the  Advancement  of  Teaching;  John  W.  M.  Rothney,  Wisconsin  Univer- 
sity, Secretary;  Donald  J.  Shank,  Assistant  to  the  President,  American 
Council  on  Education;  Eugene  R.  Smith,  The  Beaver  Country  Day  School, 
Chairman;  Arthur  E.  Traxler,  Educational  Records  Bureau;  Edmund  G. 
Williamson,  University  of  Minnesota;  Ben  Wood,  Cooperative  Test  Service. 


03    5J 

l! 


e  « 

:§£ 


•^  o 
a.  P 


,S    8     O 

O  § 


pq   » 
PQ   *> 


-53    S 


u 

O  m  O\O  O  C\       CA 

c\  GS  r^  co  oo  oo     in 

fh 

*<t  co  o  o  T-H  r»     -t 

*o 

b 

c\  en  oo  oo  oo  Tf     fs. 

1 

S 

04  O  ON  ON  CO  Gs       00 

O  O  LO  CA  LO  CO         O 

CO  VO  (N  r-i  -^  M         <N 

2; 

(N  <M  (N  <N  CM  <N       CXI 
CO  CO  CO  cO  cO  CO       CO 

^ 

\0  CN  O  h-  G>t^        O 
CN  CN  C\  00  r^  C\       00 

t 

CO  "t  h-O  CN^>       <N 

b 

CMO  CM  Is-  CO  l>       C\ 

•S 

VH 

O  00  OOCNtN  'O       Tf 

=0 

s 

00  T-<  \O  O  VD  CS        ON 
"^f  CO  CO  r-l  r~  XO         T-I 

X 

m  LO  m  t_n  Lfi  in     m 

u 

VO  <M  O  00  xf  O       00 
00  <^>  C7\  ^t  m  Cs       00 

&5 

b 

<N  O  OCNGNVO       r^- 
O\  CO  T-I  CO  00  O       \O 

3 

S 

m  oi  oooocsj     o 

(N  00  00  00  "t  Tl-        \O 

•<fr  ^  r-»         \O^         CO 

15 

§§8§§§   8 

SH 

r-i  \o  co  vocMn     <N 

ON  00  |>  00  00  00       •<> 

vo  oooo^  r^  oo     m 

N 

b 

O  ON  00  Cs<N  CO       00 

•^ 

C\  ON  00  CO  T)-  00       00 

% 

£ 

Tf  rf  t^  c\  co  o     m 
co  m  cs  T-H  "t  co      <N 

z 

C<I  oq  <M  (N  <N  <N       N 

m  in  in  ijn  m  m      m 

General  accuracy 
Beyond  data 
Caution 
Crude  errors 
True  —  false 
Insufficient  data 
Probably  true  —  prob- 
ably false 

509 


5io        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


i 

co  in  vo  CM  TH  TH   co 

ON  ON  OO  vO  CO  ON    CO 

o  vo  co  "t  T-H  oo   tn 

O  rH  OO"fr  •« 
T-H  TH       ^ 

tf-^fr    CM 

-H  T-H      T-H 

O  00  ON  in  CN  in    T—  i 

co  rf  TJT  rn  -^  "t1   m 
"3-  «*•  CM  T-H  m  rh   co 

CM  CM  CM  CM  CM  CM    CM 

i 

OO  ON  T-H  F"-  O  CN    CO 
ON  00  ON  xj-  t--  O\   00 

m  CM  in  vo  o  ON   m 

CM  00  O  cO  CM  T-H   T-H 

CM  o  vo  r--  vo  m   ^t- 

in  CM  co   t--  vo   T-H 

in  in  m  m  in  in   in 

CM  CM  CM  CM  CM  CM    CM 

O 

co  vo  in  CM  r*>-  vo   vo 
co  co  oo  t"~-  vo  oo   oo 

cO  vo  CO  ON  Tj"  CO    O 

C*^  00  00  "<t  O  CM    Tf- 

TH  CN  CO  00  U 

O  CO    CO 

O  vo  CO  O  00  00    CO 

m  -<t  CM  TH  \o  -tf-   co 

m  LO  m  m  m  m   in 

cO  cO  cO  cO  cO  cO   CO 

CQ 
1 

,-,  T-H  T-H  ON  CO  00      T-H 

ON  ON  ON  VO  !>-  00    Cs 

o«ncM  m  CM  TH   T-H 

O  OTH  m  CO  •*•    !>• 

oo  Tt-  CM  TH  co  oo   m 

r-r-cM  Tf  o  CM   CM 

"*•  "^  CM  TH  \O  Tt-      >!*• 

CO  CO  CO  CO  CO  CO    CO 

cO  cO  cO  cO  cO  cO   cO 

School  A 

O  cO  ON  in  ^ 
ON  ON  00  vo  r 

4"  ON   vO 
-CO    CO 

CM  OO  VOr-H|>    xf 

o  co  T-H  m  *< 

*•  m   co 

TT  00  Tf-  00  CM  00    l> 

TH  TH  in  m  in  o-   ON 
•^-  m  CM  TH  in  co   CM 

???$; 

**  5 

General  accuracy 
Beyond  data 
Caution 
Crude  errors 

TV,,.*  f~l  — 

Insufficient  data 
Probably  true  —  prob- 
ably false 

O 


o  o  CM  o  vo  in 

00 

S 

TH  O  ^  ON  CO  vo 

fs.  O  TH  Tf  00  CM 

T-H 

§ 

«*•  Tf  CM  r^  \0  "^t 

Tj- 

co 

cO  CO  cO  cO  CO  CO 
CO  CO  cO  cO  CO  CO 

CO 

cO 

t*-  vQ  TH  CM  cO  vo 
CO  00  ON  f>-  00  00 

CO 

o  "*3"  "^i*  CM  r^~  in 

in 

s 

co  i>  CM  m  vo  TH 

T-H 

1 

00  ON  T)-  ON  TH  ^t" 

m  co  r-  co  co  m 
in  TH  co   t--  r-> 

CO 
CM 

CM  CM  CM  CM  CM  CM 
cO  cO  cO  cO  cO  cO 

CM 
cO 

vO  ON  vo  CO  TH  CM 
CO  CO  CO  CM  vO  00 

CO 
CO 

o 

CO  00  ^t-TH  00  O 
OO  ON  00  CO  CO  T-H 

* 

*§ 

co  oo  r—  T-H  oo  vo 

TH 

^ 

o  o  m  o  o  co 

m 

ON  ON  CN  ON  ON  ON 

ON 

r- 

CM  m  TH  r-  co  o 

CN  ON  CN  ""3"  vo  ON 

00 
00 

2 

Tf  O  m  ON  CM  vO 

O  CO  TH  CO  TH  00 

«n 

o 

O  ON  CM  r-  CM  TH 

TH 

CO 

CM  CM  xt-  CM  TH  TH 

in  rj"  CM  TH  vo  in 

ON 
CO 

CM  CM  CM  CM  CM  CM 

ON 
CM 

oo  Th  TH  m  TH  -rf 
OO  ON  CN  vo  t>  00 

00 
CO 

o  CM  in  T-H  TH  m 

ON 

"N 

ON  CM  CM  in  CM  CM 

TH 

•^ 

vo  oo  m  r-  ON  o 

ON 

co 

t*^  cO  O  CO  CO  ON 

•^t"  *^"  CO  rH  lO  Xl" 

O 
CO 

o  o  o  o  o  o 
m  m  m  m  m  m 

O 

in 

General  accuracy 
Beyond  data 
Caution 
Crude  errors 
True—  false 
Insufficient  data 
Probably  true  —  prob- 

j 

APPENDIX 


CM  ON  CO  rf  t^-  CN         O 
ON  CN  C\  \O  l>  CO         CN 

s 

T~<  G-N  CO  O  ^  CN         \O 
O  LO  TH  ^f  TH  cO         \O 

o 

NO  ^  CO  \O  CM  Tf         O 
LO  en  CM        \O  LO        TJ- 

co  en  en  co  co  co       co 

CO  CN  CN  "O  r*-  CN        CN 

"8 

CO  O  CN  f"--  CN  CM        xt* 
C^  ON  \o  cO  T-I  T-H        cO 

3 

*o  co  o  r  —  LO  Ti*      CM 

cO  eO  r^-  "^"  LO  r^»        e30 
VD  TH  CO         CO  CO         CM 

CS|  CM  CM  CM  CM  CM         CM 

00  CN  CM  ^J*  O  CO        CN 
CO  CO  CN  LO  CO  CO         CO 

5 

^d~vw  2 

a 

NO  CM  CM  CN  O  LO         \O 

ON  CO  CO  O  CO  r-<           -Tf 
"^*  CO  CM  T—I  r*-  NO         CM 

CN  CN  CN  CN  CN  CN        CN 

CN  LO  CM  r*-  CM  CO         CN 

CN  CN  CN  TJ"  VO  CO          OO 

DO 

•"*•  T-I  CM  OO  LO  LO         CO 

O  CM  O  CO  OT-H         \O 

1 

LO  \O  I>-  CN  NO  NO        SO 

Tj-  O  CO  T-I  TH  CM         TH 
LO  Tf  CM  TH  vo  NO         Th 

LO  LO  LO  LO  LO  LO            tO 

cO  cO  CO  cO  CO  cO        cO 

CM  vO  "<#"  LO  CN  CM         to 
CO  CO  OO  NO  LO  CO         OO 

^i 

CO  NO  CO  CN  O  CM         O 

r--  oo  co  ^t-  o  T-H      ^t- 

§ 

r**  co  co  co  f"»  CN      r*» 

•^-  *sJ-  CM  VH  so  xf       co 

LO  LO  LO  LO  LO  LO           LO 

"xtf"   -^-   T^-  ^  -rj-  xj*            'TJf' 

*   .t 

03                                    4-* 
JH                                   03     oj 

illjp 

512         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

TABLE    2 

Correlations  between  Scores  on  Form  2.51  (Corrected  for  Attenuation)  for 
284  Pupils  in  Two  Large  Public  High  Schools  not  in  the  Eight-Tear  Study 


Score 

Accuracy 
with 
True- 
False 

Accuracy 
with 
Probably 
True  and 
Probably 

Accuracy 
with 
Insuf- 
ficient 
Data 

Beyond 
Data 

Caution 

Crude 
Errors 

i 

False 

. 

General 

I 

Accuracy 

.766 

.650 

.786 

-.734 

-.132 

-.§80 

Accuracy  with 

true-false 

.470 

.314 

.075 

-.359 

-.882 

Accuracy  with 

probably 

; 

true  and 

1 

probably 

' 

false 

-.002 

-.052 

-.741 

-.301 

Accuracy  with 

insufficient 

data 

-.981 

.491 

-.tl9 

Beyond  data 

-.590 

.637 

Caution 

-.150 

APPENDIX 


5*3 


CO  S^ 

H  -S 

M  -Si 

$  4' 


l« 

^00<3t-ivOt-» 
oo  r-  co  ITS  co  t^ 

Qi 

OOOtN  OCOO\ 

b 

O"-i'<*tis-ioo 

<o 

O\CSO  CS  OO\ 

% 

focoo  t^oo 

O  CN  <N  *-<  10  t>- 

£ 

CS  <N  CN  CS  CS  CS 

^ 

<Nt^O«O^O 
O\  O\O\vOoO  O\ 

fe, 

O  OCN  00  <N  -H 

i 

•*  O  o  06  10  10 

i-t  CNTH        ^H  (M 

§ 

S 

l^  rt<^  rocot^. 

»O  -*<N       lOU-> 

£ 

ON  O\OsO\C>Ov 
CM  CSCN  CS  CN  CS 

^ 

*O  O  O  00  CQ  t-- 
O>  OM>-  IO  l^-  ON 

SQ 

IN  <N  OOOfOlO 

IO  <N  t>*t>.CN<N 

T-I  <s         »H  es 

«0 

»O«OIOOO<N  o 

s 

O  iN  rj*  00  f^.  *-. 

O  CO  ^H  r-l  O  O 

15 

M  «N  CS  M  IN  CN 

«N  cses  CSCNCS 

I* 

Moooooioa 

O\  00  t>.  t>.  00  00 

[VN 

8 

b 

fsooo^o 

$ 

S 

f^oo  fOOOO 
<0  fC  00  O  'H  0 
IOIO  vHCNNQ'* 

£ 

<O  vO  O  vO  vO  O 
<N  CN  CSCSCSCS 

IH 

OOOOCN     •     • 

oooo  r-1^    •    • 

^ 

>0«>vOOO    •    • 

•8 

,0 

O  f*  <N*  «O     •     . 

<o 

Co 

a 

lO*5t<J>.t>-      •      • 

oo"  "tf  t>I  O\    •    " 
Tt*iO(NTH     .     . 

£ 

oO  oo  oooo 
\O\OsOvO 

( 

j 

i 

5 

General  accuracy 
Beyond  data 
Caution 
Crude  error 
Agree  —  disagree 
Uncertain 

OO-^^r-CX 

ON  ON  00  •<*  00  00 

O 

OnOOf*}  (N«y5 

1 

CNO^vOOO 

<0 

'-j  \o  oo  cs  oo  es 
CN  O  O  ^  -^  -r-* 
Otocs  »-<\or^ 

oooooo 

^  ^  •*  rjt  rj<  ^# 

OQO"*  OOt^rJt 
00  O%  00  \ooooO 

b, 
*S 

fOO\ON  O(N  fO 
•^O  ^  odiorh 

8 

5 

00»-J  (NrOsOO 
»o*  *H  o^  10  r^  f) 

»O  "*  TH  T-l  If,  IO 

C^fCrOfDc^  fO 
CNCS  CMCSCS  CS 

•^OO  Ot^^H 
ONONOO^OOON 

Q 

»OfONOrO^  O\ 

CS  f^  (M  <O  IO  l>- 

^0 

•<*  (N  00  00  CS  O 

•*'  (N  ON  r^  <N  06 

VO  VO  -rH  iH  O  -* 

CSCSCNCSCN  CS 

ONOfOOO-MiO 

OOO\vOrJ<vO  00 

Kl 

1 

CS  CNlOCSO»>- 

*H  r>.  t%!  vo  ON  rH 

s 

OiNONvOt^-* 

»O  00  10  •*  00  ro 

NOPO-rHTHvOO 

cscs  cscscses 

r-oOPOO    •    • 
oooo^io 

•"! 

8 

COONOOON     •     • 
iN  OOO\I>     '.     I 

CO 

\OCNJ>f»«     •     " 

OfO"*  Ov    '•    '. 
lOvO--tTH     .     . 

VOvQvOO 

General  accuracy 
Beyond  data 
Caution 
Crude  error 
Agree  —  disagree 
Uncertain 

ONOCSOO-^  ON 
00  ON  <O  "*»•-  C. 

fcj 

CS  O\  00  Th  <N  fO 

1 

*-*NOO\OO\O 

to 

^qcs-MONoq 

!>•  t^-  (TJ  »O  ^  f5 

\O  r*5  r^*-t  ts.  \o 

•NCSCSCSCSCN 
««5  f*7  rf5  fO  «^>  CO 

OOOONOCNT^ 
ON  00  r-»  t>»  CO  00 

fe, 

§ 

10  00  10  00  CO  «0 

*4  rj5  Ox  00*  ro  ro" 

3 

'^'H^NOONON 

00  ON'  rH  \O  t^  00 
XD  CS  T-I  <-i  \O  O 

^^^^^^ 
cOfOfOcorotO 

l^  1-1  •*  co  00  *-• 

ON  CO  t»  T^IV,  co 

« 

"8 
o 

OV'iffS  Ot^PO 

co  c5  06  10'  "1  6 

S 

ONOvDvO  OCS 

rjJ  ro-^co  OvO 

vOCS-r-<^-H>.^. 

CO  CO  fO  co  ro  f*5 
CS  CSCSCSCSCS 

OOCOOOCOON 

ON  ON  t*  vO  00  00 

{j 

•r-4  CSiOCSOCO 

8 

\ONOoOoOroiO 

^ 

ON  t>-  00  PO  l>  O 

ON  CN  ••-(  rj<  cs  vO 
lOCO^H  »Ht>.vO 

00  OOOO  OOOOOO 

cscscscscscs 

'HCSO  «O     •     • 
ONO\OC  IO     '     * 

x 
1 

cooes  PO    •    • 

^2^  ;  ; 

£ 

t*»  «o  vo  q  •  •_ 

ON  O  to  t^           I 

»O-*TH   T-l        -        . 

io»om  10 

VONONOO 

General  accuracy 
Beyond  data 
Caution 
Crude  error 
Agree  —  disagree 
Uncertain 

5M        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


TABLE   4 

Correlations  between  Certain  Scores  on  Form  1.3bfor  283  Pupils 
in  Two  Schools  in  the  Eight-Tear  Study 


C/l 

•M 

^ 

5 

-w 

o 

w 

I—1 

S 

1 

03 

bo 

2 

1jj> 
&    M 

2 

to 

PH    cfl 

J3 

«  •§ 

A> 

fc.  ^ 

Si     »» 

U      « 

JH  .y 

M-l 

Score 

Accural 
Conclus 

bo 
2 

Percent 
Reason: 

|t 
ll 

Numbe 
Control 

Numbe 
Analogi 

Numbe 
Analogi 

lsr 

Column 

1 

8 

9 

12 

16 

18 

19 

25 

Uncertain  rea- 

sons, lack  of 

knowledge 

5 

.34 

Number  reasons 

7 

.15 

Right  reasons 

8 

.53 

Percent  reasons 

right 

9 

.68 

Number 

principles 

11 

.42 

Number  right 

principles 

12 

.53 

.85 

Percent 

principles  right 

13 

.62 

Number  right 

controls 

16 

.38 

.04 

Number  analogies 

18 

.17 

Number  right 

analogies 

19 

.21 

.70 

.59 

.01 

Number 

authorities 

21 

.61 

Number  right 

authorities 

22 

.58 

.40 

.01 

.54 

Percent  ridicule, 

Tel.,  A.  C. 

25 

-.46 

Percent 

inconsistent 

27 

-.20 

.24' 

APPENDIX 


5*5 


oo                   o  r-  VD          vo                                co      m 

* 

2 

vo  m          o                                CM      i  — 

tt 

o 

b 

T-»                        cO  l>-  r**             CN                                        vo        oo 

1 

28 

T—  1              TM                         T—» 

£^ 

1 

&z 

a 

2 

c^cooo           ov                                 a,      TH 

4 

2 

Tf                   oo  vo  oo          c\                                CM      r^- 
CM  T-i  m          c^                                T-i 

5 
••&• 

O  CO  O               OO                                              CM        CM 

b 

.^ 

f& 

T-I                           CO  OO  t  —               ON                                              ^O        t*- 

£ 

T-I    O 
_,    T-l 

T-l              T-l                         T-i 

^ 

%  \\ 

JH      'I 

Q 

« 

fi 

CO 

•s 

Pn^l 

CvJ 
(U 

o                    "^  vo  r--           \o                                 *-*      vo 

1 

2 

-^-                                    «rH  VO  CO                    CO                                                             CM           I"*- 

CO    TH    IO                        f^                                                                            T-l 

g 

^ 

o 

^ 

OOOcOOOOOOOOOOCOvOOO^T-iri-xtvOOOM-T-* 

lit 

•§ 

rt 

pq 

t-5 

D 

T-^T-lOc^cMcocovOlr)^rilnlr>eNcNTHeNT-^co^-cM^- 

XJ 

3 

« 

.<£> 

T-<              T-H               T—  1                         T—t 

cO 

jS 

"1 

cO  ^> 

s 

<*} 

.  CO 

T-<    00 

CM 

1 

^ 

^^^^C^^OOOO^T^^VOCMCOCMCM^^COCMI- 

^ 

1 

3 

1 

« 

2 
.s 

vOCMC7\COCMI>-O         mt^         T-tLnr-iOOvOT-ivO 

co  co  o  co  vo  oo  ON      Is*  r^      co  r-  vo  "f  vo  m  vo 

4-> 
fl 

<3 

t) 

13 

§" 

P^ 

{g 

<3 

O 

3 

T3 

C 

•£? 

e 

<u 

1 

1 

o 

C3                                                          „,: 

13 

fc&\ 

-3                                                                                                                                  QfT 

fc 

M                  *U-l                                                                                                      .  ^ 

5 

<s 

-a 

g      °           -           n    $<* 

•ig    -8             u5    -^    -1   l-r  - 

o 

4-> 
g 

££* 

•3 

2^*^           ,-2                   ^           "0*C             H            Q    £„     O  cf|j  '"5   "fl    •*•» 

^S 

PC 

.Sfl^^w        .^Pu.S  ««     §  83  ^  •£  "^   «^  £  8 

1 

-S 

CO 

^3yo§«i     '"''H^  a,'"^  *^  8  '5b  §  *s  iis  ^  cT*"  "^ 

fjj 

§ 

g'S  fl  |  ^  g  o  cs'C.Sp.S  o  .5P  a  §  s  .Spr§  ^  g  8 

S 

S  §  ^'2'§rSiSj^"SrD^^^^^^"5^"S 

* 

^HHcSDls'SfS^lSpiSzi^J^lSlzjJsJziPk^pS 

516         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


CD 

T-t 

S  5  S  S 

03 

00 

~ 

CO    00    CO 

CO 

5 

oo 

00    O 
CO    GO 

r-i 

^ 

g 

r--" 

uo 

Vl 

T-, 

s 

J^ 

OO 

r~-> 

CN 

oq 

CO 

tn 

*"-. 

Tf    LO            CO 

«T} 

\o 

\O      T-4                   *>• 

CD 

CO 

| 

\ 

uo 

LO 

t 

CO 

T-H 

^H 

o  o  oo 

CO    OO    CO 

CO 

CN1 

^^ 

\ 

S" 

oo" 

T—  < 

|    J 

^ 

SS3 

CO 

00 

4.  Si 

r 

oi 

CO 

T—  < 

"S3   "§ 

•1* 

CO 

s 

°i 

tn 

•§   'S 

un 

T-H 

!"¥ 

«, 

\D  OO 

c\  ca 

r 

to 
co" 

m 
m 

ed 

1  ^ 

CO 

m 

*4§ 

^      0 

^ 

to 

un 

*0 

£  ° 

•rf 

r—  I 

13 

J  1 

& 

^    M    00    *    U,    ^    ^    CO    C,    0 

.1 

*S       ^j 

3 

.S 

"?- 

1 

*"§ 

.s 

"-S 

OT 

s 

•S 

4^                    0 

fl 

*c* 

^3 

0 

°=J 

,_,        t-H          C^          C      *^~< 

'& 

D 

pj    J^    j>    ctf     G 

2 

rt 

w      j-j      CC      >    ^      ^    ^      ^ 

*> 

<u 

S 

^O  *£3  jy  13     tJ    S           g 

TJ 

a 

^2  *!  «  s  *i  '«  -s  5  S 

"S 

cti 

2 

rHSC^.|_».)_jC^'>jIjr* 

CJ 

T3 

«s 

SS.IIHl'Bll 
1§  ^^^  B  §|J^9 

<u 

s 

rt 
00 

** 

2^(^KS^fS<£<^ 

APPENDIX 


5*7 


1 

05 

£S 


r-    -§  * 


Inter 


1 


CO 

•r-t 

CN                          LO                          CO                                         CN                  i-l 

TH               o               o                       <N          in 

CNJ 

CO 

T-t 

CM 

5            S            8                  S      -R 

LO 
10 

£ 

T-l 

O                      *O                      xh                                    ^ 
LO                       CO                        O                                     t^ 

LO 

£ 

0 

r-i 

oo                 r-~                 o                           <ss 

T|-                        <N                        CO                                     00 

r- 

S 

CN 

o 

C<l 

00 

LO 

CO 

^                      0                      CN1 

o                r-<               in 

00 

t-1 

2 

"• 

CN                r-                TJ< 

LO                               ^t"                               t» 

CO 

2 

>0 

CO                \O  Ci 

\O                 LO  t-» 

LO 

T-l 

0 

LO 

CO                     b» 

cO 

^ 

^                r-. 

CO 

TH 

•* 

CN                       (N 

LO                               00 

3 

CN 

CO 

S  00 

s 

LO 

CM 

S 

CO 
CN 

cO 

o 

CO 

CO 

00 

fl 

a 

.HC^             TCU.XO             t-00             CX                     Ol-H             Nm 

"3 

u 

t    &- 

60        2         S          DO               S                                                  15 

.s  s  £    4  a  .s      *    u      «  «    -  .a 

HI    HI   ||    |      ||    || 

r! 

o 
o 

CO 

L,                           ""rt                     52                                                     1> 

ho             2             ^  Si       o                     TI  « 
•S             |              S  >      1          «         s  * 
|            1              ^^      1          |          S^ 

Deviation 

P^O                               n^                             *"^lS                 rS^                 r^H^                   C> 

^i   si    li  ^  *  ^i  ii 

fi 
<u 

tandard 

13   *fl                     Cti    'd                   ,,,   'tn            ^    'fl            "Trt    '?            U_i     ^ 

0|          0|          -S^       6f       S|      "Si 

CO 

Appendix  III 
TABLES  FOR  CHAPTER  III 


ceo 


TABLE  1 

Means,  Standard  Deviations^  and  Reliabilities  for  Test  1.41 


Grade  10 

Grade  11 

Grade  12 

Total 

Mean 

Sigma 

r 

Mean 

Sigma 

r 

Mean 

Sigma 

r 

Mean 

Sigma 

.r 

Total  Reasons1  

54.2 

11  1 

8? 

55  3 

13   7 

87 

46.8 

11  9 

8Q 

51  8 

12  9 

87 

Accurate  Reasons1  .  .  . 

37.5 

8  3 

,85 

39  8 

9  5 

.89 

36.5 

9  0 

.87 

37  9 

9  0 

87 

Ratfo1 

4  5 

98 

82 

4  9 

1   1 

87 

4  5 

1   1 

84 

4  6 

1    1 

85 

No   Inconsistent1  .... 

6.4 

4  2 

.78 

5   7 

4.3 

.76 

3.5 

2  6 

65 

5   1 

3  9 

77 

%  Inconsistent1 

9  1 

8  0 

8  0 

7  5 

4  8 

5  5 

7  2 

7  2 

Untenable2   . 

6.2 

2  5 

T> 

6  2 

2  6 

44 

4  4 

2  5 

S? 

5  5 

2   7 

SO 

Irrelevant2    

3.9 

1  9 

3  5 

2  2 

2.7 

1   7 

3  3 

2  0 

Undemocratic  Values2. 

6.4 

4.7 

84 

5  5 

4  8 

.86 

3.8 

4  2 

86 

5   2 

4.7 

86 

Democratic  Values2.  .  . 

22.3 

8  6 

90 

25.4 

8  9 

.91 

23.7 

9.3 

92 

23  8 

9  0 

.91 

Rationalization2  ... 

8.2 

3.1 

.54 

7.7 

3.7 

.72 

5.9 

3.0 

63 

7  2 

3.4 

67 

%  Democratic  Values3 

62  9 

16.4 

.70 

1  Computed  by  split-half  method. 

2  Computed  by  Kuder-Richardson  formula. 

3  Computed  by  correlating  two  forms  of  the  test  1.41  and  1.42. 


sis 


APPENDIX 

TABLE  2 

Reliability  Coefficients  for  Test  4.21-4.31 


519 


9th 
Grade 
(108) 

10th 
Grade 
(145) 

llth 
Grade 
(169) 

12th 
Grade 
(179) 

Total 
(601) 

Liberalism 
D      Col    1  

.74 

78 

78 

80 

79 

ER            2  

.77 

78 

80 

84 

81 

LU            3 

80 

83 

85 

86 

84 

R              4    .  . 

81 

84 

88 

86 

86 

N              5     .. 

66 

75 

79 

80 

77 

M              6  

.79 

84 

88 

86 

86 

Conservatism 
D     Col.    7  

.62 

70 

76 

72 

74 

ER            8  

.71 

73 

81 

78 

78 

LU            9 

70 

77 

79 

78 

77 

R             10 

83 

77 

83 

77 

81 

N             11 

66 

72 

75 

69 

72 

M            12    ... 

71 

79 

82 

80 

80 

Uncertainty 
D      Col.  13  

.72 

86 

86 

83 

85 

ER           14  

.81 

82 

82 

82 

82 

LU           15  

.82 

84 

.85 

.83 

84 

R             16  

.77 

74 

.79 

.83 

.79 

N             17  

M            18 

.78 
84 

.82 
84 

.80 
85 

,81 
v83 

.81 
84 

Consistency 
D     Col.  19     .    . 

.54 

42 

57 

32 

56 

ER          20..     . 

.42 

42 

.56 

.51 

.51 

LU          21  , 

.48 

57 

57 

57 

.57 

R             22    .. 

44 

44 

.58 

58 

54 

N             23  

.23 

.38 

.51 

.54 

.46 

M            24.... 

.32 

.55 

.68 

.65 

.61 

Totals 
Liberalism  

.93 

.94 

.95 

.95 

.95 

Conservatism  
Uncertainty  

.91 
.96 

.92 
.96 

.94 
.96 

.92 
.96 

.93 
.96 

Consistency  

.82 

.78 

.87 

.88 

.85 

52,0 


ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


I 


S 

O 


<u 

1 

o 


o 

"d 


O 


O   O   O   O   m   T-H  CM   OO   CO  en   SO   h- 

t>*  u""  r~-*  \o  u-J  oo  un  ^  xj-  *<3*  ^  to" 

r-4    \O    en    rH    CO    en r-    T-H    xf-    |>.    CO    Xh 

-vf-   CT*   ON    to"   tO*   O  O   O   CO   I>"   CM*   ^J- 

^_T-HT_<CS]^CVJ  ^-^^-<T_<T^T-<r-< 

LO 

en  CO  T-I  Tt"  1--  O  CA  to  CO  CNJ  CO  VD 

O  ^t"  T-"  to  oo*  CO  G\  f-*  VD  x|-  C7*  CO* 
CO  T-<  CM  T-H  V-H  CM 

so  LO  r-^  T-H  ON  vo  oo  o  CD  •<*•  r^-^Lh 

ocNod"^--rHr^  cr\LncNr^LOT-< 

vQ    -rj-    tO    'O    to    tn  T-I    <N    T-I    T-4    CM    O3 

OCr5T_,r-,OOCO  OOVDLDt^-^— ic<~5 

r^vDt^voLoK  -^^TfcnTt-'Lo 

o  "r-4  oo  m  T-H  \o  to  co  un  -^t-  "^j-  co 

x^^-HCNunvdcN  CTVLOC^LO^CO 

^  ^  ^  ^  ^  ^  ^-  vq  r^  "sh  ^  r-; 

oivdcn-oovd  odvOLncood-o 

CO   TH    CM   T-H    CM   <N 

CM" 

cJCMco-r-iijom  oo^~ioocMcni>- 

in  \d  vd  06  o  tn  vocNLOTt-cOvo 

SO   to    \Q    \Q    up   \Q T— <    CM    T-H    T-H    CM    T-I 

T— ^\oco^"Ovo  r~*  T-<  T— »  i>-  o  co 

r^ioj>-*vdvoo6  LOJjOLOTfxfvo" 

^4   f^.    cO    CO    !>•    Tj"  CO    T-I r-o~XO    NO  "OO 

^CX?OvdvOT-4  r-Hr^'-st'""^^^ 

T-«     T-H      CM      CM      T-H      CM T~«      T-4      V-<__X— I      T-I      V- ' 

r^  !>•    ijO   00    O   CO  CN   en    OO   ^H    TH    CM 

CXJCOaNCOOOr-^  CAI>^XlDlOC7NC7N 
CM    r-<    T-<    Y-H    T-H    CM 

en  to  CM  un  CD  CM  oo  tn  ON  en  <m  T-H 

r-^  «jn  ^  r^  o  en  c\  ^  cd  t-4  u-J  en 

to  rj-  LQ  LQ  up  LQ  T-H  CM  T— i  CM  CM  CM 

oo^-cor---^'^  CN""^c7\T-i\O'O 

so  in  \d  LO  LO  r-*  ^t-  -^  TJ-  ^d-  M-  uo 

QQ     Q  QQ  j^  ^  [-^  TH  OO  CN 

cTNxhcnr^cM'cn 

cr\  \o  ^  CM  en  Tf  xf  T-H  -^h  en  -rH  T-H 

c^encDLnoocM*  OOOI^'^-CTNCTC 

CM  T-H   CM   T-H   T-H   CM  TH 

r—  ^j-  r^  CM  r-  T-I  oo  T~<  \o  o\  CM  oo 

cr\tn\dencJv6  or^<oi>^i^ScM 

tn  xf  to  sQ  LQ  to  CM  CM  <N  T-H  CM  CM 

cncMcMovD'O  LocM^cncnoo 

\d  xn  so  to  ^t-*  \o*  T^  T}-*  ^  -^  Tf"  -^ 

tO   Xf    CM    C\    C\    Tf-  C7\    TH    t— i   O    CTx    O 

<N  r-"  t-^  CD  CM*  vo"  oo"  ^"  <N  06  T-H"  CM" 

r-.   T-H   CM   uT)  \O   C7\  LO   O   CD  OO   CM   en 

cTstocD'or^T-"  THo6o6enT-HCD* 

CNr-<CMT-<T"HCM  *-•                             TH    T-« 

cn  CM  o_  oo  CTN  o-   ON  oo" T-H  c-- o~r-- 

c\  o  vo"  o5  co  rh  CM"  o  CM*  «jo  T-H"  uo 

tn  to  to  \o  xh  un  CM  CM  CM  T-H  en  CM 

s  i  i  i  I  M|  i  ;  ;  I  II 

.jj  ......  ~Q  

J  Q  «  J  &  &  %  |  Q  ^  2  pj  ^*  S 
3  CJ 


APPENDIX 


521 


0 

to 

CM  O 

vo  m 

vO 

P-. 

CM  xt- 

oo 

LO 

CN 

00  O 

p- 

^ 

to 

sO  "*sl~ 

tn  m 

CO 

CM 

CO  CM 

CM 

CO 

CO 
CM 

0  00 
CM  CM 

CM 

o 

to 

TH   OO 

xf  CM 

so 

CN 

LO  CO 

tn 

to 

tn 

xf  0 

f^ 

2 

CO 

P-  vO 

m  vo 

^ 

r- 

P-  0 

to 

p- 

T}- 

O  xf 

CM 

CN 

00 

o  to 

TH   CN 

^ 

UO 

CM  OO 

TH 

o 

ON 

-rf  ON 

vO 

CN 

p- 

OO  xfr- 

co  p~- 

2 

^ 

0  P^. 

ON 

CM 

CM 

cO  cO 

tn 
m 

oo 

TH 

CM  P- 

rt  CO 

in 

CM 

^-  0 

tn 

CM 

rf 

f-v_  o 

so 

ON 

so 
CM 

CM  OO 

CM   TH 

CM  CN 

CM  TH 

p- 

10 

O 

to 

vo  tn 
m  vo 

O 
tn 

o 

sO 

to 

TH  CM 
CM  CM 

tn 
tn 

CM 

CM 

xh  co 

Tf  vo 

vo 

P-- 

TH  in 

O 

so 

vO 

vO  CM 

m 

vO 

m 

m  xj- 

to  to 

co 

CM 

CO  CM 

CO 

co 

CN 
CM 

CO  tO 
TH  CM 

CO 

xf- 

cO 

ON  TH 

O  r—  I 

to 

CN 

O  CO 

r-- 

o 

CO 

cO  vO 

to 

CM 

p- 

xf  OO 

tn  xj- 

2 

p- 

P-  0 

VO 

00 

Tf 

C\  CM 

co 

ON 

r- 

tO  CM 

CM  CO 

CO 

m 

CM  cO 

CM 

ON 

CM 

CM  xh 

xi- 

OO 

vo 

SO  xj- 

r-  P- 

to 

CO 

TH  OO 

ON 

CM 

CM 

P-  ON 

CO  xf 

ON 

r- 

CO 

1-1  vO 

O  CO 

O 

r- 

Tf  CO 

CM 

vO 

TH 

SO  P- 

xf 

£ 

CM 
CM 

oo  p- 

TH   TH 

O  OO 

CM   TH 

so 

so 

CM  CO 

vO  vo 

to 

vO 

CM 

vO 

CO  Tf 
TH  CM 

ON 
in 

o 

oo 

CO   CO 

00  O 

CO 

ON 

CM  vo 

ON 

OO 

O 

vO  O 

to 

OO 

to 

'°   ^ 

to  p- 

CO 

CM 

CO  CM 

CM 

CO 

O 
CO 

CO  O 
CM  cO 

co 

0 

CM 

CO  OO 

O  vo 

TH 

O 

vO  P- 

0 

ON 

o 

CO  O 

to 

2 

CN 

oo  P- 

so  r-- 

to 

CN 

P*.   TH 

vO 

CO 

TH1 

TH  LO 

cO 

tn 

TH 

"*•  co 

TH  O 

o 

vo 

CN  CO 

ON 

tn 

CM 

xj-  CM 

P-- 

s 

ON 

ON  tO 

CN  O 

TH 

TH" 

r- 

ON  p- 

CO 

TH 

m 

o 

xf  0 

xt-  in 

m 

o 

CM 

CM  1-1 

CM  ON 

ON 

vO 

CM  CM 

p1*- 

vo 

vO 

CM  TH 

p*- 

CO 

CO 

vO  CM 
CM  CM 

to  xr 

CM  CM 

m 

LO 

o 
m 

to  TH 
to  vo 

ON 

Tj- 

m 

CM 

tO 

CM  to 
CM  CM 

in 

CM 

vO 

TH   SO 

r*-  vo 

CO 

m 

CM  CM 

VO 

CM 

Tj- 

CO  OO 

CO 

p- 

m 

vO  CO 

m  vo 

CO 

CM 

CO  CM 

CM 

CO 

to 

CM 

CN  OO 
TH  CM 

0 

•Tf 

oo 

0  0 

p-  xf 

0 

TJ- 

to  m 

m 

TH 

P~- 

CN  xf 

CO 

Tl- 

oo 

p-  to 

m  vo 

CO 

vo 

p-  oo 

xi- 

vO 

CM 

CN  Xf 

O 

ON 

CO 

CM  00 

CO   TH 

vO 

vO 

sO  P~- 

TH 

p- 

CM 

O  OO 

0 

CN 

oo 

oo  rh 

CO  ON 

TH" 

r- 

CN  P- 

CN, 

TH* 

ON 

O 

TH 

so  "3- 

to 

p- 

00 

CN  OO 

TH   OO 

tO 

p*- 

vO  O 

xh 

"^t" 

VO 

O  xf 

o 

CN 

TH 

p- 

CM 

CM  CN 

CM  1-1 

CO  CM 
CM  CM 

OO 

LO 

0 

tn 

CO  Tf 

to  vo 

0 

tn 

OO 

to 

tO 

CO  CM 
CM  CM 

m 
in 

O 

CM 

CO  vo 

O  CO 

vO 

m 

OO  CM 

CO 

p- 

CM 

O  CM 

co 

vO 

to 

tO  CO 

tO  vO 

cO 

CM 

CM  CM 

CM 

CM 

CM 

CN  sO 

TH* 

0 

Tf 

0  TH 

o  p- 

CO 

to 

sO  ^h 

ON 

xi- 

TH 

to  TH 

CO 

CM 

Is- 

VO  tO 

xj-  m 

2 

VO 

m  oo 

CM 

cO 

CM 

CN  CO 

TH 

O 

TH 

CO  vO 

vO  P- 

CM 

co 

^f  o 

CM 

ON 

CM 

CM  vO 

Tt* 

CN 

P- 

r-  co 

P-  00 

CO 

p^- 

ON  OO 

CO 

o 

0 

OO  TH 

to 

O 

OO 

CO  O 

O  P- 

p- 

CN 

CO  Tf 

vO 

xh 

T~t 

TH   00 

if 

CO 

CO 
CM 

TH  m 

CM  TH 

CM  CM 

CM 
uO 

CO 

CM  vo 

tn  vo 

3 

tn 

tO 

to 

xh  O 
CM  CM 

m 

£* 

J£  +* 

o 

OT 

CO  .Q 

B 

ct 

1, 

g  g 

"3 
1 

% 

P  . 

J  p4 

X* 

'lo 
cS 

& 

3* 

z 

* 

la 

0  G 

U  P 

e 

522         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

TABLE  4 

Intercorrelations  of  Certain  Scores  on  Scale  of  Beliefs  4.21-4.31 


Score 

Liberalism 

Conservatism 

Totals 

D  IER 

LIT 

R 

4 

N 

D 

ER 

LU 

R 

N 

Lib. 

Con. 

Unc. 

Liberalism 
ER  Col.  2 

1  !    2 

3 

5 

7 

8 

9 

10 

11 

25 

26 

27 

59! 

54 

54 

76 

.57 

59 

57 

73 

—  .37 

—  .40 

—  .33 

LIT              3 

70  J   64 

R                4 

59;   30 

N               5 

64!   43 

52 

M               6 
Conservatism 
ER             8 

~5~9;   33 

51 

52 

LU             9 

71 

62 

R              10 

61 

31 

N              11 

61 

42 

61 

M             12 

.52 

32 

48 

45 

Totals 
Lib.         25 
Con.         26 

Unc.        27                                                                                                 —  69 

Consi.      28                                                                                                          .  66 

—  42 

Appendix  IV 
TABLES  FOR  CHAPTER  IV 


Students  from  a  large  public  senior  high  school  are  the  only  ones  who  have  taken  the 
final  revised  form  3.32.  Eleven  classes,  distributed  as  follows,  constituted  the  population. 

TABLE  1 


Grade 

Boys 

Girls 

Total 

10 

56 

59 

115 

11 

46 

56 

102 

12 

52 

66 

118 

Total 

154 

181 

335 

523 


524        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 

TABLE  2 

Means,  Standard  Deviations,  and  Estimates  of  Reliability  of  "Appreciation"  Scores 
on  Parts  7,  //,  and  III  of  Form  3.32 


Mean 


Part  7  (35  items) 

Grade  10 57.0 

Grade  11 61.8 

Grade  12 66.6 

Part  II  (40  items) 

Grade  10 47.0 

Grade  11 52 . 4 

Grade  12 ' 55.4 

Part  III  (25  items) 

Grade  10 49.7 

Grade  11 57.0 

Grade  12 53,6 

Total  (100  items) 

Grade  10 61.6 

Grade  11 51.6 

Grade  12 53.3 


17.89 
17.09 
17.66 


18.18 
18.84 
17.73 


17.09 
19.32 
17.38 


15.75 
16.84 
14.33 


.85 
.84 
.85 


.86 
.88 
.86 


.73 
.80 
.77 


.92 
.94 
.91 


APPENDIX 


525 


TABLE  3 

Means,  Standard  Deviations,  and  Estimates,  of  Reliability  of  " Non- Appreciation" 
Scores  on  Parts  7,  //,  and  III  of  Form  3.32 


Mean 


Part  I  (35  items) 

Grade  10 , 36.8 

Grade  11   ,  32  3 

Grade  12 29 . 2 

Part  II  (40  items) 

Grade  10 47.8 

Grade  11 40.8 

Grade  12 39.5 

Part  III  (25  items) 

Grade  10 41.5 

Grade  11 34.2 

Grade  12 39.2 

Total  (100  items) 

Grade  10 42,3 

Grade  11 36 . 1 

Grade  12 35.0 


17.28 
16.26 
17.01 


17.08 
18.60 
15.45 


17.90 
17.76 
15.94 


15.24 
16.35 
14.53 


.84 
.83 
.85 


.84 
.88 
.82 


.77 
.79 
.74 


.92 
.94 
.92 


526         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


TABLE  4 

Means,  Standard  Deviations,  and  Estimates  of  Reliability  of  "Uncertain"  Scores  on 
Parts  7,  //,  and  III  of  Form  3.32 


Mean 


Part  I  (35  items) 

Grade  10 8.0 

Grade  11 7.9 

Grade  12 6.1 

Part  II  (40  items) 

Grade  10 7.5 

Grade  11 8.8 

Grade  12 7.5 

Part  III  (25  items) 

Grade  10 8.4 

Grade  11 10.3 

Grade  12 10.1 

Total  (100  items) 

Grade  10 8.2 

Grade  11 8.5 

Grade  12 ' 7  A 


8.98 
7.63 
6.75 


8.01 
9.21 
8.39 


11.67 
11.89 
11.82 


8.45 
8.50 
8.17 


.81 
.78 
.74 


.79 
.84 
.80 


.79 
.83 

.77 


.92 
.93 
.92 


APPENDIX 

TABLE  5 


527 


Means )  Standard  Deviations,  and  Estimates  of  Reliability  of  "Appreciation"  Scores 
on  Parts  II A,  IIB,  IIC,  and  IID  of  Form  3.32 


Mean 


Part  HA  (10  items) 

Grade  10 67.2 

Grade  11 73.6 

Grade  12 73.8 

Part  HB  (10  items) 

Grade  10 32.1 

Grade  11 36.5 

Grade  12 41.4 

Part  IIC  (10  items) 

Grade  10 48.8 

Grade  11 52.2 

Grade  12 54.1 

Part  IID  (10  items) 

Grade  10 54.8 

Grade  11 60.9 

Grade  12 65.8 


20.01 
21.14 
21.60 


23.57 
25.10 
25.67 


24.01 
24.99 
20.21 


23.85 
25.45 
25.66 


.51 
.65 
.66 


.73 
.75 
.74 


.68 
.73 
.59 


.67 
.73 

.75 


528         ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


TABLE  6 

Means,  Standard  Deviations ,  and  Estimates  of  Reliability  of  "Non- Appreciation'* 
Scores  on  Parts  II A,  HB,  IICy  and  IID  of  Form  3.32 


Mean 

er 

r 

Part  IIA  (10  items) 
Grade  10   

30.7 

19.23 

.53 

Grade  11        

23.9 

16.69 

.49 

Grade  12         

8.4 

18.00 

.56 

Part  HB  (10  items) 
Grade  10   

68.4 

24,77 

.72 

Grade  11        

61.3 

28.32 

80 

Grade  12  

58.7 

27.98 

.79 

Part  IIC  (10  items) 
Grade  10         

56  5 

24  40 

69 

Grade  11  

51.7 

24.55 

.72 

Grade  12  

50.4 

20.07 

.58 

Part  IID  (10  items) 
Grade  10  

48.5 

24.19 

.69 

Grade  11  

39.6 

23.04 

68 

Grade  12  

36.8 

23.64 

73 

APPENDIX 


529 


TABLE  7 


LIST  OF  PAINTINGS  USED  IN  THE  TEST 


Wo.          Artist 

1.  Picasso 

2.  Michelangelo 

3.  C6zanna 

4.  Corot 

5.  van  Gogh 

6.  Vermeer 

7.  van  Gogh 
& 

8.  {Rembrandt 

9.  Durer 

10.  Mainardi 

11.  Breughel 

12.  Rembrandt 

13.  El  Greco 

14.  Hals 

15.  Gauguin 

16.  Breughel 

17.  Gorot 

18.  Kokoschka 

19.  Rembrandt 

20.  Gauguin 

21.  Durer 

22.  van  Gogh 

23.  Lorenzo  di 

Credi 


Name  of  the  Painting 
The  Absinth-drinker 
Head  of  Adam 

Peasant 

Girl  with  Pearl 
Self  Portrait 

Portrait  of  a  Young  Girl 
Self  Portrait 
Self  Portrait 
Self  Portrait 

Portrait    of    a    Young 

Man 
The  Winter 

A  Boy  Reading 
View  of  Toledo 
A  Fool  with  a  Mandolin 

Farm  at  the  Pouldu 
The  Summer 

Paysage 

Towerbridge,  London 
Jakob  blessing  Joseph's 
sons 

Landscape  in  Britanny 
Self  Portrait 

Dr.  Gachet 
Portrait  of  a  Girl 


Collection — Catalogue 
Hamburg  Museum 
(Detail)  Creation  of  Adam — • 

Sistina,  Rome 
Conger  Goodyear,  New  York, 

(Venturi,  No.  687) 
Louvre  (Robaut,  No.  1507) 
V.  W.  van  Gogh — Amsterdam 

(De  la  Faille  No.  344) 
Hague,  Royal  Gallery   (Hof- 
stede, No.  44) 
Museum,  Amsterdam  (De  la 

Faille,  No.  522) 
Kunsthist.    Museum,    Vienna 

(Hofstede,  No.  580) 
Pinakothek-Muenchen  (Tietze 

No.  164) 
K.  Friedrich  Museum,  Berlin 

(Cat.  No.  86) 
Kunsthist.    Museum,    Vienna 

(deLoo  A  24) 
Kunsthist.    Museum,    Vienna 

(Hofstede  No.  238) 
Metropolitan  Museum,   New 

York  (A.  L.  Mayer,  No.  315) 
G.  de  Rothschild,  Paris  (Hof- 
stede No.  98) 
Collection  Vollard,  Paris 
Metropolitan  Museum,  New 

York  (deLoo  A  25) 
Louvre,    Paris    (Robaut,   No. 

1625) 

Museum,  Hamburg 
Gallery,  Cassel  (Hofstede,  No. 

22) 

Collection  Mesnard,  Paris 
Prado,    Madrid    (Tietze,   No. 

152) 
Gallery,  Frankfurt  M.  (De  la 

Faille  No.  753) 

K.  Friedrich  Museum,  Berlin 
(Cat.  No.  80) 


530        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


JVb.          Artist 
24.  Picasso 


25.  Cezanne 

26.  Vermeer 

27.  Diirer 

28.  Corot 

29.  El  Greco 

30.  van  Gogh 

31.  Hals 

32.  G6zanne 

33.  Breughel 

34.  Kokoschka 

35.  El  Greco 

36.  van  Gogh 


Name  of  the  Painting 
The  Guitarist 

The  Card  Players 
The  Kitchenmaid 


Collectio  n — Cat  alog  ue 
Art  Institute,  Chicago  (Zervos: 
Picasso  1895-1906,  No.  202) 
Louvre,    Paris    (Venturi    No. 

558) 
Collection  Six,  Amsterdam 

(Hofstede,  No.  17) 
Hieronymus  Holzschu-     German  Museum,  Berlin  (Tie- 

her  tze  No.  957) 

Interrupted  Reading         Art    Institute,    Chicago    (Ro- 

baut,  No.  1431) 
Art  Institute,  Chicago  (A.  L. 
Mayer,  No.  298) 
Collection  V.  W.  van  Gogh, 
Amsterdam  (De  la  Faille  No. 
405) 

Museum,    Leipzig    (Hofstede 
No.  96) 
Collection     George     Renard, 

Paris  (Venturi  No.  307) 
Kunsthist.    Museum,    Vienna 

(deLoo  A  26) 

Flowers  on  the  Window    Munich 

Mater  Dolorosa  Munich  (A.  L.  Mayer,  No.  86) 

Blossoming  Almond  Collection  V.  W.  van  Gogh, 

Spray  Amsterdam  (De  la  Faille  No. 

392) 

37.  Michelangelo  Adam,  Creation  of  (Detail]  Sisu'na,  Rome 

Adam 

38.  Rembrandt      A  Young   Girl   at   an    Art  Institute,   Chicago    (Hof- 

Open  Half  Door  stede  No.  324) 

39.  Cezanne  Basket  of  Apples  Art  Institute,  Chicago   (Ven- 

turi No.  600) 

40.  Hals  The  Gipsy  Girl  Louvre,   Paris   (Hofstede  No. 

119) 


St.  Martin  and  the 
Beggar 
Pear  Tree  in  Blossoms 


A  Mulatto 
A  Village 
The  Autumn 


APPENDIX 


531 


TABLE    8 

LIST  OF  PAINTINGS  USED  IN  THE  COMPARABLE  FORM 


Jio. 
1. 

2. 
3. 
4. 
5. 
6. 
7. 
8. 

9. 
10. 


Artist 
Breughel 

Bronzino 
van  Gogh 
Rembrandt 

Roger  v.  d. 

Weyden 
Ambrogio  da 

Predis 
Modersohn- 

Becker 
Breughel 

Gauguin 
Michelangelo 


Name  of  the  Painting 
The  Peasants'  Wedding 

Bia  de  Medici 
Sun  Flowers 
Self  Portrait 

The   Knight   with   the 

Arrow 
Portrait  (Beatrice 

d'Este) 
Still-life  with  Flowers 

Fight  of  Lent  with  Gar- 
nival 

The  Girl  with  the  Fan 

Head  of  the  Prophet 
Jeremiah 

Cardinal  Fernando 
Nino  Guevara 

Portrait  Nicolas  di 
Sforzore 

The  Smoker 


11.  El  Greco 

12.  Memling 

13.  C6zanne 

14.  Vermeer  A  Lady  at  the  Virginals 


15. 
16. 

17. 
18. 

19. 

20. 


M.  Laurencin  Portrait  of  a  Girl 
C6zanne  Vase  of  Tulips 


R.  Dufy 
van  Gogh 

Carl  Hofer 
van  Gogh 


21.  D6gas 


22. 
23. 

24. 


D6gas 

Modersohn- 
Becker 
Breughel 


Window  in  Nice 
Portrait  of  an  Old 
Peasant 

Girls  Throwing  Flowers 
The  Zouave 

Woman  Drying  her 
Neck 

Girls  Ironing 
Still-life  with  Fruits 

The  Unfaithful  Shep- 
herd 


Collection — Catalogue 
Kunsthist.    Museum,    Vienna 

(deLoo  A  27) 
Uffizi,  Florenz   (A  McComb, 

p.  61) 
Collection  V.   W.   van  Gogh 

Amsterdam  (De  la  Faille  458) 
Louvre,   Paris   (Hofstede  No. 

569) 
Museum,  Brussels 

Ambrosiana,  Milan 
Museum,  Hamburg 

(Detail)    Kunsthist.    Museum, 

Vienna  (deLoo  A  2) 
Folkwang  Museum,  Essen 
Sistina,  Rome 

Metropolitan   Museum,   New 

York  (A.  L.  Mayer,  No.  331) 
Spinelli     Museum,     Antwerp 

(Weale:  Memling  p.  13) 
Kunsthalle,  Mannheim  (Ven- 

turi,  684) 
Royal  Collection  Windsor 

(Hofstede,  No.  28) 
Pallas  Gallery 
Art  Institute,  Chicago   (Ven- 

turi,  617) 

Art  Institute,  Chicago 
Collection  Bernheim  jeune 

Paris  (De  la  Faille  444) 
Art  Institute,  Chicago 
Collection    Unger    =     Mens, 

Rotterdam  (De  la  Faille  424) 
Louvre,  Paris 

Louvre,  Paris 


Pennsylvania  Museum  of  Art 
(deLoo  A  29) 


532 

JV0.          Artist 
25.  Goya 


ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


26.  Winslow 

Homer 

27.  Rousseau, 

Henri 

28.  C6zanne 


Name  of  the  Painting 

The  Bandit  Margate, 
Shot 
The  Gulf  Stream 


The  Cascade 
Man  in  a  Cotton  Cap 
29.  Rembrandt      Self  Portrait 


30.  Rubens 


Portrait  of  a  Bearded 
Man 


Collectio  n — Cat  alog  ue 

Art  Institute,  Chicago  (A.  L. 
Mayer,  No.  597e) 
Art  Institute,  Chicago 

Art  Institute,  Chicago 

Museum  of  Modern  Art,  New 

York  (Venturi,  73) 
Kunsthist.    Museum,    Vienna 

(Hofstede  581) 
Liechtenstein,  Vienna 


31.  Barent 

Eli  and  Samuel 

Art  Institute,  Chicago 

Fabritius 

32.  C6zanne 

Seine  at  Bercy 

Kunsthalle,   Hamburg    (Ven- 

turi 242) 

33.  Rousseau, 

Summer 

Collection  Flachfeld,  Paris 

Henri 

34.  van  Gogh 

Montmartre 

Art  Institute,  Chicago  (De  la 

Faille  272) 

35.  El  Greco 

St.  Francis  and  the 

Art  Institute,  Chicago  (^.  L. 

Skull 

Mayer,  No.  267) 

36.  Corot 

The  Haywagon 

Collection    Dollfus    (Robaut, 

No.  1117) 

37.  Vermeer 

The  Lacemaker 

Louvre,  Paris  (Hofstede  No.  11) 

38.  Manet 

Mile.  Victorine  as  an 

Metropolitan  Museum,   New 

Espada 

York 

39.  Corot 

Morning  on  the  Lake 

Robaut,  No.  1625 

40.  Gauguin 

Tahitian  Woman  with 

Art  Institute,  Chicago 

Children 

41.  Winslow 

Adirondacks  Guide 

Art  Institute,  Chicago 

Homer 

42.  Carl  Hofer 

Landscape  in  the  Tessin 

43.  Goya 

Boy  on  a  Ram 

Art  Institute,  Chicago 

44.  Chardin 

Girl  Scraping  Vege- 

Liechtenstein, Vienna  (Wild- 

tables 

enstein,  No.  46) 

45.  Vermeer 

Lady  with  a  Lute 

Metropolitan   Museum,   New 

York 

46.  Dufy 

Regatta  at  Deauville 

Louvre,  Paris 

47.  D6gas 

L'Absinth 

Louvre,  Paris 

48.  Breughel 

The  Crash  of  Ikarus 

Museum,  Brussels 

49.  C6zanne 

The  Aqueduct 

Museum  of  Occidental  Art, 

Moscow  (Venturi  477) 

APPENDIX 


533 


fl 

1) 

TS 

5 

•s 


H 

M 

^      < 

W        H 

»    S 

H      M 
>_1 


8 
I 


£ 
o 


a 


•o 

rt 

cS 


WFpS03[0;5I 

TH 

i> 

O 

ON 

in 

£ 

T[20-J-)  ITBA 

s 

<M  CNJ  en  CN  NO  T* 

o 

T-l 

« 

O 
O 

3^3 

2 

T-I  TH  CM  en  en  TM 

O 

T-t 

« 

8 

y 

30IOQ 

CN 

- 

en 

0 

CN 

O 
CN 

1 

oaamftA 

5 

THTHTHT. 

- 

o  • 

- 

O 
0 

2 

<d 
O 

VauqoK* 

o 

T,            CM^T,^ 

NO 

0 

* 

o 
f- 

1 

SFH 

ON 

TH  TH  TH  CM  en  CN 

en 

« 

o 
o 

pqfta* 

CO 

T-l  TH   CN  Cn  TH 

en 

0 

M 

o 

ON 

OD3JO  13 

- 

~-«  >-« 

en 

0 

« 

0 

J3JTIQ 

* 

CN  CN  CN  en 

en 

0 

en 

O 

opStrepipi^ 

m 

^ 

TpJBUIBW 

-ipajQ 

-<*• 

^ 

SLa  • 

..  $-< 

sjsq.iv 
jo  jsqtiirv^jj 

« 

m  TH  en  o  en  T-* 

8 

LO 

o 

2 

s 

00 

fiS 

4J    -M 

OnT2"yr 

CM 

•«*•  m  i>  o  oo  so 

8 

5 

8 

in 

o 

0 

88 

H° 

-SJCIBJT      C[ 

-, 

enenoent-O 
en  "*•  o  oo  co  -<*• 

8. 

« 

<n 

r^ 

CO 

m 
m 

03 

a 

4j 

"0 

1 

a 

3 

t 

I 

1 

0 
A 

t* 

O 

$ 

JD 

+3 

3 

3 
A 

1 

O 

3 

^ 

i 

-g| 

3 

1 
o 

cimum  p 

c§ 

•M 

s 

0 
o 
CO 

8 

T) 
<D 

S 

a 

3 

^•S 

^ 

2 
co 

OS 

s 

j 

& 

s 

g 
O 

534        ADVENTURE  IN  AMERICAN  EDUCATION 


i 

o    O 

Sijg 
a 

3  ^o 


§2 

•&  o 


0? 


d  >• 

•Ss 

•s-.s 


jood 
jnpqnop  jo 


jnpqnop  jo 


jood 


Xjqtsso^ 


^njjqnop  jo 


pnjjqnop  jo 
pooS  AJ3A 


Ajqtssod 

AJ3A 


pooS 


sjxrapnjs  jo 


T 


•»-<  CO  <N  CO 


J3      8 

'o    *a 


8-Ss 


*IIS 

s  g:S 


iff 

a  "S  a 

TJ    <U    P 

a  o-a 


£ 


is 

si 


•» 


Appendix  V 
TABLE  FOR  CHAPTER  V 


Reliabilities,  Means,  and  Standard  Deviations  for  "Like"  of  the  Different  Categories 
in  8.2afor  a  Population  of  542  Students  (261  Boys,  281  Girls')  in  the  11th  Grade 


No.  of 
Items  in 
Category 

Category 

Mean 
% 

Sigma 
% 

r 

No.  of 
Items  in 
Category 

Category 

Mean 
% 

Sigma 
% 

r 

24 

Soc.          Total 

39  3 

27  2 

.92 

16 

Home      Total 

50  8 

28.6 

88 

Sci.        Boys 

41   6 

27  6 

Eco-n.     Boys 

30  8 

21  5 

80 

Girls 

37.1 

25.4 

Girls 

69  4 

22.2 

.81 

16 

Biology   Total 

45  6 

27.8 

87 

16 

Ind.         Total 

50  8 

25.6 

.82 

Boys 

44  4 

29  4 

Arts       Boys 

59  2 

26  2 

Girls 

46  6 

26.3 

Girls 

42  9 

23.3 

16 

Phys.       Total 

50   1 

29.5 

89 

16 

Fine         Total 

45  8 

30  0 

89 

Sci.         Boys 

60  4 

27  6 

87 

Arts       Boys 

33  2 

26  8 

87 

Girls 

40  6 

28  0 

.88 

Girls 

57  4 

28.2 

.88 

16 

English    Total 

48  7 

26  4 

85 

16 

Music      Total 

46  8 

29  4 

.89 

Boys 

39  5 

27  0 

Boys 

37  0 

28.6 

Girls 

57  2 

23  0 

Girls 

55  8 

27.3 

16 

Foreign   Total 

47  4 

31  0 

90 

16 

Sports     Total 

55  2 

23  5 

.79 

Lang.     Boys 

36   7 

29  8 

Boys 

56  8 

24  3 

Girls 

57  2 

29.0 

Girls 

53  6 

23.6 

16 

Mathe-    Total 

36  3 

29  0 

89 

38 

Manipu-  Total 

45  2 

19  1 

85 

matics  Boys 

45  8 

29  2 

lative     Boys 

41  6 

19.2 

Girls 

27  6 

25  6 

Girls 

48  2 

18  3 

16 

Busi-        Total 

55  9 

23  6 

.80 

35 

Read-      Total 

47  0 

22  6 

.90 

ness       Boys 

56.8 

24  3 

ing         Boys 

45   8 

23  6 

Girls 

55.0 

23.9 

« 

Girls 

48  0 

22.5 

535 


Appendix  VI 
TABLES  FOR  CHAPTER  VI 


TABLE    1 

Ranges^  Means,  Standard  Deviations,  and  Reliabilities  of  the  Different  Categories 
in  8.2b  from  a  Random  Sample  of  1000  Students  (7th  Grade  through  12th  Grade} 


No.  of 
Items  in 
Category 

Category 

Likes 

Dislikes 

Range 

Mean 
% 

Sigma 
% 

r 

Range 

Mean 
% 

Sigma 
% 

r 

19 

25 

32 
16 
28 
10 
18 
26 
16 
16 
16 
16 

Aggression                 .  .  . 

0-94 

0-100 
0-100 
0-100 
0-99 
0-100 
0-100 
0-74 
0-100 
0-100 
0-94 
0-100 

28.9 

51.8 
49.3 
34.6 
48.5 
55  0 
48  8 
21  3 
35.0 
42  2 
26.1 
39.8 

.  18.2 

19  8 
19  4 
26.2 
20  2 
1  23.2 
21   2 
12   1 
22.8 
24.8 
21.4 
21.8 

:  .75 

.80 

.84 
.86 
.85 

:  .62 

.76 
,  .62 
.  .79 
,  .82 
.80 
.77 

0-99 

0-95 
0-90 
0-100 
0-95 
0-91 
0-95 
0-99 
0-100 
0-100 
0-95 
0-95 

:  36.2 

,  16.9 
21  4 
27.4 
19  5 
21   9 
18  9 
47.2 
22.7 
21.1 
28  0 
23.8 

19.4 

13.2 
14  0 
"  24.2 
IS..  5 
17.3 
14  5 
15   8 
19  8 
19  6 
21   8 
18  4 

,76 

,72 
.78 
.86 
.79 
59 
.68 
.71 
.79 
.79 
.80 
.75 

Out-of~School  Activi- 
ties .      »  

Family 

Dramatics 

Opposite  Sex 

Same  Sex      

School  Activities  .  . 
Authority  

Leadership 

Fantasy 

Magic                     . 

Mystery    

536 


APPENDIX 


537 


TABLE    2 

Ranges,  Means,  Standard  Deviations,  and  Reliabilities  of  the  Different  Categories 
in  8.2c  from  a  Random  Sample  of  1000  Students  (7th  Grade  through  12th  Grade) 


Likes 

Dislikes 

No.  of 

Items  in 

Category 

Category 

Range 

Mean 

Sigma 

r 

Range 

Mean 

Sigma 

r 

14 

Aggression  

0-100 

31.6 

20  8 

.73 

0-95 

32  5 

20  2 

.71 

16 

Severity  .  . 

0-90 

33  5 

18  6 

70 

0-95 

29  9 

16  6 

64 

24 

Life-Death-Universe 

0-99 

33  0 

22  6 

.86 

0-99 

26  9 

21.0 

.86 

26 

Preoccupation  with 

Cleanliness 

0-90 

47  2 

18  6 

.78 

0-80 

22  6 

13  6 

.73 

24 

Humor  

0-99 

47  0 

19.8 

.80 

0-80 

21   9 

15  4 

.76 

24 

Self  -acceptance  

0-95 

42  5 

19  4 

.78 

0-85 

28  7 

15  8 

.72 

25 

Methodical     

0-100 

42.0 

20  2 

.81 

0-95 

23  0 

17.3 

.79 

16 

Identification  with 

Others 

0-100 

49.0 

23  4 

.79 

0-85 

16  9 

13.6 

.62 

16 

Non-identification 

with  Others  

0-90 

34  2 

20  0 

.73 

0-95 

30  8 

18  0 

.66 

18 

Solitary  

0-85 

40  0 

15.4 

.53 

0-85 

33.5 

15.3 

.56 

INDEX 


Ability,  level  of,  in  score  interpreta- 
tion, 435-436 

Ability  to  Apply  Social  Facts  and 
Generalizations  test,  168-175;  be- 
havior, analysis  of,  in,  172-173; 
criteria  for  appraisal,  173-174;  ob- 
jective, analysis  of,  168-169 

Achievement,  analysis  of,  in  Appli- 
cation of  Principles  test,  104-111 

Achievement  tests,  inadequacies  of 
early,  3-4 

Activities:  out-of -school,  369;  school, 
evaluation  of,  368-369;  records, 
use  of,  166;  records,  reliability  and 
validity  of,  330 

Adaptation,  role  of,  in  adjustment, 
354 

Adjustability  (see  also  Adjustment), 
social,  482 

Adjustment:  maturation  and  adapta- 
tion in,  354;  meaning  of,  350- 
354;  optimum,  353-354 

Administration  of  Evaluation  Pro- 
gram, 439-459 

Administrative  problems  in  obtain- 
ing records,  449-450,  454 

Adolescents:  interests  of,  316;  verbal 
expression  of  art  statements  by, 
279  n 

Aggression,  evaluation  of,  366-377 

Aims  (see  Objectives) 

Ambivalence:  between  general  and 
specific  values,  241-242;  in  social 
beliefs,  431-432 

Analogy  as  type  of  behavior,  49, 
101 

Analysis,  power  and  habit  of,  in 
Behavior  Description,  480 

Analysis  of  Controversial  Writing 
test,  150-154;  conclusion  concern- 
ing, 154;  scoring,  152-154;  sample 
problems  in,  151-152;  criteria  for 
selecting  items,  150 


f^*f^^f^f»f^fm.f^m.f<f^f^m.f^.f^^^A'*f^Atf^ftff^f^f»f^»f^^f^'' 

XV\  VxVXVs  XVx  xSNrxVv^SLS^ 

"Anecdotal  method"  of  recording, 
466 

Anecdotal  records:  criteria  for  se- 
lecting, 163  n;  inadequacies  of, 
in  evaluating  art  appreciation,  279; 
social  sensitivity,  160-161, 163-164 

Application  of  principles  of  logical 
reasoning:  evaluation  instruments, 
development  of,  114-122;  objec- 
tive, analysis  of,  111-114  (see 
also  Application  of  Principles  of 
Logical  Reasoning  test) 

Application  of  Principles  of  Logical 
Reasoning  test,  111-126;  readiness 
of  class  for,  126;  sample  problem 
in,  119-121;  scores,  summary  and 
interpretation  of,  122-124;  state- 
ments, kinds  of,  in,  121;  validity 
and  reliability  of,  124-126 

Application  of  Principles  test,  77- 
111;  analogy  of  statements  in,  101; 
authorities,  statements  of,  in,  101; 
construction  of,  80-111;  data  sheet 
sample,  102;  directions  for,  ex- 
ample, 88-89;  errors  in  responses, 
83-84;  essay-type  vs.  objective 
form,  84-85;  problem  situations 
in,  80-111;  reasons  for  responses 
in,  82-84;  sample  problem,  89-90; 
scores,  summary  and  interpreta- 
tion of,  103-111;  social  values 
tested  in,  95-101;  types  of  re- 
sponses in,  81-82 

Application  of  Science  Principles 
(see  Application  of  Principles) 

Applying  Social  Facts  and  Generali- 
zations to  Social  Problems  test 
(see  also  Ability  to  Apply  Social 
Facts  and  Generalizations),  175, 
197-203;  behaviors  evaluated  in, 
197-198;  description  of,  198-203; 
objective,  analysis  of,  197-198; 
sample  exercises  in,  199-203;  uses 
of,  243 


539 


540 


INDEX 


Appraisal  (see  Evaluation,  Reports, 
Tests) 

Appreciation,  Aspects  of  (see  also 
Appreciation  of  Art,  of  Literature, 
of  Social  Values),  245-312 

Appreciation  of  art  (see  also  Art): 
evaluation  of,  276-312 

Appreciation  of  literature,  246-276; 
meaning  of,  in  the  Study,  246; 
behaviors  in,  249;  test  of,  250-276 

Appreciation  of  social  values,  use 
of,  240 

Areas  of  activity  (see  also  Areas 
of  Living),  interest  tests  and,  318 

Areas  of  Living,  interests,  role  of, 
in,  317-318 

Areas  of  thought  in  Behavior  De- 
scription, 485 

Argumentum  ad  Hominem  principle, 
112 

Art  (see  also  Art  Appreciation, 
Painting,  Art  Experience,  expres- 
sion, sensitivity),  interest  in,  tests 
of,  277 

Art  Appreciation  (see  also  Art  Ex- 
perience ) :  assumptions  concern- 
ing, 283-285;  evaluation  of,  276- 
312;  meaning  of  term,  280- 
281,  283-284;  objectives,  276- 
277;  psychology  of,  280-283;  rec- 
ords, inadequacies  of  most,  279; 
test  (see  Art  Appreciation  test) 

Art  Appreciation  test  ( see  also  Find- 
ing Pairs  of  Pictures  test):  ad- 
ministration of,  299-300;  assump- 
tions underlying,  283-285;  criteria 
for,  279-280;  287-289;  description 
of,  289-292;  development  of,  283- 
289;  interpretation  of,  292-299; 
reliability  of,  300-301;  score  range, 
304;  scoring,  292-294;  use  of,  306- 
307;  validity  of,  301-303,  305- 
306 

Art  Experience  (see  also  Art):  and 
creativity,  282-283;  emotional  re- 
action in,  285;  meaning  of,  281; 
methods  of  data  gathering,  277- 
278;  nature  of,  285;  spectator's 
role  in,  281-283 


Art  expression  and  "Gestalt"  psy- 
chology, 280-281 

Art  History  as  an  Academic  Study, 
283-284 

Art  sensitivity,  meaning  of,  283-284 

Art  teaching,  purposes  of,  276 

Art  test  (see  Art  Appreciation  test) 

Art  values,  sensitivity  to,  276-277, 
278 

Art  and  verbal  facility,  278-279 

Artist's  reactions,  283-284 

Arts  (see  Art,  Dramatics,  Theater) 

Aspects  of  Appreciation  (see  Ap- 
preciation ) 

Assumptions,  basic,  of  Evaluation 
Staff,  11-15 

Assurance  in  Behavior  Description, 
485 

Authority,  reactions  to,  370 

Background  data  in  one  case  study, 
409-410 

Battery  of  instruments,  reasons  for, 
406-408 

Behavior:  central  pattern  of,  430- 
431;  classifications,  351-352,  484- 
485;  combinations  of,  determined, 
433-434;  descriptions  (see  Be- 
havior Description);  deviant,  hy- 
potheses concerning,  431-432; 
motivation,  role  of,  in  evaluating, 
351;  objectives  defined  in  terms  of, 
19-20;  organic  unity  of,  7,  405; 
patterns,  11-12,  13,  19-20 

Behavior  Description,  470-487;  ad- 
vantages of,  486-487;  classifica- 
tions in,  484-485;  on  college-en- 
trance blank,  496-497;  Commit- 
tee on,  470;  data  interpretation, 
functions  of,  in,  403-404;  Manual, 
485-486,  496;  records,  279,  466, 
471,  474-487,  493;  in  subject 
fields,  501-502 

Belief:  as  type  of  social  attitudes, 
205;  instruments,  208-209 

Beliefs  About  School  Life  test,  208, 
229-234;  results  of,  in  one  school, 
437 

Beliefs  on  Economic  Issues,  charac- 
teristics of,  235-236 


INDEX 


541 


Beliefs  on  Economic  Issues  test,  208, 
234-238 

Beliefs  on  Housing,  209 

Beliefs  on  Social  Issues  test,  208- 
234;  consistency  evaluated  in,  223; 
data  sheet  sample,  221-225;  de- 
scription of,  215-229;  evaluation 
of,  209-215;  honesty  in,  225-226; 
language's  role  in  validity  of,  225; 
reliability  studies  of,  228;  sample 
statements  in,  216-217;  sampling 
and  statement  formulation  in,  209- 
212;  score  patterns,  224;  scores, 
interpretation  of,  220-225;  scoring 
and  summarizing  results,  217-220; 
uncertainty  evaluated  in,  222; 
validity  and  reliability  of,  225- 
229 

Beyond  data,  55,  56,  62,  408 

Bibliography  of  evaluation  instru- 
ments, 21-22  n 

Biology  (see  Science) 

"Birth-Life-Death"  fantasies,  370 

Carnegie  Foundation  for  the  Ad- 
vancement of  teaching,  494  n 

Carroll,  Herbert,  246 

Case  study,  one,  based  on  test  data, 
408-429 

Caution  score,  55 

Changes  (see  also  Growth,  Student 
Growth ) :  behavior,  as  educational 
objective,  11-12;  diagnoses  of,  by 
tests,  242-243;  in  school  practices, 
resulting  from  evaluation,  457;  in 
school  programs,  resulting  from 
evaluation,  436-437;  in  students, 
evaluation  as  check  of,  436 

Checklists:  data  summaries  of,  in 
reading  interests,  334-337;  validity 
of,  333-334 

Chemistry  (see  Science) 

Classroom  situations  as  source  of 
evaluation  data,  446 

Classroom  teacher  (see  Teacher) 

Cleanliness,  preoccupation  with,  366 

"Clear    thinking"    objectives,    35-37 

College:  changes  in  information  for 
admission  to,  495;  Committee  on 
Admission,  report  to,  494-498; 


"Junior  Year"  blank  for,  498;  trans- 
fer from  school  to,  form,  494-498 

Committee:  on  Admission  to  Col- 
lege, report  to,  494-498;  on  Evalu- 
ation in  the  Arts,  276;  on  the 
Evaluation  of  Interests,  313;  on 
the  Evaluation  of  Interests  and 
Appreciations,  245;  on  the  Evalua- 
tion of  Reading,  247;  on  Evalua- 
tion and  Recording,  xx;  on  the 
Interpretation  of  Data,  38;  on  Re- 
ports and  Records,  464;  on  School 
and  College  Relations  of  the  Edu- 
cational Records  Bureau,  495;  on 
the  Study  of  Adolescents,  349 

Community  (see  also  Home,  Parents, 
Public  Relations ) ,  evaluation's 
role  in  school's  relations  with,  10 

Compulsiveness,  evaluation  of,   366 

Conservatism:  beliefs,  213;  in  social 
beliefs,  217-219;  terms,  as  indi- 
cating direction,  213,  217,  220 

Consumer  aspect  of  applying  logical 
principles,  114 

Content,  course,  as  means  to  ends, 
11 

Controversial  Writing  test,  analysis 
of,  150-154 

Cooperative  planning  (see  also  Eval- 
uation program  planning),  440- 
442 

Counselor  (see  also  Teacher):  In- 
terest Questionnaire,  value  of,  to, 
345-347,  396-399;  interpretation 
of  evaluation  data  by,  452;  inter- 
views with,  in  one  case  study,  413- 
417 

Course  revision,  evaluation  in,  26- 
27 

Creation  (see  also  Creativeness), 
meaning  of,  474 

Creativeness:  in  art  experience,  282- 
283;  in  appreciation  of  literature, 
248,  251;  characteristics  of,  474- 
475;  evaluation  of,  475-476;  and 
Imagination  in  Behavior  Descrip- 
tion, 474-476,  478 

Critical-mindedness  in  Reading  of 
Fiction  test,  265-267 


542 


INDEX 


"Critical"  thinking  (see  Clear  think- 
ing, Logical  Reasoning) 

"Crude  errors"  in  data  interpreta- 
tion, 47,  55 

Cultural  activities  in  one  case 
study,  417-418 

Curiosity  in  Appreciation  of  Litera- 
ture test,  248,  251 

Curriculum:  based  on  hypotheses, 
7-8;  changes  in,  resulting  from 
evaluation,  436-437;  effectiveness 
of,  appraised,  453-454;  improve- 
ment of,  one  purpose  of  evalua- 
tion, 403,  432-436;  Reading  ques- 
tionnaires in  appraising,  275-276; 
and  school  program  (see  School 
program ) 

Dale,  Edgar,  328  n 

"Dartmouth  Visual  Survey,"  473  n 
Data  (see  also  Evaluation  Data); 
classifications  of,  41-42;  criteria  for 
selection,  42-43;  dependability  of, 
evaluating,  40;  interpretation  of, 
38-76;  kinds  of,  for  interpreta- 
tion, 41-43;  presentation  of,  forms, 
41;  selection  and  use  of,  31-32; 
sources  of,  42 

Deductive  thinking,  78 

Definitions  principle,  112 

Democracy  (see  also  Democratic): 
as  interest  area  in  social  issues, 
209;  liberalism  and  conservatism 
regarding,  217;  in  school,  229 

Democratic:  meaning  of  term,  183; 
attitudes,  evaluation  data  useful 
in  developing,  457;  tenets  (see 
also  Social  Problem  values),  175, 
179;  values  appraised  in  Social 
Problems  test,  183-184,  187 

Descriptive  Trait  Profile,  358,  383- 
384,  388 

Devices  (see  Instruments,  Tests) 

Directing  Committee  of  the  Study, 
3-4 

Drama  Questionnaire,  253,  264 

Dramatics,  interest  in,  371-372 

Drives  and  impulses,  organization 
of,  364-367 


Economic  issues  (see  also  Beliefs 
on  Economic  Issues  test),  beliefs 
on,  234-238 

Economic  relations:  as  interest  area 
in  social  issues,  209;  liberalism 
and  conservatism  regarding,  217- 
218 

Education:  continuity  of,  494-495; 
purpose  of,  11 

Eells,  Walter  Crosby,  326 

Emotional  adjustment  fostered  by 
the  arts,  276 

Emotional  control  in  Behavior  De- 
scription, 485 

Emotional  disposition  and  "aca- 
demic" interests,  396 

Emotional  Responsiveness,  in  Be- 
havior Description,  481 

Emotional  tendencies,  interpretation 
of  (see  Interests  and  Activities 
Questionnaire,  interpretation  of ) 

"Empathy,"  280 

Environment  and  individual,  rela- 
tionship, 468 

Essay-type  test:  criticisms  of,  84-85; 
and  Form  2.52,  correlation  be- 
tween, 67-73 

Esthetic  experience,  280,  283 

Evaluating,  habit  of,  33 

Evaluation  (see  also  Evaluation 
Data  Tests ) :  continuity  of,  essen- 
tial, 438,  442;  complexities,  rea- 
sons for,  6-7;  definition  of,  in  the 
Study,  5;  influences  of,  on  teach- 
ing and  learning,  14;  interpretation 
of  ( see  also  Interpretation ) ,  6,  25- 
28;  methods,  selection  of,  21-23; 
role  of,  in  educational  process, 
29-30;  purposes  of,  7-11,  403,  432- 
437;  results  of,  use  of,  454-459; 
school's  responsibility  for,  14; 
traditional,  inadequacies  of,  146; 
whole-faculty  responsibility  for, 
438 

Evaluation  adviser,  458 

Evaluation  of  Art  Appreciation  (see 
also  Art  Appreciation),  276-312 

Evaluation  data:  assumptions  under- 
lying, 405-408;  available  in  plan- 
ning program,  445;  case  study 


INDEX 


543 


illustrating  synthesized,  408-429; 
circulation  of,  444-447,  449-454; 
collection  of,  methods,  444;  faculty 
attitude  toward,  445-456;  in  guid- 
ance, 430-432;  interpretation  of, 
and  teachers,  437-438;  interpreta- 
tion and  user  of,  403-438;  nature 
of,  405-408;  sources  of,  446;  sum- 
marizing and  circulating,  449- 
454;  synthesized,  case  study  illus- 
trating, 408-429;  uses  and  interpre- 
tation of,  403-438 

Evaluation  devices  (see  also  Evalua- 
tion instruments,  Tests),  develop- 
ing and  improving,  23-25 

Evaluation  Instruments  (see  also 
Tests):  Bibliographies  of,  21-22; 
development  of,  43-60;  need  for 
new,  4 

Evaluation  of  Interests  (see  also  In- 
terests), 313-348 

Evaluation  of  Personal  and  Social 
Adjustment  (see  also  Personal  and 
Social  Adjustment  test),  349-402 

Evaluation  Program:  concept  of,  by 
teachers,  442;  division  of  labor  in, 
28-29;  interpretation  and  uses  of 
data  in  (see  also  Data,  Interpreta- 
tion), 403-438;  as  integral  part  of 
school,  459;  limitations  in  plan- 
ning, 443-444;  as  method  of 
teacher  education,  30;  misconcep- 
tions about,  442;  needs  served  by, 
443;  planning  and  administering, 
439-459;  procedures  in  develop- 
ing, in  the  Study,  15-28;  purpose 
of,  442;  scope  and  emphasis  of, 
441-444;  summary  of,  459;  sum- 
mary of  planning  and  administer- 
ing, 439 

Evaluation  specialist,  inadvisability 
of  having,  440 

Evaluation  Staff:  basic  assumptions 
by,  11-15;  members  of,  4,  5 

Evaluation  techniques,  wide  range 
of,  needed,  13-14 

Examinations  (see  Tests) 

Experimentation  in  creativeness,  475 

Extrapolation,  39,  45-46 


Faculty  (see  also  Counselor,  School 
Staff,  Teacher):  attitude  of,  to- 
ward evaluation  data,  455-456; 
continuity  of  study  and  collective 
thinking  by,  454-456;  participa- 
tion of  whole,  in  evaluation  pro- 
gram planning,  441,  457-458;  re- 
sponsibility of  whole,  in  securing 
data,  446 

Family  relationships,  evaluation  of, 
367 

Fantasy:  "Birth-Life-Death/*  370; 
behavior,  370-371;  in  Interest  and 
Activities  Questionnaire,  364,  370- 
372 

Feelingtone,  type  of  social  attitude, 
205 

General  accuracy,  definition  of,  in 
test  response,  51 

General  science  (see  also  Applica- 
tion of  Principles  test,  Science), 
test  construction  for  applying 
principles  in,  80-111 

Generalizations  ( see  also  Application 
of  Social  Facts  and  Generaliza- 
tions), testing  for  formulation  of, 
24-25 

"Gestalt"  psychology  and  art  expres- 
sion, 280-281 

Grades  and  awards  (see  also  Marks, 
Reports),  as  area  in  Beliefs  about 
School  Life  Test,  232 

Group:  life,  as  area  in  Beliefs  about 
School  Life,  231;  progress,  proc- 
esses to  -estimate,  433-436 

Growth  (see  also  Changes,  Pupil 
growth);  group's,  measure  of,  433- 
436;  individual,  reports  of,  489- 
490 

Guidance  (see  also  Counselor)  con- 
tinuity of  fostered  by  records, 
465;  evaluation  data,  use  of,  in, 
8-9,  430-432,  454-455;  reports  in, 
492;  and  transfer,  recording  for, 
463-504 

Gullibility,  153 

Habits,  work,  appraisal  methods 
needed  for,  31-33 


544 


INDEX 


Home  reports  (see  also  Parents,  Re- 
ports), 488-493;  records  as  bases 
of,  465;  and  teacher  reports,  iden- 
tical, 492 
Homeroom  teacher,  evaluation  data 

summaries  interpreted  by,  452 
Hoskins,  Luella,  329  n 
Housing,  Beliefs  on,  test,  209 
Human  Behavior  (see  Behavior) 
"Human   Relationships"   as   area  in 
Interests  and  Activities  Question- 
naire, 364,  367-370 
Humor,  activities  in  expressions  of, 

372 

Hypotheses,  validation  of,  as  one 
purpose  of  evaluation,  7-8 

Identification:  in  appreciation  of  lit- 
erature, 248,  251;  with  others, 
evaluation  of,  368 

If-then  principle,  112 

Imagination  in  creativeness,  475 

Impressing  others,  activities  in,  369 

Impulses  and  drives,  organization  of, 
as  area  in  Interests  and  ctivities 
Questionnaire,  364-367 

Indirect  argument  principle,  112 

Inferences:  in  data  interpretation, 
39;  test  to  measure,  60-62 

Influence  in  Behavior  Description, 
478-479 

Inquiring  Mind  in  Behavior  Descrip- 
tion, 479 

"Insight,"  meaning  of  term,  398- 
399 

Instruments  (see  Evaluation  Instru- 
ments, Tests) 

Intelligence,  general,  relation  of,  to 
Social  Problems  test  results,  196 

Intercorrelation  of  scores  in  Interpre- 
tation of  Data  test,  59  n 

Interest  (see  also  Interest  Index,  In- 
terest Questionnaire,  Interests ) : 
and  appreciation,  distinctions  be- 
tween, 245;  in  Reading  test,  valid- 
ity and  reliability  of,  330-334 
Interest  Index  (see  also  Interest 
Questionnaire,  Interests  and  Ac- 
tivities Questionnaire),  338-348; 


areas  in,  339;  in  one  case  study, 
415;  data  sheet  sample,  341;  in- 
terpretation of,  340-345;  uses  of, 
347-348 

Interest  Questionnaire  (see  also  In- 
terest Index,  Interests  and  Activi- 
ties ) :  analysis  of,  sample,  377; 
and  checklists,  337;  construction 
of,  338-340;  use  of,  in  developing 
personality  test,  358-359,  360- 
361;  value  of,  to  counselor  and 
teacher,  345-347 

Interests  (see  also  Interest,  "Interest 
Index%  Interest  Questionnaire,  In- 
terests and  Activities,  Recreational 
Interests):  "academic",  and  emo- 
tional dispositions,  -°96;  adolescent 
vs.  adult,  316;  data  sources  for 
revealing,  313;  evaluation  of,  313- 
348;  as  index  of  personality  pat- 
tern, 359;  as  means  and  ends, 
313-314;  objectives,  analysis  of, 
313-318;  questionnaire  (see  also 
Interest  Questionnaire),  338-348; 
patterns  of,  as  revealed  by  check- 
lists, 334-337;  recreational  (see 
Recreational  Interests ) ;  signifi- 
cance of,  in  personality  evalua- 
tion, 359-360;  uniqueness  of,  344 

Interests  and  Activities  Question- 
naire (see  also  Interest  Question- 
naire): administration  of,  400; 
areas  in,  364;  categories  in,  363- 
372;  criteria  for  item  selection  in, 
362-363;  drives  and  impulses,  or- 
ganization of,  as  area  in,  364-367; 
interpretation  of,  372-384,  390- 
392;  interpretation  of,  to  students, 
399;  validity  of,  387-396 

Interpolation  in  interpreting  data, 
39 

Interpreter,  importance  of,  in  tests, 
154-155 

Interpretation  (see  also  Interpreta- 
tion of  Data):  ability  to  make 
original,  67-74;  ability  to  judge 
by  others,  65-67;  behavior  descrip- 
tions^ one  function  of,  403-404; 
functions  of,  403-405;  over-all,  by 


INDEX 


545 


staff  member,  452-453;  overgen- 
eralized,  46;  undergeneralized,  47 

Interpretation  of  Data  (see  also  In- 
terpretation of  Data  test),  38-76; 
accurate,  46;  classifications  of 
types  of,  46-47;  original  vs.  stated, 
40-41,  65;  types  of,  45-46 

Interpretation  of  Data  test,  47-60; 
appropriateness  of,  for  high-school 
level,  67;  construction  of,  48-51, 
66;  form  of,  for  junior  high  school, 
63-65;  forms  of,  67-73;  reliability 
of,  74-76;  response  patterns  to, 
73;  validity  of,  65-76 

Interpretation  and  Uses  of  Evalua- 
tion Data,  403-438 

Interschool  Committee,  28-29 

Judging  the  Effectiveness  of  Writ- 
ten Composition  test,  265,  267- 
268;  Junior  High  school:  Applica- 
tion of  Principles  test  for,  91;  n; 
Interpretation  of  Data  test  for, 
63-65 

"Junior  Year'*  blank,  498 

Kuder-Richardson  formula,  65 

Labor  and  unemployment:  as  inter- 
est area  in  social  issues,  209;  lib- 
eralism and  conservatism  regard- 
ing, 218 

Language  ( see  also  Words ) :  choice 
of,  in  statements  of  social  beliefs, 
210-212 

Leadership,  activities  in,  369 

Learning,  influence  of  evaluation  on, 
14 

Liberalism:  meaning  of  term,  213, 
217,  220^  in  social  beliefs,  217- 
219 

Life,  philosophy  of,  appraised,  34 

Literature  (see  also  Appreciation  of 
Literature,  Recreational  Interests), 
appreciation  of,  246-276 

Logical  reasoning:  behaviors  in,  112- 
113;  meaning  of,  111-112;  test  of 
(see  Application  of  Principles  of 
Logical  Reasoning) 


Magazines  (see  also  Reading  maga- 
zines) :  checklist  of,  326;  classifica- 
tion of,  by  types,  326 

Maladjustment  ( see  also  Adjustment, 
Personal  and  Social  Adjustment), 
kinds  of,  353 

Manipulation  in  creativeness,  475 

Marks  ( see  also  Grades  and  Awards, 
Home  Reports,  Parent  Reports, 
Reports,  Teacher  Reports):  for 
college  admission,  inadequacies  of, 
488-489,  494;  and  interests,  316; 
as  objectives,  494;  in  records  and 
reports,  467,  468 

Maturation,  role  of,  in  adjustment, 
354 

Methodical  activities,  evaluation  of, 
366 

Methods,  evaluation:  means  to  ends, 
11;  selection  and  trial  of,  21-23 

Militarism:  as  interest  area  in  social 
issues,  209;  liberalism  and  con- 
servatism regarding,  218 

Motivation:  personal  and  social  ad- 
justment study  yields  insight  into, 
401;  role  of,  in  evaluating  be- 
havior, 351 

Movies,  checklists  for  revealing 
recreational  interests  regarding, 
328-329 

Mystery-interests,  371 

Nationalism:  as  interest  area  in 
social  issue,  209;  liberalism  and 
conservatism  regarding,  219 

Nature  of  proof,  36;  assumptions  in, 
129;  behaviors  in  achieving,  129; 
definition  of,  127-129;  objective, 
analysis  of,  126-130;  senses  in  ar- 
riving at,  128;  test  (see  Nature  of 
Proof  test) 

Nature  of  Proof  test  (see  also  Na- 
ture of  Proof),  126-148;  check  on 
responses  to,  144-147;  develop- 
ment of,  130-141;  sample  prob- 
lems in,  132-134,  136-139;  scores, 
summary  and  interpretation  of, 
141-143;  structure  of,  135-141; 
validity  and  reliability  of,  143- 
148 


546  INDEX 


Objectives:  agreement  on,  needed  for 
evaluation,  6;  analysis  of,  38-43; 
application  of  logical  reasoning, 
analysis  of,  111-114;  application 
of  scientific  principles,  77,  111;  an 
evaluation  program,  areas  of,  406; 
"breaking  up,"  405;  changes  in 
behavior  patterns  as,  11-12;  classi- 
fication of,  16-18;  "clear  thinking" 
as,  35-37;  "comprehensive,"  406; 
concern  in  evaluation,  5;  defining, 
in  terms  of  behavior,  19-20; 
evaluation  data  collection  regard- 
ing, 444-446;  evaluation  program 
as  a  check  on  achievement  of,  432- 
436;  formulation  of,  15-16;  gen- 
eral and  specific,  relation  between, 
441-442;  of  growth  reports,  489- 
490;  illustrations  of,  12;  "intangi- 
ble," 439;  interests  as,  317-318; 
interests  and  appreciations  as,  245; 
limited  overemphasis  on,  reasons 
for,  xvi;  marks  as,  494;  propa- 
ganda analysis,  149-150;  record 
forms  for,  in  subject  fields,  500- 
502;  in  records,  463-469;  re-ex- 
amination of,  essential,  16;  re- 
formulation of,  30;  selection  of, 
basis  for,  15-16;  situations  showing 
achievements  of,  20-21;  state- 
ments of,  inadequacies  of,  xv;  in 
subject  fields,  study  of,  499-500; 
teacher  consideration  of,  465-466; 
types  of,  18;  working,  for  records 
and  reports,  467-469 

Omissions,  scoring  of,  in  test,  55 

Open-mindedness  in  Behavior  De- 
scription, 479-480 

Organization  of  Impulses  and  Drives, 
as  area  in  Interests  and  Activities 
Questionnaire,  364-367 

Out-of-school  activities,  evaluation 
of,  369 

"Overcaution"  in  data  interpretation, 
47 

"Overcritical"  students,  97 

Painting  (see  also  Appreciation  of 
Art,  Art),  field  of,  chosen  for  art 
test,  286 


Parents  (see  also  Community,  Home 
Reports ) :  participation  of,  in  sug- 
gesting areas  of  social  beliefs,  207; 
reading  questionnaires,  results  of, 
for  parents,  275;  reports  to,  488- 
493;  reports  of  evaluation  useful 
to,  456;  security  of,  fostered  by 
evaluation,  9-10 

Pencil-and-paper  tests,  44;  use  of, 
in  collecting  evaluation  data,  446- 
447 

Personal  adjustment  (see  also  Ad- 
justment, Maladjustment,  Person- 
ality): meaning  of,  350;  and  social 
adjustment  (see  Personal  and  So- 
cial Adjustment) 

Personal  and  social  adjustment  (see 
also  Interests),  206;  appraisal, 
techniques  of,  354-358;  cleanli- 
ness, preoccupation  with,  in,  366; 
differentiation  between,  350;  eval- 
uation of  (see  also  Personal  and 
Social  Adjustment  test),  349-402; 
interests,  significance  of,  in,  359- 
360;  objective,  history  of,  349- 
350;  summary  regarding,  400- 
402 

Personal  and  Social  Adjustment  test: 
characteristics,  desirable,  of,  355- 
358;  Interest  Questionnaire,  use  of, 
in  developing,  358-359,  360-361; 
Interests  and  Activities  Question- 
naires for,  361-402 

Personality  (see  also  Adjustment, 
Personal  Adjustment,  Personal  and 
Social  Adjustment ) :  one  case 
study  of,  376-384;  information 
about,  need  for,  xix;  meaning  of 
term,  350;  measurement  of,  351 
n;  projective  methods  for  study- 
ing, 36;  rating  scale,  858 

Philosophy  and  objectives  underlying 
recording,  463-469 

Philosophy  of  life,  appraisal  of,  34 

Physical  energy  in  Behavior  De- 
scription, 485 

Physics  (see  Science) 

Planning  and  administering  the  eval- 
uation program,  439-459 

Prejudices  in  social  attitudes,  206 


INDEX 


547 


Pre-tests,  238,  243 

Primitive  drives  and  impulses,  364- 
365 

Principles  of  logical  reasoning  (see 
Application  of  Principles  of  Logi- 
cal Reasoning  tests,  Logical  rea- 
soning ) 

Programs  (see  also  Evaluation  pro- 
gram), evaluation  data  useful  in 
making  out,  456 

Proof,  nature  of  (see  Nature  of 
proof ) 

Propaganda:  definitions  of,  149; 
analysis,  148-154;  behaviors  re- 
lated to,  149-150 

Public  relations  (see  also  Commu- 
nity, Home,  Parents),  evaluation 
as  a  basis  for,  10 

Pupil  ( see  also  Student ) :  develop- 
ment in  subject  fields,  499-504; 
growth,  objectives  of,  classifica- 
tion of  reports  on,  490;  and  teacher 
relations,  231 

Purposes   (see  Objectives) 

Qualitative  vs.  quantitative  under- 
standing, 82 

Questionnaire;  techniques,  assump- 
tions in,  252;  on  voluntary  read- 
ing (see  Questionnaire  on  Volun- 
tary Reading) 

Questionnaire  on  Voluntary  Reading, 
253-264;  criteria  for  item  selec- 
tion on,  255-257;  data  sheet,  sam- 
ple, 259;  description  of,  253-257; 
scoring,  258-264;  summarizing, 
257-264;  use  of,  273-275 

Race:  as  interest  area  in  social  issues, 
209;  liberalism  arid  conservatism 
regarding,  218 

Radio:  checklists  for  revealing  recre- 
ational interests,  328,  329-330; 
preferences,  329-330 

"Rating"  (see  also  Grades  and 
awards,  Marks),  486 

Reading  (see  also  Appreciation  of 
Literature,  Reading  Record,  Recre- 
ational Interests):  fiction,  classifi- 
cation of,  by  type,  322,  324;  check- 


list of,  interests,  334-337;  maga- 
zines, 325-327;  non-fiction,  classi- 
fication of,  322,  325;  points,  45; 
reactions  to  (see  Reading  reac- 
tions, Reading  Reactions  Question- 
naire), records  (see  Reading  rec- 
ords); voluntary  (see  Reading 
Questionnaire ) 

Reading  reactions  (see  also  Reading 
Reactions  Questionnaire ) :  evalu- 
ation, need  for,  249,  252;  meaning 
of,  248-249;  synthesis  of  data  in 
one  case  study,  417-418;  tests  of, 
265;  types  of,  248-249,  251-252 

Reading  Reactions  Questionnaire, 
250-273;  "direct"  forms  of,  271- 
272;  direct  observations  and  ques- 
tionnaire techniques,  difference 
between,  269-270;  student  honesty 
in,  269-270;  uses  of,  273-276; 
validity  of,  268-273 

Reading  records:  for  revealing  in- 
terests, 319-325;  samples  of  classi- 
fication in,  322;  use  of,  166 

Reasoning,  logical  (see  Application 
of  principles  of  logical  reasoning, 
Logical  reasoning) 

Record  forms:  objectives  for,  467- 
469;  for  objectives  in  subject  fields, 
500-501;  purpose  of,  464 

Record  keeping,  decentralized,  in- 
adequacies of,  450-451 

Records  (see  also  Behavior  Descrip- 
tion, Record  forms):  activities, 
166;  activity,  validity  and  re- 
liability of,  330;  behavior,  206  n; 
observational,  43,  449-450;  read- 
ing (see  also  Reading  records), 
319-325;  and  reports,  objectives, 
467-469 

Recreational  interests:  areas  of,  318; 
checklists,  use  of,  334-337;  maga- 
zine checklist  for  revealing,  325- 
326;  movie  checklists  for  reveal- 
ing, 328-329;  newspaper  question- 
naire for  revealing,  327-328;  radio 
checklist  for  revealing,  328-330; 
reading  record  for  revealing,  319- 
325;  validity  and  reliability  of 
tests  for,  330-334 


548 


INDEX 


Reflective  thinking  (see  also  Appli- 
cation of  Principles  of  Logical 
Reasoning),  process  of,  36 

Relationships:  family,  evaluation  of, 
367;  human,  as  area  in  Interests 
and  Activities  Questionnaire,  364, 
367-370;  with  opposite  sex,  evalu- 
ation of,  367;  with  same  sex,  eval- 
uation of,  367;  in  social  values, 
175 

Reliability  of  scores,  63 

Religion,  beliefs  on,  208  n 

Report  cards  (see  Report  forms) 

Report  forms  (see  also  Reports): 
490-491;  traditional,  488 

Reports:  objectives  of,  489-490;  to 
parents,  456,  480-493;  on  pupil 
growth,  classifications  of  objec- 
tives in,  490;  records  as  basis  and 
part  of,  405 

Reports  and  Records:  Committee  on, 
464;  objectives  for,  467-469 

Responsibility-Dependability  in  Be- 
havior Description,  477-478 

Sampling,  as  type  of  data  interpre- 
tation, 46 

Satisfaction,  evaluation  of,  in  Ap- 
preciation of  Literature  test,  248, 
251 

Scales  of  Beliefs  (see  also  Beliefs): 
207,  239;  on  economic  issues  (see 
Beliefs  on  Economic  Issues  test); 
on  social  issues  (see  Beliefs  on 
Social  Issues  test);  uses  of,  240- 
241 

Schedule  for  testing,  447-448 

School:  democracy  in,  229;  evalua- 
tion, responsibility  of,  for,  14; 
evaluation  of  activities  in,  368- 
369;  government  as  area  in  Be- 
liefs about  School  Life,  230-231; 
life,  beliefs  about  (see  also  Be- 
liefs about  School  Life),  208;  ob- 
jectives (see  Objectives,  school); 
program  (see  also  Curriculum), 
changes  in,  436-437;  program, 
hypotheses  underlying,  436;  re- 
sources, evaluation  program 
limited  by,  443-444;  spirit,  233; 


staff  (see  also  Faculty,  Teachers): 
security  of,  fostered  by  evalua- 
tion, 9-10;  training  of,  for  in- 
terpreting evaluation  results,  27- 
28 

Science  principles  (see  also  Appli- 
cation of  Principles ) :  application 
of,  77-111;  meaning  of,  78-80 

Score  (see  also  Scores):  "beyond 
data,"  55;  caution,  55;  crude  er- 
rors, 55;  omissions  in,  55;  deriva- 
tion of,  54;  general  accuracy,  51 

Scores:  analysis  of,  on  Interpreta- 
tion of  Data  test,  56-60;  intercor- 
relation  of,  59  n;  reliability  of,  in- 
creased, 63;  students*  knowledge 
of,  inadvisable,  in  Interests  and 
Activities  Questionnaire,  397-399; 
summary  and  interpretation  of, 
in  Application  of  Principles  test, 
103-111 

Secondary  school  (see  School) 

Security,  psychological,  fostered  by 
evaluation,  9-10 

Self-reliance  in  Behavior  Descrip- 
tion, 485 

Senses,  use  of,  in  arriving  at  proofs, 
128 

Seven  Modern  Paintings  test,  307- 
312 

Short-answer  tests,  47 

Social  action,  skill  in  securing  evi- 
dence of,  161-168 

Social  adjustability:  in  Behavior  De- 
scription, 482;  meaning  of,  350 

Social  attitudes:  analysis  of  behavior 
in,  204-209;  belief,  as  type  of, 
205;  characteristics  of,  205;  defini- 
tion and  classification  of,  161,  203- 
209;  evaluation  of  (see  Applying 
Social  Facts  and  Generalizations); 
expressions  of,  206-207;  feeling- 
tone  as  type  of,  205;  tendency  to 
act,  as  type  of,  205 

Social  awareness,  meaning  of,  161 

Social  beliefs  (see  also  Beliefs  on 
Social  Issues):  ambivalence  in, 
possible  reasons  for,  431-432; 
areas  of,  207-209;  characteristics 
of,  212-214;  conservatism  in,  217- 


INDEX 


549 


219;  consistency  of,  213-214;  in- 
struments of  evaluation,  208-209; 
liberalism  in,  217-219;  scale  con- 
struction of,  214-215;  about  school 
life  (see  Beliefs  about  School 
Life);  statements  of,  language  in, 
210-212;  test  (see  Belieis  on  So- 
cial Issues,  Social  sensitivity); 
threshold  in  statement  of,  210 

Social  consciousness  (see  Social  sen- 
sitivity ) 

Social  generalizations,  169-172 

Social  information,  meaning  of,  161 

Social  issues  (see  also  Beliefs  on  So- 
cial Issues ) :  areas  of,  208;  areas  of 
interest  in,  209-210;  direction  of 
positions  toward,  212-213;  test 
(see  Beliefs  on  Social  Issues  test) 

Social  Problems  test  ( see  also  Beliefs 
on  Social  Issues  test ) :  comprehen- 
siveness appraised,  174,  184-186; 
consistency  appraisal  in,  174,  184, 
189-190;  criteria  for  choosing 
items  in,  176;  data  sheet  sample, 
185;  democratic  values  appraised 
in,  183-184,  187;  development  of, 
177-184;  intelligence,  relation  of, 
to  results  of,  196;  key  for,  182- 
183;  logical  aspects,  interpretation 
of,  in,  189;  rationalization  ap- 
praised in,  188;  relevance  ap- 
praisal in,  174,  187;  results  of, 
related  to  interests,  318;  results, 
summarized,  184-190;  scoring, 
validity  of,  191-192;  structure  for, 
176;  student  interviews,  as  va- 
lidity checks  of,  194-195;  teach- 
ers observations  compared  with 
results  of,  193-194;  use  of,  240, 
241,  244;  validity  of  construction 
of,  191;  validity  and  reliability  of, 
190-197;  value  patterns  in,  177, 
179,  183,  189 

Social  science,  generalizations  taught 
in,  list  of,  169-170 

Social  sensitivity:  anecdotal  records 
in  obtaining  evidence  of,  160-161, 
163-164;  aspects  of,  159-162;  be- 
haviors involved  in,  158,  159-162; 


evaluation  of,  157-244;  free-re- 
sponse tests  in  obtaining  evidence 
for,  166;  meanings  of,  158-159; 
objectives,  origin  and  scope  of, 
157-159;  pattern  of,  166-167; 
students'  writings  as  means  of  se- 
curing evidence  about,  164-166 

Social  values  (see  also  Beliefs  on 
Social  Issues  test),  158-159;  ap- 
plication of,  175-197,  406;  appli- 
cation of,  test  construction  on, 
175-180;  behavior  in  applying, 
174;  beliefs  test,  uses  of,  238-244; 
tests  (see  Application  of  Social 
Facts  and  Generalizations,  Social 
Problems);  use  of  tests,  238-244 

Society,  demands  of,  coriforrning  to, 
352-353 

Solitary  activities,  evaluation  of,  369 

Strong  Vocational  Interest  Blank, 
318 

Student  (see also  Pupil)  Background 
of,  important  in  social  tests,  233; 
behavior  patterns,  organization, 
12-13;  development,  evidence  of, 
sources  for,  444-449;  interviews, 
as  validity  checks,  194-195,  331- 
333;  participation  of,  in  test  con- 
struction, 207,  211;  philosophy  of 
life,  appraisal  of,  34;  programs, 
evaluation  useful  in  making  out, 
456;  scores  on  Interests  and  Ac- 
tivities Questionnaires,  unwise  to 
show  to,  397-399;  security  fos- 
tered by  evaluation,  9-10;  self- 
observations  in  test  construction, 
252 

Study,  conditions  for  effective,  81; 
skills  and  work  habits  needing  ap- 
praisal, 31-33 

Subject  fields:  objectives  in,  record- 
ing of,  499-500;  record  forms  for, 
500-501,  503,  504 

Suggestibility,  152 

Teacher  (see  also  Counselor,  Faculty 
Teachers,  Teaching):  education 
through  evaluation  programs,  30; 
and  pupils,  relation,  231-232;  rat- 


55° 


INDEX 


ing  and  test  results  compared,  193- 
194;  reports,  410-413,  492;  train- 
ing, 459 

Teachers :  concern  of,  in  behavior  de- 
velopment, essential,  238,  239; 
evaluation  program,  reaction,  20, 
432;  insights  translated  into  prac- 
tice by,  454-455;  Interest  Ques- 
tionnaire, value  of,  to,  345-347;  as 
interpreters  of  evaluation  data, 
458-459;  objectives  considered  by, 
465-466;  observations  of,  com- 
pared with  test  results,  193-194; 
realization  of  objectives  in  sub- 
ject fields  by,  499-500;  security  of, 
fostered  by  evaluation,  9-10;  sub- 
ject-field forms  useful  to,  504; 
training  of,  in  interpreting  evalua- 
tion results,  27-28 

Teaching  ( see  also  Guidance ) :  eval- 
uation data  used  in,  454-455;  in- 
fluence of  evaluation  on,  14 

Tension,  inadvisable  to  point  out,  to 
student,  398 

Test  (see  also  Evaluation,  Tests): 
construction,  114-122;  data,  sum- 
mary of,  in  one  case  study,  415- 
429;  responses,  terminology  de- 
scribing, 51;  scoring,  traditional 
inadequacies  of,  44;  schedule  for, 
447-448;  situation,  total,  156 

Tests :  achievement,  inadequacies 
of  early,  3-4;  allocation  of,  to 
faculty,  448;  bibliographies  of,  21- 
22;  essay-type,  criticisms  of,  84- 
85;  pencil-and-paper,  44,  162,  187; 
science,  principles  of  applying,  77- 
111;  readministration  of,  155,156; 
short-answer,  47;  structure  of,  in- 
terpreter's understanding  of,  154- 
155;  written,  shortcomings  of, 
14-15 

Theater  arts  (see  also  Dramatics), 
interest  in,  371-373 


Thinking  (see  also  Application  of 
Principles  of  Logical  Reasoning, 
Clear  thinking,  Logical  reasoning, 
Social  thinking):  Aspects  of,  35- 
156;  as  objective,  35 

Thirty  Schools  (see  also  School),  re- 
sponsibility of,  for  evaluation,  3, 
14-15 

Thurstone,  L.  L.,  214 

Time:  effective  use  of  for  study,  31; 
recording  data,  economy  of,  in,  45, 
454 

Traits  (see  also  Behavior,  Behavior 
Description),  470,  473 

Transfer:  Behavior  Description  card 
useful  in,  486-487;  to  college,  form, 
494-498;  recording  for,  465,  494- 
498 

Trends,  recognition  and  compari- 
son of,  45-46,  49 

"Undemocratic"  (see  also  Democ- 
racy, Democratic),  meaning  of 
term,  183 

Units  for  college  admission,  inade- 
quacies of,  494 

Value:  judgment,  45;  pattern,  ambiv- 
alence in,  244 

Values  ( see  also  Social  Values ) ,  gen- 
eral vs.  specific,  241-242 

Verbal  facility  and  art,  278-279 

Vocabulary,  appropriateness  of,  in 
administering  tests,  239 

Vocational  tests,  interests  sampled 
by,  318 

Work  habits:  in  Behavior  Descrip- 
tion, 482;  and  study  skills  needing 
appraisal,  31-33 

Wert,  James  E.,  327 

Whole-faculty  (see  Faculty) 

Wickman,  E.  K.,  353  n 

Words,  "people-describing,"  470- 
471 


00 


c 


126271