Skip to main content

Full text of "Comparative electro-physiology, a physico-physiological study"

See other formats


\ 
. 2 7 - 
“+ 
a : 
- 

<: 

en 

‘ re 
= 

. + én E, 

- 

; “4 
’ 4 
- 
: 
- . 

be TA 
‘ te - P 
Lo 5 - 
‘= 4 ' f : 
) 5 ~ 
. + 

aes : ; : 
is, 
oh aa i 
7 } 5 

. 


Digitized by the Internet Archive 
in 2007 with funding from 
Microsoft Corporation 


http://www.archive.org/details/comparativeelect0OOboseuoft 


COMPARATIVE 
ELEC LBR-O-P YSIOLOGY 


WORKS BY THE SAME AUTHOR 


RESPONSE IN THE LIVING AND NON- 
LIVING. With 117 Illustrations. 8vo. 10s. 6d. 
1902. 


“PLANT RESPONSE; as a means of Physio- 
logical Investigations. With 278 Illustrations. 
8vo. 21s. 1906. 


LONGMANS, GREEN & CO., 39 Paternoster Row, London, 
New York, Bombay, and Calcutta 


x ry 


~? COMPARATIVE 
ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


A PHYSICO-PHYSIOLOGICAL STUDY 


BY 


JAGADIS CHUNDER BOSE, M.A., D.Sc. 


PROFESSOR, PRESIDENCY COLLEGE, CALCUTTA 


WITH ILLUSTRATIONS 


LONGMANS, GREEN, AND CO. 


39 PATERNOSTER ROW, LONDON 
NEW YORK, BOMBAY, AND CALCUTTA 


1907 


All rights reserved 


is 


i 


= 
_ 
Ly 
- 2 
i 
ro 


PREFACE 


THIs volume concludes the line of investigation on respon- 
sive phenomena in general, which I commenced with the 
publication of a Memoir’ at the International Congress of 
Science, Paris, 1900. In this first of my publications on the 
subject I undertook to show the similarities of response in 
inorganic and living substances. The method which I[ at 
that time employed for obtaining my response-records was 
that of Conductivity Variation. With the object of showing 
that the similarity of response here demonstrated to exist 
was due to some fundamental molecular reaction, common 
to matter in general, and therefore to be detected by any 
method of recording response, I next undertook to record 
the Electro-motive Variation under stimulus. Believing, as 
I did, in the continuity of these responsive phenomena, I 
used the same experimental devices by which I had already 
succeeded in obtaining the electric response of inorganic sub- 
stances, to test whether ordinary plants also, meaning those 
usually regarded as insensitive, would or would not exhibit 


excitatory electrical response to stimulus. The stimulation 


' * De la Généralité des Phénoménes Moléculaires produits par 1|’Electricité 
sur la Matiére Inorganique et sur la Matiére Vivante’ (7ravaux du Congres 
International de Physique, Paris, 1900). See also ‘On the Similarity of Effects 
of Electrical Stimulus on Inorganic and Living Substances,’ Xefort Brit: Assoc., 
Bradford, September 1900 (Z/ectrician). > 


vi COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


employed was mechanical and quantitative, thus obviating 
many sources of complication. By this method I was able 
to show that every plant, and every organ of every plant, gave 
true excitatory electrical response. As observations similar 
to these were subsequently made by another investigator, 
I quote here the following summary of my results. from the 
preliminary account which I communicated to the Royal 
Society, May 7, and afterwards read, with accompanying ex- - 
perimental demonstration, before the Society, on June 6, 1goI. 
‘An interesting link, between the response given by inor- 
ganic substances and the animal tissues, is that given by plant 
tissues. By methods. somewhat resembling that described 
above, I have obtained from plants a strong electric response 
to mechanical stimulus. The response is not confined to 
sensitive plants like Mimosa, but is universally present. I 
have, for example, obtained such response from the roots, 
stems, and leaves of, among others, horse-chestnut, vine, 
white lily, rhubarb, and horse-radish. . ‘The “current of 
injury” is, generally speaking, from the injured to the 
uninjured part. A “negative variation ” is also produced. | 
obtained both the single electric twitches and tetanus. Very 
interesting also are the effects of fatigue, of temperature, of 
stimulants, and of poison. Definite areas killed by poison 
exhibit no response, whereas neighbouring unaffected portions 
show.the normal response.’ ! | | , 
It may be well to point out here that at the time when this. 
communication was made, the view that ordinary plants 
were excitable, and responded to mechanical stimulus by 


1A more complete: account: will be. found in the report of my ‘ Friday 
Evening Discourse’ before the Royal Institution, May 10, 1901, and in the 
Journal of the Linnean Society, vol. xxxv.. Pp. 275. 


PREFACE vii 


definite electro-motive changes, was regarded as highly 
controversial. Indeed, in the discussion which followed 
the reading of my Paper, on June 6, 1901, Sir John Burdon 
Sanderson went so far as to state that this excitatory 
response of ordinary plants to mechanical stimulation was 
an impossibility. 

My next investigation was directed towards the question 
whether the responsive effects which I had shown to occur 
in ordinary plants might not be further exhibited by means 
of visible mechanical response, thus finally removing the dis- 
tinction commonly assumed to exist between the ‘sensitive’ 
and supposed non-sensitive. These results were published 
in my work on Plant Response,’ where the effects of various 
environmental stimuli on the different plant organs were 
demonstrated by means of responsive movements. Many 
anomalous effects hitherto ascribed to specific sensibilities 
were here shown to be due to the differential excitability 


of anisotropic structures, and to the opposite effects of 
external and internal stimuli. Among other things, it was 


there shown that internal stimulus was in reality derived from 
external sources, and that the term ‘autonomous response’ 
was a misnomer, since all movements were due, either to the 
immediate effects of external stimulus, or to stimulus previously 
absorbed and held latent in the plant, to find subsequent ex- 
pression. It was further shown that not gross mechanical 
movements alone, but also other invisible movements, were 
initiated by the action of stimulus ; that external stimulus, 
so far from invariably causing a run-down of energy, more 
often brought about its accumulation by the plant ; and that 


the various activities, such as the ascent of sap and growth, 


' Plant Response as a Means of Physiological Investigation, 1906, 


vill COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


were thus in reality different reactions to the stimulating action 
of energy supplied by the environment. 

With regard to these points, my results have been in direct 
opposition to current views, according to which the effect 
induced by stimulus is always disproportionately greater 
than the stimulus, From the plausible analogy of the 
firing-off of a gun by the pulling of a trigger, or the action 
of a combustion-engine, it has been customary to suppose 
that all response to stimulus must be of the nature of an 
explosive chemical change, accompanied by an inevitable 
run-down of energy. This supposition, however, overlooks 
the obvious fact that the plant is not consumed by the 
incessant and multifarious stimuli of its environment. 
Rather, as we all know, it is the energy of the environ- 
ment which is the agent that fashions the microscopic 
embryo into the gigantic banyan-tree. And it is clear 
that, for this to be possible, the energy contributed by the 
blow of external stimulus must have been largely conserved. 

In the course of the present work, I have not only been 
able to corroborate, by means of electrical response, the 
various results which I had already established, with regard 
to the plant, by mechanical response, but I have also ex- 
tended the electrical method in various directions, so as to 
include many more recondite problems in connection with 
the irritability of living tissues. It was my original inten- 
tion to confine this investigation to the Electro-physiology 
of Plants. But, finding that in the results so obtained I pos- 
sessed a key to that of the animal also, I proceeded to apply 
the same methods of inquiry, and to use the same experi- 
mental devices, in the one case as in the other. I have thus 


been able to trace out the gradual differentiation of various 


PREFACE ix 


responsive peculiarities, characteristic of given tissues, from © 
their simplest types in the plant to their most complex in the 
animal. The value of such a comparative method of study, 
for the elucidation of biological problems in general, is 
sufficiently obvious. Exception may be taken with regard 
to the unorthodox point of view from which various ques- 
tions in animal physiology have been approached. It must 
be remembered, however, that in this work the attempt has 
been to explain responsive phenomena in general on the 
consideration of that fundamental molecular reaction which 
occurs even in inorganic matter. My mode of investigation 
has thus been determined by the necessary progression 
from simple to complex, and by my conviction as to the 
continuity which existed between them. And from. this 
attempt it will be seen that various results, which, accord- 


ing to the so-called vitalistic assumption were anomalous, 


are, in fact, capable of an increasingly simple and _ satis- 


factory explanation. It must also be understood that my 
work deals mainly with the electrical response of plants, 
and that its extension into the field of Animal Electro- 
physiology was intended for the demonstration of the con- 
tinuity between the two. It was therefore impossible, in 
the short space at my disposal, to make more than the 
brief necessary references to the different theories already 
in vogue concerning the response of various animal tissues. 
These will be found, in all their detail, in the excellent 
account given in the standard work of Biedermann.' 

For the sake of clearness, however, I shall at this point 
enumerate a few only of the points of difference between 


current views and the results, obtained from actual experi- 
1 Biedermann, Ziectro-physiology (English translation), 1896. 


x COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


ment, which I have set forth in the present volume. 
The reactions of different tissues have hitherto been re- 
garded as specifically different. As against this, a continuity 
has here been shown to exist between them. Thus, nerve 
was universally regarded as typically non-motile; its re- 
sponses were believed to be characteristically different from 
those of muscle. I have been able to show, however, that 
nerve is not only indisputably motile, but also that the 
investigation of its response by the mechanical method is 
capable of greater delicacy, and freedom from error, than 
that by the electrical. The characteristic variations in the 
response of nerve, moreover, are, generally speaking, similar 
to those of the muscle. It has been customary, again, to 
regard plants as devoid of the power to conduct true excita- 
tion. But I have shown that this view is incorrect. Experi- 
ments have been described, showing that the response of 
the isolated vegetal nerve is indistinguishable from that of 
animal nerve, throughout a long series of parallel variations 
of condition. So complete, indeed, has that similarity 
between the responses of plant and animal, of which this 
is an instance, been found, that the discovery of a given 
responsive characteristic in one case has proved a sure guide 
to its observation in the other, and the explanation of a 
phenomenon, under the simpler conditions of the plant, has 
been found fully sufficient for its elucidation under the more 
complex circumstances of the animal. , 

Many anomalous conclusions, with regard to the response 
of certain animal tissues, had arisen from the failure to take 
account of the differential excitability of anisotropic organs. 
Now this is a subject which, in the case of the simple plant 


organ, is capable of very exact investigation. I have been 


ety 


a eI ed i i ag KE a ) ee 


gr ee 


é 


PREFACE X1 


able to show that this differential excitability is widely - 
present as a factor in determining the character of special 
responses, and that it finds its culminating expression in the 
electrical organs of certain well-known fishes, — 


Few conclusions in. Electro-physiology have been sup- 


_posed to rest on securer foundations than the generalisation 


known as Pfliiger’s Law of the polar effects of currents. I 
have found, however, that this law is not by any means 
of such universal application as had been supposed, since, 
above and below a certain range of electromotive intensity, 
the polar effects of currents are precisely opposite to those 
enunciated by Pfliiger. © 

Finally, that nervous impulse, which must necessarily 
form the basis of sensation, was supposed to lie beyond 
any conceivable power of visual scrutiny. But it has here 
been shown that this impulse is actually attended by change 


of form, and is therefore capable of direct observation. This 


wave of nerve-disturbance, moreover, instead of being single, 
has been shown to’ be of two different kinds, in which fact, 
as I have further explained, lies the significance of the two 
different qualities or tones of sensation. 

In the concluding portion. of the paper which I read 
before the Bradford meeting of the British Association in 
the year 1900, I said :— | 

‘In the phenomena described above there is little 
breach of continuity. It is difficult to draw a line and 
say : “ Here the physical process ends, and the physiological 
process begins”; or “That is a phenomenon of inorganic 
matter, and this is a vital phenomenon, peculiar to living 
organisms”; or “These are the lines of demarcation that 
separate the physical, the physiological, and the beginning 


xil COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


of psychical processes.” Such arbitrary lines can hardly 
be drawn. , : 

‘We may explain each of the above classes of phenomena 
by making numerous and independent assumptions ; or, 
finding some property of matter common and persistent 
in the living and non-living substances, attempt from this 
common underlying property to explain the many phe- 
nomena which at first appear so different. And for this 
it may be said that the tendency of science has always 
been to attempt to find, wherever facts justify it, an under- 
lying unity in apparent diversity.’ 

It was for the demonstration of this underlying unity 
that I set out on these investigations seven years ago. 
And now, in bringing to its close another stage of their 
publication, I may, perhaps, be permitted to express the 
hope that by them not only may a deeper perception of 
this unity have been made attainable, but also that many 
regions of inquiry may prove to have been opened out, which 
had at one time been regarded as beyond the scope of experi- 
mental exploration. 

I take this opportunity to thank my assistants for their 
efficient help in these researches. 


J. C. BOSE. 


PRESIDENCY COLLEGE, CALCUTTA : 
August 1906, 


CONTENTS 


CHAPTER I 


THE MOLECULAR RESPONSIVENESS OF MATTER 
PAGE 
Response to stimulus by change of form—Permeability variation— Variation 
of solubility— Method of resistivity variation : (a) positive variation ; (4) 
negative variation—Sign of response changed under different molecular 
modifications—Response of vegetable tissue by variation of electrical 
resistance—Response by electro-motive variation in inorganic substances 
—The method of block—Positive and negative responses—Similar 
responses in living tissues—Effects of fatigue, stimulants, and poisons on 
inorganic and organic responses-—-Method of relative depression, or 
negative variation, so called ; : F ; - j ; : I 


CHAPTER II 


THE ELECTRO-MOTIVE RESPONSE OF PLANTS TO DIFFERENT 
FORMS OF STIMULATION 


Historical—Difficulties of investigation—-Electrical response of pulvinus of 
Mimosa—Simultaneous mechanical and electrical records—Division of 
plants into ‘ordinary’ and ‘sensitive’ arbitrary —Mechanical and 
electrical response of ‘ ordinary’ plants—Direct and transmitted stimu- 
Jation—All forms of stimulus induce excitatory change of galvanometric 
negativily . ‘ ce , . : é : j i aE LR 


CHAPTER III 


THE APPLICATION OF QUANTITATIVE STIMULUS AND KELATION 
BETWEEN STIMULUS AND RESPONSE 


Conditions of obtaining uniform response—Torsional vibration as a form of 
stimulus— Method of block— Effective intensity of stimulus dependent 
on period of vibration—Additive action of feeble stimuli—Response 


Xiv COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


, PAGE 
recorder—Uniform electric responses—List of suitable specimens— 


Effect of season on excitability—Stimulation by thermal shocks —Thermal 
stimulator—Second method of confining excitation to one contact—In- 
creasing response to increasing stimulus—Effect of fatigue—Tetanus . 29 


CHAPTER IV 


OBSERVATION BY RHEOTOME ON ELECTRIC RESPONSE IN 
PLANTS 


Response-curve showing general time-relations—Instantaneous mechanical 
stimulation by electro-magnetic release—Arrangement of the rheotome 
—Tabular statement of results of rheotomic observations—Rhythmic 
multiple responses. ; ; : ; ‘ : ; : - “5 


CHAPTER V 


THE ELECTRICAL INDICATIONS OF POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE 
TURGIDITY-VARIATIONS 


Motile responses of opposite signs, characteristic of positive and negative 
turgidity-variations—Indirect hydrostatic effect of stimulus causes 
expansion and erection of leaf-—Positive and negative work—Wave of 
increased hydrostatic tension transmitted with relatively greater velocity 
than wave of true excitation— Method of separating hydro-positive and 
excitatory effects— Indirect effect of stimulus, causing positive turgidity- 

variation induces galvanometric positivity—Antagonistic elements in the 
electrical response—Separation of hydro-positive from true excitatory 
effect by means of physiological block ; [ ; ‘ , - Sg 


CHAPTER VI 


EXTERNAL STIMULUS AND INTERNAL ENERGY 


Hydraulic transmission of energy in plants—True meaning of tonic condi- 
tion-—Opposite expressions of internal energy and external stimulus 
seen in growth-response—Parallelism between responses of growing and 
motile organs—Increased internal energy caused by augmentation o1 
temperature finds expression in enhanced rate of growth; erection of 
motile leaf ; curling movement of spiral tendril ; and galvanometric 
positivity—External stimulus induces opposite eftect in all these cases— 
Sudden variation of temperature, acting as a stimulus, induces transient 
retardation of growth ; depression of motile leaf ; uncurling mevement 
of spiral tendril ; and galvanometric negativity— Laws of mechanical 
and electrical response ° : ‘ / : : 69 


| 
: 
. 
" 


CONTENTS 


CHAPTER VII 


ABSORPTION AND EMISSION OF ENERGY IN RESPONSE 


Sign of response determined by latent energy of tissue, and by intensity of 


external stimulus—Sub-tonic, normal and hyper-tonic conditions—The. 


critical level—Outward manifestation of response possible only when 
critical level is exceeded—Three typical cases: response greater than 
stimulus ; response equal to stimulus; and response less than stimulus 
—lInvestigation by growth-response—The sum of work, internal and 
external, performed by stimulus constant—Positive response of tissues 
characterised by feeble protoplasmic activity or sub-tonicity—Enhance- 
ment of normal excitability of sub-tonic tissue by absorption of stimulus 


CHAPTER VIII 


VARIOUS TYPES OF RESPONSE 


Chemical theory of response—Insufficiency of the theory of assimilation and 
dissimilation --Similar responsive effects seen in inorganic matter— 
Modifying influence of molecular condition on response—Five molecular 
stages, A, B, C, D, E—Staircase effect, uniform response, fatigue—No 
sharp line of demarcation between physical and chemical phenomena— 
Volta-chemical effect and by-productions—Phasic alternation —Alter- 
nating fatigue—Rapid fatigue under continuous stimulation—In sub- 
tonic. tissue summated effect of latent components raises tonicity and 
excitability—Response not always disproportionately greater than 
stimulus—Instances of stimulus partially held latent: staircase and 


additive effects, multiple response, renewed growth . : ; ° 
CHAPTER IX 
DETECTION OF PHYSIOLCGICAL ANISOTROPY BY ELECTRIC 
RESPONSE 


Anomalies in mechanical and electrical response—Resultant response deter- 
mined by differential excitability—Responsive current from the more to 
the less excitable—Laws of response in anisotropic organ—Demonstra- 
tion by means of mechanical stimulation—Vibrational stimulus—Stimu- 
lation by pressure—Quantitative stimulation by thermal shocks 


CHAPTER X 


THE NATURAL CURRENT AND ITS VARIATIONS 


Natural current in anisotropic organ from the less to the more excitable— 
External stimulus induces responsive current in opposite direction— 
Increase of internal energy induces positive, and decrease negative, 


XV 


PAGE 


76 


86 


107 


xvi COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


. PAGE 
variation of natural current—Effect on natural current of variation of 
temperature—Effect of sudden variation—Variation of natural current 
by chemical agents, referred to physiological reaction—Agents which 
render tissue excitable, induce the positive, and those which cause excita- 
tion, the negative variation—Action of hydrochloric acid—Action of 
Na,CO,—Effect modified by strength of dose—Effect of CQ, and of 
alcohol vapour—Natural current and its variations—Extreme unrelia- 
bility of negative variation so-called as a test of excitatory reaction— 
Reversal of natural current by excessive cold or by stimulation—Re- 
versal of normal response under sub-tonicity or fatigue . ; ; + eto 


CHAPTER XI 


VARIATIONS OF EXCITABILITY UNDER CHEMICAL REAGENTS 


Induced variation of excitability studied by two methods: (1) direct 
(2) transmitted stimulation—Effect of chloroform—Effect of chloral— 
Effect of formalin—Advantage of the Method of Block over that of 
negative variation—Effect of KHO—Response unaffected by variation 
of resistance—Stimulating action of solution of sugar—Of sodium carbon- 
ate—Effect of doses— Effect of hydrochloric acid—Diphasic response on 
application of potash—Conversion of normal negative into abnormal 
positive response by abolition of true excitability . ‘ ; ‘ « o129 


CHAPTER XII 


VARIATIONS OF EXCITABILITY DETERMINED BY METHOD OF 
INTERFERENCE 


Arrangement for interference of excitatory waves—Effect of increasing 
difference of phase—Interference effects causing change from positive to 
negative, through intermediate diphasic—Diametric balance—Effect of 
unilateral application of KHO—Effect of unilateral cooling . ; - Iq! 


CHAPTER XIII 


CURRENT OF INJURY AND NEGATIVE VARIATION 


Different theories of current of injury—Pre-existence theory of Du Bois- 
Reymond—Electrical distribution in a muscle-cylinder—Electro-mole- 
cular theory of Bernstein—Hermann’s Alteration Theory— Experiments 
demonstrating that so-called current of injury is a persistent after-effect 
of over-stimulation—Residual galvanometric negativity of strongly excited 
tissue—Distribution of electrical potential in vegetable tissue with one 
end sectioned—Electrical distribution in plant-cylinder similar to that in 
muscle-cylinder—True significance of response by negative variation—- 
Apparent abnormalities in so-called current of injury—‘ Positive ’ 
current of injury ; ‘ : ‘ eas ; ‘ . 149 


CONTENTS Xvil 


CHAPTER XIV 


CURRENT OF DEATH—RESPONSE BY POSITIVE VARIATION 
PAGE 
Anomalous case of response by positive variation—Inquiry into the cause— 
Electric exploration of dying and dead tissue: death being natural— 
Determination of electric distribution in tissue with one end killed— 
Dying tissue shows maximum negativity, and dead tissue, positivity to 
living—Explanation of this peculiar distribution—Response by negative 
or positive variation, depending on degree of injury—Three typical cases 
—-Explanation by theory of assimilation and dissimilation misleading — 
All response finally traceable to simple fundamental reactions é - 164 


CHAPTER XV 


EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE ON ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 


General observation of effect of temperature on plant—Effect of fall and rise 
of temperature on autonomous response of Desmodium—Effect of frost 
in abolition of electrical response—A fter-effects of application of cold, in 
Eucharis, Ivy and Holly—Effect of rise of temperature in diminishing 
height of response—This not probably due to diminution of excitability 
—Similar effect in autonomous motile response of Desmodium—En- 
hanced response as after-effect of cyclic variation of temperature—Aboli- 
tion of response at a critical high temperature. ‘ : ‘ . 180 


CHAPTER XVI 


THE ELECTRICAL SPASM OF DEATH 


Different fost-mortem symptoms—Accurate methods for determination of 
death-point—Determination of death-point by abolition or reversal of 
normal electrical response —Determination of death-point by mechanical 
death-spasm—From thermo-mechanical inversion—By observation of 
electrical spasm : (a) in anisotropic organs: (4) in radial organs—Simul- 
taneous record of electrical inversion and reversal of normal electrical 
response—Remarkable consistency of results obtained by different 
methods— Tabulation of observations . ; , 2 . 2 - 582 


CHAPTER XVII 


MULTIPLE AND AUTONOMOUS ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 


Repeated responses under single strong stimulus—Multiple mechanical 
response in Biophytwm—Multiple electrical responses in various animal 
and vegetable tissues —Continuity of multiple and autonomous response 
—Transition from multiple response to autonomous, and dice versa— 


a 


XVili COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


PAGE 
Autonomous mechanical response of Desmodium gyrans and its time- 
relations—Simultaneous mechanical and electrical records of automatic 
pulsations in Desmodium—Double electrical pulsation, principal and 
subsidiary waves—Flectrical pulsation of Desmodium leaflet under 
physical restraint— Growth- pulsation — So-called current of rest in grow- 
ing plants . : ? MF Sette : ; ; ; ‘. . 207 


CHAPTER XVIII 


RESPONSE OF LEAVES 


Observations of Burdon Sanderson on leaf-response in Déone@a—Leaf and 
stalk currents—Their opposite variations under stimulus—Similar leaf- 
and-stalk currents shown to exist in ordinary leaf of Ficus religiosa— 
Opposite-directioned currents in Cztrus decumana—True explanation of 
these resting-currents and their variations—Electrical effect of section 
of petiole on Dzonea and Ficus religiosa—Fundamental experiment of 
Burdon Sanderson on lamina of Dzone@a— Subsequent results—Expeti- 
mental arrangement with symmetrical contacts—Parallel experiments on > 
sheathing leaf of /usa—Explanation of various results : ‘ 1223 


CHAPTER XIX 
THE LEAF CONSIDERED AS AN ELECTRIC ORGAN 


Electrical organs in fishes—Typical instances, Zorpedo and Malepterurus— 
Vegetal analogues, leaf of Pterospermum and carpel of D¢llenia indica 
or pitcher of Mefenthe—Electrical response to transmitted excitation 
—Response to direct excitation—Uni-directioned response to homo- 

- dromous and heterodromous shocks—Definite-directioned response 
shown to be due to differential excitability—Response to equi-alternating 
electrical shocks—Rheotomic observations—Multiple  excitations— 
Multiplication of terminal electromotive effect, by pile-like arrangement, 
in bulb of Uric/zs lily : ‘ ‘ , ; ‘ , . . 241 


CHAPTER XX 


THE THEORY OF ELECTRICAL ORGANS 


Existing theories—Their inadequacy—-The ‘ blaze-current’ so called—Re- 
sponse uni-directioned, to shocks homodromous or heterodromous, 
characteristic of electric organs—Similar results with inorganic specimens 
—Uni-directioned response due to differential excitability— Electrical 
response of pulvinus of A//mosa to equi-alternating electric shocks—Re- 
sponse of petiole of AZ#sa—Of plagiotropic stem of Cucurdita—-Of Eel— 

The organ-current of electric fishes—Multiple responses of electrical 
organ—Multiple responses of Bzofhyium . — . > : : . 250 


CONTENTS XIX 


CHAPTER XXI 


DETERMINATION OF DIFFERENTIAL EXCITABILITY UNDER © 
ELECTRICAL STIMULATION 
| ) PAGE 
Advantage of electrical stimulation, in its flexibility—Drawbacks due to~ 
fluctuating factors of polar effects, and counter polarisation-current— 
Difficulties overcome by employment of equi-alternating © electric 
shocks—Methods of the After-effect and Direct-effect—Experiment of 
Von Fleischl on response of nerve—Complications arising from use of 
make and break shocks—Rotating reverser— Motor transformer—Re- 
sponse of Musa to equi-alternating shocks—Abolition of this response by 
chloroform—Response records of plagiotropic Cucurbita and Eel— 
Differential excitability of variegated leaves, demonstrated by electric 
response. é ita : > ; - ; : : eee 


CHAPTER XXII 


RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 


Currents of rest and action—Currents in animal skin—Theories regarding 
these—Response of vegetal skin—Stimulation by Rotary Mechanical 
_Stimulator—Response of intact human skin--Isolated responses of upper 
and. lower surfaces of specimens —Resultant response brought about by 
differential excitability of the two surfaces—Differences of excitability 
between two surfaces accounted for—Response of animal and vegetal 
skins not essentially different—General formula for all types of response 
of skin—Response of skin to different forms of stimulation gives 
similar results— Response to equi-alternating electric shocks : (1) Method 
of the After Effect ; (2) Method of Direct Effect—Response of grape 
skin—Similar response of frog’s skin—Phasic variation of current of 
rest induced as result of successive stimulation in (a) grape skin ; (4) frog’s 
skin; (¢c) pulvinus of Jimosa—Phasic variation in autonomous me- 
chanical response of Desmodium gyrans—Autonomous variation of 
current of rest—True current of rest in skin from outer to inner—This 
may be reversed as an excitatory after-effect of preparation—Electrical 
response of skin of neck of tortoise—Electrical response of skin of 
tomato—Normal response and positive after-effect—Response of skin 
of gecko—Explanation of abnormal response. . ; . . 287 


CHAPTER XXIII 


RESPONSE OF EPITHELIUM AND GLANDS 


Epidermal, epithelial, and secreting membranes in plant tissues—Natural 
resting-current from epidermal to epithelial or secretory surfaces —Current 
of response from epithelial or secretory to epidermal surface:—Response 

a2 


XX COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


; PAGE 
of Dil/enza—Response of water-melon—Response of foot of snail—The 
so-called current of rest from glandular surface really due to injury— 
Misinterpretation arising from response by so-called ‘ positive variation ’ 
—Natural current in intact foot of snail, and its variation on section— 
Response of intact human armpit—Response of intact human lip— 
Lingual response in man—Reversal of normal response under sub- 
minimal or super-maximal stimulation—Differential excitations of two 
surfaces under different intensities of stimulus, with consequent changes 
in direction of responsive currents, diagrammatically represented in 
characteristic curves—Records exhibiting responsive reversals. rita. 3 Se 


CHAPTER XXIV 


RESPONSE OF DIGESTIVE ORGANS 


Consideration of the functional peculiarities of the digestive organ—Alter- 
nating phases of secretion and absorption—Relation between secretory 
and contractile responses. [Illustrated by (a) preparation of Mimosa; 
(4) glandular tentacle of Drvosera—General occurrence of contractile re- 
sponse—True current of rest in digestive organs—Experiments on the 
pitcher of Wepenthe—Three definite types of response under different con- 
ditions—Negative and positive electrical responses, concomitant with 
secretion and absorption— Multiple responses due to strong stimulation 
—Response in glandular leaf of Drosera—Normal negative response 
reversed to positive under continuous stimulation— Multiple response in 
Drosera—Response of frog’s stomach to mechanical stimulation—Re- 
sponse of stomach of tortoise—Response of stomach of gecko— Multiple 

_ response of frog’s stomach, showing three stages—-negative, diphasic, 
and positive —Phasic variations : : ; ‘ , 329 


CHAPTER XXV 
ABSORPTION OF FOOD BY PLANT AND ASCENT OF SAP 


Parallelism between responsive reactions of root and digestive organ—Alter- 
nating phases of secretion and absorption—Association of absorptive 
process with ascent of sap—Electrical response of young and old roots— 
Different phasic reactions, as in pitcher of /Vepenthe—Response to 
chemical stimulation—Different theories of ascent of sap-——Physical 
versus excitatory theories—Objections to excitatory theory—Assumption 
that wood dead unjustified—Demonstration of excitatory electrical re- 
sponse of sap-wood—Strasburger’s experiments on effect of poisons on 
ascent of sap—Current inference unjustified . : : ‘ ; - 349 


CONTENTS 


CHAPTER. XXVI 


THE EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 


Propagation of excitatory wave in plant attended by progressive movement of 
water—Hydraulic response to stimulus—The Shoshungraph— Direct and 
photographic methods of record—Responsive variations of suction under 
physiological modifications induced by various agents—Effects of lower- 
ing and raising of temperature—Explanation of maintenance of suction, 
when root killed—Effect of poison influenced by tonic condition—Effect 
of anzesthetics on suctional response—Excitatory versws osmotic action 
—Stimulation by alternating induction-shocks—Terminal and sub-ter- 
minal modes of application—-Three modes of obtaining response-records, 
namely (1) the unbalanced, (2) the balanced, (3) the over-balanced— 
Renewal of suction previously at standstill, by action of stimulus--Re- 
ponsive enhancement of suction by stimulus—After-effect of stimulus— 
Diminution of latent period as after-effect of stimulus—Response under 
over-balance—Response under sub-terminal stimulation—Variation of 
response under seasonal changes . 


CHAPTER XXVII 


RESPONSE TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 


Heliotropic plant movements reducible to fundamental reaction of contrac- 
tion or expansion—Various mechanical effects of light in pulvinated and 
growing organs—Electrical response induced by light not specific, but 
concomitant to excitatory effects—Electrical response of plant to light 
not determined by presence or absence of chloroplasts—Effect of 
unilateral application of stimulus on transversely distal point— Positive 
response due to indirect. effect and negative to transmission of true 
excitation—Mechanical response of leaf of AM/¢mosa to light applied on 
upper half of pulvinus—Mechanical response consists of erection or 
positive movement, followed by fall or negative movement—FElectrical 
response of leaf of A/zmosa to light applied on upper half of pulvinus ; 
induction in lower half of pulvinus of positivity followed by negativity— 
Longitudinal transmission of excitatory effect, with concomitant galvano- 
metric negativity --Direct effect of light and positive after effect— 
Circumstances which are effective in reversing normal response—Plants 
in slightly sub-tonic condition give positive followed by negative response 
—Exemplified by (a) electrical and (4) growth response—Examples of 
positive response to light—Periodic variation of excitability— Multiple 
mechanical response under light—Direct and after-effect— Multiple 
electrical response under light, with phasic alterations of (— + — +) or 
(+ — + —)--After-effects ; unmasking of antagonistic elements, either 
plus or minus—Three types of after-effects 


f 


S 


XXi 


PAGE 


365 


392 


xxii COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


CHAPTER: XXVIII 


RESPONSE OF RETINA TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 
PAGE 
Response of retina—Determination of true current of rest—Determination _ 

of differential excitabilities of optic’ nerve and cornea, and optic nerve 
and retina—The so-called positive variation of previous observers 
indicates the true excitatory negative—Retino-motor effects—Motile 
responses in nerve— Varying responsive effects under different conditions 
—Reversal of the normal response of light due to (1) depression of 
excitability below par ; (2) fatigue—The sequence of responsive phases 
during and after application of light—Demonstration of multiple 
responses in retina under light, as analogous fo those in vegetable 
tissues — Three types of after-effect—Multiple after-excitations in human 
retina—Binocular Alternation of Vision—Demonstration of pulsatory 
response in human retina during exposure to light —. , ‘ - 415 


CHAPTER XXIX 


GEO-ELECTRIC RESPONSE 


‘Theory of Hydrostatic Pressure and Theory of Statoliths—Question regarding 
active factor of curvature in geotropic response, whether contraction or 
expansion—Crucial experiment by local application of cold—Reasons for 
delay in initiation of true geotropic response—Geo-electric response of 
shoot —Due to active contraction of upper side, with concomitant gal-— 
vanometric negativity—Geo-electric response of an organ physically 
restrained . ; : ; : . , . : ; ; «5 @34 


CHAPTER XXX 


DETERMINATION OF VELOCITY OF TRANSMISSION OF 
EXCITATION IN PLANT TISSUES ; 


Transmission of excitation in plants not due to hydromechanical disturbance, 
but instance of transmission of protoplasmic changes—Difficulties in 
accurate determination of velocity of transmission—A perfect method— 
Diminution of conductivity by fatigue—Increased velocity of transmission 
with increasing stimulus—Effect of cold in diminishing conductivity — 
Effect of rise of temperature in ‘enhancing conductivity— Excitatory 
concomitant of mechanical and electrical response—Electrical methods 
of determining velocity of transmission—Method of comparison of longi- 
tudinal and transverse conductivities—Tables of comparative velocities in 
animal and plant —Existence of two distinct nervous impulses, positive 
and negative ; ; Saas , ° . ; ; ; » 444 « 


CONTENTS atte xxiii 


CHAPTER XXXI 


ON x ‘NEW METHOD FOR THE QUANTITATIVE STIMULATION 
OF NERVE 

WA : PAGE 
Drawbacks to use of electrical stimulus in nehenpading electrical response — 
Response to equi-alternating electrical shocks—Modification of response 
by. decline of injury—Positive after-effect—Stimulation of nerve by 
thermal. shocks—Enhancement of normal response after tetanisation — 
Untenability of theory of evolution of carbonic acid—Abnormal positive 
response converted into normal negative after tetanisation—Gradual 
transition from positive to negative, through intermediate diphasic— 
Effect of depression of tonicity on excitability and conductivity—Con- 
version of abnormal into normal response by increase of stimulus-intensity 
—Cyclic variation of response under molecular modification . , - 456 


CHAPTER XXXII 


ELECTRICAL RESPONSE OF ISOLATED VEGETAL NERVE 


Specialised conducting tissues—Isolated vegetal nerve—Method of ob- 
taining electrical response in vegetal nerve—Similarity of responses of 
plant and animal nerve: (a) action of ether—(4) action of carbonic 
acid—(c) action of vapour of alcohol—(d) action of ammonia—(e) ex- 

‘ hibition of three types of response, negative, diphasic and positive— 
(/) effects of tetanisation of normal and modified specimens—Effect of 
increasing stimulus on response of modified tissue ‘ ; : . 468 


CHAPTER XXXIII 


THE CONDUCTIVITY BALANCE 


Receptivity, conductivity, and responsivity—Necessity for distinguishing 
these—Advantages of the Method of Balance —Simultaneous comparison of 
variations of receptivity, conductivity and responsivity—The Conductivity 
Balance—Effect of Na,CO, on frog’s nerve—Effect of CuSO,—Effect 
of chemical reagents on plant nerve—Effect of CaCl, on responsivity— 
‘Responsivity variation under KCl—Comparison of simultaneous effects 
of NaCl and NaBr on responsivity—Effects of Na,CO, in different 
dilutions on conductivity—Demonstration of two different elements in 
conductivity, velocity and intensity—Conductivity versus responsivily— 
(a) effect of KI—(é) Effect of NaI—Effect of alcohol on receptivity, 
conductivity, and responsivity Comparison of simultaneous effects of 
alcohol—(a) on receptivity verses conductivity-~ (4) on receptivity Versus 
responsivity... : : : , : ps reas , . -. 479 


€ 
i 
‘ 
=: 
i 
oi 
f 
JF, 


* 


> 
7 
* 


> 
rel 


<e 


2 


ee ee 


—~ 
a 
at 


XXIV COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


CHAPTER XXXIV 


EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE AND AFTER-EFFECTS OF STIMULUS 


ON CONDUCTIVITY 


Effect of temperature in inducing variations of conductivity : (@) by Method 
of Mechanical Response ; (4) by Method of Electric Balance—Effect of 
cold—Effect of rising temperature—The Thermal Cell—After-effect of 
stimulation on conductivity—The Avalanche Theory—Determination of 
after-effect of stimulus on conductivity by the Electrical Balance —After- 
effects of moderate stimulation—After-effect of excessive stimulation 


CHAPTER XXXV 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE 


Current assumption of non-motility of nerve—Shortcomings of galvano- 
metric modes of detecting excitation— Mechanical response to continuous 
electric shocks—Optical Kunchangraph—Effect of ammonia on the 
mechanical response of nerve—Effect of morphia—Action of alcohol— 
Of chloroform—Abnormal positive or expansive response converted into 
normal contractile through diphasic, after tetanisation—Similar effects 
in mechanical response of vegetal nerve—Mechanical response due to 

- transmitted effects of stimulation—Determination of velocity of trans- 
mission—Indeterminateness of velocity in isolated nerve—Kunchan- 
graphic records on smoked glass—Oscillating recorder— Mechanical 
response of afferent nerve—Record of mechanical response of nerve due 
to transmitted stimulation, in gecko—Fatigue of conductivity —Conver- 
sion of normal contractile response into abnormal expansive, through 

- diphasic, due to fatigue 


CHAPTER XXXVI 


MULTIPLE RESPONSE OF NERVE 


Great sensitiveness of the high magnification Kunchangraph—Individual 
contractile twitches shown in tetanisation of nerve—Sudden enhance- 
ment of mechanical response of nerve on cessation of tetanisation— 
Secondary excitation— Multiple mechanical excitation of nerve by single 
strong stimulation—Multiple mechanical excitation of nerve by drying . 


CHAPTER XXXVII 


RESPONSE BY VARIATION OF ELECTRICAL RESISTIVITY 


Variation of resistance in Dionea, by ‘ modification’—Excitatory change, 
its various independent expressions—Characteristic difficulties of investi- 


PAGE 


497 


597 


532 


CONTENTS XXV 


PAGE 
gation—Morographic record by variation of resistivity—Inversion of 
curves at death-point—-Similarities between mechanical, electro-motive 
and resistivity curves of death—The true excitatory effect attended by 
diminution of resistance—Response of plant nerve by resistivity varia- 
tion—Independence of resistivity and mechanical variations—Responsive 
resistivity variation in frog’s nerve, and its modification under anzesthetics 540 


CHAPTER XXXVIII 


FUNCTIONS OF VEGETAL NERVE 


Feeble conducting power of cortical tissues—Heliotropic and geotropic 
effects dependent on response of cortical tissues only— Phenomenon of 
correlation—Excitability of tissue maintained in normal condition only 
under action of stimulus—Physiological activities of growth, ascent of 
sap, and motile sensibility, maintained by action of stimulus—Critical 
importance of energy of light—Leaf-venation a catchment-basin-—Trans- 
mission of energy to remotest parts of plants—Plant thus a connected 
and organised entity . , ? : , , P ; ; . 551 


CHAPTER XXXIX 


ELECTROTONUS 


Extra-polar effects of electrotonic currents on vegetal nerve —Electrotonic 
variation of excitability— Bernstein’s polarisation decrement—Hermann’s 
polarisation increment— Investigation into the law of electrotonic varia- 
tion of conductivity—Investigation on variation of excitability--Con- 
ductivity enhanced when excitation travels from places lower to higher 
electric potential, and depressed in opposite direction—When feeble, 
anode enhances and kathode depresses excitability—All electrotonic 
phenomena reducible to combined action of these factors—Explanation 
of apparent anomalies . > , ; ; : ; : , . 560 


: CHAPTER XL 
INADEQUACY OF PFLUGER’S LAW 


Reversal of Pfliiger’s Law under high E.M.F.—Similar reversals under 
feeble E.M.F.—Investigation by responsive sensation—Experiments on 
living wounds— Under moderate E.M.F., intensity of sensation enhanced 
at kathode, and depressed at anode—Under feeble E.M.F., sensation 
intensified at anode and depressed at kathode Aas {apaeernre of electrical 
currents in medical practice s : : ¢ : ; ‘ »'s 578 


XXVi COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


CHAPTER XLI 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION AND ITS 
TRANSMISSION : | 

‘PAGE 

Two opposite responsive manifestations, negative and positive—Such 

opposite responses induced by polar effects of currents of different signs 

—Arbitrary nature of term ‘ excitatory ’"—Pro-excitatory and anti-excita- 

tory agents—Molecular distortion under magnetisation in magnetic sub- 

stances—Different forms of response under magnetic stimulation— 

Mechanical, magneto-metric, and electro-motive responses—Uniform 

magnetic responses—Response exhibiting periodic groupings—TIneffec- 

tive stimulus made effective by repetition—Response by resistivity- 

variation— Molecular model—Response of inorganic substance to electric 

radiation—Effect of rise of temperature in hastening period of recovery 

and diminishing amplitude of response—Sign of response reversed under 

feeble stimulation—Conduction of magnetic excitation—The Magnetic 

Conductivity Balance—Effect of A-tonus.and K-tonus, on excitability 

and conductivity—Conducting path fashioned by stimulus—Transmission 

of excitation temporarily blocked in iron wire, as in bporisar 7 nerve— 

Artificial nerve-and-muscle preparation ; ‘ : ‘ Mega 9 


CHAPTER XLII 


MODIFICATION OF RESPONSE UNDER CYCLIC MOLECULAR 
VARIATION 


Anomalies of response—Explicable only from consideration of antecedent 
molecular changes—Continuous transformation from sub-tonic to hyper- 
tonic .conditions—Two methods of inquiry, first by means of character- 
istic curves, second by progressive change of response—Abnormal re- 
sponse characteristic generally of A or sub-tonic state—Abnormal trans- 
formed into normal, after transitional B state—B state characterised by 
staircase response—Responses at C stage normal and uniform—At stages 
D and E responses undergo diminution and reversal—Responsive pecu- 
liarities seen during ascent of curve, repeated in reverse order during 
descent—All these peculiarities seen not only in living but also in in- 
organic substances, under different methods of observation—Elucidation 
of effect of drugs—Response modified by tonic condition and past 
history . F : 3 ; , : , ; é ; . OFS 


CHAPTER XLIII 


CERTAIN PSYCHO-PHYSIOLOGICAL PHENOMENA—THE PHYSICAL 
BASIS OF SENSATION uf 


Indications of stimulatory changes in nerve : 1, Electrical ; 2, Mechanical— 
Transmission in both directions—Stimulatory changes in motor and 


5 
q 
; 


CONTENTS XXVii 


: PAGE 
sensory nerves similar— Responsive molecular changes and the correlated 
tones of sensation—Two kinds of nervous impulse, and their character- 
istics— Different manifestations of the same nervous impulse determined 
by nature of indicator-—Electrical, motile, and sensory responses, and 
their mutual relations—The brain as a perceiving apparatus—Weber- 
Fechner’s Law—Elimination of psychic assumption from explanation of - 
particular relation between stimulus and resultant sensation—Explana- 
tion of the factor of quality in sensation—Explanation of conversion from 
positive to negative tone of sensation after tetanisation—Various effects 
of progressive molecular change in nerve—Effects of attention and inhibi- 
tion—Polar variations of tonus, inducing acceleration and retardation . 644 


CHAPTER XLIV 


DISSOCIATION OF COMPLEX SENSATION 


Conversion of pleasurable into painful sensation, and vice versa, by electro- 
tonus—The Sensimeter—Mechanical stimulation—Stimulation by ther- 
mal shocks—Chemical stimulation—Opposite effects of anode and 
kathode—Normal effects reversed under feeble E.M.F. —Negative tone 
of sensation blocked by alcohol and anzesthetics—Separation of positive 
and negative sensations, by lag of one wave behind the other—Dissocia- 
tion of sensation by depression of conductivity—A bolition of the negative 
or painful element by block of conduction . ; : : ; . 666 


CHAPTER XLV 


MEMORY jos oe be - 677 


CHAPTER XLVI 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ISOTROPIC ORGANS - . 687 


CHAPTER XLVII 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ANISOTROPIC ORGANS _ + 700 


CHAPTER XLVIII 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF NERVE AND RELATED PSYCHOLOGICAL 
PHENOMENA . : é ' S55 


CLASSIFIED LIST OF EXPERIMENTS ‘ ‘ ‘ : « » 925 


OR ete See ee A go? gy 


a, ; a ; _ oe -s = i ; . 7 : F 
2 Pes pS nb pease fio 
ation Pai? : ‘ , A , 
Sako ' 4° as. bet get s Te re fuices 
> rs Ri ” all . ? * 
j at . Lae = visa iE Le a 


oh stole ar eee 39 Bombe? + Sell dans ‘qu 
: Cage : 


Joes ans mae Ma nT 
: mes Oy ore. SS aiees 
- ay aon te ; o- 


sa, ig Fee 
; 


. cre) ae 


< Fy Le vo Ae sie 


; ” 7 Agta se i La “ibn. s 


PA na 2 Be 5 teh = 
itt >. ix aeeahe Giant ved ee Wiest sak “eh atte s 
nae cd 

Nees ata, ta pesen tye 


‘ 


i “ 
pu Bt sehns Jatsoit Bhi iar hoe 


atthe 


pirat ye 


‘Yrs j ¢ 4 
ee ee 


Oa a Oa 


FIG. 


— 
BO APES Pet 


ILLUSTRATIONS 


Series of Contractile Responses in Muscle 

Response of Indiarubber 

Response of Selenium to the Siimulas of Light. 

Negative Response of Galena to Hertzian Radiation 

Positive Response of Ag’ to Electric Radiation... 

Electric Response in Metals é 

Uniform Electric Response in Tin ; 

Fatigue in the Electric Response of Metals . ; 
Stimulating Action of Na,CO, on Electric Response of Siatinum 
Abolition of Response in Metal by Oxalic Acid 


.. Response by Method of Relative Depression 
12. 


Arrangement for observing Simultaneous Mechanical aa Flectrical 
Responses ; 

Simultaneous Merhantcal aa Hlectrical tone | in Biophytum 

Photographic Record of Electrical Response by Galvanometric 
Negativity of Pulvinus of J/mosa, when leaf is_ physically 
restrained from falling : . Se 

Method of Transmitted Stimulation . 

Excitation by Sudden Tension ’ 

Excitatory Response to Tension and Corspavarion 

The Mechanical Tapper 

The Torsional Vibrator 

The Vibratory Stimulator P 

Complete Apparatus for Method of Block a Nitcators Stinrulation . 

Influence of Suddenness on the Efficiency of Stimulus . iets 

Spring Attachment for obtaining Vibration of Uniform Rapidity 

Additive Effect . ‘ ‘ ; : : : 

Response Recorder . ; : 

Photographic Record of Uniform 7 te (Radish) 

Stimulation by Thermal Shocks 

Photographic Record of Uniform Response in Peiiole Fe em to 
transmitted excitation 


Taps of increasing strength I : 2: 3: “4 sadectan ep obey Sibente | 


in leafstalk of turnip 


ba] 
2 
OO ON ANUP WWD SY GF 


— 
ons 


XXX COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Increased Response with Increasing Vibrational Stimuli (Caulifiower- 
stalk) ‘ 

Responses to Indicating Stimulus obtuined: with’ Two Rpeelneas of 
Stalk of Cauliflower 

Genesis of Tetanus in Muscle 

Photographic Record of Genesis of Tetanus in Methaeieal Response 
of Plants (Style of Datura alba) ‘ ee 

Fusion of Effect of Rapidly Succeeding SGmuli : ; - 

Response of (a) quickly reacting Amaranth ; (6) of iia Coletta 

Arrangement for Instantaneous Stimulation . Fos 

General Arrangement for Rheotomic Observation 

Enlarged View of Balanced Keys 

Curve showing Rise and Fall of Rc aesive E, M. Change: onder 
moderate stimulation 

Response Curve from Rheotomic ch eee in Sin sf tuarcith 
under strong stimulation . : 

Artificial Hydraulic Response of Minioss ° 

Experimental Arrangement for obtaining Records on Staoked Devas 
of Responses given to Direct and Indirect Stimulation, by Leaf of 
Mimosa . 

Mechanical Responiies: of Eoat of ‘Mihasa. 

Mechanical Response of Bzophytum to Thermal Stimuiation . 

Record of Response of A/imosa Leaf, taken on a fast-moving drum 

The Abnormal Positive preceding the Normal Negative in Mechanical 
and Electrical Responses in Biophytum 

Photographic Record of Electrical Response of Petiole 7 Ceutinwer 

Photographic Record of Electrical Responses of Potato-tuber 

Photographic Record of Electrical.Response of Petiole of Fern . 


Longitudinal Contraction and Retardation of Growth under Light in 


Hypocotyl of Szzapzs nigra 
Record of Growth in Crinum at Pen petacats of 34° C. and 3 5° C. 
Balanced Record of Variation of Growth in Flower-bud of Crinum 
Lily under Diffuse Stimulation of Light 
Diagrammatic Representation of the Tonic Level 


Photographic Record of Abnormal Positive passing into Recoial 


Negative Response in a Withered ica crie of Leaf-stalk of 
Cauliflower . ‘ 

Photographic Record af Sesieade Reeaenae | in Vascioias Net erve 

Staircase Increase in Electrical Response of Petiole of Pe 
rendered sluggish by cooling . : 

Photographic Record of Uniform Ronusaals: (Radic 

Photographic Record of Uniform Response in Petiole of Fern 

Record showing Diminution of Response, when sufficient Time is not 
allowed for Full Recovery 

Fatigue in Celery 

Fatigue in Leaf-stalk of Caulidowse” 

Photographic Record showing Fatigue in Tin Wire which had ids 
continuously stimulated for Several Days . : : + ti 


er”, = ee 


Sl ee i 


ea 


78. 


QI. 
92. 


ILLUSTRATIONS xXxxi 


Effect of Over-strain in producing Fatigue 

Rapid Fatigue under Continuous Stimulation in (a) Muscle’; (3) Beak. 
stalk of Celery (Electrical Response) ~ 

Photographic Records of Normal Mechanical Gee of Mitiova to 
Single Stimulus (upper figure), and to Continuous Stimulation 
. (lower figure) < ‘ i ; 

Effect of Continuous Vibration (dictieh 50°) i in Carrot . 

Oscillatory Response of Arsenic -acted on Continuously by iertion 
. Radiation : 

Mesusia Fatigue (a) in "Electrical Radpohies of Petiole of Cauli. 
flower; (4) in Multiple Electric Responses of Peduncle of 

. Biophytum ; (c) in Multiple Mechanical Responses of Leaflet of 

. Biophytum ; and (d@) in Autonomous Responses of Desmodium . 

Photographic Record of Periodic rio in the Automatic Pulsation 

. of Desmodium Eyraus . ‘ ‘ : ; ; 

Periodic . Fatigue in Pulsation of Frog’s s Heart (Pembrey and 
Phillips) . 

Photographic Record at Peisdie: F sae sete Continous: Sitiala: 

. tion in.Contractile Response (Filament of Uric/is Lily) . 

Fatigue in the.Contractile Response of Indiarubber 

Reversed Response of Fatigued Nerve 


PAGE 
94 
96 
od 
97 


98 


98 
99 
99 
100 


Iol 
102 


Preliminary Staircase, followed by iigiey in the Responses Br 


Muscle (Brodie) . . 

Preliminary Staircase, Riteeeics followed = ueasn: in we Rediolae 
of Galena to Hertzian Radiation ; 

Photographic Record of Responses of Style of Datura alba in oes 
Growth had come to a Temporary Stop ‘ : cae 

Differential Contractile Response of Artificial Strip 

Responses of AZzmosa to Sunlight. of not too long Duration 

Transverse Response of Pulvinus of Mimosa. ‘ 

Diametric Method of Stimulation of-an Anisotropic Crab 

The Thermal Variator 

Responsive Current in. Petiole of Musa frou Concave to Comvis Side 

Parallelism of Natural Current in Pulvinus of AZmosa and Sheathing 
Petiole of W/usa 2 got 

Effect of Variation of Tuapeecuse on Natural Cotrene: \; which in 
Petiole. of MZusa flows from Convex to Concave Side ‘ 

Photogiaphic Record showing effect of Sudden, followed by steady 
Rise of Temperature on Natural Current, |, in A/usa 

Action of 7 per cent. Solution of Na,CO, on Natural Current of Musa 

Effect of CO, on Natural Current of Musa . : 

Variation of the Transverse Natural and icapalaee Curfents: in 
. Pulvinus of JZ@mosa. 

Photographic Record of Effect of @hidroform on ecnbaak of Chae 

Photographic Record showing Action of Chloral Hydrate on the 
. Responses of Leaf-stalk. of Cauliflower . ; ‘ 

Photographic Record showing Action of Formalin (Radish) . 

Abolition of Response at both A and B Ends "7 yg Action of 
NaOH ‘ : : , ; 


103 
103 


104 
108 
109 
110 
III 
113 
115 


118 
119 
120 
122 
122 


127 
130 


131 


-132 


134 


XXXii COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


FIG, 


116. 


118. 


119. 


Photographic Record showing the nearly complete Abolition of 
Response by strong KOH 


Photographic Record showing the Siieiilataky Aisin of Sdhition of 


Sugar : Ne 

Photographic Record Dowie Coctinens Actes wae 2 per seat: 
Na,CO, Solution . ‘ 

Photographic Record showing the Depressing Action af 5 per ceil 
HCl Acid . ‘ 

Photographic Record deeine Effect of I per oat KHO. 

Photographic Record of Effect of 5 per cent. KHO. : 

Striking-rods for stimulation of two ends of specimen and inducing 
phase-difference . 

Isolated and diphasic responses weit one aihesence of shins ; 

Photographic Record showing Negative, Diphasic, and Positive 
Resultant Responses in Tin ; 

Photographic Records of Response of Bryphyltin 

Photographic Record of ee of Petiole of Cauliflower by ‘the 
Diametric Method F : , ; 

Distribution of Electrical Tension in Maecte> oylinder . 

Photographic Record showing Persistent Electrical After- Effect i in 
Inorganic Substance under Strong Stimulation . 

Photographic Record exhibiting Persistent Galvanometric Negativity 
in Plant Tissue after Strong Stimulation 

Experimental Arrangement for ier Electrical Effect due to 
Section ‘ 

Records showing increasing Poe Galvansmeuie ‘Regativiey; 
according as injury is caused nearer to proximal contact . 


Curve showing the Electrical Distribution in Stem with one Sectioned © 


End. ‘ : ‘ ‘ . : ‘ : ‘ é 

Electrical Distribution in Plant-cylinder with Opposite Ends 
Sectioned 

Record of es wnces | in Plant (Leaf: stalk | Coulifiowes by Method 
of Negative Variation : - d 

Response by Positive Variation of Reasina Current 

Distribution of Electric Potential in Lamina of Colocasia along a enciial 
line from dead to living through intermediate stages 

Straight Form Potentiometer ° 

Distribution of Electric Potential in Penal of Nymphica ott one 
end of which has been killed 

Photographic Records of Responses of Vereinbie N erve, one end of 
which has been injured . ‘ 

Typical Cases of Variation of Current of Rest and ction: Corsent, 
Specimen originally isotropic , 

Typical Cases of Variation of Current of Rest and ‘Actiow: Cunrent:; 
intermediate point naturally less or more excitable than either of 
terminal 

Typical Cases of Vor aeon of Guceat of Het and Acton: Comeonit 
Anisotropic organ, B end originally more excitable than A 


PAGE 
135 
136 


136 


w= =—- 7 


Ve ee ee ee 


eS SS Oe 


' FIG, 


120. 
121. 
122. 
123. 
124. 
125. 
126. 
527. 


128. 
129. 


130. 


131. 
132. 


133. 


134.. 


135. 


136. 


137. 


138. 


139. 


140. 
I4I. 
142. 


143. 
144... 


145. 


ILLUSTRATIONS | xXXxXi 


b 


—e 
me 


PAGE 
Photographic Record showing Effect of Rapid Cooling, by Ice-cold 
Water, on Pulsations of Desmodium gyrans 181 
Photographic Record of Pulsations of Desmodium during Continuous 
Rise of Temperature from 30° C. to 39° C. f . 182 
Diminution of Response in Zucharis by Lowering of Tesh peretavé «+883 
_After-effect of Cold on Ivy, Holly, and Zucharis Lily . 184 
Photographic Record of Responses in Zucharis Lily during the Rise 
and Fall of Temperature - 185 
Diminished Amplitude of Response with Rising Temperature (Stem 
‘of Amaranth) .. .- “ght 186 
Photographic Records of AenblaGiindeas Dibleanie in Driiahisaes, 
showing Increase of Amplitude and Decrease of Frequency, with 
Lowering of Temperature 188 
Photographic Record showing Effect of Sieant 3 in shicliabitinie ihiehoaes 190 
Record of Electric Responses of Amaranth at various Temperatures. 195 
Photographic Record of Thermo-mechanical Curve given by Coronal 
Filament of Passiflora 198 
Thermo-mechanical Curve of Two Different nineciaiee of Style of 
Datura alba, obtained from Flowers of the same Plant 198 
The Thermal Chamber 200 
Photographic Record exhibiting Bicetsie Spades in the Petiole of 
Musa 202 
Photographic Réecond showed Electric Taversion at Death siecle 
59°5°, in the Petiole of Amaranth . 203 
Record showing Inversion of Electric Curve and ‘Siti Nalepsises 
Reversal of Electric Response in Stem of Amaranth. 204 
Multiple Mechanical Response of Aiophytum, due to a Single 
Strong Thermal Stimulus . 208 
Multiple Electro-tactile Response in ‘Stem of Wenosa due # Bite 
Strong Thermal Stimulus . ; 209 
Photographic Record of Multiple Electrical Ripon: in Lae a 
Biophytum . 209 
Multiple Electrical ieasheses ade Different Bonus val Siiehlies | in 
Different Organs 210 
Photographic Record of Multiple Electrical Response to Siteie 
Thermal Shock in Frog’s Stomach : 210 
Induction of Autonomous Response in Siophytum ‘a Moderately 
High Temperature of 35°C. . 211 
Initiation of Multiple Response in Ester Leaflet of ‘iceman 
originally at Standstill . : : ‘ 212 
Photographic Record of Autonomous Mechanical Pislestion in 
Desmodium Leaflet . 213 
Spark-record of Single Pulsation in . Leaflet of Detwotiiuats . 214 
Photographic Records of Simultaneous Mechanical and Electrical 
Pulsation of Desmodium Leaflet é ; 218 
Photographic Record of. Simultaneous Mathaniont tnd Electrical 
Pulsation in Leaflet of Desmodium, before and after Physical | 
Restraint of Leaflet . é : ; ; ‘ : 


« 220 


“‘Xxxiv COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


FIG. 
146. ~ 
147... 
148. . 
149. 
150. . 
I5I. 
152. : 
153. 
154. . 


155: 


156. | 
187. 
7858... 
159. 
160. 
161. 
162. 


163... 
164. 


165. 
166. 
167. 
168. 
169... 
$70. 
St. 


172. 
7% 


PAGE 
Crescographic Record of ee Growth- “responses in Peduncle of . 
Crocus. .« ‘ : ‘ 221 
Natural and db co nonctet Guuveeste 3 in Teavek cp 2284 
Burdon Sanderson’s Fundamental Experiment on Dioian Leaf 229° 
_ Parallel Experiment in Sheathing Petiole of A/usa » «2229 
Positive Response of certain Leaves of Dionea — 230 
Diphasic Response of Leaf of Dzonea ‘ in Hapa Positive followed 
. by negative. . . . ; 230 
Positive Response of same bet when ‘ modified ? by" peeiiiies ae" 
stimulation. .. .. ” : 230 
. Experimental Connections with Piosive asicviinn to the second 
‘Experimental Method of Burdon Sanderson’... ioe RO Bam © 
Response. of Under-surface of, Leaf of Dionea, with Electrical 
Connections as in Fig. 153: Wee: iss 
- Photographic Records of Positive, Tiphasie, ee Megative Rape 
of. Petiole of J/Zusa depending on the Effective antensity OF <3 35 
. Transmitted Stimulus .. . ° ° 238. 
Electrical Response of Lamina of Nymphaea alba due to Pamelor. ; 
Excitation from Petiole . / 246 
Diagrammatic Representation by Du fois: Reymond for Explanation 
_ of Electrical Response in Organ’of Zorpedo . : i gaz 
Photographic Records of Responses given by Leaf of Coleus wromae ais: 
when both Surfaces are Excited Simultaneously by cigars 
VSRGGK To ye~ 4 Apa 248 
Experimental Adan deiace ig Mhovicant Ghicteniices 252 
Records of Two Successive Responses in Leaf of Soph lies 
calycinum under Equi-alternating Electrical Shocks. . ° - 253 
. Response-curve from Rheotomic Observation on Leaf of Nymphaea 
alba . ; : : es | 
Series of Responses ees i hast a Fidrosferamum subtrifolium to 
Stimulus of Equi-alternating Electrical Shocks . : 0 255 
Photographic Record of Responses of Carpel of Dzl/enia nikita 256 
Photographic Record of Normal Responses given by Pitcher of 
Nepenthe, under Equi-alternating Electric Shocks . ‘ . 256 
Responsive Currents in Lead Wire , 264 
Flat Strip of Lead, of which lower Surface is omnittated:| . 265 
Photographic Records of After-effect of Homodromous ¢ and Hetero- 
dromous | Induction-shocks:in prepared Strip of Lead .. ¥- 6 Lee 
Photographic Record of Responses to -Equi-alternating Electric °. 
Shocks in Prepared Lead Strip. -. ae » 267 
Response of Pulvinus of AMtmosa- to Equi alternating Electric ey: 
Shocks .:+ . ; ; : 268 
Experimental Arrangement. for Detantiination of Excitatory ‘Afver- 
effect of Equi-alternating Electrical Shocks. 276 
Method of Direct Effect of Excitation by Equi- caltamvating Shocks 280 
Excitation by Equi-alternating Shocks 281 
Photographic Record of Response of Petiole of Bis: to “Equi: “alee 
nating Electric Shocks, before and after Application of Chloroform 284 


A 
: 
’ 


~ 


ILLUSTRATIONS Tse XXXV 


Photographic Record of Responses of ee Stem of Cac ‘bila 
to Equi-alternating Electric Shocks. ¥: 
Electrical Responses of Eel to Equi-alternating Electrical Shocks 
Rotary Mechanical Stimulator. _.. - aes ea 
Diagram Representing Different Levels - Excitability, ne ‘Leto, 
and Minus .. . - 
Electrical Response of Grape: arte to Rotary Méchanical Stimulation 
Electrical Response of Frog’s Skin to Rotary Mechanical Stimulation 
Photographic Record of Electrical Kesponses of Upper Surface of 
Intact Human Forefinger to Rotary Mechanical Stimulation — 
Photographic Record of Electrical’: Responses ee ork skin to 
. Thermal Shocks .. } 
Photographic Record of Electrical Rexpceseh of Grape-skin to Stimid 
_ lation by. Equi-alternating Electrical.Shocks - 
Photographic Record of Series of Electrical Responses of Frog’s s Skin 
. to Equi-alternating Electrical Shocks .. -. 
Photographic Record of Transverse Response of. Palvinus af Mimosa 
. to Equi-alternating Electrical Shocks... ray 
Continuous. Photographic Record of Autonomous Pulsation of Des- 
_ modium gyrans from 6 P.M. to 6 A.M, an, 
Photographic Record of Electrical Responses in Skin ‘of Neck of 
. Tortoise.to Stimulus-of Equi-alternating Electrical Shocks .. - . 
Isolated Responses of Upper and Lower Surfaces of Skin of Tomato 
. to Rotary Mechanical Stimulus. ~ 
Photographic Record of Series of Wesncaaa: in Skin of Eatante er 
. Equi-alternatin Electrical Shocks. . 
A Single Response of Skin of Tomato to Equbslternating Shock 
. recorded on Faster Moving Drum . 
Photographic. Record of Series of Normal Responses in Skin ve 
- Geckow’ :, 
Photographic Riou of ‘AtiiGened Dipbasis aiienonecs in Skin of 
Gecko, converted to Normal, after Tetanisation : 
Transverse Section of Tissue of Hollow Peduncle of Diialis aly: 
Photographic Record of. Responses of Water-melon to- meek 
- nating Electric Shocks . 
Photographic Rece d of- Electrical Responses ved Tnteet: Hiutaah 
- Armpit’. . +. : ‘y 
Experimental Arrangement ioe Ritepoiine of Hein’ Lip ey 
Photographic Record of Electrical Response of Intact Human Lip 
Possible Variations of Responsive Current, as. between. Two Surfaces 
- A and R, shown by. Means- of "Diagrammatic. iss paca of 


. Characteristic Curves. --.. - a ae y SLE ety 
Photographic Record: showing Resariid of N ofmial 2 eee in 
. Pulvinus of A/émosa due to Fatigue - 2 a A Ee 2 - 


.. Photographic Record showing Reversal of Resconse in Carpe of 


. Dillenia indica, wnder Sub-minimal Stimulation - 
Pitcher of Vepenthe, with lid removed. - oo 
Glandular Surface of a Portion of the aug Menibtaine: ofthe Pitcher 
> Of Nepenthe-.  -.0 2 were ie Se eee el 


336- 


XXXVi COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


FIG, 

202. 
203. 
204. 
205. 
206. 
207. 
208. 
200. 
210, 
211. 
212. 
213. 
214. 
215. 


216. 
217. 


218, 
219, 


220, 
221. 
222. 
223. 


224. 
225. 
226. 
227. 


228. 
229. 


230. 


Transverse Section of Tissue of Pitcher of Vepenthe. ‘ ; 

Photographic Record of Series of Normal Negative Responses of Glan- 
dular Surface of Wefenthe in Fresh Condition to Equi-alternating 
Electric Shocks 

Photographic Record of Réspdmses nf Pitcher i in Intermediate Stage, 
having Attracted a Few Insects . : 

Photographic Record of Responses of Pitcher in Third Stags) the 
whole Glandular Surface thickly Coated with Insects 

Multiple Response of Pitcher of Vepenthe, in First or Fresh Stage, to 
Single Strong Thermal Shock 

Multiple Response of Pitcher pf Wideathe: 3 in Third Siage, to Single 
Strong Thermal Shock . 

Photographic Record of Responses in Fresh Dest of D osera to Bau 
alternating Electrical Shocks 

Photographic Kecord of Multiple Remniaas é asd of \ Detniesh in 
Positive Phase 

Photographic Record of N etstad Negative Riepooies of Frog’: s 
Stomach to Mechanical Stimulation 

Photographic Record of Normal Negative Responses of Stomach of 
Tortoise to Stimulus of Equi-alternating Electric Shocks . 

Photographic Record of Normal Response in Stomach of Gecko to 
Equi-alternating Shocks, seen to be reversed after Tetanisation 

Photographic Record of Multiple Responses in Stomach of Frog to a 

_ Single Strong Thermal Shock 

Photographic Record of Normal Negative Relpdanie of Voume Root 
of Colocasia 

Photographic Record of Dasities Riaponie’ in Older Root of Caberastn 

Photographic Record of Electrical Response of Sap-wood 

Photographic Record showing Normal Responses of Living Wood te 
Vibrational Stimulus, and the Abolition of Response by a Toxic 
Dose of Copper Sulphate . a, ‘ 

The Shoshungraph 

Curve showing Normal Suction: at 23° Cy “Pricresisal Suction at 35° 
C., and the After-effect persisting on Return to Normal Tempera- 
ture . 

Action of Ansehibeticn 3 in ; Aiohiien of Suetion 

Effect of Strong KNO, Solution 

Effect of Strong NaCl Solution . 

Record of Blank Experiment showing Alwence of asiy Disturbenss 
of Record from Induction-shocks as such 

Terminal Mode of Application of Stimulus 

Sub-terminal Mode of Application of Stimulus 

Renewal of Suction, Previously at Standstill, by Action of Stineuies 

Photographic Record of Effect of Stimulus in Enhancing Rate of 
Suction -,. , ; ‘ 

Variation of Latent Peisad as Afar aie of Gitiaiie 5 

Photographic Record showing Variation of Latent Period as After- 
effect of Stimulus ; 

Suctional Response under Over- Galaace 


363 
369 


372 


375 
376 
377 


378 
380 
380 


383 


383 
384 


385 
386 


Se atl at ee 


ILLUSTRATIONS ’ XXXVii- 


Photographic Récord of Effect of Stimulus on Over-balance . : 

Photographic Record of Response to Continuous Sub- terminal Stimu- 
lation . 

Experimental Arauigeasent a Bictection of Electrical Change 

_ induced at the Point transversely Distal to Point stimulated . 


- Record of Response to Moderate Unilateral Stimulation under the 


Experimental Arrangement described . oles 

Record of Different Specimen under same Experimental boca: 
ment when Stimulus is first Moderate and then Increased 

Mechanical Response of Pulvinus of A/imosa to Continuous Action 
of Light from Above . : ° , 

Electrical Response in the Lower Half is the Pulvinus of Mimosa 

due to Stimulation of Distal Upper Half by Light . , 

Photographic Record of Series of Negative Responses of Petiole of 

. Bryophyllum to Stimuli of Sunlight . ‘ A : 

Record. of Responsive Growth-variation taken under céniliteie of 
balance in slightly Sub-tonic Flower-bud of Crz#zuwm Lily under 
Diffuse Stimulation of Light . , 

Photographic Record of Positive Response of the Petiole of Cauli- 
flower to Light . ‘ . 

Multiple Mechanical Response of Leaflet of Biohytun aa the 
Continuous Action of Light f : 

Photographic Record of Multiple Electrical eonceee in oe, 6 
Bryophyllum under Continuous Action of Light . . 

Diagrammatic Representation of Phasic Alternations, and After- effect 
in Type I. , . : : 

Photographic Record of Pieaie Kisccnationk. showing Direct and 
After Effects of Lightin Type I., represented by Bryophyllum . 

Diagrammatic Representation of Phasic Alternations, and After 
Effect in Type III. . ; . 

Photographic Record of Phasic Aaveriatien, showing ‘Disect a 

_ After Effects in Type III., represented by Petiole of Cauli- 
flower . 

Photographic Recut of Pals of Diiciecee dabei witha a Sicknd 
Specimen of Cauliflower, representative of Type ITI. ‘ ‘ 

Experimental Arrangement for Determination of Differential Excita- 
bility of Optic Nerve and Cornea . 

Series of Photographic Records of Excitatory etieniore 3 in F vis ’s 
Eye to Equi-alternating Electric Shocks 

Experimental Arrangement for Demonstration of Differential Excita- 
bility as between Retina and Optic Nerve . 

Series of Photographic Records of Excitatory teauiies in F mt s 
Retina to Equi-alternating Electric Shocks 

Photographic Record of Multiple Response of Retina of Frog snide 
Continuous Action of Light. , 

Response of petiole of Bryophyllum. Fighé: was cut off on sities 


ment of maximum positivity in the second of the multiple 


KespOnses, «.. ‘ : , ; , orig Ge 


PAGE 


386 
388 
397 
398 
399 
400. 
401 


402 
405 


407 


Xxxviii © COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


FIG. 


254. 
255. 


256. 
257: 
258. 
259. 
260. 
261. 
262. 
263. 
264... 
265. 
266. 
267. 


. 268. 
260. 


270.. 
\ 27 


272. 


273. 
274. 
275. 
276. 
277. 


278, 


279. 


Similar effect in response of tetina of Ophiocephalus-fish’. = . 

The same with another specimen. ‘Light was here‘cut ‘off after the 
first oscillation 

Response of retina of Ophistephalus whe slightly aid 

Response of frog’s eye (Kiihne and Steiner) -. ‘ 

After-effect of Light on Silver Bromide. . yal he : 

Response of petiole of cauliflower. Light was here cut off’ on attain. 
ment of maximum negativity... .° . reve ise 

Response of retina of Ophdocephalus fish hee ‘depneskeas te 

Response of isolated retina of fish as.observed by Kiihne and Steiner 


Inclined Slits for Stereoscope and Composite Image formed i in the 
Two Eyes : : 

Composite Indaaiphésable Word, “68 which Components iare . Seen 
Clearly on Shutting the Eyes - 2. -~. é 


Diagrammatic Representation: of a Multicellular. Organ laid ‘Hotei 

_ tally and Exposed to Geotropic Stimulus: . - 

Effect on Apogeotropic Movement of Temporary Application of Cold 
on Upper and Lower Surfaces respectively. 

Diagrammatic Representation of Experiment showing Curvature 
. Induced by Unilateral Pressure Exerted by Particles . : 

Record of Responsive Curvature Induced in Bud of Crinum ee by 
Unilateral Pressure of Particles .°  -.. 

Record of Apogeotropic Response in Scape of Bricks! Lily” 

Photographic Record of Geo-electric Response in the Scape - of 
Uriclis Lily laid horizontally. . ere: 

Experimental Arrangement. for Sidijecting: Organ to Geetespie 
‘Stimulus, Mechanical Response.being Restrained: 

Geo-electric Response of the’ Physically. Restrained Scape of Uriclis 
Labyet Ss: wae Lo . 

Diagrammatic Representation af Electrical Consheann for Deter- 
_mination of ‘Velocities of . Centrifugal and -Centripetal ‘Trans- 
missions .- . | s A ¢: ae i a 

Experimental Adregsinent fot coopster the Relative Conduc- 
tivities in Transverse and Longitudinal Directions eis 

Response of Frog’s Nerve under Simultaneous Excitation of both 
Contacts, by Equi- aereee Electrical Shek: one. Contact 
. being Injured e ; 

Bahbaticement of Amplitude of agus as After- effect * Thermal 
. Tetanisation, in Frog’s Nerve 

Conversion of Abnormal Positive into Normal Negative Response 
. after Thermal Tetanisation ..  -. ia 

Gradual Transition from Abnormal Boultvh tieoaish Diphasic, i 
Normal Negative Responses. in Frog’s’ Nerve - 

Abnormal Positive Response converted through Dipheate to grat 
Negative under the increasingly Effective Intensity.of Stimulus, 
brought about by Lessening the ssa between the ee 
and Stimulated Points... ...: % é ; 

Frond of Fern with Conducting Nerves akgaed ptha as sith. 


PAGE . 
427 

427 
428 
428 . 
429 
430 
430 
430° 
432 - 
432- 
435 - 
436 
437 


437 
438 


440° 
441 
442° 
447 
454 
457 
462 
463 
464 
466 
469 


’ FIG. 
280. 


281. 
282. 
283. 
. 284. : 


285. 


286. 


287. 


288. ° 


- ILLUSTRATIONS: » ‘! XX XIX 


Photographic Record of Effect of Ether on he Electrical Response of 
Plant-nerve . 

Photographic Ratants et Effect of Co, on Electrical Rescane of 
Plant-nerve . 


Photographic Record of Ripken of Response by Strong Apeicatton 


_ of Alcohol 


‘Photographic Record of Effect of Ammonia on 1 Ordinary ‘Tissue: sf 


Petiole of Walnut . 


Photographic Record of Effect. of. Similar Application of Socata . 


. on Plant-nerve. 


Photographic Resaca of Exhibition of Three Sues sf Response, . 


Normal Negative, Diphasic, and Abnormal Positive, in Nerve 
. of Fern under: Different Conditions : 

Photographic Record of Effect of Tetanisation in ES Enhative: 
ment of Normal Negative Response in Nerve of Fern . 

Photographic Record of Conversion of the Abnormal Di-phasic hitn 
. Normal Negative, after Tetanisation,.T, in. Nerve of Fern 

Photographic Record showing how the Abnormal Positive Response 
is converted through’ Diphasic into Normal Negative by the 

' Increasing Effective Intensity of Stimulus, due to Lessening the 
. Distance between the Responding and Stimulated Points. 


. Diagrammatic Representation of the Conductivity Balance . : 


Photographic Record made during Preliminary Adjustment fay 
Balance of Nerve of Fern . ‘ ‘ é : ‘ ; 

. Complete Apparatus of Conductivity Bitarice 

Effect of Na,CO, Solution on Responsive Excitability a F Bee s Nerve 

Effect of CuSO, on Frog’s Nerve 

Photographic -Record showing iighsocemeat o Rimehivile by 
Application of CaCl, . 


. - Photographic Record showing Haviedslcn of Rendaastie Excitability 


by Application of KCl . 
Photographic Record exhibiting Comparative Effects of NaCl tad 
NaBr on Responsivity 


Photographic Record of Effect Fi Dilute (* 5 per cee. ) Sates of 


Na,CO, on Variation of Conductivity 


Giistageanis Record of Effect of Stronger Dobe: (2 per ents i) of 


. Na,CO, Solution on Conductivity . ~- . é 


. RESPONSIVITY versus CONDUCTIVITY under KI ~ 


RESPONSIVITY versus CONDUCTIVITY under Nal 
. Effect of Alcohol on the Responsivity of Frog’s Nerve 


Photographic Record of Effect of Alcohol Vapour on Receptivity: 


- Photographic Record of Effect of Alcohof on Conductivity . 


. . Photographic Record showing Effect. of Alcohol on Responsivity 
- Diagrammatic Representation of Experimental Arrangement’ for 


’ Demonstration. of RECEPTIVITY versus CONDUCTIVITY, or of 
RECEPTIVITY versus RESPONSIVITY . ne haere 
RECEPTIVITY versus RESPONSIVITY under Alcohol 
Photographic Record aan Effect of spsiii on » Cohductivity of 
PjJant-nerve. : . . . 


PAGE 
472 
473 
473 
474 


474 


475 
476 


477 


477 
482 


482 
484 
485 
485 . 


486 
487 
487 
488 


489 
491 
492 
492 
493 
494 
494 


- 495 


495 


499 


COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


The Cork Chamber for Gradual Raising of the Temperature of one 
Arm of the Balance .- : 

Photographic Record Skiing Effect of Riau Tenipedsaded on 
Conductivity i 

Experimental Arrangement for Riadyiing ‘After- effect of Seances on 
Conductivity and Excitability : 

Photographic Record Showing Effect of ‘ Mosiaeate Siteaniatiend in 
Enhancing Conductivity and Excitability 


- Photographic Record showing Effect of Excessive Stimulation i in De- 


pressing Excitability and Conductivity 
Record of Contractile Response in Frog’s Nerve indi Contiawdus 
Electric Tetanisation . 


. . Optical Kunchangraph for Machsiticel Résponss of Hise 


Diagrammatic Representation of meee for Obtaining Petes. 
mitted Effect of Stimulus . 

Photographic Record of Effect of aia on pivecbanines Resneiize 
-of Frog’s Nerve . : . 

Photographic Record showing Abobasis of Mechanics Response oa 
Frog’s Nerve by Action of Solution of Morphia 

Photographic Record showing Preliminary Exaltation in Mechsnical 
Response of Frog’s Nerve after Application of Alcohol ‘ 

Photographic Record showing Effect of Chloroform on Mechanical 
Response of Frog’s Nerve . 


. . Photographic Record showing Mocornal hace commento’ into 


‘Negative Response after Tetanisation 

Photographic Records showing Gradual Dldatpéatanice oe Positive 
‘Element in diphasic Mechanical Responses of Frog’s Nerve 
and Plant-nerve 


. . Photographic Record sional Sinairense Effect in Medalees Re- 


sponse of Frog’s Nerve . 


. . Photographic Reproduction of Recon of Méckanicat Rea ponus af 


Frog’s Nerve and Plant-nerve obtained on Smoked Glass Surface 
of Oscillating Recorder 


324. Record of Mechanical Responses to Electrical Stamatis chaaiviell on. 
Smoked Glass, and given by the Optic Nerve of Fish Ophiocephalus 
325. Record, obtained on Smoked Glass, of Transmitted Effect of Stimu- 
lation on Nerve of Gecko . , z ee 
326. . Initiation of Multiple Response by Drying of Ne erve : . 
‘327. . Diagrammatic Representation of Experimental Arrangement for Re- 
: cording Response by Resistivity Variation . 
328. . Photographic Record of the Morographic Curve takin by Method of 
; Resistivity Variation in Pistil of Hibiscus. Critical point of in- 
version at 60°8° C, . ‘ 
329. Photographic Record of the Morographic aes ken os Method of 
Eléctromotive Variation in’ Petiole of JZusa. Critical point of 
inversion at 59:6° C.  . 
330. Photographic Record of the Morgeaphie Cave ‘sane by Method of 


Mechanical Response in Filament of Passifora. Critical point of 


inversion at 59°6 .C. . ; ; ‘ ° ‘ + Dhue . 


PAGE — 


500 
501 
504 
504 
505 


510 
511 


512 
516 
516 
517 
518 


521 


522 


524 


528 
529 
530 
539 
544 
546 


546 


546 


339- 
341. 
342. 


343- 
344. 


345. 


346. 


347: 
348. 
349- 


350. 


351. 


352. 


353+ 


354: 


ILLUSTRATIONS 


Response Records by Resistivity Variation, in the Nerve of Fern 

Effect of Chloroform seen in benim i of ee tg dk in 
Frog’s Nerve . 

Photographic Record of Effect of ali dienes in Enhancing Mechani- 
cal Response of Plant-nerve . 

Photographic Record showing Bekeucemeat of Excitability dndér 
Action of Light in Nerve of Fern 

Distribution of Fibro-vascular Elements in Siigle Sane of Stem df 
Papaya 

Extra-polar Kat- dccaponie Effect 

Extra-polar An-electrotonic Effect » 

Extra-polar Electrotonic Effects under an Maiae E. M. F, shins rises 
from *6 to 1°4 Volts . : 

Diagram illustrating Bernstein’s Dechanieat of Kat- cbiitioale Caiént 

Diagram illustrating Bernstein’s Decrement of An-electrotonic Current 

Diagram representing Hermann’s Polarisation-increment under 
Tetanising Shocks, with reversed polarising Current 

Diagram representing Hermann’s Polarisation-increment fades 
Tetanising Shocks, with reversed polarising Current : 

Experiment with Petiole of Fern demonstrating Variation of Cou 
ductivity by Polarising Current, Excitation travelling electrically 
Downhill : 

Experiment with Petiole of Fern desisghstraliais Masten of Bex: 
ductivity by Polarising Current, Excitation travelling electrically 
Uphill é ‘ 

Photographic Records of Rassonaes salons in last inegariseents when 
Excitation was transmitted with and against the Polarising Current. 

Photographic Record of Modification of Conduction during Passage 
of Excitation from Anodic to Kathodic Region, under Increasing 
Intensity of Polarising E.M.F. 

Photographic Record showing Eshanced Condectizin fie Kathodic 
to Anodic Region . 

Experimental Arrangement to Exhibit the eliccnmnent of xciat 
bility at Anode, when the Acting E.M.F. is feeble 

Experimental Arrangement to Exhibit Depression of Excitability a at 
Kathode, when the Acting E.M.F. is feeble . 

Photographic Records of Response, illustrating the Hevanchebiews: bf 
Excitability at Anode, and Depression at Kathode, under Feeble 
Acting E.M.F. in two Specimens of Nerve of Fern a and é. : 

Experimental Arrangement demonstrating the Joint Effects of 
Variation of Conductivity and Excitability by Polarising Current . 

Experimental Arrangement demonstrating the Joint Effects of 
Variation of Conductivity and Excitability by Polarising Current, 
when Current is Reversed . ‘ 

Photographic Record of Response under the Kerapasenenis given in 
Figs. 351, 352 in Nerve of Fern 

Experimental Arrangements for Showing so- called Polatisn tan: 
increment by the Joint Effect of Increased Excitability at Anode 
and Enhanced Conduction of Excitation electrically Uphill . 


Cc 


xli 


PAGE 


548 
55° 
554 
557 
558 
560 
560 
561 
562 
562 
563 


563 
565 . 


565 


566 


567 
568 
569 


569 


570 


572 


572 


572 


574 


xlfi 


FIG, 


355: 


356. 
357: 
359. 


360. 


365. 


COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Experimental Arrangements for Showing so-called Polarisation- 
increment by the Joint Effect of Increased Excitability at Anode 
and Enhanced Conduction of Excitation electrically Uphill. 
Direction of Current in this is Reversed : 

Photographic Record of Responses in Nerve of Fern, andes Anite 
and Kathodic Action as described in Figs. 354 and 355. 

Photographic Record of Similar Effects in Nerve of Frog . 

Make-kathode and Break-anode Effects in Biophytum : 

Effect of Anode and Kathode on Responsive Sensation in er 
Hand 

Polar Effects of Cureate ia to Eoclned Aprile on ones 
Half of Pulvinus of Erythrina indica 7 

Experimental Arrangement for Magnetometric. Method of Recard 

Photographic Record of Uniform Magnetic Responses of Iron 

Photographic Record of Periodic Groupings in Magnetic Responses 

Photographic Record of Response and Recovery of Steel under 
Moderate and strong Magnetic Stimulus 

Photographic Record showing Ineffective Stimulus made Effective by 
Repetition 

Molecular Model 

Method of Resistivity Macaious 

Photographic Record of Response of prin esiaaine Powder in  Sugpish 
Condition to Stimulus of Electric Radiation 

Photographic Record Showing Uniform Response of -Ahenvbetocn 
Powder to Uniform Stimulus of Electric Radiation . 

Photographic Record of Response of Tungsten 

Experimental Arrangement for obtaining Response in iro * i 
duction Current : ; 

Magnetic Conductivity Balance 

Process of Balancing illustrated by Pivotopra pate Record af Reiponnes. 

Effect of K- and a-Tonus on Magnetic Conduction : 

Opposite Effects of K-Tonus when moderate and strong 

Effects of k- and A-Tonus on Magnetic Excitability 

Gradual Enhancement of Conductivity by the Action of Stiendies 

Characteristic Curve of Iron under increasing Force of Magnetisation 


. Characteristic Conductivity Curve of Sensitive Metallic Particles be- 


longing to Negative Class, under increasing Electro-motive Force. 

Cyclic Curves of Magnetisation and of Conductivity 

Photographic Record of Magnetic Tetanisation of Steel, éthiblting 
Transient Enhancement of aba Speed on Cessation . 

Mechanical Response of Frog’s Nerve to successive equal Stimuli, 
applied at Intervals of One Minute 

Mechanical Response of Frog’s Nerve, showing Conversion of Ab- 
normal Positive into Normal Negative Response after Tetanisation 


. Photographic Record showing Conversion of Abnormal ‘ Down’ Re- 


sponse in Tin to Normal ‘ Up,’ after Tetanisation 


PAGE 


574 


575 
575 
579 


582. 


589 


594 
594 
594 
595 
595 
598 
600 


601 


602 


603 


604 
607 
608 
609 
610 
611 
612 
620 


621 
622 


623 
625 
627 


628 


ee St is. 


ILLUSTRATIONS 


Gradual Transformation from <Abonrek: to Normal Response in 
Platinum ; 

Normal Electro-motive Resnse 3 in Tin, sakuiced after Tetkiteation 

Photographic Record of Abnormal Response of Selenium Cel con- 

. verted into Normal after Tetanisation 

Photographic Record showing Moderate Normal Geshcees of Sclendigs 
enhanced after Tetanisation 

Photographic Record of Abnormal aN e unpeied to Electric 
-Radiation, converted after Tetanisation into Diphasic and Normal . 

Moderate Normal Response of Aluminium, enhanced after Tetanis- 
ation 

Photographic Record of Eahansemeat of ‘Mieneue neseouse aier 
Tetanisation 

Vertical Series of Recsds sowie Tensiorantion of iioecisl rao 
Normal Response after Tetanisation in Living and Inorganic alike 
in the A phase 

Series showing how Delamighon etieecese: ee Ressnien' in ‘the 
B Phase 

. Photographic Racorit storing Responses niaadian with different 

parts of characteristic curve in frog’s nerve 

Photographic Record of Response of Tungsten zh as finkance- 
ment of Response after moderate Tetanisation, and Reversal of 
Response, due to Fatigue under stronger Tetanisation . 

. Series showing reversal of Normal Response by mee due to cong 

Tetanisation inducing the E phase . 

Fatigue in Indiarubber giving rise to Dinkaue ana: Beveyied Re- 
sponses 

Fatigue age ss Diphasic Variation ana Reversal of N Scania Response 
in Frog’s Nerve 

Abnormal Response of ae ty Rieiasiobisens followed ii et 
Response of Contraction 

Record of Response in Nerve of Gecke showing the Effect of eat 
metically increasing Stimulus : : 

Response of Nerve of Bull-frog to Stimuli 1, 2, a peo Bs is Rn 
in Arithmetical Progression . 

Response of Optic Nerve of Ophioephalus e Arithmetically i sicenaiite 
Stimuli 1, 2, 3, 4, 5,6, 7 . ‘ 

Mechanical Response of Nerve of Fern to Arithmetiealy i increasing 
Stimulus 

Photographic Record my Magretic “Responses in Steel t to Arith- 
metically increasing Stimulus . : ‘ . - : 

The Sensimeter 

Revival of latent Image in Metal 


xii 
PAGE 


629 
629 


630 
yee 
632 
632 


633 


633 
634 


635 


638 
639 
642 
642 
649 
657 
658 
659 
659 
660 


670 
683 


whew 
Sas = 
Py) 


s2 Sk, hb git Tei, eC) 


COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


CHAPTER I 
THE MOLECULAR RESPONSIVENESS OF MATTER 


Response to stimulus by change of form—Permeability variation—Variation of 
solubility — Method of resistivity variation: (a) positive variation ; (4) 
negative variation—Sign of response changed under different molecular 
modifications—Response of vegetable tissue by variation of electrical re- 
sistance—Response by~electro-motive variation in inorganic substances— 
The method of block—Positive and negative responses—Similar responses in 
living tissues—Effects of fatigue, stimulants, and poisons on inorganic and 


organic responses — Method of relative depression, or negative variation, so 
called, 


IN studying the properties of living tissues, we find one of 
their most important characteristics is found in the fact 
that they exhibit the state of excitation under the impact of 
stimulus. On the cessation of stimulus, again, the excited 
tissue returns to its original condition. The excitatory change 
thus undergone is fundamentally due to the derangement, or 
upset, of the molecules of the living tissue from their normal 
equilibrium, recovery being brought about by their restoration 
to that state. The excitatory condition is sometimes shown 
by change of form, as in the case 
of the shortened length of excited 
muscle (fig. 1). This might be 
compared with the shortening of 
stretched india-rubber under ther-  *'~ ees ei rirstheag 
mal stimulus (fig. 2). 


Now it is clear that the molecular change consequent 
on excitation must occasion various concomitant physical 


B 


2 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


changes, and it should be theoretically possible to detect and 
measure this induced molecular change by recording such 
concomitant variations, Thus the stimulus of light, for 
example, may induce a mole- 
cular change which may in its 
turn induce, say, a variation 
in the permeability of the sub- 
stance to liquid. Bichromated 
gelatine becomes less perme- 
able under the action of light. 
The solubility of a substance 
Fic. 2. Response of India-rubber may again undergo variation 
sepsis ini ty nO at under external stimulus —sul- 
phur, for example, usually 
soluble in carbon disulphide, is rendered insoluble under 
the action of light. 

In order, then, to study the effect of a given stimulus 
with accuracy, we should be able to detect and measure the 
extent of the changes induced. The two effects which have 
just been referred to are not, as will be seen, highly susceptible 
of accurate measurement. But in the detection of molecular 
changes by electrical means, we have at our disposal methods 
-for the measurement of such changes, the ease and delicacy 
of which leave nothing to be desired. Two such methods 
may be used—that of Resistivity and that of Electro-motive 
Variation. According to the method of resistivity variation, 
the substance to be experimented on is placed in an electrical 
circuit, including a delicate galvanometer and a suitable 
electro-motive force, such as to cause a small deflection of 
the galvanometer. The impact of the stimulus on the sub- 
stance under examination now induces in it a molecular 
change by which its resistance is made to undergo a variation, 
which in the case of certain substances may be an increase, 
or in that of others adiminution. On the cessation of external 
stimulus, the substance shows recovery, with a corresponding 
return to its original conductivity. Thus in the case of 
selenium, for instance, the conductivity is increased, or the 


THE MOLECULAR RESPONSIVENESS OF MATTER 3 


resistance decreased, under the action of light. In fig. 3 is 
shown a number of responses to light, given on a series of 
separate exposures, each of one second’s duration, the inter- 
vening periods allowed for recovery being of one minute each. 


Fic. 3. Response of Selenium to the Stimulus of Light 
(Resistivity variation method) 


These responses were obtained by recording the increased 
deflection due to decreased resistance under the impact of 
light, and the subsequent recovery. Such responses, by means 
of decreased resistance, we may arbitrarily distinguish as 
negative. Similar responses are 
also given by a mass of metallic 
particles when acted upon by 
electric radiation. In fig. 4 are 
seen several of these negative 
responses given by galena under 
the action of this stimulus. 
There are, on the _ other 
hand, some substances: which _ 
give positive responses ; that is to aes ee SPORE oF Soul 
say, their resistance is increased, (Resistivity variation method) 
or conductivity decreased, under 
the action of stimulus. The deflection of the galvanometer 
under a constant electro-motive force now undergoes diminu- 
tion during the impact of stimulus. Such positive responses 


are obtained with potassium and sodium. ~That the sign 
B2 


4 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


of response does not depend on the electro-positivity or 
negativity of the substance is seen in the fact that while 
highly electro-positive potassium gives positive response, 
the equally electro-negative dioxide of lead gives a response 
of the same sign. Substances like magnesium, aluminium, 
and iron give negative response. 

It is found, again, that the same substance, under different 
molecular conditions, will give responses of opposite signs. 
For example, a particular molecular variety of silver, Ag’, 
gives positive (fig. 5), whereas ordinary silver gives nega- 
tive response. Again, while 
Ag’ normally gives positive, 
yet the sign of this response 
is gradually reversed to nega- 
tive, under the long-continued 
action of very strong stimulus. 
By the employment of the 
same method of resistivity 
variation, I have been able to 
obtain excitatory response records from living tissues also. 
Details of these will be given in a subsequent chapter. 

The electric response, however, employed to obtain 
the excitatory reaction of living tissues, depends upon the 
electro-motive variation of the substance under stimulation. 
This electric reaction has. been regarded as vitalistic in 
contradistinction to physical. But I have shown that 
similar responses are given by inorganic substances also. 
That is to say, the molecular excitability on which the 
phenomenon of response depends is not distinctive of animal 
tissues alone, but is common to all matter, both organic and 
inorganic. If, then, we desire to understand those funda- 
mental reactions which underlie the response of living tissues, 
it will be well to observe its occurrence in the much simpler 
case of the inorganic body.' 

If we take an inorganic substance, say a piece of metal 


Fic. 5. Positive Response of Ag’ to 
Electric Radiation 


' For a detailed account cf, Bose, Aesponse in the Living and the Non- 
Living. 


THE MOLECULAR RESPONSIVENESS OF MATTER 5 


wire, and if its molecular condition be the same throughout, 
it is obvious that its physical properties will likewise be 
uniform. Hence its electrical condition will also be the 
same at every point; in other words, it will be iso-electric. 
But if a portion of this wire should now be made to undergo 
a molecular change, as, say, by hammering, the physical 
condition of this portion will be made different from that of 
the rest. There will, therefore, be an electrical difference, 
and the wire will no longer be iso-electric. This fact can 
be verified by making suitable connections between the 
molecularly strained and unstrained portions of the wire, 
and a galvanometer, when a current will be found to flow 
through the galvanometer, showing that a difference of 
electrical potential has been brought about by the induced 


(2) (c) 


Fic. 6. Electric Response in Metals 


(a) Method of block; (4) Equal and opposite responses when the ends A 
and B are stimulated ; the dotted portions of the curves show recovery ; 
(c) Balancing effect, R, when both the ends are stimulated simultaneously. 


inequality of molecular conditions in different parts of the 
same wire. 

I shall now describe the method by which electrical 
responses to molecular disturbance may be obtained from 
inorganic substances. For this purpose, two different methods 
may be employed—first, the method of block, and, secondly, 
the method of relative depression. According to the first 
of these, the wire to be experimented on is held clamped 
at the middle, electrical connections being made with a 
galvanometer at two points, A and B, by means of two 
non-polarisable electrodes (fig. 6). We may now produce 


6 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


excitation of the A end of the wire, by imparting a torsional 
vibration, the molecular disturbance being prevented from 
reaching the B end by the intervening block. Using this 
method of experiment, I have obtained with different sub- 
stances two different types of response—namely, positive 
and negative. In the positive, the responsive current flows 
through the wire from the unexcited to the excited, or 
towards the excited—that is to say, the excited point 
becomes galvanometrically positive. Responses of this kind 
are given by tin, zinc, platinum, and other metals. In fig. 7 
is seen a uniform series of such responses 
to uniform stimulus. The intensity of the 
response, moreover, does not appear to 
depend on the chemical activity of the 
substance. For the response of the chemi- 
cally inactive tin is much stronger than 
that of the active zinc. The very inactive 
platinum is also found to give a fairly 
strong response, although the electrolytic 
contacts are made with pure water. 

The electro-motive response may also be obtained by other 
modes of molecular excitation. Thus, instead of torsional, 
we may use longitudinal vibration. A metallic rod of brass, 
AC, is clamped in the middle. A thin copper wire is led 
sideways from the clamp and connected with a piece of 
brass, B. A and B are connected with a galvanometer by 
means of non-polarisable electrodes. If now the C end of 
the rod be rubbed with resined cloth, A may be thrown into 
longitudinal vibration, B being little affected by this. It is 
here interesting to observe the concomitance of the electrical 
response with the sonorous tesponse of the rod, and the 
dying of the electrical response with the waning of the musical 
note. The stronger the molecular vibration, the stronger the 
sound, and also the stronger the response. The direction of 
the responsive current in the metal is from the less excited B 
to the more excited A. 

We found under the method of conductivity variation 


Fic. 7. Uniform Elec- 
tric Response in Tin 


THE MOLECULAR RESPONSIVENESS OF MATTER 7 


that when a substance is molecularly modified, the sign of 
its response tends to be reversed. Thus, as already said, 
ordinary silver gives positive, and modified silver, Ag’, 
negative response. But the latter, under strong and long- 
continued stimulation, has its response re-converted, as it 
were, to the normal positive. In the same way, under the 
method of electro-motive variation also, we find the normal 
positive response of, say, tin, or platinum, becoming con- 
verted by molecular modification into negative, to be again re- 
converted under continuous stimulation to the normal positive. 

There are, again, certain other substances, of which the 
normal response is negative. Thus a wire of brominated 
lead, for instance, when suitably prepared, is found to give an 
electro-motive response in which the current flows from the 
excited to the unexcited or away from the excited, the excited 
point becoming galvanometrically negative. 

These electro-motive responses of the inorganic have thus 
the same characteristics as those which have been observed 
in the case of animal tissues. Certain tissues, such as highly 
excitable muscle and nerve, give negative response—that is 
to say, the excited point 
becomes galvanometrically 
negative. Other tissues, 
again, the skin for example, 
give positive response. The 
normal responses, moreover, 
are sometimes found to be 
reversed under molecular 
modification, and to be re- 
reversed to normal response 
under long-continued stimu- 
lation. 


The electrical responses 


; : Fic. 8. Fatigue in the Electric 
of metals, again, are subject Response of Metals 


to an increase or decrease 
which is paralleled. by the same phenomenon in the response 
of animal tissues under similar circumstances. That is to 


8 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


say, fatigue is found to depress the response of the inorganic 
as of the organic (fig. 8). As in the case of animal tissues, 
again, so also in that of metals, certain chemical substances 
act as excitants, enhancing the response (fig. 9), others as 
depressors and still a third class—such as oxalic acid—as 
poisons, abolishing response altogether (fig. 10). 

By taking advantage of the last of these facts, we arrive 
at a second means of obtaining response—that is to say, the 
method of relative depression. If both the contact points 


Before * After 
Fic. 9. Stimulating Action of Na,CO, on Electric Response of Platinum 


Records to the left exhibit response before, to the right after the 
application of reagent. 


A and B be equally excited—-that is to say, subjected to diffuse 
stimulation—the responsive ‘currents will be opposed, and 
there will be no resultant galvanometric effect. This was 
overcome, according to the method of block, by localising 
excitation at one point, say, A; we might, however, neutralise 
altogether the counteracting excitatory effect at B by 
abolishing the excitability of that point, as, say, by the 
application of oxalic acid, A being left in its normal con- 
dition. If now the wire be subjected to diffuse stimulation 


THE MOLECULAR RESPONSIVENESS OF MATTER 9 


by vibrating it as a whole, a resultant response will occur. 
But by the application of oxalic acid to one contact, a resting 
or permanent current has been induced in the circuit. The 
responsive or action current 
originated under stimulation 
is now found to flow in a 
direction opposite to that of 
this resting current—that is 
to say, it causes a negative 
variation of it (fig. 11). 
The method of response 
by the so-called negative 
variation, which is generally 
employed in studying re- 
sponsive phenomena in 
animal tissues, is in reality, 


After 


as will be seen later, a form Before 4 

: : : Fic. 10. Abolition of Response in 
of this method of relative siétal by Oxélic Acid 
depression. i 


Various means have been described, in the course of this 
chapter, for the detection and record of that excitatory change 
which is brought about by the upsetting of molecular equi- 
librium under stimulus, 
and the subsequent re- 
covery. The responsive 6 
change may find expres- 


sion in different ways. 

This expression may, ; a 

for instance, in the case 

of living tissues, take the 

form of mechanical con- Fic. 11. Response by Method of Relative 
traction, or of the elec- Depression 

trical variation of gal- _ * TePresents current of rest ; ¥ represents 


vanometric negativity. 

Or the opposite change, expressed mechanically as ex- 
pansion, will be evidenced by galvanometric positivity. But 
it must be borne in mind that neither of these expressions 


10 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


is consequent on the other It would be as incorrect to 
suppose that the electrical effect depended on the mechanical, 
as to assume that the mechanical was brought about by the 
electrical. The two are independent expressions of the same 
fundamental molecular change, brought about by the shock 
of stimulus. 

Again, those various responsive phenomena and.their modi- 
fications which are the subject of our inquiry, are such as are 
induced by different external agencies. Under the influence 
of certain conditions, the responses of living matter undergo 
an abolition—the change which we associate with death. In 
inorganic matter also, we find a similar change of responsive- 
ness into irresponsiveness, to take place. The death-change 
in the case of living matter is thus not due to a change from 
the organic to the inorganic condition, but to some molecular 
transformation. And the nature of this very obscure trans- 
formation may one day be elucidated by a careful study of 
the changes which take place in inorganic matter, when it 
passes from responsivity to irresponsivity. 

The word ‘ physiological’ is generally used to distinguish 
phenomena which are believed to be exclusively characteristic 
of the properties of living matter. Such phenomena, however, 
- are found, as our power of investigation grows, to be increas- 
ingly capable of analysis into physico-chemical processes. 
In my own use of the term ‘physiological,’ therefore, it will 
be understood as a convenient expression for describing the 
response-phenomena of plant or animal tissues, but as in no 
sense opposed to the word ‘ physical.’ 

We shall, in the following chapters, study excitatory 
effects in living tissues, and their variations under different 
conditions, using the methods of electrical response. These 
phenomena will be studied with special detail in the case of 
vegetable tissues, and it will be found that there is no 
responsive reaction exhibited by any one amongst the various 
types of animal tissues, which has not its exact corre- 
spondence in the vegetable. Those anomalies, further, which 
have been observed in the response of the animal, will be seen 


THE MOLECULAR RESPONSIVENESS OF MATTER II 


to be fully elucidated by the study of similar phenomena 
under the simpler conditions of the plant. And finally an 
attempt will be made to arrive at some generalisation which 
will show the continuity between the simplest form of 
response in the inorganic and the most complex which occur 
in the highest type of animal tissue. 


CHAPTER II 


THE ELECTRO-MOTIVE RESPONSE OF PLANTS TO DIFFERENT 
FORMS OF STIMULATION | 


Historical—Difficulties of investigation—Electrical response of pulvinus of 
Mimosa—Simultaneous mechanical and electrical records—Division of plants 
into ‘ ordinary’ and ‘sensitive’ arbitrary—Mechanical and electrical response 
of ‘ordinary’ plants—Direct and transmitted stimulation — All forms of 
stimulus induce excitatory change of galvanometric negativity. 


IT has been customary to divide plants, as regards their 
responsiveness, into two distinct classes: ‘ordinary’ and 
‘sensitive. Of these only the latter class, represented by 
such plants as Wzmosa and Dionea, was regarded as excitable. 
Hence the attention of observers desirous of investigating 
excitatory electro-motive phenomena in vegetable tissues was 
mainly attracted towards the reactions exhibited by these 
plants. The experiments undertaken in this field by Kunkel, 
Burdon Sanderson, and Munk are well known. Burdon 
Sanderson and Munk worked on the sensitive leaves of Dzonega 
and Kunkel on-those of W/zmosa.' 

Electro-motive variations were observed to ‘ake place in 
all these plants on stimulation, but the conclusions arrived 
at by the investigators were not concordant. Burdon Sander- 
son and Munk found out that the resting-current between 
two selected points of Dzonza exhibited variation on applica- 
tion of stimulus to the leaf. They thus obtained di-phasic 
and sometimes even tri-phasic responses, consisting of positive 
and negative variations. More definite results were obtained 
by Burdon Sanderson, in the responsive variations of the 
normal leaf-current, flowing between the proximal and distal 


1 For a more detailed account cf. Biedermann, ZEéectro-physiology, English 
translation, 1898, vol. il. pp. 1-31. 


— = 


ae 


THE ELECTRO-MOTIVE RESPONSE OF PLANTS 13 


points of the midrib of the leaf. On stimulation of the 


lamina, this current was found to undergo a diminution, or 


negative variation. But the current in the stalk, or petiole 
was found to undergo the opposite change—that is to say, a 
positive variation. Kunkel, in working with the pulvinus of 
Mimosa, found that on excitation a series of opposite or 
oscillatory electro-motive variations was induced. He also 
found electro-motive differences to be induced as the result 
of the flexure or injury of ordinary stems He believed all 
these electrical phenomena to be consequent on hydrostatic 
disturbance or water-movement. 

By means of this ‘ migration of water’ Kunkel attempted 
to explain all the electrical phenomena in vegetable organs. 
The electro-motive difference between different parts of an 
organ was due, according to him, to their different powers of 
absorption of water. The greater absorptiveness of one 
point, with its consequent greater movement of water, would 
render that point relatively positive. As against this, how-— 
ever, Haake pointed out that electrical differences between 
different points were also to be found, even in submerged 
plants, like Valisnaria and Nztella, in which there could not 
possibly be any differences of absorptiveness. This difference, 
therefore, he suggested, must be ascribed to some vital 
process, inasmuch as the P.D. is seen to undergo a change 
whenever the respiratory process is interfered with, as, say, 
by the substitution of hydrogen for oxygen. 

We shall study later in greater detail the conditions on 
which this ‘current of rest,’ so called, actually depends 
(Chapter X.). But more important is the excitatory variation 
induced by stimulus. It has already been stated that 
Kunkel found oscillatory variations of the current to occur 


“on stimulation of the leaf of MMZzmosa. These alternating 


variations were difficult to reconcile with his theory of the 
active, single-phased displacement of water, as he did not 
fail to see, and he suggested that the first negative swing 
observed might be due to the disturbance of the diffusion- 
process through alterations of the protoplasm. ~ 


14 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Munk attempted to explain the complicated. electrical 
effects which he observed in Dzon@a by assuming the existence 
of two different kinds of electro-motive elements, affected in 
opposite ways, the maximum changes being initiated in one 
set earlier than in the other. In this way, he thought, it 
might be possible to explain the occurrence of positive and 
negative variations, holding that the upper parenchymatous 
layer of the leaf, and the upper midrib, went through the 
negative, and the under layer and the under midrib through 
the positive change. | 

Burdon Sanderson, in his ‘ fundamental experiment’ on the 
lamina of Dzone@a, had his led-off circuit connected with the 
upper and lower surfaces of one lobe, stimulus being applied at 
the other. In the experiments described in the ‘ Phil. Trans.’ 
of 1882, he found that the upper or inner surface of the leaf 
become positive on excitation. This he regarded as the 
true excitatory change. The upper contact now, however, 
after a certain interval became negative, a change which 
Burdon Sanderson designated as the after-effect. This after- 
effect he ascribed to the electrical variations caused by that 
movement of water which had been observed by Kunkel. 
But with regard to the preceding positive variation he says : 


‘The excitatory disturbance which immediately follows 
excitation is an explosive molecular change, which by 
the mode of its origin, the suddenness of its incidence, 
and the rapidity of its propagation is distinguished from 
every other phenomena except the one with which I 
have identified it, namely, the corresponding process in 
the excitable tissues of animals... . The direction of 
the excitatory effect in the fundamental experiment is 
such as to indicate that in excitation excited cells 
become positive to unexcited, whereas in animal tissues 
excited parts always become negative to unexcited.’! 


In a subsequent series of experiments, however, given in 
‘Phil. Trans,’ for 1888 Burdon Sanderson finds the reaction 


' Phil. Trans. vol. clxxiii. 


ee eae es ee 


a a 


ee eee Se eae ee ee 


THE ELECTRO-MOTIVE RESPONSE OF PLANTS 15 


of leaves ‘in their prime’ to be somewhat different. A strong 
negative phase was now observed on stimulation, but pre- 
ceded by a short-lived positive reaction. These results will 
be discussed in detail in a subsequent chapter, but it may be 
said here that from the records given by Burdon Sanderson 
it is difficult to know which of the various responsive phases 
are to be regarded as those of true excitation, and which as 
the results of some other cause. 

It will thus be seen that the results arrived at and the 
theories advanced by different observers in this field are some- 
what at variance with eachother. This must have been due 
to the difficulties met with in disentangling the fundamental 
reaction from those various subsidiary effects which are apt 
to be found in combination with it. Chief among these 
difficulties was (1) the fact that positive and negative 
variations are generally measured in terms of an existing 
current of rest. But, as a matter of fact, these two 
apparently opposite responsive variations, positive and 
negative, are not always indicative of opposite reactions. 
For an identical excitatory reaction, added algebraically to 
the resting-current, might appear, according to circumstances, 
as either of the two. There was also (2) the difficulty of 
discriminating two opposed electrical effects, one of which 
was due, as I shall show, to true excitation, and the other 
to increase of internal energy brought about by mechanical 
movement of water. Under different conditions, it may be 
either the one or the other of these which becomes prominent. 
I shall hope to show that each of them is definite and dis- 
tinguishable from the other. 

With regard to the response of plants in general, ae 
it may be well to state here that the first fact to be demon- 
strated in the course of the present work is that such 
responsive phenomena as may be observed in the case of 
sensitive plants like Dzon@a, are not unique, but occur 
under similar circumstances even in ordinary plants, and are 
characteristic of all plant organs. I shall be able to show, 
moreover, that an explanation of these phenomena, much 


16 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


simpler than the theory of electro-motive molecules, is 
available. It will also be proved that the electrical response 
due to true excitation is quite distinct from that which is 
brought about by the hydrostatic disturbance, its sign being 
in fact opposite. This true excitatory electrical response, 
again, will be shown to be modified by all those conditions 
which affect the physiological state of the tissue. And, 
lastly, it will be proved that there is no breach of continuity 
as between the electrical responses in plant and animal, 
for not only is the sign of response in both cases the 
same, but it is also true that every type of response and 
modification of response, which occurs in the animal tissue, is 
to be found under parallel circumstances in that of the 
plant also. 

In order to determine what is the electrical response 
characteristic of excitation, we first select for experiment a 
sensitive plant, say J/zmosa, because here, in the responsive 
fall of the leaf, we have a visible indication of the excitatory 
reaction. It is desirable at this point to say a few words re- 
garding the use of the terms ‘ excitatory ’ and ‘true excitation.’ 
We are all familiar with the fact that, when muscle is excited 
by stimulus, it responds in a conspicuous manner by con- 
traction. This is universally accepted as the phenomenon of 
excitation, its electrical concomitant being galvanometric 
negativity. Having once applied the term ‘excitatory’ to this 
particular aspect of molecular response in living tissues, it is 
of course important that we should henceforth distinguish 
carefully between it and its possible opposite, namely, ex- 
pansion, with concomitant galvanometric positivity. Under 
stimulation there is a contraction of, and expulsion of water 
from, the excited pulvinus, which brings about the depression 
of the leaf. It is generally. supposed that only the lower half 
of the pulvinus is excitable. This, however, is an error, for 
both upper and lower halves are excitable, and contract 
under stimulation. If localised stimulus be applied on the 
upper side, that side contracts, and, by the concavity thus 
induced, the leaf is erected. But, though both halves are 


+. eee 


Pe — = ."* 
CO ea eee 


THE ELECTRO-MOTIVE RESPONSE OF PLANTS 17 


sensitive, yet the excitability of the lower is, generally 
speaking, greater, and diffuse stimulation therefore causes 
greater contraction of that half. Hence the resultant fall is 
due to the differential contraction of the two sides of the 
organ. The excitatory reaction of the organ, then, consists 
of a contraction ; expulsion of water with consequent diminu- 


Fic. 12. Arrangement for observing Simultaneous Mechanical and 
Electrical Responses 


Leaf stimulated by electro-thermic stimulator. Mechanical record obtained 
by excursion of spot of light reflected from Optic Lever, which falls on 
the right of drum. Electric record obtained by excursion of galvano- 
meter spot of light adjusted to fall on the left side of the drum. 


tion of turgidity, or negative turgidity variation ; and fall of 
the leaf. : 

In order next to determine the electrical concomitant of 
this reaction, we make suitable electrical connections by 
non-polarisable electrodes, with a galvanometer. One of 
these is made with the pulvinus, whose excitation is to be 
observed, and the other with a distant indifferent point. In 
this way we can obtain the excitatory effect at the pulvinus, 
uncomplicated by that at the distal point. A spot of light, 
reflected from the galvanometer mirror, is thrown on the 

C 


18 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


recording drum. This spot of light, hitherto quiescent, shows, 
by its sudden deflection, the occurrence of the excitatory 
change. 

To show the concomitance of the mechanical and elec- 
trical responses, and in order to detect with certainty the 
exact moment of the initiation of the former, a magnifying 
arrangement is obtained by attaching the end of the leaf to 
an Optical Lever. The pull exerted by the falling leaf rotates 
the fulcrum-rod, carrying a light mirror. The spot of light. 
from this mirror moves in a vertical direction, and that — 
of the galvanometer horizontally or laterally. For the 
purpose of simultaneous record, it is necessary to have the 
two in one direction. The up and down movement of 
the spot from the Optic Mirror is therefore converted into 
lateral, by means of reflection from a second mirror, suitably 
inclined. 

Stimulation may be effected in the neighbourhood of 
the pulvinus by means of the electro-thermic stimulator, 
which has the advantage of producing no mechanical dis- 
turbance. This consists of a V-shaped platinum wire, 
suddenly heated by the momentary passage of an electric 
current. On applying stimulus in this manner it is found 
‘that the two responses—mechanical and electrical—take 
place at the same moment, the mechanical fall of the leaf 
being practically coincident with the induced electrical 
variation. As regards the sign of this electrical change, the 
excited point is found to become galvanometrically negative : 
that is to say, the electro-motive variation induced in the 
excited vegetable, is the same as that observed in animal 
tissues. I give below a series of these simultaneous records 
of mechanical and electrical response (fig. 13), obtained from 
Biophytum, whose lateral leaflets give motile indications. It 
will be seen that the responsive fall of the leaflet, and the 
subsequent recovery, are synchronous with the electrical 
variation of galvanometric negativity, and its subsequent 
recovery. For convenience of inspection. I shall always, 
unless specially stated to the contrary, represent the normal 


THE ELECTRO-MOTIVE RESPONSE OF PLANTS 19 


responses of mechanical depression and galvanometric nega- 
tivity by up-curves. The erectile movement and galvano- 
metric positivity will, conversely, be represented as down. 

A few words may be said here as regards the syn- 
chronism between the two forms of response. The excitatory 
molecular change takes place instantaneously, and the 
electro-motive variation is, as far as can be judged, strictly 
concomitant with it. This is shown by the rheotomic 
method of observation, described in Chapter IV. It will there 
be seen that a very considerable electro-motive change has 
already been induced, in a period so short as ‘oI of a second 
after the reception by the tissue of the stimulating shock. 


Fic. 13. Simultaneous Mechanical (M) and Electrical (E) Responses in Biophyium 


These responses are seen to take place at the same moment. 


In the electrical response, then, of highly excitable tissues 
there is practically no latent period. But if the same elec- 
trical variation be recorded by the galvanometer, there will 
be a lag in the response, owing to the inertia of the galva- 
nometer needle. Similarly, in the mechanical response, 
though the excitatory reaction is immediate, yet the motile 
response is delayed, by the antagonistic actions of the upper 
and lower halves of the pulvinus, the sluggishness of the 
tissue, and the mechanical inertia of the indicating leaf. 
The latent period of the mechanical response of a vigorous 
Mimosa, owing to all these causes, I find to be about 
twenty-four-hundredths of a second. But this may be still 


further prolonged when the tissue is in a state of depressed 
C2 


20 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


excitability. It will thus be seen that even when the funda- 
mental excitatory reaction is instantaneous, its outward 
expression, whether mechanical or electrical, may nevertheless 
appear to be subject to delay in consequence of the inertia 
of the particular indicator concerned. 

As regards these two forms of response, it should further 
be remembered that the mechanical and electrical responses 
are independent indications of the fundamental excitatory 
reaction, and that neither is 
dependent for its occurrence 
on the other. Thus, when 
the mechanical response is 
physically restrained, the 
electrical response takes 
place unimpeded. I shall 
here relate an experiment 
in illustration of this point. 

A leaf of Mimosa re- 
sponds to strong stimulus 
by complete collapse, and 
the recovery from this state 
is somewhat prolonged, 
taking from five to eighteen 
minutes, according to the 


Fic. 14. Photographic record of Elec- : 
trical Response by Galvanometric season. In order to obtain 


Negativity of Pulvinus of Mimosa, . : : 
when leaf is physically restrained from a series of FCSPONSe with 


falling. The first series in response to these recoveries, within a 
Series to stimuli twice as strong. Teasonable time, I find that 
it is necessary to apply 

moderate stimulus. There is then a moderate fall, without 
complete collapse, and recovery under such circumstances is 
found to take place within a minute or so. In order to show 
that electrical response takes place, even when the leaf is 
prevented from giving mechanical expression, I held the 
petiole in a clamp, and obtained the set of electrical responses 
seen in fig. 14. The first series of this record were taken 
in answer to uniform stimuli of a given intensity, and the 


THE ELECTRO-MOTIVE RESPONSE OF PLANTS 2I 


second to stimuli twice as strong. We may here see how 
response is increased by increased intensity of stimulus. 
One peculiarity to be noticed in this figure is the trend of 
the base-line downwards, showing the increasing positivity 
of the pulvinus. In order to obtain a photographic record 
the experiment had to be carried out in a dark room, and 
under these circumstances the pulvinus undergoes an increase, 
or positive variation, of turgidity. And we shall see later 
that a positive turgidity variation is associated with galvano- 
metric positivity in the same way as the negative turgidity 
variation is found to be accompanied by galvanometric 
negativity. | 

In consequence of the impression produced by the con- 
spicuous movements of the leaf of M/zmosa,it was assumed that 
only plants showing such movements were to be regarded as 
excitable. I have already shown elsewhere, however, that 
this test of lateral motile responses, as a sign of sensitiveness, 
is fallacious in the extreme. Such mechanical display is 
possible only when the two halves of an organ are unequally 
contractile, and there is consequently a greater expulsion of 
water from one, in response to stimulus, than from the other. 
If these conditions are not fulfilled, even the so-called ‘ sensi- 
tive’ Mimosa would appear to be insensitive. Thus, when 
we place a cut branch of J/zmosa in water, the pulvini of the 
leaves, on account of vigorous suction at the cut end, are 
rendered over-turgid, and the leaves become highly erected. 
On now applying stimulation, no responsive fall is found to 
take place ; this is due to the difficulty encountered in the 
expulsion of water from the gorged tissue. An intact plant, 
again, which in the light has been found highly sensitive, 
will often be found insensitive after a short time spent in a 
dark room. It will then be difficult to believe that the plant 
is of the sensitive class, for the hardest blow will fail to 
evoke any mechanical response. And not only does the 
Mimosa cease to show responsive movement under these cir- 
cumstances, which may perhaps be regarded as exceptional ; 
but under perfectly normal conditions also, its sensitiveness 


22 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


varies so much that its motile response would seem at times 
almost to have disappeared. I have already pointed out 
that it is by the unequal excitabilities of the upper and 
lower halves of the pulvinus that that differential contrac- 
tion is induced which brings about the lateral response of 
the Mzmosa leaf. Now it is clear from this that if the 
differential excitability should be reduced or abolished, by 
any means whatsoever, there will then be a corresponding 
diminution or abolition of response. We shall see later that 
the excitability of a tissue depends upon its state of turgor, 
and in Mzmosa, from internal causes, a periodic variation is 
induced in the relative turgescence of the two halves of the 
pulvinus. We might then expect, in consequence of this, to 
find a periodic variation of motile sensibility. And certainly, 
whatever may be the cause, a long course of observation 
will convince the inquirer of the occurrence of great varia- 
tions in the sensibility of MW/zmosa at different times of the 
day. Thus, I had six specimens of this plant growing in 
pots in the open, and I found, watching them in the month of 
August, that at eight o’clock in the morning the pulvini of 
the leaves of all these plants were sensitive in the highest 
degree. Half an hour later, however, this sensitiveness had 
so far waned that they would give hardly any motile indi- 
cation. It is, perhaps, worth while to remark, in connection 
with this, that-a constant observer is able to judge, by a 
peculiar, though indescribable, attitude of the leaves, whether 
or not this condition of insensitiveness has supervened. Thus 
the mechanical movements of the belauded sensitive plants, 
such as MJzmosa,on which depended the arbitrary assumption 
that ‘ordinary’ plants were insensitive, rest on a basis which 
is itself extremely unreliable. For by this standard one 
identical plant ought to be classed as belonging to both the 
sensitive and insensitive groups, according to the time of day 
at which any particular observation is made. 

The fact that when the mechanical response of the leaf of 
Mimosa is physically restrained, excitatory electrical response 
takes place unimpeded, shows that we have a criterion by 


THE ELECTRO-MOTIVE RESPONSE OF PLANTS 23 


which to test the excitability of a plant, independently of 
any motile indication. On applying this test, I have found 
that not the so-called sensitive plants alone, but all plants 
and all organs of all plants, respond to stimulation. And 
from this I was led to the discovery that ordinary plants also, 
in spite of current misconceptions, exhibit motile response by 
mechanical contraction. The common error of regarding 
these plants as insensitive has arisen from the fact that in a 
radial organ, diffuse stimulation induces equal contractions 
on all sides. Hence those lateral movements, dependent on 
differential contraction, which are seen so conspicuously dis- 
played in Mimosa, cannot take place here. But that the 
organ as a whole undergoes a responsive contraction has 
been demonstrated by recording the consequent induced 
shortening of its length. Such longitudinal contraction is 
sometimes very considerable ; for instance, in the filamentous 
corona of Passiflora it may sometimes be as much as 20 per 
cent. of the original length. 

Having thus shown that all plants are excitable, I shall 
proceed to demonstrate the fact by means of electrical 
response. In studying the excitatory effect on ordinary 
plants, we must bear in mind that there are two different 
ways of stimulating a given point: that is to say, locally or 
directly, and by transmission of excitation from a distance. 
Conducting tissues are capable of stimulation in either of 
these ways, but the feebly conducting must be subjected to 
local excitation, since the effect of stimulus applied at a 
distance cannot, in their case, reach the responding point. 
Organs containing fibro-vascular elements are fairly good 
conductors, and stimulus applied on them at one or two 
centimetres from the point to be stimulated will thus easily 
reach it. It must, however, be remembered that stimulus 
becomes enfeebled by transmission through a long tract, its 
effect at a great distance being negligible. Parenchymatous 
tissues are bad conductors of excitation, and in order to 
excite them, stimulus must therefore be applied directly. © 

Turning first to the transmitted mode of stimulation, 


24 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHIYSIOLOGY 


we take a petiole or stem, and making suitable electrical 
connections (fig. 15), apply stimulus, say by contact of hot 
wire at the point marked x. After a short interval, necessary 
for the excitation to traverse the intervening distance, an 
electrical response is obtained, of galvanometric negativity. 
It is thus seen that the electrical response of ‘ ordinary’ is of 
the same sign as that of ‘sensitive’ plants, and that in both, 
again, it is like that of animal tissues. 


I shall next proceed to demonstrate a very important _ 


proposition : namely, that all effective forms of stimulation 
induce an identical excitatory response of galvanometric 
negativity. Any sudden change of environmental conditions 
may constitute an efficient stimulus. Such are: sudden rise 


Fic. 15. Method of Transmitted Stimulation 


Stimulus applied to the right at x. Excitation reaches right contact first, 
causing galvanometric negativity of the point. 


of temperature ; any variation of pressure, whether of tension 
or compression ; mechanical blows or torsional vibration ; any 
prick or cut; the application of a chemical agent, such as 
acid ; the application of light ; the incidence or variation of 
electrical currents; and, lastly, the action of gravity. The 
stimulatory action of all these agents has already been 
demonstrated in my work on ‘ Plant Response,’! the excita- 
tion induced being there shown to find expression in 
appropriate mechanical movements. In the present volume 
I shall deal more particularly with the electrical reactions 
which they induce. The effects of the stimulating action of 


1 Bose, Plant Response as a Means of Physiological Investigation, 1906. 


a 


Se eee 


RE hae 8 ce tla A al tnt ls YL 


THE ELECTRO-MOTIVE RESPONSE OF PLANTS 25 


electrical currents, of light and of. gravity, will be taken 
up in special chapters devoted to their consideration, while 
here I shall demonstrate the exci- 
tatory effects of the other forms 
of stimulus enumerated. 

We have already observed the 
responsive effect which results 
from the sudden application of 
heat, by means of a hot wire. 
The effects of various forms of 
mechanical stimulation may now 
be subjected to demonstration, 
and first we have to observe the 
effect of the stimulus resulting from 
sudden tension. The specimen is 
clamped securely in the middle 


(fig. 16), so that when a vertical pull Fic. 16. Excitation by Sudden 
Tension 


is given to the upper half, that Plant securely clamped. When 
half alone is subjected to a sud- suddenly pulled, tension in- 

. 4 duces galvanometric nega- 
denly increased tension, the lower tivity of A. 


being left entirely unaffected. | 

Under these circumstances, there is an electrical response, 
A becoming galvanometrically negative. A is next subjected 
to mechanical compression, and for this purpose the piece of 
moistened cloth surrounding the specimen, and making the 
electrical connection at A, is placed between the two grooved 
halves of a cork. The enclosed plant tissue at A may now 
be made to undergo sudden compression, by squeezing the 
pieces of cork together. This gives rise to the same electrical 
response as before. 

This fact, that both tension and compression will give rise 
to similar excitatory responses of galvanometric negativity, 
may receive independent demonstration by first making an 
electric connection at A with the upper side of the speci- 
men (fig. 17). When the tissue at A is now suddenly bent 
down, this upper side becomes convex: that is to say, it is 
subjected to tension. This gives rise to the excitatory 


26 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


response of negativity. The electrical connection at A is 
next removed to the lower side of the specimen, at a point A’. 
On now repeating the sudden flexure, A’ undergoes com- 


_ Fic, 17. Excitatory Response to Tension 


and Compression 


pression instead of ten- 
sion. The result is a 
similar negative  elec- 
trical response. 
Excitation may, 
again, be produced by 
means of a sudden blow 
at a point. This blow 
may be delivered by 
means of a spring-tapper 
(fig. 18), in which S is 


When E is connected with the upper point A x 
a sudden bending down causes tension of the spr ing proper and 


A. When connection is made with a’ the theattached rod Rcarries 


same flexure causes compression of A’. 
Both induce galvanometric negativity. 


at its end the tapping 
head T. A _ projecting 


rod—the lifter L—passes through SR. It is provided with 
a screw-thread, by means of which its length, projecting 
downwards, is regulated. By means of this the height or 


ee peed) 


Fic. 18. The Mechanical Tapper 


intensity of the stroke may 
be varied. As one of the 
spokes of the cog-wheel c 
is rotated past L, the spring 
is lifted and released, and 
T delivers a sharp tap. 
The height of the lift, and 
therefore the intensity of 
the stroke, is measured by 
a graduated scale _ not 
shown in the figure. We 
can increase the intensity 


of this stroke through a wide range, first, by augmenting the 
projecting length of the spring by a sliding catch. We may 
give isolated single taps, or superpose a series in rapid 
succession according as the wheel is rotated slowly or quickly. 


THE ELECTRO-MOTIVE RESPONSE OF PLANTS 27 


Stimulation, again, may be effected by the prick of a needle 
or pin in the neighbourhood of A. Response to this also 
occurs by the normal galvanometric negativity. Successive 
pricks may thus give rise to successive responses. 

Or the specimen may be subjected to torsional vibration. 
It is here held in the middle by a clamp, and stimulus of 
torsional vibration is applied (2) @) 
at one end. - The stimulation a 
of A makes that end gal- 
vanometrically negative, the 
direction of the current 
outside the circuit being 
towards, and in the tissue 
away from, A. Vibration of Blah op is 2 or es 

3 j Current of response when 
B induces responsive nega- 2B is stimulated > 
tivity of B (fig. 19), the 
current of response being 


B 


Fic. 19.. The Torsional Vibrator 
(a) The plant is clamped at c, between 


now reversed. In the cases 7 A pee B. Pig tee" 
; : . : esponses obtained by alternately 
just described, it will be stimulating the two ends. Stimula- 
noticed that stimulus is ap- tion of A produces upward response ; 


of B gives downward response. 


plied directly. This method 

is, therefore, specially applicable when we wish to study the 
excitability of such tissues as are not good conductors of 
excitation, the method of transmitted stimulation being here, 
therefore, inapplicable. 

In order to observe the effect of chemical sijinailabion, the 
given agent—sulphuric or hydrochloric acid—is applied at 
x at ashort distance from the proximal contact. The trans- 
mitted excitation is now again demonstrated by the induced 
galvanometric negativity of that contact. It will thus be seen 
that, whatever be the effective form of stimulus employed, 
it gives rise to a definite and invariable electrical response 
whose sign is always one of galvanometric negativity. 

It was shown, then, in the course of this chapter that 
the excitatory change in ‘sensitive’ plants is characterised 
by contraction, negative turgidity variation, mechanical 
depression of the leaf, and by the electricals response of 


28 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


galvanometric negativity, all these effects being concomitant. 
It was further shown that electrical response is independent 
of the mechanical, being unimpeded in its occurrence when 
the leaf is physically restrained. 

The same electrical response of galvanometric negativity 
is also obtained from the tissues of the so called ‘ ordinary’ 
plants. And these electrical responses of plant tissues, it was 
further noted, are identical in sign with the corresponding 
responses given by animal tissues. | 

All forms of stimulus, moreover—mechanical, thermal, 
photic, chemical, and electrical—induce the same excitatory 
response of galvanometric negativity. 


CHAPTER III 


THE APPLICATION OF QUANTITATIVE STIMULUS AND 
RELATION BETWEEN STIMULUS AND RESPONSE 


Conditions of obtaining uniform response—Torsional vibration as a form of 
stimulus—Method of block—Effective intensity of stimulus dependent on 
period of vibration—Additive action of feeble stimuli—Response recorder— 
Uniform electric responses—List of suitable specimens—Effect of season on 
excitability—Stimulation by thermal shocks—Thermal stimulator —Second 
method of confining excitation to one contact—-Increasing response to increas- 
ing stimulus— Effect of fatigue—Tetanus. 


A QUALITATIVE demonstration has been given in the last 
chapter of the induction of galvanometric negativity in plant 
tissues, in response to the excitation caused by various forms 
of stimulus. This galvanometric response is thus a sign or 
indication of the state of excitation ; and under normal con- 
ditions it will be of uniform extent, provided only that the 
stimuli are also uniform. Assuming this ideal condition to 
be secured, it is clear that the physiological modifications 
induced by various agents will be manifested by a corre- 
sponding modification of response. The conditions essential 
to such application of stimulus are, then, (1) that it should be 
capable of uniform repetition ; (2) that it should be capable 
of increase or decrease by definite amounts; and (3) that it 
should be of such a nature as to cause no injury, by which 
the excitability of the tissue might be changed in some 
unknown degree. These conditions, on which the success of 
the electro-physiological investigation depends, are very 
difficult to meet. Chemical stimulation, for example, cannot 
be uniformly repeated. Electrical stimulation, again, which 
has the advantage of being easy to render quantitative, is 
open to the objection that by escape of current it may induce 


30 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


galvanometric disturbance. Indeed, as the response is 
electrical, it is obvious that if we are to obtain unimpugnable 
results, a non-electrical form of stimulus is almost a necessity. 
But it is only after providing against various sources of error 
that the electrical form of stimulation can be used with con- 
fidence. The stimulation caused by mechanical blows can be 
repeated, it is true, with uniform intensity. But the point 
struck is subjected to increasing injury, and its excitability 
thus undergoes an unknown variation. | 
I have, however, been able to devise two different modes 
of stimulation, in which all these difficulties have been — 


Ss 


(a) (b) 


L : ) 


Fic. 20. The Vibratory Stimulator 


Plant P is securely held byavicev. The twoends are clamped by holders 
cc’. By means of handles H H’, torsional vibration may be imparted 
to either the end A or end B of the plant. The end view (4) shows 
how the amplitude of vibration is predetermined by means of movable 
stops, Ss’. 


successfully overcome. rendering the results as perfect as 
possible. These are (1) torsional vibration, and (2) the 
application of thermal shocks. For the obtaining of perfect 
responses, it must be said here that there is still another 
condition to be fulfilled. If we wish to obtain the pure 
effect of stimulus at one contact, say A, special care must be 
taken that excitation does not reach the second contact, B; 
for otherwise, unknown effects of interference will occur. 
This may, it is true, be obviated by means of the method of 
relative depression or method of negative variation, so called, 
to be described in a subsequent chapter. But the experi- 
mental mode which I am about to describe, in which a block 


THE APPLICATION OF QUANTITATIVE STIMULUS 31 


is interposed between A and B, is much more perfect. 
According to this arrangement, the specimen is tightly 
clamped in the middle, by which device the excitation’ of 
either end is practically precluded from affecting the other. 
Stimulation is brought about by means of torsional 
vibration. The stem or petiole is fixed, at its middle, in a 
vice, V, the free ends being held in tubes, C C’, each provided 


:/ 


AZ 
Fic, 21. Complete Apparatus for Method of Block and Vibratory 
Stimulation 


Amplitude of vibration which determines the intensity of stimulus is 
measured by the graduated circle seen to the right. Temperature is 
regulated by the electric heating coil R. For experiments on action 
of anzesthetics, vapour of chloroform is blown in through the side tube. 


with three clamping jaws. A torsional vibration may now 
be imparted to the specimen at either end by means of the 
handles H and H’ (fig. 20). The amplitude of vibration 
which determines the intensity of stimulus can be accurately 
measured by the graduated circle, and may be predeter- 
mined by means of the sliding stops s Ss... The complete 
vibrational apparatus, by means of which various experi- 
mental investigations may be carried out, is given in fig. 21, 


32 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Moistened cotton threads in connection with the non-polari- 
sable electrodes, E E, make secure electrical contacts with A 
and B. For experimenting on the effects of temperature, 
there is an electrical heating coil, R, inside the chamber. 
For the study of the effects of different gases, there are inlet 
and outlet tubes, which enable a stream of the required gas 
or vapour to be circulated through the chamber. 

If the A end of the specimen be now suddenly torsioned 
through a given number of degrees, a responsive electro- 


motive variation takes place, which after- 


wards subsides gradually. If next the 
torsioned end be suddenly brought back 
to the original position, a second electro- 
motive response is obtained, similar to the 
first. Hence, in the case of a to-and-fro 
ab e ad_ vibration, the responsive effects are addi- 
a tive, and we have the further advantage 
7 aa 22. Influence of that the tissue at the end of the operation is 
uddenness on the F im ‘ a 
Efficiency of Stimu- returned to its original physical condition. 
his In order that successive stimuli may 
The curves a, 4, ¢, d, 2 : 
are responses to be equally effective, another factor besides 


vibrations of the the constancy of the amplitude of vibra- 


same amplitude, , : 2 
30°. Ina the vi- tion has to be considered. It is to be 


bration was. Very borne in mind that the effectiveness of the 


slow; in 6 it was 

less slow; it was 

rapid in c, and very 
_ rapid in @. 


stimulus in evoking response depends also 
on the rapidity of the onset of the dis- 


turbance. In the application of vibratory 
stimulation to plants, I find the extent of response to depend 
to some degree on the quickness with which the vibration 
is effected. I give below records of responses to successive 
stimuli, induced by vibration through the same amplitude, 
which were delivered with increasing rapidity (fig. 22). It 
will be noticed that an increasing quickness of vibration 
increased the response, but that this reached a limit. If 
we wish, then, to maintain the effective intensity of stimulus 
constant, we must meet two conditions. First, the amplitude 
of vibration must be kept the same. This is done by means 


Se ee ee er 


So EL A TIA tatty, alas Batis sb: 


THE APPLICATION OF QUANTITATIVE STIMULUS 33 


of the graduated circle and movable stops: and, second, the 
vibration period must be uniform. This last condition is 
effected by an arrangement shown in fig. 23. The torsion- 
head is kept tense by means of a stretched spiral spring, s, 
made of steel. From this torsion-head there projects an 
elastic brass piece, B. R is a striker which can be made to 
give a quick stroke to B, by the rotation of the handle. A 
quick to-and-fro vibration is thus produced, by the blow 
given to B, acting against the tension of the antagonistic 
spring S. The amplitude of the angular vibration is at the 


E oc 


Fic. 23. Spring Attachment for obtaining Vibration of Uniform Rapidity 


same time predetermined by means of the stops P and qQ. 
The arrangements described are as used in ordinary work. 
But for certain experiments on differential excitability, a 
second striker, R’, may be attached to the other end of the 
apparatus, and by this means the opposite contacts in con- 
nection with E and E’ may be excited simultaneously. 

In order to obtain responses of great amplitude, it is now 
necessary to increase the amplitude of vibration. But this 
may give rise to fatigue. By way of avoiding this, therefore, 
it is still possible to obtain enlarged response by the additive 
effect of repeated feeble stimuli. In the electrical response 
of plants a sub-minimal stimulus, singly ineffective, is found 

D 


34 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


to become effective by the summation of several. This is 
seen in fig. 24, where a single vibrational stimulus of 3°, 


b 


a 


—-. 


1 1 ' 
+ JOsec> 


Fic. 24. Additive 
Effect 


(z) A single stimulus 
of 3° vibration pro- 
duced little or no 
effect, but the same 
stimulus when 
rapidly superposed 
thirty times pro- 
duced the large 
effect (4). (Leaf 
stalk of turnip.) 


alone ineffective, was found to evoke a 
large response when repeated with rapidity 
thirty times in succession. 

For the delivering of such equal and 
rapidly succeeding stimuli, I substitute for 
the single striker R an eight-spoked wheel, 
a complete rotation of which, by means of 
the handle, gives rise to a definite sum- 
mated effect: and a series of responses to 
such summated stimulations is found to 
be uniform. The galvanometer used for 
these experiments is a dead-beat instru- 
ment of D’Arsonval type. The sensitive- 
ness of this is such that a current of 10° 
ampere causes a deflection of I mm. at 
a distance of I metre. For a quick and 
accurate method of obtaining. records, I 


devised the following form of response-recorder. The 
curves are obtained directly, by tracing the excursion of the 


Fic. 25. Response Recorder 


galvanometer spot of light on a revolving drum (fig. 25). 
This drum, on which is wrapped the paper for receiving 
the record, is driven by clockwork. Different speeds of 


THE APPLICATION OF QUANTITATIVE STIMULUS) 35 


revolution can be given to it by adjustment of the clock- 
governor, or by changing the size of the driving-wheel. The 
galvanometer spot is thrown down on the drum by the 
inclined mirror M. The galvanometer deflection takes place 
at right angles to the motion of the paper; a stylographic 
pen attached to a carrier rests on the writing surface. The 
carrier slides over a rod parallel to the drum. As has been 
said before, the galvanometer deflection takes place parallel 
to the axis of the drum, and as long as the plant rests un- 
stimulated, the pen, remaining coincident with the stationary 
galvanometer spot on the revolving paper, describes a 
straight line. If, on stimulation, we trace the resulting 
excursion of the spot of light, by moving the carrier which 
holds the pen, the rising portion of the response curve will 
be obtained. The galvanometer spot will then return more 
or less gradually to its original position, and that part of the 
curve which is traced during this process constitutes the 
recovery. The ordinate in these curves represents the 
electro-motive variation, and the abscissa the time. 

_ We can calibrate the value of the deflection by applying 
a small known E.M.¥., say of ‘1 volt, to the circuit, and 
noting the deflection which results. This gives us the value 
of the ordinate. The value of the abscissa which represents 
time is determined by the distance through which the 
recording surface moves, in unit time. In this simple 
manner accurate records are obtained. It has the additional 
advantage of enabling the observer to see at once whether 
the specimen is suitable for the purpose of investigation. A 
large number of records might be taken by this means, in a 
comparatively short time. 

It is also easy to take the records photographically by 
wrapping a photographic film round the recording drum. 

I give in fig. 26 a series of responses taken from the 
root of radish (Raphanus sativus), in which the stimuli were 
applied at intervals of one minute. This shows how ex- 
tremely uniform the responses may be rendered, if proper 
precautions are taken, It may here be once more pointed 

D2 


36 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


out, that for convenience of inspection, the records in this 
book have been so taken that the normal electrical responses © 
of galvanometric negativity, unless specially stated to the 
contrary, are seen as up-curves, galvanometric positivity being 
represented by down-curves. These excitatory responses of 


Fic. 26. Photographic Record of Uniform Responses (Radish) 


galvanometric negativity are obtained with all plants, and 
with every organ of the plant. I give here a table containing 
a list of specimens which will be found on stimulation to give 
fairly large electro-motive effects, occasionally as high as 
‘rt volt. 


Organ Specimen 


Carrot (Daucus carota) 
Radish (Raphanus sativus) 


Geranium (Pelargonium) 
Stem : : . | Vine (V7tds vinifera) 
Amaranth (Amaranthus) 


Horse Chestnut (4 sceulus hippocastanum) 
Turnip (Brassica napus) 

Petiole . , . | Cauliflower (4rasszca oleracea) 

Celery (Apium graveolens) 

| Eucharis lily (2ucharis amazonica) 


Peduncle. ; . | Arum lily (Azcardia africana) 


Fruit ° . | Egg-plant (Solanum melongena) 


THE APPLICATION OF QUANTITATIVE STIMULUS 37 


These responses, being physiological, vary in intensity 
with the condition of the specimen. The same plant which 
gives strong electrical response in spring or summer, may 
exhibit but feeble responsiveness in autumn or winter. Again, 
we shall see in a subsequent chapter that any agent which 
depresses physiological activity will also depress the electri- 
cal response; and, lastly, when the specimen is killed, the 
normal response is abolished. 

I shall next describe a second and equally perfect method 
of stimulation, by means, namely, of thermal shocks. We 
have seen that a sudden thermal variation acts as an efficient 
stimulus. I have also shown in my ‘Plant Response’ that 
thermal radiation acts as a stimulating agent, in inducing 
excitatory contraction. Hence, if a tissue be surrounded by 
a platinum wire, through which an electrical heating-current 
can be sent, the enclosed tissue will be subjected to a sudden 
variation of temperature, and also to the thermal radiation 
proceeding from the heated wire. Now if in successive 
experiments the duration and intensity of the current 
flowing through the wire be maintained constant, the 
stimuli also will thereby be rendered constant. The thermal 
stimulator, as already said, surrounds the specimen, but is 
not in actual contact with it. This is to prevent any injury 
to the tissue, by scorching. The current is so adjusted as to 
make the platinum wire red-hot and this heating-current is 
closed for about half a second at a time. Should larger 
response be desired, it-can be obtained by the summated 
effect of a number of such shocks, or the thermal stimulator 
may be put in direct contact with the tissue, if care be 
taken that the rise of temperature is not so great as to 
injure it. : 

The difficulty of ensuring similarity of duration to each 
individual shock is overcome by the use of a balanced key 
actuated by a metronome (fig. 27). A second rod is attached 
at right angles to the vibrating rod of the metronome, and 
carries a bent piece of brass in the form of two prongs. 
During the course of each vibration these prongs dip into 


38 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


two cups of mercury, thus closing the electrical circuit for a 
brief and definite time. When a second press-key, not shown 
in the figure, is open, the circuit is incomplete, and there 
is no thermal stimulation. The observer then presses this 
key, and counts, say, 
five strokes of the 
metronome,. after 


is again opened. In 
this way, the sum- 
mated effect is ob- 
tained, of five equal 
thermal shocks. This 
Fic. 27. Stimulation by Thermal Shocks process is repeated 
as often as desired, 
at intervals of, say, one minute, by which time the tissue is 
generally found to have completely recovered from its ex- 
citatory electrical variation. : 

In the case of the experimental arrangements of which 
the diagram is given in fig. 27, stimulation is confined to one 
contact of the responding circuit. The method by which 
excitation was here prevented from reaching the distal 
contact is important. I shall, in the course of the present 
work, show that the parenchymatous tissue of the lamina of 
a leaf or leaflet is a bad conductor of excitation. Hence if 
the second contact of the circuit be made with this tissue, 
the stimulus does not reach the distal point. It is true that 
a certain small proportion might conceivably be conducted 
through the attenuated fibro-vascular channel of the midrib. 
But even so remote a contingency is provided against by a 
transverse cut across the midrib on the hither side of the 
contact. 

The arrangements, then, being made in the manner de- 
scribed, the tissue may be subjected to the action of successive 
uniform stimuli. How regular the resulting responses may be 
rendered will be seen from fig. 28, in which is given a series 
of responses obtained from the petiole of a fern (fig, 28) 


which the press-key © 


8 eee 


THE APPLICATION OF QUANTITATIVE STIMULUS 39 


under successive thermal shocks, imparted at intervals of one 
minute. We have hitherto studied the responses caused by 
uniform stimuli. We shall next observe the increase of 
responsive effects brought about by increase of stimulus. In 


Fic, 28. Photographic Record of Uniform Response in Petiole of Fern 
to transmitted excitation 


animal tissues it is found, speaking generally, that increasing 
stimuli induce increasing effects, but that this process has a 
limit ; and in plant tissues the same is found to be the case. 
In order to obtain effects of the simplest type, not compli- 
cated by any secondary phenomena, 
it is necessary to choose specimens 
which exhibit little fatigue. In the 
first of these the stimulus was ap- 
plied by means of the spring-tapper. 
The first stimulus was given by a 
fall of the striking-lever from the 
height h; the second from 2h; and 


; Fic. 
the third from 3h. The response- ee 
F Taps of increasing strength 
curves (fig. 29) clearly show the in- 1:2:3:4 producing in- 


crease of effect due to this increasing acheter eaiinat ees 
stimulus. 

In the second series, the stimulus applied was vibrational, 
and increased from 2°5° to 12°5° by steps of 2°5° at a time. 
Fig. 30 shows how the intensity of response tends under 
these conditions to approach a limit. The following table 
gives the absolute values of the responsive electro-motive 
variations. ; 


40 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


TABLE SHOWING THE INCREASED ELECTRO-MOTIVE VARIATION INDUCED 
BY INCREASING STIMULUS. 


Angle of vibration Induced E.M.F. 
rg 044 volt. 
5° "O75 
7:5° "090 55 
10° E0045 
12°5° "106 ,, 


In such normal cases an inerease of response is always 
induced with increasing stimulation. A diminution of 
response may, however, sometimes appear, with increasing 


° 


23° 5° 74° 10 12° 


Fic. 30. Increased Response with Increasing Vibrational Stimuli 
(Cauliflower-stalk) 


Vertical line = +1 volt. Stimuli applied at intervals of three minutes. 


stimulus. But this is merely a secondary effect, due to 
fatigue. The following records (fig. 31) will show in what 
manner this may be brought about. They were taken with 
specimens of the petiole of cauliflower, in one of which (A) 
fatigue was absent, while in the other (B) it was present. In 
the first specimen the recovery from each stimulus was 


THE APPLICATION OF QUANTITATIVE STIMULUS 4qI 


complete. Every response in this series starts, therefore, 
from a position of equilibrium, and the height of each single 
response increases with increasing stimulation. In the 
second case, however, the molecular derangement consequent 
on stimulation is not completely removed after any single 


40° so 


FIG. 31. Responses to Increasing Stimulus obtained with Two Specimens 
of Stalk of Cauliflower 


In (a) recovery is complete, in (4) it is incomplete. 


stimulus of the series. That the recovery is only partial is 
seen in the gradual shifting of the base-line upwards. In 
the former case the base-liné had been horizontal, represent- 
ing a condition of complete equilibrium. Now, however, the 
base-line, or line of modified equilibrium, is tilted upwards. 
Thus, even here, if we measure the heights of successive 


42 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


responses from the line of absolute equilibrium, they will be 
found to increase with increasing stimulus. Ordinarily, how- 
ever, no allowance is made for the shifting of the base-line, 
the responses being measured instead from the -place of its 
previous recovery, or point of modified equilibrium. In this 
way these responses undergo an apparent diminution. 

I have occasionally observed another curious phenomenon 
in connection with the subject of response under increasing 


stimulus. During the gradual increase of the stimulus from — 


a low value, there would at first be no response. But on 
reaching a certain critical value, a response would suddenly 
be evoked which was maximum—that is to say, would not be 
exceeded, even when the stimulus was further increased. 
We have here a parallel case to what is known in animal 
physiology as the ‘all or none’ principle. In the case of 
cardiac muscle, for example, there is a certain minimal 
intensity of stimulus which is effective in inducing response. 
‘But further increase of stimulation causes no concomitant 
increase of effect. 

When a tissue is subjected to rapidly succeeding stimuli, 
the excitatory effects are superposed upon each other. In 


muscle, for example, the contractile effect of the second 


stimulus is added to that of the first, before that has time to 
disappear. The result is a summation of effects more or less 
complete ; and these attain a maximum. With moderate 
frequency of stimulation, such a tetanic effect is incomplete, 
tending to become more and more complete, with the 
progressive increase of frequency (fig. 52). I have obtained 
results in every way similar to these, with the mechanical 
response of ordinary plants. In fig. 33 is given a photo- 
graphic record of tetanus, taken from the longitudinal motile 
responses of the style of Datura alba. Similar tetanic effects 
are also obtained in the electric response of plants, of which 
the records seen in fig. 34 form an example. 

The difficulties in the quantitative observation of electrical 
response have thus been overcome by the employment of two 
different methods of stimulation—namely, torsional vibration, 
and stimulation by thermal shocks. In the case of the 


EE -_ _ 


THE APPLICATION OF QUANTITATIVE STIMULUS 43 


former, the intensity of stimulation was seen to depend on 
the amplitude of vibration. In the latter, stimulus intensity 
was determined by that of the thermal variation, which again 
was regulated by the intensity and duration of the electrical 
heating-current. It was also seen to be important that the 


Fic. 32. Genesis of Tetanus in Muscle 


Record to left shows incomplete tetanus, with moderate frequency of 
stimulation. Record to right shows tetanus more complete, with 
greater frequency of stimulation (Brodie). 


excitation of one contact should be prevented from reaching 
the other, and this was provided against in two different 
ways. In the first of these, a physical block was interposed 
between the two contacts. In the second, the distal contact 
was made with the non-conducting tissue of a lateral leaf. 


& 
a es (b) 
Fic. 33. Photographic Record Fic. 34. Fusion of Effect ot 
of Genesis of Tetanus in Rapidly Succeeding Stimuli 


Mechanical KResponse of 


i scle ; (4) i t. 
Plants (Style of Datura alba) wi stg aati | let tin 


When these precautions were observed, it was found that 
uniform stimuli induced uniform response. Stimuli which 
were singly ineffective, were found on repetition to become 
effective. A tetanic effect was obtained by the rapid super- 
position of stimuli. 


CHAPTER IV 


OBSERVATION BY RHEOTOME ON ELECTRIC RESPONSE 
IN PLANTS 


Response - curve showing general time - relations — Instantaneous mechanical 
stimulation by electro-magnetic release—Arrangement of the rheotome— 
Tabular statement of results of rheotomic observations—Rhythmic multiple 
responses. 


IN taking records of the electric response of plants, a 
galvanometer of fairly high sensitiveness is required. One 
which gives a deflection of I mm. at a scale-distance of 
I-metre, under a current of 10°° ampere is found, as already 
said, to be suitable for practical purposes. I used for most of 
the experiments in this work a dead-beat galvanometer of 
the D’Arsonval type. The natural period of swing in these 
galvanometers is somewhat long, however, and the response- 
record thus lags behind the electro-motive changes induced 
by stimulus. 
In order, therefore, to investigate the time-relations of a 
growing electro-motive reaction in a plant, after the recep- 
tion of the stimulating shock, it is necessary to employ a 
rheotomic mode of investigation. An account of this, and 
of the results obtained, will be given in the course of the 
present chapter. The after-effect of stimulus is found to 
be somewhat persistent and to vary in duration in different 
specimens. In some cases, recovery is complete in a very 
short time; in others it takes very much longer. For the 
purpose of forming a general idea of this difference two 
response-records are given here, one of which was taken from 
a stem of the quickly-reacting Amaranth (fig. 35), and the 
other from the more sluggish Colocasza. It will be seen that 


oe a ee 


OBSERVATION BY RHEOTOME ON ELECTRIC RESPONSE 45 


while in the first of these the recovery was completed in 
fifteen seconds, in the latter, even after the lapse of forty 
seconds, it was still far from complete. Indeed, in this case 
it was not altogether accomplished till after several minutes. 
The character of the tissue again is an important factor in 
determining the time required for recovery. Thus it will be 
shown that in a vegetable structure functioning as nerve, 
recovery is much more rapid than in ordinary tissue. The 
physiological modification induced by season, moreover, is 
seen in the fact that response and recovery are quicker in 


Fic. 35- Response of (a) quickly reacting Amaranth ; 
(4) of sluggish Colocasia 


summer than in winter. This difference is demonstrated by 
mechanical response also, for in that of the leaf of Mzmosa, 
as already stated, it is found that, whereas in summer the 
period is six minutes, in winter it is as long as eighteen, or 
three times as much. 

In the study of the time-relations of response, we may 
overcome the difficulty of the galvanometer-inertia by using, 
as already said, some modification of the rheotome, originally 
devised by Bernstein. The relative values of electrical 
variation induced at various periods after the impact of 
the excitatory shock may here be found by making brief 


46 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


galvanometric contacts of equal period at the required 
intervals. : 3 

The difficulty in this investigation lies in the instan- 
taneous application of a stimulus at a definite moment, and 
in the successful adjustment of the subsequent interval at 
which the resulting responsive current is to be led to the 
gsalvanometer and recorded. Instantaneous stimulation can, 
it is true, be effected by electrical shock. But polarisation, 
and other disturbances caused by it, might give rise to 
unknown variations in the responsive effect. Hence, it is 
advisable when recording the electrical response to employ, 
if possible, a non-electrical form of stimulus. And it is only 
after the successful employment of such an unimpeachable 
method, that we can feel any confidence in the use, after due 
precautions have been taken, of the electrical stimulus itself, 
as will be described ina later chapter. Another obstacle to 
be overcome is the elimination of the unknown element of 
time required for transmission when stimulus is applied at 
a distance from the responding point. This uncertainty 
can only be removed by applying the stimulus directly on 
the responding point itself. All these difficulties I have 
successfully met by employing the mechanical form of 
stimulation, which I am now about to describe. We have 
seen that a stimulus of definite intensity may be imparted 
by a quick torsional vibration of either twist or un-twist, or 
of the one followed by the other. The intensity of this 
stimulus, as we saw, depends on the angle of torsion, and 
remains constant as long as that angle is maintained the 
same. For this-purpose I use the vibrational apparatus 
already described, successive excitations being produced on 
one side only, say the right. The torsion-head is set by 
pulling a vertical thread by which the index is made to rest 
against the stop P. This pull of the vertical thread is 
against the antagonistic action of the spiral spring S (fig. 36). 
During the process of the setting, which is carried out slowly, 
there is a slight excitatory disturbance. But this is allowed 
to subside, The vertical thread by which the torsion-head 


OBSERVATION BY RHEOTOME ON ELECTRIC RESPONSE 47 


is ‘set,’ is kept pulled up by an electro-magnetic arrangement 
shown in fig. 37, where the electro-magnet is seen to hold 


a soft iron armature at the 
end of the thread. At the 
moment when stimulation is 
to be effected the current 
which energises the electro- 
magnet is interrupted by an 
automatic arrangement which 
will be described later. By 
the break of the current the 
armature is released and a 
semi-vibration of the torsion- 


Fic. 36. Arrangement for In- 
stantaneous Stimulation 
Torsion-head set by string against stop Q 
is suddenly let go by electro-magnetic 

release seen in fig. 37. 


head is suddenly produced, the amplitude of which has been 
predetermined by suitable adjustment of the stop P. Suc- 


Fic. 37. General arrangement for Rheotomic Observation 


A, B, striking rods attached to revolving rheotomic disc; K,, key for 
electro-magnetic release of torsional stimulator ; K,, for unshunting the 
galvanometer, G; E, electro-magnet with its armature by which the 
vibration-head, Vv, is set at a definite torsion-angle; N,, N,, non- 
polarisable electrodes making electric contacts with specimen ;?c, com- 


pensator. 


cessive stimuli of equal intensity may thus be applied on 
the experimental tissue at whatever time may be desired. 


48 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


The next point is to secure an automatic arrangement 
by which galvanometric connections can be made with the 
experimental tissue, for a short period of time, say, ‘o1 of a 
second. In order to study the growing electro-motive changes, 
these short-lived contacts are to be effected in successive 
experiments at gradually increasing intervals of ‘oI, ‘02, ‘03 
seconds, and so on after stimulation. It should be remem- 
bered in connection with this subject that the reactions in 
plant tissues are much more sluggish than those in the. 
animal. The time-intervals here provided for, therefore, are 
even smaller than would have been strictly necessary, 

The general plan of the apparatus for carrying out this 
investigation is seen in fig. 37. The revolving rheotome-disc — 
carries two striking-bars, A and B, of which A is fixed, and 
B capable of an increasing angular adjustment behind A. 
The bar A, striking against the key K,, interrupts the electro- 
magnetic circuit E, thus causing stimulation. All this time, 
the galvanometer G is short-circuited by key K,, and it is 
-only when the striker B unshunts the galvanometer, by 
striking against K,, that the responsive current can act on the 
galvanometer. C is the compensating potentiometer, the 
object of which will be described presently. The rheotome- 
disc is rotated by means of a motor, provided with a perfect 
governor, the period of asingle rotation being adjusted to 
one second. The circumference of the disc is 100 cm. One 
centimetre of this circumference therefore represents an in- 
terval of time of ‘o1 second. The breadth of the striker B is 
also 1 cm. and it will therefore pass over a given point in the 
course of ‘or second. These striking-rods attached to the 
disc, impinge, as already said, against two electrical keys K,, 
and kK, which are adjusted along the same radius of the disc. 
K, is a balanced key, one end of which carries a two-pronged 
brass fork, both prongs of which are normally dipped in cups 
of mercury, thus completing the particular electric circuit. 
By the blow given by the striker A on a projecting rod 
attached to K,, this fork is tilted upwards, and the circuit is 
broken. The striker B then impinges on the second key, K,. 


OBSERVATIGN BY RHEOTOME ON ELECTRIC RESPONSE 49 


Here, the prong is kept down by a spring S, the circuit being 
re-made, as soon as the breadth of the striker B has passed 
over the projecting rod—that is to say, in ‘oI second (fig. 38). 
The interval of time be- 
tween the actions on the 
two keys, by which the 
two different electrical cir- 
cuits are broken in suc- 
cession, can be gradually 
increased, by increasing 
the angle between A and B. 

The key K,, as already 
said, controls the electro- 
magnet, which, on _ its 


release, instantaneously 
effects. the mechanical 7 

“stimulation of the tissue. *'* 38. en Sian £3 
The rotation of the rheo- K,, actuated by rod A, K, by B. 


tome-disc does not at once 

become uniform, on the Starting of the motor, but attains 
this when one revolution has been completed. Therefore 
the experimental observations are not made till the speed 
has become steady. By pressing the key K, during the first 
revolution (fig. 37), the break-action of A on K, is postponed. 
K, is then opened, and during the next revolution, stimulation 
is effected. 

In order to obtain the galvanometric effect of excita- 
tion at definite short intervals of time after the stimulus 
has been applied, the galvanometer short circuit, as stated 
before, is removed at those definite intervals. The adjust- 
ment of the striking-rod: B, in relation to A, enables us to 
open the short circuit, for ‘ol of a second, at increasing 
intervals. When the rod B is at a distance of I cm. from 4, 
the short circuit is removed after ‘ol second, when at 2 cm. 
after ‘02 second, and so on. Thus, in the arrangement just 
described, the galvanometer is short-circuited, except at those 
definite intervals required for observation. In a second 

E 


50 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


arrangement, the galvanometer is kept open, and closed only 
during ‘or of a second at the required increasing intervals of 
time. Fe . 

There may be a pre-existing difference of potential 
in the plant, as between the two points of galvanometric 
contact, N, and N, In order that this may not be a source 
of disturbance, a compensating potentiometer arrangement, 
C, is employed. The slider of the compensator is so adjusted 
that it exactly balances the resting difference of potential 
in the specimen. Under these circumstances, neither make 
nor break of the galvanometer occasions any deflection. And 
this balance is readjusted for each experiment of the series. 

I give below two tables which. summarise rheotomic 
observations on specimens of the petiole of cauliflower. By 
successive intervals of a given length should always be under- 
stood the mean interval: that is to say, the period between-the 
application of stimulus and the mid-point in the removal of 
the short circuit. Thus, for the mean interval of ‘02 of a 
second, the middle of the .striking-rod 8B, whose breadth is 
I cm., placed at a distance of 2 cm. from A. The galvano- 
meter is therefore acted on for ‘oI of a second, throughout 
‘the period from ‘o15 to ‘025 of a second, after stimulation. 
In the first two sets of results here given, the observations will 
-be seen to have been taken at the somewhat long intervals 
of ‘1 of asecond. The stimulus applied in these cases was 
moderate. In the case of a third specimen, the observations 
on which will be given subsequently, the results were recorded 
at intervals of ‘o1 of a second. 


SPECIMEN I SPECIMEN II 
Mean interval Galvanometric Mean interval Galvanometric .- 
after excitaton deflection after excitation deflection 
‘I second 70 divisions — “I second 26 divisions 
‘2 ” 100 29 | 2 ” ifele) 95 
3 se 310 $9 ey 7° ” 
"4 iy 220 99 “4 ” 64 9 
Hh sas 104 ee ae 56 2 
I°O ” 7° re) i ie) ne) 42 $9 
2°0 29 15 re) 2°O 55 24 <7 


OBSERVATION BY RHEOTOME ON ELECTRIC RESPONSE 51 


It will be seen from the observations made on the first of 
these two specimens that the maximum electro-motive effect 
was attained in three-tenths of a second after excitation. In 
the second case, the maximum was reached in two-tenths 
of a second. The curve given in fig. 39 shows how quickly 
the electro-motive variation attains a maximum, and how 
rapid is its decline after reaching this point. There apppears 
to be practically no latent period, the induction of the electro- 
motive effect being apparently immediate. This will be 
made evident by the results given in the next series. 


Fic. 39. Curve showing Rise and Fall of Responsive E.M. Change, 
under moderate stimulation 


Ordinate represents galvanometric deflection ; abscissa, time. Large 
division = 1 second. (Petiole of cauliflower. ) 


For the next experiment, I took the stem of Amaranth, 
which I find to be more excitable and more quickly reacting - 
than the petiole of cauliflower. The intensity of stimulus 
was here greater than in the last case. I may state now what 
will be demonstrated in full later, that a strong stimulus 
often gives rise, not to a single, but to multiple responses. 
I had previously detected these multiple responses by means 
of both mechanical and galvanometric indications, and found 
them to have periodicities varying from some I5 seconds to 
several minutes. . Indeed, had they been much*quicker than 
they were, they could not have been detected, owing to the 

E2 


§2 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


inertia of the motile leaflets or of the galvanometer needle. 
By the employment of the rheotomic method, however, I was 
able to detect multiple responses having periodicities of the 
order of one-tenth or so of a second. All the experiments 
carried out on Amaranth gave two or three waves of electro- 
motive variation, the character of which will be understood 
from the following table and curve (fig. 40). 


Mean interval after excitation | Galvanometric deflection 


| 


‘OI second 63 divisions 

"05 29 77 29 ‘ | 
7S eee | 82 55 | 
“20 Fs; | 68 - 
"30 | 765 | 
*40 rr 75 re) | 
60s; | ce ea | 
70 55 86 oe) | 
80, | OF 555 | 


It will here be seen that even after so short an interval as 
‘ol of a second, a considerable electromotive change had 


Fic. 40. Response Curve from Rheotomic Observation in Stem of 
Amaranth under strong stimulation 


Note occurrence of multiple response. Large division of abscissa = *1 second. 


already been induced. The first maximum occurred after 
y5, the second after *3, and the third after ‘6 of a second. 


Nea 


ce eae ee. ee ee eae ee ee 


OBSERVATION BY RHEOTOME ON ELECTRIC RESPONSE 53 


We thus see that there is a rhythm in these multiple 
responses, the successive maxima being here found to 
occur at periods which constitute multiples of ‘15 second. 
The third of these rhythms may be presumed to be missing, 
owing to the fact that no observation was taken at ‘45 second, 
as, at the time when these experiments were carried out, 
I was unaware of the existence of such rhythmicities. It was 
by the curves plotted from these data that my attention was 
first drawn to their occurrence. 

It will thus be seen that the electro-motive variation is 
initiated practically simultaneously with the impact of 
stimulus on the organ. With moderate stimulus, the maxi- 
mum variation is reached within two-tenths of a second, or 
this period may be made still shorter by the employment 
either of a more quickly reacting tissue, or of a greater 


_intensity of stimulus. Strong stimulation is apt to give rise 


to rhythmic multiple responses. 


CHAPTER'..V 


THE ELECTRICAL INDICATIONS OF POSITIVE AND 
-NEGATIVE TURGIDITY VARIATIONS 


Motile responses of opposite signs, characteristic of positive and negative 
 turgidity-variations—Indirect hydrostatic effect of stimulus causes expansion 
and erection of leaf—Dositive and negative work—Wave of increased hydro- 
static tension transmitted with relatively greater velocity than wave of true 
excitation — Method of separating hydro-positive and excitatory effects—In- 
. direct effect of stimulus, causing positive turgidity-variation’ induces galvano- 
‘metric positivity—Antagonistic elements in the electrical response —Separation 
of hydro-positive from true excitatory effect by means of physiological block. 


~HAVING now described that fundamental electrical response 
of galvanometric negativity which is characteristic of excita- 
tion, I shall next proceed to deal with an opposite type 
of response—namely, that of galvanometric positivity. The 
combination of these two factors, in varying degrees of each, 
in the electrical response of plants, has been a source in the 
past of the greatest perplexity, leading investigators to con- 
tradictory results. And it can only be by disentangling them, 
and by ascertaining the conditions under which each invari- 
ably occurs, that precision will be arrived at in the field of 
electrical response. 

We have seen that excitation of the pulvinus of Mimosa 
induces negative turgidity-variation, with fall of the leaf, and 
galvanometric negativity. What, then, would be the out- 
ward expression of an increase of turgidity—that is to say, of 
the positive turgidity-variation ? 

With regard, first, to the mechanical expression, we may 
subject the question to an experimental test. The cut end 
of a branch of Mimosa, bearing leaves, is fixed watertight 
in one end of a U-tube, filled with water, and the other 


POSITIYE AND NEGATIVE TURGIDITY-VARIATIONS 55 


end is connected alternately with a vacuum and a force 
pump, by means of which a diminution or increase of hydro- 
static pressure may be induced at will. In this way it is 
possible to suck water away from, or force it into, the plant 
and its organs, thus producing negative and positive turgidity- 
variations at will. When turgidity is thus diminished the 
indicating leaf is seen to fall. This is what happens also 
under the ordinary negative turgidity-variation induced by 
excitation. When turgidity is increased, on the other hand, 
the leaf is erected (fig. 41). In the case of the negative 
variation of turgidity, the pulvinus as a whole loses water, 


10° 20° 30° 40° 50’ 60’ 70’ 80° 90° 


Fic. 41. Artificial Hydraulic Response of AZimosa 


The plant was subjected to diminished pressure up to a, and to normal 
pressure to 4, after which the pressure was increased. The effect 
of diminished pressure, in the depression of the leaf, continues for a 
while. The ordinate represents movement of tip of leaf in cm., 
abscissa represents time. 


but more from the lower and more excitable half than 
from the upper. In the case of the positive turgidity- 
variation, also, it is again the more excitable lower half 
which absorbs the greater quantity of water. Thus, in Mzmosa, 
and in Bzophytum, the mechanical indication of increased 
turgidity is an erection of the leaf or leaflet. The 
characteristic electrical indication of this will be observed 
presently. 

It has already been mentioned that the direct application 
of stimulus at a point causes a negative turgidity-variation 
of that point. We shall now see whether, under any 


56 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


circumstances, stimulus will induce a positive turgidity- 
variation. If we now apply moderate stimulus on the 
stem of J/zmosa, at a certain distance below the pulvinus, an 
excitatory expulsion of water will occur at the point directly 
stimulated. Such an expulsion of water will then cause, it 
is clear, a hydrostatic disturbance of increased pressure. 
And this hydro-mechanical disturbance will be transmitted 
with relatively great velocity. Now such an increase of 
pressure, as we have seen, causes an increase of turgidity at 
the puivinus, in consequence of 
which the leaf ought to be erected. 
And although this hydrostatic 
disturbance is transmitted very 
quickly, yet a certain time is con- 
sumed in the process of forcing 
water into the pulvinus, by which 
to bring about the erection of the 
leaf. After the passage of the 
hydrostatic wave, there follows 
Fic. 42. Experimental Arrange- the wave of true excitation, passing 
sea arn ares from cell to cell, and inducing the 
given to Direct and In- Characteristic reaction, of negative 
eee eee by Leaf of tyroidity - variation. And when 
Thermal stimulator at s produces this excitatory wave reaches the 
trey smlstion my gene pulvinus, the previous erectile 
stimulation, at a distant point, Movement should give place ~ to 
Sys eterna to indirect effect excitatory depression, or fall of 
the leaf. In fig. 42 are seen the 

arrangements for an ptoknehiueiit on Mimosa by which these 
inductions may be verified. Moderate thermal stimulus is 
applied at S, at a certain distance below the indicating leaf. 
This latter is attached by a thread to a writing-lever, which 
traces the response-record on a smoked revolving drum. 
When the stimulus is applied at a point S very near the 
pulvinus, the response takes place by a negative turgidity- 
variation, with a concomitant fall of the leaf, seen in fig. 43 
(a) as an up-curve. When a moderate stimulus is applied 


a lla le i 


POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE TURGIDITY-VARIATIONS 57 


at a greater distance S,,, the hydrostatic wave causing the 
positive turgidity-variation brings about an erectile twitch. 
This is followed by a responsive fall, when the true excitation 
reaches the organ (fig. 43, 0). 

It has thus been shown that by separating the responding 
point from the point stimulated, or the receptive point, it is 
possible to discriminate two 
different effects which are both 
brought about by stimulus. It 
is most important, moreover, to 
distinguish between these two 
factors : namely, the direct effect 
of stimulus causing contraction, 
and its indirect effect, causing 
expansion. We have seen that 
direct excitation and transmitted 
excitation both induce contrac- 
tion, negative turgidity-variation 
and fall of the leaf. Unfor- 
tunately, in animal physiology, Fic. 43. Mechanical Responses 


it has been customary to apply Reet eaaesmesa 
eee (a) record of responsive fall when 
the term zuzdirect to that form stimulus applied near the re- 
of stimulation which is applied sponding organ; (4) response 
. : * when stimulus is applied on 
at a distance. And it has not same side, but at greater dis- 
. . tance, s,,. A preliminary erectile 
been noticed that such stimulus seuliocie ins bere: fallowed.” bey 
is capable of inducing diametri- the true excitatory depression. 
: ' This is due to the indirect effect 
cally opposite results, according first transmitted being succeeded 
as the true excitatory effect by the direct. Had the stimulus 
applied been feebler, or more 
reaches, or does not reach, the distant, there would have been 
responding organ. When the ons first, or indirect erectile 


intervening tissue is highly 
conducting, the transmitted effect induces exactly the same 
result as if stimulus were applied directly. But we shall 
see that when the intervening tissue is non-conducting or 
feebly conducting, true excitation is not transmitted, and the 
effect which makes its appearance at the responding point 
is then due to increase of hydrostatic tension, causing positive 


58 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


turgidity-variation, with the concomitant effect of expansion, 
and, in the case of J/zmosa, of erection of the leaf. This 
latter effect of positive turgidity-variation and expansion, 
I shall therefore distinguish as the INDIRECT EFFECT of 
stimulus, in contradistinction to the term INDIRECT STIMU- 
LATION, as it is generally used. The last-named, however, 
I shall myself always refer to under the title of TRANS- 
MITTED STIMULATION. If the intervening tissue be of 


Fic. 44. Mechanical Response of Biophy/um to Thermal Stimulation 


Stimulus was applied at some distance from the responding leaflet. And 
the preliminary erectile twitch is due to the prior arrival of the 
hydrostatic disturbance. Thick dot represents moment of application 
of stimulus. 


moderate conducting power, we shall, as in the case of the 
experiments on J/zmosa, obtain a preliminary erectile twitch, 
due to the indirect effect of stimulus, followed by a fall, in 
consequence of the transmission of the true excitatory effect. 
In fig. 44 is seen this twofold expression of the indirect 
and transmitted effects of stimulus, given by the leaflet of 
Biophytum. 

These two waves, then, of increased hydrostatic tension 


ah a hh ll al ee ee 
’ 


“Me as tare, 


POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE TURGIDITY-VARIATIONS 59 


and of true excitation, induce, as we have now seen, opposite 
responsive reactions. But of these, that due to true excita- 
tion is, generally. speaking, greatly predominant. Hence, 
when these two waves reach the responding organ in close 
succession, as is the case when the point of stimulation is 
very near, the excitatory effect masks the hydrostatic. In 
order, then; to ‘separate them, we may employ various 
methods. First, in the case of highly conducting tissues, the 
stimulus must be applied at a sufficient distance to make the 
slow excitatory wave lag adequately behind the quickly 
travelling hydrostatic wave. Or we may choose a direction 
of transmission of excitation which will be relatively slow. 
Thus I have found that transmission across a stem, for 
example, is very much slower than along its length. Hence, 
on applying moderate stimulus at S, (fig. 42) ata point on the 
stem diametrically opposite the pulvinus, of the given leaf, it 
is found that the excitation reaches the pulvinus only after a 
measurable interval, the hydrostatic effect inducing erectile 
response much earlier. Thus in a given experiment, whose 
record was taken on a fast-moving drum (fig. 45), the erectile 
response took place ‘6 second after the application of stimu- 
lus, whereas the true excitatory fall did not occur till 3:45 
seconds had elapsed—that is to say, 2°85 seconds later. It is 
to be borne in mind that’ a certain interval of time passes, 
even after the arrival of the respective waves, before the tur- 
gidity-variation is able to give rise to the motile indication. 
Let us next examine the results at the responding tissue 
of the indirect effect of stimulus. The distant receptive point 
contracts on stimulation, and sends to the responding organ 
a wave of increased hydrostatic tension. This, as we have 
seen, forces water in, and expands the tissue.. Work is thus 
done ox the tissue which increases its store of energy. In 
this the indirect is unlike the typical direct effect of stimulus. 
For the latter causes the impulsive fall of the leaf, which 
represents work done dy the tissue, and an expenditure of 
energy. We must, therefore, recognise two distinct respon- 
sive effects, according as the work done is fosétive—done on 


60 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


the tissue—or negative: that is to say, done by the tissue. 
The outward manifestations of these two processes are 
respectively expansion and contraction. The positive, as we 
shall see, is not the result of hydrostatic disturbance as such, 
but is the effect of energy transmitted hydraulically. 

The indirect effect of stimulus, then, gives rise to positive 
turgidity-variation, and increases the internal or -potential 


Fic. 45. Record of Response of A/zmosa Leaf, taken on a fast- 
moving drum 


Stimulus applied at moment @, on point of stem diametrically opposite to 
responding leaf. Hydro-positive erectile response occurs at 4, *6 second 
after application of stimulus. True excitatory response of fall takes 
place at c, 3°45 seconds after application of stimulus. Time-marks 
represent fifths of a second. 


energy of the organ. A positive turgidity-variation is thus 
concomitant with an increase of internal energy and 
a negative turgidity-variation with the reverse. In observing 
the mechanical response, we saw that the expression of 
positive turgidity-variation, due to the indirect effect of 
stimulus, consisted of an erection, and was therefore of 
opposite sign to that of the negative turgidity-variation, 
caused by true excitation, and expressing itself in a fall, of 


POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE TURGIDITY-VARIATIONS 61 


the leaf. We shall now see whether a similar difference 
exists between the electrical expressions of the positive and 
negative turgidity-variations. 

In carrying out this experiment, I took a specimen of 
Biophytum and applied stimulus at a distance from the par- 
ticular leaflet whose responses were to be observed, arranging, 
at the same time, for a simultaneous record of the mechanical 
and electrical responses. It will be seen from fig. 46 that the 
preliminary erectile twitch, due to the positive turgidity- 
variation, has, as its concomitant, galvanometric positivity. 
And this is followed in both records by its opposite: namely, 
the contractile fall and the galvanometric negativity of true 
excitation. 

It will thus be seen that the increase of internal energy, with 
its positive turgidity-variation, has, as its electrical expression, 
galvanometric positivity. Besides this, the mere physical 
movement of water in 
the tissue gives rise to 
a certain electrical varia- 
tion of positivity, and 
this can still be detected, 
even after the tissue is 
killed. The question of 
how to discriminate what 
proportion of the electro- 
positivity was due to this 
mere water - movement, 
and what to the increase 
of turgidity, associated 
with the. increase of in- 


Fic. 46. The Abnormal Positive preceding 
the Normal Negative in Mechanical and 
ternal energy, I at first Electrical Responses in Biophytuni 
found it very difficult to * represents the moment of application of 
; , stimulus. The upper is the mechanical 
decide. But I ultimately and the lower the’ electrical record. 


The records downward indicate erection 


succeeded in doing this of the leaf or galvanometric positivity. 


by bringing a plant to 
a condition just short of death, and thus abolishing its 
true excitatory reaction. In this condition, the responsive 


62 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


indication was found to be one of considerable electro- 
positivity. On finally killing the-plant, however, the positive 
change due to water-movement was found to represent so 
insignificant a proportion of the whole as to be negligible, 

In order to exhibit the electrical expression of the 
excitatory and hydro-positive effects of stimulation, in 
ordinary plants, I took a petiole of cauliflower and made 
one connection, the proximal, 
with a point on it, and the other 
with an indifferent point on the 
surface of the lamina. In order 
to obtain the unmistakable hydro- 
static effect, the petiole was sud- 
denly squeezed, at a distance of 
6.cm. from the proximal contact, 
and this gave rise, as will be seen 
(fig. 47, a), to a positive response, 
represented downwards. This 
was repeated once more, and the 
same effect observed. I next 
applied thermal stimulus at a dis- 
tance of 4 cm. from the respond- 
ing point. In this case hydro- 
static and excitatory disturbances 

reached the contact, the hydro- 

PIG, ty vical Reponse of Peticle Static shortly followed by the 

of Cauliflower excitatory, as in the case of the 

a, hydro-positive ; 2, di-phasic; experiment on Mimosa. This 

c, excitatory negative responses. : : 

is seen in the record, as a di- 

phasic response, the hydrostatic positive being followed by 
the excitatory negative (fig. 47, 0). 

Reference has already been made to the observation of 
Burdon Sanderson, that in the lamina of the Dzonea leaf the 
immediate response was one of galvanometric positivity. Mis- 
taking this for the true excitatory effect, he concluded that 
the response of the plant was of opposite sign to that of the 


POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE TURGIDITY-VARIATIONS 63 


animal. From the experiment just described, however, it will 
be seen that the effect observed by him was in reality due, 
not to true excitation, but to the hydrostatic disturbance, or 
indirect effect of stimulus. 

In the next record (fig. 47, ¢) we see the effect of stimulus 
applied nearer: that is to say, at a distance of 2 cm. from the 
proximal contact. Owing to the propinquity of the point of 
stimulation, the two disturbances are not now sufficiently 
separated, and the excitatory negative reaction completely 
masks the hydrostatic positive effect. 

It is thus seen that, as has been said, one method of 
exhibiting these two effects separately is to apply stimulus 
at a point so distant from the proximal contact that there 
is an interval between the arrival of the two waves of hydro- 
static and excitatory disturbance respectively. It is obvious, 
then, that if the tissue under experiment be a good con- 
ductor of excitation, we must place the point of stimulation 
at a long distance from the first electrode, in order that 
the effect of excitation may lag sufficiently behind the 
hydrostatic wave. Similarly, in a bad conductor of excita-— 
tion, it will be the indirect effect alone which will reach 
the proximal contact, unless the stimulus applied be very 
near, and very strong. 

In order to distinguish these two opposite effects from 
each other, I shall in future refer to that hydrostatic 
effect which causes expansion and galvanometric positivity 
as ‘the hydro-positive effect, by way of differentiating it 
from ‘the true excitatory. effect, of negative turgidity- 
variation and galvanometric negativity. 

It has already been said that tissues which exhibit 
a high degree of. conduction are characterised by more 
or less of protoplasmic continuity. Hence, fibro-vascular 
elements are relatively good, and parenchymatous. tissues 
bad, conductors of excitation. The cells of the potato tuber 
for this reason exhibit very little power of transmitting 
excitation. When, therefore, in experimenting with this 


64 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


tissue, stimulation was caused by application of a hot wire 
so near as I cm. to the proximal contact, it was the hydro- 
positive effect alone which reached it, giving rise to posi- 
tive response. It was only, indeed, by applying the stimulus 
very near, at a distance of 3 mm., that the true excitatory 

response of galvanometric nega- 

tivity was in this case . obtained 
. (fig. 48). 

From what has been said, it 
will be seen that when a given 
point is excited by transmitted 
stimulation, two antagonistic elec- 
trical effects are induced—one of 
positivity, due to hydro-positive 
action, and the other of negativity, 
due to true excitation. When 
the stimulator is near to, or co- 
incides with, the responding point, 
the tissue is subjected to rapidly 
succeeding positive and negative 
turgidity-variations, and the elec- 
trical indication of the latter being 
the more intense, it masks the 

Fic. 48. Photographic Record former, and the resulting response 
of Electrical Responses of . : A 
Potato-tuber is determined by an _ algebraical 

a, Positive response to stimulus summation of the two. In a 

applied at distance ; 4, Negative yjgorous specimen, whose excit- 
response to stimulus applied tan : 

near. ability is great, the excitatory gal- 

vanometric negativity masks the 
positivity. The resultant electrical response in general is 
expressed by the formula N,z—P,, where N, is the galvano- 
metric negativity due to the true excitatory effect,and P,, the 
positivity due to the hydro-positive effect, which immediately 
precedes it. From this, it is clear that, as regards the 
resultant galvanometric response of vegetable tissues under 
stimulation, there may occur the two typical cases displayed 
in the following table : 


POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE TURGIDITY-VARIATIONS 65 


TYPICAL CASES OF RESULTANT RESPONSE 


Conditions . Constituent Factors Resultant Response 
Excitability great Nez >Pu Galvanometric negativity 
Excitability diminished Ng <Pxy Galvanometric positivity 

or nearly abolished 


We thus see how, under the physiological modifications 
induced by various agents, the normal negative response of 
a tissue may undergo diminution, or even reversal. We 
have, for the sake of simplicity, assumed that the two 
antagonistic effects act on the specimen simultaneously. 
But, asa matter of fact, under certain circumstances, their 
time-relations may be subjected to change. Hence various 
effects of interference may be seen, giving rise to diphasic 
responses, such as positivity followed by negativity, or the 
reverse. Or, instead of diphasic, there may even be multi- 
phasic responses, since it will be shown that very strong 
stimulus may cause not a single but repeated responses. 
Some of these effects will be described in detail in a sub- 
sequent chapter. 

I shall here meanwhile describe another method, in which, 
by means of a selective physiological block, we can unmask 
the contained hydrostatic effect of positivity, from the 
ordinary response of galvanometric negativity. The oc- 
currence of the excitatory contraction, in J/zmosa for 
example, as shown by the fall of the leaf, depends on a 
favourable excitatory condition of the tissue. If this ‘motile 
excitability should be in any way depressed or abolished as, 
say, by application of ice-cold water on the pulvinus, the 
true excitatory response could not take place. But if, under 


these circumstances, we applied stimulus at the diametrically 


Opposite point S, (fig. 42), the hydrostatic wave alone would 

be found to reach the organ. and, vz e¢ armzs, to produce 

expansion there, with a consequent erection of the leaf. 

It will thus be seen that the hydrostatic transmission of the 

indirect effect of stimulus is not to any great extent affected 
F 


66 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


by the loss of excitability of the tissue. Similarly, the 
transmission of true excitation may be selectively blocked, 
in a tissue, by the application of various depressing agents, 
such as anesthetics, without appreciably affecting the 
passage of the wave of increased hydrostatic tension. 

For the present experiment I took a leaf of fern, and 
made the proximal electrical connection with the petiole, and 
the distal with an indifferent point on a leaflet. By means 
of the thermal stimulator (p.38), I now applied successive © 
uniform stimuli at intervals of one minute, on a point 
15 cm. below the proximal .contact. The first. three 


Fic. 49. Photographic Record of Electrical Response of Petiole of Fern 


First part of record shows normal negative responses ; second part shows 
positive response unmasked by selective physiological block of chloro- 
form ; in the third part is seen the abolition of response when stimulus 
is applied on anzsthetised area itself. 


responses of the series are seen (fig. 49) to be more or less 
uniform, and negative. Such resultant response is, as has 
been pointed out, due to the summation of two antagonistic 
effects, of which the excitatory is predominant. In order 
therefore to eliminate from this resultant response its excita- 
tory component, I applied chloroform on a narrow belt inter- 
mediate between the point of stimulation and the proximal 
contact. From the next three responses it will be seen that 
the hydrostatic effect of positivity, represented by down- 
responses, was thus successfully unmasked. The application 
of chloroform is thus seen to act here as a selective block, 


POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE TURGIDITY-VARIATIONS 67 


the wave of increased tension being transmitted across its 
area, whereas the wave of true excitation is arrested. 

But the hydrostatic disturbance itself was caused by the 
excitatory contraction of the distant point. The abolition of 
the excitability of an intermediate point did not, as we have 
seen, block the hydrostatic wave. If now the stimulator be 
brought nearer, and placed over the strongly anzsthetised 
area, the expulsion of water dependent on true excitation 
can no longer take place. On doing this, therefore, we find 
that neither the true excitatory negative, nor its consequent 
hydrostatic positive effect, is exhibited at the responding 
point. Response is thus totally abolished. 

We thus see that incident stimulus gives rise in the plant 
to two distinct responsive expressions. In that which we 
shall consider first—namely, the direct, or true excitatory 
effect—there is an expenditure of energy, which is indicated 
by a contraction, negative turgidity-variation, mechanical 
fall, and galvanometric negativity. In the second, or indirect, 
of these two effects, we have the opposite of ali these. That 
is to say, we have here an increase of internal energy, expan- 
sion, positive turgidity-variation, erection of leaf, and galvano- 
metric positivity. The galvanometric negativity, due to 
negative turgidity-variation, is generally speaking of much 
greater intensity than the galvanometric positivity, due to 
positive turgidity-variation. Hence, when the two effects 
act on a tissue in rapid succession, that of electro-negativity 
masks the positive. The wave of increased hydrostatic tension, 
by which the indirect effect of stimulus is transmitted, has a 
greater speed of transmission than that of true excitation. It 
may be transmitted even across tissues of which the excita- 
bility has been depressed or abolished, whereas the trans- 
mission of true excitation is arrested by the intervention of 
such a physiological block. 

The occurrence of these opposed effects of galvanometric 
negativity and positivity has long been regarded as a pheno- 
menon of a highly perplexing nature. It was too hastily 
assumed that wherever these opposed electrical changes took 

F.2 


65 = COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


place, they were necessarily to be ascribed to opposite or 
antagonistic chemical processes—namely, of assimilation and 
dissimilation. That this, however, cannot be universally true, 
has here been proved by tracing the two effects to their 
definite respective origins, in positive and negative turgidity- 
variations. I shall enter into this subject in greater detail in 
the subsequent chapters. 


CHAPTER VI 


EXTERNAL STIMULUS AND INTERNAL ENERGY 


Hydraulic transmission of energy in living tissue ~True meaning of tonic con- 
dition—Opposite expressions of internal energy and external stimulus seen in 
growth-response— Parallelism between responses of growing and motile organs 
—Increased internal energy caused by augmentation of temperature finds 
expression in enhanced rate of growth ; erection of motile leaf; curling move- 
ment of spiral tendril ; and galvanometric positivity—External stimulus induces 
opposite effect in all these cases—Sudden variation of temperature, acting as a 
stimulus, induces transient retardation of growth; depression of motile leaf ; 
uncurling movement of spiral tendril ; and galvanometric negativity—Laws of 
mechanical and electrical response. 


We have seen in the last chapter that external stimulus, 
directly applied, induces in an excitable vegetable tissue a 
contractile response, with concomitant galvanometric nega- 
tivity. An increase of internal energy, on the other hand, 
was there seen to give rise to expansion, and galvanometric 
positivity. These two factors of external stimulus and internal 
energy are thus seen to be antagonistic in their general 
expressions. 

But while stimulus from outside, impinging on an excit- 
able area, thus caused an expenditure of energy at that area, 
by inducing excitatory response there, yet it was also found 
that, by its indirect effect, it brought about an increase of 
energy in neighbouring tissues. By the sudden contraction, 
and expulsion of water from the excited area, energy was © 
transmitted hydraulically, and the consequent positive tur- 
gidity-variation caused an erectile response of a neighbour- 
ing motile organ. In the simple case in which the point of 
receptivity was at a distance from that of response, it was 
seen that these two effects of stimulus, direct and indirect, 
were easily discriminated from one another. But as the 


7O COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


stimulator was brought nearer and nearer, the two effects 
became superposed, and one was masked by the other. 
Even in this case, however, on careful examination, it is 
possible to infer the results due to the action of the internal 
factor. 

Thus, we may suppose stimulus to be applied directly on 
the pulvinus of M/zmosa, bringing about a responsive fall of 


the leaf. The expelled water from the excited pulvinus will — 


‘now be forced into the neighbouring tissues, making them 
over-turgid, and raising their energy above par. On the 


cessation of stimulus, the water that has been forced away - 


will flow back from the region of heightened, to that of 
lowered tension, and re-establish the normal turgidity of the 
pulvinus, which had undergone a negative variation, The 
erectile recovery of the leaf is thus seen to be, not a merely 
passive process, but an effect dependent on the internal energy 
of the plant.. This is also shown by the fact that in autumn 
and winter, when the internal energy is low, the period of 
recovery is very long, being sometimes as much as eighteen 
minutes ; whereas in summer, on the other hand, with the 
increased internal energy of the plant, it takes place nearly 
three times as quickly. I have elsewhere shown that it is 
this internal energy which is vaguely referred to as the tonic 
condition of the tissue, and that it consists of the sum total 
of energy derived from external stimuli previously absorbed 
and held latent by the plant. The different forms of stimulus 
may be very various. We have for instance tonicity, as 
imparted by light, or phototonus ; by favourable temperature, 
thermotonus ; by electrical current, edectrotonus; by internal 
hydrostatic pressure, hydrotonus ; or by the presence of favour- 
able chemical substances, chemotonus. 

We have seen that, as a general rule, external stimulus 
and internal energy find responsive expressions of opposite 
sign, the former inducing a negative, and the latter, a positive 
turgidity-variation. It is, nevertheless, important to demon- 
strate the extensive applicability of this law, by many different 
results and different modes of manifestation. And such a 


ee ea a ne 


ne A aut RE ip rt ee Se Be 


EXTERNAL STIMULUS AND INTERNAL ENERGY oe oe 


demonstration would undoubtedly become still more con- 
vincing, if we should succeed in discovering some mode of 
response in which the antagonistic effects of internal energy 
and external stimulus found opposite expressions. 

-- Such an example, of a very striking character, I have in 
my work on ‘ Plant Response’ shown to be found in growth- 
response. But the same opposition, between the effects of 
external stimulus and internal energy, I shall now proceed to 
demonstrate not only by means of growth-response, but also 
through mechanical and electrical responses. In the case of 
srowth, the responsive expression of the growing organ, under 
increased turgidity, consists of an expansive elongation. If 
the organ be growing at a uniform rate, an increase of internal 
energy will enhance that rate. But when external stimulus 
acts directly on the growing organ, the normal rate of growth 
is retarded during the action of stimulus. Thus it will be seen 
that though the mechanical response of a motile organ, and 
the movement of a growing organ, appear so different, yet 
these two expressions are not fundamentally distinct. For 
while in one, the application of direct stimulus, causing nega- 
tive turgidity-variation and contraction, induces depression of 
the leaf, in the other, the same negative turgidity-variation 
and contraction under external stimulus causes a depression 
of the rate of growth. And, on the other hand, indirect effect 
of stimulus, er increase of internal energy, inducing positive 
turgidity-variation, brings about in the one case the erection 
of the leaf, in the other an enhancement of the normal rate 
of growth. This parallelism is displayed in detail in the 
table given below. 

It is thus understood that the indication of response to 
external stimulus is the depression of the motile leaf, or 
depression of the rate of growth, while the effect of increased 
internal energy is the erection of the motile leaf, or enhance- 
ment of the rate of growth. 

We shall first deal with the responsive expression to 
that positive turgidity-variation which is due fo the increase 
of internal energy. One mode of increasing the internal 


72 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


TABULAR STATEMENT SHOWING COMPARATIVE EFFECTS OF EXTERNAL 
STIMULUS AND INTERNAL ENERGY ON PULVINATED AND GROWING ORGANS 


| Mechanical response Growth response 
Effect of normal turgidity : Effect of normal turgidity : 

Normal horizontal position of leaf. Uniform rate of growth. 

Local action of external stimulus : Local action of external stimulus : 

Contraction ; Contraction ; 

Diminution of turgidity ; Diminution of turgidity ; 

and concomitant depression of leaf. and concomitant depression of rate 

of growth. 
Action of internal energy exhibited by | Action of internal energy exhibited by 
(a) Recovery ; (az) Recovery: 

Re-establishment of turgidity and Re-establishment of turgidity and 
gradual return of leaf to normal gradual return of organ to normal 
horizontal position. rate of growth. 

(4) Increased hydrostatic pressure : (4) Increased hydrostatic pressure : 

Erection of leaf. Increased rate of growth. - 


energy of a plant is by a moderate rise of temperature. And 
this finds expression in the case of a motile organ, by the 
erection of the leaf. Thus, when J/zmosa is raised in tem- 
perature, all its leaves become highly erect. A diminution 
of energy, on the other hand, by cooling, brings about a 
depression of the leaves. 

In the same way, the rate of growth is exalted by rise of 
temperature. Thus in a growing flower of Crinum lily the 
normal rate of growth at 30° C. was ‘oo4o mm. per minute, 
and this was exalted to ‘o113 mm. per minute, or nearly 
three times, when the temperature was raised*to 35°5° C. 
Lowering of temperature, on the other hand, greatly de- 
presses the rate, and may even, if it proceed far enough, 
cause arrest of growth. 

As regards external stimulus, on the contrary, we have 
seen that its effect on the motile organ is one of depression. 
The following record (fig. 50) shows that it has a similar 
influence on the rate of growth. The first part of the curve 
shows the normal rate of elongation. But after the applica- 
tion of stimulus of light, growth is not only retarded, but 
there is an actual shortening of the organ. On the cessation 
of stimulus, the normal rate of growth is gradually re- 
established. 


EXTERNAL STIMULUS AND INTERNAL ENERGY 73 


I shall next give examples in which the opposite effects 
of external stimulus and internal energy are exhibited in 
growth response, motile response, and electrical response. 
To take first the response of growth: we have seen that 
a steady rise of temperature brings about an increase of 
internal energy, while a sudden variation of temperature 
acts as a stimulus. Thus, if we effect a sudden augmenta- 
tion of temperature, this will act on the organ, during the 
period of variation, as a stimulus; but afterwards, when 
the temperature itself, or its rate of rise, has become 


mm, 
“| 


“2 


| 


= =. ee eS ! { 


Be) Seas SESE Le ve = Oe SES. ! parsed WEISEL" 
10° 15° 20’ 25° 30\35’ 40° 45’ 50’ 55’ 60’ 65° 70’ 


Fic. 50. Longitudinal Contraction and Retardation of Growth under 
Light in Hypocotyl of Smaps nigra 


The first part of the curve shows the normal rate of growth. Arrow (1) 
indicates moment of application of diffuse light, which is seen not only 
to retard growth, but also to induce a’ marked contraction. The 
second arrow indicates moment of withdrawal of light, and dotted 
portion of the curve shows recovery. 


steady, the condition will act by increasing the internal 
energy of the organ. These opposite results are seen to 
be strikingly illustrated by growth response in the case of 
the following record (fig. 51), 

The normal rate of growth at 34° C. was here ‘o15 mm. 
per two minutes. By a sudden application of heat, raising 
the temperature of the chamber ultimately by 1° C., a respon- 
sive contraction was caused, as is seen in the record. But, 
on the attainment of a steady augmented temperature of 
35° C., an increased rate of growth, which now amounted to 
024 mm. per two minutes, was observed, owing to the 
increase of internal energy. 


74 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Or the same difference may be demonstrated by means 
of mechanical response. A Mzmosa is placed in a small 
chamber and subjected to a sudden rise of temperature. 
In consequence of this there is a preliminary excitatory 
depression, followed, on the attainment of a steady rise, by 
gradually increasing erectile re- 
sponse, which carries the leaf 
above its original level. 

These opposed motile effects 
can be shown, moreover, even 
in the case of ordinary plants. 
We take a_ spiral tendril of 
Passiflora. In this, the outer 
or convex surface is more ex- 
citable than the inner or con- 
cave, and external stimulus, 
causing greater contraction of 
this more excitable outer side, 
induces a movement of un- 
curling. This movement corre- 

» sponds to the excitatory fall 

Fag, St, gpa cf Groth of Mimosa. The response by 
and 35° C. increase of internal energy is, 
The dotted line represents the however, the opposite of this, 


variable period of temperature 


change. Note the contractile and consists of a movement of 
twitch and transient highly ac- : “1. 
celerated growth which follows. curling. When the tendril is 


The rate of growth became con- placed in a vessel of water, of 

stant when the temperature be- bch 4h b 

came permanent at 35° C. which the temperature can be 

varied at will, a sudden rise of 

temperature causes a preliminary excitatory movement of 

uncurling, followed by a movement of curling, when the 
higher temperature has become steady. 

The electrical expressions of external stimulus and in- 
ternal energy are similarly opposed. In fig. 85 (Chapter X.) 
will be seen a record showing that sudden variation of 
temperature, acting as an external stimulus, induced a 


responsive galvanometric negativity, whereas steady rise of 


EXTERNAL STIMULUS AND INTERNAL ENERGY 75 


temperature had the opposite effect—that, namely, of. 
inducing galvanometric positivity. 

It is thus seen that while the characteristic effect of 
external stimulus on an excitable tissue is to cause a nega- 
tive turgidity-variation, that of increased internal energy 
is to induce a positive turgidity-variation. The former of 
these, or negative turgidity-variation, finds electrical expres- 
sion in galvanometric negativity ; in a motile organ by the 
fall of the leaf, and in a growing organ by retardation of the 
rate of growth. The latter, or increase of internal energy, 
on the other hand, is expressed electrically by galvanometric 
positivity ; mechanically, by erection of the leaf; and in a 
growing organ by an acceleration of the normal rate of 
growth. 

We thus arrive at the following laws of response of 
isotropic organs : 

1. Effective stimulation induces contraction and galvano- 
metric negativity. 

_ 2. Increase of internal energy induces expansion and 
galvanometric positivity. 


CHAPTER VII 


ABSORPTION AND EMISSION OF ENERGY IN RESPONSE 


Sign of response determined by latent energy of tissue, and by intensity of 
external stimulus— Sub-tonic, normal and hyper-tonic conditions — The 
critical level—Outward manifestation of response possible only when critical 
level is exceeded— Three typical cases: response greater than stimulus; 
response equal to stimulus ; and response less than stimulus—Investigation by 
growth-response—Instance of sum of work, internal and external, performed 
by stimulus constant—Positive response of tissues characterised by feeble 
protoplasmic activity or sub-tonicity—Enhancement of normal excitability 
of sub-tonic tissue by absorption of stimulus. 


WE shall find, in this and succeeding chapters, that the 
nature and intensity of response are determined not merely 
by the intensity of stimulus, but also by the molecular con- 
dition of the responding substance. The excitatory mani- 
festation is dependent upon the occurrence of a particular 
directioned molecular distortion. Hence, if by the action of 
the stimuli of the environment, an incipient distortion in this 
direction has already been induced in the tissue, the incidence 
of even moderate stimulus will then prove sufficient to 
precipitate visible excitatory manifestation. A tissue in this 
condition is said to be highly excitable or fully tonic. If, on 
the cther hand, the tonic condition be less favourable, long- 
continued stimulation will be necessary to evoke the excita- 
tory effect. Here, during the first part of application, 
stimulus will appear to be ineffective. As a matter of fact, 
however, it is at work to give such a predisposition to the 
molecules that the action of subsequent stimulus shall be 
rendered effective. 

In the simple case in which the tonic condition is favour- 
able, a given stimulus will induce a given responsive expres- 


Py Ee Pe PL Re 


el aT FS 


ee 


ABSORPTION AND EMISSION OF ENERGY IN RESPONSE 77 | 


sion. If further, the tissue, on recovery, return to its original 
condition, then a second similar stimulus will induce the 
same responsive expression as the first. These responses 
will thus be uniform. But if, owing to the after-effect of 
stimulus, the condition of the tissue itself be changed, the 
responses will be found to exhibit either staircase increase or 
fatigue decline, according to the particular molecular con- 
siderations involved, which will be described in detail in the 
following chapter. 

It will be interesting here, however, to take an extreme 
case of a tissue in the opposite condition—namely, of great 
sub-tonicity. It is clear that since the excitability of the 
tissue is here feeble, there will be little or no outward 
manifestation of ercztatory response. The incident stimulus 
will thus be absorbed, and entirely held latent. And the 
increase of internal energy thus brought about may find 
expression mechanically by expansion, or electrically by 
galvanometric positivity. 

In the case which we have selected, the excitability of the 
tissue is too low for the ordinary excitatory expression to occur. 
Hence the incident stimulus will be entirely absorbed, and 
there will be a gain of energy without any loss. The whole 
responsive expression will in this case consist, not of con- 
traction but of expansion. In other words, it will be exactly 
opposite to that which is usually consequent upon external 
stimulus. In order to demonstrate this, I took a seedling of 
Tamarindus indicus which had been cut off from its supply 
of external energy, and was consequently sub-tonic. Owing 
to the insufficiency of internal energy, its growth had, in 
fact, come to a standstill. On now subjecting this seedling 
to thermal stimulation, the absorbed stimulus raised the 
internal energy and found expression in growth expansion. 
We have seen that the effect of external stimulus on a growing 
organ in normal tonic condition was the retardation or arrest 
of growth. But here, in a tissue which is sub-tonic, we find 
that the effect is exactly opposite. : 

Without, however, taking so extreme a case, as that in 


78 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


which sub-tonicity is manifested by arrest of growth, we may 
select a specimen in which, while the tissue is not fully tonic, 
there is still, nevertheless, a feeble rate of growth. In such 
a case we may expect the income from the absorption of 
stimulus to prove greater than the expenditure in the form of 
true excitatory response. Hence, if we subject such a tissue 
to the constant action of an external stimulus, we shall in 
the first stage obtain the predominant effect of the internal 
factor, with its positive turgidity-variation and enhanced rate 
of growth. 

But by the continued action of this accumulating income, 
the tonic condition of the tissue will be raised to the normal, 
with a concomitant increase of excitability. It will now 
therefore be the excitatory component which becomes pre- 
dominant, resulting in the negative turgidity-variation, con- 
traction, and retardation of growth. 

In order to detect these variations of the normal rate of 
growth, under the action of stimulus, it is necessary to have 
at our disposal some very delicate means of record. This 
need I have, however, been able to meet, by devising the 
Balanced Crescograph, more fully described in my book 
on ‘ Plant Response.’ Here, the uniform rate of elongation 
of a growing organ causes a rotation of the recording Optic 
Lever. The spot of light from this lever falls upon a second 
mirror, which is subject to a compensating movement. When 
the balance is exact, the spot of light, reflected from the two 
mirrors, remains quiescent. When, however, the normal rate 
of growth, under the action of any agent, undergoes varia- 
tion, the balance is upset. Thus, when growth is accelerated, 
there is a movement of the recording spot of light in one 
‘direction, say up, and when retarded, a movement in the 
opposite, say down. 

In order to study the effect of external stimulus on a 
tissue in a slightly sub-tonic condition, I took a flower-bud 
of Crinum lily,and first obtained a balanced record, seen as a 
horizontal line (fig. 52). Stimulus of light was now applied, 
and it will be seen that after a short latent period the 


QE 


ABSORPTION AND EMISSION OF ENERGY IN RESPONSE 79 


absorbed stimulus induced a positive turgidity-variation, with 
enhanced rate of growth, as seen by the up-curve. But by 
this absorption of the stimulus itself, the tonic condition 
of the specimen was raised, with consequent increase of ex- 
citability, and the response became normal. That is to say, 
it now consisted of contraction and retardation of growth 
as seen by the downward curve. The external stimulus was 
now cut off and the 

dotted portion of the 
curve shows the after- 
effect. The after-effect 
is thus not a mere re- 
covery, but an enhanced 
rate of growth, due to the 
increased energy which 
remains latent. It was 
only when this was ex- 
hausted that the normal 
rate of growth was re- Fic. 52. Balanced Record of Variation of 
established, as seen in Growth in Flower-bud of Crinum Lily 


eo £ under Diffuse Stimulation of Light 
the horizontal part o the Continuous lines represent the effect during 


curve. And as the tissue application of light, the dotted line on 


: f ‘ withdrawal of light. The plant was 
was now in full tonic originally in a sub-tonic condition, and 


ia 


5 es re 1 { ars. _S 
10’ 15° 20’ 25’ 40° 35’ 40° 45 


condition the renewed application of light at x, after short 
aged ; latent period, induces preliminary ac- 
application of stimulus celeration of growth. After this follows 
. : . the normal retardation. On withdrawal 
of light did : “ gs hers of light, in the dotted portion of the curve 
induce a preliminary en- is seen the after-effect, followed by 


be Sith ty f return to the normal rate of . growth. 
ancement of the rate o A second and long-continued application 


growth, but the normal of light induces retardation, followed by 
A oscillatory response. 

contraction and retarda- 

tion. Its long-continued application gave rise to the further 

phenomenon of multiple response, a subject which will be 

fully dealt with in a future chapter. 

Now owing to this fact that the response of growth gives 
us by means of the enhancement or depression of its rate, 
effects which correspond to the positive and negative, we are 
able clearly to perceive : 


80 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


(1) That when the tonic condition or the excitability of 
the tissue is low, the predominant effect will be the positive. 
This has been shown during the course of the present chapter 
in the case of a very sub-tonic tissue of Zamarindus indicus, 
where the positive or growth-expansion effect was initiated 
by the action of stimulus. 

(2) That when the sub-tonicity of a tissue is not very 
great, incident stimulus will at first give the positive effect of 
an enhanced rate of growth. But with the absorption of the 
stimulus itself, the tonic condition of the tissue will be raised, 
and we shall then obtain the true excitatory reaction of con- 
traction and retardation of growth. Thus, in this intermediate 
case, the positive response will be seen to pass into normal 
negative. , 

(3) And, lastly, that when the tonic condition is already 
high, the excitatory negative response will predominate and 
we shall obtain normal contractile response. Both this and 
the previous intermediate cases are illustrated by the experi- 
ment described on Crinum lily. It was there seen that the 
first effect of incident light was positive, the tissue being 
sub-tonic ; subsequently, the tonic condition being raised, 
this response was converted into the excitatory negative. 
And on renewed application of stimulus thereafter the 
immediate response continued to be negative. 

The fact that by means of growth-response, it is possible 
to obtain indications of the external and internal work per- 
formed by absorbed stimulus, enables us to demonstrate a 
proposition of great importance, that, namely, under certain 
conditions, the sum of the work done, internally and 
externally, by a given stimulus, is constant. This will be the 
case where there is little or no dissipation of energy in the 
course of transformation. In considering the question of the 
relative proportions of the incident stimulus utilised for in- 
ternal and external work respectively, we find it clear, from 
considerations already adduced, that the lower the tonic con- 
dition the greater will be the proportion of stimulus held 
latent for the performance of internal work. The nearer is 


4 
F 
> 


wat “Se = ee 


ie oy - 


414. 4t4 >. a A 


ee ae ee 


ABSORPTION AND EMISSION OF ENERGY IN RESPONSE 81 


the tonic condition, on the other hand, to the critical level, 
the greater will be the excitatory overflow, and the smaller 
the latent component. The internal and external factors 
will thus be complementary to each other. pia 
‘On subjecting this inference to experimental demon- 
stration by means of growth-response, I fully succeeded in 
verifying it. According to this method of growth-response, 
it will be remembered, the true excitatory effect is measured 
by retardation of the normal rate of growth, the internal 
factor of increased latent energy being represented, on the 
other hand, by a corresponding enhancement of the rate of 
growth. This being understood, it was found that in a 
particular specimen of growing tissue, whose tonic condition 
was somewhat low, the external and internal effects caused 
by a given stimulus were in the proportion of 32 to 13'5. 
When the tonic condition of the specimen was raised, how- 
ever, and the same stimulus was applied, the external effect 
was found to be enhanced to 38 at the expense of the internal, 
which was now found to be lowered to 8°5. The sum of the 
work done, both internally and externally, is seen to be in 
both these cases approximately the same, being in the former 
experiment 455, and in the latter 46's. . 
~ We have seen that, of the two antagonistic factors of 
response, the positive will predominate if the excitability 
of the tissue be in any way diminished. Such a loss of 
excitability may occur in either of two ways: (1) by the 
sub-tonicity of the tissue itself; (2) by the depression con- 
sequent on fatigue. Under either of these conditions then 
we may expect to obtain the exhibition of the positive effect. 
The exhibition of the positive effect. under fatigue will be 
described in the course of the next chapter. We shall here 
consider instances in addition to those already given, of the 
occurrence of the positive effect in a tissue which is sub-tonic. 
We have to bear in mind that the work which incident 
stimulus is called upon to perform is two-fold, both internal 
and external, and that there is a certain critical excitatory 
level, above which only is the normal responsive expression 
G : 


So COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


possible. The actual potential or excitatory level of a tissue 
depends on its tonic condition and the intensity of the 
incident stimulus. Now this existing potential of the tissue - 
may be anything within a wide range, S T, when sub-tonic, 
N when normal, or H T when hyper-tonic or above the 
ordinary normal degree (fig. 53). Since 
wb Roes Ph FeRAt ara it is necessary that the incident stimulus 
should cause the critical level C to 
be slightly exceeded, if there is to 
be an excitatory overflow, we can see 
that the intensity of the stimulus re- 
oh. ee. quisite to evoke response will be greater 
eg Poe in proportion as the tonicity of the tissue 
itself is low. Thus when the tissue is 
extremely sub-tonic, a stimulus of or- 
dinary intensity could never avail to 
raise the energy of the system above 
See rad fa se the critical point, and the response 
Tonic Level must then therefore be positive. Under 
N,normal;sT,sub-tonic; these circumstances it will only be by 
HT, hyper-tonic ; and » : ; z 
c, the critical level | the impact of excessively strong sti- 
mulus, or by the cumulative action of 
a series of moderate stimuli, that the critical point can be 
reached and passed, and the normal negative response 
evoked. 

Thus the intensity of the minimally-effective stimulus in 
evoking normal response will afford us a measure of the 
tonicity of the tissue. If the latter be high, then the feeblest 
stimulus will precipitate outward response, and indeed, if 
excessive, response will occur on little or no provocation, and 
such movements we call‘autonomous.’ It must be remem- 
bered, however, that it was by the previous absorption of 
stimuli that the tissue was brought to this point of unstable 
equilibrium at which the added impact of an infinitesimal 
stimulus causes it to bubble over, as it were, into apparently 
spontaneous activity. 

The predominant expression of the highly tonic tissue 


ee ee eT 


ABSORPTION AND EMISSION OF ENERGY IN RESPONSE 83 


being thus negative, we must go to the other extreme of great 
sub-tonicity if we are to be successful in demonstrating the 
occurrence of the unmixed positive response. This considera- 
tion leads us to expect that positive response will be evoked 
on moderate stimulation from tissues that are either not 
highly tonic or protoplasmically defective. I shall show in 
Chapter XXII. that in cells of epidermis, where the proto- 
plasmic contents have been reduced to a minimum, response 
to moderate stimulus tends in general to be positive, Even 
highly excitable tissues like nerve, as will be shown later, 
when cut off from their supply of energy, often become so 
sub-tonic as to give positive response. I shall here show 
how ordinary tissues exhibit this effect, when the tonic con- 
dition is allowed to fall to such an extent as to render the 
tissue extremely sub-tonic. For this purpose I took a cut 
specimen of petiole of cauliflower, and kept it without water 
for a couple of days. By this process the specimen, became 
somewhat withered. I next proceeded to take records of its 
electrical responses under increasing stimuli. The intensity 
of these stimuli rose from I to 10 units. It will be seen 
from the record (fig. 54) that each stimulus up to 9 evoked 
positive response, and that it was the strong stimulus of | 
10 which gave rise to the normal response of negativity. 
This constitutes the first instance of a phenomenon which 
I shall show later to be of very extended occurrence—the 
induction, namely, of one effect under moderate, and its 
opposite under very feeble stimulation. It is not so easy to 
demonstrate this fact with a highly excitable, as with a some- 
what sub-tonic tissue, where the critical intensity of stimulus 
for the evoking of normal response need not be impracticably 
low. A point to be taken into account here is the after-effect 
of sub-minimal stimulus in enhancing subsequent normal 
excitability. Thus it is found in taking the record of 
responses to a succession of feeble stimuli, that though they 
are at first abnormal positive, they are afterwards converted 
into ‘normal negative. That it is the after-effect of the. 
previous stimulation which thus enhances previous excitability. 
G2 


84 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


may also be demonstrated by subjecting the tissue to con- 
tinuous stimulation or tetanisation, when the abnormal 
positive is found to pass into normal negative. 

From the experiments that have been described, it would 
appear that the several kinds of response characteristic of 
various tissues are relatively rather than absolutely different. 
The true excitatory reaction of an excitable tissue, is one 
of galvanometric negativity. Any diminution of the ex-— 
citability—whether by lowering of tonic condition or other 


Fic. 54. Photographic Record of Abnormal Positive passing into Normal Nega- 
tive Response in a Withered Specimen of Leaf-stalk of Cauliflower 


Stimulus was gradually increased from I to 10, by means of spring-tapper. 
When the stimulus intensity was 10, the response became reversed into 
normal negative. (Parts of 8 and 9 are out of the plate.) This record 
is to be read from right to left. _Down-records stand for positive, and 
up-curves for negative responses. 


causes—will bring about a decrease of this negativity, which 
may culminate in actual positivity. Thus negative is not 
separated from positive response by any break of continuity ; 
but we are able, on the contrary, to trace a gradual transition 
from one to the other. Moreover, in every response we 
have the two antagonistic elements, positive and negative, 
either actually or potentially present. The form taken by 
the resultant response is entirely determined by the question 
of what proportion of the stimulus impinging upon the tissue 
becomes latent ; and this in its turn depends upon the tonic 


See? poe, 


ABSORPTION AND EMISSION OF ENERGY IN RESPONSE 85 


condition of the tissue. When the absorbed stimulus is 
wholly retained, response is positive, but by this absorption 
the tonicity of the tissue and its excitability are both raised. 
When the tonic condition of the tissue, on the other hand, is 
already high, and its excitability great, a large proportion of 
the energy finds outward expression, and we obtain the 
normal negative response. Between these two extremes, we 
may observe many effects of interference, due to the play 
of the two antagonistic elements. If, then, the time-relations 
be not coincident, variations will be induced which will find 
expression in different types, diphasic response, positive 
followed by negative, and vzce versa. 

The question considered in the course of the present 
chapter has been that of the energy received and given out 
by the tissue, and the molecular work, positive and negative, 
performed during these processes. Such work, however, is 
itself the result of molecular distortions brought about by 
stimulus, and the question of the amplitude of response, as 
related to the degree of distortion, will be discussed in the 
following chapter. 


CHAPTER VIII 


VARIOUS TYPES OF RESPONSE 


Chemical theory of response—Insufficiency of the theory of assimilation and dis- | 


similation—Similar responsive effects seen in inorganic matter—Modifying in- 
fluence of molecular condition on response— Five molecular stages, A, B, C, D, E 
—Staircase effect, uniform response, fatigue—No sharp line of demarcation 
between physical and chemical phenomena—Volta-chemical effect and by- 
products—Phasic alternation—Alternating fatigue—Rapid fatigue under con- 
tinuous stimulation—In sub-tonic tissue summated effect of latent components 
raises tonicity and excitability—Response not always disproportionately greater 
than stimulus—Instances of stimulus partially held latent : staircase and ad- 

 ditive effects, multiple response, renewed growth— Bifurcated responsive ex- 
pression. 


ACCORDING to current theories, living matter is maintained 
in a state of equilibrium by the two opposed chemical pro- 
cesses of assimilation and dissimilation. It is supposed that 
stimulus causes a down or dissimilatory change, which is 
again compensated during recovery by the building-up or 
assimilative change. In the case of uniform responses, again, 
these two processes are regarded as balancing each other. 
On this theory, when the down change is the greater of the 
two, the potential energy of the system falls below par ; for 
the building-up process cannot then sufficiently repair the 
chemical depreciation caused by it. Hence occurs dimi- 
nution of response, or fatigue, which is supposed to be further 
accentuated by the accumulation of deleterious fatigue-stuffs. 
The disappearance of fatigue after a period of rest is ex- 
plained by the renovating action of the blood-supply, which 
is also regarded as the means of carrying away the fatigue- 
stuffs. 

A serious objection to these explanations lies, however, 
in the fact, that even excised and bloodless muscles exhibit 
recovery from fatigue after a period of rest. In isolated 


Se ee a a a el te te 


ee ee ee ee 


VARIOUS TYPES OF RESPONSE 87 


vegetable tissues, again, where there is no active circulation 
of renovating material, the same effect, and its removal after 
a period of rest, are observed. Thus the difficulties en- 
countered in explaining fatigue, on purely chemical ‘con- 
siderations, are great enough; but still greater are those 
difficulties which arise when we come to deal with the stair- 
case effect—typically shown in cardiac muscle—in which 
successive responses to uniform stimuli exhibit a gradual 
enhancement of amplitude. The results obtained here are 
in direct opposition to the theory described; for in this 
particular case we have to assume that the same stimulus 
which is usually supposed to cause a chemical breakdown, 
has become efficient to induce. an effect exactly the reverse. 

Of the two antagonistic elements in the electrical response, 
moreover, it is the positive which is supposed to be associated 
with the assimilative, and the negative with the dissimilative 
change. If this supposition were correct, however, it would 
be natural to expect that the positive response would be 
manifested predominantly in vigorously growing tissues, in 
which assimilation must be at its greatest. Fatigued tissues 
on the other hand, in which dissimilatory changes are sup-— 
posed to be predominant, should manifest negativity as their 
characteristic response ; moribund tissues, in contrast with the 
actively growing, might also be expected to exhibit respon- 
sive negativity. In actual fact, however, the very reverse is 
the case. For in vigorous tissues, normal response is by 
galvanometric negativity ; and it is the over-fatigued or 
- moribund which characteristically exhibit the positive re- 
sponse. 

It would be difficult again to conceive of assimilation 
and dissimilation in the case of inorganic matter. Yet even 
in inorganic matter we find reproduced all the various types 
met with in the response of living tissues: namely, uniform 
response, the staircase effect, and fatigue. Response being 
really due to molecular upset from a condition of equilibrium, 
we can see how different forms of responsive expression will 
occur, according to the various molecular conditions of the 


88 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


substance at the time being. One of the most important 
factors, then, in determining the character of response is the 
molecular condition of the substance itself. The numerous 
anomalies hitherto encountered in our interpretation of 
responsive phenomena are all traceable to our failure to take 
this factor of molecular condition into account. For a full 
exposition of the modifying influence which it exercises on 
response, however, though I shall here state some of the 
principal conclusions which I have arrived at, the reader is — 
referred to Chapter XLII. 

From the fact, that every type of response is to be 
obtained from inorganic matter, where chemical assimilation 
and dissimilation are obviously out of the question, it is clear 
that the fundamental phenomenon must be dependent on 
physical or molecular, and not on such hypothetical chemical 
changes. It must, however, be remembered that though re- 
sponse phenomena and their modifications are undoubtedly 
in the first place physical or molecular, yet in the borderland 
between physics and chemistry there is no sharp line of 
demarcation. For example, yellow phosphorus becomes 
converted, under the stimulus of light, into the red, or 
allotropic, variety. This molecular change, however,cis also 
attended by a concomitant change in the chemical activity, 
phosphorus in its allotropic condition being less active than 
in the yellow. Under certain circumstances, further, it is 
possible to have a secondary series of chemical events follow- 
ing upon a condition of unequal molecular strain. A homo- 
geneous living tissue, when unstimulated, is iso-electric. 
When stimulated, however, an electro-motive difference is 
induced, as between the stimulated and unstimulated parts of 
the tissue. . The result is an electrical current attended 
by electro-chemical changes. As a consequence of such 
volta-chemical action, when prolonged, by-products (fatigue 
stuffs?) may be accumulated, and these may have a de- 
pressing effect on the activity of the tissue. Hence, just 
as, after very prolonged activity of a voltaic combination, it 
is necessary to renew the active element and change the 


VARIOUS TYPES OF RESPONSE 89 


electrolyte, surcharged with by-products, so after sustained 
activity of a living tissue, the process of renewal, or renova- 
tion, will be necessary. It is thus seen how upon the funda- 
mental molecular derangement, a chain of very various 
chemical events may follow, as its after-effect. And it is 
only by going in this way to the very root of the pheno- 
menon that we can avoid the many contradictions with 
which we are confronted by the chemical theory. 

In studying various response phenomena, our conclusions 
are necessarily based upon the observation of the amplitude 
of responses. It is therefore important at this point to draw 
attention to the danger of hasty inferences. On finding, for 
instance, that the amplitude of response in a given case is 
diminished, we are apt to infer that the responding tissue 
has undergone depreciation. But this is not invariably the 
case. In the entire process of response, while stimulus 
induces molecular upset, we must remember that there is 
also an internal factor, which brings about molecular restitu- 
tion. Now, if this force of restitution be inany way enhanced, 
it is easy to see that the responsive distortion of the mole- 
cules will find itself opposed, with consequent diminution of 
amplitude. We shall thus often find that a rise of tem- 
perature, by enhancing the force of recovery, actually causes 
a diminution of response. That this is not due, however, to 
any depreciation of the tissue is seen from the fact that the 
same rise of temperature enhances another excitatory pro- 
perty of the tissue—namely, the speed of its conduction. 

I shall now give a brief account of the modifying influence 
exercised on response by the molecular condition. It will be 
shown, in the Chapter (XLII) on the Modification of Response 
under Cyclic Molecular Variation, that a given response is 
not determined merely by the nature of the responding 
substance, but also by the amount of the energy which it 
possesses. Starting from the lowest condition of sub-tonicity, 
a substance undergoes progressive molecular transformation 
by the action of the impinging stimulus itself. Five stages 
may be roughly distinguished in this transformation. In the 


gO COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


first, or A, stage of extreme sub-tonicity, we have absorption 
without excitatory response. By this absorption the sub- 
stance passes into the next, or B, stage, which is the stage of 
transition, where response is converted from the abnormal to 
normal. Above this stage the rate of molecular transforma- 
tion is very rapid. From the residual after-effect of stimulus, 
the substance now passes from the stage B to the stage 0, 
which is a condition of more or less stability. Further | 
stimulation carries the substance to stages D and E. Here 
the molecular distortion from the normal equilibrium is very 
great. Stimulation applied in this condition has little 
further effect in inducing response. That is to say, excit- 
ability is here reduced to a minimum. In this extremely 
distorted position, moreover, the substance has a strong 
tendency to revert to the position of normal equilibrium. 

In the A condition of extreme sub-tonicity, since there 
‘is absorption without excitation, the response which we 
obtain is abnormal positive. Intense or long-continued 
stimulation carries the substance into the B stage, with its 
normal negative response often preceded by diphasic. An 
example of this has already been given in fig. 54, obtained 
from the sub-tonic petiole of cauliflower. We shall meet, 
however, with numerous other examples in a great variety 
of tissues. Arriving at the B stage, the substance is still 
somewhat sub-tonic, and the rate of molecular transformation 
here is rapid. From the after-effect of stimulus the mole- 
cules of the somewhat inert substance become incipiently 
distorted in the same direction as that of normal response. 
A proportion of the incident stimulus is thus utilised in 
inducing a favourable molecular disposition. A repetition of 
the original stimulus will now give rise to a greater excitatory 
reaction than before. Thus at the B stage we obtain a stair- 
case increase of response. This fact—that by the after-effect 
of previous stimulation the molecules may be incipiently dis- 
torted in a direction favourable to excitatory response—finds 
‘illustration in stimuli individually ineffective being made 
effective by repetition, The result here is evidently made 


VARIOUS TYPES OF RESPONSE QI 


conspicuous by the summation of the after-effects of all the 
preceding stimuli with the direct effects of their successors. 
The staircase effect is seen in the two accompanying records. 
In fig. 55 is given a photographic record of the staircase 
increase.in the electrical response of a vegetable nerve! in 
somewhat sub-tonic condition, In fig. 56 we have a second 
example of this effect, seen in the electrical response of the 
petiole of Sryophyllum, rendered artificially sub-tonic by 
cooling. 

We next arrive at the C stage, which is, as has been said, 
one of more or less stability. Expenditure is here, for a 
certain length of time, balanced by 
income. The molecular condition 
of the tissue being thus constant, 
the responses are uniform. I give 
below records of such uniform re- 
sponses to uniform stimuli, ex- 


Fig. 56. Staircase Increase 
in Electrical Response of 


Fic. 55. Photographic Re- Petiole of Bryophyllum, 
cord of Staircase Response rendered sluggish by 
in Vegetable Nerve cooling 


hibited by different tissues. In fig. 57 are seen uniform 
electrical responses to uniform mechanical stimuli, given 
by the root of radish. Fig. 58 shows uniform electrical 
responses to uniform thermal stimuli, given by the petiole 
of fern. 

The C€ is succeeded by stages D and E, representing 
a condition of over-strain. In fig. 59, a, are shown uni- 
form responses to uniform stimuli, applied at intervals 


" An account of the discovery of certain vegetable tissues, with the function of 
nerves, will be found in Chapter XXXII, 


92 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


of one minute. An inspection of the record shows that 
there is in such cases a complete recovery, at the end of 
which the molecular condition is the same as before stimula- 
tion. Hence, successive responses are exactly similar to each 
other. The stimulation-rhythm was now changed, to intervals 


Fic. 57. Photographic Record of Uniform Responses (Radish) 


of half a minute instead of one, while the stimuli were main- 
tained at the same intensity as before. It will be noticed 
(fig. 59, &) that these responses are now of much smaller 


Fic. 58. Photographic Record of Uniform Response in Petiole of Fern 


amplitude, in spite of the equality of stimulus. An inspec- 
tion of the figure also shows that, when greater frequency of 
stimulation was introduced, the tissue had not had time to 
effect complete recovery from previous strain. The mole- 
cular swing towards equilibrium had not yet abated, when 


VARIOUS TYPES OF RESPONSE 93 


the new stimulus with its opposing impulse was received. 
There is thus a diminution of height in the resultant 


(a) (4) (<) 
Fic. 59. Record showing Diminution of 
Response, when sufficient Time is not 


allowed for Full Recovery : : 
ake ed : ; Fic. 60. Fatigue in 
In (a) stimuli were applied at intervals of one Celery 


minute ; in (4) the intervals were reduced ; - : 
to half a minute ; this caused a diminution Vibration of 30° at in- 
of response. Jn (c) the original rhythm: is tervals of half a minute. 
restored, and the response is found to be 

enhanced (Radish). 


response. The original rhythm of one minute was now 
restored, and the succeeding records (fig. 59, c) at once show 
increased response. 

Residual strain is thus seen to 
be one of the principal reasons of 
reduced response or fatigue. This 
is also shown in a record which 
I have obtained with a petiole of 
celery (fig. 60). It will be noticed 
there that, owing to imperfect 
molecular recovery, during the 


Fic. 61. Fatigue in Leaf-stalk of 


time allowed for rest, the heights Cauliflower 

of succeeding responses undergo Stimulus : 30° vibration at interval 
; rier . ‘ f one minute. 

acontinuous diminution. Fig. 61 


gives a photographic record of 
fatigue in the petiole of cauliflower, and fig. 62 of fatigue in 
inorganic response. ! 

It is evident that residual strain, other things being equal, 
will be greater if the stimuli have been excessive. This is 


94 


COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


seen in fig. 63, where the first set of these responses, A, is for 
an intensity of mechanical stimulation of 45° vibration, and 
the second set, B, of augmented amplitude, for an intensity 
of go° vibration. 


Fic. 62. Photographic Record showing 
Fatigue in Tin Wire which had been 
stimulated for 


continuously 
Days 


On reverting, in C, to the first stimulus- 


intensity of 45°, the re- 
sponses are seen to undergo 
a great diminution, as com- 
pared with the first set, A. 
This change is due to the 
over-strain of the previous 
excessive stimulation. But 
we should expect that the 
effect of such over-strain » 
would disappear with time, 
and the responses regain 
their former height, after a 
period of rest. In order to 
verify this, therefore, I re- 
newed stimulation (at the 
intensity of 45°) fifteen 


‘minutes after c. It will be seen from the record D how far 
fatigue had been removed in this interval. 


45 


Os 


a 


Fic. 63. 


90 


B 


Effect of Over-strain in producing Fatigue 


45 45 


C , D 


Successive stimuli applied at intervals of one minute. The intensity of 
stimulus in C is the same as that of A, but response is feebler owing to 


previous over-stimulation. 


Fatigue is to a great extent removed after 


fifteen minutes’ rest, and the responses in D are stronger than those in 
c. The vertical line between arrows represents ‘05 volt. (Turnip 


leaf-stalk. ) 


One peculiarity that will be noticed in these curves is 
that, owing to the presence of comparatively little strain, the 
first response of each set is relatively large. The succeeding 


VARIOUS TYPES OF RESPONSE 95 


responses are approximately equal, where the residual strains 
are similar. The first response in fig. 63, A, shows this, because 
there had been long previous rest. The first of B shows it, 
because we are there passing for the first time to an increased 
intensity of stimulus. The first of C does not show it, because 
of the strong residual strain from the preceding excessive 
stimulation. And the first of D, again, does show it, because 
the strain has now been removed, by the interval of. fifteen 
minutes’ rest. 

Of the antagonistic elements of positivity and negativity 
which are present in response, we have seen that the positive 
becomes predominant when the excitability of the tissue is 
in any way depressed. And since a tissue under fatigue has 
its excitability lowered, it follows that in this condition it 
may be expected to exhibit a tendency towards positive 
response : that is to say, expansion in the case of mechanical. 
and galvanometric positivity in the case of electrical, response. 
Thus, when a tissue is subjected to continuous stimulation, 
the first effect will be the maximum negative response, 
contraction and galvanometric negativity. But on the setting- 
in of fatigue, the positive effect will predominate, inducing 
a fatigue-reversal of the response. In cases where such 
fatigue is very great, as, for instance, in certain muscles, 
the top of the tetanic curve undergoes rapid decline (fig. 64, a). 
The normal contraction now exhibits a reversal, or relaxation. 
In the sensitive plant, 1/zmosa, similarly, continuous stimula- 
tion by electrical shocks gives rise to results which are essen- 
tially the same. It will be noticed that after the responsive 
fall of the leaf it returns to its former erect position, in spite 
of the fact that stimulus is still being continued. Here also, 
as in the corresponding case of muscle, we have the usual 
sequence, of (1) normal contraction and (2) fatigue relaxation 
(fig. 65). 

In electrical response, also, under continuous stimulation, 
the normal galvanometric negativity, owing to the increasing 
positive effect, undergoes decline or abolition. © This is seen 
in fig. 64, 6, which exhibits the decline of electrical response 


96 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


under continuous stimulation in the petiole of celery. The 
fatigue in the mechanical response of muscle under similar 
conditions is given in @ for the purpose of comparison. The 
effect of rest in inducing molecular recovery, and hence in 
the removal of fatigue, is illustrated in the following set of 
photographic records (fig. 66). The first of these shows the 
curve of electrical response, obtained with a fresh plant. It 
will be seen that under a continuous stimulation of two minutes 
the response first attains 
a large amplitude, after 
which it declines, in a 
fatigue-reversal. Another 
two minutes were now 


(a) 


(6) 


Fic. 65. Photographic Records of 
Normal Mechanical Response of 
Mimosa to Single Stimulus: (upper 

’ ‘ figure), and to Continuous Stimu- 

Fic. 64. Rapid Fatigue under Con- gt s 
tinuous Simutation in (az) Muscle ; lation (lower, agure) ‘ } 
(6) Leaf-stalk of Celery (Electrical In the latter case the leaf is erected in 
Response) spite of continuous stimulation. 


allowed for recovery, and we observe that a partial recovery 
takes place. Stimulation was now repeated throughout 
the succeeding two.minutes, to be followed once more by 
two minutes’ rest. The response in this case is seen to be 
decidedly smaller than at first. The same effects are seen 
in the third response. A period of rest of five minutes was 
next given, and the curve subsequently obtained: under the 


VARIOUS TYPES OF RESPONSE 97 


same two minutes’ stimulation as before shows greater 
response than the preceding, owing to the partial removal 
of residual strain. | 

There is one aspect of the subject of fatigue-reversal 
which now demands our attention. Wehave seen that under 
continuous stimulation, a maximum contraction is induced, 
which is attended by the depression of the leaf of A/zmosa. 


Fic. 66. Effect of Continuous Vibration (through 50°) in Carrot 


In the first three records, two minutes’ stimulation is followed by two 
minutes’ recovery. The last record was taken after the specimen had 
a rest of five minutes. The response, owing to removal of fatigue by 
rest, is stronger. 


This is followed, however, by a reversal—namely, expansion, 
with re-erection of the leaf. According to the chemical 
theory of assimilation and dissimilation, the fatigue-effect 
is assumed to be due to an explosive dissimilatory change, 
with consequent run-down of energy. Inthe case of Wzmosa, 
however, it is difficult to understand how, by a mere run- 
down of energy and consequent passivity of the tissue, an 
active movement of erection—involving the performance of 
work in lifting the weight of the leaf—could be brought 


H 


98 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


about. Now we have seen that the diminution of normal 
response may be brought about by the augmentation of the 
internal factor, tending to enhance the force of restitution, 
and the necessary augmentation of 
the internal factor may be the result 
of an increase of internal energy. 
Thus while the plant is the recipient 
of a continuous income, its responsive 
expression is alternately one of emis- — 
sion and absorption of energv. Thus 
negative and positive succeed each 
Fic. 67. Oscillatory Re- other or vice versa. Such a phasic 

sponse of Arsenic acted ; : pe 

-on Continuously by alternation is widely present, as we 

Hertzian Radiation - 

shall see, in the response, not only 

Taken by method of con- * oe ‘ 

‘ ductivity variation. of various living tissues, but also of 

inorganic substances. The follow- 

ing record (fig. 67) exhibits this oscillatory response in arsenic 
under the continuous stimulation of electric radiation. In 


the case of Wzmosa, under continuous stimulation, we obtain 


Fic. 68. Alternate Fatigue (2) in Electrical Responses of Petiole of 
Cauliflower; (4) in Multiple Electric Responses of Peduncle of 
Biophytum ; (c) in Multiple Mechanical Responses of Leaflet of Bio- 
phytum ; and (ad) in Autonomous Responses of Desmodium 


a single alternation, and a certain period must then elapse, 
before the response can be repeated. In other cases, how- 
ever, continuous stimulation may give rise to two, or three, or 
a large number of similar alternations. 


VARIOUS TYPES OF RESPONSE 99 


In connection with this subject of phasic alternation I 
may describe a certain curious phenomenon, which I have 
often noticed ; I refer to the periodic waxing and waning of 
both mechanical and electrical responses. The simplest 
example of this will be a case in which the responses are 
alternately large and small. But others are to be found in 
which the groupings are more complex. In fig. 68a@ is seen 
such a simple alternation, in the electrical response of the 
petiole of cauliflower, under successive uniform stimuli. In 
6, c, and d@ are shown similar alter- 
nations in multiple and autonomous 
responses. I give alsoa photographic 
record (fig. 69) of a similar alterna- 
tion in the automatic pulsations of 
the leaflet of Desmodium gyrans. 


IANA ATAVATANLE 


Fic. 69. Photographic 
Record of Periodic 


Fatigue in the Auto- Fic. 70. Periodic Fatigue in 
matic Pulsation of Des- Pulsation of Frog’s Heart 
modium gyrans (Pembrey and Phillips) 


Similar alternations are sometimes observed in the beating 
of frog’s heart (fig. 70). | 

In the following record of mechanical response (fig. 71), 
taken from the style of Datura alba, we find that fatigue, as 
already understood, would not explain the phenomenon 
observed. For here, under the continuous action of stimulus, 
without any intervening period of rest for the so-called 
‘assimilatory’ recuperation, we see that a second response 
occurs. I shall later give other instances in which pulsating 
responses, with their alternating negative and positive phases, 
are given, under the action of continuous stimulation. We 
pass here imperceptibly from the ordinary phenomenon of 

H2 


TOO COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


individual response to individual stimulus, into that of 
multiple response, either to continuous, or to a single strong 
stimulation. The excess of energy derived from impinging 
stimulus is in the latter case held latent in the tissue to find 
subsequent expression in phasic alternations of negative and 
positive variations in series (cf. Chapter XVII). 

There can be no doubt that these effects of periodic 
alternation of phase are due to two antagonistic reactions, 
becoming effectively predominant by turns. Thus the con-— 
tinuous impact of stimulus on a tissue may first give rise to 
the negative phase of response. But by the continuous 
absorption of incident stimulus, the internal energy is in- 
creased, with its. opposite 
reaction of _ positivity. 
Hence, the negativity will 
be gradually diminished, 
and the _ positive phase 
become predominant. The 
existence of these’ two 
antagonistic factors will be 
understood, from an inspec- 
“Fre. 71. Photographic Record of tion of the top of a tetanic 

Periodic Fatigue under Continuous curve. Here, the more or 

Stimulation in Contractile Response ‘ , 

(Filament of Uriclis Lily) less horizontal line repre- 

| sents a state of balance 
between the two opposite forces of excitatory response by 
contraction, with galvanometric negativity and recovery or 
expansion, with galvanometric positivity. When this state 
of balance is disturbed, by a sudden cessation of the 
hitherto continuously acting stimulus, a brief overshooting 
of the response in the negative direction is sometimes seen, 
followed by recovery. We shall meet with examples of 
this in, among others, the responses of retina and certain 
vegetable tissues under light. Such facts it has been 
suggested afford a demonstration of the two antagonistic 
processes of assimilation and dissimilation, characteristic of 
living tissues. But that they are really to be accounted for 


VARIOUS TYPES OF RESPONSE IO! 


from molecular considerations will be seen from the fact 
that effects exactly similar are met with in the response of 
inorganic matter (cf. figs. 258 and 383.) 

In the case of responses exhibiting fatigue from over- 
strain, we have a diminution of normal response, which may 
ultimately culminate in reversal. We may imagine a spiral 
spring, undergoing increasing compression from a gradually 
augmenting force. The responsive compression will at first 
be considerable. But this will soon reach a limit, beyond 
which added force will seem to have but little power to induce 
further responsive distortion. In a somewhat similar way, 
we may visualise the condition of the responding molecule at 
the stage D or E. Here, molecular distortion has almost 
reached its limit. It follows that added stimulus can induce 


Fic. 72. Fatigue in the Contractile Response of India-rubber 
Note the periodic alternation and the reversal at the end. 


little further distortion. But the maximally distorted mole- 
cule has now a great tendency to revert to the position of 
equilibrium, and the shock of stimulus, instead of inducing 
excitatory action, induces the reverse. That this is to be ex- 
plained by molecular rather than chemical considerations, 
is seen in the following record (fig. 72) of the contractile 
response of india-rubber to thermal stimulation. This 
represents the last part of a long series of responses, whose 
amplitude was already undergoing a progressive decline. 
Further symptoms of growing fatigue are seen in the periodic 
alternations of amplitude, and in the final reversal of response 
to one of expansion. I shall later give another record in 
which the normal negative response is seen reversed to 
positive through an intermediate diphasic. 


102 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


The fact that the normal response of living tissues may be 
reversed under fatigue, I am here able to show by an 
experiment of an unexpected character. It is usually 
supposed that fatigue is typical of such tissues as muscle, 
and absent from nerve. But I shall show with regard to all 
the various types of response, that there is none of these 
which is distinctive of any one tissue. The difference is one 
of degree and not of kind. The same intensity and duration 
of stimulus which is efficient to cause 
fatigue in muscle will not be enough to 
do so in the case of nerve. But even 
nerve will display fatigue when ex- 
cessively stimulated. In the record given 
in fig. 73, a particular nerve of frog 
had been previously fatigued, by over- 
stimulation, and on now taking individual 
responses to individual stimuli, it was 
found that they had become reversed to 
positive. 

Thus a particular type of response is 
the result of a particular condition of 
the responding substance, and there is 
none which is exclusively characteristic 

. ' of any one tissue. Were it otherwise, 
Fic. 73. Reversed Re- ordinary muscle, in which the explosive 
sponse of Fatigued ; 

Neve molecular change is supposed to be so 

predominant, should typically show only 
the fatigue, and never the staircase effect. But the following 
record (fig. 74) shows that this is not the case. For at first 
it exhibits a characteristic staircase effect ; the responses are 
then for a time uniform; and lastly, we see fatigue, in a 
manner exactly corresponding to the theoretical considera- 
tions which we have anticipated in stages B,C, and D. The 
staircase response is thus not peculiar to cardiac muscle, but 
is to be seen, under appropriate conditions, in skeletal 
muscle, in nerve, and even in inorganic substances. In 
fig. 75 is given a series of responses of Galena to Hertzian 


VARIOUS TYPES OF RESPONSE 103 


radiation, which in its various phases of staircase, uniform 
and fatigue-decline, is parallel to that just seen in muscle. 
The phasic change, due to molecular transformation, which 
I have already pointed out under continuous stimulation, is 
seen in both these records in the shifting of the base-line. In 
fig. 64. under continuous stimulation, we see the mechanical 
response of muscle passing from a condition of growing 
contraction into one ofrelaxation. In the record of individual 
responses given in fig. 74, the same is seen to take place: 
A similar phenomenon is ob- 
served in the mechanical re- 
sponse of Mimosa (fig. 65). 
When the mode of record, 
however, is electro-motive, in- 


Fic. 75. Preliminary Staircase, In- 
crease, followed by Fatigue, in the 


Fic. 74. Preliminary Staircase, Response of Galena to Hertzian 
followed by Fatigue, in the Radiation 
Responses of Muscle (Brodie) (Resistivity variation method) 


stead of mechanical, the increasing galvanometric negativity 
which corresponds to increasing contraction, is found gradually 
to give place to positivity (fig. 64 4). | And finally, when the 
mode of record is by resistivity-variation, we find, by the 
shifting of the-base line in fig. 75, that the residual negative 
variation of resistance at first waxes and then wanes. 
Instances have been given, in which a portion of the in- 
cident stimulus has been seen to be held latent to do internal 
work. And from this it is clear that the current assumption that 
response must always be larger than stimulus is quite un- 


104 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


tenable. There are cases, again, in which a large portion of the 
incident stimulus is held latent for a time, to find subsequent 
manifestation externally. This I have been able to demon- 
strate by the discovery of multiple response in plants. Thus 
while a single moderate stimulus in such cases evokes a 
single response, a single strong stimulus is found to give 
repeated or multiple responses. This I have shown, not 
only in mechanical, but also 
in electrical response, and the 
latter subject will be taken 
up in detail in a subsequent 
chapter. 

And, lastly, it follows 
from what has been. said, 
that incident stimulus need 
not always cause depreciation 
of the energy of the tissue, 
but that, on the contrary, 
it may actually raise it above 
par. I shall now describe an 

example in which incident 
-Fic. 76.- Photographic Record of . 
' Responses of Style of Datura alba stimulus was seen to find 


in which Growth had come toa bifurcated expression. In 
Temporary Stop f 63 : h 

The up curve shows contraction. As 8 ps fey ees ee rien 
long as the base-line is horizontal, graphic record of contractile 
growth is seen to be at standstill. ; ; ‘ ty] £ 
Renewal of growth at sixth re- responses in the style o 
sponse, after which growth-elon- Datura alba, in which growth 
gation is shown by the trend of : 5 
the base-line downwards. had previously been in a state 


of standstill. The first five 
responses of this series are seen to be uniform. A portion 
of the stimulus applied must, however, from the first 
have been absorbed and held latent in the organ, thus 
increasing that internal energy, or tonic condition, on 
which growth depends. For at the sixth response we 
find that growth recommences, and the stimulus now 
finds bifurcated expression, in maintaining response and in 
renewing growth, as seen in the trend downwards of the 


oe ty ee ee ee ‘a di tale 


_ 


VARIOUS TYPES OF RESPONSE TO5 


hitherto horizontal base-line. This bifurcation causes the 
first contractile response of the now growing organ—sixth of 


the series—to be smaller than usual. But, as a favourable 


tonic condition is gradually established by the absorption of 
energy and the molecular mobility of the responding organ 
is increased, the contractile response becomes larger, and 
growth goes on at a certain steady rate. This constitutes 
an instance in which stimulus, so far from lowering the 
energy of the responding system, has actually raised it 
above par. 

It would thus appear that while the theory of assimilation 
and dissimilation is insufficient for the explanation of the 
various characteristics of response, the difficulties there en- 
countered are, on the contrary, satisfactorily explained, on 
taking full account of the influence on response of the 
molecular condition of the responding substance. From 
the chemical hypothesis of an explosive molecular change, 
with its attendant dissimilation and run-down of energy, it 
would follow that previous stimulation should always induce 
a depression of the subsequent responses. Instead of this, 
however, it is found that previous stimulation sometimes 
exalts, and at other times depresses, the subsequent re- 
sponses. This apparent anomaly we have seen to be ex- 
plained by the consideration of molecular transformation. 
From the sluggish condition A, we have seen tissues trans- 
formed, by the impact of moderate stimulus, to condition B, 
with its greater excitability. It is only when the molecular 
condition has been brought to D or E, that the responses 
undergo a diminution or reversal. 

The molecular condition, then, undergoes a continuous 
transformation, in consequence of the action of stimulus, from 
the extreme of sub-tonicity A to the overstrained molecular 
conditions D and E. In the A stage, there is no true ex- 
citatory expression, response to stimulus being here by the 
abnormal positive variation. The substance is next trans- 
formed into stage B, where response exhibits a staircase 
character. In the next stage C, the responses are uniform. 


106 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Under over-stimulation, the stages D and E are reached, 
characterised by diminished amplitude of response, or actual 
reversal into positive. There are thus two conditions under 
which we obtain abnormal positive responses. One of these 
is that of sub-tonicity, and the other, the reversal due to 
fatigue. 

There is, again, no tissue which is exclusively characterised 


by any specific type of response. All these—staircase, 


uniform, and fatigue—will occur in muscle, nerve, plant, 
and even inorganic matter, under certain definite and ap- 
propriate conditions. In a future chapter, we shall study in 
detail the characteristic molecular curve, from which light 
will be thrown on the internal molecular condition of the 
tissue, and the influence of that condition on response. 


Ne fi teat tn Oo el 


os 


CHAPTER IX 


DETECTION OF PHYSIOLOGICAL ANISOTROPY BY ELECTRIC 
RESPONSE 


Anomalies in mechanical and electrical response—Resultant response determined 
by differential excitability—Responsive current from the more to the less 
excitable—Laws of response in anisotropic organ—Demonstration by means 
of mechanical stimulation—Vibrational stimulus—Stimulation by pressure— 
Quantitative stimulation by thermal shocks. 


IT has been customary, as we know, to ascribe the varied 
movements of plant-organs under external stimulus, to the 
presence of different specific sensibilities ; and, indeed, it 
would seem at first sight impossible to reduce such highly 
complex and apparently unrelated phenomena, to the terms 
of a single fundamental reaction, common to all alike. There 
is no denying, for instance, that certain plant-organs, when 
acted on by light, bend towards it, and others away. I have 
elsewhere shown,' however, that all these diverse movements 
are clearly traceable to one fundamental excitatory reaction, 
and that the different effects observed are due merely to the 
differential excitabilities of various parts of the structure ; 
and that the resultant movement is in all cases brought 
about by the greater contraction of the more excited side. 
Passing next to the electrical response of living tissues, 
animal and vegetable, we encounter many anomalies. Not 


only will one tissue give positive, and another negative 


response, but we find also that the same tissue will give 

sometimes one and sometimes the other. ‘These apparent 

inconsistencies are often due, as we shall find, to the dfferentzal 

excitability of anisotropic structures—a factor in the problem 

which has not hitherto been recognised. An investigation on 
1 Bose, Plant Response. 


108 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


this subject, then, demands that we first discover some means 
of determining the relative excitabilities of different parts of 
a tissue. 

As the simplest example of an anisotropic structure, we 
may take a compound strip of ebonite and stretched india- 
rubber, glued firmly together throughout their length. Of 
these, the india-rubber is the more contractile, and when the 
strip as a whole is subjected to periodic thermal stimulation, 
response takes place by the greater contraction induced in > 
the india-rubber. Ifthe strip be held, with the india-rubber 
below, response will be by the induced concavity of the 
lower side. -In fig. 77 is shown a series of these responses of 
the compound strip, taken on a smoked 
surface by means of a recording lever. 

In anisotropic motile organs, such as 
the pulvinus of J/zmosa, response takes 
place by differential contraction, the more 
excitable side being that which under 
diffuse stimulation becomes concave. 
If we apply very moderate stimulus 
AG 21. DiRerenge locally on the upper half of the pulvinus, 

sponse of Artificial we shall find that, by the excitatory con- 

sh traction of this half, the leaf is raised. 
A. similar contractile effect, though of greater intensity, is 
induced when the lower half of the pulvinus is stimulated 
locally, the leaf in this case undergoing a depression. When 
both upper and lower halves, then, are excited simultaneously, 
the resulting fall of the leaf shows that the contraction of the 
lower half must in this case be the greater, or, in other words, 
that this half is the more excitable of the two. This experi- 
ment may be carried out very easily by using the stimulus of 
light. Fig. 78 gives the results observed (a), showing the up 
movement consequent on stimulation of the upper half; (¢) 
that caused by equal stimulation of the lower half; and (c) the 
resultant fail when the two are excited simultaneously. In the 
case of mechanical response, then, we find it true that response 
is by the greater contraction of the more excitable. 


DETECTION OF PHYSIOLOGICAL ANISOTROPY fore 


We shall next observe what is the electrical mode of 
response for.a tissue which is anisotropic, or unequally ex- 
citable on two sides. For this purpose we may again take 
the pulvinus of A/zmosa, and make electrical connections at 
two diametrically opposite points on the upper and lower 
halves of the pulvinus respectively. It is to be remembered 
that electrical response takes place on excitation, whether 
the leaf be free to move, or physically restrained. We may, 
therefore, hold it in a fixed position; and indeed this is 
advisable, in order to avoid that shifting of the electrical 
contacts which might 
possibly take place if 
it were allowed to fall. 

The two contacts 
are made with two 
fine straws filled with 
kaolin paste, moistened 
in normal saline. On 
now applying a series 
of thermal stimuli, on 
the petiole, near the 


pulvinus, I obtained the 
responses given in fig. Fic. 78. Responses of AZ/mosa to Sunlight of 


; not too long Duration 
79. It will be seen 
: (a) Light acting on pulvinus from above ; (64) light 
that the responsive acting on pulvinus from below ; (c) light acting 
current flows in the simultaneously from above and below. Dotted 


3 line represents recovery on cessation of light. 
tissue from the rela- 


tively more excited lower, to the less excited upper, half 
of the organ. 

We thus arrive at a comprehensive law of the mechanical 
and electrical response of anisotropic organs: 

Diffuse stimulation induces greater contraction and 
galvanometric negativity of the more excitable side. 

The laws of electric response in the anisotropic organ 
may then be detailed ‘as follows :— 

1. On simultaneous excitation of two points, A and B, the 
responsive current flows in the tissue from the more to the less 
excited. 


IIo COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


2. Conversely, if under simultaneous excitation the responsive 
current be from B to A, then B is the more excitable of these 
two points. 

These form only an instance of the general law that the 
responsive current always flows from the more to the less 
excited. For when a point, B, is excited locally—-this point, 
that is to say, being the more excited—the responsive 
current is found to flow away from it to a neutral or in- 
different point, A, for which any distant point will serve, 
provided the tissue be non-conducting. Should it be con- 
ducting, the neutrality of A is maintained by interposing a 


Fic. 79. Transverse Response of Pulvinus of AZimosa 


The petiole is securely held to prevent movement, and diametric electric 
contacts made in the upper and lower surfaces of pulvinus. Re- 
sponsive current is from lower to upper surface. 


block. Should the stimulus, however, not be local, but diffuse, 
a resultant response may still be obtained by injuring or 
killing the point A, and thus diminishing or abolishing its 
excitability. On stimulation, the point B is now necessarily 
the more excited, and the responsive current is still away 
from B, towards A. And finally, owing to physiological 
anisotropy, B may be naturally more excitable than A, and 
6n stimulation the responsive current will then be found 
to flow from the more excited B to the less excited A. 

The comparison of the excitabilities of the two points 
A and B, therefore, reduces itself to the application of similar 


DETECTION OF PHYSIOLOGICAL ANISOTROPY III 


stimuli to the two points simultaneously, and then ascertaining 
the direction of the responsive current. 

For this purpose we might employ any form of stimulus, 
and it is extremely interesting to find that, however diverse 
the. stimuli, the results obtained by them are always 
identical And here we have not merely a means of 
qualitative demonstration, but in some cases one of quanti- 
tative also. i 

If we take an erect stem of Cucurbita, it being radial 
and isotropic, all its flanks will be found equally excitable. 
Hence, if two diametrically opposite contacts are made, there 


Fic. 80. Diametric Method of Stimulation of an Anisotropic Organ 


Diametrically opposite contacts are made at A and B, and tissue subjected 
to vibrational stimulus. 


will, on diffuse stimulation, be no resultant response. But 
when such a stem becomes recumbent, the upper side, being 
now constantly exposed to light, becomes fatigued by over- 
stimulation, with consequent diminution of its excitability. 
This is true only when the stimulus has been excessive and 
long continued ; for we have seen moderate stimulus may 
sometimes enhance the excitability. By the unilateral action 
of light, then, the organ has been converted from radial into 
anisotropic, the lower side being that which we shall expect 
to find the more excitable. 

On mounting such a stem in the vibratory apparatus 
(fig. 80), and making diametrically opposite contacts on the 
two anisotropic surfaces, we find that on applying vibration 


112 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


both sides are subjected to similar stimulus simultaneously ; 
and the responsive current is now found to flow across the 
tissue, from the lower to the upper side. The lower is thus, 
as we expected, the more excitable. Since we can by means 
of vibration apply measured stimuli, it will be seen that we 
have here a quantitative method of investigation. Moreover, 
as the stimulus is applied directly, it is applicable not only 
to conducting but also to non-conducting tissues. 


If we next take a radial stem or petiole of Cucurbita, and 


slit it longitudinally, we obtain, in either of the halves, a 
specimen having an inner and an outer surface. As one of 
these has been exposed to light and the other protected 
from it, we should expect to find, on examination, that there 
has been an induction of physiological anisotropy. As such 
a specimen is not very well adapted for vibrational stimula- 
tion, we may use that of pressure. Two moistened rags, in 
_ connection with non-polarisable electrodes, pass through two 
pieces of cork, adjusted on the two surfaces—outer and 
inner—at diametrically opposite points. When the inter- 
posed tissue is now subjected to sudden pressure its two 
surfaces are excited simultaneously, and the responsive 
- current is found to flow from the inner concave to the 
outer convex surface, proving that the former was the more 
excitable. | 

We might again use the chemical form of stimulation, 
and the results obtained by this method will be described in 
the course of the next chapter. But these forms of stimulus 
—by pressure, or by chemical means—are not capable of 
exact measurement. For quantitative observations, then, it 
is necessary to employ some other form of stimulus, and the 
electrical offers us in this respect many advantages. There 
are, however, in this case many possible disturbing influences 
to be considered, all of which must be carefully eliminated 
before the method can be used without misgiving. How this 
may be done will be shown in a future chapter. For the 
present I shall describe another method of stimulation which 
I have been able to bring to great perfection, by which 


ee 


DETECTION OF PHYSIOLOGICAL ANISOTROPY 113 


two points of an anisotropic organ may be simultaneously 
excited, under a series of stimuli of uniform or increasing 
intensity. 

This mode of excitation, by thermal shocks, will be found 
in every way satisfactory and convenient. The Thermal 
Variator, by which stimulation is effected, consists of a spiral 
of german-silver wire, the diameter of the spiral being about 
3 cm. The electrical circuit, through which the heating- 
current is sent, is closed periodically for a definite length of 


Fic. 81. The Thermal Variator 


The anisotropic tissue-petiole of J/wsa is held in ebonite clip, c. 8, &’, 
electrodes connected with opposite sides. Specimen after adjustment 
pushed inside heating-spiral, T, by slide, s. Spiral heated periodically 
by closure of electric circuit by metronome, M. 


time, by means of a metronome (fig. 81). The thermal 
variation within the coil can be controlled by a suitable 
adjustment of the battery-power, or by the duration of 
closure, or both. The experimental tissue is held in an 
ebonite clip, C, fixed on a slide, S, on the same stand as the 
heating-spiral. This slide is pulled out for the purpose of 
adjustment. Square or circular pieces of wetted muslin make 
contacts with equal areas on two opposite sides of the 
experimental tissue, these pieces of cloth being connected 
with non-polarisable electrodes, E and E’ After the adjust- 


y 


IIl4 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


ment is made, the slide is pushed in, till the tissue is well in 
the centre of the coil. When the circuit is completed, for a 
brief period, both the sides A and B are subjected to the same 
sudden variation of temperature, which, as we know, acts as 
a stimulus. As the two contacts are thus in practice raised 
to the same temperature, there will be no thermo-electrical 
disturbance. The responsive current, therefore, will be 
determined by any difference of excitability which may exist 


as between A and B. The spiral also gives out heat-radia- 


tion, which acts as a contributory stimulus. That it is the 
thermal variation, and not the temperature, which acts as 
the efficient external stimulus, is seen from the fact that 
when the tissue is subjected to the higher temperature con- 
tinuously, the galvanometric deflection obtained is opposite 
in direction to that induced by the thermal shock. This is 
because the absorption of heat, as such, increases the internal 
energy, and thus induces an electrical effect opposite to that 
caused by external stimulation. 

As experimental tissue, we may use the sheathing 
petiole of Musa. The required piece is cut and mounted in 
the apparatus, the concave surface being taken, say, as B, 
and the convex as A. I have mentioned J/usa as suitable 
for this purpose, because I find it, when fresh, to show 
practically no sign of fatigue in its responses. There are 
many other sheathing petioles, which would doubtless answer 
the same purpose more or less perfectly. | 

In obtaining records with this specimen, it is found that 
the responsive current flows across the petiole, from the inner 
concave surface B to the outer convex surface A, showing 
that it is the inside which is more excitable. Uniform 
stimuli of short duration were applied at intervals of one 
minute, and the responses obtained are seen to be fairly 
uniform (fig. 82). The specimen was next subjected to the 
anesthetic action of chloroform. This, it will be seen, in- 
duced a very great depression of the response. 

It has thus been shown that just as the greater contrac- 
tion and concayity of a motile organ enables us to discrimi- 


DETECTION OF PHYSIOLOGICAL ANISOTROPY I15 


nate which side of two is the more excitable, so here also the 
more excitable side is that which, on diffuse stimulation, 
exhibits galvanometric negativity relatively to the other. 
From this it becomes possible to determine the relative 
excitabilities of any anisotropic organ, even though it be 
non-motile, and therefore incapable of exhibiting any con- 
spicuous mechanical response. The difficulty of applying 
equal and quantitative stimulus on two sides simultaneously 


Fic. 82. Responsive Current in Petiole of Musa from Concave to 
Convex Side 


First series, normal ; after application of chloroform subsequent 
depression. 


has now been overcome by vibrational stimulation, and by 
the perfection of the method of thermal shocks. Thus a 
definite resultant response has been shown to be determined 
by the differential excitabilities of two parts of an experi- 
mental tissue. And that from this consideration it becomes 
further possible to resolve many of the remaining anomalies 
of electrical response will be fully demonstrated in a sub- 
sequent chapter. 


CHAPTER X 
THE NATURAL CURRENT AND ITS VARIATIONS 


Natural current in anisotropic organ from the less to the more excitable—External 
stimulus induces responsive current in opposite direction—Increase of internal 
energy induces ‘positive, and decrease negative, variation of natural current— 
Effect on natural current of variation of temperature—Effect of sudden 
variation—Variation of natural current by chemical agents, referred to 
physiological reaction—Agents which render tissue excitable, induce the 
positive, and those which cause excitation, the negative variation—Action of 
hydrochloric acid—Action of Na,CO,—Effect modified by strength of dose— 
Effect of CO, and of alcohol vapour—Natural current and its variations— 
Extreme unreliability of negative variation so-called as test of excitatory 
reaction— Reversal of natural current by excessive cold or by stimulation — 
Reversal of normal response under sub-tonicity or fatigue. 


WE have seen that when the pulvinus of A/zmosa is excited 
by an external stimulus, there is a relatively greater expulsion 
of water from the more excitable lower half, with a con- 
comitant greater contraction. Conversely, the lower half of 
the pulvinus is capable of absorbing more water, and of 
expanding to a greater extent, than the upper. Increased 
internal energy, in contrast to the action of external stimulus, 
has the effect of causing a greater expansion of the lower 
half of the pulvinus, and thus raising the leaf. This we saw 
exemplified when the plant was subjected to a gradually 
rising temperature, so as to increase its internal energy, its 
leaves being thereby made to show increased erection (p. 72), 
Hence the more excitable tissue in the pulvinus of Wzmosa 
is characterised, both by greater power of absorption and by 
greater emission of energy, according to circumstances. In 
this we see a close analogy to the action of inorganic bodies, 
in which also we find the greatest power of emission to be 
associated with a correspondingly great power of absorption of 


energy. 


THE NATURAL CURRENT AND ITS VARIATIONS I17 


We have thus seen that in order to maintain a high state 
of excitability, absorption of energy is necessary. On 


excitation, emission of energy occurs. In this latter case, of 


emission, we observe a concomitant galvanometric negativity 
of the more excited lower side. Since to have maintained its 
excitability the opposite process of absorption would have 
been necessary, it follows that the more excztable lower side 
must under normal conditions be galvanometrically positive. 
This is found to be the case. For when the leaf is in an 
excitable condition, there is an electro-motive difference 
between the upper and lower halves of the pulvinus, in con- 
sequence of which a current flows across the tissue, from the 
less excitable upper to the more excitable lower half, which 
is thus galvanometrically positive, in relation to the upper. 
We have here, then, an additional instance of the opposite 
effects of internal energy and external stimulus. Internal 
energy, maintaining a greater excitability of the lower half 
of the pulvinus, induces in it a relative galvanometric positivity. 
External stimulus, on the other hand, gives rise to precisely 
the opposite effect—namely, the relative galvanometric 
negativity of the lower half. Under typical conditions, then, 
we may expect the more excitable point to be galvano- 
metrically positive; and the more excited to be galvano- 
metrically negative. 

Turning next to non-motile tissues, we find the same 
conclusions to hold. good. We saw that in the case of the 
sheathing petiole of M/usa, the concave was more excitable 
than the convex side. The concave is thus normally positive 
to the convex side, and the natural current flows across the 
tissue from the convex to the concave. While the natural 
current flows from the more excitable to the less excitable, 
external stimulus gives rise to a responsive current in the 
opposite direction, from the more excited to the less excited, 
constituting a negative variation of the current of rest. . 

Let us next consider what would be the effect of an 
increase of internal energy on the natural current. Since the 


action of internal energy is opposite to that of external 


stimulus, we should expect it to induce a positive variation of 


118 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


the natural current. Diminution of internal energy on the 
other hand might be expected to cause a negative variation. 
These effects are diagrammatically represented in fig. 83, 
which also exhibits the parallelism between the electric 
responses of motile pulvinus and non-motile anisotropic 
organ. : 

We next proceed to subject the question of the effect of 


increased or diminished internal energy on the natural 
current to experimental verification. As regards the increase ~ 


of internal energy, we have already seen that this can 
be secured by a gradually 


id Than rising _ temperature, its 
¥ diminution being, con- 
eas 2G, trariwise, secured by a 
2 falling temperature. In 


the case of Mzmosa, we 


Fic. 83. Parallelism of Natural Current . 
in Pulvinus of Mimosa and Sheathing 54W that the former in- 


Petiole of Musa duced an erection of the 


Upper and less excitable surface of former Jeayes and the latter a 
corresponds with outer or convex sur- ‘ 


face of latter. The natural current, N, gradual depression. In 


is in both from the less to the more ex- 
citable. In both excitatory current, E, order, then, to observe the 


is in opposite direction, z.e. from the effect of increase or de- 
more to the less excitable. In Musa ae 

increase of internal energy (+ }) in- Crease O internal energy 
duces a positive; and diminution of on the existing current of 


internal energy (— *) a negative, varia- 
tion of the natural current. rest, we have only to sub- 
ject the specimen to gradual 
thermal ascent or descent, and record the consequent 
variation of current. 

The specimen of Zusa is placed in a chamber and two 
diametrically opposite contacts are made, with the internal 
and external surfaces, and led off to the galvanometer. To 
take first the effect of cooling: a stream of ice-cold water is 
sent through a hollow tube in the chamber: this gradually 
lowers the temperature, say from 30° C. to 27° C. It will be 
seen from left-hand curve of fig. 84 that this has the effect 
of diminishing the natural current of rest in the tissue, as 
represented by the dotted arrow |}. When the chamber is 


THE NATURAL CURRENT AND ITS VARIATIONS I1I9Q 


allowed to’ return to the temperature of the room, this 
diminution of current is annulled. 

To study the effect, on the other hand, of a rising tempera- 
ture, the chamber is gradually heated, by means of the 
electric heating-coil already described. In thus raising the 
temperature, it is found (fig. 84, right-hand curve) that the 
natural resting-current undergoes an increase. On cooling 
again to the original = 
temperature, this in- 
crease is annulled. 
That these effects are 
due to induced elec- 
tro-motive variations, 
and not to any 
changes of resistance, 
is demonstrated from 
the fact that the effect 
described takes place 
even when the original 
E. M. F. is exactly 
balanced by a poten- 


tiometer. Fic. 84. Effect ot Variation of Temperature 
It ‘all on Natural Current, {, which in Petiole of 
was specially Musa flows from Convex to Concave Side 
stated that these ob- Effect of cooling from 30° C. to 27° C., seen 
servations with regard on left, induces negative variation of natural 
current. Restoration to original value on 
tothe effect of varying return to surrounding temperature. Warm- 
t | ing induces positive variation (see record to 
emperatures apply right). In this and subsequent figures in 
only to steady varia- the present chapter { indicates the direction 
of the natural current of rest. 


tions. In the case of 

thermal ascent, we 

have seen that a steady rise brings about an increase of 
the existing current. But since sudden variation of tempera- 
ture acts as a stimulus, we shall, in the preliminary stage, 
obtain an excitatory reaction, which will cause a transient 
diminution of the current of rest. This will be followed, 
when the rise of temperature is steady, by am increase of 
the current of rest. I give here (fig. 85) a photographic 


120 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


record of these contrasted effects. In the first part of the 
curve we observe a sudden movement of the record upwards 
corresponding to the sudden rise of temperature. This is so 
great as to carry the curve out of the photographic field. We 
have here, then, a sudden excitatory diminution of the natural 
current. In the next stage, while 
the temperature is steadily ascending, 
we find a reversal of the curve, and 
the natural current is enhanced above. 
the normal. On now allowing the 
chamber to cool down to the original 
temperature of the room, the natural 
current was found to return more or 
less to its normal value. 

We shall next study the effect 
of chemical agents on the natural 
current. The mode of procedure 
is to apply the given agent on both 
the contacts at the same time. If 


Fic. 85. Photographic 
Record showing effect of 
Sudden, followed by 


steady Rise of Tem- 
perature on Natural Cur- 
rent, ¥, in A/usa 


the substance be liquid, it can be 
applied by a pipette. If it be gaseous, 


the specimen is placed in a chamber 
through which the gas or vapour is 
allowed to stream. 


' During sudden variation of 
temperature an excitatory 
negative variation of 
natural current: takes 
place, as shown by first 
up curve; when rise of 

- temperature becomes 
steady there is a positive 
variation, as shown by 
the down curve ; on re- 
turn to surrounding tem- 
perature, the normal cur- 
rent is restored to its 
original value. 


In observing 
the effects of various agents we 
obtain results which are at first 
sight very perplexing. For example, 
certain substances will be found to 
induce a diminution of the natural 
current, and others an increase. The 
effect, moreover, is found to be modi- 
fied by the strength of the dose. Thus an agent which, 
in a given strength, will cause a diminution of the natural 
current, may often be found to cause an increase, when 
sufficiently diluted. This inquiry is of great importance, 
since it is directly connected with many equally obscure 
problems in medical practice, where the effect of a drug 


THE NATURAL CURRENT AND ITS VARIATIONS I12I 


is well known to be modified by the amount of the 
dose. 

Much light appears to be thrown on this subject, when 
we consider the electrical reactions of the chemical agents as 
due to their physiological action. If a drop of hydrochloric 
acid be applied to the pulvinus of J7/zmosa, the leaf falls, 
showing that the more excitable side has undergone a greater 
excitatory contraction. We have also seen that when a drop 
of this acid is applied on any tissue in the neighbourhood of, 
but not directly touching, an electrical contact, it induces an 
excitatory galvanometric negativity. If now we apply it in 
solution, say, of 10 per cent. on the two diametrically opposite 
contacts of J/usa, we shall expect that the greater excitatory 
reaction induced on the concave side will give rise on that 
side to a relative galvanometric negativity, resulting in a 
negative variation of current of rest. On the application of 
the reagent this is found to be the case, the responsive 
current flowing in a direction opposite to that of rest: that 
is to say, it flows from the more excited concave to the less 
excited convex. 

It is by considering chemical agents from the point of 
view of their physiological reaction, that we are able to 
explain their diversity of effects, according to the strength of 
the dose and the duration of application. We have seen that 
while a strong stimulus induces the excitatory effect of 
negativity, a feeble stimulus will bring about the opposite, 
or positivity. This abnormal positive response, however, by 
the continued action of moderately feeble stimulus becomes 
converted into normal negative. Now a chemical substance 
which in a certain strength acts as an efficient excitatory 
agent, may, when sufficiently diluted, act as a feeble stimulus, 
inducing a positive response. If the same agent again were 
applied in a slightly greater concentration, its immediate. 
effect might be positive, to be succeeded under continued 
application by the normal negative. 

We might thus expect, using a strong salon of a given 
chemical reagent, to obtain a negative variation of the current 


122 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


of rest; using a dilute solution, to obtain a positive varia- 
tion ; and, lastly, applying a dose of intermediate strength, 
to discover the very interesting case in which the reagent 
would give rise immediately to a positive variation, and after 
a longer or shorter continuance of its action, to a reversed, or 
negative variation, of the current of rest. These inductions 
are found fully verified in the experiment which I am now 
about to describe. 


1 

| 
i 

‘ 

‘ 
Vv 


Fic. 86. Action of 7 per cent. Solution Fic. 87. Effect of CO, on 

of Na,CO, on Natural Current of M/zsa Natural Current of AZwsa 
Preliminary positive variation represented Preliminary positive seen to be’ 
by down curve followed by reversal, succeeded by negative variation 
50 seconds after application. 5 minutes after application. ; 


Applying a strong solution of sodium carbonate —10 per 
cent. or above—at the electrical contacts on Musa, the result 
is a negative variation of the natural current. If nowa dilute 
solution of I per cent. be applied on a similar specimen, we 
obtain a response by positive variation. And if, lastly, we 
use a 7 per cent. solution, we obtain, as will be seen from the 
record (fig. 86) the preliminary positive, succeeded, under the 
continued action of the agent, by reversal to the negative, 
variation. 

We pass next to the question of the effect of gases. In 
fig. 87 is given a record of the action of carbonic acid on 


i MN i a 


THE NATURAL CURRENT AND ITS VARIATIONS 123 


the natural current in /usa. It will be seen here that in the 
first stage there was an enhancement, or positive variation, 
of the existing current. In a later stage, however, this is 
followed by a reversal, the resting-current now undergoing a 
diminution. We have here an effect parallel to that of the 
intermediate dose of sodium carbonate. Vapour of alcohol 
also exerts an effect very similar ; that is to say, it induces a 
preliminary exaltation, followed by a depression of the natural 


current. 


In connection with this subject, of the changes induced 
in the natural electro-motive difference between the two 
surfaces, by the action of a chemical reagent, it is well to 
distinguish between the effects of two different factors: 
namely, the electrical variation caused by the chemical sub- 
stance as such, and that brought about by the excitatory 
reaction. Let us suppose both the electrical contacts to be 
made on iso-electrical surfaces, with normal saline solution; 
there will then be no difference of potential, as between the 
two. But this state of things will be disturbed, by the appli- 
cation of another chemical solution, say acid, on either one 
of the two contacts. The resulting disturbance may be dis- 
tinguished as due to heterogeneity of chemical application. 
But if the same chemical agent be applied at both the 
contacts, no such chemical heterogeneity will ensue. If, 
then, any electro-motive difference be induced, it must be due 
primarily to some induced physiological change. The contact 
which has been rendered more excitable will become increas- 
ingly positive ; that which is more excited, on the other. hand, 
will become increasingly negative. That the induced electro- 
motive variation under such circumstances is indicative ot 
a variation of excitability or excitation, was seen in the fact 
that the same chemical agent—for example, Na,CO,— caused 
a positive variation when dilute, a negative when strong, and 
positive followed by negative under the continued action of 
an intermediate dose. This conclusion—that the variation 
of the existing current, by the simultaneous application 
at the two contacts of the same chemical reagent, is due 


124 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


to a physiological reaction, the positive variation being a 
sign of relatively increased, and the negative of decreased, 
excitability—will be verified by an independent mode of in- 
quiry, to be described in the following chapter. | 

It follows from the experimental demonstration which has 
just been given that the phenomena of the natural current 
and its variations may be summarised in general as follows : 

1. Under normal conditions, the current of rest flows in 


the tissue from the less to the more excitable. In other 


words, the more excitable is galvanometrically positive to the 
less excitable. 

2. Increase of internal energy induces an increase or. 
positive variation of the existing current ; and diminution of 
internal energy induces a negative variation. 

3. External stimulus induces a negative variation of the 
true or natural current of rest. 


The natural current and its variations under normal con- . 


ditions have now been studied. We shall next proceed to trace 
out those conditions under which abnormal results may occur. 
Excessive cooling, by diminishing internal energy, may thus 
reverse the normal current, which reversal may become more 
or less persistent. It has been shown, moreover, that in an 
anisotropic organ, external stimulus gives rise to a current 
opposite in direction to the natural current. . By this excita- 
tory reaction the more excitable side, hitherto positive, is 
rendered negative, and if the excitatory reaction be great, it 
may remain fora considerable period in this reversed condition 
of galvanometric negativity. 

We have seen that under normal conditions, the direction 
of the natural or true current of rest is from the less to the 
more excitable, and that external stimulus causes a responsive 
current in the opposite direction, which thus constitutes a 
negative variation of the current of rest. This state of 
things we shall distinguish as the primary condition. It 
frequently happens, however, in consequence of previous 
stimulation, with its after-effect, that extremely varied effects, 
appearing at first very anomalous, occur, with regard to the 


‘Le 


ee 


THE NATURAL CURRENT AND ITS VARIATIONS 125 


direction, not only of the current of rest, but also of the 
current of response. We shall be able. to obtain a clear 
understanding of these effects, on subjecting the underlying 
phenomena to close analysis. As a concrete example, we 
may take for our investigation the various effects to be 
observed in the pulvinus of Mzmosa. 

In this anisotropic organ, the directions of the resultant 
current of rest and the current of response are determined, 
as we have seen, by the differential excitability and differential 
excitation of the two sides of the organ. We shall fix our 
attention, however, for the sake of simplicity, on the changes 
which occur in the more effective lower half. I shall here 
succinctly describe the various post-primary phases in the 


changing effect, culminating in the onset of fatigue from over- 


stimulation. In the primary condition, as we have seen, the 
lower half of the pulvinus is positive to the upper, the 
direction of the resting-current being down ¥. On stimula- 
tion, the lower half becomes negative and. the response 
current is up t. Response thus takes place by a negative 
variation of the resting-current. 

We may now suppose the pulvinus to be in a state of 
slight excitation, its molecular condition at or about the B 
stage. This existing state of moderate excitation will annul 
the previous positivity, and the current of rest will now be 
zero. At this stage, however, as we have seen, the excit- 
ability of a tissue is enhanced. Hence, on stimulation, the 
lower half of the pulvinus will exhibit responsive negativity, 
and the direction of the response current will be up ft: We 
are, however, unable to describe this variation in terms of the 
current of rest, since that, as we have already seen, is zero. 

A condition of still stronger excitation, bringing the 
tissue to the C condition, will induce galvanometric negativity 
of the lower half. The so-called current of rest is thus 
upwards t. But as the excitability of the lower half is. still 
relatively greater than that of the upper, it follows that 
external stimulus will bring about a responsive current whose 
direction is upwards t, This normal excitatory response, 


126 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


then, in this particular case, appears as a positive variation of 
the resting-current. 

And, finally, we may imagine the pulvinus to have been 
strongly excited, so as to be in the condition Dor E. The 
resting-current will in this case be upwards ft. But the ex- 
citability of the lower half, owing to fatigue, has now become 
depressed, a condition which, as we have seen, tends to give 
rise to the abnormal positive response, the responsive current 


being thus downwards ¥. As the modified current of rest | 


is upwards, this abnormal current of response will appear as 
a negative variation of it. 
All these cases are conveniently tabulated as follows : 


TABULAR STATEMENT OF THE RELATIVE DIRECTIONS OF THE CURRENT 
OF REST AND THE RESPONSIVE CURRENT UNDER VARIOUS CONDITIONS. 


Current of rest. Current of re- | Variation of current of rest. 


ndition. 
Co sponse. 


Negative variation 


Primary condition , | 
Feebly excited 
Moderately excited 
Strongly excited and 
| fatigued . : 


* 

t fi. :, 
+ Positive variation 
1 


Negative variation 


| > -o< 


The various conditions mentioned may be induced 
accidentally in the responding organ, or may be brought 
about by the excitatory effect of experimental prepara- 
tion. I give here the records of certain experiments 
performed on J/zmosa, in which some of these changes were 
seen to occur as the result of stimulation (fig. 88). The 
normal natural current is seen to be from above to below, as 
represented by the dotted arrow. The first strong stimulus, 
applied at the moment represented by the thick dot, gives 
rise to a responsive current whose direction is from below to 
above, Owing to the strong intensity of the stimulation, 
there is here a slight indication of multiple response. As an 
after-effect of stimulus, we observe that the normal resting- 
current has undergone a reversal, the lower surface, which 
was formerly positive, having now become relatively 
negative, A second stimulus now gave rise to a response 


— oe 


—— 


THE NATURAL CURRENT AND ITS VARIATIONS 127 


similar to the first. But after this, owing to the greater 
fatigue with loss of excitability induced in the lower half of 
the pulvinus, the succeeding responses are seen to be reversed, 
the responsive current being henceforth from above to below. 

From the table given on the previous page, it will be 
seen that hardly could any standard have been devised for 
the study of excitatory reaction, so likely to be prolific of 
confusion as this, of the so called variation of the resting- 
current. For in the first 
three cases displayed, we 
see one identical excitatory 
effect, appearing now as a 
negative, again as doubtful, 
and a third time as a positive 
variation of the current of 
rest. In the fourth case, 
again, it is actually the 
abnormal response’ which 
appears as the normal nega- 
tive variation! But while 
these responses appear to 
be so various, the underlying 


Fic. 88. Variation of the Transverse 
Natural and Responsive Currents in 
Pulvinus of Wzmosa 


Natural current } which -is normally 
down, reversed in consequence of 
strong external stimulus. The first 
two responses are normal, z.¢. current 
being from below to above. Strong 
stimulus is here seen to induce mul- 


reaction is nevertheless con- 
stant. The direction of the 
responsive current is always 
from the more to the less 


tiple responses. After the second 
response on account of the greater 
fatigue induced in the lower half of 
the pulvinus, the direction of the 
responsive current is seen to be 


reversed. Thick dots represent 


excited. moment of application of stimulus. 


It has thus been shown | 
in the course of the present chapter that under normal 
conditions the current of rest flows in the tissue from 
the less to the more excitable; that increase of internal 
energy causes a positive variation of the current of rest , 
while its diminution gives rise to a negative variation ; that 
reagents which increase excitability induce a positive, and 
those which cause excitation a negative, variation of the 
resting-current ; and, finally, that external stimulus induces 
a negative variation of the resting-current. While these are 


128 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


the normal reactions, however, under abnormal conditions, 
they may be reversed. Thus, excessive cooling or strong 


external stimulation may reverse the normal current of rest. . 


There are, moreover, two different conditions, those, namely, 
of sub-tonicity (cf p. 106) and fatigue, which may be effective 
in bringing about a reversal of the normal direction of the 
responsive current. In this way, by means of induced varia- 
tions of the resting-current and of the responsive current, 
many very varied effects become possible. 


Al ett i od 


_— Te eee 


—o 


CHAPTER XI 


VARIATIONS OF EXCITABILITY UNDER CHEMICAL 
REAGENTS com : 


Induced variation of excitability studied by two methods: (1) direct (2) trans- 
mitted stimulation—Effect of chloroform—Effect of chloral— Effect of formalin 
—Advantage of the Method of Block over that of negative variation—Effect 
of KHO—Response unaffected by variation of resistance—Stimulating action 
of solution of sugar—Of sodium carbonate— Effect of doses—Effect of hydro- 
chloric acid— Di-phasic response on application of potash—Conversion of normal 
negative into abnormal positive response by abolition of true excitability. 


IT has been said in a previous chapter that the electrical 
response is a true physiological response. This is demon- 
strated by the fact that, while a vigorous specimen gives 
strong electrical response of galvanometric negativity, the 
same specimen, when killed, whether by heat or by poison, 
ceases to respond. This particular electrical response is 
thus seen to be a concomitant of physiological efficiency. 
It follows that, whatever diminishes physiological activity 
will, parz passu, modify the amplitude of the response. But 
in cases in which the death of the tissue is brought about by 
steam or by poison, it is the last stage only, namely the 
abolition of response, that can be observed. It is also impor- 
tant, however, to be able to trace the growth of physiological 
changes through the concomitant modification of response. 
In this way it is possible not only to study the gradual onset 
of death, as induced by a poison, but also the action of other 
chemical agents, some of which might be of such a nature 
as to induce exaltation, others depression, and still others, 
like the narcotics, a temporary abolition of the electrical 
response. 
K. 


130 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


An essential condition of this investigation is first to obtain 
a uniform series of responses. Having once done this, those 
subsequent changes in the response which are due to the appli- 
cation of a given reagent can be demonstrated in an unmis- 
takable manner. I have already explained in Chapter III. 
that this may be done by either of two different methods: 
namely, those of direct and of transmitted stimulation. Inthe 
first of these we employ vibrational stimulus, using the Method 


ql 
i 


Before 4 After 


Fic. 89. Photographic Record of Effect of Chloroform on Responses of Carrot 


Stimuli of 25° torsional vibration at intervals of one minute. 


of Block. In the second, the stimulus of thermal shocks is 
used, the excitation of the proximal contact being due to 
transmitted stimulation. 

We shall investigate the effect of chemical reagents by 
both these methods. And first I shall give results obtained 
by the employment of the Method of Block, the tissue being 
subject to direct stimulation. In cases where the effect of 
gaseous reagents, like chloroform, is to be studied, the 
vapour is blown into the plant-chamber (see fig. 21). In 


EXCITABILITY UNDER CHEMICAL REAGENTS 131 


cases of liquid reagents, they are applied on the points of 
contact A and B, and in their close neighbourhood. The 
experiment is carried out by first obtaining a series of 
normal responses to uniform stimuli, applied at~ regular 
intervals of time, say one minute, the record being taken the 
while on a photographic plate. Then, without interrupting 
this procedure, the given agent—say, vapour of chloroform— 
is applied, by being blown into the chamber. It will be seen 
from fig. 84 how rapidly chloroform induces depression of 
response, and how the effect grows with time. If the speci- 
men be subjected for a short time only to the anesthetic, 
the depressing action proves transient, passing off on 
the reintroduction of fresh air. But too strong or too pro- 
longed an application induces a permanent abolition of 
response. 

I give below (figs. 90, 91), two sets of records, one of 
which shows the effect of chloral and the other formalin. 


: 


A A 
— \ ' \ \ A £ a 
Ne NAA 


Before + After 


Fic. 90. Photographic Record showing Action of Chloral Hydrate on the 
Responses of Leaf-stalk of Cauliflower 


Torsional vibration of 25° at intervals of one minute. 


These reagents were applied as solutions on the tissue at 

the two leading contacts and adjacent surfaces. Both 

are scen to induce a rapid decline of the response. In the 
K 2 


I 32 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


normal responses, shown in fig. QI, is seen a very interesting 
instance of alternating fatigue. 

In order to bring out clearly the main phenomena, I 
have postponed till now the consideration of a point of some 
difficulty. To determine the influence of a reagent in 
modifying the excitability of a tissue, we rely upon its effect 
in exalting or depressing the responsive E.M. Variation, and 
we read this effect by means of changes induced in the 
galvanometric deflection. Now as long as the resistance of 
the circuit remains constant, an increase or decrease of 
galvanometric deflection will accurately indicate a heightened 
or depressed E.M. Variation, due to augmented or lowered 


AAA 


Sy Me, 


a 


Before t After 
Fic. 91. Photographic Record showing Action of Formalin (Radish) 


excitability, induced by the reagent in the tissue. But by 
the introduction of the chemical reagent the resistance of 
the tissue may undergo a change, and, owing to this cause, 
modification of response, as read by the galvanometer, may 
be induced without any E.M. Variation; The observed 
variation of response may thus be partly owing to some 
unknown change of resistance, as well as to that of the 
E.M. Variation. 

This difficulty may, however, be obviated by interposing 
a very large and constant resistance in the external circuit. 
The variation in the tissue then becomes negligible, the 
galvanometric deflections being now proportional to the 
electro-motive variation, An actual experiment will make 


EXCITABILITY UNDER CHEMICAL REAGENTS 133 


this point clear. Taking a carrot as a specimen, I found its 
resistance f/us the resistance of the non-polarisable electrodes 
to be 20,000 ohms. The application of a chemical reagent 
reduced this to 19,000 ohms. The resistance of the galva- 
nometer used was 1,000 ohms, and the high constant external 
resistance interposed was I million ohms. The variation of 
resistance induced in the circuit by the application of the 
reagent was thus 1,000 in 1,020,000, or less than one part in 
a thousand. : 

In studying the variation of excitability in animal tissues, 
the method of negative variation is employed. But I may 
here draw attention to the advantage which is afforded by 
the employment of the Method of Block instead. For, 
in the method of negative variation, one contact being 
injured, the chemical reagents act on injured and uninjured 
unequally. It thus happens that by this unequal action the 
resting difference of potential is indefinitely altered. But the 
intensity of response in this method of injury may to a 
certain extent be dependent on the resting difference. It is 
thus seen that, when this method is employed, a factor is 
introduced which may give rise to complications. 

According to the Block Method, however, the two contacts 
are made with uninjured surfaces, and the effect of the 
reagents on both is similar. Thus no advantage is given to 
either contact over the other. The changes now detected in 
the response are therefore due to no adventitious circum- 
stance, but to the reagent itself. If further proof be desired 
of the effect ascribed to the action of the reagent, we can 
now obtain it by the alternate stimulation of the two ends 
A and B. I give below (fig. 92) a record of responses 
obtained in this way from the petiole of turnip. This petiole 
was somewhat conical in form, and owing to this difference 
between the A and B ends, the responses given by one were 
slightly smaller than those given by the other, though the 
stimuli were equal in the two cases. A few drops of a 
10 per cent. solution of NaOH were applied at both ends. 
The record shows how quickly this reagent abolished the 


134 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


response of both. In the next figure (fig. 93) is given a photo- 
graphic record, showing the marked depression of response 
induced by a strong solution of KOH, and in order to show 
that under the given experimental conditions, the variation 
of resistance does not in any way affect the responses, the 
deflection produced in the galvanometer by the application 
of an E.M.F. of ‘1 volt to the circuit is shown at the 
beginning and end of the record. The equality of these two 
deflections shows that the resistance in the circuit has 
remained practically the same throughout the experiment. 


Before t After 


Fic. 92. Abolition of Response at both A and B Ends by the Action 


of NaOH 
Stimuli of 30° vibration were applied at intervals of one minute to A and 
B alternately. Response was completely abolished twenty-four 


minutes after application of NaOH. 


Therefore, the change in the amplitude of the E. M. 
responses recorded may be taken as due entirely to the 
variation in the excitability of the tissue. 

In the experiments just described, the stimulus was 
applied directly at the responding point. By the application 
of a chemical reagent, not only was the responsive excitability 
of the tissue modified, but its receptivity, or power of 
receiving stimulus, also underwent a change. It will be 
shown later that the receptive excitability and the responsive 
excitability are not necessarily the same. The records which 
have just been given show what is, strictly speaking, the 


EXCITABILITY UNDER CHEMICAL REAGENTS — 135 


effect of the reagent on both receptivity and responsivity 
jointly. 

If, however, we wish to study the effect of the reagent on 
responsive excitability alone, it will be necessary to separate 
the receptive from the responding point, and apply the reagent 
on the latter. This may be done by the method of trans- 
mitted stimulation described previously. Successive uniform 


Before + Afier 


Fic. 93. Photographic Record showing the nearly complete Abolition 
of Response by strong KOH 
The two vertical lines are galvanometer deflections due to ‘1 volt, before 


and after the application of reagent. It will be noticed that the total 
resistance remains unchanged. 


stimuli applied at a given point cause excitatory response at 
the separate responding point, the record of which is taken ; 
after this, the chemical reagent is applied locally at the 
responding point. It will be seen that the receptive 
excitability and the conductivity of the intervening tissue 
remain unaffected, changes being induced at the responding 
area alone. 


I shall now describe effects obtained by this method. 


I 36 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


The specimen employed was the petiole offern. The thermal 
stimulator was at a distance of 1°5 cm. from the proximal 
electrode. In fig. 94 is shown the stimulating action of a 


Fic. 94. Photographic Record showing the Stimulatory Action 
of Solution of Sugar 


2 per cent. solution of sugar, inducing a continuous enhance- 


ment of response for some time. 
Another stimulating agent is a dilute solution of Na,Co,. 


This when applied in 1 per cent. solution induces an 


Fic. 95. Photographic Record showing Continuous Action of 
2 per cent. Na,CO, Solution 


Preliminary exaltation followed by depression. 


enhancement of amplitude of response, but when given in 
strong solution, induces depression. An intermediate strength 
of solution shows preliminary enhancement followed by de- 


pression (fig. 95). 


EXCITABILITY UNDER CHEMICAL REAGENTS 137 


While pursuing another line of inquiry on the effect of 
various strengths of solution of Na,Co, on the natural 
current, I obtained results which were parallel (p. 122). 
It was there shown that dilute solution of Na,Co, induced 
a positive variation of the natural current ; a strong solution, 
a negative variation, and that a solution of intermediate 
strength induced a preliminary positive followed by a negative 
variation. Thus the positive variation in the last-named 
experiments, already shown to be indicative of increased 
excitability, was here seen to correspond with heightened 
amplitude of response, while the negative variation on the 
other hand is seen to coincide with depression of excitability. 
The application of a strong solution inducing excitation, 
carries the molecular condition of the tissue to the stage 
E, where, as we know, the excitability is depressed. 

Another fact elucidated by this and similar inquiries, 
which I have pursued elsewhere,' lies in the fact that the 
difference between stimulants and poisons, so called, is often 
one merely of degree. Thus a stimulatory reagent, if given 
in large quantities, will be found to induce a profound 
depression, whereas a poisonous reagent in minute quantities 
may be found to act as a stimulant. In carrying out a 
similar investigation with regard to growth response, | found 
that sugar, for instance, which is stimulating in solutions of, 
say, I to 5 per cent., becomes depressing when the solution 
is very strong. Copper sulphate again, which is regarded as 
a poison, is only so at 1 per cent. and upwards, a solution of 
‘2 per cent. being actually a stimulant. The difference 
between sugar and copper sulphate is here seen to lie in the 
fact that in the latter case the range of safety is very narrow. 
Another fact, which must be borne in mind in this connec- 
tion, is that a substance like sugar is used by the plant for 
general metabolic processes, and thus removed from the 
sphere of action. Thus continuous absorption of sugar could 
not for a long time bring about sufficient accumulation to 
cause depression. With copper sulphate, however, the case 

1 Bose, Plant Response, p. 488. 


i138 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


is different. Here, the constant absorption of the minimal 
stimulatory dose would cause accumulation in the system, 
and thus ultimately bring about the death of the plant. 


Fic. 96. Photographic Record showing the Depressing 
Action of 5 per cent. HCl Acid 


The effect of very dilute acids is often to induce an 
enhancement of excitability, while strong solutions induce 
depression and abolition. In fig. 96 is shown the depression 


Fic. 97, Photographic Record showing Effect of I per cent. KHO 


Note the preliminary positive twitch at the fourth response after application. 


and abolition induced by the rene. of a 5 per cent. 
solution of hydrochloric acid. 

In dealing with the question of electrical response, we 
have seen that two opposed electrical effects occur in the 


EXCITABILITY UNDER CHEMICAL REAGENTS 139 


tissue subjected to stimulation. One of these is the positive 
effect, and the other, the true excitatory change of galvano- 
metric negativity. As the latter is, under normal conditions, 
predominant, the simultaneous effect of both is a resultant 
negativity. The positive effect may, however, be unmasked, 
as we have seen, by abolishing the true excitatory effect 
of negativity (p. 66). This positivity may also be un- 
masked, if, by the action of a chemical reagent, the time- 


Fic. 98. Photographic Record of Effect of 5 per cent. KHO 


Note the complete reversal of response to positive at the beginning, 
and its subsequent abolition. 


relations of the two responses are changed, so that instead of 
occurring simultaneously, the one is made to lag behind the 
other. This case will be seen very strikingly illustrated in 
fig. 97, which exhibits the effect of a 1 per cent. solution of 
KHO, on response to transmitted stimulation in the petiole 
of fern. In the normal responses here given, we observe 
the resultant response of galvanometric negativity. The 
application of KHO is first seen to reduce the excit- 
ability, as indicated by the reduced height of the responses. 
Later, we observe that the true excitatory effect is delayed. 


140 COMPARATIVE ELECT RO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Hence the positive effect is no longer completely masked. 
Its existence is now seen as a preliminary downward 
twitch in a di-phasic response, in the case of the fourth 
and succeeding records, after the application of KHO. In 
fig. 98, a stronger, namely a 5 per cent. solution of KOH, was 
used. And here, by the almost complete abolition of the 
excitatory factor, the response has undergone an apparent 
conversion to positive; this positive response is, however, 
subsequently abolished by the death of the plant. 


os 


Seer ae - 


CHAPTER XII 


VARIATIONS OF EXCITABILITY DETERMINED BY METHOD 
OF INTERFERENCE 


Arrangement for interference of excitatory waves—Effect of increasing difference 
of phase—Interference effects causing change from positive to negative, 
through intermediate di-phasic—Diametric balance—Effect of unilateral 
application of KHO—Effect of unilateral cooling. 


I HAVE explained how the variations of excitability brought 
about by various agencies may be determined, by recording 
the corresponding amplitudes of response, I shall now pro- 
ceed to describe a new and interesting method of making 
such determinations, by means of which it will be found 
possible to elucidate certain questions which without it must 
remain obscure. This method is, moreover, of extreme 
delicacy, enabling the investigator to detect the slightest 
variation of excitability, induced by any agent. 

Let two points in the experimental tissue, say A on the 
right, and B on the left, be suitably connected with the galva- 
nometer, and let the occurrence of excitation at A on the 
right be represented by an ‘up’ response record, the excita- 
tory effect at B, on the left, being represented as ‘down.’ 
If now the two points, A and B, be excited simultaneously, 
the resultant electrical response will be due to the algebraical 
summation of the two excitatory electro-motive effects E, and 
Ey, these standing for the individual electrical effects at the 
two points A and B, Now if the intensities of the two effects 
be the same, and if their time-relations be also the same, 
it is evident that these two excitatory electrical waves, being 
of equal amplitude and having the same ‘phase but of 
opposite signs, will, by their mutual interference, neutralise 


142 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


each other. Under such balanced conditions, therefore, on 
simultaneous excitation of A and B, the resultant response 
will be zero. If now, under the modifying action of any 
external agency, the excitability of A be enhanced, it is clear 
that the resultant response will be ‘up, showing the greater 
excitability of the right-hand point. A similar effect will 
also be produced if the excitability of B be depressed. | 
Similarly the depression of the excitability of A, or enhance- 
ment of B, would cause a resultant response which would be 
‘down.’ If, again, the two waves of excitation be not of the 
same phase, we shall obtain various di-phasic effects resulting 
from the algebraical summation of the constituent response- 
curves. The resultant zero-response may thus be converted 
into di-phasic, by the action of any agency which is capable 
of changing the time-relations of either of the constituent 
responses. 

_ I shall now proceed to describe the experimental arrange- 
ments by which two points in connection with E and E’ may 
be excited, and the resulting electrical disturbances made to 
interfere with each other. For this purpose we may use the 
vibrational stimulation which has already been described, 
with certain necessary additions .(fig. 99). The angle of 
torsional vibration which regulates the intensity of excitation 
is determined by two stops, P and Q. An elastic piece of 
brass, B, projects from the torsion-head. When a single 
stroke is given to this, a quick to-and-fro vibration is induced, 
the backward pull being supplied by the attached spring, s. 
The amplitude of this vibration remains always the same, as 
determined beforehand by the setting of the stops P and Q. 
The stroke is given by the striking-rod R, set in motion by 
the turning of a handle. What has already been said about 
the excitation of the right-hand side of the specimen applies 
equally to the left-hand, arrangements for the purpose being 
a duplicate of those just described. After deciding on a 
suitable angle of torsional vibration for the right, and taking 
the response at that point, we proceed to adjust the torsional 
angle on the left, so that the response there may be exactly 


EXCITABILITY DETERMINED BY INTERFERENCE 143 


the same as that on the right. If the excitability of the two 
points had been exactly the same, equal amplitudes of vibra- 
tion would have resulted in the equal stimulation of both. 
But in practice the excitabilities are found to be slightly 
different and the angle of vibration of the one must, therefore, 
be so adjusted as to induce an excitatory effect exactly equal 
to that of the other. 

The two striking-rods, one on the right, R, and the other 
on the left, R’, can be adjusted so that both are in the same 


E 
a 


a 


us 


rt ie 
B 
\ 


4 P 


ee 


< 

= 
Nee” : 
Fic. 99. R, R’, striking-rods for stimulation of two ends of specimen ; 


B, elastic brass tongue projecting from torsion-head. For producing 
phase-difference R is adjustable in azimuth. . 


vertical plane, or so that one is in advance of the other. 
The left rod is permanently fixed to the rotating axis, but 
the right can be set at any angle that is desired, with the 
other. When the right striking-rod is set, pointing to zero of 
the scale, the two rods are in the same vertical plane, and the 
rotation of the handle causes equal vibrational stimulus by 
the two at the same moment. The excitatory reactions on 
right and left are now, therefore, of the same phase and of 
equal intensity, but opposed to each other. In fig. 100, a, 
are reproduced the two separate and equal constituent 
responses given by a specimen of stem of Amaranth. The 


144 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


‘down’ curve was given by the individual excitation of the 
left, and the ‘up’ by the right. On the simultaneous excita- 
tion of the two points, the resultant response was zero (6). 
But if the excitation of one—say, the right—be increased by 
increasing the angle of vibration, the resultant differential 
response is found to be ‘up.’ It is obvious that’ a similar 
effect would have been observed had the stimulation of the 
right been kept the same, while its excitability was increased . 
by any external agent. In these cases we have two opposed 
excitatory waves of similar phase, and of the same or unequal 
intensities, interfering with each other. 


Fic. 100. (a) Isolated response of left side (down) and right side (up) ; 
(4) null-effect when excitations are simultaneous ; (c), (d@), (e) di-phasic 
responses obtained with increasing difference of phase. 


We shall next take some simple instances in which, while 
the stimulation is maintained constant, there is an increasing 
difference of phase. If the right-hand striking-rod R, instead 
of being set at zero, be set to the right, or at a p/us angle, 
the rotation of the handle will cause a slightly earlier excita- 
tion of the right than of the Jeft. If, on the other hand, the 
rod be set at a mznus angle, the excitation of the right will 
be later than that of the left. Under these circumstances, 
instead of the null-effect due to continuous balance, we shall 
have a di-phasic response. It is also clear that as the phase 
difference is increased, the neutralisation of -effects will 
become. less and less perfect, the separate constituent respon- 
ses being thus rendered increasingly apparent, In fig. 100, ¢, 


EXCITABILITY DETERMINED BY INTERFERENCE 145 


is seen the di-phasic effect which was induced when the 
excitation of the right was made to lag slightly behind that 
of the left, by the adjustment of the striking-rod at a 
small mznus angle. The first of the two twitches, which is 
downwards, indicates the relatively earlier excitation of the 
left-hand contact. As the phase-difference was increased 
progressively as in (d) and (e), it is seen that the constituent 
elements of the di-phasic response are increased corre- 
spondingly. It is also clear from this that, having obtained 
the null-effect, if any agents were afterwards applied locally 
which would make the excitation of the one point earlier 
than that of the other, we must then expect the null-effect 
to be modified to di-phasic. An earlier ‘up’ twitch would 
now indicate that the right-hand contact, having had its re- 
action quickened, was the first to respond ; an earlier ‘down’? 
twitch the opposite. 3 

We thus see how the conversion of the null-effect into a 
resultant ‘down’ negative or ‘up’ positive, could .be utilised 
as a test of the excitatory or depressing nature of a given 
reagent. We alsosee how the conversion of this null into a 
di-phasic effect would give us indications as to the change 
of time-relations induced by the reagent. I shall here, 
before going on to describe the results obtained with plants, 
give a photographic record (fig. 101) of certain positive, nega- 
tive, and di-phasic effects obtained in the electrical response 
of the inorganic substance, tin, under appropriate modification 
of the excitability of its two contacts by various chemical 
reagents.’ 

Turning now to the question of the determination of 
the effects of the various reagents by the Method of 
Interference, we may, as we have seen, cause simultaneous 
excitation of right and left, by means of the apparatus which 
has just been described, and which I shall distinguish as the 
Longitudinal Balance. There is, again, another and simple 
method of accomplishing the same object, by means, namely, 
of the Diametric Balance, the diagram of which has already 

1 Bose, Response in the Living and Non-Living, p. 115. 
L 


146 ' COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


been given in fig. 80. Using this arrangement, the specimen 
is clamped at one end, the vibration-head being at the other. 
Electrical connections are now made with the two dia- 
metrically opposite points, A and B, of which one, say A, is 
the upper, and B the lower. Ina tissue -which is isotropic, 
vibrational stimulus will induce equal and simultaneous 
excitation at the two points A and B. The effect of any 
given agency is tested by applying it locally, say at A, and 
observing the resultant variation of the response. I shall 


(a) (4) (c) 
Fic. 101. Photographic Record showing Negative, Di-phasic, and 
Positive Resultant Responses in Tin under appropriate modifications 
of excitation of the two contacts 


here give examples of results obtained by both these methods, 
thus affording an indication of the extent of their applicability 
in various investigations. , 

We have seen in the previous chapter that the application 
of strong solution of potash will abolish the excitability of 
a tissue. Using the Longitudinal Balance, I took a petiole 
of Bryophyllum and first made such adjustments that the 
right ‘up’ and left ‘down’ responses were almost equal. 
On now producing simultaneous excitation of the two ends, 
a di-phasic response was obtained, due to the fact that the 
left-hand point was the quicker to respond. Strong solution 


EXCITABILITY DETERMINED BY INTERFERENCE 147 


of potash was next applied on the right-hand point, and 
from the record it is seen that the ‘up’ part of the di-phasic 
response, due to the excitation of the right-hand side, was 
thus completely abolished, the ‘down’ response being at the 

| same time increased. by the 
suppression of this opposing 
response (fig. 102). 

In order to demonstrate the 
use of the Diametric Balance 
Method, I undertook to investi- 
gate by its means the influence 
of the lowering of tempera- 
ture on excitability. For this 


Fic. 103. Photographic Record of 
Response of Petiole of Cauliflower 
Fic. 102. Photographic Records. by the Diametric Method 
(a) Di-phasic response of petiole A contact was naturally more excitable, 
of Bryophyllum, the up compo- hence resultant ‘up’-response. Ex- 


nent being due to the excitation citability of A being depressed by 
of right side. Strong application local application of ice, the re- 
of KHO on the right abolished sultant response became converted 
this responsive component, giving to ‘down’; normal ‘up’-response 
rise in (4) to enhanced down was restored on allowing the tissue 


response to return to surrounding temperature. 


purpose, I took a petiole of cauliflower. In this instance, 
the natural excitability of the upper contact, A, was greater 
than that of the lower, B. Hence the resultant response was 
not zero, but ‘up.’ The point A was now cooled locally by 
ice. This process so lowered its excitability that that of B 
was now relatively the greater, hence the resultant response 
was found to be reversed or ‘down. The point A was next 


L:2 


148 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


allowed to return to the surrounding temperature of the room, 
records of the response being taken meanwhile, at intervals 
of one minute. It will be seen how, by means of the gradual 
restoration of the original excitability of A, the resultant 
response changes gradually from negative to zero, and then 
again from zero back to positive, indicating the restoration 
of the naturally greater excitability of A (fig. 103). 

We have thus studied two different methods, both of 
which depend on interference, for the determination of the 
variations of excitability induced by different external agents. 
In a subsequent chapter we shall study a modification of this 
method, by means of which it is possible to demonstrate the 
variations not only of excitability but also of conductivity 
under various reagents. 

e 


CHAPTER XIII 
CURRENT OF INJURY AND NEGATIVE VARIATION 


Different theories of current of injury—Pre-existence theory of Du Bois- 
Reymond—Electrical distribution in a muscle-cylinder—Electro-molecular 
theory of Bernstein—Hermann’s Alteration Theory—Experiments demon- 
strating that so-called current of injury is a persistent after-effect of over- 
stimulation—Residual galvanometric negativity of strongly excited tissue— 
Distribution of electrical potential in vegetable tissue with one end 
sectioned—Electrical distribution in plant-cylinder similar to that in muscle- 
cylinder—True significance of response by negative variation—Apparent 
abnormalities in so-called current of injury—‘ Positive’ current of injury. 


IF a section be made of an uninjured nerve or muscle, the 
transverse contact will be found to be galvanometrically 
negative, as compared with an uninjured longitudinal contact. 
I shall have occasion in the present chapter to give a simple 
explanation of this phenomenon and of the excitatory nega- 
tive variation of the current of injury. It is, therefore, only 
necessary to recapitulate briefly the three theories which have 
hitherto been proposed on this subject. 

The Pre-extstence Theory of Du Bois-Reymond supposed 
that the smallest particle had the same electro-motive 
characteristics as the entire tissue, each such electro-motive 
molecule consisting of two bi-polar portions, the positive 
poles of any two molecules being always face to face with 
each other. This theory was based upon the fact that a 
muscle-cylinder, for example, exhibited a peculiar distribu- 
tion of electrical tension. There are in such a cylinder, one 
longitudinal and two transverse surfaces. Midway in the 
cylinder is the equatorial zone of the longitudinal surface, 
and this zone is positive to all the rest. Thus the electro- 
motive difference between one electrode placed on the 


I50 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


equator, and the other, is increased as the latter is moved 
further and further away, say towards the right transverse 
section. The distribution 
of electrical tension on 
the left side of the 
equator is symmetrical 
with this (fig. 104). On 
these facts was based the 
theory of Du _ Bois- 
Reymond; but this has 


Fic. 104. Distribution of Electrical : me ‘nite hed 
Tension in Muscle-cylinder. since been found to be in- 


adequate. I skall later 
return to the explanation of the particular distribution of 
electrical tension involved. 


According to the theory of Bernstein, known as the 
Electro-chemical Molecular Theory, the fundamental attribute 
of the molecule is chemical. Its poles are supposed to attach 
to themselves electro-negative groups of atoms, while its 
sides attach oxygen, and stimulation is supposed to be 
attended by explosive chemical changes. 

According to Hermann’s A/teration Theory, finally, all the 
_ electro-motive activities of living tissues are supposed to be due 
to chemical rather than molecular changes of the substance. 
In amplification of this theory, Hering attributes all electro- 
motive phenomena to the disturbance of equilibrium by 
up and down chemical changes. 

It is my intention to show in the course of the present 
chapter that the current of injury is an after-effect of 
over-stimulation. And since excitation is fundamentally 
due to molecular upset, we shali best understand the 
electro-motive changes concomitant with it, if we first study 
it and its after-effect under the simplest conditions, namely 
those of inorganic substances. For here the action of such 
complicating factors as assimilation and dissimilation is 
clearly out of the question. 

We have found for example that a piece of well-annealed 
wire was iso-electric throughout its length. In the first 


CURRENT OF INJURY AND NEGATIVE VARIATION I5I 


place, when a portion of it was subjected to any molecular 
disturbance, an electro-motive difference was induced, as 
between the molecularly disturbed or excited and the un- 
disturbed areas. The intensity of this electro-motive change, 
in the second place, was seen to increase with intensity 
of excitation. And, thirdly, the recovery from excitation 
was seen to be delayed, where the intensity of stimulus 
was strong (fig. 105). This is shown in the electrical 


° 


° ° me) ° -o -° 
5 Io Is 20 25 30 35 45 


Fic. 105. Photographic Record showing Persistent Electrical After-Effect 
in Inorganic Substance under Strong Stimulation. Note the tilt of 
base-line upwards 


The vertical line to the right represents ‘1 volt. 


response of tin as a persistent after-effect, the sign of 
which is the same as that of the excitatory electro-motive 
change. 

A similar state of things is exhibited mechanically in a 
torsioned wire. When the torsion is moderate, and the 
molecular distortion slight, the released wire quickly re- 
covers its original position of equilibrium. But when the 
torsion is excessive and the wire strained beyond a certain 
limit, it remains for a long time in a torsioned condition, 


152 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


even after it has been set free. Recovery is thus, in such 
a case, indefinitely delayed. In other words, a molecularly 
over-strained substance exhibits a persistent after-effect. 
Turning next to plant response, we find a similar per- 
sistence of the after-effect to occur in consequence of over- 
stimulation. And first we shall take the simplest case— 
that in which the tissue is directly stimulated. Here the 
specimen was petiole of cauliflower, and increasing stimuli 


24° eC 74° 10° 123° 


Fic. 106. Photographic Record exhibiting Persistent Galvanometric 
Negativity in Plant Tissue after Strong Stimulation 


Stimuli applied at intervals of three minutes. Vertical line = ‘1 volt. 


were applied, at intervals of three minutes, by means of a 
gradually increasing angle of torsional vibration. It will be 
noticed that whereas the electrical recovery from moderate 
stimulation—as seen in the first of the series—is complete, 
it becomes, with increasing stimulus, more and more in- 
complete (fig. 106). In other words, the tissue, after strong 
stimulation, is seen to exhibit an after-effect of residual 
galvanometric negativity, which is really due to incomplete 
molecular recovery, in consequence of over-strain. 

In the cases just given, stimulation was applied directly. 


CURRENT OF INJURY AND NEGATIVE VARIATION 153 


We shall now, however, take an instance in which excitation 
is transmitted and observe the persistent negative after-effect, 
due to strong stimulation. We know that a cut (mechanical 
section) or the application of a hot wire (thermal section) 
acts as a strong stimulus, and the effective intensity of such 
stimulation will obviously decrease with increasing distance 
from the point of stimulation. Hence, if we observe the 
persistent excitatory change of galvanometric negativity, 
which is induced as between an indifferent point—say, the 
surface of a leaf—and points increasingly near to the zone 
of section, we shall find that the electro-motive change 
is greatest at the point of section, and is progressively 
lessened as we recede from it. This induction may be 
verified experimentally by taking readings of the persistent 
negativity, as between an indifferent point, B, and points 
such as the contacts a, 0, c, d, A (fig. 107), which are further 
and further removed from the point of section. For this 
purpose we may employ a capillary electrometer, whose indica- 
tions are independent of the varying resistance of the interposed 
tissue. The magnifying power of the observing microscope 
was so adjusted that ‘1 volt gave a reading of 100 divisions 
of the micrometer. In carrying out an experiment on the 
leaf of Colocasza I found the electrical distribution, as between 
an indifferent point on the lamina and points on the 
sectioned petiole, at increasing distances from the section, 
to be as shown in the following table : 


TABLE SHOWING ELECTRICAL DISTRIBUTION IN SPECIMEN OF Co/ocasia. 


[The sectioned end was negative. 100 divisions = ‘1 volt. | 


Tiintaie thom section E.M. difference between indifferent 
and given points 


*5 cm. 50 divisions 
I 99 40 ” 
2 9 33 99 
3 9° 29 99 
4 


99 27 cd 


It will thus be seen that points near the sectioned end are 
more negative than others further away. 


154 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


I shall next describe the more sensitive galvanometric 


method of investigation. The resistance is here maintained 
constant by having the permanent. contacts at A and the 
indifferent point B (fig. 107). The specimen is a stem of 
Calotropis gigantea. A thermal section is made at first, say, 
at a distance of 3 cm. from A. The persistent galvanometric 


negativity of A will now be due to the after-effect of stimula- 
tion by section. The thermal injury is now repeatcd at | 


LL 


of ih 
cai 5 


3 2 15 3 
Fic. 107. Experimental Arrange- Fic. 108. Records showing in- 
ment for determining Electrical creasing Persistent Galvano- 
Effect due to Section metric Negativity, according 


as injury is caused nearer to 
proximal contact A, z.e. moved 
from 3 to'5 cm, distance 


decreasing distances from A. I give a series of records 
(fig. 108), from which it will be seen that when the stimulus 
of thermal section occurs at some distance, there is no 


persistent after-effect, recovery being complete. But as the 


injury is made nearer and nearer A, the permanent after- 


effect becomes greater and greater. From observations made . 


in the course of a similar experiment, I obtained the following 
results, given in tabular form, which show the increasing 
value, with lessening distance, of this persistent galvano- 
metric negativity. 


eee 


CURRENT OF INJURY AND NEGATIVE VARIATION 155 


TABLE SHOWING PERMANENT GALVANOMETRIC NEGATIVITY AT 
DIFFERENT DISTANCES FROM POINT OF INJURY, 


Distance from section Galvanometric negativity 
25 cm. 220 divisions 
5» 180; 
IO ,, 120 os 
| 5% FINS | 
2°0 35, 49 ” | 
| 370 5, inst Sb | 


In fig. 109 we have a curve which illustrates these results, 
and explains why the maximum negativity is at the zone 
of section, diminishing rapidly as we recede from it. It is 
obvious that if these sections had been made to the right as 


Fic. 109. Curve showing the Electrical Distribution in Stem with 
one Sectioned End 


Ordinate represents galvanometric negativity ; abscissa, the distance 
from sectioned end. 


well as to the left of A, the result would have been a duplicate 
series of changes of galvanometric negativity in reference 
to A, on. that side also. Such a series is represented in 
fig. 110, by means of dotted lines. It will also be seen from 
this figure that the greatest electro-motive difference exists 


156 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


as between the equatorial point A and the two terminal 
sections a and a’; that symmetrical points cc’, 0 0’, a a’, are 
equipotential ; and that a point relatively nearer the terminal 
section is galvanometrically negative, in reference to one 
further away from it. It will also be seen that this electrical 
distribution is exactly the same as that seen in a muscle- 
cylinder, with terminal sections, as given in fig. 104. 

Thus, without the postulation of any electro-motive mole- 
cules so-called, these experimental results afford a simple 
and direct explanation of the so-called current of injury, as 
the excitatory after-effect of strong stimulation. 


Fic. 110. Electrical Distribution in Plant-cylinder with Opposite 
Ends Sectioned 


The Current of Injury is simply therefore an excitatory 
after-effect, due to incomplete recovery from over-strain. 
But even after strong stimulation a slow recovery may occur, 
and the Current of Injury will thus undergo a progressive 
diminution. This will probably account for Engelmann’s 
observation that in medullated nerves the E.M.F. of the 
artificial cross-section fell, by as much as from 25 to 60 
per cent., in the first two hours after section, and disappeared 
altogether within twenty-four. The renewal of the cross- 
section he found to renew the original difference. This is 
an obvious case of renewal of the effect by re-stimulation. 

The disappearance of negativity, owing to recovery, only 


CURRENT OF INJURY AND NEGATIVE VARIATION 157 | 


takes place when the injury has not been excessive. If, 
however this has been too great, the injured tissue will then 
pass gradually into a condition of permanent death. But 
the electrical change concomitant with death is one of 
positivity, as I shall show in the next chapter. Thus the 
subsidence of the galvanometric negativity of an injured point 
may be brought about by either of two processes, which are 
exactly opposite—namely, recovery or death. 

Turning next to the subject of the Negative Variation 
of an existing current of rest, as a reliable index to the state 
of excitation, two different questions arise. First: why, in 
order to obtain response to diffuse stimulation, is it necessary 
previously to subject one of the contacts to injury? And 
secondly: why is the responsive action-current opposite in 
direction to the resting-current, thus constituting a negative 
variation of it? 

With reference to the first of these questions, we have 
already seen that when two points, A and B, are simul- 
taneously excited, the resultant electro-motive response is 
equal to E,—E,. If, then, the excitabilities of these two 
points are the same, it is clear that the resultant response 
will be zero. From this we can see that, in order to obtain 
a resultant response, we must depress or abolish the excita- 
bility of one of the two contacts. 

This inference may be verified by the employment of the 
Method of Block and of longitudinal balance. Two equal 
and opposite responses are first obtained at A and B. Then 
one end, say B, is injured by thermal section. The specimen 
being now replaced in the vibratory apparatus, it is found 
that, whereas the A half gives strong response, the end B 
gives none. Or the B end of the specimen may be injured 
by a few drops of strong potash, the other end remaining 
uninjured. The end A is then stimulated, and a strong 
response is obtained. The end B is next stimulated, and 
there is little or no response. The block between A and B is 
now removed, and the specimen stimulated throughout its 
length. Though the stimulus now acts on both contacts, 


158 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


yet, owing to the irresponsive condition of B, there is a 
resultant response, and the direction of this action-current 
is found to be from A to B. We have thus experimentally 
verified the assumption that in the same tissue an uninjured 
portion will be thrown into a greater excitatory state than 
an injured, by the action of the same stimulus. ; 

When the point B is injured, there is generally speaking 
a more or less persistent current set up which flows from | 
Bto A. But we saw that the direction of the action-current 
was opposite—that is to say, from Ato B. This will explain 
the reason why the action-current causes a diminution or 
negative variation of the current of injury, so called. One 
method of doing this is to cause injury to one of the two 
points. If this be such as to kill the tissue, then its 
excitability is permanently abolished. Or by causing the 
excessive stimulation of injury, we may simply depress the 
excitability of the tissue for a longer or shorter time. 

I shall now give a few instances of response in plants by 
negative variation, Taking the petiole of turnip, we injure an 
area on its surface, say B. A current is now observed to 
flow in the petiole from the injured B to the uninjured A. 
The induced difference of potential depends on the condition 
of the plant, and the season. In the experiment here 
described, its value was ‘13 volt. A sharp mechanical tap 
was now given to the petiole, between A and B, and a sudden 
diminution, or negative variation, of current occurred, the 
resting potential difference being decreased by ‘026 volt. A 
second and stronger tap induced a second response, causing 
a greater diminution of potential difference by ‘047 volt. 

In another experiment, the specimen employed was a 
petiole of cauliflower (Brassica oleracea). ‘The first up-line 
to the right indicates the current of injury. The three re- 
sponses which succeed are induced by a given intensity of 
stimulus, the next series of six, being in response to stimulus 
nearly twice as strong, exhibit signs of fatigue (fig. 111). 

The current of injury generally undergoes a diminution 
with time. This is often, as has been explained, on account 


CURRENT OF INJURY AND NEGATIVE VARIATION 159 


of slow recovery from the excessive stimulation of injury. 
Response by negative variation is then found to undergo a 
decline. It is in general vaguely accepted that, in order to 
obtain a response by negative variation an‘antecedent current 
of injury is necessary, by whose induced variation we may 
be able to record responsive effects. In cases of the dis- 
appearance of the current of injury, it is supposed that 
response must necessarily vanish, since its antecedent con- 
dition no longer exists. But I have already shown, and shall 


sayy 


Fic. 111. Record of Responses in Plant (Leaf-stalk of Cauliflower) 
by Method of Negative Variation 


The first three records are for stimulus intensity I ; the next six are for in- 
tensity twice as strong; the successive responses exhibit fatigue. The 
vertical line to the left represents ‘I volt. The record is to be read 
from right to left. 


have occasion again to show in the next chapter, that these 
suppositions are altogether erroneous. For we may obtain the 
usual response when the current of. injury is zero, or even 
positive. In fact, the only essential condition for the obtaining 
of resultant response is that at one contact the excitability 
should be in a state of relative depression. 

In that case in which response becomes enfeebled, with 
the gradual decline and vanishing of the current of injury, 
a simple explanation is often applicable. When the tissue is 
injured, it does not necessarily die. In fact, I have often 


160 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO- PHYSIOLOGY 


found that, in order to ensure death—in the case for instance 
of thermal section—a prolonged application of the fatal 
temperature is necessary. In ordinary cases of injury caused 
by the application of heat, I find that we have merely exces- 
sive stimulation of the point, with depression of excitability. 
But after a long interval, excitability is more or less restored, 
with the gradual passing away of the effect of injury. The 
subsidence of the current of injury thus also denotes the 
restoration of excitability to a greater or less extent. Hence 
that differential action between the uninjured and injured 
contacts, which determines the amplitude of the resultant 
response, will become correspondingly diminished. And 
when response has undergone diminution from this cause, 
a fresh injury is found to renew its amplitude. This is due 
to the reduction of excitability now freshly brought about at 
one of the contacts. € 

There are, however, two other additional factors which 
may further contribute to the enhancement of response after 
a recent injury. We have seen that, as a general rule, the 
resultant response will be E,—£E, where E, means the 
excitatory electrical change induced at A, and E, that induced 
at B. It would therefore appear that this value will be at its 
maximum when the excitability of B is totally abolished by 
reason of injury, the resultant effect being due to the 
unopposed electrical excitation at A. But we have seen 
that when the true excitatory negative variation of a point 
is abolished, it may nevertheless exhibit a positive electrical 
variation, due to hydro-positive action. When this happens 
to be the case, this positive effect at B, conspiring with the 
true excitatory effect at A, may bring about a response larger 
than we should have supposed to be maximum. 

Again, though over-stimulation of a point diminishes its 
excitability, yet moderate stimulation often enhances it. 
The effect of this, in enhancing resultant response, is well seen 
in the case of conducting nerves. Thus, when the point B is 
injured, the excitation caused by injury reaches A, and causes 
moderate stimulation of that point. As an after-effect of this 


CURRENT OF INJURY AND NEGATIVE VARIATION IOI 


moderate stimulation, A often becomes more than normally 
excitable.! It is thus seen how, after a recent injury, these 
two factors—of a hydro-positive effect at the injured, and of 
increased excitability at the uninjured contact, in consequence 
of moderate transmitted stimulation—may act to enhance the 
response. 

We have seen that the common effect of injury is to 
induce a galvanometric negativity of the point injured. We 
have further seen that in such a case the response to external 
stimulus is by a negative variation of the current of injury. 
We have next, then, to take up various instances which appear 
highly anomalous, cases, that is to say, in which the injured 
point, relatively to the uninjured, is, for some hitherto 
unknown reason, galvanometrically positive. As a result 
of this and other causes, there are, in addition to the cases 
already described in a previous chapter, instances in which 
response is found to take place, not by a negative, but by 
a positive, variation of the current of rest or of injury as 
the case may be. 

The first point to be considered in connection with such 
abnormal responses is whether the experimental tissue is 
physiologically isotropic, that is to say, of equal excitability 
throughout, or anisotropic, possessed of unequal excitabilities 
at different points. The discussion of the first of these 
cases, the isotropic, I propose to defer to the following chapter. 
The anisotropic will be touched upon here, though its detailed 
consideration will be entered upon in the next. 

As an example of the anisotropic organ, we may take 
the pulvinus of MWzmosa, in which the lower side is more 
excitable than the upper. In animal tissues also, such aniso- 
tropy is not uncommon. For example, we may have a 
_ muscular tissue terminating in a glandular. Owing to this 
anisotropy, the muscular and glandular surfaces are unequally 
excitable, and it will be shown in a later chapter that, 
generally speaking, it is the glandular which exhibits more 
intense excitatory galvanometric negativity. When such a 

‘ For further details, see Chapter XLII. 
M 


162 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


preparation is made, by cutting across the muscle, it is found 
that an electrical current flows from the uninjured gland to 
the injured muscle. From this it has been supposed that 
such a current was not the current of injury at all, but 
something of an unknown nature, essentially different. The 
consequent perplexity is the result of a failure to understand 
on the one hand that there is no such thing as a current of 
injury fer se, except as the after-effect of strong stimulation, 
and on the other, that the current induced in the tissue is 
always from the more excited to the less excited. In the 
present case of muscle-and-gland preparation, the excessive 
stimulation due to section becomes diffused all over the 
tissue, and since the glandular surface is the more excitable, 
its excitatory galvanometric negativity is greater than that 
of the sectioned muscle, which thus becomes relatively positive. 
We have here a striking demonstration of the necessity for 
regarding the electrical reaction as the sign, not of injury, but 
of the excitation caused by injury. In the case described, 
for instance, the physical injury is obviously incapable of 
transmission, and it is the consequent excitation which is con- 
ducted to the gland. 
_ The account of an experiment on a sensitive leaf of 
Mimosa will serve to elucidate the foregoing argument. If 
one contact, A, be made with the upper half of the pulvinus, 
and the other, C, with a distant and indifferent point, then, on 
giving a prick near A, we shall find that that contact, owing 
to excitation by injury, becomes galvanometrically negative. 
If, next, we make two contacts at diametrically opposite 
points of the pulvinus, A on the upper, and B on the lower, 
surfaces, it will then be found, on causing injury at the upper 
point A, that that point, relatively to B, becomes galvano- 
metrically positive. This is because the stimulus caused bv 
the injury has become diffused throughout the pulvinus, witn 
the effect of causing greater excitation and consequent 
sreater galvanometric negativity at the more excitable B. 

It has been seen that a mechanical or thermal section 
acts as a strong stimulus. It has also been shown that 


CURRENT OF INJURY AND NEGATIVE VARIATION 163 


recovery from a strong stimulus is very protracted. Hence, 
after such stimulation, there is persistent galvanometric 
negativity as an after-effect. As the intensity of this after- 
effect depends upon the intensity of stimulation, it will be 
seen that the galvanometric negativity near the section will 
be greater than at a distant point, where the transmitted 
effect of stimulation is feeble. From this it follows, that the 
so-called current of injury will flow in the tissue from the 
neighbourhood of the cut, to the distant and relatively un- 
excited end. The current of injury is thus an after-effect of 
strong stimulus. The peculiar electrical distribution which 
occurs in a muscle-cylinder is also found in a plant-cylinder, 
and both are equally explicable from the fact that the 
greatest excitatory after-effect occurs at the two sectioned 
ends, and that this decreases progressively towards the 
equator. The over-stimulated area of injury has its excit- 
ability depressed or abolished ; diffuse stimulation, causing 
sreater excitation of the uninjured contact, induces in it a 
greater excitatory effect of negativity, and this gives rise to a 
diminution of the existing difference of potential, as between 
the injured and uninjured. This is the explanation of 
response by the so-called negative variation. 

In an anisotropic tissue the excitation caused by injury, 
when diffused, induces greater galvanometric negativity of 
the more excitable part. If this be the distal end, the re- 
sultant persistent current will be from the distal uninjured to 
the proximal injured. An apparently anomalous case: will 
thus arise of a ‘ positive’ current of injury, so-called. 


CHAPTER XIV 


CURRENT OF DEATH—RESPONSE BY POSITIVE VARIATION 


Anomalous case of response by positive variation—Inquiry into the cause— 
Electric exploration of dying and dead tissue : death being natural—Determi- 
nation of electric distribution in tissue with one, end killed—Dying tissue 
shows maximum negativity, and dead tissue, positivity to living —Explanation 
of this peculiar distribution—Response by negative or positive variation, 
depending on degree of injury—Three typical cases—Explanation by theory of 
assimilation and dissimilation misleading— All response finally traceable to 
simple fundamental reactions. 


WE have seen in the last chapter that, in order to obtain 
response by negative variation, it is customary among 
investigators on animal physiology to kill one end of the 
experimental tissue, say by scalding. It is generally sup- 
posed also that dead tissue is negative to living. On stimu- 
lation, the induced negativity of the living contact, now 
superposed on the existing P.D. of the unilaterally killed 
tissue, causes a negative variation of it. This mode of investi- 
gation, by means of the negative variation, is one which has 
hitherto, as we have seen, been universally regarded as reliable. 

In the course of my investigations on the response of 
vegetable tissues, by this mode of negative variation, however, 
I have sometimes found response to take place by the positive 
variation. Taking, for example, a stem of Lalsam, I killed 
one end by immersion in boiling water. On now subjecting 


this to diffuse vibrational stimulus, the responsive action was- 


found to induce a positive variation of the existing current. 
On further investigation, I found that the excitatory electrical 
variation at the living contact had remained normal; that is 
to say, the direction of the responsive current was away 
from the excited living, and towards the killed end. I next 


a 


ee ee a 


Lt gO ae ee 


I 
Nae 


RESPONSE BY POSITIVE VARIATION 165 


found that the so-called ‘current of injury’ had in this case, 
owing to some hitherto unknown cause, undergone a reversal, 
and was now from the living to the dead, the latter being 
galvanometrically positive to the former, to the extent of 
‘08 volt. The abnormality of the response lay, then, in this 
fact, that the current of reference had become reversed, and 
that the responsive current, due to excitation, was now con- 
cordant with it, instead of antagonistic, thus constituting a 
positive variation (fig. 112). Later 
on, I discovered many instances in 
which the killed end was positive 
to the unkilled. Since, then, it is 
possible for the current of reference 
itself to undergo such obscure and 
spontaneous reversals, from un- 
known causes, it is easy to see 
how uncertain the study of re- 
sponsive phenomena must become, 
if we are to depend upon the 
negative variation as our only Fic, 112. Response by Positive 
reliable means for their investiga- Variation of Resting Current 
tion. I next, therefore, turned my acti a aaeys howe Py 
attention towards an inquiry into reversed, the killed end having 
the causes of these anomalous Dans cathe a aise si 
reversals. » 

The subject therefore resolved itself into an investigation 
as to what conditions determined the negativity or positivity 
of a tissue at the onset of death. My first attempt, then, was 
to study a case in which the approach of death was natural, 
and not the result of any sudden or violent change, such as 
might conceivably give rise to abnormal reactions. And in 


‘ my search for suitable specimens, I noticed that often, owing 


to local mal-nutrition or other causes, the leaves of plants 
exhibited spots or areas, from which, as centres, death pro- 
ceeded in constantly widening circles. Thus, in the leaves 
of Colocasia, for example, we find such dead and dying areas 
in otherwise fairly healthy leaves. The innermost of these 


166 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


patches may be quite dark and discoloured, while, as the 
living tissue is approached, this dark passes imperceptibly 
into yellow colour. And beyond this, again, we find the 
discolouration of yellow passing into the vivid green of living 
tissue. Proceeding thus in a radial direction inwards, towards 
the centre of such a patch from the living green, we shall 
find all possible stages of death, from its initiation, somewhere 
on the border-line between green and yellow, to its phase of 
completion, in the dark central area. On testing the electrical 
conditions of these different parts, I found that the border 
between green and yellow was negative to the living green 
surface. But the same point was also negative to the dead 
central area, and more negative to this than to the living 
tissue. Hence the dead was relatively positive to the living. 
Or if we make one fixed contact on the living tissue, and 
if the second exploring contact be made with various points 


_ successively on a radial line passing from this to the centre 


of the dead area, these contacts will pass in succession through 
the living, the dying, and the dead. The variation of elec- 
trical potential will be found to be at its greatest along this 
line. The electro-motive difference between the point which 
has been fixed on the living tissue, and the exploring second 
contact, will at first be found to increase. The maximum 
difference is attained on reaching the border-line between 
green and yellow, or very little beyond this, this point being 
galvanometrically the most negative. On now passing further 
inward from this point, the maximum difference is found to 
decrease, till we come to a point in the dead tissue which is 
iso-electric with the living. On now again passing inwards, 
to the still more completely dead tissue of the central area, 
we find that we are approaching points which are more and 
more galvanometrically positive, as compared with the living 
tissue. The dying point on the border-line between green 
and yellow is thus the most negative, and points to the right 
or left of this are positive in comparison with it, the dead, 
however, being more positive than the living. It has been 
said that the electro-motive variation is most rapid along the 


CURRENT OF DEATH 167 


radial line. On the other hand, we obtain series of equi- 
potential surfaces whose outlines closely follow those of the 
boundaries of the different degrees of discolouration. 
[shall next proceed to give quantitative measurements. 
The first point to be considered is that of the choice of a 
definite electrical level, which is to be used as a standard. 
If this point be selected in the living tissue, we shall find that 
our standard of comparison is extremely variable, since the 
tonic condition, on which its electrical level depends, is itself 
subject to change. The only condition which cannot be 
modified in any way is that of complete death. This may 
be taken, then, as the standard level. The method of experi- 
ment will thus consist in selecting a series of equidistant points, 
abcd,and so on, 5 mm. apart, along a radial line, passing 
outwards from the central area, which is completely dead, to 
the green tissue. The non-polarisable contacts E and E’ are 
first placed on a and 4, then on @ and ¢, ¢c and d, and so forth. 
The external circuit contains a high resistance, compared with 
which any difference of resistance, as between any 5 mm. of 
interposed tissue, becomes negligible. Hence, the successive 
deflections of the galvanometer indicate the electro-motive 
difference that exists between a and @, 0 and «¢, and so on. 

One difficulty which is experienced, in these measure- 
ments of small electro-motive differences, lies in securing the 
iso-electric condition of the non-polarisable electrodes them- 
selves. Whatever precautions are taken in the construction of 
these, a small electro-motive difference will sometimes be 
found to exist between them. The existence of such a 
difference is easily tested by bringing the kaolin ends of the 
two electrodes in contact, or by dipping both of them close 
together in a vessel of normal saline solution. Any electro- 
motive difference of the electrodes, however small, will now 
give rise to a large galvanometric deflection. 

This difficulty may be overcome by first taking special 
precautions as to the purity of zinc rods and the chemicals 
employed, and secondly, by keeping the electrodes for 
a long time short-circuited, with their ends dipped in normal 


168 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


saline. In very obstinate cases, however, I succeeded in 
eliminating all differences by subjecting the electrodes to 
cyclic variations of alternating electro-motive force. By 
means of a Pohl’s commutator, without cross-bars, the 
electrodes were put in connection with an alternating source 
of E.M.F., and with the galvanometer intended to test 
the resulting variation in the E.M. difference, by turns. — 
A small hand-driven alternating-current generator was used 
for this purpose. The speed of rotation of this machine 
was gradually raised to a maximum, and afterwards as 
gradually slowed down. Thus at each cycle the electrodes 
were subjected to ascending and descending intensities of 
alternating electro-motive variations. The effect of such cyclic 
changes, in diminishing the existing electro-motive difference 
between a pair of electrodes, specially selected for carelessness 
of preparation, will be clearly seen from the following tabular 


results: 

Condition at starting Galvanometric deflection E.M. difference 
Original difference . , 360 divisions 009 volt 
After first cycle : F 40 Js 52?) 
After second cycle . ; fe) ie One, 
After third cycle. ; fe) - ee 


It will thus be seen that, after a very short time of this treat- 
ment, the two electrodes were rendered iso-electric. 

I next proceeded to determine the distribution of elec- 
trical potential in the various portions, living and dead, of 
the leaf, In order to remove any accidental strain, the leaf 
was placed in tepid water, and kept there for about half an 
hour, till the water was cooled to the surrounding temperature. 
The experiment was then carried out, in the manner already 
described, and the following tabular statement shows the 
results obtained. The electrodes, it will be remembered, were 
placed successively at points 5 mm. apart from each other, 
along a radial line proceeding from the dead tissue to the 
living, the first point being taken as zero; 


CURRENT OF DEATH 169, 


* Position of Electrodes | Calapaner Deflection 
| o— 5mm. e) division 
5 ane ei E. =~ 1D 9 
IO i568 5 igs e520. 1 255 
15 — 20 55 ® i 45 9 
20— 25 ,, 225400055 
25— 39 5, — 140 29 
30 — 40 ,, | + TIO +95 
40 — 45 9 ‘+ 20 5) 


It will be observed that as we proceed from the dead to 
the dying, the negativity of the latter rapidly increases, the 
maximum being at 30 mm. from the zero-point taken on the 


5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 


| 
: HM vr 
Wh 


te 


tI 


400 ee 


Fic. 113. Distribution of Electric Potential in Lamina of Co/ocaséa along 
a radial line from dead to living through intermediate stages. Ab- 
scissa gives distance in mm. from chosen centre in dead tissue, ordinate 
represents galvanometric negativity in divisions. Dead tissue repre- 
sented dark, dying shaded, and living white. 


dead tissue. This point of maximum-negativity almost 
coincides with the visible border-line between the yellow 
and the green. Beyond this, however, there is an electrical 


170 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


reversal, the living becoming increasingly positive, as com- 
pared with the dying. An inspection of the curve (fig. 113) 
shows that while there is a point in the tissue between the 
dying and the dead, which is equipotential with the living, 
the completely dead tissue is positive to the living. 

I next carried out an experiment in which death was 
artificially induced, by immersing a portion of the tissue in 
boiling water. In connection with this, I may say that it is 
extremely difficult to ensure the complete death of a thick — 
tissue. It is only the outside layers which undergo death 
easily, but the interior tissues, from their protected position, 
are extremely resistant, and it is only after prolonged 
immersion in boiling water that death can really be ensured 
throughout. In the present experiment, however, where 
only a part of the tissue is to be killed, such prolonged 
immersion would cause death to encroach upon those 
portions of the tissue which were intended to be kept 
alive. This difficulty was met by choosing a specimen, 
the inside of which was accessible to boiling water. The 
peduncle of the water-lily (Nymphea alba) in transverse 
section appears extremely reticulated, and there is thus no 
difficulty in exposing all its parts to the direct action of the 
hot water. 

The upper end of the peduncle was kept surrounded by 
a cloth moistened in ice-cold water, the lower end being 
immersed in boiling water for ten minutes. The specimen 
was then placed.in tepid water, and allowed to cool down 
slowly. In this way a length of the peduncle was ob- 
tained, in which one end was completely killed, whereas 
the other remained fully alive, the intermediate portions 
showing all stages of the transition from the living to 
the dead condition. In order to determine the electrical 
distribution in its different parts, I now employed the 
potentiometer method of balance. One electrode was per- 
manently connected with that dying point which by a 
previous test had been found to exhibit maximum nega- 
tivity. The second electrode was placed at successive points, 


I eS Ee oS 


CURRENT OF DEATH 171 


each of which was nearer than the last by 5 mm. to the dead 
end, which was to the left. The same process was now re- 
peated, the successive | 
readings however being 
taken towards the right 
or living end. At each 
point, the electro-motive 
difference was balanced 
by the potentiometer. 
This straight form of 
potentiometer had a 


Fic. 114. Straight Form Potentiometer 
scale divided into one x8 isa stretched wire with added resistances, 


Rand R’. Sis a storage cell. When the 
thousand parts (fig. 114), key, K, is turned to the right, one scale 


and when its terminal division = ‘ooI volt, when turned to the 
. left one scale division = ‘or volt. P is the 
electro-motive force was plant. 


adjusted to 1 volt, each } 
division of the potentiometer was equal to ‘oor volt. The 
following table gives the results obtained : 


Distance from maximum . * 

Towards left or dead end, : : Towards right or living end 
; ; t t = ; ; , 
E.M. difference in yo55 volt “ee ee, iene C + (—) or E.M. difference in y;55 volt 

I*2 *5 cm 9 

5"0 FO 5, 2°5 

18°2 I"5 55 4°7 

22°0 rig! ae 6°8 

23°0 HES 10°0 

23°3 7 hee 12°8 

ee 3°55» I 5 6 

—— 4°O' 754 16°8 

a: 4°55 17°5 

— BD igs 18°'0 


Here, also, as in the case of natural death, we find a point 
in the dying tissue which is most negative. From the curve 
given in fig. 115, it will also be seen that as we pass away 
from this point in either direction towards the living or dead 
area, we find an increasing positivity ; the curve for the dead 
portion is, however, much steeper than that for the living. 
Thus two points, one 1'5 cm. to the left in the dead tissue, 
and another 5 cm. to the right in the living tissue, are 
iso-electric. But while the maximum positivity of the living 


172 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


is ‘O18 volt, that of the dead is 0233 volt. Hence the 
dead tissue is here positive to the living, to the extent of 
0053 volt. 

We have seen that the prevailing idea is that the dead is 
negative to the living. But from the results here shown, we 
can see that this is not a complete statement of the case. 
Since then the electro-motive variation, instead of showing a 


25° 


' 
‘ 
t 
i 
! 
i 
Pi ——— + -\ 1. Lesne= 
i] 
i 
i 
i 
i 
{ 
j 


he ee ee 


ew ee Rn ee we we ee oes = oe a «. 


2 ee me ee ee fe me me me ee ee ee 


oe ee ee ee ee es ee ee ee ee ee ee ee es eee ee es es 


he a a ee ee ee 


ee a Ca 


2 3 4 +e) 
Fic. 115. Distribution of Electric Potential in Petiole of Vymphea alba, 
one end of which has been killed. 


The point of maximum negativity is taken as zero, distances to the left 
or towards the dead taken as mznws, to the right or living, as Aplus. 
Ordinate represents potential difference in thousandths of a volt. 


progressive change from the living to the dead, exhibits a 
maximum difference, followed by a reversal, it may be asked, 
what is the reason of this anomaly ? 

Much light is thrown on this subject from the results 
given by another line of inquiry, to be explained in detail in 
Chapter XVI. _ It is there shown that the plant-tissue on the 
first onset of death exhibits a sudden contraction, indicative 


CURRENT OF DEATH B73 


of a strong excitatory reaction. This corresponds with the 
rigor mortts of the animal, and by means of suitable 
apparatus, the concomitant mechanical response can be 
recorded. An electrical record of the same phenomenon 
may also be obtained, in the form of an electrical spasm of 
galvanometric negativity. Succeeding to this rigor of the 
dying tissue, a post-mortem relaxation takes place, with 
a concomitant change from galvanometric negativity to 
positivity. 

Now in a tissue which has been killed unilaterally only, 
it will be understood that all possible gradations are to be 
expected. Passing from the completely dead to the fully 
_alive, we must necessarily pass through various zones, 
beginning with the abnormally relaxed, through the inter- 
mediate highly contracted. and rigored tissue on the death- 
frontier, to the living, which is not so contracted as the 
dying, and not so relaxed as the dead. At the point 
where the onset of death-.is recent, the rigor, or excitatory 
contraction and galvanometric negativity, are at their 
maximum, Compared with:this, the slightly tonically con- 
tracted living is positive, but not so positive as the abnormally 
relaxed dead. : 

The death-frontier, however, is not fixed. It is con- 
tinually encroaching on-the living. The line of maximum 
rigor and galvanometric negativity is thus also shifting in 
the same direction. Along with this, however, the opposite 
process of post-mortem relaxation is proceeding; so that a 
point which was, in consequence of rigor, maximally negative, 
becomes gradually converted to positive. 

This positivity of dead tissue as compared with living, 
which has here been demonstrated in the case of the plant, 
I find to be also true of animal tissue, in those cases which 
I have investigated. ‘Thus, while an injured.and dying area 
in a frog’s nerve is negative, an already dead area is 
positive, relatively to the living nerve. There is, moreover, 
an intermediate area, between the dying and dead portions 
of the nerve, which is iso-electric to the living, 


174 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Hence, having one contact fixed on a living area and 
the other on (1) the dying, (2) the intermediate, and (3) 
the dead tissue, we shall obtain three different types of 
what is known as the ‘injury-current.’ In the first of these 
the second contact will be negative, a condition which has 
hitherto been assumed to be the sole characteristic of the 
current of injury. But there are two other cases to be 
considered. Of these, when the second contact is made at 
a point intermediate between the dying and dead tissues, 
we shall find it to be iso-electric with the first, or living 
contact. And thirdly, when the second contact is on a 
dead area, the latter will be positive to the first, or living 
contact. We thus find three cases of the current of injury— 
the first being negative, the second zero, and the third 
positive. 

Taking the first of these—that in which the injured 
contact is negative—the action-current, in response to 
stimulus, will bring about a negative variation of the so- 
called current of injury. In the second, the result will be 
indeterminate, since the injury-current is zero. In the third, 
the response will be by a positive variation of the current of 
injury. 

I give below three photographic records in illustration of 
these three cases, obtained with vegetable nerve. I may state 
here that I have often observed results precisely similar in 
the case of frog’s nerve also. In the first record, in fig. 116, 
the thermal injury was moderate. The injured point was 
thus negative, and the current of injury is represented here 
by an up-line. The responses are seen to be by negative 
variation. In the second record the injury was greater, and 
the injured point was almost neutral; that is to say, on 
making contact there was a slight up-twitch, which subsided 
to zero. There is here, then, no current of injury. The 
subsequent responses are, however, down, the action-current 
being away from the living contact. In the third record 
the injury was so great as completely to kill the injured 
point, which thus became positive to the living. The 


RESPONSE BY POSITIVE VARIATION 175 


reversed injury-current is represented as down, the subse- 
quent excitatory responses are also down, and constitute a 
positive variation of the current of injury. 

It will thus be seen that an identical excitatory reaction 
of the living tissue appears to give rise to directly opposite 


Fic. 116. Photographic Records of Responses of Vegetable Nerve, 
one end of which has been injured 


In the first injury was slight; current of injury represented up, response 
by negative variation. In the second, injury greater; injured point 
neutral, response down. In the third, injured point killed; injury 
current reversed down, response by positive variation. 


effects—namely, a negative or a positive variation of the 
injury-current. : 

I give below a short summary of the diversities of 
response which may occur when either the natural, or the 
injury-current, is taken as the current of reference. 


(a) (6) 
< Cc >C 
B— m , Br —iD pj 
>R —>R 


Fic. 117. Typical Cases of Variation of Current of Rest and Action- 
Current. Specimen originally isotropic 


(a) A, end slightly injured and negative; Cc, current of injury; R, action- 
current, a negative variation of Cc. (4) A, end killed and positive ; 
C, current of injury; R, action-current, a positive variation of c. 


First—we take the case where the point A is slightly 
injured (fig. 117,@). The current of injury, Cc, is A> B, and 
the responsive current, Rk, is B- A, constituting a negative 
variation. 


176 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


But when A is killed, the current of injury C is B> A, the 
responsive current is also BA, constituting a positive 
variation (fig. 117, 0). 

Second —we take an instance where, owing to some physio- 
logical difference, an intermediate point A is less excitable 
than B or B’ (fig. 118, a). The primary natural current will 
here be from the less to the more excitable : that is to say, C will 


be A>B’ and A+B. If stimulus be now applied at x onthe 


right, an identical excitatory current R, flowing away from the 
excited point from right to left, will cause seemingly opposite 
effects: that is to say, a negative variation of A+B’ and a 
positive variation of A> B. 


(a) (6) 
~< c a >C < 
Bop al ‘sas Xp! 


Fic. 118. Typical Cases of Variation of Current of Rest and Action- 
Current ; intermediate point naturally less or more excitable than 
either of terminal. 


(a) Intermediate A, less excitable, shown by vertical shading ; current or 
rest A>B and A->B’; when right-hand point x excited, action-current 
R from right to left, gives rise to negative variation of As’, and 
positive variation of A>B. (4) Intermediate B, more excitable, shown 
by horizontal shading ; current of rest A>B and A’->B ; action-current 
on excitation at x, from right to left, giving rise to positive variation 
of a’/—>B and negative variation of AB. 


_ Again, we ‘may have the intermediate point B naturally 
more excitable than A’ or A. The natural current C will 
be A>B and A’>B (fig. 118, 4). Stimulation at x will now 
give rise to an excitatory current R, from right to left. The 
results here will, however, appear to be exactly the reverse 
of those in the last case: that is to say,.an identical current, 
R, will give rise to a positive variation of A’ > B, and negative 
variation of A>B. Instances of these effects will be given 
in Chapter XVIII. 

And, lastly, we may have a typically anisotropic tissue, 
composed of two halves, which are unequally excitable—as, 
for instance, the upper and lower halves of the pulvinus of 
Mimosa, or the muscle and gland in a muscle-and-gland 


-RESPONSE BY POSITIVE VARIATION 177 


preparation. Under normal conditions the primary or natural 
current C is from the less excitable A to the more excitable B, 
represented by A > B (fig. 119, a). The action-current R, being 
in the opposite direction B > A, constitutes a negative variation. 
- But owing to the after-effect of excitation, such as may 
occur in isolating the specimen by section, the normal resting 
current C is reversed toB > A (fig. 119, 4). Here the end B may 
still be the more excitable of the two, hence the action-current 
B- A will constitute a positive variation of the current of rest. 
But when B becomes fatigued, its excitability is reduced 
below that of A; hence the action-current is from the 
relatively more to less excitable, ze. A>B. In this case, 


(a) (6) : (c) 
——_>C es ta F 
A B A oon - ae LIB 
R<——_ R <———- ———>R 


Fic. 119. Typical Cases of Variation of Current of Rest and Action- 
Current. Anisotropic organ, B end originally more excitable than A 


- (a) Current of rest A->B ; action-current, R, in opposite direction ; response 
by negative variation. (4) Owing to excitatory after-effect, current of 
rest reversed to B—>A; B nevertheless more excitable than A; action- 
current, R, is B—>A; response by positive variation. (c) Current of 
rest reversed BA; action-current also reversed AB, by depression 
of excitability of B, owing to fatigue ; response by negative variation. 


on account of the reversal of both the current of rest and 
action-current, the latter appears to constitute a negative 
variation of the former (fig. 119, c). 

It will thus be seen how intricate and diverse are the 
responsive variations of the resting current, induced by 
stimulus. Sometimes negative, sometimes positive, it would 
appear as if there were no guiding principle to regulate these 
phenomena. The so-called explanations hitherto attempted 
have consisted in assigning the positive variation to a 
hypothetical process of assimilation, and the negative to 
dissimilation. Such explanatory phrases:reach the climax 
of absurdity when we find ourselves compelled to ascribe | 
one identical excitatory reaction now to assimilation and 
then to dissimilation. 


N 


178 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Indeed it must be said that, however suggestive the 
general theory of assimilation and dissimilation may have 
been found, its abuse has often stood in the way of physio- 
logical inquiry. The inquirer, when faced with any difficulty, 
instead of attempting to surmount it by patient inquiry, was 
tempted rather to evade it by invoking the aid of an hypothesis 
which could be made with equal ease to explain a given 
fact or its direct opposite. We must remember that in the — 
investigation of obscure problems, the danger is always, 
instead of seeking an underlying law, to become satisfied 
with the mere registration of phenomena, and by naming 
these to imagine that they have been explained. The 
resulting chaos in the present case has served to deepen 
the impression that vital phenomena must always remain 
capricious and mystical. 

But when we come to survey the facts that have been 
described, we find the phenomena of response, however 
diverse they may at first sight appear, to be in no way 
governed by chance or caprice. ‘They are, on the contrary, 
definite and uniform under definite conditions. 

As regards the so-called current of rest in a naturally 
isotropic tissue, of which one end has been subjected to 
injury, we must remember that the effect of injury is one of 
excitation, its sign, within limits, being contraction and 
galvanometric negativity. But we have seen that when a 
point is over-stimulated, fatigue-changes appear which give 
rise to a reversal of its normal sign of response, from con- 
traction to expansion, from negative to positive (cf fig. 64) 
The change at death, in which contractile rigor passes into 
post-mortem relaxation, is analogous to this. ‘Thus when one 
end of the specimen is merely injured, that end becomes 
more or less persistently galvanometrically negative, the 
current flowing away from it. But when the same end is 
actually killed, the electrical change may be reversed, to one 
of galvanometric positivity. 

In an isotropic tissue, then, we may, by moderate injury, 
bring about a state of anisotropy, under which the uninjured 


RESPONSE BY POSITIVE VARIATION 179 _ 


end is rendered relatively the more excitable, and galvano- 
metrically positive, compared with the inexcitable injured end. 
In a naturally anisotropic organ, we have a state of things 
which is analogous. In this case, in the primary condition, 
the more excitable surface is galvanometrically positive. But 
under the excitation due to preparation, or accidental dis- 
turbance, this more excitable surface becomes the more excited, 
and, relatively to the other, gaivanometrically negative. These 
varying changes in the direction of the so-called resting 
current, or current of reference, are the cause of the existing 
anomalies in the interpretation of response by the positive 
or negative variation. 

But the direction of the action-current under normal 
conditions is always the same. On diffuse stimulation it is 
always from the more excited B to the less excited A. 
The differential excitability or anisotropy, may be either 
natural, or artificially induced, as by injuring one end of an 
isotropic tissue. There are two different conditions under 
which the normal effect may undergo reversal, those, namely, 
of great sub-tonicity or excessive fatigue. But the statement 
that the responsive current is always from the more excited to 
the less excited, remains universally true. Numerous illustra- 
tions, in verification of the cases laid down, will be met with 
in the course of subsequent chapters. 


CHAPTER XV 
EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE ON ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 


General observation of effect of temperature on plant—FEffect of fall and rise of 
temperature on autonomous response of Desmodium —Effect of frost in abolition 
of electrical response—After-etfects of application of cold, in EZucharis, Ivy 
and Holly—Effect of rise of temperature in diminishing height of response— 
This not probably due to diminution of excitability—Similar effect in auto- 
nomous motile response of Desmodium—Enhanced response as after-effect of 
cyclic variation of temperature—Abolition of response at a critical high 
temperature. 


WE have now seen that the physiological activity of a living 
tissue may be gauged by means of its electrical response. 
We know further that the influence of temperature is of 
importance in the maintenance of a proper physiological 
_condition. There is a certain range of temperature which is 
favourable to this, and above or below these limits physio- 
logical efficiency is diminished. If the plant be kept too long 
at or above a certain maximum temperature, it is liable to 
undergo death. Similarly, there is a minimum point at which 
physiological activity is arrested,and below which death is apt 
tooccur. The plant has thus two death-points, one above the 
maximum and the other below the minimum temperature. 
Some can resist these extremes better than others, and 
length of exposure is also a factor which should not be for- 
gotten in the question of the ultimate survival of the plant 
under the given unfavourable conditions. Certain species 
are hardy, while others succumb easily. 

An unmistakable indication of the effect of temperature 
on physiological activity is found in the variations induced 
by it in the autonomous motile pulsations of the telegraph 
plant, Desmodium gyrans. Here, too great a lowering of the 


OE OO EE 


EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE ON ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 181 


temperature abolishes the pulsation. In fig. 120 are seen 
(1) the records of normal pulsations ; (2) their arrest under 
the application of ice-cold water; and (3) their revival, as 
the plant regains the temperature of the room. In fig. 121 
is shown the effect on similar pulsations of a rising tempera- 
ture. The records in this case were obtained with a different 


Fic, 120. Photographic Record showing Effect of Rapid Cooling, by 
Ice-cold Water, on Pulsations’of Desmodium gyrans 


Normal pulsations recorded to the left. Effect of application of ice-cold 
water is seen in the production of diminished amplitude and abolition 
of pulsation. Gradual return to the temperature of the room revives 
the pulsation in a staircase manner, the period remaining approximately 
constant. Note that cooling displaces the pulsation in a downward or 
contracted direction. Gradual warming, conversely, is seen to produce 
the opposite displacement towards expansion. Up-records represent 
the fall of the leaflet, down-records its rise. 


specimen, and it is seen that the pulsations are diminished | 
in amplitude while their period is quickened, with rise of 
temperature. When the temperature is raised still higher, 
they come to a stop altogether. 

_ We shall next proceed to observe the effect of temperature 
on the electrical response of plants. As regards the influence 
of cold, for example, I have found, during the course of a 
research carried out in England, that after frosty weather, 


182 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


the electrical responses undergo an almost complete aboli- 
tion. During a certain week, for instance, the temperature 
was 10° C., and the electrical responses then obtained from 
radish (Raphanus sativus) were considerable, giving an E.M. 
response which varied from ‘o5 to ‘1 volt. Two or three 
days afterwards, however, as the effect of frost, I found the 
electrical response of this plant to have practically dis- 
appeared. A few specimens were found nevertheless which 
were somewhat resistant. But even in these the average 
E..M. response had only a value of ‘003 volt, instead of the 
normal mean of ‘075 volt. That is to say, their average 
sensitiveness had been reduced to one twenty-fifth. On now 


Fic. 121. Photographic Record of Pulsations of Desmodium during 
Continuous Rise of Temperature from 30° C. to 39° C. 


warming these radishes to 20° C. there was an appreciable 
revival, as shown by their increasing response. But in those 
specimens which had been frost-bitten, warming effected no 
restoration. From this it would appear that frost killed some, 
which could not be subsequently revived, whereas others 
were reduced to a condition of torpidity from which, on 
warming, there was a revival. 

I have also investigated the effect of an artificial lowering 
of temperature on the electrical response of plants. The 
Eucharis lily is particularly sensitive to cold. In this case I 
took the petiole, and obtained response at the ordinary 
temperature of the room, which was at the time 17°C. I 
then placed it for 15 minutes in a cooling chamber at a 


EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE ON ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 183 . 


temperature of —2°C. On now again trying to obtain 
response, it was found that it had practically disappeared. 
The same specimen was next warmed to 20° C., and this 
induced a revival of response (fig. 122). 

I was next desirous of studying the after-effect of lowered 
temperatures on different plants. For this purpose I chose 
three specimens (1) the 
petiole of Eucharts Lily, 
(2) the stem of Ivy, 
and (3) Holly. I took 
their normal responses 
at 17° C.,, and after- 
wards placed them in 
an ice-chamber at a 
temperature of o° C. 
for 24 hours. The 
specimens were then 
taken out, and their 
responses under stimu- 
lation once more re- 
corded (fig. 123). From 
these it will be seen 
that while the respon- 
siveness of the delicate (h) 
Eucharis Lily was com- | | 
pletely abolished, that eagnss 
of the hardier plants, 110,128, Diminuton of Response in 
Holly and Ivy, exhibited (z) Normal response at 17° C. 
complete revival. (6) The response almost disappears when plant 


; : f is subjected to —2° C. for fifteen minutes. 
One interesting fact (c) Revival of response on warming to 20° C. 


which I have noticed 

is that when a plant approaches its death-point, by reason 
of excessively high or low temperature, not only is its re- 
sponse, of galvanometric negativity, diminished to zero, but 
it is even occasionally reversed to positive. This effect 
is due to the unmasking of the positive, by the abolition 
of the true excitatory component. 


(a) (Cc) 


184 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


We shall next study the effect on the electrical response 
of the plant of a rise of temperature. The great difficulty of 
this investigation lies in raising the plant-chamber to the 
various determinate temperatures required. I was able, 
however, to accomplish this by means of electric heating. <A 
spiral of german silver wire was placed in the plant-chamber 
(cf. fig. 21), and by varying the intensity of the current 
the temperature was then regulated at will. In the process — 
of this electric heating a complicating factor was found in the 
excitatory action of any sudden variation of temperature. But 
no such excitatory disturbance occurs if the rise of tempera- 
ture be not fluctuating, but gradual .and continuous. I was 
able to secure this, by selecting at the beginning of the 


aL 


\ a o b 
} KA be 


Holly Eucharis 
Fic. 123. After-effect of Cold on Ivy, Holly, and Zucharis Lily 


_ a, The normal response ; 4, response after subjection to freezing temperature 
for twenty-four hours. 


experiment, a suitable strength of current, such as to raise 
the temperature of the chamber continuously, at an approxi- 
mately uniform rate. Care had also to be taken that thermal 
radiation from the wire should not strike the specimen, since 
such radiation, as we shall see later, acts as a stimulus. The 
interposition of a sheet of mica, however, obviated this diff- 
culty, mica being opaque to thermal radiation. 

While, under these conditions, the temperature was being 
raised, uniform vibrational stimuli were applied at intervals, 
and responses recorded, the temperature of the chamber at 
the moments of stimulation being carefully noted. In this 
way I obtained the following photographic record, with 
a petiole of Hucharis lily, affording a general idea of the 
effect of temperature on response. It will be seen that 


EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE ON ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 185 


while the temperature was rising from 20° C. to 22° C. the 
amplitude of the response was increased. After this, however, 
it fell rapidly in height with rise of temperature and became 
very small, at or near 60° C.- On allowing the temperature 
to: fall, however, the responses revived, with this peculiarity, 
that during the cooling, as compared with those given during 
the ascent of temperature, 
they were markedly en- 
hanced (fig. 124). 

The heating arrange- 
ments in this case were 
such that the temperature 
was made to rise some- 
what rapidly. It will be 
noticed that response had 
not here disappeared, 
even at 65° C., though, 
as we shall see in the 
next chapter, the death- 
point is at about 60° C., 


This apparent anomaly 
is due to the fact that 
the plant, which is a bad 
conductor, was not al- 
lowed time fully to attain 
the temperature of its 
surroundings. We shall 


Fic. 124. Photographic Record of Responses 
in Hucharis Lily during Rise and Fall of 
Temperature 


Stimulus constant, applied at intervals of 
one minute. The temperature of plant- 
chamber gradually rose on starting current 
in the heating coil; on breaking current, 
the temperature fell gradually. Tem- 
perature corresponding to each record is 
given below. 


see that when the tem- Temperature rising: (1) 2 0 °, (2) 20°, (3) 22°, 

perature is raised at a RE ATL Y (765 51°, (To) 

slower rate—about 1° C. 45°, (11) 40°, (12) 38°. 

in I1°5 minute—and when 

the specimen is not too thick, excitatory response disappears 

with the approach of death, at a temperature very near 60° C, 
I give below a record of the effect of temperature 

varying from 30° C. to 50° C. on the response of the stem 

of Amaranth (fig. 125). In order to obtain perfect results, 

it is necessary that the specimen should not exhibit any 


186 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


fatigue, and I have found that this plant is but little subject 
to it. It will be noticed how the response is continuously 
depressed, as the temperature rises from 30° C. upwards. 
In this case, the thermal ascent took place at the rela- 
tively slow rate of 1° C. in 1I°5 minute, so that there was 
time for the plant to attain the temperature of its sur- 
roundings. A tabular statement is given below, showing 
the effect of temperature on amplitude of response, in two - 
different specimens of Amaranth : 


SPECIMEN I, SPECIMEN II. 
Temperature 1 Height. ofresponse || Temperature. Height of respotise | 
|" | 
30° age Ce | 200 divisions || 30° C. 110 divisions | 
35° I * 2» | a5. go ”» 
40° | re) | 40 40 ” 
45° | 2 ”» 45° 25 » 
50° 43 "99 50° IO vA 


The same fall in amplitude of response, when a certain 
point has been reached in the thermal ascent, to which | 
have already referred, in the 
case of Eucharzs lily, has 
also been noticed in that of 
muscle. From this it might 
be concluded that rise of 
temperature beyond 30° C. 
or so, induced depression of 
excitability. But here we 


40° 

are met by an anomaly. 
45° For growth, which I have 
50° shown to be a phenomenon 
of excitatory response, in- 
- ad : creases, in the case of . 
Fic. 125. Diminished Amplitude of the plant, throughout the 
Response with Rising Temperature. thermal ascent up to an 

(Stem of Amaranth) ; Sarr 
optimum point at or near 


35° C. Conductivity, again, which is, to a certain extent, 
correlated with excitability, undergoes enhancement with 


40° 


35° 


EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE ON ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 187 © 


rising temperature. Thus in a certain specimen of Szo- 
phytum, for example, while the velocity of transmission 
at 30° C. was 3°77 mm. per second, it became enhanced to 
9'I mm.—that is, nearly three times—when the temperature 
was raised to 37°C. From these considerations, it would 
appear that the diminution in amplitude of electrical response, 
under a rising temperature below the rigor-point, might not 
always be due to a diminution of excitability, but to some 
other cause. | 

In connection with this, it must be borne in mind that 
two factors are included in the process of response: namely, 
the external stimulus which induces contraction, or galvano- 
metric negativity, and the internal factor which brings about 
recovery. For I have already shown that whereas the action 
of stimulus induces one effect—the contraction, for example, of 
an excited tissue with galvanometric negativity—an increase 
of internal energy causes exactly the opposite —that is to say, 
the expansion of the tissue and galvanometric positivity. 
External stimulus and internal energy thus act antagonisti- 
cally. A steady rise of temperature causes, as we have seen, 
an increase of internal energy. Hence, increased energy 
due to rise of temperature, enhancing the force of recovery, 
may cause a diminution of response, which is not due to 
diminution of excitability. 

The inference that it is the increased internal energy due 
to rise of temperature which, by augmenting the force of 
recovery, diminishes the amplitude of response, appears the 
more probable from certain characteristics observed in the 
autonomous pulsation of Desmodium gyrans. If rise of 
temperature increased the force of recovery, we should 
expect, conversely, that a fall of a few degrees would have 
the effect of diminishing this force of recovery, and con 
sequently enhancing the response. That this actually occurs 
will be seen in fig. 126, in the first part of which is given 
a series of responses at the temperature of the room, which © 
was 29° C. When the temperature of the plant-chamber 
was now lowered to 25° C., the force of recovery would appear 


188 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


to be diminished, since the amplitude of response was con- 
siderably enhanced. That the general excitability of the 
plant was not increased by the lowering of temperature, 
is seen from the fact that the frequency of pulsation was 
reduced on cooling to about two-thirds of its original value. 
The diminution of response with rising temperature may 
thus be due to an increase of internal energy, which tends 


Fic. 126. Photographic Records of Autonomous Pulsations in Des- 
modium, showing Increase of Amplitude and Decrease of Frequency, 
with Lowering of Temperature 


The pulsations to the left were recorded at the ordinary temperature of 
the room, 29° C. Those to the right, when the temperature had been 
lowered to 25° C. 


to cause antagonistic expansion and consequent galvano- 
metric positivity. This view finds support from the records 
seen in figs. 129 and 133, given in the next chapter. The 
first of these (fig. 129) shows the expansion, with consequent 
physical elongation, of the filament of Passzfora under a 
rising temperature. In the second (fig. 133) is seen the in- 
creasing galvanometric positivity of a specimen of Amaranth 
under similar circumstances. 

It is now clear that when the temperature of the tissue 


EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE ON ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 189 


is below a certain thermotonic minimum, the effect of a rise 
of temperature will be to enhance the amplitude of response 
by removing molecular sluggishness. This fact has been 
illustrated in the gradually heightened mechanical response 
of the autonomous pulsation of Desmodium gyrans when a 
plant artificially cooled was allowed to return to the normal 
temperature of the room (fig. 120). If similarly a plant 
tissue be first cooled and then allowed to return to the 
surrounding temperature, its electrical responses to suc- 
cessive uniform stimuli being recorded throughout, a stair- 
case increase of response will be observed during the return. 
When the temperature, however, is raised above a certain 
optimum, a depression of the amplitude of response begins, 
not by the depression of excitability, but by the increasing 
force of recovery due to an augmentation of the internal 
factor. True depression only takes place when the plant 
is approaching a condition of heat-rigor. 

One very curious effect of temperature-variation which 
has been touched upon is the marked increase of sensi- 
tiveness which often makes its appearance as its after-effect. 
This is seen exemplified in the record given in fig. 124, 
showing the effect of a cyclic variation of temperature 
on Eucharzs lily. In another experiment with Scotch kale, 
the response at the temperature of 30° C. was eleven 
divisions, and at 50° C. eight divisions, during the thermal 
ascent. During the descent, however, the amplitude at 
50° C. was sixteen, and at 30° C. twenty-three divisions. 
The sensitiveness was thus doubled. This enhancement 
may be due in part to the increased molecular mobility 
consequent on the annealing effect, as it were, of temperature- 
variation. But it may also be regarded as partly due to the 
difference of the antagonistic forces which the excitatory 
response has to overcome during ascent and descent. During 
the thermal ascent, the opposing expansive force is being 
rapidly accelerated. During the thermal descent, on the 
other hand, this is no longer the case, for the force of re- 
covery is now undergoing a diminution. 


190 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


When the temperature is raised above a certain critical 
point, the plant is killed, and its electrical response dis- 
appears at the same time. This is demonstrated visually in 
the accompanying photographic record (fig. 127). In this 
case, normal responses were first obtained at the usual tem- 
perature of the room. Steam was next introduced into the 


Before * After 


Fic. 127. Photographic record showing effect of Steam in abolishing 
Response 


The two records to the left exhibit normal response at 17° C. Sudden 
warming by steam induced at first an inorease of response, but five 
minutes’ exposure to steam killed the plant (carrot) and abolished the 
response. 

Vibrational stimulus of 30° applied at intervals of one minute; vertical 
line = ‘I volt. 


plant-chamber, and kept streaming in during the course ot 
the experiment, electrical responses being recorded mean- 
while at intervals of one minute. It will be seen that at first 
a transitory augmentation ot excitability was induced. But 
this quickly disappeared, and in five minutes the plant was 
effectively killed, as is shown in the waning and final aboli- 
tion of response. This experiment affords us a qualitative 
demonstration of the abolition of response at death under 


EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE ON ELECTRICAL RESPONSE I9QI 


the influence of high temperature. In the next chapter we 
shall enter upon an exact determination of this critical point 
of death. cate 

It is thus seen that temperature modifies the electrical 
response of plants. There isa temperature-minimum below 
which response is abolished. If the plant be kept too long 
at this temperature it is apt to be killed. In the case of a 
delicate species like Eucharis, which is highly susceptible of 
the injurious effect of cold, the electrical response is per- 
manently abolished by long exposure. But hardier plants, 
like Holly and Ivy, show revival of electrical response, on a 
return to a favourable temperature. The electrical response 
disappears also at a certain maximum temperature con- 
stituting the death-point. 


CHAPTER XVI 
THE ELECTRICAL SPASM OF DEATH 


Different fost-mortem symptoms—Accurate methods for determination of death- 
point— Determination of death-point by abolition or reversal of normal elec- 
trical response—Determination of death-point by mechanical death-spasm— 
From thermo-mechanical inversion—By observation of electrical spasm : (a) in 
anisotropic organs: (4) in radial organs—Simultaneous record of electrical 
inversion and reversal of normal electrical response—Remarkable consistency 
of results obtained by different methods—Tabulation of observations. 


IT will be seen from the last chapter that there is in the case 
of every plant a certain high temperature which is critical, 
since above it life passes into death. Much difficulty has 
been experienced in the exact determination of this critical 
point, because no sure criterion of death was hitherto avail- 
able, such as would furnish an immediate and reliable indica- 
tion of its occurrence. The various symptoms of death, such 
as drooping, withering, discoloration and the escape of 
coloured cell-sap, do not manifest themselves at the onset of 
death, but at some time indeterminately later. Even when a 
plant has been subjected to a temperature in excess of the 
fatal degree, it continues to appear fresh and living ; and it is 
not till after some longer or shorter interval that the death 
symptoms are seen. 

To take, for example, the symptom of drooping, it is 
clear that the loss of turgidity on which this depends cannot 
at once make itself visible. In a thick tissue, again, death 
may take place in the superficial layers of the plant, the 
interior tissues, owing to feeble thermal conductivity, remain- 
ing comparatively unharmed. Or, if we employ the test of 
discoloration, which we shall find to occur some time after 
the initiation of death we find that the exact moment at 


THE ELECTRICAL SPASM OF DEATH 193 


which discoloration begins cannot be detected with sufficient 
precision. When we place the specimen in a thermal bath 
under a rising temperature, the beginning of discoloration 
- after death is so slight as to be impossible of detection, and 
by the time it becomes marked, the temperature has already 
passed several degrees above the fatal point. I have found, 
for example, that the colour of the milk-white style of Datura 
_ .alba has changed to brown by the time that the temperature 

of the bath has risen to about 64° C. In the petals of 
Sesbania coccineum, again, a striking change of colour is 
detected, under similar conditions. Rich crimson here turns 
into pale blue at a temperature of about 67° C.. The fila- 
mentous corona of Passiflora quadrangularis, finally, in which 
the filaments are barred by purple rings, loses its colour 
normally at about 68° C. In all these cases, the initiation of 
the loss of colour must have been imperceptible. Hence, all 
that can be determined from such experiments is that the 
death-changes must have commenced at some. temperature 
~ lower than 64° to 68° C. 

Before proceeding further, it is necessary to obtain a clear 
idea of what is meant by the death-point. In animals, an 
early symptom of death consists in the setting in of rigor 
mortts. But this does not synchronise throughout the 
body, certain parts of the organism undergoing the death- 
change earlier than others. Thus the only definition of the 
death-point which can be made at all precise is that which 
regards it as the point of initiation of some unmistakable 
sign of death. I shall next proceed to describe several 
death-symptoms and the modes by which they may be de- 
tected with certainty. 

With regard to such detection, I Siti pointed out else- 
where that, theoretically, it should be possible to make such 
a determination by watching the waning of some effect 
characteristic of the living condition, the death-point being 
known by its cessation at a given moment. Such a test, as 
we shall presently see, is afforded by the electrical responses. 
The ideally perfect method, however, would be by the 

O 


194 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


detection of some effect which at the moment of death under- 
went a sudden reversal to its opposite. There would not 
here be even that minor degree of uncertainty which is in- 
cidental to the determination of the exact vanishing-point of 
a waning effect. And such methods are afforded by my 
discovery of the occurrence of mechanical and electrical 
spasms at the moment of death. 

Turning now to the method of the waning effect, we have 


seen that response to stimulus by galvanometric negativity is _ 


distinctive of the living condition. When the plant is killed, 
this normal response disappears. At the moment of death 
from high temperature, therefore, we may expect to see the 
abolition of this normal excitatory response of negativity. 

For this investigation I took a batch of six radishes. 
The specimens were kept for five minutes previous to each 
experiment in water at a definite temperature (say of 17° C.), 
and were then mounted in the vibration-apparatus and 
their responses observed. Each specimen was next dis- 
mounted and replaced in the bath at a higher tempera- 
ture (say of 30° C.) for another five minutes. After this, a 
second set of responses, to the same stimulus as before, was 
taken. In this way observations were made with each plant, 
till the temperature at which response almost or altogether 
ceased was reached. I give below (p. 195) a table of the 
results obtained with the six radishes. 

From these experiments it would appear that in these 
cases the responses disappeared at about 55° C. It should 
be stated here that this investigation was carried out in the 
winter season in England, and it will be shown later that 
the incidence of cold has the effect of lowering the normal 
death-point by about 4° or 5° C. 

I was next desirous of substituting, for this method of 
discontinuous observations, one which should be continuous. 
I, therefore, subjected the specimen—a stem of Amaranth— 
to a continuous rise of temperature, and took records of 
responses to uniform stimuli after every few degrees of the 
ascent. I found here that not only was there a gradual 


i Ge ties Ee 


THE ELECTRICAL SPASM OF DEATH 195 


TABLE SHOWING EFFECT OF HIGH TEMPERATURE IN ABOLISHING 
RESPONSE OF RADISH (Raphanus sativus) 


‘Dien vt te 6" see Re et a tl i te i ee 


Specimen Temperature penis ensinp papa é 

: 178-6, 70 
53° C. 4 
aie  & oot OF 160. 
i Sg I 

17° CG; 100 

3 50° C. 2 
1 ee OF 80 

4 55° C. fe) 
17° C. 40 

5 | 60° C, fe) 
6 a7? C, 60 
55°.C. oO 


decrease of response, tending towards its abolition, with 
rising temperature, but also that, at the death-point, it under- 
40 


43 


50 


60° 
Fic. 128. Record of Electric Responses of Amaranth at various temperatures 
The response undergoes reversal to positive at the critical temperature of 60° C. 


went actual reversal from the normal galvanometric nega- 


tivity to positivity (fig. 128). This was due to the fact that 
02 


196 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


on reaching the death-point, the contained positive com- 
ponent in response was unmasked by the abolition of the true 
excitatory effect. But this positive response disappears also 
after a short time. It will thus be seen that by this method 
the death-point is capable of determination within very narrow 
limits, having been, in the present case, near 60° C. When 
the tissue is thin, this temperature soon proves fatal. But 


should it be thick, a very much longer exposure to it 


is necessary, if the interior of the tissue is to be killed 
effectively. 

I have also discovered another method of obtaining the 
death-point with precision, in the symptoms afforded by 
mechanical responses. For I found thata death-spasm occurs 
at a certain critical moment in a plant, which is analogous 
to the death-throe of the animal. The experimental plant— 
Mimosa, for instance—was placed in a bath of water, whose 
temperature was being raised gradually, at a uniform rate of, 
say, 1° C, per one minute and a half, until the death-point was 
reached. During all this time there was no responsive fall 
of the leaf, for, we have seen, it isa sudden variation, and not 
a gradual rise of temperature, which acts as an excitatory 
stimulus. This gradual rise, on the other hand, increases the 
internal energy of the plant, by which the turgidity of the 
pulvinus is continuously augmented. In this process the 
increase of turgidity is more energetic in the more excitable 
lower half than in the upper. The greater expansion of the 
lower side of the pulvinus thus raises the leaf continuously. 
But immediately on reaching the death-point, there is a 
reversal of thls movement, and an abrupt fall of the leaf. 
This spasmodic movement is sudden and well-defined. In 
a vigorous J/zmosa the death-spasm is found to occur at 
or very near 60°C. This contraction of death is followed 
after some time by a fost-mortem relaxation. 

That the death-response is an excitatory phenomenon 
is seen from the fact that any circumstance which lowers 
physiological activity lowers the death-point also. Thus, 
after a spell of cold weather, I found that the death-point 


ee ee 


THE ELECTRICAL SPASM OF DEATH 197 


of Mimosa was lowered from the normal 60° C. to about 
53° C. This latter value, it will be remembered, was ap- 
proximately the same as that obtained with radish, in winter, 
by the method of electrical response. Again, I find fatigue 
to induce a lowering of the death-point, the extent of which 
depends upon the degree of fatigue. When this was moderate, 
I have found the death-point of MW/zmosa to be lowered to 
56° C. 

This spasmodic contraction, indicative of the initiation of 
death, may express itself in diverse ways. For example, 
if the tubular peduncle of Ad/zum be filled with water, and 
raised gradually in temperature, there comes a moment at 
which a sudden expulsion of the contained water occurs. A 
spiral tendril of Passzfora, under the same circumstances, 
exhibits a sudden uncurling. The florets of the ray, in certain 
Composite, show characteristic movements, either up or down. 
In all these cases alike, under normal circumstances, the 
death-point is found to be at or near 60° C. 

Turning next to the radial organs of ordinary plants, 
these also exhibit a sudden longitudinal contraction at the 
onset of death. I have shown elsewhere how, by means 
of the Morograph, an instrument which I devised for this 
purpose, a thermo-mechanical curve is recorded by the 
specimen, while it is being subjected to the continuous rise of 
temperature, culminating in the death-point. The ordinate 
of this curve represents the induced variation of length, and 
the abscissa the temperature. The expansion described in 
the case of MWimosa is seen here in the form of a gradual 
elongation, up to the moment of reaching the death-point. 
When this point is reached, however, a sudden contraction 
takes place, giving rise to an inversion of the curve. This 
turning point is very abrupt. The curve as a whole is 
thus one of life-and-death, in which the point of inversion 
separates the two. I give below a photographic record of 
this thermo-mechanical curve, obtained with the coronal 
filament of Passtfora. The death-point occurred here at 
59°6° C. (fig. 129). The thermo-mechanical curve is very 


198 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


similar in similar specimens under normal conditions. 
Fig. 130 gives two records of two different styles of Datura 
alba, obtained from flowers of the same plant. The death- 
point is seen to have occurred at 60°C. In recording the 
thermo-mechanical curve, there is found to be, normally 
speaking, a continuous expansion up to the death- point. 
In the case of vigorous specimens, in a good tonic condition, 
the inversion does not take place 
till about 59°6° or 60° C. But in 
less vigorous specimens, a certain 
hesitation, as it were, is seen to 
occur in the record at or near 
55° C. With vigorous specimens, 


Fic. 129. Photographic Record 
of Thermo-mechanical Curve 
given by Coronal Filament 
of Passiflora 

The first or down part of the so 30° gee oe S* S0° 55° 60° 6S° 
curve shows expansion, but 


on reaching death-point, at Fic. 130. Thermo-mechanical Curve of « 
59°6° C., there ska sudden Two Different Specimens of Style of 
inversion, due to spasmodic Datura alba, obtained from Flowers 
death-contraction. of the same Plant 


there may be the merest indication of this hesitation ; in other 
cases, with less favourable tonic condition, the hesitation is 
prolonged, but the expansion finally proceeds, ahd the death 
inversion takes place at the usual temperature of about 
60° C. When the specimen, however, is enfeebled, or has 
been subjected to unfavourable circumstances, the point of 
transient instability becomes fatal, and the inversion takes 
place there. As an example of what has just been referred 
to—namely, the influence of unfavourable external circum- 


THE ELECTRICAL SPASM OF DEATH . 199 — 


stances in lowering the death-point—it may be mentioned here 
that the sudden incidence of cold weather will lower it by 
some 4° or 5° C. Intense fatigue will lower it by as much 
as 19° C. 

_ [have already said that this spasm, taking place at the 
moment of death, is an excitatory response. On this theory 
it occurred to me that it should also be possible to determine 
the onset of death by an electrical spasm. It may be well 
at this point, therefore, to examine some of the conditions 
under which such a spasm, supposing it to take place, might 
be displayed most conspicuously. We may suppose a radial 
organ, with the usual electrical contacts, A and B, to have 
its temperature raised gradually up to the death-point. 
The excitatory effect of death may now be expected to 
cause the galvanometric negativity of a given point. But 
since these excitatory effects are equal and similar at 
A and B, they will balance each other, and there will be 
little or no resultant galvanometric response. In order to 
obtain a marked resultant effect, then, we must have an 
organ in which the excitabilities of the two points A and B 
are different. This difference of excitability, necessary to the 
exhibition of a resultant response, may be either natural or 
artificially induced. For the former, we may take a specimen 
which is not radial, but anisotropic, thus affording us two points 
of galvanometric contact, possessed of unequal excitabilities. 

We have seen that the inner surface of the petiole of 
Cucurbita maxima was more excitable than the outer. 
The same is true of the hollow peduncle of Uvzcits lily. 
There is also a great difference of excitability as between the 
upper and lower sides of the scale of the bulb of the same 
lily, in the season of flowering, the concave surface of this 
scale being more excitable than the convex. Any of these 
specimens described I find to answer asmizably for the purpose 
of this investigation. 

Taking the petiole of Cucurbita, then, I divided it longi- 
tudinally, and rejected one-half, ‘thus obtaining a half-tube, 
of which the inner concave surface was more excitable than 


200 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


the outer convex. Electric contacts were now made 
through non-polarisable electrodes with equal and opposite 
areas on the two sides. These adjustments were made in a 
heating chamber containing electrical arrangements by which 
the temperature could be raised continuously. This was 
satisfactorily accomplished by an incandescent electrical 
lamp which was placed in a second chamber, vertically below | 
the plant-chamber. There was a wooden partition between 
the two, by which the light of the lamp was excluded from 
the specimen (fig. 131). For the radiation itself will be 


y 
E Ro Rx 


| an 
et || sa 
[A625 My 
| 


Fic. 131. The Thermal Chamber 


Electric lamp in the lower compartment raises temperature of the upper. 
E, E’, electrodes making contacts with the specimen ; T, thermometer. 


shown to constitute stimulus, and the object in the present 
case was to eliminate all exciting factors except death itself. 
By means of side-openings, the heated air was enabled to 
pass into the plant-chamber, thus raising the temperature. A 
rheostat included in the lamp-circuit made it possible to 
adjust the rate of this rise of temperature, its average being 
about 1° per minute. 

The natural current through the petiole is, under normal 
circumstances, from the less excitable outer to the more 
excitable inner surface: that is to say, the inner is galvano- 


THE ELECTRICAL SPASM OF DEATH ZO} 


metrically positive. This is true when the excitation, due 
to section made for the purpose of preparation, has subsided. 
This stimulation, by causing greater excitation of the inner 
surface, is liable to induce there a temporary negativity. 
A gradual rise of temperature, as we saw, caused an increased 
turgidity of the more excitable lower side of the pulvinus 
of Mimosa, and this increased turgidity was exhibited 
mechanically by the erection of the leaf. But the electrical 
sign of increased turgidity is galvanometric positivity. We 
have also seen that electrical responses occur equally in 
motile and non-motile tissues. In the petiole of Cucurbita 
then, on its more excitable inner surface, we obtain, during 
the gradual rise of temperature, an increasing galvanometric 
positivity. This is true only, as has been said before, when 
the rise is continuous, and not marked by fluctuation. For 
any sudden variation will act as a stimulus, causing galvano- 
metric negativity of the more excitable inner side. For this 
reason it is necessary that the rheostatic resistance inter- 
posed in the lamp-circuit, for the adjustment of the uniform 
rate of rise of temperature, should be made at the beginning 
of the experiment, such tissue being very sensitive to this 
particular stimulatory action. At the commencement of my 
investigation I experienced much trouble from the erratic 
movement of the galvanometer spot of light, and the 
obtaining of a steady electrical curve seemed at that time 
almost hopeless. Later on, however, I found that these 
fluctuations were traceable to temperature-variations, unavoid- 
ably associated with the attempt to regulate the rise of 
temperature by movement of the rheostatic slide. It is for 
this reason, then, that the adjustment must be made, once for 
all, at the beginning. 

Carrying out the experiment in this manner, I obtained, 
with various anisotropic organs, a sudden inversion of the 
electric curve at the death-point. This death-point was 
found, in all vigorous specimens, from which traces of injury 
had been removed by previous rest, to occur accurately 
at 59°6° or 60° C._ In these electrical curves, the same 


202 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


point of instability, already noticed in the thermo- mechanical 
curve, was often found to occur at or about 55°C. And 
if the specimen were not in favourable tonic condition, or 
had been suffering from injury, the death-point was lowered 
to this degree. 3 
I give below an electrical curve showing the point of 
inversion at death (fig. 132). It was obtained with the 
sheathing petiole of Musa. The 
inner or concave side of this petiole 
is more excitable, as we have seen, 
than the outer. These responsive 
electrical variations were very large, 
and could not be represented within 
the limits of the photographic plate. 
I therefore took a photographic 
record between the temperature of 
54° C. and 67° C. only. The first 
part of the curve represents the 
increasing galvanometric  posi- 
tivity of the more excitable inner 
surface of the specimen. The 
same process of increasing posi- 
tivity under the continuous rise of 
temperature, had been going on 
Hieltae whctourerkak Reacont previously, it is to be understood, 
exhibiting Electric Spasmin before arrival at 54° C., at which 
he Fe Hole oh Ae the photographic record was com- 
Sudden electric inversion takes ee 
place at the death-point, menced. This increasing galvano- 
59°5°. Record was com- metric positivity corresponds to 
menced at 54° C., and suc- : . 
cessive gaps in the record the gradual erection of the leaf in 
cae C. rise of tem-  Yy0sa, and to the expansion of 
a radial organ, such as a coronal 
filament of Passzflora, all alike being due.to the positive 
variation of turgidity. In order to give an indication of the 
particular temperature at each portion of the curve, the re- 
cording light was obscured for about 15 seconds after each 
degree of temperature. The successive gaps, then, are one 


THE ELECTRICAL SPASM OF DEATH 203 


degree centigrade of temperature apart. These interruptions, 
however, were not made after the occurrence of the inversion. 
As soon as the death-point was reached, in the present case 
at 59°5° C., there was a sudden inversion of the electrical 
curve (fig. 132), corresponding with the point of inversion of 
the thermo-mechanical curve (fig. 129). Each of these 
curves is seen to bear a striking resemblance to the other. 
In both cases, the inversion was due to the same fact of 
sudden excitation, finding expression in the one, in induced 
galvanometric negativity, and in the other, in mechanical 
contraction. 

In the case of organs which are more or less radial, and 
in which there is little differential excitability, it is necessary 
to abolish the excitability of one 
contact, as, say, by previous scalding. 
For this experiment I took a leaf of 
Amaranth, and injured a portion of 
the lamina by immersion in boiling 
water. The two contacts were 
made, one with the petiole, and the 
other with the injured lamina. On 
raising the temperature continuously, 
the more excitable petiole became 
increasingly positive. The photo- 
eraphic record in this case was com- 
menced only on reaching 55° C., and 
the death-inversion took place at date Pitre, saree 
59°5° C. (fig. 133). version -at Death-point, 

We have already seen that, besides 525» Patrice. a 
this electrical reversal, there is another 
means of detection of the death-point, afforded by the con- 
version of the response to external stimulus from the normal 
negative into positive. Now it occurred to me that it would 
be interesting, if in the same specimen, both these tests could 
be applied at the same time. We could then see whether 
two methods so independent of each other furnished mutual 
corroboration or not. For this purpose I took a stem of 


204 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 
Amaranth, and abolished the excitability of one of the two 
contacts—a lateral leaf—by scalding. The electrical curve, 


under a continuously rising temperature, was now taken in 
The existing electro-motive difference 


the usual manner. 
between living and injured contacts underwent the usual 


40° 
fe] 
45 
Oe é 
‘ 4 
f 
S H 
oe ! 
. 50° 
MA. 59° 
‘ 
55} 
: \ ‘| 
‘ ' 58 
‘ ! 
Vv 
57° 


Fic. 134. Record showing Inversion of Electric Curve (represented 
by dotted line) and Simultaneous Reversal of Electric Response in 


Stem of Amaranth 
{ indicates current of injury from injured to uninjured contact, which, 
Normal 


reaching a maximum, undergoes reversal at death-point. 
response up, also becomes reversed to down after death-point. 


increase, reaching a maximum at the death-point. Meanwhile 
electrical responses to uniform vibrational stimuli were taken 
at intervals a few degrees apart. It will be seen (fig. 134) 
that the electrical inversion took place at 57° C., this 
moderate lowering of the death-point being due, in all 
probability, to the slight depression caused by scalding at 


THE ELECTRICAL SPASM OF DEATH © 205 


the distal contact, which had not had time to pass off. The 
test-responses to uniform mechanical stimulation which 
were being taken meanwhile show a continuous diminution 
towards the death-point. When this, however, had been 
passed, the response is seen to be reversed in direction to 
positive. As has been said before, this positive response also 
disappears after a time. We thus obtain a very striking 
demonstration of the fact that the reversal of the electrical 
curve and the reversal of the sign of response are concomitant. 

It may be mentioned here, in anticipation of a future 
chapter, that the death-point may also be obtained by the 
sudden inversion of the curve of electrical resistivity, and 
that the value obtained in this way coincides with those 
already given. 

I give below a table showing the death-points deter- 
mined by various methods: 


TABLE SHOWING DEATH-POINTS OBTAINED BY DIFFERENT METHODS 


Specimens Method pac 
Cc. 
1. Flower of French Marigold . | Opening or closing of flower 59° 
2 Peduncle of Aliium  . . | Expulsion of contained water 59° 
3. Spiral tendril of Passifora . | Movement of uncurling . Z 
4.  Pulvinus of AZ@imosa  . . | Spasmodic lateral movement | 59°-60° 
5-8. Style of Datura (four speci- 
mens). Each gave . . | Morograph : : e} , '60? 
9-12. Style of Azbzscus (four speci- 
mens). Each gave . : ” , ; ‘ 60° 


13-18. Coronal filament of Pass¢flora 


(six specimens). Each gave » ‘ 60° 
Anisotropic organs . | Electric inversion 

19. Petiole of Cauliflower . . es os : ; 60° 
20. Scale of Uriclis bulb. ; ae rey : ¥ 60° 
21. Petiole of Musa . , . 43 ve 60° 
22. Radial organ: petiole of 

Amaranth : 4 ‘ ss a : - | 59°6° 
23. Petiole of Amaranth . . | Reversal of sign of electrical | 59°6° 

response 

24. Style of Hzbéscus . ‘ . | Resistivity variation . : 60° 


It is thus seen that employing different methods and 
using plant-organs which are equally diverse—flowers, bulbs, 
petioles, and others—a death-point is determined which 


206 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


is very definite and practically the same for all phanero- 
gamous plants. Among the mechanical methods, that 
which depends on the thermo-mechanical curve, giving 
the death-point by a sudden inversion, is specially accurate. 
Of an equally precise character is the death-point obtained 
by the inversion of an electrical curve, and also that which 
is given by reversal of the electrical response. And it is in 


the highest degree remarkable that the points of inversion — 


in the mechanical and electrical curves respectively, together 
with the point of reversal of the electrical response itself, 
should so exactly coincide. 


ate Lars 


CHAPTER XVII 
MULTIPLE AND AUTONOMOUS ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 


Repeated responses under single strong stimulus—Multiple mechanical response 
in Biophytum-—Multiple electrical responses in various animal and vegetable 
tissues— Continuity of multiple*and autonomous response—Transition from 
multiple response to autonomous, and vice versa—Autonomous mechanical 
response of Desmodium gyrans and its time-relations — Simultaneous 
mechanical and electrical records of automatic pulsations in DVesmodium— 
Double electrical pulsation, principal and subsidiary waves—-Electrical 
pulsation of Desmod7um \eaflet under physical restraint—Growth- ema 
—So-called current of rest in growing plants. 


WE have seen that when a plant organ is acted on by a 
single stimulus of sufficient intensity, it exhibits a single 
excitatory effect, which may show itself in two independent 
ways, as mechanical and electrical response. We have also 
seen that part of the impinging stimulus may become latent, 
to find appropriate expression later. It was also shown 
that with increasing intensity of stimulus the amplitude of 
response reaches a limit. It may thus happen that a very 
strong stimulus, not finding adequate expression in a single 
response, will exhibit itself by means of repeated responses. 
The incident energy in such cases is held latent for a 
time,’ to manifest itself later in a rhythmic manner. 

I have been able to demonstrate the occurrence of this 
multiple excitation, in response to a single strong stimulus, 
by several different and independent methods. The simplest 
and most striking of these depends on the recording of the 
motile effects in the leaflets of Bzophytum. In fig. 135 are seen 
no less than sixteen multiple pulsations resulting from a single 
strong thermal stimulation of the petiole bearing the leaflets. 


1 For more detailed account see Bose, Plant Response, pp. 2779-357. 


208 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


The average period of each pulsation is here about thirty 
seconds ; but this may vary in different cases from half of this 
toone minute. I have also been able to detect these multiple 
excitatory waves, during their transit through non-motile con- 
ducting tissues such as stems. ‘The imperceptible volumetric 
changes which occur on the arrival of excitation were here 
detected electrically by variations of pressure induced in an 
enclosing microphonic contact. In fig. 136 is seen such 


Fic, 135. Multiple Mechanical Response of Biophytum, due to a Single 
. Strong Thermal Stimulus 


multiple electro-tactile response in the stem of MW/zmosa, due 
to a single thermal stimulus. 

I have also been able to record such multiple excitatory 
effects by means of electro-motive response. In fig. 137 is 
seen a photographic record of a series of such responses, 
given by the leaf of Bzophytum, the individual thermal 
stimuli being here applied at intervals of five minutes. It will 
here be noticed that each single stimulus gave rise to 
from five to eight responses, the average period of which 
was thirty seconds. From the corresponding mechanical 


MULTIPLE AND AUTONOMOUS ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 209 


responses, it will be remembered that the average period of 
these had also this value. 

These multiple responses to 
a single strong stimulus, while 
very strikingly manifested by 
such plant-tissues as the pul- 
vinules of Azophytum, and in 
the animal, by the cardiac 
tissue, are also exhibited by 
almost all kinds of tissues 
under favourable circumstances. 
In fig. 138 will be seen records 
which show this in the case 
of different vegetable organs 
under diverse forms of stimu- ge 

A . 3 Fic. 136. Multiple Electro-tactile 
lation. In fig. 139 1S given Response in Stem of AZ/mosa, 
a series of multiple responses Savi Single Strong. ’Thermal 
which I have obtained from 
frog’s stomach. I have also 
detected the occurrence of multiple responses in nerves of 
animals, which will be described in a later chapter. 


(Original record reduced to }.) 


Fic. 137. Photographic Record of Multiple Electrical Response in 
Leaf of Biophytum 


First series of eight responses to a single thermal stimulus ; second stimulus, 
after interval of five minutes, evoked five responses ; third stimulus, after 
second interval of five minutes, gave six responses. Average period of 
each response, thirty seconds nearly. 

We have seen that multiple response takes place in 


consequence of some sufficient increase of internal energy. 
P 


210 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


In the cases. mentioned, 


this was derived from impinging 


stimulus. The internal energy of a tissue may, however, be 


Fic. 138. Multiple Electrical Responses under Different Forms of 
Stimulus in Different Organs 


(a) In Mimosa due to thermal, and (4) to chemical stimulation ; (c) in 
peduncle of Azophytum, due to thermal stimulus ; [N.B.— This ‘series 
persisted for two honrs.] (d) in hypocotyl of Zamarindus indica, due 


to stimulus of cut. 


increased, and multiple 


initiated in other ways, as, 


Fic. 139. Photographic Record 
of Multiple Electrical Re- 
sponse to Single Thermal 
Shock in Frog’s Stomach 


response may consequently be 
for instance, by adequately raising 
the temperature of the plant. 
This is seen in the following 
record of pulsatory responses as 
induced in a young leaflet of 
Biophytum, when the temperature 
was raised to 35° C. With the 
increase of internal energy, the 
turgidity of. the tissue was en- 
hanced, and the excessive hydro- 
static tension thus brought about 
induced autonomous. pulsations 
(fig. 140), as in a quiescent snail’s 
heart similar pulsations are in- 
duced by increase of the internal 
hydrostatic pressure. I have else- 
where shown that the energy 
which expresses itself in pulsatory 


movements may be derived by the plant, either directly from 
immediate external sources; or from an excess of such 


MULTIPLE AND AUTONOMOUS ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 2II 


energy already accumulated and-held latent in the tissue, 
aided by the incidence of external stimulus; or from the 
excessive accumulation of such latent energy alone. There 
is thus a continuity between multiple and autonomically 
responding plants. Azophytum, which under ordinary cir- 
cumstances belongs to the former of these classes, becomes 
converted into the latter under exceptionally favourable 
tonic conditions. That is to say, it responds by a single 
response to a single moderate stimulus, and by multiple 


Fic. 140. Induction of Autonomous Response in Biophy/um 
at Moderately High Temperature of 35° C. 
Note the diminution of amplitude of response with falling temperature. 
The pulsations came to a stop below 29° C. 


responses. to a strong stimulus. Under exceptionally favour- 
able tonic conditions, however, it exhibits spontaneous or 
autonomous responses. Desmodium gyrans, on the other hand, 
which ordinarily exhibits autonomous response, will, under 
unfavourable circumstances, cease to exhibit spontaneous 
movements. It then exhibits a single response to a single 
moderate stimulus, and multiple responses to a single strong 
stimulus : 

When the leaflet of this plant, owing to deficit of internal 
energy, is in a state of standstill, a renewal of the supply of 
stimulus will restore it to a condition of autonomous response. 
This is seen in the following record (fig. 141) of the response 

P.2 


212 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


of the leaflet of Desmodium. The leaflet was in a quiescent 
condition, but under the action of stimulus of light, it ex- 
hibited multiple responses ; and these, owing to the increasing 
absorption of energy, showed a staircase enhancement of 
amplitude. On the cessation of light, the energy absorbed 
maintained the pulsation for some time. 

It is thus the absorption of energy which is the cause of the 
so-called autonomous movements. The energy, as already 
stated, may be derived by the plant either directly from ex- 
ternal sources ; or from the excess already accumulated and 
held latent in the tissue, aided by incident external stimulus ; 
or from an excess of latent energy previously accumulated. 


Fic. 141. Initiation of Multiple Response in Lateral Leaflet of 
Desmodium originally at Standstill 


Light applied at x and continued till the end of the sixth response, as 
shown by the thick line. The responses show a staircase increase 
with increase of absorbed energy. Pulsations persist for a short time 
even on the cessation of stimulus. 


It. would be impossible to conceive of movement without 
an exciting cause. Only under the action of stimulus 
can a living tissue give responsive indications. An ex- 
ternal stimulus may either give rise to an immediate 
responsive expression, or be partly or wholly reserved 

latent form for subsequent manifestation. ‘Inner stimuli’ 
are simply external stimuli previously absorbed and _ held 
latent. A plant or animal is thus an accumulator which 
is constantly storing up energy from external sources, and 
numerous manifestations of life—often periodic in their 
character—are but responsive expressions of energy which | 
has been derived from external sources and is held latent in 


the tissue. 


MULTIPLE AND AUTONOMOUS ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 213 


In Desmodium gyrans, as is well known, we have the typical 
example of autonomous response, its secondary leaflets 
executing periodic up and down or elliptical movements. 
The movement of the leaflet in some instances takes place by 
jerks, in others it is more uniform. The period of a com- 
plete up-and-down movement varies between two and four 
minutes. The length of this period is much affected by 
temperature, being less when this is moderately high. From 
the normal, or highest position, the leaflet sinks somewhat 
rapidly ; having reached its maximum depressed position, it 
rests fora while. There 
is next a rather slow 
rise to its original posi- 
tion. This up-and-down 
motion is in some cases 
approximately straight. 
In others, the pulvinule 
of the leaflet is slightly 
twisted after its descent, 
and the corresponding 
curve described becomes 
more circular. 

In view of certain Fic. 142. Photographic Record of Autonomous 
peculiarities to be ob- Mechanical Pulsation in Desmodium Leaflet 

Bs , Period of each complete pulsation 

served in the electrical =. 2°7 minutes. 

response of Desmodium, 

it is necessary here to enter into some detail regarding the 
time-relations of its mechanical response during the two 
phases of down and up movements. The great difficulty 
in recording the pulsatory movements of Desmodium lies 
in the extreme slenderness of the lateral leaflets. This 
is such that the friction of a light recording-lever against 
the recording surface is sufficient to bring these movements 
to a stop. This difficulty has, however, been overcome, as 
stated elsewhere, by means of the Optical Lever.! Fig. 142 


‘ See also Bose, Plant Response, p. 5. 


214 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


gives a photographic record of a series of autonomous 
pulsations exhibited by a leaflet of Desmodium. 

For the accurate observation of the rate of movement of 
the Desmodium \eaflet during its different phases I have 
also been able to make records by means of a series of 
punctures produced by electrical sparks on a recording- 
surface. The sparks occur at the short gap between the 
end of the recording arm of a very light aluminium lever 
and the drum, these being connected respectively with the 
two electrodes of a Ruhmkorff’s coil. The electrical dis- 
turbance does not affect the 
plant, as the pulsating leaflet 
is separated from the other 
arm of the lever by a long 
silk thread. The primary 
current in the Ruhmkorff’s 
coil is broken at intervals of 
five seconds. Hence succes- 
sive punctures in the record 

Fic. 143. Spark-record of Single | represent intervals of five 
| ci cia in pene of Desmodium seconds each. I give here 
Showing time-relations of down- and . 

up-movements in single pulsation (fig. 143) a record obtained 
of leaflet of Desmodium. Up- in this manner, of a single 

curve represents down-movement ‘ 5 

of leaflet and wce versa. In- Mechanical pulsation of a 

i has successive dots  Jeaflet of Desmodium. It is 

to be understood that the 
up-movement in the record represents the down-movement 
of the leaflet. 

These movements are produced by excitatory con- 
tractions of the lower and upper halves of the pulvinus 
alternately. An inspection of the record given shows that 
after a pause in the highest position a sudden excitatory 
impulse is developed in the lower half, which is gradually 
exhausted as the lowest position is reached. The maximum 
rate of movement to which this excitation gives rise is in 
this particular case ‘7 mm. per second. After the lowest 
position is attained there is a pause. The up-movement 


ble ee a 


+e i 


i Dt Sie El ae ae al 


MULTIPLE AND AUTONOMOUS ELECTRICAL. RESPONSE 215. 


then takes place more gradually and at a much slower rate. 
This movement is due to natural recovery, aided by a 
moderate excitatory contraction of the upper half of the 
pulvinus. 1 give herewith a table showing the characteristic 
rates of movement in the different phases of the entire 
pulsation. 


TABLE SHOWING RATES OF MOVEMENT AT DIFFERENT STAGES OF 
PULSATION IN DESMODIUM. — 


Down-movement Up-movement | 


Total period. : 70 seconds 
Average rate . ‘61 mm. per second | Average rate . ‘4 mm. per second | 
Praximiim tte. "F* 5, nS gs Maximumy rate. *5 5, 53 55 


Duration of pause 40 seconds | Duration of pause. 35 seconds | 


Total period. ‘ 45 seconds 


Several facts are brought out in this table which are of 
special importance, and first we observe that the excitatory 
impulse which causes the down-movement is brief and quickly 
exhausted. -This is seen by the great distance covered 


during the first ten seconds, after which the movement 


gradually slows down. This indicates a short-lived impulsive 
action, the subsequent movernent of the leaflet being mainly 
due to inertia. There is then a pause in the down-position, after 
which the up-movement commences. It will be noticed here 
that this movement is more gradual and prolonged than the 


down-movement. From the indications given by these 


characteristic movements, we may conclude that the excita- 
tory reaction by which the down-movement is caused is 
relatively more intense and more quickly exhausted than 
that which brings about the up-movement. We may gauge 
the relative intensities of the two impulses approximately, 
either from the maximum or the average rates of the down 


and up motions. The former gives the ratio of a0 the 


latter gives Ohar52, The intensity of the downward 


impulse may therefore be taken to be roughly one and a-half 
times as great as that which occasions the up-movement. 


216 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


The total duration of the down-movement is again much less 
than that of the up. 

We have seen that a single excitation has a single con- 
comitant electrical pulsation. We have also seen the multiple 
electrical responses corresponding to multiple excitations. 
It remains, then, to find out whether autonomous pulsations 
have any electrical concomitant, and if so, of what nature. 
I shall here, therefore, describe experiments for the recording 
of the electrical pulsation of the Desmodium leaflet. For this 
purpose I selected specimens in which the movement of the 
leaflet was not spasmodic, but gradual and continuous, and 
where the up and down movements were pebromicnatcly ina 
straight line. 

In order to obtain those responsive electro-motive changes 
which might accompany the automatic movements of the 
leaflets, it was necessary to make one of the electric contacts 
at a point on the tissue which was free from excitation, the 
second contact being made at a place where the excitatory 
reaction was at its maximum. I have shown elsewhere! that 
the seat of autonomous excitation in Desmodium is neither 
central nor peripheral, but localised at the slender pulvinated 
joints to which the leaflets are attached, the latter thus serving 
merely as indicating flags. Acting on these considerations, I 
made one contact with the slender pulvinule of a lateral 
leaflet, the other being made with the common petiole. 
These electrical connections were made securely by means of 
cotton threads moistened with normal saline solution, and 
attached to non-polarisable electrodes. The electro-motive 
variations induced in the plant now gave rise to correspond- 
ing deflections in the galvanometer in circuit. On taking 
records of these electrical responses, I was surprised to find 
that, corresponding with each complete mechanical vibration, 
there was a double electrical pulsation—a large principal 
followed by a smaller subsidiary wave. In a given case, 
where the period of the complete mechanical vibration was 
about 3°5 minutes, the period of the principal of these two 

' Bose, Plant Response, p. 299. 


MULTIPLE AND AUTONOMOUS ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 217 


waves of electrical response was slightly less than I minute, 
and that of the subsidiary wave a little over 2°5 minutes. 
These double electrical pulses, corresponding to a single 
mechanical vibration, are at first very puzzling, and I under- 
took special investigations to ascertain the reason of this 
peculiarity. In order to obtain an insight into the relation 
between these mechanical and electrical responses it was 
necessary to take simultaneous records of the two on the 
same recording drum. This was accomplished by having 
the two recording spots of light—one from the galvanometer 
and one from the optic lever—thrown on the same horizontal 
slit, in front of the revolving drum, round which was wrapped 
a sensitive photographic film. The galvanometer spot of 
light, and consequently the electrical response record, was 
the lower of the two. The vertical movement of the spot of 
light which records the mechanical response is to be under- 
stood as converted into horizontal by reflection from a 
second mirror suitably inclined. This experimental arrange- 
ment is similar to that employed for simultaneous mechanical 
and electrical records in the case of Mimosa, as shown in 
fig. 12. 

The record given in fig. 144 exhibits the simultaneous 
mechanical and electrical responses thus obtained. It will 
be seen that the minor electrical wave took place while the 
leaflet was moving up from (a) and coming to its highest 
position at (6). This was followed by a wave of higher 
amplitude but shorter period, which coincided with the 
movement of the leaflet again from its highest to its lowest 
positions. It will thus be seen that the subsidiary electrical 
wave of small amplitude and relatively long period coincided 
with the slow up-movement of the leaflet, and that the 
principal wave, characterised by large amplitude and short 
period, corresponded with the quick down-movement of the 
leaflet. These galvanometric deflections indicated, it must be 
understood, a condition of galvanometric negativity of the 
pulvinule at the moments of its excitatory up and down 
movements. The following considerations make it easy to 


218 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


understand why two electrical waves correspond to one 
mechanical pulsation. First, we know that an excitatory 
change at a given point will have, as its concomitant, an 
electro-motive variation of galvanometric negativity. Second, 
the intensity of this electro- 
motive variation depends 
on the intensity of the 
excitatory change. And 
lastly, on the cessation of 
excitation there is an elec- 
trical recovery. 

Now we have seen from 
the spark-record (fig. 143) 
that the leaflet during one 
complete mechanical pul- 
sation is subjected to two 
excitatory impulses, occur- 
tring in the upper and 
lower halves of its pul- 
vinule alternately. It is 
these two excitations 
which give rise to the two 
electrical disturbances of 
galvanometric negativity. 
And the different ampli- 
Fic. 144. Photographic Records of tude and period in the two 

etenliapos Merten] yd, Pies” canes ‘ace. fully’ abooitted 
ab (upper figure) represents up-movement for by the different period 

of leaflet; a 4 (lower figure) corre- gnd intensity of the two 


sponding electrical subsidiary wave ; : ; 
6 a’ (upper figure) down-movement €xCitatory impulses. It 


of leaflet; 4 a' (lower figure) corre- yj] be remembered that 
sponding principal electrical wave. ; ; 
the excitatory impulse 
which produced the up-movement was the feebler and more 
protracted of the two. It is consequently attended by an 
electrical disturbance of moderate intensity and correspond- 
ing persistence. On the cessation of the up-movement, as we 
have seen, there is a pause, and during this time we find that 


MULTIPLE AND AUTONOMOUS ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 219 


electrical recovery takes place; but before this is complete, 
and while there is still a certain residual galvanometric 
negativity, there occurs that more intense and short-lived 
excitatory reaction which finds mechanical expression in the 
downward-movement. The same reaction finds electrical 
expression in a brief and intense response of. galvanometric 
negativity ; on the expenditure of this excitatory impulse 
there is again an electrical recovery, which becomes prac- 
tically complete. 2 

In the instance given in the spark-record, it was found 
that the relative intensity of the down to the up impulse was 
approximately as I'5 is to 1. And it is interesting to see 
that in the photographic record of the electrical responses 
(fig. 144), the ratio of the amplitudes of the corresponding 
electrical waves is also the same. In other instances, the 
relative intensity of the principal wave is still higher. I give 
below two tables showing the absolute. values of the electro- 
motive variations in two different cases. 


TABLE I. TABLE II. 
Syren rim variation. | E.M. variation. Deere | E.M. variation. | E.M. variation. 
ation rincipal wave Subsidiary wave ations Principal wave Subsidiary wave 
I ‘0014 volt | ‘odds 5 volt I | ee: folt "0016 volt | 
Ee 0k, ep 00051 re 2 "0025. 55 “OOI5, 55 
3 ‘OOI4 ,, 00054 5, 3 Lege ot, | oo16 ,, 
4 "OOI5 , "00054 5, 4 "0025 5, | O07. 55 
§ 54 "0016 4; — 5 | ‘0026 ,; | — 


It might: be thought that these two electrical waves had 
been induced by the mechanical movement of the leaflet as 
such. We have seen, however, that the electrical response 
is a concomitant of the excitatory condition, whether such 
excitation be followed by any mechanical response or not. 
This we saw in the absence of mechanical movement in the 
case of ordinary plants. The same was found also in the 


220 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


case of sensitive plants when responsive mechanical move- 
ments were prevented from taking place by physical 
restraint (p. 20). The mechanical and electrical responses 
are thus independent modes of expression of a single funda- 
mental excitatory process. In order to demonstrate this in 
the case of the autonomous pulsation of Desmodium, | first 
obtained simultaneous mechanical and electrical responses of 


Fic. 145. Photographic Record of Simultaneous Mechanical and 
Electrical Pulsation in Leaflet of Desmodium, before and after Physical 
Restraint of Leaflet. 


The first part of this record shows both mechanical and electrical pulsa- 
tion. In the second part, leaflet was physically restrained, as seen 
in the mechanical record, becoming horizontal. Electrical pulsation 
now seen to persist with even greater vigour than before. 


the leaflet (ig. 145). In the next part of the same record the 
mechanical movement of the leaflet was restrained, as seen in 
the upper mechanical record, which here becomes a straight 
line. But the lower record, which gives the electrical re- 
sponse, still shows the double electrical pulsation unimpeded. 
Indeed, so far from the mechanical response having been 
the cause of the electrical, we find that on its arrest, at 
least in this particular case, the latter becomes very 


MULTIPLE AND AUTONOMOUS ELECTRICAL RESPONSE 221 


much enhanced. In fact, it appears as if the fundamental 
excitatory reaction, being now deprived of one of its two 
modes of expression, exhibited the other with the greater 
energy. . 

We thus see that not only does the electrical response 
sive us a means of detecting the action of external stimulus 
on a tissue, but that 
the same mode of 
indication enables us 
further to demon- 
strate the existence 
of those internal ex- 
citations which may 
find mechanical ex- 
pression in the so- 
called ‘autonomous’ 
movements, 

One such autono- 
mous pulsation pre- 
‘sent in all plants is 
that of growth, and 
by means of the 
highly magnified re- 
cord given by the 
Crescograph. I have 
eee ue to: conaest Fic. 146. Crescographic Record of Multiple 
of the additive effects Growth-responses in Peduncle of Crocus 


of multiple minute The ordinate represents the extent of responsive 
elongations in mm. ; the abscissa, time in 
seconds. 


20” 40" 


pulsatory movements. 
Wesee this in fig. 146, 
which gives a series of records of multiple growth responses 
obtained with the peduncle of Crocus at different times of 
the day. In the present case, the average period of each 
pulsation is twenty seconds. In the case of relatively slow 
pulsations like these, if one electrical connection be made 
with the growing-point, where such movements are in pro- 
gress, and the other with an old leaf in which they have 


222 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


ceased, the galvanometer-spot of light is thrown into a state 
of oscillation, indicative of the local excitatory reactions at | 
the growing-point. 

These. multiple pulsations of growth consist of alternating 
positive and negative turgidity-variations. In dealing with 
the concomitant electrical response, however, we have seen 
(p. 64) that the induced galvanometric negativity, owing to 
its greater intensity, always overpowers the galvanometric 
positivity, if the two occur in rapid succession. In growth- 
pulsation, the constituent pulses are often extremely rapid. 
Hence a growing-point may be expected to exhibit, galvano- 
metrically, a resultant negativity. This consideration may 
explain the observation of Johannes Miiller-Hetlingen, other- 
wise unexplained, that the growing-points of both shoot and 
root, in the seedling of Pzsum sativum, are negative, as 
compared with the indifferent cotyledons. 


CHAPTER XVIII 
- RESPONSE OF LEAVES 


Observations of Burdon Sanderson on leaf-response in Dionea—Leaf-and- 
stalk currents—Their opposite variations under stimulus—Similar leaf-and- 
stalk currents shown to exist in ordinary leaf of F2cus religiosa—Opposite- 
directioned currents in Czt¢rus decumana—True explanation of these resting- 
currents and their variations—Electrical effect of section of petiole on Dzonca 
and Ficus religtosa—Fundamental experiment of Burdon Sanderson on 
lamina of Dzonea—Subsequent results—Experimental arrangement with 
symmetrical contacts—Parallel experiments on sheathing leaf of A/usa— 
Explanation of various results. 


IT was pointed out in Chapter II. that progress in the in- 
vestigation of the subject of excitatory phenomena in plants 
had been long delayed, in consequence of the prevalent idea 
that only motile plant-organs were ‘excitable.’ The atten- 
tion of investigators was thus mainly confined within the 
narrow range of the so-called ‘sensitive’ plants, such as 
Dionea. It was also shown, in the same place, that the 
results already arrived at by observers in this field had not 
been altogether concordant, and presented many anomalies, 

As it has now been demonstrated, however, in the course 
of previous chapters, that ordinary plants are fully sensitive, it 
will be well to proceed to show that the various effects 
observed in the ‘sensitive’ Dzong@a may be still better studied 
in ordinary leaves. It will be possible, moreover, by follow- 
ing this line of inquiry, to determine those general laws, of 
which the peculiarities observed in Dzonga are only instances ; 
and thus we shall be the better able to offer an explanation 
of such cases as now appear anomalous. Se 

Before doing this, I shall briefly recapitulate the principal 
effects observed by Burdon Sanderson in the leaf of Dzonea. 


224 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


These observations relate firstly to the existing current of 
rest in the petiole and midrib, and the variations of this 
resting-current, whether under excitation of the lamina, or by 
section of the petiole, or again, by the action of electro- 
tonus; and secondly, to the induction of variations of a 
transverse current between the upper and lower surfaces, of 
the lamina. As regards the current in the petiole and its 
prolongation the midrib, which I shall distinguish as ‘the 
longitudinal petiolar current, Burdon Sanderson found this 


Fic. 147. Natural and Responsive Currents in Leaves 


(a) Leaf- and stalk-currents in Dzonga. Natural current, N, flows out- 
wards < A-, s being stalk-current, and L leaf-current. Stimulation 
of lamina at x gives rise to responsive current, R, from right to left, 
inducing negative variation of leaf-current and positive variation of 
stalk-current. When stimulus, however, is applied on left at x, 
responsive current, R, is from left to right, inducing ¢ffects exactly 
opposite of former, viz. negative variation of stalk-current and positive 
variation of leaf-current; (4) Leaf- and stalk-currents in Ficus rel- 
giosa similar to those of Déowea. Natural current flows outwards 
<A->. Stimulation of lamina at x gives rise to responsive current, 
R, inducing negative variation of leaf- and positive variation of stalk- 
currents; (c) Leaf- and stalk-currents of C7ztrus decumana, opposite 
to those of Ficus and Dionea, > A<-. Stimulation at x induces 
positive variation of leaf and negative variation of stalk-currents. 


to flow in the midrib, from the end proximal to the stalk to 
the distal end. This he designated as the ‘normal leaf- 
current.’ He further found that if electrical connections were 
made, so that one contact was near the lamina, and the other 
away from it, the stalk-current was opposite in direction to 
the leaf-current (fig. 147 (a) ). 

On stimulation of the lamina, these resting leaf-and-stalk 
currents were found to undergo responsive variations. But 
these changes were exactly opposite to each other. That is 
to say, the leaf-current underwent a negative, and the stalk- 


RESPONSE OF LEAVES ik eee 


current a positive, variation. No explanation has as yet been 
offered, regarding either the existence of these opposite- 
directioned currents of rest, or the. apparently anomalous 
result, that an identical stimulus would induce, in one case 
a-negative, and in the other a positive, variation of them. 

As regards these peculiar currents of rest we have 

seen (p. 176), that if an intermediate point be physio- 
logically less excitable than either of the two terminal 
points, then a resting current will flow from the less to the 
more excitable. This is the particular current-distribution 
in the leaf of Dzonga. It is not a unique phenomenon, for I 
have noticed other such instances in ordinary leaves. The 
point of junction of the petiole with the lamina of Ficus 
religiosa, for example, is galvanometrically the most negative 
point in that petiole-and-midrib. _The currents here also, 
then, as in the case of Dzon@a, flow outwards from the point 
of junction—the leaf-current towards the tip of the leaf, and 
the stalk-current in the opposite direction (fig. 147 (0) ). 
_ We also saw, however, in the same place, that there may 
be instances in which an intermediate point is more excitable 
than either of the two terminal. When this is so, the 
currents of rest will be reversed in direction, and flow 
inwards. This I find to be the case in the leaf of Cztrus 
decumana (fig. 147 (c) ). 

Next with regard to the excitatory variation of these 
resting-currents in leaf and stalk, we must remember 
that the effect of stimulation is to give rise to a true 
excitatory current, flowing away from the excited. If then 
there be already a resting-current, the responsive current will 
be added to this algebraically. When the lamina to the 
right is excited, the responsive current flows from right to 
left. This would naturally, in the case of Dzonea, induce a 
negative variation of the leaf-current, and a positive variation 
of the stalk-current (fig. 147 (a) ). The same thing is seen 
on stimulating the lamina of Ficus religiosa, where also the 
excitatory current, being of opposite sign to the leaf-current, 
and of the same sign as the stalk-current, induces a negative 


Q 


226 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


variation of the former, and positive variation of the latter 
(fig. 147 (6) ). The same stimulus thus induces effects which 
are apparently opposite. Or an interesting variation of the 
phenomenon may be obtained, on repeating the experiment 
with the leaf of Cztvus. Here, on stimulating the lamina, 
we observe a positive variation of the leaf-current, and a 
negative variation of the stalk-current (fig. 146 (c)). This is 
because the currents of reference or resting-currents are the 
_ opposite of those in Dzonea and Ficus religiosa. 

Another series of variations exactly the reverse of these, 
and therefore at first sight anomalous, is caused by simply 
changing the point of application of stimulus, from the right 
end on the lamina, to the left end on the stalk. The direc- 
tion of the excitatory current is thus reversed, being now 
from left to right (fig. 147 (a) ). By algebraical summation, 
there now occurs a negative variation of the stalk-current, 
and a positive variation of the leaf-current, in Dzon@a and 
Ficus religtosa, while the very opposite takes place in Cztrus. 

I shall here draw attention once more to those errors to 
which an investigator becomes liable when he infers that 
positive and negative variations must necessarily be the 
expression of assimilatory and dissimilatory processes. For 
we have just seen that the same responsive current, by alge- 
braical summation with two opposite-directioned resting- 
currents, may appear to be both positive and negative, at 
one and the same time. Again, with a single resting-current, 
it is possible to obtain either a positive or a negative varia- 
tion, according as the same stimulus is applied to the right 
or the left. It is now abundantly clear that the one uni- 
versal effect of stimulus is to give rise to a responsive current 
which flows from the more to the less excited portions of the 
tissue. If there be already an existing current, the responsive 
current is added to this algebraically, and induces, according 
to circumstances, either a positive or a negative variation. 
Much confusion, and many erroneous inferences would be 
avoided, if instead of looking at these variable indications 
attention were centred on the one constant criterion, namely 


RESPONSE OF LEAVES 227 


that the excitatory current always flows from the more to the 
less excited portions of the tissue. 

Another effect observed by Burdon Sanderson was, that 
on cutting the petiole across, the existing normal leaf-current 
was increased, the amount of this increase being determined 
by the length of the petiole cut off, in such a way that the 
shorter the petiole left, the stronger the leaf-current became. 
In Nature (vol. x. p. 128), he suggested an explanation of this 
phenomenon. In the leaf of Dzonga, as already said, there 
is a resting-current in the stalk, opposed in direction to that 
in the leaf. Thus ‘the electrical conditions on opposite sides 
of the joint between stalk and leaf are antagonistic to each 
other ; consequently, so long as the leaf and stalk are united 
each. prevents or diminishes the manifestation of electro- 
motive force by the other.’ He thus inferred that the pro- 
gressive removal of the antagonistic element, by section of the 
stalk, would serve to enhance the intensity of the leaf-current. 

Taking the ordinary leaf of Fzcus religiosa, | have myself 
been able to obtain results precisely similar to those described 
in Dzonea, by making successive sections of the petiole, at 
shorter and shorter distances from the point of junction. 
The leaf-current at each section underwent an increment. 
The parallelism of the two sets of effects will be seen from 
the following table. 


Ficus LEAF. DION#A LEAF (BURDON SANDERSON). 

=e eh Sy a ae 
' Length of stalk Galvanometric deflection | Length of stalk | Galvanometric deflection 

om. | 16 divisions 2°5 cm. 40 divisions 

ty Stee 
4 >» 36 re) | 1°25 5, ; 50 2” 
2 » 5° ” | 06 ,, 65 ” 
| 
I 5, | 60 ;; bid AOS 5 opal re 90 5; 


Burdon Sanderson’s suggested explanation that the suc- 
cessive augmentations of the leaf-current were due to suc- 
cessive removals of the antagonistic element, by section, is 

Q2 


228 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


quite untenable. He failed to see that the effect was, on the 
contrary, due to the increasing excitatory action of the 
sections themselves. Similar results may be obtained, even 
without the bodily removal of the supposed antagonistic 
element, if, instead, we apply an increasing intensity of 
stimulus, as say, by contact of a hot wire at points nearer 
and nearer to that of junction. In the case of the trans- 
verse section, the cut acts as a stimulus, and the respon- 
sive current flows from the left to the right... Algebraical 
summation of this with the existing leaf-current, which is 
also from left to right, causes an increase, or positive variation 
of it, ina manner exactly the converse of the negative varia- 
tion induced in the leaf, when the stimulus was applied on 
the lamina. - As the section is made nearer and nearer to the 
point of junction, the degree of stimulation, and the con- 
sequent positive variation of the resting-current, must become 
greater and greater. 

And lastly, in the case of the longitudinal lesfieasPauk 
Burdon Sanderson found that if a current from a battery 
were directed through a leaf-stalk, at the same time that the 
two ends of the midrib were led off to the galvanometer, the 
difference previously existing between the ends of the midrib 
would be increased, if the current led through the leaf-stalk 
were in the same direction with the leaf-current, and 
diminished, if it were in the opposite direction. A similar 
effect, as seen in the conducting tissues of ordinary plants, 
will be studied in detail, when we take up the question of 
the extra-polar effects’ induced by electrotonic currents 
(Chap. XXXIX.). | 

We have already seen that, by means of induced varia- 
tion of the longitudinal stalk-current, under the stimulation 
caused by section of the petiole, it is easy to obtain an un- 
mistakable indication of the nature of the true excitatory 
electrical change. Burdon Sanderson, however, laboured 
under the disadvantage, as already said, of having failed to 
recognise that a section acts as a stimulus. His _ investi- 
gation, therefore, on the character of the excitatory variation, 


RESPONSE OF LEAVES 229. 


was chiefly carried out by means of experiments on electrical 
variations induced in the lamina. These depended (1) on 
variations in the cross-difference of existing potential between 
the upper and lower surfaces, according to his ‘fundamental 
experiment, and (2) on electrical variations in the led-offs of 
symmetrical surfaces of contact on the under-side of opposite 
lobes. The results which he obtained, however, by these 
methods, appear to the reader to have been very conflicting, 
and in fact the experimental methods described by him 
would seem to have been open to many sources of complica- 
tion of which he himself was unaware. 


Fic. 148. Burdon Sanderson’s Funda- Fic. 149. Parallel Experiment in 
mental Experiment on Dzonea Leaf Sheathing Petiole of Musa 
Electrical stimulus applied on distal Thermal stimulus applied on distal 
lobe, 7, induces responsive effect side induces responsive effect on 
on led-off circuit fw. Upper or led-off circuit. Upper or ‘internal’ 
internal surface, f, more excitable surface more excitable than lower. 


than lower, 772. 


I shall deal first with Burdon Sanderson’s ‘fundamental 
experiment, of which the excitatory electrodes are seen on the 
left lobe, and the led-off on the right in fig, 148, In fig. 149 
is given a diagram of a parallel experiment carried out by 
myself on the petiole of Musa. ‘According to Burdon- 
Sanderson, as the result of excitation, a + current is induced 
in the right lobe of Dzonea (fig. 150). This means, of 
course, that the upper or more excitable surface of the right 
lobe has become positive to the lower. This current, how- 
ever, he termed ‘excitatory,’ regarding it as the analogue of 
the ‘action-current’ known to animal physiology. After 
this first phase, when a certain interval had elapsed, he 


230 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


observed a second phase to set in, in which the upper surface 
became relatively negative to the lower. This negative 
change, which he called the ‘after-effect,’ he described as 
taking place at that moment at which the mechanical effect 
of excitation also made itself evident. 

This negative phase—called by him the ‘after-effect’— 
Burdon Sanderson regarded as connected with those electrical 
changes which had been observed by Kunkel to be induced 
by movement of water in the tissues. The first effect on the 


‘an 


Fics. 150,151,152. Recordsjof Electrical Responses of Different Leaves 
of Dionea according to Fundamental Experiment of Burdon Sanderson 


HATTA 


| 


| 


Fig. 150. Positive response of certain leaves of Dzonea. Time-marks 
20 per second (Burdon Sanderson). 
Fig. 151. Diphasic response of leaf of Droxea ‘in its prime.’ Positive 
followed by negative. Time-marks Io per second (Burdon Sanderson). 
Fig. 152. Positive response of same leaf when ‘ modified’ by previous 
stimulation. Time-marks Io per second (Burdon Sanderson). 
The above récords were obtained with capillary electrometer. 


contrary, which immediately preceded this, and was charac- 
terised by relative positivity of the upper surface, he regarded, 
as already mentioned, as the true excitatory or action-effect. 
The following is from his summary : 


‘The first phase of the variation--the effect which 
immediately follows excitation, and has an opposite 
sign to the after-effect, and a much higher electro-motive 
force—does not admit of a similar explanation: for it 
cannot be imagined that a change which spreads over 
the whole lamina in less than one-twentieth of a second 
can be dependent on migration of water. The excita- 
tory disturbance which immediately follows excitation 


RESPONSE OF LEAVES 231 


is an explosive molecular change, which by the mode of 
its origin, the suddenness of its incidence, and the 
rapidity of its propagation, is distinguished from every 
other phenomenon except the one with which I have 
identified it—namely, the corresponding process in the 
excitable tissues of animals. Of the nature of this 
preliminary disturbance (to which alone the term ex- 
citatory variation ought to be applied, it alone being 
the analogue of the ‘action-current’ of animal physio- 
logy) we know nothing. ... The direction of the ex- 
citatory effect in the fundamental experiment is such as 
to indicate that in excitation, excited cells become 
positive to unexcited, whereas in animal tissues excited 
parts always become negative to unexcited. The ap- 
parent discrepancy will probably find its explanation in 
the difference of the structural relations of the electro- 
motive surfaces.’ } 


_ From this quotation it will be seen that Burdon 
Sanderson had fallen into the basic error of mistaking what 
I have demonstrated to be the hydro-positive, for the true 
excitatory effect, and wece versa. 

In a subsequent Paper again (Phz/. Trans. vol. 179, 1889) 
Burdon Sanderson published certain results, which differed 
from those referred to above. He had previously found that 
usually speaking the upper surface of each lobe was negative 
to the lower. Later, however, he came to the conclusion 
that in the leaf of Dzonga in its ‘prime, the upper surface 
was positive to the under. On repeating his ‘fundamental 
experiment’ moreover, with these vigorous leaves, he found 
that instead of the pronounced positive response which he had 
previously observed, he now obtained a short-lived positive 
effect succeeded by a strong negative (fig. 151). He was 
unable to offer any definite explanation of this difference 
between the two sets of results, but suggested that it might 
arise, in some way, from changes of the resting-current. 


’ Phil, Trans. 1882, vol. 173, p- 55: 


232 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


‘In the leaf, observed facts show most conclusively 
that the two sets of phenomena—those of the excited 
and those of the unexcited state—are linked together 
by indissoluble bands: that every change in the state 
of the leaf when at rest conditionates a corresponding 
change in the way in which it responds to stimulation, 
the correspondence consisting in this, that the sign, that 
is the direction, of the response is opposed to that of the 
previous state, so that, as the latter changes sign in the 
direction from + to |, the former changes from | to +.’ } 


In making this statement, Burdon Sanderson was _ prob- 
ably guided by the prevalent opinion that response takes 
place by a negative variation of the existing current of rest. 
We have seen, however, that this supposition is in fact 
highly misleading. For, owing to such fluctuating factors as 
age, season, previous history, or excitation due to prepara- 
tion, the so-called current of rest may and frequently does 
undergo reversal. Thus a single excitatory effect might, as we 
have seen (pp. 175-177) under different circumstances, appear 
either as a positive or a negative variation of the existing 
current. The assumption of the universality of response by 
negative variation is thus seen to be unjustifiable. 

Indeed, it would appear from the description of some of 
the experiments actually related by Burdon Sanderson him- 
self, that response did not, even in these cases, always take 
place by negative variation of the existing current. For 
instance, while in the leaf of Dzon@a in its ‘prime’ (upper 
surface positive) the response is negative, and while this 
latter becomes reversed to positive, as he tells us, in conse- 
quence of ‘ modification’ due to previous excitation (fig. 152), 
yet headmits that even in these circumstances the upper surface 
had first returned to positivity (zbzd. p. 447). Thus, though 
the responses of the leaf in its ‘prime, and of the ‘ modified ’ 
leaf are opposed, yet the antecedent electrical condition of 
the modified leaf has not in this case undergone reversal. 


Phil. Trans. 1889, vol. 179, p. 446. 


RESPONSE OF LEAVES 233 


The suggestion, therefore, that the reversal. of response is 
due, in some way unexplained, to a reversal of the electrical 
condition of the leaf, cannot hold good. Nor does the use of 


the term ‘ modification’ in any way 
of the phenomenon. A satisfactory 
explanation of this reversal of 
response, then, still remains to be 
found. 

So much for the ‘fundamental 
experiment. The next experi- 
mental arrangement employed by 
Burdon Sanderson consists of a leaf 
which is led off by symmetrical 
contacts on the under surfaces of 
its two lobes (fig. 153). If now the 
right lobe was excited, by touching 


assist in the elucidation 


FIG, 153. Experimental Con- 
nections with Dzonea ac- 
cording to the Second 
Experimental Method of 
Burdon Sanderson 


one of the sensitive filaments (on the upper surface) with a 
camel’s-hair pencil, in the neighbourhood of the leading-oft 
contact, it was found that the under-surface of the right lobe 
became first positive, and subsequently negative (fig. 154), 


relatively to the left (zdzd. p. 440). 


Mn 


i 


Fic. 154. Response of Under-surface of Leaf of Dzon@za, with Electrical 
Connections as in Fig. 153 


{I 


Mechanical excitation of upper surface of right lobe. shows relative 


positivity of under surface of same right 
its relative negativity (down curve). 
(Burdon Sanderson). 


lobe (up curve), followed by 
Time-marks 20 per second 


Summarising these various observations, then, we find 
results which are very much at variance. First, according 
to the ‘fundamental experiment,’ certain leaves are seen to 
give rise to the positive response; other leaves, in their 


prime, give diphasic response, the 


upper surface becoming 


234 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


first positive and then negative. These latter again, after 
previous excitation, become so modified as to show only 
positive changes. And lastly, using the experimental 
arrangement of symmetrical contacts, a diphasic variation 
is obtained—positive followed by negative—on the under- 
surface, instead of the upper, of the lobe excited. No theory 
is advanced, however, by which a comprehensive explanation 
might be afforded of these apparently anomalous results. 

But from the generalisations which I have already esta- 
blished, regarding the electrical signs of the hydro-positive 
and true excitatory effects respectively, and from the results 
of certain experiments on ordinary leaves which I shall 
presently describe, it will be found easy to arrive at a true 
explanation of the various observations related by Burdon 
Sanderson, which would otherwise have appeared inexplic- 
able. The fact that hydrostatic disturbance induces galvano- 
metric positivity, and that true excitation induces negativity, 
has already been clearly demonstrated under conditions from 
which all possible sources of complication had been elimi- 
nated (p. 61). The experimental arrangement adopted by 
Burdon Sanderson, however, laboured under the double dis- 
advantage, not only of a liability to confuse the hydro- 
positive and true excitatory effects, but also of the com- 
plexity arising from the differential excitability of the 
responding organ. It is only indeed by the closest analysis 
that it is possible to discriminate, in his results, between such 
as are due to true excitation and those arising from the hydro- 
positive effect. 

The various electrical phenomena which are possible in 
an anisotropic organ in consequence of the hydro-positive 
and excitatory effects respectively, may be clearly exhibited, 
as I have already shown, by means of the mechanical 
response of the leaf of MW/zmosa. With regard to this, we 
have seen (pp. 59, 60) that direct stimulation of the pulvinus 
induces a negative mechanical response, or fall of the leaf, 
by the greater contraction of the more excitable lower half 
of the organ. The corresponding electrical variation would 


RESPONSE OF LEAVES 235 


thus consist in the greater galvanometric negativity of this 
more excitable lower, in relation to the less excitable upper half. 
If the stimulus, however, be applied at some considerable 
distance, so that true excitation cannot reach the responding 
point, then we have an erectile or positive mechanical 
response of the leaf. This is brought about by the relatively 
greater expansion of the more excitable. The corresponding 
electrical response will be the galvanometric positivity of 
this more excitable, in relation to the less excitable half of 
the organ. Between these two extremes lies that experi- 
ment in which stimulus is applied at some intermediate 
point, the consequence of which is that the hydro-positive 
wave, with its greater velocity, reaches the responding organ 
earlier than true excitation, thus bringing about a pre- 
liminary erectile or positive response, followed by the ex- 
citatory negative or fall of the leaf. The corresponding 
electrical response would therefore be diphasic, positive 
followed by negative. 

But the occurrence of this second or negative phase is 
only possible when the conductivity is so great as to allow 
the wave of true excitation to reach the organ. We may 
imagine that in a very vigorous plant, with its great con- 
ductivity, we have found a point, at the maximum distance 
from which the true excitatory effect of a given stimulus is 
capable of transmission to the organ. With such a speci- 
men, in its ‘prime,’ we shall observe a diphasic effect—pre- 
liminary positive followed by negative. But if we took a 
less vigorous specimen, and applied the stimulus at the same 
distance from the responding point, the true excitatory wave 
would fail to reach the responding organ, and we should see 
there, only the positive effect due to hydro-positive action. 
Hence, two different specimens, treated in exactly the same 
way, may exhibit two different effects, one diphasic, and the 
other positive alone; this difference being due to their 
unequal vigour, and concomitant inequality of excitability 
and conductivity. This will account for the diphasic and 
positive responses which were exhibited by the more and 


236 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


less vigorous leaves respectively of Dzonga, when stimulation 
was applied on the distal lobe, according to the fundamental 
experiment of Burdon Sanderson. 

We must next refer to the reason why a leaf that origin- 
ally gives diphasic response—positive followed by negative 
—undergoes such ‘ modification, in consequence of: previous 
excitation, as thereafter to give only positive response. We 
have seen that the negative element of the diphasic response 
is due to the arrival at the responding point of the true 
excitatory wave originated at the distant point of stimula- 
tion. Now it has been shown (p. 65), that if by any 
means the conductivity of an intervening region should 
become diminished, we may expect that the hydro-positive 
effect will continue to be transmitted, although the passage 
of true excitation is partly or wholly blocked. By-means of 
this selective block, I was able to unmask the hydro-positive 
component present in resultant response (cf. fig. 49). 

I have shown elsewhere! that the conducting power of a 
tissue will be impaired by the fatigue consequent on previous 
stimulation. Thus, in the petiole of Bzophytum, | found that 
while the plant, when fresh, had a conductivity measured 
by the velocity of transmission of excitation, at a rate of 
1°88 mm. per second, the same plant, when partially fatigued 
by four successive stimulations, had its conductivity dimi- 
nished, the velocity of transmisson being now only 1°54 mm. 
per second. The diminution in this case, then, was about 
18 per cent. I shall moreover show in a later chapter 
that in consequence of growing fatigue the passage of true 
excitation may at a certain stage be arrested, the hydro- 
positive effect alone being then transmitted. It is thus easy 
to explain how it was that in Burdon Sanderson’s experi- 
ment, of stimulus applied on the distal lobe, the wave of 
true excitation became blocked, and the ‘ modified’ leaf gave 
positive response alone. These considerations will be found 
as I think, to offer a satisfactory explanation of the conflicting 
results arrived at by Burdon Sanderson, 

1 Plant Restonse, Pp. 244. 


RESPONSE OF LEAVES 237 


I shall now, however, proceed to describe a series of ex- 
periments exactly parallel to the ‘fundamental experiment’ 
on Dionea, carried out on ordinary plants. We have seen 
that the inner or concave surface of the sheathing petiole of 
Musa is relatively more excitable than the outer or convex. 
Thus it corresponds with the ‘internal’ or upper surface of the 
leaf of Dzonga. The more excitable internal surfaces of both 
these, again, correspond with the more excitable lower half 
of the pulvinus of AZzmosa. In fig. 149 is shown an experi- 
mental arrangement with a specimen of JZusa which will be 
seen to be parallel to that of Burdon Sanderson’s funda- 
mental experiment on Dionea. In order to avoid any such 
disturbance as might conceivably arise from current-escape, if 
the electrical form of stimulus were used, I employed the 
thermal mode of stimulation. A momentary heating-current 
passed through a thin platinum wire gave the thermal varia- 
tion required, and was found to furnish a very satisfactory 
form of stimulus. The led-off circuit was at first placed at a 
distance of 16 mm. from the point of stimulation. As the 
stimulation was moderate, and as the conductivity of the 
tissue was not great, the effect induced at the respond- 
ing circuit was hydro-positive, the more excitable concave 
surface becoming positive (fig. 155 (a) ). This response is the 
same as the positive responses given by the ‘unmodifiable 
leaf’ of Dzonea (fig. 150), as well as that of a vigorous leaf 
which had been ‘modified’ by fatigue (fig. 152). On next 
taking a second pair of led-off points, at the shorter distance 
of 8 mm., the hydro-positive effect reached the led-off points 
earlier, and was followed by the true excitatory wave. This 
is seen as a preliminary positive response, followed by the 
excitatory negative (fig. 155 (2)). This again is the same 
as the di-phasic response of a Lzonea leaf in its ‘prime’ 
(fig. 151). Inthe experimental arrangement with J/usa the 
led-off circuit was now brought still nearer to a distance of 
4 mm. There was now little interval between the arrival 
at the led-off points of the hydro-positive and true excitatory 
effects; and since the latter is of predominant electrical 


238 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


expression, the former is masked by it, and we obtain here 
only the excitatory negative variation (fig. 155 (¢) ). 

It only remains to consider the responses which Burdon 
Sanderson obtained with symmetrical contacts (fig. 152) on the 
under-surfaces of the two lobes. In the next figure (fig. 153) 
is reproduced his record of electrical response, obtained on 
mechanical stimulation of a sensitive filament situated on 
the upper surface of ‘the right lobe, vertically above the 
right-hand led-off. This response is, as will be seen, di- 


Fic. 155. Photographic Records of Positive, Diphasic, and Negative 
Responses ot Petiole of A/usa depending on the Effective Intensity 
of Transmitted Stimulus 


(az) Here stimulus was applied at a distance and hydro-positive effect 
alone transmitted ; (6) Stimulus was applied nearer, and the positive 
effect was succeeded by the true excitatory negative; (c) Stimulus was 
applied very near, with the result of true excitatory negative response. 


phasic, its first phase being one of relative positivity of 
the under-surface of the excited lobe, and the second 
representing its subsequent relative negativity. This first 
phase is clearly due to the earlier transmission of the 
hydro-positive or indirect effect of excitation, from the 
stimulated point on the upper surface. It was supposed 
by Burdon Sanderson that the second phase of this re- 
sponse represented the later arrival of the same positive 
effect at the distal second contact, which would thus induce 
reversal. But it appears much more probable that this 
second phase of negativity is due to the arrival at the 


RESPONSE OF LEAVES 239 


under-surface of the wave of true excitation, initiated 
vertically above. This relative negativity of the under- 
surface may or may not be helped by the induction of 
positivity at the distal, due to the transmission of the hydro- 
positive effect. This view is supported by the fact that in a 
corresponding experiment on an ordinary leaf, in which the 
second contact was at a distance too great to allow of the 
effective transmission of any hydro-positive wave, the 
stimulation of the upper surface induced a similar diphasic 
response at a point diametrically opposite, on the under side. 
In this case the second or negative component of the 
response could not be due to anything but the subsequent 
arrival of the true excitatory wave with its concomitant 
negativity. 

It is now clear that among the various results 
obtained from the study of the electrical responses of the 
leaf of Dzonga, there are some which do not represent 
true excitation at all, while in others it is only one of the two 
phases which is significant of this, the other being due to the 
hydro-positive effect. We have also seen that Burdon 
Sanderson at starting fell into the error of wrongly identify- 
ing the true excitatory electrical effect with that which was 
due to the hydro-positive effect, and vice versa. We have seen 
that there is not a single response given by the so-called 
excitable leaf of Dzong@a, which cannot be obtained under 
similar conditions from the leaves of ordinary plants also. 
In fact it has been by means of experiments carried out on 
the latter that we have been enabled to unravel all the 
intricacies which were offered by the recorded responses of 
the lamina of Dzonea. : 

It has further been shown in the course of the present 
chapter that the leaf and stalk currents observed in Dzonga 
are also found in, for instance, the leaf of Ficus religiosa. 
These have been shown, moreover, to be due to physiological 
differences between an intermediate and the terminal points. 
The negative variation of the leaf-current, and the positive 
variation of the stalk-current, on the stimulation of the 


240 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


_ lamina, were both alike shown to be the result of the alge- 
braical summation of a definite excitatory current with the 
two opposite-directioned resting-currents. The positive 
variation of the leaf-current, again, on section of the petiole, 
has been traced to the same cause, namely the stimulatory 
action of mechanical section, giving rise to an excitatory 
current which was summated with the existing leaf-current. 
Finally the positive response of the concave surface of 
Dionega has been shown to arise, not from any specific 
difference between plant and animal response, but from the 
fact that in this particular case it was the indirect hydro- 
positive effect of stimulus that was transmitted, inducing an 
action opposite to that of true excitation. 


CHAPTER XIX 
THE LEAF CONSIDERED AS AN ELECTRIC ORGAN 


Electrical organs in fishes—Typical instances, Zorfedo and Malepterurus— 
Vegetal analogues, leaf of Pterospermum and carpel of Déllenta indica or 
pitcher of Mefenthe—Electrical response to transmitted excitation — Response 
to direct excitation—Uni-directioned response to homodromous and hetero- 
dromous shocks—Definite-directioned response shown to be due to differential 
excitability— Response to equi-alternating electrical shocks—Rheotomic ob- 
servations-—Multiple excitations—Multiplication of terminal electromotive 
effect, by pile-like arrangement, in bulb of Uyzc/zs lily. 


IT has been shown that by a study of the peculiarities of 
electrical response in plants, it is possible to obtain an 
insight into the obscurities of similar responses in the animal 
tissue. Among animal structures, there is one—the elec- 
_trical organ of certain fishes—the explanation of whose action 
offers unusual difficulties to the investigator. But I shall 
attempt to show in the course of the present chapter, that 
there are also, on the other hand, vegetable structures, the 
study of which will be found to elucidate the electro-motive 
action here involved. Taking that of the Torpedo as type, 
we find that the electrical organ is disposed in the form of 
columns, each column consisting of. numerous electrical 
plates, arranged in series, one over the other, like the plates 
in a voltaic pile. Each electrical plate consists of a rich 
plexus of nerve-fibres imbedded in a gelatinous mass. 
There are thus two surfaces, one nervous and the other non- 
nervous. Each disc then becomes electro-motive under the 
impulse from the nerve. Though the induced electro-motive 
force in each plate is small, yet in consequence of their serial 
arrangement in columns, the elements are coupled for inten- 
sity, and the resulting E.M.F. of discharge becomes high, 
R 


242 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Fritsch estimates the total number of these plates in some of 
the Torpedos to be over 150,000. 

From the point of view of their development, these 
electrical organs in general constitute modified muscles, 
containing nerve-endings, The electrical fish known as 
Matepterurus of the Nile is an exception to this rule, inas- 
much as morphological evidence goes to prove that in its 
~ case it is glandular, rather than muscular, elements which 
have been so modified. 

The peculiar characteristic of the discharge of electrical 
organs in general, is that it takes place in a definite direction 
at right angles to the plates. It was Pacini who tried to 
establish the generalisation that the direction of the dis- 
charge would be found to be dependent on the morphological 
character of the organ. He found that as a general rule 
the discharge takes place in a direction from that surface of 
the disc which receives the nerve (henceforth to be referred 
to as the anterior surface) to the opposite non-nervous, or 
posterior, surface. Thus in the Zorfedo, where the plates 
are horizontal, and the anterior or nervous surface constitutes 
the ventral aspect of the disc, the discharge is from the 
ventral or anterior, to the dorsal or posterior surface. In 
Gymmnotus again, the plates or discs are vertical to the long 
axis, The anterior or nervous surface is here towards the 
tail-aspect, and the discharge is from tail to head. If these 
cases had been all, Pacini’s generalisation, as regards the 
direction of discharge—from the anterior nervous to the 
posterior non-nervous—would have been complete, and from 
it some attempt might have been made to offer an explana- 
tion of the phenomena. Unfortunately, however, this is not 
so, since Malepterurus presents a hitherto inexplicable ex- 
ception to the rule. In this fish, though the anterior or 
nervous surface is towards the tail-aspect as in Gymnotus, 
yet the discharge is in the opposite direction towards the 
head : that is to say, from the posterior surface to the anterior. 
The difficulties in the way of an explanation of the activity 
of these electrical organs of certain fishes are thus seen to be 


THE LEAF CONSIDERED AS AN ELECTRIC ORGAN 243, 


very great. Is the activity something specific occurring in 
these fishes alone, and unrelated to other electro-motive 
phenomena in the animal tissues? Or is it related to the 
electromotive action already observed in excited muscles? 
In support of the latter view, it is urged that most of the 
electrical organs consist of modified neuro-muscular elements, 
Against this argument, however, as we have just seen, is the 
instance of Malepterurus, in which, from a morphological 
standpoint, the organ is to be regarded as a modified gland, 
and therefore not muscular in character, 

There are certain peculiarities, further, about the action 
of these organs which call for elucidation. Among these 
is the question of the character of the natural current of rest, 
about the significance of which there have been differences 
of opinion. There is also the fact that the organ, under a 
single strong excitation, gives rise not to one, but to a series 
of electrical responses. 

We have seen that the apparently unique character of 
this group of organs constitutes an added difficulty in 
arriving ata correct theory on the subject. But it is clear 
that if we could succeed in discovering among vegetable 
organs any cases which showed similar characteristics, 
we should then be so much the nearer to the determination 
of that fundamental reaction on which the phenomenon in 
animal and vegetable alike depends, 

In the typical case of Zorpedo, it has been seen that the 
conducting nerve, when entering into an electrical plate, 
breaks into an extensive ramification, and thus forms the 
nervous surface, in contradistinction to the jelly-like sub- 
stance in which it is imbedded, forming the opposite, and 
here indifferent surface of the plate. Now this arrangement 
is closely imitated by many ordinary leaves, in which the 
vascular elements break, on reaching the lamina, into a pro- 
fuse arborisation. | 

I must here anticipate matters to say that I have discovered 
in the fibro-vascular bundles of plants (see ei hap. XXXII.) 
elements which are in every way peel eres to the nerves of 

R2 


244 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


animals. For an exact vegetal analogue to the electrical 
plate of Zorpedo, we may take certain leaves in which the 
ventral, or anterior, surface is formed of a prominent network 
of highly excitable nervous elements, while the upper consists 
of an indifferent and relatively inexcitable mass of tissue. An 
example of this may be found in the leaf of Pterospermum 
subertfolium (Rox.) whose lower surface is characterised by | 
a remarkably perfect venation, while the upper or posterior 
is dry and leathery. Thus the nerve passing into an elec- 
trical plate of Zorvpedo corresponds with the petiole attached 
to the leaf just described, since in the two cases alike, it is the 
ventral surface which contains the highly excitable nervous 


elements. 
In the exceptional JMJalepterurus, on the other hand, - 


it is, as we have seen, a modified gland, and not a modified 
muscle, which forms the posterior surface of an individual 
electrical element. Morphologically speaking, the vegetal 
analogue is found in such organs as the carpellary leaf of 
Dillenta indica, or the pitcher of Nepenthe, both of which 
are glandular on their upper or inner surfaces. In point 
of structure, then, these leaf-organs are analogous to single 
discs or elements of the electrical organs of TZorgedo and 
Malepterurus respectively. But we have still, in the course 
of the present chapter, to inquire whether the electrical 
reactions are equally correspondent—that is to say, whether, 
on stimulation, the excitatory current in the type of vege- 
table organ represented by the leaf of Pterospermum is or 
is not, from the lower or anterior surface to the upper 
posterior, as in the electrical plate of Zorpedo; and, con- 
versely, whether in the type represented by the carpel of 
Dillenia or the pitcher of Wefenthe the excitatory current is 
from the posterior to the anterior surfaces, corresponding 
with the discharge in the electrical element of MWalepterurus, 
from the posterior glandular to the anterior non-glandular 
surface. 

While dealing with the theory of the action of electrical 
organs, I shall be in a position to show that the characteristic 


THE LEAF CONSIDERED AS AN ELECTRIC ORGAN 245 


reaction of each of these two types is governed entirely by 
the question of the relative excitabilities of the two surfaces. 
The physiological anisotropy on which the distinctive effect 
of the type depends is very pronounced in the representa- 
tive cases of the vegetal analogues which have been named. In 
many other cases, however, though the results under normal 
conditions are fairly definite, and approach one or other of 
the two types, yet the characteristic responses are liable to be 
reversed under the physiological modifications induced by 
age and surrounding conditions. In this way it may be said 
of the leaf of water-lily (Vymphea alba), of Bryophyllum 
calcineum, and of Coleus aromaticus that when vigorous, and 
in their proper season, their responses are of the first of these 
two types, while those of the bulb-scale of Uviedis lily, with 
its glandular inner surface, are of the second type. 

The electrical organ of the fish may be excited indirectly 
by means of stimulus transmitted through the nerve; or 
direct stimulation may be applied, as by means of induction- 
shocks. Under either of these conditions the excitatory 
discharge is definite in its direction. In the case of Jorpedo, 
as already mentioned, this is always from the ventral and 
anterior to the dorsal or posterior surface. Turning then to 
the corresponding vegetable organ of the first type, I shall 
show that transmitted stimulus induces an effect exactly 
similar ; and I shall demonstrate this experimentally by means 
of the leaf of Vymphea alba. Suitable galvanometric connec- 
tions were made with the ventral anterior and with the dorsal 
posterior surfaces of the lamina. Thermal shocks, by means 
of the electro-thermic stimulator, were applied on the petiole, 
close to the lamina, at intervals of one minute, records being 
taken photographically of the resulting responses. It should 
be remembered here that excitation is transmitted to the 
lamina by the conducting nerve-like elements present in the 
petiole. The records (fig. 156) show that the effect of this 
periodically transmitted stimulation was a series of respon- 
sive currents, whose direction was like that of the discharge 
in Torpedo, from the anterior surface to the posterior. 


246 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Of great importance was the investigation carried 
cut by Du Bois-Reymond on the effects induced in the 
electrical organ by the passage of currents in different 
directions. Polarising-currents in the direction of the natural 
discharge of the organ are distinguished, in the terminology 
introduced by Du Bois-Reymond, as homodromous, and 
those in the opposite as heterodromous. Polarisation-effects 
in the direction of the natural discharge he distinguishes as 
‘absolutely positive polarisation, and 
against that direction ‘as absolutely 
negative.’ A polarisation-current in 
the same direction as the polarising- 
current he calls ‘relatively positive,’ 
and in the opposite direction ‘rela- 
tively negative’ polarisation. It 
was found by him that polarising- 
currents of fair intensity and short 
duration, whether homodromous or 
heterodromous, would always give 
rise to polarisation-currents in the 
same direction as the natural dis- 
mea, re6. Mice charge. He believed this to be due 

of Lamina of Vympheaalba to the occurrence in the electrical 
due to Transmitted Excita- % . ors. 
ah an Prole organ of two different polarisation- 
Direction of responsive current effects, positive and negative. This 
incaat ttl avper serace will be understood from his own dia- 
grammatic representation (fig. 157) 
of the effect which he supposed to take place immediately 
on the passage of the polarising-current. In the upper 
figure the ascending arrow represents the homodromous 
polarising-current. This gives rise, according to Du Bois- 
Reymond, to two opposite polarisation-effects. The de- 
flection seen in the galvanometer is the resultant of these, 
represented by the shaded part of the figure. The resultant 
of a homodromous current, then, is positive polarisation, 
both absolute and relative. The heterodromous current, on 
the other hand, induces absolutely positive and relatively 


THE LEAF CONSIDERED AS AN ELECTRIC ORGAN 247 


negative polarisation. 


According to Du_ Bois-Reymond, 


further, heterodromous shocks induce no relatively positive 
polarisation, or only infinitely little (see down curve in lower 


part of figure). 
from Saxton’s. machine, he 
obtained only the absolutely 
positive _ polarisation - effect. 
This he accounted for by 
supposing the relatively nega- 


tive polarisations in both 
directions to cancel each 
other; the heterodromous 


positive to be so small as 
to be practically negligible ; 
and the homodromous posi- 
tive. therefore to be alone 
effective. 

Du Bois-Reymond failed 


to recognise the element of 


excitation. in. these 
nomena. . What he 
positive polarisation has been 
shown by subsequent workers 
to be due to local polar ex- 
citation. But the question 
as to how polarising-currents 
in both directions could give 
rise to a_ single-directioned 
responsive effect has not up 
to the present, so far as I am 
aware, been explained fully 
and satisfactorily. The ex- 


phe- 
calls 


On sending congruent wireless currents 


Fic. 157. Diagrammatic Representa- 
tion. by Du Bois-Reymond for Ex- 
planation of Electrical Response in 
Organ of Torpedo. 


- The natural discharge is here supposed 


to be from below to above. A 

’ homodromous current + (upper half 
-of figure) is supposed to, induce 
two opposite polarisations, positive 

» and negative. The resultant, repre- 
sented by shading in "figure, is 
absolutely and relatively positive. 
A heterodromous current |, on the 
other hand, is regarded as inducin 
a resultant absolutely positive and 
relatively negative polarisation 
(lower part of BEUrE ja: 


periments carried out on leaves, which I am sidan to 
describe, will, however, throw much light on this subject. 

It has already been shown, from anatomico-physiological 
considerations, that there are certain leaves which approxi- 
mate to the character of single plates of such electrical organs 


248 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


as that of Zorpedo. One such leaf, already mentioned, was 
that of Pferospermum. When induction-shocks are sent in 
both homodromous and heterodromous directions through 
such a leaf, between upper and lower surfaces, the leaf being, 
it is understood, in a normal condition, a responsive current 
is found to be evoked, always in one direction—that is to say, 
from the lower or anterior to the upper or posterior surface. 
This is strictly parallel to the electrical reaction observed by 
Du Bois-Reymond in Zorfedo. 

That this result is really due to the excitatory effect 
is proved by the fact that the same is found to occur 
when other forms of stimulation 
are used. Thus, if we place the 
leaf of Coleus aromaticus within 
a surrounding thermal helix, suc- 
cessive thermal shocks, acting 
simultaneously on both surfaces, 
give rise to responsive currents 
which are, as in the last case, 
Fic. 158. Photographic Records from the lower anterior to the 

pete silat Site se Seat upper posterior surface. Fig. 158 

ae te Thema Pee gives a series of such responses. 
Resultant responsive current from From the fact which has already 

more excitable anterior toless been fully established, that on 

sa rca a the simultaneous excitation of 
two points the responsive current is always from the more to 
the less excitable, it is quite clear that in the present case it 
is the lower or anterior surface of the leaf which is the more 
excitable. These responsive currents, obtained under a non- 
electrical form of stimulus, and similar to those evoked by 
electrical shocks, completely demonstrate the fact that the 
result is brought about, not by polarisation, either positive 
or negative, but by the afferential excitability of the tissue 
itself. The response of electrical organs in general, then 
may be summarised in the following law: 

The excitatory discharge is determined by the physiological 
anisotropy of the organ, its definiteness of direction being deter- 


THE LEAF CONSIDERED AS ‘AN ELECTRIC ORGAN 249 


mined by the fact that the responsive current is always from the 
more to the less excitable of the two surfaces. 

Referring once more to the definite-directioned _after- 
current which we have seen to be induced as the result of 
polarising-currents, whether homodromous or heterodromous, 
it is now clear that these currents act as an _ electrical 
form of stimulus. The intensity of the after-current 
here seen in the galvanometer, however, is not wholly due 
to the excitatory electro-motive change, but in part also to 
physical polarisation, which is added to it algebraically. Thus, 
an exciting homodromous shock gives rise to an electrical 
after-effect, in which the excitatory current is opposed by 
a counter-current of negative polarisation. Under a hetero- 
dromous shock, on the other. hand, the excitatory electrical 
change becomes summated with the negative polarisation, 
which is now in the same direction as itself. In these cases, 
though the preponderating nature of the excitatory effect 
determines the definite direction of the after-effect, yet it is 
difficult to know how much of the latter is actually due 
to excitatory action as such, and how much to ordinary 
polarisation helping or opposing this. 

Very much greater complexities ensue again in practice 
rom the difference between anodic and kathodic actions 
on the two unequally excitable surfaces. In Torpedo, for 
instance, according to Du Bois-Reymond, the electrical organ 
responds better to a homodromous than to a heterodromous 
exciting current, while in J/alepterurus, according to Gotch, 
the reverse is the case, the heterodromous being more efficient 
than the homodromous. Such diversity of results is prob- 
ably to be accounted for by the considerations to which I 
have referred. 

If we take, tor example, the simplest case, that in which 
the anterior surface is more excitable than the posterior, and 
if we suppose an induction-current of moderate intensity 
to be sent in a homodromous direction, we may assume that 
Pfliiger’s Law—the kathode excites at make, and the anode 
at break—will hold good. We shall here, for the sake of 


250 . COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


simplicity, neglect any effects that may accrue from anode- 
make and kathode-break. Under a homodromous induction- 
shock, then, two different excitatory electrical changes will 
be induced, on the lower and upper surfaces respectively, the 
consequent currents through the tissue being in opposite 
directions. On these, moreover, will be superposed again the 
polarisation-current. Calling the effect induced by anode- 
break as A, and that of kathode-make as K,, we shall obtain 
a resultant consisting of A,on the more excitable anterior 
surface, mznus K,, on the less excitable posterior, mznxus the 
negative polarisation-effect. Under a heterodromous shock, 
on the other hand, we shall have K,, on the more excitable 
anterior surface, mznus A’ on the less excitable posterior, plus 
the negative polarisation-effect. 

Even this, however, does not exhaust the possibilities 
of complication. _ For I shall show in a subsequent Chapter, 
and have already shown elsewhere, that under a high E.M.F. 
Pfliiger’s Law does not apply. The relative excitatory 
values of anode and kathode may indeed undergo one or 
more reversals, according to the intensity of the acting 
electro-motive force. Thus, under a moderately high E.M.F. 
in what I have designated the A stage, both the anode and 
kathode are found to excite at make, and either kathode or 
anode at break. In the B stage, under a still higher E.M.F., 
it is the anode which excites at make, and the kathode at 
break. 

It will thus be seen what a number of complicating 
factors may be present when an organ is excited by currents 
of varying direction and intensity. If, then, we wish to study 
the purely excitatory reaction of an organ, as dependent 
solely upon its individual characteristics, uncomplicated by 
defects inherent in the method of excitation, we must see 
first that the applied stimulus is equal on both surfaces, and, 
secondly, that such factors as are not excitatory—that is to say, 
negative or counter-polarisation—are eliminated. These ends 
may be accomplished by subjecting the responding organ 
to symmetrical and alternating equal and opposite shocks, 


THE LEAF CONSIDERED AS AN ELECTRIC ORGAN 25I 


following each other in rapid succession. For the resultant 
negative polarisation will in practice be neutralised, if the 
primary polarising currents are similar, equal, and opposite. 
The stimulus applied on the two surfaces, moreover, will be 
equal, if the two rapidly succeeding and opposite-directioned 
shocks be so symmetrical as to be interchangeable. Which- 
ever may. be the factor of excitation will then act equally on 
both surfaces. The response, therefore, will now be determined 
solely by the natural difference of excitability as between the - 
two surfaces. | 

It has been said that in order to accomplish these experi- 
mental conditions, the two opposite shocks should be equal in 
intensity and in point of time-relations. An ordinary make- 
and-break Ruhmkorff’s shock does not fulfil this condition, since 
the break-shock is there the quicker'and more intense of the 
two. Moreover, owing to the varying residual magnetisation 
in the iron core, successive shocks may not be equal. These 
defects are overcome by sending round the primary, with 
a constant rapidity, two equal and opposite currents in 
alternation. During one semi-cycle, then, the primary current 
varies from + C to — C,and during the next from — C to 
+ C, and since these two changes are effected with the same 
rapidity, the induced currents are. symmetrical, equal, and 
opposite. | 

Such reversal of current is accomplished by means ofa 
rotating reversing-key. The key R is wound up against the 
tension of a spring S, being maintained in this set position 
by the electro-magnet E, acting on the armature. When the 
current in the electro-magnet is broken, the alternating 
double shock from the induction coil I is passed through the 
experimental leaf L, by means of non-polarisable electrodes 
N, N,. In the case just described the sequence of the 
current through the. primary coi] was, say, right-left-right. 
In the next experiment, by means of the Pohl’s commutator, 
K,, this sequence may be made left-right-left (fig. 159). 

Empioying this method, I have carried out rheotomic 
observations for determining the time-interval after the shock 


fr 
} 
\ 

{ 


252 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


at which the E.M.F. attained its maximum. The general 
arrangement here is similar to that described in Chapter IV. 
(cf. fig. 37). C is the compensator by which any existing 
electro-motive difference is compensated at the beginning of 
each experiment. The striking-rod A breaks the current 
in the electro-magnet E, by which the rotating reverser R is 
actuated, which brings about equal and opposite shocks to 
the leaf. The galvanometric after-effect, at any short 


All lis 


= Tc = 


bt 
> Sh on See 


Fic. 159. Experimental Arrangement for Rheotomic Observations 


A, B, striking-rods attached to revolving rheotomic disc; K,, key for 
electro-magnetic release of rotating reverser R; K,, key for unshunting 
the galvanometer when pressed by rod, B, for a definite period; ky, 
key for preliminary adjustment ; E, electro-magnet with its armature 
by which rotating reverser, R, is set against antagonistic spring, S ; 
K,, Pohl’s commutator; Cc, compensator; P, primary, and 1, the 
secondary, of the exciting induction-coil ; N,, N,, non-polarisable elec- 
trodes, making electrical contacts with posterior and anterior surfaces 
of leaf. 


interval after excitation, is obtained by the un-shunting of 
the galvanometer, caused by the striker B impinging against 
the key K, (fig. 159). We have seen that, owing to the 
presence of various complicating factors, as well as to the 
occurrence of negative polarisation, successive responses to 
homodromous and heterodromous shocks are unequal. By 
the employment of equi-alternating induction-currents, how- 


THE LEAF CONSIDERED AS AN ELECTRIC ORGAN 253 


ever, we obtain true excitatory effects, unmodified by any 
such elements of uncertainty. In order to show how perfect 
the results obtained by this method become, I give here 
(fig. 160) the records of two successive excitatory responses 
obtained from a leaf of Bryophyllum calycinum, the responsive 
current being from the lower or anterior surface to the upper 
or posterior. In this mode 
of stimulation, by equal 
and opposite shocks, as 
already said, no advantage 
is given to either surface 
over the other. Neverthe- 
less, I thought it well to 
take two successive records 
under shocks, in which the 
alternating currents in the 
primary circuit were first 
right-left-right, and then left- 
right-left. 

In the electrical organ 
of TZorpedo Gotch found 
the maximum electromotive 
change to be attained in 
about ‘oI second after the 
application of the excitatory 
shock. In leaves, again, I 
find the rapidity with which 
the maximum effect is at- 

. Fic. 160. Records of Two Successive 
tained to depend on the Responses in Leaf of Bryophylium 
nature of the tissue. and calycinum under Equi-alternating 

‘ Electrical Shocks 

also on the intensity of the 

exciting shock. In sluggish specimens this may be as long 
as ‘2 second. It should be remembered that in the case 
of mechanical stimulation of moderate intensity also, this 
period was, similarly, about *2 second (p. 51). With very 
vigorous leaves of Vymphaea alba, however, and employing 
a stronger electrical stimulus, the maximum effect was 


254 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


attained in a much shorter time —that is to say, in about 
‘03 second. I give below a table showing the rheotomic 
observations made on such a leaf at gradually increasing 
intervals after the exciting shock. It should be remembered 


TABLE OF RHEOTOMIC OBSERVATIONS. 


Mean interval after the shock | Galvanometric deflection 
| 
‘ol of a second ‘20 divisions | 
°03 9° 99 | 63 99 | 
"O§ 9° > I 7 Je | 
°O7 3° +] I 5 9 | 
I 99 39 20 99 
°2 9° bi] 9 23 | 
‘ 99 ” 8 oe) | 
*h 29 rm) 5 ? 


that the recording galvanometer was un-shunted for ‘ol 
second, The curve given in fig. 161 has been plotted from 
these results. The maximum 
electro-motive change took place, 
as already pointed out, in ‘03 
second after the application of 
stimulus. This curve shows 
multiple apices, as was also the 
case, it will be remembered, after 
a strong mechanical stimulation 
(cf. fig. 40). This point will be 
referred to in greater detail in 
the next chapter. In the course 
of half a second after the shock, 
the . excitatory  electro-motive 
change had subsided to about 
one-twelfth of the maximum, 
Fic. 161. Response-curve from It has been said that the 
scp teeer epee onLeaf excitatory current depends for 
Ordinate represents galvanometer its definiteness of direction on 
deflection; abscissa,: time in the physiological anisotropy of 
hundredths of a second. 
the organ. In_ those leaves 
in which the physiological differentiation of the upper and 
lower surfaces is not strongly marked, the differential 


I CU 


THE LEAF CONSIDERED AS AN ELECTRIC ORGAN 255 


excitability of the two is liable to undergo reversal, under 
the changing conditions of age, season, and fatigue induced 
by previous stimulation. In the leaf of Pzerospermum, 
however, which I have here taken as the type corresponding 
with Zorpedo, the normal differential excitability is generally 
very persistent. Here the excitability of the posterior 
surface, which is leathery, is slight, and practically negligible. 
But the anterior surface, with its rich and prominent venation, 
is highly excitable, The excitatory discharge of such a leaf 
is thus from the anterior to the posterior. I give in fig. 162 
a series of its responses to equi-alternating electric shocks, 
It will be seen that these are 
very uniform, and exhibit 
practically no signs of fatigue. 
We have thus found a 
vegetable organ whose re- 
sponses are exactly parallel 
to those of a single plate of 
the electrical organ of Zorpedo 
and its type. We shall next ens 
study the responsive pecu- TiC; 162. Series of Responses, given 
liarities of the vegetable organ folium to Stimulus of Equi-alternat- 
Base responses correspon d ng eectneal Shocks at Intervals ot 
with those of the organ of 
Malepterurus. \t has been mentioned that the posterior surface 
of each single element of this electrical organ is regarded as 
consisting of a glandular, rather than a muscular, modification. 
Among corresponding leaf-organs, then, the carpellary leat 
of Dzllenta indica might, as we also saw, be taken as the 


‘type, its posterior surface: being glandular. Or the analogy 


will be still more perfect if we take as the vegetal type the 
pitcher of Vepenthe. Here the internal or posterior surface 
is richly provided with glands. -The next point to be deter- 
mined is whether, in these cases also, on excitation the 
responsive current is from the posterior surface to the anterior, 
as in the electrical element of Madepterurus. And on sub- 
jecting them to equi-alternating electrical shocks, I found 


256 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


this to be the case. The responsive current here flowed from 
the glandular posterior to the non-glandular anterior surface. 
From this experiment we see that a glandular surface is 
exceptionally excitable, a conclusion which will be found to 
be supported by the numerous experiments on glandular 
organs in general, to be described in Chapter XXIV. I give 
in fig. 163 a series of photographic records, obtained on 
excitation of Dzllenza indica. In the next record (fig. 164) 
are seen the responses given by the pitcher of Wepenthe. 


Fic. 164. Photographic Record 
of Normal Responses given by 


; ic Record of Re- ; saat 
Fic. 163. suis ge Gy rae Pitcher of Mepenthe, under Equi- 
sponses 0 a alternating Electric Shocks 
Natural current from posterior to an- ‘ ene 
terior, and responsive current from sie scare aca woes harden 
anterior to posterior surfaces. hg nae he donee 
glandular surface. Note ten- 


dency to multiple response. 


An interesting fact to be noticcd in the latter is the tendency 
to multiple response. ' 

Similar results were also obtained on taking any single 
scale of the bulb of Uvzclzs lily about the time of flowering. 
In each of these the lower or outer surface is invested with a 
more or less dry and glistening membrane, while the upper 
or concave is moist and glanduloid. The moisture observed 
inside each scale is in fact exuded from this inner surface. 
On subjecting one of these scales, then, to the electrical 


THE LEAF CONSIDERED AS AN ELECTRIC ORGAN 257 


excitation already described, it is found that a very strong 
responsive current is obtained, whose direction is, as in the 
last case, from the glanduloid to the non-glanduloid surface. 

The effect of the serial arrangement, again, in enhancing 
the electro-motive force—as seen in the pile-like arrangement 
of the electrical organ of fishes—may be exemplified, in 
the parallel instance of the plant-organ, by means of the 
superposed scales of the bulb, as found in nature. The bulb 
may be divided longitudinally into halves, of which the 
right-hand half is mounted, for experiment, with the scales 
vertical. It will be understood that all the glanduloid 
surfaces here face the left, while the non-glanduloid are 
turned to the right. Thus the left aspect of this pile 
corresponds to the head aspect of the organ of JZalepterurus. 
The latter, on excitation, responds by a current in the 
direction of head to tail—that is to say, from glandular to 
non-glandular ; and similarly, in the pile-like half-bulb of 
Uricls lily, the responsive current is from glanduloid to non- 
glanduloid—that is to say, from left to right. 

Another interesting way to perform the same experi- 
ment is without making any section of the bulb. We 
take a bulb of Uvzclzs, with the peduncle rising out of the 
middle. When this hollow peduncle is cut across, it allows of 
an electrical connection being made with the centre of the 
interior of the bulb. An equatorial belt makes the second, 
or outer, connection. On subjecting this to equi-alternating 
shocks, the resulting response will be found to be from the 
inner surface to the outer, through the numerous intermediate 
scales, the individual effect in each being concordant and 
additive: . 

We have thus seen how the response of a leaf gives us an 
insight into the action of a plate of an electrical organ ; how 
the differential excitabilities of the two surfaces give rise on 
stimulation to an induced E.M.F. as between the two; how 
a nervous and indifferent-tissued surface will give rise toa 
response in one direction, and a glandular and non-glandular 

S 


258 COMPARATIVE. ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


in the other; and finally how, by a serial arrangement, the 
terminal electro-motive effect becomes enhanced. The light 
thus thrown on the two types of response, known to occur in 
the electrical organs of fishes, is evident. Further con- 
siderations, relating to the theory of electrical organs, will be 
given in detail in the next chapter. 


EE a i 


et EA 


ee 


CHAPTER XX 


THE THEORY OF ELECTRIC ORGANS 


Existing theories—Their inadequiacy—The ‘blaze-current’ so called— Response 
uni-directioned, to shocks homodromous or heterodromous, characteristic of 
electric organs—Similar results with inorganic specimens — Uni- directioned 

_response due to differential excitability—Electrical response of pulvinus of 

| Mimosa to equi-alternating electric shocks—Response of petiole of Musa— 
Of plagiotropic stem of Cucurbita—Of Eel—The organ-current of electric 
fishes—- Multiple responses of electrical organ — Multiple responses of 
Biophytum. 


ONE of the most perplexing problems in connection with 


the phenomena of electrical organs is the question as to 


whether the activity of such organs is specific—that is to say, 
peculiarly characteristic of them—or-falls into line with the 
other electro-motive reactions observed in animal tissue. 
Many arguments have been brought forward for and against 
the identity of these phenomena with the excitatory reactions 
of the nerve and muscle. : 

From the experimental results which I have Aéecribert 


however, it would already appear that such reactions as these 


of the electrical organ are not specifically characteristic, even 
of the animal structure, but may equally well be observed in 
plant tissues. It is therefore essential, if we are to determine 
that basic reaction which is common to all alike, that we 
should find a wider generalisation than has hitherto been 
contemplated. This basic reaction, as we have already seen, 
depends upon the differential excitability of an anisotropic 
organ, and this aspect of the case we are now. about to study 
in greater detail. Before doing this, however, we shall briefly 
glance at various theories which have been suggested, but are 
generally admitted to be inadequate. | 
$2 


260 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Bell, for example, thought it might be possible to explain 
‘the discharge of the electrical organ solely by the negative 
variation of the nerve-current, concomitant with innervation. 
In this arrangement the dorsal surface of the electrical plate 
(of Zorpedo) would, at the moment of innervation, become 
positive, the ventral surface negative, as actually occurs.’ 

As against this, it was pointed out by Du Bois-Reymond 
that this hypothesis in the first place predicates the existence 
of a current of rest, caused by the ‘natural’ cross-sections 
(acting like artificial sections) of the nerves in the plates, and 
accordingly heterodromous to that of the discharge. Instead 
of this permanent current—which must correspond in E.M.F. 
with the discharge, if the nerve-current is to disappear in the 
negative variation—there is only an inessential P.D. during 
rest, and the resulting ‘organ-current’ is always homodromous 
with the discharge.! | 
Even had these objections not existed, however, Bell’s 
hypothesis would have failed to explain the electrical action 
of Malepterurus. Du Bois-Reymond himself tried to explain 
the action of the electrical organ, ‘not by the negative 
variation of the nerve current, but by a process in the 
electrical plates transformed from muscles, comparable with 
the negative variation of the muscle-current, as set forth from 
the standpoint of the pre-existence theory.’ Here, however, 
it is perhaps sufficient to point out that the pre-existence 
theory, on which the hypothesis was based, is now held to be 
invalidated. 

There remains only the Chemical, or Alteration Theory, 
which associates all electrical changes with the corresponding 
chemical processes of assimilation and dissimilation. But 
it has not been made clear in what way these can bring about 
the characteristic discharge of the electrical organ. 

There is another point, not altogether unrelated to this 
subject, which may be dealt with on the present occasion, 
I allude to the so-called ‘blaze current’ of Dr. Waller. By 
this is meant an after-current in the same direction as the 

' Biedermann, Z/ectro-Physiology (English translation), vol. II. p. 462. 


THE THEORY OF ELECTRIC ORGANS 261 


exciting current. It is, in, fact a new name for that phe- 
nomenon which Du Bois-Reymond indicated as ‘ positive 
polarisation-current. Du Bois-Reymond had also shown 
that this particular effect was most markedly exhibited when 
the functional activity or ‘livingness’ was at its highest. 
Under opposite conditions, again, it would disappear. The 
intensity of this homodromous after-effect was thus dependent 
on the degree of vitality of the tissue under experiment. 
Hermann and Hering, however, afterwards showed that what 
Du Bois-Reymond called ‘ positive polarisation’ was in reality 
excitatory reaction. These excitatory effects are known to 
be caused by either the anode or the kathode!; and I have, 
in the course of the last chapter, demonstrated the fact that 
it is the differential excitability of a tissue which determines 
such uni-directioned response. It is difficult, therefore, to see 
the necessity of a new name for these phenomena. Dr. 
Waller himself, however, offers the following as an important 
reason : 


‘The great mass of living things, whatever else they 
may give and take from their surroundings, take oxygen 
and give carbonic acid ; they may live slowly or they 
may live quickly—sluggishly smoulder or suddenly 
blaze. A muscle at rest is smouldering: a muscle in its 
contraction is blazing ; the consumption of carbohydrate 
and the production of CO,, never absolutely in abeyance, 
even in the most profound state of rest, are sharply 
intensified when the living machine puts forth its full 
power, and there is then a sudden burst of heat, and an 
electrical discharge... .’? 


This amounts to another way of saying that the cause of 
the excitatory galvanometric effect is some explosive dis- 
similatory change, a view which I have already shown in 


1 ‘Within a given ‘ physiological ” range of strength of current the negative 
kathodic must, equally with the positive anodic, be designated an ‘“‘irritative ” 
after-current, due entirely to ‘‘ polar current-action.” ’—Biedermann, Zéctro- 
Physiology (English translation), vol. I. p. 448. | 

* Waller, Signs of Life, p. 74. 


262 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


previous chapters to be quite untenable. I shall presently 
describe experiments which will further show that galvano- 
metric responses, not to be distinguished from this, take place 
when there is no possibility of any consumption of carbo- 
hydrates or production of CO,. , 

The fact, however, that the excitatory after-effects de- 
scribed, disappear on the death of the tissue, has led Dr, 
Waller to put forward the generalisation that this so-called 
‘blaze-current’ is the final distinction between living and 
non-living matter. His formula, with regard to this, is, ‘ If 
the object of examination exhibits blaze in one or in both 
directions, it is living.’ He admits, nevertheless, that a sub- 
stance which is undoubtedly living will not always exhibit 
the ‘blaze-current.’ -But it is contended that the occurrence 
of ‘blaze’ is an undoubted ‘sign of life, and that thus 
a strong distinction is to be made between vitalistic and 
non-vitalistic, or physical, reactions. Hence, as there is 
supposed to be no excitatory reaction possible in non-living 
or inorganic matter, it would follow that electrical shocks 
passed through such a substance, in either direction, should 
give rise only to those counter-polarisation currents which 
are known to physicists. In such cases, on reversing the 
direction of the shock, the direction of the after-current 
is also reversed ; but in the living substance, it is maintained, 
the case is quite different. If the direction of the shock be 
here reversed, the after-current will still appear, with direction 
unchanged, because in this latter instance it is not generated 
by the shock, but is, on the contrary, an inexplicable function 
of the living material, set in action by it, in the same way as 
a loaded gun is fired by pulling the trigger. The possibility 
of obtaining from the given substance such a uni-directioned 
after-current, independently of the direction of the shock, is 
thus to be taken as the test and token of ‘ vitality.’ 

_ Now, while it is certainly true that.the domain of physio- 
logical phenomena has not yet been so thoroughly explored 
as that of the physical, it is nevertheless equally true that 
no one could venture to claim that even physical phenomena 


THE THEORY OF ELECTRIC ORGANS 263 


had up to the present been exhaustively studied. It is, then, 
somewhat hazardous to declare that because a particular 
phenomenon has not yet been observed to occur in inorganic 
matter, it is by that fact demonstrated to be hyper-physical 
in its nature, and must be relegated to the different and 
mystical category of the exclusively vitalistic. The very 
foundation of such a statement would be swept from under 
it, the moment it was shown that the same phenomenon 
followed, under the same circumstances, in conditions which 
were admitted to be purely physical. 

I have shown, it will be remembered, in the previous 
chapter, that the uni-directioned response to electrical 
shocks in either direction was due to the differential 
excitability of the structure. The response of any aniso- 
tropic organ would always be from the more to the less 
excitable, the more excitable becoming relatively galvano- 
metrically negative. There may here be various cases of 
excitation, all giving results of the same type, say, a respon- 
‘sive current from B to A. The first is that in which, on 
excitation, both B and A become galvanometrically negative, 
A being the less so of the two. In the second case, the excit- 
ability of A being slight, or negligible, B alone becomes 
negative. And in the third ‘case, excitation induces 
positivity of A and negativity of B. In all these cases the 
relative negativity of B being greater, the responsive current 
will flow from B to A. The resultant current is made up, : 
in the first case, by subtracting the galvanometric negativity 
of A from that of B; in the second case, it consists of the 
galvanometric negativity of B, that of A being zero; and in 
the third case, it is produced by the addition of the effect 
at A to that at B. Examples of the last of these will 
be found in certain animal and vegetable skins, described 
in Chapter XXII. 

These being the conditions, then, for the induction of 
the uni-directioned responsive current, it appeared to me 
probable that the same result could be obtained with 
inorganic substances, provided that the specimen were so 


264. COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


prepared as to be anisotropic, one side having a greater 
potentiality of galvanometric negativity under excitation 
than the other. In that case, further, it was clear that the 
strongest resultant current would be obtained, if one surface 
of the structure became galvanometrically positive, and the 
other negative, on excitation. I have already stated, in 
Chapter I., that different inorganic substances give electrical 
responses of opposite signs. Thus the response of lead is 
positive, while that of brominated lead is negative. If, then, 
we take a lead wire A B, 
clamped in the middle 
at C, as represented 
in the upper diagram 
(fig. 165), and stimulate 
the right-hand end B, 
say by mechanical 
vibration, a responsive 

- current will be induced, 
Fic. 165. Responsive Currents in Lead Wire which will flow. to- 


Upper figure—Excited wire, galvanometrically : 
positive. Simultaneous excitation of both wards the stimulated, 


ends balance each other ; resultant response B thus becoming gal- 
‘-Z6ro. . : : 
Lower figure—Left portion, lead wire, right vanometrically posi- 
portion, brominated lead wire, shown as tive. The same will 
shaded. Response of first positive, of second A 
negative. Simultaneous excitation of the be the case with 4, 


erg andes, fella reponse which“ on stimulation. When 
both A and B are simul- 

taneously stimulated, it is evident that the two responsive 
currents, being antagonistic, will cancel each other. But 
if the right-hand half B’ of the wire consist of brominated 
lead (lower diagram, fig. 165), while the left-hand half a’ is of 
lead like that used in the first case, then stimulation of B’ will 
cause a responsive current to flow away from the right-hand 
excited end, B’ thus becoming galvanometrically negative ; 
A’, on the other hand, will give rise to a positive response 
towards the excited. Simultaneous excitations of A’ and B’ 
will not then be antagonistic, but additive in their effects. 
The resultant response will thus be from the negative b’ to 


<n 


THE THEORY OF ELECTRIC ORGANS 265 


the positive A’. We may next take a flat strip of lead, of 
which the lower surface B is brominated... On mechanical 
stimulation of both surfaces by vibration it will now be 
found that the resultant responsive current flows across the 
strip, from the negatively-responding B to the positively- 


_responding A (fig. 166). It will be seen that here—as the 


molecular disturbance 
on which excitation 
depends is caused by a 
non-electrical form of 
stimulus—the electrical 
character of the response 


obtained is unexcep- Fic. 166, © Flat Strip of Lead, of which 

tional in its freedom lower Surface is Brominated 

from any complication Response of upper surface positive, of lower 
y Pi : surface negative. Resultant response 

by the  polarisation- from lower to upper. 

factor. 


Let us next consider what would be the effect of sending 
induction shocks across such a strip. If inorganic bodies 
be really inexcitable, then we can here obtain only the 
counter-polarisation current as the after-effect. That is to 
say, on sending a shock from below to above in an ascending 
direction, we should observe only negative polarisation as 
an after-effect, or a descending current from above to below. 
A descending shock should, on the other hand, give rise to 
an ascending after-current. But if inorganic substances 
should prove to be excitable, and if B were to become on 
excitation relatively negative to A, as we found in the 
experiments on mechanical stimulation, then, whatever the 
direction of the shock, we should have a uni-directioned 
response, from below to above, exactly like the excitatory 
discharge of the electrical organ of Zorpedo, from the ventral 
surface to the dorsal. 


‘ In order to brominate this surface as required, it may be held exposed to 
vapour of bromine. Or an electrical deposit may be made by electrolysis, in a 
bath of potassium bromide solution. A certain depth of deposit and time of 
formation are important to the successful preparation of the plate. When these 
are secured, the proper condition of responsiveness is found to be long-enduring 
as will be seen from the photographic records. 


266 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


I give below (fig. 167) photographic records of the 
responsive after-effects actually obtained on carrying out 
this experiment on the lead strip prepared as described. It 
is here seen that whatever had been the direction of the 
‘shock, homodromous + or heterodromous |, the responsive 
current was always one-directioned : that is to say, from the 
more negatively-excitable lower to the positively-excitable 
upper surface. If the test of the ‘blaze-current’ is to be 
accepted, then we shall be compelled to refer this. metallic 
strip to the category of organic matter in a living condition ! 


Fic. 167. Photographic Records of After-effect of Homodromous *¢ and 
Heterodromous | Induction-shocks in Prepared Strip of Lead 


Note uni-directioned response, whatever be direction of induction-shock. 


Since it is admitted that the electrical response of living 
tissues is due to molecular excitation, and since we obtain 
similar electrical effects, as just seen, from the inorganic, it 
is clear that excitability is the property, not of the organic 
alone, but of all matter. It is also clearly seen that the one. 
directioned response of an anisotropic structure—so charac- 
teristic, among other things, of the electrical organ of 
certain fishes — depends on its differential molecular excit- 
ability. “The phenomenon is thus reduced to what are almost 
its last terms—namely, the molecular. It is therefore un- 
necessary to call in the aid of such indeterminate factors as 
vitalism, or assimilation and dissimilation, 


THE THEORY OF ELECTRIC ORGANS 267 


In taking these responses to homodromous and hetero- . 
dromous shocks, the counter-polarisation-current is super- 
posed upon the excitatory effect. For this reason we see, 
in fig. 164, that the homodromous response, in which 
negative polarisation was an opposing factor, is smaller than 
the heterodromous, in which polarisation conspires with the 
excitatory current. We have seen, in our experiments on 
the leaf-organ, that when this negative polarisation is 
annulled by taking responses under equi-alternating elec- 
trical shocks, successive responses, giving the true excitatory 
effect, become equal. ~ I 
give here (fig. 168) a series 
of responses of the pre- 
pared lead strip, under 
these conditions of equi- 
alternating shocks, in 
which they are seen to 
have become equal, the 
resulting responsive cur- 
rent being from the lower 
surface to the upper, as 
before. 


Fic. 168. Photographic Record of Re- 


Having thus demon- sponses to Equi-alternating Electric 
strated in various ways See et Si of One Minute in 
epared Lead Strip 
the nature of that funda- Responsive Current as before from lower 
mental condition, which to upper surface. 


determines the excitatory discharge of the electrical, organ, 
we may next take other organs which are known to be 
differentially excitable, and observe whether they also, 
under electrical excitation, give definite uni-directioned 
responses. 

The differential excitability of the upper and lower 
surfaces of the pulvinus of J/zmosa are well known. On 
subjecting this organ, then, to equi-alternating electrical 
shocks, I obtained uni-directioned responsive currents, their 
direction being from the more excitable lower to the less 
excitable upper half, of which a photographic record is given 


268 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


in fig. 169. In this particular specimen, owing to growing 
fatigue, the responses are seen to undergo diminution. In the 
sheathing petiole of Musa also, we found, as will be remem- 
bered, that the inner or concave surface was more excitable 
than the outer or convex. In taking records of the responses 
of such a specimen to equi-alternating electrical shocks, the 
direction of the responsive current was found to be from the 
more excitable inner to the less excitable outer surface. 

It is now evident that in the 
structure of any single element of 
an electrical organ, the essential 
question is not ‘as to the corre- 
spondence of one part or another 
with muscle, nerve, or gland; but 
merely that of anisotropy. Any 
tissue which is anisotropic is 
potentially an electrical element. 
Thus a stem of Cucurbita origin- 
ally radial, becomes physiologically 
anisotropic when it happens to 
assume the recumbent posture, 
owing to the a-symmetrical action 
Fic. 169. Response of Pulvinus of environmental stimuli. Such 

of Mimosa to Equi-alternating __ : , 

Flectric Shocks an anisotropic organ, when elec- 
Responsive current from more trically excited, gives a one- 

Srna gad to less excit- Girectioned responsive current 

from the lower or ventral to the 
upper or dorsal surface. A similar reaction is, curiously enough, 
shown by the body of the Eel, which also, on excitation, gives 
a responsive current from the ventral surface to the dorsal. 

The responsive peculiarity of the constituent elements of 
the electrical organ is thus not unique. The extraordinary 
character of the organ depends merely upon the serial 
arrangement of innumerable such elements, connected with 
a nerve-trunk, by means of which a voluntary impulse is 
enabled to bring about the excitatory discharge of the 
electrical pile, and so convert it into a weapon of offence. 


THE THEORY OF ELECTRIC ORGANS 269 


The most difficult problem with regard to these electrical 
organs having thus been solved, it remains to say a few 
words concerning two other points: namely, the natural 
current of rest and the repeating character of the excitatory 
discharge. 

As regards the first of these, Du Bois-Reymond found 
the current of rest—which he designated as the organ-current 
—to be in the direction of the electrical discharge. We have 
found, however, that, as a general rule, in the primary 
condition, the true natural current flows in the opposite 
direction to that of excitation, that is to say, from the less 
to the more excitable. In agreement with this, I find, in 
the leaf of Pterospermum, the electrical reaction of which 
is similar to that of the plate of Zorpedo, that while its 
excitatory current is from the more excitable ventral to 
the less excitable dorsal, its resting-current is opposite to 
this—that is to say, from dorsal to ventral. With regard 
to the electrical organ, then, there can be little doubt that 
here also the true resting-current would have been found 
to be opposite in direction to the excitatory, if it could 
have been observed and recorded under an absolute con- 
dition of physiological rest. In a highly excitable struc- 
ture, however, the shock of preparation, as we have seen 
and shall further see, leaves an after-effect which reverses 
the natural current of rest. For this reason, the current 
of rest observed in preparations of the electrical organ 
has not, in all probability, represented the true current, but 
rather the excitatory reversal of it. This view is supported 
by the fact that while in the organ-preparation of 
Torpedo, this reversed, or ingoing-current of rest is con- 
siderable, in the intact fish it is negligible. It must be borne 
in mind that the fish is spontaneously excitable, and also 
that there must remain a certain residual effect from the 
organ-discharges. Nevertheless Zantedeschi found a resting- 
current to occur in the intact fish in the reverse direction to 
that of the excitatory discharge. This was evidently the 
true resting-current. This view of the organ-current, of 


390; COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Du Bois-Reymond, as, in reality, a persistent after-effect of 
excitation, gathers confirmation from an observation made 
by Gotch, that it is considerable in 7 orpedo, in which the 
excitatory effect also is known to be very persistent ; but in 
Malepterurus, where recovery from excitation is rapid, this 
particular organ-current is practically absent from excised 
preparations. | | 7 

We may turn now to the repeated or oscillatory character 
observed in the electrical discharge by both Gotch and 
Schénlein. This is seen in the multiple apices ‘of its 
rheotomic curve. Multiple apices are also found, as we have 
seen, in the rheotomic observations on vegetable organs, 
given in fig. 40. Gotch attempts to explain these repeated 
responses of the electrical organ—which he calls ‘auto- 
excitation ’—by the passage through the tissue of the intense 
current due to the response; this he regards as exciting the 
tissue again, and bringing about the repetition of the same 
effect. With regard to this hypothesis, however, it is un- 
necessary to suppose that it is the intense electrical current 
of the first response which is the exciting cause of the 
second, and so on. For we must bear in mind that multiple 
response is not exclusively characteristic of these electrical 
organs, with their high intensity of discharge-current, but 
is exhibited by tissues of various kinds. These, as we have 
seen in Chapter XVII., may be thrown into a condition of 
rhythmic or multiple excitation by any form of stimulus, 
provided that its intensity be beyonda certain value. It thus 
happens, as we have seen, that while a single moderate stimulus 
induces a single response, a single strong stimulus induces 
multiple responses. 

Indeed this fact—that it is not the intensity of the 
first responsive current, causing a new excitation of the 
tissue, which is accountable for the second, and so on— 
becomes quite clear, as soon as we make quantitative obser- 
vations of response, in those cases in which it receives 
undoubted visible manifestation, by the orderly fall of leaflets 
serially arranged—that is to say,in such plants as Biophyium. 


THE THEORY OF ELECTRIC ORGANS 271 


The sensitiveness of these leaflets is of that order which 
requires on an average an E.M.F. of 12 volts to cause excita- 
tion by kathode-make or anode-break. And this value is 
considerably higher for anode-break than for kathode-make. 
The lowest E.M.F. which I have found with highly excitable 
Biophytum to be effective in causing excitation, was 4 volts, 
and in less sensitive specimens it might be as high as 
20 volts. ) 

Now, on applying a strong stimulus, say by contact of 
hot wire, on the petiole bearing the sensitive leaflets, an 
excitatory wave is initiated, which, during its progress, brings 
about depression of the leaflets in serial order. After an 
interval of about half a minute, a second wave is found to 
be initiated from the original point of excitation, causing a 
second series of responses, shown by the same serial depres- 
sion of the leaflets as before. And such recurrent excitations 
may take place from a single stimulus, as often as twenty times. 
On determining the E.M.F. value of each of these excitatory 
waves, however, I have found it to be of the order of ‘or volt. 
This, it will be seen, is only about four one-hundredths 
of the minimum E.M.F. necessary to induce excitation of 
the leaflet. It clearly follows that here the original stimulus 
is the cause of these multiple excitations, and not the first 
response the cause of the second. 

The phenomenon of multiple response then, is, as we have 
seen, of very extensive occurrence, and not confined to elec- 
trical organs. We have seen it exhibited even by ordinary 
tissues, and we shall find in subsequent. chapters that 
such repeated responses are actually induced in nervous 
and glandular tissues, under the action of an intense stimulus. 
This is interesting in view of the fact that the electrical 
organs of fishes are made up of either neuro-muscular or 
neuro-glandular elements. 


CHAPTER 3X1 


DETERMINATION OF DIFFERENTIAL EXCITABILITY 
UNDER ELECTRICAL STIMULATION 


Advantage of electrical stimulation, in its flexibility—Drawbacks due to 
fluctuating factors of polar effects, and counter polarisation-current—Difficulties 
overcome by employment of equi-alternating electric shocks—Methods of the 
After-effect and Direct-effect—Experiment of Von Fleischl on response ot 
nerve —Complications arising from use of make and break shocks-—Rotating 
reverser—Motor transformer—Response of A/wsa to equi-alternating shocks— 
Abolition of this response by chloroform— Response records of plagiotropic 
Cucurbita and Eel—Differential excitability of variegated leaves, demonstrated 
by electric response. 


WE have seen, in Chapter IX. that the differential excitability 
of any two points in a tissue can be detected, by observing 
the direction of resultant response, when both the points are 
simultaneously excited by an identical stimulus. It was 
seen in the same place also that the more excitable point 
becomes, under diffuse stimulation, relatively galvanometrically 
negative. I further described the various forms of quanti- 
tative stimulus which might be employed for this purpose, 
those, namely, of mechanical vibration and of stimulation by 
thermal shocks. By employing these non-electrical forms of 
stimulus the fundamental law of differential response was 
firmly established, in such a way as to exclude every source 
of uncertainty. 

The electrical form of stimulation is characterised never- 
theless by many advantages. Its intensity, for instance, 
is easily graduated. But its principal superiority lies in its 
sreat flexibility of application. Any two points, however 
remote or difficult of access they may be, may by this means 
be subjected to a required stimulus, if we can only apply two 


DETERMINATION OF DIFFERENTIAL EXCITABILITY 273 


electrodal points to them. These qualities, however, are 
counterbalanced by many serious drawbacks. For the re- 
sultant electrical change, induced at a given point by an 
electrical shock, is the sum of a number of changing factors, 
which may be broadly classified as excitatory and polarising. 
Let us suppose the induction shock to enter at a point. We 
shall there have the physiological polar effects of anode-make 
and anode-break. There is again, on the cessation of the 
current, a counter-electro-motive force due to polarisation. 
The physiological reaction, moreover, will depend on the 
excitability of the point. 

Next, as regards the second or kathodal point, where the 
shock-current leaves the tissue. We have here the physiological 
effects of kathode-make and break, the factor of the ex- 
citability of the point, and the counter electro-motive force 
due to polarisation, all contributing to the resulting electrical 
change at that point. That relative electrical difference 
induced between the points A and Bb, which determines the 
observed electrical variation, thus equals the sum of the 
fluctuating factors acting on A, mznus the sum of the 
fluctuating factors acting on B. These elements of variation 
are sufficiently complicated ; but still another, as already 
said, is added to them, in the fact that, with regard to the 
excitatory polar effects of currents themselves, we have not 
to deal simply with the law enunciated by Pfliiger, that the 
kathode excites at make and the anode at break. The com- 
plete law, as will be shown in a later chapter, is complicated by 
the fact that the result depends on the intensity of the electro- 
motive force. With feeble and again with excessive E.M.F., 
the actual facts are the opposite of conclusions arrived at by 
Pfliiger. Under these circumstances it would appear at first 
sight impossible that any reliable results could be obtained by 
the employment of the electrical form of stimulation. I shall 
now, however, proceed to show in what manner all these 
difficulties may be overcome, and the electrical form of 
stimulus made extremely reliable. This will perhaps be best 
understood if we take a concrete example. Let us suppose 

T 


274 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


that a single electrical shock of moderate intensity enters an 
isotropic tissue at the point A, and leaves it at B ; A will then 
be the anode, and B the kathode. Here the true excitatory 
effect is found to take place only at the kathode, probably 
because the anode-break excitation takes place at much 
higher intensities of E.M.F. than the kathode-make. At these 
higher intensities, then, the anode-break effect also will occur. 
At an excessively high E.M.F. again, these relations, for 
reasons already explained, may undergo reversal. The point 
of such reversal would depend on the nature and excitability 
of the tissue. Though, for all these reasons, the relative 
excitations of A and B remain a matter of doubt, yet we may 
be sure of the excitation of both points, if two, or any equal . 
number, of exactly equal shocks be sent through the tissue in 
opposite directions, in rapid alternation. If, again, instead of 
two alternate shocks only, we give z alternating shocks, abso- 
-lutely equal, and if, further, the natural excitability of the two 
points A and B have been the same, then there will be nothing 
to distinguish the excitatory effect induced at A from that at B. 
In other words, the two excitations will be exactly equal. 
These strictly equal and opposite alternating currents, more- 
over, can have no resultant polarisation-effects, for the effect 
arising from an induction shock, in either direction, will be 
counteracted by that caused by the opposite. 

We thus see that by the employment of this method 
the only change induced at the two electrodes will be the 
excitatory change, the physical polarisation-factor being 
eliminated. Thus, on subjecting two points, A and B, to 
equal stimulation, the induced galvanometric negativity, at 
both the points, will be equal, if the natural excitabilities of 
. the two have been the same. But if the tissue be anisotropic, 
and the natural excitability of one point, say b, greater than 
that of A, then we shail obtain a resultant responsive current, 
which will flow in the tissue from the more excited B to the 
less excited A, the induced galvanometric negativity of B 
being relatively the greater. We have here what is merely 
a repetition, by electrical stimulation, of the results which 


DETERMINATION OF DIFFERENTIAL EXCITABILITY 275 


were described in Chapter IX., as obtained by thermal and 
mechanical stimulus. How perfect and how consistent, due 


precautions being taken, these results may be rendered, will 


be seen from the numerous records in this and the following 
chapters. 

I have postponed till now the consideration of the mode 
of application of these alternating shocks. The usual alter- 
nating current from a Ruhmkorff’s coil would be entirely 
unsuitable for delicate and crucial experiments ; first, because 
the excitatory values of the slow make- and quick break-shocks 
are unequal ; and, secondly, because such currents leave their 
residual polarisation effects. 

These defects I have been able, as stated in the last 
chapter to avoid, by quick reversals of the primary current 
which actuates an induction-coil. When the primary 
current is reversed from the js/us to the mznus direction, 
we obtain an induction-current due to magnetic varia- 
tions of lines of force, from, say, plus n to minus n. When 
the primary current is re-reversed, from mznus to plus, 
we obtain an opposite induction-current, due to magnetic 
variation, from mznus n to plus n. It will be seen that if 
these reversals of the primary current are made with equal 
rapidity, the alternating induced currents will be equal 
and opposite. The reversals are accomplished by means of 
a Pohl’s commutator, worked up and down by a crank, in 
connection with an electric revolving motor (fig. 170). The 
intensity of the induction-shock may be varied by sliding 
the secondary nearer to, or further away from, the primary. 
Having now described the general means of producing equi- 
alternating electric shocks, it still remains to explain two 
distinct methods of applying them, for the determination of 
the differential excitability of the tissue. These may be 
distinguished as: (1) the Method of the AFTER-EFFECT ; and 
(2) the Method of DirRECT-EFFECT. According to the first 
of these—the method of the after-effect—the tissue is excited 
for a definite length of time, and the excitatory effect 
observed, by connecting it immediately afterwards with the 

"3 


276 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


galvanometer-circuit. The manner in which this is done will 
be understood from fig. 170. We have a highly insulating 
electrical key, K, of ebonite. P and Q are connected with two 
points A and B of the tissue, whose relative excitabilities are 
to be determined. A spiral spring keeps the key down, con- 
necting the two points in the specimen with the galvanometer. 
Any existing difference of potential, as between the two points 


al 


M 


Fic. 170. Experimental Arrangement for Determination of Excitatory 
After-effect of Equi-alternating Electrical Shocks 


M, electrical moter working Pohl’s commutator for alternate reversal of 
current in primary, P. Note that the connecting-rod, A, works simply 
up and down, causing reversals of current ; $s, secondary coil; k, 
ebonite key kept down by elastic spring, the two surfaces of the 
specimen being thus in circuit with galvanometer, G. When key is 
pressed, these are put in circuit with exciting coil, s. When key is 
released, after-effect of excitation on specimen exhibited by galvano- 
meter deflection. C, the compensator. 


is balanced by the compensating potentiometer c. Under these 
circumstances, the galvanometer spot of light would remain 
steady, whether the key was up ordown. By pressing the key 
K, the galvanometer-circuit is broken, and the tissue is put in 
series with the exciting circuit S, which forms the secondary 
of the induction-coil. When the key is again released, the 
galvanometer-circuit is rapidly made, at a definite short 
interval after the cessation of the exciting shock, and the 
resulting deflection of the galvanometer indicates the differ- 


ale ee Va ee ee ies © 


DETERMINATION OF DIFFERENTIAL EXCITABILITY 277° 


ential excitation as between A and B. In this way records 
may be taken af a series of the after-effects of brief 
excitations, at intervals of, say,a minute. The direction of 
this responsive current, which in the tissue is from the more 
to the less excitable, enables us to determine the relative 
excitabilities of the two points, A and B. 

Much more delicate is the second method, that, namely, 
which depends on the record of the Direct-Effect of equi- 
alternating shocks; but for its perfect working, certain 
difficulties have to be overcome. One of the first conditions 
to be fulfilled lies in the perfect equality of the alternating 
shocks. The importance of this will be understood on 
observing the effect of the alternating shocks given by a 
Ruhmkorff’s coil, when actuated by a vibrating hammer. 
Here, the make- and break-shocks are of unequal intensity 
and duration, and the following sources of disturbance come 
into play: (1) a galvanometric drift in one direction or the 
other ; (2) a resultant inequality of polarisation-effects ; and 
(3) the inequality of the excitatory values of the two shocks. 

The galvanometer-drift, owing to the inequality of the 
induction shocks, becomes very troublesome, when we have 
to employ, as is necessary, an instrument of high sensibility. 
If the differential excitability of the specimen be very great, 
this drift may be masked by the predominant excitatory 
effect. In other cases, however, the excitatory effect itself 
may be overpowered by the drift. The difference of 
intensity as between the make- and break-shocks in the 
Ruhmkorff’s coil described, thus becomes a strongly dis- 
turbing element. The necessity to make the two shocks 
absolutely equal will be understood when we find that 
alternating telephonic currents, which are generally re- 
garded as equal and opposite, induce a drift of the 
galvanometer in one direction or the other, on account of a 
slight difference of intensity between the two alternating 
currents. 4 

The difficulty arising from inequality of polarisation- 
effects is tao obvious to require further elucidation. Still 


278 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


more important, moreover, is the disturbing factor last 
mentioned, that of the inequality of excjtatory values as 
between the make- and break-shocks themselves. This 
element of uncertainty is very clearly seen in the experi- 
ments of Von Fleischl on the response of nerve. The 
resulting deflection he found to be in the direction of the - 
break-shock. The explanation of this phenomenon has 
hitherto been regarded as a matter of great difficulty, 
authorities being much divided on the subject. In these 
experiments, alternating currents from a Ruhmkorff’s coil 
are sent through the galvanometer and the nerve, in series. 
The two points on the nerve, A and B, are presumably of 
equal excitability. A make-shock of relatively lower E.M.F. 
passes, say, from A to B, followed by a break-shock of higher 
E.M.F..in the opposite direction, from B to A. Confining 
our attention to the excitatory effects of these shocks, we 
have at A, during make and break, the following four effects : 
(a) feeble anode-make; (4) feeble anode-break ; (c) strong 
kathode-make ; and (d@) strong kathode-break. At B, on the 
other hand, at the same time, we shall have: (a’) feeble 
kathode-make ; (6’) feeble kathode-break ; (c’) strong anode- 
make ; and (@’) strong anode-break. Now, since the excitatory 
value of anode-break is probably the stronger and more 
persistent, and since the intensity of this effect will depend, 
within certain limits, on the intensity of the anodic shock, it 
follows that the (@’) or strong anode-break effect at B will, as 
a general rule, be the most conspicuous of these. That is to 
say, as a result of all these excitatory effects in combination, 
greater galvanometric negativity will be induced at B than at 
A, the responsive current being thus from B->A, in the same 
direction as the break shock, which was the actual result. In 
any case, whatever may have been the cause of this, it is 
clear that the employment of such unequally exciting shocks 
of make and break would be fatal to any attempt to 
determine accurately the natural difference of excitability as 
between the two points. I may state here, that when I have 
employed absolutely equal alternating shocks on a specimen 


DETERMINATION OF DIFFERENTIAL EXCITABILITY 279 


of nerve, I have obtained no resultant deflection whatever, 
showing that such shocks induce exactly equal excitations 
in an isotropic tissue. But if the excitability of one of the 
two points be first abolished by killing, then a definite 
resultant responsive current is obtained, from the excitable 
living to the inexcitable dead. So perfect were in fact the 
results secured by means of these equi-alternating electric 
shocks, that I was desirous not only to detect, but also to 
record photographically, the responses thus obtained. In 
this a certain difficulty is experienced, inasmuch as the 
alternating shocks are apt to render the recording spot of 
light tremulous, and thus to spoil the photographic im- 
pression. This may, however, be overcome by making the 
alternation frequency so high, in reference to the period of 
the needle or suspended coil of the galvanometer, that the 
unsteadiness of the deflection ceases. 

I shall now describe the practical means employed to 
obtain equi-alternating shocks of any frequency that may be 
desired. This I have been able to do in several ways, and, 
among others, by using a Rotating Reverser. This consists 
of an ebonite disc, on the periphery of which there are strips 
of metal of equal breadth, and separated from each other by 
equal distances. The odd strips (1, 3, 5, and so on) are 
connected together and led to a metallic ring on the left of 
the disc. The same is done with the even strips, which are 
led to the right. The two electrodes of a battery are led 
through a key, K, to these two metallic rings and are con- 
nected with them by means of brushes. Thus one ring, with 
all the odd strips, is connected, say, with the positive, and the 
other, with all the even strips, with the negative pole of the 
battery. The current is led off by a second pair of brushes, 
placed diametrically opposite to each other on the disc, in 
the primary circuit, P, of an induction coil (fig. 171). Let us 
suppose the upper brush to be connected with an odd strip, 
the lower will then be connected with an even. The current 
in the primary coil now flows in one direction. When the 
disc is rotated, so as to bring the next pair of strips in 


280 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


contact with the brushes, the upper will then be connected 
with the even strip and the lower with the odd. Thus the 
direction of the current will be reversed, and rapid rotation 
of the disc will give rise to equi-alternating currents in the 
primary of the induction coil. This will in turn induce 
equi-alternating induction currents in the secondary, the 
intensity of which can, as already said, be varied within wide 
limits by appropriate changes of distance between the 
primary and the secondary. The number of strips in the 
apparatus used is fifty, and when the disc is rotated, by 


Fic. 171. Method of Direct Effect of Excitation by Equi- 
alternating Shocks 


R, rotating reverser, in circuit with primary coil, P. Duration of stimula- 
tion determined by metronome, M._ 8, secondary coil in series with 
' specimen and galvanometer. 


means of an electrical motor, at a rate of one revolution 
per second, there will be fifty alternations of current in a 
second. The duration of the application of the stimulating 
shock to the tissue is regulated by a metronome, which 
completes the primary circuit for a definite short length of 
time. When the metronome, M, is so adjusted as to complete 
the circuit for *5 second, then a stimulus of that duration will 
be imparted at each stroke. A second interrupting key, not 
shown in the figure, is included in the circuit. When this 
key is closed, a single beat of the metronome gives a stimu- 
lating shock of *5 second’s duration. ‘The key is now opened 


DETERMINATION OF DIFFERENTIAL EXCITABILITY 281 


for one minute for recovery. In this way, records of response 
and recovery are obtained, at intervals, say, of one minute. 
Another very effective means of producing equi-alter- 
nating shocks is by the employment of an alternating- 
current dynamo, driven by an electrical motor, M (fig. 172). 
The alternating current is led to the primary of a Ruhm- 
korffs coil in the usual manner. The motor is driven by an 
electrical supply from the street mains, its speed being 
adjusted by a regulation of the current, which is effected by 


TO THE MAIN 


Fic. 172. Excitation by Equi-alternating Shocks 


M, motor, rotating armature of alternating-current dynamo, D; R, liquid 
rheostat, in circuit with street-mains, for regulating speed of rotation 
of motor; Pp’, idle coil; P, primary coil; I, resonating index; s, 
secondary coil, in series with specimen and galvanometer. Duration 
or excitation determined by pressure of key, K. 


an electrolytic rheostat, R. As the dynamo is provided with 
a permanent horse-shoe magnet, the intensity of the alter- 
nating current is determined by the speed of rotation of 
its armature. If the speed be kept always constant, the 
number of alternations will also be constant, and the ex- 
citing value of the electric shocks will depend simply upon 
the distance of the primary from the secondary. It is thus 
possible day after day to use the same intensity of stimu- 
lation, and thus to compare the relative excitabilities of 


282 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


different tissues. The constancy of the speed of rotation of 
the alternating-current dynamo is secured by means of the 
resonating index, I. This consists of a short steel spring 
with a long index. When the frequency of alternation is 
the same as the natural period of vibration of the spring, the 
resonator is thrown into strong sympathetic vibration. At 
first the rheostatic resistance, which determines the speed of 
the motor, is made slightly too large. The movable plate is 
now gradually brought nearer, till the proper speed has been 
arrived at, and this point is at once indicated by the induced 
vibration of the resonator. | . 

A further difficulty has to be overcome in the main- 
tenance of the uniformity of speed. When the open circuit 
of the alternating dynamo is closed, by the interposition of 
the primary of the Ruhmkorff’s coil, the speed undergoes 
a sudden diminution, owing to the work which the dynamo 
has now to perform. In order to avoid this fluctuation, 
then, the dynamo circuit is kept closed by means of an idle 
primary coil, P’, which is a duplicate of the primary, P, of the 
Ruhmkorff’s coil. When the key, K, is pressed, the alter- 
nating current is transferred from P’ to P. There is thus no 
fluctuation in the speed of the dynamo, and the duration of the 
closure determines that of the stimulus. I may mention 
here, that instead of employing a separate motor to drive 
the alternating-current dynamo, I have sometimes used, with 
equal success, a motor transformer, giving rise to alter- 
nating currents. It is easy to construct a very compact and 
portable form of this latter apparatus. 

In this manner we may apply uniform stimuli of equi- 
alternating shocks at regular intervals of time, say of one 
minute. The usual preliminary test of the successful 
elimination of all sources of disturbance may here be made 
in the following way. The kaolin ends of the non-polaris- 
able electrodes are connected with each other, without the 
interposition of a specimen, and alternating shocks from the 
secondary are passed through the circuit. These should 
give rise to no deflection in the galvanometer. It may be 


Seg ON ee 


ie ae 


a. 


DETERMINATION OF DIFFERENTIAL EXCITABILITY 283 


said here that I use a D’Arsonval type of galvanometer, in 
which, instead of a suspended needle, we have a suspended 
coil. There is thus here not even the remote contingency 
of disturbance which might arise from the demagnetisation 
of the magnetic needle. Having thus tested, by null action, 
the symmetry of the electrodes and the galvanometer, the 
differentially excitable tissue, say the sheathing petiole of 
Musa, is interposed, with its concave and more excitable 
surface upwards. On now: applying excitation by equi- 
alternating shocks, the responsive current will be found to 
flow downwards, from concave to convex, giving a deflection 
of the galvanometer, say to the right. And this deflection 
will continue to be to the right, even if the battery current 
(fig. 171) be reversed by means of key K. The direction of 
the excitatory current, moreover, depending solely, as it does, 
on the relative excitabilities of the two surfaces of the 
specimen, will remain constant, even if the connections with 
the secondary coil, S, be reversed. The zinc rod, N, of the 
non-polarisable electrode in connection with the concave 
surface (fig. 172) has thus, up to the present, shown induced 
galvanometric negativity, the galvanometric deflection being 
to the right. But if we exchange the zinc rods of the non- 
polarisable electrodes, it will then be N’ which will be con- 
nected with the more excitable concave surface, and it will 
now be this electrode N’ which will show galvanometric nega- 
tivity. This reversal of the galvanometer deflection with the 
reversal of the electrodes affords additional confirmation of 
the greater excitability of the concave surface of the specimen 
of Musa. | 

In these experiments the existing current of rest may 
be balanced previously by a potentiometer. But this is not 
absolutely necessary. I give below a series of records 
obtained with a specimen of the sheathing petiole of Musa 
(fig. 173), in which we know the inner or concave surface 
to be more excitable than the outer or convex. The 
responsive current is seen under this form of electrical 
stimulus, as we found to be the case under mechanical and 


284 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


thermal stimulation, to flow from the more excitable concave 
to the less excitable convex. In order next to demonstrate 
the physiological character of these responses, I subjected 
the tissue to the action of chloroform, and the record in the 
second part of the figure shows the consequent Bepresion 
of the response. 

The great delicacy and pliability of this mode of applica- 
tion of stimulus enable us to attack many difficult problems, 
on the difference of excit- 
ability between two points in 
a tissue, with perfect ease. 
To how many distinct in- 
vestigations it can be suc- 
cessfully applied will be set 
forth in detail in succeeding 
chapters. As there is nothing 
to prevent the two exploring . 
electrodes from being applied 
on any two points, however 
distant, of the same organism, 


‘Fic. 173. Photographic Record of Re- 
sponse of Petiole of usa to Equi- 
alternating Electric Shocks, before 
and after Application of Chloroform. 


it is seen that we have here 
a means of determining, not 
only the differential excit- 


ability of any two points of 
the same organ, but also that of any two organs of the same 
specimen. For the present I shall, however, content myself 
with giving a few instances only in illustration of the ex- 
treme delicacy of this method in detecting physiological 
differences as between two points. 

We shall first turn our. attention to those physiological 
modifications which are due to the a-symmetrical action of 
the environment on the organism, and here we shall select 
the case of the plagiotropic stem of Cucurbita. We have 
seen that in the recumbent stem of this plant the tissue of 
the upper side is rendered relatively fatigued by the con- 
tinuous action of sunlight, and thus becomes permanently 
less excitable than the lower side. We have also found that 


DETERMINATION OF DIFFERENTIAL EXCITABILITY 285 


while the natural resting-current was from the less excitable 
upper to the more excitable lower side, the responsive cur- 
rent under mechanical stimulation was in the opposite direc- 
tion—namely, from the lower to the upper (p. 112). Using 
now the electrical form of stimulus, we obtain results which 
are identical. Fig. 174 gives a series of such responses 
under equi-alternating electrical shocks. Curiously enough, 
as pointed out in the last chapter, I have detected a similar 
plagiotropy in the case of the eel. The head of the fish 
was cut off, and voluntary action thus eliminated ; electrical 
connections were then made, after a period of rest, with the 
dark dorsal upper surface, and the 
colourless skin of the ventral or 
lower. A natural current was now 
found to flow from the upper 


Fic. 174. Photographic Record 


of Responses of Plagiotropic Fic. 175. Electrical Responses of 
Stem of Cucurbita to Equi- Eel to Equi-alternating Electrical 
alternating Electric Shocks Shocks 

Direction of responsive current from Current of response from ventral 
ventral to dorsal surface. surface to dorsal. 


surface to the lower, as in the case of the plagiotropic stem 
of Cucurbita. Electrical excitation was now applied, and 
the result was a responsive current from the more excitable 
lower to the less excitable upper surface again, as in the 
case of Cucurbita. In fig. 175 is seen a series of records in 
illustration of this. 

Another investigation which I thought might be interest- 
ing had reference to the variegated colouring of certain 
foliage leaves, A striking example of this is found in the 


286 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


tropical elephant creeper (Pothos), the rich green of which is 
barred by longitudinal streaks of milk-white. This dis- 
tribution of colour is found even in the youngest and most 
vigorous leaves. The question whether such colouring was 
accidental or associated with physiological differences could, 
I thought, be determined by the delicate mode of investiga- 
tion which was now at my disposal. On making electrical . 
connections, then, with the green and white portions of a leaf, 
I found that the natural current of rest was from white to 
green through the tissue, and, on further testing the differ- 
ential excitability in the usual manner, the responsive current 
was observed to flow from the green to the white. This 
showed that pallidity was here associated with a depressed 
physiological condition, | 


CHAPTER XXII 
RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 


Currents of rest and action—Currents in animal skin—Theories regarding these 
—Response of vegetal skin—Stimulation by Rotary Mechanical Stimula- 
tor—Response of intact human skin—Isolated responses of upper and 
lower surfaces of specimens—Resultant response brought about by differ- 
ential excitability of the two surfaces—Differences of excitability between 
two surfaces accounted for— Response of animal and vegetal skins not 
essentially different—General formula for all types of response of skin— 
Response of skin to different forms of stimulation gives similar results— 
Response to equi-alternating electric shocks : (1) Method of the After Effect ; 
(2) Method of Direct Effect—Response of grape skin—Similar response of 
frog’s skin—Phasic variation of current of rest induced as result of 
successive stimulation in (a) grape skin ; (4) frog’s skin ; (c) pulvinus of 
Mimosa—Phasic variation in autonomous mechanical response of Des- 
modium gyrans—Autonomous variation of current of rest—True current of 
rest in skin from outer to inner—This may be reversed as an excitatory 
after effect of preparation—Electrical response of skin of neck of tortoise — 
Electrical response of skin of tomato —Normal response and positive after- 
effect — Response of skin of gecko—Explanation of abnormal response, 


IN this and the next few chapters it is my intention to make 
an inquiry into the responsive peculiarities of the skin, 
epithelium, and glandular tissues, alike in plant and animal. 
By the study of such simple cases as are found in plants, it 
should be possible to obtain’a clear insight into the various 
factors which go to make the corresponding phenomena 
in animal tissues so complicated and obscure as to be 
difficult of reconciliation with each other. 

It is not possible in a short space to give any but the 
briefest summary of the work hitherto done on this extended 
subject in animal physiology. All that can be attempted is 
to indicate some of the leading theories and_results, at the 
same time drawing attention to those outstanding questions 
which still remain open. Some of the methods which I have 


288 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


employed in the investigation of plant phenomena, moreover, 
have proved so highly satisfactory that records will be given 
of the results obtained by their means in the case of 
animal tissues also. And this will, I hope, show the great 
reliability and simplicity which it is thus possible to intro- 
duce into the investigation as a whole. 

With regard to the electrical effects in animal skin, 
epithelium, and glands, the inquiry resolves itself into the 
determination of, (1) the direction of the current of rest ; (2) 
that of the excitatory current ; and, lastly, (3) a consideration 
of theories regarding these. The first of these, the current 
of rest, was found by Du Bois-Reymond and Engelmann in 
the skin of frog to be ‘ingoing ’—that is to say, passing from 
the outer surface to the inner. Hermann also found a 
similar current in the skin of eel. He regarded the source of 
electro-motive action as lying in the partial mucin-metamor- 
phosis of single cells. From the fact that in the toad, where 
the ingoing current is specially strong, the skin glands are 
vigorously developed, and from the discovery by Rosenthal 
that in the mucous glands of the stomach the current is also 
ingoing, it was assumed that the observed electro-motive 
forces were due to the glandular nature of the tissues. The 
skin current of the frog and of the fish, and the glandular 
current of the stomach, are thus usually regarded as due to 
the same cause. 

There is, however, a serious discrepancy in this view, 
_ inasmuch as, while local stimulation of the upper surface of 
the frog’s skin induces.a positive change, a similar stimula- 
tion of an unmistakably glandular surface is found to bring 
about a negative. If then the electrical effect on the skin 
of frog be the same as on a glandular surface, the dis- 
crepancy of their responsive reactions becomes inexplicable. 

As regards the excitatory change, very diverse results 
have been recorded when stimulus has been applied indi- 
rectly —that is to say, through the nerve. This fact is not to 
be wondered at, since the responsive effects are subject, as will 
be shown, to numerous modifying influences. It is generally 


Fo a ee ee 


RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 289 — 


supposed, in the preparations made for these experiments, 
that it is one surface only which is electro-motive. I shall 
show, however, that the responsive effects are brought about 
by the differential excitability of the two. This response, 
again, is modified by relative changes induced in the two 
surfaces. And, in addition to these, still further compli- 
cations are introduced when the stimulus is indirect—that is 
to say, applied through the nerve. In this case, the relative 
excitations of the two surfaces will be determined by the 
particular distribution of the nerve-endings. Again, we 
shall see that, in an isolated preparation, the nerve itself 
is liable to undergo certain changes by which its trans- 
mitted effect may be modified even to reversal (p. 530). 
Thus, so long as it remains highly excitable, the transmitted 
effect is one of true excitatory galvanometric negativity. But 
with physiological depression, the conductivity of the nerve 
is very much lowered, and the effect transmitted becomes 
reversed to positivity. 

For all these reasons, if we wish to study the specific 
reactions of skin, epithelium, and gland preparations, it is 
better to do so by observing them under direct stimulation. 

Engelmann, in studying the responsive reactions to 
direct stimulation of frog’s skin, found a negative variation of 
the current of rest. Since the latter was naturally ‘ingoing,’ 
as regards the upper or’ epidermal surface, this meant that 
the responsive current was ‘outgoing.’ Reid, again, work- 
ing on the skin of the eel, obtained ingoing response, or 
positive variations of the resting-current, by single induction 
shocks in either direction. Biedermann, in the mucous 
membranes of the tongue and stomach, obtained both 
positive and negative variations of the current of rest, 
Waller, using single induction shocks in either direction, 
found in the digestive mucosa both ingoing and outgoing 
responses, the former being much predominant. 

Even under the simple conditions imposed by direct 
stimulation, then, the results obtained are seen to be in- 
consistent. They would appear to show both that the 

U 


290 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


responsive effects in the different preparations are different 
and that, even in the same preparation, they may be reversed 
under unknown changes of circumstances. 

It appeared to me, as already said, that much light might 
be thrown on the questions thus raised by means of an 
investigation carried out on plants. The most perfect 
method of experiment here would consist in observing the 


separate responsive effects on upper and lower surfaces of 


the preparation. Waller employed single induction shocks 
for this purpose, observing the after-effect. But in this 
case, the action of polarisation was not excluded. It 
would thus be more satisfactory, in order to eliminate 
this unknown element, to employ either a non-electrical 
mode of stimulus or an electrical form which would leave 
no resultant polarisation effect. The latter condition, as 
we have seen, was fulfilled by the employment of rapidly 
alternating currents, whose alternating components were 
absolutely equal. 

As regards the application of a non-electrical form of 
stimulus, both thermal and mechanical forms may theoreti- 
cally be employed. Engelmann and others used heated 
metals in the proximity of one of the electrodes, for the 
production of thermal stimulus. This, however, has the 
disadvantage of thermo-electrical variation, due to unequal 
heating of the two contacts. Besides this, there is also the 
effect of a rising temperature, which, as we have seen, is 
opposite to that of sudden variation, the latter alone consti- 
tuting the excitatory effect. I have already explained 
how these difficulties may be overcome by using thermal 
shocks in which a sudden thermal variation is made to act 
on both contacts at once. The resultant response thus 
obtained was shown to be determined by the differential 
excitability of the two contacts under examination. As 
regards the mechanical mode of stimulation, previous 
observers have employed pressure or friction. Such stimulus, 
however, is at best merely qualitative. If it be applied at 
the contact itself, objections may be taken to the effect, as 


Rae a A meh 


RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 291 


due to, or modified by, the variation of contact resistance. 
‘And if to avoid this the mechanical stimulus be applied, not 
at the electrode, but at a neighbouring point, the results will 
be quite different, according as the conductivity of the 
intervening tissue is great or slight. In the former case 
they will consist of the transmitted effect of true excitation ; 
in the latter of the indirect effect, whose electrical sign is 
the exact opposite. 


Fic. 176. Rotary Mechanical Stimulator 


Specimen of skin pinned on hinged platform which is pressed against 
electrodes by elastic india-rubber. Electrodes rotated by cord, c c’. 
S, antagonistic spring, made of elastic. Enlarged view of electrode 
seen to the right. T, outer fixed brass tube ; T', inner rotating tube, 
holding non-polarisable electrode. P, pumice-stone. 


A perfect method of direct mechanical stimulation has 
been described in Chapter III., the stimulus being vibrational. 
But for investigations on limp structures, such as skin, this 
method is not practicable, and the modification which I am 
now about to describe is necessary, in order to meet the 
difficulties of the case. The apparatus consists of a hinged 
platform, P (fig. 176), on which the specimen is securely 
pinned. The two electrodes, E and E’, rest with a definite 
pressure on the two points A and B, whose excitatory re- 
actions are to be studied. These electrodes have at their 

U2 


292 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


lower ends projecting pumice-stone cylinders of equal sec- 
tions, soaked in normal saline. When the electrode E is 
rotated, the mechanical friction induces local excitation of 
the point A. B may also be subjected to similar isolated 
excitation in the same way. In order that such successive 
excitations may be quantitative and uniform, it is necessary 
first that a definite area at A or B should be stimulated. In. 


other words, there must be no lateral slip. For this reason | 


the electrodes are passed through tubular holders which, from 
the description presently to be given, will be seen to allow 
rotation about a definite vertical axis. The extreme bases 
of the pumice-stone cylinders are, as has been said, of equal 
section. The glass electrode tube is tightly fixed, by means 
of a cork, inside a brass rotating tube. The latter, again, 
plays inside an outer brass tube, which is fixed. The inner 
brass tube is provided with two collars, one below and one 
above, by means of which rotation can take place without 
up or down movement. A string is also wound round it, by 
pulling which rotation is produced. The electrodes are 
perpendicular to the plane of the platform which carries the 
-specimen. It will thus be seen that any variation of the 
surface subjected to stimulation is prevented. 

The next difficulty to be overcome is that of liability 
to variation in the pressure of the contact. It will be 
remembered that the platform is hinged. It is further held 
up against the electrodes by the tension of an elastic piece 
of india-rubber or a spiral spring of steel. This pressure can 
be regulated to a suitable value, and kept constant. 

The final difficulty is to apply successive stimuli of equal 
value, and to render them capable also of graduation from 
low to high values. This could be secured by rendering the 
successive rotations of the exciting electrodes equal in number 
and ‘in time of execution. The intensity of stimulus might 
then be increased by increasing the number of rotations or 
the pressure of the electrodes on the specimen. 

In order to apply successive rotations of definite number, 
one end of the string wound round the ‘inner brass tube is 


a 


a 


Sea hile A rea 


RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 293 — 


attached to a piece of stretched india-rubber, which is fixed 
by its other end to the apparatus. The second end of the 
string is tasselled, after being passed through a fixed ring. 
This position of the string is adjusted by means of a knot, so 
that the india-rubber at its other end is already in a state 
of tension. When the tassel is now suddenly pulled and let 
go, it gives rise to a number of rotations in the positive, 
followed by an equal number of rotations in the opposite 
direction, the latter work being performed by the stretched 
antagonistic spring. It should be remembered that a 
mechanical rotation, whether p/us or mznus, gives rise to the 
same excitatory reaction. Next, to make the number of 
rotations definite, let us suppose the inner brass tube to have 
a circumference say of I cm. If the string be now pulled 
through a distance of 5 cm., and let go, there will be five 
rotations in the positive, followed by five rotations in the 
negative direction. A second knot in the string, at the 
distance of 5 cm. from the first, exactly limits the length of 
the pull; and increase or decrease in the intensity of the 
stimulus.can be brought about by a change in the distance 
of the regulating knot. 

The distance between the two electrodes being always 
the same, the resistance of the interposed tissue remains 
approximately constant. To nullify any accidental variation, 
a high and constant external resistance is interposed in the 
galvanometer circuit. When the excitatory electro-motive 
variation of the specimen is very great, it is possible to use 
an external resistance as high as one million ohms. It should, 
however, be remembered that even if there be any unavoid- 
able variation of resistance, it will not in any way affect the 
discrimination of sign of the characteristic electro-motive 
response. For the excitatory effect at either electrode may 
be tested by repeating the experiment with the other. The 
experiments which will be described afforded definite and 
characteristic records, which were found capable of repetition. 
The physiological character of such responses was further 
demonstrated by repeating the experiment, after killing the 


294. COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


tissue with boiling water, when these electro-motive variations 
were found to disappear. How very reliable these responses 
can be rendered is shown by the photographic record in 
fig. 180, which is of very special interest, giving as it does the 
record of responses afforded by the intact human skin. 
Turning now to the nature of the response of. the skin, 


it has been found by Engelmann and others, as already said, 
that in the frog, while the natural resting current is from the — 


outer surface to the inner, the responsive current is from 
inner to outer. Dr. Waller, again, undertook to analyse the 
constituent elements of this response, by passing induction 
shocks along each of the surfaces, first upper and then lower, 
and in both directions. He then observed the after-effect 
at one of the excited points, in relation to an indifferent 
point. In this way he found that an excited point on the 
upper surface becomes galvanometrically positive, the current 
being thus outgoing. The inner surface, however, he found 
to be ineffective. When an induction shock is passed across 
the tissue, the resultant response from lower to upper is thus, 
according to Dr. Waller, due to induced positivity of the 
upper surface. 

With vegetable specimens, however, such as the outer 
skin of apple, the results obtained by him were opposite to 
those of the frog’s skin. ‘The responsive currents were here 
found to be ingoing, the excited point being galvanometrically 
negative. The explanation offered, in regard to these results, 
is that living tissues have the peculiarity of responding by 
‘blaze currents’ to electrical shocks. The use of this phrase, 
however, as already said, offers no real explanation ; but even 
apart from this point, the question remains, Why should the 
blaze currents, so called, be directly opposed in the cases of 
frog’s skin and of the particular vegetable skins which are 
mentioned, respectively? In answer to this, the hypothesis 
put forward by Dr. Waller is, that the difference arises from 
the different natures of animal and vegetable protoplasms.' 


' ¢ Vegetable protoplasm is in major degree an instrument of synthesis and 
accumulation, in minor degree the seat of analysis and emission, Animal 


a = 
BOS Ae hk Pe ESS Satna, yt 


RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 2905 — 


I shall, however, be able to adduce facts and considera- 
tions from which it will be possible to arrive at a simpler and 
more conclusive explanation of these phenomena, on the 
basis of the differential physiological excitability of the two 
surfaces. I shall show, moreover, that the difference between 
animal and vegetable protoplasm, thus assumed to exist, has 
nothing to do with the question. 

We have seen that when the physiological activity of a 
tissue is in any way impaired, its normal excitatory re-action 
of galvanometric negativity is depressed. This may even go 
so far as to cause an actual reversal of the response, to 
galvanometric positivity, as we found in the case of depressed 
tissues (p. 84). Taking a vegetable specimen, then, say a 
hollow petiole or peduncle, we find that the outer surface, 
which is habitually exposed to the manifold influences of the 
environment, becomes histologically modified, being much 
more cuticularised than-the inner. Thus these outer and 
exposed cells generally become reduced in size, and thick- 
walled, with little protoplasmic contents. Hence, as regards 
functional activity, these epidermal cells are in a_physio- 
logical sense very much degraded. We should then expect 
their excitability to be proportionately lowered in comparison 
with, say, the inner surface of the same tube, protected as 
that has been from outside influences. And the variation of 
physiological excitability thus induced may involve not only 
the surface, but also the subjacent layers to a certain extent. 

Theoretically, then, the induced galvanometric negativity 
of the outer would be less than that of the inner surface,! 
and simultaneous excitation of both, by whatever means 
produced, should give rise to a resultant responsive current 


protoplasm is in major degree an instrument of analysis and emission, in minor 
degree the seat of synthesis and accumulation. The vegetable, in most immediate 
contact with inert things, combines, organises, and accumulates, The animal, 
in less immediate contact with inert matter, disrupts, utilises, and dissipates 
in their fragments organic compounds that it has received ready made from 
other animals and from plants.’—Waller, Signs of Life, p. 85. 


1 This refers to normal skin, and not to that in which the surface is typically 
glandular. 


296 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


from inner to outer. The degree of this diminution of 
excitatory negativity in the outer surface, moreover, cul- 
minating as this may in actual positivity, will depend upon 
the extent of its transformation. In connection with this 
it should be remembered that, in order to bring out the 
differential excitabilities of the two surfaces, it is necessary 
to apply localised stimuli of an intensity not too excessive. 
For, if the stimulus be very strong, there is always a 
possibility of its affecting deeper layers of the tissue and 
thus causing complications in the resultant excitatory 
changes. The intensity of stimulus which may be safely 

used without bringing about 


3 y such complications will depend 
b- a zl on the conductivity of the 
Pd A creme tissue. Epidermal cells are, 

ee Gar pas generally speaking, feeble con- 
os | ductors, but in this matter it 
ee must be understood that the 


; differences in this respect be- 
Fic. 177. Diagram Representing ‘ : 
Different Levels of Excitability, tween different tissues are not 
Pika: sels ease eee ' absolute, but a question of 
Diagram to right of figure shows ; 
degree, and may to a certain 


how resultant up response (inner 
to outer) may be obtained when extent be modified under dif- 


induced change at A is plus, and 
at B minus, or when induced ferent circumstances. Thus a 
eee less negative than feebly conducting tissue, under 
a favourable condition of tem- 
perature and strong intensity of stimulation, will become to 
a certain extent conducting. Highly conducting tissues like 
nerve, on the other hand, under unfavourable circumstances 


may, as I shall show later, be converted into very feeble 


conductors. 
Returning now to the question of the responsive reactions 


of skin, we see the theoretical possibility of the following 
typical reactions. Let the scale of excitabilities be repre- 
sented by diagram to the left of fig. 177. Now, if the trans- 
formation of the outer epidermal surface, A, be maximum, the 
sign of its reaction will exhibit the greatest extent of deviation 


ee 


RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 267 


from the normal negativity. That is to say, its response will 
become absolutely positive, as represented by a above the zero 
line. The response of the inner surface, B, may be normal and 
strongly negative, as represented by e below the zero line. 
When both these surfaces, then, are simultaneously excited, 
the excitatory positive variation, or ‘outgoing’ current at A, 
will conspire with the ‘ingoing’ current at B and the 
resultant electro-motive difference will be B, + A,, the direction 
of the responsive current being thus from inner to outer. 
But the same resultant up-response will also be induced, even 
if the reaction of both surfaces be negative, provided only 
that that of the outer, A,, be less negative than that of the 
inner, B,, as explained by the diagram to the right of fig. 177. 
The responsive current will then be represented as B,—A,, 
that is to say, as proceeding from the more negative B to 
the less negative A. We have thus examined the two 
extreme cases possible under the following formula, in which 
the arrows show the direction of the responsive current, from 
the more to the less induced negative: 


end>-co> boa. 


I shall next proceed to demonstrate the existence of 
these two extreme types, taking vegetable skins as the 
experimental specimen. It was supposed by Dr. Waller, as 
will be recalled here, that owing to characteristic differences 
between animal and vegetable protoplasm the response of 
vegetable skin was opposite to that of animal skin: that is to 
say, the former was ‘ingoing’ and the latter ‘outgoing.’ That 
this generalisation is not, however, justified, will be seen from 
the experiments which I am about to describe, carried out 
on the skin of grape. 

These results, it should here be pointed out, are not 
dependent upon any one method of inquiry, for each 
problem was subjected to attack and analysis by four 
different modes of experiment. The first of these (1) was by 
the Rotary Method of Mechanical Stimulation. This 
method has the great advantage that by it the absolute 


298 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


response of each surface is displayed separately, without 
either being affected by the other. There is here, moreover, 
no complication due to the polarisation factor, inevitable 
when uni-directioned induction shocks are employed for 
excitation. Thus, after the individual responses of each 
surface have been analysed, we are able to arrive at.a definite 
conclusion as to what would be the character of the resultant 


response if both the surfaces were simultaneously excited. | 


This conclusion is then submitted to three other tests. Thus, 
(2) the two surfaces of the specimen are subjected simul- 
taneously to the same thermal shocks, according to the 
method already described. Again, (3) the Method of 
the After Effect under equi-alternating shocks is employed. 
And, finally, (4) the Direct Effect of these equi-alternating 
shocks of moderate intensity is recorded. The results 
obtained by all these diverse methods are in complete 
concordance with each other, and fully support the theo- 
retical inferences which have already been made. 

I took the skin of a ripe muscatel grape, such as are 
available in Calcutta. On making the galvanometric con- 
nections with the outer and inner surfaces, a resting current, 
-C, was found to flow in the skin from the outer to the 
inner, just as in the skin of the frog (right-hand diagram, 
fig. 178). The grape skin was now mounted in the rotary 
stimulating apparatus, first, for the stimulation of the outer 
or epidermal surface, with the outer layer placed upwards. 
The distance between the two electrodes was always the 
same—namely, 2 cm. On now stimulating one of the two 
contacts, response took place by the induced galvanometric 
positivity of that point. That is to say, the current was 
‘outgoing, into the galvanometer circuit, from the surface 
of the skin. When the second point was now stimulated the 
deflection previously obtained was reversed, the second contact 
thus also exhibiting galvanometric positivity on excitation. 

The position of the skin in the apparatus was now 
changed, the inner surface being placed upwards. In this 
way points diametrically opposite to those in the last case 


eT ee 


Pw a cn rt i i es ti. 


RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 299 — 


were subjected to excitation. It was now found that the 
responses of the inner surface were normal—that is to say, of 
galvanometric negativity. I give here (fig. 178, a) records 
of two successive sets of responses obtained from the external 
and internal points A and B. These records clearly demon- 
strate that the resultant up-response, on simultaneous exci- 
tation of the outer and inner surfaces, is brought about 
by the induced galvanometric positivity of the outer added 
to the induced negativity of the inner surface. As the resist- 
ance of the circuit in the two successive experiments was 
maintained approxi- 


A B 
mately the same, the 
. + bs 
amplitude of these re- Pile F 
sponses gives a fairly el t 
a . One 
sf \ 


accurate idea of the ta oe 
relative electrical effects F 


4 
induced on the two sur- Fic. 178. Electrical Response of Grape- 
faces. The positive or skin to Rotary Mechanical Stimulation 


(a) A, positive response of outer surface ; 
B, negative response of inner surface ; 


‘outgoing’ effect of the 


outer surface is here (6) c, current of rest, from outer to inner ; 

; R, excitatory response from inner to outer, 
slightly greater than the consisting of summated results of positive 
‘ ingoing’ effect. The pig aes of outer with negative response 


diagram in fig. 178, 3, 
shows how the individual effects conspire to give rise toa 
responsive current from the inner to the outer. 

In order to test the reality of the correspondence between 
this response of the grape skin and that of the frog, I now 
repeated these experiments, employing the same apparatus 
for mechanical stimulation, on the skin of the frog. From 
the records given in fig. 179 it will be seen that the isolated 
response of the outer surface is positive, or ‘outgoing,’ that 
of the inner being negative, or ‘ingoing. The amplitude 
of the former was, however, much greater than that of the 
latter. These responses disappeared altogether when the 
tissue had been killed by immersion in boiling water. From 
isolated responses obtained by means of induction shocks, 
Dr. Waller had been led to regard the outer surface of frog’s 


300 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSioLOGY 


skin as alone active, the inner being, to his thinking, in- 
effective. This particular result may possibly be accounted 
for by supposing that he used a stimulus intensity which 
was not sufficiently strong. In my own experiments I 
obtained clear demonstration of the effectiveness of both 
surfaces in opposite ways electrically, though the effect 
obtained from the outer was undoubtedly the more intense 
of the two. By comparing these two experiments, then, on 
the grape skin and skin of frog, it will be seen that the 
inference that the vegetable protoplasm reacts in any way 


A B 
o = 
c R 
A B A ae 
ee es B —e 
v f 
b 
a a 
Fic. 179. Electrical Response of Frog’s Skin to Rotary Mechanical 


Stimulation 


(a) A, positive response of outer ; B, negative response of inner; (a’) A 
and B exhibit abolition of response in skin on boiling ; (4) Cc, current 
of rest from outer to inner ; R, excitatory response from inner to outer, 
being summated effect of positive response of outer and negative 


response of inner. 


essentially different from that of the animal is quite unjus- 
tified. How widely applicable is the method of mechanical 
excitation by rotary stimulus will be seen in an attempt, 
successfully carried out, to determine the very difficult ques- 
tion of the characteristic response of the intact human skin. 
This will be seen in the following record of the results 
obtained with the skin of a forefinger. The responsive elec- 
trical changes represented by the down records, exhibit induced 
galvanometric positivity of the excited surface (fig. 180). 

I shall next describe the results obtained by simultaneous 
excitation of the inner and outer surfaces of grape-skin. 
The responses now given, under stiniulation by thermal 


EE ee 


RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 301 


shocks, are seen in fig. 181, the resultant current being seen 
to be ‘ up ’—that is to say, from the inner to the outer. 

On observing the excitatory after-effect of equi-alternating 
shocks, the results were found to be the same, the responsive 
current being now once more from the inner to the outer. 

I next took a series of records of the direct effect of 
equi-alternating shocks, the results of which were precisely 
the same as before. On applying stimulation, by exactly 
equal and alternating shocks, we, as already explained, 


Fic. 180. Photographic Record 


of Electrical Responses of Fic. 181. - Photographic -Record of 
Upper Surface of Intact . Electrical Responses of Grape-skin 
Human Forefinger to Rotary to Thermal Shocks at Intervals of a 
Mechanical Stimulation. Minute 

Down responses here indicate Responsive current from inner to outer, 
induced galvanometric posi- 
tivity. 


obtain a result which is due solely to the differential excita- 
tion of the two opposite surfaces. This is not complicated 
in any way by the factor of polarisation, although the latter 
could not have failed to be present if the exciting shocks 
had been one-directioned. Under the conditions of these 
equi-alternating shocks, then, a certain effect is often 
seen, in the phasic variation of the base-line, which is ex- 
tremely characteristic. We have already seen (p. 98) that 
when a tissue is subjected to repeated or continuous stimula- 
tion, its condition undergoes a phasic or periodic variation. 
Thus from a neutral or positive condition, it may pass into 
one of maximum contraction or galyanometric negativity, to 


302 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


be subsequently reversed to positive once more. Such phasic 
changes, moreover, may be repeated. They find visible 
indications in appropriate shiftings of the base line of the 
record. Similar effects are also shown in the differential 
response as seen in the records given in fig. 182; this feature 
; is very noticeable. We here ob- 
tain the resultant response of the 
two surfaces of grape-skin, from 
. the inner to the outer. As the 
result of the series of stimuli 
applied, the existing current of 


FIG. 182. Photographic Re- 
cord of Electrical Responses 
of Grape-skin to Stimula- 
tion by Equi-alternating 
Electrical Shocks at In- 
tervals of a Minute Fic. 183. Photographic Record of Series 

of Electrical Responses of Frog’s Skin to 

Equi-alternating Electrical Shocks applied 


Responsive current from inner 


to outer. Note periodic 

variation of resting-current, at Intervals of One Minute 

causing shifting of base-line, Direction of responsive current from inner to 
down and up. outer. Note also variation of base-line. 


rest undergoes a periodic variation. If this had remained 
constant, the base-line would have been horizontal. In the 
present case, the original current of rest was from outside 
to inside. This, at first, underwent an increase; then a 
decrease ; to be followed, later, by another increase. Thus, 
in the course of about ten minutes, it exhibited an alterna- 
tion of almost one whole cycle. 

In the next figure (fig. 183) I give a series of results obtained 


RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 303 


with frog’s skin in direct response to equi-alternating shocks. 
Here we find the usual ‘up’ responses, showing that, as 
before, the direction of the responsive current is from within 
to without ; and here also we see the existing current of rest 
undergoing a periodic change. 

It has now been fully demonstrated that the response 
of skin is determined by the differential excitabilities of its 
two surfaces, upper and lower, that of the lower being. the 
greater. That the resultant responsive current from lower to 
upper, is in such cases brought about by the greater excit- 
ability of the lower, has been fully shown, in a previous 
chapter, by experiments on 
the pulvinus of J/zmosa. 
I next made records of a 
long series of responses 
given by the last-named 
specimen, with the object 
of finding out whether or 
not these also exhibited a 
periodic variation of the 
resting-current similar to 
those just observed in the 
anisotropic skins of grape 


Fic. 184. Photographic Record of Trans- 
and of frog. Electrical verse Response of Pulvinus of A/imosa 


: to Equi-alternating Electrical Shocks 
connections were made : ; . . 
The direction of the responsive current is 


with diametrically opposite from the more excitable lower to the 
; less excitable upper. Note the cyclic 
points on the Upper and variation of the current of rest. 


lower surfaces of this 

organ, and they were subjected to equi-alternating shocks. 
Owing to the conducting power of the tissues, it was 
not now the upper and lower skin surfaces merely, but 
the upper and lower halves of the organ that became 
excited. And the responsive current was from the lower 
to the upper, as already demonstrated. In the particular 
record seen in fig. 184, the general resemblance to the 
responses of skin is sufficiently obvious. The interesting 
feature of this record is the periodic changes in the resting- 


304 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


current, which exhibit a complete cycle in the course of 
thirteen minutes. Thus, as a consequence of the after-effect 
of stimulus, a cyclic variation of relative conditions is 
induced, as between any two anisotropic surfaces, such as 
those of skin or pulvinus. This cyclic variation of relative 
conditions is indicated by the concomitant variations induced 
in the resting-current, shown in the shiftings of the base line. 

I have been able, further, to demonstrate the interesting 
fact that such phasic variations are capable of exhibition | 
even through mechanical response. I have already ex- 
plained that autonomous pulsations, such as those of the 
lateral leaflets of Desmodium gyrans, may be regarded as 
the after-effect of stimuli previously absorbed and held 
latent by the tissue. In taking the record of a series 
of such pulsations, I have often found phasic variations 
to occur, similar to those obtained with long-continued 
response of skin or pulvinus. If, for example, the lower 
half of the pulvinus of the lateral leaflet of Desmodium 
undergoes an increase of turgidity above the average, that 
half will become more convex, and the base-line of the 
record will be correspondingly tilted. The converse will 
take place under the opposite change. Thus the phasic 
variations shown in the record (fig. 185) clearly indicate 
that the relative turgidities of the two surfaces of an 
anisotropic organ may undergo a periodic change. The 
corresponding electrical expression of this we have seen in 
the variation of the current of rest. This variation may 
sometimes be so great as actually to reverse the normal 

current of rest. Thus, while under normal standard con- 
ditions the resting-current in the pulvinus of Mzmosa is from 
the upper half to the lower, across the organ, this normal 
direction may sometimes be found to be reversed. 

It may now be asked, What is it, in the case of the skin, 
which determines the respective directions of the resting- 
current and the current of response? We have seen that 
the current of rest in the frog’s skin, from outer to inner, is 
generally attributed to the possession of glands by the outer, 


RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 305 


a supposition seemingly supported by Rosenthal’s discovery, 
already referred to, that an apparently similar ‘ingoing’ 
current was to be observed in the mucous coat of the frog’s 
stomach. Against this may be urged the conclusion, to 
which Hermann drew attention, that the skin glands are nor- 
mally nearly closed to the external surface, and cannot there- 
fore have any external galvanic relation. There are, moreover, 
other arguments. First, a similar current is observed in the 
case of grape-skin, where there is no special glandular layer. 
Second, the specific response of a glandular surface is 


HE MH 

i Ahi lyf iH i 
Wi iH if! Hii yy iii TAA), 
}! 


itt ALE My 
Hill Ih) 


Nyy AWE Mii Wi 


6 P.M. 9 P.M. I2 


nA AAV 


aaa wt WW) iyo 


WV VV" 


12 3 A.M. 6 A.M. 


Fig. 185. Continuous Photographic Record of Autonomous Pulsaticn of 
Desmodium gyrans from 6 P.M. to 6 A.M. 


The lower record is in continuation of the upper. Note phasic variation. 


definite, and is by galvanometric negativity, whereas the 
response of the outer surface of frog or grape skin on 
excitation is by galvanometric positivity. And, thirdly, we 
shall see that the current observed in the mucous membrane 
of stomach is most probably not the natural current of rest, 
but the excitatory after-current. 

It will be remembered, however, that we have always 
found the natural current to flow ‘in the tissue from the less 
to the more excitable, and the current of response in the 
opposite direction. In the skin, owing to physiological and 

iS 


306 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


histological modifications, the outer surface is reduced in 
excitability. The epidermal layers have little protoplasmic 
contents, and may be transformed in various ways, becoming 
corneated or cuticularised. The extent of such transforma- 
tion may be small or great, but the external layer will as 
a general rule become less excitable than the inner tissue. 
Hence, under normal conditions, we have a current of rest 
from without to within. 

If the inner layer be only moderately excitable, or if its 
power of recovery from excitation be great, then the dis- 
turbance caused by the prepara- 
tion of the specimen will be 
slight, or will pass off quickly. 
It is to be remembered that as 
the inner surface is the more 
excitable, the responsive current 
due to the mechanical stimulus 
of preparation wil! be from 
inner to outer; and therefore 
its after-effect, proving in certain 


Fic. 186. Photographic Record cases persistent, may give rise 
of Electrical Responses in Skin to g current apparently the re- 
of Neck of Tortoise to Stimulus PP y 
of Equi-alternating Electrical verse of the true normal current. 
aera at Intervals of One Thus the direction of the current 

The direction of the responsive of rest, which we should have 
current was from inner to inferred theoretically to be from 
outer. The so-called current ‘ 
of rest was also in this case, the less to the more excitable, 
owing to the excitatory after- may occasionally be found re- 
effect of preparation, from inner ; ; 
to outer. versed, owing to the excitatory 

after-effect of preparation. The 
current of rest, moreover, is liable to autonomous periodic 
variation, as we have seen. 

The most satisfactory method of determining the relative 
excitabilities of two surfaces, then, lies in subjecting them to 
simultaneous excitation, and observing the direction of the 
responsive current. In the skin, unless the tissue was 


fatigued, I have always found this to be from the more 


RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 407 


excitable inner to the less excitable outer, even in those 
cases where the normal direction of the resting-current had 
been reversed, as an excitatory after-effect of preparation. 
This fact is well illustrated in the following record, taken 
with the skin of the neck of tortoise. As an after-effect of 
preparation, the resting-current so called was here reversed, 
flowing from inner to outer. But the excitatory responsive 
current was nevertheless from the more excitable inner 
to the less excitable outer (fig. 186). 

It was stated at the beginning of this chapter that the 
resultant response from inner to outer merely expresses the 
general fact that the vs 
excitability of the inner 
is greater than that of 
the outer. And this will a 
still remain true, even A : 
when the transformation ae 
of the external layer is — iN Se 3 
not so great as actually e : 4 


to reverse its individual 
g Fic. 187. Isolated Responses of Upper and 
galvanometric response Lower Surfaces of Skin of Tomato to Rotary 


from negativity to posi- Mechanical Stimulus 


an - (a) A, negative response of feeble intensity in 
tivity. The experi- rs outer ahce : “ negative response of iach 
mental results obtained greater intensity in inner surface ; (4) Cc, cur- 
‘ : ‘ rent of rest from outer to inner. Resultant 
with the skin of ripe excitatory response from inner to outer, due 


tomato form a case in to greater induced galvanometric negativity 
point. The natural ndeoat | 

current of rest is here, as usual, from the outer to the 
inner, and the excitatory responsive current in the opposite 
direction. But from the analysis of individual responses, 
on the outer and inner surfaces, obtained by means of 
the rotary apparatus for mechanical stimulation, it will be 
seen (fig. 187) that both the surfaces alike give the 
normal excitatory response of galvanometric negativity. 
This responsive negativity of the inner, B, is, however, 
very much greater than that of the outer. The resultant 


response, then, representing as this does the difference in 
; b dy 


308 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


degree between two negativities, is still from inner to 
outer, owing to the greater excitatory reaction of the 
inner. 

That the direction of the resultant response is actually 
from the inner to the outer is seen in the series of records 
given in fig. 188. The stimulus consisted of equi-alter- 
nating electrical shocks, applied at intervals of one minute. 
The record shows negative responses, followed appa- 
rently by the positive after-effect. In order to observe the 
peculiarities of this response in greater detail, the record 
was taken on a faster-moving drum (fig. 189). From this 
figure it will be seen that 
there was a short latent period 
of no responsive reaction. 
Response then rose to a 
maximum, and again sub- 
sided. After now reaching 
the zero position, the record 
proceeded in the positive 
direction, and again reverted 
back to zero. In_ similar 
records, the occurrence of 


Fic. 188, Photographic Record of 
Series of Responses in Skin of 


Tomato under Equi-alternating 
Electrical Shocks applied at In- 
tervals of One Minute 

Direction of resultant current from 
inner to outer followed by feeble 
opposite after-effect. 


this latent period, and _ posi- 
tive after-variation, has been 
adduced by certain physio- 
logists as affording visible 
demonstration of the exist- 


ence of opposite processes of assimilation and dissimilation. 
It has been supposed that the various features of the response 
were the outcome of a sort of tug-of-war between the two 
opposed forces, the preliminary pause being the expression 
of a short-lived balance, while the subsequent negative and 
positive variations were to be regarded as indicating the 
predominance, now of the one process, and then of the other. 
That in the present case such an assumption is unwarranted 
will be evident when we observe the isolated responses of the 
upper and lower surfaces separately (fig. 187). In each of 


RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 309 


these we see the normal response of galvanometric negativity, 
followed by recovery, without evidence of any antagonistic 
process, such as might give rise to subsequent positivity. 
The difference between these two responses lies simply in 
their time-relations. On simultaneous excitation of the two, 
the predominant negativity of the inner gives the first half 
of the negative response. The persistence of the excitatory 
reaction of the outer, on the other hand, after the subsidence 
of the effect on the inner, gives rise to the apparently 


Fic. 189. A Single Response of Skin of Tomato to Equi-alternating 
Shock recorded on Faster Moving Drum 


positive after-effect. Thus, here the supposed tug-of-war 
between two opposite processes of assimilation and dis- 
similation is, in reality, between two normal responses 
having ‘different time-relations. It is from a failure to 
recognise the fact that the excitatory reaction is not con- 
fined to one, but takes place on both surfaces, that such 
erroneous assumptions as that referred to have often been 
occasioned, 


310 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


The result which I have described—namely, a greater 
responsive negativity of the inner than of the outer, giving 
rise to a resultant responsive current from inner to outer—is 
that which occurs in the majority of cases with tomato. 
But, as establishing a continuity between this response and 
that of grape-skin, | may mention the interesting fact that 
in a few instances I obtained records in which, while the 
inner surface on excitation exhibited a strong negativity, 
the outer, under the same stimulus, exhibited a feeble 
positivity. 

The normal response of skin is sometimes, however, 
found to be reversed, and no explanations have yet been 


Fic. 190. Photographic Record of Series of Normal Responses 
in Skin of Gecko 


Responsive current from inner to outer surface. 


offered to account for this. But I have shown two definite 
conditions of universal application, which are liable to bring 
about the reversal of normal response. These are, on the 
one hand, sub-tonicity, and, on the other, fatigue. Should 
the condition, in a given case, be the former of these, then 
the impact of stimulus will of itself, by raising the tonic 
condition, restore the normal response. ‘Thus in a case of 
abnormal positive response due to sub-tonicity, an inter- 
vening period of tetanisation will tend to convert the ab- 
normal response to normal. An abnormal positive will thus 
pass into diphasic, and thence into the normal negative. 


RESPONSE OF ANIMAL AND VEGETAL SKINS 311 


For the following experiments, I took the skin of gecko, 
which can be detached from the body with very little injury. 
This animal offers remarkable facilities for many electro- 
physiological experiments. Its isolated tissues can be main- 
tained in a living condition for a very long time. Its sciatic 
nerve affords us a specimen about 15 cm.in length. Thus 
for electro-physiological investigation, it provides much 
greater advantages than the frog. 


Fic. 191. Photographic Record of Abnormal Diphasic Responses in Skin 
of Gecko, converted to Normal, after Tetanisation 


Taking a specimen of gecko skin, which was in a favour- 
able tonic condition, I obtained the series of normal responses 
to equi-alternating shocks, which is given in fig. 190. The 
responsive current here flowed from the inner to the outer 
surface. I next took another specimen, which was in a less 
favourable tonic condition, and obtained records of its 
responses, here seen (fig. 191) to be diphasic. An intervening 
period of tetanisation is seen, however, to restore the normal 
response. 


CHAPTER XXIII 
RESPONSE OF EPITHELIUM AND GLANDS. 


Epidermal, epithelial, and secreting membranes in plant tissues—Natural 
resting-current from epidermal to epithelial or secretory surfaces—Current of 
response from epithelial or secretory to epidermal surfaces—Response of 
Dillenia—Response of water-melon—Response of foot of snail—The so- 
called current of rest from glandular surface really due to injury— 
Misinterpretation arising from response by so-called ‘positive variation’ 
—Natural current in intact foot of snail, and its variation on section — 
Response of intact human armpit—Response of intact human lip—Lingual 
response in man—Reversal of normal response under sub-minimal or super- 
maximal stimulation—Differential excitations of two surfaces under different 
intensities of stimulus, with consequent changes in direction of responsive 
currents, diagrammatically represented in characteristic curves—Records ex- 
hibiting responsive reversals, 


HAVING now seen how the responsive peculiarities of the 
epidermis may be elucidated by the responses of similar 
tissues in the plant, we shall next take up an inquiry as to 
the parallelism between the responses of epithelium and 
glands in animal and in vegetable tissues. And here, as in 
the last case, we shall find the obscurities of the one made 
clear by the study of the other. 

If we take the hollow peduncle of a Uviclis lily, and, 
cutting this into longitudinal halves, take a portion from the 
upper end of one, we shall observe noticeable differences 
between the investing membranes of the outer and inner 
surfaces. On the outer, as we have seen elsewhere, the cells 
are dry, thick-walled, and cuticularised. This surface then 
is naturally distinguished as epidermal. The internal mem- 
brane of the hollow tube, however, is very thin, and its cells 
very little differentiated (fig. 192). The internal membrane 
may thus be distinguished as epithelial. 

If now we examine this inner membrane continuously 


A ote ate 34K =] 


RESPONSE OF EPITHELIUM AND GLANDS 313 


from the top to the bottom, at the point where the peduncle 
rises from the bulb, we shall find that the epithelial layer of 
the upper end passes imperceptibly into a markedly secret- 
ng (glandular?) layer at the lower. By this, secretion is 
constantly taking place, filling up the hollow tube with fluid. 
In one instance which I measured, the amount of this 
secretion was as much as IO grammes in the course of the 
day. These secreting cells in this, which may be called the 
glanduloid layer, are very thin- 
walled and excessively turgid, 
and, from an evolutionary point 
of view, these gradual transitions 
from epidermal to epithelial, and 
from epithelial to secretory layers, 
observed under conditions of such 
great simplicity, are extremely 
interesting. | 

When we come to test the 
electrical reactions of these tissues, 
we find, on making electrical con- 
nections with the external epi- 
dermis and the internal epithelium, 
that a natural current flows in the 
tissue from the external surface yc, 192, Transverse Section of 
to the internal: This would indi- tissue of Hollow Peduncle of 

: ‘ Uriclis Lily 
cate that the internal was the Cells of epidermis are small and 
more excitable of the two. This thick-walled, those of inner 

: . surface large and thin-walled. 
conclusion is confirmed on the 
application of simultaneous excitation to outer and inner ; 
for the direction of the responsive current is found to be from 
the internal surface to the external. _ 

If, next, electrical connections be made with the.epidermal 
and secretory layers, a current of rest is once more observed 
from the external to the internal. On excitation, a very 
strong electrical response is given, its direction being from 
the highly excitable secreting layer to the less excitable 
epidermal. From these experiments we see that the 


314 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


epidermal cells are, generally speaking, the least, and the 
secreting cells the most, excitable. 

I shall show, moreover, that all the responsive character- 
istics of these secretory cells are to be found repeated in those 
admittedly glandular layers which occur in the lining of the 
pitcher of Vepenthe, and cover the upper surface of the leaf of 
Drosera. Before, however, entering upon the consideration of 
these highly differentiated organs of Nepenthe and Drosera, - 
which are further characterised by some of the digestive 
functions, I shall first discuss in detail the reactions of a 
simpler type of vegetable organ. This is exemplified by 
a single unripe carpel of Dz/lenia indica, already referred to, 
When this is carefully removed from the inside of the 
pseudocarp, and opened, the inside is found to be filled with 
a gelatinous secretion. This is gently removed, and electrical 
connections are made with the inner and outer surfaces. It 
must be borne in mind that these vegetable organs, being not 
highly excitable, admit of experimental preparations being 
made with little or no excitatory effect of injury. Allowing 
now for a period of rest after making the preparation, it will 
always be found that the current of rest is from the outer 
layer to the secreting inner layer, which latter is thus, 
relatively speaking, galvanometrically positive. From the 
fact which we have generally observed, that the natural 
current of rest is from the less to the more excitable, it would 
appear, then, that the inner layer is here the more excitable. 
This conclusion, moreover, is in agreement with the inference 
already arrived at, in connection with the tissues of the 
Uriclis lily, that secreting cells as a rule are relatively the 
most excitable. This inference may, however, be subjected 
to the test of direct experiment. 

I first tested the response of the same specimen by means 
of thermal shocks, applied to both surfaces simultaneously. 
The definite direction of the responsive current, from the 
inner to the outer, across the tissue, proved conclusively that 

* the inner surface was the more excitable, becoming as it did, 
galvanometrically negative in relation to the outer, A similar 


RESPONSE OF EPITHELIUM AND GLANDS 315 


effect was obtained as an after-effect of equi-alternating 
shocks. I next took records of the direct effect of equi-alter- 
nating shocks. The direction of the responsive current was 
found, as before, from the inner to the outer. 

In the fruit of water-melon we obtain another specimen 
whose interior cavity is filled with secretion. On making a 
suitable preparation of this specimen, and arranging electrical 
connections with the outer epidermal and inner secretory 
surfaces, I found the responsive current, under equi-alter- 
nating electrical shocks, to be from the inner secretory to the 
outer epidermal. Fig. 193 gives a photographic record of 
these responses. 

From the typical responsive effects thus obtained with 
vegetable specimens under the simplest conditions, we are 
enabled to see that the effect of 
localised stimulus depends on 
the characteristic response of 
the surface layers of the organ. 
When dealing with this question 
of the electrical reaction of epi- 
thelium and glands in animal 
tissues, Biedermann rightly Fic. 193. Photographic-Record of 

: , Responses of Water-melon to 
came to the conclusion that it Equi-alternating Electric Shocks 
was the surface epithelial layer Responsive current from internal 
which was, in an electro-motive stale to external epidermal 
sense, most effective, the term, 
in its widest sense, including the epithelium of glands and 
papillz. 

One complicating factor present in the electrical reactions 
of animal epithelia and glands, but relatively absent under 
the simpler conditions of the plant, is the effect of injury caused 
by the process of isolation. The very fact of making the neces- 
sary section involves a stimulus of great intensity, and unless 
the effect of this has thoroughly subsided, the after-effect of 
such stimulation may be so strong as to reverse the normal 
current of rest, and otherwise modify the excitability of the 
tissue, This is seen, for example, in an experiment carried 


316 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


out by Dr. Waller,’ on the isolated paw of a cat. The 
current of rest was found by him to flow from the surface 
of the pad to the section. From this he was led to the 
conclusion that this current could not have been due to 
injury, since in that case it would have flowed from the 
sectioned to the uninjured surface, and not in the opposite 
direction, as was found to be the case. 


This misconception arises from a failure to realise that. 


the so-called ‘current of injury’ is, in fact, an after-effect of 
excitation. In the case under consideration, the section, 
acting as an intense stimulus, simply induces greater excita- 
tory reaction of the more excitable, which in this case 
happens, as we should have expected, to be the glandular 
surface. The injury-current here, then, is necessarily from 
the more excited glandular to the less excited non-glandular. 

A precisely similar result was obtained in the case of 
anisotropic plant-organs, where excitation caused by injury 
of the less excitable side, becoming internally diffused, 
induced greater galvanometric negativity of the more ex- 
citable distal point (p. 162). For a typical experiment on 
a glandular preparation, showing the principal effects, and 
the complications that may arise owing to injury, we may 
take the detached foot of the pond-snail, the lower surface of 
which, as is well known, secretes a slimy fluid. On allowing 
‘for the necessary period of rest, and then making electrical 
connections, we observe a current of rest, so-called, which 
flows from the glandular to the sectioned end. This is not 
to be mistaken for the true natural current of rest, being in 
fact the after-effect of a greater galvanometric negativity at 
the more excited glandular surface, consequent on section. 
An independent experiment, in support of this induction, 
will be described presently. On now simultaneously ex- 
citing the two surfaces, by equi-alternating shocks, the 
responsive current is found to flow from the gland inwards, 
the more excitable gland becoming thus galvanometrically 
negative. The responsive current is in this case in the same 


' Waller, Signs of Life, p. 101, 


RESPONSE OF EPITHELIUM AND GLANDS 317 


direction as the so-called current of rest, constituting a 
positive variation of it. 

From these experiments it is clear that the responsive 
current is due to the greater intensity of the induced gal- 
vanometric negativity at the more excitable glandular 
surface. It must, however, be noted here that this definite 
understanding of the phenomenon has been arrived at by 
fixing our attention on the relative excitatory reactions at 
the two contacts. If, instead of this, we had regarded it from 
the usual point of view, of variations of the resting-current 
only, we must have interpreted it as apparently an abnormal 
positive variation ;! for the so-called resting-current, in such 
a case, on account of the excitatory after-effect of injury, 
must also, as we have seen, flow from the more excited gland 
to the less excited muscle. Great confusion, and resultant 
misinterpretation of observations, have arisen from not 
sufficiently recognising these facts, that the resting-current 
may be originated in either of two distinct ways, and that 
the excitatory effect may consequently be summated with 
it in different manners. The resting-current in the primary 
condition is, as I have demonstrated elsewhere, the natural 
current. This originates in the natural differences of ex- 
citability between different points, and is, in the intact 
specimen, through the tissue from the less to the more ex- 
citable. External stimulus now gives rise to a responsive 
current, which is in the opposite direction, and therefore 
constitutes a negative variation of this, This takes. place 
because the more excitable point, which was naturally 
positive, has now become negative. But we may have a 
current of rest which is due to previous excitation, or 
injury, such as may be caused by the shock of the pre- 
paration. This current, though usually regarded as the 
resting-current, is not the true natural current of rest. It 
is really, as it were, the responsive current. become persis- 
tent. Succeeding stimuli, inducing responsive galvanometric 


1 We shall find in Chapter XX VII. that similar misinterpretations have arisen 
with regard to the responsive current in the retina, p. 417. 


218 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


negativity of the more excitable, will now give rise to a current 
in the same direction as this resting-current, thus constituting 
a positive variation of it. It is only when fatigue has set in 
at the more excitable, and induced great depression of 
excitability there, that the response-current may undergo a 
reversal, its direction now being from what was. originally 
the less excitable, to the originally more excitable (p. 177). 


An example of this I found in the sectioned foot of the large 


Indian garden-snail. Here the excitatory action on the 
glandular surface, due to the shock of preparation, was ex- 
tremely great, as evidenced by the profuse secretion which 
occurred iinmediately. Owing to this over-stimulation, 
fatigue was induced, with consequent great depression of ex- 
citability. Hence the responsive current was now found 
to be reversed, having, with reference to the glandular 
surface, become outgoing instead of ingoing. 

It has been stated above that the ingoing current of rest, 
observed at the glandular surface under preparation, was not 
the true natural current, but due to the excitatory after-effect 
of injury. ‘This I was able to verify by observing the current 
of rest under natural conditions, without excitation. The 
snail was allowed to crawl on a glass surface, in the middle of 
which was a strip of linen moistened in normal saline. This 
brought the glandular surface into electrical connection with 
one of two non-polarisable electrodes. When the snail had 
of its own accord come to a temporary standstill on this piece 
of linen, the other electrode was quietly placed against the 
skin of the upper side of the protruding body. The natural 
current of rest was now found to be outgoing, as regards 
the glandular surface of the foot, the more excitable being 
thus galvanometrically positive. The absolute electro-motive 
difference was found to be + ‘0013 volt. The foot was now 
sectioned, and the difference of potential between the same 
points was found to have undergone a reversal. The 
supposed resting-current was now ingoing, through the 
glandular surface. Thus, owing to the excitation consequent 
on preparation, the more excitable surface, originally positive, 


7). ee ee ee 


4064 cee ( 


RESPONSE OF EPITHELIUM AND GLANDS 319 


had become negative. The induced variation from the 
original condition, in the present case, was from + ‘OoI3 
volt to — ‘0020 volt. It will thus be seen that any irritation 
is liable to change the natural positivity of a highly excitable 
glandular surface to negativity. The supposed similarities 
between the ingoing responsive currents of frog’s skin, and 
the glandular surface of the stomach, are therefore not real. 
That the two cases are quite different is proved indeed by 
the fact that local stimulation of the surface of the skin 
induces galvanometric positivity, whereas a similar stimulation 
of the glandular surface induces negativity. 

In experimenting on animal tissues, it is therefore ad- 
visable, wherever possible, to use intact specimens. The 
numerous experimental difficulties with which we are in that 
case confronted, may be overcome by the method of simul- 
taneous and equi-alternating shocks which has been described. 
How practicable this method has been 
rendered will appear from the experi- 
ments which | have yet to describe on 
human subjects. 

We have seen that a protected sur- 
face is likely, other things being equal, 
to be more excitable than an exposed 
one. Partly owing to this fact, and 
partly also to its richer possession of 
imbedded glands, it appeared to me 
probable that the inner surface of the 
armpit would prove electrically more pig. 194. Photographic 
excitable than a corresponding area, on, Record of Electrical 

Responses of Intact 
say, the upper and outer surface of the Human Armpit 
same shoulder. In the records which I Responsive current from 
succeeded in obtaining (fig. 194), this “Pit to shoulder. 
supposition was fully borne out. Equi-alternating shocks of 
one second’s duration were applied at intervals of one minute, 
and the direct effect photographically recorded. The re- 
sulting responses were found to be ingoing as regards the 
armpit, thus proving that that surface was the more excitable. 


320 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


In order next to show that epithelial cells in the animal 
are relatively more excitable than epidermal, as we have 
already found to be the case in vegetable tissues, I performed 
the following experiment on the human lip. Here it was 
important that the electrical connections should be main- 
tained steady. A light spring-contact key was therefore 


made, as seen in the lower part of fig. 195. The lower. 
contact of this spring-clip consisted of an amalgamated plate . 


of zinc leading to the lower electrode. Over this were tied 
four thicknesses of blotting- 
paper soaked in zinc sulphate 
solution. On this again were 
placed three more thicknesses of 
blotting-paper, soaked in normal 
saline. The zinc plate which 
formed the upper limb of the 
clip, in connection with the 
second electrode, was similarly 
covered with separate layers of 
blotting-paper, soaked in zinc 
sulphate and normal saline re- 
spectively. The protruded lower 
lip was now placed in the clip, 
as shown in the upper figure, in 
| such a way that the latter made 
: ee eS of Towne a gentle but secure contact. A 

Lip. galvanometer and a source of 
Lower figure gives an enlarged view equi-alternating currents were 

oe also placed in the circuit. Of 
the two electrodes, the upper was in connection with the 
epithelial, and the lower with the epidermal surfaces. The 
natural current was now found to flow in the tissue, as in the 
corresponding cases of plant specimens, from the epidermal 
to the epithelial. The perfect steadiness of the contact was 
evidenced by the stillness of the deflected galvanometer spot 
of light. On now applying the alternating excitatory 
shock, the responsive current was found to be in the oppo- 


RESPONSE OF EPITHELIUM AND GLANDS 321 


site direction to the natural current, thus demonstrating the 
fact that the epithelial layer was here, as in the plant, the 
more excitable of the two. The regularity of this effect will 
be seen from the series of photographic records given below 
(fig. 196), in which is exhibited a slight staircase effect. 

We next proceed to deal with the response of the glan- 
dular organ, the tongue. The tongue of the frog has formed 
the subject of a very extended series of researches, by 
Engelmann and Biedermann. On_ very 
careful isolation, entailing as little injury 
as possible, it was found by these workers 
that the natural current was ‘entering’ 
that is to say, it flowed across the tongue 
from the upper surface to the lower. Both 
electrical and mechanical stimulation was 
found by these observers to cause a negative 
variation of this natural current. 

As isolation of such a highly excitable 
organ as the tongue may, however, give rise 
to unknown excitatory after-effects, it ap- 
peared to me very desirable that an investi- 
gation on this subject should be carried out * pei Se prides 
on the intact human tongue. In connection Electrical Response 
with this, I must point out that both the Sere pen 
surfaces of the tongue are excitable. Our Responsive current 
inquiry, therefore, is into the relative excit- ‘Tom epithelial to 

epidermal surface. 
abilities of its upper and lower surfaces. Here 
the experimental difficulty lies in this very high excitability 
of the organ, on account of which—except when in a quies- 
cent state and with a very steady contact—the galvanometer 
spot of light is apt to be erratic in its movements. Much 
of this difficulty is overcome, however, by holding the pro- 
truded tongue lightly clamped between the teeth. The upper 
and lower surfaces may then easily be held in the clip-key 
already described. From this double support of the clip and 
the teeth it is, with a little practice, possible to arrange 
matters in such a way that the galvanometer spot is 


Y 


322 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


practically stable. The current of rest in the intact human 
tongue is then found to be from the upper to the lower surface, 
as in the frog. This, according to our previous results, would 
indicate that the upper surface is the less excitable. This 
inference finds independent verification, when we subject the 
organ to the stimulus of equi-alternating shocks. A very 


strong responsive current is now found to flow through the 


tongue, from the lower to the upper surface. 


The tongue is so extremely sensitive that its characteristic 


response can be evoked even with very feeble stimulus. I 
have already explained that the alternating currents induced 
by speaking before a telephone are not exactly equal and 
opposite, the current being slightly stronger in one direction. 
Hence, if such currents be made to play upon an organ in 
which the excitability is only moderately differential, the 
preponderance of one of the two elements of the alternating 
shocks is then likely to mask the true excitatory effect. But 
the differential excitability of the tongue is so great that the 
responsive current is always from below to above, whether 
the exciting current be made to act in a favourable or 
unfavourable direction. Thus, if one speak, even in a very 
ordinary voice, into an exciting telephone, which is in series 
with the rest of the circuit, with its poles direct or reversed, 
a definite lingual current. is induced in response. This, as 
already said, is always in direction from the lower surface to 
the upper—surely a curious instance of the speech of one 
inducing lingual response in ariother, by direct, and not by 
provocative action ! 

The results which have been described are the normal 
effects given in response to stimulus of moderate intensity. 
By moderate stimulus is here meant that intensity of current 
which is obtained when the primary coil is slightly within 
the secondary. By feeble, on the other hand, is meant the 
intensity produced when the primary is at a distance from 
the secondary. Excessively strong stimulus again occurs 
when the primary is pushed fully within the secondary. I 
shall now proceed to describe occasional variations which 


a ee 


RESPONSE OF EPITHELIUM AND GLANDS 333 


may be observed when the stimulus is either very feeble or 
excessively strong. 

We have seen (p. 83) that when the intensity of stimulus 
is below the critical degree which is sufficient to induce 
response, its effect is to increase the internal energy of 
the tissue. We have also seen that the sign of this 
increased internal energy is galvanometric positivity, being 
thus opposite to the excitatory effect. Hence, in a dif- 
ferentially excitable tissue, we may expect to find instances 
in which stimulus that falls below the threshold of true 
excitation will act by inducing a greater galvanometric 
positivity of the more excitable, whereas, under normal 
intensity of stimulus, the more excitable would have 
become galvanometrically negative. We can thus see the 
possibility of response being reversed under very feeble 
stimulus. 

It must be remembered that the excitability of both 
the contacts is a factor in the response, which has hitherto 
been overlooked. A second very important factor, which 
has not yet been taken into consideration, is the difference 
between the characteristic curves of the tissues at the 
two different surfaces. By characteristic curve is here meant 
the curve which shows the relation between intensity 
of stimulus and response. This difference will be better 
understood from the diagram of the theoretical curves given 
below (fig. 197). This exhibits all the cases that can possibly 
exist. : . 

Let the curve A aa’ a" represent the characteristic curve 
of the surface A. Let the curve B 0 0’ 6” similarly represent 
the characteristic curve of the surface B. Of these two 
surfaces, B is under moderate stimulation, normally the more 
excitable. In the middle portion of the curve, representing 
response under moderate intensity of stimulus, the induced 
galvanometric negativity of B is thus greater than that of A. 
Under moderate excitation, therefore, the current is d’>a! 
through the tissue in the direction from B to A. But below 
the threshold of true excitation, B would be positive, and A 

¥2 


324. COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


relatively negative to it. Hence there would here be 
a reversal of response, the direction of the responsive current 
a> through the tissue being now from A to B. This current 
will be recorded by the galvanometer, provided the induced 
difference between A and B be sufficiently great. 

Having thus inferred the different effects possible under 
sub-minimal and moderate stimuli, we shall next consider 


the differential effect which may sometimes be induced by | 


excessively strong stimulus. In the middle part of the curve, 


| - ie 
4 | 
o , b 
é b 
z 
“1 
a 
A @ 
ped STIMULUS 
| 6 


Fic. 197. Possible Variations of Responsive Current, as between Two 
Surfaces A and B, shown by Means of Diagrammatic ‘Representations of 
Characteristic Curves 

A, a, a’, a”, characteristic curve of surface A; B, 4, 6’, 6", that of B. 
Under moderate stimulation, B is the more excitable, its induced 
galvanometric negativity being greater, and the direction of current 
from 4’ Zo a', as in the middle part of the curve. Under sub-minimal 
and super- -maximal stimulation the direction of the responsive current 
is reversed to a > 6 and a" - 6” respectively. 


B40! b” is seen to be very much steeper than Aad a’ 


tae 


that is to say, the excitatory effect increases very rapidly 
with the stimulus, in the more excitable of the two surfaces. 
But this increase may sooner or later reach a limit, that curve 
tending to become horizontal, aided in this process, possibly, 
by growing fatigue. The curve A a@a' a’, however, though 
not so steep, may yet continue to rise throughout a longer 
abscissa, representing increasing intensity of stimulus. In 
such a case, there would be a second crossing-point, and 


Mint thas do rn MMe 2 gee ae m . 


RESPONSE OF EPITHELIUM AND GLANDS 325 


a second reversal of normal response into a@’’>0", under 
excessively strong stimulation. We are thus enabled to see 
the theoretical possibility of the reversal of normal response 
under the two conditions of sub-minimal and super-maximal 
stimulation. All these phases may not be displayed in the 
same specimen; but it may be possible to find different 
specimens exhibiting one or the other. 

In some cases the difference a—d is too small to allow 
of an appreciable galvanometric effect, and in their higher 
parts the curves do not cross. In such specimens, then, 
there is no response under sub-minimal stimulus, and only 
normal response under increasing intensities, however strong. 
The only exception to this will take place when fatigue 
supervenes, a case which will be dealt with presently. I find 
that this type of response is the most common. 

We have next to consider those cases in which in the - 
sub-minimal region, the difference a—é is appreciable, the 
reversal to normal 6’>a’ taking place under higher in- 
tensities of stimulus. There need not in such an instance, 
be any second reversal. Here, then, the normal response 
under moderate or strong stimulus is reversed when the 
stimulus is sub-minimal. An example of this will be given 
presently. 

Lastly, there may be a type of response in which in the 
sub-minimal region the difference a—Jd is slight, and the 
normal 6’-<a' is reversed to a''+6" in the region of excessive 
stimulation. I shall be able to give an example of this also. 

I have been able, by taking different specimens, to 
demonstrate the occurrence of these theoretical reversals of 
response, under sub-minimal and super-maximal stimulation. 
I have not yet been able to find a single specimen ex- 
hibiting both reversals, but it is not impossible that this 
exists. | | 

The type in which response remains normal throughout 
a wide range of stimulus-intensity is too numerous to 
require special illustration. But this normal response may 
be reversed under fatigue. Generally speaking, a highly 


320 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


excitable tissue may be expected to show earlier or greater 
fatigue, than, other things being equal, a less excitable 
tissue.' Thus, under strong or long-continued stimulation, 
the excitability of the originally more excitable B may be 
depressed, so as to fall below that of A, with consequent 
reversal of response. This will be seen clearly ina typical 
experiment on the pulvinus of Mzmosa. Electrical con- 
nections were here made with the upper and less excitable 
surface A and the more excitable lower surface B, records 
being then taken of normal responses to equi-alternating 


Fic. 198. Photographic Record showing Reversal of Normal Response 
in Pulvinus of AZzmosa due to Fatigue 


(az) Series of normal responses, direction of current being from more 
excitable lower to less excitable upper; (4) Reversed responses in 
same specimen, due to previous tetanisation, causing fatigue. 


electric shocks, The stimulus employed was of moderate 
intensity, the secondary being placed slightly overlapping 
the primary. The responses (fig. 198, a) are seen to be 
normal, the responsive current being from the lower to 
the upper. They also show signs of slight fatigue, their 
amplitude undergoing diminution. The secondary was then 
pushed over the primary, and the tissue subjected to the 
consequent intense stimulus for two minutes continuously. 


' In this matter, the nature of the tissue must be taken into consideration, 
nerve, for example, being less subject to fatigue than muscle. 


RESPONSE OF EPITHELIUM AND GLANDS 327 


The secondary was next brought back to its original position 
and a record once more taken of its successive responses 
to stimuli of the same intensity as before. It will be seen 
(fig. 197, 6) that the response is completely reversed by the 
relative depression of the excitability of the lower surface 
B. Similar reversal under fatigue will be shown in the 
glandular organ of Drosera in the next chapter (fig. 208). 
I have obtained other interesting variations of normal 
response induced by fatigue. Thus, taking the carpel of 
Dillenia indica, and making electrical contacts with its inner 
and outer surfaces, the responses under moderate stimulus 
were found to be normal—that is to say, from inner to outer 
—the secondary being here understood to be partially over- 
lapping the primary at a distance of six divisions of the 
scale. The secondary was now pushed home, and the tissue 
subjected for a short time to strong and continuous stimula- 
tion. Moderate fatigue was thus induced. When the 
secondary was now brought back to the distance of six 
divisions of the scale the response was found to be reversed. 
Thus, moderate fatigue had here been sufficient to bring 
about the reversal of the relative excitabilities of the two 
surfaces of the carpel of Dz/lenia indica, when the testing 
stimulus was of original intensity. But when the intensity 
of stimulus was increased, by pushing in the secondary to a 
position marked three divisions on the scale, the response 
became once more normal. Thus, fatigue had in this case 
modified the excitability of the two surfaces in such a way 
that an intensity of stimulus, which was formerly effective to 
induce greater excitation of the originally more excitable, was 
now ineffective ; and its greater excitation, with the restoration 
of normal response, could now only be evoked under stronger 
stimulus. 

We shall next describe the reversal ot normal response 
under sub-minimal stimulation. For this we shall once 
more select the carpel of Dzd/enia indica. A record of its 
normal response—the direction being from inner to outer—is 
given in the first series of records in fig. 199. After taking 


328 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


this record the intensity of stimulus was reduced by pulling 
out the secondary further away from the primary. The 
responses were now found reversed, as seen in the subsequent 
series. | 
We have last to consider the reversal induced by intense 
stimulation. Such instances must not be confused with the 
effect of fatigue. The two 
can be distinguished by the: 
fact that the fatigue-reversal 
takes place after a series of 
normal responses, whereas 
the true reversal, due to 
strong intensity of stimulus, 
which we are now discuss- 
ing, is exhibited at the very 
beginning. Such an effect 
I have observed in the 
response of the human 
Fic. 199. Photographic Record showing lip. The direction of 
Reversal of Response in Carpel of the responsive current was 


Dillenia indica, under Sub-minimal 
Gisiawbatins normally, under moderate 


The first series show normal electrical stimulus, from the epi- 
responses under moderate stimulus, 41,1; . 
responsive current from internal to thelial to : the epidermal 

- external surface; the second series surfaces. Under very strong 


exhibit reversed response under sub- ° : 
minimal stimulation, current from stimulus, however, this 
external to internal surface. normal direction was found 
to be reversed. 

But the employment of excessively strong stimulation 
introduces other complicating factors. ‘The applied stimulus 
may be supposed to be localised only when it is of moderate 
intensity. With intense stimulus the subjacent tissues are 
liable to be involved in giving rise to excitatory response ; 
and it then becomes a difficult problem to discriminate how 
much of the observed effect is due to the superficial layer, 
and how much to others more deeply situated, 


CHAPTER XXIV 
RESPONSE OF DIGESTIVE ORGANS 


Consideration of the functional peculiarities of the digestive organs— Alternating 
phases of secretion and absorption—Relation between secretory and con- 
tractile responses. [Illustrated by (a) preparation of AM/zmosa; (6) glandular 
tentacle of Drosera—-General occurrence of contractile response—True current 
of rest in digestive organs—Experiments on the pitcher of Mepenthe—Three 
definite types of response under different conditions-—Negative and positive 
electrical responses, concomitant with secretion and absorption—Multiple 
responses due to strong stimulation— Response in glandular leaf of Drosera— 
Normal negative response reversed to positive under continuous stimulation 
—Multiple response in Drosera—Response of frog’s stomach to mechanical 
stimulation — Response of stomach of tortoise—Response of stomach of gecko , 
—Multiple response of frog’s stomach, showing three stages—negative, 
diphasic, and positive—Phasic variations. 


HAVING now dealt with the responsive characteristics of the 
skin, epithelium, and glands, alike in plant and animal, the 
next subject to be taken up is that of the response of the 
digestive mucosa. And here we have to determine, first, 
whether or not there is, broadly speaking, any continuity 
between the responses of digestive organs and those which 
we have just been studying ; and, secondly, to what extent 
the functional specialisation of the tissue has acted in 
accentuating certain of its responsive peculiarities. 

Surveying the function of digestion as a whole, we see 
that it consists, briefly, of two different processes—those, 
namely, of a previous secretion, by which food is rendered 
soluble, and of a subsequent absorption, by which the dis- 
solved foodstuffs are absorbed. In the membrane of the 
simplest digestive organ, then, the epithelial ‘lining, using 
that term in its most inclusive sense, must be endowed with 
the two properties of secretion and absorption under different 


330 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


circumstances. The question then suggests itself, what are 
the circumstances which determine this outflow or inflow ? 
In the digestive processes, moreover, in plant and animal 
alike, each of the reactions referred to, whether of secretion 
or absorption, must be more or less long-continued. Thus 
these responsive actions, instead of being single and spas- 
modic, are likely to be multiple and long-sustained. The 


characteristic response to stimulus of a secreting organ is. 


understood to be by secretion. Is this reaction essentially 
different from those fundamental processes which underlie 
the responses of contractile organs, or are we to regard con- 
traction and secretion as but different expressions of a single 
responsive phenomenon ? | 

In order to test this question, of the connection between 
responsive secretion and contraction, it will be well here to 
draw attention to certain experiments of Sachs, on the 
response of J/zmosa, though our inferences will be somewhat 
. different from those which their author intended. If we take 
a longitudinal slice of the lower half of the pulvinus of 
Mimosa and keep it in a moist chamber for some time till 
_ the tissue has recovered from the excitation due to section, 
and if we then subject it to fresh excitation, water will be 
found to ooze out, or undergo secretion from the excited 
tissue. We may explain this occurrence in either of two 
ways: first, that, in consequence of the molecular changes 
induced by stimulus, contraction and permeability-variations 
take place in the cells, the expulsion of water being an 
expression of the active process of contraction ; or, secondly, 
that the oozing-out of the water is a passive process, due to 
permeability-variation of the turgid cells alone, without con- 
traction. 

The two theories may be distinguished broadly as those 
of active contraction and passive secretion. In the thin 
section of the Mzmosa pulvinus it is the exudation of water 
that is noticeable, and not any marked movement character- 
istic of contraction. But in the intact pulvinus, owing to,its 
anisotropic structure, the greater contraction of the more 


ea bad _ 


ht Li 


aaa aii i 


pan” 
PRBS 


RESPONSE OF DIGESTIVE ORGANS 331 


excitable lower half is exhibited in a marked manner by 
downward mechanical movement, magnified as this is by the 
long petiolar index. The intact organ, moreover, is invested 
with an impervious skin, hence the excitatory exudation, or 
expulsion, of water, being internal, is not seen outwardly. 
Thus a single identical reaction may appear from different 
points of view, as either secretory or contractile. 

The occurrence of contraction is thus most easily demon- 
strable when it is accompanied by conspicuous movement. 
This, however, demands considerable physiological aniso- 
tropy, the differential contraction then giving rise to a very 
marked lateral movement, as in J/zmosa. Ih radial organs 
of plants, on the other hand, owing to balanced contractions 
of opposite sides, there is no marked responsive movement. 
Hence ordinary plant-organs have hitherto been regarded as 
non-contractile and insensitive. But I have shown that all 
these radial organs exhibit longitudinal contraction, to be 
detected and recorded by means of suitable magnifying 
devices. All motile responses are brought about, it must be 
remembered, by transference or redistribution of fluids. Now, 
in organs invested with impervious membranes the effect of 
fluid-transference is. manifested by mechanical movement ; 
whereas, in naked tissues, the fluid-transference is directly 
visible as secretion. | 

From the very important series of researches carried out 
by Darwin, on the excitatory reactions in the tentacles of 
Drosera, we know that the pedicel, carrying the gland on its 
summit, is somewhat flattened, and that it is this anisotropic 
lower part which is alone capable of movement. The gland- 
cells on the head of the tentacle have been shown by 
Gardiner to be provided with delicate uncuticularised cell- 
walls, which are curiously pitted on their upper or free 
surfaces. The terminal organ, or head, which is radial, 
would thus seem to be peculiarly fitted for the exudation of 
liquid on excitation. In the anisotropic motile portion of 
the pedicel, on the other hand, the responsive reaction mani- 
fests itself by bending. It would thus appear that the same 


332 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


excitatory reaction may exhibit itself in different parts, even 
of the same organ, by mechanical movement and secretion 
respectively, according to the facilities which one or the other 
portion offers. 

That the phenomenon of contraction is behind the ex- 
citatory expulsion of water in a vegetable organ, would 


appear highly probable from certain results obtained in the | 


electrical response. The stimulation of an ordinary vegetable 
tissue gives rise to two distinct electrical effects at a distance. 
The first of these is the arrival of the hydro-positive effect of 
galvanometric positivity, with positive turgidity-variation. 
The second is the wave of true excitation, with its character- 
istic of negative turgidity-variation and concomitant gal- 
vanometric negativity. The first, consisting, as this does, 
of a hydrostatic blow delivered at a distance, can only, it 
appears to me, be ascribed to an active process of contrac- 
tion, causing the squeezing-out of water in the excited region. 
A passive escape of fluid, due to mere permeability-variation, 
could not, as I think, originate that impulsive hydrostatic 
shock which is transmitted to a distance. For such a result 
to take place an active expulsion would seem to be 
requisite. 

In view of these facts, is it necessary to hold the doctrine 
of discontinuity, or, when there is evidence in its favour, are 
we to believe in the continuity of these apparently different 
reactions? The excitatory reactions of different classes of 
tissues have hitherto been regarded as different, chiefly 
because some were looked upon as motile, and others as 
non-motile ; muscle, for example, was held to be typical of 
the first, and nerve of the second, of these classes. In this 
case of the nerve, it has been believed that there was no 
visible manifestation of the excitatory change. I shall, 
however, be able to show that even this supposition is 
incorrect, since the excitatory reaction in the nerve is in fact 
attended by contraction. The electrical indication of gal- 
vanometric negativity which is concomitant with contraction 
in contractile tissues, is also obtained in the case of excited 


al i kk 


« bb SD WAL et OF ee Ser 


Tet! 


RESPONSE OF DIGESTIVE ORGANS 333 


glands. The visible changes which occur under stimulation 
in these three types of tissues would thus appear to differ 
only in degree. ae Io 

We may now turn more especially to the question of the 
electrical reactions of the digestive mucosa. As regards the 
natural current of rest, we have seen that Rosenthal and 
others found that this current was ingoing—that is to say, 
the mucous layer was negative, as compared to the muscular 
coat of the stomach. Biedermann had also noticed a strong 
current of rest between the glandular surface of Drosera and 
the stalk. But it will be shown that the glandular coat of 
the stomach is more excitable than the muscular layer. 
Hence we should have expected that the natural current of 
rest would have been from the less excitable to the more 
excitable, the mucous layer in a state of rest being thus 
relatively galvanometrically positive. The opposite direc- 
tion of the current which has been observed, would rather 
appear to be ascribable to the excitatory after-effect of pre- 
paration. I have already described how the glandular foot 
of the snail, under conditions of perfect rest, is galvano- 
metrically positive. But the excitation caused by prepara- 
tion renders this highly excitable glandular surface negative 
That the fact of cutting open the stomach, to make the 
experimental preparation, similarly, would cause intense 
excitation with galvanometric negativity, was to have been 
expected. I shall be able, indeed, to show, by means of 
experiments to be described presently, that the shock con- 
sequent on this preparation is to give, not one, but a pro- 
longed series of multiple electrical responses. 

I have almost invariably found, in making electrical 
contacts after section, with the inner and outer surfaces of 
frog’s stomach, that the multiple responses caused by section, 
persisted for more than an hour; and until these had 
subsided no fresh experiment could be undertaken, to 
obtain records of the response of the stomach to external 
stimulus. I have also found that many of these frogs were 
in the habit of swallowing stones and pebbles, the persistent 


334 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


mechanical irritation of which would contribute to the 
negativity of the mucous lining. 

Finding, then, that it would be impossible to obtain the 
natural current, in a stomach which had to be cut open, I 
next turned my attention to stomachs which are naturally 
open. These are seen in the upper concave surface of the 
leaf of Drosera, for instance, which is provided with glandular 
tentacles. I here made electrical connections with the upper 
and lower surfaces respectively. But the tentacles excited by 
the contact of the electrode bent and clasped it round, an 
excitation which was seen in the galvanometer as negativity 
of that surface. From this may be gauged the difficulties 
which attend the observation of the true natural current of 
rest in such an excitable organ as the stomach. The 
demonstration, however, of the galvanometric positivity of 
the snail’s foot, and of the inner glandular surface of the 
carpel of Dzllenza indica, lead to a strong presumption in 
favour of the true resting-current in the stomach being from 
the non-mucous layers to the mucous. 

Having thus seen the difficulties imposed by the high 
motile excitability of the tentacles of Drosera, 1 next turned 
my attention to other specimens. We have seen that there 
is a secretion of fluid at the lower end of the hollow interior 
of the peduncle of Uvzcls lily, and that the secreting inner 
layer is here galvanometrically positive in a state of rest. 
As this tube, however, is closed, it cannot be regarded as 
subserving the absorption of food-material. But the same 
limitation does not apply to those modified foliar structures, 
the pitchers of Vepenthe ' (fig. 200). These, as is well known, 
are open. They have a histological differentiation, moreover, 
of their lining membrane, actual glands being present 
(figs. 201, 202), which are admitted to be comparable to those 
of the animal digestive organ, though of a much simpler type. 
A fluid is secreted by these glands, and insects entrapped in 
the pitcher are in it dissolved or decomposed. The products 


1 I have to thank the authorities of the Botanical Garden, Sibpur, for 
supplying me with these valuable specimens. 


— , a 


Og WO pee er 


RESPONSE OF DIGESTIVE ORGANS : 335 


are subsequently absorbed by the tissue, as in corresponding 
cases, by the stomach of animals. From the study of the 
responsive peculiarities of so primitive a type of stomach, we 
might then expect to gain much light on the action of more 
complex and highly specialised digestive organs. In order 
first to obtain the true current of rest, 
I took a young pitcher which had 
previously been kept free from all 16 
disturbance. I next made electrical INN 
; yl 
connections, by means of non-polaris- Ly ans 
able electrodes, with the inner (glan- i Bt ha 
dular) and outer surfaces of this iy 
pitcher respectively. As some excita- ianuil 
tory reaction may be induced in a 1 Mm ZN 
highly excitable organ, even by the wo BE 
contact of normal saline, the cotton ] 
threads in connection with these non- F 
polarisable electrodes were moistened Vy WE 
with the natural secretion of the Woe 
pitcher itself. On carrying out the 
experiment under these ideal condi- 
tions, I found, as I had expected, 
that the current of rest flowed from 
the outer non-glandular to the inner glandular surface, the 
latter thus being galvanometrically positive. 

In investigating next the excitatory reaction, I obtained 
three different types of responses—negative, diphasic, and 
positive—characteristic of certain definite conditions. Before 
entering upon the details of these experiments, it is advisable 
to discuss here the probable significance of the negative and 
positive electrical reactions observed. 

We have seen that a slice of tissue from the’ pulvinus of 
Mimosa excretes water under excitation. The electrical 
reaction under these circumstances is one of galvanometric 
negativity. But during the process of recovery, when the 
tissue is absorbing water, this negativity diminishes, a change 
that is tantamount to that increase of positivity with which 


Fic. 200. Pitcher of Ve- 
penthe, with lid removed. 


336. COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


we are already familiar, as the invariable accompaniment of a 


positive turgidity-variation. 


Thus, if galvanometric negativity is to be taken as the 
concomitant of the expulsion or secretion of fluid, it would 


x ieee 
q\ S EN 


AD 
PSS) 


ay 08 ges 
of: - e< 


\ 
an {gee we ue i 


Fic. 201. Glandular Surface of a iin 
of the Living Membrane of the Pitcher 


appear that the opposite 
process of absorption would 
be indicated by the respon- 
sive galvanometric 
tivity. 

Again the fresh pul- 
vinus of J7Zzmosa responds, 
when excited, by. a me- 
chanical fall, a negative 
turgidity-variation, and by 
galvanometric negativity. 


posi-_ 


of Nepenthe. 
But after long-continued 


stimulation, these normal responses are found to undergo 
reversal. The pulvinus expands; water must be re-absorbed, 
and the leaf is re-erected. The normal galvanometric nega- 
tivity is now reversed to positivity. It will thus be seen that 


while, in a fresh tissue, stimulus gives rise to expulsion of 
fluid—the electrical indi- 


cation of this 
being galvanometric nega- 
tivity—in a tissue which 
has already, on the other 
hand, been under con- 
tinuous stimulation there 
will be a tendency towards 
the phasic reversal of re- 
sponse to galvanometric 
positivity, indicative of the 
process of absorption. 

In the case of motile 
tissues, these excitatory 
reactions of the outflow and inflow of fluids appear to us of 
little consequence, except in the form of those appropriate 


Transverse Section of Tissue of 


FIG. 202. 
Pitcher of Mepenthe. 


wu, outer surface; L, inner surface; g, glands 
present in the internal surface. 


process © 


i WR 


RESPONSE OF DIGESTIVE ORGANS 337 


motile responses which they occasion. But in glandular 
organs, they become possessed of much greater significance, 
since they constitute the main function of such structures. 

Electrical responses, in every way analogous to those 
which have been described, are obtained from the glandular 
surfaces of digestive organs. That is to say, the glandular 
surface when fresh exhibits responsive galvanometric nega- 
tivity on excitation. In Drosera, for example, under these 
conditions secretion is seen to take place. Thus the negative 
phase of response, in this as in the case of Mzmmosa, is asso- 
ciated with expulsion of fluid or secretion. After continuous 
stimulation, again, the responsive phase here, as in MWzmosa, is 
found to be reversed to positive, indicative, as there is every 
reason to believe, of absorption. From a consideration of 
the functions of the digestive organ, we should be prepared, 
as already pointed out, to expect the occurrence of two 
alternating processes. In the fresh state, ingestion of food, 
acting as a stimulus, would naturally induce excitatory 
secretion; and this excitatory secretion must be followed 
later by the absorption of dissolved food. These alternating 
phases of secretion and absorption indubitably occur. We 
shall also find, in the electrical response-records, a phasic alter- 
nation of negative and positive under appropriate conditions. 

We have seen that. there is a continuity between the 
different reactions of non-glandular and glandular tissues. We 
have also seen that, as in the one case, so too in the other, 
a phasic change takes place from negative to positive. Inthe 
digestive organ, however, we have to deal mainly with the 
fluctuations of fluids—secretion and absorption—and the 
attendant electrical variations, which consist of two opposite 
phases, positive and negative. As far as I have found it 
possible to test the matter experimentally, it has invariably 
been the case that the negative electrical phase was associated 
with secretion ; and everything points to the probability that 
the converse of this—the association, namely, of. the positive 
electrical phase with the process of absorption—-holds equally 
good. 


338 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


We now return to the question of the normal response of 
the pitcher in its three different conditions. Of these, in the 
youngest, no flies are present; such a specimen will be 
known as ‘fresh.’ Jn others, somewhat older, are a few 
insects. These pitchers may be regarded as moderately 
excited, owing either to the struggles of the insects or to the 
supply of food, or to both. Still another class is found, in 
which the glandular part of the inner surface of the pitcher is 
practicaily coated with captured insects, and has thus already 
been subjected to long- 
continued stimulation. 
The responses of these 
three classes of specimens 
are in each case, as I shall 
show, very characteristic. 

I shall first describe 
experiments carried out 
on fresh = specimens. 
Records were made of 
their responses to equi- 
alternating shocks of 
moderate intensity, at 


Photographic Record of Series 


F1G. 203. 


of Normal Negative Responses of Glan- 
dular Surface of Wepenthe in Fresh Con- 


dition to Equi-alternating Electric 
Shocks given at Intervals of Two Minutes 


Responsive current from internal glandular 
to external non-glandular surface. Note 
occurrence of multiple response and trend 


intervals of two minutes. 
The responsive current 
was here found to flow 
from the internal glan- 
dular to the external non- 


of base-line upwards. if 
glandular surface. Fig. 


203 gives a series of such responses. It was said at the 
beginning that the responses of digestive organs were likely 
to be multiple. This is seen to be true even under the 
moderate stimulus applied in the present case. But under 
the action of stronger stimulus, such as that of a thermal 
shock, the response is found to consist of a long and multiple 
series, records of which will be seen later. 

Another peculiarity to be noticed, in the series of re- 
sponses given in fig. 203, is that the base-line of the record 


~RESPONSE OF DIGESTIVE ORGANS. 339 


trends upwards. This indicates that the glandular surface, 
by the residual effect of stimulation, is being rendered more 
and more galvanometrically negative. This explains why 
the internal surface of the stomach has been found by 
different observers to be negative, a condition of more or 
less persistent negativity being thus clearly due to the ex- 
citatory after-effect of preparation. Had it not been for the 
exceptional opportunity afforded by the open pitcher of 
Nepeuthe, it would have been impossible to make galvano- 
metric connections with the intact inner glandular surface 
and thus to ascertain that such a surface is naturally gal- 
vanometrically positive. 

I may here point out the very interesting medics 
tion of response which occurs in the same specimen under 
a long-continued series of stimulations. This modification, 
due to fatigue so-called, makes its appearance first in dimi- 
nution of the height of the responses. Some of the con- 
stituent multiple responses due to a single stimulus are 
then found to be reversed to positive, and after this they 
show a tendency to become more or less completely 
reversed. 

It is also interesting to find that the same modifications 
make their appearance, in the same order, in those pitchers 
which have been subjected to continuous stimulation, to a 
greater or less extent, by the supply of insects. That is 
to say, a pitcher containing a few insects is found to give 
responses, the multiple constituents of which are sometimes 
positive and sometimes negative. This intermediate phase is 
seen well illustrated in the record given in fig. 204. But in 
the pitcher whose inner surface is already thickly coated with 
insects, and which has long been exposed to the continuous 
action of such stimulation, the characteristic response is 
found to be the reverse of that of the fresh specimen. — It 
will be seen from the record in fig. 205 that in such a case 
the individual effect of a single stimulus is a series of mul- 
tiple responses which are positive. In this record a curious 


effect is again seen, that of the shifting om the base-line, now 
ae 


340 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


downwards. This indicates an increasing positivity of the 
glandular surface. 

The results which have thus been described, in the case 
of the fresh pitcher, and of one subjected for a long period to 
the stimulus of food, are fully compatible, it will be observed, 
with the theory of digestion as a diphasic process, in which 
galvanometric negativity is associated with a predominant 
secretion, and the subsequent galvanometric positivity with a. 
predominant absorption by 
the glandular membrane. 


Fic. 205. Photographic Record of Re- 
sponses of Pitcher in Third Stage, the 
whole Glandular Surface thickly 

Fic.. 204. Photographic Record Coated with Insects. Stimuli applied 
of Responses of Pitcher in at Intervals of two Minutes 
Intermediate Stage, having 
Attracted a Few Insects 


The response here is in the positive phase, 
direction of current being from non- 


Note here the occurrence of two glandular to glandular. Notealso the 
phases in constituent responses, multiple character of responses to 
positive being predominant. single stimuli. 


It is also important to notice that while in the fresh 
condition the glandular surface is positive, and in the 
moderately stimulated condition negative, yet positivity of 
the glandular surface is not always to be taken as a sign of 
its fresh condition. For we have here seen that under long- 
continued stimulation, the electrical condition is apt to be 
reversed to one of positivity, 


RESPONSE OF DIGESTIVE ORGANS 341 


It has been stated that on account of the highly excitable 
nature of the digestive organ, a single stimulus, if strong, 


Fic. 206. Multiple Response of Pitcher of Mefenthe, in First or Fresh 
Stage, to Single Strong Thermal Shock 


The constituent responses are both negative and positive, the former being stronger. 


would give rise in it to a multiple series of responses. The 
two following records (figs. 206 and 207) illustrate this fact 
in two different speci- 
mens which were in 
somewhat different con- 
ditions. The stimulus 
employed in each case 
was a _ single strong 
thermal shock, and the 
multiple responses were 
found to persist, in both, 
for quite an hour. In 
the first of these figures, 
the constituent responses 
of the series were both 
negative and positive, ye, 207, Multiple Response of Pitcher of 
the former being pre- Nepenthe, in Third Stage, to Single Strong 
dominant. In the second gna ae 

The constituent responses are here - pre- 
record (fig. 207 ) the dominantly positive. 
positive phase is pre- 
dominant in the responses, and the trend of the base-line down- 
wards shows increasing positivity of the glandular surface. 


342 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


In taking up this investigation on the pitcher of Vepenthe 
it appeared to me that much light would be thrown, by the 
study of this simple organ, on the many: difficulties connected 
with the response of the more complex digestive organs of 
the animal. This surmise has proved to be fully justified, 
for in the experiments which I have carried out in the latter 
field, the results are a mere repetition of these typical effects 
seen in Wepenthe under corresponding circumstances. 

Before passing from 
Nepenthe to the study 
of digestive tissues in 
animals, it will be well 
to deal here with the 
more complex type of 
vegetal digestive organ 
seen in the plant 
Drosera. 1 took for my 
experiment a specimen 
of the Indian Dvrosera 
longifolia, the upper 
surfaces of whose leaves 
are covered, as is well 


known, with glandular 
Fic. 208. Photographic Record of Responses : 
in Fresh Leaf of Drosera to Equi-alternating tentacles. Here, as ‘5 
Electrical Shocks the case of the pitcher 
The first series show normal responses. Current J h 
from upper glandular to lower non-glandular of Nepentne, the re- 


surface. In the second series normal response sponse of leaves which 
is reversed to positive, after tetanisation, T. 
are fresh and have not 


been subjected to previous excitation, is by induced negativity 
of the glandular surface, and this is reversed to positive under 
long-continued stimulation. These two phases are seen in 
figure 208, in which the first series is a record of normal 
responses of galvanometric negativity, to equi-alternating 
shocks applied at intervals of one minute; and the second, 
the reversed responses exhibited by the same leaf, to the 
same stimulus, when it has, in the meantime, been subjected 
to tetanising shocks for three minutes continuously. It is 


RESPONSE OF DIGESTIVE ORGANS 343 


curious and interesting to note here, as in the case of 
Nepenthe, the trend of the base-line up, when the response 
is the normal negative, and down when it is the reversed 
positive, indicating in the one case increasing negativity, and 
in the other increasing positivity. 

As in the Mepenthe, so also in the leaf of Drosera, 
specimens which are not fresh—that is to say, previously 
unexcited——are apt to exhibit the positive phase of response. 
I give below a series of multiple 
responses (fig. 209) induced in such 
a leaf by a strong stimulation. The 
stimulus was in this case given by 
sectioning the leaf, and the response 
therefore illustrates the fact that 
preparation itself acts as a stimulus. 
In the present case, electrical con- 
nections with the galvanometer, 
were made with the upper and 
lower surfaces of the leaf on the 
plant, intact. On now cutting the 
petiole across, a long series of 
multiple responses, lasting for about 
45 minutes, was found to be set 
up. These pulsations were at first 
rapid, and then slowed down 
gradually, the average period of a r pae, Pestommpnic Bacol 
single pulsation being about 30 of Drosera in Positive Phase 
seconds. Only a portion of the ‘Stimulus was caused here by 

: : section of the petiole. 
record is shown in fig. 209. 

Having thus seen the typical responses exhibited by 
the digestive organs of plants, we shall now pass to the 
consideration of the reactions induced in animal stomachs. 
Here, again, two different subjects of inquiry arise, the 
direction, namely, of the natural current of rest, and that of 
the action or responsive current. As regards the first of 
these, it will be remembered that Rosenthal found it to be 
strongly ‘ingoing’—that is to say, from the mucous to the 


344 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


muscular coats of the stomach. From this it was supposed, 
as we have seen, that the mucous coat of the stomach of the 
frog had the same electro-motive reaction as its outer skin. 
We shall find, however, that there is in reality no such 
similarity between the two, inasmuch as, while the excitatory 
reaction makes the outer skin galvanometrically positive, its 
effect on the mucous surface under normal conditions is to 
induce galvanometric negativity. In the case of Nepenthe, 
further, we have seen that the natural current of rest is from 
the non-glandular outer to the glandular inner surface, and 
that this is liable to reversal, as an excitatory after-effect of 
preparation. The i ingoing current, therefore, observed in the 
preparation of frog’s stomach, is to be regarded, not as the 
natural current of rest, but as the excitatory after-effect due 
to isolation. 

With regard, next, to the current of action, Biedermann 
states that direct electrical excitation, by rapidly alternating 
shocks, induces a negative variation usually preceded by a 
positive swing. Since the so-called current of rest is ingoing, 
a ‘negative variation’ of it evidently means an outgoing 
current—that is to say, galvanometric positivity of the 
mucous coat. Hence the responsive action of the mucous 
coat, as described by Biedermann, is a transient negativity 
followed by positivity. 

In dealing with this question of the electrical response 
of the digestive organ, we must be prepared, as the result of 
previous experiments on plants, to meet with variations of 
the excitatory effect, due to the phasic condition of the tissue. 
And first, for the clear demonstration of the effect of ex- 
citation on the mucous surface, uncomplicated by changes 
induced at the second contact, I employed the Rotary 
Method of Mechanical Stimulation of the given area. The 
rotating electrodes were applied to the inside of a properly 
mounted frog’s stomach, and experiment commenced some 
time after the cessation of the multiple response due to 
preparation. The following record (fig. 210) exhibits the 
first four of these responses to individual mechanical stimuli, 


ee 


DL we and Oh, cocaine arth lh ee 


RESPONSE OF DIGESTIVE ORGANS 345 


applied at intervals of one minute. The responsive variation 
took place by the induced galvanometric negativity of the 
excited area. Under long-continued stimulation fatigue was 
found to be induced, the responses becoming diminished 
and even tending to a reversal from the normal to 
positive. 

In order to show that the inner mucous surface is 
relatively more excitable than the muscular coat, I next 
subjected the two to simultaneous excitation by equi-alter- 
nating electrical shocks, And for the sake of establishing a 


Fic, 210, Photographic Fic. 211, Photographic Record 
Record of Normal of Normal Negative Re- 
Negative Responses sponses of Stomach of Tor- 
of Frog’s Stomach to toise to Stimulus of Equi- 
Mechanical Stimula- alternating Electric Shocks 
tion applied at Intervals of One 

Minute 


generalisation as to the reaction of the stomach, I now took 
a different specimen—namely, the stomach of tortoise. The 
responses in the figure (fig. 211) showed relative galvano- 
metric negativity of the inside of the stomach. 

We have seen that fresh vegetable stomach responds by 
normal negativity, but that, under continuous stimulation, 
a phasic change is induced, by which response is reversed to 
positive (cf. fig. 208). I shall next demonstrate the corre- 
sponding effect in the animal stomach. Taking.a preparation 
of the stomach of gecko, I obtained normal responses, whose 
direction was from the glandular internal to the muscular 


346 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


external surface. After an intervening period of tetanisation, 
however, the responses are seen to be reversed (fig. 212). 

The next record has been selected for the purpose of 
showing the gradual process of transition from the normal 
negative to the reversed positive response. The specimen 
taken was frog’s stomach. At the commencement of the 
experiment the galvanometer spot was quiescent, but when 
the specimen was subjected to a single strong thermal shock, 
a prolonged series of multiple responses was initiated, per- 
sisting for more than an hour, Of this series I here re- 
produce four different 
portions (fig. 213). The 
first of these (a) con- 
sists of pulses of gal- 
vanometric negativity 
of the internal surface. 
The recoveries are here 
incomplete, and _ the 
base-line shifts upwards, 
showing an increasing 
negativity of that sur- 
face. The negative 
pulses are then reversed 
Fic. 212, Photographic Record of Normal to positive, through an 

Response in Stomach of Gecko to Equi- intermediate di-phasic 

cient aaa seen to be reversed after (6). Tn the: first part 

of this pronouncedly 
positive response (c) the base line is horizontal. It then 
begins to shift downwards (@), thus exhibiting a decreasing 
negativity—or increasing positivity—of the internal surface. 
In this periodic variation of the electrical condition we have 
a significant parallel to the records which we have already 
seen in Wepenthe and in Drosera (figs. 203, 205, 208, and 209). 

It has already been pointed out that, in view of the 
functional peculiarities of the digestive organ, it might be ex- 
pected that the alternate reactions of secretion and absorption 
would neither of them be single and spasmodic, but each long- 


RESPONSE OF DIGESTIVE ORGANS 347 


sustained. In this connection it is suggestive that the digestive 
organs should show so strongly marked a characteristic of 
multiple response. It would thus appear, as already said, that 
mechanical stimulation during ingestion of food gives rise to 
the responsive reaction of secretion, evidenced electrically by 
response of galvanometric negativity of the internal surface. 
There then sets in the opposite phase, associated with the 


Fic. 213. Photographic Record of Multiple Responses in Stomach of 
Frog to a Single Strong Thermal Shock 


Four parts are given of this long series. (a) Negative series; (4) Alter- 
nating negative and positive constituent responses ; (c) Positive series ; 
(Z) Positive series. Note trend of base-line upwards in negative a, 
and downwards in positive d. 


reversal of electrical response, probably indicating the ab- 
sorptive process. This reversal of response, to galvanometric 
positivity, may be the work of three different factors, which 
may or may not be mutually dependent. In the first place, 
we have seen that long-continued stimulation was apt of 
itself, other things being equal, to give rise to a reversal of 
response. Secondly, after secretion has reached its maxi- 
mum, the empty mucous cells in contact with fluid would 
naturally tend to reabsorb. And, lastly, we have seen that 
an increase of internal energy, in whatever way produced, 


348 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


tends to give rise to a responsive reaction, whose sign is 
opposite to that of excitation—expansion instead of con- 
traction. Now such an increase of internal energy could not 
fail to be the result of the absorption of the chemically- 
dissolved food. 

Another interesting consideration to be remembered in 
connection with digestive organs is that periodically-acting 


forces give rise to an induced periodicity, which persists for | 


a time, even in the absence of the periodically-exciting cause. 
A well-known illustration of this is met with in the nycti- 
tropic movements, so-called, of plants, induced as these are 
by the periodic variation of night and day. These move- 
ments persist for a certain length of time, even when the 
plant is kept in continuous darkness. Similarly, animals 
accustomed to the supply of food at regular intervals would 
undoubtedly exhibit alternating phasic changes apparently 


spontaneous, in the condition of the digestive organ in 


consequence of the original periodicity of the exciting cause. 
Such an organ, therefore, must necessarily exhibit periodic 
electrical variations. 


OO a 


~~. fT) ce ee ee 


CHAPTER XXV 


ABSORPTION OF FOOD BY PLANT AND ASCENT OF SAP 


Parallelism between responsive reactions of root and digestive organ— Alternating 
phases of secretion and absorption—Association of absorptive process with 
ascent of sap—Electrical response of young and old roots—Different phasic 
reactions, as in pitcher of Mepenthe—Response to chemical stimulation— 
Different theories of ascent of sap—Physical versus excitatory theories— 
Objections to excitatory theory—Assumption that wood dead unjustified— 
Demonstration of excitatory electrical response of sap-wood—Strasburger’s 


experiments on effect of poisons on ascent of sap—Current inference unjus- 
tified. : 


WE have seen in the last chapter that in the digestive pro- 
cess as a whole there must be alternating phases of secretion 
and absorption. The secretion of dissolving fluids, by which 
insoluble substances are rendered soluble, we found to take 
place under stimulation, and to be succeeded by a process 
of absorption, by means of which the now dissolved food- 
material found access into the organism. These functions, 
though seen characteristically in the digestive organs of 
animals, are also to be observed in some plants, such as the 
pitcher of Nepenthe, or the leaf of Drosera. Here, situated 
externally, we find what are practically open stomachs, 
digesting, as do those of animals, solid organic food. But 
plants in general have to depend on the supply of inorganic 
food-material, often presented in solid or insoluble forms, for 
their nourishment. In this case also it is obvious that the 
same sequence of solution by dissolving fluids, and subse- 
quent absorption, must be gone through. And the organ by 
which this takes place must evidently be the root. In this 
regard the well-known experiments on the corrosion of 
marble by the root of a growing plant are sufficient to show 


350 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


that these organs secrete acids, by means of which insoluble 
substances are made soluble. It is equally clear, further, that 
the inorganic solids so dissolved are afterwards absorbed by 
the plant. Thus it will be seen that these alternating pro- 
cesses of secretion and absorption of food-material, as they 
take place in the vegetable organism, are not very different 
in their essential features from the ordinary phenomenon of 
digestion as known to us. The chief distinction between the 
two would now seem to lie in the fact that in the animal the 
supply of food is in the main organic, and in the plant inor- 
ganic. Even here, however, we meet with connecting links 
in the form of insectivorous plants, in whose case the organic 
supply is obtained by means of the digesting leaf, and the 
inorganic through the roots. We may regard digestion, 
therefore, in its widest sense, as a process of absorption of 
insoluble food rendered soluble, whether such food be organic 
or inorganic. Apparently, then, in the case of the plant the 
root functions as a digestive organ. But whether or not this 
analogy is merely superficial can only be determined by an 
experimental inquiry into the parallelism which may or may 
not exist between the various excitatory reactions of the 
root on the one hand and a typical digestive organ on the 
other. 

In order to obtain the large quantity of inorganic material 
which is necessary to the nutrition of a tree, for instance, it 
is clear that fresh quantities of charged fluid must be con- 
stantly taken up. In order further that this process may be 
maintained continuously it must be possible to get rid of the 
useless water, which is accordingly passed off, chiefly from the 
transpiring leaves, in the form of vapour. The absorption of 
food and the ascent of.sap, or transpiration-current, would 
appear therefore to be related phenomena. I shall, in the 
course of the present and following chapters, then, take up in 
detail the consideration of these two aspects of the problem, 
which will thus constitute two main lines of inquiry: 

(1) Whether or not the excitatory reaction of the root has 
any similarity to that of digestive organs in general; and 


ABSORPTION OF FOOD BY PLANT 351 


(2) whether or not the ascent of sap is fundamentally due to 
similar excitatory reactions. 

With regard to the latter of these questions it may be 
stated here that the nature of the efficient cause of the 
ascent of sap is universally regarded, in plant physiology, 
as constituting a problem of the greatest obscurity. The 
various non-physiological theories which have hitherto been 
advanced are admitted to be inadequate, as we shall see later. 
We are thus confronted either with an insoluble problem or 
with the necessity of finding physiological reactions which 
will account for the ascent of sap. 

As regards the latter of these alternatives, haurcaes. ob- 
jections apparently very serious have been brought forward. 
Against the physiological character of the action it has 
been urged (a) that wood, being supposed to be dead, 
could take no part in the ascent. of sap. It is known 
moreover (4) that killing the roots with boiling water does 
not prevent the ascent of sap. And, lastly (c), in the well- 
known experiments of Strasburger it was found that strongly 
poisonous solutions can be carried to the tops of trees. From 
these facts it has been held to be proved that the ascent of 
sap cannot be dependent on the livingness of the tissues 
concerned. ~* . 

But if, on the other. hand, it could be shown that these 
objections were not valid, and if, further, some crucial experi- 
ment were devised to demonstrate that excitatory action was 
attended by a concomitant responsive movement of water in 
the tissue, it might then be claimed that the physiological 
theory of the ascent of sap had been established on a firm 
basis. The attempt to do this will form the subject of the 
next chapter. 

The first question that falls within the scope of our 
investigation, then, is as to whether the reactions of the root 
are or are not similar to those of digestive organs in general. 
We have seen, in the case of the latter, that as there are two 
opposite activities, of secretion and absorption, so also there 
are two opposite responsive phases, negative and positive, the 


352 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


former being the more characteristic of the fresh condition, 
and the latter of a specimen which has been previously sub- 
jected to continuous stimulation. Before giving any account, 
however, of these electrical responses, it will be interesting 
to demonstrate here the occurrence of secretion in young or 
fresh specimens, when subjected to excitation. . The fact 
that young rootlets secrete, on excitation by contact, has 


already been seen in the well-known experiment on the 


corrosion of marble, mentioned above. But I shall now 
describe a new experiment, in which this fact is even more 
convincingly demonstrated. I took a specimen of Colocasza, 
srowing in marshy soil. The plant was lifted bodily, with 
earth adhering, and placed in water, so as to expose the 
roots gradually, without causing injury. It was then kept 
overnight, with the roots in normal saline solution, which 
was slowly absorbed by the tissues. Next morning, again, 
it was carefully washed till there was no trace of salt ad- 
hering. One of the very young roots was now immersed 
in very dilute solution of silver nitrate. If the previous 
washing haa oeen effective, there ought now to be no white 
precipitate, or only the merest trace, formed in the silver 
solution. The two electrodes of a Ruhmkorff’s coil were 
next connected, one with the silver solution,*and the other 
with the stem of the plant. On now passing tetanising 
shocks, the immersed root became excited, and secreted its 
contained salt solution, this being seen in the silver nitrate as 
streams of white precipitate. 

Turning next to the electrical mode of investigation, we 
have found that in the digestive organs, the galvanometric 
negativity, which is the characteristic response of a specimen 
in the fresh condition, becomes reversed to positivity under 
continuous stimulation. In the case of Mepenthe, very young 
pitchers exhibited this normal response of negativity, which 
was converted, under continuous stimulation, into diphasic, 
tending towards positivity. Older specimens, again, pre- 
viously stimulated by the presence of excitatory food-material, 


Diane tN He line odes Py 


ABSORPTION OF FOOD BY PLANT 353 


were found to be in the positive phase, giving rise to response 
by galvanometric positivity. 3 | 
In the case of the root, it is interesting to find that 
there is a similar alternation of responsive phases. For 
this demonstration I again took the root of Colocasia, and 
recorded its responses to equi-alternating electric shocks. 
Among the mass of roots there are naturally some which 
are dead and decaying. One of these was selected for one 
electrical contact, while the other was made with a young 
and vigorous root. The responsive reaction, under these 
conditions, was found to take place 
by galvanometric negativity (fig. 214). 
Under long-continued stimulation, 
however, I have often found this 
normal response by galvanometric 
negativity to be reversed to its 
opposite, positivity. From this we 
may pass to the consideration of 
response in older roots, where the 
phasic reaction is typically positive. a ee ae 
Now we have seen in previous Record of Normal Nega- 
chapters, as will be remembered, that ns Response..of Young 
oot of Colocasia 
there are two different conditions 
under which the positive may be substituted for the normal 
negative response. The first is that of reversal under long- 
continued stimulation, which we ,have just seen. And the 
second occurs when the stimulus falls below the critical 
level which is necessary to the evoking of true excitation. 
In this latter case, as we saw further, the incident stimulus 
increases the internal energy, and causes expansion, positive 
turgidity-variation, and galvanometric positivity. It would 
thus appear that one identical stimulus may induce one effect, 
that of galvanometric negativity, in a highly excitable tissue, 
and the opposite, or galvanometric positivity, in a tissue that 
is less excitable. In connection with this question the ex- 
perimental results which I am about to describe are very 
significant. 


AA 


354 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO=PHYSIOLOGY 


We have seen that a young root of Colocasia, when fresh, 
gives the normal response of. galvanometric negativity. 
Taking next an older root of the same plant, and employ- 
ing the same intensity of stimulus as before, I found the 
responses to take place, generally speaking, by galvanometric 
positivity (fig. 215). This would appear to suggest a ten- 
dency towards specialisation of fufction, galvanometric nega- 
tivity being associated, as we have seen, with secretion, and | 
positivity, in all probability, with the opposite—namely, 
absorption. A similar specialisation of certain cells for 
secretion and others for absorption is manifested more 
unmistakably in the digestive organs 
of the higher animals. | 

Thus in the young roots the pre- 
dominant reaction would seem to be 
secretion, reversed under continuous 
stimulation to absorption. In the older 
roots, on the other hand, the pre- 
dominant reaction must be supposed 
to be absorptive. Here, then, judging 
from the electrical indications, we 
Fic. 218. Photographic would seem to have proof of that 

Pe One Rea physiological activity in virtue of which 

Colocasia water is taken up by the root, thus 

! giving rise to the so-called ‘root- 
pressure. We can also see how, by the summated activities 
of numerous roots, this ‘ root-pressure ’ is kept approximately 
constant for a certain length of time. Taking longer periods 
into account, further, we can see that this physiological 
activity is likely to undergo periodic change, a fact which is 
evidenced by the known periodic variation of root-pressure. 
The question, however, of the actual influence of excitation 
on the process of the ascent of sap will be dealt with in the 
next chapter. 

One form of stimulus to whose action the roots must often 
be subjected is that of the chemical substances present in 
the soil, and I undertook to test the electrical variations 


--ABSORPTION OF FOOD BY PLANT | 355 


induced by these. The results obtained, at least with the 
specimens which I have tried, are in general parallel to those 
obtained by the electrical form of stimulation. Thus, in 
young roots, in the majority of cases, when subjected to the 
action of so dilute a solution as ‘5 per cent. of sodium car- 
bonate, an electrical change of galvanometric negativity was 
induced. The continued action of this solution, however, 
tended to induce a reversal to galvanometric positivity. But 
the reactions of older roots were different—that is to say, in 
the greater number of the latter cases, a solution of °5 per 
cent. induced galvanometric positivity ; and it required a 
much stronger solution, of from 5 to 10 per cent., to bring 
about the reaction of galvanometric negativity. 

We have thus seen that in the root, as in the digestive 
organ, there are alternating phases of secretion and absorp- 
tion, and that it is by means of the secreted fluid that solid 
inorganic substances are rendered soluble, for subsequent 
absorption as food. We have seen moreover that the elec- 
trical reactions in the two cases are similar; that in the 
young root, as in the young glandular organ of Wepenthe, the 
characteristic response is by galvanometric negativity ; and 
that long-continued stimulation induces diphasic variation, 
with a tendency towards the reversal to positive. We saw 
further that older roots, like the glands in the older pitchers 
of epenthe, have a phasic reaction which is predominantly 
positive. And now, having thus completed our first line of 
inquiry, we shall turn to the second---the question, namely, 
as to whether the ascent of sap is or is not essentially due to 
physiological reaction. 

The possible explanations of the ascent of sap may be 
grouped broadly under two different heads, as either physical 
or physiological. Under the former of these must be named 
such theories as those of atmospheric pressure, capillarity, 
osmosis, and evaporation from leaves. Under the latter, 
the physiological, the movement of water is regarded as 
mainly due, in some hitherto undefined way, to excitatory 
actions by which the sap is propelled in a uni-directioned 


AA2 


356 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


manner, this primary movement being aided by accessory 
factors. 

Afnong the physical theories which have been pro- 
pounded for the explanation of the ascent of sap those of 
atmospheric pressure and of capillarity are admitted to be 
inadequate. But that of osmosis and transpiration, put 
forward by Dixon, Joly, and Askenasy, is of much greater 


weight. According to this, the ascent is brought about by — 


transpiration from leaves. The fluid in the mesophyll cells 
of the leaves becomes concentrated by evaporation; thus 
osmotic attraction is set up by the leaves, and the suction 
thereby exerted is supposed to be transmitted backwards as 
far as the roots, through cohering columns of water. The 
difficulties in the way of this theory lie (1) in explaining how 
a slow osmotic action could produce so rapid a water-current ; 
and (2), in the absence of any conclusive proof that, under 
actual conditions within the plant, the water-column could 
have sufficient tensile strength. Even apart from these 
objections, however, the fact remains that energetic water- 
movements take place in the plant in the entire absence of 


transpiration. For example, sap exudes from the cut end of 


a tree which may exert a pressure as great as that of a 
column of liquid 13 metres in height. 

It is thus seen that there is an independent activity o: 
some kind which maintains the movement of water through 
the plant. That this activity, moreover, is not resident in the 
root merely is seen from the fact that exudation of water 
takes place from the tips of grass-blades when their cut stems 
have been placed in water. Criticising the theory of trans- 
piration, Strasburger rightly remarks that transpiration only 
makes a place for inflowing water, but cannot furnish the 
force necessary to convey a large volume of fluid rapidly for 
a considerable distance through wood. From the considera- 
tion of these and other facts, Pfeffer, in his summary, was 
led to the conclusion which he states as follows: ‘A satis- 
factory explanation of the means by which the transpiration- 
current is maintained has not yet been brought forward. If 


a ae 


ET: he gece 


wb Sc 5 Whe) So en 


ABSORPTION OF FOOD BY PLANT bi? ae 


no vital actions take part in it, then it is obvious that we 
have only an incomplete knowledge of the causes at work 
and of the relationship of the different factors concerned.’ * 

The inadequacy of these theories to explain the ascent 
of sap has, then, been freely admitted. With special refer- 
ence, further, to that of osmosis, I shall myself be able to 
show that the movement of water often takes place in the 
plant, in a direction contrary to what it would be if osmosis 
alone were involved. The fact that the absorption of water 
is not a merely passive process, but a phenomenon connected 
with irritability, will be further shown in the depression of 
the rate of water-movement by such conditions as depress 
irritability, whereas the opposite circumstance will be found 
to enhance it. : 

We are thus driven to examine the possibility of a 
physiological explanation of the ascent of sap. On such 
a theory it must be supposed to be brought about by the 
action of stimulus, inducing reactions expressed in the re- 
sponsive movement of water. The objections made to the 
physiological explanation have already been recapitulated. 
They are (1) that the movement of water is known to take 
place rapidly, and by preference, through woody tissues 
which are supposed to be dead; and (2) that when the roots 
have been killed by hot water, or when poison is supplied, 
the transport of sap continues to take place. I shall now 
proceed to examine these arguments, and to show that the 
objections raised, though apparently so strong, are not really 
valid. 

We shall first refer to the argument which has been 
based upon the fact that in trees conduction takes place 
very rapidly through woody portions which are regarded as 
dead. It is not, it must be noted, implied by this that 
the presence of wood is essential to the ascent of sap, in- 
asmuch as even in trees there are tracts of living cortical 
tissue in the roots which have to be traversed before the 
water can reach the woody tissues. In seedlings of Gramine 

’ Pfeffer, Physiology of Plants (English translation), 1903, p. 224. | 


358 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


only one or two days old, again, water ascends, and is ex- 
creted at the tip of the yet unopened leaf. Transpiration 
is here at its minimum, and the fibro-vascular elements at 
this early stage cannot be regarded as ‘dead wood.’ Finally, 
in herbaceous plants, where woody elements are ee Ane 
the ascent of sap is seen to take place. 

Assuming, for the sake of argument, that in the case of 


the tree, the mass of wood in the interior were dead, it might. 


still conceivably be of use in the irrigating system as a 
central reservoir. This would certainly be advantageous to 
rapidity of transit. At the lower end of the tree, the wood 
abuts upon the delicate parenchymatous tissues of the root, 
and at the upper upon those of the leaves. According to 
the physiological theory, then, it might be supposed that it 
was by the multiple activity of the cells of the root that 
water was pumped into the wood ; and that at the other end 
the central reservoir was able to furnish a supply to make 
up for the constant loss by transpiration. Laterally also, in 
the stem itself the cortical tissues could draw upon this 
central supply. Under such an arrangement no part of the 
_ plant could be very far away from the reservoir. 

As a matter of fact, this sketch corresponds roughly to 
the working of the tree as an hydraulic machine. The 
system is, however, somewhat more complex than has been 
indicated. Besides the central, we have also to remember 
the presence of lateral reservoirs, in the parenchymatous 
tissues of the cortex. But the transport of water through 
these is not, of course, so rapid as through the central, more 
specifically conducting, system. In the case of herbaceous 
plants, where the quantity of wood is insignificant, we may 
regard the central channels as abolished. Here we have 
soft cortical tissues extending continuously from root to 
leaves through the stem, and it is obviously through these 
that the ascent of water takes place. In woody trees, then, 
there is no reason to suppose that the cortical tissues could 
not play a similar part in the conveyance of water. The 
difference is, that in this case there is also an added and 


F ried ¢ ral ” al 
ae Bee De ee ee ee tr Vee oe 


Ses | le 


ABSORPTION OF FOOD BY PLANT 359 


better channel available, which will naturaily come into 
requisition where quick transit is required. 

In a woody trunk, then, we have (1) the outer cortical 
cylinder of water-conducting tissues, by which the ascent of 
sap takes place slowly. We have (2) the highly-conducting 
central woody tissue, which not only allows of water ascend- 
ing rapidly through it, but is also (3) in lateral communi- 
cation with the outer cylinder. The hydraulic system thus 
consists of a large central canal, as it were, connected with 
innumerable lateral reservoirs, which are the cells of the 
cortex. When a demand arises for rapidity of water-supply 
on account of transpiration, we can now see that no less than 
three different factors are brought into requisition. First 
there is the rapid upward transit through the wood ; 
secondly, the slow ascent through the cortex ; and thirdly, 
the lateral supply from the cortex by way of the nearest 
wood. : 

As regards the last of these, the cortical tissues in contact 
with the wood act ina manner not very unlike that of the 
roots towards the soil. That is to say, under different cir- 
cumstances, they absorb water from it, and excrete water 
into it, these alternating processes being by no means 
accidental, but guided by appropriate excitatory reactions. 

Turning our attention for a moment to the movements 
of Mimosa \eaf, we find that on excitation the expelled water 
makes its way to the fibro-vascular tissue. There is here, in 
the excitable tissue, unlike the case of secretory organs, no 
external vent, and we see the necessity of a central reservoir 
to which water excitatorily expelled may find access. On 
the subsidence of excitation, the water is re-absorbed by the 
organ, causing expansion and re-erection of the leaf. Such 
movements of inflow and outflow evidently take place in the 
trunk of the tree itself. Under the stimulus of sunlight, 
the excited cortical tissue will squeeze water inwards into the 
central reservoir. If this takes place, the effect will be seen 
in a diametric contraction. At the time when transpiration 
is most rapid, under the action of sunlight, there is thus 


360 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


besides the water coming from the roots, an additional 
supply available from the lateral reservoirs. The loss of 
water thus sustained by the cortex during the day is made 
up again at night, when it will suck water outwards from the 
central reservoir. We have here a case analogous to the 
action of the excitatory tissue of the pulvinus of J/imosa 


expelling water into the wood on excitation, and re- 
absorbing it on the cessation of excitation. The occurrence | 


of these reactions in the cortex explains the observation 
made by Kraus that the organs of the plant diminish in bulk 
from morning to afternoon, the reverse seo taking place 
from afternoon to morning. 

We have thus seen how important a factor is excitatory 
reaction in the observed movements of water, even on the 
supposition that the woody tissue, being dead, is a merely 
passive agent. The question has still to be attacked, 
however, whether this assumption, so generally made, is 
correct, that the wood used for conduction of water is dead. 
This supposition has arisen from the chemical transformation 
undergone by the protoplasm in woody vessels. We have 
seen, however, in the case of the epidermal cells of the skin, 
that it is possible for chemical transformation to occur, 
without necessarily being accompanied by the death-change. 

Before proceeding to inquire whether the conducting 
woody channels are really dead, it is desirable to say a few 
words as to the particular tissues in the wood, which are 
most effective for this purpose. Many experiments have 
been carried out to determine this. Among other things 
various staining fluids have been employed. But an objec- 
tion raised in the case of some of these has been that the 
water of such solutions travels faster than the dye dissolved 
in it. For my own part, I have found the employment of 
dilute solution of phenolpthaline to be exceedingly delicate 
and useful for the purpose of this investigation. It is 
perfectly colourless, and the staining appears only after 
appropriate development. ‘The cut end of the stem is placed 
in this dilute solution and left for some time. Transverse 


<7. ae 


i. 


~ " - = » 
PRAT SO te bee 


“* eee eee ee ee ee ee ee ee lr Cll 


ABSORPTION OF FOOD BY PLANT 361 


sections of the stem at different heights are then made and 
placed in the field of a microscope. There is now nothing 
distinguishing to be seen, as the solution sucked up by the 
stem was colourless. Dilute solution of potash, however, will 
act on this as a developer. The particular tissues, therefore, 
through which the solution has been conducted, on now 
being subjected to the action of this agent, become of a rich 
crimson colour. By this means it is easy to see, as already 
determined by various observers, that it is the younger, or 
‘sap-wood, which is concerned in the work of the rapid 
conduction of water, the older, or ‘heart-wood,’ being 
ineffective for this purpose. If, however, the wood taking 
part in the ascent of sap had been dead, and acted as a 
passive agent merely, it is difficult to understand the reason 
of this selective action of the younger, and presumably more 
living, woody tissues. | 

It occurred to me, finally, that as electrical response is an 
indubitable concomitant of the excitatory reaction of living 
tissues, the question as to whether sap-wood was alive or 
dead could be subjected to a decisive test. For this purpose 
I took various strips of sap-wood from different woody plants. 
The cortical tissue was in each case carefully removed, and 
the specimens were placed in water, allowing them a period 
of rest. The first experiment was to observe whether local 
stimulation by the Rotary Mechanical Stimulator did or did 
not evoke electrical response. I found from this, that mecha- 
nical excitation of the sap-wood induced considerable excita- 
tory response of galvanometric negativity. I then subjected 
the same tissue to the action of boiling water for: a length of 
time, and again tested its electrical reaction by the same 
method. The wood was found to be very resistant to the 
action of heat, and it was only after long immersion that 
the responses were entirely abolished. Drying was in fact 
found, significantly enough, to be an easier method than the 
application of heat, to kill, and therefore to. abolish the 
responsiveness of, the wood. If the wood be first dried, and 
then soaked in water, it entirely ceases to manifest electrical 


362 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


response. The ordinary wood of commerce exhibits no 
response. The familiar fact that the cut end of a woody 
stem, when not placed in water immediately, ceases to 
suck up water, has been supposed to be due solely to 
the intervention of air-bubbles. From the experiment which 
I have described, however, it would appear that the death 
of the exposed tissue by drying must be included here as a 
factor in this abolition of suction. 


”_ 


With sap-wood I was 
also able to obtain the in- 
dication of galvanometric 
negativity in response to 
thermal stimulation. Hot 
platinum wire was applied 
at a distance of 5 mm. 
from the proximal contact. 
Response was thus due to 
the transmitted effect of 
excitation. 

I was next desirous 
to obtain photographic 

records of the normal 

Fic. 216. Photographic Record of +s 

Electrical Response of Sap-wood ied Sells of living wood 
The normal negative responses seen in the and its variations under 

| first series are depressed after application chemical agents. For this 

of chloroform in the second. 

purpose I employed both 
the electrical and vibrational modes of stimulation. For the 
first of these, the strip of sap-wood was cut in the form 
of a two-pronged fork, of which one prong was killed by 
exposure to the drying influence of the air, while the other 
was kept alive by immersion in water. The specimen was 
now placed in water as a whole, in order to moisten the dried 
half. After this, electrical connections were made in the 
usual manner with the killed and unkilled ends of the speci- 
men. On next subjecting it to equi-alternating shocks’ 
response was obtained as induced galvanometric negativity 
of the living prong. These responses are seen in fig. 216 in 
the first series of records to the left. Chloroform was next 


ABSORPTION OF FOOD BY PLANT 363 


applied, and we observe the consequent depression of re- 
sponse; when the chloroform was blown off the responses 
were found to undergo revival. 

In the next experiment, a specimen of living wood was 
mounted in the vibrational apparatus, and its normal re- 
sponses taken. I next applied copper sulphate, and the 
record shows the consequent abolition of response (fig. 217). 
I have thus been able to establish the fact that the woody 
vessels of the sap-wood are not dead but living, and hence 
fully susceptible of physiological reaction. This will, I think, 
be found to dispose of one of 
the difficulties raised in regard 
to the physiological theory of 
the ascent of sap. 

We come, secondly, to the 
objections that have been based 
on the ground of the ascent of 
sap through a tree whose roots 
have been killed by boiling 


Fic. 217. Photographic Record 


water, and, further, on the ex- showing Normal Responses ot 
: * Living Wood to Vibrational 
periments of Hartig and Stras- Stimulus, and the Abolition of 


burger. These observers set cut Response by a Toxic Dose of 
ends of trees in tubs of poisonous Wei eee 

solutions, such as copper sulphate, which were found, in spite 
of their toxic character, to ascend to the leaves. It is clear 
that if such violent protoplasmic poisons ascend the trunk, 
they must kill all the cells lying in their path. And from 
this it was inferred that the living cells in the stem could 
not be necessary to the rise of sap. Strasburger was thus 
led to the conclusion that ‘the supposition that the living 
elements in any way co-operate in the ascent of the 
transpiration current is absolutely precluded.’’ 

It does not, however, appear that this inference on the 
part of Strasburger was justified, for we must remember the 
fact that any cut piece of stem when placed in water is found 
to exhibit suctional activity. Hence the active cells con- 
cerned—if the process is to be regarded as due to such— 


' Strasburger, Zext-dook of Botany (English translation), 1903, p. 188. 


364 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


must be distributed throughout the plant. Death of a given 
zone, then, would arrest the activity of that particular zone 
but not that of another zone higher up. Thus a poisonous 
solution would only abolish activity in those cells which it 
had already reached. The activity of cells above would 
remain unaffected. That the death of cells below offers no 
resistance to the passage of water, when suctional activity is 


maintained above I have demonstrated elsewhere, and shall | 


again show in the course of the next chapter, in cases in 
which the lower part of the plant was killed by boiling 
water. Under the action of poisons, similarly, I have been 
able to show that a poison can: pass easily through killed 
tissues owing to the suctional activity of cells higher up. 
This was demonstrated by means of experiments on Des- 
modium gyrans, where the cut end of the petiole was placed 
in copper sulphate solution. It is fortunate that in this case, 
during the ascent of poison, we have areas whose activity 
is manifested visibly by the rhythmic motile indications of 
the pulvini of the inserted lateral leaflets. That copper 
sulphate solution arrests rhythmic activity, and induces 
death, is seen by the rapid stoppage of pulsation when 
we apply it directly on the pulvini of the pulsating leaflets. 
When it is applied, however, at the cut end of the petiole, the 
arrest of pulsation only takes place after sufficient time has 
elapsed for the poison to ascend through the intervening 
distance. This shows clearly that successive zones are killed 
one after another, and that the death of a point below does 
not stop the suction above. From this experiment it is evi- 
dent that the application of poison, at the root, or the cut end 
of a stem, need not be expected to arrest suction until the 
whole plant has been killed, and from Strasburger’s account of 
his experiment it is evident that the movement of water did 
come to a stop when the poison reached the top of the tree. 

We thus see that the objections which have been raised, 
with regard to the physiological nature of the ascent of sap, 
are not valid. I shall therefore proceed in the next chapter 
to describe crucial experiments in demonstration of the fun- 
damentally excitatory character of this process, 


Aw yi he et Rie aah tent ti ae) 


ih ieee) 


SS ee ee eee ee 


CHAPTER XXVI 
THE EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL, RESPONSE 


Propagation of excitatory wave in plant attended by progressive movement of 
water — Hydraulic response to stimulus—The Shoshungraph—Direct and photo- 
graphic methods of record—Responsive variations of suction under physiological 
modifications induced by various agents—Effects of lowesing and raising of 
temperature—Explanation of maintenance:of suction, when root killed— 
Effect of poison influenced by tonic condition—Effect of anzesthetics on 
suctional response—Excitatory verszs osmotic action—-Stimulation by alter- 
nating induction-shocks—Terminal and sub-terminal modes of application— 
Three modes of obtaining response-records, namely (1) the unbalanced, 
(2) the balanced, (3) the over-balanced—Renewal of suction previously at 
standstill, by action of stimulus—Reponsive enhancement of suction by 
stimulus—After-effect of stimulus —Diminution of latent period as after-effect 

of stimulus—Response under over-balance—Response under sub-terminal 
stimulation —Variation of response under seasonal changes. 


IN the last chapter it was shown that the various objections 
hitherto urged against the excitatory nature of the ascent of 
sap were not justified. In the course of the present chapter, 
therefore, I shall adduce proofs that the water-movement in 
the plant is the result of stimulatory action. Instead of 
vaguely referring the phenomenon to physiological activity, 
moreover, we shall attempt, proceeding from the ‘basis 
of other excitatory reactions, already clearly established, 
first to see whether inferences based on these are capable of 
explaining the present problem, and secondly, to subject 
those inferences themselves to the test of experimental in- 
vestigation. 

We cut a certain length of the stem of M/zmosa, and keep 
it immersed for some time ina very dilute solution of common 
salt, until the tissue has become charged with this. The 
specimen is then taken out, and thoroughly rinsed with clean 
water. It is now held vertically, with the lower end dipped 


366 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


in a highly dilute solution of silver nitrate. A portion of the 
tissue higher up is now excited by contact with a hot wire. 
The excitation thus induced is then found to travel through 
the intervening distance, with a velocity characteristic of the 
conducting power of the tissue. The arrival of the excita- 
tory wave at the lower end is attended by an expulsion of 
the cell-sap containing the salt solution previously absorbed.. 


This expulsion is instantly made visible by the formation of | 


a dense white precipitate of silver chloride. From this 
experiment it is seen that the passage of excitation is at- 
tended by a forward movement of water in the direction of 
propagation. | 

The next point to be realised is that a strong or a long- 
continued stimulus will give rise in the tissue, not to one, 
but to a multiple series of propagated waves. If the tip of 
a leaf of Biophytum be strongly excited, we see successive 
waves of excitation, marked by the serial fall of the motile 
leaflets, proceeding again and again in the centripetal 
direction, from the terminal excited point. If, similarly, the 
end of theroot be excited, by any means, an excitatory move- 
ment of water will be induced, proceeding away from this 
end, in an upward direction. It must be remembered, how- 
ever, that excitation proceeds in all directions from the 
excited point. If then the point of excitation be terminal, it 
is evident that the direction of propagation, being away from 
this, will be upwards. But if the tip of the root be highly ex- 
citable, then, owing to local excitation, there will also be a 
certain amount of secretion into the soil. Even highly 
excitable tissues, however, after continuous stimulation, show 
a tendency, as we have seen, to the reversal of their character. 
istic response. This secretion at the terminal point of the 
root will tend to become changed into absorption. Again, 
looking at the succession of excitatory waves propelling 
water upwards, we can see that these will leave a deficit of 
cell-sap behind, which will further act rather for the absorp- 
tion than for the secretion of fluid. And in addition to 
these, if there be any other directive influences, such as 


a a a a ii i i a i 


Pad 


Sev apie 


EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 367 


evaporation from the leaves, they will tend to help the 
particular uni-directioned flow. We have seen that these 
conclusions are confirmed by the results obtained in the 
electrical response of the roots. We there saw that very 
young roots give at first negative responses, which are after- 
wards reversed to positive, under continuous stimulation. 
Older roots, as we also saw, give response by positivity. We 
saw, further, that there was much reason for regarding 
negative response as associated with the secretion, and 
positive with the absorption of fluid. 

Thus the serial propagation of excitation from cell to 
cell, with the concomitant movement of water, will normally 
be upwards. In this connection it is very significant that the 
younger portion of the fibro-vascular bundle is the preferential 
channel for the conduction, at once of water and of excitation. 
It is thus seen that a one-directioned movement of water 
may be produced by the multiple excitatory activity 
of the tissue. And just as the multiple activity of certain 
tissues, say, for instance, the leaflets of Desmodium, may 
be gauged by their multiple mechanical movements, so 
in the rate of the water-movement we have a means of 
measuring the intensity of the multiple rhythmic activity of 
those which are concerned in the ascent of sap. This would 
be analogous to the measurement of the rhythmic activity of 
the heart, by a determination of the rate of flow of the 
circulating blood. In the case of the plant, however, this rate 
of movement might be measured, either by means of the pro- 
pulsion of water forwards, or by the suction exerted behind. 

In order to demonstrate the fact that the water movement 
in the ascent of sap is mainly dependent on excitatory 
reactions, it is necessary to have at our disposal some means 
of rapidly observing and recording the variations induced by 
physiological changes in the rate of ascent. For this pur- 
pose I was successful in devising the Shoshungraph or 
suction-recorder, described in detail in my book on ‘ Plant 
Response.’ ! : 


' Bose, Plant Response, pp. 364-371. 


368 © ‘COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


-° This instrument consists of (1) an arrangement by which 
the specimen may be subjected rapidly to the action of 
different excitatory or depressing agents ; (2) a potometric 
tube for the measurement of changes of suctional activity, 
under different external conditions ; and (3) a contrivance by 
tmneans of which the movements of the water-index, with 
their time-relations, are recorded. The principal parts of the 
apparatus are seen in fig. 218. V is the plant-vessel, in 
which the specimen is mounted, with or without roots, by 
means of a watertight india-rubber cork. R is the 
reservoir, which may be filled with hot or cold water, or with 
the required chemical solution. By appropriate manipula- 
tion of stopcocks, by means of key K, the water in the plant- 
vessel may be replaced quickly by any of these, and the 
effect of the changed condition on the rate of water-move- 
ment observed. 

A direct record of the rate of movement may be made on 
the revolving drum, actuated by clockwork, as shown in the 
figure. For this purpose, a pen, P, is fitted over the 
potometric tube, by means of a brass collar, which has a 
rectangular opening, kept always coincident with the water- 
index. The collar, carrying the pen, is attached to a thread, 
which passes round small pulleys. One end of this thread 
carries a counterpoise, M, and the other is wound round a 
wheel, W, which can be so manipulated as to make the pen 
follow the movements of the water-column. When the 
water-index is followed in the way described, a direct record 
of the water-movement in the plant is obtained. A curve 
is thus traced, the ordinate of which represents the quantity 
of water sucked up, and the abscissa the time. The slope of 
the curve thus gives the rate of movement. As long as 
suction is uniform, this slope remains constant. If, however, 
any exciting agent increases the rate of suction, there is an 
immediate flexure in the curve, which thus becomes steeper. 
A depressing agent lessens the slope, and when suction is 
abolished, the record becomes horizontal. For the detection 
of the slightest variation in the rate of suction, the Method of 


Se a 


a 


EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 369 


Balance is employed, which depends upon an application of 
compensation, by which, under normal conditions, the water- 
index is kept stationary, though the suctional movement in 


Fic, 218. The Shoshungraph 


V, plant-vessel ; R, reservoir ; C, compensator, whose balancing height is 
adjusted by rack and pinion, s; kK, key for manipulation of four-way 


stop-cock ; P, recording pen, with counterpoise M, manipulated by 
wheel, w. The drumjis rotated by the clock at uniform speed. 


BB 


370 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


the plant is in no way disturbed. The balance is obtained 
by allowing water to enter the plant-vessel, from the com- 
pensator, C, at a rate exactly equal to that of its withdrawal 
by suction. Thus, under a condition of balance, the record 
‘becomes horizontal. An exciting agent now produces an 
inclination of the record upwards, or a depressing agent a 


declination downwards. This was the instrument employed. 
by me for the obtaining of records in a simple manner of. 


‘suctional response and its variations, under different physio- 
logical modifications. A fuller account of the method and 
the results obtained by it will be found in my book on 
‘Plant Response.’ As the subject of the ascent of sap is, how- 
ever, of extreme importance, I thought it desirable to see to 
what extent the sensitiveness of this instrument could be 
raised, and also to devise means, in connection with it, for the 
automatic record of results obtained. For the latter purpose 
I employed photography. As the water-index, whose ex- 
cursions in the potometric tube are to be recorded, is trans- 
parent, it is necessary to provide an additional opaque index, 
which shall move in and out with it. This consists of a 
short length of mercury, lying in contact with the end of the 
“water-column. The potometer tube is placed in the field of 
a magic lantern, and the index is focussed on a moving 
photographic plate by means of an objective. The sensitive- 
ness of this method of record may be increased in two ways. 
First, the bore of the capillary tube may be made finer and 
finer. But this cannot be carried to an extreme, as the 
capillary offers great resistance to the free movement of the 
index. Secondly, the sensitiveness may be increased to any 
extent by the employment of a highly-magnifying and 
short-focus objective. By the combination of both these 
devices, we are able in practice to arrive at an extraordinary 
degree of sensitiveness, qualifying us to attack some of the 
most difficult problems with the greatest ease. For our 
present purpose, however, it is not by any means necessary 
to approach the limit of this sensitiveness. The photo- 
graphic records given in the course of the present chapter 


a a 


EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 37I 


were obtained with a tube having a bore I square mm. in 
section, and employing an ordinary magic lantern objective, 
which casts an image without any magnification. The 
Method of Balance again, as we have seen, affords us another 
opportunity of arriving at an experimental adjustment of 
creat sensitiveness. 

The experimental delicacy obtained by high tiouihen 
tion can only be used to the greatest advantage when 
coupled with the Method of Balance, if we have means at 
our disposal for securing the utmost possible perfection of 
the balance. We have seen that the balance is adjusted 
when the rate at which water is removed from the plant- 
vessel by suction is exactly equal to the rate of its inflow 
from the compensating vessel, Cc. This compensation is 
roughly effected by the rack and pinion, Ss, which serves to 
regulate the flow by raising or lowering the compensating 
vessel (fig. 218). For the purpose of the final adjustment 
the narrow bore of the thick india-rubber tubing, which con- 
nects the compensator with the plant-vessel, is capable of 
gradual constriction. This must be accomplished by equal 
compression on all sides, as bilateral compression alone 
would act to induce a discontinuous closure and sudden 
arrest of flow. If the compressing arrangement be something 
after the model of the iris-diaphragm, then the bore, which 
regulates the flow, may be constricted gradually and con- 
tinuously. With such an arrangement it is easy to arrive at 
a balance so perfect that the index appears to be quite 
stationary. Under-balance would now make it move, say to 
the right ; and over-balance to the left. 

We shall now proceed to show that those agents shih 
exalt physiological activity also act to enhance suction; and 
that those which induce physiological depression will also 
depress suction. One of those which enhance the multiple 
activity of the tissue is, as we know, the rise of temperature ; 
whereas cooling, or lowering of temperature, tends to depress 
it, even to the extent of abolition. Thus an automatically 
vibrating leaflet of Desmodium has its vibration-frequency 

BB2 


372 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


enhanced by a rise, and depressed or arrested by a fall, of 
temperature. A similar effect is seen to occur in the suctional 
response of plants. Thus, in a given specimen of Cvoéon, 
application of cold water at 4° C. to the root was found to 
arrest the suction in-the course of 8 minutes. This arrest 
by cold was not permanent, for the normal rate of suction re- 
appeared on the return of the water to a normal temperature. 
On applying water of raised temperature to the root, on the 
other hand, the rate of suction was immediately found to 
be enhanced. This is illustrated in the following record 
(fig. 219), in which the normal rate of suction at 23° C. was 
7 cubic mm. in volume, or 7 mg. 
in weight per minute. The ap- 
plication of water at 35° C. now 
induced a steep rise of the curve, 
indicating an enhanced rate of 
suction of 58 mg. per minute, or 
more than eight times the rate at 
23° C. On now once more sub- 
stituting water at 23° C., the rate 
is seen to become lowered, but 
not to fall so low as at the 
ere ttn at oso C, factesed beginning. It now fell from 58 
Suction at 35° C., and the to 14 mg. per minute, instead of 
io ideal Pee Ketur returning to the original 8 mg. 
This is due to the fact that 
the internal energy of the tissue has been raised in the 
meantime by the absorption of warm water, the enhancement 
of the normal rate being a persistent after-effect of this. 

We shall next take up the apparently anomalous case, 
in which, when the root has been killed by pouring boiling 
water over it, the suction of the plant is nevertheless main- 
tained. In such an experiment the normal record was first 
taken, and boiling water was then passed through the plant- 
vessel continuously for some time till the roots were killed. 
On allowing the water in the vessel to return to the tempera- 
ture of the room, it was found that suction was taking place 


EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 373 


at an even greatcr rate than ordinary. This result would 
at first appear to show that protoplasmic activity had 
nothing to do with the ascent of sap, and the objection 
would have been fatal if the activity which produces suction 
had been confined to the roots alone. But in reality, as we 
have already seen, such activity is present, to a greater or 
less extent, throughout every zone of the plant. The only 
part which is killed, however, in the experiment - just 
described, is that which is actually immersed in, or in 
immediate contiguity with, the boiling water; and the 
unkilled tissues above continue their suctional activity 
unabated. The increase in the rate of suction is to be ex- 
plained by the fact that the entrance of water, instead of 
being effected through the extremely attenuated channels 
of the root-hairs, now takes place through the whole mass of 
the root, acting virtually as a wet rag tied round the base 
of the living stem. The mass of water which it is thus 
possible to suck up directly through the broad-sectioned 
stem is evidently much greater than could have been taken 
in through the resistant, organically-conducting channels of 
the rootlets. | 
The fact thus demonstrated, that the local death of a 
given zone does not fer se arrest the suctional activity of the 
tissues above it, explains why a poison may be carried to the 
top of atree. It is evident that only when it has thus been 
conveyed, and when all the tissues have thus been killed, 
could a permanent arrest take place. And Strasburger him- 
self admits that arrest under these conditions does occur. 
With reference to the effect of poison, again, it is im- 
portant to bear in mind that its toxic effect depends, toa 
certain extent, on the tonic condition of the tissue. Thus a 
Desmodium \eaflet which was moderately vigorous had its 
pulsatory movement arrested soon after the local application 
of the copper sulphate solution. In more vigorous leaflets, 
however, the arrest did not take place till after a considerable 
length of time. Indeed, in some cases, after a preliminary 
arrest, the leaflet is able to shake off the effects of the poison 


374 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


absorbed, by some process of accommodation. I have shown 
elsewhere! that the effect of poison on the response of 
growth is modified to a remarkable extent by the different 
tonic conditions of the tissue. The experiments in question 
were carried out on similar specimens of Crinum lily, in 
which the only difference induced depended on the fact that 
one set had been kept at a temperature of 30° C., and were 


thus in moderate tonic condition, while the others had been. 


maintained at 34° C., bringing about, as I have shown, an 
optimum tonic condition. The application of a 5 per cent. 
solution of copper sulphate to one of the first of these was 
found to induce the rapid decline and final arrest of growth, 
while a similar application, on a specimen in the optimum 
condition of the second set, induced a preliminary exaltation 
followed by a slow depression and ultimate arrest of growth, 
the last-named, however, being reached only after the lapse 
ofa considerable time. 

The difference of effect under different tonic conditions 
was still more strikingly exhibited by the application of a 
smaller dose—namely, of a I percent. solution. This was 
found to induce a depression of growth, which was ultimately 
fatal to the plant, in the case of specimens kept at 30° C. 
But when the same dose was applied to a plant which had 
been kept at 34° C, the effect was seen in a marked 
exaltation of the rate, for a fairly long time, after which it 
shook off the effect of poison altogether, resuming its normal 
rate of growth. 

Thus the effect of poison on the various activities of the 
plant is seen to depend not only on the amount of the 
agent, and the duration of application, but also on the 
tonic condition of the tissue. Strong and prolonged applica- 
tions will abolish all active processes, by inducing the death 
of the plant. In accordance with this, I find that in certain 


plants under the action of copper sulphate the arrest of 


suction is more rapid than in others. All alike, however, 
exhibit permanent arrest sooner or later. 
1 Bose, Plant Response, pp. 487-488. 


~— ST ae |, ae 


wethcept 


—" 
EN 


EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 375 


The question of the excitatory nature of the ascent of 
sap may again. be tested by the application of anzsthetic 
agents, such as solution of ether. The record is first taken 
of the actual rate of suction, and then by quick manipulation 
_of the double key, the water in the plant-vessel down to the 
base of the specimen is replaced by ether solution. The 
original rate of suction had in a particular case been 40 
cubic mm. per minute. After 
the application, however, this 
became depressed, and in the 
course of four minutes under- 
went a preliminary arrest. 
This short arrest was  suc- 
ceeded by reversal, or expul- 
sion, which lasted for fourteen 
minutes, and then gave place 
to what was practically per- 
manent arrest (fig. 220). 

I have occasionally ob- 
served an interesting variation 
in the arrest of suction induced 
by ether. Shortly after the 
application just described an 
arrest of suction is induced. 
This, however, is only pre- 
liminary, suction after an 
interval being renewed at a_ Fic. 220. Action of Anesthetics in 


: Abolition of Suction 
very slow rate. When this 3 
Seca : Solution of ether substituted for water 
has proceeded for some time, at point marked with dots... Suc- 
the process undergoes a tion abolished within fifteen minutes. 


eradual and final arrest. 

It has already been said that though the excitatory 
reaction is to be regarded as the fundamental cause of the 
transport of water in the plant, yet there are other factors 
which undoubtedly contribute to that result, One of these 
may be the favourable disposition of osmotic substances— 
for example, the concentration of cell-sap consequent on 


376 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


evaporation, in the leaves. That this osmotic effect is, 
however, merely secondary, and that the ascent is chiefly 
due to excitatory action, is seen in certain experiments 
which may be mentioned here, in which the ascent takes 
place with even greater vigour than before, when it is opposed 
by an osmotic influence. . 

The various solutions of salts are very unequal in their 
physiological action : some, like potassium nitrate, are neutral,’ 
but others, as strong solutions of sodium chloride, are ex- 
citatory. Thus the action of 
a strong solution of potassium 
nitrate is physiologically more 
or less neutral, while its osmotic 
action, at the same time, is 
pronounced. A strong solution 
of common salt, on the other 
hand, is both excitatory and 
osmotic. 

If then we apply KNO, 
solution to the cut end of a 
stem, water will be osmotically 
withdrawn from the plant, in 
| opposition to the normal ascent 
Fic. 221. Effect of Strong KNO, of se ; There. will thus be . 

Solution depression of the rate of suction 


The first record shows the normal after the application, as seen in 
and the second the depressed rate . 
the following record (fig. 221), 


of suction caused by the reagent. 

which I obtained with a cut 
branch of Cvoton. If, however, in a similar experiment, 
a strong solution of NaCl be applied, two antagonistic 
reactions will be set up. One, due to the osmotic action, 
will oppose suction, and the other, due to the excitatory 
nature of the reagent, will accelerate it, while the resultant 
effect will be modified by the excitability of the experimental 
plant. In fig. 222 is shown the effect of NaCl solution in 


' It should be mentioned here that even such neutral salts, in strong solution, 
induce physiological depression. 


EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 377 


increasing the suction of a specimen of ‘Croom in a favourable 
condition of excitability. We have here a very great en- 
hancement of the ascensional movement caused by this solu- 
tion, which would, acting osmotically, have retarded the 
normal rate. The physiologically excitatory action of strong 
sodium chloride, however, is not permanent, and the enhanced 
excitability is followed by depression. The effect on suctional 
response in such cases, then, is modified by the factor of time, 
the. first enhancement being 
followed by a fall below the 
original normal rate of suction. 

The experiments which I 
have here described show how 
intimately suctional response is 
connected with excitatory re- 
action. Having thus seen that 
any physiological modification 
of the tissue is attended by an 
appropriate change in the rate 
of suction, it only remains to 
demonstrate finally the fact that 
stimulation is attended by a 
responsive movement of water Fic. 222. Effect of Strong NaCl 
in a tissue. In order to do this, Solution 


we should have at our disposal The first record shows the normal 
. and the second the exalted rate 
mens means of applying of suction caused by the reagent. 


stimulus which is capable of | 

measurement, capable of graduation, and not in itself of 
a nature to disturb the delicate balance of the shoshun- 
graphic record. The end sought after was first to record 
the normal rate of suction, and then to observe the 
effect immediately induced in this by the application of the 
stimulus, the process of record being uninterrupted mean- 
while. The only form of stimulus which would comply 
with these conditions is the electrical, given by tetanising 
induction shocks, of longer or shorter duration. The possible 
objections to the use of this form of stimulus are as follows : 


378 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


1. The water in the plant-vessel may be supposed to undergo 
decomposition by electrolysis. There may also be a certain 
evolution of heat in the plant-vessel. 2. In a sluggish 
tissue, such as that of the plant, the excitatory value of 
induction- shocks may not prove sufficient to Soles suctional 
response. 

With regard to the first of these objections, it is to be 
borne in mind that the shocks, being alternate, will produce | 
but little polarisation effect. Fhe heating effect of a current 
so small in quantity, moreover, is also likely to be very 
slight. ~The extent of 
the disturbance from 
these causes can, how- 
ever, be determined by 
a blank experiment. 
The electrodes, by 
whose proper applica- 
tion the plant is ex- 
cited, are allowed to 
hang down in the plant- 
vessel without direct 


Fic. 223. Record of Blank Experiment showing 
Absence of any Disturbance of Record from attachment to the plant 


Induction-shocks as such itself. The alternating 


Photographic record of excursion of .mercury t AT 
index in Shoshungraph. The thick white CUrrent will now pass 


line shows duration of application of shock. through the water, only 
Time-marks in this and other photographic : 
records represent intervals of five minutes. a very small fraction of 
it, incapable of pro- 
ducing effective excitation, passing through the plant. 
The Shoshungraph is adjusted for the balanced condition, 
given as we have seen, a horizontal line of record, and 
that intensity of induction-current which is to be used 
in subsequent experiments is now passed through the 
plant-vessel for twenty minutes continuously. It will be 
seen from the record (fig. 223), that no disturbance was 
induced by this in the balanced line, thus proving that such 
disturbing effects, if they exist, are in practice negligible. In 
actual experiments, where the excitatory effect is to be studied, 


EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 379. 


the duration of application is often less than a minute, and 
seldom exceeds five minutes. It should also be borne in 
mind that individually and collectively the effects of the dis- 
turbing causes enumerated would, if anything, be towards 
expulsion from the plant-vessel. We shall see, however, 
that the typical responsive effect is movement in the opposite 
direction, indicative of an enhancement of suction. With 
regard to the second cause of misgiving, as to whether plant- 
tissues may or may not be made to exhibit excitatory varia- 
tion by means of induction-shocks, I have found that some 
specimens, notably those of Cvofon, are sufficiently suscep- 
tible to this form of stimulation. For this purpose it is 
necessary to use very strong induction-shocks from a large 
coil, This necessity is further increased by the fact that 
much of the induction-current is uselessly and unavoidably 
shunted by the water in the vessel. | 
This electrical stimulation may be applied to the cut end 
of the branch, immersed in the water of the plant-vessel, in 
either of two different ways. The first of these may be 
described as the Zerminal Mode of Application, its object 
being to localise the excitation more or less at the lower 
end of the specimen. This is done by tying two small 
pieces of platinum, in connection with the electrodes, to 
diametrically opposite sides of the base of the stem by means 
of a thread. Or two pins, in connection with the electrodes, 
may be pricked into the lower section of the specimen near 
its circumference. This transverse mode of stimulation, 
across the diameter of the stem, is not, theoretically, so 
effective as longitudinal stimulation would be, but under 
the particular experimental conditions nothing better could 
be devised (fig. 224). The second mode of applying this 
electrical stimulus I shall distinguish as Swd-terminal. Here, 
two pins in connection with the electrodes pierce through the 
stem, one above the other, in planes at right angles to each 
other (fig. 225). After arranging the electrical connections 
in any one of the ways enumerated, the specimen is adjusted 
in the Shoshungraph, and allowed a period of rest for the 


380 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


passing off of excitatory effects of preparation. The record 
of normal suction is then taken by means either of the ordi- 
nary recorder or of photography. The variation induced in 
the record after the application of stimulus, then, exhibits the 
effect of excitation. 

In order to test the results in as many ways as possible, I 


arranged for three different methods of record. The first 
was without balance. Here the slope of the suction-curve | 


indicates the normal rate of suction, the enhanced rate under 
stimulus being indicated by flexure and increased steepness. 


Fic. 224. Terminal Fic, 225. —Sub-terminal 


Mode of Applica- Mode of Application 
tion of Stimulus of Stimulus 


A diminution in the rate of suction, or an expulsive effect, 
would, on the other hand, be indicated by a corresponding 


diminution of the slope, or reversal of the curve. 
The second method of record is carried out under exact 


balance, and is, as already explained, an extremely delicate 
means of detecting variations in the rate of suction. If this 
rate be enhanced the curve rises suddenly from the balanced 
horizontal line, a depression inducing, on the contrary, a 
downward movement. The last method is that of Over- 


balance, where, owing to a supply of water from the com- 


— ee a 


Piaget oe 


ee 


= LS ee, 


EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 381 


pensator at a uniform rate in larger quantity than necessary, 
a movement of the index is induced in a direction opposite 
to that of suction. Here the slope of the curve, due to over- 
balance, is down, and should the stimulus cause any enhance- 
ment of suction, the steep down-curve must be replaced by a 
less steep, or horizontal, or up-curve. The Method of Balance 
is, as I have said, the most delicate, but this Method of Over- 
balance enables us to detect the after-effect of stimulus in a 
striking manner. 

As regards the form of this response by movement of 
water, reference has already been made to previous results, 
in which we saw that the excitatory effect in the plant, as 
studied by electrical response, was of two different types, 
according to certain phasic conditions. Highly excitable 
roots, we found, gave response by galvanometric negativity, 
indicating secretion or expulsion of water. Less excitable 
roots, on the other hand, gave response by positive variation, 
most probably indicating responsive absorption. These two 
opposite effects actually occur, as I find, under. different 
phasic conditions, in the suctional responses which I am 
about to describe. : 

I shall deal first with the results which I invariably 
obtained in carrying out experiments on Crofom and certain 
other plants during the month of February. The Indian 
winter was just over, and the spring had not yet fully set in. 
The nights were still cold, though the days were growing 
warmer. The plants, therefore, owing to these peculiar 
climatic conditions, must be regarded as having been some- 
what sub-tonic. My first experiment related to the initiation 
of suctional activity in a specimen which, in this respect, 
had been previously at standstill. Stimulus might here be 
expected to renew that multiple activity on which suctional 
response, according to our theory, depends. We may here 
refer once more to the initiation of multiple activity by 
stimulus in a leaflet of Desmodium previously at standstill. 

I have shown that when this plant is deprived of its 
store of latent energy by unfavourable conditions, then the 


382 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


multiple movement ofits leaflets is arrested. If the plant, 
for instance, be kept for some time in a dark room, the leaf- 
lets cease to pulsate. But if now an electrical shock of 
moderate intensity be given to the pulvinus, the incident 
stimulus, by its excitatory action, gives rise to a number of 
responsive movements, which again come to a stop as soon 
as the imparted energy is exhausted. Or we may, in such a 
case, employ the stimulus of light. A record of the subse- 
quent effect has already been given on a. previous page 
(cf. fig. 141). 

It will there be noticed He that the quiescent leaflet is 
thrown into pulsatory movements after the lapse of a short 
latent period ; (2) that the increasing absorption of stimulus 
has the effect of augmenting the amplitude of response ; and 
lastly (3) that, owing to the presence of latent energy derived 
from the impinging stimulus, the activity of the leaflet con- 
tinues for some time, even on the cessation of stimulus itself. 
It is, in. fact, by that enhancement of the tonic condition, 
which comes about by the continuous absorption of energy 
from the’ environment, that the apparently autonomous 
response of the leaflet is maintained. There is, as has been 
said ‘before, no essential difference between multiple and 

autonomous response. A tissue, which was responding 

autonomously, comes to a state of standstill when its store 
of latent energy falls below par. Conversely, by the acces- 
sion of energy from external stimulus, activity is resumed, 
multiple passing into autonomous response. 

Returning, then, to the hydraulic mode of response, as 
observed in variations of suction, we might expect that the 
irapact of stimulus would initiate this in a tissue at suctional 
standstill. For this experiment I took a specimen of Croton 
and mounted it, with terminal electrodes, in the Shoshun- 
graph, At this time it showed a moderate rate of suction. 
It was then kept undisturbed in a dark room for forty-eight 
hours, at the end of which time, owing to the run-down of its 
latent energy, the suctional activity was found to be arrested. 
But on the application of electrical stimulation, the suctional 


a a 


EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 383 


activity was again renewed, after a latent period of two 
minutes, and found to persist for a considerable time, even 
on the cessation of stimulation (fig. 226). We have: here, 
theny an exact parallel to the renewal of the so-called 
autonomous response of Desmodium leaflet referred to 
above, ee 

We have thus studied the phenomenon of the variation of 
suction by renewal when found at zero. We shall next con- 


A 
2 Uaioe Geer sagt ee ee ea * Fic, 227. Photographic 

: Record of Effect of 
Stimulus in Enhancing 
Rate of Suction 


Fic. 226. Renewal of Suction, Pre- 
viously at. Standstill, by Action of 
Stimulus This record was. taken 

under balanced con- 
ditions. Vertical line 
represents moment of 

, application of stimulus 

for 30 seconds. 


The half-shaded portion of figure repre- 
sents time of application. of stimulus. 
Suctional response is seen to be 
initiated after a latent.period of one 
minute, and to persist after the cessa- 
tion of stimulus, 


KA 


sider the case of a variation induced in the existing rate by 
the action of stimulus. The normal rate is exactly balanced, 
a condition which is represented in the photographic record by 
the straight line which results from the stationary position of 
the mercury index. Stimulus of 5 seconds’ duration was now 
applied, and the responsive acceleration is seen as a steep rise 
in the record (fig. 227). This responsive acceleration persists 


' Ifa cut branch of any plant be kept in water for several days, its suctional 
activity, as is well known, disappears. This is commonly attributed to the 
blocking of the cut end by mucilage and bacterial growths, since the making of 
a fresh section is found to renew the activity. This making of a fresh section, 


384 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


during a period that depends on the intensity of stimulus and 
the condition of the tissue, after which it declines slowly. 
After recovery, however, the rate is generally somewhat 
higher than at first. This is due to the persistent after- 
effect of stimulus absorbed, If 
the enhanced suctional rate be 
now again balanced, and 
stimulus. applied once more, 
there will be a still further 
enhancement of the rate. In 
this way, owing to the succes- 
sive increase of latent energy, 
the suctional activity is enhanced 
till it reaches a limit, after which 
there is but little additional effect 
to be induced by stimulus. 
There is another and _ in- 
teresting effect which is often 
observed, in consequence of 


Variation of Latent 
Period as After-effect of Stimulus 


Fic. 228. 


The record was taken under balanced 
conditions. Half-shaded portion 


represents application of stimulus 
for 30 seconds. Lower record 
shows latent period to be 45 
seconds. After _re-balance, 
stimulus of 30 seconds was once 
more applied. -Upper record 
now shows reduction of latent 


energy absorbed from previous 
stimulation. This is the diminu- 
tion of the latent period, after 
which response takes place. 
This will be seen in the following 


period to 30 seconds. 


| record (fig. 228). The initial 
rate of suction was in this case balanced, as usual, and 
stimulus of thirty seconds’ duration was applied. The 
responsive acceleration is seen to take place forty-five 
seconds after the cessation of stimulus. The enhanced rate 
was balanced, and a stimulus of thirty seconds’ duration 


however, does not decide the question; for in making it, is involved the 
strong mechanical stimulus of a cut. The outgrowths may, no doubt, obstruct 
the passage of water, and yet the total abolition of suction not be due to this 
cause alone. The more effective cause is, in fact, the run-down of energy, as 
proved by the experiment described above. In another experiment I took a cut 
stem in which suction had come to a standstill, and, without disturbing the 
mucilaginous end, I supplied it with water somewhat above the ordinary tem- 
perature. This thermal stimulation at once initiated renewed suctional_ activity 
with great vigour. 


EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 385 


was applied once more. It is here seen that this stimulus 
induces a further acceleration. The latent period, however, 
of this second response is reduced from forty-five seconds, 
which was its value in the first case, to thirty seconds. 

This variation of latent period is brought out still more 
clearly by the application of a stimulus of shorter duration, 
in which case the latent period is more prolonged, and its 
variations, therefore, more easily observed. In order to show 
this, I took a fresh specimen of Cyvof¢on, and, after the initial 
balance, applied stimulus of five seconds’ duration. It will 
be seen from the photographic record (fig. 229) that the 


Fic, 229. Photographic Record showing Variation of Latent Period 
as After-effect of Stimulus 


Stimulus applied was for 5 seconds. Moment of application represented 
by vertical line. Lower record shows latent period to be 25 minutes. 
After re-balance, stimulus of 5 seconds was again applied. Upper 
record now shows reduction of latent period to 20 minutes, 


latent period was here very long, being as much as twenty- 
five minutes. After re-balance stimulus was once more 
applied, lasting, as before, for five seconds. The latent 
period in the second case is seen to be reduced to twenty 
minutes. | 

As an interesting and independent verification of the 
enhancement of suction by stimulus, I now took a number 
of response-curves, using the Method of Over-balance. Here, 
it will be remembered, the normal over-balance is indicated 
by a down-curve, and acceleration of suction by diminution 
of the slope, or even by reversal, of this curve. In fig. 230 
is seen a record obtained in this manner the first down part 

CC 


386 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


of which shows the curve of over-balance. Stimulus of halt 
a minute’s duration was now applied, and it will be noticed 


t {Fic. 230. Suctional Response 
under Over-balance 


Stimulus of 30 seconds neutralises 
over-balance and reverses curve, 


that on account of the re- 
sponsive acceleration the 
slope becomes increasingly 
diminished, till,- after an 
interval of one minute and 
a half, the curve becomes 
horizontal. After this it is 
reversed to the upward direc- 
tion. It will thus be seen 
that the responsive accelera- 
tion has here, induced a rate 
of suction which is not merely 
sufficient to compensate the 
over-balance, but greatly ex- 
ceeds it. .In the next photo- 


eraphic record (fig. 231) I have been successful in showing 
the immediate and persistent after-effects. In order to do 


Fic. 231. Photographic Record of Effect of Stimulus on Over-balance 


First stimulus for 2 seconds, represented by first vertical line, neutralises 
and reverses over-balance. Horizontal record after reversal represents 
persistent after-effect. Second stimulus for 2 seconds, represented 
by second vertical line, gives risejto up-record, the persistent after- 
effect being represented by a curve of diminished slope. 


this within the limited range of a photographic plate, I 
employed stimulus of the short duration of two seconds. 


EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 387 


The first part of the record shows the normal down-curve of 
over-balance. Stimulus of two seconds’ duration was now 
applied, at the place marked in the record with a vertical 
line. This is seen to induce a growing diminution, in the 
slope of the curve culminating in reversal; and afterwards, 
owing to the persistence of the after-effect of stimulus, the 
record becomes horizontal. A second stimulus of two 
seconds’ duration was now applied. ‘This is seen to induce a 
further enhancement of the rate, which is shown by the up- 
curve. The slope of this curve undergoes a slow decline with 
the waning of the immediate effect of stimulus. But on | 
account of that component of the stimulus which remains 
latent in the tissue, there is induced a more or less persistent 
after-effect, which is greater than the after-effect due to the 
first stimulus. For while the after-effect of the first stimulus 
was seen to make the record horizontal, the second after- 
effect renders the curve slightly ascending. From these 
and other facts previously enumerated it will be under- 
stood that the effect ot latent stimulus derived from external 
sources is to increase suctional activity up to a certain 
limit. 

Having now described the various effects induced in 
a slightly sub-tonic tissue, under the simplest mode of 
stimulation, namely, the terminal, we shall proceed to inquire 
as to what are the effects induced under a somewhat more com- 
plex mode of stimulation. This is the case with sub-terminal 
stimulus, where the point stimulated is not on the external 
extremity, but within the tissue, though near the lower end. 
Under these conditions the excitatory wave will proceed in 
two opposite directions, upwards and downwards. The 
short terminal zone, however, being close to the directly 
stimulated area, will be more intensely affected than the 
extended upper region. For this reason there is likely to 
be a predominant expulsion of water from the lower end. 
Under continued strong stimulation, however, the more 
intensely excited lower zone may become fatigued. The 


natural upward suction, probably enhanced by the action of 
cc2 


388 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


stimulus, may now be expected to reassert itself, thus con- 
verting expulsion into renewed suction. 

In any case, whatever the explanation, I find that the 
result of this mode of stimulus is, first, a movement of 
expulsion, followed, under continued intense stimulus, by 
renewal of the upward suction. These various effects 
are seen in the following photographic record, where 
the up-curve represents the normal unbalanced -suction 
(fig. 232). Continuous 
stimulation was now applied 
at the point marked with a 
verticalline. It will be seen 
that normal suction is here 
diminished, and afterwards 
reversed into expulsion. 
This expulsive movement 
continues, as I already knew 
from previous experiments, 
for a considerable length of 
time, before the second re- 
versal to suction is brought 
Fic. 232. Photographic Record of about by fatigue of the lower 


. 4 i b- i l . 
cat ee Continuous Sub-termina zone. In order to expedite 


Response was taken without balance the reversal, so that the 


Continuous stimulation applied from cyrye might remain within 
. moment represented by vertical line. 


This induced diminution, arrest, and the plate, I applied a still 
reversal of response to expulsion. : : 

Stronger stimulation applied at second SLrenges stimulation, at the 
vertical line. This induced a second point marked by the second 


reversal to suction. Thin white line . ‘ A 
shows duration of application of vertical line. This was done 
stimulus of moderate intensity; and by increasing the voltage 
subsequent thick line, of greater ; 4a ae 
intensity. which worked the primary 
of the induction coil from 
six to eight volts. It will be seen how this reversed the 
expulsion, converting it into renewed suction. 
We have seen, as already stated in the electrical response 
of roots, that while less excitable old roots will generally 


give positive response, highly excitable young roots give 


EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 389 


negative. We saw, further, that there was reason to 
associate this positive response with the process of absorp- 
tion, and the negative, conversely, with that of expulsion 
or secretion. With various kinds of tissues, moreover, we 
have found, and shall see further, that as a general rule 
positive response is obtained, either when the tissue is 
sub-tonic, and very slightly. excitable, or when the stimulus- 
intensity is feeble, and when the tissue is fatigued by over- 
stimulation. Negative response, on the other hand, is 
characteristic of highly excitable tissues. Under natural 
conditions, then, when the roots are subjected to the 
moderate stimulation of such factors as contact with soil, 
water, and food, we might expect their response to be 
positive or absorptive. In cut branches, also, in which the 
tissue is not extremely excitable, a simple terminal applica- 
tion of stimulus induces, as we saw, the absorpto-positive 
effect, either by initiating suction, or by enhancing that 
which was already taking place. 

But in highly excitable young roots, in contact with a 
stimulating supply of inorganic food, the characteristic 
response, as we have seen, is by secretion. The electrical 
response also of young roots we found to be negative. 
Between these two extremes of positive and negative, then, 
there must be an intermediate case in which the responsive 
action to external stimulus will be zero. 

Applying this to the parallel case of suctional response 
in cut branches, we should expect to meet with. two 
different cases besides that already given. In one, where 
the tissue is very highly excitable, the response under simple 
terminal stimulation will be negative or expulsive. In 
tissues, however, which are not so highly excitable, but more 
excitable than those sub-tonic specimens whose characteristic 
responses I have already described, it might be possible to 
find cases in which the suctional response to stimulus will 
be zero. In the sub-tonic tissues referred to, we have already 
seen that in consequence of increase of internal energy by 
external stimulus, the suctional response tends to reach a 


390 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


limit, after which further stimulation would produce little or 
no effect. 7 

Thus we see that if a tissue from any cause be sub-tonic, 
even moderately strong stimulus will induce positive or 
absorptive response. If it, on the other hand, be highly 
excitable, the response may be expected to be negative or 
expulsive. Between these two, in the intermediate state of 
excitability, the effect will be neither one nor the other, that 
is to say, zero. These results concern the application of 
somewhat strong stimulus, such as that of electrical shocks. 
Feeble stimulus will generally evoke response by absorption. 
We can also clearly see that the tonic condition, and there- 
fore the excitability, of a tissue will vary with the seasons, 
being low at the end of winter, and high in spring or summer. 
Thus the same strong stimulus which in the one season will 
induce absorption, might be expected in the other to provoke 
expulsion. In experimenting on suction during the period 
of seasonal variation, I obtained results which verified these 
inferences, 

These experiments, as will be remembered, were begun 
_ in February, when the spring had scarcely commenced, and 
the plants were in a sub-tonic condition. Under these 
circumstances, we saw that the terminal application ot 
stimulus uniformly evoked positive response, by enhance- 
ment of suction. By the end of February, however, when 
warmer weather prevailed, and the vigour of the plants was 
evidently greater, I was surprised to find that the same 
stimulus, applied in the same way, to similar specimens of 
Croton evoked little or no response. A week later—that is 
to say, in the beginning of March, when the Indian spring 
was well advanced, and the physiological activity of the 
plants high—I found that the response which had thus seemed 
to disappear was renewed, but had become reversed in sign. 
Strong terminal stimulation now, as a rule, evoked responsive 
expulsion. 

From the considerations enumerated at the beginning of 
these investigations, it was seen that the various physical 


EXCITATORY CHARACTER OF SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 391 


theories brought forward to account for the ascent of sap 
were admittedly inadequate. The further objections, urged 
against the fundamentally excitatory nature of the processes 
involved, on the ground of the important part played in the 
ascent by sap-wood, generally regarded as dead, I have also 
shown to be untenable. The sap-wood I have shown to be 
not dead, but living, and to exhibit the normal response of 
living tissues to excitation. The long persistence of: suction, 
when the roots are killed with hot water, or the cut 
specimen placed in poison, was shown to be accounted for 
by the fact that the death of any individual zone does not 
arrest suction in those above. I have shown, moreover, that 
those agents, such as rising temperature, which exalt the 
general physiological activity of the tissue, enhance suctional 
activity also. Those which, like cold or anzsthetics, act, on 
the other hand, to depress the general physiological activity, 
will depress and arrest its suctional activity also. And, 
finally, the fact. that the water-movement is a form of 
excitatory response has been fully demonstrated by the 
experiments described and the records given in the course 
of the present chapter. The physiological theory of the 
ascent of sap may thus be regarded as established. 


CHAPTER XXVII 
RESPONSE TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 


Heliotropic plant movements reducible to fundamental reaction of contraction 
or expansion—Various mechanical effects of light in pulvinated and growing 
organs—Electrical response induced by light not specific, but cencomitant to 
excitatory effects—Electrical response of plant to light not determined by 
presence or absence of chloroplasts—Effect of unilateral application of stimu- 
lus on transversely distal point—Positive response due to indirect effect and 
negative to transmission of true excitation—Mechanical response of leaf of 
Mimosa to light applied on upper half of pulvinus—Mechanical response 
consists of erection or positive ovement, followed by fall or negative move- 
ment—Electrical response of leaf of A/¢mosa to light applied on upper halt 
of pulvinus ; induction in lower half of pulvinus of positivity followed by 
negativity—Longitudinal transmission of excitatory effect, with concomitant 
galvanometric negativity—Direct effect of light and positive after-effect— 
Circumstances which are effective in reversing normal response—Plants in 
slightly sub-tonic condition give positive followed by negative response— 
Exemplified by (a) electrical and (6) growth response—Examples of positive 
response to light—Periodic variation of excitability—Multiple mechanical 
response under light—Direct and after effect—Multiple electrical response 
under light, with phasic alternations of (— + — +) or (+ — + —)—After 
effects; unmasking of antagonistic elements, either A/ws or minus—Three 
types of after-effects. 


THE first important point that arises, in connection with the 
response of living tissues to light, is the question whether 
such response is peculiar in its character, or fundamentally 
similar to that which is evoked by other forms of stimulus. 
The mechanical movements of plants under light are 
apparently so diverse that it would at first sight appear 
almost impossible to derive them all from any common 
fundamental reaction. Thus, some plant organs are found 
to turn towards the light, others away from it, and 
others again to remain perpendicular to it. Thus three 
different typical effects—positive, negative, and dia-helio- 


RESPONSE TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 393 


tropic—are induced in different cases by the same 
stimulus. These effects, moreover, are found to occur in 
growing as well as in pulvinated organs. This incon- 
sistency of effects has been a source of great perplexity, 
inclining observers to the belief that the action of any given 
plant organ under light is determined, not by some definite 
reaction, but by its own power to decide what is for its 
individual advantage. 

I have shown elsewhere, however,! that as regards 
mechanical response, the reaction of plant organs to the 
stimulus of light is extremely definite. This, like other 
forms of stimuius, induces negative turgidity variation and 
contraction, as well as consequent retardation of growth in 
growing organs. Such excitatory effects, moreover, if the 
tissue be of fair conducting power, may be transmitted in 
either a transverse or a longitudinal direction. The intensity 
of this transmitted excitatory effect is thus dependent, as I 
have shown, on the intensity and duration of stimulus and 
on the conductivity of the tissue. If neither the intensity of 
the stimulus nor the conductivity of the tissue be great, it 
will be the indirect or hydro-positive effect which will reach 
the distant point, there to induce a positive turgidity 
variation and expansion. The foregoing observations relate 
to tissues in a normal condition of excitability. When the 
tissue is sub-tonic, however, the absorbed stimulus, as we have 
seen, increases the internal energy and brings about a re- 
sponsive expansion. 

I have also shown that the various mechanical move- 
ments induced by the unilateral action of light, depend (1) 
upon whether the stimulus remains localised on the proximal 
side of the organ or is conducted to the distal ; and (2) on 
the relative excitabilities of proximal and distal. I have 
shown, moreover, that all the diverse effects induced by light 
are demonstrably traceable to the action of these various 
factors in varying combination. And, finally, certain highly 
excitable tissues, owing to excess of energy derived from 

1 Plant Response, pp. 551 to 685. 


304 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


continuous stimulation, exhibit alternations of phase, negative 
and positive, or vice versa, constituting multi-phasic or 
oscillatory response. On these considerations it is possible 
to summarise the principal effects caused by light, as 


follows: 


MECHANICAL EFFECTS OF LIGHT ON PULVINATED AND 


GROWING ORGANS. 


II. | Normally — ex- 
citable organ 
subjected to | 
unilateral 
light. 

A. Organ radial. 


| 
| 
| 
| 


| B. Organ aniso- 
| | tropic or dorsi- 
ventral. 


of internal energy. 


A 1. Moderate light, 
causing excitatory 
contraction of proxi- 
mal and hydro-posi- 


tive expansion of | 


distal. 


A 2. Strong light. Ex- 
citatory effect trans- 
mitted to distal, neu- 
tralising first. 


A 3. Intense and long- 
continued light. 
Fatigue of proximal 
and excitatory con- 
traction of distal. 


Description . 
Case of Hate Action Effect observed 
-| 
I. | Tissuesub-tonic. | Stimulus causesincrease | Expansion or enhanced 


rate of growth, e.g. 
Pileus of Coprinus 
drooping in dark- 
ness, made re-turgid 
by light. Renewed 
growth of  dark- 
rigored plant ex- 
posed to light. 


. Curvature towards 


light, e.g. positive 
curvature of seed- 
lings of  Sinapis; 
positive curvature of 
Lepidium seedlings 
(Oltmanns). . 

Neutral effect, e.g. 
Sinapis and Lepfi- 
dium (Oltmanns) 
under strong and 
long-continued light. 


- Reversed or nega- 


tive response, e.g. 
Sinapis and Lept- 
dium (Oltmanns). 


BI. Excitatory con- 
traction of proximal 
predominant, owing 
either to greater ex- 
citability of proximal 
or feeble transverse 
conductivity of tissue. 

B 2. Transmission of 
excitation through 
highly conducting 
tissue to more ex- 
citable lower or 
distal. Greater con- 


traction of distal. 


_ 


N 


. Positive response, 


e.g. upward folding 
of leaflets in so-called 
‘diurnal sleep’ of 
Robinia, Erythrina 
indica, and Clitorta 
ternatea. 


. Negative response, 


e.g. downward fold- 
ing of leaflets in so- 
called ‘ diurnal sleep’ 
of Oxalis, Biophy- 
tum, and Averrhoa. 


RESPONSE TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 395 | 


Case 


Description Action Effect observed 
of tissue 


III. | Tissues -which | Considerable absorp- “Initiation of multiple 


exhibit mul- tion of energy, im-- response in Desmo- 
tiple or auto- mediate or prior. dium gyrans_pre- 
nomous _re- _ viously at standstill ; 
sponse. multiple response 

| under continuous 


| 

| | action of light in 

| Biophytum; photo- 

| tactic movements 

| of swarming spores ; 
cf. multiple visual im- 
pulses in retina. 


Turning now to the electrical responses induced by light, 
our investigation is resolved into the inquiry whether these 
are not the electrical concomitants of those excjtatory effects 
which we have already been able to analyse through 
mechanical response. But, before proceeding to this question, 
I shall first briefly refer to certain ‘electrical effects of light 
upon green leaves’ which have been observed by Dr. Waller.' 
- His experiments were performed by making galvanometer 
connections with two halves of the same leaf, one being 
strongly illuminated and the other unilluminated. With leaves 
of different plants he obtained opposite electrical effects under 
the action of light. From the leaves of Iris, for example, during 
illumination, he obtained response of galvanometric negativity, 
with reversal, or positivity, on the cessation of light as its after- 
effect. With leaves of Zrope@olum and Mathiola, on the con- 
trary, he obtained positive response during illumination and 
subsequently negative. Beyond the suggestion that negativity 
may be associated with dissimilation and positivity with 
assimilation, Dr. Waller offers no explanation of this opposi- 
tion of effects observed by him. He states, however, that he 
regards the presence of ‘chloroplasts’ as essential to these 
electrical reactions under light, inasmuch as petals, he found, 
gave no response. Even in the case of the green leaves ‘of 
ordinary garden shrubs and trees,’ moreover; he found no 


' Waller, Proc. Roy. Soc. vol. \xvii. pp. 129-137. 


396 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


response. This he ascribes to their ‘low average metabolism.’ 
It will thus be seen that no satisfactory explanation is offered 
either of the mutual opposition of the electrical effects ob- 
served or of the way in which the presence of ‘chloroplasts’ 
acts as a determining factor. 

To turn now to the subject of my proper inquiry, it has 
to be determined whether those electrical responses which 
may be observed in vegetable tissues under the action of - 
light are or are not another expression of the same excita- 
tory reactions under light which I have already demonstrated 
by means of mechanical response. And it may be as 
well to say at the outset that it is the excitability of the 
tissue, and not the presence or absence of ‘chloroplasts,’ that 
is the critical factor in determining this electrical response. 
For I have obtained strong responses under light from pul- 
vini, stems, and other tissues which are relatively deficient in 
‘chloroplasts’; and again, while the lamina of a plant rich 
in ‘chloroplasts’ would give but moderate response to light, 
the petiole of the same plant, characterised by less ‘chloro- 
plasts,’ would often give much stronger response. The etio- 
lated stem of celery, moreover, gives strong electrical response. 
And, finally, it is an error to suppose that petals of flowers 
are irresponsive to light, for I have obtained strong response 
from petals of Seshania coccineum and from Eucharis lily. 
Animal nerve, again, in which there is no chlorophyll, gives 
response to light. 

As I have already shown the typical heliotropic effects 
exhibited by plants to be brought about by differential 
excitatory action on the proximal and distal sides of the 
same organ, I shall now proceed to exhibit the electrical 
counterparts of these experimentally. As I wish, moreover, 
to show that the general electrical response to the unilateral 
action of light is fundamentally the same as that induced by 
other forms of stimulus, the first experiment to be described 
will be one depending on the unilateral application of a non- 
luminous stimulus, say thermal. 

If on one side of a growing organ we apply a series of 


RESPONSE TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 397 


thermal shocks, by means of the electro-thermic stimulator, 
the proximal side will undergo contraction, while the ex- 
pelled water, by its hydro-positive effect, will induce expan- 
sion on the distal side. By means of these two conspiring 
actions the organ will be bent towards the source of 
stimulus. The electrical variation on the distal side will 
therefore be positive ; but if the stimulus applied be suffi- 
ciently strong and long-continued, true excitation will be 
transmitted across the tissue to the distal side. This will 
neutralise the first mechanical movement, and the corre- 
sponding electrical effect will be a reversal of the previous 


Fic. 233. Experimental Arrangement for Detection of Electrical Change 
induced at the Point transversely Distal to Point stimulated 


Upper point stimulated by thermal shocks from electro-thermic stimulator, 
the lower being the transversely distal point. 


positive into the excitatory negative. Similar electrical 
effects will also be observed if the organ be restrained from 
movement, or if it be so old as to have lost its power of 
motility. 

Taking now a young stem of 4ryophyllum, 1 applied a 
series of thermal stimuli at the proximal point (fig. 233). 
Periodic closure of the electrical circuit by means of a metro- 
nome caused rapidly succeeding thermal shocks to act on the 
upper or proximal side. By adjusting the heating current 
the stimulus was at first made moderate. It will be seen 


398 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


from the photographic record (fig. 234) that this gave rise 
electrically to an increasing positive effect at the diametrically 
opposite point. This clearly shows that the latter under- 
went a positive turgidity variation, in consequence of the 
forcing-in of water expelled by excitatory contraction from the 
upper side. It is at this stage the indirect and not. the true 
excitatory effect of stimulus that is being transmitted to B. 
By this experiment it is 
also demonstrated that the 
positive curvature induced 
by the unilateral applica- 
tion of any stimulus is the 
joint effect of the direct 
excitatory contraction of 
the proximal side and the 
indirect or hydro-positive 
expansion of the distal.! 


Another interesting 
F1G. 234. Record of Kesponse to Moderate h b b 
Unilateral Stimulation under the Experi- P®¢nomenon to © Cn 


mental Arrangement described served in this curve is that, 
Response of distal point by increased after the maximum effect 
galvanometric positivity due to hydro- 
positive effect. Note initiation of multi: has been reached, there 
ple response. . , . 
is a series of oscillatory 
multiple responses. In this result there may possibly be two 
factors in operation: first, after the maximum hydro-positive 
tension has been set up there may be a gradual percolation 
of the true excitatory effect, with its opposite reaction, the 
unstable balance thus produced manifesting itself in oscilla- 
tions; and, secondly, we know that increased hydrostatic 


tension has the effect of initiating multiple responses, as seen 


1 In records of such a response as I have just described, when exhibited by 
highly excitable tissues, a preliminary negative twitch, of momentary duration, 
may sometimes be observed. This is not due to the conduction of true excitation, 
but to pseudo-conduction. The sudden blow delivered by the hydrostatic wave 
on its arrival at the distal point is, in a highly excitable structure, sufficient of 
itself to induce a short-lived excitatory effect. Thus this does not represent the 
true transmission of excitation, but its initiation de novo by a secondary mecha- 
nical cause (p. 446). 


RESPONSE TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 399 


in the snail’s heart, under a sufficiently high degree of 
internal hydrostatic pressure. 

As regards conduction in general, we know that a 
strong stimulus is transmitted to a greater distance than a 
weak. In the next record (fig. 235) this may be seen in an 
interesting manner. At first a moderate stimulus was em- 
ployed, and this gave rise to (2) a maximum positive varia- 
tion of the distal point B. The stimulus was then increased 
and we observe that (0) the 
excitatory effect, now reach- 
ing B, causes a reversal of 
the curve, owing to induced 
galvanometric negativity. If 
at the beginning we had used 
a stimulus of fairly strong 
intensity the first effect would 
have been a positivity of B, 
due to the indirect effect of 
stimulus ; and, secondly, the 
excitatory effect would have 
reached the point gradually, 
neutralising and afterwards 


reversing the first. I shall FIG. 235. Record of Different Specimen 


now describe the correspond- under same Experimental Arrange- 
: ‘ ment when Stimulus is first Moderate 
ing effects, both mechanical and then Increased 


and electrical, which are in- (a) Positive response, due to hydro- 
positive effect ; this is converted to 


duced by stimulus of light. negative in (6) due to transmission of 
Wetake a Winiosa plant, and excitatory effect under stronger stimu- 
lation. 


subject only the upper half of 

one of its primary pulvini to the action of sunlight. The 
effects thus induced are (1) the local contraction of the excited 
upper half, and the expansion of the lower half by the hydro- 
positive effect ; (2) the gradual percolation of true excitation 
to the lower half, and consequent initiation of excitatory con- 
traction there; and (3) the continued action of excitation 
and increasing contraction on the more excitable lower half. 
All these effects. are exhibited in the mechanical response 


400 - COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


shown in fig. 236, where the first is seen in the up-curve, 
which thus indicates the joint action of contraction in the 
upper and expansion in the lower, giving rise to an erectile 
movement. After an interval of one minute the excitatory 
effect is seen to have reached the lower half, giving rise now 
to a reversed or down movement, which, on account of the 
greater excitability of the lower, is seen to carry the leaf 
downwards, much below its original position. The dotted 
portion of the curve shows the after effect on the cessation of 
the stimulating light. It is 
here interesting to observe 
that it is possible to obtain 
an after-effect which is posi- 
tive and of opposite sign to 
the true excitatory effect, this 
positivity being due to the 
increased internal energy 
consequent on the absorption 
Fic. 236. Mechanical Response of Of stimulus. 


Pulvinus of AZmosa to Continuous 
Action of Light from Above applied In fig. 237 are shown the 
at Moment marked | electrical effects consequent 


- Positive heliotropic movement caused on _ stimulation by light in 
by excitation of upper half neu- ; 

tralised by transmission to distal another specimen of the 
side, and ultimately reversed owing ulvinus of Mimosa The 
to greater excitability of lower half. : , : 

Dotted line represents recovery on electrical contacts are made 


cessation of light. Note final erec-  ; : : 
tion of leaf above original position in this case, one with the 


as after-effect of absorbed stimulus. lower half of the pulvinus and 
the other with a distant in- 

different point. Stimulus of light is applied, as in the 
last case, on the upper half. This experimental method 
is free from the objection which has been urged against 
Dr. Waller's experiments on green leaves, that the result 
was complicated by the direct action of light on the electrode 
itself. It should be pointed out, however, that the presence 
of such photo-electric action is more important theoretically 
than practically, being of relatively small amount. A further 
complication which arises from the direct action of light on 


RESPONSE TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 401 


one of the electrodes lies in rise of temperature. Though in 
all such experiments the incident light should pass to the 
organ through a thick stratum of water, which absorbs its 
heat-rays, yet the absorption of light by the tissue must 
necessarily occasion a slight rise of temperature. In con- 
nection with this should be remembered the fact I have 
elsewhere demonstrated, that though sudden variation of 
temperature acts as an excitatory agent, yet a slow and 
gradual rise, enhancing the internal energy, brings about 
only a slight positivity, opposite to the effect of true excita- 
tion. In this particular ex- 
periment, however, as the 
electrical contacts are not 
directly acted on by light, 
we obtain results uncom- 
plicated by such disturbing 
factors. 

The first electrical effect 
brought about in the lower 
half of the pulvinus by yy. 237. Electrical Response in the 


icati : Lower Half of the Pulvinus of 
sd plication of light ii the Mimosa due to Stimulation of 
distal upper half is seen in Distal Upper Half by Light 


fig. 237 as an increasing gal- Observe the first phase of positivity, 
; ee ‘ due to hydro-positive effect, con- 
vanometric positivity. This verted subsequently into negative 
is concomitant to the hydro- NF ae transmission of true ex- 
positive effect at the lower 
half, which, conspiring with the contraction of the upper, 
produces that up-movement of the leaf seen in the previous 
figure. The excitatory effect next reaches the lower half, 
and we there obtain increasing galvanometric negativity in 
consequence. This corresponds with the mechanical move- 
‘ment of depression. From this experiment it is clear that 
light, like other forms of stimulus, induces, as its true excita- 
tory reaction, galvanometric negativity, the indirect or hydro- 
positive effect being one of galvanometric positivity. 
In the last case, then, we obtained a transverse trans- 
mission of the true excitatory effect. Similar effects are 
DD 


402 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


also obtainable by longitudinal transmission. In order to 
do this an organ must be selected which is a fairly good 
conductor. I have thus been able to observe a series of 
responses to transmitted stimulus of light, using such speci- 
mens as the petiole of Bryophyllum. Light was here applied 
at a distance of 5 mm. from the proximal contact, and this 
gave rise to a series of true excitatory responses of galvano- 
metric negativity. 

Having thus established unmistakably the negative sign 
of the excitatory electrical variation induced and transmitted 
under stimulus of light, I shall 
next proceed: to give records of 
experiments in which light was 
applied directly. The effect ob- 
served in these cases is naturally 
much larger, as there is no 
enfeeblement by _ transmission. 
Fig. 238 shows a series of such 
responses, obtained at intervals of 
two minutes, by the application 
of sunlight — previously passed 
through a stratum of water— 
during five seconds only in each 
Hie eA? Phaeatic Be. ~case, on the petiole of a vigorous 
“cord of Series of Negative leaf of Bryophyllum. It will be 

Responses of Petiole of noticed that after each response 

Bryophyllum to Stimuli of : nee 

Sunlight of Five Seconds?’ Of galvanometric negativity there 

Dt eee ee Inter- is an after-effect of positivity, in 
Observe the positive after-effect, consequence of which the base 

due to increase of internal line of the series, instead of re- 

energy, which causes down- site : 

ward trend of base-line. maining horizontal, trends down- 

wards. This power of holding 

stimulus latent, for the increase of internal energy, we shall 
later see to be important, as heralding the initiation of 
multiple response. The exhibition of these after-effects, due 
to increase of latent energy, is also to be observed in the 
record given already of the mechanical response of Jzmosa 


RESPONSE TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT ~ 403 


(fig. 236), where the leaf, after its excitatory fall, was re- 
erected, on cessation of stimulus, above its original height. 
This, as we saw, was due to a certain portion of the incident 
stimulus becoming latent, and thus increasing the internal 
energy. 

We shall next take up the subject of the occurrence of 
positive response, as sometimes induced by light. This may 
be the result of various different causes. There is one fact, 
however, in connection with the action of light which it is 
important to bear in mind. Thus, if we subject the lower 
half of the pulvinus of A/zmosa, for instance, to the action of 
sunlight, its responsive fall will be gradual, unlike the sudden 
depression caused by thermal or mechanical stimulation. 
This is because light, usually speaking, constitutes a stimulus 
of only moderate intensity. We have seen that a stimulus 
which falls below a certain critical level of excitatory intensity 
will evoke positive, instead of negative response. We have 
also seen that from a sub-tonic tissue the positive response is 
more easily obtained than from one which is highly ex- 
citable. Now, as the excitatory efficiency of a mechanical 
stimulus is very great, and as that stimulus is also incapable 
of finely graduated decrease, it follows that, in order to ex- 
hibit positive response under such stimulation, it is necessary 
that the tissue stimulated should be extremely depressed, or 
even moribund. Under such conditions I have shown 
(p. 83) that it is possible under feeble stimulus to obtain 
positive response, which, under stronger, will pass into the 
normal negative. 

The stimulus of light, then, whose action is very moderate, 
discriminates more finely between tonic gradations of the 
tissue than can other forms of stimulus. If this tonic con- 
dition be very favourable, and the excitability high, the re- 
sponse will be by normal galvanometric negativity. If the 
tonic condition, however, be less favourable, the response is 
liable to be positive. This latter fact will be very strikingly 
demonstrated, in a later chapter, by experiments carried out 
on nerves, It will there be shown that while highly excitable 

DD2 


404 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


nerve gives the normal negative response, the same tissue, 
if its tonic condition be below par, gives a more or less per- 
sistent positive response. It is only when the tonic condition 
of the nerve has again been raised, by long-continued 
stimulation, that it will once more give normal response. 
Fatigue is another condition which is liable to give rise to 
the abnormal positive response. 

The use of the stimulus of light carries with it, also, a 
further limitation. A mechanical stimulus, say vibrational, 
throws into activity the whole mass of tissue, not only in its 
superficial, but also in its deeper lying strata. Now we have 
seen that the epidermal layer of living tissues is less excitable 
than those which are deeper seated. It may even, in fact, on 
loca! excitation, give positive response (p. 298). It is to be 
noticed, moreover, that light acts from outside, its excitatory 
influence affecting the outmost tissue first, and only by 
gradual percolation passing to the subjacent. Owing to 
these two facts, then, of the moderateness of this stimulus 
and the superficial character of its action, the tissue, if not 
highly excitable, is apt, under its application, to give positive 
_ response. We have seen, further, that various circumstances, 
such as age and season, have an important effect in varying 
the excitatory reaction of a tissue. We saw the effect of 
age exemplified in the responses given by two different 
specimens of roots (p. 353), in which a young root gave nega- 
tive and an older positive responses. Again, we shall see 
presently that there is a diurnal period, on account of which 
the state of turgor, the excitability, and the sign of response, 
are all alike liable to undergo periodic variations. Under 
the stimulus of light these varying excitabilities may be ex- 
pected to find varying expressions. 

That sub-tonicity tends to make the response, under 
moderate stimulation, positive, is seen in the fact that an 
etiolated petiole of celery gives positive response under light 
Again, I have noticed that leaves of Bryophyllum, which 
usually give normal negative responses, sometimes exhibit 
positive, if the plant, during the previous night, have been 


RESPONSE TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 405 


subjected to unusual cold. Even in such a case, however, 
though the first responses are positive, successive exposures 
to light, by raising the tonic condition, are found to restore 
the response to the normal negative. 
The same facts receive interesting illustration in the re- 
sponse of growth. If the growing organ be in a normally 
excitatory condition, the 
stimulus of light, inducing 
negative turgidity varia- 
tion, causes retardation 
of growth. If the tissue, "|s° w0" 1s" 20" 25° 80° 35° 40°45 
however, be in an ex- 
tremely sub-tonic con- 
dition, light stimulus, by 
increasing the internal 
energy, gives rise to the 
positive effect, that is to 
say, the initiation, or en- 


Fic. 239. Record of Responsive Growth- 
variation taken under condition of balance 


hancement, of the rate in slightly Sub-tonic Flower-bud of 
Cri ; : enuha tian Gf 
at growth. If the erow- rane Lily under Diffuse Stimulation o 


ing tissue, again, be only Continuous lines represent the effect during 

; 3 : application of light, the dotted line on 
slightly sub sek gie ‘ = withdrawal of light. The plant was 
shall have a preliminary originally in a sub-tonic condition, and 


+4 ° application of light at x, after short 
positive, or enhancement, latent period, induces preliminary ac- 


followed by the negative celeration of growth. After this follows 
: the normal retardation. On withdrawal 

response, or retardation of light, in the dotted portion of the 
of the rate of growth. curve is seen the negative after-effect, 
eT - followed by return to the normal rate of 
This is seen in the fol- growth. A second and long-continued 


application of light induces retardation, 


lowing record, which I followed by oscillatory response. 


obtained from a slightly 

sub-tonic flower-bud of Crzzum lily, in the induced variations 
of the normal rate of growth under the stimulus of light 
(fig. 239). This record was made with the Balanced Cresco- 
graph, where the normal rate of growth is recorded as a 
horizontal line, enhancement or positive variations of the 
rate being represented by up-curves, and retardation, or 
negative variations, by down-curves. It will be noticed that 


406 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


the first effect of light on this sub-tonic tissue was to induce 
a positive response, followed subsequently by the normal 
negative. Continuous stimulation is seen later to give rise 
to oscillatory responses. 

Having thus shown the continuity between the normal 
negative and positive responses, I give below a record of the 
response to light of a petiole of 
cauliflower (fig. 240), a specimen 
which usually, though not always, 
exhibits galvanometric positivity. 
Each stimulus, by exposure for 
five seconds, was in this case 
applied after an interval of two 
minutes. It should be mentioned 
here that the same tissue which 
gives positive response to the 
moderate stimulus of light will 
Fic. 240. Photographic Re. Show the normal negative when 

cord of Positive Response sybjected to a stronger stimulus, 


of the Petiole of Cauliflower er 
to Light of Five Seconds’ Such as the mechanical. 


OE Honea ee ana The effects studied up to the 
present have consisted of single 

responses induced by light. But this stimulus also induces 
multiple response, as we saw in the oscillatory variations 
of growth in the Crinum lily in fig. 239. The same 
phenomenon is observed in the case of motile response. 
For example, I took a plant of Bzophytum, in which the leaf- 
lets are outspread, in the presence of diffuse light, and threw 
upon it direct sunlight. A series of multiple responses was 
now induced, under the continuous action of stimulus, a 
record of which is given in fig. 241. The up-curves here 
represent excitatory downward movements, and the down- 
curves their partial recoveries. Owing to the incomplete 
character of these recoveries, the leaflets, as the result of a 
series of such responses, are finally closed downwards. In the 
outspread diurnal position the lower half of the pulvinus of the 
leaflet is somewhat more turgid than the upper half, and in 


RESPONSE TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 407 


consequence of repeated responses there is also a relatively 
greater contraction and loss of turgidity in that lower half. 
The leaflets are thus closed by the additive effect of multiple 
normal negative responses or downward movements. 

When a Szophytum plant is kept in the dark the leaflets 
undergo a closure which is outwardly similar to that induced 
by light, but actually arises from a cause precisely opposite. 
Under strong stimulus of light a general contractile effect 
drives the water inwards to the interior of the plant, and, the 
loss of turgor in the lower halves of the pulvini being, as we 
have just seen, greater than in the 
upper brings about the closure 
of the leaflets. But in complete 
darkness the reverse process is set 
up. The water returns to the 
pulvini, making them over turgid. 
Under this condition of excessive 
turgor, however, the upper half of 
the pulvinus becomes more turgid 
than the lower, and, being thus 
rendered more convex, closure of 
the leaflets is brought about. Ex- 
posure to light in this condition 
: Fic. 241. Multiple Mechanical 
induces a greater loss of turgor by Response of Leaflet of 
the upper than by the lower half, Biophyium under the Con. 

: tinuous Action of Light 

and we consequently obtain the 

outspread or erected position of the leaflets by a series of 
small positive movements. Thus, as the result of internal 
changes, the response of an organ may undergo reversal 
from the ordinary negative to positive. The change from 
light to darkness, by which we have here seen the character 
of response to be so greatly modified, occurs in the diurnal 
periodic alternation. And we can see that, in consequence 
of such an imparted periodicity, an alternation of phase is 
impressed upon the organism, which will carry with it a 
periodic variation of excitability. If this diurnal periodicity 
acted alone its after-effect would be comparatively simple, 


408 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


but it is complicated by other periodicities, such as that of 
temperature variation, which do not always coincide in 
maxima and minima with these variations of light. 

We have seen from mechanical indications that multiple 
excitations are induced by light. We have, therefore, to 
determine whether similar multiple excitations can be detected 
electrically. Fig. 242 is a photographic record of a series of 
such electrical responses ob- 
tained from the lamina of 
a leaf of Bryophyllum sub- 
jected to the continuous 
action of light. We see here 
an alternation of phase in 
the response, negative being 
followed by positive in each 
case throughout the series. 
This constitutes a parallel 

case to that of the mechanical 
Fic. 242. Photographic Record of ; ; 

Multiple Electrical Response in FeSponse in fig. 241, inasmuch 

ue keer Con- as, owing to incomplete re- 

covery from each negative 
phase, the base line is gradually tilted upwards. But this 
need not always occur, for the two phases may be equal, and 
in that case the base line remains horizontal. 

We have seen, in the chapter on Multiple and Autono- 
mous Response, that these effects are due to the absorption 
of an excess of energy. When this absorption is great the 
energy may find an outward expression, even after the 
cessation of the stimulus. An example of this was seen in 
the mechanical response of a Desmodium \eaflet (fig. 141): 
The plant was at first in a sub-tonic condition, and the auto- 
nomous pulsation of its lateral leaflets had come to a stand- 
still. One of these was now exposed to the continuous 
action of light and its record taken. It will be noticed that 
under this stimulus of light multiple responses were initiated, 
which persisted for a time as an after-effect, even on the ces- 
sation of light. In taking electrical records of the after-effect 


RESPONSE TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 409 


of stimulation by light, I have obtained similar multiple 
after-effects. And in this connection I have discovered certain 
characteristic peculiarities of the electrical after-effect which 
would appear to throw much light on the obscure subject 
of after-effects in the retina. The electrical after-effect of 
stimulus of light varies greatly in different conditions of the 
tissue, but is capable of classifica- 
tion under three different types. ‘ / 

The first of these types refers - 
to those multiply responding tis- 
sues which give the usual single 
response of galvanometric nega- 


tivity under short exposure to | 

light. When such specimens are a ‘a at 
subjected to the continuous action \ 

of this stimulus they give multiple \ 
responses whose phases alternate e\ 


in the sequence of *mznus-plus- 
minus-plus (—+—-+). Confining 


Fic. 243. Diagrammatic Repre- 


our attention to the first two pairs 
of such phasic alternations under 
continuous stimulation of light 
(fig. 243), we observe that, in the 
first period, excitatory . negativity 
attains its maximum at J, after 
which the phase becomes reversed 
to positive, in which process the 
curve may arrive at the original 
base line, or stop short of this or 
go beyond it. The maximum of 


sentation of Phasic Alter- 
nations, and After-effect in 
Type I. 


During action of light, the 


phasic. alternations are a 4, 
ba',a' b’,o'a' (—+—+); 
6 is here the maximum 
negative, and a’ the maxi- 
mum positive. Withdrawa 
of stimulus at point of re- 
versal a’ causes unmasking 
of positive component, which 
is exhibited by overshooting 
of the curve in the positive 


_ direction, @’ c. 


this phase, a’, we shall designate as maximum positivity. 
Under the still-continued action of the stimulus the phase 
now changes once more to negative, and so on. 

It has already been stated (p. 100) that these periodic 
alternations in phase were brought about by antagonistic 


reactions becoming effectively predominant by turns. 


Thus, 


as was there pointed out, in those cases which normally give 


410 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


negative response, the internal energy may be so increased 
by the continuous absorption of impinging stimulus as to 
induce a continuously increasing antagonistic reaction of 
positivity. Hence, in such cases, negativity is gradually 
diminished, and ultimately reversed. After the attainment 
of maximum positivity, however, the negative element once 
more becomes predominant. 

With a fresh specimen, exhibiting multi-phasic responses 
under the continuous action of light, I find it easy to obtain 
a convincing demonstration of the presence of the positive 
factor by a sudden cessation of stimulus at an appropriate 
moment. If the stimulus be stopped at 4, the increasing 
internal energy and natural tendency to recovery will con- 
spire with each other, and the after-effect will, generally 
speaking, be 6a’. That is to say, this effect is the same as 
that of natural recovery. 

When the second or positive phase has reached its maxi- 
mum a’, the response under the continued action of stimu- 
lation is once more reversed to negative a’ J’, as we have 
seen. At this point of reversal a’, the positive, is balanced 
by the negative. If then at this point the impinging stimu- 
lus be suddenly withdrawn, the positive element, finding 
itself unopposed, will overshoot the line of balance, and the 
curve will proceed in the positive direction towards c (fig. 243). 
This phasic sequence (Type I.) then, during stimulation, and 
as an after-effect of it, should thus be (— + -:-), the dotted 
sign representing the after-effect. That this is actually so, 
is seen in the following series of photographic records 
(fig. 244), where the dotted portion of the record exhibits 
the after-effect. The intensity of this positive after-effect 
varies with the freshness and vigour of the specimen. The 
influence of fatigue, in the gradual diminution of the effect, 
is seen in the two subsequent series (4) and (c) obtained 
from the same specimen. This diminution culminates in 
the gradual diminution of the positive after-effect, and its 
conversion into a negative after-effect, as seen in (d). 

We thus arrive at an intermediate case (Type II.), exem- 


RESPONSE TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 411 


plified by specimens which are not very fresh. The sequence 
ishere(— + ...). In some instances, if the stimulus be stopped 
at the end of the first negative phase, we obtain a small 
increment of negativity as the after-effect. The sequence 
here, then, is (— ...). 


Fic. 244. Photographic Record of Phasic Alternations, showing Direct 
and After Effects of Light in Type I., represented by Bryophyllum 


a: First record of the series. Positive after-effect represented by. dotted 
curve is here strong. 4andc: Less strong positive after-effects due to 
fatigue. Inc light was stopped slightly beyond the second phase of 
maximum positivity; d, owing to fatigue, after-effect converted into 
negative. Dotted portions of curve represent, as usual, the after-effect 
on cessation of light. 


Turning to Type III., we take specimens whose charac- 
teristic response to short-lived stimulus of light is by 
galvanometric positivity, the multiple phases in which, under 
its continuous action, may be expected to_be in the se- 
quence represented in figure 232, that is tosay(+ — + —) 
As the responses here are the exact opposite of those in 


412 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Type I., we may expect to obtain the unmasking of the nega- 
tive element in the response, as the after-effect, on the with- 
drawal of stimulus. Here there should be cessation of stimulus 
at the end of the second phase, in this case, maximum mznus. 
The negative element, thus freed from its opposing positive, 
will demonstrate its presence by over-shooting. in the 
negative direction (fig. 245). This 
is seen in the next two figures 
(246 and 247). The specimen 
; taken was the petiole of cauli- 
flower, in that particular condition 
which gives the positive as its 
immediate response. The _alter- 
nation of plus-minus-plus-minus, 
under continuous stimulation, is 
seen in the first part of fig. 246. 
Stimulus was now stopped ata 
point short of the maximum nega- 
tivity, and we see the consequent 
overshooting in the negative 
direction. In the next figure 
(fig. 247), is seen a single alter- 
nation of p/us-minus during stimu- 
lus, in a different specimen, with 


ue eS 


FIG. 245. Diagrammatic Repre- 
sentation of Phasic Alter- 
nations and After Effect in 
Type III. 


During action of light the phasic 
alternations are a 0, b a’, 
a’ b', b’ a" (+—+-—);3 is 
here the maximum positive, 


and a’ the maximum nega- 
tive. Withdrawal of stimulus 
at point of reversal a’ 
causes unmasking of negative 
element, which is exhibited 
by the overshooting of the 
curve in the negative direc- 
tion, a’ ¢. 


its after-effect and recovery from 
that effect. The impinging stimu- 
lus was in this second case stopped 
at the exact point of maximum 
negativity, and the overshooting 


curve shows, by its abrupt steep- 
ness, its sudden freedom from the restraint imposed by the 
opposite element of positivity. Thus, in this instance of 
Type III., we arrive at the typical formula of (+ — ...). 
It may then be said that under this head, of alternation of 
phase, we have examined representative cases of the two ex- 
treme Types I. and III., between which lie many variations, 
classified as intermediate, or Type II. 


RESPONSE TO STIMULUS‘*OF LIGHT 413 


It has thus been seen that the electrical response of 


plants to light is not essentially different from their response 


to other forms of stimulus. The various effects observed 
are the electrical concomitants of 


those excitatory effects which we 
had already seen to be exhibited 
in mechanical response. In a 


ft 


ae se 
ee ee ee 


re te spe see ee en te A ne en ao 
i 
Sa 
- 
entice 


i 

| 

| 
/ 


som pga saCe é 


Fic. 246. Photographic Record of Phasic 
Alternation, showing Direct and After 
Effects in Type III., represented by with a Second Specimen of 
Petiole of Cauliflower Cauliflower, representative of 

Continuous line represents effect during appli- Type III. 
cation of light. Stimulus was withdrawn 
slightly before the attainment of the second 
maximum negativity, resulting in over- 
shooting of curve in negative direction. 
Dotted portion of record represents after- 
effect on cessation of light, 


FiG. 247. Photogiaphic Record 
of Pair of Responses obtained 


The stimulus of light was in 
these cases withdrawn exactly 
on the attainment of the 
negative maximum. Dotted 
portions of the record ex- 
hibit the after-effect. 


highly excitable tissue the direct effect gives rise to galvano- 
metric negativity. The indirect effect of stimulus, however, 
is one of galvanometric positivity. Positive response may 
also be obtained from a tissue which is not highly ex- 


414 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


citable or in a sub-tonic condition or fatigued. The pre- 
sence or absence of chlorophyll is not a determining factor 
in electrical response. As in the case of mechanical and 
growth responses, so also in the electrical, continuous stimu- 
lation gives rise to multiple responses, as its direct or after- 
effect. ‘The sequence of phases, under the direct action of 
continuous stimulation, may be (— + — +) or(+ — + —). 

Taking into account both the direct and after-effect, the 
observed results may be classified under three types. In. 
the first type, that of fresh and highly excitable tissues, the 
formula is(— + -i-). In the second or intermediate type, it 
is (— + ...) or (— ...). And in the third type we have 
the formula (+ -— ...). 


CHAPTER XXVIII 
RESPONSE OF RETINA TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 


Response of retina—Determination of true current of rest—Determination o: 
differential excitabilities of optic nerve and cornea, and optic nerve and 
retina —The so-called positive variation of previous observers indicates the 
true excitatory negative—Retino-motor effects—Motile responses in nerve— 
Varying responsive effects under different conditions—Reversal of the normal ° 
response of light due to (1) depression of excitability below par ; (2) fatigue— 
The sequence of responsive phases during and after application of light— 
Demonstration of multiple responses in retina under light, as analogous to 
those in vegetable tissues—Three types of after-effect—Multiple after- 
excitations in human retina—Binocular Alternation of Vision—Demonstra- 
tion of pulsatory response in human retina during exposure to light. 


THE nature of the electrical reaction of the retina under 
stimulus of light constitutes a problem which has attracted 
many investigators, chief and earliest among these being 
Holmgren, Kiihne and Steiner, Dewar and McKendric. The 
results obtained by these workers have been confirmed by 
the later researches of others; but while their general observa- 
tions are fairly concordant, the way in which the phenomena 
they have described are related to the excitatory reaction is a 
question which has hitherto remained undetermined. 
Observed results, in fact, would seem to show that the 
electrical reactions of the retina are of a nature quite different 
from those exhibited by other animal tissues. For whereas 
nerve or muscle, for example, responds on excitation by 
negative variation, the retina, under the same normal con- 
ditions, is said to exhibit a positive variation. The subject 
is very. much complicated, moreover, by the confusion which 
has unfortunately arisen as to the meaning of the terms 
positive and negative. These positive and negative varia- 
tions are so named in reference to the existing current ot 


416 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


rest, which, as we have already seen, does not constitute a 
very definite or unvarying standard. A still further source 
of complication is introduced again when, under certain 
internal changes in the retina itself, the normal response is 
found to be reversed. 

The present inquiry, then, resolves itself into the follow- 
ing questions :—(1) What is the true current of rest, and do 
the various currents which have been observed really fall 
under this head or not? (2) What is the galvanometric 
character, positive or negative, of the excitatory reaction of 
the retina—that is to say, is the excitatory phenomenon in 
the eye similar to, or different from, that of other tissues? 
' (3) May we not discover, in the case of the retina, those mul- 
tiple excitatory reactions which we have already found to be 
induced by light in vegetable tissues? (4) And, lastly, what 
are the after-effects of this particular stimulus in the retina? 

Of these we shall deal first with the question of the direction 
of the current of rest. In an eyeball which is isolated entire 
it has been found that the nerve is negative to the cornea, 
and the current of rest is therefore believed to flow through 
the eye from nerve to cornea. In an isolated nerve-retina 
preparation, on the other hand, the rod-surface of the retina 
is negative to the nerve, the current of rest being thus from 
retina to nerve. These observed currents, however, may not 
be true currents of rest, but rather excitatory after-effects, 
due to injury in preparation. In such cases we have seen 
that the naturally more excitable becomes persistently nega- 
tive. We have also found that the true current of rest flows 
from the less to the more excitable, the latter being thus 
galvanometrically positive. In order, therefore, to determine 
the true nature of the resting-current, we have first to deter- 
mine which of each two surfaces, nerve and cornea, and 
nerve and retina, is the more excitable. 

I have already described how, by means of equi-alter- 
nating electrical shocks, the differential excitability of a 
preparation can be determined. We saw also that under 
such conditions the resulting responsive current flows from 


RESPONSE OF RETINA. TO STIMULUS.OF LIGHT 417 


the more to the less excitable. The experimental arrange- 
ment used in this investigation is shown in fig. 248. In such 
an experiment, equi-alternating shocks are given to the’ pre- 
paration by means of a secondary coil included in the circuit. 
The existing current, from nerve to cornea, may be balanced 
previously by a potentiometer. Whether the record be taken 
under balanced or under ordinary conditions, it is found that 
in anormal eyeball the responsive current is from the nerveto 
the cornea, showing that the nerve 
is relatively the more excitable of 
the two. Fig. 249 gives a series 
of such responses. It will also 
be noticed that the excitatory 
current is in the same direction 


>» { 


>R 
Fic. 248. Experimental 
'. Arrangement for Deter- 
mination of Differential © 
_. Excitability of Optic — ; 
Nerve and Cornea Fic. 249. Series of Photographic Records of 
‘Excitatory Responses in Frog’s Eye to 


Equi-alternating Electric Shocks at Inter- 
_ vals of One Minute 


C, injury current, or so-called 
current of rest; R, re- 
sponsive current. 

Current of response from nerve to cornea. 


as the existing current, and thus constitutes a positive varia- 
tion of it. Thzs positive variation of the current in the eyeball 
thus indicates a true excitatory reaction of the optic nerve. 
From the fact that the nerve is more excitable than the 
cornea, it is clear that the sectioning of it for isolation of the 
eyeball, acting as an intense stimulus, will result in its excita- 
tory negativity, which will persist for a time and slowly dis- 
appear. Owing to this fact, a current flows from the more 
excited nerve to the less excited cornea. That this current 
E E 


—A18 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


is not the true resting-current, but rather an excitatory effect 
consequent on preparation, is supported by the known fact 
that it undergoes a decline more or less rapid. On the other 
hand, from the fact just demonstrated that the nerve is the 
more excitable, we should expect that, under natural or 
primary conditions—that is to say, inthe absence of excitatory 
disturbance—the resting-current would be from the less to the 
more excitable, or in other words, from cornea to nerve, This | 
conclusion I was able to verify by carefully dissecting away 
the socket of the frog’s eye, and 
making connections with the 
longitudinal surface of the un- 
detached nerve and the cornea. 
Under these ideal conditions 
the true resting-current was 
detected, and was, as expected, 
from the cornea to the nerve. 

In order, next, to determine 
whether the current, observed to 
flow, in a nerve-retina prepara- 
< tion from retina to nerve, is a 
‘ee true: resting-current, or merely 


C 
R 
Fic. 250, Experimental Arrange- an excitatory after-effect, I pro- 


ment for Demonstration of ‘ i 
Differential Excitability as bee ceeded to determine, in the 


tween Retina and Optic Nerve — manner already described, which 


c, so-called current of rest; R, re- 


sponsive current. of the two surfaces was the more 


excitable. Under the excita- 
tory effect of equi-alternating shocks, the responsive current 
was found to flow from the retina to the nerve, thus 
proving that the retina was the more excitable (fig. 250). 
The first series of responses in fig. 251 gives a record of these 
effects with a moderate intensity of stimulation, while the 
second series shows the responses under an intensity nearly 
twice as great. The so-called current of rest observed in the 
nerve-retina preparation is thus to be taken as due to that 
after-effect of preparation which is inseparable from the 
isolation of such a highly excitable tissue. /¢ well also be 


RESPONSE OF RETINA TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 4I9 


noticed that the true excitatory effect on the retina ts tn the same 
direction as the existing injury-current, and thus constitutes 
a positive variation of tt. . 
Thus, as regards forms of stimulus other than light, such 
as, for example, the electrical, the responsive reaction of the 
retina is by induced galvanometric negativity. This at once 
disposes of the doubt that the general reaction of the retina 
might be of a different sign from that of any other tissue. 
But we have still to determine whether or not the stimulus 
of light, in particular, induces the same normal negative 
change, The next question to be taken up, then, is that of 
the true nature of the responsive variation induced in the 


Fic. 251. Series of Photographic Records of Excitatory Responses in Frog’s 
Retina to Equi-alternating Electric Shocks at Intervals of One Minute 


Responsive current from retina to nerve. First series show response to 
stimulus of moderate intensity ; the second series to stimulus of intensity 
nearly twice as great. 


retina by light. We have seen that the effect recorded as 
normal by numerous observers, whether in the eyeball or in 
the isolated retina (Kiihne and Steiner) was a positive varia- 
tion. But since we now know that retinal response to stimu- 
lation in general is not unlike that of other tissues, and since, 
with regard to the stimulus of light'in particular, we have 
seen it induce true excitation in vegetable tissues, we might 
expect the responsive reaction of the retina to light to take 
place by galvanometric negativity. We must then accept 
one of two conclusions. Either the positive change is a 
misnomer for the phenomenon observed in the eye, or the 
inference which we have drawn from the analogy of vegetable 
tissues is not justified. 


420 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


As, in reference to this latter point, however, it may be 
urged that the retina is exceptionally sensitive to light, 
while the reactions of vegetable tissues are generally slug- 
gish, it may be worth while to point out here that vegetable 
tissues are not so insensitive as is generally supposed, but 
are often, on the contrary, highly susceptible to the: action of 
light. It. was, for instance, found by Darwin that the coty- 
ledons of Phalaris canariensts were, in the course of some _ 
hours’ exposure, curved towards a small lamp, placed ata 
distance from them of 12 feet.. The intensity of the stimulus 
of light was in this case extremely feeble. I have myself 
observed, again, the remarkable sensitiveness to light of the 
terminal leaflet of Desmodztum,.which, on the mere striking 
of a match in its vicinity, was thrown into a state of pulsatory 
movement. 

I shall, in the course of the present chapter, describe 
certain definite effects on the retina, which will prove, in an 
- unmistakable manner, that, when exposed to light, it under- 
goes a change of galvanometric negativity. It is now there- 
fore necessary to show clearly that what has been described 
as the positive variation is really due to the excitatory nega- 
tive change of the retina. What has to be demonstrated, 
then, is the way in which this simple underlying reaction of 
negativity comes to appear as a positive variation of the 
opposite-directioned currents of rest in the a and 
isolated retina respectively. 

As regards experiments on the eyeball, with contacts at 
nerve and cornea, we have seen that. the existing injury- 
current is from nerve to cornea, and that this undergoes a 
positive variation when the nerve is stimulated in any way. 
This is because the added. negativity induced in the nerve 
gives rise to an increase, or positive variation of, the existing 
current (fig. 248). Now, when light falls on the eye it -acts 
on both the cornea andthe retina. The former, however, 
is relatively inexcitable, especially to so moderate a stimulus 
as that of light. But the retina is excited, and its excitatory 
condition is rapidly transmitted to the optic nerve, which 


eee 


RESPONSE OF RETINA TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 421. 


thus becomes galvanometrically negative. The so-called 
positive» variation observed under such an experimental 
arrangement, then, is really the result of the true excitation 
of the retina conducted to the optic nerve. 

In experiments- on nerve-and-retina preparations, with 
contacts on the nerve and the retina, the existing injury- 
current is from retina to nerve.- When do¢/ nerve and retina 
are excited simultaneously, by equi-alternating electrical 
shocks, we have seen that, on account of the greater ex- 
citability of the retina, the resultant responsive current, here 
differential. is from retina to nerve, constituting a positive 
variation of the existing injury-current (p. 419). In the case 
of stimulation by light, however, it is the retina which is directly 
excited. The added negativity thus induced in the retina 
gives rise to an increase or positive variation of the existing 
injury-current (fig. 250). Hence, in all these cases, the so- 
called positive variation really indicates the normal excitatory 
negative effect. The apparent anomaly involved in the 
supposition that the response of the retina to light was 
positive, and thus of opposite character to that of other ex- 
citable tissues, is thus seen to be due to a misinterpretation 
of observed results. It is unfortunate that, as a consequence 
of this misinterpretation, the effects described by different 
observers, of the response of the eye as ‘ positive,’ are really 
to. be understood as ‘negative, and vice versa. When 
quoting these results, therefore, I shall always give the actual 
effect indicated in italics and in parentheses, 

Another observation which lends independent support 
to the view that the retina exhibits the true excitatory re- 
action, is found in the fact—noted by Van Genderen-Stort 
and Engelmann—that the cones exhibit a motile effect by 
retraction under light. It has. been supposed from this that 
the optic nerve contains not merely sensory, but also retino- 
motor fibres. But I shall show that it.is not that the endings 
only of the optic nerve, but also that nerve itself, which 
exhibit true excitatory contraction under stimulation (cf. 
figs. 324,404). All nerves, in fact, will be shown in a later 


422 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


chapter to exhibit normal excitatory contraction, and it will 
be by the study of these motile responses in nerves, and their 
variations under different condition, that we shall be able 
unerringly to relate the abnormal responses sometimes seen 
in the retina to those changes of condition to which they are 
due. : 
With regard to these abnormal responses, I have already 
shown that various tissues exhibit them under the two 
different conditions of sub-tonicity and fatigue. With regard 
to nervous tissue in particular, however, I may refer here, 
by anticipation, to results which will be given in detail 
in Chapter XXXV. concerning the mechanical response 
of nerve and its variations. In normal conditions of ex- 
citability the nerve gives response by contraction, and this 
is its true excitatory response, concomitant to the electrical 
response of galvanometric negativity. This excitatory re- 
sponse, then, whether by contraction or by negativity, will 
here, as in preceding chapters, be designated ‘negative.’ 
We have seen that the maintenance of the normal condition 
of a highly excitable tissue depends on its supply of energy. 
Hence, when such a tissue is isolated from the organism of 
which it forms a part, it is liable to fall below its normal 
tonic level. This depressed condition, however, does not 
connote any permanent chemical depreciation, but only a 
temporary depression of its fund of energy. Under such 
an induced lowering of the tonic condition, the response is 
reversed to positive. But under continuous stimulation, 
excitability is again enhanced, and the abnormal positive is 
converted to normal negative. 

Thus we obtain, in a somewhat sub-tonic tissue, the 
following results : 

(1) Under a short-lived or instantaneous stimulus, whose 
effective value falls below the true excitatory level, response 
is positive (expansion), 

(2) Under the continuous action of stimulus the effective 
value is at first below, but after a time rises above, excitatory 
efficiency. In consequence of this we obtain a first phase of 


RESPONSE OF RETINA TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 423 . 


response which is positive, followed by a second which is 
negative (expansion followed by contraction). 

The second condition to induce a reversal of the normal 
response occurs in consequence of the fatigue due to previous 
over-stimulation. 

(3) A reversed positive response (expansion) in conse- 
quence of fatigue. | | 

The various anomalies which occur in the response of the 
eye will all be found resolvable into one or other of these 
cases. I shall first describe certain experiments which will 
demonstrate the reversal that is brought about by induced 
sub-tonicity. We have seen that the normal response of an 
eyeball, with two contacts at nerve and cornea, consists 
of a current from the nerve to the cornea. I have already 
given records of such normal responses, obtained by sub- 
jecting the preparation to equi-alternating electric shocks 
(fig. 249). In certain cases, however, in which the isolated 
eyeball of the frog had fallen into a sub-tonic condition, the 
response was found to be reversed, the nerve, under excita- 
tion, becoming relatively positive instead of normally nega- 
tive. It has already been said that such a reversal is due to 
great depression in the condition of the nerve. In dealing 
with these cases, therefore, it occurred to me that it ought to 
be possible to restore the normal response by the application 
of some exciting reagent—say, dilute Na,CO,—to the nerve 
in its depressed condition. As the result of this application 
I found the reversed response to be restored to the normal. 

I obtained results precisely similar to these with isolated 
retina of the frog. The normal responses—a current from 
retina to nerve (fig. 238)—under equi-alternating shocks, 
were here found, in a depressed specimen, to be reversed. 
But the application of Na,CO, solution on the retinal surface 
brought the responses back to the normal. These abnormal 
responses, then, rectifiable to normal, are those due to sub- 
tonicity. And under fatigue finally, induced by long- 
continued, or over stimulation, I find the normal response 
of the eye to be reversed. 


a2a COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


This is the place in which to refer to various anomalous 
effects observed. by previous investigators, and to offer satis- 
factory explanations of them from the results which I have 
already demonstrated. I shall therefore give a brief sum- 
mary of these from the admirable account of Biedermann. 

(a) ‘In light, frogs—z.e, such as have been exposed for 
hours to the full effect of daylight—the positive fore-swing 


of the negative variation concomitant~with the impact of © 


light is entirely wanting, or appears as a trace only.’ ‘ Since 
this posztzve varzation of the existing current really means, as 
I have already shown, the ‘true excitatory negative, and the 
negative variation conversely, the occurrence of positive 
response, this observation means that a frog’s eye, previously 
exposed for a long time to light, gives positive response. 

This, then, is a simple instance of the reversal of response 
from negative to positive under fatigue, which I have already 
dealt with above as case (3). 

(0) ‘The fact that the three phases of the retinal action 
current, due to transitory illumination, appear in sensitive 
preparations, even when, as with the electric spark, the 
impact of light is momentary, shows that the medium nega- 
tive (fosttzve) phase must not be regarded merely as the 
consequence of permanent illumination, since it is just this 
phase which alone appears in less excitable preparations with 
instantaneous light stimuli.’ 

‘With regard to this it may be said that the medium 
positive phase here referred to, as given by. the excitable 
retina under continuous stimulation, is not the same as that 
positive response which ‘alone appears in less excitable pre- 
parations with instantaneous light stimuli. The former is 
due to fatigue-reversal, while the latter is an instance of the 
abnormal positive response of a sub-tonic tissue to feeble 
stimulus. This will be understood from the following con- 
siderations. In the retina, under continuous stimulation, the 
first phase of response is the true excitatory negative. This 
gives place to a second, or positive, due to _fatigue-decline. 

' Biedermann, Zéectro- Physiology (English translation), vol. ii. pp. 474-477. 


RESPONSE OF RETINA TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 425 - i g 
And this is again succeeded bya transitory negative effect 

on the sudden cessation of: light. These constitute the three 

phases of retinal action-current just referred to, under con- 

tinuous stimulus. This sequence is wrongly represented in 
symbols as (+ — +), since the actual changes concerned are 
(—+-—). ; . 

Now in highly excitable. tissues, under instantaneous 
stimulation, we observe a sequence of response apparently 
similar, the first being normal_ negative, the second positive, 
owing- partly to recovery and: partly to the positive after- 
effect ; and the last phase representing a return from this 
positive. These three phases, therefore, though apparently 
similar, are not really the same as those just referred to 
under continuous stimulation, where the ‘medium negative 
(postteve) phase’ was the result of fatigue-reversal. The so- 
called negative (fosztive) effect which alone appears on the 
instantaneous stimulation of relatively inexcitable tissues is, 
again, the positive response of a sub-tonic tissue to a stimulus 
deficient in true excitatory value, already described (p. 422) 
as case (1). Similar positive responses of vegetable tissues 
in a sub-tonic condition under the action of light were seen 
in fig. 240. 

We come next to the question whether or not we may 
discover multiple responses in the retina analogous to those 
which have already been demonstrated in the case of vege- 
table tissues. The occurrence of such an effect has not 
hitherto been suspected. We have seen that, under the 
continued action of light, vegetable tissues exhibit multiple 
responses ; and since we have found a general close analogy 
to exist between the responses to light of the retina and of 
these, we should expect that similar multiple responses would 
be found in the eye also. The reason why these were not 
hitherto detected lay in the inevitable depression of excit- 
ability in the isolated retina or eyeball. In the retina of certain 
fishes, where the excitability does not appear to decline so 
rapidly, I have often obtained records of multiple responses 
For example, in the retina of Wallago attu fish the stimulus of ° 


nee’ COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


light applied for three seconds gave as its after-effect multiple 
responses which lasted for ten minutes, the average period of 
each oscillation being twenty seconds. I have also obtained 
such multiple responses from the eye of vigorous bull-frogs, 
a photographic record of one of these results being given in 
fig. 252. We shall also see at the end of this chapter that 
it is quite easy to detect the occurrence of these multiple 
responses in the intact human eye under stimulus of light. 

As we have seen that multiple response is the expression 
of energy previously absorbed, and held latent in the tissue 
for a time, and since, as has been stated, the retina itself 
exhibits multiple response, it is easy to see that this organ, 
on the cessation of light, will show after-effects. 

In my experiments on the effects 
of light on vegetable tissues I found, 
as has already been said, three 
different types of direct- and after- 
effect. The first of these related to 
those highly excitable tissues which 
under continuous stimulation gave 

normal responses (—~+—+). In 
Fic. 252.—Photographic Re- gych cases, if the stimulus was 
cord of Multiple Response ; 

of Retina of Frog under stopped on the attainment of 

Continuous Action of Light 14.imum positivity the immediate 
after-effect was an increase of positivity. The formula 
was thus (—+ ++). In the third type, with sub-tonic 
tissues, the sequence, under continuous stimulation, was 
(+ —-+-—), and on the stoppage of stimulation at maximum 
negativity this negativity became suddenly augmented. The 
formula here was thus (+ —---). In the second or inter- 
mediate type, again, the formula of the direct and after- 
effects was either (— + -+-) or (— -:-). 

I shall now discuss in some detail the various types of 
after-effects met with in the retina, corresponding, as I shall 
be able to show, with those met with in vegetables tissues. 
After-effects like those of Type I., had not hitherto been 
noticed in the retina for the reason that their demonstration 


‘RESPONSE OF RETINA TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT —<— | 


can only be obtained in a tissue of normal high excitability, 
and not in one which has undergone depression in con- 
sequence of isolation. I was fortunate enough, however, to 
meet with a species of fish, Ophiocephalus marulius, whose 
vitality is so exceptional that it lives for days when taken 
out of water. When the fish is pithed, its heart continues 
beating vigorously for many hours. I made a preparation 
of the eye of this fish and carried out experiments on it 
under the action of light. | 

In fig. 254 is given a record obtained with this specimen 
during the application of light and on its cessation. It will 


FIG. 253. FIG. 254. FIG. 255. 


FIGs. 253, 254, 255. Parallel Records of Responses given by Plant and 
Retina, during and after Illumination, illustrative of Type I 
(— + +). In all these cases up-curve represents induced galvano- 
metric negativity ; down-curves, positivity. White background in this 
and following records represent light, and shaded, darkness. 


Fig. 253. Response of petiole of Aryophylium. Light was cut 
off on attainment of maximum positivity in the second of 
the multiple responses. 

Fig. 254. Similar effect in response of retina of Ophzocephalus fish. 

Fig. 255. The same with another specimen. Light. was here 
cut off after the first oscillation. 


be seen that during the application of light the sequence was 
(—+—+). It will be noticed that, after completing two 
oscillations, and after the response-curve was even slightly 
reversed at its maximum positive phase the light was with- 
drawn. The immediate effect was a sudden increase of posi- 
tivity, followed by a series of after-effect oscillations. In the 
next figure (255), obtained with a different specimen of the 
same fish, light was withdrawn at the exact moment of 
maximum positivity, and the result is seen to be similar to 
the last—namely, an immediate enhancement of positivity, 


- 


ageiRfe. 428 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


followed by an after-oscillation. The essential similarities 
between these and corresponding records obtained on a  fast- 
moving drum, of the response of the petiole of Sik wi det 
under light (fig. 253), are sufficiently obvious. 

When the excitability of the tissue is not so high, we 
may obtain after-effects of Type II., in which the formula is 
—-+-+--) or (—---). This was exemplified in vegetable 
tissues (cf. fig. 245 @).. In Ophziocephalus, 1 was able to obtain — 
this result also, when the specimen was slightly fatigued 
(fig. 256). With the eye of the frog, Kiihne and Steiner 
obtained .the record given in fig. 257, which is seen to be 
parallel to that given in fig. 256, its true significance being 
shown in the formula (— +---). 


| 
Fic. 256. FIG. 257. 


Fics. 256, 257. Parallel Records given by Plant and Eye; during and 
after Illumination, as illustrative of Intermediate Type II. (— + ---) 


Fig. 256. _ Response of retina of Ophzocephalus when slightly 
fatigued. 
Fig. 257. Response of frog’s eye (Kiithne and Steiner). 

A sub-case of Type II. is represented again, by (—-+--), 
where on the sudden cessation of light, there is a transient 
increase of excitatory response, This, as we saw, was due to 
the abrupt withdrawal of the antagonistic influence of a 
reversing force. I may state here that I have been able to 
demonstrate an exactly parallel effect with nerve of frog, 
where the excitatory negative effect during stimulus under- 
goes a brief and sudden augmentation on its cessation (p. 5 36). 
This sudden augmentation on the stoppage of stimulus has 
been taken as a proof of the existence of antagonistic pro- 
cesses of assimilation and dissimilation, rather than as due to 
molecular-derangement by external stimulus and its after-effect. 


RESPONSE OF RETINA TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 429 


That, for its explanation, however, it is not necessary to 
postulate the two processes of assimilation and dissimilation is 
clearly seen from the fact that I have obtained: an exactly 
similar effect in inorganic. substances, 
suchas silver bromide, a record of whose 
response is seen in fig. 258. | 
We next turn to what has been- 
designated as Type III.,-in which the 
sequence of responses, owing to the 
depressed. condition of the -tissue, is. 
reversed, the formula here. being 
(+—+-—), while the direct and after- 
effects are represented by (+ —--:). asiecih —— 
This result was already obtained in the f 
sub-tonic tissue of the petiole of 
cauliflower, a record of this being given 
in fig. 259. Iwas able to detect similar 
effects in a, retina of Ophzocephalus, in 


=> 


oom ee we wee eee eee 


—_—_—_— 


which response had become reversed 
under the sub-tonicity- due to long 
isolation (fig. 260). These results will 
explain the somewhat anomalous re- 
sponse which Kiihne and _ Steiner 
obtained with the isolated retinz of 
certain fishes (fig. 261). 

Returning now from the question 


. rieet . Fic. 258.  After-effect 
of multiple excitations during and after OF bight “on “Silver 
the exposure to light—in prepared speci- Bromide 


mens, where results must. be modified The thick line represents 


response during light 
to.an unknown extent by the effects ~ (half a minute’s ex- 


' 

\ 

' 

t 

' 

' 

' 

' 

' 

' 

1 

\ 

\ 
{ 
\ 
‘ 
‘ 

\ 
\ 
\ 
\ 


<a 


i i Een Ne posure), and dotted 

- pasate — i cade to that line the recovery dur- 

of the responses of the intact. eye, ing darkness. Note 
= t t s ] as © 

where alone we may expect to obtain igh Be eect 


the truly normal effects, we may attack 7 

the problem by means of visual sensation itself. Multiple 
excitation, as the after-effect of strong luminous stimulation, 
is here somewhat easy of demonstration. But its exhibition 


430 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


during the action of light presents unusual experimental 
difficulties. 

It is a well-known fact that if, after looking at a bright 
object for some time, we close the eyes, we see the image 
repeated many times, The occurrence of these after-images 
is somewhat different under a transitory flash of illumination, 
and with more persistent exposure to light. As the visual 
sensation is to be regarded as the excitatory effect, there 
will—owing to the fact that this excitation must reach its 
maximum some time after the cessation of an instantaneous 
exposure—be a persistence of the excitatory after-effect, with 


FIG. 259. FIG, 260, | Fic. 261, 


FIGS. 259, 260, 261. Parallel Records of Responses given by Plant and 
Retin, during and after Illumination, illustrative of Type III. 
(+ —...) 

Fig. 259. Response of petiole of cauliflower. Light was here cut 
off on attainment of maximum negativity. 

Fig. 260. Response of retina of Ophiocephalus fish when de- 
pressed. 

Fig. 261. Response of isolated retina of fish as observed by 
Kiihne and Steiner. 

N.B.—The true excitatory negative response was by these 
observers described as the ‘ positive variation.’ 


its corresponding visual sensation. This is not so, however, 
when the eye is closed after looking at a bright object for a 
considerable time. In this case there is a positive rebound— 
opposite to the true excitatory negative effect—with con- 
comitant sensation of darkness. The next rebound is negative, 
giving rise to a repetition of the original sensation ; and these 
alternating phases may be repeated manytimes. With the eyes 
closed, the negative or luminous phases are the more prominent. 

The same phenomenon may be observed in a somewhat 
different manner when, after: staring at a bright object, we 


RESPONSE OF RETINA TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 431 — 


look towards a well-lighted wall. The dark phases will now 
become the more noticeable. If, however, the wall be dimly 
lighted, both the dark and bright phases will be noticed alter- 
nately. It has been suggested that these phenomena might 
be due to some obscure form of fatigue, but the regular alter- 
nations observed clearly demonstrate them to be a case of 
oscillatory changes and multiple response. _ 

The demonstration of multiple responses in the human 
retina, as an after-effect, is thus seen to be easy. But their 
occurrence during continuous exposure of the eye to light is 
more difficult to prove, This arises from the fact that the 
waxing and waning of the effect are so gradual: as not to be 
noticeable, unless against some definite standard of com- 
parison. I shall now prove that, during the continuance of 
constant light, pulsatory visual effects are produced. These 
pulsations go unnoticed in visual sensation, not only for want 
of a standard of comparison, but also because of the remark- 
able fact which I have discovered, that while the impressions 
of each individual retina undergo variation, the sum total of 
the two remains always approximately constant. I have 
been able to provide the necessary comparison-standards by 
having two distinguishable images produced in the two eyes, 
the fluctuation of the visual excitation in one eye being thus 
capable of detection, by comparison with that in the other. 
It would have been impossible to detect this fluctuation, had 
the excitatory variation taken place in the two eyes simulta- 
neously, ze. if the maximum excitation in the one had 
occurred at the same moment as the maximum excitation in 
the other. But I have found that, as regards excitation 
there is a relative difference of phase, of half a_ period, 
between the two retinz, so that the maximum effect at a 
given moment in one eye corresponds to the minimum in 
the other. This constitutes the phenomenon which I have 
designated the Binocular Alternation of Vision. It is owing 
to this fact that the periodic excitations in each retina are 
unmistakably brought out by the following experiment, 
which consists in looking through a stereoscope that holds, 


432 COMPARATIVE. ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


instead of photographs, incised plates with two inclined cuts, 
the right eye seeing the slit ‘inclined to the right, while the 
left sees that inclined to the left. When. the. observer looks 
through the stereoscope turned to the bright sky, his two 
eyes are acted on by strong stimulus of light through these 
divergently-inclined slits... The resultant sensation: forms the 
image of an inclined cross (fig. 262).. On now closing the 
eyes the multiple after-effect is observed as recurring images. © 
The relative difference of half a period, already referred to, is 
here perceived in a very interesting manner, for the after- 
image is not now the complete inclined cross. Instead of this, 
each of the two arms is seen in regular sequence alternately. 


4 ; | 0) 


. . FIG. 263. Composite Indecipherable 
Inclined Slits ‘Word, of which Components are 
Seen Clearly on Shutting the Eyes 


Fic. 262. 
for Stereoscope and Com- 
posite Image formed in 
the Two Eyes 


That is to say, when the one is at its brightest the other has 
completely disappeared, and vice versa. The impression in 
each eye thus undergoes a periodic fluctuation. Some yery 
curious effects can thus be exhibited, when, instead of the 
two inclined bars, the incisions to be looked through com- 
pose a word. For instance, two letters forming half of the 
word may be seen by one eye, and the other two by the 
other. The result of this is that, as long as the eyes are 
open, we obtain an indecipherable image, due to super- 
position, like the following (fig. 263). » 

But when the eyes are closed, then, owing to dznocular 
alternation of the multiple after-effect, the blur disappears, 
and the word is spelt out in repeated succession in the field 


RESPONSE OF RETINA TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 433, 


of dark vision, as RO ME RO ME, and so on. In this 
curious instance one sees better with eyes closed than open! 

The next problem that presents itself is that of demon- 
strating the pulsatory nature of the visual sensation in each 
eye, under the constant action of light. The stereoscope, 
with its two inclined slits, is now taken and turned towards 
the bright sky, and when the design is looked at steadily it 
will be found that, owing to pulsatory excitation in each eye, 
and the binocular alternation of vision, when one bar of the 
cross begins to be dim the other becomes bright, and vce 
versa. The success of this experiment is determined by the 
fact that the image in each eye forms a comparison-standard 
for the other. And as the changes in the two eyes proceed 
in opposite directions, the visual fluctuations during the con- 
tinuous action of light, are brought out with the greatest 
distinctness. It may be stated here that the period of this 
visual oscillation has an average value of about four seconds. 
It is, generally speaking, shorter with young people, and 
longer with old. Even in the same individual, again, it is 
modified, according as the previous condition has been one of 
rest or activity. I give here a set of readings given by an 
observer : 


| Time of day | Period Time of day | Period 
SAM. . , - 3 seconds 6 P.M. ; | 5°4seconds | 
I2noon . ‘ page OFM ay RG: 755 
3PM. . : | eat II P.M. Z | 6b 


In this connection it may be interesting to mention that 
the period of a single oscillation in a multiple response, 
measured in frog’s retina, under experimental conditions, was 
ten secands. In the case of the retina of Wadllago, the aver- 
age period was twenty seconds. 


CHAPTER XXIX 
GEO-ELECTRIC RESPONSE 


Theory of Hydrostatic Pressure. and Theory of Statoliths—Question regarding 
active factor of curvature in geotropic response, whether contraction or ex- 
pansion— Crucial experiment by local application of cold—Reasons for delay 
in initiation of true geotropic response—Geo-electric response of shoot— 
Due to active contraction of upper side, with concomitant galvanometric 
negativity—Geo-electric response of an organ physically restrained. 


IN the case of the action of external stimulus on plant 
organs it is possible, given the direction of incident uni- 
lateral stimulus, to predict the nature of the responsive 
movement. I have shown elsewhere, in my work on Plant 
Response, that all the actual movements of a plant organ 
can be deduced from the simple law that it is the more 
excited side that becomes concave. But though this law is 
sufficient guide in dealing with the action of a known ex- 
ternal stimulus, yet the problem becomes much more obscure 
when we have to account for any movement which occurs in 
response to a stimulus whose seat is internal. An example 
of this class is afforded in the responsive curvature associated 


with gravity. Thus, a shoot laid horizontally will curve up-. 


wards till the free end becomes vertical. In connection with 
this subject there are two different points to be elucidated. 
First, is the question as to the mode in which gravity exer- 
cises stimulation ; and second, that of the method by which, 
in answer to this stimulus, a definite responsive curvature 
takes place. As regards the first of these, it may be said 
that the only conceivable way in which gravity could produce 
stimulation is by some differential effect of weight acting on 
the responding cells. According to this, the necessary dif- 
ferential weight-effect may be due to the weight of cell- 


. ee oe 


eee | a ey 


GEO-ELECTRIC RESPONSE 435. 


contents, whether of the sap itself or of those heavy particles 
like starch-grains, which are contained in it. The former, or 
Theory of Hydrostatic Pressure, was suggested by Pfeffer 
and supported by Czapek ; the other, or Theory of Stato- 
liths, has been advocated by Noll, Haberlandt, and Nemec. 
In the case of a multicellular plant laid horizontally 
(fig. 264), E and E’ may be regarded as areas in which stimu- 
lation is caused by the 
weight of the particles. 
It is obvious that the 
effects produced on the E Pe ee a eee 
upper and lower sides of 
the shoot are antago- 
nistic;. yet, in spite of 
this, we obtaina resultant &' 


oecvclecceclecocelessosl., 


: ! 1 

curvature upwards. This P a 
- shows that the excitation Fic. 264. Diagrammatic Representation of 
of one side must be a Multicellular Organ laid Horizontally 


and Exposed to Geotropic Stimulus. 
greater than that of the On the upper side the statoliths act on the 
other, the particular direc- inner, and on the lower side on the 


tioneditapedeaieal a eee, age poe wall (after Francis 
being due to this fact. 

It is clear, then, that the induced curvature upwards of the 
horizontally-laid shoot is due to the effective action of the 
weight particles on either the upper or the lower side of the 
shoot. We can see that if it is the upper which is the more 
effective, then curvature must take place by excitatory con- 
traction ; if, on the contrary, the lower be the more effective, 
the curvature is then to be regarded as the result of respon- 
sive expansion. There has been considerable uncertainty as 
to which of these is actually the case, the prevailing view 
being that it is the expansion of the lower surface which is 
the active factor.' 

That it is, however, the excitatory contraction of the 
upper side which is the active factor in this curvature I have 
already demonstrated by alternate unilateral applications of 


1 For more detailed account see Plant Response, pp. 495 to 511. 
’ ; : FF2 


436 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


cold. It is known that the continuous application of cold 
diminishes or abolishes the excitability of a tissue. If, then, 
it is by excitatory action on the upper side that curvature is 
induced, it will be found that the local application of cold on 
that side will retard or arrest this responsive curvature, the 
application of cold on the lower side producing practically no 
effect. If, on the contrary, this curvature should be mainly 
due to some excitatory action on the lower side, we may then 
expect to find that the application of cold on that side has 


FIG. 265. Effect'on Apogeotropic Movement of Temporary Application 
of Cold on Upper and Lower Surfaces respectively 


Application above is seen to produce arrest of movement, while application 
below has no perceptible effect. Ordinate of curve represents up-move- 
ment of tip of organ in mm ; abscissa represents time. 

the effect of arresting the growing curvature, its application 
on the upper being more or less ineffective. On carrying out 
these experiments it was found that cold had the effect of 
arresting curvature only when it was applied on the upper 
side of the shoot (fig. 265). This conclusively proved that 
gravitational stimulus, acting on the horizontally-laid shoot, 
induced response by excitatory contraction of the upper side. 
This fact, of response by contraction, is fully concordant with 
what we know of the effect on the plant of other forms of 
stimulation. This question I intend, however, in the course 


GEO-ELECTRIC RESPONSE 437 


of the present chapter, to submit to independent examina- 
tion by means of electrical response. __ 
I have already shown that . F 

the unilateral pressure of par- 
ticles on the growing organ is 
effective in inducing curvature set | 
in such a way that the side 
acted upon becomes concave. 
This experiment was carried 
out by subjecting one side of 
a growing organ to the pres- 
sure of iron particles, which 
were pressed against it by the pig 266. 


ETE 
LUT 
uu 


Diagrammatic Repre- 


action of an electro-magnet on sentation of Experiment showing 
‘ ‘ Curvature Induced by Unilateral 
the opposite side (fig. 266). Pressure Exerted by Particles 


The magnetic particles in this. F, flower-bud of Crinum; s, india- 


f ti d ; | rubber strip studded with iron 
case tunctione as virtua particles attracted by  electro- 


statoliths, and a curvature was magnet, M, causing palate 
: . ° pressure on growing region ; 1, 
induced by the excited side iodux attached to Hower 


becoming concave (fig. 267). 

There is, however, one difficulty in connection with the 
statolithic theory of stimulation. When the stem is held 
erect the particles rest on the bases of the cells, and their 
general distribution on 
the two sides of the 
organ is symmetrical. 
Asymmetry of distri- 
bution is induced, how- 
ever, when the shoot is 
laid . horizontally, and 
stimulation might beex- Fic. 267. Record of Responsive Curvature 

-.4.-- -Induced in Bud of Crinum Lily by Uni- 
pected to follow within lateral Pressure of Particles 
a very short time, since 
the. displacement of particles from base to side cannot take 
long. This being so, the geotropic curvature of the shoot 
upwards should take place within a short time. But, instead 
of this, we find that the curvature is at first downwards; and | 


438 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


it is not till after the lapse of a period, which is sometimes 
as much as an hour in duration, that there is a reversal of this 
downward movement into the normal apo-geotropic move- 
ment. Fig. 268 gives a curve which exhibits this prelimi- 
nary movement persisting for nearly forty minutes before its 
ultimate reversal into the usual normal apogeotropic curva- 
ture upwards. 

This delay in the appearance of the characteristic response 
may, however, be explained from the known fact that steady 
tension increases, whereas compression retards, the rate of 
growth. Thus, in a horizontally-laid shoot, there is a ten- 
dency to curve down by its own weight, the upper side being 


Fic, 268.. Record of Apogeotropic Response in Scape of Uriclis Lily’ 


The up-curvature due to apogeotropic action proper commenced forty 
minutes after the specimen was laid horizontally. 


in this way subjected to tension, and the lower to compression. 
The effect of these is an increased rate of growth on the 
upper, associated with a decreased rate on the lower, sides of 
the specimen, giving rise to a downward curvature. The fact 
that geotropic stimulus has to overcome this action before its 
own characteristic effect can be exhibited may account for 
the observed delay in its appearance. A crucial experiment 
in support of this explanation will be given presently. 

We have seen in the last chapter that the presence of 
internal excitation is capable of detection by electrical indi- 
cations, and by taking advantage of this fact we have an 
independent means of coping with the obscurities of the 
present problem. I have demonstrated, by means of experi- 
ments already referred to, that it is the upper side which, 


GEO-ELECTRIC RESPONSE 439 


under the action of gravity, undergoes excitatory contraction. 
It follows that, as an indirect effect of such contraction, the 
water expelled from the upper will reach the lower side of 
the shoot, and cause an increased turgidity and expansion of 
that side. The apogeotropic curvature is thus brought about 
directly by the contraction of the upper, and indirectly by the 
expansion of the lower, side of the horizontally-laid shoot. 
This being so, we may expect, in accordance with our pre- 
vious investigations on the true excitatory and hydrostatic 
effects respectively, that that side will become galvano- 
metrically negative which is excited by geotropic stimulus. 
And it is found that it is in fact the upper side of the hori- 
zontally-laid shoot which exhibits galvanometric negativity ; 
the lower, in which the positive turgidity-effect had been 
indirectly induced, being found to show galvanometric posi- 
tivity. 3 
But this result is only obtained simultaneously with the 
induction of the normal apogeotropic curvature. And, before 
this, other disturbing effects may occur. We take an erect 
stem and make two galvanometric contacts, one on each side 
of its growing region. The stem is next laid horizontally. 
As regards the electrical effects which are now exhibited 
three distinct phases are to be distinguished. First, in con- 
sequence of the mechanical disturbance due to the laying of 
the organ on one side, there will be an excitatory electrical 
effect on both upper and lower sides, and the resultant re- 
sponsive current will be determined by the difference of 
excitability which may happen to exist, in this particular 
individual, between the two. This variable stage is termi- 
nated in the course of about ten minutes. We next have a 
steady condition, brought about by the effects of tension and 
compression already described as acting on the upper and 
lower sides respectively. This gives rise, as we have seen, to 
increased growth, with its attendant positive turgidity, on the 
upper as compared with the lower side. | 
Hence, during this stage, when the curvature is proceed- 
ing downwards, we may expect that the upper side will 


440 COMPARATIVE. ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


be galvanometrically positive in relation to the lower, the 
direction of the current in the horizontal shoot being from 
below to above. And finally, in the third stage, we have the 
geotropic stimulation effectively overpowering and reversing 
this downward movement. And since it is the upper side 
that is now geotropically excited to an effective extent, we 
find that that side becomes galvanometrically negative. 
Thus, the electrical indication, like the mechanical, gives 
in this ultimate stage the characteristic response of the 


Fic. 269. Photographic Record of Geo-electric Response in the Scape 
of Uriclis Lily laid horizontally 


In the first phase of response, the current is from the upper surface to the 
lower, the upper being galvanometrically positive. After fifty minutes, 
the excitatory geotropic effect reverses the current, which is now 
ascending, or from below to above, the excited upper-surface being 
galvanometrically negative. _ (Compare corresponding mechanical 
record, fig. 268.) 


plant-tissue to gravitational stimulus. The excitatory 
effect is now exhibited mechanically by the contraction 
and concavity, and electrically by the galvanometric 
negativity of the upper side of the horizontally laid shoot 
(fig. 269). These similarities between the mechanical and 
electrical records will be seen on comparing this figure with 
fig. 268. We have thus seen that, owing to secondary 
mechanical disturbances, the proper exhibition of the true 
geotropic response is delayed. And from this it is difficult 
to say how quickly the geotropic response follows the dis- 


GEO-ELECTRIC RESPONSE 441 


placement of the hypothetical statolithic particles. I have 
been able to overcome this difficulty, which at first appears 
very great, in the following way. It has been shown that 
the state of excitation, even when all mechanical expression 
of it is restrained, may be detected by galvanometric nega- 
tivity. Those secondary effects, due to mechanical dis- 
turbance, which mask for a time the excitatory effect of 
gravitational stimulus, may thus be eliminated completely by 
restraining all movement of the shoot. The problem thus 
resolves itself into the fixing of an experimental shoot, say 
the peduncle of Uviclis lily—in such a way that mechanical 


Fic, 270, Experimental Arrangement for Subjecting Organ to Geotropic 
Stimulus, Mechanical Response being Restrained 


response is completely restrained. The next point is to 
subject the specimen, at a given moment, to the stimulus of 
gravity, and record the consequent electric response. 

I shall now describe the experimental method by which 
these conditions were successfully met. It is clear that when 
any two points are acted on symmetrically by the force of 
gravity, there is no resultant geotropic action. This is the 
case in regard to two diametrically opposite points, A and B, 
situated laterally on an erect shoot. When the shoot is laid 
horizontally, two lateral points are again acted on sym- 
metrically by. the force of gravity, and there is thus xo differ- 
ential action as. between the two. But if the shoot be now 
rotated on itself,.so that one of these points is diametrically 


442 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


above, and the other below, a differential action will be 
induced as between the upper and lower sides, the upper 
being relatively the more excited. In the following ex- 
periment I took a specimen of Uvzclzs lily, and fixed the 
entire plant horizontally, as seen in the figure (fig. 270). The 
pivoted support allowed the responsive points A and B to 
be at first lateral. Owing to symmetry there was now no 


Fic. 271. Geo-electric Response of the Physically Restrained Scape 
of Uriclis Lily 


Up-curve represents responsive current from upper to lower surface during 
action of geotropic stimulus, Down-curve represents recovery on 
cessation of stimulus. Response commenced after latent period of one 
minute ; after-effect persisted for two minutes. Breaks in curve are 
due to obscuration of recording spot of light at brief intervals. . 


differential action of gravity, nor consequent electrical varia- 
tion, as between the two. The galvanometric record was 
now a horizontal line. The specimen, on its support, was 
next quickly rotated through 90°. The statolithic particles 
were thus displaced, falling on the inner tangential wall of 
the upper side, and outer tangential wall of the lower. An 
electrical response was perceived in about one minute, which 
went on augmenting with time, the upper side being in- 
creasingly galvanometrically negative (fig. 271). The fact 


GEO-ELECTRIC RESPONSE 443 


already demonstrated by the alternate cooling of the upper 
and lower surfaces is again seen here: namely, that it is the 
upper surface which exhibits the true excitatory effect, by 
induced concavity in the case of mechanical and by galvano- 
metric negativity in that of electrical response. It will also 
be seen that when secondary disturbing causes are removed, 
the response to gravity is immediate, showing that there is 
no anomalous delay between the displacement of the hypo- 
thetical particles and the consequent response. By now 
rotating the specimen back through 90°, the action of gravity 
is virtually removed. The after-effect persists for two 
minutes, and after this the response curve shows the usual 
recovery, 


CHAPTER: XXX 


DETERMINATION OF VELOCITY OF TRANSMISSION OF 
EXCITATION IN PLANT TISSUES 


Transmission of excitation in plants not due to hydromechanical disturbance, but 
instance of transmission of protoplasmic changes—Difficulties in accurate 
determination of velocity of transmission—A perfect method—Diminution of 
conductivity by fatigue—Increased velocity of transmission with increasing 
stimulus—Effect of cold in diminishing conductivity—Effect of rise of tem- 

- perature in enhancing conductivity—Excitatory concomitant of mechanical 
and electrical response—Electrical methods of determining velocity of trans- 
mission—Method of comparison of longitudinal and transverse conductivities 
—Tables of comparative velocities in animal and plant— Existence of two 
distinct nervous impulses, positive and negative. 


WHEN a point in the tissue is stimulated the state of excita- 
tion is often found to be transmitted to a distance. This is 
well seen in the case of sensitive plants, where the excitation 
applied at one point is found to give rise to motile responses 
of the distant leaf or leaflets. 

In this transmission of excitatory impulses to a distance 
in the plant we have a phenomenon which would seem to be 
analogous to nervous transmission in the animal. For certain 
reasons to be given presently, however, it has usually been 
supposed that there is actually nothing in common between 
the two. ‘The nervous system belongs exclusively to the 
animal organisation, and, indeed, to the more highly de- 
veloped Metazoa only. Plants, unicellular animals, and the 
lower Metazoa have no nerves, and if in exceptional cases 
(as in the excitatory movements of many plants) there are 
forms of activity which resemble the vital manifestations of 
the animal organisation, as effected by nerves, it is easy to 
prove that the resemblance is merely superficial.’ * 


' Biedermann, Zlectro- Physiology, English translation, 1898, Vol. ii. p. 32. 


VELOCITY OF TRANSMISSION OF EXCITATION 445 


This assumption, that there could be nothing in common 
between the transmission of excitatory impulses in plant and 
animal, was thought to derive support from Pfeffer’s experi- 
ment on the effect of anesthetics on conduction in J/zmosa. 
The anzsthetisation of the pulvinus is found to abolish its 
motile excitability. The effect of strong stimulus was never- 
theless found by Pfeffer to be transmitted across the anzsthe- 
tised area, giving rise to the depression of leaves beyond. It 
was natural to infer from this that, as the motile excitability 
of the pulvinus was abolished by the anesthetic, so must 
the protoplasmic conductivity also have been abolished. It 
was therefore inferred that—unlike the conduction of stimulus 
in animal tissues, where such transmission is known to take 
place by the propagation of protoplasmic changes—the ap- 
parent conduction of excitation in a plant was purely hydro- 
mechanical. 

But I have shown elsewhere, and shal]! demonstrate again 
in Chapter XX XIII. by different means, that though excita- 
bility and conductivity are related phenomena, yet the varia- 
tion of the one is not necessarily identical with that of the 
other. Thus a certain degree of anzsthetisation may be 
sufficient to induce arrest of motile excitability, and yet may 
not always abolish conductivity.! 

Another objection which has been urged against the 
theory of the transmission of protoplasmic changes through 
the plant is based on certain experiments of Haberlandt. In 
these the excitation in J/zmosa is said to have been propa- 
gated over dead tracts of the petiole, these portions having 
been killed by scalding. But it is extremely difficult to 
ensure the death of interior tissues by such means as super- 
ficial scalding. I have found that a portion of a plant tissue 
which had been subjected locally to the action of boiling 
water afterwards exhibited signs of true excitatory electrical 
response. It is only by prolonged immersion in boiling water 
that the electrical response is. totally abolished. Only after 
such treatment, therefore, can one be quite sure that the 

' Bose, Plant Response, pp. 227-229. - 


446 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


interior tissue is really killed by scalding, and unless this is 
thoroughly done it is easy to see that the inner cells may 
continue to conduct excitation. 

There is, moreover, another possibility, that of pseudo- 
conduction, by which the effect of stimulus might appear to 
be transmitted across dead areas. In Haberlandt’s experi- 
ment, even if the intervening tissues had been killed, there 
would still be two masses of tissue, separated from each other | 
by an intervening area of dead tissue. A strong stimulus 
applied to either of these might then cause an excitatory 
expulsion of water, capable, when transmitted across the 
dead. area, of imparting a mechanical blow to the second 
living tissue, sufficient to set up excitation de novo in that 
portion of the petiole. , 

I shall, however, be able to show that conduction is 
brought about in the plant by the same transmission of 
excitatory protoplasmic changes which occurs in the animal ; 
and that those agencies—such as cold, anesthetics, fatigue, 
and the polar effects of currents—which induce its variation 
in the one case, have the same identical effect in the other 
also. The electrical responses, again, afford us a crucial 
‘method of distinguishing between hydrostatic and excitatory 
effects. For, had the transmission of excitation taken place 
in plants by means solely of the propagation of hydrostatic 
disturbance, its electrical sign would then have been one of 
galvanometric positivity alone. But we have found, on the 
contrary, that the sign of the true excitatory reaction is 
always of galvanometric negativity. The distinct characters 
of these true excitatory and hydrostatic waves have already 
been demonstrated in various experiments, in some of which 
the hydrostatic has been seen as a positive twitch preceding 
the true excitatory negative, while in others the transmission 
of the negative was abolished by the application of a selective 
block, such as chloroform, thus bringing about the exhibition 
of the positive alone (p. 66). So true indeed is it that 
excitatory changes are propagated in the plant as in the 
animal, that I have actually been able to isolate certain 


VELOCITY OF TRANSMISSION OF EXCITATION 447 


tissues whose responsive peculiarities are indistinguishable 
from those of animal nerves. These must therefore be 
regarded as vegetable nerves (Chapter XX XII.). 

The determination of the velocity of transmission of 
excitation in sensitive plants may be made by applying a 
stimulus at any point and observing the interval which 
elapses before motile effects are visible at a given distance. 
It is, however, impossible by such means to obtain accurate 
and consistent results until certain factors of variation are 
successfully eliminated. These are (1) indefinite changes of 
excitability owing to in- 
jury caused by stimulus at 
the point of application ; 
(2) changes of conductivity 
caused by fatigue; and 
(3) the unknown effects 
of varying intensities of 
stimulus on velocity of 
transmission. 

As a result of investi- 
gations on this subject I 
found that the velocity of 


Fic. 272. Diagrammatic Representation 
of Electrical Connections for Deter- 


transmission can only be mination of Velocities of Centrifugal 
‘ and Centripetal Transmissions 
regarded as a determinate Fe 
: ‘ X A and B are exciting’ electrodes, and L the 
quantity when the intensity indicating leaflet. 


of stimulus has a definite 

value. Excessive stimulation, again, is found, by causing 
injury, to modify the excitability and conductivity of the 
tissue. These difficulties, however, are overcome by the 
employment of a stimulus which does not cause injury, and 
which is capable of repetition at uniform intensity. One 
such form of stimulation is obtained by the use of discharge 
from a condenser previously charged to a known,voltage 
(fig. 272). As regards those changes of conductivity which 
are due to fatigue, I have found that fatigue is removed, and 
conductivity fully restored, after the lapse of a definite period 
of rest, varying in duration in different plants from four to 


448 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


ten minutes. In the case of Azophytum, for instance, the 
required interval was found to. be about five minutes. | 
Taking a specimen of Szophytum, and employing the 
method of determination which has been described, I found 
successive values which were very consistent ; and, having 
thus secured conditions which made it possible to obtain 


exact results, I proceeded next to investigate the effects of 


various factors—such as fatigue, intensity of stimulus, and 


temperature—in modifying the velocity of transmission. 


Before proceeding to describe these results in detail, how- 
ever, it should be mentioned that, though the velocity of 
transmission of excitation is constant in the same plant 
under uniform conditions, yet this is not necessarily the case, 


if the direction of conduction be reversed. In the petiole of 


Biophytum, for example, the centrifugal velocity is always 
higher than the centripetal, being about fifty per cent. 
_ greater. . 

Specimen I 


Direction Distance | Time | Velocity 

: | | | 

Centripetal . ; . 22°5mm. | 11'2seconds 2 mm. persec.| 
Centrifugal . : ‘ 45 mm. i ar 2°79 TMs. i ys 


Specimen IL 


Direction | Distance | Time | Velocity 
| 
| 
| 


15*2 seconds |1°84mm. persec. 
17°5 a |2*2 mm. 99 


Centripetal . : Bi 28 mm. 
Centrifugal . : - 39°5 mm. 


In order to study the effect on velocity of progressive 
fatigue, we may gradually shorten the interval of rest. The 
velocity of transmission in the petiole of Bzophytum when 
fresh was found to be 1°88 mm. per second in the centripetal 
direction. Before making a second experiment on the same 
specimen, an intervening period of rest of three minutes was 
allowed. This was found to reduce the velocity slightly, it 
being now 1°86 mm. per second. The following table shows 
the results obtained by a series of five experiments on the 


4. >: eee 


VELOCITY OF TRANSMISSION OF EXCITATION 449 
same specimen. The distance through which the transmis- 
sion was observed was 27 mm. 


TABLE SHOWING VARIATIONS OF VELOCITY OF TRANSMISSION AND. OF 
AMPLITUDE OF RESPONSE WITH INCREASING FATIGUE 


Intervals of rest Time igen aenaae = Velocity 
The plant fresh. . | 14°3 seconds 34 divisions 1°88mm. persec. 
3 minutes . : or} BS SeL 55 20 a 1°86mm. ,, 
2 minutes , , ta ye We See Oe eee I°72mm. ,, 
I minute ; : Ag eh eee he SURI 1°64mm. ,, 
= minute ; : SPSS Lr tes 154mm. ,, 


It will thus be seen that the fatigue due to having only 
half a minute’s rest reduced the normal velocity of the 
specimen by 18 per cent. 

The effect of intensity of stimulus on velocity of trans- 
mission was next studied. The stimulus employed was that 
of condenser discharge, increased intensity being obtained 
by an increasing voltage of charge. In this way it was 
found that velocity increased with increasing intensity 
of stimulus. This fact is shown in the following table, 
which gives the results of an experiment on a petiole of 
Liophytum. 


TABLE SHOWING INCREASE OF VELOCITY WITH INCREASING STIMULUS 


Specimen I.—Centripetal T; ransmisston 
The distance traversed by stimulus was 27 mm. 


Stimulus Time | Velocity 
‘o1 Microfarad charged to 8 volts 14°9 seconds | 1°8 mm. per second 
> $9 I2 >> 14°4 29 I°9 mm. >? 
en BULSIOL 5 TAB * 55 | 2° mm. Pe 


Spectmen 11.—Centrifugal Transmission 
The distance traversed by stimulus was 38 mm. 


Stimulus Time Velocity 
‘ol Microfarad charged to 8 volts 11°6 seconds 3'27 mm. per second | 
» » 16 ,, 10'2 ;; 3°72 mm. 9 
os Ps 24; ROE 25, 3°76 mm. = ee 
” oo) 32 4 9°9 ” 3°83 mm. ” 


450 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


With regard to the effect of temperature, I found that 
cold reduced the velocity of transmission. Thus, in one 
experiment, slight cooling reduced it to one-third, and when 
carried still further, it abolished the conductivity altogether. 
A rise of temperature, on the other hand, had the effect of 
enhancing velocity of transmission. The following table 
shows that a rise of temperature from 30° C. to 35° C. 
doubled the velocity, and that at 37° C. the rate was. 
almost three times that at the first temperature. The 
velocity was in this case determined in the centrifugal 
direction. 


TABLE SHOWING THE EFFECT OF RISE OF TEMPERATURE ON VELOCITY. 


Distance traversed by stimulus 41 mm. 


Temperature Time | Velocity 
307¢), II seconds 3°7 mm. per second 
35° C. 5 33 7°4 mm » 
37° C. 4°5 9 | g*t mm. ry) 


Transmission of excitation, as I have shown elsewhere, 
and shall show again, is depressed or abolished by the action 
_of anesthetics. We shall also see, further, that the polar 
effects of currents on the velocity of transmission are the 
same in the plant as in the animal, being opposite, accord- 
ing as it is the anode or kathode. In the case of a so-called 
‘sensitive’ plant, by taking advantage of the motile indica- 
tions afforded by the leaf or leaflet, it is possible to determine 
the velocity of transmission of excitation and its modifica- 
tions. With ordinary plants, however, no such indications 
being available, it is obvious that we must find some other 
means of detecting and observing the excitatory wave during 
transit. Onesuch I have described elsewhere as the Electro- 
tactile Method. It is found that the passage of the excitatory 
wave, even through an ordinary tissue, brings about minute 
form-changes. These give rise to pressure-variations as 
between two enclosing contacts. And this variation of 
pressure, in turn, can be recorded by means of a sensitive 
electrical device. 


VELOCITY OF TRANSMISSION OF EXCITATION 451. 


There is, however, a more direct way of detecting the 
excitatory wave during its passage through a vegetable 
tissue. In this—the Electro-motive Method—the galvano- 
meter takes the place of the motile leaflet. It has been 
shown that. when the plant tissue is directly excited, the 
state of excitation is invariably accompanied by an electro- 
motive variation, the excited point becoming galvano- 
metrically negative. Hence any excitatory wave which is 
transmitted through the tissue will always have an electro- 
motive wave as its strict concomitant. The moment, there- 
fore, at which excitation reaches any given point, may 
always be determined by observing the arrival at that point 
of the excitatory electrical disturbance of galvanometric 
negativity. In order to prove that the arrival of excitation 
at the given point is attended by this specific electrical 
response, we may perform an experiment on a plant such 
as Biophytum, which is provided with motile leaflets. One 
of the indicating leaflets is attached to the optic lever, its 
base being connected with one of the electrodes of the 
galvanometer, while the second is attached to a distant point 
on the leaf. The two spots of light, one from the optic 
lever indicating the mechanical response, and the other from 
the galvanometer, indicating the electrical, are so adjusted 
as to lie one above the other, on the same revolving-drum. 
On now applying a stimulus, say thermal, at.a distant point, 
it will be found, after the lapse of a definite interval; that 
both spots of light are deflected at the same time, showing 
that both alike give an outward indication of that state of 
molecular disturbance which is synonymous with excitation. 
These manifestations, of both kirids, would therefore take 
place at an identical moment, if only the inertia of the 
two indicators were absolutely the same. But, just as the 
same impulse would be indicated at slightly different times, 
if one indicating-lever were light, and the other heavy, so 
here also there may be a slight difference as regards time 
between the appearance of the mechanical and electrical 

GG2 


452 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


responses, according as the virtual inertia of the one indicator 
exceeds that of the other. 

In determining velocity of transmission by the Electro- 
motive Method, a previous experiment gives us the loss of 
time due to the inertia of the galvanometer. This, deducted 
from the observed interval between the application of 
stimulus and response, gives the time required for trans- 
mission through the given distance. In this manner I have 
been able to determine the rate of transmission of excitation 
in ordinary plants. I give below a table which shows these 
velocities as determined by me in the case of sensitive plants, 
and of ordinary plants, and for the purpose of comparison, 
those obtained by other observers, in the nerves of some of 
the lower animals, from which it will be seen that all these 
are more or less of the same order. 


TABLES GIVING VELOCITIES OF TRANSMISSION OF EXCITATORY WAVE 


(a) Animal. 


Subject Velocity 


Nerve of Anodon : . | IO mm. per second 
| 
| 


Nerve of Eledone (observed by Uexkiill) ‘5 to 1 mm. Ms 
(6) Sensitive Plants. 
Subject | Velocity 
| eens 
Mimosa pudica: petiole. : : ; | 14 mm. per second 
Neptunia oleracea: petiole . : ; . | I°I mm. ie 
Biophytum sensitivum : | 
Petiole of, direction centripetal . 7 2°I mm. 
Petiole of, direction centrifugal . 3°38 mm. os 
Peduncle of . F : : ; | 3°7 mm. ze 
(c) Ordinary Plants. 
Subject | Velocity 
= = - po LiLE 
Fern: isolated nerve of . , . 2 50 mm. per second 
Ficus religtosa: stem. : ; ; a 9°4 mm. ‘3 
Cucurbita : tendril ; ; | 5 mm. ¥3 
Jute: stem P e ‘ : ‘ - | 3°5 mm. Pe 
Artocarpus: petiole . ; ; ; Fae *54 mm. ae 


VELOCITY OF TRANSMISSION OF EXCITATION 453. 


Since the conduction of excitation takes place by the 
transmission of protoplasmic changes, it is evident that it must 
occur most easily along those paths in which there is greatest 
protoplasmic continuity. It is clear, then, that certain 
elements in the fibro-vascular bundles will furnish the best 
conducting medium. Cells of indifferent tissue, on the other 
hand, like the parenchyma of the leaf, are divided from each 
other by more or less complete septa, the fine filaments, by 
which neighbouring cells may be protoplasmically connected, 
being so minute that the conduction of stimulus through such 
imperfect channels must be comparatively feeble. Such 
tissues are, therefore, indifferent conductors of excitation, 
the stimulus remaining more or less localised in them. 
Plant-organs, then, which contain fibro-vascular elements, 
such as the stem, peduncle, and petiole, are for that reason 
relatively good conductors. Conductivity in such an organ, 
again, is,as we should expect, much greater along the length 
than across. 

I shall now describe an important method by which the 
relative conductivity of a tissue in different directions may 
be experimentally determined, verifying by its means the 
difference in the power of a tissue to transmit stimulus 
longitudinally and transversely. For this purpose I took 
a thick peduncle of J/usa, and made two electrical con- 
nections, of which one was at a fixed point B, transversely 
situated as regards C, the point of application of stimulus. 
The second point, A, was longitudinally above Cc, and its 
distance from it could be varied in successive experiments 
(fig. 273). 

If we now take a point, A, in such a position that CA is 
equal to CB, then, on account of the better conductivity along 
CA, the excitation will reach the A contact earlier than that 
at B, making that point galvanometrically negative. The 
direction of the first responsive current, therefore, will be 
from A->B in the tissue. If, next, the longitudinal contact 
be moved to A”, that is to say, so far that the excitation 
reaches the B contact first, then the responsive current. will 


454 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


be reversed, flowing now from B>A”. A point of transition, 
or of balance, A’, may now be found by searching, at which 
the movement of the exploring contact, nearer or further, 
will give rise to opposite responsive currents. The con- 
ductivity along the longitudinal direction will then be, to 
that in the transverse direction, as the balancing-distance 
CA’ is to CB. With a given specimen of the peduncle 
of Musa the transverse distance CB was 3°7 cm., and the 
longitudinal balancing-distance CA” was determined at 10°4 
cm. Hence the longitudinal velocity was 2°8 times that in 
the transverse direction. 


Fic. 273. Experimental Arrangement for Comparing the Relative 
Conductivities in Transverse and Longitudinal Directions 


C, point of application of stimulus ; B, permanent transverse contact ; 
A, A’, A’, exploring points of longitudinal contact for obtaining 
balance, 


It has been shown that different tissues in the plant may 
possess extremely different powers of conducting stimulus. 
In animals there are specialised channels of conduction 
known as nerves, and in plants also I have been able to 
discover similar conducting tissues, which can be isolated 
for the study of their responsive peculiarities. Experiments 
on this subject will be related in detail in Chapter XXXII. 
It may be said here, however, in anticipation, that the 
velocity of transmission of true excitation through these 
nervous channels is, generally speaking, fairly high, being at 
the rate of about 50 mm. per second in the case of isolated 


_ —— 


VELOCITY OF TRANSMISSION OF EXCITATION 455 _ 


nerve of fern. This, for the relatively sluggish vegetable 
tissue, is undoubtedly very high. 

In connection with this question of velocity of trans- 
mission, a fact not hitherto taken into account is, that there 
are two distinct kinds of nervous impulses, travelling with 
different velocities—namely, the hydro-positive and the true 
excitatory negative. Of these the velocity of the former is 
greater. In the nerves of higher animals, where the velocity 
of transmission of true excitation is also great, it is not 
generally easy to distinguish one from the other, so rapid 
is their succession. But their occurrence as distinct waves, 
even in animal tissues, I shall be able to demonstrate in 
a subsequent chapter. In plants, however, where the velocity 
of transmission of true excitation is not very high, it generally 
lags perceptibly behind the positive wave (p. 59). Burdon 
Sanderson, in his determination of the velocity of trans- 
mission of excitation in Dzonea, arrived at the exceptionally 
high result of 200 mm. per second. I have shown, however, 
that the wave whose velocity he measured was not of true 
excitation, but of hydro-positive disturbance (p. 231). 

In the present chapter it has been my object to demon- 
strate the reality of true excitatory propagation in plants 
similar to that in the animal. The examples given will be 
found more fully described on referring to my book on ‘ Plant 
Response.’ I shall, however, in the course of the present 
work describe new and extremely delicate means by which 
the modifications of conductivity may be studied in plants 
unde- varying physiological conditions, 7 


CHAPTER XXXI 


ON A NEW METHOD FOR THE QUANTITATIVE 
STIMULATION OF NERVE 


Drawbacks to use of electrical stimulus in recording electrical response— Response 
to equi-alternating electrical shocks—Modification of response by decline of 
injury—Positive after-effect—Stimulation of nerve by thermal shocks — 
Enhancement of normal response after tetanisation—-Untenability of theory of 
evolution of carbonic acid—Abnormal positive response converted into normal 
negative after tetanisation—Gradual transition from positive to negative, 
through intermediate diphasic—Effect of depression of tonicity on excitability 
and conductivity—Conversion of abnormal into normal response by increase 
of stimulus-intensity—Cyclic variation of response under molecular modifica- 
tion. 


IN the study of the electrical effects of excitation on the 
nerve, the chief experimental difficulty lies in the selection 
of a form of stimulus which can be made quantitative. In 
such investigations it is usual to employ the electrical form 
of stimulus, because of the great facilities which it offers. 
A marked drawback to its use, however, lies in the fact that 
unless extraordinary precautions are taken it is liable to lead 
to serious error. It must be remembered that for the detec- 
tion of responsive variations in the nerve an extremely 
sensitive galvanometer has to be employed. The excitatory 
effect which is to be detected being indicated by the relatively 
feeble electrical response, and the form of stimulus being also 
electrical and being of high intensity, the results are liable to 
be disturbed in an unknown manner by leakage of the stimu- 
lating current. 

In some cases it is possible to take the bold step of 
including the experimental nerve itself in a circuit in which 
the exciting coil and the galvanometer are in series. Under 
these circumstances, and employing strictly equi-alternating 


QUANTITATIVE STIMULATION OF NERVE 457 


shocks, we have seen that the resultant response is due to 
the differential excitabilities of the two nerve-contacts A and 
B. If, for instance, we wish to obtain the responsive reaction 
of one point only, say A, uncomplicated by that of 8, it is” 
only necessary to abolish the excitability of the latter. This 
can be done to a greater or less extent by injury, as, say, by 
making a transverse section, or by scalding. Response will 
then take place by the induction of relative galvanometric 
negativity at A. In fig. 274 is seen 
a series of records obtained in this 
manner. The responses here apparently 
indicate growing fatigue of the nerve. 
They also exhibit the positive after- 
effect. 

With reference to the method of 
obtaining response by injuring one con- 
tact, commonly employed, it may be 
said that the assumption that the ex- 
citability of the injured point is totally 
abolished is not justified; for I have 
found that though recent injury causes 
a great depression of excitability, yet py. 274. Response of 
after a lapse of time the injured point — Frog’s Nerve under 


‘ F ‘ar Simultaneous Excita- 
tends to recover its excitability to a tion of both Contacts, 


greater or less extent. In such a case by Equi - alternating 
. é Electrical Shocks, one 
we may expect two different effects to Contact being Injured 


be exhibited in the responses. The re- Note the positive after- 
sultant response being due, as we have i 3 

seen, to the differential excitability of A and B, the gradual 
restoration of the excitability of B will progressively diminish 
the amplitude of the resultant response, thus giving it the 
appearance of fatigue. Under these conditions, and after 
a sufficiently long interval, response may almost disappear. 
This appears to me to be the true explanation of the gradual 
fall in the amplitude of response, when the specimen is a 
nerve, having one contact at the transverse section. It also 
explains why, in such a nerve, a fresh section, causing 


458 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


renewed depression of excitability, is necessary in order to 
obtain renewed amplitude of response. 

The second effect due to this depression, without abolition 
of the excitability of B, is seen in the diphasic character of 
the responses. The positive after-effect observed in the record 
shown in fig. 274 may thus be ascribed to the later induction 
of negativity at the depressed point B. The electrical re- 
sponse of the nerve is apparently liable in this way to great _ 
variations, when the method of record employed is differential. 
But it must be remembered that true characteristic variations 
of the response as determined by physiological modification 
can only be obtained by finding some means which shall 
be strictly independent of this differential factor. With this 
object, I have succeeded in devising a new mode of observing 
and recording the direct effect of stimulus on the nerve, 
uncomplicated by the differential factor. In a subsequent 
chapter we shall, using this method, be able to determine 
the conditions which induce the characteristic variations in 
the response of nerve, from the staircase increase to the fatigue- 
decline, or even reversal, through the intermediate phase of 
uniform reponses. 

The method which. has just been described, of exciting 
the nerve at both contacts by equi-alternating shocks, is not 
applicable, however, where the object of investigation is the 
conductivity of an intervening tract of nerve between the 
exciting and the led-off circuits. Here the employment of 
electrical shocks as exciting stimulus gives rise to disturbing 
unipolar effects, which persist even when the physiological 
conductivity of the intervening tract is destroyed as, say, by 
ligature or by crushing. Thus— 


‘If the nerve of a frog’s leg is laid across two 
electrodes connected with the poles of a secondary coil, 
so as to close the induction circuit, a ligature being then 
applied to the myopolar tract, tetanus may still be 
observed in the isolated leg, on making the lead off from 
it at a certain distance of coil... These unipolar effects 


QUANTITATIVE STIMULATION OF NERVE 459 


may obviously be very disturbing, and are indeed pro- 
ductive of fallacies in vivisection and also in experi- 
ments with the galvanometer, if not avoided by due 
precautions. Hering has pointed out that in experiments 
such as the investigation of the negative variation of 
nerve-currents, in which galvanometers and exciting 
circuits are separated by a long tract of nerve, the most 
complete insulation of the two circuits is no guarantee 
against the overflow of induced electricity through the 
interpolar part of the nerve into the galvanometer 
circuit... . This kind of unipolar stimulation is an 
obvious danger in all experiments on action-currents and 
negative variation in nerve, while it shows what narrow 
bounds restrict the intensities of current that may be 
safely used in these experiments.’ ? 


From this it will be seen how important it is to have at 
our command some non-electrical form of stimulation, when 
the response to be recorded is electrical. Heidenhain. em- 
ployed a mechanical form of stimulation, by which the nerve 
was subjected to blows from an ivory hammer, which was 
kept vibrating by means of an electro-magnetic arrangement. 
The employment of this mode of stimulation would there- 
fore eliminate all that uncertainty—arising from the possible 
escape of current—which is inseparable from the use of 
electrical stimulus. Though this method must be regarded 
as one of great value, yet it is impossible to say how far the 
excitability of a given point in a structure so delicate as 
nerve will remain unmodified under the repeated action of 
such blows. In any case, it appeared desirable to inquire 
whether there was no other non-electrical form of stimulus 
that could be rendered practicable. 

Besides the mechanical, the only remaining non-electrical 
forms of stimulus are the chemical and the thermal. Of 
these, the former is obviously incapable either of repetition or 


* Biedermann, Z/ectro- Physiology (English translation), 1898, vol. ii. pp. 222- 
223. 


460 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


of being rendered quantitative. As regards the latter, I~have 
already shown its practicability for experiments on excitatory 
phenomena in vegetable tissues. Thus a single loop of 
platinum wire may be made closely to surround the experi- 
mental tissue. A definite current sent through the platinum 
loop for a given length of time will now subject the encircled 
area to a sudden thermal variation, which acts as a stimulus. 
Successive closures of the circuit for a definite length of 
time are ensured by means of a key actuated by a metro- 
nome. The intensity of stimulus may be graduated in a pre- 
determined manner by the adjustment of the heating-current. 
Excitation may then be caused either by one or by a 
summated series of thermal shocks. 

I was now desirous of determining whether this form of 
stimulation would prove advantageous to experiments on 
the nerve, and in the course of the investigation I found it to 
be extremely convenient and appropriate. With good speci- 
mens of nerve I have been able, using thermal stimulus, to 
obtain long-sustained records of perfectly regular responses. 
As regards its pliability and facility of application this form 
of stimulus is quite unique. How many difficult problems 
-are made possible of attack by its means will be realised in 
the course of the two following chapters, where the responsive 
variations of different conducting tissues under changing 
conditions are subjected to investigation. 

In order to obtain the electrical responses of animal 
nerve—that of frog, for example—the distal contact is killed 
and appropriate electrical connections made with the galvano- 
meter. The heating current is then adjusted for the desired 
amount of excitation.: The thermal variation, it must be 
remembered, should not be so great as to injure the tissue 
in any way. The platinum loop is not in this case in contact 
with the specimen, and this is the mode generally employed. 
Should a more intense stimulation be desired, however, the 
nerve may be allowed to rest on the platinum loop. In 
such a case care must be taken to see that the rise of 
temperature is not so great as in any way to injure the 


QUANTITATIVE STIMULATION OF NERVE 461 


tissue. The nerve, as usual, must be enclosed in a moist 
chamber, a convenient form of which, as employed i in 1 practice, 
will be seen in fig. 291. | 

I shall next give a few records in illustration of the ease 
and efficiency with which this mode of stimulus may be 
applied. These records will show the characteristic varia- 
tions of response given by the nerve under different con- 
ditions. When making records of electrical responses with 
frog’s nerve, under electrical stimulus, Dr. Waller obtained 
responses of three different types. The first of these was 
the normal, and consisted of negative responses ; the second 
was diphasic ; and the third was the abnormal positive. This 
last he regarded as characteristic of stale nerve. 

These normal negative responses of the first of the three 
classes were found by him to undergo enhancement after a 
period of tetanisation ; while the third, that of the abnormal 
response of stale nerve, underwent a change into diphasic, 
or a reversal to normal, after tetanisation. 

From the fact that carbonic acid enhances the normal 
negative response of nerve, Dr. Walier has suggested that 
the enhancement of response in normal nerve after tetanisa- 
tion, and the tendency of the modified nerve to revert to the 
normal, are results of the hypothetical evolution ot carbonic 
acid in the nervous substance, due to metabolism accompany- 
ing excitatory reactions. It must be said, however, that no 
trace of the presence of carbonic acid has yet been detected 
in such cases. I shall be able to show, moreover, that these 
effects are in no way due to the evolutions of carbonic acid, 
but take place in consequence of molecular changes induced 
in the responding tissue, which find concomitant expression 
in changes of conductivity and excitability. 

I shall now give records of responses of these various 
types obtained under the action of thermal stimulus. In 
order to exhibit the effect of tetanisation I give, in fig. 275, 
a series of normal responses by induced _galvanometric 
negativity, given by nerve of frog in its normal excitatory 
condition. ‘This nerve was then subjected to tetanic thermal 


462 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


shocks, after which its responses to individual stimuli of the 
former intensity were recorded once more. The subsequent 
responses show, as is seen in the record, an enhancement of 
amplitude. . 
The next series of responses, in fig. 276, exhibits abnormal 
galvanometric positivity. It may be mentioned here that 
; these abnormal responses are 
not, as supposed by Dr. 
Waller, exclusively character- 
istic of the stale condition of 
the nerve. For employing 
other and more delicate 
methods of record I have 
found even fresh nerves, 
under certain conditions, to 
exhibit this effect. Neither 
is this positive response due 
in general to any chemical 
degradation. Instead of this, 
as we shall see in the present 
and succeeding chapters, it 
may be attributed to the 
run-down of the latent energy 
of the specimen, a process 


Fic. 275. Enhancement of Amplitude 


of Response, as After-effect of which becomes accelerated in 
Thermal Tetanisation, in Frog’s . ; 
Notice , 5° isolation. When such a de- 


The first three responses are normal. pressed specimen is supplied 


Brief thermal tetanisation is here again with the requisite 
applied, and the responses subse- 


quently obtained under original energy, it becomes normally, 
stimulation are seen to be en- 
added: or even supernormally, ex- 
citable. The first part of the 
following record (fig. 276) gives a series of abnormal positive 
S §: #/9) § p 
responses obtained from a specimen of frog’s nerve, which 
was in a somewhat sub-tonic condition. After the appli- 
cation of tetanic thermal shocks it will be noticed that the 
responses in the second part of the figure have become 


converted into normal. — 


QUANTITATIVE STIMULATION OF NERVE 463 


Between these two extremes of normal negative and 
abnormal positive responses there lies the intermediate 
diphasic. All these—positive, diphasic, and negative—may 
be exhibited in the same specimen, in the course of a sus- 
tained record of responses to single stimuli, without tetani- 
sation. This fact is illustrated in fig. 277, where the first 
series shows the unmixed abnormal positive. 
then passes by a gradual 
transition into diphasic— 
positive followed by negative 
—and this phase, lastly, is 
succeeded by a series of 
purely negative responses. 

We come next to the ex- 
planation of these phenomena. 
We have seen that on account 
of isolation the tonic condition 
of a highly excitable tissue 
will undergo a graduai decline. 
On account of this its ex- 
citability and conductivity 
will fall below par. We have 
also seen that in this de- 
pressed condition the normal 


The response 


Fic. 


Conversion of Abnormal 


276. 


response by negativity tends 
to be reversed to positivity. 
With regard to the con- 
duction of excitation it may 


Positive into Normal Negative Re- 
sponse after Thermal Tetanisation 


to left 
into normal’ negative 
on right, after intervening tetanisa- 
tion. 


Abnormal positive response 
converted 


be said that this condition of 
depression will lower the power of the tissue to conduct true 
excitation. Thus a stimulus of given intensity, capable under 
normal conditions of transmission to a certain distance, will, 
when the tissue is thus depressed, fail of conduction to the 
same distance. It will now, therefore, be the hydro-positive 
effect of stimulus which will make its appearance alone at 
the distant responding point. And the electrical expression 
of this will be galvanometric positivity. 


464 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


We have also seen that a tissue which is not in the 
highest tonic condition may have its tonicity increased by 
the action of impinging stimulus, with consequent enhance- 
ment of its excitability. I shall also demonstrate, in 
Chapter XXXIV, that the effect of an impinging stimulus 
on a sub-tonic tissue is a similar enhancement of con- 
ductivity. The result of this will be either (1) that a tissue 
which has already conducted a moderate intensity of 


ot — 


FIG. 277. Gradual Transition from Abnormal Positive, through 
Diphasic, to Normal Negative Responses in Frog’s Nerve 


Cf. similar effect in response of skin of gecko, fig. 191. 


stimulus to a distant point will show, after continuous stimu- 
lation, an enhanced power of conduction ; or (2) that in a 
very sub-tonic tissue, in which true excitation has at first 
failed to reach the responding point, the true excitatory 
negative is subsequently transmitted instead of the hydro- 
positive alone. 

Under actual experimental conditions, where the stimulus 
is applied at a distant point, the twofold effects of exaltation of 
excitability and conductivity under tetanisation both come 
into play. In normally responding nerve, the increased con- 


QUANTITATIVE STIMULATION OF NERVE 465 


duction of excitation, and the enhanced excitability of the 
responding-point, give rise to an increased amplitude of 
response after tetanisation, as already seen in fig. 275. Ina 
depressed nerve, as the transmitted effect is positive, and the 
tendency of the responding point itself, owing to sub-tonicity, 
is to the abnormal positive, the record will exhibit the 
‘abnormal positive alone, as in fig. 276. But under a series 
of successive stimuli, the conductivity and excitability of the 
tissue are both gradually raised, and the effect of this is seen 
in the consequent gradual restoration of the normal negative 
response, through the intermediate diphasic (fig. 277). Or, if 
we do not wish to trace out the intermediate steps of transi- 
tion, we may tetanise the depressed nerve for a certain 
length of time, and record only the terminal change to 
the restored normal negative, as is seen in fig. 276. 

Taking one of the extreme cases—say that in which the 
response to transmitted stimulus is positive, and is converted 
into normal negative after tetanisation—we see that the first 
result is due to inefficient conductivity, allowing only the 
hydro-positive effect to cause response. After this, increasing 
conductivity, making an increasing transmission of true exci- 
tation possible, gives rise to a diphasic, and ultimately to 
the normal negative response. This result is analogous to 
the three types of responses—positive, diphasic, and negative 
—which we have already obtained with the imperfectly con- 
ducting tissue of the petiole of cauliflower and the tuber of 
potato (figs. 47, 48). We there saw that where excitatory 
efficiency of transmitted stimulus was sufficiently great, it 
gave rise to the normal negative response. When this, how- 
ever, was not so great, we obtained ‘the diphasic. Finally, 
when the true excitatory effect could not be transmitted, 
only the abnormal positive response appeared. That 
gradation by which the transmitted stimulus was made 
fully, partially, or non-effective, to induce true excitation, 
was simply and most conclusively carried out in the case of 
the potato, by removing the point of stimulation to an 
increasing distance from the responding point. In the cases 

H H 


466 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


described, then, the three types of response are exhibited by 
the same tissue, in indubitable relation to the variation of 
its effective conductivity. If, then, results exactly parallel 
can be demonstrated to occur in the case of nerve also, it. 
follows that there is no necessity there to make any such 
hypothetical assumption as that of the evolution of carbonic 
acid, suggested by Dr. Waller, in explanation of the conver- 
sion of abnormal response to normal. 

In order to show how a varying conduction will give rise 
to these three types of responses, I shall now describe an 


Fic. 278. Abnormal Positive Response convérted through Diphasic 
to Normal Negative under the increasingly Effective Intensity of 
Stimulus, brought about by Lessening the Distance between the 
Responding and Stimulated Points 


experiment which I carried out with a frog’s nerve in some- 
what subtonic condition. Here, when the stimulator was 
placed at some distance from the responding point, the 
response was the abnormal positive (fig. 278). When the 
effective intensity of transmitted stimulus was now slightly 
increased by moving the point of application a little nearer, 
the response became diphasic ; and finally, when the stimu- 
lator was placed still nearer, the response became normal 
negative. Thus with an identical specimen we may obtain 
at will either negative, diphasic, or positive response, by 
making changes only in the effective intensity of stimulus 
employed. We have also seen, moreover, that if we kept 


QUANTITATIVE STIMULATION OF NERVE 467 


the stimulator at a certain distance from the responding 
point, such as at first to cause only positive response, succes- 
sive stimulations would then act to enhance conductivity 
gradually, and thus give rise to the appropriate changes, 
diphasic and negative in the response. 

The ultimate cause of these variations must therefore lie 
in the molecular condition of the tissue. Under varying cir- 
cumstances, this undergoes a cyclic change, the responsive 
reaction at any given moment constituting an indication of 
the particular molecular condition of the tissue. A more 
complete demonstration of this, carried out by an altogether 
different method, will be given in a subsequent chapter. My 
principal object in this chapter has been to prove the 
efficiency of the thermal shock as a mode of stimulation of 
nerve. Its wider applicability, in the case of other related 
investigations, will be treated in the two succeeding chapters, 


HH 2 


CHAPTER XXXII 
ELECTRICAL RESPONSE OF ISOLATED VEGETAL NERVE 


Specialised conducting tissues—Isolated vegetal nerve—Method of obtaining 
electrical response in vegeta] nerve—Similarity of responses of plant and 
animal nerve: (a) action of ether—(4) action of carbonic acid—(c) action 
of vapour of alcohol—(d) action of ammonia—(e) exhibition of three types 
of response, negative, diphasic and positive—(/) effects of tetanisation of 
normal and modified specimens—Effect of increasing stimulus on response 
of modified tissue. 


IT has been shown in the previous chapter that the state of 
excitation is transmitted to a distance in vegetable tissues. 
It has also been proved that such transmission is not due to 
the propagation of hydrostatic disturbance but to that of 
protoplasmic changes, precisely as in the case of animal 
tissues. It is obvious, further, that such transmission will be 
the more perfect the less the interruption of protoplasmic con- 
tinuity. Hence tissues like stems and petioles, which contain 
fibro-vascular elements, are found to be good conductors 
of excitation, whereas indifferent tissues, such as those of 
leaves and tubers, are relatively feeble as regards this power, 
excitation in their case remaining somewhat localised. 

Even with regard to stems and petioles themselves, a 
contrast is found to exist in this respect between the fibro- 
vascular elements and the ground tissue. Thus, in the case 
of a petiole of cauliflower, I made two experimental prepara- 
tions. In the first, the ground tissue was cut away, leaving 
the fibro-vascular elements ; and in the second, a column of 
ground tissue was left outstanding, denuded of fibro-vascular 
elements. The former of these was found to transmit 
excitation to a certain distance, whereas in the latter the 
transmission was practically absent. In the case of a third 


RESPONSE OF ISOLATED VEGETAL NERVE 469 


preparation I bifurcated the specimen, stripping away from 
one of the two limbs the fibro-vascular elements, and from 
the other most of the ground tissue. Galvanometric connec- 
tions were now made with the free ends of the fibro-vascular 
and ground tissues respectively, and stimulus was applied by 
means of transverse cut, or by application of a hot plate 
across the area of union. The transmitted effect was now 
perceived as galvanometric negativity, at the end of that 
strip which was composed of fibro-vascular elements. 

In studying this | 
subject of conduction, 
I found the transmitted 
effect of excitation to 
be universally well ex- 
hibited in the petioles 
of ferns, successive re- 
sponses, obtained at a 
distance from the point 
of stimulation, being 
in their case singularly 
perfect and uniform. 

From this I was led 
to the conclusion that 
the disposition of the. 
conductors must here fic. 279. Frond of Fern with Conducting 
hie par ticularly uel} Nerves N exposed in Enlarged Figure to Right 
adapted to their purpose. I had long been desirous of isolating 
whatever elements in the vegetable tissue were to be regarded 
as performing the function of nerves, and it appeared to me 
that I had here found a good subject for this investigation ; 
and accordingly, on carefully breaking the hard casing of the 
petiole, and pulling it away in both directions, I was able 
to isolate the conducting fibro-vascular threads, which were 
long, soft, and white in colour, remarkably similar in their 
appearance to animal nerves (fig. 279). These threads vary 
in number with different species of ferns, and resemble 
animal nerves in general appearance. It is sometimes 


470 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


possible to detach one of them having a length of 20 cm. 
or more. 

Now the essential feature of a nerve is its protoplasmic 
continuity, which is ensured by its fibrous structure. And 
in what I have called the vegetable nerve we find.the same 
characteristic to hold good. On viewing this structure, as 
it appears on making a transverse section of the petiole, 
we find it enclosed within sheath-like sclerenchyma. It © 
mainly consists in itself of a bundle of fine fibres with a 
few vessels in the centre. But however remarkable these 
external resemblances may seem, they are by no means so 
startling as the more fundamental similarities which are 
demonstrated so soon as we proceed to subject this vegetable 
structure to those tests of electrical response which are 
characteristic of animal nerve. It may be said that for the 
following investigation the nerves of the common maiden- 
hair fern (Adtantum) and Nephrodium molle were found most 
suitable. 

In obtaining a plant nerve for purposes of experiment 
it is possible to dissect it out and at the same time to avoid 
injury. It is then placed in normal saline solution for about 
half an hour, so as to remove all traces of excitation due to 
handling. When the external temperature is not high, the 
excitability of the isolated plant nerve is found to remain 
relatively unaffected for a considerable period, but in the hot 
weather it undergoes rapid decline; and the only way in 
which I could overcome this difficulty was by placing the 
specimen in normal saline solution which was ice-cold. The 
experimental precautions to be taken are precisely the same 
as those observed in corresponding experiments with animal 
nerve; that is to say, the specimen should be placed in a 
moist chamber. For the process of drying is found to induce 
a transient increase of excitability followed by a permanent 
abolition of responsiveness, in the one case as in the other. 
In order to obtain responses, one end of the specimen may 
be killed by the local application of hot salt solution. The 
galvanometric connections are then made, one with the killed, 


RESPONSE OF ISOLATED VEGETAL NERVE 471 


and the other with the unkilled portions of the specimen 
higher up. In order to ensure that the electrical indication 
be a true responsive reaction, it is well to use a non-electrical 
form of stimulus. One of the most perfect forms—as we 
have seen in the previous chapter, on excitation of animal 
nerve—is the thermal, and this may be applied in precisely 
the same manner, that is to say, by means of a platinum 
wire, surrounding, but not necessarily in contact with, the 
given area of the specimen, this wire being heated periodically 
in the manner previously described, by means of a metro- 
nome closing an electric circuit. With a good specimen, 
a single thermal shock, lasting for less than a second, will be 
found sufficient to induce a considerable electrical response, 
or a response of still greater amplitude may be obtained by 
the summated effects of several such stimuli. One of the 
most noticeable differences between this plant nerve and 
other vegetable tissues lies in its greater excitability. For 
example, while a single thermal shock of less than one second’s 
duration is sufficient, as has been said, to evoke immediate 
and considerable response from the isolated nerve, we find that, 
in order to evoke similar response from the petiole of the fern 
as a whole, it is necessary to submit it to the same stimulus 
some twenty times in succession, the response even after this 
taking place with relative sluggishness. | 

A still further characteristic is its indefatigability. A long 
series of responses to uniform stimuli, such as would in the 
case of ordinary tissues bring about marked fatigue, will in 
that of nerve induce little or none. Rapidly succeeding 
tetanising shocks, moreover, such as in other tissues induce 
rapid’ decline, induce, generally speaking, but little of such 
an effect on the response of nerve. In the case of this vege- 
table nerve also the same statements hold good. A long 
continued series of responses shows little fatigue. After 
tetanisation, moreover, we find that the responses of nerve, 
whether animal or vegetable, become enhanced. 

In the matter of the effects induced by chemical re- 
agents on animal and vegetable nerves, a further remarkable 


A472 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


parallelism is to be observed. The completeness of this may 
be seen in greater detail in the next chapter. I shall, at the 
present point, confine myself to giving a few typical cases. 
Ether, for example, when acting on animal nerve, induces a 
preliminary exaltation of excitability, which is followed under 
its long continued action by depression. On blowing off 
the ether vapour again the original state of excitability is 
restored. In fig. 280 are seen the similar effects of this 
reagent on vegetable nerve, where (@) exhibits the normal 
response, (0) the immediate exaltation due to ether, (c) the 


Fic, 280, Photographic Record of effect of Ether on the Electrical 
Response of Plant-nerve 


(a) Normal response: application of ether at point marked with ¢; 
(4) Enhanced response in first stage of action of ether; (c) Subse- 
quent depression ; (2) Restoration of normal response after blowing-oft 
of ether. . 


subsequent effect of depression, which becomes marked after 
continuous action during twenty-five minutes, and (d) the 
restoration of the original condition on the blowing-off of 
the ether. 

Carbonic actd is known, in the case of animal nerve, to 
have the effect, in the first stage, or in small quantities, of 
inducing exaltation, which passes under its prolonged action, 
or, in the case of a stronger application, into depression. A 
similar effect is seen in fig. 281, where (a) shows the normal 
response of a vegetable nerve, and (0) the preliminary exalta- 
tion due to carbonic acid introduced into the vegetable nerve- 


RESPONSE OF ISOLATED VEGETAL NERVE 473 


chamber. This is seen to increase continuously for some 
twenty minutes in (c). But after the expiration of half an 
hour depression makes its appearance (¢). This becomes still 
more marked, after the fortieth minute, in (e), | 


Fic. 281. Photographic Record of Effect of CO, on Electrical 
Response of Plant-nerve 


a, normal responses ; 4 and ¢, enhanced response during first stage of 
action ; @ and e, subsequent growing depression. 


Alcohol vapour in strong, or long-continued applications, 
induces marked decline of response in animal nerve. Parallel 
effects are seen in the case of vegetable nerve in fig. 282. 

The effect of ammonia on animal tissues is character- 
istically different, according as the subject of experiment is 


Fic. 282. Photographic Record of Abolition of Response by Strong 
Application of Alcohol 


nervous or ordinary tissue. While the excitability of the 
muscle, for example, is but little affected by its application, 
that of nerve is quickly abolished. In order to see whether 
the same characteristic difference would be exhibited, as 


A74 ~ COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


between ordinary vegetable tissues and vegetable nerve, I 
first studied its effect on the ordinary tissue of the petiole of 


Fic. 283. Photographic Record of Effect of Ammonia on Ordinary 
Tissue of Petiole of Walnut 


Note that the effect of ammonia here is practically negligible. 


walnut. It will be seen from fig. 283 that ammonia here 
induced a Digs teayy no change in the excitability. But when 
the same reagent was applied 
to the isolated nerve of 
fern the response underwent 
depression, followed by total 
abolition, in the course of 
five minutes (fig. 284). 

One very curious charac- 
teristic of the _ electrical 
response of frog’s nerve is 
the occurrence, as referred 
to in the last chapter, of 
three distinct types of re- 
sponses, according to its 


Fic. 284. Photographic Record of aye : 
Effect of Similar Application of condition. Thus, as_ has 


Ammonia on Plant-nerve already been said, while 
The response here is rapidly diminished highly-excitable nerve é€x- 
and finally abolished. sae ; 

hibits the normal negative 
response, the same nerve, when it has become sub-tonic, will 
give a mixed or diphasic response; and a nerve which is 


RESPONSE OF ISOLATED VEGETAL NERVE A75 


modified to a still greater extent will show a purely abnormal 
or positive electrical response. Inthe case of vegetable nerve, 
J find exactly the same three types of response repeated, under 
the same conditions. This will be seen in the three sets of 
records given in fig. 285. The normal responses, which are 
negative, are here represented as ‘up, while the abnormal 
positive is represented as ‘ down.’ : 

Still more remarkable is the parallelism observed between 
the effects of tetanisation, on animal and vegetable nerve, 
both normal and modified. In the case of fresh frog’s 
nerve the responses are, as we have seen, enhanced, after 


Fic. 285. Photographic Record of Exhibition of Three Types of 
Response, Normal Negative, Diphasic, and Abnormal Positive, in 
Nerve of Fern under Different Conditions 


a period of tetanisation. The effect of tetanisation on 
vegetable nerve is precisely similar, as is seen in fig. 286. 
In the case of the modified frog’s nerve, moreover, it is-found 
that the abnormal positive response tends, after tetanisation, 
to become normal. This is seen in the abnormal response, 
whether positive or diphasic, being converted to the normal 
negative type. I have obtained exactly parallel effects in 
the case of modified vegetable nerve. In fig. 287 we see 
the abnormal diphasic response of vegetable nerve converted, 
after tetanisation, into normal negative. 

Thus, as in the response of animal nerve, so also in that 
of the vegetable, tetanisation is found to have the effect of 
enhancing the normal, or converting the abnormal into 


476 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


normal response. The abnormal response of nerve we found 
to be due to the joint depression of conductivity and excita- 
bility, on account of which the positive alone, instead of the 
true excitatory negative, was exhibited. In experimenting 
with frog’s nerve we saw that abnormal! response might, at 
will, be converted into normal through the intermediate 
diphasic by appropriately increasing the effective intensity 
of stimulation. A simple means of effecting this was to 
bring the stimulator gradually nearer the responding point. 


Fic, 286. Photographic Record of Effect of Tetanisation in Inducing 
Enhancement of Normal Negative Response in Nerve of Fern 


The first series of responses seen to he enhanced after intervening tetani- 
sation, T. 


In the response of vegetable nerve effects exactly parallel are 
to be observed. 

With a given specimen of vegetable nerve, the stimulator 
had at first been placed at a distance of 2 cm. from the 
proximal galvanometric contact, and the responses then 
taken were found to be of the abnormal positive type. The — 
stimulator was now brought nearer, the distance being 
reduced to I cm., and the next pair of responses is seen to 
be diphasic, consisting of a positive twitch followed by the 


RESPONSE OF ISOLATED VEGETAL NERVE 477 


normal negative response. The distance was next reduced 
still further, namely, to ‘5 cm., with the result that the 


Fic. 287. Photographic Record of Conversion of the Abnormal 
Diphasic into Normal Negative, after Tetanisation, Tr, in Nerve of 
Fern 


Fic. 288. Photographic Record showing how the Abnormal Positive 

' Response is converted through Diphasic into Normal Negative, by 
the Increasing Effective Intensity of Stimulus, due to Lessening the 
Distance between the Responding and Stimulated Points 


responses now became normal negative (fig. 288). It is thus 
seen that there is a continuity of response in the same 


478 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


tissue, as between the abnormal and normal, through the 
intermediate diphasic. 

From the various experiments, then, which have been 
given in this chapter, it will be seen that the response of the 
isolated vegetable nerve is in every respect similar to the 
corresponding responses of animal nerve. And we shall also 
see how, by means of the study of this vegetable nerve, we 
are enabled to elucidate many obscurities in the responses 
of the corresponding animal tissue. We shall in the next 
chapter enter in detail into the question of the modifications 
induced in the conductivity and excitability of vegetable 
nerve under the action of various external agencies, and 
these will be found to exhibit the strictest parallel with 
corresponding variations induced in the animal. . 


CHAPTER XXXIII 


THE CONDUCTIVITY BALANCE 


Receptivity, conductivity, and responsivity—Necessity for distinguishing these— 
Advantages of the Method of Balance—Simultaneous comparison of variations of 
receptivity, conductivity, and responsivity—The Conductivity Balance—Effect 
of Na,CO, on frog’s nerve—Effect of CuSO,—Effect of chemical reagents on 
plant nerve—Effect of CaCl, on responsivity—Responsivity variation under 
KCl—Comparison of simultaneous effects of NaCl and NaBr on responsivity 
—Effects of Na,CO, in different dilutions on conductivity—Demonstration of 
two different elements in conductivity, velocity, and intensity—Conductivity 
versus responsivity—(a) effect of KI—(4) Effect of Nal— Effect of alcohol on 
receptivity, conductivity, and responsivity—Comparison of simultaneous effects 
of alcohol—(a@) on receptivity versus conductivity—(2) on receptivity versus 
responsivity, 


WE know that when any point in a tissue is acted on by 
external stimulus, it receives the stimulation and is thrown 
into a state of excitation. This excitation is then conducted 
along the length of the tissue, and may be made outwardly 
manifest at some distant point by means of a suitable in- 
dication such as motile or galvanometric response. There 
are thus three different aspects of the excitatory effect to 
be distinguished from each other, namely, first the excita- 
tory effect at the point of reception of stimulus, which I 
have elsewhere designated receptive excitability, or simply 
Receptivity : secondly, the power of transmission of excita- 
tion, or Conductivity: and thirdly, the excitatory effect 
evolved at the distant responding region, which I shall 
henceforth term Responszvity. ‘Though these three aspects of 
the excitatory reaction are all alike dependent upon the 
molecular derangement caused by stimulus, it is nevertheless 
important to consider them separately, since their variation is 
not always the same under the same circumstances. We 


480 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


have seen, for example, that a rise of temperature, by in- 
creasing molecular mobility, enhances conductivity. But 
this increase of molecular mobility and internal energy also 
goes to augment the force of recovery, and, owing to this, 
the amplitude of excitatory response may be decreased. 
Thus, while a rise of temperature increases conductivity, it 
may appear to decrease responsive excitability. So much 
for the necessity of a distinction between conductivity and | 
responsivity. The term ‘excitability’ is commonly used for 
receptivity and responsivity indifferently. But I shall show 
in the course of the present chapter that it is important to 
make a distinction between these, since ‘the same external 
agent may effect the two differently.’ In the ‘following 
investigation, receptive excitability, or receptivity, will be 
represented by R, conductivity by cC, and responsive ex- 
citability, or responsivity, by E. 

In determining the effect of any external condition such 
as the application of a chemical reagent on responsive ex- 
citability, in the case of animal nerve, it is usual to take a 
series of normal responses, and then to record the modified 
responses after the application of the reagent. By com- 
paring a number of such series of records, representing the 
action of various reagents on different specimens, the relative 
effect of each chemical may be inferred. The drawback to 
this method lies, first, in the fact that by the addition of the 
chemical reagent the resistance of the electrical circuit 
undergoes an unknown change, thus inducing a variation in 
the amplitude of response, which is not necessarily due to the 
excitatory electromotive change fer se. It is true that this 
difficulty may to a greater or less extent be obviated by 
interposing a high external resistance in the circuit, but this, 
by reducing the deflection, necessarily reduces the sensitive- 
ness of the method also. Different specimens again cannot 
but be characterised by slight individual peculiarities, and the 
experimental arrangements therefore can only be considered 
to be perfect when we are able to compare the effects of two 


! See also Bose, Plant Response, pp. 215-230. 


THE CONDUCTIVITY BALANCE 4381- 


agents on an identical specimen. Again, in a series of 
chemical compounds which differ but slightly in effect from 
one another, an arrangement has to be devised by which 
the most minute excitatory variations will be conspicuously 
displayed. The same delicacy of experimental adjustment 
also becomes necessary when we wish to investigate the 
varying effects of time and quantity in the application. 

Similar considerations are involved when we attempt to 
observe the effects of various agents on conductivity and 
receptivity ; and still more complicated are the difficulties to 
be overcome when we have to study the property of con- 
ductivity versus responsivity or receptivity, or of receptivity 
versus responsivity, under the action of the same external 
agent. The methods hitherto available are neither perfect 
nor delicate enough for a complete and satisfactory determina- 
tion by their means of the various problems which arise in 
this connection. I' shall now, however, describe a very 
perfect and delicate method carried out by an experimental 
arrangement which I have devised, and shall designate as the 
Conductivity-Balance, by which the variation of an affected 
region may be continuously compared with a normal area 
as regards each of the three different aspects of the ex- 
citatory reaction, namely, receptivity, conductivity, and 
responsivity. In this method, moreover, the result is un- 
affected by any variation of resistance in the circuit that may 
be induced by changed conditions. It also enables us to 
solve the various difficulties encountered in comparing the 
relative changes induced in conductivity with those induced 
in receptivity or responsivity, or in the two last in respect to 
each other, under the influence of a given reagent. 

In fig. 289 is given a diagrammatic representation of the 
principal parts of this Conductivity-Balance. The thermal 
stimulator produces stimulation of the enclosed area of the 
specimen. The excitatory wave travels along both arms of 
the balance, through the conducting region C and c’, and 
induces excitatory electromotive effects at the two responsive 
points Eand E’. The excitatory electrical effects at E and E’ 

IT 


482 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


are opposed, and when these are equal, and balance each 
other, the galvanometer indication is then reduced to zero. 
E and E’ are usually at a distance of about 4 cm. from each 
other. When the stimulator is brought too near to the left 
contact E’, the excitatory effect of 
galvanometric negativity which is 
induced there is relatively greater 
than at E. The balance is thus dis- 
turbed, and the resultant responsive 
deflection is then, say, downwards. 
When the stimulator is placed, on 
the other hand, too near the contact 
E, to the right, the resultant galvano- 
metric deflection will be up.! By 
suitable movement of the stimulator, 
to and fro between these two ex- 
tremes, a point may be found where 
the excitatory effects at E and E’ 
will exactly balance each other. I 
give here (fig. 290) a record taken 


Fic. 290. Photographic Re- 
cord made during Pre- 
liminary Adjustment for 
Balance of Nerve of Fern 


The first two down-responses 
show over-balance, when 
S is too near the left, 
E’ being relatively more 


Fic. 289. Diagrammatic Representa- 


tion of the Conductivity Balance excited. The up-responses 

s, thermal stimulator; c and c’, the indicate over - balance 
conducting arms of the balance ; caused by s_ being too 
E and E’, responding points. Dif- much to the right. The 
ferential excitatory electrical effects horizontal record shows 
at E and E’ recorded by galvano- attainment of — exact 
meter, G. balance. 


during this preliminary stage of adjustment. The first two 
down-responses were obtained when the stimulator was too 
far away from the balancing-point to the left. The next two 


1 It is to be understood that what is said here refers to nerve in a normal 
condition of conductivity. 


THE CONDUCTIVITY BALANCE. 483 


up-responses were obtained when it-was contrariwise too far 
to the right. More careful adjustment reduced this up- 
movement, as seen in the next two responses, and finally, 
when the exact balancing-point was reached, the effect was 
null, as seen in the horizontal record. 

In studying the question of the variation of responsive 
excitability induced by any given reagent, the agent is 
applied at the point E to the right. Any variation of excit- 
ability will then upset the balance. If the reagent be of a 
stimulatory character we shall obtain a resultant up-response, 
but if it be of a depressing nature, E will be rendered rela- 
tively the less excitable of the two points, and the response 
will consequently be down. It will thus be seen that that 
upsetting of the balance by which either up- or down-responses 
are induced is due simply to the relatively excitatory or 
depressing effect of the reagent, and is completely inde- 
pendent of any variation of resistance which might be 
brought about by its application. In the course of the 
following investigation, it is to be understood that the elec- 
trical connections are so made that the greater excitation of 
the right-hand contact is always represented by up-response, 
and vice versd. If it be desired to make a comparison 
between the excitatory reactions of two reagents, then the 
two are applied simultaneously, one at E and the other at E’. 
The resulting record then affords us a continuous graphic 
illustration of the relative and varying effects of the two. 

If, again, it is the influence of any agent on conductivity 
that is to be studied, we first take a balanced record and then 
apply the given reagent on an area of about I cm. at Con 
the conductingarm. In this case, the responsive excitabilities 
of the two points E and E’ are the same, but if the effect of 
the agent have been to induce increased conductivity of Cc, 
then the excitation transmitted to the right-hand side, E, will 
be greater, and the response caused by the upsetting of the 
balance will be upwards. Conversely, a down-response will 
indicate that the effect of the agent has been to depress the 
conductivity. Again, we can compare the relative effects in 

II2 


484 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


conductivity-variation induced by two different agents which 
are applied simultaneously, one on the arm C and the other 
enc, 

It is possible again to compare the variation of con- 
ductivity with that of responsivity, by applying one agent at 
a responding region, say E, and the other on the opposite arm 
of the balance atc’. The mode of investigation of receptivity 
changes will be described presently. : 

In fig. 291 we have the complete apparatus. The animal 
or vegetal nerve, N N, rests on non-polarisable electrodes of 


E. E2 Es; E 4 


Fic. 291. Complete Apparatus of Conductivity Balance 


The nerve N supported on electrodes E,, E,;. The two other electrodes 
E, E, are not used in this experiment, but are employed for experiments 
on electrotonus; T, thermal stimulator, the relative lengths of the 
arms of the balance being adjusted by the slide s. 


a U-shape. For the present experiments, two electrodes, E, 
and £,, are sufficient, their mutual distance being capable of 
any variation by movement along a sliding-bar. The same 
apparatus might be used for experiments on electrotonus, in 
which two additional electrodes would be required. The 
position of the electrothermic stimulator T is capable of very 
careful adjustment for purposes of balance, by means of the 
sliding-rod s. A glass cover, not shown in the figure, fits into 
the groove which is represented by a double dotted line sur- 
rounding the apparatus, and thus enables the chamber con- 
taining the nerve to be kept in a properly humid condition. 


THE CONDUCTIVITY BALANCE 485 


In all these experiments by balance, it is to be borne in 
mind that adjustment is always made for perfect balance 
at the beginning of the record, and represented by a short, 
more or less horizontal, line. te 
In order to show the typical effects of induced variations 
of excitability, in upsetting the balance, I shall first give 
records of experiments carried out on the nerve of frog. 
Dilute sodium carbonate is known to be an agent which 
enhances excitability. A long-continued application, or the 
application of a stronger dose, 
may, however, bring about a 
depression. When a dilute 
solution of Na,CO, was ap- 


Fic. 292. Effect of Na,CO, Solution 
on Responsive Excitability of Frog’s 
Nerve 

In this and following records the hori- 
zontal line at the beginning indicates 
exact balance. The upsetting of the 
balance in the up-direction repre- 
sents either the enhanced respon- 
sivity of the right-hand responding 
eg _E, = the Leer a con- 

uctivity of the right-hand arm c, 
Riowascueeal eee ont correspond- Fic. ASS SEM Se SRR On Et0g's 
ing absolute or relative depressions. Nerve 
Na,CO, applied to E is seen to exalt The down record shows depression of 
the responsivity of that point. excitability. 


plied at the responsive point E on the right side, the up- 
setting of the balance upwards immediately indicated the 
greater excitability induced by the reagent. The long- 
continued action of this reagent, however, showed that the 
enhanced excitability was undergoing a gradual decline 
(fig. 292). 

In order to exhibit the characteristic upset caused by 
a depressing agent, I employed on another specimen a 
toxic solution of copper sulphate, applying it at E on the 
right. The previous state of equilibrium is seen by the 
horizontal line at the beginning of the record, and the 


486 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


subsequent depression of excitability at E is shown by the 
upsetting of the balance downwards (fig. 293). 

I shall next take up the determination of the changes 
induced by chemical agents on the excitability of plant nerve 
and shall begin by describing the different effects which occur 
on the application of calcium and potassium salts. For this 
purpose, deci-molecular solutions were employed. Fig. 294 

shows the effect of CaCl, on. 
vegetable nerve, the solution 
being applied at E on the 
-right-hand side. It will be 
noticed that this caused an 
upset of the balance, showing 
an increase of excitability 
that becomes considerable 
after the expiration of five 
minutes. In the case of KCl, 
however, this effect was re- 
versed, that is to say, a de- 
pression was induced. This 
, is seen in fig. 295, where the 
_ Fic. 294. Photographic Record show- balanced record gives way, 
ing Enhancement of Responsivity Gat te diphasic, and :atter- 


by Application of CaCl, 
CaCl, applied to E is seen to exalt wards to a down-response, 


the responsivity of that point. indicating an effect of de- 

| pression at E. These two 
experiments show the effect of the basic moiety in in- 
ducing changes of responsive excitability. 

I shall next describe experiments by which the simul- 
taneous effects of two different reagents on the responsivity 
of a given tissue may be compared. For this purpose, one 
agent is applied at one end of the balance E, the other being 
administered at E’. In the case of animal nerve, it was 
shown by Griitzner, that both NaCl and NaBr induce ex- 
citatory effects, that induced by NaBr being relatively the 
greater. But the continued action of either of these reagents 


THE CONDUCTIVITY BALANCE 487 


induces depression, which sets in earlier in the case of NaBr. 
The effect of these two reagents on vegetable nerve is pre- 


Fic. 295. Photographic Record showing Depression of Responsive 
Excitability by Application of KCl 


cisely the same, as will be seen from an inspection of the 
record given in fig. 296. The NaBr was applied on the 


Fic. 296. Photographic Record exhibiting Comparative Effects of 
NaCl and NaBr on Responsivity 


NaCl was applied on E’ and NaBr on £, the formula being E’yaciEnapr- 
The record shows the greater and earlier effect of NaBr at E in 
causing relative excitation followed by relative depression. 


right-hand side E, and NaCl on the left-hand E’,a process 
which is expressed, for the sake of brevity, by the formula 
E'wacil’wapr =0Lhe greater and earlier. excitatory effect of 


488 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


NaBr, applied on the right-hand side, is shown by the 
resultant up-responses. But after a time, E being now 
depressed by. the continued, action of NaBr, the effect of 
NaCl, applied on the left, becomes relatively predominant, 
a fact demonstrated by the upset of the balance in the oppo- 
site direction, with concomitant down-responses. 

We shall next take up the subject of variations induced 
in conductivity. We have seen that dilute solutions of 
Na,CO, have the effect of exalting responsive excitability. 


FIG. 297. Photographic Record of Effect of Dilute (+5 per cent.) Solution 
of Na,CO, on Variation of Conductivity 


Reagent applied on right arm c. Record shows immediate enhancement 
of conductivity giving rise to up-curves, followed by depression, seen 
in down-curves. Note the appearance of a down-twitch at the be- 
ginning of the sixth response due to the later arrival of excitation at E. 
Note further the replacement of up- by increasing down-responses. 


Long-continued applications, or strong solutions, however, 
have the effect of inducing a depression. Similarly, I find 
that this reagent has the effect of enhancing conductivity, 
provided the solution is sufficiently dilute. In the case of 
the petioles of ferns, a 2 per cent. solution was found to 
induce a preliminary exaltation of excitability, followed 
by a depression (p. 136). In dealing with the conductivity- 
variation in certain isolated vegetable nerves, however, a 
2 per cent. solution was found to induce a depression of con- 
ductivity, but a °5 per cent. solution caused an enhancement 


THE CONDUCTIVITY BALANCE 489 


of conductivity, followed, after long-continued action, by 
depression. 

These facts are illustrated in an extremely interesting 
manner in the records given in figs. 297 and 298. In both 
these cases the solution was applied on the right arm of the 
balance at C, the difference being only that in the first experi- 
ment the strength of solution was ‘5, and in the second 2 per 
cent. An inspection of fig. 297 shows that the application 
of the first induced a great and immediate enhancement of 
conductivity, causing resultant up-responses, which were par- 
ticularly marked during the first four minutes. This increased 


Fic. 298. Photographic Record of Effect of Stronger Dose (2 per cent.) 
of Na,CO, Solution on Conductivity. 


The solution was applied on the right arm of the balance c. Note grow- 
ing depression and appearance of diphasic effect. 


conductivity is then seen to undergo a continuous decrease 
and reversal into growing depression, as seen in the substi- 
tution of increasing down-responses. This record deserves 
special attention, inasmuch as it affords us an insight into a 
phenomenon which could not otherwise have been suspected. 
Greater conductivity is usually associated with increased 
velocity of transmission. It would appear, however, that the 
term conductivity really covers two different phenomena 
which may not always be concomitant. That is to say, 
an increase of conductivity may mean either a greater speed 
of transmission of excitation or a greater intensity of the 


490 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


excitation transmitted. In the first four records of the present 
series the induced enhancement of conductivity is shown by 
the occurrence of up-responses only. The fifth record, how- 
ever, shows a marked preliminary twitch in the negative 
direction, followed by an up-response of some amplitude. 
This shows that the excitatory effect reached the. right end 
E later than the left, though the intensity still remained 
greater. The continued action of the reagent subsequently ~ 
reduced the intensity also, so that this diphasic ultimately 
became converted into a purely monophasic down-response, 
gradually increasing to a maximum. In fig. 298 we observe 
the depression of conductivity by a stronger dose of 2 per 
cent. solution of Na,CO,, applied on the right-hand side at C. 
Here, again, we can see the separated effects of the two 
elements of conductivity—that is to say, the intensity of the 
effect transmitted and the speed of transmission. In the first 
‘few responses of this series we see the diminished intensity 
of transmission to the right giving rise to resultant responses 
which are entirely downwards. Later, this transmission of 
enfeebled excitation becomes delayed also, and by the phase- 
difference thus induced we obtain the growing diphasic effects 
which have already been fully explained on p. 144, fig. 100. 
Owing now to this growing difference of phase, the two 
opposed effects no longer neutralise each other to the same 
extent as before, and we obtain increasing amplitude of both 
the constituent phases. The down-curve in the diphasic 
response represents the earlier arrival, and relatively greater 
intensity, of effect at the left contact E’. And the up-curve 
shows the later arrival of the less intense effect at the right- 
hand contact: E. It is thus clearly seen that conductivity 
includes two different elements of speed and intensity which 
may not in all cases be coincident. 

I shall next describe experiments which will demonstrate 
the variation of conductivity versus that of responsive excit- 
ability under the action of the same reagent. In animal nerve 
responsive excitability is diminished by the action of strong 
solutions of neutral salts, and potassium salts induce greater 


THE CONDUCTIVITY BALANCE 491 


depression than corresponding sodium salts. But neutral 
salts, generally speaking, affect conductivity to a much 
slighter extent than responsivity. There is, however, a very 
curious exception to this rule in the case of animal nerve, 
where 6'1 per cent. of Nal is found to affect the conductivity 
to a much greater extent than the responsive excitability. 
I find a remarkable parallelism to these effects in the case of 
vegetable nerve, which 
is capable of striking 
demonstration by the 
comparative method of 
simultaneous variations 
of conductivity and 
excitability already de- 
scribed. In order to 
demonstrate these con- 
trasted effects of KI 
and Nal on conductivity 
and excitability, I shall 
here give an account 
of two different experi- 
ments. In the first, 
after obtaining the pre- 
liminary balance, KI 
was applied at ton Fic. 299. KESPONSIVITY versus 


ie pia ; CONDUCTIVITY under KI 
SHE Sie eee, SOS saat This photographic record shows the effect of 


reagent being also ap- KI on responsivity and conductivity when 

Tiead th ee reagent applied at £’ and C simultaneously. 
pile at € end E o The formula is E’x;Cx;. Record shows 
the left arm, this pro- greater depression of responsivity than of 


; conductivity. 
cess being represented 


by the formula E’,..C,,.. The record seen in fig.'299 shows, 
by its resultant up-responses, that a greater depression of 
responsivity at E’ than of conductivity at c has been induced. 
In the next experiment (fig. 300) Nal was applied instead of 
KI, on C to the right, and E’ to the left, the formula thus 
being E’,..C.., The resultant responses were now down- 
wards, showing that there was a relatively greater depression 


492 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


of conductivity than of responsivity. In respect of conduc- 
tivity and responsivity, therefore, the effects of these two 
drugs, KI and Nal, are seen to be opposite. 

In order to observe the effect of alcohol on nervous tissue, 
by means of the conductivity balance, I first experimented 
on the nerve of frog. A 5 per cent. solution was applied 
at the responding point E. This is seen (fig. 301) to induce 
a depression of responsivity. 
A more dilute solution generally 
induces a preliminary exaltation 
followed by depression. 

We said in the previous 
chapter that when alcohol 
vapour was passed into the 
chamber of the vegetable nerve 
the responses underwent a rapid 
abolition. This result, however, 


FIG. 300. RESPONSIVITY versus 
CONDUCTIVITY under Nal 


The formula in this case is E’ya1Cnar. 
Photographic record shows an 
effect opposite to that of KI as 


Fic. 301. Effect of Alcohol on 
the Responsivity of Frog’s 


previously described, there being Nerve 

now a relatively greater de- Upsetting of the balance in the 
pression of conductivity than of downward direction shows 
responsivity. depression. 


was due to the joint action of the variations of receptivity, 
conductivity, and responsivity, some of which may possibly 
have been in the positive and others in the negative 
direction. In order to determine the effect of each of these 
we must, then, perform separate experiments. Such a deter- 
mination I have made, using the method of the so-called 
‘negative variation, in which the proximal galvanometric 


THE CONDUCTIVITY BALANCE 493 


contact was on an unkilled, and the distal on a killed 
area. The first of these experiments was on variation of 
receptivity. The thermal stimulator was provided with mica 
shields, so that the receptive area was strictly circumscribed 
at the centre of the thermal platinum loop. Normal responses 
were first taken; the receptive area was next touched with 
I per cent. solution of alcohol, and the modified responses 
were recorded. The results are seen in fig. 302, which gives 


Fic, 302, Photographic Record of Effect of Alcohol Vapour on 
Receptivity 


The three normal responses to the left are seen to be exalted after applica- 
tion of ether onsthe receptive point. 


a striking demonstration of the increased receptivity induced 
by dilute alcohol. 

The effect on conductivity, however, is in curious con- 
trast to this, On applying I per cent. solution in the 
conducting region between the stimulator and the proximal 
contact, a very great diminution of the conducting power 
is observed, as seen in fig. 303. It may be stated here 
that a similar enhancement of receptive excitability, and 
depression of conductivity, are found to be the result of the 
action of alcohol in animal nerve also.. In the next ex- 
periment, it is the variation of responsivity under the action 
of dilute alcohol which is tested. After taking the normal 


4904 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


a 


responses as usual, a I per cent. solution of alcohol was 
applied at the proximal contact. It will be seen from the 
record in fig. 304 that the immediate effect was a depression 


Fic. 303. Photographic Record of Effect of Alcohol on Conductivity 


The three large responses to the left show the normal effect of transmitted 
excitation. Responses almost abolished, as seen on the right, by 
depression of conductivity. 


of the amplitude of response. This subsequently becomes 
converted into a diphasic response, consisting of a preliminary 
positive followed by the normal negative; and finally the 


Fic. 304. Photographic Record showing Effect of Alcohol on 
Responsivity 


a, normal responses, depressed, after application of alcohol, to d@; and 
converted later to abnormal positive responses c. 


response was totally reversed to positive, by the abolition of 
the true excitatory effect. 

It is thus seen that while dilute alcohol exalts the recep- 
tive excitability, it induces a depression of both conductivity 


THE CONDUCTIVITY BALANCE 495 


and responsivity. I shall now describe further experiments 
by which the. relative effects of alcohol are compared, as 
between conductivity and receptivity, and as between recep- 
tivity and responsivity. : 

For the purposes of such a comparison, a new balancing 
arrangement has to be employed (fig. 305). Here, two 
electro-thermic stimulators 
are in series, so that ex- 
citations may be produced 
at two different points 
simultaneously. The gal- 
vanometer contacts E’ and 


Fic. 305. Diagrammatic Repre- 
sentation of Experimental Ar- 
rangement for Demonstration 
of RECEPTIVITY versus CON- FIG. 306. RECEPTIVITY versus RESPON- 
DUCTIVITY, or of RECEPTIVITY SIVITY under Alcohol 
versus RESPONSIVITY 


Alcohol was applied at the receptive point 


s and s’ are exciting thermal loops to the left R’, and the responsive point 
in series ; R and R’, the enclosed to the right E. The formula was 
receptive points ; C and Cc’, con- R'aic.Eatc, The photographic record 
ducting arms; E and E’, the shows the relative enhancement of 
responsive points. receptivity. 


E are made with two points intermediate between the stimu- 
lators. The distance of one of the two stimulators is kept 
constant, at, say, 2 cm. to the left of E’, while the other is 
moved nearer to, or further from, E, until a balance is obtained. 
A 1 per cent. solution of alcohol is then applied to the left 
receptive point, R’, and the right conducting area, C, the 
formula now being R’,.C,,... The fact that the receptive 
excitability is heightened by this reagent, and conductivity 
depressed, receives independent confirmation “from the upset 
of the balance, giving rise to a downward response. 


496 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


The next experiment consists of a comparison of the 
simultaneous variations of receptivity and_ responsivity. 
Alcohol is applied at R’ and E, the formula thus being 
RlacEace .And we find here in confirmation. of our 
previous results that, on account of the opposite effects of 
this agent on the receptive and responsive excitabilities, the 
resultant response is downwards (fig. 306), showing that the 
receptivity has been relatively exalted. Thus the experi- 
ments which I have here described show that the same agent 
may have different effects on receptive and _ responsive 
excitability, and thus accentuate the necessity of clearly 
distinguishing between the two. 


CHAPTER XXXIV 


EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE AND AFTER-EFFECTS 
OF STIMULUS ON CONDUCTIVITY 


Effect of temperature in inducing variations of conductivity : (2) by Method of 
Mechanical Response ; (4) by Method of Electric Balance—Effect of cold— 
Effect of rising temperature—The Thermal Cell—After-effect of stimulation 
on conductivity—The Avalanche Theory—Determination of the after-effect 
of stimulus on conductivity by the Electrical Balance—After-effect of moder- 
ate stimulation—After-effect of excessive stimulation. 


IN studying the effect of temperature in inducing variations 
of conductivity, we may use either of two different methods— 
in the first place the method of mechanical, or in the second 
that of electrical response. For the first of these it is neces- 
sary to have what is generally known as a ‘sensitive’ plant, 
the leaves or leaflets of which afford conspicuous motile 
indications of the arrival of the excitatory wave from 
a distance. In such a case the time-interval between the 
application of stimulus and the response of a leaflet at a 
known distance gives us a measure of the velocity of con- 
duction ; and if we carry out successive experiments at 
different temperatures we have a means of determining the 
effect of temperature on conductivity. Employing this 
method, I have elsewhere given a, determination of the 
effect of temperature on the velocity of transmission in 
Biophytum sensitivum. It was there shown that lower- 
ing of temperature reduced the velocity of transmission 
even to the extent of abolition, when the cooling was suf- 
ficiently intense. With moderate cooling the velocity was 
found to be decreased to about one-third. ‘The effect of 
rise of temperature was, on the contrary, an increase of 
KK 


498 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


velocity. When it rose from 30° C. to 35° C., for example, 
the velocity was doubled. 

By employing the electrical method of response, however, 
we are rendered independent of the use of sensitive plants, 
and by means of the Conductivity Balance we are enabled to 
demonstrate the slightest variation of conductivity, as between 
the left arm of the balance, which is maintained at standard 
temperature, and the right, which is subjected to the given | 
change. , 

Thus in a definite experiment on a nerve of fern the 
temperature of the room was 30°C. After first obtaining 
the balanced record, the temperature of a portion of the 
right arm of the balance was lowered. This one-sided 
cooling was effected by supporting the right arm of the 
nerve, through a certain length, in the concavity of a U-tube 
through which cold water at 15° C. was passed. Stimuli 
were now applied at intervals of one minute. Previously, 
as will be understood, such stimuli, owing to balance, had 
induced no resultant effect. But now, on account of the 
depression of conductivity on the right side, brought about 
by cooling, the balance was disturbed, and the resultant 
down-response seen in fig. 307 shows the diminished con- 
ductivity of the right arm. On the ‘gessation of the flow of 
cold water the balance was gradually restored, in concomit- 
ance with the return to the original temperature. 

I next investigated the results of a rise of temperature, and 
here I specially desired to observe the conductivity variations, 
not at any one degree, but throughout a graduated and con+ 
tinuous rise. I was confronted at the outset of this investi- 
gation by the difficulty arising from the fact that there was 
no convenient and satisfactory means for the local variation 
of the temperature of the nerve, in definite and known 
degrees. In connection with this there was also the further 
difficulty that a sudden variation of temperature will, in 
itself, act as a stimulus. Hence, in studying the effects of 
temperature per se, it is essential that there should be no 
such sudden variation. These difficulties were overcome by 


EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE ON CONDUCTIVITY 499 


the employment of an electrical arrangement to bring about 
the graduated and continuous rise of temperature. 

A certain length of the vegetable nerve on the right arm 
of the Conductivity Balance was thus raised continuously in 
temperature, and its conductivity compared with that of the 
left arm of the balance, the latter being maintained at the 
temperature of the room, which happened at the time to be 
33°C. The device by means of which this was accomplished 


Fic, 307. Photographic Record showing Effect of Cooling on Con- 
ductivity of Plant-nerve 


Balance was obtained at starting, when temperature of both arms was 
30° C. On cold being applied on right arm, the balance was dis- 
turbed, showing diminished conductivity on that side. On restoration 


of normal temperature, the balance is seen at the end of the record to 
be again restored. 


will be understood from fig. 308. A piece of cork has a 
small chamber cut into it measuring I cm. each way. In 
this is placed moist blotting-paper, which keeps it damp, and 
across it passes a. length of 1 cm. of the right arm of the 
vegetable nerve in the Conductivity Balance. This cork- 
chamber has inlet and outlet tubes ¢ and #.. The first of 
these contains a spiral, H, of platinum, which can be heated 
to a suitable degree by means of an electrical current, the 


KK 2 


500 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


intensity of which is capable of careful adjustment. The 
cork chamber is closed with a cover, through which passes 
a thermometer, T, for the indication of the temperature 
within, The tube Z’ is connected with an aspirator, and air 
is thus sucked in by 4, and, passing through the platinum 
spiral, is warmed, and raises the temperature of the nerve in 
the chamber. This rise of tem- 
perature is adjusted (1) by regu- 
+ lating the electrical current which | 
heats the spiral, and (2) by con- 
trolling the inflow of air... As 
regards the first of these two 
processes, the electrical heating- 
circuit has a carbon rheostat 
interposed, by which the rate of 
rise of temperature may be 
regulated. The movement of the 
current of air, on the other hand, 
is controlled by adjusting the 
stopcock of the aspirator. By the 
joint manipulation of both these 
the rate of rise of temperature 
t inside the chamber may be made 
perfectly uniform, and in my 
yoga ee Rouing ate experiments this rate was approxi- 
Temperature of one Armof mately 1° C. per minute. 
ene As already said, I selected a 
A and B, the two halves of the -  , 
chamber ; T, thermometer ; piece of vegetable nerve and took 
at ia cap aerate a balanced record. After this the 
heating. temperature of the thermal cell 
on the right-hand side was raised 
continuously, the response-record being taken at each degree 
of the rise, till a temperature of 50° C. had beenat tained. 
From the record given in fig. 309 it will be seen that the 
conductivity was always greater at temperatures up to 47° C. 
than it was on the left-hand side, which was all the time 
maintained at the constant temperature of 33°C. At 48°C, 


EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE ON CONDUCTIVITY 501 


however, the reversal of response showed that the conduc- 
tivity was now being depressed. And at still higher tem- 
peratures it was found to undergo a very great depression, 
as is seen by the abrupt downward movement of the curve. 
It is thus seen that, by means of the Method of Balance, this 
very difficult problem of the variation of conductivity under 
variation of temperature is made capable of exact study. 

I shall next describe the results of an investigation into 
the after-effects of stimulus on conductivity and excitability, 


Fic. 309. Photographic Record Showing Effect of Rising Péapelatons 
on Conductivity 


Balance obtained at starting at 33° C. Successive responses recorded at 
each degree C. of rise of temperature. Record shows increasing 
conductivity up to 472 C. A depression of conductivity is seen by 
reversal of curve to set in at 48° C., and this becomes extremely pro- 
nounced at 50° €. 


a subject of much difficulty and of considerable theoretical 
importance. It has been found in Animal Physiology 
that the sciatic nerve of a frog, for instance, is not equally 
excitable throughout its length. When such a nerve, with 
its attached terminal muscle, is cut off from the spinal cord, it 
is seen to be more excitable the further from the muscle is 
the point on the nerve that is subjected to stimulation. 
From this fact that excitation increased with the distance of 
the point excited from the motor organ, Pfliiger was led to 


502 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


the ‘ Avalanche Theory,’ namely, that during the passage of 
excitation down the nerve it. actually gathers strength. But it 
is clear that this cannot be true, since we have seen that, other 
things being equal, excitation is always greater the nearer the 
point of stimulation to the responding region, and on this fact 
have depended all those experiments already described, which 
involved a delicate balance of equal excitations. It follows 
that the observed enhancement of excitability of a point on the 
nerve which is distal from the muscle, and in the neighbour- 
hood of a section, must be ascribed to some other cause. In 
reference to this Heidenhain, indeed, explained the greater 
excitability of higher tracts of divided nerve by the proximity 
of the artificial section. For the lower end of the nerve at 
once exhibits the same marked activity as the upper end if 
a section be made lower down. Excitability is, in fact, 
raised near the section, wherever the section may be. The 
distance travelled by the excitation could not, therefore, be 
the determining factor in the magnitude of effect. For so far 
from increasing it, this, as a matter of fact, causes diminution. 
It is to be remembered that though the excitability is 
increased near the point of section, yet at the section itself 
it is almost abolished, otherwise there could not have been 
any response by'so-called negative variation. The question 
now arises, Why should the excitability be raised near: the 
point of section ? 

It has been supposed that this was due to certain electri- 
cal changes induced by section, which in turn gave rise to 
electro-tonic variations of excitability. We shall see, in 
Chapter XL, that the passage of an electrical current through 
a living tissue induces changes of excitability. And this 
phenomenon is known as the electro-tonic effect.. Now any 
‘injury, such as a mechanical or thermal section, is known 
to induce galvanometric negativity, or anodic change, at or 
near the point of section. But it is the kathode-effect which 
is excitatory. And the observed greater excitability of the 
nerve near a point of section is supposed to be due to kat- 
electrotonus, produced within a certain tract from the cross- 


OE i at ty ok de ie 


AFTER-EFFECT OF STIMULUS ON CONDUCTIVITY 503 


section by internal short-circuiting of the nerve-current. 
That this explanation, however, does not meet all the 
requirements of the case will appear from certain experi- 
ments which I shall describe, where, under exactly similar 
electrotonic changes due to section, a result directly the 
opposite of this, that is to say, of depression, is seen to be 
induced. . 

All these various facts will be found fully reconcilable, 
however, on the basis of a proposition which I shall establish, 
namely that zz a nerve, moderate stimulation enhances ex- 
citability and conductivity, while excessive stimulation has the 
opposite effect of bringing about the depression of both. It is 
indeed natural to expect that while moderate stimulation, by 
increasing molecular mobility, would bring about one effect, 
excessive stimulus, by inducing overstrain, would result in 
exactly the opposite. Before proceeding to give an experi- 
mental demonstration of this hypothesis, we shall first 
consider the explanation which it affords of the peculiar 
excitatory changes observed in the case of cross-sectioned 
nerve. In the first place we know that a cut acts as a 
stimulus. And since we found that the effect of stimulus 
decreases with the distance from the point of stimulation, it 
would appear that at the section itself the stimulation would 
be excessive ; moderately strong at a certain distance from 
it; and practically negligible when very far away. In 
complete accordance with this is the resulting increase of 
excitability which has been observed near the point of 
section, while at the point itself the nerve is relatively 
inexcitable. 

The fact that stimulation, when not excessive, increases 
the conductivity and excitability, we found illustrated in the 
staircase increase of electrical response, and in the enhance- 
ment of amplitude after tetanisation, in vegetable and animal 
nerves (figs. 275 and 286). The same fact will be demonstrated 
later by means of the mechanical response of nerve. I shall 
now describe certain experiments which demonstrate it once 
more in a new and interesting manner. 2 


504 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


A vegetable nerve was adjusted for balance, with the 
ends projecting some distance beyond the electrodes. In 
order to show that the effect of injury is due to stimulus as 
such, and not to any particular form of it, I now made a 
thermal instead of mechanical section, by applying salt 
solution heated to about 60° C. in the region A, at a distance 
of I cm. to the right of E (fig. 310). The effect of this 
stimulation was to induce a moderate excitation of the right | 
arm of the balance, relatively to the left. If this moderate 
stimulation were to induce 
any increase of excitability 
and conductivity, that fact 
would be demonstrated by 


Fic. 311. Photographic Record 
Showing Effect of Moderate 
Stimulation in Enhancing 
Conductivity and  Excita- 


Fic. 310. Experimental Arrange- 
ment for Studying After-effect of 
Stimulus on Conductivity and 


Excitability 


The stimulator adjusted to obtain 
balance between Eandk’. Stimulus 
of moderate or strong intensity is 
applied to a point on the right of E. 
Upsetting of the balance in an 
_upward direction shows an en- 
hancement, and in a downward 
direction, depression, of con- 
ductivity and excitability. 


bility 

otted line at beginning shows 
the resting-current, as a per- 
sistent effect of stimulation. 
The upsetting of the balance 
upwards constitutes a positive 
variation of the resting- 
current, and indicates en- 
hanced conductivity and 
excitability. 


the upsetting of the balance, the resultant response being 
upwards. That this is what actually occurs will be seen 
from the records in fig. 311. It will be noticed that in 
consequence of stimulation to the right of E, that point 
became, more or less permanently, galvanometrically negative. 
This is represented by the dotted line upwards at the 
beginning of the record. It must be remembered that before 
the application of the thermal section, the right and left hand 
excitations, proceeding from the electro-thermically stimu- 


AFTER-EFFECT OF STIMULUS ON CONDUCTIVITY 505 — 


lated point in the middle, were exactly equal and balanced. 
The fact that after this application, however, there are 
resultant responses which are upwards, shows, as already 
said, that by the moderate stimulation of the right-hand 
side, both excitability and conductivity have been increased. 
The resultant upward response here is, then, in the same direc- 
tion as the so-called ‘ current of injury, and forms, as it were, 
a positive variation of it. 

In another experiment, in which I wished to try the 
effect of excessive stimulation, instead of applying a hot 
solution at 60° C., I produced 
greater injury and consequent 
excessive stimulation, by scorch- 
ing the nerve at the same point 
as before, witha red-hot platinum 
wire. In this case resultant 
response was downwards, show- 
ing that the excitability and 
conductivity of the right-hand 


Fic. 312. Photographic Record 


side of the balance had been showing Effect of Excessive 
; P Stimulation in Depressing Ex- 
depressed by over-stimulation. citability and Conductivity 


I was next desirous: of Up-line at starting shows the rest- 


: . ing-current due to after-effect of 
demonstrating that the excita- stimulation. The upsetting of 


bility of the over-stimulated or balance in a downward direction 

: ‘ constitutes a negative variation 

excited point undergoes depres- of the resting-current, and shows 

sion. For this purpose I took depression of conductivity and 
excitability. 


a fresh specimen and first ob- 
tained a state of balance. Similar excitation of E’ and E 
produced a balanced or null effect. The point E was then 
injured by touching it with a hot platinum wire. On now 
proceeding to take records, it is seen that the responses 
were downwards, showing the depression of excitability at 
the injured E (fig. 312). 

The fact that galvanometric negativity had been induced 
at E, by reason of injury, is demonstrated at the beginning 
of the record as an up-line. The subsequent resultant 
responses due to simultaneous excitation of E and E’ are 


506 ~~. COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


-seen to be a negative variation of the resting-current due 
to injury. It is thus seen that while simultaneous-excita- 
tions of two normally excitable points E and E’ are prevented 
by balance from giving rise to any response, the excitatory 
response becomes manifest when the balance is disturbed 
by the injury of either point of galvanometric contact ; and 
that, under these circumstances, the response is a negative 
variation of the current of injury. This experiment is | 
important as giving a theoretical insight into the so-called 
response by negative variation. It also shows how limited 
is the applicability of the assumption that response is always 
by negative variation. For, in the similar experiment, 
previously described, under moderate injury, the response was 
by positive variation of the resting-current. 

It is further seen from these experiments that the 
enhancement of excitability, under the stimulation due to 
moderate injury, could not be caused by the suggested 
electrotonic effect. For the same anodic change induced 
by injury at E causes, in the case of moderate injury, an 
enhancement, and under greater injury a depression, of 
excitability. It is thus clear that the modifying influence 
is the effective intensity of stimulation. This fact, that 
moderate stimulation enhances, and excessive stimulation 
depresses excitability, will be further demonstrated ina future 
chapter, by the independent method in which the effects of 
electrotonus are completely eliminated. ) 


CHAPTER XXXV 
MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE 


Current assumption of non-motility of nerve—Shortcomings of galvanometric 
modes of detecting excitation—Mechanical response to continuous electric 
shocks—Optical Kunchangraph—Effect of ammonia on the mechanical 
response of nerve—Effect of morphia—Action of alcohol—Of chloroform— 
Abnormal positive or expansive response converted into normal contractile 
through diphasic, after tetanisation—Similar effects in mechanical response 
of vegetable nerve—Mechanical response due to transmitted effects of 
stimulation—Determination of velocity of transmission—Indeterminateness 
of velocity in isolated nerve—Kunchangraphic records on smoked glass— 
Oscillating recorder—Mechanical response of afferent nerve—Record of 
mechanical response of nerve due to transmitted stimulation, in gecko— 
Fatigue of conductivity—Conversion of normal contractile response into 
abnormal expansive, through diphasic, due to fatigue. 


I HAVE already referred to the distinctions which are com- 
monly insisted on, as between the reactions of different 
animal tissues. Certain of these are regarded as motile 
and others as. non-motile. From an evolutionary point of 
view, however, it is difficult to conceive of such a hard-and- 
fast distinction. It would be easier, believing -in continuity, 
to suppose that a certain responsive reaction, characteristic 
of the simplest living substance, had become accentuated 
in some tissues, and not so accentuated in others, according 
to their different functional requirements. Thus the belief 
held so implicitly by physiologists that nerves exhibit no 
motile response whatsoever’ becomes questionable, and is 
seen to require investigation. After submitting it to this, 
moreover, one finds it difficult to understand how such an 

‘ ‘Nerves are irritable; when they are stimulated, a change is produced in 
them ; this change is propagated along the nerve, and is called a nervous impulse ; 


there is no change of form in the nerve visible to the highest powers of the- 
microscope.’ (Kirke’s Handbook of Physiology, 15th edition, p. 105.). 


508 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


idea ever gained currency, unless, indeed, it was due to the 
tyranny imposed on our thought by these arbitrary classifi- 
cations themselves. 

Before entering, however, on the question whether the 
excitatory reaction in nerve finds motile expression or not, 
we shall first examine the only method at present available 
for the detection of the condition of excitation. Since ex- 


cited nerve has hitherto been supposed to exhibit no visible 


change, it followed that the only method possible for the 
detection of the excitatory change was the electrical. In- 
vestigations on nerve, therefore, had perforce to be carried 
out by this means, through the medium either of the capillary 
electrometer or of the sensitive galvanometer. But the elec- 
trical method labours under certain inherent disadvantages, 
and first of these is the objection which it raises to the free 
employment of the most convenient form of stimulus, that, 
- namely, by induction shocks. For we have seen that unless 
extraordinary precautions are taken, we have here, owing to 
the possible escape of current, an element of error and un- 
certainty in the results. If, on the other hand, it should 
become possible to obtain mechanical response from the 
_ nerve, this particular form of stimulation might be employed 
without misgiving. | 

The second limitation which the electrical mode of 
detection imposes upon us is that arising from the differ- 
ential character of the response which it indicates. For 
stimulus induces electrical changes at both the contacts— 
proximal and distal-—the record made being finally due to 
the algebraical summation of the two. It is true that the 
excitability of one contact is artificially depressed by injury. 
But it is often difficult to say how far this injury has been 
effective in completely abolishing the excitability of this 
point. The depression of excitability, due to partial injury, 
will sometimes disappear to a certain extent, with lapse of 
time, and much uncertainty sometimes occurs as to whether 
a certain curious variation in the response of the nerve— 
negative followed by positive—is due to this or some other 


edd A St at a 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE 509 


cause. With mechanical response, however, provided this 
could be rendered practicable, no such difficulty need arise. 
For in that case it would be the direct effect of the exci- 
tatory change, uncomplicated by any other disturbance, 
which would be recorded. 

Finally, as regards the detection of the excitatory change 
itself, the galvanometer is unable to indicate any change 
below a certain high intensity of excitation. Thus it gives 
no indication when excitation is due to one or to a few 
shocks: it can only detect an excitatory effect which is much 
stronger than this, having been brought about by the super- 
posed effects of tetanic shocks of a certain duration. In 
order to obtain even such effects, a galvanometer of very 
high’ sensitiveness is necessary. That of a fairly delicate 
instrument, detecting a current of about ‘ooI ampere, will 
have to be exalted some ten millions of times before it can 
give efficient indications of excitatory effects in nerves ; and 
in such a degree of galvanometric sensitiveness we approach 
a limit which cannot be very much exceeded. 

Returning now to our original question, we have first to 
determine whether excitation causes any motile effect in 
nerve. Under observation, it is easily seen that when the 
nerve is excited by tetanic electrical shocks it increases in 
thickness and at the same time shortens in length. We 
have here a phenomenon in every way analogous to the 
thickening and shortening of muscle under excitation. The 
contraction which occurs in nerve, moreover, is of an order 
by no means microscopic. I give here a record (fig. 313) 
of the contractile response of nerve under continuous 
stimulation by fairly strong tetanising electric shocks. This 
record was obtained by means of the ordinary lever-recor@er, 
the magnification employed being only three times. The 
induced contraction in this particular case was about 14 per 
cent. It will also be seen that this contraction reached a limit, 
at which state of maximum contraction the nerve remained 
for a considerable time. After this we observe a tendency 


5 £0 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


to decline, owing to fatigue. In some other cases, moreover, 
I have obtained a contraction of as much as 20 per cent. 

If we wish to obtain a series of successive responses, 
however, it is desirable to avoid over-stimulation of the 
tissue. In order, then, to obtain a response-record under 
moderate stimulation, we have to employ a higher magni- 
fication.. This magnification, if made about 200 times, is 
more than sufficient for all practical purposes, and the photo- 
graphic records given in the course of the present chapter 
are of this order. With long ‘specimens of nerve, however, 
a magnification of fifty times would be enough, and in the 


Fic. 313. Record of Contractile Response in Frog’s Nerve under 
Continuous Electric Tetanisation. 


Magnification, three times. 


course of the next chapters, I shall give certain records on this 
scale, obtained directly on a smoked glass surface. The 
apparatus used for the purpose was the Kunchangraph 
(Sanskrit, Aunchan, contraction), which I had already devised 
afd employed in recording the contractile responses of plant- 
tissues. This apparatus, as adapted for the purpose of 
recording mechanical response in nerves, consists of, first, 
a nerve-chamber, N; secondly, a modified Optical Lever, 0 ; 
and thirdly, a photographic recorder, D (fig. 314). 

_ Of these, the nerve-chamber consists of a small rectangular 
ebonite box, the front of which is closed by a semi-cylindrical 


N, nerve chamber containing nerve with electrical connections, E E’, 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE Sil 


FIG, 314. Optical Kunchangraph for Record of Mechanical 
Response of Nerve 


Thread tied to lower end of nerve, and attached to short arm of optic 
lever, O. Beam of light from L reflected from mirror of optical lever, 
oO, falls on recording-drum, D. Adjustment of reflected spot of 
light made by micrometer screw, Ss. Periodic electric stimulation at 
intervals of one minute is automatically made by means of key regulated 
by clock-work. Air bubbles through water at w, and is led on by 
india-rubber tubing, T, to nerve-chamber, thus kept humid. By 
proper manipulation of stop-cock any vapour—as chl oroform—con- 
tained in vessel V, may be passed through nerve-chamb er, subsequent 
responses showing effect. 


S12 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


glass cover. The nerve is placed vertically within this, and 
held, at its upper end, by a clamp. The lower end of the 
nerve is connected with the short arm of the Optical Lever 
by means of a thread, which passes through a hole in the 
floor of the chamber. A second thread of cotton moistened 
with saline solution hangs loosely from the end of the nerve, 
and is connected with the electrode E’.. When the electrodes 
E and E’ are put in connection with the secondary of an 
induction coil R, the entire length of the nerve is subjected to. 
direct excitation. When, on the other hand, we wish to study 
the effect of transmitted excitation, 
the nerve is lightly clamped at B 
(fig. 315). Excitation is then induced 
in the portion of the nerve A a, and 
after transmission through the inter- 
vening tract, causes the motile effect 
in the responding portion of the 
nerve B C, : 

One precaution which I find to be 
very necessary is the maintenance of 
the properly humid condition in the 
i ._,_nerve-chamber. This is_ specially 

IG. 315. Diagrammatic , . ; 

Representation of Ar- important in the warm weather which 

rangonen irae characterises the greater part of the 

Stimulus, 1, indicating year in India. The usual means of 

Sai keeping the chamber moist, by a large 
quantity of blotting-paper soaked in water, is not sufficient 
to bring about the maintenance of the normal excitability of 
the nerve for any length of time. This need was met by 
keeping moist vapour in uniform circulation through the 
nerve-chamber. An air-bag is kept under suitable pressure, 
and the air, bubbling through water in the vessel W, is made 
to enter the nerve-chamber through an entrance-pipe, and 
to escape by an exit-pipe. In warm weather it is well to 
keep fragments of ice in the water-vessel. By proper mani- 
pulation of the stop-cock of the air-bag, a gentle stream of 
cooled and humid air is kept in constant circulation through 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE 513 


the chamber. Observing these precautions, I have been 
able to obtain responses from a given nerve for as much as 
three hours continuously, whereas, without this care, they 
would have come to a stop in a very short time. By a 
modification of this arrangement, we are also enabled to 
study the effect on the excitability of the nerve of various 
gases and vapours contained in a second vessel, v. A series 
of responses is first taken, under normal conditions—that 
is to say, when the nerve is surrounded simply by a moist 
atmosphere. On now turning a three-way tap in a given 
direction, the water-vapour can be made to: pass through the 
vessel V, filled with the given gas or vapour, before reaching 
the nerve-chamber. The series of responses then obtained 
will show either the immediate or the after-effect of the 
reagent at will. For it is easy, by means of the three-way 
cock, to shut off the gas and re-establish the first or normal 
condition, after which the responses will afford an indication 
of the nature of the after-effect. 

_ The lower end of the nerve, as has been said, is attached 
to the arm of the lever which passes through the fulcrum-rod. 
A light .mirror is fixed on the fulcrum-rod, its face being 
downwards. The pull caused by the excitatory contraction of 
the nerve causes rotation of the fulcrum-rod, and this in turn 
gives rise to a deflection of the spot of light reflected from 
the mirror. A responsive relaxation of the nerve would 
give rise, on the other hand, to a deflection of the spot of 
light in the opposite direction. The long arm of the lever, 
it will be noticed, is here the ray of light. The responsive 
movement of the spot of light is recorded on a moving 
photographic plate vertically below the mirror, and whose 
movement, regulated by clockwork, is in a direction at right 
angles to that of the spot of light. The photographic plate, 
or the film wrapped round the drum, moves under a fixed 
wooden cover, not shown in the figure, which is provided with 
a narrow incised slit. The length of this is parallel to the 
direction of the movement of light, and at right angles to that 
of the plate or film. The advantage of having the plate 


LL 


514 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


vertically below the mirror lies in the fact that a lighted 
candle may be placed in the dark room without spoiling 
the record by diffuse illumination. The only way in which 
such diffuse light could now find access to the plate would 
be by reflection from the ceiling. But if the ceiling of the 
experimental room is blackened, or a black cover placed over 
the nerve-chamber at a certain height, even this possibility is 
eliminated. The advantage which the observer enjoys, when, 
instead of groping in semi-darkness, he can work in a fairly 
well-lighted room is obvious. By making the arm of the 
lever to which the nerve is attached sufficiently short, and by 
placing the recording plate sufficiently far away, a wide 
range of magnification, from several hundreds to several 
thousands, may be obtained. It may sometimes be desirable 
to subject the nerve to a certain amount of tension, and 
this is secured by placing a small weight on the arm of 
the lever. With high magnification, due adjustment, which 
is very troublesome, lies in bringing the spot of light con- 
veniently over the recording plate. This difficulty is obviated, 
however, by means of a fine micrometer screw S which moves 
the whole nerve-chamber up or down, in relation ‘to the 
Optical Lever. The adjustment of this screw in a right- 
handed manner then moves the spot of light in one direction, 
say to the left, while its left-handed rotation moves it to the 
right. This movement can be made very fine, and the spot 
adjusted to any part of the photographic field. 

It remains to deal with the possible disturbances inci- 
dental to the high magnification employed. Apprehension, 
in this matter, is often more fanciful than real. Disturbances 
might no doubt occur, however, when proper conditions are 
not secured for the experiment. If the nerve-chamber, for 
example, be supported on a different stand from that of the 
Optical Lever, then the slightest tremor of the common 
pedestal would result in relative movements of the two 
supports, causing constant disturbance of the spot of light. 
Under these conditions, heavy stone pedestals, erected on 
steady foundations, afford no security against the ground- 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE 515 


vibrations of a busy city. But when both the nerve- 
chamber and the Optical Lever are fixed to the same 
supporting-rod, relative movements, due to external disturb- 
ance, are practically eliminated. This common supporting- 
rod may be screwed securely to a wall. With these precautions, 
I have been able to take records, without the least dis- 
turbance from the adjacent electric tram line. Asa matter of 
fact, when the magnification required is only of a few hundred 
times, nothing but gross carelessness could allow any source 
of disturbance to remain. It is only when the magnification 
has to be pushed to the order of a hundred thousand that 
unusual care is necessary to avoid errors of disturbance. 
One precaution which should, however, be taken, is that 
arising from disturbance of the mirror by convection 
currents of air. The remedy for this is obvious, namely; 
a suitable glass cover. 

This is the order of magnification which is necessary for 
the recording of response under a degree of stimulation 
- usual in making observations of excitatory electrical variation 
with a very sensitive galvanometer. But while the sensitive- 
ness of the galvanometric method of detecting response is 
here nearing its limit, that of the mechanical method is in its 
first stage only, and how greatly the sensitiveness of the 
latter may be exalted when required will be shown in the 
next chapter. 

I shall: ‘flow “explain how easy it is to study the aiisin: 
logical variations induced in the animal nerve under various 
agencies by means of the mechanical response. The following 
experiments were performed on specimens of the sciatic 
nerve of frog. A well-known reagent for abolition of ex- 
citability of the nerve is ammonia. Its effect on mechanical 
response is seen in fig. 316. In all the following experiments, 
the stimulus applied was by fairly strong tetanising electrical 
shocks, which were usually of two seconds’ duration. Two 
series of records were taken, successive responses being 
recorded at intervals of one minute, before and after the 


application of the chemical reagent. In fig. 316, the normal 
L L 2 


516 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


responses seen in the first series are found to be abolished 
when the nerve has been subjected to strong vapour of 
ammonia for some time. It should be mentioned here that 
this abolition takes place under the action of a strong dose. 
When highly diluted with air, the vapour ey cause a 
temporary exaltation. 

In the next figure (fig. 317) is shown the effect of szorphia. 
After the application of this solution for a certain length of 
time, the response is seen to 
‘be abolished. The strength 
of application which brings 
about this abolition I find to 
vary according to the condi- 


Fic. 316. Photographic Record 
of Effect of Ammonia. on 
Mechanical Response of Frog’s 


Nerve FiG.. “317: Photographic 
First series of responses are nor- Record showing Abolition 
mal. Second series show effect of Mechanical Response 
of ammonia in practical aboli- of Frog’s Nerve by Action 
tion of response. of Solution of Morphia 


tion of the nerve. Another agent by which the mechanical 
response of the nerve is found to be abolished is aconite. 
And it is of special interest to note that I have often found 
this to act as an antidote, for the revival of response 
previously almost completely abolished by morphia. ‘The 
condition of the nerve here also appears to be a determining 
factor in the mutually antidotal action of these two poisons. 
A strong application of alcohol after long-continued action 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE 517 


abolishes the response of nerve. But its preliminary effect 
is often one of exaltation, as seen in fig. 318. 

I shall next describe the effect of chloroform, which dis- 
plays many interesting features. We have seen that when a 
tissue is excited by impinging stimulus, two opposite effects 
are induced : one of these is the increase of energy, by the 
absorption of stimulus, and the other is the expenditure 
of energy by excitatory response. The former, as we have 
seen, finds expression in galvanometric 
positivity and expansion. The latter, 
on the other hand, is exhibited: as 
galvanometric negativity and contrac- 
tion. In the record of excitatory 
response, the former of these elements 
is generally masked by the predominant 
negative or contractile effect. We have 
also seen that this hidden positive may 
be unmasked. in either of two: ways: 
first, by retarding the expression of one 
effect in relation to the other ; or, second, 
by abolishing the excitatory negative 
altogether. In the first of these cases, 
the negative response is converted into 
diphasic, say positive followed by Fic. 318. Photographic 

: Record showing Pre- 
negative. In the latter, the response limitiary Bimaltation it 
becomes positive, by the suppression Mechanical Response 

: . of Frog’s Nerve after 
of the negative. An example of this Application of Alcohol 
unmasking of the positive element, by 
suppression of the negative, we have already seen to occur 
under the action of chloroform (cf. fig. 49). This demon- 
stration was made on a vegetable tissue, the test employed 
being electrical. 

The experiment which I am now about to describe is 
interesting from the fact that effects parallel to those there seen 
in a vegetable tissue are in it shown to occur also in the highly 
specialised animal nerve. The unmasked ‘electro-positive 
effect, moreover, is here seen to correspond with an expansive 


518 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


response of the tissue. A record of the various phases in 
the effect of chloroform on the mechanical response of nerve 
is found in fig. 319. It will be seen here that the first effect 
of chloroform was to cause a great enhancement of ex- 
citability, which in this case lasted for about a quarter of an 
hour. I have given only two responses of this series. After 
this, the responses began to decline, and another very 


Fic. 319. Photographic Record showing Effect of Chloroform on 
Mechanical Response of Frog’s Nerve 


First pair of responses, normal; second pair, preliminary exaltation on 
application of chloroform; last series exhibit subsequent effect of 
chloroform in unmasking the positive component as diphasic response. 
Expansion is here followed by contraction. Note regular waning of 
both components with growing aneesthetisation. 


interesting reaction made its appearance. The impinging 
stimulus had hitherto induced only an immediate contractile 
response. But by the action of the chloroform the excitatory 
effect was delayed, and the positive, or mechanically ex- 
pansive response was unmasked in the form of a preliminary 
downward twitch. Response was now, therefore, diphasic— 
positive followed by negative. Immediately on the applica- 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE 519 


tion of stimulus, as may be seen from the record, there is a 
sudden expansive movement downwards, followed by an 
equally rapid reversed movement of contraction upwards, 
and this followed again by a slow recovery. Each of the 
successive stimuli evokes the same diphasic responsive 
sequence. It must be noted that the downward twitches are 
the preliminary, and not the after-affect. It is also interesting 
to note, as the tissue approaches death, under the continued 
action of chloroform, how regularly in both negative and 
positive directions the responses decrease in amplitude. 

We shall next undertake an independent investigation 
into the causes which bring about the three types of response 
—abnormal positive, diphasic, and normal negative—known 
to be exhibited in the electrical response of the animal, and 
already demonstrated as occurring also in that of the vege- 
tal nerve. While discussing these three types of electrical 
response and their variations in Chapter XXXI. it was 
stated that the differences of effect involved were due to 
changes in conductivity and excitability, brought. about by 
varying tonic conditions. It was also explained, in the same 
place, that the continued isolation of so highly excitable a 
tissue as nerve, from its accustomed supply of energy, would 
be sufficient of itself to depress its tonic condition below 
par, with concomitant depression of its conductivity and 
excitability. The result of this depression of excitability 
will be to render inefficient a stimulus which was formerly 
efficient, to evoke the true excitatory reaction of galvano- 
metric negativity. The absorbed stimulus will now induce 
only a responsive positivity. The depression of conductivity 
also would cause the transmission: of the hydro-positive, 
instead of the excitatory negative, wave. Owing, then, to the 
joint action of these two factors, stimulus induces a positive 
response—the so-called ‘abnormal ’—at a distant responding 
point, when the tonic condition of the tissue has become 
depressed. Absorption of stimulus, however, by supplying 
the requisite energy, raises the tonic condition, with con- 
sequent restoration of conductivity and excitability. As 


520 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


a result of this, the abnormal positive will pass into normal 
negative response, through an intermediate diphasic, after 
the impact of a series of stimuli, or after tetanisation. The 
enhancement of conductivity and excitability thus conferred 
on the tissue by the absorbed stimulus will now act by 
still further tetanisation, to bring about the  enhance- 
ment of the normal negative response. Starting thus, 
with the most depressed condition of the tissue, and sub-. 
jecting it to continuous action of stimulus, we obtain four 
typical stages : (1) the abnormal, passing after short tetanisa- 
tion into (2) the diphasic ; this in its turn giving place to 
(3) the normal negative alone ; which finally becomes (4) the 
enhanced negative. | 

In studying electrical response, both of animal and vege- 
tal nerves, under appropriate experimental conditions, we 
have already seen various examples of these different types 
of response and their transformations. But under such modes 
of experiment as have been described, the effects were, as 
already stated, due to joint changes in excitability and con- 
ductivity. I shall now, however, describe a still simpler 
experimental arrangement, in which the stimulus is applied 
directly on the tissue, and the responsive variations are, 
therefore, due to variations in the excitability alone. These 
changes, moreover, will be recorded by means of their direct 
mechanical expression, namely, contraction, or its opposite 
expansion. 

With regard to abnormal response, I have already stated 
that this is brought about, not by ‘staleness,’ or the moribund 
condition, with its concomitant chemical changes, but by 
the run-down of the energy of the tissue in isolation. On 
investigating this subject, by means of mechanical response, 
with its superior sensitiveness, this conclusion finds inde- 
pendent support of the strongest character. On taking 
even the freshest specimen, I generally find that its 
responses at first are the abnormal positive. These gradually 
pass into moderate negative through diphasic. This is due 
to the raising of the tonic condition by the absorption of 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE 521 


the stimulus, and after a series of stimulations, the isolated 
tissue, which was originally depressed, has its tonic condition 
so much heightened, that the responses are enhanced to an 
unprecedented magnitude. A specimen, in fact, which was 
at first almost irresponsive, may generally be brought to 
any state of exalted excitability desired, with concomitant 
increase in amplitude of response, by merely subjecting it for 
a certain length of time to the action of impinging stimulus. 


Fic. 320. Photographic Record showing Abnormal Positive converted 
into Negative Response after Tetanisation 


First series, abnormal positive ; second series, persistence of this positive 
after very brief tetanisation ; third series, conversion to negative, after 
a tetanisation of longer duration. 


I shall now describe in detail some of the principal 
experiments, Selecting a specimen of frog’s nerve, I took 
a series of responses to electrical shocks, of three seconds’ 
duration, at intervals, in each case, of one minute. The 
testing stimulus was kept always the same throughout the 
experiment, except for certain intervening periods of tetani- 
sation. The variations seen in the responses thus give 
a visual demonstration of the variations in excitability. 
The record of these is given in fig. 320. The responses in 


522 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


the first series are by the abnormal positive variation; that is 
to say, by expansion. The tissue here being sub-tonic, the 
impinging stimulus could not induce the true excitatory effect. 
The tissue. was now subjected to short-lived tetanisation. 
But the absorbed stimulus was not yet sufficient to induce 
the normal responsiveness. The next series of. records, 

= therefore, still exhibited the 
abnormal positive response. 
Tetanic shocks of longer 
duration were next applied. 
This gave rise to a short- 
lived positive twitch down- 
wards, succeeded by large 
contractile response upwards, 
After the cessation of the 
second tetanisation, the 
absorbed energy is seen to 
have brought the tissue to 
a condition of more or less 
normal responsiveness. This 
is seen in the third series, 
where the first responses are 
diphasic, but the positive 
component (the downward 
twitch) becomes perceptibly 


Fic. 321. Photographic Records show- smaller and the negative 


ing Gradual Disappearance of Positive larger, in each of the succeed- 
Element in Diphasic Mechanical . Tt. wheal 
Responses of Frog’s Nerve and INS responses. shou 


Plant-nerve also be noticed that che 

Note also the staircase increase. recovery from positive ts much 
quicker than trom negative response. This fact is important, 
in connection with certain psycho-physiological phenomena 
to be described in a later chapter. 

The effect of successive stimuli, in enhancing normal 
response, when the nerve is not yet in maximum. tonic 
condition is illustrated in a still more striking manner in the 
record given in fig. 321, obtained with a different specimen 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE 523 


of frog’s nerve. In this, also, the first series of responses 
was purely positive. But the record shown here begins at 
the point where, in consequence of previous tetanisation, 
response has become diphasic. Here it will be noticed that 
the true excitatory effect of contraction is undergoing a con- 
tinuous increase, while the abnormal positive is decreasing. 
The excitatory response, indeed, becomes so great as to be 
incapable of record within the plate. _ 

I have already shown how similar in every respec 
are the responsive characteristics of the vegetal nerve to 
those of the animal. This fact finds an interesting illus- 
tration in the various phases of its mechanical response. 
That is to say, plant nerve in a sub-tonic condition gives 
positive, passing into diphasic and normal negative response, 
under tetanisation. On arriving at this second stage of 
diphasic response, successive responses undergo enhance- 
ment in a manner precisely the same as holds good in the 
corresponding cases with frog’s nerve. This is sufficiently 
illustrated in the two records given side by side in 
fig. 321, the first of which, as already said, is of frog’s nerve, 
and the second, of nerve of fern. We see here again, as 
already in numerous cases before, how the responsive pecu- 
liarities and their modifications in the one are in every 
respect paralleled by those of the other. The only differ-— 
ence between them lies in the degree of their excitability, 
that is to say, two stimuli of equal intensity will in general 
induce a more intense excitatory effect in the nerve of frog 
than in that of fern; or in order to obtain from both an 
equal intensity of response, we must, in the case of fern, 
employ. a stronger stimulus. We have seen that in con- 
sequence of the absorption of stimulus, not only does the 
abnormal positive phase disappear, giving place to the 
normal negative, but the subsequent negative responses 
themselves also show an enhancement in a staircase manner. 
I give here (fig. 322) another record showing the mechanical 
response of frog’s nerve to undergo this staircase enhance- 
ment. From this effect then it is easy to understand that an 


524 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


intervening period of tetanisation will markedly enhance the 
negative response. 

We have now seen that, by the direct mode of investiga- 
tion afforded in mechanical response, we are able to trace 
out the causes which determine the three types of response 
found in nerves. It has been shown that all ‘these are 
brought about by the varying tonic condition of the tissue. 
From this it is easy to understand that the three types of | 
electromotive responses in nerve are also due to the same 
cause. In the experimental method 
there employed, the variations of con- 
ductivity appropriate to the tonic 
condition are superposed on parallel 
modifications of the excitability. Thus 
not only is the responsive change of 
a sub-tonic responding point positive, 
but the effect which is transmitted to it 
through sub-tonic conducting tissues is 
also positive; after tetanisation, how- 
ever, the tonic condition of the tissue is 
raised. The power of transmitting 
true excitation, previously in abeyance, 
is now not only restored, but gradually 

~ enhanced to a degree, depending within 

aes pices ces limits, on the amount of tetanisation. 

case Effect in Me- The excitability also undergoes a similar 
chanical Response of : 

Frog’s Nerve transformation, from the abnormal 

positive to normal negative, which latter 
again becomes enhanced to a degree that depends, within 
limits, on previous excitation. These effects, seen in electrical 
response to transmitted stimulation applied at a distance, 
I find repeated also in the mechanical response of nerve, 
under similar circumstances. That is to say, an isolated 
nerve, by the very fact of its being cut off from its normal 
sources of energy in the body of the intact animal, is apt 
to be rendered sub-tonic, and under these conditions no 
true excitation is transmitted, and it is only when the tonic 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE 525 


condition of the tissue has been raised, by the application 
of fresh energising stimulus, that the conducting power can 
be gradually restored. 2 

This leads me to what is theoretically a very interesting 
mode of determining the velocity of transmission in nerve, 
by the mechanical response of the nerve itself, which will be 
understood from the diagram already given (fig. 315). In 
that figure, A B C is the nerve, so clamped at B as to prevent 
any mechanical slip, but not tightly enough to obstruct the 
transmission of excitation. The nerve, when brought to a 
normal excitatory condition, is first excited at A a by a pair 
of electrodes in connection with an induction coil. The 
transmitted excitation, reaching B C, induces a contractile 
mechanical response there, observed by the highly. magnify- 
ing optic lever. Records of the transmitted effect of stimulus 
obtained in this manner. will be given later in the chapter. 
The interval of time, 7, between the application of stimulus 
and the initiation of response is accurately determined by 
the usual methods. Stimulus is next applied at B 4, and the 
interval of time 7’ between stimulus and response again 
determined. The difference (¢—7’) is the time required for 
the stimulus to travel the intervening distance a 4. By this 
means, I found the velocity of transmission in a certain 
specimen of nerve of fern to be 50 mm. per second. 

It is thus easy, by means of two successive experiments, 
to eliminate from the observation the element of the latent 
period. It is to be understood that the molecular change, 
ultimately to be expressed as contraction, begins to be 
initiated as soon as excitation reaches the responding area. 
As the contractile effect exhibited by the nerve is relatively 
small, we can see that a certain time will elapse before it 
becomes sufficient to be perceptible, unless the magnification 
employed is very high. With a magnification of the order 
of 100,000 times, however—which, as I shall show, is quite 
practicable—this loss of time is much lessened. 

It was while working out this investigation that I realised 
how indefinite must be any determination of the velocity of 


526 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


transmission in an isolated nerve. The conductivity, even 
in the intact organism, we have seen to be liable to modifica- 
tion from various factors such as fatigue, and it is easy to 
understand that it will become still more fluctuating when 
the conducting tissue is isolated. The inevitable changes 
consequent on separation from the natural sources: of energy 
at once begin totake place. As the result of this sub-tonicity, 
even a typically conducting tissue, like nerve, will cease to. 
be the conductor of true excitation, and there will then be, 
properly speaking, no physiological distinction between such 
a structure and a non-conducting tissue. By the absorption 
of stimulus, however, a transformation sets in, and the non- 
conducting becomes gradually reconverted, first, into a 
feebly, and then into a very highly conducting structure. 
The possible variations in conductivity, therefore, are not a 
matter of some few units per cent. quantitatively, but even 
considered qualitatively range from non-conductivity to the 
highest conductivity. And even, further, when the nerve 
has been once more rendered conducting, its velocity of 
transmission will vary greatly with the tonic condition 
conferred by previous stimulation. Over-stimulation, again, 
by inducing fatigue, diminishes the power of conduction of 
true excitation. This fact I shall be able to demonstrate by 
special experiments. 

That such changes are not peculiar to the isolated nerve, 
where the manifestation can be traced unmistakably to its 
true cause, is seen in those cases of living animals where, 
owing to mal-nutrition, or for other reasons, the tonic 
condition of the nerve falls below par, with growing non- 
conductivity and paralysis as the effect. And here it may 
be said that the transformation again from non-conducting 
or feebly-conducting to the normal state of conductivity 
may in general be brought about by the same means as are 
employed with the isolated nerve, namely, by the frequent 
repetition of tetanising electric shocks. 

The photographic method of recording the response of 
nerve, employed in the Kunchangraph, has the advantage 


~~ ee 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE 527 


that, as the record is made by the moving spot of light, the 
recording-point, as it were, encounters no friction, and the 
characteristic form of the response curve is thus unmodified. 
But prolonged work in the photographic dark-room is very 
fatiguing to the observer. I was, therefore, desirous of so 
perfecting the ordinary mode of record by the movement of 
the tracing-point of a lever over a smoked surface, that it 
would be adequate for most purposes. The difficulties 
involved in carrying this out lie, first, in the obtaining of a 
sufficiently high magnification, and, second, in the overcoming 
of friction at the writing point. A long lever, such as is 
necessary for high magnification, entails a heavy weight. 
But this can be obviated by employing a light and thin 
aluminium wire, 50 cm. in length. The fulcrum-rod, to 
which the lever-index is attached, has a diameter of 2 mm. 
A thread attached to the contracting nerve is wound once 
round this fulcrum-rod. The radius of the latter being 1 mm., 
the magnification produced by this arrangement is 500 
times. The magnification may in this manner be raised as 
high as 1,000, by taking a longer lever. For the tracing 
point the end of the lever is bent at right angles, and a fine 
bristle attached. Even this degree of magnification is not 
always necessary, as I have already said. The records which 
immediately follow have a magnification of only fifty times. 
The next difficulty, as already stated, lies in the friction 
to be overcome. The friction offered by a writing-surface of 
smoked paper is too great to be employed. A surface of 
plate-glass, coated with a thin and uniform layer of smoke, 
offers considerably less resistance. But even this retards the 
free movement of the tracing-point. I was therefore led to 
the construction of my Oscillating Recorder. The glass 
plate, on which the record is made, is carried on a primary 
frame, which is moved at a uniform rate, regulated by clock- 
work, on wheels, over rails. The plate is mounted on this 
primary frame in a secondary frame, which is held away from 
the primary, at a certain fixed distance, by means of spiral 
springs. This secondary frame, by means of an electro- 


528 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


magnetic arrangement, can be maintained in a state of to- 
and-fro oscillation, always strictly parallel to the primary. 
The recording-index moves in a vertical plane, and the 
smoked plate backwards and forwards, at right angles to 
this, the extent of its oscillation being about 1 mm. The 
recording point is adjusted, barely to touch the smoked » 
surface. Thus the oscillation of the plate brings it periodi- 
cally in contact with the tracing-point, which is thus practi- 
cally free to execute its movements unimpeded. When the 
oscillation frequency of the plate is sufficiently high, and the 
speed of the recording-surface low, the curve of record 


Fic. 323. Pho ographic Reproduction of Record of Mechanical Re- 
sponses of Frog’s Nerve (left-hand record) and Plant-nerve (right-hand 
record) obtained on Smoked Glass Surface of Oscillating Recorder 


appears as continuous. In other experiments, where the 
determination of time-relations is important, a high speed 
can be given to the plate by the regulation of the clockwork, 
and the record will then appear as a succession of dots. 
From these, and a knowledge of the oscillation-frequency 
of the plate, the time-relations of different parts of the curve 
can be determined with accuracy. I give here two different 
series of uniform mechanical responses recorded with this 
instrument, obtained from the nerves of frog and of fern 
respectively (fig. 323). 

I have also been able, by means of this instrument, to 
demonstrate a very important fact, namely, that the responses 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE 529 


of the afferent or sensory nerves are in every way the same 
as those of the efferent, or motor. The numerous records 
already given are of the latter. For the demonstration of 
the former I took the optical nerve of Ophzocephalus, and 
recorded its responses to uniform electrical stimuli, on a 
smoked surface. The following (fig. 324) is a photographic 
reproduction of the record. Owing to sub-tonicity, the first 
response is seen here to be abnormal positive. Successive 
stiniulation converts this, through diphasic, into normal 
negative, in a manner exactly the same as has already been 
observed in the sciatic nerve of frog. Another interesting 
record obtained with the optical 3 
nerve is given later (fig. 404). 

I also give in: the next figure 
(fig. 325) a series of effects of 
transmitted. stimulation, which 
show.in avery interesting manner 
the effect of fatigue in the modi- 
fication of the conductivity of a 
nerve. Itis customary to suppose 


Fic. 324. Record of Mechanical 


that the nerve is indefatigable. Responses to Electrical Stimu- 
But I shall be able to show that lus obtained on Smoked Glass, 

: gs ds and given by the Optic Nerve 
not only is the conductivity of a of Fish Ophiocephalus 


nerve liable to fatigue, but its Note the abnormal positive re- 
_excitability also. The demon- natch a ree ed 
stration of the latter will be given 

in a succeeding chapter. For the demonstration of the 
effect of fatigue on conductivity I selected a length of 
10 cm. from the sciatic nerve of gecko. This was attached 
for experiment to the Kunchangraph, in the manner 
diagrammatically represented in fig. 315. The length Bc, 
which showed contraction, in response to stimulus trans- 
mitted from A, measured 5 cm. The two exciting elec- 
trodes, A a, were 2 cm. apart. The intervening tract, 
through which excitation was transmitted, was, therefore, 
3 cm. At the beginning of the experiment, owing to the 
depression of tone which the nerve had undergone, from 

MM 


530 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


isolation, its conductivity was below par, and the responses 
obtained were positive. After a series of stimulations, 
however, the true excitatory wave was transmitted, with the 
concomitant negative or contractile responses. In order to 
demonstrate the effect of fatigue on conductivity, the 
recording of this series was commenced only after many 
normal responses had already been given. In the series 
recorded we can see that the responses at first exhibit 
periodic fatigue. The accentuation of fatigue is then mani- 
fested by a rapid decline in the amplitude of the responses. 
A remarkable change next begins to appear. It has been 


Fic. 325. Record, obtained on Smoked Glass, ot Transmitted Effect 
of Stimulation on Nerve of Gecko 


Note here the progressive effect of fatigue, seen first as periodic fatigue ; 
second as diphasic effect ; and third as reversal into abnormal positive. 


shown that the true excitatory negative response contains a 
masked positive element. Owing now to growing fatigue, the 
exhibition of the negative is delayed, and the positive thus 
shows itself as a preliminary down-curve in a diphasic response. 
Afterwards, the excitatory negative is completely abolished, 
and the positive response by expansion alone remains, as 
seen in the last of the series. Ultimately, when the nerve is 
killed, by excessive stimulation, even the positive response 
disappears. 

We may notice here the interesting fact that nerve, which 
is regarded as a conductor, par excellence, will sometimes 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF NERVE 531 


become a non-conductor. Conduction, therefore, is not 
alone dependent on anatomical structure, but requires also 
a certain molecular condition. A nerve, whose continuity 
remains uninterrupted, may nevertheless undergo paralysis 
and cease to conduct. Recovery may then, in many in- 
stances, be brought about by tetanisation. 

Thus, by means of mechanical response, obtained with 
a magnification of only fifty times, we have been able to 
demonstrate not only those results which may be observed 
by the most sensitive galvanometer, but also others which 
were never so detected. The magnification thus employed 
in the Kunchangraph, however, is here, as already stated, 
only in its lowest terms. When this is further exalted, 
still further and important phenomena regarding the exci- 


tatory changes in nerve are revealed, and some of these will 
be described in the next chapter. 


CHAPTER XXXVI 
MULTIPLE RESPONSE OF NERVE 


Great sensitiveness of the high magnification Kunchangraph—Individual con- 
tractile twitches shown in tetanisation of nerve—Sudden enhancement of 
mechanical response of nerve on cessation of tetanisation—Secondary excita- 
tion—Multiple mechanical excitation of nerve by single strong stimulation — 
Multiple mechanical excitation of nerve by drying. 


WE have already seen that, in order to detect the excitatory 
_ changes in nerve by the electrical method, the moderate 
sensitiveness of an ordinary galvanometer has to be exalted 
more than a million times. Galvanometric indications, more- 
over, are liable, as we have seen, to be complicated by the 
occurrence of differential effects at the two contacts. In 
- the Kunchangraphic method of record, however, there is no 
possibility of such complications, for the response curve here 
represents the direct effect of stimulus. We also saw that, 
according to this method, a very moderate magnification 
would give us all the variations that could be detected by 
the most sensitive galvanometer, and, besides this, owing to 
its simplicity, it makes it possible to observe other phenomena, 
whose occurrence the galvanometer could not satisfactorily 
have demonstrated. 

Such a magnification, however, as I have already said, is 
in its first stage only. With due precautions it is possible 
to obtain a Kunchangraphic magnification of a hundred 
thousand times. It will easily be seen that this places at 
our disposal an instrument of incomparable sensibility, by 
whose aid many of the phenomena of the nervous change, 
hitherto beyond our power of observation, may be brought 
within the sphere of investigation. 


a 


21 et dhe Ot TS 


i i, eee 


——— a ae ee ee ee 


MULTIPLE RESPONSE OF NERVE 533 


This magnification, of the order of a hundred thousand 
times, may be accomplished in either of two different 
ways. A magnified image of the end of the long lever 
may, in the first place, be thrown on a distant screen. 
By this compound magnification the sensitiveness of the 
record may be raised to the extent desired. Or, in the 
second place, we may employ a battery of two levers in 
series. The first of these gives a magnification, say, of 
five hundred times, and is connected with a second optical 
lever, by which a multiplying magnification of two hundred 
times is easily obtained. It is unnecessary to point out that 
special care should in this second case be taken to ensure 
the steadiness of the support. With due precautions it is, 
however, not difficult to secure the entire elimination of all 
disturbing elements. 

When the spot of light from the second lever is thus 
thrown on a distant screen, it is very interesting to watch the 
various changes induced in the nerve by the environmental 
conditions. An isolated nerve in a moist chamber, cut off 
from its natural sources of energy, becomes increasingly 
sub-tonic. This process is attended by an abnormal relaxa- 
tion, which causes a steady movement of the spot of light 
in one direction. When the nerve has become very sub- 
tonic, the effect of stimulus, as that of electric shocks, is to 
enhance the tonic condition, and by this the downward 
drift of the spot of light is retarded or arrested. In 
cases of extreme sub-tonicity there is no further response, 
beyond this arrest. But where the sub-tonicity is less 
pronounced, stimulus will induce the abnormal positive 
response by a sudden positive variation of the drift, which 
is followed by recovery in the opposite direction. The 
after-effect of absorption of stimulus is further effective in 
causing the gradual retardation and final arrest of the 
downward drift. By such absorption of stimulus the tonic 


' The abnormal positive response is also obtained from ‘nerve in ordinary 
tonic condition, it should be remembered, by the application of excessively 
feeble stimulus. 


534 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


condition is raised, and the normal excitability consequently 
enhanced. From this point onwards the responses are con- 
tractile or normal negative. At this stage the response of 
the nerve exhibits the staircase increase, the nerve itself 
showing a certain amount of tonic contraction. The 
excitability of the nerve then attains a maximum, and the 
successive responses become uniform. Long and intense 
stimulation will, after this, bring on fatigue. This stage is 
characterised, again, by a growing relaxation of the nerve as 
a whole, and its responses may become, first, diminished in 
amplitude, second, of a diphasic character, and, thirdly, 
reversed to the abnormal positive, according to the amount 
of fatigue which supervenes. We may thus, for the sake of 
convenience, distinguish four stages in the response of 
nerve. The first of these is the initial phase, SUB-TONIC 
RELAXATION, and the characteristic response to individual 
stimuli is here abnormal positive. The second phase 
is that of the TRANSITION to normal response. The 
characteristic responses to individual stimuli here show a 
staircase increase, with more or less permanent contraction 
as its after-effect. If at this stage the nerve is allowed to 
remain long without stimulation, it slowly reverts to the 
first stage of sub-tonic relaxation, with its growing relaxa- 
tion and abnormal positive response. Stimulation, how- 
ever, brings it back once more to the second or transition 
stage. In the third stage of UNIFORM responsiveness, the 
responses are normal and take place by equal contraction. 
In the fourth, or FATIGUE stage, there is a tendency, as already 
said, to relaxation on the part of the nerve as a_ whole, 
and it thus outwardly mimics the stage of sub-tonicity. 
The responses now, therefore, diminish in amplitude, and 
show the diphasic or the abnormal positive character. 
Further characteristics of these four stages, and_ their 
relations to each other, will be treated in detail in 
Chapter XLI. 

A few words may be said about the mechanical response of 
the nerve, when it is in a favourable condition of excitability. 


MULTIPLE RESPONSE OF NERVE 535 


We have seen that in order to obtain a galvanometric record 
of the electrical response of nerve, one or even a few shocks 
will not be sufficient to induce the necessary electromotive 
change. For this, tetanic shocks of a certain duration are 
necessary, and the responsive electromotive change is not 
immediately perceptible. In consequence of this intensity of 
stimulation, moreover, complete electrical recovery can only 
take place after a perceptible interval. With the low magnifi- 
cation Kunchangraph, too, tetanic shocks of something like 
a second in duration are necessary, and complete recovery 
here also requires a period of about one minute. But with 
the greater sensitiveness available in the highly magni- 
fying apparatus, response with a highly excitable specimen 
of nerve is obtained with even so short-lived a stimu- 
lation as that of two or three vibrations of the vibrating 
interrupter of the secondary coil, lasting less than one-tenth 
of asecond. The responsive contraction of this short-lived 
stimulus, and its recovery, are also quick. It is in conse- 
quence of the rapidity of this response and recovery that the 
responsive contractions due to the rapidly intermittent 
tetanising shocks do not become fused, but show themselves 
in the response-curve as consisting of successive twitches, 
corresponding to the component shocks. Owing to the high 
amplitude of these responses, and the trend of the base-line 
either up or down, it is difficult in practice to obtain photo- 
graphic records of these effects. But it is easy enough to obtain 
definite visual demonstration of various characteristic effects 
in the response by the employment of the following device. 
The spot of light from the optic lever is made incident on 
a revolving mirror, and reflected from it to a large white 
screen at some distance. During a period of repose the 
quiescent spot traces a more or less horizontal line of light. 
This may trend either in a downward or an upward direction 
continuously, according as there is induced a continuous 
sub-tonic relaxation or a growing contraction, due to the 
after-effect of stimulus. Somewhere between these two 
extremes may be obtained a condition of more or less 


536 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


stability, where the record made by the spot of light appears 
as a horizontal line. Under normal conditions, then, the 
response to excitation is a sudden movement, due to con- 
traction, say upwards, followed by recovery down. In 
the response-curve projected on the screen, the vertical 
movement or ordinate represents the amplitude of the re- 
sponse, and the horizontal abscissa the time. Under tetani- 
sation a series of curves corresponding in frequency to the | 
frequency of the shocks is observed as serrations. 

Another very interesting observation often made in the 
mechanical response of nerve is that of the after-effect on 
the cessation of continuous stimulation ‘by tetanic shocks. 
It has been found, it will be remembered (p. 428), that the 
response of the retina to the action of continuous light often 
shows on its cessation a sudden transient increase. This 
phenomenon has been regarded as peculiar tothe retina. But 
I have found exactly parallel effects to occurin the mechanical 
response of nerve. Under continuous stimulation there is a 
tendency to the attainment of a maximum contraction, which 
suddenly, on the cessation of stimulation, overshoots, to be 
followed by the usual recovery. I have already referred to 
the two different effects of an opposite character caused by 
incident stimulus, namely, the effect of negativity, and its 
converse positivity. In the case of mechanical response, it is 
the former which is effective in inducing contraction, while 
the latter is associated with expansion, and is a factor in re- 
covery. It will also be seen, in a general way, that by the - 
antagonistic action of these two elements, and by the differ- 
ing relative intensities of their after-effects, many diverse 
results may be exhibited. In the case of the after-effect in 
question, which occurs by a sudden positive variation of the 
contraction, the excitatory effect would appear to be pre- 
dominant. Even when, after this brief positive variation, 
recovery is taking place, the excitatory element, with its 
contractile tendency, appears to persist; for if a second 
stimulus be applied, some time before the recovery is com- 
plete, the consequent contractile response takes place almost 


MULTIPLE RESPONSE OF NERVE 537 


instantaneously. But when the recovery is once complete, 
a similar stimulation will not induce a similar immediate 
response. Instead of this there will be a brief period of 
hesitation or latency before its initiation. 

Another interesting phenomenon, which I was first able 
to observe by the help of the highly magnifying Kunchan- 
graph, was the occurrence of multiple response in nerve 
under intense stimulation. I was led to this discovery by 
an investigation which I had undertaken for the demon- 
stration of the identity of response in animal and vegetable 
nerves. After showing the extended parallelism which 
exists between the two, under similar conditions and varia- 
tions of conditions, as already described, I was desirous of 
seeing whether a plant nerve could be substituted in certain 
experiments for the animal nerve. In accordance with this 
I used the vegetal nerve in the experiment known as 
secondary contraction. Here a nerve-and-muscle preparation 
of frog is taken, and a second piece of frog’s nerve is suitably 
laid, with one end lying upon the end of the other nerve. 
On excitation of this second detached nerve, say by electric 
shocks, excitatory electrical variation is found to cause 
responsive contraction in the muscle of the first preparation. 
In my own rendering of this experiment I employed, instead 
of the second piece of: frog’s nerve, a length of nerve of 
fern. In order that the experiment should not be open to 
any objection arising from the escape of stimulating current, 
I employed a non-electrical form of stimulus. This was 
done by touching the plant nerve with a strongly heated wire. 
The terminal muscle would then, under favourable conditions, 
begin to respond by strong spasmodic contraction. When 
this had subsided, a new series of tetanic contractions 
began ; and this was repeated at short intervals, for nearly 
half an hour. When this series of spasms had come to 
a stop, I have often succeeded by a fresh application of the 
hot wire to the vegetal nerve in obtaining a second series 
of such repeated responses. It thus appeared that the 
strongly excited plant nerve gave rise to a series of multiple 


538 |. COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


excitations, the indications of which were afforded by the 
nerve-and-muscle preparation. 

The only perplexing feature of these responses was the 
abnormally long period of ten to fifteen seconds which was 
generally found to elapse between the application of the 
strong stimulus to the plant nerve, and the response subse- 
quently given by the terminal muscle. Here it must be 
remembered that the excitation applied at one end of the 
plant nerve has to travel the entire length before its excita- 
tory electrical variation can be communicated to the nerve 
of the frog-preparation. The transmitted excitatory varia- 
tion in the primary has, moreover, to reach a certain intensity 
before it can effectively excite the secondary preparation. We 
know, further, that an isolated piece of nerve is liable to fall 
into a sub-tonic or depressed condition, in which its conducting 
power is much lowered, to be gradually restored again under 
strong or long-continued stimulation. These considerations 
will probably be found to account for the delay in the occur- 
rence of the first of these responses. It would thus appear from 
the last experiment that a nerve, when subjected to a single 
strong stimulus, will give a multiple series of responses. In 
order to test this by direct experiment I employed the highly 
magnifying Kunchangraph, and subjected an experimental 
nerve of frog to a single strong thermal stimulation. This 
gave rise, at first, either to an abnormal positive response or 
to a moderate negative. But there followed, after a longer or 
shorter pause, a series of multiple contractile responses, which 
generally grew in intensity for a considerable time. There 
were in the series a number of short pauses, each followed by 
a veritable storm of excitation, in which individual responses 
were so rapid that the up or down movement of the spot of 
light appeared as brief flashes, in which all distinctness was 
obliterated. This experiment conclusively shows that the 
nerve, like certain other tissues, is susceptible of multiple 
excitation. 

If the nerve in a nerve-and-muscle preparation be 
allowed to dry, the muscle is seen to be thrown into a series 


MULTIPLE RESPONSE OF NERVE 


of spasmodic contractions. 


caused by multiple excitations induced in the nerve. 


539 


This also we may regard as 


And 


the correctness of this supposition I have been able to 


verify by experiment. As 
the individual responses in 
these multiple series were of 
fairly large amplitude, I ex- 
pected to be able to obtain 
a record of such a series 
by an ordinary magnification 
on smoked glass. In order 
to* obtain this record under 
normal conditions a stream’ 
of air, bubbling through 
water, was passed through 
the chamber at a uniform 
rate. Owing to the run- 
down of the latent energy 
in the nerve, we are able 
to observe a_ consequent 
growing relaxation. By the 
manipulation of a stop-cock 
the air is passed through 
a calcium chloride tube, in- 
stead of a vessel containing 
water. In this way the nerve 
is quickly subjected to dry 
air instead of moist vapour. 
This substitution is repre- 
sented in the record by an 
upward arrow 7, and it will 
be noticed how at this point 


Initiation of Multiple Re- 
sponse by Drying of Nerve 


FIG. 326. 


The nerve, owing to growing sub- 
tonicity, was showing a growing 
relaxation, as seen in the first part 
of the record. Air passed through 
CaCl, tube, and, thus dried, was 
now passed through nerve-chamber 
at point marked with upward arrow 
*. This gave rise to a large con- 
tractile response, followed by sub- 
sequent multiple responses. Original 
record on smoked glass here reduced 
photographically to 4. 


the relaxation is suddenly converted into excitatory con- 


traction (fig. 326). 


Under this process of drying, this single 


contractile movement is followed by a long-continued series 
of multiple responses, here seen to fall into a somewhat 


irregular periodicity. 


CHAPTER XXXVII 
RESPONSE BY VARIATION OF ELECTRICAL RESISTIVITY 


Variation of resistance in Dionea, by ‘ modification’—Excitatory change, its 
various independent expressions—Characteristic difficulties of investigation— 
Morographic record by variation of resistivity—Inversion of curves at death- 
point—Similarities between mechanical, electro-motive and resistivity curves 
of death—The true excitatory effect attended by diminution of resistance 
— Response of plant nerve by resistivity variation—Independence of resistivity 
and mechanical variations—Responsive resistivity variation in frog’s nerve, 
and its modification under aneesthetics. 


IT was noticed by Burdon Sanderson that leaves of Dionga 


after ‘modification’ exhibited a diminished electrical resist- 
ance; and this ‘modification’ he found to be most easily 
induced after the passage of an electrical current through 
the tissue. Subsequent observers have also noticed a diminu- 
tion of resistance in many cases when a tissue has been 
subjected to electric shocks. These diminutions of resistance 
are observed as more or less permanent after-effects. The 
experiments in these cases depend on obtaining the galvano- 
meter deflections caused by a small E.M.F., before and after 
the modification. The larger deflection due to the same 
E.M.F. after modification shows that the resistance of the 
tissue has undergone a diminution. 

This method, however, is open to several objections. 
The passage of constant or induction currents through the 
tissue would not only give rise to polarisation effects, but 
would also induce an unknown electromotive variation at 
the two contacts on the surfaces of the tissue, to an extent 
depending on their differential excitability. The observed 
deflection by a small testing E.M.F. is thus affected, not 


Oo ty ihe fan. BP 


RESPONSE BY VARIATION OF ELECTRICAL RESISTIVITY 541 


only by .the variation of resistance, but also by varying 
polarisation and. excitatory electromotive effects. 

The question still remains, What is the nature and 
significance of this induced variation of resistance? As the 
effects which have been referred to are generally seen to be 
induced after excitation, and to constitute its after-effect, 
does it follow that the diminution of resistance takes place 
as a remote consequence of other excitatory changes? 
Or is it but a different manifestation of that fundamental 
molecular change, induced by excitation, of which the 


electromotive variation and change of form are other and 


independent expressions? 

I showed in the first chapter of this book that one 
identical molecular change may be detected in different 
ways, according to the method of observation. Thus the 
same excitatory change is shown both in change of form 


-and in electromotive variation. That either manifestation 


takes place in entire independence of the other is shown, 
for instance, when the mechanical response of A/zmosa or 
Desmodium is restrained, under which condition the electro- 
motive response proceeds as before. Excitatory changes, 
similarly, may express themselves independently either by 
electromotive variations or by changes of electric resistance. 
It was, in fact, by means of the latter method, that of the 
variation of resistance, that I first demonstrated the responsive 
molecular changes which take place in inorganic matter.! 

If living tissues, therefore, really respond to excitation 
in a manner similar to the inorganic, it should be possible 
to obtain from them response-records by a new method, 
that of Resistivity Variation alone. In order to demonstrate 
this inference, it will be necessary to show that such varia- 


tion of resistance takes place immediately on excitation, 


and not as an after-effect. We ‘must, however, ascertain 
whether this method of Resistivity Variation does or does 


' Bose, De la Généralité des Phénomines Moléculaires produits par ? Elec- 
tricité sur la matidre Inorganique et sur la Maticre Vivante. (Travaux du 
Congrés International de Physique. Paris, 1900.) 


542 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


not give us those two opposed effects, positive and negative, 
which we have already seen to be exhibited by living tissues 
in other forms of response whether mechanical or electro- 
motive. Of these, again, supposing them to occur, it will 
also be necessary to determine whether it is the increase or 
decrease of resistance which corresponds to the negative and 
positive mechanical and electromotive responses respectively ; 


and finally it must be determined what are the effects of the. 


various physiological modifications, induced by different 
agencies, on the response by resistivity variation. 

In this investigation many serious experimental difficulties 
have first to be overcome. These will be dealt with in series 
in the detailed description of the method to be employed. 
It will be well, however, to see in what important respects 
the conditions for the obtaining of response here are unlike 
those of the electromotive variation. In the latter case, it 
we employ an isotropic tissue, diffuse stimulation will induce 
similar excitatory electromotive variations in every part of 
the tissue. The differential electromotive variation, there- 
fore, on which the recording of response depends, will, under 
_ these circumstances, be impossible. In this case, therefore, 
it is necessary to injure or kill the tissue at one of the two 
contacts. Such artificial induction of anisotropy would not, 
however, be necessary under an experimental method which 
was not dependent on any differential action. Thus an 
isotropic tissue would give response by longitudinal con- 
traction when the stimulus was diffuse. Similarly, though 
an isotropic tissue fails to give an electromotive response 
under diffuse stimulation, yet we may expect it to exhibit 
response by variation of resistance. The recording of 
excitatory response by resistivity variation has thus one 
advantage over that of the electromotive variation, inas- 
much as the record is not affected by complications due to 
differential action, but is the expression of the direct effect 
of excitation. The question which we have next to deter- 
mine, then, is whether or not the excitatory variation of the 
living tissue is attended by any variation of its resistance, 


i. ae os fe 


RESPONSE BY VARIATION OF ELECTRICAL RESISTIVITY 543 


and whether, if so, such variation is or is not of two opposite 
signs, according to the tonic condition of the tissue con- 
cerned. In subjecting this question to experimental investi- 
gation, it is well to employ a non-electrical form of stimula- 
tion, in order to avoid any possible disturbance of the 
galvanometer record from polarisation or current escape. 

The first point to be decided is the character, positive or 
negative, of that resistivity variation by which the true 
excitatory change finds expression. We have already seen 
that when a tissue is subjected to a gradually rising tem- 
perature it exhibits response, which is expressed mechani- 
cally as increasing expansion, and electrically as increasing 
positivity. When the temperature, however, has reached 
the definite critical point. of death, we have seen that 
there is a sudden excitatory effect induced, attended by 
a reversal of the sign ‘of response. This is expressed 
mechanically by a sudden contraction, and electrically by a 
change to galvanometric negativity. I have already ex- 
plained in Chapter XVI. that, in mechanical and electrical 
morographic curves, the abrupt point of inversion represents 
the death-point. I have also shown that this death-response 
is a true physiological response; that the temperature at 
which it takes place is definite in all phanerogamous plants, 
being at, or very near, 60° C. in normal specimens; and that 
it displays depression, by transposition te a lower tempera- 
ture, when the tissue is physiologically depressed by such 
influences as fatigue.' 

From these facts we might expect, if a tissue sfioied 
response by variation of resistivity, that up to 60° C., or so, 
there would be a continuous one-directioned change of 
resistance, succeeded on reaching 60° C. by an abrupt 
reversal to the opposite-directioned change. In that case, it 
would be the second of the two, which would be indicative 
of true excitation. To carry out this experiment I took a 
radial and physiologically isotropic pistil of, Wzb¢scus, and 
mounted it on two non-polarisable electrodes. The specimen 


1 Bose, Plant Kesponse, p. 177. 


544 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


was now made the fourth arm of a Wheatstone’s bridge 
(fig. 327), by which electrical resistance is usually determined. 
The plant specimens employed generally possessed high 
resistance, of the order of several hundreds of thousands of 
ohms. In the Wheatstone’s arrangement employed by me, 
P and Q represented the ratio-arms; R a standard. megohm, 


lic. 327. Diagrammatic Representation of Experimental Arrange- 
ment for Recording Response by Resistivity Variation 


P Q, ratio arms of Wheatstone’s bridge; R, standard 1 or *5 megohm; 
S, specimen. 


or half-megohm; and s the specimen whose variations of 
resistance were to be determined. It is now evident that 


Cpa? 
when the bridge is balanced, S = ~ R. 
P 


The ratio-arms, P and Q, consist of resistance-boxes, 
which allowed a variation of from I to 10,000 ohms. In 
order to obtain balance, of course, the ratio of the two had to 
be suitably adjusted. A highly sensitive galvanometer was 
used, and the electromotive force employed to obtain balance 
was only ‘o5 volt. This low E.M.F. was obtained by the 
use of a suitable potentiometer slide. It will be seen that, 
owing to the very low E.M.F. and the high resistance in 
the circuit, the current flowing through the specimen was 
rendered extremely feeble. This was done in order to avoid 


RESPONSE BY VARIATION OF ELECTRICAL RESISTIVITY 545 


any complication such as might result from the passage of a 
strong current. 7 4 

In order to subject the specimen to a gradual and 
continuous rise of temperature, it was placed in the thermal 
chamber, which has already been described (fig. 131). Before 
the gradual raising of the temperature is initiated, an exact 
balance is first obtained, the galvanometer spot of light being 
thus adjusted to zero. This position can be maintained for an 
indefinite length of time, provided the specimen is subjected: 
to no variation of temperature. We have already seen that no, 
resultant electro-motive variation is induced, in consequence 
of stimulus, in a physiologically isotropic tissue. Any 
change now recorded under a gradual rise of temperature, 
by the movement of the galvanometer spot of light, must, 
therefore, be due toa resulting variation of resistance. The 
movement of the galvanometer spot of light is recorded in 
the usual manner, on a photographic plate, a down-record 
representing an increase of resistance, and an up-record a 
diminution. In order that the curve should also give indica- 
tions of different temperatures, light is cut off for a short 
time at every 2° C. of rise of temperature. Thus each of the 
successive gaps in the record indicates a temperature-ascent 
of 2° C. 

Taking now the specimen of pistil of Wzdzscus, balanced as 
described, it was seen, on beginning gradually to raise the 
temperature, that the balance was upset, while the growing 
deflection of the galvanometer spot indicated an incréasing 
resistance. The method of experiment, which has been 
described, proved now so delicate that it was impossible to 
record the entire curve within the range of the photographic 
plate. It was, therefore, necessary to choose for record only 
that part of the deflection which included the interesting and 
significant point of inversion. The photographic record 
thus commences at 56° C., it being understood that there 
has been, before this, a larger and continuously growing 
deflection downwards, indicative of increasing resistance. 
During record the deflection continues to increase, till the 


NWN 


546 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


critical point is reached. And here, though the temperature 
still goes on ascending at the same rate as before, we see a 
sudden reversal in the characteristic curve of resistivity, 
showing that the hitherto increasing has suddenly become a 
decreasing resistance. This abrupt inversion represents the 


Fic. 328. FIG. 329. FIG. 330. 

Fic. 328. Photographic Record of the Morographic Curve taken by 
Method of Resistivity Variation in Pistil of Azdéscus. Critical point 
of inversion at 60°8° C, 

Fic. 329. Photographic Record of the Morographic Curve taken by 
Method of Electro-motive Variation in Petiole of Musa. Critical point 
of inversion at 59°6° C. 

Fic. 330. Photographic Record of the Morographic Curve taken by 
Method of Mechanical Response in Filament of 7ass¢flora. Critical 


point of inversion at 59°6° C 


excitatory effect which occurs at the point of initiation of 
death, and is in the present case at 60°8° C. (fig. 328). 

It is astonishing to find that the morographic curves 
obtained from different specimens, by three methods so 
different as the mechanical, the electro-motive, and that of 


RESPONSE BY VARIATION OF ELECTRICAL RESISTIVITY 547 


resistivity, should bear so strong a resemblance to each other, 
as is here seen to be the case, in the three records given side 
by side (figs. 328, 329, 330). The excitatory effect may thus 
be manifested by contraction, galvanometric negativity, or 
diminution of resistance. We have already seen that the 
electromotive is not a consequence of the mechanical re- 
sponse, but is exhibited independently, when physical move- 
ment is restrained. The response by resistivity variation 
likewise, is, as we shall see, an independent expression of the 
fundamental molecular change due to excitation. 

Having thus established the fact that true excitatory 
response is exhibited by diminution of resistance, we‘ have 
next to ascertain whether this method of resistivity-variation 
is capable of being employed in the study of excitatory 
phenomena in general, with as great facility as those 
mechanical and electro-motive methods with which we are 
already familiar. In order to determine this question I 
employed the same Wheatstone’s bridge arrangement as 
before. As it was important, for reasons previously given, 
to use a non-electrical form of stimulus, I employed those 
thermal shocks which we have already found to be so reli- 
able. The thermal loop of platinum wire enclosed the 
specimen as before, without being in contact with it. A 
short-lived passage of heating-current, controlled by a metro- 
nome, would now give rise to that sudden thermal variation 
which we have seen to be effective in causing stimulation 
It should be remembered that the rise of temperature, as 
such, induces a responsive increase of resistance. But as, on 
the other hand, the sudden thermal variation acts as a 
stimulus, it should induce the excitatory response, by a 
transient diminution of resistance. In the following experi- 
ments, I employed the physiologically isotropic nerve of fern. 
The resistance of this tissue, when balanced, was found to be 
400,000 ohms. It should be stated here that this specimen 
was in a very good tonic condition, and might be expected 
therefore to give normal response. It was now subjected to 
a series of stimuli of uniform intensity, at intervals of five 

NN2 


548 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


minutes. The consequent responses are seen to be uniform, 
and to take place by that diminution of resistance which we 
already know to constitute the normal mode of response 
(fig. 331). In order to obtain some idea of the magnitude 
of these resistance variations the balance was upset at the 
end of the response record, to the extent of 4,000 ohms. 
The deflection seen to the right of the figure represents the 
effect of this variation of resistance. The normal resistance 
of the tissue, including that of the non-polarisable electrodes 
was, as stated before, 400,000 ohms. The resistance of the 
electrodes themselves was 50,000 ohms. That of the tissue 


FiG. 331. Response Records by Resistivity Variation, in the Nerve 
of Fern ; Stimuli at Intervals of Five Minutes 


Kesponse to stimulation is by the negative variation or diminution of 
resistance. The record to the right shows deflection due;to variation 
of resistance of 4,000 ohms. 


alone was thus 350,000 ohms. The variation of resistance 
induced by stimulus, therefore, was, in the present case, 
approximately I per cent. 

In order. next to determine whether the resistance 
variation was a consequence of the responsive change of 
form, or an independent expression of the fundamental 
molecular change, I clamped a nerve of fern suitably, at 
its two ends, to prevent any possible change of length, the 
two non-polarisable electrodes being led off in such a way 
as to include a certain length of the specimen. On 
carrying out the experiment in this manner I obtained 
response by diminution of resistance, exactly as in the last 


RESPONSE BY VARIATION OF ELECTRICAL RESISTIVITY 549 


case. It is thus seen that the response, by resistivity 
variation, is an independent expression of the excitatory 
variation. . a 

In taking records of the responses of animal and vege- 
table nerves, by the methods of mechanical and electro- 
motive variations, we saw that, while the normal response 
was negative, this was liable to become reversed to positive, 
under two different conditions—namely, sub-tonicity and 
the fatigue due to excessive stimulation. Similar reversals 
are observed under similar conditions, when the method 
of resistivity variation is employed. We saw, further, that 
the abnormal positive response, due to sub-tonicity, could be 
gradually converted into normal negative, through inter- 
mediate diphasic, by tetanisation—further tetanisation acting, 
moreover, to exalt this feeble into enhanced negative 
response. Parallel results are observed in the case of 
resistivity variation. The initial abnormal response, by 
increase of resistance, is found, after short tetanisation, to 
be converted into diphasic—an increase of resistance or 
positive response preceding the true excitatory or negative 
effect of diminution of resistance. Further tetanisation 
brought about the disappearance of this preliminary positive, 
and the enhancement of the negative, response. — 

I used the same method, finally, for the observation of 
response and its modifications, by means of anesthetics in 
animal nerve. For this purpose I took a nerve of frog, and 
subjected it to chloroform. It will be remembered that in 
studying the effect of anzsthetics by the electro-motive 
variation method, we found it, first, to reverse the normal 
response to positive, and finally to’ induce an abolition, 
which might prove to be either temporary or permanent. 
The same thing is seen under the resistivity variation 
method. In the first series of records, given in fig. 332, we 
find normal responses by diminution of resistance, to a 
series of stimuli applied at intervals of two minutes. 
After the application of chloroform, the normal responses 
are seen to have disappeared. Stimulus now evokes 


550 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


either no response or an occasional flutter, in the positive 
direction. 

We have thus seen, in the course of the present chapter, 
that, in addition to the mechanical and electro-motive modes 
of response, there is also a third mode available—namely, 
that by Resistivity Variation. We have also seen that the 
results obtained under these three methods are identical. 
It has been shown that the normal excitatory effect is in 


AW WW Vi 


FIG. 332. Effect of Chloroform seen in Modification of Resistivity 
Variation in Frog’s Nerve 


The normal responses seen to the left by diminution or negative variation 
of resistance were evoked by stimuli at intervals of one minute. 
Those to the right exhibit the effect of chloroform. The normal 
response is thus abolished, and we have either no response or only an 
occasional flutter in the positive direction. 


all three cases negative, consisting of mechanical contraction, 
galvanometric negativity, or diminution of resistance, as the 
case may be. In recording the morographic curve by these 
three methods, we find that up to the critical point of death, 
at or near 60° C., we obtain expansion, galvanometric posi- 
tivity, and increasing resistance. At that point, however, 
there is a sudden reversal of the curve, indicating conversion 
to negative, contraction, galvanometric negativity, and de- 
crease of resistance. 


CHAPTER XXXVIIi 
FUNCTIONS OF VEGETAL NERVE 


Feeble conducting power of cortical tissues—Heliotropic and geotropic eflects 
dependent on response of cortical tissues only—Phenomenon of correlation 
—FExcitability of tissue maintained in normal condition only under action of 
stimulus—Physiological activities of growth, ascent of sap, and motile 
sensibility, maintained by action of stimulus—Critical importance of energy of 
light—Leaf-venation a catchment-basin—Transmission of energy to remotest 
parts of plants—Plant thus a connected and organised entity. 


IN the animal body, different kinds of tissues are possessed 
of different degrees of conductivity, the nerve being specialised 
for the rapid transmission of stimulus. And it is now seen 
that in the plant also we have a similar state of things, 
cortical tissue, for example, though excitable, having feeble 
conductivity, whereas the vegetal nerve possesses this power 
in high degree. The question next arises: What is the 
function subserved in the economy of the plant by a tissue 
so highly specialised for the rapid conduction of stimulus ? 
The various growth curvatures, by means of which plants 
place themselves under the directive action of light and 
gravity, are of advantage to the organism. But in bringing 
about these movements, the plant-nerve takes little or no 
part. And this is the case, even when the responsive 
curvature takes place at a certain distance from the point of 
stimulus. Here the transmission takes place slowly, through 
the feebly-conducting cortical tissues. For example, in 
Avena, curvature in consequence of such transmitted effect is 
observed, even when the fibro-vascular bundles have been cut 
across. , 
If, indeed, the highly conducting nervous elements ha 
been concerned, these curvatures in response to unilateral 


552 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


stimulus could not well have taken place. This will be clear 
if we consider the case of a radial organ, such as the stem, 
unilaterally acted on by light. Here a positive heliotropic 
movement is induced, by which the growing organ is placed 
in the most favourable position as regards illumination. The 
peculiarity of this phenomenon lies in the responsive 
contraction of the side acted on by stimulus, with consequent 
concavity and curvature towards light. This heliotropic | 
movement continues until the organ has placed itself in the 
direction of incident radiation. When this orientation has 
become perfect there is no further movement, because the 
proximal and distal sides are now equally stimulated. Had 
the cortical tissue, on whose differential responsive action the 
curvature depended, been as highly conducting as the 
vegetable nerve, this particular-directioned movement would 
have been an impossibility, for the stimulus, instead of 
remaining localised on one side, would in that case have 
become diffused, with the result of inducing antagonistic 
effects on the proximal and distal sides, under which there 
could have been no resultant curvature. Indeed this neutral- 
ising action of conduction, in nullifying responsive curvature, 
is seen even when unilateral stimulus is excessively strong. 
For under these circumstances stimulus is conducted 
transversely, through the imperfectly conducting tissue, with 
the result of undoing the previous curvature. And it is 
obvious that had the conductivity of the tissue been higher, 
this neutralisation would have taken place, even under feeble 
stimulation. 

I have also shown elsewhere that, in the responsive 
movements of leaves, conduction through nervous elements 
plays little or no part. For the blade of the leaf may be 
acted on by light without showing any responsive movement. 
Hence the lamina is not to be regarded as the perceptive 
organ. The organ by which, on the contrary, the responsive 
movements of the leaves are determined is the pulvinus or 
pulvinoid. This is at once perceptive and motile. When 
such an organ, then, is acted on directly and locally by light, 


FUNCTIONS’ OF VEGETAL NERVE 553 


a responsive movement is\induced. These are facts which 
can be demonstrated by shielding the lamina and pulvinus 
alternately from the action of light. When the lamina alone 
is exposed there is no action; but when it is the pulvinus 
to which light is admitted, there is an immediate responsive 
movement. 

The lamina, it is true, is provided with a fine fibro-vascular 
network containing the nervous strands. But the stimuli 
received by this extensive system are ultimately conducted 
along the thicker channels of which it forms the terminal 
ramification, and serve to stimulate the plant as a whole. 
When such stimuli reach the petiole, then, they cannot act in 
that direct and unilateral manner in the motile organ which 
is required for the responsive movement of the leaf. The 
petiole, it is true, from its dorsiventral character, is unequally 
excitable on its two sides. But since,in the process of the 
transference of stimulus from the nervous to the ordinary 
elements there is a great loss, and since, moreover, the motile 
tissues in the case of most petioles are very sluggish, the 
diffusely transmitted stimulus induces practically no directive 
effect. Nor could there, in any case, have been any com- 
parison between the effective strength of an external 
stimulus acting directly and unilaterally, and a transmitted 
stimulus acting diffusely. 

It will thus be seen that conduction through specialised 
nervous elements is by no means the essential factor in 
bringing about those numerous directive curvatures which 
subserve so many important functions in the life of the plant. 
The question, therefore, as to what is the part in the economy 
of the organism played by the vegetable nerve still remains 
to be answered. 

One very obscure problem in connection with Vegetable 
Physiology is that of Correlation. Thus various complex 
activities may be set up in one part of the plant, when 
another part, more or less distant from it, is subjected to the 
variation of some excitatory influence. In this way every 
part of the plant-organism would appear to be ez rapport with 


554 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


the rest, and this intimate connection between outlying areas 
becomes comprehensible, when we are made aware of the 
easy communication afforded by the existence of specialised 
conducting elements. 

I shall now proceed to deal with. the importance ot 
stimulus, and its conduction to the interior of the plant, as 
the essential factor in sub- 
serving the various life-. 
activities of the organism. 
That the reception of stimu- 
lus is important, in main- 
taining the excitability of a 
plant, is easily seen in the 
case of Mzmosa, when de- 
prived of light, for example. 
Under these conditions, its 
motile excitability is found 
to disappear. And this is 
only restored on re-exposure 
to light. We have seen 
again, in the course of the 
last chapter, that the isolated 
vegetable nerve, deprived, as 
it is, of normal favourable 
RiGcsrin Pilostanhins Recmdcc! conditions, becomes sub-tonic 


Effect of Tetanisation in En- or moribund, and then its 
hancing Mechanical Response of 


Plant-nerve ordinary responsive power is 

The first series of responses heightened abolished or even reversed. 
to second series, after intermediate Under these ‘clvcumatantes 
tetanisation. 


the normal excitability is 
found to be restored by the continued action of stimulus. 
Abnormal positive response is thus found to be converted 
into normal negative. Again, after an intervening period of 
stimulation, response of ordinary amplitude is found, as 
in fig. 333, to become enhanced. It is thus seen that a tissue, 
when cut off from the supply of stimulus, loses its normal 
excitability, and that a@ more or less continuous supply of 


FUNCTIONS OF VEGETAL NERVE 555 


stimulus ts essential to the maintenance of the proper excitatory 
condition of a tissue. | | 

It is known that in the animal, when the conduc- 
tivity of a nerve is abolished by nerve degeneration, the 
connected muscles also rapidly waste away. Thus the 
maintenance of the proper excitability of various tissues is 
dependent on their constant reception of stimulus or energy 
through the mediation of the attached nerve. It is therefore 
highly probable that the excitability of the indifferent vegetable 
tissues is kept at its normal level by the reception of energy | 
of stimulus through the conducting nerve. | 

I shall next briefly refer to a fact which I have 
demonstrated fully elsewhere, that all the principal physio- 
logical activities of the plant, such as autonomous movement, 
ascent of sap, and growth are fundamentally excitatory 
phenomena. Thus, for example, the autonomous rhythmic 
movements of the lateral leaflets of Desmodium gyrans come 
to a standstill when their store of latent energy is exhausted. 
And it is only by the accession of fresh stimulus from 
outside that these multiple responses can be renewed. We 
describe the state of the plant, when its internal energy is 
below par, by saying that it is sub-tonic, the normal tonic 
condition, or health of the plant, being dependent on the 
sum total of stimulation previously absorbed by it. Turning 
next to the question of the ascent of sap, I have shown that 
the most important factor in bringing this about is the 
multiple rhythmic activity of certain interior tissues of the 
plant. Under such circumstances as cause the tonic condi- 
tion of the tissues to fall below par, the rate of the ascent of 
sap will be lowered, or it may possibly even be brought to a 
standstill, owing to the depression of excitability induced. 
On now supplying fresh stimulus, we find the excitability to 
be renewed and the normal rate of ascent restored (p. 383). 
Another instance of this is seen in the fzleus of Coprinus 
which droops when kept too long in the dark, but recovers 
its normal turgidity on exposure to light. 

Growth, again, I have shown to be a result of multiple 


5 56 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


rhythmic excitation. When the tonic condition of the plant 
falls below par, growth is arrested, however large the amount 
of formative material present. But if stimulus be applied, 
while the plant is in this state of growth-standstill, there is a 
renewal of responsive growth. 

The importance of the absorption of stimulus of light to 


the response of growth is seen in the development of certain 
seedlings. These, when grown in the dark, become diseased | 


and perish. The growth and the development of organs cease, 
even though the cotyledons still contain considerable 
quantities of unused formative substance. They become 
moribund, as the expression of their loss of tonic condition, 
which, as we have seen, depends upon the supply of stimulus 
from outside. And in the present case the critical element 
is the stimulus of light. 

This fact that the stimulus of light may enhance the ex- 

citability of a tissue, is shown in the following photographic 
records of the mechanical response of vegetal nerve. The 
first three responses show the extent of normal response to 
a given electrical stimulus applied in the dark. The specimen 
was then subjected to the light of an electrical arc lamp, and 
the next series show the consequent enhancement of response, 
under the same stimulus as before. Light was now cut off, 
and after an interval response once more taken. This is seen 
to have been of the same amplitude as at the beginning 
(fig. 334). Thus light is seen to enhance excitability. 

In the cases which we have just been considering we have, 
for the sake of simplicity, confined our attention to a single 
factor—namely the photo-tonic—among the many which 
finally determine the general tonic condition of the plant. 
And we have found that when the organism is deprived of 
this source of stimulus, its motile activity, its suctional 
activity, and its response of growth, all disappear. When 
the plant, however, is restored to the direct action of light, all 
these various activities reappear. Now it is clear that this 
illumination cannot. directly penetrate to many of those 
interior tissues, whose activity is nevertheless essential to the 


oe 


Pei Ba) Ras 


FUNCTIONS OF VEGETAL NERVE 557 


maintenance of life. How, then, is the external stimulus 
conveyed to these? It is evident that this can only be 
accomplished through the agency of the nervous elements. 
This fact, of the transmission of the excitatory effect of 
an external stimulus from one part of the plant to another at 
a distance, there to maintain the tonic condition, is again 
still more clearly seen in the well-known experiment on the 
sensitiveness of Wzmosa when partially kept in the dark. If 
one branch of this plant be covered by a dark box, while the 
rest of it is exposed to light, it is found that the leaves of the 


FIG. 334. Photographic Record showing Enhancement of Excitability 
under Action of Light in Nerve of Fern 


First series, normal mechanical responses to electrical stimuli, in dark ; 
second series, the same, taken under light; third series, taken after 
withdrawal of light. 


first undergo no loss of motile sensitiveness. It is thus 
evident that the photo-tonic stimulus has been transmitted 
from the illuminated to the unilluminated portions of the 
plant, through conducting channels, in order to maintain the 
normal excitatory condition. | 

Next comes @ie very interesting experiment of Sachs, in 
which a long shoot of Cucurbita was made to grow inside a 
dark box, the rest of the plant being exposed to light. The 
covered part of the plant, under these circumstances, showed 
normal growth of stem and leaves. Normal.flowers and a 
large fruit were also produced in the same confinement. 
The tendrils inside the box, moreover, were found to be fully 


558 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


as sensitive as those outside. The transmission of stimulus 
by the plant, in such a way as effectively to maintain such 
complex life-activities as motility and growth, even in the 
absence of direct stimulation, is. thus fully demonstrated. 
And we may gather an idea from this fact of the funda- 
mental importance, to the life of the plant, of those nervous 
elements by which this is rendered possible. 

One of the most important functions of the venation ot 
the leaf, not hitherto suspected, is now made clear to us: 
Among external stimuli, none 
perhaps is so essential, or so 
universally and easily available 
to green plants, as energy otf 
light. And we now see that 
the fine ramification of fibro- 
vascular elements over as wide 
an area as possible in the leaf, 
provides a virtual catchment- 
basin for the reception of stimu- 
lus. The expanded lamina is 
Re Sages STistginurine ore Ties thus not merely a specialised 

vascular Elements in Single structure, for the purpose of 

Bevan heel ot hapaye photo-synthesis, but also a sen- 
There are at least 20: such layers eb ‘ 

engirdling the stem. sitive area for the absorption 

of stimulus, the effect of which 
is gathered into larger and larger nerve-trunks, in the course 
of its transmission downwards into the body of the plant. 

And even in the interior of the plant the distribution o1 
these is such that no mass of tissue is too remote to be ex- 
cited by the stimulus conducted through the nervous ele- 
ments buried in them. How reticulated they may be, even 
in the trunk, is seen in the accompanying photograph of the 
distribution of fibro-vascular elements in the main stem of 
Papaya (fig. 335). This network, of which only a small 
portion is seen in the photograph, girdles the stem through- 
out its whole length, and in this particular case there were 
as Many as twenty such layers, one within the other. 


crawls, 


‘aomsn mei; pooner STONE es 


Pe ee le el 


ae: 


FUNCTIONS OF VEGETAL NERVE 559 


It is thus seen how all parts of the plant are, by means of 
nerve-conduction, maintained in the most intimate communi- 
cation with each other. It is, then, in virtue of the existence 
of such nerves, that the plant constitutes a single organised 
whole, each of whose parts is affected by every influence that 
falls upon any other. | 


CHAPTER XXXATX 


KLECTROTONUS 


Extra-polar effects of electrotonic currents on vegetal nerve—Electrotonic 
variation of excitability—Bernstein’s polarisation decrement —Hermann’s 
polarisation increment—Investigation into the law of electrotonic variation 
of conductivity —Investigation on variation of excitability—Conductivity en- 
hanced when excitation travels from places of lower to higher electric potential, 
and depressed in opposite direction—When feeble, anode enhances and kathode 
depresses excitability—All electrotonic phenomena reducible to combined 
action of these factors—Explanation of apparent anomalies. 


WHEN an electrical current is Jed through a portion of a 
nerve— entering, say, at A, and leaving by K—it is found that 
electro-motive changes are induced by it in the extra-polar 


AD aS = 
FIG. 336. FIG. 337. 
Extra-polar Kat- and An-electrotonic Effects 


Fig. 336 shows kat-electronus, E near K being galvanometrically negative. 
Fig. 337 shows an-electronus, E near A being now galvanometri- 
cally positive. 


regions. On the kathodic side, the electric potential near 
K is found to be lowered in reference to a point further away. 
On the anodic side similarly, the electric potential of a point 
near A is found to be raised. These changes induced in the 
electric potential are indicated by the galvanometric nega- 
tivity of the point near the kathode, and positivity of that 
near the anode (figs. 336, 337). In the tissue itself the 
current is assumed to flow in a direction contrary to that in 


> 


ELECTROTONUS 561 


the external galvanometric circuit, as indicated by the dotted 
arrow.' 

In the medullated animal nerve, the electrotonic currents 
increase with the intensity of the polarising current. In the 
vegetal nerve, I have obtained exactly similar results. 
Taking afresh and vigorous specimen, the polarising elec- 
trodes were placed at a distance of 2°5 cm. from each other, 
the pair of extra-polar electrodes, where the electrotonic 
effects are observed, being separated from these by 2 cm. and 
divided from each other by 2 cm. also. The value of the 
acting polarising E.M.F. could be varied by the use of a 


Fic, 338. Extra-polar Electrotonic Effects under an Acting E,M.F. 
which rises from °6 to 1°4 Volts 


A, an-electrotonic deflections seen to left ; K, kat-electrotonic to right. 


potentiometer arrangement. In order to induce in the extra- 
polar electrodes, an-electrotonic and kat-electrotonic effects 
alternately, the current in the polarising circuit can be sent 
in one direction or the other by means of a reversing-key. 
The record of the galvanometer deflection, in the extra-polar 
circuit, gives a measure of the electrotonic effect induced. 
In fig. 338 is seen such a record of. effects both an-electro- 
tonic and kat-electrotonic, taken while the acting E.M.F. was 
increased from ‘6 to 1°4 volt, by steps of ‘2 volt at a time. 


' Certain considerations, which need only be referred to here, cast some 
doubt on the validity of this assumption. But as it is so widely current in 
physiological literature, I shall confine myself, in dealing with the subject of the 
electrotonic current and its variations, to those indications in the external circuit 
which are afforded by the galvanometer. 


OO 


562 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


I give here a table which shows the galvanometric deflection 
corresponding to each particular E.M.F. 


TABLE OF GALVANOMETRIC DEFLECTIONS AND CORRESPONDING E.M.F. 


Volts | An-electrotonic effect Kat-electrotonic effect 
6 _ §0 divisions 58 divisions - 
8 65 9 78 9 
| ae) 107 ) IIO -s 
1‘2 126 ee 124 - 
1°4 150°; | 148s, 


In this particular experiment, it will be seen that the 
an-electrotonic and kat-electrotonic effects are practically 
equal. But, to be more accurate, the an-electrotonic are 
slightly lower with low E.M.F., and slightly higher with 
high, than the corresponding kat-electrotonic deflections. A 
constant electrical current is thus seen to induce electro- 
motive variations, outside its poles, in the vegetal nerve. 

We next turn to the question of the variation of 
excitability induced in a tissue, by the passage of a con- 
stant current. On this subject the most important con- 
tributions have been made by Bernstein and Hermann. 
Bernstein, experimenting on the sciatic nerve of frog, found 
that excitation induced a polarisation decrement. This 
experiment is illustrated in the following diagram (figs. 339, 


n 
—— 
eceet 
—o 
<a 
Siete 


9 
— . x — =? SSS 
i EE Kk A j See eee ere 
33 Kat~<--—- = 3300 An----> Tah 
FIG, 339. FIG. 340. 


Figs. 339, 340. Diagrams illustrating Bernstein’s Electrotonic Decrement 


Fig. 339 shows decrement of kat-electrotonic, and fig. 340 of an-electro- 
tonic currents, under stimulation at s. In this and following figures 
the inside thin arrow indicates direction of polarising current, the 
outside thick arrow the direction of responsive current. 


340). In fig. 339 the kathodal effect is seen induced 
in the extra-polar circuit. When the nerve is now excited 


by tetanising electric shocks, a diminution of the extra-polar 
current is induced. When the anodal effect is induced in 


ELECTROTONUS ; 563. 


the extra-polar circuit, by reversal of polarising current, 
the an-electrotonic current, opposed in direction to the former 
kat-electrotonic, also undergoes diminution on excitation 
of the nerve (fig. 340). It has been suggested that this 
diminution of electrotonic current was due to a supposed 
diminution, during excitation, of the susceptibility of the nerve 
to polarisation. 

But this explanation is negatived by an experiment of 
Hermann, showing the occurrence of polarisation increment 
during excitation. In figs. 341 and 342 we havea polarising 
_ and exciting circuit in series, excitation being caused by the 


ot eh 


= Yo a a aEEERRREaEned 
A pea af K K <—__—_—_ A 
Fic. 341. FIG. 342. 


Figs. 341, 342. Diagrams representing Hermann’s Polarisation- 
increment under Tetanising Shocks 


Inside thin arrow indicates the direction of polarising current ; the 
outside thick arrow, the direction of excitatory current. 


secondary coil of an inductiorium. With such an arrange- 
ment, the polarising current, whether from left to right or from 
right to left, is found, during excitation, to undergo an 
augmentation. Hermann refers these facts to alterations 
of intensity in the negative wave of excitation, during its 
passage through the nerve, when the latter is polarised. ‘ It is, 
indeed, more pronounced at any point of the nerve, the more 
strongly positive and weakly negative the polarisation of the 
latter, ze. it increases when it is becoming algebraically 
more positive, and diminishes when it advances upon more 
negative points’ (Hermann’s Law of the ‘ Polarisation 
Increment’ of excitation).! 

It would thus appear that the observations hitherto made, 
as to the effects of electrotonus on excitatory response, are of 


! Biedermann, Blectro- -phystology (Engl. transl. ), vol, ii. p. 315. 
002 


564 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


a somewhat discordant character. I shall, however, be able 
to show that their complexity is due to the combination of 
the different effects of the polarisation current on conductivity 
and on excitability. These separate effects may, according 
to circumstances, either conspire or act antagonistically. 
Hence the great variety of results, which appears at first 
sight incapable of a consistent explanation. 

In order, then, to discover the laws by which an electric. 
current induces a variation of conductivity and excitability, 
we must first determine the pure effect of the current on 
conductivity, apart from any excitatory variation; and, 
secondly, its effect on excitability, uncomplicated by any 
variation of conductivity. | | 

To take conductivity first: the ideally perfect arrange- 
ment would be to have the polarising electrodes, in relation 
to the region whose conductivity-variation is to be tested, at 
a distance so great that they could exert no predominant 
an- or kat-electrotonic influence upon it. In a led-off 
circuit, moreover, a differential action, unless proper,pre- 
cautions are taken, is exerted on two electrodes placed side 
by side. It is, therefore, desirable to remove one of these 
outside the sphere of action. Such, then, being the con- 
ditions to be observed, in order to eliminate the effect of 
the poles themselves, and thus determine the influence of the 
direction of current on conductivity alone, I took a petiole 
of fern 20cm. in length, and connected its ends through a 
reversing-key with a Daniell cell (E.M.F.= 1 volt). The 
responding galvanometer-circuit had one electrode about the 
middle of the petiole, near the insertion of a certain lateral 
leaflet, the other electrode being connected with the lamina 
of the same leaflet, whose midrib, however, was cut across to 
prevent transmission of the excitatory effect (figs. 343, 344). 
It is thus seen that the led-off electrodes are at a relatively 
great distance from the polarising electrodes, and further, 
owing to one electrode being placed out of the way, on the 
lateral leaflet, and the other symmetrically between anode 
and .kathode, it is clear that anodal or kathodal action is 


ee eS ee rae eee eee eee eee 


ELECTROTONUS 565 


reduced to wz. The excitation from the stimulator is 
transmitted across the intervening conducting region, either 
along the slope of a falling electrical potential, that is to 
say, from-a galvanometrically positive to a galvanometrically 
negative point, or against that direction, namely, in an 
electrically uphill manner, from the galvanometrically 
negative to the galvanometrically positive. Now, if the 
direction of an electrical current have an effect on the 
conduction of excitation, this fact will be detected by 
the modification induced in the normal response during the 
passage of the current. : 

The results of the present experiment will be found to 
determine this question. Excitation was induced by means 


Fic. 343. : Fic. 344. 
Figs. 343, 344. Experiment with Petiole of Fern demonstrating Variation 
of Conductivity by Polarising Current 


of the thermal stimulator, and the normal responses taken, 
shown in fig. 345 a, as ‘up. The polarising current was 
now sent from left to right; hence excitation will now be 
transmitted through the intervening conducting region in an 
electrically downhill manner, or in the direction of the falling 
potential—that is to say, from the region of the anode to that 
of the kathode. It will be seen presently that conduction is 
retarded or abolished when excitation is made to travel elec- 
trically downhill from the anode to the kathode.' If this be 
so, we shall expect to detect the fact by the diminution of 
the amplitude of the normal response, or even by its actual 
reversal. For we have seen that when the excitatory re- 
action of galvanometric negativity is sufficiently retarded, its 
opposite, the positive effect, often makes its appearance alone. 


1 These remarks apply to a feeble or moderate rate of fall of potential. 


566 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


In fig. 345 4, this is seen to have actually occurred. There 
will be other cases where, the depression of conductivity 
induced being not too great, it will be possible to watch the 
gradually lessening amplitude of response until it ends in 
actual reversal. We have next to determine the effect on 
conductivity, of the passage of excitation in an uphill 
direction—that is to say, from the kathodic to the anodic 
region. For this purpose the polarising current was reversed 


Fic. 345. Photographic Records of Responses taken in last Experiment, 
when Excitation was transmitted with and against the Polarising 
Current 


a, Normal response of petiole of fern to transmitted excitation; 4, 
Reversal of response when excitation was travelling electrically downhill 
or with the current; c, Normal response once more ; @, Enhanced 
response due to increase of conductivity when excitation travels electri- 
cally uphill, or against the polarising current. . Upward arrow 4 
indicates that polarising current is in same direction as normal 
response. Downward arrow { shows polarising current in opposite 
direction. The same in the two following figures. 


by means of a reversing key, the left-hand end of the petiole 
being now made the kathode. Before doing this, however, I 
stopped the current, and took a second set of records of 
normal responses. It will be seen that by reason of the 
cessation of the previously acting left-to-right current, 
leaving an after-effect, these were slightly enhanced above 
the first normal responses (fig. 345 ¢c). On now reversing 
the current, conductivity was found to be enhanced, as seen 
in the greater amplitude of response (fig. 345 @). 


aan 


ELECTROTONUS 567 


I next undertook an investigation into the effect on 
conductivity, of variations of intensity in a moderate polaris- 
ing current. This is shown in fig. 346, in which excitation 
travels electrically downhill—that is to say, from the anodic to 
the kathodic region. In a we have the normal response 
before the passage of the current. In 4 we have the re- 
sponses reduced by the diminution of conductivity con- 
sequent on the application of ‘1 volt for polarisation. On 
the application of *5 volt in c, there was a tendency towards 


Fic. 346. Photographic Record of Modification of Conduction during 
Passage of Excitation from Anodic to Kathodic Region, under Increasing 
Intensity of Polarising E.M.F. 


a, Normal response ; 4, Diminished response where terminal E.M.F. was 
"I volt; c, Response still further diminished and rendered diphasic 
under *5 volt ; @, Response reversed under I volt. 


reversal, the response being now diphasic, positive followed 
by negative. Finally, on the application of 1 volt in -d, we 
see the response reversed to positive, the conduction of the 
true excitatory effect being here altogether abolished. 

In a second set of experiments, carried out on a fresh 
specimen, I investigated the effect of an increasing intensity 
of the polarising current, when the excitation was made to 
travel, electrically uphill, from the kathodic to the anodic 
region. It will be seen, from figure 347, that the application 
of a polarising E.M.F. of 1 volt increased the conductivity, 
as seen in the heightened responses shown in 4, as compared 


568 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


with the normal responses in a. The application of higher 
polarising E.M.F. of °5, 1, and 1°5 volts respectively, now 
- induced appropriate increments of conductivity, as seen in ¢, 
d, and e. I was unable to use an E.M.F. of higher than 
I'5 volts because the galvanometer spot of light became 
unsteady. It is to be borne in mind that the specimens in 
these experiments were 20 cm. long, and the maximum 
potential gradient employed was only ‘07 volt per cm. In 
experimenting on electrotonic effects, it must be remembered 
that the E.M.F. employed is, generally speaking, feeble. 


Fic. 347. Photographic Record showing. Enhanced Conduction from 
Kathodic to Anodic Region 


a, Normal responses; 4, c, d, ¢, Responses gradually enhancing under 
increasing polarising current. 


From the results described, then, we arrive at the follow- 
ing law of the effect of a moderate or feeble constant electric 
current on conductivity. 

A moderate polarising E.M.F. induces variations of conduc- 
tivity. The conductivity is increased in the direction from the 
kathodic to the anodic region, and depressed in the opposite, 

Having thus demonstrated the pure effect of a constant 
electric current on conductivity, we have next to study the 
unmixed effect of polarisation on excitability. We. have 
seen that when two equally excitable points in the same 
circuit are simultaneously excited by an identical stimulus, 
there is no resultant response, since the two excitatory 


* eLECTROTONUS 569 


effects balance each other. The galvanometric effect is 
then zero. But if one of the two have its excitability en- 
hanced in any way, this balance will be disturbed, and a 
resultant current will flow through the circuit, the more ex- 
citable contact becoming galvanometrically negative. I now 
took a long piece of isolated vegetal nerve and connected 
it with the galvanometer at E and E’,, a secondary coil, 
giving equi-alternating electric shocks, being also in the 
circuit (fig. 348). The 
two longitudinal con- 
tacts E’ and E being 
more or less equally 
excitable, there was at 
first no. resultant re- 
sponse to stimulation. 
E’ was now made the 
anode, an E.M.F. of +1 
volt being used for the 
purpose, and a perma- 
nent current was found 
to flow in the galvano- 
meter in the direction 
of E’GE shown by the 
thin inner arrow, E’ 
being galvanometrically 


FIG, 349. 

Experimental Arrangement 
to Exhibit the Enhancement of Excit- 
ability at Anode, and its Depression at 
Kathode, when the Acting E.M.F. is 
feeble 


Figs. 348, 349. 


positive. On now ap- 
plying equi-alternating 
shocks, the balance was 


Fig. 348 shows enhancement of excitability 
at anode; Fig. 349, the depression of 
excitability at kathode. 


Inside thin arrow indicates direction of tantarins 


ing current. Outside thick arrow, direction 
of excitatory current. Note that in both 
thereis a so-called polarisation-decrement. 


found to have been dis- 
turbed, and the re- 
sponsive. current to be in the opposite direction, namely, 
EGE’, as shown by the thick arrow; E’ now undergoing an 
excitatory negative variation. This shows that the ex- 
citability of E’ has become enhanced by being made 
anode. E’ was next made kathode, in consequence of 
which the permanent current in the galvanometer was now 
in the direction of EGE’ (fig. 349). E’ is now the kathode, E 


570 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


in relation to it being anode. On excitation the responsive 
current was in the opposite direction to the permanent 
current, in consequence of the induced depression of the 
kathode E’ or the induced enhancement of excitability at 
the relative anode E. In fig. 350 are given the records of 
these effects. Two different experiments were carried out, 
on two different specimens of plant nerve, whose records are 
given in a and 4, fig. 350. In each of these we observe, first, 


Fic. 350. Photographic Records of Response, illustrating the Enhance- 
ment of Excitability at Anode, and Depression at Kathode, under 
Feeble Acting E.M.F. in two Specimens of Nerve of Fern a and 6 


Before application of polarising current there was no resultant response in 
either case. When E’ was made anode, there was an up-response, 
indicating enhanced excitability of that point.. The dotted arrow ¥ 
seen below shows the direction of polarising current. The responsive 
current is then opposite in direction to the polarising current. When 
E’ is made kathode the resultant response is down, showing depression 
of the excitability of the point. The responsive current is here also 
opposite in direction to the polarising current. 


the enhanced excitability due to E’ being made anode, 
which gives rise to up-responses, opposite in direction to the 
permanent current, shown by the dotted arrow below. The 
second pair of responses in each case shows the depression 
of excitability at the kathodic point E’, which is tantamount 
to enhancement of excitability at the relative anode E. An 
inspection of figs. 348 and 349 will show that the responsive 
current is always in a direction opposite to that of the existing 
polarising current, thus constituting the so-called polarisation 


+ 


FOO OO ee eee ee oe 


TGR conse pL SS ee 


Seb er aN ea 


ee 


nace -2t 
WwW | 


ELECTROTONUS S71 


decrement. But we shall presently find that the direction of 
the responsive current is the only constant factor here, de- 
termined as this is by the relative excitabilities of the. two 
electrodes. An identical variation of excitability may, as 
I shall show, appear under different circumstances, either as 
a polarisation-increment or as a decrement. 

I have obtained results precisely like the foregoing, with 
the sciatic nerve of frog. It should be mentioned here that 
such effects are obtained without much difficulty in the first 
stages of polarisation. But, if this be prolonged, there is 
a certain liability to reversal. 

From the experiments which have been described on 
variations of excitability by the polar action of currents, we 
arrive at the following law: 

A feeble E.M.F. induces modifications in the excitability of a 
tissue: the anode enhances and kathode depresses excitability. 

This result is startling, contravening, as it does, Pfliiger’s 
Law. A factor that had not been taken into account 
was the range of E.M.F., within which this law might 
be applicable. In the present case, the acting E.M.F. is 
relatively feeble, and we shall see later that Pfliiger’s Law 
does not apply above or below a certain medium range. 

Having thus obtained the isolated effects of electric 
currents on conductivity and excitability respectively, I 
next took up those more complex cases in which both 
effects were present in various combinations. This problem 
was attacked by means of the Conductivity Balance. In 
this experiment, carried out on the petiole of fern, the led-off 
points E and E’ were at a distance of 6 cm. from each other. 
The distance of each of the polarising electrodes A and K 
outside the led-off circuit E’ and E was 2 cm. (figs. 351 and 
352). The thermal stimulator S was so adjusted before 
the passage of the current that the excitations at E’ and E 
were exactly equal, as seen in the balanced horizontal record 
n fig. 353a. It should be mentioned here thatthe galvano- 
meter connections were so arranged that an _ increased 
excitability of the left-hand contact E’, would be shown by 


572 


COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


means of ‘down’ and that of the right hand contact E by 


means of ‘up’ responses. 


An E.M.F. of *5 volt was now 


eens Gorrtey rep 
fa = Ame fl 
A E’ E K K; E E H 
noe 
FIG, 351. FIG. 352. 


Figs. 351, 352. 


Experimental Arrangement demonstrating the Joint Effects of 


Variation of Conductivity and Excitability by Polarising Current 


employed to induce polarisation, the anode being in the first 


case to the left. 


Fic. 353. Photographic Record of Response 
under the Arrangements given in Figs. 351, 
352 in Nerve of Fern 


a, Balanced record before passage of polarising 
current. 

4, Resultant response downwards when polar- 
ising current is from left to right, as shown 
by arrow —. This shows excitability of 
FE’ and conductivity in direction SE’ to be 
relatively enhanced. 

c, Resultant response upwards when polarising 
current is from right to left <. This 
shows excitability of E and conductivity in 
direction SE to be relatively enhanced. 


Here we have a greater excitability induced 


at E’ by the proximity 
of the anode, that of E 
being depressed by the 
proximity of kathode. 
Of the two waves of ex- 
citation, moreover, which 
proceed in opposite di- 
rections from the stimu- 
lator S, that towards E’ 
is moving electrically 
uphill, or towards the 
anode, and owing to 
increased conductivity 
in that direction the 
excitation is better con- 
ducted than in the case 
of the second wave, 
which is _ proceeding 
electrically downhill to- 
wards the kathode at 
E. It must also be 


remembered that not only is the intensity of excitation 
which reaches E’ greater than that which reaches E, but also 
that the point E’ is itself rendered more excitable by the 


ELECTROTONUS 573 


contiguity of the anode, while E is depressed by that of the 
kathode. Hence, by the concordant action, at each end of 
the balance, of conductivity and excitability changes, and 
owing to the opposite nature of these changes at opposite 
ends, the original balance is disturbed, and we obtain 
resultant down responses, showing the greater excitation and 
galvanometric negativity caused at E’ than at E, when anode 
is to the left and kathode to the right. This is exhibited in 
the first pair of down responses in figure 353 4. When, 
however, the polarisation current is reversed (fig. 352), the 
excitation at the right-hand side, E being now near the anode, 
is relatively the greater, and we find the resultant responses 
to be upwards, as seen in the third record in fig. 353. To 
go back to the question of the relative directions of electro- 
tonic and responsive currents, we find in that case, when 
the anode is to the left, that E’ is galvanometrically positive, 
while the excitatory change makes it galvanometrically 
negative. This means that the excitatory response takes 
place by the so-called polarisation decrement. When the 
anode again is to the right, the galvanometrically positive E 
tends by excitation to become galvanometrically negative. 
This will be clearly understood from the arrows which 
accompany the diagram, in figs. 351, 352. The inner and thin 
arrows represent the direction of the polarising current, and 
the thick outer arrows the responsive current. These results 
are tantamount to an example of the so-called polarisation- 
decrement. In order to show, however, that the. same 
excitatory reaction might appear as a polarisation-increment, 
I shall describe another experiment. 

The experimental tissue is here the isolated nerve of fern, 
and the method employed is again that of the Conductivity 
Balance. In this case, however, it will be noticed (figs. 354, 
355), that the galvanometer is included in series with the 
polarising E.M.F., instead of being placed as a shunt, as in the 
lastcase. The stimulator was first adjusted at halance. The 
left electrode was now made kathode, the right being anode, 
the E.M.F. employed being °2 volt (fig. 354). On account 


574 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


of the increased excitability of A at the anode, and also 
of the greater intensity of excitation conducted towards it, 
the balance was disturbed, and the resultant response ‘took 
place by the enhanced galvanometric negativity of that 
point. The responsive current thus constituted an increment 
of the polarisation-current, as seen from the arrows; of which 
the thin inner represents the polarisation and the thick outer 
the responsive current. On now reversing the current again; 
the right-hand end being made anode and more excitable, 
the resultant response was found to take place by the 
enhanced negativity of that point, thus again constituting 
a polarisation increment (fig. 355). I give here two different 


Fic. 354. T'IG. 355. 
Figs. 354, 355. - Experimental Arrangements for Showing so-called 
Polarisation-increment by the Joint Effect of Increased Excitability 
at Anode and Enhanced Conduction of Excitation electrically Uphill 


sets of photographic records, obtained with the nerves of 
fern and frog respectively. Balance was first obtained at 
the beginning of the record, but on the passage of the 
polarisation current, this balance was found to be disturbed. 
When the right end of the balance was made anode, the 
resultant response on excitation was up, demonstrating the 
enhanced excitability of the anodic point. When the right- 
hand end, however, was made kathode, the balance was 
upset in the opposite direction, that is to say, down, showing 
that the left-hand anodic point was now the more excitable. 
Fig. 356 gives a record of these responses as obtained from 
the nerve of fern, and fig. 357 from the nerve of frog. The 
responsive currents in these cases, it should be noted, are in 
the same direction as the polarising current. 


ELECTROTONUS - 575 


On referring to the experiments on polarisation increment 
and decrement which have just been described it will be noticed 
that the excitatory reaction is the same in both cases, taking 
place by the enhanced galvanometric negativity of the more 
excitable anodic point. The seeming difference in the 
electrotonic variation in the two cases lies simply in the fact 
of the different dispositions of the galvanometer. This 
occupied, in the first case, the position of a shunt in the 
polarisation-circuit, while in the second it was placed in 
series. 

From the investigations which have been described, we 
shall now find ourselves in a position to explain the various 


FIG. 356. FIG. 357. 


Fic. 356. Photographic Record of Responses in Nerve of Fern, under 
Anodic and Kathodic Action, as described in Figs. 354 and 355. 
The upsetting of the balance is upwards, when the right-hand end of 
the balance is made anode, proving the enhanced excitability thus 
induced. Resultant response downwards when the right-hand end of 
the balance is made kathode. 


Fic. 357. Photographic Record of Similar Effects in Nerve of Frog. 


experiments of Hermann and Bernstein, and to show that 
these, although apparently conflicting, are really mutually 
consistent. First, then, to take Hermann’s experiment, and 
referring back to figs. 341 and 342, in which the galvanometer 
is placed in series in the polarisation-circuit, we find this to 
be an instance in which we have to deal almost exclusively 
with the effect of anode and kathode on excitability. 
Simultaneous excitation of the anodic and kathodic points 
by alternating induction currents, induces greater excitation, 
and consequent enhanced negativity of A. The responsive 


576 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


current, being thus concordant with the electrotonic current, 
causes an increase of it, the so-called polarisation-increment. 
In Bernstein’s experiment on polarisation-decrement, we 
are confronted with a question of greater complexity, for 
here we have to deal with changes of conductivity and 
excitability at the same time. We shall first take the -case 
(fig. 339) in which one electrode of the led-off circuit E is 
under kat-electrotonus. EE’ is therefore relatively anodal, 
and consequently more excitable. The excitation from the 
stimulator S which reaches E’ is in this case impeded in 
reaching E by the fact that it has to travel electrically 
downhill—that is, from the anodal E’ to kathodal E. Thus, 
owing to the greater excitation which reaches E’, and owing 
also to the greater excitability induced in it by the fact that 
it is relatively anode, excitation induces a relatively greater 
galvanometric negativity of that point. The thick arrow in 
the figure indicates the excitatory current, which is opposite 
in direction to the polarisation current, which latter is 
indicated by the thin arrow. In the second case, when the 
polarisation current is reversed (fig. 340), E is anodal, and 
_ therefore relatively more excitable, and E’ kathodal, and there- 
fore less excitable. Unlike the last case, the excitation from 
s, in order to reach FE, has now to travel electrically uphill 
from the kathodal to the anodal points. The excitation of E 
therefore is in this case not impeded. Hence greater excit- 
ability of E makes that point, on stimulation, galvanometrically 
negative, and the responsive current, represented by a thick 
arrow, brings about a diminution of the polarisation-current. 
It has thus been shown, in the course of the present 
chapter, that the same electrotonic effects are exhibited in 
the case of the plant, as in that of animal, nerves. It has 
been shown that various apparently anomalous results may 
be brought about by simple combinations of two different 
factors. Thus, the so-called polarisation increment and 
decrement are not mutually conflicting. They are, on the 
contrary, due to the distinct and definite effects induced by 
electrotonus on conductivity and excitability respectively, 


ELECTROTONUS SGe- 


As regards conductivity, it has been shown that excitation 
travels best in the direction electrically uphill—that is to say, 
from a place of low to one of high electric potential. In 
consequence of this fact a moderate excitation becomes 
enhanced when travelling from the kathodic to the anodic 
region. Conductivity is depressed, on the other hand, from 
anode to kathode. An excitatory impulse is thus retarded 
in travelling electrically down-hill. For these reasons, a 
normal negative excitatory effect may, during transmission, 
undergo either diminution of intensity, or actual reversal to 
positive. 

With reference, again, to electrotonic variations of excita- 
bility, we have seen that under feeble E.M.F. it is the anode 
that exalts, and the kathode that depresses. This conclusion. 
is obviously opposed to the generalisation known as Pfliiger’s 
Law, the extent of the applicability of which will be discussed 
in detail in the following chapter. 


P,P 


CAP LER 
INADEQUACY OF PFLUGER’S LAW 


Reversal of Pfliiger’s Law under high E.M.F.—Similar reversals under feeble 
E.M.F.—Investigation by responsive sensation—Experiments on _ living 
wounds—Under moderate E. M.F., intensity of sensation enhanced at kathode, 
and depressed at anode—Under feeble E.M.F., sensation intensified at anode 
and depressed at kathode—Application of electrical currents in medical 
practice. 


IN studying polar variations of excitability in nerves, in 
the last chapter, we found that, during the passage of the 
current, it was the anode which enhanced excitability and 
the kathode which induced depression. Now this conclusion, 
as will be remembered, is directly opposed to what is known as 
Pfliigers Law, the universal applicability of which has 
hitherto been regarded as beyond dispute. Pfliiger’s Law 
lays it down that the kathode excites at make, and the anode 
at break; and that, moreover, during the passage of a 
constant current, excitability is raised at or near the kathode, 
and depressed at or near the anode. We are next, then, led 
to inquire: Under what conditions is this law applicable, and 
when does it fail to hold good? Now, as regards the effects 
at make and break, I have shown elsewhere, in the course of 
experiments on plants, that these are not determined by 
anode and kathode alone, but also by the intensity of the 
acting electro-motive force. Thus, in the case of the sensitive 
Biophytum, in a given experiment it was found that, using the 
moderately strong E.M.F. of 24 volts, the excitatory wave at 
make was found to be initiated at kathode, and to travel in 
both directions, causing depression of nine pairs of leaflets. 
The forward half of this wave of excitation, stopped only at 


i i as 


ET A A 
_ 
' Sn 
. ————— 
ST 


INADEQUACY OF PFLUGER’S LAW 579 


one pair of leaflets before the anode. There was no action 
at the anode itself at make. After a suitable interval, during 
which the leaflets re-erected themselves, the current was 
interrupted. There was now no action near the kathode at 
break; but excitation was induced at the anode, as was 
shown by the fall of three pairs of leaflets in its vicinity (fig. 358), 
The experiment was now repeated by reversing the direction 
of the current. The poles being thus reversed, eight pairs of 
leaflets fell at the new kathode, in and out. There was no 
effect, however, at the new anode at make. But at break, 
excitatory reaction was _ ini- 
tiated at the anode, and none 
at the kathode. 

These are the normal 
effects, falling under Pfliiger’s 
Law, which holds good within 
a certain medium range of 
E.M.F. But when the E.M.F. 
is much higher, I find that 
these normal effects become Fic. 358. Make-kathode and Break- 


reversed. Thus, employing anode Effects in Bzophytum 
EMF. of lts-j Upper figure shows effect at make, 
an i.M.I'. Of 220 volts, it was excitation being produced at 
found that excitation took kathode. Lower figure shows 
effect at break, excitation being 
place at the anode at make, now produced at anode. 


the excitatory depression of | 

the leaflets passing slowly thence towards the kathode. At 
break, excitatory action was initiated at the kathode, the 
wave of excitation then passing towards the anode. I thus 
found, by the employment of a very high E.M.F., that the 
normal polar effects were completely reversed. Intermediate 
between these two extremes of normal and reversed action, 
I obtained a transitional phase, in which both anode and 
kathode were seen to excite at make. At break also there 
was here occasional excitation, at either anode or kathode. 
Similar reversals and transitional effects have also been 
noticed, in the case of certain protozoa by Kiihne and 


Verworn. Thus Pelomyxa is excited by the anode at make, 
PP2 


580 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


and by the kathode at break. Actinospherium, again, shows 
excitation on make, at both anode and kathode, and on break 
at the kathode only. 

Having thus demonstrated the fact that an excessively 
strong E.M.F. induces a reversal of the normal polar effects, 
it may not appear improbable that there should be a similar 
reversal of these effects when the intensity of E.M.F. is 
varied in the opposite direction, that is to say, when it is | 
very weak. I have already drawn attention, in many places, 
to the importance of this factor of intensity in determining 
the excitatory effect of a stimulating agent. A chemical 
reagent, for instance, when administered in moderate or very 
dilute doses, will induce one effect, say that of exaltation, 
and, in greater quantities, the very opposite, or depression. 
A poisonous reagent, again, which usually induces depression, 
will, if given in sufficiently minute quantities, have the effect 
of exaltation. These reversals, under varying intensities of 
the external agent, are noticeable again in different physico- 
chemical phenomena. Thus it is well known that in the 
formation of the photographic image, while a moderate 
_ intensity of light gives us the normal ‘negative,’ a stronger 
intensity will produce a ‘ positive,’ and a still more intense 
light, bring about a re-reversal. We may thus have a series 
of recurrent reversals. 

- Returning, then, to the question of polar action on 
excitability, we find that the typical results of Pfliiger with 
nerve and muscle preparations, were obtained when using 
a moderately strong E.M.F. In this, which is sometimes 
distinguished as the third stage, excitatory contraction of the 
muscle is induced, only on the closure of the descending 
current, or opening of the ascending. In the former case, 
the kathode is nearest the muscle, and as there is no inter- 
mediate block the excitation is clearly due to the make-action 
of the kathode. In the second case, similarly, the break of 
the anode, which is now near the muscle, causes excitation. 
With very weak E.M.F., however—that is to say, in his first 
stage—Pfliiger found that excitation took place by the make 


INADEQUACY OF PFLUGER’S LAW 581 


of both ascending and descending currents, but not at the 
break of either. Extending the clear inference of the 
previous case to cover this, it was supposed that here, too, 
the kathode—at one time near to, and at another far from, the 
responding muscle—excited at make. But this is not so 
conclusive, since the anode might equally well be regarded 
here as causing the excitation at make. Indeed this 
supposition that a very weak anode might cause excitation 
at make derives some support from Heidenhain’s experi- 
ments. For, using a weak E.M.F., he found make-excitation 
to occur in the first stage only, when the current was 
ascending—that is to say, when the anode was near the 
responding muscle. This result would tend to show that 
there was a possibility of the reversal of normal polar effects 
when thé acting E.M.F. was weak. With regard to this 
particular effect of minimal currents, there are considerable 
differences of opinion. The obtaining of such an effect is 
probably only possible when the nerve-muscle preparation is 
in an exceptionally favourable condition of excitability, a 
state of things not always possible to secure in the isolated 
specimen. It therefore occurred to me that the effect of 
a feeble anode in enhancing excitability might be demon- 
strated conclusively in the case of the vigorous intact animal. 
With this consideration in view, I carried out a number of 
experiments on certain of my students and myself. 

If we make a slight wound, say one square cm. in area, on 
the back of the hand, and apply a solution of salt, which is 
not too strong, a constant sensation is induced which cannot 
be called painful, but may best be described as smarting or 
irritating. If now we apply one non-polarisable electrode 
on this wound, and the other on a distant and indifferent 
point, then, on applying an E.M.F. to the circuit, charac- 
teristic variations of sensation will be induced, depending 
on whether the wound-spot is made anode or kathode 
(fig. 359). Employing an E.M.F. of 2 volts, it will be found 
that when the spot is made kathode, the sensation, which 
was previously one of mere general irritation, becomes 


582 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


intensely painful. This is because the kathode, at make and 
during its continuation, induces an enhancement of the excit- 
ability of the wound-spot. On the cessation of the current, 
the painful sensation disappears, and the normal smarting is 
restored. The wound-spot was next made anode, with 


Fic. 359. Effect of Anode and Kathode on ROSES Sensation 
in Human Hand 


By means of reversing key, R, E in connection with the esha: -spot may be 
made anode, and E' kathode, and wice versa. By alternately pressing 
the keys, K’ and kK, feeble or moderately strong E.M.F. may be em- 


ployed. 
the same E.M.F. as before. The sensation now experienced 
was one of soothing, the sense of smarting irritation having 
disappeared. On the stoppage of this current the original 
irritation was again restored. 

In these experiments we have typical instances of the 
kathode inducing increase of excitability, and the anode 


INADEQUACY OF PFLUGER’S LAW 583. 


depressing it, during the continuation of the current, a 
verification, by means of responsive sensations, of Pfliiger’s 
Law. Having thus, with moderate E.M.F. obtained the 
excitatory effect at kathode, and depressing effect at the 
anode, by means of the contrasted sensations of intense 
irritation and soothing, I was next desirous of seeing 
whether, with low E.M.F., these effects would be reversed. 
I therefore undertook investigations on a dozen different 
individuals, to determine the effect of anode and kathode, 
as the E.M.F. was gradually increased from *3 to 2 volts. 
It should be mentioned here that the subjects of the 
experiments were totally ignorant of the object of the 
investigations, and were simply asked to describe their 
sensations at different points. Their ages varied from 
eighteen to twenty-five. As the critical point may undergo 
some variation with the season, it may be worth while also 
to mention that the experiments were carried out in 
summer, in the month of August. 
The following case may be taken as typical : 


PoLAR EFFECTS OF E.M.F. oF VARIOUS INTENSITIES ON RESPONSIVE 


SENSATION 
Acting E.M.F. | Effect on wound-spot when kathode Effect on wound-spot when anode 
*3 volt Slightly soothing Marked increase of irritation 
So _ Slightly soothing Marked increase of irritation 
os eee Increase of irritation Indifferent 
ES 155 Increase of irritation Slightly soothing 
2°0 volts Painful Soothing 


It will thus be seen that the kathode, which, at the 
moderately intense E.M.F. of 2 volts, induced a painful 
sensation, owing to the increase of excitability, induced the 
very opposite effect of depressing excitability at the low 
E.M.F. of -3 volt. Precisely the reverse, moreover, was the 
case with the anode. Here, with ‘3 volt, excitability was 
found to be enhanced, causing increase of irritation, while, 
with the moderately strong E.M.F. of 2 volts, it induced the 
opposite effect of soothing, by depression of excitability. 


584 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


The critical point of reversal would in this instance appear 
to be slightly below 1 volt, in the case of the kathode, while 
in that of the anode, it was at 1 volt, or slightly above. The 
effect observed at the extreme points were the same in all 
cases. Individual differences were concerned only with the 
exact point of reversal. Thus the point of reversal for the 
kathode varied in different cases between ‘6 and 1 volt; 
whereas with the anode it varied from 1 to 1°5 volt. Ina. 
subsequent chapter, this phenomenon of reversal of sensation 
under varying intensities of E.M.F., when other forms ot 
stimulus are applied, will be studied in more detail. It may 
be stated here, however, that though the critical point of 
reversal varies to some extent with different individuals, and 
under different forms of stimulation, yet the law holds good 
that the excitatory effects induced by moderate E.M.F. are 
exactly reversed under feeble. 

The main results regarding this opposition of the effects 
of feeble and strong E.M.F. may be still better demonstrated 
by the method of successive contrasts. In the last experi- 
ments, a long course of observations on the same individual, 

would be liable to fatigue the tissue. Moreover, the fine 
gradation of the changes induced is not calculated to exhibit 
the contrasts involved in their full intensity. Having, then, 
determined, from the previous experiments, that an E.M.F. 
of ‘5 volt and another of 2 volts were opposed in their 
excitatory effects, I now made special arrangements for 
applying these two intensities of E.M.F. alternately. For 
this purpose, I arranged a potentiometer which gave an 
E.M.F. of 2 volts between L and N (fig. 359), and of 
‘5 volt between Land M. The end, L, was connected with 
the wound-spot by means of a non-polarisable electrode. 
A distant indifferent point, say on the surface of the finger, 
was connected with a double key KK’. When kK’ was 
pressed, ‘5 volt was applied, and when K, 2 volts. Further, 
by means of a reversing-key, P, the wound-spot could be 
made either anode or kathode at will. In this way, first 
making the wound-spot anode, I applied alternately the 


INADEQUACY OF PFLUGER’S LAW 585 __ 


E.M.F. of ‘5 and of 2 volts respectively. The lower voltage 
now gave rise to intense excitatory pain. On the cessation 
of the current the normal smarting sensation, due to salt, 
was restored, and on now applying 2 volts, this slight 
irritation was superseded by a sensation of soothing. This 
result was found to be repeated many different times. 

The kathodic effect was next put to the test, and found 
to induce responsive sensations exactly the reverse. The 
application of *5 volt caused a soothing sensation, due to 
depression of excitability. An E.M.F. of 2 volts, on the 
other hand, induced an increase of excitability, with con- 
sequent pain. | 

These results are shown in the following table : 


METHOD OF SUCCESSIVE CONTRASTS TO SHOW REVERSAL OF SENSATION 
UNDER POLAR CURRENTS 


Wound-spot anode Wound-spot kathode 
E.M.F. ‘5 volt _ E.M.F. 2 volts E.M.F. of ‘5 volt E.M.F. of 2 volt 
Intense pain Soothing Soothing Painful 


From these experiments, then, it will be seen that during 
the passage of the current, and when the E.M.F. is low, it 
is the anode which increases the excitability, and the 
kathode which depresses. Pfliiger’s Law is thus seen not 
to be universally applicable, but to be true only within 
certain limits, the very reverse of this law holding good, in 
the case of excessively high, and in that of low E.M.F. 
The demonstration which has just been given of the latter 
of these two facts, is independently borne out by the results 
of electrotonic variations of excitability in nerves, described 
in the last chapter, where we saw that, with moderately 
feeble E.M.F., excitability was enhanced by the anode and 
depressed by the kathode. 

It will thus be seen that polar variations ot excitability 
are not always the same, but differ in character, according as 
the intensity of the acting E.M.F. is moderate or low. The 
great significance of this fact is apparent, with regard to the 


586 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


medical application of electricity, since the failure to recog- 
nise that reversal of effects which is to be expected under a 
feeble E.M.F. might here lead to a result the very opposite 
of that intended. 

It has thus been shown that Pfliiger’s Law of the Polar 
Variation of Excitability is not universally applicable. It 
fails when the E.M.F. is either too high or too low, the 
effects observed under these circumstances being precisely. 
the opposite of those enunciated by Pfliiger. Under a low 
E.M.F. then, it is the anode which enhances the excitability, 
depression being induced by the kathode. This important 
fact, and the further fact that with low E.M F. conductivity 
is increased in the direction of the rising electrical potential, 
and depressed in that of the falling potential, will be found 
to explain all the varied electrotonic phenomena of nerves 
described in the previous chapter. 


SD ADM) 5 nent Bek eee 
» 


one A ae 


min em meee om mn = Aan «et i, 
A , - ” 


Aron 
rn 


te 


— pa a ae 
RA oes See ret YAR 0884 = 
\ i oe hl 


ar. ee 


aaa ae 


ore mete Me 
> cia. ae 


—— a 
Sree Syst? nah 
Wea 


SIL F wg 
& 


- 


CHAPTER XLI 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION AND ITS 
TRANSMISSION 


Two opposite responsive manifestations, negative and positive—Such opposite re- 
sponses induced by polar effects of currents of different signs—Arbitrary nature 
of term ‘excitatory ’—Pro-excitatory and anti-excitatory agents—Molecular 
distortion under magnetisation in magnetic substances—Different forms of re- 
sponse under magnetic stimulation—Mechanical, magneto-metric, and electro- 
motive responses— Uniform magnetic responses—Response exhibiting periodic 
groupings—Ineffective stimulus made effective by repetition—Response by 
resistivity-variation—Molecular model—Response of inorganic substance to 
electric radiation—Effect of rise of temperature in hastening period of re- 
covery and diminishing amplitude of response—Sign of response reversed under 
feeble stimulation—Conduction of magnetic excitation—The Magnetic Con- 

- ductivity Balance—Effect of A-tonus and K-tonus, on excitability and con- 
ductivity— Conducting path fashioned by stimulus—Transmission of excitation 


temporarily blocked in iron wire, as in conducting nerve—Artificial nerve- 
and-muscle preparation, 


IT is admitted that the excitation of living tissues is brought 
about by some kind of molecular disturbance, and that the 
passage of this molecular disturbance from point to point zs 
the transmission of excitation. As we do not possess the 
power of molecular vision we have perforce to be contented 
with the vagueness of the ideas which these terms connote, 
complicated as they are by the concomitance of other 
apparently mysterious properties of living tissues. If re- 
sponse and its variations, however, be in truth mainly de- 
pendent on the molecular condition of the tissue and its 
upset, then, from molecular considerations alone, it must 
be possible to explain why, under certain conditions, the 
responding substance is increased in excitability, and under 
others depressed. It has hitherto been found impossible to 
determine what is the nature of the antecedent molecular 


588 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


conditions to which these differences may be due: what it 
is that so determines the Zone, that the excitability of a tissue 
is made to undergo a profound change during the action of 
a particular stimulus or on its cessation; and what finally 
causes the fact that one identical stimulus, say that of 
tetanising shocks, will sometimes act to exalt, and at others 
to depress, the excitability of the same tissue. It is the 
caprice which has seemed to preside over these phenomena. 
that has forced observers upon the postulation of a hyper- 
physical ‘vital force. In the course of the present work, 
however, it has been shown that not only the simple pheno- 
mena of response, but all their complex variations also, are 
to be met with in the inorganic as in living matter, and that 
their explanation, therefore, must be sought for in the nature 
of antecedent molecular changes. As in the inorganic, the 
conditions of investigation are less complex than in living 
tissues, it follows that the study of molecular transformations 
and their after-effects there, is likely to throw much light 
upon that phenomenon of response which we have thus seen 
to be universal. 
| Taking first the response of living tissues, we find that 
the responsive change is of two kinds. This may be illus- 
trated by the following experiment, carried out on the 
pulvinus of Erythrina indica during the season of its greatest 
sensitiveness. The stimulus employed was that of a con- 
stant electrical current. When the upper half of the pul- 
vinus was made anode, response was found to take place by 
local expansion. This is seen in the up-record of figure 360. 
On the break of the anode, we observe a movement of 
recovery in the opposite direction. The pulvinus was next 
subjected to kathode-make, and we observe a_ responsive 
contraction. At kathode-break, however, we have a recovery 
by expansion. We have thus observed two opposite re- 
sponsive effects, according to the different polarities of the 
stimulating agent—namely, expansion at the make of anode, 
and contraction at that of kathode. 
Since responsive effects must be due to molecular upset, 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION 589 


or to new conditions of alignment, it is clear that contraction 
must be brought about by a one-directioned, and expansion 
by the opposite-directioned, change. This is evident in the 
present case, since the polar stimulating agents are opposite 
in their characters, and the opposition of their effects must 
correspond to this. Now it is necessary to distinguish these 
two responsive effects by opposite terms, which must needs 
be somewhat arbitrary. 
The contractile effect has 
thus been taken as the 
normal excitatory and 
negative. Having once 
adopted such a nomencla- 
ture, it is of course im- 
portant that it should be 
strictly adhered to. Thus, 
if contraction be the nor- 
mal response, then any- 


thing which tends to Up-curve represents expansion and con- 
vexity. Down-curve represents con- 


traction and concavity. Continuous 
curve represents the action at make. 


Fic. 360. Polar Effects of Currents due 
to Localised Application on Upper 
Half of Pulvinus of Zryihrina indica 


enhance it must be re- 


garded as excitatory, and 
anything which opposes or 
retards it as depressing. 


The dotted curve shows the effect at 
break. Am = convexity induced at 
anode-make. Ad = responsive con- 


cavity at anode-break. Km =induced 
concavity at kathode-make. Ké = 
expansion induced at kathode-break. 
The time-marks represent minutes. 


This word ‘depressing’ is, 
however, unfortunate, since 
by it might be indicated a 
permanent depreciation of the tissue, while diminution of 
the normal response is possible without such depreciation. 
Moderate rise of temperature, for example, with its 
expansive tendency, will lessen the contractile response 
without necessarily depreciating the tissue (p. 187). Revert- 
ing once more to the kathodic mode of stimulation, we 
know that a certain intensity of kathode is necessary, for 
the visible initiation of contractile response.’ Should the 
intensity employed be just short of this, there will be an 


1 These kathodic and anodic effects refer to the normal moderate range of 
E.M.F, within which Pfliiger’s Law is applicable. 


590 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


incipient molecular distortion, in the same direction as that 
which precipitates the excitatory response, hence kat-electro- 
tonus should prove to be excitatory. But a moderate 
anode, with its incipient molecular distortion in the opposite 
direction, will retard the normal response, and thus appear 
to be depressory. I must here point out that these terms 
excitatory and depressing have ordinarily speaking no ab- 


solute meaning, and can only acquire a definite significance 


when we have first fixed on that form of response which is 
to be regarded as normal. If, instead of contraction, we had 
regarded expansion as the normal response, then the effect 
of anode would have been regarded as excitatory, and that 
of kathode as depressing. We must therefore recognise that 
the very fact of contractile response being taken as excita- 
tory, entails as a consequence the designation of all agencies, 
such as K-tonus, which predispose the tissue to contraction, 
-as excitatory, or better pro-excitatory, while those which, like 
an-electrotonus, oppose this, must be regarded as depressory, 
or better anti-excitatory. 

From what has been said, it will be understood that it is 
the direction of the molecular derangement which determines 
‘the character of the response. That molecular upset, which 
expresses itself as excitatory contraction, we may call the 
K-effect, and the reversed molecular movement, expressed 
as expansion, the A-effect. ‘Thus, under anode, in fig. 360, 
the molecular distortion in one direction induces the expan- 
sive A-effect. On the cessation of this, the rebound of 
recovery causes a movement in the opposite direction, which 
may carry the molecules back, not merely as far as the 
equilibrium position, but beyond this. This movement, 
however, is in the same direction as that induced by K-make. 
Hence we may understand how excitation is caused, not only 
by K-make, but also by strong A-break. We may also 
understand how it is that the excitatory effect is much 


enhanced when A-break is immediately followed by K-make. 


We also see, in a general way, that a particular-directioned 
molecular movement would have the most intense excitatory 


Ss 


RO —— I enaataetiiar otal 
Se al ea 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION 591 


value when the molecular distortion was proceeding at a 
rapid rate, and not so much when a condition of permanent 
distortion had been attained. It is for this reason that, 
usually speaking, the excitatory effect is most pronounced 
at either kathode-made or anode-break,! and not so much 
during the continuation of kathodic action. 

To recapitulate some of the principal facts enumerated 
above, the term ‘excitatory’ being applied to a particular- 
directioned distortion or K-effect, then anything which 
induces an incipient molecular distortion in the same direc- 
tion, tending to aid the K-effect, and therefore to enhance 
that response, will be known as K-tonus. Anything, on the 
other hand, which induces an incipient distortion in the 
opposite direction, will oppose or retard the normal K-effect, 
and will, therefore, be known as inducing A-tonus. 

In the examples given, the opposite K- or A-effects - 
observed were the outward manifestations of the aggregate 
molecular effects induced. And from these we inferred the 
opposite-directioned changes which must have been their 
antecedent cause. In working with inorganic substances, 
however, and particularly in dealing with magnetic bodies, 
our power of molecular scrutiny is much keener. A rod of 
iron, for example, is known to consist of magnetic particles, 
each one of which is-a true magnet, possessed of polar 
properties. Under ordinary circumstances these magnetic 
molecules are in close chains, but under the action of 
magnetising forces they become distorted in a directive 
manner. Under north-magnetising force they are distorted 
in one direction, and under south polar induction in the 
reverse. The intensity of the induced magnetisation is a 
measure of the degree of molecular distortion, and can be 
gauged by the deflection of the freely suspended needle of a 
magnetometer in the neighbourhood. Increasing magnetising 
force is thus seen to induce greater magnetometric deflections, 

! It is conceivable that there should be occasions in which the final condition 
of distortion is not attained quickly, but slowly ; or where it is Huctuating instead 


of stable. Under such circumstances the excitation induced would be more or 
less persistent or tetanic. 


592 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


and on the cessation of the inducing force there is usually a 
molecular recovery, with a concomitant return of the magneto- 
metric indicator to its original position. 

Here, then, we have a means of recording the molecular 
distortions induced in a substance under a given external 
force. We are able also to study the relation between the 
acting force and the distortion induced, while it is increased 
or diminished in a known manner. And further, keeping 
the acting force the same, we are here able to study the 
effects of various modifying agents on the response, as 
recorded by the magnetometer, 

In all these cases, then, we have a strict parallel to the 
excitatory molecular changes and their variations induced in 
a living tissue under stimulus. But besides this local action 
we have also, in the living tissue, nerves possessing the 
property of transmitting the state of excitation—that is to 
say, the molecular disturbance—to a distance ; and this trans- 
mission is modified appropriately by the various modifica- 
tions which may be induced in the conducting nerve. Simi- 
larly I shall be able to show that, in an iron wire, excitatory 
magnetic disturbance is propagated to a distance; this con- 
duction likewise being modifiable by the molecular changes 
induced in the conducting wire. 

Thus, in those particular cases where molecular scrutiny 
is possible, we are enabled to visualise with considerable 
accuracy those molecular events on which excitation and its 
transmission depend. Afterwards, discarding this illustrative 
class of magnetic substances, I shall refer to other methods, 
by which the responsive manifestations of ordinary substances 
under stimulus, and the modifications of these responses 
under various conditions, will be recorded. From so compre- 
hensive a study we shall find that whatever be the mode ot 
record, and whatever the experimental substance employed, 
the fundamental reaction, and its variations under particular 
conditions, are curiously similar. It will then be realised 
that the response of living tissues is not alone of its own 
kind, but falls under a wide generalisation, 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION 593 


But before proceeding further with the magnetic responses, 
we must call to mind two different responsive manifestations 
of living tissues. We have observed these under the polar 
action of electric currents, one being the K- and the other the 
A-effect. Similarly in magnetic substances also, under the 
action of the magnetising forces, we observe two different 
effects brought about by opposite polar changes. One of 
these is the result of north and the other of south polar in- 
duction ; and of these, for the sake of convenience, we shall 
fix our attention on the effect induced by the north pole as 
the normal negative or K-effect. 

The fundamental molecular change induced may here, 
as in the case of living tissues, be recorded in various ways. 
In the present case, of the response of magnetic substances 
under magnetic stimulation, the methods of record may be 
classified as mechanical, magnetometric, and electro-motive. 
Joule discovered that a rod of soft iron undergoes a change 
of length on magnetisation. Though this variation is very 
small, I find it comparatively easy to demonstrate and record 
the responsive change concerned by means of the following 
device. One end of the iron rod is fixed, and the free end, 
carrying a wooden disc, rests on a tambour covered with 
stretched indiarubber. The tambour chamber is closed 
except at the point where a capillary tubing of glass enters 
it. This tube contains a short index. On now suddenly 
inducing magnetisation by a magnetising coil, the rod under- 
goes instantaneous elongation, and the resulting expulsion 
of air from the tambour causes a corresponding movement 
of the index outwards. Cessation of the magnetising current 
is attended by immediate recovery. It need only be men- 
tioned here that by making the diameter of the tambour 
sufficiently large, and that of the capillary tube sufficiently 
small, and by optically magnifying the movement of the 
index, it is easy to obtain for this mode of experiment a very 
high degree of sensitiveness. 5 

It is much easier, however, to record responsive molecular 
changes by the usual magnetometric, or by the induction or 


QQ 


594. COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


electro-motive method. According to the former of these, 
the magnetising coil, C, is placed broadside on, in reference 


Fic. 361. Experimental Arrangement 


for Magnetometric Method of Record Fic. 362. Photographic 

M, magnetometer ; C, magnetising coil ; Record of Uniform 

B, balancing coil; A, ammeter; -R, Magnetic Responses 
rheostat ; K, key actuated by metro- , of Iron 


nome. 


to a freely suspended magnetic needle with its attached 


mirror (fig. 361). 


Fic. 363. Photographic 
Record of Periodic 
Groupings in Mag- 
netic Responses 


A second balancing coil, B, is placed on 


the other side, and so adjusted as to 
nullify any disturbance of the needle 
by the magnetising coil. The experi- 
mental rod of iron is then introduced 
inside C, and the responsive molecular 
action induced by the exciting current 
is recorded in the usual manner by the 
deflected spot of light from the mag- 
netometer, M, thrown on a revolving 
drum. The intensity of the exciting 
current, measured by the ammeter, 4, 
is capable of adjustment by means of 
the rheostat, R. The duration of appli- 
cation of the exciting current is deter- 


mined by a metronome, and thus kept uniform in successive 
experiments. In fig. 362 is seen a series of records obtained 
in this manner, employing stimulation of moderate intensity. 
In fig. 363 is seen a curious instance of periodic groupings 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION 595 


in magnetic responses, similar to those obtained in living 
tissues. In the next figure (fig. 364) is shown the effect of 
strong stimulation, which gives rise to responses, not only 
of greater amplitude, but also of prolonged recovery. Under 
the strong stimulation here employed, owing to persistent 
molecular strain, the recovery did not become complete. 
This is analogous to the contracture in strongly excited 
-muscle. Such persistent strain may be removed by miole- 
cular vibration, the hastened recovery in the present record 

4 being the result of a tap. 
Magnetic stimuli, individually 
ineffective, become effective 
by repetition. In fig, 365 is 


Fic. 364. Photographic Record or 
Response and Recovery of Steel 
under Moderate and Strong Mag- 
netic Stimulus 


First pair of records show response and 


recovery under moderate stimulus. FIG. 365. spare sie ay Record 
In the next two, stronger stimulus sabes: neffective Stimulus 
induces response of greater ampli- made Effective by Repetition 


tude and incomplete recovery. 
Molecular vibration by tap, at point 
marked by down-arrow |, hastens 
recovery. 


Asingle brief magnetic stimulation 
induced little or no effect, but 
when rapidly repeated thirty 


times it became effective. 
seen a record of this. Tetanisation also induces the maxi- 
mum effect of fusion—as will be seen in the following chapter. 
Tetanisation, again, induces interesting after-effects in mag- 
netic responses, precisely the same as those seen in living 
tissues. Under certain conditions, moreover, to be fully 
described later, tetanisation, as we shall see, enhances the 
subsequent responses, while under. other conditions, by in- 
ducing fatigue, it brings about their depression. 

I have already mentioned the fact that in addition to 


QQ2 


596 . COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


the mechanical and magnetometric methods of studying 
response in magnetic substances, there is also a third .means 
available, in the Induction or Electro-motive Method, to be 
fully described at the end of the present chapter. 

I have now explained how the extent of molecular 
distortion induced in a magnetic substance by an external 
force can be gauged or measured by magnetometric or 
electric indications. For the detection of similar changes, 
however, in matter which is not pronouncedly magnetic, it 
is necessary to devise a method of record of more universal 
application. Such a method we have, as already said, in 
the record by resistivity-variation. It is here desirable, 
however, to give. a more detailed account of this and the 
principle involved. | | 

Our object being the detection of the molecular changes 
induced by stimulus, let us briefly consider certain well- 
known cases of molecular transformation induced by various 
stimulating agencies. Thus, when sulphur is subjected for 
a certain length of time to the action: of light, there is no 
visible sign of any change. Its solubility in carbon 
disulphide, however, has been altered, and we can dis- 
criminate-the portions acted upon from those unacted, by 
means of this ‘developing’ solution. But such discrimina- 
tion is only possible when the molecular or allotropic 
modification has gone so far as to be somewhat stable—that 
is to say, when the after-effect of stimulus is persistent. 
The development of any after-effect would have been 
impossible had the substance in the meanwhile exhibited 
self-recovery. Between the original condition A, again, and 
the terminal modification D, the substance must have passed 
through many gradations of condition, of a more or less 
impermanent stability. This case is analogous to that of a 
piece of iron under the action of magnetising forces, with 
their consequent molecular modifications. When the acting 
force is moderate, and the specimen has the power of self- 
recovery, the induced molecular distortion—that is to say, the 
induced ‘magnetisation—is fugitive, and there is no after- 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION 597 


effect on the cessation of the force. But, under intense 
magnetisation, the molecular transformation is more or less 
persistent, and we observe an after-effect in the induced 
permanent magnetisation. To revert here to the illustration 
of sulphur, it is only because the persistent terminal change 
is the most easily distinguishable that we single it out for the 
name of ‘allotropic change. As a matter of fact we see 
that this is but the climax of a series of changes, and so 
incomplete a view has been made current by the fact that 
we had no means of recording the intermediate changes 
while they were in progress. 

The next question is as to the possibility of making such 
records of molecular transformations, or of induced varia- 
tions in the state of molecular aggregation, while they are 
taking place. This may be accomplished, as I shall show, 
by the concomitant variation of electrical conductivity. It 
is to be borne in mind that the state of molecular aggrega- 
- tion plays an important part in determining the conductivity 
of a substance, and as an example we may take the case 
of carbon, which exhibits wide differences of conductivity in 
its two allotropic conditions of graphite and diamond. Let 
us imagine a piece of carbon in an intermediate or neutral 
state between these two. We-may suppose that an external 
force distorts it to a small extent towards the more con- 
ducting state of graphite. This distortion would be attended 
by an increase of conductivity, from which latter the extent 
of molecular distortion or upset involved might be inferred. 
Now, during the distortion from the equilibrium position, a 
force of restitution will tend to restore the carbon to its 
original neutral condition. If the’ distortion does not 
proceed beyond the elastic limit, then, on the cessation of 
the external stimulus, it will recover its original state, and 
this will be evidenced by the restoration of its original con- 
ductivity. But if the distortion be of a sub-permanent or 
permanent type, the recovery will be very much protracted, 
or will not take place at all. Such more or less permanent 
distortion, known as allotropic transformation due to stimulus 


598 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


of light, is seen in the production of red phosphorus from the 
yellow variety, and the insoluble from the soluble variety of 
sulphur. 

It will thus be seen that the conductive aspect of a given 
substance is not definite, but variable, the conductivity being 
dependent on the particular molecular condition of the sub- 
stance. This peculiarity may be represented in the accom- 
panying model (fig. 366), if we give the cylinder representing 
the sensitive molecule three main-conducting aspects A B C. 
The non-conducting aspect is represented by c. With the 
sensitive substance in this 
particular condition, inter- 
posed in the electric circuit, 
the current in the galvano- 
meter would be zero. A is 
the semi-conducting aspect 
of the substance, under 
which we may imagine the 
corresponding deflection of 
the galvanometer to be 50. 
B is the highly conducting 
aspect, the corresponding 
galvanometer reading being 
100. 

Fic. 366. Molecular Model The model representing 

the sensitive substance has 
its surface divided into six parts, the opposite sextants being 
put in electric communication. The opposite sextants CC 
are coated with shellac to represent the non-conducting 
aspect ; the sextants AA are coated with graphite to repre- 
sent the semi-conducting aspect; and the highly conducting 
aspect is represented by the sextants BB coated with tinfoil. 
The three main aspects of the sensitive substance are thus 
represented in the model; it is to be understood that with 
sensitive substances, under the action of stimulus, the transi- 
tion from one aspect to the next is gradual, and not abrupt, 
as represented. ‘The sensitive substance is interposed between 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION 599 — 


two electrodes. The torsion of the wire by which the 
cylinder is suspended represents the force of restitution. 
The galvanometer coil, by its deflections, exhibits indirectly 
the molecular strain produced in the substance by the action 
of stimulus. 

Let us suppose that we start with the substance in its 
normal state A, with moderate conductivity, and let the corre- 
sponding galvanometer deflection be 50. Let the substance 
belong to the negative class which exhibits an increase of 
conductivity, or diminution of resistance, under the action of 
stimulus. The stimulus will therefore distort the substance 
to a state of increased conductivity, the increased conductive 
aspect BB being brought opposite the electrodes. The 
enhanced current thus produced causes a deflection of, 
say, 100 in the galvanometer. If the strain has not been 
excessive, the substance will return, on the cessation of 
stimulus, to its original position of equilibrium, and the 
galvanometer deflection will fall from 100 to the original 
value 50. If the substance belong to the positive class, the 
distortion will be in the opposite direction, and the effect 
of stimulus will be to induce a responsive increase of 
resistance. 

The coil of the indicating galvanometer thus moves in 
perfect response to the varying molecular strain induced in 
the sensitive substance by the action of stimulus. The 
invisible molecular distortions are thus revealed by the 
visible deflections of the galvanometric indicator—the effect 
on one is merely the reflection of the effect on the other. 
A curve of the molecular effect, induced by the action of 
stimulus, may thus be obtained with the galvanometer de- 
flection as ordinate, and the time as abscissa. It is thus 
seen that these response-curves faithfully represent the in- 
visible molecular strain-effect due to the stimulus, and the 
subsequent recovery. = 

I shall now describe how in practice, by this method of 
resistivity variation, we obtain responses of various sub- 
stances to the stimulus of visible or invisible radiation. The 


600 COMPARATIVE EILECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


sensitive substance may be made the fourth arm of the 
Wheatstone’s bridge, and the responsive galvanometric de- 
flection and subsequent recovery of the spot of light—by 
the upsetting of the balance, under the action of stimulus 
of radiation—is recorded in the usual manner, on a moving 
photographic plate. Or the sensitive substance may be 
placed in series with a galvanometer, a small E.M.F. giving 
a steady permanent deflection. Taking first selenium as the. 
sensitive substance, the molecular change induced by the 
action of light, with its concomitant variation of resistance, 
causes a deflection of the galvanometer spot of light. On 
the cessation of the stimulus, molecular recovery takes place, 
and the deflected spot of light returns to its original position. 
A series of such responses will be found on referring to 
page 3, fig. 3. The parallel method employed in recording 
the responsive resistivity varia- 
tion of masses of metallic par- 
ticles of various kinds, under 
the stimulus of electric radia- 
: tion, will be understood from 
Fic. 367. Method of Resistivity "8+ 367. On obtaining records 
Variation of the responses given under 
Sensitive metallic particles placed in this method, I find, as I pointed 
tube in series with galvanometer 5 
and E.M.F. This gives a steady out in the first chapter, that 
permanent deflection. Stimulus the responding substances are 


‘of electric radiation induces a ; 
responsive variation of resistance of two different types. The 


with. concomitant variation of first, of which aluminium may 
galvanometric deflection. 
be taken as the example, re- 
spond by diminution, or negative variation of resistance. The 
second, illustrated by potassium or arsenic, respond by an in- 
crease, or positive variation of resistance. In living tissues 
also, tested by various modes of response, we have seen two 
opposite types to occur—highly excitable nerve giving one, 
say, negative, while skin, on the other hand, gave the positive. 
In the case of the inorganic substances referred to, we 
have extreme types, whose response is generally either 
positive or negative. There are, however, intermediate 


at ane a 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION 601 


cases, where it is liable to change of sign according as the 
stimulus is feeble or strong. Certain substances, again, 
cannot quickly recover from the after-effect of stimulus ; 
while, in others, recovery is fairly rapid. Recovery from 
intense stimulation is generally, other things being equal, 
more protracted than from feeble or moderate. Anything, 
however, which enhances molecular freedom or mobility will 
tend to hasten recovery. 

I shall now give several typical records in illustration 
of the peculiarities of this form of response by resistivity 
variation, under various conditions. The first example 


Fic. 368. Photographic Record of Response of Aluminium Powder in 
Sluggish.Condition to Stimulus of Electric-Radiation. . 


The first two responses exhibit incomplete recovery, which becomes com- 
plete on application of warmth. Note that warmth, increasing force 
of recovery, hastens recovery and also diminishes amplitude of 
response, as seen in the two succeeding records. 


given, that of aluminium powder, will» be of the negative 
type, the response being by diminution of resistance. When 
the substance tested happens to be in a sluggish condition, 
recovery is very protracted. There is then a _ response- 
remainder of persistent negative variation, corresponding to 
the contraction-remainder in muscle, or persistent electro- 
motive negativity in other living tissues. But we know that 
a moderate rise of temperature is favourable to recovery ; 
and on applying gentle heat, at the end of the second 
response, with its incomplete recovery, the persistent effect is 
seen to be removed, and -there is an immediate completion of 
recovery (fig. 368). 

This is the place to refer once more to an apparent 


602 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


anomaly, in the response of living tissues, where a slight rise 
of temperature increases conductivity, at the same time that 
it appears to diminish excitability, inasmuch as it brings 
about a lessened amplitude of response (Chapter XV.). 
This latter, however, may not really be due to diminution of 
excitability, since the same effect might equally well be 
brought about by an enhancement of the force of recovery. 
This view is supported by the further records given in 
fig. 368. We see here that incomplete recovery became 
complete, under the application of gentle heat. The next 
response given by this slightly warmed substance is seen to 


Fic. 369. Photographic Record Showing Uniform Response of Alu- 
miniun Powder to Uniform Stimulus of Electric Radiation. 


show complete recovery within a relatively short time, this 
enhanced force of recovery bringing about at the same time 
a diminution in the height of response. The temperature of 
the substance was now again raised to a slightly higher 
degree, and the next response shows a still further diminished 
height and a considerably quickened recovery. When a 
substance is in a normal condition of excitability, its succes- 
sive responses to uniform stimuli are found to exhibit com- 
plete recovery, and to be of equal amplitude. Figure 369 
shows such a series obtained with powdered aluminium. 

We have seen that the increased molecular mobility con- 
ferred by warming hastens recovery. A similar hastening of 
recovery may be brought about by a mechanical tap, as has 
already been shown (fig. 364) in the case of magnetic response. 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION 603 


In fig. 370 is seen an example of the same thing in tungsten, 
where recovery from the effect of electric radiation is hastened 
by a tap. 

It has been shown that the normal response by negativity 
in living tissues is liable to reversal under very feeble 
stimulation. This is better observed when the tissue is 
not highly excitable; because in this case it is easy to 
adjust the intensity of stimulation, so as to fall below the 
critical value for excitation. 
It is very interesting to ob- 
serve similar opposed effects, 
under feeble and moderately 
strong stimulations, in the re- 
sponse of inorganic substances. 
For the reason just mentioned, 
it is desirable to select a sub- 
stance for this purpose, which 
possesses a moderate degree 
of sensibility. Using a mass 
of tungsten particles, I found 
that under strong intensity 
of electric radiation—-brought 
about by placing the radiator 
within a short distance of the Fic. 370. Photographic Record 


of Response of Tungsten 
substance—the response was ; : 
The incomplete recovery is hastened 


by the normal negative varia- by application of tap at points, 
tion, or diminution of resist- marked with downward arrow. 
Cf. fig. 364. 


ance. But when the intensity 
of stimulus was dimfnished by placing the radiator at a 
greater distance, then the response was converted to positive. 
A record of this abnormal effect under feeble stimulation will 
be given later. 

Thus, having observed molecular response and its varia- 
tions by the Magnetic and Resistivity Methods of record, we 
now proceed to study the transmission of the state of 
excitation. We have seen that the essential condition of the 
transmission of excitation in living tissues lies in the 


604 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


propagation of molecular disturbance from point to point. 
The characteristics of such propagation must be—(1) that the 
transmitted molecular disturbance becomes enfeebled with 
distance, so that at a certain point the transmitted excitation 
would be reduced to zero; (2) that while a moderate 
stimulus is transmitted to a short distance, a stronger 
stimulus would be carried further ; and (3) that the intensity 
of excitation transmitted would depend on the conducting 
power of the intervening tract, this conductivity being 
capable of enhancement by certain agencies, and depressible 
by others. 

I shall now proceed to show that in an iron wire a 
transmission of molecular disturbance takes place which is 


Fic. 371. Experimental Arrangement for obtaining Response in Iron by 
Induction Current 


similar to that at the basis of the transmission of excitatory 
changes, both, as I shall show, being modifiable by similar 
circumstances in a similar manner. For these investigations 
I have employed the Induction or Electro-motive Method of 
observation. In the experimental arrangement—a diagram- 
matic representation of which is shown in fig. 371—S is 
the stimulating or exciting coil appliéd at the point to be 
excited. The conducting region intervenes between S and R, 
which is the responding point, over which is wound the 
receiving coil, placed in series with either a telephone or a 
galvanometer. When the excitatory molecular disturbance 
reaches R, it gives rise to an induction current in the coil, 
which in turn causes a sound in the telephone, or a 
responsive deflection in the galvanometer. For the purpose 
of simplicity, we shall take north polar or K-excitation as 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION 605 


normal, and the resulting deflection of the galvanometer to 
the right as the normal response. ~The direct effect of the 
coil S on the coil R may be regarded as negligible, when 
they are separated from each other by a sufficient distance, 
and this would be even more true if the intervening iron wire 
were bent at an angle of 90 degrees. 

Employing this mode of obtaining records of response to 
K- or.A-excitation, we meet with several curious analogies 
to the responsive effects seen in living tissues, under the 
electrical mode of stimulation. Electrical excitation of 
nerve and muscle, for example, is most effective when it is 
longitudinal, and ineffective when transverse. The same is 
true of magnetic excitation of iron, where longitudinal 
excitation is effectively transmitted to a great distance, 
whereas transverse excitation is relatively ineffective. 

Again, in the case of the electrical excitation of living 
tissues, it is at the instant of kathode-make, as we have seen, 
that excitation is induced. Continued action exhibits in 


general no effect. The same normal excitatory effect seen 


at kathode-make, is induced again, but at anode-break. 
Similarly, in the case of an iron wire, the normal galvano- 
metric response is seen at the moment of K-magnetic 
excitation, but not during its continuance. The same 
excitation is also obtained, at break of A, or south polar 
magnetisation. . 

We are led from such close analogies, not only to visualise, 
but also to obtain some insight into the sequence of 
molecular events which is the concomitant of excitation. 
I have already pointed out that excitation and its opposite, 
depression, being phenomena of molecular. distortion, it 
is to be expected that a particular-directioned distortional 
movement should be associated with one of these, and the 
opposite with the other. We also know the further 
suggestive fact that it is the sudden change of the environ- 
ment, inducing a sudden responsive molecular disturbance, 
that is most effective in bringing about excitation. The 
latent period, and a slowly-rising excitation, correspond to the 


606 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


slow initiation of the molecular upset. After this the rate of 
molecular distortion will be rapid, and in this second period 
we find that the excitatory reaction also is at its maximum. 
In any case, it is rather during the period of increasing 
molecular distortion that we should expect to see the 
most intense excitation, than when a static condition of 
derangement has been attained. Thus it is at the moment 
of K-make that we obtain the excitatory indication, and not 
afterwards, when the molecules are being maintained in the 
distorted position. , 

Returning once more to the iron wire, we find that when 
the distorted molecules have been set free by the break of kK, 
there is a sudden movement of recovery in the opposite 
direction. If now the K-effect, with its particular-directioned 
molecular movement, be termed the excitatory, then the oppo- 
site movement must be regarded as one of depression, and it 
is interesting to note that in a living tissue there is an after- 
effect of depression at kathode-break. The anode-make, on 
the other hand, with its opposite molecular distortion, is, as 
' one would expect, depressory. But at the break, the 
direction of the rebound of the released molecules being the 
same as that brought about by K-make, must be excitatory. 
The close parallelism which we have thus traced out, forces 
upon us the conclusion that the molecular actions which 
underlie the excitation of living tissues may be-polar in their 
character. 

The fact that magnetic excitation undergoes diminution 
during transmission, can be shown by moving the receiving 
coil R further and further away from S, when the responsive 
sound in the telephone, or deflection in the galvanometer, will 
be found to undergo a graduated diminution, till, with a given 
stimulus, the effect, from being considerable, is reduced at a 
certain distance to zz/, Keeping this distance the same, 
however, a stronger stimulus will be found efficient to evoke 
response, and the responding coil will now have to be moved 
further, in order again to reduce the response to zero. 

We shall next study the variation of conductivity induced 


-_. = 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION 607 


by an external agent, as. modifying the intensity of the 
transmitted effect. In order to study the phenomenon of 
conduction and its modification, as will be remembered, a 
delicate form of Conductivity Balance, fully described in 
Chapter XX XIII. was used. Excitation was here caused by 
S at a middle point, the transmitted excitatory effects at E’ 
and E being made to balance, This condition of balance 
was obtained when one arm, say the left E’, was kept at a 
fixed distance from Ss, and the other, or right, was moved 
towards S, or away from it, as required. When E was too far 
from S the excitatory effect would be smaller than at E’, and 
this under-balance would be indicated by a response, say 
downwards. When, again, E was too near to S, there would 
be an over-balance, the resultant response being upwards. 
Between these could be found a point of exact balance 
where the record was horizontal (cf figs. 289, 290). 

A high degree of delicacy in the study of: similar 
phenomena in the case of iron wires may be obtained by 


Fic. 372. Magnetic Conductivity Balance 


S, magnetising coil, by which north-polar or K-impulses are sent out in 
two directions as shown by arrows. E BE’, receiving coils, adjusted 
at balance. M, permanent magnet, by ,which either A- or K-tonus 
is induced at the responding ends of the iron rod. T, tonic coil, 
by which A- or K-tonic molecular dispositions may be induced in 
one arm of the balance. 


the employment of the Magnetic Conductivity Balance 
(fig. 372), which I shall now describe. The magnetic 
stimulator, S, consists of a pair of similar coils wound in 
opposite directions, When a magnetising current is suddenly 


608 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


sent through these two coils, in a proper direction, two equal 
north-polar impulses will be generated simultaneously, and 
travel, one to the right, towards E, and the other to the left, 
towards E’. [n order to obtain a balance of the excitatory 
effects at E and E’, we keep E’ at a fixed distance, and move 
E backwards and forwards till the balance is found. This 
process of balancing will be found graphically illustrated in 
the records given in fig. 373. E was placed at first too near 
to S, and the over-balance is seen as up-responses. The coil 
was then moved away very gradually, and the response of 
over-balance is seen at each step to undergo a diminution or 
approach towards balance. We next note the attainment 
of exact balance, where the record is seen to be horizontal. 
The coil is now moved still further to the right, and the con- 


Fic, 373. Process of Balancing illustrated by Photographic Record 
| of Responses 


sequent increasing under-balance is exhibited by the gradually 
increasing reversed down-responses. 

Having thus obtained balance, we are able to record the 
variations induced in conductivity by a given agent. This 
is applied on the right arm of the balance, the subsequent 
upset of which, in one direction or the other, indicates the 
enhancement or depression of conductivity. Resulting up- 
responses will indicate enhancement, and down-responses 
depression. A well-known agent for the enhancement or 
depression of the conductivity of the nerve is the polar 
action of the kathode and anode. Moderate kat-electro- 
tonus enhances conductivity, whereas the anode depresses 
or inhibits it. The explanation which I have already offered, 
regarding anodic and kathodic effects on excitability, will 
also. be found applicable in the case of conductivity. An 
excitatory or kathodic effect will be facilitated in trans- 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION 609 


mission, if the molecules in its path are already incipiently 
orientated, so that the incident stimulus finds them pre- 
disposed to respond in that direction and to transmit the 
excitation. Hence the kathodic effect is more easily trans- 
mitted through a tract which is in a state of K-tonus, 
whereas it is retarded or inhibited under A-tonus. We shall 
now study the corresponding effect in magnetic conduction. 
Normal excitation in these experiments, it should be remem- 
bered, is taken as that which is brought about by the north- 
polar or K-effect. If there is a tonic coil, T, surrounding one 
arm of the balance, then, by sending a permanent current of 
moderate intensity round the coil, in one direction or the 
other, we may induce at will, in that arm, either K-tonus 
or A-tonus. The molecular disposition induced by the 
stimulus, and by K- and A-tonus respectively, will be under- 
stood from the diagrammatic representation given in fig. 372. 
Local variation of excitability at E may be induced by 
bringing near to’it either the north 
or south pole of a permanent 
magnet M. 

I shall now exhibit the enhance- 
ment or depression of magnetic 
conductivity by K- or A-tonus. A 
balanced record is first obtained, 


Fic, 374. Effect of K- and 


and K-tonus then induced in the A-Tonus on Magnetic Con- 
right arm. Successive K-make ex- duction 
aes . . The first series exhibit by 
citations are now applied, starting resultant over-balance up- 
from the centre of the balance at wards the effect of K-tonus 
‘ ; -% in enhancing ‘conductivity 
S, and proceeding onwards to left of right arm, The next 
and right simultaneously. The — series, with resultant 
down-responses, show de- 
resulting responses are recorded, pression by A-tonus. 


records of the break-effect being 

avoided by timely interruptions of the galvanometer-circuit. 

It will be seen (fig. 374) from the upsetting of the balance in 

an upward direction, that K-tonus has induced enhancement 

of conductivity in the right arm. On now causing A-tonus, 

by reversing the current in the enclosing tonic coil, T, a de- 
¢ RR 


610 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


pression of conduction is induced, as shown by the upsetting 
of the balance in a downward direction. 

- We next turn to the question of the variation of conduc- 
tivity induced by K-tonus, when moderate or excessively 
strong ; and it is here important to forecast from theoretical 
considerations what is to be expected under varying intensities 
of the polarising force. It is easy to understand that moderate 
K-tonus, inducing an incipient orientation of the molecules, 
will predispose them to easy upset in a particular direction, 
thus greatly facilitating the transmission of excitation from 
point to point. Thus a moderate K-tonus will enhance con- 
ductivity. But if the K-tonus in question be excessive, so that 
the molecules are already distorted to their maximum position, 
incident stimulus can then induce no further change, and under 
such circumstances there can be little transmission. Hence, 
under increasing intensity of K-tonus, we may expect to obtain 
increasing conductivity up to a certain point. But, beyond 
this, the conductivity will be decreased, or even actually 
inhibited. 

These anticipations are seen fully verified in the accom- 
panying record (fig. 375), which shows the opposite effects 
on conductivity of moderate 
and strong K-tonus. The 
upsetting of the balance 
in an upward direction, 
K, shows the effect of 
moderate K-tonus. Strong 
K-tonus was next applied, 
with the effect of upsetting 
the balance in the opposite 
direction, K’. Thus we see that, while under moderate 
K-tonus the conductivity is enhanced, under a much greater 
intensity it becomes depressed. This will, I think, be found 
to explain a somewhat anomalous occurrence, which has 
been observed in regard to the conduction of excitation 
through a kathodic region in nerve-and-muscle preparation. 
A stimulus applied on the extra-polar region in nerve is 


Fic. 375. Opposite Effects of kK-Tonus 
when moderate and strong 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION OI 


found tobe transmitted through the kathodic area, inducing 
enhanced response of the indicating muscle, if the polarising 
current be weak. But when the intensity of the kathode is 
made stronger, even the strongest stimulus will fail to induce 
response. This is evidently due to the fact that a strong 
kathode induces a depression or abolition of conductivity. 

Moderate K-tonus, then; we have seen to induce enhanced 
conductivity, because of the favourable molecular disposition 
which it brings about. Even on the cessation of K-tonus 
this disposition remains, owing to molecular ‘ retentiveness, 
with its concomitant enhanced conductivity as an after- 
effect. This induction of a favourable molecular disposition 
or habit is an interesting phenomenon, which we shall meet 
with again. : 

We shall next study the enhancement or depression of local 
excitability by K- or A-tonus. We saw, in Chapter XXXIIL, 
that by means of the Con- 
ductivity Balance we might 
determine the variations, not 
only of conductivity, but also 
of local excitability. In mag- 
netic experiments the responsive 
area at the right-hand end of 
the balance may be made either 
K-tonic or A-tonic, by bringing 
near it one or other pole of 
a permanent magnet. Under 
induced A-tonus, the molecular 
excitability is depressed, and 


Fic. 376. Effects of K- and 


the balance upset in a down- A-Tonus on Magnetic Ex- 
. ‘ ae citability 
ward direction; while under 
4 Ae From the resulting upset of the 
K-tonus excitability is enhanced, balance, A is seen to induce de- 


the resulting response being up- FF coun SNR EETER Es 
wards (fig. 376). 

A still more interesting case is that in which the stimulus 
itself fashions,:as it were, the path for its own conduction. 
The receiving coil is placed at such a distance from s 


RR2 


612 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


(fig. 371) that, owing to imperfect conductivity of the inter- 
vening tract, but little excitation reaches it. Excitation at S, 
however, distorts the molecules in its immediate neighbour- 
hood, in a certain direction, incipiently distorting others at a 
little greater distance in the same favourable way. A second 
stimulus is therefore transmitted a little further, bringing 
about the same predisposition still further on, Thus an im- 


proved conducting-path is made, in a substance formerly but. 


an indifferent conductor, by the action of the stimulus itself. 
In this way transmitted excitation, at first relatively ineffec- 
tive, becomes increasingly effective (fig. 377). It is very 
interesting to note that I have obtained an effect exactly 
parallel in the case of nervous tissues. 
For example, when attempting to 
obtain the transmitted effect of ex- 
citation by mechanical response, in 
a vegetable or animal nerve in de- 
pressed tonic condition, the first series 
Fic. 377. Gradual Enhance- Of tetanising shocks would induce no 
RED oe Caen response. It would sometimes be 
} only after long repetition that con- 
ductivity would be gradually restored, as seen in the initiation 
and subsequent enhancement of responses given at the distant 
responding-point: 
- It may be that few phenomena connected with the 
response of living tissues, bring home to us, so effectively 
aS an experiment on a nerve-and-muscle preparation, the 
sense of the specific and mysterious character of the 
responsive manifestations of the living. The nerve, at its 
central termination, is locally excited by electric shocks, 
and some obscure impulse then passes through the long 
conducting tract to the muscle at the other end. Arriving 
there, this invisible nervous impulse initiates a new Series 
of events, which find expression in visible motile indications 
The work performed at the responding end may be out of 
all proportion to the strength of the stimulus imparted at the 
centre. It is as if the nervous impulse tapped a relay, and 


a 


AR ee php ee EER Bee ae ee eT ee Ne ae a 


rine nerf 
mo 


In pa ym ae | 


eau 


THE MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION 613 


set free a local store of latent energy. The conductor, 
moreover, is seemingly unlike the conductor in an electrical 
circuit, where the line wire and return wire must be 
periodically connected with terminals of an electro-motive 
source, for any message to be transmitted. In the nerve 
we have only a single conductor, without a return, an 
arrangement by which it would appear as difficult to send 
a message, as it would be to apply the two poles of a 
battery at the end of a single wire, in the expectation of 
a signal from the recorder at the far end. Inorganic matter 
again, is popularly regarded as susceptible only of impulses 
from the grosser physical forces, while the nerve—the 
vehicle of psychic impulses—is conceived of as_ played 
upon by forces of a finer order, ‘and as itself modifiable, 
by subtler influences, notably that of its own previous 
history,or memory. There are, as we know, some conditions 
which induce such changes in the nervous channel itself, 
that messages from outside, previously scarcely perceptible, 
are accentuated. Under opposite influences, again, the 
conduction of impulse is interrupted. Similar results are 
brought about by certain agents of a polar character, like 
the action of anode and kathode. Under electrotonic action 
the transmission of impulses through the nerve may be 
blocked, conduction being renewed as soon as the electric 
block is removed. Or electrotonic action, again, may be 
used for the opposite purpose, of accelerating the trans- 
mission of impulses. Nothing more convincing than such 
facts could have been urged in support of the hyper-physical 
character of the phenomena in question. 

But the experiments which I have described, relating 
to the conduction of excitatory molecular changes in a 
piece of iron wire, show that parallel phenomena occur in the 
physical domain also; and in order to demonstrate this in 
a striking manner, I cannot do better than describe an 
arrangement which I have devised, and which may be 
taken as an artificial nerve-and-muscle preparation. This 
consists of a thin iron rod for the transmission of magnetic 


614 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY = - 


excitation applied at one end, with a responding arrange- 
ment, to give motile indications, at the other. This latter 
consists of a secondary coil, which may be slipped over the 
responding point, being in series with some sensitive metallic 
powder in circuit with a galvanic recorder, and a voltaic 
cell as source of energy. The excitatory molecular dis- 
turbance transmitted through the conducting iron rod gives 
rise, on reaching the responding-point, to an_ electrical 
disturbance in the secondary coil connected with the motile 
indicator. This electrical disturbance causes secondary 
excitation of the sensitive substance, in consequence of 
which the electric conductivity of the particles becomes 
suddenly enhanced. By this ‘relay’ action the stored-up 
energy of the cell is suddenly released, with a consequent 
‘induction of motile response in the galvanic recorder. It 
is thus seen that this motile response, initiated by the 
transmitted stimulus, need not be proportionate to its 
primary exciting cause, since it may possibly be much en- 
hanced by the amount of energy set free in the responding 
circuit itself. This transmission of excitation is liable, 
moreover, as in nerve, to be modified by subtle molecular 
changes induced in the conducting tract through which it 
takes place. Excitation may be arrested in the one case 
by an electrical block ; and in the other, similarly, we are 
able to stop the transmission of a message, by means of a 
magnetic block. It is by no gross physical restraint that 
the impulse is so arrested, but by invisible molecular 
distortion within the rod. Molecular freedom is next re- 
stored by the removal of- the magnetic block, and we find 
that the message, which, though constantly reiterated, was 
hitherto inhibited, is suddenly allowed to rush onwards 
and bring about the signal. ; 


CHAPTER XLII os 


MODIFICATION OF RESPONSE UNDER CYCLIC 
MOLECULAR VARIATION . 


Anomalies of response—Explicable only from consideration of antecedent 
molecular changes— Continuous transformation from sub-tonic to hyper-tonic 
conditions—Two methods of inquiry, first by means of characteristic curves, 
second by progressive change of response—Abnormal response characteristic 
generally of A or sub-tonic state—Abnormal transformed into normal, after 

_ transitional B state—B state characterised by staircase response—Responses at 
C stage normal and uniform—aAt stages D and E responses undergo diminution 
and reversal— Responsive peculiarities seen during ascent of curve, repeated 
in reverse order during descent—All these peculiarities seen not only in living 
but also in inorganic substances, under different methods of observation— 
Elucidation of effect of drugs—Response modified by tonic condition and past 
history. 


WE have seen that, normally, the phenomenon of response 
in living tissues is very definite’ There are other con- 
ditions, however, under which it is found to be modified or 
even reversed. These abnormal effects may be brought 
about, either by feeble stimulation, or by changes in the 
responding tissue itself. Thus, though moderate stimulus 
evokes normal negative response, a feeble stimulus will often 
be seen to induce the abnormal positive, and this is most 
easily observed in certain particular modifications of the 
tissue associated with sub-tonicity. The fatigue-changes 
due to excess of stimulation are also, curiously enough, 
effective in bringing about the same abnormalities of response. 

It is open to-us to regard these anomalies as the result of 
obscure vital actions, and therefore incapable of further 
analysis. Or,since the phenomenon of response itself is 
admitted to be due to the molecular upset caused by 
stimulus, their origin may be looked for in the antecedent 


616 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


molecular condition of the responding substance. There is 
a school of investigators, again, who, appearing to discard the 
theory of vital action, in accounting for these changes, have 
substituted for it the hypothetical anabolic, or up-building, 
and catabolic, or down-breaking, chemical changes. And 
such assumptions have certainly the advantage of meeting 
every emergency, whether it be an expected effect or its 
direct opposite which occurs, for by their means it is always _ 
possible to make a reference to the one process or the other, 
whatever be the inconsistency involved. 

- As regards the interminable controversy on the physical 
versus chemical nature of response-phenomena, I have 
already drawn attention to the fact that on the border-line 
between Physics and Chemistry it is impossible to make any 
sharp demarcation. Changes, in themselves undoubtedly 
molecular or physical, may be attended by concomitant 
changes of chemical activity. An example will perhaps 
make this clear. We may take, for instance, the photo- 
graphic action of light on a sensitive plate. This is re- 
garded as due to chemical dissociation or break-down. If 
this were so, however, the effect would be permanent. But, 
instead of this, the latent image is liable to disappear, and in 
a Daguerreotype plate the after-effect of light—that is to say, 
the persistence of the image—has a duration of a few hours 
only. Such images, moreover, due to the action of light, 
have been found to form themselves even on elementary and 
inert chemical substances like gold. Here, any chemical 
break-down, in the ordinary sense of the word, is out of the 
question. 

Stimulus in general we have seen to induce molecular 
distortion, the persistence of which is dependent on the 
strength of the stimulus, and also on the power of self- 
recovery characteristic of the given substance. We have 
further seen a difference of electrical potential to be induced, 
as between molecularly strained and unstrained areas. 
When the substance, therefore, thus differentially acted upon, 
is placed in a suitable electrolyte, volta-chemical actions are 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR VARIATION 617 


necessarily set up, by which material in one part may be 
accreted, and in another dissolved. In this way a positive 
or negative image may be developed. 

We have also seen, in the responses of living tissues, that 
while moderate stimulation induces one effect, the same 
stimulation, long continued, may cause the so-called fatigue- 
reversal, such reversals sometimes, in fact, becoming 
recurrent. It is interesting to note that in a similar fashion 
a photographic plate, subjected to various durations of 
exposure, will give either negative or reversed positive 
images, or recurrences of these." : 

From such facts it is clear that for the elucidation of 
response and its variations, we must look to its molecular 
antecedents, and not to its secondary chemical or other 
consequences. If response phenomena in general, then, are 
determined by molecular conditions, as such, it follows that 
in order to unravel the anomalies which occur in - the 
response of living tissues, we must attempt to ascertain those 
conditions which induce any given variation of response in 
matter in general. That these phenomena are not peculiar 
to the response of living tissues, but take place in all matter 
under similar circumstances, is a fact which has been often 
reiterated in the course of previous chapters, and which I 
first pointed out in the course of my investigations on 
‘Response in the Living and Non-Living,’ ? 

In the work in question, referring to the occurrence of 
abnormalities in response, I said : 

‘Calling a// normal response negative, for the sake of 
convenience, we observe its gradual modification, correspond- 
ing to changes in the molecular condition of the substance. 
Beginning with that case in which molecular modification is 
extreme, we find a maximum variation of response from the 
normal, that is to say, to positive. Continued stimulation, 
however, brings the molecular condition to normal, as 


* Bose, ‘On Strain Theory of Photographic Action,’. Proc. Roy. Soc. 
1902. 
See Response in the Living and Non-Living (1902), pp. 129, 130. 


618 COMPARATIVE -ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


evidenced by the progressive lessening of the positive 
response, culminating in- reversion ‘to the normal negative. 
This is equally true of nerve and metal. In the next class 
of phenomena the modification of molecular condition is not 
so great. It now exhibits itself merely as relative inertness, 
and the responses, though positive, are feeble. Under 
continued stimulation, they increasé in the same direction as 
in the last case—that is to say, from less negative to more 
negative, this being the reverse of fatigue. This is evidenced 
alike by the staircase effect and by the increase of response. 
after tetanisation, seen, not. only in nerve, but also in 
platinum and tin. The substance may next be in what we 
call the normal condition. Successive uniform stimuli now 
evoke uniform and equal negative responses—that is to say, 
there is no fatigue. But after intense or long-continued 
stimulation, the substance is overstrained. The responses 
now undergo a change from -negative to Jess negative: 
fatigue, that is to say, appears. Again, under very much 
prolonged stimulation, the response may decline to zero, or 
even undergo a reversal to positive, a phenomenon which we 
shall find instanced in-the reversed response of retina, under 
the long continued stimulus of light. 

‘We must, then, recognise that a substance may exist in 
various molecular conditions, whether due to internal changes 
or to the action of stimulus. .The responses give us indica- 
tions of these conditions. A complete cycle of molecular 
modifications can be traced, from the abnormal positive to 
the normal negative, and then again to positive, seen in 
reversal under continuous stimulation.’ ! 

It is the molecular cycle here referred to, with the con- 
comitant cyclic variation of response, that forms the subject 
of the present chapter.. I shall attempt to show that the 
various anomalies in-the response of living tissues, which 
were referred to in an earlier passage, may be elucidated 


' In the above quotation I have, in accordance -with the convention which 
I now uniformly oe referred to normal response as negative, and abnormal 
as positive.—/. C. B _ 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR:VARIATION  — 619 


by this consideration. I explained, im the first chapter of 
the present work, the fact that the molecular derangement 
of matter under stimulus might be studied by recording 
any one of several concomitant physical changes. -These 
are: (a) the change of form—contraction or expansion ; 
(3) the electro-motive change ; and (c) the variation of electric 
resistivity. By means of the first of these we investigated 
the responsive effects induced by stimulus in animal and 
vegetable tissues, and in the inorganic substance indiarubber. 
By the second, that of electro-motive variation, the excitatory 
change and its variations were studied in living tissues, animal 
and vegetable, and in inorganic bodies like metal wires. 
And, lastly, by variation of resistivity, we have obtained 
records of excitatory changes in living tissues, as also in 
masses consisting of metallic particles. In the last chapter, 
moreover, I have shown that the molecular responses of a 
magnetic substance may be recorded by means of appropriate 
magnetometric or galvanometric methods. 

I shall now take up the question of the nature of those 
obscure molecular modifications which the response of a 
substance is found to undergo, and in consequence of which 
it exhibits variations either of intensity or of sign. The 
only conceivable reason for such changes would lie in some 

unknown transformation of the antecedent molecular con- 
dition. This being so, the next question is, whether we could 
possibly discover what these transformations are. 

The properties of a substance at any given moment, we 
must remember, are not determined solely by the nature of 
that substance, but also by the energy which it possesses. 
It is obvious, for instance, that the responsive properties of 
matter, when its energy is depleted or its condition-is a-tonic, 
will be different from those of matter in a higher tonic-con- 
dition, and that there will be many gradations intermediate 
between the two. Thus, as a substance is gradually trans- 
formed, from a state of depletion to one of excessive energy, 
we~can see that, theoretically, there -might - be two possible 
ways of obtaining an insight into the progressive molecular 


620 ' COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


changes occurring in it. First of these would be the con- 
tinuous observation of the character of the replies made by 
the changing substance to the shock of stimulus, with the 
progressive modification of those replies. And the second 
method would lie in taking a continuous record of some 
property of the substance, as a whole, which was undergoing 
a concomitant change. In the first of these modes of 
scrutiny the information would be obtained by an inspec- 
tion of the varying responses. In the second, it would be 
arrived at by the examination of certain characteristic curves ; 


z 
= 
fe es 
+ 
oR) 
eS 
wi 
ra 
a) 
< 
= 


MAGNETISING FORCE 


Fic. 378. Characteristic Curve of Iron under increasing Force of 
Magnetisation 


and finally, if both these methods gave correct indications of 
the molecular state at the time being, then each particular 
response of the first method would be found to have its 
own place in the characteristic curve of the second. This 
characteristic curve will be best understood from a con- 
tinuous record of the induced molecular change occurring 
under the action of an increasing external force. The 
simplest example of this is afforded by the curve which 
shows the relation between induced magnetisation and 
inducing magnetic force (fig. 378). This induced magne- 
tisation, as will be understood, measures the amount of 
molecular distortion. A characteristic curve, essentially 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR VARIATION 621 


similar, is obtained from filings of a substance belonging 
to the negative class under increasing electro-motive force 
(fig. 379). Taking first the substance in a low or indifferent 
condition, we find the curve in its earliest stage, A, to be 
almost horizontal. That is to say, the molecular distortion 
induced is here very slight. We next arrive, however, at a 
stage, B, which I shall call transitional, where increasing 
force induces change at a rapid rate. In the third stage, 
subsequent to this, there is a decline in the rate of change, 
the molecules now approaching their maximum distortion, 


fa 
Pa 
Lid 
A 
a 
a 
O 


ELECTROMOTIVE FORCE 


Fic. 379. Characteristic Conductivity Curve of Sensitive Metallic 
Particles belonging to Negative Class, under increasing Electro- 
motive Force 


These principal features are common to characteristic curves 
in general, slight deviations from the type being met with 
occasionally. In the cases given, for example, the substance 
starts from an indifferent condition. But it might have been 
in a still lower, or a-tonic, condition at starting. Under such 
circumstances I find that the tendency of the first part of 
the curve is to fall below the zero-line, crossing it, however, 
in an upward direction, at the transitional point B. When 
the curve, again, has reached the highest point, c, it may 
remain horizontal for a considerable time, or there may be a 
decline, owing, as we shall see, to fatigue. 


622 - COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


If we take a cyclic curve, recording the effects. under 
increasing, followed by those under diminishing, force, it will 
be found that the forward and return portions of the curve do 
not in general coincide (fig: 380 a). Thus, when an increasing 
magnetising force starting from’ zero acts on an iron rod, and 
is brought back to zero, the condition of the rod at the end 
is not exactly the same as at the beginning. A certain 
amount of- molecular work, which is not reversible, has been 
done during the cycle.. A certain molecular distortion persists 
as an after-effect in residual magnetisation. Similarly, when 


Fic. 380. Cyclic Curves of Magnetisation (2) and of Conductivity (4) 


metallic particles. are subjected to cyclic electro-motive varia- 
tion, an after-effect is found to persist in a change of con- 
ductivity. In substances belonging to the negative class 
the after-effect is one of enhanced conductivity ! (fig. 380 4). 

Referring again to that molecular condition of the 
substance which is represented by @ in fig. 378, we find 
that a new increment or accession of force will raise its 
condition to 4’. In this case the acting force has been 
continuously operative and continuously increasing. On 
the cessation of the acting foree, a substance possessing 
marked self-recovery will fall back from 6’ to 4 But 
if. there be a certain persistence of after-effect, then a 


1 Bose, ‘On the Change of Conductivity of Metallic Particles under Cyclic 
Electro-motive Variation.’—7he Electrician, September 1901. 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR VARIATION 623 


stimulating force which had raised the substance to - 0’ 
would, when again applied, after a very brief interval, 
raise it to J’, and so on. That is to: say, the molecular 
effect would in this case be additive. Tetanisation - will 
thus give a curve bearing a great resemblance to the charac- 
teristic curve. This will be seen from the following record, 
obtained by magnetic tetanisation of steel (fig. 381), which 
bears so close a resemblance to the record of electrical 
tetanisation of nerve (cf fig. 313). Both these curves, again, 
resemble the typical characteristic curve seen in fig. 378. In 


Fic. 381. Photographic Record of Magnetic Tetanisation of Steel, 
exhibiting Transient Enhancement of Response on Cessation 


In a tetanising shocks were moderate, in 4 strong. 


all these we find that the curve rises, after a longer or shorter 
horizontality, in an abrupt manner ; that its rate of rise then 
undergoes a decline, the curve tending again to become 
horizontal ; after which fatigue-decline may be initiated. In 
the. photographic record of the magnetic tetanisation of 
steel (fig. 331), a remarkably suggestive phenomenon is ob- 
served. In that part of the tetanic curve which is horizontal 
the one-directioned molecular distortion, due to stimulus, is 
exactly balanced by the force of restitution. On the sudden 
cessation of tetanisation the state-of balance is disturbed, 
and we obtain here the remarkable occurrence of a_ brief 
overshooting, Or positive variation, in the curve, followed 


624 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


by recovery. This is exactly parallel to the sudden 
enhancement of response in the retina on the cessation of 
tetanising light, or to the enhancement of response in 
the nerve when the tetanising electrical shock is suddenly 
withdrawn (pp. 428, 536). From the present experiment it 
will be seen that the suggested explanation of the pheno- 
menon, as due to anabolic or katabolic changes, is gratuitous. 

In responding substances, where the persistence of after-_ 
effect is relatively great, the successive shocks for the obtaining 
of the tetanic curve need not be repeated so quickly as where 
recovery is rapid. The shifting of the base-line of a series 
of even such responses as indicate incomplete tetanus, will 
give an indication of the form of the characteristic curve. 
The progressive molecular modification of a substance may 
thus be gauged, as already pointed out, in either of the two 
different ways—by progressive changes in the character of 
the response, or by means of the characteristic curve of the 
substance, And if both these, again, represent correctly the 
molecular condition of the material, we shall further find 
that definite parts of the characteristic curve have each their 
peculiar responsive features. In order to take these records 
‘of the characteristic curve and corresponding responses of 
a substance, moreover, we may adopt any method that is 
convenient—mechanical, electro-motive, magnetic, or that of 
the resistivity variation. The feasibility of such records is 
obviously a matter of the extent of the change induced and 
the sensitiveness of the recording apparatus. Of the various 
methods here mentioned, it may be said that there are no 
particular sources of uncertainty to be guarded against in 
regard to variations of resistance, of magnetisation, or of 
length. But in the method of electro-motive variation, as the 
change to be recorded is relative, being measured against a 
neutral or indifferent point, some difficulty occurs in securing a 
point which is invariable. This may be done more or less per- 
fectly, however, by choosing an injured or killed point on the 
tissue for the second contact, in order that it may be subject 
to as little variation as possible from environmental changes. 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR VARIATION 625 


‘We shall now proceed to the description of the distinctive 
characteristics of certain. molecular states, We may take 
first the case of ‘nerve, which gives different characteristic 
responses under different’ conditions; and here, employing 
the simplest mode of record—namély, the mechanical—we 
find, as already said; that, when it is cut off from all sources 
of energy, the specimen is apt°to fall into a condition of 
stowing sub-tonicity,, This is indicated in the mechanical 


Fic. 382. Mechanical Response of Frog’s Nerve to successive equal 
Stimuli, applied at Intervals of One Minute 


The sloping line at the beginning shows growing elongation due to sub- 
tonicity, Stimulus here causes positive response. Fourth, fifth, and 
sixth responses are distinctly diphasic. Responses become normal 
and ‘increasingly negative’ after the seventh, with marked staircase 
increases. Molecular transformation is seen to be very rapid, above the 


B point of transition. Record is a photographic reduction, half original 
size, of tracings obtained on smoked glass, 


record by an increasing abnormal elongation, as in fig. 382, 
given above. When the nerve is now subjected to the 
action of stimulus, its. tonic condition is gradually restored, 
progressing towards a normal excitatory condition. The 
molecular transformation involved here is at first expressed 
by growing retardation of the abnormal elongation, and 
afterwards by gradual contraction. At the point of trans- 
ition from positive to negative, or from elongation to 


sS 


626 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


contraction, that is’to’say, in stagé B,we shall find that the 
rate of transformation becomes very rapid. 

The second test, by which we may judge of the progress 
of molecular transformation in the experimental specimen, 
consists, as we have seen, in the nature of its reply to 
stimulus. Thus, in the sub-tonic condition, with its tendency 


to elongation, the responses are abnormal. positive. From 
this they pass gradually, with the progress of molecular. 


transformation, into the normal negative, the intermediate 
responses being either diphasic or zero. As the process is 
very rapid after passing the point of transition, the succeeding 
responses near this point show a staircase increase. 

Or if we do not wish to record the intermediate series, 
but merely to observe the terminal transformation into 
negative, or enhanced negative, due to the ascent of the 
molecular curve above the transitional point, we may apply 
a rapid series of stimuli, or tetanisation. We may here, 
according to circumstances, and the point started from, 
obtain either (1) abnormal positive transformed to normal 
. negative responses; or (2) diphasic, passing into normal 
negative; or (3) feeble, becoming enhanced, negative 
response. The idea has been put forward, as already said, 
that tetanisation enhances the responsiveness of the nerve, 
by some supposed evolution of carbonic acid. That this, 
however, is erronéous, has been shown by numerous experi- 
ments already related, and by the fact that even in inorganic 
substances, under given circumstances, tetanisation enhances 
response. Nor is it invariably true, in any case, that its effect 
is always to enhance response. Under certain conditions, 
it may actually cause depression. The decisive ‘element in 
the question of its effect lies in that part of the characteristic 
curve at which it is applied. If this be immediately after 
the point of transition its result will be an enhancement. 
Should tetanisation, however,.be applied above the maximum 
or highest point in the curve, its effect will be the diminution 
of response by fatigue. ; 

In order clearly to exhibit the fact that continuous 


Ne ee 


i - hs 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR VARIATION 627 


molecular transformation shows itself in two different ways— 
by a progressive physical change of the substance itself as 
exhibited’ by the characteristic curve, and also by a pro- 
gressive variation in the character of the responses—I shall 
here give a pair of records of the mechanical response of 
frog’s nerve. In fig. 382 the continuous molecular trans- 
formation caused by impinging stimulus is shown by the 
growing contraction, or responsive mechanical negativity of 
the nerve, as seen in the 
shifting of the base-line 
upwards. It is also in- 
teresting to notice here 
the continuous  trans- 
formation of the in- 
dividual responses from 
the abnormal positive 
through diphasic to the 
normal negative. There 
is also the noticeable 
additional fact that after 
the point of transition 


is passed the response Fic. 383. Mechanical Response of Frog’s 


undergoes a marked Nerve, showing Conversion of Abnormal 
. ; Positive into Normal Negative Response 
staircase increase. In after Tetanisation 


fig. 383 is given another Note also the shifting of the base-line up- 


: : wards, and that the individual period of 
record, obtained with positive is shorter than that of negative 


frog’s nerve, where, after responses. 
an intervening period of 
tetanisation, the abnormal positive response is converted into 
normal negative with staircase increase. The shifting of the 
base-line upwards is also very noticeable here. Effects pre- 
cisely similar are observed in the mechanical response of 
vegetal nerve. 

If we now turn to a different ae of observation—-say 
that by the electro-motive variation—the records will. be 
found to bear a remarkable resemblance, in every. particular, 


to those which have just been given. We find here the same 
SS 2 


628 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


continuous transition, from abnormal positive to normal 
negative, :as before, through intermediate diphasic, with: a 
shifting: of ‘the base-line upwards, exhibiting an increasing 
negativity. “The gradual transformation of the character of 
the response may be seen when a long series of successive 
responses to successive stimuli is taken (cf fig. 277). Or the 
abnormal positive may be-séen’ transformed: into normal 
negative, after an intervening period .of tetanisation. (cf 
fig. 276). Or when the point of molecular transition is 
passed, ‘the effect of intervening 
tetanisation is to enhance the ampli- 
tude of response (cf fig.275). It will 
thus be seen that the characteristic 
response in the sub-tonic condition A 
is abnormal positive ; and that, when 
the substance ‘is transformed by 
stimulation, to a point above the 
transitional B, the response is con- 
verted into normal negative, and 
lastly, since the rate of transforma- 
tion is very rapid above the point B, 
that successive responses in that 
region exhibit a. staircase increase, 
or moderate negative becomes the 
| enhanced negative, after an_inter- 
Fic. 384. Photographic Re- yening period of tetanisation.. The 

cord showing~ Conversion ‘ : q 

of Abnormal ‘Down’ Re- underlying transformation is thus 

yee Re aeenites indicated by changes in the response, 

and also by the shifting of the base- 
line upwards, in exhibition of the characteristic curve. These 
changes, which have now been described in the case of nerve, 
will be found to apply in all other instances of melecular 
transformation equally, 

Results in every way parallel are obtained with inorganic 
substances. In fig. 384 is seen the abnormal electro-motive 
response, represented as ‘down,’ converted into normal 
‘up’ after an intervening period of tetanisation. In the 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR VARIATION 629 


next figure (fig. 385) is shown how this abnormal response 
in platinum, in consequence of successive ‘stimulation, is 


Fic. 385. Gradual Transformation from Abnormal to Normal Response 
in Platinum 


The transition will be seen to have commenced at the third and ended at 
the seventh, counting from the left. 


eradually transformed-into a growing normal, through the 
intermediate diphasic. In fig. 386 is seen how the normal 


Fic, 386. Normal Electro-motive Response in Tin, enhanced after 
Tetanisation 


630 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


response is enhanced, after an intervening cet, in 
a specimen of tin wire. 

We pass next to the third mode of record, that, namely, 
by the Conductivity or Resistivity Variation. A selenium 


Fic. 387. Photographic Record of Abnormal Response of Selenium Cell 
converted into Normal after Tetanisation 


Stimulus applied was high frequency electric shocks, 


cell I find to be sometimes in a certain molecular condition 
in which it will respond to high frequency equi-alternating 


FIG, 388, Photographic Record showing Moderate Normal Response of 
Selenium enhanced after Tetanisation 


Stimulus applied was light. 


electrical shocks, of the order of a million times per second, 
by an increase of resistance. ‘Tetanisation is found to induce 
a transformation, attended by a diminution, or negative 
variation, of resistance. After. this, the responses are found 


es ie 


Se NPE TY 


ee ee ee 


FPR RR NTE TT 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR VARIATION 631- 


to be converted into normal: that is to say, they now take 
place by the diminution of resistance. Fig. 387. gives a 
photographic record of these effects. Selenium cells, again, 
under normal conditions, respond to light by a diminution 
of resistance. After tetanisation, or continued application of 
light, the normal responses, under certain circumstances, 
undergo an enhancement. The transformation induced by 
tetanisation, it is interesting to note, also shows itself by the 
shifting of the base upwards (fig. 388). 

I have found similar effects, again, to be exhibited by 
various metallic powders, under the stimulus of electric 
radiation. The record given in fig. 389 exhibits the 


abnormal positive response, of increase of resistance, as 


given by tungsten. After a short period of tetanisation the 
base-line is. seen to be shifted upwards, the molecular 
condition being transformed, in a negative direction, and 


thus exhibiting a permanent diminution of resistivity. The 


response in this particular transformed condition is seen to 
be diphasic—positive followed by negative. A further 
period of tetanisation carries this transformation still further 
in the negative direction, and the individual responses now 
seen are augmented normal negative. I give also a second 
pair of records in which the normal response of moderate 
amplitude in aluminium is enhanced, after an ee rsning 
period of tetanisation (fig. 390). gf 

We have seen, lastly, that molecular response may be 
recorded by means of the magnetic variation. «And it is 
interesting to see, by employing this mode of record, that 
under certain conditions tetanisation will enhance erence 
response (fig. 391). - 7 

In order to make a striking demoristration of Hie fact 
that the various phenomena described are not the result of 
some specific property of living tissues, with their hypo- 
thetical assimilation and dissimilation, but are determined 
by molecular conditions common to matter both living and 
inorganic; I shall now give in vertical. columns; several series 
of records of responses, obtained, under parallel conditions, 


632 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


from living tissues, animal and: vegetal, and from inorganic 
bodies. As the methods also, by which these records were 


Fic, 389. Photographic Record ot Abnormal Response of Tungsten to 
Electric Radiation, converted after Tetanisation into Diphasic and Normal 


obtained, were so different as those of the mechanical, the 
electro-motive, the resistivity and the magnetic variations, it 


Fic. 390. Moderate Normal Response of Aluminium, enhanced after 
Tetanisation 


follows that their similarities under parallel circumstances 
can only be due to certain fundamental molecular reactions, 
which are common to all alike. Further, since some of th 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR VARIATION 633 


responding substances were elementary, and the experimental 


arrangements offered no. possibility 
of chemical reaction, it follows that 
similar responses, in other cases like- 
wise, are determined, not» by some 
antecedent chemical, but by molecular 
action, though chemical action may 
take place as a consequence of. their 
responsive molecular derangement., 
_In the first of the series of records 
in vertical columns (fig. 392) we 
have: (a) a mechanical record of 
abnormal response by expansion, 


FIG. 391. Photographic Record of Enhancement 
of Magnetic Response after Tetanisation 


passing into normal response by con- 
traction, after intervening tetanisation, 
in frog’s nerve. In (0) we observe a 
similar transformation, as seen in the 
electro-motive response of frog’s nerve. 
The abnormal response by galvano- 
metric positivity is here converted by 
tetanisation into the normal megazzve. 
Turning next to inorganic substances, 
and taking the method _ of Resistivity 
Variation, we find in (c) the abnormal 
postive response of tungsten converted 
by tetanisation into normal negazzve, 
Finally (d) where the specimen is tin 
wire, and the record made by electro- 


Fig, 392. Vertical Series of 


a, 


Records showing Trans- 
formation of Abnormal 
into Normal Response 
after Tetanisation in 
Living and Inorganic 
alike in the A phase 


Mechanical response of 
frog’s nerve to electric 
stimulation ; 4, Electro- 
motive response of frog’s 
nerve to thermal stimu- 
lation; c, Response by 
resistivity variation in 
tungsten to electric radia- 
tion; .d@, Electro-motive 
response of tin wire to 
mechanical stimulation. 


634 


FIG. 393. 


COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


motive variation, the abnormal response 
is seen to be converted into normal after 
tetanisation. It will be noticed, in all 
these cases, that the antecedent molecular 
transformation, on which the conversion 
from abnormal to normal . response 
depends, is also shown independently, 
by the shifting of the base-line of the 
record in the direction of the normal 
response—that is to say, upwards, 

In the next series, again, in fig. 393, 
is shown the effect. of tetanisation in 
enhancing feeble normal response. This 
moderate normal response, it will be 
remembered, is characteristic of the 
molecular condition, just above the 
point of transition from abnormal to 
normal. In (a) is seen the enhancement 
of mechanical response in nerve of fern. 
In (6) we have the enhanced electro- 
motive response of frog’s nerve. In (c) 
a similar enhancement of electro-motive 
response is shown in plant nerve. In (@) 
we see the enhancement of response 
after tetanisation in aluminium powder 
by the method of resistivity variation, 
the stimulus employed being Hertzian 
radiation. In (e) the method of record 
is also by resistivity variation, in a 
selenium cell, under the stimulus of 
light. And finally, in (7) are given the 
responses of platinum wire, under the 


Series showing how Tetanisation enhances Normal Response 


in the B Phase 


a, Mechanical response of frog’s nerve; 6, Electro-motive response of 


frog’s nerve; c, Electro-motive’ response of plant-nerve; @, Response 
by resistivity variation in aluminium powder; ¢, Response of selenium ; 
f, Electro-motive response of tin. 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR VARIATION 635 


method of electro-motive variation, before and after mechanical 
tetanisation. The antecedent molecular transformation to 
which this enhancement is due may also be gauged, in all 
these cases, by the shifting of the base-line upwards. 

We have up to this time dealt with the first part only of 
the characteristic curve, up to a point slightly above that of 
transition. The responding substance, however, in con- 
sequence of the after-effect of stimulation, now passes into 


FIG. 394. Photographic Record showing Responses corresponding with 
different parts of characteristic curve in frog’s nerve 


a, Abnormal subtonic ; 4, Staircase ; c, Uniform ; d@, Fatigue decline ; 
: ¢, Fatigue reversal, 


various different phases of molecular condition, These may 
be short-lived, or more or less persistent. 

We shall next study all the responsive modifications dué 
to these induced molecular conditions, from the subtonic A 
to the post-maximum E conditions,-in order, Selecting as 
our specimen for this purpose the nerve of frog, the different 
phases through which this is capable of passing. may, for 
convenience, be divided into five classes (fig: 394). . In the 
first of thesé—the abnormal A phase—the nerve is sub-tonic. 
It is here undergoing a relaxation, and its characteristic 


636 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


response to -individual stimulus. is abnormal: positive. In 
consequence- of stimulation, however; we have seen this 
relaxation to be arrested, and to pass into growing con- 
traction. ‘The characteristic’ of response at this transitional 
stage is to be diphasic, passing gradually into the normal 
negative. On reaching this, the B phase, the responses, as we 
have seen, commence with feeble normal, and undergo a 


staircase increase. We may arrive at an idea of the rate of 


molecular transformation, in this and succeeding phases, 
from the curve of the mechanical response of nerve under 
tetanisation (cf fig. 313).. We there saw that in the B, or 
transitional, phase, the rate of contraction was very rapid ; 
we also found the individual responses at this stage to show 
a staircase effect. The rate of contraction next became 
slower, and the curve was afterwards more or less horizontal. 
Beyond this, fatigue-relaxation set in. 

We have now to observe the responsive variations 
characteristic of these different phases. For this purpose, a 
high magnification of three hundred times has to be em- 
ployed. Records so obtained are given in fig. 394. The 
method of procedure is as follows: We first take two or 
three test-responses to individual stimuli of definite intensity, 
at the A phase. This test-stimulus is subsequently main- 
tained at the same intensity. When the record of the A 
stage has thus been taken, continuous stimulation is applied 
for a time, till we arrive at the B stage, when the record of 
responses to individual stimuli is taken once more. The 
contraction due to the previous tetanising stimulus employed 
for the conversion of phase, is now so great that the record- 
ing spot of light is carried out of the field. At the com- 
mencement of each phasic record, therefore, the spot has to 
be brought back to the plate by suitable adjustment of. the 
reflecting mirror. Thus the first record of each series really 
shows the effect of the termination of tetanisation, the sub- 
sequent records showing response to individual stimuli. We 
may, however, obtain some idea of the characteristic changes 
occurring in the nerve as a whole, by joining the tops of the 


a Ae ee 


as) Noe ne Weer ae 


Ee 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR VARIATION. ~ 637 


response records, Fromm the inclination of the line thus pro- 
duced it is possible to. see whether the nerve at each ‘different 
phase was contracting, had assumed a stable length, or was 
relaxing. -In the B phase, as here shown, for instance, it will 
be seen that the nerve, when undergoing an. increasing con- 
traction, shows a staircase enhancement of response; at C 
we observe this change to arrive at a climax,: with con- 
sequent stability of condition and uniformity of response. 

_ _ The characteristic curve, after this, undergoes a reversal : 
that is to say, responsive contraction is now diminished, and 
eventually gives place to relaxation ; and it is curious to find 
that all the responsive phenomena observed during the 
ascent are now repeated, but in the reverse order, That is 
to say, during the ascent of the curve we obtained the se- 
quence of abnormal positive, diphasic, and increasing normal 
responses. And during the reversed process we obtain 
diminishing normal, diphasic, and the culminating abnormal 
positive response. The cycle of molecular phases, with their 
attendant variations, is thus complete. 

An inspection of D shows the change in the condition of 
the nerve from the contracted to the relaxing state. The 
onset of fatigue is also seen in the diminishing amplitude of 
the responses, This process is seen accentuated, to the 
actual reversal of response, in the last phase E, I shall later 
give a special record exhibiting the diphasic responses inter- 
mediate between D and E, | 

The response of nerve has hitherto been supposed, as 
already mentioned, to be specifically different from that of 
ordinary tissues. One characteristic particularly’ insisted 
upon was its indefatigability, or incapacity for fatigue, the 
nerve in this respect differing essentially from the muscle. 
On taking a general review, however, of nerve and muscle- 
response, we find that there is no essential difference between 
the two. During the first phase of contraction, both alike 
show staircase increase. This is followed, in both, by a 
series of uniform responses. And in the stage of fatigue, 
in both, the process of contracture gives place to one of 


638 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


relaxation. Theé’only difference lies in the fact that fatigue — 
makes its appearance in the one case earlier than in the other. 

When dealing with the subject of the enhancement of 
response by tetanisation, I stated that here it was not 
tetanisation, as such, which formed the determining factor in 
bringing about the increase of response; this was rather due 
to a phasic molecular transformation, induced by tetanisation. 
If the substance happen to be in the transitional B phase, 
then and then only will tetanisation enhance its response. 
If, however, it should happen to be in the optimum C phase, 
then the same tetanisation will have the effect of carrying 
it into D and E, the phases of fatigue. The response here, 


Fic. 395. Photographic Record of Response of Tungsten showing 
Enhancement of Response after moderate Tetanisation, and Reversal 
of Response, due to Fatigue under stronger Tetanisation 


instead of being enhanced, will be decreased or reversed. 
This is seen in the following record (fig. 395) obtained with 
tungsten when moderate tetanisation enhances response, 
whereas strong tetanisation, by bringing on fatigue, reverses 
the normal response. | 

How universal are these phenomena will be seen from 
the accompanying series of records, obtained. not only with 
various living tissues, but also with inorganic substances 
under parallel conditions, the normal responses being in all 
these cases reversed by tetanisation, in consequence of the 
transformation from the C to the E phase. In fig. 396 (a) 
is seen the normal contractile response of a frog’s nerve 
reversed to the positive or expansional, after tetanisation. 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR VARIATION 


639 


In (6) is seen the reversal of the normal electro-motive 
response in the digesting leaf of Drosera, after tetanisation, 
the stimulus here also being electrical. 


reversal after tetanisation in the elec- 
tro-motive response of the pulvinus 
of Mimosa. And in (d) is given a 
similar reversal after tetanisation, in 
the response of tungsten powder, the 


record being here of the resistivity 


variation, under the stimulus. of 
Hertzian radiation. These, and other 
results already given, have _been 
obtained by the employment. of 
different forms of stimulation. We 
may, therefore, regard these charac- 
teristic transformations as: brought 
about by all forms of stimulus alike. 

We thus see that one identical 
stimulus may give rise to opposite 
effects, according to the molecular 
condition of the responding tissue. 
This molecular transformation, more- 
over, may be brought about by the 
previous action of the stimulus itself. 
These considerations will, I think, be 
found to elucidate the very obscure 
question of the effect of drugs, with 
special reference’ to the opposite 
actions of large and small doses. 


Since a chemical substance acts in- 


a manner. not unlike that-of other 
stimulating agents, a moderate dose 
of a given reagent might be expected 
to induce effects similar to that of 
the action of moderate stimulation. 


FI 


a; 


[In (¢) we have 


G. 396. Series showing 
reversal of Normal Re- 
sponse by fatigue due to 
strong Tetanisation in- 
ducing the E phase 


Mechanical response of 
frog’s nerve; 4, Electro- 
motive response of Dvo- 
sera; ¢, electro-motive 
response of pulvinus of 
Mimosa; ad, Response 
by resistivity variation 
of tungsten powder. 


Hence_its effect in 
inducing molecular transformation will generally be to en- 
hance excitability, as from B to ©. Too long-continued action, 


640 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


‘however, carrying the substance acted upon ‘to the phase of 
D or E, will cause depression, Or it is conceivable that the 
same depression might be more rapidly induced by more 
intense stimulation—that is to say, by a larger dose. 
Now that this is what actually takes place has already 
been shown in several experiments which have been 
described, We saw, for example (fig. 95) that the continued 
action of the moderately stimulating agent, sodium. car- 
bonate, at first induced an exaltation of response, followed 
later by depression. In the case of vegetal nerve, again 
(ff. fig. 297), we found that the same agent, in smaller doses, 
caused at first an enhancement of conductivity, followed 
later by slow depression. A stronger dose of the same 
reagent, however, was found to cause rapid depression 
(fig. 298). Even in the case of poisons, so-called, the same 
facts make their appearance. Here an agent which’ proves 
toxic in large, appears as a stimulant when given in minute, 
doses, Thus in studying the effect of various chemical 
agents on growth-response, I found that while a one per cent. 
solution of copper sulphate was toxic, the same reagent 
proved stimulatory, if given in a solution of ‘2 per cent. 
_ A more detailed account of these experiments will be found 
in my work on ‘Plant Response,’ from which I quote the 
following summing up: | 
‘A survey of the effects of drugs, both stimulating and 
poisonous, reveals the striking -fact that the difference 
between them is [often] a question of quantity. Sugar, for 
instance, which is stimulating. when given in solutions of, 
say, I to 5 per cent., becomes depressing: when the solution 
is very strong. Copper sulphate, again, which is regarded as 
a poison, is only so at I per cent. and upwards, a solution of 
‘2 per cent. being actually a stimulant, The difference 
between sugar and copper sulphate is here seen to lie in the 
fact that in the latter case the range of safety is very narrow. 
Another fact which must be borne in mind in this connection 
is that a substance like sugar is used by the plant for 
general metabolic processes, and thus removed from the 


ECR ene See ae el ee ee 


Se IT Oe ee a a 


i EY INR Peat 


Pee, 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR VARIATION 641 


sphere of action. Thus, continuous absorption of sugar 
could not for a long time bring about sufficient accumulation 
to cause depression. With copper sulphate, however, the 
case is different. Here, the constant absorption of the 
sub-tonic stimulatory dose. would cause accumulation in the 
system, and thus ultimately bring about the death of the 
plant’? . = 

From all this it is clear that the progress of medicine may 
be greatly facilitated when the attention of investigators is 
drawn to the importance of the molecular aspects of the 
phenomena with which they have to deal. Thus, in examin- 
ing the action of drugs, a threefold question is seen to arise. 
It must first be determined what is the nature of the respon- 


sive molecular change induced by the given reagent under 


normal conditions. The second matter of inquiry is, What is 
the critical dose, above and below which opposite effects may 
be expected? And, finally, as the nature of the response has 
been seen to be influenced by the part of the molecular curve, 
at which the responding tissue has arrived, when the chemical 
reagent is applied, it rollows that an important element in 
the problem lies in the determination of the tonic condition 
of the tissue. How important is this last factor will be seen 
from an experiment to be described at the end of the present 
chapter, where an identical course of treatment+‘in one 
condition of the tissue revives it from inanition, and in 
another hastens its death. 

I have shown that when the condition of the substance 
is transformed from C€ to E, the response ‘also/is reversed 
from normal negative to abnormal positive. I shall now, 
therefore, proceed to show that in the course of this 
transition there is an intermediate stage of diphasic 
response. Before exhibiting this in the case of nerve, I shall 
give an interesting record in which the same thing is seen to 
take place in the mechanical response of fatigued indiarubber. 
Before the onset of fatigue, the normal'contractile responses 
were large, but at that stage—that is to say before the record 

1 Bose, Plant Response, p. 488. 
yes iy 


642 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


commences —they had begun to decline. In the series then 
recorded (fig. 397) we see how the depressed contractile 
responses are gradually transformed into’ abnormally expan- 
sive, through an intermediate diphasic. : ; 


Fic. 397. Fatigue in Indiarubber giving rise to Diphasic and Reversed 
Responses 


In the next series of mechanical records, obtained from 
nerve of frog (fig. 398), we have results exactly similar. The 
depressed contractile negative here passes through diphasic 
to abnormal positive. Thus, during the descent of the 
characteristic curve we obtain, as has been said before, a 


Fic. 398. Fatigue inducing Diphasic Variation and Reversal of Normal 
a Response in Frog’s Nerve 
a, Diminished normal response; after tetanisation, enhanced fatigue in- 
duces diphasic passing'into reversed positive response, 4 ; a period of rest 
after this revived the normal response in c ; after long-continued tetani- 
sation, response is seen to be abolished in.d, by the death of nerve. 


repetition, but in reverse order, of all the phenomena seen 
during the molecular ascent; the sequence of responsive 
variation was from abnormal positive, through diphasic, to 
increasing negative. During the descent the sequence is 
diminishing normal response, diphasic, and abnormal positive. 


ow wererter os Se SS ee ee ee ee eee 


CYCLIC MOLECULAR VARIATION 643 


The two halves of the cycle are thus strangely alike, one 
being, as it were, a reflection of the other. The cycle begins 
with sub-tonicity, due to a deficit of absorbed stimulus, and 
ends with the abnormality caused by excess of stimulation. 
The starting-point of the one may be supposed to meet the 
end of the other in a common fatality. The tissue comes to 
the same death by inanition on the one hand, through lack 
of stimulation, and by fatigue, on the other, through over- 
stimulation. But though the one half thus mimicks the other, 
there is, as it were, a polar difference between the two, by 
reason of the difference in their past histories. To revive the 
dying tissue, in the beginning of the cycle, stimulation is 
necessary ; to revive it afresh, at the termination of the cycle, 
a period of rest is essential. 


TT2 


CHAPTER XLIII 


CERTAIN PSYCHO-PHYSIOLOGICAL PHENOMENA—THE 
PHYSICAL BASIS .OF SENSATION 


Indications of stimulatory changes in nerve: 1, Electrical; 2, Mechanical— 
Transmission in both directions—Stimulatory changes in motor and sensory 
nerves similar—Responsive molecular changes and the correlated tones of 
sensation—Two kinds of nervous impulse, and their characteristics— Different 
manifestations of the same nervous impulse determined by nature of indicator 
—Electrical, motile, and sensory responses, and their mutual relations —The 
brain as a perceiving apparatus—Weber-Fechner’s Law—Elimination ot 
psychic assumption from explanation of particular relation between. stimulus 
and resultant sensation—Explanation of the factor of quality in sensation— 
Explanation of conversion from positive to negative tone of sensation after 
tetanisation—Various effects of progressive molecular change in nerve—FEffects 
of attention and inhibition—Polar variations of tonus, inducing acceleration 
and retardation. 


IT is admitted that the molecular changes induced in the 
nerve by stimulus, are followed by sensations perceived in 
the brain. The question as to the nature of these antecedent 
changes induced in the nerve, and the quality of the sensation 
that succeeds them, falls properly, then, within the scope of a 
physiological inquiry ; and it is certain aspects of this which 
will be treated in the present chapter. I may here point out 
that the results which I have to describe consist of deductions 
drawn from direct experiment. They will in some cases 
lend support to the psychological hypotheses already ad- 
vanced ; while in others they will be found to be opposed. 
In such cases, therefore, it is perhaps not too much to hope, 
from their strictly experimental character, that they will 


prove of use in deciding between rival theories; while in — 
others they will be found to introduce facts and considera- — 


tions which are entirely new. 


———— ee, 


o> 
yp oer 


PHYSICAL BASIS OF SENSATION 645 


Referring to the excitatory changes on which sensation 
depends, there has been much discussion as to whether the 
effects of stimulus in efferent and afferent nerves are of the 
same or of different natures. The difficulty in deciding this 
point lay in the fact that the indications of the state of 
excitation are different in the two cases, one exhibiting it 
objectively by the motile effect, and the other subjectively 
by sensation. It has been supposed, as we have seen, that 
the excitatory changes transmitted by the nerves were un- 
accompanied during their progress by any direct visible 
indications. It has been shown, however, in the course of 
previous chapters, that a change of form does in fact ac- 
company the transmission of the wave of excitation along 
the nerve. It was also shown that this mechanical indica- 
tion could be rendered extremely delicate, ranking, in 
degree of sensitiveness, between the galvanometer and the 
brain. Employing this mode of investigation, then, we 
found not only that the wave of excitation might be trans- 
mitted in either direction in any given nerve, but also that 
the changes induced by stimulus were similar in afferent and 
efferent nerves (p. 529). 

Regarding the nature of this molecular change, again, 
it has been supposed that the nerve under excitation exhi- 
bited a specific variation, known as the xeura/, totally unlike 
those changes which take place, for instance, in muscle. We 
have seen, however, that this is not the case, the mechanical 
and electrical expressions of the molecular changes in excited 
nerve being of a nature essentially similar to those observed 
in muscle also.. Even in the matter of conduction, we have 
seen that non-neural tissues transmit'the state of excita- 
tion to a certain distance beyond the point of stimulation. 


The difference in this respect is one of degree, and not of 
kind. 
We have next to deal with the question of sensation as 


induced by molecular changes in the nerve. It is widely 
admitted that the changes induced in the nerve by stimulus 
will cause responsive sensations. But the relation between 


646 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


the responsive sensation and the character of the molecular 
change that induces it has been regarded as unascertainable. 
‘That many of our feelings depend immediately upon the 
condition of the nervous elements is beyond doubt... . 
What is the peculiar nature of the excitation upon which 
the different feelings depend ‘for their differences of 
quality ? What is the characteristic change in the excita- 
tion that gives rise to two kinds of tone which the feelings 
possess, to pleasure and to pain? Physiological psycho- 
logy can answer none of these questions with much con- 
fidence.’ ! 

The fundamental contrast of tone in question raises the 
inquiry, therefore, whether it may be possible to discover any 
antecedent nervous changes of opposed character. Taking 
an instance of response by some simple form of sensation, it 
is well known that while moderate stimulus produces a feeling 
which may be described in general as not unpleasurable, or 
even distinctly pleasurable, an intense stimulus of the same 
nature will cause a displeasurable or even painful, sensation. 
These fundamental differences of quality are classified as 
‘positive and negative Zones’ of sensation, the term ‘ positive’ 
being here associated with perceptions which are not un- 
pleasant, or even actually pleasant, while ‘negative’ refers to 
the reverse. While the sensations ensuing under moderate 
stimulus, then, such as moderate pressure or moderate light, 
are of ‘positive’ tone, those brought about by more intense 
stimulus are apt to become converted into negative. The 
positive sensation grows to a maximum, according to the rise 
of stimulus-intensity within a certain limit. Beyond this 
point, sensation becomes, first, less and less positive, and then 
increasingly negative, as the intensity of stimulus continues 
to be augmented. Ora simple stimulus, suchas a light blow, 
which evokes a positive sensation, will, when often repeated— 
that is to say, when employed tetanically induce a negative 
or painful sensation. It is thus seen that the tone of sensation 
is in some way associated with the intensity or duration of 

* Ladd, Outlines of Phystological Psychology (1891), p: 387. 


' rhe a i P - 
cae ee ae ta 2 ab a) Shee ae 


PHYSICAL BASIS OF SENSATION 647 


stimulus. The question, however, remains, whether or not. 
these opposite sensation-tones could be demonstrated to be 
dependent upon characteristic nervous changes of opposed 
characters. lf we should succeed in making such a demon: 
stration a physico-physiological basis of. psychical effects 
would have been established which would unquestionably 
prove to be of great value, 

Now we have seen, referring to previous investigations 
on nerves, (1) that a feeble stimulus applied to the nerve is 
transmitted as a pulse of expansion. This we have 
designated the positive wave. The propagation of this 
wave being more or less of the nature of a hydrostatic 
disturbance, we have seen that its transmission is not affected 
to any great extent, even when the conductivity of the tissue 
is diminished. (2) A more intense stimulus we have found 
competent to give rise to a disturbance of opposite or negative 
sign—that is to say, toa pulse of contraction. The velocity with 
which this second, or,as we have called it, the true excitatory 
wave, was conducted, we found to increase with the intensity 
of the stimulus. While with feeble stimulus the positive 
wave alone was transmitted, with stronger, both negative and 
positive were propagated, but the more intense negative was 
liable to mask the feeble positive. As the negative wave 
was dependent on the conductivity of the tissue for its 
propagation, we have seen that it was possible to separate 
the two by any means which would diminish the conductivity 


"of the tissue. By such means the negative could be made to 


lag behind the positive; or, by its complete suppression, it 
was even possible to exhibit the positive alone. Thus a 
tissue which normally gave only negative response, owing to 
the masking of the positive, might, by the depression of its 
conductivity, be made to give diphasic, or positive response 
alone (p. 530). 

So far then, as regards the detection of two nervous im- 
pulses of opposite sign by means of the delicate mechanical 
method. The same facts may also be demonstrated by the 
less sensitive method of electrical response, according to 


648 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


which we saw that the two nervous impulses were exhibited 
by two opposite electro-motive variations—those of galvano- 
metric positivity and negativity respectively. This reaction 
of expansion and galvanometric positivity, however, may 
also occur as the expression of the increase of internal 
energy, in whatever way produced. Indeed, the positive 
form of response under moderate stimulation may be 


regarded as a case falling within this definition. Thus we . 


see that, beginning with very moderate stimulus, we obtain 
in the tissue a purely positive effect; and that, as the 
stimulus is augmented, the true negative excitatory effect 
also begins to make its appearance in increasing degree, the 
positive component of the response being now more or less 
masked. The energy that afterwards remains latent in the 
tissue goes to enhance the tonic condition. The amount 
thus held latent depends on the difference between income 
and expenditure. As a general rule, it will be under 
intense stimulation that the expenditure of energy will be 
likely to exceed the income. Thus we have two extreme 
cases, first, that in which moderate stimulus brings about 
increase of energy ; secondly, that in which excessive stimulus 
brings about run-down of energy ; and between the two a 
large range of variation, within which either one condition or 
the other may predominate. It must, of course, be under- 
stood that anything which increases the tonic condition is for 
the well-being or health of the organism, and is associated 
with positivity. Similarly, any fall of the tonic condi- 
tion below par makes for exhaustion and against healthy 
tone. 

We have next to take a rapid survey of the changes 
induced by stimulus in the conducting nerve itself, or any of 
its attached indicators. Such variations may, for purposes of 
convenience, be classified as motile, electrical, and sensory. 
In the nerve itself we have found, as has already been 
pointed out, by means of the Kunchangraph, that the motile 
change induced by feeble stimulus was one of expansion, the 
same change being shown electrically by galvanometric 


—_- 


ee ee ee ee ee ee ee ee 


—— 


PHYSICAL BASIS OF SENSATION 649 


positivity. The change induced by strong stimulus, on the 
other hand, was of contraction and galvanometric negativity. 

In the terminal motile indicator also, there are two 
different modes of response of opposite signs—namely, 
expansion and contraction. -In the highly excitable muscle, 
the occurrence of the former of these, brought about, as it is, 
by very feeble stimulus, is not easy to demonstrate. Bearing 
in mind, however, the fact that in nerve positive response is 
more easily obtained when the excitability is depressed, 
I succeeded in obtaining positive expansional response of the 
muscle, in a nerve-and-muscle preparation of frog, which had 
been depressed by the anzsthetic action of chloroform. At 
a certain stage of anzsthetisation, the response of the muscle 
under stimulation of the 
nerve was found to take 
place by expansion, 
followed by recovery. 
Just as in a nerve in 
a somewhat depressed 
condition, ee Fic. 399. Abnormal Response of Muscle by 
stimuli evoke positive Relaxation, followed by Normal Response 
response converted later af, Contraction 


‘ 1 ; The first two responses by relaxation are 
into normal negative, followed by two contractile responses. 


so, in the muscle-pre- 
paration described, the abnormal positive was followed by 
the normal negative response. In fig. 399 I give a photo- 
graphic reproduction of the myographic record obtained on 
a smoked-glass surface. 

In all these different effects we obtain, by means of the 
mechanical response of the terminal organ, what is merely 
a parallel expression of changes occurring in the nerve itself. 
As in the nerve, so also in the muscle, there are two different 
kinds of responsive expression—namely, expansion and con- 
traction. Thus we see that the various manifestations 
registered by different modes of indication are only so many 
diverse expressions of the same fundamental molecular 
changes. 


650 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


We turn next to the sensory mode of indication, that is 
to say, to the psychic effects registered in the central 
perceiving organ by the positive and negative waves 
conveyed to it along the afferent nerves. We have already 
seen that the stimulatory changes induced in these sensory 
nerves are precisely the same as those which occur in the 
efferent. What, then, are the effects in the central apparatus 


induced (1) by that positive impulse which is associated with . 


expansion, and (2) by the negative impulse associated with 
contraction ? : 

It is a matter of universal experience, as already 

mentioned, that feeble stimulus gives rise to sensations of 
positive or pleasurable tone, while an intense stimulus of the 
same kind will induce a responsive sensation which is negative 
or painful. We have also seen, in the course of the present 
work, that a feeble stimulus will give rise to a wave of 
expansion and galvanometric positivity, while the same 
stimulus, when intense, will give rise to a negative wave. 
We are therefore justified in regarding the positive impulse, 
associated with expansion and galvanometric positivity, as 
_pleasure-bearing, and its, opposite as pain-bearing or dolori- 
ferous. Numerous experiments—some being of a crucial 
character—will be given, in the course of the present and 
succeeding chapters, which will be found to lend full support 
to this conclusion. 

This fact, that the same stimulus may induce positive 
sensation in the central and expansion in the motile organ, 
or the negative painful sensation with muscular con- 
traction, according only to the nature of the indicator, 
will furnish grounds of reconciliation to those who 
hold on the one hand that the motor reaction is secon- 
dary to the mental, and on the other, that sensation 
is merely an accompaniment of movements reflexly in- 
duced.’ 

‘Many hold the motor reaction to be secondary to the mental. Of the 
coarser emotions it has been argued by James that the feeling does not cause, 


but is caused by, the bodily expression, The bodily changes, according to him, 
follow directly the perception of the exciting fact, and our feeling of the same 


Ue ed es ee eee ee eee 


i pw eal a Eager 


pment ns 


eae 


PHYSICAL BASIS OF SENSATION 651 


If the sensation be in fact due to definite and ascertainable 
physico-physiological changes in the nerve, then the various 
modifications of sensation must, in like manner, be traceable 
to corresponding modifications in the physico-physiological 
process. In that case, the particular relation which is known 
to exist as between stimulus and sensation—expressed as 
Weber-Fechner’s Law — must be demonstrable as directly 
dependent upon molecular changes induced, and not on the 
existence of some assumed psychic factor. This molecular 
theory, further, if it expresses a truth of universal applica- 
tion, ought to be capable of explaining not only the 
quantitative relation between stimulus and sensation, but 
also that qualitative variation of which Weber-Fechner’s 
Law is unable to take account. Should the Molecular 
Theory prove adequate to this, its truth may be regarded as 
demonstrated. | 

We shall, however, subject this theory to further and still 
more crucial tests. If it be true that our sensations, 


‘painful and pleasurable, are due to nervous impulses of 


opposite character, then any modification of either of these 
impulses by any given agent should appropriately modify 
the resultant sensation. We have seen, for instance, that 
the negative wave is complex, and contains within it the 
masked positive. We have also seen that by appropriate 
means these two waves may be made to exhibit themselves 
separately ; or the positive, by the total suppression of the 
negative, may be displayed alone. I shall therefore show 
that, by the employment of the same means, the subjective 
sensation of painful or negative tone may also be analysed 
into its component parts, which may thus be made to exhibit 
themselves in succession; and, on the other hand, that by 


changes as they occur zs the emotion. Certain experiments furnish evidence— 
not highly satisfactory—that all pleasurable states of consciousness are accom- 
panied by bodily movements of extension, and all painful by movements. of 
flexion. These movements may be very slight. Miinsterberg concludes that 
the feeling of agreeableness is the mental accompaniment and outcome of 
reflexly-produced movements of extension, and disagreeableness of the move- 
ments of flexion.’--Schafer, Zext-Book of Physiology, vol. ii. (1900), p. 975- 


652 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


the complete obliteration of the negative element an already 
painful sensation may be converted into pleasurable. 

Of the scheme thus laid down, the first part will be 
carried out in the present, and the second in the succeeding, 


chapters. 
Turning now to our sensations themselves, it may be well 


to consider some of the characteristics of the central perceiving | 


apparatus. Asa detector of nervous changes the brain is 
undoubtedly the most delicate of instruments, surpassing in 
this respect not only the galvanometer, but also the 
Kunchangraph. It fails, however, strictly speaking, as an 
accurate metrical apparatus. It is not able to discrimi- 
nate quantitatively, for instance, by means of sensation, 
through any wide range, between the finer differences of 
intensity in the nervous impulses it receives. In the pain- 
and-pleasure series, again, the distinctions which it is able to 
make are, to a certain extent, of a merely qualitative 
character, unmistakable only as between the two extremes 
of the series, the intervening region tending to be somewhat 
indefinite. The sensitiveness of the physical instruments, 
Kunchangraph and galvanometer, is always constant and 
reliable. For example, in the galvanometer, by adjusting 
the controlling magnet, we can obtain varying degrees of 
sensibility, which at any particular adjustment will remain 
constant. But in the perceiving apparatus, not only is the 
sensitiveness of different individuals widely different, but even 
in a single individual it undergoes great variation under 
different conditions. 

By deliberate attention or inhibition, as by raising or 
lowering of the controlling magnet in the galvanometer, the 
sensitiveness of the perceiving field can be almost indefinitely 
varied. Pursuing this analogy of the galvanometer further, 
we find that in the brain, instead of a single coil, with its 
one pair of terminals, there are many coils with many pairs 
of terminals, receiving impulses from every part of the 
organism. Confining our attention, moreover, to any single 
circuit among these, we find again that the impulses it 


o <x eet 


meso 


SR a 
a es 


SaaS RE De Sew ee 


x 


Tp SN RES 


4b 


Marah ee 


. 


PHYSICAL BASIS OF SENSATION 65 3 


conveys are varied in their character. There are, for 
instance, the immediate effects of stimulus, whether positive 
or negative, and also the persistent after-effects of stimuli 
previously absorbed. The central apparatus, however, is not 
acted on by these impulses from any single circuit alone, 
but from many circuits at the same time, the whole resulting 
in a ‘vague tremor of generalised consciousness. The 
individual sensation evoked by any particular stimulus bears 
to the rippling surface of this consciousness the relation of 
a larger or smaller wave. 

Thus we see that there are tele conditions which will 
contribute to the intensity of the sensation evoked by an 
jndividual stimulus. There will be, first—to revert to the 
simile of the galvanometer-——the enhancement of the con- 
ductivity of the particular circuit involved; and, secondly, 
the suppression of all interference caused by the semi- 
conscious activity of other circuits. By the action of the 


will, producing the condition of attention or expectation, 
the excitability of the receptive or responsive points, and the 


conductivity of particular channels, may be exalted, while 
they may be depressed in others by the reverse process of 
inhibition. The extent to which it is claimed that this 
power of inhibition may, with practice, be carried, would 
appear almost unimaginable. -I have myself known of an 
authenticated instance in which the pulsation of the heart was 
arrested and renewed at will. In India, indeed, it has been 
held, from very remote times, that such practices are capable 
of reduction to a science. It is thus believed to be possible 
that all nervous impulses due to external causes may one by 
one be inhibited, until the attention is concentrated on a 
given point, in complete isolation from any interference 

whatever by the physical organism. Regarding the physical 
aspects of these processes of inhibition and concentration, 

more will be said at the end of this chapter. : 

There are again other elements calculated to bring about 
further variations in the sensitiveness of the instrument 
which lie more or less beyond the control of the observer, 


654 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


His previous habits and prepossessions all contribute, as is 
well known, to modify it more or less permanently. The 
principal reason for the constancy of the records made by 
the physical indicators lies in the greater or less constancy 
of the properties of those elements of which they are com- 
posed. Even here there is a fluctuation of sensitiveness, 
owing to changes in the properties of the material. But 
these changes are neither so rapid nor so considerable as 
in the neural apparatus, whose excitability is extremely 
susceptible of modification under the influence of fatigue, 
and of such varying factors as health and tonicity, as well as 
by the action of the stimulus itself. 7 : 
Looking now at the whole range of impulses generated 
in the nerves under increasing stimulus, we shall see that 
the positive effect is gradually augmented till it reaches a 
maximum. It then undergoes a decline, passing into a 
resultant negative. This resultant negative response con- 
tains, as we know, a masked positive component, which 
can be separated and exclusively demonstrated by appro- 
priate methods. With increasing stimulus the negative 
response undergoes an enhancement till a limit is reached. 
Expressed in terms of sensation, then, the effect perceived 
is at first of positive tone, and this, growing in intensity, is 
pleasurable. This positive tone, however, afterwards under- 
goes a diminution, and finally passes over thé zero-line ; 
this constitutes the commencement of a somewhat extended 
range, in which the resultant negative, with its masked 
positive, undergoes increase. Referring to the curve at the 
crossing of the zero-lirie, it must be said that we. have here 
a neutral point. This is not, however, the same as 
that absolute zero at which the curve of sensation was 
initiated, for here the neutrality is not due to absence of 
effect, but to the fluctuating equilibrium of two opposite 
effects, one positive and the other negative. It is thus to 
be understood that, while in the positive region the tone is 
simple, in the region of the resultant negative it is complex, 
the sensation here being.compounded of positive and nega- 


PHYSICAL BASIS OF SENSATION 655 | 


tive (pleasure-pain). In this region, then, it is theoretically 
possible to bring out the positive element alone, by suppress- 
ing the negative. It is the predominance of either one of 
the two components which at any given moment determines 
the pleasure-pain character of the complex sensation. At 
a certain critical point it is the element of pain which will 
begin to appear as relatively conspicuous, this proceeding 
towards a climax with increasing stimulation. 

It must be understood that we are dealing in general 
with nervous response under normal conditions of excitability. 
It will be sufficient here to make a cursory reference to 
certain exceptional cases which may occur. We have found 
that the character of the response given by a tissue is deter- 
mined by the two factors of (1) the effective intensity of 
stimulus, and (2) the excitability of the responding tissue. 
If the nature of a given stimulus be such as to produce but 
a moderate effect, there will be a greater likelihood of its 
evoking only positive response. Or the responding tissue, 
in another case, may possess exceptional excitability ; hence 
the responsive indication here will tend from the beginning 
to be negative. The different effects depending on the vary- 
ing excitatory characteristics of the tissue, we have already 
seen illustrated in several cases. The slightly excitable 
epidermis was seen to give a predominantly positive response, 
whereas nerve which had been rendered highly excitable 
tended, on the other hand, to give negative response. 

We shall now proceed further to consider the corre- 
spondence between the responsive sensation and the degree 
of nervous change induced by stimulus. And here the first 
question that arises is that of the relation which sensation 


bears to the intensity of the stimulus that provokes it. The 


difficulty of this investigation lies in the generally unsatis- 
factory character of the subjective standard, and in the 
variations induced by stimulus itself in the sensitiveness of 
the neurile elements. . 

According to what is known as Weber-Fechner’s Law, 
the strength of stimulus must increase in geometrical ratio 


656 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


in order that the intensity of the sensation may increase 
arithmetically. The method of experiment on which this 
result was based is not, however, altogether unexceptionable. 
Against the generalisation itself, many objections have been 
urged, and even its supporters claim for it only a very 
limited range of application. It has been pointed out that 
Fechner starts with the assumption that the change of 


sensation under varying intensities of stimulus is merely 


quantitative ; he does not take into account that the quality 
or sign of sensation is also liable to change. 

Confining our attention to that range of sensation which 
does not involve any change of quality, it is urged that, 
starting from the minimally effective intensity of stimulus, 
excitation at first increases very rapidly, and subsequently 
more slowly, under successive equal increments of intensity. 
This particular complexity led Fechner to suppose that such 
complex phenomena as the quantitative relation between 
stimulus and sensation were not determined by mere physical 
or physiological factors. He therefore maintained that his 
generalisation expressed an ultimate law concerning the 
relation between physical stimuli and psychical reactions, 
or, in other words, between body and soul. 

It is now possible, however, turning away from specula- 
tive hypotheses, to subject this question to direct and simple 
experiment. We have seen how delicate and free from 
complication is the record of the mechanical response of 
nerve. In these tracings we have the record of the direct 
effect of stimulatory action on the nerve itself. In order, 
then, to study the effect of stimuli of varying intensities, I 
first chose a specimen of the sciatic nerve of gecko. A 
sliding electric inductorium was used for the purpose of 
stimulation. From a previous experiment with a ballistic 
galvanometer the absolute intensity of the electric shocks at 
different distances of the secondary from the primary was 
calibrated. Marks were then made on the sliding base, 
which gave various intensities of stimulation—1, 2, 3, 4, 5—- 
and so on. Thus, keeping the duration of the exciting 


—— Sew 


PHYSICAL BASIS OF SENSATION 657 


primary current the same, and bringing the secondary nearer 
and nearer the primary, the intensity of the stimulus could 
be gradually increased quantitatively. The effective intensity 
of stimulation might also. be increased in a graduated 
manner by fixing the primary inside the secondary, and 
gradually increasing the duration of stimulation. 

The following record shows the effect of stimuli in- 
creasing from one to ten, by increments of one at a time 
(fig. 400). It will be seen that at the beginning, owing to 
growing sub-tonicity, the nerve was undergoing a gradual 
relaxation, the downward slope of the beginning of the record. 
On application of stimulus of intensity as shown by 1, there 
is a sudden responsive 
relaxation, followed by 
a partial recovery. As 
the intensity of stimu- 
lus is successively in- 
creased, the positive 
response undergoes an 
increase, the maxi- 
mum-positive response 
being evoked when the FIG. 400. Record of Response in Nerve of 

- : oye Gecko showing the Effect of Arithmetically 
stimulus-intensity is 3. increasing Stimulus 
After this the ampli- 
tude of response undergoes a progressive decline, the response 
to stimulus 8 being practically zero. This neutral point, as 
was shown earlier, is not to be regarded as the true zero, being 
in fact the balancing point of positive and negative. This 
will be seen when we inspect the record of intensity 9, where 
the increasing negative actually induces a minute diphasic 
response—positive followed by negative. The next response 
to stimulus 10 gives us a sudden large negative response. 
Above this point of transition I find, as will be seen in the 
following records, that there is a rapid enhancement of normal 
negative response. Still later, this increase of rate would 
decline, and later again, by the setting-in of fatigue, the 
responses might undergo an actual diminution.. ‘Thus, in that 

UU 


658 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


part of the record which lies beyond the point of transition, 
we can see that the excitatory change at first increases very 
rapidly, and afterwards more slowly, under successive equal 
increments of stimulus-intensity. 

In order to test the universality of these characteristics of 
nerve-responses, I obtained records with many different 
specimens. The record just shown was taken, as already 
said, with a sciatic nerve of gecko, which was in somewhat 
sluggish condition. The next (fig. 401) was obtained from 
the sciatic nerve of a vigorous bull-frog. On subjecting this 
nerve to increasing stimuli, I, 2, 3, 4,.. . it was found that 
the positive response reached 
a maximum, after which it 
declined, and the response be- 
came diphasic. With gradu- 
ally increasing stimulus, the 
positive element in the re- 
sponse now became smaller 
and smaller, while the negative 
grew larger. Above. this the 
negative responses underwent 

a very rapid increase. 
Fic. 401.—Response of Nerve of ; ; 
Bull-frog to Stimuli. 1, 2, 3, Up to this point, I have 
. . . 12, increasing in Arithmetical peen describing the peculiari- 
Progression : : 
ties of response, as seen in 
motor nerves. In order to show, however, that the same 
characteristics hold good of the responses of sensory nerves, 
I next took a specimen of the optic nerve of Ophzocephalus. 
As I here wished to demonstrate the possibility of the response 
after continuous increase, reaching a limit, I employed the very 
moderate magnification of only thirty times, in the recording 
Kunchangraph. With larger magnifications, the record 
exceeds the recording-plate, and such a demonstration is 
impossible. The object being thus to record the peculiarities 
of response in the negative region only, that stimulus which 
was taken as the unit was sufficiently strong to induce a 
contractile response which, under the given magnification, 


Ne anmenee 


PHYSICAL BASIS OF SENSATION 659 


appeared moderate. Records were then made under increas- 
ing stimulus 1, 2, 3, 4, 5. ... It will be seen from these 
(fig. 402) that increasing stimulus induces at first a rapid 
augmentation of the negative response, after which a limit 
is reached. 

The same characteristics I find to hold good of the 
response of plant-nerve (fig. 403). Here the response-records 
were commenced at a point just above that of transition. 
Now, if these particular relations between stimulus and 
response be due, in the case 
of man, to some specific 
psychic reaction, then the 
same must also be true not 


Fic. 402.—Response of Optic Nerve FIG. 403. Mechanical’ Response 
of Ophiocephalus to Arithmetically of Nerve of Fern to Arithmetic- 
inereasing Stimuli 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7 ally increasing Stimulus 


only of the animal but also of the plant. And further, the 
records themselves, being mechanical; were direct records 
of undeniably physical changes. Even in the case of the 
inorganic, again, the same characteristic relations obtain, as 
we shall find in the next figure. Hence it is not true that 
the relation between stimulus and the responsive reaction is 
different in the inorganic from what it is in those living 
nervous tissues whose changes we_ perceive .as_ sensation. 
That is to say, it is determined, in all cases alike, by the 
same underlying physical factors, 
UU2 


660 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


In fig. 404 we have a series of magnetic responses to arith- 
metically increasing magnetic stimulation. The magnetising 
current by which this was accomplished increased from :2 
ampére to 2 amperes by steps of ‘2 ampere ata time. The 
responses at first, as will be seen, increased at a very rapid 
rate, and afterwards the rate declined. The remarkable 
similarity between this record and that obtained with the 
optic nerve of Ophzocephalus is at once apparent. 

In the chapter on the modification of Response under Cyclic 
Molecular Variation, I have already shown how the responsive 
change differs in. intensity at different 
parts of the characteristic molecular 
curve. We there saw that just beyond 
the point of transition, in the B state, 
the rate of change was very rapid, and 
that it became slower in the higher 
part of the curve. The induced trans- 
formations in the condition of the 
nerve, by which it passes from the 
phase B to @, and so on, are of them- 
selves sufficient to elucidate not only 
these, but also other obscure charac- 
teristics of the phenomenon of sensa- 
Fic, 404. Piciogta ht tion. They will, for example, explain 

spied Mies sc ge the curious case in which an identical 

Arithmeticallyincreas- stimulus given at regular intervals 

sh is proves at first not unpleasant, and at 
the end of the series actually painful. Here sensation has 
become intensified, though the exciting stimulus remains the 
same. I may quote here an account of an experiment by 
Professor Sherrington, which exhibits this fact in a very 
interesting manner : 

‘I found that by focussing the heat rays from a lamp 
upon a skin area of about 25 sq. mm. (on the back of the 
hand), and allowing a perforated screen to intermittently 
intercept the radiation, the heat-pain begins to be perceptibly 
intermittent when, the times of play and of interception 


PHYSICAL BASIS OF SENSATION 661 


being equal, the intermittence falls below the rate of thirteen 
per minute. An intensity of this intermittent radiation 
stimuli, at first not painful, and yielding sensations strikingly 
discrete, soon becomes dolorific, and then the sensations 
remain discrete no longer, but are fused more or less to- 
gether,’ ! i 

This anomaly of gradual heightening of sensation, and 
later, fusion of effects, appears at first sight inexplicable. I 
shall give a satisfactory explanation of the latter point in the 
next chapter ; but as regards the former of its two elements— 
namely, the heightening of sensation under uniform intensity 
of stimulus—we have seen that, owing to the after-effect of 
stimulus, the condition of responding substances in general, 
and nerve in particular, is gradually transformed, from a 
point below the transition B to one above it. But, owing to 
this transformation, the character of the response is changed 
(cf. fig. 382). If the original response be positive, it will be 


converted later into negative. If it be moderately negative, 


it will be converted into more intense negative. 

It will also be seen that the intensity of response is not 
solely determined by the intensity of stimulus, but is modified 
by the existing condition of the nerve. And this latter under- 
goes a change by the action of stimulus itself. Thus there 
are certain times of the day when, owing to sluggishness 
of the tissues, our power of perception is dull. But acuity 
of perception becomes enhanced with each successive stimulus 
responded to. This is also true, more or less, of each indi- 
vidual undertaking. These facts are easily understood from 
a consideration of the different responses which are charac- 
teristic of the ascent of the molecular curve above the point 
of transition. 

We have also to consider the fact that this progressive 
molecular modification, under certain circumstances, imposes 
the anticipation of the responsive maximum, with increasing 
stimulus, or even, in other cases, the induction.of an actual 
decline. The molecular variation, in consequence of the 

1 Text-book of Physiology, edited by Schifer (1900), vol. ii. p. 998. 


662 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


after-effect of previous stimulation, is seen by the shifting 
upwards of the base-line. We may here refer back to fig. 31 
where the responses of two different tissues to increasing 
stimulus, show, in the one case complete recovery, and in 
the other, a persistent after-effect. The base-line in the 
former coincides with the line of absolute equilibrium ; and 
increasing stimulus consistently shows an increasing response, 
till, owing to molecular distortion reaching a maximum 
value, a limit in the response was reached. In the second 
of these records the persistent after-effect shifts the base- 
line, which now becomes the line of modified equilibrium. 
Owing to this fact, the relative maximum of variation 
from the condition of modified equilibrium is reached much 
earlier than would otherwise have happened. The extent 
of individual response, after this, appears in fact to undergo 
an actual diminution, though, measured from the line of 
absolute equilibrium, this is not the case. In our sensation 
we are unable to take cognisance of the absolute zero, 
the changed condition of the nerve itself at any given 
moment providing us with a relative zero, which is our 
only standard of comparison, and our whole perception of 
intensity depends upon induced variations from this chang- 
ing zero, 

The record which I have given in figs. 400 and 401, show- 
ing the mechanical responses in the nerves of gecko and frog, 
under increasing stimulus, not only explains the quantitative 
change in the sensation, but also that change of quality or sign, 
under appropriate conditions, which had not hitherto found any 
explanation. We see in‘the curve of response, as has been 
said, that with feeble stimulus response is foszdeve, and that 
this positive response attains a climax, after which it diminishes 
in amplitude, and then changes sign and passes over into 
negative under increasing stimulation. In the corresponding 
response by sensation, we find similarly, not only a difference 
of quality or sign, but also a transformation from one to the 
other, under increasing stimulation, from a climax where the 
sensation attains a maximum of the pleasurable tone. After 


' aie Shere 
eee ee a ee ee ee ee ee ee 


eae 


PHYSICAL BASIS OF SENSATION 663 ~ 


this the positive declines, and passes over the line of trans- 
ition into the region of the negative or painful. 

The fact, again, that a moderate stimulus, such as is 
efficient to induce the positive sensation, will, if tetanically 
applied, become negative or painful, finds satisfactory explana- 
tion from the peculiar characteristics of the molecular curve. 
For we have seen, under tetanisation, that the curve is raised 
from the positive region, below the point of transition, into — 
the negative, above it (cf fig. 383). 

We have thus seen that by molecular transformation 
the excitability of the nervous tissue may be enhanced or 
depressed. There are other conditions also under which the 
conducting power of the nerve, as well as its excitability, 
appear to be modified by the exercise of will. Thus, by 
attention, a stimulus which was previously scarcely per- 
ceptible may be raised to sensory prominence. The reaction- 
time, again, may be diminished by attention. The very 
opposite of these effects, again, are induced by inhibition. 
As an example of the latter may be mentioned the inhibi- 
tion by the will of the muscular movements natural under a 
given stimulus. It was at one time thought that this inhibi- 
tion of movement was brought about by the in-nervation of 
antagonistic muscles. This theory, however, is held to be 
disproved by the fact that such inhibitory effects are seen, 
even where antagonistic muscles are not present. It would 
thus appear that the nerve is susceptible of being thrown 
into certain conditions, in response to internal action, of an 
unknown character, by which the transmission of excitation 
through it is either accelerated or inhibited. This power 
has been said to be ‘ unique and mysterious.’ 

That such opposite dispositions of the nerve may actually 
result from opposite incipient distortions of the molecules is 
well shown, however, by the action of electrotonus. Here 
we have seen that by the influence of one pole an incipient 
molecular distortion is brought about, which facilitates the 
transmission of the true excitatory wave, while by the oppo- 
site this transmission is hindered or blocked, Thus an 


664. COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


identical nerve will be rendered acceleratory or inhibitory 
by the opposite.effects of the inducing tonus. | 

If an external force be thus capable, according as it is 
positive or negative, of inducing opposite molecular disposi- 
tions, by which the conducting power of nerve is so pro- 
foundly modified as to render it for the time being-either an 
accelerator or an inhibitor, then it is not difficult to under- 


stand that these molecular dispositions may also be yaried — 


in a similar manner by impulses from an internal source. 
The molecular dispositions themselves, by which these effects 
are brought about, are no doubt curious, but they are neither 
mysterious nor unique; for we have seen that by molecular 
distortions of one sign or another, artificially induced in 
magnetic substances, a given magnetic impulse may be either 
accelerated or retarded. 

We have seen that the transmission of excitatory changes 
is facilitated,.if the nerve be subjected to incipient molecular 
distortion in a favourable direction. Now in considering the 
attitude of attention, we can see at once, if only from the 
muscular indications which it induces, that the nervous 
channel is probably thrown by it into a state of moderate 
contraction. Inattention, on the other hand, must generally 
be attended by an attitude of relaxation. We have again 
found that while incipient contraction or K-tonus enhances 
conduction, an intense contraction, or very strong K-tonus, 
will inhibit, because the molecular distortion thus induced 
is already maximum, and external stimulus can produce 
little further effect (p. 610). It is here interesting to note 
that the expression ‘steeling the nerves to pain’ is not 
altogether fanciful or metaphorical. The attitude of pre- 
paredness thus denoted is one of rigid contraction. 


In the course of the present chapter, then, it has been 
shown that there are two distinct nervous impulses, positive 
and negative. The former, induced by feeble stimulus, gives 
rise to a positive tone of sensation. The negative, on the 
other hand, due to stronger stimulus, gives rise to a sensation 


Pee ee 


rane ost : p 
i atalil it  o 


PHYSICAL BASIS OF SENSATION 665 


of painful or negative tone. The particular relation which 
exists between stimulus and sensation has been shown to be 
an expression of the peculiar characteristics of the molecular 
curve. The ascent in the curve, being rapid immediately 
above the point of transition, and slow later, equal increments 
of stimulation cause in this region an increase of sensation 
which is at first rapid and then slow. Below the point of 
transition responses are positive. But tetanisation, inducing 
a molecular transformation, carries the curve above the point 
of transition. Hence moderate stimulus, inducing positive 
sensation, is converted, when tetanically applied, into negative 
or painful. The positive response is simple, but the negative 
is complex, containing a masked positive; and we shall see 
in the next chapter how this complex sensation can be 
analysed into its component parts. 

We have also seen that, in addition to moderate stimula- 
tion, there are other agencies by which the tonus of nerves 
may be altered. In a magnetic substance, the incipient 
molecular distortions of one sign induced by K-tonus, enhance 
the excitability and conductivity, while those due to A-tonus 
depress them. Strong K-tonus, again, inhibits conduction. 
By such polar actions, then, an impulse is either accelerated 
or inhibited. Similar polar changes are seen in the nerve, 
moreover, under the action of an- and kat-electrotonus. In 
like manner, by the exercise of the internal stimulus of will, 
the tonus of the nerve may be so varied that, under different 
circumstances, the transmission of excitatory impulses is 
accelerated or inhibited. 


CHAPTER. XLIV 
DISSOCIATION OF COMPLEX SENSATION 


Conversion of pleasurable into painful sensation, and vice versa, by electrotonus— 
The Sensimeter—Mechanical stimulation--Stimulation by thermal shocks— 
Chemical stimulation—Opposite effects of anode and kathode--Normal effects 
reversed under feeble E.M. F.—Negative tone of sensation blocked by alcohol 
and anzesthetics—Separation of positive and negative sensations, by lag of 
one wave behind the other—Dissociation of sensation by depression of con- 
ductivity—-Abolition of the negative or painful element by block of conduction. 


I SHALL next proceed to demonstrate, by means of decisive 
experiments, the fact that sensation and its variations are 
associated with certain physiological changes and _ their 
appropriate modifications. In the mechanical and electrical 
response of the nerve, we found that while moderate 
stimulus gave positive response, a stronger stimulus gave 
negative ; that while the positive was unmixed or elementary, 
the negative contained a masked positive ; and further, that 
the velocity of the transmission of the positive was greater 
than that of the negative wave. 

We shall now study the correspondence of the responsive 
sensation with these various nervous changes induced by 
stimulus, and their modifications under different agencies. 
I shall next, therefore, show, in accordance with the chief aim 
of this chapter, that the same conditions which determine the 
exhibition of positive, mechanical, or electrical response, will 
also, pari passu, determine positive tone in the responsive 
sensation. Those conditions, on the other hand, which bring 
about a negative mechanical or electrical change in the 
nerve, will also induce a negative tone in the sensation. The 
resultant transmitted effect recorded in the responding 
apparatus, was, as we saw, determined by the receptive ex- 


ay aah”, es ee ee “4 


Se Fay A ee ee eS 


DISSOCIATION OF COMPLEX SENSATION 667 


citability of the stimulated point, and the conductivity of the 
transmitting tissue. We shall, therefore, investigate separ- 
ately the effects of various agents in the modification of 
receptive excitability and of conductivity respectively. — 

We saw, again, in the last chapter, that as the intensity of 
stimulation is continuously increased, the response gradually 
passes from the positive, through the point of transition, into 
the negative. - This happens, as we have seen, immediately 
above the point B. This region, therefore, may be referred 
to as critical and indifferent, and must be regarded as having 
the peculiarity that if, by any means, the molecular curve be 
raised above it, the corresponding sensation tends to become 
actually painful, or if carried below, becomes pleasurable. 

It follows that, in this critical indifferent region, any 
condition which tends to exalt the excitability of the tissue, 
and thus the negativity of the response, will also accentuate 
the negative tone of sensation, its mixed character thus 
passing into the distinctly painful. Those conditions, on the 


other hand, which lower excitability, and therefore responsive 


negativity, will also act by detracting from the painful 
element in the indifferent sensation in the critical region, and 
rendering it to a greater or Jess extent positive, soothing, or 
pleasurable. In this way, by depressing or raising the 
excitability at will, a stimulus which had already caused a 
painful sensation might be made not unpleasant, and an 
indifferent or positive sensation could be made negative or 
painful. We shall presently see how, by variations due to 
the presence of internal or external modifying factors, the 
positive tone of sensation is actually rendered negative, and 
vice versa, under such transpositions. 

Now we have at our disposal various well-known agencies 
by which the excitability of the tissue can be raised or 
lowered. In order to lower excitability, certain anzesthetic 
agents may be employed, and their effect in the modification 
of sensation-tones will presently be described. The ideally 
perfect means, however, not only for the exaltation or 
depression of excitability at will, but also for the rapid 


668 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


interchange of the one effect with the other, is that of 
electrotonus. 

Under the normal condition of a medium intensity of 
E.M.F., we know that it is the kathode which enhances excit- 
ability, and the anode which depresses it. Hence an indifferent 
sensation, however caused, will be rendered painful: when the 
excited point is made kathode, or pleasurable when anode. If, 


again, the stimulus be of sufficient intensity: to render the | 


resulting sensation moderately painful, kat-electrotonus will 
make it intensely painful, and an-electronus convert it into 
soothing, by taking away from it the negative element. 

The an- and kat-electrotonic effects referred to here are 
those which fall under the generalisation made by Pfliiger. 
I have shown, however, that the application of this law is 
but limited. Under the action of a feeble E.M.F. these an- 
and kat-electrotonic effects are exactly reversed, and it is 
then the anode which renders the tissue excitable, the 
kathode inducing relative depression. If, then, sensation- 
changes are dependent, both qualitatively and quantitatively, 
on antecedent physiological changes, we may expect corre- 
sponding reversals to take place in sensation, according as 
the anodic or kathodic applications are feeble or moderate. 

And, lastly, we might expect to meet with other charac- 
teristic changes in responsive sensation, due to the peculiar 
differences shown in figs. 320 and 401, between the positive 
and negative effects. We there saw that the positive 
response was short-lived, whereas the negative was more 
persistent. Even the negative response itself further was, 
under moderate stimulation, of briefer duration than under 


strong. In accordance with these facts there ought to bea _ 


difference in the fusion of sensation. Let us suppose that 
the frequency of stimulus, for the induction of the indifferent 
sensation, be adjusted in such a way as to cause a sensation 
which is so fused as to be almost continuous. Under normal 
kathode, this response, being converted into more persistent 
negative, will now become completely fused and _ painful 
Under normal anode, on the other hand, the individua 


we ee 7 * 


DISSOCIATION OF COMPLEX SENSATION 669 


responses being converted into short-lived positive, the 
resultant sensation will become discrete and pleasurable 
Before proceeding to describe these experiments in detail, it 
will be well to present these inferences in the form of a 
tabulated summary : 


EFFECT OF ELECTROTONUS ON INDIFFERENT SENSATION 
IMPERFECTLY FUSED 


Intensity of E.M.F. Anode Kathode 
Moderate. Soothing and conspicuously | Painful and continuous. 
discrete. 
Feeble. Painful and continuous. . Soothing and conspicuously 
discrete. 


These generalisations I shall now demonstrate by means 
of experiments carried out under, not one, but various forms 
of stimulation. For experimental adjustment two conditions 
have to be fulfilled. The first is so to regulate the intensity of 
stimulus as to: cause the indifferent sensation. The second 


is so to adjust its frequency as to induce a fusion of effects 


all but complete. It may here be pointed out that in general 
the degree of frequency necessary to bring about this semi- 
fusion. will be a question of the effective intensity of 
stimulation employed, and the excitability of the subject. 
Having first decided on the intensity of stimulus to be 
employed, it is easy to adjust its frequency by means of an 
adjustable interrupter. 

I shall first describe the Sensimeter, by means of which 
these experiments were carried out. Mechanical stimula- 
tion, when required, was caused by a tapper, actuated by an 
electro-magnetic arrangement (fig. 405). When an electrical 
current is sent round the electro-magnet, the soft iron arma- 
ture is pulled down against an antagonistic spring. When 
the current is stopped the armature is released, and the 
tapper delivers a blow. The height from which the tapper 
falls determines the intensity of the blow, the former being 
dependent on the strength of the electro-magnetic pull, 
which is determined in its turn by the intensity of the 


670 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


current. This latter is adjusted by means of a carbon 
rheostat. In order to adjust the frequency of stimulation, 
a toothed-wheel is fixed on clock-work, the normal period of 
rotation of the axle being once in four seconds. This 
speed, however, may be continuously adjusted within a 
certain range by means of a regulating governor. When 
the toothed wheel has sixteen teeth, the frequency of stimu- 
lation is four times in a second. By inserting wheels with 
different numbers of teeth, and by means of the regulating 


Fic. 405. The Sensimeter 


governor, it is possible to obtain any desired graduation of 
frequency. 

The same experimental arrangement, with slight modi- 
fications, may be employed to give a series of thermal shocks, 
For this purpose the electro-magnetic tapping arrangement 
is removed from the apparatus, and the electro-thermic 
stimulator attached to the circuit, instead of the tapper. The 
frequency of the thermal shocks may be regulated in the 
usual manner, their intensity being dependent on that of 
the heating current, which is adjusted by the rheostat. 


DISSOCIATION OF COMPLEX SENSATION Crt 


In experimenting on the effect of mechanical stimulation, 
the intensity and frequency were so adjusted as to bring 
about a neutral and imperfect fusion. In a_ particular 
experiment, for example, the frequency of stimulus was four 
times in a second. The receptive point for this experiment 
was the very sensitive back of the end joint of the human 
fore-finger—that is to say, the space immediately adjacent to 
the quick of the nail. In order to study the electrotonic 
effect, one electrode was applied by means of a piece of 
cotton, moistened in normal saline, and placed on the 
receptive area, the second being on a different finger. After 
so adjusting the intensity and frequency of stimulus as to 
sive the required semi-continuous and indifferent sensation, 
the receptive point was made kathode, the E.M.F. employed 
being moderate—that is to say, of 1°5 volt. The resulting 
sensation was distinctly painful and continuous. By now 
reversing the electrotonic current, the receptive point was 
made anode, and the resulting sensation was not only 
| positive or soothing, but also strikingly discontinuous. In 
order to exhibit the reversal of these effects under feeble 
; E.M.F., I employed an E.M.F. of ‘2 volt. The initial 
indifferent sensation was now found to be converted under 
anode, to negative and continuous, the kathode inducing a 
soothing and discontinuous sensation. The effect under 
a feeble E.M.F. is thus found to be in every way the opposite 
of that under strong. When frequent experiments are 
catried out on the same finger, the result is apt to be blurred 
in consequence of fatigue. It is therefore advisable to carry 
out the preliminary adjustment with one, and to repeat the 
experiment on another finger. The normal opposite effects 
of anode and kathode may in general be easily demonstrated 
by an E.M.F. of 1°5 volt. But the value of the feeble 
E.M.F. which induces the reversal of these, varies with 
different individuals and with different modes of stimulation. 
It is, therefore, advisable to start with the lowest possible 
E.M.F. of the order of about ‘or volt, and increase it 
gradually, till the reversal of effects is most pronounced. On 


672 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


increasing the E.M.F. still more, the reversed effect passes 
first into neutral and then back to the normal, characteristic 
of ‘a moderate E.M.F. 

Similar effects are also obtained on employing the 
stimulus of thermal shocks. The electro-thermic stimulator 
is now inserted in the Sensimeter, in place of the mechanical 
tapper. The indifferent and incompletely fused sensation 
becomes. continuous and painful, on making the receptive 
point kathode, under a moderate E.M.F. of 2 volts. On now 
making the receptive point anode, the sensation becomes 
converted into a markedly discrete positive. The reversal of 
these normal effects under a feeble E.M:F. was obtained in 
a given experiment by thermal shocks, when the polarising 
E.M.F. was ‘03 volt. 

Turning next to the chemical mode of stimulation, it will 
be remembered that an experiment has already been 
described (p. 582) where a continuously irritating sensation 
was caused by the application of salt on a wounded spot, 
this moderate sensation of pain being rendered soothing 
~when the spot was made normal anode, and intensely painful 
when normal kathode. Effects precisely opposite resulted 
from the application of a feeble E.M.F. It will thus be seen 
that those same conditions which depressed the normal 
negative response, also acted to obliterate the negative tone 
from the resulting sensation. Those conditions, on the other 
hand, which exalted the negativity of the response, would 
convert the positive tone of sensation into negative or 
painful. These general facts have been demonstrated by 
the employment of different forms of stimulation, the neces- 
sary depression or exaltation of excitability having been 
brought about by electrotonus. So far we have dealt with 
the modifications induced in the responsive sensation by 
the variation of the receptivity of the excited point. We 
shall next briefly discuss the effects which ensue on the 
variation of the excitability and conductivity of the nerve by 
different agents. In order to induce depression, there are 
various anesthetic agents which might be used. 


DISSOCIATION OF COMPLEX SENSATION 673 


In investigating the influence of alcohol (p. 493) we saw 
that three distinct effects were induced by it on the 
receptivity, responsivity, and conductivity of a tissue 
respectively, these effects themselves being further modifiable 
by the duration and intensity of the application. It was 
shown that, in the first stage of its application, alcohol exalted 
the power of receptivity. But its effect on conductivity and 
responsivity, especially after a certain duration of application, 
was one of great depression. The total effect of alcohol is 
thus somewhat complex. At an early stage of its applica- 
tion there is an effect of exaltation. After a while, however, 
the conducting power becomes increasingly depressed by its 
action. This means that the passage of the true excitatory 
or negative wave is progressively impeded or even blocked, 
the positive alone continuing to be transmitted for a time. 
At a certain stage of alcoholisation, therefore, the negative 
tone of an existing sensation, normally painful, will, as it 
were, be erased. And this withdrawal of the painful element, 
by causing sudden relief, together with the actual trans- 
mission of the positive wave, may induce a tone of sensation 
which might even perhaps be regarded as pleasurable. In 
any case the abolition of conductivity must eliminate the 
element of pain, and it was undoubtedly this which, in 
pre-anzesthetic medicine, caused its employment for certain 
minor operations. Long and intense alcoholisation will, of 
course, obliterate all sensation. On the after-effects of so 
depressing a reagent it is unnecessary to dilate. 

Effects, in some respects parallel, may be observed under 
etherisation, where, at a certain stage in the action of the 
narcotic, there is a cessation, not of all sensation, but of its 
painful element alone. Thus, by depression of conductivity, 
and’ consequent suppression of the negative wave, the positive 
may be ‘dissociated’ from the negative sensation. Sustained 
pressure on a nerve is also known to depress conductivity, 
and it is interesting to note here that pressure.on the ulnar 
nerve-trunk will abolish painful sensation, the positive, or 
mere contact-sensibility remaining practically undiminished. 

X X 


674 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


We shall next study the various effects induced by the 
variation of conductivity. We have already seen that a 
moderately intense stimulus gives rise to two waves, one 
positive and the other negative, the former having the 
greater velocity of the two. Thus these two waves, starting 
from the receptive and reaching the distant responding point, 
will give rise to two different responsive effects, separated 


from one another by an appreciable interval. The separation 


depending on the lag of one wave behind the other, will be 
greater the greater the length of the conducting tract. The 
first to arrive at the perceiving centre being the positive 
impulse, we shall have there a positive sensation of mere 
touch or contact. The later-arriving negative wave gives 
rise, according to the nature of the indicator, to mechanical 
contraction, galvanometric negativity, or a painful sensation, 
as the case may be. If the conducting tract be not 
sufficiently long, the two waves will be superposed, or 
indistinguishable, one masking the other. But they may be 
analysed, or separated, by anything which diminishes the 
conductivity of the intervening tract, and this is rendered 
possible by the fact that the positive wave is not much 
affected by changes of conductivity. On the other hand, the 
velocity and intensity of the negative wave are both 
diminished by anything that diminishes the conductivity. 
Hence a diphasic response—the sensation of touch followed 
by pain—may be expected, wherever the intervening con- 
ducting tract between receptive and responsive points is 
sufficiently long. In other cases, where the tract is shorter, 
a sufficiently strong stimulation will give rise to a single 
sensation of negative or painful tone. This negative 
sensation, however, is complex in its character, masking as it 
does, a contained positive element. If then the conductivity 
of this transmitting nerve be in any way diminished to a 


moderate extent, the complex sensation will be found to be 


analysed, the negative being made to lag behind the positive. 
There will thus be a dissociation of the dual elements of the 
complex sensation, and the result will be a diphasic response 


$:- ? 


tS a ag ina 


At 


DISSOCIATION OF COMPLEX SENSATION 675 


—the positive or sensation of contact, followed by that of 
pain. With: still greater depression of conductivity, the 
normally painful sensation, by the blocking of its negative 
element, will be turned into one of positive tone. 

We shall now proceed to verify these theoretical inferences. 
The fact that there are actually two distinct nervous impulses, 
and that of these the positive travels faster than the negative, 
is demonstrated by the well-known experiment in which a 
smart tap is applied on the ball of the foot, with the result 
that we perceive, first, the sensation of touch, and then, at an 
appreciable interval afterwards, that of pain. The possibility 
of this demonstration in a normal nerve is due to the length 
of the conducting nerve here concerned. 

In other cases the same dissociation is seen to be effected 
under varying degrees of loss of conductivity, in different 
forms of nervous paralysis. The considerations which I have 
advanced, will, however, I believe, offer a satisfactory explana- 
tion of those curious instances of ‘dissociation’ and ‘ delayed 


pain’ which are known to pathology. In such cases, a prick 


with a pin, for instance, is first perceived as mere contact, 
and then, after an appreciable interval, as the sense of pain. 
Other cases are known in which, the loss of conductivity 
being very great; the patient could handle burning coal with- 
out pain, the resultant perception being entirely positive. 
From the diverse phenomena described in this and the 
previous chapters, it is clear that a sensation of positive tone 
is associated with that particular responsive change in the 
nerve, which is expressed as expansion and galvanometric 
positivity. We have also seen that such effects are brought 
about by a feeble intensity of stimulus, which acts to increase 
the internal energy without inducing the true excitatory 
negative effect. Of anything which thus increases the 
internal energy, it may be said in general that it will induce 
positive mechanical and electrical expressions with concomi- 
tant positive sensation, With stronger stimulus, as we have 
seen, the negative mechanical and electrical responses are 
induced. But such negative responses contain, as we have 
XX2 


676 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


also seen, the masked positive component, and from the point 
of view of energy, they represent the algebraical summation of 
income and expenditure, positive and negative, increase and 
diminution. Considered as sensation, then, this particular 
effect will be composed of dual elements, a mixture of positive 
and negative tones. With very strong stimulation, finally, 
the negative effect will be very great; expenditure will be 
greater than income; and the sensation will assume a pre- 
dominantly and persistently negative tone. 

The complex negative, moreover, containing a masked 
positive, is capable of dissociation into its component 
elements. By the partial or complete block of conductivity, 
it is exhibited as dual response of ‘dissociation, or by the 
total suppression of the negative, as positive alone. In this 
way, a sensation, originally painful, may be rendered not 
unpleasurable, or even pleasurable. It was further shown, 
employing electrotonus and the sensimeter, that an indif- 
ferent and semi-fused tone of sensation can, by merely exalting 
the excitability of the tissue, be converted at will into one 


which is continuous and painful. By depressing the excit- 


ability, on the other hand, this indifferent sensation may be 
made pleasurable and strikingly discrete. 


| 
‘ 
| 


CHAPTER XLV 
MEMORY 


Memory an after-effect of stimulus— Persistence dependent on strength of stimulus 
—Rate of forgetting—Multiple after-effects in retina—Spontaneous revival of 
after-images— Theories of memory— Latent images and their revival—After- 
effect of stimulus on excitability and conductivity—Differential effect of diffuse 
stimulus—Revival of latent image in metal—Revival of latent image on phos- 
phorescent surface—Negative or reversed memory-image—Psycho-physio- 
logical version of this experiment—Differential excitation under diffuse 
stimulus, internal or external—Continuity of this seen in mechanical response 
of plagiotropic stem and pulvinus of A/cmosa, in the electrical discharge of 
certain fishes, and in psychic response of memory. 


Up to the present we have considered only the immediate 
effects of stimulus. We know, however, that excitation 
entails, not only an immediate, but also an after-effect.. We 
are thus led to the question of the physical aspects of the 
phenomenon known as memory, which is admittedly a 
matter of after-effects. Even with the somewhat insensitive 
apparatus for the detection of nervous changes which is at 
our disposal—the galvanometer or the Kunchangraph—we 
find that the after-effect of strong is more persistent than 
that of feeble stimulus. In the very sensitive neurile 
apparatus, nervous changes and their after-effects are more 
distinctly perceived. The psychological retention of an 
impression follows, in general, the curve of response and 
recovery. The fact that physiological recovery from the effect . 
of strong stimulus is less rapid than from feeble, has its 
correspondence in the period required for the fading of 
sensory impressions, of which also it may be said that the 
stronger persist longer than the weak. 

This fact that the after-effect of strong stimulus siteitls 


678 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


for a longer time than that of feeble, may be exhibited in.an 
interesting manner as follows. A simple design is made 
with magnesium powder and fired in a dark room. A 
second observer, unacquainted with the design, observes the 
flash and closes his eyes. The instantaneous flash, 
obscured as it is by dense smoke, does not at once produce 
any definite impression. In the retina, however, the ob- 
scuring image of the smoke, being of little luminosity, 
quickly passes off, and the after-effect of the brilliant flash, 
thus separated from the obscuring smoke, grows into perfect 
distinctness. In this manner I have often been able, by sub- 
sequent closure of the eyes, distinctly to observe luminous 
phenomena of brief duration, which, while the eyes were 
open, had been indistinguishable. : 

We have thus touched upon the question of the time 
required for the obliteration of a mental impression. 
Another interesting aspect of this subject lies in the rate at 
which fading takes place, or, in other words, the rate of 
molecular recovery. In the curve of recovery we find that 
this rate is at first very rapid, and becomes increasingly slow 
with the descent of the curve. This is also the characteristic 
of the process of forgetting, Ebbinghaus, for instance, found 
that the forgetting of a series of ‘nonsense syllables, at first 
quick, became increasingly slower with time. 

Certain excitable tissues, such as nerve and cardiac 
muscle, give responses to strong stimulus, which, as we have 
seen, are multiple in character. The retina, again, under 
intense stimulus of light, exhibits multiple after-excitations, 
which may be detected by a galvanometer (p. 426). This 
fact explains the multiple after-image often seen on closing 
the eyes after strong light. Another proof that these 
multiple after-images are physiological lies in the fact that 
their periodicity is modified by a previous condition of rest 
or activity. Thus, early in the morning, when fresh from 
rest, this period I find to be at its shortest, and later in the 
day to become gradually longer, owing to growing fatigue. 
In a given instance, the period at 8 A.M. was 3 seconds, 


4 - 4h & - » q ae 
pista BO oc dchaia’ aif’: > Sy? sate 


22 Rey hae erie Ea 9 Soe i beets a” 


— 


Magy eet Mag 


ee! 


MEMORY 679 


which had lengthened to 5 seconds by 3 P.M.; and was 
further prolonged to 6°5 seconds by II P.M. | 

One way of exhibiting the after-images in the retina is, 
as we have already seen, by means of a stereoscope con- 
taining two incised slits inclined to each other, instead of 
photographs. On looking through this at the bright sky for 
ten seconds, or longer, a composite image is formed of an 
inclined cross. The eyes are now closed, and the first. effect 
noticed is one of darkness, due to the molecular rebound. 
By reason of the Binocular Alternation of Vision, already 
referred to, one luminous arm of the inclined cross now 
projects itself aslant the dark field, and then slowly disap- 
pears, after which the second, perceived by the other eye, 
shoots out suddenly in a direction athwart the first. This 
multiple alternation proceeds for a long time, and produces 
the curious effect of two luminous blades crossing and re- 
crossing each other. At first the after-images of the cross, 
seen with the eyes closed, are very distinct, so distinct that 
any unevenness in the design at the edges of the slanting 
cuts can be made out clearly. There is here no doubt of 
the ‘objective’ nature of the strain impressed on the retina, 
which, on the cessation of direct stimulus of light, gives rise 
to after-oscillations with concomitant visual recurrences. 
This recurrence may be taken as a proof of the existence of 
physical strain in the retina. The recurrent after-image is 
very distinct at the beginning, but becomes fainter with each 
repetition. A time comes when it is difficult to tell whether 
the image is the objective after-effect due to previous strain, 
or merely an effect of ‘memory.’ There is, in fact, no hard- 
and-fast line of demarcation between the two—one merges 
simply into the other. In connection with this, it is 
interesting to note that some of the phenomena of memory 
also are admitted to be recurrent. 

Visual impressions and their recurrence often persist 
for a very long time. It usually happens that, owing to 
weariness, the recurrent images disappear, but in some in- 
stances, long after this disappearance, they will spontaneously 


680 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


appear at most unexpected moments. Thus in a given case 
of the present experiment, performed in the afternoon, the 
subject perceived this recurrence for some time, after which 
the alternating impressions seemed to disappear, and were 
completely forgotten. On retiring at night, however, these 
recurrent images suddenly reappeared. Thinking the matter 
to be an effect of light, the observer hastily extinguished his 
lamp. But the recurrent images now became only the more © 
intense. 

In another case the recurrence was observed in a dream, 
about three weeks after the original impression was made, 
and in this case it was seen as the crossing and recrossing 
of bright swords. These instances of the revival at night of 
impressions made in the daytime, when the interference o 
distracting influences is withdrawn, is significant. Since an 
intense stimulation of nerve is liable to recur spontaneously, 
‘without the action of the will, or even in spite of it, it follows. 
that any single impression, when very intense, may become 
dominant, and persist in recurring automatically. Examples 
of this are only too familiar. 

We have hitherto dealt with that aspect of memory in 
which it isa more or less immediate after-effect of sensory 
stimulation. But we encounter a much more difficult pro- 
blem when we come to the question of the revival of an 
image long after it has apparently faded. It has been sug- 
gested that this process of revival depends upon the existence 
of some ‘scar, or fixed impression, in the brain, or on a 
certain persistent disposition or tendency to movement 
created there. It is perhaps worth while to point out here, 
however, that though when a blow is recent, the smarting 
effect will persist for some time, yet; when once healed, no 
scar could of itself reproduce the original excitation. It is of 
course recognised that such expressions are merely figurative, 
and that the entire process is not clearly understood. We 
are more likely, however, to arrive at a true explanation of 
the phenomenon if we recognise in it two distinct factors, 
first, that of molecular change, with concomitant change of 


MEMORY 681 


properties ; and, second, the effect of an internal stimulus, 
delivered as a blow from within, by an impulse of the will, 
upon the sensitive surface in which the image is latent. _ 

We shall now first observe in some detail those changes 
which remain as an after-effect of previous stimulation. The 
differential effect caused by primary stimulus fixes the latent 
image, and it is only by the reproduction of the same differen- 
tial excitation, that the memory-image can subsequently be 
revived. 

We must here recall briefly the results which were estab- 
lished in Chapter XLII., on the modification of response 
under cyclic molecular variation. It was there shown that, 
under the action of stimulus, the molecular condition of a 
substance undergoes a progressive variation, exhibited in its 
characteristic curve ; that the forward and return curves do 
not exactly coincide, because the history behind the two 
half-cycles has not been the same; and, finally, that on the 
cessation of stimulus, the original molecular condition is not 
exactly restored, a certain effect being residual. Owing to 
this residual effect, the properties of the responding sub- 
stance are changed. We also saw that, in consequence of 
this impressed change, the conductivity and excitability of’ 
the substance might be enhanced. A frequent repetition 
of a stimulus was thus shown to create a habit or disposition 
by which the mass of a substance, formerly almost non-con- 
ducting, might be made a conductor of excitation. 

These impressed molecular changes may not leave any 
visible impression behind. But let us look at the responding 
properties of a given substance at different points on the 
characteristic curve. In a sluggish A condition, that is to 
say, before it has even been excited, the power of response of 
the substance to a given stimulus will be slight or negligible. 
Let us suppose next that by the action of stimulus the sub- 
stance is raised above B. On the cessation of stimulus a slow 
recovery will then take place, whose completion may be 
indefinitely prolonged. The substance will thus approach 
very near the point B in the curve, without actually reaching 


682 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


it. This difference between B and the point actually reached 
may be so small as to be undetectable by any ordinary 
mode of inspection. We therefore term the impression 
latent. But the properties of this B area, formerly acted 
upon, have been profoundly changed, being rendered more 
excitable by the impressed effect of previous stimulus. In 
this sensitive impression-surface will be certain areas in the 


A and certain others in the B condition, the former sluggish. 


and the latter characterised by enhanced excitability. By 
the shock of an internal diffuse stimulus, a differential excita- 
tion may now be induced, exactly similar to that caused by 
primary stimulus. This zs.the revival of the memory-image. 

_ We may carry out a physical experiment exemplifying 
this process of the rise of a latent impression into vividness 
under the action of diffuse stimulus. We may take a sensi- 
tive surface, in which different areas, in consequence of pre- 
vious excitation, have impressed on them latent variations of 
excitability. Thus indifferent portions of the surface A, A,, 
may have their excitability represented by zero, another 
portion B, whose excitability has been exalted as the after- 
_effect of stimulatory agents previously applied, will have its 
normal excitability enhanced. In still a third portion, C, the 
excitability is artificially depressed or abolished. The 
responding substance was a tin wire; dilute solution of 
sodium carbonate, which is an exciting agent, was applied 
on the area B. The depressing or poisonous reagent, oxalic 
acid, was applied at c. After a short period of this applica- 
tion the wire was washed, and there was no outward indica- 
tion of any difference between the areas A, Bandc. Elec- 
trically also there was little or no permanent difference 
between them. One non-polarisable electrode connected 
with a galvanometer was kept permanently applied on the 
indifferent surface A,. The second exploring electrode was 


now moved along the wire, and while it rested on any point. 


the wire was excited as a whole by vibration. The galvano- 
meter, under this arrangement, would detect differential 
excitability. As long as the exploring wire moved over 


ae 


MEMORY 683 


indifferent areas there was no effect detected in the galvano- 
meter. But as soon as the exploring electrode rested on the 
area B, the latent enhancement of excitability there showed 
itself by a sudden responsive up-movement of the galvano- 
meter. When the electrode again passed over B and reached 
an indifferent area, A,, response disappeared. But when it 
reached C, with its depressed excitability, there was another 
responsive movement, this time in the reversed or down 
direction. It is thus seen that the impress made by the 


Fic. 406. Revival of latent Image in Metal 


action of stimulus, though it remain latent and invisible, can 
be revived by the impact of a fresh excitatory impulse 
(fig. 406). 

Again, this revival of the latent image by a subsequent 
stimulation may be exemplified ina simpler and more striking 
way. We take a card and coat it with some so-called phos- 
phorescent material, such as luminous paint. This is kept 
a long time in the dark, till the whole is reduced to a uniform 
A condition. From a previous experiment we have deter- 
mined what is that duration of exposure, T, to a given 


684 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


intensity of light, which will evoke a luminous or phosphor- 
escent response. A stencilled pattern is now placed on the 
prepared card, and the whole is exposed to light for the time T. 
On now cutting off the light and removing the stencil a 
luminous pattern is seen, which is the primary response. 
This impression slowly fades out. But the cardboard now 
contains a latent image, whose revival will be analogous to 


that of memory. The stimulated areas which have now 


ceased to respond are still, in virtue of previous stimulation, 
in the B condition, which is more excitable than the indif- 
ferent A. A feeble diffuse stimulus should now, by its 
differential action, prove efficient to revive the latent image. 
We now expose the whole card to diffuse stimulation of light, 
of a duration shorter than T. The excitation of the indifferent 
background will by this means be ineffective, whereas it will 
be effective wherever the image proper has been impressed. 
We shall, therefore, obtain a revival of the positive image— 
that is to say, an image of the same kind as the original. 

An interesting case occurs here, showing the theoretical 
possibility of obtaining a negative or reversed ‘memory 
image.” The possibility of this will be understood, from an 
inspection of the characteristic curve. We saw that in the 
region B the substance rises in excitability. But in the 
region of D and E, where the maximum molecular distortion 
has already been reached in consequence of over-stimulation, 
fatigue changes are induced, by which the excitability is 
depressed below the normal. It follows from what has 
already been said that an impressed image of this character 
will be revivable, under subsequent diffuse stimulation, but as 
a negative, or reversed memory-image. 

I shall now describe a psycho-physiological version of this 
experiment. Let the observer stare at the incandescent fila- 
ment of an electric lamp, preferably with one eye, say the 
right, the left being kept closed all the time. The right eye 
is next closed, and is further covered by the hand. Multiple 
after-images will now be seen for some time, till the impression 
seems to have completely disappeared. No trace of the 


ae Se 


. oe 


ee, Se Sie eee 


MEMORY 685 


latent image is now perceivable in the field of dark vision. 
When this point has been reached, the hand is suddenly 
withdrawn from its position over the closed right eye. The 
light in the room now percolates through the semi-translucent 
eye-lid, and suddenly gives a moderate diffuse stimulus to 
the retina. Under these circumstances, the latent image is 
revived, as a negative—that is to say,as a very dark filament 
against a brighter background. Thus the essential condition 
for reviving the latent impression of stimulus would seem to 
be the subjecting of the unequally impressed tissue to diffuse 
stimulation. The revival of the image as positive or negative 
will then be a question of whether the stimulus have been 
moderate or intense. 

I have already shown, by actual experiment on nervous 


tissues themselves, that the differential excitability induced 


as an after-effect of moderate stimulus (memory-impression) 
will give rise, on diffuse stimulation, to one kind of response, 
and the after-effect induced by strong stimulus to the reverse 
(cf. figs. 311, 312). In the former case, the moderately 
stimulated area, on diffuse re-stimulation exhibits induced 
galvanometric negativity, as compared with the indifferent 
contact, this being the sign of its relatively greater excitation. 
In the second case, the sign of response is reversed, the 
over-stimulated area, on re-stimulation, becoming galvano- 
metrically positive. 

The revival of memory-images is thus seen to be due to 
differential response, evoked by diffuse stimulus, in an organ 
rendered anisotropic, by the unequal impressions which it 
contains of previous stimulation, A similar differential effect 
under diffuse stimulation has been seen in plagiotropic stems. 
Here the upper surface has a deep impression or memory of 
over-stimulating sunlight, and on diffuse stimulation this 
upper surface becomes galvanometrically positive, a respon- 
sive current flowing from below to above. It will thus be 
seen that there is a continuity between the impressions made 
on the sensitive neurile elements, and the physiological 
anisotropy induced by the differential action of past stimulus. 


686 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Diffuse stimulus, moreover, whether internal or external, 
acting on the differentially excitable tissue, gives rise to a 
marked indication, which may be either motile, electrical, or 
psychic. A stimulus is applied to the stem of JM/zmosa. 
This is transmitted as an excitatory impulse, and reaches the 
differentially excitable organ, the pulvinus. As-‘far as this 
organ is concerned, the transmitted stimulus may be re- 
garded as internal. This internal stimulus, then, gives rise 
to a conspicuous differential effect, shown in the fall of the 
leaf. In electrical fishes, similarly, the internal stimulus, 
delivered by the will of the animal upon the differentially 
excitable organ, becomes evident as an excitatory discharge 
In man, again, the revival of memory constitutes a psychic 
response, due to the play of the diffuse internal stimulus of 
will upon a sensitive surface rendered differentially excitable . 
by the presence of a latent image. 

It will thus be seen that various after-effects of stimulus 
find expression as the phenomena of memory. The effect 
of primary stimulus does not disappear at once, but fades 
gradually, with a concomitant fading of the sensory impres- 
sion. From the fact that the after-effect of feeble, is less 
persistent than that of strong, stimulus, we understand that 
the sensory or memory impression also lasts longer in the 
latter case than in the former. Very intense stimulation, 
again, is apt to give multiple responses as its after-effect, and 
the corresponding psycho-physiological phenomenon is seen 
in the recurrent after-images in the retina. 

When a considerable interval has elapsed after the 
primary stimulus, there is apparently no trace left of the 
latent image. But the properties of the impressed portions 
of the sensitive surface have undergone a more or less 
permanent change in consequence of stimulation. Certain 
channels have been rendered more conducting, and certain 
areas more excitable. By an internal diffuse impulse it 
is now possible to cause differential excitation, and thus to 
revivify the latent image. 


CHAPTER XLVI 
REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ISOTROPIC ORGANS 


Laws of response—Opposite responsive expressions of true excitation and 
increase of internal energy—-Separation of the positive and negative waves— 
Position in molecular cycle determines character of response—Abnormal 
sub-tonic positive and reversed fatigue positive—Effect of tetanisation— 
Similar effects in the inorganic—Phasic alternations—Multiple and auto- 
nomous response—Unmasking of antagonistic element by overshooting — 
Different expressions of a single fundamental molecular change —Response 
by change of form, by secretion or absorption, by variation of electric 
resistivity, or by electro-motive change—For the last, induction of anisotropy 
necessary—Perfect modes of stimulation : (a) Torsional vibration ; (¢) Rotary 
mechanical stimulation ; (c) Thermal shocks; (@) Equi-alternating electric 
shocks—Accurate determination of the death-point by mechanical and 
electrical spasms—Current of injury due to after-effect of stimulus— 
Explanation of characteristic electric distribution in plant and muscle 
cylinders—Relative positivity of dead tissue—Reversal of current of injury 
so-called—Unreliability of response by negative variation. 


IT has been shown, in the foregoing chapters, that all the 
diverse phenomena of response may be summarised in the 
two following formule : 

1, Excitatory response takes place by contraction and 
galvanometric negativity. 

2. Increase of internal energy induces the opposite effect, 
of expansion and galvanometric positivity. 

The first of these effects is simply demonstrated by direct 
excitation of an excitable tissue. In order to demonstrate 
the second, stimulus is applied at a distance from the 
responding point. In consequence jof sudden local con- 
traction at the receptive area, a wave of increased hydrostatic 
tension is transmitted with great rapidity. Energy is thus 
conveyed hydraulically, and at the distant responsive point 
the transmitted effect induces expansion and galyanometric 


688 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


positivity. This is followed by the more slowly transmitted 
wave of true excitation, which on its arrival gives rise to the 
normal response of contraction and galvanometric negativity. 
The two responsive effects can thus be exhibited separately, 
when one lags behind the other. When the intervening 
tract is short, or the conductivity great, the excitatory 
negativity masks the hydro-positive effect. But this hydro- 


positive may again be unmasked, by various forms of. 


selective physiological block, which depress the conduction 
of the true excitatory, without interfering to any appreciable 
extent with the passage of the hydraulic wave. In this way, 
the positive may be separated from the contained negative, 
the response being thus rendered diphasic— positive followed 
by negative. Or, by the complete suppression of the 
excitatory negative wave, a response originally negative may 
be converted into purely positive (figs. 45, 47, and 49). 

As response is an expression of molecular derangement, 
it is the extent of this which determines its amplitude. The 
character of response is also modified by the molecular 
condition of the responding substance, and the different 
molecular conditions through which a substance may pass 
are indicated by the characteristic curve. From the study of 
such a characteristic curve we find that these molecular 
transformations are not specific, but of general occurrence— 
alike in inorganic and living tissues. When the energy of 
the ‘responding substance is for any reason below par, that is 
to’ say, when it is in the extremely sub-tonic A condition, 
external stimulus will be absorbed without evoking the 
normal excitatory expression. Response will then be 
abnormal, or of opposite sign to the true excitatory effect. 
By the absorption of impinging stimulus the substance now 
passes into the next stage B, where molecular transformation 
proceeds at a rapid rate. At this stage, the previous 
abnormal response is not only reversed to normal, but 
successive responses exhibit a staircase increase. At the 
next or C stage, the responses are uniform. Following this, 
we arrive at the maximally distorted position D, Stimulus 


thininiaie, -_ ate ie 


~~ ee oe 7 


SE ——EE—E———=—— se  CUreO 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ISOTROPIC ORGANS 689 | 


at this stage induces little further excitatory distortion, while 
the tendency to recovery is great. In this fatigue-state the 
amplitude of response undergoes a decline, and in. the 
succeeding stage E an actual reversal. : 

_ The various corresponding types of response—sub-tonic 
abnormal, staircase, uniform, fatigue-decline, and fatigue- 
reversal—are not exhibited by any one particular kind, but 
by all forms of tissues. Thus muscle may exhibit a short- 
lived staircase effect, and nerve, supposed to be indefatigable, 
not only shows decline, but even reversal of normal response, 
under extreme fatigue. 

There are two definite conditions ander which the normal 
negative response is converted into abnormal positive, with 
an intermediate diphasic. These are (1) the condition of 
extreme sub-tonicity, and (2) that of fatigue brought on by 
over-stimulation. As regards the first, it is to be remem- 
bered that the normal excitability of a tissue is maintained 
by the supply of energy from the rest of the organism of 


which it forms a part. Under isolation, the latent energy or 


tonic condition of the tissue is liable to fall below par, under 
which circumstances the response becomes abnormal positive. 
By the absorption of the energy of stimulus, the substance is 
transformed from the A to the B condition, with restoration 
of its normal response. The process of gradual .trans- 
formation may be seen in a series of records to successive 
stimuli, when the abnormal gradually passes into the normal, 
through an intermediate diphasic.. Or, an intervening 
tetanisation will serve to convert response from the abnormal 
to the normal. Abnormal or reversed response is also seen 
to occur under fatigue, but its genesis in the molecular curve 
is here in reversed order to that of the abnormal response of 
sub-tonicity. In the latter, during the continuous trans- 
formation from the A to the © phase, stimulation converted 
the abnormal response into normal, through diphasic. But 
now, during transformation induced by stimulus from C to E, 
the normal negative passes into abnormal positive, through 
intermediate diphasic. To transform the abnormal positive 
¥-Y¥ 


690 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO- PHYSIOLOGY 


into normal negative in the first case, stimulation is necessary. 
To do the samein the second case, rest is necessary. In 
the records obtained from different animal tissues, various 
anomalies are met with, of which there has not hitherto been 
any satisfactory explanation. Thus the same tissue at 
different times will be found to give either the normal 
negative, or di-phasic, or abnormal positive response. Thus 
it has been shown that in the two extreme cases alike, of sub-. 
tonicity and fatigue, the response of nerve is abnormal positive 
(p. 636). Numerous other examples of this fact have been met 
with in the course of this work, in, for example, the response 
of skin (p. 311), that of the glandular and digestive organs 
(pp. 342, 346), and that of retina (p. 423). It will thus be 
seen how important is the molecular condition of the tissue 
in determining the nature of response. This is strikingly 
shown in the fact that the same tetanisation which in the A 
condition converts the abnormal to normal, in the D will 
convert the normal into abnormal. Again, if tetanisation be 
applied at the beginning of the B stage, the subsequent re- 
sponses are enhanced, whereas the same tetanisation at the 
_end of C induces a fatigue reversal (figs. 394, 395, and 398). 
That the explanation of these various results is to be 
sought for in molecular considerations, and not in that hypo- 
thetical assimilation and dissimilation which really explain 
nothing, is fully demonstrated by the fact that precisely 
similar responsive variations are obtained, in the same cir- 
cumstances, in the case of inorganic matter, under different 
forms of stimulus and different methods of record. As an 
example, may be cited the transformation of abnormal 
response into normal, in tungsten, after tetanisation (fig. 
391), the stimulus employed being electric radiation, and 
the mode of record, resistivity-variation. Parallel effects have 
been shown in the case of tin, the response being recorded 
by the electro-motive variation, and the stimulus employed 
mechanical (fig. 386). The enhancement of normal response 
also under tetanisation, when at the B stage, has been shown 
in tin (fig. 388); and finally, the reversal of normal response 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ISOTROPIC ORGANS 691 


by fatigue was shown in tungsten under electric radiation, 
while in the contractile response of indiarubber under thermal 
stimulation it took place with intermediate diphasic: (fig. 
397). The characteristic curve has been shown to exhibit 
the history of molecular transformation under continuous 
stimulation. In the first part of this curve a progressive 
change is shown to be manifested outwardly by increasing 
contraction or galvanometric negativity. In the second part 
a reversal of this process is seen to occur. This is illustrated 
in records of response under continuous stimulation. Thus 
muscle shows increasing contraction, to be followed by 
fatigue-relaxation (fig. 64). The same thing is observed in 
Mimosa, as a fall of the leaf, followed by its re-erection 
(fig. 65). Electrically, this is observed as increasing galvano- 
metric negativity, followed by reversal to positivity. These 
phasic alternations may in some cases be exhibited only 
once, and in others repeatedly. Thus, in a certain style of 
Datura such phasic alternation is seen to occur twice (fig. 
76); and again, in leaflets of Desmodium gyrans, at first 
quiescent, continuous stimulus of light gives rise to those 
repeated alternations of negative and positive which consti- 
tute multiple response (fig. 141). The distinction between 
the tissue which gives only one such alternation, and others 
which display it in repeated succession, is not, it should be 
borne in mind, rigid. Even skeletal muscle, under certain 
circumstances,.is found to give rise to rhythmic excitations. 
The fact that multiple response is a phenomenon of wide- 
spread occurrence, and not specifically characteristic of any 
particular kind of tissue, has been fully demonstrated in the 
course of the present work, 

It would appear that there is a tendency of the incident 
stimulus, when applied continuously, to find an expression 
whose predominant characteristics are alternating. We may 
first have the exhibition of excitatory molecular distortion. 
But when this has reached a maximum, no further excitatory 
expression being possible, the incident energy becomes 
relatively effective in increasing the internal factor,- with 

YY2 


692 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


attendant expansion, galvanometric positivity, and enhanced 
power of recovery. At the maximum point—that is to say, 
at the top of the tetanic curve—the two forces are balanced ; 
and at this point, if the stimulus be suddenly withdrawn, the 
particular state of unstable balance is often manifested by a 
brief overshooting in one or other direction. This effect’ is 
often noticed in the retina and in certain vegetable structures 


under the action of light (figs. 244, 247, 254, and 260), and 


in nerve under electric tetanisation (p. 536). That it is not 
primarily dependent on assimilation and dissimilation, but 
on the molecular factor, is seen in the fact that similar effects 
are also to be observed, under corresponding circumstances, 
in the response of inorganic substances (figs. 258 and 383). 


The phasic molecular alternation so conspicuously ex- 


hibited under continuous stimulation may also be seen in the 
record of responses to successive stimuli. . The phenomenon 
is then regarded as an after-effect and shown by the shifting 
of the base-line of the record (figs. 208 and 396). 

Since the effect of stimulus is to induce a molecular 
upset, the change in question must be attended by various 
concomitant physical changes. It will therefore be possible 
to record the excitatory effect by recording the attendant 
variations of any one of these. The effect of stimulus may 
thus be recorded by (1) the accompanying change of form, 
in: contraction or expansion; (2) an attendant secretion or 
absorption; (3) a variation of electric resistivity by dimi- 
nution or increase of resistance; and (4) electro-motive 


changes of galvanometric negativity or positivity. The 


changes in the responding substance as a whole, may be 
recorded by any one of the first three methods. But in the 
last, or that by the electro-motive variation, the method 
depends on the relative variations of the electric potential at 
two different points. -For if the substance be isotropic and 


subjected to diffuse stimulation, the electro-motive change at 


the two contacts being similar, there will be no resultant 
effect to record. For the recording of electro-motive response, 
then, it is necessary to obtain an effect which is differential. 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ISOTROPIC ORGANS 693 


The first way of doing this is to localise the stimulus at one 
of the two contacts. This may be done by interposing a 
physiological block between the two, so that the excitation 
of one does not reach the other. The second method is to 
select an experimental specimen which is anisotropic, whether 
naturally or artificially. Artificial anisotropy is induced by 
injuring one of the two contacts, and so bringing about a 
relative depression of excitability at that point. 

The method of resistivity variation which I had pre- 
viously employed, in observing the response of inorganic 
substances, proved capable of sufficient perfectibility for the 
study of similar phenomena in living tissues also. The 
main difficulty in applying this method had hitherto lain 
in the disturbing electro-motive variation, consequent on 
a-symmetrical excitation, or the differential excitability 
of the structure. A detailed account of the means by 
which this method was rendered reliable will be found in 


' Chapter XXXVII., where it will be seen that records of 


the excitatory variation obtained by it are in every way 
similar, to those made by other methods. All the different 
modes of taking records which have been enumerated are, it 
must be remembered, independent expressions of a common 
fundamental molecular change. Thus, on physically restrain- 
ing that mechanical movement in a motile organ which. is 
due to excitatory change, the electromotive response of 
galvanometric negativity continues to be given. Similarly, 
in a tissue in which, under the experimental arrangements, 
there can be no resultant electro-motive change and no con- 
tractile movement, the excitatory change may, nevertheless, 
be observed by means of the resistivity variation (p. 548). 

For the obtaining of the electromotive response, the 
electrical mode of stimulation, unless special precautions 
are taken, is subject to various disturbing influences, such as 
current-escape and the occurrence of polarisation. For this 
reason it was desirable to devise sore non-electrical form of 
stimulation which should be capable of quantitative appli- 
cation ; and this I have been able to secure by no less than 


604 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


three distinct methods. I found that torsional to-and-fro 
vibration constituted an effective form of stimulus, the 
amplitude of which could be increased by increasing the 
angle of vibration. The intensity of stimulus was found 
to remain constant so long as the period and amplitude 
vibration were kept constant. The tissue, moreover, was 
not subject to injury by the use, within limits, of this method 


(p. 31). My second method was that of Rotary Mechanical 


Stimulation, in which friction of the terminal area of a pumice- 
stone electrode constituted the stimulus, whose intensity was 
determined by the number of rotations (p. 291). The third 
non-electrical mode of stimulation employed was that of 
thermal shocks. The area to be stimulated was, in this case, 
enclosed within a thermal loop of platinum or german-silver 
wire, the requisite thermal variation being produced by the 
passage of a heating electrical current round the loop. The 
intensity of the stimulus could in this case be varied by 
increasing the intensity or duration of the heating current 
(p. 38). And finally I have shown that the drawbacks inci- 
dental to the electrical mode of stimulation might be over- 
come by the use of equi-alternating shocks, the indefinite 
polarisation factor being thus neutralised (p. 251). 

As the intensity of stimulus is gradually increased, it 
is found that the amplitude of response reaches a limit. 
Beyond this, increase of stimulus evokes no increase of 
response. On the application of a very strong stimulus, 
then, there is an amount of energy which is unable to 
find expression in the single response given by the tissue. 
Under such circumstances, the excess of energy is held 
latent, and often finds responsive expression later in a 
series of multiple responses. This phenomenon of multiple 
response to a single strong stimulus I find to be of very 
extensive occurrence. As examples of the different kinds of 
tissues in which this may be observed, may be mentioned 
the stems and petioles of various plants (fig. 138), the 
digesting leaves of Drosera (fig. 209), the pitcher of Mepenthe 


heh Bt BES ST 


=e ee ee 


See ae ae SE ESE RR Py a ny eS Sate iaike 


=i fet 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ISOTROPIC ORGANS 695 


(fig. 206), the animal stomach (fig. 213), nerves of animals, 
and the retina (fig. 252). 

I have also shown that thers is no strict line of 
demarcation between the phenomena of such multiple 
response and autonomous response so-called. Bzophytum, for 
example, which, usually speaking, exhibits a single response 
to a single moderate stimulus, and multiple response to a 
strong stimulus, will, under exceptionally favourable tonic 
conditions—that is to say, when it has absorbed from its 
surroundings an excess of energy—exhibit responses which 
are apparently autonomous. A typically autonomous plant 
like Desmodium gyranus, again, when deprived by unfavour- 
able circumstances of that excess of energy which it 
requires, will be reduced to the condition of a multiply 
responding plant. merely. It then responds by a single 
response to moderate, and by multiple responses to strong, 
stimulus. When the energy imparted by strong stimulus is 
exhausted, these multiple responses come to a stop, to be 
once more renewed, on a fresh accession of strong stimulus. 
Or a lateral leaflet of Desmodium, originally quiescent, may 
be put into, and maintained in, a state of pulsation by the 
action of sunlight. 

It is from the stored-up energy derived from _ its 
surroundings that the tonic condition of the plant is so 
raised as to maintain its so-called autonomous activity. 
From this it will be seen that, strictly speaking, there is no 
such thing as automatism. Movement can only be produced 
by the immediate action of stimulus, or by energy previously 
absorbed. 

In recording the autonomous pulsation of the lateral 
leaflets of Desmodium gyrans, it is found that while the 
down-movement brought about by the contractile action of 
the lower half of the pulvinule is very rapid, the up-move- 
ment due to recovery, and to contraction of the upper half 
of the organ, is relatively slow. The two alternating 
excitatory impulses, in the lower and upper halves respec- 
tively, are in the ratio approximately of 1°5 to 1, This 


696 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


explains the peculiar electrical responses of Desmodium 
gyrans, which are concomitant with the autonomous 
mechanical pulsations of the leaflet. I find that, corre- 
sponding with one complete mechanical pulsation, there are 
two electrical pulses. Of these the principal electrical wave 
coincides with the down-movement of the leaflet, and the 
smaller with the up. The electro-motive intensity of the 


principal wave is nearly 1°5 times that of the subsidiary. | 


In a particular experiment, for example, while the value of 
the former was ‘0024 volt, that of the latter was, ‘oo16 volt. 
These electro-motive variations are expressions of funda- 
mental excitatory effects, and not dependent on the 
mechanical movement of the leaflets. For when the 
responding leaflet is physically restrained, the electro-motive 
responses exhibit even greater intensity than before. This 
will be seen in the simultaneous records of mechanical and 
electrical pulsations given in fig. 145. 

An important subject of inquiry lay in the accurate 
determination of the death-point. This investigation afforded 
striking demonstration of the fact that it is a single 
excitatory reaction which is expressed in different ways 
under different modes of record. It has been shown that 
when the experimental tissue is subjected to a gradual rise 
of temperature, there is a definite point at which an 
excitatory spasm occurs, marking the initiation of death. 
If a continuous record be taken of the concomitant variation 
of length, increasing expansion is found to be converted at 
this point into a sudden contraction. In an anisotropic 
organ like the pulvinus of Mzmosa the erectile movement of 
the leaf is abruptly transformed into one of fall. A curled 
tendril exhibits at this point a sudden uncurling. Taking, 
again, the electro-motive method of record for the detection 
of the death-point, the increasing positivity of .the 
specimen is spasmodically reversed to negativity. Finally, 
on employing for the record the method of resistivity 
variation the increasing is seen to become suddenly 
changed into a diminishing resistance. It is found,. how- 


OS ae ee 


ee ae oe 


a ee ee ie 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ISOTROPIC ORGANS 697 — 


ever, employing numerous specimens, that these mechanical 
and electrical spasms take place, under normal conditions, 
at the same point. In the case of phanerogamous plants, 
this is found to be at or very near 60° C. (figs, 328, 329, 
330). That these mechanical and electrical spasms, further, 
constitute a true case of excitatory response, is proved 
by the fact that induced physiological depression also 
induces depression of the death-point. Fatigue may thus 
lower the death-point by as much as 19° C. : 

The response of contraction, initiated at the death-point, 
is later converted into fost-mortem relaxation, and galvano- 
metrically the negativity initiated at the same moment 
becomes subsequently a post-mortem positivity. 

With regard to the so-called Current of Injury it. was 
shown that this arises as the after-effect of strong stimulus. 
It should be remembered that a cut, or the application of a 
heated wire, constituting mechanical and thermal sections 
respectively, will act as a strong stimulus, and, further, that 
the after-effect of excitatory galvanometric. negativity is 
persistent when the stimulus is strong.. The excitatory 
effect, moreover, is transmitted from the point of application 
to greater or less distances, according to the strength of 
stimulus and the conductivity of the tissue. As this trans- 
mitted effect undergoes diminution with distance, it is 
obvious that the most intense negativity will be induced at 
the point of section, undergoing a gradual diminution as 
we move further away from it. If, taking a given length of 
isotropic tissue, we make two opposite terminal sections, we 
shall clearly have a symmetrical. distribution of electrical 
potential as regards the middle or equatorial zone, the two 
ends being most negative, while the equator is relatively 
most positive (fig. 110). . Two points symmetrically situated 
as regards this equator would thus be equi-potential, while 
a-symmetrical points would show appropriate differences of 
potential, a zone near the equator being relatively positive 
to one which is further away from it, or nearer to the 
terminal section. These considerations, supported as they 


698 © COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


are by experimental results, account satisfactorily for the 
particular electrical distribution in a muscle-cylinder. 

It is often supposed that dead tissue is negative to 
living. But I have shown that this is not the case, the 
dead being actually positive to the living. It has already 
been mentioned, in connection with experiments described, 
on the mechanical and electrical spasms of death, that at 


the initiation of death, a tissue exhibits excitatory con-. 


traction and negativity, while the post-mortem effect is one 
of relaxation and positivity. This explains the peculiar 
electrical distribution which I have observed, in the explora- 
tion of tissues, of which some parts were dead, others 
dying, and still others, again, fully alive. It was there shown 
(figs. 113, 115) that the greatest negativity occurred on the 
death-frontier. Proceeding in either direction from this 
point, whether towards the living or towards the dead, 
it is found that these points are increasingly positive, or 
decreasingly negative. But the maximum positivity of the 
dead portion is greater than that of the living. From this 
it is clear that the dead is positive to the living. 

From these facts, that the dying is negativé, and the 
dead relatively positive to the living, it is clear that the 
so-called current of injury is liable to reversal. In the case 
of the former, the current of injury will be from the 
dying to the living; in the latter, from the living to the 
dead. This demonstration of the occurrence of a hitherto 
unsuspected reversal, demonstrates to us the possibility of 
many complications, and wrong theoretical inferences, For 
response by the negative variation of the current of injury 
is usually taken as the concomitant of the chemical process 
of dissimilation, while the positive variation is held to be 
associated with assimilation. Now, by the reversal of the 
so-called current of injury, one identical excitatory reaction 
may be made to appear, now as a negative, and again 
as a positive variation. This is. sufficient to indicate the 
unreliability of the so-called Method of Negative Variation, 


ane. tae 


a 


Orr ee ee 


A Se Eee 


SUAS Io ate fi arses 


eS ES SNR NY A A I NE A AR RR 
rT _ * Te a 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ISOTROPIC ORGANS 699 _ 


an unreliability of which we shall further meet with many 
glaring instances. Pe ; 

An assumption more or less current is, that in order to 
obtain response, there must be an antecedent current, by 
whose negative variation it can be detected. Hence the 
supposed necessity of a current of injury prior to response. 
The real reason, however, for thus injuring one of the 
contacts is so to depress its excitability that, on diffuse 
stimulation, the excitatory response of the uninjured may 
remain unbalanced, and therefore unannulled. That it is 
this depression of excitability, and not the current of injury 
as such, which is the essential condition for obtaining 
resultant response, is seen from the fact that excitatory 
response may still be obtained, even when the so-called 
current of injury is zero, or reversed positive (fig. 116). 


CHAPTER XLVII 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ANISOTROPIC ORGANS 


Anisotropic organs—Laws of response in anisotropic organs—Natural current of 
rest and current of response— Reversal of natural current of rest-—Unreliability 
of positive and negative variations of current of rest-—Determination of the 
differential excitability of a tissue—Resultant response of skin due to induced 
stronger negativity of inner surface and feebler negativity (tomato skin) or 
positivity (skins of grape and frog) of outer—Response of intact human skin — 
Response of intact human lip—High excitability of secretory and glandular 
surfaces—Response of glandular foot of snail—Response of intact human 
tongue—Response of digestive organs—Phasic alternations of secretion and 
absorption—Multiple response of digestive organs—Phasic changes induced 
by previous activity—Response of digestive organs of Mepenthe and Drosera 
—Electro-motive peculiarities of skin and mucous coat of stomach not similar 
—Normal response by galvanometric negativity in mucous coat of stomach of 
frog, gecko, and tortoise— The root as a digestive organ—Excitatory secretion 
and galvanometric negativity of young roots—Phasic alternations of secretion 
and absorption — Cognate subject of ascent of sap—.Sap-wood not really dead— 
Proofs of physiological character of suctional response—Water-movement a 
mode of excitatory response—Response of electrical organs—Two types, 
Torpedo and Malepterurus—Vegetal analogues to electric plates of two types, 
Torpedo and Malepterurus, in Pterospermum and pitcher of MWepenthe—- 
Multiple character of response of electric organs—Response of electrical 
organs constitutes an extreme case of differential excitability of anisotropic 
structures--Similar effects with inorganic structures—Excitatory effect of 
light on plant tissues—Phasic alternations—Initiation of multiple and autono- 
mous response by light—Three types of direct and after-effects—Response of 
retina like, and not different from, that of other tissues—Error introduced by 
method of negative variation—Multiple responses in retina and their visual 
correspondences —Binocular Alternation of Vision—Three types of direct and 
after-effects in retina under light—Geo-electric response. 


I SHALL next pass in review another class of phenomena, 
the want of a clear understanding of which is at the root of 
many supposed anomalies in the response of animal tissues. 
I allude to the natural anisotropy, with consequent differential 
excitability, of various organs, 


i 


re 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ANISOTROPIC ORGANS /7oT ~ 


_ As an example of a differentially excitable organ we may 
take the pulvinus of J/tmosa, in which the lower half is more 
excitable than the upper. In this case, strictly localised 
stimulation of either the upper or the lower evokes con- 
traction and galvanometric negativity of that particular half, 
the effect in the lower half being the greater. But if the 
stimulus be diffused, whether internally or externally, the 
response will be differential, by the greater contraction or 
galvanometric negativity of the more excitable. From this 
we arrive at the general law of the electrical response of 
anisotropic organs. 

1. On simultaneous excitation of two points A and B, the 
responsive current flows in the tissue from the more to the less 


~ excited. 


2. Conversely, if under simultaneous excitation, the responsive 
current be from B to A, B is the more excitable of these two 
points. 

The second of these two laws enables us to determine 
the relative excitabilities of any two points. As a simple 
example of the anisotropy induced in a tissue by the unequal 
action of the natural stimuli of the enviroment, we may take 
a tubular organ, such as the hollow peduncle of Uriclis lily. 
Here the exposed outer surface, constantly subjected to 
external stimuli such as light, becomes as it were fatigued, 
and reduced in excitability. Other histological modifications 
follow on this, the external cells becoming thus cuticularised 
and protoplasmically defective. Owing to the depression of 
excitability on this epidermal surface, the intensity of its 
normal excitatory change by galvanometric negativity is 
decreased, a change which, in the case of certain skins, 
culminates in responsive positivity. The inner surface of the 
hollow peduncle, which may be regarded as epithelial, being, 
on the other hand, protected, remains normally excitable 
and is thus more so than the outer surface. The outer sur- 
face, however, probably by reason of the action of the external 
stimuli to which it is constantly exposed, is naturally 
negative, relatively to the protected and more excitable inner 


702 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


surface. And it will generally be found true that while this 
natural cnrrent of rest is from the less excitable A to the 
more excitable B, the current of response, on the other hand, 
which occurs on excitation, is from the potentially more 
excitable, and therefore now more excited B, to the less 
excitable and therefore less excited A. 

Such is the course of events in the normal or primary 
condition. But under the excitation due to preparation, or - 
accidental disturbance, the more excitable surface becomes 
the more excited, and relatively to the other, galvanometri- 
cally negative. In consequence of this, the natural current 
is reversed, and we have a resting-current due to the after- 
effect of injury or accidental excitation, flowing from the 
more to the less excitable. Thus, while the natural current, 
in the primary condition, was from the less excitable A to 
the more excitable A, that is to say, A > B, this reversed 
current of rest, due to accidental excitation or injury, is from 
B->A. Even now, however, & may be more excitable 
than- A, hence fresh stimulation will induce a responsive 
current from B to A. In the primary condition, such a 
responsive current would have appeared as a negative varia- 
tion of the natural current AZ. But when the primary 
condition has been so modified that the natural current is 
reversed, and has become B-A, the normal responsive 
current B-+A will appear as a positive variation. Still 
another variation is possible, when the normal response itself 
undergoes reversal owing to fatigue, under which condition 
this abnormal response, relatively to the reversed current of 
rest, appears as if it were the normal negative variation 
(fig. 119). It has, however, been shown that if we discard 
this unreliable test of response, by the variation induced in 
an antecedent current of rest—the so-called negative varia- 
tion—it will be found that the responsive current always 
flows from the more to the less excited. 

In order to determine which of two points in an anisotropic 
tissue is the more excitable, it is necessary, as now under- 
stood, to determine the direction of resultant response, under 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ANISOTROPIC ORGANS 703 


stimulation which is equal and simultaneous. In order to do 
this, we may employ such a non-electrical form of stimu- 
lation as the mechanical or the thermal. For this it is 
possible to employ (1) the Vibratory Stimulator; (2) the 
Rotary Mechanical Stimulator ; or (3) stimulation by thermal 
shocks. When results are obtained according to these 
methods, there can be no uncertainty as to those compli- 
cations of effects which might conceivably arise when the 
electrical form of stimulus is employed. The last-named 
may, however, be used without misgiving, when stimu- 
lation is effected by equi-alternating shocks, The ordinary 
Ruhmkorff’s make- and break-shocks are not suitable for 
this purpose, inasmuch as the effective intensity is unequa] 
for make and break, besides which the polarisation-effect may 
not be exactly neutralised. The equi-alternating shocks, 
from which these defects have been eliminated, are obtained 
by means of (1) a rotary reverser in the primary coil (fig. 170), 
or (2) a motor-dynamo (fig. 172). The responses again, 
under these electrical forms of stimulation, may be photo- 
graphically recorded as either the direct or the after-effect of 
stimulus. It was shown, by the employment of all these 
various methods of stimulus, mechanical, thermal, and elec- 
trical, that the responsive current to be obtained with an 
anisotropic organ was definite in direction, being always, 
under normal conditions, from the more excitable B to the 
less excitable A. I shall now proceed to recapitulate briefly 
the results obtained by these methods in various cases of 
anisotropic tissues, such as skin, epithelium, glands, animal, 
and vegetal digestive organs, and electric organs generally. 
Taking first the skin of tomato it has been shown that 
the separate responses of the outer and inner surfaces are 
unequal. The outer, owing to cellular modification under 
the stimuli of the environment, gives only a feeble negative 
response, whereas the internal surface gives a much stronger 
normal response by galvanometric negativity. On simul- 
taneous excitation of both inner and outer surfaces, the 
responsive current is found to flow from the inner to the 


704 ~ COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


outer. Here the resultant’ current’ is ‘brought about~ by 
the difference between the stronger responsive negativity of 
the inner, and the feebler responsive negativity of the outer 


surface, which © may be represented as” 4 With certain 


specimens of tomato skin, however, the modification of the 
outer surface is so great that its individual response is 
reversed to positive, that of the inner being the normal 


strong negative. The resultant response, then, is still from 


inner to outer, but equals the summated effect of the two ne 
From this we pass to the response of grape-skin, which 
resembles the latter of these two cases. The response of 
the skin of frog is also of this type, and it may be said 
of skins in general that their response is from the. more 
excitable inner surface to the less excitable outer. This 
conclusion has been verified by experiments on various 
skins, both vegetable and animal. Among the latter of these 
may be mentioned the skin of the neck-of tortoise, and that 
from various parts of the body of gecko.- 

When the skin is isolated with very great care, so as to 
reduce to a minimum the excitatory effect of preparation, it 
is found-that the natural current of rest is from the less 
excitable outer to the more excitable inner surface; the 
excitatory current being in,the opposite direction. Owing to 
the excitatory effect of preparation; the current of rest of the 
skin of tortoise-was found reversed. The responsive current, 
however, was found to flow from inner to outer, thus proving 
that the inner surface was the more excitable. In illustration 
of the great practicability of the methods employed, I may 
refer to the photographic records obtained of the response of 
the skin of the intact human forefinger (fig. 180). . 

In describing the differential excitability of the hollow 
peduncle of Uviclts lily, it was shown that protected surfaces 
are, as a rule, more excitable than those which are exposed, 
and have thus undergone a greater degree of modification. 
On taking the plagiotropic stem of Cucurbita, the lower 
surface of which is protected from light, it is. found that, 


apt peiy 


a SS ee |) h6U lee 


Le ee ee ae oe 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ANISOTROPIC ORGANS 705 


while the current of rest flows from the exposed upper to 
the protected lower surface, the direction of the responsive 
current is opposite, namely from the lower to the upper, 
proving that the protected lower is the more excitable of 
the two. Similarly, in the case of the intact human lip, I 
found that the resting current was from the epidermal to 
the epithelial, the responsive current being in the opposite 
direction (fig. 196). . Again, on testing the differential 
response of armpit and shoulder, I found that the respon- 
sive current was from armpit to shoulder, the former being 
thus the more excitable of the two (fig. 194). 

We have seen that the lining membrane of the inner 
surface of the peduncle of Uvric/zs lily is very thin, and that, 
in distinction to the outer or epidermal membrane, it may be 
regarded as epithelial. As we approach the bulb-end of the 
peduncle, this inner layer of cells is found to be highly turgid, 
and secretion is found to take place into the hollow tube. 
The inner surface of the carpellary leaf of Dzllenta indica, 
again, secretes a mucilaginous substance. In these two cases 
there are no definite glands, But definite glands are found to 
occur on the inside of the pitcher of Wepenthe. In all these 
cases the secreting layer, whether provided with glands or 
not, is found to be very highly excitable, and to respond 
by strong galvanometric negativity. Taking a carpel of 
Ditllenia indica, it is found that the natural current is from 
the outer epidermal to the inner secreting surface, the respon- 
sive current being in the opposite direction. On making very 
careful connections, with the skin of the protruded body of 
the snail, and the glandular under-surface of its foot, it is 
found that the natural current is from the non-glandular to 
the glandular, but the responsive current from glandular to 
non-glandular. As an example of the way in which the 
true natural current of rest may be reversed by the excitatory 
effect of preparation, I showed that, while in the intact snail 
the natural current was from non-glandular to glandular— 
the gland being in this case relatively positive, to the extent 
of -0013 volt—after the sectioning of the foot, the original 

ZZ 


706 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


natural current was reversed, owing to the greater relative 
excitation induced at the glandular surface, which now 
became relatively negative, to the extent of —-‘0020 volt. 
With the intact human tongue, further, I found that a 
very strong responsive current was induced on excitation, 
from the lower to the upper surface, thus showing that the 
lower was the more excitable of the two. 

The response of digestive organs may now be passed in 
review. In these, as in glandular organs, excitatory response 
is supposed to take place by secretion. In connection with 
this, it must be borne in mind that in the tissue of the 
pulvinus of J/zmosa, on the removal of the impervious skin, 
excitation induces secretion of the contained fluid, which, 
again, is re-absorbed on the cessation of excitation. We 
know the pulvinus to be contractile, and may therefore 
regard this secretion as an effect of contraction, causing 
expulsion of water. Apart from the differential action of 
the upper and lower halves of the organ, and the magnifying 
petiolar index, the fundamental contractile action would, in 
the case of J/zmosa, as in others, have passed unnoticed. 
This goes to show that it is not impossible that the phe- 
- nomenon of secretion through a permeable membrane may 
be associated with excitatory contraction. In favour of such 
continuity, it may be urged that tissues, hitherto regarded as 
non-motile, have been shown to exhibit excitatory contrac- 
tion. In digestion, as a whole, we have to recognise two 
different processes, those, namely, of secretion, and of sub- 
sequent absorption. Parallel to these, we find that the 
electrical response of digestive organs exhibits phasic alter- 
nations of negativity and positivity. 

It was shown that the pitcher of Wepenthe—which may 
be regarded as an open stomach—affords us unique facilities 
for the observation of the normal responses of digestive 
organs. In experimenting with the animal stomach, the 
specimen has to be cut open, in order to make the necessary 
connections ; and, owing to the highly excitable character of 
the organ, this gives rise to intense excitatory action, the 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ANISOTROPIC ORGANS 707 | 


after-effect of which is necessarily to reverse the normal 
current of rest, With a pitcher of Mepenthe in a fresh 
condition, the natural current of rest is from the outer to 
the inner, the responsive current being in the opposite 
direction, and the glandular surface, on simultaneous exci- 
tation of the two, becoming galvanometrically negative 
(fig. 203). Digestive organs, moreover, tend to -exhibit 
multiple responses, the response to a single strong stimulus, 
say thermal, or of mechanical section, consisting, whether in 
animal or vegetable organs, of a series that may persist for 
nearly an hour (figs. 206, 209, and 213), When the pitcher 
of Nepenthe contains a large number of captured flies, that is 
to say, when it has been subjected to long-continued stimu- 
lation, it exhibits a phasic change, the responses now 
becoming reversed to positive (fig. 205). This, as pointed 
out above, is probably significant of absorption. In Drosera, 
the normal response of the glandular surface is by induced 
negativity, but on long-continued stimulation, this is reversed 
to positivity (fig. 208). 

In the animal stomach, the sine current of rest is 
generally from the glandular to the non-glandular surface. 
From the fact that the skin of the toad, which is also 
possessed of imbedded glands, gives a current of rest from 
the outer surface to the inner, it has been supposed that the 
mucous coat of the stomach of the frog had the same electro- 
motive reaction as its outer skin, That this, however, is not 
the case is seen from the fact that on excitation the skin 
becomes galvanometrically positive, while the mucous mem- 
brane of the stomach becomes galvanometrically negative. 
The observed current of rest in the stomach would appear, 
from WVepenthe, to be, not the natural current of rest, but the 
reversed current, due to the excitatory effect of preparation. 
The normal effect of excitation in the stomach, I uniformly 
find, in such different instances as frog, gecko, and tortoise, to 
be by galvanometric negativity of the mucous surface (figs. 
210, 211, and 212). On applying a strong thermal stimulus 


to the stomach of frog, I obtained an interesting series of 
ZZ2 


708 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


responses, of which the first were negative, the second part 
diphasic, and the last portion reversed positive (fig. 213). 
Looking at the phenomenon of digestion, we see that it 
consists first of a secretory process, by which certain solid 
substances are dissolved, and secondly of the absorption of 
these dissolved substances. Similar functions are subserved 
in vegetable life by the root, by which solid inorganic food- 


materials are first dissolved by secreted acids, and then 


absorbed. The proof of the former is seen in the well- 
known corrosion-figures produced by growing rootlets on a 
marble surface. I have also been able to demonstrate the 
phenomenon of excitatory secretion in young roots by allow- 
ing them to absorb dilute salt solution, and then under exci- 
tation to secrete it into highly dilute silver nitrate solution: 
This last was attended by the visible formation of a white 
precipitate. The electrical response of young roots of 
Colocasia, moreover, I found to be by induced galvanometric 
negativity (fig. 214), which, under long-continued stimulation, 
was apt to show reversal to positivity. The older roots, on 
the other hand, under the same intensity of stimulation, gave 
response by galvanometric positivity (fig. 215). The former 
of these responses, there is every reason to believe, is as- 
sociated with secretion, and the latter with absorption. 

This question of the absorption of inorganic food materials 
by the plant is naturally connected with the subject of the 
Ascent of Sap, which is regarded as one of the most difficult 
problems in plant physiology. The non-physiological theories 
advanced are admittedly inadequate to the explanation of 
this phenomenon. That the ascent, nevertheless, could not 
be due to physiological action was held to have been proved 
by the facts (1) that water-conduction takes place pre- 
ferentially through sap-wood, assumed to be dead ; and (2) 
that poisonous solutions, such as would kill a living tissue, 
have been found to be transported through the roots, or the 
cut ends of their trunks, to the tops of trees. 

I have, however, been able to show that these objections 
are not valid. For in the first place, the supposed dead 


me PET, 


mer ae ee a ed 


aa eee ee se ee ee 


i % 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ANISOTROPIC ORGANS 709 | 


wood, concerned in the transport of sap, through the trunks 
of trees, can be proved, by electrical tests, to be fully 
alive. This living wood responds to stimulation by in- 
duced galvanometric negativity, such response disappearing 
on the death of the tissue, as, say by drying, after which 
it cannot be restored. The response of living wood is 
also depressed by anesthetics, and abolished by poisons 
(figs. 216, 217). As regards the argument based on the 
transport of poison, it has been shown that as the active 
elements concerned in the transport of sap are diffused 
throughout the length of the trunk, the death of one indi- 
vidual zone, to which the poison has ascended, does not 
abolish the suctional activity of the zone above. It is only 
when the plant has been killed throughout, by the arrival 
of the solution at its top, that the complete arrest of suction 
could be expected to take place. And this is found to be 
the case. Various agents, on the other hand, which are 
known to induce changes, whether of exaltation or depres- 
sion, in the physiological activity, are found to induce corre- 
sponding modifications in the rate of suction. A very 
delicate means of investigating this question has been 
shown to be that afforded by the records obtained with 
the Shoshungraph (fig. 218). Here, under the ordinary 
method of record, the slope of the curve indicates the 
normal rate of suction, and the effect of various agencies 
is immediately shown by the resulting flexure of the curve. 
This method of record, again, becomes extremely sensitive, 
when it is carried out under balanced conditions. By means 
of these records, it has been shown that depressing agents, 
such as cold or anesthetics, depress or arrest suction, 
whereas warmth exalts it. It has been shown, further, that 
just as the multiple activity of the Desmodium leaflet is 
arrested, when the latent energy of the plant falls below 
par, so also, under similar circumstances, the suctional activity 
falls into abeyance, and that, as in the one case, so also in 
the other, the activity is renewed, by the application of an 
external stimulus. It has also been shown that the latent 


710 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


period which elapses, before the initiation of this responsive 
variation to external stimulus, is longer when the plant is in 
a sub-tonic condition than in the same plant when its tonic 
condition has been slightly raised by previous stimulation. 
Crucial experiments, finally, have been described, showing 
that water-movement is a mode of excitatory response. 

A difficult problem in connection with electrical response 
is that of the discharge from the electrical organs of certain . 
fishes. In a large number of cases, of which Torpedo may 
be taken as the type, the discharge takes place in a direction 
from the anterior or nervous to the posterior and non- 
nervous surface. Pacini’s generalisation that the responsive 
discharge is always from the anterior to the posterior surface 
is negatived by the instance of Malepterurus, in which it is 
from the modified glandular posterior to the anterior surface. 
Another peculiarity of the response of electric organs in 
general is that the responsive current is always in the same 
direction—that, namely, of the organ-discharge — whether 
the exciting shock be homo- or hetero-dromous. No theory 
has yet been found which will fully explain all these 
peculiarities. 

I have shown, however, that this phenomenon is not alone 
of its kind ; nor is it dependent on any specific characteristic 
of the animal nerve-and-muscle, or gland, of which different 
electric organs are modifications. The response of the electric 
organ simply constitutes an extreme case of differential 
excitability, and follows the general law of response in 
anisotropic organs—namely, that on diffuse stimulation the 
responsive current flows from the more to the less excitable. 
The peculiarity of the organ simply depends upon the fact 
that owing to the serial arrangement of its elementary aniso- 
tropic plates the terminal electro-motive effect becomes very 
large by summation. We find vegetal analogues to the two 
types of electrical plates of Zorpedo and Malepterurus, in 
the leaves of Pterospermum, and the pitcher.of Vepenthe. In 
the first of these, Pzerospermum, as in Torpedo, the anterior 
nervous surface is relatively more excitable than the mass 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ANISOTROPIC ORGANS 7II 


of indifferent tissue on the posterior surface. Hence the 
current of response is from the more excitable anterior to 
the less excitable posterior. In the second type—the pitcher 
of Nepenthe and the electrical plates of Malepterurus—the 
posterior surface being glandular and therefore exceptionally 
excitable, the responsive current is from posterior to anterior. 

In taking rheotomic observations on the response to 
electrical stimulation in various anisotropic leaves—virtually 
acting, as has been shown, like electrical plates—it was found 
that in sluggish specimens the maximum electro-motive value 
was attained ‘2 second after the exciting shock. This was 
also the value of the period which elapsed after the applica- 
_tion of moderate mechanical stimulation. With vigorous 
specimens, however, such as the leaves of Vymphea alba, 
the maximum effect was attained in a much shorter time, 
that is to say, in about ‘03 second. In the electrical organ 
of Yorpedo the corresponding period has been found to be 
‘o1 second. The response of electrical organs is found to be 
repeated or multiple. In the rheotomic records obtained 
with leaves, further, the multiple apices of the curve show 
that the response of vegetable organs also has this multiple 
character Multiple response, however, is not the peculiar 
characteristic of the electrical organ, but has been shown to 
take place in various kinds of animal and vegetable tissues. 

Again, that this peculiarity—of definitely uni-directioned 
response, whether the excitation be homodromous or hetero- 
dromous—is not distinctive of life, with its specific powers 
of assimilation and dissimilation, but of anisotropy in 
general, with its consequent differential excitability, was 
shown by the fact that similar uni-directioned responses to 
homo- or hetero-dromous shocks were given by an inorganic 
structure, consisting of prepared lead (fig. 167). 

We have next to pass in review the question of the 
response of plant and animal tissue to stimulus of light 
The various motile responses, induced by light in plants, are 
so diverse and so apparently incapable of being explained 
by any single reaction of fundamental excitation, that it 


712 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


was thought that the effect of this stimulus was different 
in different cases, the specific reaction in each organ being 
determined by the ultimate advantage of the plant. But I 
have been able to show that the excitatory effect of light is 
normal and like that of any other form of stimulus. The 
various results induced by it depend, first, on the question 
whether stimulus has remained localised at the point of 


application, or been transmitted to distant areas. The effect | 


is thus modified by the intensity of the stimulus and the 
conductivity of the tissue. These results, however, may be 
further modified by the differential excitability of the organ. 
Here, as in other cases of stimulation, the general rule holds 
good that response is by greater contraction and galvano- 
metric negativity of the more excited. As a concrete example 
may be mentioned the case of the pulvinus of MW/zmosa, 
when the upper surface alone is subjected to the stimulus 
of light. Here, owing to local excitatory contraction of 
the upper, the expelled water reaches the lower half of the 
pulvinus and induces there the hydro-positive effect of 
expansion, both of these effects conspiring, in this first stage 
of response, to erect: the leaf. The electrical variation at 
the lower half is here, then, found to be positive. But as 
the excitatory effect itself is gradually conducted to the 
lower half, it induces there an increasing contraction. The 
mechanical response is now therefore reversed, from one of 
erection to one of depression, the electrical variation of the 
lower half of the pulvinus undergoing at the same time a 
corresponding change from positivity to negativity (fig. 237). 
From this experiment it is clear that the electrical response 
under light exhibits the same stimulatory changes which are 
also visibly demonstrated by mechanical response. We see, 
moreover, from this experiment that light in general acts 
as a moderate stimulus. For while mechanical or thermal 
stimulus induces a sudden collapse of the leaf of AZzmosa, the 
application of light brings about only a gradual fall. 

Owing to this moderateness of the stimulus of light, and 
to the fact that its application is strictly local, it is easy to 


eet ne ox eel 


i's SS att. Pe Tae ee 


aS ee ee ee ee 


ee gree ee 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ANISOTROPIC ORGANS 713 


understand the possibility of certain modifications occurring 
in the response. Thus, in highly excitable and conducting 
tissues, the responses will be by galvanometric negativity, 
and the state of excitation will be conducted to a certain 
distance. But we have seen that in tissues which are not 
highly excitable, stimulus, falling below the excitatory value, 
gives rise to positive response. Thus, under the action of 
light, we obtain in plants two types of response, negative 
and positive. Moreover, under continuous stimulation of 
light, these may undergo phasic alternations (— + — +) or 
(+ — +.—). Asan example of negative response to direct 
or transmitted stimulation of light may be seen the response 
of Bryophyllum (fig. 238), the positive response being exem- 
plified in the record obtained with a petiole of cauliflower 
(fig. 240). 

It has been explained how these alternating phasic 
responses lead us to the phenomenon of multiple and 
autonomous response. | 

A leaflet of Bzophytum, or a Desmodium \eaflet in a state 
of standstill, under the continuous action of strong light, will 
exhibit multiple mechanical responses. The corresponding 
multiple electrical responses are seen in the response of the 
lamina of Bryophylum under the action of continuous light 
(fig. 242). It has also been shown that these phasic alterna- 
tions are brought about by the fact that the antagonistic 
elements in the response become effectively predominant by 
turns. Either of these antagonistic factors may be unmasked 
more effectively by the arrest of external stimulus at a 
particular phasic maximum, Thus, in the case where the 
normal alternation is (— + -— +), if the stimulus be sud- 
denly withdrawn at the end of the second phase, or positive 
maximum, the response overshoots in the positive direction 
(figs. 243, 244). The characteristic direct and after-effects 
in this Type I., then, during the application of light and its 
remoyal are ((— + +). In specimens whose characteristic 
response under continuous stimulation is (+ — + —), if 
stimulus be again withdrawn at the end of the second 


714 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


phase—here negative maximum—the response overshoots 
in the negative direction (figs. 245, 247). The direct and 
after-effects in this Type III., therefore, may be represented 
by the formula (+ —...). Between these two extremes 
lie instances of an intermediate Type II., which has cases (a) 
and (6), according as the stimulus is removed at maximum 
of the first or negative phase, or at maximum of the second 


or positive phase. The formula of Type II. (a) is thus 


(— + «.), while that of Type II. (0) is(—... ). 

The response of the retina furnishes us with the most 
striking examples of the action of stimulus of light. The 
true character of this response has been supposed hitherto 
to be unlike that of other tissues, for while excited nerve 
and muscle were said to show response by ‘ negative 
variation, the response of the retina was referred to as by 
‘ positive variation.’ This furnishes us with an instance of 
the confusion which is apt to result from making the 
so-called resting-current the standard of reference. On 
testing for the natural current, by making connections with 
the longitudinal surface of the optic nerve, and with the 
cornea, in an undetached eyeball of frog, I found that it 
flowed from the cornea to the nerve. But when the eye is 
detached, by section of the optic nerve, the after-effect of 
excitation on the more excitable nerve reverses this current, 
the nerve becoming relatively galvanometrically negative. 
The normal effect of transmitted excitation from the retina 
would now make the nerve still more galvanometrically 
negative, and this would appear as a positive variation of the 
reversed natural current. Hence, the responsive positive 
variation, met with in the eye under light, is in reality the same 
normal excitatory response, by galvanometric negativity, with 
which we are already familiar. 

I have also shown, by means of equi-alternating electric 


shocks, that under normal conditions the optic nerve is more. 


excitable than the cornea, and that the retina is more 
excitable than the optic nerve. The eyeball and retina 
have often been found by different observers to exhibit 


pinot Aided ser sommes ee 


ee ee ed 


a 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ANISOTROPIC ORGANS 715 


abnormal or reversed response. Now, with regard to reversed 
response in general I have shown it to be due to either of 
two different conditions which hold good for all responding 
tissues. These are in the first place sub-tonicity, and 
secondly, fatigue. The abnormal response caused by the first 
has been shown to be converted into normal, in the case of 
the retina, by the action of an agent which enhanced the 
excitability. 

Another phenomenon which I discovered in the response 
of retina was that of multiple response, induced by the 
application of strong or of continuous stimulation. These 
multiple responses have visual correspondences in the 
multiple after-images seen in the retina, and in the visual 
fluctuations which occur under the constant stimulus of 
light. The latter of these facts was demonstrated by a 
specially devised stereoscope (p. 432). In this connection 
may be mentioned the interesting phenomenon of Binocular 
Alternation of Vision. 3 

The various types of direct and after-effects observed in 
vegetable tissues under light I find to have their close 
correspondences in the responses of the retina. Just as in 
the highly excitable lamina of Bryophyllum, constituting 
Type I., we have the formula of (— + -i-), so also, in the 
highly excitable retina of Ophzocephalus, the same sequences 
of direct and after-effects is observed. In less highly 
excitable vegetable tissues, such as the petiole of cauli- 
flower, affording us Type III., the sequence was shown 
to be (+ —...). In correspondence with this may be 
mentioned the response of the isolated retina of fish, 
observed by Kiihne and Steiner. In this case, as the effect 
of isolation, the retina must have become sub-tonic, which 
supposition is borne out by the fact that its response to the 
immediate action of light was abnormal positive instead of 
negative. I found a similar sequence to occur in an isolated 
sub-tonic retina of Ophiocephalus (figs. 260, 261). 

Finally, in somewhat fatigued specimens, an intermediate 
Type II. was found, in which the sequence was (— + ...) 


716 - COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


or (—...). Examples of these are afforded by the eye of 
the frog. These correspondences, between the effects of 
light in vegetable tissues and in the retina, will be clearly 
understood from the series of figs. 253 to 261. 

The next subject to be summarised is that of the 
electrical response of plants to gravitational stimulus. In 
an apogeotropic organ like the stem, when laid horizontally, 
the mechanical response is such as to make the shoot once 
more vertical. The active factor in this curvature might 
obviously be, either the responsive contraction of the upper 
side, or the responsive expansion of the lower. The question 
to be decided here was whether the response of the plant, 
to geotropic stimulus, was or was not of the same nature as 
its response to other effective forms of stimulation—that is 
to say, by excitatory contraction. An experiment has been 
described (p. 436) in which this question was subjected to 
tests. The local application of cold is known to bring 
about the temporary abolition of the excitatory effect, and 
in the present case, its application on the lower side of a 
horizontally laid shoot was not seen to induce any effect on 
the response, while, when applied on the upper, it retarded 
- and arrested response to gravitation. This shows that in 
this response it is the contraction of the upper side which is 
the active factor. This is independently verified by the 
test of electrical response, where I find that the upper 
side, when subjected to gravitational stimulus, exhibits the 
sign of true excitation—namely, by induced galvanometric 
negativity. 

The important Theory of Statoliths offers us a suggestive 
explanation of the manner in which gravity exercises stimu- 
lation upon the responding tissue, by the weight of solid 
particles. When the stem is vertical, in consequence of the 
symmetry of distribution of the particles on all sides, there 
is no resultant action; and as soon as this symmetry is 
disturbed by laying the stem horizontally, response might be 
expected to be initiated. This, however, is not the case. 
The shoot first bends down, and it is not until after the 


is on 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF ANISOTROPIC ORGANS 717 


expiration of nearly three-quarters of an hour that the first sign 
of apogeotropic action appears. This anomaly is probably due 
to the induced mechanical curvature caused by weight which 
has first to be overcome. We may, however, regard ourselves 
as independent of the mechanical indications, when recording 
the effect of gravitational stimulus by geo-electric response. 
The excitatory electric effect, as we have seen in other cases, 
takes place as before, when all responsive mechanical 
indications are restrained. Proceeding on this principle, 
therefore, I found that the geo-electric response was initiated 
within so short a time of subjecting the specimen to gravi- 
tational stimulus as one minute (fig. 271). This experiment 
shows of what widespread application is the electrical mode 
of detecting the excitatory response of tissues, to many 
different forms of stimulus, 


CHAPTER XLVIII 


REVIEW OF RESPONSE OF NERVE AND RELATED 
PSYCHOLOGICAL PHENOMENA 


Transmission of excitation in plants—Vegetal nerve—Similar variations of 
receptivity, conductivity, and responsivity, under parallel conditions in plant 
and animal nerves—Conductivity balance—After-effect of section on con- 
ductivity and excitability—Function of vegetal nerve in plant-economy— 
Laminz of plant form a catchment-basin for stimulus—Motile response of 
nerve— Molecular cycle and characteristic changes in response of nerve— 
Effect of fatigue on transmitted excitation—Similarity of excitatory molecular 
changes in both afferent and efferent nerves—Multiple response induced by 
strong stimulus in nerve—Multiple excitations in nerve during drying — 
Individual contractile responses to constituent tetanising shocks—Negative 
after-effect on abrupt cessation of tetanisation—Extra-polar effects similar in 
plant and animal nerve —Inadequacy of Pfliiger’s Law— Under feeble E.M.F. 
excitability enhanced by anode and depressed by kathode—Demonstration by 
subjective response—Under feeble current excitation travels better against 
than with it—Response by variation of electrotonic current due to algebraical 
superposition of excitatory effect—Physico-physiological basis of sensation— 
Identification of positive tone of sensation with hydro-positive wave and 
negative tone of sensation with negative wave—Natural and artificial induc- 


tion of dissociation of sensation—Physical explanation of Weber-Fechner’s — 


Law—Quality of sensation also a factor—Conversion from painful to pleasur- 

‘able and vice versa at will by electrotonus—Memory as an after-effect of 
stimulus—-Persistent after-sensation— Revival of latent memory-image through 
differential excitation induced by diffuse stimulation—-Same effects demon- 
strated in the inorganic. 


THE next subject to be reviewed is that of the conduction 
of stimulus. It has been supposed that plants do not con- 
duct excitation by the transmission of protoplasmic changes, 
as certain animal tissues are known to do. Even in the well- 
known case of Mimosa, where stimulus is seen to induce move- 
ment at a distance, this was supposed to be the result of hy- 
dro-mechanical disturbance. This conclusion has been shown, 
however, to be erroneous, for pure hydrostatic disturbance 


Pa 


RESPONSE OF NERVE 719 


has been proved to occasion an erectile movement of the 
leaf with galvanometric positivity (figs. 44, 45, and 46). The 
transmission of true excitation, on the other hand, gives rise 
to a fall of the leaf and the electrical response of galvano- 
metric negativity. Again, the transmission of excitation in 
the plant is modified similarly by those varying physiological 
conditions- which influence it in the case of the animal. 
Thus, a strong stimulus is transmitted more quickly, other 
things being equal, than a feeble. Fatigue, on the other 
hand, is found to depress the velocity. The application of 
cold reduces or temporarily abolishes the transmission, while 
warmth enhances its velocity. Anzsthetics, again, are found 
to depress conductivity. And lastly, the polar effect of 
currents, in the plant as in the animal, is to induce opposite 
changes, according as anode or kathode is applied. I have, 
moreover, been able to isolate certain tissues specially fitted 
for the conduction of excitation. These are found in the 
soft parts of the fibro-vascular bundles, and are particularly 
easy to isolate in the case of fern. They here possess the 
relatively high velocity of about 50 mm. per second. It 
may be said, in view of their peculiar responsive charac- 
teristics, and the modifications of their response under given 
conditions, that these structures are indistinguishable from 
animal nerves, and may therefore be rightly designated 
vegetal nerves. On isolation, for example, these highly 
excitable vegetal nerves, like the animal nerve, when isolated, 
are liable to fall into a state of sub-tonicity, on account of 
which their conducting power is temporarily impaired. The 
transmitted effect of stimulus, then, as in the corresponding 
case of animal nerve, becomes one of abnormal galvano- 
metric positivity. Continuous stimulation when in this state, 
however, by carrying the tissue out of the A into the 
B condition, converts the abnormal positive response into 
normal negative, through an intermediate diphasic, in the 
plant as in the animal nerve. When in the B stage, again, 
tetanisation -has the effect of enhancing response in both, 
The effects of ether, carbonic acid, alcohol vapour, and 


720 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


ammonia are the same.in the one case as in the other. The 
effects of various drugs on the receptivity, conductivity, and 
responsivity of the vegetal nerve are the same as on those 
of the animal, and finally, in the action of different salts, the 
acid and basic moieties exhibit the same characteristic effects 
in plant and animal nerves alike (Chapters XXXII. and 
XXXITI.). 


In order to study the variation of excitatory effects in. 


nerves I was able to devise a very delicate instrument, the 


Conductivity Balance: (fig. 291). This apparatus not only 


enables us to study the modifications of conductivity, excit- 
ability, and responsivity, induced by a given agent separately, 
but also to compare relative variations as between any two 
of these, say, for instance, conductivity against excitability, 
or receptive excitability against responsivity. It also enables 
us to compare and contrast the action of two different 
reagents applied simultaneously in different parts of the 
same nerve. In this way the factor of uncertainty introduced 
by the unknown individual differences between two nerves is 
eliminated. 

_ The principle on which the Method of Conductivity 
Balance depends is that of applying stimulus at a point 
which, in the excitatory sense, is exactly midway between 
the electrodes E and E’; the excitatory effects at E and E’ 
exactly balance each other, and the galvanometric deflection 
is then zero. When the excitability of the right hand, E, or 
the conductivity of the right arm, C, of the balance is 
enhanced, the balance is upset and the resultant response 
is, say, up ; depression, on the other hand, upsets it in the 
opposite direction. Not only may the effects of various 
chemical agents be determined by this method, but it is 
easy also to study by its means the effects of temperature 
on conductivity. Cold is thus found to depress, and warmth 
to enhance it (figs. 307, 309). Another important investi- 
gation carried out by this means was on the curious 
phenomenon presented by the effect of section in enhancing 
the excitability of adjacent points. It was shown that this 


— a 


Ss re 


RESPONSE OF NERVE 721 


was due to the molecular transformation caused by the 
stimulus of the mechanical or thermal section. The effect 
of such stimulus on neighbouring points is to induce moderate . 
excitation, raising them to the higher excitability of the 
condition B (fig. 311).. At or very near the section point 
itself, on account of over-stimulation, the transformation is 
to condition D or E, and the result should be one of loss of 
excitability. In accordance with this, it is found that at 
such points there is depression of excitability (fig. 312). 

As regards the place of the vegetal nerve in the plant 
economy, it may be said that the normal excitability of 
a tissue, by which its proper functions are discharged, can 
only be maintained fully by a supply of energy, which must 
be received from the environment. Both animal and vegetal 


’ nerves have been shown, when isolated, to lose their normal 


conductivity and excitability, their response becoming ab- 
normal or being abolished. It is only by the accession of 
fresh energy of stimulus that the normal conductivity and 
excitability are restored. It is known, further, that when the 
nerve loses its excitability, undergoing consequent degenera- 
tion, the attached muscle also exhibits rapid decay. It will 
thus be seen that the various tissues of the organism are 
maintained in their normal functional activities by means of 
energy conveyed to them through the nerves. 

One of the principal forms of energy in maintaining the 
tonic condition of a green plant is sunlight; when deprived 
of this, its various normal activities come gradually to a stop, 
and the plant ultimately dies. But if any portion of the 
plant be exposed to light even its shaded parts will be found 
to continue in natural vigour. This is exemplified by the 
experiment of Sachs, in which an undetached branch of 
Cucurbita was kept in a dark box, and was found to grow, 
and produce flowers and fruits, as if under normal conditions. 
The fact that a plant, when totally deprived of sunlight, dies, 
shows how essential to its tonic condition is energy of light. 
The fact that so long as a portion of it is kept in light the 
whole flourishes, proves the transmission of energy from one 


3 A 


722 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


part to another, a transmission which is now made com- 
prehensible, being effected through the intervention of the 
plant-nerves, whose existence I have demonstrated. In 
the case of trees, again, the interior tissues whose: functions 
are of great'importance in various ways, are inaccessible to 
such external energy as that of light. But no part of 
them:is far removed from the vegetal nerves, whose outer 
endings are found in the ramified venation of the leaves. 
The lamine of the plant thus in their aggregation form an 
extensive catchment-basin for the reception of energy from 
‘outside and its ultimate transmission within the plant. An 
experiment has been described which shows the enhancement 
of the excitability of the plant-nerve by energy of light 


(fig. 334). 


I have next to summarise a new method for the study of’ 


excitatory reactions in nerves. It has been supposed that in 
certain respects the reaction of the nerve is specifically 
different from that of the muscle. It has been regarded as 
typically non-motile, the highest power of the microscope 
being incapable, it was said, of detecting any effect in respon- 
sive change of form. I have shown, however, that this 
- conclusion was erroneous, there being in this respect a con- 
tinuity between the responses of muscle and nerve. In 
a particular case of frog’s nerve the responsive contraction 
under strong stimulation was as much as 14 per cent. of the 
original length, and in others, it was as much as 20 per cent. 
or more. With a magnification of about 200 times, which is 
afforded by my moderately sensitive Kunchangraph, the 
observer is able to study all the excitatory phenomena in 
nerve with as great ease, and much greater accuracy, as 
by the employment of a very highly sensitive galvanometer. 
Records of the electrical responses of nerve are obtained by 
the differential effects of excitation at the two contacts, when 
one of these has been subjected to injury. It has been 
shown that such injury does not always completely abolish 
the excitability of the second contact, for which reason there 
may be induced a local reaction of feeble negative or reversed 


RESPONSE OF NERVE 3 723 


positive response. The interference of this with the normal 
response at the uninjured contact is thus apt to give rise to 
various complications. In contrast with this we have the 
reliability of the mechanical response of the nerve, in which 
the effect recorded is direct, and not differential. Again, the 
electrical form of stimulus, which is almost universally 
employed for the excitation of nerve, is liable by leakage, 
unless very great precautions are taken, to vitiate the results 
obtained by the electrical mode of response. When the 
response observed, however, is not electrical but mechanical, 
this source of error is obviously eliminated. } : 
By means of mechanical response, the molecular trans- 
formations through which the nerve passes, under the action 
of stimulus itself, may be observed with the greatest clearness. 
An isolated nerve, cut off from its natural supply of energy, 
generally falls into a sub-tonicity indicated in the mechanical 
record, as an increasing abnormal relaxation; and the 
application of stimulus induces at this point an abnormal 
positive response, of sudden expansion. Successive or con- 
tinuous stimulations, however, transform the nerve from 
condition A to condition B; the abnormal expansion being 
arrested and converted into increasing contraction. . During 
this stage, then, the responses to individual stimuli are trans- 
formed from the abnormal expansive positive to the normal 
contractile negative, through an intermediate  diphasic. 
Molecular transformation is here very rapid and the re- 
sponses show a staircase increase (fig. 382)... An intervening 
period of tetanisation will now have the effect of enhanc- 
ing the response (fig. 383). In the clear demonstration 
thus obtainable of a progressive molecular transformation, 
with corresponding variations of response at its different 
stages, we arrive at the true explanation of the change from 
the abnormal positive to the normal negative, in electrical 
response, and also of the enhancement of the normal negative 
after an intervening period of tetanisation (figs. 275-278). 
The next stage to be reached is C, where the responses 
are uniform. After this, we arrive at D, where fatigue-decline 
3 A 2 


724 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


begins to make its appearance. Up to this point, the nerve 
as a whole has been undergoing increasing contraction, the 
base-line of the series of records being thus tilted upwards. 
But after D, it begins to show relaxation, and at the stage E, 
the responses to individual stimuli are actually reversed, 
the region of transformation from diminishing to reversed 
response being often marked by the appearance of diphasic 
(figs. 396 and 400). The entire responsive cycle may thus be 
viewed as consisting of two halves of which one is the reverse 
of the other. From the state of extreme sub-tonicity at A 
with its abnormal positivity, the responses are transformed 
through diphasic to feeble normal negative at B. They here 
increase in a staircase manner, till they become uniform at C. 
After this begins the reversing process, due to fatigue, 
brought on by overstrain, with its diminishing normal re- 
sponses at D, through diphasic, to abnormal positive once 


more,at E. Excessive sub-tonicity and excessive stimulation 


alike find their extreme case in the abolition of all response 
at death. The difference between the abnormal positive 
response of sub-tonicity and the abnormal positive response of 
fatigue lies in their previous history. The one is due to lack 
of stimulation and the other to itsexcess. For the restoration 
of normal response, the treatment in the two cases must be 
opposite. In the first, the application of stimulus is necessary ; 
in the second, it is its cessation, or rest, which is required. 

_ Similar effects are also met with, in the case of trans- 
mitted excitation. In the sub-tonic condition, conductivity 
is depressed, and the transmitted effect is abnormal positive. 
By the action of stimulus, however, the conductivity is 
gradually restored, and the response to transmitted stimu- 
lation is converted from abnormal. positive to normal 
negative through the intermediate diphasic. After this, 
under increasing fatigue the diminishing responses are con- 
verted to abnormal positive through an intermediate diphasic 
(fig. 325). Another important demonstration was that of the 
perfect similarity of the molecular changes induced by 
stimulus in the afferent and efferent nerves respectively. 


RESPONSE OF NERVE 725 


The experiments which have just been described were carried 
out on the efferent gastrocnemius of frog and gecko. With 
the afferent optical nerves of certain fishes I obtained 
mechanical responses which were exactly the same as these 
(figs. 324, 402). That is to say, in a sub-tonic condition the 
optic nerve gave the abnormal positive or expansive re- 
sponse, and this was subsequently converted into the normal 
contractile responses, through an intermediate diphasic. 

The mechanical response of nerve just described, may be 
recorded either photographically, by a reflected spot of light, 
or directly on a smoked-glass surface by means of a writing- 
point. The difficulties due to friction in the latter case are 
obviated by the use of the Oscillating Recorder. By means, 
however, of a battery of levers, and using the optical method 
of record, it is possible to have a magnification by the 
Kunchangraph of one hundred thousand times or more, the 
sensitiveness of the record being correspondingly enhanced. 
By this means many new phenomena may be brought under 
observation, one of these being the multiple response induced 
by strong stimulus in nerve. It is known, again, that nerve 
becomes highly excitable during the setting-in of ‘drying, and 
under these conditions, in a nerve-and-muscle preparation, 
repeated mechanical spasms are exhibited by the attached 
muscle. In taking the mechanical record of nerve, it is found 
that the substitution of dry for moist air at once induces a 
visible contraction. Now this state of partial contraction, 
bringing the nerve, as it does, into condition B, we know to 
be significant of enhanced excitability. If the drying of the 
nerve be now allowed to continue, it is found that there is 
induced a series of multiple responses (fig. 326). And the 
multiple spasms seen during drying in the muscle of a 
muscle-and-nerve preparation undoubtedly have, as one of 
their factors, these multiple excitations thus demonstrated 
to take place in the nerve. ; 

With the highly magnifying Kunchangraph, again, the 
individual effect of a single shock is demonstrated with the 
greatest clearness. Under rapidly succeeding tetanising 


726 COMPARATIVE .ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


shocks, the response shows a serration of the apical line, 
proving that the individual responses are not completely 
fused. On the abrupt cessation of tetanising shocks a 
sudden enhancement of.the contractile effect occurs, followed 
by the usual recovery. : This is analogous to the sudden 


enhancement. of response on the cessation of tetanisation, 


seen in the retina, in magnetic response, and in the response 
of certain sensitive inorganic preparations under similar 
circumstances (pp. 536, 428, 383). 

The next subject to be surveyed is that of electrotonus, 
and the variations in excitatory effects induced by it. It has 
been shown that the polarising currents induce extra-polar 
currents in the plant nerve, exactly as in that of the animal 
(fig. 338). As regards the effect of electrotonic currents 
generally on excitability, the results obtained by Bernstein 
are described as polarisation-decrement, whereas those of 
Hermann are known as polarisation-increment. That is 
to say, with one experimental arrangement the induced 
electrotonic current is seen to undergo a diminution under 
excitation, and with a different arrangement an. increase. 
These and other electrotonic variations appear to be very 
anomalous, and incapable of mutual reconciliation. 

I have, however, been able to show that all these effects 
may be regarded as combining the variations of two distinct 
factors, namely conductivity and excitability, under the 
influence of an electrical current. One of the principal 
difficulties in the correct explanation of these phenomena 
has hitherto lain in the assumption that Pfliiger’s Law, 
relating to the polar effects of currents, was of universal 
application. I have shown, however, to the contrary, that 
it applies only to a certain middle range of electro-motive 
intensity, the excitatory effect at anode and kathode being, 
at a very high E.M.F., exactly reversed. Going, again, to the 
other extreme, of a low electro-motive force, I have shown 
that, in opposition to Pfliiger’s generalisation, it is the anode 
that enhances the excitability of a nerve, while the kathode 
depresses it. This I have been able to demonstrate by 


a ee ee 


“pS aa as el Pal a al i ah lt ral a Re ar ON a solr ey, 


“RESPONSE OF NERVE | 727 


numerous experiments (figs. 350 and 353). The fact that 
under feeble E.M.F. the variation of excitability is opposite 
to that under moderate E.M.F. can be demonstrated with 
great simplicity by means of the subjective response of 
sensation. A wound was made on the back of the hand, and 
the application of a dilute solution of salt caused a moderate 
irritation. The application of kathode to this wound now 
rendered the irritation intolerably painful, while that of the 
anode at once made it soothing, removing: even the normal 
discomfort due to the salt. These effects—coming under 
Pfliiger’s generalisation that kathode enhances excitability, 
while anode depresses—held good so long as the acting 
E.M.F. was about 1°5 volt. But when the acting E.M.F. 
was reduced to ‘5 volt, the kathode was found to induce 
a soothing sensation, whereas the anode. became painful. 

I have also found that the passage of a current pro- 
foundly modifies the conduction of excitation in a directive 
manner, according as the excitation has to travel with or 
against it. In the simplest cases, where the polarising elec- 
trodes are so far apart as to eliminate the direct excitatory 
effect of the poles, and using a feeble current, I have shown 
that excitation travels better electrically uphill, that is to say, 
against the current, than down, or with it. Thus the normal 
responses to transmitted stimulation are found to be enhanced 
when the polarising current is against the ‘direction of trans- 
mission. A polarising current in the same direction as that 
of excitation, has, on the other hand, the effect of retarding 
it. The normal responses are then diminished, or even 
reversed to positive, by the diminution or abolition of the 
power of true conduction (figs. 345, 346, 347). 

The various effects described as polarisation-increment 
and decrement have been shown further to be due to the 
increased galvanometric negativity of the more excited of 
two points, the responsive current being algebraically sum- 
mated with the existing electrotonic current. The greater 
excitation of one of these two points was also shown to be 
due to the greater intensity of excitation conducted to it, or 


728 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


to the greater excitability induced by the action of the anode, 
or to both. 

This summary of results will conclude with a brief 
account of the demonstration of the physico-physiological 
nature of the basis of sensation. The effect of a single 
stimulus has been shown to consist of two different waves 
sent out from the point stimulated, of which the hydro- 


positive travels with a greater velocity than the true excita- . 


tory or negative. If the stimulus applied, moreover, be 
feeble, the positive wave alone will be transmitted. If the 
stimulus, again, be very strong and the path of conduction 
short, one wave will be superposed over the other, the nega- 
tive masking the positive. The two waves, however, may 
be separated from each other by inducing a depression of the 
conductivity of the nerve, when the negative will be made to 
lag behind the positive. By the suppression of the negative, 
owing to sufficient reduction of conductivity, the positive 
may be made to arrive at the responding point alone. 
Nervous impulses have thus been shown to be of two 
different kinds, positive and negative, and contrary to the 
universal assumption that the nerve gives no visible indication 
of its state of excitation, it has been shown that these are 
accompanied by waves of expansion and contraction respec- 
tively. In addition to these visible mechanical expressions, 
we have also the concomitant electrical expressions of 
galvanometric positivity and negativity. I have been able, 
moreover, to identify the wave of expansion as the vehicle 
of that change which gives rise to the positive tone of 
sensation, which may be described as pleasurable or at 
least not unpleasurable. The negative or contractile wave, 
similarly, has been shown to be doloriferous. These two 
waves we saw to be separable from each other, whenever the 
conducting nerve was sufficiently long. Thus, when the sole 
of the foot receives a smart stroke from a rod, two different 
impulses are sent out, first the positive or sensation of con- 
tact, which is not unpleasurable, followed by the negative, 
with its different and painful tone of sensation. In various 


AES Sea See Ree ee ee 


RESPONSE BY SENSATION 729 


well-known cases of nerve-disease, bringing on diminished 
conductivity, this dissociation of sensation is met with patho- 
logically. In paralysis, again, burning coals may be held in 
the hand and induce only the feeling of contact, without any 
sensation of pain. 

One very difficult problem in connection with psycho- 
logical response is that of the peculiar relation between the 


intensity of stimulus and that of response. The generalisa- 


tion known as Weber-Fechner’s law, asserts that stimulus 
must increase in geometrical, for sensation to increase in 
arithmetical, progression. Fechner, moreover, regarded this 
relation, not as due to any physical or physiological factor, 
but as a particular case of some specific psychological law. 
On an inspection of the mechanical responses of animal 
nerve, given in figs. 400, 401, and 402, however, we see that 
the peculiar relation between stimulus and sensation follows 
inevitably on the physiological character of those responses. 
We there see that under feeble stimulus the response is 
positive, connoting, as we know, a positive tone of sensation. 
After this, as stimulus increases, the sign of response under- 
goes a reversal into normal negative. From this point 
onwards, for some time, the response to increasing stimulus 
shows a rapid rate of increase; but this increase tends to 
reach a limit as the maximum molecular distortion is 
approached. These facts follow naturally from the mole- 
cular theory of response which has been described, and in 
such considerations we obtain an explanation of those 
changes in the tone or quality of sensation of which Weber- 
Fechner’s law was unable to take account. That these 
responsive characteristics, again, are not peculiar to the 
animal nerve, has been seen in the fact that vegetal nerves 
also show a similar relation between stimulus and response 
(fig. 403). That this relation indeed is universal, will be 
understood from the response of an inorganic substance to 
increasing stimuli, as given in fig. 404. | 

Another interesting proof of the dependence of the 
psychological upon physico-physiological changes is afforded 


730 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


by the ‘polar action of currents. It has been shown that 
the positive response is short-lived, whereas the negative is 
relatively more persistent, its persistence increasing with the 
intensity of the response. Now, by means of the Sensimeter 
(fig. 405), we can apply a series of stimuli of measured 
intensity in such a way as to induce the neutral sensation 
which is neither pleasurable nor painful. The frequency of 


this stimulation is so adjusted as to appear all but continuous. 


If we now render the excited point moderately anode, and 
thus reduce its excitability, the neutral will be converted to 
the positive tone, and the sensation, moreover, will be rendered 
strikingly discrete. If, next, the excitability of the stimu- 
lated point be enhanced by the application of moderate 
kathode, the neutral sensation will become converted into 
painful, becoming, further, fused and continuous. 
It will thus be seen that in the determination of sensation 
the internal plays as important a part as the external. By 
the peculiar molecular disposition of the nerve, it is indeed 
possible, as we have seen, to convert one quality of sensation 
into another, and such dispositions are to a greater or less ex- 
tent under the control of the will. It is not external circum- 
stances, then, which are the dominant factor psychologically, 
for the impression created by these is capable of indefinite 
modification in any direction, by the action of habitual 
induced dispositions, The reader will see for himself what 
illimitable possibilities are opened up by the line of thought 
here suggested. Ee) | 
The last subject to be reviewed is the phenomenon ot 
memory, which is an after-effect of stimulus. The after- 
effect of strong stimulus is in general more persistent than 
that of feeble. Similarly, the memory of a strong sensation 
is more¥enduring than of a weak one. Very strong stimulus, 
again, gives rise, as we have seen, to multiple responses. In 
the retina these are perceived as multiple after-images, which 
sometimes appear to be renewed spontaneously. This fact 
will often be found a sufficient explanation of visual phantoms 
and hallucinations. ' This, however, is not the usual method 


ta een 


0? (iret 


ae Tee ag ge ae, eee te ee ee 


Ws 


= ie OS 


RESPONSE BY SENSATION ~ 731. 


of reviving memory-images. Long after every trace of the 
primary stimulation has disappeared we can revive it by an 
impulse of the will. Memory-impressions are often likened 
to scars. Of this metaphor, however, it may be said, that 
though, no doubt, when the blow is recent the smarting effect 
will persist for some time, causing an ever-diminishing after- 
sensation, yet, when the scar has healed, how could it, of 
itself, reproduce the original sensation? To do this, the 
original excitation would require to be reproduced, in the 
absence of the primary exciting cause. If, then, instead of 
regarding it as a scar, we translate the original impression 
into shades of light and darkness, we see that such a picture 
was produced by different intensities of the primary stimulus 
acting on the sensitive surface—in other words, by means of 
induced differential excitation. To bring back the picture we 
have to reproduce, in the absence of prirnary stimulus, the 
same state of differential excitation as was at first induced 
by it. 

Such a revival is possible, as already shown, under the 
combined action of two different factors. It has been shown 
that when an isotropic tissue is locally acted upon by stimulus, 
the excitatory manifestation thus induced disappears after a 
time. There is now nothing visible by which to. distinguish 
the stimulated from the unstimulated areas. In consequence 
of this stimulation, however, there has been a transformation 
of the molecular condition of the portions acted upon. The 
tissue, which was originally isotropic, has now become an- 
isotropic, by the impression of this latent image. On diffuse 
stimulation, the differentially excitable structure will now 
exhibit the latent image, by various forms of differential 
excitation, of which some one particular manifestation will, 
in the case of any given organ, be the most conspicuous. 

Thus, in a metallic plate containing latent positive and 
negative chemical impressions, we shall obtain, on the appli- 
cation of diffuse stimulus, corresponding positive and negative 
galvanometric responses. In a phosphorescent plate, again, 
a small area may be subjected to the action of light. On the 


732 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


cessation of stimulus this will give luminous response, which 
may be taken as the immediate effect of primary stimulus. 
On the fading of this image, if the whole plate be subjected 
to feeble diffuse illumination for a short time, the latent image 
will once more appear as a bright patch against a dark back- 
ground. This is because, as the after-effect of stimulus, the 
area B has been rendered more excitable. Hence, diffuse 


stimulation evokes more intense response from it than from | 


its more inert background. Similarly, the memory-image 
is capable of revival by the internal impulse of the will, 
acting as a diffuse stimulus to evoke a differential sen- 
sation, which reproduces the light and shade of the primary 
picture. | 

The responsive phenomena seen in living matter are, 
_undoubtedly, wonderful and mysterious ; but those shown 
by the inorganic are no less wonderful. By-ascribing all 
physiological occurrences to specific reactions, and by con- 
stantly postulating the intrusion of forces of a new order, 
the road to the further advancement of knowledge is closed. 
By the conception of matter itself, on the other hand, as 
possessed of sensibility—that is to say, of molecular respon- 
siveness—we attain an immediate accession of insight into 
those physical interactions which must furnish the terms of 
any ultimate analysis. We are led by it to the discovery 
of the impressive fact of continuity as existent between the 
responses of the most complex living, and the simplest 
inorganic matter. Limiting ourselves, again, to the realm of 
living matter, we are impelled to recognise parallelisms, in 
the response of plant and animal, whose extent could never 
otherwise have been suspected. All the responsive phe- 
nomena of the animal are thus found to be foreshadowed in 
the plant, and this to such a degree that in the common 
script of the response-record the one is indistinguishable 
from the other. In both we observe a similar series of 
excitatory effects, whether these be exhibited mechanically 
or electrically. Both alike are responsive, and similarly 
responsive, to all the diverse forms of stimulus that impinge 


ost SAE 


Pike CeO te 2 Sere ed ts We 


RESPONSE BY SENSATION rates 


upon them. We ascend, in the one case as in the other, 
from the simplicities of the isotropic to the complexities of 
the anisotropic ; and the laws of these isotropic and aniso- 
tropic responses are the same in both. The responsive 
peculiarities of epidermis, epithelium, and gland; the re- 
sponse of the digestive organ, with its phasic alternations ; 
and the excitatory electrical discharge of an anisotropic 
plate, are the same in the plant as in the animal. The 
plant, like the animal, is a single organic whole, all its 
different parts being connected, and their activities co-ordi- 
nated, by the agency of those conducting strands which are 
known as nerves. As in the plant-nerve, moreover, so also 
in the animal, stimulation gives rise to two distinct impulses, 
exhibiting themselves by twofold mechanical and electrical 
indications of opposite signs. It is the nature of the indica- 
tor, again, which determines in any given instance the form 
of the responsive expression. A single molecular derange- 
ment may thus find manifestation as change of form, alteration 
of electrical condition, and subjective sensory variation. The 
dual qualities or tones known to us in sensation, further, are 
correspondent with those two different nervous impulses, of 
opposite signs, which are occasioned by stimulation. These 
two sensory responses—positive and negative, pleasure and 
pain—are found to be subject to the same modifications, 
under parallel conditions, as the positive and negative 
mechanical and electrical indications with which they are 
associated. And finally, perhaps, the most significant 
example of the effect of induced anisotropy lies in that 
differential impression made by stimulus on the sensory 
surfaces, which remains latent, and capable of revival, as the 
memory-image. 
In this demonstration of continuity, then, it has been found 
that the dividing frontiers between Physics, Physiology, and 
Psychology have disappeared. 


2 


a 


a 


CLASSIFIED. LIST OF EXPERIMENTS 


MOLECULAR RESPONSIVENESS OF MATTER . 


Mechanical response : 


PAGE 
1. Contractile response in indiarubber ; . : > . ‘ 2 
Electromotive response : 
2. Response of tin . ‘ . 6 
3. Fatigue in inorganic response : 7 
4. Action of sce on response of batirnast : - d are 8 
5. Action of ‘ poison’ in abolishing response of metal . 9 
6. Response in metal by method of negative variation 9 
Response by resistivity variation : 
7. Response of selenium to light 3 
8. Response of galena to electric radiation 3 
g. Response of allotropic silver Ag’ to electric vailission 4 
FUNDAMENTAL PHENOMENON OF RESPONSE IN PLANTS 
10. Simultaneous record of mechanical and electrical response 19 
11. Electrical response of pulvinus of Mimosa when physically re- 
strained . ; 2 . 20 
12. Response to sudden sailation of tension 25 
13. Response'to sudden compression . ; 25 
14. Response to tension and compression . 2 
15. Response to mechanical blow ‘ . . : - : e726 
16. Response to vibrational stimulus 27 
17. Response to chemical stimulus : : : ; 2. 27 
18. Response to thermal shock ‘ : : ‘ee ae 
19. Influence of sudden variation on efficiency or siirialation * 32 
20. Additive effect . ‘ ‘ : ‘ : ; 34. 
21. Genesis of tetanus in mechanical response of plants . 43 
22. Genesis of tetanus in electrical response of plants 43 
23. Rheotomic observation of time relation . ‘ é 48 
24. Response to increasing intensity of stimulus of mechanical blows 39 
25. Response to increasing intensity of vibrational stimulus ae gies 
26. Response to increasing stimulus, with or without complete recovery. 41 


COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


POSITIVE. AND NEGATIVE RESPONSE 


. Hydraulic response in Mimosa 

. Positive mechanical response followed by anlative in 5 Wioshysas ¢ 

. Positive mechanical response followed by negative in AZzmosa. . 

. Simultaneous record of positive and ign mechanical and electrical 


response in Bzophytum . 


. Positive, diphasic and negative Seanoues in petiolé of cauliieer 

. Positive and negative responses in tuber of potato 

. Unmasking of positive element in response by selective block : 

. Effect of stimulus of light on growth . 

. Effects of steady and sudden variation of Pome on a prowth 


response 


VARIOUS TYPES OF RESPONSE 


. Abnormal positive response in sub-tonic tissue 

. Staircase response in tissue originally sluggish 

. Staircase response in vegetal nerve 

. Staircase response in galena 

. Uniform response in plant 

. Fatigue due to rapidly succeeding Tre oem 

. Fatigue due to overstrain . ‘ ae 
. Fatigue-decline under continuous stimulation, i in medhanical response 


of Mimosa. 


. Fatigue-decline under continuoas ecmueaens in tecnica! response 


of celery 


. Oscillatory response in arsenic 

. Phasic alternation in mechanical dessonne of style of Diiave 

. Periodic fatigue in electric response of plants . ‘ 

. Periodic fatigue in autonomous response of Desmodium an 

. Reversal of normal response in fatigued nerve . : . 
. Bifurcated expression of response as growth and mechantesl response 


DETECTION OF PHYSIOLOGICAL ANISOTROPY 


. Differential response of compound strip . : 
. Isolated responses of upper and lower halves of ihe inus af Mitosa, 


with resultant differential response . 


. Transverse differential electrical response of eee af Mian 
. Transverse differential electrical response of plagiotropic stem of 


Cucurbita 


. Transverse differential dlectrical: reponse i cetiole of Musa 


NATURAL CURRENT OF REST AND ITS VARIATIONS 


. Effect of rise of temperature on current of rest 

. Effect of falling temperature on current of rest 

. Effect of Na,CO, solution on current of rest 

. Effect of CO, on current of rest ‘ 
. Reversal of natural current of rest as after- stint of esimuladion 


103 


102 
104 


108 


109 
1@ fe) 


III 
114 


119 
119 
122 
122 
127 


CLASSIFIED LIST OF EXPERIMENTS 737 


EFFECT OF CHEMICAL AGENTS ON EXCITABILITY 


PAGE 
61. Effect of chloroform : : ‘ : ‘ é ; ; - 130 
62. Effect of chloral hydrate . ; : ‘ ; RIM faerie an BSE 
63. Effect of formalin . : : ; : ; ‘ ; ; . +932 
64. Effect of NaOH. ; , ‘ ; : : , , na 5." 
65. Effectof KOH . ‘ . i a é ; a : 835 
66. Effect of sugar solution . “ ; ; ee ‘ 2. oe ae 
67. Effect of Na,CO, solution. ° : ; : ; ; . 136 
68. Effect of HCl . ; ‘ ; ; (on Ea 
69. Effect of moderate and rong aa of KOH : . P i - 139 
METHOD OF INTERFERENCE 
70. Responsive effect of variation of phase ; F : ‘ 5 enh el 
71. Diphasic response and its variation 4 : ; : : a EAS 
72. Diametric method of recording response. ‘ , e . » 147 
CURRENT OF INJURY AND NEGATIVE VARIATION 

73. Electrical after-effect in inorganic substances under strong stimu- 
lation . : ‘ I51 

74. Resistant saicuaomnisis saath: in vegetable tinea cases sti 
stimulation . : ‘ , Las oa 
75. After-effect of persistent negativity ise to section 2), i aig 
5 76. Electrical distribution in plant cylinder. : ; d Sf. age 
77. Response by negative variation . : : : : d . 158 

CURRENT OF DEATH AND RESPONSE BY POSITIVE VARIATION 

78. Response by abnormal positive variation . : : g OS 
79. Electric exploration of dying and dead tissue . : ’ ‘ . 169 
80. Electric exploration of tissue one end of whichis killed . . . 171 
81. Response by negative and positive variations of current of injury . 174 


EFFECT OF TEMPERATURE 


82. Effect of cold in arresting autonomous pulsation in Desmodium . . 181 


83. Effect of warmth on autonomous pulsation of Desmodium : . 182 
84. Effect of cold in pezmanent abolition of response , ‘ ote SZ 
85. Effect of cold in temporary abolition of response. ‘ - 184 
86. Effect of cyclic variation of temperature on electric response site EBS 
87. Effect of rising temperature on amplitude of response ; . 186 
88. Effect of rising temperature on conductivity pe , o Avca BOF 
89. Abolition of response by high temperature ~ . e258 190 
90. Effect of cooling on frequency and amplitude of pulsation a Des- : 
modium  , ‘ , : : ‘ : . ° jet OSS 
3 8 


738 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


DETERMINATION OF DEATH-POINT 


91. Determination of death-point by abolition of electrical response . 195 
92. Determination of death-point by means of thermo-mechanical curve 198 
93. Determination of death-point ie means of inversion of electro- 
motive curve . . : - + 202 
94. Simultaneous reversal of fectital curve ea Seen ‘ : . 204 


MULTIPLE AND AUTONOMOUS RESPONSE 


95. Multiple mechanical response in Biophytum  . ; : ae ae a 
96. Multiple electrotactile response in Mimosa . : ; ; . 208 
97. Multiple electromotive response in Biophytum . : , oe ROO 
98. Multiple electric response in various tissues . ; , ‘ . 210 
99. Multiple electric response in stomach of frog. j ; ae 8 Ie, 
100. Autonomous response in Biophyium - ; eT 
101. Initiation of multiple response in Deiwiutint cider light - eee ee 
102. Spark record of autonomous pulsation in Desmodium . : 214 
103. Simultaneous record of mechanical and electrical response of pul 
sating Desmodium leaflet . : : 217 ; 
104. Record of electrical responses in are ee leaflet nena movement 
is restrained . : ‘ ; : : : : Shea 
105. Multiple response of Seine ; : : : : } gee +f 


RESPONSE OF LEAVES 


106. Effect of section of petiole of /zcus on ihe current of rest : a: Bay 
107. Effect of section of petiole of C7¢7ws on the current of rest . S96 
108. Effect of stimulation of lamina. : . 225 
109. Parallelism between response of J/usa aiid that of es eae 


OO“. ee 


ELECTRIC DISCHARGE IN ANISOTROPIC ORGAN 


110. Response of leaf of Vymphea to transmitted stimulation ‘ + =2a5 
111. Response of leaf of Co/ews to thermal shocks . A ; <4 She 4 
112. Rheotomic observations. a ee 
113. ‘ Blaze current,’ so-called, in lament pence : : ices etn 


Response to equi-alternating shocks in : 


114. Carpellary leaf of Didlenza . 3 . ; : . 256 
115. Leaf of Plerospermum  . : : ; : ‘ : Re 
116. Pitcher of Nepenthe . , : : ; , : : . 256 
117. Pulvinus of AZzmosa : : : : . : : ~ 4 ams 
118. Bulb of Mriciis lily. : ‘ . : ‘ ‘ : i SY 
119. Prepared strip oflead_. ; ; : : : : < i. Oe 
120. Petiole of Alusa . : , : : ‘ .- (283 
‘121. Plagiotropic stem of [ome f : : : : buts eos 
$22, er. . . ; : : : ; ey ees 
123. Variegated leaf of Pothos . . . . ; : ; ~ -« 206 


124. 
125. 


‘126. 
127. 
128. 
129. 
130. 
131. 
132. 
133. 
134. 
135. 


136. 


137. 
138. 


139. 
140. 
I4I. 
142. 
143. 
144. 
145. 
146. 
147. 
148. 


149. 


150. 
151. 
152. 
153- 
154. 
155- 
156. 


157: 


CLASSIFIED LIST OF EXPERIMENTS 


RESPONSE oF SKIN 


Current of rest in vegetal skin : 

Isolated responses to mechanical stimulus of id ali tower porbiecs 
of grape skin . : : : ‘ 

Isolated responses of frog’s an? 

Isolated responses of skin of tomato 

Response of grape-skin to thermal shocks . ‘ 

Response of grape-skin to equi-alternating shocks 

Response of frog’s skin to equi-alternating shocks . 

Response of tomato-skin to equi-alternating shocks 

Exhibition of positive after-effect . 

Response of skin of tortoise 

Response of intact human skin . ‘ 

Illustrative response of pulvinus of AZmosa waniae sei? variation 

Illustrative autonomous senanee of Desmodium exhibiting cyclic 
variation : 

Normal response of skin of ake: ; A 

Abnormal diphasic response of skin of gecko, ceerieds to aaian | 
after tetanisation 


RESPONSE OF EPITHELIUM AND GLANDS 


Resting current in foot of snail 

Reversal of true resting current by injury . 

Response of water-melon F 

Response of glandular foot of snail 

Response of human arm-pit . 

Response of human lip 

Response of human tongue . : 
Normal response of pulvinus of eee révened sue eigntaation 
Response of Dz//enia and its reversal after tetanisation 

Reversed response of Dz//enza under feeble stimulation . 


RESPONSE OF DIGESTIVE ORGAN 


Determination of natural current of rest in digestive organ of 
Nepenthe . 

Excitatory electric deagolise of fash vibe of Nepenthe 

Response of Wefenthe at intermediate stage — 

Response of pitcher of Vefenthe with entrapped iseets 

Normal response of digestive leaf of Drosera and its reversal éter 
tetanisation 

Response of frog’s atitibeuth to iiebhaniéed pildautadion 

Response of stomach of tortoise to electric stimulation 

Normal response of stomach of ee and its reversal after eebht: 
sation ° 


Multiple response to trou Sith ule in sf piteher of Wepenilic thes: 
fresh . - ‘ : 


3B2 


739° 


PAGE 


298 


299 
300 
307 
301 
302 
393 
308 
309 
3097 
300 
303 


304 
310 


311 


318 
319 
315 
316 
319 
320 
322 


326 
328 


335 
338 
339 
339 


342 
345 
345 
346 


341 


749 


158. 


159. 
160. 


161. 
162. 
163. 
164. 
165. 


166. 
167. 
168. 
169. 
170. 
171. 
T 72: 
‘$93. 
174. 
175. 


176. 
i or 
178. 
179. 
180. 
181. 
182. 
183. 
184. 
185. 
186. 
187. 
188. 


189. 
190. 


gl. 
192. 


193. 


COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Multiple response in pitcher of Wepenthe with entrapped insects 
Multiple response in Drosera . , 
Multiple response in stomach of frog 


ASCENT OF SAP AND SUCTIONAL RESPONSE 


Electrical response of young root 

Electrical response of older root 

Responsive secretion by root 

Response of root to chemical stimulation : ; 

Electrical response of sii wood and its lepresson aniter anses- 
thetic . 

Abolition of gedit: pepenae a weed by poison 

Hydraulic response to stimulus . 

Effect of cold on suctional response 

Effect of rise of temperature on suctional responsé 

Effect of anzesthetic on suctional response 

Excitatory versus osmotic action 

Initiation of suctional response and enhanceiient under atcaatite 

Diminution of latent period as after-effect of stimulus 

Responsive variation of suction under overbalance. 

Response to sub-terminal stimulation 


RESPONSE TO STIMULUS OF LIGHT 


Transverse transmission of effect of moderate stimulus 
Transverse transmission of effect of strong stimulus 
Mechanical response of Mimosa to unilateral light . 
Electrical response of A/imosa to unilateral light 
Transmitted effect of stimulus of light 

Electrical response of Bryophy/lum to light 

Electrical response of petiole of cauliflower 

Multiple growth response under light 

Multiple mechanical response of Biophytum under hone 
Multiple electrical response in Bryophyl/um under continuous light 
Normal negative and positive after-effect under light 
Influence of fatigue on after-effect . 

Third type of direct and after-effects in alae bac light . 


RESPONSE OF RETINA 


Determination of differential excitability as between optic nerve and 
cornea ‘ 

Determination of differential away as orcas Gina aoe opéle 
nerve . 

Determination of true current 96 ea in ae S aes . ‘ 

Conversion of abnormal retinal response to normal by escitatocy 
agents 

Normal retinal response of Ophiocephatus. 


397 


401 
402 
402 


405 
407 
408 
409 
4II 
412 


417 


419 
418 


423 
427 


194. 
195. 


196. 


BOP 
198. 
199. 
200. 
201. 


202. 
203. 
204. 
205. 


206. 
207. 
208. 
209. 
210. 
211. 
212. 


213. 
214. 
215. 


216. 


217. 


218. 


219. 


220. 


221. 
222. 


223. 
224. 


225. 


CLASSIFIED LIST OF EXPERIMENTS 741. 


PAGE 
Reversed retinal response and after-effect in Ophiocephalus Fecctady 
Three parallel types of direct and after-effect of light in plant and 


animal . ; Pau Rab aaas ~ 5p Si 430 
Multiple response in hoe’ S aoe , : , , ; 426 
Multiple response in retina of Wallago. + . : : ‘ - 433 
Multiple response in human retina . : ie ep ae 
Pulsating response in human retina under juiiaueam Tight : 592 
Binocular alternation of vision . , ; : ; “ae i! 
Analysis of composite image by after- shect ‘ F i . +: 432 


GEO-ELECTRIC RESPONSE 


Response to unilateral pressure of particles : ‘ ; 7 = 37 
Determination of excited area under geotropic stimulus . ‘ - 436 
Geo-electric response . ; S ~ Sye§40 
Geo-electric response of an oad Ghesicaite ae F : - 442 


VELOCITY OF TRANSMISSION > 


Determination of velocity of transmission by mechanical response . 447 
Determination of centripetal versus centrifugal velocity . : . 448 
Effect of fatigue on velocity of transmission — . ; ; . « 449 
Effect of intensity of stimulus on velocity . ‘ ‘ : - 449 
Effect of temperature on velocity. .; . 450 
Determination of velocity of transmission by pleetroniutive wiethoul 452 
Longitudinal versus transverse conduction < : i . eee 


ELECTRIC RESPONSE OF NERVE TO THERMAL STIMULUS 


Electrical response of frog’s nerve to equi-alternating shocks . . 457 
Normal response of animal nerve to thermal stimulation . . 460 . 
Enhancement of normal response after thermal tetanisation . . 462 
Abnormal positive converted to normal negative after thermal 

tetanisation . . ‘ 463 
Gradual transition oe {honda ester : sonal Woeative 

through diphasic . P 465 
Conversion of abnormal positive to sired eis faust Sige 

by increasing intensity of stimulus : ; ° : . . 466 


ELECTRIC RESPONSE OF PLANT NERVE 


Effect of ether on electric response of plant nerve . : ; of eaQye: 
Effect of carbonic acid . 5 ‘ : : . ; a: Cae 
Effect of alcohol . ; : ‘ 5 F ‘ < «tae Oe 
Effect of ammonia . - P : spt WEA 
Effect of ammonia on response of indifferent iSueale:: : ; - 474 
Three parallel types of response in plant and animal nerve Ae iak 7A. 


Effect of tetanisation on enhancement of normal negative response. 475 


742 


226. 
227, 


228. 
229. 
230. 
231. 
232, 
233. 
234. 
mae. 
236. 
237. 
238. 
239. 
240. 
241. 
242. 
243. 
244. 
245. 


246. 
247- 
248. 
249. 
250. 
25%. 
252. 


253: 


254. 
255. 
256. 
257: 
258. 


259. 
260. 
261. 


COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Abnormal diphasic converted into normal negative after tetanisation 
Conversion of abnormal positive to normal negative through inter- 
mediate diphasic under increasing intensity of stimulation . 


CONDUCTIVITY BALANCE 


Effect of Na,CO, solution on responsivity of frog’s nerve 

Effect of CuSO, solution on responsivity of frog’s nerve . 

Effect of CaCl, on responsivity of ai nerve 

Effect of KCl ; 
Comparative effects of NaCl ant N ae on deeds 

Effect of dilute solution of Na,CO, 

Effect of stronger solution of Na,CO, 

Excitability versus conductivity under KI 

Excitability versus conductivity under NaI 

Effect of alcohol on responsivity of frog’s nerve 

Effect of alcohol on receptivity of plant nerve 

Effect of alcohol on conductivity of nerve 

Effect of alcohol on responsivity of nerve . 

Receptivity verses responsivity under alcohol 

Effect of cold on conductivity . 

Effect of warmth on conductivity . ; 

After-effect of moderate stimulation on peaduclivits and excitability 
After-effect of strong stimulation on conductivity and excitability 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF ANIMAL NERVE 


Mechanical response of frog’s nerve under electric tetanisation . 

Effect of ammonia on mechanical response 

Effect of morphia 

Effect of aconite . 

Effect of alcohol 

Effect of chloroform 

Conversion of abnormal positive to ‘Homial peace ‘heough sisbacies 
by tetanisation 

Occurrence of staircase seer 

Comparison of mechanical responses in accanl and slit nerve . 

Mechanical response of optic (sensory) nerve . 

Fatigue of conductivity in nerve of gecko. ‘ : 

Transient enhancement of contraction on cessation of tetanisation 

Multiple mechanical response in nerve as effect of drying . 


MECHANICAL RESPONSE OF PLANT NERVE 


Enhancement of excitability after tetanisation 
Effect of light in enhancing excitability of plant nerve 
Determination of velocity by mechanical response of plant nerve 


PAGE 


476 


477 


485 


485. 
486 © 


486 
487 
488 
489 
491 
492 
492 
493 
493 
494 
495 
499 
$01 
504 
595 


509 
516 
516 
516 
517 
518 


521 
524 
528 
529 
530 
536 
539 


554 
556 
525 


. 
. 


262. 


263. 
264. 


265. 
266. 
267. 
268. 
269. 
270. 
271. 


272. 


273: 
274. 


are. 


CLASSIFIED LIST OF EXPERIMENTS 


RESPONSE OF LIVING TISSUE BY RESISTIVITY VARIATION 


Determination of death-point by inversion of curve of goles 
Excitatory response by resistivity variation 


Action of anesthetics on response by resistivity iivintod in fido%s s 
nerve . . . . . . Oo . . *. e 
ELECTROTONUS 


Extra-polar effects in plant nerve 

Variation of conductivity in plant nerve by piece of suse 

Conduction of excitation with the current 5 

Conduction of excitation against the current . 

Modification of excitability by anode 

Modification of excitability by kathode . 

Variation of excitability and conductivity with pélacitiie sisesindes 
in shunt ‘ 

Variation of excitability st cinddeivity $ith patiniateg Medtrblen 
in series . 


INADEQUACY OF PFLUGER’s LAW 


Normal effects of anode and kathode on plant . 

Reversal of normal effect under high E.M.F. 

Demonstration by subjective response of opposite effects cotaeed by 
moderate and feeble E.M.F. 


MOLECULAR THEORY OF EXCITATION 


Mechanical Response : 


276. 


Mechanical response of pulvinus of Zrythrina indica, by make of 
kathode and anode 


Magnetic Response : 


277. 
278. 


279. 


280. 
281. 


282. 
283. 
284. 


285. 
286. 


287. 


Uniform magnetic responses 

Periodic groupings in magnetic response 

Incomplete magnetic recovery after strong aieniation 

Additive effect of magnetic stimuli, individually ineffective 

Magnetic tetanisation, with transient enhancement of effect on 
cessation . 

Demonstration of anes raeoene balenas 

Effects of A-tonus and K-tonus on magnetic comico . 

Opposite effects of moderate and strong K-tonus on esagrnnite con- 
duction . 

Effects of A-tonus and k K- ewe on rielgrietic excitability 

Enhancement of magnetic G-menen: by successive fongnetic 
stimuli. ‘ ‘ : ° . : 

Blocking of trainee of magnetic excitations 


743 © 


PAGE 
546 
548 


549 


561 
566 
567 
568 
569 
579 


- 572 


574 


579 
579 


582 


7A4 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


PAGE 
Response by resistivity variation : 

288. Response of aluminium powder to electric radiation . . * <3, OL 
289. Persistent after-effect on strong stimulation . : : : . 603 
290. Effect of warmth in hastening recovery . > ; gag? at one, OE 
291. Uniform responses to electric radiation . apes 602 

292. Cyclic molecular variation and concomitant snodification a response 
—characteristic curves . ‘ ; : ‘ ; : 5: ce GRD 

Effect of tetanisation at A stage, conversion of abnormal positive to 

normal negative seen in : 

293. Mechanical response of frog’s nerve. ; ; , , « 627 
294. Electromotive response of tin . : . 628 
295. Conversion of abnormal to normal thioagh dighaiivs in 7 caaioae - 629 
296. Response by resistivity variation in selenium. — . : ‘ - 631 
297. Response by resistivity variation in tungsten. : ; : «eee 

Effect of tetanisation at B stage, enhancement of normal response 

seen in : 

298. Mechanical response of frog’s nerve. : : : : - 634 
299. Responsive resistivity variation in selenium ; : . . woe 
300. Responsive resistivity variation in aluminium powder . . < 63 
301. Electromotive response of tin . : ; . : : ~' «O30 
302. Magnetic response of iron. ‘ ; ; ‘ ‘ - | 633 


Effect of tetanisation in inducing E stage, diminution or reversal of normal 
response seen in : 


303. Mechanical response of frog’s nerve . , : : * . ted. 98 
304. Mechanical response of nerve of gecko . ‘ ‘ ‘ é - “he 
305. Mechanical response of india-rubber. : ° ; : eh 16GS 
306. Responsive resistivity variation in tungsten . ‘ ‘ : . 638 


CORRELATION OF PSYCHIC AND PHYSIOLOGICAL RESPONSE 


307. Expansive and contractile response in muscle . : . 649 
308. Relation between stimulus and response in sciatic nerve ar seks «) g? 
309. Relation between stimulus and response in sciatic nerve of bull-frog 658 
310. Relation between stimulus and response in sensory optic nerve of 


Ophiocephalus . ; : : . 659 
311. Relation between stimulus anid fespolise in plant i nerve. » s B59 
312. Relation between stimulus and response in magnetic substance . 660 


EXPERIMENTS WITH SENSIMETER ON ELECTROTONIC VARIATION OF 
SENSATION 


313. Conversion of pleasurable sensation to painful under kathode 
(mechanical stimulation) . ; ‘ : . . 670 
314. Conversion of painful to pleasurable Scaaeepe ar anode (mechani- 
cal stimulation) . ; : i ; , ; y ae, ST 


315. 
316. 


317. 
318. 


319. 
320. 


321. 


CLASSIFIED LIST OF EXPERIMENTS 


Positive tone of sensation due to thermal stimulus converted to 
negative under kathode . 

Negative tone of sensation due to thermal ainalas. eoaneeed to 
positive under anode . : ‘ 


Reversal of normal effects under feeble E. M. F. : ; 671, 672 


745 © 


PAGE 
672 


672 


Differences of fusion in sensation according as it is modified to 
positive or negative . 5 F : : ; : 671, 672 
MEMORY 


Revival of latent impression in metal under diffuse stimulation é 

Revival of latent or ‘memory image’ in phosphorescent screen under 
diffuse stimulation 

Reversed or negative image . 


683 


684 
685 


INDEX 


ADDITIVE effect, 34, 595 
After-effect, persistence of, under strong stimulation, 151-154 
Anesthetics, effect of, on excitability, 130 
on response of nerve, 472, 518, 673 
on response of wood, 362 
~ re on responsive resistivity variation, 549 
Sa on suctional response, 375 
eieoazons induced by stimulus, in memory image, 686 
in metallic plate, 683 
bd "e b in phosphorescent screen, 684 
3 PR in plagiotropic stem, III 
Arm-pit, sckpouiee of human, 319 © 
Ascent of sap, various theories of, 356 
Assimilation and dissimilation, 68, 87, 308 
Autonomous response, continuity with multiple, 211 
* a in Biophytum, 211 
is in Desmodium, 212 
Reslenche theory, 502 


bP) 99 29 


BERNSTEIN on polarisation decrement, 562 
Biedermann on response of glands, 289 ase riff 
“ > of stomach, 289 

Binocular alternation of vision, 431 
Biophytum, mechanical response of, 58 

aS multiple response of, 209 

Pe positive and negative response in, 59 
Blaze current, so-called, in lead, 266 
Block, advantages of method of, 133 

» method of, 5 

Burdon Sanderson, on response of Dione@a, 12 © 


CHARACTERISTIC curves of conductivity, 621 


3 », Of differentially excitable surfaces, 324 
pr », of magnetisation, 620 
Complex sensation, dissociation of, by lag, 674, 675 
‘4s », obliteration of negative element in, by selective block, » 073s 675 


Composite image, analysis of, by after-effect, 432 
Conduction of true excitation in plants, 446 


748 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Conductivity balance, experiments with : 
+ = sy on condectivny versus receptivity sek alcohol, 


495 
Wa - ~ on conductivity versus receptivity under KI and 
Nal, 491-492 
- cs - on receptivity under alcohol, 493 
ee a me case versus responsivity under alcohol, 
495 
PP > a on responsive variation under alcohol, 494 
re sy rs on variation of conductivity under alcohol, 494 
Conductivity balance, experiments with : 
si », Variation of responsivity by alcohol on frog’s nerve, 492 
a * Py re by CaCl, on plant nerve, 486 
$5 re ne . by CuSO, on frog’s nerve, 485 
‘“e Fi Pe te by KCI on plant nerve, 487 
& Pe ‘ys te by Na;CO, on frog’s nerve, 485 
5 ms ‘6 es contrasted effects of NaCl and 
NaBr, 487 
a Pe ‘i conductivity by Na,CO, on plait ne ve, 488- 
489 
Conductivity balance, experiments with : 
% effect of cold on conductivity, 499 
+3 »» Of electric current, 565-568 
- », Of excessive stimulus, 505 
ae »» Of moderate stimulus, 504 ‘ 


», Of warmth, so1 
el oa: 553 
Corrosion figures, 349 
Crescograph, 221 
Cucurbita, electric response of plagiotropic stem of, 111, 285 
Current of death, 166 
Current of injury : anomalous variation in, 165 


mA », diminution of response with diminution of, 159 
ear i explanation of, 156 
= op its decline, 158 
a 33 positive and negative variation of, 161 
ee various theories of, 149 
Current of Reet, effect of CO, on, 122 
oe », effect of fall of temperature on, 119 
i », effect of Na,CO, on, 122 
5 », effect of section of petiole on, 225-227 
os », effect of steady rise of temperature on, 119 
‘5 »» effect of sudden variation of temperature on, 120 
- »» in animal skin, 288 
a o> ©6in Citrus, 224 
a », in Dionea, 224 
"ys »» in Ficus, 224 
9 », in foot of snail and its variation on injury, 318 


” », in frog’s eye, 418 


INDEX | 749 


Current of rest, in Mepenthe, 335 


iz », in vegetal skin, 298 

ef ,, natural, and its variations, 317 

Bs: »» phasic changes in, 302, 303 

se », physiological condition determining, 117 
aoe ,» positive and negative variations of, 126 

% ,, reversal of, as after-effect of stimulus, 124 

“ »» variation of, 175-177 


DARWIN on excitatory reaction in Drosera, 331 
Dead tissue, positivity of, 170 

Death, different Jost-mortem symptoms of, 192 
Death-point, accurate methods of determining, 194 


ss determination of, by abolition or reversal of response, 195 

od = by electric spasm, 202 

a 36 by inversion of curve of electric resistivity, 546 
a x by thermo-mechanical inversion, 198 


Depression, response by method of relative, 9 
Desmodium pulsation, electro-motive response of free leaflet, 218 


»” » » “ leaflet physically restrained, 220 

» ” > ” principal and subsidiary waves it; 
; 219 

<5 sy initiation of, under stimulus, 212 

” » » Yate of, 215 

a ss spark record of, 214 


Bens: and McKendrick on retinal current, 415 
Differential excitability, determination of: in eel, 285 


Yr) $9 ee in Musa, 114, 284 

>» 2 Pe in plagiotropic stem, III, 285 
” 2” ” in pulvinus, 303 

an. » % in retina, 419 

” » 2 in variegated leaves, 286 


Differential excitation, in memory revival, 680 
Differential response, of compound strip, 108 


e $3 of Mimosa, 108, 303 
+ laws of, 109 
Dicesinie organ, alternating phasic reactions in, 329 
i »» current of rest in, 335, 344 
Z », multiple response in, 333, 341, 343, 347 
¥5 5, response of, in Drosera, 342, 343 
9 99 » in frog, 345, 347 
» 99 9 in gecko, 346 
a : 5 in Nepenthe, 339-341 
sa a Je in tortoise, 345 
Digestive organ, response of, : 
is iy i reversal of, in Dvosera after tetanisation, 342 
ia = 4 3 in gecko after tetanisation, 346 


Dillenia indica, normal response of, 256 
» » » y9 reversal of, under fatigue, 327 


750° COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Dillenia indica, reversal of normal response, under feeble stimulation, 328 
Direct and after-effect, methods of, 275 


x 5 of light, in plant, 409-414 
ss »5 in retina, 427-430 
asoeis tia and delayed pain, 675 
ne of complex sensation, by depression of conductivity, 674 
2” oe) 29 by lag, 675 


Dose, effect of, on excitability, 136 
Drosera, response in digestive leaf of, 342, 343 
Drugs, modification of effect by tonic condition, 641 
», significance of effect of dose, 639, 640 
Drying, effect of, on nerve, 539 
Du Bois-Reymond on current of rest in frog’s skin, 288 


ee “4 on organ current, 260 

a se on positive and negative polarisation, 246 

- ¥ pre-existence theory of, 149 ; 
Dying tissue, negativity of, 169, 173 a : 
EBBINGHAUS, on rate of forgetting, 678 } 
Eel, electric response of, 285 % 
Electric discharge under excitation in leaf organ : ; 

»» In Bryophyllum, 253 F 

»» in bulb of Uriclés lily, 257. 1 

»» in Coleus, 248 3 

» in Mimosa, 268 

», in Musa, 284 ‘ 

» in Mymphea, 246 3 


in pitcher of Vepenthe, 256 
in Pothos, 286 

in Pterospermum, 255 
Electric distribution, explanation of, in dying and dead tissue, 173 


ic. “ in plant and muscle cylinder, 150, 156 
Electric organ, anterior and posterior surfaces, 242 
= », laws of response in, 248 
is », theories of, 260 
Electrical response, in absence of mechanical, 20, 220 
ey ne laws of, in anisotropic organs, 109 
a3 af »» in isotropic organs, 75 
Electrotonus, Bernstein’s polarisation decrement, 562 
i conversion of qualities of sensation by, 670-672 
KA extrapolar effects in plant nerve, 561 
ie Hermann’s polarisation increment, 563 ; 
Ws law of variation of conductivity under, 568 
oe ‘ excitability under, 571 
Pe iodiication of conductivity by, 566-568 
i variation of excitability by, 569 


Energy, hydraulic transmission of, 69 
Engelmann on current of rest in skin, 288 
Equi-alternating shocks: their advantage, 274 


INDEX 


Excitability, variation of, by chloral, 131 


9? 


9) 


29 


chloroform, 130 
formalin, 132 
HCl, 138 

KHO, 138 
NaHO, 134 
Na,CO,, 136 
sugar solution, 136 


BacBakion: true caoe of, 16 


FATIGUE, alternating, 98 
in response of metal, 7 
reversal of normal response in D2//enza under, 327 


9 


99 


93 


99 


“= on 


93 


93 


be) 


_ in Drosera under, 342 
in Mimosa under, 326 
in nerve under, 102 
in stomach of gecko under, 346 


transmitted effect in nerve under, 530 
under continuous stimulation, 95 

under overstrain, 96 

Fibro-vascular bundles, distribution of, in stem, 558 
Forgetting, rate of, 678 
Frog, response in retina of, 418, 426 
in skin of, 300, 302 

in stomach of, 345, 347 
Functions of vegetal nerve, 559 


GALENA, response of, 3 
Gecko, fatigue of conductivity in nerve of, 530, 

response in nerve of, 657 

in skin of, 310, 311 

in stomach of, 346 

Geo-electric response, in apogeotropic organ, 440 

in organ physically restrained, 442 
Gdotrenic action, hydrostatic theory of, 435 

statolithic theory of, 435 

Geotropic stimulus, determination of area excited by, 436 
Gotch on oscillatory character of electric discharge, 270 
Growth, effect of stimulus on, 73 

; temperature on, 73 


33 


be] 


9? 


59 


99 


9? 


99 


Growth pulsation, 221 
Gymnotus, electrical discharge in, 242 


HAAKE on electromotive difference in plants, 13 
Hartig on ascent of poison, 363 
Heidenhain on enhancement of excitability by section, 502 


Hermann on current of rest in skin, 288 


2) 


on polarisation increment, 563 
Holmgren on retinal current, 415 


751 


: 
—--- = ' 


752 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Human lip, response of, 321 

», skin, response of, 300 

», tongue, response of, 322 
Hydraulic response, 55 


INJURY, degree determining sign of action current, 175 _ 
see Current of Injury . 
Interference, induced difference of phase causing, 142 


af method of: effect of chemical agents determined by, 147 
* Be effect of cold determined by, 147 
Inversion of thermo-mechanical curve, 198 
“f of electro-motive curve, 202 
Pr. of curve of resistivity, 546 


KUHNE and Steiner on retinal current, 415 

Kiihne on polar effects in Protozoa, 579 

Kunchangraph, 511 

Kunkel on electro-motive variation due to water movement, 13 
», on electric reaction in plants, 13 


LATENT image, revival in phosphorescent screen, 684 | 
_ Latent impression, revival of, in metal, 683 : | 
Laws of differential response, 109 | 
s» 5, electrical response of isotropic tissue, 75 | 
s» 3, response in electric organs, 248 

+» 9, variation of conductivity under electrotonus, 568 


rey) 9 ,», excitability under electrotonus, 571 
Leaves, electric response of Bryophyllum, 253 
oes 7 i »» bulb of Uriclis, 257 4 
” ” re) », Coleus, 248 
» » >». 3, Déllenia indica, 256 
% 2 or », Mimosa, 268 
” ” - », Musa, 284 
le oe » », Wymphea alba, 246 
9 » 3 »» pitcher of Wepenthe, 256 
fe 5 a », Lothos, 286 
a 5 », LPterospermum, 255 
Light, stimulus of : clecticel response of A/zmosa under, 401 
Ae ‘ mechanical response of Mzmosa under, 400 
op ic: mechanical response under, in pulvinated and growing 
organs, 394 
A a multiple electrical response induced by, in plant, 406, 408 
‘9 19 multiple mechanical response induced by, 407 
> » » ” » ” in retina, 426 
. multiple visual impulse induced by, 430 


transmitted effect due to, 402 
Libs influence of fatigue on after-effect of, 411 
»» negative and positive responses to, 402, 406 
»» phasic responsive alternations under, 408 


INDEX 3 753 


Light, positive and negative after-effects under, 409, 412 
,», three types of direct and after-effect in plant under, 409-414 
,, three types of direct and after-effect in retina under, 427-429 
Lip, response of human, 321 


MAGNETIC balance, 607 
Magnetic conduction, blocking of, 614 


<, 93 effect of A- and K-tonus on, 609 

<3 aye enhancement of, by successive stimuli, 612 

> cs opposite effects of moderate and strong K-tonus on, 610 
3 response, additive effect in, 595 

a fe direct effect of tetanisation and transient after- effect, 623 
+5 s3 effect of A- and K-tonus on excitability, 611 

xe =F induction record, 604 

= magnetometric record of, 594 

ye ie mechanical record of, 593 

Re xi periodic, 594 

.* . uniform, 594 


Malepterurus, electrical organ of, 242 
Mechanical and electrical response, simultaneous record of, 17, 19 
Mechanical stimulator, rotary, 291 
Melon, electrical response of, 315 
Memory, an after-effect of stimulus, 677 

4 explanation of revival of, 685 

ee persistence, dependent on strength of stimulus, 677 

a5 spontaneous revival of, 680 
Memory image, negative, 684 
P Metal, abolition of response by poison, 9 
1 : »» fatigue in response of, 7 

»» response of, 6 

Mimosa, electrical response of, 20, 110, 127, 268, 326 

re electrical response under light, 401 

»» hydraulic response in, 55 

»,  hydro-positive and negative response of, 56, 59 

»» | mechanical response under light, 400 

5, phasic changes in response of, 303 

»»  teversal of response by fatigue, 326 

5, Variation of motile sensibility of, 21 

differential response of, 108 
etecaiae model, 598 ; 
Molecular modification, reversal of normal response due to, 7 
Molecular response, persistent after-effect in, 597, 601, 603 
Molecular theory of excitation, 590 
Molecular transformation, external tests of, 620 
Morograph, 197 
Morographic record by electro-motive response, 202 
vs », by mechanical response, 198 ae 
43 »» by resistivity variation, 546 

Motor transformer, 281 


754 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Munk on response in Dionea, 12 

Musa, electrical response of, parallel to Dionea, 237 
Muscle cylinder, electrical distribution in, 150 
Multiple response, electrical in Biophytum, 209 


9» wi oe in Desmodium, 218, 220 
>» » ” in Drosera, 343 

és » - €lectro-tactile in Mimosa, 209 

~ electrical in pitcher of Wefenthe, 341 
% $5 re in stomach of frog, 347 
re x mechanical, in Biophytum, 208 

93 95 se in Desmodium, 212 
” ” 9 in nerve, 539 

mf oe rheotomic record showing, 52 

re 9 under light in Bzophytum, 407 

» »» ” in Bryophyllum, 408 
4 as “ in cauliflower, 406 

on re ‘5 in frog’s retina, 426 

» % 5 in human retina, 430 
. és of growth, 221 


Nepenthe, current of rest in, 335 
a multiple response in, 341 
- three types of response in, 338-340 
Nerve, excitatory electrical changes in, 508 
me sg mechanical changes in, 509 
,, failure to conduct, 530 
Nerve of animal, electrical response of, 


me ay oa . conversion of positive to negative after 
~ thermal tetanisation, 463 

" % . - conversion of positive to negative by in- 
creasing intensity of stimulus, 466 

* +3 Ra y»» ° employment of electrical stimulus, errors 
due to, 458 

” . os Pr enhancement of normal response after 
thermal tetanisation, 462 

re 7 e sa gradual transformation from positive to 
negative through diphasic, 464 

> » » 7 to equi-alternating shocks, 457 

- 5a e a to injury of one contact, complications 
arising from, 458 


ge +3 “ ay under stimulation by thermal shocks, 460 
see also Conductivity Balance. ; 
Nerve of animal, mechanical response of, 


» ” +s i constituent twitches during tetanisation 
in, 535 

oe) ry) 2 99 effect of drying on, 539 

” ” ” ” effect of alcohol on, 517 

» » » 99 », Of ammonia on, 516 


» » » 9 »» Of chloroform on, 518 


+ 


| 
. 
| 
. 
| 
| 


INDEX Lip SF 


Nerve of animal, mechanical response of, effect of morphia on, 516 


» ” » » Sg ee tetanising electric shocks, 510 
» e 3 ss five stages in, 89, 635 

” » a -3 multiple response in, 538° | 

os 0» 33 re relation between stimulus and, 657-659 
et - e a similarities of, in plant and animal, 528 
» ” ” ” % in motor and sensory, 529 

” y *” * three types of, 519 

ox a 2 *F transient enhancement of, on cessation 


of tetanisation, 536 
Nerve of plant, discovery of, 470 


oe »» response of, conversion of abnormal to normal after tetanisation, 
475 ; 
»» » a conversion of abnormal to normal by increasing in- 
tensity of stimulus, 476 
| a ” >» a3 effect of alcohol on, 473 
i 9 > $s effect of ammonia on, 474 
‘ ss st effect of CO, on, 472 
9 ¥ 3 effect of ether on, 472 
me fe “4 effect of tetanisation in enhancing, 476 
$9 ee 4 three types in, 475 F 
a 5, Similarities of response in, and in animal, 471-478 
Nerve at plant, mechanical response of, 528 
% » 2 » determination of velocity by, 525 
» » ” % effect of light on, 557 
» > >» 9 enhancement after tetanisation, 554 
Nervous impulse, two kinds of, 647 
PS various direct and indirect manifestations of, 648 


Noll, Haberlandt, and Nemec, statolithic theory of, 435 
5 Nymphea alba, response of leaf of, 246 


Ophiocephalus, mechanical response of optical nerve of, 529 
$5 response in retina of, 427-429 

Oscillating recorder, 527 

Optic nerve and cornea, differential excitability between, 417 


PAcINI, law of, 242 
Pfeffer and Czapek on theory of geotropism, 435 
Pfliiger, avalanche theory of, 502 
Pfliiger’s law ; its failure with high E.M.F., 579 

5 aS oa with low E.M.F., 581 

5 », Of polar effects of currents, 578 
Phasic reactions, alternating, 100 

,» Variations of current of rest, 302, 303 
ns turgidity, 305 

Phenoipthalines detection of transport channels by, 360° 
| Polar effects of currents, demonstration of : s 
i ay oe os by motile response in plants, 579 
“= sy oa “3 . by subjective sensation, 582 


g 


756 - COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Poison, action of, modification of, by dose and tonic condition, 640, 641 
a on inorganic response, 9 
Positive response of sub-tonic tissue, 83 
a unmasking of, from resultant negative, 66 
Pterospermum, analogy with Torpedo, 255 
Pulvinated and growing organs, similarities of response in, 394 


REID, on response of skin of eel, 289 
Resistivity variation, determination of death-point by, 546 


3 +3 response by, and its correspondence in the other modes of 
response, 549 
v bs s3 experimental difficulties of, 540 
~ a », of metallic particles by, 3, 600-603 
a ag », Of frog’s nerve by, 549 
ae », of selenium by, 3 
Response, bifurcated expression of, 104 
- considered as molecular derangement, 590 
rs law of differential, 109 
= law of isotropic, 75 
‘3 positive, diphasic, and negative in cauliflower, 62 
” =a 29 ” in potato, 64 
‘5 simultaneous record of mechanical and electrical, 17, 19 
re various forms of, 2 . 
Response recorder, 34 
Retina, determination of differential excitability of, and optic nerve, 418 . ; 
5» excitatory after-effect in, 427-430 - 7 
», multiple response in human, 430 : 
29 2 9 in frog, 426 . 
bere es Wallago, 425 
Pe raion of, in Ophiocephalus, 427-429 
= oy in frog, 418 


5, so-called positive response of, .419 
Retino-motor effect, 421 
Retinal response, conversion of abnormal to normal, 423 
oY ue explanation of abnormal, 423 
ae ae three types of after-effect in, 427-429 
Reverser, oscillating, 276 
53 rotating, 280 
Rheotome, observations with, 47, 254 
Root as digestive organ, 349 
», response of young and old, 353, 354 
5» responsive secretion of, 352 
Rosenthal on currént of rest in stomach, 288 
Rotary Mechanical Stimulator, 291 


SACHS on growth of Cucurbita in darkness, 557 
Schonlein on oscillatory character of electric discharge, 270 
Season, influence of, on recovery from stimulus, 45 

es o on suctional response, 390 


INDEX ; TBR 


Selenium, response of, 3 
Sensation, conversion from positive to negative by tetanisation a 66 5 
+ different tones of, 646 


ks dual elements in, 674 
F effect on, of attention and inhibition, 652 
are effect on, of molecular disposition, 663, 664 
ss effect on, of progressive molecular change in nerve, 661 
is identification with opposite nervous changes, 650 
4. interchange of pleasurable and painful under electrotonus, 670- Oe 
», - negative element in, blocked by anesthetics, 673 | 
a3 positive and negative, 664 ; 
ny positive simple, negative complex, 665 


Sensimeter, 670 

Shoshungraph, 368 

Skin, abnormal response converted to normal after Seeanie ols, 31 3 
», response of, in frog, 300, 302 ' 


+r A in gecko, 310, 311 
Re By in grape, 299, 301, 302 
a Ss in intact human, 300 
rs - in tomato, 397, 308, 309 
i in tortoise, 307 
Snail, foot of, response in, 316 
aS ets resting current and its variation in, 318 


Staircase response, 91, 103, 522,524, 625, 635 
Stimulants, action of on inorganic response, 8 
Stimulation, by chemical agents, 27 


ae by equi-alternating electric shocks, 277 
% by taps, 26 
“4 by tension and compression, 25 
‘i by thermal shocks, 24 
© effect of moderate, on conductivity, 504 
a », of strong, on conductivity, 505 

a instantaneous mechanical, 47 

en response to various forms of, 24-27 

re », to increasing intensity of, 40 

ae rotary mechanical, 291 
$3 vibratory, 27 

Stimulator, electro-thermic, 38 ‘ 
“ vibratory, 30 
Stimulus, effect of, on growth, 73 

8 importance of, seen in ascent of sap, 382, 555 

” 99 9 autonomous response, 212, 555 
29 ” 9 growth, 556 

» » »» motile excitability, 554 

- opposite effects of, on highly tonic and sub-tonic’ G'tissues, 77 
4g relation between, and response : 

” » in plant-tissue, 40, 41 

2 ‘* in nerve of bull-frog, 658 

» 2 in nerve of fern, 659 

| 


e 


758 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Stimulus, relation between, in nerve of gecko, 657 

9 * in optic nerve, 659 

.; ig magnetic, in iron, 660 
Strasburger on ascent of poisons, 363 
Suctional response, diminution of latent period of, 384 


» 3 effect of anzesthetics on, 375 

* . effect of poison on, 374: 

. »» effect of season on, 390 

a 4 effect of variation of temperature on, 372 

Pe =f initiation of, by stimulus, 382 

a9 om osmotic versus excitatory action, 376, 377 

x es persistence of, particular zone being killed, 373 
= oe under method of balance, 383 - 

$s ‘5 aa »» *  overbalance, 386 

= i: », unbalanced method, 388 


TABULAR statement : 


a is electro-motive variation in Desmodium leaflet, 219 
9 . heliotropic effects, 394 

Be determination of death-point, 205 

$9 velocity of transmission in living tissues, 452 


Tapper, the mechanical, 26 


Temperature, different effects of, on conductivity and excitability, 187 


‘a effect of cold on electric response, 183 

ae effect of cyclic variation of, on electric response, 189 
9 effect of high, in abolishing response, 190 

% effect of low, on autonomous response, 181 

» effect of, on growth, 74 

* effect of rise of, on autonomous response, 182 

¥ effect of rise of, on conductivity, 450, 501 

93 effect of rise of, on electric response, 186 

i effect of steady and sudden variation of, 74 


Tetanus, genesis of mechanical and electrical, 43 
Tetanisation, transient enhancement of response on cessation of : 


os » ‘3 in brominated silver, 429 
» 9 99 in frog’s nerve, 536 
» ” 3 in frog’s retina, 428 
” » 9 in magnetic response, 623 


Tetanisation, effect of : 
A stage—conversion of abnormal to normal response, 
Electromotive response, 


+ - continuous transformation from abnormal to normal 
in platinum, 629 
‘5 in nerve of fern, 475 
1 zi in nerve of frog, 463 
5 55 in skin of gecko, 311 
Pn ‘ in tin, 628 
Mechanical response, 
an os conversion from abnormal expansive 


contractile response in frog’s nerve, 627 


to normal 


INDEX a 759 


Tetanisation, effect of : 
Response by resistivity variation, ; 
as Ai 3 in selenium, 629 
aK fe a in tungsten, 630 ’ 
B stage—enxhancement of normal response, 
. | Electro-motive response, 


ot i in nerve of fern, 475 
‘a »» in nerve of frog, 462 
= = in response of tin, 630 
Mechanical response, 
58 ss in frog’s nerve, 634 
Pe we in nerve of fern, 554 
Response by resistivity variation, 
sf me in aluminium powder, 632 
re Bn in selenium, 630 
Magnetic response, 
3 $s in response of iron, 632 


D stage—/fatigue reversal of normal response, 
Electro-motive response, 


9 ‘ reversal of normal response in Drosera, 342 
” 4 38 a in stomach of gecko, 346 
Mechanical response, 
z ee reversal of normal contractile response in frog’s nerve, 
635, 642 
9 9 rr rs <3 in indiarubber, 
642 
oe) ” > ” 9 in nerve of 


gecko, 530 
Response by resistivity variation, 
a », fatigue reversal in tungsten, 638 


Thermal chamber, 200 
os cork chamber, 500 
¥ shocks, quantitatiye stimulation of nerve by, 460 


3 »» stimulation by, 38 
variator, 113 
Time-relations, difference of, in different tissues, 45 
+s rheotomic observations on, 50 


Tongue, response of human, 322 
Torpedo, electrical organ of, 241 
Tortoise, response of skin of, 307 
a me of stomach of, 345 
Transportation channels, detection of, by phenolpthaline, 36 


VELOCITY of transmission : 
determination of, by electro-motive Inethed, 452 


oe by mechanical method, 447 
ss centrifugal versus centripetal, 448 
” effect of fatigue on, 449 


*” 


760 COMPARATIVE ELECTRO-PHYSIOLOGY 


Velocity of transmission : 
determination of, effect of intensity of stimulus on, 449 

+ effect of temperature on, 450 
2 longitudinal versus transverse, 454 

Verworn on polar effects of currents on Protozoa, 579 

Vibratory stimulator, 30 

Vision, binocular alternation of, 431 

Visual impulse, pulsatory character, 432 

Von Fleisch] on response of nerve, 278 


WALLER on ‘blaze current,’ 260 
7 on response of green leaves to light, 395 
ss on response of skin, 294 
oe on three types of response in nerve, 461 
Waves, positive and negative : their characteristics, 67 
Weber-Fechner’s law : its inadequacy, 656 
Wood, demonstration of living characteristics of, 361 
»» effect of anzsthetics and poison on pane of, 362, 36 
»» effect on, of drying, 361 
», normal response of, 361 


ZANTEDESCI on organ current, 269 


PRINTED BY 
SPOTTISWOODE AND CO. LID., NEW-STREET SQUARE 
LONDON 


YL | | aiversity of Toronto 


ae Library 


Acme Library Card Pocket 
|| LOWE-MARTIN CO. LIMITED 


= 
are 
Ee gi eT 


ee, ees ee, 
son ee * 


oes