Skip to main content

Full text of "The study of animal life"

See other formats


r yl 


Ly 


New York 
State C\pllege of Agriculture 


Al, Pornell University 
Ithaca, N. Y. 


Library 


Cornell University Library 
QL 47.747 


of anim: 


OFA i 
3 1924 003 422 676 


3 


Cornell University 


Library 


The original of this book is in 
the Cornell University Library. 


There are no known copyright restrictions in 
the United States on the use of the text. 


http://www.archive.org/details/cu31924003422676 


UNIVERSITY EXTENSION MANUALS 


EDITED BY PROFESSOR KNIGHT 


THE STUDY OF ANIMAL LIFE 


‘‘ But, for my part, which write the English story, I acknowledge 
that no man must looke for that at my hands, which I have not 
received from some other : for I would bee unwilling to write anything 
untrue, or uncertaine out of mine own invention; and truth on every 
part is so deare unto me, that I will not lie to bring any man in love 
and admiration with God and his works, for God needeth not the lies 


of men.’ 
‘Torsei's Apologia (1607). 


PREFACE 


Tuis book is intended to help those who would study 
animal life. From different points of view I have made 
a series of sketches. I hope that when these are united 
in the mind of the reader, the picture will have some 
truth and beauty. 

My chief desire has been to give the student. some 
impulse to joyousness of observation and freedom of 
judgment, rather than to satisfy that thirst for knowledge 
which leads many to intellectual insobriety. In pursu- 
ance of one of the aims of this series, I have also tried 
to show how our knowledge of animal life has grown, 
and how much room there is for it still to grow. 

A glance at the table of contents will show the plan 
of the book ; first, the everyday life of animals, next, their 
internal activities, thirdly, their forms and structure, and 
finally, the theory of animal life. This is a commonly 
accepted mode of treatment, and it is one by which it is 
possible in different parts of the book to appeal to students 
of different tastes. For, in lecturing to those who attend 
University Extension Courses, I find that seniors are 
most interested in the general problems of evolution, 
heredity, and environment; that others care more about 
the actual forms of life and their structure ; that many 
desire to have a clear understanding of the functions of 
the animal body; while most wish to study the ways of 
living animals, their struggles and loves, their homes and 


vi Preface 


societies. To each of these classes of students a quarter 
of this volume is dedicated; perhaps they will correct 
their partiality by reading the whole. 

As to the two Appendixes, I may explain that instead 
of giving references at the end of each chapter, I have 
combined these in a connected bibliography; the other 
Appendix on “ Animal Life and Ours” may show how my 
subject is related to some of the others usually discussed 
in University Extension Courses. 

My friend Mr. Norman Wyld has written the three 
chapters on “ The Powers of Life,” pp. 125-166, and Iam 
also indebted to him for helpful suggestions in regard to 
other parts of the book. I have to thank Mr. Murray, 
Messrs. Chambers, and Mr, Walter Scott, for many of 
the illustrations; while several original drawings have 
been made for me by my friend Mr. William Smith. 

Professor Knight and Mr. John Murray have given 
me many useful hints while the book was passing through 
the press, and Mr. Ricardo Stephens was good enough 
to read the proof sheets. 


ae, 


ScHOOL OF MEDICINE, 
EDINBURGH, May 1892 


CONTENTS 


PART I 


THE EVERYDAY LIFE OF ANIMALS 


CHAPTER I 
THE WEALTH OF LIFE 
1. Variety of life—e2. Haunts of life—3. Wealth of form—4. Wealth 
of numbers—s. Wealth of beauty . ' . Pages 1-17 


CHAPTER II 


THE WEB OF LIFE 


1, Dependence upon surroundings—z2. Inter-relations of plants and 
animals—3. Relation of animals to the earth—4. Nutritive rela- 
tions—s. More complex interactions : . » 18-31 


CHAPTER III 
THE STRUGGLE OF LIFE 


1. Nature and extent of the struggle—2. Armour and weapons— 
3. Different forms of struggle—4. Cruelty of the struggle. 32-45 


viii Contents PART 1 


CHAPTER IV 
SHIFTS FOR A LIVING 


1. Insulation —2, Concealment— 3. Parasitism—4. General resem- 
blance to surroundings—s. Variable colouring—6. Rapid change 
of colour—7. Special protective resemblance—8. Warning colours 
—g. Mimicry—1o. Masking—11. Combination of advantageous 
qualities—12, Surrender of parts - . Pages 46-66 


CHAPTER V 
SOCIAL LIFE OF ANIMALS 
1, Partnershipfs—2. Co-operation and division of labour—3. Gre- 
garious life and combined action—4. Beavers—s. Bees—6. Ants 
—7. Termites—8. Evolution of social life—g. Advantages of 


social life—1o, A note on ‘‘the social organism"—11. Con- 
clusions « . ‘ . . ‘ + 67-94 


CHAPTER VI 
THE DOMESTIC LIFE OF ANIMALS 


1. The love of mates—z, Love and care for offspring . 95-116 


CHAPTER VII 


THE INDUSTRIES OF ANIMALS 


1. Hunting—2. Shepherding—3. Storing—4q. Making of homes— 
5. Movements . . , . « 127-124 


PART IT Contents ix 


PART II 


THE POWERS OF LIFE 


CHAPTER VIII 
VITALITY 


1. Lhe task of physiology—z. The seat of life—3. The energy of life— 
4. Cells, the elements of life—y. The machinery of life—6. Proto- 
plasm—7. The chemical elements of life—8. Growth—g. Origin 
of life . . i r * . Pages 125-142 


CHAPTER IX 
THE DIVIDED LABOURS OF THE BODY 


1. Dtvision of labour—2. The functions of the body: Movement; 
Nutrition ; Digestion ; Absorption; The work of the liver and 
the kidneys; Respiration ; Circulation ; The changes within the 
cells; The activities of the nervous system —3. Sketch of 
Psychology . ‘ , . , + 143-152 


CHAPTER X 


INSTINCT 


1. General usage of the term—2, Careful usage of the term—3. 
Examples of instinct—4. The origin of instinct + 153-166 


x Contents PART III 


PART III 


THE FORMS OF ANIMAL LIFE 


CHAPTER XI 
THE ELEMENTS OF STRUCTURE 


t. The resemblances and contrasts between plants and animals—2. 
The relation of the simplest animals to those which are more com- 
plex—3. The parts of the animal body , . Pages 167-183 


CHAPTER XII 
THE LIFE-HISTORY OF ANIMALS 


1. Modes of reproduction—z. Divergent modes—3. Historical—4, The 
egg-cell or ovum—5. The male-cell or spermatozoon—6, Matura- 
tion of the ovum—q7. Fertilisation—8. Segmentation and the first 
Stages in development—g. Some generalisations: the ovum 
theory, the Gastrea theory, fact of recapitulation, organic con- 
tinuity é ‘ : 4 F » 184-203 


CHAPTER XIII 
THE PAST HISTORY OF ANIMALS 


1. The two records —2, Imperfection of the geological record — 3. 
Paleontological series—4. Extinction of types—s5. Various difi- 
culties—6. Relative antiquity of animals . + 204-209 


CHAPTER XIV 
THE SIMPLEST ANIMALS 


1. The simplest forms of lite—2. Survey of Prot 3. The 
Ameba—4, Structure of the Protozoa—s. Life of Protozxoa—é, 
Psychical life of the Protozoa—7. History of the Protozsoa—8. Rela- 
tion to the earth—g, Relation to other er of life—10, Relation 
to man . . . . + 210-221 


PART IV Contents xi 


CHAPTER XV 
BACKBONELESS ANIMALS 
1. Sponges—2. Stinging-animals or Celenterata—3. ‘‘Worms"— 
4. Echinoderms—s. Arthropods—6, Molluscs. Pages 222-247 
CHAPTER XVI 
BACKBONED ANIMALS 


1. Balanoglossus—2,. Tunicates—3. The Lancelet-—4. Round-mouths 
or Cyclostomata — 5. Fishes —6. Amphibians —7, Reptiles —8, 
Birds—g9. Mammals . ¥ ‘ + 248-272 


PART IV 


THE EVOLUTION OF ANIMAL LIFE 


CHAPTER XVII 
THE EVIDENCES OF EVOLUTION 


1. The idea of evolution—2. Arg ts for evolution: Physiological, 
Morphological, Historical—3. Origin of life . + 273-281 


CHAPTER XVIII 
THE EVOLUTION OF EVOLUTION THEORIES 


1. Greek philosophers—2. Aristotle—3. Lucretius—4. LEvolutionists 
before Darwin—s. Three old masters: Buffon, Erasmus Darwin, 
Lamarckh—6. Darwin—q. Darwin's fellow-workers—8. The 
present state of opinion . . . + 282-302 


CHAPTER XIX 
THE INFLUENCE OF HABITS AND SURROUNDINGS 


1. The influence of function—2. The influence of surroundings—3. 
Our own environment . 7 . . * 303-319 


xii Contents PART IV 


CHAPTER XX 
HEREDITY 


1, The facts of heredity—2, Theories of heredity, historical retrospect— 
3. The modern theory of heredity—4. The inheritance of acquired 
characters—s, Social and ethical aspects—6, Social inheritance 

Pages 320-339 


APPENDIX I 


ANIMAL LIFE AND OURS 


A. Our relation to animals: 1. Affinities and differences between man 
and monkeys—2. Descent of man—3. Various opinions about the 
descent of man—4, Ancestors of man—s. Possible factors in the 
ascent of man. B. Our relation to Biology: 6. The utility of 
science—7. Practical justification of biology—8. Intellectual 
justification of biology . : A . + 340-350 


APPENDIX II 
SOME OF THE BEST BOOKS ON ANIMAL LIFE 


A. Books on '‘ Zoology” —B. Books on '‘ Natural History''—C. Books 
on ‘' Biology" . ‘ ‘ é + 351-369 


INDEX . . . : . + 371-375 


PART I 


THE EVERYDAY LIFE OF ANIMALS 


CHAPTER I 
THE WEALTH OF LIFE 


1. Variety of Life—2. Haunts of Life—3. Wealth of Form— 
4. Wealth of Numbers—s5. Wealth of Beauty 


THE first steps towards an appreciation of animal life must 
be taken by the student himself, for no book-lore can take 
the place of actual observation. The student must wash 
the quartz and dig for the diamonds, though a book may 
help him to find these, and thereafter to fashion them into 
a treasure. 

Happily, however, the raw material of observation is not 
rare like gold or diamonds, but near to us as sunshine and 
rain-drops. Within a few hours’ walk of even the largest 
of our towns the country is open and the animals are at 
home. Though we may not be able to see “the buzzard 
homing herself in the sky, the snake sliding through 
creepers and logs, the elk taking to the inner passes of 
the woods, or the razor-billed auk sailing far north to 
Labrador,” we can watch our own delightful birds building 
their ‘homes without hands,” we can study the frogs from 
the time that they trumpet in the early spring till they or 
their offspring seek winter quarters in the mud, we can 
follow the bees and detect their adroit burglary of the 

B 


2 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


flowers. And if we are discontented with our opportunities, 
let us read Gilbert White’s Aistory of Selborne, or how 
Darwin watched earthworms for half a lifetime, or how 
Richard Jefferies saw in the fields and hedgerows of Wilt- 
shire a vision of nature, which seemed every year to grow 
richer in beauty and marvel. It is thus that the study 
of Natural History should begin, as it does naturally begin 
in childhood, and as it began long before there was 
any exact Zoology,—with the observation of animal life in 
its familiar forms, The country schoolboy, who watches 
the squirrels hide the beech nuts and pokes the hedgehog 
into a living ball, who finds the nest of the lapwings, 
though they decoy him away with prayerful cries, who 
catches the speckled trout in spite of all their caution, and 
laughs at the ants as they expend hours of labour on booty 
not worth the having, is laying the foundation of a naturalist’s 
education, which, though he may never build upon it, is 
certainly the surest, For it is in such studies that we get 
close to life, that we may come to know nature as a friend, 
that we may even hear the solemn beating of her heart. 

The same truth has been vividly expressed by one 
whose own life-work shows that thoroughness as a zoologist 
is consistent with enthusiasm for open-air natural history. 
Of the country lad Dr. C. T. Hudson says, in a Presi- 
dential Address to the Royal Microscopical Society, that he 
“‘wanders among fields and hedges, by moor and river, sea- 
washed cliff and shore, learning zoology as he learnt his 
native tongue, not in paradigms and rules, but from Mother 
Nature’s own lips. He knows the birds by their flight and 
(still rarer accomplishment) by their cries, He has never 
heard of @dicnemus crepitans, the Charadrius pluvialis, or 
the Sguatarola cinerea, but he can find a plover’s nest, and 
has seen the young brown peewits peering at him from 
behind their protecting clods. He has watched the cun- 
ning flycatcher leaving her obvious and yet invisible young 
in a hole in an old wall, while she carries off the pellets 
that might betray their presence; and has stood so still to 
see the male redstart that a field-mouse has curled itself on 
his warm foot and gone to sleep.” 


CHAP. I The Wealth of Life 3 


But the student must also attempt more careful studies 
of living animals, for it is easy to remain satisfied with 
vague “general impressions.” He should make for himself 
—to be corrected afterwards by the labours of others—a 
“Fauna” and “Flora” of the district, or a ‘“ Naturalist’s 
Year Book” of the flow and ebb of the living tide. He 
should seléct some nook or pool for special study, seeking 
a more and more intimate acquaintance with its tenants, 
watching them first and using the eyes of other students 
afterwards. Nor is there any difficulty in keeping at least 
freshwater aquaria—simply glass globes with pond water 
and weeds—in which, within small compass, much wealth 
of life may be observed. Those students are specially 
fortunate who have within reach such collections as the 
Zoological Gardens and the British Museum in London; 
but this is no reason for failing to appreciate the life of the 
sea-shore, the moor-pond, and the woods, or for neglecting 
to gain the confidence of fishermen and gamekeepers, or 
of any whose knowledge of natural history has been gathered 
from the experience of their daily life. 

1. Variety of Life.— Between one form of life and another 
there often seems nothing in common save that both are 
alive. Thus life is characteristically asleep in plants, it is 
generally more or less awake in animals. Yet among the 
latter, does it not doze in the tortoise, does it not fever in 
the hot-blooded bird? Or contrast the phlegmatic am- 
phibian and the lithe fish, the limpet on the rock and the 
energetic squid, the barnacle passively pendent on the float- 
ing log and the frolicsome shrimp, the cochineal insect like 
a gall upon the leaf and the busy bee, the sedentary corals 
and the free-swimming jellyfish, the sponge on the rock 
and the minute Night-Light Infusorians which make the 
waves sparkle in the summer darkness. No genie of Oriental 
fancy was more protean than the reality behind the myth 
—the activity of life. 

2. Haunts of Life.—The variety of haunt and home 
is not less striking. There is the great and wide sea with 
swimming things innumerable, our modern giants the whales, 
the seals and walruses and the sluggish sea-cows, the flip- 


4 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


pered penguins and Mother Carey’s chickens, the marine 
turtles and swift poisonous sea-serpents, the true fishes in 
prolific shoals, the cuttles and other pelagic molluscs ; 
besides hosts of armoured crustaceans, swiftly-gliding 
worms, fleets of Portuguese Men-of-War and throbbing jelly- 
fish, and minute forms of life as numerous in the waves as 
motes in the sunlit air of a dusty town. 


‘¢ But what an endless worke have I in hand, 
To count the seas abundant progeny, 
Whose fruitful seede farre passeth those on land, 
And also those which wonne in th’ azure sky ; 
For much more eath to tell the starres on hy, 
Albe they endlesse seem in estimation, 
Then to recount the seas posterity ; 
So fertile be the flouds in generation, 
So huge their numbers, and so numberlesse their nation.” 


Realise Walt Whitman’s vivid picture :— 


“ The World below the brine. 

Forests at the bottom of the sea—the branches and leaves, 

Sea-lettuce, vast lichens, strange flowers and seeds—the thick 
tangle, the openings, and the pink turf, 

Different colours, pale grey and green, purple, white, and gold— 
the play of light through the water, 

Dumb swimmers there among the rocks—coral, gluten, grass, 
rushes—and the aliment of the swimmers, 

Sluggish existences grazing there, suspended, or slowly crawling 
close to the bottom : 

The sperm-whale at the surface, blowing air and spray, or dis- 
porting with his flukes, 

The leaden-eyed shark, the walrus, the turtle, the hairy sea- 
leopard, and the sting ray. 

Passions there, wars, pursuits, tribes—sight in those ocean depths 
—breathing that thick breathing air, as so many do,” 


The sea appears to have been the cradle, if not the 
birthplace, of the earliest forms of animal life, and some 
have never wandered out of hearing of its lullaby. From 
the sea, animals seem to have migrated to the shore and 
thence to the land, but also to the great depths. Of the 
life of the deep sea we have had certain knowledge only 


CHAP. I The Wealth of Life 5 


Fic. 1.—Suggestion of deep-sea life. (In part from a figure by W. Marshall.) 


6 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


within the last quarter of a century, since the Challenger 
expedition (1872-76), under Sir Wyville Thomson’s leader- 
ship, following the suggestions gained during the laying of the 
Atlantic cables and the tentative voyages of the Lightning 
(1868) and the Porcupine (1870), revealed what was 
virtually a new world. During 3} years the Challenger 
explorers cruised over 68,900 nautical miles, reached with 
the long arm of the dredge to depths equal to reversed 
Himalayas, raised sunken treasures of life from over 300 
stations, and brought home spoils which for about twenty 
years have kept the savants of Europe at work, the results 
of which, under Dr. John Murray’s editorship, form a 
library of about forty huge volumes. The discovery of this 
new world has not only yielded rich treasures of knowledge, 
but has raised a wave of wider than national enthusiasm 
which has not since died away. 

We are at present mainly interested in the general 
picture which the results of these deep-sea explorations 
present,—of a thickly-peopled region far removed from 
direct observation, sometimes three to five miles beneath 
the surface—a world of darkness relieved only by the living 
lamps of phosphorescence, of silent calm in which animals 
grow into quaint forms of great uniformity throughout wide 
areas, and moreover a cold and plantless world in which 
the animals have it all their own way, feeding, though 
apparently without much struggle for existence, on their. 
numerous neighbours, and ultimately upon the small organ- 
isms which in dying sink gently from the surface like snow- 
flakes through the air. 

Far otherwise is it on the shore—sunlight and freshening 
waves, continual changes of time and tide, abundant plants, 
crowds of animals, and a scrimmage for food. The shore 
is one of the great battlefields of life on which, through 
campaign after campaign, animals have sharpened one 
another’s wits. It has been for untold ages a great school. 

Leaving the sea-shore, the student might naturally seek 
to trace a migration of animals from sea to estuary, and 
from the brackish water to river and lake. But this path, 
though followed by some animals, does not seem to have 


CHAP. I The Wealth of Life 7 


o 


been that which led to the establishment of the greater part 
of our freshwater fauna. Professor Sollas has shown with 
much conclusiveness that the conversion of comparatively 
shallow continental seas into freshwater lakes has taken 
place on a large scale several times in the history of the 
earth. This has been in all likelihood accompanied by the 
transformation of marine into freshwater species. It is 
thus, we believe, that our lakes and rivers were first peopled. 
Many freshwater forms differ from their marine relatives in 
having suppressed the obviously hazardous free-swimming 
juvenile stages, in bearing young which are sedentary or in 
some way saved from being washed away by river currents. 
Minute and lowly, but marvellously entrancing, are 
numerous Rotifers, of which we know much through the 
labours of Hudson and Gosse. These minute forms are 
among the most abundant tenants of fresh water, and their 
eggs are carried from one watershed to another on the 
wings of the wind and on the feet of birds, so that the same 
kinds may be found in widely separate waters. Let us 
see them in the halo of Hudson’s eulogy: “To gaze into 
that wonderful world which lies in a drop of water, crossed 
by some atoms of green weed; to see transparent living 
mechanism at work, and to gain some idea of its modes of 
action ; to watch a tiny speck that can sail through the 
prick of a needle’s point ; to see its crystal armour flashing 
with ever-varying tints, its head glorious with the halo of its 
quivering cilia; to see it gliding through the emerald 
stems, hunting for its food, snatching at its prey, fleeing 
from its enemy, chasing its mate (the fiercest of our 
passions blazing in an invisible speck); to see it whirling 
in a mad dance to the sound of its own music—the music of 
its happiness, the exquisite happiness of living,—can any 
one who has once enjoyed this sight, ever turn from it to 
mere books and drawings, without the sense that he has 
left all fairyland behind him?” Not less lively than the 
Rotifers are crowds of minute crustaceans or water-fleas 
which row swiftly through the clear water, and are eaten in 
hundreds by the fishes. But there are higher forms still : 
crayfish, and the larve of mayflies and dragonflies, mussels 


8 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


and water-snails, fishes and newts, the dipper and the king- 
fisher, the otter and the vole. 

As we review the series of animals from the simplest 
upwards, we find a gradual increase in the number of 
those which live on land. The lowest animals are mostly 
aquatic—the sponges and stinging-animals wholly so; 
worm-like forms which are truly terrestrial are few com- 
pared with those in water; the members of the starfish 
group are wholly marine; among crustaceans, the wood- 
lice, the land-crabs, and a few dwellers on the land, are 
in a small minority ; among centipedes, insects, and spiders 
the aquatic forms are quite exceptional ; and while the great 
majority of molluscs live in water, the terrestrial snails and 
slugs are legion. In the series of backboned animals, 
again, the lowest forms are wholly aquatic; an occasional 
fish like the climbing-perch is able to live for a time 
ashore ; the mud-fish, which can survive being brought from 
Africa to Europe within its dry “nest” of mud, has learned 
to breathe in air as well as in water; the amphibians really 
mark the transition from water to dry land, and usually 
rehearse the story in each individual life as they grow from 
fish-like tadpoles into frog- or newt-like adults, Among 
reptiles, however, begins that possession of the earth, which 
in mammals is established and secure. As insects among 
the backboneless, so birds among the backboned, possess the 
air, achieving in perfection what flying fish, swooping tree- 
frogs and lizards, and above all the ancient and extinct 
flying reptiles, have reached towards. Interesting, too, are 
the exceptions—ostriches and penguins, whales and bats, the 
various animals which have become burrowers, the dwellers 
in caves, and the thievish parasites, 

But it is enough to emphasise the fact of a general ascent 
from sea to shore, from shore to dry land, and eventually 
into the air, and the fact that the haunts and homes of animals 
are not less varied than the pitch of their life. 

3. Wealth of Form.—As our observations accumulate, 
the desire for order asserts itself, and we should at first 
classify for ourselves, like the savage before us, allowing 
similar impressions to draw together into groups, such as 


CHAP. I The Wealth of Life g 


birds.and beasts, fishes and worms. At first sight the types 
of architecture seem confusingly numerous, but gradually 
certain great samenesses are discerned. Thus we distin- 
guish as 4zgher animals those which have a supporting rod 
along the back, and a nerve cord lying above this; while 
the Jower animals have no such supporting rod, and have 
their nerve-cord (when present) on the under, not on the 
upper side of the body. The higher or backboned series 
has its double climax in the Birds and the furred Mammals. 
Indissolubly linked to the Birds are the Reptiles,—lizards 
and snakes, tortoises and crocodiles—the survivors of a 
great series of ancient forms, from among which Birds, and 
perhaps Mammals also, long ago arose. Simpler in many 
ways, as in bones and brains, are Amphibians and Fishes in 
close structural alliance, with the strange double-breathing, 
gill- and lung-possessing mud-fishes as links between them. 
Far more old-fashioned than Fishes, though popularly in- 
cluded along with them, are the Round-mouths—the half- 
parasitic hag-fish, and the palatable lampreys, with quaint 
young sometimes called ‘“nine-eyes.” Near the base of 
this series is the lancelet, a small, almost translucent 
animal living in the sea-sand at considerable depths. 
It may be regarded as a far-off prophecy of a fish, 
Just at the threshold of the higher school of life, the 
sea-squirts or Tunicates have for the most part stumbled ; 
for though the active young forms have been acknowledged 
for many years as reputable Vertebrates, almost all the 
adults fall from this estate, and become so degenerate that 
no zoologist ignorant of their life-history would recognise 
their true position. Below this come certain claimants for 
Vertebrate distinction, notably one Balanoglossus, a worm- 
like animal, idolised by modern zoology as a connecting link 
between the backboned and backboneless series, and 
reminding us that exact boundary-lines are very rare in 
nature. For our present purpose it is immaterial whether 
this strange animal be a worm-like vertebrate or a 
vertebrate-like worm. 

Across the line, among the backboneless animals, it 
is more difficult to distinguish successive grades of higher 


Io The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


and lower, for the various classes have progressed in very 
different directions. We may liken the series to a school 
in which graded standards have given place to classes which 
have “specialised” in diverse studies; or to a tree whose 
branches, though originating at different levels, are all strong 
and perfect. Of the shelled animals or Molluscs there 
are three great sub-classes, (a) the cuttlefishes and the 
pearly nautilus, (6) the snails and slugs, both terrestrial and 
aquatic, and (c) the bivalves, such as cockle and mussel, 
oyster and clam, Simpler than all these are a few forms 
which link molluscs to worms, 

Clad in armour of a very different type from the shells 
of most Molluscs are the jointed-footed animals or Arthro- 
pods, including on the one hand the almost exclusively 
aquatic crustaceans, crabs and lobsters, barnacles and 
“ water-fleas,” and on the other hand the almost exclusively 
aerial or terrestrial spiders and scorpions, insects and centi- 
pedes, besides quaint allies like the “king-crab,” the last of 
a strong race. Again a connecting link demands special 
notice, Peripatus by name, a caterpillar- or worm-like 
Arthropod, breathing with the air-tubes of an insect or 
centipede, getting rid of its waste-products with the kidneys 
of a worm. It seems indeed like ‘‘a surviving descendant 
of the literal father of flies,” and suggests forcibly that 
insects rose on wings from an ancestry of worms much as 
birds did from the reptile stock. 

Very different from all these are the starfishes, brittle- 
stars, feather-stars, sea-urchins, and sea-cucumbers, animals 
mostly sluggish and calcareous, deserving their title of 
thorny-skinned or Echinodermata. Here again, moreover, 
the sea-cucumbers or Holothurians exhibit features which 
suggest that this class also originated from among 
“worms.” 

But “ Worms” form a vast heterogeneous “ mob,” heart- 
breaking to those who love order. No zoologist ever speaks 
of them now as a “class”; the title includes many classes, 
bristly sea-worms and the familiar earthworms, smooth 
suctorial leeches, ribbon-worms or Nemerteans, round hair- 
worms or Nematodes, flat tapeworms and flukes, and many 


CHAP. I The Wealth of Life 11 


others with hardly any characters in common, To us these 
many kinds of ‘ worms” are full of interest, because in the 
past they must have been rich in progress, and zoologists 
find among them the bases of the other great branches— 
Vertebrates, Molluscs, Arthropods, and Echinoderms. 
“Worms” lie in a central (and still muddy) pool, from 
which flow many streams. 

Lower still, and in marked contrast to the rest, are the 
Stinging-animals, such as jellyfish throbbing in the tide, 
zoophytes clustering like plants on the rocks, sea-anemones 
like bright flowers, corals half-smothered with lime. In the 
Sponges the type of architecture is often very hard to find. 
They form a branch of the tree of life which has many 
beautiful leaves, but has never risen far. 

Beyond this our unaided eyes will hardly lead us, yet 
the pond-water held between us and the light shows vague 
specks like living motes, the firstlings of life, the simplest 
animals or Protozoa, almost all of which have remained mere 
unit specks of living matter. 

It is easy to write this catalogue of the chief forms of 
life, and yet easier to read it: to have the tree of life as a 
living picture is an achievement. It is worth while to 
think and dream over a bird’s-eye view of the animal king- 
dom—to secure representative specimens, to arrange them 
in a suitably shelved cupboard, so that the outlines of the 
picture may become clear in the mind. The arrangement 
of animals on a genealogical or pedigree tree, which 
Haeckel first suggested, may be readily abused, but it has 
its value in presenting a vivid image of the organic unity 
of the animal kingdom. 

If the catalogue be thus realised, if the foliage come to 
represent animals actually known, and if an attempt be 
made to learn the exact nature, limits, and meaning of the 
several branches, the student has made one of the most 
important steps in the study of animal life. Much will 
remain indeed—to connect the living twigs with those whose 
leaves fell off ages ago, to understand the continual renewal 
of the foliage by the birth of new leaves, and finally to 
understand how the entire tree of life grew to be what it is. 


12 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


SXirds 


Snakes Lizards 


a 


ae 
echinoderms. Ze 


Fic. 2 —Genealogical Tree 
The small branches in the centre indicate the classes of “worms”; the 
letters P?, B, and S indicate the positions of Peripatus, Balanoglossus, and 
Sphenodon or Hatteria respectively. 


CHAP. I The Wealth of Life 13 


There is of course no doubt as to the fact that some forms 
of life are more complex than others. It requires no faith 
to allow that the firstlings or Protozoa are simpler than 
all the rest; that sponges, which are more or less loose 
colonies of unit masses imperfectly compacted together, are 
in that sense simpler than jellyfish, andsoon. The animals 
most like ourselves are more intricate and more perfectly 
controlled organisms than those which are obviously more 
remote, and associated with this perfecting of structure there 
is an increasing fulness and freedom of life. 

We may arrange all the classes in series from low to high, 
from simple to complex, but this will express only our most 
generalised conceptions. For within each class there is 
great variety, each has its own masterpieces. Thus the 
simplest animals are often cased in shells of flint or lime 
whose crystalline architecture has great complexity. The 
simplest sponge is little more than a double-walled sack 
riddled by pores through which the water is lashed, but 
the Venus’ Flower-Basket (EZzflecte//a), one of the flinty 
sponges, has a complex system of water canals and a 
skeleton of flinty threads built up into a framework of 
marvellous intricacy and grace. The lowest insect is not 
much more intricate, centralised, or controlled than many 
a worm of the sea-shore, but the ant or the bee is a very 
complex self-controlled organism. More exact, therefore, 
than any linear series, is the image of a tree with branches 
springing from different levels, each branch again bearing 
twigs some of which rise higher than the base of the branch 
above. A perfect scheme of this sort might not only express 
the facts of structure, it might also express our notions of 
the blood-relationships of animals and the way in which we 
believe that different forms have arisen. 

But the wealth of form is less varied than at first sight 
appears. There is great wealth, but the coinage is very 
uniform. Our first impression is one of manifold variety ; 
but that gives place to one of marvellous plasticity when 
we see how structures apparently quite different are redu- 
cible to the same general plan. Thus, as the poet Goethe 
first clearly showed, the seed-leaves, root-leaves, stem-leaves, 


14 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


and even the parts of the flower—sepals, petals, stamens, 
and carpels, are in reality all leaves or appendages more 
or less modified for diverse work. The mouth-parts of a 
lobster are masticating legs, and a bird’s wing is a modified 
arm. The old naturalists were so far right in insisting on 
the fact of a few great types. Nature,’ Lamarck said, is 
never brusque; nor is she inventive so much as adaptive. 
4. Wealth of Numbers.— Large numbers are so unthink- 
able, and accuracy in census-taking is so difficult, that we 
need say little as to the number of different animals. The 
census includes far over a million living species—a total so 
vast that, so far as our power of realising it is concerned, 
it is hardly affected when we admit that more than half 
are insects. To these recorded myriads, moreover, many 
newly-discovered forms: are added every year—now by the 
individual workers who with fresh eye or improved micro- 
scope find in wayside pond or shore pool some new thing, 
or again by great enterprises like the Challenger expedition. 
Exploring naturalists like Wallace and Semper return from 
tropical countries enriched with new animals from the dense 
forests or warm seas. Zoological Stations, notably that of 
Naples, are “register-houses” for the fauna of the neigh- 
bouring sea, not merely as to number and form, but in 
many cases taking account of life and history as well. Nor 
can we forget the stupendous roll of the extinct, to which 
the zoological historians continue to add as they disentomb 
primitive mammals, toothed birds, giant reptiles, huge 
amphibians, armoured fishes, gigantic cuttles, and a vast 
multitude of strange forms, the like of which no longer 
live. The length of the Zoological Record, in which the 
literature and discoveries of each year are chronicled, the 
portentous size of a volume which professes to discuss with 
some completeness even a single sub-class, the number of 
special departments into which the science of zoology is 
divided, suggest the vast wealth of numbers at first sight so 
bewildering. More than two thousand years ago Aristotle 
recorded a total of about 500 forms, but more new species may 
be described in a single volume of the Challenger Reports. 
We speak ahout the number of the stars, yet more than one 


CHAP. I The Wealth of Life 15 


family of insects is credited with including as many different 
speciés as there are stars to count on a clear night. But far 
better than any literary attempt to estimate the numerical 
wealth of life is some practical observation, some attempted 
enumeration of the inmates of your aquarium, of the tenants 
of some pool, or of the visitors to some meadow. The 
naturalist as well as the poet spoke when Goethe celebrated 
Nature’s wealth: “In floods of life, in a storm of activity, 
she moves and works above and beneath, working and 
weaving, an endless motion, birth and death, an infinite 
ocean, a changeful web, a glowing life; she plies at the 
roaring loom of time and weaves a living garment for God.” 

5. Wealth of Beauty.—To many, however, animal life 
is impressive not so much because of its amazing variety 
and numerical greatness, nor because of its intellectual 
suggestiveness and practical utility, but chiefly on account 
of its beauty. This is to be seen and felt rather than 
described or talked about. 

The beauty of animals, in which we all delight, is usually 
in form, or in colour, or in movement. Especially in the 
simplest animals, the beauty of form is often comparable to 
that of crystals ; witness the marvellous architecture in flint 
and lime exhibited by the marine Protozoa, whose empty 
shells form the ooze of the great depths. In higher animals 
also an almost crystalline exactness of symmetry is often 
apparent, but we find more frequent illustration of graceful 
curves in form and feature, resulting in part from strenuous 
and healthful exercise, which moulds the body into beauty. 

Not a little of the colour of animals is due to the 
physical nature of the skin, which is often iridescent ; 
much, on the other hand, is due to the possession of pig- 
ments, which may either be of the nature of reserve-products, 
and then equivalent, let us say, to jewels, or of the nature of 
waste-products, and thus a literal “beauty for ashes.” It 
is often supposed that plants excel animals in colour, but 
alike in the number and variety of pigments the reverse is 
true. Then as to movement, how much there is to admire ; 
the birds soaring, hovering, gliding, and diving; the monkey’s 
gymnastics ; the bat’s arbitrary evolutions ; the grace of the 


16 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


fleet stag ; the dolphin gamboling in the waves ; the lithe 
lizards which flash across the path and are gone, and the 
snake flowing like a silver river; the buoyant swimming of 
fishes and all manner of aquatic animals; the lobster darting 
backwards with a powerful tail-stroke across the pool; the 
butterflies flitting like sunbeams among the flowers. But 


Fic. 3.—Humming-birds (/lovisuga mellivora) visiting flowers. (From Belt.) 


are not all the delights of form and colour and movement 
expressed in the songs of the birds in spring ? 

I am quite willing to allow that this beauty is in one 
sense a relative quality, varying with the surroundings 
and education, and even ancestral history, of those who 
appreciate it. A flower which seems beautiful to a bee 
may be unattractive to a bird, a bird may choose her mate 
for qualities by no means winsome to human eyes, and a 


CHAP. I The Wealth of Life 17 


dog may howl painfully at our sweet music. We call the 
apple- blossom and the butterfly’s wings beautiful, partly 
because the rays of light, borne from them to our eyes, 
cause a pleasantly harmonious activity in our brains, partly 
because this awakens reminiscences of past pleasant experi- 
ences, partly for subtler reasons. Still, all healthy organisms 
are harmonious in form, and seldom if ever are their colours 
out of tone with their surroundings or with each other,—a 
fact which suggests the truth of the Platonic conception that 
a living creature is harmonious because it is possessed by 
a single soul, the realisation of a single idea. 

The plants which seem to many eyes to have least 
beauty are those which have been deformed or discoloured 
by cultivation, or taken altogether out of their natural set- 
ting ; the only ugly animals are the products of domestica- 
tion and human interference on the one hand, or of disease 
on the other; and the ugliest things are what may be called 
the excretions of civilisation, which are-certainly not beauty 
for ashes, but productions by which the hues and colours of 
nature have been destroyed or smothered, where the natural 
harmony has been forcibly put out of tune—in short, where 
a vicious taste has insisted on becoming inventive. 


CHAPTER II 
THE WEB OF LIFE 


1. Dependence upon Surroundings—z2. Inter-relations of Plants and 
Animals—3. Relation of Animals to the Earth—4. Nutritive 
Relations—s. More Complex Interactions 


In the filmy web of the spider, threads delicate but firm 
bind part to part, so that the whole system is made one. 
The quivering fly entangled in a corner betrays itself 
throughout the web ; often it is felt rather than seen by the 
lurking spinner. So in the substantial fabric of the world 
part is bound to part. In wind and weather, or in the 
business of our life, we are daily made aware of results 
whose first conditions are remote, and chains of influence 
not difficult to demonstrate link man to beast, and flower to 
insect. The more we know of our surroundings, the more 
we realise the fact that nature is a vast system of linkages, 
that isolation is impossible. 

1. Dependence upon Surroundings.—Every living body 
is built up of various arrangements of at least twelve 
“elements,” viz. Oxygen, Hydrogen, Carbon, Nitrogen, 
Chlorine, Phosphorus, Sulphur, Magnesium, Calcium, Pot- 
assium, Sodium, and Iron. All these elements are spread 
throughout the whole world. By the magic touch of life 
they are built up into substances of great complexity and 
instability, substances very sensitive to impulses from, or 
changes in, their surroundings. It may be that living matter 
differs from dead matter in no other way than this, The 


CHAP. II The Web of Life 19 


varied forms of life crystallise out of their amorphous 
beginnings in a manner that we conceive to be analogous to 
the growth of a crystal within its solution. Further, we do 
not believe in a “vital force.” The movements of living 
things are, like the movements of all matter, the expression 
of the world’s energy, and illustrate the same laws. But 
to these matters we shall return in another chapter. 

Interesting, because of its sharply defined and far-reaching 
significance, and because the essential mass is so nearly 
infinitesimal, is the part played by iron in the story of life. For 
food-supply we are dependent upon animals and plants, and 
ultimately upon plants. But these cannot produce their 
valuable food-stuffs without the green colouring-matter in 
their leaves, by help of which they are able to utilise the 
energy of sunshine and the carbonic acid gas of the air. 
But this important green pigment (though itself perhaps 
free from any iron) cannot be formed in the plant unless 
there be, as there almost always is, some iron in the soil. 
Thus our whole life is based on iron. And all our supplies 
of energy, our powers of doing work either with our own 
hands and brains, or by the use of animals, or through the 
application of steam, are traceable—if we follow them far 
enough—to the sun, which is thus the source of the energy 
in all creatures. 

2. Inter-relations of Plants and Animals,—We often 
hear of the “balance of nature,” a phrase of wide appli- 
cation, but very generally used to describe the mutual 
dependence of plants and animals. Every one will allow 
that most animals are more active than most plants, 
that the life of the former is on an average more intense 
and rapid than that of the latter. For all typical plants 
the materials and conditions of nutrition are found in water 
and salts absorbed by the roots, in carbonic acid gas 
absorbed by the leaves from the air, and in the energy of 
the sunlight which shines on the living matter through a 
screen of green pigment. Plants feed on very simple sub- 
stances, at a low chemical level, and their most char- 
acteristic transformation of energy is that by which the 
kinetic energy of the sunlight is changed into the potential 


20 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


energy of the complex stuffs which animals eat or which 
we use as fuel. But animals feed on plants or on creatures 
like themselves, and are thus saved the expense of build- 
ing up food-stuffs from crude materials. Their most 
characteristic transformation of energy is that by which the 
power of complex chemical substances is used in locomotion 
and work. In so working, and eventually in dying, they 
form waste-products—water and carbonic acid, ammonia 
and nitrates, and so on—which may be again utilised by 
plants, 

How often is the inaccurate statement repeated “ that 
animals take in oxygen and give out carbonic acid, 
whereas plants take in carbonic acid and give out oxygen” ! 
This is most misleading. It contrasts two entirely dis- 
tinct processes—a breathing process in the animal with 
a feeding process in the plant. The edge is at once 
taken off the contrast when the student realises that plants 
and animals being both (though not equally) alive, must 
alike breathe. As they live the living matter of both is oxi- 
dised, like the fat of a burning candle; in plant, in animal, 
in candle, oxygen passes in, as a condition of life or com- 
bustion, and carbonic acid gas passes out as a waste-pro- 
duct. Herein there is no difference except in degree between 
plant and animal. Each lives, and must therefore breathe. 
But the living of plants is less intense, therefore the breath- 
ing process is less marked. Moreover, in sunlight the 
respiration is disguised by an exactly reverse process — 
peculiar to plants—the feeding already noticed, by which 
carbonic acid gas is absorbed, its carbon retained, and part 
of its oxygen liberated. 

There is an old-fashioned experiment which illustrates 
the ‘balance of nature.” In a glass globe, half-filled 
with water, are placed some minute water-plants and water- 
animals. The vessel is then sealed. As both the plants 
and the animals are absorbing oxygen and liberating car- 
bonic acid gas, it seems as if the little living world enclosed 
in the globe would soon end in death. But, as we have seen, 
the plants are able in sunlight to absorb carbonic acid and 
liberate oxygen, and if present in sufficient numbers will 


CHAP. II The Web of Life 21 


compensate both for their own breathing and for that of 
animals. Thus the result within the globe need not be 
suffocation, but harmonious prosperity. If the minute 
animals ate up all the plants, they would themselves die 
for lack of oxygen before they had eaten up one another, 
while if the plants smothered all the animals they would 
also in turn die away. Some such contingency is apt to 
spoil the experiment, the end of which may be a vessel of 
putrid water tenanted for a long time by the very simple 
colourless plants known as Bacteria, and at last not even 
by them. Nevertheless the “ vivarium” experiment is both 
theoretically and practically possible. Now in nature there 
_is, indeed, no closed vivarium, for there is no isolation and 
there is open air, and it is-an exaggeration to talk as if our 
life were dependent on there being a proportionate number 
of plants and animals in the neighbourhood. Yet the 
“balance of nature” is a general fact of much importance, 
though the economical relations of part to part over a wide 
area are neither rigid nor precise. 

We have just mentioned the very simple plants called 
Bacteria. Like moulds or fungi, they depend upon other 
organisms for their food, being without the green colouring 
stuff so important in the life of most plants. These very 
minute Bacteria are almost omnipresent ; in weakly animals 
—and sometimes in strong ones too—-they thrive and 
multiply and cause death. They are our deadliest foes, but 
we should get rid of them more easily if we had greater love 
of sunlight, for this is their most potent, as well as most 
economical antagonist. But it is not to point out the 
obvious fact that a Bacterium may kill a king that we have 
here spoken of this class of plants; it is to acknowledge 
their beneficence. They are the great cleansers of the 
world. Animals die, and Bacteria convert their corpses 
into simple substances, restoring to the soil what the plants, 
on which the animals fed, originally absorbed through 
their roots. Bacteria thus complete a wide circle; they 
unite dead animal and living plant. For though many a 
plant thrives quite independently of animals on the raw 
materials of earth and air, others are demonstrably raising 


22 The Study of Animal Life PART i 


the ashes of animals into a new life. A strange partner. 
ship between Bacteria on the one hand and leguminous and 
cereal plants on the other has recently been discovered. 
There seems much likelihood that with some plants of 
the orders just named Bacteria live in normal partner- 
ship. The legumes and cereals in question do not thrive 
well without their guests, nay more, it seems as if the 
Bacteria are able to make the free nitrogen of the air 
available for their hosts. 

3. Relation of Animals to the Harth.—Bacteria are 
extremely minute organisms, however, and stories of 
their industry are apt to sound unreal. But this cannot 
be said of earthworms. For these can be readily seen 
and watched, and their trails across the damp footpath, 
or their castings on the grass of lawn and meadow, are 
familiar to us all. They are distributed, in some form or 
other, over most regions of the globe; and an idea of their 
abundance may be gained by making a nocturnal expedition 
with a lantern to any convenient green plot, where they 
may be seen in great numbers, some crawling about, others, 
with their tails in their holes, making slow circuits in search 
of leaves and vegetable débris. Darwin estimated that there 
are on an average 53,000 earthworms in an acre of garden 
ground, that 10 tons of soil per acre pass annually through 
their bodies, and that they bring up mould to the surface at 
the rate of 3 inches thickness in fifteen years. Hensen found 
in his garden 64 large worm-holes in 144 square feet, and 
estimated the weight of the daily castings at about 2 
cwts. in two and a half acres. In the open fields, how- 
ever, it seems to be only about half as much. But whether 
we take Darwin’s estimate that the earthworms of England 
pass annually through their bodies about 320,000,000 tons 
of earth, or the more moderate calculations of Hensen, or 
our own observations in the garden, we must allow that the 
soil-making and soil-improving work of these animals is 
momentous. 

In Yorubaland, on the West African coast, earthworms 
(Siphonogaster) somewhat different from the common Lum- 
bricus are exceedingly numerous. From two separate square 


CHAP. I The Web of Life 23 


feet of land chosen at random, Mr. Alvan Millson collected 
the worm-casts of a season and found that they weighed 
when dry 10? Ibs. At this rate about 62,233 tons of sub- 
soil would be brought in a year to the surface of each 
square mile, and it is also calculated that every particle of 
earth to the depth of two feet is brought to the surface once 
in 27 years. We do not wonder that the district is fertile 
and healthy. 

Devouring the earth as they make their holes, which are 
often 4 or even 6 feet deep; bruising the particles in their 
gizzards, and thus liberating the minute elements of the soil ; 
burying leaves and devouring them at leisure ; preparing the 
way by their burrowing for plant roots and rain-drops, and 
gradually covering the surface with their castings, worms have, 
in the history of the habitable earth, been most important 
factors in progress. Ploughers before the plough, they 
have made the earth fruitful. It is fair, however, to 
acknowledge that vegetable mould sometimes forms inde- 
pendently of earthworms, that some other animals which 
burrow or which devour dead plants must also help in the 
process, and that the constant rain of atmospheric dust, as 
Richthofen has especially noted, must not be overlooked. 

In 1777, Gilbert White wrote thus of the earthworms— 


‘The most insignificant insects and reptiles are of much more 
consequence and have much more influence in the economy of 


nature than the incurious are aware of. .. . Earthworms, though in 
appearance a small and despicable link in the chain of Nature, yet, 
if lost, would make a lamentable chasm. . . . Worms seem to be 


the great promoters of vegetation, which would proceed but lamely 
without them, by boring, perforating, and loosening the soil, and 
rendering it pervious to rains and the fibres of plants; by drawing 
straws and stalks of leaves and twigs into it; and, most of all, by 
throwing up such infinite numbers of lumps of earth called worm- 
casts, which, being their excrement, is a fine manure for grain and 
grass. Worms probably provide new soil for hills and slopes where 
the rain washes the earth away; and they affect slopes probably to 
avoid being flooded. . . . The earth without worms would soon 
become cold, hard-bound, and void of fermentation, and con- 
sequently sterile. . . . These hints we think proper to throw out, in 
order to set the inquisitive and discerning to work. A good mono- 


24 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


graph of worms would afford much entertainment and information 
at the same time, and would open a large and new field in natural 
history.” 


After a while the discerning did go to work, and Hensen 
published an important memoir in 1877, while Darwin’s 
“good monograph” on the formation of vegetable mould 
appeared after about thirty years’ observation in 1881; and 
now we all say with him, “It may be doubted whether there 
are many other animals which have played so important a 
part in the history of the world as have these lowly-organised 
creatures.” 

Prof. Drummond, while admitting the supreme import- 
ance of the work of earthworms, eloquently pleads the claims 
of the Termite or White Ant as an agricultural agent. This 
insect, which dwelt upon the earth long before the true ants, 
is abundant in many countries, and notably in Tropical 
Africa. It ravages dead wood with great rapidity. “If 
a man lay down to sleep with a wooden leg, it would be a 
heap of sawdust in the morning,” while houses and decaying 
forest trees, furniture and fences, fall under the jaws of the 
hungry Termites. These fell workers are blind and live 
underground ; for fear of their enemies they dare not show 
face, and yet without coming out of their ground they cannot 
live. 


“* How do they solve the difficulty? They take the ground out 
along with them. I have seen white ants working on the top of a 
high tree, and yet they were underground. They took up some of 
the ground with them to the tree-top. They construct tunnels 
which run from beneath the soil up the sides of trees and posts ; 
grain after grain is carried from beneath and mortared with a sticky 
secretion into a reddish sandpaper-like tube; this is rapidly ex- 
tended to a great height—even of 30 feet from the ground—till 
some dead branch is reached. Now as many trees in a forest are 
thus plastered with tunnels, and as there are besides elaborate 
subterranean galleries and huge obelisk-like ant-hills, sometimes 
10-15 feet high, it must be granted that the Termites, like the 
earthworms, keep the soil circulating. The earth-tubes crumble 
to dust, which is scattered by the wind; the rains lash the forests 
and soils with fury and wash off the loosened grains to swell the 
alluvium of a distant valley.” 


CHAP, II The Web of Life 25 


The influences of plants and animals on the earth are 
manifold. The sea-weeds cling around the shores and 
lessen the shock of the breakers. The lichens eat slowly 
into the stones, sending their fine threads beneath the sur- 
face as thickly sometimes “as grass-roots in a meadow-land,” 
so that the skin of the rock is gradually weathered away. 
On the-moor the mosses form huge sponges, which mitigate 
floods and keep the streams flowing in days of drought. 
Many little plants smooth away the wrinkles on the earth’s 
face, and adorn her with jewels; others have caught and 
stored the sunshine, hidden its power in strange guise in 
the earth, and our hearths with their smouldering peat or 
glowing coal are warmed by the sunlight of ancient summers, 
The grass which began to grow in comparatively modern 
(z.e. Tertiary) times has made the earth a fit home for flocks 
and herds, and protects it like a garment ; the forests affect 
the rainfall and temper the climate, besides sheltering multi- 
tudes of living things, to some of whom every blow of the 
axe is a death-knell. Indeed, no plant from Bacterium to 
oak tree either lives or dies to itself, or is without its 
influence on earth and beast and man. 

There are many animals besides worms which influence 
the earth by no means slightly. Thus, to take the minus 
side of the account first, we see the crayfish and their 
enemies the water-voles burrowing by the river banks and 
doing no little damage to the land, assisting in that process 
by which the surface of continents tends gradually to 
diminish. So along the shores in the harder substance 
of the rocks there are numerous borers, like the Pholad 
bivalves, whose work of disintegration is individually slight, 
but in sum-total great. More conspicuous, however, is the 
work of the beavers, who, by cutting down trees, building 
dams, digging canals, have cleared away forests, flooded 
low grounds, and changed the aspect of even large tracts 
of country. Then, as every one knows, there are injuri- 
ous insects innumerable, whose influence on vegetation, on 
other animals, and on the prosperity of nations, is often 
disastrously great. 

But, on the other hand, animals cease not to pay their 


26 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


filial debts to mother earth. We see life rising like a mist 
in the sea, lowly creatures living in shells that are like 
mosques of lime and flint, dying in due season, and sinking 
gently to find a grave in the ooze. We see the submarine 
volcano top, which did not reach the surface of the ocean, 
slowly raised by the rainfall of countless small shells. Inch 
by inch for myriads of years, the snow-drift of dead shells 
forms a patient preparation for the coral island. The 
tiniest, hardly bigger than the wind-blown dust, form when 
added together the strongest foundation in the world. The 
vast whale skeleton falls, but melts away till only the ear- 
bones are left. Of the ruthless gristly shark nothing stays 
but teeth. The sea-butterflies (Pteropods), with their frail 
shells, are mightier than these, and perhaps the microscopic 
atomies are strongest of all. The pile slowly rises, and the 
exquisite fragments are cemented into a stable foundation 
for the future city of corals. 

At length, when the height at which they can live is 
reached, coral germs moor themselves to the sides of the 
raised mound, and begin a new life on the shoulders of death. 
They spread in brightly coloured festoons, and have often 
been likened to flowers. The waste salts of their living 
perhaps unite with the gypsum of the sea-water, at any rate 
in some way the originally soft young corals acquire strong 
shells of carbonate of lime. Sluggish creatures they, living 
in calcareous castles of indolence! In silence they spread, 
and crowd and smother one another in a struggle for stand- 
ing-room. The dead forms, partly dissolved and cemented, 
become a broad and solid base for higher and higher growth. 
At a certain height the action of the breakers begins, great 
severed masses are piled up or roll down the sloping sides. 
Clear daylight at last is reached, the mound rises above the 
water. The foundations are ever broadened, as vigorously 
out-growing masses succumb to the brunt of the waves and 
tumble downwards. Within the surface-circle weathering 
makes a soil, and birds resting there with weary wings, or 
perhaps dying, leave many seeds of plants—the begin- 
nings of another life. The waves cast up forms of 
dormant life which have floated from afar, and a ter- 


CHAP. 11 The Web of Life 27 


restrial fauna and flora begin. It is a strange and beautiful 
story, dead shells of the tenderest beauty on the rugged 
shoulders of the volcano; corals like meadow flowers 
on the graveyard of the ooze; at last plants and trees, 
the hum of insects and the song of birds, over the coral 
island. 

4. Nutritive Relations.—What we may call “nutritive 
chains” connect many forms of life—higher animals feed- 
ing upon lower through long series, the records of which 
sound like the story of ‘The House that Jack built.” On 
land and on the shore these series are usually short, for 
plants are abundant, and the carnivores feed on the 
vegetarians. In the open sea, where there is less vegeta- 
tion, and in the great depths, where there is none, carni- 
vore preys upon carnivore throughout long series—fish feeds 
upon fish, fish upon crustacean, crustacean upon worm, 
worm on débris. Disease or disaster in one link affects 
the whole chain. A parasitic insect, we are told, has killed 
off the wild horses and cattle in Paraguay, thereby influencing 
the vegetation, thereby the insects, thereby the birds. Birds 
of prey and small mammals—so-called ‘‘vermin”—are killed 
off in order to preserve the grouse, yet this interference seems 
in part to defeat itself by making the survival of weak and 
diseased birds unnaturally easy, and epidemics of grouse- 
disease on this account the more prevalent. A craze of vanity 
or gluttony leads men to slaughter small insect-eating birds, 
but the punishment falls—unluckily on the wrong shoulders 
—when the insects which the birds would have kept down 
increase in unchecked numbers, and destroy the crops of 
grain and fruit. In a fuel-famine men have sometimes 
been forced to cut down the woods which clothe the sides 
of a valley, an action repented of when the rain-storms wash 
the hills to skeletons, when the valley is flooded and the 
local climate altered, and when the birds robbed of their 
shelter leave the district to be ravaged by caterpillar and 
fly. American entomologists have proved that the ravages 
of destructive insects may be checked by importing and 
fostering their natural enemies, and on the other hand, the 
sparrows which have established themselves in the States 


28 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


have in some districts driven away the titmice and thus 
favoured the survival of injurious caterpillars. 

5. More Complex Interactions.—The flowering plants 
and the higher insects have grown up throughout long 
ages together, in alternate influence and mutual per- 
fecting. They now exhibit a notable degree of mutual 
dependence; the insects are adapted for sipping the 
nectar from the blossoms; the flowers are fitted for 
giving or receiving the fertilising golden dust or pollen 
which their visitors, often quite unconsciously, carry from 
plant to plant. The mouth organs of the insects have 
to be interpreted in relation to the flowers which they 
visit; while the latter show structures which may be 
spoken of as the “footprints” of the insects. So exact is 
the mutual adaptation that Darwin ventured to prophesy 
from the existence of a Madagascar orchid with a nectar- 
spur I1 inches long, that a butterfly would be found in the 
same locality with a suctorial proboscis long enough to 
drain the cup; and Forbes confirmed the prediction by 
discovering the insect. 

As information on the relations of flowers and insects is 
readily attainable, and as the subject will be discussed in 
the volume on Botany, it is sufficient here to notice that, so 
far as we can infer from the history half hidden in the 
rocks, the floral world must have received a marked impulse 
when bees and other flower-visiting insects appeared ; that 
for the successful propagation of flowering plants it is 
advantageous that pollen should be carried from one indi- 
vidual to another, in other words, that cross-fertilisation 
should be effected; and that, for the great majority of 
flowering plants, this is done through the agency of insects. 
How plants became bright in colour, fragrant in scent, rich 
in nectar, we cannot here discuss ; the fact that they are so 
is evident, while it is also certain that insects are attracted 
by the colour, the scent, and the sweets. Nor can there be 
any hesitation in drawing the inference that the flowers 
which attracted insects with most success, and insects which 
got most out of the flowers, would, Aso facto, succeed better 
in life. 


CHAP. II The Web of Life 29 


No illustration of the web of life can be better than the 
most familiar one, in which Darwin traced the links of 
influence between cats and clover. If the possible seeds in 
the flowers of the purple clover are to become real seeds, 
they must be fertilised by the golden dust or pollen from 
some adjacent clover plants. But as this pollen is uncon- 
sciously carried from flower to flower by the humble-bees, 
the proposition must be granted that the more humble-bees, 
the better next year’s clover crop. The humble-bees, how- 
ever, have their enemies in the field-mice, which lose no 
opportunity of destroying the combs; so that the fewer 
field-mice, the more humble-bees, and the better next year’s 
clover crop. In the neighbourhood of villages, however, it 
is well known that the cats make as effective war on the 
field-mice as the latter do on the bees, So that next year’s 
crop of purple clover is influenced by the number of humble- 
bees, which varies with the number of field-mice, that is to 
say, with the abundance of cats; or, to go a step farther, 
with the number of lonely ladies in the village. It should 
be noted, however, that according to Mr. James Sime there 
were abundant fertile clover crops in New Zealand before there 
were any humble-bees in that island. Indeed, many think 
that the necessity of cross-fertilisation has been exaggerated. 

Not all insects, however, are welcome visitors to plants ; 
there are unbidden guests who do harm. To their visits, 
however, there are often obstacles. Stiff hairs, impassably 
slippery or viscid stems, moats in which the intruders 
drown, and other structural peculiarities, whose origin may 
have had no reference to insects, often justify themselves 
by saving the plant. Even more interesting, however, is 
the preservation of some acacias and other shrubs by a 
bodyguard of ants, which, innocent themselves, ward off 
the attacks of the deadly leaf-cutters. In some cases the 
bodyguard has become almost hereditarily accustomed to 
the plants, and the plants to them, for they are found in 
constant companionship, and the plants exhibit structures 
which look almost as if they had been made as shelters 
for the ants. On some of our European trees similar 
little homes or domatia constantly occur, and shelter small 


30 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


insects which do no harm to the trees, but cleanse them 


from injurious fungi. 


In many ways plants are saved from the appetite of 


Fic. 4.—Acacia (A. spherocephala), with hol- 
low thorns in which ants find shelter. 
(After Schimper.) 


animals. The nettle 
has poisonous _ hairs ; 
thistles, furze, and holly 
are covered with spines; 
the hawthorn has _ its 
thorns and the rose 
its prickles ; some have 
repulsive odours; others 
contain oils, acids, fer- 
ments, and poisons 
which many animals 
dislike ; the cuckoo-pint 
(Arum) is full of little 
crystals which make our 
lips smart if we nibble 
a leaf. In our studies 
of plants we endeavour 
to find out what these 
qualities primarily mean 
to their possessors ; here 
we think rather of their 
secondary significance 
as protections against 
animals. For though 
snails ravage all the 
plants in a district ex- 
cept those which are 
repulsive, the snails are 
at most only the second- 
ary factors in the evolu- 
tion of the repulsive 
qualities, 


The strange inter-relations between plants and animals 
are again illustrated by the carnivorous, generally insecti- 
vorous, plants. It is not our business to discuss the 
original or primary import of the pitchers of pitcher-plants, 


CHAP. 11 The Web of Life 31 


or of the mobile and sensitive leaves of Venus’ Fly-Trap ; 
nowadays, at any rate, insects are attracted to them, 
captured by them, and used. Let us take only one case, 
that of the common Bladderwort (Utricularia). Many of 
the leaflets of this plant, which floats in summer in the 
marsh pond, are modified into little bladders, so fashioned 
that minute “ water-fleas ”—-which swarm in every corner of 
the pool—can readily enter them, but can in no wise get out 
again. The small entrance is guarded by a valve or door, 
which opens inwards, but allows no egress. The little crusta- 
ceans are attracted by some mucilage made by the leaves, or 
sometimes perhaps by sheer curiosity ; they enter and cannot 
return ; they die, and their débris is absorbed by the leaf. 

Again, in regard to distribution, there are numerous 
relations between organisms, Spiny fruits like those of 
Jack-run-the-hedge adhere to animals, and are borne from 
place to place; and minute water-plants and animals are 
carried from one watercourse to another on the muddy 
feet of birds. Darwin removed a ball of mud from the 
leg of a bird, and from it fourscore seeds germinated. Not 
a bird can fall to the ground and die without sending a 
throb through a wide circle. 

A conception of these chains or circles of influence 
is important, not only for the sake of knowledge, but also as 
a guide in action. Thus, to take only one instance among 
a hundred, it may seem a far cry from a lady’s toilet-table 
to the African slave-trade, but when we remember the ivory 
backs of the brushes, and how the slaves are mainly used for 
transporting the tusks of elephants—a doomed race—from 
the interior to the coast, the riddle is read, and the respon- 
sibility is obvious. Over a ploughed field in the summer 
morning we see the spider-webs in thousands glistening 
with mist-drops, and this is an emblem of the intricacy of 
the threads in the web of life—to be seen more and more 
as our eyes grow clear. Or, is not the face of nature like 
the surface of a gentle stream, where hundreds of dimpling 
circles touch and influence one another in an infinite com- 
plexity of action and reaction beyond the ken of the wisest ? 


CHAPTER III 
THE STRUGGLE OF LIFE 


1. Nature and Extent of the Struggle—2. Armour and Weapons— 
3. Different Forms of Struggle—4. Cruelty of the Struggle 


1. Nature and Extent of the Struggle.—If we realise 
what is meant by the “web of life,” the recognition of 
the “struggle for existence” cannot be difficult. Animals 
do not live in isolation, neither do they always pursue 
paths of peace. Nature is not like a menagerie where 
beast is separated from beast by iron bars, neither is it 
a mélée such as would result if the bars of all the cages 
were at once removed. It is not a continuous Waterloo, 
nor yet an amiable compromise between weaklings. The 
truth lies between these extremes. In most places where 
animals abound there is struggle. This may be silent and 
yet decisive, real without being very cruel, or it may be 
full of both noise and bloodshed. 

This struggle is very old; it is older than the conflicts 
of men, older than the ravin of tooth and claw, it is as old 
as life. The struggle is often very keen—often for life or 
death. But though few animals escape experience of the 
battlefield—and for some there seems no discharge from 
this war—we must not misinterpret nature as ‘a continual 
free-fight.” One naturalist says that all nature breathes a 
hymn of love, but he is an optimist under sunny southern 
skies ; another compares nature to a huge gladiatorial 
show with a plethora of fighters, but he speaks as a pes- 


CHAP, III The Struggle of Life 33 


simist from amid the din of individualistic competition. 
Nature is full of struggle and fear, but the struggle is 
sometimes outdone by sacrifice, and the fear is sometimes 
cast out by love. We must be careful to remember 
Darwin’s proviso that he used the phrase “struggle for 
existence ” ‘in a large and metaphorical sense, including the 
dependence of one being on another, and including (which 
is more important) not only the life of the individual, but 
success in leaving progeny.” He also acknowledged the 
importance of mutual aid, sociability, and sympathy among 
animals, though he did not carefully estimate the relative 
importance of competition on the one hand and sociability 
on the other. Discussing sympathy, Darwin wrote, “In 
however complex a manner this feeling may have originated, 
as it is one of high importance to all those animals which 
aid and defend one another, it will have been increased 
through natural selection; for those communities which 
included the greatest number of the most sympathetic 
members would flourish best, and rear the greatest number 
of offspring.” I should be sorry to misrepresent the 
opinions of any man, but after considerable study of 
modern Darwinian literature, I feel bound to join in the 
protest which others have raised against a tendency to 
narrow Darwin’s conception of ‘‘the struggle for existence,” 
by exaggerating the occurrence of internecine competitive 
struggle. Thus Huxley says, ‘Life was a continuous free- 
fight, and beyond the limited and temporary relations of 
the family, the Hobbesian war of each against all was the 
normal state of existence.” Against which Kropotkine 
maintains that this ‘view of nature has as little claim to 
be taken as a scientific deduction as the opposite view of 
Rousseau, who saw in nature but love, peace, and harmony 
destroyed by the accession of man.” . . . ‘‘ Rousseau has 
committed the error of excluding the beak-and-claw fight 
from his thoughts, and Huxley is committing the opposite 
error; but neither Rousseau’s optimism nor Huxley’s pessi- 
mism can be accepted as an impartial interpretation of 
nature.” 

2. Armour and Weapons.—If you doubt the reality 

D 


34 The Study of Animal Life PART } 


of the struggle, take a survey of the different classes of 
animals. Everywhere they brandish weapons or are forti- 
fied with armour. ‘The world,” Diderot said, “is the 
abode of the strong.” Even some of the simplest 
animals have offensive threads, prophetic of the poison- 
ous lassoes with which jellyfish and sea-anemones are 
equipped, Many worms have horny jaws; crustaceans 
have strong pincers; many insects have stings, not to 
speak of mouth organs like surgical instruments ; spiders 
give poisonous bites ; snails have burglars’ files; the cuttle- 
fish have strangling suckers and parrots’ beaks. Among 
backboned animals we recall the teeth of the shark and the 
sword of the swordfish, the venomous fangs of serpents, the 
jaws of crocodiles, the beaks and talons of birds, the horns 
and hoofs and canines of mammals. Now we do not say 
that these and a hundred other weapons were from their 
first appearance weapons, indeed we know that most of 
them were not. But they are weapons now, and just as we 
would conclude that there was considerable struggle in a 
community where every man bore a revolver, we must 
draw a similar inference from the offensive equipment of 
animals. 

As to armoured beasts, we remember that shells of lime 
or flint occur in many of the simplest animals, that most 
sponges are so rich in spicules that they are too gritty to 
be pleasant eating, that corals are polypes within shells 
of lime, that many worms live in tubes, that the members 
of the starfish class are in varying degrees lime-clad, that 
crustaceans and insects are emphatically armoured animals, 
and that the majority of molluscs live in shells. So among 
backboned animals, how thoroughly bucklered were the 
fishes of the old red sandstone agaifist hardly less effect- 
ive teeth, how the scales of modern fishes glitter, how 
securely the sturgeon swims with its coat of bony mail! 
Amphibians are mostly weaponless and armourless, but 
reptiles are scaly animals far excellence, and the tortoise, 
for instance, lives in an almost impregnable citadel. Birds 
soar above pursuit, and mammals are swift and strong, 
but among the latter the armadillos have bony shields of 


CHAP. HI The Struggle of Life 35 


marvellous strength, and hedgehog and porcupine have 
their hair hardened into spines and quills. Now we do not 
say that all these structures were from the first of the 
nature of armour, indeed they admit of other explanations, 
but that they serve as armour now there can be no doubt. 
And ‘just as we conclude that a man would not wear 
a chain shirt without due reason, so we argue from the 
prevalence of animal armour to the reality of struggle. 

For a moment let me delay to explain the two saving- 
clauses which I have inserted. The pincers of a crab are 
modified legs, the sting of a bee has probably the same 
origin, and it is likely that most weapons originally served 
some other than offensive purpose. We hear of spears 
becoming pruning-hooks ; the reverse has sometimes been 
true alike of animals and of men. By sheer use a structure 
not originally a weapon became strong to slay; for there 
is a profound biological truth in the French proverb: “A 
Jorce de forger on devient forgeron,” 

And again as to armour, it is, or was, well known that a 
boy’s hand often smitten by the “‘ tawse” became callous as 
to its epidermis. Now that callousness was not a device— 
providential or otherwise—to save the youth from the pains 
of chastisement, and yet it had that effect. By bearing 
blows one naturally and necessarily becomes thick-skinned. 
Moreover, the epidermic callousness referred to might be 
acquired by work or play altogether apart from school 
discipline, though it might also be the effect of the blows. 
In the same way many structures which are most useful as 
armour may be the “mechanical” or natural results of 
what they afterwards help to obviate, or they may arise 
quite apart from their future significance. 

3. Different Forms of Struggle.—If you ask why 
animals do not live at peace, I answer, more Scottico, 
Why do not we? The desires of animals conflict with 
those of their neighbours, hence the struggle for bread 
and the competition for mates. Hunger and love solve 
the world’s problems. Mouths have to be filled, but 
population tends locally and temporarily to outrun the 
means of subsistence, and the question ‘“‘ which mouths” 


Csayeg worg) ‘seqouy Sunpune (vyivynnew asc) rapids Zurqoyeo-pag—'S ‘1 


CHAP. III The Struggle of Life 37 


has to be decided—sometimes by peaceful endeavour, as 
in migration, sometimes with teeth clenched or ravenous. 
Many animals are carnivorous, and must prey upon weaker 
forms, which do their best to resist. Mates also have to 
be won, and lover may fight with lover till death is stronger 
than both. But these struggles for food and for mates are 
often strivings rather than strife, nor is a recognition of the 
frequent keenness and fierceness of the competition incon- 
sistent with the recognition of mutual aid, sociability, and 
love. ‘There is a third form of the struggle,—that between 
an animal and its changeful surroundings. This also is a 
struggle without strife. Fellow competitors strive for their 
share of the limited means of subsistence; between foes 
there is incessant thrust and parry; in the courtship of 
mates there are many disappointed and worsted suitors ; 
over all are the shears of fate—a changeful physical 
environment which has no mercy. 

An analysis of the various forms of struggle may be 
attempted as follows : 


{ (a) Between animals of the same kind which 
compete for similar food and other 
necessaries of life—Struggle between 

For fellows. 
Food (4) Between animals of different kinds, the 
one set striving to devour, the other set 
. endeavouring to escape their foes, e.g. 
' between carnivores and herbivores— 
L Struggle between foes. 

doe (c) Between the rival suitors for desired 
Loud ee between rivals in 
ae (2) Between animals and changeful surround- 

hold J ings—Struggle with fate. 


In most cases, besides the egoism or individualism, one 
must recognise the existence of altruism, parental love and 
sacrifice, mutual aid, care for others, and sociality. 


38 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


Before we consider these different forms of struggle, let 
us notice the rapid multiplication of individuals which 
furnishes the material for what in ‘“‘a wide and meta- 
phorical sense” may be called a “ battlefield.” 

A single Infusorian may be the ancestor of millions by 
the end of a week. A female aphis, often producing one 
offspring per hour for days together, might in a season be 
the ancestor of a progeny of atomies which would weigh 
down five hundred millions of stout men. ‘The roe of a 
cod contains sometimes nearly ten million eggs, and sup- 
posing each of these produced a young fish which arrived 
at maturity, the whole sea would immediately become a 
solid mass of closely packed codfish.” The unchecked 
multiplication of a few mice or rabbits would soon leave no 
standing-room on earth. 

But fortunately, with the exception of the Infusorians, these 
multiplications do not occur. We have to thank the 
struggle in nature, and especially the physical environment, 
that they do not. The fable of Mirza’s bridge is continually 
true,—few get across. 

(a) It is often said that the struggle between fellows of the 
same kind and with the same needs is keenest of all, but 
this is rather an assumption than an induction from facts. 
The widespread opinion is partly due to an @ prior con- 
sideration of the problem, partly to that anthropomorphism 
which so easily besets us. We transfer to the animal 
world our own experience of keen competition with fellows 
of the same caste, and in so doing are probably unjust. 
Thus Mr. Grant Allen says— 


“The baker does not fear the competition of the butcher in the 
struggle for life; it is the competition of the other bakers that 
sometimes inexorably crushes him out of existence. . . . In this 
way the great enemies of the individual herbivores are not the 
carnivores, but the other herbivores. . . . It is not so much the 
battle between the tiger and the antelope, between the wolf and 
the bison, between the snake and the bird, that ultimately results 
in natural selection or survival of the fittest, as the struggle between 
tiger and tiger, between bison and bison, between snake and snake, 
between antelope and antelope. . . . Homo homint lupus, says 
the old proverb, and so, we may add, in a wider sense, /upus /upo 


CHAP, Il] The Struggle of Life 39 


lupus, also. . . . The struggle is fierce between allied kinds, and 
fiercest of all between individual members of the same species.” 


I have quoted these sentences because they are clearly and 
cleverly expressed, after the manner of Grant Allen, but I 
do not believe that they are true statements of facts. The 
evidence is very unsatisfactory. In his paragraph sum- 
marised as “struggle for life most severe between indi- 
viduals and varieties of the same species; often severe 
between species of the same genus,” Darwin gave five 
illustrations; one species of swallow is said to have ousted 
another in North America, the missel-thrush has increased 
in Scotland at the expense of the song-thrush, the brown 
rat displaces the black rat, the small Asiatic cockroach 
drives its great congener before it, the hive-bee imported 
to Australia is rapidly exterminating the small, stingless 
native bee. But the cogency of these instances may be 
disputed: thus what is said about the thrushes is denied by 
Professor Newton, And on the other hand, we know that 
reindeer, beavers, lemming, buffaloes and many other 
animals migrate when the means of subsistence are unequal 
to the demands of the population, and there are other 
peaceful devices by which animals have discovered a way 
out of a situation in which a life-and-death struggle might 
seem inevitable. Very instructive is the fact that beavers, 
when too numerous in one locality, divide into two parties 
and migrate up and down stream. The old proverb which 
Grant Allen quotes, Homo homind lupus, appears to me a 
libellous inaccuracy; the extension of, the libel to the 
animal world has certainly not been justified by careful 
induction. For a discussion of the alleged competition 
between fellows, I refer, and that with pleasure and grati- 
tude, to Kropotkine’s articles on “Mutual Aid among 
Animals,” Vineteenth Century, September and November 
1890. 

(4) Of the struggle between foes differing widely in kind 
little need be said. It is very apparent, especially in wild 
countries. Carnivores prey upon herbivores, which some- 
times unite in successful resistance. Birds of prey devour 


40 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


small mammals, and sometimes have to fight hard for their 
booty. Reptiles also have their battles—witness the combats 
between snake and mongoose. In many cases, however, 
carnivorous animals depend upon small fry; thus many 
birds feed on fishes, insects, and worms, and many fishes 
live on minute crustaceans. In such cases the term 


Fic. 6.—Weasel attacking a grouse. (From St. John’s II 7d S/orts.) 


struggle must again be used “in a wide and metaphorical 
sense.” 

(c) Ina great number of cases there is between rival males 
a contest for the possession of the females,—a competition 
in which beauty and winsomeness are sometimes as im- 
portant as strength. Contrast the musical competition 
between rival songsters with the fierce combats of the stags, 


CHAP. IIT The Struggle of Life 41 


Many animals are not monogamous, and this causes strife ; 
a male seal, for instance, guards his harem with ferocity. 

(@) Finally, physical nature is quite careless of life. Changes 
of medium, temperature, and moisture, continually occur, 
and the animals flee for their lives, adapt themselves to 
new conditions, or perish. Cataclysms are rare, but 
changes are common, and especially in such schools of 
experience as the sea-shore we may study how vicissitude 
has its victims or its victors, 

The struggle with Fate, that is to say, with changeful 
surroundings, is more pleasant to contemplate than the 
other kinds of struggle, for at the rigid mercilessness of 
physical nature we shudder less than at the cruel competi- 
tion between living things, and we are pleased with the 
devices by which animals keep their foothold against wind 
and weather, storm and tide, drought and cold. One illus- 
tration must suffice: drought is common, pools are dried up, 
the inhabitants are left to perish. But often the organism 
draws itself together, sweats off a protective sheath, which 
is not a shroud, and waits until the rain refreshes the pools. 
Not the simplest animals only, but some of comparatively 
high degree, are thus able to survive desiccation. The 
simplest animals encyst, and may be blown about by the 
wind, but they rest where moisture moors them, and are 
soon as lively as ever. Leaping a long way upwards, we 
find that the mud-fish (Profofterus) can be transported 
from Africa to Northern Europe, dormant, yet alive, 
within its ball of clay. We do not believe in toads appear- 
ing out of marble mantelpieces, and a paleontologist will 
but smile if you tell him of a frog which emerged from an 
intact piece of old red sandstone, but amphibians may 
remain for a long time dormant either in the mud of their 
native pools or in some out-of-the-way chink whither they 
had wandered in their fearsome youth. 

A shop which had once been used in the preparation 
of bone-dust was after prolonged emptiness reinstated in a 
new capacity. But it was soon fearfully infested with mites 
(Glyciphagus), which had been harboured in crevices in a 
strange state of dry dormancy. Every mite had in a sense 


42 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


died, but remnant cells in the body of each had clubbed 
together in a life-preserving union so effective that a return 
of prosperity was followed by a reconstitution of mites and 
by a plague of them. Of course great caution must be 
exercised with regard to all such stories, as well as in 
regard to the toads within stones. Of common little 
animals known as Rotifers, it is often said, and sometimes 
rightly, that they can survive prolonged desiccation. In a 
small pool on the top of a granite block, there flourished a 
family of these Rotifers. Now this little pool was period- 
ically swept dry by the wind, and in the hollow there 
remained only a scum of dust. But when the rain returned 
and filled the pool, there were the Rotifers as lively as 
ever. What inference was more natural than that the 
Rotifers survived the desiccation, and lay dormant till 
moisture returned? But Professor Zacharias thought :he 
would like to observe the actual revivification, and taking 
some of the dusty scum home, placed it under his micro- 
scope on a moist slide, and waited results. There were the 
corpses of the Rotifers plain enough, but they did zof revive 
even in abundant moisture. What was the explanation ? 
The eggs of these Rotifers survived, they developed rapidly, 
they reinstated the family. And of course it is much easier 
to understand how single cells, as eggs are, could survive 
being dried up, while their much more complex parents 
perished. Ido not suggest that no Rotifers can survive 
desiccation, it is certain that some do; but the story I 
have told shows the need of caution. There is no doubt, 
moreover, that certain simple ‘‘ worms,” known as “paste- 
eels,” ‘ vinegar-eels,” etc., from their frequent occurrence 
in such substances, can survive desiccation for many years. 
Repeated experiments have shown that they can lie dormant 
for as long as, but not longer than, fourteen years! and it 
is interesting to notice that the more prolonged the period 
of desiccation has been, the longer do these threadworms 
take to revive after moisture has been supplied. It seems 
as if the life retreated further and further, till at length it 
may retreat beyond recall. In regard to plants there are 


many similar facts, for though accounts of the germination 
\ 


CHAP. 111 The Struggle of Life 43 


of seeds from the mummies of the pyramids, or from the 
graves of the Incas, are far from satisfactory, there is no 
doubt that seeds of cereals and leguminous plants may 
retain their life in a dormant state for years, or even for 
tens of years. 

But desiccation is only one illustration out of a score 
of the manner in which animals keep their foothold against 
fate. I need hardly say that they are often unsuccessful ; 
the individual has often fearful odds against it. How many 
winged seeds out of a thousand reach a fit resting-place 
where they may germinate? Professor Mébius says that 
out of a million oyster embryos only one individual grows 
up, a mortality due to untoward currents and surroundings, 
as well as to hungry mouths. Yet the average number of 
thistles and oysters tends to continue, “So careful of the 
type she seems, so careless of the single life.” Yet though 
the average usually remains constant, there is no use trying 
to ignore, what Richard Jefferies sometimes exaggerated, 
that the physical fates are cruel to life. But how much 
wisdom have they drilled into us? 


“¢ For life is not as idle ore, 

But iron dug from central gloom, 
And heated hot with burning fears, 
And dipt in baths of hissing tears, 

And battered by the shocks of doom 

To shape and use.” 


4. Cruelty of the Struggle.—Opinions differ much as 
to the cruelty of the “struggle for existence,” and the 
question is one of interest and importance. Alfred Russel 
Wallace and others try to persuade us that our conception 
of the ‘‘cruelty of nature” is an anthropomorphism ; that, 
like Balbus, animals do not fear death; that the rabbit 
rather enjoys a run before the fox; that thrilling pain soon 
brings its own anesthetic; that violent death has its 
pleasures, and starvation its excitement. Mr. Wallace, 
‘who speaks with the authority of long and wide ex- 
perience, enters a vigorous protest against Professor 
Huxley’s description of the myriads of generations of 


44 The Study of Animal Life PART } 


herbivorous animals “which have been tormented and 
devoured by carnivores” ; of both alike ‘‘ subject to all the 
miseries incidental to old age, disease, and over-multiplica- 
tion”; of the “more or less enduring suffering” which is 
the meed of both vanquished and victor; of the whole 
creation groaning in pain. ‘There is good reason to 
believe,” says Mr. Wallace, “that the supposed torments 
and miseries of animals have little real existence, but are 
the reflection of the imagined sensations of cultivated men 
and women in similar circumstances ; and that the amount 
of actual suffering caused by the struggle for existence 
among animals is altogether insignificant.” ‘Animals are 
spared from the pain of anticipating death ; violent deaths, 
if not too prolonged, are painless and easy; neither do 
those which die of cold or hunger suffer much ; the popular 
idea of the struggle for existence entailing misery and pain 
on the animal world is the very reverse of the truth.” He 
concludes by quoting the conclusion of Darwin's chapter on 
the struggle for existence: ‘When we reflect on this 
struggle, we may console ourselves with the full belief that 
the war of nature is not incessant, that no fear is felt, that 
death is generally prompt, and that the vigorous, the 
healthy, and the happy survive and multiply.” Yet it was 
Darwin who confessed that he found in the world ‘too 
much misery.” 

We have so little security in appreciating the real life— 
the mental and physical pain or happiness—of animals, that 
there is apt to be exaggeration on both sides, according as 
a pessimistic or an optimistic mood predominates. I there- 
fore leave it to be settled by your own observation whether 
hunted and captured, dying and starving, maimed and half- 
frozen animals have to endure “an altogether insignificant 
amount of actual suffering in the struggle for existence.” 

But I think we must admit that there is much truth in 
what Mr. Wallace urges. Moreover, the term cruelty can 
hardly be used with accuracy when the involved infliction 
of pain is necessary. In many cases the carnivores are 
less “cruel” to their victims than we are to our domesti- 
cated animals. We must also remember that the “ struggle 


CHAP. III The Struggle of Life 45 


for existence” is often applicable only in its “wide and 
metaphorical sense.” And it is fair to balance the happiness 
and mutual helpfulness of animals against the pain and 
deathful competition which undoubtedly exist. 

What we must protest against is that one-sided inter- 
pretation according to which individualistic competition is 
nature’s sole method of progress. We are told that animals 
have got on by their struggle for individual ends ; that they 
have made progress on the corpses of their fellows, by a 
‘blood and iron” competition in which each looks out for 
himself, and extinction besets the hindmost. To those who 
accept this interpretation the means employed seem justified 
by the results attained. But it is only in after-dinner talk 
that we can slur over whatever there is of pain and cruelty, 
overcrowding and starvation, hate and individualism, by 
saying complacently that they are justified in us their 
children; that we can rest satisfied that what has been 
called “a scheme of salvation for the elect by the damnation 
of the vast majority” is a true statement of the facts; that 
we can seriously accept a one-sided account of nature’s 
regime as a justification of our own ethical and economic 
practice. 

The conclusions, which I shall afterwards seek to 
substantiate, are, that the struggle for existence, with its 
associated natural selection, often involves cruelty, but 
certainly does not always do so; that joy and happiness, 
helpfulness and co-operation, love and sacrifice, are also 
facts of nature, that they also are justified by natural 
selection ; that the precise nature of the means employed 
and ends attained must be carefully considered when 
we seek from the records of animal evolution support 
or justification for human conduct; and that the tragic 
chapters in the history of animals (and of men) must be 
philosophically considered in such light as we can gather 
from what we know of the whole book. 


CHAPTER IV 
SHIFTS FOR A LIVING 


1. Lnsulation—2. Concealment—3. Parasitism—4. General Re- 
semblance to Surroundings—s. Variable Colouring—6. Rapid 
Change of Colour—7. Special Protective Resemblance—8. 
Warning Colours—9. Mimicry—io. Masking—11. Com- 
bination of Advantageous Qualities—12. Surrender of Parts 


GRANTING the struggle with fellows, foes, and fate, we are 
led by force of sympathy as well as of logic to think of the 
shifts for a living which tend to be evolved in such con- 
ditions, and also of some other ways by which animals 
escape from the intensity of the struggle. 

1. Insulation.—Some animals have got out of the 
struggle through no merit of their own, but as the result 
of geological changes which have insulated them from 
their enemies. Thus, in Cretaceous times probably, the 
marsupials which inhabited the Australasian region were 
insulated. In that region they were then the only re- 
presentatives of Mammalia, and so, excepting the “native 
dog,” some rodents and bats, and more modern imports, 
they still continue to be. By their insulation they were 
saved from that contest with stronger mammals in which 
all the marsupials left on the other continents were 
exterminated, with the exception of the opossums, which 
hide in American forests. A similar geological insulation 
accounts for the large number of lemurs in the island of 
Madagascar. 


CHAP. IV Shifts for a Living 47 


2. Concealment.—A change of habitat and mode of life 
is often as significant for animals as it is for men, It is 
easy to understand how mammals which passed from 
terrestrial to more or less aquatic life, for instance beaver 
and polar bear, seals, and perhaps whales, would enjoy 
a period of relative immunity after the awkward time 
of transition was over. So, too, many must have passed 
from the battlefield of the sea-shore to relative peace 
on land or in the deep-sea. Ina change from open air 
to underground life, illustrated for instance in the mole, 
many animals have sought and found safety, and the 
change seems even now in progress, as in the New 
Zealand parrot Stringofs, which, having lost the power 
of flight, has taken to burrowing. Similarly the power 
of flight must have helped insects, some ancient saurians, 
and birds out of many a scrape, though it cannot be 
doubted that this transition, and also that from diurnal to 
nocturnal habits, often brought only a temporary relief. 

3. Parasitism.—From the simple Protozoa up to the 
beginning of the backboned series, we find illustrations of 
animals which have taken to a thievish existence as unbidden 
guests in or on other organisms. Flukes, tapeworms, and 
some other ‘“ worms,” many crustaceans, insects, and mites, 
are the most notable. Few animals are free from some kind 
of parasite. There are various grades of parasitism ; there 
are temporary and permanent, external and internal, very 
degenerate, and very slightly affected parasites. Some- 
times the adults are parasitic while the young are free-liv- 
ing, sometimes the reverse is true ; sometimes the parasite 
completes its life in one host, often it reaches maturity only 
after the host in which its youth has been passed is de- 
voured by another. In many cases the habit was probably 
begun by the females, which seek shelter during the period 
of egg-laying; in not a few crustaceans and insects the 
females alone are parasitic. Most often, in all probability, 
hunger and the search for shelter led to the establishment 
of the thievish habit. Now, the advantages gained by a 
thoroughgoing parasite are great—safety, warmth, abund- 
ant food, in short, “complete material well-being.” But 


48 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


there is another aspect of the case. Parasitism tends to be 
followed by degeneration — of appendages, food - canal, 
sense-organs, nervous system, and other structures, the 
possession and use of which make life worth living. More- 
over, though the reproductive system never degenerates, 
the odds are often many against an embryo reaching a fit 
host or attaining maturity. Thus Leuckart calculates that 
a tapeworm embryo has only about 1 chance in 83,000,000 
of becoming a tapeworm, and one cannot be sorry that 
its chance is not greater. In illustration of the degenera- 
tion which is often associated with parasitism, and varies 
as the habit is more or less predominant, take the case of 
Sacculina—a crustacean usually ranked along with bar- 
nacles and acorn-shells. It begins its life as a minute free 
“nauplius,” with three pairs of appendages, a short food- 
canal, an eye, a small brain, and some other structures 
characteristic of many young crustaceans. In spite of this 
promiseful beginning, the young Saccu/ina becomes a para- 
site, first within the body, and finally under the tail, of a 
crab. Attached by absorptive suckers to its host, and 
often doing no slight damage, it degenerates into an oval 
sac, almost without trace of its former structure, with 
reproductive system alone well developed. Yet the 
degeneration is seldom so great as this, and it is fair to 
state that many parasites, especially those which remain as 
external hangers-on, seem to be but slightly affected by their 
lazy thievish habit ; nor can it be denied that most are well 
adapted to the conditions of their life. But on the whole 
the parasitic life tends to degeneration, and is unprogress- 
ive. Meredith writes of Nature’s sifting— 


‘* Behold the life of ease, it drifts. 
The sharpened life commands its course : 
She winnows, winnows roughly, sifts, 
To dip her chosen in her source. 
Contention is the vital force 
Whence pluck they brain, her prize of gifts.” 


4. General Resemblance to Surroundings. — Many 
transparent and translucent blue animals are hardly 


CHAP. IV Shifts for a Living 49 


visible in the sea; white animals, such as the polar bear, 
the arctic fox, and the ptarmigan in its winter plumage, 
are inconspicuous upon the snow; green animals, such 
as insects, tree-frogs, lizards, and snakes, hide among the 
leaves and herbage ; tawny animals harmonise with sandy 
soil; and the hare escapes detection among the clods. So 
do spotted animals such as snakes and leopards live unseen 
in the interrupted light of the forest, and the striped tiger 
is lost in the jungle. Even the eggs of birds are often well 
suited to the surroundings in which they are laid. There 
can be no doubt that this resemblance between the colour 
of an animal and that of its surroundings is sometimes of 
protective and also aggressive value in the struggle for 
existence, and where this is the case, natural selection 
would foster it, favouring with success those variations 
which were best ae and eliminating those which were 
conspicuous. 

But there are many instances of resemblance to sur- 
roundings which are hard to explain. Thus Dr. A. Seitz 
describes a restricted area of woodland in South Brazil, where 
the great majority of the insects were blue, although but 
a few miles off a red colour was dominant. He maintains 
that the facts cannot in this case be explained as due either 
to general protective resemblance or to mimicry. 

I have reduced what I had written in illustration of 
advantageous colouring of various kinds, because this 
exceedingly interesting subject has been treated in a readily 
available volume by one who has devoted much time and 
skill to its elucidation. Mr. E. B. Poulton’s Colours of 
Animals (International Science Series, London, 1890) is a 
fascinating volume, for which all interested in these aspects 
of natural history must be grateful. With this a forth- 
coming work (Azimal Coloration, London, 1892) by Mr. 
F, E. Beddard should be compared. 

5. Variable Colouring—Some animals, such as the 
ptarmigan and the mountain-hare, become white in winter, 
and are thereby safer and warmer. In some cases it 
is certain that the pigmented feathers and hairs become 
white, in other cases the old feathers and hairs drop 

E 


50 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


off and are replaced by white ones; sometimes the 
whiteness is the result of both these processes. It is 
directly due to the formation of gas bubbles inside 
the hairs or feathers in sufficient quantity to antagonise 
the effect of any pigment that may be present, but in 
the case of new growths it is not likely that any pig- 
ment is formed. In some cases, ¢.g. Ross’s lemming and 
the American hare (Lepus americanus), it has been clearly 
shown that the change is due to the cold. . It is likely that 
this acts somewhat indirectly upon the skin through the 
nervous system. We may therefore regard the change as 
a variation due to the environment, and it is at least 
possible that the permanent whiteness of some northern 
animals, e.g. the polar bear, is an acquired character of 
similar origin. There are many objections to the theory 
that the winter whiteness of arctic animals arose by the 
accumulation of small variations in individuals which, being 
slightly whiter than their neighbours, became dominant by 
natural selection, though there can be no doubt that the 
whiteness, however it arose, would be conserved like other 
advantageous variations. 

To several naturalists, but above all to Mr. Poulton, we 
are indebted for much precise information in regard to the 
variable colouring of many caterpillars and chrysalides. 
These adjust their colours to those of the surroundings, and 
even the cocoons are sometimes harmoniously coloured. 
There is no doubt that the variable colouring often has 
protective value. Mr. Poulton experimented with the 
caterpillars of the peacock butterfly (Vanessa zo), small 
tortoise-shell (Vanessa urtice), garden whites (Pieris 
brassice and Pierts rape), and many others. Caterpillars 
of the small tortoise-shell in black surroundings tend to be- 
come darker as pupz; in a white environment the pupe 
are lighter; in gilded boxes they tend to become golden. 
The surrounding colour seems to influence the caterpillar 
“during the twenty hours immediately preceding the last 
twelve hours of the larval state,” “and this is probably the 
true meaning of the hours during which the caterpillar 
rests motionless on the surface upon which it will pupate.” 


CHAP. 1V Shifts for a Living 51 


“Tt appears to be certain that it is the skin of the larva 
which is influenced by surrounding colours during the 
sensitive period, and it is probable that the effects are 
wrought through the medium of the nervous system.” 
Accepting the facts that caterpillars are subtly affected 
by surrounding colours, so that the quiescent pupz har- 
monise with their environment, and that the adjustment has 
often protective value, we are led to inquire into the origin 
of this sensitiveness, That the change of colour is 
not a direct result of external influence is certain, but 
of the physiological nature of the changes we know little 
more than that it must be complex. It may be main- 
tained, that “the existing colours and markings are at any 
rate in part due to the accumulation through heredity 
of the indirect influence of the environment, working 
by means of the nervous system;” “to which it may 
be replied,” Poulton continues, ‘that the whole use and 
meaning of the power of adjustment depends upon its 
freedom during the life of the individual ; any hereditary 
bias towards the colours of ancestors would at once destroy 
the utility of the power, which is essentially an adaptation 
to the fact that different individuals will probably meet with 
different environments. As long ago as 1873 Professor 
Meldola argued that this power of adjustment is adaptive, 
and to be explained by the operation of natural selection.” 
Poulton’s opinion seems to be, that the power of producing 
variable colouring arose as a constitutional variation apart 
from the influence of the environment, that the power was 
fostered in the course of natural selection, and that its 
limits were in the same way more or less defined in adapta- 
tion to the most frequent habitat of the larve before 
and during pupation. The other theory is that the power 
arose as the result of environmental influence, was accumu- 
lated by inheritance throughout generations, and was fostered 
like other profitable variations by natural selection. The 
question is whether the power arose in direct relation 
to environmental influence or not, whether external influence 
was or was not a primary factor in evolving the power of 
adapting colour, and in defining it within certain limits. 


52 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


6. Rapid Change of Colour.—For ages the chamzleon 
has been famous for its rapid and sometimes striking 
changes of colour. The members of the Old World 
genus Chameleo quickly change from green to brown 
or other tints, but rather in response to physical irrita- 
tion and varying moods than in relation to change of 
situation and surrounding colours. So the American 
“chameleons” (Azolis).change, for instance, from emerald 
to bronze under the influence of excitement and various 
kinds of light. Their sensitiveness is exquisite; ‘a pass- 
ing cloud may cause the bright emerald to fade.” Some- 
times they may be thus protected, for “‘ when on the broad 
green leaves of the palmetto, they are with difficulty per- 
ceived, so exactly is the colour of the leaf counterfeited. 
But their dark shadow is very distinct from beneath.” Most 
of the lizards have more or less of this colour-changing 
power, which depends on the contraction and expansion 
.of the pigmented living matter of cells which lie in layers 
in the under-skin, and are controlled by nerves. 

In a widely different set of animals—the cuttle-fishes— 
the power of rapid colour-change is well illustrated. When 
a cuttle-fish in a tank is provoked, or when one almost 
stranded on the beach struggles to free itself, or, most 
beautifully, when a number swim together in strange unison, 
flushes of colour spread over the body. The sight suggests 
the blushing of higher animals, in which nervous excitement 
passing from the centre along the peripheral nerves influ- 
ences the blood-supply in the skin ; but in colour-change the 
nervous thrills affect the pigment-containing cells or chroma- 
tophores, the living matter of which contracts or expands 
in response to stimulus. It must be allowed that the colour- 
change of cuttle-fish is oftenest an expression of nervous 
excitement, but in some cases it helps to conceal the 
animals. 

More interesting to us at present are those cases of 
colour-change in which animals respond to the hues of 
their surroundings. This has been observed in some 
Amphibians, such as tree-frogs ; in many fishes, such as 
plaice, stickleback, minnow, trout, Godcus ruthensparri, 


CHAP. IV Shifts for a Living 53 


Serranus ; and in not a few crustaceans. The researches 
of Briicke, Lister, and Pouchet have thrown much light on 
the subject. Thus we know that the colour of surround- 
ings affects the animals through the eyes, for blind plaice, 
trout, and frogs do not change their tint. The nervous 
thrill passes from eye to brain, and thence extends, not down 
the main path of impulse—the spinal cord—but down the 
sympathetic chain. If this be cut, the colour-change does 
not take place. The sympathetic system is connected with 
nerves passing from the spinal cord to the skin, and it is 
along these that the impulse is further transmitted. The 
result is the contraction or expansion of the pigment in the 
skin-cells. Though the path by which the nervous influence 
passes from the eye to the skin is somewhat circuitous, the 
change is often very rapid. As the resulting resemblance 
to surroundings is often precise, there can be no doubt that 
the peculiarity sometimes profits its possessors, 

7. Special Protective Resemblance. — The likeness 
between animals and their surroundings is often very precise, 
and includes form as wellas colour. Thus some bright butter- 
flies, e.g. Kallima, are conspicuous in flight, but become 
precisely like brown withered leaves when they settle upon 
a branch and expose the under sides of their raised wings ; 
the leaf-insects (Phyliium) have leaf-like wings and legs ; 
the “ walking-sticks ” (Phasmide), with legs thrown out at 
all angles, resemble irregular twigs; many caterpillars (of 
Geometra moths especially) sit motionless on a branch, 
supported in a strained attitude by a thin thread of silk, and 
exactly resemble twigs; others are like bark, moss, or 
lichen. Among caterpillars protective resemblance is very 
common, and Mr. Poulton associates its frequent occurrence 
with the peculiarly defenceless condition of these young 
animals, ‘‘The body is a tube which contains fluid under 
pressure ; a slight wound entails great loss of blood, while 
a moderate injury must prove fatal.” ‘“ Hence larve are so 
coloured as to avoid detection or to warn of some unplea- 
sant attribute, the object in both cases being the same—to 
leave the larva untouched, a touch being practically fatal.” 
Among backboned animals we do not expect to find many 


54 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


examples of precise resemblance to surrounding objects ; 
but one of the sea-horses (Phyllopteryx egues) is said to be 
exceedingly like the seaweed among which it lives. It is 
very difficult at present to venture suggestions as to the 
constitutional tendencies which may have resulted in 
“ walking-leaves” and “walking-sticks,” but forms related 
to these tend to resemble leaves or sticks sufficiently to deter 


nil il ! 
He aN 


i 


Fic. 7.—Leaf-insect seated on a branch. (From Belt.) 


one from postulating a mere sport as the origin of the 
peculiarity which distinguishes Phy//um or Phasma. On 
the other hand, some of the strangely precise minute 
resemblances may be the fostered results of slight indefinite 
sports. It is also possible that some of the cleverer 
animals, such as spiders, learn to hide among the lichens 
and on the bark which they most resemble. But in every 
case, and especially where there are many risks, as among 


CHAP, IV Shifts for a Living 55 


caterpillars, the protective resemblance would be fostered 
in the course of natural selection. 


Fic. 8.—Moss insect. (From Belt.) 


8. Warning Colours.—While many animals are con- 
cealed by their colouring, others are made the more 
conspicuous. But, as the latter are often unpalatable or 
dangerous, Wallace suggested that the colours were 
warnings, which, as Poulton says, ‘assist the education 
of enemies, enabling them to easily learn and remember 
the animals which are to be avoided.” Expressing 
the same idea, Belt says, “the skunk goes leisurely along, 
holding up his white tail as a danger-flag for none to come 
within range of his nauseous artillery.” So, the brightness 
of the venomous coral-snake (£/ags) is a warning; the 
rattlesnake, excitedly shaking its rattle, ‘warns an intruder 
of its presence”; the cobra ‘“ endeavours to terrify its enemy 
by the startling appearance of its expanded hood and con- 
spicuous eye-like marks.” The language in which conspicu- 
ous colours are described by many naturalists tends to 
exaggerate the subtlety of animals, for the intentional 
warning of possible molesters involves rather complex ideas. 
Belt’s description of the skunk, for instance, recalls a more 
familiar sight—a cat showing fight to a dog—in regard to 
which Mantegazza gravely tells us that the cat “ bristles up 
her fur, and inflates herself to appear larger, and to frighten 
the dog who threatens her”! In our desire to be fair to the 
subtlety of animals, it is indeed difficult to avoid being 
credulous. 


56 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


Perhaps the best illustration which Belt gives is that of 
acertain gaily-coloured frog :— 

‘In the woods around Santo Domingo there are many frogs. 
Some are green or brown, and imitate green or dead leaves, and 
live amongst foliage. Others are dull earth-coloured, and hide in 
holes and under logs. All these come out only at night to feed, and 
they are all preyed upon by snakes and birds. In contrast to these 
obscurely-coloured species, another little frog hops about in the 
daytime dressed in a bright livery of red and blue. He cannot be 
mistaken for any other, and his flaming vest and blue stockings 
show that he does not court concealment. He is very abundant 
in the damp wood, and I was convinced that he was uneatable 
so soon as I had made his acquaintance, and saw the happy sense 
of security with which he hopped about. I took a few specimens 
home with me, and tried my fowls and ducks with them, but none 
of them would touch them. At last, by throwing down pieces of 
meat, for which there was a great competition amongst them, I 
managed to entice a young duck into snatching up one of the 
little frogs. Instead of swallowing it, however, it instantly threw 
it out of its mouth, and went about jerking its head, as if trying to 
throw off some unpleasant taste.’ 

Admirable, also, are the illustrations given by Mr. Poulton 
in regard to many caterpillars, such as the larva of the 
currant or magpie moth (Adraxas grossulariata), which is 
conspicuous with orange and black markings on a cream 
ground, and is refused altogether, or rejected with disgust, 
by the hungry enemies of other caterpillars. Professor’ 
Herdman and Mr. Garstang have also shown that the 
Eolid Nudibranchs (naked sea-slugs), with brightly-coloured 
and stinging dorsal papilla, are rarely eaten by fishes ; and 
the same is true of some other conspicuous and unpalat- 
able marine animals. 

The general conclusion seems fairly certain that the 
conspicuousness of many unpalatable or noxious animals is 
imprinted on the memory of their enemies, who, after pay- 
ing some premiums to experience, learn to leave animals 
with “warning colours” alone. It will be interesting 
to discover how far the bright colour, the nauseous taste, 
the poisonous properties, the distasteful odour, sometimes 
found associated, are physiologically related to one another, 
but to answer these questions we are still unprepared. 


CHAP. IV Shifts for a Living 57 


9. Mimicry.—Mr. Poulton has carefully traced the transi- 
tion from warning to mimetic appearance, and it is evident 
that if hungry animals have been so much impressed with 
the frequent association of unpalatableness and conspicuous 
colours that they do not molest certain bright and nauseous 
forms, then there is a chance that palatable forms may 
also escape if they are sufficiently like those which are 
passed by. The term mimicry is restricted to those cases 


Fic. 9.—Hornet (Priocnenzis) above, and mimetic bug (SAiniger) beneath. 
(From Belt.) 


“in which a group of animals in the same habitat, character- 
ised by a certain type of colour and pattern, are in part 
specially protected to an eminent degree (the mimicked), and 
in part entirely without special protection (the mimickers) ; so 
that the latter live entirely upon the reputation of the former.” 
The fact was “discovered by Bates in Tropical America 
(1862), then by Wallace in Tropical Asia and Malaya 
(1866), and by Trimen in South Africa (1870)”; while Kirby, 
in 1815, referred to the advantage of a certain fly being 
like a bee, and of a certain spider resembling an ant. 


58 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


The constant conditions of mimicry are clearly and tersely 
summed up by Wallace. They are :— 

1. That the imitative species occur in the same area, 
and occupy the very same station, as the imitated. 

2. That the imitators are always the more defenceless. 

3. That the imitators are always less numerous in 
individuals. 

4. That the imitators differ from the bulk of their 
allies. 


Fic. 1o.—Humming-bird moth (A/acroglossa titan), and humming-bird 
(Lofphornis Gouldiz). (irom Bates.) 


5. That the imitation, however minute, is ea¢erval and 
visible only, never extending to internal characters or to 
such as do not affect the external appearance. 

Many inedible butterflies are mimicked by others quite 
different. Many longicorn beetles exactly mimic wasps, 
bees, or ants. The tiger-beetles are mimicked by more 
harmless insects ; the common drone-fly (£7¢s/a/7s) is like 
a bee ; spiders are sometimes ant-like. Mr. Bates relates 
that he repeatedly shot humming-bird moths in mistake for 
humming-birds. Among Vertebrates genuine mimicry is 
rare, but it is well known that some harmless snakes mimic 


CHAP. IV Shifts for a Living 59 


poisonous species. Thus, the very poisonous coral-snakes 
(Zlaps), which have very characteristic markings, are 
mimicked in different localities by several harmless forms. 
Similarly in regard to birds, Mr. Wallace notices that the 
powerful “friar-birds” (Zyofidorhynchus) of Malaya are 
mimicked by the weak and timid orioles. “In each of the 
great islands of the Austro-Malayan region there is a dis- 
tinct species of Zvopidorhynchus, and there is always along 
with it an oriole that exactly mimics it.” 

That there may be mimetic resemblance between distinct 
forms there can be no doubt, and the value of the resem- 
blance has been verified ; but there is sometimes a tendency 
to weaken the case by citing instances or using terms which 
have been insufficiently criticised. Thus the facts hardly 
justify us in saying that the larve of the Elephant Hawk 
Moth (Cherocampa) “terrify their enemies by the sugges- 
tion of a cobra-like serpent ;” or that the cobra, which “ in- 
spires alarm by the large eye-like ‘spectacles’ upon the 
dilated hood, offers an appropriate model for the swollen 
anterior end of the caterpillar, with its terrifying markings.” 

There is only one theory of mimicry, namely, that among 
the mimicking animals varieties occurred which prospered 
by being somewhat like the mimicked, and that in the 
course of natural selection this resemblance was gradually 
increased until it became dominant and, in many cases, 
remarkably exact. 

As to the primary factors giving rise to the variation, we 
can only speculate. To begin with, indeed, there must 
have been a general resemblance between the ancestors of 
the mimicking animal and those of the mimicked, for cases 
like the Humming-Bird and its Doppel-Ganger moth are very 
rare. But this does not take us very far. The beginning 
of the mimetic change is usually referred to one of those 
“indefinite,” “fortuitous,” “spontaneous ” variations which 
are believed to be common among animals. It is logically 
possible that this may have been the case, and that there 
was at the very beginning no relation between the variation 
of the mimicker and the existence of the mimicked. But 
as illustrations of mimicry accumulate—and they are already 


PART I 


The Study of Animal Life 


60 


(auia3g snieg s19yje y1ed uy) 


yuo 


‘(saz143gnG) aBuods v Aq paiaaoo qvio v (Sempuy 101jP) 


Ss 
pa} 


SOUOWIAUL-VaS YITM qvio WUMY YW *-SUIYSeY JO suorjessn{[[—"1L “O17 


UIMOIS 3} 


Aydooz pue paaarvas yA quid Jayjour 


CHAP. IV Shifts for a Living 61 


very numerous—one is tempted to ask whether there may 
not be in many cases some explanation apart from the action 
of natural selection upon casual changes. May not the 
similar surroundings and habits of mimickers and mimicked 
have sometimes something to do with their resemblance ; 
may it not be that the presence of the mimicked has had a 
direct, but of course very subtle, influence on the mimickers ; 
is it altogether absurd to suppose that there may be an 
element of consciousness in the resemblance between oriole 
and friar-bird ? 

10. “Masking” is one of the most interesting ways 
in which animals strengthen their hold on life. It is best 
illustrated on the sea-shore, where there is no little struggle 
for existence and much opportunity for device. There many 
animals, such as crabs, are covered by adventitious dis- 
guises, so that their real nature is masked. Elsewhere, 
however, the same may be seen; the cases of the caddis- 
worms—made of sand particles, small stones, minute shells, 
or pieces of bark—serve at once for protection and conceal- 
ment ; the cocoons of various caterpillars are often masked 
by extrinsic fragments. The nests of birds are often well 
disguised with moss and lichen. 

But among marine animals masking is more frequent. 
“Certain sea-urchins,” Mr Poulton says, “‘ cover themselves 
so completely with pebbles, bits of rock and shell, that one 
can see nothing but a little heap of stones ; and many marine 
molluscs have the same habits, accumulating sand upon the 
surface of the shell, or allowing a dense growth of Algz to 
cover them.” 

This masking is in many cases quite involuntary. Thus 
the freshwater snails (Lymnaus) may be so thickly covered 
with Algze that they can hardly move, and some marine 
forms are unable to favour or prevent the growth of other 
organisms upon their shells. But how far this is from be- 
ing the whole story is well known to all who are acquainted 
with our shore crabs. For though they also may be invol- 
untarily masked, there is ample evidence that they some- 
times disguise themselves. 

The hermit-crabs are to some extent masked within 


62 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


their stolen shells, especially if these be covered by the 
Hydroid Hydractinia or other organisms. Various other 
crabs (Stenorhynchus, Inachus, Maia, Dromia, Pisa) are 
masked by the seaweeds, sponges, and zoophytes which 
cover their carapace. Moreover, the interest of this mask- 
ing is increased by the fact observed by Mr. Bateson at 
Plymouth that the crabs sometimes fix the seaweeds for 


Fic. 12.—Sack-bearing caterpillar (Saccophora). (From Bates.) 


themselves. Mr. Bateson describes how the crab seizes a 
piece of weed, tears off a piece, chews the end in his mouth, 
and then rubs it firmly on his head and legs until it is 
caught by the curved hairs and fixed. ‘The whole pro- 
ceeding is most human and purposeful. Many substances, 
as hydroids, sponges, Polyzoa, and weeds of many kinds 
and colours, are thus used; but these various substances 
are nearly always symmetrically placed on corresponding 


CHAP. IV Shifts for a Living 63 


parts of the body, and particularly long plume-like pieces 
are fixed on the head.” Thus, as Carus Sterne says, is the 
story of “ Birnam’s walking wood” re-enacted on the sea- 
shore. Furthermore, a Stenorhynchus which has been 
cleaned will immediately begin to clothe itself again, with 
the same care and precision as before. Mr. Robertson of 
Millport often saw Stenorhynchus longirostris—a common 
crab—picking about its limbs and conveying the produce 
to its mouth. “If other observations confirm the view that 
this animal is a true vegetarian, we shall have one example 
at least of an independent agriculturist, who is not only 
superior of his lands, but carries them with him when 
he removes.” I also have seen the crab doing what ‘“ the 
naturalist of Cumbrae” observed. In further illustration 
of masking we may cite Dromia vulgaris, often covered 
with sponge; Dromza excavata, with compound ascidians; 
the Amphipod Azylus, with seaweed; while a species of 
Dorifpe is said to bear a bivalve shell, or even a leaf, as a 
shield, and another crab cuts off the tunic of a sea-squirt 
and hitches it on his own shoulders, 

Sometimes this masking serves as a warning or deterrent ; 
witness that hermit-crab (Pagurus cuanensis) whose stolen 
shell is surrounded by a bright orange sponge (Suderites 
domuncula). As this sponge is full of flinty needles, has a 
strong odour and a disagreeable taste, we do not wonder 
that Mr. Garstang finds that fish dislike it intensely, nor 
can we doubt that the hermit-crab trades on the reputation of 
its associate. In other cases the masking will aid in con- 
cealment and favour attack. To the associations of crabs 
and sea-anemones we shall afterwards refer. 

11. Combination of Advantageous Qualities. — Mr. 
Poulton describes, in illustration of the combination of 
many methods of defence, the case of the larva of the 
puss moth (Cerura vinula). It resembles the leaves of the 
poplar and willow on which it lives, When disturbed it 
assumes a terrifying attitude, mimetic of a Vertebrate 
appearance! The effect is heightened by the protrusion of 
two pink whips from the terminal prongs of the body, and 
finally the creature defends itself by squirting formic acid. 


64 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


Yet in spite of all this power of defence, the larva often falls 
a victim to ichneumon-flies. These manage to lay their eggs 
within the caterpillar, which 
by and by succumbs to the 
voracity of the hatched 
ichneumon maggots. Mr. 
Poulton believes that the 
puss moth larva “has been 
saved from extermination 
by the repeated acquisition 
of new defensive measures. 
But any improvement in 
Fic. 13.—“ Terrifying attitude” of the the means of defence has 
cerns of Corura vommies (From been met by the greater 
ingenuity or boldness of 
foes; and so it has come about that many of the best- 
protected larvz are often those which die in the largest 
numbers from the attacks of enemies. The exceptional 
standard of defence has been only reached through the 
pressure of an exceptional need.” 

12. Surrender of Parts.—Among the strange life - pre- 
serving powers which animals exhibit, we must also 
include that of surrendering parts of the body in the 
panic of capture or in the struggle to escape. A rat 
will gnaw off a leg to free itself from a trap, and I have 
heard of a stoat which did not refrain from amputating 
more than one limb. But the cases to which we now refer 
are not deliberate amputations, but reflex and unconscious 
surrenders. Many lizards (such as our British “ slowworm ”) 
will readily leave their tails in their captor’s grasp ; crus- 
taceans, insects, and spiders part with their limbs and 
scramble off maimed but safe; starfishes, brittle-stars, and 
feather-stars resign their arms, and the sea-cucumbers their 
viscera. A large number of cases have been studied by 
Frédéricq and Giard. 

Among Crustacea the habit is most perfectly developed 
in the crabs, e.g. the common shore-crab (Cavcinus meanas), 
and in the spiny lobster (Padzurus), but it is also exhibited 
by the crayfish (As¢acus), the common lobster (//omarus), 


CHAP. IV Shifts for a Living 65 


the shrimp (Cyangon), and the prawn (Palemon). In crabs 
and in the spiny lobster the surrender of a limb is effected 
by the forcible contraction of the basal muscles, and the 
line of rupture is through the second-lowest joint. Frédé- 
ricq’s researches seem to prove conclusively that the sur- 
render is a reflex and unconscious act, but its protective 
value is not less great. The chances are in favour of the 
crab escaping, the residue of muscle prevents hemorrhage 
from the stump, and in the course of time the lost limb is 
replaced by a new growth. The crab does not know what 
it is doing, but it unconsciously illustrates that it is better 
that one member should perish than that the whole life 
should be lost. 

Not a few insects readily surrender their legs, but these 
are not replaced. Spiders are captured if the legs are fixed 
without irritating the nerves, for that is an essential con- 
dition of the reflex amputation. In regard to lizards, also, 
it has been shown that a reflex nervous excitement, and not 
mere brittleness, is the condition of surrender. Here, how- 
ever, the lost tail may be replaced. Among Mollusca a 
surrender of parts has been recorded of Harfa ventricosa, 
Doris cruenta, Stenopus, some species of Helix, the razor- 
shell Solex ; while it is well known that male cuttle-fishes 
sometimes part with one of their arms for special sexual 
purposes. A great many “ worms” break very easily, and 
the severed parts are sometimes able to regrow the whole 
organism. 

Among the Echinoderms the tendency to disrupt is exhi- 
bited to an extraordinary degree. Thus Professor Preyer has 
shown that the seven-rayed starfish (Asterias tenuispina) 
surrenders its arms with great readiness, often giving off 
three or four at a time. But each ray may reproduce an 
entire starfish. Professor Edward Forbes tells how a speci- 
men of Lzidia, which he had dredged, was disappearing 
over the side of the boat when he caught it by one of its 
arms; it surrendered the arm and escaped, giving ‘“‘a wink 
of derision ” with one of its eyes. Brittle-stars (Ophiuroids) 
of many kinds are true to their popular name, and the 
Crinoids are not less disruptive. Not only are the arms 

F 


66 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


readily given off, but these break into many fragments. 
There can be no doubt that this habit, combined with the 
marvellous power of regrowth which these animals possess, 
is of great protective value, while it is also probable in regard 
to both Echinoderms and some worms, that the disruption 
of parts may really increase the number of individuals. 

There is no need to enumerate all the protective habits 
and devices which animals exhibit. Some “ feign” death, 
by falling in panic into a state allied to hypnotic trance, 
perhaps in some of the higher animals by conscious decep- 
tion; others roll themselves up into balls, as in forms so 
different as myriapods and armadillos ; but, finally, I shall 
cite from Dr. Hickson’s WVaturalist in North Celebes one 
other device. “I often saw advancing slowly over the 
sea-gardens, in parties of from four to six, a group of 
cuttle-fish, swimming with an even backward movement, 
the fringes of their mantles and their arms trembling, 
and their colour gradually changing to what seemed to 
me an almost infinite variety of hues as they passed 
over the various beds of the sea-bottom. Then suddenly 
there would be a commotion in what was previously a 
calm and placid scene, the striped and speckled reef fishes 
would be seen darting away in all directions, and of the 
cuttle-fishes all that remained were four or five clouds of ink 
in the clear water. They had thrown dust in the eyes of 
some small shark or voracious fish.” 

But I should not like to suggest the idea that animals 
are always careful and anxious, or forced to continual 
struggle and shift. 


“¢ They do not sweat and whine about their condition, 

They do not lie awake in the dark and weep for their sins, 

They do not make me sick discussing their duty to God, 

Not one is dissatisfied, not one is demented 

With the mania of owning things; 

Not one kneels to another, nor to his kind that lived thousands 

of years ago ; 

Not one is respectable or unhappy over the whole earth.” 

WaLT WHITMAN. 


CHAPTER V 
SOCIAL LIFE OF ANIMALS 


1. Partnerships—2. Co-operation and Division of Labour—3. Gre- 
garious Life and Combined Action—4. Beavers—s. Bees—6, 
Ants—7. Termites—8. Evolution of Social Life—g. Advantages 
of Social Life—io. A Note’ on the Social Organism—11. 
Conclustons 


THE over-fed plant bears many leaves but its flowers 
are few; the animal which eats too much becomes fat; 
and we know that within the living body one part may 
grow out of proportion to the others. It seems as if 
organ competed with organ within the living engine, as 
if one tissue outgrew its neighbours in the living web, as 
if there were some struggle for existence between the 
individual units which form the city of cells in any of the 
higher animals. This idea of internal competition has 
been elaborated by a German biologist, Roux, in a work 
entitled The Struggle of Parts within the Organism, and 
it is full of suggestiveness. It can be verified from our 
own experience ; but yet it seems strange. For we rightly 
think of an organism as a unity in which the parts are 
bound together in mutual helpfulness, being members one 
of another. 

Now, just as a biologist would exaggerate greatly if he 
maintained that the struggle of parts was the most im- 
portant fact about an organism, so would a naturalist if he 
maintained that there was in nature struggle only and no 
helpfulness, 


68 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


Coherence and harmony and mutual helpfulness of 
parts—whether these be organs, tissues, or cells—are 
certainly facts in the life of individuals; we have now 
to see how far the same is true of the larger life in 
which the many are considered as one. 

1. Partnerships.—Animals often live together in strange 
partnerships. The “beef-eater” birds (Buphagus) perch 
on cattle and extract grubs from the skin; a kind of plover 
(Pluvianus egyptius) removes leeches and other parasites 
from the back of the crocodile, and perhaps ‘picks his teeth,” 
as Herodotus alleged ; the shark is attended by the pilot-fish 
(Naucrates ductor), who is shielded by the shark’s reputa- 
tion, and seems to remove parasites from his skin. 

Especially among marine animals, we find many almost 
constant associations, the meaning of which is often obscure. 
Two gasteropods Rhzzochilus and Magilus grow along 
with certain corals, some barnacles are common on whales, 
some sponges and polypes are always found together, with- 
out there being in any of these cases either parasitism or 
partnership. But when we find a little fish living con- 
tentedly inside a large sea-anemone, or the little pea-crab 
(Pinnotheres) within the horse-mussel, the probable explana- 
tion is that the fish and the crab are sheltered by their 
hosts and share their food. They are not known to do 
harm, while they derive much benefit. They illustrate one 
kind of ‘‘commensalism,” or of eating at the same table. 

But the association between crabs and sea-anemones 
affords a better illustration. One of the hermit-crabs of 
our coast (Pagurus prideauxit) has its borrowed shell 
always enveloped by a sea-anemone (Adamsia palliata), 
and Pagurus bernhardus may be similarly ensheathed by 
Adamsia rondeletit. Mobius describes two crabs from 
Mauritius which bear a sea-anemone on each claw, and _ in 
some other crabs a similar association occurs. It seems 
that in some cases the crab deliberately chooses its ally and 
plants it on its shell, and that it does not leave it behind at 
the period of shell-changing. Deprived of its polype com- 
panion, one was seen to be restlessly ill at ease until 
it obtained another of the same kind. The use of the sea- 


CHAP, v Social Life of Animals 69 


anemone as a mask to the crab—and also perhaps as 
aid in attack or defence—is obvious ; on the other hand, 
the sea-anemone is carried about by the crab and may 
derive food from the crumbs of its bearer’s repast. 

Commensalism must be distinguished from parasitism, 
in which the one organism feeds upon its host, though it is 
quite possible that a commensal might degenerate into a 
parasite. Quite distinct also is that intimate partnership 
known as symbiosis, illustrated by the union of algoid and 
fungoid elements to form a lichen, or by the occurrence of 
minute Algze as constant internal associates and helpful 
partners of Radiolarians and some Ccelenterates. 

2. Co-operation and Division of Labour.—The idea 
of division of labour has been for a long time familiar to 
men, but its biological importance was first satisfactorily 
recognised by Milne-Edwards in 1827. 

Among the Stinging-animals there are many animal 
colonies, aggregates of individuals, with a common life. 
These begin from a single individual and are formed by 
prolific budding, as a hive is formed by the prolific egg- 
laying of a queen-bee. The mode of reproduction is 
asexual in the one case, sexual in the other; the resulting 
individuals are physically united in the one case, psychically 
united in the other; but these differences are not so great 
as they may at first sight appear. Many masses of coral 
are animal colonies, but among the members or “ persons,” 
as they are technically called, division of labour is very rare ; 
moreover, in the growth of coral the younger individuals 
often smother the older. In colonial zoophytes the 
arborescent mode of growth usually obviates crushing ; and 
there is sometimes very marked division of labour. Thus 
in the colony of Hydractinia polypes, which is often found 
growing on the shells tenanted by hermit-crabs, there may 
be a hundred or more individuals all in organic connection. 
The polypes are minute tubular animals, connected at 
their bases, and stretching out from the surface of the shell 
into the still water of the pool in which the hermit-crab is 
resting. But among the hundred individuals there are 
three or four castes, the differences between which probably 


70 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


cesult from the fact that in such a large colony perfect 
uniformity of nutritive and other conditions is impossible, 
Individuals which are fundamentally and originally like one 
another grow to be different, and perform different func- 
tions according to the caste to which they belong. 

Many are nutritive in form like the little freshwater 
Hydra—tubular animals with an extensile body and with a 
terminal mouth wreathed round by mobile tentacles. On 
these the whole nutrition of the 
colony depends. Beside these 
there are reproductive ‘“ per- 
sons,” which cannot feed, 
being mouthless, but secure 
the continuance of the species 
and give rise to embryos which 
start new colonies. Then there 
are long, lank, sensitive mem- 
bers, also mouthless, which 
serve as thesense-organs of the 
colony, and are of use in de- 
tecting food or danger. When 
danger threatens, the polypes 
cower down, and there are left 
projecting small hard spines, 
Ws which some regard as a fourth 
Fic. 14.—Colony of Hydractinia class of individuals—starved, 

echinata. a, nutritive individuals; abortive members like the 

b, reproductive individuals;  c, 

abortive spines; and there are thorns on the hawthorn hedge. 

ako, long mouths, indtluals In recognising their utility to 

Chambers's Encyclop.; after All. the colony as a whole we can 

many hardly overlook the fact that 
their life as individuals is practically nil. They well illus- 
trate the dark side of division of labour. 


Herbert Spencer and Ernst Haeckel have explained very clearly 
one law of progress among those animals which form colonies. 
The crude form of a colony is an aggregate of similar individuals, 
the perfected colony is an z#egrate in which by division of labour 
greater harmony of life has resulted, and in which the whole colony 
is more thoroughly compacted into a unity. Among the Stinging- 


CHAP. V Social Life of Animals WI 


animals, we find some precise illustrations of such integrated colonies, 
especially in the Siphonophora of which the Portuguese Man-of-War 
(Physalia) is a good example. There is no doubt that these 
beautiful organisms are colonies of individuals, which in structure 
are all referable to a ‘‘ medusoid ” or jellyfish-like type. But the 
division of labour is so harmonious, and the compacting or 
organisation of the colony is so thorough, that the whole moves and 
lives as a single organism. 

E. Perrier in his work entitled Les Colonies Animales (Paris, 
1882), shows how organic association may lead from one grade of 
organisation and individuality to another, and explains very clearly 
how sedentary and passive life tends to develop mere aggregates, 
while free and active life tends to integrate the colony. With this 
may be compared A. Lang’s interesting study on the influence of 
sedentary life and its connection with asexual reproduction—Das 
Einfluss des Festsitzen (Jena, 1889). Haeckel, in his Generelle 
Morphologie (2 vols., Berlin, 1866), was one of the first to shed a 
strong clear light on the difficult subject of organic individuality, 
its grades and its progressive complexity. To Spencer, Principles 
of Biology (2 vols., London, 1863-67), we owe in this connection 
the elucidation of the transition from aggregates to integrates, and 
of the lines of differentiation, z.e. the progressive complication of 
structure which is associated with division of labour. 


3. Gregarious Life and Combined Action.— Most 
mammals are in some degree gregarious. The solitary 
kinds are in a distinct minority. The isolated are ex- 
posed to attack, the associated are saved by the wisdom 
of their wisest members and by that strength which union 
gives. Many hoofed animals, such as deer, antelopes, 
goats, and elephants, live in herds, which are not mere 
crowds, but organised bands, with definite conventions and 
with a power of combined resistance which often enables 
them to withstand the attacks of carnivores. Marmots and 
prairie-dogs, whose “cities” may cover vast areas, live peace- 
ful and prosperous lives. Monkeys furnish many illustra- 
tions of successful gregarious life. As individuals most of 
them are comparatively defenceless, and usually avoid com- 
ing to close quarters with their adversaries ; yet in a body 
they are formidable, and often help one another out of 
scrapes. Brehm tells how he encountered a troop of baboons 
which defied his dogs and retreated in good order up the 


72 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


heights. A young one about six months old being left 
behind called loudly for aid. “One of the largest males, 
a true hero, came down again from the mountain, slowly 
went to the young one, coaxed him, and triumphantly led 
him away—the dogs being too much astonished to make an 
attack.” 


Fic. 15.—Chimpanzee (A 2thropopithecus or Troglodytes calvus). 
(From Du Chaillu.) 


Many birds, such as rooks and swallows, nest together, 
and the sociality is often advantageous. Kropotkine cites 
from Dr. Coues an observation in regard to some little cliff- 
swallows which nested in a colony quite near the home of a 
prairie-falcon. ‘The little peaceful birds had no fear of 
their rapacious neighbour ; they did not let it even approach 
to their colony. They immediately surrounded it and 


CHAP. V Social Life of Animals 73 


chased it, so that it had to make off at once.” Of the 
‘cranes, Kropotkine notes that they are extremely “sociable 
and live in friendly relations, not only with their congeners, 
but also with most aquatic birds.” They post sentries, send 
scouts, have many friends and few enemies, and are very 
intelligent. So is it also with parrots. “The members of 
each band remain faithfully attached to each other, and they 
share in common good or bad luck.” ‘They feed together, 
fly together, rest together ; they send scouts and post sen- 
tinels ; they find protection and pleasure in combination. 
Like the cranes, they are very intelligent, and safe from 
most enemies except man. 

On the other hand, some of the most successful carni- 
vores, é¢.g. wolves, hunt in packs, and not a few birds of 
prey (some eagles, kites, vultures) unite to destroy their 
quarry. Combination for defence has its counterpart in 
combination for attack. In both cases the collective action 
is often associated with the custom of posting sentinels, who 
warn the rest, or of sending scouts to reconnoitre. Pecu- 
liarly interesting are those cases in which the relatively 
weak unite to attack the strong ; thus a few kites will rob an 
eagle, and wagtails will persecute a sparrow-hawk. Kropot- 
kine has noticed how the aquatic birds which crowd on the 
shores of lakes and seas often combine to drive off intrud- 
ing birds of prey. “In the face of an exuberant life, the 
ideally armed robber has to be satisfied with the off-fall of 
that life.” 

Among many animals there is co-operation in labour, as 
well as combination for attack or defence. Brehm relates that 
baboons and other monkeys act in thorough concert in 
plundering expeditions, sending scouts, posting sentinels, and 
even forming a long chain for the transport of the spoil. It 
is said that several Hamadryad baboons will unite to turn 
over a large stone, sharing the booty found underneath. 
When the Brazilian kite has seized a prey too large for it 
to carry, it summons its friends; and Kropotkine cites a re- 
markable case in which an eagle called others to the car- 
case. Pelicans fish together in great companies, forming a 
wide half-circle facing the shore and catching the fish thus 


74 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


enclosed. Burial beetles unite to bury the dead mouse or 
bird in which the eggs are laid, and the dung-beetles help 
one another in rolling balls of food. But of all cases of 
combined activity the migration of birds is at once the most 
familiar and the most beautiful—the gathering together, the 
excitement before starting, the trial flights, the reliance 
placed in the leaders. Migration is*usually social, and 
is sustained by tradition. 

4. Beavers.—That the highly-socialised beavers have 
been exterminated in many countries where they once 
abounded is no argument against their sociality, for man 
has ingenuity enough to baffle any organisation. A family 
of about six members inhabits one house, and in suitable 
localities—secluded and rich in trees—many families con- 
gregate in a village community. The young leave the par- 
ental roof in the summer of their third year, find mates for 
themselves, and establish new homesteads. The community 
becomes overcrowded, however, and migrations take place 
up and down stream, the old lodges being sometimes left to 
the young couples. It is said, moreover, that lazy or other- 
wise objectionable members may be expelled from the society, 
and condemned to live alone. Under constraint of fear or 
human interference, and away from social impulse, beavers 
may relapse into lazy and careless habits, and in many 
cases each family lives its life apart; but in propitious 
conditions their achievements are marvellous. The burrow 
may rise into a constructed home, and the members of 
many families may combine in wood-cutting and log-rolling, 
and yet more markedly in constructing dams and digging 
canals. Make allowances for the exaggeration of enthusiastic 
observers, but read Mr. Lewis Morgan’s stories of the evolu- 
tion of a broken burrow into a comfortable lodge, varying 
according to the local conditions ; of the adaptation of the 
dams against the rush of floods ; of canals hundreds of feet 
in length—labours without reward until they are finished ; 
of the short-cut waterways across loops of the river ; and of 
“locks” where continuous canals are, from the nature of the 
ground, impossible. The Indians have invested beavers 
with immortality, but it is enough for us to recognise that 


CHAP, V Social Life of Animals 15 


they exhibit more sagacity than can be explained by heredi- 
tary habit, for they often adapt their actions to novel condi- 
tions in a manner which must be described as intelligent. 
Especially when we remember that the beaver belongs to a 
somewhat stupid rodent race, are we inclined to believe that 
it is the cleverest of its kind because the most socialised. 

5. Bees.—Many centuries have passed since men first 
listened to the humming of the honey-bees, and found in the 
hive a symbol of the strength of unity. From Aristotle’s 
time till now naturalists have 
been studying the life of bees, 
without exhausting either its 
facts or its suggestions. The 
society is very large and 
complex, yet very stable and 
successful. Its customs seem 
now like those of children at 
play, and now like the real- 
ised dreams of social refor- 
mers. The whole life gives 
one the impression of an old- 
established business in which 
all contingencies have been 
so often experienced that 
they have ceased to cause 
hesitation or friction. There 
is indeed much mortality, . 16.—Honey-bee (Apis mellifica). 


some apparent cruelty, and Dc oe ie ne) aa 


the constantly recurring ad- 

venture of migration; but though hive may war against 
hive, inter-civic competition has virtually ceased, and the 
life proceeds smoothly with the harmony and effectiveness 
of a perfected organisation. 

The mother-bee, whom we call a “ queen ”—though she 
is without the wits and energy of a ruler—is to this extent 
head of the community, that, by her prolific egg-laying, she 
increases or restores the population. Very sluggish in 
their ordinary life are the numerous males or “drones,” 
one of whom, fleet and vigorous beyond his fellows, will pair 


76 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


with a queen in her nuptial flight, himself to die soon after, 
saved at least from the expulsion and massacre which await 
all the sex when the supplies of honey run short in autumn. 
The queen and drones are important only so far as multiplica- 
tion is concerned. The sustained life of the hive is wholly in 
the hands of the workers, who in brains, in activity, and 
general equipment are greatly superior to their “queen.” 
“ The queen has lost her domestic arts, which the worker pos- 
sesses in a perfection never attained by the ancestral types ; 
while the worker has lost her maternal functions, although 
she still possesses the needed organs in a rudimentary state.” 

What a busy life is theirs, gathering nectar and pollen 
unwearyingly, while the sunshine lasts, neatly slipping into 
the secrets of the flowers or stealing their treasures by 
force, carrying their booty home in swift sweeping flight, 
often over long distances unerringly, unloading the pollen 
from their hind-legs and packing it into some cells of the 
comb, emptying out the nectar from their crop or honey- 
sac into store-cells, and then off again for more—such is 
their socialised mania for getting. But, besides these 
“foragers ”—for the most part seniors—there are younger 
stay-at-home “nurses,” whose labours, if less energetic, are 
not less essential. For it is their part to look after the 
grubs in their cradles, to feed them at first with a “ pap” of 
digested nectar, and then to wean them to a diet of honey, 
pollen, and water; to attend the queen, guiding her move- 
ments and feeding her while she lays many eggs, sometimes 
2000 to 3000 eggs ina day. Mr. Cheshire, in his incom- 
parably careful book on Bees and Beekeeping, laughs at the 
“many writers who have given the echo to a medieval 
fancy by stating that the queen is ever surrounded by a 
circle of dutiful subjects, reverently watching her move- 
ments, and liable to instant banishment upon any neglect 
of duty. These it was once the fashion to compare to the 
twelve Apostles, and, to make the ridiculous suggestion 
complete, their number was said to be invariably twelve!” 
But Mr. Cheshire’s own account of the nurses’ work, and of 
the whole life of the hive, is more marvellous than any 
medizeval fancy. 


CHAP. V Social Life of Animals "9 


We have not outlined nearly all the labours of the 
workers. There is the exhausting though passive labour of 
forming the wax which oozes out on the under-surface of the 
body, and then there is the marvellous comb-building, at 
which the bees are very neat and clever workers, though they 
do not deserve the reputation for mathematical insight once 
granted them. ‘Their combs,” Mr. Cheshire says, “are 
rows of rooms unsurpassably suitable for feeding and nurtur- 
ing the larve, for giving safety and seclusion during the 
mystic sleep of pupa-hood, for ensconcing the weary worker 
seeking rest, and for safely warehousing the provisions ever 
needed by the numerous family and by all during the 
winter’s siege. Corridors run between, giving sufficient 
space for the more extensive quarters of the prospective 
mother, and affording every facility to the busy throng 
walking on the ladders the edges of their apartments supply ; 
while the exactions of modern hygiene are fully met by air, 
in its native purity, sweeping past the doorway of every 
inhabitant of the insect city.” 

We shall not seek to penetrate into the more hidden 
mysteries of the life of bees ; for instance, “ how the drones 
have a mother but no father,” or how high feeding makes 
the difference between a queen and a worker. An outline 
of the yearly life is more appropriate. From the winter’s 
rest the surviving bees reawaken when the early-flowering 
trees begin to blossom ; the workers engage in a “spring 
cleaning,” and the queen restores the reduced population 
by egg-laying. New supplies of food are brought in, new 
bees are born, and in early summer we see the busy life in 
all its energy. The pressure of increased population makes 
itself felt, and migration or “swarming” becomes impera- 
tive. In due time and in fair weather “the old mother 
departs with the superabundance of the population.” 
Meanwhile in the parent-hive drones have been born, and 
several possible queens await liberation. The first to be 
set free has to hold her own against newcomers, or it may be 
to die before one of them. The successful new queen soon 
becomes restless, issues forth in swift nuptial flight, is 
fertilised by a drone, and returns to her home to begin 


78 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


prolific egg-laying, and perhaps after a time to lead off 
another swarm. During the busy summer, when food is 
abundant, the lazy males are tolerated; but when their 
function is fulfilled, and when the supplies become scarce, 
they are ruthlessly put to death. “No sooner does income 
fall below expenditure, than their nursing sisters turn 
their executioners, usually by dragging them from the hive, 
biting at the insertion of the wing. The drones, strong for 
their especial work, are, after all, as tender as they are 
defenceless, and but little exposure and abstinence is 
required to terminate their being. So thorough is the war 
of extermination, that no age is spared; even drone eggs 
are devoured, the larvee have their juices sucked and their 
‘remains’ carried out—a fate in which the chrysalids are 
made to take part, the maxim for the moment being, He 
that will not work, neither shall he eat.” This Lycurgan 
tragedy over, the equilibrium of the hive is more secure, 
and the winter comes. 

The social life of hive-bees is of peculiar interest, 
because it represents the climax of a series of stages. 
Hermann Miiller has traced the plausible history of the 
honey-bee from an insect like the sand-wasp, and has 
shown in other kinds of bees the various steps by which 
the pollen-gathering and nectar-collecting organs have been 
developed. The habits of life gradually lead up to the 
consummately social life of the hive. Thus Prosopis, which 
lays its eggs in the pith of bramble-stems ; the wood-boring 
Xylophaga ; and the leaf-cutting Megachile, which lines its 
burrows with circles cut from rose leaves, are solitary bees. 
The various species of humble- or bumble-bee (Bombus), so 
familiarly industrious from the ‘spring, when the willows 
bear their catkins, till the autumn chill benumbs, are half- 
way to the hive-bees ; for they live in societies of mother, 
drones, and workers during summer, while the sole surviv- 
ing queens hibernate in solitude. From the humble-bee, 
moreover, we gain this hint, that the home is centred in 
the cradle, for it is in a nest with honey and pollen stored 
around the eggs that the hive seems to have begun. 

6. Antgs.--Even more suggestive of our own social organ- 


CHAP. V Soctal Life of Animals 79 


isation is the Liliputian world of the ants, who, like micro- 
scopic men, build barns and lay up stores, divide their labour 
and indulge in play, wage wars and make slaves. Like the 
bee-hive, the ant-nest includes three kinds of individuals— 
a queen mother or more than one, a number of short-lived 
males, and a crowd of workers. The queen is again pre- 
eminently maternal, and, if we can trust the enthusiastic 
observers, she is attended with loyal devotion, not without 
some judicious control. Farren White describes how the 
workers attend the queen in her perambulations: ‘They 
formed round her when she rested ; some showed their regard 
for her by gently walking over her, others by patiently watch- 
ing by her and cherishing her with their antenne, and 
in ‘every way endeavouring to testify to their affectionate 
attachment and generous submission.” Gould ventures 
further, alleging that ‘in whatever apartment a queen 
condescends to be present, she commands obedience and 
respect, and a universal gladness spreads itself through the 
whole cell, which is expressed by particular acts of joy and 
exultation. They have a peculiar way of skipping, leaping, 
and standing up on their hind legs, and prancing with the 
others. These frolics they make use of both to congratu- 
late each other when they meet, and to show their regard 
for the queen.” These are wonderful lists of assumed 
emotions! Should an indispensable queen be desirous to 
quit the nest, the workers do not hesitate, it is said, to 
keep her by force, and to tear off her wings to secure her 
stay. It is certain at least that as the queens settle down 
to the labour of maternity, their wings are lost—perhaps in 
obedience to some physiological necessity. From the much 
greater number of the wingless workers, we are apt to forget 
that the males and mothers of the social ants are winged 
insects ; but this fact becomes impressive if in fine summer 
weather we are fortunate enough to see the males and 
young queens leaving the nest in the nuptial flight, during 
which fertilisation takes place. Rising in the air they 
glitter like sparks, pale into curling smoke, and vanish. 
“Sometimes the swarms of a whole district have been 
noticed to unite their countless myriads, and, seen at a dis- 


80 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


tance, produce an effect resembling the flashing of the 
Aurora Borealis ; sometimes the effect is that of rainbow 
hues in the spray of laughing waterfalls ; sometimes that of 
fire; sometimes that of a smoke-wreath.” ‘Each column 
looks like a kind of slender network, and has a tremulous 
undulating motion. The noise emitted by myriads and 
myriads of these creatures does not exceed the hum of 
a single wasp. The slightest zephyr disperses them.” 
After this midsummer day’s delight of love, death awaits 
many, and sometimes most. The males are at best short- 
lived, but the surviving queens, settling down, may begin 


Fic. 17.—Saiiba ants at work ; to the left below, an ordinary worker ; to the 
right a large-headed worker ; above, a subterranean worker. (From Bates.) 
to form nests, gathering a troop of workers, or sometimes 

proceeding alone to found a colony. 

A caste of workers (ze. normally non-reproductive 
females) distinct from the males and queens, involves, of 
course, some division of labour; but there is more than 
this. Workers of different ages perform different tasks— 
foraging or housekeeping, fighting or nursing, as the case 
may be; and just as the various human occupations leave 
marks both for good and ill in those who follow them, so 
the division of labour among ants is associated with differ- 
ences of structure. Thus, in the Satiba or Umbrella Ant of 
Brazil (Zecodoma cephalotes), so well described by Bates in 


CHAP. V Social Life of Animals 81 


his Naturalist on the Amazons, there are three classes of 
workers. All the destructive labour of cutting sixpence-like 
disks from the leaves of trees is done by individuals with 
small heads, while others with enormously large heads 
simply walk about looking on. These “ worker-majors” 
are not soldiers, nor is there any need for supervising 
officers. ‘I think,” Bates says, “they serve, in some 
sort, as passive instruments of protection to the real 
workers, Their enormously large, hard, and indestructible 
heads may be of use in protecting them against the 
attacks of insectivorous animals. They would be, on this 
view, a kind of pieces de résistance, serving as a foil against 
onslaughts made on the main body of workers.” The 
third order of workers includes very strange fellows, with 
the same kind of head as the worker-majors have, but “the 
front is clothed with hairs instead of being polished, and 
they have in the middle of the forehead a twin simple 
eye,” which none of the others possess. Among the 
honey ants (yrmecocystus mexicanus) described by Dr. 
M‘Cook from the “Garden of the Gods” in Colorado, the 
division of labour is almost like a joke.’ The workers 
gather “honey” from certain galls, and discharge their 
spoils into the mouths of some of their stay-at-home fellows. 
These passive “honey-pots” store it up, till the abdomen 
becomes tense and round like a grape, but eventually they 
have even more tantalisingly to disgorge it for other mem- 
bers of the community. But this habit of feeding others is 
exhibited, as Forel has shown, by many species of ants. 
The hungry apply to the full for food, and get it. A 
refusal is said to be sometimes punished by death! 
Marvellous in peace, the ants may also practise the 
anti-social “art of war,” sometimes against other com- 
munities of the same species, sometimes with other kinds, 
“‘ Their battles,” Kirby says, “have long been celebrated ; 
and the date of them, as if it were an event of the first 
importance, has been formally recorded.” Eneas Sylvius, 
after giving a very circumstantial account of one contested 
with great obstinacy between a great and small species on 
the trunk of a pear tree, gravely states, “This action was 
G 


82 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


fought in the pontificate of Eugenius IV., in the presence of 
Nicholas Pistoriensis, an eminent lawyer, who related the 
whole history of the battle with the greatest fidelity.” In 
the fray the combatants are thoroughly absorbed, yet at a 
little distance other workers are uninterruptedly treading 
their daily paths ; the mélée is intense, yet every ant seems 
to know those of its own party; the result of it all is often 
nothing. We laugh at the ants—the laugh comes back on 
ourselves. 

In some cases an expedition has the definite end of slave- 
making, as is known to be true of Formica sanguinea—a 
British species, and of Polyergus rufescens, found on the Con- 
tinent. The former captures the larvee of Formica fusca, 
carries them home, and owns them henceforth as well- 
treated slaves; while the Amazon Ant (Polyergus) draws 
its supply from both /. fusca and F. cunzularta, and 
seems to have become almost dependent on its captives. 
Indeed, Hiiber says that he never knew the Amazons take 
nourishment but from the mouth of the negro captives ; 
while Lubbock notes that every transition exists between 
bold and active’ baron-like marauders and enervated masters, 
who are virtually helpless parasites upon their slaves—a 
suggestive illustration of laziness outwitting itself. 

Slaves somewhat painfully suggest domesticated animals, 
and these are also to be found among ants. For what 
Linnzus said long ago, that the ants went up trees to “ milk 
their cows, the Aphides,” is true. The ants tickle these little 
plant-lice with their antennze, and lick the juice which oozes 
from them ; nay more, according to some, they inclose and 
tend these milch kine, and even breed them at home. 
Seed-harvesting and the like may be fairly called agricul- 
tural, and do not the leaf-cutters grow mushrooms, or at 
least feed on the fungi which grow on the leaves, stored 
some say with that end in view? The driver ants, 
“whose dread is upon every living thing,” when they 
are on the stampede, remind us of the ancient troops 
of nomad hunters, though some of them are blind. Thus 
there are hunting, agricultural, and pastoral ants—three 
types, as Lubbock remarks, offering a strange analogy 


CHAP. V Social Life of Animals 83 


to the three great phases in the history of human develop- 
ment. 

Very quaint is another habit of this “little people, so 
exceeding wise,”—that of keeping or tolerating guests in the 
home! These are mostly little beetles, and have been 
carefully studied by Dr. Weismann, who distinguishes true 
guests (Atemeles, Lomechusa, Claviger) which are cared for 
and fed by the ants, from others (Dinarda, Heterius, 
Formicoxenus) which are tolerated, though not treated with 
special friendliness, and which feed on dead ants or vege- 
table débris ; while a third set are tolerated—like mice in 
our houses—only because they cannot be readily turned out. 
Of the genuine guests, the best known is Azemeles, a lively 
animal, constantly moving its feelers, and experimenting 
with everything. If one be attacked by a hostile ant, it 
first seeks to pacify its antagonist by antennary caresses, 
but if this is hopeless it emits a strong odour, which seems 
to narcotise the ant. These little familiars are really 
dependent upon their hosts, who feed them and get 
caresses in return. It is easy to understand the presence 
of pests in the ants’ home, but Azemeles and Lomechusa 
are pets, taken away by the owners when there is a flitting, 
and exhibiting, as Lubbock also observes, “international 
relations,” since they can be shifted from one nest to 
another, or even from species to species. It seems likely 
enough, as Emery suggests, that these semi-domesticated 
pets are moralised intruders, and, like our cats, they seem 
to retain some of their original traits. 

I cannot linger longer over the interesting character- 
istics of ants, though I should like to speak of their archi- 
tecture, of their roads, tunnels, bridges, and covered 
ways; of their care for the young, and sometimes even for 
the disabled ; of their proverbial industry, and yet of their 
indulgence in “ sportive exercise.” It would be profitable 
to think about the contrast between solitary ants (A/udilid@) 
who have no “workers,” and the complex life of a com- 
munity in which there are half a million residents ; or about 
their esthetic sensitiveness, for they see light and hear 
sound for which our eyes and ears are not adapted; or 


84 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


about their power of recognising their fellow-citizens (even 
when intoxicated), and of communicating definite impres- 
sions to one another by a subtle language of touch and 
gesture ; or about their instincts and intelligence, and the 
limitations of these. But it will be better to read some of 
the detailed observations, endeavouring, though necessarily 
with slight success, to think into the nature of ants,—their 
pertinacity, their indomitable “pluck,” their tireless in- 
dustry, their organic sociality. Surely all will agree with 
Sir John Lubbock, to whose patient observations we owe 
so much, that, “when we see an ant-hill, tenanted by 
thousands of industrious inhabitants, excavating chambers, 
forming tunnels, making roads, guarding their home, 
gathering food, feeding the young, tending their domestic 
animals, each one fulfilling its duties industriously and 
without confusion, it is difficult altogether to deny them the 
gift of reason,” or, perhaps more accurately, intelligence, 
for we cannot escape the conviction “that their mental 
powers differ from those of men not so much in kind as 
in degree.” 

Kropotkine says that the work of ants is performed 
“according to the principles of voluntary mutual aid.” 
“ Mutual aid within the community, self-devotion grown into 
a habit, and very often self-sacrifice for. the common wel- 
fare, are the rule.” The marvels of their history are “the 
natural outcome of the mutual aid which they practise at 
every stage of their busy and laborious lives.” To this 
mode of life is also due ‘‘ the immense development of indi- 
vidual initiative.” Ants are not well protected, but “their 
force is in mutual support and mutual confidence.” “And 
if the ant stands at the very top of the whole class of In- 
sects for its intellectual capacities; if its courage is only 
equalled by the most courageous Vertebrates, and if its 
brain—to use Darwin’s words—‘is one of the most mar- 
vellous atoms of matter in the world, perhaps more so than 
the brain of man,’ is it not due to the fact that mutual aid 
has entirely taken the place of mutual struggle in the com- 
munities of ants ?” 

7. Termites.—The true ants are so supremely interest- 


CHAP. V Soctal Life of Animals 8s 


ing, that the Termites or “ white ants ” (which are not ants at 
all) are apt to receive scant justice. Perhaps inferior in intel- 
ligence, they have the precedence of greater antiquity and 
all the interest which attaches to an old-established society. 
Nor is their importance less either to practical men or to 
speculative biologists. In 1781 Smeathman gave some 
account of their economy, noting that there were in every 
species three castes, “first, the working insects, which, for 
brevity, I shall generally call Zabourers; next, the fighting 
ones or soldiers, which do no kind of labour ; and, last of 
all, the winged ones, or Zerfect insects, which are male and 
female, and capable of propagation.” 

The “ workers,” blind and wingless, and smallest in the 
ant-hill, do all the work of foraging and mining, attending 
the royal pair and nursing the young. The soldiers, also 
blind and wingless, are much larger than the workers, but 
there are relatively only a few in each hill. ‘“ They stand,” 
Prof. Drummond says, “ or promenade about as sentries, at 
the mouths of the tunnels. When danger threatens, in the 
shape of true ants, the soldier termite advances to the fight.” 
“With a few sweeps of its scythe-like jaws it clears the 
ground, and while the attacking party is carrying off its 
dead, the builders, unconscious of the fray, quietly continue 
their work.” At home, in the ant-hill, shut up in a chamber 
whose door admits workers but is much too small for the 
tenants to pass out if they would, a fortunate investigator 
sometimes finds the royal pair. The male is sometimes 
even larger than the soldier, and is in many ways different, 
though by no means extraordinary. The queen-mother, 
however, is a very strange organism. She measures two 
to six inches, while the worker is only about a fifth of an 
inch in length. Like her mate, she sees, and she once had 
wings like his, but they have dropped off. The hind 
part of the body is enormously distended with eggs, and 
“the head bears about the same proportion to the rest of 
the body as does the tuft on his Glengarry bonnet to a six- 
foot Highlander.” In her passivity and “ phenomenal cor- 
pulence,” she is a sort of reductio ad absurdum of femaleness 
—‘“a large, cylindrical package, in shape like a sausage, 


86 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


and as white as a bolster.” But have some admiration for 
her: she sometimes lays 60 eggs per minute, or 80,000 
in a day, and continues reproducing for months. As she 
lays, she is assiduously fed by the nursing-workers, while the 
eggs are carried off to be hatched in the nurseries. At the 
breeding season, numerous winged males and females leave 


Fic. 18.—Diagrammatic section of a termite’s nest (after Houssay). In the walls 
there are winding passages (4); uppermost is a well-aired empty attic (D) 
the next story (C) is a nursery where the young termites are hatched on 
shelves (a) and (4); the next is a hall (B) supported by pillars; beneath this 
is a royal chamber (7) in which the king and queen are imprisoned ; around 
this the chambers of worker-termites (s) and some store-chambers Qn) 5 
excavated in the ground are holes (c) out of which the material used in 
making the termitary was dug. The whole structure is sometimes 10-15 
feet in height. 


the hill and its workers in swarms, most of them simply to 
die, others to mate with individuals from another hill and 
to begin to form new colonies. 

The plot of the story becomes more intricate, however, 
when we notice Fritz Miiller’s observations, that ‘“ besides 


CHAP. V Social Life of Animals 8) 


the winged males and females which are produced in vast 
numbers, and which, leaving the termitary in large swarms, 
may intercross with those produced in other communities, 
there are (in some if not all of the species) wingless 
males and females which never leave the termitary where 
they are born, and which replace the winged males or 
females whenever a community does not find, in due time, a 
true king or queen.” There is no doubt as to the existence 
of both winged and wingless royal pairs. According to 
Grassi, the former fly away in spring, the others ascend the 
throne in summer. The complementary kings or viceroys 
die before winter; their mates live on, widowed but still 
maternal, till at least the next summer. 

This replacement of royalty reminds us that hive-bees, 
bereft of their queen, will rear one from the indifferent grub, 
but the termites with which we are best acquainted seem 
almost always to have a reserve of reproductive members. 
This other difference between termites and ants or bees 
should be noticed, that in the latter the “workers” are 
highly-developed, though sterile females, while in the former 
the workers seem to be arrested forms of bothsexes. They 
are children which do not grow up. 

8. Evolution of Social Life.—To Professor Alfred 
Espinas both naturalists and sociologists are greatly in 
debted for his careful discussion of the social life of animals. 
It may be useful, therefore, to give an outline of the mode 
of treatment followed in his work—Des Sociétds Animales : 
Etude de Psychologie Comparée (Paris, 1877) :— 


Co-operation, which is an essential characteristic of all society, 
implies some degree of organic affinity. There are, indeed, 
occasional associations between unrelated forms—‘‘ mutualism,” in 
which both associates are benefited; ‘‘commensalism,” in which 
the benefit is mainly one-sided; parasitism, which is distinctly 
anti-social, deteriorating the host and also the rank of the tempor- 
arily benefited parasite. Of normal societies whose members are 
mutually dependent, two kinds may be distinguished—(a) the 
organically connected colonies of animals, in which there is a 
common nutritive life ; (8) those associations which owe their origin 
and meaning to reproduction. Of the latter. some do not become 
more than domestic, and these are distinguished as conjugal (in 


88 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


which the parents alone are concerned), maternal (in which the 
mother is the head of the family), and paternal (in which the male 
becomes prominent), But higher than the pair and the family is 
what Espinas calls the ‘‘peuplade,” what we usually call the 
society, whose bonds are, for the most part, psychical. 


But let us consider this problem of the evolution of 
sociality. The body of every animal—whether sponge or 
mammal—is a city of living units or cells. But there are 
far simpler animals than sponges. The very simplest 
animals, which we call firstlings or Protozoa, differ from all 
the rest, in being themselves units. The simplest animals 
are single cells; each is comparable to one of the myriad 
units which make up a sponge, a coral, a worm, a bird, a 
man. 

Here, therefore, there is an apparent gulf. The simplest 
animals are units—single cells; all other animals are com- 
binations of units—cities of cells. How is this gulf to be 
bridged? It is strange that evolutionists have not thought 
more about this, for on the transition from a unit to a com- 
bination of units the possibility of higher life depends. 

Every higher animal begins its individual life as a single 
cell, comparable to one of the firstlings. This single cell, 
or egg-cell, divides ; so do most of the Protozoa. But when 
a Protozoon divides, the results separate and live in- 
dependent lives; when an egg-cell divides, the results 
of division cohere. Therefore, the whole life of higher 
animals depends upon a coherence of units. 

But how did this begin? What of the gulf between 
single-celled Protozoa and all the other animals which are 
many-celled? Fortunately we are not left to mere specula- 
tion. The gulf has been bridged, else we should not exist ; 
but, more than that, the bridge, or part of it, is still left. 
There are a few of the simplest animals which form loose 
colonies of units, which, when they divide, remain together. 
Whether it was through weakness, as I am inclined to 
believe, that the transition forms between Protozoa and 
higher animals became strong, or for some hidden reason, 
we do not know. Some speak of this coherence of 
firstlings as a primal illustration of organic association, 


CHAP. V Social Life of Animals 89 


co-operation, surrender of individuality, of sociality at a low 
level, but it is unwise to apply these words to creatures 
so simple. All that we certainly know is that some of the 
simplest animals form loose colonies of units, that the gulf 
between them and the higher animals is thus bridged, and 
that the bridging depends on coherence. Our first con- 


Fic. 19.—Siphonophore colony, showing the float (a), the swimming-bells (4) ; 
the nutritive, reproductive, and other members of the colony beneath. (From 
the Zvolution of Sex; after Haeckel.) 


clusion, therefore, is, that the possibility of there being any 
higher animals depends, primarily at least, not on competition 
but on the coherence of units. 

Our next step is this: When we study sponges, or 
zoophytes, or most corals, or some types usually classed as 


go The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


“worms,” we see that the habit of forming colonies is 
common. Every sponge is a simple sac to begin with, 
but it buds off others like itself, and the result is a coherent 
colony. A zoophyte is not one individual, but a connected 
colony of individuals. Throughout the colony there is one 
life ; all the individuals have a common origin, and all are 
members one of another. In varying degrees of perfection 
the life of the whole is unified. Moreover, the unity is 
often increased, not diminished, by the fact that the indivi- 
duals are not all alike. There is division of labour among 
them; some may feed while others reproduce, some feel 
much while others may be quite callous. Thus, as we 
already mentioned, the Portuguese Man-of-War, a colony 
of small jellyfish-like individuals, has much division of 
labour, and yet there is much, though by no means perfect, 
unity of life. 

Our second conclusion is that among many animals— 
beginning with sponges and ending with the sea-squirts, 
which are acknowledged to be animals of high degree— 
the habit of forming colonies is common, and that these 
colonies, though organically continuous, illustrate the essence 
of society ; for in them many individuals of common descent 
and nature are united in mutual dependence and help- 
fulness. 

The next step towards an understanding of the social 
relations of animals is very different from that in which we 
have recognised the habit of forming colonies. The factor 
which we have now to acknowledge is the love of mates. 
This also has its history, this also has its prophecies among 
the firstlings, but we shall simply assume as a fact that 
among crustaceans and insects first, in fishes and amphi- 
bians afterwards, in reptiles too, but most conspicuously 
among birds and mammals, the males are attracted to the 
females, and in varying degrees of perfection enter into 
relations of mutual helpfulness. The relations and the 
attractions may be crude enough to begin with, but perhaps 
even we hardly know to what heights of devotion their 
highest expressions may attain. To mere physical fondness 
are added subtler attractions of sight and hearing, and 


CHAP. V Social Life of Animats gt 


these are sublimed in birds and mammals to what we call 
love. This love of mates broadens out ; it laps the family in 
its folds ; it diffuses itself as a saturating influence through 
the societies of animals and of men. “ Sociability,” Espinas 
says, “‘is based on the friendliness of mates.” 

The fourth step is the evolution of the family. From 
monkeys and beavers and many kinds of birds, to ants and 
bees and diverse insects, many animals illustrate family 
life. There is no longer the physical continuity charac- 
teristic of the colony, but there is a growing psychical 
unity. It is natural that the first ties of family life should 
be those between mother and young} and should be strong- 
est when the number of offspring is not very large. But 
even in some beetles, and more notably in certain fishes 
and amphibians, the males exhibit parental care and affec- 
tion; while in higher animals, especially among birds, the 
parents often divide the labours of the family. ‘* Children,” 
Lucretius said, “children with their caresses broke down 
the haughty temper of parents.” 

The fifth step is the combination of families into a 
society, such as we find illustrated by monkeys and 
beavers, cranes and parrots, and in great perfection by 
ants. The members are less nearly related than in the 
family, but there may be even more unity of spirit. 

I do not say that it is easy to understand how coherence 
of units led to the formation of a “ body,” how colonies 
became integrated and the labours of life more and more 
distributed, how love was evolved from apparently crude 
attractions between the sexes, how the love of mates was 
broadened into parental and filial affection, or how families 
well knit together formed the sure foundations of society ; 
but I believe that it is useful to recognise these steps in the 
history. 

We hardly know how to express ourselves in regard to 
the origin of affection. But I cannot get beyond Aristotle’s 
fundamental principle of evolution, that there is nothing in 
the end which was not also in the beginning. 

Yet we may fairly say that the sociality and helpfulness 
of animals are flowers whose roots are in kinship. Off- 


92 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


spring are continuous in nature with their parents; the 
family has a unity though its members be discontinuous 
and scattered ; ‘‘the race is one and the individual many.” 

9. Advantages of Social Life.—But animals are social, 
not only because they love one another, but also because 
sociality is justified of her children. “The world is the 
abode of the strong,” but it is also the home of the loving ; 
“contention is the vital force,” but the struggle is modified 
and ennobled by sociality. 

(a) Darwin's Position. — Darwin observed that “the 
individuals which took the greatest pleasure in society 
would best escape various dangers; while those that 
cared least for their comrades, and lived solitary, would 
perish in greater numbers.” He distinctly emphasised 
that the phrase “the struggle for existence” was to be 
used in a wide and metaphorical sense—to include all the 
endeavours which animals make both selfishly and un- 
selfishly to strengthen their foothold and that of their 
offspring. But he was not always successful in retaining 
this broad view, nor was he led to compute with sufficient 
care to what extent mutual aid is a factor in evolution 
counteractive of individualistic struggle. 

Without losing sight of the reality of the struggle for 
existence ; without disputing the importance of natural 
selection as a condition of evolution—securing that the 
relatively fittest changes succeed; without ignoring what 
seems almost a truism, that love and social sympathies have 
also been fostered in the course of natural selection; we 
maintain—(1) that many of the greatest steps of progress 
—such as those involved in the existence of many-celled 
animals, loving mates, family life, mammalian motherhood, 
and societies—were not made by the natural selection of 
indefinite variations ; (2) that affection, co-operation, mutual 
helpfulness, sociality, have modified the struggle for material 
subsistence by lessening its intensity and by ennobling its 
character. 

(b) Kropotkine’s Position.—Against Prof. Huxley’s con- 
clusion that “ Life was a continual free-fight, and beyond 
the limited and temporary relations of the family the 


CHAP. V Social Life of Animals 93 


Hobbesian war of each against all was the normal state of 
existence,” let me place that of Kropotkine, to whose admir- 
able discussion of mutual aid among animals I again 
acknowledge my indebtedness. 

“ Life in societies is no exception in the animal world. 
It is the rule, the law of nature, and it reaches its fullest 
development with the higher Vertebrates. Those species 
which live solitary, or in small families only, are relatively 
few, and their numbers are limited. . . . Life in societies 
enables the feeblest mammals to resist, or to protect them- 
selves from, the most terrible birds and beasts of prey; 
it permits longevity; it enables the species to rear its pro- 
geny with the least waste of energy, and to maintain its 
numbers, albeit with a very slow birth-rate ; it enables the 
gregarious animals to migrate in search of new abodes. 
Therefore, while fully admitting that force, swiftness, pro- 
tective colours, cunning, and endurance of hunger and cold, 
which are mentioned by Darwin and Wallace as so many 
qualities making the individual or the species the fittest 
under certain circumstances, we maintain that under any 
circumstances sociability is the greatest advantage in the 
struggle for life... The fittest are thus the most soci- 
able animals, and sociability appears as the chief factor of 
evolution, both directly, by securing the well-being of the 
species while diminishing the waste of energy, and indirectly 
by favouring the growth of intelligence. . . . Therefore 
combine—practise mutual aid! That is the surest means 
for giving to each and to all the greatest safety, the best 
guarantee of existence and progress—bodily, intellectual, 
and moral. That is what nature teaches us.” 

10. A Note on “The Social Organism.”—lIt is com- 
mon nowadays to speak of society as “the social organism,” 
and the metaphor is not only suggestive but convenient 
—suggestive because it is profitable to biologist and soci- 
ologist alike to follow out the analogies between an organism 
and society, convenient because there is among organisms 
—in aggregates like sponges, in perfected integrates like 
birds—a variety sufficient to meet all grades and views of 
society, and because biologists differ almost as much in 


94 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


their conceptions of an “organism” as sociologists do in 
regard to “ society.” 

It may be questioned, however, whether we need any 
other designation for society than the word society sup- 
plies, and whether the biological metaphor, with physical 
associations still clinging to it, is not more illusory than help- 
ful. For the true analogy is not between society and an 
individual organism, but between human- society and those 
incipient societies which were before man was. Human 
society is, or ought to be, an integrate—a spiritual integrate 
—of organisms, of which the bee-hive and the ants’ nest, 
the community of beavers and the company of monkeys, 
are like far-off prophecies. And in these, as in our own 
societies, the modern conception of heredity leads us to 
recognise that there is a very real unity even between 
members physically discontinuous. 

The peculiarity of human society, as distinguished from 
animal societies, depends mainly on the fact that man is a 
social person, and knows himself as such. Man is the realis- 
ation of antecedent societies, and it is man’s realisation of 
himself as a social person which makes human society what 
it is, and gives us a promise of what it will be. As bio- 
logists, and perhaps as philosophers, we are led to conclude 
that man is determined by that whole of which he is a 
part, and yet that his life is social freedom ; that society is 
the means of his development, and at the same time its 
end; that man has to some extent realised himself in society, 
and that society has been to some extent realised in man. 

But I am slow to suppose that we, who in our ignorance 
and lack of coherence are like the humbler cells of a great 
body, have any adequate conception of the social organism 
of which we form part. 

11. Conclusions.— I would in the main agree with 
Kropotkine that “ sociability is as much a law of nature as 
mutual struggle” ; with Espinas that “‘ Le milieu social est 
la condition nécessaire de la conservation et du renouvelle- 
ment de la vie”; and with Rousseau that “man did not 
make society, but society made man.” 


CHAPTER VI 
THE DOMESTIC LIFE OF ANIMALS 


1. Zhe Love of Mates—2, Love and Care for Offspring 


WINTER in our northern climate sets a spell upon life. 
The migrant birds escape from it, but most living things 
have to remain spell-bound, some hiding with the supreme 
patience of animals, others slumbering peacefully, others in 
a state of “latent life” stranger than death. But within 
the hard rind of the trees, or lapped round by bud scales, 
or imprisoned within the husks of buried seeds, the life of 
plants is ready to spring forth when the south wind blows ; 
beneath the snow lie the caterpillars of summer butterflies, 
the frogs are waiting in the mud of the pond, the hedgehog 
curled up sleeps soundly, and everywhere, under the seeming 
death, life rests until the spring. ‘For the coming of 
Ormuzd, the Light and Life Bringer, the leaf slept folded, 
the butterfly was hidden, the germ concealed, while the sun 
swept upwards towards Aries.” 

But when spring does come, heralded by returning 
migrants—swallows and cuckoos among the rest—how 
marvellous is the reawakening! The buds swell and burst, 
the corn sends up its light green shoots, the primrose and 
celandine are in blossom, the mother humble-bee comes 
out from her hiding-place and booms towards the willow 
catkins, the frogs croak and pair, none the worse of their 
fast, the rooks caw noisily, and the cooing of the dove is 
heard from the wood. Then, as the pale flowers are suc- 


96 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


ceeded by those of brighter tints, as the snowy hawthorn 
gives place to the laburnum’s “ dropping wells of fire ” and 
the bloom of the lilac, the butterflies flit in the sunshine, the 
chorus of birds grows stronger, and the lambs bleat in the 
valley. Temperature rises, colours brighten, life becomes 
strong and lusty, and the earth is filled with love. 

1. The Love of Mates.—In human life one of the 
most complex musical chords is the love of mates, in the 
higher forms of which we distinguish three notes — 
physical, emotional, and intellectual attraction. The love 
of animals, however, we can only roughly gauge by 
analogy; our knowledge is not sure enough to appreci- 
ate it justly, though we know beyond any doubt that in 
many the physical fondness of one sex for another is sub- 
limed by the: addition of subtler emotional sympathies. 
Among mammals, which frequently pair in spring, the 
males are often transformed by passion, the “timid” hare 
becomes an excited combatant with his rivals, while in the 
beasts of prey love often proves itself stronger than hunger. 
There is much ferocity in mammalian courtship—savage 
jealousy of rivals, mortal struggles between them, and suc- 
cess in wooing to the strongest. In many cases the love- 
making is like a storm—violent but passing. The animals 
pair and separate—the females to motherhood, the males to 
their ordinary life. A few, like some small antelopes, seem 
to remain as mates from year to year; many monkeys are 
said to be monogamous; but this is not the way of the 
majority. 

Birds are more emotional than mammals, and their love- 
making is more refined. The males are almost always 
more decorative than their mates, and excel in the power of 
song. They may sing, it is true, from sheer gladness of 
heart, from a genuine joy of life, and their lay rises 
“like the sap in the bough”; but the main motive of 
their music is certainly love. It may not always be music 
to us, but it is sweet to the ears for which it is meant—to 
which in many tones the song says ever “ Hither, my love! 
Here I am! Here!” Nor do the male birds woo by 
singing alone, but by love dances and by fluttering displays 


cuap. vi The Domestic Life of Animals 97 


98 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


of their bright plumage; with flowers, bright pods, and 
shining shells, the bower-birds decorate tents of love for 
their honeymoon. The mammals woo chiefly by force ; the 
birds are often moved to love by beauty, and mates often 
live in prolonged partnership with mutual delight and help- 
fulness. Sixty years before Darwin elaborated his theory of 
sexual selection, according to which males have grown more 
attractive because the most captivating suitors were most 
successful in love, the ornithologist Bechstein noted how the 
female canary or finch would choose the best singer among 
a crowd of suitors ; and there seems some reason to believe 
that the female’s choice of the most musical or the most 
handsome has been a factor in progress. Wallace, on the 
contrary, maintains that the females are plainly dressed 
because of the fate which has befallen the conspicuous during 
incubation, and surely they must thus be handicapped. To 
others it seems more natural to admit that there is truth in 
both Darwin’s and Wallace’s conclusions, but to regard the 
males as stronger, handsomer, or more musical simply 
because they are males, of more active constitutional habit 
than their mates. To this view Mr. Wallace himself inclines. 

Compared with the lion’s thunder, the elephant’s trum- 
peting, or the stag’s resonant bass, and the might which 
lies behind these, or with the warble of the nightingale, 
the carol of the thrush, the lark’s blithe lay, or the mocking- 
bird’s nocturne, and the emotional wealth which these ex- 
press, the challenges and calls of love among other classes 
of animals are apt to seem lacking in force or beauty. But 
our human judgment affords no sure criterion. The frogs 
and newts, which lead on an average a somewhat sluggish 
life, wake up at pairing time, and croak according to their 
strength. The males are often furnished with two reson- 
ating sacs at the back of the mouth, and how they can croak 
dwellers by marsh-land know ; the North American bull- 
frog bellows by himself, and the South American tree-frogs 
hold a concert in the branches. 

Of the mating of fishes we know little, but there are some 
well-known cases alike of display and of tournament. The 
stickleback fights with his rivals, leads his mate to 


cuar. vi The Domestic Life of Animals 99 


the nest by captivating wiles, dances round her in a frenzy, 


Fic. 21.—Male and female bird of paradise (Paradisea minor). (From Evolu- 
tion of Sex ; after Catalogue of Dresden Museum.) 


and afterwards guards the eggs with jealous care. The 


100 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


male salmon, with their hooked lower jaws, fight with their 
rivals, sometimes to the death. 

Among insects the love-play is again very lively. Like 
birds, many of these active animals are very beautiful in 
colour and form, especially in the male sex, and a display of 
charms has often been noticed. Like birds, though in a 
different fashion, some of them are musical, using their 
hard legs and wing-edges as instruments. The crickets 
chirp merrily, the cicadas “sing,” and the death-watch taps 
at the door of his mate. 

In the summer night, when colours are put out by the 
darkness, the glow-worm shines brightly on the mossy bank. 
In the British species (Lampyris noctiluca) the winged 
male and the wingless female are both luminous ; the latter 
indeed excels in brightness, while her mate has larger eyes. 
Whatever the phosphorescence may mean to the constitution 
of the insect, it is certainly a love-signal between the sexes. 
But we know most about the Italian glow-worm (Luczola 
italica), of whose behaviour we have a lively picture—thanks 
to Professor Emery’s nocturnal observations in the meadows 
around Bologna. The females sit among the grass; the 
males fly about in search of them. When a female catches 
sight of the flashes of an approaching male, she allows her 
splendour to shine. He sees the female’s signal, and is 
swiftly beside her, circling round like a dancing elf. But 
one suitor is not enough. The female attracts a levée. 
In polite rivalry her devotees form a circle and await the 
coquette’s choice. In the two sexes, Emery says, the 
colour of the light is identical, and the intensity seems 
much the same, though the love-light of the female is more 
restricted. The most noteworthy difference is that the 
luminous rhythm of the male is more rapid, with briefer 
flashes ; while that of the female is more prolonged, with 
longer intervals, and more tremulous—illumined symbols of 
the contrast between the sexes. 

While recognising the genuinely beautiful love-making of 
most birds, we did not ignore that the courtship of most 
mammals is somewhat rough. So, after admiring the love 
dances of many butterflies, the merry songs of the grass- 


car. vi The Domestic Life of Animals 101 


hoppers, and the flashing signals of the glow-insects, it is 
just that we should turn to the strange courtship of spiders, 
which is less ideal. Of what we may be prepared to find 
we get a hint from a common experience. Not long ago I 
found in a gorge some spiders which I had never seen 
before. Wishing to examine them at leisure, I captured a 
male and a female, and, having only one box, put them, 
with misgivings, together. When I came to examine 
them, however, the male was represerited by shreds. 
Such unnatural conduct, though by no means universal 
among spiders, is common. The tender mercies of spiders 
are cruel. We have lately obtained an account of the 
courtship of spiders from George W. and Elizabeth G. 
Peckham, from whose careful observations I select the 
following illustrations : 


According to these observers, ‘‘ there is no evidence that the 
male spiders possess greater vital activity; on the contrary, it is 
the female that is the more active and pugnacious of the two. 
There is no relation in either sex between development of colour 
and activity. The Lycoside, which are the most active of all 
spiders, have the least colour-development, while the sedentary orb- 
weavers show the most brilliant hues. In the numerous cases 
where the male differs from the female by brighter colours and 
ornamental appendages, these adornments are not only so placed 
as to be in full view of the female during courtship, but the atti- 
tudes and antics of the male spider at that time are actually such as 
to display them to the fullest extent possible. The fact that in the 
Aitide the males vie with each other in making an elaborate dis- 
play, not only of their grace and agility, but also of their beauty, 
before the females, and that the females, after attentively watching 
the dances and tournaments which have been executed for their 
gratification, select for their mates the males that they find most 
pleasing, points strongly to the conclusion that the great differences 
in colour and in ornament between these spiders are the result of 
sexual selection.” 

These conclusions support Darwin’s position that the female’s 
choice is a great factor in evolving attractiveness, and are against 
Wallace’s contention that bright colours express greater vitality, 
and that the females are less brilliant because enemies eliminate 
the conspicuous. It is quite likely that Darwin’s view is true in 
some cases (¢.g. these spiders), and Wallace’s conclusion true in 
others (e.g. birds and butterflies), or that both may be true in 


102 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


many cases; while the fact that the males of these spiders are 
always more brilliant than their mates suggests again that the 
brilliancy is wrapped up along with the mystery of maleness, which 
it is not sufficient to define merely as superabundant vitality, or as 
greater activity, but rather as a tendency towards a relative increase 
of destructive or disruptive vital changes over those which are 
constructive or conservative. But the problem is very complex, 
and dogmatic conclusions are premature. We need to know 
the chemical nature and history of the pigments to which the 
colour is due; we need to have an approximate balance-sheet 
of the income and expenditure of the two sexes. Enough of this, 
however ; let us return to the pictures. We talk about romance— 
listen to these patient observers : 


Fic. 22.—Two male spiders (Habrocestum splendens to the left, and Astia 
vittata to the right) displaying themselves before their mates. (After 
G. W. and E. G, Peckham.) 


“‘On reaching the country we found that the males of Sadz¢7s 
pulex were mature and were waiting for the females, as is the way 
with both spiders and insects. In this species there is but little 
difference between the sexes. On May 24th we found a mature 
female and placed her in one of the larger boxes, and the next day 
we put a male in with her, He saw her as she stood perfectly 
still, twelve inches away. The glance seemed to excite him, and 
he at once moved toward her. When some four inches from her 
he stood still, and then began the most remarkable performances 
that an amorous male could offer to an admiring female. She eyed 
him eagerly, changing her position from time to time, so that he 
might always be in view. He, raising his whole body on one side 
by straightening out the legs, and lowering it on the other by fold- 


cuar. vi The Domestic Life of Animals 103 


ing the first two pairs of legs up and under, leaned so far over as to 
be in danger of losing his balance, which he only maintained by 
sidling rapidly toward the lowered side. The palpus, too, on this 
side was turned back to correspond to the direction of the legs 
nearest it. He moved in a semicircle of about two inches, and 
then instantly reversed the position of the legs and circled in the 
opposite direction, gradually approaching nearer and nearer to the 
female. Now she dashes toward him, while he, raising his first pair 
of legs, extends them upward and forward as if to hold her off, but 
withal slowly retreats. Again and again he circles from side to 
side, she gazing toward him in a softer mood, evidently admiring 
the grace of his antics. This is repeated until we have counted one 
hundred and eleven circles made by the ardent little male. Now 
he approaches nearer and nearer, and when almost within reach 
whirls madly around and around her, she joining and whirling with 
him in a giddy maze. Again he falls back, and resumes his semi- 
circular motions with his body tilted over; ‘she, all excitement, 
lowers her head and raises her body, so that it is almost vertical. 
Both draw nearer, she moves slowly under him, he crawling over 
her head, and the mating is accomplished.” The males are quarrel- 
some and fight with 
one another ; but after 
watching <‘‘ hundreds 
of seemingly terrible 
battles” between the 
males of twelve differ- 
ent species, the obser- 
vers were forced to the 
conclusion that ‘‘ they 
are all sham affairs 
gotten up for the pur- 
pose of displaying be- 
fore the females, who 
commonly stand by in- 
terested spectators.” 
“Tt seemed cruel sport 
at first to put eight or 
foes ales ee a Fic. 23.—Two male a aes yes bettint) 


phantes capitatus) into “fighting, (After G. W. and E. G. Peckham.) 
a box to see them fight, 


but it was soon apparent that they were very prudent little fellows, 
and were fully conscious that ‘he who fights and runs away will 
live to fight another day.’ In fact, after two weeks of hard fighting 
we were unable to discover one wounded warrior. . . . The 
single female (of Phidippus morsitans) that we caught during the 


104 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


CC 


Fic. 24.—Male argus pheasant displaying its plumage. (From Darwin.) 


summer was a savage monster. The two males that we provided 
for her had offered her only the merest civilities when she leaped 


cuar. vi Lhe Domestic Life of Animals 105 


upon them and killed them.” ‘‘The female of Dendryphantes 
elegans is much larger than the male, and her loveliness is accom- 
panied by an extreme irritability of temper, which the male seems 
to regard as a constant menace to his safety; but his eagerness 
being great, and his manners devoted and tender, he gradually 
overcomes her opposition. Her change of mood is only brought 
about after much patient courting on his part.” In other species 
(Phileus militaris) the males take possession of young females and 
keep guard over them until they become mature. We sometimes 
hear of courtship by telephone, In the Epeiride spiders ‘it seems 
to be carried on, to some extent at least, by a vibration of web 
lines,” as M‘Cook and Termeyer have also observed. 


Surely it is a long gamut this, from a mammal’s clamant 
call and forcible wooing, or from the sweet persuasiveness 
of our singing birds, and the fluttering displays of others, to 
the trembling of a thread in the web of a spider. But, 
however varied be the pitch of the song and the form of 
the dance, all are expressions of love. 

Mates are also attracted to one another by odours. 
These are best known in mammals (¢.g. beaver and civet) 
and in reptiles; they predominate in the males, and at the 
breeding season. They usually proceed from skin glands ; 
but we understand little about them. They serve as 
incense or as stimulant, but perhaps this usefulness is 
secondary. The zoologist Jaeger regards the odoriferous 
substances in plants and animals as characteristic of and 
essentially associated with each life; but without going so 
far we may recognise that in the general life of flowers 
and animals alike odours are very important. We know, 
too, that certain odours make much impression upon us; 
such as those of hawthorn and of the hay-field, of newly- 
mown grass and of withered leaves, of violet and of 
lavender; and furthermore, that in some mysterious way 
some fragrances excite or soothe the system, and have 
become associated with sexual and other emotions. 

2. Love and Care for Offspring. — Gradual as the 
incoming of spring has been the blossoming of parental 
love among animals. We cannot tell in what forms it 
first appeared in distinctness. We cannot say Lo here! or 
T.o there! for it is latent in them all, 


106 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


In many of the lower animals the units which begin 
new lives are readily separated from the parent; but in 
others, ¢,g. some of the simplest, or some by no means 
simple “worms,” and even some insects, the parent life 
disappears in giving birth to the young. Reproduction or 
the continuance of the species often involves a sacrifice of 
the individual life. 

It is strangely true, even in the highest forms, that 
reproduction, though a blossoming of the whole life, is also 
the beginning of death. It is costly, and brings death as 
well as life in its train. This is tragically illustrated by 
many insects, such as butterflies, who die soon after repro- 
ducing, though often not before they have, in obedience to 
instinctive impulse, cared most effectively for their eggs— 
the results of which they do not live to see. Think also of 
the mayflies, or Ephemeride, who, after a prolonged aquatic 
life as larvee, become winged, dance in the sunlight for an 
hour, mate and reproduce, and die. 

Picture the long larval life in the water, and the short 
aerial happiness lasting for an evening or two. Long life, 
compared with the span of many other insects, but short 
love; there may be years of patience, and but a day 
of pleasure; great preparations, and the anti-climax 
of death. The eggs lie half conscious in the water, 
faintly stirred by the growing life within, lapped round 
about by peace,——though the trout thin them sorely. 
In the survivors the embryos become conscious, awaken 
from their rocking, and turn themselves in their cradles. 
See the larve creep forth, wash themselves gaily in 
the water, and hungrily fall upon their prey, some 
smaller insects. The little “water-wings” grow, and 
the air soaks into the blood; the larvae cast their skins 
many times, and hide from the fishes. At length comes 
the final moult, and the making ot the air-wings, of which 
in the summer evening you may see the first short flight as 
the insects rise like a living mist from the pool. But even 
yet a thin veil, too truly suggestive of a shroud, encumbers 
them; and they rest wearily on the grass or on the 
branches of the willow. Watch them writhe and jerk, as if 


cuar. vi The Domestic Life of Animals 107 


impatient, till at length their last encumbrance — their 
“ghost,” as naturalists call it—is thrown off. Now the 
other life, the life of love, begins. Merrily they dance up 
and down, dimpling the smooth water into smiling with a 
touch—chasing, embracing, separating. See the filmy fairy 
wings, the large lustrous eyes of the males, the tail fila- 
ments gracefully sweeping in the dance! They never 
pause to eat—they could not if they tried; hunger is past, 
love is present, and in the near future is death. The 
evening shadows grow longer,—shadows of death to the 
Ephemerides. The trout jump at them, a few rain-drops 
thin the throng, the stream bears others away. The 
mothers lay their eggs in the water, and wearily die forth- 
with—cradle and tomb are side by side; the males seem 
to pass in a sigh from the climax of loving to the other 
crisis of dying. But the eggs are in the water, and 
the dance of love is more than a dance of death. 
Turning homewards, we cannot but think sadly of other 
Ephemerides, of patient larval life, of the gradual 
revealing of the higher self, of shrouds thrown aside and 
wedding robes put on, of hunger eaten up by love, of the 
sacrifice of maternity, of cradle and tomb together. Yet 
we remember the eggs in the water, the promise of the 
future beneath the surface of the stream. Under the horse- 
chestnut tree, too, the wind has blown the shed petals like 
white foam, but the tree itself is strong like Ygdrasil, and 
among the branches a bird sings in the twilight. 

Returning in more matter-of-fact mood to parental care, 
we need not dwell upon those cases where the young 
are simply sheltered for a while about the body of the 
mother, hanging to a jellyfish, on some sea-urchins hidden 
in tents of spines, in one or two sea-cucumbers half buried 
in the skin, adhering to the naked ventral surface of the 
common little leech (C/epséme), imprisoned in modified 
tentacles in some marine worms, carried about in a dorsal 
brood-chamber in many water-fleas, or under the curved 
tail of higher crustaceans, retained within the gills of 
bivalves, and so on. Such adaptations are interesting, 
they involve prolonged physical contact between mother 


108 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


and offspring, but we are in search of cases where the 
parent acts as if she cared for her young. 

But this care, as we said, begins very gradually. Thus, 
in some lowly crustaceans the young may return to the 
brood-chamber of the mother, even after hatching and 
moulting ; and young crayfish are said to return to the 
shelter of the maternal tail after they have been set adrift. 
Strange, too, are the males of some sea-spiders (Pycno- 
gonida), who carry about the ova on their legs. It is con- 
fidently stated that the headless freshwater mussel keeps 
the embryos imprisoned even after the normal period, 
until some freshwater fish be present, to which they may 
attach themselves ; while some cuttle-fishes are said to exert 
themselves in keeping their egg-clusters clean and safe. 

But it is among insects, with their full, free life, that we 
see the best examples of parental care in backboneless 
animals. Some scoff at the “beetle-pricker” or the scara- 
beist,—and such genial laughter as that of the Professor at 
the Breakfast Table has a healthy resonance,—but those 
who scoff have not read Kirby’s Zeéfers, else they would 
feel that the student of insects watches at a well-head of 
romance and marvel inexhaustibly fresh. What, for 
instance, shall we say of the worker-bees, who, though no 
parents, tend and nurse the grubs with constant care ; or of 
the likewise sexless worker-ants, whose first endeavour 
when the nest is disturbed is to save, not themselves, but 
the young; or of the care that flies, moths, and other 
insects will take to lay their eggs in substances and situa- 
tions best fitted for the future young? We must think back 
into the past history of climatic and other conditions if 
we would understand the frequently elaborate provision 
which mother insects make for offspring which they never 
see; the ancestors had probably a longer life, and had the 
gratification of seeing the result of their labours, and now 
the inherited habit works on, perhaps with no vision of 
the future. We must also allow that the offspring mis- 
takenly deposited by an imperfect maternal instinct would 
most likely die, and thus leave the race more select. But 
after thinking out these explanations, the facts remain mar- 


cuar. vi Lhe Domestic Life of Animals 109 


vellous. Thus W. Marshall saw an Ichneumon fly 
(Polynema natans) remain twelve hours under water, 
without special adaptations for,such a life, swimming about 
with her wings, and depositing her eggs within the larvze of 
caddis-flies ! 

We are accustomed, the same naturalist says, to look 
upon a hen which gathers her brood under her wings as a 
picture of loving care, but we must recognise that the same 
is true of earwigs, spiders, and scorpions. Many of us 
have lifted a large stone on the dry bank, and seen the 
hurry-scurry of small animals; there are earwigs among 
the rest, and the pale-yellowish young crowd quickly under 
the shelter of their mothers, who stand guard with open 
pincers. Female spiders, too, so fierce and impatient as 
mates, are most “respectable mothers.” Some make nests, 
guard, feed, and even fight for the young; others carry the 
eggs about with them. “I have often,” Marshall says, 
“made fun of the little creatures, taking away their 
precious egg-sac and removing it to a slight distance. It 
was interesting to see how eagerly they sought, and how 
joyously, one may even say, they sprang upon their ‘one 
and all’ when they found it again. Sometimes I cheated 
them with a little ball of wool of the size, form, and colour 
of the egg-sac, which they quickly seized, and as rapidly 
rejected.” 

Many fishes lay their eggs by hundreds in the water, 
and thenceforth have nothing more to do with them, but 
even among these cold-blooded animals there are illustra- 
tions of parental care. From a bridge over the river you 
may be able to watch the female salmon ploughing a 
furrow in the gravelly bed, and there laying her eggs, care- 
ful not to disturb the places where others have already 
spawned, In quiet by-pools you may find the gay male 
stickleback guarding the nest which he has made of twined 
fibres partly glued together with mucus. There the female 
has laid eggs, but he has driven her forth: he will do all 
the nursing himself. No approaching enemy is too large 
for him to attack; his courage equals his seeming pride. 
When the young are hatched, but not yet able to fend for 


II0 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


themselves, his cares are increased tenfold. It is hard to 
keep the youngsters in the cradle. ‘No sooner has he 
brought one bold truant back, than two others are out, and 
so it goes on the whole day long.” 

We are not clever enough to understand why the males 
among many fishes are so much more careful than the 
females. For the stickleback is not alone in his excellent 
behaviour. The male Chinese macropod (Polyacanthus) 
makes a frothy nest of air and mucus, in which he places 
his mate’s eggs. He, too, watches jealously over the brood, 
and “has his hands—or rather his mouth—full to recover 
the hasty throng when they stray, and to pack them again 
into their cradle.” Of all strange habits, perhaps that 
is strangest which some male fish (e.g. Azdus) have of 
hatching the eggs in their mouths ; what external dangers 
must have threatened them before this quaint brooding- 
chamber was chosen! Or is it not almost like a joke to see 
the male sea-horse swelling up as 
the eggs which he has stowed 
away in an external pocket hatch 
and mature, ‘till one day we 
see emerging from the aperture a 
number of small, almost transpar- 
ent creatures, something like 
marks of interrogation.” But 
some female fishes also carry 
their eggs about, attached to the 
ventral surface (in the Siluroid 
fish, Asfredo), or stowed away in 
a ventral pouch (in Solenostoma, 
allied to pipe-fishes), arrange- 
ments which recur among amphi- 

Sanguine ultilaeneyen | (Eioe bians, but on the dorsal surface 

Evolution of Sex; after Atlas of the body. 

of Naples. Station: Amphibians, like fishes, to 
which they are linked by many ties, are either quaint or 
careless parents. Again, the males assume the responsi- 
bilities of nurture. The obstetric frog (4A/y/es obstetricans), 
common in some parts of the Continent, takes the eggs from 


Fic. 25.—Sea-horse (Hippo- 


coar. vi The Domestic Life of Animals III 


his mate, winds them round his hind-legs, and retires into a 
hole, whence, after a fortnight or so, he betakes himself to the 
water, there to be relieved by the speedy hatching of his 
precious burden. Even quainter is the habit of the male of 
a Chilian frog (Rhinoderma darwinit), who keeps the eggs and 
the young in a pouch near the larynx, turning a resonating 
sac in a most matter-of-fact way into a cradle. He is some- 
what leaner after it is all over. It is interesting to notice 
how similar forms and habits recur among animals of dif- 
ferent kinds, like the theme in some musical compositions. 
The spiral form of shell common in the simple chalk-forming 
Foraminifers recurs in the pearly nautilus; the eye of a 
fish is practically like that of many a cuttle, though the 
two are made in quite different ways; and an extraordinary 
development of paternal care may signalise animals so 
distinct as sea-spider, stickleback, and frog. 

But we must not be unfair to the female amphibians. 
Without doubt most of them are willing to be quickly rid 
of their eggs or young, and as these are usually very 
numerous, the mortality in the pools is of little moment. 
In some cases, however, water-pools are less available 
than in Britain, and then we find adaptations securing the 
welfare of the young. The black salamander of the Alps, 
living at elevations where pools are rare, retains her twin 
offspring until more than half of the tadpole life is past. 
They breathe and feed in a marvellous way within the 
body of the mother, and are born as lung-breathers. In 
the case of the Surinam Toad (Ppa), the male places half 
a hundred eggs on the back of the female, where they 
become surrounded by small pockets of skin, from which 
the young toads writhe out fully formed. In two other 
cases (Nototrema and Notodelphys), the above somewhat 
expensive adaptation, which involves a great destruction 
of skin, is replaced by a dorsal pouch in which the eggs 
hatch, an arrangement dimly suggestive of the pouch of 
kangaroos and other marsupial mammals. 

Fishes and amphibians are linked closely by their likeness 
in structure, and, as we have seen, they are somewhat alike 
in parental habits; but how great is the contrast between 


112 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


the habits of birds and reptiles, in spite of their genuine 
blood-relationship. Yet the python coiled round her eggs 
is a prophecy of the brooding birds, as in past ages the 
flopping Saurians prophesied their swift-winged flight. The 
sharpness of the contrast is also lessened by the fact that a 
few birds, like the mound-builders, do not brood at all; while 
others, it must be confessed, are somewhat careless. But, 
exceptions and criminals apart, birds are so lavish in their 
love, so constant in their carefulness, that it is difficult to 
speak of them without exaggeration. I am quite willing to 
allow that they often act without thought (that is half 
the beauty of it); nor do I doubt that many species 
would have gone to the wall long since in the struggle of 
life if the parents had not taken so much care of the young ; 
but I would rather emphasise at present the reality that 
they do sacrifice themselves for the sake of their young 
to a most remarkable degree, and spend themselves not for 
individual ends, but for their offspring. 

Before the time of egg-laying the birds build their nests, 
eagerly but without hurry, instinctively yet with some plas- 
ticity, and often with much beauty. On the laid eggs, 
which require warmth to develop, the mothers brood, 
and though to rest after reproduction is natural, the brood- 
ing is not without its literal patience. Among polygamous 
birds the males are, as one would expect, more or less 
careless of their mates, but most of the monogamous males 
are careful either in sharing the duty of brooding or in, 
supplying the females with food. After the eggs hatch, 
the degree of care required varies according to the 
state of the young; for many are precociously energetic 
and able to look after themselves, while others still require 
prolonged nurture. They need large quantities of food, 
to supply which all the energies of both parents seem 
sometimes no more than adequate; they may still require 
to be brooded over, and certainly to be protected from 
rain and enemies. After they are reared, they have to be 
taught to fly, to catch food, to avoid danger, and a dozen 
other arts. With what apparent love—willing and joyous 
—is all this done for them! 


cnar. vi Lhe Domestic Life of Animals 113 


Consider the cunning often displayed in leaving or 
approaching the nest, in removing débris which would 
betray the whereabouts of the young, or in distracting 
attention to a safe distance ; remember, too, that some birds 


Fic. 26.—Nest of tailor-bird (Orthotoneus benettii). (After Brehm.) 


will shift either eggs or young to a new resting-place when 
extreme danger threatens; estimate the energy spent in 
feeding the brood, sometimes on a diet quite different 
from that of adult life; and acknowledge that the parental 
instinct is very deeply rooted, since fostering young not 
their own may be practised by orphaned birds of both 
sexes. Listen to the bird which has been bereaved, and tell 
me is not the “lone singer wonderful, causing tears” ? 

The female of the Indian and African hornbill nests in 
a hole in a tree, the entrance to which she plasters up so 
that no room is left either for exit or entrance. The 

I 


114 The Study of Animal Life PART J 


Malays imagined that this was the work of the jealous 
male, but it is the female’s own doing. “She sits,” 
Marshall says, “securely hidden, safe from any carnivore 
or mischievous ape or snake stealthily climbing, while the 
male exerts himself lovingly to bring his mate those 
delightful things in which the tropical forest is rich—fruits 
above all, but occasionally a delicate mouse or juicy 
frog. He flies with his booty to the tree and gives a 
peculiar knock, which his mate knows as his signal, and 
thrusts her beak through the narrow window, welcoming her 
meal.” At the end of the period of incubation, C. M. Wood- 
ford says, “the devoted husband is worn to a skeleton.” 

But animals, like men, have their vices, and birds, gener- 
ally so ideal in their behaviour, are sometimes criminals. 
Ornithologists assure us that the degree of parental care 
varies not only in nearly related species, but also among 
members of the same species. We need not lay much 
stress on the fact that a bird occasionally slips its egg 
into a neighbour’s nest, for when a partridge thus uses a 
pheasant’s rough bed, or a gull that of an eider-duck, it 
is likely enough that the intruder had been disturbed 
from her own resting-place when about to lay. We 
approach something different in the case of the American 
Ostrich (Rhea), the female of which is quite ready to 
utilise a neighbour’s burrow; nor does the owner seem 
to object, for all the brooding is discharged by the male, 
“and it is no great art to be patient and magnanimous at 
another’s expense.” Again, in the case of the American 
Ani (Crotophaga ani), of whose habits we unfortunately 
know little, a number of females sometimes lay their eggs 
in a common nest. 

We are so glad to hear the cuckoo’s call in spring that 
we almost forget the wickedness of the voluble bird. The 
poets have helped us, for they have generously idealised, in 
fact idolised, the cuckoo, the “darling of the spring,” “a 
wandering voice babbling of sunshine and of flowers,” a 
“ sweet,” nay more, a ‘‘blessed bird.” But the cuckoos 
have hoaxed the poets, for they are even worse than their 
legendary reputation of being sparrow-hawks in disguise ; 


cuar. vi Zhe Domestic Life of Animals 11s 


they are “greedy feeders,” says Brehm, ‘discontented, ill- 
conditioned, passionate fellows; in short, decidedly unamiable 
birds.” The truth must be told, the cuckoo is an immoral 
vagabond, an Ishmaelite, an individualist, a keeper of game 
“preserves.” There are so many males that they have 
perverted and thoroughly demoralised the females ; there is 
no true pairing; they are polyandrous. The birds are too 
hungry for genuine love, though there is no lack of passion; 
while by voraciously devouring hairy caterpillars they have 
acquired a gizzard-fretting feltwork in their stomachs, and 
for all I know are cursed by dyspepsia as well as by a con- 
stitutionally evil character. It is not quite correct to say 
that the cuckoo-mother is immoral because she shirks the 
duties of maternity; it is rather that she puts her young out 
to nurse because she is immoral.!_ The so-called “ parasitic” 
trick is an outcrop of an egoistic constitution which shows 
itself in many different ways. The young bird, “a dog in 
the manger by birth,” evicts the helpless rightful tenants 
whether they are still passive in the eggs or more assertive 
as nestlings, and as he grows up a spoilt child his foster 
parents lead no easy life. But though the poets have been 
hoaxed, I do not believe that the nurses of the fledgling 
are; it seems rather as if the naughtiness of their 
changeling had some charm. 

Of course there is another way of looking at the cuckoo’s 
crime. It is advantageous, and there is much art in the 
well-executed trick by which the mother foists her several 
eggs, at intervals of several days, into the nests of various 
birds, which are usually insectivorous and suited for the 
upbringing of the intruder. I think there is at least some 
deliberation in this so-called instinct. Nor should one forget 
that the mother occasionally returns to the natural habit of 
hatching her own eggs,—a pleasant fact which several trust- 
worthy observers have thoroughly established. Still, in 
spite of the poets, the note of this “blessed bird” must be 
regarded as suggestive of sin ! 

1 The student will notice that I have occasionally used words 
which are not strictly accurate. I may therefore say definitely that I 


do not believe that we are warranted in crediting animals with moral, 
zesthetic, or, indeed, any conceptions, 


116 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


There is much to be said about the domestic life of 
animals—their courtship, their helpful partnership, and their 
parentage—but perhaps I have said enough to induce you 
to think about these things more carefully. Many of the 
deepest problems of biology—the origin and evolution of 
sex, the relation of reproduction to the individual and to the 
species—should be considered by those who feel themselves 
naturally inclined to such inquiries ; moreover, in connection 
with our own lives, it is profitable to investigate among 
animals the different grades of the love of mates, and the 
relation between the rate of reproduction and the degree of 
development. First, however, it were better that we should 
watch the ways of animals and seek after some sympathy 
with them, that we may respect their love, and salute them 
not with stone or bullet, but with the praise of gladdened 
eyes. 

Ruskin’s translation of what Socrates said in regard to 
the halcyon is suggestive of the mood in which we should 
consider these things. 


Chaerophon. ‘‘ And is that indeed the halcyon’s cry? I never 
heard it yet; and in truth it is very pitiful. How large is the 
bird, Socrates?” 

Socrates. ‘*Not great ; but it has received great honour from the 
gods, because of its lovingness; for while it is making its nest, all 
the world has the happy days which we call halcyonidz, excelling 
all others in their calmness, though in the midst of storm.” 

“We being altogether mortal and mean, and neither able to see 
clearly great things nor small, and for the most part being unable to 
help ourselves even in our own calamities, what can we have to say 
about the powers of the immortals, either over halcyons or night- 
ingales? But the fame of fable, such as our fathers gave it to us, 
this to my children, O thou bird singing of sorrow, I will deliver 
concerning thy hymns; and I myself will sing often of this religious 
and human love of thine, and of the honour thou hast for it from 
the gods.” 

Chaerophon. “It is rightly due indeed, O Socrates, for there is 
a twofold comfort in this, both for men and women, in their rela- 
tions with each other.” 

Socrates. ‘‘ Shall we not then salute the halcyon, and so go back 
to the city by the sands, for it is time?” 


CHAPTER VII 
THE INDUSTRIES OF ANIMALS 


1. Hunting—2. Shepherding—3. Storing—4. Making of Homes 
5. Movements 


IT is likely that primitive man fed almost wholly upon fruits. 
His early struggles with animals were defensive rather than 
aggressive, though with growing strength he would become 
able for more than parrying. We can fancy how a band of 
men who had pursued and slain some ravaging wild beast 
would satisfy at once hunger and rage by eating the warm 
flesh. Somehow, we know, hunting became an habitual art. 
We can also fancy how hunters who had slain a mother animal 
kept her young alive and reared them. In this or in some 
other way the custom of domesticating animals began, and 
men became shepherds. And as the hunter’s pursuits were 
partially replaced by pastoral life, so the latter became in 
some regions accessory to the labours of agriculture, with 
the development of which we may reasonably associate the 
foundation of stable homesteads. Around these primary 
occupations arose the various human industries, with division 
of labour between man and woman, and between man and 
man. : 

These human industries suggest a convenient arrange- 
ment for those practised by animals. For here again there 
are hunters and fishers—beasts of prey of all kinds—pursuing 
the chase with diverse degrees of art; shepherds, too, for some 
ants use the aphides as cows; and farmers without doubt, 


118 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


if we use the word in a sense wide enough to include those 
who collect, modify, and store the various fruits of the 
earth. 

In illustrating these industries, I shall follow a charming 
volume by Frédéric Houssay, Les Industries des Animaux, 
Paris, 1890, 

1. Hunting.—Of this primary activity there are many 
kinds. The crocodile lies in wait by the water’s edge, 
the python hangs like a lian from the tree, the octopus 
lurks in a nook among the rocks, and the ant-lion 
(Myrmeleon) digs in the sand a pitfall for unwary 
insects. The angler-fish (Lophius piscatorius) is some- 
what protectively coloured as he lies on the sand among 
the seaweeds; on his back three filaments dangle, and 
possibly suggest worms to curious little fishes, which, 
venturing near, are engulfed by the angler’s horrid maw, 
and firmly gripped by jaws with backward-bending teeth. 
Many animals prowl about in search of easy prey—eggs 
of birds, sleeping beasts, and small creatures like white 
ants; others would be burglars, like the Death’s Head 
Moth (Sphinx atropos) who seeks to slink into the homes 
of the bees ; others are full of wiles, witness the cunning fox 
and the wide-awake crow. Many, however, are hunters by 
open profession, notably the carnivorous birds and mammals. 
If these hunters could speak we should hear of many strange 
exploits; such, for instance, as that of a large spider which 
landed a small fish. The ins and outs of their ways are 
most interesting, especially to the student of comparative 
intelligence. Think of the Indian Zoxotes, a fish which 
squirts drops of water on insects and brings them down 
most effectively; several birds which let shells drop from 
a height, eg. the Greek eagle (Gypaétos barbatus), which 
killed AEschylus by letting a tortoise drop on his head; 
the grey-shrike (Lanius excubitor), which spikes its victims 
on thorns ; and, strangest perhaps, the slave-making expedi- 
tions of the Amazon ants. All strength and wiles not- 
withstanding, the chase is often by no means easy; the 
hare grows swift as.well as the fox, many grow cautious 
like trout in a much-fished stream, scouts and sentinels 


CHAP, VII The Industries of Animals 119 


are often utilised, the weak combine against the strong, 
and the victims of even the strong carnivores often show 
fight valiantly. 

2. Shepherding.—Although the ants are the only animals 
which show a pastoral habit in any perfection, and that 
only in four or five species (¢.g. Lasius niger and Lasius 
brunneus), 1 think that the fact is one about which we 
may profitably exercise our minds. I shall follow Espinas’s 
admirable discussion of the subject. 

We may begin with the simple association of ants and 
aphides as commensals eating at the same bountiful table. 
But as ants discovered that the aphides were overflowing 
with sweetness, they formed the habit of licking them, the 
aphides submitting with passive enjoyment. Moreover, as 
the ants nesting near the foot of a tree covered with 
aphides would resent that others should invade their pre- 
serves, it is not surprising to find that they should continue 
their earthen tunnels up the stem and branches, and should 
eventually build an aerial stable for some of their cattle. 
Thither also they transport some of their own larvee to be 
sunned, and as they carried these back again when the 
rain fell, they would surely not require the assistance of an 
abstract idea to prompt them to take some aphides also 
downstairs. Or perhaps it is enough to suppose that the 
aphides, by no means objecting to the ants’ attentions, 
did not require any coaxing to descend the tunnels, and 
eventually to live in the cellars of the nests, where they 
feed comfortably on roots, and are sheltered from the bad 
weather of autumn. In autumn the aphides lay eggs in 
the cellars to which they have been brought by force or 
coaxing or otherwise, and these eggs the ants take care of, 
putting them in safe cradles, licking them as tenderly as 
they do those of their own kind. Thus the domestication 
of aphides by ants is completed. 

Now what is the theory of this shepherding? (1) We 
have no warrant for saying that the ants have deliberately 
domesticated these aphides, as men have occasionally 
added to the number of their domesticated animals. It 
does not seem to me probable that even primitive man 


120 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


was very deliberate in the steps which led to the first 
domestications, (2) Nor is it likely that the process 
began in a casual way, and that it became predominant 
in four or five species in the course of natural selection. 
For the habit is more a luxury than a necessity, and 
it is not likely to have been evolved before the estab- 
lishment of the sterile caste of workers, who have no means 
of transmitting their experience. Moreover, initial steps are 
always difficult to explain on this.theory. (3) The theory 
which seems to me warrantable is that the habit arose by 
a gradual extension of habits previously established, that it 
was neither deliberate nor casual in its origin, but a natural 
growth, beginning neither in a clever experiment nor in a 
fortunate mistake of an individual worker ant, but the 
outcome of the community’s progressive development in 
‘intellectual somnambulism,” helped in some measure by 
the sluggish habits of the aphides. And, if you wish, the 
formula may be added, “ which was justified in the course 
of natural selection.” 

3. Storing—Not a few animals hide their prey or their 
gatherings, and with marvellous memory for localities 
return to them after a short time. But genuine storing 
for a more distant future is illustrated by the squirrels, which 
hide their treasures like misers. Many mice and other rodents 
do likewise, and in some cases the habit seems to become 
a sort of craze, so large are the supplies laid in against the 
winter’s scarcity. Very quaint are the sacred scarabees 
(Azeucus sacer), which roll balls of dung to their holes, and 
sometimes collect supplies at which they gnaw for a couple 
of weeks. Some ants (¢.g. Atta barbara) accumulate stores 
of grain, occasionally large enough to be worth robbing ; 
and there is no doubt that they are able to keep the seeds 
from germinating for a considerable time, while they stop 
the germination after it has begun by gnawing off plumule 
and radicle and drying the seeds afresh. Dr. M‘Cook’s 
account of the agricultural ant of Texas (Pogomyrmex 
barbatus) gives even more marvellous illustrations of 
farming habits, for these ants to a certain extent at least 
cultivate in front of their nests a kind of grass with a rice- 


CHAP. VII The Industries of Animals 121 


like seed. They cut off all other plants from their fields, 
and thus their crops flourish. 

But animals store for their offspring as well as for 
themselves. The habit is very characteristic of insects, 
and is the more interesting because the parents in many 
cases do not survive to see the rewards of their industry. 
Sometimes, indeed, there is no industry, for the stores of 
other insects may be utilised. Thus a little beetle (Sz¢arzs- 
muralis) enters the nest of a bee (Anthophora pilifera) and 
lays its eggs in the cells full of honey. More laudable are 
the burying-beetles (JVecrophorus), which unite in har- 
monious labour to bury the body of a mouse or a bird, 
which serves as a resting-place for ‘their eggs and as a 
larder for the larve. The Sfhex wasp makes burrows, 
in which there are many chambers. Each chamber con- 
tains an egg, and is also a larder, in which three or four 
crickets or other insects, paralysed by a sting in the nervous 
system, remain alive as fresh meat for the Sphex larva 
when that is hatched. After the Spex has caught and 
stung its cricket and brought it to the burrow, it enters 
at first alone, apparently to see if all is right within. 
That this is thoroughly habitual is evident from Fabre’s 
experiment. While the Spex was in the burrow, he stole 
away the paralysed cricket, and restored it after a little; 
yet the wasp always reconnoitred afresh, though the trick 
was played forty times in succession. Yet when he substi- 
tuted an unparalysed cricket for the paralysed one, 
the Sphex did not at once perceive what was amiss, but 
soon awoke to the gravity of the situation, and made a 
fierce onslaught on the recalcitrant victim. So it is not 
wholly the slave of habit. 

4. Making of Homes,—Houssay arranges the dwellings 
of animals in three sets——(a) those which are hollowed 
out in the earth or in wood; (4) those which are 
constructed of light materials often woven together; and 
(c) those which are built of clay or similar material. We 
may compare these to the caves, wigwams, and buildings 
in which men find homes. 

Burrows are simplest, but they may be complex in 


122 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


details. Those of the land-crabs (Gecarcinus), the wood- 
cutting bees (Xy/ocopa), the sand-martens, the marmots, the 
rabbit, the prairie dogs, illustrate this kind of dwelling in 
various degrees of perfection. 

The male stickleback (Gasterosteus) weaves and glues 


Fic. 27.—Swallows (Chelidonaria urbica) and their nest. (After Brehm.) 


the leaves and stems of water-plants ; the minutest mouse 
(Mus minutus) twines the leaves of rushes together ; the 
squirrel makes a rougher nest; the orang-utan and the 
chimpanzee construct shelters among the branches ; but the 
nests of many birds are by far the most perfect works of 
animal art. 

Of buildings, the swallows’ nests by the window, and the 


CHAP. VII The Industries of Animals 123 


paper houses which wasps construct, are well known; but we 
should not forget the architecture of the mason-bees, the 
great towers of the termites, and the lodges of the beavers. 

Perhaps I may be allowed to notice once again, what I 
have suggested in another chapter, that while many of the 
shelters which animals make are for the young rather than 
for the adults, the line of definition is not strict, and some 
which were nests to begin with have expanded into homes 
—an instance of a kind of evolution which is recognisable 
in many other cases. 

5. Movements,—But animals are active in other ways. 
All their ways of moving should be considered—the marvel- 


Fic. 28.—Flight of crested heron, ten images per second. (From Chambers’s 
Encyclop.; after Marey.) 


lous flight of birds and insects, the power of swimming 
and diving, the strange motion of serpents, the leap, the 
heavy tread, the swift gallop of Mammals. All their 
gambolings and playful frolics, their travels in search of 
food, and their migrations over land and sea, should be 
reckoned up. 

Most marvellous is the winged flight of birds. As a 
boat is borne along when the wind fills the sails, or when 
the oars strike the water, and as a swimmer beats the 
water with his hands, so the bird beating the air backwards 
with its wings is borne onward in swift flight. But the 
air is not so resistent as the water, and no bird can float in 
the air as a boat floats in the water. Thus the stroke has 


124 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


a downward as well as a backward direction. When there 
is more of the downward direction the bird rises, when there 
is more of the backward direction it speeds forward ; but 
usually the stroke is both downwards and backwards, for 
the lightest bird has to keep itself from falling as it flies. 
The hollowness and sponginess of many of the bones com- 
bine strength of material with lightness, and the balloon- 
like air-sacs connected with the lungs perhaps help the 
birds in rising from the ground; but, buoyant as many birds 
are, all have to keep themselves up by an effort. But the 
possibility of flight also depends upon the fact that the 
raising of the wing in preparation for each stroke can be 
accomplished with very little effort; the whole wing and 
its individual feathers are adjusted to present a maximum 
surface during the down-stroke, a minimum surface during 
the elevation of the wing. There are many different kinds of 
flight, which require special explanation—the fluttering of 
humming-birds, the soaring of the lark, the masterful 
hovering of the kestrel, the sailing of the albatross. The 
effortless sailing motion of many birds is comparable to 
that of a kite, “‘the weight of the bird corresponding to the 
tail of the kite;” it is possible only when there is wind or 
when great velocity has been previously attained. 


Fic. 29.—From St. John’s Wild Sports. 


PART FH 


THE POWERS OF LIFE 


CHAPTER VIII 
VITALITY 


1, The Task of Physiology—2. The Seat of Life—3. The Energy of 
Life—4. Cells, the Elements of Life—5. The Machinery of 
Life—6. Protoplasm—y. The Chemical Elements of Life—8. 
Growth—g. Origin of Life 


1. The Task of Physiology.—So far we have been 
considering the ways of living creatures, as they live and 
* move, feed and grow, love and fight; as they build their 
homes and tend their young. We shall now turn to 
the inner mysteries, and seek, so far as we may, to fathom 
the wisdom of the hidden parts. We shall describe the 
machinery—the means by which the forces of life cause 
those movements by which we recognise their presence. 

This study is called physiology; and the plan of our 
sketch of present knowledge will be as follows: We shall 
first try to realise what we mean by life; we shall then 
limit ourselves to the consideration of certain kinds of life, 
and attempt to make plain in what parts of all living 
creatures are the forces of life most active. Having done 
this, we shall describe the life processes of the simplest 
creatures, and then those of the higher animals. 

It is not easy to say clearly what we mean by life ; but 
we recognise as one of its characteristics the power of 
movement. 


126 The Study of Animal Life PART II 


Still, this gives no distinction between the blowing of 
wind and the life of man; but the other characteristics of 
life will be realised as we proceed in our analysis; it is 
certain that without movement there is no life. Further 
thought may lead us to define life as that ‘complex of 
forces which produces form.” Thus the star-like crystals 
of a snowflake, the diamond drops of dew, the over- 
shadowing mountains, would all be imaged in our 
minds as living, though of more lowly life than the 
lichens of the bare hill-tops, the grass of the plains, or man 
himself. We have no space here to trace the connections 
between such an idea and the beliefs of all simple peoples, 
and the inspirations of all poets, but the similarity is 
evident, and the usefulness in philosophy of such general- 
ised conceptions is great. 

But the physiology which we shall sketch here will be a 
narrower one; it will be confined to the life of plants and 
animals, and we shall attempt to show precisely how that 
life is separated from the life of the dust and of the air. 

2. The Seat of Life-——Now in what parts within the 
living body are the life forces most actively at work ? 

When we look at any living creature we are all too 
willing, even if the wonder of life stirs within us, to remain 
satisfied with a vague apprehension of a mystery. It is 
strange that so many generations of men passed away 
before any steps were taken towards a conception of the 
intimate material processes of life and growth and death. 
The moving train has been watched, but the engine and 
the stoker have been almost unnoticed. 

Let us consider the growth of atree. The outward manner 
of its growth we can observe, a few superficial details of its 
inner life we already know, and of this knowledge we may 
look for great expansion, but the ultimate processes of its 
life are still a complete mystery to us. 

The tree is alive, but is it all alive? Cut a stake from 
its heart and plant it in the ground; it will not grow, and 
shows no signs of life, but we are not puzzled; the tree, we 
think, can only live as a whole, and we know how easily 
most living things are killed by local injuries. But if we 


CHAP. VIIT Vitalety 124 


cut a stake from the outer part of the tree, leaving the 
bark on, and set it in the ground, it may happen that buds 
will appear, pushing through the bark, and stretching out 
into shoots. 

There is a mystery for us to begin with: some parts of 
a tree may have a life of their own. Indeed, we all know 
that gardeners do not rear geraniums and other plants from 
seeds, but from cuttings. Potatoes, as we know, will give 
origin to new plants, and even small parts of potatoes will 
do so. Roses are grafted into the stems of the wild brier, 
and in this. way two life-currents are mingled. We may 
remember, too, that all seeds are only parts that have 
become separated from the parent plant. We ourselves, 
formed in the darkness of the womb, were separated at 
birth from the mothers who bore us. 

Let us think of the seeds of plants for a little. Formed 
in the warmth and brightness of the summer sun, ripened in 
the glow of autumn, they fall to the ground, are carried 
hither and thither by trickling runlets of water, by the 
winds, by animals, and scattered over new pastures. 
Through the long chill of winter they remain asleep ; but 
not dead,—-slow preparation is being made for the new 
day. With the warm winds of spring—when the birds 
come back to us and sing their first songs of love and 
courtship—the countless buds of the woods, the gardens, 
and the hedgerows, all the seeds we sowed in the autumn, 
all the corn we scattered in the first hours of the new 
morning, awake; the buds burst, the tiny leaves unroll ; in 
the seeds there is a great activity,—the slender shoots 
stretch forth—-spring passes into summer—and we await the 
harvest. 

3. The Energy of Life.— What is the cause of this 
strength of life? How is it that in an acre of forest tons of 
solid matter are lifted high into the air, while the branches 
waving under the blue sky seem to enjoy the brightness of 
the sun after the gloom of winter? This assertion of the 
poets of the gladness of nature at the springtime is no mere 
wandering fancy, it is simple truth ; the intensity of life at 
that time is due entirely to the greater warmth of the air 


128 The Study of Animal Life PART II 


and increased brightness of the sun; and since there is no 
reason why we should not believe in a simple order of 
consciousness in all simple creatures, that consciousness 
would certainly become gladness with increased vigour of 
life, just as it is with ourselves. 

The energy of life, we say, is due to the energy reaching 
us from the sun; but how is the radiant energy of the ether 
used to place the growing shoots on the forest tops, and how 
is it transformed into the potential energy of wood and 
other substances? Our trains move by virtue of the 
energy stored in the coal; we burn that in a certain 
place and get expansive energy of steam, which by 
mechanical arrangements we convert into the moving 
energy of the engine; but where is the boiler, and 
where the machinery in the plant, by which the energy 
of the sun’s rays is transmuted into life? The partial 
answer to this riddle has been found. If we break a 
newly-grown branch we find the tissues filled with a watery 
slimy sap. If we open a bud we find the slimy stuff again; 
under the bark of trees we find it once more; it is within 
the tissues of a bulb, and in growing seed; indeed, in all 
those parts of a plant which are capable of independent 
life we always find what we may call for the moment 
this slimy sap; while in the hard inner parts of the tree, 
which we know can live no more, we find nothing of the 
kind,—such tissues are quite empty. Life, therefore, we 
find to have something to do with a certain substance, and 
this is the first step towards understanding the machinery 
we have spoken of; we have, as it were, found that the 
movement of the train is due to the engine, but we do not 
understand that engine. 

4. Cells, the Elements of Life.—Let us leave the trees 
now for a little, and turn to the simplest of all living 
creatures, which live in water and in damp places. They 
are so small that only a few of the larger ones can be seen 
as tiny specks moving about in the water in which they 
live. But they can be seen quite easily with a microscope. 
We find them to be little transparent drops of living 
matter. They are not really drops; many of them have 


CHAP. VIII Vitality 129 


distinct shapes, others constantly change their form. They 
move, indeed, by a kind of flowing ; one part of their body is 
pushed out and a part on the farther side drawn in. Some 
of these lowly creatures have skeletons or shells of lime or 
of flint. Great numbers of these shells, when the little 
inmates are dead, form beds of chalk and ooze. Now 
all living creatures begin life in this way; at first they 
are tiny masses of a jelly-like translucent stuff. Each 
mass gets a skin or surrounding wall; if fed, it grows 
larger, and a wall is built up inside it, making its house a 
two-roomed one. This process goes on and on; the whole 
mass grows larger and larger, and becomes divided up into 
a corresponding number of compartments. The chambers 
are not quite separated ; there are always holes left in the 
walls, through which strands of the jelly-like stuff pass, and 
so all of them are connected. The divisions in each 
separate kind of animal or plant take place in a special 
way, until’ at last the whole body is built up, with all its 
peculiarities of form and internal arrangement. The cells 
of an animal’s body do not, however, form walls as definitely 
as do those of plants. 

In an ordinary plant there are millions of those com- 
partments; they are called cells, from their likeness in 
general appearance to the cells of a honeycomb; and the 
enclosed stuff that we have spoken of as jelly is called 
protoplasm, because it is believed that the first living things 
that were formed were little drops of jelly-like stuff, not 
unlike that within the cells, or composing the animalculz in 
water. Protoplasm, wherever it occurs, from the highest 
to the lowest forms of life, is supposed to have, within 
certain limits, a similarity of nature. 

In some plants the cells are large enough to be visible 
‘to the naked eye, but the cells of most plants and animals 
are so small that they can only be seen with a microscope. 

We can now give a complete answer to the question, 
What parts of a tree are alive? It is only the protoplasm 
of the cells. The walls of the cells are more or less dead. 
As the cells grow older and larger, and the walls become 
thicker, the amount of protoplasm within gets relatively 

K 


130 The Study of Animal Life PART II 


less ; at last it slowly dies and withers away; the cells are 
left empty ; and that is why the stake cut from the old hard 
part in the middle of the tree could not grow,—it was quite 
dead. 

5. The Machinery of Life.— We have found that, in 
some way, the protoplasm within the cells is the machinery 
of life. For simplicity, we shall speak of protoplasm as 
living matter. This living matter in plants is such that 
it can transform the energy of-sunlight into potential energy 
of complicated substances such as wood. This trans- 
formation of energy is one of the chief labours of plants in 
the world. A great deal of the energy that reaches their 
living matter is used for their own upward growth ; so that, 
as we said before, thousands of tons of matter are every 
year, over every acre of forest, raised high into the air. 

In animals the living machinery is in certain ways of a 
different nature. An animal eats the substances made by 
the plant; the potential energy stored in these is used by it 
in moving about, and so transformed into energy of motion. 
The life of plants is chiefly shown in the storage of energy, 
the life of animals in the use of that store. Chiefly we say, 
for plants also move to a slight extent; as a whole, when 
they twine around a tree or bend towards the sun; and in 
their parts when the sap rises and falls. Animals also, to a 
slight extent, build up substances of high potential energy. 

So far all is certain, but when we inquire by what arrange- 
ment of parts the living matter is able to be a machine for 
the transformation of energy, we are unable to form any con- 
ception. Soon after the discovery of the cells and their living 
contents, certain philosophers, who must have very faintly 
realised the necessary physical conditions, arguing from the 
analogy of machines as men construct them, supposed that 
the activities of the living machinery could be deduced 
from the structural arrangements of the cells; they sup- 
posed that the living matter, a part within it called the 
nucleus, and the cell-wall, were in themselves the parts of 
a machine, and that the various activities of the cells were 
due to varying shapes of wall, and disposition of its visible 
parts. It was soon shown, however, that the wall was not 


CHAP. VII Vitality 131 


a necessary part of the living matter, and that the nucleus 
did not always occur. The cell is a machine, not in virtue 
of the disposition of its visible parts, but as a consequence 
of the arrangement of its molecules. We know this much 
about the living machinery, that it is far more perfect 
than the machinery of our steam-engines, the perfection of a 
machine being measured by the relation between the energy 
which enters it and that which leaves it as work done. 

“Joule pointed out that not only does an’ animal much 
more nearly resemble in its functions an electro-magnetic 
engine than it resembles a steam-engine, but also that it is 
a much more efficient engine; that is to say, an animal, 
for the same amount of potential energy of food or fuel 
supplied to it—call it fuel to compare it with other engines 
—gives you a larger amount converted into work than any 
engine which we can construct physically.” And Joly has 
expressed the contrast between an inanimate material 
system and an organism as follows: ‘“‘ While the transfer 
of energy into any inanimate material system is attended 
by effects retardative to the transfer and conducive to dis- 
sipation, the transfer of energy into any animate material 
system is attended by effects conducive to the transfer 
and retardative of dissipation.” 

It is from protoplasm that we must start in our study 
of living machinery; let us see how far we can attain to 
exact conceptions of its nature. We will first describe 
shortly what is known as to the structure of protoplasm o1 
living matter, chiefly to show how hopeless is any attempt 
at a solution of the problem in terms of visible structure. 
The powers of the microscope are limited by the physical 
nature of light, and that limit has already nearly been 
reached ; and yet we know the structure of matter is so ex- 
cessively minute that within the compass of the finest fibre 
visible with the microscope there is room for the most intri- 
cate structural arrangements. 

6. Protoplasm.—Protoplasm used commonly to be de- 
scribed as a structureless mass ; we now know that it often 
has a structure somewhat like a heap of network. It isa 
complex of finely-arranged strands, with knots or swellings 


132 The Study of Animal Life PART II 


at the junctions of the strands, and with, in each cell, one 
or more central and larger swellings, probably of a highly 
specialised nature, called nuclei. The size of the meshes 
varies, and they are filled “now with a fluid, now with a 
more solid substance, or with a finer and more delicate 
network, minute particles or granules of variable size being 
sometimes lodged in the open meshes, sometimes deposited 


Fic! 30-—Adjacent animal cells showing the nucleus, the protoplasmic network, 
and the bridges vitally uniting cell and cell across the intervening inter- 
cellular substance. (From the Zvolution of Sex ; after Pfitzner.) 


in the strands of the network. Sometimes, however, the 
network is so close, or the meshes filled up with material so 
identical in refractive power with the bars or films of the 
network, that the whole substance appears homogeneous.” 
The only means we have of getting any further knowledge 
of this arrangement is by staining it with various dyes, and 
observing the effects of the dyes upon the various parts. 
“ Analysis with various staining and other reagents leads to 


CHAP, VIII Vitality 133 


the conclusion that the substance of the network is of a 
different character from the substance filling up the meshes. 
Similar analysis shows that at times the bars or films of the 
network are not homogeneous, but composed of different 
kinds of stuff; yet even in these cases it is difficult, if not im- 
possible, to recognise any definite relation of the components 
to each other such as might deserve the name of structure.” 
Plainly there is not much light to be got by further investi- 
gations in this direction. Ordinary chemical analysis, too, 
is of little avail; for how can we say what parts of the 
mass are alive, even if we could separate part from part? Is 
it only the meshwork that is really living matter, or are the 
granules part of it, or are the fluid contents the chiefly vital 
substance ? 

Let us turn now to the activities of the living stuff, and 
see what we can learn from them. We have already spoken 
of one of the activities of living matter, especially of plant 
protoplasm, that of surrounding itself with a wall. Now we 
might at first be inclined to suppose that the wall was simply 
due to a hardening and drying of the soft substance at those 
places where it touched the air. It is possible that that may 
have been the stimulus which caused, as a reaction, the 
wall-making at the dawn of life, and which may still have 
some connection with it; but what we have to take note of 
is the fact that the walls, as they are made by the higher 
plants, have always a definite structure and chemical nature. 

If we examine the cells of the leaves of a plant growing 
in the sunlight, we find the green colouring matter to be 
generally collected in little rounded masses. Looking more 
closely, we find it to be the fluid which fills the meshwork 
of the masses. At certain points in the meshwork we find 
minute masses of starch constantly being formed. They seem 
to pass along the strands and collect in the centre of the net- 
work, until quite a large mass of starch is accumulated there. 
If we examine the plant at night, some time after darkness 
has set in, we find no traces of starch in the cells of the 
leaves. There is evidence that the starch has been trans- 
formed into sugar, and can then, by osmotic and perhaps 
by other processes, be removed from the leaves, and 


134 The Study of Animal Life PART 1 


carried by the vessels to all parts of the plant. So we get 
a’ first notion of how a plant is fed. Starch is a com- 
pound containing carbon and the elements of water. The 
carbon, we know, comes from the carbonic acid of the air; 
the water is absorbed by the roots from the soil. In some 
way the living matter of the cells, by means partly of the 

. green colouring matter, is able to transform the energy of the 
sun’s rays into potential energy of a combustible substance 
—starch ; so we get clear evidence of a machinery for the 
transformation of energy. We have taken a plant as our 
example throughout, partly because the cells are more 
evident than in animals, and partly because the chemical 
processes give evidence of a transformation of kinetic into 
potential energy more clearly than do those of animals; for 
the animals eat the plants, and so by using the potential 
energy of plant substance are able to live and move. 

We have now some idea of the sources of the energy of 
life. Plants get their food from the air by their leaves, and 
from the soil by their roots, which absorb water and salts 
dissolved in water. By aid of the energy of sunlight they 
build these up into complex substances, which they use for 
the growth of their living matter, for the formation of sup- 
porting structures, and for other purposes. Animals eat 
these substances. They build up their own bodies of living 
matter and supporting structures, and they move about. 

In order to get a clearer notion of the nature of living 
matter we must attempt to trace the manner in which these 
various substances are built up. We have first to discover 
what are the substances that are made. In all living 
creatures there are, in addition to water and salts, such as 
common salt and soda, three groups of stuffs :— 

Carbohydrates, such as starch and sugar, made of carbon 
with hydrogen and oxygen in the same proportions as they 
occur in water ; 

Fats—substances containing the same three elements, 
but with a smaller proportion of oxygen ; 

Proteids—substances containing always carbon, hydrogen, 
oxygen, and nitrogen, with a small percentage of sulphur. 

The constitution of proteids is difficult to determine. 


CHAP. VIII Vitality 135 


The above elements are always present, and in proportions 
which vary within narrow limits; but in addition to 
these substances there seem to be always present others 
which, when the proteids are burnt, remain as ash in the 
form of salts chiefly chlorides of sodium and potassium, but 
also small quantities of calcium, magnesium, and iron, as 
chlorides, phosphates, sulphates, and carbonates. The mole- 
cule of a proteid must be very complex ; thus that of albumen 
is, at its smallest, Cy... Hyg), Nog, Ogg, Sy3 most probably it 
is some multiple of this. 

The food-stuffs of plants, then, are salts, water, and car- 
bonic acid, and a certain amount of oxygen. Of these, by 
means of the sun’s energy, they build up complex substances 
—carbohydrates, fats, proteids, which, with salts, water, and 
oxygen, serve as the food of animals, The living matter, 
the machinery by which all this is done, is, if it can be 
classed at all, a proteid. But this only means that all 
living matter contains the five essential elements and some 
others which in the ash exist as salts. 

The various services which the different food-materials 
are set to within the body will be described later, when we 
are considering the details of the animal economy. Here 
we shall take note of the elements that enter into the 
construction of the food-stuffs. 

7. The Chemical Elements of Life.—There are sixty- 
eight elements to be found in varying abundance upon the 
earth, but by analysis of the food-stuffs and of living matter 
itself, we find that only twelve of these occur with any con- 
stancy in organisms. They are carbon, hydrogen, oxygen, 
nitrogen, sulphur, phosphorus, chlorine, potassium, sodium, 
calcium, magnesium, and iron. 

Now nine of these elements form sixty-four per cent by 
weight of the earth’s crust; while aluminium and silicon, 
substances that are only very occasionally found in living 
creatures, form thirty-five per cent, being the chief consti- 
tuents of quartz and felspar, sand and clay, in short the 
greater part of all rocks. All the other elements, three of 
which—hydrogen, nitrogen, and phosphorus—enter into 
life, form the remaining one per cent. 


136 The Study of Animal Life PART I] 


Since the ultimate analysis: of the objective side of life 
seems to show that life is to be pictured as matter in an 
unstable and constantly altering condition, it will be of 
interest to find the conditions that determine which of the 
elements are to take part in it. 

It seems that matter in order to enter into life must be— 

(1) Common, (2) mobile, that is capable of easily 
entering into solution or becoming gaseous, and (3) capable 
of forming many combinations with other elements. 

Nine of the elements fulfil the first condition ; a tenth, 
nitrogen, is the chief constituent of the atmosphere ; 
while hydrogen is present everywhere in water. Why do 
not aluminium and silicon take their share in life? Because 
they do not fulfil conditions (2) and (3). Their oxides are 
quartz and aluminia, two of the hardest substances known. 
Emery and ruby are two forms of aluminia ; while the oxide 
of carbon, the source of all the carbon used in life, is a gas. 

Carbon, which takes so great a part in life processes that 
the chemistry of organic substances is commonly spoken of 
as the chemistry of the compounds of carbon, fulfils all three 
requirements in an eminent degree. For although in its 
pure form a solid, and sometimes a very hard substance, yet 
it readily forms an oxide which is present in the atmosphere, 
and, as we know, serves as one of the chief foods of plants. 
Its power of entering into combination with other elements 
is practically infinite. Nitrogen, although by itself an inert 
form of matter, is able to combine with carbon compounds 
and add fresh complexity. 

It is easy to see why water is so important in life. It 
dissolves the other substances, and so allows them to come 
into closer contact, and to change in position more easily, 
than if they were solid. So the first stuff that was complex 
and unstable enough to be properly described as living was 
almost certainly formed in water, long ago, when the condi- 
tions of greater heat, and consequently greater mobility of 
all substances, made chemical changes more active. 

The importance of the solvent power of water in a com- 
plex organism is obvious when we think of the blood, the 
great food stream and drain. It is shown in an interesting 


CHAP. Vill Vitality 137 


way by the suspended animation of a dried seed, which will 
remain for years dormant, but ready when moistened to 
spring into active life. 

How, then, are these substances built up into living 
creatures? Let us, that we may see this matter clearly, 
think for a moment of the conditions of life of the simplest 
creatures, the formless masses of living matter. All that 
the simplest plants need is water holding oxygen, carbonic 
acid, and salts in solution. Out of these simple materials, 
by the magic touch of their living bodies, they can build up 


the complex matter of which those bodies are made; so that 
they can grow and divide until there may be hundreds in 
place of a single one. The image we must form of this 
increase of living matter is that step by step substances of 
an ever-growing complexity are made, one from the other, 
until at last a substance so unstable is made that it begins 
to break down into simpler forms of matter at the least 
deviation from the precise conditions in which it was made, 
and perhaps also with a ferment-like action causes changes 


138 The Study of Animal Life PART Il 


in all that it touches; and this we call the living matter. 
As this living matter breaks down into simpler substances, 
or as it causes surrounding substances to break down, 
energy is set free for use in movement. We may make 
a diagram of this process, The steps go up and down; the 
top one we call protoplasm (Fig. A). This shows only one 
line of ascent and one of descent. There may be many, all 
going on at the same time, as is shown in Fig. B. But 
these are much too simple ; they show continuous ascending 
and descending stairs, but each step does not really result 
directly from the one below, but must result from two or 
more stairs meeting ; and at each meeting there must be 
substances formed which are useless, and begin to break 
down, or are cast out of the system at once, 

8 Growth.—The power of growth, of adding to itself 
substance of the same nature as itself, is the real mystery 
of living matter. A crystal grows out of its solution, the 
star or pyramid is built up with perfect regularity, but the 
process is much simpler than the growth of living matter. 
The substance of which it will be formed is already there, 
but the protoplasm has to make its own substance as it 
grows. This is the true difference between the two pro- 

cesses, and not, as is usually stated, that a crystal grows by 
depositing matter on its surface, while a cell grows by putting 
matter within itself. For when two cells fuse, which often 
occurs, growth is really as much by aggregation as in the 
case of a crystal, and such manner of growth is made 
possible simply because the two cells are masses of matter 
of equal complexity. But when less complex matter is given 
to a cell it cannot add that matter to itself until it has been 
transformed into substance as complex as itself. This 
change can only be effected within the little laboratory of 
the cell itself’ The fact that the growth of a crystal may 
be endless, while that of a cell is limited, which is usually 
cited as the distinctive difference, is a consequence of the 
necessity of the protoplasm for forming its own substance 
within its own substance. For when a cell grows in size 
the ratio of its surface to its volume constantly decreases ; 
and therefore, since new material can only be absorbed 


CHAP. VIII Vitality 139 


through the surface, there must be a certain size of cell at 
which the rate of absorption is just sufficient for the nourish- 
ment of the protoplasm. Beyond this point a cell cannot 
grow; but if it divides, then the mass to be fed remains the 
same, while the absorbing surface is increased. This, then, 
is the necessitating cause of cell-division. But it would be 
unwise to suppose that there are not other causes that help 
to produce this result, which has as a consequence the 
possibility of immense variety of disposition of the daughter 
cells, and therefore of organic forms; for, to begin with, a 
more obvious means of obtaining increased surface would 
be for the cell merely to become flattened or to spread out 
irregularly, which, indeed, we see in many of the Protozoa. 

Since their growth implies cell-division as one of its con- 
sequences, and since cell-division is the basis of reproduc- 
tion, synonymous, indeed, in the Protozoa with reproduc- 
tion, we get the idea of successive generations of animals as 
merely the continued growth of former generations. This 
makes intelligible to us all the facts of heredity which are 
so surprising if we conceive of each generation as a number 
of untried souls that have left some former dwelling-place to 
come and live among us. Our children are, in truth, abso- 
lutely portions of ourselves. If this be so, we must imagine 
in the ovum—the tiny mass of protoplasm from which we 
are formed by continued: division—a most extraordinary 
subtlety of constitution. 

Try to picture the complexity of the arrangement of parts. 
There are two tiny masses of protoplasm ; so far as we can 
see they are the same, yet from one will grow a man, from 
the other a tree. If the germ that will grow into a human 
being could only properly be fed outside the body of the 
mother, so far as we know it might leave that body as 
an almost invisible cell, and would grow and divide, add 
cell to cell, until the creature was fully formed—sculp- 
tured out of dust and air. Our early life within the womb, 
our nourishment by the blood of our mother, is only nature’s 
way of preserving us from injury. What we shall be is 
already marked out before the egg begins to grow. 

It is only the highest animals who are thus shielded. 


140 The Study of Animal Life PART II 


The birds cover their eggs with their wings. The butterfly 
lays hers where the grubs will find their food. The star- 
fish cast theirs adrift in the sea. The same story is true 
of the flowers. They are the nursing mothers. In their 
heart the young plant grows until the first leaves appear ; 
not till then does it drop away, and not without food 
prepared and placed ready for use—enclosed in what we 
call the seed. But the seaweed, like the star-fish that 
crawls upon it, allows its young seeds quite unformed to be 
floated away by the tide. 

All seeds, then, are parts of the living matter of the 
parent ; some leave naked and without food; others are 
protected by shells or by husks which are filled with food ; 
others live within the mother until they have ceased really 
to be seeds, and are fully formed new creatures. 

Now the living matter of any simple organism is so much 
the same throughout the whole body that almost any part 
of it will do to build the new generation from. Thus, 
although the sea-anemone does sometimes set apart certain 
cells as seeds, yet any part of the body will, if cut off, grow 
into a complete creature. The same thing is true of a moss 
plant. But the more highly organised animals have their 
living matter set to such different service in their various 
organs that most of it does not keep all the qualities of the 
whole creature, but only of that part to which it belongs. 
Thus if a starfish lose its arm, another will grow from the 
stump. A snail can in this way repeatedly regrow its horns. 
Even so highly developed an animal as a lizard can grow a 
new tail. With ourselves this power is confined to growing 
new skin if we lose part of it, or mending a bone if it be 
broken, and other similar processes. 

When we clearly understand in what way the offspring 
of all creatures arise from their parents, how they are, as 
Erasmus Darwin said long ago, like separated buds, then 
we see the truth of the often-made comparison between any 
or all species of animals and an organism. The individuals 
of a species are not indeed bound together by protoplasmic 
strands, but their interdependence is not less complete. A 
single species utterly destroyed might modify the life of the 


CHAP. VIII Vitality 141 


whole earth. Life itself is dependent upon the invariable 
presence of minute quantities of iron in the soil. A wan- 
dering tribe of savages is an organism not quite so high in 
the scale of social organisms as is the hydra in the scale 
of individuals ; for the cells of the hydra, although divided 
broadly into an outer and an inner layer, are yet more 
divided in their functions than are the members of a 
savage tribe. For there are only two kinds of person in 
such a tribe—hunters and cooks; while a highly civilised 
community, with its immense variety of workmen, is prob- 
ably not so well organised as any mammal; for there are 
in such a state thousands of persons, untrained to any special 
labour, merely a burden to themselves and to the nation. 

9. Origin of Life——We have said that life probably 
began when the conditions of heat and solubility of sub- 
stance were more favourable to the formation of peculiar 
and complex matter than at present. But such a state- 
ment is often thought to be unphilosophical in view of the 
fact that we have at present no experience of the formation 
of such substances, and that it has been conclusively proved 
that living creatures always proceed from pre-existing life. 
But those who urge such objections forget that all that has 
been proved is, that the simplest creatures knowz to be alive 
at present can be formed only by cell-division one from 
another, and not from simple chemical materials. But we 
must remember that those simplest animals are highly 
developed in comparison with the complex matter from 
which we conceive life to have sprung; and no one 
would now expect that such comparatively highly developed 
animals could arise from simple matter. There is certainly 
no evidence of the formation at present of the very simplest 
and original living matter. But, in the first place, could 
we see it, even with a microscope, if it were to be formed ? 
Might it not be formed molecule by molecule? And, 
secondly, what chance of survival would such elementary 
creatures have among the voracious animals that swarm in 
all places where such simplest creatures might possibly be 
formed? Instantly they would be devoured, before they 
could grow large enough to be seen. 


142 The Study of Animal Life PART II 


Lastly, let it be carefully observed, such a belief as this 
as to the origin of life, and of the basis of all life in chemical 
processes, carries with it no necessary adherence to the 
doctrines of Materialism. The materialist analyses the 
whole objective world of phenomena into matter and motion. 
So far, his conclusion is perfectly legitimate ; but when he 
maintains that matter and motion are the only realities of 
the world, he is making an unwarrantable assumption. 
Matter in motion is accompanied by consciousness in our- 
selves. We infer a similar consciousness in creatures like 
ourselves. As the movements and the matter differ from 
those that occur within our body, so will the accompanying 
consciousness. The simplest state of affairs or “ body” 
we can imagine is that of a gas such as hydrogen. But 
such a simple state of matter may have its accompanying 
consciousness, as different from ours as is the structure of 
our bodies from that of a hydrogen molecule. This is, of 
course, also an assumption, but it is one that harmonises 
with the facts of experience. 

The opposite extreme to Materialism is Idealism, and in 
this school of philosophy an assumption precisely similar, 
and exactly opposite to that of Materialism, is made. The 
idealist says the objective world of phenomena has no exist- 
ence at all, it is the creation of mind. An objection to 
such a theory lies in the question, If matter and energy are 
the creation of mind, how is it that we find them to be 
indestructible ? 

Popular philosophy has made an assumption which lies 
midway between these extremes. It postulates two reali- 
ties, matter and spirit, having little effect upon one another, 
but acting harmoniously together. 

But the view that is here set forth postulates neither 
matter nor spirit, but an entity which is known objectively 
as matter and energy, and subjectively as consciousness. 
This philosophy goes by the name of Monism. The term 
consciousness is used for lack of any other to express the 
constant subjective reality. Carefully speaking, it is, of 
course, only the more complex subjective processes that 
form consciousness. 


CHAPTER IX 
THE DIVIDED LABOURS OF THE BODY 


1. Division of Labour—2. The Functions of the Body: Movement ; 
Nutrition ; Digestion ; Absorption ; The Work of the Liver 
and the Kidneys; Respiration ; Circulation; The Changes 
within the Cells; The Activities of the Nervous System— 
3. Sketch of Psychology 


1. Division of Labour,—The simplest animals are one- 
celled; the higher animals are built up of numberless cells. 
All the processes of life go on within a single cell. In a 
many-celled animal the labours of life are divided among 
the various groups of cells which form tissues and organs. 
The history of physiological development is the history of 
this division of labour. 

When a dividing cell, instead of separating into two 
distinct masses, remained, after the division of its nucleus, 
with the two daughter masses lying side by side, joined 
together by strands of protoplasm, then the evolution of 
organic form took a distinct step upwards, and at the same 
time arose the possibility of greater activity, by means of 
the division of labour. For when the process had resulted 
in the formation of an organism of a few dozen cells, 
arranged very likely in the form of a cup, the outer cells 
might devote the greater part of their energies to movement 
and the inner cells to the digestion of food. In the com- 
mon Hydra the body consists of two layers of cells arranged 
to form a tube, the mouth of which is encircled by tentacles. 


144 The Study of Animal Life PART II 


The cells of the outer layer are protective, nervous, and 
muscular ; the cells of the inner layer are digestive and 
muscular. The cells of Hydra are therefore not so many- 
sided in function as are Amcebe. In animals higher than 
the simplest worms, a middle layer of cells is always formed 
which discharges muscular, supporting, and other functions. 

With advancing complexity of structure the specialisa- 
tion of certain cells for the performance of certain functions 
has become more pronounced. In the human body the 
division of labour has reached a state of great perfection ; 
we shall give a slight sketch of its arrangements. 

2. The Functions of the Body.—Our objective life 
consists of movement, and of feeding to supply the energy 
for that movement. Growth, reproduction, and decay are 
elsewhere treated of. 

Movement.—We move by the contraction of cells massed 
into tissues called muscles. Contractility is a property of 
all living matter ; in muscle-cells this function is predomi- 
nant. This is all that need be said here of movement ; the 
processes of nutrition we must follow more closely. 

Nutrition.—All the cells of our bodies are nourished by 
the stream of fluid food-stuff, the blood, which flows in a 
number of vessels called arteries, veins, or capillaries, 
according to their place in the system. From this stream 
each cell picks out its food; and into another stream— 
the lymph stream—moving in separate channels—the 
lymphatics, which, however, join the blood channels, each 
cell casts its waste material; just as a single-celled animal 
takes food from the water in which it lives and casts its 
waste into it. 

Nutrition must therefore consist of two series of activi- 
ties. One series will have for its object the preparation 
of food-matter so that it may enter the blood, and the 
excretion of waste products out of the blood. The other 
series will consist of the activities of the individual cells, — 
the manner in which they feed themselves. 

The first step in the preparation of the blood is digestion. 
Most food-stuff is solid and indiffusible ; before it can enter 
the blood it must be made soluble and diffusible. The 


cuap. 1x The Divided Labours of the Body 145 


supply of oxygen to the tissues is also a part of these first 
processes of nutrition. Being a gas, it is treated in a 
special way which will be described immediately. 

Digestion.—The various food-stuffs have various chemi- 
cal qualities. After being swallowed they enter a long 
tube, the digestive tract or alimentary canal. Within 
this canal they are subjected to the action of various 
digestive juices prepared by masses of cells called glands. 
Saliva is one of these juices, gastric juice is another, 
pancreatic juice is another. The effect of these juices 
upon the food is that most of it is dissolved in the juice 
and made diffusible. Thus we see an example of the 
division of labour. An amceba flows round a solid particle 
of food and digests it. In the higher animals the cells of 
the digestive glands are specialised for this particular func- 
tion and do little else. 

Absorption.—After the food is digested it leaves the 
alimentary canal, and is absorbed into the blood-vessels 
and lymphatics in the walls of the canal. Absorption is not 
a mere process of diffusion. It is diffusion modified by the 
cells lining the alimentary tract. Certain chemical changes 
are effected at the same time. Most of the absorbed food 
passes to the liver; but the fat does not go directly into 
the blood, being first absorbed into that other system of 
vessels, the lymphatics. Eventually it also gets into the 
blood ; for the two streams are connected. 

The Work of the Liver and the Kidneys.—The cells 
of the liver secrete a juice called bile, which is poured into 
the alimentary canal. The exact function of this juice is 
still doubtful. It has a certain use in the digestion of fats, 
but it is largely an excretion. The stream of food-stuff 
going to the liver contains sugar, the result of the digestion 
of carbohydrates ; albumen, the result of the digestion and 
absorption of proteids; and certain waste nitrogenous 
matters formed during the digestion of proteids. 

The cells of the liver retain the sugar, store it within 
themselves, in the same sort of way that a potato stores up 
starch, and give it up gradually to the blood again. So 
far as is known they do not affect the albumen in any way, 

L 


146 The Study of Animal Life PART II 


but the waste nitrogenous matter is altered and then sent 
on in the blood stream to the kidneys. 

The cells of the kidneys take this stuff, which was pre- 
pared in the liver, and other waste nitrogenous products 
out of the blood, pass them and a certain amount of water 
along to the urinary bladder, which empties itself from time 
to time. 

Respiration.—Breathing consists of two distinct acts, 
inspiration and expiration. During an inspiration air is 
drawn into lungs. Thence the oxygen passes by diffusion, 
modified by the fact that the essential membrane is a living 
one, into the blood. There it enters into a loose combina- 
tion with hemoglobin, the red colouring matter of the 
blood cells, and is thus carried to the cells of the tissues to 
be absorbed into their living matter. During an expiration 
we breathe out air which has less oxygen and more carbonic 
acid gas than normal air. The carbonic acid is a waste- 
product formed by the cells of the body ; it first enters the 
blood, is then carried to the lungs, and leaves the blood- 
vessels by a process of diffusion similar to that by which 
the oxygen entered. The close association of these two 
processes is simply due to the fact that an organ fitted for 
the diffusion of one gas in one direction will do for the 
diffusion of all gases in any direction. 

Circulation.—The blood is maintained in a healthy state 
by the processes we have described. By the active con- 
traction of the heart it is pumped round and round the 
body, continually carrying with it fresh food to the tissues, 
and carrying away with it the waste matter cast out of the 
tissues. All the blood-vessels, except the very smallest, 
have muscular walls. The heart is a large hollow mass of 
muscles, is a part of a pair of large blood-vessels that have 
been bent upon themselves, and arranged so as to form 
four separate chambers, two upper and two lower, an upper 
and a lower opening directly into one another on each side. 
By the contraction of the lower chamber of the left pair the 
blood is forced through all the vessels of the body; these 
collect and empty the blood into the upper chamber of the 
right pair; from this it passes into the lower chamber on 


cuar. ix Lhe Divided Labours of the Body 147 


the same side, and from this it is forced through the vessels 
of the lungs, returning to the upper chamber of the left 
side, and so to the lower chamber of the left side. 

The way in which the blood is able to nourish the 
tissues is as follows :__The outgoing vessels—arteries—enter 
each mass of tissue; within it they break up into number- 
less very small, very thin-walled vessels—capillaries ; the 
blood oozes through these into the small spaces—lymph 
spaces—that occur throughout the tissues ; adjacent to these 
spaces are the cells, which take from the lymph—the fluid 
that fills the small spaces and the vessels connected with 
them—what they need, and cast into it their waste. The 
lymph spaces open into lymph vessels, which, as has been 
noted, join the blood-vessels. Oxygen and carbonic acid, 
being gases, pass directly from the blood through the walls 
of the vessels to the tissues, and from the tissues to the 
blood. 

The Changes within the Cells.—In speaking of proto- 
plasm an outline of the kind of knowledge that we possess 
of the chemical changes that take place within the cells 
has been given. We know little more than the sub- 
stances that enter the cells and the substances that leave 
them. 

Perhaps even this is too much to say ; more exactly, we 
know the substances that enter the body by the mouth and 
nose, and through the alimentary canal and lungs; we 
know the substances that leave the body through the 
kidneys, and, in expiration, through the nose. A large 
amount of water and traces of other matters leave the body 
as perspiration ; but the chief use of sweating is probably 
the regulation of the temperature of the body, and the skin 
should not be thought of as an excreting organ in the same 
way that the kidneys are. The undigested matter that 
passes from the food-canal has never been within the 
blood, and does not therefore concern us in this inquiry. 

But we know very little more than this; the analysis of 
the precise changes that any particular mass of tissue exerts 
upon the blood—z.e. the differences that must exist between 
the substances entering it and the substances leaving it—is 


148 The Study of Animat Life PART II 


very difficult of determination, because of the immense 
quantity of blood that passes through any tissue in a short 
time. This concludes our sketch of the interchange of 
matter within the body. 

The Activities of the Nervous System.—We have now 
to consider the arrangement of the nervous system—first 
merely as the means by which all the varied activities of 
the tissues of the separate parts of the body are co-ordi- 
nated and wrought into an harmonious series of actions, 
and then as the associate of consciousness and of mental 
processes. 

Just as protoplasm may be called the physical basis of 
life, so is nervous tissue Jar excellence the physical basis of 
consciousness and of mind, Throughout the whole animal 
kingdom it has a superficial similarity of structure, and 
consists of the same three parts. 

(1) First there are cells adapted to receive notice of 
change in the outer world. Changes in the sur- 
rounding medium and affecting such cells are 
called stimuli. These cells sensitive to stimuli 
form the chief part of the sense organs — the 
eyes, ears, nose, tongue. Also in the skin 
there are cells sensitive to alterations of touch 
and temperature. The effect of stimuli upon 
such cells is probably to set them into a state of 
molecular agitation, which may or may not result 
in chemical changes. 

(2) There are connecting fibres or nerves, which, 
being connected with the sensory cells, take up 
the vibrations or possibly the chemical changes 
of the sensory cells and transmit them to the 
“centre.” 

(3) There are “central” cells, in which the nerves 
end, and which are set in molecular agitation by 
the vibrations of the nerves. This molecular 
agitation is often, when the central cells are in 
the brain, accompanied by consciousness. Ap- 
parently also agitations may arise ‘“ spontane- 
ously” within these central cells and stimulate 


cuar. 1x The Divided Labours of the Body 149 


the outgoing nerves, and cause muscular move 
ments, or the activity of glands, or other cellular 
activities. 

In many ways analogous to the nervous system is 
the telegraph system of a country; the receiving stations 
are the nerve cells, among which are the cells of the sense 
organs ; the connecting wires are the nerve fibres; the 
central stations are the groups of cells called ganglia, the 
chief of which are in the brain and spinal cord. The less 
important ganglia are like the branch offices, they receive 
messages and transmit them unaltered; the higher ganglia 
are like the offices of a government, in which messages are 
received, plans elaborated on the strength of the news, and 
orders sent out to various parts of the country. All such 
actions when they take place in the nervous system are 
called reflex actions, whether a received message be sent 
on unaltered, or whether the receiving cell regulates the 
transmission according to the needs of the parts of the 
body. The analogy of telegraph stations, even with the 
living clerk to work them and with responsible persons to 
direct ‘the clerk, does not give a much too complex idea of 
the activities of nerve cells. 

3. Sketch of Psychology.—The following is largely 
derived from Professor Lloyd Morgan’s Andmal Life and 
Intelligence, to which we refer the reader, and to which we 
acknowledge our indebtedness. 

It very often happens that changes in nervous matter, 
caused by changes in the outer world, result in what we 
call a change of consciousness, 

Consciousness is the subjective side of molecular dis- 
turbance in brain or other nerve matter. 

Changes in the outer world which cause disturbance of 
nerve matter are called stimuli. 

Changes of consciousness produced by stimuli are called 
impressions. 

If impressions left no permanent trace in conscious- 
ness behind them, there would be no such thing as mind. 
For mind is based upon memory, and memory is the re- 
vival of past impressions, which, we must suppose, have in 


150 The Study of Animal Life PART 11 


some way been stored within the cells of the brain in the 
form, from the objective point of view, of a certain 
arrangement of its particles. 
When, after the revival of past impressions, we are able 
to discriminate between them, we call them sensations. 
Now sensations are referred, in consciousness, not to 


Fic. 31.—Attitude of a hen protecting her brood against a dog 
(From Darwin's Lafression of Emotions.) 


the brain cells which discriminate between them, but to the 
cells of the sense organs which received them. 

Further, we refer, by experience, the causes of sen- 
sations to the outer world. We do this by a mental pro- 
cess which is called perception. 

Now out of perceptions, and through associations, there 
arise expectations. The mental process involved in the 
formation of an expectation is called inference. 


cuap. 1x The Divided Labours of the Body 1§1 


Inference is of two kinds : 

(1) Perceptual, drawn from direct experience, as in 
the inference as to a rain storm from a black 
cloud. 

(2) Conceptual, which, though based upon experience, 
yet can predict events that have never been 
experienced. For instance, one who had studied 
in books only the causes of volcanic activity, 
might predict with a certain amount of confidence 
a flow of lava from a volcano, when he saw it in 
that state of activity which he knew usually pre- 
ceded an eruption of lava. 

What we call the emotions, love, hate, fear, and others, 
are, so far as we can tell, agitations of nervous matter 
which affect consciousness. Their exciting stimuli—infer- 
ences for example—proceed, immediately, from within the 
brain, ultimately, from changes in the outer world. 

We have, therefore, the following orders of conscious- 
ness, which are easily distinguishable in theory : 

(1) Impressions, or the effects of environmental 
changes upon nervous matter; the retaining and 
revival of these constitutes the basis of memory. 

(2) Sensations, which occur when the differences that 
exist between impressions are discriminated. 

(3) Perceptions, which are the outward projections 
into the world, by mental acts, of the molecular 
disturbances caused in the brain by environ- 
mental changes. For example, light falls upon 
the'retina, stimulates the optic nerve, and causes 
a molecular disturbance in the brain, but the 
consciousness excited in us is not of the brain 
disturbance, but of the light. 

It is most essential that the distinction between per- 
ceptual and conceptual inference be clearly realised, as it is 
probable that it is the faculty for the latter which more 
than anything else separates man from the lower animals. 
We may be nearly certain that many animals exercise per- 
ceptual inference, and we may affirm with little doubt that 
none has ever performed a’conceptual one. It has been 


152 The Siudy of Animal Life PART Il 


stated that a monkey that had learned to screw and 
unscrew a handle from a broom had learned “ the principle 
of the screw.” This is entirely erroneous. The monkey 
merely observed that a certain movement given to the 
handle caused it to separate from the broom, and inferred 
perceptually that the same result would always follow from 
the same action. 

It is evident that a sound comparative psychology of 
the animal kingdom, or even of a few of the highest 
species, is beyond the present possibilities of science. 


CHAPTER X 
INSTINCT 


1. General Usage of the Term—2. Careful Usage of the Term— 
3. Examples of Instinct—4. The Origin of Instinct 


# 

In considering the mental life of animals, we must settle 
how far it is comparable to that of man. ,We judge of the 
mental processes of human beings, other than ourselves, 
from their actions; and we can only do the same when. 
dealing with animals. If we often err in inferring the 
mental states of our fellow-men, how much more are we liable 
to error when we are considering creatures different from 
ourselves. Still, believing as we do in the continuity of 
life, both objective and subjective, by careful proceeding it 
is probable that in time we may arrive at a certain state 
of precision and exactness in comparative psychology. 

Since the idea that is formed of the world is gained 
entirely from sensations, the world of every creature must 
be largely constructed from its dominant sense ; in a dog, 
for instance, from scent. 

In common speech the actions of animals are all ascribed 
to instinct. The notion which underlies the term is, that 
while the actions of men are determined by reason, those of 
animals are prompted by a blind power of doing that which 
is fitted to the successful conduct of their lives. This, as 
we shall see, is a notion that requires modification. 

1. General Usage of the term Instinct,—Every one has 
a general notion of what is meant by instinct, but few are 


154 The Study of Animal Life PART II 


agreed as to the precise usage of the word; thus when the 
birds build their nests, or when the bees collect honey and 
form their combs, their acts are with one accord said to be 
Instinctive ; but some would demur at using such a term to 
describe the love of parents for their children, the courage 
of brave men, or the artist’s perception of beauty. But, 
even supposing we agree to mean by instinct all those actions 
which are neither simply reflex nor purely rational, there 
will still remain great difference of opinion as to its origin. 
Thus the love of parents will not be imagined as due to 
practice, either in the individual or its ancestors, but rather 
to take origin in some hidden necessity of nature; while 
the rapid closure of the eyes as protection from an expected 
blow would seem in all likelihood to have begun in a rapid 
exercise of intelligence, which, by being often repeated, 
had ceased to be accompanied by conscious effort. 

It seems to us that there is still need of a vast amount 
of observation and experiment before a theory of the origin 
of instinct that will be at all satisfactory can be framed. As 
already remarked, it is not easy to decide even in what 
sense the term ought to be used. This being so, we shall 
content ourselves with mapping the field of thought and 
indicating the lines of inquiry that must be followed before 
a just view of the subject will be possible. 

If we arrange examples of all the movements of animals 
in the order in which they are performed in the lifetime of 
the individuals, not limiting ourselves to those acts which 
involve the whole organism, but considering also those which 
a single organ or mass of tissue may execute, we shall see 
at a glance all the possible varieties of activity with which 
we can be concerned. It is, of course, only the move- 
ments of comparatively large masses of tissue with which 
we can deal at present. The molecular movements which 
lie at the base of all the visible ones are as yet almost 
unknown. 

Even before birth, visible movements of the parts of the 
higher animals occur ; as, for instance, the beating of the 
heart. Such movements may be either “ automatic” or 
reflex. At birth, in addition to such movements of its parts, 


CHAP. X LInstinet 155 


the organism acts as a whole ; it reacts to its environment, 
and in time performs “ voluntary ” actions. 

The acts of the parts of an organism may be— 

(1) ‘‘ Automatic,” as, for example, the beating of the 
heart. 

(2) Reflex, as, for example, the intestinal movements 
which force the food through the alimentary 
canal, or the movements involved in sneezing. 

(3) Mixed actions which are partly automatic and 
partly reflex, such as the respiratory movements. 

The movements of the entire organism may be of a very 
complex nature. They may be— 

(1) Reflex; as when we start at a sudden noise. 

Si “ Tnnate,” commonly called instinctive ; these are 
best observed in newly-born animals, for in them 
intelligence, which must be based upon experience, 
is necessarily at a minimum. 

(3) “ Habitual,” such as are rapidly learned and are 
then performed without mental effort, which imply 
an innate capacity, and are therefore allied to (2). 

(4) Intelligent, such as imply mental activity, which 
consists in the combining and rearranging all the 
other possible acts of the order—(1), (2), or (3) 
and which may be recognised in all adaptations 
to novel circumstances. 

This classification possesses most obvious faults, but it 
has certain advantages. It reveals some of the difficulties 
that delay the would-be definer of instinct. For the essen- 
tial criterion of an instinctive action is that all the machinery 
for its performance, as a reflex to a certain stimulus, lies 
ready formed within the organism; but the apparently in- 
soluble questions present themselves, How soon may not 
actions be modified by intelligence ? and How in a mature 
animal with considerable experience is one to separate 
the purely instinctive acts from the intelligently modified 
instinctive acts ? 

Also it is evident that “habitual” actions may be “‘instinct- 
ive” actions deferred until the creature be further devel- 
oped, as the flight of many birds is deferred ; or they may 


156 The Study of Animal Life PART I 


be actions in the formation of which intelligence has had a 
considerable share. 

Now all these ‘activities of an entire organism may be 
studied from four points of view :— 

(1) Of natural history, or general description, such as 
occurs here and there throughout this work : 

(2) Of physiology, or the analysis of the muscular, 
nervous, and other mechanisms involved; as 
treated generally in the last chapter : 

(3) Of psychology, or the investigation into the states 
of consciousness and mental processes concerned ; 
as sketched in the last chapter : 

(4) Of etiology, or study of the factors in their origin 
and development. 

We shall first define more carefully than we have yet 
done what we shall speak of as instinct, then give a few 
examples, and finally discuss the zetiology of it. 

2. The Careful Usage of the term Instinct,—We have 
enumerated all the possible varieties of action, and the pos- 
sible states of consciousness with which they may be asso- 
ciated have been described in the last chapter. If we retain 
the use of the term instinct we must explain to what order 
of activity we shall apply it. In our use of the term we 
shall not strive after any great precision ; for, as already 
noted, the difficulty of precision seems to us to be at present 
insurmountable. In a general way we shall call any action 
which does not require for its execution any immediate 
exertion of perceptual inference an instinctive action, Thus 
a burned child dreads the fire; such dread and its conse- 
quent avoidance of fires may, with propriety, be termed 
instinctive. After the first burn the avoidance will, for a 
short time, be the result of perceptual inference; but in 
perhaps a few days only the avoidance becomes “ instinct- 
ive”; or it might be called “ habitual,” as hinted previously. 
It is, of course, to be understood that an “instinctive” 
action is not necessarily the result of this “lapsed intelli- 
gence,” as it has been called. Thus, when a worm 
wriggles away from a fire it probably has not at any time 
reasoned out to itself the advantages of such procedure, 


CHAP. X Lnstinet 157 


yet it may well be said to avoid the fire instinctively. It is 
obvious that, if we agree to use the term as defined, we 
must call all the actions of the lower animals, whose con- 
sciousness has never risen to the level of perceptual infer- 
ence, instinctive. This definition is based upon the assump- 
tion that we can determine the conscious states of animals ; 
but, as we have repeatedly said, it is only within very wide 
limits that we can with any certainty do this. The inten- 
tion, however, is to preclude all those actions which are 
certainly or probably rational, and at the same time include 
adaptive reflex actions. 

Mr. Herbert Spencer has defined instinct as compound 
reflex action, while Mr. Romanes separates it from reflex 
action and from reason as follows :— 

“Reflex action is non-mental neuro-muscular adjustment 
due to the inherited mechanism of the nervous system, 
which is found to respond to particular and often-recurring 
stimuli, by giving rise to particular movements of an 
adaptive though not of an intentional kind.” 

“Instinct is reflex action into which there is imported 
the element of consciousness. The term is therefore a 
generic one, comprising all those faculties of mind which 
are concerned in consciousness and adaptive action, 
antecedent to individual experience, without necessary 
knowledge of the relation between means employed and 
ends attained, but similarly performed under similar and 
frequently-recurring circumstances by all the individuals of 
the same species.” 

“Reason or intelligence is the faculty which is con- 
cerned in the intentional adaptation of means to ends. It 
therefore implies the conscious knowledge of the relation 
between means employed and ends attained, and may be 
exercised in adaptation to circumstances novel alike to the 
experience of the individual and that of the species.” 

Mr. Romanes therefore separates reflex action from 
instinctive action by limiting the term instinct to those 
actions which are, as a matter of fact, conscious reflexes. 
His definition is open to objection on the same ground that 
ours is, only in a greater degree ; for it is easier to deter- 


158 The Study of Animal Life PART Il 


mine the presence of perceptual inference than the absence 
of consciousness; this criterion may be of theoretical 
interest,—it is of no practical use. The other attributes he 
enumerates should be carefully studied. 

Prof. Lloyd Morgan also separates, but by no hard-and- 
fast line, the automatic and reflex actions, which are 
reactions to definite stimuli, from instinctive actions, which, 
according to him, are “ sequences of co-ordinated activities, 
performed by the individual in common with all the mem- 
bers of the same more or less restricted group, in adaptation 
to certain circumstances, oft recurring or essential to the 
continuance of the species.” 

He separates these from intelligent actions, which are 
‘performed in special adaptation to special circumstances.” 

Instinctive activities he conceives to be performed 
“ without learning or practice.” Ifthe actions need a little 
practice he calls them “incomplete instincts” ; ifa great deal 
of practice be necessary they are called “habitual activities” ; 
if they are not perfectly developed at birth but after further 
development can be performed without practice they may be 
called ‘‘ deferred instincts.” A further useful classification 
of instincts is into “perfect ” and “ imperfect,” according to 
the precision of their adaptation to the desired end. 

Mr. Lloyd Morgan’s definition, like the others, implies that 
one can separate rational from non-rational actions ; but he 
safeguards himself by defining instincts as “ oft-recurring or 
essential to the continuance of the species,” in contra- 
distinction to intelligent actions which are performed in 
special adaptation to special circumstances, It is important 
to notice that the terms of the definition are that instincts 
are either oft-recurring or essential, and not oft-recurring 
and essential, for many instincts are only either one or the 
other and not both. But it is not always possible to say of 
a certain action that it isa special adaptation to a special 
circumstance, and is therefore rational, and not in reality 
an instinctive adaptation to circumstances that are of frequent 
occurrence although we have not observed them to be so. 

This definition, however, emphasises the fact that instincts 
are common to species ; it is, however, not easy to estimate 


CHAP. X Instinct 159 


the exact significance of this fact, for the apparent similarity 
in the actions of individuals of the same species must, to a 
certain extent, be due to incompleteness of observation. 

It is after considering all these definitions that we have 
come to the conclusion that it is convenient to describe all 
those actions of animals which are not immediately rational 
or intelligent as instincts. If we classify an instinct as 
reflex in cases where the exact chain of internal events is 
known and use the other qualifications already enumerated, 
we reach a simplicity and precision of speech that is 
convenient. 

At the same time all such criteria as adaptiveness and 
similarity of performance in all the individuals of a species 
can obviously be applied as they are discovered. 

The essential distinction, we believe, between non- 
intelligent, that is, instinctive, and intelligent actions, is 
that non-intelligent actions are performed in virtue of the 
innate and co-ordinated mechanisms of the organism, 
whereas for an intelligent action the organism has to do a 
greater or less amount of the co-ordination for itself. 

3. Examples of Instinct.—If we classify all the actions 
of animals according to the period of life at which they are 
performed, we shall find that there are three distinct classes 
of action which may with convenience be considered 
separately. 

They are— 

(1) Those which are performed at birth, or shortly 
afterwards, as perfectly, or nearly so, as at any 
future time ; 

(2) Those more varied actions which are characteristic 
of the mature life of any animal : 

(3) Those which are associated with reproduction. 

The first of these classes must evidently consist of very 
pure instincts, since the creature cannot be supposed to 
reason before it has any store of experience. 

The second class is well typified by the marvellous 
actions of insects, such as ants, bees, and wasps, These 
may be instinctive, but it is very probable that many of 
them are, at least, improved by intelligence. 


160 The Study of Animal Life PART II 


(a) They may be perfected by perceptual inferences on 
the part of the individuals, and the mental efforts may or 
may not, after a certain number of repetitions, be replaced 
by reflexes. 

(4) They may be perfected by less complex mental 
efforts, such as those involved in imitation or in receiving 
instruction from other members of the species. 

Actions of the third class may be as purely instinctive 
as any of those in the first class, or may be improved by 
intelligence like those of the second class; but among 
them are many of the most wonderful performances of 
animals, for they often seem to show a prevision of an 
unknown future. 

Some interesting experiments have been made upon 
instincts of the first class. The observations show that the 
precision of the neuro-muscular co-ordinations of some 
newly-born creatures is very surprising. Mr. Spalding 
blindfolded some chickens immediately after they were 
hatched, and removed the hood after two or three days 
when they were stronger. He says that “almost invariably 
they seemed a little stunned by the light, remained motion- 
less for several minutes, and continued for some time less 
active than before they were unhooded. Their behaviour, 
however, was in every case conclusive against the theory 
that the perceptions of distance and direction by the eye 
are the result of experience, or of associations formed in the 
history of each individual life.” 

“Often at the end of two minutes they followed with 
their eyes the movements of crawling insects, turning their 
heads with all the precision of an old fowl. In from two to 
fifteen minutes they pecked at some speck or insect, show- 
ing not merely an instinctive perception of distance, but an 
original ability to judge, to measure distance, with some- 
thing like infallible accuracy. They did not attempt to 
seize things beyond their reach, as babies are said to grasp 
at the moon, and they may be said to have invariably hit 
the objects at which they struck, they never missed by a 
hair’s-breadth, and that too when the specks at which they 
aimed were no bigger and less visible than the smallest dot 


CHAP. X Instinct 161 


of anz. To seize between the points of the mandibles at 
the very instant of striking, seemed a more difficult opera- 
tion. I have seen a chicken seize and swallow an insect at 
the first attempt; most frequently, however, they struck 
five or six times, lifting once or twice before they succeeded 
in swallowing their first food.” Again, “The art of scrap- 
ing in search of food, which, if anything, might be acquired 
by imitation, for a hen with chickens spends the half of her 
time in scratching for them, is nevertheless another indis- 
putable case of instinct. Without any opportunities of 
imitation, when kept quite isolated from their kind, chickens 
began to scrape when from two to six days old. Generally 
the condition of the ground was suggestive; but I have 
several times seen the first attempt, which consists of a sort 
of nervous dance, made on a smooth table.” Another 
experimenter “hatched out some chickens on a carpet, 
where he kept them for several days. They showed no 
inclination to scrape, because the stimulus supplied by the 
carpet to the soles of their feet was of too novel a character 
to call into action the hereditary instinct ; but when a little 
gravel was sprinkled on the carpet, and so the appropriate 
or customary stimulus supplied, the chickens immediately 
began their scraping movements.” 

Another instance of the first class of instincts is the fear 
said to be shown by many animals for their natural foes ; 
but on this point we find a certain conflict of evidence. 
Thus kittens are said to show disgust at a dog, and, while 
still blind, at a hand that has touched and smells of a dog, or 
to tremble with excitement at the smell of a mouse. A chicken 
or young turkey will show evident signs of fear at hearing 
the cry of ahawk. Ants of various species that are mutually 
hostile recognise an enemy, and fight; but, on the other 
hand, there are observations to the effect that, if taken young 
enough, ants of several such species may be brought up 
together as a happy family. 

The instinctive tameness or wildness of many animals 
towards man is probably the effect of intelligence and infor- 
mation given to one another; as is the avoidance of the 
same kind of trap, after a short experience of its properties, by 

M 


162 The Study of Animal Life PART Il 


all the mice of a house or birds of a district. The wild rabbit 
is extremely timid, but the domesticated variety is as tame 
as possible. In explanation of such cases we might easily 
invoke the aid of “the principle of cessation of natural selec- 
tion” when the safety of the species ceased to depend upon 
wildness, but we prefer to suppose that the direct action 
of intelligence is to a great extent operative. 

As already noted, what are perhaps the most striking 
examples of instinct of the second class occur among in- 
sects. The comb-building of bees, the wars, the slave-using, 
the agricultural pursuits of ants, have been so often de- 
scribed that they need not detain us here. The brain of an 
ant was to Darwin the most wonderful piece of matter in 
the world. Wonderful, indeed, it would be if we supposed 
that all the acts of an ant were truly instinctive, that is, 
that the nervous machinery of co-ordination was ready, 
waiting only the appropriate stimulus to evoke any one of 
that series of nicely-adjusted actions. But if we suppose 
that individual intelligence has a considerable share in that 
co-ordination, then the brain of an ant, though still very 
wonderful, is not to us quite so astounding: an arrangement 
of particles as it was to Darwin. 

The third class of instincts, those connected with 
reproduction, comprise such actions as the building of 
homes and nests, the storage of food for the use of 
young that may never be seen, and the care of young after 
birth. 

The nest-building of birds would form a very good sub- 
ject upon which to experiment, in order to determine how 
far such a complex act may be truly instinctive, and how 
far it is perfected by training, by imitation, and by intelli- 
gent practice and observation. The method would be to 
isolate young birds from their parents and from all other 
creatures of their kind, so as to preclude training and 
imitation, and then see how far the nests that they 
built resembled the typical nest of their species. Then one 
might remove other birds from their parents but allow them 
the society of the members of an allied species, but one 
whose nests differed to some observable extent from those 


CHAP. X Instinct 163 


of the species under experiment. So far as we know, no 
adequate experiments have been carried out. 

It is stated that the nests of British birds let loose in 
Australia differ very greatly from the nests that they would 
build at home. Now this may be due to the absence of 
training and the possibilities of imitating the specific nest, 
or it may be due to the absence of the materials with which 
to build the characteristic nest. 

When we consider the careful provision that the Spher 
wasp makes for its young, young which she will never see ; 
or the selection by a butterfly of the leaf upon which she 
lays her eggs, the only sort of leaf, it may be, that will serve 
the grubs, when hatched, for food, food of a kind which the 
butterfly herself does not eat, we have before us examples 
of instincts most wonderful in their perfection and most 
obscure in their origin. The ideas involved are too com- 
plex for us to believe that the actions can be the result of 
intelligence. So far as we can see at present, such 
instincts can only be accounted for by the Natural Selection 
of fortunate varieties of habit. The care of young and the 
habit of incubation are instincts upon which a certain 
amount of thought has been bestowed. For ourselves, we 
incline to the idea which may seém mystical to some, that 
such habits are born of an inevitable affection for what is so 
nearly related to the very body of the parents. To escape 
an explanation of such habits in terms of affection, certain 
naturalists have suggested the demonstrably absurd notion 
that birds sit upon their eggs in order to cool a fevered 
breast. If such were her object, the very worst place that 
a bird could select as her seat would be a woolly hairy 
nest containing eggs which, in virtue of internal chemical 
changes, generate heat of themselves. 

4. The Origin of Instinct,—The theory of the evolu- 
tion of instinct has been worked at by Darwin, and since his 
day.by various other authors, notably Mr. Romanes, Mr. 
A. R. Wallace, and Professor Eimer; while Professor 
Weismann’s doctrines have necessitated the revision of 
certain plausible hypotheses. 

Darwin, of course, supposed that Natural Selection 


164 The Study of Animal Life PART <I 


was the means of the evolution of habit as much as of 
form. 

Mr. Romanes, starting from this as a basis, has con- 
structed a well-reasoned and lucid theory. He supposes that 
while many instincts have been evolved by Natural Selection, 
such instincts being called Primary, other habits become 
instinctive through the “lapse of intelligence.” Actions 
performed at first with mental effort, becoming after suffi- 
cient repetition so ingrained upon the nervous system that 
a mechanism of neuro-muscular co-ordination has been 
established, are referred to as Secondary Instincts. He 
imagines also a third class of Mixed Instincts in which 
there are primary instincts that have been altered and 
improved by intelligent variations of habit, or secondary 
instincts that have been modified by natural selection. 

Obviously, therefore, he supposes that intelligence may 
be a factor in the formation of any habit that may be under 
consideration. 

But this theory of instinct becomes impossible if we accept 
Professor Weismann’s doctrine that acquired characters can 
not be inherited (this doctrine will be discussed in a later 
chapter). If this be true, the only possibilities are primary 
instincts, and secondary instincts formed afresh during each 
individual lifetime, and mixed instincts of the same nature, 

The exact antithesis to Professor Weismann’s theory is 
upheld by Professor Eimer, who believes that instincts have 
been evolved chiefly by the perpetuation of what Mr. 
Romanes calls secondary instincts. There is little evidence 
that this is the case. The value of Eimer’s work really lies 
in his insistence upon the intelligent action of animals as 
apart from purely instinctive action. 

Mr. Wallace has begun the analysis of the particular 
forms which the intelligence of animals takes. He supposes 
that imitation of parents and other members of the species 
has a great influence upon the actions of individuals. He 
has dwelt especially upon such cases as the song and nest- 
building of birds. 

It may be pointed out as a matter for consideration that, 
granted that parents teach their offspring, as, for instance, 


CHAP. X Instinct 165 


birds teach their fledglings to fly, and ants their young their 
place in the community of the nest, and that animals imi- 
tate cach other, it is quite possible, and indeed probable, 
that an instinct may be steadily improved throughout suc- 


Fic. 32.—Young ducks catching moths. (From St. John’s Wild Sports.) 


cessive generations by the intelligence of the individuals of 
a species, without any acquired character being inherited. 

The possible factors in the evolution of instinct are 
therefore— 

(1) Natural Selection, which might develop innate 
capacity; this is certainly insufficient for the 
development of form, and therefore, probably, also 
of mind. 


166 The Study of Animal Life PART 


(2) Individual intelligence, which directly modifies the 
actions of individuals, and is also used when, by 
imitation and education, the habits of a species 
are steadily improved. 

(3) The “‘lapsing of intelligence,” forming “ second- 
ary,” and helping to form “ mixed ” instincts. 

But the probable factors are the first two. 


PART I] 


THE FORMS OF ANIMAL LIFE 


CHAPTER XI 
THE ELEMENTS OF STRUCTURE 


1. The Resemblances and Contrasts between Plants and Animals— 
2. The Relation of the simplest Animals to those which are 
more complex—3. The Parts of the Animal Body: Organs, 
Tissues, Cells 


THE study of form and structure (Morphology), and the 
study of habit and function (Physiology), are both as essen- 
tial to science as the realities are in life. It is with the 
forms of animal life and their structure that we have now 
to do, but it seems useful at the outset to compare plants 
and animals. 

1. The Resemblances and Contrasts between Plants 
and Animals,—Every one could point out some differences 
between a tree and a horse, but many might be puzzled to 
distinguish clearly between a sponge and a mushroom, 
while all have to confess their inability to draw a firm line 
between the simplest plants and the simplest animals. For 
the tree of life is double like the letter V, with divergent 
branches, the ends of which, represented, let us say, by 
a daisy and a bird, are far apart, while the bases gradually 
approach and unite in a common root. 

Plants and animals are alike, though not equally, alive. 


168 The Study of Animal Life PART II 


Diverse as are the styles of animal and vegetable archi- 
tecture, the materials are virtually the same, and the 
individuals in both cases grow from equally simple 
beginnings. 

Even movement, the chief characteristic of animals, 
occurs commonly, though in a less degree, among plants. 
Young shoots move round in leisurely circles, twining 
stems and tendrils bend and bow as they climb, leaves 
rise and sink, flowers open and close with the growing 
and waning light of day. Tendrils twine round the 
lightest threads, the leaves of the sensitive plant respond 
to a gentle touch, the tentacles of the sundew and 
the hairs of the fly-trap compare well with the sensitive 
structures of many animals. The stamens of not a few 
flowers move when jostled by the legs of insects, and the 
stigma of the musk closes on the pollen. 

Plants and animals alike consist of cells or unit masses 
of living matter. The structure of the cell and the apparent 
structure of the living stuff is much the same in both. We 
may liken plants and animals to two analogous manufac- 
tories, both very complex; we study the raw materials 
which pass in, many of the stages and by-products of 
manufacture, and the waste which is laid aside or thrown 
out, but in neither case can we enter the secret room where 
the mystery of the process is hidden. 

In the pond we find the eggs of water-snails and water- 
insects attached to the floating leaves of plants; in the 
ditches in spring we see in many places the abundant 
spawn of frogs and toads; we are familiar with the heavily 
yolk-laden eggs of birds. Now, with a little care it is quite 
easy to convince ourselves that an egg or ovum is to begin 
with a simple mass of matter, in part, at least, alive, and that 
by division after division the egg gives rise to a young animal. 
We are also well aware that in most cases the egg-cell, for 
cell it is, only begins to divide after it has been penetrated, 
and in some subtle way stimulated, by a male unit or sperm. 
The great facts of individual history or development then 
are, the apparent simplicity in the beginning, the pre- 
liminary condition that the egg-cell be united with a male 


CHAP, XI The Elements of Structure 169 


unit, and the mode of growth by repeated division of the 
ovum and its daughter-cells. In those plants with which 
we are most familiar, the facts seem different, for we watch 
bean and oak growing from seeds which, instead of being 
simple units, are very complex structures. But the seed is 
not the beginning of a plant, it has already a long history 
behind it, and when that history is traced back to the seed- 
box and possible seeds of the parent plant, there it will be 
seen that the beginning of the future herb or tree is a single 
cell. This is the equivalent of the animal ovum, and, like 
it, begins its course of repeated divisions after it has been 
joined by a kernel or nucleus from the pollen grain. 

Thus, to sum up, along three different paths we reach 
the same conclusion, that there is a fundamental unity 
between plants and animals. In the essential activities 
of their life, in the stones and mortar of their structure, 
and lastly, in the way in which each individual begins and 
grows, there is a real unity. 

Yet, after all, plants and animals ave very different. 
The two kinds of organisms may be ranked as two great 
branches ‘of one tree of life, yet the branches diverge 
widely and bear different foliage. The facts of divergence 
and diversity are as undeniable as the inseparable unity of 
the basal trunk and the genuine sameness of life throughout 
the whole tree. I have stated the chief contrasts between 
plants and animals in a tabulated summary— 


aaissed Apjuvurwoperd are pue “10a 
use} x9 10 UOT}OW UT Adsaua 91371] AfaATyered 
-woo puadxe “(pioe oruoqied jo) sisonp 
-a1 Alpeoyswejoeieys are Aayy {synis-pooy 
xajdwoo assay} jo Adioua yeouayo [erjusjod 
ay) OUT IYStUNs jo ABroua StyaUTy oY} 719A 
-u0o Aay} Ssaoueysqns xapduoo ojur [elayeur 
-pooy ajduns Ajjesrways ‘apnio dn ping Asyy, 


aarjoe ApueU 
-Twopaad are pur ‘ssastptxo Aypeonstajoeieyo 
aie Aayy {340M ][eule}xa pue UOTIOUIOIOT 
ur AS1aua oNaury oyur AZisua peyusjod s1y} 
yraauoo Aayy {speurue sz9y30 Aq 10 sjuejd Aq 
dn paysiom Apvarye [elsajyeul-pooy asin Aayy, 


ANoqry] JO uoIsTATp 


sso] yonta aseiaae ue uo jqQIYXe s]]99 sy 


, onsprajorsey9 
SI S][99 ay} Suoue noqey Jo uoIstAtp parse] 


S][99 paxeu ysvay ye ouIT & 
toy savy sjurjd ajduns auiog 


yoreys 0} pare Aypeorurayo [ersreqeur v ‘asoy 
“Nyaa Aq ul payjem oie s]jao yuauoduios ayy, 


asopny[ao 
jo 9013 Aue MOYs Jaaou JsouY pur ‘aouTIs 
-qns-[]20 ey} Woz JarayIp A[qesysuoulap 
way} aavy Ajarer ‘s[jem-]]a9 ajtuyep Aaa 
ou asey UazO YIYA s[jao jo ysIsuod Aay Ty, 


Io sjainbs-eas aatssed aq] jo 
apiyNd JO o1unz ay3 JO sow 
SULIO} puke ‘suvlIosnjuy auros 
Ul INd90 0} SUeas asO[N]a*) | 


sueIpIosy | 
\ 
- 


WAydoroyyo ou savy 
joaysered ouios pue 1gung 


saoueysqns xatdwoo dn Surpying ur pue 
(usshxe jo wonviaqy WIM) plow oruoqieo 
aionpar ur 3qZuns yo ASiaua ay} sesipan 
Joyeur Sura ey3 yoy Jo pre Aq yuaursid 
usaiZ ayy ‘Y[Aydosopyo ssassod Ayofew ayy, 


yAydosopyo Aue Ajarer Aras aavy Aa, 


uaeai3-yjAqdoioyyqa 
quam [eoruapr 10 0} snod 
-oreue Ajasopo Aaa quowsid 
usaisg aay (‘O70 ‘wepey 
ayy ‘asuods 197eMyse1yy 947 
‘eozojo1g auios ~5°7) May: 


uOHT3NU ATaYr 
ur yeuorjdeoxe jred ut are 
soyiseivd oulos pue ‘13uny 
‘sjuzjd snoioatuies ‘uesy 


syonpoid-ajsem snouasosjiu Jo pit 
ya3 jou op Aayy, “[los 9y} jo sayeiqu ayq3 
Ajjersedsa ‘spunoduioo snouagoinu afduits 
wo uasoijtu aysinbax ay} ureyqo Aayy 


syonpoid-sjsem snouad 
-OI]IU JO PIT 39S 0} UMOUY are Ulay2 JO Iso 
“suistuesi0 sayjo Aq opeu ‘sprourmmnae 
uvy} Jajduis jou spunoduroo snouasos1U 
woi wagon oystnber ayy urejqo Aaqy 


squryd 
SAIL peay o1 a]qe A;qeqoad 
are vozojoig auios ‘ulesy 


Ajddns 
uoqivd jo saommos 13410 
puy sayisered autos pue 
‘TZung ‘sjuejd snoroarured 


AayeM JO MWe ay} ur sed plow stu0q 
-1ed UWlOTZ UOGIeD ayIsInbat ay} Ule}qO ABYT 


4 sjeurtue 
qoqio Aq 30 syueyd Aq spew ‘o39 ‘sey AeSns 
‘qoreys Worf uOgi¥d ayIsInbar ayy ure}qo Ady T, 


op sjuejd se plow stu0qie9 
asIIN 0} a]qe aq 03 Ueas 


(939) Boz0J01g Ue213 sUIOS 


poo} ayqnyjos quosqe AayT, 


Poo} Pros ssay Io axoUr uO paay AaqT, 


: qaosqe Aydurts 
sayiseied pu voz0j}01g awi0s 


SNOILCHIXY ANOS 


SLINVTd AO SOILSINSLOVAVHD 


STIVWINY 4O SOILSIMHLIVAVHD 


SNOILdSIXA ANOS 


CHAP. XI The Elements of Structure 171 


The net result of this contrast is that animals are more 
active than plants. Life slumbers in the plant; it wakes 
and works in the animal. The changes associated with the 
living matter of an animal are seemingly more intense and 
rapid ; the ratio of disruptive, power-expending changes to 
constructive power-accumulating changes is greater; most 
animals live more nearly up to their income than most 
plants do. They live on richer food; they take the pounds 
which plants have accumulated in pence, and spend them. 
Of course plants also expend energy, but for the most part 
within their own bodies; they neither toil nor spin. They 
stoop to conquer the elements of the inorganic world, but 
have comparatively little power of moving or feeling. They 
are more conservative and miserly than the liberally spend- 
thrift animals, and it is possible that some of the most 
characteristic possessions of plants, e.g. cellulose, may be 
chemical expressions of a marked preponderance of con- 
structive and up-building vital processes. It is enough, 
however, if we have to some extent realised the common- 
places that plants and animals live the same sort of life, 
but that the animals are on an average more active and 
wide-awake than the plants. 

2. The Relation of the Simplest Animals to those 
which are more Complex. From the pond-water catch in 
a glass tube one of the small animals, suppose it be a tiny 
water-flea or a minute “‘ worm”; how does it differ from one 
of the simplest animals, such as an Infusorian? It consists of 
many units of living matter instead of only one. The con- 
trast is like that between an egg and the bird which is 
hatched from within it. The simplest animals are single 
cells, all the others from sponge to man are many-celled. 
The Protozoa are units; all others—the Metazoa—are 
composite aggregates of units, or cities of cells. 

Compare the life of one of the Protozoa with that of 
a worm, a frog, or a bird. Both are alive, both may be 
seen moving, shrinking away from what is hurtful, drawing 
near.to what is useful, engulfing food, and getting rid of 
refuse. Both are breathing, for carbonic acid will poison 
them, and dearth of oxygen will kill them; both grow and 


172 The Study of Animal Life PART II 


multiply. But in the single-celled Protozoon all the pro- 
cesses of life occur within a unit mass of living matter. In 
the many-celled Metazoon the various processes occur at 
different parts of the body, are discharged by special sets of 
cells, among which the labour of life has been divided. The 
life of the Protozoon is like that of a one-roomed house 
which is at once kitchen and work-room, nursery and coal- 
cellar. The life of the Metazoon is like that of a mansion 
where there are special rooms for diverse purposes. 

In having no “body” the Protozoa are to some extent 
relieved from the necessity of death, Within the compass 
of a single cell they perform a crowd of functions, but tear 
and wear are often made good again, the units have great 
power of self-recuperation. They may, indeed, be crushed 
to powder, and they lead no charmed life safe from the 
appetite of higher forms. But these are violent deaths. 
What Weismann and others have insisted on is that 
the unicellular Protozoa, in natural conditions, need never 
die a natural death, being in that sense immortal. It 
is true that a Protozoon may multiply by dividing into two 
or more parts, but only a sort of metaphysical individuality 
is thus lost, and there is nothing left to bury. We would 
not, however, give much prominence to a strange idea of 
this kind. For the “immortality of the Protozoa” is little 
more than a verbal quibble; it amounts to saying that our 
common idea of death, as a change which makes a living 
body a corpse, is hardly applicable to the unit organisms. 
I believe, moreover, that the idea has been exaggerated ; 
for instance, the Protozoa in the open sea, in their natural 
conditions, seem to die in large numbers, 

The combination of all the vital activities within the 
compass of a single-cell involves a very complex life within 
the unit,—not more complex than the entire life of a many- 
celled animal, but fuller than that of one of its component 
cells. While a Protozoon is relatively simple in structure, 
its life of crowded functions, such as moving, digesting, 
breathing, is exceedingly complex. The simpler an organism 
is in structure the more difficult will it be to study its separate 
functions. Physiological or functional simplicity is in inverse 


CHAP, XI The Elements of Structure 173 


ratio to structural or morphological simplicity. Thus the 
physiologist makes most progress when he seeks to under- 
stand animals with many parts, for there he can find a large 
number of units, all as it were working at one task. The 
life of a Protozoon is more manifold and complex than that 
of any unit from a higher animal, just as the daily life of 
the savage—at once hunter, shepherd, warrior—is more 
varied than ours. 

Already it has been recognised that every many-celled 
animal begins its life as a single cell,—as an egg-cell with 
which a male element has united. Every Metazoon begins 
its life as a Protozoon, no matter how large the animal, 
for the whales arise from ova ‘‘no larger than fern-seed,” 
no matter how lofty the result, for man himself has to begin 
his life at the literal beginning. The fertilised egg-cell 
divides and re-divides, its daughter-cells also divide, the 
resultant units are arranged in layers, clubbed together to 
form tissues, compacted to form young organs, and the 
result is such a multicellular body as we possess; but while 
this body-making proceeds, certain units are kept apart, in 
some way insulated from the process of growth, to form the 
future reproductive elements, which, freed from the adult 
body, will begin a new generation. Back to the beginning 
again every Metazoon has to go, and if we believe that the 
Protozoa are not only the simplest, but also represent. the first 
animals, we have here the first and perhaps most important 
illustration of the fact that in its development the individual 
more or less recapitulates the history of the race. The 
simplest animals are directly comparable with the repro- 
ductive cells of higher animals, but the divided cells of the 
ovum remain clubbed together to form a young animal, while 
the daughter-cells of a Protozoon separate from one another, 
each as a new life. 

The gulf between the single-celled and many-celled 
animals is a deep one, but it has been bridged. Otherwise 
we should not exist. Traces of the bridge now remain in 
what are called ‘colonial Protozoa,” which, however trouble- 
some to those who like crisp distinctions, are most instruc- 
tive to those who would appreciate the continuity of the 


174 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


tree of life. These exceptional Protozoa are loose colonies 
of cells, descendants or daughter-cells of a parent unit, 
which have remained persistently associated instead of 
going free with the usual individualism of Protozoa. They 
illustrate to some minds a primitive co-operation of cells ; 
they show us how the Metazoa or multicellular animals may 
have arisen. 

3. The Parts of the Animal Body.—The physiologist 
investigates life or activity at different levels, passing from 
his study of the animal as a unity with habits and a tem- 
perament, to consider it as an engine of organs, a web of 
tissues, a city of cells, or finally as a whirlpool of living 
matter. So the morphologist investigates the form of the 
intact animal, then in succession its organs, their component 
tissues, the minuter elements or cells, and finally the struc- 
ture of the living stuff itself. Moreover, as there is no real 
difference between studying a corpse and a fossil, the pale- 
ontologist is also among the students of morphology; and 
most of embryology consists of studies of structure at dif- 
ferent stages in the animal’s life-history. 

The outer form of normal animals seems to be always 
artistically harmonious. It has a certain hardly definable 
crystalline perfection which pleases our eyes, but those who 
have not already perceived this will not see much meaning 
in the assertion, nor in Samuel Butler’s opinion that “ form 
is mind made manifest in flesh through action.” 


‘* [ believe a leaf of grass is no less than the journey-work of the 

stars, 

And the pismire is equally perfect, and the grain of sand, and 
the egg of the wren, 

And the tree-toad is a chef-d’ceuvre for the highest, 

And the running blackberry would adorn the parlours of heaven, 

And the narrowest hinge in my hand puts to scorn all machinery, 

And the cow crunching with depressed head surpasses any statue, 

And a mouse is miracle enough to stagger sextillions of infidels!” 

WaLT WHITMAN, 


It is also important to think of the different kinds of 
symmetry, how for instance the radiating sea-anemones and 
jellyfishes, which are the same all round, differ markedly 


CHAP, XI The Elements of Structure 175 


from bilaterally symmetrical worms, lobsters, fishes, and 
most other animals. Then there is the difference between 
unsegmented animals which are all one piece (like the 
lower worms and the molluscs), and those whose bodies 
consist, as in earthworm and crayfish, of a series of more or 
less similar rings or segments, due to conditions of growth 
of which we know almost nothing. 

Organs are well-defined parts, such as limb or liver, heart 
or brain, in which there is a predominance of one or a few 
kinds of vital activity. Gradually, alike in the individual 
and in the race, do they take form and function. There is 
contractility before there are definite contractile organs or 
muscles; there is diffuse sensitiveness before there are 
defined nerves or sense-organs. The progress of structure, 
alike in the individual and in the race, is from simplicity 
to complexity, as the progress of function is from homo- 
geneous diffuseness to heterogeneous specialisation. The 
two great kinds of progress may be illustrated by contrasting 
a sea-anemone and a bird. The higher animal has more 
numerous parts or organs, the division of labour within its 
body has brought about more differentiation of structure, 
but it is also a more perfect unity, its parts are more 
thoroughly knit together and harmonised. There is pro- 
gress in integration as well as in differentiation. 


“‘ The shoulder-girdle of the skate,” W. K. Parker says, ‘‘ may be 
compared to a clay model in its first stages, or to the heavy oaken 
furniture of our forefathers that stood ponderous and fixed by its own 
massy weight. As we ascend the vertebrate scale, the mass becomes 
more elegant, more subdivided, and more metamorphosed, until, 
in the bird class and among mammals, these parts form the frame- 
work of limbs than which nothing can be imagined more agile or 
more apt. So also as regards the sternum ; at first a mere outcrop 
of the feebly developed costal arches in the amphibia, it becomes 
the keystone of perfect arches in the true reptiles, then the fulcrum 
of exquisitely constructed organs of flight in the bird; and lastly, 
forms the mobile front wall of the heaving chest of the highest 
vertebrate.” 


Of the order in which organs appear or have appeared 
we can say little. The simplest sponges and polypes are 


176 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


little more than two-layered cups of cells, the cavity of the 
cup being the primitive food-canal. A parallel stage 
occurs in the early life-history of most animals, when the 
embryo has the form of a two-layered sac of cells, or is in 
technical language a gastrula. Both in the racial and 
individual life-history the formation of this primitive food- 
canal occurs very early. But it is not certain that it—the 
primitive stomach—was not at a still earlier stage an in- 
ternal brood-cavity ! 

But instead of speculating about this, let us seek to 
understand what is meant by the correlation of organs. 
Certain parts of the body stand or fall together, they are 
physiologically knit, they have been evolved in company. 
Thus heart and lungs, muscles and nerves, are closely 
correlated. Sometimes it is obvious why two or three 
structures should be thus connected, for it is of the very 
essence of an organism that its parts are members one of 
another. In other cases the reason of the connection is 
obscure. 

When organs either in the same or in different animals 
have a similar origin, and are built up on the same funda- 
mental plan, they are called homologous. Those whose 
resemblance is merely that they have similar functions are 
termed analogous. Even Aristotle recognised that some 
structures apparently different were fundamentally the 
same, and no small part of the progress of morphology has 
consisted in the recognition of homologies. Thus it was a 
great step when Goethe and others showed that the sepals, 
petals, stamens, and carpels of a flower were really modified 
leaves, or when Savigny discerned that the three pairs of 
jaws beside an insect’s mouth were really modified legs. 
To Owen the precision of our conceptions in regard to 
homologies is in great part due, though subsequent studies 
in development have added welcome corroboration to many 
of the comparisons which formerly were based solely on the 
results of anatomy. Thus an organ derived from the outer 
embryonic layer cannot be homologous with one derived 
from the innermost stratum of embryonic cells. Homo- 
logous organs in one animal are well illustrated by the 


CHAP. XI The Elements of Structure 177 


nineteen pairs of appendages borne by a crayfish or lobster. 
These differ greatly in form and in function; many of them 
are not analogous with their neighbours, one feels and 
another bites, one seizes and another swims, but they are 
all homologous. So are the different forms of fore-limb, 
the pectoral fin of a fish, the fore-leg of a frog or lizard, the 
wing of a bird, the flipper of a whale, the fore-leg of a tiger, 
the arm of man. But the wing of an insect is merely 
analogous not homologous with that of a bird, while the 
wings of bats and birds are both analogous and homo- 
logous. 


Fic. 33-—Bones of the wing in pigeon (A), bat (B), extinct pterodactyl (C). 
(From Chambers’s Zncyclop.) 


Change of Function.— Organs are not mechanisms rigidly 
adapted for only one purpose. In most cases they have a 
main function and several subsidiary functions, and changes 
may take place in organs by the occasional predominance 
of a subsidiary function over the original primary one. 
Thus the swim- or air-bladder which grows out dorsally 
from the food-canal of most fishes, seems usually to be a 
hydrostatic organ; in a few cases it helps slightly in 
respiration, but in the double-breathing mud-fishes or 
Dipnoi it has become a genuine lung. An unimportant 
(allantoic) bladder at the hind end of the gut in frogs, is 
represented in the embryos of reptiles and birds by a very 
important respiratory (and sometimes yolk-absorbing) birth- 

N 


178 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


robe, and in almost all mammals by part of the placenta 
which unites mother and unborn offspring. 

Substitution of Organs.—To the embryologist Kleinen- 
berg we owe a suggestive conception of organic change, 
which he speaks of as the development of organs by sub- 
stitution: An organ may supply the stimulus and the 
necessary condition for another which gradually supersedes 
and replaces it. In the simplest backboned animals, such 
as the lancelet, there is a supporting gristly rod along the 
back; among fishes the same rod or notochord is largely 
replaced by a backbone; in yet higher Vertebrates the 
adults have almost no notochord, its replacement by the 
backbone is almost complete. So in the individual life- 
history, all vertebrate embryos have a notochord to begin 
with; in the lancelet and some others this is retained 
throughout life, in higher forms it is temporary and serves 
as a scaffolding around which, from a thoroughly distinct 
embryological origin, the backbone develops, What is the 
relation between these two structures——notochord and 
backbone? According to Kleinenberg, the notochord 
supplies the necessary stimulus or condition for the 
development of the backbone which replaces it. 

Rudimentary Organs. —(a) Through some ingrained 
defect it sometimes happens that an organ does not 
develop perfectly. The heart, the brain, the eye may be 
spoilt in the making. Such cases are illustrations of 
arrested development. (d) A parasitic crustacean, such as 
the Saccudina which. shelters beneath the tail of a crab, 
begins life with many equipments such as legs, food-canal, 
eye, and brain, which are afterwards entirely or nearly 
lost ; the sedentary adult sea-squirt or ascidian has lost the 
tail, the notochord, the spinal cord which its free-swimming 
tadpole-like larva possessed. Such cases are illustrations 
of degeneration. In these instances the retrogression is 
demonstrable in each lifetime, in other cases we have to 
compare the animal with its ancestral ideal. Thus there 
are many cave-animals whose eyes are always blind and 
abortive. The little kiwi of New Zealand has only apologies 
for wings. We need have no hesitation in calling these 


CHAP. XI The Elements of Structure 179 


animals degenerate in eyes and fore-limbs respectively. 
(c) But somewhat different are such structures as the 
following: The embryonic gill-clefts of reptiles, birds, and 
mammals, which have no respiratory significance, or the 
embryonic teeth of whalebone whales, of some parrots and 
turtles, which in no case come to anything. They are 
vestigial structures, which are partly explained on the 
assumption, justified also in other ways, that the ancestors 
of reptiles, birds, and mammals used the gill-clefts as fishes 
and tadpoles do, that the ancestors of whalebone whales, 
birds, and turtles had functional teeth. No one can say 
with certainty of vestigial structures that they are entirely 
useless, nor can one precisely say why they persist after 
their original usefulness has ceased. They remain because 
of necessities of growth of which we are ignorant, and 
they may be useful in relation to other structures though 
in themselves functionless. 

Classification of Organs.—We may arrange organs 
according to their work, some, such as limbs and weapons, 
being busied with the external relations of the organism ; 
others, such as heart and liver, being concerned with 
internal affairs. Or we may classify them according to 
their development from the outer, middle, or inner layer of 
the embryo. Thus brain and sense-organs are always mainly 
due to the outer stratum (ectoderm or epiblast), muscles 
and skeleton arise from the middle mesoderm or mesoblast, 
the gut and its outgrowths such as lungs and liver primarily 
originate from the inner endoderm or hypoblast. Or we 
may arrange the various structures more or less arbitrarily 
for convenience of description as follows: the skin and its 
outgrowths, appendages, skeleton, muscular system, nervous 
system, sense-organs, the food-canal and its outgrowths, 
the body-cavity, the heart and blood-vessels, the respiratory 
organs, the excretory system, the reproductive organs. 

Tissues.—To the school of Cuvier we owe the analysis 
of the animal organism into its component organs; but as 
early as 1801 Bichat published his Anatomie Générale, in 
which the analysis was carried a step farther. He reduced 
the organs to their component tissues, and maintained that 


180 The Study of Animal Life PART It 


the function of an organ might be expressed in terms of the 
properties of its tissues. 

If we pass to the next step of analysis, and think of the 
body as a complex city of cells, we are better able to 
understand what tissues are. Each cell corresponds to a 
house, a tissue corresponds to a street of similar houses. 
Ina city like Leipzig many streets are homogeneous, formed 
by houses or shops in which the predominant activity is the 
same throughout. A street is devoted to the making of 
clothes, or of bread, or of books. So in the animal body 
aggregates of contractile cells form muscular tissue, of 
supporting cells skeletal tissue, of secreting cells glandular 
tissue, and so on. 

It is enough to state the general idea that a tissue is an 
aggregate of more or less similar cells, and to note that the 
different kinds may be grouped as follows :— 


I. Nervous tissue, consisting of cells which receive, 
transmit, or originate nerve-stimuli. 
II. Muscular tissue, consisting of contractile cells. 

III. Epithelial tissue, consisting of lining and covering 
cells, which often become glandular, exuding the 
products of their activity as secretions. 

IV. Connective tissue, including cells which bind, 
support, and store. 


Cells.—To the discovery and perfecting of the micro- 
scope we owe the analysis of the body into its unit masses 
of living matter or cells. From 1838-39, when Schwann 
and Schleiden stated in their ‘“‘cell doctrine” that all 
organisms—plants and animals alike—were built up of 
cells, cellular biology may be said to date. It was soon 
shown asa corollary that every organism which reproduced 
in the ordinary fashion arose from a single egg-cell or 
ovum which had been fertilised by union with a male-cell 
or spermatozoon. Moreover, the position of the simplest 
animals and plants was more clearly appreciated ; they are 
single cells, the higher organisms are multicellular. 

Now the cells of the animal body are necessarily varied, 
for the existence of a body involves division of labour 


CHAP. XI The Elements of Structure 181 


among the units. Some, such as the lashed cells lining 
the windpipe, are very active, like the Infusorian Protozoa ; 
others, for instance the fat-cells and gristle-cells of connective 
tissue, are very passive, something like the Gregarines ; 
others, such as the white blood corpuscles or leucocytes, 
are between these extremes, and resemble the amceboid 
Protozoa. 

But it is true of most of them that they consist (1) of a 


Fic. 34.—Animal cell, showing the coiled chromatin threads of the nucleus (a), 
and the protoplasmic network (4) round about. (From £volution of Sex ; 
after Carnoy.) 


complex, and in part living cell-substance, in which keen 
eyes looking through good microscopes detect an intricate 
network, or sometimes the appearance of a fine foam ; (2) 
of a central kernel or nucleus, which plays an important 
but hardly definable part in the life of the cell, especially 
during the process of cell-division; (3) of a slight outer 
membrane, varying much in definiteness and sometimes 
quite absent, through which communications with neigh- 


182 The Study of Animal Life PART II 


bouring cells are often established ; and (4) of cell contents, 
which can be chemically analysed, and which are products 
of the vital activity rather than parts of the living substance, 
such as pigment, fat, and glycogen or animal starch. 

The growth of all multicellular animals depends upon a 
multiplication of the component cells. Like organisms, 
cells have definite limits of growth which they rarely exceed ; 
giants among the units are rare. When the limit of 
growth is reached the cell divides. 

The necessity for this division has been partly explained 
by Spencer and Leuckart. If you take a round lump of 
dough, weighing an ounce, another of two ounces, a third 
of four ounces, you obviously have three masses success- 
ively doubled, but in doubling the mass you have not 
doubled the surface. The mass increases as the cube, the 
surface only as the square of the radius. Suppose these 
lumps alive, the second has twice as much living matter as 
the first, but not twice the surface. Yet it is through the 
surface that the living matter is fed, aerated, and purified. 
The unit will therefore get into physiological difficulties as 
it grows bigger, because its increase of surface does not 
keep pace with its increase of mass. Its waste tends to 
exceed its repair, its expenditure gains on its income. 
What are the alternatives? It may go on growing and die 
(but this is not likely), it may cease growing at the fit 
limit, it may greatly increase its surface by outflowing 
processes (which thus may be regarded as life-saving), or 
it may divide. The last is the usual course. When the 
unit has grown as large as it can conveniently grow, it 
divides; in other words, it reproduces at the limit of 
growth, when processes of waste are gaining on those 
of construction. By dividing, the mass is lessened, the 
surface increased, the life continued. 

But although we thus get a general rationale of cell- 
division, we are not much nearer a conception of the 
internal forces which operate when a cell divides; for in 
most cases the process 1s orderly and complex, and is 
somehow governed by the behaviour of the nucleus. Few 
results of the modern study of minute structure are more 


CHAP. Xt The Elements of Structure 183 


marvellous than those which relate to dividing cells, From 
Protozoa to man, and also in plants, the process is strik- 
ingly uniform. The nucleus of the cell becomes more 
active, the coil or network of threads which it contains is 
undone and takes the new and more regular form of a 
spindle or barrel. The division is most thorough, each of 
the two daughter-cells getting an accurate half of the 
original nucleus. Recent investigators, moreover, assert 
that from certain centres in the cell-substance an influence 
is exerted on the nuclear threads, and they talk of an archo- 
plasm within the protoplasm, and of marked individuality of 
behaviour in the nuclear threads. 

From the cell as a unit element we penetrate to the 
protoplasm which makes it what it is. Within this we 
discern an intricate network, within this, special centres of 
force—“ attractive spheres” and “central corpuscles,” or 
an “archoplasm” within the protoplasm! We study the 
nucleus, first as a simple unit which divides, years after- 
wards as composed of a network or coil of nuclear threads 
which seem ever to become more and more marvellous, 
“behaving like little organisms.” We split these up 
into “‘microsomata,” and so on, and so on. But we do 
not catch the life of the cell, we cannot locate it, we cannot 
give an account of the mechanics of cell-division. It is a 
mystery of life. After all our analysis we have to confess 
that the cell, or the protoplasm, or the archoplasm, or the 
chromatin threads of the nucleus, or the ‘‘microsomata” 
which compose them, baffle our analysis; they behave as 
they do because they are aliye. Were we omniscient 
chemists, such as Laplace imagined in one of his specula- 
tions, and knew the secret of protoplasm, how its touch 
upon the simpler states of matter is powerful to give them 
life, we should but have completed a small part of those 
labours that even now lie waiting us; what further investi- 
gations will present themselves we cannot tell. . 


CHAPTER XII 
THE LIFE-HISTORY OF ANIMALS 


1. Modes of Reproduction—2, Divergent Modes of Reproduction— 
3. Historical—4. The Egg-Cell or Ovum—5. The Male- 
Cell or Spermatozoon—6. Maturation of the Ovum—7. 
Fertilisation —8. Segmentation and the first stages in 
Development—g. Some Generalisations—The Ovum Theory, 
the Gastrea Theory, Fact of Recapitulation, Organic Con- 
tinuity 


IN his exercitation “on the efficient cause of the 
chicken,” Harvey (1651) confesses that “although it be a 
known thing subscribed by all, that the foetus assumes its 
original and birth from the male and female, and conse- 
quently that the egge is produced by the cock and henne, 
and the chicken out of the egge, yet neither the schools of 
physicians nor Aristotle’s discerning brain have disclosed 
the manner how the cock and his seed doth mint and 
coine the chicken out of the egge.” The marvellous 
facts of growth are familiar to us—the sprouting corn 
and the opening flowers, the growth of the chick within 
the egg and of the child within the womb; yet so 
difficult is the task of inquiring wisely into this marvel- 
lous renewal of life that we must reiterate the old 
confession: “‘ingratissimum opus scribere ab iis que, 
multis a natura circumjectis tenebris velata, sensuum 
lucis inaccessa, hominum agitantur opinionibus,” 

1. Modes of Reproduction.—The simplest animals 
divide into two or into many parts, each of which becomes a 
full-grown Protozoon. There is no difficulty in understanding 


cHar.xur The Life-History of Animals 185 


why each part should be able to regrow the whole, for each 
is a fair sample of the original whole. Indeed, when a 
large Protozoon is cut into two or three pieces with a knife, 
each fragment is often able to retain the movements and 
life of the intact organism. Among the Protozoa we find 
some in which the multiplication looks like the rupture of a 
cell which has become too large; in others numerous buds 
are set free from the surface ; in others one definitely-formed 
bud (like an overflow of the living matter) is set free; in 
others the cell divides into two equal parts, after the 
manner of most cells; and numerous divisions may also 
occur in rapid succession and within a cyst, that is, in 
limited time and space, with the result that many “spores” 
are formed. These modes of multiplication form a natural 
series. 

In the many-celled animals multiplication may still pro- 
ceed by the separation of parts; indeed the essence of 
reproduction always is the separation of part of an organ- 
ism to form—or to help to form—a new life. Sponges bud 
profusely, and pieces are sometimes set adrift; the Hydra 
forms daughter polypes by budding, and these are set free ; 
sea-anemones and several worms, and perhaps even some 
star-fishes, multiply by the separation of comparatively large 
pieces. But this mode of multiplication—which is called 
asexual—has evident limitations. It is an expensive way 
of multiplying. It is possible only among comparatively 
simple animals in which there is no very high degree of 
differentiation and integration. For though cut-off pieces 
of a sponge, Hydra, sea-anemone, or simple worm may 
grow into adult animals, this is obviously not the case 
with a lobster, a snail, or a fish, Thus with the excep- 
tion of the degenerate Tunicates there is no budding 
among Vertebrates, nor among Molluscs, nor among 
Arthropods. ; 

The asexual process of liberating more or less large 
parts, being expensive, and possible only in simpler animals, 
is always either replaced or accompanied by another 
method—that of sexual reproduction. The phrase “ sexual 
reproduction ” covers several distinct facts: (a) the separa. 


186 The Study of Animal Life PART IT! 


tion of special reproductive cells ; (6) the production of two 
different kinds of reproductive cells (spermatozoa and ova), 
which are dependent on one another, for in most cases an 
ovum comes to nothing unless it be united with a male-cell 
or spermatozoon, and in all cases the spermatozoon comes 
to nothing unless it be united with an ovum; (c) the pro- 
duction ot spermatozoa and ova by different (male and 
female) organs or individuals. 

(a) It is easy to think of simple many-celled animals 
being multiplied by liberated reproductive cells, which 
differed but little from those of the body. But as more 
and more division of labour was established in the bodies 
of animals, the distinctness of the reproductive cells from 
the other units of the body became greater. Finally, the 
prevalent state was reached, in which the only cells able 
to begin a new life when liberated are the reproductive 
cells. They owe this power to the fact that they have not 
shared in making the body, but have preserved intact the 
characters of the fertilised ovum from which the parent 
itself arose. 

(4) But, in the second place, it is easy to conceive of a 
simple multicellular animal whose liberated reproductive 
cells were each and all alike able to grow into new 
organisms. In such a case, we might speak of sexual 
reproduction in one sense, for the process would be different 
from the asexual method of liberating more or less large 
parts. But yet there would be no fertilisation and no sex, 
for fertilisation means the union of mutually dependent 
reproductive cells, and sex means the existence of two 
physiologically different kinds of individuals, or at least 
of organs producing different kinds of reproductive cells, 
We can infer from the Protozoa how fertilisation or the union 
of the two kinds of reproductive cells may have had a 
gradual origin. For in some of the simplest Protozoa, e.g. 
Protomyxa, a large number of similar cells sometimes flow 
together ; in a few cases three or more combine; in most a 
couple of apparently similar units unite; while in a few 
instances, ¢.g. Vorticella, a small cell fuses with a large one, 
just as a spermatozoon unites with an ovum. 


cuar, xr The Life-History of Animals 187 


(c) But the higher forms of sexual reproduction imply 
more than the liberation of special reproductive cells, more 
than the union of two different and mutually dependent 
kinds of reproductive cells, — they imply the separation 
of the sexes. The problem of sexual reproduction becomes 
less difficult when the various facts are discussed separ- 
ately, and if you grant that there is no great difficulty 
in understanding the liberation of special cells, and 
no great difficulty in understanding why two different 
kinds should in most cases have to unite if either is to 
develop, then I do not think that the remaining fact — 
the evolution of male and female individuals—need remain 
obscure. 

If we study those interesting Infusorian colonies, of 
which Volvox is a good type, the riddle may be at least 
partially read. Though Protozoa, they are balls of cells, in 
which the component units are united by protoplasmic 
bridges and show almost no division of labour, From 
such a ball of cells, units are sometimes set free which 
divide and form new colonies. In other conditions a less 
direct multiplication occurs. Some of the cells—apparently 
better fed than their neighbours—become large; others, 
less successful, divide into many minute units. The large 
kind of cell is fertilised by the small kind of cell, and there 
is no reason why we should not call them ova and sperma- 
tozoa respectively. In such a Volvox, two different kinds 
of reproductive cell are made within one organism. But 
we also find Volvox balls in which only ova are being 
made, and others in which only spermatozoa are being 
made. The sexes are separate. Indeed we have in Vol- 
vox, as Dr. Klein—an enthusiastic investigator of this form 
—rightly says, an epitome of all the great steps in the 
evolution of sex. 

So far I have stated facts ; now I shall briefly state the 
theory by which Professor Geddes has sought to rationalise 
these facts. 

All through the animal series, from the active Infusorians 
and passive Gregarines, to the feverish birds and sluggish 
reptiles, and down into the detailed contrasts between order 


188 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


and order, species and species, an antithesis may be read 
between predominant activity and preponderant passivity, 
between lavish expenditure of energy and a habit of storing, 
between a relatively more disruptive (Aafabolic) and a re- 
latively more constructive (anabolic) series of changes in 
the protoplasmic life of the creature. The contrast between 
the sexes is an expression of this fundamental alternative of 
variation. 

The theory is confirmed by contrasting the characteristic 
product of female life—passive ova, with the characteristic 
product of male life—active spermatozoa ; or by summing 
up the complex conditions (abundant food, favourable 
temperature, and the like) which favour the production of 
female offspring, with the opposite conditions which favour 
maleness; or by contrasting the secondary sexual char- 
acters of the more active males (e.g. bright colours, smaller 
size) with the opposite characteristics of their more passive 
mates. 

Apart from the general problem of the evolution of sex, 
those who find the subject interesting should think about 
the evolution of the so-called “sexual instincts,” as illus- 
trated in the attraction of mate to mate. As to the actual 
facts of pairing and giving birth, it seems to me that I have 
suggested the most profitable way of considering these in a 
former part of this book where courtship and parental care 
are discussed, though I believe firmly with Thoreau, that 
“ for him to whom sex is impure, there are no flowers in 
nature.” 

2. Divergent Modes of Reproduction.—(2) Herma- 
phroditism.—Especially among lower animals, both ova 
and spermatozoa may be produced by one individual, 
which is then said to be hermaphrodite. So most common 
plants produce both seeds and pollen. Some sponges and 
stinging animals, many “ worms,” ¢.g. earthworm and leech, 
barnacles and acorn-shells among crustaceans, one of the 
edible oysters, the snail, and many other molluscs, the sea- 
squirts, and the hagfish, are all hermaphrodite. But it 
should be noted that the organs in which ova and sperma- 
tozoa are produced are in most cases separate, that the two 


cuap. xu The Life-History of Animals 189 


kinds of cells are usually formed at different times, and 
that the fertilisation of ova by spermatozoa from the same 
animal very rarely occurs. It is very likely that the 
bisexual or hermaphrodite state of periodic maleness and 
femaleness is more primitive than that of separate sexes, 
which, except in tunicates, a few fishes and amphibians, 
and casual abnormalities, is constant among the backboned 
animals. 

(6) Parthenogenesis seems to be a degenerate form of 
sexual reproduction in which the ova produced by female 
organisms develop without being fertilised by male cells. 
Thus “the drones have a mother but no father,” for they 
develop from ova which are not fertilised. In some rotifers 
the males have never been found, and yet the fertility of the 
females is very great; in many small crustaceans (‘‘ water- 
fleas”) the males seem to die off and are unrepresented for 
long periods; in the aphides males may be absent for a 
summer (or in a greenhouse for years) without affecting the 
rapid succession of female generations. 

(c) Alternation of Generations.—A fixed asexual zoophyte 
or hydroid sometimes buds off and liberates sexual swim- 
ming bells or medusoids, whose fertilised ova develop into 
embryos which settle down and grow into hydroids, This is 
perhaps the simplest and clearest illustration of alternation 
of generations. 

In autumn the freshwater sponge (Sfomgil/a) begins 
to suffer from the cold and the scarcity of food. It dies 
away; but some of the units club together to form 
“gemmules” from which in spring male and female 
sponges are developed. The males are short-lived, but 
their spermatozoa fertilise the ova of the females, The 
fertilised ovum develops into a ciliated embryo, and this 
into the asexual sponge, which produces the gemmules. 

The large free-swimming and sexual jellyfishes of the 
genus Aurelia produce ova and spermatozoa; from the 
fertilised ovum an embryo develops not into a jellyfish, but 
into a sessile Hydra-like animal. This grows and divides 
and gives origin asexually to jellyfish. 

Similar but sometimes more complicated alternations 


190 The Study of Animal Life PART Ill 


occur in some worm-types (some flukes, threadworms, etc. ), 
and as high up in the series as Tunicates ; while among 
plants analogous alternations are very common, c.g. in the 
life-cycles of fern and moss. 


Sep eae eee 


Fic. 35.—Diagram of a hydroid colony, some of the individuals of which have 
been modified as swimming- bells or medusoids; one of these has been 


liberated. 


Historical.—In the seventeenth and eighteenth cen- 
turies, naturalists had a short and easy method of dealing with 
embryology. They maintained that within the seed of a 
plant, within the egg of a bird, the future organism was 
already present in miniature. Every germ contained a 
miniature model of the adult, which in development was 


cHar. x1 The Life-History of Animals 191 


simply unfolded. It was to this unfolding that the word 
evolution (as a biological term) was first applied. But not 
only did they compare the germ to a complex bud hiding 
the already formed organs within its hull, they maintained 
that it included also the next generation and the next and 
the next. Some said that the ovum was most important, 
that it required only the sperm’s awakening touch and it 
began to unfold; others said that the animalcules or 
spermatozoa produced by male animals were most im- 
portant, that they only required to be nourished by the 
ova. The two schools nicknamed one another “ ovists” 
and “animalculists.” The preformation-theories were false, 
as Harvey in the middle of the seventeenth century discerned, 
and as Wolff a century later proved, because germs are 
demonstrably simple, and because embryos grow gradually 
part by part. But in a later chapter we shall see that the 
theories were also strangely true. 

4. The Egg-cell or Ovum produced by a female animal, 
or at least by a female organ (ovary), exhibits the usual 
characteristics of a cell. It often begins like an Ameeba, 
and may absorb adjacent cells; in most cases it becomes 
surrounded by an envelope or by several sheaths; in 
many cases it is richly laden with yolk derived from various 
sources. In the egg of a fowl, the most important part 
(out of which the embryo is made) is a small area of trans- 
parent living matter which lies on the top of the yellow 
yolk and has a nucleus for its centre; round about 
there is a coating of white-of-egg; this is surrounded by 
a double membrane which forms an air-chamber at the 
broad end of the egg; outermost is the porous shell of 
lime. 

While there must be a general relation between the size 
of the bird and that of the egg, there are many inconsisten- 
cies, as you will soon discover if you compare the eggs 
of several birds of the same size. It is said that the eggs 
of birds which are rapidly hatched and soon leave the nest 
tend to be large, and that there is some relation between the 
size of eggs and the number which the bird has to cover. 
It seems probable, however, from what one notices in the 


192 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


poultry yard and in comparing the constitution of different 
birds, that a highly-nourished and not very energetic bird 
will have larger eggs than one of more active habits and 
sparser diet. 

The egg-shell consists almost wholly of carbonate of 
lime, and the experiments of Irvine have shown that a hen can 
form a carbonate of lime shell from other lime salts, It is 
formed around the egg in the lower part of the oviduct, and 
is often beautifully coloured with pigments allied to those of 
blood and bile. These colours often harmonise well with 
the surroundings, but how this advantageous result jaas 
been wrought out is uncertain. 

Eggs differ greatly in regard to the amount of yolk which 
they contain; thus those of birds and reptiles have much, while 
those of all mammals except the old-fashioned Monotremes 
have hardly any. This is related partly to the number of 
eggs which are produced, and partly to the amount of food- 
capital which the embryo requires before other sources 
of supply become available. The young of birds and 
reptiles feed on the yolk until they are hatched, the unborn 
young of all the higher (placental) mammals absorb food 
from the mothers. The different sizes of egg usually 
depend upon the amount of yolk, for the really vital portion 
out of which the embryo is made is always very small. 

There are many differences also in regard to the outer 
envelopes, witness the jelly around the spawn of frogs, the 
firm but delicate skin around the ova of cuttlefish, the 
“horny” mermaid’s-purse enclosing the skate’s ‘egg, the 
chitinous sheath surrounding the ova of many insects, the 
calcareous shell in birds and most reptiles. 

5. The Male-Cell or Spermatozoon produced from a male 
animal, or at least from a male organ (testis), is very differ- 
ent from the ovum. It is very minute and very active. If 
we compare an ovum to an Ameeba or to an encysted 
Gregarine among Protozoa, we may liken the spermatozoon 
to a minute monad Infusorian. It is a very small cell, 
bearing at one end a “head,” which consists mostly of 
nucleus, prolonged at the other end into a mobile “ tail,” 
which lashes the head along. 


cuar. xn. The Life-History of Animals 193 


The spermatozoon, though physiologically the comple- 
ment of the ovum, is not its morphological equivalent. 
The precise equivalent of the ovum is a primitive male-cell 
or mother-sperm-cell, which divides repeatedly and forms a 
ball or clump of spermatozoa. This division is to be com- 
pared with the division or segmentation of the ovum, which 
we shall afterwards discuss. 

In some cases spermatozoa which have been transferred 
to a female may lie long dormant there. Thus those 
received by the queen-bee during her nuptial flight may last 
for a whole season, or even for three seasons, during which 
they are used in fertilising those ova which develop into 
workers or queen-bees. Quite unique is the case of one of 
Sir John Lubbock’s queen-ants, which, thirteen years after 
the last sexual union with a male, laid eggs which 
developed. 

6. Maturation of the Ovum.—Most ova before they are 
fertilised are subject to a remarkable change, the precise 
meaning of which is not certainly known. The nucleus of 
the ovum moves to the surface and is halved twice in rapid 
succession. Two minute cells or polar globules are thus 
extruded, and come to nothing, while the bulk of the 
nucleus is obviously reduced by three-fourths. It may be 
that the ovum is only behaving as other cells do at the 
limit of growth, or that it is exhibiting in an ineffective sort 
of way the power of independent division which all the re- 
productive cells of very simple many-celled animals perhaps 
possessed ; it may be that it is parting with some surplus 
material which is inconsistent with or no longer necessary 
to its welfare, and there are other theories. One fact, 
however, seems well established, that parthenogenetic ova, 
which are able to develop into embryos without being 
fertilised, extrude only one polar globule, a fact which 
suggests that the amount of nucleus thus retained some- 
how makes up for the absence of a spermatozoon. 

7. Fertilisation, When a pollen grain is carried by an 
insect or by the wind to the stigma of a flower, it grows: 
down through the tissue of the pistil until it reaches the 
ovule and the egg-cell which that contains. Then a nuclear 

(0) 


194 The Study of Animal Life PART IT 


element belonging to the pollen cell unites with the nucleus 
of the egg-cell. The union is intimate and complete. 

When spermatozoa come in contact with the egg-shell 
of a cockroach ovum, they move round and round it in 
varying orbits until one finds entrance through a minute 
aperture in the shell. It works its way inwards until its 
nuclear part unites with that of the ovum, The union is 
again intimate and complete. 


Fic. 36.—Diagram of the development of spermatozoa (upper line), of the 
maturation and fertilisation of the ovum (lower line). 

a, primitive amceboid sex-cell; A, ovum with nucleus (7); B, ovum extruding 
the first polar body (/1) and leaving the nucleus (71) reduced by half ; C, 
extrusion of the second polar body (2), the nucleus (77) now reduced to a 
fourth of its original size ; 1, a mother-sperm-cell, dividing (2 and 3) into 
spermatozoa (sf); D, the entrance of a spermatozoon into the ovum; E, the 
male nucleus (sf.7) and the female nucleus (7%) approach one another, and 
are about to be united, thus consummating the fertilisation. (From the 
Evolution of Sex.) 


Both in plants and in animals the male cell is attracted 
to the female cell, the two nuclei unite thoroughly, and, 
when fertilisation is thus effected, the egg-cell is usually 
impervious to other sperms. 

A single nucleus of double origin is thus established, 
and the egg-cell begins to divide. Some idea both of the 
orderly complexity of the nuclear union and of the careful- 
ness of modern investigation may be gained from the fact 
that the nuclei of the two daughter-cells which result from 


cuar. xn =. The Life-History of Animals 195 


the first division of the egg-cell have been shown to consist 
in equal proportions of material derived from the male- 
nucleus and from the ovum-nucleus. 

Yet in the last century naturalists still spoke of an “aura 
seminalis,” and believed that a mere breath, as it were, of 
the male cell was sufficient to fertilise an egg, and it was 
only in 1843 that Martin Barry discerned the presence of 
the spermatozoon within the ovum. 

8. Segmentation and Development, — The fertilised 
egg-cell divides, and by repeated division and growth of 
cells every embryo, of herb and tree, of bird and beast, is 
formed. On the quantity and arrangement of the yolk the 
character of the segmentation depends. When there is 
little or no yolk the whole ovum divides into equal parts, as 
in sponge, earthworm, starfish, lancelet, and higher mammal. 
When there is more than a little yolk, and when this sinks 
to the lower part of the egg-cell, the division is complete 
but unequal, and this may be readily seen by examining 
freshly-laid frog spawn. When the yolk is accumulated in 
the core of the egg-cell, the more vital superficial part 
divides, as in insects and crustaceans. Lastly, when the 
yolk is present in large quantity as in the ova of gristly 
fishes, reptiles, and birds, the division is very partial, being 
confined to a small but rapidly extending area of formative 
living matter, which lies like a drop on the surface of the 
yolk. 

As the result of continued division, a ball of cells is 
formed. This may be hollow (a Jdlastosphere), or solid 
(a morula, z.e, like a mulberry), or it may be much modi- 
fied in form by the presence of a large quantity of yolk. 
Thus in the hen’s egg what is first formed is a disc of cells 
technically called the blastoderm, which gradually spreads 
around the yolk. 

The hollow ball of cells almost always becomes dimpled 
in or invaginated, as an india-rubber ball with a hole in it 
might be pressed into a cup-like form. The dimpling is the 
result of inequalities of growth. The two-layered sac of cells 
which results is called a gastvu/a, and the cavity of this sac 
becomes in the adult organism the digestive part of the 


196 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


food-canal. Where there is no hollow ball of cells, but 
some other result of segmentation, the formation of a gastrula 
isnot so obvious. Yet 
in most cases some 
analogous infolding is 
demonstrable, 

In the hollow sac 
of cells there are 
already two layers. 
The outer, which is 
called the ectoderm 
or epiblast, forms in 
the adult the outer 
skin, the nervous 
system, and the most 
important parts of the 
sense - organs. The 
inner, which is called 
the endoderm or hypo- 
blast, forms the lining 
of the most import- 
ant part of the food- 
canal, and of such 
appendages as lungs, 
Fic. 37.—The formation of the two-layered gas- met, ane Panerees 

trula from the invagination of a hollow sphere which are outgrowths 

of cells. (From the Lvolution of Sex; after from it But in all 

Haeckel.) 5 x 

animals above the 
Sponges and Ccelenterates, a middle layer appears between 
the other two, From this—the mesoderm or mesoblast— 
the muscles, the internal skeleton, the connective-tissue, etc., 
are formed. 

9. Some Generalisations——(a) The “ Ovum - Theory.” 
To realise that almost every organism from the sponge to 
the highest begins its life as a fertilised egg-cell, and is 
built up by the division and arrangement, layering and fold- 
ing of cells, should not lessen, but should greatly enhance, 
the wonder with which we look upon life. If the end 
of this constantly repeated process of development be 


cHar. xu = The Life-History of Animals 197 


something to marvel at, the same is equally true of its 
beginning. 

(6) The Gastrea Theory. From the frequent, though 
not universal occurrence of the two-layered gastrula stage in 
the development of animals, Haeckel concluded that the 
first stable form of many-celled animal must have been 
something very like a gastrula. He called this hypothetical 
ancestor of all many-celled animals a Gastr@a, and his infer- 
ence has found favour with many naturalists. Some of the 
simplest sponges, polypes, and “ worms” are hardly above 
the gastrula level. 

(c) Recapitulation. When we take a general survey 
of the animal series, we recognise that the simplest animals 
are single cells, that the next simplest are balls of cells like 
Volvox, and that the next simplest are two-layered sacs of 
cells like the simple sponges, polypes, and worms above 
referred to. These represent the three lowest steps in the 
evolution of the race. They are not hypothetical steps in a 
hypothetical ladder of ascent, they are realities. 

When we take a general survey of the individual 
development of many-celled animals, we recognise that all 
begin as single egg-cells, and that the ova divide into balls 
of cells, which become in most cases two-layered sacs of 
cells. It is therefore evident that the first three chapters in 
individual history are precisely the first three steps in racial 
history. 

Von Baer, one of the pioneer embryologists in the first 
half of this century, discerned that the individual life-history 
was in its general course a recapitulation of the history of 
the race. He recognised that even one of the higher 
animals, let us say a rabbit, began at the beginning as a 
Protozoon, that it slowly acquired the features of a primitive 
Vertebrate, that it subsequently showed the character of a 
young fish, afterwards of a young reptile, then of a young 
mammal, then of a young rodent, finally of a young rabbit. 
He confessed his inability to distinguish whether three very 
young embryos, freed from their surroundings, were those 
of reptiles, birds, or mammals. In stating Von Baer’s 
vivid idea of development as progress from the simple 


198 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


to the complex, from the general to the special, we must 
be careful to notice that he did not say that the young 
mammal was once like a little fish, afterwards like a reptile, 
and so on; he compared the embryo mammal at one stage 
with the embryo fish, at another stage with the embryo 
reptile, which is a very different matter. 


Fic, 38.—Embryos of fowl, a; dog, 8; man,c. (From Chambers’s Eucyc/of. ; 
after Haeckel.) 


Fritz Miller, in his Facts for Darwin, illustrated the 
same idea in relation to Crustacea. When a young cray- 
fish is hatched, it is practically a miniature adult, When 
a young lobster is hatched, it differs not a little from the 
adult, and is described as being at a A/ysts stage,—JZysts 
being a prawn-like crustacean. It grows and moults and 
becomes a little lobster. When a crab is hatched, it is 
quite unlike the adult, it is liker one of the humblest 
Crustacea such as the common water-flea Cyclops, and is 
described as a Zoea. This Zoea grows and moults and 
becomes, not yet a crab but a prawn-like animal with ex- 
tended tail, a stage known as the Megalopa. This grows and 
moults, tucks in its tail, and becomes a young crab. And 
again, when the shrimp-like crustacean, known as Peneus, 
is hatched, it is simpler than any known crustacean, it is 
an unringed somewhat shield-shaped little creature with 
three pairs of appendages and a median eye. It is known 
as a Nauplius and resembles the larve of most of the simpler 
crustaceans. It grows and moults and becomes a Zoea, 
grows and moults and becomes a A/yszs, grows and moults 
and becomes a Peneus. 


cuap. xu The Life-History of Animals 199 


Now these life-histories are hardly intelligible at all 
unless we believe that Pez@us does in some measure recapi- 
tulate the steps of racial progress, that the crab does so 
to a slighter extent, that the lobster has abbreviated its 
obvious recapitulation much more, while the crayfish has 
found out a short cut in development. Let us exercise our 
imagination and think of the ancestral Crustacea perhaps 
not much less simple than the Nauplius larve which many 


Fic. 39.—Life-history of Peneus; the Nauplius. 


of them exhibit. In the course of time some pushed for- 
ward in evolution and attained to the level of structure 
represented by the Zoea larvae. At this station some 
remained and we have already mentioned the “ water-flea ” 
Cyclops as a crustacean which persists near this level. But 
others pushed on and reached a stage represented by 
Mysis, and finally the highest crustaceans were evolved. 
Now to a certain extent these highest crustaceans have 
to travel in their individual development along the rails 
laid down in the progress of the race. Thus Penaeus, 


200 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


starting of course as an ovum at the level of the Protozoa, 
has to stop as it were at the first distinctively crustacean 
station—the Nauplius stage. After some change and 
delay, it continues to progress, but again there is a halt and 
a change at the Zoea station. Finally there is another 


Fic. 39a.— Life-history of Peneus ; the Zoea. 


delay at the AZyszs stage before the Penzus reaches its 
destination. The crab, on the other hand, stops first at 
the Zoea station, the lobster at the JZys¢s station, while the 
crayfish though progressing very gradually like all the 
others has—if you do not find the simile too grotesque—a 
through-carriage all the way. 


Fic. 39.—Life-history of Penaeus ; a later stage. 


to 


02 


The Study of Animal Life PART IIL 


One must be careful not to press the idea of recapitulation 
too far, (1) because the individual life-history tends to skip 


\\ 


Fic. 39¢.—Life-history of Peneus ; Mysis stage. 


W 


(From Fritz Miiller.) 


stages which occurred in the an- 
cestral progress ; (2) because the 
young animal may acquire new 
characters which are peculiar to 
its own near lineage and have 
little or no importance in connec- 
tion with the general evolution of 
its race; (3) because, in short, 
the resemblance between the indi- 
vidual and racial history (so far 
as we know them) is general, not 
precise. Thus we regard Nauplius 
and Zoea rather as adaptive larval 
forms than as representatives of 
ancestral crustaceans. More- 
over, if one insists too much on 
the approximate parallelism be- 
tween the life-history of the indi- 
vidual and the progress of the 
race, one is apt to overlook the 
deeper problem —how it is that 
the recapitulation occurs to the 
extent that it undoubtedly does. 
The organism has no feeling for 
history that it should tread a 
sometimes circuitous path, be- 
cause its far-off ancestors did so. 
To some extent we may think of 
inherited constitution as if it were 
the hand of the past upon the 
organism, compelling it to become 
thus or thus, but we must realise 
that this is a living not a dead 
hand ; in other words these meta- 


morphoses have their efficient causes in the actual con- 
ditions of growth and development. The suggestion of 
Kleinenberg referred to in a preceding chapter helps us, for 


coar. xr = The Life-History of Animals 203 


if we ask why an animal develops a notochord only to have 
it rapidly replaced by a backbone, part of the answer surely 
is that the notochord which in the historical evolution supplied 
the stimulus necessary for the development of a backbone, is 
still necessary in the individual history for the same purpose. 

But there is no doubt that the idea of recapitulation is a 
very helpful one, in regard to our own history as well as in 
regard to animals, and we would do well to think of it 
much, and to read how Herbert Spencer (Principles of 
Biology, Lond. 1864-66) has discussed it in harmony with his 
general formula of evolution as a progress from the homo- 
geneous to the heterogeneous ; how Haeckel (Generelle Mor- 
Phologie, Berlin, 1866) has illustrated it, and pithily summed 
it up in his “fundamental law of biogenesis ” (Bzogenetisches 
Grundgesetz), saying that ontogeny (individual develop- 
ment) recapitulates phylogeny (racial history) ; how Milnes 
Marshall (see ature, Sept. 1890) has recently tested and 
criticised it, defining the limits within which the notion 
can be regarded as true, and searching for a deeper rationale 
of the facts than the theory supplies. 

(2) Organic Continuity. in a subsequent chapter on 
heredity, which simply means the relation of organic 
continuity between successive generations, I shall explain 
the fundamental idea that the reproductive cells owe their 
power of developing, and of developing into organisms like 
the parents, to the fact that they are in a sense continuous 
with those which gave origin to the parents. A fertilised 
egg-cell with certain qualities divides and forms a “body” 
in which these qualities are expressed, distributed, and 
altered in many ways by division of labour. But it also 
forms reproductive cells, which do not share in the up- 
building of the body, which are reproductive cells in fact 
because they do not do so, because they retain the intrinsic 
qualities of the original fertilised ovum, because they 
preserve its protoplasmic tradition. If this be so, and 
there is much reason to believe it, then it is natural and 
necessary that these cells, liberated in due time, should 
behave as those behaved whose qualities they retain. It is 
necessary that like should beget like. 


CHAPTER XIII 
THE PAST HISTORY OF ANIMALS 


1. The two Records—2. Imperfection of the Geological Record— 
3. Paleontological Series—4. Extinction of Types—s. Various 
Difficulties—6. Relative Antiquity of Animals 


1. The Two Records.—Reviewing the development of the 
chick, W. K. Parker said, “Whilst at work I seemed 
to myself to have been endeavouring to decipher a palimp- 
sest, and that not erased and written upon just once, but 
five or six times over. Having erased, as it were, the 
characters of the culminating type—those of the gaudy 
Indian bird—I seemed to be amongst the sombre grouse, 
and then, towards incubation, the characters of the Sand- 
Grouse and Hemipod stood out before me. Rubbing these 
away, in my downward walk, the form of the Tinamou 
looked me in the face; then the aberrant Ostrich seemed 
to be described in large archaic characters; a little while 
and these faded into what could just be read off as per- 
taining to the Sea Turtle; whilst, underlying the whole, 
the Fish in its simplest Myxinoid form could be traced 
in morphological hieroglyphics.” 

There is another palimpsest—the geological record 
written in the rocks. For beneath the forms which dis- 
appeared, as it were, yesterday,—the Dodo and the Solitaire, 
the Moa and the Mammoth, the Cave Lion and the Irish 
Elk,—there are mammals and birds of old-fashioned type the 
like of which no longer live. Beneath these lie the giant 


car. x1 Lhe Past History of Animals 205 


reptiles, beneath these great amphibians, preceded by hosts 
of armoured fishes, beyond the first traces of which only 
backboneless animals are found. Yet throughout the 
chapters of this record, written during different zeons on the 
earth’s surface, persistent forms recur from age to age, 
many of them, such as some of the lamp-shells or Brachio- 
pods, living on from near the apparent beginning even until 
now. But other races, like the Trilobites, have died out, 
leaving none which we can regard as in any sense their 
direct descendants. Other sets of animals, like the Ganoid 
fishes, grow in strength, attain a golden age of prosperous 
success, and wane away. As the earth grew older nobler. 
forms appeared, and this history from the tombs, like. 
that from the cradles of animals, shows throughout a 
gradual progress from simple to complex. 

2. Imperfection of the Geological Record,—If complete 
records of past ages were safely buried in great treasure- 
houses such as Frederic Harrison proposes to make for the 
enlightenment of posterity, then palzeontology would be easy. 
Then a genealogical tree connecting the Protist and Man 
would be possible, for we should have under our eyes what 
is now but a dream—a complete record of the past. 

The record of the rocks is often compared to a library 
in which shelves have been destroyed and confused, in 
which most of the sets of volumes are incomplete, and 
most of the individual books much damaged. When we 
consider the softness of many animals, the chances against 
their being entombed, and the history of the earth’s crust, 
our wonder is that the record is so complete as it is, that 
from “the strange graveyards of the buried past” we can 
learn so much about the life that once was. 

We must not suppose the record to be as imperfect as 
our knowledge of it. Thus many regions of the earth’s 
surface have been very partially studied, many have not 
been explored at all, many are inaccessible beneath the sea. 

As to the record, the rocks in which fossils are found 
are sedimentary rocks formed under water, often they have 
been unmade and remade, burnt and denuded. The 
chances against preservation are many. 


206 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


Soft animals rarely admit of preservation, those living 
on land and in the air are much less likely to be preserved 
than those living in water, the corpses of animals are 
often devoured or dissolved. Again the chances against 
preservation are many. 

3. Paleontological Series.—Imperfect as the geological 
record is, several marvellously complete series of related 
animals have been disentombed. Thus, a series of fossilised 
freshwater snails (Planorbis) has been carefully worked 
out; its extremes are very different, but the distinctions 
between any two of the intermediate forms are hardly 
perceptible. The same is true in regard to another set of 
freshwater snails (Pa/udina), and on a much larger scale 
among the extinct cuttlefishes (Ammonites, etc.) whose shells 
have been thoroughly preserved. The modern crocodiles 
are linked by many intermediate forms to their extinct 
ancestors, and the modern horse to its pigmy progenitors. 
In cases like these, the evidences of continuously progress- 
ive evolution are conclusive. 

4. Extinction of Types.—A few animals, such as some 
of the lamp-shells or Brachiopods, have persisted from almost 
the oldest rock-recorded ages till now. In most cases, 
however, the character of the family or order or class has 
gradually changed, and though the ancient forms are no 
longer represented, their descendants are with us. There 
is an extinction of individuals and a slow change of 
species. 

On the other hand there are not a few fossil animals 
which have become wholly extinct, whose type is not 
represented in the modern fauna. Thus there are no 
animals alive that can be regarded as the lineal descendants 
of Trilobites and Eurypterids, or of many of the ancient 
reptiles. There is no doubt that a race may die out. 
Many different kinds of heavily armoured Ganoid fishes 
abounded in the ages when the Old Red Sandstone was 
formed, but only seven different kinds are now alive. 
The lamp-shells and the sea-lilies, once very numerous, are 
now greatly restricted. Once there were giants among 
Amphibians, now almost all are pigmies. 


cuar.xir 9 The Past History of Animals 204 


It is difficult to explain why some of the old types 
disappeared. The extinction was never sudden. Formid- 
able competitors may have helped to weed out some; for 
cuttlefish would tend to exterminate trilobites, and voracious 
fishes would decimate cuttlefish, just as man himself is 
rapidly and inexcusably annihilating many kinds of beasts 
and birds. But, apart from the struggle with competitors, 
it is likely that some types were insufficiently plastic to save 
themselves from changes of environment, and it seems likely 
that others were victims to their own constitutions, becoming 
too large, or too sluggish, or too calcareous; or, on the 
other hand, too feverishly active. The “scouts” of evolution 
would be apt to become martyrs to progress; the “laggards” 
in the race would tend to become pillars of salt; the 
path of success was oftenest a via media of compromise. 
Samuel Butler has some evidence for saying that ‘‘ the race 
is not in the long run to the phenomenally swift, nor the 
battle to the phenomenally strong ; but to the good average 
all-round organism that is alike shy of radical crotchets and 
old world obstructiveness.” 

5. Various Difficulties.—Nowadays it seems natural 
to us to regard the fossils in the rocks as vestiges of a 
gradual progress or evolution. As some still find difficulty 
in accepting this interpretation, I shall refer to three 
difficulties occasionally raised. 

(2) It is said that the number of fossils in successive 
strata does not increase steadily as we ascend to modern 
times—that the numerical strength of the fauna is strangely 
irregular. Thus (in 1872) it was computed that 10,000 
species were known from the early Silurian rocks, while the 
much later Permian yielded only 300. But those who use 
such arguments should mention that a large number of the 
Silurian species were discovered by the marvellous industry 
of one man in a favourable locality, and that the rocks of 
the Permian system are ill adapted for the preservation of 
fossils. Moreover, we cannot compute the relative dura- 
tion of the different periods, we cannot infer evolutionary 
progress from the number of species, and we must make 
many allowances for the imperfections of the record. 


208 The Study of Animal Life PART II] 


(4) It is said that the occurrence of Fishes in the 
Silurian, and of many highly organised Invertebrates in the 
still earlier Cambrian, is inconsistent with a theory which 
would lead us to expect very simple fossil forms to begin 
with. But to say so is to forget that we have no concep- 
tion of the vast duration of periods like the Silurian and 
Cambrian, while the antecedent Archzean rocks in which we 
might look for traces of simple ancestral organisms have 
been shattered and altered too thoroughly to reveal any 
important secrets as to the earliest animals. 

(c) It is maintained that organic evolution proceeds very 
slowly, and that the geologists and biologists demand more 
millions than the experts in astronomical physics can grant 
them. But there is considerable difference of opinion as to 
the unthinkable length of time during which the earth may 
have been the home of life; we are apt to measure the rate 
of evolutionary change by the years of a man’s lifetime which 
lasts but for a geological moment ; and there is reason to 
believe that the simpler animals would change and take 
great steps of progress much more rapidly than those of 
high degree. 

6. Relative Antiquity of Animals,—I have not much 
satisfaction in submitting the following table showing 
the relative antiquity of the higher animals. Such a table 
is only an approximation; it does not suggest the great 
differences in the duration of the various periods, nor how 
the classes of animals waxed and waned, nor how some types 
in these classes dropped off while others persisted. But the 
general fact which the table shows is true,—in the course 
of time higher and higher forms of life have come into 
being. It is true that the remains of mammals are of more 
ancient date than those of birds, but it is likely that the 
remains of the earliest birds have still escaped discovery ; 
moreover, the earliest known mammalian remains seem to 
be of those of very simple types. 


The Past History of Animals 209 


CHAP. XIII 


Quaternary or 


Post-Tertiary 
Sig Pliocene Man? 
as . 
8s Miocene 
STS 
A ns Senn [nents SNnnnnnnn Net nntnnntnt) QOCCRCcnonn Olt tt nooo cocks 
) 
K° Eocene 
ES e Cretaceous 
os ; 
Zs Jurassic Birds 
SS pO Netedacdacless eicekead labroreeditacl ommeet saanten | teva eatieseariinns ; 
SS _ feeteaseseneefersereteetsefecsessesteedeetreneeesepeerteeetees treey eeslentet gions 
3 Triassic Mammals 
r . . 
Permian Reptiles 
8 
i I Preece (eetreeeeen (oc sucvmasuoais casas 
8 Carboniferous Amphibians 
x ee I slesa aunseahia autoacare meenae dies aacuAtaNeaa nana 
& Devonian 
Rd cee nenctfecccccccescpecsccceccnceeneceeenenenaeeerersesee seateneseeannsraeeereseoasseerrene ees 
8 . . . 
Silurian Fishes 
= Cambrian | Many Invertebrates 
Q 
( Archean 


CHAPTER XIV 


THE SIMPLEST ANIMALS 
1. The Simplest Forms of Life—2. Survey of Protozoa—3. The com- 
mon Ameba—4. Structure of the Protozoa—s. Life of Protozoa 
—6. Psychical Life of the Protocoa—]. History of the Protozoa 
8. Relation to the Earth—g. Relation to other Forms of Life— 
10. Relation to Man 


1. The Simplest Forms of Life,—lIt is likely that the first breath 
of life was in the water, for there most of the simplest animals and 
plants have their haunts. Simple they are, as an egg is simple 
when contrasted with a bird. They are (almost all) unit specks of 
living matter, each comparable to, but often more complex than, one 
of the numerous unit elements or cells which compose any higher 
plant or animal, moss or oak-tree, sponge or man. It is not merely 
because they are small that we cannot split them into separate parts 
different from one another, —size has little to do with complexity,— 
but rather because they are unit specks or single cells. But they are 
not ‘‘structureless””; in fact, old Ehrenberg, who described some of 
them in 1838 as “‘ perfect organisms” and fancied he saw stomachs, 
vessels, hearts, and other organs within them, was nearer the truth 
than those who reduce the Protozoa to the level of white of egg. 
Nor are they omnipresent, swarming in any drop of water. The 
clear water of daily use will generally disappoint, or rather please 
us by showing little trace of living things. But take a test-tube of 
water from a stagnant pool, hold it between your eyes and the light, 
and it is likely that you will see many forms of life. Simple plants 
and simple animals are there, the former represented by threads, 
ovals, and spheres in green, the latter by more mobile almost 
colourless specks or whitish motes which dance in the water. But 
besides these there are jerky swimmers whose appearance almost 
suggests their popularname of “‘ water-fleas,” and wriggling ‘‘ worms,” 


CHAP. XIV The Simplest Animals 211 


thinner than thread and lither than eels : both of these may be very 
small, but closer examination shows that they have parts and organs, 
that they are many-celled not single-celled animals. 

Vary the observations by taking water in which hay stems or other 
parts of dusty dead plants have been steeped for a few days, and even 
with the unaided eye you will see a thick crowd of the mobile whitish 
motes which, from their frequent occurrence in such infusions, are 
usually called Infusorians. Or if a piece of flesh be allowed to rot in 
an open vessel of water, the fluid becomes cloudy and a thin flaky scum 
gathers on the surface. Ifa drop of this turbid liquid be examined 
with a high power of the microscope, you will see small colourless 
rods and spheres, quivering together or rapidly moving in almost 
incalculable numbers. These, though without green colour, are the 
minutest forms of plant life; they are Bacteria or Bacilli, the 
practically omnipresent microbes, some of which as disease germs 
thin our human population, while others as cleansers help to make 
the earth a habitable dwelling-place. 

2. Survey of Protozoa.—Three great types of unicellular 
animals or Protozoa have been recognised in almost every classi- 
fication. 

(2) The Infusorians, so abundant in stagnant water, have a 
common character of activity expressed in the possession of actively 
mobile lashes of living matter known as cilia or flagella. Thus 
the slipper-animalcule (Paramecium) is covered with rows of lash- 
ing cilia, while smaller, equally common forms, generally known 
as Monads, are borne along by the undulatory movement of one or 
two long whips or flagella, The bell-animalcules (Vorticelfa) which 
live in crowds,—a white fringe on the water weeds,—are generally 
fixed by stalks, but are crowned with active cilia at the upper end of 
the somewhat urn-shaped cell. 

(6) In. marked contrast to these are the parasitic Protozoa, the 

. Gregarines, which infest most backboneless animals, notably the 
male reproductive organs of the earthworm or the gut of lobster and 
cockroach. Sluggishness and the absence of all locomotor pro- 
cesses are their characteristics. 

(c) Between these two extremes of activity and passivity there 
is a third type well represented by the much-talked-of 4meda which 
glides about on the mud of the pond, by the sun-animalcules 
(Actinospherium) which float in the clear water of brooks, by the 
limy-shelled, chalk-forming Foraminifera which move slowly on 
seaweeds or at the bottom of shallow water, or in some cases float 
at the surface of the sea, and by the flinty-shelled Radiolarians 
which live in the open ocean. In all these the living matter 
spreads out in thick or thin, stiff or plastic, free or interlacing pro- 
cesses, which often admit of a slow gliding motion, and are still 


212 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


more useful in surrounding minute food particles. To these root- 
like processes, which are capable of very considerable, often almost 
constant, change, these 
Protozoa owe their gen- 
eral name of Khizopods. 
In contrast to the two 
preceding types which 
have definite boundaries 
or ‘¢skins,” the Rhizo- 
pods are naked, and their 
living matter may over- 
flow at any point. 

As the Infusorians 
are for the most part 
provided with cilia from 
which flagella differ only 
in detail, we may speak 
of the type as ciliated ; 
the self-contained Gre- 
garines, often wrapped 
up within a sheath, we 
may call predominantly 
encysted ; while those 
forms which are inter- 
mediate between these 


Fic. 40.—A foraminifer (Polystomella strigillata) 

with interlacing processes of the living matter tWO extremes, and ex- 

flowing out on all sides. Magnified 10 times. hibit outflowing pro- 

a LEncyclop.; after Max cesses Of living matter, 

are called amceboid in 
reference to their most familiar type, the common Amceba. 

But though the members of each class are characterised by the 
predominance of one of the three phases of cell-life, they sometimes 
pass from one phase to another. Thus the ciliated or the amceboid 
units may become encysted. 


Fic. 41.—Protomyxa. 1, encysted ; 2, dividing into many units ; 3, these escap- 
ing as flagellate cells; 4, sinking into an amazboid phase ; 5, fusing into a 
plasmodium. (From Chambers's Lucyclop. ; after Haeckel. 


As the three phases represent the three physiological possibilities 


CHAP. XIV The Simplest Animals 213 


ot cell-life, it is natural to find that the very simplest Protozoa, such 
as Protomyxa, exhibit a cycle of amceboid, encysted, and flagellate 
phases, not having taken a decisive step along any one of the three 
great paths. Moreover, the cells of higher animals may be classified 
in the same way. The ciliated cells of the windpipe or the mobile 
spermatozoa correspond to Infusorians; mature ova, fat-cells, de- 
generate muscle-cells, correspond to Gregarines, while white blood- 
corpuscles and young ova are amoeboid. 

3. The common Amecba.—To find Amcebz, which is not 
always easy, some water and mud from a pond should be allowed 
to settle in a glass vessel. Samples from the surface of the sediment 
should then be removed in a glass tube or pipette, dropped on a 
slide, and patiently examined under the microscope. Among the 
débris, traversed in most cases by swift Infusorians, the sought - for 
Ameba may be seen, as an irregular mass of living matter, often 
obscured with various kinds of particles and minute Algee which it 
has engulfed, but hardly mistakable as it ploughs its way leisurely 
among the sediment, sending out blunt and changeful finger-like 
processes in the direction towards which it moves, and drawing 
in similar processes at the opposite side. From some objects it 
recoils, while others of an edible sort it surrounds with its blunt 
processes and gets outside of. Intense light makes it contract, and 
a minute drop of some obnoxious reagent causes it to round itself off 
and lie quiescent. Such is the simple animal which, in 1755, an 
early microscopist Résel von Rosenhof was delighted to describe, 
calling it the ‘* Proteus animalcule.” 

4. Structure of the Protozoa.—Most of these Protozoa are 
units or single cells, but this contrast between them and the higher 
animals is lessened by the fact that many Infusorians, some 
Radiolarians, and some of the very lowest forms live inclose combina- 
tion, a number of apparent individuals being substantially united in 
co-operation. In two quite different ways this compound life of some 
Protozoa arises. The ‘‘Flower of Tan” (Fulzgo or Athalium 
septicum) which in the summer months spreads as a yellowish slime 
on the bark of the tanyard, and supplies the student with the 
*¢ largest available masses of undifferentiated protoplasm,” arises from 
the flowing together and fusion of a number of smaller amoeboid 
units. But in some Infusorians and Radiolarians the colony 
arises quite otherwise. Protozoa multiply by division ; each unit 
splits into two which thenceforth live separate lives, and by and 
by themselves divide. Suppose, however, that the unit divide 
incompletely ; suppose that the daughter-units, distinct though 
unsevered, redivide, and that the process is continued ; a “ colonial ” 
Protozoon is the result. In this case the units do not flow together, 
they were never separated. But the ‘‘ wisdom” of some of these 


214 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


early associations has been justified in their far-off children, for in 
this way the many-celled animals began. 

The cell-substance of a Protozoon is living matter, along with 
nutritive materials which are approaching that climax, and waste 
materials into which some of the cell substance has disintegrated. 
The cell has a kernel or nucleus, or more than one, essential to its 
complete life. There are bubbles of water taken in along with 
food particles, and in nearly all freshwater forms there are one or 
two special regions of internal activity, pulsating cavities or con- 
tractile vacuoles, which become large and small sometimes rhythmic- 
ally, and may burst open on the surface of the cell. They are be- 
lieved to help in getting rid of waste, and also in internal circulation. 
There is a rind in the Infusorians and Gregarines, and shells of flint 
and lime are characteristic of most Foraminifers and Radiolarians. 

5. Life of Protozoa.—tThe life-histories of the Protozoa are 
very varied, but some chapters are common to most. They expend 
energy in movement ; they regain this by feeding; their income 
exceeds their expenditure, and they grow; at the limit of growth 
they reproduce by dividing into two or many daughter-units; in 
certain states two individuals combine, either interchanging nuclear 
elements (in the ciliated Infusorians) or fusing together (as in some 
Rhizopods); in drought or in untoward conditions, or before 
manifold division, they often draw themselves together and encyst 
within a sweated-off sheath. 

The Protozoa often multiply very rapidly. One divides into 
two, the two become four, and in rapid progression the numbers 
increase. On Maupas’s calculation a single Infuscrian may in four 
days have a progeny of a million. The same observer has shed a 
new light on another process—that of conjugation, the temporary 
or permanent union of two Protozoa, which in the ciliated Infusorians 
involves an interchange of nuclear particles. In November 1885, 
Maupas isolated an Infusorian (S¢ylonichia) and observed its genera- 
tions till March 1886. By that time there had been two hundred and 
fifteen generations produced by ordinary division, but since these 
lowly organisms do not conjugate with near relatives, conjugation 
had not occurred. The result, corroborated in other cases, was 
striking. The whole family became exhausted, small, and 
“senile”; they ceased to divide or even to feed; their nuclei 
underwent u strange degeneration; they began to die. But 
individuals removed before the process had gone too far were 
observed to conjugate with unrelated forms and to live on. The 
inference was obvious. Conjugation in these Infusorians is of little 
moment to any two individuals ; during long periods it need never 
occur, but it is essential to the continued life of the species. ‘‘ It 
is a necessary condition of their eternal youth.” 


CHAP. XIV The Simplest Animals 215 


We must return, however, to the everyday life of the Protozoa. 
Rhizopods move by means of outflowing processes of their living 
matter which stream out at one corner and are drawn in at another ; 
the Infusorians move more rapidly by undulating flagella or by 
numerous cilia which work like flexible oars; the parasitic 
Gregarines without any definite locomotor structures sometimes 
writhe sluggishly. A few Infusorians have a spasmodic leaping or 
springing motion, while the activity of others (like Vorticel/a) which 
in adult life are fixed, is restricted to the contraction and expansion 
of a stalk and to the action of cilia around the opening which serves 
asa mouth. Avcell/a is aided in its movements by the formation of 
gas bubbles in different parts of its cell-substance. 

The food consists of other Protozoa, of-minute Algz, and of 
organic débris, simply engulfed by the Amcebee, wafted by cilia 
into the ‘‘ mouth” of most Infusorians. The parasitic Gregarines 
absorb the débris of the cells or tissues of the animals in which they 
live, while not a few suck the cell-contents of freshwater Algze like 
Spirogyra. A few Protozoa are green, and some are able to use 
carbonic acid after the manner of plants. Almost all Radiolarians 
and a few Foraminifers live in constant and mutually helpful 
partnership or symbiosis with small Algee which flourish within 
their cell-substance. 

As to the other functions, the cells absorb oxygen and liberate 
carbonic acid, digest the food-particles and excrete waste, produce 
cysts or elaborate shells. 

6. Psychical Life of the Protozoa.—We linger over the 
Protozoa because they illumine the beginnings of many activities, 
and we cannot leave them without asking what light they cast upon 
the conscious life of higher animals. Is the future quite hidden in 
these simple organisms or are there hints of it ? 

According to some, the Protozoa, with frequently rapid and 
useful movements, with capacities for finding food and avoiding 
danger, with beautiful and intricate shells, are endowed with the 
will and intelligence of higher forms of life. According to others, 
their motions are arbitrary and without choice, they are only much 
more complex than those of the potassium ball which darts about 
on the surface of water, the organisms are drawn by their food 
instead of finding it, their powers of selection are sublimed chemical 
affinities, their protective cysts are quite necessary results of partial 
death, and their houses are but crystallisations. In both interpreta- 
tions there is some truth, but the first credits the Protozoa with too 
much, the second with too little. 

Cienkowski marvelled over the way in which Vampyrella sought 
and found a Sg:vogyra filament and proceeded to suck its contents ; 
Engelmann emphasised the wonderful power of adjustment in Arce/la 


216 The Study of Animal Life PART UI 


which evolves gas bubbles and thus rises or rights itself when cap- 
sized, and also detected perception and decision in the motions of 
young Vorticelle or in the pursuit of one unit by another ; Oscar 
Schmidt granted them only ‘‘a very dim general feeling” and the 
power of responding in different ways to definite stimuli ; Schneider 
believed that they acted on impulses based upon definite impressions 
of contact ; Moebius would credit them with the power of reminis- 
cence and Eimer with will. 

Romanes finds evidence of the power of discriminative selection 
among the protoplasmic organisms, and he quotes in illustration Dr. 
Carpenter’s account of the making of shells. ‘‘Certain minute 
particles of living jelly, having no visible differentiation of organs 
. . . build up ‘tests* or casings of the most regular geometrical 
symmetry of form and of the most artificial construction. . . . 
From the same sandy bottom one species picks up the coarser quartz 
grains, cements them together with phosphate of iron (?) which must 
be secreted from their own substance, and thus constructs a flask- 
shaped ‘test’ having a short neck and a single large orifice. Another 
picks up the finer grains and puts them together with the same 
cement into perfectly spherical ‘tests’ of the most extraordinary 
finish, perforated by numerous small tubes, disposed at pretty regular 
intervals. Another selects the minutest sand-grains and the terminal 
points of sponge spicules, and works these up together apparently 
with no cement at all, but by the ‘laying’ of the spicules into 
perfect spheres, like homceopathic globules, each having a single 
fissured orifice.” This selecting power is marvellous; we cannot 
explain it; the animals are alive and they behave thus. But it 
must be remembered that even ‘dead’ substances have attractive 
affinities for some things in preference to others, that the cells of 
roots and those lining the food-canal of an animal or floating in its 
blood show a power of selection. Moreover, if we begin with a 
unit which provides itself with a coating of sponge spicules, 
at first perhaps because they were handiest, it is not difficult to 
understand why the future generations of that species should con- 
tinue to gather these minute needles. Being simply separated parts 
of their parents, whose living matter had become accustomed to 
the stimulus of sponge spicules, the descendants naturally sustain 
the tradition. This organic memory all Protozoa must have, 
for the young are separated parts of the parents. 

Haeckel was one of the first (1876) to urge the necessity of 
recognising the ‘‘soul” of the cell. He maintained that the con- 
tinuity of organic life led one to assume a similar continuity of 
psychical life, that an egg-cell had in it not only the potency of 
forming tissues and organs but the rudiments of a higher life as well, 
that the Protozoa likewise must be regarded not only as physical 


CHAP. XIV The Simplest Animals 217 


but as psychical, in fact that the two are inseparable aspects of one 
reality. ‘The cell-soul in the monistic sense is the sum-total of 
the energies embodied in the protoplasm, and is as inseparable 
from the cell-substance as the human soul from the nervous 
system.” For several years Verworn has been investigating the 
psychical life of the Protozoa. He has conducted his researches 
with great care and thoroughness, observing the animals both in 
their natural life and in artificial conditions. I shall cite his con- 
_ clusions, translating them freely: ‘‘ An investigator of the psychical 
processes in Protists (simple forms of life) has to face two distinct 
problems. The first is comparative, and inquires into the grade of 
psychical development which the Protists may exhibit—the known 
standard being found of course in man ; the second is physiological, 
and inquires into the nature of these psychical processes. Since 
we know these only through the movements in which they are 
expressed, the investigation is primarily a study of the movements 
of Protists. 

‘* On a superficial observation of these movements the impression 
arises in the observer’s mind that they are the result of higher 
psychical processes, like the consciously willed activities of men. 
Especially the spontaneous movements of advance and recoil, of 
testing and searching, give us the impression of being intentional 
and voluntary, since no external stimulus can account for them ; 
while even some of the movements provoked by stimuli appear on 
account of their marked aptness to arise from conscious sensation 
and determination. 

«But a critical study of the results yielded by an investigation 
of spontaneous and stimulated movements warrants a more secure 
judgment than that of the superficial observer, and leads to a con- 
clusion opposed to his. To this conclusion we are led, that none 
of the higher psychical processes, such as conscious sensations, 
representations, thoughts, determinations, or conscious acts of will, 
are exhibited by Protists. A number of criteria show that the 
movements are in part impulsive and automatic, and in part reflex, 
and in both cases expressions of unconscious psychical processes. 

*¢ This opinion is corroborated by an examination of the structure 
of these Protists, for this does not seem such as would make it 
possible for the individual to have an idea of its own unified self, 
and the absence of self-consciousness excludes the higher psychical 
processes. Small fragments cut from a Protist cell continue to 
make the same movements as they made while parts of the intact 
organism. Each fragment is an independent centre for itself. 
There is no evidence that the nucleus of the organism is a psychical 
centre. There is no unified Psyche. 

«Since the characteristic movements persist in such small frag- 


218 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


ments, they cannot be the expression of any individual consciousness, 
for the individuality has been cut in pieces.” 

The dilemma is obvious ; either there are no psychical processes 
in the Protists, or they are inseparable from the molecular changes 
which occur in the parts of the material substance. 

If no psychical processes occur in the Protists, where do they 
begin? There is no distinct point in the animal series at which a 
nervous system may be said to make its first appearance. If there 
are none, even rudimentarily, in the Protists, then these simple 
organisms do not potentially include the life of higher organisms. 
If there are none in the Protists, are there any in the germs from 
which men develop ? 

Verworn seizes the other horn of the dilemma, maintaining that 
the superficial observers are wrong in crediting the Protozoa with 
their own intelligence or with some of it, but right in concluding 
that psychical processes of some sort are there. But since he 
cannot in any way locate these processes, since he finds that even 
small fragments retain their life for a time and behave much as the 
entire cells did, he maintains that all life is psychical. 

7. History of the Protozoa.—We know that the Protozoa 
have lived on the earth for untold ages, for the shells of Fora- 
minifera and others may be disentombed from almost the oldest 
rocks. The word Protozoa, a translation of the German Urthzere or 
primitive animals, suggests that the Protozoa are not only the 
simplest, but the first animals, or the unprogressive descendants of 
these. Nowadays we can hardly feign to consider this proposition 
startling, for we know that all the higher animals, including our- 
selves, begin life at the beginning again as single cells. From the 
division and redivision of an apparently simple fertilised egg-cell an 
embryo is built up which grows from stage to stage till it is 
hatched, let us say, as a chick. It is only necessary to extend this 
to the wider history of the race. What the egg is to the chick the 
original Protozoa were to the animal series; the present Protozoa 
are like eggs which have lived on as such without making much 
progress. 

We do not know how the Protozoa began to be upon the earth, 
whether they originated from not living matter or in some yet more 
mysterious way. The German naturalist Oken, a prominent type 
of the school of ‘* Natural Philosophers” who flourished about the 
beginning of this century, dreamed of a primitive living slime 
(Urschleim) which arose in the sea from inorganic material. His 
dream was prophetic of the modern discovery of very simple forms 
of life, in connection with one of which there is an interesting and 
instructive story. That one, perhaps I should say that supposed 
one, was called Bathybius, and since those who are eager to make 


CHAP. XIV The Simplest Animals 219 


points against science (that is to say against knowledge) always tell 
the story wrongly, I shall make a digression to tell it rightly. 

In 1857 Captain Dayman, in charge of a vessel engaged in con- 
nection with cable-laying, discovered on the submarine Atlantic 
plateau the abundant presence of slimy material which looked as if 
it were alive. Preserved portions of this formless slime were after- 
wards described by Huxley, and he named the supposed organism, 
partly from its habitat, partly after his friend Haeckel, Bathybzus 
Haeckelit. On the Porcupine expedition Professors Wyville Thomson 
and Carpenter observed it in its fresh state, and Haeckel afterwards 
described some preserved specimens. Its interest lay in its 
simplicity and apparent abundance; Oken’s dream seemed to be 
coming true; it seemed as if life were a-making in the still depths. 

But when the Challenger expedition went forth, and the bed of 
the ocean was explored for the first time carefully, the organism 
Bathybius was nowhere to be found. But this was not all; the 
cruellest blow was yet tocome. Dr. John Murray saw reason to 
suspect that Bathybius was not an organism at all, that it could be 
made in a test-tube, and was nothing but a gelatinous form of 
sulphate of lime precipitated from the sea water by the action of the 
alcohol in the preserving vessels. He renounced Bathybius, 
Wyville Thomson acquiesced, Huxley surrendered his organism to 
the chemists, and the obscurantists rejoiced exceedingly over the 
mare’s nest. Bathybius became famous, it was trotted out to 
illustrate the fallibility of science, a useful if it were not a somewhat 
superfluous service. 

But the non-existence of Bathybius was not proved by the fact 
that the Challenger explorers failed to find it, nor was it certain 
that Murray’s destructive criticism covered all the facts. Haeckel 
clung with characteristic pertinacity to Bathybius, and his con- 
stancy has been to some extent justified by the fact that in 1875 
Bessels, on a North Polar expedition, dredged from 92 fathoms of 
water in Smith’s Sound abundant quantities of a closely- similar 
slime. He observed its vital movements, and called it Proto- 
Bathybius. It may be that it consists of the broken-off portions 
of Foraminifera ; we require to know yet more about it, but I have 
said enough to show that it is unfair to stop telling the story with 
the words ‘‘mare’s nest.” But whether there be a Bathybius, a 
Proto-Bathybius, or no Bathybius at all, we are as students of science 
compelled to confess our complete ignorance as to the origin 
of life. 

8. Relation to the Harth,— The floor of the sea for a 
variable number of miles (not exceeding 300) from the shore is 
covered with a heterogeneous deposit, washed in great part from 
the nearest continent. In this deposit shells of Foraminifera usually 


220 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


occur, but they become more numerous farther from the land, 
where the floor of the sea is often covered with a whitish “ooze,” 
most of which consists of Foraminifera which in dying have sunk 
from the surface to the bottom. They are forming the chalk of a 
possible future, just as many chalk-cliffs and pure limestones repre- 
sent the ooze of a distant past. In other regions the hard parts 
of Radiolarians or Diatoms (small plants) or Pteropods (minute mol- 
luscs) are very abundant. As the Foraminifers have made much 
of the chalk, so Radiolarians have formed less important siliceous 
deposits, such as the Barbados Earth, from which Ehrenberg 
described no fewer than 278 species. At marine depths greater 
than 2500 fathoms the Globigerina or other Foraminifer shells are 
no longer present, not because there are none at the surface, but 
apparently owing to the solution of the shells before they reach 
such a vast depth. Here the floor is covered with a very fine 
reddish or brownish deposit, often called ‘‘red-clay,” a very 
heterogeneous deposit of meteoric and volcanic dust and of residues 
of surface-animals. Along with this, in some of the very deepest 
parts, ¢.g. of the Central Pacific, there are accumulations of Radio- 
larian shells, which do not readily dissolve.1 

9. Relation to other Forms of Life.—On the one hand 
the Protozoa are devourers of organic débris and the enemies of 
many small plants; on the other hand they form the fundamental 
food of higher animals, helping, for instance, to make that thin sea- 
soup on which many depend. Moreover, among them there are 
many parasites both on vegetable and animal hosts. 

10. Relation to Man,—In many indirect ways these firstlings 
affect human life, nor are there wanting direct points of contact ; 
witness a few Protozoa parasites in man, an Amoeba, some Gre- 
garines, and some Infusorians, which are very trivial, however, in 
comparison with the numerous plant-parasites—the Bacteria. 

Among the earliest human records of Protozoa is the notice 
which Herodotus and Strabo take of the large coin-like Nummu- 
lites, the ‘‘ Pharaoh’s beans” of popular fancy. But the minute- 
ness of most Protozoa kept them out of sight for ages. They were 
virtually discovered by Leeuwenhoek (b. 1632) about the middle of 
the seventeenth century, and soon afterwards demonstrated by 
Hooke to the Royal Society of London, the members of which 
signed an affidavit that they had really seen them! In 1755 Résel 
von Rosenhof discovered the Amoeba, or ‘‘ Proteus animalcule ;” 
but his discovery was ineffective till Dujardin in 1835 demonstrated 
the simplicity of the Foraminifers, and till Von Siebold in 1845 


1 For details, see conveniently H. R. Mill's Realm of Nature (Lond. 
1892), 


CHAP. XIV The Simplest Animals 221 


showed that Infusoria were single cells comparable to those which 
make up a higher animal. For the resemblance between some of 
the spirally twisted shells of Foraminifera and those of the 
immensely larger Molluscan Ammonites and Nautili led many to 
maintain that the Foraminifera were minute predecessors or else 
dwindling dwarfs of the Ammonites. So Ehrenberg (1838) figured 
the presence of many organs within the Infusorian cell. But as 
the microscope was perfected naturalists were soon convinced that 
the Protozoa were unit masses of living matter. This is their great 
interest to us; they are, as it were, higher organisms analysed into 
their component elements, We see them passing through cycles 
of phases, from ciliated to amceboid, from amoeboid to encysted, 
cycles which shed light upon changes both of health and of disease 
in higher animals. Again, they seem like ova and spermatozoa 
which have never got on any farther. 


CHAPTER XV 


BACKBONELESS ANIMALS 
V 
1. Sponges—2. Stinging-Animals or Celenterata—3. ‘‘ Worms” — 
4. Echinoderms—5. Arthropods—6. Molluscs 


1. Sponges.—Sponges are many-celled animals without organs, 
with little division of labour among their cells. A true ‘‘ body” is 
only beginning among sponges. 

Adult sponges are sedentary, and plant-like in their growth. 
With the exception of the freshwater sponge (Spongilla) they live 
in the sea fixed to the rocks, to seaweeds and to animals, or to the 
muddy bottom at slight or at great depths. They feed on micro- 
scopic organisms and particles, borne in with currents of water 
which continually flow through the sponge. The sponge is a 
Venice-like city of cells, penetrated by canals, in which incoming 
and outflowing currents are kept up by the lashing activity of 
internal ciliated cells. These ciliated cells, on which the whole life 
of the sponge depends, line the canals, but are especially developed 
in little clusters or ciliated chambers. The currents are drawn in 
through very small pores all over the surface ; they usually flow out 
through much larger crater-like openings. 

Sponges feed easily and well, and many of them grow out in 
buds and branches. A form which was at first a' simple cup may 
grow into a broad disc or into a tree-like system. And as trees are 
blown out of shape by the wind, so sponges are influenced by the 
currents which play around them, as well as by the nature of the 
objects on which they are fixed. Like many other passive 
organisms, sponges almost always have a well-developed skeleton, 
made of flinty needles and threads, of spicules of lime, or of fibres 
of horn-like stuff. While sponges do not rise high in organic 
rank, they have many internal complications and much beauty. 

Sponges may be classified according to their skeleton, as 


CHAP. XV Backboneless Animats 223 


calcareous, flinty, and horny. (a) The calcareous forms with 
needles of lime have a world-wide distribution in the sea, from 
between tide-marks to depths of 300 to 400 fathoms. They often 
retain a cup-like form, but vary greatly in the complexity of their 
canals. The sac-like Sycandva (or Grantia) compressa is common 
on British shores. (4) The siliceous sponges are more numerous, 
diverse, and complicated, and the flinty needles or threads are often 
combined with a fibrous “‘horny ” skeleton. Venus’-Flower-Basket 
(Zuplectella) has a glassy skeleton of great beauty, Mermaids’ 
Gloves (Chalina oculata) with needles of flint and horny fibres is 
often thrown up on the beach, the Crumb-of-Bread Sponge 
(Halichondria panicea) spreads over the low-tide rocks, Some 
have strange habits, witness Come which bores holes in oyster 
shells, or Suberites domuncula which clothes the outside of a whelk 
or buckie shell tenanted by a hermit-Crab. Unique in habitat is 
the freshwater sponge (.Sfomgz//a) common in some canals and lakes, 
notable for plant-like greenness, and for the vicissitudes of its life- 
history. (c) The ‘‘horny” sponges which have a fibrous skeleton 
but no proper spicules are well represented by the bath-sponges 
(Zuspongia) which thrive well off Mediterranean coasts, where they 
are farmed and even bedded out. 

Sponges are ancient but unprogressive animals. Their sedentary 
habits, from which only the embryos for a short time escape, have 
been fatal to further progress. They show tissues as it were in the 
making. They are living thickets in which many small animals 
play hide-and-seek. Burrowing worms often do them much harm, 
but from many enemies they are protected by their skeletons and by 
their bad taste. 

2. Stinging-Animals or Celenterata.—It is difficult to 
find a convenient name for the jellyfish and zoophytes, sea-anemones 
and corals, and many other beautiful animals which are called 
Ccelenterates ; but the fact that almost all have poisonous stinging 
+ lassoes in some of their skin-cells suggests that which we now use. 

Representatives of the chief divisions may be sometimes found 
in a pool by the shore. Ruddy sea-anemones, which some call 
sea-roses, nestle in the nooks of the rocks ; floating in the pool and 
throbbing gently is a jellyfish left by the tide; fringing the rocks 
are various zoophytes, or, if we construe the name backwards plant- 
like animals; besides these, and hardly visible in the clear water, 
are minute translucent bells some of which have a strange relation- 
ship with zoophytes; and there are yet other exquisitely delicate, 
slightly iridescent globes—the Ctenophores which move by comb- 
like fringes of cilia. But we must search an inland pool to find 
one of the very simplest members of this class—the freshwater 
Hydra which hangs from the floating duckweed and other plants. 


224 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


This Aydra is a tubular animal often about quarter of an inch in 
length. One end of the tube is fixed, the other bears the mouth 
surrounded by a crown of mobile tentacles. It is so simple that 
cut-off fragments if not too minute may grow into complete animals ; 
when well fed, the Hydra buds out little polypes like itself, and 
these are eventually set free. 

If we suppose the budding of Hydra continued a hundred- 
fold, till a branched colony of connected individuals is formed, 
we have an idea of a hydroid or zoophyte colony. For a zoophyte 
is a colony of many hydra-like polypes, which are supported by a 
continuous outer framework and share a common life. Numerous 
as may be the ‘‘ persons” on a branched hydroid, all have arisen 
from one more or less Hydra-like individual. 

Sometimes, however, there is a marked division of labour in such 
a colony, as in Aydractinda which has nutritive, reproductive, 
sensitive, and perhaps also protective ‘‘ persons,”—three or four 
castes into which the colony is divided. The difference between 
nutritive and reproductive members is often well marked, and 
this has a special interest in the case of many zoophytes. For 
many of these, especially among those known as Tubularians and 
Campanularians, have reproductive individuals which are set adrift 
as small swimming-bells or medusoids, somewhat like miniature 
jellyfish. A fixed plant-like, asexual hydroid colony buds off 
free-swimming, sexual medusoids, from the fertilised eggs of which 
embryos develop which grow into hydroids. This is known as 
alternation of generations, and is a remarkable illustration of 
activity and passivity combined in one life-cycle. 

But all the miniature jellyfish in the sea are not the liberated 
reproductive buds of hydroid colonies. Some which are in 
structure exceedingly like the liberated medusoids never have any 
connection with a hydroid. Their embryos grow into medusoids 
like the parents. Quite distinct from these medusoids, though 
sometimes superficially like them, are the true jellyfishes which are 
sometimes stranded in great numbers on the beach. These medusz 
belong to a different series, and some of their features link them 
rather to the sea-anemones than to the hydroids. 

The sea-anemones and the corals are tubular animals whose 
mouths are encircled by tentacles, but they are more complicated 
internally than the polypes of the Hydra or hydroid type. For the 
latter are simple tubes, while the sea-anemones and their relatives 
have turned-in lips which make a kind of gullet, and the inside 
tube thus formed is connected with the outer wall of the body by 
many radiating partitions some idea of which can be gained by 
looking at the skeletons or shells of many corals. Related to the 
sea-anemones but different in some details, are many colonies, of 


CHAP. XV Backboneless Animals 225 


which Dead-men’s-fingers (Alcyonium digitatum) is a common 
type. Animals resembling sea-anemones have often much lime 
about them, and the same is true of others which resemble 
Alcyontum ; in both cases we call these calcareous forms corals. 

In this bird’s-eye view of Stinging-animals, we have recognised 
the great types, but we have left others of minor importance out of 
account, especially certain corals belonging to the hydroid series 
and known as Millepores, also the Portuguese Man-of-War and 
its relatives (Siphonophora), which are colonies of more or less 
medusoid individuals with much division of labour, and lastly the 
Ctenophores, such as Beroé and Pleurobrachia, which represent the 
climax of activity among Ccelenterates. 

A brief recapitulation will be useful : 

First Sertes—Hydroid and Medusoid types (Hydrozoa) :— 

(1) The freshwater Hydra and a few forms like it. 

(2) The hydroids or zoophytes, each of which may be regarded 
as a compound much-branched Hydra ; including (a) many 
whose reproductive persons are not liberated, especially 
Sertularians and Plumularians ;—(4) many whose repro- 
ductive persons are liberated as swimming bells or 
medusoids, especially Tubularians and Campanularians. 

(3) Free medusoids, anatomically like the liberated bells of 2 (4), 
but without any connection with zoophytes. 

(4) A few colonial medusoids such as the Portuguese Man-of- 
War (Physalia). 

(5) A few hydroid corals or Millepores. 

Second Series—Jellyfish and Sea-Anemone types (Scyphozoa) :— 

(1) The true jellyfishes or Meduse, including (a) a form like 
Pelagia which is free-swimming all its life through, (4) the 
common Azrelia whose embryos settle down and become 
polypes from which the future free-swimming jellyfishes 
are budded off, (c) the more or less sedentary jellyfish 
known as Lucernarians. 

(2) The sea-anemones and their relatives, including (a) sea- 
anemones proper (¢.g, Acéémia) and their related reef- 
building coral-colonies (e.g. star-corals Astrea, brain-coral 
Maandrina) ; and (6) Dead-men’s-fingers (A/cyonium) and 
others like it, also with related corals, e.g. the organ-pipe 
coral (Zubi~ora musica) and the ‘noble coral” of com- 
merce (Corallium rubrum), 

Third Sertes— 

The Ctenophores, which are markedly contrasted with corals, 
being free and light and active. Many (e.g. Beroé and 
Pleurobrachia) swarm in our seas in summer, iridescent in 
daylight, phosphorescent at night. They differ in many 


Q 


N 
N 
n 


The Study of Animal Life PART IIT 


ways from other Ccelenterates, thus the characteristic 
stinging cells are modified into adhesive cells. 

The first and second series, separated by differences of structure 
and development, are yet parallel. In both there are polype-types ; 
in both medusoid types; in both there are single individuals and 
colonies of individuals; in both there are “corals.” We may 
compare a Aydra with a sea-anemone, a medusoid with a jelly- 
fish, a hydroid colony with Dead-men’s- fingers, Millepores with 


Fic. 42.—The alternation of generations in the common jellyfish 4uvelia. 19 
the free-swimming embryo; 2, the embryo settled down; 3, 4, 5, 6, the de- 


veloping asexual stages, or hydra-tubz; 7, 8, the formation of a pile of 


individuals by transverse budding; 9, the liberation of these individuals ; 
10, 11, their progress towards the free-swimming sexual medusa form. (From 
the Evolution of Sex ; after Haeckel.) 


the commoner reef-corals. Moreover, we may compare a medusoid 
liberated from a hydroid with Azrelza liberated from its fixed polype- 
stage, and permanently-free medusoids with jellyfishes like Pelagia. 
These are physiological parallels. 

The sedentary polypes are somewhat sluggish, with a tendency 
to bud and to form shells or skeletons of some kind. The free- 
swimming medusoid types are active, they rarely bud, they do not 
form skeletons, but their activity is sometimes expressed in 


CHAP. XV Backboneless Animals 227 


phosphorescence, and their fuller life is associated with the develop- 
ment of sense-organs and a more compacted nervous system. In 
both sets the food usually consists of small organisms, in securing 
which the tentacles and the stinging cells are of use. 

All the Stinging-animals are marine except the species of Hydra, 
a minute relative called Microhydra, the hydroid Cordylophora which 
occurs in brackish water and in canals, a strange form Polypodium 
which is parasitic in its youth on the eggs of the Russian sturgeon 
or sterlet, and a freshwater jellyfish (Lzmmnocodium) which was 
found in the tanks at Kew. The rest live in the sea. Hydroids 
grow on rocks and shells and on the backs of crabs and other 
animals which they mask ; sea-anemones live on the shore-rocks 
—hbut not a few are found at considerable depths ; the medusoid 
types frequent the opener sea where Siphonophores and Ctenophores 
bear them company. 

Various kinds of corals should be contrasted. Dead-men’s- 
fingers with numerous jagged spicules of lime in its flesh is just 
beginning to be coralline. Similar spicules have been fused together 
in an external tube in the organ-pipe coral. In the red coral 
the calcareous material forms an axis around which the individuals 
are clustered. Very different are the reef-building corals, where 
the cup in which each individual lived is more or less well marked 
according as it has remained distinct or fused with its neighbours, 
and where an image of the fleshy partitions of the sea-anemone- 
like animal is seen in the radiating septa of lime. 

Corals are passive, and like many animals of similar habit have 
calcareous shells, but how do they get the carbonate of lime of 
which these are composed? Is that salt—by no means abundant in 
sea-water—plentiful near coral-reefs, or is there a double-decomposi- 
tion between the abundant calcium sulphate and the coral’s waste- 
products, as has been suggested by Irvine and Murray? On what 
do the corals feed, for they seem always to be empty? Do their 
bright pigments enable them, as Hickson suggests, to feed like 
plants on carbonic acid ? 

The struggle for standing-room should also be thought of, and 
the throngs of gaily-coloured animals which browse and hide on 
the coral banks. 

Many of the Stinging-animals have forms and colours which 
delight our eyes, and the quaint partnerships between sea-anemones 
and crabs are interesting. 

But it is through corals that Coelenterates come into closest touch 
with human life. For the stinging of bathers by jellyfish is a 
minor matter, and the thousands which are cast upon the beach 
are of no use as manure, being little more than animated sea- 
water. 


228 The Study of Animal Life PART 111 


As sponges showed tissues in the making, so among Stinging- 
animals organs begin—eyes and ears, nerve-rings, and special 
reproductive structures. The zoologist has much to learn in regard 
to the alternation of hydroid and medusoid in one life-cycle, the 
division of labour in Aydractinia and other colonies, and the 
meaning and making of a skeleton, Nor can we forget the long 
past in which there were ancient coral reefs, and types of coral 
hardly represented now, and strange Graptolites whose nature we 
do not yet clearly understand. 

We begin the series of many-celled animals with Sponges and 
Ceelenterates, partly because they are on the whole simplest, but 
more precisely because their types of structure are least removed 
from that two-layered sac-like embryo or gastrula which recurs in 
the life-history of most animals, and which we have much warrant 
for regarding as a hint of what the first successful many-celled 
animals were like. The Sponges and Ccelenterates differ from the 
higher animals : (1) In retaining the symmetry of this gastrula, in 
being like it radially symmetrical, and in so growing that the axis 
extending from the mouth to the opposite pole corresponds to the 
long axis of the embryo; (2) in being two-layered animals, for 
between the outer skin and the lining of the internal food-cavity 
there is only a more or less indefinite jelly instead of a definite 
stratum of cells; (3) in having only one internal cavity, instead of 
having, like most other animals, a body-cavity within which a dis- 
tinct food-cavity lies. 

3. “Worms.” — This title is one of convenience, without 
strict justification. For there is no class of ‘‘worms,” but an 
assemblage of classes which have little in common. ‘* Worm” 
is little more than a name for a shape, most of the animals so 
called differing from anemones and jellyfish in having head, tail, and 
sides. The simplest worms were apparently the first many- 
celled animals to move persistently head foremost, thus acquiring 
distinct bilateral symmetry, and a definite nervous centre or brain 
in that region which had most experience—the head. In our 
survey we are helped a little by the fact that many consist of a 
series of rings or segments, while others are all one piece or unseg- 
mented. It is generally true that the latter are in structure simpler 
and more primitive than the former. 

st Set of Worms. Plathelminthes or flat worms. rst 
Class. —Turbellaria or Planarians.— These are small worms, 
living in the sea or in fresh water, or occasionally in damp earth, 
covered externally with cilia, very simple in structure, usually 
feeding on minute animals. The genus Plaxaria, common in fresh 
water; green species of Vortex and Convoluta, which are said by 
some to owe their colour to minute partner algze ; M/icrostoma, which 


CHAP. Xv Backboneless Animals 229 


by budding forms temporary chains of eight or sixteen individuals 
as if suggesting how a ringed worm might arise ; Gada, with a hint 
of internal segmentation ; and two parasitic genera—Grafilla and 
Anoplodium—may be mentioned as representatives of this class. 
You will find specimens by collecting the waterweeds from a pond 
or seaweeds from a shore-pool, and the simplicity of some may be 
demonstrated by observing that when they are cut in two each half 
lives and grows. 

2nd Class. —Trematoda or Flukes. These are parasitic ‘‘ worms,” 
living outside or inside other animals, often flat or leaf-like in 
form, provided with adhesive and absorbing suckers. Those which 
live as ectoparasites, ¢.g. on the skin of fishes, have usually a 
simple history ; while those which are internal boarders have an 
intricate life-cycle, requiring to pass from one host to another of a 
different kind if their development is to be fulfilled. Thus the 
liver-fluke (Déstomum hepaticum), which causes the disease of liver- 
rot in sheep, and sometimes destroys a million in one year in Britain 
alone, has an eventful history. From the bile-ducts of the sheep 
the embryos pass by the food-canal to the exterior. If they reach a 
pool of water they develop, quit their egg-shells, and become for 
a few hours free-swimming. They knock against many things, but 
when they come in contact with a small water-snail (Lymneus 
truncatulus) they fasten to it, bore their way in, and, losing their 
locomotor cilia, encyst themselves. They grow and multiply in a 
somewhat asexual way. Cells within the body of the encysted 
embryo give rise to a second generation quite different in form. 
The second generation similarly produces a third, and so on. 
Finally, a generation of little tailed flukes arises ; these leave the 
water-snail, leave the water too, settle on blades of grass, and lose 
their tails. If they be eaten by a sheep they develop into adult 
sexual flukes, Others have not less eventful life-cycles, but that of 
the liver-fluke is most thoroughly known. If you dissect a frog 
you are likely to find Polystomum integerrimum in the lungs or 
bladder ; it begins as a parasite of the tadpole, and takes two or 
three years to become mature in the frog. Quaint are the little 
forms known as Digorpa which fasten on the gills of minnows, and 
unite in pairs for life, forming double animals (Diplozoon) ; and 
hardly less strange is Gyvodactylus, another parasite on freshwater 
fishes, for three generations are often found together, one within 
the other. The most formidable fluke-parasite of man is Bilharzia, 
or Dist h tobium, common in Africa. 

3rd Class. Cestoda or Tapeworms. These are all internal 
parasites, and, with the exception of one (Archigetes), which fulfils 
its life in the little river-worm Zdzfex, the adults always occur in 
the food-canal of backboned animals. Like the flukes, they have 


230 The Study of Animal Life PART If 


adhesive suckers, and sometimes hooks as well; unlike flukes and 
planarians, which have a food-canal, they absorb the juices of their 
hosts through their skins, and have no mouth or gut. Like 
the endo-parasitic flukes, the tapeworms have (except Archigetes) 
intricate: life-histories. Both Turbellarians and Trematodes are 
small, rarely more than an inch at most in length, but the tape- 
worms may measure several feet. In the adult Zena solium, 
which is sometimes found in the intestines of man, we see a small 
head like that of a pin; it is fixed by hooks and suckers to the 
wall of the food-canal; it buds off a long chain of ‘‘joints,” each 
of which is complete in itself. As these joints are pushed by con- 
tinued budding farther and farther from the head, they become 
larger, and distended with eggs, and even with embryos, for the 
bisexual tapeworm seems able to fertilise itself, which is a very rare 
thing among animals. The terminal joints of the chain are set free, 
one or a few at a time, and they pass down the food-canal to the 
exterior. The tiny embryos which they contain when fully ripe 
are encased in firm shells. It may be that some of them are eaten 
by a pig, the shells are dissolved away in the food-canal, small six- 
hooked embryos emerge. These bore their way into the muscles of 
the pig and lie dormant, increasing in size however, becoming 
little bladders, and forming a tiny head. They are called bladder- 
worms, and it was not till about the middle of this century that they 
were recognised as the young stages of the tapeworm. For if the 
diseased pig be killed and its flesh eaten (especially if half-cooked) 
by man, then each bladder-worm may become an adult sexual tape- 
worm. The bladder part is of no importance, but the head fixes 
itself and buds off a chain. For many others the story is similar ; 
the bladder-worm of the ox becomes another tapeworm (Ze@nia 
saginata) in man ; the bladder-worm of the pike or turbot becomes 
another (Bothrdocephalus latus); the bladder-worm of the rabbit 
becomes one of the tapeworms of the dog, that of the mouse passes 
to the cat, and so on. A bladder-worm which forms many heads 
destroys the brain of sheep, etc., and has its tapeworm stage (Z7enia 
cenurus) in dog or wolf. Another huge bladder-worm, which has also 
many heads, and sometimes kills men, has also its tapeworm stage 
(Tenia echinococcus) in the dog. But enough of these vicious 
cycles. 

2nd Set of Worms. Ribbon Worms or Nemerteans— 
4th Class, Nemertea.—In pleasing contrast to the flukes and tape- 
worms, the Nemerteans are free-living ‘‘ worms.” They are 
mostly marine, often brightly coloured, almost always elongated, 
always covered with cilia. There is a distinct food-canal with 
a posterior opening, a blood-vascular system for the first time, 
a well-developed nervous system, a remarkable protrusible ‘ pro- 


CHAP. XV Backboneless Animals 231 


boscis” lying in a sheath along the back, a pair of enigmatical 
ciliated pits on the head. The sexes are almost always separate. 
Almost all Nemerteans are carnivorous, but two or three haunt other 
animals in a manner which leads one to suspect some parasitism ; 
thus Malacobdella lives within the shells of bivalve molluscs. We 
find many of them under loose stones by the sea-shore; one 
beautiful form, Limeus mardnus, sometimes measures over twelve 
feet in length. Some, such as Cerebvatulus, break very readily into 
parts, even on slight provocation, and these parts are said to be 
able to regrow the whole. To speculative zoologists, the Nemer- 
teans are of great interest on account of the vertebrate affinities 
which some of their structures suggest. Thus the sheath of the 
‘* proboscis” has been compared with the vertebrate notochord 
(the structure which precedes and is replaced by a backbone), and 
the two ciliated head-pits with gill-slits. 

3rd Set of Worms. Nemathelminthes or Round- 
Worms—sth Class, Nematoda or ‘Thread - Worms.—The 
‘“‘worms” of this class are usually long and cylindrical, and the 
small ones are like threads. The skin is firm, the body is 
muscular ; in most a simple food-canal extends from end to end of 
the body-cavity now for the first time distinct; the sexes are 
separate. Many of the Nematodes live in damp earth and in 
rottenness ; many are, during part of their life, parasitic in animals 
or plants. We have already noticed how long some of them— 
‘¢ paste -eels,” ‘‘ vinegar-eels,” etc.—may lie in a dried-up state 
without dying. The life-histories are often full of vicissitudes ; thus 
the mildew-worm (Zydenchus triticd) passes from the earth into the 
ears of wheat, and many others make a similar change; the female 
of Spherularia bombi migrates from damp earth into humble-bees, 
and there produces young which find their way out; others, ag. 
some of the thread-worms found in man (Oxyuris, Trichocephalus), 
pass from water into their hosts ; others are transferred from one 
host to another; as in the case of the Zrichtna with which pigs 
are infected by eating rats, and men infected by eating diseased 
pigs, or the small 72/aria sanguznis hominis, sometimes found in 
the blood of man, which seems to pass its youth in a mosquito. 
Somewhat different from the other Nematodes are those of which 
the horse-hair worm Gordius is atype. They dre sometimes found 
inside animals (water-insects, molluscs, fish, frog, etc.), at other 
times they appear in great numbers in the pools, being, according 
to popular superstition, vivified horse-hairs, 

6th Class, Acanthocephala.—Including one peculiar genus of 
parasites (Zchinorhynchus). 

4th Series of Worms. The Annelids or Ringed Worms 
—v7th Class, Chetopoda or Bristle-footed ‘‘worms.”—In the 


232 The Study of Animal Life PART II] 


earthworms (Lumbricus, etc.), in the freshwater worms (/Vais, 
Tubifex, etc.), in the lobworms (Arenzcola piscatorum), and in the 
sea-worms (Wereis, Aphrodite, etc.), all of which are ranked as 
Cheetopods, the body is divided into a series of similar rings or 
segments, and there are always some, and often very many, bristles 
on the outer surface. The segments are not mere external rings, 
but divisions of the body often partially partitioned off internally, 
and there is usually some repetition of internal organs. Thus in 
each segment there are often two little kidney-tubes or nephridia, 
while reproductive organs may occur in segment after segment. 
Moreover, there are often two feet on each ring. The nervous 
system consists of a dorsal brain and of a double nerve-cord lying 
along the ventral surface. The nerve-cord has in each segment a 
pair of nerve-centres or ganglia, and divides in the head region to 
form a ring round the gullet united with the brain above. The 
existence of nerve-centres for each segment makes each ring to some 
extent independent, but the brain rules all. This type of nervous 
system represents a great step of progress ; it is very different from 
that of Stinging-animals, which lies diffusely in the skin or forms 
a ring around the circumference ; different from that of the lower 
**worms,” where the nerve-cords from the brain usually run along 
the sides of the body; different from that of molluscs, where the 
nerve-centres are fewer and tend to be concentrated in the head ; 
different finally from the central nervous system of backboned animals, 
for that is wholly dorsal. But the type characteristic of ringed 
‘* worms ”—a dorsal brain and a ventral chain of ganglia—is also 
characteristic of crustaceans, insects, and related forms. 

Of bristle-footed ‘* worms,” there are two great sets, the earth- 
worms and the sea-worms. The former, including the common 
soil-makers and a few giants, such as the Tasmanian AZegascolides, 
sometimes about six feet long, have bristles but no feet ; sense- 
organs, feelers, and breathing organs are undeveloped as one would 
expect in subterranean animals. The sea-worms, on the other 
hand, have usually stump-like bristly feet, and eyes and tentacles 
and gills, but there is much difference between those which swim 
freely in the sea (e.g. Alcéope and Tomofteris and some Nereids) 
and the lobworms which burrow and make countless castings upon 
the flat sandy shores, or those which inhabit tubes of lime or 
sandy particles (e.g. Serpula, Spirorbis, and Lanice or Terebella 
conchilega). The earthworms with comparatively few bristles 
(Oligocheeta) are bisexual, while almost all the marine worms with 
many bristles (Polychaeta) have separate sexes. Moreover, those of 
the first series usually lay their eggs in cocoons, within which the 
embryos develop without any metamorphosis, while the sea-worms, 
though they sometimes form cocoons, have free-swimming larvae 


CHAP. XV Backboneless Animals 233 


usually very different from the adults—little barrel-shaped or pear- 
shaped ciliated creatures known as Trochospheres. 

Some of the Chztopods multiply not only sexually, but asexually 
by dividing into two or by giving off buds from various parts of 
their body. Strange branching growths, which eventually separate 
into individuals, are well illustrated by the freshwater Mazs, and 


YS 
(puts 
x 3 i a ‘ 

) 


* - 

NY NYY! Wt 

Ny) HO GS 
. a 


Ae 
ena 


Fic. 43.—A budding marine worm (Sy@lis ramosa). From Evolution of Sex ; 
after M‘Intosh’s Challenger Report.) 


still better by a marine worm, Sy///s ramosa, which almost forms 
a network. 

Many sea-worms have much beauty, which some of their names, 
such as Werers, Aphrodite, Alciope, suggest, and which is said to 
have induced a specialist to call his seven daughters after them. 

Along with the Chzetopods, we include some other forms too 
unfamiliar to find more than mention here, the Myzostomata which 
form gall-like growths on the feather-stars which they infest, the 
strange Bonel/ia in which the microscopic male lives as a parasite 
within the female, and some very simple forms which are sometimes 
called Archi-Annelids. 


234 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


8th Class, Hirudinea or Discophora or Leeches.—These are 
blood-sucking animals, which often cling for a long time to their 
victims. They live in salt and in fresh water, and sometimes on 
land. The body is elastic and ringed, but the external markings 
do not correspond to the internal segments. There are no legs, 
but the mouth is suctorial, and there is another adhesive sucker 
posteriorly. The body-cavity is almost obliterated by a growth 
of spongy tissue, whereas that of Cheetopods is roomy. Leeches 
are hermaphrodite, and lay their eggs in cocoons, within which 
the young develop without metamorphosis. 

The medicinal leeches (Hrudo medicinalzs) live in slow streams 
and marshes, creeping about with their suckers or sometimes 
swimming lithely, preying upon fishes and amphibians, and both 
larger and smaller animals. They fix themselves firmly, bite with 
their three semicircular saw-like tooth-plates, and gorge themselves 
with blood. When they get an opportunity they make the most 
of it, filling the many pockets of their food-canal. The blood is 
kept from coagulating by means of a secretion, and on its store the 
leech may live for many months. 

The horse-leech (Hemopis sanguisuga) is common in Britain 
and élsewhere. The voracious Azdastoma is rather carnivorous 
than parasitic. The land-leeches (e.g. Haemadipsa ceylonica), 
though small and thin, are very troublesome, sucking the blood of 
man and beast. Among the others are the eight-eyed Mephelis of 
our ponds, the little C/epsize which sometimes is found with its young 
attached to it, the warty marine Portobdel/a which fastens on rays, 
Pisctcola on perch and carp, Branchellion with numerous lateral 
leaflets of skin, and the largest leech—the South American Macro- 
bdella valdiviana which is said to attain a length of over two 
feet. 

Possibly related to the Annelid series are two other 
classes— 

oth Class—Cheetognatha, including two genera of small arrow- 
like marine “‘ worms,” Sagztta and Spadella. 

toth Class—Rotifera, ‘‘ wheel animalcules,’? abundant and 
exquisitely beautiful animals inhabiting fresh and salt 
water and damp moss. The head-region bears a ciliated 
structure, whose activity produces the impression of a 
swiftly rotating wheel. Many of them seem to be 
entirely parthenogenetic, Some can survive being made 
as dry as dust. 

Fifth set of Worms—a doubtful combination including— 

11th Class—Sipunculoidea, ‘‘ spoon-worms”” living in the sea, 
freely or in tubes, e.g. Szpzencelus. 

12th Class—Phoronidea, including one genus, Phoronis. 


CHAP. XV Backboneless Animals 235 


‘ 

13th Class—Polyzoa or Bryozoa, with one exception forming 
colonies by budding, in fresh water or in the sea, ¢.g. the 
common sea-mats or horn-wracks (//ustra). 

14th Class—Brachiopoda or Lamp-shells, a class of marine 
shelled animals once much richer in members, now 
decadent, They have a superficial, but only a superficial, 
resemblance to Molluscs. 

I have not catalogued all these classes of “worms” without a 
purpose. To ignore their diversity would have lent a false simplicity 
to our survey. If you gain only this idea that there is a great 
variety—a mob—of worm-like animals, which zoologists have not yet 
reduced to order, you have gained a true idea. The ‘‘ worms” lie 
as it were in a central pool among backboneless animals, from which 
have flowed many streams of progressive life. They have affinities 
with Echinoderms, with Insects, with Molluscs, with Vertebrates. 

To practical people the study of ‘‘ worms” has no little interest. 
The work of earthworms is pre-eminently important ; the sea- 
worms are often used as bait; the leech was once the physician’s 
constant companion ; numerous parasitic worms injure man, his 
domesticated stock, and the crops of his fields. 

4. Echinodermata.—lIn contrast to the ‘‘ Worms,” the series 
including starfishes, brittle-stars, feather-stars, sea-urchins, and 
sea-cucumbers, is well defined. 

The Echinodermata are often ranked next the stinging animals, 
mainly because many of the adults have a radiate symmetry, as 
jellyfishes and sea-anemones have. But radiate symmetry is a 
superficial character, perhaps originally due to a sedentary habit of 
life in which all sides of the animal were equally affected. More- 
over, the larvee of Echinoderms are bilaterally symmetrical, that is to 
say, they are divisible into halves along a median plane. We 
place Echinoderms after and not before ‘‘ worms,” because the 
simplest worm-like animals are much simpler, much nearer the 
hypothetical gastrula-like ancestor than are any Echinoderms, and 
also because it is likely that Echinoderms originated from some 
worm type or other. 

Haeckel used to hold a theory of the starfish which was in some 
ways suggestive. You know the five-rayed appearance of the 
animal like a conventional star; you have perhaps watched it 
moving slowly in a deep rock pool by the shore; you have 
perhaps discovered that it will surrender one of its arms when you 
try to capture it. Now Haeckel compared the starfish to a colony 
of five worms united in the centre. Each “arm” or ‘‘ray” is 
complete in itself. Each has a nerve-cord along the ventral 
surface, a little eye at the tip, prolongations of the food-canal, blood- 
vessels, and reproductive organs. Each is anatomically comparable 


236 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


to a worm. Furthermore, when an arm is separated, it may bud 
out other four arms and thus recreate an entire starfish. Each arm 
has therefore some physiological independence. 

But there is no likelihood that a starfish arose as a colony of 
worms ; the facts of development do not corroborate the sug- 
gestion. 

Of Echinoderms there are seven classes, two of which are 
wholly extinct. These—the Cystoids and Blastoids—are of great 
interest because of their relationship with the feather-stars or 
Crinoids, which stand somewhat apart from the other four extant 
classes, The Cystoids are more primitive than the Crinoids, and 
connect them with the starfishes or Asteroids. The Asteroids are 
nearly related to the brittle-stars or Ophiuroids, and they are also 
linked to the sea-urchins or Echinoids. These in turn are the 
nearest allies of the Holothuroids or sea-cucumbers. 

The Echinoderms are all marine. The sea-urchins and Holo- 
thurians are mud-cleansing scavengers; the Holothurians and 
Crinoids feed for the most part on small organisms, though the 
former are sometimes mud-eaters ; the starfishes are more emphatic- 
ally carnivorous, and often engulf small molluscs. 

Among starfishes, sea-urchins, and sea-cucumbers, we find 
occasional cases of prolonged external connection between the 
mothers and the young. 

The Echinoderms are sluggish animals, though many brittle- 
stars are lithe gymnasts, and though the commonest Crinoids 
(Comatulids, such as the rosy feather-star, Amtedon rosacea), differ 
from their stalked relatives and adolescent stages in being to some 
extent swimmers. Perhaps the sluggishness is expressed in the 
abundance of lime in the skin and other parts; for, as the name 
suggests, the Echinoderms are thorny-skinned, being usually pro- 
tected by calcareous plates and spines. The sea-cucumbers are the 
most muscular and the least limy, indeed in some almost the only 
calcareous parts are a few anchors and plates scattered in the skin. 

Another frequent characteristic is the radial symmetry, but we 
remember that the larvee are bilateral. 

Very important is the development ot a peculiar system of 
canals and suctorial ‘‘ tube-feet ’—the water-vascular system. By 
means of the tube-feet the starfishes and sea-urchins move, in the 
others their chief use seems to be in connection with respiration, 
and it is likely that in some at least they also help in excretion. 

Another characteristic of the Echinoderms is the strangeness of 
the larval forms. For not only are they very different from the 
parents, and very remarkable in form, but in no case do they 
grow directly into the adult. The development is “indirect,” 
the larva does not become the adult; the foundations of the 


CHAP. Xv Backboneless Animals 237 


Fic. 44.—A Holothurian (Cucumaria crocea) with its young attached to its skin. 
(From Lvolution of Sex ; after Challenger Narrative.) 


238 The Study of Animal Life PART It 


adult are laid anew within the body of the larva, which is absorbed 
or partly rejected. 

Not only the starfishes but also the brittle-stars and the 
feather-stars often surrender their arms when captured, or even 
when slightly irritated, and « part or a remnant can in favourable 
conditions regrow the whole. The Holothurian Synapta breaks 
readily into pieces, and others contract themselves so forcibly that 
the internal organs are extruded. 

The relations of Echinoderms to other animals are many. A 
little fish, /erasfer, goes in and out of Holothurians; the de- 
generate Myzostomata form galls on the arms of Crinoids; star- 
fishes are deadly enemies of oysters. On the other hand, some 
sea-snails and fishes prey upon Echinoderms in spite of their 
grittiness. Except that the unlaid eggs of some sea-urchins are 
edible, and that some sea-cucumbers are considered delicacies, the 
Echinoderms hardly come into direct contact with human life. 

5. Arthropods,—Lobsters, centipedes, insects, spiders, agree 
with the Annelid ‘ worms” in being built up of a series of rings 
or segments. Some or all of these segments bear limbs, and these 
limbs‘ are jointed, as the term Arthropod implies. The skin 
forms an external sheath or cuticle of a stuff called chitin, and 
this firm sheath helps us to understand how the limbs became 
well-jointed. The chitin seems in some way antagonistic to the 
occurrence of ciliated cells, for none seem to occur in this large 
series unless it be in the strange type Perzsatus. The chitin has 
also to do with the moulting or cuticle-casting which is common 
in the series, for the cuticle is generally rigid and does not expand 
as the body grows, hence it has to be cast and a new one made. 
Finally, Arthropods have a nervous system like that of Annelids— 
a double dorsal brain connected by a ring round the gullet with a 
double chain of ganglia along the ventral surface. But the life of 
most Arthropods is more highly pitched than that of Annelids. 
The sense-organs are more highly developed, brains are larger and 
more complex, the ganglia of the ventral chain tend to become 
concentrated ; there is division of labour among the appendages ; 
there are new internal organs such as a heart; the whole body 
is better knit together. A crayfish may part with his claw and 
grow another in its place, but the animal will not survive being cut 
in two as some kinds of Annelids do, 

The series includes at least five classes :— 

Crustacea, almost all aquatic, and breathing by gills. 
Protracheata, represented by the genus Peripatus. 
Myriapoda, centipedes and millipedes. 

Insecta, more or less aerial. 

Arachnida, spiders, scorpions, mites, etc. 


CHAP. XV Backboneless Animals 239 


The members of the last four classes usually breathe by means 
of air-tubes or trachez, which penetrate into every part of the body, 
or in the case of spiders and scorpions, by ‘‘lung-books,”’ which 
seem like concentrated and plaited trachee. The King-crab 
(Zémuius), which is very often ranked along with Arachnids, is 
aquatic, and breathes by peculiar ‘‘ gill-books.” 

(2) Crustacea.—Except the wood-lice, which live under bark 
and stones, the land-crabs which visit the sea only at the breeding 


Fic. 45.—Nauplius of Sacculina. (From Fritz Miller.) 


time, and some shore-forms which live in great part above the tide- 
mark, the Crustaceans are aquatic animals, and usually breathe by 
gills. Each segment of the body usually bears a pair of append- 
ages, and each appendage is typically double. Among these ap- 
pendages much division of labour is often exhibited, some being 
sensory, others masticatory, others locomotor. In the higher forms 
the life-history is often long and circuitous, with a succession of 
larval stages. 

The lower Crustaceans are grouped together as Entomostraca. 
They are often small and simple in structure; the number of 


240 The Study of Animal Life PART II] 


segments and appendages varies greatly. The little larva which 
hatches from the egg is usually a ‘‘ Nauplius”—an unsegmented 
creature with only three pairs of appendages and a median eye. 

The brine-shrimps (Artemia), the related genus Branchipus, 
the old-fashioned freshwater Afzs ; the common water-flea Daphnia 
and its relatives, like Zeftodora and Afoina, are united in the order 
of Phyllopods. 

The small ‘‘ water-fleas” of which Cyfris is a very common 
representative, and which are very abundant in sea and lake, form 
the order of Ostracods. 

Another ‘‘ water-flea ” Cyclops and many more or less degenerate 
‘¢fish-lice”” and other ectoparasites (e.g. Chondracanthus, Caligus, 
Lern@a) are known as Copepods. The free-swimming forms often 
occur in great swarms and are devoured by fishes. 

The acorn-shells (Zalanus) crusting the rocks, the barnacles 
(Zefas) pendent from floating ‘‘timber,” and the degenerate Sacculina 
under the tail of crabs, represent the order Cirripedia. 

The higher Crustaceans are grouped together as Malacostraca. 
The body usually consists of nineteen segments, five forming the 
head, eight the thorax, six the abdomen or tail. In most cases the 
larva is hatched at a higher level of structure than the Nauplius 
represents, but the shrimp-like Peneus begins life as a Nauplius 
while the crab is hatched as a Zoea, the lobster in a yet higher 
form, and the crayfish as a miniature adult. 

Simplest of these higher Crustaceans, in some ways like a 
survivor of their hypothetical ancestors, is the marine genus Vela/ia, 
but we are more familiar with the Amphipods (e.g. Gammarus) 
which jerk themselves along sideways or shelter under stones both 
in fresh and salt water. The wood-louse Onzscus has counterparts 
(Asellus, Idotea) on the shore, and several remarkable parasitic 
relatives. Among the highest forms are the long-tailed lobsters 
(Homarus, Palinurus), and crayfishes (Astacus), and shrimps 
(Crangon), and prawns (Palemon, Pandalus); the soft-tailed 
hermit crabs (Pagurus); and the short -tailed crabs (e.g. Cancer, 
Carcinus, Dromia). 

(6) Protracheata,—leripatus. This remarkable genus, repre- 


Fic. 46.—Peripatus. (From Chambers’s Excyclop.; after Moseley.) 


sented by about a dozen widely-distributed species, seems to be a 
survivor of the ancestral insects. Worm-like or caterpillar-like in 


CHAP. XV Backboneless Animals 241 


appearance, with a soft and beautiful skin, with unjointed legs, with 
the halves of the ventral nerve-cord far apart, and with many other 
remarkable features, it has for us this special interest that it 
possesses the air-tubes characteristic of insects and also little 
kidney-tubes similar to those of Annelids. 

(c) Myriapoda.—Centipedes and Millipedes.—These animals 
have very uniform bodies, there is little division of labour among the 
numerous appendages. The head is distinct, and bears besides the 
pair of antennz (which Feripatus and Insects also have) two pairs 
of jaws. The Centipedes are flattened, carnivorous, and poisonous ; 
the Millipedes are cylindrical, vegetarian, and innocuous ; moreover, 
they have two pairs of legs to most of their segments. 


Fic. 47.-—Winged male and wingless female of Pneumora, a kind of 
grasshopper. (From Darwin.) 


(¢) Insecta,—Insects are the birds of the backboneless series. 
Like birds they are on an average active, most have the power of 
flight, many are gaily coloured, sense-organs and brains are often 
highly developed. 

Contrasted with Per?patus and Myriapods, they have a more 
compact body, with fewer but more efficient limbs. They are 
Arthropods, which are usually winged in adult life, breathe air 
by means of trachee, and have frequently a metamorphosis in their 
life-history. To this definition must be added the anatomical facts 
that the adult body is divided into three regions, (1) a head with 
three pairs of mouth-appendages (=legs) and a pair of sensitive 
outgrowths (antennz or feelers) in front of the mouth, (2) a thorax 
with three pairs of walking legs, and usually two pairs of wings, 
and (3) an abdomen without appendages, unless occasional stings, 
egg-laying organs, etc., be remnants of these. 

R 


242 The Study of Animal Life PART Ill 


The wings are very characteristic. They are flattened sacs of 
skin, into which air-tubes, blood-spaces, and nerves extend. It is 
possible that they had originally a respiratory, rather than a 
locomotor function, and that increased activity induced by bettered 
respiration made them into flying wings. 

The breathing is effected by means of the numerous air-tubes 
or tracheze which open externally on the sides and send branches 
to every corner of the body. As the air is thus taken to all 
the tissues, the blood-vascular system has little definiteness, though 
there is (as in other Arthropods) a dorsal contractile heart. The 
larvee of some insects, ¢.g. dragonflies, mayflies, etc., live in 
the water, and the tracheze cannot open to the exterior (else the 
creature would drown), but they are sometimes spread out on 
wing-like flaps of skin (‘‘ tracheal gills”), or arranged around the 
terminal portion of the food-canal in which currents of water are 
kept up. 

The student should learn something about the different mouth- 
organs of insects and the kinds of food which they eat ; about the 
various modes of locomotion, for insects ‘‘ walk, run, and jump with 
the quadrupeds, fly with the birds, glide with the serpents, and 
swim with the fish;” about the bright colours of many, and the 
development of their senses. 

In the simplest insects—the old-fashioned wingless Thysanura 
and Collembola—the young creature which escapes from the egg- 
shell is a miniature adult. There is no metamorphosis. So with 
cockroaches and locusts, lice and bugs; except that the young are 
small, have undeveloped reproductive organs, and have no wings, 
they are like the parents, and all the more when the parents (e.g. 
lice) also are wingless. 

In cicadas there is a slight but instructive difference between 
larvee and adults. The full-grown insects live among herbage, the 
young live in the ground, and the anterior legs of the larvee are 
adapted for burrowing. Moreover, the larval life ends in a sleep 
from which an adult awakes. But much more marked is the differ- 
ence between the aquatic larvee of mayflies and dragonflies and the 
aerial adults, in which we have an instance of more thorough though 
still incomplete metamorphosis. 

Different, however, is the life of all higher insects—butterflies 
and beetles, flies and bees. From the egg-shell there emerges a 
larva (maggot, grub, or caterpillar), which often lives an active 
voracious life, growing much, and moulting often. Rich in stores 
of fatty food, it falls into a longer quiescence than that associated 
with previous moults and becomes a pupa, nymph, or chrysalis. 
In this stage, often within the shelter of a silken cocoon, great 
transformations occur; the body is undone and rebuilt, wings bud 


CHAP. XV Backboneless Animals 243 


out, the appendages of the adult are formed, and out of the pupal 
husk there emerges an imago, an insect fully formed. 

(¢) Arachnida.—Spiders, Scorpions, Mites, etc.—This class is 
unsatisfactorily large and heterogeneous. In many the body is 
divided into two regions, the head and breast (cephalothorax), with 
two pairs of mouth parts and four pairs of walking legs, the 
abdomen with no appendages. Respiration may be effected by 
the skin in some mites, by tracheze in other mites, by trachee plus 
“‘lung-books” in many spiders, by ‘‘lung-books” alone in other 
spiders, by ‘gill-books” in the divergent king-crab. 

The scorpions with a poisoning weapon at the tip of the tail, 
the little book-scorpions (Chelifer), the long-legged harvest-men 
(e.g. Phalangium); the spiders proper—spinners, nest - makers, 
hunters; the mites; the strange parasite (femtastomum) in the 
dog’s nose; the quaint king-crab (Lzmudus)—last of a lost race, 
with which the ancient Trilobites and Eurypterids were connected ; 
all these are usually ranked as Arachnids ! 

6. Molluscs.—It seems strange that animals, the majority of 
which are provided with hard shells of lime, should be called 
mollusca ; for that term first used by Linneeus is a Latinised version 
of the Greek malakza, which means soft. Aristotle applied it 
originally to the cuttlefish, which are practically without shells, so 
that its first use was natural enough, but the subsequent history of 
the word has been strange. 

Cockle, mussel, clam, and oyster; snail and slug, whelk and lim- 
pet ; octopus, squid, and pearly nautilus ; what common character- 
istics have they? Most of them have a bias towards sluggishness, 
and on the shields of lime which most of them bear, do we not read 
the legend, ‘‘ castles of indolence”? But this sluggishness is only 
an average character, and the shell often thins away. The scallop 
(ecten) and the swimming Zzwa are active compared with the 
oyster, and they have thinner shells ; the snails which creep slowly 
between tides or on the floor of the sea are heavily weighted, while 
the sea-butterflies (Pteropods) have light shells, and most cuttlefish 
have none at all. 

The shell is very distinctive, but we are not able to state 
definitely how it is formed or what it means. In most of the 
embryo molluscs which have been studied there is a little pit or 
*¢ shell-gland” in which a shell begins to be formed, but the shell 
of the adult is in all cases made by a single or double fold of skin 
known as the ‘‘ mantle.” In some cases where the shell seems to 
be absent, ¢.g. in some slugs, a degenerate remnant is still to be found 
beneath the skin, while in other cases (eg. most cuttlefish) its 
absence is to be explained as a loss, since related ancestral species 
possess it. There are, however, two or three primitive forms 


244 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


(Chetoderma, Neomenta) where an incipient shell is represented 
only by a few spicules or plates of lime. 

The shell is made by the folded skin or ‘‘ mantle”; it consists 
for the most part of carbonate of lime along with a complex organic 
substance called conchiolin ; it shows three layers, of which the 
outermost is somewhat soft and without lime, while the innermost 
shines with mother-of-pearl iridescence. The whole product is a 
cuticle—something formed from the skin; its varied colours and 
forms are beautiful ; it is a protective shield ; but there are many 


” 


Fic. 48.—The common octopus. (From Chambers’s Zxcyclop. ; after Brehm.) 


questions about shells which we cannot answer. Where does the 
carbonate of lime come from, since that salt is often far from 
abundant in the water in which most molluscs live? Have they 
the power of changing the abundant sulphate of lime in sea-water 
into carbonate of lime, perhaps by an interaction with waste products 
excreted from the skin? Is the shell an expression of the constitu- 
tional sluggishness of the animal since it seems on the whole to be 
most massive in the most sluggish, least so in the most active forms ? 

Most molluscs are marine, on the shore, in the open sea, in the 
great depths; there are also many freshwater forms, ¢.g. the 
mussels Avodon and Unio, and the snails, Lymneus, Planorbis, 


CHAP. XV Backboneless Animals 245 


and Paludina ; the terrestrial snails and slugs are legion. Among 
those of the shore the naked Nudibranchs are often in colour and 
form protectively adapted to their surroundings; those of the open 
sea (Heteropods, Pteropods, and many cuttlefish) are active and 
carnivorous, with light shells or none; in the dark depths many 
are blind or in other ways rudimentary, but food seems to be so 
abundant that there is almost no need to struggle for it. 

As to diet, there are three kinds of eaters—carnivores, such as 
the active swimmers we have mentioned, besides the whelks and 
many other burglars who bore through their neighbours’ shells, and 
the Zestacella slugs ; vegetarians, like the periwinkle, the snail, and 
most slugs; and thirdly, almost all the bivalves, which feed on 
microscopic plants and animals, and on organic débris wafted to 
the mouth by the lashing of the cilia on the gills and lips. In this 
connection it is important to notice that all molluscs except bivalves 
have in their mouths a rasping ribbon or toothed tongue (vadula, 
odontophore) by which they grate, file, or bore with marked effect. 
Of parasites there are few, but one Gasteropod, Lyxtoconcha 
mirabilis, which lives inside the Holothurian Synapta, is very 
remarkable in its degeneration. It starts in life as a vigorous 
embryo like that of most marine snails, it becomes a mere sac of 
reproductive organs and elements. 

In structure, molluscs differ remarkably from the arthropods 
and higher ‘‘ worms” in the absence of segments and serial 
appendages. They are not divided into rings, and they have no 
legs. 

To begin with, they were doubtless (bilaterally) symmetrical 
animals, and this symmetry is retained in primitive forms like the 
eight-shelled CAztow and in the bivalves. But most of the snails 
are twisted and lop-sided, they cannot be symmetrically halved. 
For this asymmetry the strange dorsal hump formed by the viscera, 
and the tendency that the single shell would have to fall to one side, 
are sometimes blamed. That this lop-sidedness is not necessarily 
a defect, but rather the reverse, is evident from the success not 
only of the snail tribe but of many other asymmetrical animals. 

The skin has a remarkable fold (double in the bivalves) known 
as the ‘‘ mantle,” the importance of which in making the shell we 
have already recognised. Another very characteristic structure is 
the so-called ‘‘ foot,” 2 muscular protrusion of the ventral surface, 
an organ used in creeping and swimming, leaping and boring, but 
almost absent in the sedentary oysters. 

We rank the molluscs high among backboneless animals, partly 
because of the nervous system, which here as elsewhere is a 
dominating characteristic. There are fewer nerve centres than in 
most Arthropods or in higher ‘‘ worms,” but this is in most cases 


246 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


a sign of concentration. There is a (cerebral) pair with nerves 
which supply the head region, another (f/eura/) pair with nerves to 
the sides and viscera, a third (gedal) pair whose nerves govern the 
foot, and ofien other accessory centres of which the most important 
are visceral, In the somewhat primitive eight-shelled C/ztom and its 
neighbours, the nervous system is the most readily harmonised with 
that of other Invertebrates ; in bivalves the three pairs of centres 
are far apart ; in most snails and in cuttlefish the three are con- 
centrated in the head-regions, and it is those forms with concentrated 
ganglia which show some signs of cleverness and emotion. 

Life-History.—Most molluscs pass through two larval stages 
before they acquire their characteristic adult appearance. The 
first is interesting because it is virtually the same as the young 
stage of many ‘‘worms.” It is a barrel-shaped or pear-like 
embryo with a ring of locomotor cilia in front of the mouth, and is 
known as a Zrochosphere. 

After a while this changes into a more characteristic form called 
the Veliger. It bears on its head a ciliated cushion or velum often 
produced into lobes ; the body has a ventral ‘‘ foot” and a dorsal 
‘‘shell-gland.” In aquatic Gasteropods the visceral hump begins 
to appear at this stage. 

The eggs of cuttlefish differ from those of other molluscs in their 
rich supply of yolk, which serves for a prolonged period as capital 
for the young, and the two larval stages noticed above are skipped 
over. There are other interesting modifications in the life-history 
of terrestrial and freshwater forms, witness the little larvae of the 
freshwater mussel which are kept within the gills of the mother till 
the approach of some sticklebacks or other fish, to which the 
liberated young then fix themselves. 

History.—The shells of molluscs are well preserved in the rocks, 
and paleontologists have been very successful in tracing long series. 

The chief types are all represented in the Cambrian strata—a 
fact which forcibly suggests the immensity of yet earlier ages. 
They are abundant from the Silurian onwards. The snails have 
gone on increasing, and are now more abundant than ever ; the 
bivalves cannot be said to have diminished, but the Cephalo- 
pod tribe has dwindled. Of the Nautiloid type of Cephalopods, 
of which there are crowds of fossil forms, only the pearly Nautilus 
now survives, and though there are many kinds of naked cuttle- 
fish in our seas there are ten times as many shelled ancestors 
in the rocks. The geological record confirms what we should 
otherwise expect, that the lung-breathing snails and the freshwater 
bivalves were somewhat late in appearing. 

Prof. Ray Lankester has reconstructed an ideal ancestor which 
combines the various molluscan characteristics in a satisfactory 


CHAP. XV Backboneless Animals 247 


fashion, and may be something like the original mollusc. Whence 
that original sprang is uncertain, but the common occurrence of the 
trochosphere larva and some of the characters of the primitive 
Gasteropods (Meomenia, Chetoderma, Chiton) suggest the origin 
of molluscs from some “ worm” type or other. We can be sure of 
this, however, that the series must have divided at a very early 
epoch into two sets, the sluggish, sedentary, headless bivalves on 
the one hand, and the more active and aggressive snails and cuttle- 
fish on the other. 

Relation to Man,—Irresistibly we think first of oysters, which 
Huxley describes as ‘gustatory flashes of summer lightning,” 
and over which neolithic man smacked his lips, But many others, 
cuttlefish, ear-shells (Haldotzs), mussels (AZytzlus edulis), peri- 
winkles (Lzttorena littorea), cockles (Cardium cordatum), etc. etc., are 
used as food, and many more as bait. In ancient days, as even 
now, the shells of many were used for ornaments, instruments, 
lamps, vessels, coins, etc. ; the inner layer of the shell furnishes 
mother-of-pearl; concretions around irritating particles become 
pearls in the pearl-oyster (Margaritana) and in a few others ; the 
Tyrian purple was a secretion of the whelk Pzrfura and the 
related Murex ; and the attaching byssus threads of the large bivalve 
Pinna may be woven like silk. 

On the other hand, a few cuttlefish are large enough to be 
somewhat dangerous ; the bivalve Zéeredo boring into ship-bottoms 
and piers is a formidable pest, baulked, however, by the pre- 
valent use of metal sheathing; the snails and slugs are even more 
voracjous than the birds which decimate them. 

Conchology was for a while a craze, rare shells have changed 
hands at the cost of hundreds of pounds, such is the human “ mania 
of owning things.” But the shells are often fascinating in their 
beauty, and poetic fancy has played lovingly with such as the 
Nautilus. 


CHAPTER XVI 
BACKBONED ANIMALS 
1. Balanoglossus—2, Tunicates—3. The Lancelet-—q4. Round- 


Mouths or Cyclostomata—5. Fishes —6. Amphibians — 
7. Reptiles—8. Birds—g. Mammals 


ACCORDING to Aristotle, fishes and all higher animals were ‘‘ blood- 
containing,” and thus distinguished from the lower animals, which 
he regarded as ‘‘ bloodless.” He was mistaken as to the absence of 
blood in lower animals, for in most it is present, but the line which 
he drew between higher and lower animals has been recognised in 
all subsequent classifications. Fishes, amphibians, reptiles, birds, 
and mammals differ markedly from molluscs, insects, crustaceans, 
“worms,” and yet simpler animals. The former are backboned 
(Vertebrate), the latter backboneless (Invertebrate). 

It is necessary to make the contrast more precise. (a) Many 
Invertebrates have a well-developed nerve-cord, but this lies on the 
ventral surface of the body, and is connected anteriorly, by a ring 
round the gullet, with a dorsal brain in the head. In Verte- 
brates the whole of the central nervous system lies along the dorsal 


REAEMINS SETS ET 
= 


Fic. 49.—Diagram of ‘‘ Ideal Vertebrate,” showing the segments of the body, 
the spinal cord, the notochord, the gill-clefts, the ventral heart. (After Haeckel.) 


surface of the body, forming the brain and spinal cord. These 
arise by the infolding of a skin groove on the dorsal surface of the 
embryo. (2) Underneath the nerve-cord in the Vertebrate embryo 


CHAP. XVI Backboned Animals 249 


is a supporting rod or notochord. It arises along the roof of the 
food-canal, and serves as a supporting axis to the body. It per- 
sists in some of the lowest Vertebrates (e.g. the lancelet) ; it persists 
in part in some fishes; but in most Vertebrates it is replaced by a 
new growth—the backbone—which ensheaths and constricts it. 
(c) From the anterior region of the food-canal in fishes and tadpoles 
slits, bordered by gills, open to the exterior. Through the slits 
water flows, washing the outsides of blood-vessels and aérating the 
blood. These slits or clefts are represented in the young of all 
Vertebrate animals, but in reptiles, birds, and mammals they are 
transitory and never used. Amphibians are the highest animals in 
which they are used for breathing, and even then they may be 
entirely replaced by lungs in adult life. They are evident in tad- 
poles, they have disappeared in frogs. (d¢) Many an Invertebrate 
has a well-developed heart, but this always lies on the dorsal 
surface of the body, while that of fish or frog, bird or man, lies 
ventrally. (e) It is characteristic of the eye of backboned animals 
that the greater part of it arises as an outgrowth from the brain, 
while that of backboneless animals is directly derived from the skin. 
But this difference is less striking when we remember that it is from 
an infolding of skin that the brain of a backboned animal arises. 

But while the characteristics of backboned animals can now be 
stated with a precision greater than that of sixty years ago, it is no 
longer possible to draw with a firm hand the dividing line between 
backboned and backboneless. Thus fishes are not the simplest 
Vertebrates ; the lamprey and the glutinous hag belong to a more 
primitive type, and are called fishes only by courtesy ; simpler still 
is the lancelet ; the Tunicates hesitate on the border line, being 
tadpole-like in their youth, but mostly degenerate when adults ; 
and the worm-like Balanoglossus is perhaps to be ranked as an 
incipient Vertebrate. The extension of knowledge and the appli- 
cation of evolutionary conceptions obliterate the ancient landmarks 
of more rigid but less natural classification. 

1. Balanoglossus.— Zalanoglossus is a worm - like animal, 
represented by some half-dozen species, which eat their way 


Fic. 50.—Balanoglossus, showing proboscis, collar, and gill-slits. 


through sandy mud off the coasts of the Channel Islands, Brittany, 
Chesapeake Bay, and other regions. Its body is ciliated and divided 
into distinct regions—a large ‘‘ proboscis ” in front of the mouth, a 


250 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


firm collar behind the mouth, a part with numerous gill-slits behind 
the collar, and finally a soft coiled portion with the intestine and 
reproductive organs. The size varies from about an inch to 6 
inches, the colours are bright, the odour is peculiar; the sexes are 
separate. But Balanoglossus is most remarkable in having a dorsal 
supporting rod (like a notochord) in the “proboscis” region, a 
dorsal nerve-cord running along the back and especially developed 
in the collar, and a series of gill-clefts on the anterior part of the 
food-canal. It is therefore difficult to exclude Ba/anoglossus from 


Fic. 51.-—Cephalodiscus, a single individual, isolated from a colony, It is much 
magnified. (From Chambers’s Zucyclop.; after Challenger Report, by 
M‘Intosh and Harmer.) 


the Vertebrate series, and it is likely that the same must be said 
of another strange animal, Cephalodiscus, discovered by the Chal- 
lenger explorers. 

2. Tunicates.—Hanging to the pennon-like seaweeds which 
fringe the rocky shore and are rarely uncovered by the tide, large 
sea-squirts sometimes live. They are shaped like double-mouthed 
wine - bags, 2 or 3 inches in length, and water is always being 
drawn in at one aperture and expelled at the other. Usually they 
live in clusters, and their life is very passive. We call them sea- 
squirts because water may spout forth when we squeeze their 


CHAP. XVI Backboned Animals 251 


bodies, while the title Tunicate refers to a characteristic cloak ot 
tunic which envelops the whole animal. 

There is not much to suggest backbonedness about these Tuni- 
cates, and till 1866 no one dreamt that they could be included in 
the Vertebrate series. But then the Russian naturalist Kowalevsky 
discovered their life-history. The young forms are free-swimming 
creatures like miniature tadpoles, with a dorsal nerve-cord, a sup- 
porting rod in the tail region, gill-slits opening from the food canal, 
a little eye arising as an outgrowth of the brain, and a ventral 
heart. 

There are only two or three genera of Tunicates, especially one 
called Appendicularia, in which these Vertebrate characteristics are 
retained throughout life. The others lose them more or less com- 
pletely. The young Tunicates are active, perhaps too active, for a 
short time ; then they settle down as if fatigued, fix themselves by 
their heads, absorb their tails, and become deformed. The nervous 
system is reduced to a single ganglion between the two apertures ; 
the original gill-slits are replaced by a great number of a different 
character ; the eye is lost. From the skin of the degenerate animal 
the external tunic is exuded. It is a cuticle, and consists, in part 
at least, of cellulose, the substance which forms the cell-walls of 
plants. Thus this characteristically vegetable substance occurs 
almost uniquely in the most passive part of a very passive animal. 
The sea-squirt’s metamorphosis is one of the most signal instances 
of degeneration ; the larva has a higher structure than the adult ; 
the young Tunicate is a Vertebrate, the adult is a nondescript. We 
cannot tell how this fate has befallen the majority, nor why a few 
are free-swimmers, nor why Appendicularia retains throughout life 
the Vertebrate characteristics of its youth. Do the majority over- 
exert themselves when they are “tadpoles,” or are they constitu- 
tionally doomed to become sedentary ? 

Tunicates are hermaphrodite—a very rare condition among Ver- 
tebrates ; some of them exhibit ‘‘ alternation of generations,” as the 
poet Chamisso first observed ; asexual multiplication by budding is 
very common, and not only clusters but more or less intimate 
colonies are thus formed. 

Tunicates live in all seas, mostly near the coast from low water 
to 20 fathoms, and usually fixed tostones and rocks, shells and sea- 
weed. A few are free-swimming, such as the fire-flame (Pyrosoma), 
a unified colony of tubular form, sometimes 2 or 3 feet in length, 
and brilliantly phosphorescent. Very beautiful are the swimming 
chains of the genus Sa/éa, whose structure and life-history alike are 
complicated. 

Tunicates feed on the animalcules borne in by the water 
currents, and some of them must feed well, so rapidly do they grow 


252 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


and multiply. Unpleasant to taste, they are left in peace, though 
a crab sometimes cuts a tunic off as a cloak for himself. 

3. The Lancelet.—The lancelet (Amphioxus) is a simple 
Vertebrate, far below the structural rank of fishes. It is only 
about 2 inches in length, and, as both English and Greek names 
suggest, it is pointed at both ends. On the sandy coasts of warm 
and temperate seas it is widely distributed. 

From tip to tail of the translucent body runs a supporting noto- 
chord ; above this is a spinal cord, with hardly a hint of brain. 
The pharynx bears « hundred or so gill-slits, which in the adult 
are covered over by folds of skin, so that the water which enters 
by the mouth finds its way out by a single posterior aperture. 
Although Amphioxus has no skull, nor jaws, nor brain, nor limbs, 
it deserves its position near the base of the Vertebrate series. The 
sexes are separate, and the eggs are fertilised outside of the body. 
The development of the embryo has been very carefully studied, 
and is for a time very like that of Tunicates. 

4. Round-Mouths or Cyclostomata.—The hag-fishes and 
the lampreys and a few allied genera must be excluded from the 
class of fishes. They are survivors of a more primitive race. 
They are jawless, limbless, scaleless, and therefore not fishes. 

The lampreys (Petromyzon) live in rivers and estuaries, and also 
in the wider sea. They are eel-like, slimy animals. The skeleton 
is gristly ; the simple brain is imperfectly roofed ; the single nostril 
does not open into the mouth; the rounded mouth has horny teeth 
on the lips and on the piston-like tongue; there are seven pairs of 
gill-pouches which open directly to the exterior and internally into 
a tube lying beneath and communicating with the adult gullet ; the 
young are blind and otherwise different from the parents, and may 
remain so for two or three years. 

Though lampreys eat worms and other small fry, and even dead 
animals, they fix themselves aggressively to fishes, rasping holes 
in the skin, and sucking the flesh and juices. They also cling to 
stones, as the name Petromyzon suggests. 

Some species drag stones into a kind ot nest. They spawn in 
spring, usually far up rivers, for at least some of the marine 
lampreys leave the sea at the time of breeding. The young are in 
many ways different from the parents, and that of the small river 
lampern (Petromyzon branchialis) used to be regarded as a distinct 
animal— Ammocetes. The metamorphosis was.discovered two 
hundred years ago by Baldner, a Strasburg fisherman, but was 
overlooked till the strange story was worked out in 1856 by 
August Miiller, Country boys often call the young “ nine-eyes,” 
miscounting the gill apertures, and the Germans also speak of 
neun-augen. 


CHAP. XVI Backboned Animals 253 


The sea lamprey (7. marinus) may measure three feet; the 
river lamprey (P. fluviatziis) about two feet ; the small lampern or 
stone-grig (P. bvanchialis or planeri) about a foot. The flesh is 
well known to be palatable. 

The glutinous hag (Myxine glutinosa) is an eel-like animal, 
about a foot in length, of a livid flesh colour. It is common at 
considerable depths (40 to 300 fathoms) off the coasts of Britain 
and Norway, and, when not feeding, lies buried in the mud with 
only its nostril protruded. Like the lamprey, it has a smooth 
slimy skin, a gristly skeleton, a round suctorial mouth with teeth. 
The single nostril communicates with the food-canal at the back 
of the mouth, and serves for the inflowing of water; the six gill- 
pockets on each side open directly into the gullet, but each has an 
excurrent tube, and the six tubes of each side open at a common 
aperture, The animal lives away from the light, and its eyes are 
rudimentary, hidden beneath skin and muscles. The skin exudes 
so much slime that the ancients spoke of the hag ‘turning water 
into glue.” 

In several ways the hag is strange. Thus J. T. Cunningham 
discovered that it is hermaphrodite, first producing male elements, 
and afterwards eggs, and Nansen has corroborated this. The eggs 
are large and oval, each enclosed in a ‘‘horny” shell with knotted 
threads at each end, by which 4 number are entangled together. 
How they develop is unknown. The hags devour the bait and 
even the fish from the fisherman’s lines, and some say that they bore 
their way into living fishes such as cod. 

5. Fishes.—Fishes are in the water as birds in the air,—swift, 
buoyant, and graceful. They are the first backboned animals with 
jaws, while scales, paired fins, and gills are their most character- 
istic structures. The scales may be hard or soft, scattered or 
closely fitting, and are often very beautiful in form and colour. 
The paired fins are limbs, as yet without digits, varying much in 
size and position, and helping the fish to direct its course. The 
gills are outgrowths of skin with a plaited surface, on which the 
branching blood-vessels are washed by the water. They are the 
breathing organs of all fishes, but in the double-breathing mud- 
fishes (Dzpno0z) the swim-bladder has come to serve as a lung, and 
there are hints of this in a few others. 

There are at least four orders of fishes :— 

(1) The cartilaginous fishes (Elasmobranchs or Selachians) are for 
the most part quite gristly, except in teeth and scales. Among 
them are the flattened skates and rays with enormous fore-fins, 
while the sharks and dogfish are shaped like most other fishes. 
Their pedigree goes back as far as the Silurian rocks, in which 
remains of shark-like forms are found. A Japanese shark (Ch/a- 


254 The Study of Animal Life PART III 


mydoselachus) is said to be very closely allied to types which occur 
in the Old Red Sandstone. Allied to the Elasmobranchs, but 
sometimes kept in a separate division, are two genera, the Chimera 
or King-of-the-Herrings, and Callorhynchus, its relative in Southern 
Seas. 

(2) The Ganoid fishes are almost, if not quite, as ancient as the 
Elasmobranchs, but their goldenage, long since past, was in Devonian 
and Carboniferous ages. There are only some seven different kinds 
now alive. Two of these are the sturgeons (4cipenser) and the 
bony pike (Lepzdosteus). The latter has a bony skeleton; the 
sturgeon is in part gristly. An armature of hard scales is very 
characteristic of this decadent order. 

(3) In Permian times, when Reptiles were beginning, a third 
type of fish appeared, of which the Queensland mud-fish (Ceratodus) 
seems to be a direct descendant. In this type the air-bladder is 
used as a lung, thus suggesting the transition from Fishes to Am- 
phibians. Perhaps this order was always small in numbers ; now- 
adays at least there are only two genera— Ceratodus, from the 
fresh water of Queensland, and Protopterus, from west and tropical 
Africa ; while another form, sometimes called a different genus 
(Lepidostren), is recorded from the Amazons. Double-breathers or 
Dipnoi we call them, for they do not depend wholly upon gills, but 
come to the surface and gulp air into their air-bladder. Mud- 
fishes they are well named, for as the waters dry up they retire into 
the mud, forming for themselves a sort of nest, within which they 
lie dormant. 

(4) In the Chalk period the characteristically modern fishes 
(Teleosteans), with completely bony skeletons, began. Herring 
and salmon, cod and pike, eel and minnow, and most of the com- 
monest fishes belong to this order. Heavy ironclads yield to swift 
gunboats, and the lithe Teleosteans have succeeded better than the 
armoured Ganoids. 

The wedge-like form of most fishes is well adapted for rapid 
swimming. Most flat fish, whether flattened from above down- 
wards like the gristly skate, or from side to side like the flounders 
and plaice, live at the bottom; those of eel-like shape usually 
wallow in the sand or mud; the quaint globe-fish float passively. 
The chief organ of locomotion is the tail ; the paired fins help to 
raise or depress the fish, and serve as guiding oars. In the climb- 
ing perch they are used in scrambling ; in the flying fish they are 
sometimes moved during the long swooping leaps. In eels and 
pipe-fish they are absent ; in the Dipnoi they have a remarkable 
median axis. The unpaired fins on the back and tail and under 
surface are fringes of skin supported by rays. 

Fishes are often resplendent in colours, which are partly due to 


CHAP. XVI Backboned Animats 255 


pigments, partly to silvery waste-products in the cells of the outer 
skin, and partly to the physical structure of the scales. Some- 
times the males are much brighter than the females, and grow 
brilliant at the breeding season. In some cases the colours har- 
monise with surrounding hues of sand and gravel, coral and sea- 
weed ; while the plaice and some others have the power of rapidly 
changing their tints. 

Fishes feed on all sorts of things. Some are carnivorous, others 


Fia. 52.—The gemmeous dragonet (Caldionymus lyra), the male above, 
the female beneath. (From Darwin.) 


vegetarian, others swallow the mud. By most of them worms, 
crustaceans, insect-larvee, molluscs, and smaller fishes are greedily 
eaten. Strange are some of large appetite (e.g. Chéasmodon niger), 
who manage to get outside fishes larger than their own ormaat 
size! 

Of their mental life little is known. Yet the cunning of trout, 
the carefulness with which the mother salmon selects a spawning- 
ground, the way the archer-fish (Zoxofes) spits upon insects, the 
nest-making and courtship of the stickleback and others, the pug- 
nacity of many, show that the brain of the fish is by no means asleep. 


256 The Study of Animal Life PART 111 


The males are often different from the females—smaller, brighter, 
and less numerous. In some cases they court their mates, and 
fight with their rivals. Most of the females lay eggs, but a few 
bony fishes and many sharks bring forth living young. In two 
sharks there is a prophecy of that connection between mother and 
offspring which is characteristic of mammals. The fish’s egg is 
usually a small thing, but those of Elasmobranchs are large, being 
rich in yolk and often surrounded by a mermaid’s purse. This 
egg-case has long tendril-like prolongations at the corners, these 
twine automatically around seaweed, and the embryos may be 
rocked by the waves until the time of hatching. When the egg is 
enclosed in a sheath, or when the young are hatched within the 
body of the mother, fertilisation must take place internally, but in 
most cases the male accompanies the female as she spawns, and 
with his milt fertilises the eggs in the water or on the gravelly 
spawning-ground. As love for offspring varies inversely with their 
number, there is little parental care among the prolific fishes. 

Most fishes live either wholly in the sea or wholly in fresh 
water, but some are indifferent, and pass, at spawning time espe- 
cially, from one to the other. A few, such as the climbing 
perch, venture ashore, while the mud-fishes and a few others 
can survive drought for a season. In caves several blind fishes 
live, and species of Mverasfer find more or less habitual lodging 
inside sea-cucumbers and some other animals, 

The fishes which live in deep water are interesting in many 
ways. Giinther has shown that from 80 to 200 fathoms the eyes 
are rather larger than usual, as if to make the most of the dim 
light. Beyond 200 fathoms ‘‘small-eyed fishes as well as large-eyed 
occur, the former having their want of vision compensated for by 
tentacular organs of touch, whilst the latter have no such accessory 
organs,” and can see only by the fitful light of phosphorescence. 
“In the greatest depths blind fishes occur, with rudimentary eyes, 
and without special organs of touch.” The phosphorescence is pro- 
duced by numerous marine animals and by the fishes themselves. 

6. Amphibians.—The Amphibians which now live are neither 
numerous nor large. Giant Amphibians or Labyrinthodonts began 
to appear in the Carboniferous period, but most of the modern 
frogs and toads, newts and salamanders, are relatively pigmies. 

Young Amphibians always breathe by gills, as Fishes do, and in 
some cases these gills persist in adult life. But whether they do 
or not, the full-grown Amphibians have lungs and use them. The 
skin is characteristically soft, naked, and clammy. Amphibians 
are the first Vertebrates with hands and feet, with fingers and toes. 
Unpaired fringes are sometimes present on the back and tail as in 
Fishes, but are never supported by fin-rays. 


CHAP. XVI Backboned Animals 257 


The class includes four orders, of which the Labyrinthodonts 
are wholly extinct, the other three being represented by tail-less 
frogs and toads (Anura), by newts and salamanders (Urodela) with 
distinct tails, and by a few of worm-like form and burrowing 
habit, e.g. Cecilia. Some, the last for example, are terrestrial, but 
usually live in damp places ; most pass their youth at least in fresh 
water ; none can endure saltness, and they are therefore absent from 
almost all oceanic islands. The common British newts (Z77zton and 
Lissotriton), and the often brightly-coloured salamanders (Sa/a- 
mandra) have in adult life no trace of gills; the rice-eel (Amphiuma) 
and the genus Menopoma lose their gills, but persistent clefts indi- 
cate their position ; the blanched blind Proteus from caves and the 
genus Menobranchus keep their gills throughout life. The remark- 
able Axolotl from North American lakes occurs in two forms, both 
of which may bear young; the one form (Axo/ofl) has persistent 
gills, the other form (AmdJlystoma) loses them, and the change 
from the A.xolotl to the Amédlystoma is in part associated with the 
passage from the water to the swampy shore. A large fossil dis- 
covered by Scheuchzer in the beginning of the eighteenth century 
was quaintly regarded as a fossil man and as a testimony of the 
deluge. But Cuvier showed that Scheuchzer’s Homo diluvid testis 
was but a large newt. 

The common frogs (Rana), the Surinam toad (Pga), the 
common toads (Bufo), and the tree-frogs (/7y/a) illustrate the tail- 
less order Anura. In none of them is there in adult life any trace 
of gills, 

The worm-like, limbless, burrowing Amphibians (Gymnophiona) 
must not be confused with the blind- or slow-worms, which are 
lizards. There are only very few genera, Siphonops, Rhinatrema, 
Epicrium, Cacilia, The newly-born Cecilia has external gills, 
but these are soon lost. The eyes are covered with skin, but are 
well developed. 

The race of Amphibians began in the Carboniferous ages. 
Most of the Labyrinthodonts which flourished then and in the two 
succeeding periods were newt-like in form, but some were serpen- 
tine. They seem to have been armoured, and were sometimes 
large. 

Se Gunhibtans are naturally sluggish. For long periods they can 
fast and lie dormant; they can survive being: frozen quite stiff, 
and though tales of toads within stones are mostly due to mistakes 
or fancies, there are some authentic cases of prolonged imprison- 
ment. 

Few are found far from water, and the gilled condition of 
the young is skipped over only in a few cases. In the black 
salamander (Salamandra atra) of the Alps, which lives where 

s 


258 The Study of Animal Life PART III 
pools are scarce, the young, after living and breathing for a time 
within the mother, are born as lung-breathers; also in some 
species of tree-frogs (//y/odes), which live in situations where water 
is scarce, the gilled stage is omitted. 

The development of the common frog should be studied by 
every student of natural history. The eggs are fertilised as they 
are being laid. The division of the ovum can be readily observed. 
In its early stages the tadpole is fish-like, with a lamprey-like 


Fic. 53.—The life-history of the l’rog, 


mouth. External gills are replaced by an internal set, and as 
metamorphosis is accomplished these disappear and the lungs 
become active. The larva feeds first on its own yolk, then on 
freshwater plants, then on small animals or even on its own 
relatives ; then it fasts, absorbing its tail, and finally it becomes an 
insect-catching frog. 

The food of adult Amphibians usually consists of insects, slugs, 
and worms; most of the larve are for a time vegetarian. Though 
Amphibians often live alone, crowds are often found together at the 
breeding season. Then the sluggish life wakes up, as the croak- 
ings of frogs remind us. Quaint are many of their reproductive 
habits, to some of which allusion has already been made. Such 


CHAP. XVI Backboned Animals 259 


animals as the Surinam toad (ga americana) and the Obstetric 
frog (Alytes obstetricans) suggest that the Amphibians make ex- 
periments in eugenics. 

7. Reptiles.—Fishes and Amphibians are closely allied; so 
Reptiles are linked to Birds, and more remotely to Mammals also. 
Those three highest classes—Reptiles, Birds, and Mammals—are 
‘very different from one another, but they have certain characters in 
common. Most of them have passed from the water to dry land ; 
none of them ever breathe by gills ; all of them have two embryonic 
birth-robes—amnion and allantois—which are of great importance 
in early life. Compared with the other Vertebrates, the brains are 
more complex, the circulation is more perfect, the whole life has a 
higher pitch. As symbols of mammal, bird, and reptile, take the 
characteristic coverings of the skin—hair, feathers, and scales. 
Hair typifies strength and perhaps also gentleness ; feathers suggest 
swift flight, the beauty which wins love, and the down which lines 
the warm nest ; scales speak of armour and cold-blooded stealth. 

But we need not depreciate reptiles, nor deny the justice of that 
insight which has found in them the fittest emblems of the omni- 
potence of the earth. If Athene of the air. possesses the birds, 
surely the power of the dust is in the grovelling snakes. Few 
colour arrangements are more beautiful than those which adorn the 
lithe lizards. The tortoise is an example of passive energy, self- 
contained strength, and all but impenetrable armature. The 
crocodiles more than the others recall the strong ferocity of the 
ancient extinct dragons. Nor should we judge reptiles exclusively 
by their living representatives, any more than we should judge 
the Romans by those of the decadent Empire. It is interesting to 
remember the long-tailed toothed Avcheopteryx, the predecessor of 
modern birds, just as it is to recall the giant sloths which pre- 
ceded the modern Edentate mammals ; but it is essential to include 
in our appreciation of Reptiles the giant dragons of their golden 
age. Most modern forms are pigmies beside an Jchthyosaurus 25 
feet long, a AMegalosaurus of 30, a Titanosaurus of 60, or an 
Atlantosaurus of 100, all fairly broad in proportion. We have still 
pythons and crocodiles and other reptiles of huge size, and we do 
not deny Grant Allen’s remark that a good blubbery ‘‘ right whale” 
could give points to any deinosaur that ever moved upon Oolitic 
continents, but the fact remains that in far back times (Triassic, 
Jurassic, and Cretaceous) reptiles had a golden age with a pre- 
dominance of forms larger than any living members of the class. 
Besides size, however, the ancient saurians had another virtue, 
apparently possessed by both small and great— they were pro- 
gressive. For, with toothed birds on the one hand and flying or 
flopping reptiles on the other, it seems probable that birds had 


260 The Study of Animal Life PART Il] 


their origin from feverish saurians which acquired the power of 
flight, and it is also possible that some, perhaps pathological, 
mother reptile, overflowing in the milk of animal kindness, and 
retaining her young for a long time within her womb, was the fore- 
runner of the mammalian race, 

While there are many orders of extinct reptiles—Ichthyosaurs, 
Plesiosaurs, Deinosaurs, Pterosaurs, and other saurians not yet 
classified with certainty—the living forms belong to four sets—the 
lizards, the snakes, the tortoises, and the crocodiles—to which a 
fifth order should perhaps be added for the New Zealand “lizard” 
Hatteria or Sphenodon, which is in several respects a living fossil. 

The Lizards (Lacertilia).—The lizards form a central order of 
Reptiles, but the members are a motley crowd, varied in detailed 
structure and habit, Usually active in their movements, though 
fond, too, of lying passive in the sunshine, they are often very 
beautiful in form and colour, and not uncommonly change their 
tints in sympathetic response to their surroundings. Most lay eggs, 
but in some, eg. the common British lizard (Lacerta or Zootoca 
vivipara), and the slow-worm, the young are hatched within the 
mother. 

Among the remarkable forms are the Geckos, which with 
plaited adhesive feet can climb up smooth walls ; the large Monitors 
(Varanus), which may attain a length of 6 feet, and prey upon 
small mammals, birds, frogs, fishes, and eggs; the poisonous 
Mexican lizard (Heloderma horridum), with large venom-glands 
and somewhat fang-like teeth; the worm-like, limbless Amhzs- 
bena; the likewise snake-like slow-worm (Anguds fragilis), which 
well illustrates the tendency lizards have to break in the spasms of 
capture; the large Iguanas, which frequent tropical American 
forests, and feed on leaves and fruit; the sluggish and spiny 
‘“‘Horned Toad” (Phrynosoma); the Agamas of the Old World 
comparable to the Iguanas of the New ; the Flying Dragon (Draco 
volans), which, with skin outstretched on extended ribs, swoops 
from tree to tree; the Australian frilled lizards (Chlamydosaurus) 
and the quaint thorny AZoloch; the single marine lizard (Oreo- 
cephalus or Amblyrhynchus cristatus) from the Galapagos ; and the 
divergent Chameleons, flushing with changeful colour. 

The New Zealand Hatteria or Sphenodon is quite unique, and 
seems to be the sole survivor of an extinct order—Rhynchocephalia. 
It was in it first of all that the pineal body—an upgrowth from the 
mid-brain of backboned animals—was seen to be a degenerate 
upward-looking eye. 

Snakes or Serpents (Ophidia). — These much modified 
reptiles mostly cleave to the earth, though there are among them 
clever climbers, swift swimmers, and powerful burrowers. Though 


Backboned Animals 


XVI 


CHAP. 


(‘Jewry wor payiduroy) 
‘soroads aumes at} JO SaI}OTIVA PayIeU puv painojoo AjUaiayip saiyy “(S77V-LNUL YZ4GIVT) PILZI-TTEM YT —"PS “O17 


262 The Study of Animal Life PART II! 


they are all limbless, unless we credit the little hind claws of some 
boas and pythons with the title of legs, they flow like swift living 
streams along the ground, using ribs and scales instead of their lost 
appendages, pushing themselves forward with jerks so rapid that 
the movement seems continuous. Without something on which to 
raise themselves they must remain at least half prostrate, but in the 
forest or on rough ground there are no lither gymnasts. Their 
united eyelids give them an unlimited power of staring, and, accord- 
ing to uncritical observers, of fascination; yet most of them seem 
to see dimly and hear faintly, trusting mainly for guidance to the 
touch of their restless protrusible tongue and to their sense of 
smell. Their only language is ahiss or a whine. Most of them 
have an annual period of torpor, and all periodically cast off their 
scales in a normally continuous slough, which they turn outside-in 
as they crawl out. Almost all lay eggs, but in a few cases (2g. 
the adder) the young are hatched within the mothers, and this 
mode of birth may be induced by artificial conditions. Think not 
meanly of the serpent, ‘‘it is the very omnipotence of the earth. 
That rivulet of smooth silver—how does it flow, think you? It 
literally rows on the earth with every scale for an oar; it bites the 
dust with the ridges of its body. Watch it when it moves slowly— 
a wave, but without wind! a current, but with no fall! all the 
body moving at the same instant, yet some of it to one side, some 
to another, or some forward, and the rest of the coil backwards ; 
but all with the same calm will and equal way—no contraction, no 
extension ; one soundless, causeless, march of sequent rings, and 
spectral procession of spotted dust, with dissolution in its fangs, 
dislocation in its coils. Startle it—the winding stream will become 
a twisted arrow; the wave of poisoned life will lash through the 
grass like a cast lance. It scarcely breathes with its one lung (the 
other shrivelled and abortive) ; it is passive to the sun and shade, 
and cold or hot like a stone; yet ‘it can outclimb the monkey, 
outswim the fish, outleap the zebra, outwrestle the athlete, and 
crush the tiger.’ It is a Divine hieroglyph of the demoniac power 
of the earth—of the entire earthly nature. As the bird is the 
clothed power of the air, so this is the clothed power of the dust ; 
as the bird is the symbol of the spirit of life, so this of the grasp and 
sting of death.”} 

This well-known and eloquent passage is not perfectly true,— 
thus the serpent breathes not scarcely but strongly with its one 
lung,—but, while you may correct and complete it as you will, I am 
sure that you will find here more insight into the nature of serpents 
than in pages of anatomical description. 


1 Ruskin’s Queen of the Air. 


CHAP. XVI Backboned Animals 263 


A few snakes have mouths which do not distend, skull bones 
which are slightly movable, teeth in one jaw (upper or lower) 
only, and rudiments of hind legs. These are included in the 
genera 7yphlops and Anomalepsis, and are small simple ophidians. 

Many are likewise non-venomous snakes, but with wider gape 
and more mobile skull bones, and with simple teeth on both jaws. 
Some are very large and have great powers of strangling. Such 
are the Pythons, the Boa, and the Anaconda. To these our grass 
snake (Zropidonotus natrix) is allied, 

Many poisonous snakes have large permanently erect grooved fangs 
in the upper jaw, and a salivary gland whose secretion is venomous. 
Such are the cobra (Maja tripudians), the Egyptian asp (Vaya haze), 
the coral snakes (Z/afs), and the sea snakes (Aydrophis). 

Other poisonous snakes have perforated fang teeth, which can 
be raised and depressed. Such are the vipers (ViZera), the British 
adder (Pelas berus), the copperhead (Axcistrodon contortrix), the 
rattlesnakes (Cvotalzs). 

Tortoises and Turtles (Chelonia).—Boxed in by a bony 
shield above and by a bony shield below, and often with partially 
retractile head and tail and legs, the Chelonians are thoroughly 
armoured. On the average the pitch of their life is low, but their 
tenacity of life is great. Slow in growth, slow in movement, slow 
even in reproduction are many of them, and they can endure long 
fasting, It is said that a tortoise walked at least 200 yards, twenty- 
four hours after it was decapitated, while it is well known that the 
heart of a tortoise will beat for two or three days after it has been 
isolated from the animal. In connection with their sluggishness it 
is significant that the ribs which help to some extent in the respira- 
tory movements of higher animals are soldered into the dorsal 
shield, thus sluggish respiration may be in part the cause, as it is 
in part the result, of constitutional passivity. All the Chelonians 
lay eggs in nests scooped in the earth or sand. 

The marine turtles (e.g. Sphargis, Chelone), the estuarine soft- 
shelled turtles (2g. <Aspidonectes), the freshwater turtles (e.g. 
mys), and the snapping turtle (CAeZydra) are more active than the 
land tortoises, such as the European Zestudo greca, often kept as 
apet. The tortoise of the Galapagos Islands ( Zestudo elephantopus), 
the river tortoise (Podocnemys expansa) of the Amazon, the bearded 
South American turtle (Chelys matamata), and the green turtle 
(Chelone mydas) attain a large size, sometimes measuring about 
3 feet in length, 

Crocodilians (Crocodilia).—Crocodiles, alligators, and gavials 
seem in our present perspective very much alike—strong, large, 
heavily armoured reptiles, at home in tropical rivers, but clumsy 
and stiff-necked on land, feeding on fishes and small mammals, 


264 The Study of Animal Life PART IL 


growing slowly and without that definite limit which punctuates 
the life-history of most animals, attaining, moreover, a great 
age, freed after youth is past from the attacks of almost every 
foe but man. The teeth are firmly implanted in sockets ; the 
limbs and tail are suited for swimming, and also for crawling ; the 
heart is more highly developed than in other reptiles, having four 
instead of three chambers. The animals lie in wait for victims, 
and usually drown them, being themselves able to breathe while 
the mouth is full of water, if only the nostrils be kept above the 
surface. 

In many ways Reptiles touch human life, the poisonous snakes 
are very fatal, especially in India; crocodilians are sometimes 
destructive ; turtles afford food and ‘‘ tortoise shell ;” lizards are 
delightfully beautiful. 

8. Birds.—What mammals are to the earth, and fishes to the 
sea, birds are to the air, Has anything truer ever been said of 


Wi. 55-—The Collocalia, which from the secreted juice of its salivary glands 
builds the edible-bird’s-nest. (Adapted from Brehm.) 


them than this sentence from Ruskin’s Queen of the Air? “The 
bird is little more than a drift of the air brought into form by 
plumes; the air is in all its quills, it breathes through its whole 
frame and flesh, and glows with air in its flying, like a blown 


CHAP. XVI Backboned Animals 265 


flame : it rests upon the air, subdues it, surpasses it, outraces it ;— 
és the air, conscious of itself, conquering itself, ruling itself.” 

Birds represent among animals the climax of activity, an index te 
which may be found in their high temperature, from 2°-14° Fahren- 
heit higher than that of mammals. In many other ways they rank 
high, for whether we consider the muscles which move the wings 
in flight, the skeleton which so marvellously combines strength 
with lightness, the breathing powers perfected and economised by a 
set of balloons around the lungs, or the heart which drives and 
receives the warm blood, we recognise that birds share with 
mammals the position of the highest animals. And while it is true 
that the brains of birds are not wrinkled with thought like 
those of mammals, and that the close connection between mother 
and offspring characteristic of most mammals is absent in birds, it 
may be urged by those who know their joyousness that birds feel 
more if they think less, while the patience and solicitude con- 
nected with nest-making and brooding testify to the strength of 
their parental love.. Usually living in varied and beautiful sur- 
roundings, birds have keen eyes and sharp ears, tutored to a sense 
of beauty, as we may surely conclude from their cradles and love 
songs. They love much and joyously, and live a life remarkably 
free and restless, qualities symbolised by the voice of the air in 
their throat, and by the sunshine of their plumes. There is more 
than zoological truth in saying that in the bird ‘the breath or spirit 
is more full than in any other creature, and the earth power least,” 
or in thinking of birds as the purest embodiments of Athene of 
the air. 

But just as there are among mammals feverish bats with the power 
of true flight, and whales somewhat fish-like, so there are excep- 
tional birds, runners like the ostriches and cassowaries, swimmers 
like the penguins, criminals too like the cuckoos and cow-birds in 
which the maternal instincts are strangely perverted. As we go 
back into the past, strange forms are discovered, with teeth, long 
tails, and other characteristics which link the birds of the air to the 
grovelling reptiles of the earth. Even to-day there lives a 
“ reptilian-bird ”— Opdsthocomus—which has retained more than 
any other indisputable affinities with the reptiles. Professor W. K. 
Parker, one of the profoundest of all students of birds, described 
this form in one of his last papers, and there used a comparison 
which helps us to appreciate birds. They are among backboned 
animals what insects are among the backboneless—winged pos- 
sessors of the air, and just as many insects pass through a cater- 
pillar and chrysalis stage before reaching the acme of their life as a 
flying imago, so do the young birds within the veil of the egg- 
shell pass through somewhat fish-like and somewhat reptile-like 


266 The Study of Animal Life PART IIT 


Fic. 56.—Decorative male and less adorned female of Spathura—a genus of 
Humming-birds. (From Darwin, after Brehm.) 


CHAP. XVI Backboned Animals 267 


stages before they attain to the possession of wings and the enjoy- 
ment of freedom. 

The great majority of birds are fliers, and possess a keeled 
breast-bone, to which are fixed the muscles used in flight. To 
this keel or carina they owe their name Carinate. The flying 
host includes the gulls and grebes, the plovers and cranes, the 
ducks and geese, the storks and herons, the pelicans and cormo- 
rants, the partridges and pheasants, the sand grouse, the pigeons, 
the birds of prey, the parrots, the pies, and about 6000 Passerine or 
sparrow-like birds, including thrushes and warblers, wrens and 
swallows, finches and crows, starlings and birds of paradise. To 
these orders we have to add Opzsthocomus, from which it is perhaps 
easier to pass to some of the keeled fossil birds, some of which 
possessed teeth. 

Distinct from the keeled fliers, both ancient and modern, 
are the running-birds, which 
are incapable of flight, and 
therefore possess a flat raft- 
like breast bone, to which 
they owe their title Ratita. 
Nowadays these are few in 
number, the Ostrich and the 
Rhea, the Cassowary and 
Emu, and the small Kiwi. 
Beside these must be ranked 
the giant Moa of New Zea- 
land, not long extinct, and 
the more ancient, not less 
gigantic /¢pyornis of Mada- 
gascar, while farther back 
still, from the Chalk strata 
of America, the remains of 
toothed keelless birds have 
been disentombed. 

The most reptilian, least 
bird-like of birds is the 
oldest fossil of all, placed in 
a sub-class by itself, the 
Archaeopteryx (lit. ancient Fic. 57.—Restoration of the extinct moa (Din- 

. . ornis ingens), and alongside of it the little 
bird) from strata of Jurassic kiwi (Apteryx mantelli). (rom Cham- 
age. bers’s Excyclop. ; after F. vy. Hochstetter.) 

9. Mammalia.—Of the 
highest class of animals—the Mammalia—lI need say least for they 
are most familiar. Most of them are terrestrial, four-footed, and 
hairy. Bats and whales, seals and sea-cows, are obviously excep- 


268 The Study of Animal Life PART II. 


tional. The brain of mammals is more highly developed thar 
that of other animals, and in the great majority there is a prolonged 
(placental) connection between the unborn young and the mother. 
In all cases the mothers feed the tender young with milk. 

In the class there are three grades :— 

(1) In the Duckmole (Ornithorhynchus) and the Porcupine 
Ant-Eater (Echidna), and perhaps another genus Proechidna, the 
females lay eggs. In many other ways these exclusively Austral- 
asian mammals are primitive, exhibiting affinities with reptiles. 

(2) In the Marsupials, which, with the exception of some 
American Opossums, are also Australasian, the young are born at 
a very tender age, as it were, prematurely. In the great majority 
of genera, the mothers stow them away in an external pouch, where 
they are fed and sheltered till able to fend for themselves. In 
Australia the Marsupials have been saved by insulation from stronger 
mammals, which seem to have exterminated them in other parts 
of the earth, the Opossums which hide in American forests being 
the only Marsupials surviving outside Australasia, though fossils 
show that the race had once a much wider distribution. In their 
Australian retreat, apart from all higher Mammalia (mice, rabbits, 
and the like being modern imports) the Marsupials have evolved 
along many lines, prophetic of the higher orders of mammals. 
There are ‘‘carnivores” like the Thylacine and the Dasyure, 
‘herbivores’? like the Kangaroos, ‘‘ insectivores ” like the banded 
ant-eater AZyrmecobius, and ‘‘ rodents” like the Wombat. 

(3) In all the other orders of mammals there is a close con- 
nection between mother and unborn offspring. 

Two orders are lowly and distinctly separate from the others 
arfd from one another—the Edentata represented by sloths, 
ant-eaters, armadillos, pangolins, and the Aard-Vark ; and the 
Sirenia or Sea-Cows which now include only the dugong and the 
manatee. 

Along one fairly definite line we may rank three other orders 
—the Insectivores, the Bats, and the Carnivores. The hedgehog, 
which is at once a lowly and « central type of mammal, may be 
taken as the beginning of this line. Along with shrews, moles, 
porcupines, the hedgehogs form the order Insectivora. To these 
the Bats (Cheiroptera), with their bird-like powers of flight, are 
linked, while the Carnivora (cats, dogs, bears, and seals), though 
progressive in a different direction, seem also related. 

Comparable to the Insectivores, but on a different line, are the 
gnawing Rodents, rabbits and hares, rats and mice, squirrels and 
beavers. This line leads on to the Elephants, from the company 
of which the mammoths have disappeared since man arose on the 
earth, With the Elephants, the rock-coneys or Hyraxes—‘‘a feeble 


CHAP. XVI Backboned Animals 269 


folk”’—seem to be allied. Both are often included in the great 
order of hoofed animals or Ungulates, along with the odd-toed 


= 


AAAS 


Fic. 58.—Phenacodus primevus, a primitive extinct mammal from the lower 
Eocene of N. America. The actual size of the slab of rock on which it rested 
was 49 inches in length, (From Chambers’s Zxcyclop.; after Cope.) 


animals—horse, rhinoceros, and tapir, and a larger number of 
even-toed forms, hog and hippopotamus, camel and dromedary, 


Tic. 59.—Head of gorilla. (From Du Chaillu.) 


and the true cud-chewers or ruminants such as sheep and cattle, 
deer and antelopes. From the ancient predecessors of the modern 


270 The Study of Animal Life PART IIT 


Ungulates, it seems likely enough that the Cetaceans (whales and 
dolphins) diverged. 

A third line, which we may call median, leads through the 
Lemurs on to Monkeys. It must be noted, however, that these 
lines, which seem distinct from one another if we confine our 
attention to living mammals, are linked by extinct forms. Thus a 


Fic. 60.—Head of male Semnopithecus. (From Darwin.) 


remarkable fossil type, Phenacodus, is regarded by Cope as pre- 
senting affinities with Ungulates, Lemurs, and Carnivores. 

The monkeys which most closely resemble man in structure, 
habits, and intelligence, are the so-called anthropoid apes, the 
gorilla, the chimpanzee, the orang-utan, and the gibbon. <A 
second grade is represented by the more dog-like, narrow-nosed 
Old World apes, such as the baboons and mandrills. Lower in 
many ways are the broad-nosed New World or American monkeys, 
e.g. the numerous species of Cedzs, some of which are the familiar 


CHAP. XVI Backboned Animals 271 


companions of itinerant musicians, while lowest and smallest among 
true monkeys are the South American marmosets. Distinct from 
all these, probably outside the monkey order altogether, are the 
so-called half-monkeys or Lemurs. 

We might describe the clever activities of monkeys, the shelters 
which some of them make, their family life, parental care and 
sociality, their docility, their intelligent habits of investigation, and 
their quickness to profit by experience ; but it would all amount to 
this, that their life at many points touches the human, that they 
are in some ways like growing children, in other ways like savage 
men, though with more circumscribed limits of progress than either. 


ORDERS OF MAMMALS. 


MON|KEYS 


UN\GULATES CARNI/VORES 


BATS 
LEM|URS 


RO\ DENTS 


‘STVINGOV 1d 


IN/SECTIVORES 
CETACEANS 


SIRENIA EDENTATA — 


MARSUPIALS 


MONOTREMES 


272 


The Study of 


Animal Life 


SURVEY OF THE ANIMAL KINGDOM 


PART III 


VERTEBRATES. 


_| 


CCELOMATA. 


BIRDS. 
Flying-Birds. Running-Birds, 


Placentals, 
MAMMALS. Marsupials. 
Monotremes. 


Snakes. Lizards. 


REPTILES. Crocodiles, 


Tortoises, 


Double-Breathers, 

Bony-Fishes, 
FISHES. : 

Ganoids. 


Elasmobranchs, 


AMPHIBIANS, 
Newt, Frog. 


CYCLOSTOMATA, 
Lamprey. Hagfish, 


LANCELET. 


TUNICATES. 


Insects, Arachnid 


BALANOGLOSSUS, 


Cuttlefish. 
Gasteropods. 


Myriapods. 
Peripatus. 


ARTHROPODS. 
Crustaceans. 


ANNELIDS. 


“WORMS.” 


FLAT-WORMS, 


MOLLUSCS, 
Bivalves, 


Feather-stars, 
Brittle-stars, 
Starfish, 


ECHINODERMS, 


Sea-urchins, 
Sea-cucumbers, 


INVERTEBRATES. 


Ctenophores. Jellyfish. 


STINGING-ANIMALS or CGELENTERATES, 


Medusoids and 


Sea-Anemones. Corals. 


Hydroids, 


SPONGES. 


‘VOZV.LAIN 


Infusorians, 
SIMPLEST A 


Rhizopods, 


Gregarines, 
NIMALS. 


“VOZ 
-OLOUd 


PART 1¥ 


THE EVOLUTION OF ANIMAL LIFE 


CHAPTER XVII 
THE EVIDENCES OF EVOLUTION 


1. Lhe Ldea of Evolution—2. Arguments for Evolution: Physio- 
logical, Morphological, Historical—3. Origin of Life 


WE observe animals in their native haunts, and study their 
growth, their maturity, their loves, their struggles, and their 
death ; we collect, name, preserve, and classify them; we 
cut them to pieces, and know their organs, tissues, and 
cells ; we go back upon their life and inquire into the secret 
working of their vital mechanism ; we ransack the rocks for 
the remains of those animals which lived ages ago upon the 
earth ; we watch how the chick is formed within the egg, 
and yet we are not satisfied. We seem to hear snatches of 
music which we cannot combine. We seek some unifying 
idea, some conception of the manner in which the world of 
life has become what it is. : 

1. The Idea of Evolution.—We do not dream now, 
as men dreamed once, that all has been as it is since all 
emerged from the mist of an unthinkable beginning; nor 
can we believe now, as men believed once, that all came 
into its present state of being by a flash of almighty volition. 
We still dream, indeed, of an unthinkable beginning, but 
we know that the past has been full of change; we still 

T 


274 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


believe in almighty volition, but rather as a continuous reality 
than as expressed in any event of the past. Thus Erasmus 
Darwin (1794), speaking of Hume, says ‘he concluded 
that the world itself might have been generated rather than 
created ; that it might have been gradually produced from 
very small beginnings, increasing by the activity of its 
inherent principles, rather than by a sudden evolution of 
the whole by the Almighty fiat.” In short, we have 
extended to the world around us our own characteristic 
perception of human Azs¢tory ; we have concluded that in all 
things the present is the child of the past and the parent 
of the future. 

But while we dismiss the theory of permanence as 
demonstrably false, and the theory of successive cataclysms 
and re-creations as improbable,! without feeling it necessary 
to discuss either the falsity or the improbability, we must 
state on what basis our conviction of continuous evolution 
rests. ‘La nature ne nous offre le spectacle d’aucune 
creation, c’est d’une continuation éternelle.” ‘As in the 
development of a fugue,” Samuel Butler says, ‘‘ where, 
when the subject and counter-subject have been announced, 
there must thenceforth be nothing new, and yet all must 
be new, so throughout organic nature—which is a fugue 
developed to great length from a very simple subject— 
everything is linked on to and grows out of that which 
comes next to it in order—errors and omissions excepted.” 

2. Arguments for Evolution.—What then are the facts 
which have convinced naturalists that the plants and the 
animals of to-day are descended from others of a simpler 
sort, and the latter from yet simpler ancestors, and so on, 
back and back to those first forms in which all that suc- 
ceeded were implied? I refer you to Darwin’s Origin of 
Species (1859), where the arguments were marshalled in 
such a masterly fashion that they forced the conviction 

1] use the word in its literal sense—‘‘ not admitting of proof.” Itis 
not my duty nor my desire to discuss the poetical, or philosophical, 
or religious conceptions which lie behind the concrete cosmogonies of 
different ages and minds, To many modern theologians creation 


really means the institution of the order of nature, the possibility of 
natural evolution included, 


cuar. xvit The Evidences of Evolution 275 


of the world. To the statements of the case by Spencer, 
Haeckel, Huxley, Romanes, and others, I have given 
references in the chapter on books. Darwin’s arguments 
were derived (a) from the distribution of animals in space ; 
(2) from their successive appearance in time, (c) from actual 
variations observed in domestication, cultivation, and in 
nature ; (d) from facts of structure, eg. homologous and 
rudimentary organs, (¢) from embryology. I shall simply 
illustrate the different kinds of evidence, and that under 
three heads—(a) physiological, (4) structural, (c) his- 
torical. 

(2) Physiological.—A study of the life of organisms 
shows that the ancient and even Linnzan dogma of the 
constancy or immutability of species was false. Organisms 
change under our eyes. They are not like cast-iron; they 
are plastic. One of the most striking cases in the Natural 
History Collection of the British Museum is that near the 
entrance, where on a tree are perched domesticated pigeons 
of many sorts—fantail, pouter, tumbler, and the like— 
while in the centre is the ancestral rock-dove Columba livia, 
from which we know that all the rest have been derived. 
In other domesticated animals, even when we allow that 
some of them have had multiple origins, we find abundant 
proof of variability. But what occurs under man’s super- 
vision in the domestication of animals and in the culti- 
vation of plants occurs also in the state of nature. Natural 
“ yarieties” which link species to species are very common, 
and the offspring of one brood differ from one another and 
from their parents. How many strange sports there are 
and grim reversions! and, as we shall afterwards see, 
modifications of individuals by force of external conditions 
are not uncommon. Those who say they see no variation 
now going on in nature should try a month’s work at identi- 
fying species. I have known of an ancient man who dwelt 
in a small town ; he did not believe in the reality of railways 
and to him the testimony of observers was as an idle. tale ; 
he was not daunted in his scepticism even when the railway 
was extended to his town, for he was aged, and remained 
at home, dying a professed unbeliever in that which he had 


The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


Fic. 61.—Vari 


eties of domestic pige 
(Columba livia). (Based on Darwin's figures.) 


on arranged around the ancestral rock-dove 


cuav. xvi The Evidences of Evolution 277 


never seen. Conviction depends on more than intelligence, 
often on emotional vested interests. 

(2) Morphological.—There are said to be over a million 
species of living animals, about half of them insects. 
Even their number might suggest blood-relationship, but our 
recognition of this becomes clear when we see that species 
is often united to species, genus to genus, and even class 
to class, by connecting links. The fact that we can make 
at least a plausible genealogical tree of animals, arranging 
them in series along the lines of hypothetical pedigree, is 
also suggestive, 

Throughout long series, structures fundamentally the 
same appear with varied form and function ; the same bones 
and muscles are twisted into a variety of shapes. Why this 
adherence to type if animals are independent of one 
another? How necessary it is if all are branches of one 
tree, 

By rudimentary organs also the same conclusion is 
suggested. What mean the unused gill-clefts of reptiles, 
birds, and mammals, unless the ancestors of these classes 
were fish-like ; what mean the teeth of very young whale- 
bone whales, of an embryonic parrot and turtle, unless they 
are vestiges of those which their ancestors possessed? There 
are similar vestigial structures among most animals. In 
man alone there are about seventy little things which might 
be termed rudimentary ; his body is a museum of relics. We 
are familiar with unsounded or rudimentary letters in many 
words ; we do not sound the “o” in leopard nor the “1” 
in alms, but from these rudimentary letters we read the 
history of the words. 

(c) Historical.—Every one recognises that animals have 
not always been as they now are ; we have only to dig to 
be convinced that the fauna of the earth has had a history. 
But it does not follow that the succession of fauna after 
fauna, age after age, has been a progressive development. 
What evidence is there of this ? 

In the first place, there is the general fact that fishes 
appear before amphibians, and these before reptiles, and 
these before birds, and that the same correspondence 


278 


The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


between order of appearance and structural rank is often 
true in detail within the separate classes of animals. There 


3S 6 


Fic. 62. — Fore 
and hind feet of 
the horse and 
some of its an- 
cestors, show- 
ing the gradual 
reduction in 
the number of 
digits. (From 
Chambers’s Z- 
cyclop.; after 
Marsh. 


are some marvellously complete series of fos- 
sils, especially, perhaps, that of the extinct 
cuttlefishes, in which the steps of progressive 
evolution are still traceable. Moreover, the 
long pedigree of some animals, such as the 
horse, has been worked out so perfectly that 
more convincing demonstration is hardly pos- 
sible. In Professor Huxley’s American Ad- 
dresses, or in that pleasant introduction to 
zoology afforded by Professor W. H. Flower’s 
little book on the horse (Modern Science 
Series, Lond., 1891), you will find the story 
of the horse’s pedigree most lucidly told: 
how in early Eocene times there lived small 
quadrupeds about the size of sheep that 
walked securely upon five toes, how these 
animals lost, first the inner toe, while the 
third grew larger, and then the fifth ; how the 
third continued to grow larger and the second 
and fourth to become smaller until they dis- 
appeared almost entirely, remaining only as 
small splint bones ; and how thus the light- 
footed runners on tiptoe of the dry plains 
were evolved from the short -legged splay- 
footed plodders of the Eocene marshes. Fin- 
ally, there are many extinct types which link 
order to order and even class to class, such 
as that strange mammal Phenacodus, which 
seems to occupy a central position in the 
series, so numerous are its affinities, or such 
as those saurians which link crawling reptile 
to soaring bird. 

Another historical argument of great im- 
portance is that derived from the study of 


the geographical distribution of animals, but this cannot be 
appreciated without studying the detailed facts. These 
suggest that the various types of animals have spread from 


cHap. xvit Lhe Evidences of Evolution 279 


definite centres, along convenient paths of diffusion, varying 
into species after species as their range extended. 

But the history of the individual is even more instructive. 
The first three grades of structure observed among living 
animals are: (1) Single cells (most Protozoa), (2) balls of 
cells (a few Protozoa which form colonies), and (3), two- 
layered sacs of cells (¢.g. the simplest sponges). But these 
three grades correspond to the first three steps in the indi- 
vidual life-history of any many-celled animal. Every one 
begins as a single cell, at the presumed beginning again ; 
this divides into a ball of cells, the second grade of struc- 
ture; the ball becomes a two-layered sac of cells. The 


Fic. 63.—Antlers of deer (1-5) in successive years; but the figure might almost 
represent at the same time the degree of evolution exhibited by the antlers 
of deer in successive ages. (From Chambers’s Zxcyclog.) 

correspondence between the first three grades of structure 

and the first three chapters in the individual’s life-history is 
complete. It is true as a general statement that the indi- 
vidual development proceeds step by step along a path 
approximately parallel to the presumed progress of the 
race, so far as that is traceable from the successive grades 
of structure and from the records of the rocks. Even in 
regard to details such as the development of antlers on stags 
the parallelism of racial and individual history may be 
observed. Of this correspondence it is difficult to see any 
elucidation except that the individual in its life-history in 
great part re-treads the path of ancestral evolution. 

I have illustrated these evidences of evolution very 


280 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


briefly, for they have been stated many times of late years. 
The idea of evolution has also justified itself by the light 
which it has cast not only on biological, but on physical, 
psychological, and sociological facts. There has never been 
a more germinal idea; it is fast becoming organic in all 
our thinking. 

To those who feel a repugnance to the doctrine of 
descent, I suggest the following considerations :— 

(1) In so far as conclusions do not affect conduct, it 
seems wise to conserve what makes one happiest. If your 
intellectual and emotional necessities are better satisfied, 
for instance, by any one of the creationist theories than 
by that of a gradual and natural progress from simple 
beginnings to implied ends, and if you feel that your sense 
of the marvel, beauty, and sacredness of life would be 
impoverished by a change of theory, then I should not seek 
to persuade you. 

(z) But as we do not think a tree less stately because 
we know the tiny seed from which it grew, nor any man 
less noble because he was once a little child, so we ought 
not to look on the world of life with eyes less full of wonder 
or reverence, even if we feel that we know something of its 
humble origins. 

(3) Finally, we should be careful to distinguish between 
the doctrine of natural descent, which, to most naturalists, 
seems a solemn fact, and the theories of evolution which 
explain how the progressive descent was brought about. 
For in regard to the causal, as distinguished from the modal 
explanation of the world, we are or ought to be uncertain. 

3. Origin of Life—It is no dogma, nor yet a “law 
of Biogenesis,” but a fact of experience, to which no excep- 
tion has been demonstrated, that living organisms arise 
from pre-existent organisms—-Omne vivum e vivo. 

As to the origin of life upon the earth we know nothing, 
but hold various opinions. (1) Thus it is believed that life 
began independently of those natural conditions which come 
within the ken of scientific inquirers ; in other words, it is 
believed that the first living things were created. That 
this belief presents intellectual difficulties to many minds 


car. xv The Evidences of Evolution 281 


may mean that its fittest expression in words has not been 
attained, or is unattainable. (2) It has been suggested 
that germs of life reached this earth in the bosom of 
meteorites from somewhere else. This at least shifts the 
responsibility of the problem off the shoulders of this planet. 
(3) It is suggested that living matter may have been evolved 
from not-living matter on the earth’s surface. If we accept 
this suggestion, we must of course suppose that in not-living 
matter the qualities characteristic of living organisms are 
implicit. The evolutionist’s common denominator is then 
as inexpressibly marvellous as the philosopher’s greatest 
common measure. 


CHAPTER XVIII 
THE EVOLUTION OF EVOLUTION THEORIES 


1. Greek Philosophers—2. Aristotle—3. Lucretius—4. Evolution- 
ists before Darwin—5. Three old Masters: Buffon, Erasmus 
Darwin, Lamarck—6. Charles Darwin—7. Darwin's Fellow- 
workers—8, The Present State of Opinion 


THE conception of evolution is no new idea, it is the human 
idea of history grown larger, large enough to cover the 
whole world. The extension of the idea was gradual, as 
men felt the need of extending it ; and at the same moment 
we find men believing in the external permanence of one 
set of phenomena, in the creation of others, in the evolution 
of others. One authority says human institutions have been 
evolved ; man was created ; the heavens are eternal. Ac- 
cording to another, matter and motion are eternal ; life was 
created ; the rest has been evolved, except, perhaps, the 
evolution theory which was created by Darwin. 

1. Greek Philosophers.—Of the wise men of Greece 
and what they thought of the nature and origin of 
things, I shall say little, for I have no direct acquaintance 
with the writings of those who lived before Aristotle. 
Moreover, though an authority so competent as Zeller has 
written on the ‘Grecian predecessors of Darwin,” most of 
them were philosophers not naturalists, and we are apt to 
read our own ideas into their words. They thought, indeed, 
as we are thinking, about the physical and organic universe, 
and some of them believed it to be, as we do, the result of 


cu, xvi The Evolution of Evolution Theories 283 


a process; but here in most cases ends the resemblance 
between their thought and ours. 

Thus when Anaximander spoke of a fish-like stage in the 
past history of man, this was no prophecy of the modern 
idea that a fish-like form was one of the far-off ancestors of 
backboned animals, it was only a fancy invented to get over 
a difficulty connected with the infancy of the first human 
being. 

Or, when we read that several of these sages reduced 
the world to one element, the ether, we do the progress of 
knowledge injustice if we say that men are simply returning 
to this after more than two thousand years. For that 
conception of the ether which is characteristic of modern 
physical science has been, or is being, slowly attained by 
precise and patient analysis, whereas the ancient conception 
was reached by metaphysical speculation. If we are 
returning to the Greeks, it is on a higher turn of the spiral, - 
so far at least as the ether is concerned. 

When we read that Empedocles sought to explain the 
world as the result of two principles—love and hate— 
working on the four elements, we may, if we are so inclined, 
call these principles “attractive and repulsive forces”; we 
may recognise in them the altruistic and individualistic 
factors in organic evolution, and what not; but Empedocles 
was a poetic philosopher, no far-sighted prophet of evolu- 
tion. 

But the student cannot afford to overlook the lesson 
which Democritus first clearly taught, that we do not 
explain any result until we find out the natural conditions 
which bring it about, that we only understand an effect 
when we are able to analyse its causes. We require a so- 
called ‘‘ mechanical,” or more strictly, a dynamical explana- 
tion of results. It is easy to show that it is advantageous 
for a root to have a root-cap, but we wish to know how the 
cap comes to be there. It is obvious that the antlers of a 
stag are useful weapons, but we must inquire as precisely 
as possible how they first appeared and still grow. 

2, Aristotle.—As in other departments of knowledge, 
so in zoology the work of Aristotle is fundamental. It is 


284 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


wonderful to think of his knowledge of the forms and ways 
of life, or the insight with which he foresaw such useful dis- 
tinctions as that between analogous and homologous organs, 
or his recognition of the fact of correlation, of the advan- 
tages of division of labour within organisms, of the gradual 
differentiation observed in development. He planted seeds 
which grew after long sleep into comparative anatomy and 
classification. Yet with what sublime humility he says: “I 
found no basis prepared, no models to copy. Mine is the 
first step, and therefore a small one, though worked out with 
much thought and hard labour.” Aristotle was not an 
evolutionist, for, although he recognised the changefulness of 
life, the world was to him an eternal fact not a stage in a 
process. 


«Tn nature, the passage from inanimate things to animals is so 
gradual that it is impossible to draw a hard-and-fast line between 
them. After inanimate things come plants, which differ from one 
another in the degree of life which they possess. Compared with 
inert bodies, plants seem endowed with life; compared with 
animals, they seem inanimate. From plants to animals the passage 
is by no means sudden or abrupt; one finds living things in the 
sea about which there is doubt whether they be animals or plants.” 
«¢ Animals are at war with one another when they live in the same 
place and use the same food. If the food be not sufficiently 
abundant they fight for it even with those of the same kind.” 


3. Lucretius——Among the Romans Lucretius gave 
noble expression to the philosophy of Epicurus. I shall 
not try to explain his materialistic theory of the concourse 
of atoms into stable and well-adapted forms, but rather 
quote a few sentences in which he states his belief that the 
earth is the mother of all life, and that animals work out 
their destiny in a struggle for existence. He was a cosmic, 
but hardly an organic evolutionist, for, according to his 
poetic fancy, organisms arose from the earth’s fertile bosom 
andnot bythe gradual transformation of simpler predecessors. 


“Tn the beginning the earth gave forth all kinds of herbage and 
verdant sheen about the hills and over all the plains; the flowery 
meadows glittered with the bright green hue, and next in order to 
the different trees was given a strong and emulous desire of grow- 


cu. xvur The Evolution of Evolution Theories 285 


ing up into the air with full unbridled powers. . . . With good 
reason the earth has gotten the name of mother, since all things 
have been produced out of the earth. 

‘We see that many conditions must meet together in things in 
order that they may beget and continue their kinds ; first a supply 
of food, then a way in which the birth-producing seeds throughout 
the frame may stream from the relaxed limbs.’. . . And many 
races of living things must then have died out and been unable to 
beget and continue their breed. For in the case of all things which 
you see breathing the breath of life, either craft or courage or else 
speed has from the beginning of its existence protected and pre- 
served each particular race. And there are many things which, 
recommended to us by their useful services, continue to exist con- 
signed to our protection. 

‘*Tn the first place, the first breed of lions and the savage races 
their courage has protected, foxes their craft, and stags their prone- 
ness to flight. But light-sleeping dogs with faithful heart in breast, 
and every kind which is born of the seed of beasts of burden, and at 
the same time the woolly flocks and the horned herds, are all con- 
signed to the protection of man. For they have ever fled with 
eagerness from wild beasts, and have ensued peace, and plenty of 
food obtained without their own labour, as we give it in requital of 
their useful services. But those to whom nature has granted none 
of these qualities, so that they could neither live by their own 
means nor perform for us any useful service, in return for which 
we should suffer their kind to feed and be safe under our protection, 
those, you are to know, would lie exposed as a prey and booty 
of others, hampered all in their own death-bringing shackles, until 
nature brought that kind to utter destruction.” 


4. Evolutionists before Darwin.—From Lucretius I 
shall pass to Buffon, for the intervening centuries were un- 
eventful as regards zoology. Hugo Spitzer, one of the histo- 
rians of evolution, finds analogies between certain medieval 
scholastics and the Darwinians of the nineteenth century, 
but these are subtle comparisons, Yet long before Darwin’s 
day there were evolutionists, and the first of these who can | 
be called great was Buffon. 

We must guard against supposing that the works of 
Buffon, or Lamarck, or Darwin were inexplicable creations 
of genius, or that they came like cataclysms, without warning, 
to shatter the conventional traditions of their time. For all 
great workers have their forerunners, who prepare their 


286 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


paths. Therefore in thinking out the history of evolutionist 
theories before that of Buffon, we must take account of 
many forces which began to be influential from the twelfth 
century onwards. ‘Evolution in social affairs has not 
only suggested our ideas of evolution in the other sciences, 
but has deeply coloured them in accordance with the 
particular phase of social evolution current at the time.” ! 
In other words, we must abandon the idea that we can 
understand the history of any science as such, without 
reference to contemporary evolution in other departments 
of activity. The evolution of theories of evolution is bound 
up with the whole progress of the world. 

In trying to determine those social and intellectual forces 
of which the modern conception of organic evolution has 
been a resultant, we should take account of social changes, 
such as the collapse of the feudal system, the crusades, the 
invention of printing, the discovery of America, the French 
Revolution, the beginning of the steam age; of theological 
and religious movements, such as the Protestant Reforma- 
tion and the spread. of Deism; of a long series of evolu- 
tionist philosophers, some of whom were at the same time 
students of the physical sciences, — notably Descartes, 
Spinoza, Leibnitz, Herder, Kant, and Schelling; of the 
acceptance of evolutionary conceptions in regard to other 
orders of facts, especially in regard to the earth and the 
solar system ; and, finally, of those few naturalists, like De 
Maillet and Robinet, who, before Buffon’s day, whispered 
evolutionist heresies. The history of an idea is like that 
of an organism in which cross-fertilisation and composite 
inheritance complicate the pedigree. 

5. Three old Masters.—Among the evolutionists before 
Darwin I shall speak of only three—Buffon, iirasiaus Darwin, 
_ and Lamarck. 

BUFFON (1707-1788) was born to wealth re was wedded 
to Fortune. Hesat in kings’ houses, his statue adorned their 
gardens. As Director of the Jardin du Roi he had oppor- 
tunity to acquire a wide knowledge of animals. He com- 
manded the assistance of able collaborateurs, and his own 


1 Article ‘‘ Evolution’ (P, Geddes) in Chambers's Encyclopedia, 


cu. xvir The Evolution of Evolution Theories 287 


industry was untiring. He was about forty years old when 
he began his great Natural History, and he worked till he 
was fourscore, He lived a full life, the success of which 
we can almost read in the strong confidence of his style. 
‘Le style, c'est "homme méme,” he said; or again, “Le 
style est comme le bonheur ; il vient de la douceur de l’4me.” 
Rousseau called him “La plus belle plume du siécle;” 
Mirabeau said, ‘‘Le plus grand homme de son siécle et de 
bien d’autres ;” Voltaire first mocked and then praised him ; 
and Diderot also eulogised. Buffon was first a man then 
a zoologist, which seems to be the natural, though by no 
means universally recognised, order of precedence, and we 
have pleasant pictures of his handsome person, his magnifi- 
cence, his diplomatic manners, and a splendid genius, which 
he himself called ‘(a supreme capacity for taking pains.” 

Buffon’s culture was very wide. He had an early 
training in mathematics, and translated Newton’s Fluxions ; 
he seems to have been familiar with the chemistry and 
physics of his time; he was curious about everything. 
Before Laplace, he elaborated an hypothesis as to the origin 
of the solar system ; before.Hutton and Lyell, he realised 
that causes like those now at work had in the long past 
sculptured the earth; he had a special theory of heredity 
not unlike Darwin’s, and a by no means narrow theory of 
evolution, in which he recognised the struggle for existence 
and the elimination of the unfit, the influence of isolation 
and of artificial selection, but especially the direct action of 
food, climate, and other surrounding influences upon the 
organism. It is generally allowed that there is in Buffon’s 
writings something of that indefiniteness which often charac- 
terises pioneer works, and a lack of depth not unnatural in 
a survey so broad, but they exhibit some remarkable illustra- 
tions of prophetic genius, and a lively appreciation of 
nature. 

It is probable that Buffon’s treatment of zoology gained 
freedom because he wrote in French, having shaken off the 
shackles which the prevalent custom of writing in Latin 
imposed, and it cannot be doubted that his works did some- 
thing to prepare the way for the future reception of the 


288 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


doctrine of descent. He had a vivid feeling of the unity 
of nature, throwing out hints in regard to the fundamental 
similarity of different forms of matter, suggesting that heat 
and light are atomic movements, denying the existence of 
hard-and-fast lines—“ Le vivant et animé est une propriété 
physique de la matiére!” protesting against crude distinctions 
between plants and animals, and realising above all that 
there is one great family of life. Naturalists had been 
wandering up and down the valleys studying their charac- 
teristic contours ; Buffon took an eagle’s flight and saw the 
connected range of hills,—‘‘lenchainement des étres.” 

ERASMUS DARWIN (1731-1802), grandfather to the 
author of the Origin of Species, was a large-hearted, thought- 
ful physician, whose life was as full of pleasant eccentricities, 
as his stammering speech of wit, and his books of wisdom. 
We have pleasant pictures of the philosophical physician 
of Lichfield and Derby, driving about in a whimsical un- 
stable carriage of his own contrivance, prescribing abundant 
food and cowslip wine, rich in good health and generosity. 
Comparing his writings with those of Buffon, an acquaint- 
ance with which he evidently possessed, we find more 
emotion and intensity, more of the poet and none of the 
diplomatist. He approached the study of organic life on 
the one hand as a physician and physiologist, on the other 
hand as a gardener and lover of plants; and, apart from 
poetic conceits, his writings are characterised by a direct- 
ness and simplicity of treatment which we often describe as 
“ common-sense.” 

He believed that the different kinds of plants and animals 
were descended from a few ancestral forms, or possibly 
from one and the same kind of “vital filament,” and that 
evolutionary change was mainly due to the exertions which 
organisms made to preserve or better themselves. He 
showed that animals were driven to exertion by hunger, by 
love, and by the need of protection, and explained their 
progress as the result of their endeavours. Buffon under- 
rated the transforming influence of action, and laid emphasis 
upon the direct influence of surroundings ; Erasmus Darwin 
emphasised function, and regarded the influence of the 


cu. xvinr The Evolution of Evolution Theories 289 


environment as for the most part indirect. Let us quote 
some conclusions from his Zoonomia (1794) :— 


“Owing to the imperfection of language the offspring is termed 
a new animal, but is in truth a branch or elongation of the 
parent, since a part of the embyron animal is, or was, a part of the 
parent, and therefore in strict language cannot be said to be entirely 
new at the time of its production; and therefore it may retain 
some of the habits of the parent-system.” 

‘The fetus or embryon is formed by apposition of new parts, 
and not by the distention of a primordial nest of germs included 
one within another like the cups of a conjuror.” 

‘From their first rudiment, or primordium, to the termination 
of their lives, all animals undergo perpetual transformations ; which 
are in part produced by their own exertions in consequence of 
their desires and aversions, of their pleasures and their pains, or 
of irritations, or of associations ; and many of these acquired forms 
or propensities are transmitted to their posterity.” 

‘* As air and water are supplied to animals in sufficient profusion, 
the three great objects of desire, which have changed the forms of 
many animals by their exertions to gratify them, are those of lust, 
hunger, and security.” 

‘« This idea of the gradual generation of all things seems to have 
been as familiar to the ancient philosophers as to the modern ones, 
and to have given rise to the beautiful hieroglyphic figure of the 
mp&rov @dv, or first great egg, produced by night, that is, whose 

_ origin is involved in obscurity, and animated by épus, that is, by 
Divine Love; from whence proceeded all things which exist.” 


On LAMARCK (1744-1829) success did not shine as it 
did on the Comte de Buffon or on Dr. Erasmus Darwin. 
His life was often so hard that we wonder he did not say 
more about the struggle for existence. As a youth of six- 
teen, destined for the Church, he rides off on a bad horse 
to join the French army, then fighting in Germany, and 
bravely wins promotion on his first battle-field. After the 
peace he is sent into garrison at Toulon and Monaco, 
where his scientific enthusiasm is awakened by the Flora 
of the south. Retiring in weakened health from military 
service, he earns his living in a Parisian banker’s office, 
devotes his spare energies to the study of plants, and 
writes a Flore francaise in three volumes, the publica- 
tion of which (1778) at the royal press was secured by 

U 


290 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


Buffon’s patronage. As tutor to Buffon’s son, he travels 
in Europe and visits some of the famous gardens, and we 
‘can hardly doubt that Buffon influenced Lamarck: in many 
ways. After much toil as a literary hack and scientific 
drudge, he is elected to what we would now call a Professor- 
ship of Invertebrate Zoology, a department at that time 
chaotic. In 1794 he began his lectures, and each year 
brought increased order to his classification and museum 
alike. At the same time, however, he was lifting his anchors 
from the orthodox moorings, relinquishing his belief in the 
constancy of species, following (we know not with what 
consciousness) the current which had already borne Buffon 
and Erasmus Darwin to evolutionary prospects. In 1802 
he published Researches on the Organisation of Living 
Bodies; in 1809 a Philosophie Zoologique; from 1816- 
1822 his Natural History of Invertebrate Animals, a large 
work in seven volumes, part of which the blind naturalist 
dictated to his daughter. Busy as he must have been with 
zoology, his restless intellect found time to speculate—it 
must be confessed to little purpose—on chemical, physical, 
and meteorological subjects. Thus he ran an unsuccessful 
tilt against Lavoisier’s chemistry, and published for ten 
years annual forecasts of the weather, which seem to have 
been almost always wrong. Nor did Lamarck add to his 
reputation by a theory of Hydrogeology, and his scientific 
friends who were loyal specialists shrugged their shoulders 
more and more over his intellectual knight-errantry. 
Poverty also clouded his later years, his treasured 
collections had to be sold for bread, his theories made no 
headway, his merits were unrecognised. Yet now a La- 
marckian school is strong in France and in America, and 
even those who deny his doctrines admit that he was one 
of the bravest of pioneers. 

Of Lamarck’s Philosophie Zoologigue, Haeckel says, 

“ This admirable work is the first connected and thoroughly 
logical exposition of the theory of descent.” And again, he 
says, “To Lamarck will remain the immortal glory of 
having for the first time established the theory of descent 
as an independent scientific generalisation of the first order, 


cu. xvi Zhe Evolution of Evolution Theories 291 


as the foundation of the whole of Biology.” But the verdict 
of the majority of naturalists in regard to Lamarck’s doctrines 
has not tended to be eulogistic. Cuvier, in his Zloge de M. 
de Lamarck delivered before the French Academy in 1832, 
said, ‘A system resting on such foundations may amuse 
the imagination of a poet, etc, .. . but it cannot for a 
moment bear the examination of any one who has dissected 
the hand, the viscera, or even a feather.’ The great Cuvier 
was a formidable obscurantist. 
But let us hear Lamarck himself :— 


“ Nature in all her work proceeds gradually, and could not pro- 
duce all the animals at once. At first she formed only the simplest, 
and passed from these on to the most complex.” 

‘¢ The limits of so-called species are not so constant and unvary- 
ing as is commonly supposed. Spontaneous generation started 
each particular series, but thereafter one form gives rise to another. 
In life we should see, as it were, a ramified continuity if certain 
species had not been lost.” 

‘©The operations of Nature in the production of animals show 
that there is a primary and predominant cause which gives to 
animal life the power of progressive organisation, of gradually 
complicating and perfecting not only the organism as a whole, but 
each system of organs in particular.” 

“ First Law. Life by its inherent power tends continually to 
increase the volume of every living body, and to extend the 
dimensions of its parts up to a self-regulated limit. 

‘* Second Law. The production of a new organ in an animal body 
results from the occurrence of some new need which continues to 
make itself felt, and from a new movement which this need origin- 
ates and sustains. 

“‘ Third Law. The development of organs and their power of 
action are constantly determined by the use of these organs. 

‘* Fourth Law. All that has been acquired, begun, or changed in 
the structure of individuals during the course of their life is pre- 
served in reproduction and transmitted to the new individuals 
which spring from those which have experienced the changes.” 

These four laws I have cited from Lamarck’s Histocre Naturelle, 
but in illustration of the emphasis with which he insisted on use 
and disuse, I take the following passages, translated by Samuel 
Butler, from the Phzlosophie Zoologique :— 

‘¢ Every considerable and sustained change in the surroundings 
of any animal involves a real change in its needs,” 


292 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


‘‘Such change of needs involves the necessity of changed 
action in order to satisfy these needs, and, in consequence, of new 
habits.” 

‘*Tt follows that such and such parts, formerly less used, are 
now more frequently employed, and in consequence become more 
highly developed ; new parts also become insensibly evolved in the 
creature by its own efforts from within.” 

‘« These gains or losses of organic development, due to use or 
disuse, are transmitted to offspring, provided they have been com- 
mon to both sexes, or to the animals from which the offspring have 
descended.” 


The historian of the €volution of evolution theories 
should take account of many workers besides Buffon, 
Erasmus Darwin, and Lamarck; of Treviranus (1776- 
1837), whose Biology or Philosophy of Living Nature (1802- 
1805) is full of evolutionary suggestions ; of Geoffroy St. 
Hilaire, who in 1830, before the French Academy of 
Science, fought with Cuvier, the fellow-worker of his youth, 
an intellectual duel on the question of descent; of Goethe 
who, in his eighty-first year, heard the tidings of Geoffroy’s 
defeat with an interest which transcended the political 
anxieties of the time, and whose own epic of evolution sur- 
passes that of Lucretius ; of Oken’s speculative mist, amid 
which the light of evolutionary ideas danced like a will-o’- 
the-wisp; of many others in whose mind the truth grew if 
it did not blossom. But we must now recognise the work 
of Charles Darwin. 

6. Darwin.—Though the general tenor of Darwin’s life 
—the impression of an industrious open-minded observer 
and thinker, the picture of a man full of mercy, kindliness, 
and peace—was familiar to many, his biography has filled 
in those little details which make our impression living. 
We see him now, as in a Holbein picture, with all the 
paraphernalia of daily pursuit round about him. His high 
chair, his orderly shelves, his torn-up reference books, his 
window-sill laboratory, his yellow-back novels, his snuff- 
box, and a hundred little touches, make the picture alive. 
We learn, too, his methods of laborious but never toilsome 
work, and the gradual progress of his thought from the con- 
ventionalism of youth to the convictions of matured man- 


cu. xvii The Evolution of Evolution Theories 293 


hood. We read the curve of his moods, steadier than that 
of most men, without any climax of speculative ecstasy, free 
from any fall to a depth of pessimism. We hear his own 
sincere voice in his simple autobiography, and even more 
clearly perhaps in the unconstrainedness of his abundant 
letters. There was seldom a great life so devoid of little- 
ness, seldom a record of thought so free from subtlety. 
There seems to be almost nothing. hid which we could 
wish revealed, or uncovered which we could wish hidden. 
Darwin’s life was as open as the country around his her- 
mitage, 

Marcus Aurelius gives thanks in his roll of blessings 
that he had not been suffered to keep quails ; so Darwin, in 
recounting his mercies, does not forget to be grateful for 
having been preserved from the snare of becoming a 
specialist. From a more partial point of view, we have 
reason to be thankful that he became a specialist, not in 
one department, but in many. As a disciple of Linnzus, 
he described the species of barnacles in one volume, and 
followed in the steps of Cuvier in anatomising them in 
another. Of tissues and cells he knew less, being as 
regards these items an antediluvian, and outside the guild 
of those who dexterously wield the razor, and in so doing 
observe the horoscope of the organism. Of protoplasm, 
in regard to which modern biology says so much and knows 
so little, he was not ignorant, for did he not study the 
marvels of the state known as “aggregation” ? 

But it is not for special research that men are most 
grateful to Darwin. Undoubtedly, if clear insight into the 
world around us be esteemed in itself of value, the author 
of Lusectivorous Plants, The Fertilisation of Orchids, The 
Movements of Plants, The Origin of Coral Reefs, The 
Formation of Vegetable Mould, etc., runs no risk of being 
forgotten. But though our possession of these results swells 
the meed of praise, we usually regard them as outside of 
Darwin’s real work, which, as every one knows, was his 
contribution to the theory of organic life. 

This contribution was threefold—(a) He placed the 
theory of descent on a sure basis; (4) he shed the light 


294 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


of this doctrine on various groups of phenomena ; and (c) he 
essayed the problem of the factors in evolution. 

(2) The man who makes us believe a fact is to us 
more important than the original discoverer. And so 
Darwin gets credit for inventing the theory of descent, 
which in principle is as old as clear thought itself, and in 
its biological application was stated a hundred years before 
the publication of the-Ovigin of Species (1859). The con- 
ception was no new one, but Darwin first made men believe 
it. The idea was not his, but he gave it to many. He did 
not originate ; he established. He converted naturalists to 
an evolutionary conception of the organic world. 

(4) Having got people to believe the theory of 
descent,—the theory of development out of preceding 
conditions,—Darwin went on to show how the conception 
would illumine all facts to which it was applicable. In his 
work on the expression of emotions, and in scattered chap- 
ters, he showed how the light might be shed upon the 
secrets of mental activity. Whenever it was seen that 
the doctrine could justify itself in regard to general organic 
life, it was eagerly seized as an organon for the exploration 
of special sets of facts. The phcenix revived and flew 
croaking amid the smoke of burning systems. How one 
discussed the evolution of language, and another that of 
industry ; how the natural history of ethics was sketched 
by one thinker, and the descent of institutions by another ; 
how the conception has forced its way into the cloister 
and the political arena, and has even found expression in 
theories of literature, art, and religion, —is an often-repeated 
story. 

(¢) We have noticed that Buffon, and, let us add, Trevir- 
anus, firmly maintained that the direct influence of the 
external conditions of life was an important factor in evolu- 
tion. We have also seen that Erasmus Darwin and Lamarck 
were strongly convinced of the transforming power of use 
and disuse. When Charles Darwin began to think and 
write on the origin of species, he also recognised the trans- 
forming influences of function and of environment. But 
with the Buffonian or Lamarckian position he was never 


| 


cn. xvirr Lhe Evolution of Evolution Theories 295 


satisfied ; he advanced to one of his own—to the theory of 
natural selection, the characteristic feature of Darwinism. 

Let us state this theory, which was foreseen by Matthew, 
Wells, Naudin, and others, was developed simultaneously 
by Darwin and by Alfred Russel Wallace, and has attained 
‘remarkable acceptance throughout the world. 

All plants and animals produce offspring which, though 
like their parents, usually differ from them in possessing 
some new features or variations. These are of more or 
less obscure origin, and are often termed fortuitous. or in- 
definite. But throughout nature there is a struggle for 
existence in which only a small percentage of the organisms 
born survive to maturity or reproduction. Those which 
survive do so because of the individual peculiarities which 
have made them in some way more fit to survive than their 
fellows. Moreover the favourable variation possessed by 
the survivors is handed on as an inheritance to their off- 
spring, and tends to be intensified when the new generation 
is bred from parents both possessing the happily advan- 
tageous character. This natural fostering of advantageous 
variations and natural elimination of those less fit, explain 
the general modification and adaptation of species, as well 
as the general progress from simpler to higher forms of 
life. 

This theory that favourable variations may be fostered 
and accumulated by natural selection till useful adaptations 
result is the chief characteristic of Darwinism.’ Of this 
theory Prof. Ray Lankester says :; “ Darwin by his discovery 
of the mechanical principle of organic evolution, namely, the 
survival of the fittest in the struggle for existence, completed 
the doctrine of evolution, and gave it that unity and au- 
thority which was necessary in order that it should reform 
the whole range of philosophy.” And again he says: ‘The 
history of zoology as a science is therefore the history of 
the great biological doctrine of the evolution of living things 
by the natural selection of varieties in the struggle for exist- 
ence,—since that doctrine is the one medium whereby all 
the phenomena of life, whether of form or function, are 
rendered capable of explanation by the laws of physics and 


296 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


chemistry, and so made the subject-matter of a true science 
or study of causes.” I have quoted these two sentences 
because they illustrate better than any others that I have 
seen to what exaggeration enthusiasm for a theory will lead 
a strong intellect. But listen to a few sentences from 
Samuel Butler, which I quote because they well illustrate 
that the critics of Darwinism may also be extreme, and in 
the hope that the contrast may be sufficiently interesting to 
induce you to think out the question for yourselves. 

“Buffon planted, Erasmus Darwin and Lamarck watered, 
but it was Mr. Darwin who said ‘That fruit is ripe,’ and 
shook it into his lap. ... Darwin was heir to a dis- 
credited truth, and left behind him an accredited fallacy. 

. Do animals and plants grow into conformity with 
their surroundings because they and their fathers and 
mothers take pains, or because their uncles and aunts go 
away? ... The theory that luck is the main means of 
organic modification is the most absolute denial of God 
which it is possible for the human mind to conceive. . . .” 

7. Darwin's Fellow-workers.—But we must bring this 
historical sketch to a close by referring to four of the more 
prominent of Darwin’s fellow-workers—Wallace, Spencer, 
Haeckel, and Huxley. 

ALFRED RUSSEL WALLACE, contemporary with Darwin, 
not only in years, but in emphasising the truth of evolution- 
ary conceptions, and in recognising the fact of natural 
selection, has been justly called the Nestor of Biology. No 
one will be slow to appreciate the splendid unselfishness 
with which he has for thirty years sunk himself in the Dar- 
winian theory, or the scientific disinterestedness which leads 
him from the very title of his last work! to its close, to 
say so little—perhaps too littkk—of the important part 
which he has played in evolving the doctrine. ‘It was,” 
Romanes says, “in the highest degree dramatic that the 
great idea of natural selection should have occurred inde- 
pendently and in precisely the same form to two working 
naturalists ; that these naturalists should have been country- 
men; that they should have agreed to publish their theory 

1 Darwinism, London, 1889. 


cu. xvir1 The Evolution of Evolution Theories 294 


on the same day ; and last, but not least, that, through the 
many years of strife and turmoil which followed, these two 
English naturalists consistently maintained towards each 
other such feelings of magnanimous recognition that it is 
hard to say whether we should most admire the intellectual 
or the moral qualities which, in relation to their common 
labours, they have displayed.” 

Mr. Wallace is a naturalist in the old and truest sense, 
rich in a world-wide experience of animal life, at once 
specialist and generaliser, a humanist thinker and a social 
striver, and a man of science who realises the spiritual 
aspect of the world. 

He believes in the “ overwhelming importance of natural 
selection over all other agencies in the production of new 
species,” differs from Darwin in regard to sexual selection, 
to which he attaches little importance, and agrees with 
Weismann in regard to the non-inheritance of acquired 
characters. 

But the exceptional feature in Wallace’s scientific philo- 
sophy is his contention that the higher characteristics of 
man are due to a special evolution hardly distinguishable 
from creation. 

Wallace finds their only explanation in the hypothesis 
of “a spiritual essence or nature, capable of progressive 
development under favourable conditions.” 

HERBERT SPENCER must surely have been an evolution- 
ist by birth; there was no hesitation even in the first strides 
he took with the evolution-torch uplifted. A ponderer on 
the nature of things, and the possessor of encyclopaedic 
knowledge, he grasped what was good in Lamarck’s work, 
and as early as 1852 published a plea for the theory of 
organic evolution which is still remarkable in its strength 
and clearness. The work of Darwin supplied corroboration 
and fresh material, and in the Principles of Biology (1863-66) 
the theory of organic evolution first found philosophic, as 
distinguished from merely scientific expression. To Spencer 
we owe the familiar phrase “the survival of the fittest,” 
and that at first sight puzzling generalisation, “ Evolution is 
an integration of matter and concomitant dissipation of 


298 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


motion, during which the matter passes from an indefinite 
incoherent homogeneity to a definite coherent heterogeneity, 
and during which the retained motion (energy) undergoes a 
parallel transformation.” He has given his life to establish- 
ing this generalisation, and applying it to physical, biological, 
psychological, and social facts. As to the factors in organic 
evolution, he emphasises the change-producing influences of 
environment and function, and recognises that natural selec- 
tion has been a very important means of progress. 

ERNST HAECKEL, Professor of Zoology in Jena, and 
author of a great series of monographs on Radiolarians, 
Sponges, Jellyfish, etc., may be well called the Darwin of 
Germany. He has devoted his life to applying the doctrine 
of descent, and to making it current coin among the people. 
Owing much of his motive to Darwin, he stood for a time 
almost alone in Germany as the champion of a heresy. 
Before the publication of Darwin’s Descent of Man, Haeckel 
was the only naturalist who had recognised the import of 
sexual selection; and of his Natural History of Creation 
Darwin writes: “If this work had appeared before my 
essay had been written, I should probably never have com- 
pleted it.” His most important expository works are the 
above-mentioned Natiirliche Schipfungsgeschichte (1st ed. 
1868; 8th ed. 1889); and his Amthropogente (1874, trans- 
lated as The Evolution of Man). These books are very 
brilliantly written, though they offend many by their remorse- 
less consistency, and by their impatience with theological 
dogma and teleological interpretation. His greatest work, 
however, is of a less popular character, namely, the Generelle 
Morphologie (2 vols., Berlin, 1866), which in its reasoned 
orderliness and clear generalisations ranks beside Spencer’s 
Principles of Biology. 

HUXLEY, by whose work the credit of British schools of 
zoology has been for many years enhanced, was one of the first 
to stand by Darwin, and to wield a sharp intellectual sword 
in defence and attack. No one has fought for the doctrine 
of descent in itself and in its consequences with more keen- 
ness and success than the author of AZan’s Place in Nature 
(1863), American Addresses, Lay Sermons, etc., and no one 


cu. xvi The Evolution of Evolution Theories 299 


has championed the theory of natural selection with more 
confident consistency or with more skilfully handled 
weapons, : 

8. The Present State of Opinion.—As Wallace says in 
the preface to his work on Darwinism, “Descent with 
modification is now universally accepted as the order of 
nature in the organic world.” But, while this is true, there 
remains much uncertainty in regard to the way in which the 
progressive ascent of life has come about, as to the mechan- 
ism of the great nature-loom. The relative importance of 
the various factors in evolution is very uncertain.! 

The condition of evolution is variability, or the tendency 
which animals have to change. The primary factors of 
evolution are those which produce variations, which cause 
organic inequilibrium. Darwin spoke of variations as 
“ fortuitous,” “‘ indefinite,” “ spontaneous,” etc. and frankly 
confessed that he could not explain how most of them arose. 

Ultimately all variations in organisms must be due to 
variations in their environment, that is to say, to changes in 
the system of which organisms form a part. But this is 
only a general truism. 


1 All naturalists, however uncertain in regard to the factors in 
evolution, accept the doctrine of descent—the general conception of 
evolution—as a theory which has justified itself. It is not indeed so 
demonstrable as is the doctrine of the conservation of energy, but it is 
almost as confidently accepted. Few naturalists, however, have 
attempted any philosophical justification of their belief. This is strange, 
since it should surely give pause to the dogmatic evolutionist to reflect 
that his own theory has been evolved like other beliefs, that his 
scientific demonstration of it rests upon assumptions which have also 
been evolved, that the entire system of evolutionary thought must be a 
phase in the development of opinion, that, in short, he cannot be 
dogmatic without being self-contradictory. See A. J. Balfour's Defence 
of Philosophic Doubt, pp. 260-274 (London, 1879). In regard to the 
philosophical aspects of the doctrine of evolution see Prof. Knight's 
essay on ‘‘ Ethical Philosophy and Evolution” in his Studies ix Philo- 
sophy and Literature (Lond. 1879), and, with additions, in Essays in 
Philosophy (Boston and New York, 1890) ; Prof. St. George Mivart’s 
Contemporary Evolution (Lond. 1876); E. von Hartmann’s Wahrheit 
und Irrthum im Darwinismus; an article by Prof. Tyndall on ‘' Vir- 
chow and Evolution” in Winetcenth Century, Nov. 1878 ; and articles 
on ‘‘ Evolution” by Huxley and Sully in Axcyclopedia Britannica. , 


300 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


There are evidently three direct ways in which organic 
changes may be produced: (1) From the nature of the 
organism itself; z.e. from constitutional or germinal peculiar- 
ities which are ultimately traceable to influences from 
without ; (2) from changes in its functions or activity, in 
other words, from use and disuse; or (3) from the direct 
influence of the external conditions of life—tfood, temperature, 
moisture, etc. 

Thus some naturalists follow Buffon in emphasising the 
moulding influence of the environment, or agree with 
Lamarck in maintaining that change of function produces 
change of structure. But at present the tide is against 
these opinions, because of the widespread scepticism as to 
the transmissibility of characters thus acquired. 

Those who share this scepticism refer the origin of 
variations to the nature of the organism, to the mingling 
of the two different cells from which the individual life 
begins, to the instability involved in the complexity of the 
protoplasm, to the oscillating balance between vegetative 
and reproductive processes, and so on. 

One prevalent opinion regards variations as arbitrary 
sports in “a chapter of accidents,” but according to the 
views of a minority variations are for the most part definite, 
occurring in a few directions, fixed by the constitutional 
bias of the organism. The minority are “Topsian” evolu- 
tionists who believe that the modification of species has 
taken place by cumulative growth, influenced by function 
and environment, and pruned by natural selection. To the 
majority the theory that new species result from the action 
of natural selection on numerous, spontaneous, indefinite 
variations, is the “ quintessence of Darwinism ” and of truth. 

Until we know much more about the primary factors 
which directly cause variations it will not be possible to 
decide in regard to the precise scope of natural selection 
and the other secondary factors which foster or accumulate, 
thin or prune, which in short establish a new organic 
equilibrium. ‘The argument has been too much in regard 
to possibilities, too little in regard to observed facts of 
variation. 


cu. xvi The Evolution of Evolution Theortes 301 


The secondary factors of evolution may be ranked under 
two heads :— 

1. Natural Selection, or the survival of the fittest in the 
struggle for existence, and 2. Isolation, or the various means 
by which species tend to be separated into sections which 
do not interbreed. 

Natural selection is a phrase descriptive of the course of 
nature, of the survival of the fit and the elimination of the 
unfit in the struggle for existence. It involves on the one 
hand the survival, z.e. the nutritive and reproductive success 
of the variations fittest to survive in given conditions, and 
on the other hand the destruction or elimination of forms 
less fit. Suitable variations pay ; nature or natural selection 
justifies and fosters them. Maternal sacrifice or cunning 
cruelty, the milk of animal kindness or teeth strong to 
rend, distribution in space or rate of reproduction, are all 
affected by natural selection. But it is another thing to say 
that all the adaptations and well-endowed species that we 
know have been produced by the action of natural selection 
on fortuitous, indefinite variations. This is what Samuel 
Butler calls the “accredited fallacy.” 

Secondly, there seem to be a great many ways by which 
a species may be divided into two sections which do not 
interbreed, and if this isolation be common it must help 
greatly in divergent evolution. i 

Thus Romanes, who has been the chief exponent of the 
importance of isolation, on which Gulick has also insisted, 
says: ‘ Without isolation, or the prevention of free inter- 
crossing, organic evolution is in no case possible. It is 
isolation that has been ‘the exclusive means of modifica- 
tion,’ or more correctly, the universal condition to it. 
Heredity and variability being given, the whole theory of 
organic evolution becomes a theory of the causes and con- 
ditions which lead to isolation.” 


302 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 
‘SUMMARY OF EVOLUTION THEORIES. 
Variations all ultimately due to External Influences. 

g Direct Organismal, Use and 

8 action of the constitutional, disuse and 

| environment congenital, change 

m| ~ produces or germinal of function 

a environ- variations produce 

‘= mental may be either functional 

m variations, definite or indefinite. 


Secondary Factors. 


which 
if trans:missible, 


may 
accumulate 

as 
environ- 
mental 

modifications 

of 

species, 


* 


All'cases 


: (certainly 
transmis- 
sible). 


By natural 
: selection in 


By the 
persistence 
of the original: the struggle 
conditions : for existence 


these may : these may 
grow into : give rise to 
new z new 
species, species. 


eer er | 


may 


be affectied by 


variations 


whtich, 
if transmissible, 


may 
accumulate 
as 
functional 
modi- 
fications 
of 
species. 


x 


‘ isolaition.”” 


The process of natural selection will affect all cases, but is 
less essential for those marked *, 


“suOTIeIIeA JO ULSI 


§ Jo UISIO 


‘saroad 


CHAPTER XIX 
THE INFLUENCE OF HABITS AND SURROUNDINGS 


1. The Influence of Function—2. The Influence of Surroundings— 
3. Our own Environment 


1. The Influence of Function.—A skilled observer can 
often discern a man’s occupation from his physiognomy, 
his shoulders, or his hands. In some unhealthy occupa- 
tions the death-rate is three times that in others. Disuse 
of such organs as muscles tends to their degeneration, for 
the nerves which control them lose their tone and the 
circulation of blood is affected; while on the other hand 
increased exercise is within certain limits associated with 
increased development. A force de forger on devient 
Jorgeron. 

If we knew more about animals we might be able to cite 
many cases in which change of function produced change of 
structure, but there are few careful observations bearing on 
this question. 

Even if we could gather many illustrations of the 
influence of use and disuse on individual animals, we should 
still have to find out whether the precise characters thus 
acquired by individuals were transmissible to the offspring, 
or whether any secondary effects of the acquired characters 
were transmissible, or whether these changes had no effect 
upon succeeding generations. As there are few facts to argue 
from, the answers given to these questions are not reliable. 

It is easy to find hundreds of cases in which the constant 


304 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


characters of animals may be hypothetically interpreted as 
the result of use or disuse. Is the torpedo-like shape of 
swift swimmers due to their rapid motion through the 
water, do burrowing animals necessarily become worm-like, 
has the giraffe lengthened its neck by stretching it, have 
hoofs been developed by running on hard ground, are horns 
responses to butting, are diverse shapes of teeth the results 
of chewing diverse kinds of food, are cave-animals blind 
because they have ceased to use their eyes, are snails lop- 
sided because the shell has fallen to one side, is the 
asymmetry in the head of flat fishes due to the efforts made 
by the ancestral fish to use its lower eye after it had formed 
the habit of lying flat on the bottom, is the woodpecker’s 
long tongue the result of continuous probing into holes, are 
webbed feet due to swimming efforts, has the food-canal in 
vegetarian animals been mechanically lengthened, do the 
wing bones and muscles of the domesticated duck compare 
unfavourably with those of the wild duck because the habit 
of sustained flight has been lost by the former ? 

But these interpretations have not been verified; they 
are only probable. “It is infinitely easy,” Semper says, 
“to form a fanciful idea as to how this or that fact may be 
hypothetically explained, and very little trouble is needed to 
imagine some process by which hypothetical fundamental 
causes—equally fanciful—may have led to the result which 
has been actually observed. But when we try to prove by 
experiment that this imaginary process of development is 
indeed the true and inevitable one, much time and laborious 
research are indispensable, or we find ourselves wrecked on 
insurmountable difficulties.” 

Not a few naturalists believe in the inherited effects 
of functional change mainly because the theory is simple 
and logically sufficient. If use and disuse alter the 
structure of individuals, if the results are transmitted and 
accumulate in similar conditions for generations, we require 
no other explanation of many structures. 

The reasons why not a few naturalists disbelieve in the 
inherited effects of functional change are (1) that definite 
proof is wanting, (2) that it is difficult to understand how 


cu. xix Lnfluence of Habits and Surroundings 305 


changes produced in the body by use or disuse can be 
transmitted to the offspring, (3) that the theory of the 
accumulation of (unexplained) favourable variations in the 
course of natural selection seems logically sufficient. I 
should suspend judgment, because it is unprofitable to argue 
when ascertained facts are few. 

But if you like to argue about probabilities, the following 
considerations may be suggestive :— 

The natural powers of animals—horses, dogs, birds, and 
others—can be improved by training and education, and 
animals can be taught tricks more or less new to them, but 
we have no precise information as to any changes of 
structure associated with these acquirements. 

Individual animals are sometimes demonstrably affected 
by use or disuse. Thus Packard cites a few cases in which 
some animals—usually with normal eyes—have had these 
affected by disuse and darkness ; he instances the variations 
in the eyes of a Myriapod and an Insect living in partial 
daylight near the entrance of caves, the change in the eyes 
of the common Crustacean Gammarus pulex after confine- 
ment in darkness, the fact that the eyes of some other 
Crustaceans in a lake were smaller the deeper the habitat. 

There are many more or less blind animals, and Packard 
says ‘‘no animal or series of generations of animals, wholly 
or in part, can lose the organs of vision unless there is some 
appreciable physical cause for it.” If so, it is probable that 
the appreciable physical cause has been a dvect factor in 
producing the blindness. 

Not a few young animals have structures, such as eyes 
and legs, which are not used and soon disappear in adult 
life. Thus the little crab Prznotheres, which lives inside 
bivalves and sea-cucumbers, keeps its eyes until it has 
established itself within its host. Then they are completely 
covered over and degenerate. The same is true of many 
internal parasites, and Semper concludes that ‘we must 
refer the loss of sight to disuse of the organ.” Perhaps the 
same is true of some blind cave-animals, in which the eyes 
are less degenerate in the young, and of the mole, whose 
embryos have between the eyes and the brain normal optic 

x 


306 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


nerves which usually degenerate in each individual life- 
time. 

The theory that many structures in animals are due to 
the inherited results of use and disuse has this advantage, 
that it suggests a primary cause of change, whereas the 
other theory assumes the occurrence of favourable variations 
and proceeds to show how they might be accumulated in 
the course of natural selection, that is to say by a secondary 
factor in evolution. 

When we find in a large number of entirely distinct 
forms that the same habit of life is associated with the same 
peculiarities, there is a likelihood that the habit is a direct 
factor in evolving these. Thus sluggish and sedentary 
animals in many different classes tend to develop skeletons 
of lime, as in sponges, corals, sedentary worms, lamp-shells, 
Echinoderms, barnacles, molluscs. Professor Lang has re- 
cently made a careful study of sedentary creatures, and this 
result at least is certain that the same peculiarity often occurs 
in many different types with little in common except that 
they are sedentary. But till one can show that sedentary life 
necessarily involves for instance a skeleton of lime or some- 
thing equivalent, we are still dealing only with probabilities. 

2. The Influence of Surroundings.—In ancient times 
men saw the threads of their life passing through the hands 
of three sister-fates—of one who held the distaff, of another 
who offered flowers, and of a third who bore the abhorred 
shears of death. In Norseland the young child was visited by 
three sister Norns, who brought characteristic gifts of past, 
present, and future, which ruled the life as surely as did 
the hands of the three Fates. So too in days of scientific 
illumination, we think of the dread three, but, clothing our 
thoughts in other words, speak of life as determined by the 
organism’s legacy or inheritance, by force of habit or 
function, and by the influences of external conditions or 
environment, What the living organism is to begin with, 
what it does or does not in the course of its life, and what 
surrounding influences play upon it,—these are the three 
Fates, the three Norns, the three Factors of Life. { Organ- 
ism, function, and environment are the sides of the bio- 


cu. xix Lnfluence of Habits and Surroundings 304 


logical prism. ) Thus we try to analyse the light of life, But 
inheritance in its widest sense is only another name for the 
organism itself, and function is simply the organism’s activity. 
The organism is real; the environment is real, in it we live 
and move; function consists of action and reaction between 
these two realities. Yet the capital which the organism 
has to begin with is very important; conduct has some 
relation to character, and function to structure; the sur- 
roundings—the dew of earth and the sunshine of heaven 
—silently mould the individual destiny. 

A living animal is almost always either acting upor its 
surroundings or being acted upon by them, and life is the 
relation between two variables—a changeful organism and 
a changeful environment. And since animals do not and 
cannot live zz vacuo, they should be thought of in relation 
to their surroundings. You may kill the body and cut it to 
pieces, and the result may be interesting, but you have lost 
the animal just as you lose a picture if you separate figure 
from figure, and all from the associated landscape or interior. 
The three Fates are sisters, they are thoroughly intelligible 
only as a Trinity. 

The most certain of all the relations between an organism 
and its surroundings is the most difficult to express. We 
see a small whirlpool on a river, remaining for days or 
weeks apparently constant, with the water circling round 
unceasingly, bearing the same flotsam of leaves and twigs. 
But though the eddy seems the same for many days, it is 
always changing, currents are flowing in and out; it is the 
constancy of the stream and its bed which produces the 
apparent constancy of the whirlpool. So, in some measure, 
is it with an animal in relation to its surroundings. Streams 
of matter and energy are continually passing in and out. 
Though we cannot see it with our eyes, the organism is 
indeed a whirlpool. It is ever being unmade and remade, 
and owes much of its apparent constancy to the fact that 
the conditions in which it lives—the currents of its stream 
—are within certain limits uniform. 

But as we cannot understand the material aspects of an 
animal’s life without considering the streams of matter and 


308 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


energy which pass in and out, neither can we understand 
its higher life apart from its surroundings. 

To attempt a natural history of isolated animals, whether 
alive or dead, is like trying to study man apart from society. 
For it is only when we know animals as they live and move 
that we discover how clever, beautiful, and human they 
are. Thus Gilbert White’s Selborne is a natural history ; and 
therefore we began our studies with the natural life of 
animals—their competition and helpfulness, their adaptations 
to diverse kinds of haunts, their shifts and tricks, their 
industries and their loves. 

At present, however, we have to do with the relation 
between external and internal changes. We must find out 
what the environment of an organism is, and what power it 
has. Ina smithy we see a bar of hot iron being hammered 
into useful form. Around a great anvil are four smiths 
with their hammers. Each smites in his own fashion as 
the bar passes under his grasp. The first hammer falls, 
and while the bar is still quivering like a living thing it 
receives another blow. This is repeated many times till the 
thing of use is perfected. By force of smiting one becomes 
a smith, and by dint of blows the bar of iron becomes 
an anchor. So is it with the organism. In its youth 
especially, it comes under the influence of nature’s hammers ; 
it may become fitter for life, or it may be battered out of 
existence altogether. Let us try to analyse the various 
environmental factors. 

(a) Pressures.—First we may consider those lateral and 
vertical pressures due to air or water currents and to 
the gentle but potent force of gravity. The shriek of the 
wind as it prunes the trees, the swish of the water as it 
moulds the sponges and water-leaves, illustrate the tunes of 
those pressure-hammers. Under artificial pressure embryos 
have been known to broaden ; even the division of the egg is 
affected by gravity ; water currents mould shells and corals. 
The influence of want of room must also be noticed, for by 
artificial overcrowding naturalists have slowed the rate of 
development and reared dwarf broods; and the rate of 
human mortality sometimes varies with the size of the 


cu. xix [Influence of Habits and Surroundings 309 


dwelling. It is difficult, however, to abstract the influence 
of restricted space from associated abnormal conditions, 

(4) Chemical Influences.—Quieter, but more potent, are 
the chemical influences which damp or fan the fire of life, 
which corrode the skin or drug the system, which fatten or 
starve, depress or stimulate. Along with these we must 
include that most important factor—food. 

When a lighted piece of tinder is placed in a vessel 
full of oxygen it burns more actively. Similarly, super- 
abundance of oxygen makes insects jump, makes the 
simplest animals more agile, and causes the ‘ phosphores- 
cent” lights of luminous insects to glow more brightly ; 
and young creatures usually develop more or less rapidly 
according as the aération is abundant or deficient. The 
most active animals—birds and insects—live in the air and 
have much air in their bodies ; sluggish animals often live 
where oxygen is scarce; changes in the quality of the 
atmosphere may have been of importance in the historical 
evolution of animals. Fresh air influences the pitch of 
human life, and lung diseases increase in direct ratio to 
the amount of crowded indoor labour in an area. 

By keeping tadpoles in unnatural conditions the usual 
duration of the gilled stage may be prolonged for two or three 
years. The well-known story of the Axolotl and the Ambly- 
stoma is suggestive but not convincing of the influence of 
surroundings. These two newt-like Amphibians differ slightly 
from one another, in this especially that the Axolotl retains 
its gills after it has developed lungs, while the Ammblystoma 
loses them. Both forms may reproduce, and they were 
originally referred to different genera. But some Axolotls 
which had been kept with scant water in the Jardin des 
Plantes in Paris turned into the Amdélystoma form ; the two 
forms are different phases of the same animal, It was a 
natural inference that the Axolotls were those which had 
remained or had been kept in the water, the Amédlystoma 
forms were those which got ashore. But both kinds may 
be found in the water of the same lake and the metamor- 
phosis may take place in the water as well as on the shore. 
For these and for other reasons this oft-told tale is not 


PART IV 


310 The Study of Animal Lt 


re) 


cogent. In another part of this book I have given examples 
of the state of lifelessness which drought induces in some 


NANG 
MW it 


Fic. 64.—Axolotl (in the water) and Amblystoma (on the land). 


simple animals, and from which returning moisture can 


after many days recall them. 
Changes may also be due to the chemical composition 


of the medium, as was established by the experiments of 


Fic. 65.—Side view of male Artemia salina (enlarged). 
5 = ee 4 
(From Chambers’s /ncyclop.) 


Schmankewitsch on certain small Crustaceans. Among the 
numerous species of the brine-shrimp Ardem, the most 
unlike are A. selina and A. milhausent’ ; they differ in the 


cu. xix Lifluence of Habits and Surroundings 311 


shape and size of the tail and in the respiratory appendages 
borne by the legs ; they are not found together, but live in 
pools of different degrees of saltness. Now Schmankewitsch 
took specimens of A. salina which live in the less salt water, 


Fic. 66.—Tail-lobes of Artemia salina (to the left) and of Artemza milhausenii 
(to the right) ; between these four stages in the transformation of the one into 
the other. (From Chambers’s Zxcyclop.; after Schmankewitsch.) 


added salt gradually to the medium in which they were 
living, and in the course of generations turned them into 
A. milthausenit. He also reversed the process by freshening 
the water little by little. Moreover, he accustomed A. salina 
to entirely fresh water, and then found that the form had 
changed towards that of a related genus, Branchipfus. This 
last step has been adversely criticised, but it is allowed that 
one species of brine-shrimp was changed into another. 

Many interesting experiments have been made on the 
effect of chemical reagents on cells, but these are perhaps 
of most interest to the student of drugs. Still the fact that 
the form of a cell and its predominant phase of activity 
may be entirely changed in this way is important, especially 
when we remember that. it was in single cells that life first 
began, and is now continued. Even Weismann agrees with 
Spencer’s conclusion that “the direct action of the medium 
was the primordial factor of organic evolution.” 

To Claude Bernard, the main problem of evolution 
seemed to be concerned with variations in nutrition: 
“ Tévolution, c’est ensemble constant de ces alternatives 
de la nutrition; c’est la nutrition considerée dans sa 
réalité, embrassée d’un coup d’ceil a travers le temps.” 
John Hunter and others have shown how the walls of the 


312 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


stomach of gulls and other birds may be experimentally 
altered by change of diet, and the same is seen in nature 
when the Shetland gull changes from its summer diet of 
grain to its winter diet of fish, The colours of birds’ feathers, 
as in canaries and parrots, are affected by their food. A 
slight difference in the quantity and quality of food deter- 
mines whether a bee-grub is to become a queen or a 
worker, royal diet evolving the reproductive queen, sparser 
less rich diet evolving the more active but unfertile worker. 
Abundant food favours the production of female offspring, 
while sparser food tends to develop males. Thus, in frogs, 
the proportion of the sexes is normally not very far from 
equal ; in three lots of tadpoles an average of 57 per hun- 
dred became females, 43 males. But Yung has shown that 
the nutrition of the tadpoles has a remarkable influence on 
the sex of the adults. In a set of which one half kept in 
natural conditions developed into 54 females to 46 males, 
the other half fed with beef had 78 females to 22 males. 
In a second set of which one half left to themselves 
developed 61 females to 39 males, the other half, fed with 
fish, had 81 females to 19 males. Finally, in a third set, 
of which one half in natural conditions developed 56 females 
to 44 males, the other half, to which the especially nutritious 
flesh of frogs was supplied, had no less than 92 females 
to 8 males. 

When food is abundant, assimilation active, and income 
above expenditure, the animal grows, and at the limit of 
growth in lower animals asexual multiplication occurs, 
Checked nutrition, on the other hand, favours the higher 
or sexual mode of multiplication. Thus the gardener 
prunes the roots of a plant to get better flowers or repro- 
ductive leaves. The plant-lice or Aphides, which infest 
our pear-trees and rose-bushes, well illustrate the combined 
influence of food and warmth. All through the summer, 
when food is abundant and the warmth pleasant, the 
Aphides enjoy prosperity, and multiply rapidly. For an 
Aphis may bring forth young every few hours for days 
together, so rapidly that if all the offspring of a mother 
Aphis survived, and multiplied as she did, there would 


cu. xix Lnfluence of Habits and Surroundings 313 


in the course of a year be a progeny which would weigh 
down 500,000,000 stout men. But all through the 
summer these Aphides are wholly female, and therefore 
wholly parthenogenetic ; no males occur. In autumn, how- 
ever, when hard times set in, when food is scarcer, and the 
weather colder, males are born, parthenogenesis ceases, 
ordinary sexual reproduction recurs. Moreover, if the 
Aphides be kept in the artificial summer of a greenhouse, 
as has been done for four years, the parthenogenesis con- 
tinues without break, no males being born to enjoy the 
comforts of that environment. Periods of fasting occur 
in the life-history of many animals, and these are very 
momentous and progressive periods‘in the lives of some, 
for the tadpole fasts before it becomes a frog, and the 
chrysalis before it becomes a butterfly. Lack of food, how- 
ever, may stunt development, as we see every day in the 
streets of our towns, 

(c) Radiant Energy.—Of the forms of radiant energy 
which play upon the organism, we need take account only 
of heat and light, for of electrical and magnetic influence 
the few strange facts that we know do not make us much 
wiser. 

We know that increased warmth hastens motion, the 
development of embryos, and the advent of sexual maturity. 
An Infusorian (Stylonichia) studied by Maupas was seen 
to divide once a day at a temperature of 7°-10° C., twice 
at Io0°-15°, thrice at 15°-20°, four times at 20°-24°, five 
times at 24°-27°C. At the last temperature one Infusorian 
became in four days the ancestor of a million, in six days 
of a billion, in seven days and a half of 100 billions, weigh- 
ing 100 kilogrammes. By consummately patient experi- 
ments, Dallinger was able to educate Monads which lived 
normally at a temperature of 65° Fahr., until they could 
flourish at 158° Fahr. 

Cold has generally a reverse action, checking activity, 
producing coma and lifelessness, diminishing the rate of 
development, tending to produce dwarf or larva-like forms. 
The cold of winter acting through the nervous system 
changes the colour of some animals, like Ross’s lemming, 


314 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


to advantageous white. Not a few animals vary slightly 
with the changing seasons. Thus many cases are known 
where a butterfly produces in a year more than one brood, 


Pic. 67.—Seasonal dimorphism of Pafélio ajax; to the left the winter form 
(variety 7edamonides), to the right the summer form (variety Jlarcellus). 
(From Chambers’s Lxcyclop.; after Weismann.) 

of which the winter forms are so different from those born 

in summer that they have often been described as different 

species. It is possible that this is a reminiscence of past 
climatic changes, such as those of the Ice Ages, as the 


i) 
Fic. 68.—Seasonal changes of the bill in the puflin (7yatercula arctica): to the 


left the spring form, to the right the winter form, both adult males. (After 
Bureau.) 


result of which a species became split up into two varieties. 
Thus Araschiia levana and Araschnia prorsa are respect- 
ively the winter and summer forms of one species. In the 


cu. xix Lnfluence of Habits and Surroundings 315 


glacial epoch there was perhaps only A. /evana, the winter 
form; the change of climate has perhaps evolved the 
summer variety A. prorsa. Both Weismann and Edwards 
have succeeded, by artificial cold, in making the pupze which 
should become the summer A. Jvorsa develop into the winter 
A, levana. Nor can we forget the seasonal moulting and 
the subsequent change of the plumage in birds, so marked 
in the case of the ptarmigan, which moults three times in 
the year. In the puffins even the bill is moulted and 
appears very different at different seasons. But in these 
last cases the ‘influence of environment must be very 
indirect. 

Light is very healthful, but it is not easy to explain its 
precise influence. Our pulses beat faster when we go out 
into the sunlight. Plants live in part on the radiant 
energy of the sun, and perhaps some pigmented animals do 
the same. Perhaps the hundreds of eyes which some mol- 
luscs have are also useful in absorbing the light. It is also 
possible that light has a direct influence on the formation of 
some animal pigments, as it seems to have in the develop- 
ment of chlorophyll. We know, from Poulton’s experiments, 
that the light reflected from coloured bodies influences the 
colouring of caterpillars and pupz, but this influence seems 
to be subtle and ‘indirect, operating through the nervous 
system. It is also certain that living in darkness tends to 
bleach some animals, and it is probable that the absence 
of light stimulus has a directly injurious effect upon the 
eyes of those animals which live in caves or other dark 
places. But I have already explained why dogmatism in 
regard to these cases should be avoided. 

One case of the influence of light seems very instructive. 
It is well known that flat fishes like flounders, plaice, and 
soles lie or swim in adult life on one side. This lower side 
is unpigmented; the upper side bears black and yellow 
pigment-containing cells. 

One theory of the presence of pigment on the upper 
side and its absence on the other is that the difference is 
a protective adaptation evolved by the natural selection of 
indefinite variations. But it is open to question whether the 


316 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


characteristic is so advantageously protective as is usually 
imagined: thus the coloured upper side in soles is very 
often covered with a layer of sand. Soles come out most 
at night, most live at depths at which differences of colour 
are probably indistinct. In shallower water the advantage 
is likely to be greater, though the white under-side slightly 
exposed as the fish rises from the bottom may attract atten- 
tion disadvantageously. Moreover, if we find in a large 
number of different animals that the side away from the 
light is lighter than that which is exposed, and if we can 
show that this has in many cases no protective advantage 
whatever—and I believe that a few hours’ observation will 
convince you that both my assumptions are correct—then 
there is a probability that the absence of light has a direct 
influence on the absence of pigment. 

But we are not left to vague probabilities; Mr. J. T. 
Cunningham has recently made the crucial experiment of 
illuminating the under sides of young flounders. Out of 
thirteen, whose under-sides were thus illumined by a mirror 
for about four months, only three failed to develop black 
and yellow colour-cells on the skin of the under-sides. It 
is therefore likely that the normal whiteness of the under- 
sides is due in some way to the fact that in nature little light 
can fall on them, for they are generally in contact with the 
ground, 

(@) Animate Surroundings.—We have given a few 
instances showing how mechanical or molar pressures, 
chemical and nutritive influences, and the subtler physical 
energies of heat and light, affect organisms. There is a 
fourth set of environmental factors—the direct influence of 
organism upon organism. In a previous chapter we spoke 
of the indirect influences different kinds of ‘organisms exert 
on one another, and these are most important, but there are 
also results of direct contact. 

Much in the same way as insects produce galls on 
plants, so sea-spiders (Pycnogonide) affect hydroids, a 
polype deforms a sponge, a little worm (AZyzostoma) makes 
galls on Crinoids. Prof. Giard has described how certain 
degenerate Crustaceans parasitic on crabs injuriously affect 


cu. xix Influence of Habits and Surroundings 317 


their hosts, and some internal parasites produce slight 
modifications of structure. Interesting also are the shelters 
or domatia of some plants, within which insects and mites 
find homes. 

We can speak more confidently about the influence of 
surroundings than we could in regard to the influence of 
use and disuse, because the ascertained facts are more 
numerous. Those interested in-the theoretical importance 
of these facts should attend to the following considerations. 

It is essential to distinguish between cases in which 
we know that external conditions influence the organism 
and those in which we think they may have done so. Thus 
it is probable that the degeneracy and other peculiarities of 
many parasites are results of external influence and of 
feeding, and also in part of disuse, but we cannot state 
this as a fact. ; 

Most of the observations on the influence of external 
conditions give us no information as to the transmissi- 
bility of the results. It is not enough to know that a 
peculiarity observed to occur in peculiar surroundings was 
observed to recur in successive generations living in the 
same surroundings. For (1) it might be an indefinite 
variation—a sport due to some germinal peculiarity— 
which happened to suit. In such a case it would be 
transmissible, but it would not be a change due to the 
environment. And (z) even when it has been proved that 
the peculiarity is due to the direct influence of the environ- 
ment, and observed to recur in successive generations, still 
its transmissibility is not proven, for it may be hammered 
on each successive generation as it was on the first. We 
can say little about the transmissibility or evolutionary 
importance of changes of structure due to surroundings 
because most of the observations were made before the 
scepticism as to the inheritance of acquired characters 
became dominant. Only in a few cases, such as that of the 
brine-shrimps, was the cumulative influence traced through 
many generations. In dearth of facts we should not be 
confident, but eager for experiment. 

Surroundings may influence the organism in: varying 


318 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


degrees. There may be direct results, rapid parries after 
thrusts, or the results may be indirect ; they may affect the 
organism visibly in the course of one generation, or only 
after several have passed. 

Some animals are more susceptible and more plastic 
than others. Young organisms, such as caterpillars and 
tadpoles, are more completely in the grasp of their environ- 
ment than are the adults. ‘Thus Treviranus, who believed 
very strongly in the influence of surroundings, distinguished 
two periods of vita minima—in youth and in old age— 
during which external conditions press heavily, from the 
period of véta maxima—in adult life—when the organism 
is more free. To some kinds of influence, e.g. mechanical 
pressures, passive and sedentary organisms such as sponges, 
corals, shell-fish, and plants, are more susceptible than are 
those of active life. And it is during a period of quiescence 
that surrounding colour tells on the sensitive caterpillars. 

3. Our own Environment.—The human organism, like 
any other, may be modified by its environment, for we 
lead no charmed life. Those external influences which 
touch body and mind are to us the more important, since 
we have them to some extent within our own hands, and 
because our lives are relativelylong. Even if the changes 
thus wrought upon parents are not transmissible, it is to 
some extent possible for us to secure that our children grow 
up open to influences known to be beneficial, sheltered from 
forces known to be injurious. 

As the influence of surroundings is especially potent on 
young things—such as caterpillars and tadpoles—all care 
should be taken of the young child’s environment during 
the earliest months and years, when the grip that externals 
have is probably much greater than is imagined by those 
who believe themselves emancipated from the tyranny of 
the present.? 

As passive organisms are more in the thrall of their 
surroundings than are the more active, we feel the import- 
ance of beauty in the home, that the organism may be 


1 Cf. Matthew Arnold's poem, ‘‘ The Future,” and Walt Whitman's 
“ Assimilations."’ 


cu. xix Influence of Habits and Surroundings 319 


saturated with healthful influence during the periods in 
which it is most susceptible. The efforts of Social Unions, 
Kyrle Societies, Verschénerungs-Vereine, and the like, are 
justified not only by their results,! but by the biological 
facts on which they more or less unconsciously depend. 
There would be more progress and less invidious com- 
parison of ameliorative schemes, if we realised more vividly 
that the Fates are three. Though it is not easy to appre- 
ciate the three sides of a prism at once, of what value is 
liberty on an ash-heap, or equality in a hell, or fraternity 
among an overpopulated community of weaklings? Organ- 
ism, function, and environment must evolve together, and 
surely they shall. 

Poets have often compared human beings to caterpillars ; 
it may be that no improvement in constitutions, functions, 
or surroundings will make us winged Psyches, yet it may be 
possible for us to be ennobled like those creatures which in 
gilded surroundings became golden. Surely art is warranted 
by the results of science, as these in time may justify them- 
selves in art. 


1 Tdeally stated in Emerson's well-known poem of Art.” 


CHAPTER XX 
HEREDITY 


1. The Facts of Heredity —z. Theories of Heredity: theological, 
metaphysical, mystical, and the hypothesis of pangenesis— 
3. The Modern Theory of Heredity—4. The Inheritance of 
Acquired Characters—5. Social and Ethical Aspects—6, Social 
Inheritance 


WE have spoken of the three Fates which were believed to 
determine of what sort a life should be. With the decay 
of poetic feeling, and in the light of common science, the 
forms of the three sisters have faded. But they are realities 
still, for men are thinking more and more vividly about the 
factors of life, which to some are ‘ powerful principles,” 
to others living and personal, to others unnameable. 
Biologists speak of them as Heredity, Function, and En- 
vironment: the capital with which a life begins, the 
interest accruing from the investment of this in varied vital 
activities, and the force of circumstances. But while it is 
useful to think of Heredity, Function, and Environment as 
the three fates, we must not mystify matters by talking as 
if these were entities acting upon the organism. They 
are simply aspects of the fact that the animal is born and 
lives. The inheritance is the organism itself, and heredity 
is only a name for the relation between successive genera- 
tions. Moreover, the function of an organism depends 
upon the nature of the organism, and so does its suscep- 
tibility to influences from without, 

I would at present define heredity as the organic relation 


CHAP. XX fTeredity 321 


between successive generations, choosing this definition 
because it is misleading to talk about “heredity” as a 
“basal principle in evolution,” as a “great law,” as a 
“power,” or as a “cause.” When I call heredity a 
‘‘ Fate,” it is plain ‘that I speak fancifully, but “ principle” 
and “law” are dangerous words to play with. We cannot 
think of life without this organic relation between parents 
and offspring, and had species been created instead of being 
evolved there would still be heredity. 

1. The Facts of Heredity.— An animal sometimes 
arises as a bud from its parent, and in rare cases from an 
egg which requires no fertilisation, but apart from these 
exceptions, every animal develops from an egg-cell with 
which a male-cell has united in an intimate way. The 
egg-cell supplies most of the living matter, but the nucleus 
of the fertilised egg-cell is formed in half from the nucleus 
of the immature ovum, in half from the nucleus of the 
spermatozoon. Let us emphasise this first fact that each 
parent contributes the same amount of nuclear material to 
the offspring, and that this nuclear stuff is very essential. 

Another fact is more obvious, the offspring is very like 
its kind. One of the first things that people say about an 
infant is that it is like its father or its mother, and the 
assertion does not arouse any surprise, although the truer 
verdict that the infant is like any other of the same race is 
received with contempt. But every one admits that “like 
begets like.” 

This likeness between offspring and parent is often far 
more than a general resemblance, for peculiar features and 
minute idiosyncrasies are frequently reproduced. Yet one 
must not assume that because a child twirls his thumbs 
in the same way as his father did the habit has been 
inherited. For peculiar habits and structures may readily 
reappear by imitation, or because the offspring grow up in 
conditions similar to those in which the parents lived. 

Abnormal as well as normal characters, “natural” to 
the parents, may reappear in their descendants, and the list 
of weaknesses and malformations which may be transmitted 
is long and grim. But care is required to distinguish 

v 


322 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


between reappearance due to inheritance and reappearance 
due to similar conditions of life. 

Then there is a strange series of facts showing that an 
organism may reproduce characteristics which the parents 
did not exhibit, but which were possessed by a grandparent 
or remoter ancestor. Thus a lizard in growing a new 
tail to replace one that has been lost has been known to 
grow one with scales like those of an ancestral species. To 
find out a lizard’s pedigree, a wit suggests that we need only 
pull off its tail. When such ancestral resemblance in ordi- 


Ne bar cone 
sy Pia Ref Mae 
Taka AM ha My eo > 


Fic. 69.—Devonshire pony, showing the occasional occurrence of ancestral 
stripes. (J*'rom Darwin.) 


nary generation is very marked, we call it ‘atavism” or 
“reversion,” but of this there are many degrees, and 
abnormal circumstances sometimes force reversion even 
upon an organism with a normal inheritance. A boy 
“takes after his grandfather” ; a horse occasionally exhibits 
stripes like those of a wild ancestor ; a blue pigeon like the 
primitive rock-dove sometimes turns up unexpectedly in a 
pure breed ; or a cultivated flower reverts to the simpler 
and more normal wild type. So children born during 
famine sometimes show reversions, and some types of 
criminal and insane persons are to be thus regarded. 


CHAP, XX fTevedity 323 


But every animal is usually a little different from its 
parents, and except in cases of “identical twins” cannot be 
mistaken for one of its fellow-offspring. The proverbial 
“two peas” may be very unlike. Organisms are variable, 
and this is natural, for life begins in the intimate 
mingling of two units of living matter perhaps very dif 
ferent and certainly very complex. The relation between 
successive generations is such that the offspring is like 
its parents, but various causes producing change diminish 
this likeness, so that we no longer say “like begets like,” 
but “like zemds to beget like.” 

There are, I think, two other important facts in regard 
to heredity, but both require discussion—the one because 
some of the most authoritative naturalists deny it, the other 
because it is difficult to understand. 

I believe that some characters acquired by the parent as 
the result of what it does, and as impacts from the surround- 
ing conditions of life, are transmissible to the offspring. In 
other words, some functional and environmental variations 
in the body of the parents may be handed on to the 
offspring. This is denied by Weismann and many others. 

The other fact, which has been elucidated by Galton, 
is that through successive generations there is a tendency 
to sustain the average of the species, by the continual 
approximation of exceptional forms towards a mean. 

2. Theories of Heredity — historical retrospect.— 
Theories of heredity, like those about many other facts, 
have been formulated at different times in different kinds 
of intellectual language—theological, metaphysical, and 
scientific—and the words are often more at variance than 
the ideas. 

(a) Theological Theortes.—lt was an old idea, that the 
germ of a new human life was possessed by a spirit, some- 
times of second-hand origin, having previously belonged to 
some ancestor or animal. So far as this idea persists in the 
minds of civilised men, it is so much purified and sublimed 
that if the student of science does not believe it true, 
he cannot wisely call it false. 

(6) “ Metaphysical Theortes.’—-For a time it was com- 


324 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


mon to appeal to “v2res formative,” “ hereditary tendencies,” 
and “principles of heredity,” by aid of which the germ 
grew into the likeness of the parent, and this tendency 
to resort to verbal explanations is hardly to be driven from 
the scientific mind except by intellectual asceticism. For 
my own part, I prefer such “metaphysical” mist to the 
frost of a ‘“‘materialism” which blasts the buds of wonder. 

(c) “ Mystical Theories.”—During the eighteenth cen- 
tury and even within the limits of the enlightened nineteenth, 
a quaint idea of development prevailed, according to which 
the germ (either the ovum or the sperm) contained a miniature 
organism, preformed in all transparency, which only required 
to be unfolded (or “evolved,” as they said), in order to 
become the future animal. Moreover, the egg of a fowl 
contained not only a micro-organism or miniature model of 
the chick, but likewise in increasing minuteness similar 
models of future generations. Microcosm lay within micro- 
cosm, germ within germ, like the leaves within a bud 
awaiting successive unfolding, or like an infinite juggler’s 
box to the “evolution” of which there was no end. This 
“ preformation theory” or “mystical hypothesis” was virtu- 
ally but not actually shattered by Wolff’s demonstration of 
“ Epigenesis” or gradual development from an apparently 
simple rudiment. But the preformationists were right in 
insisting that the future organism lay (potentially) within 
the germ, and right also in supposing that the germ involved 
not only the organism into which it grew but its descendants 
as well, The form of their theory, however, was crude and 
false. 

(@) Theories of Pangenests.—Scientific theories of here- 
dity really begin with that of Herbert Spencer, who in 
1864 suggested that “physiological units” derived from 
and capable of growth into cells were accumulated from the 
body into the reproductive elements, there to develop the 
characters of structures like those whence they arose. At 
dates so widely separate as are suggested by the names of 
Democritus and Hippocrates, Paracelsus and Buffon, the 
same idea was expressed—that the germs consist of samples 
from the various parts of the body. But the theories of 


CHAP. XX Lfleredity 325 


these authors were vague and in some respects entirely 
erroneous suggestions. The best-known form of this type 
of theory is Darwin’s “provisional hypothesis of pan- 
genesis” (1868), according to which (a) every cell of the 
body, not too highly differentiated, throws off characteristic 
gemmules, which (4) multiply by fission, retaining their 
peculiarities, and (c) become specially concentrated in the 
reproductive elements, where (¢) in development they grow 
into cells like those from which they were originally given 
off. This theory was satisfactory in giving a reasonable 
explanation of many of the facts of heredity, it was unsatis- 
factory because it involved many unverified hypotheses. 

The ingenious Jeger, well known as the introducer of 
comfortable clothing, sought (1876) to replace the “gem- 
mules” of which Darwin spoke, by characteristic ‘ scent- 
stuffs,” which he supposed to be collected from the body 
into the reproductive elements. 

Meanwhile (1872) Francis Galton, our greatest British 
authority on heredity, had been led by his experiments 
on the transfusion of blood and by other considerations 
to the conclusion that “the doctrine of pangenesis, pure 
and simple, is incorrect.” As we shall see, he reached 
forward to a more satisfactory doctrine, but he still allowed 
the possibility of a limited pangenesis to account for those 
cases which suggest that some characters acquired by the 
parents are “faintly heritable.” He admitted that a cell 
“may throw off a few germs” (ze. “gemmules”) “that 
find their way into the circulation, and have thereby a 
chance of occasionally finding their way to the sexual 
elements, and of becoming naturalised among them.” 

W. K. Brooks, a well-known American naturalist, pro- 
posed in 1883 an important modification of Darwin’s theory, 
especially insisting on the following three suppositions : 
that it is in wxqwonted and abnormal conditions that the cells 
of the body throw off gemmules; that the male elements 
are the special centres of their accumulation ; and that the 
jemale cells keep up the general resemblance between 
offspring and parents. For further modifications and for 
criticism of the theories of pangenesis, I refer the student 


326 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


to the works of Galton, Ribot, Brooks, Herdman, Plarre, 
Van Bemmelen, and De Vries. 

3. The Modern Theory of Heredity.—In the midst of 
much debate it may seem strange to speak of the modern 
theory of heredity, but while details are disputed, one clear 
fact is generally acknowledged, the increasing realisation of 
which has shed a new light on heredity. This fact is the 
organic continuity of generations. 

In 1876 Jeger expressed his views explicitly as follows : 
“Through a long series of generations the germinal proto- 
plasm retains its specific properties, dividing in develop- 
ment into a portion out of which the individual is built up, 
and a portion which is reserved to form the reproductive 
material of the mature offspring.” This reservation, by 
which some of the germinal protoplasm is kept apart, during 
development and growth, from corporeal or external influ- 
ences, and retains its specific or germinal characters intact 
and continuous with those of the parent ovum, Jeger 
regarded as the fundamental fact of heredity. 

Brooks (1876, 1877, 1883) was not less clear: “The 
ovum gives rise to the divergent cells of the organism, but 
also to cells like itself. The ovarian ova of the offspring 
are these latter cells or their direct unmodified descendants. 
The ovarian ova of the offspring thus share by direct 
inheritance all the properties of the fertilised ova.” 

But before and independently of either Jeger or Brooks 
or any one else, Galton had reached forward to the same 
idea, We have noticed that he was led in 1872 to the 
conclusion that “the doctrine of pangenesis, pure and 
simple, is incorrect.” His own view was that the fertilised 
ovum consisted of a sum of germs, gemmules, or organic 
units of some kind, to which in entirety he applied the 
term stirp. But he did not regard this nest of organic 
units as composed of contributions from all parts of the 
body. He regarded it as directly derived from a previous 
nest, namely, from the ovum which gave rise to the parent. 
He maintained that in development the bulk of the stirp 
grew into the body—as every one allows—but that a cer- 
tain residue was kept apart from the development of the 


CHAP. Xx Flevedity 327 


“body” to form the reproductive elements of the offspring. 
Thus he said, in a sense the child is as old as the parent, 
for when the parent is developing from the ovum a residue 
of that ovum is kept apart to form the germ-cells, one of 
which may become a child. Besides Galton, Jeger, and 
Brooks, several other biologists suggested this fertile idea 
of the organic continuity of generations. Thus it is ex- 
pressed by Erasmus Darwin and by Owen, by Haeckel, 
Rauber, and Nussbaum. But it is to Weismann that the 
modern emphasis on the idea is chiefly due, 

Let us try to realise more vividly this doctrine of organic 
continuity between generations. Let us begin with a fertil- 
ised egg-cell, and suppose it to have qualities adcryz. This 
endowed egg-cell divides and redivides, and for a short 
time each of the units in the ball of cells may be regarded 
as still possessed of the original qualities abcxyz. But 
division of labour, and rearrangement, infolding and out- 
folding, soon begin, and most of the cells form the ‘“ body.” 
They lose their primitive characters and uniformity, they 
become specialised, the qualities ad predominate in one 
set, dc in another, zy in another. But meantime certain 
cells have kept apart from the specialisation which results 
in the body. They have remained embryonic and un- 
differentiated, retaining the many-sidedness of the original 
egg-cell, preserving intact the qualities adcxyz. They form 
the future reproductive cells—let us say the eggs. 

Now when these eggs are liberated, with the original 
qualities aécxyz unchanged, having retained a continuous 
protoplasmic tradition with the parent ovum, they are evi- 
dently in almost the same position as that was. There- 
fore they develop into the same kind of organism. Given 
the same protoplasmic material, the same inherent quali- 
ties, the same conditions of birth and growth, the results 
must be the same. A single-celled animal with qualities 
abcxyz divides into two; each has presumably the qualities 
of the original unit; each grows rapidly into the form of 
the full-grown cell. We have no difficulty in understanding 
this. In the sexual reproduction of higher animals, the 
case is complicated by the formation of the “body,” but 


328 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


logically the difficulty is not greater. A fertilised egg-cell 
with qualities adcxyz divides into many cells, which, becom- 
ing diverse, express the original qualities in various kinds 
of tissue within the forming body. But if at an early stage 
certain cells are set apart, retaining the qualities or charac- 
ters abcxyz in all their entirety, then these, when liberated 
after months or years as egg-cells, will resemble the original 
ovum, and are able like it to give rise to an organism, 
which is necessarily a similar organism. 

To call heredity “the relation of organic continuity 
between successive generations,” as I define it, seems a 
truism to some, but it is in the realisation of this truistic 
fact that the modern progress in regard to heredity consists. 

To ask how the inherent qualities of the ovum become 
divergent in the different cells of the body, or how some 
units remain embryonic, or how the egg-cell divides at 
all, is to raise the deepest problems of biology, not of 
heredity. To answer such questions is the more or less 
hopeless task of physiological embryology, not that of the 
student of heredity. Recognising the fact of organic con- 
tinuity, various writers such as Samuel Butler, Hering, 
Haeckel, Geddes, Gautier, and Berthold, have sought in 
various ways to make it clearer, e.g. by regarding the re- 
production of like by like as an instance of organic memory. 
As these suggestions are unessential to our argument, I 
shall merely notice that there are plenty of them. 

How far has this early separation of the future repro- 
ductive cells from the developing body been observed? It 
has been observed in several worm-types—leeches, Sagé¢éa, 
thread-worms, Polyzoa,—in some Arthropods (eg. oina 
among crustaceans, Chzvonomus among Insects, Phalangidze 
among spiders), and with less distinctness in a number of 
other organisms, both animal and vegetable. In most. of 
the higher animals, however, the future reproductive cells 
are not observable till development has proceeded for some 
days or weeks. To explain this difficulty, Weismann has 
elaborated a theory which he calls ‘the continuity of the 
germ-plasma.” The general idea of this theory is that of 
organic continuity between generations, and this Weismann 


CHAP. &X LfTeredity 329 


has done momentous service in expounding. But for the 
detailed theory by which he seeks to overcome the diffi- 
culty which has been noticed above I refer those interested 
to Weismann’s Pagers on Heredity (Trans. Oxford, 1889). 

4. The Inheritance of Acquired Characters.— (a) His- 
torical We have seen that variations, or changes in char- 
acter, may be constitutional, t.e. innate in the germ; or 
Junctional, i.e, due to use or disuse ; or environmental, 2.2. 
due to influences of nutrition and, surroundings. Many 
naturalists have believed that gains or losses due to any of 
these three sources of change might be transmitted from 
parent to offspring. But nowadays the majority, with 
Profs. Weismann and Lankester at their head, deny the 
transmissibility of either functional or environmental 
changes, and believe that inborn, germinal, or constitu- 
tional variations alone are transmissible. 

This scepticism is not strictly modern. The editor, 
whoever he was, of Aristotle’s Historia Animalium, differed 
from his master as to the inheritance of injuries and the 
like.’ Kant maintained the non-inheritance of extrinsic 
variations, and Blumenbach cautiously inclined to the same 
negative position. In more recent times the veteran morpho- 
logist His expressed a strong conviction against the inherit- 
ance of acquired characters, and the not less renowned 
physiologist Pfliiger is also among the sceptics. A few 
sentences from Galton (1875), whose far-sightedness has 
been insufficiently acknowledged, may be quoted: “The 
inheritance of characters acquired during the lifetime of the 
parents includes much questionable evidence, usually diffi- 
cult of verification. We might almost reserve our belief 
that the structural cells can react on the sexual elements at 
all, and we may be confident that at the most they do so in 
avery faint degree—in other words, that acquired modifica- 
tions are barely, if at all, zwherdted in the correct sense of 
that word.” 

But Weismann brought the discussion to a climax by 
altogether denying the transmissibility of acquired charac- 
ters. 

(4) Weismann’s position.— Weismann’s reasons for 


330 The Study of Animal Life BART IV 


maintaining that no acquired characters are transmissible 
are twofold,—first because the evidence in favour of such 
transmission consists of unverifiable anecdotes ; second 
because the ‘‘germ-plasma,” early set apart in the de- 
velopment of the body, remains intact and stable, unaffected 
by the vicissitudes which beset the body. 

It is natural that Weismann, who realised so vividly the 
continuity between germ and germ, should emphasise the 
stability of the ‘germ-plasma,” that he should regard it 
as leading a sort of charmed life within the organism un- 
affected by changes to which the body is subject. But has 
he not exaggerated this insulation and stability ? 

Of course Weismann does not deny that the body may 
exhibit functional and environmental variations, but he 
denies that these can spread from the body so as to affect 
the reproductive cells thereof, and unless they do so, they 
cannot be transmitted to the offspring. 

On the other hand, innate or germinal characters 
must be transmitted. They crop up in the parent be- 
cause they are involved in the fertilised egg-cell. But as 
the cell which gives rise to the offspring is by hypothesis 
similar to and more or less directly continuous with the 
cell which gave rise to the parent, similar constitutional 
variations will crop up in the offspring. 

We must admit that most of the old evidence adduced 
in favour of the transmission of acquired characters may 
be called a “handful of anecdotes.” For scepticism was 
undeveloped, and when a character acquired by a parent 
reappeared in the offspring, it was too readily regarded as 
transmitted, whereas it may often have been ee by 
the offspring just as it was by the parent. 

Weismann has two saving clauses, which make 7 
ment against his position peculiarly difficult, (1) He 
admits that the germ-plasma may be modified “ever so 
little” by changes of nutrition and growth in the body; 
but may not an accumulation of many “ever-so-littles ” 
amount to the transmission of an acquired character? (2) 
He admits that external conditions, such as climate, may 
influence the reproductive cells along with, though not 


CHAP. XX LfTeredity 331 


exactly ¢hrough, the body; but this is a distinction too 
subtle to be verified. 

These two saving-clauses seem to me to affect the strin- 
gency of Weismann’s conclusion, but in his view they do 
not affect the main proposition that definite somatic modifi- 
cations or changes in the body due to function or environ- 
ment have no effect on the reproductive cells, and therefore 
no transmission to offspring. 

(c) Arguments against Weismann’s position.—In arguing 
against Weismann’s position that no acquired characters 
are inherited, I shall first illustrate the arguments of others, 
and then emphasise that which appears to me at present 
most cogent. 

(1) Some have cited against Weismann various cases 
where the effects of mutilation seemed to be transmitted, 
and Weismann has spent some time in experimenting with 
mice in order to see whether cutting off the tails for several 
generations did not eventually make the tails shorter. It 
did not—a result which might have been foretold. For we 
have known for many years that the mutilations inflicted 
on sheep and other domesticated animals had no measur- 
able effect on the offspring. Even the numerous cases of 
tailless kittens produced from artificially curtailed cats have 
no cogency in face of the fact that tailless sports often arise 
from normal parents. Moreover, it is for many reasons not 
to be expected that the results of curtailment and the like 
should be inherited. For there is great power of regener- 
ating lost parts even in the individual lifetime; the result 
of cutting off a tail is for most part merely a minus quantity 
to the organism; the imperfectly known physiological re- 
action on nerves and blood-vessels might perhaps result in 
a longer rather than a shorter tail in the offspring. 

(2) Various pathologists, led by Virchow, have empha- 
sised the fact that many diseases are inherited, but their 
arguments have usually shown how easy it is to misunder- 
stand Weismann’s position. No doubt many malformations 
and diseases reappear through successive generations, but 
there is lack of evidence to show that the pathological 
variations were not germinal to begin with. It is sadly 


332 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


interesting to learn that colour-blindness has been known 
to occur in the males only of six successive generations, 
deaf mutism for three, finger malformations for six, and so 
with harelip and cleft palate, and with tendencies to con- 
sumption, cancer, gout, rheumatism, bleeding, and so on. 
But these facts do not prove the transmission of functional or 
environmental variations ; they only corroborate what every 
one allows, that innate, congenital, constitutional characters 


Fic. 70,—Half-lop rabbit, an abnormal variation, which by artificial selection 
has become constant ina breed. (From Darwin.) 


tend to be transmitted. Yet some cases recently stated by 
Prof. Bertram Windle seem to suggest that some patho- 
logical conditions acquired by function may be transmitted. 
But even if a non-constitutional pathological state acquired 
by a parent reappeared in the offspring, we require to show 
that the offspring did not also acquire it by his work or 
from conditions of life, as his parent did before him. 

(3) Some individual cases seem to stand some criticism. 

Two botanists, Hoffmann and Detmer, have noted such 
facts as the following—scant nutrition influenced the flowers 
of poppy, Végel/a, dead-nettle, and the result was trans- 


CHAP, XX Heredity 333 


mitted; peculiar soil conditions altered the root of the 
carrot, and the result was transmitted. 

Semper gives a few cases such as Schmankewitsch’s 
transformation of one species of brine-shrimp (Artemia) into 
another, throughout a series of generations during which 
the salinity of the water was slowly altered. 

Eimer has written a book of which even the title, “The 
Origin of Species, according to the laws of organic growth, 
through the inheritance of acquired characters,” shows how 
strongly he supports the affirmative side of our question. 
But much as I admire and agree with many parts of Eimer’s 
work, I do not think that all his examples of the inheritance 
of acquired characters are cogent. One of the strongest 
is that cereals from Scandinavian plains transplanted to 
the mountains become gradually accustomed to develop 
more rapidly and at a lower temperature, and that when 
returned to the plains they retain this power of rapid 
development. I am inclined to think that the strongest 
part of Eimer’s argument is that in which he maintains that 
certain effects produced upon the nervous system by peculiar 
habits are transmissible. 

(4) Another mode of argument may be considered. To 
what conception of evolution are we impelled if we deny 
the inheritance of acquired characters? Weismann believes 
that he has taken the ground from under the feet of 
Lamarckians and Buffonians, who believe in the inheritance 
of functional and environmental variations. The sole fount 
of change is to be found in the mingling of the kernels of 
two cells at the fertilisation of the ovum. On these varia- 
tions natural selection works. 

But even if we do not believe in the inheritance of 
acquired characters, it is open to us to maintain that by 
cumulative constitutional variations in definite directions 
species have grown out of one another in progressive evolu- 
tion. Thus we are not forced to restrict our interpreta- 
tions of the marvel and harmony of organic nature to the 
theory of the action of natural selection on indefinite for- 
.tuitous variations. 

Prof. Ray Lankester’s convictions on this subject are so 


334 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


strong, and his dismissal of Lamarckian theory is so 
emphatic, that I shall select one of his illustrations by way 
of contrasting his theory with that of Lamarckians. 

Many blind fishes and crustaceans are found in caves. 
Lamarckians assume, as yet with insufficient evidence, that 
the blindness is due to the darkness and to the disuse 
of the eyes. Changes thus produced are believed, again 
with insufficient evidence, to be transmitted and increased, 
generation after generation. This is a natural and simple 
theory, but it is not a certain conclusion. 

What is Prof. Ray Lankester’s explanation ? 

“The facts are fully explained by the theory of natural 
selection acting on congenital fortuitous variations. Many 
animals are born with distorted or defective eyes whose 
parents have not had their eyes submitted to any peculiar 
conditions. Supposing a number of some species of Arthro- 
pods or fish to be swept into a cavern, those individuals with 
perfect eyes would follow the glimmer of light and eventually 
escape to the outer air, leaving behind those with imperfect 
eyes to breed in the dark place. In every succeeding 
generation this would be the case, and even those with 
weak but still seeing eyes would in the course of time 
escape, until only a pure race of eyeless or blind animals 
would be left in the cavern.” This is a possible explanation, 
but it is not a certain conclusion. 

(5) The argument which I would urge most strongly is 
based on general physiological considerations. It gives 
no demonstration, but it seems to establish a presump- 
tion against Weismann’s conclusion. He maintains that 
functional and environmental changes in the body cannot 
be transmitted because such changes cannot reach the 
stable and to some extent insulated reproductive elements. 
But this cazmot requires proof, just as much as the converse 
can. 

The organism is a unity; cell is often linked to cell by 
bridges of living matter; the blood is a common medium 
carrying food and waste ; nervous relations bind the whole 
in harmony. Would it not be a physiological miracle if the. 
reproductive cells led a charmed life unaffected even by 


CHAP. XX Fleredity 335 


influences which touch the very heart of the organism? Is 
it unreasonable to presume that some influences of habit and 
conditions, of training and control, saturate the organism 
thoroughly enough to affect every part of it? 

A slight change of food affects the development of the 
reproductive organs in a bee-grub, and makes a queen out of 
what otherwise would have been a worker. A difference of 
diet causes a brood of tadpoles to become almost altogether 

female. There is no doubt that some somatic changes 
affect the reproductive cells in some way. Is it incon- 
ceivable that they affect them in such a precise way that 
bodily changes may be transmitted ? 

It must be admitted that it is at present impossible to 
give an explanation of the way in which a modification 
of the brain can affect the cells of the reproductive organs, 
The only connections that we know are by the blood, by 
nervous thrills, by protoplasmic continuity of cells. But 
there are many indubitable physiological influences which 
-spread through the body of which we can give no rationale. 
Because we cannot tell how an influence spreads, we need 
not deny its existence. 

It is at least conceivable that a deep functional or 
environmental change may result in chemical changes 
which spread from cell to cell, that characteristic products 
may be carried about by the blood and absorbed by the 
unspecialised reproductive cells, that nervous thrills of 
unknown efficacy may pass from part to part. Nor do we 
expect that more than a slight change will be transmitted 
in one generation. 

Weismann traces all variations ultimately to the action 
of the environment on the original unicellular organisms. 
These are directly affected by surrounding influences, and 
as they have no “body” nor specialised reproductive 
elements, but are single cells, it is natural that the char- 
acters acquired by a parent-cell should also belong to the 
daughter-units into which it divides. And if so, is it not 
possible that the reproductive cells of higher animals, being 
equivalent to Protozoa, may be definitely affected by their 
immediate environment, the body? Moreover, if it were 


336 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


proved that the definite changes produced on an individual 
by influences of use, disuse, and surroundings, do not reach 
the reproductive cells, and cannot, therefore, be transmitted, 
it is not thereby proved that secondary results or some results 
of such definite changes may not have some effect on the 
germ-cells. The conditions are so complex that it Seems 
rash to deny the possibility of such influence. 

Certainly it is no easy task to explain all the adapta- 
tions to strange surroundings and habits, or the majority of 
animal instincts, or the progress of men, apart from the 
theory that some of the results of environmental influence 
and habitual experience are transmitted. I am certainly 
unable to reconcile myself to the opinion that the progress 
of life is due to the action of natural selection on fortuitous, 
indefinite, spontaneous variations. 

I believe that the conclusion of the whole matter should 
be an emphatic “not proven” on either side, while the 
practical corollary is that we should cease to talk so much 
about possibilities (in regard to which one opinion is often 
as logically reasonable as another), and betake ourselves 
with energy to a study of the facts. 

5. Social and Ethical Aspects.—All the important 
biological conclusions have a human interest. 

The fact of organic continuity between germ and germ 
helps us to realise that the child is virtually as old as the 
parent, and that the main line of hereditary connection 
is not so much that between parent and child as ‘that 
between the sets of elements out of which the personal 
parents had been evolved, and the set out of which the 
personal child was evolved.” ‘The main line,” Galton 
says, “may be rudely likened to the chain of a necklace, 
and the personalities to pendants attached to the links.” 
To this fact social inertia is largely due, for the organic 
stability secured by germinal continuity tends to hinder 
evolution by leaps and bounds either forwards or backwards. 
There is some resemblance between the formula of heredity 
and the first law of motion. The practical corollary is 
respect for a good stock. 

That each parent contributes almost equally to the off- 


CHAP, XX Fleredity 337 


spring suggests the two-sided responsibility of parentage ; 
but the fact has to be corrected by Galton’s statistical con- 
clusion that the offspring inherits a fourth from each 
parent, and a sixteenth from each grandparent! Inherited 
capital is not merely dual, but multiple like a mosaic. 

If we adopt a modified form of Weismann’s conclusion, 
and believe that only the more deeply penetrating acquired 
characters are transmitted, we are saved from the despair 
suggested by the abnormal functions and environments of 
our civilisation, 

And just in proportion as we doubt the transmission of 
desirable acquired characters, so much the more should we 
desire to secure that improved conditions of life foster the 
individual development of each successive generation. 

That pathological conditions, innate or congenital in the 
organism, tend to be transmitted, suggests that men should 
be informed and educated as to the undesirability of 
parentage on the part of abnormal’ members of the com- 
munity. 

But while no one will gainsay the lessons to be drawn 
from the experience of past generations, it should be noticed 
that Virchow and others have hinted at an “optimism of 
pathology,” since some of the less adequately known abnor- 
mal variations may be associated with new beginnings not 
without promise of possible utility. It seems, moreover, 
that by careful environment and function, or by the inter- 
crossing of a slightly tainted and a relatively pure stock, a 
recuperative or counteractive influence may act so as to 
produce comparatively healthy offspring, thus illustrating 
what may be called ‘‘the forgiveness of nature.” 

6. Social Inheritance. — The widest problems of 
heredity are raised when we substitute “ fraternities” for 
individuals, or make the transition to social inheritance— 
the relation between the successive generations of a society. 

The most important pioneering work is that of Galton, 
whose unique papers have been recently summed up in a 
work entitled Natural Inheritance. Galton derived his 
data from his Records of Family Faculties, especially con- 
cerning stature, eye-colour, and artistic powers; and his 

7 


338 The Study of Animal Life PART IV 


work has been in great part an application of the statistical 
law of Frequency of Error to the records accumulated. 

The main problem of his work is concerned with the 
strange regularity observed in the peculiarities of great 
populations throughout a series of generations. ‘The 
large do not always beget the large, nor the small the 
small; but yet the observed proportion between the large 
and the small, in each degree of size and in every quality 
hardly varies from one generation to another.” A specific 
average is sustained. This is not because each individual 
leaves his like behind him, for this is not the case. It is 
rather due to the fact of a regular regression or deviation 
which brings the offspring of extraordinary parents in a 
definite ratio nearer the average of the stock. 

‘‘ However paradoxical it may appear at first sight, it is 
theoretically a necessary fact, and one that is clearly con- 
firmed by observation, that the stature of the adult offspring 
must on the whole be more mediocre than the stature of 
their parents—that is to say, more near to the median 
stature of the general population. Each peculiarity of a 
man is shared by his kinsmen, but oz an average in a less 
degree. It is reduced to a definite fraction of its amount, 
quite independently of what its amount might be. The 
fraction differs in different orders of kinship, becoming 
smaller as they are more remote.” 

Yet it must not be supposed that the value of a good stock 
is under-estimated by Galton, for he shows how the offspring 
of two ordinary members of a gifted stock will not regress 
like the offspring of a couple equal in gifts to the former, 
but belonging to a poorer stock, above the average of which 
they have risen. 

Yet the fact of regression tells against the full transmission 
of any signal talent. Children are not likely to differ from 
mediocrity so widely as their parents. ‘The more bounti- 
fully a parent is gifted by nature, the more rare will be his 
good fortune if he begets a son who is as richly endowed as 
himself, and still more so if he has a son who is endowed 
more largely.” But ‘‘ The law is even-handed ; it levies an 
equal succession-tax on the transmission of badness as of 


CHAP, XxX flevedity 339 


goodness, If it discourages the extravagant hope of a gifted 
parent that his children will inherit all his powers, it no less 
discountenances extravagant fears that they will inherit all 
his weakness and disease.” 

The study of individual inheritance, as in Galton’s 
flereditary Genius, may tend to develop an aristocratic and 
justifiable pride of race when a gifted lineage is verifiable 
for generations. It may lead to despair if the records of 
family diseases be subjected to investigation. 

But the study of social inheritance is at once more demo- 
cratic and less pessimistic. The nation is a vast fraternity, 
with an average towards which the noble tend, but to which 
the offspring of the under-average as surely approximate. 
Measures which affect large numbers are thus more hopeful 
than those which artificially select a few. 

Even when we are doubtful as to the degree in which 
acquired characters are transmissible, we cannot depreciate 
the effect on individuals of their work and surroundings. 
In fact there should be the more earnestness in our desire to 
conserve healthful function and stimulating environment of 
every kind, for these are not less important if their influences 
must needs be repeated on each fresh generation. ‘There 
was a child went forth every day; and the first object he 
looked upon, that object he became; and that object 
became part of him for the day, or a certain part of the 
day, or for many years, or for stretching cycles of years.” 1 

Nor can we forget how much a plastic physical and 
mental education may do to counteract disadvantageous 
inherited qualities, or to strengthen characters which are 
useful. 

Every one will allow at least that much requires to be 
done in educating public opinion, not only to recognise all 
the facts known in regard to heredity, but also to admit the 
value and necessity of the art which Mr. Galton calls 
“eugenics,” or in frank English “ good-breeding.” 


1 Walt Whitman's ‘‘ Assimilations.” 


APPENDIX 1 
ANIMAL LIFE AND OURS 


A. Our Relation to Animals 


1. Affinities and Differences between Man and Monkeys. 
In one of the works of Broca, a pioneer anthropologist of renown, 
there is an eloquent apology for those who find it useful to con- 
sider man’s zoological relations. 

‘¢ Pride,” he says, ‘‘ which is one of the most characteristic traits 
of our nature, has prevailed with many minds over the calm testi- 
mony of reason. Like the Roman emperors who, enervated by all 
their power, ended by denying their character as men, in fact, by 
believing themselves demigods, so the king of our planet pleases 
himself by imagining that the vile animal, subject to his caprices, 
cannot have anything in common with 47s peculiar nature. The 
proximity of the monkey vexes’ him, it is not enough to be king of 
animals ; he wishes to separate himself from his subjects by a deep 
unfathomable abyss ; and, turning his back upon the earth, he takes 
refuge with his menaced majesty in a nebulous sphere, ‘the human 
kingdom.’ But anatomy, like that slave who followed the con- 
queror’s chariot crying, Memento te hominem esse, anatomy comes 
to trouble man in his naive self-admiration, reminding him of the 
visible tangible facts which bind him to the animals.” 

Let us hearken to this slave a little, remembering Pascal’s 
maxims: ‘It is dangerous to show man too plainly how like he is 
to the animals, without, at the same time, reminding him of his 
greatness. It is equally unwise to impress him with his greatness, 
and not with his lowliness. It is worse to leave him in ignorance 
of both. But it is very profitable to recognise the two facts.” 

It is many years since Owen—now a veteran among anatomists 
—described the ‘‘all-pervading similitude of structure” between 


APP. I Animal Life and Ours 341 


man and the highest monkeys. Subsequent research has continued 
to add corroborating details. As far as structure is concerned, 
there is much less difference between man and the gorilla than 
between the gorilla and a monkey like a marmoset. Yet differences 
between man and the anthropoid apes do exist. Thus man alone 
is thoroughly erect after his infancy is past, his head weighted with 
a heavy brain does not droop forward, and with his erect attitude 
his perfect development of vocal mechanism is perhaps connected. 
We plant the soles of our feet flat on the ground, our great toes 
are usually in a line with the rest, and we have better heels than 
monkeys have, but no emphasis can be laid on the old distinction 
which separated two-handed men (Bimana) from the four-handed 
monkeys (Quadrumana), nor on the fact that man is peculiarly 
naked. We have a bigger forehead, a less protrusive face, smaller 
cheek-bones and eyebrow ridges, a true chin, and more uniform 
teeth than the anthropoid apes. More important, however, is the 
-fact that the weight of the gorilla’s brain bears to that of the smallest 
brain of an adult man the ratio of 2 : 3, and to the largest human 
brain the ratio of 1 : 3; in other words, a man may have a brain 
three times as heavy as that of a gorilla. The brain of a healthy 
human adult never weighs less than 31 or 32 ounces; the average 
human brain weighs 48 or 49 ounces; the heaviest gorilla brain 
does not exceed 20 ounces. ‘‘The cranial capacity is never less 
than 55 cubic inches in any normal human subject, while in the 
orang and the chimpanzee it is but 26 and 274 cubic inches 
respectively.” 

But differences which can be measured and weighed give us little 
hint of the characteristically human powers of building up ideas and 
of cherishing ideals. It is not merely that man profits by his 
experience, as many animals do, but that he makes some kind of 
theory of it. It is not merely that he works for ends which are 
remote, as do birds and beavers, but that he controls his life 
according to conscious ideals of conduct. But I need not say much 
in regard to the characteristics of human personality, we are all 
conscious of them, though we may differ as to the words in which 
they may be expressed; nor need I talk about man’s power of 
articulate speech, nor his realisation of history, nor his inherent 
social sympathies, nor his gentleness. For all recognise that the 
higher life of men has a loftier pitch than that of animals, while 
many think that the difference is in kind, not merely in degree. 

2. Descent of Man.—The arguments by which Darwin and 
others have sought to show that man arose from an ancestral type 
common to him and to the higher apes are the same as those used 
to substantiate the general doctrine of descent. For the Descent 
of Man was but the expansion of a chapter in the Origin of Species ; 


342 The Study of Animal Life APP. 


the arguments used to prove the origin of animal from animal were 
adapted to rationalise the ascent of man. 

(a) Phystological.—The bodily life of man is like that of mon- 
keys ; both are subject to the same diseases ; various human traits, 
such as gestures and expressions, are paralleled among the ‘‘ brutes ” ; 
and children born during famine or in disease are often sadly 
ape-like. 

(4) Morphological.—The structure of man is like that of the 
anthropoid apes, none of his distinctive characters except that of 
a heavy brain being momentous, and there are about seventy 
vestigial structures in the muscular, skeletal, and other systems. 

(c) Héstorical.—There is little certainty in regard to the fossil 
remains of prehistoric man, but some of these suggest more primi- 
tive skulls, while the facts known about ancient life show at least 
that there has been progress along certain lines. Moreover, there 
is the progress of each individual life, from the apparently simple 
egg-cell to the minute embryo, which is fashioned within the womb 
into the likeness of a child, and being born grows from stage to 
stage, all in a manner which it is hard to understand if man be 
not the outcome of a natural evolution. 

3. Various Opinions about the Descent of Man.—But 
opinion in regard to the origin of man is by no means unanimous. 

(2) A few authorities, notably A. de Quatrefages, maintain a 
conservative position, believing that the evolutionist’s case has not 
been sufficiently demonstrated. But the majority of naturalists 
believe the reverse, and think that the insufficiencies of evidence in 
regard to man are counterbalanced by the force of the argument 
from analogy. 

(4) Alfred Russel Wallace has consistently maintained a position 
which seems to many a very strong one. ‘‘I fully accept,” he 
says, ‘‘ Mr. Darwin’s conclusion as to the essential identity of man’s 
bodily structure with that of the higher mammalia, and his descent 
from some ancestral form common to man and the anthropoid apes. 
The evidence of such descent appears to me overwhelming and 
conclusive. Again, as to the cause and method of such descent 
and modification, we may admit, at all events provisionally, that 
the laws of variation and natural selection, acting through the 
struggle for existence and the continual need of more perfect 
adaptation to the physical and biological environments, may have 
brought about, first that perfection of bodily structure in which he 
is so far above all other animals, and in co-ordination with it the 
larger and more developed brain, by means of which he has been 
able to utilise that structure in the more and more complete sub- 
jection of the whole animal and vegetable kingdoms to his 
service.” 


1 Animal Life and Ours 343 


‘* But because man’s physical structure has been developed 
from an animal form by natural selection, it does not necessarily 
follow that his mental nature, even though developed gard passu 
with it, has been developed by the same causes only.” Wallace 
then goes on to show that man’s mathematical, musical, artistic, 
and other higher faculties could not be developed by variation and 
natural selection alone. ‘* Therefore some other influence, law, or 
agency is required to account for them.” Indeed this unknown 
cause or power may have had a much wider influence, extending 
to the whole course of his development. ‘‘ The love of truth, the 
delight in beauty, the passion for justice, and the thrill of exulta- 
tion with which we hear of any act of courageous self-sacrifice, are 
the workings within us of a higher nature which has not been 
developed by means of the struggle for material existence.” At 
the origin of living things, at the introduction of consciousness, in 
the development of man’s higher faculties, ‘*a change in essential 
nature (due, probably, to causes of a higher order than those of the 
material universe) took place.” ‘‘ The progressive manifestations 
of life in the vegetable, the animal, and man—which we may 
classify as unconscious, conscious, and intellectual life—probably 
depend upon different degrees of spiritual influx.” 

In discussing problems such as this there is apt to be misunder- 
standing, for words are ‘‘ but feeble light on the depth of the un- 
spoken,” and perhaps no man appreciates his brother’s philosophy. 
Therefore, I refrain from seeking to controvert what Wallace has 
said, especially as I also believe that the nature of life and mind 
are secrets to us all, and that the higher life of man cannot be 
explained by indefinite variations which happened to prosper in the 
course of natural selection. 

But it seems to me (1) to be difficult to divide man’s self into 
an animal nature which has been naturally evolved and ‘‘a 
spiritual nature which has been superadded,” or to separate man’s 
higher life from that of some of the beasts. (2) When we find 
that any fact in our experience, such as human reason, cannot 
be explained on the theory of evolution which we have adopted, it 
does not follow that the reality in question has not been naturally 
evolved, it only follows that our theory of evolution is imperfect. 
A theory is not proved to be complete because it explains many 
facts, but it is proved to be incomplete if it fails to explain any. 
Thus if man’s higher nature cannot be explained by the theory of 
natural selection in the struggle for existence, then that theory is 
incomplete, but there may be other theories of evolution which are 
sufficient. (3) It is difficult to know what is meant by spiritual 
influx—for our opinions in regard to those matters vary with 
individual experience. We may mean to suggest the interpola- 


344 The Study of Animal Life APP, 


tion of a power of a secret and supersensory nature, distinct from 
that power which is everywhere present in sunbeam and rain- 
drop, bird and flower. Then we are abandoning the theory of a 
continuous natural evolution. Or we may mean to suggest that when 
life and mind and man began to be, then possibilities of action and 
reaction hitherto latent became real, and all things became in a 
sense new. Then, while maintaining that life and mind are new 
realities with new powers, we are still consistent believers in a con- 
tinuous natural evolution. (4) Perhaps the simplest conception is 
that more than once suggested in this book, that the world is one 
not twofold, that the spiritual influx is the primal reality, that there 
is nothing in the end which was not also in the beginning. 

(c) Prof. Calderwood has recently stated with clearness and 
conciseness what difficulties surround the task of those who would 
explain the evolution of man. ‘So far as the human organism is 
concerned, there seem no overwhelming obstacles to be encountered 
by an evolution theory ; but it seems impossible under such a theory 
to account for the appearance of homo sapiens—the thinking, self- 
regulating life, distinctively human.” Again, I have no desire to 
enter into controversy, for I recognise the difficulties which the 
student of comparative psychology must tackle, but it seems 
important that the following consideration should be kept in mind. 

It is not the first business of the evolutionist to find out how one 
reality has grown out of another, but to marshal the arguments 
which lead him to conclude that one reality Zas so evolved. We 
have only a vague idea how a backbone arose, but that need not 
hinder us from believing that backboned animals were evolved. from 
backboneless if there be sufficient evidence in favour of this con- 
clusion. We do not know how birds arose from a reptile stock, 
but that they did so arise is fairly certain, We cannot explain the 
intelligence of man in terms of the activity of the brain; we are 
equally at a loss in regard to the intelligence of an ant. What we 
have to do is to compare the structure of man’s brain with that of 
the nearest animals, and the nature of human intelligence with that 
of the closest approximations, drawing from the results of our 
comparison what conclusion we can. The general doctrine of 
descent may be established independently of the investigations of 
physiologist and psychologist, valuable as these may be in elucidat- 
ing the way in which the great steps of progress have been made. 

(d) Finally there is the opinion of many that man is altogether 
too marvellous a being to have arisen from any humbler form of 
life. But to others this ascent seems the stamp of man’s nobility. 

4. Ancestors of Man.—Of these we know nothing. The 
anthropoid apes approach him most closely, each in some particular 
respect, but none of them nor any known form of life can be called 


I Animal Life and Ours 345 


man’s ancestor. It is possible that the race of men—for of a 
first man evolutionists cannot speak—began in Miocene times, 
as offshoots from an ancestral stock common to them and to the 
anthropoids. We often hear of ‘ the missing link,” but surely no 
one expects to find him alive. And while we have still much to 
learn from the imperfect geological record, it must be remembered 
that what most distinguishes man will not-be remarkable in a fossil, 
for brains do not petrify except metaphorically, nor can we look for 
fossilised intelligence or gentleness. 


Fic. 71.—Young gorilla. (From Du Chaillu.) 


5. Possible Factors in the Ascent of Man,—In regard to 
the factors which secured man’s ascent from a humbler form of life 
we can only speculate. 

(a) We have already explained that organisms vary, that the 
offspring differ from their parents, that the more favourable changes 
prosper, and that the less fit die out of the struggle. Thus the race 
is lifted. Now, from what we know of men and monkeys, it seems 
likely that in the struggles of primitive man cunning was more 
important than strength, and if intelligence now became, more than 
ever before, the condition of life or death, wits would tend to 
develop rapidly. 

(6) When habits of using sticks and stones, of building shelters, 
of living in families, began—and some monkeys exhibit these—it is 
likely that wits would increase by leaps and bounds. 

(c) Professor Fiske and others have emphasised the importance of 
prolonged infancy, and this must surely have helped to evolve the 
gentleness of mankind. 


346 The Study of Animal Life APP. 


(Z) Among many monkeys society has begun. Families com- 
bine for protection, and the combination favours the development 
both of emotional and intellectual strength. Surely ‘man did not 
make society, society made man.” 


B. Our Relation to Biology. 


6. The Utility of Science.—As life is short, all too short for 
learning the art of living, it is well that we should criticise our 
activities, and favour those which seem to yield most return of 
health and wealth and wisdom. 

We are so curious about all kinds of things, so omnivorously 
hungry for information, that the most trivial department of know- 
ledge or science may afford exercise and mental satisfaction to its 
votaries. The interest and pleasantness of science is therefore no 
criterion. 

Nor can we be satisfied with the assertion that science should 
be pursued for science’s sake. As in regard to the kindred dictum, 
‘art for art’s sake,” we require further explanation—some ideal of 
science and art. For it is not evident that knowledge is a good in 
itself, especially if that knowledge be gained at the expense of the 
emotional wealth which is often associated with healthy ignorance. 

Nor is it safe to judge scientific activity by the material results 
which the application of knowledge to action may yield. For aseed 
of knowledge may lie dormant for centuries before it sends its shoots 
into life, and many of the material results of applied science are not 
unmixed blessings. Moreover, too narrow a view may be taken of 
material results, so-called ‘‘ necessaries ”’ of existence may be exalted 
over the ‘‘super-necessaries ” essential to life; in short, what lies 
about the mouth—the nose, the ears, the eyes, the brain—may be 
forgotten. 

We are nearer the truth if we combine the different standards of 
science, and unify them by reference to the human ideal.!_ The utility 
of science, and of biology among the other kinds of knowledge, is 
to supply a basis of fact— 

(a) For the practice of useful arts (such as hygiene and 
education), and for the guidance of conduct : 

(4) For the satisfaction of our desire to understand and enjoy 
the world and our life in it. 

7. Practical Justification of Biology.—The world of life 
is so web-like that almost any part may touch or thrill us. It is 
therefore well that we should learn what we can about it. 

On plants we are very dependent for food and drink, for shelter 


1 See Ruskin, The Eagle's Nest (1880). 


1 Animal Life and Ours 347 


and clothing, and for delight. Their evil influence is almost 
restricted to that of disease germs and poisonous herbs. 

Animals likewise furnish food (perhaps to an unwholesome 
extent); and parts of their bodies are used (sometimes carelessly) 
in manifold ways. Among those which are domesticated, some, 
such as canary and parrot, cat and dog, are kept for the pleasure 
they give to many; others, such as dog, horse, elephant, and 
falcon, are used in the chase; others, notably the dog, assist in 
shepherding; horse and ass, reindeer and cattle, camel and 
elephant, are beasts of burden; others yield useful products, the 
milk of cows and goats, the eggs of birds, the silk of silkworms, 
and the honey of bees. 

Formerly of much greater importance for good and ill as direct 
rivals, animals have, through man’s increasing mastery of life, become - 
less dangerous and more directly useful. Only in primitive con- 
ditions of life and in thinly-peopled territories is something of the 
old struggle still experienced. Their influence for ill is now for the 
most part indirect,—on crops and stocks. Parasites are common 
enough, but rarely fatal. The serpent, however, still bites the 
heel of progressive man. 

Man’s.relations with living creatures are so close that systematic 
knowledge about them is evidently of direct use. Indeed it is in 
practical lore that both botany and zoology have their primal roots, 
and from these, now much strengthened, impulses do not cease to 
give new life to science. 

If increase of food-supply be desirable, biology has something to 
say about soil and cereals, about fisheries and oyster-culture. The 
art of agriculture and breeding has been influenced not a little by 
scientific advice, though much more by unrationalised experience. 
If wine be wanted, the biologist has something to say about grafting 
and the Phylloxera, about mildew and Bacteria. It is enough to 
point to the succession of discoveries by which Pasteur alone has 
enriched science and benefited humanity. 

But if we take higher ground and consider as an ideal the health- 
fulness of men, which is one of the most obvious and useful 
standards of individual and social conduct, the practical justification 
of biological science becomes even more apparent. 

Medicine, hygiene, physical education, and good-breeding (or 
“eugenics ”) are the arts which correspond to the science of 
biology, just as education is applied psychology, as government is 
applied sociology, and as many industries are applied chemistry and 
physics. It would be historically untrue to say that the progress in 
these arts was due to progress in the parallel sciences ; in fact the 
progressive impulse has often been from art to science. ‘La 
pratique a partout devancé la théorie,” Espinas says, and all 


348 The Study of Animal Life APP, 


historians of science would in the main confirm this. But it is 
also true that science reacts on the arts and sometimes improves 
them. 

There may be peculiar aberrations of the art of medicine due to 
the progress of the science thereof, but these are because the science 
is partial, and hardly affect the general fact that scientific progress 
has advanced the art of healing. The results of science have like- 
wise supplied a basis to the endeavours to prevent disease and to 
increase healthfulness, not only by definite hygienic practice but 
perhaps still more by diffusing some precise knowledge of the 
conditions of health, 

The generalisations of biology, realised in men’s minds, must in 
some measure affect practice and public opinion. Spenceyr’s 
induction that the rate of reproduction varies inversely with the 
degree of development sheds a hopeful light on the population 
question ; the recognition of the influence which function and sur- 
roundings have upon the organism suggests criticism of many 
modes of economic production; a knowledge of the facts and 
theory of heredity must have an increasing influence on the art of 
eugenics. Nor can I believe that the theory of evolution which 
men hold, granting that it is in part an expression of their life and 
social environment, does not also react on these. 

In short, the direct application of biological knowledge in the 
various arts of medicine, hygiene, physical education, and eugenics, 
helps us to perfect our environment and our relations with it, helps 
us to discover—if not the ‘‘elixir vitee”—some not despicable 
substitute. And likewise, a realisation of the facts and principles of 
biology helps us to criticise, justify, and regulate conduct, suggest- 
ing how the art of life may be better learned, how human relations 
may be more wisely harmonised, how we may guide and help the 
ascent of man. 

8. Intellectual Justification of Biology.— But another 
partial justification of Biology is found in our desire to understand 
things, in our dislike of obscurities, in our inborn curiosity. There 
is an intellectual as well as a practical and ethical justification of 
the study of organic life. 

Through our senses we become aware of the world of which we 
form a part. We cannot know it in itself, for we are part of it and 
only know it as it becomes part of us. We know only fractions of 
reality—real at least to us—and these are unified in our experience. 

(1) In the world around us we are accustomed to distinguish 
four orders of facts. ‘* Matter” and ‘‘ energy ” we call those which 
seem to us fundamental, because all that we know by our senses 
are forms of these. The study of matter and energy—or perhaps 
we may say the study of matter in motion—considered apart from 


I Animal Life and Ours 349 


life, we call Physics and Chemistry, of which astronomy, geology, 
etc., are special departments. 

(2) But we also know something about plants and animals, and 
while all that we know about them is still dependent upon changes 
of matter and motion, yet we recognise that the activities of the 
organism cannot at present be expressed in terms of these. There- 
fore we find it convenient to speak of life as a new reality, while 
believing that it is the result of some combination of matters and 
energies, the secret of which is hidden. 

(3) But we are also aware of another reality, ourown mind. Of 
this we have direct consciousness and greater certainty than about 
anything else. And while some would say that what we are 
conscious of when we think is a protoplasmic change in our brain 
cells or is a subtle kind of motion, it is truer to say that we are 
conscious of ourselves. It is our thought that we know, it is our 
feeling that we feel, and as we cannot explain the thought or the 
feeling in terms of protoplasm or of motion, we find it convenient to 
speak of mind as a new reality, while believing it to be essentially 
associated with some complex activity of protoplasm the secret of 
which is hidden. For our knowledge of our own mental processes, 
and of those inferred to be similar in our fellows, and of those 
inferred to be not very different in intelligent animals, we establish 
another science of Psychology. 

(4) But we also know something about the life of the human 
society of which we form a part. We recognise that it has a unity 
of its own, and that its activities are more than those of its 
individual members added up. ‘We find it convenient to regard 
society as another synthesis or unity—though less definite than 
either organism or mind—and to our knowledge of the life and 
growth of society as a whole, we apply the term’ sociology. 

Thus we recognise four orders of facts and four great sciences— 


4. Society . . . . . Sociology. 

3. Mind. . . z . Psychology. 

2. Life . . . . . . . Biology. 

1. Matter and Energy . . . Physics and Chemistry. 


Each of these sciences is dependent upon its predecessor. The 
student of organisms requires help from the student of chemistry 
and physics ; mind cannot be discussed apart from body; nor can 
society be studied apart from the minds of its component members. 

Each order of realities we may regard as a subtle synthesis of 
those which we call simpler. Life is a secret synthesis of matter 
and energy; mind is a subtle form of life; society is a unity of 
minds. 

But it must be clearly recognised that the ‘‘ matter and energy” 
which we regard as the fundamental realities are only known to us 


350 The Study of Animal Life APP. 1 


through what is for us the supreme reality—ourselves—mind. And 
as in our brain activity we know matter and energy as thought, I 
have adopted throughout this book what may be called a monistic 
philosophy. 

Having recognised the central position of Biology among the 
other sciences, we have still to inquire what its task precisely is. 

Our scientific data are (1) the impressions which we gather 
through our senses about living creatures, and (2) the deductions 
which we directly draw in regard to these. Our scientific aim 
is to arrange these data so that we may have a mental picture of 
the life around us, so that we may be better able to understand 
what that life is, and how it has come to be what it seems to be. 
Pursuing what are called scientific methods, we try to make the 
world of life and our life as organisms as intelligible as possible. 
We seek to remove obscurities of perception, to make the world 
translucent, to make a working thought-model of the world. 

But we are apt to forget how ignorant we are about the realities 
themselves, for all the time we are dealing not with realities, but 
with impressions of realities, and with inferences from these im- 
pressions. On the other hand, we are apt to forget that our deep 
desire is not merely to know, but to enjoy the world, that the heart 
of things is not so much known by the man as it is felt by the child. 


APPENDIX II 
SOME OF THE ‘‘ BEST BOOKS” ON ANIMAL LIFE 


To recommend the ‘best books” on any subject is apt to be like 
prescribing the ‘‘ best diet.” Both depend upon age, constitution, 
and opportunities. The best book for me is that which does me 
most good, but it may be tedious reading for you. Moreover, 
books are often good for one purpose and not for another; that 
which helps us to realise the beauty and marvel of animal life may 
be of little service to those who are preparing for any of the 
numerous examinations in science. But the greatest difficulty is 
that we are often too much influenced by contemporary opinion, 
so that we lose our power of appreciating intellectual per- 
spective. 

The best way to begin the study of Natural History is to 
observe animal life, but the next best way is to read such accounts 
of observation and travel as are to be found in the works of 
Gilbert White, Thoreau, Richard Jefferies, and John Burroughs, 
or in Bates’s Maturalist on the Amazons, Belt’s Naturalist in 
Nicaragua, and Darwin’s Voyage of the “ Beagle.” Sooner or later 
the student will seek more systematic books, but it is not natural 
that he should begin with a text-book of elementary biology. 

In introducing you to the literature devoted to the study of 
animals, I shall avoid the bias of current opinion by following the 
history of zoology. I shall first name some of the more technical 
books ; secondly, some of the more popular; thirdly, some of the 
more theoretical. If I may make the distinction, I shall first 
mention books on zoology, secondly those on natural history, 
thirdly those on biology. 


A. Zoology. 


(1) We can form a vivid conception of the history of zoology 
by comparing it with our own. In our childhood we knew and 


352 The Study of Animal Life APP. 


cared more about the useful, dangerous, and strange animals than 
about those which were humble and familiar; we had more in- 
terest in haunts and habits than in structure and history; we 
were content with rough-and-ready classification, and cherished 
a feeling of superstitious awe in regard to the indistinctly-known 
forms of life. We were inquisitive rather than critical; we 
accepted almost any explanation of facts, and, if we tried to inter- 
pret, forced our borrowed opinions upon nature instead of trying 
to study things for ourselves. So was it with those naturalists who 
lived before Aristotle. 

We must also recognise that the science of zoology had its 
beginnings in a practical acquaintance with animals, just as botany 
sprang from the knowledge of ancient agriculturists and herb- 
gatherers. Much information in regard to the earliest zoological 
knowledge has been gathered from researches into the history of 
words, art, and religious customs, and there is still much to be 
gleaned. Therefore I should recommend the student to dip into 
those books which discuss the early history of man, such as 
Lubbock’s Prehistoric Times (1865), and Origin of Civilisation 
(1870); Tylor’s Primitive Culture (1871), and Anthropology 
(1881); Andrew Lang’s JZyths, Ritual, and Religion; besides 
works on the history of philosophy, such as those of Schwegler and 
of Zeller, which give some account of ancient cosmogonies. 

(2) But just as there are precocious children, so there was an 
early naturalist, whose works form the most colossal monument to 
the intellectual prowess of any one thinker. The foundations of 
zoology were laid by Aristotle, who lived 384-322 Bc. He 
collected many observations, and argued from them to general 
statements. He records over five hundred animals, and describes 
the structure and habits, the struggles and friendliness, of some 
of these. His is the first definite classification. His work 
was dominated by the idea that animal life is 4 unity and part 
of a larger system of things. In part his works should be read, 
and besides the great edition by Bekker (Berlin, 1831-40), 
there is a translation of Zhe Parts of Animals by Dr. Ogle, and 
of Zhe History of Animals by BR. Cresswell. See also G. J. 
Romanes’s ‘‘ Aristotle as a Naturalist,” Wineteenth Century (Feb. 
1891, pp. 275-289). 

(3) After the freedom of early childhood, and in most cases 
after precocity too, there comes a lull of inquisitiveness. Other 
affairs, practical tasks, games and combats, engross the attention, 
and parents sigh over dormant intellects; so the historian of 
zoology sighs over the fifteen centuries during which science 
slumbered. The foundations which Aristotle had firmly laid 
remained, but the walls of the temple.of knowledge did not rise, 


1 Some of the “ Best Books” on Animal Life 353 


The seeds which he had sown were alive, but they did not germi- 
nate. Men were otherwise occupied, with practical affairs, with 
the tasks of civilisation alike in peace and war, though some at 
their leisure played with ideas which they did not verify. There 
were some exceptions; such as Pliny (23-79 A.D.), a diligent but 
uncritical collector of facts, and Galen (130-200 A.D.), a medical 
anatomist, who had the courage to dissect monkeys; besides the 
Spanish bishop Isidor in the seventh century, and various Arabian 
inquirers. It will not be unprofitable to look into the Matural 
History of Pliny, which has been translated by Bostock and 
Riley. 

(4) But just as. there is in our life a stage—happy are those who 
prolong it—during which we delight in fables and fairy tales, so 
there was a long period of mythological zoology. The schoolboy 
who puts horse-hairs into the brook, and returns after many days 
to find them eel-like worms, is doing what they did in the Middle 
Ages. For then fact and fiction were strangely jumbled; credulity 
ran riot along the paths of science ; allegorical interpretations and 
superstitious symbolisms were abundant as the fancies which flit 
through the minds of dreamers. Scientific inquiry was not en- 
couraged by the theological mood of the time; and just as Scotch 
children cherish The Beasts of the Bible as a pleasantly secular book 
with a spice of sacredness which makes it legitimate reading on 
the Sabbath, so many a medizeval naturalist had to cloak his 
observations in a semi-theological style. 

In illustration of the mood of the medizval naturalists, which 
is by no means to be carelessly laughed at, read John Ashton’s 
Curious Creatures (Lond., 1890), in which much old lore is retold, 
often in the words of the original writers. The most characteristic 
expression of mythical Zoology is a production often called Physzo- 
Jogus. It is found in about a dozen languages and in many 
different forms, being in part merely a precipitate of floating 
traditions. It is partly like a natural history of the beasts of the 
Bible and prototype of many similar works, partly an account of 
the habits of animals, the study of which modern zoologists are 
apt to neglect, partly a collection of natural history fables and 
anecdotes, partly a treatise on symbolism and suggestive of the 
poetical side of zoology, partly an account of the medicinal and 
magical uses of animals. For many centuries it seems to have 
served as a text-book, a fact in itself an index to the slow progress 
of the science. Its influence on art and literature has been con- 
siderable, and it well illustrates the attempt to secure for the 
unextinguishable interest in living things a sanction and foothold 
under the patronage of theology. A series of fifty emblems is 
described, among others the lion which sleeps with its eyes open, 


2A 


354 The Study of Animal Life APr. 


the lizard which recovers its sight by looking at the sun, the eagle 
which renews its youth, the tortoise mistaken for an island, the 
serpent afraid of naked man, and the most miserable ant-lion, 
which is not able either to take one kind of food or digest the 
other. 

(5) But delight in romance is replaced by a feeling of the need 
for definite knowledge, and: the earlier years of adolescent man- 
hood and womanhood are often very markedly characterised by a 
thirst and hunger for information. Which of us—now perhaps 
blasé with too much learning—does not recall the enthusiasm for 
knowing which once swayed our minds? Stimulated in a hundred 
ways by new experiences and responsibilities, our appetite for facts 
was once enormous. ‘This was the mood of naturalists during the 
next great period in the history of zoology. 

The freer circulation of men and thoughts associated with the 
Crusades ; the-discovery of new lands by travellers like Marco 
Polo and Columbus; the founding of universities and learned 
societies ; the establishment of museums and botanic gardens; the 
invention of printing and the reappearance of Aristotle’s works in 
dilution and translation ; and many other practical, emotional, and 
intellectual movements gave fresh force to science, and indeed to 
the whole life of man. If we pass over some connecting links, 
such as Albertus Magnus in the thirteeenth century, we may call 
the period of gradual scientific renaissance that of the Encyclo- 
pedists. This somewhat cumbrous title suggests the omnivorous 
habits of those early workers. They were painstaking collectors 
of all information about all animals; but their appetite was 
greater than their digestion, and the progress of science was 
in quantity rather than in quality. Prominent among them were 
these four, the Englishman Edward Wotton (1492-1555), who 
wrote a treatise De Differentits’ Animalium; the Swiss Conrad 
Gesner (1516-65), author of a well-known Historia Animalium ; 
the Italian Aldrovandi (b. 1522); and the Scotsman Johnston 
(b. 1603), 

About the middle of the eighteenth century the best aims of the 
Encyclopzedists were realised in Buffon’s Aizstocre Naturelle, which 
appeared in fifteen volumes between 1749 and 1767. This work 
not only describes beasts and birds, the earth and man, with an 
eloquent enthusiasm which was natural to the author and pleasing 
to his contemporaries, but is the first noteworthy attempt to 
expound the history or evolution of animals. Its range was very 
wide ; and its successors are not so much single books as many 
different kinds of books, on geology and physical geography, on 
classification and physiology, on anthropofogy and natural history. 
There is a good French edition of Buffon’s complete works by 


it Some of the “ Best Books” on Animal Life 355 


A. Richard 1825-28), and at least one English translation. Three 
large modern books on natural history correspond in some degree 
to the Histoire Naturelle, viz. Cassell’s Natural History, edited by 
P. Martin Duncan (6 vols. ; London, 1882); Zhe Standard or 
Riverside Natural History, edited by J. S. Kingsley (6 vols. ; 
London, 1888); and a remarkable work well known as Brehm’s 
Thierleben, of which a new (3rd) edition is at present in progress 
(10 vols.; Leipzig and Wien, 1890). Those who read German 
will find in Carus Sterne’s (Ernst Krause’s) Werden und Vergehen 
(3rd ed.; Berlin, 1886) the most successful attempt hitherto 
made to combine in one volume a history of the earth and its 
inhabitants. 

(6) From Buffon till now the history of biology shows a pro- 
gressive analysis, a deeper and deeper penetration into the structure 
and life of organisms. From external form to the internal organs, 
from organs to the tissues which compose them, from tissues to 
their elementary units or cells, and from cells to the living matter 
itself, has been the progress of the science of structure—Jor- 
phology. From habit and temperament to the work of organs, 
from the functions of organs to the properties of tissues, from these 
to the activities of cells, and from these finally to the chemical and 
physical changes in the living matter or protoplasm, has been the 
progress of the science of function—Physzology. Such is the lucid 
account which Prof. Geddes has given of the last hundred years’ 
progress; see his article ‘‘ Biology” in the new edition of 
Chambers’s Encyclopedia. Following the metaphor on which we 
have already insisted, we may compare this century of analysis to 
the period of ordered and more intense study which in the individual 
life succeeds the abandonment of encyclopzedic ambitions. 

We should clearly understand the history of this gradually 
deepening analysis of animals; for if we would be naturalists 
we must retread the same path. The history of biology has still 
to be written, but there are already some useful books and papers, 
notably—J. V. Carus, Geschichte der Zoologie (Miinchen, 1872) ; 
J. Sachs, Geschéchte der Botanik (Miinchen, 1875), translated 
into English (Oxford, 1890); W. Whewell, story of the 
Inductive Sciences (London, 1840); articles ‘‘ Morphology ” 
and ** Physiology,” Lwcyclopedia Britannica, by P. Geddes and 
M. Foster; H. A. Nichol8on, WMatural History: tts Rise and 
Progress in Britain (Edinburgh, 1888); A. B. Buckley, Short 
FHiistory of Natural Science; E. Perrier, La Philosophie Zoologique 
avant Darwin (Paris, 1884); Ernst Krause (Carus Sterne), Dze 
Allgemeine Weltanschauung in ihrer historischen Entwickelung 
(Stuttgart, 1889). Very instructive, not least so in contrast, 
are two articles, “Biology” (in Chambers’s Encyclopedia), by 


386 ‘The Study of Animal Life / APP, 


P. Geddes, and ‘‘ Zoology” (in Excyclopedia Britannica), by E. 
Ray Lankester, 

If we think over the sketch which Professor Geddes has given, 
we shall see how easy it is to arrange the literature—the first step 
towards mastering it. (a) The early anatomists were chiefly 
occupied with the study of external and general features, very 
largely moreover with the purpose of establishing 4 classification. 
The Systema Nature of Linnzeus (1st ed., 17353 12th, 1768) is 
the typical work on this heavily-laden shelf of the zoological 
library. It is to such books that we turn when we wish to 
identify some animal, but the shelf is very long and most of the 
volumes are very heavy. Each chapter of Linné’s Systema has 
been expanded into a series of volumes, or into some gigantic 
monograph like those included in the series of ‘‘ Challenger” Reports, 
or Zhe Fauna and Flora of the Gulf of Naples. If I am asked 
to recommend a volume from which the eager student may identify 
some British flower, I can at once place Hooker’s Flora in his 
hands. But it is more difficult to help him to a work by which he 
may identify his animal prize. There are special works on British 
Mammals, Birds, Fishes, Molluscs, Insects, etc., but a compact 
British Fauna is much wanted. I shall simply mention Bronn’s 
Klassen und Ordnungen des Thierreiches, a series of volumes still 
in progress; Leunis, Sywopsis des Thierreiches (Hanover, 1886) ; 
the British Museum Catalogues (in progress); and P. H. Gosse’s 
Manual of Marine Zoology of the British Islands (1856). 

(4) Cuvier’s Régne Animal (1829) is the typical book on the 
next plane of research—that concerned with the anatomy of organs. 
I should recommend the student on this path to begin with Pro- 
fessor F. Jeffrey Bell’s Comparative Anatomy and Phystology (Lond., 
1886) ; after which he will more readily appreciate the text-books 
on Comparative Anatomy by Huxley, Gegenbaur, Claus, Wieders- 
heim, Lang, etc. As an introduction I may also mention my 
Outlines of Zoology (Edin., 1892). As a book of reference 
Hatchett Jackson’s edition of Rolleston’s Forms of Animal Life 
(Oxford, 1888) is of great value, not least on account of its 
scholarly references to the literature of zoology. The zoological 
articles in the Lxcyclopadia Britannica, many of which are pub- 
lished separately, are not less useful. As guides in serious practical 
work may be noticed—A Course of Elementary Instruction in 
Practical Biology by Profs. T. H. Huxley and H. N. Martin, 
revised by Profs. G. B. Howes and D. H. Scott (Lond., 1888) ; 
Howes’s Atlas of Practical Elementary Biology (Lond., 1885); 
A Course of Practical Zoology by Prof. A. Milnes Marshall and 
Dr. C. H. Hurst (3rd ed., Lond., 1892); Prof. C. Lloyd 
Morgan’s Animal Biology (Lond., 1889); Vogt and Yung, 7razé 


un Some of the “ Best Books” on Animal Life 354 


@’ Anatomie comparée pratique (Paris, 1885-92) or in German 
(Braunschweig) ; Prof. W. K. Brooks’s Handbook of Invertebrate 
Zoology for Laboratories and Seaside Work (Boston, 1882) ; Prof. 
T. J. Parker’s Zootomy (Lond., 1884) and Practical Biology 
(Lond., 1891). 

(c) As early as 1801, Bichat had penetrated beneath the organs 
to the tissues which compose them, and his Anatomie Générale is 
the forerunner of many works on minute anatomy or histology. 
From the comparative histology of animals by Leydig (Histologie, 
1867) the zoological student must begin, but to follow it up he must 
have recourse to the pages of scientific journals. Asa guide in 
microscopic work, Dr. Dallinger’s new edition of Carpenter’s well- 
known work, Zhe Microscope (Lond., 1891) may be cited. 

(d) In 1838-39, Schwann and Schleiden, two German naturalists, 
clearly stated a doctrine towards which investigation had been 
gradually tending, namely, that each organism was built up of 
cells, and originated from a fertilised egg-cell. In the establish- 
ment of this ‘‘cell-theory ” the study of structure became deeper, 
and the investigation of animal cells still becomes more and more 
intense. To gain an appreciation of this step in analysis, the 
student may well begin with the article ‘‘ Cell” in the new edition 
of Chambers’s Lxcyclopedia, and with the articles ‘‘ Morphology” 
and ‘‘ Protozoa” in the Excyclopedia Britannica. From these he 
will discover how his studies may be deepened. 

(e) Finally, with the improvement of microscopic instruments 
and technique, investigation has touched the bottom, as far as 
biology is concerned, in the study of the living stuff or protoplasm 
itself. Again, I refer you to the articles ‘‘ Protoplasm” in the 
Encyclopedia Britannica and in Chambers’s Encyclopaedia. 

I shall not follow the history of physiology in detail, but content 
myself with saying that (z) from the conception of a living body 
ruled by spirits or dominated by a temperament, physiologists passed 
to consider it (4) as an engine of living organs, then (c) as a com- 
plex web of tissues, then (d) as a city of cells, and finally (e) as a 
whirlpool of living matter. I recommend you to read first the 
article <‘ Physiology” in the Excyclopadia Britannica, then Huxley’s 
Crayfish (International Science Series), and his Zlementary Text- 
book of Physiology, then Jeffrey Bell’s Comparative Anatomy and 
Physiology and Lioyd Morgan’s Animal Biology, after which you 
may pass to larger works such as the text-books of Kirkes (new ed., 
1892); Bunge (Lond., 1890); Landois and Stirling, McKendrick, 
and Foster, and to the studies on comparative physiology by 
Krukenberg, Vergleichend-Physiologische Studien and Vortrige 
(Heidelberg, 1882-88). 

In the above summary nothing has been said about the history 


358 The Study of Animal Life APP. 


of animals in their individual life (embryology), nor of their gradual 
appearance upon the earth (palzontology), nor about their distri- 
bution in space. As regards embryology, begin with the article 
in Chambers’s Excyclopedia, and pass thence to the text-books 
of A. C. Haddon, F. M. Balfour, M. Foster and F. M. Balfour, 
O. Hertwig, Heider and Korschelt, etc. A short account of 
distribution in time will be found in A. Heilprin’s Distribution of 
Animals (International Science Series), from which advanced 
students may pass to the Zext-book of Paleontology by H. A. 
Nicholson and R, Lydekker (2 vols., Lond. and Edin., 1889), to 
the French work of Gaudry, Les enchati ts du de animal 
dans les temps gtologiques (Paris, 1888-90), or to the German 
works of Zittel and of Neumayr. Heilprin’s book is again the 
best introduction to the study of distribution in space, while 
Wallace’s Geographical Distribution of Animals (Lond., 1876) 
remains the principal work of reference. 

For progressive research I may refer the student to the Journal 
of the Royal Microscopical Society (edited by Prof. F. Jeffrey Bell), 
which gives summaries of recent researches ; the Quarterly Journal 
of Microscopical Science (edited by Profs. E. Ray Lankester, Klein, 
Sedgwick, and Milnes Marshall) ; and of course Mature, in which 
summaries and discussions are often to be found. More popular 
journals are the American Naturalist and Natural Science. 

Of all elementary books the best to begin with are two volumes 
by A. B. Buckley, Life and her Children (backboneless animals), 
and Winners in Lifes Race (backboned animals) ; but I shall now 
mention other ways of beginning. 


B. Natural History. 


“Certain dreadfully scientific persons, who call themselves by 
the name of naturalists, seem to consider zoology and comparative 
anatomy as convertible terms. When they see a creature new to 
them, they are seized with a burning desire to cut it up, to analyse 
it, to get it under the microscope, to publish a learned book about 
it which no one can read without an expensive Greek lexicon, and 
to put up its remains in cells and bottles. They delight in an 
abnormal hemapophysis ; they pin their faith on a pterygoid pro- 
cess; they stake their reputation on the number of tubercules on 
a second molar tooth; and they quarrel with each other about a 
notch on the basisphenoid bone.”’ Thus, in a breezy way, did the 
Rev. J. G. Wood laugh at the morphological zoologists. But his 
good-humoured criticism is apt to be misleading, For if science, 
as such, be justifiable, the work of the anatomist is warranted as 


u Some of the “ Best Books” on Animal Life 389 


part of it, and is neither less nor more valuable than that of the 
field naturalist. We may criticise the details of the anatomist’s 
analysis, we may believe that his discipline is often pressed 
unnaturally upon students, we may beseech him to be less 
pedantic; but to remind him that the study of structure requires 
to be supplemented by the study of life is like reminding the field 
naturalist that animals have bones and muscles. Both are true 
statements, but somewhat obvious. 

The zoologist has deliberately given himself up to analysis, and 
if the world is to become translucent to us, we must include within 
our knowledge what he can tell us about the structure and activities 
of animals, alike as unities and as complex combinations of organs, 
tissues, and cells. Let us agree to call this serious study, including 
the morphological and physiological aspects which we have already 
explained, ‘‘zoology.” We must acknowledge that few of us can 
become zoological experts. But let not this hinder us from per- 
ceiving that it is not difficult to understand towards what end and 
by what method Linnzus and Cuvier, Bichat and Claude Bernard, 
and the other great masters worked ; nor let it deter us from using 
all natural opportunities of practically observing the forms and 
powers of animal life. We shall soon feel that ‘‘zoology” is 
neither less interesting nor less essential than the work of the 
field naturalist, we shall recognise that its terminology is not more 
complex than that of seamanship, and we may even admit that 
from clear zoological thinking our contemplation of nature acquires 
an additional intensity of emotion. ‘Tout naturaliste cachait plus 
ou moins un amateur d’idylles ou d’éclogues.” What Hamerton 
says with reference to an artist’s education applies also to the 
student of science: ‘‘The harm is not in the study (of plants), 
it is in the forgetfulness of large relations to which this minute 
observation of nature has occasionally led those who were addicted 
to it.” Zoologists are not the only workers who sometimes lose 
their sense of perspective. 

Now, however, I would address those who have little time or 
opportunity for “zoology,” but who have an interest in the life 
and habits of animals, and desire to appreciate these more 
thoroughly. This knowledge of animals as personalities — in 
struggle and friendliness, in hate and love, in birth and death, 
I would call “natural history,” in contrast to analytic ‘‘zoology ” 
on the one hand, and generalising ‘‘ biology” on the other. For 
I restrict the latter term to the general theory of life—its nature 
and origin, its growth and continuance. It matters little what 
names are given to these three aspects of the study of animal life ; 
thus what I call ‘Natural History” Prof. Ray Lankester calls 
more precisely ‘‘ Bionomics” ; but it is important to recognise all 


360 The Study of Animal Life APP. 


the three as essential, and to cease from drawing prejudiced com 
parisons between them. 
Their relations may be summarised as follows :— 


“NATURAL HISTORY.” a 
g 
g 
fauna gp 
a 
ares in relation 5 
Study of the pea to g 
real life genus one another a 
of a8 er and to their & 
ea surroundings, ° 
pairs s 
individuals 5 4 . 
EAE 
(5) Organism. a & is 
Ze 16 
(4) Organs. Ee a 
Po = 
(3) Tissues, & 
(2) Cells. 8 
(z) Protoplasm, 5 
i=] 
Study of Structure Study of Activities 5 
(Morphological) (Physiological), 8 
ne 5 
‘‘ ZOOLOGY.” B 


To those interested in ‘‘ Natural History,” there is little need 
to give the primary word of counsel ‘‘ OBSERVE,” for to do so is 
their delight ; nor do they need to be told that sympathetic feeling 
with animals, delight in their harmonious beauty, and poetical 
justice of insight which recognises their personality, are qualities 
of a true naturalist, as every one will allow, except those who are 
given up to the idolatry of that fiction called ‘* pure science.” 

There is a maniacal covetousness of knowledge which one has 
no pleasure in encouraging. We do ot want to know all that is 
contained even in Chambers’s Zxcyclopedia, though we wish to 
gain «the power of understanding, realising, and enjoying the 
various aspects of the world around us. We do xof wish brains 
laden with chemistry and physics, astronomy and geology, botany 


un Some of the “ Best Books” on Animal Life 361 


and zoology, and other sciences, though we would have our eyes 
lightened so that we may see into the heart of things, our brains 
cleared so that we may understand what is known and unknown 
when we are brought naturally in face of problems, and our emotions 
purified so that we may feel more and more fully the joy of life. 
Therefore I would, in the name of education, urge students to 
begin naturally, with what interests them, with the near at hand, 
with the practically important. A circuitous course of study, 
followed with natural eagerness, will lead to better results than the 
most logical of programmes if that take no root in the life of the 
student, 

Let me suggest some of these indirect ways of beginning. 
Begin with domesticated animals and their history. See Darwin’s 
Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication (1868), etc. 
Concentrate your attention on some common animals. See, for 
instance, Darwin’s Formation of Vegetable Mould through the 
action of Worms (1881); Mivart’s Frog (Nature Series, London) ; 
Huxley’s Crayfish (Internat. Sci. Series, London); M‘Cook’s 
North American Spiders (2 vols., Philadelphia, 1889-90); F. 
Cheshire’s Bees and Bee-heeping (vol. i., Lond., 1886) ; Lubbock’s 
Ants, Bees, and Wasps (Internat. Sci. Series, London); Flower’s 
Horse (Lond., 1891). 

Enjoy your seaside holiday. See Charles Kingsley’s Glaucus ; 
J. G. Wood’s Common Objects of the Sea-Shore (1857); P. H. 
Gosse’s Manual of Marine Zoology (1856), and Tenby; G. H. 
Lewes’s Seaside Studies (Edin. 1858); L. Frédéricq, La Lutte pour 
Pexistence chez les Animaux Marins (Paris, 1889). 

Form an aquarium. See J. G. Wood's Fresh and Salt Water 
Aquarium ; P. H. Gosse, The Aquarium (1854), and many similar 
works. 

Begin a naturalist’s year-book. See the Waturalist’s Diary 
by Roberts; the Fied Naturalis’s Handbook, by J. G. and 
Th. Wood (Lond., 1879); and K. Russ, Das hetimische 
Naturleben im Kreislauf des Jahres; Ein Jahrbuch der Natur. 
(Berlin, 1889). 

Observe the animals you see on your country walks. See 
J. G. Wood’s Common Objects of the Country (1858), The Brook 
and its Banks (1889); Life of a Scotch Naturalist, Thomas 
Edward, by Samuel Smiles; Zhe Moor and the Loch, by J. 
Colquhoun (Edin. 1840, 8th ed. 1878) ; Wild Sports and Natural 
History of the Highlands, by Charles St. John (Lond., illust. ed., 
1878); Woodland, Moor, and Stream, edited by J. A. Owen 
(Lond., 1889); W. Marshall, Spazierginge eines Naturforschers 
(Leipzig, 1888); Lloyd Morgan’s Sketches of Animal Life (Lond., 
1892), etc. etc. 


362 The Study of Animal Life APP. 


Another natural way of beginning is to work out some subject 
which attracts you. It becomes a centre round which a crystal 
grows. Muybridge’s photographic demonstrations of animal loco- 
motion have interested us in the flight of birds, let us follow this 
up by observation and by reading, ¢g., Ruskin’s Love's Meinie 
(1881); Pettigrew’s Animal Locomotion (Internat. Sci. Series, 
1873); Marey’s Animal Mechanism (Internat. Sci. Series, 1874); 
Marey’s Le Vol des Oiseaux (Paris, 1890). 

The colours of animals appeal to many people. Read E. B. 
Poulton’s volume (1890) in the Internat. Sci. Series, and Grant 
Allen’s Colour Sense, and F. E. Beddard’s Animal Colouration 
(Lond., 1892). 

The relations between plants and animals are entrancingly 
interesting. Watch the bees and other insects in their flight, 
and read Darwin’s volumes on the Fertilisation of Orchids (1862) 
and on Cross-Fertilisation (1876); Hermann Miiller’s Fertzl¢sation 
of Flowers (transl. by Prof. D’Arcy Thompson, Lond., 1883); 
Kerner’s Flowers and their Unbidden Guests; the articles on 
‘‘Insectivorous Plants,” in Excyclop. Britannica, and in Chambers’s 
Encyclop., or Darwin’s work (1875). 

Again, many of us are directly interested in foreign countries. 
Let the practical interest broaden, it naturally becomes geographical 
and physiographical, and extends to the natural history of the 
region. No more pleasant and sane way of learning about the 
ways and distribution of animals could be suggested than that 
which follows as a gradual extension of physiographical knowledge. 
See Dr. H. R. Mill’s Realm of Nature, and the following samples 
from the long list of books by exploring naturalists :— 


A. Agassiz, Three Cruises of the ‘‘ Blake” (Boston and New York, 
1888). 

Ss. W. Baa Wild Beasts and Ways: Reminiscences of Europe, 
Asia, Africa, and America (London, 1890). 

H, W. Bates, Naturalist on the Amazons (5th ed., London, 
1884). 

T. Belt, tT iota in Nicaragua (2nd ed., London, 1888), 

W. T. Blanford, Observations on Geology and Zoology of Abyssinia 
(Lond., 1870). 

P. B, Du Chaillu, Explorations and Adventures in Equatorial Africa, 
(Lond., 1861); Ashango Land (1867). 

R. O. Cunningham, Notes on the Natural History of the Straits of 
Magellan (Edin., 1871). 

Darwin, Voyage of the ‘' Beagle"’ (1844, new ed. 1890). 

H. Drummond, Tropical Africa (Lond., 1888), 

H. O. Forbes, 4 Naturalist's Wanderings in the Eastern Archi- 
pelago (Lond., 1885). 

Guillemard, Cruise of the ‘‘ Marchesa"’ (Lond., 1886). 


uu Some of the “ Best Books” on Animal Life 363 


A. Heilprin, The Bermuda Islands (Philadelphia, 1889). 

S. J. Hickson, A Naturalist in North Celebes (London, 1889). 

W. H. Hudson, The Naturalist in La Plata (Lond., 1892). 

A. v. Humboldt, Travels to the Equinoctial Regions of America; 
Aspects of Nature (Trans. 1849); Cosmos (Trans. 1849-58). 

Lumholtz, Among Cannibals (Lond., 1889). 

H. N. Moseley, Notes by a Naturalist on the ‘‘ Challenger" (Lond., 
1879, new ed. Lond., 1892). 

A. E. Nordenskiéld, Voyage of the ‘‘ Vega’’ (Lond., 1881). 

F, Oates, ed, by C. G. Oates, Matabele Land and the Victoria 
Spe Naturalists Wanderings in the Interior of S. Africa 
1881). 

N. M. Przewalski, Wéssenschaftliche Resultate der nach Centralasien 
unternommenen Reisen (Leipzig, 1889). 

H, Seebohm, Szberia in Europe (Lond., 1880); and Siberia in Asia 
(1882). 

J. E. Tennent, Natural History of Ceylon (Lond., 1861). 

Wyville Thomson, Zhe Depths of the Sea (Lond., 1873); Narrative of 
the Voyage of the ‘‘ Challenger’ (1885). Cf. A. de Folin, Sous 
les Mers (Paris, 1887); H. Filhol, La Vie au fond des Mers 
(Paris, 1886); W. Marshall, Die Ziefsee und thr Leben (Leipzig, 
1888). 

Pra Flora and Fauna of Palestine. 

Tschudi, Thierleben der Alpenwelt. 

A. R. Wallace, Malay Archipelago (Lond., 1869); Tropical Nature 
(1878) ; Zsland Life (1880). 

Ch. Waterton, Wanderings in South America (ed. by J. G. Wood, 
1878). 

C. M. Woodford, Naturalist among the Head-hunters (London, 
1890). 


Prominent among those who have helped many to realise the 
marvel and beauty of nature, a widely-felt gratitude ranks Gilbert 
White, Henry Thoreau, Charles Kingsley, Richard Jefferies, J. G. 
Wood, John Ruskin, and John Burroughs. 


GILBERT WHITE (1720-1793) is known to all as the author of 
The Natural History and Antiquities of Selborne, in the County of 
Southampton (1788), a book consisting of a series of letters addressed 
to a few friends. A good edition is that by J. E. Harting (6th ed., 
London, 1888), but there is a cheaper one, edited by Richard 
Jefferies, in the Camelot Series, 


Henry THorEAv (1817-1862), the author of Walden, A Week 
on Concord, and other much-loved books. 


CHARLES KINGSLEY (1819-1875). See his Glaucus (Lond., 
1854); Water-Babies ; and popular lectures. 


364 The Study of Animal Life APP. 


RICHARD JEFFERIES (1848-1887). 


See The Eulogy of Richard Jefferies, by Walter Besant (London, 
1888), and the following works, some of which are published in 
cheap editions: Zhe Gamekeeper at Home (1878); Wild Life 
in a Southern County (1879); The Amateur Poacher (1880) ; 
Round about a Great Estate (1881); Nature near London 
(1883); Life of the Fields (1884); Red Deer (1884); The Open 
Air (1885). 


J. G. Woop, whom we have lately lost, has done more than 
any other to popularise natural history in Britain. 


See Life of J. G. Wood, by his son, Theodore Wood (Lond., 1890) ; 
My Feathered Friends (1856); Common Objects of the Seashore 
(1857); Common Objects of the Country (1858); his large 
Natural History (1859-63) ; Glimpses into Petland (1862) ; Homes 
without Hands (1864); The Dominion of Man (1887); and other 
works, 


Joun Ruskin. See the Zagle’s Nest, Queen of the Air, Love's 
Meinie, Proserpina, Deucalion, and Ethics of the Dust. 
JoHN BuRROUGHS. 


See the neat shilling editions of Wake Robin (1871), Winter Sun- 
shine (1875), Birds and Poets (1877), Locusts and Wild Honey 
(1879), Pepacton (1881), Fresh Fields (1884), Signs and Seasons 
(1886). 


See also :— 


GranT ALLEN, Zhe Evolutionist at Large; Vignettes from 
Nature, etc. 


FRANK BUCKLAND, Curiosities of Natural History (London, 
1872-77), and his Life. 


P. H. Gossz. Romance of Natural History (London, 1860-61). 


P. G. HAMERTON, Chapters on Animals; The Sylvan Year 
(3rd ed., London, 1883). 


W. Krirpy AND W. SPENCE, Jntroduction to Entomology 
(London, 1815). 


F, A. Knicut, By Leafy Ways ; Idylls of the Field (London, 
1889). 


Putt Rosinson, Zhe Poet’s Birds (London, 1883); and The 
Poet's Beasts (London, 1885). 


ANDREW WILSON, Leaves from «u Naturalist’s Note-Books ; 
Chapters on Evolution, etc. 


u Some of the “ Best Books” on Animal Life 365 


C. Biology. 


Having offered counsel to those who would study the literature 
of Zoology and of Natural History, I shall complete my task of 
giving advice by addressing those who are strong enough to 
inquire into the nature, continuance, and progress of life. It is 
to students of mature years that this ‘‘biological” study is most 
natural, for young folks should be left to see and enjoy as much 
as possible, till theories grow in them as naturally as ‘‘ wisdom 
teeth.” This also should be noted in regard to the study of 
evolution and the related problems of biology, that though all the 
generalisations reached must be based on the research and observa- 
tion of zoologists, botanists, and naturalists, and are seldom fully 
appreciated by those who have little personal acquaintance with 
the facts, yet sound and useful conclusions may be, and often are, 
obtained by those who have had no discipline in concrete scientific 
work, 

Besides the general question of organic evolution there are 
special subjects which the student of biology must learn to think 
about: Protoplasm, or ‘‘the physical basis of life;” Repro- 
duction, Sex, and Heredity, or ‘‘the continuance of life;” and 
Animal Intelligence, or ‘‘the growth of mind.” Before passing 
to the literature on these subjects, it may be noted that there are 
two general works of pioneering importance, namely, Herbert 
Spencer’s Principles of Biology (2 vols., Lond., 1864-66), and 
Exnst Haeckel’s Generelle Morphologie (2 vols., Berlin, 1866). 

Protoplasm.—Of this the student should learn how little we 

-know. Yet this is not very easy, since the most important recent 
contributions, such as those of Professors Hering and Gaskell, are 
inaccessible to most. The gist of the matter, however, may be 
got hold of by reading: (a) three articles in the Zycyclopedia 
Britannica, ‘** Physiology” (Prof. M. Foster), ‘ Protoplasm” 
(Prof. P. Geddes), and ‘* Protozoa ””—the large type—(Prof. E. Ray 
Lankester) ; (4) the Presidential Address to the Biological Section 
of the British Association, 1889, by Prof. Burdon Sanderson 
(Nature, xl., September 1889, pp. 521-526); and (c) the article 
“¢ Protoplasm” in the new edition of Chambers’s Aucyclopedia. 
Of the abundant literature on the philosophical questions which 
the scientific conception of living matter raises, I shall mention 
Huxley’s address on ‘‘The Physical Basis of Life,” published 
among his collected essays; Hutchison Stirling’s tract, ‘‘As 
regards Protoplasm ;” the chapter on ‘‘Vitalism” in Bunge’s 
Physiological Chemistry (translated, London, 1890). 

Reproduction, Sex, and Heredity.—For adult students, 
and no others should be encouraged to face the responsibility of 


366 The Study of Animal Life APP. 


inquiry into such matters, the most convenient introduction will 
be found in The Evolution of Sex (Contemporary Science Series, 
Lond., 1889), by Prof. Geddes and myself. In that work there 
are references to others, A survey of modern opinions and con- 
clusions in regard to heredity may be obtained from the article in 
Chambers’s Encyclopedia, whence the student will pass unbiassed 
to the essays of Weismann, Pagers on Heredity and Kindred 
Suljects (translated by E. B. Poulton, S. Schénland, and A. E. 
Shipley, Oxford, 1889), to the works of Francis Galton, especially 
his Natural Inheritance (Lond., 1889), and to other important 
books mentioned in the article referred to. 

Animal Intelligence.—A recent work by Professor C. Lloyd 
Morgan, Animal Life and Intelligence (Lond., 1890), supplies the 
best introduction to those interesting questions in the discussion of 
which the biologist becomes a psychologist. The most reliable 
treasury of facts is certainly G. J. Romanes’s Animal Intelligence 
(Internat. Sci. Series, 4th ed., Lond., 1886), to which may 
be added Couch’s ///ustrations of Instinct (1847), Lauder Lindsay’s 
Mind in Animals (1879), Biichner’s Aus dem Getstesleben der Thiere 
(2nd ed., Berlin, 1877) and Liebe und Liebesleben in der Thierwelt 
(Berlin, 1879) ; Max Perty, Ueber das Seelenleben der Thiere (Leipzig, 
1876); Houzeau, Des Facultés mentales des Animaux (Brussels, 
no Of unique value is the work of A. Espinas, Des Sociétés 

Animales, Etude de Psychologie comparée (Paris, 1877). See also 
P. Girod, Les Sociétés chez les animaux (Paris, 1890). I should 
also mention that Brehm’s 7%derleben (1863-69), a great work now 
in process of re-edition (10 vols., Leipzig), is a marvellous 
treasury of information in regard to the ways and wisdom of 
animals, and that we have in Verworn’s Psycho-Phystologische 
Protisten Studien (Jena, 1889) a very interesting and important 
study of the dawn of an inner life in the simplest animals or 
Protozoa. Of the ingenious work of animals, an admirably terse 
description is given in F. Houssay’s Les Industries des Animaux 
(Paris, 1889). For theories of instinct, see especially Romanes, 
Mental Evolution in Animals (Lond., 1883); Darwin, Origin of 
Species; Wallace, Contributions to the Theory of Natural Selection ; 
Spencer, Principles of Psychology and Principles of Biology; G. H. 
Lewes, Problems of Life and Mind (Lond., 1874-79); Samuel 
Butler, Life and Habit (Lond., 1878); J. J. Murphy, Hadet and 
Intelligence; E. von Hartmann, Das Undewusste vom Stand- 
’ punkte der Physiologie und Descendenztheorie (2nd ed., Berlin, 
1877); Schneider, Der Thierische Wille (Leipzig, 1880) ; Eimer, 
Organic Evolution; Weismann, Pagers on Heredity. 
The Fact of Organic Evolution.—The student’s first task 
in regard to Evolution is to make himself acquainted with the 


1 Some of the “ Best Books” on Animal Life 367 


arguments which show that the animals and plants now alive are 
descended from simpler ancestors, these from still simpler, and 
so on back into the mists of life’s beginnings. To realise that the 
present is child of the past is to realise the fact of Evolution, and 
the surest way to grasp the biological verification of this fact is to 
undertake a course of practical study. Failing this, we must, I 
suppose, read up the subject. Romanes’s Avidences of Evolution 
(Nature Series, Lond.) gives a convenient statement of the case, 
and his Rosebery Lectures will be more exhaustive. Clodd’s 
Story of Creation: a plain account of Evolution (Lond., 1888) 
sums up the evidence in small compass; another very terse state- 
ment will be found in H. De Varigny’s Experimental Evolution 
(Lond., 1892); Haeckel’s Natural History of Creation (Berlin, 
1868)—the most popular of his works, now in its eighth edition 
(Jena, 1890)—is available in translation (Lond., 1879); Huxley’s 
American Addresses (Lond., 1877) have even greater charm of 
style; Carus Sterne’s Werden und Vergehen (3rd ed., Berlin, 
1886) is perhaps the best of all popular expositions; while the 
thorough student will find most satisfaction in the relevant 
portions of Darwin’s Origin of Species, and Spencer’s Principles 
of Biology. 

History of Evolution Theories.—As the idea of Evolution 
is very ancient, and as it was expounded in relation to animal life 
by many competent naturalists before Darwin’s intellectual coin 
became current throughout the world, it is unwise that students 
should restrict their reading to Darwinian and post-Darwinian 
literature. The student of Evolution should know how Buffon, 
Erasmus Darwin, Lamarck, Treviranus, the St. Hilaires, Goethe, 
even Robert Chambers, and many other pre-Darwinians dealt with 
the problem. Those who desire to preserve their sense of historical 
justice should read one or more of the following : Huxley’s article 
on “Evolution” in the Zxcyclopedia Britannica; Samuel Butler’s 
interesting volume on Evolution Old and New (Lond., 1879); 
Perrier’s Philosophie Zoologique avant Darwin (Paris, 1884); the 
historical chapters of Haeckel’s Matural History of Creation ; 
Carus’s Geschichte der Zoologie, and some other historical works 
already referred to (p. 355); A. de Candolle’s Histoire des 
Sciences e des Savants dépuis deux Siecles (Gentve, Bale, 1883) ; 
Carus Sterne’s (Ernst Krause’s) excellent work, Dze Al/gemeine 
Weltanschauung (Stuttgart, 1889); De Quatrefages, Charles 
Darwin et ses précurseurs francais (Paris, 1870). 

Darwinism.—The best account of the Darwinian theory of 
Evolution, especially of the theory of natural selection which 
Charles Darwin and Alfred Russel Wallace independently elabo- 
rated, is Wallace’s Darwinism (Lond., 1889). From this the 


368 The Study of Animal Life APP. 


student will naturally pass to the works of Darwin himself—7e 
Origin of Species by means of Natural Selection; or, the Pre- 
servation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life (Lond., 
1859); Zhe Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication 
(2 vols., Lond., 1868); Zhe Descent of Man, and Selection in 
Relation to Sex (Lond., 1871), etc. ; the earlier works of Wallace, 
especially his Contributions to the Theory of Natural Selection 
(Lond., 1871); Spencer’s Principles of Biology—cf. his articles 
on ‘The Factors of Organic Evolution” (Mineteenth Century, 
1886); Haeckel’s Generelle Morphologie, and Natural History of 
Creation. As a popular account of Darwin’s life and work, Grant 
Allen’s Charles Darwin (English Worthies Series, 3rd ed., Lond., 
1886) has a deserved popularity; G. T. Bettany’s similar work 
(Great Writers Series, Lond., 1886) has a very valuable biblio- 
graphy; but for full personal and historical details reference must 
be made to the Life and Letters of Charles Darwin, by his son 
Francis Darwin (3 vols., Lond., 1887). 

Recent Contributions to the Theory of Evolution.— 
At the present time there is much discussion in regard to the 
factors of organic Evolution, The theory of Evolution is still 
being evolved; there is a struggle between opinions. On the 
one hand, many naturalists are more Darwinian than Darwin was, 
—that is to say, they lay more exclusive emphasis upon the theory 
of natural selection; on the other hand, not a few are less Darwinian 
than Darwin was, and emphasise factors of Evolution and aspects 
of Evolution which Darwin regarded as of minor importance. 

Of those who are more Darwinian than Darwin, I may cite as 
representative: Alfred Russel Wallace who, in his Darwexism, 
subjects Darwin’s subsidiary theory of sexual selection to destructive 
criticism ; August Weismann who, in his Essays on Heredity, 
denies the transmissibility of characters acquired by the individual 
organism, as the results of use or disuse or of external influence ; 
and E. Ray Lankester, see his article ‘‘ Zoology” in the Excycio- 
padia Britannica, and his work on the Advancement of Science 
(Lond., 1890). The student should also read an article by Prof. 
Huxley, ‘‘The Struggle for Existence, and its Bearing upon Man” 
in the Wineteenth Century, Feb. 1888. 


See also :— 

Samuel Butler, Evolution Old and New (Lond., 1879), Luck or 
Cunning (Lond., 1887), and other works. 

Prof. E. D. Cope, Origin of the Fittest (New York, 1887). 

Prof. G. H. T. Eimer, Organic Evolution, as the Result of the 
Inheritance of Acquired Characters, according to the Laws of 
Organic Growth (Jena, 1888), Trans. by J. T. Cunningham 
(Lond, 1890). 


tt Some of the“ Best Looks” on Animal Life 369 


Prof. T. Fiske, Outlines of Cosmic Philosophy (Lond., 1874), Dar- 
winism, and other Essays (Lond., 1875). 

Prof. P. Geddes, Article ‘‘ Variation and Selection,” Lxcyclopedia 
Britannica ; ‘' Evolution,” Chambers's Encyclopedia, new ed. 
Cf. The Evolution of Sex, and forthcoming work on Evolution, 
Organic and Social, 

E. Gilou, La Lutte pour le Bien-étre (1890). 

Rev. J. T. Gulick, Divergent Evolution, through Cumulative Segre- 
gation (Journ, Linn. Soc. xx., 1888). 

P. Kropotkine, ‘‘ Mutual Aid among Animals,"" Nineteenth Century 
(Sept. and Nov. 1890). 

Lanessan, La Lutte pour l' Existence et [ Association pour la Lutte 
(Paris, 1882). 

Prof, St. George Mivart, Zhe Genesis of Species (Lond., 1871), 
Lessons from Nature (Lond., 1876), Ox Truth (Lond., 1889). 

Prof. C. Lloyd Morgan, Animal Life and Intelligence (Lond., 
1890). : : 

Prof. C. V. Néageli, Mechanisch -physiologische Abstammungslehre 
(Miinchen and Leipzig, 1884). 

Prof. A. S. Packard, Introduction to the Standard or Riverside 
Natural History (New York and Lond., 1885). 

Dr. G. J. Romanes, Physiological Selection (Journ. Linn. Soc. xix., 
1886), and forthcoming Rosebery Lectures on the Philosophy 
of Natural History. 

Prof. K. Semper, Zhe Natural Conditions of Existence as they affect 
Animal Life (Internat. Sci. Series, Lond., 1881). 

Dr. J. B. Sutton, Ax Introduction to General Pathology (Lond., 
1886). Evolution and Disease (Contempor. Sci. Series, Lond., 
1890). 


iQ 
pe 


INDEX 


ABSORPTION, 145 
Acacias guarded by ants, 29, 30 
Acquired characters, 329-336 
Actions, automatic, 155 
habitual, 155 
innate, 155 
intelligent, 155 
Alternation of generations, 189 
Ameeba, 213 
Amphibians, 9, 256, 257 
parental care among, I10, III 
Amphioxus, 252 
Angler-fish, 118 
Animalculists, 191 
Animals, everyday life of, 1-124 
domestic life of, 95, 116 
industries of, 117-124 
life-history of, 184-203 
past history of, 204-209 
social life of, 67-94 
and plants, resemblances and 
contrasts, 167-171 
relation of simplest to more 
complex, 171-174 
Annelids, 231-234 
Antlers, 279 
Ants, 78-84 
and aphides, 119, 120 
and plants, 29 
Aphides, 82, 312 
multiplication of, 38 
Arachnida, 243 
Archoplasm, 183 


Aristotle, 283, 284 
Armour of animals, 34, 35 
Artemia, 310, 311 
Arthropods, 10, 238 
Atavism, 322 

Autotomy, 64-66 

Axolotl, 309 


BACKBONED animals, 9, 222-247 
Backboneless animals, 9, 10, 248- 
272 

Bacteria, 21, 22 
Balance of nature, 19-21 
Balanoglossus, 9, 249, 250 
Bathybius, 219 
Beauty of animals, 15-17 
Beavers, 25, 74, 75 
Bees, 78-84 
Biology, justification of, 34-50 
Birds, 9, 264-267 

parental care among, 114, 115 
Blind animals, 305 
Body, functions of, 144-149 

parts of, 174-183 
Books, 351-369 
Boring animals, 25 
Bower birds, 98 
Brachiopoda, 235 
Brine-shrimp, 310, 311 
Buffon, 286 


CADDIS worms, 61 
Carbohydrates, 134 


372 


Caterpillars, 50, 51 

Cats and clover, 29 

Cave-animals, 334 

Cell-division, 158-183 

Cells, 128, 147, 179-183 

Centipedes, 241 

Cestoda, 229 

Chzetopoda, 231-233 

Challenger Expedition, 5, 6 

Chameleons, 52 

Chemical elements, 135 
influences in environment, 309, 

313 

Circulation, 146 

Classification of animals, 8-11 

Ccelenterates, 222-228 

Cold, effect of, 313 

Colonies, 70, 71 

Colour-change, 52, 53 

‘Colouring, protective, 48, 49 
variable, 49-51 

Colours of animals, 49-53 
of flat-fishes, 315 

Commensalism, 68, 69 

Competition, internal, 67 

Concealment of animals, 47 

Conjugation, 214 

Consciousness, 150-152 

Co-operation, 69 

Corals, 26, 27, 227 

Coral snakes, 59 

Courtship of birds, 96 
mammals, 96 
spiders, IoI-105 

Crabs, masking of, 61, 62 
and sea-anemones, 68, 69 

Cranes, gregarious life of, 73 

Crayfish, 25 

Crocodilians, 263, 264 

Cruelty of nature, 43-45 

Crustacea, 239, 240 
life-history of, 198-202 

Cuckoo, 114, 115 

Cuttlefish, 52, 66 

Cyclostomata, 252 


Darwin, Charles, 292-296 
Erasmus, 288, 289 


The Study of Animal Life 


Deep-sea fishes, 256 

life, 6 
Descent of man, 341-345 
Desiccation, 41-43 
Digestion, 145 
Distribution of animals, 3-8 
Disuse, results of, 305, 306 
Division of labour, 69-71, 143, 

144 

Dormant life, 41-43 
Drought, effect of, 41-43 


EARTHWORMS, 22-24 

Echinoderms, 10, 65, 66, 235-238 

Ectoderm, 196 

Eggs, 191, 192 

Elaps, 59 

Elephant hawk-moth, 59 

Encystation, 41 

Endoderm, 196 

Environment, 306-319 

Ephemerides, 106, 107 

Epiblast, 196 

Epigenesis, 324 

Evolution, evidences of, 273-281 
factors of, 299-302 
theories, history of, 282-301 
of sex, 188 

Extinct types, 206, 207 


FAMILY, evolution of, 91 
life, 9 

Fats, 134 

Feigning death, 66 

Fertilisation, 193-195 

Filial regression, 338 

Fishes, 9, 253-256 
parental care among, I09, 110 

Flight of birds, 123, 124 

Flowers and insects, 28, 29 

Flukes, 229 

Food, influence of, 310-313 

Freshwater fauna, 6-8 

Friar-birds, 59 

Frog, 258 

Function, influence of, 303 


GASTRAA theory, 197 


Gastrula, 195, 196 
Genealogical tree, 12, 13 


Geological record, imperfection of, 


205 
Germ-plasma, ‘328 
Giant reptiles, 259 
Glow-worm, courtship of, 100 
Gregarines, 211 
Gregarious animals, 71-74 
Grouse attacked by weasel, qo 


Hasitat, change of, 47 
Habitual actions, 155 
Haeckel, 298 

Hagfish, 253 

Halcyon, 116 

Hatteria, 260 

Heat, influence of, 313 
Heredity, 320-339 
Hermaphroditism, 188 
Hermit-crabs, masking of, 63 
Hirudinea, 234 

Homes, making of, 121-123 
Hornbill, brooding of, 114 
Horse, pedigree of, 278 
Hunting, 118, 119 

Huxley, 298 

Hydractinia, 69, 70 
Hypoblast, 196 


ICHNEUMON flies, 64 
Idealism, 142 
Impressions; 151 
Industries of animals, 116-124 
Infusorians, 211 
maultiplication of, 38 
Innate actions, 155 
Insects, 241-243 
parental care of, 108 
and flowers, 28 
Instinct, 153-166 
origin of, 163-166 
Instincts defined, 11 
incomplete, 158 
mixed, 163 
primary, 163 
secondary, 163 
Insulation of animals, 46 


Index 


Intelligence, lapse of, 166 
Intelligent actions, 155 
Iron, importance of, 19 
Isolation, 300, 301 
Ivory, 3 


JELLYFISH, 226 


KALLIMA, 53 
Kidneys, work of, 145 


LAMARCK, 289-292 
Lamprey, 252 
Lancelet, 9, 252 
Land animals, 8 
Leaf insects, 54 
Leeches, 234 
Lemming, Ross's, 50 
Lemurs, 46 


137 
energy of, 127 
haunts of, 3-8 
machinery of, 130, 131 
origin of, 140-142, 280 
struggle of, 32-45 
variety of, 3 
wealth of, 1-17 


Liver, work of, 145 
Living matter, 131-135 
Lizards, 260 

Love of mates, 90, 91, 96 


and death, 106 
Luciola, courtship of, 100 
Lucretius, 284, 285 


Mammals, 9, 267-271 

Man as a social person, 94 
considered zoologically, 

346 

Marine life, 3-6 

Marsupials, 46 

Masking, 61-63 

Materialism, 141, 142 


Light, influence of, 315, 316 


373 


Life, chemical elements of, 135- 


and care for offspring, 105-116 


MACROPOD, parental care of, 110 


340- 


‘ 


374 


Mates, love of, 90, g1, 96 

Mayflies, 106, 107 

Metamorphosis of Insects, 243 

Mesoderm or mesoblast, 196 

Migration of birds, 74 

Millepedes, 241 

Mimicry, 57-61 

Mites, desiccation of, 41, 42 

Molluses, 10, 243-247 

Monkeys, 270, 271, 341 
gregarious life of, 71 

Monogamous mammals, 96 

Moss-insect, 55 

Moulting, 315 

Movement, 144 

Movements of animals, 123, 124 

Mud-fish, 8 

Mygale, 36 

Myriapoda, 241 


NATURAL selection, 295 
Nematoda, 231 
Nemerteans, 230 

Nervous system, 148 
Nudibranchs, 56 

Number of animals, 14, 15 
Nutrition, 144 

Nutritive relations, 27, 28 


Opours and sexual attraction, 
105 
Offspring, care for, 105-116 
Ontogeny, 203 
Ooze, 220 
Organic continuity, 203, 326-329 
Organs, 175 
change of function of, 178 
classification of, 178 
correlation of, 176 
order of appearance of, 175, 
176 
rudimentary, 178, 179 
substitution of, 178 
Orioles, 59 
Ovists, 191 
Ovum, 191-193 
theory, 196 
Oysters, mortality of, 43 


The Study of Animal Life 


PALAONTOLOGICAL series, 206 
Paleontology, 204-209 
Pangenesis, 324 
Parasitic worms, 229-231 
Parasitism, 47, 48 
Parthenogenesis, 189-193 
Partnerships among animals, 68, 
69 
Perception, 151 
Peripatus, 10, 240 
Phasmidee, 53 
Phenacodus, 269, 270 
Phyllopteryx, 54 
Phylogeny, 203 
Physiology, 125-152 
Pigeon, 275, 276 
Pineal body, 260 
Plants and animals, 19, 20, 28-31, 
168-171 
Polar globules, 193 
Polyzoa, 235 
Preformation theories, 191, 324 
Pressures, effect of, 300 
Protective resemblance, 53 
Proteids, 134, 135 
Protomyxa, 212 
Protoplasm, 131-135 
Protopterus, 41 
Protozoa, II, 210-221 
colonial, 173, 174 
classes of, 211 
life of, 214 
‘‘immortality’ of, 172 
psychical life of, 215-218 
structure of, 213 
and Metazoa, transition between, 
88, 89, 171-174 
Psychology, 149 
Pupee of caterpillars, 50 
Puss-moth, 63, 64 
RADIANT energy, influence of, 
313-316 
Recapitulation, 197, 279 
Reflex actions, 155 
Reproduction, 184-190 
Reptiles, 9, 259-264 
Respiration, 146 


Index 


Reversion, 322 
Rhizopods, 212 

Rotifers, 7, 42, 234 
Round-mouths, 9, 252 
Rudimentary organs, 277 


SACCOPHORA, 62 

Sacculina, 48 

Sea-horse, parental care of, 110 

Seasonal dimorphism, 314 

Segmentation, 195 

Sensations, 151 

Sex, 96 

Sexual reproduction, 186-188 
selection, 98 

Shells of molluscs, 243 

Shepherding, rrg9, .120 

Shifts for a living, 46-66 

Skunk, 55 

Snails and plants, 30 

Snakes, 260-263 

Social inheritance, 337-339 
life of animals, 67-94 
organism, 93, 94 

Societies, evolution of, 87 

Song of birds, 96 

Spencer, 297, 298 

Spermatozoon, 192, 193 

Sphex, 121 

Spiders, courtship of, rot-105 
bird-catching, 36 

Sponges, 11, 222 

Spongilla, 186 

Spring, biology of, 95, 96 

Starfish, 235 


Stickleback, courtship of, 99 
parental care of, 109, £10, 122 

Stinging-animals, 11, 223-228 

Storing, 120, 121 

Struggle for existence, 32-45 

Surrender of parts, 64-66 

Symbiosis, 69 


TAPEWORMS, 229 
Termites, 24, 84-87 
Tissues, 179, 180 
Tortoises, 263 
Toxotes, 118 
Trematoda, 229 
Tunicates, 9, 250, 251 
Turbellaria, 228 


VARIATION, 299 
Vertebrata, characters of, 19, 248, 


249 
Vital force, 19 
Vivarium, 20, 21 
Volvox, 187 


WALLACE, 296, 297 
Warning colours, 55, 56 
Weapons of animals, 34 
Web of Life, 18-31 

White Ants. See Termites 
Worms, 10, 11, 228-235 


YOLK, 195 


ZooLocy, history of, 352-357 


THE END 


Printed by R. & R. Ciark, LimiteD, Edinburgh, 


375 


THE UNIVERSITY SERIES 


EDITED BY PROFESSOR WM. KNIGHT 


CHARLES SCRIBNER’S SONS, Publishers 


A SERIES of volumes dealing with separate sec- 

tions of Literature, Science, Philosophy, History 
and Art, and designed to supply the need so widely 
felt of authorized books for study and reference both 
by students and by the general public. 


VOLUMES IN PREPARATION 


AN INTRODUCTION TO PHYSICAL SCIENCE 
By Joun Cox, Professor of Physics, McGill College. 


THE ENGLISH POETS 


From Blake to Tennyson. By Rev. SToprorD A. BROOKE, 
Trinity College, Dublin. 


\ THE HISTORY OF ASTRONOMY 
By ArTHUR Berry, Fellow of King’s College, Cambridge. 


A HISTORY OF EDUCATION 


By JAmMEs DoNnALDsON, University of St. Andrew. 


AN INTRODUCTION TO PHILOSOPHY 
By Prof. KnicHT. 


ELEMENTS OF PSYCHOLOGY 


By GrorcE Croom ROBERTSON, late Greek Professor, Uni- 
versity College, London. Edited from Notes of the 
Lectures delivered at the College, 1870-1892. By C. A. 
Foley Rhys-Davids. (University Series.) 12mo, $1.00 
jel. 


ELEMENTS OF GENERAL PHILOSOPHY 


By the same author. Edited from Notes of the Lectures de- 
livered at the College, 1870-1892 By‘C. A. Foley Rhys- 
Davids. (University Series.) Izmo. $1.25 net. 


These two volumes have been compiled from notes taken by the stu- 
dents of Dr. Robertson, and in a way are a memorial to that great teacher. 
At the same time they take their place in the University Series by reason 
of the exceptional educational value they have, and will be found to afford 
not merely an introduction to psychology and also to philosophy, but an 
introduction to philosophy by way of psychology. No othertwo manuals 
so adapted have yet appeared. 


2 THE UNIVERSITY SERIES 


OUTLINES OF ENGLISH LITERATURE 


By Witiiam REnTon, Lecturer to the Scottish Uni- 
versities. 12mo, with Diagrams, $1.00 met. 


ConTEnTs: First Period [600-1600], pages g-112: I. The 
Old English Metric and Chronicle [600-1350], 2. Anglo- 
Saxon; 4. Anglo-Norman—II. The Renascence [1350-1500] 
—III. The Reformation [1550-1600]—IV. The Romantic 
Drama [1550-1650]. Second Period [1600-1900], pages 
132-232—V. The Serious Age [1600-1700]—VI. The Age of 
Gaiety [1650-1750]—VII. The Sententious Age [1700-1800]— 
VIII. The Sympathetic Age [1800-1900]—Appendix: Litera- 
ture of America [1600-1900]—Index: Conspectus of British 
and American Poetry. 

The general arrangement of the book and valuable diagrams showing 
the division of literature according to ages and characteristics combine to 
make this manual especially fitted to use in the class-room. 

Criticism is supplemented by exposition, with extracts to exhibit the 
fashion of a period, or the style of a master. The number of authors 
indicates the importance of a period, and intrinsic power the importance 
of an author. American literature is considered as a part of the whole, 
but a brief summary of its history and general characteristics is also given. 


THE PHILOSOPHY OF THE BEAUTIFUL 


By Wittiam Knicut, Professor of Philosophy in the 
University of St. Andrews. In two parts. 1r2mo, 
each $1.00 met. 


(Part I. Irs History.) Contents: Introductory—Pre- 
historic Origins—Oriental Art and Speculation—The Phil- 
osophy of Greece—The Neoplatonists—The Graco-Roman 
Period—Medizvalism—The Philosophy of Germany—of 
France—of Italy—of Holland—of Britain—of America. 


(Part II. Irs THEORY AND ITs RELATION TO THE ARTS.) 
ConTENTs: I. Prolegomena—II. The Nature of Beauty—III. 
The Ideal and the Real—IV. Inadequate or Partial Theories 
of Beauty—V. Suggestions towards a more Complete Theory 
of Beauty—VI. Art, Its Nature and Functions—VII. The 
Correlation of the Arts—VIII. Poetry, 2. Definitions and 

- Distinctions ; 4. Theories of Poetry ; c. A Suggestion ; d. The 
Origin of Poetry—IX. Music, a. Its Nature and Essence ; 4. 
The Alliance of Music with Poetry and the other Arts; ¢. 
The Origin of Music—X. Architecture—XI, Sculpture—XII. 
Painting—XIII. Dancing—Appendix A: Russian Aesthetic 
—Appendix B: Danish Aesthetic. 


THE UNIVERSITY SERIES 3 


THE USE AND ABUSE OF MONEY 


By Dr. W. CunnincHam, Cambridge. 12mo, $1.00 mez, 


A popular treatise, and the headings, Social Problems, Practical Ques- 
tions, and Personal Duty, give a broad view of the scope of the book. 
The subject is Capital in its relation to Social Progress, and personal re- 
sponsibility enters into the questions raised. The volume contains a syl- 
labus of subjects and a list of buoks for reference. 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SENSES 


By Joun McKenprick, Professor of Physiology in 
the University of Glasgow, and Dr. Snopcrass, 
Physiological Laboratory, Glasgow. 127 Illustra- 
tions, 12mo, 340 pages, $1.50 nef. 


The aim of this book is to give an account of the functions of the 
organs of sense as found in man and the higher animals. Simple experi- 
ments are suggested by which any one may test the statements for him- 
self, and the Book has been so written as to be readily understood by 
those who have not made physiology a special study. It will be found a 
suitable Dieperatoe for entering upon the questions that underlie physio- 
logical psychology. Excellent illustrations abound. 


~ 


ENGLISH COLONIZATION AND EMPIRE 


By ALrrep CaLpEcoTT, St. John’s College, Cam- 
bridge. 12mo, with Maps and Diagrams, $1.00 zez. 


The diffusion of European, and more particularly, of English, civiliza- 
tion is the subject of this book. The treatment of this great theme covers 
the origin and the historical, political, economical and ethnological devel- 
opment of the English colonies. There is thus spread before the reader a 
bird’s-eye view of the colonies, great and small, from their origin until the 
age time, with a summary of the wars and other great events which 

ave occurred in the progress of this colonizing work, and with a careful 
examination of some of the most ayes eet questions, economical, com- 
mercial, and political, which now affect the relation of the colonies and 
the parent nation. 


THE JACOBEAN POETS 


By Epmunp Gossr, Hon. M.A., Trinity College, 
Cambridge. 12mo, $1.00 net. 


This little volume is an attempt to direct critical attention to all that 
was notable in English poetry from 1603-1625. It is the first book to con- 
centrate attention on the poetry produced during the reign of James I. 
Many writers appear here for the first time ina book of this nature. The 
aim has been to find unfamiliar beauties rather than to reprint for the 
thousandth time what is already familiar, 


4 THE UNIVERSITY SERIES 


THE FINE ARTS 


By G. BaLpwin Brown, Professor of Fine Arts in the 
University of Edinburgh. r2mo, with Illustrations, 
$1.00 net. 


ConTENTS: Part I—Art as the Expression of Popular 
Feelings and Ideals:—The Beginnings of Art—The Festival 
in its Relation to the Form and Spirit of Classical Art— 
Medieval Florence and her Painters. Part IIL.—The Formal 
Conditions of Artistic Expression:—Some Elements of Effect 
in the Arts of Form—The Work of Art as Significant—The 
Work of Art as Beautiful. Part II1.—The Arts of Form:— 
Architectural Beauty in Relation to Construction—The Con- 
ventions of Sculpture—Painting Old and New. 


YALE ART SCHOOL, NEW HAVEN, CONN. 
Messrs. CHARLES SCRIBNER’S SONS, 

Gentlemen:—As a text-book for the study of the ‘Fine Arts, there 
is nothing in the literature of the subject that answers the requirements as 
this little book. ; 

The originality of Professor Brown’s work is apparent. Outofa wide 
familiarity with the classical literature of the subject he has sifted the essen- 
tialtruths. And of the modern writers on esthetics he knows and digests 
everything from Winkelmann to Whistler. But what distinguishes this 
book from others and gives it a special value is the treatment of the ‘‘Fine 
Arts” from their technical side. This is especially evident in his chapter 
on Pane which contains many suggestions of value to the young artist 
an 


amateur. 
Respectfully yours, JOHN H. NIEMEYER. 


THE LITERATURE OF FRANCE 


By H. G. Krenz, Hon. M.A. Oxon. 12mo, $1.00 
net. 


ConTENTS: Introduction—The Age of Infancy (2. Birth) 
—The Age of Infancy (4. Growth)—The Age of Adolescence 
(Sixteenth Century) —The Age of Glory, Part I. Poetry, etc. 
—The Age of Glory, Part II. Prose—The Age of Reason, 
Part I.—The Age of Reason, Part II1.—The Age of ‘‘ Nature” 
—Sources of Modern French Literary Art: Poetry—Sources 
of Prose Fiction—Appendix—Index. 


Epwarp S. Jovnes, Professor of Modern Languages, South Caro- 
tina College.—‘' My first impressions are fully confirmed. The book is 
interesting and able. It would be difficult to compress into equa com- 

ass a more satisfactory or suggestive view of so greata subject. Asan 
introductory text for schools and colleges or private readers, I have seen 
notice so good. The book deserves, and I hope will receive,a wide 
welcome,’ 


THE UNIVERSITY SERIES 5 


THE REALM OF NATURE 


An Outline of Physiography. By Hucu Ropert 
Mit, D.Sc. Edin.; Fellow of the Royal Society 
of Edinburgh: Oxford Lecturer. Maps and 68 
Illustrations. 12mo, $1.50 xet. 


ConTENTsS :—Story of Nature—Substance of Nature— 
Power of Nature—The Earth a Spinning Ball—The Earth a 
Planet—The Solar System and Universe—The Atmosphere 
—Atmospheric Phenomena—Climates—The Hydrosphere— 
Bed of the Oceans—Crust of the Earth—Action of Water on 
Land—Record of the Rocks—Continental Area—Life and 
Living Creatures—Man in Nature—Appendices—Index. 


Prof. W. M. Davis, of Harvard.— An excellent book, clear, com- 
prehensive and remarkably accurate. . . . One who reaches a good 
understanding of the book may regard himself as having made a real 
advance in his education towards an appreciation of nature.” 


Prof. JAMES D. Dana, Yale.—“ Evidently prepared by one who under- 
stood his subject.’’ 


JOURNAL OF EpucATION.—“ It should not only be read, but owned by 
every teacher.” 


THE ELEMENTS OF ETHICS 


An Introduction to Moral Philosophy. By J. H. 
MuiIRHEAD, M.A., Royal Holloway College, Eng- 
land. 12mo, $1.00 ned. 


ContTENTS: Book I. The Science of Ethics: Problems of, 
Can there be a Science of, Scope of the Science—Book II. 
Moral Judgment : Object of, Standard of, Moral Law—Book 
III. Theories of the End: As Pleasure, as Self-sacrifice, 
Evolutionary Hedonism—Book IV. The End as Good: As 
Common Good, Forms of the Good—Book V. Moral Prog- 
ress: Standard as Relative, as Progressive, as Ideal—Bibli- 
ography. : 

Tue Acapemy, London.—' There is no other introduction which can 
be recommended.” 


Prof. J. A. QuARLES, Washington and Lee University.—‘1 am 
pleased with Muirhead’s ‘Elements of Ethics.’ It seems fresh, bright, 
thoughtful, stimulating. I shall use it probably next year.” 


Prof. J. STEARNS, University of Wisconsin.—‘‘ An admirably clear 
presentation and criticism of the teachings of the chief schools of thought 
upon the leading points of ethical theory.” 

Prof. GeorGE S. FULLERTON, University of Penn.—‘I find the book 
very clear, simple, and forcible, and I shall take pleasure in recommending 
it to my students.” 


6 THE UNIVERSITY SERIES 


THE STUDY OF ANIMAL LIFE 


By J. ARTHUR THomsoy, M.A., F.R.S.E., University 
of Edinburgh. 12mo, Illustrated, $1.50 ev. 


Contents: Part I. THe Everypay LiFe oF ANIMALS. 
The Wealth of Life—The Web of LirE—The Struggle— 
Shifts for a Living—Social Life—Domestic Life—Industries. 
Part II. THE Powers oF Lire. Vitality—The Divided 
Labors of the Body—Instinct. Part III. THe Forms or 
ANIMAL Lire. Elements of Structure—Life History—Past 
History—The Simplest Animals—Backboneless Animals— 
Backboned Animals. Part IV. THE EvoLuTION oF ANI- 
MAL Lire. Evidences of Evolution—Evolution Theories— 
Habits and Surroundings—Heredity. Appendix I. Ani- 
mal Life and Ours. Appendix II. ‘‘ Best Books” on Ani- 
mal Life. 

Prof. J. H. Comstock, Leland Stanford, Junior, University.—“1 
have read it with great delight. -It is an admirable work, giving a true 
view of the existing state and tendencies of zoology; and it possesses the 
rare merit of chee an elementary work, written from the standpoint of 


the most advanced thought, and in a manner to be understood by the 
beginning student.” 


THE FRENCH REVOLUTION 
By Cuartes E. MALtet, Balliol College, Oxford. 


12m0, $1.00 xe. 

This book has a special value to students and readers who do not own 
the great works of such writers as De Tocqueville, Taine, Michelet, and 
Von Sybel. Mr. Mallet presents economic and political aspects of society 
before the Revolution; attempts to explain why the Revolutioncame; why 
the men who madeit failed to attain the liberty they so ardently desired, or 
to found the new order which they hoped to See in France; by what arts 
and accidents, owing to what deeper causes, an inconspicuous minority 
gradually grew into a victorious party; how external circumstances kept 
the Tevolutionaty fever up, and forced the Revolution forward. History 
oes no problem of more surpassing interest and none more perplexing 
or obscure. 


GREECE IN THE AGE OF PERICLES 


By ARTHUR J. Grant of King’s College, Cam- 
bridge. 12mo, with Illustrations, $1.25 me?. 

Contents: I, The Essentials of Greek Civilization—II. 
The Religion of the Greeks—IIJ. Sparta—IV. The Earlier 
History of Athens—V. The Rivalry of Athens and Sparta— 
VI. Civil War in Greece—VII. The Athenian Democracy— 
VIN. Pericles: His Policy and his Friends—IX. Society in 
Greece—X. The Peloponnesian War to the Death of Peri- 
cles—XI. The Peloponnesian War—XII. Thought and Art 
in Athens, 


THE UNIVERSITY SERIES 7 


LOGIC, INDUCTIVE AND DEDUCTIVE 


By Wititiam Minto, M.A., Hon. LL.D., St. An- 
drews, Late Professor of Logic in the University 
of Aberdeen. With Diagrams. 385 pages. 12mo, 
$1.25 met. 


FROM THE PREFACE.—" In this little treatise two things are 
attempted, One of them is to put the study of logical formule ona 
historical basis. The other, which might at first appear inconsistent 
with this, is to increase the power of Logic as a practical discipline. 
The main purpose of this practical science, or scientific art, is con- 
cetved to be the organization of reason against error, and error in its 
various kinds 1s made the basis of the division of the subject. To carry 
out this practical aim along with the historical one ts not hopeless, 
because throughout its long history Logic has been a practical science ; 
and, as I have tried to show at some length in introductory chapters, 
has concerned ttself at different periods with the risks of error peculiar 


to each,” 
CHAPTERS IN MODERN BOTANY 


By Patrick GEppEs, Professor of Botany, Univers- 
ity College, Dundee. 12mo, Illustrated, $1.25 met. 


ae Lge with some of the strangest forms and processes of the 
vegetable world [Pitcher Plants], it exhibits these, not merely as a vege- 
table menagérie, but to give, as speedily and interestingly as may be: 

(a) Some general comprehension of the processes and forms of vege- 
table life, and, from the very first, 

b) Some intelligent grasp of the experimental methods and reasoning 
employed in their investigation. 

Other Insectiverous Plants, with their Movements and Nervous Ac- 
tion, are discussed. The Web of Life, Relations between Plants and 
Animals, Spring and its Studies, Geographical Distribution, Landscapes, 
Leaves, etc., form the subject of other chapters, and handled in a way to 
open the general subject of systematic botany most invitingly. 


THE EARTH’S HISTORY 


An Introduction to Modern Geology. By R. D. 
Roszerts, M.A., Camb., D.Sc. Lond. With col- 
ored Maps and Illustrations. 12mo, $1.50 med. 


A sketch of the methods and the results of geological inquiry to help 
those who wish to take up the study in its most interesting features. The 
pee is to answer such questions as readily suggest themselves to the 

tudent, among which may be mentioned the following : What is the nature 
of the crust movements to which the land-areas and mountain ranges are 
due? What was the distribution of land and water that obtained in the 
area when each group of rocks was formed? What was the condition ofits 
surface, and what the forms of life inhabiting it? What were the oceanic 
conditions ; the depths in different parts; the forms of life inhabiting the 
water; and the nature and extent of the materials brought down by the 
rivers that poured into the seas from the land-areas of that period? 


8 THE UNIVERSITY SERIES 


THE ENGLISH NOVEL 


Being a Short Sketch of its History from the Ear- 
liest Times to the Appearance of Waverley. By 
WaLTER RALEIGH, Professor of Modern Litera- 
ture at University College, Liverpool. 12mo, 
$r.25 ned. 

The book furnishes critical studies of the work of the chief English 
novelists before Scott, connected by certain general lines of reasoning and 
speculation on the nature and development of the novel. Most of the 


material has been given by the author In the form of lectures to his classes, 
aud possesses the merit of being specially prepared for use in the class- 


, HISTORY OF RELIGION 


A Sketch of Primitive Religious Beliefs and Prac- 
tices and of the Original Character of the Great 
Systems. By ALLAN Menziss, D.D., Professor of 
Biblical Criticism in the University of St. Andrews. 
12mo, 438 pages, $1.50 mez. 


This book makes no pretence to bea guide to all the mythologies or 
to all the ee practices which have prevailed in the world. It is 
intended to aid the student who desires to obtain a general idea of com- , 
parative religion by exhibiting the subject as a connected and organic 
whole, and by indicating the leading points of view from which each of 
the great systems may be best understood. 


LATIN LITERATURE 


By J. W. Macxait. Sometime Fellow of Balliol 
College, Oxford. r12mo, 286 pages, $1.25 mez. 


Prof. TRacy Peck, Yale University.—''1 know not where to find in 
such a convenient compass so clear a statement of the peculiar qualities 
of Rome’s Literature, and such sympathetic and defensible judgment in 
the chief authors.” 


SHAKSPERE AND HIS PREDECESSORS 
By FRrepericx S. Boas. Formerly Exhibitioner at 
Balliol College, Oxford. 12mo, $1.50 med. 


Shakspere’s writings are treated in this work in their approximate 
chronological order. The relation of the writings to their sources, their 
pecumdne and general import, and their points of contact with the litera- 
ture of their own and earlier times, engage the author’s attention. The 
Rise of the English Drama is clearly sketched, while Shakspere’s kinship 
to his predecessors is given much greater prominence than is usual. 


CHARLES SCRIBNER’S SONS 


153-157 FIFTH AVENUE - * New York City 


Pai a rai 


goa