Skip to main content

Full text of "Color problems : a practical manual for the lay student of color"

See other formats


s-=f 






ffiomell llroet:]Sitg Jifct^tg 



FROM THB FUND GIVEN BY 

CfSoI&nim Smith 



1909 



Cx.?, 1^58- 



ND 1280.V23"l90r"'"'""'"'^ 



'^?iMi'i;iE«SSIS!,.? practical manual for th 




3 1924 008 632 964 




The original of tiiis bool< is in 
tine Cornell University Library. 

There are no known copyright restrictions in 
the United States on the use of the text. 



http://www.archive.org/details/cu31924008632964 



COLOR PROBLEMS 



COLOR PROBLEMS 



A PRACTICAL MANUAL FOR 
THE LAY STUDENT OF COLOR 



By 
EMILY NOYES VANDERPOEL 



^ITH ONE HUNDRED AND SEVENTEEN COLORED PLATES 



f 



LONGMANS, GREEN, AND CO. 

91 AND 93 Fifth Avenue, New York 
LONDON AND BOMBAY 



1903 

£1 V 









Copyright, igoi, by 
Emily Noyes Vanderpoel. 



All rights reserved. 



First Edition, January, 1902. 
Reprinted, January, 1903. 



BOSTON, U.S.A. 



To 

My Father 

WILLIAM CURTIS NOYES 



PREFACE 



IjlROM a scientific standpoint admirable works 
-^ on color have been written, but they demand 
more time and study than many can give to them, 
and are too theoretical to be easily understood ; 
while those written from an artistic standpoint 
may be useful to those who paint pictures but 
are not of much benefit to larger classes of people 
who are artists in other occupations. Painters of 
pictures must study color as well as lines and 
composition ; but a better understanding of color 
would also be of great value to decorators, de- 
signers, lithographers, florists, dressmakers, and 
milliners ; women in their dress and home decor- 
ation, and many others. For such, to combine 
the essential results of the scientific and artistic 
study of color in a concise, practical manual, and 
to classify the study of color in individual eyes, 
in light, in history and in nature, has been the 
aim of the author of this book. Also, as color 



Vlll PREFACE 

cannot be fully appreciated by any written de- 
scription, the text has been made as brief as possi- 
ble, the plates full and elaborate. 

It has been asked by artists who have given 
years of study to form, perspective and composi- 
tion, why it should be necessary to study color if 
one has a good eye for it, to which another ques- 
tion may serve as answer. Suppose a person in- 
tending to make art his life work has a good eye 
for form, will he, therefore, begin to paint pictures 
before learning to draw, or without going through 
a thorough drill in perspective ? Later, having 
some subject in his mind which he wishes to put 
on canvas, he does not stop to review all the rules 
he studied of form and perspective ; the knowl- 
edge and facility he gained in that study will 
enable him unconsciously to crystallize his thought 
into better shape on his canvas. Does the posses- 
sor of a naturally fine voice think he can dis- 
pense with the time and trouble of cultivating it ? 
The same reasoning may well be applied to color 
and its study. 

E. N. T. 



INTRODUCTION 



"TT^OE some years I have known of the study and 
research the author of "this book has devoted 
to problems in Color, and its uses in the arts of 
Design and Decoration, and it is gratifying to me 
that the result of much of this work is to be given 
to the public for the use of those who are inter- 
ested in the subject. 

A great deal will be found in these pages that 
will be of practical service, particularly to those 
who have not been able to read the works of 
Chevreul, Von Bezold, Rood, Church, and others. 
Indeed, even in these, careful study would be nec- 
essary to select passages describing combinations 
that could be applied to special work. 

Much attention is here given to contrasts of 
modified or subdued colors, such colors as would be 
required constantly in decorative designs covering 
large 'spaces, against which points of more positive 
color would be placed. One of the greatest diffi- 



X INTRODUCTION 

culties in arranging a color design is in determin- 
ing the qualities and quantities of color in an 
effective and agreeable way, and very few works 
give the useful hints on this subject contained in 
this book. Under the heading of " Historic Color " 
are some very interesting and original diagrams, 
presented in a way easily to be understood and 
made use of in actual practice. 

The study of color from the scientific side has 
very little attraction for the layman, and it is even 
difficult for a painter to get out of such study 
much that will help him in his work ; but the 
presentation of some of the salient points of the 
scientific side, by one who has also borne in mind 
the artistic side, cannot fail to make this book 
attractive and useful to a great number who wish 
to know something of the laws that underlie 
agreeable arrangements of color. 

R. SWAIN GIFFORD. 



CONTENTS 



Chaptkb Paob 

Pbeface vii 

Introduction .... . . . . . ix 

List of Plates xiii 

I. Colok-Blindness 3 

II. CoLOK Theobies 13 

III. CoLOB Qualities 26 

IV. CONTKASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 48 

V. - Colok-Habmonies 73 

VI. HisTOBic Color 107 

VII. Natktbe Color Ill 

VIII. Special Suggestions 115 



Appendix A — Definitions 125 

Appendix B — Books fob Rbfebbncb 133 



LIST OF PLATES 



I. Wools as sorted by a Color-Blind Man. 
II. Solar Spectra. 

III. Table of Spectral Colors. 

IV. The Spectral Colors (a) In their order of Luminosity ; 

(b) Pure, and Grayed. 
Y. Advancing and Retiring Colors. 
VI. Advancing and Retiring Colors. 
VII. Tints. 
VIII. Shades. 
IX. Violet with Its Extremes. 
X. Blue with its Extremes. 
XI. Green with its Extremes. 
XII. TeUow with its Extremes. 

XIII. Orange with its Extremes. 

XIV. Red with its Extremes. 
XV. Shades by Contrast. 

XVI. Spectral Colors on Black, "White, and Gray. 
XVII. White on Spectral Colors. 
XVIII. Black on Spectral Colors. 
XIX. Gray on Spectral Colors. 
XX. Spectral Red with its Complement. 
XXI. Spectral Red Disk for Experiment in Complements. 
XXII. Spectral Bed and its Complement, Blue-Green, in their 
relative Proportions. 

XXIII. Spectral Orange and its Complement, Green-Blue, in 

their relative Proportions. 

XXIV. Spectral Yellow and its Complement, Spectral Blue, or 

Spectral Blue and Its Complement, Spectral Yellow, in 
their relative Proportions. 
XXV. Specliral Green and its Complement, Purple, in their 

relative Proportions. 
XXVI. Spectral Violet and its Complement, Yellow-Green, in 

their relative Proportions. 
XXVII. Milton-Bradley Color Machine. 
XXVIII. Table of Complements arranged in Pairs. 
XXIX. Table of Complements arranged in a Circle. 
XXX. Contrast Diagram. 



XIV 



LIST OF PLATES 



XXXI. Color analysis fi-om a Prize Dinner-table. 

XXXII. Color analysis from Teacup and Saucer. 

XXXIII. Harmony of one Color ; Harmony of Contrast ; Com- 

plex Harmony. 

XXXIV. Color analysis of a Book Advertisement. 
XXXV. Harmony helped by Outline. 

XXXVI. Good Dyads, or Pairs. 

XXXVII. Good Triads. 

XXXVIII. Harmony by Gradation. 

XXXIX. Harmony by Change of Quality. 

XL. Harmony by Change of Quantity. 

XLI. Harmony by Change of Both Quality and Quantity. 

XLII. Harmony by the Addition of another Color. 

XLIII. Harmony by the Addition of Black. 

XLIV. Harmony from a Dominant Hue. 

XLV. Harmony by Interchange. 

XL VI. Harmony by Counterchange. 

XL VII. The True Character of some of the so-called " Whites." 

XLVIII. Some Changes by Gradation. 

XLIX. Color analysis from Assyrian Tiles. 

L. Color analysis from Assyrian Tiles. 

LI. Color analysis from Assyrian Tiles. 

LII. Color analysis from a Mummy Cover. 

LIII. Color analysis from an Egyptian Mummy Case. 

LIV. Color analysis from a Mummy Case. 

LV. Color analysis from a Mummy Cloth. 

LVI. Color analysis from a Mummy Cloth. 

LVII. Color analysis from a Mummy Cloth. 

LVIII. Color analysis from a Mummy Cloth. 

LIX. Color analysis from a Mummy Cloth. 

LX. Color analysis from a Mummy Cloth. 

LXI. Color analysis from a Mummy Cloth. 

LXII. Color analysis from an early Greek Vase. 

LXIII. Color analysis from a Greek Vase. 

LXIV. Color analysis from a Greek Vase. 

LXV. Color analysis from a Greek Vase. 

LXVI. Color analysis from Arab Mosaics. 

LXVII. Color analysis from Arab Illumination. 

LXVIII. Color analysis from Moorish Tiles. 

LXIX. Color analysis from a Panel of the Alhambra. 

LXX. Color analysis from a Panel of the Taj Mahal, India. 

LXXI. Color analysis from Damascus Tiles. 

LXXII. Color analysis from Celtic Ornament. 

LXXIII. Color analysis from Italian Majolica Vase. 

LXXIV. Color analysis from Panel of Dutch Inlaid Cabinet of 

the 15th Century. 

LXXV. Color analysis from Spanish Embroidery. 

LXXVI. Color analysis from Spanish Embroidery. 

LXXVII. Color analysis from an Antique Persian Rug. 

LXXVIII. Color analysis from an Antique Rug. 



LIST OF PLATES 



■XV 



LXXIX. Color analysis from an Antique Rug. 

LXXX. Color analysis from an Antique Rug. 

LXXXI. Color analysis from an Antique Rug. 

LXXXII. Color analysis from an Antique Rug. 

LXXXIII. Color analysis from an Antique Rug. 

LXXXIV. Color scheme of an Antique Rug. 

LXXXV. Color analysis from an Antique Rug. (Plate Ixxxiv.) 

LXXXVI. Color analysis from Japanese Silk Tapestry. 

LXXXVII. Color analysis from Japanese Silk Tapestry. 

LXXXVIII. Color analysis from Japanese Silk Brocade. 

LXXXIX. Color analysis from border of Japanese Cloisonn6 
Vase. 

XC. Color analysis from Japanese Cloisonn6 Vase. 

XCI. Color analysis from Japanese Skirt Panel. 

XCII. Color analysis from Japanese Brocade. 

XCIII. Color analysis from Chinese Porcelain. 

XCIV. Color analysis from a Black Hawthorn Vase. 

XCV. Color analysis from a Rose-colored Vase. 

XCVI. Color analysis from Yellow Chinese Porcelain Vase. 

XCVII. Color analysis from a Chinese " Egg-shell " Plate. 

XCVIII. Color analysis from a Butterfly. 

XCIX. Color analysis from a Stone. 

C. Color note from a Discolored Propeller Flange. 

CI. Color note from Leaves on a Tree. 

CII. Color note from a Sunset Sky. 

cm. Color note from Bare Woods on the Edge of a Meadow. 

CIV. Color note from Evergreens against a Gray-Blue Rain- 
cloud. 

CV-. Color note from a Shadow on White Ground. 

CVI. Color note from a Blue-bird. 

CVII. Color note from a Slice of an Orange. 

CVIII. Color note from an Orange Canna Blossom. 

CIX. Color note from a Bunch of Azaleas. 

ex. Color note from Oak-leaves against a Distant Hillside. 

CXI. Color note from Oats seen from the Edge of the Field. 

CXII. Color note from a^ Pussy Willow. 

CXIII. Color note from a Trout Pond. 

CXIV. Color note from a Tree Fungus. 

CXV. Color scheme from Winter Landscape. 

CXVI. Spectral Red, neutralized by Black and White. 

CXVII. Spectral Yellow, neutralized by Black and White. 



COLOR PROBLEMS 



COLOR PROBLEMS 



CHAPTER I 

COLOR-BLINDJiTESS 



THE relation of color to light is much the same 
as that of miisic to sound. Color has its 
many hues, its long scales of tints and shades, its 
true and its false chords. Mere sound gives us but 
little pleasure ; when developed, however, into its 
highest form, music, we are thrilled, as by the song 
of a bird, a favorite ballad, or a Beethoven Sym- 
phony. So in light, our enjoyment culminates at 
the glories of color in a flower or a sunset, at the 
shadows that play over the hills, or at the varied 
hues of a salt marsh. Hence we may aptly term 
color the music of light ; and when we think of the 
wonderful ways in which it has been used and 
combined by painters and designers for himdreds 
of years, it must seem strange to us that its har- 
monies have not been as thoroughly studied and 
classified as those of sound. 

Furthermore, color has come to be so closely 



4 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

connected with all the occupations and enjoyments 
of mankind that it is hard for us to realize that 
many persons are wholly or partially blind to its 
beauties. It is well known that there are some 
individuals with such perfect organs of hearing 
that they are able to distinguish the slightest 
sounds, who yet are so utterly unable to distinguish 
between two tones or between the harmonies and 
discords of music that they are said to have " no 
ear." So there are those whose eyes are as well 
formed for seeing all and distant objects, but who 
are unable to see color as it is seen by people with 
normal eyes. Such individuals may be said to 
have "no eye" for color, and are scientifically 
termed " color-blind." 

This fact is not so well known ; and, in view of 
it, any one interested in color will understand the 
wisdom of beginning a study of color with some 
knowledge of color-blindness, and, if possible, with 
having his eyes examined by an expert. Such an 
examination is a short and simple matter. Dr. 
William Thomson of Philadelphia has devised what 
he calls a " color stick," on which colored wools are 
so hung and numbered that it is not even necessary 
to be an expert to use it, and with the help of 
which color-blindness can easily be detected. It 
has been ■ used with great success over some fifty 
thousand miles of railroad. From the same hand 



I COLOR-BLINDNESS 5, 

has lately come a newer and simpler form of the 
same invention. 

Color-blindness is seldom a total want of the 
power to see colors, but is rather a want of the 
true normal perception of colors, and it is more 
common than is generally supposed. The most 
common form of the defect, which has been called 
by some " red-blindness," is that of not seeing 
red, but of confusing it with green, as, for instance, i^ 
being unable to see any difference between the red 
flower of a geranium and the green of its foliage ; 
between green grass and red autumn leaves. A 
color-blind person will sort variously colored wools 
in the strangest way, putting the reds among 
the greens, and mixing the blues and the violets 
together. 

Plate I shows part of the result of an examina- 
tion of a color-blind man by Doctor Thomson. The 
patient was given one hundred and fifty different- 
colored wools to sort in little heaps according as 
he saw them to be red, blue, green, etc. ; he seemed 
to hesitate over but few of them. These he put by 
themselves in a heap called neutral. To a normal 
eye the result is almost incomprehensible, as he 
mixed green with all the other colors and made 
other as strange combinations. Di-chromatic vision ^ 
has been suggested as a fitting term for such de- 
fective color perception, as colors to red-blind 



6 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

persons amount to but two, viz., yellow and blue, 
with a long range of neutral grays between. 

There are other forms of color-blindness which 
are less common. Some persons seem to see but 
red and blue, classing yellow and green with red. 
A less common defect is that of not seeing violet, 
while there are a few cases on record where all 
sensation of color is wanting, everything appear- 
ing in differing degrees of gray. One such 
instance coming under the notice of the writer 
occurred temporarily from over-strained nerves in 
a person gifted with an abnormally fine color- 
sense. No doubt some people are born color-blind, 
but the defect is also brought on by disease, by the 
excessive use of tobacco, alcohol, and other stimu- 
lants, and may, or may not, prove permanent. 
According to Abney, the disease begins in the 
centre of the eye, so that those suffering from its 
early stages can match colored wools correctly, but 
when given instead small colored pellets to match 
make many mistakes, because a pellet may happen 
to be directly before the small blind spot that is 
insensible to its color, while the larger mass of 
wool extends before the whole retina. Doctor 
Charcot and his school in Paris have made many 
examinations into visual disturbances, and through 
these examinations much of the peculiar coloring 
and mannerism of some of the modern painters of 



I COLOR-BLINDNESS 7 

the so-called impressionist, tachist, mosaist, gray- 
in-gray, violet colorist, archaic, vibraist, and color 
orgiast schools has been explained. The artists 
tell the truth when they say that nature looks to 
them as they paint it, but they are suffering from 
hysteria or from other nervous derangements by 
which their sight is affected. 

For a long time railroad engineers would not 
believe that examinations for color-blindness were 
necessary, but when shown the results of such 
an examination the surprise of those with normal 
eyes was intense. They realized what it would be to 
travel on a train in charge of an engineer who did 
not know when the red danger signal had been put 
in place of the usual green one. In other spheres 
of life correct knowledge of color is not so vitally 
necessary, yet to artisans of many kinds — decora- 
tors, florists, manufacturers, dressmakers, milliners, 
etc. — it is both useful and important. 

As to the extent of color-blindness, it has been 
estimated that in England about one person in i^ 
eighteen is more or less afflicted with it. In 1873 
and 1875 Dr. Farre examined in France one 
thousand and fifty officials of various grades, and 
found among them ninety-eight color-blind, or nine 
and thirty-five hundredths per cent. In 1876 
Professor Holmgren examined in Sweden two 
hundred and sixty-five persons on the Upsala 



8 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

Gefle line, with the result that thirteen were found 
to be color-blind. Seebach found five young per- 
sons out of forty-one in a gymnasium who were 
color-blind. None of them had been at all con- 
scious of the defect. 

Among the visitors to the International Health 
Association in London, in 1884, Mr. F. Galton 
foimd a large number of men and a small number 
of women with more or less defective color-per- 
ception. In this country, examinations in the 
army and navy and among railroad engineers 
reveal that color-blindness, if not as general as in 
England, is quite common. Dr. Thomson states 
that as far as has been gathered from statistics 
generally, the percentage of color-blind men in 
the civilized world is four per cent., or one in 
twenty-five, — among women one in four thousand. 
While he has seen a great number of color-blind 
men he has never met a woman with the defect. 

Singularly enough this color-blindness — the 
confounding of one color with another, or the 
want of perception of certain colors — does not 
prevent great enjoyment of both nature and art. 
A person so color-blind as to see no difference 
between the scarlet of a geranium blossom and 
the green of its leaves, or who buys a pair of 
bright green gloves supposing them to be brown, 
is still an enthusiastic and seemingly an intelligent 



I COLOR-BLINDNESS 9 

admirer of landscape and art. One cannot say 
from what the enjoyment arises, but it is certainly 
there. 

There is a noted instance of a man who learned 
in later life that he was color-blind, and then 
first understood why he had never been able to 
pick as many strawberries as his boy companions, 
because with his defect he saw no difference be- 
tween the colors of the berry and that of its 
leaf. 

There is, however, a very simple way in which 
it is possible for some color-blind persons to correct 
in a measure their erroneoiis impressions. If they 
have something green to match and fear they may 
mistake red for the green, by looking at their 
samples through a green or red glass they can 
prove whether or not they are correct. Through '^ 
a green glass the green will keep its color, while 
the red will look nearly black. Through a red 
glass the red will remain unchanged and the green 
will seem nearly black. 

Color-blind people can have colored glasses 
mounted as spectacles at small cost, which will 
almost entirely relieve their defect and be of great 
help in their work. 

How far the eye of a color-blind person is sus- 
ceptible of education is still uncertain. Sufficient 
experiment has not been made in that direction, 



10 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

but the fact that women notice color more than do 
men and are, as a general rule, more correct in 
their judgment of color, points to the fact that 
the eye is unconsciously educated by its surround- 
ings. The constant discrimination in choice of 
dress and home decoration which enters early into 
a girl's life gives an education which men, in 
Europe and America at least, are deprived of, 
from generally wearing black or quiet colors. 

That an eye normal in its perceptions of colors 
is capable of cultivation cannot be doubted. " It 
does not admit of doubt that individual sensibility 
to color admits of large variations, and that it is 
susceptible of immense improvement. This culti- 
vation of the sense of color is, however, rather psy- 
chological than physiological, rather mental than 
physical. It is not that the organ of vision is im- 
proved, but our power of interpreting and coordi- 
Qating the senses which it transmits to the brain. 
And here it is that the effects of association come 
most prominently, though often unconsciously, into 
play. We try to trace out the causes of the vast 
numbers of color sensations which we are contin- 
ually receiving, but we constantly find that the cold 
methods of analysis fail to explain the mental ap- 
preciation with which we regard the astounding 
fertility of nature in its gifts of color." ^ 

' Church, Colour. 



I COLOR-BLINDNESS 11 

Artists often find that when the eyes are over- 
stimulated by false lights or colors, or want of bal- 
ance in the colors looked at, the nerveis are so irri- 
tated that a confusion of color and complementary 
tones takes place. If continued to any length of 
time the nerves become so fatigued that the color 
sense is lost, and the eye responds only to grada- 
tions of black and white. 

That there are also subtle shades of difference 
in the sensibility to color even of good, normal eyes, 
no one who has paid any attention to art can fail 
to know. These shades of difference it is impos- 
sible to gauge, and they can only be known by the 
differing qualities of work produced. In a studio 
where perhaps a dozen pupils may be painting 
from one piece of still life, a vase, or bit of drapery, 
such differences can be clearly seen. One pair of 
eyes may have a tendency to see more violet than 
the others, another pair sees everything more bril- 
liantly or in a higher key than the others. One stu- 
dent may have more difficulty in harmonizing on 
his canvas the different colors of the model than 
the rest, while another with perhaps less skill in 
using the paint may have such a fine eye for har- 
mony as by the mere charm of his color to delight 
every one in the room. 

There comes with advancing years a subtle 
change in the condition of the eye which it is well 



12 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. i 

to understand. With age the lens of the eye loses 
its purity or whiteness and becomes tinged with 
yellow. This is not generally known, and the 
change is not always strongly marked, but it pro- 
duces a decided effect upon the perception of blue 
and bluish colors. The case of the English painter 
Mulready may be cited as a good instance. His 
pictures in his later years were different in color 
from his earlier ones, being much colder in tone, 
that is bluer or less yellow. If, however, they 
were looked at through a piece of slightly yellow 
glass they appeared of the same coloring as his 
earlier work, painted when his eyes were normal. 



CHAPTER II 



COLOR THEOEIES 



A FULL review of the theories held about 
color is not necessary in a work of this 
nature, and those who have more time for and 
further interest in the subject will find mentioned 
in Appendix B to this volume the titles of a num- 
ber of admirable works and treatises. 

The sensation of color is first and preeminently 
produced by light. But an electric discharge, in- 
ternal causes, or even pressure on the eyeball 
may also cause it ; just how, we do not know. 
In fact, the whole subject of color, its causes, 
and its mechanism, is still in the region of 
speculation, although of speculation that may be 
useful. 

Leaving aside the theory of color production by 
other causes, we will give our attention to that 
color sensation caused by the light of the sun, and 
briefly to that produced by artificial light. 

The cut on page 14 shows the construction of the 
eye viewed from the side. We see that light enters 
the front of the eye through the cornea and lens 
a,nd strikes the interior coating, which is the retina. 

13 



i^ 



14 COLOR problp:ms ch. 

This is a wonderful membrane, very thin, but com- 
posed, as we "see in the next illustration, magnified 
many times (page 15), of a marvellous network 
made of minute nerves and blood vessels ending 
on the innermost surface in tiny rods and cones. 
These rods and cones in some mysterious way are 
acted upon by light, and, like the outposts of an 
army, send messages of form and color to the brain. 




THE CONSTRUCTION OP THE HUMAN ETE AS VIEWED FROM THE SIDE. 

(Nearly life size.) 



Color is therefore spoken of as " an internal sensa- 
tion," and is fine or poor as are the eyes and 
brain of the person who sees it. 

What is light, we ask ? Scientists answer that 
it is something which comes to us from a luminous 
or light-giving body. Sir Isaac Newton pronounced 
it to consist of fine atoms moving toward us rapidly. 
A later theory is called the loave theory ■ — that 



n 



COLOR THEORIES 



15 



there exists throughout space a fine impalpable 
medium, " the light-bearing ether," — that this 
ether moves in waves, which, beating upon the ret- 
inas of our eyes as ocean waves beat upon the 
shore, produce what we call light. 

Sunlight comp&,red to candle or gas light appears 
to be white; this white was proved by Sir Isaac 
Newton in 1672 to consist of many colors com- 



RODSANO 




I^ONES. 



CROSS-SECTION OF THE KETINA, SHOWING THE BODS AND CONES. 

* (Very much magnified.) 



bined in one ray. He -jvas the first to divide such 
a ray of sunlight, which he did by letting it fall 
through a slit in the window of a darkened room, 
then through a prism, or three-sided piece of glass, 
on white paper. If this experiment be repeated 
there will be seen " a long streak of pure and beau- 
tiful colors which blend into each other by gentle 
gradations." Anyone who has seen a rainbow has 



16 COLOR PROBLEMS gh. 

seen the same separation of colors, as the raindrops 
lact in the same way as the prism and divide the 
rays of sunlight into their component colors. 

The " spectrum " is the narde given to the streak 
of colors when produced by the help of the prism, 
and it and the rainbow contain the same colors in 
the same order. The experiment has also been 
made of passing this streak of colors through a 
second prism, when they again unite and the ray 
of simple white light reappears. 

An instrument called a " spectroscope" has been 
invented, and is constantly used by scientific 
students of color, which analyzes a ray of light 
still better than the simple prism. With its aid, 
early in this century, WoUaston and Fraunhofer 
discovered that the spectrum of sunlight, in addi- 
tion to its colors, was crossed by many fine, dark, 
fixed lines. These have been named Fraunhofer 
lines, and are most useful in dividing and map- 
ping out the limits of the different colors. Still 
a later invention called a " diffraction grating," 
made either of speculum metal or of glass sil- 
vered on the back and ruled with fine parallel 
lines, sometimes as many as eighteen thousand to 
the English inch, is used in place of a prism. 
With the use of improved methods Professor 
Rowland of Johns Hopkins University has made 
one ruled with some fifty or sixty thousand lines. 



11 COLOR THEOJIIBS 17 

A ray of sunlight can be divided by this without 
the disadvantage of crowding the colors in the 
middle, as is unavoidable by the wedge-shaped 
glass of the prism. 

Plate II shows a solar spectrum as produced by 
a prism and also one as shown by a diffraction 
grating. They both give the colors and the main 
Fraunhofer lines, the latter being numbered. 

Although not essential to the practical use of 
this manual, we will now return to the theories 
of the primary colors, so called,, upon which differ- 
ing views have been held. Sir David Brewster's 
theory of three primaries — red, yellow, and blue 

— has been the most popular, because of the ease 
with which the three so-called secondary colors 
may be made by mixing paint of the three prima- 
ries, as follows : red and blue, violet ; blue and 
yellow, green; yellow and red, orange. Artists 
have generally adopted it ; Chevreul, the great 
director of the Gobelin tapestries, based his whole 

' color system on the theory of three primary colors 

— red, yellow, and blue; three secondary colors 
made by combinations of the first three — orange, 
green, and violet ; and three tertiary colors made 
from combinations of the second three — olive, 
russet, and citrine. We must, however, discrimi- 
nate carefully between pigments, paints, and light. 
By experiment we prove that yellow and blue light 



18 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

do not make green, but white ; that redjaid-gxaen. 
light make yellow ; and so on, so that the theory 
of ThomasToung is now more generally followed 
by scientists. As Rood gives it in his Modern 
Chromatics, " there can be in an objective sense no 
such thing as three fjindamental colors, or three 
primary kinds of colored light. In a totally differ- 
ent sense, however, somethipg of this kind is not 
only possible, but, as the recent advances of science 
show, highly probable. We have already seen in a 
previous chapter that in the solar spectrum the eye 
can distinguish no less than a thousand different 
hues. Every small, minute, almost invisible por- 
tion of the retina possesses this power, which leads 
us to ask whether each atom of the retina is sup- 
plied with an immense number of nerve fibrils for 
the reception and conveyance of this vast number 
of sensations. 

" According to the theory of the celebrated 
Thomas Young, each minute elementary portion of 
the retina is capable of receiving and transmitting 
three different sensations ; or we may say that each 
elementary portion of its surface is supplied with 
three nerve fibrils, adapted for the reception of 
three sensations. One set of these nerves is 
strongly acted on by long waves of light and pro- 
duces the sensation we call red ; another set re- 
sponds most powerfully to waves of medium 



COLOR THEORIES 



19 



length, producing the sensation we call green; 
finally, the third set is strongly stimulated by 
short waves, and generates the sensation known 
as violet." (This might perhaps rather be called 
violet blue, as scientists differ as to the exact 
shade.) " The red of the spectrum, then, acts 
powerfully on the first set of these nerves ; but 
according to Young's theory, it also acts on the 




Red 



Green 



Blue 

R O Y G B V 

DIAGRAM ILLUSTKATING THE YOUNG-DELMHOLTZ THEORY OF COLOR 
SENSATION. 



two other sets, but with less energy. The same is 
true of the green and violet rays of the spectrum ; 
they each act on all three sets of nerves, but most 
powerfully on those specially designed for their 
reception." All this will be better understood by 
the aid of the accompanying diagram, which is 
taken from Helmholtz's great work. Physiologi- 
cal Optics. In this figure, along the horizontal 



20 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

lines 1, 2, 3 are placed the colors of the spectrum 
properly arranged, and the curves above them 
indicate the degree to which the three kinds 
of nerves are acted on by these colors. Thus we 
see that nerves of the first kind are powerfully 
stimulated by red light, are much less affected 
by yellow, still less by green, and very little by 
violet light. Nerves of the second kind are much 
affected by green light, less by yellow and blue, 
still less by red and violet. The third kind of 
nerves answer readily to violet light, and are suc- 
cessively less affected by other kinds of light in 
the following order: blue, green, yellow, orange, 
red. The next point in the theory is that if all 
three sets of nerves are simultaneously stimulated 
to about the same degree the sensation which we 
call white will be produced. This result would 
almost lead us into calling white a color — and the 
most brilliant one of all. These are the main 
points of Young's theory, which was published as 
long ago as 1802, and more fully in 1807. Atten- 
tion has been called to it within the last few years 
by Helmholtz, and it is mainly owing to his labors 
and those of Maxwell that it now commands such 
respectful attention. Thus far the study of color- 
blindness has furnished evidence in favor of the 
theory of Young, and its phenomena are more 
easily explained by this than by any other theory. 



n COLOR THEORIES 21 

A recent invention by Frederick E. Ives of 
Philadelphia has also been cited in its support. 
Through the use of what he calls a photo-chro- 
moscopic camera he takes through three colon 
screens — a red, a green, and a blue one — - three 
negatives. These negatives, placed in an instru- 
ment called by him a stereo-photo-chromo-scope 
(which resembles a stereoscope, and which also holds 
three screens of the same colors), produce to the 
eyes an image so perfect in color and relief that 
"people have been seen to place their hand in 
front of it before they were convinced that they 
did not see a direct reflection." Various sets of 
three hues, or modified hues, might be used to pro- 
duce the same effect. 

In 1878, having re-investigated the subject thor- 
oughly, Hering published in Vienna a paper advo- 
cating another theory. According to this "the 
retina is provided with three visual substances, and 
the fundamental sensations are not three, but six, — 

Black and white, 

Ked and green. 

Blue and yellow. 
Each of these three pairs corresponds to an as- 
similation or diassimilation process in one of the 
visual substances ; thus red light acts on the red- 
green substance in exactly the opposite way from 
green light, and when both kinds of light are 



22 



COLOR PROBLEMS 



CH. 



present in suitable proportions a balance is effected, 
and both sensations, red and green, vanish." ^ 

One of the latest accounts of these theories (of 
Young-Helmholtz and Hering), written in English, 
is to be found in Dr. Foster's Text-book of Physi- 
dlogy. It contains a full and clear discussion of the 
merits and demerits of both theories from a scien- 
tific standpoint. From it we give the accompany- 




R O Y G B V 

DIAGRAM ILLUSTRATING HERING'S THEORY" OF COLOR SENSATION. 



ing diagram illustrating Bering's theory of color 
vision. 

Edridge Green also discusses both theories fully 
in connection with color-blindness. 

On one point all these theories agree, which is 
that perfect or normal color vision is made up of 
three factors, or as Foster says, it is " tri-chromic, 

^ Rooil , Modern Chromatics, 



11 COLOR THEORIES 23 

based on three or the equival'ent of three primary 
sensations." The first, the Brewster theory, states 
that they are red, yellow, and blue colors ; the sec- 
ond, the Young-Helmholtz theory, that there are 
three kinds of nerve fibrils in the retina, affected 
respectively by red, blue, and green, and their com- 
binations of the spectrum ; while that of Hering 
is that in the eye there are three changeable visual 
substances which are increased or diminished ac- 
cordingly as the rays of black and white, yellow 
and blue, or red and green, fall upoli them. 

Le Conte, in his work Sight, says of the latter 
part of this theory, " according to Hering, com- 
plementary colors are the result of opposite afEec- 
tions of the retina, so that there are only two 
essentially distinct color affections of the retina, 
which, with their opposites, produce two pairs of 
complementary colors ; the one with its opposite 
produces red and green ; the other with its opposite, 
yellow and blue. This, though more doubtful, seems 
a probable cause of complementariness." Also, 
" Stanley Hall . . . believes that color is per- 
ceived by the cones (in the retina) alone ; further, 
that different parts of the same cone vibrate with 
different degrees of rapidity, and therefore respond 
to different colors, and the conical form is adapted 
for this purpose. In order to gain a clearer con- 
ception we may imagine each cone to be made up 



24 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

of a number of buttons of graduated sizes joined 
together. These buttons, on account of their dif- 
ferent sizes, Avould vibrate with different degrees 
of rapidity, and therefore co-vibrate with different 
colors. White light, he supposes, vibrates the 
whole series ; red light the thicker, and violet the 
thinner portion of the series ; or, taking Hering's 
view of the primary colors, we may imagine that 
red and green rays affect one portion and yellow 
and blue rays another portion of the same cone." 

From the fact that in 1876 F. Boll discovered 
that the retina contained a red or purple substance 
that quickly disappeared on exposure to light, 
Kuhne elaborated, after further experiments with 
light upon that substance, a still later theory of 
color vision which supposes that the light waves 
produce in the retina different compounds that give 
rise to the sensation of the different colors. 

Mrs. Franklin of Baltimore has lately given us 
a theory of "light sensation," as she prefers to call 
it, which has been favorably received.^ The ques- 
tion of the specific uses of the rods and cones in 
the retina has been a puzzling one, and she sug- 
gests that they may be of the same nature, but in 
different stages of development, — in other words, 
that the rods are undeveloped cones. As there 
are more cones than rods in the middle of the 

i"Mind,"n,s., Vol. II. 1893. 



II COLOR THEORIES 25 

retina, and as color is seen more vividly there, ,the 
inference is that the cones are susceptible to both 
light and color, while the rods are only sensitive 
to light. Such a theory seems to explain the re- 
sults of many experiments heretofore made by 
scientists. Some discussion of the subtile and 
beautiful colors produced by interference, refrac- 
tion, absorption, and polarization, as well as by 
opalescence, fluorescence, and phosphorescence, 
might aptly follow here, but that such discussion 
hardly comes within the scope of this mainly 
practical book. Readers who wish to understand 
and experiment with them are referred to the 
works of Rood, Church, and Dove. 



CHAPTER III 

COLOR QUALITIES 

HUE, PURITY, luminosity' OOLD AND WARM COLORS 

TINTS, SHADES, BROKEN TINTS 

COLORS have three principal qualities, called 
scientifically " constants of color," which 
should be studied as a preparation for the study of 
the harmony of colors. These qualities are hue, 
purity, and luminosity. To make these as clear as 
possible, we will for the present, at least, ignore 
the delicate divisions of the spectrum made by 
both scientists and artists of which about one 
thousand have been counted, and divide it arbi- 
trarily into six pure spectral colors differing from 
each other by their hues as by their wave 
lengths ; the wave lengths we give according to 
Rood, expressed in ten-millionths of a millimetre 
(nyuiTinyaff)- (See Plate III.) These six divisions 
can be placed beside and compared with flowers 
and colored materials, and are printed to imitate 
colored light as nearly as pigments and paper 
can give them. At best, any such imitation 
falls far short of nature. 

The first quality or constant of colors is hue, 

26 



CH. Ill COLOR QUALITIES 27 

this term being generally agreed upon by scientists 
to mean color pure and simple, according to its 
wave length in the spectrum. Plate III gives us 
six hues — violet, blue, green, yellow, orange, and 
red. Each of these is quite different from the next 
one, as the violet hue is from the blue hue, the blue 
hue from the green hue. 

The second quality or constant of colors is purity, 
that is, its lack of any mixture of white, black, 
or any other color. These not only weaken the ■ 
color but change its character, as will be found 
by mixing white paint with vermilion paint, which 
will be seen to grow more pink, as well as lighter, 
as the white is added'. 

The third quality or constant of colors is their 
luminosity or brightness, also sometimes called 
clearness. It is measured by the total amount of 
light reflected to the eye, and is therefore inde- 
pendent of hue and purity. The amount of lumi- 
nosity of a color can be determined correctly by 
means of an invention called Maxwell's Disks. 
These disks date back to the time of Ptolemy, but 
were brought into use early in this century by 
Maxwell. A disk, or round piece of cardboard, 
painted with the color to be tested, is put behind 
two smaller disks, one of white and one of black, 
which can be so adjusted 'that on turning them all 
rapidly the gray formed by the mingling of black 



28 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

and white matches in luminosity the one back 
of it. 

From such experiments we see that a room 
papered or painted in yellow will give you the 
lightest room, because it will reflect more light to 
the eye than any of the other colors ; one done m 
orange will come next, and so on through the list. 
A practical knowledge of these different luminosi- 
ties is most useful in decoration, both on account 
of the contrast between colors for this reason as 
well as for their hues. Also for the ability to 
lighten a dark part of a room by placing there a 
piece of luminous coloring, and vice versa to darken 
what is too bright. We must here add that these 
terms, purity of color and luminosity, are used by 
artists in quite a different sense, as they call paint- 
ings noticeable for purity of color, meaning only 
that the tints in them have no tendency to look dull 
or dirty, but not at all implying the absence of 
white or gray light. They call color in a painting 
luminous simply because it actually recalls to the 
mind the impression of light, not because it ac- 
tually reflects much light to the eye. Plate No. IV 
gives the six spectral colors in their order of 
luminosity. 

We will now take up in turn each of the six 
hues by itself and study it in its variations towards 
its neighboring hues. 



Ill COLOR QUALITIES 29 

That we do not appreciate the influence of color 
upon man as well as upon the lower animals, is 
true ; but color has not been studied by us as it 
probably will be in the near future. The powers 
of attraction of different colors for ants and bees 
have occupied the time and close observation of 
Sir John Lubbock and of many other scientists, 
and now the effect of different colors is being 
tried on the children in some schools and on the 
patients in certain insane asylums. A few facts 
are enough to show that there is still much to 
learn in that direction, and that these questions 
can be investigated with profit. One of these 
facts is that a certain shade of purple always pro- 
duced the condition of the skin commonly known 
as " goose-flesh " upon a girl in a normal condition 
of health. 

Goethe in his Theory of Colour, as translated 
by Sir Charles Eastlake, records observations and 
experiments of the most minute character with 
regard to light and colors — of a character hardly 
touched upon by others. His suggestion of using 
colored glass for study in colors is very valuable. 
He says, "People experience a great delight in 
color generally. The eye requires it as much as it 
requires light. We have only to remember the 
refreshing sensation we experience, if on a cloudy 
day the sun illumines a single portion of the 



30 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

scene before us and displays its colors. That 
healing powers were ascribed ,to colored gems 
may have arisen from the experience of this inde- 
finable pleasure. 

" From some of our earlier observations we can 
conclude that general impressions produced by 
single colors cannot be changed, that they act 
specifically and miist produce definite specific 
states in the living organ. 

" They likewise produce a corresponding influence 
on the mind. Experience teaches us that particu- 
lar colors excite particular states of feeling. It is 
related of a witty Frenchman, " II pretendoit que 
son ton de conversation avec Madame etoit chang^ 
depuis qu'elle avait change en cramoisi le meuble 
de son cabinet, qui ^toit bleu." (He imagined that 
the tone of his conversation with Madame was 
changed since she had changed the coloring of her 
sitting-room from blue to crimson.) 

"In order to experience these influences com- 
pletely, the eye should be entirely surrounded with 
one color ; we should be in a room of one color, or 
look through a colored glass. We are then identi- 
fied with the hue, it attunes the eye and mind in 
mere unison with itself.^ 

" The colors on the plus side are yellow, red-yel- 

' The use of this suggestion as to colored glass is strongly urged by the 
author, as it is a capital way of seeing how the world would look were overy- 
thiag in it blue, or any other color. 



Ill COLOR QUALITIES ' 31 

low and yellow-red. The feelings they excite are 
quick, lively, and aspiring. 

" The colors on the minus side are blue, red-blue 
and blue-red. They produce a restless, susceptible, 
anxious impression." 

Each of these six hues can be divided roughly 
into three, as they are pure or tend toward their 
neighboring hues. So violet, of which we have 
pure normal or spectraj violet, with red-violet on 
one hand, blue-violet on the other ; or yellow, of 
which we have pure normal or spectral yellow, with 
orange-yellow on one side, green-yellow on the other. 

Violet is a cold color, red-violet warmer than 
blue-violet. It is grave, dignified, as compared 
with the other colors. Being a retiring color, it 
will serve well as a background, as it will throw 
forward any more luminous color put upon it. In 
flowers we have examples of this color in its va- 
riety in violets, lilacs, asters, sweet peas, and morn- 
ing-glories. In the latter it is exquisitely shaded 
from one extreme to the other. The wild Eupato- 
rium furnishes a fine example of red-violet, the 
cultivated variety an equally good one of the blue- 
violet, almost cold enough for a blue. There is no 
sound pigment which can be used alone to paint 
this color. The violet in the originals for these 
plates was made with French blue and crimson 
lake, and crimson lake is not considered a perma- 



/ 



32 ^ COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

nent color. Violet of all kinds suffers from artifi- 
cial light, losing much of its blue, and becoming 
more red and dull. 
^ Blue is a cold color, and a retiring one, espe- 
cially suited for backgrounds, as one will notice in 
studying a blue sky, against which the landscape 
stands out with great beaujyj In flowers, ex- 
amples of this color are more rare than of others. 
The blue gentian is not a true blue, it is so close 
on blue- violet. Forget-me-nots, chicory, centaureas, 
and larkspur give us blue in diflfering varieties. 
The sky from the deep violet blue of a winter's 
night to the pale, greenish tones near the horizon 
on a summer's day shows us an unsurpassed scale 
of this hue. 

Goethe says of it, " It may be said that blue 
brings a principle of darkness with it. 

" This color has a peculiar and almost indescrib- 
able effect on the eye. As a hue it is powerful, 
but it is on the negative side, and in its highest 
"^^ purity is, as it were, a stimulating negation. 
"«^ Its appearance, then, is a kind of contradiction be- 
tween excitement and repose. 

" As the upper sky and distant mountains appear 
blue, so a blue surface seems to retire from us. 

'' But as we readily follow an agreeable object 
that flies from us, so we love to contemplate blue, 
not because it advances to us, but because it draws 
us after it. 



m COLOR QUALITIES , 33 

" Blue gives us an impression of cold, and thus 
again reminds us of shade. We have before 
spoken of its affinity with black. ^ 

" Rooms which are hung with pure blue appear 
in some degree larger, but at the same time empty 
and cold. 

" The appearance of objects seen through a blue 
glass is gloomy and melancholy. 

" When blue partakes in some measure of the 
plus side the effect is not disagreeable ; sea-green 
is rather a pleasing color." 

Genuine ultramarine is an expensive but very 
pure blue paint made from lapis-lazuli. Artificial 
ultramarine generally inclines towards violet. A 
good deal of green and violet light is reflected from 
cobalt blue. There is some green in Prussian blue, 
in indigo, and in cerulean blue. Prussian blue, if 
used quite thickly, reflects some red. The blue for 
the original of Plate X was made of French 
blue (artificial ultramarine), tinged on the violet 
end with crimson lake, and on the greenish end 
with emerald green, which latter is, not a perma- 
nent color, but which approaches nearest of any 
pigment to the green hue in the spectrum. Blue 
is one of the colors most used in decoration. 

Grreen may be cold or warm, retiring or advanc- 
ing according as it approaches blue or yellow, 
although pure spectral green is of a cold nature. 



34 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

When one studies the great scale of greens as seen 
in a landscape lit up with full sunshine, and 
notices the intense yellow green where the sun 
shines through the leaves, the pale gray greens 
produced by the sun's glancing over the polished 
surfaces of others, and the rich dark green in the 
shadows, it seems as if no other color would admit 
of so varied a scale or be more restful to the eye. 

Goethe says : " The eye experiences a distinctly 
grateful impression from this color. The beholder 
has neither a wish nor the power to imagine a 
state beyond it. Hence for rooms to live in con- 
stantly, the green color is most generally selected." 
This assertion may be doubted, many persons 
objecting to green, tlie truth probably being that 
it has been found difficult to use, and not having 
been understood or well treated has not been appre- 
ciated. Its healthfulness cannot be doubted if one 
considers how refreshing the surroundings of trees 
and grass are to an invalid who has been surrounded 
by city bricks and stones. Can we not derive a like 
benefit from this color by decorating our city rooms 
with varying tones of soft gray greens, like nature, 
relieved here and there with a touch of brightness, 
as flowers, birds, and butterflies gleam amid the foli- 
age in their native haunts ? The rules for height- 
ening these contrasts with certain vai'ieties of green 
will be given in the chapter on contrasts. The 



ni COLOR QUALITIES 35 

extremes of green blend better than those of other 
colors. Emerald green has been used as being 
the best paint with which to imitate the normal 
green of the spectrum, but it must be remembered 
that it is a trifle bluer than it should be to be exact. 

Of yellow Goethe writes, *' This is the color 
nearest the light. 

" In its highest purity it always carries with it 
the nature of brightness, and has a serene, gay, 
softly exciting character. 

" In this state applied to dress^ hangings, carpets, 
etc., it is agreeable. Gold in its perfectly un- 
mixed state, especially when the effect of polish is 
superadded, gives us a new and high idea of this 
colo^; in like manner, a strong yellow, as it ap- 
pears on satin, has a magnificent and noble effect. 

" We find from experience again that yellow ex- 
cites a warm and agreeable impression. Hence in 
painting it belongs to the illumined and emphatic 
side. 

" This impression of warmth may be experienced 
in a very .lively manner if we look at a landscape 
through a yellow glass, particularly on a gray win- 
ter's day. The eye is gladdened, the heart ex- 
panded and cheered, a glow seems at once to breathe 
towards us." 

Yellow is both a warm and an advancing color, 
especially useful to apply as ornament on other 



36 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

colors, as gold embroidery is beautiful on any 
color. With the exception of white there are 
more yellow flowers than of any other color. In 
Moorish decorations, which are some of the finest 
in the world, gold is used as ornament on blue and 
red grounds ; in fact, throughout the history of 
ornament, yellow is more often used in that way 
than as a groundwork. 

A thin wash of Aurora yellow gave the color for 
the original of Plate XII. This paint, when put 
on thickly, tends too much toward orange to imi- 
tate well the very narrow band of yellow in the 
spectrum. It is made from cadmium, and, accord- 
ing to Church,' the deep or orange cadmiums are all 
more lasting than the pale or lemon-colored kinds. 

Orange is still a warmer color than yellow, and 
is also an advancing color. Goethe says, " All that 
we have said of yellow is applicable here in a 
higher degree. The red-yellow (orange) gives an 
impression of warmth and gladness, since it repre- 
sents the hue of the intenser glow of fire, and of 
the milder radiance of the setting sun." Orange is 
perhaps the most intense color and should be used 
sparingly in decoration, as it needs great care as to 
the quality and quantity of other colors to balance 
it. Orange cadmium was used for the original of 
Plate XL 

' The Chemistry of Paints and Painting. 



Ill COLOR QUALITIES 37 

Red is a warm color and an advancing one. 
Goethe says, " The agreeable, cheerful sensation 
which red-yellow excites inct'eases to an intoler- 
ably powerful impression in bright yellow-red. 

" The active side is here in its highest energy, and 
it is not to be wondered at that impetuous, robust, 
uneducated men should be especially pleased with / 
this color. Among savage nations the inclination! 
for it has been universally remarked, and when 
children left to themselves begin to use tintsl 
(paints), they never spare vermilion and minium. 

" In looking steadfastly at a perfectly yellow-red 
surface, the color seems actually to penetrate the 
organ. It produces an extreme excitement, and 
still acts thus when somewhat darkened. A yellow- 
red (scarlet) cloth disturbs and enrages animals. 
I have known men of education to whom its effect 
was intolerable if they chanced to see a person 
dressed in a scarlet cloak on a gray, cloudy day." 
In nature we have red only in small portions, a 
few red birds or those with throats or spots of red ; 
almost no butterflies, but many flowers. The rose, 
which leads in beauty the long procession of flowers, 
contains an immense scale of this color on the violet 
side, from the palest blush to the deepest crimson, 
almost purple. There being less of red in nature 
than of any other color, it becomes by contrast the 
decorative color. It has also the quality of chang- 



38 ' COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

ing less with lessening light than any other color, 
and is particularly fine in combination with blue. 
Vermilion and carmine were used to make the 
spectral red of Plate XIV, though they are far from 
reproducing the vivid quality of the original. Ver- 
milion used with oil is much more permanent 
than with water. Of the lakes, Church says in his 
Chemistry of Paints and Painting : " No artist 
who cares for his work, and hopes for its per- 
manency, should ever employ them." 

There is another quality shown in Plate III by 
which colors may be divided into the warm and 
cold classes. The six spectral colors we have so far 
been studying in this chapter may be roughly 
divided as follows : 



COLD. 


WARM. 


Violet 


Yellow 


Blue 


Orange 


Green 


Red 



although some varieties of green may be classed 
among the cold colors because of the large amount 
of blue they seem to contain, and others may be 
classed among the warm ones from their seeming 
to contain so large an amount of yellow. 

It is well to remember that cold colors seem to 
retire or go back from the eye, while the warm 
ones seem to come forward, and that the right use 



in COLOR QUALITIES 39 

of these qualities greatly affects architecture and 
decoration. (See Plates V and VI.) 

To recapitulate, we have first, three qualities or 
constants of colors: hue, purity, luminosity; then 
the qualities of being warm or cold. Following 
upon these are divisions of the tones into three 
other groups or scales of tints, shades, and gray 
or broken tints. 

These scales have been confined to six for the 
sake of simplicity, but the reader may multiply 
them infinitely to correspond with the infinite gra- 
dations in nature. 

1. Tints. — "The reduced scale — that is, the 
normal hue mixed with progressive increments (ad- 
ditions) of white, thus forming tints." The spectral 
hue of the color weakened by white. Plate VII. 

2. Shades. — "The darkened scale — that is, 
the normal hue mixed with progressive increments 
(additions) of black, thus forming shades." The 
spectral hue of the color darkened with black. 
Plate VIII. 

3. "The dulled scale — that is, the normal hue 
mixed with progressive increments of gray, thus 
forming broken tints commonly called grays." 
The spectral hue of the color changed by black and 
white. Besides these regular scales which can be 
approximately rendered in paint or colored inks 
there is an infinite variety of what we might call 



40 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

irregular scales which can never be given save in 
nature. They are those in which a color is changed 
or neutralized by one or more of the other colors. 
These cannot even be named, for their multitude. 

With the aid of a color wheel on which he used 
disks of black, white, and the six prismatic colors. 
Professor Rood has drawn up and formulated the 
proportions of 488 of these compound or neutral- 
ized colors. With the formulse a number of them 
have been printed in color quite successfully. It 
is probably the first attempt to establish standard 
colors, and a most valuable one, which it is hoped 
may bear fruit. If those and the arbitrary terms 
for colors and their different states could come 
into general use it would greatly help all descrip- 
tions of color harmonies. 

Having become familiar with the six colors, we 
now arrive at the object for which we have gone 
through the previous study; namely, the first 
kind of color harmony, one-color combinations, also 
called combinations of self-tones, the simplest 
and the preliminary harmony to that of combined 
colors. The_first_ru]e to be observed in making 
one-color combinations is to avoid putting together 
what we may call, borrowing the term from the 
language of music, the large intervals, or extremes, 
of a color in their pure spectral hues. For. ex- 
ample, in arranging a basket of flowers, never put 



Ill COLOR QUALITIES 41 

those of a crimson or violet-red, such as an Ameri- 
can Beauty rose, next to a scarlet or orange-red 
flower, such as a scarlet geranium. These are too 
unlike each other, being at the large intervals of 
the hue. They injure each other and are therefore 
disagreeable. 

As a second rule, all colors, even those above- 
named7lnay~be combined in one harmony, but 
this harmony must be produced from the fact that 
tints, or shades, or both combined, are used, rather 
than the simple spectral hues. In fact, nature 
uses pure colors most sparingly ; they appear, if 
you will remember, in small bright spots in jewels, 
in somewhat larger quantities in flowers and fruit, 
in the wings of butterflies and the plumage of 
birds, to relieve and ornataent the more subdued 
great masses of neutral greens and grays that 
make up the ordinary garb of nature. 

But to return to the combinations of larger in- 
tervals of color we were considering. For instance, 
while scarlet (orange-red) and crimson (violet- 
red) do not combine well, at a French searshore 
resort was seen the combination of a pink (that 
is, a tint of violet-red) dress, shaded by a brilliant 
scarlet (orange-red) parasol carried by its wearer. 
It was as daring a combination as could be made ; 
its success was complete owing to the pale tint of 
the dress and the correspondingly correct hue of 



42 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

the scarlet of the parasol. The effect was helped 
and complemented by the large mass of the sea as 
background. No rule can prescribe these tints or 
shades exactly, a gifted eye only can combine them 
with success ; but the fact might serve as a hint 
to those who find by examination and experiment 
that they have such an eye. 

Besides the use of tints and shades to help us in 
combining what would otherwise be inharmonious 
color, gradation is another means we can employ 
to serve our purpose. For instance, considering 
different blues, which are not agreeable together, 
we will look at a cloudless sky ; we find that 
above us it may be of a deep blue verging on 
violet blue, while, as we let the eye follow it 
down through the infinite and exquisite gradations 
it contains, near the horizon we come gently 
upon our other blue, the greenish one, and feel no 
discord. The rainbow, which is, in fact, a kind of 
spectrum, is the best possible example of the great 
use of gradation ; there we have all the pure colors, 
one differing immensely from the other, but the 
gradations between them are so fine and complete 
as to prevent the least discord. In opals and pearl 
shells, in peacock's feathers and soap bubbles, 
such coloring is also seen enhanced by being 
broken by soft grays and greens. It is caused by 
what is scientifically called interference ; that is. 



ni COLOR QUALITIES 43 

the thin layers of the material interfere or break 
up the waves of light and so produce the color. 

Reflection in colored materials can be used to 
help greatly in harmonizing them. Look at a 
piece of red sealing-wax. Hold it up by a window 
and the high gloss on it will reflect so much light 
as to make the side toward the light appear almost 
white. On another side the true or local color, the 
brilliant red, will be seen, and the .side ia shadow 
will be of another color still, darker and more 
crimson or violet-red. Red satin will have the 
same varieties in its high lights, middle, and shaded 
parts, and these whiter lights and shaded parts 
really gray and subdue the color of the material. A 
woollen cloth of the same color which has less power 
of reflection will therefore have less of the gray 
about it. With practice, fine and beaiitiful one- 
color combinations, greatly varied, can be made 
by using materials of different textures but of the 
same color. 

What has been said so far of colors applies to 
them as seen in ordinary daylight, but we must 
also know how they are . affected by lessened, in- 
creased, and artificial light. Rood made many 
elaborate experiments in this direction, too numer- 
ous to be given here. With these in view. Church 
gives the following table of the main changes that 
occur in colored objects from the changing of the 
light in which they are commonly seen : 



44 



COLOR PROBLEMS 



CH. 



Increase, — 


Diminish. 


Scarlet . 


. . Purplish. 


Orange . 


. . Eed. 


Yellow . 


. . Brown. 


Paler . 


. . Olive-green. 


Yellower 


Greener. 


More blue 


. . Greener. 


Blue . 


. . More violet. 


More blue 


. . Purple. 


Eedder 


. . More violet. 



If Light 
Red becomes 

Scarlet 
Orange 
Yellow 
Yellow-green 
Blue-green 

Art'f. ultramarine becomes Blue 
Violet 
Purple 



We must also note the effect produced by double 
light ; as, for instance, at sunset when we find in 
one direction the cool light from the blue of the 
sky, in another the warm light from the setting sun. 
This is more complicated and difficult to understand. 

Reflections from near objects produce similar 
effects ;. as, for instance, in the city, the light 
reflected from a red brick wall and that from a 
blue sky. An artist painted a portrait in which 
the likeness was spoiled by the unnatural amount 
of red in the complexion. On examination it was 
found to have been put there rightly, inasmuch as 
the artist certainly saw it ; the error lay in choos- 
ing a place for the subject where the red reflection 
from a brick wall was thrown on his face. In a 
room, a yellow wall paper and a curtain of some 
other color may throw combined and confusing 
though perhaps at the same time most interesting 
reflections on some object. The combined effects 
of daylight and gas or lamp light are similar. 



ni 



COLOR QUALITIES 



45 



We will next consider the effect upon colored 
objects of a light, itself colored, — of what is called 
a dominant light. (See Plate VI, with instruc- 
tions.) 

Chevreul made many experiments with these. 
Chitrch gives them to us, with modifications, in the 
following concise form : 

Red rays falling on white make it appear red- 



Orange 



Yellow 



Green 



i(- It 


red 


li 


il 


deeper red. 


ii « 


orange 


a 


11 


redder. 


« « 


yellow 


a 


ii 


orange. 


u a 


green 


ii 


li 


yellowish-gray. 


a « 


blue 


a 


It 


violet. 


££ a 


violet 


(C 


il 


purple. 


a It 


Hack 


(( 


11 


rusty black. 


11 li 


white 


a 


11 


orange. 


It 11 


red 


iC 


li 


reddish-orange. 


a u 


orange 


il 


il 


deeper orange- 


U li 


yellow 


ii 


It 


orange-yellow. 


ic U 


green 


li 


li 


dark yellow-green. 


it u 


blue 


11 


il 


dark reddish-gray. 


ii ii 


violet 


11 


11 


dark purplish-gray. 


ii ii 


black 


" 


(( 


brownish-black. 


ii « 


white 


cc 


il 


yellow. 


ii ii 


red 


11 


ii 


orange-brown. 


ii ii 


orange 


11 


ii 


orange-yellow. 


ii ii 


yellow 


li 


il 


deeper yellow. 


ii " 


green 


li 


li 


yellowish-green. 


ii ii 


blue 


ii 


tl 


slaty-gray. 


ii ii 


violet 


ii 


il 


purplish-gray. 


ii ii 


black 


ii 


ii 


olive-black. 


li ii 


white 


" 


ii 


green. 



46 



COLOR PROBLEMS 



CH. 



Green rays falling on red make it appear yellowish-brown. 



(i 


tt 


" orange 


il 


a 


grayish-leaf-green. 


et 


it 


" yellow 


Ci 


(( 


yellowish-green. 


(( 


li 


" green 


a 


il 


deeper green. 


tt 


tl 


" blue 


IC 


li 


bluish-green. 


t( 


It 


" violet ' 


iC 


a 


bluish-gray. 


i( 


it 


" black 


(( 


a 


dark greenish-gray. 


Blue 


li 


" white 


a 


li 


blue. 


tt 


li 


" red 


a 


a 


purple. 


" 


" 


" orange 


li 


il 


plum-brown. 


u 


li 


" yellow 


u 


li 


yellowish-gray. 


" 


tl 


" green 


li 


il 


bluish-green. 


CC 


it 


" blue 


li 


a 


deeper blue. 


(i 


IC 


" violet 


" 


i( 


bluer. 


it 


It 


" black 


" 


il 


bluish-black. 


Violet 


tt 


" white 


a 


a 


violet. 


tt 


it 


" red 


li 


il 


purple. 


ti 


tl 


" orange 


" 


*' 


reddish-gray. 


it 


C£ 


" yellow 


cc 


a 


purplish-gray. 


tc 


C£ 


" green 


u 


" 


bluish-gray. 


tt 


It 


" blue 


- 


ec 


bluish-violet. 


tc 


It 


" violet 


li 


li 


deeper violet. 


i' 


it 


" black 


u 


ii 


violet-black. 



In this table the effect of yellow light gives us 
the effect of gas or lamp light on colors, as they 
are yellow in character. To make his experiments 
with artificial light as sure as possible, Rood, or 
Chevreul, in daylight, threw the light from a gas 
burner on colors set in a camera so as to judge at 
the same time of the effects of the two kinds of 
light, for we must remember that commonly when 
we see colors by gas or lamp light we are so sur- 



m COLt)!!, QUALITIES 47 

rounded ourselves by the same yellow light that 
everything is tinged by it, and our judgment 
is affected ; all we see being yellower, yellow 
objects will look less yellow for want of the con- 
trast seen in daylight. This effect is now under- 
stood and provided for by dry goods merchants, 
who have for som'e time shown materials for 
evening dresses in rooms lighted by gas. A fairly 
good idea of the appearance which pictures, col- 
ored materials, articles of dress and decoration will 
make by gas or lamp light can be had by looking 
,at them through a piece of pale orange-yellow 
glass. 

Electric and calcium lights, being much whiter 
than that of gas or oil, make less difference in 
colors, but their intensity being different from 
'that of ordinary diffused daylight, it produces dif- 
ferent and more intense effects. 



CHAPTER IV 

CONTRASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 

GIVEN a certain amount of any color, say 
normal or spectral red, and wishing to make 
it looii as bright as it can, what color shall we 
put with it, and how much of that color, to attain 
ovir purpose ? To answer that question correctly, 
having in the last chapter studied the harmony 
possible in what have been called self-tones, or 
one-color combinations, we will take up contrasts, 
of which we have several kinds, a^s follows : 

Simultaneous contrasts of tone, neutral. 
Simultaneous contrasts of color on neutral 

grounds. 
Successive contrasts. 
Mixed contrasts. 
Contrasts of complements. 
Contrasts of other hues or lesser contrasts. 
Contrasts of brightness. 
Contrasts of purity. 
Contrasts of cold and warm colors. 

The first point to understand clearly is the law 
of simultaneous contrast of tone as studied and 

48 



CH. IV CONTRASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 49 

written about by Chevreul in his elaborate work 
on color. Church, explains this law : " Contrast 
caused by difference in brightness is commonly 
called contrast of tone. This kind of contrast 
may occur alone or it may be associated with con- 
trast of hue and contrast of purity. It will be 
well to consider first the simplest cases, in which 
contrast of tone is not accompanied by other con- 
trasts. It is impossible, however, to reduce 
experiments on tone-contrast to their simplest 
expressions, because a third element always comes 
in, namely, the background on which the pair of 
tones is placed for examination. Whether this 
background be black, white, gray, or colored, it 
must necessarily differ in some one direction from 
one or both the trial pieces, and will therefore itself 
produce a contrast. To minimize the complication 
thus introduced we may try an experiment for 
producing the phenomena of tone-contrast in three 
ways, using three backgrounds with identical trial 
pieces on each. We first take two strips of light 
gray paper, A and A', in Plate XV, and place them 
a few inches apart on a large sheet of (white) 
paper in • a good light. We then prepare two 
similar strips of a considerably darker shade of 
gray, B and B', and place them, as shown in the 
diagram, B' alongside of A' and the other the 
same distance from B' as A is from A'. On 



50 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

close observation it will be seen that A' close 
to B' appears lighter than A, which lies at 
some distance, Avhile B appears correspondingly 
darker than B'. The effect of contrast in en- 
hancing differences of tone may be studied 
thus : Make such openings, five in number, in a 
piece of card, as will serve to divide each of the 
strips A and B into three portions. When viewed 
through this card, held between the trial pieces and 
the eye, it will be found that the two adjoin- 
ing parts of the strip are most contrasted in tone, 
and the others less so in proportion to their dis- 
tance from the line of contact. The experiment 
should now be repeated with a background of 
black velvet, and again with a background of gray 
paper lighter in tone than either of the strips. 
The effect of contrast of tone is still better 
seen in a series of toned strips placed next each 
other. In such a case the effect on all tlie strips 
save the end ones is that of double contrast. For 
the second strip or second tone has one side of it 
made apparently darker by reason of the conti- 
guity of the lighter tone of strip, while the other 
side seems lighter, owing to the contiguity of the 
darker tone of strip 3. The general result of 
these double contrasts is that the whole series or 
scale of tones gives the appearance of a number 
of hollows, although, in fact, the apparent hollows 



IV CONTRASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 51 

are perfectly flat areas of uniform shade. The 
effect of this experiment is approximately repre- 
sented in Plate XV, where the real flatness of each 
tone of the six may be verified by covering up all 
the others by a card. Tones of any one color instead 
of gray may be thus employed to illustrate this 
kind of simultaneous contrast, but its characteristic 
effect is not seen unless the contrasting tones differ 
considerably in intensity, increase by regular gra- 
dations, and are near each other, or in absolute 
contact. However, if tones of a color, whether 
in tints or shades, be used, there is generally a 
complication introduced, owing to the difficulty of 
getting a series of such tones which shall be the 
same in hue. 

" This phenomenon of simultaneous contrast of 
tone of course largely affects ... all draw- 
ings in black and white and in monochrome." 

Following upon the law of simultaneous con- 
, trast of tone is the law of simultaneous contrast of 
color formulated by Chevreul, as follows : " In 
the case where the eye sees at the same time two 
contiguous (or adjoining) colors, they will appear 
as dissimilar as possible, both in their optical com- 
position and the height of their tone. We have, 
then, at the same time simultaneous contrast of 
color, properly so called, and contrast of tone." 
Plate XVI gives the simplest examples of this simul- 



52 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

taneous contrast of color, the six spectral colors 
we have been studying on grounds of white, black, 
and gray. The colors seem brighter on the black 
ground and darker on the white, while with the 
gray the yellow alone is much affected, it seeming 
to grow brighter. The following plates (Nos. 
XVII, XVIII, and XIX) give the same coloring, 
but reversed, the white, black, and gray being in 
spots or disks on the six colored grounds. By 
covering the squares on Plate XIX. with the pre- 
pared sheet of paper having a square opening just 
large enough to allow but one of its six divisions 
to be seen at a time, we shall find that each one 
of the disks or spots looks, not pure gray, but 
tinged with another color. This result gives us 
our first hint of what is called a complementary 
color. In the case of the gray on blue the gray 
will appear rusty or yellowish, yellow being the 
complement of blue ; the gray on yellow will 
appear bluer, blue being the complement of yellow ; 
on the green the gray will look purplish-red, on 
the orange greenish-blue, on red bluish-green, and 
on the violet yellowish-green. 

Black lace over colors is always affected by them 
in a similar way. Over yellow, its complement 
being blue, the lace will look at its best, that is, 
blackest ; over blue, the lace will tend to yellow, 
and will lose something of its strength and the 



IV CONTRASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 53 

fulness of its black; over greens, it will partake 
of their complement, red, and tend to look rusty. 

In connection with, this tinging of black with 
the color complementary to that of the color of the 
ground on which it is placed, Chevreul tells an 
interesting anecdote. A manufacturer was given 
black and colored wools with which to make some 
goods, the pattern to be black on colored grounds. 
"When they were delivered the man who had or- 
dered the goods complained that he had not been 
given the same black wool, that the blacks were 
not pure and clear. The manufacturer declared 
he had used the same wools. A lawsuit followed, 
in the course of which Chevreul was called upon 
to give his testimony as to color, when he proved 
that, according to the law of simultaneous contrast 
of color, the black wool was the same, but when 
woven in figures, as for instance, black on blue, the 
complementary color to blue, namely, yellow, being 
called up by the eye, made the black look a rusty- 
brownish black instead of pure clear black. He 
added that the only way to make the black on blue 
look pure would be to color it with a little of the 
blue frio as to overcome its yellowish complement. 

This delicate impression of the color comple- 
mentary to the one we are looking at, is called 
up involuntarily by the eye, of which the nerve 
fibrils become fatigued by the strong color, and 



54 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

incline to see the extreme opposite or complemen- 
tary color. The complement of a color may also 
be called up or produced by looking fixedly at a 
round spot like that on Plate XX for some time. 
After a while there may be seen a faint image 
of its complement on the white paper around it. 
A still better way of seeing the complement of a 
color is by looking fixedly for some time at a 
disk of the selected color placed on white paper 
(Use Plate XXI) ; then suddenly slip a sheet of 
white paper over it, and, continuing to look at the 
place where it was, the same-sized image of its com- 
plement will be seen. Here we have the answer 
to the question at the beginning of the chapter : 
bluish-green is the color complementary to spectral 
red. The eye becomes tired with looking at the 
red, and the nerve fibrils excited by it incline 
to see its complement, bluish-green. We can, 
however, prove this conclusion most correctly by 
means of what are called Maxwell's disks. If 
we cut out a disk or circular piece of cardboard 
and paint it spectral red, then cut a second one 
just like it but paint it bluish-green, cutting a slit 
in both from the edge to the middle so we can 
slip one into the other as shown in Plate XXII, 
and then turn them rapidly, the color in both will 
seem to fade away until, when turning fast enough, 
we shall see no color at all, — simply a complete 



iv CONTRASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 55 

disk of light gray. That result proves that spectral 
red and bluish-green are true complements of each 
other, because a certain number of parts of red neu- 
tralize a certain number of parts of bluish-green. 
If, instead of using paints and paper we were able 
to use colored light, the result would be eten 
better ; we should* have white light as the result 
of mixing the red and the bluish-green in the 
right quantities. Pigments are so dull or non- 
luminous compared with light that -with, them we 
can only produce gray, or as it has been called, 
dark white, or white in shadow. To be quite sure 
that we have gray, let us add in front of our disks 
two smaller ones of black and white, and we will 
find the gray produced by the. mixture of the 
black and white to match perfectly the gray made 
of spectral red and bluish-green. To measure the 
quantity of each color necessary, we can put be- 
hind the two disks a white disk that is not slit, 
the circumference of which is divided, as in Plates 
XXII, XXIII, XXIV, XXV, and XXVI, into 
one hundred parts. These are plates of the six 
specified spectral colors with their complements. 
The numbers give the quantity in one-hundredths 
of each color. The " number of luminosity " 
means the quantity of white in proportion to 
black, in one-hundredths, necessary to make the 
gray of that particular degree. 



56 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

The Milton-Bradley Company, of Springfield, 
Massachusetts, make an excellent little machine, 
including several sets of disks of different sizes and 
good colors, and a stout frame on which to put the 
disks, with a crank by which to turn them. It can 
be set up and screwed on a table, so tliat any one 
can make for himself these delightful experiments. 
Plate XXVII is an illustration of this machine. 
There is hardly a limit to the number of the other 
complementary colors that can be made with this 
set of disks. Study of this set of complementary 
colors is most important as a foundation for all 
contrasts. Experiment has also proved that colors 
have more than one complement. 

" Complementary colors of full brightness and 
purity afford the most striking examples of the 
effect called contrast. When each of a pair of such 
colors differs as much as possible from its fellow 
in hue, but is of the same degree of brightness, it 
is found, while the brightness of both is enhanced, 
that the hue of both is unchanged by the close 
neighborhood or contiguity of the two colors. But 
if the pair be not truly complementary, or if in 
brightness or purity one color differ from the 
other, then such difference will not be seen exactly 
as it is, but such dissimilarity as exists, whether it 
be of one hue, of purity, or of brightness, will be 
increased or enhanced by juxtaposition. This is 



rv CONTRASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 57 

the primary law of contrast, which embraces three 
varieties dependent respectively upon differences 
as to the three constants of color, namely, hue, 
putity, and brightness (or luminosity). If two 
adjacent colors differ in brightness, that which is 
the brighter, or, in other words, the more luminous, 
will increase in brightness, while the less luminous 
will have its brightness diminished. If two adja- 
cent colors differ in hue, such difference will be 
increased, each hue tending to change as if it had 
been mixed with the complementary of the other. 
In the case of complementaries no increase of 
difference in hue is, however, possible." ^ 

Plate XXVIII shows us the six spectral colors 
with their complements, not in quantity, but as a 
table. After thorough study of this table of first 
and simplest contrasts, the practical advantage of 
Plates XXII to XXVI will be apparent. To make 
it easier we give Plate XXIX, which shows the same 
set of complements. Here they are arranged in a 
circle in which each color is opposite its own com- 
plement. This circle leads us from the strongest 
contrasts of complements to lesser contrasts. This 
should also be studied till it can be remembered for 
future reference.. Being in simple spectral colors, 
it is easier than the more numerous tints of shades 
of neutralized colors, and is also a key for under- 

1 Church. 



58 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

standing and classifying them. It is well here to 
note how many complements are green or greenish 
in hue. 

Concerning the law of simultaneous contrast, 
with regard more especially to lesser contrasts, 
Rood says : " When any two colors of the chro- 
matic circle are brought into competition or con- 
trasted, the effect produced is apparently to move 
them both farther apart. In the case, for exam- 
ple, of orange and yellow, the orange is moved 
toward the red, and assumes the appearance of 
reddish-orange ; the yellow moves toward the 
green, and appears for the time to be greenish- 
yellow. Colors which are complementary are 
already as far apart in the chromatic circle as pos- 
sible ; hence they are not changed in hue, but 
merely appear more brilliant and saturated." 
Plate XXX will be found of great assistance^ in 
comparing pairs of colors with each other. Here 
we have a diagram of a chromatic circle. By 
placing over it the transparent color screen found 
at the end of the book, and moving it slowly in 
the same direction, it will be seen that red when 
contrasted with greenish-blue causes this last color 
to move away from the centre of the circle in 
a straight line ; hence, as the new point is on 
the same diameter, but farther from the centre, 
we know that the greenish-blue is not made more 

' See note on page 72. 



IV CONTRASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 59 

or less blue or green, but is _ simply caused to ap- 
pear more saturated or brilliant. The new point 
for the red lies also on the same diameter, but is 
nearer the centre of the circle ; that is, the color 
remains red, but appears duller or less saturated. 
Experience confirms this. Tf a considerable num- 
ber of pieces of red cloth, for example, are ex- 
amined' in succession, the last one will appear 
duller and inferior in brilliancy to the others, but 
it will still appear red. Proceeding with the 
examination of the effects produced on the other 
colors, we find that the orange has been moved 
toward yellow and also toward the centre of the 
circle ; hence our diagram tells us that red, when 
put into competition with orange, causes the latter 
to appear more yellowish and at the same time 
less intense. So we can go on comparing one 
color with another and find out the effect of each 
by moving the one circle over the other in differ- 
ent directions, always finding that the comple- 
ments as moved away from each other only grow 
more brilliant but more changing in color. Church 
gives us a list of the changes due to the principal 
pairs of lesser contrasts from the observations of 
Chevreul, Rood, etc., as follows : 

(It may be remarked that this table of changes 
as here given is more easily understood than in its 
original form as given by Church.) 



60 COLOR PROBLEMS ci 

Change due to Simul- 

Paies of Colors. taneous Contrast. 

Eed with orange inclines to purple. 

Orange with, red " yellow. 

Red with yellow " purple. 

Yellow with red " green. 

Eed with blue-green .... becomes more brilliant. 

Blue-green with red .... " " 

Red with blue inclines to orange. 

Blue with red " green. 

Red with violet " orange. 

Violet with red " blue. 

Red with purple " orange. 

Purple with red " blue. 

Orange with yellow " red. 

Yellow with orange " green. 

Orange with green " red. 

Green with orange .... " blue-green. 

Orange-yellow with turquoise . becomes more brilliant. 

Turquoise with orange-yellow . " " 

Orange with violet inclines to yellow. 

Violet with orange " blue. 

Orange with purple " yellow. 

Purple with orange " blue. 

Yellow with green " orange. 

Green with yellow " blue-green. 

Yellow with turquoise .... " orange. 

Turquoise with yellow .... " blue. 

Yellow with blue becomes more brilliant. 

Blue with yellow " " 

Green with blue inclines to yellow-green 

Blue with green " violet. 

Green with violet " yellow-green 

Violet with green " purple. 



IV CONTRASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 61 

Chanse dub to Simcl- 
Paies of Colors. taneous Contrast. 

Green with purple ..... becomes more brilliant. 

Purple with, green ..... " " 

Blue with violet Inclines to green. 

Violet with blue " purple. 

Violet with purple « blue. 

Purple with violet « red. 

"It must not be imagined that tlie changes 
enumerated in the above table are at all equal to 
one another in amount. We have, indeed, always 
some change, but it varies much in the case of 
different pairs. When the chromatic interval (on 
the color-circle) is small, then the change of hue, 
in virtue of simultaneous contrast, is large ; when 
the interval is large the change of hue is slight, 
but it is accompanied by change of brightness ; 
when the interval is as large as possible there is 
no change of hue, but the brightness of both hues 
is increased." 

After simultaneous contrasts Chevreul gives us 
successive contrasts, which latter " may be observed 
when we tire one set of retinal fibrils Mj gazing 
for some time on a surface of a very decided color 
and brightness. Afterward, on looking at a color- 
less surface of white, gray, or black, it will be 
found to be tinctured with the complementary of 
the first color." If we stare at a piece of bright 
red paper and then look at white paper we will 



62 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

see blue-green, the complement of the red. So, if 
we look at a series of pieces of red cloth the first 
will appear the brightest, the second less so, the 
third still less, but if the eye is rested by looking 
at a piece of bluish-green cloth the red will then 
be seen of its original brightness. When a black 
spot laid on red cloth is looked at steadily for 
some time, then is taken suddenly away, the 
place where the black spot was will appear to 
be of a brighter red than that around it on account 
of the less fatigue there has been to that part 
of the retina. A salesman who understood com- 
plementary colors could use this law of suc- 
cessive contrasts with great effect in showing 
goods. 

Still another form of contrast is called mixed 
contrast. " The distinction of simultaneous and 
successive contrast renders it easy to comprehend 
a phenomenon which we may call mixed con- 
trast ; because it results from the fact that Jhe 
eye, having seen for a time a certain color, acquires 
an aptitude to see for another period the com- 
plementary of that color and also a new color, 
presented to it by an exterior object ; the sensation 
then perceived is that which results from this new 
color and the complementary of the first. The 
following is a very simple method of observing this 
mixed contrast : One eye being closed, the right 



IV CONTRASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 63 

for instance, let the left eye regard fixedly a piece 
of paper of the color A; when this color appears 
dimmed, immediately direct the eye upon a sheet 
of paper colored B ; then we have the impress^ion 
which results from the mixture of this color B with 
the complementary color, C, of the color A. To be 
satisfied of this mixed impression it is sufficient 
to close the left eye, and to look at the color B 
with the right: not only is the impression that 
produced by the color B, but it may appear modi- 
fied in a direction contrary to the mixed impression 
C+B, or, what comes to the same thing, it appears 
to be more A+B." ^ 

That the complementary of a color exists in its 
shadow may be seen by watching a stretch of snow 
when the sun is hidden by a cloud : the snow is 
white, the shadow gray.' When the cloud passes 
away, the light on the snow makes it look yellow ; 
the shadow will 'also be seen to be more or less 
blue as the atmosphere is more or less clear and 
free from the moisture which veils the sunlight. 
The same result in a greater or less degree exists 
in all shadows, which shows how useful study of 
the complementary colors is for painters. 

The purple or , violet shadows of the " impres- 
sionists " are in many cases exaggerations. On 
snow, dust, or sand, violet shadows are to be found 

^ Cbevreul. 



64 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

in certain conditions of the atmosphere, but " im- 
pressionists " often do not seem to take into suffi- 
cient account the color called by artists " local 
color" of the substance or material on which the 
shadow is thrown, or the color of the sky reflected 
in the shadow. A true colorist detects these 
subtle varieties. An artist who has not a fine eye 
for color uses the pure colors given by scientists, 
thus making the crude, harsh pictures so much 
criticised. They are true to a great extent scien- 
tifically, but are cold and glaring, and without the 
true spirit of nature. 

In studying the complements of these six spec- 
tral hues we come across the theory that because 
a color and its complement together make white, 
therefore they must prove to be an agreeable 
harmony. Now, is that true ? At first sight we 
answer, No. We do know that if we wish to make 
a color as brilliant as possible, we must add to it 
its complement. Under certain circumstances 
that may give us a good result, but artistic taste 
declares that a pure spectral color and its com- 
plement make a combination so strong and vivid 
as almost to amount to crudeness, and to jar on 
a sensitive eye. StUl, the theory that comple- 
mentary colors make a true and perfect harmony 
is well considered in the following extract from 
Eastlake : 



rv CONTRASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 65 

"Every treatise on, the harmonious combination 
of colors contains the diagram of the chromatic 
circle njore or less elaborately constructed. These 
diagrams, if intended to exhibit the contrasts pro- 
duced by the action and reaction of the retina/ 
have one common defect. The opposite colors are 
made equal in intensity ; whereas the comple- 
mental color pictured on the retina is always less 
vivid, and always darker or lighter than the 
original color. This variety undoubtedly accords 
more with harmonious effects in painting. 

" The opposition of two pure hues of equal in- 
tensity, differing only in the abstract quality of 
color, would immediately be pronounced crude and 
inharmonious. It would not, however, be strictly 
correct to say that such a contrast is too violent ; 
on the contrary, it appears the contrast is not 
carried far enough, for though differing in color, 
the two hues, may be exactly similar in purity and 
intensity. Complete contrast, on the other hand, 
supposes dissimilarity in all respects. In addition 
to the mere difference of hue, the eye, it seems, 
requires difference in the lightness or darkness of 
the hue. The spectrum of a color relieved as a 
dark on a light ground is a light color on a dark 
ground, and vice versa. Thus, if we look at a 
bright red wafer on the whitish surface, the com- 
plemental image will be still lighter than the 



66 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

white surface ; if the same wafer is placed on a 
black surface the comple mental image will be 
still darker. The color of both these spectra may 
be called greenish (bluish-green), but it is evident 
that a color must be scarcely appreciable as such, 
if it is lighter than white and darker than black. 
It is, however, to be remarked, that the white 
surface round the light greenish image seems 
tinged with a reddish hue, and the black surface 
round the dark image becomes slightly illuminated 
with the same color, thus in both cases assisting 
to render the image apparent. 

" The difficulty or impossibility of describing 
degrees of color in words has also had a tendency 
to mislead, by conveying the idea of more positive 
hues than the physiological contrast warrants. 
Thus, supposing scarlet to be relieved as a dark, 
the complemental color is so light in degree and 
so faint in color that it should be called a pearly 
gray ; whereas the theorists, looking at the quality 
of color abstractedly, would call it a green-blue, 
and the diagram would falsely present such a hue 
equal in intensity to scarlet, or as nearly equal as 
possible. 

"Even the difference of mass which good taste 
requires may be suggested by the physiological 
phenomena, for unless the complemental image is 
suffered to fall on a surface precisely as near to 



IV CONTRASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 67 

the eye as that on which the original color was 
displayed, it appears larger or smaller than the 
original object, and this in a rapidly increasing 
proportion. Lastly, the shape itself soon becomes 
changed. That vivid color demands the compara- 
tive absence of color, either on a lighter or darker 
scale, as its contrast, may be inferred again from 
the fact that bright colorless objects produce 
strongly colored spectra. In darkness the spec- 
trum, which is first white, or nearly white, is 
followed by red ; in light, the spectnmi, which is 
first black, is followed by green. All color, as the 
author observes, is to be considered as half light, 
inasmuch as it is in every case lighter than black 
and darker than white. Hence no contrast of 
color with color, or even of color with black 
or white, can be so great (as regards lightness or 
darkness) as the contrast of black and white, or 
dark and light abstractedly. This ' distinction 
between the differences of degree and the differ- 
ences of kind is important, since a just application 
of contrast in color may be counteracted by an 
undue difference in lightness or darkness. The 
mere contrast of color is happily employed in some 
of Guido's lighter pictures, but if intense dark had 
been opposed to his delicate carnations, their com- 
parative whiteness would have been unpleasantly 
apparent. On the other hand, the flesh-color in 



68 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

Giorgione, Sebastian del Piombo (his best imitator), 
and Titian, was sometimes so extremely glowing 
that the deepest colors and black were indispen- 
sable accompaniments. The manner of Titian, as 
distinguished from his imitation of Giorgione, is 
golden rather than fiery, and his biographers are 
quite correct in saying that he was fond of oppos- 
ing red (lake) and blue to his flesh. The corre- 
spondence of these contrasts with the physiological 
phenomena will be immediately apparent, while 
the occasional practice of Rubens in opposing 
bright red to a still cooler flesh-color will be seen 
to be equally consistent. 

" It was before observed that the description of 
colors in words may often convey ideas of too 
positive a nature, and it may be remarked gener- 
ally that the colors employed by the great masters 
are, in their ultimate effect, more or less subdued 
or broken. The physiological contrasts are, how- 
ever, still applicable in the most comparatively 
neutral scale." 

Chevreul gives us in his book, Colour (a 
work published in 1835, which has gone through 
many editions and translations, having finally 
been edited and republished in 1889 by his son), an 
elaborate system of color contrasts based upon the 
older theory of three primary colors, red, yellow, 
and blue. There followed upon this in 1890 one 



IV CONTRASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 69 

by Charles La Couture, Repertoire Chromatique, 
containing an ingenious and beautiful system of 
color scales also founded upon the Brewster theory 
of red, yellow, and blue as primary colors. Of 
these color charts it has been well said that they 
are only able to display the effects, not of mixing 
colored light, but colored pigments. 

The following are rules to be used in regard to 
contrasting colors : 

Rule I. — A pair of complementary colors in 
their pure spectral tones in the proportions in 
which they neutralize or complement each other, 
as in Plates XXII to XXVI, should only be iTsed 
if you wish to produce a bold, striking, perhaps 
harsh effect ; or if you wish to create a focus in 
your picture, your room, or your decoration. In 
the latter case it will be well to soften the effect 
(especially in the case of a picture) by repeating 
the same colors in tints or shades in some other 
part of the work. 

Rule II. — Harmony of contrast -exists only in 
proportion to the changes in quality or quantity 
in equal portions of pure spectral tones. 

Rule III — The more neutral you make the 
tint or shade of one of the pair of complements, 
so much the more may you add to its quantity. 
For instance, a small quantity of bright spectral 
red will balance a large quantity of pale blue-green. 



70 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

Rule IV. — By using two or more tints, or 
shades and tints, of one of the pair of comple- 
ments, so much the finer becomes the harmony. 
The artist Turner sent to an exhibition of the 
Royal Academy in England a marine which was 
accepted and hung, but which, being a quiet pict- 
ure consisting mainly of pale, grayish sea-greens, 
attracted little attention. On varnishing day, 
however, he went to the Academy and painted in 
the foreground of his picture a scarlet buoy, when 
to the surprise of every one, owing to the correct 
balance of the quality and quantity of his comple- 
mentary contrast, the scarlet and blue green so 
intensified each other that the picture became a 
striking one, dulling the others around it and 
drawing constant admiration. 

From a dinner table set out at a flower show in 
the Madison Square Garden, which took a first 
prize, Plate XXXI is taken. It was a harmony 
of yellow and blue. 

1. Yellow chrysanthemums. 

2. Yellow lamp-.shades. 

3. Yellow satin centrepiece. 

4. Yellow candies. 

5. Yellow candies. 

6. Yellow candies. 

7. Yellow-brown almonds. 



IV CONTRASTS AND COMPLEMENTS 71 

' 8. Gold ornament on glass, china, and candies. 
9. Dark purple-blue grapes. 

In this case some of the yellow vas in pure 
spectral tones, the blue very strong, dark, and 
neutralized. 

Rule VI. — The finest harmony of contrast will 
be found where tints and shades of both the pair 
of complements can be combined. Then a small 
amount of both in spectral tones mny be introduced 
to give accent to the rest. Plate XXXII gives a 
blue and yellow harmony taken from an English 
china cup composed of two blues and two yellows, 
both neutral. The ground, being of a pale tint of 
yellow, is greater in quantity according to Rule 
III. The dainty pattern painted on it is in the 
two blues ; the delicate stems holding and uniting 
the conventional leaves and flowers are of brown 
(or dark yello'v^). The brown, being the darkest 
color, is the smallest in quantity, as the harmony 
is intended to be light and cheerful. 

Harmonies in blue and yellow have been used 
with great success in old decoration, when blended, 
modified, and interchanged with each other, and 
are one of the most useful combinations of colors 
that can be made. They are largely used in 
Italian and Spanish tiles and other porcelains. 
They are complementary colors strongly opposed 



72 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. iv 

to each, other, but the reason for their being more 
agreeable than other pairs of complements seems 
to arise from the fact that one, the yellow, is 
so much more luminous (or lighter) than the other 
that it affords a greater contrast than appears in 
the other pairs of complements. 

Rule VII. — Even pure spectral colors may be 
used with good effect by blending them in small 
portions, as in what are technically called diaper 
patterns. 

We have still a further power of adding to our 
harmony of contrasts by the use of different mate- 
rials, such as paper, paint, plaster, silk, satin, 
velvet, plush, and metals, in which the variety of 
surfaces gives an infinite number of tones, absorb- 
ing and reflecting, etc. These will be considered 
in the chapter on color-harmonies, and seem 
really inexhaustible. In that chapter is given 
a list of pairs of the lesser coijtrasting colors, 
such as have been found by observation of historic 
color to be the most agreeable to the eye. 



Note Asa transparent screen colored like Plate XXIX. could not 

\rell be made, Plate XXX. has been added. It represents the same 
color circle as Plate XXIX. except that the colors are only given by 
name. The reader la to suppose that Plate XXX. and the celluloid 
screen at the end of the book are the same as Plate XXIX. and then 
follow the directions for use given on page 58. 



CHAPTER V 

OOLOR-HAEMONIES 
HARMONIES OF COMPLEX OR VARIOUS COLORS 

IT is said that the use of agreeable and harmoni- 
ous colors tends to the sanity of the whole 
body by strengthening the nerves ; so much so, 
that part of the treatment of insane patients in a 
European asylum consists in surrounding them 
with certain colors, and, probably, of changing 
these according to certain rules. From these facts 
we surely learn that there is reason beyond that 
of our mere enjoyment of colors to lead us to 
study color harmonies. 

The most widely accepted division of these har- 
monies is that of Chevreul, who in his life of over 
one hundred years had time to formulate, revise, 
and amplify his laws of color, and, from his posi- 
tion as director of the manufacture of the Gobe- 
lin tapestries, great opportunities for experiment. 
The two chief groups, based respectively on anal- 
ogy and on contrast, are resolved into three sub- 
divisions each. These are quoted as follows from 
Church, who has added some explanations to them 
as given in The Law of Simultaneous Contrast : 

73 



74: COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

"I. HARMONIES OF ANALOGY. 

" II. HARMONIES OF CONTRAST. 

"1. The Harmony of Analogy of Scale. — This 
harmony is essentially that of a series, the har- 
mony of gradation. It includes those cases in 
which is presented a simultaneous view of three 
or more tones of the same scale, whether these 
tones be tints, or shades, or broken tones. It is 
obtained in various degrees of perfection, accord- 
ing to the number of tones present, and the value 
of the intervals between them. When the tones 
are not easily separable by the eye, and pass into 
one another, then the effect called • shading ' is 
produced. 

"2. The Harvionij of Analogy of Tones. — 
When twa or more tones of the same depth, or of 
very nearly, the same depth, but belonging to dif- 
ferent but related or neighboring scales, are viewed 
together, the harmony of tone is produced. Many 
such assortments are, however, displeasing to the 
educated eye, unless the tones be so selected as to 
fall into' a series with a gradually increasing quan- 
tity of some one of their color elements, when they 
may be arranged in the third kind of harmonies 
of analogy. 

" 3. The Harmony of a Dominant Hue. — An 
example of this harmony is afforded by viewing a 
contrasted color assortment, a bouq-uet of flowers, 



V COLOR-HARMONIES 75 

or even a landscape, through a piece of glass so 
slightly tinctured with a color as not to obliterate, 
but merely to modify, the various colors belonging 
to the atrangement or composition. 

" 1. The Harmony of Contrast of Scale is pro- 
duced by the simultaneous view of two or more 
distant tones of the same scale. 

" 2. The Harmony of Contrast of Tones is pro- 
duced by the simultaneous view of two or more 
tones of different depths belonging to neighboring 
or related scales. 

" 3. The Harmony of Contrast of Hue is pro- 
duced by the simultaneous view of colors belonging 
to distant scales, and assorted in accordance with 
the laws of contrast. This kind of contrast in- 
cludes also those cases in which the effect is still 
further enhanced by difference of tone as well as 
of color. 

" The distinction between these two classes or 
groups of harmonies is somewhat arbitrary, for 
the collocation of any two tones or any two colors, 
whether its results be agreeable or otherwise, inevi- 
tably involves the element of contrast. Color- 
harmonies, so far as- contrast is concerned, differ 
in degree and complexity, but Chevreul's harmo- 
nies of analogy pass by steps more or less marked ' 
into distinct and undoubted harmonies of contrast. 
In every harmony there is contrast of tone or of 



76 COLOR problp:ms ch. 

color, and therefore contrast cannot be employed 
as a criterion of classification. The two funda- 
mental ideas underlying complex color-harmonies 
may perhaps be expressed , as those of gradual 
change and of abrupt change. Instead of sepa- 
rating color-harmonies into two distinct groups, it 
would be better to arrange them in order upon 
the arc of a circle, pla;cing at one extremity those 
harmonies on which the succession of contiguous 
tones or hues is marked by the smallest differ- 
ences, and at the other extremity, those harmonies 
in which the elements of contrast are most strongly 
developed. About the middle of the arc will be 
arranged those transitional harmonies in which 
contrasts of tone, contrasts of color, and contrasts 
of tone and color combined, begin to make them- 
selves felt as modifying the effect of the regular 
sequence of tones and related hues. According to 
this scheme, we may commence with harmonies in 
which the succession of tones is so gentle as to 
be barely perceptible, and we may end with those 
harmonies in which the change of hue and of tone 
is most abrupt. A list of illustrative examples 
will help to elucidate the scheme : 

" 1. The passage, by insensible differences, of the 
tints, shades, or broken tones of a single hue from 
light to dark. 

" 2. The passage, by small but regular, definite, 



V COLOR-HARMONIES 77 

and perceptible steps, of the tints, shades, or 
broken tones of a single hue from light to dark. 

" 3. The passage, as in the preceding example 
(2), of the tones of one hue, from light to dark, 
when each step is separated by a neutral element, 
such as white, gray, or black. 

" 4 . The passage, by insensible differences, of one 
hue, or of the tones of one hue into another related 
hue, or its tones. 

" 5. The passage, by definite steps, of one hue, 
or of the tones of one hue, into another related hue 
or its tones. 

" 6. The passage, as above (5), of related hues 
into each other, each step separated by a neutral 
element. 

" 7. The passage, by insensible differences, of one 
hue into another chromatically remote hue. 

" 8. The passage, by definite steps, of one hue 
into another chromatically remote hue. 

■" 9. The passage, as above (8), of one hue into 
another, when each step is separated by a neutral 
element. 

"10. The collocation of distant tones. 

"11. The collocation of chromatically distant 
hues with or without the interposition of neutral 
elements. 

"It will be noticed how the idea of seriation 
or gradation becomes more and more involved 



78 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

with that of change as we follow the sequence of 
the above examples. Gradually the notion of 
orderly succession, of a regular series with the pres- 
ence of a pervading and dominant constituent, is 
lost by the abruptness of change caused by the 
introduction of foreign elements, or by the con- 
tiguity of distant tones and distant hues." 

As both of these sets of rules for harmonies of 
colors are so elaborate as to amount almost to color 
charts, and would be difficult and complicated to 
print in colors, for our practical purpose we will 
roughly divide harmonies of colors under three 
heads ; as follows (See Plate XXXIII) : 

Harmonies of one color. 
Harmonies of contrast (of color). 
Harmonies of complex or various colors. 

This division is not strictly correct, becaiise even in 
a harmony of one color the element of contrast 
will appear ; as, for instance, when we combine a 
pale tint of yellow, say straw color, with brown, 
which is a dark shade of yellow. As, however, in 
this case it is contrast of tone, not contrast of 
color, we will not let that interfere with the order 
of our arbitrary classification. The first class, 
harmonies of one color, have been considered in 
Chapter III. When simple, refined color is wanted 
in either dress or decoration, or where from in ex- 



V COLOR-HARMONIES 79 

perience one is afraid to combine cploi-s, it is best 
and safest to use this simplest kind of color 
harmony. With this class, as black and white are 
not colors, we will also include harmonies of one 
color combined with black, or white, or gray, or 
two or all three of these. From a book advertise- 
ment most successful in its clear, simple, and 
agreeable character we give Plate XXXIV. -It 
was on white paper, the proportions as follows : 
Most white, less black, least yellow, this latter 
always outlined with black. The white also 
showed through the yellow in some places and 
served to lighten the design. 

In decoration, when two tones of one color are 
used they are often separated with a fine line of 
white or black or gray. In Plate XXXV the 
useful effect of such a line of separation is, shown. 
A light tint on a dark shade does not so much 
need an outline, but, a dark shade on a light tint 
is much improved by white outlines. The white 
line increases the apparent strength of both tint 
and shade, while black will increase their bright- 
ness but diminish their purity. 

" In the consideration of the specific effects of 
the association of white, gray, or black with a 
single color, we follow the order in which the 
colors succeed each other in the spectrum, adding 
purple at the end. 



80 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

"1. Red. — Red with white becomes deeper, 
more saturated or purer, and less bright. The 
combination, as to intensity of contrast, is similar 
to that of green with white, being less than that 
of blue, violet, or purple with white, but more 
marked than that of orange or yellow with white. 

" Red with gray, when the latter is moderately 
pale, becomes brighter and less saturated, some- 
times acquiring an orange tinge. 

" 2. OfiAiirGE. — Orange with ivliite is rendered 
deeper, and perhaps a trifle more reddish. The 
contrast of tone between orange and white is much 
greater than that between yellow and white; the 
combination is consequently more effective. 

" Orange with gray, when the latter is pale, is 
deepened and reddened. With dark tones of gray 
orange becomes lighter. 

^^ Orange with &Zac^ becomes brighter and slightly 
yellower. 

"3. Yellow. — Yellow with ^o7^^^e is rendered 
deeper, less bright, and less advancing, acquiring 
a slight greenish hue. The lighter the tone of the 
yellow the less pleasing is the combination. 

" Tellow with (/ray is rendered brighter and per- 
haps slightly orange. The combination is satis- 
factory when the gray is rather dark. 

" Tellow with hlack is rendered paler, brighter, 
and more advancing. The combination affords 



V COLOR-HARMONIES 81 

the most intense contrast of tone next to that of 
white with black. The blackness of the black is 
modified by acquiring a slight bluish hue which 
enriches it. 

" 4. Geee"n. — Gh-een with wMte becomes deeper 
and purer ; the combination is capable of yielding 
very beautiful effects. 

" Green with gray becomes deeper only when the 
gray is pale ; if the gray be at all dark it acquires 
a purplish tinge. 

" Green with hlack is rendered brighter and paler, 
while the black suffers, being tinged with a red- 
dish or purplish hue. 

"5. Blue. — Blue with white constitutes a 
generally pleasing combination. The contrast of 
tone is very decided when the blue is at once pure 
and bright. The effect of strongly illuminated 
white clouds in deepening the tone of the blue of 
the sky bordering them is a good example of one 
of the chief characteristics of this combination; 
under such conditions the white often assumes a 
slightly yellowish tint. 

" Blue with gray. Gray, if pale, deepens and 
purifies blue ; the combination, though necessarily 
cold, is often most serviceable in pictorial as well 
as in ornamental art. 

'■^ Blue with Ulack. This combination is less 
agreeable than that of blue with gray, or of violet 



82 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

with black, especially when the tone of the blue 
is deep. Light tones of blue are made still paler, 
but broken tones more saturated, by contiguity 
with black. 

" 6. Violet. — Violet with white affords a strong 
contrast of tone ; the combination is an agreeable 
one, resembling that of blue with white. 

"Violet with gray. The distinctive hue of violet 
makes itself felt strongly in this combination, 
which is a quiet and agreeable one. 

" Violet with Mack gives but a slight contrast of 
tone when the violet is pure. The black acquires 
a rusty brown hue, which reduces its depth. 

" 7. PuEPLE. — Purple with lohite affords a good 
contrast of tone. Pale purples and rosy tints 
form agreeable combinations with white. 

" Purple with gray resembles in effect the com- 
bination of violet with gray ; the gray, if of 
moderate area, becomes decidedly greenish. 

" Purple with Mack is rarely a satisfactory com- 
bination ; the black acquires a greenish -hue." ^ 

The second class, harmonies of contrast, have 
been studied in Chapter IV. Where bold, striking, 
emphatic color is needed the complementary colors 
may be used. The most prominent part of a pic- 
ture, a room, or a decoration will be, as far as 
color is concerned, where some color and its com- 

» Colour. By A. H. Church. Ch. X., p 116. 



T COLOR-HARMONIES 83 

plement in nearly, or quite, spectral hues are 
given. This striking effect of contrast will 
lessen accordingly as the colors darken into 
shades, or lighten into tints, or become more 
and more neutral from the mixture with some 
other color. An eye untrained or inexperienced 
will find these complementary contrasts diffi- 
cult to use, there being danger of producing a 
crude or harsh effect. Rules for their use are 
given in Chapter IV. Classifying the comple- 
mentary pairs according to the pleasure we take 
in them we may put yellow and blue first, then 
orange and green-blue, red and blue-green, finally 
violet and green. Chevreul, Eood, Von Bezold, 
and Bruecke, having made many experiments and 
observations in their attempts to lay down rules for 
harmonious combinations, state that here we come 
upon problems that cannot be solved by purely 
scientific reasoning. By comparing the art of one 
country or of one period of one country with that 
of another,we find that throughout them all, certain 
pairs of colors have been preferred to certain others 
and we feel that 03sthetic taste, which cannot be 
explained, influences us greatly in our liking for 
certain combinations. Beside taste, inheritance, 
training, environment, and contrast all have their 
unconscious effect upon these preferences. Church 
divides pairs of colors into three classes : Pairs of 



84 COLOR PROBLEMS cii. 

the small intervals, pairs of decided differences, 
and the extremes or complements. The latter we 
have considered in Chapter IV. Pairs of the 
small intervals are such as 

Orange-red and yellowish-orange, 
Reddish-orange and orange-yellow, 
Orange and yellow, 

which, being so close to each other in the color 
scale in decoration, are apt to injure each other 
unless separated by outlines of black, white, gray, 
or gold. Rood gives the following table of small 
intervals : 

" Darkek. Lighter. 

Eed . . . . Orange-red. 



Orange-red . 
Orange . . 
Orange-yellow 
Yellowish-green 



. . Orange. 

Orange-yellow. 

. . . Yellow. 

. Greenish-yellow. 

Green ... .... Yellowish-green. 

Cyan-blue . .... Green. 

Blue Cyan-blue. 

Ultramarine-blue . . Blue. 

Violet ... Purple. 

Purple Eed." 

Church gives us the following list of pairs as, 
from his and others ' observations, they have been 
found to have been more or less agreeable : 

" An asterisk attached to the name of a color 



COLOR-HARMONIES 



85 



indicates that the mixtiire of gray or black with 
it improves the effect of its assbciation. It may 
be further remarked that in many cases where 
two colors of full depth yield a. bad or unsatisfac- 
tory assortment the reduction of the tone of one 
of them by a considerable addition of white often 
makes the combination agreeable. 



" Normal red 


"vvith violet . . 


. . bad. 


li u 


ii 


blue . . . 


excellent. 


11 ii 


it 


blue-green . . 


good, but strong 


tl Li 


ti 


green . . 


. . good, but hard. 


Li ii 




green-yellow 


. . fair. 


ic a 


11 


yellow * 


. . unpleasing. 


Scarlet 


11 


violet^ . . 


bad. 


ii 


" 


turquoise . . 


. . good. 


li 


u 


blue . . . 


. . good. 


a 


11 


yellow . 


unpleasing. ' 


ii 


ii 


green . . 


fair. 


Orange-red 


il 


violet . . 


. . good. 


ii ii 


ii 


purple . 


. . fair. 


(1 ii 


li 


blue . . . 


excellent. 


ii a 


11 


turquoise . 


. . good. 


ii ii 


11 


blue-green 


unpleasing. 


11 li 


ii . 


yellow-green 


fair. 


Orange 


il 


purple . . 


. . bad. 


ii 


11 


violet . . 


. . good. 


a 


ii 


blue . . . . 


. . good, but strong 


a 


il 


turquoise . 


good. 


11 


ii 


blue-green 


. . good. 


li 


il 


green . . 


. . fair. 


Orange-yello-w 


li 


purple . . 


. . good. 


11 li 


II 


violet . . 


. excellent. 



86 



COLOR PROBLEMS 



CH. 



Orange-yellow with blue . . . 




.' good. 


a ti 


it 


turquoise . 




. fair. 


a a 


it 


blue-green 




. moderate. 


iS a 


it 


green . . 




. bad. 


Yellow 


(( 


violet . . 




. excellent. 


li 


it 


purple . 




. good. 


it 


it 


normal red . 




poor. 


i( 


ti 


turquoise . . 




. moderate. 


u 


it 


blue-green * 




. bad. 


a 


it 


green * 




. bad. 


Greenish-yellow 


it 


purple . . . 




. good. 


a a 


it 


violet . . 




. excellent. 


u « 


it 


scarlet . . . 




. strong, and hard. 


U it 


" 


orange-red 




. fair. 


a it 


it 


turquoise . . 




. bad. 


a a 


it 


normal blue . 




. good. 


Yellowish-green 


it 


normal red . 




. good, but hard. 


« ti 


a 


purple . . . 




. difficult. 


<> a 


" 


blue-green 




. bad. 


a it 


cc 


blue . . 




. good. 


Normal green 


it 


purple . . 




. strong, but hard. 


a it 


ti 


scarlet . . 




. difficult. 


a a 


ii 


orange-red 




. hard. 


11 il 


it 


turquoise . 




. bad. 


Blue-green 


ti 


purple . . 




. fair. 


a 


it 


violet . . 




. good. 


a it 


ii 


blue .... 




. bad. 


it a 


'•' 


green . . . 




. bad. 


it it 


ii 


yellowish-green 


. bad. 


" " 


ii 


turquoise . . 




bad. 


" The abov 


s list comprises 


fi 


:ty-five only of the 


very numerous combinations 


, il 


1 pairs, of some of 


the decided hues. . . . 


It 


is assumed that in 



V COLOR-HARMONIES 87 

our experiments on their chromatic eifects, pleas- 
ing or otherwise, we have been using colored 
materials, which neither by any peculiarity of 
texture, nor quality, nor design, are capable of 
improving the results. Cloth and paper are suita- 
ble ; silk, velvet, glass, and enamel, for various 
reasons, give results which are complicated by the 
introduction of new elements. Pairs in these 
latter materials, in consequence of the presence 
of lustre, translucency, or 'throbbing' hues, in 
varying degrees, will often become quite accepta- 
ble, while in prosaic cloth, or paper, they are just 
the reverse." 

The third class, harmonies of complex or various 
colors, follows, and includes groups of three or 
mare colors. The difficulties of combination in- 
crease as the number of colors increases. It is 
well to remember, if one is bewildered with these 
difficulties, that, however fine the harmony of 
many colors may be, it can hardly surpass the 
beauty of one made of but two or three, provided 
that these are well proportioned to each other in 
quantity and quality, suited to and combined in 
some good design, or made up of various materials 
with differing surfaces. As to triads, or three-color 
combinations. Rood gives us the following groups as 
having been most extensively used, and if we draw 
on our memory we may probably recall both paint- 



88 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

ings and decorations consisting of any one of 
these combinations. (See Plates XXXV'I and 
XXXVII.) 

Spectral red, yellow, and blue. 

Purple-red, yellow, cyan-blue (greenish-blue 

like a turquoise). 
Orange, green, violet. 
Orange, green, purple-violet. 

With regard to these he calls our attention to the 
fact that in them the colors are nearly, or quite, 
120° apart on the chromatic circle, also that artists 
in their choice of these colors have been evidently 
guided by their wish to have two out of three 
warm colors. According to Bruecke : 

Carmine, yellow, and green, a favorite combina- 
tion during the middle ages, to us seems " some- 
what hard and unrefined." 

Orange-yellow, violet, and hluish-green are not 
so agreeable becavise two of the colors are cold. 
In the triad vermilion, green, and violet-blue, used 
greatly by the Italian schools, there seem at first 
to be two cold colors, but as the green was olive 
it might be. called vermilion, dark greenish-yelloiv, 
and violet-blue. 

Attempts have been made to give formulas of 
certain colors as they are supposed properly to 



V COLOR-HARMONIES 89 

balance one another, or .to make '"chromatic 
equivalents." Field elaborated this theory in his 
Chromatography, and it was adopted by Owen 
Jones in his Grammar of Ornament. Later 
writers on color, however, show that Field's ex- 
periments were not such as' to justify his con- 
clusions. The leading idea he tried to prove 
was, that to make a perfect harmony, each color 
in a given picture or design should bear such a 
mathematical relation to the whole that the com- 
bination of all should make, when seen at a dis- 
tance, " a neutralized bloom, or a whitish-gray." 
He speaks, for instance, of red, yellow, and blue. 
This has a plausible sound, but cannot be correct, 
for with a color-wheel we find that red, yellow, and 
blue will not in any proportions make a " whitish- 
gray," also because almost all of the best works of 
good colorists have throughout them some domi- 
nant hue, more generally on the warm side, such as 
yellow, orange, or red. At the same time careful 
study of texture will be very useful, as different 
weaves reflect and absorb the colors so as to pro- 
duce a sort of " neutralized bloom," such as Field 
speaks of. 

That chromatic equivalents can be made is 
shown by Maxwell's disks ; as, for instance. Church 
gives us the proportions of three colors which on 



90 COLOR PROBLEMS oh. 

being turned on the wheel rapidly produce a 
neutral gray, as follows : 

" Red 36 J + green 33^ + blue 29| = 100." 
We have also already seen in the* chapter on 
Contrasts that certain parts of one color require 
certain parts of another color to neutralize it and 
so make gray. 

As there is no end to the possible combinations 
of colors we can only give certain rules for mak- 
ing them, leaving it to the student to follow up 
his previous practice with two colors and by ex- 
perience to enlarge his knowledge and ability to 
use all colors with skill. 

A full harmony, in fact a symphony, of colors 
can hardly be better explained than by describing 
one used in the trial scene in the "Merchant of 
Venice," as given by Mr. Mansfield. The tribune 
or desk behind which Portia delivered her speech 
was white, draped with a full-hued scarlet cloth. 
The black of her gown, the strongest contrast to 
white, and the brilliant red, were admirably used 
to focus the eye upon this part of the scene just 
as the ear was focused on the speech " The quality 
of mercy is not strained." The other principal 
actors, Shylock, Antonio, and Bassanio, wore red, 
yellow, blue j bright colors, but less bright and 
less contrasting than the white, black, and scarlet. 
The attendants and spectators were in more neutral 



V COLORr-HARMONIES* 91 

aixd subdued colors, while away behind them all 
stretched a grayish blue sky seen between the 
pillars of a wide porch which formed a background 
well calculated to throw into relief the colors of 
the costumes. 

Prom what we have learned we find the follow- 
ing ways of harmonizing colors : 

First. By Gradation, that is, the gradual blend- 
ing of one color into another, or one variety of one 
color into another variety of th.e same color, as in 
the morning-glory blossom, in which the diiferent 
hues grade softly into one another from edge to 
heart ; or as in a clear sunset sky, where the blue 
above changes into green, the green into yellow, 
and the yellow into red near the horizon, and where 
still we cannot find the exact boundary of any 
one of the colors. • (See Plate XXXVIII.) 
. " These ever-present gentle changes of color in 
all natural objects give to the mind a sense of the 
richness and vastness of the- resources of Nature ; 
there is always something more to see, some new 
evanescent series of delicate tints to trace ; and, 
even where there is no conscious study of color, it 
still produces its effect on the mind of the beholder, 
giving him the sense of the fulness of Nature, and 
a dim perception of the infinite series of gentle 
changes by which she constantly varies the aspects 
qf the commonest objects. This orderly succes- 



92 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

sion of tints, gently blending into one another, 
is one of the greatest sources of beauty that we are 
acquainted with, and the best artists constantly 
strive to introduce more and more of this element 
into their works, relying for their triumphs far 
more on gradation- than on contrast. The greatest 
effects in oratory are also produced by correspond- 
ing means ; it is the modulation of the tone and 
thought, far more than sharp contrasts, that is 
effective in deeply moving audiences. We are 
very sensitive to the matter of modulation even in 
ordinary speech, and instantly form a general 
judgment with regard to the degree of cultivation 
and refinement of a stranger from the mode in 
which a few words are pronounced. All this has 
its parallel in the use of color, not only in painting, 
but also in decoration. Ruskin, speaking of gra- 
dation of color, says : ' You will find in prac- 
tice that brilliancy of hue and vigor of light, 
and even the aspect of transparency in shade, 
are essentially dependent on this character alone ; 
hardness, coldness, and opacity resulting far 
more from equality of color than from nature 
of color.' In another place the same author, 
in giving advice to a beginner, says : ' And it 
does not matter how small the touch of color 
may be, though not larger than the smallest 
pin's head, if one part of it is not darker than 



y COLOR-HARMONIES 93 

the rest, it is a bad touch ; for it is not merely 
because the natural fact is so that your color 
should be gradated ; the. preciousness and pleas- 
antness of color depends more on this than on any 
other of its qualities, for graidation is to colors just 
what curvature is to lines, both being felt to be 
beautiful by the pure instinct of every human 
mind, and both, considered as types, expressing the 
law of gradual change and progress in the human 
soul itself. What the difference is in mere beauty 
between a gradated and ungradated color may be 
seen easily by laying an even tint of rose-color on 
paper, and putting a rose-leaf beside it. The vic- 
torious beauty of the rose as compared with other 
flowers depends wholly on the delicacy and quantity 
of its color-gradations, all other flowers being either 
less rich in gradation, not having so many folds of 
leaf, -or less tender, being patched and veined in- 
stead of flushed.' " ^ 

In connection with gradation, Church says : 
" There is one quality of good color which lies at 
the very root of all successful employment of vivid 
hues. It consists in minute variations of hue and 
tone within the. same surface. A color must not 
be absolutely uniform,' flat, and monotonous unless 
it be very pale, very .dull, or very dark, when 
the absence of this 'throbbing' or 'palpitating' 
quality, though undesirable, is less observed. We 

1 Modern Chkomatios. By Prof. O. N. Eood. Ch. XVI. 



94 COLOR PROBLEMS 



CH. 



have before us, as we write, a fine old Chinese 
vase of turquoise crackle. Apart from the mosaic 
texture, resulting from the innumerable fissures in 
the glaze, what a number of variations in appear- 
ance does this turquoise color offer ! Where the 
color is thinnest it is paler, and verges more upon 
green ; where it is thickest, it is at once deeper, 
and more blue, and there are innumerable hues 
and tones. In painting, similar effects may be 
produced by unequal glazings and scumblings of 
one hue upon another, or by apposition of minute 
dots and patches of closely related colors."* 

The following is a practical way of using this 
beauty of gradation : " For instance, in the morn- 
ing glory and the sweet pea we may observe a 
perfectly beautiful Combination of crimson, purple, 
and violet. Notice the charming gradation of 
color in the morning glory; one tone runs into 
the other with a subtlety which is quite wonderful, 
and all the colors merge into the luminous green 
white centre from absolute positivism to perfect 
delicacy with an ease which is surprising. Now 
let us try to mass a large group of crimson, purple, 
violet, and greenish-white asters together with the 
same result. Alas ! what a task it is and how 
conf vised we become with the distracting color 
tones ; but we must feel our wa}' carefully and 
^stematically. First, our most powerful color — 

'Colour. By A. H. Chm-uh. Ch, XL, p. 144. 



V COLOR-HARMONIES 95 

crimson or violet — must be grouped, gracefully 
and placed in a prominent position ; next, we 
must run. our color tone either toward blue or 
crimson, as the case may be. If we have any gas- 
light near we must make use of it to accent our 
prominent group, and last, mingled slightly with 
the palest tones of dull pink and purplish-blue, 
we may group our greenish-white asters in some 
position where they will contrast well with the 
strong color group, and where they will be sure to 
have the intermediate blue and crimson tqnes act 
like a bridge to connect the color .scheme. Nothing 
distracts the eye so much as violent transitions of 
color." ^ 

A similar element of beauty in Oriental xugs, 
not always understood, and one in which they 
differ from those made by machinery, arises from 
the fact that being made by hand there are slight 
variations throughout, even in the dyeing of the 
wools. In an unusually fine specimen the rich 
green ground varied slightly in tone three or four 
times. To an uncultivated eye this might seem 
a defect ; to an artistic one, the play of color, the 
variety in unity, is far finer than the even monot- 
ony of a perfectly matched surface. 

Second. By Change of Quality ; as from 
pure spectral colors to their tints or shades. The 

1 F. Schuyler Matthews. 



96 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

greater we make this change either way, the more 
sure we may be of harmony, as a color scheme of 
very pale tints or very dark shades is almost sure 
to be good even if quite varied. In fact, contrast 
of tone, which is change of quality, will harmonize 
any two colors, as pale blue and dark green, or 
pale green and dark blue. Of pairs of con- 
trasts which in pure spectral colors we have seen 
to be crude and harsh, Rood says, " Complemen- 
tary colors are very valuable when the artist is 
obliged to use dark, dull, or pale colors, and still 
is desirous of obtaining a strong or brilliant effect." 
Another kind of change of quality helps us to 
make very beautiful combinations. It lies in the 
use of colors that are neither spectral, nor pure 
tints, nor shades, but of such as are neutralized by 
mixtures of other colors ; as, for instance, if instead 
of using pure yellow, pure red, and pure blue, we 
use a yellow toned down by an admixture of a 
little red and blue, a red toned in the same way 
with blue and yellow, and a blue that has in it 
something of red and yellow; the colors will still 
be yellow, red, and blue, but in approaching each 
other will become more related and so far more 
harmonious. Still another change of quality 
allows us to put in the place of one or more of the 
colors the same amount of a tint or a shade of the 
same color which will improve the harmony by 



V COLOR-HARMONIES 97 

varying its luminosity and by bringing all nearer 
together. (See Plate XXXIX.) 

Third. By Change of Quantity; as of a 
large amount of one of the colors to a small 
amount of the other, so as to, introduce another 
element of contrast. For want of the better bal- 
ance as given by the fourth rule it is inferior to it. 
(See Plate XL.) 

Fourth. By Change of Quality and Quan- 
tity ; or by making a small amount of a dark 
shade of one color balance a much larger amount 
of a light tint of another color, or, vice versa, a 
small amount of a light tint to balance a much 
larger amount of a dark shade, or a small amount 
of a pure color to balance a large amount of a 
more neutral color. In this case the rule is that 
accordingly as you lower or raise the quality of: 
your color so in proportion may you increase its 
quantity. (See Plate XLI.) 

Fifth. By the Addition of another Color, 
however unobtrusive, which breaks the even bal- 
ance between two colors, just as in form, where 
we may find two trees of the same size and shape 
make an unpleasant composition. There the effect 
can be much improved by the addition of a third 
tree of a different size and shape. For instance, 
with yellow and yellowish-green, the addition of 
violet would improve and harmonize them. This 



98 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

third color can be added in different ways, by out- 
lines, small masses, etc. (See Plate XLII.) 

Swcth. By the AoDiTioisr of Black, White, 
Gray, Gold, or Silver. — When two colors are 
not quite harmonious a small quantity of black 
will much improve the combination. The strong 
contrast in depth between the black and the colors 
seems to bring them together and so make them 
more related. In Chinese coloring the happy 
effect of black should be noted, also in old Japa- 
nese prints where the black hair of the figures 
acts in the same way. This black, white, gray, 
gold, or silver may be added in outlines, as the 
brass in Japanese "cloisonne, or in such lines as 
these I I I I I ., I I I drawn over the whole 
design, as seen in a wall paper, softening the colors 
and blending them with each other. It may be as 
in cement around and between the little bits of 
stone in mosaic, which produces much the same 
effect in throwing a sort of bloom over the colors. 
It may be in separating some part of the design 
from the other, as seen in a wall decoration where 
there was a rectangle of greenish-blue on a ground 
of dark violet-blue separated by white and gold, of 
which the result was excellent ; or it may be by 
little dots over all the colors. (See Plate XLIII.) 

Seventh. By a Dominant Hue, which may run 
through all the design in outlines, although 



V COLOR-HARMONIES 99 

colored outlines are not so good as those of black, 
white, gray, gold, or silver, or those which may be 
added in small spots over all the colors ; or those 
which may be added in small quantities to all the 
colors, changing their quality, and so bringing them 
to a harmony of a dominant hue. To make this 
clear, look at Plate VI. In it we have pure spectral 
yellow, pure spectral blue, and pure spectral red. 
Put over it the blue screen found in the end of 
the book ; the blue will be seen to be bluer, the 
yellow will become a greenish-yellow, the red will 
have a violet tinge to it. It will have become a 
harmony of the dominant hue of blue, but as blue 
is a cold color the harmony will not have become 
much more agreeable for the change. Try what 
making the same colors a harmony of the dominant 
hue of yellow will do by putting over it the yellow 
screen. The colors will be seen to be quite different. 
The yellow will be changed very little, only growing 
slightly darker, the red from the pure spectral hue 
will be moved toward the orange, and the blue 
will be moved toward the green. This gives us a 
fine harmony, and a favorite one with artists. 
Harmonies of the dominant hues of red, orange, 
or yellow — warm colors — are much more gener- 
ally liked than those of blue, green, or violet, the 
cold colors. Age has done much for old pictures 
by darkening and mellowing the paints and var- 



100 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

nish so as to give them harmony of the dominant 
hue. Jean Fran9ois Millet's have such harmony 
already, ovfing to his fine eye for color ; it will be 
noticed that though he may have put many fairly 
bright colors, blue, red, green, and yellow on 
one canvas, they all blend wonderfully together. 
'• Harmony " (we quote from Burnet on Colour, 
who speaks of Mengs) '' he considers to consist in 
the true equilibrium of the different colors regu- 
lated by the general tone of light by which they ' 
are illuminated ; thus, if the light is yellow, all 
the colors will appear tinged with the same hue, 
as the air interposed between them and the eye of 
the spectator is already tinged with that color." 
The harmony resiilting from a dominant hue in 
nature may also be seen in a spray of young 
leaves in spring when many hues of green and 
yellow will be found connected and harmonized by 
the red of the stem, which color runs through it 
all, carrying the red into the greens and yellows. 
(See Plate XLIV.) 

Eighth. By Interchange. — If two unbroken 
masses of the same quantity of strong color are 
put side by side the result may be unbearable. By 
interchanging them, however, in this way, in what 
are called in design diaper patterns, they may 
blend so as to be quite agreeable. Or they may 
be blended in weaving by interchange, as if one 



V COLOR-HARMONIES 101 

thread be of green, the next of purple, then again 
green. (See Plate XLV.) 

Ninth. By Counterchange. — ■ Examples of 
fine decorative art may be found of two colors 
where the design and the ground change places at 
certain intervals. It is an ingenious and beautiful 
way of obtaining variety of coloring. To make 
it successful the amount of ground color should 
balance that of the design. Plate XLVI gives 
■ us a good example. 

Tenth. By Form and Texture, as by the 
curves in a vase or any object which deepens the 
color as it goes away from the light and lightens 
it as it turns toward the light ; as in a curtain of 
which the folds modify the color ; as in rough and 
shaggy stuffs like plush, etc., which produce con- 
stant variation and vibration of color, and just so 
much added charm. The sparkle in jewels and 
colored glass, the sheen on satins, silks, and 
metals, and the down on fruit also come under 
this rule, as so many rnodifications of color 
tending to break up its flat surface and produce 
harmony. 

Eleventh. By Outlining a mass of flat color in 
a design with black or a dark color, then adding a 
second outline inside the first, but of either a light 
tint of the same color as the dark mass . or of 
another color which harmonizes with it; then 



102 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

there will be found an agreeable result. In fact, 
this will give a velvety appearance to the color. 

In making a complex color arrangement it is 
well to begin by planning first its leading parts; 
the additions will be much easier. Harmony of 
color must come not alone from the object we 
are planning for, but also from the place in which 
it is to be used, or the person who is to wear 
it. The color of an object may be beautiful 
in itself, but much of that beauty may be lost or 
neutralized by its surroundings. On the other 
hand, an object giving but one good, simple color 
note may be so appropriate to its position, may so 
exactly suit its surroimdings, as to complete a 
perfect harmony. 

Colors should also be adapted to the form of the 
object or designs on which they are to be used. 
Thus, when wishing to emphasize a part that re- 
tires from the eye, retiring colors should be used, 
and vice versa. 

In addition to the above rules a few suggestions 
for making color-harmonies may be useful : 

First, texture can be used to help the harmony. 

Second, harmonies with warm colors predomi- 
nating are preferred. 

Third, if certain colors are to be used in any 
decoration it is wise to put them together first in 
paint, paper, or plain materials, for the reason that 



V COLOR-HARMONIES 103 

any unpleasant efEect they may have on one another 
will show more quickly in such materials ; for the 
better the material, the more readily the colors 
blend on account of the richer surfaces. In 
colored, not painted, glass, this can be appreciated. 
It will be noticed that the quality of the glass and 
the brilliancy of the light through it help to 
harmonize the colors. 

Fourth, a simple pattern, if pattern at all, 
should be tried first, as the beauty of a good 
design may blind one to the quality of the coloring. 

Fifth, remember that combinations in which 
warm colors prevail are more agreeable than those 
made mainly of cold colors, while it is also true 
that the finest harmony of complex or various 
colors is that in which there is a proper balance 
of both warm and cold colors, so used that they 
enhance each other. 

Sixth, it is safe to affirm that any colors may 
be used together with success, provided that they 
are harmonized by the use of some of the rules 
here given. 

Any one unused to working with colored mate- 
rials would do wisely to begin cautiously, experi- 
menting at first with simple combinations of one 
color according to the first rule on page 78 for 
such combinations. In some flowers we do see the 
two extremes of a color combined, as in a jonquil 



104 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. 

the centre is of orange-yellow, the outer petals of 
greenish-yellow, but they are rather the exception. 
Attention here should also be had to the sugges- 
tion as to the use of differing materials of one 
color. When some skill has been gained in the 
simplest kind of color harmony, a single note of the 
complementary -color may be added. For example, 
see the dinner table harmony, page 69, of yellows 
with a strong note of dark blue. When the eye 
has become somewhat trained by practice of this 
kind, harmonies in triads or three colors may be 
tried. Constant practice in pairs and triads cannot 
be too fully recommended. Finally, trials may be 
made in complex combinations. One other way 
to begin working in color is by the use of neutral 
or grayed colors. Turner, the English artist, one 
of the greatest, if not the greatest, of modern 
landscape painters, began in this way, in the use 
of what are called " broken tints," using finally in 
his pictures the fullest palette of glowing colors. 

Let us suppose three ways of being called upon 
to make a color harmony. The first, that a 
designer has an order for a bouquet, a dress, a 
curtain, or for the decoration of a room, but is 
limited by the terms of the order to the use of 
certain colors. Then let him begin by studying 
the qualities of those colors, and ask himself if 
they are cool or Avarm, tints or shades, bright or 



V COLOE^HARMONIES 105 

dull, whether they are tones of one color, contrasts 
or complex. 

Again, suppose the order to be less limited in 
color, but that the bouquet is to be put in a room 
of certain coloring, or the dress to be worn by a 
person of such and such complexion and hair, or 
that the curtain is to be hung in a north room 
where warm color is needed, or perhaps in a light 
room where the southern sun needs to be toned 
down as it entel-s, to prevent a glare. The general 
coloring of the room must also be taken into ac- 
count, but is it not seen that the answer must be 
different in each case ? One colored flower would 
give quite a different effect from another, the dress 
that would suit a fair face with yellow hair would 
be quite unlike one becoming to a dark skin with 
black hair, while a curtain of soft yellow would 
tinge the northern light with some of the sun- 
shine color that never enters the dull room, and 
in the sunny room a curtain of cool, non-lumi- 
nous color would soften the glare and add to its 
comfort and harmony. The light and shade in 
the room should also be taken into account. The 
warm and cold tones can be arranged in such 
balance that color will glow from the shadows. 

In a third supposable case the designer is given 
unlimited choice of colors. Then every resource 
can be called in, and the work resulting should be 



106 COLOR PROBLEMS CH. v 

beautiful in proportion to the freedom of the order. 

Furthermore, colors should be appropriate ; for 
a quiet room, a quiet, commonplace person, for 
anything where quiet effect is desired, the designer 
should adhere to quiet, neutral combinations, or to 
combinations of one color. When a woman has a 
brilliant complexion, black eyes and dark hair, 
gay colors may be worn and seem all in harmony 
with the wearer, but these same gay colors would 
only emphasize the more commonplace character 
and coloring of others. 

Plates XL VII and XLVIII have been added here 
to show the true character of tvhites so-called ; as 
blue-white, which is really a very pale tint of 
blue ; and how by gradation, one color changes into 
another in nature. 



CHAPTER VI 

HISTOKIC COLOR 

TO continue our color study we must next ask 
what has been done with it in the past and 
how it has been used and combined. Our knowl- 
edge would be incomplete without the experience of 
the past. The simplest and easiest way will be 
to consult the Orammar of Ornament, by Owen 
Jones, and L' Ornement Polychrome, by Racinet, 
the two best books of the kind, remembering, how- 
ever, that there are several editions of each, varying 
in the quality of the coloring of the plates, and that 
even the best of these do not succeed in thoroughly 
reproducing the rare harmonies of color attained 
in the pictures, rugs, pottery, silks, metal, and 
jewel work that served as models. For these we 
must turn to the museums, and there is where the 
real lover of, and worker in, color must go for 
examples of the most skilful use of color by man 
up to this time. To many of them age has helped 
to give the. great charm they possess, by fading 
and refining the colors so that they blend more 
perfectly with each other. 

107 



108 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

Unfortunately, no mention is made in the 
Grammar of Ornament of Japanese color, and 
Racinet gives but small space to it. Since the 
publication of these books we have become familiar 
with it even in the r-hop windows. We must bear 
in mind, however, that intercourse with western 
nations and the increasing demand for Japanese 
goods is already lowering their artistic standard, 
especially as they are making many goods entirely 
for western markets, so that for their best work 
we must look for old specimens made when Japan 
was a shut-in nation. As a whole, nothing finer 
can be found. For pure coloring, for the most 
complex and happiest combinations, they have no 
equals. Thorough study of these is one of the 
best schools for designers. The Japanese them- 
selves are taught by being made to copy the best 
old works. 

The Japanese love of color and their sense of 
fitness went so far that they even changed the 
ornaments of their rooms with the changing 
seasons. Nay, more, their women wore garments 
of which the embroidery harmonized with the 
different months : cherry, apple, pear blossoms 
when the fruit trees bloomed, colored leaves in the 
autumn, and so on, keeping in tune with the year, 
and getting great enjoyment out of things too 
little thought of by us. 



VI HISTORIC COLOR 109 

At this point in his course the student will be 
wise to bear four things in mind: First, that as 
this is the study of color, not form, he should con- 
fine his attention to the colors as far as possible, 
as a fine design may tend to warp the judgment 
of them. Secondly, that different lights may 
vary what is really the same color. Thirdly, that 
if he isolates one color from another by means of 
such a card with a small opening in it as is to be 
found with the color screens at the end of this 
book, he will be greatly helped to understand it. 
Fourthly, that he should pay special attention to 
the proportions of the colors. 

The following plates have been taken from speci- 
mens of color of different nations, and are given in 
simple proportions of quality and quantity, the 
latter in one-hundredths, as nearly as it is possible 
to measure, when the design may be much compli- 
cated and broken up. In studying these with ref- 
erence to making the plates, it has seemed prob- 
able that those who made them took their color in 
many instances directly from nature ; as, for in- 
stance, Plate LIV reminds one of the qualities 
and quantities of color of a gayly feathered parrot. 
It is hoped that these plates may help to create 
a taste for hard study of whatever originals may 
be at hand in books, shops, private houses, or 
museums. 



110 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. vi 

Plate LXXXIV is a drawing of the antique rug 
from -which Plate LXXXV is reduced. By com- 
parison the student will see how these and the 
other plates have been made. 



CHAPTER VII 



NATURE COLOR 



" A ND you, painter, who are desirous of great 

-lIa^ practice, understand that if you do not 

rest it on the good foundation of Nature, you will 

labor with little honor and less profit ; and if you 

do it on a good ground, your works will be many 

and good, to your- great honor and advantage. 

"A painter ought to study universal Nature, and 

reason much within himself on all he sees, making 

use of the most excelleht parts that compose the 

species of every object before him. His mind will 

by this method be like a mirror, reflecting truly 

every object plafced before it, and become, as it 

were, a second nature." 

From the Treatise on Painting, by Leonardo da 

Vinci, we copy the above passages. May they 

serve as an introduction to the next branch of our 

color study, and prove a stimulus of the highest 

kind not only to painters, but to other artists; 

This final step in our study leads us to Nature, a 

step easy to make, but once made, it places us in 

a school as vast as it is great, and in one which we 

should never leave. Until our attention is called 

111 



112 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

to it, we are unconscious what apparently unprom- 
ising material may yield new and beautiful motives 
for color-harmonies. 

" We do not sufficiently study from nature ; we 
ought to draw and study vegetable forms, shells, 
fishes, birds, beasts. A continual use of your note- 
book should enable you to lay up an inexhaustible 
store of artistic materials and suggestions. . . . 
Then, again, the study of the arrangement of color 
of natural objects is almost entirely ignored ; yet 
how pregnant would it be with the most valuable 
and original suggestions. There is hardly any- 
thing in nature that is not perfect in color. A 
dead sparrow would enable you to arrange the 
marquetrie of a cabinet with faultless harmony. 
Then, again, the varied tints of any color in light, 
shade, and half tint are always harmonious. The 
gradations of color in a flower, if properly studied, 
would teach a lady to dress with a taste that 
would be the envy of her sex. That dress is not, 
more than it is, the study and recognized province 
of an artist, is a matter of wonder."^ 

Following closely upon this advice of Mr. 
Moody, an artist tells us that in Algiers he has 
seen the Arab girls working the beautiful em- 
broideries so much admired with boxes of butter- 
flies beside them, that from their harmonious 
blending of colors they may gain fresh enthusiasm 

' Lectures and Lessons on Art. F.W.Moody. P. 13L 



VII NATURE COLOR 113 

and inspiration for their work. Those who are 
not privileged to go to foreign lands in search of 
color motives can find them in our own country, 
and those who can leave the city's walls for but a 
day's holiday may find in the suburbs much that 
is new and helpful. Why not make excursions 
for the purpose ? A color hunt would surely be as 
cheap and harmless as it would be enjoyable and 
helpful. In New York City itself, the Museum of 
Natural History holds case upon case of birds, 
butterflies, shells, and minerals that can give an 
infinite number of novel motives, the florists' 
shops contain many more, and, if one keeps his 
eyes about him, even in the street he may meet 
with good and unexpected combinations, as, for 
instance, Plate C, which is from the flange of a 
propeller, of which the discoloration of the metal 
gave a fine color motive. 

The Japanese have always been distinguished 
for their intense sympathy with nature, and we 
find that a large part of the enjoyment of their 
lives the "year round comes from their constant 
study and observation of nature, the result, of 
course, showing itself in their art. 

Condor says, in The Flowers of Japan, 
" Flower-viewing excursions, together with such 
pastimes as shell-gathering, mushroom-picking, 
and moon-viewing, form the favorite occupations 
of the holiday seeker throughout the year," and 



114 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. vii 

" Snow-viewing is included as one of the flower 
festivals of the year." 

One caution must be given to those looking to 
nature for color motives, which is this : to make 
allowance for the modifications of form, contrast, 
composition, gradation, and atmosphere which 
may deceive us as to the true color of our object. 
It can be more truly judged by being looked at 
through a card with an opening in it, which thus 
isolates it from the adjoining colors. " We should 
be cautious in basing our conclusions even on 
observations made directly from nature itself ; 
for here our judgment is liable to be warped by 
the presence of beautiful form, good composition, 
exquisite gradation, and high luminosity." ^ A 
few plates made directly from nature are given, not 
for the sake of the imitation, but to suggest some 
of the many directions in which to look for fresh 
inspiration in color-designing. 

Students in art and science are constantly 
bidden to go to nature for the abundant secrets 
she is ready to reveal to those who seek and prize 
them, and why should not workers in simpler, if 
not lower, occupations, be sent to the same source, 
which is so bountiful as to contain something for 
every one, and so, profiting by her fulness, learn 
at the same time to find contentment and joy in 
their work ? 

' Rood. 



CHAPTER .VIII 

SPECIAL SUGGESTIONS 

AFTER having carried the study of color as 
far as the limits of our plan allow, a few- 
simple, practical suggestions may not come amiss. 

Students of painting and design will find Rood's 
many experiments with colors in his Modern 
Chromatics minute and valuable, especially those 
on the effects of mixing paints and their conse- 
quent loss of luminosity. .If their time for the 
scientific study of color be limited, Colour, by 
Church, is well adapted for their purpose, being 
small, clear, and admirably illustrated. It gives 
briefly the gist of what has been written heretofore 
on the subject. 

Biirnet, in Colour in Painting, is helpful on the 
artistic side. He says, " Harmony arising from 
the reflection of one color upon the adjoining, so 
as to produce a blending and union of the several 
hues, has been practised with the greatest success 
by many of the Dutch school, producing a chain 
of connections between the two extremes of hot 
and cold." 
■ As to materials for painting, Church's Chem- 

115 



116 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

istry of Paints and Painting gives much useful 
information as to their substance, permanence, 
adulteration, and effect upon one another. Recol- 
lecting, as we do frorri experiments with Maxwell's 
disks, that neutral colors are simply any one of the 
six colors diluted or changed by black or white, or 
black and white, or other colors, it is interesting 
to know that an ingeniously illustrated book, pub- 
lished in Paris by E. Guichard, La Orammaire 
de la Couleur, gives abundant examples of neutral 
colors, and printed beside them samples of the 
colors of which they are made. The author suggests 
that in embroidery any of these combinations can 
be made by twisting together threads of each of 
the colors required to make the neutral color, as 
by Plates CXVI and CXVII. 

In the matter of the choice of draperies and any 
kind of still life to be used to paint from, one of 
our leading artists advised his pupils generally to 
select old things as being usually finer than new 
ones, because age mellows and refines colors ; and 
also that objects of one country harmonize better 
with each other than those of different countries, 
and those of one period of one country still better. 

Florists, gardeners, and fruit-dealers will find a 
large part of CheA'^reul's book devoted to color as 
applied to horticulture, with notes of his experi- 
ments in the arrangement of plants and flowers. 



vin SPECIAL SUGGESTIONS 117 

While other nations love flowers and use and 
cultivate them, the Japanese, along with their 
great skill in growing them, have elaborated an 
art of arranging them, of which art a full and clear 
account, admirably illustrated, is given in The 
Flowers of Japan, and the Art of Floral Arrange- 
ment, a recent work published in Tokio. Many 
features of this art are very attractive, and much 
can be learned from them even if we do not wish to 
carry it to the same extent of form and ceremony. 
They make much of common flowers, and while 
our admiration is mainly given to the blossoms, 
they value every part of the plant, using stem, 
leaf, and bud in their arrangements so as to display 
each to advantage, with the flower as the crowning 
beauty of the whole. The author writes, " The 
arrangement of flowers has always been regarded 
in Japan as an occupation befitting learned men 
and literati. Ladies of the aristocracy have prac- 
tised it, as they have other arts, but it is by no 
means considered as an effeminate accomplishment. 
Priests, philosophers, and men of rank who have 
retired from public life have been its most enthusi- 
astic followers. Various virtues are attributed to 
professors of the art, who are considered to belong 
to a sort of aristocracy of talent, enjoying privileges 
of rank and precedence in society to which they 
are not by birth entitled. A religious spirit, self- 



118 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

denial, gentleness, and forgetfulness of cares are 
some of the virtues said to follow from a habitual 
practice of the art of arrangement of flowers." ^ 

The fact that flowers usually make a focus 
wherever they may be placed, — on a table, in a 
room, or in a land scape, — on account of their com- 
parative purity and luminosity of color, increases 
their beauty and shows the skill of the person who 
arranges them, but there is also a corresponding 
disadvantage that if discord there be, the arrange- 
ment is all the more prominent, the eye being 
called to it immediately. 

While we speak of the " comparative purity and 
luminosity" of colors we may at the same time 
quote from one of a series of interesting articles by 
F. Schuyler Matthews : ^ 

''ilven our anxiety to obtain definite names 
for definite colors is completely overshadowed by 
the stronger wish to understand the secret of their 
harmonious relationship. 

" Now let us try to discover if we can some small 
'portion of this secret. Why is it that nature 
nearly always puts yellow stamens in her white 
flowers ? Why is it that nearly all of her white 
flowers are not a colorless pure white ? Why is it 
difficult for us to find a positively blue or posi- 
tively yellow flower? What is the reason that 

'Floral Art of Japan. By Condor. ^lu The American Florist. 



VIII SPECIAL SUGGESTIONS 119 

there is such a multitude, such an infinity of color 
tones in the flowers, on the earth, over the sea, 
in the sky, everywhere ? What a perplexing, 
changeable, evasive thing the whole world of color 
is ! What is the reason of it all ? Simply this : 
Nature abhors the commonplace — she despises 
crude red, yellow, and blue. Variety she will have ; 
harmony she insists upon; positivism she only 
employs to- emphasize her love of the infinite. 
Thus we have one rather questionably perfect 
yellow marigold and a dozen others which have 
more orange in them than yellow ; one scarlet- 
lake colored gladiolus and an infinity of red roses, 
which cannot be called anything which is an ap- 
proach to the pure red color which scarlet-lake 
nearest resembles. We have the forget-me-not, 
which is nearly a true blue, but we have a host 
of so-called blue flowers, every one of which 
has barely .fifty per cent, of the true sky blue in its 
composition." 

It seems as though in the face of these facts it 
would be hardly possible to desigiiate any special 
flowers which possess the prismatic colors in an 
absolutely pure form. 

The rules for making harmonies can be made to 
apply to the arrangements of gardens, shop win- 
dows, bouquets and other decorations, as well, as 
to the catalogues of florists, etc. A recently issued 



120 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. 

catalogue strikes a true color chord in its cover. 
It shows a bunch of sweet peas and leaves of 
agreeable colors well balanced by the background 
of pale neutralized green, thus making a true and 
tempting harmony to lovers of flowers and color. 

Salesmen and women would be helped in their 
line of work by studying particularly the qualities 
of colors, and the effect on them of different kinds 
of artificial light. Knowledge of the contrasts of 
color will help greatly in showing goods to advan- 
tage, as one color may be made to heighten the 
color of another, and counters and shop windows 
may be well arranged according to the rules given 
for different classes of harmonies. 

Women in their dress, embroidery, and house 
decorations have immense opportunities ; no art is 
finer or higher for a woman however placed than 
that of being a harmonious whole herself, and of 
making or adding to a harmonious home, in which 
the imconscious influence of good color holds a 
large share. To do this it must not be thought 
that much money is necessary ; it adds, of course, 
to the ability of choice among fine goods, but 
cheap materials of good colors wisely combined 
may produce a far happier, we may even say 
healthier, result, than an unlimited purse without 
knowledge and taste. This is difficult to over- 
estimate. No woman has a right to say she has 



VIII' SPECIAL SUGGESTIONS 121 

no influence, conscious or unconscious, on the world 
around her. Does not much of the influence for 
good or ill come from a woman's dress ? It may 
be cheap, it may be plain, but it should be, and 
can be, in good taste and in harmony with the 
character and position of the person who wears it, 
and knowledge of one's own coloring and of that 
suited to it is one of the most important details. 

Women in their dress, milliners and dress- 
makers, would do well to realize that a dress or 
bonnet may be good in color in itself, when it is a 
whole, but when worn it becomes only part of a 
whole and will be harmonious and becoming, or 
inharmonious and unbecoming, as it does, or does 
not, suit the coloring of the wearer. To wear 
anything simply because it is beautiful is unwise ; 
it should first of all be suitable. Study of the law 
of contrast of color will here help immensely. 

For instance, according to that law, red and 
yellow next to each other make the yellow seem 
more yellow, the red more red, so if a woman with 
a sallow, colorless complexion wears pink roses or 
pink ribbons, the yellow in her skin is intensified 
and the small amount of pink in her cheeks is 
lost. As blue is the complement of yellow, a 
bright blue will have a still worse effect, but let 
her try a shade of not too intense yellow ; the skin 
will seem to lose its yellow, and whatever pink 



122 COLOR PROBLEMS ch. vin 

there may be will be brought out by the contrast. 
So other peculiarities may be softened or increased 
by contrast or harmony of color. White hair is 
made to seem whiter by the contrast of black or a 
very dark color ; black hair and rosy cheeks are 
made more brilliant by a white surrounding ; deli- 
cate blonde coloring will be made insipid and color- 
less by too strong colors, and a plain face may be 
made attractive by surrounding it with harmoniz- 
ing coloring. 



APPENDICES 



APPENDIX A 



DEFINITIONS 

ABSOLUTE COLORS : see Local Colors. 

ADVANCING COLOES : those of the longer wave 
lengths; those that seem to come forward; But each 
color only advances or recedes according to its relation 
to some other color. See Luminosity. 

ANALOGOUS HARMONY: see Harmony. 

BEAM OE LIGHT : a linear portion * of light made of 
a number of rays. 

BRIGHTNESS : see Luminosity. 

BROKEN COLOR: a color changed by the addition of 
black and white or gray. 

COLD COLORS : those of the shorter wave lengths, such 
•as green, blue, and violet. 

COLOR : an internal sensation, produced by various 
causes, chiefly by waves of incomplete light acting 
on the eye; as used by artists, the rich, harmonious 
effect, or full appearance produced by certain com- 
binations of colors. 

COLOR CHART : a systematic arrangement of colors in 
a geometrical design such that every variation and 

* Note. — • " A streak of light." — Bood. 
125 



126' 



COLOR PROBLEMS 



combination of hue, tint, and shade is in its proper 
place and in correct relation to all other hues, tints, 
and shades. 



Advancing Colors, 

Bright " 

Broken " 
Cold 

CoTnplementary " 
Complements of " 

Constants " '• 

Contrast " ' 

Harmony " " 

Hue " 

Intense " 

Local " 

Luminosity " " 

Luminous " 

Neutral " 

Normal " 

Opaque " 

Pigm,ent " 

Primary " 

Prismatic '' 

Pure " 

Quality of " 

Saturated " 

Secondary " 

Spectral " 



see under Advancing ; 
" " Brightness ; 
" " Broken ; 
" " Cold ; 
" " Complement ; 



a ic 



" " Constants; 

" " Contrast ; 

" " Harmony ; 

" Hue ; 

" '■' Saturated ; 

" " Local ; 

" " Brightness ; 

" " Luminous ; 

" " Neutral; 

" " Normal ; 
Opaque ; 

" " Pigment ; 

" " Primary ; 

" " Prismatic ; 

" Pure ; 

" Constants ; 

" " Saturated ; 

" " Secondary ; 

« " Normal ; 



APPENDIX A 127 

Tertiary Colors, see under Tertiary ; 
Transparent " " " Transparent ; 

Value of " " « Values ; 

Warm " " " Warm. 

COMPLEMENTS or 

COMPLEMENTAEY COLOES : any color and the color 
of its after-image ; any two colors -which when mixed 
make white. 

COMPOUND or MIXED COLOK : a color changed or 
neutralized by the addition of another color or colors. 

CONSTANTS or QUALITIES OF COLOES : Hue, Pur- 
ity, and Luminosity. 

CONTEAST : ssee Simultaneous Contrast, page 63. 

CONTEASTED HAEMONY: see Harmony. 

DIFFEACTION GEATING: a polished metal or brass 
surface ruled with fine lines and used instead of a 
prism to divide a ray of light and produce a spec- 
trum. 

DOMINANT HAEMONY : see Harmony. 

DOMINANT HUE : see Hue. 

HAEMONY: the pleasing effect due to the action upon 
each other of colors improved and made more beau- 
tiful by being put together ; such an agreement be- 
tween the different hues, tints, or shades of a design 
as will produce unity of effect. 
Analogous Harmony: an agreeable combination of 

colors that are related to a fundamental color. 
Complex Harmony: an agreeable combination of three 



128 COLOR PROBLEMS 

or more colors, or witli the addition of black or 
■white, or gray, or gold, or silver, or any or all of 
them. 
Contrasted or Complementary Harmony: an agreeable 
combination of any pair of complementary colors, or 
of their tints or shades, or tints and shades. 
Dominant Harinony : an agreeable combination of colors 
in which one color predominates by modifying all 
the other colors, by serving as a ground, or by being 
added in small portions all over the design. 
One-color Harmony, also called a Harmony of Self-tones : 
an agreeable combination of one color used in tints 
or shades, or tints and shades, or hue and tints, or 
hue and shades, or hue, tints, and shades. 

HUE : color, by wave length, much the same as color ; 
the chief quality by which one color differs from an- 
other color, as red differs from blue or green. 
Dominant Hue : the hue which predominates through 
the larger part of a design or composition. 

INTENSE: see Saturated. 

LIGHT : the chief agent that produces vision. 

LOCAL COLOR: the actual color of an object unaffected 
by shadows or reflected lights. 

LUMINOSITY : the strength of the light sent to the eye 
by any color; a luminous color sends more than a 
non-luminous one. 

LUMINOUS COLORS : those which reflect light in large 
quantities; the colors of the long wave lengths are 
more luminous than those of the short ones. 



APPENDIX A 129 

NEUTRAL COLORS: a term often incorrectly applied 

to black, white, gray, gold, and silver. 
NORMAL, SPECTRAL, PRIMITIVE, or PRISMATIC 

COLORS : those seen in the rainbow and the solar 

spectrum are generally accepted as such and are used 

as the standard for the study of colors. Pigment 

colors can only imitate these colors imperfectly. 
OIL COLORS ; pigments ground in oil. 
OPAQUE COLORS: pigment colors which are so thick 

that paper or canvas cannot be seen through them. 
PIGMENTS : materials from which paints, inks, dyes, 

and stains are made. 
PIGMENT COLORS : paints, inks, dyes, and stains used 

in the iine and industrial arts. 
PRIMARY COLORS: red, blue, and yellow; so called 

because it was supposed that all other colors could 

be made from them. 
PRIMITIVE COLORS : see Normal Colors. 
PRISM : a triangular or three-sided bar of clear glass. 
PRISMATIC COLORS: those that appear when a ray 

of white light shines through a prism. See Normal 

Colors. 
PURE COLORS : those unmixed with white light or any 

other color ; those of the spectrum. 
PURITY OF COLORS: the absence of an admixture of 

any other color or colors, or white or black. 
QUALITIES OF COLORS : see Constants of Colors. 
RAY OF LIGHT : a small linear portion or streak of 

light which may be white or any color. 



130 COLOR PROBLEMS 

RECEDING COLORS : those which seem to retire or 
recede from the eye ; those of the short wave lengths. 

RETINA: a thin inner lining of the eye. See page 20. 

SATURATED or INTENSE COLORS: colors that are 
pure and luminous to their greatest extent; without 
any mixture of white light. 

SECONDARY COLORS: orange, green, and violet; so 
called because it has been thought they were made 
from combinations of the primary colors. 

SELF-TONES : see Tone. 

SHADE : a tone of a color darkened by the addition of 
black pigments to paints, inks, dyes, and stains, or by 
the action of diminished light on immaterial colors. 

SHADOW : about the same as shade, as generally used, 
but for the sake of clearness it is best to designate by 
shadow those parts of an object which do not receive 
any direct rays of light, while those surfaces which re- 
ceive but little direct light, and are thus intermediate 
in value between the light and the shadow, are called 
shade surfaces. Then the term cast-shadow denotes the 
shadow projected by one body on another body or 
surface. 

SOLAR SPECTRUiM : see Spectrum. 

SPECTRAL COLORS : see Normal Colors. 

SPECTRUM : the result of the decomposition of a ray of 
sunlight into all the colors which form it ; the streak 
of colors formed by a ray of light that has passed 
through a prism or over a Diffraction Grating. 

STANDARD COLORS : those of the spectrum. 



APPENDIX A 131 

TEETIAEY COLORS : citrine, olive, and russet, so caUed 
because it has been thought that they were made from 
combinations of the secondary colors. 

TINT : a tone of a color produced by the addition of white to 
oil, water to water, and white light to imma,terial colors. 

TONE : the given state of a color as it may be pure, lu- 
minous, broken, compound, a tint, or a shade. 
Self-tones : tones of the same color. 

TEANSPAEENT GOLOES : those in which the color 
tints the paper or canvas, which shows through the 
color, thus helping to produce the effect. 

VALUES: the relative amount of light contained in the 
different colors of a picture, design, or composition ; 
the lightest or most luminous being called the high- 
est in value. 

WAEM COLOES : those of the longer wave lengths, as 
yellow, orange, and red. 

WATEE COLOES : pigments prepared to be used with 
water. 

WAVE LENGTHS OF COLOES : objects having no color 
in themselves possess the power' of reflecting waves of 
light; waves of light of varying lengths give us the 
effect of color. Either the amount of motion of the 
ether, or height of the wave, produces the intensity 
or brightness of the light, and the length of the 
wave produces the color; red has a wave length of 
about 3j?u,VTiV,!n;Tj of an inch, orange ^^f-iMo^s, yellow 
• sTir^A'Tysff; ff^een j^^^-iMsou, blue ^isSW.ss!,, and violet 



APPENDIX B 



A S wliatever may be of value in this little 
-^-*- work on a theme so large and complex as 
color must of necessity be drawn largely from 
what has been written before, the following list of 
books and authors is given, partly as having been 
referred to during its preparation, and partly as a 
suggestion for further reading to any student of 
color who can afEord the time and« labor necessary 
to the acquisition of a larger and wider compre- 
hension of a subject which can be treated only 
scantily enough within the scope of a single small 
volume. 

Although no pretence is here made to complete- 
ness as bibliography, yet it is believed that the 
fifty works enumerated below fairly cover the 
history of color and of its ever-growing relation 
to Art and Manufacture. For the sake of con- 
venience the list is chronologically arranged. 



183 



134 COLOR PROBLEMS 

A Tkeatise on Painting. By Leonardo da Vinci. (Lon- 
don, 1835: Mchols & Sons.) (Translation.) 
Colour. By M. E. Chevreul. (London, 1839 : Geo. Bell 

& Sons.) (Translation.) 
Theory of Colour. By J. W. von Goethe. (London, 

1840: J. Murray.) (Translation, with notes, by Sir 

Chas. Eastlake.) 
Rudiments of the Painter's Art; or a Grammar of 

Colouring. By George Field. (London, 1850 : Weale.) 
Darstellung der Farbenlehre und optische Studien. 

By W. H. Dove. (Berlin, 1863.) 
Kesearchbs on Colour-blindness. By G. Wilson. 

(Edinb., 1855: Sutherland & Knox.) 
Grammar of Ornament. By Owen Jones. (London, 1856.) 
On Colour (etc.). By Sir J. S. Wilkinson. (London, 

1858 : J. Murray.) 
Die Earbenharmonib in ihber Anwbndung auf die 

Damentoilettb. By E. Adams. (Leipzig, 1862 : J. 

J. Weber.) 
Practical Hints on Colour in Painting. By John 

Burnet. (London, 1865 : J. & J. Leighton.) 
Des Couleurs au Point de Vue Physique, Physiolo- 

GiQUE, Artistique et Industriel. By Ernst Bruecke. 

(Paris, 1866 : J. B. Bailliere & fils.) 
The Principles op the Science of Colour. By William 

Benson. (London, 1868 : Chapman & Hall.) 
Color. By M. t. Cavd. (New York, 1869.) (Translation.) 
Manual op the Science of Colour. By W. Benson. 

(London, 1871 ; Chapman & Hall.) 



APPENDIX B 135 

The Theory of Colouking. By J. Bacon. (Loudon, 
1872 : Gr. Eowney & Company.) 

L'Oknement Polychrome. By A. Eacinet. 2 vols. r°. 
(Paris, 1873-86 : Pirmin Didot.) 

A Grammar of Colouring applied to Decorative 
Painting and the Arts. By George Pield. (Lon- 
don, 1875 : Lockwood & Company.) 

Theory of Color. By Dr. Wilhelm von Bezold. (Bos- 
ton, 1876: L. Prang &, Company.) (Translation.) 

Die geschichtlichb Entwickelung des Parbensinnes. 
By Hugo Magnus. (Leipzig, 1877 : Veit.) 

The Principles of Light and Color. By E. D. Babbitt. 
(New York, 1878 : Babbitt & Company.) 

Complement des £tudes sur la Vision des Couleurs 
PAR E. Chevreul. By M. E. Cbevreul. (In Institut 
de Prance. Academie des Sciences — Memoires. T. 41, 
partie 2.) (Paris, 1879.) (English translations exist.) 

Modern Chromatics, with Application to Art and 
Industry. By 0. K Eood. (New York, 1879: D. 
Appleton.) 

The Colour Sense : its Origin and Development. By 
Grant Allen. (London, 1879 : Triibner & Company.) 

Color Blindness. By B. Joy Jeffries. (London, 1879.) 

A Handbook for Painters and Art Students on the 
Character and Use of Colours. By W. J. Muck- 
ley. (London, 1880 : T. & C. Bailliere.) 

Sight; an Exposition of Monocular and Binocular 
Vision. By Joseph Le Conte. (New York, 1881 : D. 
Appleton & Company.) 



136 COLOR PROBLEMS 

Untbrsuchungen uber den Farbencontrast vermit- 

tfELST ROTiRENDER ScHBiBEN. By Q. B. T. Schmerler. 

(Leipzig, 1882 : W. Engelmann.) 
La Gtrammaire be la Couleur. By E. Gruichard. 3 vols. 

(Paris, 1882 : H. Cagnon.) 
Die Farbenwelt. By Max Schasler. (Berlin, 1883: C. 

Habel.) 
The Laws of Contrast of Colour and Their Appli- 
cation TO THE Arts and Manufactures. By M. E. 

Chevreul. (London, 1883: Routledge.) (Translation.) 
Colour. By A. H. Cliurcli. (London, 1887 : Cassell & Com- 
pany.) 
Il Libro DEI CoLORi. Sbgreti del Secolo XV. Da 0. 

Guerrini & C. Eicci. (Bologna, 1887 : Romagnoli Dall' 

Ac qua.) 
Colour, An Elementary Treatise. By C. T. Whitmell. 

(Cardiff, 1888 : W. Lewis.) 
F. C. Scheoeder's "Systematic Index.'' By F. C. 

Schroeder. (Boston, 1888 : F. C. Schroeder.) 
Iris : Studies in Colour and Talks about Flowers. 

By A. F. Dielitzsch. (Edinburgh, 1889 : T. & T. Clark.) 

(Translation.) 
Repertoire Chromatique. By Charles La Couture. 

(Paris, 1890 : Gauthier, Villars & Fils.) 
The Chemistry of Paints and Painting. By A. H. 

Church. (London, 1890 : Seeley & Company.) 
Colour in Woven Design. By R. Beaumont. (London, 

1890 : WMttaker & Company.) 



APPENDIX B 137 

Coloue-Blindness and Colour-Perception. By F. W. 
Edridge G-reen. (London, 1891 : Kegan Paul, Trench, 
Trilbner & Company.) 

A Text-Book of Physiology. By M. Foster. (London, 
1891 : Macmillan & Company.) 

Flowers of Japan and the Art of Floral Arrange- 
ment. By Condor. (Yokohama, 1891 : 'Kelly & 
Walsh.) 

Colour Measurement and Mixture. By W. de W. 
Abney. (London, 1891.) 

Harmonious Colouring. 3 vols. F°. By C. H. Wilkin- 
son. (Manchester, 1891 : Harmonious Colouring Com- 
pany.) 

Colour Vision. By E. Hunt. (Glasgow, 1892 : Smith.) 

On a Color System. By 0. N. Eood. (New Haven, 
1892.) 

Students' Text-Book of Color ; or, Modern Chro- 
matics. By 0. N. Eood. (New York, 1892: D. 
Appleton & Company.) 

Colour Vision. By W. de W. Abney. (London, 1895: 
Low.) 

Color- Vision and Color-Blindness. By J. E. Jennings. 
(Phila., 1896 : Davis Company.) 

Colour in Nature. A Study in Biology. By M. I. 
Newbegini (London, 1898: J. Murray.) 



Plate I 



Yellows 



Keds 






Blues 



Neutrals 




WOOLS AS SORTED BY A COLOR-BLIND MAN 



Plate II 




Plate III 

















ffi^^^^^^l 














s 


[ 






<D 

> 


1 


o 


o 


- -o 


1 
1 

■t 


French Blue 

and 

Crimson 

Lake 


1 


t3 

1^ 


Si 


Vermilion 

and 
Cadmium 


Vermilion 

and 

Crimson 

Lake 


i 


2? 


id 


00 

>d 




r-^ 


As great as can be given by pigments 


so 


la 


m 


iH 


N 


■* 


!3 
o 
U 


X) 

o 
U 


3 


g 
^ 
^ 


Warm 








TABLE OF SPECTRAL COLORS 





Plate IV 








(a) (b) 

THE SPECTRAL COLORS 



(a) In their order of Luminosity 

(b) Pure and Grayed 



Plate V 




ADVANCING AND RETIRING COLORS 



Plate VI 




ADVANCING AND RETIRING COLORS 



See page 99. !»The color screens at end of volume are for 
use with this plate. 



MISSING 



PAGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 



Plate IX 




VIOLET 
with its extremes 



Plate X 















BLUE 
with its extremes 





Plate XI 



GREEN 
with its extremes 



Plate XII 







1 








YELLOW 
with its extremes 





Plate XIIl 




ORANGE 
with its extremes 



Plate XIV 











( 




RED 
with Its extremes 





Plate XV 



'^TlV^StttW-t-^-^Ii 




. i . Si> ^.aiiikii'e^M&l 




'??->-.-."■' ' - ,!S!-'?.*"''i'-''»V"l»« 



SHADES BY CONTRAST 



Plate XVI 







mm'^ 




■ SPECTRAL COLORS 
ON BLACK, WHITE AND GRAY 



Plate XVII 





WHITE 
ON SPECTRAL COLORS 



Plate XVIII 




Plate XIX 



B 




■i 


■ 




B 



GRAY 
ON SPECTRAL COLORS 



Plate XX 




SPECTRAL RED WITH ITS COMPLEMENT 



N. B. The blue-green complementary is here imitated 
as closely as possible, but when spontaneously called up by 
the eye it is really brighter than the white paper. 



Plate XXI 




SPECTRAL RED DISK FOR EXPERIMENT 
IN COMPLEMENTS 



Gaze steadily at the red disk for three minutes, cover it 
quickly with the preceding blank page without removing 
the eyes and you will see its complementary image. 



Plate XXII 




SPECTRAL RED AND ITS COMPLEMENT, BLUE- 
GREEN, IN THEIR RELATIVE PROPORTIONS 



The gray in The centre of this Plate is the gray produced by the above 
two complements when mixed on a color wheel, and corresponds exactly 
to the gnay produced by the given amounts of black and white. 

(N. B. The above proportions were obtained in an average light. 
T^ey will vary with all variations in the quality and quantity of the 
illumination. This applies as well to the following four Plates.) 



Plate XXIII 




SPECTRAL ORANGE AND ITS COMPLEMENT, 

GREEN-BLUE, IN THEIR RELATIVE 

PROPORTIONS 



The gray in the centre of this Plate is the gray produced by the 
above two complements when mixed on a color wheel, and corresponds 
exactly to the gray produced by the given amounts of black and white. 

(N- B. The above proportions were obtained in an average light. 
They will vary with all variations in the quality and quantity of the 
illuminatiou.) 



Plate XXIV 




SPECTRAL YELLOW AND ITS COMPLEMENT, 
SPECTRAL BLUE, 

OR 

SPECTRAL BLUE AND ITS COMPLEMENT, 

SPECTRAL YELLOW, 

IN THEIR RELATIVE PROPORTIONS 



The gray In the centre of this Plate is the gray produced by the 
above two coQiplements when mixed on a color wheel, and corresponds 
exactly to the gray produced by the given amounts of black and white. 

(N. B. The above proportions were obtained in an average light. 
They will vary wilh all variations in the quality, and quantity of the 
illumination.) 



Plate XXV 




SPECTRAL GREEN AND ITS COMPLEMENT, 
PURPLE, IN THEIR RELATIVE PROPORTIONS. 



The gray in the centre of this plate is the gray produced by the above 
two complements when mi;ced on a color wheel, and corresponds exactly to 
the gray produced by the given amounts of black and white. 

(N. 6, The above proportions were obtained in an average light. 
They will vary with, all variations in the quality and quantity of the illu- 
mination.) 



Plate XXVI 




SPECTRAL VIOLET AND ITS COMPLEMENT, 

YELLOW-GREEN, IN THEIR RELATIVE 

PROPORTIONS 



The gray in the centre of this plate is the gray produced by the above 
two complements when mixed on a color wheel, and corresponds exactly to 
the gray produced by the given amounts of black and white. 

(N. B. The above proportions were obtained in an average light. 
They will vary with all variations in the quality and quantity of the illu- 
mination,) 



Plate XXVII 









MILTON BRADLEY COLOR MACHINE 



Plate XXVIII 



COMPLEMENTS 



Yellow- 
Green 



Yellow 





Blue 



Purple 



Blue 



Green- 
Blue 



Blue- 
Green 




Yellow 



I 




Orange 



Red 



TABLE OF COMPLEMENTS ARRANGED IN 
PAIRS 



MISSING 



PAGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 





Plate 


XXX 




Red 

> 




— — _VK>let 




-p\irp\e^ 




/ 




/ 


Blu^ 


/£K 




IX 


Green 


Orange 




\\ 


-blue' ' 
\Blue / 


YellowC 


Yenow*~-^__ 
-green 


VSreen, 


greet^y 




CONTRAST 


DIAGRAM 




See page 58. 


Transparency accompanying 


the volume is 




for use with 


this plate. 





MISSING 



PAGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 



Plate XXXII 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM TEACUP AND SAUCER 



Yellow Tint .65 

Yellow Shade ; 5 

Blue Tint 20 

Spectral Blue 10 

100 



Plate XXXIII 



iv. 




s»' 



RMONY OF ONE COLOR 








w 






^V^, -a 




<-,umri^lLA HAKMON' 



Plate XXXIV 



"^ 1 aaaaaa 
; aaaaaa 





COLOR ANALYSIS OF A BOOK ADVERTISEMENT 

White 60 

Black 22 

Yellow 18 

100 



Plate XXXV 




Plate XXXVI 




Spectral Red and *^pe€tral Blue 



Spectral Red 




Gjeen-Blue 





* »rangc 





Spectral Orange 




Spectral violet 




Green Yellow " Spectral Violet 



GOOD DYADS OR PAIRS; 



Plate XXXVII 



Spectral 
Red 

Purple 
Red 

Spectral 
Orange 




Yellow 
Yellow 
Gi een 


Spectral 
Blue 

Blue 

Spectral 
Violet 


Spectral Spectral 
Orange Green 

GOOD TRIADS 


Purple- 
Violet 

5 



Plate XXXVIII 




HARMONY BY GRADATION 



Plate XXXIX 



" 


1 










HARMONY BY CHANGE OF QUALITY 
(In the yellow.) 



Plate XL 




HARMONY BY CHANGE OF QUANTITY 



Plate XLI 



Mm^U. : 

■■HMJIlll 

■■■nn 






HARMONY BY CHANGE OF BOTH QUANTITY 
AND QUALITY 

Three yellows, two blues. 



Plate XLII 




HARMONY BY THE ADDITION OF 
ANOTHER COLOR 



Plate XLIII 




Plate XLIV 




HARMONY FROM A DOMINANT HUE 



Plate XLV 




Plate XLVI 




MISSING 



PAGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 



Plate XLIX 



'WSMgVjfi: ■:•}:■ ■-■■:■' : "■■''»»* 


:%^-:va 3!.;..p; 




.si5'::;'Sa!a, -;v;.;^'>> !\,'.-S .jSX 
















^.: '-:■■■ 


■ 


■ 


■5? 

■ ^ 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM ASSYRIAN TILES 

Blue-Green Ground 60 

Greenish Yellow .... g 

Orange 6 

Purple-Brown ...... g 

White 20 

100 
The variation of color in the blue-green tiles is 
especially fine. 



Plate L 






I 



I 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM ASSYRIAN TILES 



Blue .............. 35 

Yellow ..... 30 

White .......... 15 

Dull Red 10 

Black 10 

100 



Plate LI 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM ASSYRIAN 
TILES 



Blue QQ 

Deep Yellow 20 

Light Yellow 10 

White .... 1 



100 



Plate LII 



■ 



I 

■I 

m 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A MUMMY COVER 

Pale Yellow . 34 

Green 27 

Blue 25 

Red. . 6 

Gold 4 

Black 2 

White 2 

100 



Plate LIIl 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM AN EGYPTIAN 
MUMMY CASE 



Black Ground 63 

Yellow (all through design) 17 

Green 9 

Red 4 

Light Red 3 

Blue 3 

White 1 

100 



Plate liv 




m 

COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A MUMMY CASE 

Green ..... 36 

Elue-Gieen .24 

Yellow ....... 14 

Red ... 11 

White 10 

Dull Red ..... 3 

Black 2 

100 
Much like a parrot's plumage. 



Plate LV 



p 


^^^^ 


n 


M 


■ 


■■ 


■■■ 


■j 


■ 


n 


■ 


n 


■■■ 


J 


n 






■■ 


■■■ 


1 

■■;i 




■ 




■■ 


■■■ 






■■ 


■■■ 






■■ 


■■■ 






M 




■■ 


■■■ 


■ i 
■ 1 




n 




■■ 


■■■ 






n 




■■ 


■■! 


I j 






n 




■■ 


■■■1 


1 


■ 


L 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A MUMMY CLOTH 

Purple Red ...... ... 91 

Black . . . . .5 

Pale Gray . . . . 4 

100 
Dull yellow ground. 



Plate LV[ 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A MUMMY CLOTH 



Dull Green . 
Bright Green 
Red . . 

Blue . . . 



29 

10 

10 

5 



Orange . 
Yellow . . . 
Ground Color 



4 

i 

40 

100 



Plate LVII 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A MUMMY CLOTH 



Deep, Dull Blue -_ 50 

Gray .... 43 

Green .... 3 

Dull Red . . . . , 2 

Pale Red ... 1 

Yellow o ... 1 

100 



■Plate LVIII 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A MUMMY CLOTH 

Light Blue 32 

Dark Blue 17 

Light Red ,33 

Dark Red . , 12 

Black Stems 6 

■ 100 

Gray ground ; the ornament a stripe of embroidered leaves 
and steins. 



Plate LIX 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A MUMMY CLOTH 

Red .25 

Green o , , ^ 25 

Yellow . . ....... ..,..-.. 2o 

Blue. • • 26 

100 
Gray ground. 



Plate LX 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A MUMMY CLOTH 



Red . 
Green , 
Blue. 
Orange . 



. 50 

.5, 24 

: 20 

6 
100 



Light gray ground. 



Plate LXI 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A MUMMY CLOTH 



Maroon . . 02 

Dull Yellow ........ 5 

Cream White ... ........ 3 

100 

Green linen ground with red border. Cream and yellow 
runs through design in small portions. 



Plate LXII 




! 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM AN EARLY GREEK 
. ' . VASE 



Gray . 72 

Black . ..... 21 

Dull Red ................ 7 

100 



Plate LXIII 




— ■■■■■■■ 

■■■■■■■■■a 
■■■■■■■■■■ 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A GREEK VASE 



Light Red 35 

Dark Red 19 

Black 46 

White 1 

100 

The Ground partly red, partly black, white in fine out- 
lines or small dotted outlines. 



Plate LXIV 




Bl ■■■■I 

COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A GREEK VASE 

Dull Orange .... 60 

Dull Red , . . . , ..... 10 
Black 30 

100 



Platk LXV 



II 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A GREEK VASE 

Gray Ground 71 

Black . 24 

Red. ....... 5 

100 



Plate LXVI 



nil 

I, 

■ r: 

■■ . 


Jl 

■■■ 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM ARAB MOSAICS 

Black .... 33 

White . 26 

Light Red .21 

Dull Red ...... . 20 

100 



Plate LXVII 



n 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM ARABIAN 
ILLUMINATION 



Blue 20 

Green 20 

Red 20 

Pale Red 10 

Gray . . g 

Gold . 10 

White 12 

100 



Plate LXVIII 



mi 


MBb 


mmmam ..._ 


HH 


■■■■m.. - 


^■1 


■■■^■^T 


■■■ 


■■ ^J!-..aaHH 


■■ !■■■■■■ 

■ ■■■■■ 
■■■■»■■■■■ 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM MOORISH TILES 





30 


White . . 


. . 20 


Yellow 


20 


Violet 


........ 30 


>■ 


100 



Plate LXIX 



■■■■■■■■■■ 
■■■■■■■■■fl 



■HL 

Hnal 

■■■I 

■I 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A PANEL OF THE 
ALHAMBRA 

Blue 40 

Red 30 

Gold 24 

White 6 

100 



Platk LXX 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A PANEL OF THE 
TAJ MAHAL, INDIA ' 



White Ground .... 52 

Pale Yellow 10 

Deep Yellow . 7 

Red ......... 5 

Pale Green .... 10 

Medium Green ..... . ... 5 

Dark Green ... 5 

Black . . 3 

Pale Pink 3 

100 
Lilies and leaves on white ground. 



Plate LXXI 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM DAMASCUS TILES 

Pale Yellow Ground . . ... 40 

Deep Cool Blue ,26 

Light Blue .............. 20 

Green ... 13 

Brown ... 2 

100 



Plate LXXII 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM CELTIC ORNAMENT 



Green .... 50 

Red 18 

Yellow 17 

Black 7 

White 8 

100 



Plate LXXIII 



■■■ — 

■m 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM ITALIAN MAJOLICA 
VASE 

White Ground 38 

Deep Blue 34 

Yellow .... ... 16 

Dark Yellow 6 

Green 6 

100 



Plate LXXIV 




I 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM PANEL OF DUTCH 
INLAID CABINET OF THE 15th CENTURY 



Brown Wood 58 

Light '• 19 

Yellow " 6 

Green " 15 

Dull Red" 2 

Black " 1 



100 



Plate LXXV 


■■■■■■■■■■ 


■■■■■■■■■ 


■■■■■■■■ „ 


■■■■■!■ <jm 


■■■■■■ timm 


■■■■■ ._■■■ 


■■■■ .■■■■ 


■■■IB ■■■■■■ 


■■■■!■■■■■ 


■■■ ■■■■■ 


COLOR ANALYSIS FROM SPANISH EMBROIDERY 


Black Ground 50 


Yellow Design 40 


Red in Design 10 


100 



MISSING 



PAGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 



Plate LXXVIl 




iPF 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM AN ANTIQUE PER- 
SIAN RUG 



Old Rose . 
Old Yellow 
Black . . 



. . . 55 

40 

... 5 

100 

The black was used in fine outlines between the rose and 
yellow to harmonize them. 

The following eight examples have had their harmony greatly increased 
by time which has tinned their colors. 



Plate LXXVIII 




ANALYSIS FROM AN ANTIQUE RUG 



OldYello-w 70 

Old Rose . 15 

Green-Blue . ,...,.,.. 9 

Black . ........... 6 

100 



Plate LXXIX 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM AN ANTIQUE RUG 

Pale Green Tiut Ground ... 50 

Yellow-Pink . . 15 

Yellow . . 13 

Blue .... 10 

Black . . 7 

White , . . 5 



100 



Black used in fine lines. 



Plate LXXX 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM AN ANTIQUE RUG 

Camel's-Hair Gray ..... 50 

Cool Blue Tint ... .20 

Green ...... 20 

Yellow 10 

100 



Plate LXXXI 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM AN ANTIQUE RUG 

Green-Blue Ground 50 

Red Tint ... 25 

Yellow ... 25 

100 



Plate LXXXIII 






^^^^1 






■i 



COLOK ANALYSIS FROM AN ANTIQUE RUG 



Neutral Red 65 

Cold Blue , 20 

Silver , . . , . 15 

100 



Plate LXXXIV 






THE COLOR SCHEME OF AN ANTIQUE RUG 

FROM WHICH PLATE LXXXV IS AN 

ANALYSIS 



Plate LXXXV 



■■■ 




■■■ 
■■■ 



r 



ANALYSIS OF AN ANTIQUE RUG 
(See Plate LXXSIV) 

Dull Blue Shade ... - 62 

Dull Yellow Shade ■ ■ ■ ■ • 38 

100 



MISSING 



PAGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 



Plate LXXXVIl 



■ ni 



mm 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM JAPANESE SILK 
TAPESTRY 



Gray (Ground 
Dark Blue , 
Light Blue . 
Gray-Blue . . 
Brown . . . 
Green . . . 



64 

8 

7 

10 

10 

1 

100 



Plate LXXXVIII 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM JAPANESE SILK 
BROCADE 



Yellow-Gray Ground'. . ... 60 

Blue-Gray Leaves .... 15 

White Daisies .... 16 

Pink Tips to Daisies 5 

Gold Veins to Leaves and Centres to Daisies .... 4 

100 



Plate LXXXIX 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM BORDER OF 
JAPANESE CLOISONNE VASE, PI. XC 

Greenish White. - • . . 66 

Blue 34 

100 



Plate XC 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM JAPANESE CLOISONNE 
VASE 



Green-Blue Ground 


. 43 


Light Red 


. 3 


Dark Blue . . 


. 14 


Lightest Red 


. . 3 


Black .... 


. 7 


Greenish Blue 


3 


Red ... . 


. 9 


Green . . . 


. . 2 


YeUow .... 


. 5 


Gray ... 


. 1 


Violet .... 


. 4 


Brass . 


, . 2 


White .... 


. 4 




100 



The fine brass outlines add much to the harmony. 



Plate XCI 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A JAPANESE SKIRT 
PANEL 



Border 




White Ground 


, 23 


Black 


. 11 


Gold Edge . . . 


. 2 


Purple-Blue . . . 


. 4 


Dul Gold. . . . 


. 6 


Dull Pink .... 


. 4 



Centre 

Green Ground 
Shades of Red 
Yellow . . 
Blue . . 
Greens . 
Lavender . 
Gold Edge 
Black . . 
Orange . . 



26 
11 
2 
2 
4 
1 
1 
1 
2 

100 



Plate XCII 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM JAPANESE BROCADE 



Brown 50 

Red ................ 10 

Dark Blue . . 8 

Dark Green . 8 

Light Blue . ... 1 

Light Green 7 

Light Brown - ■ S 

White 5 

100 
Fine example of a harmony of a dominant hue. 



Plate XCIII 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM CHINESE PORCELAIN 



Deep Lapis Lazuli Blue Ground . 50 

Turquoise Blue ....... 29 

Ochre Yellow . . 12 

Violet ... 9 

100 

Plates XCril to XCVII inclusive are from Chinese 
porcelain, the colors having remained brilliant. 



Plate XCIV 




COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A "BLACK 
HAWTHORN VASE" 



Black Ground .......... ... 30 

Green-White Flowers ..... 26 

Green Leaves .... 20 

Yellow-Green Leaves ............ 10 

Brown Stems 3 

Pale Red Flowers ...... 5 

Yellow " . , , 6 

100 



Plate XCV 



































■ 










■ 
■ 


































■ 




















■ 














■ 


S^i 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A ROSE-COLORED 
VASE 



Rose Ground 
White Panel . 
Blue-Green 
Yellow-Green 
Yellow . . 
Deep Pink 
Blue . , . 



, 60 

23 

10 

3 

7 
5 
2 

100 



Plate XCVI 



■m 



wSBmmBBBWl^m 

■■■ 

■■■ 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM YELLOW CHINESE 
PORCELAIN VASE 



Yellow Ground 44 

Light Green Leaves 23 

Dark Green " 8 

Cream White Flowers 16 

Brown Stems ....... 9 

100 



Plate XCVII 






COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A CHINESE "EGG- 
SHELL" PLATE 



Blue ..;... . . 18 

Yellow . . . . I . . , .18 

White ... . . . . 18 

Green 18 

Pink ......... 18 

Darli Pink ....... .... 3 

Dark Green . . . . . .3 

Black 2 

Gold ■ ... 2 

100 
Pale tints with delicate decoration in strong tones. 



Plate XCVm 



I 



COLOR ANALYSIS FROM A BUTTERFLY 



Dark Yellow Shade ....... SO 

Medium Yellow ....... 25 

Light Yellow ......... 20 

Silver . . . , . . . 15 

Black 10 

100 

The black was well placed to contrast with the light 
tones, the silver to contrast with the dark tone. 



Plate XCIX 




COLOR ANALVSIS FROM A STONE 

Pale Gray-Green ...... 40 

Gray-Green ...... 35 

Pale Red .... 25 

100 
Ground, pale green. 



Plate C 



COLOR NOTE FROM AN OLD AND PARTLY 
DISCOLORED PROPELLER FLANGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 



Plate CII 




COLOR NOTE FROM A SUNSET SKY 



MISSING 



PAGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 



Plate CIV 



^H- 




COLOR NOTE FROM EVERGREENS AGAINST 
A GRAY-BLUE RAIN CLOUD 



Plate CV 




COLOR NOTE FROM A SHADOW ON 
WHITE GROUND 



MISSING 



PAGE 



MISSING 



PAGE 



Plate CVII 




Plate CVIII 




COLOR NOTE FROM ORANGE CANNA BLOSSOM 
with part of leaf 



/ 1 r- i 



y 



Plate CIX 




Plate CX 





COLOR NOTE FROM OAK LEAVES AGAINST 
A DISTANT HILLSIDE 



/-s^^s 



^ f ty; ^ 



Plate CXI 



I 



COLOR NOTE FROM OATS SEEN FROM THE 
EDGE OF THE FIELp 

So the top was a mass of soft blue-gray-green, while the 
stalks were highly colored. 






Plate CXII 



\ 




COLOR NOTE FROM A PUSSY WILLOW 



Plate CXIV 




COLOR NOTE FROM A TREE FUNGUS 
Texture like velvet. 



Plate CXV 



Snow in Sunshine 



Shadows on Snow 



m 




Tict iiu^.™, ■ 



COLOR SCHEME FROM WINTER LANDSCAPE 
BETWEEN BALTIMORE AND WASHINGTON 



Plate GXVI 




SPECTRAL RED 
NEUTRALIZED BY BLACK AND WHITE 



Plate CXVII 



SPECTRAL YELLOW 
NEUTRALIZED BY BLACK AND WHITE