Skip to main content

Full text of "The descent of man : and selection in relation to sex"

See other formats


vis 
yitaese 

as 

hy 


sleet 
retary 


iti tee 


be: 

ot 
we! 
i 


aceleetecntaet te 

aeleleiMataiele sl eit 

SBR Meg 
ieee 


CORNELL 
UNIVERSITY 
LIBRARY 


i: 


Gift’in memory of 


MARY STEPHENS SHERMAN, °13 
from 


JOHN H. SHERMAN, ’11 


HATE DUE 
ae 


32 ‘MAO 100 


Cornell Univer: 


ion 


Cornell University 


Library 


The original of this book is in 
the Cornell University Library. 


There are no known copyright restrictions in 
the United States on the use of the text. 


http://www.archive.org/details/cu31924024753851 


THE DESCENT OF MAN 


AND 
SELECTION IN RELATION TO SEX 


BY 


CHARLES DARWIN, M.A., LL.D., F.R.S. 


PART ONE 


NEW YORK 
P. F. COLLIER & SON 
MCMII 
2 \4 oles 


1 3650 


SCIENCE 


PREFACE TO THE SECOND EDITION 


DuRin@ the successive reprints of the first edition of this 
work, published in 1871, I was able to introduce several im- 
portant corrections; and now that more time has elapsed, 1 
have endeavored to profit by the fiery ordeal through which 
the book has passed, and have taken advantage of all the 
criticisms which seem to me sound. I am also greatly in- 
debted to a large number of correspondents for the commu- 
nication of a surprising number of new facts and remarks. 
These have been so numerous, that I have been able to use 
only the more important ones; and of these, as well as of 
the more important corrections, I will append a list. Some 
new illustrations have been introduced, and four of the old 
drawings have been replaced by better ones, done from life 
by Mr. T. W. Wood. I must especially call attention to 
some observations which I owe to the kindness of Profes- 
sor Huxley (given as a supplement at the end of Part I.), 
on the nature of the differences between the brains of man 
and the higher apes. I have been particularly glad to give © 
these observations, because during the last few years sev- 
eral memoirs on the subject have appeared on the Conti- 
nent, and their importance has been, in some cases, greatly 
exaggerated by popular writers. 

I may take this opportunity of remarking that my critics 
frequently assume that I attribute all changes of corporeal 
structure and mental power exclusively to the natural se- 
lection of such variations as are often called spontaneous; 
whereas, even in the first edition of the ‘‘Origin of Species,”’ 
T distinctly stated that great weight must be attributed to 
the inherited effects of use and disuse, with respect both 


| (8) 


4 PREFACE TO THE SECOND EDITION 


to the body and mind. I also attributed some amount of 
modification to the direct and prolonged action of changed 
conditions of life. Some allowance, too, must be made for 
occasional reversions of structure; nor must we forget what 
I have called ‘‘correlated’’ growth, meaning thereby, that 
various parts of the organization are in some unknown man- 
ner so connected, that when one part varies, so do others; 
and if variations in the one are accumulated by selection, 
other parts will be modified. Again, it has been said by 
several critics, that when I found that many details of 
structure in man could not be explained through natural 
selection, I invented sexual selection; I gave, however, a 
tolerably clear sketch of this principle in the first edition 
of the ‘‘Origin of Species,’’ and I there stated that it was 
applicable to man. This subject of sexual selection has been 
treated at full length in the present work, simply because an 
opportunity was here first afforded me. I have been struck 
with the likeness of many of the half-favorable criticisms on 
sexual selection with those which appeared at first on natu- 
ral selection; such as, that it would explain some few de- 
tails, but certainly was not applicable to the extent to which 
Ihave employed it. My conviction of the power of sexual 
selection remains unshaken; but it is probable, or almost 
certain, that several of my conclusions will hereafter be 
found erroneous; this can hardly fail to be the case in the 
first treatment of a subject. When naturalists have become 
familiar with the idea of sexual selection, it will, as I be- 
lieve, be much more largely accepted; and it has already 
been fully and favorably received by several capable judges. 


Down, BECKENHAM, KENT, 
September, 1874 


TABLE 


OF THE 


PRINCIPAL ADDITIONS AND CORRECTIONS TO 
THE PRESENT EDITION 


First Edition 


Present Edition 


Vou. I. Vol, I. 
Page Page 
22 33-35 Discussion on the rudimentary 
points in the human ear re- 
vised. 
26 38 Cases of men born with hairy 
bodies. 
27, note. 39, note. |Mantegazza on the last molar. 
tooth in man. 
29 42-43 |The rudiments of a tail in man. 
32, note. 44, note. | Bianconi on homologous struct- 
ures, a8 explained by adapta- 
tion on mechanical principles. 
40 101 Intelligence in a baboon. 
42 102 Sense of humor in dogs. 
44 104-105 | Further facts on imitation in man 
and animals. 
47 106 Reasoning power in the lower 
animals. 
50 113-114 Pes of experience by ani- 
mals. 
53 117 Power of abstraction in animals. 
58 124-125 |Power of forming concepts in re- 
lation to language. 
64 128 Pleasure from certain sounds, 
colors, and forms. 
78 142 Fidelity in the elephant. 
79 148 Galton on gregariousness of 
cattle. 
eae =) 144-145 | Parental affection. 
the ip, note. 158, note. |! Persistence of enmity and hatred. 


@) 


6 TABLE OF THE PRINCIPAL ADDITIONS AND 


First Edition Present Edition 
Vol. L Vol. L 
Page Page 
91 155 Nature and strength of shame, 
regret, and remorse. 
94 ° 159, note. {Suicide among savages. 
97 163, note. |The motives of conduct. 

112 49 Selection, as applied to primeval 
man. 

122 58-59 Resemblances between idiots and 
animals. 

124, note. 61, note. | Division of the malar bone. 
125, note. | 59-60, note. |Supernumerary mamme and 
igits. 
128-129 64-65 Further cases of muscles proper 
to animals appearing in man. 

146 81, note. |Broca: average capacity of skull 
diminished by the preservation 
of the inferior members of 
society. 

149 84 Belt on advantages to man from 
his hairlessness. 

150 85-86 Disappearance of the tail in man 
and certain monkeys. 

169 180-181 |Injurious forms of selection in 
civilized nations. 

180 192 Indolence of man, when free from 
a struggle for existence. 

193 208 Gorilla protecting himself from 
rain with his hands, 

208, note. | 216, note. | Hermaphroditism in fish. 

209 218 Rudimentary mammeze in male 

mammals. 

239 248-251 | Changed conditions lessen fertil- 

; ity and cause ill-health among 
savages. 

245 257-260 |Darkness of skin a protection 
against the sun. 

250 263-273 | Note oy Professor Huxley on the 
development of the brain in 
man and apes. 

256 276-278 |Special organs of male parasitic 


worms for holding the female, 


CORRECTIONS TO THE PRESENT EDITION 7 


First Edition 


Present Edition 


Vol. I. Vol. I. 
Page Page 
275-276 295-297 |Greater variability of male than 
female; direct action of the en- 
vironment in causing differ- 
ences between the sexes. 

290 “310 Period of development of pro- 
tuberances on birds’ heads de- 
termines their transmission to 
one or both sexes. 

301 821-323 |Causes of excess of male births. 

314 336 Proportion of the sexes in the 
bee family. 

815 338-340 |Excess of males perhaps some- 
times detuned by selection. 

327 350 Bright colors of lowly organized 
animals. ; 

838 859 Sexual] selection among spiders. 
339 360 Cause of smallness of male 
spiders. 

345 366 Use of phosphorescence of the 
glow-worm. 

849 369 The humming noises of flies. 

350 370 Use of bright colors to Hemip- 
tera ve. 

851 370-871 |Musical apparatus of Homoptera. 

854 374-875 |Development of stridulating ap- 

859 379, note. paratus in Orthoptera. 

366 884-885 |Herman Miiller on sexual differ- 
ences of bees. ; 

387 402 Sounds produced by moths. 

397 410 Display of beauty by butter- 
flies. 

401 414 Female butterflies, taking the 
more active part in courtship, 
brighter than their males. 

412 421-493 |Further cases of mimicry in but- 
terflies and moths. 

417 424 Cause of bright and diversified 

Vol. IL Vol. I. colors of caterpillars. 
2 432 Brushlike scales of male Mallo- 


tus. 


8 


TABLE OF THE PRINCIPAL ADDITIONS AND 


First Edition 


Vol. IL 


Present Edition 


Vol. UL. 

Page Page 

14 443-444 |Further facts on courtship of 
fishes, and the spawning of 
Macropus. 

23 451 | Dufossé on the sounds made by 
fishes. 

26 454 Belt on a frog protected by bright 
coloring. 

30 458 Further facts on mental powers 
of snakes. 

82 459 Sounds produced by snakes; the 

rattlesnake. 

36 463 Combats of Chameleons. 

72 496 Marshall on protuberances on 
birds’ heads. 

91 518 Further facts on display by the 
Argus pheasant. 

108 530 Attachment between paired birds. 
118 5389 Female pigeon rejecting certain 
males. 

120 541 Albino birds not finding partners, 
in a state of nature. 

124 545-546 | Direct action of climate on birds’ 
colors. 

147-150 560-569 | Further facts on the ocelli in the 
Argus pheasant. 

152 571 Display by humming birds in 
courtship. 

157 574-575 |Cases with pigeons of color trans- 
mitted to one sex alone. — 

232 640-642 | Taste for the beautiful permanens. 
enough to allow of sexual selec- 
tion with the lower animals. 

247 652 Horns of sheep originally a mas- 
culine character. 

248 653 nese affecting horns of ani- 
mals. 

256 662 Prong-horned variety of Cervus 
virginianus. 

260 666 Relative sizes of male and female 


whales and seals. 


CORRECTIONS TO THE PRESENT EDITION 9 


First Edition 


Present Edition 


Vol. I. Vol. IL. 
Page Page 
266 672 _| Absence of tusks in male miocene 
igs. 
286 690 Dabion on sexual differences of 
ats. ; 
299 701 Reeks on advantage from pecul- 
iar coloring. 
316 719 Difference of complexion in men 
and women of an African tribe. 
337 736 Speech subsequent to singing. 
356 154 Schopenhauer on importance of 
courtship to mankind. 

859 et seg. | 756 et seg. | Revision of discussion on com- 
munal marriages and promis- 
cuity. 

3738 769-770 |Power of choice of woman in 
marriage, among savages. 
380 776 Long-continued habit of plucking 


out hairs may produce an in- 
inherited effect. 


CONTENTS 


INTRODUCTION . - ‘ e 5 . . . . ° . 


PART I 
THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 


CHAPTER I 
THE EVIDENCE OF THE DESCENT OF MAN FROM SOME LOWER FORM 


Nature of the evidence bearing on the origin of mari—Homologous struc- 
tures in man and the lower animals—Miscellaneous points of corre- 
spondence—Development—Rudimentary structures, muscles, sense- 
organs, hair, bones, reproductive organs, ete. —The bearing of these 
three great classes of facts on the origin of man _ . 3 7 


CHAPTER II 


Nn 


21 


ON THE MANNER OF DEVELOPMENT OF MAN FROM SOME LOWER FORM 


Variability of body and mind in man—Inheritance—Causes of variability 
—tLaws of variation the same in man as in the lower animals—Direct 
action of the conditions of life—Effects of the increased use and dis- 
use of parts—Arrested development—Reversion—Correlated variation 
—Rate of increase—Checks to increase—Natural selection—Man the 
most dominant animal in the world—Importance of his corporeal struc- 
ture—The causes which have led to his becoming erect—Consequent 
changes of structure—Decrease in size of the canine teeth—Increased 
size and altered shape of the skuil—Nakedness—Absence of a tail 
—Defenceless condition of man 2 f 3 . . - 7 


CHAPTER IIt 


£6 


COMPARISON OF THE MENTAL POWERS OF MAN AND THE LOWER ANIMALS 


The difference in mental power between the highest ape and the lowest 
savagé, immeénse—Certain instincts in common—The emotions— 
Curiosity—Imitation-— Attention — Memory—Imagination—Reason— 
Progressive Improvemetit—Tools and weapons used by animals— 
Abstraction, self-consciousness—Language—Sense of beauty—Beliet 


in God, spiritual agencies, superstitions . . . . . . 
(11) 


94 


12 CONTENTS 


CHAPTER IV 


OOMPARISON OF THE MENTAL POWERS OF MAN AND THE LOWER ANIMALS 
continued 


The moral sense—Fundamental proposition—The qualities of social animals 
—Origin of sociability—Struggle between opposed instincts—Man a 
social animal—The more enduring social instincts conquer other less 
persistent instincts—The social virtues alone regarded by savages—The 
self-regarding virtues acquired at a later stage of development—The 
importance of the judgment of the members of the same community — 
on conduct—Transmission of moral tendencies—Summary . : . 134 


CHAPTER V 


ON THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE INTELLECTUAL AND MORAL FAOULTIES DURING 
PRIMEVAL AND CIVILIZED TIMES 


Advancement of the intellectual powers through natural selection—Impor- 
tance of imitation—Social and moral faculties—Their development 
within the limits of the same tribe—Natural selection as affecting civ- 
ilized nations—Evidence that civilized nations were once barbarous . 173 


CHAPTER VI 
ON THE AFFINITIES AND GENEALOGY OF MAN 


Position of man in the animal series—The natural system genealogical— 
Adaptive characters of slight value—Various small points of resem- 
blance between man and ithe Quadrumana—Rank of man in the nat- 
ural system—Birthplace and antiquity of man—Absence of fossil con- 
necting-links—Lower stages in the genealogy of man, as inferred, 
first, from his atiinities, and, secondly, from his structure—LHarly _ 
androgynous condition of the Vertebrata—Conclusion . ‘ - 196 


CHAPTER VII 
ON THE RACES OF MAN 


The nature and value of specific characters—Application to the races of 
man—Arguments in favor of, and opposed to, ranking the so-called 
races of man as distinct species—Sub-species—Monogenists and polyg- 
enists—Convergence of character—Numerous points of resemblance 
in body and mind between the most distinct races of man—The state 
of man when he first spread over the earth—EHach race ~~’ 7-7 * 

' “+ from a single pair—The extinction of races—The fr 
The effects of crossing—Slight influence of * 
conditions of life—Slight or no influence of 
selection . . : : . -¥ 


CONTENTS 


PART II 
SEXUAL SELECTION 


CHAPTER VIII 
PRINOIPLES OF SEXUAL SELECTION 


Secondary sexual characters—Sexual selection—Manner of action—Excess 
of males—Polygamy—The male alone generally modified through 
sexual selection—EHagerness of the male—Variability of the male— 
Choice exerted by the female—Sexual compared with natural selec- 
tion—Inheritance, at corresponding periods of life, at corresponding 
seasons of the year, and as limited by sex—Relations between the 
several forms of inheritance—Causes why one sex and the young are 
net modified through sexual selection—Supplement on the proportional 
numbers of the two sexes throughout the animal kingdom—tThe pro- 
portion of the sexes in relation to natural selection . . . 


CHAPTER IX 


13 


- a4 


SECONDARY SEXUAL CHARACTERS IN THE LOWER OLASSES OF THE ANIMAL 


KINGDOM 


These characters absent in the lowest classes—Brilliant colors—Mollusca— 
Annelids—Crustacea, secondary sexual characters strongly developed; 
dimorphism ; color; characters not acquired before maturity—Spiders, 
sexual colors of; stridulation by the males—Myriapoda_ . . : 


CHAPTER X 
SECONDARY SEXUAL OHARAOTERS OF INSEOTS 


Diversified structures possessed by the males for seizing the females—~ 
Differences between the sexes, of which the meaning is not under- 
stood—Difference in size between the sexes—Thysanura—Diptera— 
Hemiptera—Homoptera, musical powers possessed by tho males 

Ce “SAVagO, ImMeEe musical instruments of the males, much diversi- 

Curiosity—Imitarion' Sacity 3 colors—Neuroptera, sexual differences 

Progressive ImprovemenDusnacity and colors—Coleoptera, colors; 

Abstraction, self-conscious#? apparently as an ornament; battles; 


in God, spiritual agencies, supamon to both sexes. . " - 


344 


362 


14 CONTENTS 


CHAPTER XI 
INSECTS, continued—ORDER LEPIDOPTERA (BUTTERFLIES AND MOTHS) 


Courtship of butterflies—Battles—Ticking noise—Colors common to both 
sexes, or more brilliant in the males—Examples—Not due to the direct 
action of the conditions of life—Colors adapted for protection—Colors 
of moths—Display—Perceptive powers of the Lepidoptera—Variability 
—Causes of the difference in color between the males and females— 
Mimicry, female butterflies more brilliantly colored than the males 
—Bright colors of caterpillars—Summary and concluding remarks 
on the secondary sexual characters of insects—Birds and insects 
compared 2 ‘ ‘ : : : 5 , 5 : . 401 


CHAPTER XII 
SECONDARY SEXUAL CHARACTERS OF FISHES, AMPHIBIANS, AND REPTILES 


Fisuzs: Courtship and battles of the males—Larger size of the females 
—Males, bright colors and ornamental appendages; other strange 
characters—Colors and appendages acquired by the males during the 
breeding season alone—Fishes with both sexes brilliantly colored— 
Protective colors—The less conspicuous colors of the female cannot 
be accounted for on the principle of protection—Male fishes building 
nests, and taking charge of the ova and young. AMPHIBIANS: Differ- 
ences in structure and color between the sexes—Vocal organs. REP- 
TILES: Chelonians—Crocodiles—Snakes, colors in some cases protec- 
tive—Lizards, battles of—Ornamental appendages—Strange differences 
in structure between the sexes—Colors—Sexual differences almost as 
great as with birds . : , ‘ 7 5 3 = . . 431 


CHAPTER XIII 
SECONDARY SEXUAL CHARACTERS OF BIRDS 


Sexual differences—Law of battle—Special weapons—Vocal organs— 
Instrumental music—Love-antics and dances—Decorations, perma- 
nent and seasonal—Double and single annual moults—Display of 
ornaments by the males a 3 : i : ‘ . 466 


CHAPTER XIV 
BIRDS—continued 
Choice exerted by the female—Length of courtship—Unpaired birds— 
Mental qualities and taste for the beautiful—Preference or antipathy 
shown by the female for particular males—Variability of birds— 
Variations sometimes abrupt—Laws of variation—Formation of ocelli 
—Gradations of character—Case of Peacock, Argus pheasant, and 
Urosticte ‘ ‘ a> abe . : ‘ : . : . 522 


CONTENTS 15 


CHAPTER XV 
BIRDS—continued 


Discussion as to why the males alone of some species, and both sexes of 
others, are brightly colored—On sexually limited inheritance, as ap- 
plied to various structures and to brightly colored plumage—Nidifica- 
tion in relation to color—Loss of nuptial plumage during the winter 5972 


CHAPTER XVI 
BIRDS—concluded 


The immature plumage in relation to the character of the plumage in both 
sexes when adult—Six classes of cases—Sexual differences between 
the males of closely allied or representative species—The female as- 
suming the characters of the male—Plumage of the young in relation 
to the summer and winter plumage of the adulis—On the increase 
of beauty in the birds of the world—Protective coloring—Conspicu- 
ously colored birds—Novelty appreciated—Summary of the four chap- 
ters on birds. : ef e 5 » 3 = .  . 598 


CHAPTER XVII 
" SECONDARY SEXUAL CHARACTERS OF MAMMALS 


The law of battle—Special weapons, confined to the males—Cause of 
absence of weapons in the female—Weapons common to both sexes, 
yet primarily acquired by the male—Other uses of such weapons— 
Their high importance—Greater size of the male—Means of defence— 
On the preference shown by either sex in the pairing of quadrupeds 646 


CHAPTER XVIII 
SECONDARY SEXUAL CHARACTERS OF MAMMALS—continued 


Voice—Remarkable sexual peculiarities in seals—Odor—Development of 
the hair—Color of the hair and skin—Anomalous case of the female 
being more ornamented than the male—Color and ornaments due to 
sexual selection—Color acquired for the sake of protection—Color, 
though common to both sexes, often due to sexual selection—On the 
disappearance of spots and stripes in adult quadrupeds—On the colors 
and ornaments of the Quadrumana—Summary . . ° . 619 


16 CONTENTS 


PART III 


SEXUAL SELECTION IN RELATION TO UAN 


AND CONCLUSION 


CHAPTER XIX 
SECONDARY SEXUAL CHARACTERS OF MAN 


Differences between man and woman—Causes of such differences and of 
certain characters common to both sexes—Law of battle—Differences 
in mental powers, and voice—On the influence of beauty in determin- 
ing the marriages of mankind—Attention paid by savages to ornaments 
—Their ideas of beauty in woman—The tendency to exaggerate each 
natural peculiarity ‘ : ‘ ‘ : . ‘ : . 


CHAPTER XX 


SECONDARY SEXUAL OHARACTERS OF MAN—continued 


On the effects of the continued selection of women according to a different 


standard of beauty in each race—On the causes which interfere with 
sexual selection in civilized and savage nations—Conditions favorable 
to sexual selection during primeval times—On the manner of action of 
sexual selection with mankind—On the women in savage tribes having 
some power to choose their husbands—Absence of hair on the body, 
and development of the beard—Color of the skin—Summary . ° 


CHAPTER XXI 


GENERAL SUMMARY AND CONOLUSION 


Main conclusion that man is descended from some lower form—Manner 


of development—Genealogy of man—Intellectual and moral faculties 
—Sexual selection—Concluding remarks : ‘ : F . 


InpDEx ‘ é é . a é si ° ° A ° . 


716 


158 


THE DESCENT OF MAN 


AND 


SELECTION IN RELATION TO SEX 


INTRODUCTION 


THE nature of the following work will be best understood 
by a brief account of how it came to be written. During 
many years I collected notes on the origin or descent of 
man, without any intention of publishing on the subject, but 
rather with the determination not to publish, as I thought 
that I should thus only add to the prejudices against my 
views. It seemed to me sufficient to indicate, in the first 
edition of my ‘‘Origin of Species,’’ that by this work ‘‘light 
would be thrown on the origin of man and his history’’; 
and this implies that man must be included with other 
organic beings in any general conclusion respecting his 
manner of appearance on this earth. Now the case wears 
a wholly different aspect. When a naturalist like Carl Vogt 
ventures to say in his address as President of the National 
Institution of Geneva (1869), ‘‘personne, en Europe au 
moins, n’ose plus soutenir la création indépendante et de 
toutes piéces, des espéces,’’ it is manifest that at least 
a large number of naturalists must admit that species are 
the. modified descendants of other species; and this espe- 
cially holds good with the younger and rising naturalists. 
The greater number accept the agency of natural selection; 
though some urge, whether with justice the future must de- 
cide, that I have greatly overrated its importance. Of the 
older and honored chiefs in natural science, many unfortu- 
nately are still opposed to evolution in every form. 


(17). 


18 INTRODUCTION 


In consequence of the views now adopted by most natu- 
ralists, and which will ultimately, as in every other case, be 
followed by others who are not scientific, I have been led 
to put together my notes, so as to see how far the general 
conclusions arrived at in my former works were applicable 
to man. This seemed all the more desirable, as I had never 
deliberately applied these views to a species taken singly. 
When we confine our attention to any one form, we are de- 
prived of the weighty arguments derived from the nature 
of the affinities which connect together whole groups of 
organisms—their geographical distribution in past and pres- 
ent times, and their geological succession. The homological 
structure, embryological development, and rudimentary or- 
gans of a species remain to be considered, whether it be man 
or any other animal to which our attention may be directed; 
but these great classes of facts afford, as it appears to me, 
ample and conclusive evidence in favor of the principle of 
gradual evolution. The strong support derived from the 
other arguments should, however, always be kept before 
the mind. 

The sole object of this work is to consider, first, whether 
man, like every other species, is descended from some pre- 
existing form; secondly, the manner of his development; 
and thirdly, the value of the differences between the so- 
called races of man. As I shall confine myself to these 
points, it will not be necessary to describe in detail the 
differences between the several races—an enormous subject 
which has been fully discussed in many valuable works. 
The high antiquity of man has recently been demonstrated 
by the labors of a host of eminent men, beginning with 
M. Boucher de Perthes; and this is the indispensable basis 
for understanding his origin. I shall, therefore, take this 
conclusion for granted, and may refer my readers to the 
admirable treatises of Sir Charles Lyell, Sir John Lubbock, 
and others. Nor shall I have occasion to do more than to 
allude to the amount of difference between man and the 
anthropomorphous apes; for Prof. Huxley, in the opinion 


INTRODUCTION 18 


of most competent judges, has conclusively shown that in 
every visible character man differs less from the higher apes 
than these do from the lower members of the same order 
of Primates. , 

This work contains hardly any original facts in regard to 
man; but as the conclusions at which I arrived, after drawing 
up a rough draught, appeared to me interesting, I thought 
that they might interest others. It has often and confidently 
been asserted that man’s origin can never be known; but 
ignorance more frequently begets confidence than does 
knowledge; it is those who know little, and not those 
who know much, who so positively assert that this or that 
problem will never be solved by science. The conclusion 
that man is the co-descendant with other species of some 
ancient, lower, and extinct form, is not in any degree new. 
Lamarck long ago came to this conclusion, which has lately 
been maintained by several eminent naturalists and philoso- 
phers; for instance, by Wallace, Huxley, Lyell, Vogt, Lub- 
bock, Biichner, Rolle, etc.,’ and especially by Hickel. This 
last naturalist, besides his great work, ‘‘Generelle Morpholo- 
gie’”’ (1866), has recently (1868, with a second edit. in 1870) 
published his ‘‘Natiirliche Schdépfungsgeschichte,’’ in which 
he fully discusses the genealogy of man. If this work had 
appeared before my essay had been written, I should prob- 
ably never have completed it. Almost all the conclusions 
at which I have arrived I find confirmed by this naturalist, 
whose knowledge on many points is much fuller than mine. 
Wherever I have added any fact or view from Prof. Hickel’s 


1 As the works of the first-named authors are so well known, I need not 
give the titles; but as those of the latter are less well known in England, I 
will give them: ‘‘Sechs Vorlesungen iiber.die Darwin’sche Theorie’’: zweite 
Auflage, 1868, von Dr. L. Biichner; translated into French under the title 
“‘Confétences sur la Théorie Darwinienne,’’ 1869. ‘‘Der Mensch, im Lichte 
der Darwin’sche Lehre,’’ 1865, von Dr. F. Rolle. I will not attempt to give 
references to all the authors who have taken the same side of the question, 
Thus G. Canestrini has published (‘‘Annuario della Soc. d. Nat.,’’ Modena, 
186%, p. 81) a very curious paper on rudimentary characters, as bearing on the 
origin of man. Another work has (1869) been published by Dr. Francesco 
Barrago, bearing in Italian the title of ‘‘Man, made in the image of God, was 
also made in-the image of the ape.” 


20 INTRODUCTION 


writings, I give his authority in the text; other statements 
I leave as they originally stood in my manuscript, occasion- 
ally giving in the footnotes references to his works, as & 
confirmation of the more doubtful or interesting points. 

During many years it has seemed to me highly probable 
that sexual selection has played an important part in differ- 
entiating the races of man; but in my ‘‘Origin of Species’ 
(first edition, ’p. 199) I contented myself by merely alluding 
to this belief. When I came to apply this view to man, I 
found it indispensable to treat the whole subject in full 
detail.* Consequently the second part of the present work, 
treating of sexual selection, has extended to an inordinate 
length, compared with the first part; but this could not 
be avoided. 

I had intended adding to the present volumes an essay 
on the expression of the various emotions by man and the 
lower animals. My attention was called to this subject many 
years ago by Sir Charles Bell’s admirable work. This illus- 
trious anatomist maintains that man is endowed with certain 
muscles solely for the sake of expressing his emotions. As 
this view is obviously opposed to the belief that man is de- 
scended from some other and lower form, it was necessary 
for me to consider it. I likewise wished to ascertain how 
far the emotions are expressed in the same manner by the 
different races of man. But owing to the length of the pres- 
ent work, I have thought it better to reserve my essay for 
separate publication. 


? Prof. Hackel was the only author who, at the time when this work first 
appeared, had discussed the subject of sexual selection, and had seen its ful 
importance, since the publication of the *‘Origin’’; and this he did in a very 
able manner in his various works, 


PART ONE 
THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 


a 


CHAPTER I 


THE EVIDENCE OF THE DESCENT OF MAN FROM 
SOME LOWER FORM 


Nature of the evidence bearing on the origin of man—Homologous strue- 
tures in man and the lower animals—Miscellaneous points of corre- 
spondence—Development—Rudimentary structures, muscles, sense- 
organs, hair, bones, reproductive organs, etc.—The bearing of these 
three great classes of facts on the origin of man 


E who wishes to decide whether man is the modified 

H descendant of some pre-existing form would prob- 
ably first inquire whether man varies, however 
slightly, in bodily structure and in mental faculties; and 
if so, whether the variations are transmitted to his offspring 
in accordance with the laws which prevail with the lower 
animals. Azain, are the variations the result, as far as our 
ignorance permits us to judge, of the same general causes, 
and_-are they governed bythe same general laws, as in the 
ease of other organisms; for sastance, by correlation, the 
inherited effects of use and disusé,cte-? Is man subject 
to similar malconformations, the result dretitested develop- 
ment, of reduplication of parts, etc., and dow “he display in 
any of his anomalies reversion to some former a. 4-2ncient 
type of structure? It might also naturally be inquitew 
whether man, like so many other animals, has given rise 
to varieties and sub-races differing but slightly from each 
other, or to races differing so much that they must be 
classed as doubtful species. How are such races distrib- 
uted over the world; and how, when crossed, do they react 

(21) 


22 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


on each other in the first and succeeding generations? 
And so with many other points. 

The inquirer would next come to the important point 
whether man tends to increase at so rapid a rate as to lead 
to occasional severe struggles for existence; and conse- 
quently to beneficial variations, whether in body or mind, 
being preserved, and injurious ones eliminated. Do the 
races or species of men, whichever term may be applied, 
encroach on and replace one another, so that some finally 
become extinct? We shall see that all these questions, as 
indeed is obvious in respect to most of them, must be an- 
swered in the affirmative, in the same manner as with the 
lower animals. But the several considerations just referred 
to may be conveniently deferred for a time; and we will first 
see how far the bodily structure of man shows traces, more 
or less plain, of his descent from some lower form. In suc- 
ceeding chapters, the mental powers of man, in comparison 
with those of the lower animals, will be considered. 


The Bodily Structure of Man.—It is notorious that man 
is constructed on the same general type or model as other 
mammals. All the bones in his skeleton can be compared 
with corresponding bones in a monkey, bat, or seal. So 
it is with his muscles, nerves, blood-vessels, and internal 
viscera. The brain, the most importag¢ stmt tie organs, 
follows the same law, as shown by Huxley and other anato- 
mists. Bischoff,’ who is a béstile witness, admits that every 
chief fissure and fold }7‘the brain of man has its analogy in 
that of the oren~phut he adds that at no period of deveiop- 
ment do the’ ¢orains perfectly agree; norcould perfect agree- 
eerste expected, for otherwise their mental powers would 
have been the same. Vulpian* remarks: ‘‘Les différences 


aes hirnwindungen des Menschen,” 1868, 8. 96. The conclusions of 
this ae as well as thoes of Gratiolet and Aeby, concerning the brain, will 
be diseussed by Prof, Huxley in the Appendix alluded to in the Preface to this 


aac ee sur la Physiologie,” 1866, p. 890, as quoted by M. Dally, “L’Ordre 
des Primates et le Transformisme,”’ 1868, p. 29. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 23 ~ 


réelles qui existent entre l’encéphale de l’homme et celui 
des singes supérieurs, sont bien minimes. TI] ne faut pas se 
faire d’illusions 4 cet égard. L’homme est bien plus pras 
des singes anthropomorphes par les caractéres anatomiques 
de son cerveau que ceux-ci ne le sont non-seulement des 
autres mammiféres, mais méme de certains quadrumanes, 
des guenons et des macaques.’’ But it would be superflu- 
ous here to give further details on the correspondence be- 
tween man and the higher mammals in the structure of the 
brain and all other parts of the body. 

It may, however, be worth while to specify a few points, 
not directly or obviously connected with structure, by which 
this correspondence or relationship is well shown. 

Man is liable to receive from the lower animals, and to 
communicate to them, certain diseases, as hydrophobia, vari- 
ola, the glanders, syphilis, cholera, herpes, etc.;* and this 
fact proves the close similarity‘ of their tissues and blood, 
both in minute structure and composition, far more plainly 
than does their comparison under the best microscope, er 
by the aid of the best chemical analysis. Monkeys are lia- 
ble to many of the same non-contagious diseases as we are; 
thus Rengger,* who carefully observed for a long time the 
Cebus Azarce in its native land, found it liable to catarrh, 
with the usual symptoms, and which, when often recurrent, 
led to consumption. These monkeys suffered also from 
apoplexy, inflammation of the bowels, and cataract in the 
eye. The younger ones when shedding their milk-teeth 
often died from fever. Medicines produced the same effect 
on them as on us. Many kinds of monkeys have a strong 


8 Dr. W. Lauder Lindsay has treated this subject at some length in the 
“Journal of Mental Science,’’ July, 1871; and in the ‘‘Edinburgh Veterinary 
Review,’’ July, 1858. ; 

4 A Reviewer has criticised (‘‘British Quarterly Review,’’ Oct. 1, 1871, 
2) what I have here said with much severity and contempt; but, as I do 

‘use the term identity, I cannot see that I am greatly in error. There 
rs to me a strong analogy between the same infection or contagion pro- 

mors the same result, or one closely similar, in two distinct animals, and the - 
of two distinet fluids by the same chemical reagent. 

*Naturgeschichte der Sdugethiere von Paraguay,’’ 1830, s. 50. 


Del 


24 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


taste for tea, coffee, and spirituous liquors; they will also, 
as I have myself seen, smoke tobacco with pleasure. Brehm 
asserts that the natives of northeastern Africa catch the wild 
baboons by exposing vessels with strong beer, by which they 
are made drunk. He has seen some of these animals, which 
he kept in confinement, in this state; and he gives a laugh- 
able account of their behavior and strange grimaces. On the 
following morning they were very cross and dismal; they 
held their aching heads with both hands, and wore a most 
pitiable expression; when beer or wine was offered them, 
they turned away with disgust, but relished the juice of 
lemons.?, An American monkey, an Ateles, after getting 
drunk on brandy, would never touch it again, and thus 
was wiser than many men. These trifling facts prove how 
similar the nerves of taste must be in monkeys and man, 
and how similarly their whole nervous system is affected. 

Man is infested with internal parasites, sometimes caus- 
ing fatal effects; and is plagued by external parasites, all of 
which belong to the same genera or families as those infest- 
ing other mammals, and in the case of scabies to the same 
species.° Man is subject, like other mammals, birds, and 
even insects,® to that mysterious law which causes certain 
normal processes, such as gestation, as well as the matura- 
tion and duration of various diseases, to follow lunar periods. 
His wounds are repaired by the same process of healing; and 
the stumps left after the amputation of his limbs, especially 
during an early embryonic period, occasionally possess some 
power of regeneration, as in the lowest animals.” 


6 The same tastes are common to some animals much lower in the scale, 
Mr. A. Nicols informs me that he kept in Queensland, in Australia, three indi- 
viduals of the Phaseolarctus cinereus; and that, without having been taught 
in any way, they acquired a strong taste for rum, and for smoking tobacco, 

7 Brehm, ‘‘Thierleben,’’ B. i. 1864, 8. 75, 86. On the Ateles, s. 105. For 
other analogous siatements, see s. 25, 107. os, 

‘8 Dr, W. Lauder Lindsay, ‘Edinburgh Vet. Review,’’ July, 1858, p, 3° 
® With respect to insects see Dr. Laycock, ‘On a Ger-~al Law = 


Periodicity,” “British Association,” 1842. Dr. Maccullock = ~~ ,"0r 
American Journal of Science,”’ vol. xvii. p. 305, has seen : an 
tertian ague. Hereafter I shall return to this subject. fag 
10 T have given the evidence on this head in my ‘‘Var’ Cin 
Plants under Domestication,’’ vol. ii, p. 15, and more co ¢ i] 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 25 


The whole process of that most important function, the 
reproduction of the species, is strikingly the same in all 
mammals, from the first act of courtship by the male,” 
to the birth and nurturing of the young. Monkeys are 
born in almost as helpless a condition as our own infants; 
and in certain genera the young differ fully as much in 
appearance from the adults as do our children from their 
full-grown parents.” It has been urged by some writers, 
as an important distinction, that with man the young arrive 
at maturity at a much later age than with any other animal; 
but if we look to the races of mankind which inhabit tropi- 
cal countries the difference is not great, for the orang is 
believed not to be adult till the age of from ten to fifteen 
years.” Man differs from woman in size, bodily strength, 
hairiness, etc., as well as in mind, in the same manner as 
do the two sexes of many mammals. So that the corre- 
spondence in general structure, in the minute structure of 
the tissues, in chemical composition and in constitution, 
between man and the higher animals, especially the an- 
thropomorphous apes, is extremely close. 


Embryonic Development.—Man is developed from an 
ovule, about the 125th of an inch in diameter, which differs 
in no respect from the ovules of other animals. The em- 
bryo itself at a very early period can hardly be distin- 
guished from that of other members of the vertebrate king- 
dom. At this period the arteries run in arch-like branches, 


N “Mares e diversis generibus Quadrumanorum sine dubio dignoscunt femi- 
nas humanes a maribus, Primum, credo, odoratu, postea aspectu. Mr. Youatt, 
qui diu in Hortis Zoologicis (Bestiariis) medicus animalium erat, vir in rebus 
observandis cautus et sagax, hoc mihi certissime probavit, et curatores ejusdem 
loci et alii e ministris confirmaverunt. Sir Andrew Smith et Brehm notabant 
idem in Cynocephalo. Mlustrissimus Cuvier etiam narrat multa de hae re, qué 
ut opinor, nihil turpius potest indicari inter omnia hominibus et Quadrumanis 
communia, Narrat enim Cynocephalum quendam in furorem incidere aspectu 
feminarum aliquarum, sed nequaquam accendi tanto furore ab omnibus. Sem- 
per eligebat juniores, et dignoscebat in turb4, et advocabat voce gestfique.”” 

2 This remark is made with respect to Cynocephalus and the anthropo- 
morphous apes by Geoffroy Saint-Hilaire and F. Cuvier, ‘Hist. Nat. des 
Mammiféres,”? tom. i. 1824. 

18 Huxley, ‘“‘Man’s Place in Nature,’ 1863, p. 84. 


Descent—Vot. L—2 


26 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


as if to carry the blood to branchie which are not present in 
the higher vertebrata, though the slits on the sides of the 
neck still remain (/, g, Fig. 1), marking their former posi- 
tion. Ata somewhat later period, when the extremities are 

developed, ‘‘the feet of lizards and mammals,”’ as the illus- — 
trious Von Baer remarks, ‘‘the wings and feet of birds, no 

less than the hands and feet of man, all arise from the same 

fundamental form.’’ It is, says Prof. Huxley,” ‘‘quite in 

the later stages of development that the young human_ 
being presents marked differences from the young ape, 

while the latter departs as much from the dog in its de- 

velopments as the man does. Startling as this last assertion 

may appear to be, it is demonstrably true.”’ 

As some of my readers may never have seen a drawing 
of an embryo, I have given one of man and another of a 
dog, at about the same early stage of development, carefully 
copied from two works of undoubted accuracy.’* 

After the foregoing statements made by such high au- 
thorities, it would be superfluous on my part to give a num- 
ber of borrowed details, showing that the embryo of man 
closely resembles that of other mammals. It may, however, 
be added, that the human embryo likewise resembles certain 
low forms when adult in various points of structure. For 
instance, the heart at first exists as a simple pulsating ves- 
sel; the excreta are voided through a cloacal passage; and 
the os coceyx projects like a true tail, ‘‘extending consider- 
ably beyond the rudimentary legs.’’'* In the embryos of all 
air-breathing vertebrates, certain glands, called the corpora 


4 ‘“Man’s Place in Nature,’’ 1863, p. 67. 

1% The human embryo (upper fig.) is from Ecker, ‘‘Icones Phys.,’? 1851< 
1859, tab. xxx. fig. 2. This embryo was ten lines in length, so that the 
drawing is much magnified. The embryo of the dog is from Bischoff, ‘‘Ent- 
wicklungsgeschichte des Hunde-EHies,’’ 1845, tab. xi. fig. 42 B. This drawing 
is ve times magnified, the embryo being twenty-five days old. The internal 
viscera have been omitted, and the uterine appendages in both drawings re- 
moved. I was directed to these figures by Prof. Huxley, from whose work, 
‘‘Man’s Place in Nature,’’ the idea of giving them was taken. Hackel has also 
given analogous drawings in his ‘‘Sch6pfungsgeschichte.”’ 

16 Prof. Wyman in “‘Proc. of American Acad. of Sciences,’? vol, iv., 1860, 


p. 1%. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 27 


Wolffiana, correspond with and act like the kidneys of ma- 
ture fishes." Even at a later embryonic period, some strik- 
ing resemblances between man and the lower animals may 


Fia. 1.—Upper figure human embryo, from Ecker. Lower figure that of a dog, 
from Bischoff. a. Fore-brain, cerebral hemispheres, etc. 6. Mid-brain, corpora 
uadrigemina, c. Hind-brain, cerebellum, medulla oblongata. d. Bye. e. Har. f. 
irst visceral arch. g. Second visceral arch. H. Vertebral columns and muscles 
in process of development. 7, Anterior extremities. K. Posterior extremities, L. 
Tad or 08 Coccyx. 


17 Owen, “Anatomy of Vertebrates,”’ vol. i. p. 533. 


‘ 


28 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


be observed. Bischoff says that the convolutions of the 
brain in a human foetus at the end of the seventh month 
reach about the same stage of development as in a baboon 
when adult..° The great toe, as Prof. Owen remarks,” 
“which forms the fulerum when standing or walking, is 
perhaps the most characteristic peculiarity in the human 
structure’; but in an embryo, about an inch in length, 
Prof. Wyman” found ‘‘that the great toe was shorter than 
the others; and, instead of being parallel to them, projected 
at an angle from the side of the foot, thus corresponding 
with the permanent condition of this part in the quadru- 
mana.’’ I will conclude with a quotation from Huxley,” 
who, after asking, does man originate in a different way 
from a dog, bird, frog or fish? says, ‘‘the reply is not 
doubtful for a moment; without question, the mode of 
origin, and the early stages of the development of man, 
are identical with those of the animals immediatély below 
him in the scale; without a doubt in these respects, he is 
far nearer to apes than the apes are to the dog.”’ 


Rudiments.—This subject, though not intrinsically more 
important than the two last, will for several reasons be 
treated here more fully.” Not one of the higher animals 
can be named which does not bear some part in a rudi- 
mentary condition; and man forms no exception to the rule. 
Rudimentary organs must be distinguished from those that 
are nascent; though in some cases the distinction is not 
easy. The former are either absolutely useless, such as 
the mammz of male quadrupeds, or the incisor teeth of 
ruminants which never cut through the gums; or they are 


418 “Die Grosshirnwindungen des Menschen,’’ 1868, s. 95, 

19 ‘Anatomy of Vertebrates,’’ vol. i. p. 553. 

20 ‘Proc, Soc. Nat. Hist.’? Boston, 1863, vol. ix. p, 185. 

21 ‘*Man’s Place in Nature,”’ p. 65. 

22 T had written a rough copy of this chapter before reading a valuable paper, 
“‘Caratteri rudimentali in ordine all origine del uomo”’ (‘‘Annuario della Soc. d, 
Nat.,’? Modena, 1867, p. 81), by G. Canestrini, to which paper I am consid. 
erably indebted. Hackel has given admirable discussions on this whole 
subject, under the title of Dysteleology, in his ‘“‘Generelle Morphologie,’’ and 
*‘Schépfungsgeschichte. ” 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN ap 


of such slight service to their present possessors that we 
can hardly suppose that they were developed under the 
conditions which now exist. Organs in this latter state 
are not strictly rudimentary, but they are tending in this 
direction. Nascent organs, on the other hand, though not 
fully developed, are of high service to their possessors, and 
are capable of further development. Rudimentary organs 
are eminently variable; and this is partly intelligible, as 
they are useless, or nearly useless, and consequently are 
no longer subjected to natural selection. They often be- 
come wholly suppressed. When this occurs, they are never- 
theless liable to occasional reappearance through reversion 
—a circumstance well worthy of attention. 

The chief agents in causing organs to become rudimen- 
tary seem to have been disuse at that period of life when the 
organ is chiefly used (and this is generally during maturity), 
and also inheritance at a corresponding period of life. The 
term ‘‘disuse’’ does not relate merely to the lessened action 
of muscles, but includes a diminished flow of blood to a part 
or organ, from being subjected to fewer alternations of press- 
ure, or from becoming in any way less habitually active. 
Rudiments, however, may occur in one sex of those parts 
which are normally present in the other sex; and such rudi- 
ments, as we shall hereafter see, have often originated in a 
way distinct from those here referred to. In some cases, 
organs have been reduced by means of natural selection, 
from having become injurious to the species under changed 
habits of life. The process of reduction is probably often 
aided through the two principles of compensation and econ- 
omy of growth; but the later stages of reduction, after dis- 
use has done all that can fairly be attributed to it, and when 
‘tue coving to be effected by the economy of growth would 
be very smui:,” are difficult to understand. The final and 
complete suppres.‘on of a part, already useless and much 
reduced in size, in which case neither compensation nor 


33 Some good criticisms on this subject have been given by Messrs. Murie 
and Mivart, in ‘“‘Transact. Zoolog. Soc, ’’ 1869, vol. vii, p. 92. 


30 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


economy can come into play, is perhaps intelligible by the 
aid of the hypothesis of pangenesis. But as the whole sub- 
ject of rudimentary organs has been discussed and illustrated 
in my former works,* I need here say no more on this head. 

Rudiments of various muscles have been observed in 
many parts of the human body, and not a few muscles 
which are regularly present in some of the lower animals 
can occasionally be detected in man in a greatly reduced 
condition. Every one must have noticed the power which 
many animals, especially horses, possess of moving or 
twitching their skin; and this is effected by the pan- 
niculus carnosus. Remnants of this muscle in an efficient 
state are found in various parts of our bodies; for instance, 
the muscle on the forehead, by which the eyebrows are 
raised. The platysma myoides, which is well developed on 
the neck, belongs to this system. Prof. Turner, of Edin- 
burgh, has occasionally detected, as he informs me, muscu- 
lar fasciculi in five different situations, namely in the axille 
near the scapula, ete., all of which must be referred to the 
system of the panniculus. He has also shown” that the 
musculus sternalis or sternalis brutorum, which is not an ex- 
tension of the rectus abdominalis, but is closely allied to the 
panniculus, occurred in the proportion of about three per 
cent in upward of 600 bodies: he adds, that this muscle 
affords ‘‘an excellent illustration of the statement that oc- 
casional and rudimentary structures are especially liable 
to variation in arrangement.”’ 

Some few persons have the power of contracting the 
superficial muscles on their scalps; and these muscles are 
in a variable and partially rudimentary condition. M. A. 
de Candolle has communicated to me a curious instance of 


4 ‘Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,’’ vol. * 
and 397%. See also ‘Origin of Species,”’ 5th edit. p. 535. 

2 For instance M. Richard (‘Annales des Sciences Natading a valuable paper, 
1852, tom. xviii. p. 18) describes and figures rudimer.(‘‘Annuario della Soc. d, 
“muscle pédieux de la main,’? which he says is sow Which paper I am consid- 
Another muscle, called “‘le tibial postérieur,’”’ ig-¢ discussions on this whole 
hand, but appears from time to time in a more “Generelle Morphologie,” and 

86 Prof. W. Turner, ‘‘Proc. Royal Soc. Edi 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 31 


the long-continued persistence or inheritance of this power, 
as well as of its unusual development. He knows a family 
in which one member, the present head of the family, could, 
when a youth, pitch several heavy books from his head by 
the movement of the scalp alone; and he won wagers by per- 
forming this feat. His father, uncle, grandfather, and his 
three children possess the same power to the same unusual 
degree. This family became divided eight generations ago 
into two branches; so that the head of the above-mentioned 
branch is cousin in the seventh degree to the head of the 
other branch. This distant cousin resides in another part 
of France, and on being asked whether he possessed the 
same faculty, immediately exhibited his power. This case 
offers a good illustration how persistent may be the trans- 
mission of an absolutely useless faculty, probably derived 
from our remote semi-human progenitors; since many 
monkeys have, and frequently use, the power of largely 
moving their scalps up and down.” 

The extrinsic muscles which serve to move the external 
ear, and the intrinsic muscles which move the different parts, 
are in a rudimentary condition in man, and they all belong 
to the system of the panniculus,; they are also variable in 
development, or at least in function. I have seen one man 
who could draw the whole ear forward; other men can draw 
it upward; another who could draw it backward; and, 
from what one of these persons told me, it is probable that 
most of us, by often touching our ears, and thus directing 
our attention toward them, could recover some power of 
movement by repeated trials, The power of erecting and 
directing the shell of the ears to the various points of the 
compass is no doubt of the highest service to many animals, 
-yné"ew thus perceive the direction of danger; but I have 
be very smaim sufficient evidence, of a man who possessed 
complete suppress». which might be of use to him. The 

reduced in size, in 


> Emotions in Man and Animals,’? 1872, p. 144. 
38 Some good criticisms on Annuario della Soe. dei Naturalisti, 2 Modena, 
and Mivart, in ‘Transact. Zoolog. 


32 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


whole external shell may be considered a rudiment, together 
with the various folds and prominences (helix and anti- 
helix, tragus and anti-tragus, etc.) which in the lower ani- 
mals strengthen and support the ear when erect, without 
adding much to its weight. Some authors, however, sup- 
pose that the cartilage of the shell serves to transmit vibra- 
tions to the acoustic nerve; but Mr. Toynbee,” after collect- 
ing all the known evidence on this head, concludes that the 
external shell is of no distinct use. The ears of the chim- 
panzee and orang are curiously like those of man, and the 
proper muscles are likewise but very slightly developed.” 
Iam also assured by the keepers in the Zoological Gardens 
that these animals never move or erect their cars; so that 
they are in an equally rudimentary condition with those of 
man, as far as function is concerned. Why these animals, 
as well as the progenitors of man, should have lost the power 
of erecting their ears we cannot say. It may be, though I ° 
am not satisfied with this view, that, owing to their arboreal 
habits, and great strength, they were but little exposed to 
danger, and so during a lengthened period moved their ears 
but little, and thus gradually lost the power of moving them. 
This would be a parallel case with that of those large and 
heavy birds which, from inhabiting oceanic islands, have 
not been exposed to the attacks of beasts of prey, and 
have consequently lost the power of using their wings for 
flight. The inability to move the ears in man and several 
apes is, however, partly compensated by the freedom with 
which they can move the head in a horizontal plane, so as 
to catch sounds from all directions. It has been asserted 
that the ear of man alone possesses a lobule; but ‘‘a rudi- 
ment of it is found in the gorilla’’;* and, as I hear from 
Prof. Preyer, it is not rarely absent in the negro. 


2% “The Diseases of the Ear,’’ by J. Toynbee, F.R.S., 1860, p. 12. A dis- 
tinguished physiologist, Prof. Preyer, informs me that he had lately been ex- 
perimenting on the function of the shell of the ear, and has come to nearly the 
same conclusion as that given here. 

30 Prof. A. Macalister, ‘‘Annals and Mag. of Nat. History,”’ vol. vii., 
1871, p. 342. 

31 Mr, St. George Mivart, ‘‘Elementary Anatomy,’ 1873, p. 396, 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 33 


The celebrated sculptor, Mr. Woolner, informs me of one 
little peculiarity in the external ear, which he has often ob- 
served both in men and women, and of which he perceived 
the full significance. His attention was first called to the 
subject while at work on his figure of Puck, to which he 
had given pointed ears. He was thus led to examine the 
ears of various monkeys, and subsequently more carefully 
those of man. The peculiarity consists in a little blunt 
point, projecting from the inwardly folded margin, or helix. 
When present, it is developed at birth, and, according to 
Prof. Ludwig Meyer, more frequently in man than in wo- 
man. Mr. Woolner made an exact model of one such case, 
and sent me the accompanying draw- 
ing (Fig. 2). These points not only 
project inward toward the centre of 
the ear, but often a little outward from 
its plane, so as to be visible when the 
head is viewed from directly in front 
or behind. They are variable in size, 
and somewhat in position, standing 
either a little higher or lower; and 
they sometimes occur on one ear and 
not on the other. They are not con- 
fined to mankind, for I observed as deawa ty Me Woonee 

a. The projecting point 

case in one of the spider-monkeys 

(Ateles beelzebuth) in our Zoological Gardens; and Dr. HE. Ray 
Lankester informs me of another case in a chimpanzee in the 
gardens at Hamburg. The helix obviously consists of the 
extreme margin of the ear folded inward; and this folding 
appears to be in some manner connected with the whole ex- 
ternal ear being permanently pressed backward. In many 
monkeys, which do not stand high in the order, as baboons 
and some species of Macacus,” the upper portion of the ear 
is slightly pointed, and the margin is not at all folded in- 


32 See also some remarks, and the drawings of the ears of the Lemuroidea, 
= Messrs, Murie and Mivart’s excellent paper in ‘‘Transact. Abas Soc.,’’ vol 
, 1869, pp. 6 and 90. 


84 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


ward; but if the margin were to be thus folded, a slight 
. point would necessarily project inward toward the centre, 
and probably a little outward from the plane of the ear: and 
this I believe to be their origin in many cases. On the other 
hand, Prof. L. Meyer, in an able paper recently published,** 
maintains that the whole case is one of mere variability; and 
that the projections are not real ones, but are due to the in- 
ternal cartilage on each side of the points not having been 
fully developed. Iam quite ready to admit that this is the 
correct explanation in many instances, as in those figured 
by Prof. Meyer, in which there are several minute points, or 
the whole margin is sinuous. I have myself seen, through 
the kindness of Dr. L. Down, the ear of a microcephalous 
idiot, on which there is a projection on the outside of the 
helix, and not on the inward folded edge, so that this point 
can have no relation to a former apex of the ear. Never- 
theless, in some cases, my original view, that the points are 
vestiges of the tips of formerly erect and pointed ears, still 
seems to me probable. I think so from the frequency of 
their occurrence, and from the general correspondence in 
position with that of the tip of a pointed ear. In one case, 
of which a photograph has been sent me, the projection is 
so large, that supposing, in accordance with Prof. Meyer’s 
view, the ear to be made perfect by the equal development 
of the cartilage throughout the whole extent of the margin, 
it would have covered fully one-third of the whole ear. Two 
cases have been communicated to me—one in North America, 
and the other in England—in which the upper margin is not 
at all folded inward, but is pointed, so that it closely resem- 
bles the pointed ear of an ordinary quadruped in outline. 
In one of these cases, which was that of a young child, the 
father compared the ear with the drawing which I have 
given of the ear of a monkey, the Cynopithecus niger, and 
says that their outlines are closely similar. If, in these two 


33 Ueber das Darwin’sche Spitzohr, Archiv fir Path. Anat. und Phys., 


1871, p. 486. 
% ‘The Expression of the Emotions,” p. 136. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 35 


eases, the margin had been folded inward in the normal 
manner, an inward projection must have been formed. I 
may add that in two other cases the outline still remains 
somewhat pointed, although the margin of the upper part 
of the ear is normally folded inward—in one of them, how- 
ever, very narrowly. The following woodcut (No. 8) is 
an accurate copy of a photograph of the foetus of an orang 
(kindly sent me by Dr. Nitsche), in which it may be seen 
how different the pointed outline of the ear is at this period 
from its adult condition, when it bears a close general re- 


Fie. 3.—Foetus of an Orang. Exact copy of a photograph, showing the form 
of the ear at this early age. 

semblance to that of man. It is evident that the folding 
over of the tip of such an ear, unless it changed greatly 
during its further development, would give rise to a point 
projecting inward. On the whole, it still seems to me prob- 
able that the points in question are in some cases, both in 
man and apes, vestiges of a former condition. 

The nictitating membrane, or third eyelid, with its ac- 
cessory muscles and other structures, is especially well 
developed in birds, and is of much functional importance 
to them, as it can be rapidly drawn across the whole eye- 
ball. It is found in some reptiles and amphibians, and in . 


36 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


certain fishes, as in sharks. It is fairly well developed 
in the two lower divisions of the mammalian series, namely, 
in the monotremata and marsupials, and in some few of the 
higher mammals, as in the walrus. But in man, the quad- 
rumana, and most other mammals, it exists, as is admitted 
by all anatomists, as a mere rudiment, called the semilunar 
fold.** 

The sense of smell is of the highest importance to the 
greater number of mammals—to some, as the ruminants, in 
warning them of danger; to others, as the carnivora, in find- 
. : ~~ . . . 
ing their prey; to others, again, as the wild boar, for both 
purposes combined. But the sense of smell is of extremely 
slight service, if any, even to the dark-colored races of men, 
in whom it is much more highly developed than in the white 
and civilized races.** Nevertheless it does not warn them of 
danger, nor guide them to their food; nor does it prevent 
the Eskimos from sleeping in the most fetid atmosphere, 
nor many savages from eating half-putrid meat. * In Euro- 
peans the power differs greatly in different individuals, as 
Iam assured by an eminent naturalist who possesses this 
sense highly developed, and who has attended to the sub- 
ject. Those who believe in the principle of gradual evolu- 
tion will not readily admit that the sense of smell in its 
present state was originally acquired by man as he now 
exists. He inherits the power, in an enfeebled and so far 


25 Miiller’s ‘‘Hlements of Physiology,’’ Eng. translat., 1842, vol. ii. p. 1117. 
Owen, ‘‘Anatomy of Vertebrates,’’ vol. iii. p. 260; ibid. on the Walrus, ‘‘Proe. 
Zoolog. Soc.,’? November 8, 1854. See also R. Knox, ‘‘Great Artists and 
Anatomists,’’ p. 106. This rudiment apparently is somewhat larger in Negroes 
anid Australians than in Europeans; see Carl Vogt, ‘‘Lectures on Man,’’ Eng. 
translat. p. 129. 

36 The account given by Humboldt of the power of smell possessed by the 
natives of South America is well known, and has been confirmed by others. 
M. Houzeau (‘Etudes sur les Facultés Mentales,”’ ete., tom. i., 1872, p. 91) 
asserts that he repeatedly made experiments, and proved that Negroes and 
Indians could recognize persons in the dark by their odor. Dr. W. Ogle has 
made some curious observations on the connection between the power of smell 
and the coloring matter of the mucous membrane of the olfactory region, as 
well as of the skin of the body. I have, therefore, spoken in the text of the 
dark-colored races having a finer sense of smell than the white races. See his 
paper, ‘‘Medico-Chirurgical Transactions,’’ London, vol. liii., 1870, p. 276. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 37 


rudimentary condition, from some early progenitor, to whom 
it was highly serviceable, and by whom it was continually 
used. In those animals which have this sense highly devel- 
oped, such as dogs and horses, the recollection of persons 
and of places is strongly associated with their odor; and we 
can thus perhaps understand how it is, as Dr. Maudsley has 
truly remarked,* that the sense of smell in man ‘‘is singu- 
larly effective in recalling vividly the ideas and images of 
forgotten scenes and places.”’ 

Man differs conspicuously from all the other Primates 
in being almost naked. But a few short straggling hairs 
are found over the greater part of the body in the man, and 
fine down on that of the woman. The different races differ 
much in hairiness; and in the individuals of the same race 
the hairs are highly variable, not only in abundance, but 
likewise in position; thus in some Europeans the shoulders 
are quite naked, while in others they bear thick tufts of 
hair.** There can be little doubt that the hairs thus scat- 
tered over the body are the rudiments of the uniform hairy 
coat of the lower animals. This view is rendered all the 
more probable, as it is known that fine, short, and pale- 
colored hairs on the limbs and other parts of the body occa- 
sionally become developed into ‘‘thick-set, long, and rather 
coarse, dark hairs,’’? when abnormally nourished near old- 
standing inflamed surfaces. * 

I am informed by Sir James Paget that often several 
members of a family have a few hairs in their eyebrows 
much longer than the others; so that even this slight pecu- 
liarity seems to be inherited. These hairs, too, seem to have 
their representatives; for in the chimpanzee, and in certain 
species of Macacus, there are scattered hairs of consider- 
able length rising from the naked skin above the eyes, and 
corresponding to our eyebrows; similar long hairs project 


31 ‘The Physiology and Pathology of Mind,”’ 2d edit., 1868, p. 134. 

38 Eschricht, Ueber die Richtung der Haare am menschlichen Kérper, 
‘‘Miller’s Archiv fir Anat. und Phys.,’? 1837, s. 47. I shall often have to 
refer to this very curious paper. , 

39 Paget, ‘‘Lectures on Surgical Pathology, 7? 1853, vol. i. p. 71. 


38 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


from the hairy covering of the superciliary ridges in some 
baboons. 

The fine wool-like hair, or so-called lanugo, with which 
the human foetus during the sixth month is thickly covered, 
offers a more curious case. It is first developed, during the 
fifth month, on the eyebrows and face, and especially round 
the mouth, where it is much longer than that on the head. 
A mustache of this kind was observed by Eschricht* on a 
female foetus; but this is not so surprising a circumstance 
as it may at first appear, for the two sexes generally resem- 
ble each other in all external characters during an early 
period of growth. The direction and arrangement of the 
hairs on all parts of the foetal body are the same as in 
the adult, but are subject to much variability. The whole 
surface, including even the forehead and ears, is thus thickly 
clothed; but it is a significant fact that the palms of the 
hands and the soles of the feet are quite naked, like the in- 
ferior surfaces of all four extremities in most of the lower 
animals. As this can hardly be an accidental coincidence, 
the woolly covering of the foetus probably represents the 
first permanent coat of hair in those mammals which are 
born hairy. Three or four cases have been recorded of 
persons born with their whole bodies and faces thickly 
covered with fine, long hairs; and this strange condition 
is strongly inherited, and is correlated with an abnormal 
condition of the teeth." Prof. Alex. Brandt informs me 
that he has compared the hair from the face of a man thus 
characterized, aged thirty-five, with the lanugo of a feetus, 
and finds it quite similar in texture; therefore, as he re- 
marks, the case may be attributed to an arrest of develop- 
ment in the hair, together with its continued growth. Many 
delicate children, as I have been assured by a surgeon to 
a hospital for children, have their backs covered by rather 


40 Eschricht, ibid., s. 40, 4%. 

4 See my “Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,” vol. ii, 
p. 327. Prof. Alex. Brandt has recently sent me an additional case of a father 
and son, born in Russia, with these peculiarities. I have received drawings 
of both from Paris. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 39 


long silky hairs; and such cases probably come under the 
same head. 

It appears as if the posterior molar or wisdom teeth were 
tending to become rudimentary in the more civilized races of 
man. These teeth are rather smaller than the other molars, 
as is likewise the case with the corresponding teeth in the 
chimpanzee and orang; and they. have only two separate 
fangs. They do not cut through the gums till about the 
seventeenth year, and I have been assured that they are 
much more liable to decay, and are earlier lost, than the 
other teeth; but this is denied by some eminent dentists. . 
They are also much more liable to vary, both in struc- 
ture and in the period of their development, than the other 
teeth.*? In the Melanian races, on the other hand, the 
wisdom-teeth are usually furnished with three separate 
fangs, and are generally sound; they also differ from the 
other molars in size, less than in the Caucasian races.“ 
Prof. Schaaffhausen accounts for this difference between 
the races by ‘‘the posterior dental portion of the jaw being 
always shortened’’ in those that are civilized,** and this 
shortening may, I presume, be attributed to civilized men 
habitually feeding on soft, cooked food, and thus using their 
jaws less. Iam informed by Mr. Brace that it is becoming 
quite a common practice in the United States to remove 
some of the molar teeth of children, as the jaw does not 
grow large enough for the perfect development of the nor- 
mal number.** 

With respect to the alimentary canal, I have met with 
an account of only a single rudiment, namely, the vermi- 
form appendage of the cecum. The cecum is a branch or 


4 Dr, Webb, ‘“‘Teeth in Man and the Anthropoid Apes,’’ as quoted by Dr. 
C. Carter Blake in *‘Anthropological Review,”’ July, 1867, p. 299. 

4# Owen, ‘‘Anatomy of Vertebrates,’’ vol. iii. pp. 320, 321, 325, 

4 “On the Primitive Form of the Skull.”’ Eng. translat. in ‘‘Anthropologi- 
cal Review,’’ Oct., 1868, p. 426. 

4 Prof. Mantegazza writes to me from Florence, that he has lately been 
studying the last molar teeth in the different races of man, and has come to the 
same conclusion as that given in my text, viz., that in the higher or civilized 
races they are on the road toward atrophy or elimination. 


40 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


diverticulum of the intestine, ending in.a cul-de-sac, and is 
extremely long in many of the lower vegetable-feeding mam- 
mals. In the marsupial koala itis actually more than thrice 
as long as the whole body.** It is sometimes produced 
into a long, gradually tapering point, and is sometimes con- 
stricted in parts. It appears as if, in consequence of changed 
diet or habits, the cecum had become much shortened in 
various animals, the vermiform appendage being left as a 
rudiment of the shortened part. That this appendage is 
a rudiment, we may infer from its small size, and from the 
evidence which Prof. Canestrini’ has collected of its vari- 
ability in man. It is occasionally quite absent, or again is 
largely developed. The passage is sometimes completely 
closed for half or two-thirds of its length, with the terminal 
part consisting of a flattened solid expansion. In the orang 
this appendage is long and convoluted; in man it arises 
from the end of the short cecum, and is commonly from 
four to five inches in length, being only about the third of 
an inch in diameter. Not only is it useless, but it is some. 
times the cause of death, of which fact I have lately heard 
two instances. This is due to small hard bodies, such as 
seeds, entering the passage and causing inflammation.” 

In some of the lower Quadrumana, in the Lemuride and 
Carnivora, as well as in many marsupials, there is a passage 
near the lower end of the humerus, called the supra-condy- 
loid foramen, through which the great nerve of the forelimb 
and often the great artery pass. Now in the humerus of 
man there is generally a trace of this passage, which is some- 
times fairly well developed, being formed by a depending 
hook-like process of bone, completed by a band of ligament. 
Dr. Struthers,“ who has closely attended to the subject, has 


46 Owen, ‘‘Anatomy of Vertebrates,’’ vol. iii. pp. 416, 434, 441, 

41 *‘Annuario della Soc. d. Nat.,’? Modena, 1867, p. 94. 

48 M. C. Martins (‘‘De l’Unité Organique,’’ in ‘‘Revue des Deux Mondes,”? 
sune 15, 1862, p. 16), and Hackel (‘‘Generelle Morphologie,”’ B. ii. s. 278), have 
both remarked on the singular fact of this rudiment sometimes causing death, 

49 With respect to inheritance, see Dr. Struthers in the ‘‘Lancet,’’? Feb. 15, 
1873, and another important paper, ibid., Jan. 24, 1863, p. 83. Dr. Knox, 
as I am informed, was the first anatomist who drew attention to this peculiar 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 41 


now shown that this peculiarity is sometimes inherited, as it 
has occurred in a father, and in no less than four out of his 
seven children. When present, the great nerve invariably 
passes through it; and this clearly indicates that it is the 
homologue and rudiment of the supra-condyloid foramen 
of the lower animals. Prof. Turner estimates, as he informs 
me, that it occurs in about one per cent of recent skeletons. 
But if the occasional development of this structure in man 
is, as seems probable, due to reversion, it is a return to a 
very ancient state of things, because in the higher Quadru- 
mana it is absent. 

There is another foramen or perforation in the humerus, 
occasionally present in man, which may be called the inter- 
condyloid. This occurs, but not constantly, in various an- 
thropoid and other apes, * and likewise in many of the lower 
animals. It is remarkable that this perforation seems to 
have been present in man much more frequently during 
ancient times than recently. Mr. Busk® has collected the 
following evidence on this head: Prof. Broca ‘‘noticed the 
perforation in four and a half per cent of the arm-bones 
collected in the ‘Cimetiére du Sud,’ at Paris; and in the 
Grotto of Orrony, the contents of which are referred to 
the Bronze period, as mary as eight humeri out of thirty- 
two were perforated; but this extraordinary proportion, he 
thinks, might be due to the cavern having been a sort of 
‘family vault.’ Again, M. Dupont found thirty per cent 
of perforated bones in the caves of the Valley of the Lesse, 
belonging to the Reindeer period; while M. Leguay, in a 
sort of dolmen at Argenteuil, observed twenty-five per cent 
to be perforated; and M. Pruner-Bey found twenty-six per 


structure in man; see his ‘‘Great Artists and Anatomists,’’ p. 63. See also an 
important memoir on this process by Dr. Gruber, in the ‘‘Bulletin de lAcad. 
Imp. de St. Petersburg,”’ tom. xii., 1867, p. 448. 

50 Mr, St. George Mivart, ‘‘Transact. Phil. Soc.,’’ 1867, p. 310. 

51 “On the Caves of Gibraltar,’ ‘“Transact. Internat. Congress of Prehist. 
Arch.’ Third Session, 1869, p. 159. Prof. Wyman has lately shown (Fourth 
Annual Report, Peabody Museum, 1871, p. 20) that this perforation is present 
in thirty-one per cent of some human remains from ancient mounds in the West- 
ern United States, and in Florida It frequently occurs in the negro. 


42 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


cent in the same condition in bones from Vauréal. Nor 
should it be left unnoticed that M. Pruner-Bey states that 
this condition is common in Guanche skeletons.’’ It is an 
interesting fact that ancient races, in this and several other 
cases, more frequently present structures which resemble 
those of the lower animals than do the modern. One chief 
cause seems to be that the ancient races stand somewhat 
nearer in the long line of descent to their remote animal- 
like progenitors. . 

In man, the os coccyx, together with certain other ver- 
tebre hereafter to be described, though functionless as a 
tail, plainly represent this part in other vertebrate animals. 
At an early embryonic period it is free, and projects beyond 
the lower extremities, as may be seen in the drawing (Fig. 1) 
of ahumanembryo. Hven after birth it has been known, in 
certain rare and anomalous cases,* to form a small external 
rudiment ofa tail. The os coccyx is short, usually includ- 
ing only four vertebre, all anchylosed together: and these 
are in a rudimentary condition, for they consist, with the 
exception of the basal one, of the centrum alone.* They 
are furnished with some small muscles, one of which, as I 
am informed by Prof. Turner, has been expressly described 
by Theile as a rudimentary repetition of the extensor of 
the tail, a muscle which is so largely developed in many 
mammals. 

The spinal cord in man extends only as far downward 
as the last dorsal or first lumbar vertebra; but a thread-like 
structure (the filum terminale) runs down the axis of the 
sacral part of the spinal canal, and even along the back of 
the coccygeal bones. The upper part of this filament, as 
Prof. Turner informs me, is undoubtedly homologous with 


5 Quatrefages has lately collected the evidence on this subject. ‘‘Revue 
des Cours Scientifiques,’’ 1867-68, p. 625. In 1840 Fleischmann exhibited a 
human foetus bearing a free tail, which, as is not always the case, included 
vertebral bodies; and this tail was critically examined by the many anatomists 
present at the meeting of naturalists at Erlangen (see Marshall in Niederlind- 
ischen Archiv fiir Zoologie, December, 1871). 

53 Owen ‘‘On the Nature of Limbs,’’ 1849, p. 114. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 43 


the spinal cord, but the lower part apparently consists merely 
of the pia mater, or vascular investing membrane. Even in 
this case the os coccyx may be said to possess a vestige of 
so important a structure as the spinal cord, though no longer 
inclosed within a bony canal. The following fact, for which 
I am also indebted to Prof. Turner, shows how closely the 
oS coccyx corresponds with the true tail in the lower ani- 
mals: Luschka has recently discovered at the extremity of 
the coccygeal bones a very peculiar convoluted body, which 
is continuous with the middle sacral artery; and this discov- 
ery led Krause and Meyer to examine the tail of a monkey 
(Macacus) and of a cat, in both of which they found a simi- 
larly convoluted body, though not at the extremity. 

The reproductive system offers various rudimentary 
structures; but these differ in one important respect from 
the foregoing cases. Here we are not concerned with the 
vestige of a part which does not- belong to the species in 
an efficient state, but with a part efficient in the one sex 
and represented in the other by a mere rudiment. Never- 
theless, the occurrence of such rudiments is as difficult to 
explain, on the belief of the separate creation of each spe- 
cies, as in the foregoing cases. Hereafter I shall have to 
recur to these rudiments, and shall show that their presence 
generally depends merely on inheritance, that is, on parts 
acquired by one sex having been partially transmitted to the 
other. I will in this place only give some instances of such 
rudiments. Itis well known, that in the males of all mam- 
mals, including man, rudimentary mammz exist. These in 
several instances have become well developed, and have 
yielded a copious supply of milk. Their essential identity 
in the two sexes is likewise shown by their occasional sym- 
pathetic enlargement in both during an attack of the measles. 
The vesicula prostatica, which has been observed in many 
male mammals, is now universally acknowledged to be the 
homologue of the female uterus, together with the connected 
passage. It is impossible to read Leuckart’s able descrip- 
tion of this organ, and his reasoning, without admitting the 


44. THE DESCENT OF MAN 


justness of his conclusion. This is especially clear in the 
case of those mammals in which the true female uterus 
bifurcates, for in the males of these the vesicula likewise 
bifurcates.“ Some other rudimentary structures belonging 
to the reproductive system might have been here adduced.” 

The bearing of the three great classes of facts now given 
is unmistakable. But it would be superfluous fully to 
recapitulate the line of argument given in detail in my 
“Origin of Species.’’ {The homological construction of the 
whole frame in the members of the same class is intelligible, 
if we admit their descent from a common progenitor, together 
with their subsequent adaptation to diversified conditions” 
On any other view, the similarity of pattern between the 
hand of a man or monkey, the foot of a horse, the flipper 
of a seal, the wing of a bat, etc., is utterly inexplicable.” 
It is no scientific explanation to assert that they have all 
been formed on the same ideal plan. With respect to de- 
velopment, we can clearly understand, on the principle of 
variations supervening at a rather late embryonic period, 
and being inherited at a corresponding period, how it is 


54 Leuckart, in Todd’s ‘‘Cyclop. of Anat.,’’ 1849-52, vol. iv. p. 1415, In 
man this organ is only from three to six lines in length, but, like so many other 
rudimentary parts, it is variable in development as well as in other characters. 

55 See, on this subject, Owen, ‘‘Anatomy of Vertebrates,”’ vol. iii. pp. 675, 
676, 706. 

56 Prof. Bianconi, in a recently published work, illustrated by admirable 
engravings (‘La Théorie Darwinienne et la Création dite indépendante,’’ 1874), 
endeavors to show that homological structures, in the above and other cases, 
can be fully explained on mechanical principles, in accordance with their uses, 
No one has shown so well how admirably such structures are adapted for their 
final purpose; and this adaptation can, as I believe, be explained through natu- 
ral selection. In considering the wing of a bat, he brings forward (p. 218) what 
appears to me (to use Auguste Comte’s words) a mere metaphysical principle, 
namely, the preservation ‘‘in its integrity of the mammalian nature of the ani- 
mal.’? In only a few cases does he discuss rudiments, and then only those 
parts which are partially rudimentary, such as the little hoofs of the pig and 
ox, which do not touch the ground; these he shows clearly to be of service to 
the animal. Itis unfortunate that he did not consider such cases as the minute 
teeth, which never cut through the jaw in the ox, or the mamme of male quad- 
rupeds, or the wings of certain beetles, existing under the soldered wing-covers, 
or the vestiges of the pistil and stamens in various flowers, and many other 
such cases. Although I greatly admire Prof. Bianconi’s work, yet the belief 
now held by most naturalists seems to me left unshaken, that homological 
structures are inexplicable on the principle of mere adaptation. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 45 


that the embryos of wonderfully different forms should still 
retain, more or less perfectly, the structure of their common 
progenitor. No other explanation has ever been given of 
the marvellous fact that the embryos of a man, dog, seal, 
bat, reptile, etc., can at first hardly be distinguished from 
each other. In order to understand the existence of rudi- 
mentary organs, we have only to suppose that a former 
progenitor possessed the parts in question in a perfect state, 
and that under changed habits of life they became greatly 
reduced, either from simple disuse or through the natural 
selection of those individuals which were least encumbered 
with a superfluous part, aided by the other means previously 
indicated. 

Thus we can understand how it has come to pass that 
man and all other vertebrate animals have been constructed 
on the same general model, why they pass through the same 
early stages of development, and why they retain certain 
rudiments in common. Consequently we ought frankly to 
admit their community of descent; to take any other view, 
is to admit that our own structure, and that of all the ani- 
mals around us, is a mere snare laid to entrap our judg- 
ment. This conclusion is greatly strengthened if we look 
to the members of the whole animal series, and consider the 
evidence derived from their affinities or classification, their 
geographical distribution and geological succession. It is 
only our natural prejudice, and that arrogance which made 
our forefathers declare that they were descended from demi- 
gods, which leads us to demur to this conclusion. But the 
time will before long come, when it will be thought wonder- 
ful that naturalists, who were well acquainted with the com- 
parative structure and development of man and other mam- 
mals, should have believed that each was the work of a 
separate act of creation. 


46 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


CHAPTER II 


ON THE MANNER OF DEVELOPMENT OF MAN FROM 
SOME LOWER FORM 


Variability of body and mind in man—Inheritance—Causes of variability 
—Laws of variation the same in man as in the lower animals—Direct 
action of the conditions of life—Hffects of the increased use and dis- 
use of parts—Arrested development—Reversion —Correlated variation 
—Rate of increase—Checks to increase—Natural selection—Man the 
most dominant animal in the world—Importance of his corporeal struc- 
ture—The causes which have led to his becoming erect—Consequent 
changes of structure—Decreuase in size of the canine teeth—Increased 
size and altered shape of the skull—Nakedness—Absence of a tail 
—Defenceless condition of man 


T IS manifest that man is now subject to much varia- 
bility. No two individuals of the same race are quite 
alike. Wemay compare millions of faces, and each will 

be distinct. There is an equally great amount of diversity in 
the proportions and dimensions of the various parts of the 
body, the length of the legs being one of the most variable 
points.’ Although in some quarters of the world an elon- 
gated skull, and in other quarters a short skull prevails, yet 
there is great diversity of shape even within the limits of 
the same race, as with the aborigines of America and South 
Australia—the latter a race ‘‘probably as pure and homoge- 
neous in blood, customs, and language as any in existence”’ 
—and even with the inhabitants of so confined an area as 
the Sandwich Islands.?- An eminent dentist assures me that 


1 “Investigations in Military and Anthropolog. Statistics of American Sol- 
diers,’’ by B. A. Gould, 1869, p. 256. 

2 With respect to the ‘‘Cranial Forms of the American Aborigines,*’ see Dr, 
Aitken Meigs in ‘‘Proc. Acad. Nat. Sci.,’’ Philadelphia, May, 1868. On the 
Australians, see Huxley, in Lyell’s “‘Antiquity of Man,’’ 1863, p. 8%. On 
the Sandwich Islanders, Prof. J. Wyman, ‘‘Observations on Crania,’’ Boston, 
1868, p. 18. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 47 


there is nearly as much diversity in the teeth as in the fea- 
tures. The chief arteries so frequently run in abnormal 
courses that it has been found useful for surgical purposes 
to calculate from 1,040 corpses how often each course pre- 
vails.? The muscles are eminently variable: thus those of 
the foot were found by Prof. Turner* not to be strictly alike 
in any two out of fifty bodies; and in some the deviations 
were considerable. He adds, that the power of performing 
the appropriate movements must have been modified in 
accordance with the several deviations. Mr. J. Wood has 
recorded® the occurrence of 295 muscular variations in 
thirty-six subjects, and in another set of the same number 
no less than 558 variations, those occurring on both sides 
of the body being only reckoned as one. In the last set, 
not one body out of the thirty-six was ‘‘found totally want- 
ing in departures from the standard descriptions of the mus- 
cular system given in anatomical text-books.’”’ A single 
body presented the extraordinary number of twenty-five 
distinct abnormalities. The same muscle sometimes varies 
in many ways: thus Prof. Macalister describes*® no less than 
twenty distinct variations in the palmeris accessorius. 

The famous old anatomist, Wolff,’ insists that the inter- 
nal viscera are more variable than the external parts: Nulla 
particula est que non aliter et aliter in aliis se habeat homini- 
bus. He has even written a treatise on the choice of typical 
examples of the viscera for representation. A discussion on 
the beau-ideal of the liver, lungs, kidneys, etc., as of the 
human face divine, sounds strange in our ears. 

The variability or diversity of the mental faculties in 
men of the same race, not to mention the greater differences 
between the men of distinct races, is so notorious that not 
a word need here be said. So it is with the lower animals. 


3 “Anatomy of the Arteries,’? by R. Quain. Preface, vol. i., 1844. 

4 “Transact. Royal Soc. Edinburgh,’’ vol. xxiv. pp. 175, 189. 

5 **Proc, Royal Soc.,’? 186%, p. 544; also 1868, pp. 483, 624. There is a 
» previcas paper, 1866, p. 229. 

6 “*Proc. R. Irish Academy,’’ vol. x., 1868, p. 141. 

7 “Act, Acad. St. Petersburg,’’ 1778, part ii. p. 217. 


48 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


All who have had charge of menageries admit this fact, and 
we see it plainly in our dogs and other domestic animals. 
Brehm especially insists that each individual monkey of 
those which he kept tame in Africa had his own peculiar 
disposition and temper: he mentions one baboon remarkable 
for its high intelligence; and the keepers in the Zoological 
Gardens pointed out to me a monkey, belonging to the New 
World division, equally remarkable for intelligence. Reng- 
ger, also, insists on the diversity in the various mental char- 
acters of the monkeys of the same species which he kept in 
Paraguay; and this diversity, as he adds, is partly innate, 
and partly the result of the manner in which they have been 
treated or educated.° 
XI have elsewhere’ so fully discussed the subject of Inheri- 
tance, that I need here add hardly anything. A greater 
number of facts have been collected with respect to the 
transmission of the most trifling, as well as of the most 
important, characters in man than in any of the lower ani- 
mals; though the facts are copious enough with respect to 
the latter. So in regard to mental qualities, their transmis- 
sion is manifest in our dogs, horses, and other domestic ani- 
mals. Besides special tastes and habits, general intelligence, 
courage, bad and good temper, etc., are certainly transmitted. 
With man we see similar facts in almost every family; and 
we now know, through the admirable labors of Mr. Galton,* 
that genius, which implies a wonderfully complex combina- 
tion of high faculties, tends to be inherited; and on the 
other hand, it is too certain that insanity and deteriorated 
mental powers likewise run in families. X 

With respect to the causes of variability, we are in all 
cases very ignorant; but we can see that in man, as in the 
lower animals, they stand in some relation to the conditions 
to which each species has been exposed during several gen- 


8 Brehm, ‘‘Thierleben,’’ B. 1. 3. 58, 87. Rengger, ‘‘Sdugethiere von Para- 
guay,’’ s. 57. 

9 “Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,”’ vol. ii. chap. xii. 

10 ‘‘Hereditary Genius: an Inquiry into its Laws and Consequences,’’ 1869, 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 49 


erations. Domesticated animals vary more than those in a 
state of nature; and this is apparently due to the diversified 
and changing nature of the conditions to which they have 
been subjected. In this respect the different races of man 
resemble domesticated animals, and so do the individuals of 
the same race, when inhabiting a very wide area, like that 
of America. We see the influence of diversified conditions 
in the more civilized nations; for the members belonging to 
different grades of rank, and following different occupations, 
present a greater range of character than do the members of 
barbarous nations. But the uniformity of savages has often 
been exaggerated, and in some cases can hardly be said to 
exist.* It is, nevertheless, an error to speak of man, even 
if we look only to the conditions to which he has been ex- 
posed, as ‘‘far more domesticated’’ * than any other animal. 
Some savage races, such as the Australians, are not exposed 
to more diversified conditions than are many species which 
have a wide range. In another and much more important 
respect, man differs widely from any strictly domesticated 
animal; for his breeding has never long been controlled, 
either by methodical or unconscious selection. No race or 
body of men has been so completely subjugated by other 
men, as that certain individuals should be preserved, and 
thus unconsciously selected, from somehow excelling in 
utility to their masters. Nor have certain male and female 
individuals been intentionally picked out and matched, ex- 
cept in the well-known case of the Prussian grenadiers; and 
in this case man obeyed, as might have been expected, the 
law of methodical selection; for it is asserted that many tall 
men were reared in the villages inhabited by the grenadiers 
and their tall wives. In Sparta, also, a form of selection 
was followed, for it was enacted that all children should be 


11 Mr. Bates remarks a Naturalist on the Amazons,”’ 1863, vol. ii. p. 
159), with respect to the Indians of the same South American tribe, ‘‘no two 
of them were at all similar in the shape of the head; one man had an oval vis- 
age with fine features, and another was quite Mongolian in breadth and promi- 
nence of cheek, spread of nostrils, and obliquity of eyes.” 

12 Blumenbach, ‘‘Treatises on Anthropolog.,”’ Eng, translat., 1865, p. 205. 


Descent—Vot. L—3 


50 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


examined shortly after birth; the well-formed and vigorous 
being preserved, the others left to perish.” 

If we consider all the races of man as forming a single 
species, his range is enormous; but some separate races, 
as the Americans and Polynesians, have very wide ranges. 
It is a well-known law that widely ranging species are much 
more variable than species with restricted ranges; and the 
variability of man may with more truth be compared with 
that of widely ranging species than with that of domesti- 
cated animals. 

Not only does variability appear to be induced in man 
and the lower animals by the same general causes, but in 
both the same parts of the body are affected in a closely 
analogous manner. This has been proved in such full de- 
tail by Godron and Quatrefages that I need here only refer 
to their works.“ ¥Monstrosities, which graduate into slight 
variations, are likewise so similar in man and the lower ani- 
mals, that the same classification and the same terms can 


18 Mitford’s “‘History of Greece,’? vol. i. p. 282. It appears also from a 
passage in Xenophon’s ‘‘Memorabilia,’’ B. ii. 4 (to which my attention has been 
called by the Rev. J. N. Hoare), that it was a well recognized principle with 
the Greeks, that men ought to select their wives with a view to the health and 
vigor of their children. The Grecian poet, Theognis, who lived 550 B.c., clearly 
saw how important selection, if carefully applied, would be for the improvement 
of mankind. He saw, likewise, that wealth often checks the proper action of 
sexual selection. He thus writes: 

‘‘With kine and horses, Kurnus! we proceed 
By reasonable rules, and choose a breed 
For profit and increase, at any price; 
Of a sound stock, without defect or vice. 
But, in the daily matches that we make, 
The price is everything: for money’s sake 
Men marry: women are in marriage given; 
The churl or ruffian, that in wealth has thriven, 
May match his offspring with the proudest race: 
Thus everything is mix’d, noble and base! 
If then in outward manner, form and mind, 
You find us a degraded, motley kind, 
Wonder no more, my friend! the cause is plaix, 
And to lament the consequence is vain.”’ 
—The Works of J. Hookham Frere, vol. ii., 1872, p. 334. 

14 Godron, ‘‘De l’Espéce,’’ 1859, tom. ii. livre 3. Quatrefages, ‘‘Unité 
de l’Espéce Humaine,’’ 1861. Also Lectures on Anthropology, given in the 
“Revue des Cours Scientifiques,’’ 1866-68. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 51 


be used for both, as has been shown by Isidore Geoffroy 
St.-Hilaire."* In my work on the variation of domestic 
animals, I have attempted to arrange in a rude fashion 
the laws of variation under the following heads: The di- 
rect and definite action of changed conditions, as exhibited 
by all or nearly all the individuals of the same species, 
varying in the same manner under the same circumstances. 
The effects of the long-continued use or disuse of parts. The 
cohesion of homologous parts. The variability of multiple 
parts. Compensation of growth; but of this law I have 
found no good instance in the case of man. The effects of 
the mechanical pressure of one part on another; as of the 
pelvis on the cranium of the infant in the womb. Arrests 
of development, leading to the diminution or suppression of 
parts. The reappearance of long-lost characters through 
reversion. And lastly, correlated variation. All these so- 
called laws apply equally to man and the lower animals; 
and most of them even to plants. It would be superfluous 
here to discuss all of them;’* but several are so important 
that they must be treated at considerable length. 


The direct and definite action of changed conditions. —This 
is a most perplexing subject. It cannot be denied that 
changed conditions produce some, and occasionally a con- 
siderable effect, on organisms of all kinds; and it seems at 
first probable that if sufficient time were allowed this would 
be the invariable result. But I have failed to obtain clear 
evidence in favor of this conclusion, and valid reasons may 
be urged on the other side, at least as far as the innumerable 
structures are concerned, which are adapted for special ends. 
There can, however, be no doubt that changed conditions 
induce an almost indefinite amount of fluctuating variability, 


15 ‘Hist. Gén. et Part, des Anomalies de l’Organisation,’’ in three volumes, 
tom. i. 1832. : 

16 T have fully discussed these laws in my ‘‘Variation of Animals and Plants 
under Domestication,’’ vol. ii. chaps. xxii, and xxiii. M. J. P. Durand has 
lately (1868) published a valuable essay, ‘‘De 1’Influence des Milieux,” ete, 
He lays much stress, in the case of plants, on the nature of the soil. 


52 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


by which the whole organization is rendered in some degree 
plastic. 

In the United States, above one million soldiers, who 
served in the late war, were measured, and the States in 
which they were born and reared were recorded.” From 
this astonishing number of observations it is proved that 
local influences of some kind act directly on stature; and 
we further learn that ‘‘the State where the physical growth 
has in great measure taken place, and the State of birth, 
which indicates the ancestry, seem to exert a marked influ- 
ence on the stature.’’ For instance, it is established ‘‘that 
residence in the Western States, during the years of growth, 
tends to produce increase of stature.’’ On the other hand, 
it is certain that with sailors their life delays growth, as 
shown ‘‘by the great difference between the statures of 
soldiers and sailors at the ages of seventeen and eighteen 
years.’’¥ Mr. B. A. Gould endeavored to ascertain the 
nature of the influences which thus act on stature; but he 
arrived only at negative results, namely, that they did not 
relate to climate, the elevation of the land, soil, nor even 
‘nm any controlling degree’’ to the abundance or the need 
of the comforts of life. This latter conclusion is directly 
opposed to that arrived at by Villermé, from the statistics 
of the height of the conscripts in different parts of France. 
When we compare the differences in stature between the 
Polynesian chiefs and the lower orders within the same 
islands, or between the inhabitants of the fertile volcanic 
and low barren coral islands of the same ocean," or again 
between the Fuegians on the eastern and western shores 
of their country, where the means of subsistence are very 
different, it is scarcely possible to avoid the conclusion that 


1 ‘Investigations in Military and Anthrop. Statistics,’ etc., 1869, by B. A, 
Gould, pp. 93, 107, 126, 131, 134. 

18 For the Polynesians, see Prichard’s ‘‘Physical Hist. of Mankind,” vol. 
v., 1847, pp. 145, 283. Also Godron, ‘“De lEspéce,’’ tom. ii. p. 289. There 
is also a remarkable difference in appearance between the closely allied Hindus 
inhabiting the Upper Ganges and Bengal; see Elphinstone’s ‘‘History of India,” 
vol. i, p. 324. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN BB 


better food and greater comfort do intluence stature. But 
the preceding statements show how difficult it is to arrive 
at any precise result. Dr. Beddoe has lately proved that, 
with the inhabitants of Britain, residence in towns and cer- 
tain occupations have a deteriorating influence on height; 
and he infers that the result is to a certain extent inherited, 
as is likewise the case in the United States. Dr. Beddoe 
further believes that wherever a ‘‘race attains its maximum 
of physical development, it rises highest in energy and 
moral vigor.” ’° 

Whether external conditions produce any other direct 
effect on man is not known. It might have been expected 
that differences of climate would have had a marked influ- 
ence, inasmuch as the lungs and kidneys are brought into 
activity under a low temperature, and the liver and skin 
under a high one.” It was formerly thought that the color 
of the skin and the character of the hair were determined 
by light or heat; and although it can hardly be denied that 
some effect is thus produced, almost all observers now agree 
that the effect has been very small, even after exposure dur- 
ing many ages. But this subject will be more properly dis- 
cussed when we treat of the different races of mankind. 
With our domestic animals there are grounds for believ- 
ing that cold and damp directly affect the growth of the 
hair; but I have not met with any evidence on this head 
in the case of man. 


Liffects of the increased Use and Disuse of Parts.—It is 
well known that use strengthens the muscles in the individ- 
ual, and complete disuse, or the destruction of the proper 
nerve, weakens them. When the eye is destroyed, the optic 
nerve often becomes atrophied. When an artery is tied, 
the lateral channels increase not only in diameter, but in the 
thickness and strength of their coats. When one kidney 


19 “Memoirs, Authropolog. Soc.,’’ vol. iii., 1867-69, pp. 561, 565, 567. 
® Dr. Brakenridge, ‘‘Theory of Diathesis,”’ ‘‘Medical Times,”’ June 19, and 
July 17, 1869. 


54 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


ceases to act from disease, the other increases in size, and 
does double work. Bones increase not only in thickness, 
but in length, from carrying a greater weight.” Different 
occupations, habitually followed, lead to changed propor- 
tions in various parts of the body. Thus it was ascertained 
by the United States Commission” that the legs of the sail- 
ors employed in the late war were longer by 0.217 of an inch 
than those of the soldiers, though the sailors were on an 
average shorter men; while their arms were shorter by 1.09 
of an inch, and therefore, out of proportion, shorter in rela- 
tion to their Jesser height. This shortness of the arms is 
apparently due to their greater use, and is an unexpected 
result: but sailors chiefly use their arms in pulling, and not 
in supporting weights. With sailors, the girth of the neck 
and the depth of the instep are greater, while the circum- 
ference of the chest, waist and hips is less, than in soldiers, 

Whether the several foregoing modifications would be- 
come hereditary, if the same habits of life were followed 
during many generations, is not known, but it is probable. 
Rengger® attributes the thin legs and thick arms of the 
Payaguas Indians to successive generations having passed 
nearly their whole lives in canoes, with their lower extremi- 
ties motionless. Other writers have come to a similar con- 
clusion in analogous cases. XAccording to Cranz,™ who lived 
for a long time with the Eskimos, ‘‘the natives believe 
that ingenuity and dexterity in seal-catching (their highest 
art and virtue) is hereditary; there is really something in 
it, for the son of a celebrated seal-cateher will distinguish 
himself, though he lost his father in childhood.’’ But in 
this case it is mental aptitude, quite as much as bodily 
‘structure, which appears to be inherited. It is asserted 
that the hands of English laborers are at birth larger than 


21 T have given authorities for these several statements in my ‘‘Variation 
of Animals under Domestication,” vol. ii. pp. 297-300. Dr. Jaeger, ‘“‘Ueber 
das Langenwachsthum der Knochen,”’ ‘‘Jenaischen Zeitschrift,’ B, v. Heft i, 

22 “Tnvestigations,’’ ete. By B, A. Gould, 1869, p. 288. 

% “Sdugethiere von Paraguay,”’ 1830, s. 4. 

% “History of Greenland,’’ Eng. translat., 1767, vol. i. p. 230. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 55 


those of the gentry.* From the correlation which exists, 
at least in some cases,** between the development of the ex- 
tremities and of the jaws, it is possible that in those classes 
which do not labor much with their hands and feet, the jaws 
would be reduced in size from this cause. That they are 
generally smaller in refined and civilized men than in hard- 
working men or savages, is certain. But with savages, as 
Mr. Herbert Spencer” has remarked, the greater use of the 
jaws in chewing coarse, uncooked food, would act in a direct 
manner on the masticatory muscles, and on the bones to 
which they are attached. In infants, long before birth, the 
skin on the soles of the feet is thicker than on any other 
part of the body;** and it can hardly be doubted that this 
is due to the inherited effects of pressure during a long 
series of generations. 

Tt is familiar to every one that watchmakers and engravers 
are liable to be short-sighted, while men living much out of 
‘doors, and especially savages, are generally long-sighted.™ 
Short-sight and long-sight certainly tend to be inherited.* 
The inferiority of Europeans, in comparison with savages, 
in eyesight and in the other senses, is no doubt the accumu- 
lated and transmitted effect of lessened use during many 
generations; for Rengger® states that he has repeatedly 
observed Europeans, who had been brought up and spent 
their whole lives with the wild Indians, who nevertheless 


35 “‘Intermarriage.’? By Alex. Walker, 1838, p. 377. 

26 “The Variation of Animals under Domestication,” vol. i. p. 173. 

%1 ‘“Principles of Biology,”’ vol. i. p. 455. 

3 Paget, ‘‘Lectures on Surgical Pathology,”’ vol. ii., 1853, p. 209. 

29 It is a singular and unexpected fact that sailors are inferior to lands- 
men in their mean distance of distinct vision. Dr. B. A. Gould (‘‘Sanitary 
Memoirs of the War of the Rebellion,’’ 1869, p. 530) has proved this to be the 
case; and he accounts for it by the ordinary range of vision in sailors being 
“restricted to the length of the vessel and the height of the masts.”’ 

3 “The Variation of Anim&ls under Domestication,”’ vol. i. p. 8. 

31 “Saugethiere von Paraguay,’’ s. 8,10. I have had good opportunities 
for observing the extraordinary power of eyesight in the Fuegians. See also 
Lawrence (‘‘Lectures on Physiology,”’ etc., 1822, p. 404) on this same subject. 
M. Giraud-Teulon has recently collected (‘‘Revue des Cours Scientifiques,”’ 
1870, p. 625) a large and valuable body of evidence proving that the cause 
of short-sight ‘“‘c’est le travail assidu, de pres.” 


56 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


did not equal them in the sharpness of their senses. The 
same naturalist observes that the cavities in the skull for 
the reception of the several sense-organs are larger in the 
American aborigines than in Kuropeans; and this probably 
indicates a corresponding difference in the dimensions of the 
organs themselves. Blumenbach has also remarked on the 
large size of the nasal cavities in the skulls of the American 
aborigines, and connects this fact with their remarkably 
acute power of smell. The Mongolians of the plains of 
Northern Asia, according to Pallas, have wonderfully per- 
fect senses; and Prichard believes that the great breadth 
of their skulls across the zygomas follows from their highly 
developed sense-organs. * 

The Quechua Indians inhabit the lofty plateaus of Peru; 
and Alcide d’Orbigny states® that, from continually breath- 
ing a highly rarefied atmosphere, they have acquired chests 
and lungs of extraordinary dimensions. The cells, also, of 
the lungs are larger and more numerous than in Europeans. 
These observations have been doubted; but Mr. D. Forbes 
carefully measured many Aymaras, an allied race, living at 
the height of between ten thousand and fifteen thousand 
feet; and he informs me™ that they differ conspicuously 
from the men of all other races seen by him in the circum- 
ference and length of their bodies. In his table of measure- 
ments the stature of each man is taken at one thousand, and 
the other measurements are reduced to this standard. It is 
here seen that the extended arms of the Aymaras are shorter 
than those of Europeans, and much shorter than those of 
Negroes. The legs are likewise shorter; and they present 
this remarkable peculiarity, that in every Aymara measured, 
the femur is actually shorter than the tibia. On an average, 
the length of the femur to that of the tibia is as 211 to 252; 


82 Prichard, ‘‘Phys. Hist. of Mankind,”’ on the authority of Blumenbach, , 
vol. i., 1851, p. 311; for the statement by Pallas, vol. iv., 1844, p. 407. 

38 Quoted by Prichard, ‘‘Researches into the Phys. History of Mankind,’ 
vol. v. p. 463. 

34 Mr. Forbes’s valuable paper is now published in the ‘‘Journal of the 
Ethnolog. Soc. of London,’’ new series, vol. ii., 1870, p. 193. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 57 


while in two Europeans, measured at the same time, the 
femora to the tibize were as 244 to 230; and in three Negroes 
as 258 to 241. The humerus is likewise shorter relatively 
to the forearm. This shortening of that part of the limb 
which is nearest to the body appears to be, as suggested to 
me by Mr. Forbes, a case of compensation in relation with 
the greatly increased length of the trunk. The Aymaras 
present some other singular points of structure, for instance, 
the very small projection of the heel. 

These men are so thoroughly acclimatized to their cold 
and lofty abode, that when formerly carried down by the 
Spaniards to the low eastern plains, and when now tempted 
down by high wages to the gold-washings, they suffer a 
frightful rate of mortality. Nevertheless Mr. Forbes found 
a few pure families which had survived during two genera- 
tions, and he observed that they still inherited their charac- 
teristic peculiarities. But it was manifest, even without 
measurement, that these peculiarities had all decreased; and 
on measurement, their bodies were found not to be so much 
elongated as those of the men on the high plateau; while 
their femora had become somewhat lengthened, as had their 
tibie, although in a less degree. The actual measurements 
-mnay be seen by consulting Mr. Forbes’s memoir. From 
these observations, there can, I think, be no doubt that 
residence during many generations at a great elevation 
tends, both directly and indirectly, to induce inherited 
modifications in the proportions of the body.* 

Although man may not have been much modified during 
the latter stages of his existence through the increased or 
decreased use of parts, the facts now given show that his 
liability in this respect has not been lost; and we positively 
know that the same law holds good with the lower animals. 
Consequently we may infer that when at a remote epoch the 
progenitors of man were in a transitional state, and were 


8 Dr. Wilckens (“‘Landwirthschaft. Wochenblatt,’’ No. 10, 1869) has lately 
published an interesting essay showing how domestic animals which live in 
mountainous regions have their frames modified. 


58 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


changing from quadrupeds into bipeds, natural selection 
would probably have been greatly aided by the inherited 
effects of the increased or diminished use of the different 
parts of the body. 


Arrests of Development.—There is a difference between 
arrested development and arrested growth, for parts in the 
former state continue to grow while still retaining their 
early condition. Various monstrosities come under this 
head; and some, as a cleft-palate, are known to be occa- 
sionally inherited. It will suffice for our purpose to refer 
to the arrested brain-development of microcephalous idiots, 
as described in Vogt’s memoir.** Their skulls are smaller, 
and the convolutions of the brain are less complex than in 
normal men. The frontal sinus, or the projection over the 
eyebrows, is largely developed, and the jaws are prognathous 
to an ‘‘effrayant’’ degree; so that these idiots somewhat re- 
semble the lower types of mankind. Their intelligence and 
most of their mental faculties are extremely feeble. KX They 
cannot acquire the power of speech, and are wholly incapa- 
ble of prolonged attention, but are much given to imitation. 
They are strong and remarkably active, continually gambol- 
ling and jumping about, and making grimaces. They often 
ascend stairs on all-fours, and are curiously fond of climbing 
up furniture or trees. We are thus reminded of the delight 
shown by almost all boys in climbing trees; and this again 
reminds us how lambs and kids, originally alpine animals, 
delight to frisk on any hillock, however small. x Idiots also 
resemble the lower animals in some other respects; thus 
several cases are recorded of their carefully smelling every 
mouthful of food before eating it. One idiot is described as 
often using his mouth in aid of his hands while hunting for 
lice. They are often filthy in their habits, and have no sense 
of decency; and several cases have been published of their 
bodies being remarkably hairy.*” 


36 “Mémoire sur les Microcéphales,’’ 1867, pp. 50, 125, 169, 171, 184-198, 
31 Prof. Laycock sums up the character of brute-like idiots by calling them 
theroid; ‘Journal of Mental Science,” July, 1863. Dr, Scott (‘The Deaf and 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 59 


Reversion.—Many of the cases to be here given might 
have been introduced under the last heading. When a 
structure is arrested in its development, but still continues 
growing, until it closely resembles a corresponding structure 
in some lower and adult member of the same group, it may 
in one sense be considered as a case of reversion. The lower 
members in a group give us some idea how the common pro- 
genitor was probably constructed; and it is hardly credible 
that a complex part, arrested at an early phase of embryonic 
development, should go on growing so as ultimately to per- 
form its proper function, unless it had acquired such power 
during some earlier state of existence, when the present 
exceptional or arrested structure was normal. The simple 
brain of a microcephalous idiot, in as far as it- resembles 
that of an ape, may in this sense be said to offer a case of 
reversion.** There are other cases which come more strictly 


Dumb,”’ 2d edit., 1870, p. 10) has often observed the imbecile smelling their 
food. See, on this same subject, and on the hairiness of idiots, Dr. Maudsley, 
“Body and Mind,’’ 1870, pp. 46-51. Pinel has also given a striking case of 
hairiness in an idiot. 

38 In my “Variation of Animals under Domestication’’ (vol. ii. p. 57) I 
attributed the not very rare cases of supernumerary mammee in women to rever- 
sion. I was led to this as a probable conclusion, by the additional mamme 
being generally placed symmetrically on the breast; and more especially from 
one case, in which a single efficient mamma occurred in the inguinal region of 
a woman, the daughter of another woman with supernumerary mamme. But 
I now find (see, for instance, Prof. Preyer, ‘‘Der Kampf um das Dasein,’’ 1859, 
s. 45) that mamme erratice occur in other situations, as on the back, in the 
armpit, and on the thigh; the mamme in this latter instance having given so 
much milk that the child was thus nourished. The probability that the addi- 
tional mammz are due to reversion is thus much weakened; nevertheless it still 
seems to me probable, because two pairs are often found symmetrically on the 
breast; and of this I myself have received information in several cases. It is 
well known that some Lemurs normally have two pairs of mammez on the breast. 
Five cases have been recorded of the presence of more than a pair of mamme 
(of course rudimentary) in the male sex of mankind; see ‘‘Journal of Anat. and 
Physiology,’’ 1872, p. 56, for a case given by Dr. Handyside, in which two 
brothers exhibited this peculiarity; see also a paper by Dr. Bartels in ‘‘Reich- 
ert’s and du Bois-Reymond’s Archiv.,’’ 1872, p. 304, In one of the cases al- 
luded to by Dr. Bartels, a man bore five mamme, one being medial and placed 
above the navel; Meckel von Hemsbach thinks that this latter case is illustrated 
by a medial mamma occurring in certain Cheiroptera. On the whole we may 
well doubt if additional mammz would ever have been developed in both sexes 
of mankind, had not his early progenitors been provided with more than a 
single pair. 

In the above work (vol. ii. p. 12), I also attributed, though with much hesita- 


60 THE D&SCENT OF MAN 


under our present head of reversion. Certain structures, 
regularly occurring in the lower members of the group to 
which man belongs, occasionally make their appearance 
in him, though not found in the normal human embryo; 
or, if normally present in the human embryo, they become 
abnormally developed, although in a manner which is nor- 
mal, in the lower members of the group. These remarks 
will be rendered clearer by the following illustrations. 

In various mammals the uterus graduates from a double 
organ, with two distinct orifices and two passages, as in the 
marsupials, into a single organ, which is in no way double, 
except from having a slight internal fold, as in the higher 
apes and man. The rodents exhibit a perfect series of gra- 
dations between these two extreme states. In all mammals 
the uterus is developed from two simple primitive tubes, the 
inferior portions of which form the cornua; and it is, in the 
words of Dr. Farre, ‘‘by the coalescence of the two cornua 
at their lower extremities that the body of the uterus is 
formed in man; while in those animals in which no middle 
portion of body exists the cornua remain un-united. As the 


tion, the frequent cases of polydactylism in men and various animals to rever- 
sion. I was partly led to this through Prof. Owen’s statement, that some of 
the Ichthyopterygia possess more than five digits, and therefore, as I supposed, 
had retained a primordial condition; but Prof. Gegenbaur (‘‘Jenaischen Zeit- 
schrift,’’ B. v. Heft 3, s. 341) disputes Owen’s conclusion. On the other hand, 
according to the opinion lately advanced by Dr. Giinther, on the paddle of 
Ceratodus, which is provided with articulated bony rays on both sides of a cen- 
tral chain of bones, there seems no great difficulty in admitting that six or more 
digits on one side, or on both sides, might reappear through reversion. I am 
informed by Dr. Zouteveen that there is a case on record of a man having 
twenty-four fingers and twenty-four toes! I was chiefly led to the conclusion 
that the presence of supernumerary digits might be due to reversion from the 
fact that such digits not only are strongly inherited, but, as I then believed, 
had the power of regrowth after amputation, like the normal digits of the lower 
vertebrata. But I have explained in the Second Edition of my ‘‘Variation 
under Domestication’? why I now place little reliance on the recorded cases of 
such regrowth. Nevertheless it deserves notice, inasmuch as arrested develop- 
ment and reversion are intimately related processes; that various structures in 
an embryonic or arrested condition, such as a cleft palate, bifid uterus, ete., are 
frequently accompanied by polydactylism. This has been strongly insisted on 
by Meckel and Isidore Geoffroy St.-Hilaire. But at present it is the safest 
course to give up altogether the idea that there is any relation between the 
development of supernumerary digits and reversion to some lowly organized 
progenitor of man. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 61 


development of the uterus proceeds, the two cornua become 
gradually shorter, until at length they are lost, or, as it were, 
absorbed into the body of the uterus.’’ The angles of the 
uterus are still produced into cornua, even in animals ag 
high up in the scale as the lower apes and lemurs. 

Now in women, anomalous cases are not very infrequent, 
in which the mature uterus is furnished with cornua, or is 
partially divided into two organs; and such cases, according 
to Owen, repeat ‘‘the grade of concentrative development,” 
attained by certain rodents. Here perhaps we have an in- 
stance of a simple arrest of embryonic development, with 
subsequent growth and perfect functional development; for 
either side of the partially double uterus is capable of per- 
forming’ the proper office of gestation. In other and rarer 
cases, two distinct uterine cavities are formed, each having 
its proper orifice and passage.” No such stage is passed 
through during the ordinary development of the embryo, 
and it is difficult to believe, though perhaps not impossible, 
that the two simple, minute, primitive tubes should know 
how (if such an expression may be used) to grow into two 
distinct uteri, each with a well-constructed orifice and pas- 
sage, and each furnished with numerous muscles, nerves, 
glands, and vessels, if they had not formerly passed through 
a similar course of development, as in the case of existing 
marsupials. No one will pretend that so perfect a structure 
as the abnormal double uterus in woman could be the result 
of mere chance. But the principle of reversion, by which a 
long-lost structure is called back into existence, might serve 
as the guide for its full development, even after the. lapse 
of an enormous interval of time. 

Prof. Canestrini, after discussing the foregoing and vari- 
ous analogous cases, arrives at the same conclusion as that 
just given. He adduces another instance, in the case of the 
malar bone,*® which, in some of the Quadrumana and other 


39 See Dr, A. Farre’s well-known article in the ‘‘Cyclopxdia of Anatomy 
and Physiology,” vol. v., 1859, p. 642. Owen, ‘‘Anatomy of Vertebrates, ” vol, 
jii., 1868, p. 68%. Prof. Turner in “‘Mdinburgh Med. Journal,”’ February, 1865. 

40 *Annuario della Soc. dei Naturalisti in Modena,”’ 1867, p. 83. Prof, 


62 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


mammals, normally consists of two portions. This is its 
condition in the human foetus when two months old; and, 
through arrested development, it sometimes remains thus in 
man when adult, more especially in the lower prognathous 
races. Hence Canestrini concludes that some ancient pro- 
genitor of man must have had this bone normally divided 
into two portions, which afterward became fused together. 
In man the frontal bone consists of a single piece, but in the 
embryo, and in children, and in almost all the lower mam- 
mals, it consists of two pieces separated by a distinct suture. 
This suture occasionally persists more or less distinctly in 
man after maturity; and more frequently in ancient than 
in recent crania, especially, as Canestrini has observed, in 
~ those exhumed from the Drift, and belonging to the brachy- 
cephalic type. Here again he comes to the same conclusion 
as in the analogous case of the malar bones. In this, and 
other instances presently to be given, the cause of ancient 
races approaching the lower animals in certain characters 
more frequently than do the modern races, appears to be 
that the latter stand at a somewhat greater distance in the 
long line of descent from their early semi-human progenitors. 
Various other anomalies in man, more or less analogous 

to the foregoing, have been advanced by different authors, 
as cases of reversion; but these seem not a little doubtful, 
for we have to descend extremely low in the mammalian 
series before we find such structures normally present.” 


Canestrini gives extracts on this subject from various authorities. Laurillard 
remarks that as he has found a complete similarity in the form, proportions 
and connection of the two malar bones in several human subjects and in certain 
apes, he cannot consider this disposition of the parts as simply aceidental. An- 
other paper on this same anomaly has been published by Dr. Saviotti in the 
“Gazzetta delle Cliniche,’’ Turin, 1871, where he says that traces of the division 
may be detected in about two per cent of adult skulls; he also remarks that it 
more frequently occurs in prognathous skulls, not of the Aryan race, than in 
others. See also G. Delorenzi on the same subject, ‘‘Tre nuovi casi d’anomalia 
dell’ osso, malare,’’ Torino, 1872, Also, E. Morselli, ‘‘Sopra una rara anomalia 
dell’ osso malare,’’ Modena, 1872. Still more recently Gruber has written a 
pamphlet on the division of this bone. I give these references because a re- 
viewer, without «ny grounds or scruples, has thrown doubts on my statements, 

41 A whole series of cases is given by Isid. Geoffroy St.-Hilaire, ‘“Hist. des 
Anomalies,”’ tom. iii. p. 437, A reviewer (‘Journal of Anat, and Physiology,” 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 63 


In man, the canine teeth are perfectly efficient instru- 
fhents for mastication. But their true canine character, as 
Owen" remarks, ‘‘is indicated by the conical form of the 
crown, which terminates in an obtuse point, is convex out- 
ward and flat or sub-concave within, at the base of which 
surface there is a feeble prominence. The conical form is 
best expressed in the Melanian races, especially the Aus- 
tralian. The canine is more deeply implanted, and by a 
stronger fang than the incisors.’’ Nevertheless, this tooth 
no longer serves man as a special weapon for tearing his 
enemies or prey; it may, therefore, as far as its proper func- 
tion is concerned, be considered as rudimentary. In every 
large collection of human skulls some may be found, as 
Hickel** observes, with the canine teeth projecting con- 
siderably beyond the others in the same manner as in the 
anthropomorphous apes, but in a less degree. In these 
cases, open spaces between the teeth in the one jaw are 
left for the reception of the canines of the opposite jaw. 
An interspace of this kind in a Kaffir skull, figured by 
Wagner, is surprisingly wide.** Considering how few are 
the ancient skulls which have been examined, compared to 
recent skulls, it is an interesting fact that in at least three 
cases the canines project largely; and in the Naulette jaw 
they are spoken of as enormous.‘ 

Of the anthropomorphous apes the males alone have their 


1871, p. 366) blames me much for not having discussed the numerous cases, 
which have been recorded, of various parts arrested in their development. He 
says that, according to my theory, ‘‘every transient condition of an. organ, dur- 
ing its development, is not only a means to an end, but once was an end in it- 
self.’? This does not seem to me necessarily to hold good. Why should not 
variations occur during an early period of development, having no relation to 
reversion; yet such variations might be preserved and accumulated, if in any 
way serviceable, for instance, in shortening and simplifying the course of 
development? And again, why should not injurious abnormalities, such as 
atrophied or hypertrophied parts, which have no relation to a former state 
of existence, occur at an early period, as well as during maturity? 

42 “Anatomy of Vertebrates,’’ vol. ili., 1868, p. 323. 

43 “‘Generelle Morphologie,’’ 1866, B. ii. s. elv. 

4 Carl Vogt’s ‘Lectures on Man,?’ Eng. translat., 1864, p. 151. ; 

45 CG, Carter Blake, on a jaw from La Naulette, ‘‘Anthropolog. Review,” 
1867, p. 295. Schaaffhausen, ibid., 1868, p. 426. 


o4 THE DESUVUENT OF MAN 


canines fully developed; but in the female gorilla, and ina 
less degree in the female orang, these teeth project consid- 
erably beyond the others; therefore the fact, of which I have 
been assured, that women sometimes have considerably pro- 
jecting canines, is no serious objection to the belief that their 
occasional great development in man is a case of reversion 
to an apelike prcegenitor. XHe who rejects with scorn the 
belief that the shape of his own canines, and their occasional 
great development in other men, are due to our early fore- 
fathers having been provided with these formidable weapons, 
will probably reveal, by sneering, the line of his descent. 
For though he no longer intends, nor has the power, to use 
these teeth as weapons, he will unconsciously retract his 
‘snarling muscles’’ (thus named by Sir C. Bell),*° so as to 
expose them ready for action, like a dog prepared to fight.~ 
Many muscles are occasionally developed in man, which 
are proper to the Quadrumana or other mammals. Prof. 
Vlacovich*’ examined forty male subjects, and found a mus- 
cle, called by him the ischio-pubic, in nineteen of them; in 
three others there was a ligament which represented this 
muscle; and in the remaining eighteen no trace of it. In 
only two out of thirty female subjects was this muscle de- 
veloped on both sides, but in three others the rudimentary 
ligament was present. This muscle, therefore, appears to 
be much more common in the male than in the female sex; 
and on the belief in the descent of man from some lower 
form, the fact is intelligfble; for it has been detected in 
several of the lower animals, and in all of these it serves 
exclusively to aid the male in the act of reproduction. 
Mr. J. Wood, in his valuable series of papers,‘*® has 


4 *‘The Anatomy of Expression,’’ 1844, pp. 110, 131. 

41 Quoted by Prof. Canestrini in the ‘‘Annuario,”’ etc., 1867, p. 90. 

48 These papers deserve careful study by any one who desires to learn how 
frequently our muscles vary, and in varying come to resemble those of the 
Quadrumana. The following references relate to the few points touched on 
jn my text: ‘Proc. Royal Soc., vol. xiv., 1865, pp. 379-384; vol. xv., 1866, 
pp. 241, 242; vol. xv., 186%, p. 544; vol. xvi., 1868, p. 524. I may here add 
that Dr. Murie and Mr. St. George Mivart have shown in their Memoir on the 
Lemuroidea (‘‘Transact. Zoolog. Soc.,”’ vol. vii., 1869, p. 96) how extraordinarily 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 65 


minutely described a vast number of muscular variations 
in man, which resemble normal structures in the lower 
animals. The muscles which closely resemble those regu- 
larly present in our nearest allies, the Quadrumana, are’ too 
numerous to be here even specified. In a single male sub- 
ject, having a strong bodily frame, and well-formed skull, 
no less than seven muscular variations were observed, all of 
which plainly represented muscles proper to various kinds 
of apes. This man, for instance, had on both sides of his 
neck a true and powerful ‘‘levator clavicule,’’ such as is 
found in all kinds of apes, and which is said to occur in 
about one out of sixty human subjects.“ Again, this man 
had ‘‘a special abductor of the metatarsal bone of the fifth 
digit, such as Prof. Huxley and Mr. Flower have shown 
’ to exist uniformly in the higher and lower apes.’’ I will 
give only two additional cases; the acromio-basilar muscle is 
found in all mammals below man, and seems to be correlated 
with a quadrupedal gait, and it occurs in about one out of 
sixty human subjects. In the lower extremities Mr. Brad- 
ley®* found an abductor ossis metatarsi quinti in both feet of 
man; this muscle had not up to that time been recorded 
in mankind, but is always present in the anthropomorphous 
apes. The muscles of the hands and arms—parts which are 
so eminently characteristic of man—are extremely liable to 
vary, so as to resemble the corresponding muscles in the 
lower animals. Such resemblances are either perfect or 
imperfect; yet in the latter case they are manifestly of a 
transitional nature. Certain variations are more common 
in man, and others in woman, without our being able to 


variable some of the muscles are in these animals, the lowest members of the 
Primates. Gradations, also, in the muscles leading to structures found in ani- 
mals still lower in the scale, are numerous in the Lemuroidea, 

49 See also Prof. Macalister in ‘‘Proc. R. Irish Academy,’’ vol. x., 1868, 

, 124, 

50 Mr. Champneys in ‘‘Journal of Anat. and Phys.,’’ Nov., 1871, p. 178. 

51 ‘Journal of Anat. and Phys.,’’ May, 1872, p. 421. ; 

8 Prof, Macalister (ibid., p. 121) has tabulated his observations, and finds 
that muscular abnormalities are most frequent in the forearms, secondly, in the 
face, thirdly, in the foot, ete. 


66 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


assign any reason. Mr. Wood, after describing numerous 
variations, makes the following pregnant remark: ‘‘ Notable 
departures from the ordinary type of the muscular structures 
run in grooves or directions, which must be taken to indicate 
some unknown factor, of much importance to a comprehen- 
sive knowledge of general and scientific anatomy.” © 

That this unknown factor is reversion to a former state 
of existence may be admitted as in the highest degree prob- 
able. It is quite incredible that a man should through 
mere accident abnormally resemble certain apes in no less 
than seven of his muscles, if there had been no genetic con- 
nection between them. On the other hand, if man is de- 
scended from some apelike creature, no valid reason can 
be assigned why certain muscles should not suddenly reap- 
pear after an interval of many thousand generations; in the 
same manner as, with horses, asses and mules, dark-colored 
stripes suddenly reappear on the legs and shoulders, after 
an interval of hundreds, or more probably of thousands, 
of generations. 

These various cases of reversion are so closely related to 
those of rudimentary organs given in the first chapter, that 


53 The Rev. Dr. Haughton, after giving (‘‘Proc. R. Irish Academy,’’ June 
27, 1864, p. 715) a remarkable case of variation in the human flexor pollicis 
longus, adds: ‘“‘This remarkable example shows that man may sometimes 
possess the arrangement of tendons of thumb and fingers characteristic of the 
macaque; but whether such a case should be regarded as a macaque passing 
upward into a man, or a man passing downward into a macaque, or as a con- 
genital freak of nature, I cannot undertake to say.’’ It is satisfactory to hear 
so capable an anatomist, and so imbittered an opponent of evolutionism, admit- 
ting even the possibility of either of his first propositions. Prof. Macalister has 
also described (‘‘Proc. R. Irish Acad.,”’ vol. x., 1864, p. 138) variations in the 
flexor pollicis longus, remarkable from their relations to the same muscle in 
the Quadrumana. 

54 Since the first edition of this book appeared, Mr. Wood has published 
another memoir in the ‘‘Phil. Transactions,’’ 1870, p. 83, on the varieties of 
the muscles of the human neck, shoulder and chest. He here shows how ex- 
tremely variable these muscles are, and how often and how closely the varia- 
tions resemble the normal muscles of the lower animals, He sums up by re- 
murking: ‘‘It will be enough for my purpose if I have succeeded in showing 
the more important forms which, when occurring as varieties in the human sub- 
ject, tend to exhibit in a sufficiently marked manner what may be considered as 
proofs and examples of the Darwinian principle of reversion, or law of atishie 
tance, in this department of anatomical science.’ 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 67 


many of them might have been indifferently introduced 
either there or here. Thus a human uterus furnished with 
cornua may be said to represent, in a rudimentary condi- 
tion, the same organ in its normal state in certain mammals. 
Some parts which are rudimentary in man, as the os coceyx 
in both sexes, and the mamme in the male sex, are always 
present; while others, such as the supra-condyloid foramen, 
only occasionally appear, and therefore might have been in- 
troduced under the head of reversion. These several rever- 
sionary structures, as well as the strictly rudimentary ones, 
reveal the descent of man from some lower form in an un- 
mistakable manner. ° 


Correlated Variation.—In man, as in the lower animals, 
many structures are so intimately related, that when one part 
varies so does another, without our being able, in most cases, 
to assign any reason. We cannot say whether the one part 
governs the other, or whether both are governed by some 
earlier developed part. Various monstrosities, as I. Geof- 
froy repeatedly insists, are thus intimately connected. 
Homologous structures are particularly liable to change 
together, as we see on the opposite sides of the body, and 
in the upper and lower extremities. Meckel long ago re- 
marked, that when the muscles of the arm depart from their 
proper type, they almost always imitate those of the leg; and 
so, conversely, with the muscles of the leg. The organs of 
sight and hearing, the teeth and hair; the color of the skin 
and of the hair, color and constitution, are more or less cor- 
related.** Prof. Schaaffhausen first drew attention to the 
relation apparently existing between a muscular frame and 
the strongly pronounced supra-orbital ridges, which are so 
characteristic of the lower races of man. 

Besides the variations which can be grouped with more 
or less probability under the foregoing heads, there is a large 
class of variations which may be provisionally called spon- 


55 The authorities for these several statements are given in my ‘‘Variation 
of Animals under Domestication,’’ vol. ii. pp. 320-335, 


68 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


taneous, for to our ignorance they appear to arise without 
any exciting cause. It can, however, be shown that such 
variations, whether consisting of slight individual differ- 
ences, or of strongly marked and abrupt deviations of 
structure, depend much more on the constitution of the 
organism than on the nature of the conditions to which 
it has been subjected. 


Rate of Increase.—Civilized populations have been known 
under favorable conditions, as in the United States, to double 
their numbers in twenty-five years; and, according to a cal- 
culation by Euler, this might occur in a little over twelve 
years.°’ At the former rate the present population of the 
United States (thirty millions) would in 657 years cover 
the whole terraqueous globe so thickly that four men would 
have to stand on each square yard of surface. The primary 
or fundamental check to the continued increase of man is the 
difficulty of gaining subsistence, and of living in comfort. 
We may infer that this is the case from what we see, for 
instance, in the United States, where subsistence is easy, 
and there is plenty of room. If such means were suddenly 
doubled in Great Britain, our number would be quickly 
doubled. With civilized nations this primary check acts 
chiefly by restraining marriages. The greater death-rate 
of infants in the poorest classes is also very important; as 
well as the greater mortality, from various diseases, of the 
inhabitants of crowded and miserable houses, at all ages. 
The effects of severe epidemics and wars are soon counter- 
balanced, and more than counterbalanced, in nations placed 
under favorable conditions. Emigration also comes in aid 
as a temporary check, but, with the extremely poor classes, 
not to any great extent. 

There is reason to suspect, as Malthus has remarked, that 


56 This whole subject has been discussed in chap. xxiii. vol. ii. of my 
*‘Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication.’’ 

51 See the ever-memorable ‘‘Essay on the Principle of Population,’’ by the 
Rey, T, Malthus, vol, i,, 1826, pp, 6, 517. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 69 


the reproductive power is actually less in barbarous than in 
civilized races. We know nothing positively on this head, 
for with savages no census has been taken; but from the 
concurrent testimony of missionaries, and of others who 
have long resided with such people, it appears that their 
families are usually small, and large ones rare. This may 
be partly accounted for, as it is believed, by the women 
suckling their infants during a long time; but it is highly 
probable that savages, who often suffer much hardship, and 
who do not obtain so much nutritious food as civilized men, 
would be actually less prolific. I have shown in a former 
work,®* that all our domesticated quadrupeds and birds, and 
all our cultivated plants, are more fertile than the corre- 
sponding species in a state of nature. It is no valid objec- 
tion to this conclusion that animals suddenly supplied with 
an excess of food, or when grown very fat; and that most 
plants on sudden removal from very poor to very rich soil, 
are rendered more or less sterile. We might, therefore, ex- 
pect that civilized men, who in one sense are highly domes- 
ticated, would be more prolific than wild men. It is also 
probable that the increased fertility of civilized nations would 
become, as with our domestic animals, an inherited charac- 
ter: it is’ at least known that with mankind a tendency 
to produce twins runs in families.” 

Notwithstanding that savages appear to be less prolific 
than civilized people, they would no doubt rapidly increase 
if their numbers were not by some means rigidly kept down. 
The Santali, or hill-tribes of India, have recently afforded a 
good illustration of this fact; for, as shown by Mr. Hunter,” 
they have increased at an extraordinary rate since vaccina- 
tion has been introduced, other pestilences mitigated, and 
war sternly repressed. This increase, however, would not 
have been possible had not these rude people spread into 


58 ‘Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,”’ vol. ii. pp. 111- 
113, 163, ‘i 
89 Mr. Sedgwick, ‘British and Foreign Medico-Chirurg. Review,’’ July, 


1863, p. 170. 
60 “The Annals of Rural Bengal,” by W. W. Hunter, 1868, p. 259, 


70 : THE. DESCENT OF MAN 


the adjoining districts, and worked for hire. Savages almost 
always marry; yet there is some prudential restraint, for 
they ‘do not commonly marry at the earliest possible age. 
The young men are often required to show that they can 
support a wife; and they generally have first to earn the 
price with which to purchase her from her parents. With 
savages the difficulty of obtaining subsistence occasionally 
limits their number in a much more direct manner than with 
civilized people, for all tribes periodically suffer from severe 
famines. At such times savages are forced to devour much 
bad food, and their health can hardly fail to be injured. 
Many accounts have been published of their protruding 
stomachs and emaciated limbs after and during famines. 
They are then, also, compelled to wander much, and, as 
I was assured in Australia, their infants perish in large 
numbers. As famines are periodical, depending chiefly on 
extreme seasons, all tribes must fluctuate in number. They 
cannot steadily and regularly increase, as there is no arti- 
ficial increase in the supply of food. Savages, when hard 
pressed, encroach on each other’s territories, and war is the 
result; but they are indeed almost always at war with their 
neighbors. They are liable to many accidents on land and 
water in their search for food; and in some countries they 
suffer much from the larger beasts of prey. Even in India, 
districts have been depopulated by the ravages of tigers. 
Malthus has discussed these several checks, but he does 
not lay stress enough on what is probably the most impor- 
tant of all, namely, infanticide, especially of female infants, 
and the habit of procuring abortion. These practices now, 
prevail in many quarters of the world; and infanticide 
seems formerly to have prevailed, as Mr. M‘Lennan™ has 
shown, on a still more extensive scale. These practices 
appear to have originated in savages recognizing the diffi- 
culty, or rather the impossibility, of supporting all the in- 
fants that are born. Licentiousness may also be added to 
the foregoing checks; but this does not follow from failing 


61 ‘‘Primitive Marriage,’’ 1865. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 71 


means of subsistence; though there is reason to believe that 
in some cases (as in Japan) it has been intentionally encour- 
aged as a means of keeping down the population. 

If we look back to an extremely remote epoch, before 
man had arrived at the dignity of manhood, he would have 
been guided more by instinct and less by reason than are 
the lowest savages at the present time. Our early semi- 
human progenitors would not have practiced infanticide or 
polyandry; for the instincts of the lower animals are never 
so perverted” as to lead them regularly to destroy their own 
offspring, or to be quite devoid of jealousy. There would 
have been no prudential restraint from marriage, and the 
sexes would have freely united at an early age. Hence 
the progenitors of man would have tended to increase rap- 
idly; but checks of some kind, either periodical or constant, 
must have kept down their numbers, even more severely 
than with existing savages. What the precise nature of 
these checks were we cannot say, any more than with most 
other animals. We know that horses and cattle, which are 
not extremely prolific animals, when first turned loose in 
South America, increased at an enormous rate. The ele- 
phant, the slowest breeder of all known animals, would in 
a few thousand years stock the whole world. The increase 
of every species of monkey must be checked by some means; 
but not, as Brehm remarks, by the attacks of beasts of prey. 
No one will assume that the actual power of reproduction 
in the wild horses and cattle of America was at first in any 
sensible degree increased; or that, as each district became 
fully stocked, this same power was diminished. No doubt 


6 A writer in the “‘Spectator’’ (March 12, 1871, p. 320) comments as fol- 
lows on this passage: ‘‘Mr. Darwin finds himself compelled to reintroduce a 
new doctrine of the fall of man. He shows that the instincts of the higher 
animals are far nobler than the habits of savage races of men, and he finds him- 
self, therefore, compelled to reintroduce—in a form of the substantial orthodoxy 
of which he appears to be quite unconscious—and to introduce as a scientific 
hypothesis the doctrine that man’s gain of knowledge was the cause of a tem- 
porary but long-enduring moral deterioration, as indicated by the many foul 
customs, especially as to marriage, of savage tribes. What does the Jewish 
tradition of the moral degeneration of man through his snatching at a knowl- 
edge forbidden him by his highest instinct assert beyond this?”’ 


72 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


in this case, and in all others, many checks concur, and 
different checks under different circumstances; periodical 
dearths, depending on unfavorable seasons, being probably 
the most important of all. So it will have been with the 
early progenitors of man. 


Natural Selection.—We have now seen that man is vari- 
able in body and mind; and that the variations are induced, 
either directly or indirectly, by the same general causes, and 
obey the same general laws, as with the loweranimals. Man 
has spread widely over the face of the earth, and must have 
been exposed, during his incessant migrations,” to the most 
diversified conditions. The inhabitants of Tierra del Fuego, 
the Cape of Good Hope, and Tasmania in the one hemi- 
sphere, and of the Arctic regions in the other, must have 
passed through many climates, and changed their habits 
many times, before they reached their present homes.“ 
The early progenitors of man must also have tended, like 
all other animals, to have increased beyond their means of 
subsistence; they must, therefore, occasionally have been 
exposed to a struggle for existence, and consequently to 
the rigid law of natural selection. Beneficial variations of 
all kinds will thus, either occasionally or habitually, have 
been preserved, and injurious ones eliminated. I do not 
refer to strongly marked deviations of structure, which 
occur only at long intervals of time, but to mere individ- 
ual differences. We know, for instance, that the muscles 
of our hands and feet, which determine our powers of move- 
ment, are liable, like those of the lower animals,® to inces- 
sant variability. If then the progenitors of man inhabiting 
any district, especially one undergoing some change in its 
conditions, were divided into two equal bodies, the one-half 


63 See some good remarks to this effect by W. Stanley Jevons, ‘‘A Deduc- 
tion from Darwin’s Theory,’’ ‘‘Nature,’’ 1869, p. 231. 

64 Latham, ‘‘Man and his Migrations,’’ 1851, p. 135, 

65 Messrs. Murie and Mivart in their ‘‘Anatomy of the Lemuroidea’’ (‘‘Trans- 
act. Zoolog. Soc.,’’ vol. vii. 1869, pp. 96-98) say: ‘‘Some muscles are so irregular 
in their distribution that they cannot be well classed in any of the above groups.’? 
These muscles differ even on the opposite sides of the same individual. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 73 


which included all the individuals best adapted by their 
powers of movement for gaining subsistence, or for defend- 
ing themselves, would on an average survive in greater 
numbers, and procreate more offspring than the other and 
less well-endowed half. 

Man in the rudest state in which he now exists is the 
most dominant animal that has ever appeared on this earth. 
He has spread more widely than any other highly organized 
form, and all others have yielded before him. He mani- 
festly owes this immense superiority to his intellectual facul- 
ties, to his social habits, which lead him to aid and defend 
his fellows, and to his corporeal structure. The supreme 
importance of these characters has been proved by the final 
arbitrament of the battle for life. Through his powers of 
intellect, articulate language has been evolved; and on this 
his wonderful advancement has mainly depended. As Mr. 
Chauncey Wright remarks,” ‘‘a psychological analysis of 
the faculty of language shows that even the smallest pro- 
ficiency in it might require more brain DNS than the great- 
est proficiency in any other direction.’’ He has invented 
and is able to use various weapons, tools, traps, etc., with 
which he defends himself, kills or catches prey, and other- 
wise obtains food. He has made rafts or canoes for fishing 
or crossing over to neighboring fertile islands. He has dis- 
covered the art of making fire, by which hard and stringy 
roots can be rendered digestible, and poisonous roots or 

“herbs innocuous. This discovery of fire, probably the great- 
est ever made by man, excepting language, dates from be- 
fore the dawn of history. These several inventions, by 
which man in the rudest state has become so pre-eminent, 
are the direct results of the development of his powers of 
observation, memory, curiosity, imagination, and reason. I 
cannot, therefore, understand how it is that Mr. Wallace” 


66 Limits of Natural Selection, ‘‘North American Review,’’ October, 1870, 
p. 295, 
“Quarterly Review,” April, 1869, p. 392. This subject is more fully dis- 
cussed in Mr. Wallace’s ‘<Contributions to the Theory of Natural Selection,’ 
1870, in which all the essays referred to in this work are republished. The 


Descent—Vo., I.—4 


TE ; THE DESCENT OF MAN 


maintains that ‘‘natural selection could only have endowed 
the savage with a brain a little superior to that of an ape.”’ 

Although the intellectual powers and social habits of man 
are of paramount importance to him, we must not underrate 
the importance of his bodily structure, to which subject the 
remainder of this chapter will be devoted; the development 
of the intellectual and social or moral faculties being dis- 
cussed in a later chapter. 

Even to hammer with precision is no easy matter, as 
every one who has tried to learn carpentry will admit. 
To throw a stone with as true an aim as a Fuegian in de- 
fending himself, or in killing birds, requires the most con- 
summate perfection in the correlated action of the muscles 
of the hand, arm, and shoulder, and, further, a fine sense of 
touch. In throwing a stone or spear, and in many other 
actions, a man must stand firmly on his feet; and this again 
demands the perfect co-adaptation of numerous muscles. 
To chip a flint into the rudest tool, or to form a barbed 
spear or hook from a bone, demands the use of a perfect 
hand; for, as a most capable judge, Mr. Schoolcraft, re- 
marks, the shaping fragments of stone into knives, lances, 
or arrow-beads shows ‘‘extraordinary ability and long prac- 
tice.”” This is to a great extent proved by the fact that 
primeval men practiced a division of labor; each man did 
not manufacture his own flint tools or rude pottery, but cer- 
tain individuals appear to have devoted themselves to such 
work, no doubt receiving in exchange the produce of the 


‘‘Bssay on Man’? has been ably criticised by Prof. Claparéde, one of the most 
distinguished zoologists in Europe, in an article published in the ‘‘Bibliothéque 
Universelle,’’? June, 1870. The remark quoted in my text will surprise every 
one who has read Mr. Wallace’s celebrated paper on ‘“‘The Origin of Human 
Races deduced from the Theory of Natural Selection,”’ originally published in 
the ‘‘Anthropological Review,’’ May, 1864, p. elviii. I cannot here resist quot- 
ing a most just remark by Sir J. Lubbock (‘‘Prehistoric Times,’ 1865, p. 479) 
in reference to this paper, namely, that Mr. Wallace, ‘‘with characteristic un- 
selfishness, ascribes it (¢.e., the idea of natural selection) unreservedly to Mr. 
Darwin, although, as is well known, he struck out the, idea independently, 
and published it, though not with the same elaboration, at the same time.’’ 

6 Quoted by Mr. Lawson Tait in his ‘‘Law of Natural Selection,” ‘‘Duvlin 
Quarterly Journal of Medical Science,’’ Feb., 1869. Dr. Keller is likewise 
quoted to the same effect. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 75 


chase. Archeologists are convinced that an enormous in- 
terval of time elapsed before our ancestors thought of grind- 
ing chipped flints into smooth tools. One can hardly doubt 
that a manlike animal who possessed a hand and arm suffi- 
ciently perfect to throw a stone with precision, or to form a 
flint into a rude tool, could, with sufficient practice, as far as 
mechanical skill alone is concerned, make almost anything 
which a civilized man can make. The structure of the hand 
in this respect may be compared with that of the vocal or- 
gans, which in the apes are used for uttering various signal- 
cries, or, aS in one genus, musical cadences; but in man the 
closely similar vocal organs have become adapted through 
the inherited effects of use for the utterance of articulate 
language. 

Turning now to the nearest allies of men, and therefore 
to the best representatives of our early progenitors, we find 
that the hands of the Quadrumana are constructed on the 
same general pattern as our own, but are far less perfectly 
adapted for diversified uses. Their hands do not'serve for 
locomotion so well as the feet of a dog; as may be seen in 
such monkeys as the chimpanzee and orang, which walk on 
the outer margins of the palms, or on the knuckles.” Their 
hands, however, are admirably adapted for climbing trees. 
Monkeys seize thin branches or ropes, with the thumb on 
one side and the fingers and palm on the other, in the same 
manner as we do. They can thus also lift rather large ob- 
jects, such as the neck of a bottle, to their mouths. Ba- 
boons turn over stones and scratch up roots with their hands. 
They seize nuts, insects, or other small objects with the 
thumb in opposition to the fingers, and no doubt they thus 
extract eggs and the young from the nests of birds. Ameri- 
can monkeys beat the wild oranges on the branches until 
the rind is cracked, and then tear it off with the fingers of 
the two hands. In a wild state they break open hard fruits 
with stones. Other monkeys open mussel-shells with the 
two thumbs. With their fingers they pull out thorns and 


69 Owen, ‘‘Anat, of Vertebrates,”’ iii. p 71. 


76 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


burs, and hunt for each other’s parasites. They roll down 
stones, or throw them at their enemies: nevertheless, they 
are clumsy in these various actions, and, as I have myself 
seen, are quite unable to throw a stone with precision. 

It seems to me far from true that because ‘‘objects are 
grasped clumsily’? by monkeys, ‘‘a much less specialized 
organ of prehension’? would have served them” equally 
well with their present hands. On the contrary, I see no 
reason to doubt that more perfectly constructed hands would 
have been an advantage to them, provided that they were 
not thus rendered less fitted for climbing trees. We may 
suspect that a hand as perfect as that of man would have 
been disadvantageous for climbing, for the most arboreal 
monkeys in the world, namely, Ateles in America, Colobus 
in Africa, and Hylobates in Asia, are either thumbless, or 
their toes partially cohere, so that their limbs are converted 
into mere grasping hooks.” 

As soon as some ancient member in the great series of the 
Primates:came to be less arboreal, owing to a change in its 
manner of procuring subsistence, or to some change in the 
surrounding conditions, its habitual manner of progression 
would have been modified; and thus it would have been 
rendered more strictly quadrupedal or bipedal. Baboons 
frequent hilly and rocky districts, and only from necessity 
climb high trees;” and they have acquired almost the gait 
of a dog. Man alone has become a biped; and we can, I 
think, partly see how he has come to assume his erect atti- 
tude, "which forms one of his most conspicuous characters. 
Man could not have attained-his present dominant position 
in the world without the use of his hands, which are so ad- 


7 “Quarterly Review,” April, 1869, p. 392. 

1 In Hylobates syndactylus, as the name expresses, two of the toes regu- 
larly cohere; and this, as Mr. Blyth informs me, is occasionally the case with 
the toes of A agilis, lar, and leuciscus. Colobus is strictly arboreal and 
extraordinarily active (Brehm, “‘Thierlehen,’’? B. i. s, 50), but whether a better 
climber than the species of the allied genera, I do not know. It deserves 
notice that the feet of the sloths, the most arboreal animals in the world, are 
wonderfully hooklike. 

‘2 Brehm, ‘‘Thierleben,” B, i. s, 80. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 77 


mirably adapted to act in obedience to his will. SirC. Bell” 
insists that ‘‘the hand supplies all instruments, and by its 
correspondence with the intellect gives him universal domin- 
ion.”” But the hands and arms could hardly have become 
perfect enough to have manufactured weapons, or to have 
hurled stones and spears with a true aim, as long as they 
were habitually used for locomotion and for supporting the 
whole weight of the body, or, as before remarked, so long 
as they were especially fitted for climbing trees. Such 
rough treatment would also have blunted the sense of touch, 
on which their delicate use largely depends. From these 
causes alone it would have been an advantage to man to 
become a biped; but for many actions it is indispensable 
that the arms and whole upper part of the body should be 
free; and he must for this end stand firmly on his feet. To 
gain this great advantage, the feet have been rendered flat; 
and the great toe has been peculiarly modified, though this 
has entailed the almost complete loss of its power of prehen- 
sion. It accords with the principle of the division of physi- 
ological labor prevailing throughout the animal kingdom, 
that as the hands became perfected for prehension, the feet 
should have become perfected for support and locomotion. 
With some savages, however, the foot has not altogether 
lost its prehensile power, as shown by their manner of climb- 
ing trees, and of using them in other ways.” 

If it be an advantage to man to stand firmly on his feet 
and to have his hands and arms free, of which, from his pre- 
eminent success in the battle of life, there can be no doubt, 
then I can see no reason why it should not have been ad- 
vantageous to the progenitors of man to have become more 
and more erect or bipedal. They would thus have been 


7 *‘The Hand,” etc., ‘‘Bridgewater Treatise,’’? 1833, p. 38. 

™ Hackel has an excellent discussion on the steps by which man became 
a biped; ‘‘Natirliche Schépfungsgeschichte,’’? 1868, s. 50%. Dr. Buchner 
(‘‘Conférences sur la Théorie Darwinienne,’’ 1869, p. 135) has given good 
eases of the use of the foot as a prehensile organ by man; and has also written 
on the manner of progression of the higher apes, to which I allude in the fol- 
lowing paragraph; see also Owen (‘‘Anatomy of Vertebrates,”’ vol. iii. p. 71) on 
this latter subject. 


78 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


better able to defend themselves with stones or clubs, to 
attack their prey, or otherwise to obtain food. The best 
built individuals would in the long run have succeeded 
best, and have survived in larger numbers. If the gorilla 
and a few allied forms had become extinct, it might have 
been argued, with great force and apparent truth, that an 
animal could not have been gradually converted from a quad- 
ruped into a biped, as all the individuals in an intermediate 
condition would have been miserably ill-fitted for progres- 
sion. But we know (and this is well worthy of reflection) 
that the anthropomorphous apes are now actually in an 
intermediate condition; and no one doubts that they are 
on the whole well adapted for their conditions of life. Thus 
the gorilla runs with a sidelong shambling gait, but more 
commonly progresses by resting on its bent hands. The 
long-armed apes occasionally use their arms like crutches, 
swinging their bodies forward between them, and some kinds 
of Hylobates, without having been taught, can walk or run 
upright with tolerable quickness; yet they move awkwardly 
and much less securely than man. We see, in short, in ex- 
isting monkeys a manner of progression intermediate between 
that of a quadruped and a biped; but, as an unprejudiced 
judge” insists, the anthropomorphous apes approach in 
structure more nearly to the bipedal than to the quadru- 
pedal type. 

As the progenitors of man became more and more erect, 
with their hands and arms more and more modified for pre- 
hension and other purposes, with their feet and legs at the 
same time transformed for firm support and progression, 
endless other changes of structure would have become nec- 
essary. The pelvis would have.to be broadened, the spine 
peculiarly curved, and the head fixed in an altered position, 
all which changes have been attained by man. Prof. Schaaft- 
hausen” maintains that ‘‘the powerful mastoid processes of 


78 Prof. Broca, La Constitution des Vertébres caudales: ‘‘La Revue d’An-e 


thropologie,”? 1872, p. 26 (separate copy). 
%6 “Qn the Primitive Form of the Skull,” translated in “Anthropological 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 79 


the human skull are the result of his erect position’; and 
these processes are absent in the orang, chimpanzee, etc., 
and are smaller in the gorilla than in man. Various other 
structures, which appear connected with man’s erect posi- 
tion, might here have been added. It is very difficult to 
decide how far these correlated modifications are the result 
of natural selection, and how far of the inherited effects of 
the increased use of certain parts, or of the action of one 
part on another. No doubt these means of change often 
co-operate; thus when certain muscles, and the crests of 
bone to which they are attached, become enlarged by habit- 
ual use, this shows that certain actions are habitually per- 
formed and must be serviceable. Hence the individuals 
which performed them best would tend to survive in greater 
numbers. 

The free use of the arms and hands, partly the cause and 
partly the result of man’s erect position, appears to have 
led in an indirect manner to other modifications of structure. 
The early male forefathers of man were, as previously stated, 
probably furnished with great canine teeth; but as they 
gradually acquired the habit of using stones, clubs, or other 
weapons, for fighting with their enemies or rivals, they 
would use their jaws and teeth less and less. In this case, 
the jaws, together with the teeth, would become reduced in 
size, as we may feel almost sure from innumerable analogous 
cases. In a future chapter we shall meet with a closely 
parallel case, in the reduction or complete disappearance of 
the canine teeth in male ruminants, apparently in relation 
with the development of their horns; and in horses, in rela- 
tion to their habit of fighting with ‘their incisor teeth and 
hoofs. 

In the adult male anthropomorphous apes, as Riitimeyer™ 
and others have insisted, it is the effect on the skull of the 


Review,’? Oct., 1868, p. 428. Owen (‘‘Anatomy of Vertebrates,” vol. ii., 
1866, P. 651) on the mastoid processes in the higher apes. 

‘Die Grenzen der Thierwelt, eine Betrachtung zu Darwin’s Lehre,” 
1868, 8. 51. 


80 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


great development of the jaw-muscles that causes it to differ 
so greatly in many respects from that of man, and has given 
to these animals ‘‘a truly frightful physiognomy.’’ There- 
fore, as the jaws and teeth in man’s progenitors gradually 
became reduced in size, the adult skull would have come to 
resemble more and more that of existing man. As we shall 
hereafter see, a great reduction of the canine teeth in the 
males would almost certainly affect the teeth of the females 
through inheritance. 

_As the various mental faculties gradually developed 
themselves the brain would almost certainly become larger. 
No one, I presume, doubts that the large proportion which 
the size of man’s brain bears to his body, compared to the 
same proportion in the gorilla or orang, is closely connected 
with his higher mental powers. We meet with closely analo- 
gous facts with insects, for in ants the cerebral ganglia are 
of extraordinary dimensions, and in all the Hymenoptera 
these ganglia are many times larger than in the less intelli- 
gent orders, such as beetles.”* On the other hand, no one 
supposes that the intellect of any two animals or of any two 
men can be accurately gauged by the cubic contents of their 
skulls. It is certain that there may be extraordinary mental 
activity with an extremely small absolute mass of nervous 
matter: thus the wonderfully diversified instincts, mental 
powers, and affections of ants are notorious, yet their cere- 
bral ganglia are not so large as the quarter of a small pin’s 
head. Under this point of view, the brain of an ant is one 
of the most marvellous atoms of matter in ‘he world, perhaps 
more so than the brain of a man. 

The belief that there exists In man some close relation 
between the size of the brain and the development of the 
intellectual faculties is supported by the comparison of the 
skulls of savage and civilized races, of ancient and modern 
people, and by the analogy of the whole vertebrate series. 


8 Dujardin, ‘‘Annales des Sc. Nat.,’’ 3d series, Zoolog., tom. xiv., 1850, 
p. 203. See also Mr. Lowne, “Anatomy and Phys. of the Musca vomitoria, % 
1870, p. 14. My son, Mr. F, Darwin, dissected igs me the cerebral ganglia ‘of 
the Formica rufa, 


) 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 81 


Dr. J. Barnard Davis has proved,” by many careful meas- 
urements, that the mean internal capacity of the skull in 
Europeans is 92.3 cubic inches; in Americans 87.5; in Asi- 
atics 87.1; and in Australians only 81.9 cubic inches. Prof. 
Broca® found that the nineteenth century skulls from graves 
in Paris were larger than those from vaults of the twelfth 
century, in the proportion of 1484 to 1426; and that the 
increased size, as ascertained by measurements, was exclu- 
sively in the frontal part of the skull—the seat of the intel- 
lectual faculties. Prichard is persuaded that the present 
inhabitants of Britain have ‘‘much more capacious brain- 
cases’’ than the ancient inhabitants. Nevertheless, it must 
be admitted that some skulls of very high antiquity, such as 
the famous one of Neanderthal, are well developed and capa- 
cious.“ With respect to the lower animals, M. E. Lartet,” 
by comparing the crania of tertiary and recent mammals 
belonging to the same groups, has come to the remarkable 
conclusion that the brain is generally larger and the convo- 
lutions are more complex in the more recent forms. On.the 
other hand, I have shown™ that the brains of domestic rab- 
bits are considerably reduced in bulk, in comparison with 
those of the wild rabbit or hare; and this may be attributed 
to their having been closely confined during many genera- 
tions, so that they have exerted their intellect, instincts, 
senses, and voluntary movements but little. 


19 ‘Philosophical Transactions,’’ 1869, p. 513. 

80 “Tes Sélections,’? M. P. Broca, ‘‘Revue d’Anthropologie,’’? 1873; see 
also, as quoted in C. Vogt’s “‘Lectures on Man,’’ Eng. translat., 1864, pp. 88, 
90. Prichard, ‘‘Phys. Hist. of Mankind,”’ vol. i., 1838, p. 305. 

81 In the interesting article just referred to, Prof. Broca has well remarked, 
that in civilized nations the average capacity of the skull must be lowered by 
the preservation of a considerable number of individuals, weak in mind and 
body, who would have been promptly eliminated in the savage state. On the 
other hand, with savages, the average includes only the more capable individ- 
uals, who have been able to survive under extremely hard conditions of life. 
Broca thus explains the otherwise inexplicable fact, that the mean capacity of 
the skull of the ancient Troglodytes of Lozére is greater than that of modern 
Frenchmen, 

82 “Comptes rendus des Sciences,”’ etc., June 1, 1868. 

8 “The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication, 
pp. 124-129. : 


” vol. i. 


82 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


The gradually increasing weight of the brain and skull 
in man must have influenced the development of the sup- 
porting spinal column, more especially while he was becom- 
ing erect. As this change of position was being brought 
about, the internal pressure of the brain will also have in- | 
fluenced the form of the skull; for many facts show how 
easily the skull is thus affected. Hthnologists believe that 
it is modified by the kind of cradle in which infants sleep. 
Habitual spasms of the muscles, and a cicatrix from a severe 
burn, have permanently modified the facial bones. In young 
persons whose heads have become fixed either sidewise or 
backward, owing to disease, one of the two eyes has changed 
its position, and the shape of the skull has been altered ap- 
parently by the pressure of the brain in a new direction.“ 
I have shown that with long-eared rabbits even so trifling a 
cause as the lopping forward of one ear drags forward almost 
every bone of the skull on that side; so that the bones on 
the opposite side no longer strictly correspond. Lastly, if 
any animal were to increase or diminish much in general 
size, without any change in its mental powers, or if the 
mental powers were to be much increased or diminished, 
without any great change in the size of the body, the shape 
of the skull would almost certainly be altered. I infer this 
from my observations on domestic rabbits, some kinds of 
which have become very much larger than the wild animal, 
while others have retained nearly the same size, but in both 
cases the brain has been much reduced relatively to the size 
of the body. Now I was at first much surprised on finding 
that in all these rabbits the skull had become elongated or 
dolichocephalic; for instance, of two skulls of nearly equal 
breadth, the one from a wild rabbit and the other from a 


84 Schaaffhausen gives from Blumenbach and Busch the cases of the 
spasms and cicatrix, in ‘‘Anthropol. Review,’’ Oct., 1868, p. 420. Dr. Jarrold 
(‘‘Anthropologia,’’ 1808, pp. 115, 116) adduces from Camper and from his 
own observations, cases of the modification of the skull from the head being 
fixed in an unnatural position. He believes that in certain trades, such as that 
of a shoemaker, where the head is habitually held forward, the forehead 
becomes more rounded and prominent. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 83 


large domestic kind, the former was 8.15 and the latter 4.3 
inches in length.** One of the most marked distinctions in 
different races of men is that the skull in some is elongated, 
and in others rounded; and here the explanation suggested 
by the case of the rabbits may hold good; for Welcker finds 
that ‘‘short men incline more to brachycephaly, and tall men 
to dolichocephaly’’ ;** and tall men may be compared with the 
larger and longer-bodied rabbits, all of which have elongated 
skulls, or are dolichocephalic. 

From these several facts we can understand, to a certain 
extent, the means by which the great size and more or less 
rounded form of the skull have been acquired by man; and 
these are characters eminently distinctive of him in compari- 
son with the lower animals. 

Another most conspicuous difference between man and 
the lower animals is the nakedness of his skin. Whales 
and porpoises (Cetacea), dugongs (Sirenia), and the hippo- 
potamus are naked; and this may be advantageous to them 
for gliding through the water; nor would it be injurious to 
them from the loss of warmth, as the species which inhabit 
the colder regions are protected by a thick layer of blubber, 
serving the same purpose as the fur of seals and otters. 
Elephants and rhinoceroses are almost hairless; and as cer- 
tain extinct species, which formerly lived under an Arctic 
climate, were covered with long wool or hair, it would al- 
most appear as if the existing species of both genera had lost 
their hairy covering from exposure to heat. This appears 
the more probable, as the elephants in India which live on 
elevated and cool districts are more hairy” than those on the 
lowlands. May we then infer that man became divested of 
hair from having aboriginally inhabited some tropical land ? 
That the hair is chiefly retained in the male sex on the chest 
and face, and in both sexes at the junction of all four limbs 


8 ‘Variation of Animals,’’ ete., vol. i. p. 117, on the elongation of the 
skull; p. 119, on the effect of the lopping of one ear. 

%© Quoted by Schaaffhausen, in *Anthropolog. Review,’ aha 1868, p. 419, 

81 Owen, ‘‘Anatomy of Vertebrates,’’ vol. ili. p. 619. : 


». 


84 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


with the trunk, favors the inference—on the assumption that 
the hair was lost before man became erect; for the parts 
which now retain most hair would then have been most 
protected from the heat of the sun. The crown of the 
head, however, offers a curious exception, for at all times 
it must have been one of the most exposed parts, yet it is 
thickly clothed with hair. The fact, however, that the other 
members of the order of Primates, to which man belongs, 
although inhabiting various hot regions, are well clothed 
with hair, generally thickest on the upper surface,™ is op- 
posed to the supposition that man became naked through 
the action of the sun. Mr. Belt believes® that within the 
tropics it is an advantage to man to be destitute of hair, as 
he is thus enabled ‘to free himself of the multitude of ticks 
(acari) and other parasites, with which he is often infested, 
and which sometimes cause ulceration. But whether this 
evil is of sufficient magnitude to have led to the denudation 
of his body through natural selection, may be doubted, since 
none of the many quadrupeds inhabiting the tropics has, 
as far as I know, acquired any specialized means of relief. 
The view which seems to me the most probable is that man, 
or rather primarily woman, became divested of hair for orna- 
mental purposes, as we shall see under Sexual Selection; 
and, according to this belief, it is not surprising that man 
should differ so greatly in hairiness from all other Primates, 
for characters, gained through sexual selection, often differ 
to an extraordinary degree in closely related forms. 
According to a popular impression, the absence of a tail 


88 Isidore Geoffroy St.-Hilaire remarks (“‘Hist. Nat. Générale,’ tom. ii., 
1859, pp. 215-217) on the head of man being covered with long hair; also on 
the upper surfaces of monkeys and of other mammals being more thickly clothed 
than the lower surfaces. ~ This has likewise been observed by various authors, 
Prof. P. Gervais (‘‘Hist. Nat. des Mammifeéres,’’ tom. i,, 1854, p. 28), however, 
states that in the Gorilla the hair is thinner on the back, ‘where it is partly 
rubbed off, than on the lower surface. 

89 “The Naturalist in Nicaragua,’? 1874, p. 209. As some confirmation 
of Mr. Belt’s view, I may quote the following passage from Sir W. Denison 
(‘Varieties of Vice-Regal Lnfe,”? vol. i, 1870, p. 440): ‘It is said to be a 
practice with the Australians, when the vermin get troublesome, to singe 
themselves.”’ 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 85 


is eminently distinctive of man; but as those apes which 
come nearest to him are destitute of this organ, its disap- 
pearance does not relate exclusively to man. The tail often 
differs remarkably in length within the same genus; thus in 
some species of Macacus it is longer than the whole body, 
and is formed of twenty-four vertebre; in others it consists 
of a scarcely visible stump, containing only three or four 
vertebrz. In some kinds of baboons there are twenty-five, 
while in the mandrill there are ten very small stunted caudal 
vertebrz, or, according to Cuvier, sometimes only five. 
The tail, whether it be long or short, almost always tapers 
toward the end; and this, I presume, results from the atro- 
phy of the terminal muscles, together with their arteries and 
nerves, through disuse, leading to the atrophy of the termi- 
nal bones. But no explanation can at present be given of 
the great diversity which often occurs in its length. Here, 
however, we are more specially concerned with the complete 
external disappearance of the tail. Prof. Broca has recently 
shown” that the tail in all quadrupeds consists of two por- 
tions, generally separated abruptly from each other; the 
basal portion consists of vertebre, more or less perfectly 
channelled and furnished with apophyses like ordinary ver- 
tebre; whereas those of the terminal portion are not chan- 
nelled, are almost smooth, and scarcely resemble true verte- 
bre. A tail, though not externally visible, is really present 
in man and the anthropomorphous apes, and is constructed 
on exactly the same pattern in both. In the terminal por- 
tion the vertebre, constituting the os coccyx, are quite rudi- 
mentary, being much reduced in size and number. In the 
basal portion the vertebre are likewise few, are united 
firmly together, and are arrested in development; but they 
have been rendered much broader and flatter than the corre- 
sponding ‘vertebre in the tails of other animals; they consti- 


9 Mr. St, George Mivart, ‘‘Proe. Zoolog. Soc.,”’ 1865, pp. 562, 583. Dr. J. 
E. Gray, ‘‘Cat. Brit. Mus.: Skeletons.’? Owen, ‘‘Anatomy of Vertebrates,” 
vol. ii. p. 517. Isidore Geoffroy, ‘“‘Hist. Nat. Gén.,”’ tom. ii. p, 244, 

1 “Revue d’Anthropologie,”? 1872; ‘La Constitution des Vertébres cau- 
dales,’? 


86 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


tute what Broca calls the accessory sacral vertebra. These 
are of functional importance by supporting certain internal 
parts and in other ways; and their modification is directly 
connected with the erect or semi-erect attitude of man and 
the anthropomorphous apes. This conclusion is the more 
trustworthy, as Broca formerly held a different view, which 
he has now abandoned. ‘The modification, therefore, of the 
basal caudal vertebree in man and the higher apes may 
have been effected, directly or indirectly, through natural 
selection. 

But what are we to say about the rudimentary and vari- 
able vertebrae of the terminal portion of the tail, forming the 
os coccyz? A notion which has often been, and will no 
doubt again be, ridiculed, namely, that friction has had 
something to do with the disappearance of the external 
portion of the tail, is not so ridiculous as it at first ap- 
pears. Dr. Anderson™ states that the extremely short tail 
of Macacus brunneus is formed of eleven vertebra, including 
the imbedded basal ones. The extremity is tendinous and 
contains no vertebra; this is succeeded by five rudimentary 
ones, so minute that together they are only one line and a 
half in length, and these are permanently bent to one side 
in the shape of a hook. The free part of the tail, only a 
little above an inch in length, includes only four more small 
vertebra. This short tail is carried erect; but about a quar- 
ter of its total length is doubled on to itself to the left; and 
this terminal part, which includes the hooklike portion, 
serves ‘‘to fill up the interspace between the upper diver- 
gent portion of the callosities’’; so that the animal sits on 
it, and thus renders it rough and callous. Dr. Anderson 
thus sums up his observations: ‘“‘These facts seem to me 
to have only one explanation: this tail, from its short size, 
is in the monkey’s way when it sits down, and frequently 
becomes placed under the animal while it is in this attitude; 
and from the circumstance that it does not exterd beyond 
the extremity of the ischial tuberosities it seems as if the 


# “Prog. Zoolog. Soc.,’’? 1872, p. 310. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 87 


tail originally had been bent round, by the will of the ani- 
mal, into the interspace between the callosities, to escape 
being pressed between them and the ground, and that in 
time the curvature became permanent, fitting in of itself 
when the organ happens to be sat upon.’’ Under these 
circumstances it is not surprising that the surface of the 
tail should have been roughened and rendered callous; 
and Dr. Murie,* who carefully observed this species in 
the Zoological Gardens, as well as three other closely allied 
forms with slightly longer tails, says that when the animal 
sits down, the tail ‘tis necessarily thrust to one side of the 
buttocks; and whetlier long or short its root is consequently 
liable to be rubbed or chafed.’” As we now have evidence 
that mutilations occasionally produce an inherited effect, 
it is not very improbable that in short-tailed monkeys the 
projecting part of the tail, being functionally useless, c:suid 
after many generations have become rudimentary and dis- 
torted, from being continually rubbed and chafed. We see 
the projecting part in this condition in the Macacus brunneus, 
and absolutely aborted in the M. ecaudatus and in several of 
the higher apes. Finally, then, as far as we can judge, the 
tail has disappeared in man and the anthropomorphous apes, 
owing to the terminal portion having been injured by friction 
during a long lapse of time; the basal and imbedded portion 
having been reduced and modified, so as to become suitable 
to the erect or semi-erect position. 


I have now endeavored to show that some of the most 
distinctive characters of man have in all probability been 
acquired, either directly, or, more commonly, indirectly, 
through natural selection. We should bear in mind that 


%8 **Proc, Zoolog. Soc.,’? 1872, p. 786. 

% T allude to Dr. Brown-Séquard’s observations on the transmitted effect 
of an opera.ion causing epilepsy in guinea-pigs, and likewise more recently on 
the analogous effects of cutting the sympathetic nerve in the neck. I shall here- 
after have d2casion to refer to Mr. Salvin’s interesting case of the apparently 
inherited efiects of mot-mots biting off the barbs of their own tuil-feathers, 
See also, on the general subject, ‘‘Variation of Animals and Plants under 
Domesticati«n,’’ vol. ii. pp. 22-24. 


88 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


modifications in structure or constitution which do not serve 
to adapt an organism to its habits of life, to the food which it 
consumes, or passively to the surrounding conditions, cannot 
have been thus acquired. We must not, however, be too 
confident in deciding what modifications are of service to 
each being: we should remember how little we know about 
the use of many parts, or what changes in the blood or 
tissues may serve to fit an organism for a new climate 
or new kinds of food. Nor must we forget the principle 
of correlation, by which, as Isidore Geoffroy has shown in 
the case of man, many strange deviations of structure are 
tied together. Independently of correlation, a change in 
one part often leads, through the increased or decreased 
use of other parts, to other changes of a quite unexpected 
nature. It is also well to reflect on such facts as the won- 
deriu! growth of galls on plants caused by the poison of 
an insect, and on the remarkable changes of color in the 
plumage of parrots when fed on certain fishes, or inocu- 
lated with the poison of toads; for we can thus see that 
the fluids of the system, if altered for some special purpose, 
might induce other changes. We should especially bear 
in mind that modifications acquired and continually used 
during past ages for some useful purpose would probably 
become firmly fixed, and might be long inherited. 

Thus a large yet undefined extension may safely be given 
to the direct and indirect results of natural selection; but I 
now admit, after reading the essay of Nageli on plants, and 
the remarks by various authors with respect to animals, 
more especially those recently made by Prof. Broca, that 
in the earlier editions of my ‘‘Origin of Species’’ I perhaps 
attributed too much to the action of natural selection or the 
survival of the fittest. I have altered the fifth edition of 
the “Origin”? so as to confine my remarks to adaptive 
changes of structure; but’ I am convinced, from the light 
gained during even ‘the last few years, that very many 


% ‘The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,‘’ vol. ii. pp. 
280, 282. 4 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 89 


structures which now appear to us useless will hereafter 
be proved to be useful, and will therefore come within 
the range of natural selection. Nevertheless, I did not 
formerly consider sufficiently the existence of structures 
which, as far as we can at present judge, are neither 
beneficial nor injurious;*and this I believe to be one 
of the greatest oversights as yet detected in my work. I 
may be permitted to say, as some excuse, that I had two 
distinct objects in view: first, to show that species had not 
been separately created, and, secondly, that natural selection 
had been the chief agent of change, though largely aided 
by the inherited effects of habit, and slightly by the direct 
action of the surrounding conditions. I was not, however, 
able to annul the influence of my former belief, then almost 
universal, that each species had been purposely created; and 
this led to my tacit assumption that every detail of structure, 
excepting rudiments, was of some special, though unrecog- 
nized, service. Any one with this assumption in his mind 
would naturally extend too far the action of natural selec- 
tion, either during past or present times. Some of those who 
admit the principle of evolution, but reject natural selection, 
seem to forget, when criticising my book, that I had the. 
above two objects in view; hence if I have erred in giving 
to natural selection great power, which I am very far from 
admitting, or in having exaggerated its power, which is in 
itself probable, I have at least, as I hope, done good service 
in aiding to overthrow the dogma of separate creations. X 

It is, as I can now see, probable that all organic beings, 
including man, possess peculiarities of structure which neither 
are now nor were formerly of any service to them, and which, 
therefore, are of no physiological importance. We know 
not what produces the numberless slight differences between 
the individuals of each species, for reversion only carries the 
problem a few steps backward; but each peculiarity must 
have had its efficient cause. If these causes, whatever they 
may be, were to act more uniformly and energetically dur- 
ing a lengthened period (and against this no reason can be 


90 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


assigned), the result would probably be not a mere slight in- 
dividual difference, but a well-marked and constant modifi- 
cation, though one of no physiological importance. Changed 
structures, which are in no way beneficial, cannot be kept 
uniform through natural selection, though the injurious will 
be thus eliminated. Uniformity of character would, how- 
ever, naturally follow from the assumed uniformity of the 
exciting causes, and likewise from the free intercrossing 
of many individuals. During successive periods the same 
organism might, in this manner, acquire successive modifi- 
cations, which would be transmitted in a nearly uniform 
state as long as the exciting causes remained the same and 
there was free intercrossing. With respect to the exciting 
causes we can only say, as when speaking of so-called 
spontaneous variations, that they relate much more closely 
to the constitution of the varying organism than to the 
nature of the conditions to which it has been subjected. 


Conclusion.—In this chapter we have seen that as man at 
the present day is liable, like every other animal, to multi- 
form individual differences or slight variations, so no doubt 
were the early progenitors of man; the variations being for- 
merly induced by the same general causes, and governed by 
the same general and complex laws as at present. As all 
animals tend to multiply beyond their means of subsistence, 
so it must have been with the progenitors of man; and this 
would inevitably lead to a struggle for existence and to nat- 
ural‘selection. The latter process would be greatly aided by 
the inherited effects of the increased use of parts, and these 
two processes would incessantly react on each other. It ap- 
pears, also, as we shall hereafter see, that various unimpor- 
tant characters have been acquired by man through sexual 
selection. An unexplained residuum of change must be left 
to the assumed uniform action of those unknown agencies 
which occasionally induce strongly marked and abrupt devi- 
ations of structure in our domestic productions. 

Judging from the habits of savages and of the greater 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 91 


number of the Quadrumana, primeval meh, and even their 
apelike progenitors, probably lived in society. With 
strictly social animals, natural selection sometimes acts on 
the individual, through the preservation of variations which 
are beneficial to the community. A community which in- 
cludes a large number of well-endowed individuals increases 
in number, and is victorious over other less favored ones, 
even although each separate member gains no advantage 
over the others of the same community. Associated insects 
have thus acquired many remarkable structures, which are 
of little or no service to the individual, such as the pollen- 
collecting apparatus, or the sting of the worker-bee, or the 
great jaws of soldier-ants.. With the higher social animals 
I am not aware that any structure has been modified solely 
for the good of the community, though some are of second- 
ary service to it. For instance, the horns of ruminants and 
the great canine teeth of baboons appear to have been ac- 
quired by the males as weapons for sexual strife, but they | 
are used in defence of the herd or troop. In regard to cer- 
tain mental powers the case, as we shall see in the fifth chap- 
ter, is wholly different; for these faculties have been chiefly, 
or even exclusively, gained for the benefit of the commu- 
nity, and the individuals thereof have at the same time 
gained an advantage indirectly. 


It has often been objected to such views as the foregoing, 
that man is one of the most helpless and defenceless crea- 
tures in the world; and that during his early and less well- 
developed condition he would have been still more helpless. 
The Duke of Argyll, for instance, insists’ that ‘‘the human . 
frame has diverged from the structure of brutes, in the direc- 
tion of greater physical helplessness and weakness. That is 
to say, it is a divergence which of all others it is most im- 
possible to ascribe to mere natural selection.” He adduces 
the naked and unprotected state of the body, the absence 
of great teeth or claws for defence, the small strength and 


9% ‘Primeval Man,’’ 1869, p. 66. _ 


92 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


speed of man, and his slight power of discovering food or 
of avoiding danger by smell. To these deficiencies there 
might be added one still more serious, namely, that he can- 
not climb quickly, and so escape from enemies. The loss 
of hair would not have been a great injury to the inhabi- 
tants of a warm country, for we know that the unclothed 
Fuegians can exist under a wretched climate. When we 
compare the defenceless state of man with that of apes, 
we must remember that the great canine teeth with which 
the latter are provided are possessed in their full develop- 
ment by the males alone, and are chiefly used by them for 
fighting with their rivals; yet the females, which are not 
thus provided, manage to survive. 

In regard to bodily size or strength, we do not know 
whether man is descended from some small species, like 
the chimpanzee, or from one as powerful as the gorilla; 
and, therefore, we cannot say whether man has become 
larger and stronger, or smaller and weaker, than his ances- 
tors. We should, however, bear in mind that an animal 
possessing great size, strength, and ferocity, and which, 
like the gorilla, could defend itself from all enemies, would 
not perhaps have become social; and this would most effect- 
ually have checked the acquirement of the higher mental 
qualities, such as sympathy and the love of his fellows. 
Hence it might have been an immense advantage to man 
to have sprung from some comparatively weak creature. 

The small strength and speed of man, his want of natural 
weapons, etc., are more than counterbalanced: first, by his 
intellectual powers, through which he has formed for him- 
self weapons, tools, ete., though still remaining in a barba- 
rous state; and, secondly, by his social qualities, which lead 
him to give and receive aid from his fellow-men. No coun- 
try in the world abounds in a greater degree with dangerous 
beasts than Southern Africa; no country presents more fear- 
ful physical hardships than the Arctic regions; yet one of 
the puniest of races, that of the Bushmen, maintains itself 
in Southern Africa, as do the dwarfed Eskimos in the 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 93 


Arctic regions. The ancestors of man were, no doubt, in- 
ferior in intellect, and probably in social disposition, to the 
lowest existing savages; but it is quite conceivable that they 
might have existed, or even flourished, if they had advanced 
in intellect, while gradually losing their brutelike powers, 
such as that of climbing trees, etc. But these ancestors 
would not have been exposed to any special danger, even 
if far more helpless and defenceless than any existing sav- 
ages, had they inhabited some warm continent or large isl- 
and, such as Australia, New Guinea, or Borneo, which is 
now the home of the orang. And natural selection arising 
from the competition of tribe with tribe, in some such large 
area as one of these, together with the inherited effects of 
habit, would, under favorable conditions, have sufficed to 
raise man to his present high position in the organic scale. 


94 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


CHAPTER III 


COMPARISON OF THE MENTAL POWERS OF MAN AND THE 
LOWER ANIMALS 


The difference in mental power between the highest ape and the lowest 
savage, immense—Certain instincts in common—The emotions— 
Curiosity—Imitation—A ttention — Memory — Imagination—Reason— 
Progressive improvement—Tools and weapons used by animals— 
Abstraction, self-consciousness—Language—Sense of beauty—Belief 
in God, spiritual agencies, superstitions 


E have seen in the last two chapters that man bears 

\/ \ / in his bodily structure clear traces of his descent 
from some lower form; but it may be urged that, 

as man differs so greatly in his mental power from all other 
animals, there must be some error in this conclusion. No 
doubt the difference in this respect is enormous, even if we 
compare the mind of one of the lowest savages, who has 
no words to express any number higher than four, and who 
uses hardly any abstract terms for common objects or for 
the affections,’ with that of the most highly organized ape. 
The difference would, no doubt, still remain immense, even 
if one of the higher apes had been improved or civilized as 
much as a dog has been in comparison with its parent-form, 
the wolf or jackal. The Fuegians rank among the lowest 
barbarians; but I was continually struck with surprise how 
closely the three natives on board H.M.S. ‘‘Beagle,’’ who 
had lived some years in England, and could talk a little 
English, resembled us in disposition and in most of our 
mental faculties. If no organic being excepting man had 
possessed any mental power, or if his powers had been of 


1 See the evidence on those points, as given by Lubbock, ‘‘Prehistoric 
Times,’’ p. 354, ete. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 95 


& wholly different nature from those of the lower animals, 
then we should never have been able to convince ourselves 
that our high faculties had been gradually developed. But 
it can be shown that there is no fundamental difference of 
this kind. We must also admit that there is a much wider 
interval in mental power between one of the lowest fishes, 
as a lamprey or lancelet, and one of the higher apes, than 
between an ape and man; yet this interval is filled up by 
numberless gradations. 

Nor is the difference slight in moral disposition between 
a barbarian, such as the man described by the old navigator 
Byron, who dashed his child on the rocks for dropping a 
basket of sea-urchins, and a Howard or Clarkson; and in 
intellect, between a savage who uses hardly any abstract 
terms and a Newton or Shakespeare. Differences of this 
kind between the highest men of the highest races and the 
lowest savages are connected by the finest gradations. 
Therefore it is possible that they might pass and be 
developed into each other. 

My object in this chapter is to show that there is no 
fundamental difference between man and the higher mam- 
mals in their mental faculties. Hach division of the subject 
might have been extended into a separate essay, but must 
here be treated briefly. As no classification of the mental 
powers has been universally acceptedXI shall arrange my 
remarks in the order most convenient for my purpose; and 
will select those facts which have struck me most, with the 
hope that they may produce some effect on the reader. x 

With respect to animals very low in the scale, I shall 
give some additional facts under Sexual Selection, showing 
that their mental powers are much higher than might have 
been expected. The variability of the faculties in the indi- 
viduals of the same species is an important point for us, and 
some few illustrations will here be given. But it would be 
superfluous to enter into many details on this head, for I 
_ have found, on frequent inquiry, that it is the unanimous 

opinion of all those who have long attended to animals of 


96 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


many kinds, including birds, that the individuals differ 
greatly in every mental characteristic. In what manner the 
mental powers were first developed in the lowest organisms 
is as hopeless an inquiry as how life itself first originated. 
These are problems for the distant future, if they are ever 
to be solved by man. 

XAs man possesses the same senses as the lower animals, 
his fundamental intuitions must be the same“ Man has also 
some few instincts in common, as that of self-preservation, 
sexual love, the love of the mother for her new-born off- 
spring, the desire possessed by the latter to suck, and so 
forth. But man, perhaps, has somewhat fewer instincts 
than those possessed by the animals which come next to 
him in the series. The orang in the Hastern islands, and 
the chimpanzee in “Africa, build platforms on which they 
sleep; and, as both species follow the same habit, it might 
be argued that this was due to instinct, but we cannot fee] 
sure that it is not the result of both animals having similar 
wants, and possessing similar powers of reasoning. These 
apes, aS We may assume, avoid the many poisonous fruits 
of the tropics, and man has no such knowledge; but as our 
domestic animals, when taken to foreign lands, and when 
first turned out in the spring, often eat poisonous herbs, 
which they afterward avoid, we cannot feel sure that the 
apes do not learn from their own experience or from that 
of their parents what fruits to select. It is, however, cer- 
tain, as we shall presently see, that apes have an instinctive 
dread of serpents, and probably of other dangerous animals. 

The fewness and the comparative simplicity of the in- 
stincts in the higher animals are remarkable in contrast 
with those of the lower animals. Cuvier maintained that 
instinct and intelligence stand in an inverse ratio to each 
other; and some have thought that the intellectual faculties 
of the higher animals have been gradually developed from 
their instincts. But Pouchet, in an interesting essay,? has 


2 “T/Instinet chez Jes Insectes,’’ ‘Revue des Deux Mondes,’’ Feb., 1870, 
p. 690. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 97 


shown that no such inverse ratio really exists. Those in- 
sects which possess the most wonderful instincts are cer- 
tainly the most intelligent. In the vertebrate series, the 
least intelligent members, namely fishes and amphibians, 
do not possess complex instincts; and among mammals the 
animal most remarkable for its instincts, namely, the beaver, 
is highly intelligent, as will be admitted by every one who 
has read Mr. Morgan’s excellent work.® 

Although the first dawnings of intelligence, according 
to Mr. Herbert Spencer,* have been developed through the 
multiplication and co-ordination of reflex actions, and al- 
though many of the simpler instincts graduate into reflex 
actions, and can hardly be distinguished from them, as in 
the case of young animals sucking, yet the more complex 
instincts seem to have originated independently of intelli- 
gence. Iam, however, very far from wishing to deny that 
instinctive actions may lose their fixed and untaught charac- 
ter, and be replaced by others performed by the aid of the. 
free will. On the other hand, some intelligent actions, after 
being performed during several generations, become con- 
verted into instincts and are inherited, as when birds on 
oceanic islands learn to avoid man. These actions may then 
be said to be degraded in character, for they are no longer 
performed through reason or from experience. But the 
greater number of the more complex instincts appear to 
have been gained in a wholly different manner, through 
the natural selection of variations of simpler instinctive 
actions. Such variations appear to arise from the same 
unknown causes acting on the cerebral organization which 
induce slight variations or individual differences in other 
parts of the body; and these variations, owing to our igno- 
rance, are often said to arise spontaneously. We can, I 
think, come to no other conclusion with respect to the ori- 
gin of the more complex instincts, when we reflect on the 
marvellous instincts of sterile worker-ants and bees, which 


3 “The American Beaver and his Works,”’ 1868. 
_ 4-*"Tne Principles of Psychology,’’ 2d edit., 1870, pp. 418-443, 


Descent—Vo.u. I.—5 


98 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


leave no offspring to inherit the effects of experience and 
of modified habits. 

Although, as we learn from the above-mentioned insects 
and the beaver, a high degree of intelligence is certainly 
compatible with complex instincts, and although actions, at 
first learned voluntarily, can soon through habit be per- 
formed with the quickness and certainty of a reflex action, 
yet it is not improbable that there is a certain amount of 
interference between the development of free intelligence 
and of instinct—which latter implies some inherited modi- 
fication of the brain. Little is known about the functions 
of the brain, but we can perceive that as the intellectual 
powers become highly developed, the various parts of the 
brain must be connected by very intricate channels of the 
freest intercommunication; and as a consequence, each sepa- 
rate part would perhaps tend to be less well fitted to answer 
to particular sensations or associations in a definite and in- 
herited—that is instinctive—manner. There seems even to 
exist some relation between a low degree of intelligence and 
a strong tendency to the formation of fixed, though not in- 
herited habits; for, as a sagacious physician remarked to 
me, persons who are slightly imbecile tend to act in every- 
thing by routine or habit, and they are rendered much 
happier if this is encouraged. 

I have thought this digression worth giving, because 
we may easily underrate the mental powers of the higher 
animals, and especially of man, when we compare their 
actions founded on the memory of past events, on fore- 
sight, reason, and imagination, with exactly similar actions 
instinctively performed by the lower animals; in this latter 
case the capacity of performing such actions has been 
gained, step by step, through the variability of the mental 
organs and natural selection, without any conscious intel- 
ligence on the part of the animal during each successive 
generation. No doubt, as Mr. Wallace has argued,* much 


5 “Contributions to the Theory of Natural Selection,’? 1870, p. £12. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 99° 


of the intelligent work done by man is due to imitation and 
not to reason; but there is this great difference between his 
actions and many of those performed by the lower animals, 
namely, that man cannot, on his first trial, make, for in- 
stance, a stone hatchet or a canoe, through his power of 
imitation. He has to learn his work by practice; a beaver, 
on the other hand, can make its dam or canal, and a bird its 
nest, as well, or nearly as well, anda spider its wonderful 
web quite as well,* the first time it tries, as when old and 
experienced. 

To return to our immediate subject: the lower animals, 
like man, manifestly feel pleasure and pain, happiness and 
misery. Happiness is never better exhibited than by young 
animals, such as puppies, kittens, lambs, etc., when playing 
together, like our own children. Even insects play together, 
as has been described by that excellent observer, P. Huber,’ 
who saw ants chasing and pretending to bite each other, like 
so many puppies. 

The fact that the lower animals are excited by the same 
emotions as ourselves is so well established that it will not 
be necessary to weary the reader by many details. Terror 
acts in the same manner on them as on us, causing the mus- 
cles to tremble, the heart to palpitate, the sphincters to be 
relaxed, and the hair to stand on end. Suspicion, the off- 
spring of fear, is eminently characteristic of most wild ani- 
mals. It is, I think, impossible to read the account given 
by Sir E. Tennent, of the behavior of the female elephants, 
used as decoys, without admitting that they intentionally 
practice deceit, and well know what they are about. Cour- 
age and timidity are extremely variable qualities in the 
individuals of the same species, as is plainly seen in our 
dogs. Some dogs and horses are ill-tempered, and easily 
turn sulky; others are good-tempered; and these qualities 
are certainly inherited. Every one knows how liable ani- 


6 For the evidence on this head, see Mr. J. Traherne Moggridge’s most 
interesting work, ‘‘Harvesting Ants and Trap-door Spiders,’’ 1873, pp. 126, 128, 
1 “Recherches sur les Mceurs des Fourmis,”? 1810, p. 173. 


1v0 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


mals are to furious rage, and how plainly they show it. 
Many, and probably true, anecdotes have been published 
on the long-delayed and artful revenge of various animals. 
The accurate Rengger, and Brehm? state that the American 
and African monkeys which they kept tame certainly re- 
venged themselves. Sir Andrew Smith, a zoologist whose 
scrupulous accuracy was known to many persons, told me 
the following story of which he was himself an eye-witness: 
At the Cape of Good Hope an officer had often plagued a 
certain baboon, and the animal, seeing him approaching one 
Sunday for parade, poured water into a hole and hastily 
made some thick mud, which he skilfully dashed over the 
officer as he passed ay to the amusement of many bystand- 
ers. For long afterward the baboon rejoiced and ee 
whenever he saw his victim. 

The love of a dog for his master is notorious; as an old 
writer quaintly says,’ ‘‘A dog is the only thing on this earth 
that luvs you more than he luvs himself.’’ 

y' In the agony of death a dog has been known to caress his - 
master, and every one has heard of the dog suffering under 
vivisection, who licked the hand of the operator; this man, 
unless the operation was fully justified by an increase of our 
knowledge, or unless he had a heart of stone, must have felt 
remorse to the last hour of his life.% 

As Whewell” has well asked, ‘‘who that reads the touch- 
ing instances of maternal affection, related so often of the 
women of all nations, and.of the females of all animals, can 
doubt that the principle of action is the same in the two 
cases?’’ We see maternal affection exhibited in the most 
trifling details; thus Rengger observed an American monkey 
(a Cebus) carefully driving away the flies which plagued her 
infant; and Duvaucel saw a Hylobates washing the faces 


8 All the following statements, given on the authority of these two natural- 
ists, are taken from Rengger’s “‘Naturgesch. der Sdugethiere von Paraguay,” 
1830, s. 41-57, and from Brehm’s “‘Thierleben,’’ B, i. 8. 10-87. 

® Quoted by Dr, Lauder Lindsay, in his “Physiology of Mind in the Lower 
Animals’’; ‘‘Journal of Mental Science,’ April, 1871, p. 38. 

10 “Bridgewater Treatise,’ p. 263. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN : 101 


of her young ones in astream. So intense is the grief of 
female monkeys for the loss of their young, that it invari- 
ably caused the death of certain kinds kept under confine- 
ment by Brehm in North Africa. Orphan monkeys were 
always adopted and carefully guarded by the other monkeys, 
both males and females. One female baboon had so capa- 
cious a heart that she not only adopted young monkeys of 
other species, but stole young dogs and cats, which she con- 
tinually carried about. Her kindness, however, did not go 
so far as to share her food with her adopted offspring, at 
which Brehm was surprised, as his monkeys always divided 
everything quite fairly with their own young ones. An 
adopted kitten scratched this affectionate baboon, who cer- 
tainly had a fine intellect, for she was much astonished at 
being scratched, and immediately examined the kitten’s 
feet, and without more ado bit off the claws."* In the 
Zoological Gardens, I heard from the keeper that an old 
baboon (C., Chacma) had adopted a Rhesus monkey; but 
when a young drill and, mandrill were placed in the cage, 
she seemed to perceive that these monkeys, though distinct 
species, were her near relatives, for she at once rejected 
the Rhesus and adopted both of them. The young Rhesus, 
as I saw, was greatly discontented at being thus rejected, 
and it would, like a naughty child, annoy and attack the 
young drill and mandrill whenever it could do so with 
safety; this conduct exciting great indignation in the old 
baboon. Monkeys will also, according to Brehm, defend 
their master when attacked by any one, as well as dogs 
to whom they are attached, from the attacks of other dogs. 
But we here trench on the subjects of sympathy and fidelity, 
to which I shall recur. Some of Brehm’s monkeys took 
much delight in teasing a certain old dog whom they dis- 
liked, as well as other animals, in various ingenious ways. 


n A critic, without any grounds (‘‘Quarterly Review,”’ July, 1871, p. 72), 
disputes the possibility of this act as described by Brehm, for the sake of dis- 
crediting my work. Therefore I tried, and found that I could readily seize 
with nly own teeth the sharp little claws of a kitten nearly five weeks old. 


\ 
| 


1020 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


Most of the more complex emotions are common to the 
higher animals and ourselves. Every one has seen how jeal- 
ous a dog is of his master’s affection, if lavished on any other 
creature; and I have observed the same fact with monkeys. 
This shows that animals not only love, but have desire to 
be loved. Animals manifestly feel emulation. They love 
approbation or praise; and a dog carrying a basket for his 
master exhibits in a high degree self-complacency or pride. 
There can, I think, be no doubt that a dog feels shame, as 
distinct from fear, and something very like modesty when 
begging too often for food. A great dog scorns the snarling 
of a little dog, and this may be called magnanimity. Sev- 
eral observers have stated that monkeys certainly dislike 
being laughed at; and they sometimes invent imaginary 
offences. In the Zoological Gardens I saw 4 baboon who 
always got into a furious rage when his keeper took out a 
letter or book and read it aloud to him; and his rage was 
so violent that, as I witnessed on one occasion, he bit his 
own leg till the blood flowed. Dogs show what may be 
fairly called a sense of humor, as distinct from mere play; 
if a bit of stick or other such object be thrown to one, he 
will often carry it away for a short distance; and then, 
squatting down with it on the ground close before him, 
will wait until his master comes quite close to take it 
away. The dog will then seize it and rush away in tri- 
umph, repeating the same manceuvre, and evidently enjoy- 
ing the practical joke. 

We will now turn to the more intellectual emotions and 
faculties, which are very important, as forming the basis 
for the development of the higher mental powers. Animals 
manifestly enjoy excitement, and suffer from ennui, as may 
be seen with dogs, and, according to Rengger, with monkeys, 
All animals feel Wonder, and many exhibit Curiosity. They 
sometimes suffer from this latter quality, as when the hunter 
plays antics and thus attracts them; I have witnessed this 
with deer, and so it is with the wary chamois, and with some 
kinds of wild ducks. Brehm gives a curious account c” 


* 


ihe 


el, 
With 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 103 


instinctive dread which his monkeys exhibited for snakes; 
but their curiosity was so great that they could not desist 
from occasionally satiating their horror in a most human 
fashion, by lifting up the lid of the box in which the snakes 
were kept. I was so much surprised at his account, that I 
took a stuffed and coiled-up snake into the monkey-house at 
the Zoological Gardens, and the excitement thus caused 
was one of the most curious spectacles which I ever beheld. 
Three species of Cercopithecus were the most alarmed; they 
dashed about their cages and uttered sharp signal cries of 
danger, which were understood by the other monkeys. A 
few young monkeys and one old Anubis baboon alone took 
no notice of the snake. I then placed the stuffed specimen 
on the ground in one of the larger compartments. After 
a time all the monkeys collected round it in a large circle, 
and, staring intently, presented a most ludicrous appearance. 
‘They became extremely nervous; so that when a wooden 
ball, with which they were familiar as a plaything, was ac- 
cidentally moved in the straw, under which it was partly 
hidden, they all instantly started away. These monkeys 
behaved very differently when a dead fish, a mouse,” a liv- 
ing turtle, and other new objects were placed in their cages; 
for, though at first frightened, they soon approached, han- 
dled, and examined them. I then placed a live snake in 
a paper bag, with the mouth loosely closed, in one of the 
larger compartments. One of the monkeys immediately ap- 
proached, cautiously opened the bag a little, peeped in, and 
instantly dashed away. Then I witnessed what Brehm has 
described, for monkey after monkey, with head raised high 
and turned on one side, could not resist taking a momentary 
peep into the upright bag, at the dreadful object lying 
quietly at the bottom. It would almost appear as if mon- 
keys had some notion of zoological affinities, for those kept 
by Brehm exhibited a strange, though mistaken, instinctive 
dread of innocent lizards and frogs. An orang, also, has 


2 T have given a short account of their behavior on this occasion in my 
**Rixpression of the Emotions,’ p. 43. 


104 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


been known to be much alarmed at the first sight of a 
turtle. ** 

The principle of Jmitation is strong in man, and espe- 
cially, as I have myself observed, with savages. In certain 
morbid states of the brain this tendency is exaggerated to 
an extraordinary degree; some hemiplegic patients and oth- 
ers, at the commencement of inflammatory softening of the 
brain, unconsciously imitate every word which is uttered, 
whether in their own or in a foreign language, and every 
gesture or action which is performed near them.’* Desor’® 
has remarked that no animal voluntarily imitates an action 
performed by man, until in the ascending scale we come to 
monkeys, which are well known to be ridiculous mockers, 
Animals, however, sometimes imitate each other’s actions; 
_ thus two species of wolves, which had been reared by dogs, 
learned to bark, as does sometimes the jackal, '* but whether 
this can be called voluntary imitation is another question. 
Birds imitate the songs of their parents, and sometimes of 
other birds; and parrots are notorious imitators of any 
sound which they often hear. Dureau de la Malle gives 
an account’ of a dog reared by a cat, who learned to imi- 
tate the well-known action of a cat licking her paws, and 
thus washing her ears and face; this was also witnessed by 
the celebrated naturalist, Audouin. I have received several 
confirmatory accounts; in one of these, a dog had not been 
suckled by a cat, but had been brought up with one, together 
with kittens, and had thus acquired the above habit, which 
he ever afterward practiced during his life of thirteen years. 
Dureau de la Malle’s dog likewise learned from the kittens 
to play with a ball by rolling it about with his forepaws, and 
springing on it. A correspondent assures me that a cat in 
his house used to put her paws into jugs of milk having too 
narrow a mouth for her head. A kitten of this cat soon 


18 W, OC. L. Martin, “‘Nat. Hist. of Mammalia,’’ 1841, p. 405. 

44 Dr, Bateman ‘‘On Aphasia,’’ 1870, p. 110. 

18 Quoted by Vogt, ‘‘Mémoire sur les Microcéphales,’’ 1867, p. 168. 

16 “The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,’’ vol. i. p. 27. 
“Annales des Sc. Nat.’’ (1st Series), tom. xxii, p. 397, 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 105 


learned the same trick, and practiced it ever afterward 
whenever there was an opportunity., 

The parents of many animals, trusting to the principle 
of imitation in their young, and more especially to their 
instinctive or inherited tendencies, may be said to educate 
them. We see this when a cat brings a live mouse to her 
kittens; and Dureau de la Malle has given a curious account 
(in the paper above quoted) of his observations on hawks 
which taught their young dexterity, as well as judgment 
of distances, by first dropping through the air dead mice 
and sparrows, which the young generally failed to catch, 
and then bringing them live birds and letting them loose. 

Hardly any faculty is more important for the intellectual 
progress of man than Attention. Animals clearly manifest 
this power, as when a cat watches by a hole and prepares 
to spring on its prey. Wild animals sometimes become so 
absorbed when thus engaged that they may be easily ap- 
proached. Mr. Bartlett has given me a curious proof how 
variable this faculty is in monkeys. A man who trains 
monkeys to act in plays used to purchase common kinds 
from the Zoological Society at the price of five pounds for 
each; but he offered to give double the price, if he might” 
keep three or four of them for a few days, in order to select 
one. When asked how he could possibly learn so soon 
whether a particular monkey would turn out a good actor, 
he answered that it all depended on their power of atten- 
tion. If, when he was talking and explaining anything 
to a monkey, its attention was easily distracted, as by a fly 
on the wall or other trifling object, the case was hopeless. 
If he tried by punishment to make an inattentive monkey 
act, it turned sulky. On the other hand, a monkey which 
carefully attended to him could always be trained. 

1t is almost superfluous to state that animals have excel- 
lent Memories for persons and places. A baboon at the Cape 
of Gocd Hope, as I have been informed by Sir Andrew 
Smith, recognized him with joy after an absence of nine 
months. I had a dog who was savage and averse to all 


106 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


strangers, and I purposely tried his memory after an ab- 
sence of five years and two days. I went near the stable 
where he lived, and shouted to him in my old manner; he 
showed no joy, but instantly followed me out walking, and 
obeyed me, exactly as if I had parted with him only half an 
hour before. A train of old associations, dormant during 
five years, had thus been instantaneously awakened in his 
mind. Even ants, as P. Huber* has clearly shown, recog-. 
nized their fellow-ants belonging to the same community 
after a separation of four months. Animals can certainly 
by some means judge of the intervals of time between recur- 
rent events. 

The Imagination is one of the highest prerogatives of 
man. By this faculty he unites former images and ideas, 
independently of the will, and thus creates brilliant and 
novel results. A poet, as Jean Paul Richter remarks,” 
‘‘who must reflect whether he shall make a character say 
yes or no—to the devil with him; he is only a stupid 
corpse.’? Dreaming gives us the best notion of this power; 
as Jean Paul again says, ‘‘The dream is an involuntary art 
of poetry.” The value of the products of our imagination 
depends of course on the number, accuracy, and clearness 
of our impressions, on our judgment and taste in selecting 
or rejecting the involuntary combinations, and to a certain 
extent on our power of voluntarily combining them. <As 
dogs, cats, horses, and probably all the higher animals, even 
birds,”° have vivid dreams, and this is shown by their move- 
ments and the sounds uttered, we must admit that they pos- 
sess some power of imagination. There must be something 
special which causes dogs to howl in the night, and espe- 
cially during moonlight, in that remarkable and melancholy 
manner called baying. All dogs do not do so; and, accord- 


18 ‘Teg Mceurs des Fourmis,’’ 1810, p. 150. 

19 Quoted in Dr. Maudsley’s ‘‘Physiology and Pathology of Mind,’’ 1868, 
pp. 19, 220. 

2% Dr. Jerdon, ‘‘Birds of India,” vol. i., 1862, p. xxi. Houzeau says that 
his paroquet and canary-birds dreamed: ‘‘Facultés mentales,’’ tom. ii. p. 136. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 107 


ing to Houzeau,” they do not then look at the moon, but 
at some fixed point near the horizon. Houzeau thinks that 
their imaginations are disturbed by the vague outlines of the 
surrounding objects, and conjure up before them fantastic 
images: if this be so, their feelings may almost be called 
superstitious. 

Of all the faculties of the human mind, it will, I presume, 
be admitted that Reason stands at the summit. Only a few 
persons now dispute that animals possess some power of rea- 
soning. Animals may constantly be seen to pause, deliber- 
ate, and resolve. It is a significant fact, that the more the 
habits of any particular animal are studied by a naturalist, 
the more he attributes to reason and the less to unlearned 
instincts.” In future chapters we shall see that some ani- 
mals extremely low in the scale apparently display a certain 
amount of reason. No doubt it is often difficult to distin- 
guish between the power of reason and that of instinct. For 
instance, Dr. Hayes, in his work on ‘‘The Open Polar Sea,”’ 
repeatedly remarks that his dogs, instead of continuing to 
draw the sledges in a compact body, diverged and separated 
when they came to thin ice, so that their weight might be 
more evenly distributed. This was often the first warning 
which the travellers received that the ice was becoming thin 
and dangerous. Now, did the dogs act thus from the ex- 
perience of each individual, or from the example of the 
older and wiser dogs, or from an inherited habit, that is, 
from instinct? This instinct may possibly have arisen since 
the time, long ago, when dogs were first employed by the 
natives in drawing their sledges; or the Arctic wolves, 
the parent-stock of the Eskimo dog, may have acquired 
an instinct impelling them not to attack their prey in a 
close pack, when on thin ice. 

We can only judge by the circumstances under which 


21 “Pecultés Mentales des Animaux,’? 1872, tom. ii. p. 181. 

% Mr. L. H. Morgan’s excellent work on ‘‘The American Beaver,” pub- 
lished in 1868, offers a good illustration of this remark. I cannot help thinking, 
however, that he goes too far in underrating the power of Instinct. 


108 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


actions are performed whether they are due to instinct, or 
to reason, or to the mere association of ideas: this latter 
principle, however, is intimately connected with reason. A 
curious case has been given by Prof. Mobius,” of a pike, 
separated by a plate of glass from an adjoining aquarium 
stocked with fish, and who often dashed himself with such 
violence against the glass, in trying to catch the other fishes, 
that he was sometimes completely stunned. The pike went 
on thus for three months, but at last learned caution, and 
ceased to do so. The plate of glass was then removed, but 
the pike would not attack these particular fishes, though he 
would devour others which were afterward introduced; so 
strongly was the idea of a violent shock associated in his 
feeble mind with the attempt on his former neighbors. If 
a savage, who had never seen a large plate-glass window, 
were to dash himself even once against it, he would for a 
long time afterward associate a shock with a window-frame; 
but, very differently from the pike, he would probably re- 
flect on the nature of the impediment, and be cautious under 
analogous circumstances. Now with monkeys, as we shall 
presently see, a painful or merely a disagreeable impression, 
from an action once performed, is sometimes sufficient to 
prevent the animal from repeating it. If we attribute this 
difference between the monkey and the pike solely to the 
association of ideas being so much stronger and more per- 
sistent in the one than the other, though the pike often re- 
ceived much the more severe injury, can we maintain in the 
case of man thata similar difference implies the possession 
of a fundamentally different mind? 

Houzeau relates™ that, while crossing a wide and arid 
plain in Texas, his two dogs suffered greatly from thirst, 
and that between thirty and forty times they rushed down 
the hollows to search for water. These hollows were not 
valleys, and there were no trees in them, or any other differ- 
ence in the vegetation, and as they were absolutely dry there 


%3 *‘Die Bewegungen der Thiere,”’ etc., 1873, p. 11. 
% “‘Paculiés Mentales des Animaux,’’ 1872, tom. ii. p. 265, 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 109 


could have been no smell of dampearth. The dogs behaved 
as if they knew that a dip in the ground offered them the 
best chance of finding water, and Houzeau has often wit- 
nessed the same behavior in other animals. 

I have seen, as I dare say have others, that when a small 
object is thrown on the ground beyond the reach of one of 
the elephants in the Zoological Gardens, he blows through 
his trunk on the ground beyond the object, so that the cur- 
rent reflected on all sides may drive the object within his 
reach. Again, a well-known ethnologist, Mr. Westropp, 
informs me that he observed in Vienna a bear deliberately 
making with his paw a current in some water, which was 
close to the bars of his cage, so as to draw a piece of float- 
ing bread within his reach. These actions of the elephant 
and bear can hardly be attributed to instinct or inherited 
habit, as they would be of little use to an animal in a state 
of nature. Now, what is the difference between such ac- 
tions, when performed by an uncultivated man, and by one 
of the higher animals ? 

The savage and the dog have often found water at a low 
level, and the coincidence under such circumstances has 
become associated in their minds. A cultivated man would 
perhaps make some general proposition on the subject; but 
from all that we know of savages it is extremely doubtful 
whether they would do so, and a dog certainly would not. 
But a savage, as well as a dog, would search in the same 
way, though frequently disappointed; and in both it seems 
to be equally an act of reason, whether or not any general 
proposition on the subject is consciously placed before the 
mind.** The same would apply to the elephant and the bear 
making currents in the air or water. The savage would cer- 
tainly neither know nor care by what law the desired move- 
ments were effected; yet his act would be guided by a rude 


% Prof. Huxley has analyzed with admirable clearness the mental steps by 
which a man, as well as a dog, arrives at a conclusion in a case analogous 
to that given in my text. See his article, ‘‘Mr. Darwin’s Critics,’’ in the 
“Contemporary Review,”’? Nov. 1871, p. 462, and in his ‘‘Critiques and Es- 
says,’’ 1873, p. 279. 


110 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


process of reasoning, as surely as would a philosopher in his 
longest chain of deductions. There would no doubt be this 
difference between him and one of the higher animals, that 
he would take notice of much slighter circumstances and 
conditions, and would observe any connection between them 
after much less experience, and this would be of paramount 
importance. I kept a daily record of the actions of one of 
my infants, and when he was about eleven months old, and 
before he could speak a single word, I was continually 
struck with the greater quickness with which all sorts of 
objects and sounds were associated together in his mind, 
compared with that of the most intelligent dogs I ever 
knew. But the higher animals differ in exactly the same 
way in this power of association from those low in the scale, 
such as the pike, as well as in that of drawing inferences 
and of observation. 

The promptings of reason, after very short experience, 
are well shown by the following actions of American mon- 
keys, which stand low in their order. Rengger, a most 
careful observer, states that when he first gave eggs to 
_his monkeys in Paraguay, they smashed them and thus lost 
much of their contents; afterward they gently hit one end 
against some hard body, and picked off the bits of shell 
with their fingers. After cutting themselves only once with 
any sharp tool they would not touch it again, or would han- 
dle it with the greatest caution. Lumps of sugar were often 
given them wrapped up in paper; and Rengger sometimes 
put a live wasp in the paper, so that in hastily unfolding it 
they got stung; after this had once happened, they always 
first held the packet to their ears to detect any movement 
within.” 

The following cases relate to dogs. Mr. Colquhoun” 
winged two wild ducks, which fell on the further side of a 


26 Mr. Belt, in his most interesting work, ‘“‘The Naturalist in Nicaragua,”’ 
1874, p. 119, likewise describes various actions of a tamed Cebus, which, I 
think, clearly show that this animal possessed some reasoning power. 

27 6*The Moor and the Loch,” p. 45. Col. Hutchinson on ‘‘Dog Break- 
ing,’’ 1850, p. 46, 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN wn 


stream; his retriever tried to bring over both at once, but 
could not succeed; she then, though never before known 
to ruffle a feather, deliberately killed one, brought over the 
other, and returned for the dead bird. Col. Hutchinson 
relates that two partridges were shot at once, one being 
killed, the other wounded; the latter ran away, and was 
caught by the retriever, who on her return came across the 
dead bird; ‘‘she stopped, evidently greatly puzzled, and 
after one or two trials, finding she could not take it up 
without permitting the escape of the winged bird, she con- 
sidered a moment, then deliberately murdered it by giving 
it a severe crunch, and afterward brought away both to- 
gether. This was the only known instance of her ever 
having wilfully injured any game.’’ Here we have reason, 
though not quite perfect, for the retriever might have 
brought the wounded bird first and then returned for the 
dead one, as in the case of the two wild ducks. I give 
the above cases, as resting on the evidence of two independ- 
ent witnesses, and because in both instances the retrievers, 
after deliberation, broke through a habit which is inherited 
by them (that of not killing the game retrieved), and be- 
cause they show how strong their reasoning faculty must 
have been to overcome a fixed habit. 

I will conclude by quoting a remark by the illustrious 
Humboldt. ‘‘The muleteers in South America say, ‘IT will 
not give you the mule whose step is easiest, but la mas 
racional—the one that reasons best’’’; and, as he adds, 
‘‘this popular expression, dictated by long experience, com- 
bats the system of animated machines better perhaps than 
all the arguments of speculative philosophy.”’ Nevertheless 
some writers even yet deny that the higher animals possess 
a trace of reason; and they endeavor to explain away, by 
what appears to be mere verbiage,” all such facts as those 
above given. 


28 “*Pergonal Narrative,’’ Eng. translat., vol. iii, p. 106. 
2 Tam glad to find that so acute a reasoner as Mr. Leslie Stephen (‘‘Dar- 
winism and Divinity, Essays on Free-thinking,’’ 1873, p. 80), in speaking of 


i 


112 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


It has, 1 think, now been shown that man and the higher 
animals, especially the Primates, have some few instincts in 
common. All have the same senses, intuitions, and sensa- 
tions—similar passions, affections, and emotions, even the 
more complex ones, such as jealousy, suspicion, emulation, 
gratitude, and magnanimity; they practice deceit and are 
revengeful; they are sometimes susceptible to ridicule, and 
even havea sense of humor; they feel wonder and curiosity; 
they possess the same faculties of imitation, attention, de- 
liberation, choice, memory, imagination, the association of 
ideas, and reason, though in very different degrees. The 
individuals of the same species graduate in intellect from 
absolute imbecility to high excellence. They are also liable 
to insanity, though far less often than in the case of man.” 
Nevertheless, many authors have insisted that man is di- 
vided by an insuperable barrier from all the lower animals 
in his mental faculties. I formerly made a collection of 
above a score of such aphorisms, but they are almost worth- 
less, as their wide difference and number prove the difficulty, 
if not the impossibility of the attempt. It has been asserted 
that man alone is capable of progressive improvement; that 
he alone makes use of tools or fire, domesticates other ani- | 
mals, or possesses property; that no animal has the power 
of abstraction, or of forming general concepts, is self- 
conscious and comprehends itself; that no animal employs 
language; that man alone has a sense of beauty, is liable to 
caprice, has the feeling of gratitude, mystery, etc.; believes 
in God, or is endowed with a conscience. I will hazard a 
few remarks on the more important and interesting of 
these points. 


the supposed impassable barrier between the minds of man and the lower 
animals, says, *‘The distinctions, indeed, which have been drawn, seem to us 
to rest upon no better foundation than a great many other metaphysical distine- 
tions; that is, the assumption that because you can give two things different 
‘names, they must therefore have different natures. It is difficult to understand 
how anybody who has ever kept a dog or seen an elephant can have any doubts 
as to an animal’s power of performing the essential processes of reagoning.’’ 

30 See ‘‘Madness in Animals,’? by Dr. W. Lauder Lindsay, in ‘‘Journal of 
Mental Science,” July, 1871. : 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 113 


Archbishop Sumner formerly maintained” that man 
alone is capable of progressive improvement. That he is 
capable of incomparably greater and more rapid improve- 
ment than is any other animal, admits of no dispute; and 
this is mainly due to his power of speaking and handing 
down his acquired knowledge. With animals, looking first 
to the individual, every one who has had any experience in 
setting traps knows that young animals can be caught much 
more easily than old ones; and they can be much more- 
easily approached by an enemy. Even with respect to old 
animals, it is impossible to catch many in the same place 
and in the same kind of trap, or to destroy them by the 
same kind of poison; yet it is improbable that all should 
have partaken of the poison, and impossible that all 
should have been caught inatrap. They must learn cau- 
tion by seeing their brethren caught or poisoned. In North 
America, where the fur-bearing animals have long been pur- 
sued, they exhibit, according to the unanimous testimony 
of all observers, an almost incredible amount of sagacity, 
caution, and cunning; but trapping has been there so long 
carried. on, that inheritance may possibly have come into 
play. I have received several accounts that when telegraphs 
are first set up in any district, many birds kill themselves 
by flying against the wires, but that in the course of a very 
few years they learn to avoid this danger, by seeing, as it 
would appear, their comrades killed.” 

If we look to successive generations, or to the race, there 
is no doubt that birds and other animals gradually both ac- 
quire and lose caution in relation to man or other enemies ;* 
and this caution is certainly in chief part an inherited habit 
or instinct, but in part the result of individual experience. 


81 Quoted by Sir C. Lyell, ‘Antiquity of Man,’ p. 497. 

82 For additional evidence, with details, see M. Houzeau, ‘“‘Les Facultés 
Mentales,’’ tom. ii., 1872, p. 147. 

33 See, with respect to birds on oceanic islands, my ‘‘Journal of Researches 
during the Voyage of the Beagle,’ 1846, p. 398. ‘‘Origin of Species,”’ 5th 
edit., p. 260 


714 «THE DESCENT OF MAN 


A good observer, Leroy,” states that in districts where 
foxes are much hunted, the young, on first leaving their 
burrows, are incontestably much more wary than the old 
ones in districts where they are not much disturbed. ; 

Our domestic dogs are descended from wolves and jack- 
als,*° and, though they may not have gained in cunning, 
and may have lost in wariness and suspicion, yet they have 
progressed in certain moral qualities, such as in affection, 
-trustworthiness, temper, and probably in general intelli- 
gence. The common rat has conquered and beaten several 
other species throughout Europe, in parts of North America, 
New Zealand, and recently in Formosa, as well as on the 
mainland of China. Mr. Swinhoe,** who describes these 
two latter cases, attributes the victory of the common rat 
over the large Mus coninga to its superior cunning; and this 
latter quality may probably be attributed to the habitual 
exercise of all its faculties in avoiding extirpation by man, 
as well as to nearly all the less cunning or weak-minded rats 
having been continuously destroyed by him. It is, however, 
possible that the success of the common rat may be due to 
its having possessed greater cunning than its fellow-species 
before it became associated with man. To maintain, inde- 
pendently of any direct evidence, that no animal during the 
course of ages has progressed in intellect or other mental 
faculties, is to beg the question of the evolution of species. 
We have seen that, according to Lartet, existing mammals 
belonging to several orders have larger brains than their 
ancient tertiary prototypes. 

It has often been said that no animal uses any tool; but 
the chimpanzee ina state of nature cracks a native fruit, 
somewhat like a walnut, with a stone.” Rengger® easily 


4 “Tettres Phil. sur intelligence des Animaux,’’ nouvelle édit., 1802, p. 86. 

35 See the evidence on this head in chap. i. vol. i, ‘‘On the Variation of 
Animals and Plants under Domestication.”’ 

8 “Proc, Zoolog. Soc.,’? 1864, p. 186. 

31 Savage and Wyman in ‘‘Boston Journal of Nat. Hist.,”? vol. iv., 1843- 


44, p. 383, ; 
%8 “‘Saugethiere von Paraguay,’’ 1830, s. 51-56. . 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 115 


taught an American monkey thus to break open hard palm- 
nuts; and afterward, of its own accord, it used stones to 
open other kinds of nuts, as well as boxes. It thus also 
removed the soft rind of fruit that had a disagreeable flavor. 
Another monkey was taught to open the lid of a large box 
with a stick, and afterward it used the stick as a lever to 
move heavy bodies; and I have myself seen a young orang 
put a stick into a crevice, slip his hand to the other end, 
and use it in the proper manner as a lever. The famed 
elephants in India are well known to break off branches of 
trees and use them to drive away the flies; and this same 
act has been observed in an elephant in a state of nature.™ 
I have seen a young orang, when she thought she was going 
to be whipped, cover and protect herself with a blanket or 
straw. In these several. cases stones and sticks were em- 
ployed as implements; but they are likewise used as weap- 
ons. Brehm* states, on the authority of the well-known 
traveller, Schimper, that in Abyssinia when the baboons 
belonging to one species (@. gelada) descend in troops from 
the mountains to plunder the fields, they sometimes en- 
counter troops of another species (C. hamadryas), and then 
a fight ensues. The Geladas roll down great stones, which 
the Hamadryas try to avoid, and then both species, making 
a great uproar, rush furiously against each other. Brehm, 
when accompanying the Duke of Coburg-Gotha, aided in 
an attack with firearms on a troop of baboons, in the pass 
of Mensa in Abyssinia. The baboons in return rolled so 
many stones down the mountain, some as large as a man’s 
head, that the attackers had to beat a hasty retreat, and the 
pass was actually closed for a time against the caravan. It 
deserves notice that these baboons thus acted in concert. 
Mr. Wallace“ on three occasions saw female orangs, accom- 
panied by their young, ‘breaking off branches and the great 
spiny fruit of the Durian tree, with every appearance of 


4 The ‘Indian Field,’? March 4, 1841. 
4 “‘Thierleben,”’ B. i. s. 79, 82. 
41 “The Malay Archipelago,” vol. i., 1869, p. 8%. 


£16 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


rage; causing such a shower of missiles as effectually kept 
us from approaching too near the tree.’’ As I have re- 
peatedly seen, a chimpanzee will throw any object at hand 
at a person who offends him; and the before-mentioned 
baboon at the Cape of Good Hope prepared mud for the 
purpose. 

In the Zoological Gardens, a monkey, which had weak 
teeth, used to break open nuts with a stone; and I was 
assured by the keepers that after using the stone he hid it 
in the straw, and would not let any other monkey touch 
it. Here, then, we have the idea of property; but this idea 
is common to every dog with a bone, and to most or all 
birds with their nests. 

The Duke of Argyll remarks, that the fashioning of 
an implement for a special purpose is absolutely peculiar 
to man; and he considers that this forms an immeasurable 
gulf between him and the brutes. This is no doubt a very 
important distinction; but there appears to me much truth 
in Sir J. Lubbock’s suggestion,** that when primeval man 
first used flint-stones for any purpose, he would have acci- 
dentally splintered them, and would then have used the 
sharp fragments. From this step it would be a small one 
to break the flints on purpose, and not a very wide step to 
fashion them rudely. This latter advance, however, may 
have taken long ages, if we may judge by the immense 
interval of time which elapsed before the men of the neo- 
lithic period took to grinding and polishing their stone 
tools. In breaking the flints, as Sir J. Lubbock likewise 
remarks, sparks would have been emitted, and in grinding 
them heat would have been evolved; thus the two usual 
methods of ‘‘obtaining fire may have originated.’’ The 
nature of fire would have been known in the many volcanie 
regions where lava occasionally flows through forests. The 
anthropomorphous apes, guided probably by instinct, build 
for themselves temporary platforms; but as many instincts 


4 ‘Primeval Man,’’ 1869, pp. 145, 147. 
4# “Prehistoric Times,’’ 1865, p 473, ete, 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 117 


are largely controlled by reason, the simpler ones, such as 
this of building a platform, might readily pass into a volun- 
tary and conscious act. The orang is known to cover itself 
at night with the leaves of the Pandanus; and Brehm states 
that one of his baboons used to protect itself from the heat 
of the sun by throwing a straw-mat over its head. In these 
several habits we probably see the first steps toward some 
of the simpler arts, such as rude architecture and dress, as 
they arose among the early progenitors of man. 


Abstraction, General Conceptions, Self-consciousness, Mental 
Individuality.—It would be very difficult for any one with 
even much more knowledge than I possess, to determine 
how far animals exhibit any traces of these high mental 
powers. This difficulty arises from the impossibility of 
judging what passes through the mind of an animal; and 
again, the fact that writers differ to a great extent in the 
meaning which they attribute to the above terms causes a 
further difficulty. If one may judge from various articles 
which have been published lately, the greatest stress seems 
to be laid on the supposed entire absence in animals of the 
power of abstraction, or of forming general concepts. But 
when a dog sees another dog at a distance, it is often clear 
that he perceives that it is a dog in the abstract; for when 
he gets nearer his whole manner suddenly changes, if the 
other dog be a friend. A recent writer remarks, that in all 
such cases it is a pure assumption to assert that the mental 
act is not essentially of the same nature in the animal as in 
man. If either refers what he perceives with his senses 
to a mental concept then so do both.“* When TI say to my 
terrier, in an eager voice (and I have made the trial many 
times), ‘‘Hi, hi, where is it?’’ she at once takes it as a sign 
that something is to be hunted, and generally first looks 
quickly all around, and then rushes into the nearest thicket, 
to scent for any game, but finding nothing, she looks up into 


“4 Mr. Hookham, in a letter to Prof. Max Miiler, in the “Birmingham 
News,’ May, 1873. 


118 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


any neighboring tree for a squirrel. Now do not these ac- 
tions clearly show that she had in her mind a general idea 
or concept that some animal is to be discovered and hunted ? 

It may be freely admitted that no animal is self-conscious, 
if by this term it is implied that he reflects on such points as 
whence he comes or whither he will go, or what is life and 
death, and so forth. But how can we feel sure that an old 
dog with an excellent memory and some power of imagina- 
tion, as. shown by his dreams, never reflects on his past 
pleasures or pains in the chase? And this would be a 
form of self-consciousness. On the other hand, as Bitich- 
ner** has remarked, how little can the hard-worked wife of 
a degraded Australian savage, who uses very few abstract 
words, and cannot count above four, exert her self-con- 
sciousness, or reflect on the nature of her own existence. 
It is generally admitted that the higher animals possess 
memory, attention, association, and even some imagination 
and reason. If these powers, which differ much in different 
animals, are capable of improvement, there seems no great 
improbability in more complex faculties, such as the higher 
forms of abstraction, and self-consciousness, etc., having 
been evolved through the development and combination of 
the simpler ones. It has been urged against the views here 
maintained, that it is impossible to say at what point in the 
ascending scale animals become capable of abstraction, etc.; 
but who can say at what age this occurs in our young chil- 
dren? We see at least that such powers are developed in 
children by imperceptible degrees. 

That animals retain their mental individuality is unques- 
tionable. When my voice awakened a train of old associa- 
tions in the mind of the before-mentioned dog, he must have 
retained his mental individuality, although every atom of 
his brain had probably undergone change more than once 
during the interval of five years. This dog might have 
brought forward the argument lately advanced to crush all 


4° “‘Conférences sur la Théorie Darwinienne,”’ French translat., 1869, p. 132. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 119 


evolutionists, and said, ‘‘IT abide amid all mental moods and 
all material changes... . The teaching that atoms leave 
their impressions as legacies to other atoms falling into the 
places they have vacated is contradictory of the utterance of 
consciousness, and is therefore false; but it is the teaching 
necessitated by evolutionism, consequently the hypothesis 
is a false one.’’ * 


Language.—This faculty has justly been considered as 
one of the chief distinctions between man and the lower ani- 
mals. But man, as a highly competent judge, Archbishop 
Wakely remarks, is not the only animal that can make use 
of language to express what is passing in his mind, and can 
understand, more or less, what is so expressed by another.”’ ” 
In Paraguay the Cebus azare when excited utters at least six 
distinct sounds, which excite in other monkeys similar emo- 
tions.*° The movements of the features and gestures of 
monkeys are understood by us, and they partly understand 
ours, as Rengger and others declare. It is a more remark- 
able fact that the dog, since being domesticated, has learned 
to bark in at least four or five distinct tones. Although 
barking is a new art, no doubt the wild parent-species of the 
dog expressed their feelings by cries of various kinds. With 
the domesticated dog we have the bark of eagerness, as in 
the chase; that of anger, as well as growling; the yelp or 
howl of despair, as when shut up; the baying at night; the 
bark of joy, as when starting on a walk with his master; 
and the very distinct one of demand or supplication, as 
when wishing for a door or window to be opened. Accord- 
ing to Houzeau, who paid particular attention to the subject, 
the domestic fowl utters at least a dozen significant sounds.” 

The habitual use of articulate language is, however, 


4% The Rev. Dr. J. M’Cann, ‘‘Anti-Darwinism,’’ 1869, p. 13. 

41 Quoted in ‘“‘Anthropological Review,’’ 1864, p. 158. 

48 Rengger, ibid., s. 45. awe 

49 See my “‘Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,’* vol, 


. 27, 
Pia **Wacultés Mentales des Animaux,’’ tom ii., 1872, pp. 346-349, 


120 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


peculiar to man; but he uses, in common with the lower 
animals, inarticulate cries to express his meaning, aided by 
gestures and the movements of the muscles of the face.” 
This especially holds good with the more simple and vivid 
feelings, which are but little connected with our higher in- 
telligence. Our cries of pain, fear, surprise, anger, together 
with their appropriate actions, and the murmur of a mother 
to her beloved child, are more expressive than any words. 
That which distinguishes man from the lower animals is 
not the understanding of articulate sounds, for, as every one 
knows, dogs understand many words and sentences. In this 
respect they are at the same stage of development as infants 
between the ages of ten and twelve months, who understand 
many words and short sentences, but cannot yet utter a 
single word. It is not the mere articulation which is our 
distinguishing character, for parrots and other birds possess 
this power. Nor is it the mere capacity of connecting defi- 
nite sounds with definite ideas; for it is certain that some 
parrots which have been taught to speak connect unerringly 
words with things, and persons with events. The lower 
animals differ from man solely in his almost infinitely larger 
power of associating together the most diversified sounds 
and ideas; and this obviously depends on the high devel- 
opment of his mental powers. 

As Horne Took, one of the founders of the noble science 


51 See a discussion on this subject in Mr, E. B. Tylor’s very interesting 
work, ‘‘Researches into the Early History of Mankind,’’ 1865, chaps. ii. to iv. 

52 T have received several detailed accounts to this effect. Admiral Sir 
J. Sullivan, whom I know to be a careful observer, assures me that an African 
parrot, long kept in his father’s house, invariably called certain persons of the 
household, as well as visitors, by their names. He said ‘‘Good-morning”’ to 
every one at breakfast, and ‘“‘Good-night’’ to each as they left the room 
at night, and never reversed these salutations, To Sir J. Sullivan’s father, he 
used to add to the ‘‘Good-morning’”’ a short sentence, which was never once 
repeated after his father’s death. He scolded violently a strange dog which 
came into the room through the open window; and he scolded another parrot 
(saying ‘You naughty polly’’) which had got out of its cage, and was eating 
apples on the kitchen table. See also, to the same effect, Houzeau on, parrots, 
“Facultés Mentales,’’ tom. ii, p. 309. Dr. A. Moschkau informs me that he 
knew a starling which never made a mistake in saying in German ‘‘Good- 
morning” to persons arriving, and ‘‘Good-by, old fellow,” to those departing. 
I could add several other such cases. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 121 


of philology, observes, language is an art, like brewing or 
baking; but writing would have been a better simile. It 
certainly is not a true instinct, for every language has to 
be learned. It differs, however, widely from all ordinary 
arts, for man has an instinctive tendency to speak, as we 
see in the babble of our young children; while no child has 
an instinctive tendency to brew, bake, or write. Moreover, 
no philologist now supposes that any language has been de- 
liberately invented; it has been slowly and unconsciously 
developed by many steps.** KThe sounds uttered by birds 
offer in several respects the nearest analogy to language, for 
all the members of the same species utter the same instinc- 
tive cries expressive of their emotions; and all the kinds 
which sing, exert their power instinctively; but the actual 
song, and even the call-notes, are learned from their parents 
or foster-parents. These sounds, as Daines Barrington™ has 
proved, ‘‘are no more innate than language is in man.”’ 


The first attempts to sing ‘‘may be compared to the imper- 


fect endeavor in a child to-babble.”” The young males con- 
tinue practicing, or as the bird-catchers say, ‘‘recording,”’ 
for ten or eleven months. Their first essays show hardly 
a rudiment of the future song; but as they grow older we 
can perceive what they are aiming at; and at last they are 
said ‘‘to sing their song round.’’ WNestlings which have 
learned the song of a distinct species, as with the canary- 
birds educated in the Tyrol, teach and transmit their new 
song to their offspring. The slight natural differences of 
song in the same species inhabiting different districts may 
be appositely compared, as Barrington remarks, ‘‘to provin- 
cial dialects,’’ and the songs of allied though distinct species 


53 See some good remarks on this head by Prof. Whitney, in his ‘‘Oriental 
and Linguistic Studies,’’ 1873, p. 354. He observes that the desire of com- 
munication between man is the living force, which, in the development of 
language, ‘‘works both consciously and unconsciously; consciously as regards 
the immediate end to be attained; unconsciously as regards the further conse- 


quences of the act.”’ : 
54 Hon. Daines Barrington, in ‘‘Philosoph. Transactions,’’ 1773, p, 262, 


See also Dureau de la Malle, in ‘‘Ann. des Sc. Nat.,’’ 3d series, Zoolog., tom. 
x. p. 119. 
Descent—Vo. I.—6, 


122 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


may be compared with the languages of distinct races of man. 
I have given the foregoing details to show that an instinctive 
tendency to acquire an art is not peculiar to man. 

With respect to the origin of articulate language, after 
having read on the one side the highly interesting works of 
Mr. Hensleigh Wedgwood, the Rev. F. Farrar, and Prof. 
Schleicher,®* and the celebrated lectures of Prof. Max Miil- 
ler, on the other side, I cannot doubt that language owes its 
origin to the imitation and modification of various natural 
sounds, the voices of other animals, and man’s own instine- 
tive cries, aided by signs and gestures. When we treat of 
sexual selection we shall see that primeval man, or rather 
some early progenitor of man, probably first used his voice 
in producing true musical cadences, that is in singing, as 
do some of the gibbon-apes at the present day; and we may 
conclude from a widely spread analogy, that this power 
would have been especially exerted during the courtship 
of the sexes—would have expressed various emotions, such 
as love, jealousy, trlumph—and would have served as a 
challenge to rivals. It is, therefore, probable that the imi- 
tation of musical cries by articulate sounds may have given 
rise to words expressive of various complex emotions. The 
strong tendency in our nearest allies, the monkeys, in micro- 
cephalous idiots,** and in the barbarous races of mankind, 
to imitate whatever they hear, deserves notice as bearing on 
the subject of imitation. Since monkeys certainly under- 
stand much that is said to them by man, and’ when wild 
utter signal cries of danger to their fellows,” and since 
fowls give distinct warnings for danger on the ground, or 


5 “On the Origin of Language,”” by H. Wedgwood, 1866. ‘‘Chapters on 
Language,’ by the Rev. F. W. Farrar, 1865. These works are most interest- 
ing. See also “‘De Ja Phys. et de Parole,’’ par Albert Lemoine, 1865, p. 190. 
The work on this subject, by the late Prof. Aug. Schleicher, has been trans- 
lated by Dr. Bikkers into English, under the title of ‘‘Darwinism tested by the 
Science of Language,’ 1869. 

56 Vogt, ‘‘Mémoire sur les Microcéphales,’? 1867, p. 169. With respect to 
savages, I have given some facts, in my ‘‘Journal of Researches,” ete., 1845, 

. 206. 
Pon See clear evidence on this head in the two works so often quoted by 
Brehm and Rengger. 


ee ere 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 123 


in the sky, from hawks (both, as well as a third ery, intel- 
ligible to dogs), may not some unusually wise ape-like 
animal have imitated the growl of a beast of prey, and thus 
iold his fellow-monkeys the nature of the expected dan- 
ger? This would have been a first step in the formation 
of a language. 

As the voice was used more and more, the vocal organs. 
would have been strengthened and perfected through the 
principle of the inherited effects of use; and this would 
have reacted on the power of speech. But the relation be- 
tween the continued use of language and the development 
of the brain has no doubt been far more important. The 
mental powers in some early progenitor of man must have 
been more highly developed than in any existing ape, before 
even the most imperfect form of speech could have come 
into use; but we may confidently believe that the continued 
use and advancement of this power would have reacted on 
the mind itself, by enabling and encouraging it to carry 
on long trains of thought. A complex train of thought 
can no more be carried on without the aid of words, 
whether spoken or silent, than a long calculation without 
the use of figures or algebra. It appears, also, that even 
an ordinary train of thought almost requires, or is greatly 
facilitated by, some form of language, for the dumb, deaf, 
and blind girl, Laura Bridgman, was observed to use her - 
fingers while dreaming.” Nevertheless, a long succession 
of vivid and connected ideas may pass through the mind 
without the aid of any form of language, as we may infer 
from the movements of dogs during their dreams. We 
have, also, seen that animals are able to reason to a cer- 
tain extent, manifestly without the aid of language. The 
intimate connection between the brain, as it is now devel- 
oped in us, and the faculty of speech, is well shown by 


58 Houzeau gives a very curious account of his observations on this subject 
in his ‘‘Facultés Mentales des Animaux,’’ tom, ii. p. 348. 

59 See remarks on this head by Dr. Maudsley, ‘‘The Physiology and Pathol- 
ogy of Mind,’’ 2d edit., 1868, p. 199. 


124 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


those curious cases of brain disease in which speech is spe- 
cially affected, as when the power to remember substantives 
is lost, while other words can be correctly used, or where 
substantives of a certain class, or all except the initial letters” 
of substantives and proper names, are forgotten.” There is 
no more improbability in the continued use of the mental 
and vocal organs leading to inherited changes in their struc- 
ture and functions, than in the case of handwriting, which 
depends partly on the form of the hand and partly on 
the disposition of the mind; and handwriting is certainly 
inherited.” ‘ 
Several writers, more especially Prof. Max Miiller,” hav 

lately insisted that the use of language implies the power of 
forming general concepts; and that as no animals are sup- 
posed to possess this power, an impossible barrier is formed 
between them and man. With respect to animals, I have 
already endeavored to show that they have this power, at 
least in a rude and incipient degree. As far as concerns 
infants of from ten to eleven months old, and deaf-mutes, 
it seems to me incredible that they should be able to con- 
nect certain sounds with certain general ideas as quickly as 
they do, unless such ideas were already formed in their 


60 Many curious cases have been recorded. See, for instance, Dr. Bateman 
“On Aphasia,’? 1870, pp. 27, 31, 53, 100, ete. Also, ‘Inquiries Concerning 
the Intellectual Powers,’? by Dr. Abercrombie, 1838, p. 150. 

6 “The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,’’ vol. ii. p. 6. 

6 Lectures on ‘‘Mr. Darwin’s Philosophy of Language,’’ 1873. 

88 The judgment of a distinguished philologist, such as Prof. Whitney, will 
have far more weight on this point than anything that I can say. He remarks 
(‘Oriental and Linguistic Studies,’’ 1873, p. 297), in speaking of Bleek’s views: 
**Because on the grand scale language is the necessary auxiliary of thought, 
indispensable to the development of the power of thinking, to the distinctness 
and variety and complexity of cognitions, to the full mastery of consciousness; 
therefore he would fain make thought absolutely impossible without speech, 
identifying the faculty with its instrument. He might just as reasonably assert 
that the human hand cannot act without a tool. With such a doctrine to start 
from, he cannot stop short of Miller’s worst paradoxes, that an infant (in fans, 
not speaking) is not a human being, and that deaf-mutes do not become pos- 
sessed of reason until they learn to twist their fingers into imitation of spoken . 
words’? Max Miller gives in italics (‘‘Lectures on Mr, Darwin’s Philosophy 
of Languages,’’ 1873, third lecture) the following aphorism: ‘‘There is no 
thought without words, as little as there are words without thought,’? 
What a strange definition must here be given to the word thought! 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 125 


minds. The same remark may be extended to the more 
intelligent animals; as Mr. Leslie Stephen observes, ‘A 
dog frames a general concept of cats or sheep, and knows 
the corresponding words as well as a philesopher. And 
the capacity to understand is as good a proof of vocal in- 
telligence, though in an inferior degree, as the capacity to 
speak.’’ ; 

Why the organs now used for speech should have been 
originally perfected for this purpose, rather than any other 
organs, it is not difficult to see. Ants have considerable 
powers of intercommunication by means of their antenna, 
as shown by Huber, who devotes a whole chapter to their 
language. We might have used our fingers as efficient in- 
struments, for a person with practice can report to a deaf 
man every word of a speech rapidly delivered at a public 
meeting; but the loss of our hands, while thus employed, 
would have been a serious inconvenience. As all the higher 
mammals possess vocal organs, constructed on the same gen- 
eral plan as ours, and used as a means of communication, 
it was obviously probable that these same organs would be 
still further developed if the power of communication had 
to be improved; and this has been effected by the aid of 
adjoining and well adapted parts, namely, the tongue and 
lips.*° The fact of the higher apes not using their vocal 
organs for speech, no doubt depends on their intelligence 
not having been sufficiently advanced. The possession by 
them of organs, which with long-continued practice might 
have been used for speech, although not thus used, is paral- 
leled by the case of many birds which possess organs fitted 
for singing, though they never sing. Thus, the nightingale 
and crow have vocal organs similarly constructed, these 
being used by the former for diversified song, and by the 
latter only for croaking.* If it be asked why apes have not 


6 ‘*Hssays on Free-thinking,’’ etc., 1873, p. 82. : 

65 See some good remarks to this effect by Dr. Maudsley, ‘“‘The Physiology 
and Pathology of Mind,’’ 1868, p. 199. 

86 Macgillivray, ‘‘Hist. of British Birds,”’ vol. ii., 1839, p. 29. An excellent 
observer, Mr. Blackwall, remarks that the magpie learns to pronounce single 


126 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


had their intellects developed to the same degree as that of 
man, general causes only can be assigned in answer, and it 
is unreasonable to expect anything more definite, consider- 
ing our ignorance with respect to the successive stages of 
development through which each creature has passed. 

The formation of different languages and of distinct spe- 
cies, and the proofs that both have been developed through 
a gradual process, are curiously parallel.” But we can trace 
the formation of many words further back than that of spe- 
cies, for we can perceive how they actually arose from the 
imitation of various sounds. We find in distinct languages 
striking homologies due to community of descent, and analo- 
gies due to a similar process of formation. The manner in 
which certain letters or sounds change when others change 
is very like correlated growth. We have in both cases the 
reduplication of parts, the effects of long-continued use, and 
so forth. The frequent presence of rudiments, both in lan- 
guages and in species, is still more remarkable. The letter 
m in the word am, means J; so that in the expression J am, 
a superfluous and useless rudiment has been retained. In 
the spelling also of words, letters often remain as the rudi- 
ments of ancient forms of pronunciation. Languages, like 
organic beings, can be classed in groups under groups; and 
they can be classed either naturally, according to descent, 
or artificially by other characters. Dominant languages and 
dialects spread widely, and lead to the gradual extinction 
of other tongues. A language, like-a species, when once 
extinct, never, as Sir C. Lyell remarks, reappears. The 
same language never has two birthplaces. Distinct lan- 
guages may be crossed or blended together. We see vari- 


words, and even short sentences, more readily than almost any other British 
bird; yet, as he adds, after long and closely investigating its habits, he has 
never known it in a state of nature, display any unusual capacity for imitation. 
‘*Researches in Zoology,’’ 1834, p. 158. 

61 See the very interesting parallelism between the development of species 
and languages, given by Sir C, Lyell in ‘‘The Geolog. Evidences of the Antiquity 
of Man,’’ 1863, chap. xxiii. 

88 See remarks to this effect by the Rev. F. W. Farrar, in an interesting arti- 
ele entitled ‘‘Philology and Darwinism,’’ in ‘‘Nature,’’ March 24, 1870, p, 528, 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 127 


ability in every tongue, and new words are continually 
cropping up; but as there is a limit to the powers of the 
memory, single words, like whole languages, gradually be- 
come extinct. As Max Miiller® has well remarked: “A 
struggle for life is constantly going on among the words 
and grammatical forms in each language. The better, the 
shorter, the easier forms are constantly gaining the upper 
hand, and they owe their success to their own inherent 
virtue.’ To these more important causes of the survival 
of certain words, mere novelty and fashion may be added; 
for there is in the mind of man a strong love for slight 
changes in all things. The survival or preservation of cer- 
tain favored words. in the struggle for existence is natural 
selection. 

The perfectly regular and wonderfully complex construc- 
tion of the languages of many barbarous nations has often 
been advanced as a proof, either of the divine origin of these 
languages or of the high art and former civilization of their 
founders. Thus F. von Schlegel writes: ‘‘In those lan- 
guages which appear to be at the lowest grade of intellec- 
tual culture, we frequently observe a very high and elabo- 
rate degree of art in their grammatical structure. This is 
especially the case with the Basque and the Lapponian, and 
many of the American languages.’’” But it is assuredly 
an error to speak of any language as an art, in the sense 
of its having been elaborately and methodically formed. 
Philologists now admit that conjugations, declensions, etc., 
originally existed as distinct words, since joined together; 
and as such words express the most obvious relations be- 
tween objects and persons, it is not surprising that they 
should have been used by the men of most races during 
the earliest ages. With respect to perfection, the following 
illustration will best show how easily we may err: A Crinoid 
sometimes consists of no less than 150,000 pieces of shell,” 


6 “*Nature,’’? Jan. 6, 1870, p. 257. 
Quoted by C. 8. Wake, ‘‘Chapters on Man,” 1868, p, 101, 
1 Buckland, ‘‘Bridgewater Treatise,”’ p. 411, 


128 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


all arranged with perfect symmetry in radiating lines; but a 
naturalist does not consider an animal of this kind as more 
perfect than a bilateral one with comparatively few parts, 
and with none of these parts alike, excepting on the oppo- 
site sides of the body. He justly considers the differentia- 
tion and specialization of organs as the test of perfection. 
So with languages; the most symmetrical and complex 
ought not to be ranked above irregular, abbreviated, and 
bastardized languages, which have borrowed expressive 
words and useful forms of construction from various con- 
quering, conquered, or immigrant races. 

From these few and imperfect remarks I conclude that 
the extremely complex and regular construction of many 
barbarous languages is no proof that they owe their origin 
to a special act of creation.” Nor, as we have seen, does 
the faculty of articulate speech in itself offer any insuper- 
able objection. to the belief that man has been developed 
from some lower form. 


Sense of Beauty. —This sense has been declared to be 
peculiar to man. I refer here only to the pleasure given 
by certain colors, forms, and sounds, and which may fairly 
be called a sense of the beautiful; with cultivated men such 
sensations are, however, intimately associated with complex 
ideas and trains of thought. When we behold a male bird 
elaborately displaying his graceful plumes or splendid colors 
before the female, while other birds, not thus decorated, 
make no such display, it is impossible to doubt that she 
admires the beauty of her male partner. As women every- 
where deck themselves with these plumes, the beauty. of 
such ornaments cannot be disputed. As we shall see later, 
the nests of humming-birds and the playing passages of 
bower-birds are tastefully ornamented with gayly colored 
objects; and this shows that they must receive some kind 
of pleasure from the sight of such things. With the great 


® See some good remarks on the simplification of languages, by Sir J. Lub- 
bock, ‘‘Origin of Civilization,’’? 1870, p. 278. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 129 


majority of animals, however, the taste for the beautiful is 
confined, as far as we can judge, to the attractions of the 
opposite sex. The sweet strains poured forth by many male 
birds during the season of love are certainly admired by the 
females, of which fact evidence will hereafter be given. If 
female birds had been incapable of appreciating the beauti- 
ful colors, the ornaments and voices of their male partners, 
all the labor and anxiety exhibited by the latter in display- 
ing their charms before the females would have been thrown 
away; and this it is impossible to admit. Why certain 
bright colors should excite pleasure cannot, I presume, be 
explained, any more than why certain flavors and scents are 
agreeable; but habit has something to do with the result, 
for that which is at first unpleasant to our senses ultimately 
becomes pleasant, and habits are inherited. With respect 
to sounds, Helmholtz has explained, to a certain extent, on 
physiological principles, why harmonies and certain cadences 
areagreeable. But besides this, sounds frequently recurring 
at irregular intervals are highly disagreeable, as every one 
will admit who has listened at night to the irregular flap- 
ping of arope on board ship. The same principle seems to 
come into play with vision, as the eye prefers symmetry 
or figures with some regular recurrence. Patterns of this 
kind are employed by even the lowest savages as orna- 
ments; and they have been developed through sexual se- 
lection for the adornment of some male animals. Whether 
we can or not give any reason for the pleasure thus derived 
from vision and hearing, yet man and many of the lower 
animals are alike pleased by the same colors, graceful 
shading and forms, and the same sounds. 

The taste for the beautiful, at least as far as female 
beauty is concerned, is not of a special nature in the human 
mind; for it differs widely in the different races of man, and 
is not quite the same even in the different nations of the same 
race. Judging from the hideous ornaments and the equally 
hideous music admired by most savages, it might be urged 
that their xsthetic faculty was not so highly developed as 


180 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


in certain animals, for instance, as in birds. Obviously no 
animal would be capable of admiring such scenes as the 
heavens at night, a beautiful landscape, or refined music; 
but such high tastes are acquired through culture, and 
depend on complex associations; they are not enjoyed by 
barbarians or by uneducated persons. 

Many of the faculties which have been of inestimable 
service to man for his progressive advancement, such as the 
powers of the imagination, wonder, curiosity, an undefined 
sense of beauty, a tendency to imitation, and the love of 
excitement or novelty, could hardly fail to lead to ca- 
pricious changes of customs and fashions. I have alluded 
to this point, because a recent writer” has oddly fixed on 
Caprice ‘‘as one of the most remarkable and typical differ- 
ences between savages and brutes.’’ But not only can we 
partially understand how it is that man is from various con- 
flicting influences rendered capricious, but that the lower 
animals are, as we shall hereafter see, likewise capricious 
in their affections, aversions, and sense of beauty. There 
is also reason to suspect that they love novelty for its 
own sake. 


Belief in ee Raia There is no evidence that man 
was aboriginally endowed with the ennobling belief in the 
existence of an Omnipotent God. On the contrary, there is 
ample evidence, derived not from hasty travellers, but from 
men who have long resided with savages, that numerous 
races have existed, and still exist, who have no idea of one 
or more gods, and who have no words in their languages to 
express such an idea.“ The question is of course wholly 
distinct from that higher one, whether there exists a Creator 
and Ruler of the universe; and this has been answered in 


73 “The Spectator,’’ Dec. 4, 1869, p. 1430. 

4 See an excellent article on this subject by the Rev. F. W. Farrar, in the 
“Anthropological Review,’’ Aug. 1864, p. ecxvii. For further facts see Sir J. 
Lubbock, ‘‘Prehistoric Times,’ 2d edit., 1869, p. 564; and especially the chap- 
ters on Religion in his “Origin of Civilization,” 1870. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN | 131 


the affirmative by some of the highest intellects that have 
ever existed. ¥ 

If, however, we include under the term ‘‘religion’’ the 
belief in unseen or spiritual agencies, the case is wholly 
different; for this belief seems to be universal with the 
less civilized races. Nor is it difficult to comprehend how 
it arose. As soon as the important faculties of the imag- 
ination, wonder, and curiosity, together with some power 
of reasoning, had become partially developed, man would 
naturally crave to understand what was passing around 
him, and would have vaguely speculated on his own exist- 
ence. As Mr. M‘Lennan’ has remarked, ‘‘Some explana- 
tion of the phenomena of life, a man must feign for him- 
self; and to judge from the universality of it, the simplest 
hypothesis, and the first to occur to men, seems to have 
been that natural phenomena are ascribable to the presence 
in animals, plants and things, and in the forces of nature, of 
such spirits prompting to action as men are conscious they 
themselves possess.’’ It is also probable, as Mr. Tylor has 
shown, that dreams may have first given rise to the notion 
of spirits; for savages do not readily distinguish between 
subjective and objective impressions. When a savage 
dreams, the figures which appear before him are believed 
to have come from a distance, and to stand over him; or 
‘tthe soul of the dreamer goes out on its travels, and comes 
home with a remembrance of what it has seen.’’ ° But until 


7 “‘The Worship of Animals and Plants,’ in the ‘‘Fortnightly Review,” 
Oct. 1, 1869, p. 422. 

76 Tylor, ‘‘Early History of Mankind,’’ 1865, p. 6. See also the three strik- 
ing chapters on the Development of Religion, in Lubbock’s ‘‘Origin of Civiliza- 
tion,’? 1870. In a like manner, Mr. Herbert Spencer, in his ingenious essay in 
the ‘‘Fortnightly Review’? (May 1, 1870, p. 535), accounts for the earliest forms 
of religious belief throughout the world, by man being led, through dreams, 
shadows, and other causes, to look at himself as a double essence, corporeal 
and spiritual. As the spiritual being is supposed to exist after death and to be 
powerful, it is propitiated by various gifts and ceremonies, and its aid invoked. 
He then further shows that_names or nicknames given from some animal or 
other objett to the early progenitors or founders of a tribe are supposed after 
a long interval to represent the real progenitor of the tribe; and such animal or 
object is then naturally believed still to exist as a spirit, is held sacred, and 
worshipped as a god. Nevertheless, I cannot but suspect that there is a still 


132 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


the faculties of imagination, curiosity, reason, etc., had been 
fairly well developed in the mind of man, his dreams would 
not have led him to believe in spirits, any more than in the 
case of a dog. 

X¥ The tendency in savages to imagine that natural objects 
and agencies are animated by spiritual or living essences, is 
perhaps illustrated by a little fact which I once noticed: my 
dog, a full-grown and very sensible animal, was lying on the 
lawn during a hot and still day; but at a little distance a 
slight breeze occasionally moved an open parasol, which 
would have been wholly disregarded by the dog, had any 
one stood near it. As it was, every time that the parasol 
slightly moved, the dog growled fiercely and barked. He 
must, I think, have reasoned to himself, in a rapid and 
unconscious manner, that movement without any apparent 
cause indicated the presence of some strange living agent, 
and that no stranger had a right to be on his territory. * 

The belief in spiritual agencies would easily pass into 
the belief in the existence of one or more gods. For say- 
ages would naturally attribute to spirits the same passions, 
the same love of vengeance or simplest form of justice, and 
the same affections which they themselves feel. The Fue- 
gians appear to be, in this respect, in an intermediate con- 
dition, for when the surgeon on board the ‘‘Beagle’’ shot 
some young ducklings as specimens, York Minster declared 
in the most solemn manner, ‘‘Oh, Mr. Bynoe, much rain, 
much snow, blow much”; and this was evidently a retribu- 
tive punishment for wasting human food. So again he re- 
lated how, when his brother killed a ‘‘wild man,’’ storms 
long raged, much rain and snow fell. Yet we could never 
discover that the Fuegians believed in what we should call 
aGod, or practiced any religious rites; and Jemmy Button, 
with justifiable pride, stoutly maintained that there was no 
devil in his land. This latter assertion is the more remark- 


earlier and ruder stage, when anything which manifests power or movement is 
thought to be endowed with some form of life, and with mental faculties analo- 
gous to our own. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 133 


able, as with savages the belief in bad spirits is far more 
common than that in good ones. 

The feeling of religious devotion is a highly complex 
one, consisting of love, complete submission to an exalted 
and mysterious superior, a strong sense of dependence,’ 
fear, reverence, gratitude, hope for the future, and perhaps 
other elements. No being could éxperience so complex an 
emotion until advanced in his intellectual and moral facul- 
ties to at least a moderately high level. Nevertheless, we 
see some distant approach to this state of mind in the deep 
love of a dog for his master, associated with complete sub- 
mission, some fear, and perhaps other feelings. The behay- 
ior of a dog when returning to his master after an absence, 
and, as I may add, of a monkey to his beloved keeper, is 
widely different from that toward their fellows. In the lat- 
ter case the transports of joy appear to be somewhat less, 
and the sense of equality is shown in every action. Prof. 
Brauback goes so far as to maintain that a dog looks on his 
master as on a god.” 

The same high mental faculties which first led man 
to believe in unseen spiritual agencies, then in fetichism, 
polytheism, and ultimately in monotheism, would infallibly 
lead him, as long as his reasoning powers remained poorly 
developed, to various strange superstitions and .customs. 
Many of these are terrible to think of—such as the sacrifice 
of human beings to a blood-loving god; the trial of innocent 
persons by the ordeal of poison or fire; witchcraft, etc.—yet 
it is well occasionally to reflect on these superstitions, for 
they show us what an infinite debt of gratitude we owe to 
the improvement of our reason, to science, and to our accumu- 
lated knowledge. As Sir J. Lubbock” has well observed, 


™ See an able article on the ‘‘Physical Elements of Religion,’’ by Mr. L. 
Owen Pike, in ‘‘Anthropolog, Review,’’ April, 1870, p. lxiii.’ 

7 “Religion, Moral, etc., der Darwin’schen Art-Lehre,’’ 1869, s. 53. Itis 
said (Dr. W. Lauder Lindsay, “Journal of Mental Science,” 1871, p. 43) that 
Bacon long ago, and the poet Burns, held the same notion. 

79 ‘Prehistoric Times,’ 2d edit., p. 571. In this work (p. 571) there will 
be found an excellent account of the many strange and capricious customs of 
savages, 

1 
¥ 


134 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


“it is not too much to say that the horrible dread of unknown 
evil hangs like a thick cloud over savage life, and imbitters 
every pleasure.’’ These miserable and indirect consequences 
of our highest faculties may be compared with the incidental 
and occasional mistakes of the instincts of the lower animals. 


CHAPTER IV 


COMPARISON OF THE MENTAL POWERS OF MAN AND THE 
LOWER ANIMALS— Continued 


The moral sense—Fundamental proposition—The qualities of social animals 
—Origin of sociability—Struggle between opposed instincts—Man a 
social animal—The more enduring social instincts conquer other less 
persistent instincts—The social virtues alone regarded by savages—The 
self-regarding virtues acquired at a later stage of development—The 
importance of the judgment of the members of the same community 
on conduct—Transmission of moral tendencies—Summary 


maintain that of all the differences between man and the 

lower animals, the moral sense or conscience is by far 
the most important. This sense, as Mackintosh? remarks, 
“has a rightful supremacy over every other principle of 
~ human action’; it is summed up in that short but imperi- 
ous word ought, so full of high significance. It is the most 
noble of all the attributes of man, leading him without a 
moment’s hesitation to risk his life for that of a fellow- 
creature; or after due deliberation, impelled simply by 
the deep feeling of right or duty, to sacrifice it in some 
greatcause. Immanuel Kant exclaims, ‘‘Duty! Wondrous 
thought, that workest neither by fond insinuation, flattery, 
nor by any threat, but merely by holding up thy naked law 
in the soul, and so extorting for thyself always reverence, if 


| FULLY subscribe to the judgment of those writers' who 


1 See, for instance, on this subject, Quatrefages, “Unité de l’Espéce Hu- 
mauine,’’ 1861, p. 21, ete. 
2 “Dissertation on Ethical Philosophy,’’ 1837, p. 231, ete. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 135 


not always obedience; before whom all appetites are dumb, 
however secretly they rebel; whence thy original ?”’ * 

This great question has been discussed by many writers* 
of consummate ability; and my sole excuse for touching on 
it is the impossibility of here passing it over; and because, 
as far as I know, no one has approached it exclusively from 
the side of natural history. The investigation possesses, 
also, some independent interest, as an attempt to see how 
far the study of the lower animals throws light on one of 
the highest psychical faculties of man. 

The following proposition seems to me in a high degree 
probable—namely, that any animal whatever, endowed with 
well marked social instincts,® the parental and filial affec- 
tions being here included, would inevitably acquire a moral 
sense or conscience, as soon as its intellectual powers had 
become as well, or nearly as well, developed as in man. 
For, first, the social instincts lead an animal to take pleas- 
ure in the society of its fellows, to feel a certain amount of 


8 ‘‘Metaphysics of Ethics,” translated by J. W. Semple, Edinburgh, 1836, 
. 136, 5 
ae Mr. Bain gives a list (‘‘Mental and Moral Science,’’ 1868, pp. 543-725) 
of twenty-six British authors who have written on this subject, and whose 
names are familiar to every reader; to these, Mr, Bain’s own name, and those 
of Mr. Lecky, Mr. Shadworth Hodgson, Sir J. Lubbock, and others, might be 
added. 

5 Sir B. Brodie, after observing that man is a social animal (‘‘Psychological 
Inquiries,’’ 1854, p. 192), asks the pregnant question, ‘‘Ought not this to settle 
the disputed question as to the existence of a moral sense?’’ Similar ideas 
have probably occurred to many persons, as they did long ago to Marcus Au- 
relius. Mr. J. S. Mill speaks, in his celebrated work, ‘‘Utilitarianism’’ (1864, 
pp. 45, 46), of the social feelings as a “‘powerful natural sentiment,’’ and as 
“the natural basis of sentiment for utilitarian morality.”? Again he says, ‘‘Like 
the other acquired capacities above referred to, the moral faculty, if not a part 
of our nature, is a natural outgrowth from it; capabie, like them, in a certain 
small degree, of springing up spontaneously.’? But in opposition to all this, he 
also remarks, ‘‘if, as is my own belief, the moral feelings are not innate, but 
acquired, they are not for that reason less natural.’ It is with hesitation that 
I venture to differ at all from se profound a thinker, but it can hardly be dis- 
puted that the social feelings are instinctive or innate in the lower animals; and 
why should they not be so in man? Mr. Bain (see, for instance, “‘The Emo- 
tions and the Will,”’? 1865, p. 481) and others believe that the moral sense is 
acquired by each individual during his lifetime. On the general theory of evo- 
lution, this is at least extremely improbable. The ignoring of all transmitted 
mental qualities will, as it seems to me, be hereafter judged as a most serious 
blemish in the works of Mr. Mill. 


136 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


sympathy with them, and to perform various services for 
them. The services may be of a definite and evidently in- 
stinctive nature, or there may be only a wish and readiness, 
as with most of the higher social animals, to aid their fellows 
in certain general ways. But these feelings and services are 
by no means extended to all the individuals of the same 
species, only to those of the same association. Secondly, as 
soon as the mental faculties had become highly developed, 
images of all past actions and motives would be incessantly 
passing through the brain of each individual; and that feel- 
ing of dissatisfaction, or even misery, which invariably re- 
sults, as we shall hereafter see, from any unsatisfied instinct, 
would arise, as often as it was perceived that the enduring 
and always present social instinct had yielded to some other 
instinct, at the time stronger, but neither enduring in its 
nature nor leaving behind it a very vivid impression. It 
is clear that many instinctive desires, such as that of hun- 
ger, are in their nature of short duration; and after being 
satisfied, are not readily or vividly recalled. Thirdly, after 
the power of language had been acquired, and the wishes 
of the community could be expressed, the common opinion 
how each member ought to a for the public good would 
naturally become in a paramount degree the guide to action. 
~ But it should be borne in mind that, however great weight 
we may attribute to public opinion, our regard for the ap- 
probation and disapprobation of our fellows depends on 
sympathy, which, as we shall see, forms an essential par’. 
of the social instinct, and is indeed its foundation-stont. 
_ Lastly, habit, in the individual, would ultimately play a 
very important part in guiding the conduct of each mem- 
ber; for the social instinct, together with sympathy, is, like 
any other instinct, greatly strengthened by habit, and so 
consequently would be obedience to the wishes and judg- 
ment of the community. These several subordinate propo- 
sitions must now be discussed, and some of them at con- 

siderable length. 
It may be well first to premise that I do not wish to 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 187 


maintain that any strictly social animal, if its intellectual 
faculties were to become as active and as highly developed 
as in man, would acquire exactly the same moral sense as 
ours. In the same manner as various animals have some 
sense of beauty, though they admire widely different ob- 
jects, so they might have a sense of right and wrong, 
though led by it to follow widely different lines of con- 
duct. If, for instance, to take an extreme case, men were 
reared under precisely the same conditions as hive-bees, 
there can hardly be a doubt that our unmarried females 
would, like the worker-bees, think it a sacred duty to kill 
their brothers, and mothers would sirive to kill their fertile 
daughters; and no one would think of interfering.*° Never- 
theless, the bee, or any other social animal, would gain in 
our supposed case, as it appears to me, some feeling of right 
or wrong, or a conscience. For each individual would have 
an inward sense of possessing certain stronger or more en- 
during instincts, and others less strong or enduring; so that 
there would often be a struggle as to which impulse should 
be followed; and satisfaction, dissatisfaction, or even misery 
would be felt, as past impressions were compared during 
their incessant passage through the mind. In this case an 
inward monitor would tell the animal that it would have 
been better to have followed the one impulse rather than 
the other. The one course ought to have been followed, 


6 Mr. H. Sidgwick remarks, in an able discussion on this subject (the 
**Academy,”” June 15, 1872, p. 231), ‘‘a superior bee, we may feel sure, would 
aspire to a milder solution of the population question.’? Judging, however, 
from the habits of many or most savages, man solves the problem by female 
infanticide, polyandry, and promiscuous intercourse; therefore it may well be 
doubted whether it would be by a milder method. Miss Cobbe, in commenting 
(‘Darwinism in Morals,*? **Theological Review,”’ April, 1872, pp. 188-191) on 
the same illustration, says, the principles of social duty would be thus reversed ; 
and by this, I presume, she means that the fulfilment of a social duty would tend 
to the injury of individuals; but she overlooks the fact, which she would doubt- 
less admit, that the instincts of the bee have been acquired for the good of the 
community. She goes so far as to say that if the theory of ethics advocated in 
this chapter were ever generally accepted, ‘I cannot but believe that in the hour 
of their triumph would be sounded the knell of the virtue of mankind!’ It is 
to be hoped that the belief in the permanence of virtue on this earth is not held 
by many persons on so weak a tenure. 


188 YHE DESCENT OF MAN 


and the other ought not; the one would have been right, and 
the other wrong; but to these terms I shall recur. 


Sociability— Animals of many kinds are social; we find 
even distinct species living together; for example, some 
American monkeys; and united flocks of rooks, jackdaws, 
and starlings. Man shows the same feeling in his strong 
Jove for the dog, which the dog returns with interest. 
Every one must have noticed how miserable horses, dogs, 
sheep, etc., are when separated from their companions, 
and what strong mutual affection the two former kinds, at 
least, show on their reunion. It is curious to speculate 
on the feelings of a dog, who will rest peacefully for hours 
in a room with his master or any of the family, without the 
least notice being taken of him; but if left for a short time 
by himself, barks or howls dismally. We will confine our 
attention to the higher social animals; and pass over insects, 
although some of these are social, and aid one another in 
many important ways. The most common mutual service 
in the higher animals is to warn one another of danger by 
means of the united senses of all. Every sportsman knows, 
as Dr. Jaeger remarks,’ how difficult it is to approach ani- 
mals in a herd or troop. Wild horses and cattle do not, 1 
believe, make any danger-signal; but the attitude of any 
one of them who first discovers an enemy warns the others. 
Rabbits stamp loudly on the ground with their hind feet as 
a signal; sheep and chamois do the same with their forefeet, 
uttering likewise a whistle. Many birds, and some mam- 
mals, post sentinels, which in the case of seals are said® 
generally to be the females. The leader of a troop of 
monkeys acts as the sentinel, and utters cries expressive 
both of danger and of safety.° Social animals perform 


7 “Die Darwin’sche Theorie,’ s. 101. 

8 Mr. R. Brown in ‘‘Proc. Zoolog. Soc.,’? 1868, p. 409. 

® Brehm, ‘‘Thierleben,”’ B, i., 1864, 8.62, 79, For the case of the monkeys 
extracting thorns from each other, see s. 54. With respect to the Hamadryas 
turning over stones, the fact is given (s. 76) on the evidence of Alvarez, whose 
observations Brehm thinks quite trustworthy. or the cases of the old male 
beboons attacking the dogs, see s. 79; and with respect to the eagle, s. 56, 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 139 


many little services for each other; horses nibble, and 
cows lick each other, on any spot which itches; monkeys 
search each other for external parasites; and Brehm states 
that after a troop of the Cercopithecus griseo-viridis has rushed 
through a thorny brake, each monkey stretches itself on a 
branch, and another monkey sitting by, ‘‘conscientiously”’ 
examines its fur, and extracts every thorn or bur. 
Animals also render more important services to one 
another; thus wolves and some other beasts of prey hunt 
in packs, and aid one another in attacking their victims. 
Pelicans fish in concert. The Hamadryas baboons turn 
over stones to find insects, etc.; and when they come to a 
large one, as many as can stand round turn it over together 
and share the booty. Social animals mutually defend each 
other. Bull bisons in North America, when there is danger, 
drive the cows and calves into the middle of the herd, while 
they defend the outside. I shall also in a future chapter 
give an account of two young wild bulls at Chillingham at- 
tacking an old one in concert, and of two stallions together 
trying to drive away a third stallion from a troop of mares. 
In Abyssinia, Brehm encountered a great troop of baboons, 
who were crossing a valley: some had already ascended the © 
opposite mountain, and some were still in the valley: the 
latter were attacked by the dogs, but the old males immedi- 
ately hurried down from the rocks, and with mouths widely 
opened, roared so fearfully that the dogs quickly drew 
back. They were again encouraged to the attack; but by 
this time all the baboons had reascended the heichis, ex- 
cepting a young one, about six months old, who, loudly 
calling for aid, climbed on a block of rock, and was sur- 
rounded. Now one of the largest males, a true hero, came 
down again from the mountain, slowly went to the young 
one, coaxed him, and triumphantly led him away—the dogs’ 
being too much astonished to make an attack. I cannot 
resist giving another scene which was witnessed by this 
same naturalist: An eagle seized a young Cercopithecus, 
which, by clinging to a branch, was not at once carried off; 


140 YHE DESCENT OF MAN 


it cried loudly for assistance, upon which the other members 

of the troop, with much uproar, rushed to the rescue, sur- 

rounded the eagle, and pulled out so many feathers that he” 
no longer thought of his prey, but only how to escape. 

This eagle, as Brehm remarks, assuredly would never again 

attack a single monkey of a troop.” 

It is certain that associated animals have a feeling of love 
for each other which is not felt by non-social adult animals. 
How far in most cases they actually sympathize in the pains 
and pleasures of others, is more doubtful, especially with 
respect to pleasures. Mr, Buxton, however, who had ex- 
cellent means of observation,” states that his macaws, which 
lived free in Norfolk, took ‘‘an extravagant interest’’ in a 
pair with a nest; and whenever the female left it, she was 
surrounded by a troop ‘‘screaming horrible acclamations in 
her honor.’’ It is often difficult to judge whether animals 
have any feeling for the sufferings of others of their kind. 
Who can say what cows feel, when they surround and stare 
intently on a dying or dead companion; apparently, how- 
ever, as Houzeau remarks; they feel no pity. That animals 
sometimes are far from feeling any sympathy is too certain; 
for they will expel a wounded animal from the herd, or gore 
or worry it to death. This is almost the blackest fact in 
natural history, unless, indeed, the explanation which has 
been suggested is true, that their instinct or reason leads 
them to expel an injured companion, lest beasts of prey, 
including man, should be tempted to follow the troop. In 
this case their conduct is not much worse than that of the 
Nerth Amevican Indians, who leave their feeble comrades 
to perish o« the plains; or the Fijians, who, when their 
parents get old, or fall ill, bury them alive.” 


© Mr, Belt gives the case of a spider monkey (Ateles) in Nicaragua, which 
was heard screaming for nearly two hours in the forest, and was found with an 
eagle perched close by it. The bird apparently feared to attack as long as it 
remained face to face; and Mr. Belt believes, from what he has seen of the 
habits of these monkeys, that they protect themselves from eagles by keeping 
two or three together. ‘‘The Naturalist in Nicaragua,” 1874, p. 118, 

H ‘Annals and Mag. of Nat. Hist.,”” November, 1868, p. 382. 

2 Sir J. Lubbock, ‘‘Prehistoric Times,” 2d edit. p. 446. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 14 


Many animals, however, certainly sympathize with each 
other’s distress or danger. This is the case even with birds. 
Captain Stansbury” found on a salt lake in Utah an old and 
completely blind pelican, which was very fat, and must have 
been well fed for a long time by hiscompanions. Mr. Blyth, 
as he informs me, saw Indian crows feeding two or three of - 
their companions which were blind; and I have heard of an 
analogous case with the domestic cock. We may, if we 
choose, call these actions instinctive; but such cases are 
much too rare for the development of any special instinct." 
I have myself seen a dog who never passed a cat who lay 
sick in a basket, and was a great friend of his, without 
giving her a few licks with his tongue, the surest sign 
of kind feeling in a dog. 

It must be called sympathy that leads a courageous dog 
to fly at any one who strikes his master, as he certainly will. 
I saw a person pretending to beat a lady, who had a very — 
timid little dog on her lap, and the trial had never been 
made before; the little creature instantly jumped away, but 
after the pretended beating was over, it was really pathetic 
to see how perseveringly he tried to lick his mistress’s face, 
and comfort her. Brehm’ states that when a baboon in 
confinement was pursued to be punished, the others tried 
to protect him. It must have been sympathy in the cases 
above given which led the baboons and Cercopitheci to 
defend their young comrades. from the dogs and the eagle. 
I will vive only one other instance of sympathetic and heroic 
conduct, in the case of a little American monkey. Several 
years ago a keeper at the Zoological Gardens showed me 
some deep and scarcely healed wounds on the nape of his 
own neck, inflicted on him, while kneeling on the floor, by 


18 As quoted by Mr. L. H. Morgan, “The American Beaver,’’ 1868, p. 272. 
Captain Stansbury also gives an interesting account of the manner in which a 
very young pelican, carried away by a strong stream, was guided and encour- 
aged in its attempts to reach the shore by half a dozen old birds. 

14 As Mr. Bain states, ‘‘effective aid to a sufferer springs from sympathy 

r’?: ‘Mental and Moral Science,’ 1868, p. 245. 

% **Thierleben,’’ B. i. s. 85. 


142 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


a fierce baboon. The little American monkey, who was a 
warm friend of this keeper, lived in the same large com- 
partment, and was dreadfully afraid of the great baboon. 
Nevertheless, as soon as he saw his friend in peril, he 
rushed to the rescue, and by screams and bites so dis- 
tracted the baboon that the man was able to escape, after, 
as the surgeon thought, running great risk of his life. 

Besides love and sympathy, animals exhibit other quali- 
ties connected with the social instincts, which in us would 
be called moral; and l agree with Agassiz** that dogs possess 
something very like a conscience. 

Dogs possess some power of self-command, and this does 
not appear to be wholly the result of fear. As Braubach’’ 
remarks, they will refrain from stealing food in the absence 
of their master. They have long been accepted as the very 
type of fidelity and obedience. But the elephant is likewise 
very faithful to his driver or keeper, and probably considers 
him as the leader of the herd. Dr. Hooker informs me that 
an elephant which he was riding in India became so deeply 
bogged that he remained stuck fast until the next day, when 
he was extricated by men with ropes. Under such circum- 
stances elephants will seize with their trunks any object, 
dead or alive, to place under their knees, to prevent their 
sinking deeper in the mud; and the driver was dreadfully 
afraid lest the animal should have seized Dr. Hooker and 
crushed him to death.. But the driver himself, as Dr. 
Hooker was assured, ran no risk. This forbearance, under 
an emergency so dreadful for a heavy animal, is a wonder- 
ful proof of noble fidelity.*® 

All animals living in a body, which defend themselves 
or attack their enemies in concert, must indeed be in some 
degree faithful to one another; and those that follow a 
leader must be in some degree obedient. When the ba- 


16 ‘Ne l’Espéce et de la Classe,’’ 1869, p. 97. 
1 “Nie Darwin’sche Art-Lehre,’’ 1869, s. 54, 
8 See also Hooker’s ‘‘Himalayan Journals,”’ vol. ii., 1854, p, 333, 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 148 


boons in Abyssinia’ plunder a garden, they silently follow 
their leader; and if an imprudent young animal makes a 
noise, he receives a slap from the others to teach him 
silence and obedience. Mr. Galton, who has had excellent 
opportunities for observing the half-wild cattle in South 
Africa, says,” that they cannot endure even a momentary 
separation from the herd. They are essentially slavish, and 
accept the common determination, seeking no better lot than 
to be led by any one ox who has enough self-reliance to 
accept the position. The men who break in these animals 
for harness watch assiduously for those who, by grazing 
apart, show a self-reliant disposition, and these they train 
as fore-oxen. Mr. Galton adds that such animals are rare 
and valuable; and if many were born they would soon be 
eliminated, as lions are always on the lookout for the 
individuals which wander from the herd. 

With respect to the impulse which leads certain animals 
to associate together, and to aid one another in many ways, 
we may infer that in most cases they are impelled by the 
same sense of satisfaction or pleasure which they experi- 
ence in performing other instinctive actions; or by the same 
sense of dissatisfaction as when other instinctive actions are 
checked. We see this in innumerable instances, and it is 
illustrated in a striking manner by the acquired instincts 
of our domesticated animals; thus a young shepherd-dog 
delights in driving and running round a flock of sheep, 
but not in worrying them; a young fox-hound delights in 
hunting a fox, while some other kinds of dogs, as I have 
witnessed, utterly disregard foxes. What a strong feeling 
of inward satisfaction must impel a bird, so full of activity, 
to brood day after day over her eggs. Migratory birds are 
quite miserable if stopped from migrating; perhaps they 
enjoy starting on their long flight; but it is hard to believe 


19 Brehm, ‘‘Thierleben,’’ B. i. 8. 76. . ; ; 
20 See his extremely interesting paper on ‘‘Gregariousness in Cattle, and in 
Man,’’ ‘‘Macmillan’s Mag.,’? Feb. 1871, p. 353. 


144 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


that the poor pinioned goose, described by Audubon, which 
started on foot at the proper time for its journey of probably 
more than a thousand miles, could have felt any joy in doing 
so. Some instincts are determined solely by painful feelings, 
as by fear, which leads to self-preservation, and is in some 
cases directed toward special enemies. No one, I presume, 
can analyze the sensations of pleasure or pain. In many 
instances, however, it is probable that instincts are persist- 
ently followed from the mere force of inheritance without 
the stimulus of either pleasure or pain. A young pointer, 
when it first scents game, apparently cannot help pointing. 
A squirrel in a cage who pats the nuts which it cannot eat, 
as if to bury them in the ground, can hardly be thought to 
act thus either from pleasure or pain. Hence the common 
assumption that men must be impelled to every action by 
experiencing some pleasure or pain may be erroneous. A\l- 
though a habit may be blindly and implicitly followed, inde- 
pendently of any pleasure or pain felt at the moment, yet if 
it be forcibly and abruptly checked, a vague sense of dissatis- 
faction is generally experienced. 

It has often been assumed that animals were in the first 
place rendered social, and that they feel as a consequence 
uncomfortable when separated from each other, and com- 
fortable while together; but it is a more probable view that 
these sensations were first developed, in order that those 
animals which would profit by living in society should be 
induced to live together, in the same manner as the sense 
of hunger and the pleasure of eating were, no doubt, first 
acquired in order to induce animals to eat. The feeling 
of pleasure from society is probably an extension of the 
parental or filial affections, since the social instinct seems to 
be developed by the young remaining for a long time with 
their parents; and this extension may be attributed in part to 
habit, but chiefly to natural selection. Wath those animals 
which were benefited by living in close association, the indi- 
viduals which took the greatest pleasure in society would 
best escape various dangers; while those that cared least for 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 145 


their comrades, and lived solitary, would perish in greater 
numbers. With respect to the origin of the parental and 
filial affections, which apparently lie at the base of the social 
instincts, we know not the steps by which they have been 
gained; but we may infer that it has been to a large extent 
through natural selection. So it has almost certainly been 
with the unusual and opposite feeling of hatred between the 
nearest relations, as with the worker-bees which kill their 
brother-drones, and with the queen-bees which kill their 
daughter-queens; the desire to destroy their nearest rela- 
tions having been in this case of service to the community. 

Parental affection, or some feeling which replaces it, has 
been developed in certain animals extremely low in the 
scale, for example, in star-fishes and spiders. It is also 
occasionally present in a few members alone in a whole 
group of animals, as in the genus Forficula, or earwigs. 

y The all-important emotion of sympathy is distinct from 
that of love. A mother may passionately love her sleeping 
and passive infant, but she can hardly at such times be said 
to feel sympathy for it. The love of a man for his dog is 
distinct from sympathy, and so is that of a dog for his mas- 
ter. Adam Smith formerly argued, as has Mr. Bain re- 
cently, that the basis of sympathy lies in our strong reten- 
tiveness of former states of pain or pleasure. Hence, ‘‘the 
sight of another person enduring hunger, cold, fatigue, re- 
vives in us some recollection of these states, which are pain- 
ful even in idea.’’ We are thus impelled to relieve the 
sufferings of another, in order that our own painful feelings 
may be at the same time relieved. In like manner we are 
led to participate in the pleasures of others." But I cannot 


1 See the first and striking chapter in Adam Smith’s ‘‘Theory of Moral 
Sentiments,’’ Also Mr. Bain’s ‘‘Mental and Moral Science,”’ 1868, pp. 244 and 
275-282. Mr. Bain states, that “‘sympathy is, indirectly, a source of pleasure 
to the sympathizer’; and he accounts for this through reciprocity. He re- 
marks that ‘‘the person benefited, or others in his stead, may make up, by 
sympathy and good offices returned, for all the sacrifice.” But if, as appears 
to be the case, sympathy is strictly an instinct, its exercise would give direct 
pleasure, in the same manzer as the exercise, as before remarked, of almost 
every other instinct. 


Descent—Von. L—} 


146 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


see how this view explains the fact that sympathy is excited 
in an immeasurably stronger degree by a beloved than by an 
indifferent person. The mere sight of suffering, independ- 
ently of love, would suffice to call up in us vivid recollec- 
tions and associations. The explanation may lie in the fact 
that, with all animals, sympathy is directed solely toward 
the members of the same community, and therefore toward 
known and more or less beloved members, but not to all 
the individuals of the same species. This fact is not more 
surprising than that the fears of many animals should be 
directed against special enemies. Species which are not 
social, such as lions and tigers, no doubt feel sympathy for 
the suffering of their own young, but not for that of any 
other animal. With mankind, selfishness, experience, and 
imitation probably add, as Mr. Bain has shown, to the power 
of sympathy; for we are led by the hope of receiving good 
in return to perform acts of sympathetic kindness to others; 
and sympathy is much strengthened by habit. In however 
complex a manner this feeling may have originated, as it is 
one of high importance to all those animals which aid and 
defend one another, it will have been increased through 
natural selection; for those communities which included the 
greatest number of the most sympathetic members would 
flourish best, and rear the greatest number of offspring. 
It is, however, impossible to decide in many cases 
whether certain social instincts have been acquired through 
natural selection, or are the indirect result of other instincts 
and faculties, such as sympathy, reason, experience, and a 
tendency to imitation; or again, whether they are simply the 
result of long-continued habit. So remarkable an instinct 
as the placing sentinels to warn the community of danger 
can hardly have been the indirect result of any of these 
faculties; it must, therefore, have been directly acquired. 
On the other hand, the habit followed by the males of some 
social animals of defending the community, and of attacking 
their enemies or their prey in concert, may perhaps have 
originated from mutual sympathy; but courage, and in most 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 147 


cases strength, must have been previously acquired, proba- 
bly througl natural selection.) 

Of the various instincts and habits, some are much 
stronger than others; that is, some either give more pleas- 
ure in their performance, and more distress in their preven- 
tion, than others; or, which is probably quite as important, 
they are, through inheritance, more persistently followed, 
without exciting any special feeling of pleasure or pain. 
We are ourselves conscious that some habits are much more 
difficult to cure or change than others. Hence a struggle 
may often be observed in animals between different in- 
stincts, or between an instinct and some habitual disposi- 
tion; as when a dog rushes after a hare, is rebuked, pauses, 
hesitates, pursues again, or returns ashamed to his master; 
or as between the love of a female dog for her young pup- 
pies and for her master—for she may be seen to slink away 
to them, as if half-ashamed of not accompanying her mas- 
ter. But the most curious instance known to me of one in- 
stinct getting the better of another is the migratory instinct 
conquering the maternal instinct. The former is wonder- 
fully strong; a confined bird will at the proper season beat 
her breast against the wires of her cage, until it is bare and 
bloody. It causes young salmon to leap out of the fresh 
water, in which they could continue to exist, and thus unin- 
tentionally to commit suicide. Every one knows how strong 
the maternal instinct is, leading even timid birds to face 
great danger, though with hesitation, and in opposition to 
the instinct of self-preservation. Nevertheless, the migra- 
tory instinct is so powerful, that late in the autumn swal- 
lows, house-martins, and swifts frequently desert their ten- 
der young, leaving them to perish miserably in their nests.” 


2 This fact, the Rev. L. Jenyns states (see his edition of “‘White’s Nat, 
Hist. of Selborne,’’ 1853, p. 204), was first recorded by the illustrious Jenner, 
in ‘‘Phil. Transact.,’’? 1824, and has since been confirmed by several observers, 
especially by Mr. Blackwall. This latter careful observer examined, late in the 
autumn, during two years, thirty-six nests; he found that twelve contained 
young dead birds, five contained eggs on the point of being hatched, and three, 
eggs not nearly hatched. Many birds, not yet old enough for a prolonged 


148 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


We can perceive that an instinctive impulse, if it be in any 
way more beneficial to a species than some other or opposed 
instinct, would be rendered the more potent of the two 
through natural selection; for the individuals which had it 
most strongly developed would survive in larger numbers. 
Whether this is the case with the migratory in comparison 
with the maternal instinct may be doubted. The great per- 
sistence or steady action of the former at certain seasons of 
the year, during the whole day, may give it for a time 
paramount force, 


Man a Social Animal.—Every one will admit that man 
is a social being. We see this in his dislike of solitude, 
and in his wish for society beyond that of his own family. 
Solitary confinement is one of the severest punishments 
which can be inflicted. Some authors suppose that man 
primevally lived in single families; but at the present day, 
though single families, or only two or three together, roam 
the solitudes of some savage lands, they always, as far as I 
can discover, hold friendly relations with other families in- 
“habiting the same district. Such families occasionally meet 
in council, and unite for their common defence. It is no 
argument against savage man being a social animal that the 
tribes inhabiting adjacent districts are almost always at war 
with each other; for the social instincts never extend to all 
the individuals of the same species. Judging from the anal- 
ogy of the majority of the Quadrumana, it is probable that 
the early ape-like progenitors of man were likewise social; 
but this is not of much importance for us. Although man, 
as he now exists, has few special instincts, having lost any 
which his early progenitors may have possessed, this is no 
reason why he should not have retained from an extremely 


flight, are likewise deserted and left behind. See Blackwall, ‘‘Researches in 
Zoology,”? 1834, pp. 108, 118. For some additional evidence, although this is 
not wanted, see Leroy, ‘‘Lettres Phil.,”’ 1802, p. 21%. For swifts, Gould’s 
“Introduction to the Birds of Great Britain,’’ 1823, p. 5. Similar cases have 
been observed in Canada by Mr. Adams; ‘‘Pop. Science Review,”’ July, 1873, 
p- 283. = ars — 


a 2 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 149 


remote period some degree of instinctive love and sympathy 
for his fellows. We are indeed all conscious that we do pos- 
sess such sympathetic feelings;** but our consciousness does 
not tell us whether they are instinctive, having originated 
long ago in the same manner as with the lower animals, or 
whether they have been acquired by each of us during our 
early years. As man is a social animal, it is almost certain 
that he would inherit a tendency to be faithful to his com- 
rades, and obedient to the leader of his tribe, for these quali- 
ties are common to most social animals. He would conse- 
quently possess some capacity for self-command. He would 
from an inherited tendency be willing to defend, in concert 
with others, his fellow-men; and would be ready to aid them 
in any way which did not too greatly interfere with his own 
welfare or his own strong desires. 

The social animals which stand at the bottom of the scale 
are guided almost exclusively, and those which stand higher 
in the scale are largely guided, by special instincts in the aid 
which they give to the members of the same community; 
but they are likewise in part impelled by mutual love and 

“sympathy, assisted apparently by some amount of reason. 
Although man, as just remarked, has no special instincts 
to tell him how to aid his fellow-men, he still has the im- 
pulse, and with his improved intellectual faculties would 
naturally be much guided in this respect by reason and 
experience. Instinctive sympathy would also cause him to 
value highly the approbation of his fellows; for, as Mr. Bain 
has clearly shown,™ the love of praise and the strong feeling 
of glory, and the still stronger horror of scorn and infamy, 
“fare due to the workings of sympathy.’’ Consequently man 
would be influenced in the highest degree by the wishes, 
approbation, and blame of his fellow-men, as expressed by 


°3 Hume remarks (‘‘An Inquiry’ Concerning the Principles of Morals,’’ edit. 
of 1751, p. 132), ‘‘There seems a necessity for confessing that the happiness 
and misery of others are not spectacles altogether indifferent to us, but that 
the view of the former... communicates a secret joy; the appearance 
of the latter . . . throws a melancholy damp over the imagination.”’ 

24 “Mental and Moral Science,’’ 1868, p. 254. 


150 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


their gestures and language. Thus the social instincts, 
which must have been acquired by man in a very rude 
state, and probably even by his early ape-like progenitors, 
still give the impulse to some of his best actions; but his 
actions are in a higher degree determined by the expressed 
wishes and judgment of his fellow-men, and, unfortunately, 
very often by his own strong, selfish desires. But as love, 
sympathy, and self-command become strengthened by habit, 
and as the power of reasoning becomes clearer, so that man 
can value justly the judgments of his fellows, he will feel 
himself impelled, apart from any transitory pleasure or pain, 
to certain lines of conduct. He might then declare—not 
that any barbarian or uncultivated man could thus think 
—Iam the supreme judge of my own conduct, and, in the 
words of Kant, I will not in my own person violate the dig- 
nity of humanity. 


The more enduring Social Instincts conquer the less per- 
sistent Instincts.—We have not, however, as yet considered 
the main point on which, from our present point of view, the 
whole question of the moral sense turns. Why should a 
man feel that he ought to obey one instinctive desire rather 
than another? Why is he bitterly regretful, if he has 
yielded to a strong sense of self-preservation, and has not 
risked his life to save that of a fellow-creature? or why 
does he regret having stolen food from hunger? 

It is evident, in the first place, that with mankind the 
instinctive impulses have different degrees of strength; a 
savage will risk his own life to save that of a member of 
the same community, but will be wholly indifferent about a 
stranger; a young and timid mother, urged by the maternal 
instinct, will, without a moment’s hesitation, run the great- 
est danger for her own infant, but not for a mere fellow- 
creature. Nevertheless many a civilized man, or even boy, 
who never before risked his life for another, filled with 
courage and sympathy, has disregarded the instinct of self- 
preservation, and plunged at once into a torrent to save a 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 151 


drowning man, though a stranger. In this case man is im- 
pelled by the same instinctive motive which made the heroic 
little American monkey, formerly described, save his keeper, 
by attacking the great and dreaded baboon. Such actions 
as the above appear to be the simple result of the greater 
strength of the social or maternal instincts than that of any 
other instinct or motive; for they are performed too instan- 
taneously for reflection, or for pleasure or pain to be felt at 
the time; though, if prevented by any cause, distress or 
even misery might be felt. In a timid man, on the other 
hand, the instinct of self-preservation might be so strong 
that he would be unable to force himself to run any such 
risk, perhaps not even for his own child. 

{am aware that some persons maintain that actions per- 
formed impulsively, as in the above cases, do not come 
under the dominion of the moral sense, and cannot be called 
moral. They confine this term to actions done deliberately, 
after a victory over opposing desires, or when prompted by 
some exalted motive. But it appears scarcely possible to 
draw any clear line of distinction of this kind.” As far as 
exalted motives are concerned, many instances have been 
recorded of savages, destitute of any feeling of general be- 
nevolence toward mankind, and not guided by any religious 
motive, who have deliberately sacrificed their lives as pris- 
oners,”* rather than betray their comrades; and surely their 
conduct ought to be considered as moral. As far as deliber- 
ation and the victory over opposing motives are concerned, 
animals may be seen doubting between opposed instincts, in 
rescuing their offspring or comrades from danger; yet their 
actions, though done for the good of others, are not called 


%5 T refer here to the distinction between what has been called material and 
formal morality. I am glad to find that Prof. Huxley (‘‘Critiques and Ad- 
dresses, ’’ 1873, p. 287) takes the same view on this subject as Ido. Mr. Leslie 
Stephen remarks (‘‘Essays on Free-thinking and Plain Speaking,’’ 1873, p. 83), 
“The metaphysical distinction between material and formal morality is as 
irrelevant as other such distinctions.”’ ; 

% I have given one such case, namely, of three Patagonian Indians who 
preferred being shot, one after the other, to betraying the plans of their com- 
panions in war (‘‘Journal of Researches,’’ 1845, p. 103). 


152 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


moral. Moreover, anything performed very often by us will 
at last be done without deliberation or hesitation, and can 
then hardly be distinguished from an instinct; yet surely 
no one will pretend that such an action ceases to be moral. 
On the contrary, we all feel that an act cannot be considered 
as perfect, or as performed in the most noble manner, unless 
it be done impulsively, without deliberation or effort, in the 
same manner as by a man in whom the requisite qualities 
are innate. He who is forced to overcome his fear or want 
of sympathy before he acts, deserves, however, in one way © 
higher credit than the man whose innate disposition leads 
him to a good act without effort. As we cannot distinguish 
between motives, we rank all actions of a certain class as 
moral, if performed by a moral being. A moral being is 
one who is capable of comparing his past and future actions 
or motives, and of approving or disapproving of them. We 
have no reason to suppose that any of the lower animals 
have this capacity; therefore, when a Newfoundland dog 
drags a child out of the water, or a monkey faces danger 
to rescue its comrade, or takes charge of an orphan monkey, 
we do not call its conduct moral. But in the case of man, 
who alone can with certainty be ranked as a moral being, 
actions of a certain class are called moral, whether per- 
formed deliberately, after a struggle with opposing motives, 
or impulsively through instinct, or from the effects of slowly 
gained habit. 

But to return to our more immediate subject. Although 
some instincts are more powerful than others, and thus lead 
to corresponding actions, yet it is untenable that in man the 
social instincts (including the love of praise and fear of 
blame) possess greater strength, or have, through long habit, 
acquired greater strength than the instincts of self-preserva- 
tion, hunger, lust, vengeance, ete. Why then does man re- 
gret, even though trying to banish such regret, that he has 
followed the one natural impulse rather than the other; and 
why does he further feel that he ought to regret his conduct? 
Man in this respect differs profoundly from the lower ani- 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 153 


mals. Nevertheless we can, I think, see with some degree 
of clearness the reason of this difference. 

Man, from the activity of his mental faculties, cannot 
avoid reflection; past impressions and images are inces- 
santly and clearly passing through his mind. Now with 
those animals which live permanently in a body, the social 
instincts are ever present.and persistent. Such animals are 
always ready to utter the danger-signal, to defend the com- 
munity, and to give aid to their fellows in accordance with 
their habits; they feel at all times, without the stimulus of 
any special passion or desire, some degree of love and sym- 
pathy for them; they are unhappy if long separated from 
them, and always happy to be again in their company. So 
it is with ourselves. Even when we are quite alone, how 
often do we think with pleasure or pain of what others think 
of us—of their imagined approbation or disapprobation; and 
this all follows from sympathy, a fundamental element of 
the social instincts. .A man who possessed no trace of such 
instincts would be an unnatural monster. On the other 
hand, the desire to satisfy hunger, or any passion such as 
vengeance, is in its nature temporary, and can for a time 
be fully satisfied. Nor is it easy, perhaps hardly possible, 
to call up with complete vividness the feeling, for instance, 
of hunger; nor indeed, as has often been remarked, of any 
suffering. The instinct of self-preservation is not felt except 
in the presence of danger; and many a coward has thought 
himself brave until he has met his enemy face to face. The 
wish for another man’s property is perhaps as persistent a 
desire as any that can be named; but even in this case the 
satisfaction of actual possession is generally a weaker feeling 
than the desire: many a thief, if not a habitual one, after 
success has wondered why he stole some article.” 


21 Enmity or hatred seems also to be a highly persistent feeling, perhaps 
more so than any other that can be named. Envy is defined as hatred of 
another for some excellence or success; and Bacon insists (Essay ix.), ‘‘Of all 
other affections envy is the most importune and continual. og Dogs are very apt 
to hate both strange men and strange dogs, especially if they live near at hand, 
but do not belong to the same family, tribe, or clan; this feeling would thus 


154 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


A man cannot prevent past impressions often repassing 
through his mind; he will thus be driven to make a com- 
parison between the impressions of past hunger, vengeance 
satisfied, or danger shunned at other men’s cost, with the 
almost ever-present instinct of sympathy, and with his early 
knowledge of what others consider as praiseworthy or blama- 
ble. This knowledge cannot be banished from his mind, 
and from instinctive sympathy is esteemed of great moment. 
He will then feel as if he had been balked in following a 
present instinct or habit, and this with all animals causes 
dissatisfaction, or even misery. 

The above case of the swallow affords an illustration, 
though of a reversed nature, of a temporary, though for 
the time strongly persistent, instinct conquering another in- 
stinct which is usually dominant over all others. At the 
proper season these birds seem all day long to be impressed 
with the desire to migrate; their habits change; they be- 
come restless, are noisy, and congregate in flocks. While 
the mother-bird is feeding, or brooding over her nestlings, 
the maternal instinct is probably stronger than the migra- 
tory; but the instinct which is the more persistent gains 
the victory, and at last, at a moment when her young ones 
are not in sight, she takes flight and deserts them. When 
arrived at the end of her long journey, and the migratory 
instinct has. ceased to act, what an agony of remorse the 
bird would feel, if, from being endowed with great mental 
activity, she could not prevent the image constantly passing 


seem to be innate, and is certainly a most persistent one. It seems to be the 
complement and converse of the true social instinct. From what we hear of 
savages, it would appear that something of the same kind holds good with 
them. If this be so, it would be a small step in any one to transfer such feel- 
ings.to any member of the same tribe if he had done him an injury and had 
become his enemy. Nor is it probable that the primitive conscience would re- 
proach a man for injuring his enemy: rather it would reproach him if he had not 
revenged himself. To do good in return for evil, to love your enemy, is a height 
of morality to which it may be doubted whether the social instincts would, by 
themselves, have ever led us. It is necessary that these instincts, together 
with sympathy, should have been highly cultivated and extended by the aid of 
reason, instruction, and the love or fear of God, before any such golden rule 
would ever be thought of and obeyed. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 155 


through her mind, of her young ones perishing in the bleak 
north from cold and hunger. 

At the moment of action man will no doubt be apt to 
follow the stronger impulse; and though this may occasion- 
ally prompt him to the noblest deeds, it will more commonly 
lead him to gratify his own desires at the expense of other 
men. But after their gratification, when past and weaker 
impressions are judged by the ever- enduring social instinct, 
and by his deep regard for the good opinion of his fellows, 
retribution will surely come. He will then feel remorse, 
repentance, regret, or shame; this latter feeling, however, 
relates almost exclusively to the judgment of others. He 
will consequently resolve more or less firmly to act differ- 
ently for the future; and this is conscience; for conscience 
looks backward, and serves as a guide for the future. 

The nature and strength of the feelings which we call 
regret, shame, repentance, or remorse, depend apparently 
not only on the strength of the violated instinct, but partly 
on the strength of the temptation, and often still more on 
the judgment of our fellows. How far each man values the 
appreciation of others, depends on the strength of his innate 
or acquired féeling of sympathy, and on his own capacity 
for reasoning out the remote consequences of his acts. An- 
other element is most important, although not necessary—the 
reverence or fear of the Gods or Spirits believed in by each 
man; and this applies especially in cases of remorse. Sev- 
eral critics have objected that, though some slight regret or 
repentance may be explained by the view advocated in this 
chapter, it is impossible thus to account for the soul-shaking 
feeling of remorse. ButI can see little force in this objec- 
tion. My critics do not define what they mean by remorse, 
and I can find no definition implying more than an over- 

‘whelming sense of repentance. Remorse seems to bear the 
same relation to repentance as rage does to anger, or agony 
to pain. It is far from strange that an instinct so strong and 
so generally admired as maternal love should, if disobeyed, 
lead to the deepest misery, as soon as the impression of the 


156 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


past cause of disobedience is weakened. Even when an 
action is opposed to no special instinct, merely to know 
that our friends and equals despise us for it is enough to 
cause great misery. Who can doubt that the refusal to 
fight a duel through fear has caused many men an agony 
of shame? Many a Hindu, it is said, has been stirred to 
the bottom of his soul by having partaken of unclean food. 
Here is another case of what must, I think, be called re- 
morse. Dr. Landor acted as a magistrate in West Australia, 
and relates” that a native on his farm, after losing one of his 
wives from disease, came and said that ‘the was going to a 
distant tribe to spear a woman, to satisfy his sense of duty 
to his wife. I told him that if he did so I would send him 
to prison for life. He remained about the farm for some 
months, but got exceedingly thin, and complained that he 
could not rest or eat, that his wife’s spirit was haunting him, 
because he had not taken a life for hers. I was inexorable, 
and assured him that nothing should save him if he did.” 
Nevertheless the man disappeared for more than a year, and 
then returned in high condition; and his other wife told 
Dr. Landor that her husband had taken the life of a woman 
belonging to a distant tribe; but it was impossible to obtain 
legal evidence of the act. The breach of a rule held sacred 
by the tribe will thus, as it seems, give rise to the deepest 
feelings—and this quite apart from the social instincts, ex- 
cepting in so far as the rule is grounded on the judgment of . 
the community. How so many strange superstitions have 
arisen throughout the world we know not; nor can we tell 
how some real and great crimes, such as incest, have come 
to be held in an abhorrence (which is not however quite 
universal) by the lowest savages. It is even doubtful 
whether in some tribes incest would be looked on with 
greater horror than would the marriage of a man with a 
woman bearing the same name, though not a relation. ‘‘To 
violate this law is a crime which the Australians hold in the 


28 ‘Insanity in Relation to Law’’; Ontario, United States, 1871, p. 14. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 157 


greatest abhorrence, in this agreeing exactly with certain 
tribes of North America. When the question is put in 
either district, is it worse to kill a girl of a foreign tribe 
or to marry a girl of one’s own, an answer just opposite 
to ours would be given without hesitation.’’** We may, 
therefore, reject the belief, lately insisted on by some writ- 
ers, that the abhorrence of incest is due to our possessing a 
special God-implanted conscience. On the whole it is intel- 
ligible that a man urged by so powerful a sentiment as re- 
morse, though arising as above explained, should be led to 
act in a manner which he has been taught to believe serves 
as an expiation, such as delivering himself up to justice. 

Man, prompted by his conscience, will through long 
habit acquire such perfect self-command, that his desires 
and passions will at last yield instantly and without a 
struggle to his social sympathies and instincts, including 
his feeling for the judgment of his fellows. The still hun- 
gry or the still revengeful_ man will not think of stealing 
food or of wreaking his vengeance. It is possible, or, as 
we shall hereafter see, even probable, that the habit of self- 
command may, like other habits, be inherited. Thus at last 
man comes to feel, through acquired and perhaps inherited 
habit, that it is best for him to obey his more persistent im- 
pulses. The imperious word ought seems merely to imply 
the consciousness of the existence of a rule of conduct, how- 
ever it may have originated. Formerly it must have been 
often vehemently urged that an insulted gentleman ought to 
fight a duel. We even say that a pointer ought to point, and 
a retriever to retrieve game. If they fail to do so, they fail 
in their duty, and act wrongly. X 

If any desire or instinct leading to an action opposed 
. to the good of others still appears, when recalled to mind, 
as strong as, or stronger than, the social instinct, a man 
will feel no keen regret at having followed it; but he will 
be conscious that if his conduct were known to his fellows, 


9 FB, Tylor in ‘Contemporary Review,” April, 1873, p. 707. 


158 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


it would meet with their disapprobation; and few are so des- 
titute of sympathy as not to feel discomfort when this is 
realized. If he has no such sympathy, and if his desires 
leading to bad actions are at the time strong, and when re- 
called are not overmastered by the persistent social instincts 
and the judgment of others, then he is essentially a bad 
man; and the sole restraining motive left is the fear of 
punishment, and the conviction that in the long run it 
would be best for his own selfish interests to regard the 
good of others rather than his own. 

It is obvious that every one may with an easy conscience 
gratify his own desires, if they do not interfere with his 
social instincts, that is with the good of others; but in order 
to be quite free from self-reproach, or at least of anxiety, it 
is almost necessary for him to avoid the disapprobation, 
whether reasonable or not, of his fellow-men. Nor must 
he break through the fixed habits of his life, especially if 
these are supported by reason; for if he does, he will as- 
suredly feel dissatisfaction. He must likewise avoid the 
reprobation of the one God or gods in whom, according 
to his knowledge or superstition, he may believe; but in 
this case the additional fear of divine punishment often 
supervenes. 


The strictly Social Virtues at first alone regarded.—The 
above view of the origin and nature of the moral sense, 
which tells us what we ought to do, and of the conscience 
which reproves us if we disobey it, accords well with what 
we see of the early and undeveloped condition of this fal- 
ulty in mankind. The virtues which must be practiced, at 
least generally, by rude men, so that they may associate in 
a body, are those which are still recognized as the most 
important. But they are practiced almost exclusively in 
relation to the men of the same tribe; and their opposites 


® Dr. Prosper Despine in his ‘‘Psychologie Naturelle’? 1808 (tom. i. p. 
243; tom. ii. p. 169), gives many curious cases of the worst criminals, who 
apparently have been entirely destitute of conscience, 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 159 


are not regarded as crimes in relation to the men of other 
tribes. No tribe could hold together if murder, robbery, 
treachery, etc., were common; consequently such crimes 
within the limits of the same tribe ‘‘are branded with ever- 
lasting infamy;’’™ but excite no such sentiment beyond 
these limits. A North American Indian is well pleased 
with himself, and is honored by others, when he scalps a 
man of another tribe; and a Dyak cuts off the head of an 
unoffending person, and dries it as a trophy. The murder 
of infants has prevailed on the largest scale throughout the 
world,” and has met with no reproach; but infanticide, 
especially of females, has been thought to be good for the 
tribe, or at least not injurious. Suicide during former times 
was not generally considered as a crime,** but rather, from the 
courage displayed, as an honorable act; and it is still prac- 
ticed by some semi-civilized and savage nations without re- 
proach, for it does not obviously concern others of the tribe. 
It has been recorded that an Indian Thug conscientiously 
regretted that he had not robbed and strangled as many 
travellers as did his father before him. In a rude state of 
civilization the robbery of strangers is, indeed, generally 
considered as honorable. 

Slavery, although in some ways beneficial during ancient 
times, is a great crime; yet it was not so regarded until 
quite recently, even by the most civilized nations. And 
this was especially the case, because the slaves belonged in 


31 See an able article in the ‘‘North British Review,’’ 1867, p. 395. See 
also Mr. W. Bagehot’s articles on the Importance of Obedience and Coherence 
to Primitive Man, in the ‘‘Fortnightly Review,’’ 1867, p. 529, and 1868, 

. 457, ete. 
Po The fullest account which I have met with is by Dr. Gerland, in his 
‘Ueber das Aussterben der Naturvolker,’’ 1868; but I shall have to recur to 
the subject of infanticide in a future chapter. 

33 See the very interesting discussion on Suicide in Lecky’s ‘‘History of 
European Morals,” vol. 1., 1869, p. 223. With respect to savages, Mr. Win- 
wood Reade informs me that the negroes of West Africa often commit suicide. 
It is well known how common it was among the miserable aborigines of South 
America, after the Spanish conquest. For New Zealand, see the voyage of the 
“‘Novara,’’ and for the Aleutian Islands, Miller, as quoted by Houzeau, “Tes 
Facultés Mentales,’’ ete., tom. ii. p. 136. 

#4 See Mr, Bagehot, ‘‘Physics and Polities,’? 1872, p. 72. 


160 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


general to a race different from that of their masters. As 
barbarians do not regard the opinion of their women, wives 
are commonly treated like slaves. Most savages are utterly 
indifferent to the sufferings of strangers, or even delight 
in witnessing them. It is well known that the women and 
children of the North American Indians aided in torturing 
their enemies. Some savages take a horrid pleasure in 
cruelty to animals,* and humanity is an unknown virtue. 
Nevertheless, besides the family affections, kindness is com- 
mon, especially during sickness, between the members of 
the same tribe, and is sometimes extended beyond these 
limits. Mungo Park’s touching account of the kindness 
of the negro women of the interior to him is well known. 
Many instances could be given of the noble fidelity of sav- 
ages toward each other, but not to strangers; common ex- 
perience justifies the maxim of the Spaniard, ‘‘Never, never 
trust an Indian.’’ There cannot be fidelity without truth; 
and this fundamental virtue is not rare between the mem- 
bers of the same tribe: thus Mungo Park heard the negro 
women teaching their young children to love the truth. 
This, again, is one of the virtues which becomes so deeply 
rooted in the mind that it is sometimes practiced by savages, 
even at a high cost, toward strangers; but to lie to your 
enemy has rarely been thought a sin, as the history of mod- 
ern diplomacy too plainly shows. As soon as a tribe has a 
recognized leader, disobedience becomes a crime, and even 
abject submission is looked at as a sacred virtue. 
As during rude times no man can be useful or faithful 
to his tribe without courage, this quality has universally 
been placed in the highest rank; and although in civilized 
countries a good yet timid man may be far more useful to 
the community than a brave one, we cannot help instinc- 
tively honoring the latter above a coward, however benevo- 
lent. Prudence, on the other hand, which does not concern 
the welfare of others, though a very useful virtue, has never 


35 See, for instance, Mr. Hamilton’s account of the Kaffirs, ‘ Anthropo- 
logical Review,’’ 1870, p. xv. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 161 


been highly esteemed. As no man can practice the virtues 
necessary for the welfare of his tribe without self-sacrifice, 
self-command, and the power of endurance, these qualities 
have been at all times highly and most justly valued. The 
American savage voluntarily submits to the most horrid 
tortures without a groan, to prove and strengthen his forti- 
tude and courage; and we cannot help admiring him, or 
even an Indian Fakir, who, from a foolish religious motive, 
swings suspended by a hook buried in his flesh. 

The other so-called self-regarding virtues, which do not 
obviously, though they may really, affect the welfare of the 
tribe, have never been esteemed by savages, though now 
highly appreciated by civilized nations. The greatest in- 
temperance is no reproach with savages. Utter licentious- 
ness and unnatural crimes prevail to an astounding extent.™ 
As soon, however, as marriage, whether polygamous or 
monogamous, becomes common, jealousy will lead to the 
inculcation of female virtue; and this, being honored, will 
tend to spread to the unmarried females. How slowly it 
spreads to the male sex we see at the present day. Chastity 
eminently requires self-command; therefore it has been 
honored from a very early period in the moral history of 
civilized man. As a consequence of this, the senseless 
practice of celibacy has been ranked from a remote period 
as a virtue.*’ The hatred of indecency, which appears to 
us so natural as to be thought innate, and which is so valu- 
able an aid to chastity, is a modern virtue, appertaining 
exclusively, as Sir G. Staunton remarks,* to civilized life. 
This is shown by the ancient religious rites of various 
nations, by the drawings on the walls of Pompeii, and by 
the practices of many savages. 

We have now seen that actions are regarded by savages, 
and were probably so regarded by primeval man, as good or 


88 Mr, M‘Lennan has given (‘‘Primitive Marriage,’’ 1865, p. 176) a good 
collection of facts on this head. 

31 Lecky, ‘‘History of European Morals,’ vol. i, 1869, p. 109, 

38 *“Embassy to China,”’ vol. ii. p. 348. 


162 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


bad, solely as they obviously affect the welfare of the tribe 
—not that of the species, nor that of an individual member 
of the tribe. This conclusion agrees well with the belief 
that the so-called moral sense is aboriginally derived from 
the social instincts, for both relate at first exclusively to the 
community. The chief causes of the low morality of sav- 
ages, as judged by our standard, are, first, the confinement 
of sympathy to the same tribe. Secondly, powers of reason- 
ing insufficient to recognize the bearing of many virtues, 
especially of the self-regarding virtues, on the general wel- 
fare of the tribe. Savages, for instance, fail to trace the 
multiplied evils consequent on a want of temperance, chas- 
tity, ete. And, thirdly, weak power of self-command; for 
this power has not been strengthened through long-contin- 
ued, perhaps inherited, habit, instruction, and religion. 

T have entered into the above details on the immorality 
of savages,** because some authors have recently taken a 
high view of their moral nature, or have attributed most 
of their crimes to mistaken benevolence.” These authors 
appear to rest their conclusion on savages possessing those 
virtues which are serviceable, or even necessary, for the 
existence of the family and of the tribe—qualities which 
they undoubtedly do possess, and often in a high degree. 


Concluding Remarks.—It was assumed formerly by phi- 
losophers of the derivative’ school of morals that the foun- 
dation of morality lay in a form of Selfishness; but more 
recently the ‘‘Greatest happiness principle’ has been brought 
prominently forward. It is, however, more correct to speak 
of the latter principle as the standard, and not as the motive 
of conduct. Nevertheless, all the authors whose works I 


88 See on this subject copious evidence in chap. vii. of Sir J. Lubbock, 
“Origin of Civilization,”? 1870. 

40 For instance, Lecky, ‘“‘History of European Morals,” vol. i. p. 124. 

41 This term is used in an able article in the ‘“Westminster Review,’’ Oct. 
1869, p. 498, For the ‘Greatest happiness principle,” see J. 8. Mill, ‘‘Utili- 
tarianism,”’ p. 17. : 


7HE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 163 


have consulted, with a few exceptions,” write as if there 
must be a distinct motive for every action, and that this 
must be associated with some pleasure or displeasure. But 
man seems often to act impulsively, that is from instinct 
or long habit, without any consciousness of pleasure, in the 
same manner as does probably a bee or ant, when it blindly 
follows its instincts. Under circumstances of extreme peril, 
as during a fire, when a man endeavors to save a fellow- 
creature without a moment’s hesitation, he can hardly feel 
pleasure; and still less has he time to reflect on the dissatis- 
faction which he might subsequently experience if he did 
not make the attempt. Should he afterward reflect over 
his own conduct, he would feel that there lies within him 
an impulsive power widely different from a search after 
pleasure or happiness; and this seems to be the deeply 
planted social instinct. 

In the case of the lower animals it seems much more 
appropriate to speak of their social instincts as having been 
developed for the general good rather than for the general 
happiness of the species. The term, general good, may be 
defined as the rearing of the greatest number of individuals 
in full vigor and health, with all their faculties perfect, 
under the conditions to which they are subjected. As the 
social instincts both of man and the lower animals have no 
doubt been developed by nearly the same steps, it would 


42 Mill recognizes (‘‘System of Logic,” vol. ii. p. 422) in the clearest man- 
ner, that actions may be performed through habit withcut the anticipation of 
pleasure. Mr. H. Sidgwick also, in his Essay on Pleasure and Desire (‘‘The 
. Contemporary Review,”’ April, 1872, p. 671), remarks: ‘*‘To sum up, in contra- 
vention of the doctrine that our conscious active impulses are always directed 
toward the production of agreeable sensations in ourselves, I would maintain 
that we find everywhere in consciousness extra-regarding impulse, directed 
toward something that is not pleasure; that in many cases the impulse is so far 
jncompatible with the self-regarding that the two do not easily coexist in the 
same moment of consciousness.’”? A dim feeling that our impulses do not by 
any means always arise from any contemporaneous or anticipated pleasure 
has, I cannot but think, been one chief cause of the acceptance of the intuitive 
theory of morality, and of the rejection of the utilitarian or “Greatest happi- 
ess’’ theory. With respect to the latter theory, the standard and the motive 
of conduct have no doubt often been confused, but they are really in some 
degree blended, 


164 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


be advisable, if found practicable, to use the same definition 
in both cases, and to take as the standard of morality the 
general good or welfare of the community, rather than 
the general happiness; but this definition would perhaps 
require some limitation on account of political ethics. 

When a man risks his life to save that of a fellow- 
creature, it seems also more correct to say that he acts for 
the general good, rather than for the general happiness of 
mankind. No doubt the welfare and the happiness of the 
individual usually coincide; and a contented, happy tribe 
will flourish better than one that is discontented and un- 
happy. We have seen that, even at an early period in the 
history of man, the expressed wishes of the community will 
have naturally influenced to a large extent the conduct of 
each member; and as all wish for happiness, the ‘‘Greatest 
happiness principle’ will have become a most important 
secondary guide and object; the social instinct, however, 
together with sympathy (which leads to our regarding the 
approbation and disapprobation of others), having served 
as the primary impulse and guide. Thus the reproach is 
removed of laying the foundation of the noblest part of our 
nature in the base principle of selfishness; unless, indeed, 
the satisfaction which every animal feels, when it follows 
its proper instincts, and the dissatisfaction felt when pre- 
vented, be called selfish. 

The wishes and opinions of the members of the same 
community, expressed at first orally, but later by writing 
also, either form the sole guides of our conduct or greatly 
reinforce the social instincts; such opinions, however, have 
sometimes a tendency directly opposed to these instincts. 
This latter fact is well exemplified by the Law of Honor, 
that is, the law of the opinion of our equals, and not of all 
our countrymen. The breach of this law, even when the 
breach is known to be strictly accordant with true morality, 
has caused many a man more agony than a real crime. We 
recognize the same influence in the burning sense of shame 
which most of us have felt, even after the interval of years, 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 165 


when calling to mind some accidental breach of a trifling, 
though fixed, rule of etiquette. The judgment of the com- 
munity will generally be guided by some rude experience 
of what is best in the long run for all the members; but this 
judgment will not rarely err from ignorance and weak 
powers of reasoning. Hence the strangest customs and 
superstitions, in complete opposition to the true welfare 
and happiness of mankind, have become all-powerful 
throughout the world. We see this in the horror felt by 
a Hindu who breaks his caste, and in many other such 
cases. It would be difficult to distinguish between the 
remorse felt by a Hindu who has yielded to the temptation 
of eating unclean food, from that felt after committing a 
theft; but the former would probably be the more severe, 
How so many absurd rules of conduct, as well as so 
many absurd religious beliefs, have originated, we do not 
know; nor how it is that they have become, in all quarters 
of the world, so deeply impressed on the mind of men; but 
it is worthy of remark that a belief constantly inculcated 
during the early years of life, while the brain is impressible, 
appears to acquire almost the nature of an instinct; and the 
very essence of an instinct is that it is followed independ- 
ently of reason. Neither can we say why certain admirable 
virtues, such as the love of truth, are much more highly 
appreciated by some savage tribes than by others; -nor, 
again, why similar differences prevail even among highly 
civilized nations. Knowing how firmly fixed many strange 
customs and superstitions have become, we need feel no 
surprise that the self-regarding virtues, supported as they 
are by reason, should now appear to us so natural as to be 
thought innate, although they were not valued by man 
in his early condition. 
. Notwithstanding many sources of doubt, man can gener- 
ally and readily distinguish between the higher and lower 


8 Good instances are given by Mr. Wallace in ‘‘Scientific Opinion,’’ Sept. 
15, 1869; and more fully in his ‘‘Contributions to the Theory of Natural Selee- 
tion,”’ 1870, p. 353. 


166 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


moral rules. The higher are founded on the social instincts, 
and relate to the welfare of others. They are supported by 
the approbation of our fellow-men and by reason. The lower 
rules, though some of them when implying self-sacrifice 
hardly deserve to be called lower, relate chiefly to self, and 
arise from public opinion, matured by experience and culti- 
vation; for they are not practiced by rude tribes. 

As man advances in civilization, and small tribes are 
united into larger communities, the simplest reason would 
tell each individual that he ought to extend his social in- 
stincts and sympathies to all the members of the same 
nation, though personally unknown to him. ‘This point 
being once reached, there is only an artificial barrier to pre- 
vent his sympathies extending to the men of all nations 
and races. If, indeed, such men are separated from him 
by great differences in appearance or habits, experience 
unfortunately shows us how long it is before we look at 
them as our fellow-creatures. Sympathy beyond the con- 
fines of man, that is, humanity to the lower animals, seems 
to be one of the latest moral acquisitions. It is apparently 
unfelt by savages, except toward their pets. How little the 
old Romans knew of it is shown by their abhorrent gladia- 
torial exhibitions. The very idea of humanity, as far as 
’ I could observe, was new to most of the Gauchos of the 
Pampas. This virtue, one of the noblest with which man 
is endowed, seems to arise incidentally from our sympathies 
becoming more tender and more widely diffused, until they 
are extended to all sentient beings. As soon as this virtue 
is honored and practiced by some few men, it spreads 
through instruction and example to the young, and event- 
ually becomes incorporated in public opinion. 

The highest possible stage in moral culture is when we 
recognize that we ought to control our thoughts, and ‘not 
even in inmost thought to think again the sins that made 
the past so pleasant to us.’’“* Whatever makes any bad 


“4 Tennyson, ‘‘Idylls of the King,”’ p. 244. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 167 


action familiar to the mind, renders its performance by so 
much the easier. As Marcus Aurelius long ago said, ‘‘Such 
as are thy habitual thoughts, such also will be the character 
of thy mind; for the soul is dyed by the thoughts.’’ *° 

Our great philosopher, Herbert Spencer, has recently 
explained his views on the moral sense. He says,‘ ‘‘I 
believe that the experiences of utility, organized and con- 
solidated through all past generations of the human race, 
have been producing corresponding modifications, which, 
by continued transmission and accumulation, have become 
in us certain faculties of moral intuition—certain emotions 
responding to right and wrong conduct, which have no 
apparent basis in the individual experiences of utility.” 
There is not the least inherent improbability, as it seems 
to me, in virtuous tendencies being more or less strongly 
inherited; for, not to mention the various dispositions and 
habits transmitted by many of our domestic animals to their 
offspring, I have heard of authentic cases in which a desire 
to steal and a tendency to lie appeared to run in families 
of the upper ranks; and as stealing is a rare crime in the 
wealthy classes, we can hardly account by accidental coin- 
cidence for the tendency occurring in two or three members 
of the same family. If bad tendencies are transmitted, it is 
probable that good ones are likewise transmitted. ‘That the 
state of the body, by affecting the brain, has great influence 
on the moral tendencies, is known to most of those who 
have suffered from chronic derangements of the digestion 
or liver. The same fact is likewise shown by the ‘‘perver- 
sion or destruction of the moral sense being often one of the 
earliest symptoms of mental derangement;’’ *” and insanity 
is notoriously often inherited. Except through the principle 
of the transmission of moral tendencies, we cannot under- 


45 “The Thoughts of the Emperor M. Aurelius Antoninus,’’ Eng. translat., 
2d edit., 1869, p. 112. Marcus Aurelius was born A.D. 121. . 

46 Letter to Mr. Mill in Bain’s ‘‘Mental and Moral Science,’’ 1868, 
p. 722. 

“| Maudsley, ‘‘Body and Mind,’’ 1870, p. 60. 


168 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


stand the differences believed to exist in this respect be- 
tween the various races of mankind. 

Even the partial transmission of virtuous tendencies 
would be an immense assistance to the primary impulse 
derived directly and indirectly from the social instincts. 
Admitting for a moment that virtuous tendencies are in- 
herited, it appears probable, at least in such cases as chas- 
tity, temperance, humanity to animals, etc., that they 
become first impressed on the mental organization through 
habit, instruction, and example, continued during several 
generations in the same family, and in a quite subordinate 
degree, or not at all, by the individuals possessing such 
virtues having succeeded best in the struggle for life. My 
chief source of doubt with respect to any such inheritance 
is that senseless customs, superstitions, and tastes, such as 
the horror of a Hindu for unclean food, ought on the same 
principle to be transmitted. I have not met with any evi- 
dence in support of the transmission of superstitious customs 
or senseless habits, although in itself it is perhaps not less 
probable than that animals should acquire inherited tastes 
for certain kinds of food or fear of certain foes. 

Finally the social instincts, which no doubt were ac- 
quired by man as by the lower animals for the good of the 
community, will from the first have given to him some wish 
to aid his fellows, some feeling of sympathy, and have com- 
pelled him to regard their approbation and disapprobation. 
Such impulses will have served him ata very early period 
as a rude rule of right and wrong. But as man gradually 
advanced in intellectual power, and was enabled to trace 
the more remote consequences of his actions; as he acquired 
sufficient knowledge to reject baneful customs and super- 
stitions; as he regarded more and more not only the welfare, 
but the happiness of his fellow-men; as from habit, follow- 
ing on beneficial experience, instruction, and example, his 
sympathies became more tender and widely diffused, ex- 
tending to men of all races, to the imbecile, maimed, and 
other useless members of society, and finally to the lower 


‘ 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 169 


animals—so would the standard of his morality rise higher 
and higher. And it is admitted by moralists of the deriva- 
tive school, and by some intuitionists, that the standard 
of morality has risen since an early period in the history 
of man.* : 

As a struggle may sometimes be seen going on between 
the various instincts of the lower animals, it is not surprising 
that there should be a struggle in‘man between his social 
instincts, with their derived virtues, and his lower, though 
momentarily stronger, impulses or desires. This, as Mr. 
Galton*® has remarked, is all the less surprising, as man has 
emerged from a state of barbarism within a comparatively 
recent period. After having yielded to some temptation we 
feel a sense of dissatisfaction, shame, repentance or remorse 
analogous to the feelings caused by other powerful instincts 
or desires, when left unsatisfied or balked. We compare 
the weakened impression of a past temptation with the ever- 
present social instincts, or with habits gained in early youth 
and strengthened during our whole lives, until they have 
become almost as strong as instincts. If with the temptation 
still before us we do not yield, it is because either the social 
instinct or some custom is at the moment predominant, or 
because we have learned that it will appear to us hereafter 
the stronger, when compared with the weakened impression 
of the temptation, and we realize that its violation would 
cause us suffering. Looking to future generations, there is 
no cause to fear that the social instincts will grow weaker, 
and we may expect that virtuous habits will grow stronger, 
becoming perhaps fixed by inheritance. In this case the 
struggle between our higher and lower impulses will be 
less severe, and virtue will be triumphant. 


48 A writer in the ‘North British Review”’ (July, 1869, p. 531), well capable 
of forming a sound judgment, expresses himself strongly i in favor of this con- 
clusion. Mr. Lecky (‘‘Hist. of Morals,’’ vol. i. p. 143) seems to a certain extent 
to coincide therein. 

49 See his remarkable work on ‘‘Hereditary Genius,’ 1869, p. 349. The 
Duke of Argyll (‘Primeval Man,’’ 1869, p. 188) has some good remarks on 
the contest in man’s nature between right and wrong. 


Descent—Vo.L. I.—8 


170 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


Summary of the last two OChapters.—There can be no 
doubt that the difference between the mind of the lowest 
man and that of the highest animal is immense. An anthro- 
pomorphous ape, if he could take a dispassionate view of 
his own case, would admit that, though he could form 
an artful plan to plunder a garden—though he could use 
stones for fighting or for breaking open nuts—yet that the 
thought of fashioning a stone into a tool was quite beyond 
his scope. Still less, as he would admit, could he follow 
out a train of metaphysical reasoning, or solve a mathe- 
matical problem, or reflect on God, or admire a grand 
natural scene. Some apes, however, would probably de- 
clare that they could and did admire the beauty of the 
colored skin and fur of their partners .in marriage. They 
would admit that, though they could make other apes 
understand by cries some of their perceptions and simpler 
wants, the notion of expressing definite ideas by definite 
sounds had never crossed their minds. They might insist 
that they were ready to aid their fellow-apes of the same 
troop in many ways, to risk their lives for them, and to 
take charge of their orphans; but they would be forced 
to acknowledge that disinterested love for all living crea- 
tures, the most noble attribute of man, was quite beyond 
their comprehension. 

Nevertheless the difference in mind between man and 
the higher animals, great as it is, certainly is one of degree 
and not of kind. We have seen that the senses and intui- 
tions, the various emotions and faculties, such as love, 
memory, attention, curiosity, imitation, reason, etc., of 
which man boasts, may be found in an incipient, or even 
sometimes in a well-developed condition, in the lower ani- 
mals. They are also capable of some inherited improve- 
ment, as we see in the domestic dog compared with the’ 
wolf or jackal. If it could be proved that certain high 
mental powers, such as the formation of general concepts, 
self-consciousness, etc., were absolutely peculiar to man, 
which seems extremely doubtful, it is not improbable that 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 171 


these qualities are merely the incidental results of other 
highly advanced intellectual faculties; and these again 
mainly the result of the continued use of a perfect lan- 
guage. At what age does the new-born infant possess the 
power of abstraction, or become self-conscious, and reflect 
on its own existence? We cannot answer; nor can we 
answer in regard to the ascending organic scale. The half- 
art, half-instinct of language still bears the stamp of its 
gradual evolution. The ennobling belief in God is not 
universal with man; and the belief in spiritual agencies 
naturally follows from other mental powers. The moral 
sense perhaps affords the best and highest distinction be- 
tween man and the lower animals; but I need say nothing 
on this head, as I have so lately endeavored to show that 
the social instincts—the prime principle of man’s moral con- 
stitution®—with the aid of active intellectual powers and 
the effects of habit, naturally lead to the golden rule, ‘‘As 
ye would that men should do to you, do ye to them like- 
wise’; and this lies at the foundation of morality. 

In the next chapter I shall make some few remarks on 
the probable steps and means by which the several mental 
and moral faculties of man have been gradually evolved. 
That such evolution is at least possible ought not to be 
denied, for we daily see these faculties developing in every 
infant; and we may trace a perfect gradation from the mind 
of an utter idiot, lower than that of an animal low in the 
scale, to the mind of a Newton. 


60 “The Thoughts of Marcus Aurelius,”’ etc., p. 139, 


172 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


CHAPTER V 


ON THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE INTELLECTUAL AND MORAL 
FACULTIES DURING PRIMEVAL AND CIVILIZED TIMES 


Advancement of the intellectual powers through natural selection—Impor- 
tance of imitation—Social and moral faculties—Their development 
within the limits of the same tribe—Natural selection as affecting 
civilized nations—Evidence that civilized nations were once barbarous 


HE subjects to be discussed in this chapter are of the 
T highest interest, but are treated by me in an imper- 
fect and fragmentary manner. Mr. Wallace, in an 
‘admirable paper before referred to,’ argues that man, after 
he had partially acquired those intellectual and moral facul- 
ties which distinguish him from the lower animals, would 
have been but little liable to bodily modifications through 
natural selection or any other means. For man is enabled 
through his mental -faculties ‘‘to keep with an unchanged 
body in harmony with the changing universe.’’ He has 
great power of adapting his habits to new conditions of 
life. He invents weapons, tools, and various stratagems 
to procure food and to defend himself. When he migrates 
into a colder climate he uses clothes, builds sheds, and 
makes fires; and by the aid of fire cooks food otherwise 
indigestible. He aids his fellow-men in many ways, and 
anticipates future events. Hven at a remote period he prac- 
ticed some division of labor. 

The lower animals, on the other hand, must have their 
bodily structure modified in order to survive under greatly 
changed conditions. They must be rendered stronger, or 
acquire more effective teeth or claws, for defence against 
new enemies; or they must be reduced in size, so as to 


® “Anthropological Review,” May 1864, p. elviii. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF yan 175 


escape detection and danger. When they migray, there 
colder climate, they must become clothed with ~ ther 
fur, or have their constitutions altered. If they fail to’. 
thus modified, they will cease to exist. 

The case, however is widely different, as Mr. Wales. 
has with justice insisted, in relation to the intellectual and 
moral faculties of man. These faculties are variable; 
and we have every reason to believe that the variations 
tend to be inherited. Therefore, if they were formerly of 
high importance to primeval man and to his ape-like pro- 
genitors, they would have been perfected or advanced 
through natural selection. Of the high importance of the 
intellectual faculties there can be no doubt, for man mainly 
owes to them his predominant position in the world. We 
can see that, in the rudest state of society, the individuals 
who were the most sagacious, who invented and used the 
best weapons or traps, and who were best able to defend 
themselves, would rear the greatest number of offspring. 
The tribes which included the largest number of men thus 
endowed would increase in number and supplant other 
tribes. Numbers depend primarily on the means of subsist- 
ence, and this depends partly on the physical nature of the 
country, but in a much higher degree on the arts which are 
there practiced. As a tribe increases and is victorious, it 
is often still further increased by the absorption of other 
tribes.? The stature and strength of the men of a tribe are 
likewise of some importance for its success, and these de- 
pend in part on the nature and amount of the food which 
can be obtained. In Europe the men of the Bronze period 
were supplanted by a race more powerful, and, judging 
from their sword-handles, with larger hands;* but their 
success was probably still more due to their superiority 
in the arts. 


2 After a time the members or tribes which are absorbed into another tribe 
assume, as Sir Henry Maine remarks (‘‘Ancient Law,’’ 1861, p. 131), that they 
are the co-descendants of the same ancestors. 

3 Morlot, ‘‘Soc, Vaud. Sc. Nat.,’’ 1860, p. 294, 


172 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


that we know about savages, or may infer from their 

_vns and from old monuments, the history of which is 

ite forgotten by the present inhabitants, show that from 
the remotest times successful tribes have supplanted other 
_ tribes. Relics of extinct or forgotten tribes have. been dis- 
covered throughout the civilized regions of the earth, on the 
wild plains of America, and on the isolated islands in 
the Pacific Ocean. At the present day civilized nations 
are everywhere supplanting barbarous nations, excepting 
where the climate opposes a deadly barrier; and they suc- 
ceed mainly, though not exclusively, through their arts, 
which are the products of the intellect. It is, therefore, 
highly probable that with mankind the intellectual faculties 
have been mainly and gradually perfected through natural 
selection; and this conclusion is sufficient for our purpose. 
Undoubtedly it would be interesting to trace the develop- 
ment of each separate faculty from the state in which it 
exists in the lower animals to that in which it exists in man; 
but neither my ability nor knowledge permits the attempt. 
It deserves notice that, as soon as the progenitors of man 
became social (and this probably occurred at a very early 
period), the principle of imitation, and reason, and experi- 
ence would have increased, and much modified the intel- 
lectual powers in a way of which we see only traces in the 
lower animals. Apes are much given to imitation, as are 
the lowest savages; and the simple fact previously referred 
to, that after a time no animal can be caught in the same 
place by the same sort of trap, shows that animals learn by 
experience, and imitate the caution of others. Now, if some 
one man in a tribe, more sagacious than the others, invented 
a new snare or weapon, or other means of attack or defence, 
the plainest self-interest, without the assistance of much rea- 
soning power, would prompt the other members to imitate 
him; and all would thus profit. The habitual practice of 
each new art must likewise in some slight degree strengthen 
the intellect. If the new invention were an important one, 
the tribe would increase in number, spread, and supplant 


tw 


| 
THE DESCENT OR an OF MAN 175 
oa 


other tribes. In a tribe thus rendered more numerous there 
would always be a rather greater chance of the birth of other 
superior and inventive members. If such men left children 
to inherit their mental superiority, the chance of the birth of 
still more ingenious members would be somewhat better, and 
in a very small tribe decidedly better. Even if they left no 
children, the tribe would still include their blood-relations; 
and it has been ascertained by agriculturists* that by preserv- 
ing and breeding from the family of an animal which, when 
slaughtered, was found to be valuable, the desired character 
has been obtained. 


Turning now to the social and moral faculties. In order 
that primeval men, or the ape-like progenitors of man, should 
become social, they musf have acquired the same instinctive 
feelings which impel other animals to live in a body; and 
they no doubt exhibited the same general disposition. They 


_ would have felt uneasy when separated from their comrades, 


for whom they would have felt some degree of love; they 
would have warned each other of danger, and have given 
mutual aid in attack or defence. All this implies some de- 
gree of sympathy, fidelity, and courage. Such social quali- 
ties, the paramount importance of which to the lower ani- 
mals is disputed by no one, were no doubt acquired by the 
progenitors of man in a similar manner, namely, through | 
natural selection, aided by inherited habit. When two 
tribes of primeval man, living in the same country, came 


into competition, if (other circumstances being equal) the 


one tribe included a great number of courageous, sympa- 
thetic, and faithful members, who were always ready to 
warn each other of danger, to aid and defend each other, 
this tribe would succeed better and conquer the other. Let 
it be borne in mind how all-important, in the never-ceasing 
wars of savages, fidelity and courage must be. The ad- 


4 T have given instances in my ‘‘Variation of Animals under Domestication,” 
vol. ii. p. 196. 


176 THE, + "SCENT OF MAN 
1 


vantage which disciplined soldiers have over undisciplined 
hordes follows chiefly from the confidence which each man 
feels in his comrades. Obedience, as Mr. Bagehot has well 
shown, is of the highest value, for any form of government 
is better than none. Selfish and contentious people will not 
cohere, and without coherence nothing can be effected. A 
tribe rich in the above qualities would spread and be victo- 
rious over other tribes; but in the course of time it would, 
judging from all past history, be in its turn overcome by 
some other tribe still more highly endowed. Thus the so- 
cial and moral qualities would tend slowly to advance and 
be diffused throughout the world. 

But it may be asked, how within the limits of the same 
tribe did a large number of members first become endowed 
with these social and moral qualities, and how was the stand- 
ard of excellence raised? Itis extremely doubtful whether 
the offspring of the more sympathetic and benevolent par- 
ents, or of those who were the most faithful to their com- 
rades, would be reared in greater numbers than the children 
of selfish and treacherous parents belonging to the same 
tribe. He who was ready to sacrifice his life, as many a 
savage has been, rather than betray his comrades, would 
often leave no offspring to inherit his noble nature. The 
bravest men, who were always willing to come to the front 
in war, and who freely risked their lives for others, would 
on an average perish in larger numbers than other men. 
Therefore it hardly seems probable that the number of men 
gifted with such virtues, or that the standard of their excel- 
lence, could be increased through natural selection, that is, 
by the survival of the fittest; for we are not here speaking 
of one tribe being victorious over another. 

Although the circumstances leading to an increase in the 
number of those thus endowed within the same tribe are 


5 See a remarkable series of articles on ‘‘Physics and Politics,’ in the 
“Fortnightly Review,’’? Nov. 1867; April 1, 1868; July 1, 1869, since sepa- 
rately published. 


t 
f 


| 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 177 


too complex to be clearly followed out, we can trace some 
of the probable steps. In the first place, as the reasoning 
powers and foresight of the members became improved, each 
man would soon learn that if he aided his fellow-men, he 
would commonly receive aid in return. From this low 
motive he might acquire the habit of aiding his fellows; 
and the habit of performing benevolent actions certainly 
strengthens the feeling of sympathy which gives the first 
impulse to benevolent actions. Habits, moreover, followed 
during many generations, probably tend to be inherited. 

But another and much more powerful: stimulus to the 
development of the social virtues is afforded by the praise 
and the blame of our fellow-men. To the instinct of sym- 
pathy, as we have already seen, it is primarily due that we 
habitually bestow both praise and blame on others, while 
we love the former and dread the latter when applied to our- 
selves; and this instinct no doubt was originally acquired, 
like all the other social instincts, through natural selection. 
At how early a period the progenitors of man, in the course 
of their development, became capable of feeling and being 
impelled by the praise or blame of their fellow-creatures, 
we cannot, of course, say. But it appears that even dogs 
appreciate encouragement, praise, and blame. The rudest 
savages feel the sentiment of glory, as they clearly show 
by preserving the trophies of their prowess, by their habit 
of excessive boasting, and even by the extreme care which 
they take of their personal appearance and decorations; for 
unless they regarded the opinion of their comrades, such 
habits would be senseless. 

They certainly feel shame at the breach of some of their 
lesser rules, and apparently remorse, as shown by the case 
of the Australian who grew thin and could not rest from 
having delayed to murder some other woman, so as to pro- 
Pitiate his dead wife’s spirit. Though I have not met with 
any other recorded case, it is scarcely credible that a savage 
who will sacrifice his life rather than betray his tribe, or 
one who will deliver himself up as a prisoner rather than 


178 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


break his parole,* would not feel remorse in his inmost 
soul if he had failed in a duty which he held sacred. 

We may therefore conclude that primeval man, at a very 
remote period, was influenced by the praise and blame of his 
fellows. It is obvious that the members of the same tribe 
would approve of conduct which appeared to them to be for 
the general good, and would reprobate that which appeared 
evil. Todo good unto others—to do unto others as ye would 
they should do unto you—is the foundation-stone of moral- 
ity. It is, therefore, hardly possible to exaggerate the im- 
portance, during rude times, of the love of praise and the 
dread of blame. A man who was not impelled by any deep, 
instinctive feeling to sacrifice his life for the good of others, 
yet was roused to such actions by a sense of glory, would 
by his example excite the same wish for glory in other men, 
and would strengthen by exercise the noble feeling of ad- 
miration. He might thus do far more good to his tribe than 
by begetting offspring with a tendency to inherit his own 
high character. 

With increased experience and reason, man perceives the 
more remote consequences of his actions, and the self-regard- 
ing virtues, such as temperance, chastity, etc., which during 
early times are, as we have before seen, utterly disregarded, 
come to be highly esteemed or even held sacred. I need 
not, however, repeat what I have said on this head in the 
fourth chapter. Ultimately our moral sense or conscience 
becomes a highly complex sentiment—originating in the 
social instincts, largely guided by the approbation of our 
fellow-men, ruled by reason, self-interest, and in later times 
by deep religious feelings, and confirmed by instruction and 
habit. 

It must not be forgotten that although a high standard 
of morality gives but a slight or no advantage to each indi- 
vidual man and his children over the other men of the same 


6 Mr. Wallace gives cases in his ‘‘Contributions to the Theory of Natural 
Selection,’’ 1870, p. 354. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 179 


tribe, yet that an increase in the number of well-endowed 
men and an advancement in the standard of morality will 
certainly give an immense advantage to one tribe over an- 
other. A tribe including many members who, from pos- 
sessing in a high degree the spirit of patriotism, fidelity, 
obedience, courage, and sympathy, were always ready to 
aid one another, and to sacrifice themselves for the common 
good, would be victorious over most other tribes; and this 
would be natural selection. At all times throughout the 
world tribes have supplanted other tribes; and as morality 
is one important element in their success, the standard of 
morality and the number of well-endowed men will thus 
everywhere tend to rise and increase. 

It is, however, very difficult to form any judgment why 
one particular tribe and not another has been successful 
and has risen in the scale of civilization. Many savages 
are in the same condition as when first discovered several 
centuries ago. As Mr. Bagehot has remarked, we are apt 
to look at progress as normal in human society; but history 
refutes this. The ancients did not even entertain the idea 
nor do the Oriental nations at the present day. According 
to another high authority, Sir Henry Maine,’ “the greatest 
part of mankind has never shown a particle of desire that 
its civil institutions should be improved.’’ Progress seems 
to depend on many concurrent favorable conditions, far toa 
complex to be followed out. But it has often been remarked 
that a cool climate, from leading to industry and to the vari- 
ous arts, has been highly favorable thereto. The Eskimos, 
pressed by hard necessity, have succeeded in many ingen- 
ious inventions, but their climate has been too severe for 
continued progress. Nomadic habits, whether over wide 
plains or through the dense forests of the tropics, or along 
the shores of the sea, have in every case been highly detri- 
mental. While observing the barbarous inhabitants of Tierra 


1 “Ancient Law,’? 1861, p. 22. For Mr. Bagehot’s remarks, ‘‘Fortnightly 
Review,’ April 1, 1868, p. 452. 


180 | THE DESCENT OF MAN 


del Fuego, it struck me that the possession of some property, 
a fixed abode, and the union of many families under a chief, 
were the indispensable requisites for civilization. Such hab- 
its almost necessitate the cultivation of the ground; and the 
first steps in cultivation would probably result, as I have 
elsewhere shown,® from some such accident as the seeds of 
a fruit-tree falling on a heap of refuse, and producing an 
unusually fine variety. The problem, however, of the first 
advance of savages toward civilization is at present much 
too difficult to be solved. 


Natural Selection as Affecting Civilized Nations.—I have 
hitherto only considered the advancement of man from a 
semi-human condition to that of the modern savage. But 
some remarks on the action of natural selection on civilized 
nations may be worth adding. This subject has been ably 
discussed by Mr. W. R. Greg,°® and previously by Mr. Wal- 
lace and Mr. Galton.’ Most of my remarks are taken from 
these three authors. With savages, the weak in body or 
mind are soon eliminated; and those that survive commonly 
exhibit a vigorous state of health. We civilized men, on 
the other hand, do our utmost to check the process of elimi- 
nation; we build asylums for the imbecile, the maimed, and 
the sick; we institute poor-laws; and our medical men exert 
their utmost skill to save the life of every one to the last 
moment. There is reason to believe that vaccination has 
preserved thousands who from a weak constitution would 


8 “The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,’’ vol. i, 
» BOD, 
9 ‘‘Praser’s Magazine,’’ Sept. 1868, p. 353. This article seems to have 
struck many persons, and has given rise to two remarkable essays and a re- 
joinder in the ‘‘Spectator,’’ Oct. 3d and 17th, 1868. It has also been discussed 
in the ‘‘Q. Journal of Science,’’ 1869, p. 152, and by Mr. Lawson Tait in the 
“Dublin Q. Journal of Medical Science,’’ Feb. 1869, and by Mr. E. Ray 
Lankester in his ‘‘Comparative Longevity,’’? 1870, p. 128. Similar views ap- 
peared previously in the ‘‘Australasian,”? July*13, 186%. I have borrowed 
ideas from several of these writers. ‘ 

10 For Mr. Wallace, see ‘‘Anthropolog. Review,’’ us before cited. Mr. 
Galton in ‘‘Macmillan’s Magazine,’? Aug. 1865, p. 318; also his great work, 
‘Hereditary Genius,’’ 1870. 


’ 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 181 


formerly have succumbed to small-pox. Thus the weak 
members of civilized societies propagate their kind. No 
one who has attended to the breeding of domestic animals 
will doubt that this must be highly injurious to the race 
of man. It is surprising how soon a want of care, or care 
wrongly directed, leads to the degeneration of a domestic 
race; but, excepting in the case of man himself, hardly any 
one is so ignorant as to allow his worst animals to breed. 

The aid which we feel impelled to give to the helpless 
is mainly an incidental result of the instinct of sympathy, 
which was originally acquired as part of the social instincts, 
but subsequently rendered, in the manner previously indi- 
eated, more tender and more widely diffused. Nor could 
we check our sympathy, even at the urging of hard reason, 
without deterioration in the noblest part of our nature. The 
surgeon may harden himself while performing an operation, 
for he knows that he is acting for the good of his patient; 
but if we were intentionally to neglect the weak and help- 
less, it could only be for a contingent benefit, with an over- 
whelming present evil. We must therefore bear the un- 
doubtedly bad effects of the weak surviving and propagating 
their kind; but there appears to be at least one check in 
steady action, namely, that the weaker and inferior mem- 
bers of society do not marry so freely as the sound; and 
this check might be indefinitely increased by the weak in 
body or mind refraining from marriage, though this is more 
to be hoped for than expected. 

In every country in which a large standing army is kept 
up, the finest young men are taken by the conscription or 
are enlisted. They are thus exposed to early death during 
war, are often tempted into vice, and are prevented from 
marrying during the prime of life. On the other hand, the 
shorter and feebler men, with poor constitutions, are left 
at home, and consequently have a much better chance of 
marrying and propagating their kind.” 


" Prof. H. Fick (‘‘Einfluss der Naturwissenschaft auf das Recht,’”? June, 
1872) has some good remarks on this head, and on other such points. 


182 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


Man accumulates property and bequeaths it to his chil- 
dren, so that the children of the rich have an advantage 
over the poor in the race for success, independently of 
bodily or mental superiority. On the other hand, the chil- 
dren of parents who are short-lived, and are therefore on 
an average deficient in health and vigor, come into. their 
property sooner than other children, and will be likely to 
marry earlier, and leave a larger number of offspring to 
inherit their inferior constitutions. But the inheritance 
of property by itself is very far from an evil; for without 
the accumulation of capital the arts could not progress; and 
it is chiefly through their power that the civilized races have 
extended, and are now everywhere extending their range, so 
as to take the place of the lower races. Nor does the mod- 
erate accumulation of wealth interfere with the process of 
selection. When a poor man becomes moderately rich, his 
children enter trades or professions in which there is strug- 
gle enough, so that the able in body and mind succeed best. 
The presence of a body of well-instructed men, who have 
not to labor for their daily bread, is important to a degree 
which cannot be over-estimated; as all high intellectual 
work is carried on by them, and on such work material 
progress of all kinds mainly depends, not to mention other 
and higher advantages. No doubt wealth when very great 
tends to convert men into useless drones, but their number 
is never large; and some degree of elimination here occurs, 
for we daily see rich men, who happen to be fools or profli- 
gate, squandering away their wealth. 

Primogeniture with entailed estates is a more direct evil, 
though it may formerly have been a great advantage by the 
creation of a dominant class, and any government is better 
than none. Most eldest sons, though they may be weak in 
body or mind, marry, while the younger sons, however supe- 
rior in these respects, do not so generally marry. Nor can 
worthless eldest sons with entailed estates squander their 
wealth. But here, as elsewhere, the relations of civilized 
life are so complex that some compensatory checks inter- 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 183 


vene. The men who are rich through primogeniture are 
able to select generation after generation the more beautiful 
and charming women; and these must generally be healthy 
in body and active in mind. The evil consequences, such 
as they may be, of the continued preservation of the same 
line of descent, without any selection, are checked by men 
of rank always wishing to increase their wealth and power; 
and this they effect by marrying heiresses. But the daugh: 
ters of parents who have produced single children are them- 
selves, as Mr. Galton’? has shown, apt to be sterile; and thus 
noble families are continually cut off in the direct line, and 
their wealth flows into some side channel; but, unfortu- 
nately, this channel is not determined by superiority of any 
kind. 

Although civilization thus checks in many ways the 
action of natural selection, it apparently favors the better 
development of the body, by means of good food and the 
freedom from occasional hardships. This may be inferred 
from civilized men having been found, wherever compared, 
to be physically stronger than savages.** They appear also 
to have equal powers of endurance, as has been proved in 
many adventurous expeditions. Even the great luxury of 
the rich can be but little detrimental, for the expectation 
of life of our aristocracy, at all ages and of both sexes, is 
very little inferior to that of healthy English lives in the 
lower classes. ** 

We will now look to the intellectual faculties. If in each 
grade of society the members were divided into two equal 
bodies, the one including the intellectually superior and the 
other the inferior, there can be little doubt that the former 
would succeed best in all occupations, and rear a greater 
number of children. Even in the lowest walks of life, skill 
and ability must be of some advantage; though in many oc- 


2 “‘Hereditary Genius,’? 1870, pp. 132-140. 

18 Quatrefages, ‘‘Revue des Cours Scientifiques,’’ 1867-68, p. 659. 

14 See the fifth and sixth columns, compiled from good authorities, in the 
table given in Mr. E. R. Lankester’s ‘‘Comparative Longevity,’’ 1870, p. 115. 


184 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


cupations, owing to the great division of labor, a very small 
one. Hence in civilized nations there will be some tendency 
to an increase both in the number and in the standard of the 
intellectually able. But I do not wish to assert that this 
tendency may not be more than counterbalanced in other 
ways, as by the multiplication of the reckless and improvi- 
dent; but even to such as these, ability must be some 
advantage. 

It has often been objected to views like the foregoing, 
that the most eminent men who have ever lived have left no 
offspring to inherit their great intellect. Mr. Galton says,*® 
“T regret I am unable to solve the simple question whether, 
and how far, men and women who are prodigies of genius 
are infertile. I have, however, shown that men of eminence 
are by no means so.’’ Great lawgivers, the founders of 
beneficent religions, great philosophers and discoverers in 
science, aid the progress of mankind in a far higher degree 
by their works than by leaving a numerous progeny. In 
the case of corporeal structures it is the selection of the 
slightly better-endowed and the elimination of the slightly 
less well-endowed individuals, and not the preservation of 
strongly marked and rare anomalies, that leads to the ad- 
vancement of a species.’* So it will be with the intellec- 
tual faculties, since the somewhat abler men in each grade 
of society succeed rather better than the less able, and con- 
sequently increase in number, if not otherwise prevented. 
When in any nation the standard of intellect and the num- 
ber of intellectual men have increased, we may expect, from 
the law of the deviation from an average, that prodigies of 
genius will, as shown by Mr. Galton, appear somewhat more 
frequently than before. 

In regard to the moral qualities, some elimination of the 
worst dispositions is always in progress, even in the most 
civilized nations. Malefactors are executed, or imprisoned 


16 “‘Hereditary Genius,’’ 1870, p. 330. 
16 “Origin of Species’’ (fifth edition, 1869), p. 104. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 185 


for long periods, so that they cannot freely transmit their 
bad qualities. Melancholic and insane persons are confined 
or commit suicide. Violent and quarrelsome men often 
come to a bloody end. The restless who will not follow 
any steady occupation—and this relic of barbarism is a great 
check to civilization’’—emigrate to newly settled countries, 
where they prove useful pioneers. Intemperance is so highly 
destructive, that the expectation of life of the intemperate, 
at the age of thirty for instance, is only 18.8 years; while 
for the rural laborers of England at the same age it is 40.59 
years.’* Profligate women bear few children, and profligate 
men rarely marry; both suffer from disease. In the breed- 
ing of domestic animals, the elimination of those individuals, 
though few in number, which are in any marked manner in- 
ferior, is by no means an unimportant element toward suc- 
cess. This especially holds good with injurious characters 
which tend to reappear through reversion, such as blackness 
in sheep; and with mankind some of the worst dispositions, 
which occasionally without any assignable cause make their 
appearance in families, may perhaps be reversions to a sav- 
age state, from which we are not removed by very many 
generations. This, view seems indeed recognized in the 
common expression that such men are the black sheep 
of the family. 

With civilized nations, as far as an advanced standard 
of morality and an increased number of fairly good men 
are concerned, natural selection apparently effects but little; 
though the fundamental social instincts were originally thus 
gained. But I have already said enough, while treating of 
the lower races, on the causes which lead to the advance 
of morality, namely, the approbation of our fellow-men— 
the strengthening of our sympathies by habit—example 


" ‘Hereditary Genius,’ 1870, p. 347. 

18 E, Ray Lankester, ‘‘Comparative Longevity,’? 1870, p. 115. The table 
of the intemperate is from Neison’s ‘‘Vital Statistics.’? In regard to profligacy, 
see Dr. Farr, ‘‘Influence of Marriage on Mortality,” ‘‘Nat. Assoc. for the 
Promotion of Social Science,’’ 1858. 


186 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


and imitation—reason—experience, and even self-interest 
—instruction during youth, and religious feelings. 

A most important obstacle in civilized countries to an 
increase in the number of men of a superior class has been 
strongly insisted on by Mr. Greg and Mr. Galton,” namely, 
the fact that the very poor and reckless, who are often de- 
graded by vice, almost invariably marry early, while the 
careful and frugal, who are generally otherwise virtuous, 
marry late in life, so that they may be able to support them- 
selves and their children in comfort. Those who marry early 
produce within a given period not only a greater number of 
generations, but, as shown by Dr. Duncan,” they produce 
many more children. The children, moreover, that are born 
by mothers during the prime of life are heavier and larger, 
and therefore probably more vigorous, than those born at 
other periods. Thus the reckless, degraded, and often 
vicious members of society tend to increase at a quicker 
rate than the provident and generally virtuous members. 
Or as Mr. Greg puts the case: ‘‘The careless, squalid, un- 
aspiring Irishman multiplies like rabbits: the frugal, fore- 
seeing, self-respecting, ambitious Scot, stern in his morality, 
spiritual in his faith, sagacious and disciplined in his intel- 
ligence, passes his best years in struggle and in celibacy, 
marries late, and leaves few behind him. Given a land 
originally peopled by a thousand Saxons and a thousand 
Celts—and in a dozen generations five-sixths of the popu- 
lation would be Celts, but five-sixths of the property, of 
the power, of the intellect, would belong to the one-sixth 
of Saxons that remained. In the eternal ‘struggle for ex- 
istence,’ it would be the inferior and less favored race that 


19 “‘Frager’s Magazine,’? Sept. 1868, p. 353. ‘‘Macmillan’s Magazine,”’ 
Aug. 1865, p. 318. The Rev. F. W. Farrar (“‘Fraser’s Mag.,’? Aug. 1830, 
p. 264) lakes a different view. 

20 ‘On the Laws of the Fertility of Women,”’ in ‘‘Transact. Royal Soc.,”’ 
Edinburgh, vol. xxiv. p. 287; now published separately under the title of 
“Fecundity, Fertility, and Sterility,’? 1871. See, also, Mr. Galton, “Hereditary 
Genius,” pp. 352-357, for observations to the above effect. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 187 


44d prevailed—and prevailed by virtue not of its good 
qualities, but of its faults.’ 

There are, however, some checks to this downward ten- 
dency. We have seen that the intemperate suffer from a 
high rate of mortality, and the extremely profligate leave 
few offspring. The poorest classes crowd into towns, and 
it has been proved by Dr. Stark, from the statistics of ten 
years in Scotland,” that at all ages the death-rate is higher 
in towns than in rural districts, ‘‘and during the first five 
years of life the town death-rate is almost exactly double 
that of the rural districts.’’ As these returns include both 
the rich and the poor, no doubt more than twice the number 
of births would be requisite to keep up the number of the 
very poor inhabitants in the towns, relatively to those in 
the country. With women, marriage at too early an age 
is highly injurious; for it has been found in France that, 
“twice as many wives under twenty die in the year, as died 
out of the same number of the unmarried.’’ The mortality, 
also, of husbands under twenty is ‘‘excessively high,’’ but 
what the cause of this may be, seems doubtful. Lastly, if 
the men who prudently delay marrying until they can bring 
up their families in comfort were to select, as they often do, 
women in the prime of life, the rate of increase in the better 
class would be only slightly lessened. 

It was established from an enormous body of statistics, 
taken during 1853, that the unmarried men throughout 
France, between the ages of twenty and eighty, die in a 
much larger proportion than the married; for instance, out 
of every 1,000 unmarried men, between the ages of twenty 
and thirty, 11.8 annually died, while of the married only 
6.5 died.” A similar law was proved to hold good, during 


21 *‘Tenth Annual Report of Births, Deaths, etc., in Scotland,’’ 1867, 
. XXX. 

ane These quotations are taken from our highest authority on such questions, 
namely Dr, Fair, in his paper ‘‘On the Influence of Marriage on the Mortality 
of the French People,” read before the Nat. Assoc. for the Promotion of Social 
Science, 1858.i: 

% Dr, Farr, ib. The quotations given below are extracted from the same 
ee paper. 


188 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


the years 1863 and 1864, with the entire population above the 
age of twenty in Scotland; for instance, out of every 1,000 
unmarried men, between the ages of twenty and thirty, 
14.97 annually died, while of the married only 7.24 died, 
that is less than half.* Dr. Stark remarks on this, 
‘‘Bachelorhood is more destructive to life than the most 
unwholesome trades, or than residence in an unwholesome 
house or district where there has never been the most dis- 
tant attempt at sanitary improvement.’’ He considers that 
the lessened mortality is the direct result of ‘‘marriage, and 
the more regular domestic habits which attend that state.” 
He admits, however, that the intemperate, profligate, and 
criminal classes, whose duration of life is low, do not com- 
monly marry; and it must likewise be admitted that men 
with a weak constitution, ill-health, or any great infirmity 
in body or mind, will often not wish to marry, or will be 
rejected. Dr. Stark seems to have come to the conclusion 
that marriage in itself is a main cause of prolonged life, 
from finding that aged married men still have a considerable 
advantage in this respect over the unmarried of the same 
advanced age; but every one must have known instances of 
men who, with weak health during ‘outh, did not marry, 
and yet have survived to old age, though remaining weak, 
and therefore always with a lessened chance of life or of 
marrying. There is another remarkable circumstance which 
seems to support Dr. Stark’s conclusion, namely, that 
widows and widowers in France suffer in comparison with 
the married a very heavy rate of mortality; but Dr. Farr 
attributes this to the poverty and evil habits consequent on 
the disruption of the family, and to grief. On the whole, 
we may conclude with Dr. Farr that the lesser mortality 
of married than of unmarried men, which seems to be a 
general law, ‘‘is mainly due to the constant elimination of 


*4 T have taken the mean of the quinquennial means, given in ‘‘The Tenth 
Annual Report of Births, Deaths, etc., in Scotland, ”? 186%, The Srotation from 
Dr. Stark is copied from an article in the “Daily News,” Octob ir 117, 1868, 


which Dr. Farr considers very carefully written. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 189 


imperfect types, and to the skilful selection of the finest 
individuals out of each successive generation’’; the selection 
relating only to the married state, and acting on all cor- 
poreal, intellectual, and moral qualities.» We may, there- 
fore, infer that sound and good men who, out of prudence, 
remain for a time unmarried, do not suffer a high rate of 
mortality. $ 

If the various checks specified in’the last two paragraphs, 
and perhaps others as yet unknown, do not prevent the 
reckless, the vicious, and otherwise inferior members of 
society from increasing at a quicker rate than the better 
class of men, the nation will retrograde, as has too often 
occurred in the history of the world. We must remember - 
that progress is no invariable rule. It is very difficult to 
say why one civilized nation rises, becomes more powerful, 
and spreads more widely, than another; or why the same 
nation progresses more quickly at one time than at another. 
We can only say that it depends on an increase in the actual 
number of the population, on the number of the men en- 
dowed with high intellectual and moral faculties, as well 
as on their standard of excellence. Corporeal structure ap- 
pears to have little inflzence, except so far as vigor of body 
leads to vigor of mind. 

It has been urged by several writers, that as high intel- 
lectual powers are advantageous to a nation, the old Greeks, 
who stood some grades higher in intellect than any race that 
has ever existed,” ought, if the power of natural selection 
were real, to have risen still higher in the scale, increased in 
number, and stocked the whole of Europe. Here we have 
the tacit assumption, so often made with respect to corporeal 
structures, that there is some innate tendency toward con- 


% Dr, Duncan remarks (‘‘Fecundity, Fertility,’? etc., 1871, page 334) on 
this subject: 

‘‘At every age the healthy and beautiful go over from the unmarried side 
to the married, leaving the unmarried columns crowded with the sickly and 
unfortunate. ’’ 

96 See the ingenious and original argument on this subject by Mr. Galton, 
“Hereditary Genius,” pp. 340-342. 


190 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


tinued development in mind and body. But development 
of all kinds depends on many concurrent favorable circum- 
stances. Natural selection acts only tentatively. Individ- 
uals and races may have acquired certain indisputable 
advantages, and yet have perished from failing in other 
characters. The Greeks may have retrograded from a want 
of coherence between the many small states, from the small 
size of their whole country, from the practice of slavery, 
or from extreme sensuality; for they did not succumb until 
‘they were enervated and corrupt to the very core.’’*” The 
western nations of Hurope, who now so immeasurably sur- 
pass their former savage progenitors, and stand at the 
summit of civilization, owe little or none of their superior- 
ity to direct inheritance from the old Greeks, though they 
owe much to the written works of that wonderful people. 
Who can positively say why the Spanish nation, so 
dominant at one time, has been distanced in the race? The 
awakening of the nations of Europe from the dark ages is 
a still more perplexing problem. At that early period, 
as Mr. Galton has remarked, almost all the men of a gentle 
nature, those given to meditation or culture of the mind, 
had no refuge except in the bosom of a Church which de- 
manded celibacy;” and this could hardly fail to have had 
a deteriorating influence on each successive generation. 
During this same period the Holy Inquisition selected with 
extreme care the freest and boldest men in order to burn 
or imprison them. In Spain alone some of the best men— 
those who doubted and questioned, and without doubting 
there can be no progress—were eliminated during three 
centuries at the rate of a thousand a year. The evil which 
the Catholic Church has thus effected is incalculable, though 


21 Mr, Greg, ‘‘Fraser’s Magazine,’’ September, 1868, p. 357. 

98 ‘Hereditary Genius,’? 1870, pp. 357-359. The Rev. F. W. Farrar 
(‘‘Fraser’s Mag.,”? Aug. 1870, p. 257) advances arguments on the other side. 
Sir ©. Lyell had already (‘‘Principles of Geology,”’ vol. ii., 1868, p. 489).in a 
striking passage called attention to the evil influence of the Holy Inquisition 
in having, through selection, lowered the general standard of intelligence in 
Europe. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 191 


no doubt counterbalanced to a certain, perhaps to a large, 
extent in other ways; nevertheless, Europe has progressed 
at an unparalleled rate. 

The remarkable success of the English as colonists, com- 
pared to other European nations, has been ascribed to their 
‘daring and persistent energy’’; a result which is well illus- 
trated by comparing the progress of the Canadians of En. | 
glish and French extraction; but who can say how the 
English gained their energy? There is apparently much 
truth in the belief that the wonderful progress of the United 
States, as well as the character of the people, are the results 
of natural selection; for the more energetic, restless, and 
courageous men from all parts of Europe have emigrated 
during the last ten or twelve generations to that great coun- 
try, and have there succeeded best.” Looking to the distant 
future, I do not think that the Rev. Mr. Zincke takes an ex- 
aggerated view when he says:” ‘‘ All other series of events— 
as that which resulted in the culture of mind in Greece, and 
that which resulted in the empire of Rome—only appear to 
have purpose and value when viewed in connection with, 
or rather as subsidiary to. . . the great stream of Anglo- 
Saxon emigration to the west.’’ Obscure as is the problem 
of the advance of civilization, we can at least see that a 
nation which produced during a lengthened period the 
greatest number of highly intellectual, energetic, brave, 
patriotic, and benevolent men, would generally prevail over 
less favored nations. 

Natural selection follows from the struggle for existence; 
and this from a rapid rate of increase. It is impossible not 
to regret bitterly, but whether wisely is another question, 
the rate at which man tends to increase; for this leads in 
barbarous tribes to infanticide and many other evils, and 
in civilized nations to abject poverty, celibacy, and to the 
late marriages of the prudent. But as man suffers from 


° Mr. ‘Galton, ‘‘Macmillan’s Mag.,”? Aug. 1865, p. 325. See, also, 
“Nature,’’ “‘On Darwinism and National Life,’? Dec. 1869, p. 184. 
30 “Last Winter in the United States,’’ 1868, p. 29. 


192 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


the same physical evils as the lower animals, he has no right 
to expect an immunity from the evils consequent on the 
struggle for existence. Had he not been subjected during 
primeval ‘times to natural selection, assuredly he would 
never have attained to his present rank. Since we see ia 
many parts of the world enormous areas of the most fertile 
land capable of supporting numerous happy homes, but peo- 
pled only by a few wandering savages, it might be argued 
that the struggle for existence had not been sufficiently 
severe to force man upward to his highest standard. Judg- 
ing from all that we know of man and the lower animals, 
there has always been sufficient variability in their intel- 
lectual and moral faculties for a steady advance through 
natural selection. No doubt such advance demands many 
favorable concurrent circumstances; but it may well be 
doubted whether the most favorable would have sufficed, 
had not the rate of increase been rapid, and the consequent 
struggle for existence extremely severe. It even appears 
from what we see, for instance, in parts of South America, 
that a people which may be called civilized, such as the 
Spanish settlers, is liable to become indolent and to retro- 
grade, when the conditions of life are very easy. With 
highly civilized nations continued progress depends in a 
subordinate degree on natural selection; for such nations 
‘do not supplant and exterminate one another as do savage 
tribes. Nevertheless, the more intelligent members within 
the same community will succeed better in the long run 
than the inferior, and leave a more numerous progeny, and 
this is a form of natural selection. The more efficient causes 
of progress seem to consist of a good education during youth 
while the brain is impressible, and of a high standard of 
excellence, inculcated by the ablest-and best men, embodied 
in the laws, customs, and traditions of the nation, and en- 
forced by public opinion. It should, however, be borne 
in mind that the enforcement of public opinion depends on 
our appreciation of the approbation and disapprobation 
of others; and this appreciation is founded on our sym- 


i. 

i 
, 
( 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN Or MAN 193 


pathy, which it can hardly be doubted was originally 
developed through natural selection as one of the most 
important elements of the social instincts.” 


On the Evidence that all Civilized Nations were once 
Barbarous.—The present subject has been treated in so full 
and admirable a manner by Sir J. Lubbock,” Mr. Tylor, 
Mr. M‘Lennan, and others that I need here give only the 
briefest summary of their results. The arguments recently 
advanced by the Duke of Argyll,* and formerly by Arch. 
bishop Whately, in favor of the belief that man came into 
the world as a civilized being, and that all savages have 
since undergone degradation, seem to me weak in compari- 
son with those advanced on the other side. Many nations, 
no doubt, have fallen away in civilization, and some may 
have lapsed into utter barbarism, though on this latter head 
I have met with no evidence. The Fuegians were probably 
compelled by other conquering hordes to settle in their in- 
hospitable country, and they may have become in conse- 
quence somewhat more degraded; but it would be difficult 
to prove that they have fallen much below the Botocudos, 
who inhabit the finest parts of Brazil. 

The evidence that all civilized nations are the descend- 
ants of barbarians, consists, on the one side, of clear traces of 
their former low condition in still-existing customs, beliefs, 
language, etc.; and, on the other side, of proofs that savages 
are independently able to raise themselves a few steps in 
the scale of civilization, and have actually thus risen. The 
evidence on the first head is extremely curious, but cannot 
be here given; I refer to such cases as that of the art of 
enumeration, which, as Mr. Tylor clearly shows by reference 
to the words still used in some places, originated in count- 
ing the fingers, first of one hand and then of the other, and 


21 IT am much indebted to Mr. John Morley for some good criticisms on this 
subject: see, also, Broca, ‘Les Sélections,”’ “Revue d’Anthropologie,”’ 1872. 

# “On the Origin of Civilization,” ‘‘Proc. of the Ethnological Society,” 
November 26, 1867. 

33 **Primeval Man,’’ 1869. 


Descent—Votu. I.—9 


194 “THE DESCENT OF MAN 


lastly of the toes. We have traces of this in our own deci- 
mal system, and in the Roman numerals, where, after the 
V., which is supposed to be an abbreviated picture of a 
human hand, we pass on to VI., etc., when the other hand 
no doubt was used. So again, ‘‘when we speak of three- 
score and ten, we are counting by the vigesimal system, 
each score thus ideally made standing for 20—for ‘one man,’ 
as a Mexican or Carib would put it.’’ * 

According toa large and increasing school of philologists 
every language bears the marks of its slow and gradual 
evolution. So it is with the art of writing, for letters are 
rudiments of pictorial representations. It is hardly possible 
to read Mr. M‘Lennan’s work®® and not admit that almost 
all civilized nations still retain traces of such rude habits as 
the forcible capture of wives. What ancient nation, as the 
same author asks, can be named that was originally monog- 
amous? The primitive idea of justice, as shown by the 
law of battle and other customs of which vestiges still re- 
main, was likewise most rude. Many existing superstitions 
are the remnants of former false religious beliefs. The 
highest form of religion—the grand idea of God hating sin 
and loving righteousness—was unknown during primeval 
times. 

Turning to the other kind of evidence: Sir J. Lubbock 
has shown that.some savages have recently improved 4 little 
in some of their simpler arts. From the extremely curious 
account which he gives of the weapons, tools, and arts in 
use among savages in various parts of the world, it cannot 
be doubted that these have nearly all been independent dis- 


34 ‘Royal Institution of Great Britain,’? March 15, 1867. Also, ‘Researches 
into the Early History of Mankind,’’ 1865. : 

85 ‘Primitive Marriage,’? 1865. See, likewise, an excellent article, evidently 
by the same author, in the ‘“‘North British Review,’ July, 1869. Also, Mr. 
L. H. Morgan, ‘‘A Conjectural Solution of the Origin of the Class System of 
Relationship,’? in ‘‘Proc. American Acad. of Sciences,’’ vol. vii., February, 
1868. Prof. Schaaffhausen (‘‘Anthropolog. Review,’’ October, 1869, p. 373) 
remarks on ‘“‘the vestiges of human sacrifices found both in. Homer and the 
Old Testament.”’ 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 195 


coveries, excepting perhaps the art of making fire. The 
Australian boomerang is a good instance of one such inde- 
pendent discovery. The Tahitians when first visited had 
advanced in many respects beyond the inhabitants of most 
of the other Polynesian islands. There are no just grounds 
for the belief that the high culture of the native Peruvians 
and Mexicans was derived from abroad; many native plants 
were there cultivated, and a few native animals domesticated. 
We should bear in mind that, judging from the small influ- 
ence of most missionaries, a wandering crew from some 
semi-civilized land, if washed to the shores of America, 
would not have produced any marked effect on the natives, 
unless they had already become somewhat advanced. Look- 
ing to a very remote period in the history of the world, we 
find, to use Sir J. Lubbock’s well-known terms, a paleolithie 
and neolithic period; and no one will pretend that the art 
of grinding rough flint tools was a borrowed one. In all 
parts of Europe, as far east as Greece, in Palestine, India, 
Japan, New Zealand, and Africa, including Egypt, flint 
tools have been discovered in abundance; and of their use 
the existing inhabitants retain no tradition. There is also 
indirect evidence of their former use by the Chinese and 
ancient Jews. Hence there can hardly be a doubt that 
the inhabitants of these countries, which include nearly the 
whole civilized world, were once in a barbarous condition. 
To believe that man was aboriginally civilized and then 
suffered utter degradation in so many regions, is to take 
a pitiably low view of human nature. It is apparently a 
truer and more cheerful view that progress has been much 
more general than retrogression; that man has risen, though 
by slow and interrupted steps, from a lowly condition to the 
highest standard as yet attained by him in knowledge, 
morals, and religion. 


% Sir J. Lubbock, ‘‘Prehistoric Times,’? 2d edit., 1869, chap. xv. and xvi. 
st passim. See, also, the excellent ninth chapter in Tylor’s ‘Early History of 
Mankind,’’ 2d edit., 1870. 

31 Dr, F. Miller has made some good remarks to this effect in the ‘‘Reise 
der Novara: Anthropolog. Theil,”’ Abtheil. iii., 1868, s. 127, 


196 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


CHAPTER VI 
ON THE AFFINITIES AND GENEALOGY OF MAN 


Position of man in the animal series—The natural system genealogical— 
Adaptive characters of slight value—Various small points of resem- 
blance between man and the Quadrumana—Rank of man in the nat- 
ural system—Birthplace and antiquity of man—Absence of fossil con- 
necting-links—Lower stages in the genealogy of man, as inferred, 
first, from his affinities, and, secondly, from his structure—Harly 
androgynous condition of the Vertebrata—Conclusion 


T ‘VEN if it be granted that the difference between man 
4 and his nearest allies is as great in corporeal structure 
4— as some naturalists maintain, and although we must 
grant that the difference between them is immense in mental 
power, yet the facts given in the earlier chapters appear to 
declare, in the plainest manner, that man is descended from 
some lower form, notwithstanding that connecting-links 
have not hitherto been discovered. 

Man is liable to numerous slight and diversified varia- 
tions, which are induced by the same general causes, are 
governed and transmitted in accordance with the same gen- 
eral laws, as in the lower animals. Man has multiplied so 
rapidly that he has necessarily been exposed to struggle for 
existence, and consequently to natural selection. He has 
given rise to many races, some of which differ so much from 
each other that they have often been ranked by naturalists 
as distinct species. His body is constructed on the same 
homological plan as that of other mammals. He passes 
through the same phases of embryological development. 
He retains many rudimentary and useless structures, which 
no doubt were once serviceable. Characters occasionally 
make their reappearance in him, which we have reason to 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 197 


believe were possessed by his early progenitors. If the 
origin of man had been wholly different from that of all 
other animals, these various appearances would be mere 
empty deceptions; but such an admission is incredible. 
These appearances, on the other hand, are intelligible, at 
least to a large extent, if man is the co-descendant with other 
mammals of some unknown and lower form. 

Some naturalists, from being déeply impressed with the 
mental and spiritual powers of man, have divided the whole 
organic world into three kingdoms—the Human, the Animal, 
and the Vegetable—thus giving to man a separate kingdom.’ 
Spiritual powers cannot be compared or classed by the natu- 
ralist; but he may endeavor to show, as I have done, that 
the mental faculties of man and the lower animals do not 
differ in kind, although immensely in degree. A difference 
in degree, however great, does not justify us in placing man 
in a distinct kingdom, as will perhaps be best illustrated by 
comparing the mental powers of two insects, namely, a coccus 
or scale-insect and an ant, which undoubtedly belong to the 
same class. The difference is here greater than, though of 
a somewhat different kind from, that between man and the 
highest mammal. The female coccus, while young, attaches 
itself by its proboscis to a plant; sucks the sap, but never 
moves again; is fertilized and lays eggs; and this is its 
whole history. On the other hand, to describe the habits 
and mental powers of worker-ants would require, as Pierre 
Huber has shown, a large volume; I may, however, briefly 
specify a few points. Ants certainly communicate informa- 
tion to each other, and several unite for the same work, or 
for games of play. They recognize their fellow-ants after 
months of absence, and feel sympathy for each other. They 
build great edifices, keep them clean, close the doors in the 
evening, and post sentries. They make roads as well as 
tunnels under rivers, and temporary bridges over them, by 


1 Isidore Geoffroy St.-Hilaire gives a detailed account of the position assigned 
to man by various naturalists in their classifications: ‘“‘Hist. Nat. Gén.,’’ tom. 
ii, 1859, pp. 170-189. 


198 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


clinging together. They collect food for the community, 
and when an object too large for entrance is brought to the 
nest, they enlarge the door, and afterward build it up again. 
They store up seeds, of which they prevent the germination, 
and which, if damp, are brought up to the surface to dry. 
They keep aphides and other insects as milch-cows. They 
go out to battle in regular bands, and freely sacrifice their 
lives for the common weal. They emigrate according to a 
preconcerted plan. They capture slaves. They move the 
egos of their aphides, as well as their own eggs and co- 
coons, into warm parts of the nest, in order that they may 
be quickly hatched; and endless similar facts could be 
given.” On the whole, the difference in mental power 
between an ant and a coccus is immense; yet no one has 
ever dreamed of placing these insects in distinct classes, 
much less in distinct kingdoms. No doubt the difference 
is bridged over by other insects; and this is not the case 
with man and the higher apes. But we have every reason 
to believe that the breaks in the series are simply the results 
of many forms having become extinct. 

Prof. Owen, relying chiefly on the structure of the brain, 
has divided the mammalian series into four sub-classes. One 
of these he devotes to man; in another he places both the 
Marsupials and the Monotremata; so that he makes man as 
distinct from all other mammals as are these two latter groups 
conjoined. This view has not been accepted, as far as I am 
aware, by any naturalist capable of forming an independent 
judgment, and therefore need not here be further considered. 

We can understand why a classification founded on any 
single character or organ—even an organ so wonderfully 
complex and important as the brain—or on the high de- 
velopment of the mental faculties, is almost sure to prove 


2 Some of the most interesting facts ever published on the habits of ants 
are given by Mr. Belt, in his ‘‘Naturalist in Nicaragua,’’ 1874. See, also, Mr. 
Moggridge’s admirable work, ‘‘Harvesting Ants,’’ etc., 1873; also, ‘‘L’Instinct 
chez les Insectes,’? by M. George Pouchet, ‘‘Revue des Deux Mondes,’’ Feb- 
Tuary, 1870, p. 682. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 199 


unsatisfactory. This principle has indeed been tried with 
hymenopterous insects; but when thus classed by their 
habits or instincts, the arrangement proved thoroughly arti- 
ficial.* Classifications may, of course, be based on any char- 
-acter whatever, as on size, color, or the element inhabited; 
but naturalists have long felt a profound conviction that 
there is a natural system. This system, it is now generally 
admitted, must be, as far as possible, genealogical in arrange- 
ment—that is, the co-descendants of the same form must be 
kept together in one group, apart from the co-descendants 
of any other form; but if the parent-forms are related, so 
will be their descendants, and the two groups together will 
form a larger group. The amount of difference between the 
several groups—that is, the amount of modification which 
each has undergone—is expressed by such terms as genera, 
families, orders, and classes. As we have no record of the 
lines of descent, the pedigree can be discovered only by ob- 
serving the degrees of resemblance between the beings which 
are to be classed. For this object numerous points of re- 
semblance are of much more importance than the amount of 
similarity or dissimilarity in a few points. If two languages 
were found to resemble each other in a multitude of words 
and points of construction, they would be universally recog- 
nized as having sprung from a common source, notwithstand- 
ing that they differed greatly in some few words or points of 
construction. But with organic beings the points of resem- 
blance must not consist of adaptations to similar habits of 
life: two animals may, for instance, have had their whole 
frames modified for living in the water, and yet they will 
not be brought any nearer to each other in the natural 
system. Hence we can see how it is that resemblances in 
several unimportant structures, in useless and rudimentary 
organs, or not now functionally active, or in an embryologi- 
cal condition, are by far the most serviceable for classifica- 
tion; for they can hardly be due to adaptations within a 


8 Westwood, ‘‘Modern Class of Insects,’’ vol. ii., 1840, p. 87. 


200 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


late period; and thus they reveal the old lines of descent 
or of true affinity. 

We can further see why a great amount of modification 
in some one character ought not to lead us to separate widely 
any two organisms. A part which already differs much from 
the same part in other allied forms has already, according to 
the theory of evolution, varied much; consequently it would 
(as long as the organism remained exposed to the same ex- 
citing conditions) be liable to further variations of the same 
kind; and these, if beneficial, would be preserved, and thus 
be continually augmented. In many cases the continued 
development of a part, for instance, of the beak of a bird, 
or of the teeth of a mammal, would not aid the species in 
gaining its food, or for any other object; but with man we 
can see no definite limit to the continued development of the 
brain and mental faculties, as far as advantage is concerned. 
Therefore in determining the position of man in the natural 
or genealogical system, the extreme development of his brain 
ought not to outweigh a multitude of resemblances in other 
less important or quite unimportant points. 

The greater number of naturalists who have taken into 
consideration the whole structure of man, including his 
mental faculties, have followed Blumenbach and Cuvier, 
and have placed man in a separate Order, under the title 
of the Bimana, and therefore on an equality with the orders 
of the Quadrumana, Carnivora, etc. Recently many of our 
best naturalists have recurred to the view first propounded 
by Linnzus, so remarkable for his sagacity, and have placed 
man in the same Order with the Quadrumana, under the title 
of the Primates. The justice of this conclusion will be ad- 
mitted; for, in the first place, we must bear in mind the 
comparative insignificance for classification of the great de- 
velopment of the brain in man, and that the strongly marked 
differences between the skulls of man and the Quadrumana 
(lately insisted upon by Bischoff, Aeby, and others) appar- 
ently follow from their differently developed brains. In the 
second place, we must remember that nearly all the other 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 201 


and more important differences between man and the Quad- 
rumana are manifestly adaptive in their nature, and relate 
chiefly to the erect position of man; such as the structure 
of his hand, foot, and pelvis, the curvature of his spine, and 
the position of his head. The family of Seals offers a good 
illustration of the small importance of adaptive characters 
for classification. These animals differ from all other Car- 
nivora, in the form of their bodies and in the structure of 
their limbs, far more than does man from the higher apes; 
yet in most systems, from that of Cuvier to the most recent 
one by Mr. Flower,‘ seals are ranked as a mere family in the 
Order of the Carnivora. If man had not been his own clas- 
sifier, he would never have thought of founding a separate 
order for his own reception. 

It would be beyond my limits, and quite beyond my 
knowledge, even to name the innumerable paints of struc- 
ture in which man agrees with the other Primates. Our 
great anatomist and philosopher, Prof. Huxley, has fully 
discussed this subject,’ and concludes that man in all parts 
of his organization differs less from the higher apes than 
these do from the lower members of the same group. Conse- 
quently there ‘‘is no justification for placing man in a distinct 
order.” 

In an early part of this work I brought forward various 
facts, showing how closely man agrees in constitution with 
the higher mammals; and this agreement must depend on 
our close similarity in minute structure and chemical com- 
position. I gave, as instances, our liability to the same dis- 
eases, and to the attacks of allied parasites; our tastes in 
common for the same stimulants, and the similar effects 
produced by them, as well as by various drugs, and other 
such facts. 

As small unimportant points of resemblance between 
man and the Quadrumana are not commonly noticed in sys- 


4 “Proc. Zoolog. Soc.,’? 1863, p. 4. 
5 “*Ryidence as to Man’s Place in Nature,”’ 1863, p. 70 et passim. 


202 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


tematic works, and as, when numerous, they clearly reveal 
our relationship, I will specify a few such points. The rela- 
tive position of our features is manifestly the same; and the 
various emotions are displayed by nearly similar movements 
of the muscles and skin, chiefly above the eyebrows and 
round the mouth. Some few expressions are, indeed, al- 
most the same, as in the weeping of certain kinds of mon- 
keys and in the laughing noise made by others, during 
which the corners of the mouth are drawn backward, and 
the lower eyelids wrinkled. The external ears are curiously 
alike. In man the nose is much more prominent than in 
most monkeys; but we may trace the commencement of an 
aquiline curvature in the nose of the Hoolock Gibbon; and 
this in the Semnopithecus nasiea is carried to a ridiculous 
extreme. 

The faces of many monkeys are ornamented with beards, 
whiskers, or mustaches. The hair on the head grows to a 
great length in some species of Semnopithecus;° and in the 
Bonnet monkey (Macacus radiatus) it radiates from a point 
on the crown, with a parting down the middle. It is com- 
monly said that the forehead gives to man his noble and 
intellectual appearance; but the thick hair on the head of 
the Bonnet monkey terminates downward abruptly, and is 
succeeded by hair so short and fine that at a little distance 
the forehead, with the exception of the eyebrows, appears 
quite naked. It has been erroneously asserted that eye- 
brows are not present in any monkey. In the species just 
named the degree of nakedness of the forehead differs in 
different individuals; and Hschricht states’ that in our 
children the limit between the hairy scalp and the naked 
forehead is sometimes not well defined; so that here we 
seem to have a trifling case of reversion to a progenitor 
in whom the forehead had not as yet become quite naked. 

It is well known that the hair on our arms tends to con- 


6 Isid. Geoffroy, ‘‘Hist. Nat. Gén.,”? tom. ii., 1859, p. 217. 
1 ‘Ueber die Richtung der Haare,”’ etc. Miiller’s “‘Archiv far Anat. und 
Phys.,”? 1837, s. 51. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 203 


verge from above and below to a point at the elbow. This 
curious arrangement, so unlike that in most of the lower 
mammals, is common to the gorilla, chimpanzee, orang, 
some species of Hylobates, and even to some few American 
monkeys. But in Hylobates agilis the hair on the forearm 
is directly downward or toward the wrist in the ordinary 
manner; and in H. lar it is nearly erect, with only a very 
slight forward inclination; so that in this latter species it is 
in a transitional state. It can hardly be doubted that with 
most mammals the thickness of the hair on the back and its 
direction is adapted to throw off the rain; even the trans- 
verse hairs on. the forelegs of a dog may serve for this end 
when he is coiled up asleep. Mr. Wallace, who has care- 
fully studied the habits of the orang, remarks that the con- 
vergence of the hair toward the elbow on the arms of the 
orang may be explained as serving to throw off the rain, 
for this animal during rainy weather sits with its arms bent, 
and with the hands clasped round a branch or over its head. 
According to Livingstone, the gorilla also ‘‘sits in pelting 
rain with his hands over his head.’’*® If the above explana- 
tion is correct, as seems probable, the direction of the hair 
on our own arms offers a curious record of our former state; 
for no one supposes that it is now of any use in throwing off 
the rain; nor, in our present erect condition, is it properly 
directed for this purpose. 

It would, however, be rash to trust too much to the prin- 
ciple of adaptation in regard to the direction of the hair in 
man orhis early progenitors; for it is impossible to study 
the figures given by Eschricht of the arrangement of the 
hair4yn the human foetus (this being the same as in the adult) 
and not agree with this excellent observer that other and 
more complex causes have intervened. The points of con- 
vergencd seem to stand in some relation to those points in 
the emlpryo which are last closed in during development. 
There opear also to exist some relation between the ar- 

id 


i 
® Quoted by Reade, ‘The African Sketch Book,” vol. i., 1873, p. 152. 


204 THE DESCENT \OF MAN 


rangement of the hair on the limbs and the course of the 
medullary arteries.* , 

It must not be supposed that jthe resemblances between 
man and certain apes in the above;and many other points— 
such as in having a naked fore head, long tresses on the 
head, etc.—are all necessarily the ‘result of unbroken inheri- 
tance from a common progenitor, or of subsequent rever- 
sion. Many of these resemblancies are more probably due 
to analogous variation, which follows, as I have elsewhere 
attempted to show," from co-descended organisms having 
a similar constitution, and having been acted on by like 
causes inducing similar modificatiens. With respect to the 
similar direction of the hair on the forearms of man and’ 
certain monkeys, as this character is common to almost all 
the anthropomorphous apes, it may probably be attributed 
to inheritance; but this is not certain, as aa very distinet 
American monkeys are thus characterized. * 

Although, as we have now seen, man has no just right 
to form a separate Order for his own reception, he may per- 
haps claim a distinct Sub-order or Family. Prof. Huxley, 
in his last work,'' divides the Primates into three Sub- 
orders; namely, the Anthropidsz with man alone, the Simi- 
ad, including monkeys of all kinds, and the Wemuride 
with the diversified genera of lemurs. As far as Bileence 
in certain important points of structure are concerned, man 
may no doubt rightly claim the rank of a Sub- oder; and 
this rank is too low, if we look chiefly to his mental facul- 
ties. Nevertheless, from a genealogical point of view, it 
appears that this rank is too high, and that man ought to 
form merely a Family, or possibly even only a Sub-faigsily. 

AP 


1 

® On the hair in Hylobates, see ‘“‘Nat. History of Mammals,’* « by CL 
Martin, 1841, p. 415. Also, Isid. Geoffroy on the American mor core and 
other kinds, “ist, Nat. Gén.,”’ vol. ii., 1859, PP. 216, 248. EschriShi, ibid., 
8. 46, 55, ’el. Owen, “Anat. of Vertebrates,’ vol. iii, p. 619. Wallace, 
“Contributions to the Theory of Natural Selection,” 1870, p. 344. 

10 “Origin of Species,’’ 5th edit., 1869, p. 194. “The Variation \ Fa 
mals and Plants under Domestication,”’ vol, ii., 1868, p. 348. und An 

11 ‘*An Introduction to the Classification of Animals,’’ 1869, p. 99. ; 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 205 


Tf we imagine three lines of descent proceeding from a com- 
mon stock, it is quite conceivable that two of them might 
after the lapse of ages be so slightly changed as still to 
remain as species of the -same genus, while the third line 
might become so greatly modified as to deserve to rank as 
a distinct Sub-family, Family, or even Order. But in this 
case it is almost certain that the third line would still retain 
through inheritance numerous small points of resemblance 
with the other two. Here, then, would occur the difficulty, 
at present insoluble, how much weight we ought to assign 
in our classifications to strongly marked differences in some 
few points—that is, to the amount of modification under- 
gone; and how much to close resemblance in numerous 
unimportant points, as indicating the lines of descent or 
genealogy. To attach much weight to the few but strong 
differences is the most obvious and perhaps the safest 
course, though it appears more correct to pay great atten- 
tion to the many small resemblances, as giving a truly 
natural classification. 

In forming a judgment on this head with reference to 
man, we must glance at the classification of the Simiade. 
This family is divided by almost all naturalists into the 
Catarrhine group, or Old World monkeys, all of which are 
characterized (as their name expresses) by the peculiar 
structure of their nostrils, and by having four premolars in 
each jaw; and into the Platyrhine group or New World 
monkeys (including two very distinct sub-groups), all of 
which are characterized by differently constructed nostrils, 
and by having six premolars in each jaw. Some other 
small differences might be mentioned. Now man unques- 
tionably belongs in his dentition, in the structure of his 
nostrils, and some other respects, to the Catarrhine or Old 
“World division; nor does he resemble the Platyrhines more 
elosely than the Catarrhines in any characters, excepting 
in a few of not much importance and apparently of an 
adaptive nature. I+ is therefore against all probability that 
some New World species should have formerly varied and 


206 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


produced a manlike creature, with all the distinctivé char- 
acters proper to the Old World division; losing at the same 
time all its own distinctive characters. There can, conse- 
quently, hardly be a doubt that man is an offshoot from the 
Old World Simian stem; and that, under a genealogical 
point of view, he must be classed with the Catarrhine 
division.” 

The anthropomorphous apes, namely, the gorilla, chim- 
panzee, orang, and hylobates, are by most naturalists sepa- 
rated from the other Old World monkeys, as a distinct sub- 
group. JI am aware that Gratiolet, relying on the structure 
of the brain, does not admit the existence of this sub-group, 
and no doubt it is a broken one. Thus the orang, as Mr. 
St. G. Mivart remarks,* ‘‘is one of the most peculiar and 
aberrant forms to be found in the Order.’’ The remaining 
non-anthropomorphous Old World monkeys are again di- 
vided by some naturalists into two or three smaller sub- 
groups; the genus Semnopithecus, with its peculiar saccu- 
lated stomach, being the type of one such sub-group. But 
it appears, from M. Gaudry’s wonderful discoveries in Attica, 
that during the Miocene period a form existed there which 
connected Semnopithecus and Macacus; and this probably 
illustrates the manner in which the other and higher groups 
were once blended together. 

If the anthropomorphous apes be admitted to form a 
natural sub-group, then as man agrees with them, not-only 
in all those characters which he possesses in common with 
the whole Catarrhine group, but in other peculiar characters, 
such as the absence of a tail and of callosities, and in general 
appearance, we may infer that some ancient member of the 
anthropomorphous sub-group gave birth to man. It is not 


12 This is nearly the same classification as that provisionally adopted by 
Mr. St. George Mivart (‘‘Transact. Philosoph. Soc.,’’ 186%, p. 300), who, after 
separating the Lemuride, divides the remainder of the Primates into the 
Hominid, the Simiade which answer to the Catarrhines, the Cebide, and 
the Hapalide—these two latter groups answering to the Platyrhimes. Mr. 
Mivart still abides by the same view; see ‘‘Nature,’’ 1871, p. 481. 

18 “Transact. Zoolog. Soc.,’? vol. vii 1867, p. 214. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 207 


probable that, through the law of analogous variation, a 
member of one of the other lower sub-groups should have 
given rise to a man-like creature, resembling the higher 
anthropomorphous apes in so many respects. No doubt 
man, in comparison with most of his allies, has undergone 
an extraordinary amount of modification, chiefly in conse- 
quence of the great development of his brain and his erect 
position; nevertheless, we should béar in mind that he ‘‘is 
but one of several exceptional forms of Primates.’’ 

Every naturalist who believes in the principle of evolu- 
tion will grant that the two main divisions of the Simiade, 
namely, the Catarrhine and Platyrhine monkeys, with their 
sub-groups, have all proceeded from some one extremely 
ancient progenitor. The early descendants of this progeni- 
tor, before they had diverged to any considerable extent 
from each other, would still have formed a single natural 
group; but some of the species or incipient genera would 
have already begun to indicate by their diverging characters 
the future distinctive marks of the Catarrhine and Platyrhine 
divisions. Hence the members of this supposed ancient 
group would not have been so uniform in their dentition, 
‘or in the structure of their nostrils, as are the existing Catar- 
rhine monkeys in one way and the Platyrhines in another 
way, but would have resembled in this respect the allied 
Lemuride, which differ greatly from each other in the form 
of their muzzles,** and to an extraordinary degree in their 
dentition. 

The Catarrhine and Platyrhine monkeys agree in a mul- 
titude of characters, as is shown by their unquestionably 
belonging to one and the same Order. The many characters 
which they possess in common can hardly have been inde- 
pendently acquired by so many distinct species; so that 
these characters must have been inherited. But a natural- 
ist would undoubtedly have ranked as an ape or a monkey 


4 Mr, St. George Mivart, ‘“Transact. Phil. Soc.,’’ 1867, p. 410. 
18 Messrs. Murie and Mivart on the Lemuroidea, ‘‘Transact. Zoolog. Soc., ’? 
vol, vii., 1869, p. 5. 


208 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


an ancient form which possessed many characters common 
to the Catarrhine and Platyrhine monkeys, other characters 
in an intermediate condition, and some few, perhaps, distinct 
from those now found in either group. And as man from a 
genealogical point of view belongs to the Catarrhine or Old 
World stock, we must conclude, however much the conclu- 
sion may revolt our pride, that our early progenitors would 
have been properly thus designated.’* But we must not fall 
into the error of supposing that the early progenitor of the 
whole Simian stock, including man, was identical with, or 
even closely resembled, any existing ape or monkey. 


On the Birthplace and Antiquity of -Man.—We are natu- 
rally led to inquire, where was the birthplace of man at that 
stage of descent when our progenitors diverged from the 
Catarrhine stock? The fact that they belonged to this stock 
clearly shows that they inhabited the Old World; but not 
Australia nor any oceanic island, as we may infer from the 
laws of geographical distribution. In each great region of 
the world the living mammals are closely related to the 
extinct species of the same region. It is therefore probable 
that Africa was formerly inhabited by extinct apes closely 
allied to the gorilla and chimpanzee; and as these two spe- 
cies are now man’s nearest allies, it is somewhat more prob- 
able that our early progenitors lived on the African conti- 
nent than elsewhere. But it is useless to speculate on this 
subject; for two or three anthropomorphous apes, one the 
Dryopithecus” of Lartet, nearly as large as a man, and closely 
allied to Hylobates, existed in Europe during the Miocene 
age; and since so remote a period the earth has certainly 
undergone many great revolutions, and there has been ample 
time for migration on the largest scale. 


18 Hackel has come to this same conclusion. See ‘‘Ueber die Entstehung 
ges Menschengeschlechts,”’? in Virchow’s ‘‘Sammlung. gemein. wissen. Vor- 
trage,’? 1868, s, 61. Also his ‘‘Nattirliche Schépfungsgeschichte,’’ 1868, in 
which he gives in detail his views on the genealogy of man. 

1 Dr, C. Forsyth Major, ‘‘Sur les Singes Fossiles trouvés en Italie’; “Soc 
Ttal. des Sc. Nat.,”? tom. xv., 1872. 


YHE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 209 


At the period and place, whenever and wherever it was, 
when man first lost his hairy covering, he probably inhabited 
a hot country—a circumstance favorable for the frugiferous 

diet on which, judging from analogy, he subsisted. We 
are far from knowing how long ago it was when man first 
diverged from the Catarrhine stock; but it may have oc- 
curred at an epoch as remote as the Eocene period; for that 
the higher apes had diverged from the lower apes as early 
as the Upper Miocene period is shown by the existence of 
the Dryopithecus. We are also quite ignorant at how rapid 
a rate organisms, whether high or low in the scale, may be 
modified under favorable circumstances; we know, however, 
that some have retained the same form during an enormous 
lapse of time. From what we see going on under domesti- 
‘cation, we learn that some of the co-descendants of the same 
species may be not at all, some a little, and some greatly 
changed, all within the same period. Thus it may have 
been with man, who has undergone a great amount of 
modification in certain characters in comparison with the 
higher apes. 

The great break in the organic chain between man and 
his nearest allies, which cannot be bridged over by any ex- 
tinct or living species, has often been advanced as a grave 
objection to the belief that man is descended from some 
lower form; but this objection will not appear of much 
weight to those who, from general reasons, believe in the 
general principle of evolution. Breaks often occur in all 
parts of the series, some being wide, sharp, and defined, 
others less so in various degrees; as between the orang and 
its nearest allies—between the Tarsius and the other Lemu- 
ridsee—between the elephant, and in a more striking manner 
between the Ornithorhynchus or Kchidna, and all other 
mammals. 

But these breaks depend merely on the number of related 
forms which have become extinct. At some future period, 
not very distant as measured by centuries, the civilized 
races of man will almost certainly exterminate, and replace, 


210 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


the savage races throughout the world. At the same time 
the anthropomorphous apes, as Professor Schaaffhausen has 
remarked,** will no doubt be exterminated. The break be- 
tween man and his nearest allies will then be wider, for 
it will intervene between man in a more civilized state, as 
we may hope, even than the Caucasian, and some ape 
as low as a baboon, instead of, as now, between the negro 
or Australian and the gorilla. 

With respect to the absence of fossil remains, serving 
to connect man with his ape-like progenitors, no one will 
lay much stress on this fact who reads Sir C. Lyell’s discus- 
sion,’® where he shows that in all the vertebrate classes the 
discovery of fossil remains has been a very slow and fortui. 
tous process. Nor should it be forgotten that those regions 
which are the most likely to afford remains connecting man 
with some extinct ape-like creature have not as yet been 
searched by geologists. 


Lower Stages in the Genealogy of Man.—We have seen 
that man appears to have diverged from the Catarrhine or 
Old World division of the Simiade, after these had diverged 
from the New World division. We will now endeavor to 
follow the remote traces of his genealogy, trusting princi 
pally to the mutual affinities between the various classes 
and orders, with some slight reference to the periods, as far 
as ascertained, of their successive appearance on the earth. 
The Lemurids stand below and near to the Simiada, and 
constitute a very distinct family of the Primates, or, accord- 
ing to Hackel and others, a distinct Order.” This group is 
diversified and broken to an extraordinary degree, and in- 
cludes many aberrant forms. It has, therefore, probably 
suffered much extinction. Most of the remnants survive 
on islands, such as Madagascar and the Malayan archipelago, 
where they have not been exposed to so severe a competition 


18 “* Anthropological Review,” April, 1867, p. 236. 
19 **Blements of Geology,’’ 1865, pp. 583-586. ‘‘Antiquity of Man,”’ 1863, 
p. 145. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 211 


as they would have been on well-stocked continents. This 
group likewise presents many gradations, leading, as Huxley 
remarks,” ‘‘insensibly from the crown and summit of-the 
animal creation down to creatures from which there is but 
a step, as it seems, to the lowest, smallest, and least intelli- 
gent of the placental mammalia.’’ From these various con- 
siderations it is probable that the Simiade were originally 
developed from the progenitors of the existing Lemuride; 
and these in their turn from forms standing very low in the 
mammalian series. 

The Marsupials stand in many important characters below 
the placental mammals. They appeared at an earlier geo- 
logical period, and their range was formerly much more 
extensive than at present. Hence the Placentata are gen- 
erally supposed to have been derived from the Implacentata 
or Marsupials; not, however, from forms closely resembling 
the existing Marsupials, but from their early progenitors. 
The Monotremata are plainly allied to the Marsupials, form- ° 
ing a third and still lower division in the great mammalian 
series. They are represented at the present day solely by 
the Ornithorhynchus and Echidna; and these two forms 
may be safely considered as relics of a much larger group, 
_ representatives of which have been preserved in Australia 
through some favorable concurrence of circumstances. The 
Monotremata are eminently interesting, as leading in sev- 
eral important points of structure toward the class of 
reptiles. 

In attempting to trace the genealogy of the Mammalia, 
and therefore of man, lower down in the series, we become 
involved in greater and greater obscurity; but, as a most 
capable judge, Mr. Parker, has remarked, we have good 
reason to believe that no true bird or reptile intervenes in 
the direct line of descent. He who wishes to see what 
ingenuity and knowledge can effect, may consult Prof. 
Hickel’s works." I will content myself with a few gen- 


% ‘*Man’s Place in Nature,”’ p. 106. : i. 
9 Blaborate tables are given in his ‘‘Generelle Morphologie” (B, ii, s. cliii, 


912 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


eral remarks. Every evolutionist will admit that the five 
great vertebrate classes, namely, mammals, birds, reptiles, 
amphibians, and fishes, are descended from some one proto- 
type; for they have much in common, especially during 
their embryonic state. As the class of fishes is the most 
lowly organized, and appeared before the others, we may 
conclude that all the members of the vertebrate kingdom 
are derived from some fish-like animal. The belief that 
animals so distinct as a monkey, an elephant, a humming- 
bird, a snake, a frog, and a fish, etc., could all have sprung 
from the same parents, will appear monstrous to those who 
have not attended to the recent progress of natural his- 
tory. For this belief implies the former existence of links 
binding closely together all these forms, now so utterly 
unlike. 

Nevertheless, it is certain that groups of animals have 
existed, or do now exist, which serve to connect several of 
the great vertebrate classes more or less closely. We have 
seen that the Ornithorhynchus graduates toward reptiles: 
and Prof. Huxley has discovered, and is confirmed by Mr. 
Cope and others, that the Dinosaurians are in many impor- 
tant characters intermediate between certain reptiles and 
certain birds—the birds referred to being the ostrich-tribe 
(itself evidently a widely diffused remnant of a larger group) 
and the Archeopteryx, that strange Secondary bird, with a 
long lizard-like tail. Again, aceording to Prof. Owen,” the 
Ichthyosaurians—great sea-lizards furnished with paddles 
—present many affinities with fishes, or rather, according 
to Huxley, with amphibians: a class which, including in 
its highest division frogs and toads, is plainly allied to 
the Ganoid fishes. These latter fishes swarmed during the 


and s. 425); and with more especial reference to man in his ‘‘Natiirliche 
Schdpfungsgeschichte,’? 1868. Prof. Huxley, in reviewing this latter work 
(‘The Academy,’’ 1869, p, 42), says that he considers the phylum or lines of 
descent of the Vertebrata to be admirably discussed by Hackel, although he 
differs on some points. He expresses, also, his high estimate of the general 
tenor and spirit of the whole work. 

32 **Paleontology,”’ 1860, p. 199, 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 213 


earlier geological periods, and were constructed on what is 
called a generalized type, that is, they presented diversified 
affinities with other groups of organisms. The Lepidosiren 
is also so closely allied to amphibians and fishes, that natu- | 
ralists long disputed in which of these two classes to rank 
it; it, and also some few Ganoid fishes, have been preserved 
from utter extinction by inhabiting rivers, which are harbors 
of refuge, and are related to the great waters of the ocean in 
the same way that islands are to continents. 

Lastly, one single member of the immense and diversified 
class of fishes, namely, the lancelet or amphioxus, is so dif- 
ferent from all other fishes, that Hickel maintains that it 
ought to form a distinct class in the vertebrate kingdom. This 
fish is remarkable for its negative characters; it can hardly 
be said to possess a brain, vertebral column, or heart, etc.; 
so that it was classed by the older naturalists among the 
worms. Many years ago Prof. Goodsir perceived that the 
lancelet presented some affinities with the Ascidians, which 
are invertebrate, hermaphrodite, marine creatures perma- 
nently attached to a support. They hardly appear like ani- 
mals, and consist of a simple, tough, leathery sack, with 
two small projecting orifices. They belong to the Mollus- 
coida of Huxley—a lower division of the great kingdom 
of the Mollusca; but they have recently been placed by 
some naturalists among the Vermes or worms. Their larvae 
somewhat resemble tadpoles in shape,” and have the power 
of swimming freely about. M. Kovalevsky™ has lately 
observed that the larve of Ascidians are related to the 
Vertebrata in their manner of development, in the relative 


23 At the Falkland Islands I had the satisfaction of seeing, in April, 1833, 
and therefore some years before any other naturalist, the locomotive larve of 
a compound Ascidian, closely allied to Synoicum, but apparently generically 
distinct from it, The tail was about five times as long as the oblong head, 
and terminated in a very fine filament. It was, as sketched by me, under 
@ simple microscope, plainly divided by -transverse opaque partitions, which 
J presume represent the great cells figured by Kovalevsky, At an early stage 
of development the tail was closely coiled round the head of the larva. 

2% “Miégmoires de l’Acad. des Sciences de St. Pétersbourg,”’ tom. x., No, 
15, 1866. 


214 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


position of the nervous system, and in possessing a structure 
closely like the chorda dorsalis of vertebrate animals; and in 
this he has been since confirmed by Prof. Kupffer. M. Kov- 
alevsky writes to me from Naples, that he has now carried 
these observations yet further; and, should his results be 
well established, the whole will form a discovery of the very 
greatest value. Thus, if we may rely on embryology, ever 
the safest guide in classification, it seems that we have at 
last gained a clew to the source whence the Vertebrata were 
derived.** We should then be justified in believing that 
at an extremely remote period a group of animals existed, 
resembling in many respects the larve of our present As- 
cidians, which diverged into two great branches—the one 
retrograding in development and producing the present 
class of Ascidians, the other rising to the crown and 
summit of the animal kingdom by giving birth to the 
Vertebrata. 

We have thus far endeavored rudely to trace the gene- 
alogy of the Vertebrata by the aid of their mutual affinities. 
We will now look to man as he exists; and we shall, [ 
think, be able partially to restore the structure of our early 
progenitors, during successive periods, but not in due order 
of time. This can be effected by means of the rudiments 
which man still retains, by the characters which occasionally 
make their appearance in him through reversion, and by the 
aid of the principles of morphology and embryology. The 
various facts to which I shall here allude have been given 
in the previous chapters. 

The early progenitors of man must have been once cov- 
ered with hair, both sexes having beards; their ears were 


2% But Iam bound to add that some competent judges dispute this con- 
_elusion; for instance, M. Giard, in a series of papers in the ‘‘Archives de. 
Zoologie Expérimentale,’’ for 1872. Nevertheless, this naturalist remarks, p. 
981, ‘‘L’organisation de la larve ascidienne en dehors de toute hypothése et 
de toute théorie, nous montre comment la nature peut produire la disposition 
fondamentale du type vertébré (l’existence d’une corde dorsale) chez un in> 
vertébré par la seule condition vitale de l’adaptation, et cette simple possibilité 
du passage supprime l’abime- entre les deux sous-régnes, encore bien qu’on 

ignore par ob le passage s’est fait en réalité.”” 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 215 


probably pointed, and capable of movement; and their 
bodies were provided with a tail, having the proper mus- 
cles. Their limbs and bodies were also acted on by many 
muscles which now only occasionally reappear, but are 
normally present in the Quadrumana. At this or some 
earlier period, the great artery and nerve of the humerus 
ran through a supra-condyloid foramen. The intestine gave 
forth a much larger diverticulum or cecum than that now 
existing. The foot was then prehensile, judging from the 
condition of the great toe in the foetus; and our progenitors, 
no doubt, were arboreal in their habits, and frequented some 
warm, forest-clad land. The males had great canine teeth, 
which served them as formidable weapons. At a much 
earlier period the uterus was double; the excreta were 
voided through a cloaca; and the eye was protected by a 
third eyelid or nictitating membrane. At a still earlier 
period the progenitors of man must have been aquatic in 
their habits; for morphology plainly tells us that our lungs 
consist of a modified swim-bladder, which once served as 
a float. The clefts on the neck in the embryo of man show 
where the branchiz once existed. In the lunar or weekly 
recurrent periods of some of our functions we apparently 
still retain traces of our primordial birthplace, a shore 
washed by the tides. At about this same early period the 
true kidneys were replaced by the corpora wolffiana. The 
heart existed as a simple pulsating vessel; and the chorda 
dorsalis took the place of a vertebral colunin. These early 
ancestors of man, thus seen in the dim recesses of time, must 
have been as singly: or even still more simply, organized 
than the lancelet or amphioxus. 

There is one other point deserving a fuller notice. It has 
long been known that in the vertebrate kingdom one sex 
bears rudiments of various accessory parts, appertaining to 
the reproductive system, which properly belong to the oppo- 
site sex; and it has now been ascertained that at a very early 
embryonic period both sexes possess true male and female 
glands. Hence some remote progenitor of the whole verte- 


216 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


brate kingdom appears to have been hermaphrodite or an- 
drogynous.”* 

But here we encounter a singular difficulty. In the 
mammalian class the males possess rudiments of a uterus 
with the adjacent passage, in their vesicule prostatics; 
they bear also rudiments of mammz, and some male 
Marsupials have traces of a marsupial sac.” Other analo- 
gous facts could be added. Are we, then, to suppose that 
some extremely ancient mammal continued androgynous, 
after it had acquired the chief distinctions of its class, and 
therefore after it had diverged from the lower classes of the 
vertebrate kingdom? This seems very improbable, for we 
have to look to fishes, the lowest of all the classes, to find 
any still existent androgynous forms.” That various, acces- 
sory parts, proper to each sex, are found in a rudimentary 
condition in the opposite sex, may be explained by such 
organs having been gradually acquired by the one sex, and 
then transmitted in a more or less imperfect state to the 
other. When we treat of sexual selection we shall meet 
with innumerable instances of this form of transmission— 
as in the case of the spurs, plumes, and brilliant colors 
acquired for battle or ornament by male birds, and in- 
herited by the females in an imperfect or rudimentary 
condition. 


26 This is the conclusion of Prof. Gegenbaur, one of the highest authorities 
in comparative anatomy; see ‘‘Grundziige der vergleich. Anat.,’? 1870, s. 876, 
The result has been arrived at chiefly from the study of the Amphibia; but it 
appears from the researches of Waldeyer (as quoted in ‘‘Journal of Anat. and 
Phys.,’’ 1869, p. 161), that the sexual organs of even ‘‘the higher vertebrata 
are, in their early condition, hermaphrodite.’’ Similar views have long been 
held by some authors, though until recently without a firm basis. 

1 The male Thylacinus offers the best instance. Owen, ‘‘Anatomy of 
Vertebrates,’’ vol. iii. p. 771. 

28 Hermaphroditism has been observed in several species of Serranus, as 
well as in some other fishes, where it is either normal and symmetrical or ab- 
normal and unilateral. Dr. Zouteveen has given me references on this subject, 
more especially to a paper by Prof. Halbertsma, in the ‘‘Transact. of the Dutch 
Acad. of Sciences,’’ vol. xvi. Dr. Ginther doubts the fact, but it has now 
been recorded by too many good observers to be any longer disputed. Dr. M. 
Lessona writes to me that he has verified the observations made by Cavolini 
on Serranus. Prof. Ercolani has recently shown (‘‘Accad. delle Scienze,’’ 
Bologna, Dec, 28, 1871) that eels are androgynous. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 217 


The possession by male mammals of functionally imper- 
fect mammary organs is, in some respects, especially curi- 
ous. The Monotremata have the proper milk-secreting 
glands with orifices, but no nipples; and as these animals 
stand at the very base of the mammalian series, it is prob- 
able that the progenitors ot the class also had milk- 
secreting glands, but no nipples. This conclusion is sup- 
ported by what is known of their manner of develop- 
ment; for Professor Turner informs me, on the authority 
of Kélliker and Langer, that in the embryo the mammary 
glands can be distinctly traced before the nipples are in 
the least visible; and the development of successive parts 
in the individual generally represents and accords with 
the development of successive beings in the same line of 
descent. The Marsupials differ from the Monotremata by 
possessing nipples; so that probably these organs were first 
acquired by the Marsupials, after they had diverged from, 
and risen above, the Monotremata, and were then trans- 
mitted to the placental mammals.” No one will suppose 
that the Marsupials still remained androgynous after they 
had approximately acquired their present structure. How 
then are we to account for male mammais possessing 
mammz? It is possible that they were first developed 
in the females and then transferred to the males; but 
from what follows this is hardly probable. _ 

It may be suggested, as another view, that long after 
the progenitors of the whole mammalian class had ceased 
to be androgynous, both sexes yielded milk, and thus nour- 
ished their young; and in the case of the Marsupials, that 
both sexes carried their young in marsupial sacs. This will 
not appear altogether improbable, if we reflect that the males 
of existing syngnathous fishes receive the eggs of the females 


' 99 Prof, Gegenbaur has shown (‘‘Jenaische Zeitschrift,’ B. vii. p. 212) 
that two distinct types of nipples prevail throughout the several mammalian 
orders, but that it is quite intelligible how both could have been derived from 
the nipples of the Marsupials, and the latter from those of the Monotremata. 
See, also, a memoir by Dr. Max Huss, on the mammary glands, ibid., B. viii. 
p. 176. = ; 

Descent—VoL. f.—10 


218 : THE DESCENT OF MAN 


in their abdominal pouches, hatch them, and afterward, as 
some believe, nourish the young; that certain other male 
fishes hatch the eggs within their mouths or branchial cavi- 
ties; that certain male toads take the chaplets of eggs from 
the females, and wind them round their own thighs, keeping 
them there until the tadpoles are born; that certain male 
birds undertake the whole duty of incubation, and that 
male pigeons, as well as the females, feed their nestlings 
with a secretion from their crops. But the above sugges- 
tion first occurred to me from the mammary glands of male 
mammals being so much more perfectly developed than the 
rudiments of the other accessory reproductive parts, which 
are found in the one sex though proper to the other. The 
mammary glands and nipples, as they exist in male mam- 
mals, can indeed hardly be called rudimentary; they are 
merely not fully developed, and not functionally active. 
They are sympathetically affected under the influence of 
certain diseases, like the same organs in the female. They 
often secrete a few drops of milk at birth and at puberty; 
this latter fact occurred in the curious case, before referred 
to, where a young man possessed two pairs of mamme. In 
man and some other male mammals these organs have been 
known occasionally to become so well developed during 
maturity as to yield a fair supply of milk. Now if we 
suppose that during a former prolonged period male mam- 
mals aided the females in nursing their offspring,” and that 
afterward from some cause (as from the production of a 
smaller number of young) the males ceased to give this 
aid, disuse of the organs during maturity would lead to 
their becoming inactive; and from two well-known princi- 


30 Mr. Lockwood believes (as quoted in ‘‘Quart. Journal of Science,’’ April, 
1868, p. 269), from what he has observed of the development of Hippocampus, 
that the walls of the abdominal pouch of the male in some way afford nourish- 
ment. On male fishes hatching the ova in their mouths, see a very interesting 
paper by Prof. Wyman, in “Proc, Boston Soc. of Nat. Hist., ” Sept. 15, 1857; 
also Prof. Turner, in “Journal of Anat, and Phys.,”’ Nov. 1, 1866, p. 78. 
Dr. Giinther has likewise described similar cases. 

31 Mdlle. C. Royer has suggested a similar view in her ‘‘Origine de 
YHomme,”’ ete, 1870. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 219 


ples of inheritance this state of inactivity would probably 
be transmitted to the males at the corresponding age of 
maturity. But atan earlier age these organs would be left 
unaffected, so that they would be almost equally well devel- 
oped in the young of both sexes. 


Conclusion.—Von Baer has defined advancement or prog- 
ress in the organic scale better than any one else, as resting 
on the amount of differentiation and specialization of the 
several parts of a being—when arrived at maturity, as I 
should be inclined to add. Now as organisms have become 
slowly adapted to diversified lines of life by means of natu- 
ral selection, their parts will have become more and more 
differentiated and specialized for various functions, from the 
advantage gained by the division of physiological labor, © 
The same part appears often to have been modified first 
for one purpose, and then long afterward for some other 
and quite distinct purpose; and thus all the parts are ren- 
dered more and more complex. But each organism still 
retains the general type of structure of the progenitor from 
which it was aboriginally derived. In accordance with this 
view it seems, if we turn to geological evidence, that organi- 
zation on the whole has advanced throughout the world by 
slow and interrupted steps. In the great kingdom of the 
Vertebrata it has culminated in man. It must not, however, 
be supposed that groups of organic beings are always sup- 
planted, and disappear as soon as they have given birth to 
other and more perfect groups. The latter, though victori- 
ous over their predecessors, may not have become better 
adapted for all places in the economy of nature. Some old 
forms appear to have survived from inhabiting protected 
sites, where they have not been exposed to very severe 
competition; and these often aid us in constructing our gene- 
alogies, by giving us a fair idea of former and lost popula- 
tions. But we must not fall into the error of looking at the 
existing members of any lowly organized group as perfect 
representatives of their ancient predecessors. 


220 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


The most ancient progenitors in the kingdom of the Ver- 
tebrata, at which we are able to obtain an obscure glance, 
apparently consisted of a group of marine animals,” resem- 
bling the larvee of existing Ascidians. These animals proba- 
bly gave rise to a group of fishes, as lowly organized as the 
lancelet; and from these the Ganoids, and other fishes like 
the Lepidosiren, must have been developed. From such 
fish a very small advance would carry us on to the Am- 
phibians. We have seen that birds and reptiles were once 
intimately connected together; and the Monotremata now 
connect mammals with reptiles in a slight degree. But no 
one can at present say by what line of descent the three 
higher and related classes, namely, mammals, birds, and 
reptiles, were derived from the two lower vertebrate classes, 
namely, amphibians and fishes. In the class of mammals 
the steps are not difficult to conceive which led from the 
ancient Monotremata to the ancient Marsupials; and from 
these to the early progenitors of the placental mammals. 
We may thus ascend to the Lemuride; and the interval is 
not very wide from these to the Simiade. The Simiade 


8 The inhabitants of the sea-shore must be greatly affected by the tides; 
animals living either about the mean high-water mark, or about the mean low- 
water mark, pass through a complete cycle of tidal changes in a fortnight. 
Consequently, their food supply will undergo marked changes week by week. 
The vital functions of such animals, living under these conditions for many 
generations, can hardly fail to run their course in regular weekly periods. 
Now it isa mysterious fact that in the higher and now terrestrial Vertebrata, 
as well as in other classes, many normal and abnormal processes have one or 
more whole weeks as their periods; this would be rendered intelligible if the 
Vertebrata are descended from an animal allied to the existing tidal Ascidians, 
Many instances of such periodic processes might be given, as the gestation of 
mammals, the duration of fevers, etc. The hatching of eggs affords also a good 
example, for, according to Mr. Bartlett (‘‘Land and Water,’’ Jan. 7, 1871), the 
eggs of the pigeon are hatched in two weeks; those of the fowl in three; those 
of the duck in four; those of the goose in five; and those of the ostrich in 
seven weeks. As far as we can judge, a recurrent period, if approximately 
of the right duration for any process or function, would not, when once gained, 
be liable to change; consequently it might be thus transmitted through almost 
any number of generations. But if the function changed, the period would 
have to change, and would be apt to change almost abruptly by a whole week, 
This conclusion, if sound, is highly remarkable; for the period of gestation in 
each mammal, and the hatching of each bird’s eggs, and many other vital 
processes, thus betray to us the primordial birthplace of these animals. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 221 


then branched off into two great stems, the New World 
and Old World monkeys; and from the latter, at a re- 
mote period, Man, the wonder and glory of the Universe, 
proceeded. 

Thus we have given to man a pedigree of prodigious 
length, but not, it may be said, of noble quality. The 
world, it has often been remarked, appears as if it had 
long been preparing for the advent of man; and this, in 
one sense, is strictly true, for he owes his birth to a long © 
line of progenitors. If any single link in this chain had 
never existed, man would not have been exactly what he 
now is. Unless we wilfully close our eyes, we may, with 
our present knowledge, approximately recognize our parent- 
age; vor need we feel ashamed of it. The most humble 
organism is something much higher than the inorganic dust 
under our feet; and no one with an unbiased mind can 
study any living creature, however humble, without being 
struck with enthusiasm at its marvellous structure and 
properties. 


222 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


CHAPTER VII 


ON THE RACES OF MAN 


The nature and value of specific characters—Application to the races of 
man—Arguments in favor of, and opposed to, ranking the so-called 
races of man as distinct species—Sub-species—Monogenists and polyg- 
enists—Convergence of character—Numerous points of resemblance 
in body and mind between the most distinct races of man—The state 
of man when he first spread over the earth—Each race not descended 
from a single pair—The extinction of races—The formation of races— 
The effects of crossing—Slight influence of the direct action of the 
conditions of life—Slight or no influence of natural selection—Sexual 
selection 


called races of men; but 1 am about to inquire what 

is the value of the differences between them under a 
classificatory point of view, and how they have originated. 
In determining whether two or more allied forms ought to 
be ranked as species or varieties, naturalists are practically 
guided by the following considerations; namely, the amount 
of difference between them, and whether such differences 
relate to few or many points of structure, and whether 
they are of physiological importance; but more especially | 
whether they are constant. Constancy of character is what 
is chiefly valued and sought for by naturalists. When- 
ever it can be shown, or rendered probable, that the forms 
in question have remained distinct for a long period, this 
becomes an argument of much weight in favor of treat- 
ing them as species. Even a slight degree of sterility be- 
tween any two forms when first crossed, or in their offspring, 
is generally considered as a decisive test of their specific dis- 
tinctness; and their continued persistence without blending 
within the same area is usually accepted as sufficient evi- 


|" IS not my intention here to describe the several so- 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 223 


dence, either of some degree of mutual sterility, or, in the 
case of animals, of some mutual repugnance to pairing. 

Independently of fusion from intercrossing, the complete 
absence, in a well-investigated region, of varieties linking 
together any two closely allied forms, is probably the most 
important of all the criterions of their specific distinctness; 
and this is a somewhat different consideration from mere 
constancy of character, for two forms may be highly vari- 
able and yet not yield intermediate varieties. Geographical 
distribution is often brought into play unconsciously and 
sometimes consciously; so that forms living in two widely 
separated areas, in which most of the other inhabitants are 
specifically distinct, are themselves usually looked at as 
distinct; but in truth this affords no aid in distinguishing 
geographical races from so-called good or true species. 

Now let us apply these generally admitted principles to 
the races of man, viewing him in the same spirit as a natu- 
ralist would any other animal. In regard to the amount of 
difference between the races, we must make some allowance 
for our nice powers of discrimination gained by the long 
habit of observing ourselves. In India, as Elphinstone re- 
marks, although a newly arrived European cannot at first 
distinguish the various native races, yet they soon appear 
to him extremely dissimilar;' and the Hindu cannot at 
first perceive any difference between the several Huropean 
nations. Even the most distinct races of man are much 
more like each other in form than would at first be sup- 
posed; certain negro tribes must be excepted, while others, 
as Dr. Rohlfs writes to me, and as I have myself seen, have 
Caucasian features. This general similarity is well shown by 

~ the French _photographs | in the Collection Anthropologique 
du Muséum de Paris of the men belonging to various races, 
the greater number of which might pass for Huropeans, as 
many persons to whom | have shown them have remarked. 
Nevertheless, these men, if seen alive, would undoubtedly 


1 «History of India,” 1841, vol. i. p. 323, Father Ripa makes exactly the 
epme remark with respect to the Chinese. 


224 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


appear very distinct, so that we are clearly much influenced 
in our judgment by the mere color of the skin and hair, by 
slight differences in the features, and by expression. 

There is, however, no doubt that the various races, when 
carefully compared and measured, differ much from each 
other—as in the texture of the hair, the relative proportions 
of all parts of the body,’ the capacity of the lungs, the form 
and capacity of the skull, and even in the convolutions of 
the brain.* But it would be an endless task to specify the’ 
numerous points of difference. ‘The races differ also in 
constitution, in acclimatization, and in liability to certain 
diseases. Their mental characteristics are likewise very 
distinct; chiefly as it would appear in their emotional, but 
partly in their intellectual faculties. Hvery one who has 
had the opportunity of comparison must have been struck 
with the contrast between the taciturn, even morose, abo- 
rigines of South America and the light-hearted, talkative 
negroes. There is a nearly similar contrast between the” 
Malays and the Papuans,* who live under the same physi- 
cal conditions, and are separated from each other only by 
a narrow space of sea. 

We will first consider the arguments which may be 
advanced in favor of classing the races of man as distinct 
species, and then the arguments on the other side. If a 
naturalist, who had never before seen a Negro, Hottentot, 
Australian, or Mongolian, were to compare them, he woul 
at once perceive that they differed in a multitude of char 
ters, some of slight and some of considerable importance. 
On inquiry he would find that they were adapted to live 
under widely different climates, and that they differed some- 


? A vast number of measurements of Whites, Blacks, and Indians are given 
in the “‘Investigations in “the Military and Anthropolog. Statistics of American 
Soldiers,’’ by B. A. Gould, 1869, pp. 298-358; ‘“On the capacity of the lungs,’® 
p. 471. See also the numerous and valuable tables, by Dr. Weisbach, from the 
observations of Dr. Scherzer and Dr. Schwarz in the ‘Reise der Novara; 
Anthropolog. Theil,”? 1867. 

3 See, for instance, Mr. Marshall’s account of the brain of a Bushwoman, 
in ‘Phil. Transact.,”? 1864, p. 519. 

4 Wallace, “The Malay " Archipelago,” vol. ii,, 1869, p. 178. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 225 


what in bodily constitution and mental disposition. If he 
were then told that hundreds of similar specimens could 
be brought from the same countries, he would assuredly 
declare that they were as good species as many to which 
he had been in the habit of affixing specific names. This 
conclusion would be greatly strengthened as soon as he had 
ascertained that these forms had all retained the same char- 
acter for many centuries; and that negroes, apparently iden- 
tical with existing negroes, had lived at least 4,000 years 
ago.* He would also hear, on the authority of an excellent 
observer, Dr. Lund,* that the human skulls found in the 
caves of Brazil, intombed with many extinct mammals, be- 
longed to the same type as that now prevailing throughout 
the American Continent. . 

Our naturalist would then perhaps turn to geographical 
distribution, and he would probably declare rae ee. 
must be distinct species which differ not only in appear- 
ance, but are fitted for hot as well as damp or dry countries, 
and for the Arctic regions. He might appeal to the fact 
that no species in the group next to man, namely the 
Quadrumana, can resist a low temperature, or any consid- 
erable change of climate; and that the species which come 
nearest to man have never been reared to maturity, even 


7 


5 With respect to the figures in the famous Egyptian caves of Abou-Simbel, 
M. Pouchet says (‘‘The Plurality of the Human Races,’’ Eng. translat., 1864, 
p. 50) that he was far from finding recognizable representations of the dozen 
or more nations which some authors believe that they can recognize. Even 
some of the most strongly marked races cannot be identified with that degree 
of unanimity which might have been expected from what has been written on 
the subject. Thus Messrs, Nott and Gliddon (“‘Types of Mankind,” p. 148) 
state that Rameses II., or the Great, has features superbly European; whereas 
Knox, another firm believer in the specific distinctness of the races of man 
eid of Man,”’ 1850, p. 201), speaking of young Memnon (the same as 

meses II., as I am informed by Mr. Birch), insists in the strongest manner 
that he is identical in character with the Jews of Antwerp. Again, when 
¥ looked at the statue of Amunoph IIL., I agreed with two officers of the estab- 
lishment, both competent judges, that he had a strongly marked negro type of 
features; but Messrs. Nott and Gliddon (ibid., p. 146, fig. 53) describe him 
es a hybrid, but not of ‘‘negro intermixture.”’ 

6 As quoted by Nott and Gliddon, ‘‘Types of Mankind,” 1854, p. 439. 
They give also corroborative evidence; but C. Vogt thinks that the subject 
requires further investigation. 


226 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


under the temperate climate of Europe. He would be 
deeply impressed with the fact, first noticed by Agassiz," 
that the different races of man are distributed over the 
world in the same zoological provinces as those inhab- 
ited by undoubtedly distinct species and genera of mam- 
mals, This is manifestly the case with the Australian, 
Mongolian, and Negro races of man; in a less well-marked 
manner with the Hottentots; but plainly with the Papuans 
and Malays, who are separated, as Mr. Wallace has shown, 
by nearly the same line which divides the great Malayan 
and Australian zoological provinces. The Aborigines of 
America range throughout the Continent; and this at first - 
appears opposed to the above rule, for most of the produc- 
tions of the Southern and Northern halves differ widely: 
yet some few living forms, as the opossum, range from the 
one into the other, as did formerly some of the gigantic 
Edentata. The Eskimos, like other Arctic animals, extend 
round the whole polar regions. It should be observed that 
the amount of difference between the mammals of the sev- 
eral zoological provinces does not correspond with the de- 
gree of separation between the latter; so that it can hardly 
be considered as an anomaly that the Negro differs more, 
and the American much less, from the other races of man 
than do the mammals of the African and American conti- 
nents from the mammals of the other provinces. Man, it 
may be added, does not appear to have aboriginally inhab- 
ited any oceanic island; and in this respect he resembles 
the other members of his class. 

In determining whether the supposed varieties of the 
game kind of domestic animal should be ranked as such, 
or as specifically distinct, that is, whether any of them are 
descended from distinct wild species, every naturalist would 
tay much stress on the fact of their external parasites being 
specifically distinct. All the more stress would be laid on 
this fact, as it would be an exceptional one; for | am in- 


7 “Diversity of Origin of the Human Races,”’ in the ‘*Christian Examiner, * 
July, 1850. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 227 


formed by Mr. Denny that the most different kinds of dogs, 
fowls, and pigeons, in England, are infested by the same 
species of Pediculi or lice. Now Mr. A. Murray has care- 
fully examined the Pediculi collected in different countries 
from the different races of man;° and he finds that they 
differ not only in color, but in the structure of their claws 
and limbs. In every case in which many specimens were 
obtained the differences were constant. The surgeon of a 
whaling ‘ship in the Pacific assured me that when the Pedi- 
euli with which some Sandwich Islanders on board swarmed, 
strayed on to the bodies of the English sailors, they died 
in the course of three or four days. These Pediculi were 
darker colored, and appeared different from those proper to 
the natives of Chiloe, in South America, of which he gave 
me specimens. These, again, appeared larger and much 
softer than European lice. Mr. Murray procured four kinds 
from Africa, namely, from the Negroes of the Hastern and 
Western coasts, from the Hottentots and Kaffirs; two kinds 
from the natives of Australia; two from North and two from 
South America. In these latter cases it may be presumed 
that the Pediculi came from natives inhabiting different dis- 
tricts. With insects slight structural differences, if constant, 
are generally esteemed of specific value; and the fact of the 
races of man being infested by parasites which appear to be 
specifically distinct might fairly be urged as an argument 
that the races themselves ought to be classed as distinct 
species. ; 

Our supposed naturalist, having proceeded thus far in 
his investigation, would next inquire whether the races of 
men, when crossed, were in any degree sterile. He might 
consult the work® of Prof. Broca, a cautious and philosophi- 

al observer, and in this he would find good evidence that 
some races were quite fertile together, but evidence of an 
opposite nature in regard to other races. Thus it has been 


8 “Transact. R. Soc. of Edinburgh,” vol. xxii., 1861, p. 567. 
® “Qn the Phenomena of Hybridity in the Geaus Homo,” Eng. translat., 
1864. 


228 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


asserted that the native women of Australia and Tasmania 
rarely produce children to European men; the evidence, 
however, on this head has now been shown to be almost 
valueless. The half-castes are killed by the pure blacks: 
and an account has lately been published of eleven half- 
caste youths murdered and burned at the same time, whose 
remains were found by the police.” Again, it has often 
been said that when mulattoes intermarry they produce few 
children; on the other hand, Dr. Bachman, of Charleston," 
positively asserts that he has known mulatto families which 
have intermarried for several generations, and have contin- 
ued on an average as fertile as either pure whites or pure 
blacks. Inquiries formerly made by Sir C. Lyell on this’ 
subject led him, as he informs me, to the same conclusion.” 
In the United States the census for the year 1854 included, 
according to Dr. Bachman, 405,751 mulattoes; and this 
number, considering all the circumstances of the case, seems 

small; but it may partly be accounted for by the degraded | 
and anomalous position of the class, and by the’profligacy 
of the women. A certain amount of absorption of mulattoes 
into negroes must always be in progress; and this would 
lead to an apparent diminution of the former. The inferior 
vitality of mulattoes is spoken of in a trustworthy work” as 
a well-known phenomenon; and this, although a different 


10 See the interesting letter by Mr. T. A. Murray, in the ‘‘Anthropolog. 
Review,’’ April, 1868, p. lili, In this letter Count Strzelecki’s statement, that 
Australian women who have borne children to a white man are afterward 
sterile with their own race, is disproved. M. A. de Quatrefages has also 
collected (‘Revue des Cours Scientifiques,’’ March, 1869, p. 239) much evi- 
dence that Australians and Europeans are not sterile when crossed. 

11 “An Examination of Prof. Agassiz’s Sketch of the Nat. Provinces of the 
Animal World,’’ Charleston, 1855, p. 44. 

12 Dr, Rohlifs writes to me that he found the mixed races in the Great 
Sahara, derived from Arabs, Berbers, and Negroes of three tribes, extraordi- 
narily fertile. On the other hand, Mr. Winwood Reade informs me that the 
negroes on the Gold Coast, though admiring white men and mulattoes, have 
a maxim that mulattoes should not intermarry, as the children are few and 
sickly. This belief, as Mr. Reade remarks, deserves attention, as white men 
have visited and resided on the Gold Coast for four hundred years, so that the 
natives have had ample time to gain knowledge through experience. 

13 **Military and Anthropolog. Statistics of American Soldiers,’’ by B. A. 
Gould, 1869, p. 319. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 229 


consideration from their lessened fertility, may perhaps be 
advanced as a proof of the specific distinctness of the parent 
races. No doubt both animal and vegetable hybrids, when 
produced from extremely distinct species, are liable to pre- 
mature death; but the parents of mulattoes cannot be put 
under the category of extremely distinct species. The com- 
mon Mule, so notorious for long life and vigor, and yet so 
sterile, shows how little necessary connection there is in 
hybrids between lessened fertility and vitality; other analo- 
gous cases could be cited. 

Even if it should hereafter be proved that all the races 
of men were perfectly fertile together, he who was inclined 
from other reasons to rank them as distinct species might 
with justice argue that fertility _and_sterility are notsafe— 
criterions of specific distinctness. We know that these 
qualities are easily affected by changed conditions of life, 
or by close interbreeding, and that they are governed by 
highly complex laws; for instance, that of the unequal 
fertility of converse crosses between the same two species. 
With forms which must be ranked as undoubted species, a 
perfect series exists from those which are absolutely sterile 
when crossed, to those which are almost or completely fer- 
tile. The degrees of sterility do not coincide strictly with 
the degrees of difference betwéen the parents in external 
structure or habits of life. Man in many respects may be 
compared with those animals which have long been domesti- 
cated, and a large body of evidence can be advanced in favor 
of the Pallasian doctrine, that domestication tends to elimi- 


4% “Phe Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,’’ vol. ii. 
p. 109. I may here remind the reader that the sterility of species when crossed 
is not a specially acquired quality, but, like the incapacity of certain trees to 
be grafted together, is incidental on other acquired differences, The nature 
of these differences is unknown, but they relate more especially to the repro- 
ductive system, and much less so to external structure or to ordinary differences 
in constitution. One important element in the sterility of crossed species ap- 
parently lies in one or both having been long habituated to fixed conditions; 
for we know that changed conditions have a special influence on the reproduc- 
tive system, and we have good reason to believe (as before remarked) that the 
fluctuating conditions of domestication tend to eliminate that sterility which 
is so general with species, in a natural state, when crossed. It has elsewhere 


230 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


nate the sterility which is so general a result of the crossing 
of species in a state of nature. From these several con- 
siderations it may be justly urged that the perfect fertility 
of the intercrossed races of man, if established, would not 
absolutely preclude us from ranking them as distinct species. 

Independently of fertility, the characters presented by 
the offspring from a cross have been thought to indicate 
whether or not the parent-forms ought to be ranked as spe- 
cies or varieties; but, after carefully studying the evidence, 
I have come to the conclusion that no general rules of this 
kind can be trusted. The ordinary result of a cross is the 
production of a blended or intermediate form; but in certain 
cases some of the offspring take closely after one parent- 
form, and some after the other. This is especially apt to 
occur when the parents differ in characters which first 
appeared as sudden variations or monstrosities."* I refer 
to this point because Dr. Rohlfs informs me that he has 
frequently seen in Africa the offspring of negroes crossed 
with members of other races, either completely black or 
‘completely white, or rarely piebald. On the other hand, it 
is notorious that in America mulattoes commonly present 
an intermediate appearance. 

We have now seen that a naturalist might feel himself 


been shown by me (ibid., vol. ii. p. 185, and ‘‘Origin of Species,’’ 5th edit, 
p. 317) that the sterility of crossed species has not been acquired through 
natural selection: we can see that when two forms have already been rendered 
very sterile, it is scarcely possible that their sterility should be augmented by 
the preservation or survival of the more and more sterile individuals; for as the 
sterility increases, fewer and fewer offspring will be produced from which to 
breed, and at last only single individuals will be produced, at the rarest inter- 
vals. But there is even a higher grade of sterility than this. Both Gartner 
and Kélreuter have proved that in genera of plants including many species, a 
series can be formed from species which when crossed yield fewer and fewer 
seeds, to species which never produce a single seed, but yet are affected by the 
pollen of the other species, as shown by the swelling of the germen. It is here 
manifestly impossible to select the more sterile individuals, which have already 
ceased to yield seeds; so that the acme of sterility, when the germen alone is 
affected, cannot have been gained through selection. This acme, ard no doubt 
the other grades of sterility, are the incidental results of certam unknown 
differences in the constitution of the reproductive system of the species which 
are crossed. 
1% «The Variation of Animals,’’ etc., vol ii. p. 92. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 281 


inlly justified in ranking the races of man as distinct species; 
for he has found that they are distinguished by many differ- 
ences in structure and constitution, some being of impor- 
tance. These differences have also remained nearly constant 
for very long periods of time. Our naturalist will have 
been in some degree influenced by the enormous range 
of man, which is a great anomaly in the class of mammals, 
if mankind be viewed as a single species. He will have 
been struck with the distribution of the several so-called 
races, which accords with that of other undoubtedly distinct 
species of mammals. Finally, he might urge that the 
mutual fertility of all the races has not as yet been fully 
proved, and even if proved would not be an absolute proof 
of their specific identity. 


On the other side of the question, if our supposed natur- 
alist were to inquire whether the forms of man keep distinct 
like ordinary species, when mingled together in large num- 
bers in the same country, he would immediately discover 
that this was by no means the case. In Brazil he would 
behold an immense mongrel population of Negroes and 
Portuguese; in Chiloe and other parts of South America he 
would behold the whole population consisting of Indians 
and Spaniards blended in various degrees.’* In many parts 
of the same continent he would meet with the most complex 
crosses between Negroes, Indians, and Europeans; and, 
judging from the vegetable kingdom, such triple crosses 
afford the severest test of the mutual fertility of the parent- 
forms. In one island of the Pacific he would find a small 
population of mingled Polynesian and English blood; and 
in the Fiji Archipelago a population of Polynesian and 
Negritos crossed in all degrees. Many analogous cases 
could be added; for instance, in Africa. Hence the races 


38 M. de Quatrefages has given (“‘Anthropolog. Review,’’ January, 1869, 
p. 22) an interesting account of the success and energy of the Paulistas in 
Brazil, who are a much-crossed race of Portuguese and Indians, with a mixture 
of the blood of other races. 


prone 


232 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


of man are not sufficiently distinct to inhabit the same-conn- 
try without fusion; and the absence of fusion affords the 
usual and best test of specific distinctness. 

Our naturalist would likewise be much disturbed as soon 
as he perceived that the distinctive characters of all the 
races were highly variable. This fact strikes every one on 
first beholding the negro slaves in Brazil, who. have been 
imported from all parts of Africa. The same remark holds 
good with the Polynesians, and with many other races. It 
may be doubted whether any character can be named which 
is distinctive of a race and is constant. Savages, even within 
the limits of the same tribe, are not nearly so uniform in 
character as has been often asserted. Hottentot women offer 
certain peculiarities, more strongly marked than those oc- 
curring in any other race, but these are known not to be 
of constant occurrence. In the several American tribes, 
color and hairiness differ considerably; as does color to a 
certain degree, and the shape of the features greatly, in the 
negroes of Africa. The shape of the skull varies much in 
some races;’7 and so it is with every other character. Now 
all naturalists have learned by dearly bought experience 
how rash it is to attempt to define species by the aid of 
inconstant characters. 

But the most weighty of all the arguments against treat- 
ing the races of man as distinct species is that they graduate 
into each other, independently in many cases, as far as we 
can judge, of their having intercrossed. Man has been 
studied more carefully than any other animal, and yet 
there is the greatest possible diversity among capable judges 
whether he should be classed as a single species or race, or 
as two (Virey), as three (Jacquinot), as four (Kant), five 
{Blumenbach), six (Buffon), seven (Hunter), eight (Agas- 
siz), eleven (Pickering), fifteen (Bory St. Vincent), sixteen. 


" For instance with the aborigines of America and Australia. Prof, 
Huxley says (‘‘Transact. Internat. Congress of Prehist. Arch.,’’ 1868, p. 105) 
that the skulls of many South Germans and Swiss are ‘‘as short and as broad 
as those of the Tartars,’’ etc. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 233 


(Desmoulins), twenty-two (Morton), sixty (Crawfurd), or as 
sixty-three, according to Burke."* This diversity of judg- 


‘ment does not prove that the races ought not to be ranked 


as species, but it shows that they graduate into each other, 


and that it is hardly possible to discover clear distinctive 


characters between them. 

Every naturalist who has had the misfortune to under- 
take the description of a group of highly varying organisms 
has encountered cases (I speak after experience) precisely 
like that of man, and if of a cautious disposition, he will 
end by uniting all the forms which graduate into each other 
under a single species; for he will say to himself that he has 
no right to give names to objects which he cannot define. 
Cases of this kind occur in the Order which includes man, 
namely, in certain genera of monkeys; while in other genera, 
as in Cercopithecus, most of the species can be determined 
with certainty. In the American genus Cebus, the various 
forms are ranked by some naturalists as species, by others 
as mere geographical races. Now if numerous specimens of 
Cebus were collected from all parts of South America, and 
those forms which at present appear to be specifically dis- 
tinct were found to graduate into each other by close steps, 
they would usually be ranked as mere varieties or races; 
and this course has been followed by most naturalists with 
respect to the races of man. Nevertheless, it must be con- 
fessed that there are forms, at least in the vegetable king- 
dom,” which we cannot avoid naming as species, but which 
are connected together by numberless gradations, indepen- 
dently of intercrossing. 

Some naturalists have lately employed the term ‘‘sub- 
species’’ to designate forms which possess many of the 


18 See a good discussion on this subject in Waitz, ‘‘Introduct. to Anthro- 
pology,’’ Eng. translat., 1863, pp. 198-208, 227. I have taken some of the 
above statements from H. Tuttle’s ‘‘Origin and Antiquity of Physical Man,” 
Boston, 1866, p. 35. mi : Y 

19 Prof. Nageli has carefully described several striking cases in his 
“Botanische Mittheilungen,’? B. ii., 1866, s. 294-369. Prof. Asa Gray has 
made analogous remarks on some intermediate forms in the Composite of 
North America. 


234 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


characteristics of true species, but which hardly deserve so 
high arank. Now if we reflect on the weighty arguments 
above given, for raising the races of man to the dignity of | 
species, and the insuperable difficulties on the other side 
in defining them, it seems that the term ‘‘sub-species’’ might | 
here be used with propriety. But from long habit the term | 
‘‘race’’ will perhaps always be employed. The choice of 
terms is only sb far important in that it is desirable to use, 
as far as possible, the same terms for the same degrees of 
difference. Unfortunately this can rarely be done: for the 
larger genera generally include closely allied forms, which 
can be distinguished only with much difficulty, while the 
smaller genera within the same family include forms that 
are perfectly distinct; yet all must be ranked equally as 
species. So again, species within the same large genus by 
no means resemble each other to the same degree; on the 
contrary, some of them can generally be arranged in little 
groups round other species, like satellites:round planets.” 


The question whether mankind consists of one or several 
species has of late years been much discussed by anthropolo- 
gists, who are divided into the two schools of monogenists 
and po lygenists. Those who do not admit the principle of 
evolution must look at species as separate creations, or as 
in some manner as distinct entities; and they must decide 
what forms of man they will consider as species by the 
analogy of the method commonly pursued in ranking other 
organic beings as species. But it is a hopeless endeavor to 
decide this point, until some definition of the term ‘‘species’’ 
is generally accepted; and the definition must not include 
an indeterminate element such as an act of creation. We 
might as well attempt without any definition to decide 
whether a certain number of houses should be called a 
village, town, or city. We have a practical illustration 
of the difficulty in the never-ending doubts whether many 
closely allied mammals, birds, insects, and plants, which 


90 “Origin of Species,’’ 5th edit., p. 68. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 235 


represent each other respectively in North America and 
Hurope, should be ranked as species or geographical races; 
and the like holds true of the productions of many islands 
situated at some little distance from the nearest continent. 

Those naturalists, on the other hand, who admit the 
principle of evolution, and this is now admitted by the ma- 
jority of rising men, will feel no doubt that all the races 
of man are descended from a single primitive stock; whether 
or not they may think fit to designate the races as distinct 
species, for the sake of expressing their amount of differ- 
ence.” With our domestic animals the question whether 
the various races have arisen from one or more species is 
somewhat different. Although it may be admitted that all 
the races, as well as all the natural species within the same 
genus, have sprung from the same primitive stock, yet it is 
a fit subject for discussion, whether all the domestic races of 
the dog, for instance, have acquired their present amount 
of difference since some one species was first domesticated 
by man; or whether they owe some of their characters to 
inheritance from distinct species, which had already been 
differentiated in a state of nature. With man no such ques- 
tion can arise, for he cannot be said to have been domesti- 
cated at any particular period. 

During an early stage in the divergence of the races of 
man from a common stock, the differences between the races 
and their number must have been small; consequently, as 
far as their distinguishing characters are concerned, they 
then had less claim to rank as distinct species than the ex- 
isting so-called races. Nevertheless, so arbitrary is the term 
of species, that such early races would perhaps have been 
ranked by some naturalists as distinct species, if their dif- 
ferences, although extremely slight, had been more constant 
than they are at present, and had not graduated into each 
other. 

It is however possible, though far from probable, that 


21 See Prof. Huxley to this effect in the ‘Fortnightly Review,’’ 1865, 
p. 275. 


236 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


the early progenitors of man might formerly have diverged 
much in character, until they became more unlike each other 
than any now existing races; but that subsequently, as sug- 
gested by Vogt,” they converged in character. When man 
selects the offspring of two distinct species for the same ob- 
ject, he sometimes induces a considerable amount of conver- 
gence, as far as general appearance is concerned. This is 
the case, as shown by Von Nathusius,” with the improved 
breeds of the pig, which are descended from two distinct 
species; and in a less marked manner with the improved 
breeds of cattle. A great anatomist, Gratiolet, maintains 
that the anthropomorphous apes do not form a natural 
sub-group; but that the orang is a highly developed gib- 
bon or semnopithecus, the chimpanzee a highly developed 
macacus, and the gorilla a highly developed mandrill. If 
this conclusion, which rests almost exclusively on brain- 
characters, be admitted, we sbould have a case of conver- 
gence, at least in external characters, for the anthropo- 
morphous apes are certainly more like each other in many 
points than they are to other apes. All analogical resem- 
blances, as of a whale to a fish, may indeed be said to be 
cases of convergence; but this term has never been applied 
to superficial and adaptive resemblances. It would, how- 
ever, be extremely rash to attribute to convergence close 
similarity of character in many points of structure among 
the modified descendants of widely distinct beings. The 
form of a crystal is determined solely by the molecular 
forces, and it is not surprising that dissimilar substances 
should sometimes assume the same form; but with organic 
“beings we should bear in mind that the form of each de- 
pends on an infinity of complex relations, namely, on varia- 
tions due to causes far too intricate to be followed—on the 
nature of the variations preserved, these depending on the 


2 “Tectures on Man,’ Eng. translat., 1864, p. 468. 

23 "Die Racen des Schweines,’’ 1860, s. 46. ‘‘Vorstudien fiir Geschichte, 
etc., Schweineschaédel,’? 1864, s. 104. With respect to cattle, see M. de 
Quatrefages, ‘‘Unité de l’Espéce Humaine,’’ 1861, p. 119. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 237 


physical conditions, and still more on the surrounding or- 
ganisms which compete with each—and lastly, on inheri- 
tance (in itself a fluctuating element) from innumerable 
progenitors, all of which have had their forms determined 
through equally complex relations. It appears incredible 
that the modified descendants of two organisms, if these 
differed from each other in a marked manner, should ever 
afterward converge so closely as to lead to a near approach 
to identity throughout their whole organization. In the 
case of the convergent races of pigs above referred to, evi- 
dence of their descent from two primitive stocks is, accord- 
ing to Von Nathusius, still plainly retained in certain bones 
of their skulls. If the races of man had descended, as is 
supposed by some naturalists, from two or more species, 
which differed from each other as much, or nearly as 
much, as does the orang from the gorilla, it can hardly 
:be doubted that marked differences in the structure of 
certain bones would still be discoverable in man as he 
“now exists. 

Although the existing races of man differ in many re- 
spects, as in color, hair, shape of skull, proportions of the 
body, etc., yet, if their whole structure be taken into con- 
sideration, they are found to resemble each other closely in 
a multitude of points. Many of these are of so unimportant 
or of so singular a nature that it is extremely improbable 
that they should have been independently acquired by abo- 
riginally distinct species or races. The same remark holds 
good with equal or greater force with respect to the numer- 
ous points of mental similarity between the most distinct 
races of man. The American aborigines, Negroes, and 
Europeans are as different from each other in mind as any 
three races that can be named; yet I was incessantly struck, 
while living with the Fuegians on board the ‘‘Beagle,”’ with 
the many little traits of character showing how similar their 
minds were to ours; and so it was with a full-blooded negro 
with whom I happened once to be intimate. 


238 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


He who will read Mr. Tylor’s and Sir J. Lubbock’s 
interesting works” can hardly fail to be deeply impressed 
with the close similarity between the men of all races in 
tastes, dispositions, and habits. This is shown by the 
pleasure which they all take in dancing, rude music, act- 
ing, painting, tattooing, and otherwise decorating them- 
selves; in their mutual comprehension of gesture-language, 
by the same expression in their features, and by the same 
inarticulate cries, when excited by the same emotions. This 
similarity, or rather identity, is striking, when contrasted 
with the different expressions and cries made by distinct 
species of monkeys. There is good evidence that the art 
of shooting with bows and arrows has not been handed 
down from any common progenitor of mankind; yet, as 
Westropp and Nilsson have remarked,* the stone arrow- 
heads, brought from the most distant parts of the world, 
and manufactured at the most remote periods, are almost | 
identical; and this fact can only be accounted for by the 
various races having similar inventive or mental powers. 
The same observation has been made by archeologists” 
with respect to certain widely prevalent ornaments, such 
as zigzags, etc.; and with respect to various simple beliefs 
and customs, such as the burying of the dead under mega- 
lithic structures. I remember observing in South America,” 
that there, as in so many other parts of the world, men have 
generally chosen the summits of lofty hills to throw up piles 
of stones, either as a record of some remarkable event, or for 
burying their dead. 

Now when naturalists observe a close agreement in 
numerous small details of habits, tastes, and dispositions 
between two or more domestic races, or between nearly 


24 Tylor’s ‘‘Early History of Mankind,’’ 1865; with respect to gesture- 
language, see p. 54. Lubbock’s ‘‘Prehistoric Times,”’ 2d edit., 1869. 

25 “On Analogous Forms of Implements,’’ in ‘‘Memoirs of Anthropolog, 
Soc.,”? by H. M. Westropp. ‘‘The Primitive Inhabitants of Scandinavia,” 
Eng. translat., edited by Sir J. Lubbock, 1868, p. 104. 

26 Westropp, ‘“‘On Cromlechs,”’ etc., “‘Journal of Ethnological Soc.,”’ as 
given in ‘‘Scientific Opinion,’’ June 2, 1869, p. 3. 

21 “Journal of Researches: Voyage of the Beagle,”’ p. 46. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 289 


allied natural forms, they use this fact as an argument that 
they are descended from a common progenitor who was thus 
endowed; and consequently that all should be classed under 
the same species. ‘The same argument may be applied with 
much force to the races of man. 

As it is improbable that the numerous and unimportant 
points of resemblance between the several races of man in 
bodily structure and mental faculties (I do not here refer 
to similar customs) should all have been independently 
acquired, they must have been inherited from progenitors 
who had these same characters. We thus gain some insight 
into the early state of man, before he had spread step by step 
over the face of the earth. The spreading of man to regions 
widely separated by the sea, no doubt, preceded any great 
amount of divergence of character in the several races; for 
otherwise we should sometimes meet with the same race in 
distinct continents; and this is never the case. Sir J. Lub- 
bock, after comparing the arts now practiced by savages in 
all parts of the world, specifies those which man could not 
have known when he first wandered from his original birth- 
place; for if once learned they would never have been for- 
gotten.” He thus shows that ‘‘the spear, which is but a 
development of the knife-point, and the club, which is but 
along hammer, are the only things left.’” He admits, how- 
ever, that the art of making fire probably had been already 
discovered, for it is common to all the races now existing, 
and was known to the ancient cave inhabitants of Europe. 
Perhaps the art of making rude canoes or rafts was likewise 


| known; but as man existed at a remote epoch, when the 
‘land in many places stood at a very different level to what 


it does now, he would have been able, without the aid of 
canoes, to have spread widely. Sir J. Lubbock further re- 
marks how improbable it is that our earliest ancestors could 
have ‘‘counted as high as ten, considering that so many 
races now in existence cannot get beyond four.’’ Neverthe- 
less, at this early period, the intellectual and social faculties 


% “Prehistoric Times,’’ 1869, p. 574. 


240 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


of man could hardly have been inferior in any extreme degree 
to those possessed at present by the lowest savages; otherwise 
primeval man could not have been so eminently successful 
in the struggle for life, as proved by his early and wide 
diffusion. 

From the fundamental differences between certain lan- 

_guages, some philologists have inferred that when man first 
became widely diffused, he was not a speaking animal; 
but it may be suspected that languages, far less perfect than 
any now spoken, aided by gestures, might have been used, 
and yet have left no traces on subsequent and more highly 
developed tongues. Without the use of some language, 
however imperfect, it appears doubtful whether man’s in- 
tellect could have risen to the standard implied by his 
dominant position at an early period. 

Whether primeval man, when he possessed but few arts, 
and those of the rudest kind, and when his power of lan- 
guage was extremely imperfect, would have deserved to 
be called man, must depend on the definition which we 
employ. In a series of forms graduating insensibly from 
some ape-like creature to man as he now exists, it would 
be impossible to fix on any definite point when the term 
‘‘man’’ ought to be used. But this is a matter of very little 


importance. So, again, it is almost a matter of indifference ~ 


whether the so-called races of man are thus designated, or 
are ranked as species or sub-species; but the latter term 
appears the more appropriate. Finally, we may conclude 
that when the principle of evolution is generally accepted, 
as it surely will be before long, the dispute between the 
monogenists and polygenists will die a silent and unob- 
served death, 


One other question ought not to be passed over without 
notice, namely, whether, as is sometimes assumed, each sub- 
species or race of man has sprung from a single pair of pro- 
genitors. With our domestic animals a new race can readily 
be formed by carefully matching the varying offspring from 


% 


= 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 241 


a single pair, or even from a single individual possessing 
some new character; but most of our races have been formed, 
not intentionally from a selected pair, but unconsciously by 
the preservation of many individuals which have varied, 
however slightly, in some useful or desired manner. If 
in one country stronger and heavier horses, and in another 
country lighter and fleeter ones, were habitually preferred, 
we may feel sure that two distinct sub-breeds would be pro- 
duced in the course of time, without any one pair having 
been separated and bred from, in either country. Many 
races have been thus formed, and their manner of formation 
is closely analogous to that of natural species. We know, 
also, that the horses taken to the Falkland Islands have, 
during successive generations, become smaller and weaker, 
while those which have run wild on the Pampas have ac- 
quired larger and coarser heads; and such changes are mani- 
festly due, not to any one pair, but to all the individuals 
having been subjected to the same conditions, aided, per- 
haps, by the principle of reversion. The new sub-breeds in 
such cases are not descended from any single pair, but from 
many individuals which have varied in different degrees, 
but in the same general manner; and we may conclude that 
the races of man have been similarly produced, the modifica- 
tions being either the direct result of exposure to different 
conditions, or the indirect result of some form of selection. 
But to this latter subject we shall presently return. 


On the Extinction of the Races of Man.—The partial or 
complete extinction of many races and sub-races of man 
is historically known. Humboldt saw in South America a 
parrot which was the sole living creature that could speak 
a word of the language of a lost tribe.“ Ancient monuments 
and stone implements found in all parts of the world, about 
which no tradition has been preserved by the present inhab- 
itants, indicate much extinction. Some small and broken 
tribes, remnants of former races, still survive in isolated and 


generally mountainous districts. In Europe the ancient 
Descent—Vo. I.—11 


| This race is 
| modern, that we have ever heard of.’ * It differed, there- 


242 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


races were all, according to Schaaffhausen,” ‘‘lower in the 
scale than the rudest living savages’; they must therefore 
have differed, to a certain extent, from any existing race. 
The remains described by Prof. Broca from Les Eyzies, 
though they unfortunately appear to have belonged to a 
single family, indicate a race with a most singular com- 
bination of low or simious, and of high characteristics. 
“entirely different from any other, ancient or 


fore, from the quaternary race of the caverns of Belgium. 

Man can long resist conditions which appear extremely 
unfavorable for his existence. He has long lived in the 
extreme regions of the North, with no wood for his canoes 
or implements, and with only blubber as fuel and melted 
snow as drink. In the southern extremity of America the 
Fuegians survive without the protection of clothes, or of 


\ 


any building worthy to be called a hovel. In South Africa . 


the aborigines wander over arid plains, where dangerous 
beasts abound. Man can withstand the deadly influence of 
the Terai at the foot of the Himalaya, and the pestilential 
shores of tropical Africa. 

Extinction follows chiefly from the competition of tribe 
with tribe, and race with race. Various checks are always 
in action, serving to keep down the numbers of each sav- 
age tribe—such as periodical famines, nomadic habits, and 
the consequent deaths of infants, prolonged suckling, wars, 
accidents, sickness, licentiousness, the stealing of women, 
infanticide, and especially lessened fertility. If any one of 
these checks increases in power, even slightly, the tribe 
thus affected tends to decrease; and when of two adjoining 
tribes one becomes less numerous and less powerful than 
the other, the contest is soon settled by war, slaughter, 
cannibalism, slavery, and absorption. Even when a weaker 
tribe is not thus abruptly swept away, if it once begins to 


29 Translation in ‘‘Anthropological Review,’’ October, 1868, p. 431. 

2 ‘Transact. Internat. Congress of Prehistoric Arch.,’? 1868, pp. 172-15. 
See also Broca (translation) in ‘‘ Anthropological Review,’’ October, 1868, p. 410. 

81 Dr, Gerland ‘‘Ueber das Aussterben der Naturvélker,’’ 1868, s. 82. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 243 


decrease, it generally goes on decreasing until it becomes 
extinct.” 

When civilized nations come into contact with barbarians 
the struggle is short, except where a deadly climate gives its 
aid to the native race. Of the causes which lead to the vic- 
tory of civilized nations, some are plain and simple, others 
complex and obscure. We can see that the cultivation of 
the land will be fatal in many ways to savages, for they 
cannot, or will not, change their habits. New diseases and 
vices have in some cases proved highly destructive; and it 
appears that a new disease often causes much death, until 
those who are most susceptible to its destructive influence 
are gradually weeded out;* and so it may be with the evil 
effects from spirituous liquors, as well as with the uncon- 
querably strong taste for them shown by so many savages. 
It further appears, mysterious as is the fact, that the first 

Co. of distinct and separated peoples generates disease.™ 
Mr. Sproat, who in Vancouver Island closely attended to 
the subject of extinction, believed that changed habits of 
life, consequent on the advent of Huropeans, induces much 
ill-health. He lays, also, great stress on the apparently 
trifling cause that the natives become ‘‘bewildered and dull 
by the new life around them; they lose the motives for 
exertion, and get no new ones in their place.’’ * 

The grade of their civilization seems to be a most impor- 
tant element in the success of competing nations. A few 

enturies ago Europe feared the inroads of Hastern barba- 

| rans now any such fear would be ridiculous. It is a more 
curious fact, as Mr. Bagehot has remarked, that savages did 
not formerly waste away before the classical nations, as they 
now do before modern civilized nations; had they done so, 


82 Gerland (‘Ueber das Aussterben der Naturvélker,’’ 1868, s. 12) gives 
facts in support of this statement. 

38 See remarks to this effect in Sir H. Holland’s ‘‘Medical Notes and Reflec- 
tions,’’ 1839, p. 390. 

24 I have collected (‘‘Journal of Researches: Voyage of the Beagle,” 
p. 435) a good many cases bearing on this subject; see also Gerland (ibid., 
8. 8). Poeppig speaks of the ‘‘breath of civilization as poisonous to savages.” 
Sproat, “Scenes and Studies of Savage Life,’’ 1868, p. 284. 


244 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


Kp the old moralists would have mused over the event; but 


i there is no lament in any writer of that period over the 


perishing barbarians.*° The most potent of all the causes 
of extinction appears in many cases to be lessened fertility 
and ill-health, especially among the children, arising from 
changed conditions of life, notwithstanding that the new 
conditions may not be injurious in themselves. J am much 
indebted to Mr. H. H. Howorth for having called my atten- 
tion to this subject, and for having given me information 
respecting it. I have collected the following cases. 
When Tasmania was first colonized the natives were 
roughly estimated by some at 7,000, and by others at 
20,000. Their number was soon greatly reduced, chiefly 
by fighting with the English and with each other. After 
the famous hunt by all the colonists, when the remaining 
natives delivered themselves up to the government, they 
consisted only of 120 individuals,*” who were in 1882 trans- 
ported to Flinders Island. This island, situated between 
Tasmania and Australia, is forty miles long, and from 
twelve to eighteen miles broad: it seems healthy, and 
the natives. were well treated. Nevertheless, they suffered 
greatly in health. In 1834 they consisted (Bonwick, p. 250) 
of forty-seven adult males, forty-eight adult females, and 
sixteen children, or in all of 111 souls. In 1835 only one 
hundred were left. As they continued rapidly to decrease, 


my and as they themselves thought that they should not perish 


so quickly elsewhere, they were removed in 1847 to Oyster 
Cove, in the southern part of Tasmania. They then con- 
sisted (December 20, 1847) of fourteen men, twenty-two 
women, and ten children. But the change of site did no 
good. Disease and death still pursued them, and in 1864 
one man (who died in 1869), and three elderly women alone 


36 Bagehot, ‘‘Physics and Politics,’’ ‘‘Fortnightly Review,” April 1, 1868, 
p. 455. 

37 All the statements here given are taken from ‘‘The Last of the Tas- 
manians,’’ by J. Bonwick, 1870. 

% This is the statement of the Governor of Tasmania, Sir. W. Denison, 
“Varieties of Vice-Regal Life,”? 1870, vol. i. p. 6%. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 245 


survived. The infertility of the women is even a more 
remarkable fact than the liability of all to ill-health and 
death. At the time when only nine women were left at 
Oyster Cove, they told Mr. Bonwick (p. 886) that only two 
had ever borne children: and these two had together pro- 


things, Dr. Story remarks that death followed the attempts 
to civilize the natives. ‘‘If left to themselves to roam as 
they were wont and undisturbed, they would have reared 
more children, and there would have been less mortality.” 
Another careful observer of the natives, Mr. Davis, remarks: 
‘“The births have been few, and the deathsnumerous. This 
may have been in a great measure owing to their change of 
living and food; but more so to their banishment from the 
mainland of Van Diemen’s Land, and consequent depression 
of spirits’’ (Bonwick, pp. 888, 390). 

Similar facts have been observed in two widely different 
parts of Australia. The celebrated explorer, Mr. Gregory, 
told Mr. Bonwick that in Queensland ‘‘the want of repro- 
duction was being already felt with the blacks, even in the 
most recently settled parts, and that decay would set in.” 
Of thirteen aborigines from Shark’s Bay who visited Mur- 
chison River, twelve died of consumption within three 
months. * 

The decrease of the Maories of New Zealand has been 
carefully investigated by Mr. Fenton, in an admirable Re- 
port, from which all the following statements, with one 
exception, are taken.*® The decrease in number since 1830 
is admitted by every one, including the natives themselves, 
and is still steadily progressing. Although it has hitherto 
been found impossible to take an actual census of the na- 
tives, their numbers were carefully estimated by residents 


- 39 For these cases, see Bonwick’s ‘‘Daily Life of the Tasmanians,” 1870, 
p. 90; and ‘The Last of the Tasmanians,’’ 1870, p. 386. : 
40 “Observations on the Aboriginal Inhabitants of New Zealand,”’ published 
by the Government, 1859, 


» 


246 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


in many districts. The result seems trustworthy, and shows 
that during the fourteen years previous to 1858 the decrease 
was 19.42 per cent. Some of the tribes thus carefully ex- 
amined lived above a hundred miles apart, some on the 
coast, some inland; and their means of subsistence and 
habits differed to a certain extent (p. 28). The total num- 
ber in 1858 was believed to be 58,700, and in 1872, after a 
second interval of fourteen years, another census was taken, 
and the number is given as only 36,359, showing a decrease 
of 32.29 per cent!** Mr. Fenton, after showing in detail the 
insufficiency of the various causes usually assigned in expla- 
nation of this extraordinary decrease, such as new diseases, 
the profligacy of the women, drunkenness, wars, etc., con- 
cludes on weighty grounds that it depends chiefly on the 
unproductiveness of the women, and on the extraordinary 
mortality of the young children (pp. 31, 34). In proof of 
this he shows (p. 83) that in 1844 there was one non-adult 
for every 2.57 adults; whereas in 1858 there was only one 
non-adult for every 3.27 adults. The mortality of the adults 
is also great. He adduces as a further cause of the decrease 
. |the inequality of the sexes; for fewer females are born than 
males. To this latter point, depending perhaps on a widely 
‘distinct cause, I shall return in a future chapter. Mr. Fen- 
ton contrasts with astonishment the decrease in New Zealand 
with the increase in Ireland—countries not very dissimilar in 
climate, and where the inhabitants now follow nearly similar 
habits. The Maories themselves (p. 35) ‘‘attribute their de- 
cadence, in some measure, to the introduction of new food 
and clothing, and the attendant change of habits’; and it 
will be seen, when we consider the influence of changed 
conditions on fertility, that they are probably right. The 
diminution began between the years 1830 and 1840; and 
Mr. Fenton shows (p. 40) that about 1830 the art of manu- 
facturing putrid corn (maize), by long steeping in water, 
was discovered and largely practiced; and this proves that 
a change of habits was beginning among the natives even 


4 “New Zealand,’’ by Alex. Kennedy, 1873, p. 47. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 247 


when New Zealand was only thinly inhabited by Europeans. 
When I visited the Bay of Islands in 1885, the dress and food 
of the inhabitants had already been much modified: they 
raised potatoes, maize, and other agricultural produce, and 
exchanged them for English manufactured goods and tobacco. 

It is evident, from many statements in the life of Bishop 
Patteson,*? that the Melanesians of the New Hebrides and 
neighboring archipelagoes suffered to an extraordinary de- 
gree in health, and perished in large numbers, when they . 
were removed to New Zealand, Norfolk Island, and other 
salubrious places, in order to be educated as missionaries. 


~~ The decrease of the native population of the Sandwich 
Tslands is as notorious as that of New Zealand. It has been 


} 


roughly estimated, by those best capable of judging, that 
when Cook discovered the Islands in 1779, the population 
amounted to about 800,000. According to a loose census in 
1823, the numbers then were 142,050. In 1832, and at sever- 
al subsequent periods, an accurate census was officially taken, 


\ but I have been able to obtain only the following returns: 


\ 


Native Porutation. {Annual rate of decrease per 
— cent, assuming it to have 
(Except during 1832 and| been uniform between the 
Year. 1836, when the few for-| successive censuses; these 
eigners in the islands} censuses being taken at ir- 
were included.) regular intervals. 
DOB Diss isccevessondecteeesadeactae 130,313 
} 4.46 
TRS Giccsiwasseacereataneaccn sees 108,579 
t 2.47 
LEDS sicaesrinprcnesenesidosscincaces 71,019 
t 0.81 
L860 wieawosedacdeverssines: saeneus 67,084 
2.18 
TS GCi, cicesosss cndonanecanciaesse 58,765 
- 2.17 
LST Qi ssvciedasgansicaendenceoabees 51,531 


We here see that in the interval of forty years, between 
1832 and 1872, the population has decreased no less than 


2 “Tife of J. C. Patteson,’’? by C. M. Monge, 1874; see more especially 


- vol. i. p. 530. 


248 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


sixty-eight per cent! This has been attributed by most 
writers to the profligacy of the women, to former bloody 
wars, and to the severe labor imposed on conquered tribes 
and to newly introduced diseases, which have been on sev- 
eral occasions extremely destructive. No doubt these and 
other such causes have been highly efficient, and may ae- 
count for the extraordinary rate of decrease between the 
years 1832 and 1836; but the most potent of all the causes 
+ seems to be lessened fertility. According to Dr. Ruschen- 
berger of the U. S. Navy, who visited these islands between 
1835 and 1837, in one district of Hawaii, only twenty-five 
men out 1,184, and in another district only ten out of 687, 
had a family with as many as three children. Of eighty 
married women, only thirty-nine had ever borne children; 
and ‘‘the official report gives an average of half a child to 
each married couple in the whole island.’’ This is almost 
exactly the same average as with the Tasmanians at Oyster 
Cove. Jarves, who published his History in 1848, says that 
‘‘families who have three children are freed from all taxes; 
those having more, are rewarded by gifts of land and other 
encouragements.’’ This unparalleled enactment by the gov- 
ernment well shows how infertile the race had become. The 
Rev. A. Bishop stated in the Hawaiian ‘‘Spectator,’’ in 
1839, that a large proportion of the children die at early 
ages, and Bishop Staley informs me that this is still the 
case, just as in New Zealand. This has been attributed to 
the neglect of the children by the women, but it is probably 
in large part due to innate weakness of constitution in the 
children, in relation to the lessened fertility of their parents. 
There is, moreover, a further resemblance to the case of New 
Zealand, in the fact that there is a large excess of- male over 
female births: the census of 1872 gives 31,650 males to 25,247 
females of all ages, that is 125.386 males for every 100 fe- 
;-mnales; whereas in all civilized countries the females exceed 
\ the males. No doubt the profligacy of the women may in 
part account for their small fertility; but their changed hab- 
its of life is a much more probable cause, and which will at 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 249 


the same time account for the increased mortality, especially 
of the children. The islands were visited by Cook in 1779, 
by Vancouver in 1794, and often subsequently by whalers. 
In 1819 missionaries arrived, and found that idolatry had 
been already abolished, and other changes effected by the 
king. After this period there was a rapid change in almost 
all the habits of life of the natives, and they soon became 
“the most civilized of the Pacifie Islanders.’’ One of my 
informants, Mr. Coan, who was born on the islands, remarks 
that the natives have undergone a greater change in their 
habits of life in the course of fifty years than Englishmen 
during a thousand years. From information received from 
Bishop Staley, it does not appear that the poorer classes 
have ever much changed their diet, although many new 
kinds of fruit have been introduced, and the sugar-cane 
is in universal use. Owing, however, to their passion for 
imitating Europeans, they altered their manner of dressing 
at an early period, and the use of alcoholic drinks became 
very general. Although these changes appear inconsider- 
able, I can well believe, from what is known with respect 
to animals, that they might suffice to lessen the fertility of 
the natives.** 

Lastly, Mr. Macnamara states“ that the low and degraded 
inhabitants of the Andaman Islands, on the eastern side of 
the Gulf of Bengal, are ‘‘eminently susceptible to any change 
of climate; in fact, take them away from their island homes, 
and they are almost certain to die, and that independently of 
diet or extraneous influences.’’ He further states that the 
inhabitants of the Valley of Nepal, which is extremely hot 


4 The foregoing statements are taken chiefly from the following works 
“Jarves’s History of the Hawaiian Islands,’’ 1843, pp. 400-407. Cheever, 
“Life in the Sandwich Islands,’’ 1851, p, 277. Ruschenberger is quoted by 
Bonwick, ‘‘Last of the Tasmanians,’’ 1870, p. 378. Bishop is quoted by Sir 
E. Belcher, ‘‘Voyage Round the World,’’ 1843, vol. i. p. 272. I owe the 
census of the several years to the kindness of Mr. Coan, at the request of 
Dr. Youmans, of New York; and in most cases I have compared the Youmans 
figures with those given in several of the above-named works. I have omitted 
the census for 1850, as I have seen two widely different numbers given. 

#4 “The Indian Medica} Gezette,’’ November 1, 1871, p. 240. 


250 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


in summer, and also the various hill-tribes of India, suffer 
from dysentery and fever when on the plains; and they die 
if they attempt to pass the whole year there. 

We thus see that many of the wilder races of man are apt 
to suffer much in health when subjected to changed condi- 
tions or habits of life, and not exclusively from being trans- 
ported to a new climate. Mere alterations in habits, which 
do not appear injurious in themselves, seem to have this 
same effect; and in several cases the children are particu- 
larly liable to suffer. It has often been said, as Mr. Mac- 
namara remarks, that man can resist with impunity the 
greatest diversities of climate and other changes; but this 
is true only of the civilized races. Man in his wild condi- 
tion seems to be in this respect almost as susceptible as his 
nearest allies, the anthropoid apes, which have never yet 
survived long, when removed from their native country. 

Lessened fertility from changed conditions, as in the 
case of the Tasmanians, Maories, Sandwich Islanders, and 
apparently the Australians, is still more interesting than 
their liability to ill-health and death; for even a slight de- 
gree of infertility, combined with those other causes which 
tend to check the increase of every population, would sooner 
or later lead to extinction. The diminution of fertility may 
be explained in some cases by the profligacy of the women 
‘(as until lately with the Tahitians), but Mr. Fenton has 
shown that this explanation by no means suffices with the 
New Zealanders, nor does it with the Tasmanians. 

In the paper above quoted, Mr. Macnamara gives reasons 
for believing that the inhabitants of districts subject to 
malaria are apt to be sterile; but this cannot apply in several 
of the above cases. Some writers have suggested that the 
aborigines of islands have suffered in fertility and health 
from long-continued interbreeding; but in the above cases 
infertility has coincided too closely with the arrival of Euro- 
peans for us to admit this explanation. Nor have we ar 
present any reason to believe that man is highly sensitive 
to the evil effects of interbreeding, especially in areas so 


_ THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 251 


large as New Zealand, and the Sandwich Archipelago with 
its diversified stations. On the contrary, it is known that 
the present inhabitants of Norfolk Island are nearly all 
cousins or near relations, as are the Todas in India, and the 
inhabitants of some of the Western Islands of Scotland; 
and yet they seem not to have suffered in fertility.*® 

A much more probable view is suggested by the analogy 
of the lower animals. The reproductive system can be 
shown to be susceptible to an extraordinary degree (though 
why we know not) to changed conditions of. life; and this 
susceptibility leads both to beneficial and to evil results. 
A large collection of facts on this subject is given in chapter 
xviii. of volume ii. of my ‘‘Variation of Animals and Plants 
under Domestication.’’ I can here give only the briefest 
abstract; and every one interested in the subject may consult 
the above work. Very slight changes increase the health, 
vigor, and fertility of most or all organic beings, while 
other changes are known to render a large number of ani- 
mals sterile. One of the most familiar cases is that of tamed 
elephants not breeding in India, though they often breed in 
Ava, where the females are allowed to roam about the forests 
to some extent, and are thus placed under more natural 
conditions. The case of various American monkeys, both 
sexes of which have been kept for many years together in 
their own countries, and yet have very rarely or never bred, 
is a more apposite instance, because of their relationship to 
man. It is remarkable how slight a change in the condi- 
tions often induces sterility in a wild animal when captured; 
and this is the more strange as all our domesticated animals 
have become more fertile than they were in a state of 
nature; and some of them can resist the most unnatural 
‘conditions with undiminished fertility.** Certain groups of 


45 On the close relationship of the Norfolk Islanders, see Sir W. Denison, 
“Varieties of Vice-Regal Life,’? vol. i., 1870, p. 410. For the Todas, see 
Colonel Marshall’s work, 1873, p. 110. For the Western Islands of Scotland, 
Dr. Mitchell, ‘Edinburgh Medical Journal,’’? March to June, 1865. 

4 For the evidence on this head, see ‘‘Variation of Animals,”’ etc., vol. ii, 
p- 111. 


252 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


animals are much more liable than others to be affected 
by captivity; and generally all the species of the same 
group are affected in the same manner. But sometimes a 
. Single species in a group is rendered sterile, while the others 
are not so; on the other hand, a single species may retain its 
fertility while most of the others fail to breed. The males 
and females of some species when confined, or when allowed 
to live almost, but not quite free, in their native country, 
never unite; others thus circumstanced frequently unite but 
never produce offspring; others again produce some off- 
spring, but fewer than in a state of nature; and, as bearing 
on the above cases of man, it is important to remark that 
the young are apt to be weak and sickly, or malformed, 
and to perish at an early age. 

Seeing how general is this law of the susceptibility of 
the reproductive system to changed conditions of life, and 
that it holds good with our nearest allies, the Quadrumana, 
I can hardly doubt that it applies to man in his primeval 
state. Hence if savages of any race are induced suddenly 
to change their habits of life, they become more or less 
sterile, and their young offspring suffer in health in the 
same manner and from the same cause as do the ‘elephant 
and hunting-leopard in India, many monkeys in America, 
and a host of animals of all kinds, on removal from their 
natural conditions. 

We can see why it is that aborigines, who have long 
inhabited islands, and who must have been long exposed 
to nearly uniform conditions, should be specially affected 
by any change in their habits, as seems to be the case. 
Civilized races can certainly resist changes of all kinds far 
better than savages; and in this respect they resemble do- 
mesticated animals, for though the latter sometimes suffer 
in health (for instance, Kuropean dogs in India), yet they 
are rarely rendered sterile, though a few such instances 
have been recorded.” The immunity of civilized races and 
domesticated animals is probably due to their having been 


47 “Variation of Animals,” ete., vol. ii, p. 16, 


o 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 258 


subjected to a greater extent, and therefore having grown 
somewhat more accustomed, to diversified or varying con- 
ditions, than the majority of wild animals; and to their 
having formerly immigrated or been carried from country 
to country, and to different families or sub-races having 
intercrossed. It appeats that a cross with civilized races 
at once gives to an aboriginal race an immunity from the 
evil consequences of changed conditions. Thus the crossed 
offspring from the Tahitians and English, when settled in 
Pitcairn Island, increased so rapidly that the island was 
soon overstocked; and in June, 1856, they were removed 
to Norfolk Island. They then consisted of 60 married 
persons and 134 children, making a total of 194. Here they 
likewise increased so rapidly that, although sixteen of them 
returned to Pitcairn Island in 1859, they numbered in 
January, 1868, 800 souls—the males and females being 
in exactly equal numbers. What a contrast does this case 
present with that of the Tasmanians; the Norfolk Islanders 
increased in only twelve and a half years from 194 to 800; 
whereas the Tasmanians decreased during fifteen years from 
120 to 46, of which latter number only ten were children. 
So again in the interval between the census of 1866 and 
1872 the natives of full blood in the Sandwich Islands 
decreased by 8,081, while the half-castes, who are believed 
to be healthier, increased by 847; but I do not know 
whether the latter number includes the offspring from the 
half-castes, or only the half-castes of the first generation. 
The cases which I have here given all relate to aborigines 
who have been subjected to new conditions as the result of 
the immigration of civilized men. But sterility and ill- 
health would probably follow, if savages were compelled 
by any cause, such as the inroad of a conquering tribe, to 
desert their homes and to change their habits. It is an in- 
teresting circumstance that the chief check to wild animals 


4 These details are taken from ‘‘The Mutineers of the Bounty,’’ by Lady 
Belcher, 1870; and from ‘‘Pitcairn Island,’’ ordered to be printed by the House 
of Commons, May 29, 1863. The following statements about the Sandwich 
Islanders are from the ‘‘Honolulu Gazette,’’ and from Mr. Coan. 


254 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


becoming domesticated, which implies the power of their 
breeding freely when first captured, and one chief check to 
wild men, when brought into contact with civilization, sur- 
viving to form a civilized race, is the same, namely, sterility 
from changed conditions of life. 

Finally, although the gradual decrease and ultimate 
' extinction of the races of man is a highly complex problem, 
depending on many causes which differ in different places 
and at different times; it is the same problem as that pre- 
sented by the extinction of one of the higher animals—of 
the fossil horse, for instance, which disappeared from South 
America, soon afterward to be replaced, within the same 
districts, by countless troops of the Spanish horse. The 
New Zealander seems conscious of this parallelism, for, he 
compares his future fate with that of the native rat now 
almost exterminated by the European rat. Though the 
difficulty is great to our imagination, and really great, if 
we wish to ascertain the precise causes and their manner 
of action, it ought not to be so to our reason, as long as we 
keep steadily in mind that the increase of each species and 
each race is constantly checked in various ways; so that if 
any new check, even a slight one, be superadded, the race 
will surely decrease in number; and decreasing numbers 
will sooner or later lead to extinction; the end, in most 
cases, being promptly determined by the inroads of con- 
quering tribes. 


On the Formation of the Races of Man.—In some cases 
the crossing of distinct races has led to the formation of 
anew race. The singular fact that Europeans and Hindus, 
who belong to the same Aryan stock, and speak a language 
fundamentally the same, differ widely in appearance, while 
Europeans differ but little from Jews, who belong to the 
Semitic stock, and speak quite another language, has been 
accounted for by Broca,* through certain Aryan branches 


49 “On Anthropology,” translation, ‘‘Anthropolog. Review,’ Jan. 1868, 
p. 38. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 255 


having been largely crossed by indigenous tribes during 
their wide diffusion. When two races in close contact cross, 
the first result is a heterogeneous mixture: thus Mr. Hunter, 
in describing the Santali or hill-tribes of India, says that 
hundreds of imperceptible gradations may be traced ‘from 
the black, squat tribes of the mountains to the tall, olive- 
colored Brahmin, with his intellectual brow, calm eyes, and 
high but narrow head;’’ so that it is necessary in courts 
of justice to ask the witnesses whether they are Santalis or 
Hindus.” Whether a heterogeneous people, such as the 
inhabitants of some of the Polynesian islands, formed 
by the crossing of two distinct races, with few or no pure 
members left, would ever become homogeneous, is not 
known from direct evidence. But as, with our domesticated 
animals, a cross-breed can certainly be fixed and made 
uniform by careful selection” in the course of a few genera- 
tions, we may infer that the free intercrossing of a hetero- 
geneous mixture during a long descent would supply the 
place of selection, and overcome any tendency to reversion; 
so that the crossed race would ultimately become homo- 
geneous, though it might not partake in an equal degree 
of the characters of the two parent-races, 

Of all the differences between the races of man, the color 
’ of the skin is the most conspicuous and one of the best 
marked. It was formerly thought that differences of this 
kind could be accounted for by long exposure to different 
climates; but Pallas first showed that this is not tenable, 
and he has since been followed by almost all anthropolo- 
gists. This view has been rejected chiefly because the dis- 
tribution of the variously colored races, most of whom must 
have long inhabited their present homes, does not coincide 


50 ‘*Annals of Rural Bengal,’’ 1868, p. 134. 
51 “The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,”’ vol. ii. 


52 Pallas, ‘Act. Acad. St. Petersburg,’’ 1780, part ii. p. 69. He was fol- 
lowed by Rudolphi, in his ‘‘Beytrage zur Anthropologie,”’ 1812. An excellent 
summary of the evidence is given by Godron, ‘‘De l’Espéce,’’ 1859, vol. ii, 
p. 246, ete. 


256 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


with corresponding differences of climate. Some little 
weight may be given to such cases as that of the Dutch 
| families, who, as we hear on excellent authority, have not 
undergone the least change of color after residing for three 
centuries in South Africa. An argument on the same side 
may likewise be drawn from the uniform appearance in 
various parts of the world of gypsies and Jews, though the 
uniformity of the latter has been somewhat exaggerated.” 
A very damp or a very dry atmosphere has been supposed 
to be more influential in modifying the color of the skin 
than mere heat; but as D’Orbigny in South America, and 
Livingstone in Africa, arrived at diametrically opposite 
conclusions with respect to dampness and dryness, any con- 
clusion on this head must be considered as very doubtful.® 

Various facts, which I have given elsewhere, prove that 
the color of the skin and hair is sometimes correlated in 
a surprising manner with a complete immunity from the 
action of certain vegetable poisons, and from the attacks 
of certain parasites. Hence it occurred to me, that negroes 
and other dark races might have acquired their dark tints 
by the darker individuals escaping from the deadly influence 
of the miasma of their native countries, during a long series 
of generations. 

I afterward found that this same idea had long ago 
occurred to Dr. Wells. It has long been known that ne- 
groes, and even mulattoes, are almost completely exempt 
from the yellow fever, so destructive in tropical America.” 
They likewise escape to a large extent the fatal intermittent 


53 Sir Andrew Smith, as quoted by Knox, ‘‘Races of Man,’’ 1850, p. 473. 

54 See De Quatrefages on this head, ‘‘Revue des Cours Scientifiques,’’ Oct. 
17, 1868, p. 731. 

55 Livingstone’s ‘‘Travels and Researches in 8, Africa,’’ 1857, pp. 338, 329. 
D’Orbigny, as quoted by Godron, ‘‘De l’Espéce,”’ vol. ii. p. 266. 

58 See a paper read before the Royal Soc. in 1818, and published in his 
Essays in 1818. I have given an account of Dr. Wells’s views in the Historical 
Sketch (p. xvi.) to my ‘‘Origin of Species.’? Various cases of color correlated 
with constitutional peculiarities are given in my ‘‘Variation of Animals 
ander Domestication,’ vol. ii. pp. 227, 335. 

51 See, for instance, Nott and Gliddon, ‘‘Types of Mankind,’’ p. 68. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 257 


fevers that prevail along at least 2,600 miles of the shores of 
Africa, and which annually cause one-fifth of the white set- 
tlers to die, and another fifth to return home invalided.” 
This immunity in the negro seems to be partly inherent, 
depending on some unknown peculiarity of constitution, 
and partly the result of acclimatization. Pouchet® states 
that the negro regiments recruited near the Soudan, and 
borrowed from the Viceroy of Egypt for the Mexican war, 
escaped the yellow fever almost equally with the negroes 
originally brought from various parts of Africa and accus- 
tomed to the climate of the West Indies. That acclimatiza- 
tion plays a part is shown by the many cases in which ne- 
groes have become somewhat liable to tropical fevers after 
having resided for some time in a colder climate.” The 
nature of the climate under which the white races have 
long resided likewise has some influence on them; for dur- 
ing the fearful epidemic of yellow fever in Demerara during 
1837, Dr. Blair found that the death-rate of the immigrants 
was proportional to the latitude of the country whence they 
had come. With the negro the immunity, as far as it is the 
result of acclimatization, implies exposure during a prodig- 
ious length of time; for the aborigines of tropical America 
who have resided there from time immemorial are not ex- 
empt from yellow fever; and the Rev. H. B. Tristram states 
that there are districts in Northern Africa which the native 
inhabitants are compelled annually to leave, though the 
negroes can remain with safety. 

That the immunity of the negro is in any degree corre- 
lated with the color of his skin is a mere conjecture: it may 
be correlated with some difference in his blood, nervous 
system, or other tissues. Nevertheless, from the facts above 
alluded to, and from some connection apparently existing 


88 Major Tulloch, in a paper read before the Statistical Society, April 20, 
1840, and given in the ‘‘Athenzeum,”’ 1840, p. 353. 

59 “The Plurality of the Human Race”? (translat.), 1864, p. 60. 

6 Quatrefages, ‘‘Unité de l’Espéce Humaine,” 1861, p. 205. Waita, 
“Introduct. to Anthropology,’’ translat., vol. i., 1863, p. 124. Livingstone 
gives analogous cases in his ‘‘Travels.”’ 


258 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


between complexion and a tendency to consumption, the 
conjecture seemed to me not improbable. Consequently I 
endeavored, with but little success,” to ascertain how far 
it holds good. The late Dr. Daniell, who had long lived 
on the West Coast of Africa, told me that he did not believe 
in any such relation. He was himself unusually fair, and 
had withstood the climate in a wonderful manner. When 
he first arrived as a boy on the coast, an old and experi- 
enced negro chief predicted from his appearance that this 
would prove the case. Dr. Nicholson, of Antigua, after 
having attended to this subject, writes to me that he does 
not think that dark-colored Europeans escape the yellow 
fever more than those that are light-colored. Mr. J. M. 
Harris altogether denies that Europeans with dark hair 
withstand a hot climate better than other men: on the 
contrary, experience has taught him, in making a selec- 
tion of men for service on the coast of Africa, to choose 
those with red hair.” As far, therefore, as these slight 


61 In the spring of 1862 I obtained permission from the Director-General of 
the Medical Department of the Army to transmit to the surgeons of the various 
regiments on foreign service a blank table, with the following appended re- 
marks, but I have received no returns. ‘‘As several well-marked cases have 
been recorded with our domestic animals of a relation between the color of the 
dermal appendages and the constitution, and it being notorious that there is 
some limited degree of relation between the color of the races of man and the 
climate inhabited by them, the following investigation seems worth considera- 
tion: Namely, whether there is any relation in Europeans between the color 
of their hair and their liability to the diseases of tropical countries. If the 
surgeons of the several regiments, when stationed in unhealthy tropical dis- 
tricts, would be so good as first to count, as a standard of comparison, how 
many men, in the force whence the sick are drawn, have dark and light-colored 
hair, and hair of intermediate or doubtful tints; and if a similar account were 
kept by the same medical gentlemen, of all the men who suffered from malarious 
and yellow fevers, or from dysentery, it would soon be apparent, after some 
thousand cases had been tabulated, whether there exists any relation between 
the color of the hair and constitutional liability to tropical diseases. Perhaps 
no such relation would be discovered, but the investigation is well worth 
making. In case any positive results were obtained, it might be of some prac- 
tical use in selecting men for any particular service. Theoretically the result 
would be of high interest, as indicating one means by which a race of men 
inhabiting from a remote period an unhealthy tropical climate, might have 
become dark-colored by the better preservation of dark-haired or dark-com- 
plexioned individuals during a long succession of generations.”’ 

62 **Anthropological Review,’’? Jan. 1866, p. xxi. Dr. Sharpe also says, 
with respect to India (‘‘Man a Special Creation,’’ 1873, p. 118), that “it has 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 259 


indications go, there seems no foundation for the hypothe- 
sis that blackness has resulted from the darker and darker 
individuals having survived better during long exposure 
to fever-generating miasma. 

Dr. Sharpe remarks, that a tropical sun, which burns 
and blisters a white skin, does not injure a black one at all; 
and, as he adds, this is not due to habit in the individual, 
for children only six or eight months old are often carried 
about naked, and are not affected. I have been assured by 
a medical man that some years ago during each summer, but 
not during the winter, his hands became marked with light 
brown patches, like, although larger than freckles, and that 
these patches were never affected by sun-burning, while the 
white parts of his skin have on several occasions been much 
inflamed and blistered. With the lower animals there is, 
also, a constitutional difference in liability to the action of 
the sun between those parts of the skin clothed with white 
hair and other parts. Whether the saving of the skin from 
being thus burned is of sufficient importance to account for 
a dark tint having been gradually acquired by man through 
natural selection, I am unable to judge. If it be so, we 
should have to assume that the natives of tropical America 
have lived there for a much shorter time than the negroes in 
Africa, or the Papuans in the southern parts of the Malay 
Archipelago, just as the lighter-colored Hindus have resided 
in India for a shorter time than the darker aborigines of the 
central and southern parts of the peninsula. 

Although with our present knowledge we cannot account 
for the differences of color in the races of man, through any 


been noticed by some medical officers that Europeans with light hair and florid 
complexions suffer less from diseases of tropical countries than persons with 
dark hair and sallow complexions; and, so far as I know, there appear to be 
good grounds for this remark.’? On the .other hand, Mr. Heddle, of Sierra 
Leone, ‘‘who has had more clerks killed under him than any other man,”’ by 
the climate of the West African Coast (W. Reade, ‘‘African Sketch Book,”’ 
vol. ii. p. 522), holds a directly opposite view, as does Capt. Burton. 

6 “*Man a Special Creation,’’ 1873, p. 119. 

‘Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,’’ vol. ii. pp. 
836, 337. 


AG 


260 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


advantage thus gained, or from the direct action of climate; 
yet we must not quite ignore the latter agency, for there is 
good reason to believe that some inherited effect is thus 
produced. * 

We have seen in the second chapter that the conditions 
of life affect the development of the bodily frame in a direct 
manner, and that the effects are transmitted. Thus, as is 
generally admitted, the European settlers in the United 
States undergo a slight but extraordinarily rapid change of 
appearance. Their bodies and limbs become elongated; and 
I hear from Colonel Bernys that during the late war in the 
United States, good evidence was afforded of this fact by 
the ridiculous appearance presented by the German regi- 
ments, when dressed in ready-made clothes manufactured 
for the American market, and which were much too long for 
the men in every way. There is, also, a considerable body 
of evidence showing that in the Southern States the house- 
slaves of the third generation present a markedly different 


_appearance from the field-slaves.” 


If, however, we look to the races of man as distributed 
over the world, we must infer that their characteristic dif- 
ferences cannot be accounted for by the direct action of 
different conditions of life, even after exposure to them for 
an enormous period of time. The Eskimos live exclusively 
on animal food; they are clothed in thick fur, and are ex- 
posed to intense cold and to prolonged darkness; yet they 
do not differ in any extreme degree from the inhabitants of 
Southern China, who live entirely on vegetable food, and 
are exposed almost naked to a hot, glaring climate. The 
unclothed Fuegians live on the marine productions of their 


6 See, for instance, Quatrefages (“‘Revue des Cours Scientifiques,’’ Oct. 
10, 1868, p. 724) on the effects of residence in Abyssinia and Arabia, and 
other analogous cases. Dr. Rolle (‘‘Der Mensch, seine Abstammung,”’ etc., 
1865, s. 99) states, on the authority of Khanikof, that the greater number of 
German families settled in Georgia have acquired in the course of two genera- 
tions dark hair and eyes. Mr. D. Forbes informs me that the Quichuas in the 
Andes vary greatly in color, according to the position of the valleys inhabited 
by them. 

J 66 Harlan, ‘‘Medical Researches,’’ p. 532. Quatrefages (‘‘Unité de l’Espéce 
Humaine,’’ 1861, p. 128) has collected much evidence on this head. 


THE DESCENT OR ORIGIN OF MAN 261 


mhospitabre shores; the Botocudos of Brazil wander about 
the hot forests of the interior and live chiefly on vegetable 
productions; yet these tribes resemble each other so closely 
that the Fuegians on board the ‘‘Beagle’” were mistaken b 

some Brazilians for Botocudos. The Botocudos again, as 
well as the other inhabitants of tropical America, are wholly 
different from the Negroes who inhabit the opposite shores 
of the Atlantic, are exposed to a nearly similar climate, and 


follow nearly the same habits of life. 


Nor can the differences between the races of man be 
accounted for by the inherited effects of the increased 
or decreased use of parts, except to a quite insignificant 
degree. Men who habitually live in canoes may have their 
legs somewhat stunted; those who inhabit lofty regions may 


_ have their chests enlarged; and those who constantly use 


certain sense-organs may have the cavities in which they 
are lodged somewhat increased in size, and their features 
consequently a little modified. With civilized nations, the 
reduced size of the jaws from lessened use—the habitual 
play of different muscles serving to express different emo- 
tions—and the increased size of the brain from greater intel- 
lectual activity, have together produced a considerable effect 


n their general appearance when compared with savages.” 7 


Increased bodily stature, without any corresponding increase 
in the size of the brain, may (judging from the previously 
adduced case of rabbits) have given to some races an elon- 
gated skull of the dolichocephalic type. 

Lastly, the little-understood principle of correlated devel- 
opment has sometimes come into action, as in the case of 
great muscular development and strongly projecting supra- 
orbital ridges. The color of the skin and hair are plainly 
correlated, as is the texture of the hair with its color in the 
Mandans of North America. The color also of the skin, 


81 See Prof. Schaaffhausen, translat. in ‘‘Anthropological Review,’’ Oct. 
1868, p. 429. 

§ Mr. Catlin states (‘‘N. American Indians,’’ 3d edit., 1842, vol. i. p. 49) 
that in the whole tribe of the Mandans, about one in ten or twelve of the 
members, of all ages and both sexes, have bright silvery gray hair, which is 


, 


262 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


and the odor) emitted by it, are likewise in some manner 
connected: “With the breeds of sheep the number of hairs 
within a given space and the number of the execretory 
pores are related. If we may judge from the analogy of 
our domesticated animals, many modifications of struct- 
ure in man probably come under this principle of correlated 
development. = 
We have now seen that the external characteristic differ- 
ences between the races of man cannot be accounted for in a 
satisfactory manner by the direct action of the conditions of 
life, nor by the effects of the continued use of parts, nor 
through the principle of correlation. We are therefore led 
to inquire whether slight individual differences, to which 
man is eminently liable, may not have been preserved and 
augmented during a long series of generations through natural 
selection. But here we are at once met by the objection that 
beneficial variations alone can be thus preserved; and as far 
as we are enabled to judge, although always liable to err on 
his head, none of the differences between the races of man 
[re of any direct or special service to him. The intellec- 
tual and moral or social faculties must of course be excepted 
from this remark. The great variability of all the external 
differences between the races of man likewise indicates that 
they cannot be of much importance; for, if important, they 
would long ago have been either fixed and preserved, or 
eliminated. In this respect man resembles those forms, 
called by naturalists protean or polymorphic, which have 
remained extremely variable, owing, as it seems, to such 
variations being of an indifferent nature, and to their hav- 
ing thus escaped the action of natural selection. 
We have thus far been baffled in all our attempts to 
account for the differences between the races of man; but 


hereditary. Now this hair is as coarse and harsh as that of a horse’s mane, 
while the hair of other colors is fine and soft. 

68 On the odor of the skin, Godron, ‘‘Sur l’Espéce,’’ tom. ii. p. 217. On 
the pores in the skin, Dr. Wilckens, ‘‘Die Aufgaben der Landwirth. Zootechnik,’’ 
1869, s. 7. 


‘ 


STRUCTURE OF THE BRAIN 263 


there remains one important agency, namely Sexual Selec- 
tion, which appears to have acted powerfully on man, as on 
many other animals. Ido not intend to assert that sexual 
selection will account for all the differences between the 
races. An unexplained residuum is left, about which we 
can only say, in our ignorance, that as individuals are con- 
tinually born with, for instance, heads a little rounder or 
narrower, and with noses a little longer or shorter, such slight 
differences might become fixed and uniform, if the unknown 
agencies which induced them were to act in a more constant 
manner, aided by long-continued intercrossing. Such varia- 
tions come under the provisional class alluded to in our sec- 
ond chapter, which for the want of a better term are often 
called spontaneous. Nor do TI pretend that the effects of 
sexual selection can be indicated with scientific precision; 
but it can be shown that it would be an inexplicable fact if 
man had not been modified by this agency, which appears 
to have acted powerfully on innumerable animals. It can 
- further be shown that the differences between the races of 
man, as in color, hairiness, form of features, etc., are of a 
kind which might have been expected to come under the 
influence of sexual selection. But in order to treat this sub- 
ject properly, I have found it necessary to pass the whole 
animal kingdom in review. I have therefore devoted to it 
the Second Part of this work. At the close I shall return 
to man, and, after attempting to show how far he has been 
modified through sexual selection, will give a brief summary 
of the chapters in this First Part. 


Note oN THE RESEMBLANCES AND DIFFERENCES IN THE 
STRUCTURE AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE BRAIN 
IN MAN AND ApEs. By Pror. Huxusy, F.R.S. 


THE controversy respecting the nature and the extent of 
the differences in the structure of the brain, in man and the 
apes, which arose some fifteen years ago, has not yet come 
to an end, though the subject-matter of the dispute is, at 


264 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


present, totally different from what it was formerly. It 
was originally asserted and reasserted, with singular perti- 
nacity, that the brain of all the apes, even the highest, dif- 
fers from that of man, in the absence of such conspicuous 
structures as the posterior lobes of the cerebral hemispheres, 
with the posterior cornu of the lateral ventricle and the 
hippocampus minor, contained in those lobes, which are so 
obvious in man. 

But the truth that the three structures in question are as 
well developed in apes’ as in human brains, or even better; 
and that it is characteristic of all the Primates (if we exclude 
the Lemurs) to have these parts well developed, stands at 
present on as secure a basis as any proposition in compara- 
tive anatomy. Moreover, it is admitted by every one of the 
long series of anatomists who, of late years, have paid spe- 
cial attention to the arrangement of the complicated sulci and 
gyri which appear upon the surface of the cerebral hemi- 
spheres in man and the higher a ke that they are si on 
after the very same pattern in him, as in them. KHvery 
principal gyrus and sulcus of a chimpanzee’s brain is clearl 
represented in that of a man, so that the terminology whic 
applies to the one answers for the other. On this point there 
is no difference of opinion. Some ‘ie be since, Prof. Bischoff 
published a memoir” on the cerebral convolutions of man 
and apes; and as the purpose of my learned colleague was 
certainly not to diminish the value of the differences between 
apes and men in this respect, I am glad to make a citation 
from him: 

“That the apes, and especially the orang, chimpanzee, 
and gorilla, come very close to man in their organization, 
much nearer than to any other animal, is a well-known fact, 
disputed by nobody. Looking at the matter from the point 
of view of organization alone, no one probably would ever 
have disputed the view of Linnzus, that man should be 
placed, merely as a peculiar species, at the head of the mam- 
malia and of those apes. Both show, in all their organs, so 
close an affinity, that the most exact anatomical investiga- 
tion is needed in order to demonstrate those differences 
which really exist. So it is with the brains. The brains 
of man, the orang, the chimpanzee, the gorilla, in spite of 
all the important differences which they present, come very 
close to one another’ (1. ¢., p. 101). 


7 ‘Die Grosshirn-Windungen des Menschen;” ‘‘Abhandlungen der K. 
Bayerischen Akademie,’’ Bd. x., 1868. 


STRUCTURE OF THE BRAIN 265 


There remains, then, no dispute as to the resemblance. 
in fundamental characters, between the ape’s brain an 
man’s; nor any as to the wonderfully close similarity be- 
tween the chimpanzee, orang, and man, in even the details 
of the arrangement of the gyri and sulci of the cerebral 
hemispheres. Nor, turning to the differences between the 
brains of the highest apes and that of man, is there any seri- 
ous question as to the nature and extent of these differences. 
It is admitted that the man’s cerebral hemispheres are abso- 
lutely and relatively larger than those of the orang and chim- 
panzee; that his frontal lobes are less excavated by the up- 
ward protrusion of the roof of the orbits; that his gyri and 
sulci are, as a rule, less symmetrically disposed, and present a 
greater number of secondary plications. And it is admitted 
that, as a rule, in man the temporo-occipital or ‘external per- 

endicular” fissure, which is usually so strongly marked a 
eature of the ape’s brain, is but faintly marked. But it is 
also clear that none of these differences constitute a sharp 
demarcation between the man’s and the ape’s brain. In 
respect to the external perpendicular fissure of Gratiolet, 
in the human brain, for instance, Prof. Turner remarks:” 

‘In some brains it long simply as an indentation of 
the margin of the hemisphere, but, in others, it extends for 
some distance more or less transversely outward. I saw it 
in the right hemisphere of a female brain pass more than 
two inches outwards and in another specimen, also the right 
hemisphere, it proceeded for four-tenths of an inch outward, 
and then extended downward as far as the lower margin of the 
outer surface of the hemisphere. The imperfect definition 
of this fissure in the majority of human brains, as compared 
with its remarkable distinctness in the brain of most Quadru- 
mana, is owing to the presence, in the former, of certain su-: 

erficial, well-marked, secondary convolutions which bridge 
it over and connect the parietal with the occipital lobe. The 
closer the first of these Brdgig ri lies to the longitudinal 
fae ue shorter is the sciraal parieto-occipital fissure”’ 
« Gay Pe 12). 

The obliteration of the external perpendicular fissure of 
Gratiolet, therefore, is not a constant character of the human 
brain. On the other hand, its full development is not a con- 
stant character of the higher ape’s brain. For, in the chim- 
panzee, the more or less extensive obliteration of the exter- 


~ 1 “Convolutions of the Human Cerebrum Topographically Considered,” 
1866, p. 12. 
Descent—Vou. L—12 


266 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


nal perpendicular suleus by ‘‘bridging convolutions,'’ on one 
side or the other, has been noted over and over again by 
Prof. Rolleston, Mr. Marshall, M. Broca, and Prof. Turner. 
At the conclusion of a special paper on this subject the 
latter writes :” ; 

‘The three specimens of the brain of a chimpanzee just 
described, prove that the generalization which Gratiolet has 
attempted to draw of the complete absence of the first con- 
necting convolution and the concealment of the second, as 
essentially characteristic features in the brain of this animal, 
is by no means universally applicable. In only one speci- 
men did the brain, in these particulars, follow the law which 
Gratiolet has expressed. As regards the presence of the 
ae bridging convolution, I am inclined to think that 
it has existed in one hemisphere, at least, in a majority of 
the brains of this animal which have, up to this time, been 
figured or described. The superficial position of the second 
bridging convolution is evidently less frequent, and has as 
want believe, only been seen in the brain (A) recorded in 
this communication. The asymmetrical arrangement in the 
convolutions of the two hemispheres, which previous ob- 
servers have referred to in their descriptions, is also well 
illustrated in these specimens’ (pp. 8, 9). 

Even were the presence of ths temporo-occipital, or ex- 
ternal perpendicular, sulcus a mark of distinction between 
the higher apes and man, the value of such a distinctive 
character would be rendered very doubtful by the structure 
of the brain in the Platyrhine apes. In fact, while the tem- 
ee is one of the most constant of sulci in the 

atarrhine or Old World apes, it is never very strongly de- 
veloped in the New World apes; it is absent in the smaller 
Platyrhini; rudimentary in Pithecta;"* and more or less 
obliterated by bridging convolutions in Afeles. 

A character which is thus variable within the limits of 
a single group can have no great taxonomic value. 

It is further established that the degree of asymmetry 
of the convolution of the two sides in the human brain is 

ubject to much individual variation; and that, in those 
individuals of the Bushman race who have been examined, 
the gyri and sulci of the two hemispheres are considerably 


72 Notes more especially on the bridging convolutions in the Brain of the | 
Chimpanzee, ‘‘Proceedings of the Royal Society of Edinburgh,’’ 1865-66. 
7% Flower ‘On the Anatomy of Pithecia Monachus,’’ ‘‘Proceedings of the 


Zoological Society,’’ 1862. 


STRUCTURE OF THE BRAIN 267 


less complicated and more symmetrical than in the Euro- 
pean brain, while in some individuals of the chimpanzee 
their complexity and asymmetry become notable. This is 
particularly the case in the brain of a young male chim- 
ae aes yy M. Broca. (‘‘L’Ordre des Primates,” 
ii , Fig. 11. 

Again, as respects the question of absolute size, it is 
established that the difference between the largest and the 
smallest healthy human brain is greater than the difference 
between the smallest healthy human brain and the largest 
chimpanzee’s or orang’s brain. 

oreover, there is one circumstance in which the orang’s 
and chimpanzee’s brains resemble man’s, but in which they 
differ from the lower apes, and that is the presence of two 
corpora candicantia—the Cynomorpha having but one. 

n view of these facts I do not hesitate in this year 1874 
to repeat and insist upon the proposition which [ enunciated 
in 1863:” 

‘‘So far as cerebral structure goes, therefore, itis clear that 
man differs less from the chimpanzee or the orang than these 
do even from the monkeys, and that the difference between 
the brain of the chimpanzee and of man is almost insignifi- 
cant, when compared with that between the chimpanzee 
brain and that of a Lemur.” 

In the paper to which I have referred, Prof. Bischoff 
does not deny the second part of this statement, but he first 
makes the irrelevant remark that it is not wonderful if the 
brains of an orang and a Lemur are very different; and sec- 
ondly, goes on to assert that, ‘‘If we successively compare 
the brain of a man with that of an orang; the brain of this 
with that of a chimpanzee; of this with that of a gorilla, 
and so on of a Hylobates, Semnopithecus, Cynocephalus, 
Cercopithecus, Macacus, Cebus, Cailithrix, Lemur, Stenops, 
Hapale, we shall not meet with a greater or even as great 
a break in the degree of development of the convolutions 
as we find between the brain of a man and that of an orang 
or chimpanzee.” 

To which I reply, first, that whether this assertion be 
true or false, it has nothing whatever to do with the proposi- 
tion enunciated in ‘‘Man's Place in Nature,’’ which refers 
not to the development of the convolutions alone, but to the 
structure of the whole brain. If Prof. Bischoff had taken 


™ “‘Mau’s Place in Nature,’’ p. 102. 


268 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


the trouble to refer to p. 96 of the work he criticises, in fact, 
he would have found the following passage: ‘‘And it isa 
remarkable circumstance that though, so far as our present 
knowledge extends, there is one true structural break in the 
series of forms of Simian brains, this biatus does not lie be- 
tween man and the manlike apes, but between the lower and 
the lowest Simians, or, in other words, between the Old 
and New World apes and monkeys and the Lemurs. Every 
Lemur which has yet been examined, in fact, has its cerebel- 
lum partially visible from above; and its posterior lobe, with 
the contained posterior cornu and hippocampus minor, more 
or less rudimentary. Every marmoset, American monkey, 
Old World monkey, baboon, or manlike ape, on the con- 
trary, has its cerebellum entirely hidden, posteriorly, by 
the cerebral lobes, and possesses a large posterior cornu 
with a well-developed hippocampus minor.’ 

This statement was a strictly accurate account of what 
was known when it was made; and‘it does not appear to me 
to be more than apparently weakened by the subsequent dis- 
covery of the relatively small development of the posterior 
lobes in the Siamang and in the Howling monkey. Not- 
withstanding the exceptional brevity of the posterior lobes 
in these two species, no one will pretend that their brains, 
in the slightest degree, approach those of the Lemurs. And 
if, instead of putting Hapale out of its natural place, as Prof. 
Bischoff most unaccountably does, we write the series of ani- 
mals he has chosen to mention as follows: Homo, Pithecus, 
Troglodytes, Hylobates, Semnopithecus, Cynocephalus, Cerco- 

ithecus, Macacus, Cebus, Callithrix, Hapale, Lemur, Stenops, 
f ventate to reaffirm that the great break in this series lies 
between Hapale and Lemur, and that this break is consider- 
ably greater than that between any other two terms of that 
series. Prof. Bischoff ignores the fact that long before he 
wrote, Gratiolet had suggested the separation cf the Lemurs 
from the other Primates on the very ground of the difference 
in their cerebral characters; and that Prof. Flower had made 
the following observations in the course of his description of 
the brain of the Javan Loris:” 

‘‘And it is especially remarkable that, in the develop- 
ment of the posterior lobes, there is no approximation to 
the Lemurine, short-hemisphered brain, in those monkeys 
which are commonly supposed to approach this family in 


% ‘*Trangactions of the Zoological Society,’’ vol. v., 1862, 


STRUCTURE OF THE BRAIN 269 


other respects; viz., the lower members of the Platyrhine 
group.” ; 

So far as the structure of the adult brain is concerned, 
then, the very considerable additions to our knowledge, 
which have been made by the researches of so many investi- 
gators, during the past ten years, fully justify the statement 
which [ made in 1868. But it has been said that, admitting 
the similarity between the adult brains of man and apes, 
they are nevertheless, in reality, widely different, because 
they exhibit fundamental differences in the mode of their 
development. No one would be more ready than I to admit 
the force of this argument, if such fundamental differences 
of development really exist. But I deny that they do exist. 
On the contrary, there is a fundamental agreement in the 
development of the brain in men and apes. 

Gratiolet originated the statement that there is a funda- 
mental difference in the development of the brains of apes 
and that of man—consisting in this; that, in the apes, the 
sulci which first make their appearance are situated on 
the posterior region of the cerebral hemispheres, while, 
in the human foetus, the sulci first become visible on the 
frontal lobes.” 

This general statement is based upon two observations, 
the one of a Gibbon almost ready to Es born, in which the 

osterior gyri were ‘‘well developed,’’ while those of the 
rontal lobes were ‘‘hardly indicated’’"” (1. ¢., p. 39), and 


%8 ‘Chez tous les singes, les plis postérieurs se développent les premiers; 
les plis antérieurs se développent plus tard, aussi la vertébre occipitale et la 
pariétale sont-elles relativement trés-grandes chez le foetus. L’Homme pré- 
sente une exception remarquable quant & Vépeque de V’apparition des plis 
frontaux, gui sont Jes premiers indiqués; mais le développement général du lobe 
frontal, envisagé seulement par rapport 4 son volume, suit les mémes lois que 
dans les singes.*’ Gratiolet, ‘‘Mémoire sur les plis cérébraux de 1’Homme 
et des Primates,’’ p. 39, tab. iv. fig. 3. 

7 Gratiolet’s words are (I. ¢., p. 39): ‘‘Dans Je foetus dont il s’agit les plis 
eérébraux postérieurs sont bien développés, tandis que les plis du lobe frontal 
sont 4 peine indiqués.’’ The figure, however (Pl. iv. fig. 8), shows the fissure 
of Rolando, and one of the frontal sulci, plainly enough. Nevertheless, 
M. Alix, in his “‘Notice sur les travaux anthropologiques de Gratiolet’? 
(“Mém. de la Société d’Anthropologie de Paris,”? 1868, p. 32), writes thus: 

‘Gratiolet a eu entre Jes mains le cerveau @’un foetus de Gibbon, singe éminem- 
ment supérieur, et tellement rapproché de orang, que des naturalistes trés 
compétents l’ont rangé parmi les anthropoides. M. Huxley, par exemple, 
n’hésite pas sur ce point. Eh bien, c’est sur le cerveau d’un foetus de Gibbon 
que Gratiolet a vu les cérconvolutions du lobe temporo-sphénoidal déja développées 
dors-qu'il nexiste pas encore de plis sur le lobe frontal. Il était donc bien 
autorisé a dire que, chez ’homme les circonvolutions apparaissent d’e en @, 
tandis que chez les singes elles se développent d’« en a.”? 


270 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


the other of a human foetus at the 22d or 23d week of utero- 
gestation, in which Gratiolet notes that the insula was un- 
covered, but that, nevertheless, ‘‘des incisures sément le 
lobe antérieur; une scissure peu profonde indique la sépa- 
ration du_lobe occipital, ee uit d’ailleurs dés cette 
époque. Le reste de la surface cérébrale est encore abso- 
lument lisse.”’ 

Three views of this brain are given in Plate IL., Figs. 
1, 2, 3 of the work cited, showing the upper, lateral, and 
inferior views of the hemispheres, but not the inner view. 
It is worthy of note that the figure by no means bears out . 
Gratiolet’s description, inasmuch as the fissure (antero- 
temporal) on the posterior half of the face of the hemi- 
sphere is more marked than any of those vaguely indicated 
in the anterior half. If the figure is correct, it in no way 
justifies Gratiolet’s conclusion: ‘‘1l y a done entre ces cer- 
veaux [those of a Callithrix and of a Gibbon] et celui du 
foetus humain une différence fondamentale. Chez celui-ci, 
longtemps avant que les plis temporaux apparaissent, les plis 
frontaux essayent d’exister.”’ 

Since Gratiolet’s time, however, the development of the 
gyri and sulci of the brain has been made the subject of 
renewed investigation by Schmidt, Bischoff, Pansch,” and 
more particularly by Ecker,” whose work is not only the 
latest, but by far the most complete, memoir on the subject. 

The final results of their inquiries may be summed up as 
follows: 

1. In the human foetus, the Sylvian fissure is formed in 
the course of the third month of utero-gestation. In this 
and in the fourth month the cerebral hemispheres are smooth 
and rounded (with the exception of the Sylvian depression), 
and they project backward far beyond the cerebellum. | 

2. The sulci, properly so called, begin to ihe in the 
interval between the end of the fourth and the beginning of 
the sixth month of foetal life, but Ecker is careful to point 
out that not only the time, but the order, of their appear- 
ance is subject to considerable individual variation. In no 
case, however, are either the frontal or the temporal sulci 
the earliest. 


78 “‘Yeber die typische Anordnung der Furchen und Windungen auf den 
Grosshirn-Hemispharen des Menschen und der Affen.”” ‘‘Archiv fir Anthro- 
pologie,’? iii., 1868. a 

” “Yur Entwickelungs Geschichte der Furchen und Windungen der 
Grosshirn-Hemispharen im Foetus des Menschen.”” “‘Archiv fir Anthropolo- 
gie,”’ iii., 1868. 


STRUCTURE OF THE BRAIN 271 


The first which appears, in fact, lies on the inner face of 
the hemisphere (whence doubtless Gratiolet, who does not 
seem to have examined that face in his foetus, overlooked 
it), and is either the internal perpendicular (occipito-parietal) 
or the calcarine sulcus, these two being close together and 
eventually running into one another. As a rule the occipito- 
parietal is the earlier of the two. 

8. At the latter part of this period, another sulcus, the 
‘‘posterio-parietal,’’ or ‘‘Fissure of Rolando,” is developed, 
and it is followed, in the course of the sixth month, by the 
other principal sulci of the frontal, parietal, temporal, and 
occipital lobes. There is, however, no clear evidence that 
one of these constantly appears before the other; and it is 
remarkable that, in the ea at the period described and 
figured by Hcker (1. c., pp. 212-18, Taf. II. Figs. 1, 2, 8, 4), 
-the antero-temporal sulcus (scissure paralléle), so character- 
istic of the ape’s brain, is as well if not better developed 
than the fissure of Rolando, and is much more marked than 
the proper frontal sulci. 

aking the facts as they now stand, it appears to me that 
the order of the appearance of the sulci and gyri in the foetal 
human brain is in perfect harmony with the general doctrine 
of evolution,’ and with the view that man has been evolved 
from some ape-like form; though there can be no doubt that 
that form was, in many respects, different from any member 
of the Primates now living. , 

Von Baer taught us, half a century ago, that, in the 
course of their development, allied animals put on, at first, 
the characters of the greater groups to which they belong, 
and, by degrees, assume those which restrict them within 
the limits of their family, genus, and species; and he 
proved, at the same time, that no developmental stage of 
a higher animal is precisely similar to the adult condition 
of any lower animal. It is quite correct to say that a frog 
passes through the condition of a fish, inasmuch as at one 
period of its life the tadpole has all the characters of a fish, 
and, if it went no further, would have to be grouped among 
fishes. But it is equally true that the tadpole is very differ- 
ent from any known fish. : 

In like manner, the brain of a human foetus, at the fifth 
month, may correctly be said to be not only the brain of an 
ape, but that of an Arctopithecine or marmoset-like ape; for 
its hemispheres, with their great posterior lobes, and with 
no sulci but the Sylvian and the calcarine, present the char- 


272 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


acteristics found only in the group of the Arctopithecine Pri- 
mates. But it is equally true, as Gratiolet remarks, that, 
in its widely open Sylvian fissure, it differs from the brain 
of any actual marmoset. No doubt it would be much more 
similar to the brain of an advanced foetus of a marmoset. 
But we know nothing whatever of the development of the 
brain in the marmosets. In the Platyrhini proper, the only 
observation with which I am acquainted is due to Pansch, 
who found in the brain of a foetal Cebus Apella, in addition 
to the Sylvian fissure and the deep calcarine fissure, only a 
very shallow antero-temporal fissure (scissure paralléie of 
Gratiolet). 

Now this fact, taken together with the circumstance that 
the antero-temporal sulcus is present in such Platyrhini as 
the Saimiri, which present mere traces of sulci on the ante- 
rior half of the exterior of the cerebral hemispheres, or none 
at all, undoubtedly, so far as it goes, affords fair evidence 
in favor of Gratiolet’s hypothesis, that the posterior sulci 
appear before the anterior, in the brains of the Platyrhint. 
But it by no means follows that the rule which may hold 
good for the Platyrhint extends to the Catarrhini. We 
have no information whatever respecting the development 
of the brain in the Cynomorpha; and, as regards the An- 
thropomorpha, nothing but the account of the brain of the 
Gibbon, near birth, already referred to. At the present 
moment there is not a shadow of evidence to show that 
the sulci of a chimpanzee’s, or orang’s, brain do not appear 
in the same order as a man’s. : 

Gratiolet opens his preface with the aphorism, ‘‘I] est 
dangereux dans les sciences de conclure trop vite.’’ I fear 
he must have forgotten this sound maxim by the time he 
had reached the discussion of the differences between men 
and apes, in the body of his work. No doubt, the excellent 
author of one of the most remarkable contributions to the 
just understanding of the mammalian brain which has ever 

een made, would have been the first to admit the insuffi- 
ciency of his data had he lived to profit by the advance of 
inquiry. The misfortune is- that his conclusions have been 
employed by persons incompetent to appreciate their foun- 
dation, as arguments in favor of obscurantism.” 

But it is important to remark that, whether Gratiolet was 


80 For example, M. Abbé Lecomte in his terrible pamphlet, ‘“‘Le Darwinisme 
et lorigine de 1’Homme,”’’ 1873. 


STRUCTURE OF THE BRAIN 2738 


right or wrong in his hypothesis respecting the relative order 
of appearance of the temporal and frontal sulci, the fact 
remains that, before either temporal or frontal sulci appear, 
the foetal brain of man presents characters which are found 
only in the lowest group of the Primates (leaving out the 
Lemurs); and that this is exactly what we should expect 
-to be the case, if man has resulted from the gradual modi- 
fication of the same form as that from which the other Pri- 
mates have sprung. 


— 


PART TWO 
SEXUAL SELECTION 


CHAPTER VIII 


PRINCIPLES OF SEXUAL SELECTION 


Secondary sexual characters—Sexual selection—Manner of action—Excess 
of males—Polygamy—The male alone generally modified , through 
sexual selection—Eagerness of the male—Variability of the male— 
Choice exerted by the female—Sexual compared with natural selec- 
tion—Inheritance, at corresponding periods of life, at corresponding 
seasons of the year, and as limited by sex—Relations between the 
several forms of inheritance—Causes why ore sex and the young are 
not modified through sexual selection—Supplement on the proportional 
numbers of the two sexes throughout the animal kingdom—The pro- 
portion of the sexes in relation to natural selection 


ITH animals which have their sexes separated, the 
males necessarily differ from the females in their 
organs of reproduction; and these are the primary 

sexual characters. But the sexes often differ in what Hunter 
has called secondary sexual characters, which are not di- 
rectly connectedswith the act of reproduction; for instance, 
the male possesses certain organs of sense or locomotion of 
which the female is quite destitute, or has them more highly 
developed, in order that he may readily find or reach her; 
or again, the male has special organs of prehension for hold- 
ing her securely. These latter organs, of infinitely diversi- 
fied kinds, graduate into those which are commonly ranked 
as primary, and in some cases can hardly be distinguished 
from them; we see instances of this in the complex append- 
ages at the apex of the abdomen. in male insects. Unless 
indeed we confine the term ‘‘primary’’ to the reproductive 
(274) 


SEXUAL SELECTION 275 


glands, it is scarcely possible to decide which ought to be 
called primary and which secondary. 

The female often differs from the male in having organs 
for the nourishment or protection of her young, such as the 
mammary glands of mammals, and the abdominal sacs of 
the marsupials. In some few cases also the male possesses 
similar organs, which are wanting in the female, such as the 
receptacles for the ova in certain male fishes, and those 
temporarily developed in certain male frogs. The females 
of most bees are provided with a special apparatus for col- 
lecting and carrying pollen, and their ovipositor is modified 
into a sting for the defence of the larve and the community. 
Many similar cases could be given, but they do not here 
concern us. ‘There are, however, other sexual differences 
quite unconnected with the primary reproductive organs, 
- and it is with these that we are more especially concerned— 
such as the greater size, strength, and pugnacity of the male, 
his weapons of offence or means of defence against rivals, 
his gaudy coloring and various ornaments, his power of 
song, and other such characters. 

Besides the primary and secondary sexual differences, 
such as the foregoing, the males and females of some ani- 
mals differ in structures related to different habits of life, and 
not at all, or only indirectly, to the reproductive functions, 
Thus the females of certain flies (Culicids and Tabanidz) 
are blood-suckers, while the males, living on flowers, have 
mouths destitute of mandibles.’ The males of certain moths 
and of some crustaceans (e.g., Tanais) have imperfect, closed 
mouths, and cannot feed. The complemental males of cer- 
tain Cirripeds live like epiphytic plants either on the female 
or the hermaphrodite form, and are destitute of a mouth and 
of prehensile limbs. In these cases it is the male which 
has been modified, and has lost certain important organs 
which the females possess. In other cases it is the female 
which has lost such parts; for instance, the female glow- 


1 Westwood, *‘Modern Class. of Insects,’’ vol. ii., 1840, p. 541. For the 
gtatement about Tanais, mentioned below, I am indebted to Frits Miller. 


276 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


worm is destitute of wings, as also are many female moths, 
some of which never leave their cocoons. Many female 
parasitic crustaceans have lost their natatory legs. In some 
weevil-beetles (Curculionids) there is a great difference 
between the male and female in the length of the rostrum 
or snout;? but the meaning of this and of many analogous 
differences is not at all understood. Differences of structure 
between the two sexes in relation to different habits of life 
are generally confined to the lower animals; but with some 
few birds the beak of the male differs from that of the 
female. In the Huia of New Zealand the difference is won- 
derfully great, and we hear from Dr. Buller*® that the male 
uses his strong beak in chiselling the larve of insects out 
of decayed wood, while the female probes the softer parts 
with her far longer, much curved, and pliant beak; and 
thus they mutually aid each other. In most cases, differ- 
ences of structure between the sexes are more or less directly 
connected with the propagation of the species: thus a female, 
which has to nourish a multitude of ova, requires more food 
than the male, and consequently requires special means for 
procuring it. A male animal which lives for a very short . 
time might lose its organs for procuring food through disuse, _ 
without detriment; but he would retain his locomotive 
organs in a perfect state, so that he might reach the female. 
The female, on the other hand, might safely lose her organs 
for flying, swimming, or walking, if she gradually acquired 
habits which rendered such powers useless. 

K We are, however, here concerned only with sexual selec- 
tion. This depends on the advantage which certain_indi- 
viduals have over others of the same sex and species solely 
in respect of reproductionX When, as in the cases above 
mentioned, the two sexes differ in structure in relation to 
different habits of life, they have no doubt been modified 
through natural selection, and by inheritance limited to one 


2 See Kirby and Spence’s work, “‘Introduction to Entomology,’’ volume iif,, 
1826, page 309. 
3 “Birds of New Zealand,’’ 1872, p. 66 


SEXUAL SELECTION 277 


and the same sex. So again the primary sexual organs, 
and those for nourishing or protecting the young, come 
under the same influence; for those individuals which gen- - 
erated or nourished their offspring best would leave, ceteris 
paribus, the greatest number to inherit their superiority, 
while those which generated or nourished their offspring 
badly would leave but few to inherit their weaker powers. 
As the male has to find the female, he requires organs of 
sense and locomotion; but if these organs are necessary for 
the other purposes of life, as is generally the case, they will 
have been developed through natural selection. When the 
male has found the female, he sometimes absolutely requires 
prehensile organs to hold her; thus Dr. Wallace informs 
me that the males of certain moths cannot unite with the 
females if their tarsi or feet are broken. The males of many 
oceanic crustaceans, when adult, have their legs and antennze 
modified in an extraordinary manner for the prehension of 
the female; hence we may suspect that it is because these 
animals are washed about by the waves of the open sea that 
they require these organs in order to propagate their kind, 
and if so, their development has been the result of ordinary 
or natural selection. Some animals extremely iow in the 
scale have been modified for this same purpose; thus 
the males of certain parasitic worms, when fully grown, 
have the lower surface of the terminal part of their bodies 
roughened like a rasp, and with this they coil round and 
permanently hold the females.‘ 

When the two sexes follow exactly the same habits of 
life, and the male has the sensory or locomotive organs more 


4M. Perrier advances this case (‘‘Revue Scientifique,’? Feb. 1, 1873, 
p. 865) as one fatal to the belief in sexual selection, inasmuch as he supposes 
that I attribute ell the differences between the sexes to sexual selection. This 
distinguished naturalist, therefore, like so many other Frenchmen, has not 
taken the trouble to understand even the first principles of sexual selection. 
An English naturalist insists that the claspers of certain male animals could 
not have been developed through the choice of the female! Had I not met 
with this remark, I should not have thought it possible for any one to have 
read this chapter and to have imagined that I maintain that the choice of the 
female had anything to do with the development of the prehensile organs in 
the male. 


278 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


highly developed than those of the female, it may be that 
the perfection of these is indispensable to the male for find- 
ing the female; but in the vast majority of cases they serve 
only to give one male an advantage over another, for with 
sufficient time the less well-endowed males would succeed 
in pairing with the females; and, judging from the structure 
of the female, they would be in all other respects equally 
well adapted for their ordinary habits of life. Since in such 
cases the males have acquired their present structure not 
from being better fitted to survive in the struggle for exist- 
ence, but from having gained an advantage over other 
males, and from having transmitted this advantage to their 
male offspring alone, sexual selection must here have come 
into action. It was the importance of this distinction which 
led me to designate this form of selection as Sexual Selec- 
tion. So again, if the chief service rendered to the male by 
his prehensile organs is to prevent the escape of the female 
before the arrival of other males, or when assaulted by 
them, these organs will have been perfected through sexual 
selection, that is by the advantage acquired by certain indi- 
viduals over their rivals. But in most cases of this kind 
it is impossible to distinguish between the effects of natural 
and sexual selection. Whole chapters could be filled with 
details on the differences between the sexes in their sensory, 
locomotive, and prehensile organs. As, however, these 
structures are not more interesting than others adapted for 
the ordinary purposes of life, I shall pass them over almost 
entirely, giving only a few instances under each class. 

There are many other structures and instincts which 
must have been developed through sexual selection—such 
as the weapons of offence and the means of defence of the 
males for fighting with and driving away their rivals—their 
courage and pugnacity—their various ornaments—their con- 
trivances for producing vocal or instrumental music—and 
their glands for emitting odors, most of these latter struct- 
ures serving only to allure or excite the female. It is clear 
that these characters are the result of sexual and not of 


SEXUAL SELECTION 279 


ordinary selection, since unarmed, unornamented, or unat- 
tractive males would succeed equally well in the battle for 
life and in leaving a numerous progeny but for the presence 
of better-endowed males. We may infer that this would 
be the case, because the females, which are unarmed and 
unornamented, are able to survive and procreate their kind. 
Secondary sexual characters of the kind just referred to will 
be fully discussed in the following chapters, as being in 
many respects interesting, but especially as depending 
on the will, choice, and rivalry of the individuals of either 
sex. When we behold two males fighting for the possession 
of the female, or several male birds displaying their gorgeous 
plumage, and performing strange antics before an assembled 
body of females, we cannot doubt that, though led by in- 
stinct, they know what they are about, and consciously 
exert their mental and bodily powers. 

Just as man can improve the breed of his game-cocks 
by the selection of those birds which are victorious in the 
cock-pit, so it appears that the strongest and most vigorous 
males, or those provided with the best weapons, have pre- 
vailed under nature, and have led to the improvement of 
the natural breed or species. A slight degree of variability 
leading to some advantage, however slight, in reiterated 
deadly contests would suffice for the work of sexual selec- 
tion; and it is certain that secondary sexual characters are 
eminently variable. Just as man can give beauty, accord- 
ing to his standard of taste, to his male poultry, or more 
strictly can modify the beauty originally acquired by the 
parent species, can give to the Sebright bantam a new and 
elegant plumage, an erect and peculiar carriage—so it ap- 
pears that female birds in a state of nature have, by a long 
selection of the more attractive males, added to their beauty 
or other attractive qualities. No doubt this implies pow- 
ers of discrimination and taste on the part of the female 
which will at first appear extremely improbable; but, by the 
facts to be adduced hereafter, I hope to be able to show that 
the females actually have these powers. When, however, 


280 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


it is said that the lower animals have a sense of beauty, it 
must not be supposed that such sense is comparable with 
that of a cultivated man, with his multiform and complex 
associated ideas. A more just comparison would be between 
the taste for the beautiful in animals, and that in the lowest 
savages, who admire and deck themselves with any brilliant, 
glittering, or curious object. 

From our ignorance on several points, the precise man- 
ner in which sexual selection acts is somewhat uncertain. 
Nevertheless, if those naturalists who already believe in 
the mutability of species will read the following chapters, 
they will, I think, agree with me that sexual selection has 
played an important part in the history of the organic world. 
It is certain that among almost all animals there is a struggle 
between the males for the possession of the female. This 
fact is so notorious that it would be superfluous to give 
instances. Hence the females have the opportunity of 
selecting one out of several males, on the supposition that 
their mental capacity suffices for the exertion of a choice. 
In many cases special circumstances tend to make the strug- 
gle between the males particularly severe. Thus the males 
of our migratory birds generally arrive at their places of 
breeding before the females, so that many males are ready 
to contend for each female. I am informed by Mr. 
Jenner Weir that the bird-catchers assert that this is 
invariably the case with the nightingale and blackcap, 
and with respect to the latter he can himself confirm the 
statement. 

Mr. Swaysland, of Brighton, has been in the habit, 
during the last forty years, of catching our migratory birds 
on their first arrival, and he has never known the females 
of any species to arrive before their males. During one 
spring he shot thirty-nine males of Ray’s wagtail (Budytes 
fait) before he saw a single female. Mr. Gould has ascer- 
tained, by the dissection of those snipes which arrive the 
first in this country, that the males come before the females. 
And the like holds good with most of the migratory birds 


SEXUAL SELECTION 281 


of the United States.° The majority of the male salmon in 
our rivers, on coming up from the sea, are ready to breed 
before the females. So it appears to be with frogs and 
toads. Throughout the great class of insects the males 
almost always are the first to emerge from the pupal state, 
so that they generally abound for a time before any females 
ean be seen.* The cause of this difference between the 
males and females in their periods of arrival and maturity 
is sufficiently obvious. Those males which annually first 
migrated into any country, or which in the spring were first 
ready to breed, or were the most eager, would leave the 
largest number of offspring; and these would tend to inherit 
similar instincts and constitutions. It must be borne in 
mind that it would have been impossible to change very 
materially the time of sexual maturity in the females, with- 
out at the same time interfering with the period of the pro- 
duction of the young—a period which must be determined 
by the seasons of the year. On the whole, there can be no 
doubt that with almost all animals, in which the sexes are 
separate, there is a constantly recurrent struggle between 
the males for the possession of the females. ' 

Our difficulty in regard to sexual selection lies in under- 
standing how it is that the males which conquer other males, 
or those which prove the most attractive to the females, 
leave a greater number of offspring to inherit their superi- 
ority than their beaten and less attractive rivals. Unless 
this result does follow, the characters which give to certain 
males an advantage over others could not be perfected and 
augmented through sexual selection. When the sexes exist 
in exactly equal numbers, the worst-endowed males will 
(except where polygamy prevails) ultimately find females, 


5 J, A, Allen, on the ‘‘Mammals and Winter Birds of Florida,’’ Bull. Comp. 
Zoology, Harvard College, p. 268. 

6 Even with those plants in which the sexes are separate, the male flowers 
are generally mature before the female. As first shown by C. K. Sprengel, 
many hermaphrodite plants are dichogamous; that is, their male and female 
organs are not ready at the same time, so that they cannot be self-fertilized, 
Now in such flowers the pollen i is in general matured before the stigma, though 
there are exceptional cases in which the female organs are beforehand. 


282 THE. DESCENT OF MAN 


and leave as many offspring, as well fitted for their general 
habits of life as the best-endowed males. From various 
facts and considerations, I formerly inferred that with most 
animals in which secondary sexual characters are well 
developed the males considerably exceeded the females in 
number; but this is not by any means always true. If the 
males were to the females as two to one, or as three to two, 
or even in a somewhat lower ratio, the whole affair would 
be simple; for the better-armed or more attractive males 
would leave the largest number of offspring. But after 
investigating, as far as possible, the numerical proportion 
of the sexes, I do not believe that any great inequality in 
number commonly exists. In most cases sexual selection 
appears to have been effective in the following manner. 

Let us take any species, a bird for instance, and divide 
the females inhabiting a district into two equal bodies, the 
one consisting of the more vigorous and better-nourished 
individuals, and the other of the less vigorous and healthy. 
The former, there can be little doubt, would be ready to 
breed in the spring before the others; and this is the opinion 
of Mr. Jenner Weir, who has carefully attended to the habits 
of birds during many years. There can also be no doubt 
that the most vigorous, best-nourished, and earliest breeders 
would on an average succeed in rearing the largest number of 
fine offspring.” The males, as we have seen, are generally 
ready to breed before the females; the strongest, and with 
some species the best armed, of the males drive away the 
weaker; and the former would then unite with the more 
vigorous and better-nourished females, because they are 
the first to breed. Such vigorous pairs would surely rear 


1 Here is excellent evidence on the character of the offspring from an ex- 
perienced ornithologist. Mr. J. A. Allen, in speaking (‘‘Mammals and Winter 
Birds of E. Florida,”’ p. 229) of the later broods, after the accidental destruction 
of the first, says that these ‘‘are found to be smaller and paler-colored than 
those hatched earlier in the season. In cases where several broods are reared 
each year, as a general rule the birds of the earlier broods seem in all respects 
the most perfect and vigorous.” 

8 Hermann Miller has come to this same conclusion with respect to those 
female bees which are the first to emerge from the pupa each year. See his 


SEXUAL SELECTION _ 288 


a larger number of offspring than the retarded females, 
which would be compelled to unite with the conquered and 
less powerful males, supposing the sexes to be numerically 
equal; and this is all that is wanted to add, in the course of 
successive generations, to the size, strength, and courage 
‘of the males, or to improve their weapons. 

But in very many cases the males which conquer their 
rivals do not obtain possession of the females, independently 
of the choice of the latter. The courtship of animals is by 
no means so simple and short an affair as might be thought. 
The females are most excited by, or prefer pairing with, 
the more ornamented males, or those which are the best 
songsters, or play the best antics; but it is obviously proba- 
ble that they would at the same time prefer the more vig- 
orous and lively males, and this has in some cases been 
confirmed by actual observation.* Thus the more vigorous 
females, which are the first to breed, will have the choice 
of many males; and, though they may not always select 
the strongest or best armed, they will select those which 
are vigorous and well armed, and in other respects the most 
attractive. Both sexes, therefore, of such early pairs would, 
as above explained, have an advantage over others in rear- 
ing offspring; and this apparently has sufficed during a long 
course of generations to add not only to the strength and 
fighting powers of the males, but likewise to their various 
ornaments or other attractions. 

In the converse and much rarer case of the males select- 
ing particular females, it is plain that those which were 
the most vigorous and had conquered others would have the 
- freest choice; and it is almost certain that they would select 
vigorous as well as attractive females. Such pairs would 
have an advantage in rearing offspring, more especially if 


remarkable essay, “‘Anwendung den Darwin’schen Lehre auf Bienen,”’ ‘*Verh, 
a, V. Jabrg.,”? xxix. p. 45. 

® With respect to poultry, I have received information, hereafter to be 
giver, to this effect. Even with birds, such as pigeons, which pair for life, 
the female, as I hear from Mr. Jenner Weir, will desert her mate if he is 
injured or grows weak. 


284 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


the male had the power to defend the female during the 
pairing-season, as occurs with some of the higher animals, 
or aided her in providing for the young. The same princi- 
ples would apply if each sex preferred and selected certain 
individuals of the opposite-sex; supposing that they selected 
not only the more attractive, but likewise the more vigorous 
individuals. 


Numerical Proportion of the Two Sexes.—I have remarked 
that sexual selection would be a simple affair if the males 
were considerably more numerous than the females. Hence 
Iwas led to investigate, as far as I could, the proportions 
between the two sexes of as many animals as possible; but 
the materials are scanty. I will here give only a brief ab- 
stract of the results, retaining the details for a supplement- 
ary discussion, so as not to interfere with the course of my 
argument. Domesticated animals alone afford the means of 
ascertaining the proportional numbers at birth; but no rec- 
ords have been specially kept for this purpose. By indirect 
means, however, I have collected a considerable body of sta- 
tistics, from which it appears that with most of our domestic 
animals the sexes are nearly equal at birth. Thus 25,560 
births of race-horses have been recorded during twenty-one 
years, and the male births were to the female births as 99.7 
to 100. In greyhounds the inequality is greater than with 
any other animal, for out of 6,878 births during twelve years, 
the male births were to the female as 110.1 to 100. It is, 
however, in some degree doubtful whether it is safe to infer 
that the proportion would be the same under natural condi- 
tions as under domestication; for slight and unknown dif- — 
ferences in the conditions affect the proportion of the sexes. 
Thus with mankind, the male births in England are as 104.5, 
in Russia as 108.9, and with the Jews of Livonia as 120 to 
100 female births. But I shall recur to this curious point 
of the excess of male births in the supplement to this 
chapter. At the Cape of Good Hope, however, male 
children of European extraction have been born during 


SEXUAL SELECTION 285 


several years in the proportion of between 90 and 99 to 
100 female children. 

For our present purpose we are concerned with the pro- 
portion of the sexes not only at birth, but also at maturity, 
and this adds another element of doubt; for it is a well- 
ascertained fact that with man the number of males dying 
before or during birth, and during the first few years of 
infancy, is considerably larger than that of females. So 
it almost certainly is with male lambs, and probably with 
some other animals. The males of some species kill one 
another by fighting, or they drive one another about until 
they become greatly emaciated. They must also be often 
exposed to various dangers, while wandering about in eager 
search for the females. In many kinds of fish the males are 
much smaller than the females, and they are believed often 
to be devoured by the latter, or by other fishes. The fe- 
males of some birds appear to die earlier than the males; 
they are also liable to be destroyed on their nests, or while 
in charge of their young. With insects the female larva 
are often larger than those of the males, and would conse- 
quently be more likely to be devoured. In some cases the 
mature females are less active and less rapid in their move- 
ments than the males, and could not escape so well from 
danger. Hence, with animals in a state of nature, we must 
rely on mere estimation, in order to judge of the proportions 
of the sexes at maturity; and this is but little trustworthy, 
except when the inequality is strongly marked. Neverthe- 
less, as far as a judgment can be formed, we may conclude, 
from the facts given in the supplement, that the males of 
some few mammals, of many birds, of some fish and insects, 
are considerably more numerous than the females. 

The proportion between the sexes fluctuates slightly 
luring successive years: thus with race-horses, for every 
100 mares born the stallions varied from 107.1 in one year 
to 92.6 in another year, and with greyhounds from 116.3 to 
25.8. But had larger numbers been tabulated throughout 
an area more extensive than England, these fluctuations 


286 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


would probably have disappeared; and, such as they are, 
would hardly suffice to lead to effective sexual selection in 
a state of nature. Nevertheless, in the cases of some few 
wild animals, as shown in the supplement, the proportions 
seem to fluctuate either during different seasons or in differ- 
ent localities in a sufficient degree to lead to such selection. 
For it should be observed that any advantage gained during 
certain years or in certain localities by those males which 
were able to conquer their rivals, or were the most attractive 
to the females, would probably be transmitted to the off- 
spring, and would not subsequently be eliminated. During 
the succeeding seasons, when, from the equality of the sexes, 
every male was able to procure a female, the stronger or 
more attractive males previously produced would still have 
at least as good a chance of leaving offspring as the weaker 
or less attractive. 


Polygamy.—The practice of polygamy leads to the same 
results as would follow from an actual inequality in the 
number of the sexes; for if each male secures two or more 
females, many males cannot pair; and the latter assuredly 
will be the weaker or less attractive individuals. Many 
mammals and some few birds are polygamous, but with 
animals belonging to the lower classes I have found no 
evidence of this habit. The intellectual powers of such 
animals are, perhaps, not sufficient to lead them to collect 
and guard a harem of females. That some relation exists 
between polygamy and the development of secondary sexual 
characters appears nearly certain; and this supports the view 
that a numerical preponderance of males would be eminently 
favorable to the action of sexual selection. Nevertheless, 
many animals which are strictly monogamous, especially 
birds, display strongly marked secondary sexual charac- 
ters; while some few animals which are polygamous do not 
have such characters. 

We will first briefly run through the mammals, and then 
turn to birds. The gorilla seems to be polygamous, and the 


SEXUAL SELECTION i 287 


male differs considerably from the female; so it is with some 
baboons, which live in herds containing twice as many adult 
females as males. In South America the Mycetes caraya 
presents well-marked sexual differences, in color, beard, 
and vocal organs; and the male generally lives with two 
or three wives: the male of the Cebus capucinus differs 
somewhat from the female, and appears to be polygamous.” 
Little is known on this head with respect to most other 
monkeys, but some species are strictly monogamous. The 
ruminants are eminently polygamous, and they present sex- 
ual differences more frequently than almost any other group 
of mammals; this holds good especially in their weapons, 
but also in other characters. Most deer, cattle, and sheep 
are polygamous; as are most antelopes, though some are 
monogamous. Sir Andrew Smith, in speaking of the ante- 
lopes of South Africa, says that in herds of about a dozen 
there was rarely more than one mature male. The Asiatic 
Antilope saiga appears to be the most inordinate polygamist 
in the world; for Pallas’! states that the male drives away 
all rivals, and collecust+herd of about a hundred females 
and kids together; the female is hornless and has softer 
hair, but does not otherwise differ much from the male. 
The wild horse of the Falkland Islands and of the West- 
ern States of North America is polygamous, but, except in 
his greater size and in the proportions of his body, differs 
but little from the mare. The wild boar presents well- 
marked sexual characters, in his great tusks and some 
other points. In Europe and in India he leads a solitary 
life, except during the breeding season; but, as is believed 
by Sir W. Elliot, who has had many opportunities in India 
of observing this animal, he consorts at this season with 


10 On the Gorilla, Savage and Wyman, ‘‘Boston Journal of Nat. Hist.,”’ 
vol. v., 1845-47, p. 423. On Cynocephalus, Brehm, ‘‘Ilust. Thierleben,” 
B. i., 1864, 3. 77. On Mycetes, mengger, ‘“‘Naturgesch.: Saugethiere von 
Paraguay, °? 1830, s. 14, 20. Cebus, Brehm, ibid., s. 108. 

11 Pallas, “Spicilegia Zoolog.,’? Fase. xii., 1777, p. 29. Sir Andrew Smith, 
“Tllustrations of the Zoology of S. Africa,’’ 1849, pl. 29, on the Kobus. Owen, 
in his ‘‘Anatomy of Vertebrates”? (vol. iii., 1868, p. 633) gives a table showing 
incidentally which species of antelopes are gregarious. 


288 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


several females. Whether this holds good in Europe is 
doubtful, but it is supported by some evidence. The adult 
made Indian elephant, like the boar, passes much of his time 
in solitude; but, as Dr. Campbell states, when with others, 
‘it is rare to find more than one male with a whole herd of 
females’’; the larger males expelling or killing the smaller 
and weaker ones. The male differs from the female in his 
immense tusks, greater size, strength, and endurance; so 
great is the difference in these respects that the males when 
caught are valued at one-fifth more than the females.’?_ The 
sexes of other pachydermatous animals differ very little or 
not at all, and, as far as known, they are not polygamists. 
Nor have I heard of any species in the Orders of Cheiroptera, 
Edentata, Insectivora, and Rodents being polygamous, ex- 
cepting that, among the Rodents, the common rat, according 
to some rat-catchers, lives with several females. Neverthe- 
less the two sexes of some sloths (Hdentata) differ in the 
character and color of certain patches of hair on their shoul- 
ders.**° And many kinds of bats (Cheiroptera) present well- 
marked sexual differences, chieiti@@™™®ihe males possessing 
odoriferous glands and pouches, and by their being of a 
lighter color."* In the great order of Rodents, as far as I 
can learn, the sexes rarely differ, and when they do so, it 
is but slightly in the tint of the fur. 

As I hear from Sir Andrew Smith, the lion in South 
Africa sometimes lives with a single female, but generally 
with more, and, in one case, was found with as many as 
five females; so that he is polygamous. As far as I can 
discover, he is the only polygamist among all the terres- 
trial Carnivora, and he alone presents well-marked sexual 
characters. If, however, we turn to the marine Carnivora, 
as we shall hereafter see, the case is widely different; for 
many species of seals offer extraordinary sexual differences, 


12 Dr. Campbell, in ‘‘Proc. Zoolog. Soc.,’? 1869, p. 138. See also an 
interesting paper, by Lieut. Johnstone, in ‘‘Proc, Asiatic Soc. of Bengal,” 
May, 1868. 

13 Dr, Gray, in ‘‘Annals and Mag. of Nat. Hist.,’’ 1871, p. 302. 

14 See Dr. Dobson’s excellent paper, in ‘‘Proc, Zoolog. Soc.,’’ 1873, p. 241. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 289 


and they are eminently polygamous. Thus, according to 
Péron, the male sea-elephant of the Southern Ocean always 
possesses several females, and the sea-lion of Forster is said 
to be surrounded by from twenty to thirty females. In the 
North, the male sea-bear of Steller is accompanied by even 
a greater number of females. It is an interesting fact, as Dr. 
Gill remarks,'* that in the monogamous species, ‘‘or those 
living in small communities, there is little difference in size 
between the males and females; in the social species, or 
rather those of which the males have. harems, the males are 
vastly larger than the females.’’ 

Among birds, many species, the sexes of which differ 
greatly from each other, are certainly monogamous. In 
Great Britain we see well-marked sexual differences, for 
instance, in the wild-duck which pairs with a single female, 
the common blackbird, and the bull-finch, which is said to 
pair for life. I am informed by Mr. Wallace that the like 
is true of the Chatterers or Cotingide of South America, 
and of many other birds. In several groups I have not been 
able to discover whether the species are polygamous or mo- 
nogamous. Lesson says that birds-of-paradise, so remark- 
able for their sexual differences, are polygamous, but Mr. 
Wallace doubts whether he had sufficient evidence. Mr. Sal- 
vin tells me he has been led to believe that humming- birds 
are polygamous. The male widow-bird, remarkable for his 
caudal plumes, certainly seems to be a polygamist.'* I have 
been assured by Mr. Jenner Weir, and by others, that it is 
somewhat common for three starlings to frequent the same 
nest; but whether this is a case of polygamy or polyandry . 
has not been ascertained. 

The Gallinacez exhibit almost as strongly marked sexual 
differences as birds-of-paradise or humming-birds, and many 


18 The Eared Seals, ‘‘ American Naturalist,’’ vol. iv., Jan. 1871. 

16 “‘The Ibis,’’ vol. iii., 1861, p. 133, on the Progne Widow-bird. See also 
‘on the Vidua awillaris, ibid., vol. ii., 1860, p. 211. On the polygamy of the 
Capercailzie and Great Bustard, see L. Lloyd, ‘‘Game Birds of Sweden,’’ 1867, 
pp. 19 and 182. Montagu and Selby speak of the Black Grouse as polygamous, 
and of the Red Grouse as monogamous. 


Descent—Vou. I.—13 


290 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


of the species are, as is well known, polygamous; others 
being strictly monogamous. What a contrast is presented 
between the sexes of the polygamous peacock or pheasant 
and the monogamous guinea-fowl or partridge! Many simi- 
lar cases could be given, as in the grouse tribe, in which the 
males of the polygamous capercailzie and black-cock differ 
greatly from the females; while the sexes of the monoga- 
mous red grouse and ptarmigan differ very little. In the 
Cursores, except among the bustards, few species offer 
strongly marked sexual differences, and the great bustard 
(Otis tarda) is said to be polygamous. With the Gralia- 
tores, extremely few species differ sexually, but the ruff 
(Machetes pugnax) affords a marked exception, and this 
species is believed by Montagu to be a polygamist. Hence 
it appears that among birds there often exists a close rela- 
tion between polygamy and the development of strongly 
marked sexual differences. I asked Mr. Bartlett of the 
Zoological Gardens, who has had very large experience 
with birds, whether the male tragopan (one of the Gal- 
linacews) was polygamous, and I was struck by his an- 
swering, ‘‘I do not know, but should think so from his 
splendid colors.”’ 

It deserves notice that the instinct of pairing with a single 
female is easily lost under domestication. The wild-duck 
is strictly monogamous, the domestic duck highly polyga- 
mous. The Rev. W. D. Fox informs me that out of some 
half-tamed wild-ducks, on a large pond in his neighborhood, 
so many mallards were shot by the gamekeeper that only 
one was left for every seven or eight females; yet unusually 
large broods were reared. The guinea-fowl is strictly mo- 
nogamous; but Mr. Fox finds that his birds succeed best 
when he keeps one cock to two or three hens. Oanary- 
birds pair in a state of nature, but the breeders in England 
successfully put one male to four or five females. J have 
noticed these cases, as rendering it probable that wild mo-, 
nogamous species might readily become either temporarily 
or permanently polygamous. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 291 


Too little is known of the habits of reptiles and fishes 
to enable us to speak of their marriage arrangements. The 
stickleback (Gasterosteus), however, is said to be a polyga- 
mist;” and the male during the breeding season differs 
conspicuously from the female. 

To sum up on the means through which, as far as we 
can judge, sexual selection has led to the development of 
- secondary sexual characters. It has been shown that the 
largest number of vigorous offspring will be reared from the 
pairing of the strongest and best-armed males, victorious 
in contests over other males, with the most vigorous and 
best-nourished females, which are the first to breed in the 
spring. If such females select the more attractive, and at 
the same time vigorous males, they will rear a larger num- 
ber of offspring than the retarded females, which must pair 
with the less vigorous and less attractive males. So it will 
be if the more vigorous males select the more attractive and 
at the same time healthy and vigorous females; and this 
will especially hold good if the male defends the female, 
and aids in providing food for the young. The advantage 
thus gained by the more vigorous pairs in rearing a larger 
number of offspring has apparently sufficed to render sexual 
selection efficient. Buta large numerical preponderance of 
males over females will be still more efficient; whether the 
preponderance is only occasional and local, or permanent; 
whether it occurs at birth, or afterward from the greater 
destruction of the females; or whether it indirectly follows 
from the practice of polygamy. 


The Male Generally more Modified than the Female.— 
Throughout the animal kingdom, when the sexes differ in 
external appearance, it is, with rare exceptions, the male 
which has been the more modified; for, generally, the fe- 
male retains a closer resemblance to the young of her own 
species, and to other adult members of the same group. 
The cause of this seems to lie in the males of almost all 


x a ern re “River Gardens,’’ 1854. 


292 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


animals having stronger passions than the females. Hence 
it is the males that fight together and sedulously display their 
charms before the females; and the victors transmit their su- 
periority to their male offspring. Why both sexes do not 
thus acquire the characters of their fathers will be consid- 
ered hereafter. That the males of all mammals eagerly 
pursue the females is notorious to every one. So it is with 
birds; but many cock birds do not so much pursue the hen 
as display their plumage, perform strange antics, and pour 
forth their song in her presence. The male in the few fish 
observed seems much more eager than the female; and the 
same is true of alligators, and apparently of Batrachians. 
Throughout the enormous class of insects, as Kirby re- 
marks,’® ‘‘the law is that the male shall seek the female.”’ 
Two good authorities, Mr. Blackwall and Mr. C. Spence 
Bate, tell me that the males of spiders and crustaceans are 
more active and more erratic in their habits than the fe- 
males. When the organs of sense or locomotion are present 
in the one sex of insects and crustaceans and absent in the 
other, or when, as is frequently the case, they are more 
highly developed in the one than in the other, it is, as far 
as I can discover, almost invariably the male which retains 
such organs, or has them most developed; and this shows 
that the male is the more active member in the courtship 
of the sexes.’ 

The female, on the other hand, with the rarest ex- 
ceptions, is less eager than the male. As the illustrious 
Hunter” long ago observed, she generally ‘‘requires to be 
courted’’; she is coy, and may often be seen endeavoring 


18 Kirby and Spence, ‘‘Introduction to Entomology,”’ vol. iii., 1826, p. 342. 

19 One parasitic Hymenopterous insect (Westwood, ‘‘Modern Class. of 
Tnsects,’’ vol. ii. p. 160) forms an exception to the rule, as the male has rudi- 
mentary wings, and never quits the cell in which it is born, while the female 
has well-developed wings. Audouin believes that the females of this species 
are impregnated by the males which are born in the same cells with them; but 
it is much more probable that the females visit other cells, so that close inter- 
breeding is thus avoided. We shall hereafter meet in various classes with 
a few exceptional cases in which the female, instead of the male, is the seeker 
and wooer, 

20 “‘Hssays and Observations,’’ edited by Owen, vol. 1., 1861, p. 194, 


SEXUAL SELECTION 293 


for a long time to escape from the male. Every observer 
of the habits of animals will be able to call to mind in- 
stances of this kind. It is shown by various facts, given 
hereafter, and by the results fairly attributable to sexual 
selection, that the female, though comparatively passive, 
generally exerts some choice and accepts one male in pref- 
erence to others. Or she may accept, as appearances would 
sometimes lead us to believe, not the male which is the most 
attractive to her, but the one which is the least distasteful. 
The exertion of some choice on the part of the female seems 
a law almost as general as the eagerness of the male. 

We are naturally led to inquire why the male, in so many 
and such distinct classes, has become more eager than the 
female, so that he searches for her, and plays the more ac- 
tive part in courtship. It would be no advantage and some 
loss of power if each sex searched for the other; but why 
should the male almost always be the seeker? The ovules 
of plants after fertilization have to be nourished for a time; 
hence the pollen is necessarily brought to the female organs 
—hbeing placed on the stigma, by means of insects or the 
wind, or by the spontaneous movements of the stamens; and 
in the Alge, etc., by the locomotive power of the anthero- 
zooids. With lowly organized aquatic animals, permanently 
affixed to the same spot and having their sexes separate, 
the male element is invariably brought to the female; and 
of this we can see the reason, for even if the ova were de- 
tached before fertilization, and did not require subsequent 
nourishment or protection, there would yet be greater diffi- 
culty in transporting them than the male element, because, 
being larger than the latter, they are produced in far smaller 
numbers. So that many of the lower animals are, in this 
respect, analogous with plants.** The males of affixed and 
aquatic animals having been led to emit their fertilizing ele- 
ment in this way, it is natural that any of their descendants, 


21 Prof. Sachs (‘‘Lehrbuch der Botanik,’’ 1870, s, 633), in speaking of the 
male and female reproductive cells, remarks, ‘‘Verhalt sich die eine bei der 
Vereinig'ing activ, . . . die andere erscheint bei der Vereinigung passiv.”’ 


294 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


which rose in the scale and became locomotive, should re- 
tain the same habit; and they would approach the female 
as closely as possible, in order not to risk the loss of the 
fertilizing element in a long passage of it through the water. 
With some few of the lower animals, the females alone are 
fixed, and the males of these must be the seekers. But it 
is difficult to understand why the males of species, of which 
the progenitors were primordially free, should invariably 
have acquired the habit of approaching the females, instead 
of being approached by them. But in all cases, in order 
that the males should seek efficiently, it would be necessary 
that they should be endowed with strong passions; and the 
acquirement of such passions would naturally follow from 
the more eager leaving a larger number of offspring than 
the less eager. 

The great eagerness of the males has thus indirectly led 
to their much more frequently developing secondary sexual 
characters than the females. But the development of such 
characters would be much aided if the males were more 
liable to vary than the females—as I concluded they were— 
after a long study of domesticated animals. Von Nathusius, 
who has had very wide experience, is strongly of the same 
opinion.” Good evidence also in favor of this conclusion 
can be produced by a comparison of the two sexes in man- 
kind. During the Novara Expedition a vast number of 
measurements was made of various parts of the body in 
different races, and the men were found in almost every 
case to present a greater range of variation than the women; 
but I shall have to recur to this subject in a future chapter. 
Mr. J. Wood,” who has carefully attended to the variation 
of the muscles in man, puts in italics the conclusion that 


2% “*Vortrige iiber Viehzucht,’’ 1872, p. 63. 

28 “Reise der Novara: Anthropolog. Theil,’? 1867, s, 216-269. The 
results were calculated by Dr. Weisbach from measurements made by Drs. 
K. Scherzer and Schwarz. On the greater variability of the males of domesti- 
cated animals, see my ‘‘Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,’’ 
vol. ii., 1868, p. 75. 

34 ‘Proceedings Royal Soc.,”’ vol. xvi., July 1868, pp. 519 and 524. 


SEXUAL SELECTION ~ 295 


“the greatest number of abnormalities in each subject is 
found in the males.’’ He had previously remarked that 
“altogether in 102 subjects the varieties of redundancy were 
found to be half as many again as in females, contrasting 
widely with the greater frequency of deficiency in females 
before described.’’ Prof. Macalister likewise remarks” that 
variations in the muscles ‘‘are probably more common in 
males than females.’’ Certain muscles which are not nor- 
mally present in mankind are also more frequently devel- 
oped in the male than in the female sex, although exceptions 
to this rule are said to occur. Dr. Burt Wilder’ has tabu- 
lated the cases of 152 individuals with supernumerary digits, 
of which 86 were males, and 89, or less than half, females, 
the remaining 27 being of unknown sex. It should not, 
‘however, be overlooked that women would more frequently 
endeavor to conceal a deformity of this kind than men. 
Again, Dr. L. Meyer asserts that the ears of man are more 
variable in form than those of woman.” Lastly, the tem- 
perature is more variable in man than in woman.” 

The cause of the greater general variability in the male 
sex than in the female is unknown, except in so far as 
secondary sexual characters are extraordinarily variable, 
and are usually confined to the males; and, as we shall 
presently see, this fact is, to a certain extent, intelligible. 
Through the action of sexual and natural selection male 
animals have been rendered in very many instances widely 
different from their females; but independently of selection 
the two sexes, from differing constitutionally, tend to vary 
in a somewhat different manner. The female has to expend 
much organic matter in the formation of her ova, whereas 
the male expends much force in fierce contests with his 
rivals, in wandering about in search of the female, in exert- 


% “*Proc, Royal Irish Academy,’’ vol. x., 1868, p. 123. 

%6 **Massachusetts Medical Soc.,’’ vol. ii. No. 3, 1868, p. 9. 

21 “ Archiv fir Path, Anat. und Phys.,’’ 1871, p. 488. 

98 The conclusions recently arrived at by Dr. J. Stockton-Hough, on the 
temperature of man, are given in the ‘‘Pop. Sciente Review,’’ Jan. 1, 1874, 
p. 97. 


296 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


ing his voice, pouring out odoriferous secretions, etc.; and 
this expenditure is generally concentrated within a short 
period. The great vigor of the male during the season 
of love seems often to intensify his colors, independently of 
any marked difference from the female.” In mankind, and 
even as low down in the organic scale as in the Lepidoptera, 
the temperature of the body is higher in the male than in the 
female, accompanied in the case of man by a slower pulse.” 
On the whole, the expenditure of matter and force by the 
two sexes is probably nearly equal, though effected ia very 
different ways and at different rates. 

From the causes just specified the two sexes can hardly 
fail to differ somewhat in constitution, at least during, the 
breeding season; and, although they may be subjected 
to exactly the same conditions, they will tend to vary in 
a different manner. If such variations are of no service 
to either sex, they will not be accumulated and increased 
by sexual or natural selection. Nevertheless, they may 
become permanent if the exciting cause acts permanently; 
and, in accordance with a frequent form of inheritance, they 
may be transmitted to that sex alone in which they first 
appeared. In this case the two sexes will come to present 
permanent, yet unimportant, differences of character. For 
instance, Mr. Allen shows that with a large number of birds 
inhabiting the northern and southern United States, the 
specimens from the south are darker-colored than those 
from the north; and this seems to be the direct result of the 
difference in temperature, light, etce., between the two re- 
gions. Now, in some few cases, the two sexes of the same 
species appear to have been differently affected; in the 


29 Prof. Mantegazza is inclined to believe (‘‘Lettera a Carlo Darwin,” 
‘Archivio per P Anthropologia,’’ 1871, p. 306) that the bright colors common 
in so many male animals are due to the presence and retention by them of the 
spermatic fluid: but this can hardly be the case; for many male birds, for 
instance young pheasants, become brightly colored in the autumn of their 
first year. 

80 For mankind, see Dr. J. Stockton-Hough, whose conclusions are given in 
the ‘‘Pop. Science Review,’’ 1874, p. 97. See Girard’s observations on the 
Lepidoptera, as given in the ‘‘Zoological Record,” 1869, p. 347, 


SEXUAL SELECTION 297 


Ageleus pheniceus the males have had their colors greatly 
intensified in the south; whereas with Cardinalis virginianus 
it is the females which have been thus affected; with 
Quiscalus major the females have been rendered extremely 
variable in tint, while the males remain nearly uniform.* 

A few exceptional cases occur in various classes of 
animals, in which the females instead of the males have © 
acquired well pronounced secondary sexual characters, 
such as brighter colors, greater size, strength, or pugnacity. 
With birds there has sometimes been a complete transpo- 
sition of the ordinary characters proper to each sex; the 
females having become the more eager in courtship, the 
males remaining comparatively passive, but apparently 
selecting the more attractive females, as we may infer from 
the results. Certain hen birds have thus been rendered more 
highly colored or otherwise ornamented, as well as more 
powerful and pugnacious than the cocks; these characters 
being transmitted to the female offspring alone. 

It may be suggested that in some cases a double process 
of selection has been carried on; that the males have selected 
the more attractive females, and the latter the more attrac- 
tive males. This process, however, though it might lead 
to the modification of both sexes, would not make the one 
sex different from the other, unless indeed their tastes for 
the beautiful differed; but this is a supposition too improb- 
able to be worth considering in the case of any animal, ex- 
cepting man. There are, however, many animals in which 
the sexes resemble each other, both being furnished with 
the same ornaments, which analogy would lead us to attrib- 
ute to the agency of sexual selection. In such cases it may 
be suggested with more plausibility, that there has been a 
double or mutual process of sexual selection; the more 
vigorous and precocious females selecting the more attrac- 
tive and vigorous males, the latter rejecting all except the 
more attractive females. But from what we know of the 


31 **Mammals and Birds of E. Florida,’’ pp. 234, 280, 295. 


298 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


habits of animals, this view is hardly probable, for the male 
is generally eager to pair with any female. It is more prob- 
able that the ornaments common to both sexes were acquired 
by one sex, generally the male, and then transmitted to the 
offspring of both sexes. If, indeed, during a lengthened 
period the males of any species were greatly to exceed the 
females in number, and then during another lengthened 
period, but under different conditions, the reverse were 
to occur, a double, but not simultaneous, process of sexual 
selection might easily be carried on, by which the two sexes 
might be rendered widely different. 

We shall hereafter see that many animals exist of which 
neither sex is brilliantly colored nor provided with special 
ornaments, and yet the members of both sexes or of one 
alone have probably acquired simple colors, such as white 
or black, through sexual selection. The absence of bright 
tints or other ornaments may be the result of variations of 
the right kind never having occurred, or of the animals 
themselves having preferred plain black or white. Obscure 
tints have often been developed through natural selection 
for the sake of protection, and the acquirement through 
sexual selection of conspicuous colors appears to have been 
sometimes checked from the danger thus incurred. But in 
other cases the males during long ages may have struggled 
together for the possession of the females, and yet no effect 
will have been produced, unless a larger number of off- 
spring were left by the more successful males to inherit 
their superiority, than by the less successful: and this, as 
previously shown, depends on many complex contingencies. 

Sexual selection acts in a less rigorous manner than natu- 
- ral selection. The latter produces its effects by the life or 
death at all ages of the more or less successful individuals. 
Death, indeed, not rarely ensues from the conflicts of rival 
males. But generally the less successful male merely fails 
to obtain a female, or obtains a retarded and less vigorous 
female later in the season, or, if polygamous, obtains fewer 
females; so that they leave fewer, less vigorous, or no off- 


SEXUAL SELECTION 299 


spring. In regard to structures acquired through ordinary 
or natural selection, there is in most cases, as long as the 
conditions of life remain the same, a limit to the amount of 
advantageous modification in relation to certain special pur- 
poses; but in regard to structures adapted to make one male 
victorious over another, either in fighting or in charming the 
female, there is no definite limit to the amount of advanta- 
geous modification; so that as long as the proper variations 
arise the work of sexual selection will go on. This circum- 
stance may partly account for the frequent and extraordi- 
nary amount of variability presented by secondary sexual 
characters. Nevertheless, natural selection will determine © 
that such characters shall not be acquired by the victorious 
males, if they would be highly injurious, either by expend- 
ing too much of their vital powers, or by exposing them to 
any great danger. The development, however, of certain 
structures—of the horns, for instance, in certain stags—has 
been carried to a wonderful extreme; and in some cases to 
an extreme which, as far as the general conditions of life ° 
are concerned, must be slightly injurious to the male. From 
this fact we learn that the advantages which favored males 
derive from conquering other males in battle or courtship, 
and thus leaving a numerous progeny, are in the long run 
greater than those derived from rather more perfect adapta- 
tion to their conditions of life. We shall further see, and it 
could never have been anticipated, that the power to charm 
the female has sometimes been more important than the 
power to conquer other males in battle. 


LAWS OF INHERITANCE 


In order to understand how sexual selection has acted 
on many animals of many classes, and in the course of ages 
has produced a conspicuous result, it is necessary to bear 
in mind the laws of inheritance, as far as they are known. 
Two distinct elements are included under the term ‘‘inheri- 
tance’’—the transmission and the development of characters; 


800 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


but, as these generally go together, the distinction is often 
overlooked. We see this distinction in those characters which 
are transmitted through the early years of life, but are de- 
veloped only at maturity or during old age. We see the 
same distinction more clearly with secondary sexual charac- 
ters, for these are transmitted through both sexes, though 
developed in one alone. That they are present in both 
sexes is manifest when two species, having strongly marked 
sexual characters, are crossed, for each transmits the char- 
acters proper to its own male and female sex to the hybrid 
offspring of either sex. "The same fact is likewise manifest 
when characters proper to the male are occasionally devel- 
oped in the female when she grows old or becomes diseased, 
as, for instance, when the common hen assumes the flowing 
tail-feathers, hackles, comb, spurs, voice, and even pug- 
nacity of the cock. Conversely, the same thing is evident, 
more or less plainly, with castrated males. Again, inde- 
pendently of old age or disease, characters are occasionally 
transferred from the male to the female, as when, in certain 
breeds of the fowl, spurs regularly appear in the young and 
healthy females. But in truth they are simply developed in 
the female; for in every breed each detail in the structure 
of the spur is transmitted through the female to her male 
offspring. Many eases will hereafter be given where the 
female exhibits, more or less perfectly, characters proper 
to the male, in whom they must have been first developed, 
and then transferred to the female. The converse case of 
the first development of characters in the female, and of 
transference to the male, is less frequent; it will therefore 
be well to give one striking instance. With bees the pollen- 
collecting apparatus is used by the female alone for gather- 
ing pollen for the larve, yet in most of the species it is 
partially developed in the males, to whom it is quite useless, 
and it is perfectly developed in the males of Bombus or the 
humble-bee.* As not a single other Hymenopterous insect, 


32 M. Miiller, ‘“‘Anwendung der Darwin’schen Lehre,”’ ete., ‘Verh. d. n. 
V. Jabrg.”? xxix. p. 42. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 801 


not even the wasp, which is closely allied to the bee, is pro- 
vided with a pollen-collecting apparatus, we have no grounds 
for supposing that male bees primordially collected pollen as 
well as the females; although we have some reason to sus- 
pect that male mammals primordially suckled their young 
as well as the females. Lastly, in all cases of reversion, 
characters are transmitted through two, three, or many more 
generations, and are then developed under certain unknown 
favorable conditions. This important distinction between 
transmission and development will be best kept in mind by 
the aid of the hypothesis of pangenesis. According to this 
hypothesis, every unit or cell of the body throws off gem- 
mules or undeveloped atoms, which are transmitted to the 
offspring of both sexes, and are multiplied by self-division. 
They may ‘remain undeveloped during the early years of 
life or during successive generations; and their development 
into units or cells, like those from which they were derived, 
depends on their affinity for, and union with, other units 
or cells previously developed in the due order of growth. 


Inheritance at Corresponding Periods of Life.—This ten- 
dency is well established. A new character appearing in 
a young animal, whether it lasts throughout life or is only 
transient, will, in general, reappear in the offspring at the 
same age and last for the same time. If, on the other hand, 
a new character appears at maturity, or even during old age, 
it tends to reappear in the offspring at the same advanced 
age. When deviations from this rule occur, the transmitted 
characters much oftener appear before than after the corre- 
sponding age. As I have dwelt on this subject sufficiently 
in another work,* I will here merely give two or three in- 
stances, for the sake of recalling the subject to the reader’s 
mind. In several breeds of the Fowl, the down-covered 
chickens, the young birds in their first true plumage, and 


33 ‘The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,’’ vol. ii, 


1868, p. 75. In the last chapter but one, the provisional hypothesis of 
pangenesis, above alluded to, is fully explained. 


802 THE DESCENT.OF MAN 


the adults differ greatly from one another, as well as from 
their common parent-form, the Gallus bankiva; and these 
characters are faithfully transmitted by each breed to their 
offspring at the corresponding periods of life. For instance, 
the chickens of spangled Hamburgs, while covered with 
down, have a few dark spots on the head and rump, but 
are not striped longitudinally, as in many other breeds; in 
their first true plumage ‘‘they are beautifully pencilled,”’ 
that is, each feather is transversely marked by numerous 
dark bars; but in their second plumage the feathers all be- 
come spangled or tipped with a dark, round spot.** Hence 
in this breed variations have occurred at, and been trans- 
mitted to, three distinct periods of life. The Pigeon offers a 
more remarkable case, because the aboriginal parent-species 
does not undergo any change of plumage with advancing 
age, excepting that at maturity the breast becomes more 
iridescent; yet there are breeds which do not acquire their 
characteristic colors until they have moulted two, three, or 
four times; and these modifications of plumage are regularly 
transmitted. 


Inheritance at Corresponding Seasons of the Year.—With 
animals in a state of nature, innumerable instances occur of 
characters appearing periodically at different seasons. We 
see this in the horns of the stag, and in the fur of Arctic 
animals, which becomes thick and white during the winter. 
Many birds acquire bright colors and other decorations 
during the breeding-season alone. Pallas states,** that in 
Siberia domestic cattle and horses become lighter-colored 
during the winter; and I have myself observed and heard 


24 These facts are given on the high authority of a great breeder, Mr. 
Teebay; see Tegetmeier’s ‘‘Poultry Book,’’ 1868, p. 158. On the characters 
of chickens of different breeds, and on the breeds of the pigeon, alluded to in 
the following paragraph, see ‘‘Variation of Animals,’’ etc., vol. i. pp. 160, 249; 
vol. ii. p. 77. : 

35 ‘Nove species Quadrupedum e Glirium ordine,’? 1778, p. 7. On the 
transmission of color by the horse, see ‘‘Variation of Animals, etc., under 
Domestication,’’ vol. i. p. 51. Also vol. ii. p. 71, for a general discussion on 
‘Inheritance as limited by Sex.”’ 


SEXUAL SELECTION 303 


of similar strongly marked changes of color, that is, from 
brownish cream-color or reddish-brown to a perfect white, 
in several ponies in England. Although I do not know 
that this tendency to change the color of the coat during 
different seasons is transmitted, yet it probably is so, as 
all shades of color are strongly inherited by the horse. 
Nor is this form of inheritance, as limited by the seasons, 
more remarkable than its limitation by age or sex. 


Inheritance as Limited by Sex.—The equal transmission 
of characters to both sexes is the commonest form of inheri- 
tance, at least with those animals which do not present 
strongly marked sexual differences, and indeed with many 
of these. But characters are somewhat commonly trans- 
ferred exclusively to that sex in which they first appear. 
Ample evidence on this head has been advanced in my 
work on ‘‘Variation under Domestication,’’ but a few in- 
stances may here be given. There are breeds of the sheep 
and goat in which the horns of the male differ greatly in 
shape from those of the female; and these differences, ac- 
quired under domestication, are regularly transmitted to the 
same sex. Asarule, it is the females alone in cats which 
are tortoise-shell, the corresponding color in the males being 
rusty-red. With most breeds of the fowl, the characters 
proper to each sex are transmitted to the same sex alone. 
So general is this form of transmission that it is an anomaly 
. when variations in certain breeds are transmitted equally to 
both sexes. There are also certain sub-breeds of the fowl 
in which the males can hardly be distinguished from one 
another, while the females differ considerably in color. The 
sexes of the pigeon in the parent-species do not differ in 
any external character; nevertheless, in certain domesticated 
breeds the male is colored differently from the female.” 


36 Dr, Chapuis, ‘Le Pigeon Voyageur Belge,’’ 1865, p. 87. Boitard et 
Corbié, ‘‘Les Pigeons de Voliére,’’ etc., 1824, p. 173. See, also, on similar 
differences in certain breeds at Modena, ‘‘Le variazioni dei Colombi domestici,”’ 
del Paolo Bonizzi, 1873. 


804 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


The wattle in the English Carrier pigeon and the crop in 
the Pouter are more highly developed in the male than 
in the female; and, although these characters have been 
gained through long-continued selection by man, the slight 
differences between the sexes are wholly due to the form of 
inheritance which has prevailed; for they have arisen, not 
from, but rather in opposition to, the wish of the breeder. 
Most of our domestic races have been formed by the ac- 
cumulation of many slight variations; and as some of the 
successive steps have been transmitted to one sex alone, 
and some to both sexes, we find in the different breeds of 
the same species all gradations between great sexual dis- 
similarity and complete similarity. Instances have already 
been given with the breeds of the fowl and pigeon, and 
under nature analogous cases are common. With animals 
under domestication, but whether in nature I will not ven- 
ture to say, one sex may lose characters proper to it, and 
may thus come somewhat to resemble the opposite sex; for 
instance, the males of some breeds of the fowl have lost 
their masculine tail-plumes and hackles. On the other 
hand, the differences between the sexes may be increased 
under domestication, as with merino sheep, in which the 
ewes have lost their horns. Again, characters proper to 
one sex may suddenly appear in the other sex; as in those 
sub- breeds of the fowl in which the hens acquire spurs while 
young; or, aS in certain Polish sub-breeds, in which the 
females, as there is reason to believe, originally acquired 
a crest, and subsequently transferred it to the males. All 
these cases are intelligible on the hypothesis of pangenesis; 
for they depend on the gemmules of certain parts, although 
present in both sexes, becoming, through the influence of 
domestication, either dormant or developed in either sex. 
There is one difficult question which it will be convenient 
to defer to a future chapter, namely, whether a character at 
first developed in both sexes could, through selection, be 
limited in its development to one sex alone. If, for in- 
stance, a breeder observed that some of his pigeons (of 


SEXUAL SELECTION 305 


which the characters are usually transferred in an equal 
degree to both sexes) varied into pale blue, could he by 
long-continued selection make a breed in which the males 
alone should be of this tint, while the females remained un- 
changed? I will here only say that this, though perhaps 
not impossible, would be extremely difficult; for the natural 
result of breeding from the pale-blue males would be to 
change the whole stock of both sexes to this tint. If, how- 
ever, variations of the desired tint appeared, which were 
from the first limited in their development to the male sex, 
there would not be the least difficulty in making a breed 
with the two sexes of a different color, as indeed has been 
effected with a Belgian breed, in which the males alone are 
streaked with black. Ina similar manner, if any variation 
appeared in a female pigeon, which was from the first sex- 
ually limited in its development to the females, it would be 
easy to make a breed with the females alone thus charac- 
terized; but if the variation was not thus originally limited, 
the process would be extremely difficult, perhaps impossible.” 


On the Relation between the Period of Development of a 
Character and its Transmission to One Sex or to Both Seaes.—. 
Why certain characters should be inherited by both sexes, 
and other characters by one sex alone, namely, by that sex 
in which the character first appeared, is in most cases quite 
unknown. We cannot even conjecture why, with certain 
sub-breeds of the pigeon, black striz, though transmitted 
through the female, should be developed in the male alone, 
while every other character is equally transferred to both 
sexes. Why, again, with cats, the tortoise-shell color 


37 Since the publication of the first edition of this work, it has been highly 
satisfactory to me to find the following remarks (‘‘The Field,’’ Sept. 1872) from 
go experienced a breeder as Mr. Tegetmeier. After describing some curious 
cases in pigeons, of the transmission of color by one sex alone, and the forma- 
tion of a sub-breed with this character, he says: ‘‘It is a singular circumstance 
that Mr. Darwin should have suggested the possibility of modifying the sexual 
colors of birds by a course of artificial selection. When he did so, he was in 
ignorance of these facts that I have related; but it is remarkable how very 
closely he suggested the right method of procedure.”’ 


806 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


should, with rare exceptions, be developed in the female 
alone. The very same character, such as deficient or super- 
numerary digits, color-blindness, etc., may with mankind 
be inherited by the males alone of one family, and in 
another family by the females alone, though in both cases 
transmitted through the opposite as well as through the 
same sex.** Although we are thus ignorant, the two follow- 
ing rules seem often to hold good—that variations which 
first appear in either sex ata late period of life tend to be 
developed in the same sex alone, while variations which 
first appear early in life in either sex tend to be developed 
in both sexes. I am, however, far from supposing that this 
is the sole determining cause. As I have not elsewhere dis- 
cussed this subject, and as it has an important bearing on 
sexual selection, I must here enter into lengthy and some- 
what intricate details. 

It is in itself probable that any character appearing at an 
early age would tend to be inherited equally by both sexes, 
for the sexes do not differ much in constitution before the 
power of reproduction is gained. On the other hand, after 
this power has been gained and the sexes have come to 
differ in constitution, the gemmules (if I may again use the 
language of pangenesis) which are cast off from each varying 
part in the one sex would be much more likely to possess 
the proper affinities for uniting with the tissues of the same 
sex, and thus becoming developed, than with those of the 
opposite sex. 

I was first led to infer that a relation of this kind side 
from the fact that whenever and in whatever manner the 
adult male differs from the: adult female, he differs in 
the same manner from the young of both sexes. The gen- 
erality of this fact is quite remarkable: it holds good with 
almost all mammals, birds, amphibians, and fishes; also 
with many crustaceans, spiders, and some few insects, such 
as certain orthoptera and libellule. In all these cases the 


38 References are given in my ‘‘Variation of Animals under Domestication,” 
vol. ii. p. 72. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 307 


variations, through the accumulation of which the male 
acquired his proper masculine characters, must have oc- 
curred at a somewhat late period of life; otherwise the 
young males would have been similarly characterized; and, 
conformably with our rule, the variations are transmitted 
to and developed in the adult males alone. When, on the 
other hand, the adult male closely resembles the young of 
both sexes (these, with rare exceptions, being alike), he 
generally resembles the adult female; and in most of these 
cases the variations through which the young and old ac- 
quired their present characters probably occurred, accord- 
ing to our rule, during youth. But there is here room for 
doubt, for characters are sometimes transferred to the off- 
spring at an earlier age than that at which they first ap- 
peared in the parents, so that the parents may have varied 
when adult, and have transferred their characters to their 
offspring while young. There are, moreover, many animals 
in which the two sexes closely resemble each other, and yet 
both differ from their young; and here the characters of 
the adults must have been acquired late in life; neverthe- 
less, these characters, in apparent contradiction to our rule, 
are transferred to both sexes. We must not, however, 
overlook the possibility, or even probability, of successive 
variations of the same nature occurring, under exposure to 
similar conditions, simultaneously in both sexes at a rather 
late period of life; and in this case the variations would be 
transferred to the offspring of both sexes at a corresponding 
late age; and there would then be no real contradiction to 
the rule that variations occurring late in life are transferred 
exclusively to the sex in which they first appeared. This 
latter rule seems to hold true more generally than the second 
one, namely, that variations which occur in either sex early 
in life tend to be transferred to both sexes. As it was obvi- 
ously impossible even to estimate in how large a number of 
cases throughout the animal kingdom these two propositions 
held good, it occurred to me to investigate some striking or 
crucial instances, and to rely on the result. 


308 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


An excellent case for investigation is afforded by the 
Deer family. In all the species but one the horns are 
developed only in the males, though certainly transmitted 
through the females, and capable of abnormal development 
in them. In the reindeer, on the other hand, the female is 
provided with horns; so that in this species the horns ought, 
according to our rule, to appear early in life, long before 
the two sexes are mature and have come to differ much in 
constitution. In all the other species the horns ought to — 
appear later in life, which would lead to their development 
in that sex alone in which they first appeared in the pro- 
genitor of the whole Family. Now in seven species, belong: 
ing to distinct sections of the family and inhabiting different 
regions, in which the stags alone bear horns, I find that the 
horns first appear at periods varying from nine months after 
birth in the roebuck, to ten, twelve, or even more months, 
in the stags of the six other and larger species. But with 
the reindeer the case is widely different; for, as I hear from 
Professor Nilsson, who kindly made special inquiries for me 
in Lapland, the horns appear in the young animals within 
four or five weeks after birth, and at the same time in both 
sexes. So that here we have a structure developed at a 
most unusually early age in one species of the family, and 
likewise common to both sexes in this one species alone. 

In several kinds of antelopes, only the males are pro- 
vided with horns, while in the greater number both sexes 
bear horns. With respect to the period of development, 
Mr. Blyth informs me that there was at one time in the 
Zoological Gardens a young koodoo (Ant. strepsiceros), of 
which the males alone are horned, and also the young 
of a closely allied species, the eland (Ant. oreas), in which 


89 Tt am much obliged to Mr. Cupples for having made inquiries for me in 
regard to the Roebuck and Red Deer of Scotland from Mr. Robertson, the experi- 
enced head forester to the Marquis of Breadalbane. In regard to Fallow-deer, 
I have to thank Mr. Eyton and others for information. For the Cervus alces 
of N. America, see ‘‘Land and Water,’’ 1868, pp. 221 and 254; and for the 
C. Virginianus and strongyloceros of the same continent, see J. D, Caton, in 
“Ottawa Acad. of Nat. Se.,”? 1868, p. 13. For Cervus EHidi of Pegu, see Lieut. 
Beavan, ‘‘Proc. Zoolog. Soc.,’’ 1867, p. 762. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 3809 


both sexes are horned. Now it is in strict conformity with 
our rule, that in the young male koodoo, although ten 
months old, the horns were remarkably small, considering 
the size ultimately attained by them; while in the young 
male eland, although only three months old, the horns were 
already very much larger than in the koodoo. It is also 
a noticeable fact that in the prong-horned antelope,” only a 
few of the females, about one in five, have horns, and these 
are in a rudimentary state, though sometimes above four 
inches long; so that, as far as concerns the possession of 
horns by the males alone, this species is in an intermediate 
condition, and the horns do not appear until about five or 
six months after birth. Therefore, in comparison with what 
little we know of the development of the horns in other 
antelopes, and from what we do know with respect to the 
horns of deer, cattle, etc., those of the prong-horned antelope 
appear at an intermediate period of life—that is, not very 
early, as in cattle and sheep, nor very late, as in the larger 
deer and antelopes. The horns of sheep, goats, and cattle, 
which are well developed in both sexes, though not quite 
equal in size, can be felt, or even seen, at birth or soon 
afterward.*? Our rule, however, seems to fail in some breeds 
of sheep, for instance, merinos, in which the rams alone are 
horned; for I cannot find on inquiry* that the horns are 
developed later in life in this breed than in ordinary sheep 
in which both sexes are horned. But with domesticated 


4 Antilocapra Americana. I have to thank Dr. Canfield for information 
with respect to the horns of the female: see also his paper in ‘Proc. Zoolog. 
Soc.,”? 1866, p. 109. Also Owen, ‘‘Anatomy of Vertebrates,’’ val. iii. p. 627. 

41 Thave been assured that the horns of the sheep in North Wales can 
always be felt, and are sometimes even an inch in length, at birth. Youatt 
says (‘‘Cattle,’? 1834, p. 277), that the prominence of the frontal bone in cattle 
penetrates the cutis at birth, and that the horny matter is soon formed over it, 

42 JT am greatly indebted to Prof. Victor Carus for having made inquiries for 
me, from the highest authorities, with respect to the merino sheep of Saxony. 
On the Guinea coast of Africa there is, however, a breed of sheep in which, 
as with merinos, the rams alone bear horns; and Mr. Winwood Reade informs 
mne that in one case observed by him a young ram, born on Feb. 10, first showed 
horns on March 6, so that in this instance, in conformity with rule, the develop- 
ment of the horns occurred at a later period of life than in Welsh sheep, in 
which both sexes are horned. ; 


310 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


sheep the presence or absence of horns is not a firmiy fixed 
character; for a certain proportion of the merino ewes bear 
small horns, and some of the rams are hornless; and in most 
breeds hornless ewes are occasionally produced. 

Dr. W. Marshall has lately made a special study of the 
protuberances so common on the heads of birds,** and he 
comes to the following conclusion: that with those species 
in which they are confined to the males they are developed 
late in life; whereas with those species in which they are 
common to the two sexes, they are developed at a very 
early period. This is certainly a striking confirmation of 
my two laws of inheritance. 

In most of the species of the splendid family of the 
Pheasants, the males differ conspicuously from the females, 
and they acquire their ornaments at a rather late period of 
life. The eared pheasant (Crossoptilon auritum), however, 
offers a remarkable exception, for both sexes possess the 
fine caudal plumes, the large ear-tufts, and the crimson 
velvet about the head; I find that all these characters ap- 
pear very early in life, in accordance with rule. The adult 
male can, however, be distinguished from the adult female 
by the presence of spurs; and, conformably with our rule, 
these do not begin to be developed before the age of six 
months, as I am assured by Mr. Bartlett, and even at this 
age the two sexes can hardly be distinguished.** The male 
and female peacock differ conspicuously from each other 
in almost every part of their plumage, except in the elegant 
head-crest, which is common to both sexes; and this is 


43 “‘TJeber die knéchernen Schadelhécker der Végel,’’ in the ‘‘Niederland- 
ischen Archiv fiir Zoologie,’’ Band I. Heft 2, 1872. 

44 In the common peacock (Pavo cristatus) the male alone possesses spurs, 
while both sexes of the Java peacock (P. muticus) offer the unusual case. of 
being furnished with spurs. Hence I fully expected that in the latter species 
they would have been developed earlier in life than in the common peacock; 
but M. Hegt of Amsterdam informs me that with young birds of the previous 
year, of both species, compared on April 23, 1869, there was no difference in 
the development of the spurs, The spurs, however, were as yet represented 
merely by slight knobs or elevations. I presume that I should have been 
informed if any difference in the rate of development had been observed 
subsequently. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 311 


developed very early in life, long before the other orna- 
ments, which are confined to the male. The wild-duck 
offers an analogous case, for the beautiful green speculum 
on the wings is common to both sexes, though duller and 
somewhat smallér in the female, and it is developed early 
in life, while the curled tail-feathers and other ornaments of 
the male are developed later.** Between such extreme cases 
of close sexual resemblance and wide dissimilarity as 
those of the Crossoptilon and peacock, many intermediate 
enes could be given, in which the characters follow our two 
rules in their order of development. 

As most insects emerge from the pupal state in a mature 
condition, it is doubtful whether the period of development 
can determine the transference of their characters to one or 
to both sexes. But we do not know that the colored scales, 
for instance, in two species of butterflies, in one of which 
the sexes differ in color, while in the other they are alike, 
are developed at the same relative age in the cocoon. Nor 
do we know whether all the scales are simultaneously devel- 
oped on the wings of the same species of butterfly, in which 
certain colored marks are confined to one sex, while others 
are common to both sexes. A difference of this kind in 
the period of development is not so improbable as it may 
at first appear; for with the Orthoptera, which assume their 
adult state not by a single metamorphosis, but by a suc- 
cession of moults, the young males of some species at first 
resemble the females, and acquire their distinctive masculine 
characters only at a later moult. Strictly analogous cases 
occur at the successive moults of certain male crustaceans. 


4 Tn some other species of the Duck family the speculum differs in a greater 
degree in the two sexes; but I have not been able to discover whether its full 
development occurs later in life in the males of such species than in the male 
of the common duck, as ought to be the case according to our rule. With the 
allied Mergus cucullatus we have, however, a case of this kind: the two sexes 
differ conspicuously in general plumage, and to a considerable degree in the 
speculum, which is pure white in the male and grayish white in the female. 
Now the young males at first entirely resemble the females, and have a grayish 
white speculum, which becomes pure white at an earlier age than that at 
which the adult male acquires his other and more strongly marked sexual differ- 
ences: see Audubon, ‘‘Ornithological Biography,”’ vol. iii., 1835, pp. 249-250. 


312 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


We have as yet considered the transference of characters, 
relatively to their period of development, only in species in 
a natural state; we will now turn to domesticated animals, 
and-first touch on monstrosities and diseases. The presence 
of supernumerary digits, and the absence of certain pha- 
langes, must be determined at an early embryonic period— 
the tendency to profuse bleeding is at least congenital, as is 
probably color-blindness—yet these peculiarities, and other 
similar ones, are often limited in their transmission to one 
sex; so that the rule that characters developed at an early 
period tend to be transmitted to both sexes, here wholly 
fails. But this rule, as before remarked, does not appear 
to be nearly so general as the converse one, namely, that 
characters which appear late in life in one sex are trans- 
mitted exclusively to the same sex. From the fact of the 
above abnormal peculiarities becoming attached to one sex 
long before the sexual functions are active, we may infer 
that there must be some difference between the sexes at 
an extremely early age. With respect to sexually limited 
diseases, we know too little of the period at which they 
originate, to draw any safe conclusion. Gout, however, 
seems to fall under our rule, for it is generally caused by 
intemperance during manhood, and is transmitted from the 
father to his sons in a much more marked manner than to 
his daughters. 

In the various domestic breeds of sheep, goats, and 
cattle, the males differ from their respective females in 
the shape or development of their horns, forehead, mane, 
dewlap, tail, and hump on the shoulders; and these pecu- 
liarities, in accordance with our rule, are not fully developed 
until a rather late period of life. The sexes of dogs do not 
differ, except that in certain breeds, especially in the Scotch 
deer-hound, the male is much larger and heavier than the 
female; and, as we shall see in a future chapter, the male 
goes on increasing in size to an unusually late period of 
life, which, according to rule, will account for his increased 
size being transmitted to his male offspring alone. On the 


SEXUAL SELECTION 313 


other hand, the tortoise-shell color, which is confined to 
female cats, is quite distinct at birth, and this case violates 
the rule. There is a breed of pigeons in which the males 
alone are streaked with black, and the streaks can be de- 
tected even in the nestlings; but they become more con- 
spicuous at each successive moult, so that this case partly 
opposes and partly supports the rule. With the English 
Carrier and Pouter pigeons, the full development of the 
wattle and the crop occurs rather late in life, and, conform- 
ably with the rule, these characters are transmitted in full 
perfection to the males alone. The following cases perhaps 
come within the class previously alluded to, in which both 
sexes have varied in the same manner at a rather late 
period of life, and have consequently transferred their new 
characters to both sexes at a corresponding late period; 
and if so, these cases are not opposed to our rule: There 
exist sub-breeds of the pigeon, described by Neumeister, ** 
in which both sexes change their color during two or three 
moults (as is likewise the case with the Almond Tumbler); 
nevertheless, these changes, though occurring rather late in 
life, are common to both sexes. One variety of the Canary- 
bird, namely, the London Prize, offers a nearly analogous 
case. 

With the breeds of the Fowl, the inheritance of vari- 
ous characters by one or both sexes seems generally deter- 
mined by the period at which such characters are developed. 
Thus in all the many breeds in which the adult male differs 
greatly in color from the female, as well as from the wild 
parent-species, he differs also from the young male, so that 
the newly acquired characters must have appeared at a 
rather late period of life. On the other hand, in most of 
the breeds in which the two sexes resemble each other, the 
young are colored in nearly the same manner as their 
parents, and this renders it probable that their colors first 
appeared early in life. We have instances of this fact in 


48 “Das Ganze der Taubenzucht,’’ 1837, s. 21, 24. For the case of the 
, streaked pigeons, see Dr. Chapuis, ‘‘Le pigeon voyageur Belge,’’ 1865, p. 87, 


Descent—Vot. I.—14 


314 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


all black and white breeds, in which the young and old 
of both sexes are alike; nor can it be maintained that there 
is something peculiar in a black or white plumage which 
leads to its transference to both sexes; for the males alone 
of many natural species are either black or white, the females 
being differently colored. With the so-called Cuckoo sub- 
breeds of the fowl, in which the feathers are transversely 
pencilled with dark stripes, both sexes and the chickens are 
colored in nearly the same manner. The laced plumage of 
the Sebright bantam is the same in both sexes, and in the 
young chickens the wing-feathers are distinctly, though 
imperfectly, laced. Spangled Hamburgs, however, offer 
a partial exception; for the two sexes, though not quite 
alike, resemble each other more closely than do the sexes 
of the aboriginal parent-species; yet they acquire their char- 
acteristic plumage late in life, for the chickens are distinctly 
pencilled. With respect to other characters besides color, in 
the wild-parent species and in most of the domestic breeds, 
the males alone possess a well-developed comb; but in the 
young of the Spanish fowl it is largely developed at a very 
early age, and, in accordance with this early development 
in the male, it is of unusual size in theadult female. In 
the Game breeds pugnacity is developed at a wonderfully 
early age, of which curious proofs could be given; and this 
character is transmitted to both sexes, so that the hens, from 
their extreme pugnacity, are now generally exhibited in 
separate pens. With the Polish breeds the bony protuber- 
ance of the skull which supports the crest is partially 
developed even before the chickens are hatched, and the 
crest itself soon begins to grow, though at first feebly;* 
and in this breed the adults of both sexes are characterized 
by a great bony protuberance and an immense crest. 
Finally, from what we have now seen of the relation 


4" For full particulars and references on all these points respecting the 
several breeds of the Fowl see ‘‘Variation of Animals and Plants under Domes- 
tication,’’ vol. i. pp. 250, 256. In regard to the higher animals, the sexual 
differences which have arisen under domestication are described in the same 
work under the head of each species. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 815 


which exists in many natural species and domesticated 
races, between the period of the development of their char- 
acters and the manner of their transmission—for example, 
the striking fact of the early growth of the horns in the rein- 
deer, in which both sexes bear horns, in comparison with 
their much later growth in the other species in which the 
male alone bears horns—we may conclude that one, though 
not the sole, cause of characters being exclusively inherited 
by one sex is their development at a late age. And secondly, 
that one, though apparently a less efficient, cause of charac- 
ters being inherited by both sexes is their development at 
an early age, while the sexes differ but little in constitution. 
It appears, however, that some difference must exist be- 
tween the sexes even during a very early embryonic period, 
for characters developed at this age not rarely become’ 
attached to one sex. 


Summary and Concluding Remarks.—From the foregoing 
discussion on the various laws of inheritance, we learn that 
the characters of the parents often, or even generally, tend 
to become developed in the offspring of the same sex atthe 
same age, and periodically at the same season of the year, 
in which they first appeared in the parents. But these 
rules, owing to unknown causes, are far from being fixed. 
Hence, during the modification of a species, the successive 
changes may readily be transmitted in different ways; some 
to one sex, and some to both; some to the offspring at one 
age, and some to the offspring at all ages. Not only are the 
laws of inheritance extremely complex, but so are the causes 
which induce and govern variability. The variations thus 
induced are preserved and accumulated by sexual selection, 
which is in itself an extremely complex affair, depending, 
as it does, on the ardor in love, the courage, and the rivalry 
of the males, as well as on the powers of perception, the 
taste, and will of the female. Sexual selection will also be 
largely dominated by natural selection tending toward the 
general welfare of the species. Hence the manner in which 


816 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


the individuals of either or both sexes have been affected 
through sexual selection cannot fail to be complex in the 
highest degree. 

When variations occur late in life in one sex, and are 
transmitted to the same sex at the same age, the other sex 
and the young are left unmodified. When they occur late 
in life, but are transmitted to both sexes at the same age, 
the young alone are left unmodified. Variations, however, 
may occur at any period of life in one sex or in both, and 
be transmitted to both sexes at all ages, and then all 
the individuals of the species are similarly modified. In 
the following chapters it will be seen that all these cases 
frequently occur in nature. 

Sexual selection can never act on any animal before the 
age for reproduction arrives. From the great eagerness of 
the male it has generally acted on this sex, and not on 
the females. The males have thus become provided with 
weapons for fighting with their rivals, with organs for dis- 
covering and securely holding the female, and for exciting 
or charming her. When the sexes differ in these respects, 
it is also, as we have seen, an extremely general law that 
the adult male differs more or less from the young male; 
and we may conclude from this fact that the successive 
variations by which the adult male became modified did 
not generally occur much before the age for reproduction. 
Whenever some or many of the variations occurred early in 
life, the young males would partake more or less of the 
characters of the adult males; and differences of this kind 
between the old and young males may be observed in many 
species of animals. 

. Itis probable that young male animals have often tended 
to vary ina manner which would not only have been of no 
use to them at an early age, but would have been actu- 
ally injurious—as by acquiring bright colors, which would 
render them conspicuous to their enemies, or by acquiring 
structures, such as great horns, which would expend much 
vital force in their development. Variations of this kind 


SEXUAL SELECTION 317 


occurring in the young males would almost certainly be 
eliminated through natural selection. With the adult and 
experienced males, on the other hand, the advantages de- 
rived from the acquisition of such characters would more 
than counterbalance some exposure to danger, and some 
loss of vital force. 

As variations which give to the male a better chance of 
conquering other males, or of finding, securing, or charming 
the opposite sex, would, if they happened to arise in the 
female, be of no service to her, they would not be preserved 
in her through sexual selection. We have also good evi- 
dence with domesticated animals that variations of all kinds 
are, if not carefully selected, soon lost through intercrossing 
and accidental deaths. Consequently, in a state of nature, 
if variations of the above kind chanced to arise in the female 
line, and to be transmitted exclusively in this line, they 
would be extremely liable to be lost. If, however, the 
females varied and transmitted their newly acquired char- 
acters to their offspring of both sexes, the characters which 
were advantageous to the males would be preserved by them 
through sexual selection, and the two sexes would in conse- 
quence be modified in the same manner, although such 
characters were of no use to the females; but I shall here- 
after have to recur to these more intricate contingencies, 
Lastly, the females may acquire, and apparently have often 
acquired by transference, characters from the male sex. 

As variations occurring late in life, and transmitted to 
one sex alone, have incessantly been taken advantage of 
and accumulated through sexual selection in relation to 
the reproduction of the species; therefore it appears, at first 
sight, an unaccountable fact that similar variations have 
not frequently been accumulated through natural selec- 
tion, in relation to the ordinary habits of life. If this had 
occurred, the two sexes would often have been differently 
modified, for the sake, for instance, of capturing prey or 
of escaping from danger. Differences of this kind between 
the two sexes do occasionally occur, especially in the lower 


818 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


classes. But this implies that the two sexes follow different 
habits in their struggles for existence, which is a rare cir- 
cumstance with the higher animals. The case, however, is 
widely different with the reproductive functions, in which 
respect the sexes necessarily differ. For variations in struc- 
ture which are related to these functions have often proved 
of value to one sex, and, from having arisen at a late period 
of life, have been transmitted to one sex alone; and such 
- variations, thus preserved and transmitted, have given rise 
to secondary sexual characters. 

In the following chapters I shall treat ef the secondary 
sexual characters in animals of all classes, and shall endeavor 
in each case to apply the principles explained in the present 
chapter. The lowest classes will detain us for a very short 
time, but the higher animals, especially birds, must be treated 
at considerable length. It should be borne in mind that, for 
reasons already assigned, I intend to give only a few illus- 
trative instances of the innumerable structures by the aid of 
which the male finds the female, or, when found, holds her. 
On the other hand, all structures and instincts by the aid of 
which the male conquers other males, and by which he al- 
lures or excites the female, will be fully discussed, as these 
are in many ways the most interesting. 


Supplement on the Proportional Numbers of the Two Sexes 
in Animals belonging to Various Classes 


As no one, as far as I can discover, has paid attention to 
the relative numbers of the two sexes throughout the animal 
kingdom, I will here give such materials as I have been able 
to collect, although they are extremely imperfect. They 
consist in only a few instances of actual enumeration, and 
the numbers are not very large. As the proportions are 
known with certainty only in mankind, I will first give 
them as a standard of comparison. 

Man.—In England during ten years (from 1857 to 1866) 
the average number of children born alive year.y was 


SEXUAL SELECTION 319 


707,120, in the proportion of 104.5 males to 100 females. 
But in 1857 the male births throughout England were as 
105.2, and in 1865 as 104.0 to 100. Looking to separate 
districts, in Buckinghamshire (where about 5,000 children 
are annually born) the mean proportion of male to female 
births, during the whole period of the above ten years, was 
as 102.8 to 100; while in North Wales (where the average 
annual births are 12,878) it was as high as 106.2 to 100. 
Taking a still smaller district, viz., Rutlandshire (where 
the annual births average only 739), in 1864 the male births 
were as 114.6, and in 1862 as only 97.0 to 100; but even in 
this small district the average of the 7,885 births, during the 
whole ten years, was as 104.5 to 100; that is, in the same 
ratio as throughout England.** The proportions are some- 
times slightly disturbed by unknown causes; thus Prof. 
Faye states ‘‘that in some districts of Norway there has 
been during a decennial period a steady deficiency of boys, 
while in others the opposite condition has existed.’’ In 
France during forty-four years the male to the female 
births have been as 106.2 to 100; but during this period 
it has occurred five times in one department, and six times 
in another, that the female births have exceeded the males. 
In Russia the average proportion is as high as 108.9, and in 
Philadelphia, in the United States, as 110.5 to 100. The 
average for Europe, deduced by Bickes from about seventy 
million births, is 106 males to 100 females. On the other 
hand, with white children born at the Cape of Good Hope, : 
the proportion of males is so low as to fluctuate during suc- 
cessive years between 90 and 99 males for every 100 females. 
It is a singular fact that with Jews the proportion of male 


48 “Twenty-ninth Annual Report of the Registrar-General for 1866.’? In 
this report (p. xii.) a special decennial table is given. 

49 For Norway and Russia, see abstract of Prof. Faye’s researches, in 
“British and Foreign Medico-Chirurg. Review,’’ April, 1867, pp. 343, 345, 
For France, the ‘‘Annuaire pour J’An 1867,’’ p, 213, For Philadelphia, 
Dr. Stockton-Hough, ‘‘Social Science Assoc.,’? 1874. For the Cape of Good 
Hope, Quetelet as quoted by Dr, H. H, Zouteveen, in the Dutch translation of 
this work (vol. i, p. 417), where much information is given on the proportion 
of the sexes. 


820 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


births is decidedly larger than with Christians: thus in 
Prussia the proportion is as 118, in Breslau as 114, and 
in Livonia as 120 to 100; the Christian births in these 
countries being the same as usual; for instance, in Livonia 
as 104 to 100.° Prof. Faye remarks that, ‘‘a still greater 
preponderance of males would be met with if death struck 
both sexes in equal proportion in the womb and during 
birth. But the fact is, that for every 100 still-born females 
we have in several countries from 184.6 to 144.9 still-born 
males. During the first four or five years of life, also, more 
male children die than females; for example, in England, 
during the first year, 126 boys die for every 100 girls—a 
proportion which in France is still more unfavorable.’ * 
Dr. Stockton-Hough accounts for these facts in part by 
the more frequent defective development of males than of 
females. We have before seen that the male sex is more 
variable in structure than the female; and variations in im- 
portant organs would generally be injurious. But the size 
of the body, and especially of the head, being greater in 
male than female infants is another cause; for the males 
are thus more liable to be injured during parturition. Con- 
sequently the still-born males are more numerous; and, as 
a highly competent judge, Dr. Crichton Browne,” believes, 
male infants often suffer in health for some years after birth. 
Owing to this excess in the death-rate of male children, both 


50 In regard to the Jews, see M. Thury, ‘‘La Loi de Production des Sexes,”’ 
1863, p. 25. 

51 ‘British and Foreign Medico-Chirurg. Review,’’ April, 1867, ‘p. 343, 
Dr. Stark also remaris (‘‘Tenth Annual Report of Births, Deaths, etc., in Scot- 
land,’’ 1867, p. xxviii.) that ‘‘These examples may suffice to show that, at 
almost every stage of life, the males in Scotland have a greater liability to death 
and a higher death-rate than the females, The fact, however, of this pecu- 
larity being most strongly developed at that infantile period of life when the 
dress, food, and general treatment of both sexes are alike, seems to prove that 
the higher male death-rate 1s an impressed, natural, and constitutional peculiarity 
due to sex alone.”’ 

52 “West Riding Lunatic Asylum Reports,’’ vol. i, 1871, p. 8. Sir J. 
Simpson has proved that the head of the male infant exceeds that of the female 
by threv-eights of an inch in circumference, and by one-eighth in transverse 
diameter. Quetelet has shown that woman is born smaller than man; see 
Dr. Duncan, ‘‘Fecundity, Fertility, Sterility,’’ 1871, p. 382. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 321 


at birth and for some time subsequently, and owing to the 
exposure of grown men to various dangers, and to their 
tendency to emigrate, the females in all old-settled coun- 
tries, where statistical records have been kept," are found 
to preponderate considerably over the males. 

It seems at first sight a mysterious fact that in different 
nations, under different conditions and climates, in Naples, 
Prussia, Westphalia, Holland, France, Engiand, and the 
United States, the excess of male over female births is 
less when they are illegitimate than when legitimate.” 
This has been explained by different writers in many dif- 
ferent ways, as from the mothers being generally young, 
from the large proportion of first pregnancies, etc. But 
we have seen that male infants, from the large size of their 
heads, suffer more than female infants during parturition, 
and as the mothers of illegitimate children must be more 
liable than other women to undergo bad labors, from vari- 
ous causes, such as attempts at concealment by tight lacing, 
hard work, distress of mind, etc., their male infants would 
proportionably suffer. And this probably is the most effi- 
cient of all the causes of the proportion of males to females 
born alive being less among illegitimate children than among 
the legitimate. With most animals the greater size of the 
adult male than of the female is due to the stronger males 
having conquered the weaker in their struggles for the pos- 
session of the females, and no doubt it is owing to this fact 
that the two sexes of at least some animals differ in size at 
birth. Thus we have the curious fact that we may attrib- 
ute the more frequent deaths of male than female infants, 
especially among the illegitimate, at least in part to sexual 
selection. 

It has often been supposed that the relative age of the 


53 With the savage Guaranys of Paraguay, according to the accurate Azara 
(“Voyages dans l’Amerique mérid.,”’ tom. ii., 1809, pp. 60, 179), the women 
are to the men in the proportion of 14 to 13. 

54 Babbage, ‘‘Edinburgh Journal of Science,’’ 1829, vol. i. p. 88; also p. 90, 
on still-vorn children. On illegitimate children in England, see ‘‘Report of 
Registrar General for 1866 ”’ p. xv. 


822 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


two parents determines the sex of the offspring; and Prof. 
Leuckart® has advanced what he considers sufficient evi- 
dence, with respect to man and certain domesticated animals, 
that this is one important though not the sole factor in the 
result. So again the period of impregnation relatively to’ 
the state of the female has been thought by some to be the 
efficient cause; but recent observations discountenance this 
belief. According to Dr. Stockton-Hough,® the season of 
the year, the poverty or wealth of the parents, residence 
in the country or in cities, the crossing of foreign immi- 
grants, etc., all influence the proportion of the sexes. With 
mankind, polygamy has also been supposed to lead to the 
birth of a greater proportion of female infants; but Dr. J. 
Campbell carefully attended to this subject in the harems 
of Siam, and concludes that the proportion of male to female 
births is the same as from monogamous unions. Hardly any 
animal has been rendered so highly polygamous as the En- 
glish race-horse, and we shall immediately see that his male 
and female offspring are almost exactly equal in number. 
I will now give the facts which I have collected with respect 
to the proportional numbers of the sexes of various animals; 
and will then briefly discuss how far selection has come 
into play in determining the result. 

Horses.—Mr. Tegetmeier has been so kind as to tabulate 
for me from the ‘‘Racing Calendar’’ the births of race- 
horses during a period of twenty-one years, viz., from 1846 
to 1867; 1849 being omitted, as no returns were that year 
published. The total births were 25,560, consisting of 
12,763 males and 12,797 females, or in the proportion of 99.7 


55 Leuckart (in Wagner, ‘‘Handworterbuch der Phys.,’’ B, iv., 1853, s. 774), 
56 Social Science Assoc, of Phila., 1874. 
51 “Anthropological Review,’’ April, 1870, p. eviii. 

* 58 During eleven years a record was kept of the number of mares which 
proved barren or prematurely slipped their foals; and it deserves notice, as 
showing how infertile these highly nurtured and rather closely interbred ani- 
mals have become, that not far from one-third of the mares failed to produce liv- 
ing foals. Thus during 1866, 809 male colts and 816 female colts were born, 
and 743 mares failed to produce offspring. During 1867, 836 males aad 902 
females were born, and 794 mares failed. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 823 


males to100 females. As these numbers are tolerably large, 
and as they are drawn from all parts of England, during 
several years, we may with much confidence conclude that 
with the domestic horse, or at least with the race-horse, the 
two sexes are produced in almost equal numbers. The 
fluctuations in the proportions during successive years are 
closely like those which occur with mankind, when a small 
and thinly populated area is considered; thus in 1856 the 
male horses were as 107.1, and in 1867 as only 92.6, to 100 
females. In the tabulated returns the proportions vary 
in cycles, for the males exceeded the females during six 
successive years; and the females exceeded the males during © 
two periods each of four years: this, however, may be ac- 
cidental; at least I can detect nothing of the kind with man 
in the decennial table in the Registrar’s Report for 1866. 

Dogs.—During a period of twelve years, from 1857 to 
1868, the births of a large number of greyhounds, through- 
out England, were sent to the ‘‘Field’’ newspaper; and I am 
again indebted to Mr. Tegetmeier for carefully tabulating 
the results. The recorded births were 6,878, consisting of 
8,605 males and 3,273 females, that is, in the proportion 
of 110.1 males to 100 females. The greatest fluctuations 
occurred in 1864, when the proportion was as 95.3 males, 
and in 1867, as 116.3 males to 100 females. The above 
average proportion of 110.1 to 100 is probably nearly correct 
in the case of the greyhound, but whether it would hold 
with other domesticated breeds is in some degree doubtful. 
Mr. Cupples has inquired from several great breeders of 
dogs, and finds that all, without exception, believe that 
females are produced in excess; but he suggests that this 
belief may have arisen from females being less valued, and 
from the consequent disappointment producing a stronger 
impression on the mind. 

Sheep.—The sexes of sheep are not ascertained by agri- 
culturists until several months after birth, at: the period 
when the males are castrated; so that the following returns 
do not give the proportions at birth. Moreover, I find that 


824 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


several great breeders in Scotland, who annually raise some 
thousand sheep, are firmly convinced that a larger proportion 
of males than of females die during the first year or two. 
Therefore the proportion of males would be somewhat larger 
at birth than at the age of castration. This is a remarkable 
coincidence with what, as we have seen, occurs with man- 
kind, and both cases probably depend on the same cause. 
I have received returns from four gentlemen in England 
who have bred Lowland sheep, chiefly Leicesters, during 
the last ten to sixteen years; they amount altogether to 8,965 
births, consisting of 4,407 males and 4,558 females; that is 
in the proportion of 96.7 males to 100 females. With respect 
to Cheviot and blackfaced sheep bred in Scotland, I have 
received returns from six breeders, two of them on a large 
scale, chiefly for the years 1867-69, but some of the returns 
extend back to 1862. The total number recorded amounts 
to 50,685, consisting of 25,071 males, and 25,614 females, or 
in the proportion of 97.9 males to 100 females. If we take 
the English and Scotch returns together, the total number 
amounts to 59,650, consisting of 29,478 males and 80,172 
females, or as 97.7 to 100. So that with sheep at the age of 
castration the females are certainly in excess of the males, 
but probably this would not hold good at birth. 

Of Cattle I have received returns from nine gentlemen 
of 982 births, too few to be trusted; these consisted of 477 
bull-calves and 505 cow-calves; i.e, in the proportion of 
94.4 males to 100 females. The Rev. W. D. Fox informs 
me that in 1867, out of 84 calves born on a farm in Derby- 
shire, only one was a bull. Mr. Harrison Weir has inquired 
from several breeders of Pigs, and most of them estimate 
the male to the female births as about 7 to 6. This same 
gentleman has bred Rabbits for many years, and has noticed 


59 T am much indebted to Mr. Cupples for having procured for me the above 
returns from Scotland, as well as some of the following returns on cattle. Mr. 
R. Elliot, of Laighwood, first called my attention to the premature deaths of the 
males—a statement subsequently confirmed by Mr. Aitchison and others. To 
this latter gentleman, and to Mr. Payan, I owe my thanks for large returns 
as to sheep. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 825 


that a far greater number of bucks are produced than does. 
But estimations are of little value. 

Of mammalia in a state of nature I have been able to 
learn very little. In regard to the common rat, I have re- 
ceived conflicting statements. Mr. R. Elliot, of Laighwood, 
informs me that a rat-catcher assured him that he had always 
found the males in great excess, even with the young in the 
nest. In consequence of this, Mr. Elliot himself subse- 
quently examined some hundred old ones, and found the 
statement true. Mr. F. Buckland has bred a large number 
of white rats, and he also believes that the males greatly 
exceed the females. In regard to Moles, it is said that ‘‘the 
males are much more numerous than the females;’’ “ and 
as the catching of these animals is a special occupation the 
statement may perhaps be trusted. Sir A. Smith, in de- 
scribing an antelope of South Africa” (Kobus ellipsiprymnus), 
remarks that, in the herds of this and other species, the males 
are few in number compared with the females: the natives 
believe that they are born in this proportion; others believe 
that the younger males are expelled from the herds, and Sir 
A. Smith says that, though he has himself never seen herds 
consisting of young males alone, others affirm that this does 
occur. It appears probable that the young, when expelled 
from the herd, would often fall a prey fo the many beasts of 
prey of the country. 


Birds.—W ith respect to the Fowl, I have received only 
one account, namely, that out of 1,001 chickens of a highly 
bred stock of Cochins, reared during eight years by Mr. 
Stretch, 487 proved males, and 514 females; 7.¢., as 94.7 to 
100. In regard to domestic pigeons, there is good evidence 
either that the males are produced in excess, or that they 
live longer; for these birds invariably pair, and single 
males, as Mr. Tegetmeier informs me, can always be pur- 
chased cheaper than females. Usually the two birds reared 


60 Bell, “History of British Quadrupeds,’’ p. 100, 
*! ‘THustrations of the Zoology of 8. Africa,’ 1849, pl. 29, 


826 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


from the two eggs laid in the same nest are a male and 
a female; but Mr. Harrison Weir, who has been so large a 
breeder, says that he has often bred two cocks from the same 
nest, and seldom two hens; moreover, the hen is generally 
the weaker of the two, and more liable to perish. 

With respect to birds in a state of nature, Mr. Gould and 
others” are convinced that the males are generally the more 
numerous; and as the young males of many species resemble 
the females, the latter would naturally appear to be the 
more numerous. Large numbers of pheasants are reared 
by Mr. Baker, of Leadenhall, from eggs laid by wild birds, 
and he informs Mr. Jenner Weir that four or five males to 
one female are generally produced. An experienced observer 
remarks” that in Scandinavia the broods of the capercailzie 
and biackcock contain more males than females; and that 
with the Dal-ripa (a kind of ptarmigan) more males than 
females attend the leks or places of courtship; but this lat- 
ter circumstance is accounted for by some observers by a 
greater number of hen birds being killed by vermin. From 
various facts given by White, of Selborne,“ it seems clear 
that the males of the partridge must be in considerable’ 
excess in the south of England; and I have been assured 
that this is the case in Scotland. Mr. Weir, on inquiring 
from the dealers, who receive at certain seasons large num- 
bers of ruffs (Machetes pugnax), was told that the males 
are much the more numerous. This same naturalist has 
also inquired for me from the bird-catchers, who annually 
catch an astonishing number of various small species alive 
for the London market, and he was unhesitatingly answered 
by an old and trustworthy man, that with the chaffinch the 
males are in large excess; he thought as high as 2 males 
to 1 female, or at least as high as 5 to 8. The males of 


6 Brehm (“Tlust. Thierleben,”’ B. iv. s. 990) comes to the same conclusion, 

63 Qn the authority of L. Lloyd, ‘‘Game Birds of Sweden,’’ 1867, pp. 
12, 132. 

64 “Nat. Hist. of Selborne,’’ letter xxix., edit. of 1825, vol. i. p. 139. 

6 Mr, Jenner Weir received similar information, on making inquiries during 
the following year. To show the number of living chaffinches caught, I may 


SEXUAL SELECTION 3827 


the blackbird, he likewise maintained, were by far the 
more numerous, whether caught by traps or by netting at 
night. These statements may apparently be trusted, because 
this same man said that the sexes are about equal with the 
lark, the twite (Linaria montana), and goldfinch. On the 
other hand, he is certain that with the common linnet 
the females preponderate greatly, but unequally during 
different years; during some years he has found the females 
to the males as four to one. It should, however, be borne 
in mind that the chief season for catching birds does not 
begin till September, so that with some species partial 
migrations may have begun, and the flocks at this period 
often consist of hens alone. Mr. Salvin paid particular 
attention to the sexes of the humming-birds in Central 
America, and he is convinced that with most of the species 
the males are in excess; thus one year he procured 204 
specimens belonging to ten species, and these consisted of 
166 males and of only 88 females. With two other species 
the females were in excess; but the proportions apparently 
vary either during different seasons or in different localities; 
for on one occasion the males of Campylopterus hemileucurus — 
were to the females as 5 to 2, and on another occasion” in 
exactly the reversed ratio. As bearing on this latter point, 
I may add, that Mr. Powys found in Corfu and Epirus the 
sexes of the chaffinch keeping apart, and ‘‘the females by 
far the most numerous’’; while in Palestine Mr. Tristram 
found ‘‘the male flocks appearing greatly to exceed the 
female in number.’’” So again with the Quiscalus major, 
Mr. G. Taylor® says that in Florida there were ‘‘very few 
females in proportion to the males,’’ while in Honduras the 


mention that in 1869 there was a match between two experts, and one man 
caught in a day 62, and another 40, male chaffinches. The greatest number 
ever caught by one man in a single day was 70. 

86 ‘This,’ vol. ii. p. 260, as quoted in Gould’s ‘‘Trochilide,’’ 1861, p. 52, 
For the foregoing proportions I am indebted to Mr. Salvin for a table of hig 
results. 

67 “*This,’? 1860, p. 137; and 1867, p. 369. 

68 “This,’? 1862, p. 137. 


828 THE DESCENT. OF MAN 


proportion was the other way, the species there having the 
character of a polygamist. 


Fish.—With Fish the proportional numbers of the sexes 
can be ascertained only by catching them in the adult or 
nearly adult state; and there are many difficulties in arriving 
at any just conclusion.” Infertile females might readily 
be mistaken for males, as Dr. Giinther has remarked to me 
in regard to trout. Withsome species the males are believed 
to die soon after fertilizing the ova. With many species the 
males are of much smaller size than the females, so that a 
large number of males would escape from the same net by 
which the females were caught. M. Carbonnier,” who has 
especially attended to the natural history of the pike (Hsox 
luctus), states that many males, owing to their small size, 
are devoured by the larger females; and he believes that 
the males of almost all fish are exposed from this same 
cause to greater danger than the females. Nevertheless, in 
the few cases in which the proportional numbers have been 
actually observed, the males appear to be largely in excess. 
Thus Mr. R. Buist, the superintendent of the Stormontfield 
experiments, says that in 1865, out of 70 salmon first landed 
for the purpose of obtaining the ova, upward of 60 were 
males. In 1867 he again ‘‘calls attention to the vast dispro- 
portion of the males to the females. We had at the outset 
at least ten males to one female.’’ Afterward females suffi- 
cient for obtaining ova were procured. He adds, ‘‘from the 
great proportion of the males, they are constantly fighting 
and tearing each other on the spawning-beds.’’” This dis- 
proportion, no doubt, can be accounted for in part, but 
whether wholly is doubtful, by the males ascending the 
rivers before the females. Mr. F. Buckland remarks in 
regard to trout, that ‘‘it is a curious fact that the males 


6° Leuckart quotes Bloch (Wagner, ‘‘Handwérterbuch der Phys.,’’ B. iv., 
1853, s. 775), that with fish there are twice as many males as females. 

7 Quoted in the ‘‘Farmer,’’ March 18, 1869, p. 369. 

11 **The Stormontfield Piscicultural Experiments,’’ 1866, p. 23. The ‘‘Field”’ 
newspaper, June 29, 1867. : 


SEXUAL SELECTION 829 


preponderate very largely in number over the females. It 
invariably happens that when the first rush of fish is made 
to the net, there will be at least seven or eight males to one 
female found captive. I cannot quite account for this; 
either the males are more numerous than the females, or 
the latter seek safety by concealment rather than flight.’’ 
He then adds, that by carefully searching the banks suffi- 
cient females for obtaining ova can be found.” Mr. H. Lee’ 
informs me that out of 212 trout, taken for this purpose in 
Lord Portsmouth’s park, 150 were males and 62 females. 

The males of the Cyprinids likewise seem to be in ex- 
cess; but several members of this Family, viz., the carp, 
tench, bream, and minnow, appear regularly to follow the 
practice, rare in the animal kingdom, of polyandry; for 
the female while spawning is always attended by two males, 
one on each side, and in the case of the bream, by three 
or four males. This fact is so well known that it is always 
recommended to stock a pond with two male tenches to one > 
female, or at least with three males to two females. With 
the minnow, an excellent observer states that on the 
spawning-beds the males are ten times as numerous as 
the females; when a female comes among the males, ‘‘she 
is immediately pressed closely by a male on each side; and 
when they have been in that situation for a time, are super- 
seded by two other males.’’ ” 


Insects. —In this great Class the Lepidoptera almost alone 
afford means for judging of the proportional numbers of the 
sexes; for they have been collected with special care by 
many good observers, and have been largely bred from 
the egg or caterpillar state. I had hoped that some breeders 
of silk-moths might have kept an exact record, but after 
writing to France and Italy, and consulting various treatises, 


72 ‘“‘Tand and Water,’’ 1868, p. 41. 

3 Yarrell, ‘‘Hist. British Fishes,”’ vol. i, 1826, p. 307; on the Cyprinus 
carpio, p. 331; on the Finca vulgaris, p. 331; on the Abramis brama, p. 336, 
See, for the minnow (Leuciscus phoxinus), ‘‘Loudon’s Mag. of Nat. Hist. ,’’ vol. 
v., 1832, p. 682. 


830 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


I cannot find that this has ever been done. The general 
opinion appears to be that the sexes are nearly equal; but 
in Italy, as I hear from Prof. Canestrini, many breeders 
are convinced that the females are produced in excess. 
This same naturalist, however, informs me that in the two 
yearly broods of the Ailanthus silk-moth (Bombyx cynthia), 
the males greatly preponderate in the first, while in the 
second the two sexes are nearly equal, or the females rather 
in excess. : 

In regard to Butterflies in a state of nature, several ob- 
servers have been much struck by the apparently enormous 
preponderance of the males." Thus Mr. Bates,” in speak- 
ing of several species, about a hundred in number, which 
inhabit the Upper Amazons, says that the males are much 
more numerous than the females, even in the proportion of 
a hundred to one. In North America, Edwards, who had 
great experience, estimates in the genus Papilio the males 
to the females as four to.one; and Mr. Walsh, who informed 
me of this statement, says that with P. turnus this is certainly 
the case. In South Africa, Mr. R. Trimen found the males 
in excess in 19 species;” and in one of these, which swarms 
in open places, he estimated the number of males as fifty 
to one female. With another species, in which the males 
are numerous in certain localities, he collected only five 
females during seven years. In the island of Bourbon, 
M. Maillard states that the males of one species of Papilio 
are twenty times as numerous as the females.” Myr. Trimen 
informs me that as far as he has himself seen, or heard from 
others, it is rare for the females of any butterfly to exceed 
the males in number; but three South African species per- 
haps offer an exception. Mr. Wallace” states that the 


™ Leuckart quotes Meinecke (Wagner, ‘‘Handworterbuch der Phys.,’’ B. iv., 
1853, s. 775) that the males of Butterflies are three or four times as numerous 
as the females. 

78 “The Naturalist on the Amazons,”’ vol. ii., 1863, pp. 228, 347. 

76 Four of these cases are given by Mr. Trimen in his ‘‘Rhopalocera Africa 
Australis, ”’ 

™ Quoted by Trimen, ‘‘Transact. Ent. Soc.,”’ vol. v. part iv., 1866, p. 330. 

% ‘Transact. Linn. Soc.,’’ vol. xxv. p. 37. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 831 


females of Ornithoptera cresus, in the Malay Archipelago, 
are more common and more easily caught than the males; 
but this is a rare butterfly. I may here add, that in 
Hyperythra, a genus of moths, Guenée says that from 
four to five females are sent in collections from India 
for one male. 

When this subject of the proportional numbers of the 
sexes of insects was brought before the Entomological 
Society,” it was generally admitted that the males of most 
Lepidoptera, in the adult or imago state, are caught in 
greater numbers than the females; but this fact was attrib- 
uted by various observers to the more retiring habits of the 
females, and to the males emerging earlier from the cocoon. 
This latter circumstance is well known to occur with most 
Lepidoptera, as well as with other insects. So that, as 
M. Personnat remarks, the males of the domesticated 
Bombyx Yamamai are useless at the beginning of the sea- 
son, and the females at the end, from the want of mates.*. 
I cannot, however, persuade myself that these causes suffice 
to explain the great excess of males in the above cases of 
certain butterflies which are extremely common in their 
native countries. Mr. Stainton, who has paid very close 
attention during many years to the smaller moths, informs 
me that when he collected them in the imago state, he 
thought that the males were ten times as numerous as the 
females, but that, since he has reared them on a large scale 
from the caterpillar state, he is convinced that the females 
are the more numerous. Several entomologists concur in 
his view. Mr. Doubleday, however, and some others take 
an opposite view, and are convinced that they have reared 
from the eggs and caterpillars a larger proportion of -males 
than of females. 

Besides the more active habits of the males, their earlier 
emergence from the cocoon, and in some places their fre- 
quenting more open stations, other causes may be assigned 


7 ‘Proc, Entomolog. Soc.,’’ Feb. 17, 1868. / 
89 Quoted by Dr. Wallace in ‘‘Proc, Ent. Soc.,’’ 3d series, vol. v., 1867, p. 487. 


832 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


for an apparent or real difference in the proportional num- 
bers of the sexes of Lepidoptera, when captured in the 
imago state, and when reared from the egg or caterpillar 
state. I hear from Prof. Canestrini, that it is believed by 
many breeders in Italy that the female caterpillar of the 
silk-moth suffers more from the recent disease than the male, 
and Dr. Staudinger informs me that in rearing Lepidoptera 
more females die in the cocoon than males. With many 
species the female caterpillar is larger than the male, and 
a collector would naturally choose the finest specimens, 
and thus unintentionally collect a larger number of females. 
Three collectors have told me that this was their practice; 
but Dr. Wallace is sure that most collectors take all the 
specimens which they can find of the rarer kinds, which 
alone are worth the trouble of rearing. Birds when sur- 
rounded by caterpillars would probably devour the largest; 
and Prof. Canestrini informs me that in Italy some breed- 
ers believe, though on insufficient evidence, that in the 
first broods of the Ailanthus silk-moth the wasps destroy 
a larger number of the female than of the male caterpil- 
lars. Dr. Wallace further remarks that female caterpillars, 
from being larger than the males, require more time for 
their development, and consume more food and moisture; 
and thus they would be exposed during a longer time to 
danger from ichneumons, birds, etc., and in times of scarcity 
would perish in greater numbers. Hence it appears quite 
possible that, in a state of nature, fewer female Lepidoptera 
may reach maturity than males; and for our special object 
we are concerned with their relative numbers at maturity, 
when the sexes are ready to propagate their kind. 

The manner in which the males of certain moths congre- 
gate in extraordinary numbers round a single female ap- 
parently indicates a great excess of males, though this fact 
may perhaps be accounted for by the earlier emergence of 
the males from their cocoons. Mr. Stainton informs me 
that from twelve to twenty males may often be seen con- 
gregated round a female Hlachista rufocinerea. It is well 


SEXUAL SELECTION = 333 


known that if a virgin Lasiocampa quercus or Saturnia 
carpint be exposed in a cage, vast numbers of males collect 
round her, and, if confined in a room, will even come down 
the chimney to her. Mr. Doubleday believes that he has 
seen from fifty to a hundred males of both these species 
attracted in the course of a single day by a female in con- 
finement. In the Isle of Wight Mr. Trimen exposed a box 
in which a female of the Lasiocampa had been confined 
on the previous day, and five males soon endeavored to 
gain admittance. In Australia, M. Verreaux, having placed 
the female of a small Bombyx in a box in his pocket, was 
followed by a crowd of males, so that about 200 entered 
the house with him.* 

Mr. Doubleday has called my attention to M. Stau- 
dinger’s” list of Lepidoptera, which gives the prices of the 
males and females of 800 species of well-marked varieties 
of butterflies (Rhopalocera). The prices for both sexes of 
the very common species are of course the same; but in 114 
of the rarer species they differ; the males being in all cases, 
excepting one, the cheaper. On an average of the prices of 
the 113 species, the price of the male to that of the female 
is as 100 to 149; and this apparently indicates that inversely 
the males exceed the females in the same proportion. About 
2,000 species or varieties of moths (Heterocera) are cata- 
logued, those with wingless females being here excluded 
on account of the difference in habits between the two 
sexes: of these 2,000 species, 141 differ in price accord- 
ing to sex, the males of 130 being cheaper, and those of 
only 11 being dearer, than the females. The average price 
of the males of the 130 species, to that of the females, is 
as 100 to 148. With respect to the butterflies in this price 
list, Mr. Doubleday thinks (and no man in England has had 
more experience) that there is nothing in the habits of the 
species which can account for the difference in the prices of 
the two sexes, and that it can be accounted for only by an 


81 Blanchard, ‘‘Metamorphoses, Mceurs des Insectes,’’ 1868, pp. 225-226, 
82 “I enidopteren-Doubletten Liste,’’ Berlin, No, x., 1866. 


384 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


excess in the number of the males. But I am bound to add 
that Dr. Staudinger informs me that he is himself of a dif- 
ferent opinion. He thinks that the less active habits of the 
females and the earlier emergence of the males will account 
for his collectors securing a larger number of males than of 
females, and consequently for the lower prices of the former. 
With respect to specimens reared from the caterpillar state, 
Dr. Staudinger believes, as previously stated, that a greater 
number of females than of males die while confined in the 
cocoons. He adds that with certain species one sex seems 
to preponderate over the other during certain years. 

Of direct observations on the sexes of Lepidoptera, reared 
either from eggs or caterpillars, I have received only the few 
following cases: 


Males | Females 

The Rev. J. Hellins,* of Exeter, reared, during 1868, mmaEeH 
of 73 species, which consisted of . . 153 137 

Mr. Albert Jones, of Eltham, reared, pees 1868, imagos of 9 
species, which consisted of . . . -| 159 126 
During 1869 he reared imagos from 4 species, “consisting of .| 114 112 

Mr. Buckler, of Emsworth, Hants, during 1869, reared imagos 
from 74 species, consisting of . 180 169 

Dr. Wallace, of Colchester, reared from ‘one brood of Bombyx 
cynthia . . 52 48 

Dr, Wallace raised, from cocoons of Bombyx Pernyi_ sent from 
China, during 1869. . . 224 123 

Dr. Wallace raised, during 1868 and 1869, from two lots of 
cocoons of Bombyx yamamai . . 2. 2 es 6 we we 52 46 
Totals . 2. 2 6 © © © eo ew we we ww ew ff 984 161 


So that in these eight lots of cocoons and eggs, males 
were produced in excess. Taken together, the proportion 
of males is as 122.7 to 100 females. But the numbers are 
hardly large enough to be trustworthy. 

On the whole, from these various sources of evidence, all 
pointing in the same direction, I infer that with most species 


8 This naturalist has been so kind as to send me some results from former 
years, in which the females seemed to preponderate; but so many of the figures 
were estimates, that I found it impossible to tabulate them. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 835 


of Lepidoptera, the mature males generally exceed the fe- 
males in number, whatever the proportions may be at their 
first emergence from the egg. 

With reference to the other orders of insects, I have been 
able to collect very little reliable information. With the 
stag-beetle (Lucanus cervus) ‘‘the males appear to be much 
more numerous than the females’’; but when, as Cornelius 
remarked during 1867, an unusual number of these beetles 
appeared in one part of Germany, the females appeared to 
~ exceed the males as six to one. With one of the Elateride, 
the males are said to be much more numerous than the 
females, and ‘‘two or three are often found united with one 
female;* so that here polyandry seems to prevail.’’ With 
Siagonium (Staphylinide), in which the males are furnished 
with horns, ‘‘the females are far more numerous than the 
‘opposite sex.’’ Mr. Janson stated at the Entomological 
Society that the females of the bark-feeding Tomicus villosus 
are so common as to bea plague, while the males are so rare 
as to be hardly known. 

It is hardly worth while saying anything about the pro- 
portion of the sexes in certain species and even groups 
of insects, for the males are unknown or very rare, and the 
females are parthenogenetic, that is, fertile without sexual 
union; examples of this are afforded by several of the 
Cynipide.** In all the gall-making Cynipide known to 
Mr. Walsh, the females are four or five times as numerous 
as the males; and so it is, as he informs me, with the gall- 
making Cecidomyiiz (Diptera). With some common species 
of Saw-flies (Tenthredine) Mr. F. Smith has reared hun- 
dreds of specimens from larve of all sizes, but has never 
reared a single male; on the other hand, Curtis says,** that 


8 Giinther’s ‘‘Record of Zoological Literature,’’ 1867, p. 260. On the ex- 
cess of female Lucanus, ibid. p. 250. On the males of Lucanus in England, 
Westwood, ‘‘Modern Class. of Insects,’’ vol. i. p. 187. On the Siagonium, 
ibid. p. 172. 

85 Walsh, in ‘“‘The American Entomologist,’ vol. i., 1869, p. 103. F. Smith, 
“Record of Zoological Literature,’’ 1867, p. 328. 

86 ‘Farm Insects,’’ pp. 45-46. 


836 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


with certain species (Athalia) bred by him, the males were 
to the females as six to one; while exactly the reverse oc- 
curred with the mature insects of the same species caught 
in the fields. In the family of Bees, Hermann Miiller® col- 
lected a large number of specimens of many species, and 
reared others from the cocoons, and counted the sexes. He 
found that the males of some species greatly exceeded the 
females in number; in others the reverse occurred; and in 
others the two sexes were nearly equal. But as in most 
cases the males emerge from the cocoons before the females, 
they are at the commencement of the breeding season prac- 
tically in excess. Miiller also observed that the relative 
number of the two sexes in some species differed much 
in different localities. But as H. Miller has himself re- 
marked to me, these remarks must be received with some 
caution, as one sex might more easily escape observation 
than the other. Thus his brother, Fritz Miller, has noticed 
in Brazil that the two sexes of the same species of bee some- 
times frequent different kinds of flowers. With respect to 
the Orthoptera, I know hardly anything about the relative 
number of the sexes; Kérte,* however, says that, out 
of 500 locusts which he examined, the males were to the 
females as five to six. With the Neuroptera, Mr. Walsh 
states that in many, but by no means in all the species of 
the Odonatous group, there is a great overplus of males; 
in the genus Hetzrina, also, the males are generally at least 
four times as numerous as the females. In certain species 
in the genus Gomphus the males are equally in excess, 
while in two other species the females are twice or thrice 
as numerous as the males. In some European species of 
Psocus thousands of females may be collected without a 
single male, while with other species of the same genus 
both sexes are common.” In England, Mr. MacLachlan 


87 “Anwendung der Darwin’schen Lehre,”’ ‘‘Verh, d. n. V. Jahrg.’’ xxiv. 

8 “Die Strich, Zug oder Wanderheuschrecke,’’ 1828, p. 20. 

8 “Observations on N. American Neuroptera,’? by H. Hagen and B. D. 
Walsh, ‘‘Proc. Ent Soc. Philadelphia,’’ Oct. 1863, pp. 168, 223, 239. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 887 


has captured hundreds of the female Apatania muliebris, 
but has never seen the male; and of Boreus hyemalis only 
four or five males have been seen here.” With most of 
these species (excepting the Tenthredinz) there is at present 
no evidence that the females are subject to parthenogenesis; 
and thus we see how ignorant we are of the causes of the 
apparent discrepancy in the proportion of the two sexes. 
In the other classes of the Articulata I have been able 
to collect still less information. With Spiders, Mr. Black- 
wall, who has carefully attended to this class during many 
years, writes to me that the males, from their more erratic 
habits, are more commonly seen, and therefore appear more 
numerous. This is actually the case with a few species; 
but he mentions several species in six genera, in which 
the females appear to be_much more numerous than the 
males." The small size of the males in comparison with 
the females (a peculiarity which is sometimes carried to an 
extreme degree), and their widely different appearance, may 
account in some instances for their rarity in collections.” 
Some of the lower Crustaceans are able to propagate their 
kind asexually, and this will account for the extreme rarity 
of the males; thus Von Siebold® carefully examined no less 
than 18,000 specimens of Apus from twenty-one localities, 
and among these he found only 319 males. With some 
other forms (as Tanais and Cypris), as Fritz Miller informs 
me, there is reason to believe that the males are much 
shorter lived than the females; and this would explain 
their scarcity, supposing the two sexes to be at first equal 
in number. On the other hand, Miiller has invariably 
taken far more males than females of the Diastylide and 
of Cypridina on the shores of Brazil; thus with a species 


% ‘Proc, Ent. Soe. London,’? Feb, 17, 1868. 

*1 Another great authority with respect to this class, Prof. Thorell of Upsala 
(‘On European Spiders,’’ 1869-70, part i. p. 205) speaks as if female spiders 
were generally commoner than the males, 

® See, on this subject, Mr. O. P. Cambridge, as quoted in eee Jour- 
nal of Science, »? 1868, p. 429. 

% “Reitrige zor Parthenogenesis, ” p. 174, 


Descent—Vox. i.—15 


538 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


in the latter genus, 63 specimens caught the same day in- 
cluded 57 males; but he suggests that this preponderance 
may be due to some unknown difference in the habits of 
the two sexes. With one of the higher Brazilian crabs, 
namely, a Gelasimus, Fritz Miiller found the males to be 
more numerous than the females. According to the large 
experience of Mr. C. Spence Bate, the reverse seems to be 
the case with six common British crabs, the names of which 
he has given me. 


The Proportion of the Sexes in Relation to Natural Selection 


There is reason to suspect that in some cases man has by 
selection indirectly influenced his own sex-producing powers. 
Certain women tend to produce during their whole lives 
more children of one sex than of the other; and the same 
holds good of many animals, for instance, cows and horses; 
thus Mr. Wright, of Yeldersley House, informs me that one 
of his Arab mares, though put seven times to different 
horses, produced seven fillies. Though I have very little 
evidence on this head, analogy would lead to the belief 
that the tendency to produce either sex would be inherited 
like almost every other peculiarity, for instance, that of pro- 
ducing twins; and concerning the above tendency a good 
authority, Mr. J. Downing, has communicated to me facts 
which seem to prove that this does occur in certain families 
of short-horn cattle. Col. Marshall*t has recently found on 
careful examination that the Todas, a hill-tribe of India, 
consist of 112 males and 84 females of all ages—that is ina 
ratio of 183.8 males to 100 females. The Todas, who are 
polyandrous in their marriages, during former times invari- 
ably practiced female infanticide; but this practice has now 
been discontinued for a considerable period. Of the chil- 
dren born within late years, the males are more numerous 
than the females, in the proportion of 124 to 100. Col. 
Marshall accounts for this fact in the following ingenious 


% “The Todas,’? 1873, pp. 100, 111, 194, 196, 


SEXUAL SELECTION 339 


manner: ‘‘Let us for the purpose of illustration take three 
families as representing an average of the entire tribe; say 
that one mother gives birth to six daughters and no sons; 
a second mother has six sons only, while the third mother 
has three sons and three daughters. The first mother, fol- 
lowing the tribal custom, destroys four daughters and pre- 
serves two. The second retains her six sons. The third 
kills two daughters and keeps one, as also her three sons. 
We have, then, from the three families, nine sons and three 
daughters with which to continue the breed. But while the 
males belong to families in which the tendency to produce 
sons is great, the females are of those of a converse inclina- 
tion. Thus the bias strengthens with each generation, until, 
as we find, families grow to have habitually more sons than 
daughters.” 

That this result would follow from the above form of 
infanticide seems almost certain; that is, if we assume that 
a sex-producing tendency is inherited. But as the above - 
numbers are so extremely scanty, I have searched for ad- 
ditional evidence, but cannot decide whether what I have 
found is trustworthy; nevertheless the facts are, perhaps, 
worth giving. The Maories of New Zealand have long 
practiced infanticide; and Mr. Fenton’ states that he ‘‘has 
met with instances of women who have destroyed four, six, 
and even seven children, mostly females. However, the 
universal testimony of those best qualified to judge is con- 
clusive that this custom has for many years been almost ex- 
tinct. Probably the year 1835 may be named as the period 
of its ceasing to exist.’’ Now among the New Zealanders 
as with the Todas, male births are considerably in excess, 
Mr. Fenton remarks (p. 80): ‘‘One fact is certain, although 
the exact period of the commencement of this singular con- 
dition of the disproportion of the sexes cannot be demon- 
stratively fixed, it is quite clear that this course of decrease 
was in full operation during the years 1830 to 1844, when 


% ‘‘Aborigina Inhabitants of New Zealand; Government Report,’ 1859, 
p. 36. 


840 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


the non-adult population of 1844 was being produced, and 
has continued with great energy up to the present time.’’ 
The following statements are taken from Mr. Fenton (p. 26), 
but, as the numbers are not large, and as the census was not 
accurate, uniform results cannot be expected. It should be 
borne in mind in this and the following cases that the normal 
state of every population is an excess of women, at least in 
all civilized countries, chiefly owing to the greater mortality 
of the male sex during youth, and partly to accidents of all 
kinds later in life. In 1858 the native population of New 
Zealand was estimated as consisting of 31,667 males and 
24,303 females of all ages, that is in the ratio of 130.3 males 
to 100 females. But during this same year, and in certain 
limited districts, the numbers were ascertained with much 
care, and the males of all ages were here 753 and the females 
616; that is in the ratio of 122.2 males to 100 females. It is — 
more important for us that during this same year of 1858 the 
non-adult males within the same district were found to be 
178, and the non-adult females 142, that is in the ratio of 
125.3 to 100. It may be added that in 1844, at which period 
female infanticide had only lately ceased, the non-adult males 
in one district were 281, and the non-adult females only 194, 
that is in the ratio of 144.8 males to 100 females. 

In the Sandwich Islands the males exceed the females 
in number. Infanticide was formerly practiced there to a 
frightful extent, but was by no means confined to female 
infants, as is shown by Mr. Ellis,** and as I have been in- 
formed by Bishop Staley and the Rev. Mr. Coan. Never- 
theless, another apparently trustworthy writer, Mr. Jarves,” 
whose observations apply to the whole archipelago, remarks: 
‘‘Numbers of women are to be found who confess to the 
murder of from three to six or eight children’’; and he 
adds, ‘‘females, from being considered less useful than 
males, were more often destroyed.’’ From what is known 
to occur in other parts of the world, this statement is prob- 


9 **Narrative of a Tour through Hawaii,’’ 1826, p. 298. 
¥ “History of the Sandwich Islands,’ 1843, p. 93. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 841 


able, but must be received with much caution. The prac- 
tice of infanticide ceased about the year 1819, when idolatry 
was abolished and missionaries settled in the islands. <A 
careful census in 1839, of the adult and taxable men and 
women in the island of Kauai and in one district of Oahu 
(Jarves, p. 404), gives 4,728 males and 8,776 females; that 
is in the ratio of 125.08 to 100. At the same time the 
number of males under fourteen years in Kauai and under 
eighteen in Oahu was 1,797, and of females of the same ages 
1,429; and here we have the ratio of 125.75 males to 100 
females. 

In a census of all the islands in 1850 the males of all 
ages amounted to 36,272, and the females to 83,128, or as 
109.49 to 100. The males under seventeen years amounted 
to 10,778, and the females under the same age to 9,598, or 
as 112.3 to 100. From the census of 1872 the proportion of 
males of all ages (including half-castes) to females is as 125.36 
to 100. It must be borne in mind that all these returns for 
the Sandwich Islands give the proportion of living males to 
living females, and not of the births; and, judging from all 
civilized countries, the proportion of males would have been 
considerably higher if the numbers had referred to births.” 


% This is given in the Rev. H. T. Cheever’s ‘‘Life in the Sandwich Islands,” 
1851, p. 277. 

39 Dr. Coulter, in describing (‘‘Journal R. Geograph. Soc.,’’ vol. v., 1835, p. 
67) the state of California about the year 1830, says that the natives, reclaimed 
by the Spanish missionaries, have nearly all perished, or are perishing, although 
well treated, not driven from their native land, and kept from the use of spirits. 
He attributes this, in great part, to the undoubted fact that the men greatly ex- 
ceed the women in number; but he does not know whether this is due to a fail- 
ure of female offspring, or to more females dying during early youth. The latter 
alternative, according to all analogy, is very improbable. -He adds that ‘‘infanti- 
cide, properly so called, is not common, though very frequent recourse is had 
to abortion.’’ If Dr. Coulter is correct about infanticide, this case cannot be 
advanced in support of Col. Marshall’s view. From the rapid decrease of the 
reclaimed natives, we may suspect that, as in the cases lately given, their fer- 
tility has been diminished from changed habits of life. 

I had hoped to gain some light on this subject from the breeding of dogs; 
fnasmuch as in most breeds, with the exception, perhaps, of greyhounds, many 
more female puppies are destroyed than males, just as with the Toda infants, 
Mr. Cupples assures me that this is usual with Scotch deer-hounds. Unfortu- 
nately, I know nothing of the proportion of the sexes in any breed, excepting 
greyhounds, and there the male births are to the female as 110.1 to 100. Now 


842 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


From the several foregoing cases we have some reason 
to believe that infanticide practiced in the manner above 
explained tends to make a male-producing race; but I am 
far from supposing that this practice in the case of man, 
or some analogous process with other species, has been the 
sole determining cause of an excess of males. There may 
be some unknown law leading to this result in decreasing 
races which have already become somewhat infertile. Be- 
sides the several causes previously alluded to, the greater 
facility of parturition among savages, and the less conse- 
quent injury to their male infants, would tend to increase 
the proportion of live-born males to females. There does 
not, however, seem to be any necessary connection between 
savage life and a marked excess of males; that is, if we may 
judge by the character of the scanty offspring of the lately 
existing Tasmanians and of the crossed offspring of the 
Tahitians now inhabiting Norfolk Island. 

As the males and females of many animals differ some- 
what in habits and are exposed in different degrees to dan- 
ger, it is probable that in many cases more of one sex than 
of the other are habitually destroyed. But as far as I 
can trace out the complication of causes, an indiscriminate 
though large destruction of either sex would not tend to 
modify the sex-producing power of the species. With | 
strictly social animals, such as bees or ants, which produce 
a vast number of sterile and fertile females in comparison 
with the males, and to whom this preponderance is of para- 
mount importance, we can see that those communities would 
flourish best which contained females having a strong in- 
herited tendency to produce more and more females; and 


from inquiries made from many breeders, it seems that the females are in some 
Tespects more esteemed, though otherwise troublesome; and it does not appear 
that the female puppies of the best-bred dogs are systematically destroyed more 
than the males, though this does sometimes take place to a limited extent, 
Therefore I am unable to decide whether we can, on the above principles, ac- 
count for the preponderance of male births in greyhounds. On the other hand, 
we have seen that with horses, cattle, and sheep, which are too valuable for 
the young of either sex to be destroyed, if there is any difference, the females 
are slightly in excess, 


SEXUAL SELECTION 343 


in such cases an unequal sex-producing tendency would 
be ultimately gained through natural selection. With ani- 
mals living in herds or troops, in which the males come to 
the front and defend the herd, as with the bisons of North 
America and certain baboons, it is conceivable that a male- 
producing tendency might be gained by natural selection; 
for the individuals of the better defended herds would leave 
more numerous descendants. In the case of mankind the 
advantage arising from having a preponderance of xen in 
the tribe is supposed to be one chief cause of t!:¢ practice 
of female infanticide. 

In no case, as far as we can see, would sa inherited 
tendency to produce both sexes in equal numbers, or to 
produce one sex in excess, be a direct advantage or dis- 
advantage to certain individuals more than {o others; for 
instance, an individual with a tendency to ‘produce more 
males than females would not succeed better in the battle 
for life than an individual with an opposite tendency; and 
therefore a tendency of this kind could not be gained 
through natural selection. Nevertheless, there are certain 
animals (for instance, fishes and cirripeds) in which two 
or more males appear to be necessary for the fertilization of 
the female; and the males accordingly largely preponderate, 
but it is by no means obvious how this male-producing 
tendency could have been acquired. I formerly thought 
that when a tendency to produce the two sexes in equal 
numbers was advantageous to the species, it would follow 
from natural selection, but I now see that the whole prob- 
lem is so intricate that it is safer to leave its solution. 
for the future. 


344 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


CHAPTER IX 


SECONDARY SEXUAL CHARACTERS IN THE LOWER CLASSES 
OF THE ANIMAL KINGDOM 


These characters absent in the lowest classes—Brilliant colors—Mollusca— 
Annelids—Crustacea, secondary sexual characters strongly developed; 
dimorphism; color; characters not acquired before maturity—Spiders, 
sexual colors of; stridulation by the males—Myriapoda ' 


ITH animals belonging to the lower classes, the 
\ \/ two sexes are not rarely united in the same 
individual, and therefore secondary sexual char- 
acters cannot be developed. In many cases where the sexes 
are separate, both are permanently attached to some support, 
and the one cannot search or struggle for the other. More- 
over, it is almost certain that these animals have too imper- 
fect senses and much too low mental powers to appreciate 
each other’s beauty or other attractions, or to feel rivalry. 
Hence in these classes or sub-kingdoms, such as the Pro- 
tozoa, Coelenterata, Echinodermata, Scolecida, secondary 
sexual characters, of the kind which we have to consider, 
do not occur; and this fact agrees with the belief that such 
characters in the higher classes have been acquired through 
sexual selection, which depends on the will, desire, and 
choice of either sex. Nevertheless some few apparent ex- 
ceptions occur; thus, as I hear from Dr. Baird, the males 
of certain Entozoa, or internal parasitic worms, differ 
slightly in color from the females; but we have no reason 
to suppose that such differences have been augmented 
through sexual selection. Contrivances by which the male 
holds the female, and which are indispensable for the propa- 
gation of the species, are independent of sexual selection, 
and have been acquired through ordinary selection. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 845 


Many of the lower animals, whether hermaphrodites or 
with separate sexes, are ornamented with the most brilliant 
tints, or are shaded and striped in an elegant manner; for 
instance, many corals and sea-anemones (Actiniz), some 
jelly-fish (Meduse, Porpita, etc.), some Planariz, many 
star-fishes, Hchini, Ascidians, etc.; but we may conclude 
from the reasons already indicated, namely, the union of 
the two sexes in some of these animals, the permanently 
affixed condition of others, and the low mental powers of 
all, that such colors do not serve as a sexual attraction, and 
have not been acquired through sexual selection. It should 
be borne in mind that in no case have we sufficient evidence 
that colors have been thus acquired, except where one sex 
is much more brilliantly or conspicuously colored than the 
other, and where there is no difference in habits between 
the sexes sufficient to account for their different colors. 
- But the evidence is rendered as complete as it can ever be, 
only when the more ornamented individuals, almost always 
the males, voluntarily display their attractions before the 
other sex; for we cannot believe that such display is use- 
less, and if it be advantageous, sexual selection will almost 
inevitably follow. We may, however, extend this conclu- 
sion to both sexes, when colored alike, if their colors are 
plainly analogous to those of one sex alone in certain other 
species of the same group. 

How, then, are we to account for the beautiful or even 
gorgeous colors of many animals in the lowest classes? 
It appears doubtful whether such colors often serve as 
a protection; but that we may easily err on this head will 
be admitted by every one who reads Mr. Wallace’s excellent 
essay on this subject. It would not, for instance, at first 
occur to any one that the transparency of the Medusa, or 
jelly-fishes, is of the highest service to them as a protection; 
but when we are reminded by Hickel that not only the 
medusz, but many floating mollusca, crustaceans, and even 
small oceanic fishes partake of this same glass-like appear- 
ance, often accompanied by prismatic colors, we can hardly 


346 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


doubt that they thus escape the notice of pelagic birds and 
other enemies. M. Giard is also convinced’ that the bright 
tints of certain sponges and ascidians serve as a protection. 
Conspicuous colors are likewise beneficial to many animals 
as a warning to their would-be devourers that they are dis- 
tasteful, or that they possess some special means of defence; 
but this subject will be discussed more conveniently 
hereafter. 

We can, in our ignorance of most of the lowest animals, 
only say that their bright tints result either from the 
chemical nature or the minute structure of their tissues, 
independently of any benefit thus derived. Hardly any 
color is finer than that of arterial blood; but there is no 
reason to suppose that the color of the blood is in itself 
any advantage; and, though it adds to the beauty of the 
maiden’s cheek, no one will pretend that it has been ac- 
quired for this purpose. So again with many animals, 
especially the lower ones, the bile is richly colored; thus, 
as I am informed by Mr. Hancock, the extreme beauty of 
the Holide (naked sea-slugs) is chiefly due to the biliary 
glands being seen through the translucent integuments—this 
beauty being probably of no service to these animals. The 
tints of the decaying leaves in an American forest are de- 
scribed by every one as gorgeous; yet no one supposes that 
these tints are of the least advantage to the trees. Bearing 
in mind how many substances closely analogous to natural 
organic compounds have been recently formed by chemists, 
and which exhibit the most splendid colors, it would have 
been a strange fact if substances similarly colored had not 
often originated, independently of any useful end thus 
gained, in the complex laboratory of living organisms. 


The Sub-kingdom of the Mollusca.—Throughout this great 
division of the animal kingdom, as far as I can discover, 
secondary sexual characters, such as we are here consider- 
ing, never occur. Nor could they be expected in the three 


1 “Archives de Zoolog Expér.,’’ Oct, 1872, p. 563, 


SEXUAL SELECTION 347 


lowest classes, namely, in the Ascidians, Polyzoa, and 
Brachiopods (constituting the Molluscoida of some authors), 
for most of these animals are permanently affixed to a sup- 
port or have their sexes united in the same individual. In 
the Lamellibranchiata, or bivalve shells, hermaphroditism 
is not rare. In the next higher class of the Gasteropoda, 
or univalve shells, the sexes are either united or separate. 
But in the latter case the males never possess special organs 
for finding, securing, or charming the females, or for fight- 
ing with other males. As I am informed by Mr. Gary 
Jeffreys, the sole external difference between the sexes con- 
sists in the shell sometimes differing a little in form; for in- 
stance, the shell of the male periwinkle (Littorina littorea) 
is narrower and has a more elongated spire than that of the 
female. But differences of this nature, it may be presumed, 
are directly connected with the act of reproduction, or with 
the development of the ova. 

The Gasteropoda, though capable of iedeiotion and 
furnished with imperfect eyes, do not appear to be endowed 
with sufficient mental powers for the members of the same 
sex to struggle together in rivalry, and thus to acquire 
secondary sexual characters. Nevertheless, with the pul- 
moniferous gasteropods, or land-snails, the pairing is pre- 
ceded by courtship; for these animals, though hermaphro- 
dites, are compelled by their structure to pair together. 
Agassiz remarks,” ‘‘Quiconque a eu l’occasion d’observer 
les amours des limacons, ne saurait mettre en doute la 
séduction déployée dans les mouvements et les allures qui 
préparent et accomplissent le double embrassement de ces 
hermaphrodites.’’ These animals appear also susceptible 
of some degree of permanent attachment: an accurate ob- 
server, Mr. Lonsdale, informs me that he placed a pair 
of land-snails (Helix pomatia), one of which was weakly, 
in a small and ill-provided garden. After a short time 
the strong and healthy individual disappeared, and was 


2 “De VEspéce et de la Class.,’’ etc., 1869, p. 106. 


348 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


traced by its track of slime over a wall into an adjoining 
well-stocked garden. Mr. Lonsdale concluded that it had 
deserted its sickly mate; but after an absence of twenty-four 
hours it returned, and apparently communicated the result 
of its successful exploration, for both then started along 
the same track and disappeared over the wall. 

Even in the highest class of the Mollusca, the Cepha- 
lopoda, or cuttlefishes, in which the sexes are separate, 
secondary sexual characters of the present kind do not, as 
far as I can discover, occur. This is a surprising circum- 
stance, as these animals possess highly developed sense- 
organs and have considerable mental powers, as will be 
admitted by every one who has watched their artful endeav- 
ors to escape from an enemy.® Certain Cephalopoda, how- 
ever, are characterized by one extraordinary sexual character, 
namely, that the male element collects within one of the 
arms, or tentacles, which is then cast off, and, clinging 
by its sucking-disks to the female, lives for a time an inde- 
pendent life. So completely does the cast-off arm resemble 
a separate animal, that it was described by Cuvier as a para- 
sitic worm under the name of Hectocotyle. But this mar- 
vellous structure may be classed as a primary rather than 
as a secondary sexual character. 

Although with the Mollusca sexual selection does not 
seem to have come into play, yet many univalve and bivalve 
shells, such as volutes, cones, scallops, etc., are beautifully 
colored and shaped. The colors do not appear in most cases 
to be of any use as a protection; they are probably the direct 
result, as in the lowest classes, of the nature of the tissues; 
the patterns and the sculpture of the shell depending on its 
manner of growth. The amount of light seems to be influ- 
ential to a certain extent; for although, as repeatedly stated 
by Mr. Gwyn Jeffreys, the shells of some species living 
ata profound depth are brightly colored, yet we generally 
see the lower surfaces, as well as the parts covered by the 


3 See, for instance, the account we I have given in my ‘Journal of 
Researches, ’’ 1845, p. 7. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 849 


mantle, less highly colored than the upper and , exposed 
surfaces.* In some cases, as with shells living among corals 
or brightly tinted sea-weeds, the bright colors may serve as 
a protection. -But that many of the nudibranch mollusca, 
or sea-slugs, are as beautifully colored as any shells, may 
be seen in Messrs. Alder and Hancock’s magnificent work; 
and, from information kindly given me by Mr. Hancock, 
it seems extremely doubtful whether these colors usually 
serve as a protection. With some species this may be the 
case, as with one kind which lives on the green leaves of 
alge, and is itself bright-green. But many brightly colored, 
white, or otherwise conspicuous species do not seek conceal- 
ment; while again some equally conspicuous species, as well 
as other dull-colored kinds, live under stones and in dark 
recesses. So that with these nudibranch mollusks, color 
apparent y does not stand in any close relation to the nature 
of the places which they inhabit. 

These naked sea-slugs are hermaphrodites, yet they pair 
together, as do land-snails, many of which have extremely 
pretty shells. It is conceivable that two hermaphrodites, 
attracted by each other’s greater beauty, might unite and 
Jeave offspring which would inherit their parents’ greater 
beauty. But with such lowly organized creatures this is 
extremely improbable. Nor is it at all obvious how the 
offspring from the more beautiful pairs of hermaphrodites 
would have any advantage over the offspring of the less 
beautiful, so as to increase in number, unless indeed vigor 
and beauty generally coincided. We have not here the case 
of a number of males becoming mature before the females, 
with the more beautiful males selected by the more vigor- 
ous females. If, indeed, brilliant colors were beneficial to 


41 have given (‘‘Geolog. Observations on Volcanic Islands,’’ 1844, p. 53) 
a curious instance of the influence of light on the colors of a frondescent 
incrustation, deposited by the surf, on the coast-rocks of Ascension, and formed 
by the solution of triturated sea-shells. 

5 Dr. Morse has lately discussed this subject in his paper on the Adaptive 
Coloration of Mollusca. ‘“Proceedings of the Boston Society of Natural His- 
tory,’’ vol. xiv., April, 1871. 


350 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


a hermaphrodite animal in relation to its general habits 
of life, the more brightly tinted individuals would succeed 
best and would increase in number; but this would be a 
case of natural and not of sexual selection. 


Sub-kingdom of the Vermes: Olass, Annelida (or Sea- 
worms).—In this class, although the sexes, when separate, 
sometimes differ from each other in characters of such im- 
portance that they have been placed under distinct genera 
or even families, yet the differences do not seem of the kind 
which can be safely attributed to sexual selection. These 
animals are often beautifully colored, but as the sexes do 
not differ in this respect, we are but little concerned with 
them. Even the Nemertians, though so lowly organized, 
‘‘vie in beauty and variety of coloring with any other 
group in the invertebrate series’’; yet Dr. McIntosh® cannot 
discover that these colors are of any service. The seden- 
tary annelids become duller-colored, according to M. Qua- 
trefages,’ after the period of reproduction; and this I pre- 
sume may be attributed to their less vigorous condition at 
that time. All these worm-like animals apparently stand 
too low in the scale for the individuals of either sex to 
exert any choice in selecting a partner, or for the individ- 
uals of the same sex to struggle together in rivalry. 


Sub-kingdom of the Anthropoda: Class, Crustacea.—In this 
great class we first meet with undoubted secondary sexual 
characters, often developed in a remarkable manner. Un- 
fortunately the habits of crustaceans are very imperfectly 
known, and we cannot explain the uses of many structures 
peculiar to one sex. With the lower parasitic species the 
males are of small size, and they alone are furnished with 
perfect swimming-legs, antenna, and sense-organs; the fe- 
males being destitute of these organs, with their bodies 


6 See his beautiful monograph on “‘British Annelids,”’ part i., 1873, p. 3. 
1 See M. Perrier, ‘‘l’Origine de l’Homme d’aprés Darwin,’’ ‘“‘Revue Scien- 
tifique,”’ Feb. 1873, p. 866. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 851 


often consisting of a mere distorted mass. But these ex- 
traordinary differences between the two sexes are, no doubt, 
related to their widely different habits of life, and, conse- 
quently, do not concern us. In various crustaceans, be- 
longing to distinct families, the anterior antennz are fur- 
nished with peculiar thread-like bodies, which are believed 
to act as smelling organs, and these are much more numer- 
ous in the males than in the females. As the males, without 
any unusual development of their olfactory organs, would 
almost certainly be able sooner or later to find the females, 
the increased number of the smelling-threads has probably 
been acquired through sexual selection, by the better pro- 
vided males having been the more successful in finding 
partners and in producing offspring. Fritz Muller has de- 
scribed a remarkable dimorphic species of Tanais, in which 
the male is represented by two distinct forms, which never 
graduate into each other. In the one form the male is fur- 
nished with more numerous smelling-threads, and in the 
other form with more powerful and more elongated chele, 
or pincers, which serve to hold the female. Fritz Muller 
suggests that these differences between the two male forms 
of the same species may have originated in certain individ- 
uals having varied in the number of the smelling-threads, 
while other individuals varied in the shape and size of their 
chele; so that of the former, those which were best able 
to find the female, and of the latter, those which were best 
able to hold her, have left the greatest number of progeny 
to inherit their respective advantages. ° 

In some of the lower crustaceans, the right anterior 
antenna of the male differs greatly in structure from the 
left, the latter resembling in its simple tapering joints the 
antenne of the female. In the male the modified antenna 
is either swollen in the middle or angularly bent, or con- 


8 ‘Pacts and Arguments for Darwin,’’ Eng. translat., 1869, p. 20. See the 
previous discussion on the olfactory threads. Sars has described a somewhat 
analogous case (as quoted in ‘‘Nature.’’ 1870, p. 455) in a Norwegian crusta- 
cean, the Pontoporeia affinis, 


852 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


verted (Fig. 4) into an elegant, and sometimes wonderfully 
complex, prehensile organ.’ It serves, as I hear from Sir 
J. Lubbock, to hold the female, and for this same purpose 
one of the two posterior legs (2) on the same side of the body 
is converted into a forceps. In an-. 
other family the inferior or posterior 
antenne are ‘‘curiously zigzagged’’ in 
the males alone. 

In the higher crustaceans the an- 
terior legs are developed into chele, 
or pincers; and these are generally 
larger in the male than in the female 
—so much so that the market value of 
the male edible crab (Cancer pagurus), 
according to Mr. C. Spence Bate, is 
five times as great as that of the 
female. In many species the chelz 
are of unequal size on the opposite 
sides of the body, the right-hand one 
being, as I am informed by Mr. Bate, 
generally, though not invariably, the 

pi ae larger. This inequality is also often 
Fig. 4.—Labidocera Dar- much greater in the male than in the 


winii (from Lubbock). a. 


Part of right anterior an- 
Fe ot calc toning, female. The two chele of the male 


eee ec thaacio lese ot Often differ in structure (Figs. 5, 6, 
male. ¢. Ditto of female. and 7), the smaller one resembling 
that of the female. What advantage is gained by their 
inequality in size on the opposite sides of the body, and by 
the inequality being much greater in the male than in the 
female; and why, when they are of equal size, both are 
‘often much larger in the male than in the female, is not 
known. AsI hear from Mr. Bate, the chele are sometimes 


of such length and size that they cannot possibly be used 


9 See Sir J. Lubbock, in ‘‘Annals and Mag. of Nat. Hist.,’’ vol. xi., 1853, 
pl. i. and x.; and vol. xii., 1853, pl. vii. See also Lubbock in ‘‘Transact, Ent. 
Soc.,’’ vol. iv., new series, 1856-1858, p. 8. With respect to the zigzagged 
antenne mentioned below, see Fritz Miller, ‘‘Facts and Arguments for Darwin,” 
1869, p. 40, footnote. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 853 


for carrying food to the mouth. In the males of certain 
fresh-water prawns (Palemon) the right leg is avtually 
longer than the whole body.” The great size of the one 
leg with its cheles may aid the male in fighting with his 
rivals; but this will not account for their inequality in the 


Rn 
' Fre. 5.—Anterior part of body of Calli: nassa (from Milne-Edwards), showing the 
unequal and differently constructed right- und left-hand chelz of the male, 


N.B.—The artist by mistake has reversed the drawing, and made the left-hand 
chela the largest. 


Fig. 6.—Second leg of male Orchestia, Bia. 7.—Ditto of female. 
Tucuratinga (from Fritz Muller), 


female on the opposite sides of the body. In Gelasimus, 
according to a statement quoted by Milne-Edwards,'' the 
male and the female live in the same burrow, and this 
shows that they pair; the male closes the mouth of the 


10 See a paper by Mr. C. Spence Bate, with figures, in ‘‘Proc. Zool. Soc.,” 
1868, p. 363; and on the nomenclature of the genus, ibid., p. 585. I am greatly 
indebted to Mr. Spence Bate for nearly all the above statements with respect 
to the chele of the higher crustaceans. 

ul “Hist, Nat. des Orust.,’’ tom. ii., 183%, p. 50. 


B54 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


burrow with one of its che x», which is enormously devel- 
oped; so that here it indirectly serves as a means of de- 
fence. Their main use, however, is probably to seize and 
to secure the female, and this in some instances, as with 
Gammarus, is known to be the case. The male of the her- 
mit or soldier crab (Pagurus) for weeks together carries 
about the shell inhabited by the female."* The sexes, how- 
ever, of the common shcre-crab (Curcinus menas), as Mr. 
Bate informs me, unite directly after the female has moulted 
her hard shell, when she is so soft that she would be injured 
if seized by the strong pinccrs of the male; but as she is 
caught and carried sbout by the male before moulting, she 
could then be seized. with impunity. 

Fritz Miiller sta‘es that certain species of Melita are dis- 
tinguished from all other amphipods by the females having 
‘the coxal lamellz of the penultimate pair of feet produced 
into hook-like processes, of which the males lay hold with 
the hands of the first pair.’’ The development of these 
hook-like processes has probably followed from those fe- 
males which were the most securely held during the act 
of reproduction having left the largest number of offspring. 
Another Brazilian amphipod (Orchestia Darwinii, Fig. 8) 
presents a case of dimorphism, like that of Tanais; for 
there are two,male forms, which differ in the structure of 
their chelz.’* As either chela would certainly suffice to 
hold the female—for both are now used for this purpose— 
the two male forms probably originated by some having 
varied in one manner, and some in another; both forms 
havin;: derived certain special, but nearly equal, advan- 
tages from their differently shaped organs. 

It is not known that male crustaceans fight together for 
the possession of the females, but it is probably the case; 
for with most animals, when the male is larger than the 
female, he seems to owe his greater size to his ancestors 


2 Mr. C. Spence Bate, Brit. Assoc., ‘‘Fourth Report on the Fauna of South 


Devon.”’ 
18 Fritz Muller, ‘‘Facts and Arguments for Darwin,’’ 1869, pp. 25-28. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 855 


having fought with other males during many generations. 
In most of the orders, especially in the highest or the 
Brachyura, the male is larger than the female; the parasitic 
genera, however, in which the sexes follow different habits 


¥iac. 8.—Orchestia Darwinii (from Fritz Muller), showing the differently constructed 
: chele of the two male forms. 


of life, and most of the Entomostraca must be excepted. 
The chelz of many crustaceans are weapons well adapted 
for fighting. Thus when a Devil-crab (Portunus puber) was 
seen by a son of Mr. Bate fighting with a Carcinus menas, 
the latter was soon thrown on its back, and had every limb 


856 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


torn from its body. When several males of a Brazilian 
Gelasimus, a species furnished with immense pincers, were 
placed together in a glass vessel by Fritz Miiller, they 
mutilated and killed one another. Mr. Bate put a large 
male Carcinus menas into a pan of water, inhabited by 
a female which was paired with a smaller male; but the 
latter was soon dispossessed. Mr. Bate adds, ‘‘If they 
fought, the victory was a bloodless one, for I saw no 
wounds.’’ This same naturalist separated a male sand- 
skipper (so common on our sea-shores), Gammarus marinus, 
from its female, both of whom were imprisoned in the same 
vessel with many individuals of the same species. The 
female, when thus divorced, soon joined the others. After 
a time the male was put again into the same vessel; and 
he then, after swimming about for a time, dashed into the 
crowd, and without any fighting at once took away his wife. 
This fact shows that in the Amphipoda, an order low in 
the scale, the males and females recognize each other, and 
are mutually attached. 

The mental powers of the Crustacea are probably higher 
than at first sight appears probable. Any one who tries to 
catch one of the shore-crabs, so common on tropical coasts, 
will perceive how wary and alert they are. There is a large 
crab (Birgus latro) found on coral islands which makes 
a thick bed of the picked fibres of the cocoanut, at the 
bottom of a deep burrow. It feeds on the fallen fruit of 
this tree by tearing off the husk, fibre by fibre; and it 
always begins at that end where the three eye-like depres- 
sions are situated. It then breaks through one of these eyes 
by hammering with its heavy front pincers, and, turning 
round, extracts the albuminous core with its narrow posterior 
pincers. But these actions are probably instinctive, so that 
they would be performed as well by a young animal as by 
an old one. The following case, however, can hardly be 
so considered: A trustworthy naturalist, Mr. Gardner," 


4 “Travels in the Interior of Brazil,’? 1846, p. 111. I have given, in my 
“Journal of Researches,’’ p. 463, an account of the habits of the Birgus. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 857 


while watching a shore-crab (Gelasimus) making its burrow, 
threw some shells toward the hole. One rolled in, and three 
other shells remained within a few inches of the mouth. 
In about five minutes the crab brought out the shell which 
had fallen in, and carried it away to the distance of a foot; 
it then saw the three other shells lying near, and, evidently 
thinking that they might likewise roll in, carried them to the 
spot where it had laid the first. It would, I think, be diffi- 
cult to distinguish this act from one performed by man by 
the aid of reason. 

Mr. Bate does not know of any well-marked case of 
difference of color in the two sexes of our British crusta- 
ceans, in which respect the sexes of the higher animals 
so often differ. In some cases, however, the males and 
females differ slightly in tint, but Mr. Bate thinks not more 
than may be accounted for by their different habits of life, 
such as by the male wandering more about, and being thus 
more exposed to the light. Dr. Power tried to distinguish 
by color the sexes of the several species which inhabit the 
Mauritius, but failed, except with one species of Squilla, 
probably S. stylifera, the male of which is described as being 
‘tof a beautiful bluish green,’’ with some of the appendages 
cherry-red, while the female is clouded with brown and 
gray, ‘‘with the red about her much less vivid than in the 
male.’’'* In this case we may suspect the agency of sexual 
selection. From M. Bert’s observations on Daphnia, when 
placed in a vessel illuminated by a prism, we have reason 
to believe that even the lowest crustaceans can distinguish 
colors. With Saphirina (an oceanic genus of Entomostraca), 
the males are furnished with minute shields or cell-like 
bodies, which exhibit beautiful changing colors; these are 
absent in the females, and in both sexes of one species.’* 
It would, however, be extremely rash to conclude that these 
curious organs serve to attract the females. I am informed 


18 Mr, Ch. Fraser, in ‘‘Proc, Zoolog. Soc.,’’ 1869, p. 3. I am indebted to 
Mr. Bate for Dr. Power’s statement. 
16 Claus, ‘‘Die freilebenden Copepoden,’’ 1863, s, 35. 


358 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


by Fritz Muller that in the female of a Brazilian species of 
Gelasimus the whole body is of a nearly uniform grayish 
brown. Inthe male the posterior part of the cephalo-thorax 
is pure white, with the anterior part of a rich green, shading 
into dark brown; and it is remarkable that these colors are 
liable to change in the course of a few minutes—the white 
becoming dirty gray or even black, the green ‘‘losing much 
of its brilliancy.’’ It deserves especial notice that the males 
do not acquire their bright colors until they become mature. 
They appear to be much more numerous than the females; 
they differ also in the larger size of their chele. In some 
species of the genus, probably in all, the sexes pair and 
inhabit the same burrow. They are also, as we have seen, 
highly intelligent animals. From these various considera- 
tions it seems probable that the male in this species has 
become gayly ornamented in order to attract or excite 
the female. 

It has just been stated that the male Gelasimus does not 
acquire his conspicuous colors until mature and nearly 
ready to breed. This seems a general rule in the whole 
class in respect to the many remarkable structural differ- 
ences between the sexes. We shall hereafter find the same 
law prevailing throughout the great sub-kingdom of the 
Vertebrata; and in all cases it is eminently distinctive 
of characters which have been acquired through sexual 
selection. Fritz Muller’ gives some striking instances of 
this law; thus the male sand-hopper (Orchestia) does not, 
until nearly full grown, acquire his large claspers, which 
are very differently constructed from those of the female; 
while young, his claspers resemble those of the female. 

Class, Arachnida (Spiders).—The sexes do not generally 
differ much in color, but the males are often darker than 
the females, as may be seen in Mr. Blackwall’s magnificent 
work.** In some species, however, the difference is con- 


1 “Facts and Arguments,”’ ete., p. 79. 
18 “(A History of the Spiders of Great Britain,” 1861-64, For the follow- 
ing facts, see pp. 77, 88, 102. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 859 


spicuous; thus the female of Sparassus smaragdulus is dull- 
ish green, while the adult male has the abdomen of a fine 
yellow, with three longitudinal stripes of rich red. In cer- 
tain species of Thomisus the sexes closely resemble each 
other, in others they differ much; and analogous cases occur 
in many other genera. It is often difficult to say which of 
the two sexes departs most from the ordinary coloration 
of the genus to which the species belong; but Mr. Blackwall 
thinks that, as a general rule, it is the male; and Canestrini”® 
remarks that in certain genera the males can be specifically 
distinguished with ease, but the females with great diffi- 
culty. I am informed by Mr. Blackwall that the sexes 
while young usually resemble each other; and both often 
undergo great changes in color during their successive 
moults, before arriving at maturity. In other cases the 
male alone appears to change color. Thus the male of 
the above bright-colored Sparassus at first resembles the 
female, and acquires his peculiar tints only when nearly 
adult. Spiders are possessed of acute senses, and exhibit 
much intelligence; as is well known, the females often show 
the strongest affection for their eggs, which they carry about 
enveloped in a silken web. The males search eagerly for 
the females, and have been seen by Canestrini and others to 
fight for possession of them. This same author says that 
the union of the two sexes has been observed in about 
twenty species; and he asserts positively that the female 
rejects some of the males who court her, threatens them 
with open mandibles, and at last, after long hesitation, 
accepts the chosen one. From these several considerations 
we may admit with some confidence that the well-marked 
differences in color between the sexes of certain species are 
the results of sexual selection; though we have not here the 
best kind of evidence—the display by the male of his orna- 
ments. From the extreme variability of color in the male 


1 This author has recently published a valuable essay on_the “‘Caratteri 
gessuali secondarii degli Arachnidi,’”’ in the ‘‘Atti della Soc. Veneto Trentina 
di Sc. Nat. Padova,’’ vol. i., Fasc. 3, 1873. . 


360 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


of some species, for instance of Theridion lineatum, it would 
appear that these sexual characters of the males have not. 
as yet become well fixed. Canestrini draws the same con- 
clusion from the fact that the males of certain species present 
two forms, differing from each other in the size and length 
of their jaws; and this reminds us of the above cases of 
dimorphic crustaceans. 

The male is generally much smaller than the female, 
sometimes to an extraordinary degree,* and he is forced 
to be extremely cautious in making his advances, as the 
female often carries her coyness to a dangerous pitch. 
De Geer saw a male that ‘tin the midst of his preparatory 
caresses was seized by the object of his attentions, enveloped 
by her in a web, and then devoured, a sight which, as he 
adds, filled bim with horror and indignation.’’?” The Rev. 
O. P. Cambridge” accounts in the following manner for 
the extreme smallness of the male in the genus Nephila: 
‘‘M. Vinson gives a graphic account of the agile way in 
which the diminutive male escapes from the ferocity of the 
female, by gliding about and playing hide-and-seek over 
her body and along her gigantic limbs; in such a pursuit 
it is evident that the chances of escape would be in favor 
of the smallest males, while the larger ones would fall early 
victims; thus gradually a diminutive race of males would 
be selected, until at last they would dwindle to the smallest 
possible size compatible with the exercise of their generative 
functions—in fact, probably to the size we now see them, 
t.e., so small as to be a sort of parasite upon the female, and 
either beneath her notice or too agile and too small for her 
to catch without great difficulty.” 

Westring has made the interesting discovery that the 


2 Aug. Vinson (‘‘Aranéides des Iles de la Réunion,” pl. vi. figs. 1 and 2) 
gives a good instance of the small size of the male in Zpetra nigra. In this 
species, as I may add, the male is testaceous and the female black, with legs 
banded with red. Other even more striking cases of inequality in size between 
the sexes have been recorded (‘‘Quarterly Journal of Science,”’ 1868, July, 
p. 429); but I have not seen the original accounts. 

"1 Kirby and Spence, ‘Introduction to Entomology,” vol, i, 1818, p. 280, 

3% “Proce, Zoolog. Soc.,’? 1871, p. 621. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 861 


males of several species of Theridion™ have the power of 
making a stridulating sound, while the females are mute. 
The apparatus consists of a serrated ridge at the base of the 
abdomen, against which the hard hinder part of the thorax 
is rubbed; and of this structure not a trace can be detected 
in the females. It deserves notice that several writers, 
including the well-known arachnologist, Walckenaer, have 
declared that spiders are attracted by music.* From the 
analogy of the Orthoptera and Homoptera, to be described 
in the next chapter, we may feel almost sure that the stridu- 
lation serves, as Westring also believes, to call or to excite 
the female; and this is the first case known to me in the 


ascending scale of the animal kingdom of sounds emitted 


for this purpose.”* 

Class, Myriapoda.—In neither of the two orders in this 
class, the millipeds and centipeds, can I find any well- 
marked instances of such sexual differences as more par- 
ticularly concern us. In Glomeris limbata, however, and 
perhaps in some few other species, the males differ slightly 
in color from the females; but this Glomeris is a highly 
variable species. In the males of the Diplopoda, the legs 
belonging either to one of the anterior or of the posterior 
segments of the body are modified into prehensile hooks 
which serve to secure the female. In some species of [ulus 
the tarsi of the male are furnished with membranous suckers 
for the same purpose. As we shall see when we treat of 
Insects, it isa much more unusual circumstance that it is the 
female in Lithobius which is furnished with prehensile ap- 
pendages at the extremity of her body for holding the male.” 


% Theridion (Asagena, Sund.) serratipes, 4-punctatum et gutiatum; see 
Westring, in. Kroyer, ‘“‘Naturhist. Tidskrift,’’ vol. iv., 1842-43, p. 349; and 
vol. ii., 1846-49, p. 342. See also, for other species, ‘‘Araneze Suecicz,”’ 

. 184. 
% Dr. H. H. van Zouteveen, in his Dutch translation of this work (vol. i. 
p. 444), has collected several cases. 

% Hilgendorf, however, has lately called attention to an analogous struc- 
ture in some of the higher crustaceans, which seems adapted to produce sound; 
see ‘Zoological Record,’? 1869, p. 603. 

% Walckenaer et P. Gervais, ‘‘Hist. Nat. des Insectes: Apteres,’’ tom. iv., 
1847, pp. 17, 19, 68. 

Descent—Vot. I.—16 


562 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


CHAPTER X 
SECONDARY SEXUAL CHARACTERS OF INSECTS 


Diversified structures possessed by the males for seizing the females— 
Differences between the sexes, of which the meaning is not under- 
stood—Difference in size between the sexes—Thysanura—Diptera— 
Hemiptera—Homoptera, musical powers possessed by the males 
alone—Orthoptera, musical instruments of the males, much diversi- 
fied in structure; pugnacity; colors—Neuroptera, sexual differences 
in color—Hymenoptera, pugnacity and colors—Coleoptera, colors; 
furnished with great horns, apparently as an ornament; battles; 
stridulating organs generally common to both sexes ; 


N THE immense class of insects the sexes sometimes 
differ in their locomotive-organs, and often in their 
sense-organs, as in the pectinated and beautifully plu- 

mose antennese of the males of many species. In Chloéon, 
one of the Ephemere, the male has great pillared eyes, of 
which the famale is entirely destitute.* The ocelli are 
absent in the females of certain insects, as in the Mutillide; 
and here the females are likewise wingless. But we are 
chiefly concerned with structures by which one male is , 
enabled to conquer another, either in battle or courtship, 
through his strength, pugnacity, ornaments, or music. The 
innumerable contrivances, therefore, by which the male 
is able to seize the female, may be briefly passed over. 
Besides the complex structures at the apex of the abdomen, 
which ought perhaps to be ranked as primary organs,? “‘it 
1 Sir J. Lubbock, ‘‘Transact. Linnean Soc.,”’ vol. xxv., 1866, p. 484. With. 


respect to the Mutillide see Westwood, ‘‘Modern Olass. of Insects,’’ vol. ii., 
. 213, 

: 2 These organs in the male often differ in closely allied species, and afford 
excellent specific characters. But their importance, from a functional point 
of view, as Mr. R. MacLachlan has remarked to me, has probably been over- 
rated. It has been suggested that slight differences in these organs would 
suffice to prevent the intercrossing of well-marked varieties or incipient species, 
and would thus aid in their development. That this can hardly be the case, 
we may infer from the many recorded cases (see, for instance, Bronn, 
“Geschichte der Natur,’ B. ii, 1843, s. 164; and Westwood, ‘‘Transact, 


SEXUAL SELECTION 363 


is astonishing,’’ as Mr. B. D. Walsh* has remarked, ‘‘how 
many different organs are worked in by nature for the 
seemingly insignificant object of enabling the male to grasp 
the female firmly.” The mandibles or jaws are sometimes 
used for this purpose; thus the male Corydalis cornutus 
(a neuropterous insect in some degree allied to the Dragon- 
flies, etc.) has immense curved jaws, many times longer 
than those of the female; and they are smooth instead of 
being toothos, so that he is thus enabled to seize her with- 
out injury.‘ One of the stag- -beetles of North America 
(Lucanus elaphus) uses his jaws, 
which are much larger than those 
of the female, for the same pur- 
pose, but probably likewise for 
fighting. In one of the sand- 
wasps (Ammophila) the jaws in 
the two sexes are closely alike, 
but are used for widely different 
purposes: the males, as Prof. 
Westwood observes, ‘‘are exceed- 
ingly ardent, seizing their partners 
round the neck with their sickle- 
shaped jaws;’’* while the females 
use their organs for burrowing 
in sand-banks and making their 
nests. : 

The tarsi of the front legs are ye, 9,—Crabro cribrarius, Upper 
dilated in many male beetles, or 781" mele: lower figure, female, 
are furnished with broad cushions of hairs; and in many 
genera of water-beetles they are armed with a round, flat 


Ent. Soc.,”’ vol. iii., 1842, p. 195) of distinct species having been observed in 
union. Mr, MacLachlan informs me (vide ‘‘Stett. Ent. Zeitung,’’ 1867, s, 155) 
that when several species of Phryganide, which present strongly pronounced 
differences of this kind, were confined together by Dr. Aug. Meyer, they 
emis, and one pair produced fertile ova, 
8 «The Practical Entomologist,’ Philadelphia, vol. ii., May, 1867, p. 88. 

4 Mr. Walsh, ibid., p. 107. 

58 **Modern Classification of Insects,’’ vol. ii., 1840, pp. 205, 206, Mr, 
Walsh, who called my attention to the double use of the jaws, says that 
he has repeatedly observed this fact. 


364 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


sucker, so that the male may adhere to the slippery body 
of the female. It is a much more unusual circumstance 
that the female of some water-beetles (Dytiscus) 
have their elytra deeply grooved, and in Acilius 
sulcatus thickly set with hairs, as an aid to the 
male. The females of some other water-beetles 
(Hydroporus) have their elytra punctured for 
the same purpose.® In the male of Crabro 
cribrarius (Fig. 9), it is the tibia which is- 
dilated into a broad horny plate, with minute 
membranous dots, giving to it a singular ap- 
pearance like that of a riddle.”_ In the male of 
Penthe (a genus of beetles) a few of the middle 
joints of the antenne are dilated and furnished 
on the inferior surface with cushions of hair, 
exactly like those on the tarsi of the Carabide, 
‘tand obviously for the same end.’’ In male 
dragon-flies, ‘‘the appendages at the tip of the 
tail are modified in an almost infinite variety 
of curious patterns to enable them to embrace 
the neck of the female.’’ Lastly, in the males 
of many insects, the legs are furnished with 
peculiar spines, knobs or spurs; or the whole 
leg is bowed or thickened; but this is by no 
means invariably a sexual character; or one 
, pair or all three pairs are elongated, sometimes 
Te. 10,— Taph- 


p 
roderes distortus to an extravagant length.° 
much enlarged). 


pper figure, T’he sexes of many species in all the orders 
male; lower figure, f a : : 
female. present differences of which the meaning is 


not understood. One curious case is that of a beetle (Fig. 10), 


®§ We have here a curious and inexplicable case of dimorphism, for some 
of the females of four European species of Dytiscus, and of certain species of 
Hydroporus, have their elytra smooth; and no intermediate gradations between 
the sulcated or punctured and the quite smooth elytra have been observed. 
See Dr. H. Schaum, as quoted in the ‘‘Zoologist,’’ vol. v.-vi., 1847-48, p. 1896, 
Also Kirby and Spence, ‘‘Introduction to Entomology,”’ vol. iii., 1826, p. 305, 

7 Westwood, ‘‘Modern Class.,’’ vol. ii. p. 193. The following statement 
about Penthe, and others in inverted commas, are taken from Mr. Walsh 
**Practical Entomologist,’’ Philadelphia, vol. ii. p. 88. 

® Kirby and Spence, ‘‘Introduct.,”’ ete., vol. iii, pp. 332-336, 


SEXUAL SELECTION 365 


the male of which has the left mandible much enlarged; so 
that the mouth is greatly distorted. In another Carabidous 
beetle, Eurygnathus,’ we have the case, unique as far as 
known to Mr. Wollaston, of the head of the female being 
much broader and larger, though in a variable degree, than 
that of the male. Any number of such cases could be 
given. They abound in the Lepidoptera: one of the most 
extraordinary is that certain male butterflies have their 
forelegs more or less atrophied, with the tibiz and tarsi 
reduced to mere rudimentary knobs. The wings, also, in 
the two sexes often differ in neuration,’? and sometimes 
considerably in outline, as in the Aricoris epitus, which 
was shown to me in the British Museum by Mr. A. Butler. 
The males of certain South American butterflies have tufts 
of hair on the margins of the wings, and horny excrescences 
on the disks of the posterior pair. In several British 
butterflies, as shown by Mr. Wonfor, the males alone are 
in parts clothed with peculiar scales. 

The use of the bright light of the female glow-worm 
has been subject to much discussion. The male is feebly 
luminous, as are the larve and even the eggs. It has been 
supposed by some authors that the light serves to frighten 
away enemies, and by others to guide the male to the 
female. At last, Mr. Belt’* appears to have solved the diffi- 
culty: he finds that all the Lampyride which he has tried 
are highly distasteful to insectivorous mammals and birds. 
Hence it is in accordance with Mr. Bates’s view, hereafter 
to be explained, that many insects mimic the Lampyrida 
closely, in order to be mistaken for them, and thus to escape 


® “Insecta Maderensia,”’ 1854, p. 20. 

10 E, Doubleday, ‘tAnnals and Mag. of Nat. Hist.,”’ vol. i, 1848, p. 379. 
I may add that the wings in certain Hymenoptera (see Shuckard, ‘‘Fossorial 
Hymenop.,’’ 1837, pp. 39-43) differ in neuration according to sex. 

11H, W, Bates, in ‘“‘Journal of Proce. Linn. Soc.,’’ vol. vi., 1862, p. 74. 
Mr, Wonfor’s observations are quoted in ‘‘Popular Science Review,”’ 1868, 

. B43, 

2 “The Naturalist in Nicaragua,’? 1874, pp. 316-320, On the phos- 
phorescence of the eggs, see ‘‘Annals and Mag. of Nat. Hist.,’” Nov. 1871, 
p. 372, 


366 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


destruction. He further believes that the luminous species 
profit by being at once recognized as unpalatable. It is 
probable that the same explanation may be extended to 
the Hlaters, both sexes of which are highly luminous. 
It is not known why the wings of the female glow-worm 
have not been developed; but in her present state she 
closely resembles a larva, and as larve are so largely 
preyed on by many animals, we can understand why she 
has been rendered so much more luminous and conspicuous 
than the male; and why the larve themselves are likewise 
luminous. 

Difference in Size Between the Sexes.—With insects of all 
kinds the males are commonly smaller than the females; 
and this difference can often be detected even in the larval 
state. So considerable is the difference between the maie 
and female cocoons of the silk-moth (Bombyx mori), thay in 
France they are separated by a particular mode of weigh- 
ing.** In the lower classes of the animal kingdom, the 
greater size of the females seems generally to depend on 
their developing an enormous number of ova; and this may 
to acertain extent hold good with insects. But Dr. Wal- 
lace has suggested a much more probable explanation. He 
finds, after carefully attending to the development of the 
caterpillars of Bombyx cynthia and yamamai, and especially 
to that of some dwarfed caterpillars reared from a second 
brood on unnatural food, ‘‘that in proportion as the indi- 
vidual moth is finer, so is the time required for its meta-_ 
morphosis longer; and for this reason the female, which is 
the larger and heavier insect, from having to carry her nu- 
merous eggs, will be preceded by the male, which is smaller 
and has less to mature.’’'* Now, as most insects are short- 
lived, and as they are exposed to many dangers, it would 
manifestly be advantageous to the female to be impregnated 
as soon as possible. This end would be gained by the males 
being first matured in large numbers ready for the advent 


13 Robinet, ‘“‘Vers a Soie,’’ 1848, p. 207. 
4 “Transact. Ent. Soc.,’’ 3d series, vol. vy. p. 486. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 367 


of the females; and this again would naturally follow, as 
Mr. A. R. Wallace has remarked," through natural selec- 
tion; for the smaller males would be first matured, and thus 
would procreate a large number of offspring which would 
inherit the reduced size of their male parents, while the 
larger males, from being matured later, would leave fewer 
offspring. 

There are, however, exceptions to the rule of male insects 
being smaller than the females, and some of these exceptions 
are intelligible. Size and strength would be an advantage 
to the males which fight for the possession of the females; 
and in these cases, as with the stag-beetle (Lucanus), the 
males are larger than the females. There are, however, 
other beetles which are not known to fight together, of 
which the males exceed the females in size, and the mean- 
ing of this fact is not known; but in some of these cases, 
as with the huge Dynastes and Megasoma, we can at least 
see that there would be no necessity for the males to be 
smaller than the females, in order to be matured before 
them, for these beetles are not short-lived, and there would 
be ample time for the pairing of the sexes. So again, male 
dragon-flies (Libellulidz) are sometimes sensibly larger, and 
never smaller, than the females; and, as Mr. MacLachlan 
believes, they do not generally pair with the females until 
a week or fortnight has elapsed, and until they have assumed 
their proper masculine colors. But the most curious case, 
showing on what complex and easily overlooked relations 
so trifling a character as difference in size between the 
sexes may depend, is that of the aculeate Hymenoptera; for 
Mr. F. Smith informs me that throughout nearly the whole 
of this large group, the males, in accordance with the gen- 
eral rule, are smaller than the females, and emerge about 
a week before them; but among the Bees, the males of Apis 
mellifica, Anthidium manicatum, and Anthophora acervorum, 


18 **Journal of Proc. Ent. Soc.,’’ Feb. 4, 1867, p. lxxi. ; 
16 For this and other statements on the size of the sexes, see Kirby and 
Spence, ibid., vol. iii. p. 300; on the duration of life in insects, see p. 344, 


868 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


and among the Fossores, the males of the Methoca ichneu- 
monides are larger than the females. The explanation of 
this anomaly is that a marriage flight is absolutely necessary 
with these species, and the male requires great strength and 
size in order to carry the female through the air. Increased 
size has here been acquired in opposition to the usual rela- 
tion between size and the period of development, for the 
males, though larger, emerge before the smaller females. 

We will now review the several Orders, selecting such 
facts as more particularly concern us. The Lepidoptera 
(Butterflies and Moths) will be retained for a separate 
chapter. 

Order, Thysanura.—The members of this lowly organized 
order are wingless, dull-colored, minute insects, with ugly, 
almost misshapen heads and bodies. Their sexes do not 
differ; but they are interesting as showing us that the males 
pay sedulous court to the females even low down in the 
animal scale. Sir J. Lubbock” says: ‘‘It is very amusing 
to see these little creatures (Smynthurus luteus) coquetting 
together. The male, which is much smaller than the female, 
runs round her, and they butt one another, standing face 
to face, and moving backward and forward like two playful 
lambs. Then the female pretends to run away and the male 
runs after her with a queer appearance of anger, gets in 
front and stands facing her again; then she turns coyly 
round, but he, quicker and more active, scuttles round too, 
and seems to whip her with his antennz; then for a bit they 
stand face to face, play with their antenne, and seem to be 
all in all to one another.”’ 

Order, Diptera (Flies).—The sexes differ little in color. 
The greatest difference, known to Mr. F. Walker, is in the 
genus Bibio, in which the males are blackish or quite black, 
and the females obscure brownish orange. The genus Ela- 
phomyia, discovered by Mr. Wallace’® in New Guinea, is 


29 


™ See “‘The Transactions of the Linnean Society,’’ volume xxvi., 1868, 


p. 296, 
18 “The Malay Archipelago,”’ vol. ii., 1869, p. 313. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 369 


highly remarkable, as the males are furnished with horns, 
of which the females are quite destitute. The horns spring 
from beneath the eyes, and curiously resemble those of a 
stag, being either branched or palmated. In one of the 
species they equal the whole body in length. They might 
be thought to be adapted for fighting, but as in one species 
they are of a beautiful pink color, edged with black, with 
a pale central stripe, and as these imsects have altogether a 
very elegant appearance, it is perhaps more probable that 
they serve as ornaments. That the males of some Diptera 
fight together is certain; for Prof. Westwood" has several 
times seen this with the Tipulz. ‘The males of other Diptera 
apparently try to win the females by their music: H. Miiller” 
watched for some time two males of an Hristalis courting a 
female; they hovered above her, and flew from side to side, 
making a high humming noise at the same time. Gnats and 
mosquitoes (Culicidz) also seem to attract each other by 
humming; and Prof. Mayer has recently ascertained that 
the hairs on the antenne of the male vibrate in unison with 
the notes of a tuning-fork, within the range of the sounds 
emitted by the female. The longer hairs vibrate sympa- 
thetically with the graver notes, and the shorter hairs with 
the higher ones. Landois also asserts that he has repeatedly 
drawn a whole swarm of gnats by uttering a particular note. 
It may be added that the mental faculties of the Diptera are 
probably higher than in most other insects, in accordance 
with their highly developed nervous system.” 

Order, Hemiptera (Field-Bugs).—Mr. J. W. Douglas, 
who has particularly attended to the British species, has 
kindly given me an account of their sexual differences. 
The males of some species are furnished with wings, while 


19 «‘Modern Classification of Insects,”’ vol. ii., 1840, p. 526. 

20 Anwendung, etc., ‘‘Verh. d. n. V. Jahrg.,”? xxix. p. 80. Mayer, in 
‘‘American Naturalist,’’ 1874, p. 236. : 

21 See Mr. B. T. Lowne’s interesting work, ‘‘On the Anatomy of the Blow- 
fly, Musca vomitora,’’ 1870, p. 14. He remarks (p. 33) that “‘the captured 
flies utter a peculiar plaintive note, and that this sound causes other flies 
to disappear, ”’ 


370 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


the females are wingless; the sexes differ in the form of 
their bodies, elytra, antenne, and tarsi; but, as the signifi- 
cation of these differences are unknown, they may be here 
passed over. The females are generally larger and more 
robust than the males. With British, and, as far as Mr. 
Douglas knows, with exotic species, the sexes do not com- 
monly differ much in color; but in about six British species 
the male is considerably darker than the female, and in 
about four other species the female is darker than the male. 
Both sexes of some species are beautifully colored; and 
as these insects emit an extremely nauseous odor, their 
conspicuous colors may serve as a signal that they are un- 
palatable to insectivorous animals. In some few cases their 
colors appear to be directly protective; thus Prof. Hoffmann 
informs me that he could hardly distinguish a small pink 
and green species from the buds on the trunks of lime-trees, 
which this insect frequents. 

Some species of Reduvide make a stridulating noise; 
and, in the case of Pirates stridulus, this is said™® to be 
effected by the movement of the neck within the pro- 
thoracic cavity. According to Westring, Reduvius person- 
atus also stridulates. But I have no reason to suppose that 
this is a sexual character, excepting that with non-social 
insects there seems to be no use for sound-producing organs, 
unless it be as a sexual call. 

Order, Homoptera.—Kvery one who has wandered in a 
tropical forest must have been astonished at the din made 
by the male Cicada. The females are mute; as the Grecian 
poet Xenarchus says, ‘‘Happy the Cicadas live, since they 
all have voiceless wives.’’ The noise thus made could 
be plainly heard on board the ‘‘Beagle,’’ when anchored at 
a quarter of a mile from the shore of Brazil; and Captain 
Hancock says it can be heard at the distance of a mile. 
The Greeks formerly kept, and the Chinese now keep, these 
insects in cages for the sake of their song, so that it must be 


2 Westwood, ‘‘Modern Class. of Insects,”’ vol. ii. p. 473. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 371 


pleasing to the ears of some men.” The Cicadidw usually 
sing during the day, while the Fulgoride appear to be 
night-songsters. ‘The sound, according to Landois,” is pro- 
duced by the vibration of the lips of the spiracles, which 
are set into motion by a current of air emitted from the 
trachez; but this view has lately been disputed. Dr. Powell 
appears to have proved” that it is produced by the vibration 
of a membrane, set into action bya special muscle. In the 
living insect, while stridulating, this membrane can be seen 
to vibrate; and in the dead insect the proper sound is heard, 
if the muscle, when a little dried and hardened, is pulled 
with the point of a pin. In the female the whole complex 
musical apparatus is present, but is much less developed 
than in the male, and is never used for producing sound. 
With respect to the object of the music, Dr. Hartman, 
in speaking of the Cicada septemdecim of the United States, 
says,** “‘the drums are now (June 6 and 7, 1851) heard in 
all directions. This I believe to be the marital summons 
from the males. Standing in thick chestnut sprouts about 
as high as my head, where hundreds were around me, I ob- 
served the females coming around the drumming males.” 
He adds: ‘‘This season (August, 1868) a dwarf pear-tree 
in my garden produced about fifty larvae of Cic. pruinosa; 
and I several times noticed the females to alight near a male 
while he was uttering his ‘clanging notes.’ Fritz Miller 
writes to me from South Brazil that he has often listened 
to a musical contest between two or three males of a species 
with a particularly loud voice, seated at a considerable dis- 
tance from each other: as soon as one had finished his song, 
another immediately began, and then another. As there is 
so much rivalry between the males, it is probable that the 
females not only find them by their sounds, but that, like 


28 These particulars are taken from Westwood’s ‘*Modern Class. of Insects,’? 
vol. ii,, 1840, p. 422. See also, on the Fulgoride, Kirby and Spence, ‘‘Intro- 
duct.,”’ vol. ii. p. 401. 

*% “Zeitschrift fir wissenschaft Zoolog.,’’ B. xvii:, 1867, 8. 152-158. 

2% “Pransact. New Zealand Institute,’’ vol. v., 1873, p. 286. 

%6 IT am indebted to Mr. Walsh for having sent me this extract from a 
“Journal of the Doings of Cicada septemdecim,”’ by Dr. Hartman, 


372 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


female birds, they are excited or allured by the male with 
the most attractive voice. 

I have not heard of any well-marked cases of orna- 
mental differences between the sexes of the Homoptera. 
Mr. Douglas informs me that there are three British species 
in which the male is black or marked with black bands, 
while the females are pale-colored or obscure. 

Order, Orthoptera (Crickets and Grasshoppers).—The 
males in the three saltatorial families in this Order are 
remarkable for their musical powers, namely, the Achetida, 
or crickets, the Locustide, for which there is no equivalent 
English name, and the Acridiide, or grasshoppers. The 
stridulation produced by some of the Locustide is so loud 
that it can be heard during the night at the distance of a 
mile,” and that made by certain species is not unmusical 
even to the human ear, so that the Indians on the Amazons 
keep them in wicker cages. All observers agree that the 
sounds serve either to call or excite the mute females. 
With respect to the migratory locusts of Russia, Korte 
has given’ an interesting case of selection by the female of 
amale. The males of this species (Pachytylus migratortus) 
while coupled with the female stridulate from anger or 
jealousy if approached by other males. The house-cricket 
when surprised at night uses its voice to warn its fellows.” 
In North America the Katydid (Platyphyllum concavum, 
one of the Locustide) is described* as mounting on the 
upper branches of a tree, and in the evening beginning ‘“‘his 
noisy babble, while rival notes issue from the neighboring 
trees, and the groves resound with the call of Katy-did-she- 
did the livelong night.’’ Mr. Bates, in speaking of the 
European field-cricket (one of the Achetide), says, ‘‘The 
male has been observed to place himself in the evening 


% Guilding, ‘‘Trans. Linn. Soc.,’’ vol. xv. p. 154. 

28 T state this on the authority of Képpen, “Ueber die Heuschrecken in 
Siidrussland,”’ 1866, p, 32, for I have in vain endeavored to procure Kérte’s 
work. 

29 Gilbert White, ‘‘Nat. Hist. of Selborne,’’ vol. ii., 1825, p. 262. 

20 Harris, ‘‘Insects of New England,’’ 1842, p. 128. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 373 


at the entrance of his burrow, and stridulate until a female 
approaches, when the louder 
notes are succeeded by a more 
subdued tone, while the suc- 
cessful musician caresses with 
his antenne the mate he has 
won.’’* Dr. Scudder was able 
to excite one of these insects 
to answer him, by rubbing on 
a file with a quill." In both 
sexes a remarkable auditory 
apparatus has been discovered 


. ‘ 1 Fia. 11.—Gryllus campestris (from Lan- 
by Von Siebold, situated in dois). Bisbehend aeure. under side of 


33 part of a wing nervure, much magnified, 
the front legs. showing the teeth, st. Left-hand figure, 


In the three Families the wpperfuriace, of wiog-cover, with thé 
sounds are differently pro- which the teeth (st) are scraped. 
duced. In the males of the Achetide both wing-covers 
have the same apparatus; and this in the field-cricket 
(Gryllus campestris, Fig. 11) consists, as de-  % 
scribed by Landois,™ of from 181 to 138 sharp, ae 
transverse ridges or teeth (st) on the under side = 
of one of the nervures of the wing-cover. This 2 
toothed nervure is rapidly scraped across a 2 
projecting, smooth, hard nervure (r) on the 
upper surface of the opposite wing. First one 
wing is rubbed over the other, and then the p19 Teeth of 
movement is reversed. Both wings are raised peru of Grylius 
a little at the same time, so as to increase the Landois). 
resonance. In some species the wing-covers of the males 
are furnished at the base with a talc-like plate.** I here 
give a drawing (Fig. 12) of the teeth on the under side 


31 “The Naturalist on the Amazons,’’ vol. i., 1863, p. 252. Mr. Bates gives 
a very interesting discussion on the gradations in the musical apparatus of the 
three families. See also Westwood, ‘‘Mod. Class.,’’ vol. ii. pp. 445, 453. 

32 *“Proe, Boston Soc. of Nat. Hist.,’’ vol. xi., April, 1868, 

33 *‘Nouveau Manuel d’Anat. Comp.,’? French translat., tom. i, 1850, 

. 567, 

# *% ‘Zeitschrift fir Wissenschaft. Zoolog.,’’ B. xvii., 1867, 3, 117. 

35 Westwood, ‘“‘Modern Class. of Insects,”’ vol. i. p. 440. 


874 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


of the nervure of another species of Gryllus, viz., G. domes- 
ticus. With respect to the formation of these teeth, Dr. 
Gruber has shown® that they have been developed by the 
aid of selection, from the minute scales and hairs with which 
the wings and body are covered, and I came to the same 
conclusion with respect to those of the Coleoptera. But 
Dr. Gruber further shows that their development is in part 


Fic. 13.—Chioroccelus Tanana (from Bates). a,6. Lobes of opposite wing-covers, 


directly due to the stimulus from the friction of one wing 
over the other. 

In the Locustide the opposite wing-covers differ from 
each other in structure (Fig. 13), and the action cannot, - 
as in the last family, be reversed. The left wing, which 
acts as the bow, lies over the right wing, which serves as 
the fiddle. One of the nervures (a) on the under surface 
of the former is finely serrated, and is scraped across the 


8 ‘Ueber den Tonapparat der Locustiden, ein Beitrag zum Darwinismus,”? _ 
“Zeitsch. fiir Wissensch. Zoolog.,’’ B. xxii., 1872, p. 100. 


2 


SEXUAL SELECTION 375 


prominent nervures on the upper surface of the opposite 
or right wing. In our British Phasgonura viridissima it 
appeared to me that the serrated nervure is rubbed against 
the rounded hind-corner of the opposite wing, the edge of 
which is thickened, colored brown, and very sharp. In the 
right wing, but not in the left, there is a little plate, as 
transparent as talc, surrounded by nervures, and called 
the speculum. In Ephippiger vitium, a member of this 
same family, we have a curious subordinate modification; 
for the wing-covers are greatly reduced in size, but ‘‘the 
posterior part of the pro-thorax is elevated into a kind 
of dome over the wing-covers, and which has probably 
the effect of increasing the sound.’’ * 

We thus see that the musical apparatus is more differen- 
tiated or specialized in the Locustide (which include, I 
believe, the most powerful eperformers in the Order), than 
in the Achetide, in which both wing-covers have the same 
structure and the same function.** Landois, however, de- 
tected in one of the Locustide, namely, in Decticus, a short 
and narrow row of small teeth, mere rudiments, on the 
inferior surface of the right wing-cover, which underlies 
the other and is never used as the bow. I observed the 
same rudimentary structure on the under side of the right 
wing-cover in Phasgonura viridissima. Hence we may infer 
with confidence that the Locustide are descended from 
a form in which, as in the existing Achetide, both wing- 
covers had serrated nervures on the under surface, and 
could be indifferently used as the bow; but that in the 
Locustide the two wing-covers gradually became differen- 
tiated and perfected, on the principle of the division of 
labor, the one to act exclusively as the bow, and the other 
as the fiddle. Dr. Gruber takes the same view, and has 
shown that rudimentary teeth are commonly found on the 
inferior surface of the right wing. By what steps the more 
siniple apparatus in the Achetide originated, we do not 


31 Westwood, ‘‘Modern Class, of Insects,”’ vol. i, p. 453. 
38 Landois, ‘‘Zeitsch. f. Wiss. Zoolog.,’’ B, xvii., 1867, a. 121, 122, 


376 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


know, but it is probable that the basal portions of the 
wing-covers originally overlapped each other as they do 
at present; and that the friction of the nervures produced 
a grating sound, as is now the case with the wing-covers 
of the females.” A grating sound thus occasionally and 
accidentally made by the males, if it served them ever so 
little as a love-call to the females, might readily have 
been intensified through sexual selection, by variations in 
the roughness of the nervures having been continually 
preserved. 

In the last and third Family, namely, the Acridiide, — 
or grasshoppers, the stridulation 
is produced in a very different 
manner, and, according to Dr. 
Scudder, is not so shrill as in the 
preceding Families. The inner 
surface of the femur (Fig. 14, r) 
is furnished with a longitudinal 
f/\ row of minute, elegant, lancet- 
.5)\ shaped, elastic teeth, from 85 to 
98 in number;** and these are 
; scraped across the sharp, pro- 
Fie. 14.—Hind-leg of Stenobothrus . ‘ F 
Se es eae ee een eerste: ine 
ridge, much magnified (from Landois), covers, which are thus made to 
vibrate and resound. Harris*’ says that when one of the 
males begins to play, he first ‘‘bends the shank of the hind- 
leg beneath the thigh, where it is lodged in a furrow de- 
signed to receive it, and then draws the leg briskly up and 
down. He does not play both fiddles together, but alter- 
nately, first upon one and then on the other.’’ In many 
species the base of the abdomen is hollowed out into a 
great cavity which is believed to act as a resounding board. 
In Pneumora (Fig. 15), a South African genus belonging 


39 Mr. Walsh also informs me that he has noticed that the female of the 
Platyphyllum concavum, ‘“‘when captured, makes a feeble grating noise by 
shuffling her wing-covers together. ’’ 

“0 Landois, ibid., s. 113. 

41 “Insects of New England,’’ 1842, p. 133. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 877 


to the same family, we meet with a new and remarkable 
modification; in the males a small notched ridge projects 
obliquely from each side of the abdomen, against which the 
hind femora are rubbed.“ As the male is furnished with 
wings (the female being wingless), it is remarkable that the 
thighs are not rubbed in the usual manner against the wing- 


Fie. 15.—Pneumora (from specimens in the British Museum), Upper figure, male; 
lower figure, female, 


covers; but this may perhaps be accounted for by the 
unusually small size of the hind-legs. I have not been able 
to examine the inner surface of the thighs, which, judging 
from analogy, would be finely serrated. The species of 
Pneumora have been more profoundly modified for the sake 
of stridulation than any other orthopterous insect; for in the 


42 Westwood, ‘‘Modern Classification,’’ vol. i. p. 465. 


878 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


male the whole body has been converted into a musical 
instrument, being distended with air, like a great pellucid 
bladder, so as to increase the resonance. Mr. Trimen in- 
forms me that at the Cape of Good Hope these insects make 
a wonderful noise during the night. 

In the three foregoing families the females are almost 
always destitute of an efficient musical apparatus. But 
there are a few exceptions to this rule, for Dr. Gruber 
has shown that both sexes of Hphippiger vitiwm are thus 
provided, though the organs differ in the male and fe- 
male to a certain extent. Hence we cannot suppose that 
they have been transferred from the male to the female, as 
appears to have been the case with the secondary sexual 
characters of many other animals. They must have been 
independently developed in the two sexes, which no doubt 
mutually call to each other during the season of love. 
In most other Locustide (but not according to Landois 
in Decticus) the females have rudiments of the stridulatory 
organs proper to the male, from whom it is probable that 
these have been transferred. Landois also found such rudi- 
ments on the under surface of the wing-covers of the female 
Achetidz, and on the femora of the female Acridiide. In 
the Homoptera, also, the femaies have the proper musical 
apparatus in a functionless state; aad we shall hereafter 
meet in other divisions of the animal kingdom with many 
instances of structures proper to the male being present in 
a rudimenta. y condition in the female. 

Landois has observed another important fact, namely, 
that in the females of the Acridiide, the stridulating teeth 
on the femora remain. throughout life in the same condition 
in which they first appear during the larval state in both 
sexes. In the males, on the other hand, they become fur- 
ther developed, and acquire their perfect structure at the 
jast moult, when the insect is mature and ready to breed. 

From the facts now given, we see that the means by 
which the males of the Orthoptera produce their sounds 
are extremly diversified, and are altogether different from 


SEXUAL SELECTION 379 


those employed by the Homoptera.** But throughout the 
animal kingdom we often find the same object gained by 
the most diversified means; this seems due to the whole 
organization having undergone multifarious changes in the 
course of ages, and as part after part varied different varia- 
tions were taken advantage of for the same general purpose. 
The diversity of means for producing sound in the three 
families of the Orthoptera and in the Homoptera impresses 
the mind with the high importance of ‘these structures to 
the males, for the sake of calling or alluring the females. 
We need feel no surprise at the amount of modification 
which the Orthoptera have undergone in this respect, as 
we now know, from Dr. Scudder’s remarkable discovery, 
that there has been more than ample time. This naturalist 
has lately found a fossil insect in the Devonian formation of 
New Brunswick, which is furnished with ‘‘the well-known 
_ tympanum or stridulating apparatus of the male Locustide.”’ 
The insect, though in most respects related to the Neurop- 
tera, appears, as is so often the case with very ancient forms, 
to connect the two related Orders of the Neuroptera and 
Orthoptera. 

I have but little more to say on the Orthoptera. Some 
of the species are very pugnacious. When two male field- 
crickets (@ryllus campestris) are confined together, they 
fight till one kills the other; and the species of Mantis are 
described as manoeuvring with their sword-like front limbs, 
like hussars with their sabres. The Chinese keep these 
insects in little bamboo cages, and match them like game- 
cocks.** With respect to color, some exotic locusts are 
beautifully ornamented; the posterior wings being marked 
with red, blue, and black; but as throughout the Order the 


43 Tandois has recently found in certain Orthoptera rudimentary structures 
closely similar to the sound-producing organs in the Homoptera; and this is 
a surprising fact. See “‘Zeitschr. fiir Wissensch. Zoolog.,’”’ B. xxii. Heft. 3, 
3871, p. 348. - 

“4 “Transact. Ent. Soc.,’? 3d series, vol. ii (‘Journal of Proceedings,’ 

. 117). 
ae Yr estwrood, **Modern Class. of Insects,’’ vol. i. p. 427; for crickets, p. 446 


880 ; THE DESCENT OF MAN 


sexes rarely differ much in color, it is not probable that 
they owe their bright tints to sexual selection. Conspicuous 
colors may be of use to these insects, by giving notice that 
they are unpalatable. Thus it has been observed* that a 
bright-colored Indian locust was invariably rejected when 
offered to birds and lizards. Some cases, however, are 
known of sexual differences of color in this Order. The 
male of an American cricket*’ is described as being as white 
as ivory, while the female varies from almost white to 
greenish-yellow or dusky. Mr. Walsh informs me that the 
adult male of Spectrum femoratum (one of the Phasmide) 

“is of a shining brownish-yellow color; the adult female 
being of a dull, opaque, cinereous brown; the young of both 
sexes being green.’’ Lastly, I may mention that the male 
of one curious kind of cricket*® is furnished with ‘‘a long 
membranous appendage, which falls over the face like a 
veil’; but what its use may be is not known. 

Order, Newroptera.—Little need here be said, except as 
to color. In the Ephemeride the sexes often differ slightly 
in their obscure tints; but it is not probable that the males 
are thus rendered attractive to the females. The Libellu- 
lid, or dragon-flies, are ornamented with splendid green, 
blue, yellow, and vermilion metallic tints; and the sexes 
often differ. Thus, as Prof. Westwood remarks,” the males 
of some of the Agrionide ‘‘are of a rich blue with black 
wings, while the females are fine green with colorless 
wings.”’ But in Agrion Ramburit these colors are exactly 
reversed in the two sexes." In the extensive North Ameri- 
cans genus of Hetzrina, the males alone have a beautiful 


46 Mr. Ch. Horne, in ‘‘Proc. Ent. Soc.,’’ May 3, 1869, p. xii. 

“ The Geanthus nivalis. Harris, ‘Insects of New England,’’ 1842, p. 124. 
The two sexes of @. pellucidus of Europe differ, as I hear from Victor Carus, 
in nearly the same manner. 

4 Platyblemnus: Westwood, ‘‘Modern Clas.,’’ vol. i. p. 447. 

#2 B. D. Walsh, the ‘‘Pseudo-neuroptera of Ilinois,”’ in ‘Proc. Ent. Soc., 
of Philadelphia,’ 1862, p. 362. 

50 **Modern Class.,”’ vol. ii. p. 37. 

51 Walsh, ibid., p. 382. I a indebted to this naturalist for the following 
facts on Hetzerina, Anax, and Gomphus. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 381 


carmine spot at the base of each wing. In Anaz junius the 
basal part of the abdomen in the male is a vivid ultramarine 
blue, and in the female grass-green. In the allied genus 
Gomphus, on the other hand, and in some other genera, the 
sexes differ but little in color. In closely allied forms 
throughout the animal kingdom, similar cases of the sexes 
differing greatly, or very little, or not at all, are of frequent 
occurrence. Although there is so wide a difference in color 
between the sexes of many Libellulide, it is often difficult 
to say which is the more brilliant; and the ordinary colora- 
tion of the two sexes is reversed, as we have just seen, in 
one species of Agrion. It is not probable that their colors 
in any case have been gained as a protection. Mr. Mac- 
Lachlan, who has closely attended to this family, writes 
to me that dragon-flies—the tyrants of the insect-world— 
are the least liable of any insect to be attacked by birds 
or other * enemies, and he believes that their bright colors 
serve as a sexual attraction. Certain dragon-flies apparently 
are attracted by particular colors. Mr. Patterson observed™ 
that the Agrionids, of which the males are blue, settled 
in numbers on the blue float of a fishing line, while two 
other species were attracted by shining white colors. 

It is an interesting fact, first noticed by Schelver, that, 
in several genera belonging to two sub-families, the males 
on first emergence from the pupal state are colored exactly 
like the females; but that their bodies in a short time 
assume a conspicuous milky-blue tint, owing to the exuda- 
tion of a kind of oil, soluble in ether and alcohol. Mr. Mac- 
Lachlan believes that in the male of Lzbellula depressa this 
change of color does not occur until nearly a fortnight after 
the metamorphosis, when the sexes are ready to pair. 

Certain species of Neurothemis present, according to 
Brauer,” a curious case of dimorphism, some of the females 
having ordinary wings, while others have them very 
richly netted, as in the males of the same species.’ 


52 «Trans, Ent. Soc.,’? vol. i., 1836, p. Ixxxi. 
8 See abstract in the “Zoological Record’? for 1867, p. 450. 


882 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


Brauer ‘‘explains the phenomenon on Darwinian principles 
oy the supposition that the close netting of the veins is 
a secondary sexual character in the males, which has been 
abruptly transferred to some of the females instead of, as 
generally occurs, to all of them.’’ Mr. MacLachlan informs 
me of another instance of dimorphism in several species 
of Agrion, in which some individuals are of an orange 
color, and these are invariably females. This is probably 
a case of reversion; for in the true Libellule, when the 
sexes differ in color, the females are orange or yellow; 
so that, supposing Agrion to be descended from some 
primordial form which resembled the typical Libellule in 
its sexual characters, it would not be surprising that 
a tendency to vary in this manner should occur in the 
females alone. 

Although many dragon-flies are large, powerful, and 
fierce insects, the males have not been observed by Mr. 
MacLachlan to fight together, excepting, as he believes, in 
some of the smaller species of Agrion. In another group 
in this Order, namely, the Termites, or white ants, both 
sexes at the time of swarming may be seen running about, 
‘the male after the female, sometimes two chasing one fe- 
male, and contending with great eagerness who shall win the 
prize.’ The Atropos pulsatorius is said to make a noise 
with its jaws, which is answered by other individuals.* 

Order, Hymenoptera.—That inimitable observer, M. 
Fabre,” in describing the habits of Cerceris, a wasp-like 
insect, remarks that ‘‘fights frequently ensue between the 
males for the possession of some particular female, who 
sits an apparently unconcerned beholder of the struggle 
for supremacy, and, when the victory is decided, quietly 
flies away in company with the conqueror.’’ Westwood” 


' Kirby and Spence, ‘‘Introduct. to Entomology,” oi ii., 1828, p. 35. 

55 Houzeau, ‘‘Les Facultés Mentales,’’ ete., tom. i. . 104. 

56 See an interesting article, ‘‘The Writings of Fabre,’ ° in ‘Nat. Hist. 
Review,’’ April, 1862, p. 122. 

™ See the “Journal of the Proceedings of the Entomological Society’ for 
September 7, 1863, p. 169. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 383 


says that the males of one of the saw-flies (Tenthredinz) 
“have been found fighting together, with their mandibles 
locked.’”” As M. Fabre speaks of the males of Cerceris 
striving to obtain a particular female, it may be well to 
bear in mind that insects belonging to this Order have 
the power of recognizing each other after long intervals of 
time, and are deeply attached. For instance, Pierre Huber, 
whose accuracy no one doubts, separated some ants, and 
when, after.an interval of four months, they met others 
which had formerly belonged to the same community, they 
recognized and caressed one another with their antenna. 
Had they been strangers they would have fought together. 
Again, when two communities engage in a battle, the ants 
on the same side sometimes attack each other in the general 
confusion, but they soon perceive their mistake, and the 
one ant soothes the other. 

In this Order slight differences in color, according to sex, 
are common, but conspicuous differences are rare except in 
the family of Bees; yet both sexes of certain groups are 
so brilliantly colored—for instance in Chrysis, in which 
vermilion and metallic greens prevail—that we are tempted 
to attribute the result to sexual selection. In the Ichneu- 
monide, according to Mr. Walsh, the males are almost 
universally lighter-colored than the females. On the other 
hand, in the Tenthredinide the males are generally darker 
than the females. In the Siricide the sexes frequently 
differ; thus the male of Sirex juvencus is banded with 
orange, while the female is dark purple; but it is difficult 
to say which sex is the more ornamented. In Tremex 
columbe the female is much brighter-colored than the 
male. I am informed by Mr. F. Smith that the male ants 
of several species are black, the females being testaceous. 

In the family of Bees, especially in the solitary species, 
as I hear from the same entomologist, the sexes often differ 
in color. The males are generally the brighter, and, in 


58 DP Huber, ‘Recherches sur les Moeurs des Fourmis,’’ 1810, pp. 150, 166, 
69 “Prog, Zatomolog. Soc. of Philadelphia,’? 1866, pp. 238-239. 


384 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


Bombus as well as in Apathus, much more variable in color 
than the females. In Anthrophora retusa the male is of a rich 
fulvous-brown, while the female is quite black; so are the 
females of several species of Xylocopa, the males being 
bright yellow. On the other hand, the females of some 
species, as of Andrena fulva, are much brighter-colored 
than the males. Such differences in color can hardly be 
accounted for by the males being defenceless and thus 
requiring protection, while the females are well defended 
by their stings. H. Miiller,° who has particularly attended 
to the habits of bees, attributes these differences in color in 
chief part to sexual selection. That bees have a keen per- 
ception of color is certain. He says that the males search 
eagerly and fight for the possession of the females; and he 
accounts through such contests for the mandibles of the 
males being in certain species larger than those of the fe- 
males. In some cases the males are far more numerous than 
the females, either early in the season, or at all times and 
places, or locally; whereas the females in other cases are 
apparently in excess. In some species the more beautiful 
males appear to have been selected by the females, and in 
others the more beautiful females by the males. Conse- 
quently, in certain genera (Miiller, p. 42), the males of the 
several species differ much in appearance, while the females 
are almost indistinguishable; in other genera the reverse 
occurs. H. Miiller believes (p. 82) that the colors gained 
by one sex through sexual selection have often been trans- 
ferred in a variable degree to the other sex, just as the 
pollen-collecting apparatus of the females has often been 
transferred to the male, to whom it is absolutely useless." 


6. “Anwendung der Darwin’schen Lehre auf Bienen,’ ‘Verh. p. n. Jahrg.,”” 
xxix. 
61 M. Perrier in his article, ‘‘La Sélection sexuelle d’aprés Darwin’’ (‘‘Revue 
Scientifique,’’ Feb. 1873, p. 868), without apparently having reflected much on 
the subject, objects that as the males of social bees are known to be produced 
from unfertilized ova, they could not transmit new characters to their male off- 
spring. This is an extraordinary objection. A female bee fertilized by a male, 
which presented some character facilitating the union of the sexes, or renderad 
him more attractive to the female, would lay eggs which would produce only 


SEXUAL SELECTION 385 


Mutilla Furopea makes a stridulating noise; and accord- 
ing to Goureau both sexes have this power. He attributes 
the sound to the friction of the third and preceding abdomi- 
nal segments, and I find that these surfaces are marked with 
very fine concentric ridges; but so is the projecting thoracic 
collar, into which the head articulates, and this collar, when 
scratched with the point of a needle, emits the proper sound. 
It is rather surprising that both sexes should have the power 
of stridulating, as the male is winged and the female wing- 
less. It is notorious that Bees express certain emotions, as 
of anger, by the tone of their humming; and, according to 
H. Miiller (p. 80), the males of some species make a peculiar 
singing noise while pursuing the females. 

Order, Coleoptera (Beetles).—Many beetles are colored so 
as to resemble the surfaces which they habitually frequent, 
and they thus escape detection by their enemies. Other 
species, for instance, diamond-beetles, are ornamented with 
splendid colors, which are often arranged in stripes, spots, 
crosses, and other elegant patterns. Such colors can hardly 
serve directly as a protection, except in the case of certain 
‘flower-feeding species; but they may serve as a warning or 
mears of recognition, on the same principle as the phos- 
phorescence of the glow-worm. As with beetles the colors 
of the two sexes are generally alike, we have no evidence 
that they have been gained through sexual selection; but 
this is at least possible, for they may have been developed 
in one sex and then transferred to the other; and this view 
is even in some degree probable in those groups which pos- 
sess other well-marked secondary sexual characters. Blind 


females; but these young females would next year produce males; and will it 
be pretended that such males would not inherit the characters of their male 
grandfathers? To take a case with ordinary animals as nearly parallel as pos- 
sible; if a female of any white quadruped or bird were crossed by a male of a 
black breed, and the male and female offspring were paired together, will it 
be pretended that.the grandchildren would not inherit a tendency to blackness 
from their male grandfather? The acquirement of new characters by the sterile 
worker-bees is a much more difficult case, but I have endeavored to show in 
my ‘‘Origin of Species’? how these sterile beings are subjected to the power 
of natural selection. Bs 
62 Quoted by Westwood, ‘‘Modern Class. of Insects,’’ vol. ii, p. 214. 


Descent—Vou I.—17, 


386 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


beetles, which cannot, of course, behold each other’s beauty, 
never, as I hear from Mr. Waterhouse, Jr., exhibit bright 
colors, though they often have polished coats; but the 
explanation of their obscurity may be that they generally 
inhabit caves and other obscure stations. 

Some Longicorns, especially certain Prionide, offer an 
exception to the rule that the sexes of beetles do not differ 


ia. 16.—Chalcosoma atlas. Upper figure, male (reduced); lower figure, 
female (natural size), Y 


in color. Most of these insects are large and splendidly 
colored. The males in the genus Pyrodes,* which I saw 
in Mr. Bates’s collection, are generally redder but rather 


88 Pyrodes pulcherrimus, in which the sexes differ conspicuously, has been 
described by Mr. Bates in ‘‘Transact. Ent. Soc.,’? 1869, p. 50. I will specify 
the few other cases in which I have heard of a difference in color between the 
sexes of beetles, Kirby and Spence (‘‘Introduct. to Entomology,”’’ vol. iii. p. 
301) mention a Cantharis, Meloe, Rhagium, and the Leptura testacea; the male 
of the latter being testaceous, with a black thorax, and the female of a dull red 
all over. These two latter beetles belong to the family of Longicorns, Messrs, 
R. Trimen and Waterhouse, Jr., inform me of two Lamellicorns, viz., a Peri- 
trichia and Trichius, the male of the latter being more obscurely colored than 
the female. In Tillus elongatus the male is black, and the female always, as it 
is believed, of a dark-blue color, with a red thorax. The male, also, of Orso- 
dacna atra, a8 I hear from Mr. Walsh, is black, the female (the so-called 
O. rujicollis) having a rufous thorax. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 387 


duller than the females, the latter being colored of a more 
or less splendid golden green. On the other hand, in one 
species the male is golden green, the female being richly 
tinted with red and purple. In the genus Esmeralda the 
sexes differ so greatly in color that they have been ranked 
as distinct species; in one species both are of a beautiful 
shining green, but the male has a red thorax. On the 
whole, as far as I could judge, the females of those Prio- 
nidee in which the sexes differ are colored more richly than 
the males, and this does not accord with the common rule 
in regard to color, when acquired through sexual selection. 

A most remarkable distinction between the sexes of 
many beetles is presented by the great horns which rise 
from the head, thorax, and clypeus of the males; and in 
some few cases from the under surface of the body. These 
horns, in the great family of the Lamellicorns, resemble 
those of various quadrupeds, such as stags, rhinoceroses, 
etc., and are wonderful both from their size and diversified 
shapes. Instead of describing them, I have given figures 
of the males and females of some of the more remarkable 
forms (Figs. 16 to 20). The females generally exhibit 
rudiments of the horns in the form of small knobs or 
ridges; but some are destitute of even the slightest rudi- 
ment. On the other hand, the horns are nearly as well 
developed in the female as in the male of Phaneus lancifer, 
and only a little less well developed in the females of some 
other species of this genus and of Copris. I am informed 
by Mr. Bates that the horns do not differ in any manner 
corresponding with the more important characteristic differ- 
ences between the several subdivisions of the family; thus 
within the same section of the genus Onthophagus there 
are species which have a single horn, and others which 
have two. 

In almost all cases the horns are remarkable from their 
excessive variability; so that a graduated series can be 
formed from the most highly developed males to others 
so degenerate that they can barely be distinguished from 


588 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


Fia. 20.—Onthophagus rangifer (enlarged), 


SEXUAL SELECTION 389 


the females. Mr. Walsh" found that in Phaneus carnifex 
the horns were thrice as long in some males as in others. 
Mr. Bates, after examining above a hundred males of 
Onthophagus rangifer (Fig. 20), thought that he had at last 
discovered a species in which the horns did not vary; but 
further research proved the contrary. 

The extraordinary size of the horns, and their widely 
different structure in closely allied forms, indicate that they 
have been formed for some purpose; but their excessive 
variability in the males of the same species leads to the 
inference that this purpose cannot be of a definite nature. 
The horns do not show marks of friction, as if used for any 
ordinary work. Some authors suppose™ that, as the males 
wander about much more than the females, they require 
horns as a defence against their enemies; but as the horns 
are often blunt, they do not seem well adapted for defence. 
The most obvious conjecture is that they are used by the 
males for fighting together; but the males have never been 
observed to fight; nor could Mr. Bates, after a careful 
examination of numerous species, find any sufficient evi- 
dence, in their mutilated or broken condition, of their 
having been thus used. If the males had been habitual 
fighters the size of their bodies would probably have been 
increased through sexual selection, so as to have exceeded 
that of the females; but Mr. Bates, after comparing the two 
sexes in above a hundred species of the Copride, did not 
find any marked difference in this respect among well- 
developed individuals. In Lethrus, moreover, a beetle 
belonging to the same great division of the Lamellicorns, 
the males are known to fight, but are not provided, with 
horns, though their mandibles are much larger than ‘those 
of the female. 

The conclusion that the horns have been acquired as 
ornaments is that which best agrees with the fact of their 
having been so immensely, yet not fixedly, developed—as 


‘Prog, Entomolog. Soc. of Philadelphia,’’ 1864, p. 228. 
65 Kirby and Spence, ‘‘Introduct. Entomolog.,’’ vol. iii, p. 300. 


590 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


shown by their extreme variability in the same species, 
and by their extreme diversity in closely allied species. 
This view will at first appear extremely improbable; but 
we shall hereafter find with many animals standing much 
higher in the scale, namely, fishes, amphibians, reptiles, 
and birds, that various kinds of crests, knobs, horns, and 
combs have been developed apparently for 
this sole purpose. 
The males of Onitis furcifer (Fig. 21) 
and of some other species of the genus are 
furnished with singular projections on their 
anterior femora, and with a great fork or 
pair of horns on the lower surface of the 
, thorax. Judging from other insects, these 
Pes fare: may aid the male in clinging to the female. 
from beneath. Although the males have not even a trace 
of a horn on the upper surface of the body, yet the females 
plainly exhibit a rudiment of a single horn on the head 
(Fig. 22, a), and of a crest (0) on the thorax. That the 
slight thoracic crest in the female is a rudiment of a projee 


Fig. 22.—Left-hand figure, male of Onitis furcifer, viewed laterally. Right-hand 
figure, female. a. Rudiment of cephalic horn. 06. Trace of thoracic horn or crest. 


tion proper to the male, though entirely absent in the male 
of this particular species, is clear; for the female of Bubas 
bison (a genus which comes next to Onitis) has a similar 
slight crest on the thorax, and the male bears a great pro- 
jection in the same situation. So, again, there can hardly 
be a doubt that the little point (a) on the head of the female 
Onitis furcifer, as well as onthe head of the females of two 
or three allied species, is a rudimentary representative of the 


SEXUAL SELECTION 391 


cephalic horn, which is common to the males of go many 
Lamellicorn beetles, as in Phanwus (Fig. 18). 

The old belief that rudiments have been created to com- 
plete the scheme of nature is here so far from holding good, 
that we have a complete inversion of the ordinary state of 
things in the family. We may reasonably suspect that 
the males originally bore horns and transferred them to the 
females in a rudimentary condition, as in so many other 
Lamellicorns. Why the males subsequently lost their 
horns, we know not; but this may have been caused 
through the principle of compensation, owing to the de- 
velopment of the large horns and projections on the lower 
surface; and as these are confined to the males, the rudi- 
ments of the upper horns on the females would not have 
been thus obliterated. 

The cases hitherto given refer to the Lamellicorns, but 


an é 
Fia. 8.—Bledius taurus (magnified), Left-hand figure, male; right-hand figure, female, 
the males of some few other beetles, belonging to two 
widely distinct groups, namely, the Curculionide and 
Staphylinid, are furnished with horns—in the former on 
the lower surface of the body,” in the latter on the upper 
surface of the head and thorax. In the Staphylinide the 
horns of the males are extraordinarily variable in the same 
species, Just as we have seen with the Lamellicorns. In 
Siagonium we have a case of dimorphism, for the males 
can be divided into two sets, differing greatly in the size 
of their bodies and in the development of their horns, 
without intermediate gradations. In a species of Bledius 
(Fig. 23), also belonging to the Staphylinide, Prof. West- 
wood ‘states that ‘‘male specimens can be found in the 
same locality in which the central horn of the thorax is very 


8 Kirby and Spence, ‘‘Introduct, Entomolog.,’’ vol. iii. p. 329, 


8392 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


large, but the horns of the head quite rudimental; and 
others, in which the thoracic horn is much shorter, while 
the protuberances on the head are long.’’*’ Here we ap- 
parently have a case of compensation, which throws light 
on that just given of the supposed loss of the upper horns 
by the males of Onitis. 

Law of Battle—Some male beetles, which seem ill-fitted 
for fighting, nevertheless engage in conflicts for the pos- 
session of the females. Mr. Wallace® saw two males of 
Leptorhynchus angustatus, a linear beetle with a much elon- 
gated rostrum, ‘‘fighting for a female, who stood close by 
busy at her boring. They pushed at each other with their 
rostra, and clawed and thumped, apparently in the greatest 
rage.’’ The smaller male, however, ‘‘soon ran away, ac- 
knowledging himself vanquished.’’ In some few cases male 
beetles are well adapted for fighting, by possessing great 
toothed mandibles, much larger than those of the females. 
This is the case with the common stag-beetle (Lucanus 
cervus), the males of which emerge from the pupal state 
about a week before the other sex, so that several may 
often be seen pursuing the same female. At this season 
they engage in fierce conflicts. When Mr. A. H. Davis™ 
inclosed two males with one female in a box, the larger 
male severely pinched the smaller one, until he resigned 
his pretensions. A friend informs me that when a boy 
he often put the males together to see them fight, and he 
noticed that they were much bolder and fiercer than the 
females, as with the higher animals. The males would 
seize hold of his finger, if held in front of them, but not 
so the females, although they have stronger jaws. The 
males of many of the Lucanide, as well as of the above- 


61 ‘*Modern Classification of Insects,’’ vol. i. p. 172: Siagonium, p. 172. 
in the British Museum I noticed one male specimen of Siagonium in an inter- 
mediate condition, so that the dimorphism is not st ‘ict. 

68 “The Malay Archipelago,’’ vol. ii., 1869, p. 276. Riley, Sixth ‘‘Report 
on Insects of Missouri,’’ 1874, p. 115. 

6@ ““Fntomological Magazine,”’ vol. i., 1833, p. 82. See also, on the conflicts 
of this species, Kirby and Spence, ibid., vol. iii, p. 314; and Westwood, ibid., 
vol. i. p. 18%. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 893 


mentioned Leptorhynchus, are larger and more powerful 
insects than the females. The two sexes of Lethrus cepha- 
lotes (one of the Lamellicorns) inhabit the same burrow, and 
the male has larger mandibles than the female. If, during 
the breeding-season, a strange male attempts to enter the 
burrow, he is attacked; the female does not remain passive, 
but closes the mouth of the burrow, and encourages her 
mate by continually pushing him on from behind; and the 
battle lasts until the aggressor is killed or runs away.” 
The two sexes of another Lamellicorn beetle, the Ateuchus 
cicatricosus, live in pairs, and seem much attached to each 
other; the male excites the female to roll the balls of dung 
in which the ova are deposited; and if she is removed, he 
becomes much agitated. If the male is removed the female 
ceases all work, and, as M. Brulerie”’ believes, would remain 
on the same spot until she died. 

The great mandibles of the male Luanide are extremely 
variable both in size and structure, and in this respect re- 
semble the horns on the head and thorax of many male 
Lamellicorns and Staphylinide. A perfect series can be 
formed from the best-provided to the worst-provided or 
degenerate males. Although the mandibles of the common 
stag-beetle, and probably of many other species, are used 
as efficient weapons for fighting, it is doubtful whether their 
great size can thus be accounted for. We have seen that 
they are used by the Lucanus elaphus of North America for 
seizing the female. As they are so conspicuous and so 
elegantly branched, and as owing to their great length they 
are not well adapted for pinching, the suspicion has crossed 
my mind that they may in addition serve as an ornament, 
like the horns on the head and thorax of the various species 
above described. The male Chiasognathus grantit of South 
Chile—a splendid beetle belonging to the same family— 
has enormously developed mandibles (Fig. 24); he is bold 


Quoted from Fischer, in ‘‘Dict. Class. d’Hist. Nat.,’’ tom. x. p. 324. 
1 “Ann, Soc, Entomolog. France,’’ 1866, as quoted in ‘‘Journal of Travel,” 
by A. Murray, 1868, p. 135. 


394 THE DESCENT OF MAN . 


and pugnacious; when threatened he faces round, opens his 
great jaws, and at the same time stridulates loudly. But 


Fie. 24.—Chisognathus gran- 
tii (reduced). Upper figure, 
male; lower figure, female. 


the mandibles were not strong enough 
to pinch my finger so as to cause 
actual pain. , 

Sexual selection, which implies the 
possession of considerable perceptive 
powers and of strong passions, seems 
to have been more effective with the 
Lamellicorns than with any other 
family of beetles. With some species 
the males are provided with weapons 
for fighting; some live in pairs and 
show mutual affection; many have the 
power of stridulating when excited; 
many are furnished with the most ex- 
traordinary horns, apparently for the 
sake of ornament; and some, which 
are diurnal in their habits, are gor- 
geously colored. Lastly, several of . 
the largest beetles in the world belong 
to this family, which was placed by 
Linneus and Fabricius at the head 
of the Order.” 

Stridulating Organs.—Beetles be- 
longing to many and widely distinct 
families possess these organs. The 
sound thus produced can sometimes 
be heard at the distance of several 
feet, or even yards,” but it is not 
comparable with that made by the 
Orthoptera. The rasp generally con- 


sists of a narrow, slightly raised surface, crossed by very 
fine, parallel ribs, sometimes so fine as to cause iridescent 


72 Westwood, ‘‘Modern Class.,’’ vol. i. p. 184. 
73 Wollaston, ‘‘On Certain Musical Curculionide,” ‘‘Annals and Mag. of 
Nat. Hist.,’’ vol. vi., 1860, p. 14. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 895 


colors, and having a very elegant appearance under the 
microscope. In some cases, as with Typhceus, minute, 
bristly, or scale-like prominences, with which the whole 
surrounding surface is covered in approximately parallel 
lines, could be traced passing into the ribs of the rasp. 
The transition takes place by their becoming confluent 
and straight, and at the same time more prominent and 
smooth. A hard ridge on an adjoining part of the body 
serves as the scraper for the rasp, but this scraper in some 
cases has been specially modified for the purpose. It is 
rapidly moved across the rasp, or, conversely, the rasp 
across the scraper. 

These organs are situated in widely different positions. 


Fig. 25.—Necrophorus (from Landois). +. The two rasps. Left-hand figure, part of 
the rasp highly magnified. 


In the carrion-beetles (Necrophorus) two parallel rasps 
(7, Fig. 25) stand on the dorsal surface of the fifth abdom- 
inal segment, each rasp™ consisting of 126 to 140 fine 
ribs. These ribs are scraped against the posterior margins 
of the elytra, a small portion of which projects beyond the 
general outline. In many Crioceridew, and in Clythra 4- 
punctata (one of the Chrysomelide), and in some Tene- 
brionide, etc.,”* the rasp is seated on the dorsal apex of the 


™4 Landois, ‘‘Zeitschrift fiir Wiss. Zoolog.,’? B. xvii., 1867, s, 2217. 

7% T am greatly indebted to Mr. G. R. Crotch for having sent me many pre- 
pared specimens of various beetles belonging to these three families and to 
Others, as well as for valuable information. He believes that the power of 
stridulation in the Clythra has not been previously observed. Iam also much 
indebted to Mr. E. W. Janson, for information and specimens. I may add that 
my son, Mr. F. Darwin, finds that Dermestes murinus stridulates, but he 
searched in vain for the apparatus. Scolytus has lately been described by 
Dr. Chapman as a stridulator, in the ‘‘Entomologist’s Monthly Magazine,”’ 
vol, vi. p. 130. : 


896 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


abdomen, on the pygidium or pro-pygidium, and is scraped 
in the same manner by the elytra. In Heterocerus, which 
belongs to another family, the rasps are placed on the 
sides of the first abdominal segment, and are scraped by 
ridges on the femora.” In certain Curculionida# and Cara- 
bide” the parts are completely reversed in position, for the 
rasps are seated on the inferior surface of the elytra, near 
their apices, or along their outer margins, and the edges of 
the abdominal segments serve as the scrapers. In Pelobius 
Hermanni (one of Dytiscide, or water-beetles) a strong 
ridge runs parallel and near to the sutural margin of the 
elytra, and is crossed by ribs, coarse in the middle part, 
but becoming gradually finer at both ends, especially at 
the upper end; when this insect is held under water or in 
the air, a stridulating noise is produced by the extreme 
horny margin of the abdomen being scraped against the 
rasps. In a great number of long-horned beetles (Longi- 
cornia) the organs are situated quite otherwise, the rasp 
being on the meso-thorax, which is rubbed against the pro- 
thorax; Landois counted 238 very fine ribs on the rasp 
of Cerambyx heros. 

Many Lamellicorns have the power of stridulating, and 
the organs differ greatly in position. Some species stridu- 
late very loudly, so that when Mr. F. Smith caught a Trou 
sabulosus, a gamekeeper, who stood by, thought he had 
caught a mouse; but I failed to discover the proper organs 
in this beetle. In Geotrupes and Typhceus a narrow ridge 
runs obliquely across (r, Fig. 26) the coxa of each hind-leg 
(having in G. stercorarius 84 ribs), which is scraped by 
a specially projecting part of one of the abdominal seg- 
ments. In the nearly allied Copris lunaris, an excessively 


76 Schiddte, translated in ‘‘Annals and Mag. of Nat. Hist.,”’ vol. xx., 1867, 
. 37, 

Pn Westring has described (Kroyer, ‘‘Naturhist. Tidskrift,”’ B. ii., 1848-49, 
p. 334) the stridulating organs of these two, as well as in other families. In 
the Carabidz I have examined Hlaphrus uliginosus and Blethisa multipunctata, 
sent to me by Mr. Crotch. In Blethisa the transverse ridges on the furrowed 
border of the abdominal segment do not, as far as I could judge, come into play 
in scraping the rasps on the elytra. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 897 


narrow fine rasp runs along the sutural margin of the elytra, 
with another sharp rasp near the basal outer margin; but 
in some other Coprini the rasp is seated, ‘according to 
Leconte,” on the dorsal surface of the abdomen. In Oryctes 
it is seated on the pro-pygidium; and, according to the same 
entomologist, in some other Dynastini, on the under surface 
of the elytra. Lastly, Westring states that in Omaloplia 
brunnea the rasp is placed on the pro-sternum, and the 
scraper on the meta-sternum, the parts thus occupying 
the under surface of the body, instead of the 
upper surface, as in the Longicorns. : 

We thus see that in the different coleopter- == 
ous families the stridulating organs are won- 
derfully diversified in position, but not much 
in structure. Within the same family some 
species are provided with these organs, and 
others are destitute of them. This diversity is 
intelligible, if we suppose that originally vari- 
ous beetles made a shuffling or hissing noise by 
the rubbing together of any hard and rough 
parts of their bodies which happened to bein yg. 06. Hina. 
contact; and that, from the noise thus pro- ls of Geotrupes 


stercorarius (from 
duced being in some way useful, the rough Lyiuis)..7 Rasp. 
surfaces were gradually developed into regular * Tibia. ¢r. Tarsi. 
stridulating organs. Some beetles, as they move, now pro- 
duce, either intentionally or unintentionally, a shuffling 
noise, without possessing any proper organs for the purpose. 
Mr. Wallace informs me that the Huchirus longimanus (a 
Lamellicorn, with the anterior legs wonderfully elongated 
in the male) ‘‘makes, while moving, a low hissing sound 
by the protrusion and contraction of the abdomen; and 
when seized it produces a grating sound by rubbing its 
hind-legs against the edges of the elytra.’’ The hissing 
sound is clearly due to a narrow rasp running along the 
sutural margin of each elytron; and I could likewise make 


8 I am indebted to Mr. Walsh, of Illinois, for having sent me extracts from 
Leconte’s ‘Introduction to Entomology,’’ pp. 101, 143. 


898 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


the grating sound by rubbing the shagreened surface of 
the femur against the granulated margin of the correspond- 
ing elytron; but I could not here detect any proper rasp; 
nor is it likely that I could have overlooked it in so large 
an insect. After examining Cychrus, and reading what 
Westring has written about this beetle, it seems very doubt- 
ful whether it possesses any true rasp, though it has the 
power of emitting a sound. 

From the analogy of the Orthoptera and Homoptera, I 
expected to find the stridulating organs in the Coleoptera 
differing according to sex; but Landois, who has carefully 
examined several species, observed no such difference; nor 
did Westring; nor did Mr. G. R. Crotch in preparing the 
many specimens which he had the kindness to send me. 
Any difference in these organs, if slight, would, however, 
be difficult to detect, on account of their great variability. 
Thus, in the first pair of specimens of Necrophorus humator 
and of Pelobiws which I examined, the rasp was consider- 
ably larger in the male than in the female; but not so with 
succeeding specimens. In Geotrupes stercorarius the rasp 
appeared to me thicker, opaquer, and more promjnent in 
three males than in the same number of females; in order, 
therefore, to discover whether the sexes differed in their 
power of stridulating, my son, Mr. F. Darwin, collected 
fifty-seven living specimens, which he separated into two 
lots, according as they made a greater or lesser noise, when 
held in the same manner. He then examined all these 
specimens, and found that the males were very nearly 
in the same proportion to the females in both the lots. 
Mr. F. Smith has kept alive numerous specimens of 
Monoynchus pseudacori (Curculionide) and is convinced 
that both sexes stridulate, and apparently in an equal 
degree. 

Nevertheless, the power of stridulating is certainly a 
sexual character in some few Coleoptera. Mr. Crotch dis- 
covered that the males alone of two species of Heliopathes 
(Tenebrionide) possess stridulating organs. I examined 


SEXUAL SELECTION 399 


five males of H. gibbus, and in all these there was a well- 
developed rasp, partially divided into two, on the dorsal 
surface of the terminal abdominal segment; while in the 
same number of females there was not even a rudiment of 
the rasp, the membrane of this segment being transparent, 
and much thinner than in the male. In H. ecribratostriatus 
the male has a similar rasp, excepting that it is not partially 
divided into two portions, and the female is completely desti- 
tute of this organ; the male, in addition, has on the apical 
margins of the elytra, on each side of the suture, three or 
four short longitudinal ridges, which are crossed by ex- 
tremely fine ribs, parallel to and resembling those on the 
abdominal rasp; whether these ridges serve as an indepen- 
dent rasp, or as a scraper for the abdominal rasp, I could not 
decide: the female exhibits no trace of this latter structure. 

Again, in three species of the Lamellicorn genus Oryctes 
we have a nearly parallel case. In the females of O. gryphus 
and nasicornis the ribs on the rasp of the pro-pygidium are 
less continuous and less distinct than in the males; but the 
chief difference is that the whole upper surface of this seg- 
ment, when held in the proper light, is seen to be clothed 
with hairs, which are absent or are represented by exces- 
sively fine down in the males. It should be noticed that in 
all Coleoptera the effective part of the rasp is destitute of 
hairs. In O. senegalensis the difference between the sexes 
is more strongly marked, and this is best seen when the 
_ proper abdominal segment is cleaned and viewed as a trans- 
parent object. In the female the whole surface is covered 
with little separate crests, bearing spines; while in the male 
these crests in proceeding toward the apex become more and 
more confluent, regular, and naked; so that three-fourths 
of the segment is covered with extremely fine parallel ribs, 
which are quite absent in the female. In the females, how- 
ever, of all three species of Oryctes, a slight grating or strid- 
ulating sound is produced when the abdomen of a softened 
specimen is pushed backward and forward. 

In the case of the Heliopathes and Oryctes there can 


400 | THE DESCENT OF MAN 


hardly be a doubt that the males stridulate in order to 
call or to excite the females; but with most beetles the 
stridulation apparently serves both sexes as a mutual call. 
Beetles stridulate under various emotions, in the same man- 
ner as birds use their voices for many purposes besides sing- 
ing to their mates. The great Chiasognathus stridulates in 
anger or defiance; many species do the same from distress 
or fear, if held so that they cannot escape; by striking the 
hollow stems of trees in the Canary Islands, Messrs. Wol- 
laston and Crotch were able to discover the presence of 
beetles belonging to the genus Acalles by their stridula- 
tion. Lastly, the male Ateuchus stridulates to encourage 
the female in ber work, and from distress when she is re- 
moved.” Some naturalists believe that beetles make this 
noise to frighten away their enemies; but I cannot think 
that a quadruped or bird, able to devour a large beetle, 
would be frightened by so slight a sound. The belief that 
the stridulation serves as a sexual call is supported by the 
fact that death-ticks (Anobium tessellatum) are well known to 
answer each other’s ticking, and, as I have myself observed, 
a tapping noise artificially made. Mr. Doubleday also in- 
forms me that he has sometimes observed a female ticking,® 
and in an hour or two afterward he has found her united 
with a male, and on one occasion surrounded by several 
males. Finally, it is probable that the two sexes of many 
kinds of beetles were at first enabled to find each other by 
the slight shuffling noise produced by the rubbing together 
of the adjoining hard parts of their bodies; and that as those 
males or females which made the greatest noise succeeded 


79 M. P. de la Brulerie, as quoted in ‘‘Journal of Travel,’’? A. Murray, vol. i., 
1868, p. 135. 

8 According to Mr. Doubleday, ‘‘the noise is produced by the insect raising 
ctself on its legs as high as it can, and then striking its thorax five or six times, 
in rapid succession, against the substance upon which it is sitting.’’? For refer- 
ences on this subject see Landois, ‘‘Zeitschrift fir Wissen. Zoolog.,”’ B. xvii. s, 
131. Oliver says (as quoted by Kirby and Spence, ‘‘Introduct.,’’ vol. ii. p. 
393) that the female of Pimelia striata produces a rather loud sound by striking 
her abdomen against any hard substance, ‘‘and that the male, obedient to this 
call, soon attends her, and they pair.”’ 


SEXUAL SELECTION 401 


best in finding partners, rugosities on various parts of their 
bodies were gradually developed by means of sexual selec- 
tion into true stridulating organs. 


CHAPTER XI 


INSECTS, continued—ORDER, LEPIDOPTERA 


(BUTTERFLIES AND MOTHS) 


Courtship of butterflies—Battles—Ticking noise—Colors common to both 
sexes, or more brilliant in the males—Examples—Not due to the direct 
action of the conditions of life—Colors adapted for protection—Colors 
of moths—Display—Perceptive powers of the Lepidoptera—Variability 
—Causes of the difference in color between the males and females— 
Mimicry, female butterflies more brilliantly colored than the males 
—Bright colors of caterpillars—Summary and concluding remarks 
on the secondary sexual characters of insects—Birds and insects 
compared : 


the differences in color between the sexes of the same 
species, and between the distinct species of the same 
genus. Nearly the whole of the following chapter will be 
devoted to this subject; but I will first make a few remarks 
on one or two other points. Several males may often be 
seen pursuing and crowding round the same female. Their 
courtship appears to be a prolonged affair, for I have fre- 
quently watched one or more males pirouetting round a 
female until I was tired, without seeing the end of the 
courtship. Mr. A. G. Butler also informs me that he has 
several times watched a male courting a female for a full 
quarter of an hour; but she pertinaciously refused him, and 
at last settled on the ground and closed her wings, so as to 
escape from his addresses. 
Although butterflies are weak and fragile creatures, they 
are pugnacious, and an Emperor butterfly’ has been cap- 


i this great Order the most interesting points for us are 


1 Apatura Iris: ‘The Entomologist’s Weekly Intelligence,’? 1859, p. 139. 
For the Bornean Butterflies, see OC. Collingwood, ‘‘Rambles of a Naturalist,” 
1868, p. 183. 


402 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


tured with the tips of its wings broken from a conflict with 
another male. Mr. Collingwood, in spoaking of the fre- 
quent battles between the butterflies of Bornoo, says, ‘They 
whirl round each other with the yreatest rapidity, and appoar 
to be incited by the greatest ferocity.” 

The Ageronia feronia makes a noise like that produced 
by a toothed wheel passing under a spring catch, and which 
can be heard at the distance of sevoral yards: T noticod this 
sound at Rio de Janciro, only when two of those butterflios 
wero chasing each othor 4 in an, irregular courso, so that it is 
probably made. during the courtship of the sexes.’ 

Some moths also produce sounds; for instance, the males 
of Theophora fovea. On two occasions Mr. F. Buchanan 
White" heard a sharp, quick noise made by the male of 
MMylophila prasinana, and which he believes to be pro- 
duced, as in Cicada, by an elastic mombrane, furnished 
with a muscle. Ie quotes, also, Guonée, that Setinu pro- 
duces a sound like the ticking of a watch, apparently by 
the aid of ‘‘two large tympaniform vesicles, situated in the 
pectoral revion’’; and these ‘tare much moro developed in 
the male than in the female.’’ Ienco the sound-producing 
organs in the Lepidoptera appear to stand in some relation 
with the sexual functions. I have not alluded to the woll- 
known noise made by the Death’s Wead Sphinx, for it is 
generally heard soon after the moth has cimerged from. its 
cocoon. 

Girard has always observed that the musky odor which 
is emitted by two species of Sphinx moths is poculiar to the 
malos;* and in the higher classes we shall meet with many 
instances of the males alone being odoriferous. 

Every one must have admired the extreme beauty of 


2 Seo my ‘Journal of Resourchos,’’ 1845, p. 88. Mr. Doubleday has do- 
tected (Proc, Hint. Soc.,’? Marchi 3, 1846, p. 128) » peculiar mombranous sac 
at the base of the front wings, which is probably connected with the produc. 
tion of the sound. Vor the ease of Thecophorn, seo ‘*Zoologlen) Record,’? 1469, 
p. 401. For Mr, Buchanan White’s observations, “The Seotiish Naturallnt, ” 
July, 1872, p. 214. 

“The Scottish Naturalist, ” July, 1852, p, 218. 

4 “Zoological Record,’’ 1869, p. 347. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 408 


many butterflies and of some moths; and it may be asked, 
aro their colors and diversified patterns the result of the 
direct action of the physical conditions to which these 
insvots have boon exposed, without any benofit being thus 
derived? Or have successive variations been accumulated 
and determined as a protection, or Lor some unknown pur- 
pose, or that one sox may be attractive to the other’ And, 
aguin, what is the moaning of the colors boing widely dilor- 
ent in the malos and females of cortain species, and alike in 
the two soxes of other species of the same genus? Bofore 
attempting to answer these questions a body of facts must 
be givon. 

With our beautiful English butterflies, the admiral, pea- 
covk, and painted lady (Vaness:e), as well as many others, 
tho sexes are alike. This is also the case with the magnill- 
cont Heliconide, and most of the Danaide in the tropics. 
But in certain other tropical groups, and in some of our 
English butterflies, as the purple omporor, orange-tip, ete. 
(Apatura tris and Anthocharis cardamines), the sexes differ 
either greatly or slightly in color. No Isnguage suffices to 
describe the splendor of the males of somo tropical species. 
Eyon within the same genus wo often find specios presenting 
extraordinary differences between the sexes, while others 
have their sexes closely alike. Thus in the South Ameri- 
can genus Epicalia, Mr. Bates, to whom I am indebted for 
most of the following facts, and for looking over this whole 
discussion, informs me that he knows twelve species the two 
soxes of which haunt the same stations (and this is not al- 
ways the ease with butterflies), and which, therefore, cannot 
have been differently affected by external conditions.’ In 
nine of these twelve species the males rank among the most 
brilliant of all butterflies, and differ so greatly from the 
comparatively plain females that they wore formerly placed 
in distinct gonera. The females of these nine species re- 


5 Seo also Mr. Bates's papor in ‘Proc. Nut. Soo. of Philadelphia’? 1865, 
P 206, Also Mr. Wallace on the samo subject, in regard to Diadema, in | 
‘Transact, Entomolog. Soe. of London,’’ 1869, p, 378, 


404 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


semble each other in their general type of coloration; and 
they likewise resemble both sexes of the species in several 
allied genera, found in various parts of the world. Hence 
we may infer that these nine species, and probably all the 
others of the genus, are descended from an ancestral form 
which was colored in nearly the same manner. In the tenth 
species the female still retains the same general coloring, but 
the male resembles her, so that he is colored in a much less 
gaudy and contrasted manner than the males of the previous 
species. In the eleventh and twelfth species the females de- 
part from the usual type, for they are gayly decorated almost 
like the males, but in a somewhat less degree. Hence in 
these two latter species the bright colors of the males seem 
to have been transferred to the females; while in the tenth 
species the male has either retained or recovered the plain 
colors of the female, as well as of the parent-form of the 
genus. The sexes in these three cases have thus been ren- 
dered nearly alike, though in an opposite manner. In the 
allied genus Eubagis, both sexes of some of the species are 
plain-colored and nearly alike; while with the greater num- 
ber the males are decorated with beautiful metallic tints in 
a diversified manner, and differ much from their females. 
The females throughout the genus retain the same general 
style of coloring, so that they resemble one another much 
more closely than they resemble their own males. 

In the genus Papilio, all the species of the Auneas group 
are remarkable for their conspicuous and strongly contrasted 
colors, and they illustrate the frequent tendency to grada- 
tion in the amount of difference between the sexes. Ina 
few species, for instance in P. ascanius, the males and 
females are alike; in others the males are either a little 
brighter or very much more superb than the females. The 
genus Junonia, allied to our Vanesse, offers a nearly parallel 
case, for although the sexes of most of the species resemble 
each other, and are destitute of rich colors, yet in certain 
species, as in J. enone, the male is rather more bright- 
colored than the female, and in a few (for instance e/. an- 


SEXUAL SELECTION 405 


dremiaja) the male is so different from the female that 
he might be mistaken for an entirely distinct species. 

Another striking case was pointed out to me in the Brit- 
ish Museum by Mr. A. Butler, namely, one of the tropical 
American Thecle, in which both sexes are nearly alike and 
wonderfully splendid; in another species the male is colored - 
in a similarly gorgeous manner, while the whole upper sur- 
face of the female is of a dull uniform brown. Our common 
little English blue butterflies of the genus Lycena illustrate 
the various differences in color between the sexes almost as 
well, though not in so striking a manner, as the above exotic 
genera. In Lycena agestis both sexes have wings of a brown 
color, bordered with small ocellated orange spots, and are 
thus alike. In ZL. egon the wings of the male are of a fine 
blue, bordered with black; while those of the female are 
brown, with a similar border, closely resembling the wings 
of ZL. agestis. lastly, in Z. arion both sexes are of a blue 
color and are very like, though in the female the edges of 
the wings are rather duskier, with the black spots plainer; 
and in a bright blue Indian species both sexes are still more 
alike. 

I have given the foregoing details in order to show, in 
the first place, that when the sexes of butterflies differ the 
male, as a general rule, is the more beautiful, and departs 

_more from the usual type of coloring of the group to which 
the species belongs. Hence in most groups the females of 
the several species resemble each other much more closely 
than do the males. In some cases, however, to which I shall 
hereafter allude, the females are colored more splendidly 
than the males. In the second place, these details have 
been given to bring clearly before the mind that, within 
the same genus, the two sexes frequently present every gra- 
dation from no difference in color to so great a difference 
that it was long before the two were placed by entomologists 
in the same genus. In the third place, we have seen that 
when the sexes nearly resemble each other, this appears due 
either to the male having transferred his colors to the fe- 


406- THE DESCENT OF MAN 


male, or to the male having retained, or perhaps recovered, 
the primordial colors of the group. It also deserves notice 
that in those groups in which the sexes differ the females 
usually somewhat resemble the males, so that when the 
males are beautiful to an extraordinary degree, the females 
‘almost invariably exhibit some degree of beauty. From 
the many cases of gradation in the amount of difference be- 
tween the sexes, and from the prevalence of the same general 
type of coloration throughout the whole of the same group, 
we may conclude that the causes have generally been the 
same which have determined the brilliant coloring of the 
males alone of some species, and of both sexes. of other 
species. 

As so many gorgeous butterflies inhabit the tropics, it 
has often been supposed that they owe their colors to the 
great heat and moisture of these zones; but Mr. Bates* has 
shown, by the comparison of various closely allied groups 
of insects from the temperate and tropical regions, that this 
view cannot be maintained; and the evidence becomes con- 
clusive when brilliantly colored males and plain-colored 
females of the same species inhabit the same district, feed 
on the same food, and follow exactly the same habits of life. 
Even when the sexes resemble each other, we can hardly 
believe that their brilliant and beautifully arranged colors 
are the purposeless result of the nature of the tissues and 
of the action of the surrounding conditions. 

With animals of all kinds, whenever color has been modi- 
fied for some special purpose, this has been, as far as we can 
judge, either for direct or indirect protection, or as an attrac- 
tion between the sexes. With many species of butterflies 
the upper surfaces of the wings are obscure; and this in all 
probability leads to their escaping observation and danger. 
But butterflies would be particularly liable to be attacked 
by their enemies when at rest; and most kinds while rest- 
ing raise their wings vertically over their backs, so that the 


8 “The Naturalist on the Amazons,”’ vol. i., 1863, p. 19. 


SEXUAL SELECTION . 407 


lower surface alone is exposed to view. Hence it is this side 
which is often colored so as to imitate the objects on which 
these insects commonly rest. Dr. Réssler, I believe, first 
noticed the similarity of the closed wings of certain Vanessa 
and other butterflies to the bark of trees. Many analogous 
and striking facts could be given. The most interesting one 
is that recorded by Mr. Wallace’ of a common Indian and 
Sumatran butterfly (Kallima), which disappears like magic 
-when it settles on a bush; for it hides its head and antennz 
between its closed wings, which, in form, color, and veining, 
cannot be distinguished from a withered leaf with its foot- 
stalk. In some other cases the lower surfaces of the wings 
are brilliantly colored, and yet are protective; thus in Thecla 
rubi the wings when closed are of an emerald green, and re- 
semble the young leaves of the bramble, on which in spring 
this butterfly may often be seen seated. It is also remark- 
able that in very many species, in which the sexes differ 
greatly in color on their upper surface, the lower surface 
is closely similar or identical in both sexes, and serves 
as a protection.° 

Although the obscure tints both of the upper and under 
sides of many butterflies no doubt serve to conceal them, 
yet we cannot extend this view to the brilliant and con- 
spicuous colors on the upper surface of such species as our 
admiral and peacock Vanesse, our white cabbage-butterflies 
(Pieris), or the greater swallow-tail Papilio which haunts the 
open fens—for these butterflies are thus rendered visible to 
every living creature. In these species both sexes are alike; 
but in the common brimstone butterfly (Gonepteryx rhamnt) 
the male is of an intense yellow, while the female is much 
paler; and in the orange-tip (Anthocharis cardamines) the 
males alone have their wings tipped with bright orange. 
Both the males and females in these cases are conspicuous, 


1 See a very interesting article in the ‘‘Westminster Review”’ for July, 1867, 
p. 10. A woodcut of the Kallima is given by Mr. Wallace in ‘‘Hardwicke’s 
Science Gossip’’ for September, 1867, p. 196. 

8 Mr. G. Fraser, in ‘‘Nature,’’ April, 1871, p. 489. 


408 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


and it is not credible that their difference in color should 
stand in any relation to ordinary protection. Prof. Weis- 
mann remarks® that the female of one of the Lycene ex- 
pands her brown wings when she settles on the ground, and 
is then almost invisible; the male, on the other hand, as if 
‘aware of the danger incurred from the bright blue of the 
upper surface of his wings, rests with them closed; and this 
shows that the blue color cannot be in any way protective. 
Nevertheless, it is probable that conspicuous colors are in- 
directly beneficial to many species, as a warning that they 
are unpalatable. For in certain other cases beauty has 
been gained through the imitation of other beautiful spe- 
cies, which inhabit the same district and enjoy an immu- 
nity from attack by being in some way offensive to their 
enemies; but then we have to account for the beauty of 
the'imitated species. 

As Mr. Walsh has remarked to me, the females of our 
orange-tip butterfly, above referred to, and of an American 
species (Anth. genutia), probably show us the primordial. 
colors of the parent-species of the genus; for both sexes of 
four or five widely distributed species are colored in nearly 
the same manner. As in several previous cases, we may 
here infer that it is the males of Anth. cardamines and 
genutia which have departed from the usual type of the 
genus. In the Anth. sara from California, the orange-tips 
to the wings have been partially developed in the female; 
but they are paler than in the male, and slightly different 
in some other respects. In an allied Indian form, the [phias 
gliaucippe, the orange-tips are fully developed in both sexes. 
In this Iphias, as pointed out to me by Mr. A. Butler, the 
under surface of the wings marvellously resembles a pale- 
colored leaf; and in our English orange-tip, the under 
surface resembles the flower-head of the wild parsley, on 
which the butterfly often rests at night."° The same reason 


9 “*Rinfluss der Isolirung auf die Artbildung,’’ 1872, 
10 See the interesting observations by Mr. T. W. Wood, “The Student,” 
Sept. 1868, p. 81. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 409 


which compels us to believe that the lower surfaces have 
here been colored for the sake of protection, leads us to 
deny that the wings have been tipped with bright orange 
for the same purpose, especially when this character is con- 
fined to the males. 

Most Moths rest motionless during the whole or greater 
part of the day with their wings depressed, and the whole 
upper surface is often shaded and colored in an admirable 
manner, as Mr. Wallace has remarked, for escaping detec- 
tion. The front wings of the Bombycide and Noctuide," 
when at rest, generally overlap and conceal the hind-wings, 
so that the latter might be brightly colored without much 
risk; and they are in fact often thus colored. During 
flight, moths would often be able to escape from their 
enemies; nevertheless, as the hind-wings are then fully 
exposed to view, their bright colors must generally have 
been acquired at some little risk. But the following fact 
shows how cautious we ought to be in drawing conclusions 
on this head. The common Yellow Under-wings (Triphena) 
often fly about during the day or early evening, and are 
then conspicuous from the color of their hind-wings. It 
would naturally be thought that this would be a source 
of danger; but Mr. J. Jenner Weir believes that it actually 
serves them as a means of escape, for birds strike at these 
brightly colored and fragile surfaces, instead of at the body. 
For instance, Mr. Weir turned into his aviary a vigorous 
specimen of Triphena pronuba, which was instantly pursued 
by a robin; but the bird’s attention being caught by the 
colored wings, the moth was not captured until after about 
fifty attempts, and small portions ot the wings were repeat- 
edly broken off. He tried the same experiment in the open 
air, with a swallow and Z. fimbria; but the large size of this 
moth probably interfered with its capture.'? We are thus 
reminded of a statement made by Mr. Wallace,’** namely, 


i! Mr. Wallace in ‘‘Hardwicke’s Science Gossip,” Sept. 1867, p. 193. 

12 See also, on this subject, Mr. Weir’s paper in “Transact. Ent. Society,” 
1869, Pp: 23, 

13 **Westminster Rev.,’’ July, 1867, p. 16. 
Descent—Vou. I.—18 


410 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


that in the Brazilian forests and Malayan islands, many 
common and highly decorated butterflies are weak flyers, 
though furnished with a broad expanse of wing; and they 
‘“‘are often captured with pierced and broken wings, as if 
they had been seized by birds, from which they had 
escaped; if the wings had been much smaller in proportion 
to the body, it seems probable that the insect would more 
frequently have been struck or pierced in a vital part, and 
thus the increased expanse of the wings may have been 
indirectly beneficial.’’ 

Display.—The bright colors of many butterflies and of 
some moths are specially arranged for display, so that they 
may be readily seen. During the night colors are not 
visible, and there can be no doubt that the nocturnal 
moths, taken as a body, are much less gayly decorated than 
butterflies, all of which are diurnal in their habits. But 
the moths of certain families, such as the Zygeenidsx, several 
Sphingide, Uraniide, some Arctiidae and Saturniide, fly 
about during the-day or early evening, and many of these 
are extremely beautiful, being far brighter colored than 
the strictly nocturnal kinds. A few exceptional cases, 
however, of bright-colored nocturnal species have been 
recorded." 

There is evidence of another kind in regard to display. 
Butterflies, as before remarked, elevate their wings when 
at rest, but while basking in the sunshine often alternately 
raise and depress them, thus exposing both surfaces to full 
view; and although the lower surface is often colored 
in an obscure manner as a protection, yet in many species 
it is as highly decorated as the upper surface, and sometimes 
in a very different manner. In some tropical species the 
lower surface is even more brilliantly colored than the 
upper.’® In the English fritillaries (Argynnis) the lower 


14 For instance, Lithosia; but Prof. Westwood (‘‘Modern Class. of Insects,’? 
vol. ii. p. 390) seems surprised at this case. On the relative colors of diurnal 
and nocturnal Lepidoptera, see ibid., pp. 333 and 392; also Harris, ‘‘Treatise 
on the Insects of New England,”’ 1842, p. 315. 

1’ Such differences between the upper and lower surfaces cf the wings of 


SEXUAL SELECTION 411 


surface alone is ornamented with shining silver. Never- 
theless, as a general rule, the upper surface, which is prob- 
ably more fully exposed, is colored more brightly and 
diversely than the lower; hence the lower surface gener- 
ally affords to entomologists the more useful character for 
detecting the affinities of the various species. Fritz Muller 
informs me that three species of Castnia are found near his 
house in South Brazil: of two of them the hind-wings are 
obscure, and are always covered by the front-wings when 
these butterflies are at rest; but the third species has black 
hind-wings, beautifully spotted with red and white, and 
these are fully expanded and displayed whenever the 
butterfly rests. Other such cases could be added. 

If we now turn to the enormous group of moths which, 
as I hear from Mr. Stainton, do not habitually expose the 
under surface of their wings to full view, we find this side 
very rarely colored with a brightness greater than, or even 
equal to, that of the upper side. Some exceptions to the 
rule, either real or apparent, must be noticed, as the case 
of Hypopyra.’* Mr. Trimen informs me that in Guenée’s 
great work three moths are figured, in which the under sur- 
face is much the more brilliant. For instance, in the Aus- 
tralian Gastrophora the upper surface of the fore-wing is 
pale grayish-ochreous, while the lower surface is magnifi- 
cently ornamented by an ocellus of cobalt-blue, placed in 
the midst of a black mark, surrounded by orange-yellow, 
and this by bluish white. But the habits of these three 
moths are unknown, so that no explanation can be given of 
their unusual style of coloring. Mr. Trimen also informs 
me that the lower surface of the wings in certain other 
Geometre"” and quadrifid Noctus are either more varie- 
gated or more brightly colored than the upper surface; but 


several species of Papilio may be seen in the beautiful plates to Mr. Wallace’s 
‘“‘“Memoir on the Papilionide of the Malayan Region,’’ in ‘‘Transact. Linn. 
Soc.,’? vol. xxv. part i., 1865, 

16 See Mr. Wormald on this moth: ‘‘Proc. Ent. Soc.,’? March 2, 1868. 

11 See also an account of the S. American genus Erateina (one of the Geome- 
tre) in ‘‘Transact. Ent. Soc.,’’ new series, vol. v. pl. xv. and xvi, 


412 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


some of these species have the habit of ‘‘holding their 
wings quite erect over their backs, retaining them in this 
position for a considerable time,’’ and thus exposing the 
under surface to view. Other species, when settled on 
the ground or herbage, now and then suddenly and slightly 
lift up their wings. Hence the lower surface of the wings 
being brighter than the upper surface in certain moths is 
not so anomalous as it at first appears. The Saturniide 
include some of the most beautiful of all moths, their wings 
being decorated, as in our British Emperor moth, with fine 
ocelli; and Mr. T. W. Wood” observes that they resemble 
butterflies in some of their movements, ‘‘for instance, in the 
gentle waving up and down of the wings as if for display, 
which is more characteristic of diurnal than of nocturnal 
Lepidoptera.’’ 

It is a singular fact that no British moths which are 
brilliantly colored, and, as far as I can discover, hardly 
any foreign species, differ much in color according to sex; 
though this is the case with many brilliant butterflies. 
The male, however, of one American moth, the Saturnia 
do, is described as having its fore-wings deep yellow, curi- 
ously marked with purplish red spots; while the wings of 
the female are purple-brown, marked with gray lines.’ 
The British moths which differ sexually in color are all 
brown, or of various dull yellow tints, or nearly white. 
In several species the males are much darker than the 
females,” and these belong to groups which generally fly 


18 “Prog, Ent. Soc. of London,”’ July 6, 1868, p. xxvii. 

9 Harris, ‘‘Treatise,’’ ete., edited by Flint, 1862, p. 395. 

20 For instance, I observe in my son’s cabinet that the males are darker 
than the females in the Lasiocampa quercus, Odonestis potatoria, Hypogymna 
dispar, Dasychira pudibunda, and Cycnia mendica. In this latter species the 
difference in color between the two sexes is strongly marked; and Mr. Wallace 
informs me that we here have, as he believes, an instance of protective mimicry 
confined to one sex, as will hereafter be more fully explained. The white female 
of the Cyenia resembles the very common Spilosoma menthrasti, both sexes of 
which are white; and Mr. Stainton observed that this latter moth was rejected 
with utter disgust by a whole brood of young turkeys, which were fond of eat- 
ing other moths; so that if the Cycnia was commonly mistaken by British birds 
for the Spilosoma, it would escape being devoured, and its white deceptive color 
would thus be highly beneficial. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 413 


about during the afternoon. On the other hand, in many 
genera, as Mr. Stainton informs me, the males have the 
hind-wings whiter than those of the female—of which fact 
Agrotis exclamationis offers a good instance. In the Ghost 
Moth (Hepialus humuli) the difference is more strongly 
marked; the males being white, and the females yellow 
with darker markings.” It is probable that in these cases 
_ the males are thus rendered more conspicuous, and more 
easily seen by the females while flying about in the dusk. 
From the several foregoing facts it is impossible to admit 
that the brilliant colors of butterflies, and of some few 
moths, have commonly been acquired for the sake of pro- 
tection. We have seen that their colors and elegant patterns 
are arranged and exhibited as if for display. Hence I am 
led to believe that the females prefer or are most excited 
by the more brilliant males; for on any other supposition 
the males would, as far as we can see, be ornamented to 
no purpose. We know that ants and certain Lamellicorn 
beetles are capable of feeling an attachment for each other, 
and that ants recognize their fellows after an interval of 
several months. Hence there is no abstract improbability 
in the Lepidoptera, which probably stand nearly or quite 
as high in the scale as these insects, having sufficient mental 
capacity to admire bright colors. They certainly discover 
flowers by color. The Humming-bird Sphinx may often be 
seen to swoop down from a distance on a bunch of flowers 
in the midst of green foliage; and I have been assured, by 
two persons abroad, that these moths repeatedly visit flowers 
painted on the walls of a room, and vainly endeavor to 
insert their proboscis into them. Fritz Muller informs 
me that several kinds of butterflies in South Brazil show 
an unmistakable preference for certain colors over others: — 


21 It is remarkable that in the Shetland Islands the male of this moth, instead 
of differing widely from the female, frequently resembles her closely in color (see 
Mr, MacLachlan, ‘‘Transact. Ent. Soc.,’’ vol. ii., 1866, p. 459). Mr. G. Fraser 
suggests (‘‘Nature,”’ April, 1871, p. 489) that at the season of the year when 
the ghost moth appears in these northern islands, the whiteness of the males 
would not be needed to render them visible to the females in the twilight night, 


414 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


he observed that they very often visited the brilliant red 
flowers of five or six genera of plants, but never the white 
or yellow flowering species of the same and other genera, 
growing in the same garden; and I have received other ac- 
counts to the same effect. As I hear from Mr. Doubleday, 
the common white butterfly often flies down to a bit of paper 
on the ground, no doubt mistaking it for one of its own 
species. Mr. Collingwood,” in speaking of the difficulty 
in collecting certain butterflies in the Malay Archipelago, 
states that ‘ta dead specimen pinned upon a conspicuous 
twig will often arrest an insect of the same species in its 
headlong flight, and bring it down within easy reach of the 
net, especially if it-be of the opposite sex.”’ 

The courtship of butterflies is, as before remarked, a 
prolonged affair. The males sometimes fight together in 
rivalry; and many may be seen pursuing or crowding round 
the same female. Unless, then, the females prefer one male 
to another, the pairing must be left to mere chance, and 
this does not appear probable. If, on the other hand, the 
females habitually, or even occasionally, prefer the more 
beautiful males, the colors of the latter will have been ren- 
dered brighter by degrees, and will have been transmitted 
to both sexes or to one sex, according to the law of inheri- 
tance which has prevailed. The process of sexual selection 
will have been much facilitated, if the conclusion can be 
trusted, arrived at from various kinds of evidence in the 
supplement to the ninth chapter; namely, that the males 
of many Lepidoptera, at least in the imago state, greatly 
exceed the females in number. 

Some facts, however, are opposed to the belief that fe- 
male butterflies prefer the more beautiful males; thus, as I 
have been assured by several collectors, fresh females may 
frequently be seen paired with battered, faded, or dingy 
males; but this is a circumstance which could hardly fail 
often to follow from the males emerging from their cocoons 
earlier than the females. With moths of the family of the 


% ‘‘Rambles of a. Naturalist in the Chinese Seas,’’ 1868, p. 182. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 415 


Bombycidew the sexes pair immediately after assuming the 
imago state; for they cannot feed, owing to the rudimentary 
condition of their mouths. The females, as several ento- 
mologists have remarked to me, lie in an almost torpid 
state, and appear not to evince the least choice in regard 
to their partners. This is the case with the common silk- 
moth (B. mori), as I have been told by some Continental and 
English breeders. Dr. Wallace, who has had great.experi- 
ence in breeding Bombyx cynthia, is convinced that the 
females evince no choice or preference. He has kept above 
800 of these moths together, and has often found the most 
vigorous females mated with stunted males. The reverse 
‘appears to occur seldom; for, as he believes, the more vigor- 
ous males pass over the weakly females, and are attracted 
by those endowed with most vitality. Nevertheless, the 
Bombycide, though obscurely colored, are often beautiful 
to our eyes from their elegant and mottled shades. 

I have as yet only referred to the species in which the 
males are brighter colored than the females, and I have 
attributed their beauty to the females for many generations 
having chosen and paired with the more attractive males. 
But converse cases occur, though rarely, in which the 
females are more brilliant than the males; and here, as 
I believe, the males have selected the more beautiful 
females, and have thus slowly added to their beauty. We 
do not know why in various classes of animals the males 
of some few species have selected the more beautiful females 
instead of having gladly accepted any female, as seems to 
be the general rule in the animal kingdom; but if, contrary 
to what generally occurs with the Lepidoptera, the females 
were much more numerous than the males, the latter would 
be likely to pick out the more beautiful females. Mr. But- 
ler showed me several species of Callidryas in the British 
Museum, in some of which the females equalled, and in 
others greatly surpassed, the mates in beauty; for the 
females alone have the borders of their wings suffused 
with crimson and orange, and spotted with black. The 


416 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


plainer males of these species closely resemble each other, 
showing that here the females have been modified; whereas 
in those cases where the males are the more ornate it is 
these which have been modified, the females remaining 
closely alike. 

In England we have some analogous cases, though not 
so marked. The females alone of two species of Thecla 
have a bright-purple or orange patch on their fore-wings. 
In Hipparchia the sexes do not differ much; but it is the 
female of H. janira which has a conspicuous light-brown 
patch on her wings; and the females of some of the other 
species are brighter colored than their males. Again, the 
females of Colias deusa and hyale have ‘‘orange or yellow 
spots on the black marginal border, represented in the males 
only by thin streaks’’; and in Pieris it is the females which 
“are ornamented with black spots on the fore-wings, and 
these are only partially present in the males.”” Now, the 
males of many butterflies are known to support the females 
during their marriage flight; but in the species just named 
it is the females which support the males; so that the part 
which the two sexes play is reversed, as is their relative 
beauty. Throughout the animal kingdom the males com- 
monly take the more active share in wooing, and their 
beauty seems to have been increased by the females having 
accepted the more attractive individuals; but with these 
butterflies the females take the more active part in the final 
marriage ceremony, so that we may suppose that they like- 
wise do so in the wooing; and in this case we can under- 
stand how it is that they have been rendered the more _ 
beautiful. Mr. Meldola, from whom the foregoing state- 
ments have been taken, says in conclusion: ‘Though I am 
not convinced of the action of sexual selection in producing 
the colors of insects, it cannot be denied that these facts are 
strikingly corroborative of Mr. Darwin’s views.’’ ” 


93 **Wature,’? April 27, 1871, p. 508. Mr. Meldola quotes Donzel, in ‘‘Soe. ; 
Ent. de France,’’ 1837, p. 77, on the flight of buttertiies while pairing. See also 
Mr. G. Fraser, in ‘‘Nature,’’ April 20, 1871, p. 489, on the sexual differences 
of several British butterflies. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 417 


As sexual selection primarily depends on variability, 
a few words must be added on this subject. In respect to 
color there 1s no difficulty, for any number of highly vari- 
able Lepidoptera could be named. One good instance will 
suffice. Mr. Bates showed me a whole series of specimens 
of Papilio sesostris and P. childrene ; in the latter the males 
varied much in the extent of the beautifully enamelled 
green patch on the fore-wings, and in the size of the white 
mark, and of the splendid crimson stripe on the hind-wings; 
so that there was a great contrast among the males between 
the most and the least gaudy. The male of Papilio sesostris 
is much less beautiful than of P. childrene; and it like- 
wise varies a little in the size of the green patch on the 
fore-wings, and in the occasional appearance of the small 
crimson stripe on the hind-wings, borrowed, as it would 
seem, from its own female; for the females of this and of 
many other species in the Aineas group possess this crimson 
stripe. Hence, between the brightest specimens of Papilio 
sesostris and the dullest of P. childrene, there was but 
a small interval; and it was evident that, as far as mere 
variability is concerned, there would be no difficulty in 
permanently increasing the beauty of either species by means 
of selection. The variability is here almost confined to the 
male sex; but Mr. Wallace and Mr. Bates have shown” that 
the females of some species are extremely variable, the 
males being nearly constant. In a future chapter I shall 
_have occasion to show that the beautiful eye-like spots, 
or ocelli, found on the wings of many Lepidoptera, are 
eminently variable. I may here add that these ocelli offer 
a difficulty on the theory of sexual selection; for though 
appearing to us so ornamental, they are never present in 
one sex and absent in the other, nor do they ever differ 
much in the two sexes. This fact is at present inexplica- 


24 Wallace on the Papilionide of the Malayan Region, in **Transact. Linn, 
Soc.,”’ vol. xxv., 1865, pp. 8, 36. A striking case of a rare variety, strictly 
intermediate between two other well-marked female varieties, is given by Mr, 
Wallace. See also Mr. Bates, in ‘‘Proc. Entomolog. Soc.,’? Nov. 19, 1866, p. xl 

25 Mr, Bates was so kind as to lay this subject before the Entomological 
Society, and I have received answers to this effect from several entomologists, 


418 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


ble; but if it should hereafter be found that the formation 
of an ocellus is due to some change in the tissues of the 
wings, for instance, occurring at a very early period of 
development, we might expect, from what we know of the 
laws of inheritance, that it would be transmitted to both 
sexes, though arising and perfected in one sex alone. 

On the whole, although many serious objections may be 
urged, it seems probable that most of the brilliantly colored 
species of Lepidoptera owe their colors to sexual selection, 
excepting in certain cases, presently to be mentioned, in 
which conspicuous colors have been gained through mimicry 
as a protection. From the ardor of the male throughout 
the animal kingdom, he is generally willing to accept any 
female; and it is the female which usually exerts a choice. 
Hence, if sexual selection has been efficient with the 
Lepidoptera, the male, when the sexes differ, ought to be 
the more brilliantly colored, and this undoubtedly is the 
case. When both sexes are brilliantly colored and resemble 
each other, the characters acquired by the males appear to 
have been transmitted to both. We are led to this conclu- 
sion by cases, even within the same genus, of gradation 
from an extraordinary amount of difference to identity 
in color between the two sexes. 

But it may be asked whether the differences in color 
between the sexes may not be accounted for by other means 
besides sexual selection. Thus the males and females of 
the same species of butterfly are in several cases known” 
to inhabit different stations, the former commonly basking 
in the sunshine, the latter haunting gloomy forests. It is 
therefore possible that different conditions of life may have 
acted directly on the two sexes; but this is not probable,” 
as in the adult state they are exposed to different conditions 
during a very short period; and the larve of both are ex- 


%¢ H. W. Bates, ‘‘The Naturalist in the Amazons,’’ vol. ii., 1863, p. 228, 
A. R. Wallace, in ‘‘Transact. Linn. Soc.,’’ vol. xxv., 1865, p. 10. 

27 On this whole subject see ‘‘The Variation of Animals and Plants under 
Domestication,’’ 1868, vol. ii. chap. xxiii, 


SEXUAL SELECTION 419 


posed to the same conditions. Mr. Wallace believes that 
the difference between the sexes is due not so much to the 
males having been modified, as to the females having, in 
all or almost all cases, acquired dull colors for the sake 
of protection. It seems to me, on the contrary, far more 
probable that it is the males which have been chiefly modi- 
fied through sexual selection, the females having been com- 
paratively little changed. We can thus understand how it 
is that the females of allied species generally resemble 
* one another so much more closely than do the males. They 
thus show us approximately the primordial coloring of the 
parent-species of the group to which they belong. They 
have, however, almost always been somewhat modified by 
the transfer to them of some of the successive variations, 
through the accumulation of which the males were rendered 
beautiful. But I do not wish to deny that the females alone’ 
of some species may have been specially modified for pro- 
tection. In most cases the males and females of distinct 
species will have been exposed during their prolonged larval 
state tc different conditions, and may have been thus 
affeeted; though with the males any slight change of color 
thus caused will generally have been masked by the brilliant 
tints gained through sexual selection. When we treat of 
Birds, I shall have to discuss the whole question, as to how 
far the differences in color between the sexes are due to the 
males having been modified through sexual selection for 
ornamental purposes, or to the females having been modi- 
fied through natural selection for the sake of protection; 
so that I will here say but little on the subject. 

In all the cases in which the more common form of equal 
inheritance by both sexes has prevailed, the selection of 
bright-colored males would tend to make the females bright- 
colored, and the selection of dull-colored females would 
tend to make the males dull. If both processes were carried 
on simultaneously, they would tend to counteract each 
other; and the final result would depend on whether a 
greater number of females from being wel] protected by 


420 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


obscure colors, or a greater number of males by being 
brightly colored and thus finding partners, succeeded in 
leaving more numerous offspring. 

In order to account for the frequent transmission of 
characters to one sex alone, Mr. Wallace expresses his 
belief that the more common form of equal inheritance by 
both sexes can be changed through natural selection into 
inheritance by one sex alone, but in favor of this view I 
can discover no evidence. We know, from what occurs 
under domestication, that new characters often appear, 
which from the first are transmitted to one sex alone; and 
by the selection of such variations there would not be the 
slightest difficulty in giving bright colors to the males alone, 
and at the same time, or subsequently, dull colors to the 
females alone. In this manner the females of some butter- 
flies and moths have, it is probable, been rendered incon- 
spicuous for the sake of protection, and widely different 
from their males. 

IT am, however, unwilling without distinct evidence to 
admit that two complex processes of selection, each requir- 
ing the transference of new characters to one sex alone, 
have been carried on with a multitude of species—that the 
males have been rendered more brilliant by beating their 
rivals, and the females more dull-colored by having escaped 
from their enemies. The male, for instance, of the common 
brimstone butterfly (Gonepteryx) is of a far more intense 
yellow than the female, though she is equally conspicuous; 
and it does not seem probable that she specially acquired 
her pale tints as a protection, though it is probable that 
the male acquired his bright colors as a sexual attraction. 
The female of Anthocharis cardamines does not possess the 
beautiful orange wing-tips of the male; consequently she 
closely resembles the white butterflies (Pieris) so common 
in our gardens; but we have no evidence that this resem- 
blance is beneficial to her. As, on the other hand, she 
resembles both sexes of several other species of the genus 
inhabiting various quarters of the world, it is probable that 


SEXUAL SELECTION 421 


she has simply retained to a large extent her primordial 
colors. 

Finally, as we have seen, various considerations lead to 
the conclusion that with the greater number of brilliantly 
colored Lepidoptera it is the male which has been chiefly 
modified through sexual selection; the amount of difference 
between the sexes mostly depending on the form of inheri- 
tance which has prevailed. Inheritance is governed by so 
many unknown laws or conditions that it seems to us to act 
in a capricious manner;** and we can thus, to a certain 
extent, understand how it is that with closely allied species 
the sexes either differ to an astonishing degree, or are iden- 
tical in color. As all the successive steps in the process of 
variation are necessarily transmitted through the female, 
a greater or less number of such steps might readily become 
developed in her; and thus we can understand the frequent 
gradations from an extreme difference to none at all between 
the sexes of allied species. These cases of gradation, it 
may be added, are much too common to favor the sup- 
position that we here see females actually undergoing the 
process of transition and losing their brightness for the sake 
of protection; for we have every reason to conclude that 
at any one time the greater number of species are in a 
fixed condition. 

Mimicry.—This principle was first made clear in an 
admirable paper by Mr. Bates,** who thus threw a flood of 
light on many obscure problems. It had previously been 
observed that certain butterflies in South America, belong- 
ing to quite distinct families, resembled the Heliconide so 
closely in every stripe and shade of color that they could 
not be distinguished save by an experienced entomologist. 
As the Heliconide are colored in their usual manner, while 
the others depart from the usual coloring of the groups to 
which they belong, it is clear that the latter are the imi- 


%8 “The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,” vol. a. 


ehap. xii. p. 17. uf 
99 “Transact. Linn. Soc.,”? vol. xxiii., 1862, p. 495. 


422 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


tators, and the Heliconide the imitated. Mr. Bates fur- 
ther observed that the imitating species are comparatively 
rare, while the imitated abound, and that the two sets live 
mingled together. From the fact of the Heliconide being 
conspicuous and beautiful insects, yet so numerous in indi- 
viduals and species, he concluded that they must be pro- 
tected from the attacks of enemies by some secretion or 
odor; and this conclusion has now been amply confirmed,” 
especially by Mr. Belt. Hence Mr. Bates inferred that the 
butterflies which imitate the protected species have acquired 
their present marvellously deceptive appearance through va- 
riation and natural selection, in order to be mistaken for the 
protected kinds, and thus to escape being devoured. No 
explanation is here attempted of the brilliant colors °-* +he 
imitated, but only of the imitating butterflies. We mus 
account for the colors of the former in the same general 
manner as in the cases previously discussed in this chap- 
ter. Since the publication of Mr. Bates’s paper, similar and 
equally striking facts have been observed by Mr. Wallace 
in the Malayan region, by Mr. Trimen in South Africa, and 
by Mr. Riley in the United States.™ 

As some writers have felt much difficulty in understand- 
ing how the first steps in the process of mimicry could have 
been effected through natural selection, it may be well to 
remark that the process probably commenced long ago, 
between forms not widely dissimilar in color. In this case 
even a slight variation would be beneficial, if it rendered 
the one species more like the other; and afterward the 
imitated species might be modified to an extreme degree 
through sexual selection or other means; and if the changes 
were gradual, the imitators might easily be led along the 
same track, until they differed to an equally extreme de- 


30 «Proc, Ent. Soc.,’’ Dec. 3, 1866, p. xlv. 

2! Wallace, ‘‘Transact. Linn. Soc.,’’ vol. xxv., 1865, p. i.* also ‘Transact. 
Ent. Soc.,”’ vol. iv. (8d series), 1867, p. 301. Trimen, “‘Linn. Transact.,’’ vol. 
xxvi., 1869, p. 497. Riley, ‘‘Third Annual Report on the Noxious Insects of 
Missouri,’’ 1841, pp. 163-168. This latter essay is valuable, as Mr. Riley here 
discusses all objections which have keen raised against Mr. Bates’s theory. 


SEXUAL SELECTION 423 


gree from their original condition; and they would thus 
ultimately assume an appearance or coloring wholly unlike 
that of the other members of the group to which they be- 
longed. It should also be remembered that many species of 
Lepidoptera are liable to considerable and abrupt variations 
in color. A few instances have been given in this chapter; 
and many more may be found in the papers of Mr. Bates 
and Mr. Wallace. 

With several species the sexes are alike, and imitate the 
two sexes of another species. But Mr. Trimen gives, in 
the paper already referred to, three cases in which the sexes 
of the imitated form differ from each other in color, and the 
sexes of the imitating form differ in a like manner. Several 
cases have also been recorded where the females alone imi- 
tate brilliantly colored and protected species, the males re- 
taining ‘‘the normal aspect of their immediate congeners.'’” 
It is here obvious that the successive variations by which 
the female has been modified have been transmitted to her 
alone. It is, however, probable that some of the many 
successive variations would have been transmitted to, 
and developed in, the males, had not such males been 
eliminated by being thus rendered less attractive to the 
females; so that only those variations were preserved which 
were from the first strictly limited in their transmission to 
the female sex. We have a partial illustration of these re- 
marks in a statement by Mr. Belt,” that the males of some 
of the Leptalides, which imitate protected species, still re- 
tain in a concealed manner some of their original charac- 
ters. Thus in the males ‘‘the upper half of the lower wing 
is of a pure white, while all the rest of the wings is barred 
and spotted with black, red, and yellow, like the species 
they mimic. The females have not this white patch, and 
the males usually conceal it by covering it with the upper 
wing, so that I cannot imagine its being of any other use to 
them than as an attraction in courtship, when they exhibit it 


2 “The Naturalist in Nicaragua,’? 1874, p. 385. ~ 


424 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


to the females, and thus gratify their deep-seated preference 
for the normal color of the Order to which the Leptalides 
belong.”’ 


Bright Colors of Caterpillars.—While reflecting on the 
beauty of many butterflies, it occurred to me that some 
caterpillars were splendidly colored; and as sexual selec- 
tion could not possibly have here acted, it appeared rash 
to attribute the beauty of the mature insect to this agency, 
unless the bright colors of their larve could be somehow 
explained. In the first place, it may be observed that the 
colors of caterpillars do not stand in any close correlation 
with those of the mature insect. Secondly, their bright 
colors do not serve in any ordinary manner as a protec- 
tion. Mr. Bates informs me, as an instance of this, that 
the most conspicuous caterpillar which he ever beheld 
(that of a Sphinx) lived on the large green leaves of a 
tree on the open llanos of South America; it was about 
four inches in length, transversely banded with black and 
yellow, and with its head, legs, and tail of a bright red. 
Hence it caught the eye of any one who passed by, even — 
at the distance of many yards, and no doubt that of every 
passing bird. 

I then applied to Mr. Wallace, who has an innate genius 
for solving difficulties. After some consideration he replied: 
‘*Most caterpillars require protection, as may be inferred 
from some kinds being furnished with spines or irritating 
hairs, and from many being colored green like the leaves 
on which they feed, or being curiously like the twigs of 
the trees on which they live.’’ Another instance of pro- 
tection, furnished me by Mr. J. Mansel Weale, may be 
added, namely, that there is a caterpillar of a moth which 
lives on the mimosas in South Africa, and fabricates for 
itself a case quite indistinguishable from the surrounding 
thorns. From such considerations Mr. Wallace thought it 
probable that conspicuously colored caterpillars were pro- 
tected by having a nauseous taste; but as their skin is ex- 


SEXUAL SELECTION 425 


tremely tender, and as their intestines readily protrude from 
@ wound, a slight peck from the beak of a bird would be as 
fatal to them as if they had been devoured. Hence, as Mr. 
Wallace remarks, ‘‘distastefulness alone would be insuffi- 
cient to protect a caterpillar, unless some outward sign 
indicated to its would-be destroyer that its prey was a 
disgusting morsel.’’ Under these circumstances it would 
be highly advantageous to a caterpillar to be instantane- 
ously and certainly recognized as unpalatable by all birds 
and other animals. Thus the most gaudy colors would be 
serviceable, and might have been gained by variation and 
the survival of the most easily recognized individuals. 

This hypothesis appears at first sight very bold, but when 
it was brought before the Entomological Society® it was sup- 
ported by various statements; and Mr. J. Jenner Weir, who 
keeps a large number of birds in an aviary, informs me that 
he has made many trials, and finds no exception to the rule, 
that all caterpillars of nocturnal and retiring habits with 
smooth skins, all of a green color, and all which imitate 
twigs, are greedily devoured by his birds. The hairy and 
spinose kinds are invariably rejected, as were four conspicu- 
ously colored species. When the birds rejected a caterpillar, 
they plainly showed, by shaking their heads and cleansing 
their beaks, that they were disgusted by the taste.“ Three 
conspicuous kinds of caterpillars and moths were also given 
to some lizards and frogs, by Mr. A. Butler, and were re- 
jected, though other kinds were eagerly eaten. Thus the 
probability of Mr. Wallace’s view is confirmed, namely, that 
certain caterpillars have been made conspicuous for their 
own good, so as to be easily recognized by their enemies, 
on nearly the same principle that poisons are sold in colored 


33 **Proe, Entomolog. Soc.,’? Dec. 3, 1866, p. xlv., and March 4, 186%, 
= mee Mr, J. Jenner Weir’s paper on Insects and Insectivorous Birds, in 
‘Transact. Ent. Soc.,’? 1869, p. 21; also Mr. Butler’s paper, ibid., p. 27. Mr. 
Riley has given analogous facts in the “‘Third Annual Report on the N oxious 
Insects of Missouri,’ 1871, p. 148. Some opposed cases are, however, given 
by Dr. Wallace and M. H. d’Orville; see ‘‘Zoological Record,’’ 1869, p. 349. 


426 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


bottles by druggists for the good of man. We cannot, how- 
ever, at present thus explain the elegant diversity in the col- 
ors of many caterpillars; but any species which had at some 
former period acquired a dull, mottled, or striped appear- 
ance, either in imitation of surrounding objects, or from the 
direct action of climate, etc., almost certainly would not be- 
come uniform in color, when its tints were rendered intense 
and bright; for in order to make a caterpillar merely con- 
spicuous, there would be no selection in any definite direction. 


Summary and Concluding Remarks on Insects.—Looking 
back to the several Orders, we see that the sexes often differ 
in various characters, the meaning of which is not in the 
least understood. The sexes, also, often differ in their or- 
gans of sense and means of locomotion, so that the males 
may quickly discover and reach the females. They differ 
still oftener in the males possessing diversified contrivances 
for retaining the females when found. We are, however, 
here concerned only in a secondary degree with sexual 
differences of these kinds. 

In almost all the Orders, the males of some species, even 
of weak and delicate kinds, are known to be highly pugna- 
cious; and some few are furnished with special weapons for 
fighting with their rivals. But the law of battle does not 
prevail nearly so widely with insects as with the higher 
animals. Hence it probably arises, that it is in only a few 
cases that the males have been rendered larger and stronger 
than the females. On the contrary, they are usually smaller, 
so that they may be developed within a shorter time, to be 
ready in large numbers for the emergence of the females. 

In two families of the Homoptera and in three of the 
QOrthoptera, the males alone possess sound-producing organs 
i1an efficient state. These are used incessantly during the 
Lreeding-season, not only for calling the females, but appar- 
ently for charming or exciting them in rivalry with other 
males. No one who admits the agency of selection of any 
kind, will, after reading the above discussion, dispute that 


SEXUAL SELECTION 427 


these musical instruments have been acquired through sex- 
ual selection. In four other Orders the members of one 
sex, or more commonly of both sexes, are provided with 
organs for producing various sounds, which apparently 
serve merely as call-notes. When both sexes are thus 
provided, the individuals which were able to make the 
loudest or most continuous noise would gain partners be- 
fore those which were less noisy, so that their organs have 
probably been gained through sexual selection. It is in- 
structive to reflect on the wonderful diversity of the means 
for producing sound, possessed by the males alone, or by 
both sexes, in no less than six Orders. We thus learn how 
effectual sexual selection has been in leading to modifica- 
tions which sometimes, as with the Homoptera, relate to 
important parts of the organization. 

From the reasons assigned in the last chapter, it is prob- 
able that the great horns possessed by the males of many 
Lamellicorn, and some other beetles, have been acquired as 
ornaments. From the small size of insects, we are apt to 
undervalue their appearance. If we could imagine a male 
Chalcosoma (Fig. 16), with its polished bronzed coat of mail, 
and its vast complex horns, magnified to the size of a horse, 
or even of a dog, it would be one of the most imposing 
animals in the world. 

The coloring of insects is a complex and obscure subject. 
When the male differs slightly from the female, and neither 
is brilliantly colored, it is probable that the sexes have 
varied in a slightly different manner, and that the variations 
have been transmitted by each sex to the same, without any 
benefit or evil thus accruing. When the male is brilliantly 
colored and differs conspicuously from the female, as with 
some dragon-flies and many butterflies, it is probable that 
he owes his colors to sexual selection; while the female 
has retained a primordial or very ancient type of coloring, 
slightly modified by the agencies before explained. But in 
some cases the female has apparently been made obscure 
by variations transmitted to her alone, as a means of direct 


428 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


protection; and it is almost certain that she has sometimes 
been made brilliant, so as to imitate other protected species 
inhabiting the same district. When the sexes resemble each 
other and both are obscurely colored, there is no doubt that 
they have been in a multitude of cases so colored for the 
sake of protection. So it is in some instances when both 
are brightly colored, for they thus imitate protected species, 
or resemble surrounding objects, such as flowers; or they 
give notice to their enemies that they are unpalatable. In 
other cases in which the sexes resemble each other and are 
both brilliant, especially when the colors are arranged for 
display, we may conclude that they have been gained by 
the male sex as an attraction, and have been transferred to 
the female. We are more especially led to this conclusion 
whenever the same type of coloration prevails throughout 
a whole group, and we find that the males of some species 
differ widely in color from the females, while others differ 
slightly or not at all, with intermediate gradations connect- 
ing these extreme states. 

In the same manner as bright colors have often been 
partially transferred from the males to the females, so it 
has been with the extraordinary horns of many Lamellicorn 
and some other beetles. So, again, the sound-producing 
organs proper to the males of the Homoptera and Orthop- 
tera have generally been transferred in a rudimentary, or 
even in a nearly perfect condition, to the femaies; yet not 
sufficiently perfect to be of any use. It is also an interest- 
ing fact, as bearing on sexual selection, that the stridulating 
organs of certain male Orthoptera are not fully developed 
until the last moult; and that the colors of certain male 
dragon-flies are not fully developed until some little time 
after their emergence from the pupal state, and when they 
are ready to breed. 

Sexual selection implies that the more attractive individ- 
uals are preferred by the opposite sex; and as with insects, 
when the sexes differ, it is the male which, with some rare 
exceptions, is the more ornamented, and departs more from 


SEXUAL SELECTION 429 


the type to which the species belongs; and as it is the male 
which searches eagerly for the female, we must suppose that 
the females habitually or occasionally prefer the more beau- 
tiful males, and that these have thus acquired their beauty. 
That the females in most or all the orders would have the 
power of rejecting any particular male, is probable from 
the many singular contrivances possessed by the males, 
such as great jaws, adhesive cushions, spines, elongated 
legs, etc., for seizing thc female; for these contrivances 
show that there is some difficulty in the aci, so that her 
concurrence would seem necessary. Judging from what 
we know of the perceptive powers and affections of vari- 
ous insects, there is no antecedent improbability in sexual 
selection having come largely into play; but we have as yet 
no direct evidence on this head, and some facts are opposed 
to the belief. Nevertheless, when we see many males pur- 
suing the same female, we can hardly believe that the pair- 
ing is left to blind chance—that the female exerts no choice, 
and is not influenced by the gorgeous colors or other orna- 
ments with which the male is decorated. 

If we admit that the females of the Homoptera and 
Orthoptera appreciate the musical tones of their male part- 
ners, and that the various instruments have been perfected 
through sexual selection, there is little improbability in the 
females of other insects appreciating beauty in form or color, 
and consequently in such characters having been thus gained 
by the males. But from the circumstance of color being so 
variable, and from its having been so often modified for the 
sake of protection, it is difficult to decide in how large a pro- 
portion of cases sexual selection has played a part. This is 
more especially difficult in those Orders, such as Orthoptera, 
Hymenoptera, and Coleoptera, in which the two sexes rarely 
differ much in color; for we are then left to mere analogy. 
With the Coleoptera, however, as before remarked, it is in 
the great Lamellicorn group, placed by some authors at the 
head of the Order, and in which we sometimes see a mutual 
attachment between the sexes, that we find the males of some 


430 THE DESCENT OF MAN 


species possessing weapons for sexual strife, others furnished 

with wonderful horns, many with stridulating organs, and 
others ornamented with splendid metallic tints. Hence it 
seems probable that all these characters have been gained 
through the same means, namely, sexual selection. With 
butterflies we have the best evidence, as the males some- 
times take pains to display their beautiful colors; and we 
cannot believe that they would act thus, unless the display 
was of use to them in their courtship. 

When we treat of Birds, we shall see that they present 
in their secondary sexual characters the closest analogy with 
insects. Thus many male birds are highly pugnacious, and 
some are furnished with special weapons for fighting with 
their rivals. They possess organs which are used during 
the breeding-season for producing vocal and instrumental 
music. They are frequently ornamented with combs, horns, 
wattles, and plumes of the most diversified kinds, and are 
decorated with beautiful colors, all evidently for the sake 
of display. We shall find that, as with insects, both sexes 
in certain groups are equally beautiful, and are equally pro- 
vided with ornaments which are usually confined to the male 
sex. In other groups both sexes are equally plain-colored 
and unornamented. Lastly, in some few anomalous cases 
the females are more beautiful than the males. We shall 
often find, in the same group of birds, every gradation from 
no difference between the sexes to an extreme difference. 
We shall see that female birds, like female insects, often 
possess more or less plain traces or rudiments of characters 
which properly belong to the males and are of use only to 
them. The analogy, indeed, in all these respects between 
birds and insects is curiously close. Whatever explanation 
applies to the one class probably applies to the other; and 
this explanation, as we shall hereafter attempt to show in 
further detail, is sexual selection. 


END OF VOLUME ONE