Skip to main content

Full text of "The home life of wild birds; a new method of the study and photography of birds"

See other formats


se 


Bites 


Spies ses 


ower 
ribet eae 


ses 


Se 
Sia 
tie 


TEPER SES PR EERE ROR Be 


eres 


Sct 


Cornell University 


Library 


The original of this book is in 
the Cornell University Library. 


There are no known copyright restrictions in 
the United States on the use of the text. 


http://www.archive.org/details/cu31924090283627 


ALBERT R. MANN 
LIBRARY 


Cornett University 


Gift of 


Wiliam E. Davis, Jr. 


CORNELL UNIVERSITY LIBRARY 


DATE DUE 


Robin Family 


Cock Robin, who stands at the back, has just brought 
and delivered a load of wild red cherries.. His mate, who 
was brooding at the moment, did not leave the nest, but 


hopped to one side, and presently returned to her post, 
where you see her in the picture. 


THE HOME LIFE 
OF WILD BIRDS 


A New Method of 
the Study and 
Photography of Birds 


BY 


Francis Hopart HERRICK 


With 141 Original Wlustrations from Wature 
by the Hutbor 


G. P. PUTNAM’S SONS 


The Rnickerbocker Press 


NEW YORK AND LONDON 
1901 


CopyriGHT, MAy, I90I 
’ 
BY 


FRANCIS HOBART HERRICK 


Set up and electrotyped May, rgoz. Reprinted July, 1907 


lens 
~ Ohi - 


The Tnickerbocker Press, Tew Work 


TO THE MEMORY 
OF 


MY FATHER AND MOTHER 


COME LEAT GS LIVE WITH THE BIRDS! 


PREP ACE: 


N studying the habits of wild birds two important problems are at once encountered, 
that of approach and the control of the position of the nest. My first experiments 
were made with Redwing Blackbirds and Cedar Waxwings, and I soon perceived 
that an important principle was involved, which every subsequent experiment tended to 
confirm. Wishing to test its value as fully as possible, every available nest which came 
to hand was utilized, without the exercise of choice in regard to species. 

The observations were made for the most part in central New Hampshire, in the 
towns of Northfield and Tilton, and pertain to the common birds of the country. 


I am indebted to my sister for many practical and valuable suggestions. 


FRANCIS HOBART HERRICK. 


WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY, 
ADELBERT COLLEGE, CLEVELAND, OHIO, April, Igor, 


CONTENTS. 
CHAPTER 
PREFACE 


LIsT OF ILLUSTRATIONS . 
INTRODUCTION . 
I.—A New METHOD OF BIRD STUDY AND PHOTOGRAPHY 
II.—ILLUSTRATIONS OF THE METHOD: THE CEDAR-BIRD; THE BALTIMORE 
ORIOLE; THE REDWING BLACKBIRD, AND THE KINGBIRD . 
IIJ.—TENT AND CAMERA: THE TOOLS OF BIRD-PHOTOGRAPHY 
IV.—THE ROBIN AT ARM’S LENGTH; A STUDY OF INDIVIDUALITY 
V.—THE CEDAR-BIRD . : : : : ‘ ‘ 4 . 
VI.—RED-EYED VIREOS 
VII.—THE NEST-HOLE OF THE BLUEBIRD 
VIII.—MINUTE OBSERVATIONS ON CATBIRDS 
IX.—THE REARING OF THE NIGHT Hawk 
X.—THE KINGFISHERS AND THEIR KING Row. . $ : 
XI.—CARE OF YOUNG AND NEST. : : : : ; ‘ 3 
I.—BROODING AND FEEDING THE YOUNG. 
II.--CLEANING THE NEST. 
XII.—THE ForcE OF HABIT . : : ‘ : : F : ei 
XIII.—FEAR IN BIRDS 
XIV.—TAMING WILD BIRDS WITHOUT A CAGE 


INDEX 


vil 


vill 


ILLUSTRATIONS, 


PAGE 
Robin Family. Cock Robin, who stands at the back, has just brought and delivered a 
load of wild red cherries. His mate, who was brooding at the moment, did 
not leave the nest, but hopped to one side, and presently returned to her post, 
where you see her in the picture. Lens 97% inch focus; speed %; stop 323 
time 4 second; plate, Seeds’ No. 27 ‘‘ gilt edge’’ (which should be under- 
stood as generally used in this work) ; distance of object 4 feet; full sun; 
July 28, r900 lt. ‘ ‘ : : : ; : ; ; : Front splece 
Adult Cedar-bird . : ; i ; : : : ; ‘ . Title 
Footprints of Kingfisher when nee ehiés ee old Imprint from living bird : ‘ il 
Head of Cock Robin with large katydid and angleworm in bill . : : : : lv 
In the hill country of New Hampshire, overlooking Northfield and ‘Tilton, which is 
screened by the hill in foreground. To the south, on the left, stands Mt. 
Kearsarge ; toward the northern horizon Ragged Mt. ; ; vl 
Automatic signclen elite of a young bird, illustrated in a Cedar Waxwing 12 see old 
C2e « : ; 3 , : ; : ; : ; ; : . vil 
Chipping Guam aiming” at a yellow “target”. : : : : : ! . vill 
Kingbird perched in characteristic manner above its nest in an apple tree : ; .  Xiv 
Head of Red-tailed Hawk with frill erect. Four months old. : hs eI 
Fic. 1.—Observation tent beside Cedar-bird’s nest, which was taken w ith its brant from 
pine tree and carried to open field. At this nest the series shown on pages 11, 
57-61 was made : : : : : : . : : 2 
Fic. 2.—Tent in bushy pasture, marking position of nest of Chestnut-sided Warblers 4 
Fic. 3.—Nearer view of same tent and nest, showing brooding bird : 5 
Fic. 4.—Truncated elm, riddled by a the lower nest-hole recently Aen 
by Bluebirds 6 
Fic. 5.—Tent and Bluebird’s nest. ‘Compare Pies. 10, 59- és 
Fic. 6,—kKingbird’s nesting tree, and nesting branch—removed and mounted on stalees 
—with tent. The tent-cloth is laid in position at one end of peak, and ready 
to be drawn over frame. The Kingbird pictures were all made on this spot. 8 
Fic. 7.—Female Kingbird astride nest,—the later brooding attitude. ‘ : : 9 
Fic. 8.—Kingbird family, the female partly hidden at the back. It was an easy matter 
to focus directly upon the head of the standing or brooding bird . , : 9 
Fic. 9.—Cedar-bird about to feed young by regurgitation. Photographed at the nest 
shown in Figs. 1, 12, and 13. Zeiss Anastigmat, Ser. ii a, 64 inch; speed {; 
distance about 30 inches, in fullsun : : : : ; : = aT 


1x 


5. 30 
3 3s Conk feeding cluster of cant ingbede, 
S. 32.—Cock standing at nest immediately after serving 5 feed, ad acy for the duty af 


Illustrations 


. I10.—Female Bluebird with cricket at converted nest-hole of Flicker 


11.—Female Chestnut-sided Warbler standing over young. Compare Fig. 3 


. 12.—Cedar-bird’s nest in pine, 15 feet from the ground. Upstretched neck of the 


old bird could be seen at a point just beneath the upper arrow-head. Nesting 
bough carried to field beyond, and mounted as shown in Figs. 1 and 13 
13.—Nesting branch of Cedar-bird set up in field and tent pitched beside it. Com- 
pare Figs. 1 and 12 : : : : 
14.—Oriole inspecting young. Still timid to a degree 


. 15.—Oriole inspecting nest. Behavior freer than in last 


16.—Tent in swamp, fronting Redwing Blackbird’s nest 


;. 17.—kKingbird feeding young, and balancing herself with uplifted wings é 
>. 18.—Male Kingbird serving a Cicada or harvest-fly, which a youngster is striving to 


master. Its efforts were not in vain 


>. I9.—Unequal contest between Kingbirds and a drasow: fy. This insect was crushed 


and served up piecemeal 


. 20.—Kingbirds serving a dragon-fly, whose wings anit seiclk: Re body are seen pro- 


truding from the mouth of one of the young . 


. 21.—Male Redwing Blackbird feeding a young one 


oe ’ 


22.—Female “' bristling ’’ to keep cool, while shielding the young on morning of a 
hot day. July 11, rg00 ; 

23 —Kingbird out of its nest at age of eighiteen days; wits power of flight wall de- 
veloped. July 13, 1900 

24.—Tent, folded for carrying, cameras, and plate. bap=the eee of bitd: photography 


. 25.—Brown Thrush entering her nest to brood 2 : 
. 26.—Robin in an April snow. Wade Park, Cleveland, ‘Olio, April 4, 1900. X 3 
. 27.—Head of Cock Robin, life-size x 44 : : : : : : : 
s. 28.—Head of female Robin, life-size x 44. The slime on her bill is from the 


throat of a young bird 


: (aie Robin brooding 


inspection and cleaning 
33.—Female Robin cleaning nest 


>, 34.—Head of brooding female, life-size x 2} : : 
+. 35.—Cedar-bird chorus. The young, with wings spiead ead a- auivee with open 


mouth and upstretched necks are calling to the silent mother. Life-size x 3 


G. 36.—Cedar-bird family group, the male with full throat and black cherry in bill 
+. 37.—Cedar-bird approaching nest of young which are nearly ready to fly 
5. 38.—The same bird standing at nest with full gullet,—a little later in day after one 


of the young had left 


‘1G. 39.—The Cedar-bird approaches with alosea bill Buk full ‘heoat : : 
‘1G. 40.—After feeding she inspects the young and in this instance appears to be sitting 


with tail resting on the branch, but this is probably not the case 


‘IG, 41.—She tosses up her head, and produces a cherry 


‘1G. 42.—She is startled at a strange sound 


:. 43.—She looks curiously at the tent while fae peneins nest 


PAGE 
13 
14 


18 


Fic. 
Fic. 4 
Fic. 
Fie, 


Fic. 


Illustrations 


44.—She stands like a statuette while inspecting her family 
45.—She devours what is sometimes removed from the nest 
46.—The sac is taken directly from the cloaca of the young bird 
47.—A young Waxwing from this nest on the morning of flight, in natural atibuce 
expressive of fear. July 19, 1900 : : ; : E : . 
48.—Cedar-bird, thirty-six hours old. ‘Typical instinctive response to sound or 
vibration of nest. The stub-wings are used for support 


Cedar-bird standing at nest after delivery of food 


Fic 


Fic, 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 


Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Vic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 


Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 


Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 


Fic. 


Fic. 


Fic. 


49.—Male Red-eyed Vireo standing over nest. Life-size x3 . 
50.—Female Red-eyed Vireo with a neuropterous (?) insect in bill 
51.—Feeding a nestling ; 
52.—Female inspecting the nest in thie chainaniie manner 
53.—Male standing over the young . : : : : ; ; : 
54.—Female Red-eyed Vireo cautiously inspecting nest. Compare with the freer 
manner illustrated in Fig. 52. July 5, 1900 . 
5.—Approaching to inspect the nest and drawing back . 
6.—Cautiously feeding the young 
57.—Inspecting nest at a distance : 
58. oF oung birds from this nest just before flight 
59.—Female Bluebird on point of ooo nest-hole 
60.—Taking grasshopper to young . : 
61,—Standing at entrance with green insect-larva in eal, 
62.—Female Bluebird cleaning the nest 
63.—Cleaning the nest 
64.—Cleaning the nest ; : 
65.—Taking a final glance around bore entering nest- Hole anh eee : 
66.—Female Catbird bringing to young a limp dragon-fly, the large Lschuna heros, 
which has just issued from its pupa-skin 
67.—Catbird inspecting her young 
68.—Catbird cleaning the nest 
69.—Night Hawk on bare ground, and eposhells oa high: it eiveneed three days 
before 
70.—Night Hawk apenas dines dove ola 
71.—Night Hawk about nine days old 
72.—Night Hawk about twelve days old . 
73.—Night Hawk about sixteen days old 
74.—Front-face view of bird shown in Fig. 72 : : 
75.—Young Night Hawk in enclosure w ae it retuned ww ale: to fly 
76.—Tunnel of Kingfisher (on the right) in sand-bank overgrown with pines, pei 
country road. Northfield, New Hampshire. August, 1899 
77.—Nest of rg00 in same bank and pinbal bly of same pair. Kingfisher taking fish 
to young. Lens 93’; inch; speed ; stop 8; time #4; second; distance g ft. 8 
n.; fullsun. July 24, 1900 : ; : : ; : : : 
78.—Kingfisher backing out of tunnel. A stream of sand is started from the opening 
at every entrance and exit 
79.—Five Kingfishers from chamber at end of nels patextinatety nine ealtigs sia. 


July 19, 1900 


os mn 


Xi 


PAGE 


Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 


Illustrations 


x. 80.—Posed in line, biting and pulling 

. 81.—Posed in row to illustrate habit of sitting still 

Gc, 82.—King-row at a later stage—birds thirteen days old 

+, 83.—Kingfisher at nine days, showing feather tubes and tracts 


84.—At thirteen days. The wing-quills show one half inch of the blue- fae eit: 
tipped feather-shafts 


>. 85.—Kingfisher at fifteen days. Nearly all feathers patily dinghsathed 
. 86.—liingfishers eighteen days old. The bright blue of the upper parts and the 


white and chestnut bands across the breast are now very prominent 


. 87.—Kingfishers twenty-two days old. To illustrate how they break ranks and walk 


backwards, when placed in line. The second on the left has already taken a 
few backward steps 


. 88.—Female Brown Thrush brooding: Lens 974 inch; speed %; stop 32; time 4 sec- 


ond; distance 4 feet in full sun. July 13, rg0o 


c. 89.—Female Robin brooding. Attitude of keen attention 


go.—Female Redwing Blackbird feeding a young bird 


: ee same bird awaiting the reflex response of the throat and eullet of young. 


If not forthcoming, the food is withdrawn, and another is tested 


. 92.—Female Kingbird standing over young with drooping wings to ward off the sun. 


Typical brooding attitude during last days of life at nest 


. 93.—Kingbirds rending an unruly grasshopper 

sc. 94.—Helping a grampus down the throat of a Kingbird 

. 95.—The male grampus, Corydalus cornutus. Full size, from life 

s. 96.—Female Chestnut-sided Warbler bristling to keep cool while brooding on a hae 


June day 


. 97.—The same bird in the more common aeiuide of ‘Boeliae ain the carly life af 


the young. Lens Zeiss Anastigmat, Ser. ii a; 64 inch; speed 4; stop 32; 
time + second; distance 3 feet in full sun. June 23, 1900 : ; : 
98.—Male Chestnut-sided Warbler brings food for his little children. His mate, who 
is brooding, receives it into her own bill, but does not taste a particle herself. 


G. 99.—Female Chestnut-sided Warbler brooding with throat puffed out and head- 


feathers erect ; ; : . 
too.—Female Brown Thrush seeing fond well down i in ‘the throat, Point of bill is 
on level with external ear of young 
1o1.—The same bird cleaning the nest 
102.—Cedar-bird taking sac from cloaca of young 
103.—Female Kingbird cleaning the nest 
104.—Baltimore Oriole feeding its young 
105.—The same bird in another attitude . 


A Hatful of Kingfishers 


Fic, 
Fic. 


Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 


106.—Male Redwing Blackbird iepediig nest : ; ‘ ; 5 : 

107.—The same bird engaged in the same occupation. To illustrate the formation 
of habits in the daily routine 

108.—Cock with a large a 

10g.—Cock “‘ taking aim”’ 

110.—Cock ready to inspect the nest 

r11.—Female Robin inspecting the nest in a ‘rnieal: aeetude 


IOL 
IOlL 


I02 


102 


103 


103 


104 
105 
106 
107 
108 
109 
TIO 
112 


II2 
113 
113 
114 
114 


Fic. 


Fic. 
Figs 
Fic. 


Fic. 
Fic. 


Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 
Fic. 


Fic; 


Fic. 


Illustrations 


112.—Female Kingbird inserting an insect in the throat of a fledgling 

113.—Cedar-bird’s eggs with two blind and naked young oe six hours old 

114.—The same : : : 

115.—The same, aucceatins different phases ae instinctive Henao 

116.—Kingfishers twenty-four days old, perce to illustrate fearlessness, when cepa 
of flight : 

117.—Young Cedar-birds at nest, in hata teristic atutiide: Ginger the intone af 
fear and ready for flight. For pia oaaa see page 60 

118.—Brown Thrush startled on nest 

119.—Cock Robin startled by alarm call of his mate i 

120.—Red-tailed Hawk worried. Instinctive attitude expressive of oe nd serving 
to inspire fear : 

121.—Young Cowbird sanding at nest of Mannaie W ales its fester narents 

122.—Young Cowbird, as it appeared when found, completely filling the nest, having 
smothered its rightful occupants . ; : 

123.—Male Kingbird standing at nest, and young in eharactertete attitude 

124.—Female Robin, engaged in nest-cleaning 

125.—Female Red-eyed Vireo feeding the young 

126.—Her mate ready to inspect and clean the nest 

127.—Offering food to a Chestnut-sided Warbler, tamed etnout a cage : 

128.—Chestnut-sided Warbler family, the male above the nest on which sits his 
brooding mate 

129.—Female Chestnut-sided W arblet taking a peep at the nest, W hich then soneitned 
eggs or young birds barely hatched 

130.—The same bird inspecting her young after feeding nen 


Xill 
PAGE 
115 
118 
118 
118 


INTRODUCTION. 


a 


O describe and illustrate a new means of studying animal behavior, and to record 
a what has been learned by its aid concerning the lives of some of our common 
birds is the main purpose of this volume. It is a popular study of birds in action 

and is chiefly concerned with the homes or nests and their occupants. 

While the desire has been present to make these pages readable, no effort has been 
spared to render them accurate. Many of the observations are new; nearly all are 
original, and every statement of fact is believed to be true as it stands. 

The wish to give a human interest to every phase of animal activity is of very ancient 
origin and has done too much already in spreading the seeds of popular error and super- 
stition concerning animal life and lore. Animals should be studied as animals which they 
are, and not as human beings which they have never been and are not likely ever to 
become. 

The constant reading of human attributes into the activities of animals is to begin at 
the wrong end, and isadrag on the progress of accurate knowledge. We should first study 
the animal as far as possible from its own standpoint, and learn with exactness the facts 
of its life, taking care not to press analogies farther than the observed facts will warrant. 
Ignorance of anatomy as well as of physiology, and the desire to find in the doings of ani- 
mals a marvelous counterpart of human powers of intelligence and reason have already 
stocked our libraries with fables, anecdotes, and stories, many of which make delightful 
reading, but possess little value for the modern student. 

The first duty of the narrator of natural as well as civil history is to tell the truth, 
and to the naturalist belongs also the privilege of showing that the lives of the higher ani- 
mals, when fully and clearly revealed, possess a more vital interest than the puppet dressed 
in human clothes, however admirable the latter may be as a work of art. 

I trust that the reader will not misunderstand these remarks. Is it denied that 
animals possess intelligence or any powers of reason? Not at all! Such questions de- 
pend largely upon our definitions of words, and without fresh observations are usually 
fruitless of result. What is criticized is the gross anthropomorphism which characterizes 
much that is written upon the actions of animals. If I am an offender in this direction, 
I hope it is only in a minor degree. I am anxious to attribute to the animal every power 
which it is actually known to possess, and look for the roots of human instinct and intel- 


XV 


xvi Introduction. 


ligence all along the line of animal evolution. It tends only to confusion, however, to 
call those acts of association which lead to acquired habits, instincts, or the countless 
mechanical or chemical reactions of organisms to external stimuli, the expressions of 
intelligence and thought. ‘Go to the ant thou sluggard!”’ is good advice, but one should 
bring from the ant a trustworthy account of how it performs its wonderful works. It is im- 
portant to distinguish the root from the bud, as well as from the perfected flower and fruit. 

Although this is not a treatise on animal behavior, a general working theory has been 
adopted and will now be given. Every animal at birth inherits with its bodily organs the 
power to use them in a more or less definite way, and all but the lowest animals, of which 
the Protozoa, jelly-fishes, and possibly the worms may be taken as representatives, acquire 
some power of learning to do things in the course of their lives. Their equipment thus 
consists of (1) unlearned or inherited powers, and (2) of learned or acquired abilities, which 
are the results of experience—often very bitter. The term “ instinct ” when used in a very 
broad sense may be given to all inherited or ingrained tendencies, and ‘‘ habit” reserved 
for what is acquired or learned through a process of association of certain things with 
certain acts. An animal’s powers thus consist of free gifts at its start in life, and later 
acquisitions gained through its own efforts in the struggle for existence. 

The catalogue of instinctive acts—even in the narrower sense of involving anumber 
of different organs—is surprisingly great in an animal standing so high in the scale as the 
bird, but examples drawn from a single species will suffice. When the spring comes the 
young bird, who returns to the place of its birth, is prompted to find a mate, and with her 
soon begins to build a nest. Though unattended by instructors and unprepared by prac- 
tice, it uses the inherited tools of its guild—bill, breast, and feet — with a nice precision, 
and be it Oriole, Robin, Flycatcher, or Vireo, follows with wonderful closeness the type of 
architecture which its ancestors have used for ages. 

Why does the Robin in its first attempt at nest-building begin by laying a foundation 
of dry grass or stubble, and add to this mud softened with water and made intoa mortar, 
which it then heaps about its breast and molds into a symmetrical cup, often selecting a 
rainy day for the work? One might as well ask why the Robin lays blue eggs, or why it 
utters its well known call. It acts in these ways because it must, because Robins have 
been doing these things for hundreds of generations. It not only inherits tools, but a cer- 
tain aptitude for their use. Its organization compels or determines its actions. 

No learning of such initial actions is required or even possible since all this has been 
attended to, as one might say, centuries before the animal was born. These instinctive 
responses are spontaneous, and when the right button is pressed or the right stimulus ap- 
plied from without or within, the reaction follows as a matter of course. Of course the 
Robin must make a mortar of mud and straw; of course it must lay blue eggs, and after 
incubating them, carefully rear and feed its young. To do otherwise would not only be 
absurd, but very uncomfortable. Had its ancestors been Cowbirds it would have made 
no nest at all, but filched another’s, and foisting its eggs upon some simple minded nurse, 
shirked the duties of parents to their offspring. The Cowbird was thus very early to enter 
the field of experimental psychology. 

Every bird must follow the laws of its nature, and its inherited instincts are no more 
wonderful than its inherited organs,—its vocal cords, its keen eyes, and its marvelous 


feathers. 


Introduction. XVil 


The higher animals thus start in life with a definite equipment,—a body tuned to 
respond to the world in which they are placed, and this ingrained ability for action may 
be called instinct. 

In speaking of the “habits” of animals we usually mean the manner of their life in 
general, whilea “habit”’ in the technical sense may be regarded as a mode of action which 
the animal has learned or acquired. It is associated with pleasure, and in the course of 
repetition may become more or less fixed or “stereotyped.” In this sense habits are 
formed out of the raw material which heredity provides. The young bird learns to eat 
certain things, to avoid certain enemies, to start at certain sounds, to ignore others, to 
approach its nest in a certain way. Thus also the vertebrate sometimes acquires the 
habit of walking backward, while its instinct leads it to walk forward. 

Habits must in time take the place of instincts in a very large measure, and it would 
not be strange if a Robin’s second nest were more nearly perfect than its first, or if the 
third were better than the second, but this would also depend upon other conditions. 

The power of forming habits is a sign of intelligence, but not necessarily of reason. 
The intelligence may be a small grain and never destined to grow into a flourishing tree 
of knowledge, but it must exist along with the power of profiting by experience. 

The mental faculties of birds seem to exhibit a wide range of gradation from exces- 
sive stupidity to a fair degree of intelligence, with strong associative powers,—rarely if 
ever the association of ideas, but of things with actions,—and often with wonderful 
powers of imitation. 

The habits acquired by one generation are probably never handed on to the next, 
but this is a subject from which the dust of argument has not yet cleared away. 


II. 


That a bird in the hand is worth two in the bush may be a good motto for the an- 
atomist or epicure, but for the observer of living animals a bird within reach of the hand 
and still in the bush is of far greater worth. The problem is how to see and not be 
seen. If a bird is actually caught and kept in a cage or put under restraint in any way, 
its behavior is no longer perfectly natural and free, at least not until all fear has been 
subdued and it is no longer wild but tame. What is needed is an invisible chain which 
shall hold the animals to some fixed and chosen spot which can then be approached in 
disguise. 

Fortunately for the student of bird-habit and instinct all these conditions are fulfilled 
for a most important and interesting period, —that of life at the nest. The nest is the 
given fixed point, and parental instinct is the invisible chain. The wild bird, however, 
is bound not merely to the nest, but to its young. Wherever the young go, the old birds 
follow. By using the nearly fledged young as a lure, some species could, I believe, be 
led across country for a mile or more. I have taken them two hundred feet without 
special effort. 

Hitherto the bird-photographer has had to rely mainly upon chance in getting a 
picture of the nesting scenes. Most land birds depend upon concealment for protection 
from their enemies during the season of young. Their nests are apt to be shrouded 


XViii Introduction. 


in grass or foliage, and, if easily approached, are usually inaccessible to the camera. If 
the nest is in a high bush or tree, the difficulties of the position and light are usually an 
effectual bar to obtaining good pictures, to say nothing of seeing what takes place. 
When the nest is on or near the ground and in a well-lighted spot, conditions which are 
only rarely fulfilled, it has been customary to set up the camera, and attaching a long 
rubber tube or thread to the shutter, to retire to a distance and wait for the birds to 
appear. When one of them is seen to go to the nest, the plate is exposed by pulling the 
thread or pressing the pneumatic bulb, and, if in luck, a picture may thus be obtained. 
Many plates, however, are sure to be spoiled; little can be seen, and the observer has no 
control over the course of events. In the pages which follow, a method is described by 
which nesting birds can, in many cases, be successfully approached and studied with ease 
whatever the position of the nest. 

It is a comparatively easy matter to examine and photograph the nest, the eggs, or 
the young of such species whose dwellings are accessible to all, but to portray the free 
behavior of the adult bird of the shy land species is quite another question. 

The method is limited in its application from the necessities of the case. It is based 
on the solid ground of animal instinct, and may confidently be expected to have a wide 
application; but how wide or general its use may become can only be determined by 
well-directed experiment. 


III. 


Nearly all the illustrations of this volume are from photographs of adult land birds, 
and the reader will observe that they are in many cases arranged in series, and portray 
certain actions which are performed in a kind of routine. With very few exceptions all 
were made by means of the method, that is to say, the photographs were taken deliber- 
ately and not by chance. My plan was to watch the life at the nest very closely, hour by 
hour, and day by day, and I often made a large number of photographs to illustrate 
typical and unusual scenes at anest. The observer has the advantage of being on the 
spot, of being able to see every act performed and to seize every opportunity which 
may arise. Many of the photographs here shown could not have been obtained by any 
other means. 

What is offered now represents but a beginning in the attempt to portray the whole 
life of birds at the nest. The first furrow only has been struck in an old and still fallow 
field. These pictures will possibly seem crude when compared with those which the 
future will yield, but there is this to be said about all really good photographs of wild 
animals, that they possess a permanent interest and value, since within their limits they 
represent the truth, vigor, and freshness of nature. When this method comes to be ap- 
plied to some of the water birds, the Terns, Gulls, and their congeners along the coast, 
which are more easily approached than the shyer land species, serial pictures will be 
obtained of far greater perfection and beauty than anything which has yet appeared. 

For the portrayal of animals in action the camera is of supreme value, and if I have 
emphasized its use, it is only asa means to an end. Scientific books dealing with the 
anatomy and development of animals will always require good drawings for the illustra- 


Introduction. XIX 


tion of their subjects, and these are preferable to poor photographs, but for the study of 
animal behavior in both the invertebrates and vertebrates the camera is immeasurably 
superior to brush or pencil. Popular natural history books have already a large body of 
invaluable material to draw upon for illustrative purposes, and the often crude, impossi- 
ble, or imperfect drawings, which have so long done service in the past, will gradually 
give place to truthful delineations of animals at home, and in the midst of that nature of 
which they form a part. 


THE HOME LIFE OF WILD BIRDS. 


Crier ie Bo 


A NEW METHOD OF BIRD STUDY AND PHOTOGRAPHY. 


FTN HE method of studying the habits of wild birds which this volume illustrates con- 
sists in bringing the birds to you and then camping beside them, in watching 
a their behavior at arm’s length and in recording with the camera their varied 
activities. By means of sucha method one may live with the birds for days at a time, 
and watch the play of their most interesting habits and instincts. The actors are not con- 
fined in cages; they suffer indeed no restraint, excepting that only which their nature 
imposes. They come and go at will, and their life is as free and untrammeled as ever. 
The method enables one to see with his own eyes at a distance of a few inches or 
feet, more or less, what birds do in and about their nests, and at the same time affords 
the rare opportunity of making photographs, not a single picture or a chance shot now 
and then, but an unlimited series of pictures to illustrate the behavior of birds ia the full- 
est manner and at the most interesting period of their lives. It is often an easy matter 
to focus your camera directly upon the bird itself and to give a time exposure when 
desired. Moreover, you can approach as near as you wish, and make photographs of any 


required size. 

I will now give the reader a less enigmatical account of the method, first considering 
its psychological basis or the scientific principles on which it rests, and then recording in 
a separate chapter, as practical examples of its working, the exact history of a few of the 
cases in which it has been applied. 

The method in use depends mainly upon two conditions: 

(1) The control of the nesting site, and 

(2) The concealment of the observer. 

By nesting site is meant the nest and its immediate surroundings, such as a twig, 
branch, hollow trunk, stem, or whatever part of a tree the nest may occupy, a bush, stub, 
strip of sod, or tussock of sedge, that is—the nest with its immediate 
settings. If the nest, like that of an Oriole, is fastened to the leafy 
branch of a tree, the nesting bough is cut off, and the whole is then care- 
fully lowered. to the ground and set up in a good light, so that the branch with the nest 
shall occupy the same relative positions which they did before. The nest, however, is 
now but four instead of forty or more feet from the ground. 


Control of the 
Nesting Site. 


i 


tO 


Wild Birds. 


The nesting bough is carried to a convenient distance from the tree, and firmly 
fastened to two stakes, driven into the ground and placed in a good light. If the nest is 
in a tussock in a shaded swamp, the whole is cut out and taken to the nearest well- 
lighted place; if in the woods, it is carried to a clearing where the light is favorable for 
study. Again, when a nest like that of the Brown Thrush occupies the center of a dense 
thorn bush which no human eye can penetrate and much less that of the camera, its main 
supports are cut off, and the essential parts are removed to the outside of the clump or to 
any favorable point close at hand. If the nest is but five or ten feet up, the main stem is 
severed, and the nesting branch 
lowered to the four-foot mark, 
a convenient working height. 

I wish to emphasize the 
fact that the nest itself is usually 
not moved or disturbed, or rath- 
er that it is moved only with 
its supports. The change is 
one of space relations, which 
may change with every pass- 
ing breeze, but the relation of 
nest to support remains undis- 
turbed. 

This sudden displacement 
of the nesting bough is of no 
special importance to either old 
or young, provided certain pre- 
cautions are taken to be dwelt 
upon alittlelater. It is asif an 
apartment or living room were 
removed from the fourth story 
of some human abode to the 
ground floor, or in the case of 
the ground building birds as if 
the first story were raised to 

Fig.1. Tent in front of Cedar-bird’s nest, shown in its original position a level with the second. The 
in Fig.12, One of the birds is feeding its young. & : : 
immediate surroundings of the 
nest remain the same in any case. The nest might indeed be taken from its bough or 
from the sward, but this would be inadvisable, chiefly because it would destroy the na- 
tural site or the exact conditions selected and in some measure determined by the birds 
themselves. 

For an observatory I have adopted a green tent which effectually conceals the student 
together with his camera and entire outfit. The reader will find this fully described in 
the chapter on the tools of the bird-photographer. The tent is pitched 
beside the nest, and when in operation, is open only at one point, 
marked by a small square window, in line with the photographic lens. 


Concealment of 
the Observer. 


and the nest. 


A New Method of Bird Study and Photography. 


bo 


It seems at first thought strange and almost incredible that one may take such 
liberties with wild birds, without wreaking destruction upon the young or introducing 
such unnatural conditions as would be intolerable to every true student Principles which 
and lover of birds, but this is by no means the case. No injury is underlie the 
wrought upon old or young. The former nesting conditions are soon Method. 
forgotten, while the new are quickly adopted and defended with all the boldness and 
persistence of which birds are capable. 

This method of studying birds depends mainly upon the strength of the parental in- 
stincts which bind old to young by an invisible chain, and upon the ease with which a 
bird learns to adapt itself to new conditions. Upon more complete analysis we recognize 
the following psychological principles: 

(a) The strength of an instinct increases through exercise, and may be reénforced by 
habit ; 

(b) An instinctive impulse may be blocked or suppressed by any contrary impulse; 

(c) The instinct of fear is often temporarily suppressed by the fighting instinct, or 
permanently overcome by the repetition of any experience leading to the formation of 
new habits or associations. 

We may also add: 

(d) New habits are readily formed and reénforce or supplant those of older growth, 

(e) Abstract ideas, if they form any part of the furniture of the average bird-mind, are 
extremely hazy and fleeting ; 

(f) Finally we must recall the physiological fact that birds are guided in most of their 
operations by sight and hearing, not by scent. Their olfactory organ is very rudimentary 
at best, and avails them neither in finding food, nor in avoiding enemies. 

After a brief analysis of the parental instincts we will endeavor to show how the 
principles just given are applied to the problem of approaching wild birds in the way 
described. 

The parental instincts begin to control the life of the adult with the periodic revival 
of the reproductive functions, and vary greatly in their scope and intensity at the different 
stages of their reign as well as in different species of birds. They are periodic, recurring 
at definite intervals during sexual life and in serial form, one kind of act usually leading 
to the next in sequence, and so on until the series is complete. 

When more than one litter is produced in a season, the series of events is repeated 
with minor changes _ If we include the typical migratory movements, the principal terms 
of the reproductive cycle may be expressed more fully as follows: 

(1) Spring migration of the summer residents to the place of birth; 

(2) Mating; 

(3) Selection of nesting site and construction of the nest ; 

(4) Egg-laying ; 

(5) Incubation ; 

(6) Care of the young in the nest, including feeding, and cleaning nest and young; 

(7) Care and education of young from time of flight ; 

(8) Fall migration to winter quarters. 

Birds seem to follow one line of conduct, whether it be sitting over the eggs, brood- 
ing, or tending the young, until their instinct in that particular direction has been satisfied, 


4 Wild Birds. 


thus normally completing one term of the series before passing to the next in sequence. 
The machinery, however, rarely works with absolute precision. Perturbations are sure to 
" arise whenever a contrary impulse 
comes into the field, and either blocks 
the path or struggles for supremacy. 
The surge of parental feeling is 
often marked by an inbred pugnacity, 
which begins to show itself in certain 
species at the very beginning of the 
breeding season. This fighting mood, 
which is an adaptation for the protec- 
tion of the home and all that it con- 
tains, is by no means a measure of 
the other parental impulses. It has 
a gradual rise, reaches a maximum 
when the young are ready to leave the 
nest, at a time when protection is 
most needed, and then gradually sub- 
sides. 

When a pair are robbed during 
the breeding season, or in any way 
disturbed in mind or property, three 
courses are open to them, either to 
desert and begin operations anew, 

; ; ; ; to stay by the nest and save what is 
Fig. 2. Tent in bushy pasture beside nest of Chestnut-sided i . 
Warbler, shown in detail in Fig. 3. left, or, having done this, to fill up 
the gap by laying more eggs. The 
course eventually followed depends upon the nature of the bird, or upon the relative 
strength of fear, the parental instincts, and habit. 

The parental instinct,’ reénforced by habit, gradually increases until the young are 
reared. It is therefore safest to change the nesting surroundings when this instinct is 
approaching its culmination. 

The general feeling of fear is gradually or quickly suppressed according to the value 
of the different factors in the equation, by the parental instinct, which impels a bird at all 
hazards to go to its young wherever placed. This impulse though it be weak at first, 
is strengthened by exercise, or what amounts to the same thing,—by the growth of 
habits or associations. 

After a bird once visits the nest in its new position, it returns again and again, and 


in proportion as its visits to the old nesting place diminish and finally cease, its approaches 
to the new position become more frequent, until a new habit has been formed, or if you 
will, until the old habit is reinstated. 
When the birds approach the nest any strange objects like the stakes which support 
the bough, or the tent which is pitched beside it arouse their sense of fear or suspicion, 
'' This phrase is sometimes used for the sake of brevity and convenience in nearly the same sense as parental 


attachment or parental love. 


A New Method of Bird Study and Photography. 5 


and they may keep away for a time or advance with caution. If very shy, like most Cat- 
birds, they will sometimes skirmish about the tent for two hours or more before touching 
the nest. The ice is usually broken 
however in from twenty minutes to 
an hour, and I have known a Chip- 
ping Sparrow and Red-eyed Vireo to 
feed their young in three minutes 
after the tent was in place. 

At every approach to the nest 
in its new position, the birds see the 
same objects which work them noill. 
The tent stands silent and motion- 
less, unless it happens to be windy, 
but the young are close by, and fear 
of the new objects gradually wears 
away. Parental instinct, or in this 
case maternal love, for the instinct 
to cherish the young is usually 
stronger in the mother, wins the day. 
The mother bird comes to the nest 
and feeds her clamoring brood. The 
spell is broken; she comes again. 
The male also approaches, and their 
visits are thereafter repeated. 

Possibly the fears of the old 
birds are renewed at sight of the win- 
dow which is now opened in the tent- 
front, and of the glass eye of the 
camera gleaming through it, but the 
lens is also silent and motionless, and 
soon becomes a familiar object to be 
finally disregarded. Again there is Fig. 3. Tent beside nest of Chestnut-sided Warbler. The femzle 
the fear which the sound of the shut- broods, while the male is foraging. 
ter, a sharp metallic c/zck, at first in- 
spires, unless you are the fortunate possessor of an absolutely silent and rapid shut- 
ter, an instrument which is unknown to the trade, at least in this country. At its first 
report when two feet away, many a bird will jump as if shot, give an angry scream, 
and even fly at the tent as if to exorcise an evil spirit, while after a few hours, or on the 
second day, they will only wince; finally they will not budge a feather at this or any 
other often repeated sound, whether from shutter, steam whistle, locomotive, or the human 
voice. This illustrates the effect of the alarm clock over again. At our first experience 
with this nerve-wracking machine, we start from deep sleep and promptly heed its sum- 
mons; then we are apt to mind it less and less until we sleep on serenely in spite of it. 
If we were to place an alarm clock on or near the nesting bough, and let it off at regular 
but not too frequent intervals, the birds would soon learn to disregard it as we do, and as 
some of them disregard the babel of a city street. 


6 Wild Birds. 


It is the young, the young, always THE YOUNG in whom the interest of the old birds 
is centered, and about whom their lives revolve. They are the strong lure, the talisman, 
the magnet to which the 
parent is irresistibly 
drawn. The tree, the 
branch, the nest itself, 
what are these in compari- 
son with the young for 
whom alone they exist ? 

With some species it 
is possible to make the 
necessary change without 
evil consequences when 
there are eggs in the nest ; 
with others we must wait 
until the young are from 
When to four to nine days 
Change old. It is all a 
the Nest-question of the 
ss strength of the 
parental instinct, and this 
varies between wide limits 
in different species, and 
very considerably between 
different individuals. From 
the nature of the case 
there can be no infallible 
rule. If we know little of 
the habits of the birds in 
question it is safest to wait 
until the seventh to the 

Fig. 4, Truncated elm with nest-holes of Woodpecker, the lowermost re- ninth day after the young 
cently occupied by Bluebirds. To bring down the nest, the trunk may be cut : 
from below-orin line with arrows are hatched, or when in 

many passerine birds, as 
Robins, Orioles, and Waxwings, the feather-shafts of the wing-quills begin to appear in 
the young, or better when they project from one quarter to one half inch beyond the feather 
tubes. At this period the parental instinct is reaching its maximum, and, what is equally 
important, the sense of fear has not appeared in the young. 

When we try to formulate a rule, however, we at once encounter numerous excep- 
tions. Thus in Cuckoos the feathers do not shed their envelopes gradually as in most 
birds, but remain sheathed up to the last day in the nest. Of greater importance is the 
understanding of the principles involved, and with these in mind and judiciously applied 
very few mistakes should be made. 

At the beginning of observations a nest with eggs should be watched, but not dis- 
turbed. When the period of incubation has been determined, and the time of hatching 


A New Method of Bird Study and Photography. 7 


known, the young may be examined and photographed if it is desired. At all events 
they should be watched until the critical time arises for closer study. Mode of 
This decided upon in the manner already suggested, circumstances must Procedure. 
determine the course to be followed. 

If the nest, like that of a Robin or Kingbird, is saddled to the branch of a tree, saw 
off the whole limb and nail it to stakes driven into the ground, so placed as always to give 
the best light. The nesting bough, in case there is one, should be set with its long axis 
parallel with the course of the sun, but the position of the bough or tent may be changed 
during the day when exceptional conditions render it necessary. 

Either a dark foliage or a sky background may be chosen for the nest, according to 
the desire of the operator or the possibilities of the situation. If not satisfied with a 
natural background it would be possible to place dark or light screens behind the nesting 
bough or to use reflected 
light for softening the shad- 
ows, but no experiments 
have yet been made in this 
direction. The tent is then 
to be placed in position, or 
it may be pitched and left 
overnight beside the nest.’ 
In other words, operations 
may begin at once or be 
postponed until the follow- 
ing day, the better plan for 
a beginner until he has 
mastered minor difficulties, 
which, though small in 
themselves, are far from un- 
important. When the tent 
is closed absolute silence 
must be maintained, for 
while this is not always 
necessary, it is the best rule 
to follow during the first 
days of observation. 

The best time to begin 
is from eight to nine o’clock 
in the morning, because the 


young willthen have been [gay Aye “s 
: : 
fed, and the sun will be 
getting high enough for Fig. 5. Nest-hole of Flicker, used by Bluebird. Trunk removed from tree, 
th st «d | eae | and mounted on pivot so that it can be turned to any angle with sun. See 
€ most rapid photograph- No. 15 of table, p. 12, Fig. 10, and Chapter VII. 


ic work. One may spend 
as many hours a day, and as many days at one nest, as time permits or inclination decides. 


| Directions for use of the tent are given in Chapter III. 


8 Wild Birds. 


I will only suggest that the second day is always better than the first, and that the third 
or fourth is always sure to bring something new. If one would learn the nesting habits 
of any species thoroughly, it will hardly do to rely upon one nest. The more you see of 
different nests and different birds the better. 

I usually spend five or six hours in the tent, from nine in the morning until three in 
the afternoon, when the weather is fine. If the camping ground is near my house, as it 
usually is, I leave the tent for 
half an hour at noon, but if 
it is far, I carry a lunch and 
spend the day. When possi- 
ble, Iam always on hand dur- 
ing the last day of life at the 
nest, to see the young leave 
it, usually one at a time, and 
to witness the manceuvres 
of the parents in conducting 
them to the nearest trees. 

Young birds from one to 
five days old cannot, as a 

: rule, stand exces- 
Precautions. 
nates sive heat. Even 
Observed. When fed and 
brooded they will 
sometimes succumb, and here 
lies the serious danger to be 
guarded against. A nest of 
very young birds well shaded 
by foliage cannot be safely 
carried into the direct sun- 
shine of a hot summer’s day, 
hence the importance of be- 
ginning operations at the 
proper time when the weath- 


Fig.6. Nesting bough of Kingbird removed from apple tree in background SES. suitable, and further of 


at a point where extended arrows meet, and fixed to upright stakes, Tent- not allowing your enthusiasm 
cloth thrown over frame which is set in position, 


£ 
Ah, 
H 
j 
H 
| 
) 


to get the better of your 
judgment. 

The morning of a clear, mild day is preferable, but since we cannot order the weather, 
it is better to leave the birds to themselves, if it promises to be excessively hot or windy. 

The young may be fed or handled as much as one wishes, provided they have not 
acquired the instinct of fear. If you are uncertain as to this and your aim is to study the 
nesting habits, it is better to avoid approaching, touching, or in any way disturbing the 
young after the flight feathers have appeared. The cutting of leaves or twigs which 
obstruct the light or cast undesirable shadows should be done before this time. 

On the other hand, investigations of the young which require accurate weighing, 


Fig. 7. Female Kingbird astride nest, protecting young from heat. This and the following 
from photographs made at nest shown on facing page. 


Fig.8. Kingbird family, The male with grasshopper in bill,—his mate, partly hidden, 
behind him, 


9 


A New Method of Bird Study and Photography. 11 


measurements, or photographs of the birds themselves, place the matter in a different 
light. With these objects in view the nest must be frequently approached and the young 
taken out, and for such studies the change of the nesting site offers such obvious advan- 
tages that it is needless to dwell upon them. In taking down the nesting bough it is 
often necessary to touch the nest, and this does no harm. 

Young birds eight or nine days old stand the heat well, provided they are fed, but on 
very hot days they should 
not be allowed to go with- f 
out food for more than 
two hours at the longest. 
Should the parents bring 
no food during this time, it 
is better to feed the young 
in the nest, and to*suspend 
operations until the next 
day. 

As has been already 
said, the old birds may be 
expected to come to the 
nest in from twenty min- 
utes to an hour, when the 
tent is brought into imme- 
diate use after removal of 
the nesting bough. It is 
naturally impossible to 
predict exactly what will 
happen in any given case 
until the experiment is 
tried, since the personal 
equation or individuality 
of the birds themselves is 
an unknown and variable 
factor. One thing only is 
certain, that the parental 
instincts, reénforced by 
habit, will win in the end, 


. Fig.9. Cedar-bird at nest shown in Figs. 1, 12, and 13, prepared to feed young 
that they will cast out fear, by regurgitation: a characteristic attitude. The parallel outlines of the neck 


and draw the birds to their show thatthe golleie tall: 


young. 

I have used the tent and altered the nesting site in the case of twenty-six nests 
belonging to fifteen different species of birds. The experiments were Extent of Appli- 
made in the course of two seasons, and the entire list is tabulated as cation of the 
follows, the age of the young in most cases being only approximately Method. 
accurate :— 


12 Wild Birds. 


EXPERIMENTS IN THE USE OF THE OBSERVATION TENT AND IN CHANGE OF NESTING 
s 


SITE. 
| AGE 
Birps AND Nests, TIME. OF | OF 
YOUNG | YOUNG, 
| 
| 
t Redwing Blackbird (Nest undisturbed)............ July 14, 1899 | 3 | a1 days 
2 Redwing Blackbird (Nest swayed down one foot)...| July 19, 1900 | 3 5 days 
3 Catbird (Nest undisturbed)... 0.222. c0sssesweeen July 23, 1899 | 2 8 days 
4 Catbird (Position of nest unchanged)..........-... July 26, 1899 | 3 3-4 days 
5 Catbird (Nesting bough displaced ten feet)........ :} Aug. 4, 1899 | 3 7-8 days 
6 Catbird (Nest undisturbed)... 2. 240.c0cs0 case sae- June 21, 1900 | 4 7 days 
7 Cedar-bird (Nesting bush moved twenty feet)....... Aug. 3, 1899 | 4 g-10 days 
3 Cedar-bird (Nesting bough displaced forty feet)....) Aug.21, 1899 | 4 6 days 
g Cedar-bird( Nesting boughcutoffandmoved fifty feet).) Aug. 23, 1899 | 2 10 days 
ro Cedar-bird (Nest in pine tree; bough moved fifty feet).| July 14, 1900 | 4 7-8 days 
11 Red-eyed Vireo (Nesting twig lowered one foot)....| Aug. 3, 1899 | 2 8 days 
12 Red-eyed Vireo (Nesting tree cut down and taken 
from woods forty feet to Open).s sie. ae cect es sevens July 5, 1900 | 4 g days 
13 Robin (Nesting bough cut off and moved thirty feet).| Aug. 9, 1899 | 3 7 days. 
14 Robin (Nest in oak thirty feet up ; branch moved to 
open field sixty feeb “away ons cs: ate ek wee wees July 25, 1900 | 3 6 days. 
15 Bluebird (Nest-hole in apple tree; moved fifty feet to 
OPO Melis eruhven- 6-6 yi ehh galechae we cane our wie, eaxyaiecane oat ees Aug.15, 1899 | 4 5 days. 
16 Chestnut-sided Warbler (Bushes cleared in front of 
MESU) 5 saha ater Meee wa emn: aia a Ce aee uma aeeee June 15, 1900 | 4 eggs 
17 Chestnut-sided Warbler (Bushes cleared in front of 
TVESU) rates aa cvpracoee muha etme Ie aCe rRnOnE Noh ee eons June 28, 1900 | 4 4 days. 
18 Night Hawk (Nesting site with young enclosed with 
WaAttlOdtWARS) sar Rataeness cies sare wanale coi ag anes June 29, 1900 | I 5 days. 
19 Baltimore Oriole (Nesting branch in apple tree ; 
moved twenty-five feet)... ...0 sccase yaaa esas dees June 25, r900 | 3 8-9 days. 
20 Kingbird (Nesting branch moved twenty feet)...... July 2, 1900 | 2 6 days. 
Kingbird (Nesting branch moved twenty-five feet)...|| July 2, 1900 | 4 7 days. 
22 Wilson’s Thrush (Nest in tussock ; whole moved from 
swamp -tovopen fifty feeb) as ceiiere aes alesee tenets July 9, 1900 | 3 io days. 
23 Chipping Sparrow (Nesting bough moved twenty feet)| July 11, 1900 | 4 4-5 days. 
24 Brown Thrush (Nest in thornbush; moved fifteen feet).) July 11, 1900 | 3 4 days. 
25 Song Sparrow (Nest in dead sapling ; moved forty feet 
CO) OPEN nae tau s oe Mae GD Awe wae © See aparece ees July 17, 1900 | 3 5 days. 
26 Kingfisher (Nest in bank; opened at rear)......... July 23, 1900 | 5 9 days. 


In only three cases where the nest with its supports was moved did the young suffer, 
and in each of these from unusual conditions. A nest of Cedar Waxwings (8) though fed 
by both birds and brooded almost constantly, succumbed to the heat, the day being one 
of the hardest of the entire summer. The second, a nest of Bluebirds (15), were constantly 
fed during the day while I watched them, but the old birds were frightened off at some 
later time, and their young left to perish. The third, a nest of Song Sparrows (25), also 
came to grief on account of the heat. The day was the hottest ever recorded by the 
Weather Bureau in New England, and the nest, which was moved to the open, hap- 
pened to be in the crotch of a dead sapling, so that the birds were exposed on all sides. 


A New Method of Bird Study and Photography. 13 


In one or two instances I had serious trouble from cutting away too much foliage about 
a nest in very hot weather, but such accidents are really needless, if one follows the rule 
of leaving the birds to their own devices on days of excessive heat and humidity. In all 
the other cases, everything went well, and the young left the nest in due course. 

Kingbirds have remained in the 
nest eleven days after the change, 
Robins a week, Cedar-birds six days. 
A glance at the table will show that 
in one instance, that of the Chestnut- 
sided Warbler (16), observations were 
begun while there were eggs, and I 
have no doubt that in many species 
the whole period of life in the nest 
from hatching to the time of flight 
may be watched from the tent, but 
the subject is yet open to experiment. 
It is all a question of the strength of 
the parental instincts at the period 
in question. Where this attachment 
to nest and eggs is strong as in Owls, 
Fish Hawks, Flickers, Kingbirds, 
and the Chipping Sparrows, to men- 
tion a few cases, we may look for 
success. 

I am confident that the movable 
tent has a great future as an obser- 
vatory forthe study of bird-habit, and 
that it will be possible to watch the 


Pee fi : Fig. 10. Female Bluebird with cricket in bill, ready to enter 
building of the nest in such species as nest-hole. See Fig. 5. 


have a strong attachment to chosen 
sites, and whose plans are not easily disturbed by trifles. Here is certainly a fallow field 
which has been scratched only here and there by the plow, and where attempts to culti- 
vate it fail, no harm is done. In making experiments in this direction care should be 
taken not to approach too near with the tent, at least on the first day. Again it is pro- 
bable that many kinds of birds may be attracted by food and other lures, but the possible 
rewards of sedentary experiments in this direction are too uncertain to arouse much en- 
thusiasm in the mind of the active bird student. 

I have no desire to anticipate every objection which might be raised against the 
method, were it possible to do so, but after testing it to the best of my ability with the 


opportunities of two summers, I am confident of its value and am ready : 
Objections to 


to stand sponsor for it in judicious hands. It is hardly necessary to insist the Method. 


that it is not designed for exhibition purposes, and that its successful 
practice requires some knowledge, with more patience and time. 
To the trained naturalist patience has long ceased to bea virtue. He is accustomed 


14 Wild Birds. 


to work in the field or laboratory for weeks or months to attain a well-defined end, and 
that end he will attain, provided it can be compassed by intelligence, industry, and skill. 
Patience is the naturalist’s stock in trade, and while no success may come because of 
it alone, none can be assured without it. 

In the ten days or two 
weeks or more of life at 
the nest events move rap- 
idly and the question of 
time is important. Any 
interruptions are therefore 
opportunities for the dis- 
play of patience rather 
than for the increase of 
knowledge. 

We have already seen 
that the displacement of 
the nest or nesting branch 
does not introduce unnat- 
ural conditions of any im- 
portance into the life of 
the birds. Of course every 
change wrought by man is 
in a certain sense unnatu- 
ral. If we pluck a single 
leaf from a tree, that tree 
is no longer in its natural 
state, but the change 
counts for nothing. If we 
keep on plucking leaves, 
however, a time will come 
when the arm of the bal- 
ance is disturbed, and the 
denuded tree is sure to 


Fig.11. Female Chestnut-sided Warbler shielding the young on a warm day. 
Photographed from tent shown in Figs. 2 and 3. suffer. The removal of a 


leaf or twig about a nest 


is of no practical consequence, but this should not be carried too far, both on account of 
the young which need the protection of shade, and for the sake of natural appearances 
which we wish to preserve. 

It might be supposed that when a branch is lopped off, its foliage would at once 
wither, and unduly expose the nest or detract from the artistic value of a picture. The 
fact is, however, that there is commonly enough sap in a hard wood bough of moderate 
size to keep the leaves fresh for several days,’ and towards the close of life at the nest the 

! When the nesting branch is vertical and not too large, it can be easily kept fresh by placing it in a jug or can 


of water which should be set in the ground, 


A New Method of Bird Study and Photography. 15 


young need no protection from this source. As to this point, however, the illustrations 
in this book will speak for themselves. 

Evergreens like the pine and spruce hold their leaves bright for a long time after 
cutting, and in this respect the various deciduous trees and shrubs differ greatly, those 
with a hard, close grain keeping fresh the longer. 

As to any injury to trees which the method may be supposed to entail, it is not worth 
considering, since no valuable tree should be mutilated without first obtaining the per- 
mission of the owner, for however trifling the damage may appear, his point of view Is 
likely to be different from your own. The cutting of an occasional twig or branch, even 
if it does not trim the tree, is not regarded as an important event in this country at 
present. If every farmer who owns orchards and woodlands did his duty, he would cut out 
more useless wood in a year than a student of birds would need to doinadecade. It is 
possibly unnecessary to add that no one should set up a nest in a field, and leave the 
trouble of removing it to the owner of the land. 

A more serious objection is likely to occur to the ornithologist, namely the liability 
of exposing the birds to new enemies. I feared lest prowling cats should discover the 
young ones whose nest and branch had been brought down from the tree top, and set up 
again in plain sight within easy reach from the ground, but I was happily mistaken. 
Predacious animals of all kinds seem to avoid such nests as if they were new devices to 
entrap or slay them. 

As to the weather, barring heat which must be guarded against in the way described, 
the nesting bough is more secure when fixed firmly to supports than it could possibly 
have been before. The only depredator of whom I stand in fear is the irresponsible or 
malicious small boy, and to anticipate his possibilities for evil, I take a look at the nest now 
and then when not encamped beside it. 

When the nest is completely exposed and the weather is very hot, the young may be 
tempted to forsake it a day or two earlier than they would naturally do, but this does not 
usually happen and is not necessarily serious. Some Kingbirds, already referred to, spent 
eighteen days in the nest, and were a week old when it was moved. This was probably 
longer than common, and certainly longer than necessary. 

The tent not only conceals the observer but protects his camera, an important con- 
sideration, since the prolonged action of the sun is liable to spring a leak in the bellows. 
As to the portability and general convenience of the tent I shall speak elsewhere. 

With notebook in hand you can sit in your tent, and see and record everything which 
transpires at the nest, the mode of approach, the kind of food brought, the varied activi- 
ties of the old and young, the visits of intruders, and their combats with 
the owners of the nest, the capture of prey which sometimes goes on 
under your eye. No better position could be chosen for hearing the 
songs, responsive calls, and alarm notes of the birds. You can thus gather materials for 
an exact and minute history of life at the nest, and of the behavior of birds during this 
important period. More than this, you can photograph the birds at will, under the most 
perfect conditions, recording what no naturalist has ever seen, and what no artist could 


Advantages of 
the Method. 


ever hope to portray. The birds come and go close to your eye, but unconscious of 
being observed. 
I have watched the Night Hawk feed her chick with fireflies barely fifteen inches 


16 Wild Birds. 


from my hand, the Kingfisher carrying live fish to its brood whose muffled rattles issued 
from their subterranean gallery a few feet away. When near enough to count her respi- 
rations accurately, I have seen the Redwing Blackbird leave her nest on a hot day, hop 
down to the cool water of the swamp, and after taking a sip, bathe in full view, within 
reach of the hand; then, shaking the water from her plumage, she would return refreshed 
to the nest. I have seen the male Kingbird come to his nesting bough with feathers 
drenched from his midday bath in the river, the Orioles flash their brilliant colors all day 
long before the eye, and Chestnut-sided Warblers become so tame after several days that 
the female would allow you to approach and stroke her back with the hand. 

It is difficult to describe the fascination which this method of study affords the 
student of animal life. New discoveries, or unexpected sights wait on the minutes, for 
while there is a well-ordered routine in the actions of many birds the most charming 
pictures occur at odd moments, and there is an endless variety of detail. It is like a suc- 
cession of scenes in a drama, only this is real life, not an imitation, and there is no need 
of introducing tragedy. 

He who runs may sometimes read and observe a few things, and so may he who per- 
forms gymnastic feats in the branches of tall trees or does penance in a hundred other 
ways, but from the tent one may read the life of the nesting bird as out of an open book. 


Cir TE Ron, 


ILLUSTRATIONS OF THE METHOD. 


illustrations of the method applied I have selected four common birds, the Cedar 

Waxwing, the Baltimore Oriole, the Redwing Blackbird, and the Kingbird. The 
choice might have fallen, however, upon any others in my list, for the principles are in 
every case the same. 

Since the breeding habits of these birds will be described more fully at a later time, 
the change of their nesting site and their behavior in the face of new surroundings need 
only concern us for the present. 

On the third day of July a Cedar-bird’s nest (No. 10 of table on page 12) was discovered 
in an unusually attractive situation. It was fastened to the horizontal branch of a white pine 
about fifteen feet up, in the line of an old stone wall that bounded an 
open field. In passing beneath the tree almost daily during the follow- 
ing week, I was sure to find one of the old birds, the female as I supposed, always on the 
nest and sitting in the same alert attitude, engaged either in incubation or brooding. 
With upstretched neck she would sit motionless and silent as a statue, as if listening 
intently, her dark eye shining like a jet black bead against the background of pine 
needles. I was waiting for the propitious time to move this nest to the open field. This 
time arrived on July 14th, when the heads of the young began to appear over the rim 
of their nest. The bough was then sawn off, carried fifty feet from the tree, and set up 
in the newly mown field, in an east to west line at a height of four feet from the ground, 
and in sucha way that the birds could be “skyed,” and the light would be good from nine 
o'clock in the morning until three in the afternoon. The tent was then pitched and 
closed ; the whole operation lasted longer than usual owing to some difficulty in getting 
stakes of the right height. Fifteen minutes is usually long enough for this work. 

From peep holes the old birds could be seen in the nesting tree, and you began to 
hear their faint s-e-e-e-e-e-7, in response to calls from the young. In twenty-four minutes 
the female was on the bough and fed her brood with red bird cherries by regurgitation. 
At this point I was obliged to leave the tent and request some curious boys to keep 
away, but the mother bird was back ina moment. Ina short time the old birds began 
to alight on the peak of the tent, which was an observatory for them as well as for the 
person inside. Taking a look about, they would drop down to the nest only a step away. 
This was done more than ten times in the course of the day. Observations began at 
8.40 in the morning and closed at 4.40, so that with an intermission at noon, they lasted 
nearly seven hours and twenty minutes. During this interval the young were fed with 


17 


| is always interesting to see how birds actually behave when put to the test, and as 


The Cedar-bird. 


18 Wild Birds. 


wild red cherries, blueberries and insects, mainly grasshoppers, and nearly always by regur- 
gitation. The nest and young were regularly cleaned, and the new conditions seemed to 
have been completely adopt- 
ed. The young, whose wing- 
quills now showed half an 
inch of the feather-shaft, 
were entirely fearless. 

On July 16th, the second 
day of observation and the 
third after the removal of the 
nesting bough, the old birds 
began the work of feeding in 
exactly twelve minutes after 
the tent was in place. I will 
add here that I have always 
removed the tent at the end 
of the day’s work, although 
in some species it would be 
of undoubted advantage to 
leave it overnight. In a little 
more than three hours the 
old birds came to the nest 
eighteen times, bringing 
abundant stores of fruit and 
insects. 

On July 17th, the third 
day at this nest, feeding be- 
gan in three minutes after 
closure of the tent. It was 
the hottest day of the sum- 


Fig.12. Cedar-bird’s nest—No. ro of table—in original position marked by s 
arrows. See Figs. 9 and 13. mer, but life at the nest went 


on without accident or inter- 


ruption. The young now sat or stood with heads upturned in the characteristic attitude 
shown in one of the illustrations. They flew on the morning of the nineteenth of 
July, when thirteen days old, seeking the cover of a thicket of birches close by, where 
they were cared for by their parents until ready to leave the neighborhood. They were 
scattered over an area of several square rods, and kept calling in their monotonous 
way, 2-¢-¢-¢-e-t! s-¢-e-e-c-t/ One of their number, shown in a photograph (Fig. 47), was 
not touched or posed, but occupied a natural perch chosen by himself in his flight from 
tree to tree. : 
This Oriole’s nest (No. 19 of table) was fortunately placed in an apple tree scarcely 
twenty feet up, so that no gymnastic feat was needed to bring the branch to the ground. 
The noisy young calling with incessant reiteration, weck-2ck-wch-tck-1ck | 
The Baltimore 1 dvertised their nest to every passer by, and it was surprising that it had 


Oriole. ‘ 
remained unmolested. 


Illustrations of the Method. 19 


This beautiful nest with the entire bough to which it was strung was moved eight 
yards from the tree, set up in the way described, and the tent was closed at a quarter past 
eight o’clock. After repeated visits to the apple tree both birds disappeared, but did not 
go out of hearing of their young, who in a half-hour’s time began giving their weck-zck- 
wk-ick / with an emphasis sure to evoke response. 

The old birds began to approach, sounding now and then their peculiar rattle, and 
the female could be seen exploring the foliage of a neighboring tree. At fifteen minutes 
past nine one of them was skirmishing about the tent, and in five minutes alighted 
above the nest with a green larva in bill. This larva however had another destiny that 
was apparent at the moment, for a puff of wind frightened the bird away. At her next 
visit a strawberry was brought and safely delivered, in exactly one hour and seventeen 
minutes from the beginning of operations. 

Observations were continued until 4.25 P.M. or, allowing for the noon intermission, 
during seven and a quarter hours. In this period the parents were at the nest fifty times 
bringing insects and fruit. Sometimes the feedings would follow at two or three minute 
intervals; then longer lapses would occur. 

On the second day, June 26th, the female brought food in five minutes after the tent 
was up, and during the space of six hours and twenty-three minutes while operations 
lasted, the young 
were fed one hund- 
red and sixteen times 
by both birds. I left 
the tent and entered 
it again several times 
during the day, and 
once moved both 
bough and tent to 
improve the light. 
By this time the Ori- 
oles showed no fear, 
but came to the nest- 
ing branch in from 
one to two minutes 
after I had entered 
the tent. During an 
interval of ten min- 


m rare == 4 


utes, the young were 
fed eleven times. 
The tent was 
closed at 8.30 on the 
morning of the third 


Fig. 13. Cedar-bird’s nest in its new site. Nesting bough moved fifty feet to open field. 
day, June 27th, and Compare Figs. 1, 9, and 12. 


the first feeding came 
off in five minutes. In two hours and twenty-five minutes the old birds made forty-four 
visits to the nest bringing strawberries and insects, and towards eleven o'clock one of the 


20 Wild Birds. 


young who for a long time had been exercising his wing- and leg-muscles by climbing to 

the rim of the pouch, took his first flight, making a neighboring tree. Not long after, a 

second bird climbed out of the sack 

iy” ” and was off, lured away by its parents. 

4 The third and last bird left a little 

later, and towards evening the young 

_ were calling from trees down the 
hillside. 

On the fifth day of July a nest of 

three young Blackbirds (No. 2 of the 

table), aged five days, 


The Redwi 
mieseegis was found on the edge 


Blackbird. 
of what was once an 


alder swamp, close to the town and 
the “ Cove”’ made by the Winnipi- 
seogee River in Northfield. It was 
fixed to several slender stems of 
Spireea, amid a dense tangle of 
Cephalthus, wild roses, and purple 
milkweeds. The situation was so 
attractive and offered such fine op- 
portunities for studying these birds 
that, notwithstanding the water and 
mud, I determined to make careful 
preparations. A space four feet 
square was at once cleared of bushes 
at one side of the nest. In order 
to sky the birds, the nesting twigs 
were slightly raised, but none of 
these were severed or otherwise dis- 
Fig. 14. Baltimore Oriole inspecting young after having fed pee : : 
them. On the ninth of July I built a raft 
or platform on the cleared area, and 
painted it green, possibly an unnecessary precaution. When weighted with the observer and 
his apparatus, the flooring was barely clear of the water. On the following day, the tent was 
pitched over this stranded raft and guyed to the bushes, the tent poles having been 
previously lengthened to suit the depth of mud and water. Everything was ready for 
observations at half-past nine o'clock. At first the birds fluttered around the nest chuck- 
zg and whistling incessantly, but in less than an hour the warble of the male was 
heard, which is a sure sign of growing confidence. Then both birds went off for food, 
returned, reconnoitred the tent and nest, and after precisely one hour and twenty-three 
minutes from the beginning of observations the female came and fed her clamoring young. 
Again she was off and back three times in rapid succession. Three minutes later she was 
brooding, and remained on the nest thirteen minutes. Leaving it again, she examined 
the tent anew, then brooded ten minutes more. A little later the young were fed and the 
nest cleaned with great care. 


Illustrations of the Method. 21 


The male was more cautious and did not actually feed his young until twenty-seven 
minutes after eleven. His fears were then dispelled and life at the nest went on without 
interruption. At about noon the old birds were using the tent as a half-way house, 
alighting on its peak and guys, and foraging about it forfood. Inthe space of four hours 
on the first day, during which the birds were watched at a distance of about twenty-seven 
inches, fifty-four visits were made and the young were fed forty times. The female 
brooded her young over an hour, fed them twenty-nine times, and cleaned the nest 
thirteen times. The male made eleven visits, attending to sanitary matters but twice. 
This example illustrates as well as any which could be given the advantage which attends 
the use of the observation tent. 

On the following day, July rith, the female was at the nest and brooding her young 
in five minutes after the tent was in position. Presently she Ieft to hunt for insects, 
alighted on the tent, and five minutes later was feeding her young and cleaning the nest. 
In the course of nearly three and one half hours, fifty-five visits were made and the young 
were fed collectively or singly forty-three times. At about half-past eleven o'clock one 
of the fledglings left the nest and was fed by the old birds in the surrounding bushes of 
the swamp. The female brought food thirty-two times, cleaned the nest eight times, and 
brooded eighteen times for intervals varying from thirty seconds to eighteen minutes. 
This bird cut a queer figure while stand- 
ing or sitting in the sun, with wings 
spread and bristling like a turkey-cock 
with every feather erect, and with 
mouth agape, trying to keep cool while 
shielding her family from the heat. Her 
breathings were at the rate of 150 to 
160 times a minute. The male bird 
served food eleven times and attended 
tosanitary mattersonce. In the course 
of forty-two minutes the first young 
bird to leave the nest was fed eight 
times, seven times by the mother and 
once by the father. Three days later 
the swamp was visited at just after sun- 
down, when the young birds suddenly 
arose from the nest and flew off with 
ease and precision. 

Kingbirds pose so well, especially 
about their nests, that I was anxious to 
see how they would stand 


The Kingbird. 
the test of a sudden change sd 


in their surroundings. Accordingly, I Fig. 15. Baltimore Oriole inspecting nest when behavior has 
watched with unusual care two nests become more free. 

which were found near my house. On 

the thirteenth day of June one had two and the other four eggs all freshly laid, and these 
appeared to be the full complement. Young were hatched in each nest on or near the 
twenty-fifth of the month. 


22 Wild Birds. 


The first nest was built at the top of a hill, about a rod from the Oriole’s nest already 
described, on the horizontal limb of a small apple tree twelve feet up, and was a conspicu- 
ous object to all who passed that way. The nesting bough was removed and mounted in 
a good position on the morning of July 2d, and the tent was closed at half-past eight 
o'clock. At this time the two young were six days old and covered with light gray down. 
While the operation was in progress the old birds hovered over the nest, and with their 
usual boldness, swooped down 
close to my head, snapping their 
bills and uttering their piercing 
alarms. 

After the tent was closed, 
much to my surprise all became 
quiet, and I could see both birds 
—the female with insect in bill 
--exploring the nesting tree 
twenty feet away. She would 
fly to that point in space which 
the nest formerly occupied, and 
hover over it repeatedly, a char- 
acteristic action of many birds 
under such circumstances. Ten 
minutes later the female was 
again at the nesting tree with 
insects. Foran hour afterwards 
all was quiet. The old birds 
were sitting by in silence, prob- 
ably not far away. At ten min- 
utes before eleven o'clock one 
of the pair, probably the female, 
came with a swoop to the nest- 
ing branch, and I believe fed her 
young. Inthiscase the observer 


had to wait two hours and twen- 


Fig.16. Tent over raft in water of swamp beside Redwing Blackbird’s < : 
nest. See Figs. 21 and 22. ty minutes before having the 


birds close to his eye, but he was 
well repaid for the delay as the sequel willshow. In one minute the mother had returned, 


and now both began to make up for lost time. In five hours and six minutes (from 10.50 
A.M. to 4.36 P.M., allowing for an intermission of forty minutes when the observer was 
away), the old birds made seventy-five visits to the nest. Not only had they become 
accustomed to the tent, but soon paid little heed to anything about it, and one could 
photograph them at will, focusing directly upon the brooding or standing bird. After I 
had entered the tent, they would be at the nest in five minutes or even less time, and 
the young were often fed at half-minute intervals. Occasionally both birds were at the 
nest together, but this seldom happened unless the female was brooding. 

On the second day the male came to the nesting branch in twelve minutes after the 


Fig.17. Female Kingbird balancing herself with raised wings while feeding young. 


& 


Fig. 18, Male Kingbird seeing a cicada safely down a hungry throat. 


Fig.19. Kingbirds rending an unruly dragon-fly. The female, who stands in front, was brooding when 
the prey was brought by the male. 


Fig.20. Kingbird family, The male—to the right—has captured a dragon-fly, whose stick of a body 
is seen projecting from the mouth of a young bird, 


25 


Illustrations of the Method. 27 


tent was in position, and the panora- 
mic scenes of life at this nest went on 
without disturbance for the rest of 
that day. The birds were before 
your eye, literally at hand, and the 
observer had only to watch and re- 
cord the rapidly shifting scenes with 
pencil and camera. 

On the third day, July 4th, the 
female was on the bough in six min- 
utes, and in six and a half minutes 
from the beginning of operations fed 
her brood. 

The fourth day of study at this 
nest, or the sixth from the time of 
displacement, was the most interest- 
ing ofall. There were now two foster 
children in addition to the two born 
in the house, for I had transferred 
two birds from a former nest (No. 21 
of table). No protest was made at 


Fig. 22. Female Redwing Blackbird with feathers erect, keep- 
ing cool while shielding young from heat, 


Fig. 21. Male Redwing Blackbird feeding young. 


this intrusion, but the strangers were 
adopted almost immediately and fed 
and guarded with all the care given 
to their own offspring. 

In the space of four hours (8.54 
A.M. to 12.50 P.M.) the parents made 
one hundred and eight visits to the 
nest and fed their brood ninety-one 
times. In this task the female bore 
the larger share, bringing food more 
than fifty times, although the male 
made a good showing, having a rec- 
ord of thirty-seven visits to his credit. 
During this long interval the young 
were thus fed on the average of once 
in two and one half minutes. At each 
feeding usually one and but rarely 
two birds were served. During the 
first hour the young were fed on an 
average of once in one and a half 
minutes. The observer was kept on 


28 Wild Birds. 


the alert in recording what took place, and the scenes would often shift so quickly that it 
was difficult to decide which bird came to the nest. The mother brooded eighteen times, 
and altogether for the space of one hour and twenty minutes. The nest was cleaned 
seven times, and the nest and young were constantly inspected and picked all over 
by both birds, although the female was the more scrupulous in her attentions. 

Whenever the male brought a large dragon-fly to the young an exciting scene was 
sure to follow. If the female happened to be brooding at the time, she would seize the 
struggling insect and try to start it down one of the hungry throats. If she failed in this 
the male would snatch it from her to try his skill, and usually with as little success. In 
this way the prey would be passed back and 
forth, until it was crushed between the bills 
of the two birds, or torn limb from limb. 
Some of these unequal contests between birds 
and insects are illustrated by the photo- 
graphs. 

When the male brought a moth miller 
and accidentally dropped it close to the tent, 
he went after it like a flash, and to place its 
security beyond doubt swallowed it himself. 
Again, one of the birds while perched near by 
was seen to disgorge the indigestible parts of 
its insect food, a common practice with fly- 
catchers both old and young. 

I have added the foregoing details in 
order to show with what harmony life at the 
new nesting site proceeds when once the 


Fig. 23. Young Kingbird eighteendaysold. ‘‘Thelast ; 
to leave flew easily two hundred feet down the hillside.” severed th reads have been united. A knowl- 


edge of former conditions seemed to have 
been completely effaced. The nesting bough was defended with the same bold spirit for 
which this bird is celebrated. The young were brooded night and day, while birds of 
other species were constantly assailed and driven from the premises. 

At noon on the ninth day of July one Kingbird, then full-fledged, was standing on 
the branch beside the nest. When touched he was off like a shot, and at this signal 
the others tried their wings for the first time and landed in the grass. After being 
replaced many times, two consented to remain, and spent that night in the old home, but 
forsook it the next morning, when two weeks old. The first nest, which had been dis- 
placed in a similar way and which as we have seen eventually contained two birds, was 
occupied eighteen days. The last to leave flew easily two hundred feet down the hill- 
side on the thirteenth of July. After taking this one home to secure a photograph, 
I carried him to the hilltop and tossed him in the air. In his second flight which was 
long and good, he made a distant apple tree. Both old and young birds remained in the 
neighborhood for several weeks, and were still there when I went away in early August. 


Cie TER 
TENT AND CAMERA: THE TOOLS OF BIRD-PHOTOGRAPHY. 


HOTOGRAPHY has become so essential to the practice of the other arts and 
sciences, that the student need not suffer from lack of advice, or of detailed man- 
uals which treat every branch of the subject. 

In the notes which follow I shall confine myself mainly to the results of personal ex- 

perience in working with the tent. 

The Observation Tent.—To satisfy the student and photographer of birds, the tent 
must not only afford a perfect means of concealment, but must be light, portable, easily 
adjusted, and to the fastidious, a most important consideration,—comfortable to work in. 

The tent which I have used for two seasons and will now describe, meets these 
requirements fairly well. It is made of stout grass-green' denim, and with the frame 
weighs only six and one half pounds. It can be pitched in ten minutes almost anywhere, 
and may be compactly rolled, and carried for miles without serious inconvenience. It is 
64 ft. tall, 4} ft. long, and 34 ft wide, dimensions which will be found suitable for a person 
not much above the average height. One may spend any number of hours in it by day or 
night, and with a fair degree of comfort, excepting in very hot or sultry weather, when 
exposed to the sun on all sides. I have suspended operations but once on account 
of the heat, but there have been occasions when to have done so might have been better. 

The tent frame is in three pieces, two upright poles or stakes with folding cross-bars, 
and an adjustable ridge-pole. The stakes should be from six to six and a half feet long, 
and may be easily lengthened at any time, as when the tent is to be pitched in a swamp 
or over mud and water. They are pointed at the lower ends which are set in the 
ground, and capped above with an arch of sheet zinc or iron to receive the ridge-pole. 
The latter is held in place with two pins or wire nails which are pressed through a hole 
in the zinc cap, and through the end of the ridge-pole into the upright stake. The eaves 
of the tent consist of a double fold of cloth projecting half an inch, to each corner 
of which is sewn a covered wire ring. When in position the tent is firmly guyed by 
small cords fastened to each ring. The flaps are placed at one of the corners, and may be 
pinned together when in use. The free lower border of the tent is fixed to the ground 
by wire pins, which may be pushed through the cloth at convenient places. From four 
to eight of these pins are needed, and each should be seven or eight inches long, and 
have a large soldered loop at one end. 

The tent may be ventilated from above and made more comfortable on hot days by 


1 Brown or gray might answer as well. The green color serves to render the tent inconspicuous to both ani- 
mals and men. 


30 Wild Birds. 


cutting out a large flap on each side of the roof, extending this a foot or less, and then 
guying each corner separately, at such an angle as to admit a free passage of air under 
the peak. For convenience I prefer the simpler form. 

After working one summer with the tent I saw for the first time the interesting work 
of the brothers Kearton,' in which a different kind of blind is used. They devised an 
imitation tree-trunk, having a 
skeleton of bamboo rods and 
a covering of galvanized wire 
and green cloth, large enough 
to hold the photographer 
standing erect with his camera. 
The outside was painted in 
imitation of bark and decorat- 
ed with moss and leaves. This 
was used in cases of nests 
placed on or near the ground 
in favorable situations. Mr. 
Kearton says it would hardly 
do to set this up beside an ex- 
posed nest like a lark’s “in 
the middle of a bare ten-acre 
field,” and to suit such a case 
they constructed an artificial 
rubbish heap, from which pho- 
tographs were — successfully 
made. 

Such devices are of course 
unnecessary when the nesting 
site is brought under control, 
since in this case the birds 
must become accustomed toa 
changed environment, and the 
addition of the tent is a fac- 

Fig. 24. The tools of bird-photography: the tent rolled up in portable tor of no great importance. 
form at right, 3 
Then again the great heat of 
summer would prohibit their use in most parts of this country. Aside from the question 
of comfort however, the advantages of the tent lie in its convenience and portability. It 
is a simple means of attaining what is chiefly sought, perfect concealment. The reason it 
has not been adopted before possibly arises from the fact that the readiness with which 
many birds become accustomed to strange objects, or form new habits, has not hitherto 
been appreciated. Since individual and specific differences are so great in the class of 
birds, whose distribution is world-wide, one should not be surprised if there are many 
cases in which the tent or any similar blind would not work with success. 


1 Wild Life at Home: How to Study and Photograph It, By W. Kearton, illustrated by C. Kearton. Cassell 
& Company, 1899. 


Tent and Camera: The Tools of Bird-Photography. 31 


The Tent in Use-—Some difficulty may be experienced in pitching the tent in exactly 
che right position with reference to the nest, without the necessity of further change. 
The factors to be borne in mind are the height of the sun, the focal length of the lens, 
and the position of the window to be made in the tent-front directly opposite the nest. 
The front of the tent should be parallel with the nesting bough (when there is one), and 
the long axis of the latter should be parallel with the sun's course. The tent is so placed 
that the nest is in direct line, not with the middle of the tent, but with the window to 
one side. When the observer stands within, facing the nest, the window lies to his left, at 
one side of the vertical stake, and either just over the cross-piece or somewhere below it, 
depending on the height of the nest from the ground. The tent will not overshadow the 
nesting bough when once it is in proper position. 

If the focal length of the lens be 64 inches, and the nest that of a Cedar Waxwing, which 
is mounted at the height of four feet, and the tent be so placed that the front of the lens is 
twenty-eight inches from the rim of the nest, we shall get a picture with adequate setting on 
a 4x5 plate, like many shown in the engravings. With lenses of longer focus, which it 
is advisable to use if possible, it is not necessary to approach so near. The large Robin 
pictures were made with a 93%, inch lens on a 5 x7 plate, at a distance of about four feet. 

When the position has been determined the tent-poles are set firmly into the ground, 
the ridge-pole adjusted and the tent-cloth thrown over it. It saves time to lay one end 
of the peak in position and draw the other over to its proper place. The cross-pieces are 
then lowered from the inside and the guys loosely set. A flap about six inches square is 
then cut with scissors in the front of the tent, to the left of the pole opposite the nest, 
which can be viewed through the opening. Should the position subsequently prove to be 
wrong, the poles may be raised both together and reset. When everything is right the 
guys are tightened, and the free edges fixed to the ground with wire pins, which will hold 
the walls taut and prevent excessive flapping when there is wind. It is often convenient 
to have the flap at the front on the operator's left so that one leg of the tripod may pro- 
ject through it. 

The proper adjustment of the camera follows, the nest being the object focused until 
the old birds appear. I have found it advantageous to pin the focusing cloth firmly 
around the camera so that it is always in position for use, and to stretch a piece of green 
denim on the side of the camera next the observer, fixing it between the front fold of the 
focusing cloth and the tent so that it hangs vertical, and effectually conceals the operator 
when standing upright and setting the shutter. Peep-holes are made to command all 
directions, and of course the nesting bough to which attention is mainly given. It is con- 
venient to make small V-shaped openings which can be pinned up or down. A bird will 
sometimes detect some movement of the eye when close to such openings, so that they 
should not be made larger or more numerous than necessary. 

When a photograph has been made and the shutter is to be reset, the vertical flap is 
released from the focusing cloth and carefully drawn over the window, if the birds happen 
to be at the nest as when the female is brooding. Otherwise if timid or unaccustomed to the 
new conditions, the movement of the hand may bea source of alarm. I have successively 
photographed family groups without disturbing them, when at a distance of twenty-eight 
to thirty-six inches, after they had learned to disregard the click of the shutter. Whena 
window in a different position is wanted, the old one is patched up and a new one made. 


NO 


Wild Birds. 


Ww 


Camera.—Any good long focus camera with reversible back will answer, the size 
and weight being the considerations of greatest moment. Most naturalists and sports- 
men, who travel long distances and carry their own traps, find a camera which takes a 
4x5 plate the most convenient and economical. I have used this, but for work with 
the tent prefer the 5x7 size because it gives a larger and better picture of the object 
sought. The large camera with a heavy lens may be a drag on the mind and body of the 
most enthusiastic pedestrian, but one is usually amply repaid for the greater trouble 
involved. For long journeys however the lightest possible outfit is decidedly preferable. 

In working at short range with lenses of moderate focus the long bellows is a necessity, 
and at the same time enables one to take full sized pictures of small objects, as well as to 
use the telephoto lens should this be desired. The reversible back, making it possible 
to reverse the position of the plate without moving the camera and often without disturb- 
ing the bird, is an adjunct of the greatest convenience. 

While the best tools are always to be desired, excellent pictures can be made with a 
cheap outfit, provided the lens is rapid enough. Nearly all of my own work has been 
done in the tent with the birds at hand, but in taking quick shots of birds or quadrupeds 
when there is no lure to chain them to a given spot a hand-box camera is needed. The 
lens should be of long focus, and the adjustments such as to enable the operator to focus 
and expose as nearly simultaneously as possible. To meet these requirements the twin- 
lens and reflecting cameras, both of which are old inventions,’ have in recent years been 
placed on the market in improved and serviceable forms. 

The ‘‘twin-lens”’ consists of two cameras, set one above the other, the bellows of 
which move as one. The lower takes the picture, while the upper gives the image which 
is reflected on a glass plate set in the top of the box. Besides being expensive and 
heavy, the trade sizes of these cameras are apt to be of too short focus to be of much 
service to the animal photographer. 

The reflecting camera’ does the work of the two lenses with a single lens and 
bellows, and in the recent designs is provided with a focal plane shutter, which is one of 
the best for exposures quicker than the zt, second mark of ordinary shutters. Like the 
upper half of the ‘‘twin-lens”’ it has a movable mirror, set at an angle of 45 °, which casts 
the image made by the lens on a plate of ground glass set in the top of the box and shielded 
by an adjustable hood. The mirror is so placed between the plate and lens that the dis- 
tance from lens to sensitive plate equals the distance traversed by light in passing from lens 
to mirror and ground glass. When the object is focused, a lever is pressed which raises the 
mirror and automatically releases the shutter. One must expect to find the image on the 
ground glass somewhat dimmer than when no interposing mirror is used. To be most 
serviceable this camera should have a long bellows. 

The Lens.—In animal photography short and long focus, and telephoto lenses are 
available. My own experience has been mainly limited to the following: Zeiss Anastigmat 


1 The principle of the reflecting camera was applied as early as 1860, and various forms of the reflex type were 
devised during the next thirty years. In 1891 Dr. Kriigener of Frankfort brought out his ‘‘ Normal Reflex-Camera,” 
in which the construction, though somewhat complicated, was much improved. The principles are essentially the 
same in the later designs: see Ausfivihrliches Handbuch der Photographic, by Josef Maria Eder, Halle, 1891. For an 
account of the reflecting camera with focal plane shutter, by Mr John Rowley, see Bird Lore, April, 1900. 

* Manufactured by the Reflex Camera Co., Yonkers, N. Y. 


Tent and Camera: The Tools of Bird-Photography. 33 


Series ii a, 64 inch, speed 4; Convertible Anastigmat, Series vii a, combined equivalent 
focus 8 inches, speed 3 ; Extra Rapid Universal Lens, Series D,9 % inch, speed {.' 

The convertible anastigmats are convertible in two or three lenses of different 
foci, according as the single anastigmats are of equal or different focus. They thus com- 
bine in a single lens the possibilities of working with short and long focus, the greatest 
speed being obtained when each system of the doublet has the same focus. 


Fig. 25. Female Brown Thrush stepping into her nest to brood. 


In photographing animals close at hand the anastigmatic qualities of a lens count for 
little. It is depth of focus combined with high speed which are most needed, conse- 
quently any lens possessing these qualities will answer. 

One of the most difficult problems in bird-photography has hitherto been that of ap- 
proach within “ shooting” distance. The control of the nesting site, and the use of the tent 
offer a solution so far as life at the nest is concerned, in at least many species, and the 
tent in its general use does away with the need of the very long focus or telephoto 


lenses. 


1 These lenses are made by the Bausch & Lomb Optical Co., Rochester, N.Y. 


34 Wild Birds. 


In photographing birds sitting, brooding, or standing at the nest there is no difficulty 
with a lens of speed ¥, which requires s'; second to fully expose the plate, at a distance 
of twenty-eight inches with full lens and strong light. With scenes in which the actors 
are in constant motion, however, we require a much faster lens, which will reduce the 
exposure to at least s45 of a second. 

For photographing inaccessible nests, and birds which pose well but are unapproach- 
able under ordinary conditions, we must resort to the long focus and telephoto lenses. 
The long exposure required for the telephoto lenses now on the market, from one half a 
second to a second or more, restricts their use to comparatively rare and lucky chances 

The Tripod.cWhen two cameras are carried of the 4 x 5 and 5 x 7 size, a single 
tripod will answer for both, provided it is moderately stiff about the head. A two-length 
tripod of medium weight will serve most purposes, but a shorter one is also required for 
nests on or near the ground. This is best made by cutting down one of the ordinary 
kind, rather than resorting to those of the multifolding type, which are constantly spread- 
ing and slipping at critical moments. 

The ‘ Graphic” ball and socket clamp, used as a camera holder for the bicycle, has 
been strongly recommended as a substitute for the tripod or as an adjunct to it, as in 
photographing nests in trees, when the clamp which is screwed to the camera is fastened 
to a convenient limb, but since my own work has been of another kind, I have had little 
occasion for its use. 

The Shutter.—In photographing birds whose sense of hearing is well known to be 
acute, next to a good lens, a silent shutter is most needed, especially when the camera is 
less than three feet away. The shutter which is silent not only in name but in actual use, 
and at all speeds, is at present one of the greatest needs in the photography of animals, 
and especially of birds. 

Birds will often jump into the air as if shot, at the first click of the metallic shutter. 
Fortunately, however, the force of habit now comes to our aid, since they gradually learn 
that it is harmless, and may be safely disregarded. 

The ‘iris diaphragm shutter,” which I have mainly used, is often troublesome in that 
some part of the sound arises at the very beginning of the exposure, so that a startled 
bird in the course of s!, of a second may be all over your plate. The marks on all such 
shutters, which are conventional rather than exact time measurements, differ in different 
shutters of the same or different make, and their limit of rapidity does not exceed 
“717 second.” For greater speeds the focal plane or some other very rapid shutter 
must be used. 

Flates.—For animal photography the most rapid plates are none too fast, and any of 
the best brands can be recommended. It is always a good plan to adhere to one kind 
which has proved satisfactory. One piece of advice should not come amiss, which is to 
always use fresh plates, and all of the same emulsion if possible, and if any doubt as to 
their age exists, to test them before starting on an expedition. Old plates blacken along 
their edges in a characteristic manner, when placed in the developer, and if deterioration 
passes this stage the whole plate will fog. The dusting of plates, slides, and holders be- 
fore reloading, and the carriage of all unused plates in a dust-proof bag, are as much a 
necessity now as ever. 

Much of my own work has been done in the country with dark room and _ base of 


Tent and Camera: The Tools of Bird-Photography. 


Go 
wm 


supplies close at hand. Under these conditions it is not necessary to carry more than two 
or three dozen plates at a time. By developing on the day of exposure it is possible to 
correct errors or fill up the gaps on the day following. 

Orthochromatic plates require careful treatment, but in skillful hands offer advan- 
tages which should not be neglected. This is well illustrated in the case of birds of bril- 
liant colors like the Orioles, which on ordinary plates appear as ‘“‘ Blackbirds’ (compare 
figures 14, 15). 

Accessorics.—The minor articles which are needed to complete the photographer's 
outfit, all of which can be rolled up with the tent or better carried in a hand bag, will be 
suggested by a little experience in the field. A saw, hatchet and nails are often required, 
as well as scissors, pins, the supply of which is always liable to run out, and a small hand 
mirror for use in setting the shutter from the rear. A toilet hand mirror which can be 
turned at any angle is a convenient means of inspecting the interior of nests inaccessible 
to the hand, but within reach of the mirror attached to a pole. 


CHAPTER TY, 


THE ROBIN AT ARM’S LENGTH. 


It would be hard to find a better symbol of cheerfulness than the Robin singing through the rain. The green 
grass pricking through the April snow is a pleasant sight because it is the sign of spring. For the same reason the 
snow-laden twigs of the apple tree on the lawn take on a new interest when a Robin alights in them and turns its 
bright breast to your window. 


O bird is better known in America than the Robin who annually visits nearly every 
part of the continent. Upon the whole it shuns the forest and comes to the 
haunts of man, to the farm, the village and the city street, with their attractive 

orchards and parks, their long lines of shade trees and green lawns. 

Is it possible to say anything new 
about such a familiar personality? Not 
much, you may think, yet it will be inter- 
esting to study our friend at a closer range 
than is usually possible. In this case we 
shall ‘‘ make the mountain come to Maho- 
met,” or bring the nest from the treetop to 
a point nearer the ground, where there is 
no foliage to obscure our vision, and where 
we can see every thing that transpires, 
within reach of the hand. 

Birds differ slightly in every bodily 
character, as well as in every mental trait, 
and while we commonly meet with average 
types, extremes of temperament are by no 
means rare. This fact is illustrated by the 
Robins whose history follows. 

One pair dwelt in the woods and were 
exceedingly wary, while the other was com- 
fortably settled in town, and lived on a 
familiar footing with man. The town 
Robins had, I suspected, already led forth 
a brood from a pine tree on the bank close 
to my house, but at all events there was a 
new nest in the apple tree on the top of the hill, and on the twenty-fifth of July the 
mother bird was sitting on three blue eggs. Incubation lasted about two weeks, and life 
at the nest about twelve days. 


Fig. 26, ‘‘Robin snow” in April < 3. 


36 


Fig.27. Head of Cock Robin, life-size X 4}. 


Fig. 28. Head of female Robin, life-size x 4}. Photographed at nest immediately after the ‘ 
young were fed. The slime from their throats sticks to her bill. 


a) 


37 


The Robin at Arm's Length. 39 


When the young were three days old the mother passed some moments of great 
suspense. A small flock of Crow Blackbirds alighted on her tree, but either did not dis- 
cover the nest, or thought better of disturbing it after seeing its guardian. The wily old 
Robin stood alert on the rim of the nest, but said never a word, a plan which good sense 
and intelligence could not have improved upon. When the young were eight days old, 
the entire bough was sawn off, carefully lowered to the ground and set up on the hillside. 

In exactly fifty-five minutes from the beginning of operations the mother appeared 
with a large grasshopper, which she gave to the young, and afterwards cleaned the nest. 
The male came also, when the comparative safety of the new conditions had become 
apparent, but approached with more caution. At first both birds flew to the tree by 
their accustomed paths, and examined the place where their bough had been lopped off, 
and in their admirable and fearless manner blustered about for a while, taking no pains to 
conceal their anger. Of course they knew where their young were all the time, for in 
certain directions their vision is keener than any man’s. 

We know well with what confidence the Robin flies direct to its nest, when no 
danger threatens, but under the present circumstances their suspicions might well have 
been aroused. The absence of sound and motion in strange objects is always reassuring, 
and soon Mother Robin could be seen perched on the top of an apple tree, surveying 
the field. She called seet / seet/ while the grasshopper in her bill squirmed to get free, 
and the young chirped loudly in reply. 

When their behavior is free and spontaneous it is pleasant to see these birds act 
promptly without apparent hesitation. They haggle over nothing but follow the bent 
of their strongest instincts. In the present case the fear which controls them for a 
time, and overpowers their strong parental love, is gradually worn away. Suddenly 
down comes one of the old birds with all its weight on the limb. The young have 
felt similar vibrations before and know what to expect. Up go the three heads at 
once, each mounted on a slender stalk, and each bearing at its apex what might suggest 
a full-blown, brilliant flower, for as is well known, the extent of their gape is extraordin- 
ary and the inside of the mouth has a bright orange hue. The young tremble with violent 
emotions as they jostle, struggle, and call with undiminished zeal even after being fed. 

After the first visit had proved successful, confidence was established at once, the 
female and later the male coming to the young at intervals of about five minutes, bring- 
ing grasshoppers, and occasionally removing the excreta or devouring it on the spot. 
They frequently carried five or six insects at one load, when their bills would suggest a 
solid stalk of grasshoppers, all struggling to get free. 

The mother did not touch the nest with her feet at the time of her first visit, but 
clasped a small vertical branch, and bent down over her young, but ever after both birds 
would alight on the broad rim of the nest, and from this vantage point feed, inspect, and 
clean the young, one at atime. They suffer nothing to waste, and rarely allow a cricket 
or grasshopper to escape, but releasing one at a time see it safely down an open mouth. 
Then after inspection is over they fly to the nearest perch, and make haste to clean their 
bills and set their dress in order. This done, there is often a pause of a few moments as if 
in doubt whether to hunt more grasshoppers, to dig angleworms in yonder cornfield, or to 
try the cherry trees along the fence-row. They will take everything which their sharp 
eye discerns, and often pick up an insect close to the nest. 


40 Wild Birds. 


One Robin at the age of eleven days left the family circle early on August 13th, and 
at nine o'clock the two which remained were standing up and flapping their wings. The 
old birds would come near, displaying tempting morsels in their bills, but with no intention 
of feeding their young so long as they remained on the nest. By such tantalizing meth- 
ods they soon drew them away. Both old and young hung about the apple trees for sev- 
eral days, when they disappeared and were not seen again. 


? 


7" 


A 


Fig. 29. Female Robin brooding on a hot day —her left wing pushed up by a young bird. 


At the stage of flight the young Robins have several distinct call and alarm notes 
like those of the adult birds. They can take short, low flights, can hop briskly, and go 
to cover instinctively whether with or without warnings. They will also lie quiet in the 
grass, as in hiding, a common instinctive act. 

The second family of Robins nested high in an oak, and whenever they were ap- 
proached the old birds made an admirable show of pugnacity, scolding, screaming, erect- 
ing their feathers, snapping their bills and darting straight at your head. Their nesting 
branch was taken from the woods to a bare, open field, and set up sixty feet from 
the tree in the way already described. The first morning’s experience was rather dis- 
couraging, for neither bird would come to its nest while the tent was in front of it. They 


Fig. 30. Female Robin inspecting her household immediately after the young have been fed : 
a characteristic attitude. 


Al 


Fig.31. Male Robin serving a cluster of angleworms and a grasshopper. Notice his position 
here on the right as in all other pictures of thisnest. See Chapter XII. 


The Robin at Arm's Length. 45 


called plaintively from the trees, and circled about the nesting bough again and again, 
but always kept at a distance. Accordingly, after feeding the young, I decided to strike 
tent and wait until next day. There was a heavy thunder storm in the afternoon, 
but when I visited the nest towards evening I was pleased to find the young as lively as 
ever, and the old birds on guard with their usual spirit and tenacity unimpaired. 

The next morning they stormed vigorously about the tent and the male even came 
to the nest while I was standing near. After closing the tent I was under the cross-fire 
of their wrath for seven or eight minutes, when the alarm calls suddenly ceased, and in 
two minutes more the mother was on the nesting bough. The female actually came to 
the nest or to the branch which held it eight times in succession, in the space of twelve 
minutes, with insect ready but without delivering it. Matters did not altogether please 
her yet, and with a shrill see¢ / see¢t / away she would go, but only to return a half min- 
ute later. Finally she came boldly to the nest’s brim, uttered a sound like cuck / cuck / 
which means “ Open wide!” and produced a number of sturdy looking grasshoppers. 
Two minutes later the mother came again, and after feeding the young, picked them all 
over, spending a minute and a half in the duties of inspecting and cleaning. It was a hard 
task to conquer these birds, but they had to submit to the inevitable, and I have no 
doubt but a few days more would have brought them to the hand. 

The relative strength of the parental instinct was well illustrated by the behavior of 
these Robins. The female was always the first at the nest, and came at forty minutes 
after nine o’clock on the second day. The male though constantly skirmishing about with 
bill loaded, was not on the branch with food until two hours and ten minutes later. Mean- 
time the mother had been giving the young her constant attention. The cock, though at 
the nest or on the bough several times, did not actually have the courage to feed his little 
ones until long past noon. In the performance of this duty he was three hours and 
four minutes behind his mate. 

When the male did come at last and deliver food, he gave the nest a good cleaning, 
and flew off to acorn patch a hundred yards away. In thirteen minutes, during which 
interval the female had brought grasshoppers twice, the male returned triumphantly with 
a great cluster of writhing angleworms. After safely dispensing them, he went the rounds of 
inspection, devoured the excreta, then stood for a full minute on the rim of his nest and 
with crest erect called, wz / «wzt/ wzt/ as if to celebrate a victory and announce 
his bravery to the world. Now and again the cock came to the nesting bough but without 
food. He wished only to take a look and see that all was well. At one of these visits 
he stood on silent guard for full ten minutes, then sped away calling loudly, czt/ wr / 
wet / 

In the course of the same day a Robin, possibly a young bird, alighted on the 
peak of the tent, surveyed the situation, and passed on. When eight days old, on 
July 26th, the young began to present their spotted breasts over the walls of the nest 
and to spread, stretch, and flap their wings, the quills of which now showed half an 
inch of feather at the tips. At every visit of their elders the whole brood went wild 
with excitement, but soon quieted down, and the intervals were spent in preening 
their sprouting feathers, calling for more food, or dozing with heads hanging down 
over the edge of the nest. 

The third day opened warm and clear, and towards noon became very hot. Mother 


46 Wild Birds. 


Robin began to brood at twelve o'clock and for the space of three hours was on and off 
the nest constantly, rarely remaining longer than ten minutes at a time either at her 
post or away from it. On the fourth day, July 28th, which was destined to be hotter 
still, brooding began at exactly eighteen minutes before ten o’clock and the mother 
was quietly sitting over the little ones when the tent was struck long past noon. 


Fig. 32. Cock Robin standing at inspection, after having fed his young. 


Many charming scenes were enacted at this nest during the day, but colored phrases 
or colorless pictures do them scant justice. You must fill in the backgrounds of soft 
blues and greens, and add the touch of life and color to the actors on the stage. 

The following extracts from my notes of this day may give some idea of the panoramic 
character of the scenes, in which the element of repetition is not wanting. 

July 28, 4th day in tent. 10 A.M. The female comes to the back of. the nest, delivers 
food and gocs the rounds of inspection and cleaning, devouring the excreta on the 
spot, then settles down on the margin of the nest, steps in and gradually tucks the 
young under her breast and wings. 

10.12. A whirring sound announces the coming of the male. He approaches always on 
the observer’s right, and deliberately hops down to thenest. He is bringing a big 
cluster of earthworms. The young get the message the moment the branch is 
touched, and poke their heads out from under their mother’s tail, wings, and head, 
sometimes raising her bodily, and almost tipping her over. However, she holds 


The Robin at Arm’s Length. 47 


her place until her mate is close by, then hops up and stands to one side, finally 
leaving him to deliver what he has brought. 

10.15. The mother is back with food, but it was down the throat of a young one before 
I could tell what it was. Cleaning and brooding them followed in due course as 
before. 

10.18. Cock Robin comes again, but my eye was again off the nest, and in a moment 
the business was done. Mother Robin stays and broods. I change the shutter, 
open and close the tent window without giving her any apparent anxiety. 

10.30. Another visit from the male, who comes quickly, delivers a grasshopper or two 
and departs, while his faithful mate resumes her post of duty. 

10.45. The cock brings another coil of angleworms, and the hen, leaving her charge just 
long enough for the business of feeding, drops back on the nest. 

10.55. The male is taking it easy. This time he has an unusually large grasshopper, 
which is not cut in twain but delivered whole. At the signal of his approach the 
mother leaves, having brooded forty minutes by the watch. 

10.57. Two minutes elapse. Back comes the alma mater, loaded to the muzzle with 
blueberries, which are shot out one by one, and strike the yellow targets in the 
bull’s eye every time. She comes to the farther side and broods at the moment 
the preliminary work of feeding and inspection is over. 

11.16. The male has now brought a load of bright red choke cherries. He hops down 
the branch by the usual path and up to the nest, but the female, who is brooding, 
strangely keeps her position and, whether from absent-mindedness or caprice, 
refuses to budge. When the male gives an impatient cack / cuck / the mother can 
keep her position no longer, for the young upset her equilibrium in their struggle, 
and she hops to one side. Resuming her place she sits there in the bright sun- 
shine, with back to the tent, mouth agape, and crest erect. Twenty inches away 
are the tent, the camera, and the eye of the observer, but for none of these things 
does she now care a straw. They have been thoroughly tested and found harmless, 

11.43. Cock Robin is on hand with a beak full of grasshoppers coming, as is now his invar- 
iable custom, to the right side. On this occasion the mother hopped up promptly 
and received a part of the food into her own bill. Did she eat it? Nota particle. 
The young got it all. The male then retired, followed closely by his mate. In one 
minute she has captured prey and is back to her brood. The young erect their 
crests like their elders, and flapping their half-fledged wings, try to climb to the 
edge of the nest, but without success. 

The last day of July opened hot and sultry, and when I approached the nest one 
young Robin was already out, and making for the highest point of the nesting bough. 
He cheeped aloud for food, and looked uncomfortable, for the heat was already strong. 
The male only was in attendance as on the previous day, the female being occupied, as I 
suspected, in starting a new nest. 

It was difficult to get any food past this enterprising fledgling, who stood in the path 
and took everything that was brought. Several times the bird would make a move 
as if intending to fly to the peak of the tent, and might have done so, had I not decided 
to replace him in his nest. The expected certainly happened, for all tumbled out shriek- 
ing and squealing. Put them back and out they would go again, and flop down on the 


48 Wild Birds. 


grass. At last two birds consented to remain for a few minutes, when the male came 
with an angleworm and a large green katydid. He paused a moment while I photo- 
graphed him, and this proved to be the closing scene. The curtain dropped suddenly 
when first one bird and then the other left their home forever, not even waiting to get the 
katydid. The old bird at once led his brood to the woods, and being able to take short 
flights, they had no difficulty in finding safe quarters. 

The number of times the young are fed in the course of the day depends upon their 
age and the weather. The older they are the more food they require. At this nest the 
labor of feeding and cleaning was shared about equally by both birds, but on hot days 
the female was necessarily less active since there was much brooding to be done. 

The following table illustrates the relative activities of this pair in caring for their 
young, the time of observation being approximately from nine o’clock until three in 
the afternoon. 


| 

PERIOD OF OBSERVATION. SECOND Day. | Tuirp Day. | FourtuH Day. 
Age Of YOUNG: von secee.. 8 days g days | to days 
Bed by°malessiac gags 15 times 24 times | 15 times 
Fed by fémiales cc se ses 18 times 28 times 14 times 
Brooded by female.... 6 times (44 min.) | § times (2 hours 41 min.) 
Rate of feeding......../ Once in ri-r2 min. Once in 7-8 min. | Once in 8-g min. 
Period of observation... 9.30 A.M. to 3.53 P.M. | 9.05 A.M. to 3.44 P.M.| 9,11 A.M. to 1.58 P.M. 


The nature of the food, which depends much on the local supply or the condition of 
the market, consisted mainly of grasshoppers and angle worms, to which we must add a few 
insect larvee, beetles, locusts, and katydids, while the list of fruits included blueberries— 
most in favor—choke cherries, and raspberries. 

As to the sanitation of the nest, inspection, as we have seen, follows each feeding, 
The nest was cleaned during the period given in the table every fifteen minutes, and 
mostly by the female, who devoured a part of the excreta at the nest and carried the rest 
away. 

The Robin has been known to pass the winter in Nova Scotia, where it feeds on wild 
dry fruits, like dogwood berries, and at all intermediate points between its northern and 
southern ranges, wherever the food supply is good. Thus in the cold valleys of the 
White Mountains, where there is snow during the greater part of the year, and where the 
mercury sometimes freezes, flocks of Robins are said to spend the winter, feeding on 
the wild berries which are cached above the snow. The winter birds are probably in most 
cases migrants from farther north. 

The food of the Robin consists, as we have seen, of small animals, mainly insects and 
worms, and of wild fruits in about equal quantity. It has been shown' that cultivated 
fruits are eaten only as a makeshift and mainly in the months of June and July. 

Spring Robins reach Cleveland, Ohio, on the last days of February or the first 
of March, central New Hampshire the third week in March, and I have seen them in Bur- 


1 By Beal who found forty-two per cent of animal matter in three hundred and thirty stomachs of these birds. 


nN 


The Robin at Arm’s Length. 49 


lington, Vermont, on March 30th. A few Bluebirds are usually reported on the same day. 
In 1900, Robins were heard or seen in different parts of Cleveland on the ninth of March, 
a mild, bright day, while but a week before the country was in the grip of one of the 
worst ice-storms ever known in this region. Every exposed object was incased in solid 
ice for days and the birds fasted or starved. 

In the choice of a nesting site, the Robin, as we have seen, obeys no law. The 
apple tree, which from its mode of branching yields wide, open crotches and safe 


Fig. 33. Female Robin in act of cleaning the nest. 


horizontal supports, is generally chosen, but they also resort to the leafy elm, the ever- 
green, the dense and remote woods, or like the Phcebe, accept the hospitality of barn, 
porch, or shed. In the course of one afternoon in Sanbornton, New Hampshire, I once 
found six nests all under cover. One was fixed to a beam inside an old barn, already 
occupied by Swallows, the only means of entrance and egress being cracks between the 
boards of the gable above the haymow. The Swallows shot with unerring aim through 
these cracks, but one of their full-fledged young, which lay dead on the hay, had appar- 
ently dashed its brains out in attempting this feat. In a dilapidated shed of another 
barn, then abandoned, were three nests, two of which set in line and close together, were 
doubtless the work of the same builders. 

Where the nest has already begun to crumble into ruins by the time the young fly, 


50 Wild Birds. 


it is often abandoned and a new one built for the second brood, but whether a new nest 
shall be built or not depends rather upon habit or caprice than actual need. The old 
nest is sometimes repaired, or even occupied without change during the same season. 
On the other hand, three nests are sometimes built in line and under cover, where a sin- 
gle one if put in good repair would have answered the purpose. I once saw a Robin’s 
nest fixed to the end of a stick of wood that leaned against the side of a barn, and the 
stone-gray color of the background formed an excellent screen for its concealment. 
Many wild birds, 
such as Robins, Orioles, 
Wrens, Woodpeckers, to 
mention only a_ few 
species, breed within the 
confines of cities, and 
the question naturally 
arises,— do the birds 
come to town, or does 
the town go to them? 
We know how strong is 
the instinct for young 
birds to return to the 
place of their birth, if 
not to the selfsame spot, 
at least to the same 
neighborhood, and they 
continue to do this until 
driven off by enemies or 
by hard times. My 
house in Cleveland hap- 
pens to be placed in the 
midst of what was an 
apple orchard of a large 


farm a generation ago, 


Fig. 34. Head of female Robin—life size. x 21. 


and a few of its ancient 
trees still remain in the 
back yard. Are the Robins which nest in them to-day the descendants of the birds 
which used to come to the old farm? Possibly, for the birds will return, so long as the 
human inhabitants and the food which their presence insures remain. In this way many 
birds have undoubtedly grown into city life. As the farm became a part of the village 
and the village was swallowed by the town, the migratory species, true to their old asso. 
ciations, returned to their former haunts each spring. I have known two illustrations of 
this in Cleveland, where Red-headed Woodpeckers clung to the ancestral tree until 
enveloped by miles of city streets, and indeed until their old home was actually 
destroyed. 

On the other hand it is true that many shy and timid birds often leave their seclusion 
and come to the haunts of man, and this is not remarkable when we remember how much 


The Robin at Arm’s Length 51 


individuals differ in relative tameness and wildness, and how rapidly new habits are 
formed. 

As to the abundance of food on which bird-life depends, some species, like the Robin, 
would seem to fare equally well in the country, and as to protection, much better. 
Young Robins have no more persistent and fatal enemy than cats, and every one who 
has possessed a city yard knows to what extent it is overrun by tommies and tabbics. 
In the city also one has to reckon with the large floating population of famished 
vagrants, which the biological laboratory is never able to fully claim. They are also on 
hand to rake the young broods out of the nests, and pick up the fledglings which are 
frightened off prematurely and drop to the ground. Though forced to build high, city 
Robins find it impossible to get beyond the reach of some rough-and-ready climbers of 
whom Jan Steen was a shining example, and were every thomas as fearless and expert in 
tree-climbing as he, this race of birds would soon be driven out or exterminated. 
Some Robins used to nest in the very top of a neighboring apple tree, but Jan found 
them out and watched their actions attentively from day to day. One fine afternoon he 
decided to bring down the whole brood. He had climbed to the tree top and was claw- 
ing at the nest, when fortunately his plans and equilibrium were upset in the nick of 
time by a well-directed missile. 

Although the Robin is one of our most common birds its gregarious habits seem to 
have attracted little attention until Mr. Brewster's account appeared in 1890. His 
record for Cambridge, Massachusetts, extends back to 1867. At one roost he esti- 
mated the number of birds at 25,000 (August 4, 1875) The old males and first 
broods in spotted plumage compose these assemblages during the second and third weeks 
of June. By the middle of July the movement becomes more general and by August 
Ist, the roost is made up of young and old of both sexes and of all conditions. Mr. 
Faxon saw a male after feeding its young fly off to its roost one and one fourth miles away 
at 7.30 P.M., while the female apparently remained for the night and brooded her young. 

These local associations seem to be based upon the instinct of protection and 
sociability, and it is important to observe that the old lead the way while the young 
follow, suggesting, as Mr. Brewster remarks, what usually takes place in the annual 
migration.’ 

A winter Robin roost, in a swamp of matted reeds, resorted to at night by thousands 
of birds, has also been described in Missouri. At daybreak the host dispersed in all 
directions, some going fifty miles to their feeding grounds.* 


1 The Auk, vol, vil., October, 1890. *O, Widmann ; 7he Auk, vol. xii., 1895. 


CHa LER vy, 
THE CEDAR-BIRD. 


N the twenty-seventh of May, I saw a small company of birds settling in the top- 
most branches of an elm. You might infer from their behavior that they were 
new arrivals. They keep together, sit prim and erect, and move about as if 

under discipline. With a glass you can see their erected crests, their sleek drab plumage, 
and recognize at once the familiar Cedar or Cherry Bird. 

At Northfield, New Hampshire, the earliest nests have eggs by the first or second 
week in June, but the breeding season is not at its height until the last of July or August. 
A few still have young in the nest in early September, when many are flocking or have 
already started southward. Professor Baird speaks of finding these birds sitting on their 
unhatched eggs as late as the twelfth day of October. 

The winter flocks of Cedar Waxwings, which are occasionally seen in Northern New 
England, are probably migrants whose summer home is farther north. 

The Cedar-birds borrow no trouble from their neighbors, and seem to lead a life of 
ease and pleasure, lessening their denominator when the times are hard, but living high 
when cherries are ripe. The nesting season, which brings much that is sweet and bitter 
to the lives of most birds, appears to give them the least anxiety. The immaturity of 
their eggs at a time when most of our birds have already reared their first broods is a 
striking fact, and is due to some unknown cause which retards the growth of the ovaries. 
It is evidently not caused by a lack of suitable food as some have supposed, since the 
case of the Goldfinch is similar. The young Cedar-bird gets about the same kind of food 
as the young Robin or Oriole, and it is not likely that a greater or less amount of fruit in 
the diet of old or young would sensibly alter their condition. So quiet and retired is the 
Cedar-bird, it may live in comparative seclusion although not three rods from your house, 
and may remain on your grounds for the whole summer unnoticed, unless some one is on 
the watch, so that the name ‘“‘chatterer’’ formerly applied to the family,’ can have only 
an ironical significance in this least garrulous of birds. The fondness of this bird for the 
berries of the red cedar and for cherries is responsible for two of its commonest names, 
while the term “‘waxwing”’ has reference to the peculiar horny scales of the secondary 
wing-quills, which look as if tipped with red sealing-wax. Less commonly, the tail also 
bears similar appendages, but there is much variation in their appearance in both old and 


1 This epithet is said to have been first applied to the Bohemian Waxwing, because of its Latin name, 4 felis 
garrulus, the specific term garrudus having been suggested by the crest and slight resemblance in the color of this 
bird to the European Jay, Carrulus glandarius. See Schufeldt, Chapters on the Natural /Tistory of the United 
States. 


mn 
to 


The Cedar Bird. 53 


young. Most of the birds which I have studied at the nest have been entirely lacking in 
appendages of this kind. 

Late in spring the Cedar-birds are seen coursing about in small squads, selecting some 
treetop for an observatory, and always showing the most marked uniformity, there being 
little to distinguish the sexes either in size or color. Their plump oval forms and easy, 
undulating flight are characteristic, and their manner of flying and perching in compact 
bodies as one bird should not escape the observer. Apple trees of moderate size are in 
high favor, since they afford 
such fine opportunities for 
nest-building, and are usually 
surrounded by good feeding 
grounds. 

Two summers ago some 
Waxwings built on the hori- 
zontal bough of a pine tree, 
just above a Robin’s nest. 
Song Sparrows and Chipping 
Sparrows also occupied the 
same tree. They usually fre- 
quent scrubby pastures, select- 
ing the witch-hazel, or thorn- 
apple bushes by preference, 
and occasionally a small sap- 
ling oak or maple. The nest 
is either set in a fork or sad- 
dled to a spreading branch, at 
a height of from five to twenty 
feet. It is nicely wrought from 
vegetable and animal material 
such as dead grass, roots, fine 
twigs, weed-stems, pine need- 
les, wool, yarn, and twine. A 
nest built in an orchard was 
composed of dead clover stems, 


witch grass, with thistle-down 
and the fluffy heads of the In- 


dian tobacco, a plant growing Fig. 35. Cedar-bird chorus at the most exciting moment just before food 
’ > c=) 


is served, August 6, 1899, two days before flight and the development of the 
close by, worked over its rim sense of fear. Life-size > 3. 
and interior. 

Four or five eggs are ordinarily laid, but the total product of ten nests which I 
examined in 1899 was only thirty-six eggs, out of which about twenty-five young were 
hatched and fromm sixteen to twenty reared. 

The parental instincts during the early days of nest-building and incubation are often 
weak, and this is shown to a marked degree in the Cedar-bird, who is easily robbed and 


ever ready to take fright and abandon its eggs. 


54 Wild Birds. 


During the month of July a pair began to collect nesting material in an apple tree in 
full view from our porch, and I frequently watched them at work through an opera-glass, 
and once or twice passed under their tree. This inspection of their private affairs pleased 
them so little that they left their completed nest, and moved to the adjoining field a few 
rods away, where there was less publicity, and where five eggs hatched out on the twenty- 
sixth of August. A nest built in a young oak tree in a remote clearing was discovered 
on August 7th, when it 
contained a single egg. I 
did not see the old birds 
on this occasion and heard 
but a faint sound, which 
was evidently a murmur of 
remonstrance since their 
nest was promptly for- 
saken. 

I have camped beside 
four different nests of the 
Cedar Waxwings, and after 
having spent nearly a week 
in watching the behavior 
of both old and young 
birds at short range, feel 
that I know by heart most 
of their nesting habits. 

There is a certain rou- 
tine or etiquette which is 
observed by all birds at the 
nests. Certain duties must 
be performed over and 
over, such as the capture 
of prey, bringing it and dis- 
tributing it to the young, 
inspecting and cleaning 
the household, besides 
brooding the young, es- 


Fig. 36. The female Cedar-bird broods, while the male passes the cherries 
around. He stands at the back with his gullet loaded and a berry in bill. pecially during the early 
co) ? 


days of lifein the nest. To 
record cach visit made and every recurring act performed by the birds would make tedi- 
ous reading, but strange to say it never seems monotonous to the observer. As the 
young birds grow older, and begin to stand on the rim of the nest, they furnish ample 
excitement, and while their theme is always the same, it is delivered with innumerable 
variations. 
The method of controlling the nesting site was first suggested by some Cedar-birds, 
whose nest of four eggs was in a thorn-apple bush, and about seven feet from the ground 
The main stem supporting the nest was cut off,and fixed firmly inthe soil at a height of 


The Cedar Bird. 


Or 
Nn 


three to four feet. On returning to the spot two days later, I was pleased to find that all had 
gone well. After getting the tent up it was not many minutes before a low-murmured 
tr-c-c-c-e-k | or se-e-c-c-t ! was heard, to which the young always responded in a similar 
strain. Approaching cautiously with throat loaded to the brim with choke cherries, the 
mother bird delivered them one by one, and then inspected and cleaned her household. 
After a longer interval the pair came and stood on the edge of the nest. There was 
nothing in their bills, but 
their gullets were crammed 
full of blueberries, and 
after tantalizing the suppli- 
cating young for a mo- 
ment, up went a head, and 
presto! out came a berry, 
which was quickly placed 
in an open throat, and 
passed around until it was 
promptly swallowed. Up 
went the head again, and 
the performance was re- 
peated. It was like a ma- 
gician shaking eggs from a 
bag, and there seemed to 
be no limit to its capacity. 
Many who have witnessed 
such actions have supposed 
that the old birds were 
attempting to distribute 
the food without partiality 
to their hungry children, 
but thisis not the case. It 
is ell a question of nervous 
reaction. The food is not 
simply placed in the mouth 
but pressed well down into 
the sensitive throat, which 


4 = Fig. 37. Tantalizing the young. The mother Cedar-bird has come with food, 
promptly responds unless but hesitates to advance and deliver it. Compare with Fig. 38. 


the gullet is already full. 

The old bird watches the result intently, and if the food is not taken at once it is passed 
from one to another until a throat with the proper reaction time is found. The move- 
ments of the bird are so rapid, and the berry is so often quickly withdrawn, that it is 
difficult to make an accurate count. Usually from six to eleven blueberries and almost 
as many choke cherries are thus carried in the gullet. Wilson, who noticed the disten- 
sibility of the gullet of this bird, which will take from twelve to fifteen cedar berries at 
atime, thought that it served as a crop to prepare the food for digestion. The berries 
and insects, it is true, often come up crushed to a pulp and reeking with slime, but it is 


56 Wild Birds. 


not likely that the cesophagus serves any other purpose than a temporary receptacle far 
the food. 

When the berries had gone the rounds, both birds would suddenly leave the nest 
with a whisk. Again one would hear their murmuring call, ¢7-e-e-e-e-k / growing more dis- 
tinct as they came nearer. Then both would alight on the nest rim, and stand there a 
moment like statuettes with heads erect. After regurgitating the food and distributing 
it, they keenly eye everything in the nest, snap up the excreta from each bird in turn, 
swallow it, and are off. 
The young sat or stood on 
the nest with heads up and 
all pointed oneway. Pres- 
ently, every black bead- 
like eye was alert; four 
scarlet-orange mouths 
opened at the same mo- 
ment, and four necks were 
stretched now to this side, 
now to that, whence came 
the least sound. When 
their parents actually ap- 
proached with their low- 
whispered call, they would 
huddle together and 
stretch their legs, wings, 
and whole bodies to the 
utmost. Then would arise 
such a chorus of supplicat- 
ing cries as no parent could 
resist. Touch but a twig 
and the nest presents an 
even livelier spectacle. 
The young fairly tumble 
over each other, while 
their wings, heads, and 
bodies vibrate with an 
intensity of desire which 
their eager voices can only 


Fig. 38. Female Cedar-bird prepared to regurgitate food from the gullet. 
Notice the outlines of the neck, which mark the full throat. ‘‘Twenty min- 
utes later, the last fledgling had left the nest.” August 25, 1899. feebly express. Two days 


ago these young lay quietly 

in their nest, and when touched showed absolutely no fear, but to-day the instinct of fear 
had possessed them, and when approached, all hopped off the nest and hid in the grass. 
Another Waxwing family was discovered on August 15th, in the crotch of a witch- 
hazel bush seven feet up, in the same pasture with the Red-eyed Vireos whose history is 
yet to be told, and not many rods from their nest. A touch to the branch brought off 
the mother, who was brooding three tender young barely two days out of the shell. 


The Cedar Bird. 


After a short interval, during which 
I went to get a notebook and pencil, 
this bird was back again, and once 
more her jet-black eye and clean-cut 
profile appeared above the nest. I 
had sat down but a moment when 
the male flew past, and gave an alarm 
which brought off his mate in a flash. 
Both then alighted in the tops of 
neighboring trees, and standing erect, 
uttered their low responsive call- 
notes. 

Six days later—August 2Ist— 
the bush was removed a rod away 
and the tent placed beside it at nine 
o'clock. The familiar calls of both 
birds were now heard and_in just 
thirty-five minutes from the time of 
closing the tent a soft whirring of 
wings announced the mother bird, 
who alighted near the nest. She 


Fig. qo. 


After feeding the young—the gullet empty. 


Notice 


the ‘‘sitting’’ posture, and compare curves of throat in Fig. 3,. 


nN 
Sy 


Fig. 39. Female Cedar-bird ready to feed young 
by regurgitation—gullet stuffed with cherries. 


approached cautiously, as an intelligent 
bird should do, surveying the situation 
at every step, and finally landed on the 
After amomentary pause she be- 
gan tossing up her head and producing 
black cherries which were judiciously 
placed, one at a time, in the throats of 
her nestlings. 


nest. 


Then a thorough inspec- 
tion followed, and the sanitary condition 
of the establishment was insured by the 
method already described, after which 
the mother remained a full minute ; then 
with a low whistle she sped away. At 
her next visit she began to shield her 
young from the growing heat. With 
half-spread wings and with back to the 
sun the mother protected her little ones 
for a full hour from the broiling sun, 
while her mate came repeatedly and 
handed out the cherries. 
The Cedar-bird will 


pant with 


On 
(oe) 


Fig. 41. Regurgitating food. Up goes the head, 
and presto ! out comes a berry. 


trees, close by our house. Taking 
the hint I placed a quantity of red 
and blue yarn on the branches, and 
on some bean poles near the nesting 
site. Every thread was taken from 
the fir and worked into what became 
avery gay mansion. It was placed 
on a spreading apple bough, at a 
fork in the limb and between upright 
shoots, fifteen feet from the ground. 
The blue varn was in excess of the 
red, but I am sure this meant nothing 
to the birds. They simply took what 
was provided, and had all been red, 
it would have been accepted. 

These birds were most expedi- 
tious, for in two days the last straw 
was in place, and in six days from 
the start four eggs had been laid and 
incubation begun. Ten days later 
three of these eggs had hatched into 


Wild Birds. 


mouth agape when uncomfortably 
warm, but is never seen to erect the 
feathers generally, as many birds do 
in order to keep cool. Nothing es- 
caped that came within range of their 
sharp eyes and bills. One of the 
photographs shows the male on the 
farther side of the nest with cherry in 
beak and full neck, while the mother, 
with back to the camera, gives her 
neck a peculiar twist and looks be- 
hind her. While I was watching the 
performance, a bird of another spe- 
cies, which I was unable to recognize, 
dashed up, alighted for a moment on 
the top of my tent, and giving outa 
harsh chatter, disappeared. 

One day in July I happened to 
see a Cedar-bird tugging at the frayed 
ends of a cord which had been fast. 
ened to a branch of one of the fir 


Fig. 42. She hears a suspicious sound, 


The Cedar Bird. 


young birds, while one was addled. Born 
blind, naked, and helpless, the Cedar-bird 
begins to see when three days old, through 
narrow slits which gradually open, and 
When this 
nest was touched the young would raise 


expose the eyes to full light. 


their tremulous heads aloft, and with red 
mouths opened wide, express in silence 
the simple sign language of newly hatched 
birds. One of the brood mysteriously dis- 
appeared, so that eventually only two 
were raised, and this recalls the loss of a 
young bird from the first nest which was 
built by the same pair. When evil befalls 
a nestling, the parents either remove its 
body or abandon the whole family. The 
latter course is seldom, if ever, followed 
after the eggs have all been hatched. 
Bough and nest in this case were re- 
moved on August 23d, when the young 
were between eight and nine days old. 


Standing at inspection 


Fig. 44. 


Fig. 43. 


: a characteristic pose. 


Cedar-bird listening intently while inspecting nest. 


They were set up on a hillside, in an 
exposed position, with a house on 
one hand and a public drive and 
monument on the other, but the birds 
stood it well, as the photographic 
(Figs. 37,38.) 
Owing to unfavorable weather 
the tent was not used until the after- 
noon of August 25th. 
utes, the female was on the nesting 


record shows. 


In a few min- 


bough, coming and going, but finally 
kept her perch and examined the 
situation critically. Something un- 
usual had happened full of signifi- 
cance to herself and family, but it 
was an enigma hard to solve. Silence 
at last brought assurance, as it usually 
does in such cases. She approached 
nearer, pausing at every step, until 
she could no longer resist the mag- 
netic influence of the calling young- 
sters, who fairly palpitated in their 


eager desire for food. At this nest 


60 Wild Birds. 


the young gave the call-notes repeatedly, but the old birds usually approached without a 
sound, and were never both at the nest at the same time. On the next day the mother 
bird was feeding the young before I 
could set up the tent. Both birds 
came frequently bringing black cher- 
ries and grasshoppers. At each feed- 
ing the following order of events was 
usually observed: the parent sounds 
the call-note at a distance, to which 
the young reply, but observes strict 
silence in drawing near; the young 
are fed, inspected, and cleaned; the 
old bird flies to a convenient perch, 
rubs the bill clean, plumes, and speeds 
off to the nearest cherry trees. 

In the course of the forenoon 
these fledglings became very restless, 
and asthe heat from the sun increased, 
one crawled out, sat in the shadow 
of the leaves, and finally dropped to 
the grass. Here it was immediately 
fed,and then hopped away surprisingly 
fast. The male enticed it along, and 
thereafter took care of it, while the 
mother returned to her remaining nest- 
ling. Twenty minutes later, the last 
fledgling had left the nest, never to return, and the curtain was immediately rung 
down. The young had spent exactly two weeks in their temporary home, and had the 
weather been cooler they might have tarried at least two days longer. 

At this age the crest is not very prominent, and instead of the jet-black, triangular 
band which surrounds the eye in an old bird, the crown of the head is encircled by a 
light band, passing above the eye. At the age of ten days, or a little earlier, the tubes 
of the wing-quills burst, and the red wax-like tips of the secondaries, when present at all, 
also appear, or at least did appear in the young from this nest. 

When about ready to fly and waiting to be fed the young have the peculiar habit 
already noticed of standing erect with upturned heads. A nest of these birds, in 
this attitude makes a curious picture. Any danger signal is now likely to bring 
them off in an instant. This particular brood had their abode in a pine tree close to our 
house. On July 17th, shortly before the picture was made, the family of five was stand- 
ing bolt upright, all facing one way, as if under military discipline. When their branch 
was touched all but the two shown in Fig. 117 gained the nearest trees in their first flight 
and escaped. This pair came to the ground, and were replaced in the nest. In their second 


Fig. 45. Devouring the excreta: an unusual attitude. 


attempt made ten minutes later, the larger of the two birds was more successful. It flew 
to the roof of the barn, not far above it, and after hopping to the ridge-pole, made the 
upper branches of a tall elm. In the larger of the two birds the black band of velvety 


The Cedar Bird. 61 


feathers has appeared in front of the eye and replaces the fawn-colored fillet already 
mentioned. This change takes place in about four days. 

The fourth and in many ways the 
most interesting nest was built ina 
pine, some account of which has al- 
ready been given, in illustrating the 
change of the nesting site. I watched 
these birds over ten hours from the 
tent, saw a great many interesting 
sights, and made a long series of pic- 
tures. 

The young at this nest were vis- 
ited and fed forty-seven times during 
an interval of exactly ten hours and 
forty-seven minutes, on three differ- 
ent days. On the last day they were 
fed on the average once in ten min- 
utes. The food consisted of choke 
cherries and red bird cherries, varied 
with raspberries, blackberries, and 
blueberries, together with insects 
which, during the last days of life at 
the nest, constituted about one quar- 
ter of the fare. At one half the 
number of visits recorded, fruit alone 

. Fig. 46. Cleaning the nest. When the young are fed, the duty of 
was served. From six to ten cher- the old bird is but half done. 
ries were brought in the gullet ata 
time, and once by count eleven blueberries. Feeding was effected almost always 
by regurgitation in whole or part, and rarely was any food visible when the birds 


came to the nest. Now and then, however, a bird would approach loaded to the muzzle, 
with a berry or insect in the bill to round out the measure. Soft fruits like raspberries 
were crushed to a pulp, and insects which are commonly served with the berries, 
came up covered with saliva, and often in an unrecognizable state. The staple animal 
food was grasshoppers and I have seen the large cicada or harvest-fly brought to the 
nest, but never dragon-flies, butterflies, or moths. The cicada made a lively struggle 
for a few minutes; it was placed in one open throat after another and withdrawn eight 
different times, before a gullet was found capable of the proper reaction time. If a bird 
was slow he lost his chance, and another was tried. The key was at last fitted to the 
lock, and the bruised and battered cicada was taken in, but the old bird had not finished 
her task. She began tossing up her head and producing bird cherries. Then she gave 
the nest a thorough renovation. In doing this the mother often walks around the rim, 
and attends to each nestling in succession, sometimes even inspecting one bird more than 
once. 

At first I found it difficult to tell the old birds apart until I noticed a dis- 
tinguishing mark on the female, who had a little bare spot where the feathers had 


62 


Wild Birds. 


come out, on the right side in front of the wing. This shows plainly in many of the 


photographs. 


As I have said in another place, the female 
would often fly direct to the tent and alight on 
the end of the ridge-pole just above the nest. 
Here she would pause a moment, then go to her 
young. Should they fail to respond promptly, 
she gives a peculiar clucking sound, a habit com- 
mon to many species, which is the stimulus ap- 
plied as a last resort. At this signal every mouth 
is opened wide, even if the gullet is already full. 
Indigestible substances pass through the alimen- 
tary canal, and are never regurgitated in either 
young or adults. 

Cedar Waxwings have been seen in the act 
of sipping maple sap in March, either standing 
near a broken twig and reaching round to pick 
off the drops from the underside or hovering 
over the spot and taking sips while on the 
wing.’ 

Towards the last of August, small flocks 
of Cedar-birds are 
moving about in 
search of food, 
the low murmur of 
their call-notes be- 
ing audible for a 
moment only as 
they pass over- 
head. They know 


Fig. 47. Young Cedar-bird from nest shown in k 
Figs. 39-46: photographed on the morning of flight, when the wild cher- 


July 19,1900. The bird was not touched, but occu- 


rie i 
pies a natural perch, chosen by himself. Hee: abe TIPE; and 


never fail to visit 
the trees skirting the fields. The black cherry tree isa pleas- 
ant sight, when laden with the pendant racemes of black cher- 
ries, its tremulous foliage shining in the sun, with Robins and 
Cedar-birds fluttering about it. Every good tree is an aviary 
when its fruit is ripe in late summer and early autumn. Both 
oldand young areon hand. Then you may see one sidle along 
a bough, stretch its neck, wag its tail, and fondle another 
with its bill. Their fine breezy call-notes suggest the 
bleating of the insects in the grass below. Tent caterpil- 


Fig. 48. Cedar-bird about thirty-six 
hours old, blind, naked, and helpless: 
characteristic instinctive response to 
any sound or vibration, as when the 
parent brings food, or the nest or 
branch is tapped. Notice that the 
bird rests on its pot-belly, and uses 
both wings and legs for support. En- 
larged to life. 


lars spin large nests in these trees, but the birds prefer the acid-bitter fruit to the 
insects. Occasionally a bird will leave its perch, and dive for an insect in the air with the 


1 For this note I am indebted to Mr. Robert J. Sim, of Jefferson, Ohio. 


The Cedar Bird. 63 


ease and precision of a professional fly catcher. I have seen the Cedar-bird either taking 
the spider from his web or possibly robbing him of his prey. The birds peck at the cher- 
ries, pull them off, suck up the juicy pulp, but drop the hard stone. The ground under 
the trees, as well as beneath their favorite perches, is covered with cherry stones. Sud- 


denly there is a swirl of wings, and the band moves off rapidly to try the fruit in some 
other quarter. 


CHAPTER VI. 
RED-EYED VIREOS. 


HE moment I touched the spreading branch of a witch-hazel bush out flew a bird, 
and the next instant my eye rested on the nest of a Red-eyed Vireo. It was 
suspended between the forks of a twig about six feet from the ground, and was 

well protected and concealed by the leaves. It then contained two young birds, four or 
five days old. After examining it carefully I retired, but before doing so fixed a cord to 
the branch and drew down the nest so that its brim was horizontal, and the whole about 
four feet from the ground, a convenient height for future study. 

The young were quite naked, save for a sprinkling of light down on their heads and 
backs. They had yellow-rimmed bills, bright yellow throats, and were just beginning 
to see through the narrow vertical slits, which admit light gradually to the eyes. The old 
birds betrayed no unusual anxiety, but uttered their unobtrusive pzort / prort / and the 
female soon approached with an insect. This nest was surrounded by tall bushes with 
barely space to pitch the tent in front of it, and as I decided to make no further changes, 
a somewhat spotted leafy background was unavoidable in the pictures. Coming again on 
July 31st, the tent was soon in place. The female, who was brooding at the time, flew 
off quickly, but returned in a few moments. 

These Vireos soon became quite unconscious of being observed, although literally as 
near the eye as one would hold a book to read. I spent parts of three days on this spot 
watching a most fascinating panorama of bird-life. On the third day the tent was moved 
up to within eighteen inches of the nest, but my lack of experience at this time in photo- 
graphing moving objects at such close range was the cause of many failures. 

On the first day it required forty minutes to restore perfect confidence, or before the 
affairs of the nest were conducted with their usual regularity. The young raised their 
heads aloft and called loudly for attention, or hung drowsily over the brim of the nest. 
At this time their skin was dotted with the fine rapidly growing feathers, and the wing- 
quills looked like slender paint brushes, having just burst the tips of the cylindrical horny 
tubes in which they grow. 

The old birds examined the situation carefully. Their mournful peort / prort / was 
heard again and again, the male answering his mate as she deliberately approached 
the nest. After advancing many times, and turning back as often through fear 
or distrust, the mother hopped up briskly with a bee in her beak. Her instinct to 
care for her young was stronger than the male’s, and she almost invariably approached 
in the same way, by the path of the twig in the fork of which hung the nest. A smaller 
division in the fork gave off a still smaller branch close to the nest, and upon this the 

64 


Red-Eyed Vircos. 65 


birds always perched, and thus stood directly over their brood. Any vibration of the 
nest, as when the feet of the old bird touched the main stem to which it was fixed, or 
any sound above or below electrified the young, and up popped their heads like two jacks 
ina box. With mouths wide agape, they would clamor and quaver, expressing their emo- 
tions not only by the vibration of the wings but by the shaking of the whole body. 
But the young at this tender age are unable to discriminate with any exactness. The 
quivering of a leaf, or the stirring 
of a twig close at hand, a puff of 
wind, the flutter of a wing or the 
voice of any passing bird would 
throw them into the same state of 
excitement. But this was only 
fora moment. Their heads would 
again drop listlessly over the wall 
of the nest, and with open mouths, 
they would doze in the sunshine. 
Something would then suddenly 
arouse them, when they would in- 
stinctively go to preening them- 
selves just like old birds, although 
they had at this time no feathers 
which seemed to need this atten- 
tion. 

Quite often you would hear 
a hute ! hute which always 
aroused the young, who would 
tsip / back in earnest. While the 
mother was again coming slowly 
towards the nest with a bee in her 
mouth, another bee happened to 
cross her path. She darted after 


it but missed her aim. Then, dis- 
posing of the first insect, she Fig. 49. Male Red-eyed Vireo standing at nest after feeding the 
watched her young intently fora = 7°""* ree 

moment, stooped, picked up a small white package, and hurried away. 

At one o'clock the old birds took a midday rest, and it was full twenty minutes 
before that reassuring port / piort / was heard. Then as, step by step, the mother came 
nearer the magnet, the drawing power of which was irresistible, her livelier haze / huie / 
awoke the young, who started and replied szz¢/ swt / Thereupon the old bird quickly 
hopped along the branch, straddled the fork, and tucked a large grasshopper into one of 
the open mouths. In three minutes she was back with another, this time stopping to 
clean the nest again. Five minutes by the watch had passed when she returned with a 
brown gray-winged insect over an inch long, which an entomologist might be able to name 
from the photograph. She paused for a moment while the young called eagerly and 
stretched their necks to the utmost; then she helped the insect down the throat of the 


66 


Fig. 50. 


Fig. 52. 


Wild. Birds. 


Female Red-eyed Vireo ready to deliver a large insect. 


Standing in characteristic prone attitude of inspection. 


lucky bird. However, it stuck 
at the gullet, and the little one 
gulped hard before its protrud- 
ing wings had disappeared. 

Asis well known the young 
bird has wonderful powers of 
digestion and assimilation, and 
after the first week the rapidity 
of its development becomes 
even more striking. A lapse of 
twenty-four hours now means a 
great stride in growth. It takes 
food almost constantly through- 
out the day, and digests it quick- 
ly, though imperfectly. The 
adult Vireo like the Flycatcher 
is said to regurgitate the indi- 
gestible parts of its food in 
pellets. 

The male Vireo seldom 
came with food, and then al- 
ways with anextra degreeof cau- 
tion. Twice he followed swiftly 
after his mate, acting as herguar- 
dian while she quickly went the 
rounds. The role of the old 


Pew . . . 
‘birds in feeding was almost in- 


variably the same, as I have in 
part described. They trace a 
zigzag line to the nest, a straight 
one from it. You hear first 
their responsive call-notes. The 
mother bird with insect ready 
is ina bush a rod away; then 
she comes a step nearer,and pau- 
ses; her fzort / is now more dis- 
tinct. She slowly advances, until 
the twig which holds the nest 
is touched. Up go the heads 
of the young: they call aloud, 
stretch their necks to every 
side, gaze up to the clouds and 
around upon the leaves. Then 
as the mother hops nimbly 
along the twig, and stands over 


them, what a picture of eager 
desire, tremulous impatience, 
and keen rivalry they present ! 
The food is sometimes quickly 
placed in the throat of one, and 
as quickly withdrawn to be giv- 
en to another, and when there 
are more than two it may go 
the rounds before it is allowed 
to remain, a common practice 
the true meaning of which we 
have already seen. 

After inspection is com 
pleted and the nest cleaned, the 
parent bird flies to any conven- 
ient spot, carefully wipes the 
slime from her bill, stretches 
her wings, and smoothes out all 
the ruffles in her dress. These 
birds always look as sleek as a 
new silk hat, every feather lying 
smooth in its place. 

One day while in my tent, 
a small bird of another species 
suddenly darted down upon 
thisnest. There was a momen- 
tary flutter, a clash of beaks 
and claws, and the intruder was 
promptly driven away. 

It was always interesting 
to watch the behavior of the 
young between the intervals of 
feeding. The moisture would 
fairly glisten in their wide-open 
mouths. They snapped at every 
ant and flying insect which came 
within their reach, but I never 
saw a single capture. The prey- 
ing instinct is undoubtedly one 
of the most ancient among an- 
imals, and young birds peck 
instinctively at all kinds of small 
objects, but precision of aim 
which leads to success in cap- 
turing their prey must be ac- 


Red-Eyed Vireos. 


Fig. 53. Male Red-eyed Vireo who is less preoccupied in performing 
the same duty. 


Fig. 54. 


Female Red-eyed Vireo approaching the young. 


Fig. 55. 


Drawing back through timidity. 


OV 
ioe) 


Wild Birds. 


quired by practice. These young Vireos would often hang their heads down over the 
nest, and doze until aroused by the piping of the Robin, or by the call of some other bird. 
Then the mother would appear, with a huge green katydid, its wings crumpled and held 
tightly in her sharp bill. It was surprising how quickly and gently it was assisted down 
one of the hungry throats. 

At one of his visits the male, after cleaning the nest and young with great care, 
stepped in and settled down to brood. In a moment two downy heads shot up from 
under his breast, and I regretted that my camera was not loaded at the moment. 
He showed unmistakable signs of displeasure or uneasiness, repeatedly erecting and 
lowering his crest, and puffing out his throat. With mouth wide open he gazed keenly 
about him, and after a few moments dashed off as if in pursuit of an enemy. 

When a large grasshop- 
per which had been given to 
a young bird had made good 
its escape, the mother darted 
after it, seized it before it had 
touched the ground, and you 
may be sure that there was 
no possibility of escape this 
time A grasshopper was 
sometimes divided between 
the two young, but usually a 
single bird only was fed ata 
time. The male warbled his 
pleasant strains from a branch 
hard by, while the mother 

Fig. 56. Bending over to feed young. hunted for insects in the 

grass below. A large brown 

locust with yellow and black wings was soon brought in. The adult Vireos glean most 

of their animal food from the foliage and, as might be expected, are great caterpillar de- 

stroyers, but while feeding their young, I frequently saw them exploring the grass as any 
Robin or Song Sparrow might do, snapping up every insect which came in their path. 

On the third day, when my tent was but eighteen inches from the nest, the old birds 
came to it even more readily than before. They would still occasionally start at the 
click of the shutter, but they did not mind the shrill scream of a locomotive across the 
river, or the rumble and splash of logs which were momentarily being set free and sent 
tumbling headlong down a steep slide into the river below. They had become used to 
these sounds and had learned from experience that they were harmless. On this day, a 
great change seemed to have come over the young. They had become almost trans- 
formed in appearance, and were very restless. Their bodies were now well covered with 
feathers, and they were beginning to show the first traces of fear. Their snow-white 
breasts gleamed through the thin walls of their cup-shaped nest, or from over its rim. 


Grasshoppers, katydids, green larva, beetles, and bugs of many kinds were served again 
and again, but it would be a mistake to suppose that there was no fruit to vary this diet. 
Upon the third day the mother brought a ripe red raspberry, its juice fairly streaming 


Red-Eyed Vireos. 69 


down her bill, and after a few beetles had been taken, she appeared with a large black- 
berry. Fruit was served to the young about half a dozen times in the course of four 
hours during which watch was kept on this particular day, but I had not seen a single 
berry brought to the young before this time. 

On the first two days of observation the young were fed on the average of once in 
fifteen minutes, but upon the third day food was brought every nine minutes. 

Hitherto I had taken pains not to touch the nest, but as I approached for a final 
look at the young at about two o'clock they immediately took alarm, and popped out one 
atatime. The larger of the two disappeared, and was never scen again by me, and 
although I replaced the smaller bird in its nest time after time, it positively refused to 
stay. Like the young of so many wild birds, when once they have tasted the freedom of 
the world, they seem to look with disdain upon their old home. Although these birds 
could only flutter in their first 
attempts at flight, they could 
hop nimbly from branch to 
branch, and thus ascend readily 
to the tops of high bushes. 

Upon visiting the site of 
this nest on the following day 
one of the young birds was dis- 
covered in the grass less than 
two rods from its empty nest. 
It was calling loudly for food, 
and the old birds were tending 
it. A few hours later I returned 
in the nick of time to save its 


life by the capture of a large 
garter snake which neers wee Fig 57. Inspecting cautiously. Compare suchattitudes with Figs. 50-53, 
had discovered its opportunity. — which express no fear. 

During the past summer, 

a Vireo’s nest was found on the twenty-eighth of June, when the female was incubat- 
ing two eggs. Her plans were, however, suddenly interrupted, apparently through her 
own carelessness. A storm soon ripped up the nest, the walls of which were unusually 
weak and fragile, and the eggs were spilled. This nest was apparently the first of the 
season, and might have represented the first attempt of a young bird. There is the 
possibility, however, that this was really a second and hurried attempt at nest-building, 
due to a former accident. 

The snow and storms of winter usually knock the bottom out of the Vireos’ pendant 
nests, but some remain whole for over a year. Wilson speaks of finding the nest of the 
Yellow Warbler built inside of an old Vireo’s nest. The deer mouse sometimes takes 
possession of an abandoned nest in fall, and converts it into a snug globular house for 
itself and young. I remember the feeling of astonishment which the discovery of one of 
these converted nests gave me when a boy at school, and of wondering to what animal 
those black lustrous eyes, which appeared at the entrance, could belong. In this case the 
original framework was concealed by a symmetrical dome of thistle-down, a substance 


70 Wild Birds. 


used also in lining and covering the original walls. There was a small round hole or side 
entrance, just above the old rim. When disturbed this sleek little mouse left its warm 
house, ran down the branch and disappeared. 


Fig. 58. Young Red-eyed Vireos from the nest shown on page 68. No, 
12 of table, p. 11. 


CHAPIER WIL, 
THE NEST-HOLE OF THE BLUEBIRD. 


HE mellow note of the Bluebird is a welcome sound on March mornings when the 
air is yet wintry, and the snow stands deep in the woods. Its meaning is unmis- 
takable, but to appreciate it, one must live in the North where spring means 

literally “ turning over a new leaf,” a new order of existence. Should cold weather or 
heavy snows return, the birds retire 
for a time, but promptly re-appear 
with better days. 

Robins, Song Sparrows, Blue- 
birds, and Pheebes all arrive from the 
South during the latter part of March, 
and the personalities of these birds 
are too well marked to be mistaken. 
On March 24th, I heard a bird call- 
ing from a distant apple orchard, 
when it presently flew in my direc- 
tion, alighted on an elm beside the 
road, and repeated its low sweet call- 
notes again and again. Through the 
mist not a feather could be seen, but 
there was no mistaking this plaintive 
voice. Five days earlier in the month 
the Bluebird was seen at Northfield, 
thirty miles to the south. The males 
are first to arrive, coming singly or in 
small straggling companies. As we 


walk along the desolate country 
roads, they rise from wall and fence- 


ai ke a 1 : b li Fig. 59. Female Bluebird taking a look outside, as if hesitating, 
row, displaying their brilliant azure before going in search of food. 


wings, or when flying overhead the 
cinnamon brown and white of their under plumage. Their almost ventriloquial “ phee-wr” 
note which is heard as they fly is not peculiar to any season. 
When the females come a little later, the males are in full song, and the period of 
courtship, which is very ardent in the Bluebird, begins. The affection and gallantry of’ 
71 


72 Wild Birds. 


the Bluebird have aroused the enthusiasm of many observers. Unfortunately, we are 
obliged to add that a case of polygamy in this species has been reported.’ 

The choice of a nesting site is made with great care and deliberation. -If they accept 
the house or box prepared for them, they often have to defend it against the Wren, 
the Martin, and the House Sparrow. Wrens and Martins are easily driven off, but the 
pugnacity of the Sparrow, and the greater numbers which he can usually muster 
render all resistance hopeless. An 
abandoned Woodpecker’s hole is not 
disdained since it forms a safe, cozy 
house which needs little furnishing. 
This snug cavern is sheltered from 
sun and rain, and secure from most 
birds and beasts of prey. The rotten 
fence-post, and the many holes in the 
decayed apple trees may also contain 
the secret of the Bluebird’s nest. 

On August 11, 1899, I saw a pair 
of Bluebirds paying marked atten- 
tions to an old “auger-hole”’ in an 
apple tree, made by Golden-winged 
Woodpeckers. It was plainly a case 
of nest within nest. The female was 
carrying insects to her invisible 
young, which I supposed at this late 
date were ready to fly, but, as it af- 
terwards appeared, they were only 
five days old. This hole had been 
nicely drilled beneath the springing 
branch of a truncated and now dead 
prong of the tree, fifteen feet from 
the ground. 

When the opportunity first offered 
on August 15th, I sawed off the limb, 
two feet from the opening, and set it 
up in a convenient spot fifty feet 
away. It was so arranged that the whole trunk could be rotated, and the circular 
cntrance of this nest turned directly to the sun at any time of day. I had barely 
left the place to fetch the tent when the mother bird flew from the apple tree to 
the stump, entered the hole, and having fed the young, came out with a small, white 
parcel in her bill. This bird had her eye on the nest, and was ready to visit it in its 
new situation, when free to do so. The tent was placed two feet away, but later 
drawn up to a distance of about cighteen inches. After concluding these operations, 
I had to wait longer for the parent bird to come again. When one considers that 
the nesting branch was suddenly moved fifty feet from its original position and 

' Bulletin Nuttall Ornithological Club, vol, viii., p. 03. 


Fig. 60. Female Bluebird carrying grasshopper to young. 


The Nest-Hole of the Bluebird. 73 


fixed on the ground, and that a tent was then pitched so close to it that the birds 
could not fly straight to the entrance but had to flit first to the trunk, and then go 
around to the hole, it is not surprising that they held aloof. I waited exactly one hour 
and twenty-five minutes before the mother again brought food to her young. Meanwhile 
it was interesting to see what was happening, from a peep-hole of the tent. Both birds 
would fly to the tree which they had known as their home, and mechanically go through 
their usual motions in approaching 
the nest, hopping first to this branch, 
then to that, following a well-defined 
path, which they had traveled hun- 
dreds of times, and finally hover over 
the spot which was once occupied by 
the nest, as if to become assured 
that their eyes had not deceived 
them. 

These actions were repeated by 
both Bluebirds many times, while 
they uttered their responsive p/ee- 
wr note. Again, calling eagerly, 
both would fly towards the new po- 
sition of the nest. Finally, the fe- 
male, who in this case assumed the 
whole task of feeding the brood, 
came to the stump, paused a mo- 
ment, quickly entered the hole and 
came out in hot haste. The absolute 
stillness, however, had restored confi- 
dence, for in five minutes she re- 
turned with a huge green grasshopper 
and in ten minutes was back again 
with another. In the course of 
each visit the plaintive call would 
announce her presence as she ap- 
proached with insect in bill, and 
alighted on a half-dead peach tree ; : 

Fig. 61. Female Bluebird about to enter nest-hole with green 

close by. Afteramomentary survey  insect-larva. 

of the situation she would flit to the 

stump, sit for a few seconds on a dead branch at one side, then hop down, fly to the hole and 
catch on the bark or cling to the rough edge of the circular opening with her sharp claws, 
pausing there a tenth of a second, or long enough to cast a swift glance backwards. In 
this position she was photographed many times, with grasshoppers, crickets, green larva, 
katydids, and once with a large robber fly in her beak, the profile of her head being sharply 
vignetted by the dark circular entrance. The young must have been all a-quaver at the 
sound of their mother's wings, for the old stump seemed to become suddenly alive with 
brisk chirping sounds the moment she touched any part of it. The bird used her tail to 


Figs. 62, 63, 64. 


was served, 


Wild Birds. 


This series represents the Bluebird engaged in 
cleaning her nest on three distinct visits, at each of which food 
Nearly one half life size, 


help support her weight against the 
side of the tree, like a Woodpecker, 
and I noticed that the tail feathers 
were frayed and worn at the points. 

The male during the numerous 
visits which followed came two or 
three times and sat above the door, 
but never actually entered it, and 
never brought to the young a single 
morsel of food in the course of the 
He would warble very 
sweetly, however, and probably en- 


entire day. 


couraged the exertions of his mate. 
The next time this bird appeared 
with a grasshopper she did not trust 
herself inside, but stood at the en- 
trance, put her head in and as quickly 
drew back to take another glance 
around, then leaned far down and fed 
her clamoring brood. When she came 
again, I made a picture of her as she 
stood at the hole, and in so doing 
frightened her off, but she was back 
in an instant, and another picture was 
secured as she left the nest. At this 
moment a flock of Goldfinches flew 
overhead, and were heard calling dc- 
be! be-be /, at which the young Blue- 
birds were instantly aroused, and 
made the old stump resound again 
with theircries. After many grasshop- 
pers andcrickets had been dispatched, 
a hairy robber fly, or Aszus already 
mentioned, was brought in. Then 
another bright green katydid, with its 
wings half spread in its vain effort to 
get free, was served to the young. If 
frightened in an attempt to enter the 
nest this bird invariably returned 
shortly, and after the feeding was 
over, would take the excreta, and fly 
some distance before dropping it. In 
no case was it known to be eaten at 
thenest. During the afternoon, when 
these birds had become more at ease 


The Nest-Hole of the Bluebird. 


NI 
nN 


in their new surroundings, the nest was cleaned six times in two hours. I saw this bird 
bring to her young no less than twenty grasshoppers, four cone-headed katydids, two 
black crickets, besides larva and many small insects. During the forenoon, in the space 
of nearly three hours, the young were fed on the average of once in six minutes, and for 
two hours in the afternoon once in nine and a half minutes. 

The history of this interesting nest came to an unfortunate close, though through no 
fault of mine. The old birds were subsequently frightened away, and their five young 
ones left to perish. The young were not quite three inches long, and less than a week 
old. They had yellow skins, and bright yellow mouths, and there was a sprinkling of 
plumbeous down on the head, back, and shoulders. 

Toward evening on March 22d of the present year I saw a male Bluebird sitting 
comfortably in an old Robin’s nest, having apparently settled down to spend the night 
there. 

The Bluebird is one of the most unobtrusive of wild birds. [t goes about its busi- 
ness quietly, and seems never to fight, except in defense of its home. According to one 
authority, there are usually three broods, and before the first set of young can shift for 
themselves the female repairs the nest and gets ready for the second. The male continues 
to care for the first brood after the second has appeared, will feed his mate, and even 
take her place at the nest. 


Fig. 63. Standing at entrance with large grasshopper in bill. 


CHAPTER VIII. 


MINUTE OBSERVATIONS ON CATBIRDS. 


days of life at the nest, when any intrusion will arouse its fighting instinct to 
the highest pitch, it is under ordinary conditions exceedingly wary, suspicious, 
and hard to approach. In the account which follows I shall describe only what was seen 


é ve P i: Hy, bu? Be ce Om 


\ \ J HILE the Catbird has a strong attachment for its young, especially during later 


while camping beside two nests of these 
birds. 

The first of these attractive nests 
rested on a spray of the sweet viburnum, 
in a little clearing in dense bushes, and 
about four feet from the ground, so that 
no change in its position was necessary. 
It contained a single addle egg and two 
young with the feather-shafts of the wings 
barely exposed. 

For an hour or more after the tent 
was in position, the old birds kept up a 
perpetual din, in which their exasperat- 
ing ¢shaying note was most pronounced. 
They would circle round and round the 
tent, often coming close as if to discover 
the way in, or fluttering and screaming 
at it, as if it were a demon to be exor- 
cised. After this they gradually became 
more quiet, and began to alight on the 
tent’s guys and roof. At last the female 
was seen stealthily to approach and 
quickly feed her young. After a fresh re- 
connaissance both birds went to the nest 
together and with rapid, jerky move- 
ments stuffed red cherries into the hun- 
ery throats, inspected and cleaned each 
young bird, and then darted away. 


While in a state of mind wavering 
Fig. 66. Female Catbird bringing in a large limp dragon-fly Lae: rh = ir 
—the Aeschna heros between fear and assurance, the Catbird 


70 


“I 
“SI 


Minute Observations on Catbirds. 


passes rapidly to a branch, and spreading and pumping the tail pauses in an attitude 
of attention before making another movement. 

Both birds now began to bring an abundance of insects and fruit, as if making up for 
lost time. The female came with two 
cherries in her bill and promptly gave 
one to each of the two birds. Then 
a grasshopper was served, and still 
again a dragon-fly, with blue body 
and spotted wings (the Lzbcllula 
pulchella). Vhe insect was swallowed 
wings and all, but only after pro- 
longed efforts. As confidence was 
gradually regained, the birds would 
remain longer and longer at the nest, 
pick the young all over, and clean 
everything with care and delibera- 
tion. 

At this time (July 23d) the young 
were about eight days old, and could 
be easily approached. Two days later 
when their nest was touched, they 
tumbled out in an instant, disappear- 
ing as if by magic amid the leaves. 
I succeeded in finding one of them, 
but it refused to remain in the old 
nest. Its wing-quills now showed a 
half inch of the feather-shaft, which 
represented two days’ growth, while 
the tail feathers were still in the stub- 
brush stage. 

There were four young in the 


second nest, which was discovered in 


Fig.67. Female Catbird inspecting after having fed the young. 


some bushes close to the river bank 
onthe nineteenth of June. It rested in the crotch formed by the crossing of shoots of 
the dogwood and alder. The young were in pin-feathers, but not a tube had burst. Both 
old birds happened to be off foraging, but quickly returned with food in their mouths, 
and began to alarm the neighborhood. 

The tent was pitched in front of this nest at eight o’clock on the morning of June 23d. 
After it was closed both birds began their cautious explorations in the vicinity, ‘schaying 
incessantly and with nerve-rasping vehemence. A male Redwing Blackbird was soon 
attracted to the spot, and added his note of alarm to the general outcry, but after finding 
that the matter did not concern him, returned to his nest in the flags farther away. 

In twenty minutes the Catbirds had become more quiet, and began to pay close 
attention to the tent. The Redwing was heard con-guer-ceing in the distance. Song 


c=) 
Sparrows were singing merrily. Veeries called from the woods close at hand, and the 


78 Wild Birds. 


be-be of the Goldfinches could be heard as these birds passed leisurely overhead. The 
conditions were all reassuring, and presently the Catbirds became silent, and went off for 
food. Ina few moments a rustling of leaves was heard close to the tent and the male 
could be seen coming boldly in its direction. 

Up to this time the young lay quietly in the nest, but were alert to every sound, 
whether from the wind or any passing bird. Their wing-quills had become exposed in 
the course of two days to a length of three quarters of an inch. 

Suddenly a jubilant song burst forth from the throat of the male, and his mate thus 
encouraged approached the nest with insect in bill, but her fears were not allayed, for 
after beating about she swallowed the insect herself and went in search of another. 

The young now began to yip in earnest and to stretch their scantily feathered trans- 
parent necks. One of the lustiest of the four even climbed to the edge and sat in the 
shade. They would erect their scanty crest-feathers and pant in the sun, which though 
not excessively hot, was with the added feeling of hunger, beginning to make them 
restless. 

The sense of fear was at last overcome in the mother, who came, fed and cleaned the 
young, and flew off again. After another pause a huge dragon-fly was brought to the 
nest. The observer had to wait long at the beginning, but his reward was now quick in 
coming. The young were then fed every five or six minutes, but the male only rarely 
went to the nest himself. Still cautious to a degree, he would follow after the female, 
but stop a few feet short of the nest. Then after delivering her insect she would go at 
once to her mate, take the food from his bill, and bear it to the young. 

The following table gives the number of visits at which food was brought during 
eight consecutive hours from 8 A.M. to 4 P.M., and illustrates how the parental instincts, 
aided by habit, gradually overcome the feeling of fear in a very shy and suspicious animal. 


HOUR, | DOE TIMES NEST CLEANED. REMARKS, 
[YOUNG ARE FED. 
| ae Se I —— 
I 
3 t I I Young fed by female. 
I “cc “ “ee ee 
3 “e “ “ oc 
4 5 x ; 
5 8 2 Young fed once by male. 
6 10 I Young fed twice by male, who also brings food which 
a 5 female delivers. 
7 2 Old birds begin coming to nest together. 
& 17 4 Young fed twice by male, who also brings food for female 
| ___ to serve. aimee 


Five times in rapid succession the mother brought in dragon-flies of extraordinary 
size (the large Zischna heros), of a light greenish yellow color, and limp as wet paper, 
having just issued from their pupa cases. This bird presented an interesting sight as 
she approached with one of these long insects hanging from her bill, for she always 
held them by the head. The dragon-fly was as long as the young bird, but it was 
invariably swallowed wings and all, though only after a hard struggle. 

The young, always on the alert huddle to this and that side of the nest, and stretch- 


Minute Observations on Catbirds. 79 


ing to the utmost limit their transparent red necks display the yellow target of the open 
mouth as they ¢s7/ ¢s7t/ to the approaching mother, who sounds her well-known call. 
On one occasion I saw the female deliver a black dragon-fly, and afterwards take 
from the bill of the male, who was standing near, a carrion beetle, and pass it to the young. 
Then keenly eying her brood, she 
deliberately bent over, and as the 
body of one was raised took from it 
asmall white package and flew away. 
Many of the photographs show the 
birds performing this sanitary act, a 
practice common to many other spe- 
cies. During her first visits the female 
ate the excreta, but thereafter it was 
invariably removed from the nest. 
The food served to these young 
Catbirds consisted of dragon-flies, 
which were brought to the nest thir- 
teen times, insect larve, beetles, 
moth millers, and a great variety of 
smaller insects, varied with liberal 
courses of strawberries. At first the 
old birds approached quietly, fed 
their young hurriedly from the farth- 
er side, and were off in a few seconds, 
but as confidence in their surround- 
ings was gradually restored, they 
would come to the nest-front, with 
the camera but three feet away, re- 
main there fora full minute, and after 
assisting the young to dispose of their 
harder subjects, inspect everything 


with the greatest care. 
When this nest was visited two Fig. 68. Female Catbird cleaning the nest. 

days later the young looked bright 

and hearty. They were now in full feather, and about ready for flight. When the tent had 
been cautiously set up, I noticed that a number of leaves cast undesirable shadows on 
thenest. Though knowing well what to expect, I decided to take the risk, and reached 
out to cut them off. This was the fatal spark which fires the train of gunpowder, for all 
went off in an instant in a panic of fear, and the game was up, for Catbirds when well 
out of their nest at this stage are out for good. 


CHAPIEER 1X. 
THE REARING OF THE NIGHT HAWK. 


N crossing a clearing one day in June I flushed a Night Hawk, who showed by her 
behavior that the little depression from which she rose contained something of great 
interest to both the bird and myself. She was indeed incubating a single marbled 

gray egg, which lay on a marbled-gray patch of earth still covered with ashes and cinder. 
The bird retired quietly, dropping with a thud to the ground a few feet away. 

Two days later, if my estimate is correct, a young Night Hawk cracked his shell 
neatly in two and emerged to the light of day. When first seen on the twenty-sixth of 
June, he was well clothed in down, and looked like a little flattened ball of fluffy worsted, 
of a dark cream color mot- 
tled with brown, colors 
which harmonize well with 
the usual tints of the soil. 
You had to look a second 
time to detect the stub of 
a beak at the base of which 
the large round nostrils 
were sufficiently prominent. 
Whenever this bird was 
aroused from its all-day 
slumbers the eyelids would 
gradually open and disclose 
a pair of large, soft, deep 
blue eyes, the lower lids 
showing decided angular 
contours which became 


Fig. 69. Night Hawk and eggshells from which it emerged. Three days 
old, June 27, 1900. more striking as the bird 


grew. 

The mother brooded during the heat of the day or sat as if dozing beside her charge. 
When surprised at such times she rose and with feathers erect and tail spread fluttered 
off in a slow shambling manner as if to encourage pursuit. With her feathers raised and 
her huge mouth wide open or the mandible vibrating up and down, with an audible snap- 
ping sound, as if set on springs, this bird presented a curious appearance, recalling the 
not wholly dissimilar behavior which eagles display when stirred by similar emotions. 

80 


The Rearing of the Night Hawk. 81 


When the young Night Hawk is exposed to a hot sun, its lower jaw also begins to vibrate 
but at a much higher rate of speed, when it will toddle off and crouch in the shade of 
aleaf. It begins to walk when three or four days old, but rarely emits a sound, except 
under circumstances which will be presently described. Fearing lest the old bird should 
entice it away, I coralled it in a small enclosure of wattled twigs on July 3d. In this 
pen it remained a week longer or 
until able to fly at the age of about 
eighteen days. 

Wishing to witness the feeding 
habits of these birds, which I believe 
have never been described, I spent 
parts of three days and nights camped 
beside the enclosure and was the wit- 
ness of some interesting and curious 
sights. On the first day I set up the 
tent at three o'clock in the afternoon, 
but heard no sound for an hour, when 
the young began to fe-wp/ At five 
o'clock the prsk / of the male sound- 
ed for the first time. During the 
interval a single incident occurred to Fig. 70. Night Hawk three days old. Nearly life size. 


vary the monotony. A green snake 
in the course of his rambles had dis- 
covered the young Night Hawk, and 
when first seen was watching the bird 
intently from a stump close to the 
tent. The snake after remaining with 
elevated head keenly eying the bird 
for along time, slowly advanced, put- 
ting out his tongue, but when a few 
inches away hesitated again, and as if 
deciding not to experiment further, 
turned to one side and disappeared. 
The bird paid no attention whatever 
to the advances of the snake. At this 
juncture I left the tent for an hour, 
returning as the sun was setting at 
half-past seven o’clock. 

At dark a change begins to come 
over the Night Hawk family. The 
young bird shows signs of life, moves 


Fig. 71. Night Hawk nine days old, July 3d. Length in sit- 
about calling for food, and grows | ting posture, 3} inches. 


livelier as the darkness increases, 
making a sound like pe-ur / pe-up/ Both old birds are now alert and gyrating overhead, 
You hear their pzsk/ pisk/ and the startling sound caused by the vibration of the wings 


NO 


8 Wild Birds. 


as an old bird descends like a bolt toward the earth. As these sounds increase with their 
nearer approach, the nervous excitement of the young is curious to behold. He is all 
a-tremor, Moves now in one direction, now in another and his pe-wr / note reaches a 
pitch unknown before. 
Presently you hear a 
thud as if a clod of 
earth had dropped. 
Then the mother bird, 
crawling over the 
leaves, begins calling 
ke-ark! ke-ark! This 
sound however un- 
couth to the human 
ear, corresponds to the 
cluck ! of the hen to 
her chicks, and awak- 
ens an immediate re- 
sponse in the young 
Night Hawk. He does 
his best to go to his 
mother, but the ob- 
Fig. 72. Night Hawk twelve days old, July 6th. stacles being insur- 


mountable, she comes 
to him. She is load- 
ed with fireflies, and 
as her great mouth 
opens, you behold the 
wide jaws and throat 
brilliantly illuminated 
like a spacious apart- 
ment all aglow with 
electricity. With wings 
erect and full-spread, 
the old bird ap- 
proached to within 
fifteen inches of my 
wm ——- hand, making an elec- 


hag agar 


* tric display at every 
Fig. 73. Night Hawk sixteen days old, July roth. Length in sitting posture, 4} inches. utterance of her harsh 

ke-ark / Then stand- 
ing over her young, with raised and quivering wings, she put her bill well down into his 
throat and pumped him full. His down-covered wings were also spread and a-quiver. In this 
position they remained interlocked and silent for one or two minutes. When the feeding 
was over she tucked the little one under her breast and began to brood. It was not long 


before she was off again in the darkness, and upon returning the performance was repeated, 


Fig. 74. Front view of bird shown in Fig. 72. 


Fig. 75. Young Night Hawk in enclosure on spot where it was born, and where it remained 
until able to fly when eighteen days old. 


83 


The Rearing of the Night Hawk. 85 


after which she settled down to brood asif for the night. This young bird was fed but twice 
each evening between the hours of eight and nine o'clock, and always, as I believe, by the 
female. It is probable that another feeding time also occurs at dawn. During the earlier 
hours the male would sometimes swoop down with terrific wing-blast as if to drive away 
intruders, and he once came and sat by his chick for ten minutes after dusk without 
causing any excitement. The task of feeding was borne by the mother, and her presence 
never failed to excite the young. 

I tried to make a flash-light picture of the old and young bird interlocked in the feed- 
ing process, and could easily have succeeded had my lamp been of a kind which showed 
no light before the flash. 

In two weeks the mottled down of the Night Hawk chick has given place to mottled 
feathers, in which the tints range from dark to light brown or buff. The wing-quills are 
almost black with buff edges. The fifth quill or primary has a pure white transverse spot 
near the point of emergence from the feather tube, the first trace of what becomes a con- 
spicuous mark on the wings of an adult bird. The fledgling is more lively in the day- 
time, runs about easily, will utter his pe-wv / note, and can fly short distances. 


OG os 2. i al 8 See. 
THE KINGFISHERS AND THEIR KING ROW. 


HE Kingfisher has a strong attachment for particular nesting places, and will 

occupy the same bank for years, if unmolested, and sometimes even when robbed. 

The Belted Kingfisher, though widely distributed, seems to be nowhere very 

abundant. In New Hampshire one rarely finds more than a single pair nesting in the 

neighborhood of any village 
or town. 

The nest now to be de- 
scribed was drilled into a 
sand bank beside a country 
road. It had astraight four- 
inch bore, which four feet 
from the opening expanded 
into a low-vaulted chamber 
six inches high and ten 
inches across. When this 
dark subterranean abode 
was opened at the rear, on 
the nineteenth day of July, 
1900, I put in my hand and 
drew forth in succession five 
very strange looking creat- 
ures. They had huge coni- 
cal bills, short legs, and fat 
squatty bodies, which bris- 
tled all over with steel. gray 
“quills,” the feather tubes, 
which had not yet burst, 


’ 


suggesting an antediluvian 
monster or reptilian bird on 
a reduced plan. 

These five young King- 
fishers which were then 


Fig. 76, Tunnel of Kingfisher—the opening seen at the right—in sand bank : 
Overgrown with pines, beside country road. August, 1899. about nine days old had 


86 


Fig. 77. Nest in same bank as shown in Fig. 76, and probably belonging to same pair. Tak- 
ing fish to young. July, 1go0, 


Tee 
ae 
La, heey 


Fig. 78. Kingfisher backing out of tunnel. The sand streams from the opening at every en- 
trance and exit. 


87 


The Kingfishers and their King Row. 59 


already acquired some curious habits. They, like the adult birds, stand not on the toes 
simply, but on the whole tarsus, which corresponds to the scaly part of the leg of a fowl, 
so that the ‘drum-stick” rises from the heel. They can be posed in any position like toy 
soldiers, but if placed in line they will soon break ranks and walk backwards, even mov- 
ing up inclined planes or against 
obstacles set in their paths. They 
are rarely seen to take a single for- 
ward step for many days after reach- 
ing this stage. 

The human infant and verte. 
brated animals generally instinctive- 
ly walk forward; how then does it 
happen that the young Kingfisher 
early acquires the grotesque habit 
of walking backwards? The anom- 
aly is readily understood. From 
the time of birth the young lie hud- 
dled in a cluster in their dark un- 
derground chamber, which opens : mee : 

, 0 Fig. 79. Five Kingfishers from chamber at end of tunnel; nine days 
to the outside by means of a single oa. July 19, 1900. 
narrow tunnel. As they grow in 
size and strength the monotony of 
sitting still, often with legs and 


wings interlocked, must become 
very great, and whether for diver- 
sion or not, at all events they soon 
begin to bite and tease one another 
like young puppies. Should one be 
hard-pressed, the only way of es- 
cape lies along the narrow passage, 
which they naturally traverse head 
first; but the instinct to return to 
the warm family cluster is strong, 
and to do this they are obliged to 
walk backwards. Again when the 
rattle of the alma mater announcing 
the capture of another fish is heard, Fig. 80. Posed in line,—biting, pulling, and crowding one another. 
each struggles to get down the nar- 

row passage-way first, but when the parent enters the hole she hustles them all back. With 
each backward movement the young Kingfishers thus come to associate pleasant things,— 
food and warmth. Thus the habit is temporarily fixed. 

Wishing to see these birds take fish to their young, I decided to try the tent, al- 
though it was impossible to get nearer than eight feet, and the hole was in full light for 
only a part of the forenoon; besides, being situated on the roadside, one was in constant 
danger of interruption. The experiment succeeded, however, even better than I had 


gO Wild Birds. 


anticipated; ten visits were recorded, and the old birds were photographed in the act of 
both entering and leaving their tunnel. They brought a single fish each time, usually 
what appeared to be a small chub or dace, and I once recognized a good-sized sunfish. 
When the tent and camera were ready at nine o'clock on the morning of July 23d, 
the parent birds were away on a fishing excursion, and did not return for half an hour. 
At last a series of warning rattles, at first faint, but momentarily becoming more shrill, 
announced the approaching 
bird, who came at full tilt 
with fish in bill.  Hesitat- 
ing at sight of the tent she 
perched on the dead limb 
of a pine, flew to and fro 
from one side of the road to 
the other, and made the 
woods resound as never be- 
fore. Even the depthsofthe 
earth seemed to respond, 
as the muffled rattles of 
the five young Kingfishers 
issued from their subter- 
ranean abode. From what- 


Fig. 81. The ‘‘ King Row.” Five Kingfishers in line, illustrating habit of sit- CVE point of view we regard 
ting still. July 19, 1900. this singular note, it cer- 
tainly carries well and is ad- 
mirably adapted to arouse 
the fish under water and the 
young bird under ground. 
When the wriggling fish 
nearly slipped from her 
grasp, the bird would shift 
it about until her forceps 
had a firmer grip at a point 
just back of its head. At 
every reel of the rattle, each 
of which seemed more shrill 
and more impatient than 
the last, she would start as 


if to go to her nest a few 
Fig. 82. The ‘‘ King Row” at a later period ; thirteen days old, July 23, 1900. yards away. Occasionally 
a peculiar creaking sound es- 

caped her, suggesting the grating of dead limbs when swayed by the wind. Suddenly with 
rattle in shrillest crescendo she bolted straight into the hole, delivered the fish, remained for 
halfa minute, then came out backwards, turning in the air as she dropped from the entrance, 
and with a parting rattle was off to the river. During these visits the Kingfishers usually 
remained but a quarter or half a minute in the tunnel, and always came out backwards, 


The Kingfishers and their King Row. gI 


except on one occasion when I saw the bird turn near the entrance, and shoot out head 
first. The longest visit recorded lasted three and a half minutes. When a youngster 
was encountered near the mouth of the tunnel he was driven back to the chamber, where 
the food was distributed. Once 
only did I see an old bird pause 
at the entrance for a hasty 
glance backward, and thus 
give a good profile view of 
head with fish in bill. Unfor- 
tunately the plate had already 
been exposed, and before it 
could be changed, the oppor- 
tunity was lost. The old birds, 
however, must have often 
turned about at the entrance 
on both entering and leaving 
the hole, as shown by the deep 
furrows plowed by the bill at 
either side of the opening. 
When the young are ten 


days old, the feather tubes 
have begun to burst at the tips, Fig. 83. Kingfisher nine days old, showing feather tubes and tracts. 


and their horny substance is 
gradually shed in the form of 
powdery scales. The feathers 
grow slowly, but at the age of 
two weeks the characteristic 
colors of the adult are becom- 
ing apparent,—the slaty-blue 
of the upper parts, and the 
white of the breast which is 
traversed by a bluish-brown 
belt, with rusty brown along 
the sides. As they rattle when 
taken from the nest their 
whole body quavers. They 
will hiss, bite one another, hud- 
dle together, and erect their 


crests of long stiff feathers. Fig. 84. At thirteen days ; many of the feather tubes burst. The blue- 
: 2 black, white-tipped wing-quills project half an inch. Notice that these 
They attain to full plumage or birds always stand, not on the toes only, but on the short shank or tarsus. 


nearly so when three weeks 
old, at which time their bright fresh colors and docile natures make them most attractiy 
They can fly but little, and show no fear. At this stage their habitual expression sugges 
a peculiar sardonic grin. 

On the fourth of August I took these birds home in a basket, when twenty-five days 


es 
ts 


92 Wild Birds. 


old, if their age was correctly estimated. They were about ready to fly and would have 
voluntarily left their nest in a short time. The nesting chamber had been gradually 
opened up in front and filled at the rear, until it had advanced a foot and a half toward 
the mouth of the tunnel. At this time fear was possessing them, and a day later it was 
impossible to handle them with- 
out throwing them into a panic. 
When quiet they would still 
pose well, would strike with 
open bill, and walk backwards. 
During captivity I fed them 

on fish which, however, they 
would never seize of their own 
accord. It was necessary to 
open their bills and press the 
food well down into their dis- 
tensible throats. They would 
perch ona branch placed in their 
cage, drink water and sit in it 
by the half-hour, but never 
touch the most tempting mor- 
Fig. 85. Kingfisher fifteen days old, with nearly all feathers partly un- sels of food. Raw meat was 
i a a rejected, but they throve on 
fish if fed by the hand. When 
perched they stood as before on 
the whole tarsus or shank, and 


would sit together and in si- 
lence, with breasts thrown out, 
for hours. You heard only an 
occasional rattle, and that usu- 
ally inthe morning. The King- 
fisher’s cesophagus is very dis- 
tensible and the throat is lined 
with inwardly projecting papil- 
lee, so that when a fish is once 
taken in the throat, it is impos- 
sible for it to escape. 

The bill of the Kingfisher 
Fig. 86. Ateighteen days. The bright blue tints of the upper parts, and is grooved on the inside, thus 


the white and chestnut bands around the neck and breast are now very = 8 . 
prominent, July 28, 1900. giving the mandibles sharp cut- 


ting edges and a firm gripe on 
the prey. A fish once seized rarely makes its escape, to prevent which the bird has other 
resources. I once saw a curious trick performed by a Kingfisher, who having made a good 
capture, was perched on a dead tree over the water. In the course of its struggles the 
fish nearly got free, and for a moment was held only by its tail. The bird with a quick 
movement of the head tossed the fish in the air, and as it descended caught it by the head 
and proceeded to swallow it. 


The Kingfishers and their King Row. 93 


When liberated on August 12th, at the age of thirty-three days, the young King- 
fishers were suddenly thrown upon their own resources, and it was questionable whether 
they would be able to recover the instinct to seek and capture prey. However, they 
were strong and healthy, and I hope that nature came to their aid not only in prompting 
them to find food, but in starting them south later in the autumn. 


Fig. 87. Kingfishers twenty-two days old. Placed in line to illustrate habit of walking back- 
wards. The second bird at the left has already broken ranks and taken a few backward steps. 
August I, Igoo, 


Chive PER 2h 


CARE OF YOUNG AND NEST. 


BROODING AND FEEDING THE YOUNG. 


of the night, and in some species the young seem to require this kind of pro- 

tection as much as food. During the first days of life in the nest it is not 

easy to distinguish a brooding from a sitting bird, but this is not the case when a little 

later the mother begins to rest her wings over the rim, or spreading wings and tail stands 

astride the nest with back to the sun. The young must be protected from heat, cold, 

and rain, and the instinct to perform this duty is as strong with old birds as that of 
bringing food. 

Cedar Waxwings and Kingbirds which I have watched, brooded regularly at night, 

but I have known young Robins to be left alone in the nest. Should the day be cloudy 

but with no rain, or sunny but not too warm little or no brooding has been observed 


W HEN the callow young are hatched, brooding is the order of the day as well as 


among the various species which I have studied, but let the sun beat relentlessly upon 
the young, or the air become laden with moisture, and the faithful mother is promptly at 
her post. I have seen the Robin brood the young when eleven days old for forty minutes 
at a time, while her mate brought an abundance of food. As he approached with an 
insect or cluster of worms, she would step aside, but immediately settle back on the nest 
when the food had been safely disposed. Asa rule, however, she would brood for five or 
ten minutes, leave at the approach of the male, return promptly with food, and brood 
until her mate again appeared. I have on several occasions seen a brooding bird leave 
the nest when the sun became temporarily obscured and return when the clouds lifted. 
It was not quite certain, however, that the element of chance did not vitiate the 
observation. 

While camped beside a nest of Brown Thrushes whose photographs are shown, and 
whose young were approximately four days old, the female came to the nest for inspec- 
tion frequently on the first day of observation, and brooded intermittently, but fed her 
young only once in the space of three and a half hours. When I frightened this bird 
off with the hand stretched through the tent-window, she would dart at it, scold em- 
phatically, but in a few moments return to her brooding again, as if her young required 
this attention more than food. 

The Chestnut-sided Warbler who is represented in many characteristic attitudes 


94 


Fig. 88. Female Brown Thrush brooding her young. Lens, Extra Rapid 9; inch; speed, {; 


stop, 32; time, } second; plate, Seeds’ No. 27 ‘‘ gilt edge’’; distance, four feet; in full sun, 
July 13, 1900. 


95 


Care of Young and Nest. 97 


about the nest by a long series of photographs, only a few of which can be shown, was a 
most devoted brooder for days. She would stick to her charge until driven off by sheer 
force or by hunger. I have often seen her drop down in the grass, pick up a morsel on 
her own account, and be back to the nest in a fraction of a second before the insect was 
fairly swallowed. Again she might leave the nest twenty times in the course of an hour 
to procure food either for herself or her children. Her mate would often alight above 


. 


Fig. 89. Female Robin brooding: a characteristic attitude when alert, or listening to any unusual sound. 


the nest, bend far down and deliver the insects into the mouth of the brooding hen, who 
would promptly hop up and give every morsel to the young. 

This little warbler would sometimes sit well down in the nest, and erect some of her 
feathers and apparently inflate the throat so that the bird’s head appeared as if swollen 
to twice its natural size. She made the most comical picture, however, when on a hot 
day she stood or sat over the young, with every feather erect, striving to keep them cool 
and to be comfortable herself meanwhile. 


The female Kingbird broods constantly when the heat is severe, and at the approach 
7 


98 Wild Birds. 


Fig. go. Female Redwing Blackbird placing food in the throat of 
a nestling. 


Fig.91. The same bird watching the foodin the throat. If not 
immediately swallowed, the insect is withdrawn and passed 
around until a bird with the proper reaction time is found. 


of the male will often assist in dis- 
patching unruly insects and in seeing 
them safely downa responsive throat. 
The persistence of the Redwing Star- 
ling in this line of conduct is admir- 
able. I have seen one of these birds 
stand with drooping wings, erect 
feathers, and mouth agape, in the 
strong heat of a July day for hours 
though not continuously, for she in- 
variably left at the approach of her 
mate for a few moments’ respite, and 
then usually returned with food. 

The Cedar-bird gapes persist- 
ently when uncomfortably warm, 
but only the crest feathers are ever 
erected, and then not to the extent 
usually shown in drawings of this 
species. Both Robins and Catbirds 
bristle up when their nests and well- 
fledged young are assailed, but I have 
never seen this habit in the brooding 
bird, although their emotion is often 
expressed by raising the feathers of 
the crown. 

The duty of brooding rests 
mainly with the female in our com- 
mon land birds, but the male in some 
species either regularly or intermit- 
tently takes his turn at the nest. 

Passerine birds feed their young 
at brief intervals from early morning 
until nightfall, but apparently sel- 
dom if ever after dark. The Night 
Hawk, as has been seen, broods by 
day, and feeds its young at dusk, or 
just after dark, and probably again 
at dawn. Both sexes usually share 
in bringing food to the nest, but 
this rule is by no means universal. 

The young require animal food 
during the early days of life, and 
in the interior of the country this 
consists mainly of insects in the 
larval or mature stages, spiders, 


Fig. 92. Female Kingbird astride nest with drooping half-spread wings, shielding her 
brood from the hot sun. Notice the characteristic attitude of the young. 


Yu S 
ie 


Fig. 93. Kingbirds bruising a too active grasshopper between their bills preparatory to 
serving it to the young: the female in front with tail full-spread, 


99 


Care of Young and Nest. 101 


earthworms (at least in the Robin) and possibly slugs. Aside from the habits of the adult 
the nature of the food brought depends much upon the character of the supply. When the 
Kingfisher finds crayfish abundant they are carried to the nest, and this species has also 
been known to go to the fields for insects. Along the coast various other invertebrates un- 
doubtedly contribute to the food supply of both young and adult birds of many species. 
Birds which never taste of fruit themselves naturally do not give it to their young, while 
Robins, Orioles, Vireos, and Waxwings, to mention but a few of the berry-pickers, vary 
the diet of their fledglings with a liberal supply of fruits of various kinds. 


| 


ke 


Fig. 94. Female Kingbird assisting a grampus down the Fig.95. Male grampus, Corydalus cornutus: full size, 
throat of anestling. The long gray wings of this insect are from life. 
still protruding from the mouth. 


The food is placed not simply in the mouth of the young but well down into the 
sensitive throat, and if the bird does not immediately respond, it is withdrawn and passed 
-to another, and often toa third, until a throat is found which has the proper reaction 
time. If the gullet is already full, the swallowing power is inhibited, and the bird must 
wait. If the experiment of feeding a young bird like a Robin at the nest is tried, it will 
be found that the food passes slowly down the cesophagus, and when this is filled no 
more can be taken until the channel is clear. The gullet thus acts as a brake to the 
tendency of the greedy young bird to gorge itself to suffocation. According to Audu- 


102 Wild Birds. 


Fig. 96. Female Chestnut-sided Warbler bristling to keep 
cool while brooaing ona hot June day. 


Fig. 97. The same bird in the more common brooding attitude. 


bon, Cedar-birds will sometimes gorge 
themselves to such excess with berries as 
to be unable to fly, and a number of 
wounded birds of this species which he 
kept inacage ate of apples until suffocated. 
When opened they were found to be filled 
to the mouth. 

The automatic response given by the 
young is the signal awaited by the old bird, 
though often with impatience. The insect 
is watched after being placed in a respon- 
sive throat, until it disappears. Should it 
stick at the gullet it is withdrawn and re- 
placed time and again, or given a gentle 
pull, until it is safely down. Sometimes 
the insect is bruised against a twig, beaten 
into a pulp or crushed and torn asunder 
between the bills of the parent birds before 
it can be safely delivered. As has already 
been seen, many birds uttera peculiar note 
as a special stimulus to the young. At 
such times even the silent Cedar-bird finds 
avoice and gives an impatient cheet/ If 
this call passes unheeded it often becomes 
extremely shrill, especially in Kingbirds, 
with whom failure on part of their young 
to quick response seems to be peculiarly 
exasperating. 

While watching a Kingbird’s nest 
from the tent, a moth miller was once 
brought in by the male. It was passed to 
each one of the young in turn, but even 
under the spur of his shrill chitter, they 
were unresponsive, and he devoured the 
prey himself. This sharp economy is often 
practiced at the nest, and I have even seen 
the leg of a grasshopper picked up and 
eaten by an old bird. Not a crumb is 
allowed to go to waste. If an insect gets 
away it is usually pursued and immedi- 
ately snapped up. Once, however, Isawa 
female Kingbird fooled by a fly who owed 
its life to its small size. As she opened 
her bill in her attempt to land it safely 
in an open throat, the fly darted off. The 


Care of Young and Nest. 103 


bird seemed dazed for a moment, and 
stood gazing at the departing fly as if in 
mute astonishment. 

Exciting scenes usually follow at the 
nest of the Kingbird when a large dragon- 
fly, cicada, or grampus is brought to the 
family circle. The insect often struggles 
hard, but escape is out of the question, 
especially with both birds at the nest, who 
at once begin to rend and crush it with 
their bills. 

The male grampus (Corydalus corn- 
utus) better known as the larval hellgamite 
of which black bass are sometimes ex- 
tremely fond, has long gray wings folded 
back over the body when at rest, and the 
head is armed with horns an inch long but 
formidable only in their appearance. I 
have seen these huge insects measuring four 
inches from tip of the jaws to the extrem- 
ities of the folded wings fed to a single 
bird, and they were swallowed—wings and 
all. The operation is shown at an incom- 
plete stage in one of the _ illustrations, 
where the wings of the grampus can be 
seen projecting an inch or more from the 
mouth of the struggling bird. 

The cicada is even tougher and harder 
to manage but is beaten into subjection, 
and served up ina limp condition. Last 
August, I witnessed a street combat be- 
tween one of these cicade and an House 
Sparrow. The insect was bounding up 
and down on the ground and sounding its 
crescendo at an alarming rate, but unable 
to avoid the blows which rained from the 
Sparrow's bill. As the music of the dying 
cicada finally ceased, the Sparrow picked 
up his victim and bore it off to his brood 


Il. 
CLEANING THE NEST. 


The sanitary condition of the young 
isa matter of great concern to most birds, 


Fig. 98. A common scene atthisnest. The male brings 
food, while his mate, who is brooding, receives it into her 
own bill and passes it on to the children. 


Fig 99. The same brooding bird, with feathers erect and 
throat inflated. 


104 Wild Birds. 


who as a class are extremely neat and clean. This is especially true of many 
species who breed in holes or cavities of any kind like the Woodpeckers and Chick- 
adees, the young of which are crowded in close quarters or even piled up in more than 
one layer. The Woodpecker’'s hole and the Bluebird’s nest are always sweet and clean, 
and the nestlings immaculate. 

The duty of inspection and, if necessary, nest-cleaning follows each feeding with 
clock-like regularity, and is one of 
the most characteristic and import- 
ant activities to be observed in the 
nesting habits of a large number of 
the smaller land birds, yet apparently 
it is not mentioned in the standard 
treatises of ornithology, and I have 
found but few references to it in 
works of any kind. Audubon, who 
has probably recorded more facts on 
the behavior of American birds than 
any other writer, does not, I believe, 
mention this important function. 
The reason is not far to seek, for 
without the possibility of close ap- 
proach to the nest, and the use of a 
convenient blind, such acts are 
difficult or impossible to observe. 

The instinct of inspecting and 
cleaning the nest is mainly confined 
to the great passerine and picarian 
orders represented in this country 
by hundreds of species. It is a 
well-marked trait in Thrushes, Wax- 
wings, Vireos, Warblers, Orioles, 
Blackbirds, and Woodpeckers, to 
mention those families in which it 


has been observed. 
Fig. 100. Brown Thrush feeding a nestling. ‘‘ The food is placed The excreta of the young leave 
not simply in the mouth of the young, but well down into the sensi- the cloaca in the form of white, 
opaque or transparent, mucous sacs. 
The sac is probably secreted at the lower end of the alimentary canal, and is sufficiently 
consistent to admit of being picked up without soiling bill or fingers. The parent birds 
often leave the nest hurriedly bearing one of these small white packages in bill, an action 
full of significance to every member of the family. I have seen the Oriole carry these 
packages a few rods from the nest and drop them before alighting. The Bluebird and 
Redwing Blackbird take them a long distance before letting them fall. 
Some Crow Blackbirds which I watched last spring had their young inthe top of a 
fir tree beside a small pond, which lay between me and their nest. In approaching with 


tive throat.” 


Care of Young and Nest. 105 


food they would stealthily enter the tree on the farther side and after a few moments fly 
over the pond and drop what looked like a small white marble in the water below. This 
effected, they would veer and fly off to the feeding ground. The same action was 
repeated by birds from other nests. 

Removing the excreta piecemeal and dropping it at a safe distance, is the common 
instinctive method not only of insuring the sanitary condition of the nest itself, but 
what is even more important, of 
keeping the grass and leaves below 
free from any sign which might be- 
tray them to an enemy. 

Many other birds, of which | 
can now certify the Robin, Catbird, 
Cedar Waxwing, Red-eyed Vireo, 
Kingbird, Redwing Blackbird, Brown 
Thrush, and Chestnut-sided Warbler, 
devour a part and often the major 
part of the excreta at the nest. 
This is a very common practice 
with the Warbler, Robin, Waxwing, 
and Vireo, but was only casually 
observed in Catbirds and Brown 
Thrushes. 

The Robin has undoubtedly 
been seen by many in the character. 
istic pose shown in a number of the 
photographs standing on the rim of 
the nest with the head erect, or in- 
clined as if doting on her young 
ones and thinking what fine children 
they were, whereas this attitude is 
really one of sanitary inspection. 
When an old bird of any of the 
species mentioned above has fed one 
of the brood, its duty is but half 
done; it pauses, bends over, and 


Fig tor. Brown Thrush cleaning the nest. 


keenly scrutinizes each young bird in turn and every part of the nest. Shortly after 
being fed, the nestling becomes very uneasy, and raises its body as if to drop the sac 
over the edge of the nest. The old bird follows every movement, snaps up the 
package as it leaves the body, and either swallows it immediately or carries it off. 
When seen flying from the nest with head depressed, the Robin is usually engaged 
in errands of this kind. The Robins and Cedar-birds have frequently been seen to take 
the sacs from two or three birds in rapid succession, in which case they are always de- 
voured on the spot. The Robin will often convey the package to any convenient 
perch, and after examining it, devour a part, or reject the whole. While watching Rob- 
ins from the tent I have seen them carry the excreta thirty rods away before letting it 


106 Wild Birds. 


fall or alighting to examine it, and have tried to find the sac but usually without success. 
One day I saw a male Robin drop the “ white marble” in the grass about fifty feet from 
the nest, and proceed to peck at it. Upon going to the spot a little later I found the sac 
covered with dirt but not opened. It had a tenacious opaque white wall, was perfectly 
odorless, and contained besides a few small pellets, a whole blueberry which had survived 
the digestive process. The actions of the old bird were thus explained. He was look- 
ing for food on his own account, but in this case missed it. 

On another occasion the mother Robin devoured all the excreta which soiled the 
nest, and a moment later took it directly from the young and carried it away. Againon 
a later day, the same bird after swal- 
lowing all the excreta available, 
dropped on the nest and brooded 
her young twenty minutes by the 
watch, without showing the least 
desire to reject anything which had 
been eaten. 

The female Cedar-bird in her 
usual round of domestic duties 
comes to her nest of half-fledged 
young, regurgitates cherries, and 
after distributing them in the usual 
fashion, inspects her household with 
the closest attention, picking up and 
swallowing every particle which it is 
necessary to remove. This accom- 
plished the mother bird has been 
seen to spread her wings over her 
brood, and shield them from a hot 
August sun foroveran hour. Mean- 
time her mate came repeatedly, and 
passed the cherries around. The fe- 
male who stood erect astride the 

Fig. 102. Cedar-bird cleaning the nest. Compare this common nest, would frequently inspect and 
attitude with that shown in Figs. 45 and 46. 

clean the household. She would 

also snap at every passing insect, and I saw her catch a large red ant, and quickly transfer 

it to the mouth of a young bird. She would erect and lower her crest and stand with 

mouth agape for long intervals, but there was never a sign of ejecting anything which 


had been eaten. 

At still another nest of the Waxwing I saw the female after feeding cherries, inspect, 
and walking around the rim of the nest, take the sacs from four young birds in succession, 
direct from the body, and after swallowing them all, look for more. She then flew to a 
neighboring tree and cleaned her bill. In performing the sanitary act this bird bends 
over, and reaching forward with head turned slightly to one side, takes the sac rather 
gingerly as it leaves the cloaca, and quickly disposes of it. In the course of forty-four 
visits to their young of which exact record was made, this nest was cleaned eighteen 


Care of Young and Nest. 107 


times ; once a part of the excreta was taken away and a part eaten; five times it was re- 
moved from the nest, and on eleven visits all was devoured. 

After watching such behavior, which I have seen repeated with slight variations hun- 
dreds of times, I am convinced that the excreta in such cases is actually eaten, and not 
merely taken into the gullet to be later regurgitated. It is true that the Cedar-bird uses 
its distensible gullet as a temporary receptacle for the food destined for the young, and 
it might seem probable that the cxcreta went no farther than the cesophagus, from which 
it was later ejected. The actions of the birds just described and in many similar cases 
observed do not support this idea. s 

Not only are the young care- 
fully tended in the way explained, 
but the old birds often put the head 
down in the nest and rummage 
about for any stray particle of food 
or fragments of any kind which it is 
desirable to remove. While stand- 
ing at the nest they will sometimes 
pick energetically their own legs and 
toes, and the heads and bodies of the 
young, a very important function 
where the nest is infested with those 
minute swarming particles known as 
lice and mites. Every straw and 
fiber in the Cedar-bird’s nest shown 
in one of the photographs(Fig. 38) was 
literally covered with parasites, in this 
case a species of mite which is a poor 
and degenerate relation of the spider. 
When the nest or anything in it was 
touched they would swarm up the 


hand by hundreds, but they are 


Fig. 103. Female Kingbird attending sanitation of nest. 


barely visible to the eye, and apart 
from a slight tickling sensation between the fingers are scarcely felt. They do not seem 
to trouble the old birds much, but must give discomfort to the young, especially if from 
any other cause they happen to be weakly. 

One would suppose that cleanliness must be an imperative instinct with such a bird 
as the Kingfisher, whose nest is underground, but the semi-fluid excreta is not re- 
moved from the tunnel, which according to some observers, becomes fouled in con- 
sequence. This was not true of the nest which I had under observation last summer. 
In the course of seventeen days the nesting chamber was moved forward more than a 
foot, so that it always presented a clean surface. 

The Barn Swallow, the House Sparrow, and the wild Passenger Pigeon represent a 
considerable number of birds which secure protection in their breeding haunts by other 
means than by concealing the nest. While their nests may be clean, this is not true of 
the ground beneath. It is plainly advantageous for the smaller birds which breed in 


108 Wild Birds. 


solitude on or near the ground to remove every particle of litter which would whiten 
the grass or foliage and thus advertise the nest to their enemies, even to those who 
prowl at night. 

When a Red-eyed Vireo whose actions I was watching at close range dropped one 
of the sacs by accident, she would dart after it and snap it up before it reached the 
ground not four feet from the nest. I have also witnessed the same performance in the 
Kingbirds. Not atrace of deflement 
is ever seen about the dwellings of 
any bird possessed of the cleaning 
instinct. 

On the other hand predaceous 
birds like Eagles and Hawks pay no 
attention to such matters. The ex- 
creta of the young as of the adult is 
voided in a semi-fluid state and in 
a peculiar manner. With tail up- 
turned over the edge of the nest it 
is shot to a distance of several feet, 
and may strike the ground two or 
more yards from the nesting tree. 
In this way the eyry at least is kept 
clean. These bold and persistent 
robbers have few enemies to reckon 
with, and their nests may be as open 
to view as a castle on a hill. 

Owls, which breed in holes in 
trees, are reported to have filthy 
nests, especially when the cavity has 
been occupied for several successive 
years, but this seems to be due mainly 
to the remains of their quarry or to 
the accumulation of the rejected 
food-pellets. The haunts of certain 
sea fowl are often reeking with filth 


Fig.104. Baltimore Oriole hurriedly feeding her young before : " : a ‘i 
uring the br ing season, and the 
all fear has been subdued and behavior is free. du © eed > is 


guano-beds of the South American 
coast mark the breeding grounds of myriads of sea fowl. However, the birds themselves 
both old and young seem to manage to keep clean, and any other condition would soon 
become intolerable. 

The Turkey Buzzard seems to have touched the lowest depths of squalor to which 
any bird can descend and live. In speaking of their abodes, Audubon says that before 
the final departure of the young, a person, if forced to remain in their vicinity for half an 
hour, would be in danger of suffocation.’ 

The cleaning of the young and nest is instinctive in a very large number of birds, 


1 Ornithological Biography, vol. ii., p. 43. 


Care of Young and Nest. 109 


and so is also the care with which they avoid any defilement of the nesting site. The use 
of the excreta as food, however, is to be regarded in a different light. If it should be 
proved that in the Robin, for instance, some individuals never eat the excreta, while others 
as we know do, we should regard the action as an acquired habit. When the pellicle 
breaks in the mouth, an accident which I have seen happen in the case of the Robin, the 
bird is obliged to swallow a part in order to get rid of it. 

Much light is thrown on this 
question by the behavior of the 
Chestnut-sided Warblers, whose 
habits will be referred to again in 
the concluding chapter. Both sexes 
in this case fed, brooded, and 
cleaned the young and nest. The 
male regularly removed the excreta 
but was never seen to eat it. The 
female on the contrary often ate of 
it, and brooded so constantly that 
she was obliged to leave the nest 
to satisfy her own hunger. She 
would often be back in half a minute, 
having taken only a bite as it were. 
When the female had_ received 
the food which her mate sup- 
plied and had seen it safely deliv- 
ered, she would inspect, devour 
everything which needed removal, 
and then continue to brood. Ifa 
sac should accidentally fall, she 
would snap it off the ground, return 
to the nest, and brood as before. 
At other times when the male ap- 
proached she would stand aside 
and allow him to deliver the food 
and make the inspection. Twice 


Fig. 105. Serving a black cricket to a fleagling who has climbed to 
I saw the male take a sac to Carry the rim of the nest and is struggling to maintain his position. 


it away, and the female snatch it 

from him, swallow it, and settle down on the nest. Again another sac was torn asunder, 
and each bird went off with a half in its bill. In a moment the female returned but 
without bringing food, showing that she had been satisfying her own hunger. This not 
only proves that the excreta is used as food but illustrates how the habit of eating it 
may be forced upon a hungry brooding bird. 

Since digestion in the young is an imperfect process at best, it is easy to understand 
how any kind of pre-digested or partly digested food might be acceptable in times of 
stress when the staple article was not easy to procure. The fact that a bird only casually 
devours a pellet or swallows one and removes another is easy to understand. It is a ques- 


110 Wild Birds. 


tion of the hunger of the moment, and another illustration of the economy which birds dis- 
play in all such matters. 

While the removal of the excreta is an instinctive act, the use of it as food is pro- 
bably an acquired habit, the strength of which depends on the force of circumstances, 
and may be limited in some cases to one sex alone. 


Cia TER 211 
THE FORCE OF HABIT. 


NDER some conditions habits are formed with surprising quickness. The habit 
may be of trifling significance and have only a brief reign, but no habits are 
absolutely rigid, and the genesis of all is probably the same,—pleasurable conse- 

quences following repeated actions which may be forced or accidental. The result is in 
all cases similar,—a mental association of certain things with certain actions. 

While watching hour by hour the Robins described in Chapter IV, and recording 
their visits to their young, | began to notice on the third day that the male usually 
approached on the right side of the nest, that is on the observer’s right as he stood facing 
it, while the female frequently came to the back or on the left. From that time I recorded 
the manner of each approach, and found that the male invariably came to the right side, 
and hopped down the limb to his nest. 

In the table given below in which the visits of both birds are recorded for two con- 
secutive days, R is for the right, L for the left, and B for the far side of the nest with 
reference to the tent, while the dashes represent visits the character of which was unde- 


termined. Each sign represents a visit to the nest, at which food was usually served. 


(FemaleRR—RRLBRRRRRBREL SS ihe = 
July 27th. Third day of Ob- } ———L——R 
servation—64 hours. | Male RBRRRRRRRR—RRB—RRR——R 
7 kR— 
July 28th. Fourth day of Ob- ) “emale L BB L B— B———BB— — 
servation—4 hours. Male RRRRRRRRRRRRR—RRR 


For the fourth day I have no record of the female approaching by the right side, and 
no record of the male coming in any other way. On the two following days the female 
did not appear, and as I had reason to believe, was engaged in building a new nest. 
The male at this period always approached his nest in the habitual manner. Now 
whether the male bird had formed this habit shortly after the nest was built or shortly 
after the nesting bough was removed is of little consequence. At all events a definite 
mode of behavior had developed in a short space of time, in one case in two weeks or in 
the other in two days. On the fourth day the young had to be brooded often, owing to 
the heat, which accounts for the apparent inactivity of the female in providing food. 

Probably most birds form definite habits in the manner of approach to the nest, 
entering on a certain side, or flying to a certain twig, following the path suggested in the 
first instance by convenience or dictated by caution. A pair of Red-eyed Vireos with 
whom I spent parts of three days followed a definite course in approach with surprising 


III 


112 Wild Birds. 


regularity. They would fly to the 
main branch, hop along toward the 
fork in which the nest was suspended, 
and finally perch on a small con- 
venient twig just over their young. 
Out of sixty recorded visits they de- 
viated from this habitual method but 
three times, and then only before 
they had recovered from their first 
feelings of fear. In this case the nest- 
ing branch had been drawn down 
about a foot by means of a cord, but 
was not otherwise disturbed. 

In cleaning the nest the attitude 
is frequently the same in successive 
visits, the birds often clasping the 
same twigs, so that a number of pho- 
tographs of the act taken without 
moving the camera may be so nearly 
identical that only the most careful 
inspection will reveal the least differ- 
ence in pose or position. 

While engaged in studying some 
Redwing Blackbirds last July the 
weather was hot, and the young had 
to be brooded almost constantly. 
The female would sit on the nest, 
often with back to the tent, with 
feathers erect and mouth open in her 
efforts to keep cool. Suddenly the 
shriek of asteam whistle sounded the 
hour of noon at a mill scarcely three 
rods away. It startled me, but the 
bird did not budge a feather. It is 
not difficult to imagine that her first 
experience with this instrument of 
torture was quite different in its re- 
sult, but the case illustrates the ease 
with which birds become quickly ac- 
customed to strange and uncouth 
sounds, when, as sometimes happens, 
they place their nests in a saw-mill 


afew feet from the buzzing saw or 


Figs. 106, 107, Male Redwing Blackbird cleaning nest on two above the erinding trolley cars of a 
distinct visits; photographed under similar conditions, and illus- . = > - 
trating the formation of habit in the daily routine. city street. 


The Force of Habit. 


Every animal must adapt itself 
in some measure to changes in its 
surroundings, and with birds this 
power is well expressed in the nest, 
the position, materials, and construc- 
tion of which are subject to incessant 
change. The change may be slight 
or of a very marked character, as 
when the common type of archi- 
tecture is abandoned, or a distinct 
nest-structure wanting. Only a few 
examples of change in nesting habits 
need be considered since the facts 
are matters of common observation. 

The Swift of this country is often 
quoted as one of the most remarkable 
examples of birds whose nesting 
habits have changed in recent times. 
Formerly breeding in hollow trees 
and still doing so in places remote 
from mankind, it now attaches its 
little wicker crates to the inside of 
From the standpoint of 
the Swift the change has really been 


chimneys. 


very slight, and had it not become so 
widespread it would have attracted 
little attention. This bird was proba- 
bly drawn to the town and open coun- 
try by the greater abundance of its 
insect prey, and to the mind of the 
Swift a chimney cannot be very differ- 
ent from a hollow tree. Its instinct 
probably does not lead it to select a 
dead tree for its roosts or nests be- 
cause it is a tree, any more than it 
leads it to prefer a sycamore to an 
oak. What is probably inherited is 
the tendency to seek a dark or cav- 
ernous place with easy entrance and 
exit. The chimney which emits no 
smoke in summer and usually stands 
in the open, fulfils every requirement 
in places where hollow trees are 
scarce. 

The Swift is yet capable of 

8 


Fig. 108. Cock Robin with large grasshopper ready, 
Fig. 109. The same bird taking aim. 


114 


Fig. 110. 


Wild Birds. 


Ready to inspect. Notice that he invariably comes to 
the right side by force of habit. 


Fig. 111. 
the left. 


Inspecting the household. 


The female approaches on 


adapting its needs to conditions far 
more unlike those of the ancestral 
tree, and has been known to enter 
a barn and nest with the Barn 
Swallows. This happened in Dorset, 
Ohio,’ where some Swifts fastened 
their nest to the vertical boards 
near a hole made for the conven- 
ience of the Swallows, and just below 
the peak of the roof. Five young 
were hatched and were seen clinging 
to the boards just beneath the nest. 
The old birds would sometimes enter 
by the open door, fly straight to the 
nest and cling to the wall beside it. 
The quavering voices of the little 
Swifts would then drown every other 
sound about the place. 

In still another case,’ a pair of 
Swifts nested in the dim interior ofa 
shed beside an old saw-mill at Dor- 
chester, New Hampshire, in June, 
1899. This nest was fastened to the 
boards, well up towards the roof, and 
an open door formed easy entrance 
and egress. 

In at least one respect birds 
resemble men in their ordinary build- 
ing operations. They make use of 
the materials at hand, but in the se- 
lection of the site for the nest many 
seem to obey no rule, being ever on 
the alert to adapt themselves to their 
lot, and a habit once formed often 
leads to a steady line of conduct. 

The English Sparrow has even 
found a convenient shelf in the 
hood of the electric arc lamps, and 
although these are lowered daily to 
the street, it sticks to its nest over 
the light. I have seen this impudent 
little wretch dispossess the Eaves 


'This account was given to me by Mr. 
Robert J. Sim. 
2 Observed by Professor William Patten. 


The Force of Habit. 115 


Swallow and convert its mud retort into a grass-lined nest of its own. This occurred at 
Basin Harbor, Vermont, in 1883, before the Sparrows were so generally condemned. The 
nests were in line under the eaves of a farmer's barn, and the Swallows were still fighting 
for possession. About every other nest was then occupied by the Sparrows. 

The Osprey is not only one of the most remarkable nest-builders in the world, but a 
wonderful adept in making the most of its opportunities. In selecting a site for its 
mountain of a nest, it seems at times to exercise little choice, taking whatever offers. 
Apparently its controlling ambition is to raise a huge edifice in the construction of which 
nothing comes amiss which can be seized and carried in its powerful talons. In its build- 
ing operations this bird seems to have an eye for the centuries rather than the years, and 
some of its eyries formed on 
rocky crags have possibly ex- 
isted for more than a hundred 
years, or might last so long if 
undisturbed by man. This 
Hawk will nest on the ground, 
on rocks, in low or high trees, 
in woods or in the open, ona 
chimney, a pile of rails, a rock- 
ing buoy, or a dilapidated 
windmill. It will even suffer 
its nest to be displaced, and at 
Bristol, Rhode Island, it eager- 
ly appropriates the cart wheel 
which the hospitable farmers 
raise aloft on the tops of poles 
for the benefit of these birds. 

At Plum Island, New 
York, which was formerly col- 
onized by hundreds of Os- 
preys, Mr. Allen found their 
nests in almost every conceivable situation, about thirty or forty per cent. of them being 
on the ground. ‘High rocks on the shore, and low rocks far out in the water, scarcely 


Fig. 112. Female Kingbird inserting an insect into the throat of a fledgling. 


above high tide and swept by the autumn storms, were chosen as situations for the nests. 
A large buoy, with a lattice work top, near the west end of Fisher’s Island, was also 
‘occupied for many years by a nest of these birds, greatly to the advantage of sailors and 
fishermen, who were warned in thick weather of the position of the buoy by the screaming 
of the Fish Hawks.” ’ 

An observer who described a nest on an old windmill said that while the fan of the 
mill was gone “the rudder remained, and the wind catching this would swing the nest 
part way round, and then the wind changing slightly would swing it back again ; the sit- 
ting female not seeming to mind the movement in the least.” ” 

On the shore of Narragansett Bay, Rhode Island, which has been colonized by Fish 


1“ Breeding Habits of the Fish Hawk on Plum Island. New York.” C. 5S. Allen, 7he Awh, vol. ix., p. 315. 1892. 
2? The Osprey, vol. ii., p. 55. Many interesting pictures of nests of the Osprey have been published in this 
amagazine. 


116 Wild Birds. 


Hawks from an early period, the birds are not only protected by law, but are offered 
every inducement to make them feel at home.’ When a dead tree containing a nest is 
blown down the owner of the land will sometimes erect a tall pole, with a carriage wheel 
laid flat on top. The birds readily accept the new wheel of fortune, which becomes 
their home. 

In selecting a bare tree or a wheel on top of a pole the hawk makes a nice choice, for 
owing to its great extent of wing, as with the eagle, it is convenient to have the path to 
the nest free from obstructions. 

When an Osprey loses its mate its actions seem to depend onits character. A case is 
reported where two birds were seen to pair on the second day after each had lost a mate, 
while another who was bereft by a stroke of lightning, which destroyed both the nest 
and the sitting bird, is said to have lingered about the spot for the remainder of the 
summer, and to have even returned the next year still unmated to his solitary vigil. 

The diet of an insectivorous bird is extremely varied at all times, depending much 
upon the locality and the season of the year. While a fewkinds of insects may be avoided 
because of a repugnant odor or taste, they capture as a rule whatever comes in their 
way. The Robin commonly brings to its nest grasshoppers, crickets, katydids, and 
angleworms, because in its customary manner of search it finds and is able to secure these 
formsin abundance. The Kingbird, which takes most of its prey on the wing, discovers a 
far greater variety. When certain species of insects are abundant they are often eaten 
by many birds who under ordinary conditions would never touch them. Thus during a 
plague of Rocky Mountain locusts which visited the Western States, these insects are 
reported to have been eaten by nearly every bird in the region, and to have served 
as a staple for most of the species. Birds of prey such as the smaller hawks and 
owls devoured them eagerly. The food habits of most birds are exceedingly plastic 
and liable to sudden change under the spur of necessity. 

A good illustration of a change in feeding habits has been recently given.” It appears. 
that the Rhinoceros-bird (Buphaga erythrepyncha) was until lately regarded as so valuable a 
scavenger that it was accorded special protection by law in British East Africa. Its habit 
was to feed on the ticks and other parasites which infest wild and domesticanimals. “ Since 
the cattle plague,” says Captain Hinde, ‘“ destroyed the immense herds in Ukambani, and 
nearly all the sheep and goats were eaten during the late famine, the birds, deprived of 
their food, have become carnivorous, and now any domestic animal not constantly watched 
is killed by them. Perfectly healthy animals have their ears eaten down to the bone, 
holes torn in their backs and in the femoral regions.” The new conditions introduced by 
man have thus converted a useful animal into a dangerous pest. 

1 For an interesting -.ccount of the nesting habits of the Osprey see Forest and Stream, July 7, 1900. 
’ The Osprey, vol. ii,, p. 59. 
3 Nature, vol. \xii., p. 366, 1900. 


CHAPTER. AITI 
FEAR IN BIRDS. 


IRDS as arule are possessed of fear which is primarily an instinct, but as we shall 
see later on many species in their natural adult state are entirely devoid of this 
sense. With others it may wax or wane according to their environment or indi- 

vidual experiences. Again the nature of the fear manifested varies with age or the 
period of life. It is a generalized sense of fear, or fear of the strange and unusual, which 
comes over the young bird, while later it learns to dread particular objects or sounds with 
which some bitter experience is associated. Furthermore, the time of the appearance of 
the instinct varies in different species, coming late in some and early in others. Generally 
speaking the manifestation of fear is well timed, and is an adaptation for the good of its 
possessor. 

Let us first see how fear enters into the life of the young. Birds are sometimes 
roughly classified into altricial species, which feed their young for days or weeks at the 
nest, and przecocial birds, whose young are born clad in soft down, and are able to walk, 
run, or swim at once or very soon after hatching. The Altrices like the Robin, Wood- 
pecker, and Humming-bird are hatched from eggs which are small in relation to the size 
of the parent, and the young are at first blind, helpless, and more or less completely 
naked. In ail such the nest is only a temporary home, but is often very elaborate, while 
the instinct of fear is delayed or deferred until the time of flight, a period varying from a 
few days to three weeks or more. The Pracoces lay eggs with big yolks, upon the 
stored energy of which the unhatched young subsist until they step forth into the world, 
seeing, able to walk or swim, and in some degree their own masters. The common do- 
mestic fowls, Partridges, Ostriches, Geese, Loons, Plover, and Snipe, are some of the better 
known representatives of this group, but the dividing line is never sharply drawn, and 
there are innumerable gradations between the extremes in either class. In the praecocial 
birds the feeling of fear is either present at birth, or appears in a very few hours or days, 

As an illustration of the development of fear in the altricial kinds one might select 
any of the common passerine birds, Thrushes, Warblers, Finches, or Flycatchers, but we 
should bear in mind that the development of this instinct is not always uniformly timed, 
even in the same species. We will choose the Catbird, the Chestnut-sided Warbler, and 
the Kingfisher. 

When I first camped beside a Catbird’s nest (No. 6 of table, Chap. I.) last summer, 
the young, who were then about a week old, were incapable of fear. They would shift 
about the nest to get into the shade, pant, and erect their growing head-feathers. When 
a breeze rocked the cradle, or a Redwing Blackbird sang his conguer-ce, or the parent 

I17 


118 Wild Birds. 


came with meat or fruit they 
stretched necks, opened mouths, 
each struggling to get some advant- 
age over the other, and uttered their 
sharp ¢s7¢/ ¢si¢t/ notes. You could 
handle them at will; they were ab- 
solutely fearless. If such a nest is 
overturned they will cling to it but 
will never cower or crouch. 

As we have seen, the clipping of 
a leaf at this nest two days later sent 
them off in a panic, and all hurried 
to the nearest cover. Should you 
succeed in catching them under such 
circumstances, which is doubtful, and 
try to replace them in the nest, they 
will pop out repeatedly as if mounted 
on springs, and if you try to hold 
them in the hand they will struggle, 
squeal and fairly shriek in their en- 
deavors to escape. They are now 
covered with a coat of slate-colored 
feathers, but fly with difficulty. 
When placed on open ground they 
hop off at once toward the nearest 
bush. No greater change in the 
behavior of a wild bird is ever wit- 


Fig.114. Bird on left shows instinctive response to sound or nessed than that which the sense of 
vibratory movement, and the use of wings for support. fear brings to pass. 


Fig. 113. Eggs and young of altricial Cedar-bird. Young about 
thirty-six hours old—blind and naked. 


I have seen a young Chestnut- 
sided Warbler jump out of its nest, 
when unable to stand erect and 
much less to use its wings. In this 
case the pin-feathers of the wings 
had barely burst, and the body was 
nearly naked. When the bird was 
returned to its nest, it refused to re- 
main until the operation was many 
times repeated and it was finally 
overcome by fatigue. I have known 
the young of the Redstart to leave 
the nest remarkably early, but the 


case just recorded appears to be 


Fig.115. Bird to the left in reptilian crouching attitude; at the somewhat exceptional. 
right, as in Fig, 113, the characteristic response of the new-born . ze a 
bird is seen, and the use of the pot-belly as afoot. About 3 life-size. The instinct of fear comes with 


Fear in Birds. 119 


a certain maturity of the nervous system, with comparative suddenness, as we have just 
seen, but is usually timed to correspond with the development of the wing-quills and 
the power of flight. 

At the age of twenty-four days the Kingfisher is in full feather, but shows no fear. 
He will perch comfortably on your hand or shoulder, and pose in any desired position, 
as the photographs made at this period will show, but the instinct soon appears after this 
stage is passed. In from twenty-four to forty-eight hours later when these birds not only 
possess the power of flight, but use it at the first intimation of danger, their docile nature 
has completely changed. With them the late development of this instinct is most oppor- 


Fig. 116. Young Kingfishers twenty-four days old, They are capable of flight, but show 
no fear, 


tune, since they are not tempted to leave the security of their tunnel in the ground until 
they can make long excursions and follow their parents to the favorite fishing grounds. 

Turning now to the precocial birds, according to the best testimony, fear in the 
domestic chick hatched in an incubator is at first very slight and is soon checked by con- 
trary impulses such as to nestle in a warm place, unless the instinct be brought into 
immediate exercise. 

Mr. Charles A. Allen says that the newly hatched young of the Black Duck (Azas 
obscura) show no fear, but will ‘cuddle under one’s hand very'confidingly.” 1 once saw 
a nest of this species on the shore of Lake Champlain, near Burlington, Vermont, on the 
very verge of a high, overhanging cliff. It was set against the stems of a slender shrub, 
the pulling of which would doubtless have precipitated the entire clutch fifty feet into 


120 Wild Birds. 


the water below. A little delay in the instinctive reaction of fear could hardly come 
amiss to young in such anest. On the other hand when the ducklings have been led to 
the water no birds show a keener sense of fear than they or respond more promptly to 
the alarm signals of their parents. I was greatly impressed when a boy at the sight of a 
Black Duck leading her trim little fleet of yellow sail up the mouth of a small sedge- 
bordered stream. The old 
bird quickly gave the alarm, 
rose, veered, and flew to- 
wards the river, while the 
young scrambled to the bank 
and hid in the rushes. I 
hunted long but succeeded 
in finding only one who lay 
flat in the marsh and kept 
perfectly still, true to its in- 
herited instinct. These 
ducklings had not been 
afloat many hours, and had 
this action been repeated be- 
fore, the lesson could not 
have been taught, since, as 
we have seen, the young 
under such circumstances 
are left to their own devices. 
I have seen a young 
‘chick while feeding quietly 
close to the house suddenly 
turn its head, look straight 
at the zenith, and then run 
off in a panic of fear. Look- 
ing up also I saw a Hen 
Hawk sailing aloft like a toy 


kite, a mere speck against 
the blue heavens. I think 


Fig.117.. Young Cedar-birds in displaced nest standing in characteristic at- 
titude with upturned heads. Photographed on day of flight, July 17, 1899, 
when possessed of fear. For account, see page 60. it probable that the bird got 


an alarm signal from some 
other fowls in the yard; at all events it knew where to look, and its response was not 
slow. This chicken may have been three weeks old, and so had ample time to learn 
its lesson, if such it was. Had the dark object been a paper kite it is not likely that 
the fear evoked would have been appreciably less. 

In altricial birds the sense of fear usually comes, as we have seen, with the de- 
velopment of the flight feathers, but it is often premature, thus indirectly causing the 
death of thousands of birds every year. In July and August how many helpless spar- 
rows and thrushes are found on the ground, having left their nests too early! Some- 
times they tumble out by accident, are drawn off by hunger, or are blown out in a gale, 


Fear in birds. 121 


but I believe that by far the greater number of such strays are driven forth by fright, and 
when this perilous step has once been taken it can seldom be retraced. The young of 
such birds as the Wilson thrushes, whose nests are on or near the ground out of the 
reach of storms, are often found in this predicament.’ 

Many immature birds which I have watched at the nest show no precise powers of 
discrimination in any direc- 
tion. You will see them 
respond as promptly to the 
flutter of a leaf or the call- 
note of any passing bird as 
to their own mother’s voice 
but a more curious specta- 
cle may be witnessed when 
a fledgling of one of our 
common species like, the 
Baltimore Oriole climbs to 
the top of its nest. All the 
others immediately salute it 
as if it were an old bird, 
and with open mouths beg 
vainly to be fed. Ifa young 
bird within a day of taking 
flight cannot distinguish 
one of its brothers from its 
mother, it can hardly be 


“a 


expected to ‘‘know a hawk 
from a handsaw,” or an 
enemy from a friend. 

After taking flight the 
young of altricial birds are 
fed by one or both parents 
for a period of days or 


weeks, and much is quickly 
learned by imitation and in- 
dividual experience. Their 
ingrained sense of fear becomes in the course of time gradually specialized in certain 


Fig. 118. Brown Thrush startled while at nest: attitude of keen attention. 


directions. Fear of man, guns, hawks, snakes, cats and the various agents of destruction 
with which each species must contend in the course of its life, seems in every case to 
be acquired or learned rather than inherited. 

On the Jast day of June I found a Cowbird nearly full-fledged but either unable or 
disinclined to fly. He occupied the nest of a warbler, apparently the species known as 


' The huge pot-belly of the young altricial bird has a use quite apart from the function of digestion. It anchors 
it to the nest, and as in the modern ‘‘ Brownie” keeps it right side up. The pliant viscera conform to every move- 
ment, and form a central supporting pillar long before the legs can sustain the weight of the body. (See young 
Cedar-birds in Figs. 113-115.) 


122 Wild Birds. 


the Black and Yellow or Magnolia Warbler, and as his photograph shows, filled it com- 
pletely. He would stand on the rim of the nest and, with raised feathers, squeak and call 
vehemently for his foster parents. I took from beneath him the dried mummy of alittle 
warbler and one addled egg, which illustrates the advantage nature gives this bird over 
his competitors in early life. He showed no fear, but clung like a monkey to the nest, 
while I carried the branch several hundred feet to find a quiet place out of the wind. I 
regret that I cannot show the nurse feeding this monster, but unfortunately the day was 
stormy and the bird was soon gone. 

Many birds have alarm calls or signals of distress, which attract or arouse other 
species, as every one knows who has studied birds in the country. I remember seeing an 
unusually striking exhibi- 
tion of this fact while 
watching unobserved 
some Red Crossbills en- 
gaged in picking the seeds 
out of pine cones. They 
were on the ground in a 
run where it was impossi- 
ble for the birds to see 
out on either side. A 
Crow espied me at a dis- 
tance, gave his short quick 
alarm car / car / when the 
Crossbills went off as if 
carried in a whirlwind. 
They had apparently seen 
nothing to awaken suspic- 
ion, and the crow is not 
their enemy so far as I am 
aware. 

When a robin hears 
the alarm call of his mate, 


Fig.119. Cock Robin startled while at nest by a quick, decisive alarm call . , 
from his mate, His head shot up like a flash, and ina moment he was off. his head goes up instantly, 


and he stands for a mo- 


ment with outstretched neck, listening intently to see if he is needed. I was fortunate 
in catching the male bird at the nest in just this attitude, expressive of attention and 
wariness, bordering on fear. 

A hawk, owl, crow, cat, snake, or any well-known or dreaded enemy of birds will set the 
community ina hubbubinavery short time. Birds of other species hurry to the scene out 
of sympathy or curiosity, as some would say, but probably more from instinct of a different 
character. The smallest spark often kindles the largest blaze. Thus while passing through 
a pasture last June I happened to encounter a Robin with mouth stuffed with food, as if on 
the way to her nest. She at once set up aloud cry, and mounting the bare branch of a dead 
apple tree, in five minutes drummed up eleven different birds, among which I recognized 
the Baltimore Oriole, Brown Thrush, two Catbirds, Chestnut-sided Warbler, Red-eyed 


Fear in Birds. 123 


Vireo, Maryland Yellow Throat, Song Sparrow, Chickadee, the Redstart and a Goldfinch, 
many of which became excited and joined in the general outcry. 

On a warm July day while crossing a barren strip of land, which bore a crop of 
golden-rod and sweet-fern, my attention was called to a small brown bird with a large 
grasshopper in its beak. It was the Bay-winged Bunting or Grass Finch, and the prey 
was clearly intended for her young, but instead of delivering it she hopped nervously 
about, uttering her sharpest monosyllables, in the course of which she finally dropped the 
prey. Thinking that her young were at hand, I sat down to await developments. Pres- 
ently several Buntings dashed up to the spot a few yards away. They glanced down at 
the ground, and then at me, 
emitting such a flow of incisive 
protests as to suggest the at- 
tempt to draw attention from 
their nest. This was plainly not 
the case when some Kingbirds 
left their young in a neighboring 
tree, and raising their war-cry, 
hovered over the spot and darted 
at some object on the ground. 
Thereupon going to the place, I 
almost stepped on what looked 
at first like a coil of rubber hose 
in the grass. It proved to bea 
large black snake, whose head 
was distorted in the act of swal- 
lowing a young bird. The crisis 
for the unfortunate bird being 
past, I stood by and watched the 
proceedings. The snake had 
taken his victim head first, and 
its body was slowly disappearing 
between his distended jaws. As 
I disturbed his meal, he folded Fig. 120. Red-tailed Hawk, four months old, in attitude expressive of 
Bia dill) subbeclibe body tno, etal eed etal sleihect mite ihe tho seine 
coil and his gleaming eyes be- _ frill, and hiss at intruders. 
trayed an unpleasant frame of 
mind. When I approached nearer, he lifted his swollen head high in the air, and slowly 
glided off to enjoy his spoils in peace; but his enemy followed. On this occasion we had the 
serpent at a disadvantage, but he did not remain muzzled long. Having proceeded thus far, 
that bird had to go down, notwithstanding the throes of deglutition. It was a tax upon 
the salivary glands, but they were equal to the task, and the pliant jaws soon closed over 
their victim. What a picture of stealth this animal made as with head erect, and eyes 
darting angry glances, he stole through the grass! The first act of the tragedy being closed, 
it was time to add the final touches of the second. As I struck at him with my cane how 
he shot through the grass, and it required no little speed to reach him for the fatal blow! 


124 Wild Birds. 


We have seen that the instinct of fear is inherited, and often delayed, where it is a 
special adaptation, not only leading the young, as Lloyd Morgan remarks, to accept a 
foster parent and not to shrink from her, but what is more important, keeping the young 
in the nest, barring accidents, until they can in some degree help themselves. Fear of 
particular objects is learned, or becomes grafted on to the original stock. The instinct 
may gather force or disappear, at least in adult life, according to the nature of the 
environment and the new habits formed in consequence. The instinctive basis of fear is 


apparently handed down from generation to generation, but in the life of the full-grown 
bird, it is probably largely replaced by habit, or the formation of associations. The 
innate or latent capacity remains, but the definite association of certain actions with 
particular objects or experiences is probably handed down by tradition rather than by 
heredity. 


Fig.121. Young Cowbird and nest of Magnolia Warbler Fig. 122. Young Cowbird comfortably filling the nest 
in which it was reared. ofits foster parent, whose children it smothered: fearless, 
though nearly ready to fly. 


Chie Pe ALY, 


TAMING WILD BIRDS WITHOUT A CAGE. 


wild or shy while in others they are very tame, and the same principle underlies 

them all. Wildness is due to fear which is partly inherited and partly learned 
by experience with this wicked world. Tameness, on the other hand, comes with the 
casting out of fear, and may be brought about by the formation of new habits which are 
either spontaneous or forced. 

The House Sparrows of the Tuileries, and the pious Stork of Holland, Germany 
and France, are familiar examples of birds whose near or remote ancestors are shy and 
wary. The Stork is said to be excessively wild in the woods and marshes, yet it comes 
with confidence to the village and town, builds its nests upon house tops and steeples, 
and struts about the streets and door-yards in search of food. 

It would be interesting to know how long the Doves of Venice have enjoyed the 
freedom of the Piazza del Marco. They are probably the best fed pigeons in the world, 
and few hours pass in the course of the day when their guardian, the vendor of sacks of 
corn, is not surrounded by his flock. They will alight all over you, and take the grain 
from hand or mouth. The Pigeon, it is true, has been long domesticated and responds 
more readily to friendly influences than the wild stock from which it has sprung. 

Strange and possibly true stories are told of persons who have won the confidence 
of beast or bird. The wild bird responds to their call and the quadruped comes forth 
from his den and takes food from their hand. Such persons are popularly supposed to 
possess a mysterious power of fascination or a superior knowledge of woodcraft, but all 


Mo illustrations could be given of birds which in most parts of their range are 


this belongs in the catalogue of vulgar errors. It depends less upon the individuality of 
the person than that of the animal. Individual variation knows hardly a limit, whether 
in man or beast. Some birds are naturally tame and confiding, while their next door 
neighbors of the same kin and living in the same field may possess a temperament of 
such an opposite character as to baffle every attempt to dispel their fears. 

The power of remaining motionless like a stone or stump in the woods is often 
enough to win the temporary confidence of both mammal and bird, and many will doubt- 
less recall illustrations of this fact from their own experience. This suggests an early 
episode which impressed itself rather strongly at the time. With raised fishing-pole in 
hand I was sitting quietly by the river, possibly watching the common sunfish or bream 
standing guard over their nests, which they defend with such fiery pugnacity, when I 
suddenly had a “ bite.”” Looking up, I saw a Kingbird comfortably perched on the end 
of my rod. He doubtless had a nest in the alders close by. 


125 


126 Wild Birds. 


It is easy to conceive a state in which all animals would be tame, but it would not 
be the state of nature known to us which has developed under the laws of battle, the 
survival of the strongest, the wariest, the best protected or concealed, or the most intelli- 
gent. The higher animals either prey on one another or on the helpless invertebrates, or 
are preyed upon, and with most, tameness would soon lead to extinction. Wildness or 
wariness is not only the law of their nature, but the very condition of their existence. 
The animal which fails to 
profit by experience, or 
at least to the extent of 
learning caution, and thus 
displaying the rudiments 
of intelligence, must go 
to the wall, unless the 
conditions of its life are 
exceptional or nature 
grants it some extraordi- 
nary favor suchas protec- 
tive or deceptivecoloring. 

While most animals 
are wild in the state of 
nature and many are al- 
most untamable, a com- 
paratively large number 
submit to the taming pro- 
cess, and a few become 
tame inthe natural state. 
The principle of the sur- 
vival of the strongest or 
the fittest as a result of 
the struggle for existence 
is so general and so primi- 
tive that when we find 
animals already tame in 
nature, we must regard 
them as the descendants 
of wild ancestors. 

As a rule no wild 
beast or bird approaches man without some inducement. Unless some other instinct be 
aroused, it comes, if at all, to defend or feed its offspring, to appease its hunger, or 
in very rare cases to find protection from danger. The taming process depends, as 
we have just seen, upon the ability to form new associations and may be brought 
about artificially by restraint as when a wild animal is caged and new habits are, as 
it were, forced upon it, or by means of strong lures. Of the latter, one of the best 
is food in the presence of hunger, but the strongest of all are the young at a cer- 
tain stage of growth. In order to tame a wild animal without recourse to restraint 


Fig.123. Male standing at nest after having fed his young. Notice the character- 
istic instinctive pose of one of the fledglings. 


Taming Wild Birds without a Cage. 127 


there must be some means of breaking the ice, or beginning a course of instruction, 
by chaining it to a fixed point. In case of birds with young the invisible chain is 
parental instinct, which inhibits fear and holds the animal to a given spot. We will at- 
tempt to analyze the taming process by the use of food and young birds as lures, and 
finally consider the similar experiments which nature occasionally conducts independ- 
ently and ona larger scale. 

I throw some cracked corn out of my window, and it is soon discovered by the 
ubiquitous Sparrows. When 


= 


they see me standing behind 
the pane they are afraid to 
approach, but they are also 
hungry. At last the impulse 
to get the food overcomes 
their fears, and they are re- 
warded by the feeling of pleas- 
ure and _ satisfaction. When 
they come repeatedly, each 
time reaping a reward without 
evil consequences, a new habit 
is gradually formed by the 
repetition of the act. The 
pleasure of getting food is 
gradually associated with fly- 
ing to a certain spot in the 
presence of objects which in 
the course of time become 
familiar. If the contrary im- 
pulse, due in this case to hun- 
ger, is sufficiently strong, the 
process may be carried forward 
step by step until the birds 
come to the hand for food. 
With the gregarious Sparrow, 


however, life ina populous Fig.124. Female Robin in act of nest-cleaning. She approaches at the 
town is usually too compli- back. See Chapter XII. 

cated to admit of carrying out 

the experiment with success in any reasonable time. 

There are many species which respond more rapidly than the wily Sparrow. Of 
these, I will mention the Chickadee, Nuthatch, Canada Jay, and Goose. The Chickadee 
has to work harder for a living in winter than the Sparrow, is far less gregarious and 
wary by nature, and is seemingly endowed with a keen sense of curiosity. Mr. Chapman 
thus speaks of the behavior of some of these birds in Central Park, New York City, 
in February: “they would often flutter before one’s face and plainly give expression to 
their desire for food, which they took from one’s hand without the slightest evidence 
of fear. Sometimes they even remained to pick the nut froma shell while perched on 


128 Wild Birds. 


one’s finger.” * They become equally tame when hard pressed by hunger in the remote 
woods, and I have no doubt that the following account which was given to me by a 
man who worked at a woodchopper’s camp in New Hampshire during the winter 
is strictly true. He said that at meal times the Chickadees would come about and pick 
up any crumbs that were left over or were thrown to them, and that they soon became 
so bold as to alight on the hand, or hat, and even to take pieces of bread from the 
mouth; that he would often amuse himself by trying to “close over them” with his 
hand, and that while they were usually too quick for him, he had caught them in this 
manner. 

Early in the winter of 1899, a Red-breasted Nuthatch formed the habit of going to 
a certain yard in Jefferson, Ohio, for food.’ At first it stayed among the trees like the 
Brown Creepers, but at length came to the window-sill for scraps of suet which were 
placed there. This window 
happened to be opposite a 
pump and sink, but the Nut- 
hatch soon showed no fear 
even when one stood close by 
and worked the pump. Blue 
Jays, Downyand Hairy Wood- 
peckers, Chickadees, and Eng- 
lish Sparrows also came to the 
garden for food. After several 
weeks of this kind of treat- 
ment Mr. Sim went outside, 
placed some suet on his palm 
and rested his hand on the 
window-sill. The Nuthatch 
came to the lure, picked up a 
piece of the food, and appar- 


Fig. 125. Female Red-eyed Vireo feeding young. In these birds the be- ently tried to hide it between 
havior was perfectly free after the first day of study. his thumb and finger. After 
the Red-breasted Nuthatch de- 
parted a Whitebreast came down, helped himself to the suet and was off. After this the 
Nuthatches often came and alighted on somebody’s hand, head, or shoulder, but the Red- 
breast was much the tamest. When she was up inthe big elm tree, she would swoop down 
at call, not touching a twig between her lofty perch and the hand. Hickory nuts were 
offered and preferred to the suet, but the seeds of the Norway spruce were still more to 
her taste. She would fly to a branch with a seed, rub off its wing, and after placing it 
in a suitable notch or crack, eat it leisurely. The Red-breasted Nuthatch would drink 
from a dish held in the hand, would take the proffered food while perched near the 
ground, and once even settled down inthe hand as if going to sleep. 
These birds were seen to eat snow, and Chickadees would frequently cling to an 
icicle on the roof and catch the drops of water as they fell from a shorter icicle near by. 
1 Bird Studies with a Camera, p. 49. 
2 For this account I am indebted to Mr. Robert J. Sim of Jefferson, Ohio, 


Taming Wild Birds without a Cage. 129 


Three or four Downy and Hairy Woodpeckers came to the window-sill, and would some- 
times peck the fingers of persons feeding them. The Brown Creeper was far more cau- 
tious, and never came to the hand. 

The familiarity of the Canada Jay or Meat Bird is known to everybody who has 
hunted or camped in the northern woods; its fear is allayed by hunger even more promptly 
than in Chickadees and Nuthatches. Audubon says of these birds that “when their 
appetite is satisfied, they become shy, and are in the habit of hiding themselves among 
close woods or thickets; but when hungry they show no alarm at the approach of man.” 
While his friend was fishing in a canoe on one of the Maine lakes in the summer of 1833, 
“the Jays were so fearless as to 
alight in one end of his bark, 
while he sat in the other, and 
help themselves to his bait. . . . 
The lumberers or woodcutters 
of this state, . . . frequently 
amuse themselves in their camp 
during the eating hour with what 
they call ‘transporting the car- 
rion bird.’ This is done by cut- 
ting a pole eight or ten feet in 
length, and balancing it on the 
sill of their hut, the end outside 
of the entrance being baited with 
a piece of flesh of any kind. Im- 
mediately on seeing the tempting 
morsel, the Jays alight on it, and 
while they are busily engaged 
in devouring it, the woodcutter 
gives a smart blow to the end of 


the pole within the hut, which 
“1 n . Fig.126. Male Red-eyed Vireo prepared to inspect and clean nest. 
seldom fails to drive the birds Notice that in this series—Figs. 50-57, 125—the birds uniformly occupy 


high in the air, and not infre- thesame perch. Detail of bird shown in Fig. 49. 
quently kills them. They even 
enter the camps and would fain eat from the hands of the men while at their meals.” 
Possibly no bird has keener vision or sharper ears than the Canada Goose, which in 
its wild state is said to be vigilant, suspicious, and hard to be surprised, yet it is often easily 
and quickly tamed. There are in Cleveland nearly forty of these geese, which are descended 
from a smaller number introduced about twenty-five years ago. Their migratory impulse 
has been completely lost, and their sense of fear subdued, but their other wild instincts 
remain. They live mostly in the parks, going from one to another as the spirit moves 
them, and breed on the small artificial islands in artificial ponds. I sometimes hear 
their honk / as they fly over the city at night or in early morning, and see their “ harrow” 
or “triangle” which plows the air by day often within bow-shot from Euclid Avenue. 
When the birds are feeding on a lawn you can walk among them and drive them 
like a flock of tame geese, but they hate dogs and take to wing or water the moment one 
9 


130 Wild Birds. 


is seen to approach. They once had the habit of alighting on the roof of a tall building 
near Wade Park, but after one of their number met with the mishap of falling down a 
ventilating shaft this practice seems to have been abandoned. 

Audubon speaks of a pair of geese which bred for three years near the mouth of the 
Green River in Kentucky,’ and of his experience in feeding them at the nest. The male 
was at first very pugnacious, and once dealt him such a blow on the arm that he thought it 
was broken. In the course of a week both birds would take the proffered corn, but never 
allowed himtotouchthem. ‘ Whenever I attempted this,” says Audubon, “the male met 
my fingers with his bill, and bit me so severely that I gave it up.” Later he trapped the 
entire family of eleven, pinioned them and turned them loose in his garden. He kept 
the whole flock three years. The old birds did not breed again, but two pairs of the 
young reared new broods. 

On one of his shooting excursions Audubon shot a wild goose, and on his return 
sent it to the kitchen to be prepared for the table. The cook brought him an egg ready 
to be laid. This was placed under a hen, and in due time produced a bird, which became 
very gentle and would feed from the hand. When two years old it mated with a male 
and reared a family. 

We have seen how fear may vanish before the surge of the parental impulse which 
impels a bird to seek, nourish, and defend its offspring, even at the risk of life itself, and will 
now consider how this instinct may be used in taming wild birds at the nest and in 
bringing them to the hand. 

If young birds of those species in which the parental instincts are very strong, are 
taken from the nest when nearly ready to fly, the old birds, especially if they be among 
the class of tamer individuals, may be brought direct to the hand in a short space of 
time. To their excited vision men are as walking trees. Their attention is riveted on 
the young, and the man is nothing to them, providing he remains quiet, or moves 
about with caution. Whatever fear remains is blocked by the stronger instinct to go 
to their young. 

Every occasion on which the tent described in these pages is brought up toa nest 
of young birds is a direct experiment in the taming process. No matter how far the 
discipline is carried or how little permanency it may possess, the principle is always the 
same. By this method wild birds, while the parental instincts are at their height, can be 
tamed to a degree without use of acage. In illustration of the process, we will select the 
Robin and Chestnut-sided Warbler, although the experiments to be described were not 
carried out with this end especially in view. In any case parental instinct is the chief 
agent employed. 

The Robins now referred to have served so often in these pages as a text for the 
illustration of habit and instinct that I need only say that they nested high in an oak tree 
in some woods, and that the entire branch with the nest was carried to a perfectly bare 
field on July 25th, when the young were a week old. At this new site the young passed 
another week, taking their first flight at noon on the last day of the month. I was en- 
camped beside them for parts of six days, and spent altogether twenty-four hours at their 
nest. Although the familiar Robin is usually an easy mark for the bird-photographer, this 
particular pair were extremely wary. They showed a bold front when openly assailed, 

1 Ornithological Biography, vol. iii., pp. 8, 9. 


Taming Wild Birds without a Cage. 131 


but succumbed to fear completely, the moment the tent was closed, and refused to 
approach the nest. On the second day the female was on the nesting bough in ten 
minutes, but hesitated and made seven consecutive visits before actually feeding the 
young. After several hours their fear had become so well subdued that the wary male 
brought and delivered food while I was engaged in taking down the tent and stood close 
by. On the third day the young were fed while the tent was going up, but a full half 
hour had elapsed before their behavior was perfectly free and spontaneous. On the 
fourth day the birds came as before, and life at the nest was resumed with perfect con- 
fidence after the space of twenty minutes. The female would now sit placidly on the 
nest in face of the tent and the window in its front, across which the hand was frequently 
drawn to adjust the shutter that was clicking at random intervals but twenty-eight inches 
from her ears. 

At the close of the day’s observations, I took the camera outside the tent, and photo- 
graphed the male as he came to the nest. The moment I entered the tent to take it 
down he was back again with a mouth full of cherries. When after striking the tent and 
rolling it up I stood quietly by the nest for a few moments, the cock came for the third 
time and delivered a large grasshopper to his never-to-be-satisfied brood. 

On the first day four hours failed to bring these birds to their needy children, while 
inthe ninth and last the male, the more suspicious of the two, was on hand with food 
in seven minutes. With the new objects in constant view, new associations had been 
formed. The strong parental instinct supported by habit had banished most of their 
former fear. The first steps in the taming process had been taken, and were carried fur- 
ther in the case now to be described. 

Two nests of the Chestnut-sided Warbler, each containing fresh eggs, were found in 
a pasture on the twelfth day of June. The behavior of the birds at both nests was at 
first essentially the same, so far as it was tested. While the eggs were still fresh, the 
nests were often visited without seeing or hearing a bird, but during incubation the female, 
who is a close sitter, would allow me to approach within a yard or two feet. Then as I 
extended my hand slowly toward her she would hop out and cling with head down on the 
farther side of the nest, so that only her little tail was visible over its rim. Any one 
prone to discover protective mimicry in such cases would find a striking example of it in 
this attitude,—the little gray tail of the bird simulating so well one of the twigs which 
helped to support the gray wall of the nest. It was rather the case of an alert animal 
lying still or in hiding until a present danger might be past. If you kept your position 
long enough the bird would drop to the ground, where joined by her mate, both would 
hop about in the grass chipping nervously, but keeping well out of sight. On approach- 
ing one of the nests still later when there were young, the female was usually overtaken 
in the act of brooding. At such times it was easy to walk slowly up and place your 
hand close to the brooding bird. But before allowing you actually to touch her, she 
would flit to the grass, and with spread wings and tail practice that ‘‘art of feigning”’ as 
it is usually called, although it is not an art or anything learned or practiced for the occa- 
sion, but an inherited instinct, the end and advantage of which is to distract your atten- 
tion from the nest to the moving bird. One day I stood by and watched the little mother 
to see how long her antics would last. She would come within a yard of my feet when 
I remained perfectly quiet, and trail her wings along the ground, making repeated sallies 


132 Wild Birds. 


back and forth, flying only when close pressed, and then always away from her nest. On 
one occasion this was kept up from ten to fifteen minutes, and did not cease until I 
withdrew. 

My experiments at the first nest were begun on June 12th, by clearing away the 
bushes in front. The tent was set up two feet away on the morning of the 15th, 
while the little hen was still sitting over the eggs. She would dart out of the nest, 
return and take a peep inside, sit for a few minutes and be off. When all was quiet she 
could be seen jumping in and out repeatedly, as if equally uncomfortable whether 
away from her treasures or hugging them close. In the course of half an hour it was 
easy to photograph the 
sitting bird, who now 
paid little heed to the 
shutter, and remained un- 
disturbed on the nest 
during my preparations 
for leaving. 

On the following day 
the old bird was still per- 
sistently sitting, and even 
allowed me to erect the 
tent close beside her 
without budging. When 
finally driven off by the 
hand, she uttered a few 
tseeps and returned ina 
moment. Once the male 
came, and as I supposed, 
placed an insect in the 
nest, when his mate, who 
stood close by, hopped 


Fig.127. Offering grasshopper to a Chestnut-sided Warbler who has been tamed to the brim, put down her 
without use of a cage. It was possible to approach this bird and stroke her back 
head, and as I thought 


with the hand, without giving alarm. 

ate the food, but no, she 
was feeding the little ones, for she was now a mother. Four young birds, scarcely 
bigger than bumblebees, had just emerged from their shells. They must have been 
hatched since noon of the previous day. 

On the third day these Warblers paid no attention to either the tent or the operator, 
and before going away I was able to touch the bird on the nest, though not without 
sending her off. The fourth day found their confidence undiminished, for the sitting 
bird eagerly seized a grasshopper which I offered from the hand stretched through the 
tent window. Four days later still I spent nearly seven hours with these Warblers, and 
in the afternoon began to test more systematically the strength of the intimacy which we 
had cultivated. Taking a long twig in the hand and reaching through the window in the 
front of the tent, I touched the old bird. She resented this but little and when her back 
was scratched seemed to like the sensation. Then I left the tent to look for insects, and. 


Taming Wild Birds without a Cage. 13 


(SS) 


after a long search returned with a few small grasshoppers. When one of these was 
offered the bird would eye the squirming insect and try to seize it when held within 
reach. Wishing to economize, I held on to the insect and nearly pulled the bird off the 
nest. 

After discarding the tent I was able to walk up to this bird and stroke her back with 
my hand without disturbing her in the least. Setting up the camera outside and attach- 
ing a tube with pneumatic bulb at the end, I made a number of photographs which show 
the Warbler sharply eying an insect and prepared to seize it when held a few inches 
away. It would have been an easy matter to take her in the hand, though possibly 
not without injury to the young. Their 
early flight from the nest cut short any 
further experiments, but what could 
not have been done with a bird who 
had become so tame and confiding in 
the course of a few days ? 

The foregoing account does not 
necessarily imply that a wild bird can 
be induced to remain docile in the 
presence of man for any great length 
of time while still enjoying the free- 
dom of its wild life. If the lesson 
learned is to be a permanent acquisi- 
tion, it must be often repeated, and no 
other teachers allowed to interfere. To 
effect this the animal must as a rule 
be placed under restraint or in a cage, 
where its experiences are more uniform, 
more limited and under perfect control. 

In free life a new habit must strug- 
gle with other competitors and is liable 
to be suppressed quickly. However, I 
think it has been clearly shown that in 
the beginnings of the taming process Fig 128. Chestnut-sided Warbler family. The male, perched 

. E above, has just delivered an insect to his mate, who quickly 
which have been illustrated, where no __passedit to the young and continued to brood. The same nest 
physical restraint is used, the sense of 
fear must be combated by a stronger and contrary impulse, such as hunger or the 
parental instincts, which will lead the bird to undergo new experiences, and finally to 
adopt new habits. 

Audubon has given an interesting account of some Phcebes or Pewees which nested 
in a cave on his plantation in Pennsylvania, and became the subject of some of his earliest 
studies and experiments in ornithology. It admirably illustrates the taming process 
under the spur of natural instinct. * 

“On my first going into the cave,” he says, “ the male flew violently towards the en- 
trance, snapped his bill sharply and repeatedly, accompanying this action with a tremulous 


is shown in Figs. 3, 11, 127, 129, and 130. 


1 Ornithological Biography, vol. ii., p. 122. 


134 Wild Birds. 


rolling note, the import of which I soon guessed. . . . Several days in succession I 
went to the spot, and saw with pleasure that as my visits increased in frequency, the 
birds became more familiarized to me, and, before a week had elapsed, the Pewees and 
myself were quite on terms of intimacy. It was now the tenth of April. . . . The 
Pewees, I observed, began working at their old nest. My presence no longer alarmed 
either of them.” He was soon able to put his hand close to the sitting bird without dis- 
turbing it. 

While possessed by the incubation spirit many birds, as is well known, are indifferent 
to danger and will hug their eggs at any cost. 
In this respect few can excel the ‘‘ tame 
villagic fowl,” who displays greater stupidity 
than most wild birds, who rarely sit on an 
empty nest,’ and have been known to reject 
strange eggs. In this state birds cannot be 
considered tame although the sense of fear 
may be temporarily dulled, and one of the 
conditions of the taming process fulfilled. 
The hen will peck vigorously at the in- 
truder, and if hustled off the nest will soon 
return. Some birds like Song Sparrows 
and Brown Thrushes will remain immov- 
able as if hiding until you come dangerously 
near, when they glide off silently, but usually 
remain quiet for a moment only. The 
Robin flies off in a passion. The Tropic 
Bird fights but sticks to her egg. The 
Woodpeckers are close sitters and may 
sometimes be taken in the hand. A Chick- 
adee which I worried with a straw would 
peck angrily at it, but remained on the nest. 
The Cedar-birds retire in silence. In this 


Fig.129. Female Chestnut-sided Warbler approaching state birds become passive merely through 
nest and looking in. At this time there were eggs, or the 


young had barely pipped the shell. 


the temporary suppression of the sense of 
fear. 

Fish Hawks used to nest on Plum Island, New York, where according to Mr. C. S, 
Allen,’ they had been zealously protected by the owner of the island for upwards of 
thirty years previous to 1885. The first nest shown to him by Mr. Jerome, the faithful 
guardian of the birds, was “fairly in his door-yard, close by his front gate, and only 
about fifty yards fromhishome. It was placed upon an old pile of fence rails, rotted to 
black mould in the center, but kept up by the yearly addition of fresh rails. Mr. Jerome 
said that to his knowledge this nest had been occupied every year for forty years.” It 
had been added to yearly until its huge bulk of sticks and miscellaneous materials would 

1 For an account of a pair of Bald Eagles nesting on the ground in the New York Zoélogical Park and incubat- 


ing a good-sized stone which was placed in the improvised nest, see Aird Lore, vol. iii., p. 34. IgOT. 
* The Auk, vol. ix., p. 313, 1892 


Taming Wild Birds without a Cage. 135 


make three cart loads. It was but seven or eight feet from the ground, so that by 
stepping on a projecting rail the beautifully spotted eggs within could be seen. “ Mr. 
Jerome could pass close to the pile of rails without the birds leaving the nest, while 
I could not get nearer than thirty or forty feet.” At other places on the island, the 
birds would alight on one nest while he was examining another near by. This illustrates 
how a shy bird may become relatively tame during the breeding season, and shows clearly 
how some learn to discriminate. 

That many birds become tame in a state of nature is well known and the subject is 
full of interest. The Pine Grosbeak is as good an illustration of the fact as may be 
found in this part of the world. Pine 
Grosbeaks make their summer home in the 
vast forests of evergreens which cover the 
continent from Labrador to Alaska. A 
few, it is said, have been found breeding in 
latitude 47° in New Brunswick, and they 
have even been recorded in summer on Mt. 
LaFayette, New Hampshire. They are 
irregular winter visitors to the Northern 
States, sometimes going so far south as 
Maryland and Kansas. In the winter of 
1884, they were very common at Holder- 
ness, New Hampshire, beginning to appear 
in small flocks about the middle of Feb- # 
ruary and finally disappearing after the 
eighteenth of March. At first they were 
tame and could be approached without 
difficulty, while later they became shy and 
timid. They frequented the white pines. 
on the buds of which they fed, but occa- 
sionally came into the open, and sang loud 
and merrily. 


I remember meeting a flock of these Fig. 130. Female Chestnut-sided Warbler inspecting her 
plump, stalwart looking birds ina grove of %0"™= Met Mavine setved food. 
sapling pines on the last day of February. The woods on every side were hoary with snow 
which had been falling for hours. When a young pine drooping under its weight of 
snow suddenly blossomed with a bright company of these birds, you might travel far 
to find a more attractive winter picture. A bird would sometimes drop on a branch, 
and settle down as if going to sleep. Then suddenly aroused by the desire for 
food he would sidle to the end of the bough, pick out the terminal or largest bud, 
twirl it between his stout cone-shaped mandibles to get rid of the scales and then 
swallow the resinous morsel. After seeing this experiment performed a good many 
times, I selected a handsome male, walked up to him, and caught him with my hat, as if 
he were a butterfly. When I stooped to pick him off the snow, he squeaked and struck 
violently with his beak, uttering a peculiar car-r-r-r-r / When placed on the snow again 
he flapped about for a few moments resisting every attempt to take him, and finally rose 


136 Wild Birds. 


and disappeared among the snow-laden trees. There were about fifty birds in this flock 
and the grove resounded with their clear whistled notes. They were easily approached 
at all times and in all weathers, during the early weeks of their visit, agreeing in this re- 
spect with the Bohemian Waxwing, the Arctic representative of the Cedar-bird. Two 
small flocks of these birds visited Burlington, Vermont, November 24 and January 21, 
1882. A low plaintive call-note first attracted my attention, when a party of eight of 
these fine birds came into view. They were leisurely preening their feathers on the 
lower branches of a red cedar tree. When close upon them, they paid no attention, and 
finally wishing to see them fly, I had almost to shake them from the branches. They 
went off in a compact body like their smaller relative, giving a “ zee, sec, zee-ze /” 
call-note. 

Audubon speaks of the familiarity of Crossbills which he observed while on a 

moose hunt in the summer of 1833. They alighted on his head, showing no fear, and 
"five or six were caught at one time under a snowshoe. 

This tameness found among many Arctic species has been met with on a much wider 
scale in remote oceanic islands, where man is almost unknown and where the conditions 
of life are very different from those of the mainland. The inhabitants of the Galapagos 
Islands, which lie under the equator between five and six hundred miles from the west 
coast of South America, offer a most striking example of this anomaly. Their natural 
history which has been told in one of Darwin’s interesting chapters, first led him to reflect 
on the origin of species.” He says that many of the animals and plants are aboriginal, and 
found nowhere else, that ‘‘ there is even a difference between the inhabitants of the differ- 
ent islands; yet all show a marked relationship with those of America. . . . The 
archipelago is a little world within itself, or rather a satellite attached to America, whence 
it has derived a few stray colonists, and has received the general character of its indi- 
genous productions.” He found twenty-six species of land birds, all peculiar to the 
islands excepting only one, the Bobolink, whose summer range extends as far north as 
Labrador. 

All the common terrestrial birds of these volcanic islands were very tame, and all 
says Darwin, “ often approached sufficiently near to be killed with a switch, and some- 
times, as I myself tried, with a cap or hat. A gun is here almost superfluous; 
for with the muzzle I pushed a hawk off the branch of a tree. One day whilst lying 
down, a mocking-thrush alighted on the edge of a pitcher, made of the shell of a tortoise, 
which I held in my hand, and began very quickly to sip the water; it allowed me to lift 
it from the ground whilst seated on the vessel: I often tried, and very nearly suc- 
ceeded in catching these birds by their legs. 

“ These birds, although now still more persecuted, do not readily become wild: in 
Charles Island, which had then been colonized about six years, I saw a boy sitting by a 
well with a switch in his hand, with which he killed the doves and finches as they came to 
drink. He had already procured a little heap of them for his dinner; and he said that 
he had constantly been in the habit of waiting by this well for the same purpose.” 

Darwin remarks that the most anomalous fact on this subject which he had met was 
the wildness of certain small birds in the Arctic portions of North America, while some 
of the same species were said to be tame in their winter quarters in the United States. 

‘ Ornithological Biography, vol, il., p. 436. 2 Fournal of Researches, Chapter XVII. 


Taming Wild Birds without a Cage. {37 


‘‘ How strange it is,” says he, “that the English wood-pigeon, generally so wild a bird, 
should very frequently rear its young in shrubberies close to houses!” 

Respecting the wildness which birds exhibit towards man, Darwin could find no way 
of accounting for it except as inherited habit, but in another work, he thus refers to the 
same subject’: “If we look to successive generations, or to the race, there is no doubt 
that birds and other animals gradually both acquire and lose caution in relation to man 
and other enemies; and this caution is certainly in chief part an inherited habit or instinct, 
but in part the result of individual experience.” 

The observations which have been made on the behavior of old and young birds do 
not support any theory of the inheritance of habits to account for tameness in animals, 
but as already shown afford a better clue of how this has been brought about. Let us 
go back to the Pine Grosbeak which, when fresh from his sub-Arctic home, can be 
approached and caught with your hat as could many of the birds in the Galapagos Islands 
when Darwin visited them in 1835. So far as I know, no one has studied the young of 
this species in the nest and ascertained whether they show the same instincts of fear in 
general toward strange sights and sounds, as we find in passerine birds nesting farther 
south. Assuming that they do, and there can be little doubt of it, the instinct has 
lapsed through disuse in adult life, although the capacity of expressing fear remains 
and may be quickly aroused and directed towards particular objects. The timidity of 
this bird in March after a brief experience with the ways of men is therefore virtually 
an acquired character, and there is no evidence that it is handed down by inheritance. 

The breeding range of many northern birds covers a vast area, and in different 
sections there is reason to expect much variation in the habits of the same species. The 
timidity of the Arctic birds referred to may have been due to local conditions affecting 
a relatively small number, or the birds may have been young individuals whose intuitive 
fear had not been worn away, or old ones possessed of a wisdom derived from extensive 
travel southward. Among birds which are reputed to be shy, tamer individuals are 
to be found, and many acquire the habit of nesting in gardens and often close to houses. 
In the Galapagos Islands, where birds had lived in comparative security for ages with 
no fierce and relentless enemies to mar their tranquillity, the instinct of fear had not 
only lapsed, but the power of forming new habits had weakened. It is therefore not 
surprising that they should be slow in acquiring a fear of man, but any animal which 
finally fails in the face of constant persecution to profit by experience has touched 
the lowest depths of stupidity, and its days are numbered. 


1 The Descent of Man, p. 80. 


INDEX. 


A, 


Abdomen, size and secondary use of, in young, 121. 

Abstract ideas, the nature of, if present in birds, 3. 

Accessories, or bird-photographer’s outfit enumerated, 35. 

Alarm calls, in Catbird, 76, 77; effect of, upon birds of other species, 77, 122-123; in Robin, 123. 

Alarm clock, illustration of, 5. 

Allen, Charles A., 119. 

Allen, C, S., 115, 134. 

Altrices, definition of, 117; development of fear in young of, 120; fate of young due to premature 
development of fear in, 120-121; care and education of young in, 121; specialization of fear in 
young of, 121. 

Ampelis garrulus (Bohemian Waxwing), origin of specific name in, §2. 

Animal behavior, a working theory of, xvi. 

Animal photography, a desideratum of, 34. 

Animals, the evil of anthropomorphism in study of, xv; duty and privilege of student of, xv; the native 
equipment of, xvi; vulgar error concerning, 125; variable personalties of, 125; winning confi- 
dence of, 125-127; origin of natural wildness and tameness of, 125, 126, 137; conditions of 
taming, 126, 127. 

Anthropomorphism, evil of, in study of animal behavior, xv. 

Approach to the wild bird, the problem of, and its solution, 33. 

Audubon, John James, 104, 108, 129, 130, 133, 136. 


B. 


Bag, for accessories of bird-photography, 35; for plates, 34. 

Baltimore Oriole (see Oriole). 

Basin Harbor (Vt.), Eaves Swallow dispossessed by House Sparrows at, 114, 115. 

Bird-photography (see Photography). 

Birds, mental faculties of, xvii; instincts of, xvi; problem of approaching, xvii, 33; strongest lure for, xvii; 
guiding senses of, 3; rudimentary condition of olfactory organ in, 3; actions of, when nest is 
robbed, 4; effect of noise of photographic shutter upon, 5, 34; effect of sounds upon, 5, 68, 112; 
appearance of feather-shafts in young of, 6; behavior of, after change of nesting site, 11, 22, 39, 
73; attachment to nest, eggs, and young in, 6, 13; individualities of, 36; attractions in haunts 
of man for, 51; routine in home life of, 54; interest in watching nesting habits of, at short range, 
15, 16, 54; brooding in Cedar-birds, 17, 56; maternal instincts of, 5; digestion, assimilation, 
and growth of young of, 66; care of young in nest of, 94; brooding attitudes of, 94, 97-98; 
automatic response in gullet of young of, ror, 102; inspection and cleaning of nest in, 103-110; 
economy of food in, 102; struggles with insects at nests of, 103; cleaning or sanitary instinct 
in, 103, 104; disposal of excreta of young of, 104-110; use of excreta of young as food by, 
to5—110; force of habit displayed in, 111; cleanliness of nesting site in, 107, 108; adapta- 
bility of, 113; change of diet in, 116; classification of, based on early condition of young in, 
117; fear in old and young of, 117; fate of young of, due to premature development of fear, 
r20, 121; lack of discrimination in young of, 121; specialization of fear in young of, 121; use 
of pot-belly in young of, 121; effect of alarm calls of, on birds of other species, 77, 122, 123; 
in winter at Jefferson (O.), 128; behavior of, during incubation, 134; taming of, 125-137; 
tameness of, in nature, 135-137; of Galapagos Islands, 136, 137; of Charles Island, 136; wild- 
ness of, in Arctic America, 136-137. (See under names of species.) 


139 


140 Index. 


Blackbird, Crow or Purple Grackle (Quiscalus quiscala,. Linn.), nest-cleaning instincts of, 104, 105; 
and brooding Robin, 39. 

Blackbird, Redwing (Agelatus phaniceus, Linn.), the bathing of, 16; an attractive nest of, 20; 
preparation of nesting site of, for use of tent, 20; behavior of, 20; care of young of, 20, 21; 
brooding of, 20, 21, 112; respiration of, 21; flight of young from nest of, 21; nest-cleaning 
instinct of, 104, 105, 112; eating excreta of young by, 105; erection of feathers in female of, 
21; force of habit displayed in, r12. 

Blackbirds, nest-cleaning instincts of, 104. 

Black cherry tree as an aviary in late summer, 62. 

Bluebird (Sialia sialis, Linn.), arrival of, at Cleveland (O.), 49; nest-hole of, 71; general habits of, 
71-75; arrival at Holderness (N. H), 71; at Northfield (N. H.), 71; call-notes of, 71, 73; court- 
ship of, 71; polygamy in, 72; choice and care of nesting site, 72; defense of nest of, 72; in 
converted nest of Flicker, 72; removal of nest of, 72; adjustment of vertical tree-trunk with 
nest of, 72; behavior of, after removal of nest, 73; feeding young in, 73; use of tail for sup- 
port in, 73; strength of parental instincts in, 73-75; response of young in, 73, 74; nest-clean- 
ing of, 74, 75, 104; young of, and their food, 75; rate of feeding young in, 75; in old Robin’s 
nest, 75; pugnacity of, 75; number of broods of, 75; repair of nest in, 75. 

Breeding season, lateness of, in Cedar-bird, 52; in Goldfinch, 52. 

Brewster, William, 51. 

Bristol (R. I), nesting of Osprey at, 115, 116. 

Brooding, in Redwing Blackbird, 20, 21, 112; in Kingbird, 28, 94, 97; in Robin, 46, 47, 94, 98; in Cedar- 
bird, 17, 57; in Red-eyed Vireo, 68; in Night Hawk, 80, 82, 85; necessity of, 94; in Brown 
Thrush, 94. 

Brown Thrush (see Thrush). 

Bunting, Bay-winged (Poocetes gramineus, Gmel.), and black snake, 123. 

Burlington (Vt.), arrival of Robins at, 48-49; nest of Black Duck at, 119, 120; records of visits of 
Bohemian Waxwings at, 136. 

Buzzard, Turkey, filthiness of nests of, 108. 


Cz 


Call-notes, of Cedar-bird, 17, 56; of young of Baltimore Oriole, 18; of Robin, 45; of Red-eyed Vireo, 
64,65; of Bluebird, 73; of Redwing Blackbird, 77; of Night Hawk, 81, 82; of Kingtisher, go; 
of Canada Goose, 129; of Bohemian Waxwing, 136. 

Camera, value of, in portrayal of animals in action, xviii; size and construction of, 32; the twin lens, 
32; the reflecting, invention and history of, 32. 

Catbird (Galeoscoptes carolinensis, Linn.), shyness of, 5; minute observations on, 76-79; parental and 
fighting instincts of, 76, 77; description of nests of, 76, 77; feeding and care of young in, 77- 
79; development of wing-quills in young of, 77, 78; capture of dragon-flies by, 77, 78; alarm 
notes of, 76, 77; suppression of fear in, 76-78; rate of feeding young in, 78; behavior of 
young of, 77, 79, 117, 118; eating of excreta of young by, 105; attracted by alarm of Robin 
122. 

Cats, and young after removal of nesting bough, 15; as enemies of young birds, 51; exemplified in Jan 
Steen, 51. 

Cedar-bird, Cedar Waxwing (Ampelts cedrorum, Viell), life at nest of, after change of site, 13, 61; the 
nesting of, 17, 52, 53; removal of nesting bough of, 17, 54, 57, 59; call-notes of, 17, 56; care 
of young by, 18, 54-62, 94; flight of young of, 18, 60; spring and fall behavior of, 52, 53; 
winter flocks of, 52; late breeding of, 52; breeding season at Northfield (N. H.), 52; quiet 
nature of, 52; origin of names of, 52; appendages to feathers of wings and tails in young and 
adult of, 52-53; food of, 62, 63; position, materials, and construction of nests of, 53, 58; pro- 
portion of young of, reared, 53; carly weakness of parental instincts of, 53, 54; desertion of 

nests in, 54; the hatching of, 54; routine in nesting habits of, 54; favorite nesting trees and 

bushes of, 53; regurgitation of food for young in, 55,61; number of berries carried in gullet of , 

55; function of gullet in, 55, 56, 101; habits of nestlings of, 56; sudden appearance of sense 

of fear in nestlings of, 56, 60; timidity of, 54; response to alarm signal in, 57; brooding in, 

17, 57,94; supplying colored yarn for nest of, 58, time occupied in building nest in, 58; laying 


Index. 141 


Cedar-bird, Cedar Waxwing—Continucd 
and incubating, and hatching of eggs in, 58, 59; young of, at birth, 59; age of young of, when 
eyes open, 59; disappearance of young in nest of, 59; behavior of, in approaching nest with 
food, 60; development of color marks in fledglings of, 60, 61; appearance of feather-shafts 
and wax-like tips in wings of, 60; habits of young of, when ready to fly, 60; rate of feeding of 
young in, 61; the feeding, food, and care of young of, 55, 61, 1901; similarity in sexes of, 61, 
62; peculiar signals of, at nest, 62, 102; habit of sipping maple sap in, 62; flocking of, in Au- 
gust, 62; and the black cherry tree, 62; eating spiders or robbing them of their prey, 63; taking 
insects on the wing, 62, 63; gaping habit in, 98; gluttony of, ro2; inspection and cleaning 
the nest by, 104, 105-107; eating excreta of young by, 105, 106; parasites in nest of, 107; 
habits of, during incubation, 134. 

Central Park, Chickadees in, 127. 

Chapman, F. M., 127. 

Charles Island, tameness of birds of, 136. 

Chatterer, the origin of name as applied to the Waxwings, 52. 

Cherry Bird (see Cedar-bird). 

Chick, domestic, instinct of fear in, 119; behavior of, when Hawk passes overhead, 120. 

Chickadee (Parus atricapillus, Linn.), cleanliness of, 104; attracted by Robin’s alarm, 122, 123; habits 
and tameness of, 127, 128; during incubation, 134. 

Cicada, eaten by young of Cedar-bird, 61; struggles of Kingbirds with, 103; combat of, with House 
Sparrow, 103. 

City life, possible origin of, in many birds, 50. 

Clamp, the ‘‘Graphic”’ ball and 

Cleaning instinct, 103-110 (see Instincts). 

Cleveland (O.), spring arrival of Robins and Bluebirds at, 49; Robins in, 50; Red-headed Woodpeckers 


‘ket, 34. 


in, 50. 

Color, discrimination of, in Cedar-bird, 58; development of, in Kingfisher, 86, 91. 

Courtship in Bluebird, 71, 72. 

Cowbird, early experiments of, in animal psychology, xvi; young of, in Warbler’s nest, 121, 122; ab- 
sence of fear in, 122. 

Creeper, Brown (Certhta jamiliaris americana, Bonap.), shyness of, 128, 120. 

Crossbills, American (Loxta curvirostra minor, Brehm.), effect of alarm of Crow upon, 122. 

Crow, effect of alarms of, upon other birds, 122. 

Cuckoo, appearance of feather-shafts in, 6. 

Cycle, the reproductive, in birds, 3. 


Darwin, Charles, 136, 137. 

Dorchester (N. H.), nesting of Swift in shed at, 114. 

Dorset (O.), nesting of Swift in barn at, 114. 

Dragon-fly, capture and killing of, by Kingbirds, 28, 103; as food of young Catbirds, 77-79. 

Duck, Black (Anas obscura, Gmel.), absence of fear in newly hatched young of, rrg; nesting of, 119, 120; 


behavior of old and young of, when latter are possessed of fear, 120. 


E. 


Eagle, behavior of, when nesting, compared with that of Night Hawk, So: improvised nest of, 134. 

Earthworms, fed to young Robin, 46, 47. 

Economy of food, in Robin, 39; in the Kingbird, 28, ro2; in the Red-eyed Vireo, 68. 

Eggs, of Kingbird, 21; birds strongly attached to, 13; incubation of, in Robin, 36; lateness of laying 
of, in Cedar-bird and American Goldfinch, 52; proportion of young reared to number of, in 
Cedar-bird, 53; laying and incubation of, in Cedar-bird, 58; destruction of, in Red-eyed Vireos, 
69: hatching of, in Night Hawk, So; relation of size of, to condition of young at birth, ELG: 
of Canada Goose hatched under hen, 130; behavior of Chestnut-sided Warblers, with, 131; 
behavior of birds when incubating, 134. 


142 Index. 


Excreta, character of, in young of passerine birds, 104; disposal of, by parents, 104-107; character of, 
in young Kingfisher, 107; use of, as food by adult birds, 105-107, 109; character of, in Eagles 
and Hawks, 108; actions of Cedar-birds in taking, devouring, or removing, from nest, 106, 107; 
use of, as food in Chestnut-sided Warblers, 109. 

Eyelids, angular contour of, in young Night Hawk, 80. 

Eyes, opening of, in young of Cedar-bird, 59; in young of Red-eyed Vireo, 64. 


F., 


Fauna of Galapagos Islands, peculiar character of, 136, 137. 

Faxon, W., 51. 

Fear, the instinct of, 3; the suppression of, 4; development of, in relation to appearance of feather- 
shafts of wings, 6; suppression of, in Cedar-bird, 17, 57, 59; in Oriole, 19; in Redwing Black- 
birds, 20, 21; in Kingbird, 22, 27; in Robin, 39, 40, 45; appearance of, in young Cedar-birds, 
56, 60; in young of Red-eyed Vireo, 69; suppression of, in adult Bluebird, 72, 73; in Catbird, 
76-78; development of, in young Catbirds, 77, 79; nature, time of appearance, sudden mani- 
festation, and adaptive value of, 117-124; instinct of, in domestic chick, 119; in ducklings 
of Black Duck, 120; of Hawk in sky, expressed by chick, 120; distinction between inherited 
and acquired, 121, 137; expression of, in Robin, 122; checked by hunger, 126-128. 

Feathers, development of, as guide in controlling nesting site, 6; development of, in Cuckoos, 6; down, 
in Bluebird, 75; development of, in Red-eyed Vireos, 64, 68; in Catbirds, 77, 78; in King- 
fisher, 86, 91; in Night Hawk, 80, 85; condition of, at birth as basis for classification, 117; 
development of, relation to fear, 6, 117-124. 

Feather-shafts, appendages of, in Cedar-bird, 52, 60 (see Feathers). 

Fireflies as food of young Night Hawk, 82. 

Fish, captured by Kingfisher, 90; resources of Kingfisher to prevent escape of, 92. 

Fly, robber (Asz/us), fed to young of Bluebird, 73, 74; escape of, from grasp of Kingbird, 102, 103. 

Focusing-cloth, adjustment of, in tent, 31. 

Food, of young Cedar-birds, 17, 18, 55, 61; of young Baltimore Orioles, 19; of Kingbird, 28, 102, 103, 
116; of young Robins, 39, 48, 116; of Robin in summer and winter, 48; of Cedar-bird, 52, 62, 
63; economy of, in Kingbird, 28, 102; in Red-eyed Vireos, 68; in Chestnut-sided Warbler, 
109; distribution of, to young Cedar-birds explained, 55; and its distribution in Red-eyed 
Vireos, 67-69; of young Bluebirds, 75; of young Catbirds, 78, 79; of young Kingfishers, 
go-92; of young Night Hawk, 82; of Hawks, Owls, and other birds under exceptional con- 
ditions, 116. 

Foster-children, treatment by Kingbird, 2 

Fowl, domestic, stupidity and pugnacity of, 134. 

Fruits, fed to young by Cedar-birds, 17, 61; by Orioles, 19; by Robins, 48; eaten by Robin in winter, 
48; cultivated, eaten as makeshift, 48; served to young of Red-eyed Vireos, 68, 69; of Cat- 
bird, 76, 77, 79. 


G. 


Galapagos Islands, observations of Darwin on fauna of, 136, 137. 

Gluttony in Cedar-birds, ror, ro2. 

Goldfinch, American (Spinus tristis, Linn.) , lateness of breeding of, 52; attracted by Robin’s alarm, 123. 

Goose, Canada (Branta canadensis, Linn.), young of, 117; tamability of, 127, 129, 130; habits and 
breeding of, in captivity, 129, 130. 

Grampus (Corydalis cornutus), feeding of, to young Kingbirds, 103; formidable appearance and size 
of, Tor, 103. 

Grasshoppers brought to nest by Robin, 39. 

Grosbeak, Pine (Pinicola enucleator, Linn.), range, habits, and relative tameness of, in winter and 
spring, 135, 136; capture of male of, with hat, 135. 

Gullet, distensibility of, in the Cedar-bird, 55, 61; effect of full, in young, ror; automatic response of, 
in young birds, 55, 101, 102. 


Index. 143 
H. 


Habit, definition of, as distinguished from abils in the popular sense, xvii; the formation of, 3, 4, 5, 
rrr, 116; of Cedar-bird in alighting on tent, 62; of sipping maple sap, in Cedar-bird, 62; in 
manner of approach to nest in Red-eyed Vireo, rri-112; of walking backward and sitting 
still in young Kingfishers explained, 89; of entering and leaving tunnel, in Kingfisher, 90, 91; 
of Kingfisher to prevent escape of prey, 92; of eating excreta, how acquired in a hungry bird, 
iog, rro; illustration of, in nest-cleaning, 112; change of food, in Rhinoceros-bird, 116; force 
and variation of, in nesting of Osprey, 115, 116; plasticity of, in reference to food, 116; of fear, 
of special objects, 121. 

Hawk, the Fish (see Osprey); Hen, the effect of, in sky upon chick in dooryard, 120. 

Hawks feeding on locusts, 116. 

Hellgamite, larvee of grampus, 103. 

Hinde, Captain, 116. 

Holderness (N. H.), spring arrival of Bluebirds at, 71; habits of Pine Grosbeaks in winter and spring 
at, 135. 

Humming-bird, eggs and young of, 117. 

Hunger in relation to fear, 126-128. 


Incubation, in Kingbird, 21; in Robin, 36; in Cedar-bird, 58; of Eagle in captivity, 134; behavior of 
birds during, 134. 

Insects, struggles of, when brought to nest, 102, 103. 

Inspection of young and nest, the importance, regularity, and significance of, roy—1o06. (See also under 
names of species.) 

Instincts, defined in broad and narrow sense, xvi: illustration of, in the Robin, xvi; substitution of, by 
habits, xvi, 4; the great number of, xvi; determining cause of, xvi; the parental, analysis of, 
3; culmination of, 4; strength of, how increased, 3, 4; suppression of, 3; periodic and serial 
nature of, 3; the fighting, 3, 4 (see Pugnacity); parental, relative strength of, in birds, 5, 13; 
in Orioles, 19; in Kingbirds, 22; in Redwing Blackbirds, 20, 21; in Robins, 39, 45; in Cedar- 
birds, 17, 18, 55, 57, 59; in Red-eyed Virco, 64, 65; in Bluebirds, 72-75; in Night Hawks, 80, 
82,85; in Kingfishers, 86, 90,91; use of parental, in taming birds, 130-133; instinctive reactions 
of young of Cedar-birds, 55; of fear, suppression of, in Red-eyed Vireos, 68; appear- 
ance of, in young Red-eyed Vireos, 68; of fear, suppression of, in Bluebirds, 72, 73; walk- 
ing, in vertebrates, 89; of inspection of young and nest, 103-106; cleaning or sanitary, in birds, 
to3-110; dearth of observations upon cleaning, 104; of cleaning nesting site, 107-110; prey- 
ing, in young Red-eyed Vireos, 67; of preening in young, 65; of Chimney Swift in nest-build- 
ing, 113; of fear in old and young, 117-124; of fear in domestic chick, r19; of Canada Goose 
modified in captivity, 129, 130; of ‘feigning’ in Chestnut-sided Warbler, 1 : 

Intelligence, human, the roots of, xv, xvi; the sign of, xvii. 


1, 132, 


ic 


Jay, Canada, Moosebird (Pertsoreus canadensis, Linn.), tameness and habits of, 129. 
Jefferson (O.), winter birds at, 128. 


Kk. 


Kearton, the brothers, blinds designed by, 30. 

Kingbird (Tyrannus tyrannus, Linn.), the time spent by young of, in nest after change of site, 13: the 
breeding of, 21; displacement of the nesting bough of, 22; fighting instinct of, 22; suppression 
of fear in, 22, 23; rate of feeding young of, 22, 27; foster children of, 2 crushing prey by, 

28; the rejection of indigestible parts of food by, 28; flight from nest of, 28; brooding in, 94, 

97; economy of food in, to2; escape of-prey from bill of, 102, 103; exciting scenes at nest of, 

103; feeding of large insects to young of, 103; eating excreta of young of, 105; attracted by 

alarms of other birds, 123; perching on fishing-rod, 125. 


144 Index. 


Kingfisher, the Belted (Ceryle alcyon, Linn.), general habits of, 86-93; attachment of, to nesting site, 
86; subterranean nest of, 86; dimensions of nest of, 86; young of, 86, 89, 91-93; use of tarsus 
in foot of, 89; habits of young of, 89, 91, 92; habit of walking backward, how acquired in 
young of, 89, 90; pugnacity in young of, 89; use of tent before tunnel of, 89, 90; photographing 
adult, 90; the feeding of young, by parents, go, 91; feeding of captive young, by hand, 92; 
moving nesting chamber by, 92; habits of young of, in captivity, 92; structure of cesophagus 
and bill of, 92; trick-like performance of, 92; notes of adult and young of, go, gt, 92; visits 
of, to nest, and manner of entering and leaving tunnel by, 90, 91; colors in young of, 86, 91, 92; 
peculiar expression in young of, 91; liberation of captive young of, 93; parental instincts in, 
86, 90, 91; character of excreta in young of, 107; sanitary condition of nest in, 107; develop- 
ment of fear in young of, 119. 


L. 


Leaves, plucking or cutting of, about a nest, 8,14; keeping, fresh on branches cut from trees of various 
kinds, 14, 15; result of cutting of, at Catbird’s nest, 79. 

Lenses, kinds of, available for photographing wild animals, 32-34; the Anastigmat, 32, 33; qualities 
of, most needed in animal photography, 33; long focus, 32, 34; telephoto, 32, 34. 

Locust, Rocky Mountain, eaten by birds during plague, 116. 

Loon, eggs and young of, 117. 

Lures, the young as, xvii, 6; as a means of taming without restraint, 126, 127, 130-133. 


M. 

Maple sap, sipping of, by Cedar-birds, 62. 

Methods of bird-photography, the old, xvii, xviii; the new, xviii, 1-16; analysis of new, 3-6; applica- 
tion of, 7, 8; precautions to be observed in use of, 8-11; extent of application of, 11-13; table 
of experiments in, 12; objections to, 13-15; advantages of, 15-16; fascination of, 16; illustra- 
tion of, 17-18; original suggestion of, 54,55. (See also table, page 12, and under names of 
species.) 

Mirrors, use of, 35. 

Mites, parasitic, on young of Cedar-bird, 107. 

Mount La Favette (N. H.), Pine Grosbeak in summer at, 135. 

Mouse, Deer- or White-footed (Hesperomys leucopus, Raj, LeC.), nest of Red-eyed Vireo occupied by, 
6 °. 

ae N. 

Narragansett Bay (R. I.), Fish Hawks on shores of, 115, 116. 

Naturalist, duty and privilege of, xv; patience of, 13, 14. 

Nest, inspection and sanitation of, 103-110; displacement of, 2 (see Nesting site); photographing, 
when inaccessible to tent, 34; of Cedar-bird, 17, 53, 54, 56, 58, 61; of Baltimore Oriole, 18; 
of Robin, 36, 40, 49, 50; of Red-eyed Vireo, 64, 69; of Bluebird, 72; of Catbird, 76, 77; of 
Night Hawk, 80; of Kingfisher, 86; of Chimney Swift, 113, 114; of House Sparrow, 114; of 
Osprey, 115, 116, 134, 135; Of Black Duck, 119; of Magnolia Warbler, 121; of Chestnut-sided 
Warbler, 131; of Phoebe, 133; of Red-eyed Vireo, occupied by Yellow Warbler, 69; of same, 
used by Deer-mouse, 69, 70; destruction of, in Red-eyed Vireo, 69; Bluebird in old Robin's, 
75; movement of nesting chamber in Kingfisher, 92; filthiness of, in Turkey Buzzard, 108; 
parasites of, in Cedar-bird, 107; sanitary condition of, in Kingfisher, 107; filthiness of, in 
Owls, 108; habit in approaching, in Robins, rrr; in Red-eyed Vireos, rri, 112; in Redwing 
Blackbird, 112; adaptation in character and position of, 113, 114; unusual position of, in 
Chimney Swift, 113, 114; in House Sparrow, 114; of Bay-winged Bunting despoiled by black 
snake, 123. 

Nesting bough, removal and mounting of, 7 (see Nesting site). 

Nesting site, control of, 1; *yhen to change, 6; experiments in change of, tabulated, 12; accidents due 
to change of, guarded against, 12, 13; change of, in Cedar-bird, 17, 54, 55, 57, 59; in Oriole, 
19; in Robin, 39, 40; in Red-eyed Vireo, 64; in Bluebird, 72; in Catbird, 76, 77; in Night 
Hawk, 80; in Kingfisher, 86, 89, 90; the choice of new, by operator, 7; attachment of birds 
to, 13; of Kingfisher to, 86; of Osprey to, 115; importance of cleanliness of, in passerine birds, 
105; unusual, in Chimney Swift, rr3, 114; in Osprey, 115; lack of cleanliness of,in birds, 107, 108, 


Index. 145 


OOS 
eggs 


Night Hawk, Bull-bat (Chordeiles virginianus, Gmel.), the feeding of, 15, 16; the rearing of, 80-85; 
and incubation of, 80; behavior of, during incubation, 80; hatching of, 80; the young of, 
80-85; expression of fear in old and young of, 80, 81; coralling the young of, 81; eyes and 
eyelids in young of, 80; brooding habits of, 80, 82, 85; call and alarm notes of, 81, 82; feeding 
habits in young of, 81, 82; illumination of throat of, by phosphorescent insects, 52; encounter 
of young of, with snake, 81; fledgling of, 85. 

Northfield (N. H.), breeding of Cedar-birds at, 52; spring Bluebirds at, 71; nesting of Kingfisher at, 86. 
(See Preface.) 

Nuthatches, tameness and habits of, 127, 128. 


Objections to method considered, 13-15. 

Observations from tent, the best time for, 7, 8. 

Oriole, Baltimore (/cterus galbula, Linn.), nest of, 18; call-notes of young of, 18; removal of nesting 
bough of, 19; behavior of, 19; food brought to young by, 19; rate of feeding at nest of, 19; 
exercise of the fledglings of, 20; flight of young of, from the nest, 20; use of tent at nest of, 19; 
cleaning instinct in, to4; lack of discrimination in young of, 121; summoned by alarm of Robin, 
22: 

Osprey, the American, or Fish Hawk (Pandion haliaétus carolinensts, Gmel.), nests and nesting habits 
of, at Plum Island (N. Y.), 115, 134, 135; at Bristol (R. [.), 115, 116; nest of, on cart-wheel 
on top of pole, 116; actions of, upon loss of mate, 116; tameness of, 134, 135. 

Owls, filthiness of nests of, 108; change of feeding habits in, 116. 


P. 


Parasites on nest and young of Cedar-bird, 107. 

Partridges, eggs and young of, 117. 

Patience required in the naturalist’s work, 13, T4. 

Patten, William, 114. 

Peep-holes in observation tent, the form and size of, 3r. 

Phoebe, taming of, 133, 134. 

Phosphorescence, display of, in Night Hawk, 82. 

Photography of birds, method of, xvii; the future of, xviii; a new method of, based on animal instinct, 
Xvill; new method described, 1-16; its conditions, 1; its principles analyzed, 3; mode of 
procedure in, 7; precautions in use of, 8; extent of application of, 11-13; objections to, con- 
sidered, 13-15; advantages of, 15-16; illustrations of, 17-28; the tools of, 29-35; of birds 
after they have been tamed, in Robin, 131, in Chestnut-sided Warbler, 133. 

Pigeon, English Wood, habit of, 137 ; domestic, tameability of, 125; wild passenger, condition of nest- 
ing site in, 107. 

Plates, photographic, exposure of, 34; deterioration of, 34; carriage and care of, 34. 

Plover, eggs and young of, 117. 

Plum Island (N. Y.), breeding and habits of Osprey at, 115, 134, 135. 

Polygamy in Bluebirds, 72. 

Popular natural history, defects of, xv, xvi; illustrations of, xviii, xix. 

Precoces, 117. 

Precautions to be observed in change of nesting site, 8. 

Precision in instinctive acts of young birds, 67. 

Preening instinct in young Vireos, 65. 

Principles of new method of bird-study, 3. 

Pugnacity, the instinct of, 4; in Kingbirds, 22; in Robins, 40, 134; in Red-eyed Vireos, 67; in young 
of Kingfisher, 89; in domestic fowl, 134; in Tropic Bird, 134. 


R. 


Reaction of young bird to stimulus of food, 55. 
Redstart, flight of young of, from nest, 118; attracted by Robin's alarm, 123. 


146 Index. 


Redwing (see Blackbird). 

Regurgitation, of indigested food in Kingbirds, 28; of food for young in Cedar-birds, 55, 61; in Vireo, 66. 

Reproduction, cycle of, 3, 4. 

Respiration in Redwing Blackbird, 21. 

Response, of throat and gullet of young, 55, 101, 102; in young of Baltimore Oriole to one of their num- 
ber, 121; of young Red-eyed Vireos to notes of other birds, 68; of young of Bluebirds, 74; of 
young of Catbirds, 77, 78. 

Rhinoceros-bird (Buphaga erythra pyncha), change in food habits of, 116. 

Robin (Merula migratoria, Linn.), the instincts of, displayed in migration and nest building, xvi; no 
learning of instinctive responses required or possible, xvi; time spent by young of, in new 
nesting site, 13; as symbol of cheerfulness, 36; history of, 36-51; spring arrival of, 48, 49; 
incubation in, 36; choice of nesting site in, 49; behavior of, when nesting bough is moved, 39, 
40, 45; call-notes of, 39, 45; feeding young in, 39, 46-48; economy in food in, 39; neatness 
of, 39; spontaneous behavior in, 39; parental instincts of, 39, 40, 45; panoramic scenes at 
nest of, 46, 47; flight from nest of, 40, 47, 48; keenness of vision of, 39; habits and instincts 
of fledglings of, 40, 47, 48; peculiar notes of, for arousing the young at nest, 45; in winter, 48; 
fruits eaten by, in summer and winter, 48; nests of, under cover, 49, 50; nest of second brood of, 
50; in city life, 50, 51; gregarious habits of, in summer and winter, 51; Bluebird in nest of, 
75; eating excreta of young by, 39, 105, 106, 109; characteristic attitudes of, 105; actions of, in 
cleaning the nest, 105, 106; formation of habits in, 111; food brought to nest of, 116; eggs 
and young of, 117; effect of alarm of female of, upon cock at nest, 122; as an exponent of 
taming process, 130-131; display of pugnacity in, 4o, 134. 

Roosts of Robin in summer and winter, 51. 


S. 


Sac of excreta in young, 104; disposition of, by parents, to4-110; character of, in Robin and inspec- 
tion of, after removal from nest, 105, 106; bursting of, in mouth of Robin, 109; seizing and 
devouring of, by Chestnut-sided Warbler, 109. 

Sanbornton (N. H.), Robin’s nest under cover at, 49. 

Sanitation of nest, 103-110; in Woodpeckers, Chickadees, Thrushes, Waxwings, Vireos, Warblers, 
Orioles, Blackbirds, Bluebirds, 104; in Crow Blackbird, 104, 105. (See also under names of 
species.) 

Shutter, concealment of observer while setting, 31; iris diaphragm, 34; focal plane, 34; time marks of, 
34; rapidity of, 34; a desideratum in, 5, 34. 

Sim, Robert J., 62, 114, 128. 

Snake, rescue of Vireo from, 69; encounter of Night Hawk with, 81; black, in act of swallowing young 
bird, 123. 

Snipe, eggs and young of, 117. 

Snow eaten by Chickadees, 128. 

Sounds, effect of, upon birds, 5, 68, 112. 

Sparrow, Chipping, suppression of fear in, 5; House (Passer domesticus, Linn.), pugnacity of, 72; com- 
bat of, with cicada, 103; condition of nesting site in, 107; nesting of, in hood of electric 
street-lamps, 114; nest of Eaves Swallow appropriated by, 114, 115; tameability of, 125, 127; 
Song (Melospiza fasciata, Gmel.), nest of, 12; attracted by alarm of Robin, 123; habits of, 
during incubation, 134. 

Spiders or their prey eaten by Cedar-birds, 63. 

Stork, habits of, 125. 
Swallow, Barn (Chelidon erythrogaster, Bodd.), accident to young of, 49; condition of nesting site in, 107; 
Eaves (Petrochelidon lunifrons, Say), dispossessed of nest by House Sparrow, 114, 115. 

Swift, Chimney (Chetura pelagica, Linn.), significance in change of nesting habits of, 113, 114; nesting 
instinct of, 113; nesting of, in barn and shed, 114. 


E. 


Tail of Bluebird used for support, 73-74. 
Tameness, of birds in nature, 125, 127, 128, 135-137: of the Pine Grosbeak, 135; of Bohemian Wax- 
wing, 136; of Crossbills, 136; of birds of Galapagos Islands, 136. 


roe) 


Index. 147 


Taming birds without a cage, 125-137. 

Taming process, conditions and analysis of, 126, 127; use of tent in, 130; Robins and Chestnut-sided 
Warblers as exponents of, 130-133; Phecbe as illustration of, 133, 134. 

Tarsus, use of, in Kingfisher, 80. 

Tent, as an observatory, 2, 15, 16; time required for birds to become accustomed to, 5, 11; window of, 
5, 31; the time to use, 7; precautions in use of, 8; experiments in use of, tabulated, 12; the 
future of, as an observatory for the study of birds, 13; protection afforded by, 15; advantages 
of position of, rs; before Cedar-bird’s nest, 17, 55, 57, 59; as an observatory for the birds, 12, 
21, 45, 58; before nest of Redwing Blackbird, 20; before nest of Oriole, 19; construction of, 
29: convenience of, 30; instructions for use of, 31; before nest of Red-eyed Vireo, 64; before 
nest-hole of Blucbird, 72; beside nest of Catbird, 76, 77; before young of Night Hawk, 81, 
before tunnel of Kingfisher, 89, 90; use of, in taming birds, 130; before nest of Chestnut-sided 
Warbler, 132, 133. 

Tent-cloth, material and color of, 29. 

Tent-frame, dimensions and construction of, 2 

Tent-pins, form and use of, 29, 31. 

Tent-window, position of, 31. 

Throat, response of, in young birds, 55; color of, in young Robin, 39; as target for the parent, 49: in 
young Cedar-bird, 56; inflation of, in Red-eyed Vireo, 66; in Chestnut-sided Warbler, 97. 

Thrush, Brown (Harporhynchus rufus, Linn.), camping beside nest of, 94; brooding of young in, 94; 
eating excreta of young by, ros; attracted by alarm of Robin, 122; Wilson's or Veery (7 urdus 
fuscescens, Steph.), young of, 12; premature development of fear in young of, r2t. 

Thrushes, cleaning instinct of, to4. 

Trees, keeping fresh leaves of cut branches of, 15; mutilation of, 15. 

Tripod, best form of, 34. 

Tropic Bird, pugnacity of, during incubation, 134. 


V. 


“ 


Venice, ‘‘doves”’ or pigeons of, 125. 

Vireo, Red-eyed (Vireo olivaceus, Linn.), coming to tent, 5; nest and young of, 64; call-notes of, 65: be- 
havior of nestlings of, 64, 65, 67, 68; digestion and assimilation in young of, 66; feeding the 
young in, 65-68; inspection and cleaning the young in, 65, 67, 68; sleekness and neatness of, 
67; preying instinct in young of, 67; young of, aroused by notes of other birds, 68; capture 
of prey by, 68; indifference of, to customary sounds, 68; signs of emotion in, 68; suppression 
of fear in, 68; appearance of sense of fear in voung of, 69; rate of feeding at nest of, 69; old 
nest of, utilized by Yellow Warblers, 69; old nest of, used by deer-mouse, 69, 70; flight from 
nest of, 69; rescue of young of, from snake, 69; destruction of nest of, 69; fragility of old 
nests of, 69; carelessness in construction of nest in, 69; eating of excreta of young by, 105; 
cleanliness of nesting site in, 198; habit of approaching the nest in, 111, 112; attracted by 
alarm of Robin, 122, 123. 


W. 


Walking, instinct of, in vertebrates, 89; habit of, in young of Kingfisher, 89. 

Warbler, Yellow (Dendroica estiva, Ginel.), using nest of Red-eyed Vireo, 69; Chestnut-sided (Den- 
drotca pennsylvanica, Linn.), excreta of young of, eaten by, 105, 109; nesting habits of, 131-133; 
taming of, 132-133; photographing, without tent, 133; attracted by alarm of Robin, 122; 
development of fear in young of, 118; Magnolia (Dendroica niaculosa, Ginel.), as foster parent 
to Cowbird, 121, 122; fate of rightful young of, 122; Maryland Yellow Throat, attracted by 
alarm of Robin, 123. 

Waxwing, origin of name of, 52; Bohemian, habits and record of, 136 (see Cedar-bird). 

Wildness, of birds, origin of, 125, 126, 137. 

Woodpecker, use of old nest-hole of, by Bluebird, 72; cleanliness of nest in, 104; Downy and Hairy, 
tameness of, in winter, 128, 129; eggs and young of, 117; habits of, during incubation, 134. 


148 Index. 


Ye 


Young, as strong lure, xvii, 6, 126, 127, 130; exposure of, to intense heat, 8; study of, at nest, 8; danger 
to, from insufficient food, 10; proper age of, when nesting site is changed, 6; necessity of shade 
to, 8, 13; of Cedar-bird, 18, 60; of Baltimore Oriole, 18,19; frequency of feeding, in Orioles, 
19; call-notes of, in Oriole, 18; exercise of, in Oriole, 20; flight from nest of, in Oriole, 20; 
feeding of, in Redwing Blackbird, 21; hatching of, in Kingbird, 21; change of, in nest of King- 
bird, 27; feeding, in Kingbird, 27, 28, 103; brooding in Kingbird, 28, 94, 97; flight of, from 
nest in Kingbird, 28; gape, color of mouth, and behavior of, in Robin, 39, 40, 47; flight of, 
from nest, in Robin, 40, 47, 48; cats as enemies of, 51; instinctive reaction of, to food in Cedar- 
bird, 55; hatching of, in Cedar-bird, 58, 59; opening of eyes of, in Cedar-bird, 59; of Cedar- 
bird leaving the nest, 18, 60; time spent in nest by, in Cedar-bird, 60; appearance of feather- 
shafts and wax-like appendages to wings of, in Cedar-bird, 60; development of color-marks of, 
in Cedar-bird, 60, 61; habits of, in Cedar-bird, 60; food and care of, in Cedar-bird, 55-62; cleaning 
of, in Cedar-bird, 56, ro5—107; digestion, assimilation, and growth of, in Red-eyed Virco, 66; 
behavior of, in Red-eyed Vireo, 64-68; preying instinct of, in Red-eyed Vireos, 67; aroused 
by notes of other birds, 68; appearance of fear in Red-eyed Vireos, 69; rate of feeding of, 
in Red-eyed Vireos, 69; response and feeding of, in Bluebird, 73; food and rate of feeding, in 
Bluebird, 75; feeding and care of, in Catbird, 77, 78, 79; rate of feeding of, in Catbirds, 78; 
behavior of, in Catbirds, 78, 79; of Night Hawk, 80; hatching of, in Night Hawk, 80; color of, 
in Night Hawk, 80, 85; eyes of, in Night Hawk, 80; behavior of,in Night Hawk, 80-82; walking 
of, in Night Hawk, 81; coralling of, in Night Hawk, 81; call-notes of, in Night Hawk, 81, 82; 
feeding habits in Night Hawk, 81, 82, 85; fledgling stage in Night Hawk, 85; call-notes of, in 
Kingfisher, 90, 91, 92; colors of, in Kingfisher, 86, 91, 92; peculiar expression of, in Kingfisher, 
91; development of feathers of, in Kingfisher, 86, 91; function of tarsus of, in foot of King- 
fisher, 89; general habits of, in Kingfisher, 89,91, 92; habit of walking backward in, of King- 
fisher, how acquired, 89; habit of sitting still, in Kingfisher, 89, 90; pugnacity of, in Kingfisher, 
89; feeding of, in Kingfisher, 90, 91; habits of, in captive Kingfishers, 92; care of, 94; brooding 
and feeding of, 94-103; diet of, in Cedar-bird, 101; automatic response of gullet of, ror, 102; 
inspection and cleaning of, 103-110; character of excreta in, 104; disposal of excreta of, by 
parents, 104-107; character of excreta in Kingfisher, 107; use of excreta of, as food by adults, 
105, 107, 109; development of fear of, in Catbird, 117, 118, in Chestnut-sided Warbler, 118, 
in Kingfisher, 119; imperfect digestion of food in, 109; fear in, 117-122; condition of, at time 
of hatching, as basis for classification, 117; fear in Black Ducklings, 120; death of, due to 
premature development of fear, 120, 121; lack of discrimination in, 121; behavior of, in Bal- 
timore Orioles, 121; care and education of, 121; acquisition of fear of special objects in, 121; 
use of pot-belly of, 121; of Bay-winged Bunting attacked by black snake, 123. 


Hits