Skip to main content

Full text of "Further advances in physiology"

See other formats


>" - 


yen, 


“ , ’ ~ : 
+ Colette) 6 jee eX 


_ FURTHER ADVANCES IN PHYSIOLOGY 


UNIFORM WITH THIS VOLUME 


SECOND IMPRESSION 


RECENT ADVANCES IN 
PHYSIOLOGY & BIO-CHEMISTRY 


EDITED BY 
LEONARD HILL, M.B., F.R.S. 


CONTRIBUTORS 


BENJAMIN Moore, M.A., D.Sc. 
J. J. R. MACLEOD, M.B. 
LEONARD HILL, M.B., F.R.S. 
M. S. PEMBREY, M.A., M.D. 
A. P. BEDDARD, M.A., M.D. 


xii+744 pages. Demy &vo, 18s. net 


LONDON: EDWARD ARNOLD 


4 7 4 
—_— - 


URTHER ADVANCES IN 
-- PHYSIOLOGY 


‘ = 
- wo bis 


ia EDITED BY 
4 LEONARD HILL, M.B., F.R.S. 


j CONTRIBUTORS 


_ BENJAMIN MOORE, M.A., D.Sc., Johnston Professor of Bio- 
Chemistry in the University of Liverpool. 


MARTIN FLACK, M.A., B.M., Demonstrator of Physiology, London 
Hospital. 

THOMAS LEWIS, M.D., D.Sc., Fellow of University College. 
_ LEONARD HILL, M.B., F.R.S,, Lecturer on Physiology, London 
Hospital. 

ARTHUR KEITH, M.D., Curator of Museum, Royal College of 
___.. Surgeons, England. 
_ M. S. PEMBREY, M.A., M.D., Lecturer on Physiology, Guy’s 
a Hospital. 
_ N. H. ALCOCK, M.D., Lecturer on Physiology, St. Mary’s Hospital. 
_ JOSEPH SHAW BOLTON, M.D., M.R.C.P., Fellow of University 


College, London, Senior Assistant Medical Officer, Lancaster 
_ County Asylum. ’ 


M. GREENWOOD, Jun., M.R.C.S., Senior Demonstrator of 
Physiology and Director of Statistical Department, London 


Hospital. 


- 
‘ 


WITH DIAGRAMS 


LONDON 
EDWARD ARNOLD 
1909 


(All rights reserved) 


iy a 


‘ ; ‘ 
7 * 


Digitized by the Internet Archive 
in 2007 with funding from 


Microsoft Corporation 


ee Lee eee 
. 


PREFACE 


THE volume now put before the reader by the editor is a sequel to 
* Recent Advances in Physiology,” and deals with certain branches 
of the science other than those dealt with in that volume, the 
treatment of the subject-matter being, in both, on the same general 
lines. The aim of the editor and his coadjutors has been to write 


up their views on certain selected subjects which, both by their 


importance and interest, will stimulate the student, give him a 
view wider than that which the ordinary text-book can give him, 
and at the same time rivet his attention on subjects which have a 
particular application to Pathology and Clinical Medicine. While 
the former volume dealt mainly with problems of metabolism, 
secretion, and excretion, this is devoted to the consideration of 
certain problems concerning the circulation and respiration, the 
neuro-muscular system, and vision. Prof. B. Moore, writing on the 
relation of the heart-beat to its nutritive fluid, has developed this 
subject into a general consideration of the equilibrium of colloid 
and crystalloid in living cells. “Mr. Martin Flack has discussed the 
present position of the myogenic and neurogenic theories of the 
heart-beat, and analysed the recent researches which have modified 
the lines of thought on this subject. Dr. Thomas Lewis has given 
the reader an account of the venous pulse, and those methods of 
investigating the cardiac cycle in man which have done so much, 
in the hands of Dr. James Mackenzie and others, to elucidate the 


different forms of heart trouble. The editor has dealt, firstly, with 


the wonderful advances in experimental method made possible by 
Carrel’s surgical union of the blood-vessels ; secondly, with blood ~ 


_ pressure and its measurement in man. He has endeavoured to 
_ refute the supposed importance of blood pressure as a mechanical 
factor in the formation of lymph, production of dropsy, excretion of 


1 
L 
, 


ar 


urine, &c. Dr. Arthur Keith has contributed an account of his 
researches into the mechanisms of breathing, and thereby has upheld 


the importance of anatomy treated as a study of function. Dr. — 


Pembrey has given an account of the subject he knows so well— 
the Physiology of Muscular Work—and has incorporated therein the 
new results which have been obtained, particularly in this country, 
by the study of marching soldiers. The present views held con- 
cerning the growth, regeneration, union of nerves, and the nature of 


the nerve impulse, have been considered by Dr. N. Alcock; while ~ 


Dr. J. S. Bolton has contributed an account of the recent researches 
of himself and others on cortical localisation and the functions of 


the cerebrum, including the revolutionary views which have been - 


put forward concerning Broca’s localisation of Aphasia. Lastly, Mr. 
Major Greenwood has dealt with two especially interesting subjects 
of sense physiology—Visual Adaptation and Colour Vision. Each 
writer is responsible for the views he has set forth and the treatment 
of his subject, and the editor has done no more than select his 
-coadjutors, write his own part, and arrange the book for the press. 
He hopes it may meet with as cordial a reception as the first 
volume, serve a useful part, and be perhaps the forerunner of 
still other volumes, of “more” and “ most recent,” and even 
“ furthest’ Advances in Physiology. 


OsBoRNE House, LOUGHTON, 
March 7, 1909. 


CONTENTS 


7? 
+ By BENJAMIN MOORE 
THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING 


PAGE 


By MARTIN FLACK 
a, rer, * | 


By THOMAS LEWIS 


PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO THE EVENTS OF THE 
HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE . : : : : : : - 42 


By LEONARD HILL 
THE VASCULAR SYSTEM AND BLOOD PRESSURE . . « . 112 


| By ARTHUR KEITH 
THE MECHANISM OF RESPIRATION INMAN . .  ..—..:sOXAR2 


| By M,°8. PEMBREY 
THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK . . .  . «208 


By N, H, ALCOCK 
ME CHAPTERS ON THE PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE . « « £83 


" By JOSEPH SHAW BOLTON 


RECENT RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND ON 
THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM. . . .-. . 284 


By M, GREENWOOD, Jon. os a 
N SPECIAL SENSE puvsiology  . . . . 2 & 


FURTHER ADVANCES IN PHYSIOLOGY 


THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND 
CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 


By BENJAMIN MOORE 


THE living cell may be regarded from the physico-chemical point 
of view as a machine or mechanism through which there constantly 
: is taking place a flux of energy. The cell is continually taking 
energy up from its surroundings in certain forms, and redistribut- 
{ ing this energy in other forms, but in the process it itself under- 
' goes little or no permanent change. Certain changes, it is true, 


do occur slowly in the cell in the course of its life-history which 
have the effect of permanently altering the character of the energy 
discharged through it; but these structural changes are so slow that 
they can be put aside in the study of the cell as an energy machine 
acting upon the energy supply at any given moment. 

If the case of the green plant cell acting as an energy trans- 
former for light energy be placed on one side, it may be stated that 
the energy supplies of the cell always come to it in the form of 
organic compounds capable of yielding energy in the process of 
oxidation in the cell. 

In order that the cell may be capable of oxidising these 
chemical compounds of organic character coming in from its 
environment, it is, however, absolutely essential that its own in- 
tegrity be preserved ; and this integrity is just as completely de- 
pendent upon the presence of the ions of certain simple inorganic 
salts in the cell and its surrounding fluid media, as the exhibition — 

of the typical phenomena of cell activity is upon the supply of 

energy in the form of the organic compounds to the living cell. 

In fact, in point of time, the physiological activity of the cell is 

rapidly destroyed by removing or altering the supply of 
: A 


4 4 


2 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND i 


inorganic ions, than it is by interfering with the supply of organic 
foodstuffs. For, in the latter case, the cell can oxidise the com- 
bustible materials present in storage within it, and even use up a 
portion of its own intrinsic substance before its activities come to 

a standstill ; but when the inorganic ions, forming a constitutional 

part of the living cell, are altered, and the equilibrium between ‘ 
protoplasm and ion thus destroyed, the cell activities are imme- 
diately affected, and after a short period of pathological activity 
everything comes to rest. 

An analogy with another form of energy transformer may 
make this clearer. If the fires are banked under the boiler of.a 
steam-engine the head of steam in the boiler will for a longer or 
shorter period keep the steam-engine going, this is comparable to 
stopping the organic food supply of the cell; but if there is a 
sudden burst in the boiler, if the cylinder blows off, or if there is 
a break in any essential part of the machinery, then there is a - 
sudden stoppage of the engine, often preceded by a very brief 
period of excessive activity: this is comparable to interference 
with the inorganic ions of the cell. The inorganic ions form in fact 
an intrinsic and indispensable part of the cell’s structure, in the 
absence of which it can no longer utilise its food supply, however 
abundant that supply may be, or suitably adapted for the nutrition 
of the normal cell. 

These effects, although they may be demonstrated in any living 
tissue, are seen perhaps most typically in the case of the isolated 
and perfused heart muscle. If the fluid caused to flow through 
the heart consists of distilled water, to which organic foodstuff, 
in the form say of dextrose, has been added, the heart beat ceases 
almost instantaneously. If next the experiment be repeated upon 
a fresh heart, using instead a solution of pure sodium chloride 
in distilled water, of such concentration that it is isosmotic with 
the natural serum of the animal, there ensues a considerably longer 
period of perhaps some minutes’ duration before the heart beat 
disappears. The sodium chloride solution, however, although 
possessing the proper osmotic pressure, is unable for any con- 
siderable time to preserve the heart muscle cells in normal con- 
dition. By supplying the proper osmotic concentration it has 
prevented the cells being suddenly broken up, but it has a zero © 
pressure for certain ions indispensable to the heart’s activity 5 . 
thelis : have slowly diffused out, and the period of the heart’s action 


 GRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 3 


has been determined by that moment at which the concentration 
in the protoplasm has reached a certain minimal value. 

: The most important of the ions which have been washed out 
of the cardiac muscle cells by the current of pure sodium chloride 
solution are the potassium and calcium ions. It still possesses 
abundance of combustible organic material to furnish the energy 
for its contractions, but the structural mechanism or machine for 
oxidation has been interfered with and the cells can no longer 
draw on their supply of stored energy. 

That this is the true state of the case is shown by the effects 
of adding quite minimal traces of soluble salts of calcium and 
potassium to the pure sodium chloride solution, when the spon- 
taneous beating of the heart commences and goes on for hours, 
_ and even days, in a regular and automatic manner. 

The amounts of potassium and calcium salts necessary to 
bring out this profound difference in behaviour of the heart muscle 
are strikingly small, the optimum amount of potassium chloride 
required being only about 1 part in 10,000. More than an 
exceedingly minute trace must not be added or the heart will 
be stopped by the excess. There is only a certain range of con- 
centration which must not be passed in either direction or the 
heart will not beat normally. The meaning of this range will be 
pointed out later. 

These important experimental results were first demonstrated 
by Sidney Ringer in the case of cardiac muscle of the frog heart ; 
the complete generality of their application in all cells and tissues, 
and the causes underlying them, are only in recent times becoming 
generally appreciated, but the delicate and lightly balanced labile 
equilibrium between the colloids of the cell protoplasm and the 
osmotic pressures or concentrations of the inorganic ions and 

_ other crystalloid constituents is perhaps the most important and 
fundamental fact in the whole of biology. 

____The inorganic ions are sufficient in the case of the more slowly 

oxidising cardiac muscle of the heart of the cold-blooded animal 
_ to maintain for lengthened periods an automatic rhythmic beat ; 
_ the sufficient amount of oxygen for the oxidation being capable. 
_ of being carried at the partial pressure of one-fifth of an atmos- 
_ phere that the atmospheric oxygen possesses, and the combustible 
| _ organic material coming from the store in the cardiac muscle cells. 
But in the case of the mammalian heart, the oxygen pressure 


4 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND . 


must be increased to nearly a whole atmosphere of pure oxygen 
by bubbling oxygen gas through the Ringer’s saline heated to 
mammalian body temperature in a flask attached to the perfusion 
cannula, before a normal heart beat can be obtained. This in- 
creased oxygen pressure supplies the place of the red blood cor- 


puscles which in the body are able to carry a sufficiency of oxygen ~ 


at the lower pressure of oxygen present in the lungs. Also, in 
the case of the mammalian heart for a prolonged experiment, it 
is well, as recommended by Locke, to add dextrose to the Ringer’s 
solution to prevent exhaustion of organic combustible material 
which occurs earlier in the case of mammalian muscle on account 
of the greater expenditure of energy which here occurs at each 
heart beat. 

Before leaving the classical example of cardiac muscle for 
more general considerations, the similar action of anesthetics may 
be mentioned as an example of the relationship of drugs to the 
cell-protoplasm. 

The conditions which govern the action of chloroform upon 
the isolated mammalian heart have been beautifully demonstrated 
by Sherrington and Sowton. These observers have shown that 
at a fairly definite concentration of chloroform in the circulating 
saline the heart beat becomes affected, and if this concentration 
be passed the beat is stopped. If now the chloroform be cut off 
and pure Ringer’s solution be perfused instead of it, after a short 
time sufficient to reduce the osmotic pressure of the chloroform 
in the cardiac cells below a definite limit, the heart recommences 
its beating, and soon becomes normal once more. The conditions 
of action here are obviously the same as in the case of the in- 
organic ions above mentioned, except that the result is reversed, 
and whereas in the case of the inorganic ions a certain pressure 
or concentration of ions was essential in order to keep the heart 
functionating normally, here a certain pressure of anesthetic is 
required to still its activities. As soon as the osmotic pressure of 
the anesthetic passes below a certain limit, the cells cease to be 
anesthetised. In other words, some grouping in the protoplasm 
is free from the anesthetising influence and open to continue other 
chemical interchanges which give rise to its activity. Within 
certain well-marked limits there is a certain reduced activity or 
anesthesia. On one side of this is free activity or absence of 
anesthesia ; on the other side there is complete anchoring of proto- 


CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 5 


plasm by anesthetic or complete bondage from oxidising activity, 
resulting finally in death of the cell. 

The action of chloroform upon nerve cells in the production of 
surgical anesthesia is shown, by the known physiological effects 
connected with induction of and recovery from anesthesia, to be 

similar in nature, and hence in producing safe anesthesia we stand 
; upon that bridge or interval of partial combination between proto- 
| plasm and anesthetic, where there is just sufficient combination 
between the two to produce the stilling of activity which gives 
the absence of pain, but not sufficient to cause complete stilling 
of activity nor that degree of combination which cannot become 
reversible and dissociate off when the pressure of anesthetic is 
| lowered by discontinuing the administration and allowing the 

- process of respiration to lower the pressure of anesthetic in the 

nerve cells. 

There is fortunately here, as in the case of all drugs, a degree 
of selective absorption by different types of cells, and the cells of 
the higher nerve centres are affected before other lower centres, 
and these again before cardiac and other forms of muscle cells. 
It is on this selective effect that all the benefits of anesthesia as 
an accessory of surgery depend, for if the heart, for example, were 
affected at the same level of concentration of the anesthetic as the 
higher nerve cells, anesthesia would become impossible. Precisely 
at the same moment as anesthesia set in the heart would stop 
beating. 

In general terms it may be stated that the actions of all specific 
drugs depend upon this delicate selective action between the cells 
of different tissues, or parasitic cells, and the drugs. The problem 
of practical therapeutics is to find a drug or chemical combination 
which by its peculiar chemical conformation is capable of under- 
going adsorption at a lower pressure by a specific type of cell 

protoplasm. This subject will be treated more in detail later on 
when we have considered the general conditions governing the 
relationships of the protoplasm to crystalloids, and to the other 
substances with which it is brought in contact in the cell either 
naturally, or as the result of disease, or in the treatment of disease. 
We may now turn to a consideration of the general chemical 

nature of protoplasm in so far as this bears upon its power of 

adsorbing or combining with inorganic ions or other substances 
_which may be present in common with it in the living cell. 


6 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND 


The most essential and as it were the central constituents in 
building up the excessively complex physico-chemical. aggregation 
which we term protoplasm or bioplasm, are the protein bodies. 
By means of the proteins, the fats and carbohydrates are knitted 


together with the variously constituted proteins themselves and. 


with the ions of the inorganic salts to form a united system. The 
component parts of this system are only lightly held together, 


each is held in by the pressure of a free portion of it in the cell . 


fluid, and for the life and activity of the cell it is essential that 
the osmotic pressure of each constituent should lie within a certain 
range, so that it neither becomes fixed quite permanently nor so 


completely liberated as to be absent from the cell when it is required ‘ 


for the chemical transformations which yield the supply of energy 
to the cell. During the molecular vibrations which accompany 
this labile equilibrium in which the intensities of attachment of the 
various constituents to the bioplasm are all the time varying, the 
organic oxidisable substances, viz. the proteins themselves, the 
fats and carbohydrates, suffer temporary molecular disruptions 
during which the oxygen also held in the bioplasm comes into 
union with them, and the oxidised products as they increase in 
pressure are shed off from the cell. 

As has been stated above, the inorganic ions exercise the 
function in the cell of favouring these chemical disruptions, for 
when by lowering their osmotic pressure in the cell fluid they are 
dissociated off from the bioplasm, the oxidation processes which 
form the chemical basis for the cell’s activities also come to 
an end. 

Although’ it is impossible at the present time fo lartificially 
synthesise any of the proteins of the cells or body fiuids, and still 
less to build these up synthetically with the other constituents 
mentioned above into bioplasm, or living matter, yet we have even 


now obtained much insight as to the general character of the con- . 


stitution of proteins, and the main plan upon which details are 
still to be worked out lies before us. 

This knowledge has been arrived at by two different channels 
of approach, viz. that of studying the cleavage products of proteins 
in which Schiitzenberg was the great pioneer, followed by a host 
of others; and that of building together such cleavage products 
_ into bodies closely resembling in many respects the naturally 
occurring proteins, in which Emil Fischer is now ‘aging the way, 


CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 7 


and, by the synthesis of the polypeptides, has already shown the 
lines on which proteins must be built together. 

Under the influence of hydrolytic agents, such as heating with 
either alkalies or acids under pressure, the proteins take up the 
elements of water and yield a large number of simpler organic 
substances ; and conversely by the action of dehydrating or con- 
densing agencies these simpler organic substances or organic 
radicles of the proteins can again be made to unite. In the latter 
direction the process cannot be carried back quite to that degree 
of complexity which yields the naturally occurring protein, mainly 


constituents is so delicately balanced that the chemical manipula- 
tions cause splitting off and decomposition. 

. The synthesised products are in fact hepa to possess that 
delicately balanced power of associating and dissociating which is 
characteristic in still higher degree of living matter, and it is for 
this reason that only the living cell has hitherto been able to put 
the finishing touches upon the delicate unions which finally yield 
proteins, and beyond these up to living protoplasm, where the 
complexity and corresponding instability reach their acme. 

The organic radicles, which form the building stones, so to 
speak, for the structure of the protein molecules, may be divided 
into three classes, viz. those which are purely organic bases, those 

- which are entirely organic acids, and a third and most characteristic 
class which possess both the properties of organic acids and organic 
bases in modified degree. The compounds of this third class are 
known as the amido-acids, and it is to them that the proteins owe 
their peculiar property of building up into such complex bodies 
of high molecular weights.'_ The simplest type of amido-acid con- 
tains one organic acid radicle and one basic radicle, the acid 
character being given by the carboxyl group (COOH) and the 

d basic character by the amidogen group (NH,). As the simplest 

example, glycocoll or glycene, which is the amido-acid of acetic 

acid, may be quoted. Acetic acid is CH,.COOH, and is purely 
acid in its properties, combining with bases such for example as 


ammonia, instead of neutralising the carboxyl group, becomes 
_ attached, with the loss of one atom of hydrogen, as the group 


1 The terms amido-acid and amino-acid have the same meaning, and are used 
indiscriminately in describing members of this class of compounds. 


because at that level the degree of chemical association of the: 


ammonium to form ammonium acetate (CH,.COO.NH,). If the 


e 


8 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND 


amidogen (NH,) in the methyl group (CH,), there is formed instead 
the body CH,(NH,).COOH which is the amido-acid. The carboxyl 
group (COOH) being still free, the amido-acid retains acid properties, 
but in lessened degree, on account of the presence in the molecule 
of the basic group (NH,). The presence of the basic group also at 
the same time confers the properties of a base, so that the amido- 
acid now has the peculiar property of being able to functionate 
either as acid or base. Thus with copper it forms a deep blue 
soluble compound called copper glycocoll, and when in union with 
other organic acids it forms well-known and important substances 
found in the body, for example, the compound with benzoic acid 
known as hippuric acid, and the compound with cholalic acid 
occurring in the bile as glycocholic acid. 

Amido-acids possessing only one amidogen group are termed 
mon-amino-acids ; others exist possessing two such basic groups 
in their molecule, and these are called di-amino-acids. A number 
of both classes occur amongst the products of hydrolytic cleavage 
of the proteins. Again, there is in the majority of cases only one 
acidic or carboxyl group, but there are sometimes two or more 
such acid groups, and then the amido-acids are referred to as 
mono- and di-basic, &c., as in the case of ordinary organic acids. 

The most striking chemical characteristic of all these amido- 
acids, and that which from the point of view of protein formation 
interests us most at present, is that of undergoing conjugation or 
condensation with one another or with other organic bodies to 
form long chains in single series, or it may be main chains with 
side or branch chains arising from them. 

In each union of this kind the elements of a molecule of water 
are eliminated, a hydrogen atom being yielded by one of the two 
combining molecules and a hydroxyl radicle by the other, and in 
the great majority of instances the union occurs between the 
amidogen group of one and the carboxyl group of the other. 

For the reader who is not acquainted with the technical terms 
of organic chemistry, the nature of the process of combination to 
form protein, and further of proteins to form bioplasm, may be 
illustrated by the use of electrical terms. The amido-acid, on 
account of its possessing both an acidic and a basic group, may 
be considered as possessing a sort of polarity (indeed it does possess 
a kind of chemical polarity) ; as a result of this polarity a chemical 
' attraction exists between acidic pole and basic pole of different 


CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 9 


molecules, so that these tend to unite with the elimination of the 
elements of water above mentioned. Now it is clear, since each 
combining amido-acid had two free poles, that after this union has 
occurred there will still be left, in the new combine of double the 
molecular size, two opposite poles free ; and if this larger new mole- 
cule is brought, under suitable conditions, in chemical contact with 
more molecules, that further additions of like nature can occur. 

If it be remembered that a certain number of the amido-acids 
possess more than one basic group, or more than one acidic group, 
it is further obvious that it is not necessary for this process of 
growing to extend out in a single chain; but that branching may 
occur, and union of branches, so that a ramification or network can 
be formed in all three dimensions of space. ‘ 

There is no limit but the stability of the whole chemical system 
to this growth proceeding until a point is reached at which, with 
the particular chemical agencies for union and condensation at 
‘hand, there is an equilibrium between the forces building up or 
synthesising and the forces tending to disrupt. 

In the same way by protein unions the substances of the pro- 
toplasm or bioplasm can be formed, until new limiting conditions 
again fix a maximum, and, it may be added, though the agencies 
at work may differ in type, similarly the bioplasm can increase in 
aggregation until a maximum cell volume has been feached for 
a particular cell, and cell division becomes essential for further 
multiplication. 

In this process of growth it will be observed that there must 
be left at the end of the process a number of poles of opposite 
type. These poles, although they are chemically saturated (for as 
pointed out above the elimination of the elements of water are 
required at each union), must still possess what has been termed 
residual affinity,’ and have sufficient power to attract a group of 
opposite polarity and hold it very loosely attached. 


? This residual chemical affinity is seen when compounds, saturated as regards 
ordinary chemical values, combine with one another, such as neutral salts with 
their molecules of water of crystallisation, The energy of such residual combina- 
tions is seen when dehydrated salts-are dissolved in water, for this process always - 
causes heat development although the crystallised salts after the residual combina- 
tion is once completed always cause cooling when dissolved on account of energy 
going latent as osmotic energy from development of pressure! in the given volume 
of water. Similar heat effects are seen in dissolving alcohol in water, and in the 


} Pressure signifies in this artiele osmotic pressure. 


*fa/s ‘a4 
; -. 
a 7 a 


Rae 


10 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND 


When this feeble attachment has once occurred it may become 
altered.in different ways. First, if the growing protein aggregation | 
has not yet reached its full size, there may be a swing into true 
chemical union with the elimination of a water molecule. 

If chemical union does not take place, a diminution of pressure 
of one of the constituents may occur, causing dissociation or dis- 
ruption, or conversely an increased pressure may lead to firmer 
attachment, increasing association at the expense of dissociation, 
and favouring chemical combination. 

The form of union described above as “ feeble union ” or union 
by “residual affinities ” is usually spoken of as adsorption, although 
often a number of processes which may be dissimilar in nature are 
placed together under this term. 

Thus, the invisible layer of moisture that collects on the surface 
of glass ; the adhesion of gases on the inner surface of glass vessels 
which are in process of exhaustion; the moisture taken up by 
textile fabrics; the gases occluded by certain metals such as 
platinum, palladium, and iron; the concentration of dyes upon 
the surfaces of fibres and tissues being dyed ; the union or adhesion 
between inorganic salts or other crystalloids and colloids of various 
kinds—these and a great many other phenomena are variously 
given as examples of adsorption, and it is maintained that this 
process is physical in character and essentially different from 
chemical. combination. 

If extreme cases of adsorption on the one hand and of chemical 
combination on the other be taken for comparison, it becomes 
obvious at once that there exist great differences between them. 
Such, for example, as the hygroscopic absorption of water com- 
pared with the combination of hydrogen and oxygen to form 
water. In the former case the glass remains unaltered, and by 
heating or by drying agencies the water molecules can be removed 
unaltered from the surface ; while in the latter case, the water is 
quite different in all its physical and chemical properties from 


solution of free acids and caustic alkalies in water. Other examples are the union 

of anesthetics such as chloroform with proteins, where the chloroform or other 

anesthetic is in all cases a chemically saturated body, yet the proofs of union 

with protein are indisputable, there being finally obtained with sufficient pressure 

of chloroform actual precipitation, the precipitate containing chloroform in high 

percentage. c 
Other examples are the dyeing of tissues and fabrics by dyes, where a saturated 

dye combines with a saturated colloid substratum. In all such cases the best effects 

are obtained when the chemical sign of dye and substratum are opposite. — 


> 


_ 


, 
| 


CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 11 


either of the two gases which have, united with great evolution of 
energy to form it. 

But if the comparison be made between chemical reactions 
and adsorptions which lie closer together, it is found that the 
characteristic differences become diminished in degree, there are 
all possible gradations, and many instances in which it is impossible 
definitely to say whether the union which takes place ought to be 
described as an adsorption or a chemical combination. 

If the various criteria of a physical or chemical nature which 
are usually taken as decisive of whether any union is a chemical 
combination or an adsorption be examined critically, it is found 
that they one after another break down. 

To take an example of interest to the biologist, it was taught 
for many years in all the physiological text-books that hamo- 
globin formed with oxygen an easily dissociable compound. The 
chemical combination was said to be complete at a certain pressure 
of oxygen, and as the oxygen pressure fell, this compound, called 
oxy-hemoglobin, dissociated off into oxygen and hemoglobin or 
‘reduced hemoglobin,” the dissociation occurring over a definite 
range of pressures. So certainly established were the facts regard- 
ing this that the proofs of the formation of the compound formed 
a stock question of the examination room. One of the strongest 
proofs of this formation of the compound oxy-hemoglobin was 
supposed to be that the amount of oxygen absorbed by hemoglobin 
was not directly proportional to the partial pressure of the oxygen, 
absorption occurring in relatively greater amount at the lower 
pressures and falling off rapidly to nearly a zero increment as the 
dissociation range was passed. So that when oxygen pressures 
were graphically plotted as abscissee and amounts absorbed as ordi- 


-nates, instead of a straight line, as, say, in the case of absorption 


of oxygen (or other inert gas) by water, a curve was obtained. 

But recent research has shown that in many. cases where the 
phenomena ought to be classed under the head of adsorption, the 
plotted curve of pressure (or ‘concentration) and of amount ab- 
sorbed is not a straight line but a curve, and hence the new criterion 
is not a simple linear relationship between concentration and 
amount absorbed, but that the plotted curve shall show kinks or 
breaks upon it, that is to say, regions at which there is a sudden 
change i in the equation of the curve. Now the oxygen-hemoglobin 


curve is a smooth curve, and for this reason it has apoenaly. been 


12 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND 


maintained by Wolfgang Ostwald that in the case of oxygen and 
hemoglobin the phenomena is one of adsorption. Yet in the case 
of oxygen and hemoglobin there exists at the point where absorp- 
tion is complete an exact stochiometric relationship of one molecule 
of oxygen to one molecule of hemoglobin, the molecular weight of 
hemoglobin being fixed by the iron determinations which can be 
carried out with great exactitude. Now the existence of exact 
stochiometric relationships is usually supposed to be one of the 
strongest criteria for chemical combination. Further, there is 
the very definite and distinctive oxy-hemoglobin spectrum, quite 
definitely different from that of “reduced hemoglobin,” and the 
fact that other gases, such as carbon-monoxide, replace oxygen 
at saturation point in exactly equal volume to the oxygen required 
to saturate. 

It would appear from this conflicting evidence that the form 
of the pressure absorption curve as a criterion between adsorption 
and chemical combination breaks down, rather than to be proved 
that the uptake of oxygen by hemoglobin is adsorption and not 
chemical combination. 

The other supposed criterion that there shall exist simple 
stochiometric relationships at the saturation point between the 
two substances uniting also breaks down in the case of unions 
between colloids and crystalloids for several reasons. 

For the appearance of absence of stochiometric relationship 
may be fallacious, and there may be such relationships quite de- 
finitely and true chemical union where there is apparently absorp- 
tion, because the total mass of the colloid may not be identical or 
proportional to its active mass. For example, the crystalloid, such 
as a dye, may not penetrate the aggregate of the colloid, and the 
chemical reaction may occur on the surface of the colloid only ; 
and since it is impossible to estimate the active mass lying on the 
surface and participating in the reaction, exact relative molecular 
masses may be involved and yet there be apparently no such 
relationships. Conversely, there may be no true chemical union, 
and yet the masses of the two substances bear quite definite mole- 
cular relationships. For, if we consider, for example, the protein 
molecule, with a given number of amidogen groups each chemically 
saturated as to valency, and yet each possessing a certain residual 
amount of basic affinity, and if now to this protein we add in 
increasing quantity a substance with weak and chemically saturated 


CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 13 


acidic groups, then we have a definite number of anchorages, and 
when the saturation point of absorption is reached there must appear 
stochiometric relationships although there has occurred no con- 
densation, and breaking apart would readily take place if the osmotic 
pressure or concentration of either constituent were reduced. 

In this way a salt which crystallises with water of crystallisation 
has exact stochiometric relationships with the water, and yet the 
water and salt, which are both saturated compounds, can only be 
held together here by residual affinities. 

To carry this short sketch of the controversy as to adsorption 
versus chemical combination into detail would lead us far beyond 
the limits of this article, so we may sum up with the statement that 
between bodies of different chemical constitution there are varying 
grades of affinity for union. At the one end of the scale there 
are the typical chemical compounds, and at the other the more 
physical unions ' of a weaker type, and dependent upon the main- 
tenance of certain appreciable pressures or concentrations of the 
substances uniting, which have been called adsorptions:; but 
between these two there are all possible gradations, just as there 
are all possible stages between crystalloids and colloids. 

Whether the theories and terminology of adsorption be accepted 
or those of the formation of easily dissociable chemical combination, 
or the middle view be taken that in some cases one occurs and 
in other cases the other, the important experimental fact which 
remains indisputable is that a type of union occurs which is only 
stable so long as a certain pressure (concentration) is maintained, 
and breaks up as the pressure diminishes, showing a range there- 
fore at which association and dissociation of the union occurs in 
a fluctuating way accompanying variations in pressures within the 
range. 

1 A great deal has been written as to the mode of physical union and how it 
is brought about. It has been shown that any substance which lowers the surface 
tension at a bounding surface or interface will tend to increase in concentration 
at that surface. In this way the formation of surface films of protein and other 
colloidal solutions can be explained, similarly the formation of a layer of dye on 
a fabric may meet with explanation, dnd a great many if not all other cases of 
adsorption. But the question remains, why does the substance lower the surface 
tension ? and the view is still tenable that the surface tension is lowered beeause 
of chemical affinity for the substance forming the surface, or because the conditions 
on the surface favour chemical condensation of the substance to form larger mole- 
cules or aggregates than in the body of the solution. Also, an attraction of residual 


affinities may attach the substance by adsorption, and after this anchorage true 
chemical union may follow, 


<<; 


14 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND 


This type of union occurs par excellence, and in endless variety, 
both as to number of substances so uniting and ranges of pressure 
for union and disunion in the case of colloid with crystalloid,.and 
in particular in living cells. Also in the building up of protein 
and bioplasm, there occur endless varieties in the modes of group- 
ing of the constituent radicles, which give rise to the selective 
affinities of the cells, and cause one cell to enter into selective 
union with one constituent of the plasma at a different level of 
pressure (concentration) from another, or to possess affinities of such 
a difference in order, that a substance is taken up with avidity 
by one cell and apparently refused altogether by another. The 
groups in cell and in substance entering into union with it are 
often so delicately arranged that the change of a single radicle 
alters the result entirely ; as, for example, in the action of strychnine 
at very low pressure upon the nerve cells of the central nervous 
system changing at once and practically disappearing when a - 
methyl group is added to the strychnine molecule, or the more 
slightly poisonous action of piperidine passing into the most virulent 
action of coniine, when a propyl group (C,H,), in itself a harmless 
enough constituent, replaces one of the hydrogen atoms as a side 
chain. 

We may now pass to the consideration of the evidence that the 
various organic and inorganic constituents in tissue cells and plasma 
are held in loose union by the bioplasm or proteins. 

First, in regard to the carbohydrate material present in the 
blood, it has been shown that if a stream of carbon-dioxide be 
passed through a sample of blood, or if an anesthetic such as 
chloroform or ether be added to it, and then it be subjected to 
dialysis, the amount of sugar passing into the dialysate is con- 
siderably increased, above the amount in the case of untreated 
blood. It was at first supposed that this increased amount of 
sugar came from the blood corpuscles, but more recent work has 
demonstrated that there is practically no sugar present in the 
corpuscles, and further a similar and equal increment in amount of 
sugar dialysing out can be obtained when clear serum is used for 
the experiment instead of whipped blood. If now a stream of air 
be passed through the serum in order to remove the carbon-dioxide 
or anesthetic before the serum is subjected to dialysis, it is found 
that the yield of sugar in the dialysate has = back again to 
the normal amount. : me . 

\ ; 


CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 15 


This experiment would appear to demonstrate that the sugar 
in the serum exists in some form of feeble union with the protein 
which the action of the mild acidity of the carbon-dioxide or the 
residual affinity of the anesthetic is sufficient to break up, so 
leaving the sugar free to dialyse out. 

This union of carbohydrate and protein throws a light on the 
glycosuria which follows hyperglycemia. For in hyperglycemia 
there is an excess of sugar above that which can enter into union 
with the protein, and it is this excess which is seized upon and 
thrown out into the urine by the kidney cells. From this point 
of view it is interesting to note that in the living animal, when 
there is more than a certain percentage of carbon-dioxide in the 
respired air for a given period, even though there be in the air 
breathed more than the atmospheric proportion of oxygen, then 
there invariably appears sugar in the urine in very considerable 
amount. Also in the case of prolonged anesthesia, especially :if 
the concentration of the anesthetic administered be increased to 
the maximum limit, there always appears sugar in the urine, often 
in high percentages. The author has found as much as 11 per 
cent. of sugar in the urine of dogs after ether anesthesia, and 
has shown that the reducing material present is undoubtedly 
glucose by obtaining and separating typical glucosazone crystals 
in abundance. 

In the liver cells there is undoubtedly union between the 
bioplasm and the sugar before glycogen is formed. The glycogen 
up to a certain maximum limit at which it separates as granules 
can also exist in union in the cell, for considerable amounts of 
glycogen can be separated from the tissues long before separated 
glycogen can be shown by histo-chemical methods. 

Similar evidence has been obtained by different authors as 
to the formation of unions -between cell proteins and fats. By 
certain procedures, such as -partial interference with blood supply 
(Bainbridge and Leathes), it has been possible to make the cells 
of certain organs, notably those of the liver, take on the appear- 
ances of fatty degeneration. The cells become loaded with obvious 
fat globules, which stain with all the usual histo-chemical staining 
reagents for fatty substances. It looks at first sight as if the 
amount of fat in the organ had been enormously increased, but 
the interesting point is that comparative analysis of normal 
liver with no appearance of fat in the cells, and of such liver cells 


2. «a 
“te 


, 


16 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND. 


apparently loaded up with fat, demonstrate that the amounts of ~ 
fat in the two cases are about equal. ! 

The normal liver tissue is capable of holding 5 to 10 per cent. 
of fat in such form that it is quite transparent and invisible 
in discrete form, being in fact an integral part of the bioplasm. 
This can be done in no other way than by some type of union, for 
a fraction only of this fat in free condition would give a thick 
emulsion, showing obvious globules under the microscope, as it 
does when conditions are interfered with as above described, and 
the feeble union of the fat with the tissue broken up. 

Similar results are seen in the chemical phenomena accompany- 
ing nerve degeneration. Again in the plasma or serum itself a 
certain amount of union must take place, for, from a perfectly 
clear serum, showing no oil globules whatever under the micro- 
scope, as much as 0°5 to 1 per cent. of fat may be taken out by 
organic extractives, such as alcohol and ether. Now this amount - 
of fat, were there no agency to hold it in clear solution, would be 
sufficient to give a white milky emulsion. It is only when the 
capacity of the serum for holding fat in solution by feeble union 
with proteins is surpassed that the milky serum often found after 
a heavy fatty meal is obtainable, and this excess of fat is so soon 
taken into union by the bioplasm of the liver and other tissue 
cells, that in an hour or two no trace of any microscopically visible 
fat is seen in the serum or elsewhere. 

In this capacity of the serum for holding in union in invisible 
form a certain amount of fat is found the solution of the problem 
of fat transference in the body from one tissue to another without 
any obvious carriage as an emulsion. By this power of solution 
of fat in bioplasm is also provided the mechanism for the oxidation 
of fats, for it is obvious that previously to oxidation the fat must 
pass into simple molecular form, and that it cannot be oxidised as 
globules of liquid fat. 

Apart from direct oxidation to furnish energy for the cell’s 
work, it is obvious that these lightly held unions of the organic 
foodstuffs furnish the means for the chemical changes in the 
cell which give rise to those syntheses of one organic body from 
another which occur in animals as well as in plants; for the 
synthesis of new proteins by the union of protein radicles rich in 
amido-acids with carbohydrate radicles ; for the synthesis of fats 
from carbohydrates; and for the elaboration of those products 


-ORYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 17 


“of cell activity which we know as internal secretions, lysins, anti- 
odies, toxins, hormones, &c. 

Turning now from the organic foodstuffs and the synthesised 
uets of metabolism to the inorganic crystalloids of the cells 

oo body fluids, we find abundant evidence of union in labile 

equilibrium between these and the organic constituents of the 

body—unions which are absolutely essential to the life and work 
of the cell, specific in character from one type of cell to another, 
and which owe their peculiar and effective functional power in 
the life of the cell to the very feebleness of the union which allows 
_ of interchange and reaction. So that we have stability of the 
whole system in the midst of and indeed as a consequence of the 
instability of the constituent parts. 

The first evidence which may be quoted in favour of this form 
of union is the peculiar distribution of the inorganic salts and ions 
as between tissue cells and their environing fluids, the plasma and 
lymph. 

Although the same inorganic salts are present in the cells 
of the tissue and in the blood corpuscles as are found in 
the lymph and plasma, the quantitative distribution is very 
different in the two cases. The cells are rich in potassium and 

_ phosphatic ions, and relatively poor in sodium and chlorides, 
while the converse holds in the case of the bathing fluids of 
the cells. 

This peculiar distribution . finds an easy explanation on the 
basis that the proteins of the cells are so constituted chemically 
that they possess affinities for absorbing or uniting with potassium 
and phosphatic ions, and have no such power for holding sodium 
and chlorine, while the converse holds for the proteins of the plasma. 
For under these conditions, with the same osmotic pressure of 
dissolved constituents within and without the cell, any particular 

ion will increase in amount in-the absorbed or united form in that 

particular region where protein is found possessing an affinity 
for it. 


ar view which has obtained most adherence is that there exist 

pranes with peculiar and specific properties surrounding the 
which present a varying resistance to the passage of different 
‘These membranes, which moogatly have been regarded as 
B 


No other view which has been put forward furnishes an ade- _ 
q quate explanation of this peculiar distribution of the salts. “The - 


18 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND © 


consisting of bodies called lipoids,’ related to the fats. hat 
lecithins, are supposed to be easily permeable for some substances 
such as urea, ammonia, carbonic-ions, and the anesthetics; but 
difficultly or almost impermeable for other substances such as the 
usual inorganic ions of the plasma, viz., potassium, sodium, phos- 
phates and chlorides. As a result of this difficult permeability it 
is supposed that the potassium is retained in the cell, and the 
sodium in the plasma, while anesthetic, ammonia, and urea, for 

example, rapidly pass through.” . 

There are, however, fatal objections to this membrane view, 
viz. first, that while it makes some attempt at an explanation of 
the maintenance of the status quo, it fails entirely to explain how 
that condition was originally arrived at; secondly, it inextricably 
confuses factors which are of value in the velocity with which 
equilibrium is arrived at with the final conditions of equilibrium ; 
and thirdly, it fails to explain the phenomena of cell interchange 
and the rapid physiological effects upon the cells of variations in 
the concentration of the ions in the external medium. 

If the cell is almost impermeable to potassium ions, for example, 
it is difficult to see how it has become fully charged with them, 
and to many times the amount that these are present in the nutrient 
fluid outside. 

Again, however poor the permeability, if there is no union - 
between constituents within or without and the ion in question, it 
is obvious that when equilibrium has finally been attained, the 
concentrations at the two sides must be equal. Variations in 
permeability can only alter the time required to reach equilibrium, 
and not the final conditions of accumulation on the two sides 
corresponding to the equilibrium. 

Further, anesthesia cannot, as has been suggested by the 
upholders of the membrane theory, arise from greater solubility 
of the anesthetic in the membrane, because that would only delay 

1 The text merely refers to lipoids regarded as semi-permeable membranes. 
Using lipoid as a generic term to include the class of the lecithides and other forms 
of compound fats, there is no doubt that these play an important réle in the life 
of the cell by means of their power of éntering into combination or absorption with — 
organic poisons and toxins. But this is entirely different from a membrane action, : 
being a formation rather of easily dissociable unions of the kind shown in the text 
to exist between bioplasm and organic bodies. 

2 The questions of cell permeability, and the arguments for the membrat 


view, may be found in detail in Hamburger’s Osmotischer Druck und Io 
For the reasons given in the text they have not been stated at length. 


_ 


the arrival of the anesthetic at the active part of the cell until 
the lipoid membrane had first been saturated with anwsthetic. 

_ For this reason also greater solubility of any constituent in 
the cell substance itself will not explain the greater amount or 
concentration of any particular substance or ion in the process 
of secretion or excretion. Such greater solubility would serve to 
‘fill the cell up with it and keep it there, but would not hasten passage 
through the cell,and after the solubility in the cell substance had 
been satisfied, diffusion would then go on until the free concentra- 
tion on the secretion side equalled the concentration on the lymph 
side, but not a fraction beyond this point. 

With regard to increased or specific solubility in the cell sub- 
stance as an agency in statically loading the cell up with a given 
constituent or ion, as distinct from passing it out again in 
heightened concentration in a secretion, it may be admitted that 
this would explain the accumulation ; but this on closer examina- 
tion is essentially the same view as that of union or adsorption 
with the protein, except that the adsorption or combination view 
goes a step further and attempts to give a basis for the increased 
or specific solubility. 

Even in the simpler case where a substance in solution divides 
itself in different concentrations between two solvents which do 
not mix with each other,’ giving rise to the quotient of distribution, 

it is obvious that there is an equilibrium between the concentrations 
_ in the two media depending upon the relative affinities (or residual 
affinities) of the molecules of two solvents respectively for the 
molecules of the solute or dissolved substance. 

One may therefore quite justly assign the unequal distribution 
of the various ions in cell and environing fluid respectively to 
different solubilities in the two media; apart from membrane 
action which is out of the question unless the somewhat absurd 
hypothesis be made that the. accumulation remains and resides in 
the membrane. But knowing the nature of the constitution of the 
protein constituents, and that these must possess residual affinities 
or absorptive powers, it appears feasible to go a step further and 


w ons between protein or bioplasm and the ions. 
On this view the cell of any tissue or the blood corpuscle is a 
1 As, for example, an organic acid dividing itself between water and ethylic 


~ 


ssign the distribution and different solubility to the fopenatigg of ~ 


20 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND i 


system in equilibrium regarding ions with its surrounding medium, 
and the equilibrium is maintained by the pressure of each parti- 
cular ion, acting independently, in the surrounding medium. Also 
variations in concentration of any of the ions will cause reaction 
and variation in the equilibrium of the whole, and may cause such 
disturbances as will alter the distribution of other ions and soluble 
substances, and so cause variations in the character and types of 
reaction of the protein or bioplasm. 

Two chief factors determine the equilibrium between each ion 
and the cell; one of these is the concentration of the ion, the other 
the constant of association or adsorption between the cell ‘sub- 
stance and the ion. This constant changes its value specifically 
from one ion to another with a constant type of cell or bioplasm ; 
and with the same ion kept constant varies from type of cell to 
type of cell, so giving rise to the specific picking out of particular 
cell types by particular ions or other bodies." 

In addition to these chief factors, there is some evidence that 
certain ions can replace each other, or in other words compete for 
the same vacant places in the protein or bioplasm. This is known 
as the antagonistic action of drugs. Usually the ions so replacing 
must be of the same order of valency, a monad being unable to 
take the place of a dyad, but one dyad can replace another, and 
especially two dyads in the same periodic group of elements are 
interchangeable. For example, one heavy metal can take the 
place of another, and even the paradox is arrived at that the 
poisonous action of one of these heavy metals is decreased by the 

1 This might be put in simple mathematical form thus: if C, be the concentra- 
tion of the protein or other substratum in a given cell, Cy the concentration of 
the ion to be absorbed in the medium outside (lymph), and Cs the concentration 
of the substance adsorbed, then C3=KC,Co, where K is a constant dependent on 
the chemical and physical affinities of ion or other substance for each other, and 
hence having a different value when either ion or type of protein is changed. If 
now we keep the same ion, as in the distribution of any naturally occurring ion 
in the body, or in the action of any given drug which can only be given so that 
it is free to act on all cells in the body, then for a different cell using small letters 
we can write as before cs=kc,c, where the suffixes show the same meaning as 


before. But now 2 and c, the concentration of the ion or drug in the circulating 
medium is the sanie in both cells, and hence if we want to get the relative con- 


centration in the two types of the cell we have out Pe and further if we take 
3 1 
it that the protein concentration is the same in the two cells, we finally have 
\ 


oat or the relative distribution is in proportion to the affinities between ion or 
3 
drug and particular type of protein. 


ve 


’ 


CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 21 


simultaneous presence of another, so that instead of there being 
an additive effect of the two poisons, one balances the other and 
protects in part from its action, so that the lethal dose of either is 
increased. 

The ions of the inorganic salts at the same time that they are 
in a loose type of union with the proteins, possess a freedom of 
movement which shows itself in their giving to the solution in 
which they exist in common with the colloidal proteins many of 
| the more important physico-chemical properties of a saline solution. 

For example, in the case of the blood serum, the depression of 
freezing point is almost the same as that of an equal amount of 
salts dissolved in distilled water, showing that here every ion in 
the solution has its full effect in producing osmotic pressure not- 

withstanding its adsorption by the serum proteins. Again, the 
electrical conductivity is practically the same as that of an isosmotic 
solution of the saline constituents alone in distilled water, showing 
that any adsorption which may be present does not interfere in the 
least with the movements or velocities of the ions in the electrical 
field. Yet there is other evidence that the salts of the serum are 
in union of some type with the proteins, and that the amount of 
salts in the serum as regulated by the kidney cells is dependent 
upon the combining power of the proteins. 

One fact that gives a clear indication of this is the titration 
value for the serum in presence of one of the more stable coloured 
indicators, such as methyl orange or “‘di-methyl.” It has been 
pointed out earlier in this article that the proteins can act either 
as acids or bases, or as it is termed are amphoteric to indicators. 
Thus, blood serum is acid to phenol-phthaléin, and must have 
alkali added to it to produce the pink colour denoting alkalinity ; 

at the same time it is alkaline to methyl orange or di-methyl, and 
___ requires the addition of much acid before showing the acid colour 
of the indicator. 

The actual reaction of the serum is almost that of exact neutrality 
in the sense of physical chemistry, that is to say, the concentrations 
_ of hydrogen ion and hydroxy] ion are about equal. Now although 
it is essential that the colour of an indicator for acid and alkali ~ 
should change before the ratio of the concentrations of the two — 
ions becomes a high one, no indicator used in practice actually 
does change exactly at the chemical or exact neutral point, and 
the turning point is different for each one. Hence it is that blood 


serum appears to be acid when tested by phenol-phthaléin, and 
appears to be alkaline when tested by methyl orange, &c. Not, 
as is too often stated, because it is at the same time acid and 
alkaline, for that is absurd, but that its actual position in reaction 
lies very nearly at the neutral point, and just short of that slight 
degree of alkalinity which shows the alkaline colour to phenol- 
phthaléin on the one hand, and just short of that degree of acidity 
which gives the acid colour to methyl orange. Now these two. 
points lie very close together, for if instead of the serum we take 
distilled water and add the two indicators phenol-phthaléin and 
methyl orange in traces to it, then a single drop of dilute alkali 
will develop the alkaline colour of the phenol-phthaléin, and on 
the other hand a single drop of dilute acid will show the acid colour 
of the methyl orange. 

In the case of the serum, however, the result is quite different, 
for very considerable amounts of alkali must be added before it 
turns alkaline to phenol-phthaléin, and proceeding in the opposite 
direction still larger amounts of acid must be added before acidity 
to methyl orange is realised. The reason for this is that the pro- 
teins, which can figure either as acid or base according to whether 
there is excess of alkali or acid respectively in the solution, must first 
be satisfied before the indicators are affected ; and as the amount 
of protein is large, so the amount of acid or alkali required before 
it is neutralised and the acidity or alkalinity can commence rapidly 
to run up and affect the coloured indicator, is very considerable. 

This is a factor of great importance to the life of the cells, which 
cannot bear any appreciable degree of either acidity or alkalinity, 
and are protected from such variations by the very delicate regu- 
lation of the reaction by the amphoteric proteins. 

The regulating action of the proteins upon the reaction of the 
serum has been mentioned here, however, because it gives a strong 
indication that the proteins are in union with the inorganic salts. 
If a clear sample of serum be titrated with methyl orange or “ di- 
methyl” as.an indicator, an alkalinity equivalent to the very high 
figure of 0:17. to 0°18 normal is obtained. This alkalinity is chiefly 
due to proteins, for if the salts of the serum be separated off by 
dialysis or incubation and titrated to the same indicator, the 
alkalinity now ambunts to only 0:03 to 0:04 normal. Subtracting _ 
these amounts due to inorganic constituents from the higher figure, 
we obtain the result that the combining power of the serum pro- 


iii ae 


22 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND 


CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 23 


teins alone for acid is equivalent to about 0°14 normal.’ Now 
the interesting point about this figure is that it coincides almost 
exactly with the total osmotic concentration of all the salts naturally 
occurring in the serum or plasma. The depression of freezing 
point of mammalian sera is on the average equivalent to that of a 
0-9 per cent. solution of sodium chloride, and the molecular weight 
of sodium chloride being 58, this corresponds to a 0°15 normal 

: solution. 

j In addition to this direct evidence from the chemical side, 
there are certain physiological correspondences between amounts 
of protein and crystalloid in the blood which must be obeyed, or 
otherwise the excess of salt in the plasma is removed by the kidneys. 
This action comes into operation as soon as the plasma salts exceed 

- the amounts which can be loosely held by the proteins. 

The salts in cells are held more firmly adsorbed or combined 
than is the case in the plasma, as is shown by effects on the elec- 
trical conductivity and by the difficulty of dialysing the salts 
from the cells. 

Thus it is found that although the osmotic concentration of 
the salts in the red blood corpuscles is nearly the same as in the 
plasma, as shown by the depressions of freezing points,’ yet the 
electrical conductivity of the separated corpuscles is only one- 
fourteenth to one-seventeenth of that of the separated serum. 
Part of this difference is mechanical and due to the envelopes of 
the corpuscles rendering the conducting fluid non-homogeneous ; 
but even after removing this factor by laking, the conductivity of 
the laked corpuscles still remains only at one-fifth to one-sixth of 
that of the serum. This difference is undoubtedly due to the 
attachment of the ions to the hemoglobin interfering with the 
ionic velocities, for on dialysing, against distilled water and then 
reducing the volume of the dialysate to such a degree as to re- 
present the original concentration of the salts before dialysis, it is 
found that the conductivity of the free salts in the dialysate has 
undergone a further increase above that which they possessed 
when in union with the hemoglobin, and now lies at about one- 
half the value in the serum. Even dialysis, however, is unable. 
to detach the phosphates from the hemoglobin, and the above 


————y)~—tf 
> 


1 The amount of this combining power of the protein may be better appreciated 
by some if it be stated as equivalent to about 0°51 per cent. of hydrochloric acid. 


2 Vide infra. 


a 


24 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND 


conductivity of one-half is chiefly due to chlorides detached in 
the process of dialysis. It is only after incineration and making 
up to original volume that the conductivities of corpuscles and 
serum become practically equal. 

The following table illustrates these interesting changes in 
conductivity accompanying detachment of colloid and crystalloid 
in two experiments on separated blood corpuscles and serum. 
The figures give specific conductivity multiplied by 10° to save 
decimals. 


- 


SAMPLE I. SAMPLE II. 
Treatment to which subjected. Serum. Corpuscles. Serum. Corpuscles. 
1. Fresh . : $ : - 1705 95 1519 109 
2. Frozen solid and thawed? ,,,5 , 
(corpuscles laked) . mA ma ae — asf . 
3. Dialysed and volume re- id 1843 89] 1623 754 
duced to original j <\ 
4. Incinerated and ash made) 1608 1677 1697 1655 
up to original volume 5 


That the phosphates are more firmly held than the chlorides, 
so that the union persists even in presence of a very low concen- 
tration of phosphatic ions in the fluid, is shown by the following 
analysis for chlorides and phosphates in the dialysates of corpuscles 
and serum respectively, and in the two cases after incineration. 
The figures also illustrate the very different distribution of chlorides 
and phosphates in corpuscles and serum respectively. 


Serum Percentages. Corpuscle Percentages. 
Cl, P50... Cl. P,0,. 
Dialysis . : . 03657 0°:0197 01331 0:0329 
Incineration . . 0°3373 00219 0:1704 0°1708 


Even the chlorides are more strongly held in corpuscles than in 
serum, the figure on incineration being considerably higher than 
after dialysis, 0°1704 as against 0°1331, instead of being slightly 
lower, due to volatilisation with organic matter, as in the case of 
the serum, 0:3373 as against 0°3657. In the serum the phosphate 
figures are almost equal by the two methods, but in the corpuscles 
the evidence of union is clear, only 0:0329 per cent. dialyses out of 
the 0°1708 shown to be present by incineration. These figures are 
completely confirmed by freezing point determinations. 

This experimental evidence is interesting as showing that the 
special affinities existing in each case between protein and ion 


-— 


Tegel 


CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 25 


demand very different pressures or concentrations to preserve 
_ the equilibrium. 

We can now understand why so little phosphate is required in 
the Ringer’s solution; the union of the phosphates is so strong 
that it is not possible to run the phosphate concentration down to 
such a level as rapidly to disintegrate the phosphatic ions of the 
cardiac tissue. The merest trace given off from the heart to the 
perfusing fluid suffices to stop further loss. The level for calcium 
and potassium, though low, is somewhat higher, and traces sufficient 
to preserve equilibrium must be added, or these ions break free from 
the cardiac cells, producing irregularity of function. Finally, the 
sodium and chlorine ions are but loosely held, and hence as much as 
0-7 to 0°9 per cent. of sodium chloride must be present to preserve 
the equilibrium and normal conditions of physiological activity. 

The facts as to the constitution of the colloidal material and 
its relationship with electrolytes and other crystalloids which have 
been given above, and the interpretation put upon those facts, 
are intended to demonstrate that the living cell is a peculiarly 
constructed energy machine or energy transformer, dependent for 
its activity upon a delicate labile equilibrium giving stability as 
a whole, and yet a weakness of union causing disruption and 
. oxidation of parts, and so furnishing energy. The view put forward 

is intended as a reaction from that view which complacently re- 
gards all the work of the cell and peculiarity in its constitution 
as being due to the physical properties of inert membranes. 

The attempt has been made’to show that something is required 
more than membranes and osmotic pressure to explain the peculiar 
distribution of electrolytes in cell and nutrient medium, and going 
further to give a basis for the understanding of the peculiar energy 
exchanges of cells. It has been sought to invoke the peculiar 
chemical constitution of protein and bioplasm, and the varying 
equilibria of these with the materials brought in from the nutrient 
media at varying pressures, giving rise to transient stages of 
association and dissociation, and an accompanying play of energy 

/ changes. 

} It is not intended in doing this, however, to suggest that mem- 
branes and variations in osmotic pressure play no part in the cell’s 
work or in preserving the integrity of the cell, nor to depreciate 
work upon osmotic conditions in cell life. Because there are other 
factors to be reckoned with, it does not follow that osmosis is to 


be neglected. In fact a wider appreciation of the phenomena of 
union between the bioplasm and crystalloid constituents widens 
rather than narrows our conceptions of the cell as an osmotic 
centre, by allowing us to regard the cell as a chamber with vary- 
ing osmotic properties, both of contents and wall, rather than as 
heretofore as a-more or less fixed solution, bounded by a mem- 
brane of fixed properties also, and resembling a semi-permeable 
copper-ferrocyanide wall. 

The rigorous conception of the cell as analogous in all respects 
to a fluid medium holding crystalloids simply in solution and 
bounded by a semi-permeable wall is most pernicious in biology, 
for there are no experimental facts to warrant such a view, but 
rather, as has been shown above, quite the reverse. 

The whole chemical structure of the cell and that part of it 
which is physiologically active 7s the osmotic machine, and needs 
no membrane permeable or impermeable in order to exhibit the’ 
usual osmotic phenomena of shrinking or swelling, leading finally 
to disruption. In some cases membranes in the narrower sense 
of the word are demonstrable surrounding the cell mass, and in 
other cases which form the vast majority, no such coarsely structural 
membranes exist ; but in all cases the nature of the bioplasm is 
so differentiated chemically as to form a dividing surface readily 
permeable to the solvent, and this is all that is required, in addition 
to the varying unions or holding powers between the cell colloids 
and crystalloids, to establish an osmotic cell. As an example of 
what is meant here we may instance the swelling of fibrin, con- 
nective tissue, and gelatine under the imbibition of water. Be- 
tween gelatine and water there is no. structural membrane with 
semi-permeable pores, yet the gelatine takes.in water in a truly 
osmotic fashion, and the pressure developed, if the swelling and 
uptake of water are resisted, is very high. 

It is hence necessary to get our minds rid of the preconceived 
idea derived from too closely drawn analogies with experimentally © 
constructed osmotic cells that the cell membrane is responsible 
for the osmotic behaviour of the whole cell. 

If instead of this we take the view, which is supported by ex- 
perimental facts, that the bioplasm) holds the crystalloids in loose 
union in the cell, so that they cannot for the time escape or diffuse = 
out, and yet admits of a degree of molecular freedom to the 
crystalloids, so that they still attract water molecules by residual 


- ‘ 
se at —~ P. 


26 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND 


CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 27 


_ affinity, then we arrive at a conception which is capable of linking 
together the osmotic properties of the cell, not merely in a statical 
but in a dynamic way, and gives a basis for understanding the 
variations in osmotic effects which accompany cell activities from 
one phase to another. 

With the view of an inert semi-permeable membrane of fixed 
_ properties, not sharing the varying changes in chemical constitu- 
tion associated with life, or in other words not possessing the 
properties of bioplasm outlined above, all that can be arrived at 
is a continual tendency in one direction to a fixed equilibrium. 

The other view, that the osmotic properties are developed by 
the bioplasm itself in its varying unions with crystalloids, gives 
room for that up and down play of properties which is the out- 
standing characteristic of living matter.’ 

For example, a circulating hormone, a drug substance or a 
| nerve impulse arriving at a given set of cells in a tissue, may activate 

the cells by momentarily disrupting unions between bioplasm and 
crystalloids or the reverse, and so may cause an uptake or a giving 
out of water accompanied by certain crystalloids free in excess to 
or from the cell, or may alter water distribution in varying parts 
provoking muscular contraction or other form of protoplasmic 
movement. 

Similarly molecular movements -of radicles attached to the 
bioplasm may be induced, causing changes in molecular arrange- 
ment and synthesis of new bodies within the cell. Further, the 
osmotic pressures and concentrations of the crystalloids and other 
bodies so set free need obviously bear no immediate relationship 
to the concentration of these substances in the plasma outside the 
cell, and so the very varying concentrations of secretions may be 
understood in a way that cannot be realised on any basis of pure 
osmosis or filtration. 

The experimental facts of cell life, both in regard to the taking 
up and giving out water and substances in solution, furnish a 
clear demonstration that neither osmosis nor any other physical 

_ hypothesis which leaves out the peculiar and varying chemical 


secretion, or excretion. 


1 It is interesting to note that serum proteins exactly at their neutral point 

show no osmotic pressure whatever, but addition of minute amounts either of acid 

or alkali at once gives rise to an osmotic pressure which up to certain limits 
increases with amount of acid or alkali added. ~ 


constitution of bioplasm can yield an explanation of absorption, - _ 


28 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND i 


The whole of the experiments lend support to the view that 
the living cell exists in a periodically or phasically varying osmotic 
equilibrium with its surroundings, and not in a state of osmotic 
equality with them. The cell by its unions with crystalloids pre- 
serves a distinct osmotic condition within its bounds different 
from that in the surrounding fluid media from which its nutrient 
materials are taken up. This is particularly well seen when the 
medium without is subject to considerable and accidental varia- 
tions. Even in those cases where the outer medium is practically 
constant, as in the extreme case, for example, of blood corpuscle 
and plasma, although there appears to be an existence of osmotic 
equality within the cell, and without, yet this is due to the peculiar 
conditions having induced a close coincidence of the two sets of 
osmotic phenomena, and the existence of an equilibrium and not 
an equality may be easily shown by suitably varying the condi- 


tions. So that even in these extreme cases what we have to do - 


with is not really equality of osmotic pressures, but an equilibrium 
which happens to simulate equality from the presence of reducing 
conditions; the equality disappears as soon as these reducing 
conditions are disturbed. 

When the corpuscles of whipped blood are separated as com- 
pletely as possible from the serum by means of the centrifuge, 
and the depressions of freezing point of the corpuscles and of 
the serum separately determined, it is found that the freezing 
point of the serum lies on the average at 0:02° to 0:03° C. lower 
than that of the corpuscles. This difference, small as it is, is con- 
stant in its occurrence, and corresponds to a difference in osmotic 
pressure of approximately 200 to 300 m.m. of mercury. If the 
corpuscles after separation from the serum are thoroughly shaken 
up with saline solutions weaker and stronger than the serum, or 
as they are termed, hypo- and hyper-tonic solutions, it is always 
found, on again separating corpuscles and saline by means of the 
centrifuge, that the depression of the freezing point of the saline 
is greater than that of the corpuscles, no matter whether the saline 
employed was hypotonic or hypertonic. The differences become 
in these cases much greater than the natural differences between 
corpuscles and serum. These results show that there is established, 
as the concentration of the saline is varied, an equilibrium for 
each strength of saline, but not an equality, there always being a 
negative osmotic difference within the corpuscle. 


fi 


CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 29 


In other types of cell these differences in osmotic pressure 
within and without the cell become enormously greater. Thus, 
in plants, the root sap which carries up the electrolytes from the 
earth for the nutrition of the growing cells is exceedingly dilute, 
the depression of freezing point being only about one-fifth part 
of that of the cell juice. Similarly, in the secretion of sweat and 
_ galiva the concentration of inorganic ions, as shown both by freezing 
point methods and by direct chemical analyses, is only a small 
fraction of that of the plasma or lymph. In other cases, such 
as absorption by the intestinal cells and secretion by the kidney 
cells, the osmotic pressure on the side remote from the lymph may 
lie either above or below that on the lymph side, but nearly always 
differs widely from it. It has been shown that either distilled 
water or hypertonic salines can be taken up by the intact intestinal 
mucosa; and the A (i.e. freezing-point depression) of the urine 
may be many times greater than that of the plasma, or may 
after ingestion or intra-venous injection of much water be a mere 
fraction of the A of the plasma. 
Whether the tremendous pressure differences corresponding to 
_ these differences in A really exist within the cells must remain 
indeterminate so long as we possess no knowledge as to the degree 
to which the crystalloids of the secretion are adsorbed while the 
secretion is passing through the cell and is in contact with the 
bioplasm. By alternating or periodic dissociation and combina- 
tion between colloid and crystalloid in the actively secreting (ab- 
sorbing or excreting) cell, the pressures would appear and disappear 
alternately ; and if by the action of, the nerve supply or any stimu- 
lating substance the bioplasm is thrown into any such rhythmic 
activity of adsorption and re-separation, there would follow an 
easy explanation of the passage of both water and crystalloid 
through cells, in any concentration. For the concentration would 
depend solely on the uptake of crystalloid by the cell colloid, before 
the next explosion, disrupting colloid and crystalloid, threw the 
crystalloid free in the cell and determined, by the osmotic pressure 
_ developed: thereby in the cell, the flow of secretion. 

It would appear that in nerve and muscle at the period of 
activity only, and in injured tissue (which is excited or active 
tissue), there exists in reality a detachment of potassium ions from 
the colloid which does not exist before or after the active period 
penal : 


} 


30 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND on | 


’ 


It has been already pointed out that for each constituent 
passing into union with the bioplasm there exists an optimum 
concentration or osmotic pressure of solution in the nutrient 
medium of the cell which alone is compatible with normal physio- 
logical activity ; or rather it might better be put that there is a 
range of suitable concentrations with a minimum and maximum 
which must not be passed in either direction. 

This point is particularly well illustrated in the case of the 
respiratory gases. For both oxygen and carbon-dioxide there are 
well-marked limits of pressure which are required to be satisfied 
in order that the processes of respiration and oxidation in the 
tissues may proceed in a normal fashion. 

Since the energy for all tissue activity is derived from the 
oxidation of organic bodies it is obvious that there must be a 
minimal pressure of oxygen below which life is impossible, but it 
is not so obvious that there is an upper limit of oxygen concen- ° 
tration at which life becomes equally impossible. Yet it is found 
that when warm-blooded animals are exposed to a pressure of 
about three atmospheres of pure oxygen, death occurs in a few 
minutes after violent convulsions (Bert). Short of this excessive 
pressure, exposure to over one atmosphere of pure oxygen for a 
longer period leads, as shown by Lorrain Smith and L. Hill, toa 
pheumonic condition of the lungs. 

If pure oxygen at atmospheric pressure be breathed for a 
shorter time interval, the tissues become charged with oxygen at a 
higher pressure than normal, and Hill and Flack have shown that 
for a short period afterwards muscular work can be done at a 
more rapid rate in such forms of exercise as sprinting, hill-climbing, 
and working against resistance. After exhaustive muscular exer- 
tion also the breathing of oxygen diminishes the dyspnea and 
sense of fatigue. 

On proceeding in the direction of testing the effects of per- 
centages of oxygen less than the atmospheric, it is found that the 
results obtained depend upon the type of mammal experimented 
with, and upon the state of quiescence or activity of the animal. 
As the percentage of oxygen decreases the will and energy to do 
work diminish, and at a partial pressure of about half that present 
in the atmosphere any attempt at muscular work has to be aban- 
doned, and the mental processes also become most sluggish. 

The earlier experiments on the absolute minimum amounts of 


CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 31 


_ oxygen required to support life in animals in a state of quiescence 
were vitiated by the simultaneous accumulation of carbon-dioxide 
from the respiration of the animals. 

When means are taken to exclude this source of error by ab- 

_ sorbing the carbon-dioxide with soda-lime as rapidly as it is formed, 

it is found that animals (rabbits) can be kept alive for as long as 
forty hours on a respiratory mixture containing as little as 5 to 
6 per cent. of oxygen. With slightly less than 5 per cent. of oxygen, 
death occurs very rapidly. . 

These results, considered together, show that there is a mini- 
mum concentration of oxygen necessary for sufficient oxidation to 
support life ; that as the pressure rises the degree of combination 
or union between bioplasm and oxygen increases, quickening the 
oxidation processes in the tissues; that an optimum of activity 
exists somewhere above the normal amount present in atmos- 
pheric air; and that still higher up embarrassment occurs from 
too high pressure, causing firmer union between the oxygen and 
bioplasm. 

A very parallel set of results are obtained in the case of carbon- 
dioxide. Here it might be thought that since carbon-dioxide is 
a waste product of the oxidation process, the best possible con- 
dition would be its complete removal; but it has been clearly 
shown by Haldane that a definite minimal percentage of carbon- 
dioxide is required for the regulation of the respiratory exchange, 
and that when the percentage is reduced by artificial ventilation, 
the subject passes into apnoea or suspension of breathing until 
the amount is brought back towatds normal in the lungs and 
tissues. The normal amount of carbon-dioxide in the alveolar 
spaces lies between 4 and 5 per cent., and if it rises or falls but 
slightly from the normal, corresponding changes take place in the 
respiratory rhythm and depth which tend to restore the balance 

once more. 

| It has further been shown by Henderson that excessive and 
prolonged ventilation of the lungs by artificial means leads by 

lowering of the carbon-dioxide concentration to irregularity of the 

heart beat, and finally, if pushed, to delirium cordis and death of 

the animal. Short of this limit, stoppage of the positive ventila- 

tion has the effect of restoring the heart to regular rhythm. 

__ Passing in the opposite direction, and observing the effects of 

_ increasing amounts of carbon-dioxide, administered in artificial 


7 


32 THE EQUILIBRIUM OF COLLOID AND 


mixtures containing as high, or higher, amounts of oxygen as are 


present in atmospheric air so as to avoid asphyxiation from de- 
ficiency of oxygen, it is found that carbon-dioxide has directly 
poisonous effects upon the bioplasm. Thus, with 12 to 15 per cent. 
of carbon-dioxide and 20 to 25 per cent. of oxygen, it is, found 
that animals become somnolent, and, as above stated, that the 
urine contains glucose, while with 20 to 25 per cent. of carbon- 
dioxide, even in presence of excess of oxygen, death rapidly occurs. 

The same effects are seen upon isolated tissues. Thus Waller 
has shown that the first effects of minimal traces of carbon-dioxide 
is to increase the excitability of nerve, while larger doses diminish 
excitability, and finally all excitability disappears. Similar results 
are found in unicellular organisms and in ciliary movements. 

All these results point to varying degrees of union and corre- 
sponding stability or instability of union between carbon-dioxide 
and bioplasm. 

Exactly similar results are everywhere evident in the applica- 
tion of various drugs in therapeutics, in the action of the toxins 
of disease, and in the action of antiseptics. There is the same 
stimulating action seen, followed by paralysing action as the con- 
centration is increased and the union between bioplasm and drug 
becomes more stable and complete. 

One of the most striking results here is the adaptation between 
drug and different types of cell, due to molecular variations in 
the structure of the two reacting bodies causing them to possess 
higher affinities. and unite at lower concentrations. For this 
reason one type of cell takes up a drug and robs the other cells 
of it, lowering the pressure in these other cells and the plasma, 
so that the particular type of cell becomes loaded up at a pressure 
which scarcely causes any uptake in other cells. 

On such a basis it is easy to understand why all mercury salts 
produce the same specific action in syphilis, the result being due 
to the free mercury ion and not being affected by the anion of 
the salt used except in so far as this quantitatively alters the 
degree of ionisation, and hence the concentration of mercury ion. 
Similarly, the ferric ion in all iron salts stimulates the production 
of erythrocytes in anzmia. So too quinine, and the alkaloids 
generally, furnish a basic ion affecting specific cells of the organism 
in each case, or of pathogenic foreign organisms present in it, for 
which at different stages they possess special affinities. 


-CRYSTALLOID IN LIVING CELLS 33 


_ The same specific action due to different detail in structure of 
biopls sm, which we see exemplified in the picking out action of 
‘drugs in the multicellular organism, is seen, and from the same 
cause, in the action of different drugs upon different stages of the 
‘same parasitic organism. 

For example, quinine only attacks the malarial parasite when 
it is breaking forth from the erythrocyte. Again, the drug atoxyl 
acts on the ordinary motile form of the trypanosome, but rapid 
recurrence shows that it does not destroy the latent bodies, while 
mercury is shown, by the prolongation in the period of recurrence 
which it causes when given after atoxyl, to attack the latent bodies, 
although it has no action whatever on the ordinary motile stage. 

The closer and more detailed study of the conditions of the 
formation of these unstable unions between bioplasm and the 
dissolved substances of its natural and artificially varied environ- 

ment must furnish the key to many of the intricate problems both 
of physiology and of practical medicine ; and it may perhaps be 
added that these subjects, whether studied in the laboratory or 
by the bedside, form one organic whole, for the subject of study 
in both is the living cell in all its wealth of reaction to changes in 
its environment. 


THE HEART 
By MARTIN FLACK 


From time immemorial the heart has been the object of great, 
although perhaps not altogether scientific interest. In recent 
years many points relating to its anatomy and physiology have 
occupied the attention of investigators, so that the literature upon 
these subjects has become very large and in many respects in- 
tricate. In the following remarks the subject will be treated 
under these headings :— 


A. The microscopic anatomy of the heart. 

B. The morphology of the vertebrate heart. 

C. The nervous elements of the vertebrate heart. 

D. The heart as a muscle. 

E. The site of origin and mode of conduction of the excitatory 
wave of the heart. 

F. The movements of the heart in situ. 


A. THE Microscopic ANATOMY OF THE HEART 


The heart musculature must not be regarded as being built up 
of a number of separate cells fixed together by a cement sub- 
stance. It is really a network of cells, intimately fused on all sides, 
or as it is sometimes termed a syncytium (Kolliker, M. Heidenhain). 

In fresh and in well-prepared fixed preparations there is no 
trace of division of the musculature into short mononuclear seg- 
ments or cells. The muscle fibre is seen to pass many nuclei 
without the appearance of a division or cement line between them 
—indeed in the heart of mammals such small segmented portions 
are hard to find, and seem only to occur in the circular bands of 
muscle at the heart orifices and in the musculi papillares. 

According to M. Heidenhain, the muscle fibres of the heart are 
about one-third thinner in diameter than those of voluntary 
muscle. They possess nuclei, sarcoplasm, fibrillary substance, and 

34 


THE HEART 35 


sarcolemma. The nuclei lie at regular intervals inside the fibre ; 
they are elliptical in shape, 7-16 « long, 5-9 « broad, and possess 
a well-marked chromatin network and a nucleolus. At both poles 
of the nucleus is situated a mass of granular protoplasm, the 
sarcoplasm. This sarcoplasm is more richly developed than 
in voluntary muscle, and contains strongly refractile basophile 
granules which in the adult are frequently yellow or yellow brown. 
Their presence, so far as is known, denotes nothing abnormal. 
Running out from the central sarcoplasm between the bundles 
of fibrils to the periphery of the fibre there is a very delicate proto- 
plasmic membrane, the sarcolemma, often richly impregnated with 
+ fine granules. It is perhaps not so well developed as in striated 
muscle, and differs in not being of a chitinous nature. 

The contractile substance of the heart muscle fibre is made up 
of a number of bundles of fibrils, the sarcostyles. These are some- 
what prismatic in shape, and lie at the periphery of the fibre ; the 
centre being occupied by the nucleus and the sarcoplasm (Kolliker). 
The sarcostyles exhibit longitudinal and frequently transverse 
striation ; the former being due to its composition of fibrils ; the 
latter to the presence of singly and doubly refractile substances 
within the fibril. When the sarcolemma running between the 
fibrils is well marked, the transverse striation is masked. 

There is an intimate fusion between neighbouring fibres—a 
number of fibrils from:one fibre becoming detached and passing 
uninterruptedly into its neighbour. From this it will be seen 
that only the small amount of protoplasm situated around the 
various nuclei can be looked upon as being discontinuous and in 
any way comparable to the original cells (the myoblasts) from 

_ which the heart is developed. These cells appear to have be- 
come fused together to form a syncytium, and in this a common 
network of fibrils has been laid down. 

The question then arises that, if the lines formerly described 

by von Eberth as separating the cells of cardiac muscle from 
_ each other, do nothing of the kind, what is their function? Von 
_ Eberth thought they were a cement substance binding the cells 
together, and called them “cement lines” in consequence. This 
view: ‘has been given up. The lines do not separate cells since the 
fibrils pass through them (v. Przewosky, v. Ebner, M. Heidenhain). 
Th many places, too, it can be seen, particularly in a fresh prepara- 
ion that the so-called line does not always go completely across 


| MMR +. 
1 


yale rn 


oe 


36 THE HEART 


a fibre—it may only go part of the way across, or it may go 
across in step-like fashion. 

According to v. Ebner these lines represent a dying phenoménon 
—a thickening of the fibre due to an abnormal contraction 
during death. Eppinger regards them as pathological in origin. 
M. Heidenhain, on the other hand, believes that they are really 
present in the normal fibre, and have a special function to per- 
form in regulating the growth of the fibre. He therefore terms 
them “ Schaltstiicke ” (“regulators”). In the human heart they 
are 1-1-7 uw thick, and consist of separate parallel perpendicular 
rods. As we have said, they do not necessarily go right across a 
fibre. The intervals at which they occur are by no means regular : 
the pieces shut off by them may be of any length, and cannot be 
looked upon in any way as cells. 

To sum up, the heart muscle, according to the more recent 
investigations, is to be regarded as a syncytium in which a common ~ 
network of fibres has been developed. 


B. Tue MorpeuHoLoGy oF THE VERTEBRATE HEART 


The study of the comparative anatomy of the vertebrate heart 
greatly facilitates a proper understanding of the most recent 
anatomical work on the mammalian heart—namely, the isolation 
of the auriculo-ventricular bundle. It aids us also in estimating 
the significance of the many facts known about the heart, and 
also in arriving at the probable value of the theories held in 
regard to its working. Furthermore, the study of the comparative 
anatomy is undoubtedly very helpful in bringing out points about 
which research is still required and also in determining the lines 
of such research. 

The Primitive Vertebrate Heart.—Figure | is a generalised 
diagram of sucha heart. It consists of five chambers—(a) The sinus 
venosus ; (b) the auricular canal; (c) the auricle ; (d) the ventricle ; 
(e) the bulbus cordis. These parts are all in free muscular con- 
tinuity ; there is no break at any of the junctional lines (1.1, 2.2, 
4.4, 5, in Fig. 1). 

This is a point of interest, since in tracing the representatives 
of these primary divisions in the mammalian, and especially in — 
the human, heart, it will be of service to ascertain whether this | 
muscular continuity between the different chambers still exists, 3 


THE HEART 37 


The Sinus Venosus in the Human Heart.—The sinus venosus 
is of importance in primitive vertebrate hearts, because it is here 
that many authorities have observed the origin of the heart rhythm 
(Gaskell, MacWilliam, Engelmann). This fact makes it essential 
for us to know what parts represent the sinus venosus in the 


Fic. 1.—A generalised type of vertebrate heart—combining features found in 
the eel, dogfish, and frog (Keith) ; @, sinus venosus and veins; }, auricular canal ; 
¢, auricle; d, ventricle; ¢, bulbus cordis; /, aorta; 1-1, sino-auricular junction 
and venous valves; 2-2, canalo-auricular junction ; 3-3, annular part of auricle ; 
4—4, invaginated part of auricle ; 5, bulbo-ventricular junction. 


mammalian heart, since in them one might expect the heart 
rhythm to arise. 

The sinus venosus is represented in the mammalian heart by 
four remnants :—(1) The termination of the superior vena cava 
(the right duct of Cuvier). (2) The coronary sinus (the left duct 
of Cuvier), (3) A stratum submerged beneath auricular tissue at 
the tenia terminalis. (4) The remnants of the venous valves, 
i.e. the Thebesian and Eustachian valves. 

This does not represent a large amount of tissue, and it is diffi- 
cult to trace in the mammalian heart. I would, however, draw 
attention to the occurrence in all mammalian hearts examined 
of a remarkable remnant of primitive fibres persisting at the sino- 


' - "ee BR Soy 
cad . a oe 
(ee 


38 THE HEART E 


auricular junction—that is, where the superior vena cava joins the 
tenia terminalis of the right auricle (beneath a, Fig. 4). This corre- 
sponds in position to the right venous valve of the sinus venosus 
of the primitive heart. The remnant has been termed the “ sino- 
auricular node.” It is interesting because it is in close muscular 
connection (1) with the outer wall of the auricle; (2) with the 
interauricular septum. In the latter fibres pass from the node 
down the septum to another remnant of primitive fibres at the 
base of the septum known as the auriculo-ventricular or A-V 
node. To this we shall refer again, but it is worthy of note that 
the two nodes, identical in structure, and therefore probably in 
function, are in muscular connection with each other. 

The sino-auricular node has a special blood supply, and the 
nerves in the neighbourhood come into intimate relationship with 
it (Keith and Flack). 

The Auricular Canal of the Human Heart.—It will beseen | 
that in the simple form of vertebrate heart (Fig. 1) the auricular 
canal consists of three parts :— 

(1) A basal part opposite the auricle. 

(2) An annular part or “auricular ring ” (3.3). 

(3) An invaginated or intraventricular part (4.4). 

The basal part is the ventral wall of the primitive cardiac tube. 
The auricle (c) has developed from the dorsal wall alone, leaving 
the ventral wall unspecialised. The basal wall is therefore con- 
tinuous with— 

(a) The sinus venosus. 

(b) With the ostium of the auricle. 

(c) With the auricular ring. 

Does this continuity persist in the mammalian heart? In this 
heart the basal wall has become profoundly modified owing to 
the formation of an interauricular septum and a vestibule to the 
left auricle. Both these structures have been developed from 
the primitive basal wall. 

The “auricular ring” is that portion of the auricular canal 
interposed between the auricle and the ventricle (3.3, Fig. 1),-and 
in these hearts is of comparatively appreciable dimensions. In 
the mammalian heart, however, it is represented by a small 
but nevertheless important’remnant. This is submerged in the 
- auriculo-ventricular groove at the junction of the auricles and 
ventricles. The shape of “the ring,” however, has become 


é 


modified owing to the extension of the bases of the ventricles 
backwards under the basal wall of the auricular canal. By this 
means the mesial fold has come to rest at the base of the inter- 
auricular septum on the right side just about the top of the inter- 
ventricular septum. Most of the fibres representing the auricular 
part have become indistinguishable from the other auricular 
tissue, but the circular fibres of the auriculo-ventricular groove 
may be held to represent them. However at the spot referred 
to above, namely, at the base of the interauricular septum on the 
right side, a portion of the ring has remained undifferentiated. This 
is the node of tissue termed the “ auriculo-ventricular or A-V node,” 
and which is similar in structure to the “ sino-auricular node.” 

The invaginated portion of the auricular canal is of interest, 
since in the lower type of heart (Fig. 1, 4.4) it forms a muscular 
connection between the auricular and ventricular portions of 
the heart. Has this invaginated portion any homologue in the 
mammalian heart, since if it have then a muscular connection 
must exist between the auricles and ventricles of the mammalian 
heart ? For a long time this question was answered in the negative. 
The anatomists taught that in the mammalian heart the auricles 
were absolutely separated from the ventricles by fibrous tissue, 
so that no such muscular connection could exist. In 1893, how- 
ever, His, jun., described a muscular connection between the 
auricles and ventricles. Stanley Kent also in the same year 
came to the conclusion that the auricle and ventricle were con- 
nected by muscle. Recently in 1904 Retzer and also Briiunig 
corroborated the observation of His in certain mammalian hearts, 
but not in all. Tawara in 1905 published a most elaborate and 
accurate account of this muscular connection, its extensive nature 
and its connection with the Purkinje fibres. Both he and other 
observers have found it in all the mammalian hearts examined, 
so that now there appears to be no doubt that such a muscular 
connection exists in all mammalian hearts. Keith and the writer 
have shown that, as might be expected, it is the homologue of the 
invaginated portion of the auricular ring. As later we shall have 


well to describe it in some detail. 


_ The Muscular Connection between Auricle and Ven- 
tricle in the Mammalian Heart. The Auriculo-Ventricular 


. 


THE HEART 39° 


occasion to refer to this muscular connection, it will perhaps be - 


V) Bundle.—This connection is sometimes called after His, 


Sh 


40 THE HEART 


who first described it, the bundle of His; but it is perhaps simpler 
to term it the auriculo-ventricular or A-V bundle. The bundle, 
as first shown by Tawara, consists of four portions :— 

(1) The auriculo-ventricular or A-V node. 

(2) The main bundle. 

(3) The septal divisions, right and left. 

(4) The terminal ramifications. 

These divisions can be followed in some hearts better than in others ; 
for instance in the hearts of the sheep and ox it is a matter of great 
ease, since the fibres constituting the bundle are much paler than 
those of the surrounding musculature, and therefore easier to dissect 
out. These hearts, therefore, are recommended for preliminary 
dissections of the bundle. Microscopically also the fibres of the 
bundle present a greater contrast to the rest of the musculature in 
these hearts, so that the course and structure of the bundle are 
more easily followed through a series of sections, a matter of con- 
siderable difficulty at first in the human heart where the fibres 
are less differentiated. 

Taking the human heart, however, as a type, it may be said 
that the auriculo-ventricular node lies, as described above, at the 
base of the interauricular septum on the right side (3, Fig. 2). It 
is in close connection (1) with the fibres of the interauricular septum 
and thus indirectly with the sino-auricular node; (2) with the 
right auricle proper by means of the circular fibres of the A-V 
groove. A good guide to its position is the coronary sinus (8, 
Fig. 2). The node lies below and to the right. 

Arising from the A-V node is the main bundle (2, Fig. 2). 
This rides along the top of the interventricular septum below 
the pars membraneza septi—a spot easily found in the human 
heart by holding the organ up to the light. The knife may be 
safely entered through this spot, and the isolation of the bundle 
thereby facilitated. At this point the main bundle divides into 
its right and left septal divisions (Fig. 2). These divisions turn 
downwards. on the interventricular septum, and make for the 
septal groups of musculi papillares. On the right side the 
division is in the form of a fairly fine cord, and may run part 
of its course embedded in the tissue of the septum; but it 
usually becomes superficial as it approaches the septal group of 
musculi papillares, and it can be invariably found in that position 
' (Fig. 2, 4). On the left side close inspection of the septum will 


a 


THE HEART 41 


reveal this division of the bundle as a delicate flattened ribbon 
of fibres passing downward directly beneath the endocardium. 
The ribbon soon breaks up into smaller strands which pass to the 
musculi papillares situated on the septum (4, 6, Fig. 3). 

Arising from the groups of musculi papillares in either chamber 
are the terminal ramifications of the bundle. They frequently, 
especially on the right side, take the form of small moderator 
bands, and pass out to all parts of the ventricular wall. Here 


Fic. 2.—Right auricle and ventricle of calf (Keith). 1, Central cartilage ; 
2, main bundle; 3, A-V node; 4, right septal division; 5, moderator band; 
8, orifice of coronary sinus. 


they can be seen as delicate trabecule passing from one part to 


another. Eventually they fuse with the ventricular musculature. 


With regard to the microscopic appearances of the various 
parts of the bundle, Tawara in his book gives drawings of them for 
the sheep’s heart. In such hearts they are quite easy to recognise. 
The chief points to be noted are—(1) The peculiar branched cells of 
the A-V node. (2) The large pale cells of the main bundle with 
their large nuclei. (3) The peculiar Purkinje cells found in the 
septal divisions, and their terminal ramifications (especially in a so- 


called moderator band). 


42 THE HEART 


In the human heart the different portions of the bundle are not 
so easy to recognise microscopically. With a little practice, however, 
the bundle can be traced through a series of sections in its course 


Fic, 3.—Left ventricle of calf (Keith). 1, Left septal division of A-V bundle ; 
2, 3, subaortic musculature divided to show passage of bundle from right side of 
heart ; 4, 5, branches of \left septal division ; 6, free muscular ‘‘ moderator” bands 
containing prolongations\ of bundle; 9, left auricle; 10, aorta; 13, pulmonary 
artery. 
from auricle to ventricle. It will be found that the node is made 
up of closely interwoven fibres and a certain amount of fibrous 
tissue. The main bundle is completely separated from the re- 
maining musculature by fibrous tissue, and is made up of paler 


THE HEART 43 


staining fibres with larger nuclei.. The septal divisions are re- 
 eognised by similar differences. In the moderator band of a 
human heart true Purkinje cells are not usually found, but the 
terminal ramifications can be easily seen. The greatest help in 
' tracing the bundle is a knowledge of its course as determined 
by repeated dissections. This having been obtained, the “lie” 
of a section will be more readily appreciated, and the probable 
site of the A-V bundle located. 

The Auricles of the Human Heart.—In the primitive heart 


Fic. 4.—To show the antagonistic action of the musculatures of the right 
auricle and ventricle (Keith). A, the position of the A-V groove at the end of 
auricular systole ; B, its position at the end of ventricular systole. 


depicted in Fig. 1 it is seen that the common auricle is a well- 
marked outgrowth from the dorsal wall of the auricular canal. 
Its ostium is indicated by a ring of thick circular musculature 
_ (Fig. 1, 2.2). In the mammalian heart the development of the 
basal wall of the auricular canal has led toa separation of the two 
parts of the true auricle, namely, the appendices of the right and 
left auricles. Their original continuity, however, is still preserved 
_ by a ridge of musculature passing from the right auricle in front 
_ of the termination of the superior vena cava to the left auricle. 


44 THE HEART 


The right auricle of the human heart, therefore, consists of 
musculature from three sources—(1) Sinus venosus. (2) Auricular 
canal. (3) Auricle proper. 

The left auricle is composed of musculature from—(l) The 
auricular canal. (2) The auricle proper. (3) Possibly sinus 
venosus. All these parts are in the freest muscular continuity. 

The Ventricles of the Human Heart.—The main mass of these 
is true ventricle, although, as we have shown above, the invaginated 


Fis. 5.—The heart from behind, showing the arrangement of the musculature 
of the left auricle and ventricle (Keith). A’, the auricular base of the left ventricle 
in systole of the auricle ; A, its position in ventricular systole. 


portion of the auricular canal (4.4, Fig. 1) is represented by the 
A-V bundle. The single ventricle of the primitive heart is a 
diverticulum from the ventral wall of the primitive cardiac tube. 
The double chamber of the mammalian heart is homologous with 
it. The question of how the septum arose has, however, been 
differently answered. The old idea was that the interventricular 
septum grew up from the apex and thus divided the common 
cavity. This view is undoubtedly incorrect. What really happens 
is that the two ventricles are developed side by side from the 


— EEO — —— 


THE HEART 45 


-yentral wall of the primitive tube. The fusion of their adjacent 


walls forms the interventricular septum. The top part of this 
septum, therefore, represents the part of the primitive tube which 
has been least disturbed by the evolution of the ventricles. Now 
it is particularly interesting to note that it is at this point only in 
the mammalian heart that the invaginated portion of the auricular 
canal has persisted in the form of the A-V bundle. This point 
alone proves the mode of development of the interventricular 
septum. Further, not only has the bundle been subjected to the 
least possible amount of disturbance by this process of develop- 
ment, but in the adult heart it is undoubtedly better protected 
here than in any other possible situation (Keith and Flack). From 
this one infers that the A-V bundle is likely to vary but little 


in its course and must have a very valuable function to perform, 


since it is so well guarded both during development and in its 
final form. 

The Bulbus Cordis.—In Fig. 1 it will be seen that a fifth 
chamber exists in the primitive vertebrate heart, the bulbus cordis. 
This chamber is generally supposed to be absent in the mammalian 
heart, but the recent researches of Greil and of Keith render it 
probable that this is not the case. The infundibulum of the right 
ventricle is the homologue of this portion of the heart. Of the 
original musculature of the bulbus probably but little if any is 
left, it having become replaced entirely or, for the greater part by 
that of the ventricle proper. There is therefore, as in the primitive 
form, the freest muscular continuity in this part of the mam- 
malian heart. This being so, we see that as in the primitive 
cardiac tube we have in the mammalian heart free muscular 
continuity from one end of the organ to the other—from the 
representative of the sinus venosus to that of the bulbus cordis. 


C. Tae Nervous ELEMENTS OF THE VERTEBRATE HEART 


- The vertebrate heart is undoubtedly very rich in nervous 
elements. As considerable stress is laid by some observers upon 
this fact, it is important to ascertain as far as possible their dis-- 
tribution. These elements may be classified as (a) ganglion cells ; 
(b) nerve fibres and nerve endings. The ganglion cells are usually 

ed as the more important, since, as we shall see, the auto- 
maticity of the heart is credited to them by some physiologists. 


ae 


46 THE HEART 


Since, however, Apathy and Bethe make a similar claim for nerve 
fibres, their distribution has now an added significance. 

The nerve endings in the heart are both sensory and motor. 
There is some doubt about the exact distribution of the sensory 
fibres. It is held by some physiologists that they do not supply 
the heart at all but only the aorta; others, however, believe that 
they end as tree-like expansions very like those in fascia and 
tendons, in both the epicardium and endocardium. According to 
Smirnow, all the sensory fibres run in the depressor branch of 
the vagus, since after section of this nerve in the cat no sensory 
nerve endings can be detected. 

The motor nerve endings come from both the vagus and the 
sympathetic nerves. Gerlach states that the motor nerves accom; 
pany the fibrous tissue among the heart muscle and finally end 
as a delicate peri-muscular layer embracing the muscle fibres. 
Heymann and Demoor claim that every heart fibre is surrounded — 
by a nervous network right down to the apex of the heart. 

In regard to the ganglion cells of the heart, Dogiel has divided 
them into three types. The differences between the types are 
mainly in the shape and size of the nucleus, the length of axon 
and the number and form of the dendrites. The distribution of 
the ganglion cells is a matter of prime importance. It is certain 
that the auricular part of the heart is rich in these cells, which 
are often arranged in groups corresponding to Remak’s, Ludwig’s, 
and Bidder’s ganglia in the frog’s heart. These are respectively 
situated at the sino-auricular junction, on the interauricular septum 
and round the A-V groove. The distribution of ganglion cells in 
the ventricle is a vexed point. Dogiel and his pupils find that 
they undoubtedly occur in the upper third: of this chamber. 
The lower two-thirds is, usually speaking, ganglion-free. This is 
true for the hearts of such animals as the sheep, calf, dog, sucking 
pig, duck, turkey, and chicken (Kasem-Beck). Ganglion cells have 
also been found in the ventricle of the rabbit and ape (Vignal), and 
of the mouse (Berkley), and in the ventricle and bulbus of the 
frog (Dogiel). 

Yet against this view such authorities as Engelmann, His, 
Krehl and Romberg state that the ventricle contains no ganglion 
cells either in warm-blooded or cold-blooded animals. In many 
_ cases it appears to turn upon the interpretation placed. upon 
certain histological appearances. For instance, Engelmann claims 


THE HEART 47 


to have proved that cells which Léwit termed nervous, were really 
not nervous but endothelial. Another investigator, Schwartz, con- - 
cludes that the so-called ganglion cells seen in the endocardium 
and epicardium of the ventricle are really akin to if not actually 
-mast-cells. Recently also Bethe claims to have demonstrated by 
his methylene-blue method that ganglion cells exist at the apex 
of the ventricle. He admits that they differ slightly from what is 
regarded as the normal type of cell, but he believes that they are 
true ganglion cells. As a critic says, many people would call this 
ganglion cell a connective tissue cell. 

The question of the distribution of both ganglion cells and 
nerve fibres is therefore in a somewhat chaotic state. More work 
is required—it is of the greatest importance to know whether 
every muscle fibre has a nerve network surrounding it, and also 
whether ganglion cells exist throughout the heart. Lastly, it may 
be asked—Do nerve fibres exist in the A-V bundle? The question 
must be answered in the affirmative. It is said to contain nerve 
fibres and a few ganglion cells. Fredericq does not believe that 
nerve fibres actually pass through the bundle, and brings as 
proof evidence which we quote later on. He also states that he 
has histological proof of this, but I have not been able to find 
this piece of work. 


D. Tue Heart as Aa MUSCLE 


The chief properties of the heart should be studied on that 
part of the heart which contains no nerves. From what has been 
written in the previous section it will be obvious that this is a 
matter of no little difficulty. In determining these properties, 
however, most experiments have been made upon the apical part 
of the ventricle, on the assumption that this contains no nervous 
tissue. Although we shall have occasion to refer from time to 
time to the property of automaticity possessed by the heart, we 
shall not in this section discuss in which tissue, muscular or nervous, 
_ that property resides, but shall leave it until later, when we shall _ 
_ consider this question together with the mode of conduction of ~ 
the excitatory wave which arises as the result of this property 
of automaticity. 
___ The properties of cardiac muscle may be studied either when 
the heart is at rest or when it is beating. We shall consider the 


48 THE HEART 


former condition first, since it corresponds more closely to the 
conditions under which the other forms of muscle are studied. 


The heart may be reduced to this state of rest by four methods :—.— 


(1) By placing it in physiological saline and waiting for it to 
cease beating, the cardiac muscle being excitable for a variable 
period after such stoppage. This method is more appropriate for 
the hearts of warm-blooded than of cold-blooded animals, since 
the latter may continue beating for days under these conditions. 

(2) By shutting off the part of the heart possessing the greatest 
automatic power. This is done in the Stannius ligature experi- 
ment, more usually on the frog’s heart, in which a ligature is tied 
round the sino-auricular junction. The same result can also be 
obtained by cutting away the more automatic parts of the heart— 
in other words, by making a ventricle preparation only. 


(3) By stimulating the vagus most hearts can be reduced to 


a standstill. 

(4) By certain drugs, such as muscarine, a like condition is 
obtained. 

It must be stated, however, that under conditions 3 and 4 
the normal properties of cardiac muscle are greatly altered, and 
therefore the methods are unsuitable. Preference must therefore 
be given to methods 1 and 2. 

The ‘preparation being obtained, it is found that cardiac muscle, 
like other forms of muscle, possesses the properties of excitability 
and contractility. It responds to a single stimulus by a single 
contraction. The stimulus may be either mechanical, thermal, 
chemical, or electrical. The last is most commonly chosen, but it 
is interesting to note with regard to the chemical stimuli that the 
heart muscle responds to the stimuli for muscle but not to those 
for nerve. Thus ammonia, dilute lime water, dilute mineral acids 
when applied to the apex of a frog’s ventricle excite contraction or 
a number of contractions. These, according to Kiihne, do not 
excite motor nerves. The potent nerve stimulant, glycerine, on 
the other hand, fails to excite a single contraction from such a 
preparation. 

Different parts of the heart possess different degrees of ex- 
citability. This power of excitability, moreover, does not run 
parallel with the degree of automatism possessed by the same 
part of the heart. As an instance, the heart of the embryo chick 
at three days possesses great automatic power, but only a small 


- THE HEART 49 


‘degree of excitability to artificial stimuli; later the automatism, | 
specially that of the ventricle, decreases, while its excitability not- 
ably increases. Similarly the auricle possesses at this time a greater 
power of automatism than the ventricle, but the ventricle is the 
“more excitable of the two chambers (Fano). The power of con- 
- tractility also varies in different parts of the heart, being greatest 
in the ventricle, the part of the cardiac tube developed for pro- 
_ pulsive work.. But in dying this power is preserved longer in the 
auricle than in the ventricle, the parts of the right auricle around 
the superior vena cava and round the coronary sinus being the 
last to lose this property. 

; The muscle curve obtained as the result of the application of 
a stimulus corresponds to that of an ordinary muscle. There is 
the period of delay, the period of contraction, and the period of 
relaxation. In point of time it approaches more nearly to that 
of smooth muscle ; the latent period is well marked, and the period 
of contraction is considerably slower than that of the sartorius 
or of the gastrocnemius of the same animal. As with smooth 
muscle, the heart appears to be more easily stimulated by gradual 
than by momentary shocks. 

As with skeletal muscle, the contraction’ of the heart can 
be recorded under two conditions, namely, the isotonic and the 
isometric. This has been done more especially by O. Frank. The 
conditions in the heart, however, are not directly comparable 
with those in the skeleial muscle. In the latter, under isotonic 
conditions the actual shortening of the muscle under a constant 
weight is measured ; in the heart, owing to the anatomical arrange- 
ment of its fibres, it is impossible to do this. The isotonic curve 
is therefore obtained by measuring the alteration in the size of 
_ the cavity of the ventricle which occurs as the result of this shorten- 
‘ing of the fibres. This is done by introducing a sound into the 
_ ventricle and connecting it with a tambour. The isometric curve 
| bed skeletal muscle is obtained by preventing the muscle from 
shortening while the change in its tension is measured. The 
allied condition is brought about in the heart by causing it to 
contract against an insuperable obstacle, such as a tap introduced 
> the circuit. The tension of the ventricle wall alters under 
» conditions, but the length of the fibres remains the same. 
is alteration i in tension is measured by a manometer inserted 
n the ventricle and the insuperable obstacle. 


D 


Bits 


a 4 =e *. 


ae Te 
-o 
+ 
‘ ‘= 
~ ’ ae 


50 THE HEART 


The skeletal muscle can also be studied under the combined 
condition, namely, when “after-loaded.” Here the condition of 
the muscle is isometric while altering in tension to overcome the 
load, and isotonic after this load has been overcome and the muscle 
begins to shorten. A similar state of affairs prevails in the heart. 
The ventricle contracts under isometric conditions upon its load 
of blood until the semilunar valves open, thenceforward under 
isotonic conditions whilst its fibres shorten and its load is dis- 
charged. The heart therefore corresponds normally to an after- 
loaded muscle. Frank showed by his studies that the laws which 
govern the contraction of skeletal muscle under these conditions 
apply equally to the heart. Thus, as in skeletal muscle, the iso- 
metric curve culminates before the isotonic. He found also that 
the maximum of the isometric curve of the ventricle increases with 
increasing tension up to a certain point, and then decreases. This 


means that the contraction of the ventricle, as is well known, in- - 


creases in force with the amount of its filling up to a certain point, 
but after that it decreases and the ventricle dilates. Thus for 
the frog’s heart Frank found the following figures :— 


Volume incem. in ventricle 0 ‘18 ‘34 ‘47 ‘63 ‘84 ‘93 
Tension mm. of mercury .12 60 68 66 60 59 58 


During the isotonic curve the change of tension varies with 
the load. Increasing load defers the beginning of the contraction, 
shortens its duration, and diminishes the velocity of the move- 
ment of the free end of the muscle. Similarly in the ventricle, 
increased load—that is, increased resistance to the outflow of blood 
from the ventricle—defers the beginning of the period of expulsion, 
the .opening of the semilunar valves, shortens the period until 
their closure, and diminishes the rate of flow of blood through 
the aortic orifice, and consequently lessens the systolic discharge. 
As an example from experiments upon the frog’s ventricle, if re- 
sistance increases from 10 mm. to 40 mm. of mercury, the flow 
decreases from -06 to ‘02 per second, and the period of expulsion 
diminishes from 56 second to ‘51 second, and the systolic dis- 
charge from ‘33 cc. to ‘08 cc. This method of studying the 
ventricle under both isometric and isotonic conditions gives valu- 
able information in working out the effects of drugs upon the 
heart muscle. 

So far we have spoken of the similarity between cardiac 


a 
— 


THE HEART 51 


and other forms of muscle as regards its response to a single 
imulus. But there is this remarkable difference to be noted, 
that whereas in other forms the response varies in force according 
to the intensity of the stimulus, cardiac muscle responds with a 
- maximal contraction to all efficient stimuli, be they minimal or 
“maximal. This is sometimes termed “the all or nothing law” ; 
therefore once a heart responds to a stimulus it is of no avail to 
increase the intensity of that stimulus. It should be noted, how- 
ever, that heart muscle may give in response to the first few stimu- 
lations gradually increasing contractions, thereby manifesting the 
so-called ‘“‘Treppe or staircase phenomenon.” This is explained 
on the supposition that the first few contractions render the tissue 
more excitable to that form of stimulation. | 
Another peculiarity manifested by the heart muscle is in re- 
sponse to rhythmic stimulation. If a ventricle preparation, giving 
but one contraction to one stimulation, be treated in this way by 
single induction shocks, it starts to pulsate in regular fashion, 
but the frequency of beat is always less than the stimulation fre- 
quency. The number of contractions obtained by this method 
can be increased either by increasing the frequency of stimula- 
tion or the intensity of current with the same rate of stimulation. 
The same phenomenon can be produced upon the ventricle by 
a constant current of appropriate intensity, the normal response 
being twenty to thirty regular beats, after which it ceases. The 
ventricle can also be made to give a similar rhythmic response 
by injecting blood or normal saline into it at a suitable pressure. 
Certain drugs, such as delphinin, are said to cause a series of 
_ rhythmic contractions. 
| A further peculiarity of cardiac muscle is that it cannot: be 
tetanised. It is impossible to bring about a summation of stimuli 
in the normal heart. This, however, is said to be possible on the 
‘muscarine-poisoned heart, and on the heart stopped by excitation 
of the vagus. 
_ Thus far we have discussed the effect of stimulation upon 
ag at rest. When we come to study the effect upon the 
x heart, certain other remarkable differences from skeletal 
a tle are revealed. For instance, if a stimulus be applied to 
the rhythmically beating ventricle just before or during systole 
is without any visible effect. The heart muscle is therefore 
id to possess “‘a refractory period,” and the possession of this 


* 
- 


52 THE HEART 


property explains the inability to tetanise it. If, however, the 
stimulus be applied during diastole, a contraction is produced 
which is known as an “extra-systole.” Such extra-systoles are 
followed by a longer pause than normal. This is called the “ com- 
pensatory pause.” It is about equal in length to the pause fol- 
lowing the normal beat plus the amount cut off from the previous 
pause by the induction of the extra-systole. The ventricle there- 
fore, owing to this property, makes but the same number of systoles 
as usual in a given time. This is known as the “law of con- 
servation of rhythm to physiological stimuli” (Engelmann). 
The length of the compensatory pause is due to the refractory 
period ; the impulse causing the normal contraction reaches the 
ventricle while it is in a state of systole from the artificial stimulus, 
and it therefore has no effect. The ventricle is then not stimu- 
lated again until the next normal impulse arrives. 


If, however, the ventricle be thrown into rhythmical contrac- © 


tions by a continuous stimulus, and an extra-systole produced by 
a strong artificial stimulus, the ‘extra-systole so induced is not 
-followed by a compensatory pause, for the continuous stimulus 
still acting produces another contraction as soon as the refractory 
period of the extra-systole is passed. This is adduced as proof 
that the normal physiological stimulus is not continuous, but dis- 
continuous, inasmuch as there could be no compensatory pause 
if it were not so. 

Another important point to be noted is that the compensatory 
pause does not -follow an extra-systole induced at the ven cave. 
It therefore follows that all extra-systoles observed to be followed 
by a compensatory pause are produced by a stimulus applied to 
some other part of the heart (e.g. auricle or ventricle). It also 
follows that the stimulus is not conducted from outside to the 
venee cave, but actually arises there. 

Like other forms of muscle, the heart muscle is said to possess 
the property of tonicity This is masked in part by the incom- 
plete relaxation of the heart between the beats, since as Gaskell 
points out the degree of relaxation depends not only upon the 
tonicity of the muscle, but also upon the rate of beat, which is 
usually such that the heart muscle probably never completely re- 


laxes. An alteration in tone can therefore only be manifested — 


when the rate of-beat remains the same. The little but distinct 
evidence that exists for this change is derived from the action of 


Ka 


x 
Fr 
- 


THE HEART 53 


_ eertain solutions and drugs upon the beating ventricle. If a frog’s 
ventricle be placed in a weak solution of caustic soda (1 in 20,000 
of normal saline) it relaxes less and less between the beats and 
eventually stands still in systole, whereas if lactic acid (1-10,000 
- normal saline) be used the contractions become less and less, so 
that finally the ventricle stops in a state of complete relaxation. 
Similar results to that with alkali can be obtained by such drugs 
as digitalis and veratrine, and to that of acid with muscarine. 
Moreover, acid solutions antagonise the effect of alkaline on the 
same preparation, as does muscarine that of digitalis. The heart 
muscle, therefore, would appear to be possessed of a certain degree 
of tonicity which, like the other properties of cardiac muscle, 
probably varies in the different parts of the heart. 
_ Another peculiar form of tonicity has been observed by Fano 
(quoted by Gaskell) upon the heart of Emys Europea. If this 
heart be clamped in the A-V groove, the auricles exhibit a 
rhythmical variation of tone. A similar phenomenon has been 
seen by Botazzi in the frog’s heart. It apparently does not occur 
in many cold-blooded animals. At present a satisfactory explana- 
tion is wanting. bi 

Lastly, the heart possesses the properties of automaticity, 
rhythmicity, and stimulus conduction. Do these properties reside 
in the heart muscle itself or in the nervous tissue abounding in 
the heart? This is the vexed een which we discuss in the 
owing section. 


EZ. Tue Sire or ORIGIN AND THE MopE or CONDUCTION OF 
THE EXxcITAToRY WAVE 


By the site of origin is meant the kind of tissue, muscular or 
nervous, in which the heart impulse arises. A long controversy 
has raged around this point, and much fruitful research has been 
_ the outcome. It is necessary to keep clearly in mind the difference 
_ between the site of origin of the excitatory wave and the mode 


in regard to the site of origin of the excitatory wave are quoted 
“as evidence as to its mode of conduction. Certainly it must be 
granted that if the excitatory wave be found to arise in one form 
ssue ae is highly probable that it will also be conducted by 
tissue ; but it is not necessarily the case. There are several 


_ of its conduction. These are frequently confused, and experiments —— 


Pt 


54 THE HEART 


possibilities. The excitatory wave, for instance, may conceivably 


arise in nerve and be conducted by nerve. This is the neurogenic 
theory. Or the excitatory wave may arise in the heart muscle 
and be conducted by muscle. This is the myogenic theory. These 
certainly seem more probable than either (1) that the excitatory 
wave may arise in nerve and be conducted by muscle; or (2) 
that it may arise in muscle and be conducted by nerve. Even 
here the possibilities do not cease, for the co-ordination of the 
movements of the different chambers of the heart is essentially 
a complicated yet all-important process, so that it may well 
be that, arise the excitatory wave how it may, both muscle and 
nerve may be called into play as conducting agents in order that 
the proper sequence of contraction of the different chambers of the 
heart may be assured. 


Taking the first possibility, that the excitatory wave may arise _ 


in nervous tissue and be conducted by that tissue, let us consider 
the evidence for and against it. This view gained sway in the 
middle of the last century chiefly perhaps because it offered a 
satisfactory explanation of the great discovery by the brothers 
Weber of the action of the vagus nerve upon the heart. At that 
time such an inhibitory influence could only be explained on the 
analogy of nervous influence of centres situated in the central 
nervous system, such as the respiratory centre of the medulla and 
the motor centres of the spinal cord. The ganglion cells, there- 
fore, were thought to be the central apparatus of the heart, 
sending out a continuous stimulus to the heart muscle. Different 
afferent impulses to this central apparatus modified the stimulus 
in different ways. The very plausibility of the view immediately 
accorded it a place as a theory without any very substantial 
evidence. Its chief support is the experiment of Stannius, now 
known as the Stannius ligature experiment. If in the frog’s heart a 
ligature be placed around the sino-auricular groove and tied tightly, 
the auricle and ventricle of the heart are reduced to a standstill ; 
in consequence, it is asserted, of the cutting off of the influence 
of Remak’s ganglia in this neighbourhood. The sinus, however, 
continues to beat. If now a ligature be placed round the 
A-V groove and tied, the ventricle again starts to beat, owing, 
it is said, to the stimulation of Bidder’s ganglia in this region. 
Bidder’s ganglia are therefore looked upon as a subsidiary 
centre normally under the control of Remak’s at the sinus. In 


‘ 


THE HEART 55 


further support of this view Kaiser claims to have shown that, 
after extirpation of Bidder’s ganglia, this second ligature no longer 
has any effect in starting the ventricle. Now a stimulus applied 
is followed by only one contraction instead of by a series of con- 
tractions. We shall refer to this experiment again later on. 
Kronecker and Schmey claim that when a needle is thrust into 
a certain spot in the dog’s ventricle, the heart immediately falls 
into fibrillary contractions as far as the ventricles are concerned. 
The needle is thrust into the ventricular septum at the lower end 
of the upper third. The experiment frequently fails. It is 
adduced, however, by its authors as evidence of the neurogenic 
theory. 
The experiments of Carlson upon the heart of a horse-shoe crab 
(Limulus) undoubtedly afford evidence of neurogenic origin of a 
heart beat. The heart in this case consists of a tube 10 to 15 cm. 
long, divided into segments by the origin of arteries. During 
systole all the parts appear to contract simultaneously, although 
it is probable that there is really a rapid wave of contraction. 
Three nervous strands (one median and two lateral) run along 
the heart and anastomose freely. The median contains ganglion 
cells, and one especially large ganglion. This strand can be easily 
separated from the heart without injuring the latter. Fine 
branches pass into the muscle substance. Carlson has shown that 
if the whole of this median nerve be removed, the heart imme- 
diately ceases to pulsate ; if a part only be removed, then activity 
ceases in the corresponding portion of the heart. The ganglionated 
chain therefore appears to be the site of origin of the excitatory 
wave in this heart. 
As regards the conduction of the impulse the neurogenic theory 
holds that it is conducted by nerves. The delay in the passage 
from auricle to ventricle is said to arise in Bidder’s ganglia. 
The chief points in the evidence brought forward for nervous 
conduction are— 
(1) The assertion by Kronecker that he has produced allo- 
_ thythmia- (inco-ordination of auricle and ventricle) by cutting a 
nerve running between auricle and ventricle. 
_ (2) At first the fact that in the mammalian heart no muscular 
_ connection was known to exist between auricle and ventricle. For 
instance, MacWilliam, as the result of his researches, came to the 
aclusion that in the mammalian heart, at any rate, the mode 


56 THE HEART 


of conduction from auricle to ventricle must be nervous, as he 
could find no muscular path. Since the discovery of the A-V bundle 
this piece of negative evidence is of less value; but the fact that 
the bundle contains nerve fibres is insisted upon by the upholders 
of the neurogenic theory. This point we shall deal with when 
considering the evidence of muscular conduction by the A-V 
bundle. 

(3) In the heart of Limulus Carlson has shown that section of 
the nervous strand immediately abolishes the synchronism of the 
different parts. The parts on either side of the cut continue to 
pulsate, but with a different rhythm. This points to nervous 
conduction of the master rhythm. Further, it is interesting to 
note that in this heart, so long as the nerves are intact, section of 
the muscle produces no inco-ordination whatever. 

In estimating the value of these experiments of Carlson it 
must be pointed out that the muscle tissue of this invertebrate 
heart differs very materially in its properties from that of the 
mammalian heart, being in fact much more akin to mammalian 
smooth muscle. It possesses no refractory period, gives sub- 
maximal contractions, and is capable of tetanisation. 

It was in 1882 that the first serious criticism of the neurogenic 
theory was advanced. Gaskell came to the conclusion, as the 
result of his experiments upon the heart of the turtle, that in cold- 
blooded animals the heart’s excitatory wave arises in the heart 
muscle itself and is conducted by it. He showed that if the 
ventricle be warmed its beat was not quickened, whereas warm- 
ing of the sinus caused a quickening of the whole rhythm of the 
heart. This therefore proved that the stimulus is discontinuous 
and not continuous, as was thought before. But were the nerve 
cells concerned in the origin of the excitatory wave? Gaskell 
found that if the Stannius ligature be applied to the turtle’s heart, 
the effect at first is essentially the same as with the frog’s heart. 
Soon, however, the auricle starts beating with its own rhythm, 
slower than that of the sinus, and the ventricle follows it. If 
now the second ligature be applied, the ventricle stops for a 
moment, but quickly resumes beating with its own rhythm, which 
is slower than that of the auricle. Therefore in the same heart it 


» 


: 
i 


is possible to obtain the sinus, auricle, and ventricle beating with a 


different rhythms. It seemed that in such a case as this it would 
be strange for nerves to set a different rhythm in different parts 


* 
’ « 


THE HEART 57 


of the same heart; and, knowing the muscle of the heart to vary 
in these different portions, it was more probable that the difference 
in power of originating the automatic rhythm lay in the heart 
muscle itself. This was proved to be the case. Gaskell found that 
small strips of muscle from the different parts of the heart, so small 
as to contain no ganglion cells, show the same power of developing 
different rhythms ; a piece of ventricle, for instance, from a well- 
nourished animal beating for thirty hours. Gaskell, as we have 
said, explains the different degrees of automatism by the variation 
in the type of muscle. The more “ embryonic ”’—that is, approach- 
ing the type seen in the embryo—possesses the greatest automatic 
power. This being the case, then stimulation of the musculature 
of the sinus or of the auricular ring should originate a series of 
contractions. Gaskell showed this to be so, proving conclusively 
at the same time that the experiment of Kaiser in regard to the 
function of Bidder’s ganglia was incorrect. 

Gaskell’s result may be summarised as follows :-— 


Stimulation of auricle or ventricle . =. One contraction. 
Stimulation of Bidder’s ganglia ; - No contraction. 
Stimulation of the auricular ring. . Series of contractions, 


Consequently the ring musculature and not the ganglia were con- 
cerned in originating the rhythm. Further support in this direc- 
‘tion came from Munk, who also showed that a series of contractions 
was the normal response to the excitation of this musculature. 
Ewald found and confirmed by microscopic examination that in 
only two cases out of twenty-nine were the ganglion cells or nerves 
injured as the result of the excitation of the ring musculature, 
which, however, in all cases responded with a series of contractions. 
In the twenty-five years following Gaskell’s first work much 
has been done in endeavouring to support the myogenic con- 
tention. Engelmann and his pupils have been particularly 
strenuous in upholding it. During this time, however, the neuro- 
_ genic school have provided but little new evidence for their some- 
what slenderly-founded theory, their chief work consisting in 
_ endeavours to refute the evidence brought forward by ‘the 
_ myogenic school. The chief points which have been offered as 
_ evidence are :— 

(1) Engelmann isolated portions of the great veins said to 
contain no ganglion cells and showed that they beat automati- 


—— 


| a 
‘ > . 
‘ . 
‘ » a’ 


58 THE HEART 


cally; a small piece (2 cmm.) of the sinus of a frog beat for 
four days and recorded 17,000 contractions. 

(2) In certain molluscs, arthropods, and tunicates, the heart 
undoubtedly contains no ganglion cells but possesses automatic 
rhythm. 

(3) The apex of the mammalian heart, said to have no nerves 
whatever, shows slow rhythmic contractions. 

(4) The embryonic heart beats when no ganglion cells have 
invaded the heart, and before the muscle and nerve have become 


differentiated (His). In the chick, the heart of which pulsates - 


36 hours after the beginning of hatching, no ganglion cells appear 
until the sixth day ; in the human heart Pfliiger saw pulsations at 
the beginning of the third week, whereas no ganglion cells are said 
to occur until the end of the fourth or the beginning of the fifth 
week. Fano found that if he divided the heart of a chick into 
three or more parts, all the parts pulsated—the frequency being 
greater the nearer the part was to the venous end—although, as 
we have seen, the ventricle at that period was the more excitable. 
W. His has also found differences in the behaviour of different 
parts of the heart towards drugs such as muscarine and digitalis, 
the ventricle being the most affected. The inwandering of the 
ganglion cells leads to no noticeable effect on this. This being 
the case, it is difficult to believe that later on they should take 
over the function of initiating the heart rhythm. 

(5) Hearts can be revived many days after death—even the 
hearts of children dead of disease. In ten such hearts only three 
gave negative results. The heart of a boy dead of pneumonia 
revived in all parts 20 hours after death. In the case of the heart 
of an ape, Hering recovered the heart after 44 hours, and then froze 
it. After 28 hours 32 minutes the heart was again resuscitated. 
If now the ganglion cells be the site of origin, then Ringer’s fluid 
must possess the power of enabling them to recover their functions ; 
but if Ringer’s fluid have not this power, then the ganglion cells 
cannot have the power of automatism. Now Langendorff and 
others have shown that sympathetic ganglia and fibres die very 
quickly. Hering found in the rabbit that the pre-ganglionic 
cervical sympathetic was without action 15 minutes after death, 
the post-ganglionic 33 minutes after death, the vagus on the heart 


55 minutes after death. The corresponding times for the cat were 
11, 26, and 40 minutes. It was not found to be possible to restore 


\ 


. 
CC 


. 


THE HEART 59 


_ the functions of the cervical sympathetic by perfusing with Ringer’s 
solution, whereas the irritability of the vagus could be restored for 
some time, and that of the acceletor almost indefinitely. 

It is in the tissue of the sinus in the frog’s heart that the 
automatic power resides. This is the part of the heart which can 
be affected by such agents as heat and cold. Destruction of the 
sinus or cutting it off alters the rhythm of the remaining portions 
of the heart. One would expect, if the results obtained upon the 
frog’s heart be applicable to the mammalian heart, that similar 
results should be obtained by experimenting upon the parts repre- 
senting the sinus. Such indeed is the case. At the junction of 
the superior vena cava with the auricle Adam has succeeded in 
altering the mammalian heart rhythm by the application of heat 
and cold. Hering has shown that the rhythm in the dog can be 
completely changed by a cut in this region. Langendorff has 
obtained somewhat similar results with the Stannius ligature 
experiment upon the mammal. The results were not so marked 
as with the frog, the period of rest being slight, and the heart 
resuming its beat more quickly, although with a different rhythm. 

a As we have seen, the sino-auricular node is situated in this region, 
and it may well be that this is the spot acted upon in these experi- 
ments. Experimental evidence to this effect, however, is still 
wanting, and the true function of this node has yet to be worked 
out. In the meantime it has been suggested that in view of 
its embryonic structure it is kkely to possess great automatic 
properties, and that seeing how carefully it is supplied with blood, 
and how intimately the nerves of the heart come into contact 
with it, this isolated node of tissue is probably the site of origin 
of the heart’s impulse. On the other hand, also without physio- 
logical evidence, Tawara has claimed that the heart’s impulse 

_ normally arises in the A-V node, which he has termed the “ cardio- 
motor centre.” No proof of this is as yet forthcoming, but the 
view more generally held, although as yet without definite evi- 

dence, is that the A-V node must’be regarded as a subsidiary centre, 
and that it is only under certain circumstances that the heart 

-thythm is initiated there. While dealing with such hypothetical 

_ problems it may be suggested that the possible explanation of the 

effect of the first Stannius ligature is due to the cutting off of the 

| tic tissue at the sinus, ¢.e. in the case of mammalian heart 
oly the sino-auricular node. Under these circumstances the 


~~ —_——o = : 
. - bee 
J ” = 


* 
ty 


60 THE HEART 


auriculo-ventricular node after a time takes up the function of 
initiating the heart rhythm. Examples of this “nodal rhythm ” 
have been recorded clinically by Mackenzie. 

Coming now to the evidence of muscular conduction, it will 
be remembered that there is no histological reason against it, 
since the heart muscle is now regarded as a continuous network. 
The earliest experiment on cold-blooded. animals was the well- 
known zig-zag experiment of Engelmann, in which the ventricle 
of the frog is so cut that it is claimed that all conducting nerves 
must be cut and yet the impulse still passes. Gaskell also showed 
in the tortoise that section of the well-marked coronary nerve had 
no effect on the passage of the impulse, whereas the clamping 
of the muscular tissue induced varying degrees of allorhythmia 
(2A :IV, 3A: IV, 4A: IV), according to the tightness of the clamp. 
Gaskell also showed that section of the part of A-V grooves . 
containing the most nerves had no effect upon the ventricular 
rhythm. 

MacWilliam came to the conclusion that in the eel the con- 
duction of the impulse was by muscle. He also drew attention to 
the sino-ventricular rhythm, whereby the ventricle can follow the 
rhythm of the sinus without the auricle being influenced. In this 
case the impulse arising in the sinus passes down the basal wall 
(Fig. 1) and thence to the ventricle. MacWilliam thought there 
was evidence of such a rhythm in the mammalian heart. We 
may here point out that it is indeed quite possible, for the exci- 
tatory waves arising in the great veins may conceivably, under 
certain conditions, pass down the interauricular septum which 
corresponds to the basal wall, and thence to the ventricle, without 
affecting the other parts of the auricle during its passage. 

As regards the evidence of muscular conduction in the 
mammalian heart, Fredericq has brought good evidence to show 
that such is the case in the auricles. He found that the two 
auricles remained co-ordinate so long as he left a thin strip of 
muscle connecting them. It did not matter where this strip was, 
whether in neighbourhood of superior vena cava or of inferior 
vena cava or coronary sinus. When, however, he cut this strip, 
then he found that the two auricles became inco-ordinate. It is 
interesting to note, however, that the ventricle continued to beat = 
with the same rhythm as the right auricle. He argued that con- : 
duction in this case was muscular and not nervous, since the bridge 


u 
F f 
* 
\ - cy: 


THE HEART 61 


of tissue might be situated in any position. Histological investiga- 
tion proves this free muscular continuity (Keith and Flack). With 
regard to evidence of muscular conduction between auricle and 
ventricle in the mammalian heart, it is usually held that the 
A-V bundle effects this, and that the conduction by it is muscular. 
The evidence that the A-V bundle is the sole passage of conduction 
is also tolerably strong, although some investigators bring results 
which appear to the contrary. The chief evidence in favour is 
as follows :— 

(1) Hering cut in the region of the A-V bundle in four dogs, 
and in three cases out of four obtained allorhythmia. Tawara 
showed by histological investigation that in three only was the 
bundle cut ; the fourth had escaped. 

(2) Erlanger compressed the bundle in dogs by means of a 
specially-devised atriotome, and succeeded in obtaining varying 
degrees of arrhythmia and finally allorhythmia. Retzer confirmed 
histologically the damage done to the bundle. | 

(3) Humblet has successfully obtained allorhythmia in dogs on 
many occasions by ligaturing the A-V bundle. No allorhythmia 
was produced when any other part of the septum was tied. 

(4) The evidence afforded by syphilitic disease of the bundle 
in certain cases of Stokes-Adams disease. Several very satis- 
factory cases with tracings and post-mortem examination of the 
heart have been published. 

On the other hand, Kronecker states that in rabbits he is unable 
to obtain any allorhythmia by ligature of the A-V bundle, and in 
this he is confirmed by his pupil Imchanitsky. In some cases the 
bundle was certainly not tied, but in others it is claimed to have 
_ definitely been so. It may be that the heart of the rabbit affords 
an exception, since Biggs also appears to have obtained uncertain 
results upon the rabbit’s heart. An interesting point, however, is 
that Kronecker always attempted to tie the bundle through the 
left auricle, and Humblet found in dogs that he was not successful 
by this method. All the observers mentioned above attack the 
bundle through the right auricle. It is undoubtedly the more 
certain method. There is great danger of getting above “the 

_ bundle or only obtaining part of it from the left side, owing to 
_ the manner in which the left septal division comes off. 
The main evidence is undoubtedly in favour of the A-V bundle 
_ being the sole path for the transmission of the impulse from auricle 


a 
’ 


62 THE HEART 


to ventricle in the mammalian heart. But it does not necessarily 
prove muscular conduction, since the bundle contains nerve fibres 
and a few ganglion cells. Fredericq, however, states that he has 
destroyed all the nerves in the bundle and its neighbourhood by 
ammonia, but yet got no evidence of allorhythmia. He states 
also that histological evidence shows that the nervous elements 
of the bundle do not extend right through it. According to 
Fredericq the mode of conduction in the bundle is undoubtedly 
muscular. The opinion of Fredericq is weighty and all the more 
interesting since formerly he held the neurogenic view. Even now 
he holds that under certain conditions there is a nervous con- 
duction in the heart. He bases his view upon the following experi- 
ments. As long ago as 1886 he found that with feeble indirect 
shocks he got these results :— 


Both ventricles stopped. 


Applied to one ventricl ‘ ; : : ; 
PP pore Both auricles continued beating. 


Both auricles stopped. 


Applied to one auricle * ( Both ventricles continued beating. 


He came to the conclusion, therefore, that there must be some 
abnormality in the conduction in these cases, since ordinarily he 
could obtain reciprocal conduction, whereas this mode of con- 
duction affected both auricles and both ventricles, but did not 
connect auricles to ventricles. More recently he found that if he 
threw the heart into fibrillary contractions he obtained the same 
results. The fibrillary contractions arising in one auricle pass 
quickly to the other, but not to the ventricles ; similarly with the 
ventricles, from ventricle to ventricle but not to the auricles. 
Fredericq thinks that fibrillary contractions pass quickly by 
nervous means, and adduces the above experiments as evidence 
that the A-V bundle contains no nerve fibres passing throughout 
its course, since fibrillary contractions never pass from auricle to 
ventricle. On the other hand, the rhythm of the ventricle is 
normally that of the auricle, and is influenced whenever that of 
the auricle is altered. Therefore the heart impulse passes by 
muscular tissue and traverses the A-V bundle in its passage from 
auricle to ventricle. The rate of conduction of the impulse along 
the bundle is explained by the character of the muscle tissue of 
which it is composed, sincé Fano has shown that the rate of con- 
duction in cardiac muscle varies according to the stage of develop- 
ment. It would therefore seem that the difference in structure 


\ 


Ie 


THE HEART | 63 


- of the bundle in different hearts has, in a measure at least, to 
do with the rate of conduction required for that particular heart. 
In the dog’s heart Stassen finds the time taken is ‘08 to "10 of a 
second. 

To summarise, the chief points usually adduced in favour 
of muscular conduction in the vertebrate and especially in the 
mammalian heart are :— 

(1) The zig-zag experiment of Engelmann and the bridge 
experiment of Fredericq. 

(2) There is muscular connection between auricle and ventricle. 
Disturbance of conduction follows cutting or compression of this 
| connection. 

, (3) There is no effect upon stimulating or cutting the nerves 
from auricle to ventricle. 
(4) The conduction of the excitatory wave may occur after the 
| nerves have degenerated. 
: (5) The rate of conduction is more in accordancé with muscular 
conduction. 
| (6) Conduction takes place from the point of stimulation in all 


directions. 

(7) Reverse conduction occurs from ventricle to auricle. 

The balance of evidence lies in favour of the myogenic theory. 
Certain objections, however, may be urged against it. For 
example, the evidence brought from the invertebrate kingdom in 
its favour is no more applicable than that obtained upon the heart 
of Limulus. Against the zig-zag experiment may be brought the 
histological evidence of certain observers. If every fibre is sur- 
rounded by a nervous network, then that network may manifestly 
be the conducting medium. As to the rate of conduction, it has 
been shown by Nikolai and Garten that non-medullated nerves 
conduct at a rate compatible with that of the excitatory wave of 
the heart. On the other hand, Békelmann has found that the non- 

_ medullated terminals in the cornea of a dog are not appreciably 
slower in conduction than ordinary nerve. More recently Bethe 
has made.experiments on the warm-blooded heart, and concludes 
that the rate of conduction of nerve in the dog is 130 to 225 cm. 
per second—a result quite in accordance with that required for this 
heart. Bethe also states that it is difficult to induce degeneration 
of nerve in the manner usually employed, namely, by placing a 
ligature round the heart. He himself has been surprised to find 


64 THE HEART 


how difficult it is to induce degeneration in an isolated nerve 
by a tight ligature. Anatomical evidence of such degeneration 
in the heart must be brought, and at present this has not been 
furnished. 

In regard to the proofs offered for the automaticity of the 
heart residing in its muscle, considerable importance rests upon the 
evidence obtained from the embryonic heart. Here one may object 
that this embryonic tissue is probably neither muscular nor nervous. 
Bethe claims that recent methods show that from it both muscle 
and nerve cells develop. The action of muscarine upon the heart 
before the inwandering of the ganglion cells is also brought as 
evidence on this point. If this be the case, it leads on to the idea 
that the automaticity of the adult heart may possibly reside in 
a similar tissue neither muscular nor nervous. This tissue, akin 
to nerve-muscular cells from which the heart is developed, may 


be shut off at any early stage of development for this special © 


purpose of leading the automatic rhythm of the heart. May it 
not also in some ways correspond to the form of tissue in the 
myoneural junctions, in being easily affected by nervous influences 
and manifesting the effect upon the adjacent muscle. Why, more- 
over, may it not be acted upon by certain internal secretions, thus 
providing the heart with a further reflex mechanism for preserving 
the well- -being of the body as a whole ? 

Again in regard to the conduction of the impulse, it is possible 
that the conducting mechanism of the heart is developed from 
such a tissue, becoming nervous in one form of heart, muscular in 
another, according to the requirements of the organ in that special 
genus. This would explain the discrepancies in the evidence from 
the invertebrate kingdom. Then again the Purkinje fibres of the 
sheep’s heart are not seen in the human. This is probably some- 
thing to do with the requirements of co-ordination. The Purkinje 
fibres are totally different from the surrounding musculature—it 
is difficult to call them muscle; but it is easy to understand how 
such a fibre can be differentiated from a tissue capable of giving 
rise to both muscle and nerve. From such a tissue, moreover, a 
conducting mechanism can be evolved presenting different histo- 
logical appearances, but having the requisite rate of conduction 
for the co-ordination of that heart. 

The above are the chief facts in regard to both the neurogenic 
and myogenic theories. Neither theory is a dogma—one may 

\ ; 
" : 


THE HEART 65 


survive, both may pass away; but that only further anatomical 
and physiological research can show. 


F. THE MovEMENTS OF THE HEART IN SITU 


By virtue of the properties enumerated above the heart co- 
ordinates itself to meet the circulatory needs of the body. More 
research is needed before one can speak with certainty upon the 
exact movements made by the heart in the unopened thorax, 
although a considerable amount of work has been done on the 
subject (Ludwig, Haycraft, Keith). As emphasised by Keith, the 
heart has certain fixed fulcra from which it executes its normal 
movements, and the moment the thorax is opened these are taken 
away, and the true movements made by the heart are in part 
obscured. These fulcra are as follows :— 

(1) The venous mesocardium, or the part of the pericardium 
attached around the great veins (Fig. 4, over a, 7, 7, and b). 

(2) The arterial mesocardium, or the part of the pericardium 
attached around the great arteries (Fig. 4, over g and f). 

Except at these two points the heart lies absolutely free 
within the pericardium. But these mesocardia are attached to 
the surrounding structures, and it is by this means that steadiness 
is obtained. For instance, the part attached to the venous end 
is bound to three structures :— 

(1) To the root of the lungs;-and by the lungs to the wall of 
the thorax. 

(2) To the diaphragm, especially to the crura. 

(3) To the structures in the root of the neck through the fibrous 
tissue surrounding the superior vena cava. 

Now to this fulcrum the longitudinal muscle of the auricle is 
attached, and it is obvious that when the thorax is opened the 
auricle can no longer perform its normal movement. 

___ As we have seen, the heart’s excitatory wave arises in the great 
veins, especially in the neighbourhood of the sino-auricular groove 
between the superior vena cava and auricle. Of this there is 
physiological proof, since, as stated above, it is only in this region 
that the rhythm of the heart can be modified. The wave passes 
om here to all parts of the heart, and they respond to it in 
orderly sequence. We should expect, perhaps, that since it arises 
loser proximity to the right auricle, this auricle would contract 

E 


66 THE HEART 


slightly before the left, since the wave presumably has less ground 
to cover to fire off the right auricle. It is usually stated, how- 
ever, that the auricles beat simultaneously (cf. Tigerstedt and other 
authorities). According to Arloing and Doyon, however, Chauveau 
appears to have stated that in the horse the right auricle beats 
before the left. Fredericq also noticed this in his records on more 
than one occasion, especially when the heart was beating feebly and 
slowly. Recently further proof has been forthcoming under his 
direction. Schmidt-Neelsen finds that normally the right auricle 
precedes the left. If, however, an extra systole be induced by 
stimulation of the left auricle, then that auricle precedes the right. 
Another interesting observation made by him is that when the 
auricles are induced to beat by stimulation of the ventricle, the 
right more often precedes the left, but the results are variable. 
When the heart beats during excitation of the vagus, the normal 


order of beat is preserved. Stassen finds that in the dog the right 


auricle precedes the left by ‘02 to ‘03 second. He also finds that 
the ventricles do not beat simultaneously, but that the left nor- 
mally beats in the dog ‘03 to ‘04 second before the right. But 
when it is stimulated electrically the right precedes the left by 
a greater time than the left normally precedes the right. From 


this Stassen infers that there is a form of “‘ antidromic ”’ conduction - 


in the A-V bundle, since if the left be similarly excited, the 
interval before the right is the same as normal. 

When the auricle beats in the lower vertebrate heart, regurgi- 
tation is prevented by the action of the venous valves. In the 
mammal these valves have disappeared, and the prevention of re- 
gurgitation into the great veins is not altogether effectual. It is on 
this account that the jugular pulse can be recorded over the jugular 
bulb even in health. There is undoubtedly a guillotine action by 
the tenia terminalis tending to prevent regurgitation, but this is 


insufficient especially with high pressures; and according to the © 


degree of its insufficiency, so will the jugular tracing vary. Re- 
gurgitation through the inferior vena cava cannot be prevented 
by muscle, since there is none. The most important factor here 
is undoubtedly the high abdominal pressure, and indirectly there- 
fore the tone of the belly wall. The whole system of abdominal 


and thoracic veins may be looked upon as a large venous cistern, — 


having a capacity in man of about 430 c.c. The abdominal 


portion has by far the greater capacity (Keith). The blood is 


ia = 


THE HEART 67 


kept in this cistern by valves, e.g. femoral and jugular, and the 
. caused by the body movements must therefore send the 
blood on to the right heart. 

The chief function of the right auricle, however, is to expel the 
blood into the right ventricle at the end of joint diastole, and 
thereby place its walls on a certain tension. According to this 
degree of tension the heart muscle will contract. Within certain 
limits, the greater the tension, the more powerful the contraction. 
It is by this means in part, that the amount of blood passing to 
and from the heart is regulated to meet the needs of the body in 
general. 

It is necessary, therefore, to inquire into the movements acc :>m- 
panying auricular systole. The key to them lies in the study of 
the auricular and ventricular musculature. .The function of the 
musculi pectinati of the auricle has been neglected. In the human 
heart they are fifteen to eighteen in number, and from 1 to 2 mm. in 
diameter. They take origin from the right tenia terminalis, and 
} end in the musculature of the auricular canal in the A-V groove. 
i The tenia terminalis is a fixed point through the venous meso- 
-_ cardium; therefore when the musculi pectinati contract, they are 
2 _ drawn towards the fulcrum, the ventricle being also drawn up at 
the same time. It will therefore be seen that this movement 
which empties the auricle at the same time draws the ventricle 
over its load. There is therefore in auricular systole a movement 
of the A-V groove towards the. venous base of the heart. This 

function of the musculi pectinati has been well demonstrated by 
é injecting warm wax into the auricle, the casts so obtained showing 
that the musculi pectinati shorten to quite half their diastolic 
___ length during systole (Keith). In hearts from cases of back pres- 
sure there is great prolongation and hypertrophy of these muscles. 
Now an anatomical axiom is that every muscle in the body has 
its opponent, the opponent in this case being part of the inner 
longitudinal layer of muscle of the right ventricle. Inspection of 
_ the ventricle will show two layers of muscle—an inner longitudinal 
and an outer spiral layer. The longitudinal layer can be divided 
into two systems—(1) That to the auricle or venous base; (2) that 
to the aortic exit or arterial base. The significance of the system 
to the arterial base is perhaps at first not quite apparent. Accord- 
ng to Keith, its function is to act with the spiral fibres in 
endering the apex a fixed point. The spiral fibres will tend by 


~~ 


t a 


68 THE HEART 


their contraction to lengthen out the ventricle ; the longitudinal 
layer from the arterial base will tend by their contraction to 
shorten it. Between them, therefore, they render the apex a 
fixed point. The apex thus being fixed, the force of their com- 
bined contraction is to empty the ventricle of its contents. The 
significance of the longitudinal layer of trabecule to the auricle is 
quite clear. This layer is the opponent of the musculi pectinati 
of the auricle. The apex being a fixed point, its contraction in 
ventricular systole draws down the A-V groove towards the apex. 
By the action, therefore, of these opponent sets of muscles in the 
auricle and in the ventricle, the well-known to-and-fro movement 
of the heart at the A-V groove is executed (A, B, Fig. 4). It 
should be noted that this movement of the A-V groove towards 
the apex in ventricular systole expands the auricle and at the 
same time produces therein not only more room but also a 
negative pressure facilitating the flow of blood thereto. 

During joint diastole two things happen—(1) The base of heart 
replaces itself owing to the relaxation of the ventricular fibres ; 
(2) the ventricle opens out to fill with blood. A glance at the 
tracing of the jugular pulse given on page &8 will reveal these 
movements of the auricle and ventricle during the cardiac cycle. 
The positive wave a takes place during auricular systole ; the fall 
v is due to the contraction of the ventricle inducing the negative 
pressure in the auricle ; lastly, during diastole there is, first, a rise 
due to the reposition of the A-V groove, and secondly, a fall owing 
to the relaxation of the ventricle. 

Thus far the right side of the heart has occupied our attention. 
The manner in which the left auricle acts is more obscure, and has 
had far less attention paid to it. It is situated between the roots 
of the lungs, being firmly attached to each. By this means it is 
bound down in the unop2ned thorax to the chest wall. Behind 
it are the unyielding structures of the posterior mediastinum. 
Above, too, ‘are unyielding structures, the pulmonary arteries and 
bronchi; to the latter and to the trachea the upper border of the 
left auricle is firmly bound.. 'There is left, therefore, only (a) the 
anterior surface in contact with the ascending aorta, and (6) the 
floor in contact with the left \ventricle. Anatomical evidence, 
therefore, points to the fact that in systole of the left auricle the — 
anterior wall moves backwards and the ventricle upwards. The 
former movement would involve a \backward movement of the 


gs 


THE HEART 69° 


- aorta also, and it seems probable that in auricular systole the be- 
ginning of the aorta swings back and to the right, while in systole 
of the ventricles it moves forwards and to the left. Absolute 
physiological evidence of this movement is wanting, but Keith 
has pointed out :— 
‘ (1) That with the heart in situ, if the auricle be filled with 
injection of wax, the aorta is pushed forwards and recedes when 
it is emptied. 
_ (2) In mitral stenosis and cases where the left auricle is dilated 
_ + the aorta is pushed forward. 

(3) No other movements are available on anatomical con- 
siderations. 

(4) There is a portion of the pericardium so arranged as to act 
as a bursa for this movement. 

(5) The attachment of the musculature at the ‘Gnternurieulat 
septum is such that its only action can be to serve in this movement. 

Considering the musculature of the left auricle in more detail, 
we find that, unlike the right auricle, there are no pectinate 
muscles. This is due to its being developed as mentioned above, 
mainly from the auricular canal. The place of the pectinate 
musculature is taken by a series of muscular bands (Fig. 5) which 
are inserted into the inferior vena cava, and through it into the 
pericaridum and diaphragm. Chief of these bands is the left 
tenia terminalis (h, Fig. 5) which, arising in front at the superior 
vena cava, sweeps round to the. left in the anterior wall of the 
left auricle, and turns down between the auricular appendix and 
left pulmonary veins, to end in the inferior vena cava. Above 
and laterally the left auricle is attached to the roots of the lungs 
by the pulmonary veins and the fibrous venous mesocardium 
(xx, Fig. 5). The musculature of the left ventricle is essentially 
the same as the right. The outer spiral fibres (Fig. 5) and the 
inner longitudinal system again render the apex a fixed point. 
The opponent to the auricular musculature is the longitudinal 
System passing to the auricular part of the A-V groove. There is 
therefore the same to-and-fro movement here—the auricular mus- 
sulature in auricular systole draws the ventricle up (from A—A’ 
Fig. 5), while by ventricular systole the A-V groove is drawn from 
A —A, thereby expanding the left auricle and causing a negative 
pressure. There should therefore be a rapid flow of blood from 
the lungs to the left auricle during ventricular systole, but 


THE HEART 


there is no very positive evidence of this point, although the 
tracings of the cardio-pneumatic movements appear to indicate 
its presence. 

From anatomical considerations, therefore, we come to the con- 
clusion that the excitatory wave in its passage through the heart 
travels from the parts representing the venous end of the primitive 
cardiac tube to those developed from the arterial end. If this be 
the case, evidence of this passage should be forthcoming from the 
records obtained by different observers in regard to the electro- 
motive phenomenon of the beating heart.’ Considerable discrepancy, 
however, appears in such records. Waller and Reid found in 


several mammalian hearts that the excitatory wave normally — 


passed from apex to base. Bayliss and Starling, on the other 
hand, observed the wave to pass normally from base to apex, 


and only in the injured heart to pass in the reverse direction. | 


Schliiter’s experiments on the cat’s heart appear to support Waller 
and Reid. . 

In view of this divergence of opinion the recent valuable 
work of Gotch upon the frog’s heart is of great interest. His 
records distinctly show that it quite depends upon which part 
of the base of the heart the electrodes are placed as to what 
form of curve is obtained. Normally, however, he is convinced 
that the excitatory wave passes from the venous base of the 
ventricle to the apex and thence to the aortic base in connection 
with the great arterial trunks. Gotch’s results also show that a 
high intracardiac pressure brings into prominence the action of 
that part of the ventricle leading up to the aorta. This is there- 
fore important confirmatory evidence of Keith’s view of the action 
of the different sets of muscles in the ventricle: 

When, too, we realise how small an area the A-V bundle in 
the mammalian offers to the electrode, it is easy to understand 
that contradictory results should be obtained for the mammalian 
heart. There is every reason to suppose, therefore, that in the 
mammalian heart also the excitatory wave follows the direction 


of the primitive cardiac tube, and passes from the venous base — 


to the apex, and therice back to the aortic base, ensuring thereby 
an orderly sequence of movements. More work is wanted to 


elucidate these movements and also many other points, the im- — 


portance of which lies in the fact that “the heart... is the 
beginning of life; the sun of the microcosm; . . . it is the house- 


re . ; THE HEART 
hol old divini Nidihy which, discharging its function, nourishes, cherishes, “' 
anc quickens the whole body, and is indeed the foundation of life, 
_ the source of all action ” (Harvey). 


BIBLIOGRAPHY 


GeNERAL Reviews witH ‘Extensive LITERATURE— 

Burdon Sanderson, J., Schiifer’s Text-book of Physiology (1900), vol. ii. 
p- 439. 

Gaskell, W. H., Schiifer’s Text-book of Physiology (1900), vol. ii. p, 169. 

Heinz, R., Handbuch der exp. Path, v. Pharmakol., vol. i. p. 638. 
Gustav Fischer, Jena, 1905. 

sis dag F. B., Nagel’s Handbuch der Physiol. des Menschen, vol. i. 
_ p. 222. Braunschweig, 1905. 

Langendorg, Ergebnisse der Physiol., Abth. II. (1905), p, 764. 

PAaPERS— 

Carlson, American Journ, of Physiol., xii. p. 67. 

Erlanger, Journ, Exp. Med., viii. p. 8. 

Fredericqg, Archives Intern. de Physiol., iv. p. 57. 

Gotch, Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, Vol. 79 (1907), p. 323. 

Heidenhain, M., Anat. Anzeiger, xx. (1901). 

Hering, Pfliiger’s Archiv., eviii. p. 267. 

Humblet, Archives Intern. de Physiol., i. 278; ibid, «lili. p. 330. 

Imchanitsky, Archives Intern, de Physiol., iv. p. 1. 

Keith, Journal Anat, and Physiol. xlii. p. 1 

Keith and Flack, Lancet, August 11, 1906 ; Journal Anat. and Physiol., 
xli. p. 172. 

Stassen, Archives Intern. de Physiologie, iii. ». 338; v. p. 61. 

Schmidt-Neelsen, Archives Intern. de Physiol., iv. 

Tawara, Das Reizleitungssystem das Siiugetierherzens. Gustav Fischer, 
Jena, 1906. : 


4 


PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO THE 
EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 


. By THOMAS LEWIS 


THE study of the arterial pulse in man has occupied the attention 
of physiologists and physicians for centuries. With the advent 
of the graphic method, it yielded, at the hands of such investi- 
gators as Marey (**), Landois (81), Mosso (°°), and a host of other 
workers, results which have greatly enhanced our knowledge of 
the human cardiovascular system. Not infrequently it has served 
to stimulate research on problems of general and far-reaching 
physiological significance, and in some instances has contributed 
facts to our knowledge of the heart and arteries which it would 
have been difficult to obtain from experiments on the lower 
animals. The study of the human pulse carries with it a natural 
advantage, which can hardly be overestimated; the subject of 
experiment may be observed under normal conditions, and is 
amenable to reason and more perfect control. 

The examination of the individual curves of arterial sphygmo- 
grams, upon which an incalculable amount of labour has been 
expended, is, with the exception of a few instances, abortive in 
yielding such information to the clinician as can bring him a pro- 
fitable return for the time he expends. Empirical diagnosis from 
pulse tracings is a method fraught with abundant and dangerous 
pitfalls. It is for the evidence which the curves give of the events 
of the cardiac cycle that they are to be chiefly valued, and it is 
in this direction that the researches of modern, nay recent, years 
have yielded such fruitful results. Above all, graphic curves taken 
simultaneously from different pulsating points are invaluable, for 
they accord information of the time relations and nature of the con- 
traction of the separate chambers of the heart. The observations of 
_ the past ten years, while supplying important physiological know- 

72 


PULSE RECORDS 73 


ledge, bid fair to revolutionise the technique of cardiovascular 
diagnosis. 
Records can be obtained of the contraction of both auricles 
_ and both ventricles in a large percentage of subjects. In certain 
conditions the information so derived is of great service, for a 
particular chamber may fail to contract, its contraction may be 
imperfect, or it may enter upon its systole too early or too late. 
Many irregularities of the heart have already received careful 
analysis along these lines, and knowledge of much clinical import- 
ance has been gathered in respect of them. In regard to one 
affection of the heart, the pathological state now known as “ heart 
block,” in which the path of conduction from auricle to ventricle 
is disturbed or broken, and in which a slow pulse and its concomi- 
tant symptoms constitute the chief features of the disease, a flood 
of light has been shed upon a condition of which our ignorance 
| was formerly profound. As to the therapeutic action of cardiac 
| drugs, much work has been accomplished, and more remains to 
| 
: 


be done. A wide field for study has been opened up which awaits 
cateful and painstaking investigations ; and the day, it is hoped, 
is not. far distant when the functions of the heart, and the 
disturbances of the same, will be profitably referred to at the 
bedside, in the terms employed by Gaskell in his well-known 
researches. 

But the study of pulsations in man can never reach that pitch 
of mechanical perfection to which modern physiological experi- 
ment has attained, and the conclusions drawn from the more 
indirect observations on man must ever remain in a measure 
subservient to, and controlled by, the more direct readings acquired 
from animals. That emphasis should be laid upon this aspect 

_ of the question is imperative, for there is a tendency to-day, 
frequently noticeable in the consideration of human pulsations, to 
fly in the face of physiological knowledge, which, as it is unques- 
tionably of less fallacious origin, should guide, and form the basis 
of, all conclusions. 

In the: following pages the methods recently introduced for 
the study of the cardiac cycle in the normal subject will be 

examined ; a critical account will be given of the records obtained 
in man; and the information so obtained will be compared and 
brought so far as possible into line with the correlated knowledge 


74 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 
derived from the lower animals. In bringing together physio- 


logical facts which may serve as an introduction to their clinical 


application, pathological observations may be quoted only in so 
far as they have a direct bearing on the several problems involved. 
At the same time it must he noted that the boundary between the 
physiological and pathological states has ever been broad, and 
that so far as cardiovascular studies are concerned, recent observa- 
tions have tended to its extension rather than to its limitation. 
For the older conception of pulse irregularities as evidence of the 
pathological state is no longer justified. Apart from the irregu- 
larity, chiefly in the length of diastole, which, occurring as an 
accompaniment of the respiratory movements, and dependent 
upon vagal influences, is so familiar to physiologists, a closely 
allied irregularity, termed by Mackenzie (**”) the “ youthful irre- 


gularity” (p. 84), has been described as a normal event which ~ 


occurs at or about that epoch when the pulse diminishes in 
rate and the heart takes up the rhythm which it will maintain 
within narrow limits during adult life? Further it must be 
recognised that spontaneous ventricular contractions* are of 
common occutrence from time to time in individuals in whom no 
further evidence of ill-health is available. These “‘ extrasystoles,” 
though incompatible with our view of a heart functionating in an 
ideal fashion, may be regarded with justification as consistent 
with the indefinite borderland which lies ’twixt health and 
disease. The frequency of their occurrence, in the otherwise 


1 References to pages and figures allude to the bibliography, except where other- 
wise stated. 


2 Some of these irregularities are undoubtedly anomalous responses to respira- 
tion (cp. Hering *); but it is equally beyond dispute that others have no 
connection with breathing. 

3 It is now generally acknowledged that the ventricular systole results from 
a conducted stimulus proceeding from the auricle. But it is also recognised that 
the ventricle may beat when this source of stimulus is removed. If at any time 
this chamber contracts independently of the auricle, such a contraction may be 
termed spontaneous. The origin of the stimulus giving rise to a contraction of 
this nature is as yet imperfectly understood. There is reason to believe that it 
arises in embryonic remains to which the function of rhythmicity is particularly 
attributed. But for a further and fuller account of these autochthonous ventricular 
beats, or extrasystoles as they are frequently designated, the reader is referred to 


> 


the works of Mackenzie (*¢) and Wenchebach (*), (The term ‘‘ extrasystole ”’ is a 


here employed in its broadest sense. ) 


THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 75 


_ normal subject, over long periods, and more especially in associa- 
tion with pregnancy, hard toil, or advancing years, brings them 
into a domain, closely approaching, if not included within that of 
physiology. 


. Marey and his associates; Chauveau (*7) and Frangois- Franck (#4), 
were amongst the chief pioneers of graphic records. The collected 
observations of Marey, published in his books of 1863 (3*) and 1881 (3), 
are comprehensive. In La circulation du sang, &c., are found 
records of almost all visible pulsations of the normal human body. 
Numbers of tracings taken from the healthy or diseased, experimental 
observations, and records of apparatus combine to form a rich store- 
house of facts, never equalled before or since, for the study of 
hemodynamic problems. The apparatus figured, and employed in 
the experiments, show an almost unrivalled wealth of ingenuity, and 
many of the instruments in their original or slightly modified forms 
are still in general use. 

The methods of sphygmography are too well known to need de- 
scription. Sphygmographs fitted with additional levers are numerous, 
and serve in the procuration of simultaneous tracings from apex beat, 
arteries, jugular vein, liver, fontanelle, or chest wall. The mechanism 
used in clinical work upon the jugular pulse, the apparatus with which 
we are chiefly concerned, is described later. The best receiver for 
cardiographic records is perhaps Marey’s, consisting essentially of a 
cup, covered by a rubber membrane to which a button is attached. 
But a hollow uncovered receiver answers well for general purposes, 
and may be employed over a less restricted field. 

In animals the earliest work on intra-auricular and intra- ventricular 
pressures was carried out by Marey’s school. Small rubber balloons 
tied over the extremities of narrow metal sounds were passed through 
jugular vein or aorta into the corresponding chambers of the heart, 
and records of the pressures within these cavities were obtained by 
connecting the metal tubing to tambours supplied with levers and 
writing styles. It might be supposed that the introduction of such 
sounds into the auricle or ventricles would seriously interfere with 
their normal functions. This, however, is not the case. Chauveau 
relates that he passed a double cannula of the sort, fitted with 
balloons for auricle and ventricle, into the right heart of a horse, 
without disturbing the pulse rate, or the meal of which the animal 
_ was at the time partaking (Assoc. frang. p. Vavanc. d. Sciences, 1887). 
The apparatus used by the earlier workers for the actual records 


76 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


has undergone improvement. Many instruments have been introduced, 
amongst which those of Hiirthle require special mention. The inertia 
of the levers and membranes is reduced in Hiirthle’s instruments (?®) 
to an almost negligible quantity. Air is replaced by fluid transmission, 
for the compressibility of air renders it less reliable in obtaining records 
free from extraneous movements. The delicacy and reliability of 
Hiirthle’s instruments have been demonstrated by Bayliss and Starling 
by a photographic method (3). Hiirthle’s differential manometer is so 
constructed that two columns of fluid, each conveying the changing 
pressures of a pulsating cavity, are opposed to each other. Any 
difference in pressure in the two cavities is thus registered, and the 
instrument is of particular value in ascertaining the instant at which 
the pressures in two adjoining and communicating cavities, such as 
auricle and ventricle, or ventricle and aorta, become equal. It is 
with this instrument that information has been obtained in respect 
of two “standard movements ” of the heart, namely, the opening of | 
the auriculo-ventricular valves and the closure of the semilunar seg- 
ments. The standard movements, to which further reference will 
frequently be made, are of importance; they may be regarded as 
fixed points of time, and it is convenient to describe and figure all 
other cardiac movements, and oscillations to which these cardiac 
movements give rise, in relation to them. The third standard 
in common use is the commencement of ventricular systole, or 
perhaps more accurately, the moment at which the pressure in the 
ventricle commences to rise in systole. The onset of systole is an 
instant which it is uot difficult to fix in animal experiments; but 
in man, as will subsequently be seen, it is by no means so easy. It 
is possible that with the further elaboration of Einthoven’s recently 
introduced electrical method! more accurate indications may be 
obtained. Einthoven uses a sensitive “string galvanometer” to 
estimate the change of potential in the limbs, which occurs with 
the heart beat. The observations, however, which have as yet been 
made, have little bearing upon the subjects of this article. 

In conclusion, the attempts instituted to obtain graphic records 
of the heart sounds may be briefly referred to. Hinthoven 
and Geluk (®) employed a microphone and capillary electrometer. 
Hiirthle (26) also used the microphone for a similar purpose. Obser- - 
vations which are referred to in the text have thus been carried out 
on the instants of onset of the first and second sound ; but given more 


1 Archives Internat. de Physiologie, vol. iv., 1906-7, pp. 132-164; Archiv. 
7. d. ges. Physiol., Bd. 122, 1908, s. 517-584. 


‘THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 77 


‘convenient and reliable instruments, many problems of no inconsider- 
able clinical significance would receive elucidation. There is no 
question that by assiduous training a clinician with keen perceptive 
faculties may acquire great skill in timing and interpreting abnormal 
heart sounds. Yet the dogmatism of the present day upon the sub- 
ject of heart murmurs is entirely unjustified, a conclusion which is 
completely borne out by the inconsistent yet equally peremptory 
opinions of independent observers of. the same subject. Franck} in 
a preliminary communication has recently described a new instru- 
ment, based on the principle of the middle ear, with which graphic 
records may be taken. Einthoven? has successfully used his galvano- 
meter for the same purpose. Further results are awaited with 
expectancy, for there is every possibility that the apparatus may 
supply a long-felt want. 


I. THE VENTRICULAR CYCLE AND THE STANDARD MOVEMENTS 


The interpretation of the time relations of the human ventri- 
cular movements at present *® depends on cardiographic tracings, 
complex curves involving changes of pressure, volume and 
diameter in the left ventricle, and the errors emanating from 
this method must constantly be borne in mind. It is probable 
that the shock of the apex beat gives in thin-walled chests a 
fair approximation of the onset of systole, with an error which, 
though difficult to state accurately, probably does not exceed 
‘02 sec.* 


? Otto Franck, Miinchener mediz. Wochensch., Bd. 51, 1904, s. 953. Weiss _ 
and Joachim (Pfliiger’s Archiv., Bd. 123, 1908, s. 341) have more recently 
succeeded in obtaining numerous records of heart sounds and murmurs which 
appear to be of considerable value, 

® Pfliiger’s Archiv., Bd. 117, 1907, s. 461. 

% Eventually it will probably be controlled by electric curves. 

“In one of Hiirthle’s tracings () (Fig. 7) there is an exceptional difference 
of -06 sec. between the upstroke of the cardiogram and the registration of the first 
sound. In animals the tracings of Chauveau and Marey (7), Roy and Adami (**), 
Hiirthle (™), and most other writers show the upstroke of ventricular pressure and 
cardiogram to be synchronous ; but such tracings are taken under exceptionally 
favourable circumstances. x 


7 o—- -_--- ~~~ —— t ———— OO eee a =—_ 


— 


a BL 'Sta “TIT HL wor | 06 | ge | 90. | st | go. | zo. | | [t+ (s8op 403) (ogg) orgaamT | 
~ “[[BA JSONO CATZOOJop WOIT — OB Ctr. FSG. Tg0. | 1-+ 1+ +s gogt (x) apeysteser, | 
7 Us a, = {Gaara —— ‘a> a ra . 
S ‘sSurowy jwoSvydosgp ices ose eos oes eee nec k ees * L061 “(o9) q10[MoH] pus Zunox 
y, = *xo1dde 
= *sBurovsy [voSuyqdosa § DISMOMUITYT WoL] eee oats ata eee eee eee SI. 04 if eee . . LOGL ‘(acr) Sroquoyney 
A [ym ysoyo eatoojop Wolg | 99 | GE. ee 13. OT. He oj | ttt L06E ‘(or) 20SuBpag 
a= 0. ooo eee ° ° . C681 *(agz) organ 
os oo. ES se} 8 get (re) suIgauyy 
ZI ‘09 ‘d uo Zutovsy wo1g | OL ore. 89Z- cco. ae ee ekg . : * LEST ‘(6z) 340M 
ee eee ee en a ee sae eee | 7 ah ‘ 
; “pryoreo , ; is Tan ee : , b 
a * 4 04 vyoe moa uOISSICISUvI} soepny[oul A[qeqoig * OL FoF. C0. cgt. 660- £60. Ssst (3) nels pay s 
y mM jie. ae ea [aa] er 90. oS ee - : - GIST ‘(ge) OSSOTY - ‘4 
4 . p a i— { ae : 
a! g G-Gc| OOF | “| 980. | $80. | 80. | LI. 7 tt BIST (te) stopary 
* Z es oe | 3 sé] s 
_ E wel & )SsE| 3 
g s | & ae} =e | $$3| # 
a ee), eee oe. teers, | oe 
‘SMUVNAY 9 a E 3. | Sa A wae | xOHLAY 
2/2|2| | 28] | 22] 3 
4 2 BE| ¢ | ae2| 3 
3 re £ gen | 
a “ 
Nd I @IAVL 


—— OO 


THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 79 


TABLE II 
Delay from | Delayfrom!| Velocity, Velocity, | Velocity, 
AUTHOR, Aorta to | Carotidto | Carotid to Aorta to Aorta to 
Carotid. | Radial. Radial, Radial. | Carotid 

Landois . : ‘ ee vs er 5°7 m.p.s. a | 
Keyt. : ; ‘ ‘0279 ‘0797 7-9 m.p.s. sat: — 
eee ae 
Edgren . ‘ ; 026-7 ‘0792 nae 8 m.p.s. ws | 
-|—_|___ 

_| Griinmach (%) . : 03 06 3 9m.p.s, | 6°6 m.p.s 

Martius. : - 03 | 
Hirthle () - | 03 er ey +e | 6 m.p.s. 


The terms employed in Table I. require brief definition. It must 
not be forgotten that the curves of intra-ventricular pressure cannot 
be taken as indicative of the changes in the volume of the ventricular 
blood content. When the ventricles contract and the pressures in 
them augment, their blood content remains unchanged, for no blood 
leaves them until the pressures rise to aortic and pulmonary pressure. 
The interval which elapses in this way, prior to the raising of the 
arterial valves, constitutes what is termed the “ presphygmic ” in- 
terval. But the pressures in the arteries begin, and those of the 
ventricles continue, to rise subsequent to this interval; this continued 
rise in the ventricles is consequently spoken of as the “ period of rising 
arterial pressure.” Following upon it is the plateau, indicated in the 
pressure curve by a wavy line, which runs horizontally, or slopes 
slightly upwards or downwards. During the whole of this plateau 
the volume of the ventricles is decreasing and blood is leaving them; 
but as it leaves them at a lessened rate, it does not, necessarily, further 
raise the arterial pressures. 

At the end of the plateau, the pressures in the ventricles fall, and 
when they are reduced below the arterial pressures, the pulmonary 
and aortic valves close. It is, however, a little while before the pres- 
sures fall sufficiently to allow the auriculo-ventricular valves to open, 
and this interval is often spoken of as the “ postsphygmic ” interval. 
Following upon it there is a pause, during the whole of which the 


80 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


ventricles are expanding and receiving blood from the veins and 
auricles. The term ventricular diastole is usually loosely applied, 
and has been avoided in the table; we have not sufficient knowledge 
of the period over which the ventricles are expanding actively, if they | 
ever do so, to define the time limits of diastole with precision. . 


In referring to the accompanying tables mention must be 
made of observations recently carried out by Erlanger (?°) and 
Tigerstedt (*’), the results of which are included. The estimates 
were made from subjects in whom there were partial defects of 
the chest wall and in whom the heart was readily accessible. The 
figures are probably as accurate as any we possess; Keyt’s 
numbers (”), dependent on numerous and elaborate measurements, 
compare well with them. The figures given in the first table are 
taken as far as possible from young adult subjects, and the delays 
and velocities exampled in the second table are illustrative of | 
normal pulse rates. Some figures from a tracing after Hiirthle (7*) 
(Taf. III., Fig. 12) are also given for the comparison of man and 
the dog. No such estimates are absolute ; there is a wide varia- 
tion with numerous factors, such as pulse rate and blood pressure. 

But though eventually the cardiographic upstroke is the 
standard movement on which the chief measurements are based, 
in clinical practice the frequent absence or inversion of the apex 
beat renders it unreliable, and it becomes necessary to fix further 
standards. For this purpose the carotid pulse is chosen as the 
safest and most useful guide. Its utilisation involves under many 
circumstances a calculation of the presphygmic interval, and the 
normal transmission delay of the pulse wave from the semilunar 
valves to the carotid. In the normal subject the presphygmic 
interval may show variations of at least 03 séc., with age, pulse, 
rate, &c. In disease it may amount to as much as ‘06 sec. In 
clinical application, in which relatively slow travelling recording 
surfaces are used, an error of less than ‘05 sec. is as a rule 
negligible, and the figure ‘05 sec. may be taken as a comparatively 
secure working estimate of the presphygmic interval. The error 
entailed in calculating the transmission delay from aorta to carotid 
is probably not greater than ‘01 sec. in health and ‘03 sec. in 
disease ; the interval may be taken for clinical purposes at ‘03 sec.* 

1 Weiss and Joachim have recently measured the interval between the first 


heart sound and carotid pulsation in man (Pfliiger’s Archiv., 1908), and find it as 
a rule ‘08 to °09 sec. 


° 


THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 81 


n jugular tracings are taken simultaneously with a standard 
1s Remnant, the radial pulse is usually employed on account of 
‘its convenience. This method involves a third error, the variation 
in transmission delay from the aorta to radial vessel. The varia- 
tion in this delay is so great, and the accumulated error becomes 
80 magnified, that it is unsafe to fix the radial pulse as the 
standard movement if the carotid to radial delay is not first 
estimated and allowed for in the particular case.’ 

The determination of the position of closure of the semilunar 
valves (S.C.) and of the opening of the auriculo-ventricular 
valves (A.O.) upon human cardiographic and carotid curves pre- 
sents considerable difficulties. 

In. animals it is now held that the S.C. point occurs a short 

distance along the downstroke of intra-ventricular pressure 
(Hiirthle (*), Porter (*°), Bayliss and Starling (3), &c.). 
point is found in simultaneous tracings to correspond on the aortic 
curve to the bottom of the dicrotic depression (aortic notch) ; * 
on the carotid tracing, which is slightly delayed, it falls at or 
about the beginning of the same depression. On the cardio- 
graphic curve it corresponds in animals to a point near the end 
of the ventricular plateau; thus Marey (*”) (Fig. 40) places it 
before the end of the plateau, Hiirthle’s tracings (7) show it before 
or slightly after the end of the plateau. To sum up, there is no 
constant relationship between cardiographic and intra-ventricular 
curves at this phase. 

In man the 8.C. point is placed by Edgren (°) at the bottom 
of the cardiographic downstroke, at a point indicated by a small 
wave. According to Hiirthle (*”) it is situated in the first third of 
the downstroke, and it is placed at the same instant by Einthoven 
and Geluk (*), despite the fact that they were guided by the record 
of the second sound, which occurs later than the closure. The 
~ method in which the second sound is marked on the tracing by a 
signal worked with the hand, allowance being made for reaction 
ime, is obviously unreliable and gives varying results (cp. Edgren’s 


- 


af 
5 


3 1 The figures given and statements made are the result of a critical examination 
fa large number of statements and tracings. The space at our disposal prohibits 
full examination of the facts. There is a good deal of divergence between the 
¢s given by different authors, and further work giving more detailed analysis 
be of much value, for at present general statements are of necessity very 


DX UT 


82 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


tracings). As the cardiographic curve in its relationship to the 
second sound in man shows no constancy, and as it shows a 
similar divergence when compared at this phase to the carotid 
curve,! it cannot be regarded as a safe criterion in fixing the 8.C. 
point. The constancy of the relationship of the S.C. point to the 
aortic curve in animals shows the necessity of taking the carotid 
curve as the standard in man. The nearest approach to accuracy 
is obtainable by allowing for the transmission time from aorta to 
carotid. The S.C. point in man is thus fixed at a point ‘03 sec. 
in advance (to the left in Fig. 1 of this article) of the bottom of the 
dicrotic depression. 

The actual determination of the A.O. point in man is im- 
possible. In animals most writers are agreed in placing it at the 
bottom of the intra-ventricular downstroke.? On the aortic curve 
it is marked at the summit of the dicrotic (Porter, Hiirthle, Bayliss 
and Starling). Cardiographic curves show no constant relation- 
ships at this phase. In man the A.O. point is best fixed on the 
carotid tracing, ‘03 sec. in advance of the summit of the dicrotic 
wave. It is to be noted, however, that this introduces a possible 
source of fallacy. Galabin® states (1°) that the dicrotic summit 
recedes from the apex of the primary wave as the pulse travels 
along the vessels. The error from this alteration is probably so 
small as to be quite negligible in the carotid tracing, but it con- 
stitutes an important element in radial tracings, which should 
therefore never be employed in estimating this point, in exact 
work. It must.not be forgotten that the A.O. point in man is the 
_ most difficult of all the standard movements to ascertain ; conclusions 
drawn from it must always be received with due caution. 


II. THe RELATIONSHIP OF THE AURICULAR AND 
VENTRICULAR CYCLES 


(a) The Evidence obtained from Intra-auricular Curves 
in Animals.—The events of the auricular cycle have received 


1 Thus in Keyt’s tracings the bottom of the cardiographic downstroke corre- 
sponds on the. carotid to the bottom of the dicrotic depression, or to a point on the 
upstroke of the dicrotic itself. In Edgren’s tracings, as in Hiirthle’s, it is placed 
as a rule at the bottom of the depression; but there is very considerable divergence 
in the individual curves of these authors. 

2 Estimated by differential tracings. ; 

8 The statement of Galabin is supported by many of the tracings which 
Keyt gives. The question is discussed by Frey (Untersuch. d. Pulses, 1892). 


~ cies 


‘THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 83 


careful investigation at the hands of Chauveau and Marey, D’Es- 
pine, Fredericq ("*), Frey and Krehl (#*), and Porter. Recently 
‘Hering (***) has studied the same subject, using the perfused heart ; 
-and further observations have been made by Rautenberg (*”). 
_ For the most part the experiments have been on dogs ; those of 
‘Chauveau and Marey were carried out on the horse. 
The curves given by Porter are perhaps as much in accord 

with the general evidence as any, and it will be found convenient 

to take these as typifying the changes in intra-auricular pressure, 
eventually examining the results of experimental work in relation 
to such a type. In common with all other observers Porter found 
that auricular contraction is accompanied by a positive wave 
of pressure. At or a little before the termination of auricular 
‘systole this “ first positive wave ” * gives place to a “ first negative 
wave,” a wave which, but for the interposition of the, ‘“ second 
positive wave,” forms with the “second negative wave ” the main 
depression of auricular pressure during the cycle. (A curve illustrat- 
ing the waves and their relation to the curve of intra-ventricular 
pressure is given in the accompanying figure, Fig. 1). The second 
positive wave, which breaks this otherwise steady fall, is said by 
most writers on the subject to occur at the onset of ventricular 
systole, and is usually figured as lasting until the point which marks 
the opening of the semilunar valves.* The wave is well shown on 
the tracings of Chauveau and Marey, D’Espine, Fredericq, Porter, 
_and Rautenberg, but it is absent from those of Frey and Krehl. 
Porter (p. 526) noted its absence on occasion. Fuller details of 
its significance will be discussed later. Relaxation of the auricular 
wall is stated by Porter to last to the beginning or end of this 
wave. The point at which the second negative and “ third posi- 
tive waves” meet is at present the subject of considerable dis- 
cussion. Chauveau and Marey placed it at the end of ventricular 
systole,* but otherwise there appears a consensus of opinion, 
1 Cited by Frederieq (™, p. 524 and Fig. 19). 
* In the auricular tracings given by Porter there are in all three main elevations 
i three main depressions. The terminology of the curves is at present so cOn- 
— the employment of Porter's original designations is impossible. In the 
g pages the rises and falls will be spoken of as the first, second, and third 
‘ive, ‘and the first, second, and third negative, wave respectively. 
% "3 estimated by means of differential manometer tracings (Hirthle 
x ie tae stacing given in the Gaz. méd, d. Paris, 1861, p. 673, the second positive 

a s not shown, and the third positive wave commences during the systolic 


a 
Fi 


ae 


84 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO | 


amongst those who have taken auricular pressure curves, that it 
occurs earlier (Frey and Krehl, Fredericq, Porter, Hering, and 
Rautenberg). While it may probably be placed with a fair degree 
of certainty in the middle or last third of the systolic plateau, 
there is every possibility that it is subject to variation. The 
third positive wave ends in the “third negative wave” during 
ventricular relaxation. Generally the point is given at the bottom 
of the falling ventricular curve, the instant at which is placed the 
opening of the auriculo-ventricular valves.!_ Porter himself figures 
it at a slightly earlier time interval,? at a position in the cycle 
recognised as representing the closure of the semilunar valves, 
namely, a short distance after the end of the systolic plateau. 


(b) The Venous Pulse in Animals and Man.—Pulsation 
of the veins of the neck in pathological conditions is often such. 
an obvious phenomenon that it must have been recognised for 
many centuries. References to venous pulsation can be traced 
in the writings of Lancisi (°°, p. 182) and Morgagni (**) in the 
early and middle years of the eighteenth century. In 1794 
Hunter described pulsation in the veins of a dog; but it is ques- 
tionable if these writers appreciated the extent of the movement ; 
their observations appear to have been confined to the veins in 
the immediate neighbourhood of the heart. Later, similar move- 
ments were described by Weldemeyer (8) in the veins of a horse.* 
Friedreich (14) took venous curves from the neck of pathological 
subjects in 1865; and two years later Potain (*°) obtained simul- 
taneous tracings of the apex beat, carotid, radial and jugular 
pulses in the normal human subject, and his description of the 
events and his interpretation of them are,’ in the light of our 
present knowledge, wonderfully accurate. From the time of 
Potain’s contribution, observations have been published by many _ | 
writers, amongst whom may be mentioned, Mosso, Gottwalt (1%), 


plateau ; the tracings repeated in Marey’s book show the second positive wave, and 
the third positive wave occurs at the close of systole. The collected curves given 
by Chauveau in 1887 (Assoc. franc. p. l'avane, d. Sciences) are very various, but 
as a rule show the three chief waves distinctly. 

1 The point is estimated by differential pressure tracings, 

2 Porter’s curves were from the left auricle, and show, like most others, some — 
variation in their last phases. 

3 For full historical accounts the reader is referred to Mackenzie's earlier \ 
papers, and the recent xt pabBowtio of Baum (?). /. 


° 


THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 85 


Riegel (*), and Frangois-Franck (")._ In 1893-4 Mackenzie (***) 
published his first papers, which, with his collected observations on 
“The Study of the Pulse,” appearing in 1902, have given the 
impetus to a careful investigation of the whole subject. 

Pulsation in the veins of animals is not confined to the larger 
vessels feeding the auricles ; it is a constant phenomenon in dogs, 
cats, and rabbits, and may be seen extending in many of them 
into the smaller veins of the neck and limbs. Gottwalt was of 
opinion that the jugular veins of all normal persons pulsate, and 
this conception is receiving constant confirmation. ‘It is possible 
at the present time to obtain venous records from the neck of a 
large percentage of normal individuals ; Wenchebach states that 
it is feasible in most subjects, healthy or diseased. Pulsation is 
frequently so well marked as to be visible, and in thin or anemic 
subjects often constitutes an obvious movement. It is highly 
probable that with improved apparatus curves will be invariably 
obtainable... Any agency which tends to promote a heightened 
venous pressure, such as raised intra-thoracic or abdominal pres- 
sure, or gravitation, increases the force and visibility of venous 
pulsation. It is for this reason that tracings are best taken 
in the reclining posture, and that in those cases, where pulsation 
is feeble, expiratory suspension of respiration increases its 
prominence. 

In man, the venous pulse is seen and recorded with the greatest 
facility in those veins which have but a short distance to travel 
before reaching the heart. A special and valuable anatomical 
description of the veins of the neck in relation to this subject has 
recently been given by Keith (?§). Tracings may be taken direct 
from the external jugular vein when this is engorged, but more 
often the receiving instrument must be applied to the jugular 
bulb, which lies a little above and 25 mm. external to the sternal 
end of the clavicle.* A needle passed back into the neck at this 
_ point strikes the internal jugular vein at a point where it is 
guarded by a pair of valves, which, when the path to the auricle 
_is obstructed, or when blood is regurgitated, produces a bulging 
in the vessel from which the “ bulb” derives its name. Passed 
further on the needle transfixes the subclavian artery. Tracings 
1 If they are not already. 


_ * The receiver should be as a rule applied between the two heads of origin of 
shi sterno-mastoid muscle. 


> 


86 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


are obtained from the neck, in which the sterno-mastoid is relaxed, 
by applying to it with light pressure a small receiver, whose width 
is about 4 cm., and depth 1 cm.! The interior of the shallow 
cup communicates with a delicate tambour carrying a writing 
style. This simple apparatus is perhaps the most satisfactory as 
yet employed. The accuracy of its records has been checked by 
Mackenzie, its originator, and by Gerhardt, by comparisons with 
the tracings given by light levers directly attached to the skin 
overlying a superficial vein; and the movements of the recording 
tambour and lever, so far as the main waves are concerned, have 
had their reliability checked by comparison with the tracings 
yielded by a Hiirthle manometer (Gerhardt). Air transmission is 
employed, and the curves obtained from the jugular bulb are 
frequently complicated by the primary wave of the carotid pulse, 
which in clinical work is not without its advantages, and by the . 
respiratory movements when these are present. Curves are best 
obtained from the right side of the neck, for here the course of 
the innominate is shorter and more direct from neck to heart 
(Friedreich, p. 282); clinically they may also be taken from the 
pulsating liver. 

With regard to the rest of the mechanism little comment is 
necessary ; when the apparatus described is adjusted to a modified 
Dudgeon sphygmograph, as in Mackenzie’s original instrument, the. 
fitting of a reliable time-marker renders it cumbersome. Another 
convenient instrument for clinical work is ‘“‘ Mackenzie’s ink 
polygraph,” 2 with which tracings of any length and at various 
speeds: may be taken. It allows a simultaneous record of any 
two given pulsations, and carries a reliable time-marker. The | 
curves from the jugular vein give, considering the indirect methods 
of determining them, surprisingly constant results, and in this lies 
their chief justification. 


1 There is a tendency, not without its advantages, towards the use of smaller 
receivers. si 

2 The apparatus, an account of which is published in the British Medical 
Journal for June 13, 1908, p. 1411, is open to improvement; more particularly 
the writing styles, and that portion of the mechanism used for the record of the 
radial pulse. The momentum of the levers and membranes is considerable, and 
extraneous movements cannot be entirely excluded. For the interpretation of the _ 
main waves it answers its purpose admirably, but for the investigation of me minor 
waves more delicate apparatus would be requisite. 

It is necessary to insist on the disuse of time markers whose movement depends 
on the clockwork which drives the paper. 


‘THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 87 


In animals curves have been secured from the vein wall 
or blood stream, by Gottwalt, Frangois-Franck, Morrow (*’), 
Fredericq (?*°), Rautenberg (*°), and others. The results in man 
and animals are in such general agreement that one description 
_ of them will suffice. 

Speaking broadly, the venous pulse, like the auricular curve, 
shows three main elevations and three main depressions ;! the 
general outlines of venous and auricular curves are such as to 
leave no reasonable doubt that the factors ultimately concerned 
in the production of their waves are identical for each, a relation 
on which many writers have laid emphasis. The final evidence 
depends on the detailed analysis of each wave of the curves and 
will be undertaken subsequently. For the time being it will be 
convenient to recognise the three main elevations of the venous 
curve (the “a,” “c,” and “v” waves of Mackenzie) as corre- 
sponding to the three positive waves of the auricular pressure curve, 
and ye three main dips of the venous tracing (the “x,” “x’,” 
and “ y” depressions of Mackenzie) as corresponding to “the three 
chief cA es waves of the auricle. These events are represented 
in the accompanying figure (Fig. 1), and their relations may be 
tabulated as follows :— 


oe Slee ; : , . First positive wave. 
oh ates é : , . First negative wave. 
Sor 34 ; ° ‘ . Second positive wave. 
whe tage , ; . . Second negative wave. 
iy: i : ; . Third positive wave. 
bby? ird : 

: ile : : ; . Third negative wave. 


The waves of the jugular pulse have not received such an 
_ accurate comparison to standard movements as have the auricular 
in animals. This is but natural, for with the indirect 
methods available the sources of error are comparatively large, 
and little advantage is to be gained by the use of more rapidly 
travelling recording surfaces. But writers are in the habit of 
expressing themselves dogmatically as to the time instants. at 
which certain events are taking place, while at the same time 
they appear to lack a due appreciation of the comparatively rough 
‘methods by which the standard movements of the cardiac cycle 
fixed in man. Cardiographic curves are notoriously uncertain, 


1 A record of the venous pulse is shown in Fig. 3, at the end of this article. 


88 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


-14 05 0503 22 


"4 


06 }03 *37 Secs. 


S.C line) | 
line. 


tie 
} 
5 
/] 


2h Pes. 
Pos. 


Pou 
HA 
As * 
. 
P 
‘ 
“ ¥] as, 3 
3 
PA 
oe 


Fic. 1.—Diagrammatic representation of the events of the cardiac cycle and of 
the carotid and jugular pulses in relation to standard movements. The scale © 
of abscissee is 1 mm. to x}, sec. §.C.=semilunar valve closure; A.O.=auriculo- 
ventricular valves open. The broken lines indicate those geese of the respective 
curves over which there is doubt or controversy. 


THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 89 


and carotid tracings must be judged on their merits. The dis- 
crepancies in the findings are greater in the case of jugular curves 
than they are in the case of the auricular. Considerable confusion 
appears to exist as to the events occurring in that portion of the 
heart’s cycle which lies between the closure of the semilunar valves 
and the opening of the tricuspids. Further work is needed, and 
the first appeal must obviously be to experiments upon the time 
relations of the venous curve in animals. But though there are 
differences of opinion, it must be understood that they are in the 
main of a minor character, and their complete elucidation is 
chiefly necessary that the origin of the waves may be accurately 
determined. The differences which have so far arisen are, in 
other words, of such a nature as to in no way invalidate the 
information which can be obtained at the bedside. 

The majority of writers are agreed in timing the commence- 
ment of the “c” wave in man as synchronous or almost syn- 
chronous with the primary wave of the carotid at the same level 
of the neck. Fredericq ('°), moreover, has recently shown them 
to be simultaneous in the carotid and jugular of the dog. For 
practical purposes the two waves may be taken as starting 
together, but the actual evidence for their absolute synchronicity 
is perhaps not convincing. In certain instances Fredericq found 
a slight deviation, and Bard (?”) has obtained similar results 
in man.! 

The “a” wave and “x” dépression have together a duration 
of 4 to 4 sec. (Mackenzie, Gibson ('*), and many other writers), 
and constitute the a-c interval. This a-c interval, the time dis- 
tance between the beginnings of the waves representing the com- 
mencement of auricular and ventricular systole, is taken as a 
measure of the function of the heart in respect of the conduction 
of impulses from auricle to ventricle, and is of great clinical 
importance. 

Over the relations of the “v” wave there is some difference 
_ of opinion, and this apparently for several reasons. In the first 
place, it shows considerable variation in form; it may show a 
division into two; it assumes an entirely new outline and new 


1 Bard’s tracings (?») are not beyond criticism. It is not expressly stated that 

jhe jugular and carotid curves were taken at the same level in the neck, and the 
deviation might be attributable to transmission delay. Professor Bard has recently 
ublished further notes ('*) on this subject. Bachmann has confirmed his results 
Amer. Journ, Med. Sci., vol. 136, 1908, p. 674). 


90 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


time relations in conditions where there is engorgement of the 
right side of the heart. Secondly, different writers refer it to 
different standard events, which constitutes a wide source of 
error, and but rarely indicate the evidence from which the stan- 
dards are derived. Thirdly, it is frequently impossible to be certain 
whether writers are referring to the actual time relations of the 
events, or whether a phase of the “v” wave is attributed to a 
particular ventricular movement. It so happens that a definite 
statement of the relationship of the “v” wave to a standard 
movement is not of common occurrence. The collected evidence 
as to the time relations in the normal subject is so involved that 
it is impossible to consider it in detail. It is held by the majority 
that the wave commences during the systolic plateau. Gerhardt ("*) 
and Wenchebach consider that it is a purely diastolic event. 
Gerhardt times its occurrence with the commencement of the . 
dicrotic wave; and Wenchebach finds it synchronous with a 
notch on the downstroke of the cardiogram. The evidence of the 
tracings of individual writers is nevertheless in favour of the 
view that the point of commencement is subject to variation, and 
that it may commence during the plateau or near its termination. 
Its termination in the “y” depression is universally stated to be 
synchronous with the opening of the auriculo-ventricular valves. 
The evidence for this last relationship does not appear to be so 
conclusive as one might expect from the statements made on the 
subject. Thus in Mackenzie’s tracings the point as a rule corre- 
sponds to the bottom of the cardiographic downstroke,! while in 
certain of those of Wenchebach (as in Figs. 6 and 7 of his paper), 
it starts at a point distinctly later. A critical examination of 
the subject reveals many sources of fallacy, and the majority of 
tracings are taken on comparatively slow-moving surfaces, so that 
the error in marking the points is large. Later when cesophageal 
tracings are discussed it will be seen that the fixation of the “y” 
depression at the A.O. line involves an obvious discrepancy. 

Other waves have been described on the venous tracing; a 
positive wave preceding the “a” wave (Gibson and Ritchie’s 
sinous wave |’); a positive wave between “a” and “c” (Bard’s 
accident inter-systolique '*); a fourth positive wave following “v” 
(Morrow’s second outflow wave *°). 
opinion on the subject, and these waves cannot be regarded as 

1 Or to the bottom of the dicrotic notch. Potain and Gottwalt agree. 


There is no unanimity of — 


THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 91 


_ fundamental and constant, or at present of any great practical 
significance. It would be premature and unprofitable to discuss 
them. 


(ce) The Evidence obtained from Intra-cesophageal Trac- 
ings.—By means of an cesophageal bougie, fitted with a small elastic 
balloon, tracings have been obtained from the heart, which are 
not without value. The auricular curves are from the left auricle, 
which lies in contact with the gullet. They are to be used-with 
caution, for they suffer in a measure from the same defect as do 
cardiographic curves; it is not possible to say to what extent 
they are volume and to what extent pressure curves. Moreover 
they show considerable variation according to the level at which 
the balloon is placed (Young and Hewlett®’). In dogs such 
tracings were obtained as early as 1888 by Fredericq, and.recently 
Minkowski (*5), Rautenberg (****), Joachim (*’), Young and Hewlett, 
and others have secured them in man. The tracings are in the 
main in fair agreement, and show curves similar to those of 
intra-auricular pressure in animals. Disagreement, however, has 
arisen over that portion of the curve representing auricular 
contraction. 

According to the earlier observations, auricular contraction is 
indicated by a depression in the tracing, whereas Rautenberg (***), 
in all tracings but one (Fig. 5), attributes a convexity to the same 
event. ° 

Now cardiographic curves from the ventricles, when taken 
from any other point than the apex of the left ventricle, show a 
curve which is roughly an inverted picture of the intra-ventricular 
_ pressure. In one respect the picture shows no inversion, the 
auricular wave when present retains its intra-ventricular form. 
Briefly, those portions of the curve are inverted which are de- 
pendent on active movements of the ventricular wall. In the 
same way it would be expected that when intra-auricular and 
cesophageal curves were compared, those portions of the ceso- 
phageal curve would show inversion, which were independent_of 
ventricular movements. The curves as interpreted by Fredericq, 
Joachim, and Minkowski fulfil this expectation; Rautenberg, on 
the other hand, finds as a rule no inversion of any part of the curve. 
As Hering states, it is difficult to understand how a contracting 
left auricle can raise the cesophageal pressure, at a point at which 


92 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


this chamber is alone in contact with the gullet. Rautenberg 
has attempted to defend his position by taking tracings in animals 
from the cesophagus, simultaneously with others from the right 
auricle, the apex beat or jugular vein. But his tracings do not 
offer convincing evidence, and necessitate the assumption that the 
apex beat is long delayed, an assumption which is not borne out 
by the simultaneous tracings from apex beat and cesophagus. It 
must be admitted that the auricular contraction may give a 
negative curve, and if at the same time it is to be recognised that 
a positive curve may result, the possibility of intermediate types, 
half negative and half positive, introduces serious difficulties into 
the interpretation of the tracings. The same problem arises in 
the case of those curves obtained 

ab G from the auricle through the chest 
PNY wall. Erlanger’s curves taken from . 

the auricle, and already referred to, 

show the auricular waves inverted, 


eed while Rautenberg’s curves (#4) re- 

H . . . . 
Sa corded in a similar manner are in- 
Fic. 2. terpreted as showing a convex wave. 


Here again there is the necessary 
assumption of delay in the appearance of the apex beat, some- 
times amounting to‘lsec. An illustration (Fig. 2 of this article), 
based on one of Rautenberg’s curves (***) (No. 2), will make this 
point clearer. The upper curve is from a point on the chest wall 
overlying the right auricle; the lower curve is from the apex 
beat. Four corresponding points have been marked on the 
curves, the upper of which is open to two interpretations. 
According to Rautenberg, auricular contraction is represented 
by a-c, and there is a delay, c—d, between the commence- 
ment of systole and the upstroke of the cardiogram. On the 
other hand, it may justly be argued that the contraction of the 
auricle is represented by b—d, and that the curve is inverted ; 
and this view receives confirmation from two sources. In the first 
place, other observers do not find delays in the upstroke of the 
cardiogram of a nature comparable to those given by Rautenberg ; 
and secondly, Rautenberg’s cardiographic curve itself shows a rise 
which can only be attributed to the auricular wave, namely, at 0. 
In view of the argument which might be raised, that the auricular 
wave is delayed, it would perhaps be safer to await further work 


THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 93 


before pronouncing a definite opinion as to the time relations 
of the auricular contraction as represented in esophageal 
curves ; and this the more as the example given is one of the 
most simple. 

It is requisite that the delay in onset of the “a” wave in the 
neck should be definitely ascertained, but at Febiets the data are 
insufficient." 

(Esophageal curves show the second positive wave with great 
constancy (Fredericq, Joachim, Minkowski, Rautenberg, and 
Young and Hewlett), and its onset is said to coincide with 
ventricular systole. Young and Hewlett, and Rautenberg agree 
in showing that it occurs on these curves earlier than the “c” 
wave upon the jugular tracings. According to the former, the 
delay ® is ‘1 sec. ; according to the last named, ‘079 sec.* (average 
of six observations). The necessity of fixing the time difference be- 
tween the second positive and the “c” wave will be obvious when 
the following facts are taken into consideration. As we have seen 
the a-c interval is of great clinical importance, and is usually of 
‘2 sec. duration as recorded with the polygraph from the jugular 
vein in the neck. Such evidence as we possess tends to show that 
the time difference between the onset of auricular and ventricular 
contraction, as registered by cesophageal and direct auricular curves 
in man and animals, is less, and amounts to little more than ‘1 
sec. The difference in the two figures can only be accounted for, 
assuming them to be correct, -by the quicker appearance of the 
“a” wave in the neck. | 

In cesophageal tracings a third positive wave is present. Here, 
as in the case of the “ v ” wave, opinion is divided as to its moment 
of onset. Minkowski finds that it commences with the second 
sound; Young and Hewlett represent it as commencing in the 
middle of the ventricular plateau, and ending at or a little before 


1 The electrocardiogram taken simultaneously with the jugular curve should give 
a speedy answer to this question. A few such curves obtained by the author show 
a delay of approximately ‘08 sec. (For which see appended note and Fig. 3.) 

2 It is necessary to anticipate the discussion of the identity of the two waves. 
The full evidence will be considered later. = 

* The figures are in agreement with other values, for allowing ‘05 sec. for the — 
_ presphygmic interval and ‘03 sec. for transmission from aorta to carotid or sub- 

clavian, we obtain a delay in the arterial shock of -08 sec 
* Einthoven's electrocardiograms show an interval varying from -1 to ‘2 sec. 
between onset of auricular and ventricular — change. The average is about 
“14 sec. in man. 


94 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


the opening of the auriculo-ventricular valves. In these par- 
ticulars as to the positive wave the curves of Rautenberg agree 
in that the third negative wave starts at the end of the plateau. 
According to Young and Hewlett the wave occurs ‘1 sec. earlier 
than the “v”’ wave in the neck, and they attribute much of the 
confusion which has arisen in its interpretation to a neglected 
consideration of this delay. Rautenberg finds a delay both in 
man and animals; the average of observations on six human 
subjects gives the figure at ‘081 sec. 

If the diagram (Fig. 1) is again referred to, the discrepancy 
mentioned at the end of the last section will be understood. The 
majority of the intra-auricular pressure diagrams show the third 
negative. waye as commencing at the A.O. line. If the “y” 
depression is fixed at the same point, no delay is allowed for. For 
this reason a curve more nearly corresponding to those given by 
Porter of intra-auricular pressure is also introduced. If the 
third negative wave is regarded as terminating on the A.O. line, 
it becomes necessary to move the “y” depression to a position 
indicated in the figure by the broken line, marked +. 

In connection with the delays, it is important to note that 
transmission of pressure waves in the venous channels is slower 
than in the arterial, for the tension is less in them, and the waves 
travel ‘against and not with the stream. Young and Hewlett 
estimated the rate of transmission in a case of tricuspid failure 
at 1:2 m. per sec. Rautenberg’s figures indicate a transmission 
velocity of 1:5 m. per sec. Morrow found in animals a variation 
in velocity from 1 to 3 m. per sec.” 

The main conclusions to be drawn from cesophageal curves 
are, first, the similarity of intra-auricular curves in man and 
animals ; and secondly, the fact that the separate waves of the 
jugular pulse show an appreciable delay in transmission from 
auricle to neck.* They are also of importance in providing a 

1 A point estimated apparently as half way down the downstroke of the cardio- 
gram. It frequently happens, as in this instance, that the evidence upon which 
this standard instant is timed is not fully stated. 

2 The rate of transmission of the several waves is not of necessity the same. As 
will subsequently be seen, it is improbable that they are all of the same nature. 

° Of further conclusions which have been drawn, there is one which deserves 
notice. Joachim and Rautenberg have shown in certain cases of heart block that 
right and left auricles beat in unison. The statement is only approximately true 


and applies to clinical work. Some recent observations may be quoted: in this 
connection. Stassen (**) finds that the right auricle in dogs contracts °02 sec. 


‘THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 95 


‘means of recording the movements of the left auricle. As a 
routine practice the method is naturally impossible, and con- 
traction of the left auricle is also frequently represented on the 
cardiographic curve. In this connection it is of interest to notice 
that the auricular wave in the cardiogram often shows a shortened 
duration, as does that in the cesophageal, when compared to that 
in the jugular. 

The evidence so far considered has led to certain aaa 
as to the time relations of the events of the cardiac cycle in the 
normal human subject, and these conclusions have been incor- 
porated as carefully as possible in the figure given on page 88. 
The subsequent discussion will be based to a large extent on the 
assumption that the events are correctly represented, That they 
are approximately correct is beyond doubt, but that they are 
rigidly correct is perhaps improbable. The diagram represents 
the conclusions which the evidence we possess to-day appears to 
warrant. 


III. THe INTERPRETATION OF THE AURICULAR PRESSURE 
CURVE AND THE JUGULAR PULSE TRACING 


In passing to a consideration of the events involved in aes 
production of the three main waves and three main depressions 
of the tracings, it will first be convenient to examine the ultimate 
factors to which each in its turn is due, whether it occurs on the 
jugular, cesophageal, cardiographic, or auricular curve, and then 
to briefly notice, where necessary, the modifications which occur 
in the jugular tracing by the interposition of interfering factors. 


(a) The First Positive or “a” Wave.—tThe presystolic 
onset of this wave leaves little doubt that it is due to the con- 
traction of the auricle, and it is universally attributed to this 
cause. According to Fredericq ('°) it disappears from the 
tracing, when as a result of tetanisation of the auricle, this 
chamber ceases to beat. In ‘cases of partial heart block, in 
which the auricles maintain a rhythm which is a multiple of the 
before the left; that on the other hand the left ventricle contracts ‘03 to ‘04 sec. 


before the right ; and that the delay between the onset of auricular and ventricular 
systole is ‘08 to ‘10 sec. Schmidt-Nielsen’s figures (**) for the delay from right to 


~ left auricle agree with those of Stassen. Compare Fredericq’s remarks (!). 


1 Hering (”*) has confirmed this result by the use of vagal inhibition. 


— oe 


96 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


ventricular rhythm, the wave occurs more frequently upon the — 
jugular tracing and at equal time intervals. Experimentally it 
has also been shown that cessation of ventricular contraction does 
not affect it. The wave is consequently established as due to 
auricular contraction and auricular contraction only. 

The question arises as to how it is propagated in the neck. 
It may be supposed to result from regurgitation of blood into the 
veins at the auricular systole. Such a view could apply only to 
its production in the veins in the immediate neighbourhood of 
the heart. But whether such regurgitation actually takes place is 
by no means easy to settle. For even assuming that the right 
auricle is supplied at the entrance of the swperior vena cava with a 
specialised band of muscular tissue, the tenia terminalis (Keith), 
which effectually closes the orifice by its guillotine action, yet it 
might be supposed that a slight degree of regurgitation occurred 
prior to the complete closure. The most direct evidence which ~ 
we possess is that given by Frangois-Franck, and this applies solely 
to the veins of the neck. He found on fitting a Chauveau’s instru- 
ment into these veins (in the donkey and horse) that there was 
no sign of reflux, as indicated by a retrograde movement of the 
blood column, at any period of the cardiac cycle. 

Secondly, it may be attributed to a positive and centrifugal 
wave of pressure originating in the auricle, or at the mouth of 
the superior vena cava, and ascending the vein. Thirdly, the 
wave may be regarded as due to the filling of the veins when the 
outlet is obstructed ; namely, as consequent upon stasis. So far 
as the second and third views are concerned there is little or no 
evidence on which to base a discussion. The former tends to 
recognise the wave as one mainly of pressure, the latter as one 
mainly of volume. The curves obtained from the neck by means 
of the tambour and pelotte are chiefly pressure curves, while the. 
receiving capsule, usually used, yields curves for the most part 


1 The instrument used was Chauveau’s hemodromograph. Both Frangois- 
Franck and Chauveau and Faivre state that they have observed regurgitation from 
the auricle into the roots of the great veins. But Francois-Franck says he is con- 
vinced that there is no reflux into the neck veins by experiments too long to report. 

The evidence for the tributaries of the inferior vena cava is even less complete, 
There is of course every reason to suppose that it occurs in pathological conditions, 
Experimental work on the subject is rendered very difficult, as the opening of the 
chest which is usually necessary, produces serious alterations in the pressures of 
the veins and right heart. 


EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 97 


re ntative of volumes (Bard); it is possible that a careful 
comparison of curves obtained by the two methods might throw 
some light on the subject. 

But whatever its method of propagation, the main fact rests 
on an assured basis; the wave may be taken as an indication of 
auricular contraction. , 


(b) The Second Positive or “c” Wave.—tThe discussion 
which centres around this wave involves many questions of 
considerable intricacy. : 

It is sometimes held that no such wave occurs in the curve of 
intra-auricular pressure, but the tracings of Porter and Fredericq 
demonstrate it too clearly and constantly to allow of doubt. 
While the weight of evidence is in favour of its presence, yet Frey 
and Krehl fail to show its presence in their tracings, and recently 
Hering (?**) has published a similar curve. Hering’s experiments 
were performed under perfusion; the chest wall was removed ; 
and the conditions generally were of an extremely artificial 

character.” 
In man its presence is attested not by the jugular curves so 
- much as by the cesophageal, and the results of such observations 
warrant the statement that it is a normal event of the human 
intra-auricular pressure curve. 

It is timed by all those who admit its occurrence as commencing, 
in the auricle with ventricular.systole. According to Fredericq 
the wave persists when by faradisation the auricle is caused to 
fibrillate, and when the chest is opened. It disappears when the 
ventricle ceases to beat. The wave must consequently have its 
origin in ventricular systole.® 

It is often held to originate in the bulging of the auriculo- 
_ ventricular valves, at the onset of the systole (Chauveau, Fredericq, 
and Porter). Chauveau in examining the heart of a living horse 
(Chauveau and Faivre*) inserted his finger into the beating 

1 Porter estimated the auricular pressure in the dog during auricular contraction 
at9mm. Hg. This and other auricular pressures are given by Porter at page 533, 
and will be subsequently. quoted. They are the pressures from one curve. . 

® Moreover it must be stated that Hering (*) firmly believes in the presence 
of this wave in the auricular pressure curve. He has recently commented upon 
Song’ q his own curve and also that of Cushny and Grosh, published by 
_% Similar experiments have been performed by Fredericq with the chest wall 
act, either by vagal stimulation, or by stimulating through the chest wall. 

' G 


t 


98 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


auricle, and reported that the ring is not completely closed in 
ventricular systole, for the multiple dome of the tricuspid valves 
is palpable at this phase.1 It must be remembered that such an 
experiment is conducted with the thorax open, and also that 
the palpating finger is frequently misled in attempting to gauge 
changes of volume where there are also changes of pressure. Ina 
consideration of the possibility of this bulging the experiments of 
Roy and Adami are of importance. These observers recorded the 
contractions of the papillary muscles by means of a hook passed 
through the wall of the auricle and around a chorda; they 
registered, simultaneously, the movements of the heart wall itself. 
The conclusion arrived at was that there is an appreciable delay 
between the onset of ventricular wall and papillary muscle con- 
traction. Now this conclusion has been called in question by 
Haycraft and Paterson. In the freshly excised heart they found 


the contraction of the papillary muscles and adjoining part of the 


ventricular wall to be synchronous. The subject has recently 
received further attention from Saltzman (#4), who experimented 
on the perfused heart. His findings, though they lack complete 
uniformity, on the whole favour the view that the musculature 
of the heart contracts according to the length of the branches of 
the Purkinje system which supply the particular part considered. 
The order of contraction in his typical experiments is as follows : 
First, the base of the heart; secondly, the papillary muscles and 
adjoining wall; and thirdly, the apex. Previous results, which 
were conflicting, are thus brought nearer into line, thovgh they 
do not show complete conformity.2 In four experimeuts the base 
contracted on the average -031 sec. before the papillary muscles, 
and the papillary muscles -036 sec. before the apex. There is a 
variation on either side of these figures of ‘02 sec. Hering, how- 
ever, in a preliminary note (**), states that he finds the papillary 
muscles contract before either base or apex. Whatever the actual 
events, the delayed pull of the papillary muscles and chorde 
observed by Roy and Adami requires explanation, and it appears 
rational to assume that it is due to delayed contraction, stretching 
of the chords, or to a relatively greater shortening of the ven- 
tricular muscle, and it constitutes evidence that under the con- 
ditions of the experiment the valves bulge back into the auricle. 


1 Fredericq (}*) makes the same statement. 
2 In some cases Saltzman found the order of contraction reversed or irregular, 


{s 


THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 99 


At the moment when the valves may be supposed to balloon 
into the auricle, that chamber is rapidly enlarging, and it may be 
held that any decrease in the capacity of the auricles from the 
former source will be fully compensated by the dilatation of the 
organ. But granted that a positive wave of pressure is started 
by the tricuspid valves,! its appearance in a recognisable form upon 
the curve of intra-auricular pressure will depend on the relation- 
ship of the pressures represented in this wave and the pressures 
produced by other influences tending to expand the auricle; thus 
there is no reason to deny the possibility of the appearance of the 
wave on the auricular pressure curve. Again its conduction into 
the veins of the neck will depend on the relationship of the rate 
at which the wave is propagated and the rate at which the blood 
enters the auricle, and, assuming its origin in the auricle, there 
is every reason to believe that this relationship is such as fo allow 
of conduction. 
From the collected evidence we may conclude that the second 
a positive wave is a real auricular event in man and that it may appear 
tn the neck as a component part of the jugular pulse. 
* In considering the causal factors of the “c” wave in the neck 
z further complications arise. The shock of arterial pulsation may 
5 be transmitted to the receiver, for it occurs at or about the instant 
: when the “c” wave has its onset. There is also the possibility 
of a direct conduction of the arterial pulsation, either aortic, in- 
nominate, or carotid to the accompanying veins with which these 
vessels are in contact. Each of these factors has been advocated 
in turn as the chief element in the production of the “c” wave 
(cp. Friedreich *, p. 289, Belski*). The leading points and 
arguments alone require consideration. 
It is held by Mackenzie, Gerhardt and Wenchebach that the 
“ec” wave is due to the impact of the neighbouring artery ? alone. 
Given in the main by Mackenzie, the chief evidence in favour of 


* ‘1 The origin of the second positive wave in the bulging of the auriculo-ventricular 
valves cannot be regarded as proven, though it appears to be the most rational view 
to hold of its production. Hering has convinced himself that it is independent of 

__ the shock of the root of the aorta, and auricular branches of the same, by numerous 

experimental researches, but he does not detail the evidence (**). 

? While the actual statement is frequently made that the impact is from the 
carotid, yet it is understood that an arterial impact is intended, and that the 
?p r artery involved is that which lies in the neighbourhood of the receiver 
means of which the tracing is obtained. -In the light of Keith’s anatomia 
cription it would appear that the artery is usually the subclavian. 


100 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


this view is as follows: The proof of the occurrence of the second 
positive wave in the auricle is not considered as final’; tracings 
of an obviously arterial nature are obtained with the greatest 
ease from many points of the neck; as the receiver is moved 
higher in the neck the tracing gradually acquires the arterial 
character and eventually loses all trace of jugular waves; the 
wave is said to be synchronous with the primary wave of the 
carotid pulse ; it is occasionally absent from the jugular tracing 
when the latter has a large amplitude ;? it is said to be absent 
from a type of liver pulse known as the auricular liver pulse. 

There are many observations in opposition to this evidence. 
Morrow (°"») finds that clamping the carotid ‘near its origin does 
not affect the wave.’ Fredericq ('*°) states that in animals the 
wave persists when the artery is completely separated from the 
vein from which the record is taken. Bard (1°) has also noted the 
““c¢”? wave in a subclavicular vein, isolated from all arteries, and 
Rautenberg has recorded it in its full dimensions in the superior 
vena cava of a dog. Hering states that in heart bigeminus where 
the second pulse beat has a longer presphygmic interval than the 
first, there is no corresponding delay in the appearance of the 
““¢” wave in the neck. 

There can be no doubt that in many tracings of the jugular 
pulse the arterial shock contributes to the “c” wave; the diffi- 
culty is frequently in avoiding the arterial pulsation. The main 
question is as to whether it is the only factor, or as to whether 
on occasion it may contribute but slightly or even take no part 
in its production. The character of the wave is of importance. 
In many tracings it is very prominent and peaked, and shows 


1 This question has already been dealt with as fully as space will permit. 
It is questioned if the apparatus used in recording auricular pressures may not tend 
to the production of the wave. The apparatus has varied very greatly, as has 
also its position in the process of recording ; yet the majority of the curves show 
the same characters. Porter has registered the wave in the pulmonary veins, and 
Rautenberg in the superior vena cava ; more recently Delchef has recorded the wave 
in the inferior vena cava (Archiv. Internat. d. Physiol., vii. 1908, p. 96). 

2 Hering (*) explains this by the magnitude of the “a” wave in the tracings 
given, and states that the “ carotid shock” may also disappear. 

3 Morrow holds that the essential part of the ‘“‘c” wave is conducted from the 
auricle where it may be produced by— 

1. A force exerted during ventricular systole, through the auriculo-ventricular 
valves. i 

2. Contraction of the ring of muscle in the auriculo-ventricular junction. 

3. Pressure exerted upon the auricles by the systolic twist of the heart. 


_ THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 101 


no outward resemblance to the waves of the pure carotid pulse. 
Briefly, in such cases it must be assumed that the primary wave 
is alone transmitted, an assumption which it is difficult to make. 
It may be asked why, if the primary wave is recorded, the 
dicrotic should not also leave its impression on the tracing. Again, 
it requires as a rule a firmer pressure to bring out a maximum 
arterial tracing than it requires for the venous. Venous tracings 
of the greatest amplitude are often to be obtained by the lightest 
application compatible with closure of the mouth of the receiver. 
We have further seen that the synchronicity of the “c” wave 
and the carotid shock has not been fully established (Bard and 
Bachmann). The tracings given to show simultaneous onset often 


_ show a slight deviation, which may of course be explained by a 


difference in level at which they were obtained and to a slight 
transmission delay. The argument that the “c” wave is absent 
from the liver pulse is the strongest of those sibicht in favour 
of its arterial origin, and has not yet met with a satisfactory 
explanation. The liver tracings are of a very complex nature, 
combining expansile pulsation with up and down movements of 
the whole organ. Rautenberg (**°) has given a tracing in which 
the ““c” wave appears to be present, but it is possible that in 
this instance insufficient care was employed to avoid the trans- 
paved shock from the aorta. The author is of opinion that the 
*“¢” wave is occasionally visible in the veins of the neck. 

Finally, it cannot be affirmed at present that the “c” wave is 
purely arterial or purely venous in origin, and while : cannot be 
denied that both factors may be contributory under certain conditions, 
it is highly probable that in one case the arterial and in another the 
venous element will predominate. 

As to whether there is a transmitted shock from the aorta or its 
branches direct to the veins, there is little evidence beyond that 
already examined. Fredericq points out that, as the rate of trans- 
mission is dissimilar in artery and vein, the view is incompatible 
with the opinion that the waves appear together in the neck. 
Morrow uses, as an argument against such transmission, the fact 
that the “c” wave is frequently absent from the femoral venous 
curve, though the venous channel which connects the femoral vein 
to the heart is throughout in close contact with pulsating arteries. 


~ 2 The question has been recently discussed. more fully by the author (Brit. Med. 
Journ., Nov. 1905). 


102 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


Whatever the ultimate factors involved, the practical outcome 
remains unaffected. The “c” wave in the jugular may be safely 
taken for clinical purposes as synchronous with the primary wave of 
the arterial pulse in the neck at the same point, and it thus forms a 
valuable standard in the interpretation of tracings. 


(c) The Third Positive or “v” Wave.—The chief difficulty 


ce ” 


in interpreting the third positive or “v” wave lies in the dif- 
ference of opinion in regard to its instant of onset. The ex- 
planation of its occurrence, as given by any particular author, 
depends upon the time relations which he accepts for its various 
phases. Thus, those who believe in its onset during systole 
of the ventricle, consider that the venous flow which fills the 
auricle during this phase is an important contributory cause 
(Potain, Porter, Gottwalt, Mackenzie, Morrow, Hering). Those 
who regard it as arising with the commencement of ventricular 
diastole, attribute it to an elevation of the auriculo-ventricular 
ring (Porter,? Gerhardt). A similar view is held by Wenchebach. 
It has also been attributed in part to dicrotic rebound at aorta 
and pulmonary orifices (Riegel *) ; and to tricuspid regurgitation * 
(Mackenzie). 

For our present purposes it will be convenient to regard the 


onset of the ““v” wave as inconstant in position, and to briefly 


discuss those factors held to take part in the production of the 
wave, either in that portion of it which is said to occur before, 


1 The second positive wave was estimated by Porter as representing an auricular 
pressure of 5 mm. Hg. 

2 Porter recognised it as a contributory cause. 

3 Cp. criticisms of Gerhardt and remarks by Hering (**).- 

4 The difficulties of this question are very great. That the wave is enhanced 
when there are evidences of tricuspid regurgitation is generally admitted. The 
opinion rests chiefly on the proposition that tricuspid reflux may be a normal 
event. Such a reflux is not admitted by physiologists, but is strongly held 
by the northern schools of clinical medicine. The evidence cited is the 
presence of a systolic murmur, regarded as an indication of tricuspid insufficiency. 
Whether those presenting such a murmur, a sound which is said to be of common 
occurrence, are to be recognised as falling within the category of normal subjects, 
is a question outside the bounds of this article. Full references and many inter- 
esting observations may be found in the writings of Gibson (!") and in the first 
articles of Mackenzie. Our ignorance of the conditions under which tricuspid 
leakage may occur in its earlier stages appears to be very great, and little or no 
experimental work has been done on the subject. Statements attributing to 
Gibson the view that regurgitation is a factor in the production of the ‘‘v” wave 
are without foundation. 


THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 103 


or in that which it is claimed occurs subsequent to the S.C. 
closure. . 

During the systole of the ventricle, the auricular pressure falls 

and the blood pours into that chamber. There is no exit for the 

stream and the reservoir must gradually fill. If the systolic 
plateau is prolonged or the filling is rapid, it is reasonable to expect 
that the pressure in the auricle will rise, for any influences, other 
than the passive one, which tend to dilate the auricle, must of 
necessity be diminishing during the last phases of ventricular 
systole. When in a venous or auricular curve, there is a rise 
which can but be attributed to events occurring before the ter- 
mination of systole, it is rational to attribute it to this auricular 
filling and to a stasis wave passing back into the veins. As yet 
no curves indicative of the changing velocities in the large veins 
have been obtained. 

The second possible factor in the production of the “v” 
wave, namely, the upward spring of the auriculo-ventricular 
junction at the beginning of diastole, must be dealt with at greater 

length. 

: The direction of movement of the different parts of the heart 
7 wall has been for many years the subject of contention, and there 
is accumulated evidence that the earlier observations, in which the 
heart was exposed, were fallacious. The experiments which chiefly 
concern us are those of Briicke (°) and Haycraft (74). The method ° 
employed was the same in each case, and is in all probability a 
: very accurate one. Needles were used, and the chest wall and 
heart muscle was transfixed. The needles carried light straw 
levers from the movements of which the excursion of the heart 
___-wall could be observed. As a result it was shown that in systole 
the apex is the only fixed point of the musculature, and that all 
other parts tend to move towards the mid-line and apex. The 
auriculo-ventricular line is considered by most authorities to have 
a decided downward movement in systole, and a corresponding 
upward fling in diastole. Keith, who regards the mouths of the 
great vessels as other fixed points, has recently laid much stress - 
on this movement of the A-V line. According to this author, 
whose researches are in the main anatomical, the systole of the 
ventricle causes expansion of the auricle, and diastole of the ven- 
_ tricle its collapse. The auricle is opened like a concertina, and 
_ the upward movement of the A-V line in diastole is said to pro- 


104 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


pagate a positive wave in the uppermost chamber.! Now the 
movements of the A-V line are also offered as an explanation of 
an earlier negative wave (second negative or “‘ x’” depression), and 
the view, as we shall subsequently see, has much to support it. 
If the descent of the A-V line is sufficient to produce a negative 
wave, its ascent must assuredly be adequate to determine a second 
wave of equal intensity but of opposite sign. The explanation is 
a feasible one, and there remains but one serious difficulty. A 
positive wave of the sort, if propagated, may be entirely swamped 
by other events occurring at or about the same time. And the 
event which has to be considered is the effect of the lowering of 
ventricular upon auricular pressure. It must be noted that the 
factor can only be of avail between the S.C. and A.O. points; and 
until there is more unanimity as to the timing of these events 


opinion must necessarily vary as to the part played by the A-V line | 


in the production of the “v”’ wave.? 

The interpretation of the “v”’ wave which has just been dis- 
cussed attributes it to a movement of the ventricle, and makes it 
independent of auricular contraction or relaxation. Fredericq has 
found that when the ventricle is thrown into a state of fibrillation, 
the wave is abolished. Frangois-Franck and Morrow, on the other 
hand, give curves in which some portion of it at least remains when 
the ventricle is no longer beating. It is for this reason that Morrow 
is more inclined to attribute it to inflow and stasis. In cases of 
heart block there is as a rule no “v” wave corresponding to the 
purely auricular beats, but in a tracing of Wenckebach’s a small 
wave is seen. It ts probable that both factors play a part in the 
production of the wave under certain circumstances ; that with quick 
filling of the auricle or sustained plateau the first will be prominent, 
and that with slower filling and quicker heart beat the pressure in the 
auricle will be low when the ventricle passes into diastole, and that 
as a consequence the tricuspid valves will open later. Under these 
circumstances the second factor may be the more pronounced. 


1 This movement of the auriculo-ventricular junction was described by Chauveau 
in 1887 (Assoc. frang. p. l’avanc, d. Sc.), in a paper in which he gives a figure (No. 
12), illustrating views very similar to those recently published by Keith. The 
movement is fully discussed by Fredericq (!*), and many other writers, 

2 Allowing the S.C. to A.O. instant at ‘04 to ‘06 sec., there is time for the 
production of an appreciable wave. But this time interval is very difficult to 
measure with any degree of accuracy, and it cannot be held that our estimates of 
it are at present anything more than approximate. 


vv 


‘THE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 105 


If the view is accepted that both are contributory causes, not 
only do many of the apparently discordant observations assume 
an aspect of greater harmony, but the occasional division of the 
wave at or about the time of S.C. closure is no longer difficult to 
appreciate.’ 


(d) The First and Second Negative Waves, or “x” and 
“x’” Depressions.—The two first depressions of auricular and 
jugular tracings may be considered together, for the line of 
descent is usually continuous, and is but broken by the second 
positive or “c” wave. The complete depression commences 
with the relaxation of the auricle,? and is continued well into 
the systolic plateau. The fall in pressure is a marked one, and 
attracted much attention from earlier writers, for it causes a 
collapse of the veins of the neck. Hunter (*°) regarded systole 
of the ventricle as a valuable aid to the filling of the auricle, by 
the creation of a partial vacuum in the chest. This view was 
readvanced by Briicke and Mosso. Francois-Franck performed 
some experiments with a schematic apparatus to demonstrate its 
ft possibility. But Francois-Franck himself, and also Gottwalt and 
. Riegel, clearly showed that it is a factor of little importance, for 
the fall remains after the chest is opened. Nevertheless it cannot 
be denied that it contributes, for the systole of the ventricle pro- 
duces a fall of intra-pleural pressure, which may be recorded by a 
manometer in connection with that cavity. It is certain that it 
is not the main cause of the fall. 

Active relaxation of the auricle has never been demonstrated, 
and its enlargement in the normal chest is attributable at its 
onset to the low pressure in the chamber surrounding it (Frangois- 
Franck). But the auricular relaxation is in itself insufficient to 
account for the complete fall,* though it suffices to explain that 
portion of it which occurs before the commencement of ventricular 


| a eg 


1 Bard finds this notch so constantly that he considers, and perhaps with justi- 

fication, that the origin of the separate’ portions of the wave should be separately 
_ discussed. It must be noted that Hering and Rautenberg strenuously deny. the 
intervention of the second factor. 

Porter gives the third positive wave a pressure value of 5 mm. Hg. 

* Porter found that it occurred, as a rule, a little before relaxation of the 
auricular appendix. This writer estimates the value of the complete fall at —10 
mm. Hg. 

* Fredericq’s tracings, with intact chest wall, in which auricle and ventricle 
dissociated. 


106 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


systole (first negative wave or “x” depression). Porter states that 
relaxation of the auricular appendix proceeds for a variable time, 
terminating at the beginning or end of the second positive wave ; 
but the observation is of little value, for under the conditions of his 
experiment the relaxation would depend in the main on passive 
filling. When the ventricle is inhibited by vagal inhibition in the 
dog, or if clamped off from the auricle in the tortoise, a negative 
wave is still present (Francois-Franck); it also occurs in the 
jugular pulse in certain cases of heart block when the ventricle 
fails to respond to the auricular contraction (Wenckebach). 
Morrow’s tracing (Fig. 12) shows similar characteristics ; but while 
the ventricle is inactive, the depression, though still present, is 
reduced in size. While Francois -Franck found it abolished by irri- 
tation of the auricle, Fredericq ('*°) has shown that this is not 
the case, and states (”*) that when the auricles have passed into . 
delirium, at each ventricular contraction the auricular appendices 
diminish in volume, and are, as it were, aspirated towards the cavity 
of the auricle, and the negative wave persists.1 

From these observations it is clear that the systole of the 
ventricle plays a considerable part in causing the depression, and 
it has been attributed by many writers to an event occurring at 
this time, namely, descent of the A-V line (Fredericq, Porter, 
Wenckebach, &c.). 

In concluding this section it may be said that the two negative 
waves which together form the most prominent depression of the 


1 Frangois-Franck’s observations were carried out with the chest open. Fredericq 
has controlled his own observations by repeating the experiments with the chest 
intact, the advantages of which are obvious. 

There is a fact which is often insufficiently appreciated, namely, that the 
circulation is completely obstructed at two points during approximately half the 
cardiac cycle. The obstructions are situated at the auriculo-ventricular valves, 
Nevertheless it is only during a small fraction of the cardiac cycle, during systole 
of the auricles, that blood is not pouring into the heart. The main function of the 
auricles is not to load the ventricles ; the ventricles fill well when the auricles are 
paralysed, and by comparison the auricles are small chambers. They serve mainly 
as reservoirs, and during their brief contraction the large veins in their vicinity 
adopt this function. In contracting the auricles return to the state of potential 
reservoirs, 

So the over-engorgement of the veins, which would otherwise occur, is prevented ; 
and so it happens that in spite of the fact that the circulation of man is never an 
open path, the flow of blood through peripheral vessels is constant and uninter- 
mitting. While in heart block yenous stagnation is rare, clinical observations of 
to-day point to its frequent origin in inefficient, ill-timed, or obstructed systole of 
the upper chambers. 


EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 107 


auricular and venous curves are due to three causes; the increased 
negative pressure in the chest, consequent on ventricular systole ; the 
auricular relaxation dependent on the original intra-pleural pressure ; 
and the stretching of the auricular walls as a result of ventricular 
systole. Of these causes, the first is insignificant; the second is 
most active during the early phases, and the third most prominent 
in the later phases of the depression. 


(e) The Third Negative or “y” Depression.—With the 
diastole of the ventricle, the pressure within it falls rapidly 
and may become markedly negative. The fall of pressure is 
accompanied by the opening of the auriculo-ventricular valves, 
and the blood contained in the auricle passes into the ventricle. 
As a consequence, the pressure in the auricle falls, and the depres- 
sion in the auricular tracing timed to occur with the opening of 
the valves } is universally attributed to this cause. The depression 
may be due not only to a relief of the previous stasis, but to a 
_ transmitted pressure wave of negative sign. To what extent the 
negative pressure is thus transmitted to the auricle is uncertain. 
Porter estimated the auricular pressure corresponding to this event 
at ‘+5 mm. Hg, and as a consequence concluded that the negative 
pressure in the ventricle has little direct effect upon the pressure 
in the auricle. Porter’s experiments were carried out on dogs and 


1 The timing of this instant in man and the dangers which the methods involve 
have been repeatedly referred to; to emphasise them now entails tiresome but 
necessary reiteration. The interpretation of the ‘‘y” depression may be correct, 
but it has not been arrived at by scrupulously accurate methods. It cannot be 
said that the intra-auricular curve falls at the A.O. instant, while the curves of 
Porter and those of Young and Hewlett and Rautenberg show it otherwise. 
Moreover the drop in the jugular curve must be placed later, to allow for the 
delay in transmission which undoubtedly occurs. So that if the drop in the 
jugular curve is to be placed at the A.O. instant, the drop in the human intra- 

_ auricular curve must be placed approximately 1 sec. earlier, at a point at or near 
the closure of the semilunar valves. 

Almost all writers, early and late, express the opinion that the drop occurs both 
on auricular and jugular curves at the A.O. instant, or refer to unanimous state- 
ments to that effect. And it is attributed by similar processes of reasoning to a 
definite cause: In the above synoptic account the same line is taken, but for that 
very reason this proviso is essential. For if the auriculo-ventricular valves are 
bulged into the auricle during early systole, they must remain bulged during the 
plateau, and when the ventricular pressure begins to fall the tension in the valves 
soaeeeg decrease, The fall in auricular pressure should therefore commence slightly 

than the A.O. point, namely, at or near the S.C. point. Porter, who took 
“ifferential curves of the pressures in the two caritios, actually found that the fall 
: auricular pressure commenced at this instant. — 


108 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


the chest was open. In this connection it must be remembered 
that although lower pressures have been recorded in the ventricle 
with the chest wall uninjured, yet pressures as low as —28 and 
— 38 mm. Hg have been found in dogs by Goltz and Gaule, de Jager 
and others,! with the thorax open. 

The jugular and cesophageal curves in man often show the third 
negative wave or “y” depression to be of considerable extent, 
and a relief of stasis is certainly insufficient to account for it. 


3 
¥ 
$ 
Ys 
bd 


Fic. 3. 


The evidence of the jugular pulse in this respect is occasionally 
such as to indicate that the inflow from the veins is as great 
during ventricular diastole as during ventricular systole. It is 
probable that the larger the ventricle the greater is its power as 
a suction pump. 


APPENDED NOTE 


In sending off the proofs, the author is able to include a simul- 
taneous jugular curve and electrocardiogram (Fig. 3): The figure 
shows :—time in % sec.; the jugular curve, taken photographically 


1 Full references to these obsexvations will be found in Porter’s article. 


with the polygraph, in which a delicate lever replaced the usual 
pen, which is heavy for the purpose; and an electrocardiogram 
taken in Professor Waller’s laboratory with an Einthoven “ string 
_ galvanometer.” The jugular curve has been marked with the 
_ letters used by Mackenzie and employed in the text. The 
_electrocardiogram has been marked with the letters used by 
Kinthoven ; P represents auricular contraction, R and T are the 
result of ventricular contraction. The curves were obtained from 
a lad with a prominent jugular pulse. : 

The a-c interval, as measured in the jugular tracing, in the 
first and last curves is ‘14 and ‘12 sec. respectively. The time 
interval between auricular and ventricular contraction, as measured 
in the electrocardiogram, is ‘11, -12, and ‘12 respectively. It will 
_ be seen that the delay in the appearance of “a” and ‘‘c” waves 
in the neck is practically identical, namely -12 sec., and that the 
a-c interval is, as taken by this method, less than } sec. The 
delay of -12 sec. in the appearance of the waves in the neck is the 
uncorrected value. A deduction of ‘04 sec. must be made for 
instrumental delay in the jugular curve. The true value is 
consequently -08 sec. 

The figures given are in agreement with others obtained from 
the same subject, and accord very closely with the estimated 
values quoted in the text. 


‘ 


BIBLIOGRAPHY 


1 Bard, (a) Jour. d. physiol. e. d. pathol. gén., mai 1906, pp. 454-459. 
(b) Ibid., pp. 466-479. (c) Archiv, d, Maladies du Coeur, &c., juin 1908, 
No. 6. 
* Baum, Verhandl. d. phys.-med. Gesell. z. Wiirzburg, N. F., Bd. 38, 
1906, s. 61-102. 
* Bayliss and Starling, Internat. Monatssch. f. Anat. U. Phys., Bd. 11, 
Hit. 9, 1894, pp. 426-435. 
* Belski, Zeitsch. f, klin. Med., Bd. 57, Hft. 5 u. 6, 1905, s. 565, 
5 Briicke, Vorlesungen iiber Physiologie, Wien, 1885, Bd. 1, Aufl. 4, s. 
178-179. 
* Chauveau and Faivre, Gaz. méd. de Paris, T. xi,, 1856, p. 406. : 
? Chauveau aud Marey, ibid., 1861, p. 675. 
§ Edgren, Skand. Archiv. f, Physiol., Bd. 1, 1889, s. 67-151. 
__ * Einthoven and Geluk, Archiv. f. d. ges. Physiol., Bd. 57, 1894, s, 
617-639, 
® Erlanger, Johns Hopkins Bulletin, No. 177, vol. 16, 1905, pp, 394-397. 


ce _ 


110 PULSE RECORDS IN THEIR RELATION TO 


1 Frangois-Franck, Gaz, hebdo. d. méd, e. d. chir., fév., mars, av. 1882, 
pp. 92, 225, and 255. 

12 Fredericq, (a) Archiv. d. Biol. (Paris), T. 8, 1888, pp. 497-622. (b) Archiv. 
Internat. Physiol., iv. 1906-7, pp. 57-75. (c) Ibid., v. 1907, pp. 1-25. 

8 Frey and Krehl, Archiv. f. Anat. u. Phys., 1890, s, 31-88. 

4 Friedreich, Deutsch, Archiv. f. klin, Med., Bd. 1, 1866, s. 241-291, 

% Galabin, Journ, Anat. and Physiol., vol. 10, 1876, pp. 297-319, 

16 Gerhardt, Archiv, f. exper. Path. u. Pharmak., Bd. 34, 1894, s. 402-445, 
and Bd. 47, 1902, s. 250-266. 

1 Gibson, Edinb. Med. Journ., 1880, vol. 25, pp. 979-991. 

18 Gibson and Ritchie, Practitioner, vol. 78, No. 5, 1907, p. 602. 

1 Gottwalt, Archiv. f. d. ges. Physiol., Bd. 25, 1881, s. 1-30. 

2% Griinmach, Archiv. f. path. Anat., Bd. 102, 1885, s. 569-577. 

* Haycraft, Journ. of Physiol., vol. 12, 1891, pp. 452 and 473. 

2 Haycraft and Paterson, Journ. of Physiol., vol. 19, 1896, p. 262. 

3 Hering, (a) Archiv. f. d. ges. Physiol., Bd. 106, 1904, s. 1-16. (b) 
Verhandl. d. Kongress f. in. Med., 23. Kongress, Miinchen, 1906, s. 138. 
(c) Deutsch. med. Woch., 1907, No. 46. (d) Zentralb. f. Physiol., Bd. 21, | 
No. 22. 

*4 Hewlett, Journ. Med. Research, O.S8., vol. 17, 1907-8, pp. 119-136. 

% Hunter, A Treatise on the Blood, Inflammation, &c., London, 1794, 
pp. 185 and 187. 

6 Hiirthle, (a) Archiv. f. d. ges. Physiol., Bd. 49, 1891, s. 29-104. (b) Ibid., 
Bd. 60, 1895, s. 263-290. (c) Ibid., Bd. 43, 1888, s. 399-437. 

27 Joachim, Berl. klin. Woch., Feb. 1907, s. 215-216. 

* Keith, Journ. of Anat. and Physiol., vol. 42, Oct. 1907, pp. 1-25. 

* Keyt, Sphygmography and Cardiography, London, 1887. 

3 Lancisi, De motu cordis et aneurysmatibus, 1740 (earlier ed., Romae, 
1728, is cited by Baum, loc. czt.). 

31 Tandois, Die Lehre vom Arterienpuls, Berlin, 1872, s. 307. 

8 Mackenzie, (a) Journ. of Path. and Bact., O.S., vol. 1, 1893, p. 53 ; ibid., 
vol. 2, 1894, pp. 84-154. (b) The Study of the Pulse, &., London, 1902. 
(c) Amer, Journ. Med. Scien., July 1907, pp. 1-23. (d) Diseases of the Heart, 
London, 1908. 

33 Marey, (a) Physiologie médicale de la circulation du Sang, Paris, 1863. 
()) La circulation du Sang, &c., Paris, 1881. 

34 Martius, Zeitsch. f. klin. Med., Bd. 13, 1888, s. 344 and 346. 

3 Minkowski, Zeitsch. f. klin. Med., Bd. 62, 1907, s. 371-384 ; and Deutsch, 
med. Woch., 1906, No. 31, s. 1248-1250, 

% Morgagnt, De selibus et caus. morbor., Napoli, 1762 (cited by Baum, 
loe. ctt.). 

8? Morrow, (a) Archiv. f. d. ges. Physiol., Bd. 79, 1900, s, 442-449. (b) 
B.M.J., Dec. 22, 1906, p. 1807. 

38 Mosso, Die Diagnostik des Pulses, u.s.w., Leipzig, 1879, s. 60-63. 

3° Porter, Journ, of Physiol., vol. 13, 1892, pp. 513-553. 

© Potain, Mém. d. 1. soc. méd. d. h. d. Paris, mai 1867, pp. 3-27. 

‘1 Riegel, Deutsch. Arciiiv. f. klin, Med., Bd. 31, 1882, s. 1-62. 

42 Rautenberg, (a) Deutsch. Archiv. f. klin, Med., Bd. 91, 1907, s, 251-290. 


\ 
\ 


\ ° 


i . | 
. « _ 


ad —— hare 
‘HE EVENTS OF THE HUMAN CARDIAC CYCLE 111 
Sy joen: Klin, Woch., iv. 1907, No. 21, s. 657. (c) Zeitsch. f. klin, Med., Bd. 
r 35, Sept. 18, 1908. (d) Berl. klin, Woch., iv. 1907, No, 46, s. 1478, 
* Roy and Adami, Practitioner, Bd. 44, 1890, 
“ Saltzman, Skand. Archiv, f. Physiol., Bd. 20, 1908, s. 232-248, 
*® Schmidt-Nielsen, Archiv. Internat. Physiol., iv. 1906-7, pp. 417-433. 
* Stassen, ibid., v. 1907, pp. 60-75. 
” Tigerstedt, Skand. Archiv. f. Physiol., Bd. 20, 1908, s, 249. 

“ Weldemeyer, Untersuch, ii. d. Kreislauf. d. Blutes, Hannover, 1828 
(cited by Baum, loc. cit.). 

_ ® Wenckebach, Archiv. f. Anat. ii. Physiol. (Physiol. Abth.), 1906, s. 
~=—- 297-354. 
+5 og Young and Hewlett, Journ. Med. Research, O.S., vol. 16, 1907, p. 497- 


THE VASCULAR SYSTEM AND BLOOD 
PRESSURE 


By LEONARD HILL 


EXPERIMENTAL OPERATIONS ON THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


CARREL has opened up a new field of experimental work by show- 
ing that it is possible to join together the divided ends of arteries 
or veins, or a vein with an artery. Having inserted thread 
through each end of the divided vessel, at three points in the 
circumference apart from one another, he brings the ends of the 
vessel together by these threads and stitches intima to intima 
with a very fine needle and a running thread.1_ The outer coats 
are finally stitched together. Then the blood flow being re- 
established, he watches for the slightest sign of oozing, and if 
any, stops it by further stitches. 

He has succeeded not only in stitching an artery to a vein, 
but in inserting a length of transplanted artery or vein between 
the ends of a divided artery. More wonderful than all, he has 
succeeded in transplanting whole organs from one animal to another. 
In one experiment he dissected a length of the carotid of a young 
dog, kept it in physiological salt solution for twenty days at 32° F., 
and finally inserted the piece in the course of the abdominal aorta 
of a cat. On the forty-eighth day after he opened the abdomen 
and found the artery of normal appearance and serving its func- 
tion well. On the seventy-seventh day the cat was in perfect 
condition.* 

‘“‘Seven months ago,” he writes, “one of the animals had the 

1 The needles are sixteen cambrics, the threads single strands of Chinese twist 
silk sterilised in paraffin oil. 

2 John Hunter stated that the arteries have considerable surviving power. On 


transplanting them he obtained union thirty-six hours after their removal from the 
body. Palmer's Edit., vol. iii. p. 157. 


3 Guthrie not only has repeated this experiment, but has inserted a length of 


artery hardened in formol; at the end of twenty-two days it was serving its 
function perfectly. The dead artery is the scaffold on which repair of the living 
vessel takes place. (Journ. Amer. Med. Amie 1908, 1. p. 1035.) 

1 . 


THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 113 


peripheral end of the external jugular vein united to the central 
end of the carotid artery. Now the circulation through the vein 
is apparently as active as it was on the day of the operation. In 
another case, where the jugular has numerous collaterals, there 
appears to be some dilatation of the main portion of the vessel, 
and feeble pulsations can be detected in the jugular of the other 
side. In three cases of reversal of the circulation through the 
thyroid gland the results seem to be permanent, even after 


Fic. 1.—External jugular united to Fic. 2.—Preparation of host for receipt 
carotid artery. Reversal of circulation of kidney graft. The viscera are covered 
through the thyroid gland. A method with pads wet with warm saline during 
of modifying the function of the gland the operation. Clips on divided aorta of 
by producing hyperemia. vena cava. 


several months. On no dog kept in the laboratory has oblitera- 
tion of the vessels occurred, even after several months. 

* At the present time we can only state that seven months after 
the operation the veins have performed the main arterial functions 
and that occlusion has not occurred.” 

Carrel and Guthrie amputated and replanted a dog’s thigh. 
“The circulation was easily and entirely re-established. The 
pulsations of the femoral, popliteal, and posterior tibial arteries 
were as strong as the pulsations of the corresponding arteries of 
the other side. The arterial circulation was seemingly normal. 
The capillary circulation was exaggerated, the temperature of the 

H 


114 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


limb being higher and its hue redder, owing doubtless to the fact 
that the vaso-motor nerves were cut. The venous circulation 
was observed to be good immediately after the operation.” The 
collodion dressing, however, contracted and impeded the circula- 
tion in the limb which became swollen, so that after fifty hours 
the animal was killed.1_ The most striking of these remarkable 
experiments is that of transplanting the kidneys from one cat 
to another. Secretion of urine may begin immediately after the 
arterial circulation is re-established. In the best cases the func- 


FS. Leh ae7/. 


Fic. 3.—The graft aay for insertion. Fig. 4.—The operation completed. 


tion of the kidney was for some twenty days almost normal. 
120 to 160 c.c. of urine a day were secreted, and urea in proportion 
to the proteid eaten. When fedl on raw meat 2°7 to 5:1 grms. of 
urea were passed per diem. The\cats were fat and in good health, 
with glossy skin and good appetites, playing, running, and jumping 
about the room. Nevertheless there was some albumen in the 
urine, and cedema of the kidneys leading to their slow and pro- 
gressive enlargement. Upon the twenty-ninth day one cat was 
well, and then gastro-intestinal symptoms set in and the animal 
died on the thirty-first day. 


1 Guthrie reports “a transplanted fore-limb without any serious derangement of 
metabolism, six days after the operation.” (Journ. Amer. Med. Assoc.,1908, li. p.1658.) 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 115 


In one case the kidneys of a middle-aged cat were successfully 
grafted on to a young adult. The animal lived as a normal cat 
for fifteen days, but then became emaciated and died on the thirty- 
sixth day. The arteries of the young cat, the costal and bronchial 
cartilages and all the scar tissue where it had been incised were 
calcified as hard as glass. The transplanted kidneys and renal 
vessels, on the other hand, showed no sign of calcification. The, 
kidneys were somewhat cedematous but showed no very serious 
pathological change. This result—the calcification of the host 


Fic. 5.—The cat three weeks after the operation. 


but not of the graft—was only obtained once, but it is one which 
opens up a wide vista of new ideas and research. 

In carrying out the operations the kidneys are deprived of 
circulation for more than an hour, and are washed out with 
Ringer’s solution. The vaso-motor nerves are cut—it has been 
proved that kidneys can functionate normally after this operation— 
and the anemia probably destroys the local renal ganglia, for 
nerve cells in the central nervous system do not recover function 
after at most twenty minutes! total deprivation of blood flow... It 
is very important in such operations that the veins be given their 
normal situation and direction. The writer’s explanation of this 
is that every departure from the normal course by preventing the 


1 In one case twenty-nine minutes (Guthrie). 


116 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


full action of the respiratory movements on the renal circulation 
leads to diminished flow and cedema of the transplanted organ. 
The capillaries and veins are naturally so arranged to run that 
each muscular action of the body promotes the flow in them to- 
wards the heart. It is important also that no bleeding take 
place into the connective tissues during or after the operation, as 
blood has an irritative effect and produces sclerosis. Bier has 
utilised this property, and injected blood to excite callous forma- 
tion in cases of pseudo-arthrosis. 

The experiments of Carrel bring just within the limits of the 


Fig. 6.—Operation method of J. E. Sweet (Journ. Hap. Med. vii. 163, 1905) for 
joining the portal vein and vena cava (Eck’s operation). The double thread is the 
wire of an electric cautery, and cuts the opening between the two vessels. 


possible the operation of transplanting from a man killed by an 
accident such an organ as the thyroid into the neck or other part, 
where it may carry on its function and compensate for thyroid 


inadequacy in the recipient. Guthrie has succeeded in exchang- | 


‘ing the ovaries of black and white Leghorn hens, and finds the 
foster-mother modifies the colour of the chicks hatched from the 
eggs laid afterwards. The controls gave pure white or black 
chicks. 


THe ELASTICITY AND CONTRACTILITY OF ARTERIES 


When the carotid artery of the ox or horse is exposed immedi- 
ately after death, it is found to be soft and flaccid, more or less 
flattened in section, with a large bore usually 5 or 6 mm. in diameter. 
On exposure to the air, cooling, and especially on manipulation 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 117 
the artery quickly becomes rigid and contracted, and circular in 
section, with a calibre greatly lessened, the diameter being re- 
duced to 2 or 3mm. orless. The artery becomes often so stiff that 
a piece 7 or 8 mm. long may be held by one end in an almost 
horizontal position. Arteries left undisturbed in situ for a day or 
two after death and then exposed may show little sign of contrac- 


Ba 


a » 


nth 
il 


Vchichs 


Se 


LM 


os 
—_ 


‘se — yi oe 


ITS 


—— 
———— — 
— 


{ 


3 W3 
i 
i 


Gi 


A 


/2chicks 


Fic. 7.—B2, white o ry grafted on black ; fertilisation by white cock. Ws, black 
ovary grafted on white ; fertilisation by black cock (Guthrie). 


tion at first, but under the influence of manipulation may soon 
contract strongly and persistently, the contracted state lasting for 
q days. John Hunter noticed that the arteries of the umbilical cord — 
contracted up to the third day, and not on the fourth. Freezing 
relaxes permanently the arteries, so that if the “pluck” of a 
freshly killed ox be bought, and one piece of carotid be taken and 
frozen on a freezing microtome, and another be manipulated, a 
ing contrast between the two becomes demonstrable. 


118 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


Heating to 50° C. also causes a permanent loss of contractility. 
The carotid behaves to moderate changes of temperature like 
other unstriped muscle—retractor penis of dog, iris and bladder 


—h 


Fic. 8.—-Carotid (ox), contracted. Transverse strip; 8 hours p.m. 
Second loading after 1? hours. 


strips of cat, strips of lower portion of gullet—contracting on 
cooling and relaxing on warming. The relaxing effect of warmth 
is taken advantage of by the surgeon in passing catheters, &c. 

The aorta and pulmonary artery con- 
tract much less than the carotid, for in 
them there is less muscle and more elastic 
tissue. 

In the case of a relaxed artery 
MacWilliam —to whom we owe these 
observations—finds the greatest amount 
of extension is produced by the first 


Fic. 9.—Elongation of con- +s . . +s 
tracted artery with rise of #ddition of weight. Successive additions 


internal pressure, 0-300 mm. cause diminishing increments in length 


Hg. Length 16 mm. Ss went : 
z ct per unit increase of weight. In the 


early part of the process of stretching a contracted artery, the 
resistance is solely muscular; later, when the muscular resistance 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 119 


has been so far overcome that the strip is stretched to what would 
be the normal length of a passive or relaxed artery, further stretch- 
ing brings into play the resistance of the elastic elements and the 
extension curve then becomes like that of a 
relaxed artery. On repeating the stretching 
the curve is like that of a relaxed artery 
throughout. As the difference between con- 
tracted and relaxed arteries depends on the 
muscular element, it is much more evident 
when transverse rather than longitudinal 
strips are used. The increments of cubic 
capacity of relaxed arteries, just as of veins, 
when subjected to equal increments of in- 
ternal pressure, is greatest at first, and suc- 
cessively diminishes as the pressure is raised. 
On repeating the distension a second time 
the relaxed artery is found to yield much 
more. 

Contracted arteries, on the other hand, 
yield relatively little at first, and go on 
yielding with each increase up to very high 
pressure, but only very gradually. The dis- 
tension of an artery whose muscular coat is 


Fic. 10.—Elongation of 
: : ; relaxed artery with rise of 
thin augments up to a certain point, and _ internal pressure. Length 


21 mm. 
then becomes less as the muscular resistance ~~" 


is wholly overcome, and the elastic elements come into play. 
Second distensions of contracted arteries cause much greater ex- 
pansions. MacWilliam says that Roy’s conclusion which has been 
generally given in the text-books—that maximum distensibility 


| 


0 4. 60 80 100 20 10 160 180 200 220 B40 260 80 300 320 340 360 380 400 420 


Fig. 11.—Carotid (ox), strongly contracted (48 hours p.m.). Increase in 
capacity on rise of pressure. 


of arteries corresponds to normal blood pressure—was based on 
experiments with arteries more or less in a post-mortem state 
of contraction. Fully contracted muscular arteries and relaxed 


120 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


arteries both give results wholly different to those of Roy. 
MacWilliam finds that there is a great tendency to elongation, 
in the case of relaxed arteries, when submitted to even low or 
— moderate internal pressures. 
Prolonged and frequently 
recurring periods of relaxa- 
tion tend, he says, to pro- 
duce tortuous arteries ; 
0 8 4 GO 80 100 120 140 160 180 20 20 #0 hence the tortuosity of the 
Fic. 12.-—Carotid (ox), weak contraction anastomotic arteries which 

(5 days p.m.). Increase of capacity. dilate after the ligation of 


a main trunk, and of the arteries of the uterus and mamme | 
{ 


which develop so in size during pregnancy, and the extensive 
pulsation of arteries in inflamed parts. The pulsatile expansion 
of a contracted artery is very small at any pressure, and there 
is no evident difference in its amount at different pressures. 
Hence the assertion of the surgeon 
is explained, that there is no evi- 
dence of the transverse expansion 

of an artery when exposed and aie a ee oe 
measured with callipers (Lister). 9 ” # % & 100 20 Ho 10 10 200 
On the other hand, the pulsatile FI, 2%, =, Caotld (or). relaxed, 
expansion of a relaxed artery for several days. Length (between 
font given pulse at 0: Mimi. He, ligatures) 14 mm. ‘Increase of capacity. 
50 mm. Hg, and 100 mm. Hg is in the proportion3:2:1. The 
measurements taken by Roy and others, which show the 
extensibility of arteries to be increased in pathological states, 
such as marasmus and phosphorus poisoning, in the light of 
MacWilliam’s work, must be interpreted by showing that the 


— 


Oo 8 40 6 8 0 120 40 160 180 200 220 240 260 280 300 320 H40 360 360.400 420 
Fie. 14.—Second elevation of pressure in same portion of artery as in Fig. 12. 


muscular rather than the elastic elements are at fault, and — 
. that the power of post-mortem contraction is lacking. The 
extreme degree of contraction produced by exposure and manipu- 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 121 


lation is of the greatest importance in stopping hemorrhage from 
wounded arteries; the abolition of contractility produced by 
freezing explains the extra hemorrhage which results after freezing 
has been used as a local anesthetic for minor operations. The 
contraction of excised arteries, says MacWilliam, is excited by 
chloroform vapour and adrenalin, and abolished by sodium 
fluoride. It is a vexed question how far the extreme degrees of con- 
traction, such as may be excited post mortem, may occur in living 
arteries in continuity under the influence of drugs, or in disordered 
states of metabolism. The view has been put forward that such 
contraction does occur, and that the arteries become so rigid in 
consequence of it, that serious errors arise in the reading of blood 
pressure by the accepted methods. It has been asserted that in 
cases where high readings of blood pressure have been recorded 
much of the pressure has gone in compressing the stiff walled 
artery. 


THE ARTERIAL PRESSURE IN MAN 


There are two methods at our choice for measuring the arterial 
pressure in man. The one, the method of obtaining the maximal 
oscillation and reading the pressure at which this occurs. ‘The 
other, the method of finding the pressure at which obliteration of 
the artery occur and the pulse ceases to be felt. The instrument 
4 hitherto! generally used is the armlet, independently invented 
by Riva Rocci and by Hill and Barnard, which consists of a 
rubber bag encased in soft leather. The rubber bag is connected 
by tubing and T piece to a syringe bulb and a manometer; the 
latter may be either a Hg manometer or some form of spring gauge. 
{ Several forms of sphygmometers have been invented by 
v. Basch and others for application to the radial artery. Small 
rubber bags are used and connected with metal spring gauges. 
The graduation of such spring gauges alters with time, and the 
writer finds small bags cannot be applied so that the pressure 
always wholly reaches the artery. Some of it may go to distending 
___ the elastic wall of the bag or to distorting the surrounding tissues, 

and thus large and unavoidable errors arise with the use of these 

instruments. The rubber bag must be large enough to transmit 
pressure equally to all parts of the tissues in which the artery lies ; 

then the tissues—protoplasm contains 80 per cent. of water— 
' transmit the pressure 7d to the artery. The bag must be 


ae 
a 
‘ * 


122 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


flaccid, and so closely confined by a rigid covering that the pressure 
cannot be spent in distending its wall rather than in compressing the tis- 
sues. The armlet satisfies all the requirements if it be large enough 
(20 cm. broad), and be bound round so as to closely fit the arm. 
The writer has introduced a sphygmometer which gives the 
same readings as the armlet, and can be carried conveniently in 
the pocket. It consists of a flaccid rubber ball enclosed in a 
silk cover, and a gauge which can be carried in a case like that 
of a clinical thermometer. The gauge is formed of a glass tube 
closed at one end and having a hole in the side near the open 
end. If the open end be placed in water, the water meniscus 
rises as far as the hole in the side.1 The rubber tube of the 
bag is then slipped over the gauge until this hole is covered. 
On now pressing the bag the meniscus rises up the gauge and 


compresses the air before it. The air acts as a spring, and the - 


stem of the gauge is calibrated in mm. Hg by testing it against 
a Hg manometer. To use the instrument properly the bag must 
be only partially full of air, and must be entirely covered with 
the fingers and palm of the hand, and then pressed upon the 
radial artery, the thumb of the hand exerting a counter pressure 
against the back of the wrist.2, The arm must be held at the 
same level as the patient’s heart apex to eliminate the influence 
of gravity, and the radial artery must be felt with the fingers of 
the other hand, the second finger being employed to prevent the 
pulse regurgitating from the ulnar, while the first finger deter- 
mines when the radial pulse disappears. The operator, when 
setting the index, holds the gauge by the solid glass end, so that his 
fingers may not heat the air within. The gauge when taken out of 
the pocket must be cooled to room temperature before it is used. 

The instrument may be used, but not very easily, to obtain 
the maximal pulsation. To effect this as little air as possible 
must be used in the bag, and the bag must be closely confined to 
the hand. There is some debate as to when maximal pulsation 
occurs. According to Roy and Adami, and to Howell and Brush, it 
occurs when the pressure just exceeds the minimal or diastolic 
pressure as recorded by a Hiirthle manometer connected directly 
with the artery. Hill and Barnard, however, found when the armlet 


1 If water gets into the side hole it must be blown out, or else the meniscus will 
not rise. 
* The fingers grip the junction of tube and gauge and support the latter. 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 123 


was placed round the neck of a dog, the maximal pulsation corre- 
sponded to about the mean pressure as indicated by a mercurial 
manometer connected with the femoral artery, and C. J. Martin 
supported this finding. If the pressure just exceeds diastolic 
pressure the artery should fill in systole and be emptied in diastole. 
It takes time to do this, and when the pulse is frequent and the 
pressure oscillations are large, not only is a very rapidly acting 
instrument required to follow accurately the pulses, but there may 
not be time for the full swing of the 
artery to be carried out. Thus in 
aortic regurgitation the maximal pul- 
sation extends over a wide area of 
pressure. It is owing to this that 
divergence of opinion arises as to 
what the maximal pulsation indicates. 
The obliteration pressure, on the other 
hand, unquestionably indicates the 
maximal or systolic pressure, and the 
only error which can arise is that due 
to rigidity of arterial wall, if the 
instrument be properly used. v. Basch 
and C. J. Martin found that a 
sclerosed radial artery is collapsed 
by a few millimetres of mercury. 
The writer found the carotid of a 
child collapsed by 2 mm. Hg. 

Herringham and Womack, inves- 
tigating a number of arteries taken 
from the post-mortem room, found 4 
to 18 mm. and in two cases 30 to 
34 mm. The pressure required bore 
no relation to the age of the men 
from whom the arteries were taken ; 
the varying amounts were probably due to post-mortem contrac- 
tion, a conclusion which is confirmed by the fact that the two 
brachials in one case differed as much as 10 mm. Hg. 

W. Russell and G. Oliver disbelieve the readings of the armlet 
method in the case of contracted and sclerosed arteries. Russell 
says he has felt arteries in life almost as brittle as glass, in other 
cases rigidly contracted. He thinks it impossible that the high 


4.JU.HIGKS SOLE MAKER LONDON. PATENT. 


Fic. 15.—The 
Leonard Hill 
sphygmometer. 


MF Ba B4 BG BG ba 24 BS BA De 


SE SS 


124 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


blood pressure, e.g. 250 mm. Hg, obtained, should be endured by 
the circulatory mechanism. As to the question of brittle arteries, 
in order that the obliteration method should fail, the artery must 
be rigid in the whole of its course enclosed by the sphygmometer. 
Rigid atheromatous patches with soft parts between would not 
disturb the readings. On inquiry among clinicians and patho- 
logists the writer has not obtained from them evidence of the 
existence during life of such rigid contracted arteries as W. Russell 
describes. The post-mortem carotid of the ox, contracted maximally 
by mechanical irritation, feels as if it would require some pressure 
to obliterate it. What pressure is required the writer has not suc- 
ceeded in ascertaining owing to the difficulty of putting the whole 
length of the artery which is enclosed in the gauge in a state of 
such contraction. G. Oliver finds in states of sclerosis considerable 


differences between the armlet readings and those obtained with his . 


hemodynamometer,—a metal spring gauge used with a small fluid 
pad on the radial artery,—and believes that the maximal pulsation 


index thus obtained is the safer guide because it necessitates, not 


the obliteration of the artery, but only the balancing of the pressure 
wave. Now the maximal pulsation method shows two maxima, 
one corresponding it is said to the diastolic pressure and the 
other to the pressure which obliterates the artery, i.e. the systolic 
pressure. The second maximum is produced by the pulse wave 
striking against the upper edge of the pad or bag, and diminishes 
when, as the pressure is lowered, the systolic wave just slips through 
the artery (Erlanger). 

Oliver prefers this index to that of pulse obliteration. He 
gives readings taken from the arm and forearm, on the two arms 
of the aged, by the obliteration method, and adduces the variation 
of these as evidence that the method of obliteration is liable to 
error. Thus— 

Woman Recumbent, 90 Years old. 


exes ap repeat had Armlet Obliteration Method. 
; ., 1108 130 8 140 S 
Right side, 95 D Forearm, 35D Arn, - oD. 
: 1108 1458 180 S. 
Left side, 05 D = ED | a 


Oe 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 125 


Woman Recumbent, 93 Years old. 


—- Sete Ieee pee 
gy Sem agree Armlet Obliteration Method. 
’ so 
soos, 1868 | 175 8 a 1908 
Right side, iD | Forearm, 125D Arm, 130D 
oc NSS 155 S 180 S 
Left side, ib | a TED » TAD 


The writer, however, investigating the accuracy of small un- 
enclosed sphygmometer bags, has come to the conclusion that they 
introduce grave errors. The pressure may not be transmitted by 
them to the artery, but go to displacing surrounding tissues, or to 
stretching the rubber bag. The bag must be big, must be flaccid, 
and must be entirely enclosed by a rigid casing, e.g. the hand, or 
leather cuff of the armlet. Oliver’s hemodynamometer appears 
to him not free from the errors which pertain to small unenclosed 
bags. 

In seventeen out of the eighteen ‘ diastolic” readings taken 
with the armlet cited by Oliver, the readings are almost the same 
(within 5 mm. Hg) on forearm and arm, or right or left arm.’ On 
the other hand they exceed by 15 mm. Hg, in all except one case, 
those taken by the hemodynamometer. This suggests the inaccuracy 
of the latter instrument, for the maximal pulsation index of the 
armlet method has been tested against the blood pressure of dogs. 
The writer has found the systolic pressure of old people may vary 
with the different strokes of the heart, and in the young with 
emotional excitement, and as the obliteration method gives us 
the maximal stroke in any period, it is necessary to test the armlet 
method by having two instruments at once on the opposite arms, 
with two observers reading at the same time. Doing this Martin 
Flack and the writer have found the readings taken by the ob- 
literation method to be the same, not only in normal people but 
in several cases of high pressure (180 to 230 mm. Hg) and of 
thickened arteries. They have also taken the pressure with one 
arm up and the other down, and found that the difference corre- 
sponds to the pressure of the column of blood separating the top 
of one armlet from the top of the other—top meaning the part 
next the shoulder. If the rigidity of the arterial wall came 


126 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


seriously into play we should not expect this correspondence, for 
it is unlikely that the arteries on the two sides would be equally 
degenerated and rigid. It is also unlikely that they would be 
equally contracted, because an artery exposed to lessened blood 
pressure dilates, while to increased blood pressure it contracts. 
We should expect, therefore, the artery in the upraised arm to 
be less contracted than the other. In one case of aneurism they 
were quite unable to make these tests owing to the greatly varying 
inequality of the heart strokes. Tested on the thigh of dogs both 
the armlet and the writer’s pocket sphygmometer gives the same 
readings as the opposite femoral artery taken directly with cannula 
and manometer. 

The conclusion the writer has come to, then, is that the method 
is exact, when carried out with either the armlet or the large 


enclosed bag of his pocket sphygmometer, and that these are the . 


simplest and best methods of testing the systolic arterial pressure 
in man. The first act of obliteration must relax the artery and 
make subsequent readings exact. 

It has been suggested that during obliteration of such a large 
artery as the brachial, the general arterial pressure may rise, but 
no error arises thus, if two or three consecutive readings be taken. 
The readings should be taken when the excitement and novelty of 
the operation has passed off, as emotional excitement raises the 
pressure considerably. Thus the writer’s pressure in the morning 
is 110 to 115 in the holidays, and 140 when teaching and working 
in the laboratory. He has observed the pressure to rise 10 mm. Hg 
on addressing a question to the subject. There is no advantage 
gained by reading the pressures nearer than within 5 mm. Hg, or 
in the use of complicated instruments for recording the maximal 
oscillations, such as that of Erlanger. 

The average systolic\ pressure of the resting man according to 
a number of observations taken by H. J. Starling is— 


15 to 40 years ; ; ‘ ; 119 
41 to 60 ,, 3 - - 142 
6land over . 2 ; : 155 


but some robust old men, he says, have pressures no » higher than 
those of the young. 

The writer has found the pressure to be as low as 80 mm. Hg 
in children ; to be 80 to 110 in young adults ; and to be no higher 


—_— 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 127 


in several active men, eminent in their walk of life and carrying some 
60 years. Oliver gives armlet systolic readings of 135 in a man of 100 
years, and 185 to 190 in a woman of 96 years, and says: “‘ In women 
the pressures are generally 5 to 10 per cent. less than in men.” 
M‘Cay of Calcutta says the pressure there varied between 
83 and 118, and the average of a large number of observations 
was slightly over 100 mm. Hg (sitting position, arm level with 
heart). The low pressure there he attributes to the hot climate 
relaxing the cutaneous vessels. In the standing posture the 
arterial pressure is no lower, and may be higher, than in the hori- 
zontal posture, and this without more than 5 to 10 extra pulse 
beats a minute. In exhausted states the frequency of the heart 
may be 30 or 40 more in the standing posture, and yet the pressure 
be lower than in the horizontal position. The change of pulse 
frequency with posture is an excellent test of the vaso-motor tone. 
The diastolic pressure in the small arteries, such as the 
phalangeal, has been measured by G. Oliver by applying a small 
bag (2°5 em.x9 cm.) round the third phalanx of the middle or 
ring finger or the second phalanx of the thumb, and raising the 
pressure till the maximal pulsation is obtained and felt by the 
patient. To obtain the systolic pressure the finger is rendered 
bloodless by squeezing a stout rubber ring over it as far as the 
lower edge of the bag ; the pressure in the bag is then raised to over 
100 mm. Hg, the ring removed, and the pressure lowered till the 
bloodless finger suddenly flushes (Girtner). The lower range of 


readings so obtained by Oliver are pie a 4 the higher og - 


On the arm, he says, there is no difference in pressure between 
the brachial and radial arteries, but the pressure in the phalangeal 
artery at the level of the first phalanx is 10 to 25 mm. less, and at 
the level of the third phalanx the pressure is a little less than half 
that in the big arteries. Thus the fall is unnoticeable in the big, 
and rapid in the small arteries. In the latter too the systolic and 
diastolic pressure approximate more and more. The ingestion of 
food, according to Oliver, raises the arterial pressure in the distal 
area; the pressure in the last phalanx rises 15 to 20 mm. Hg 
within an hour of taking food, and then slowly sinks down again. 
Exercise raises the arterial pressure. In athletes immediately after 
races the writer found systolic pressures of 140 to 220 mm. Hg. 
The pressure falls markedly with over exhaustion. Thus after four 


128 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


exhausting bouts of boxing the pressure fell from 160 at the end 
of the first bout to below 100, and rose to 130 after three minutes 
rest. Inhalation of oxygen, then, lowers the pulse-rate and raises 
the blood pressure, changes the respiration from the thoracic pressure 
to the abdominal type, and restores the vigour of the athlete.- 
The heart seems to be poisoned by unoxidised products such 
as lactic acid during extreme efforts, and therefore the com- 
pressive action of the diaphragm is held in check (L. Hill and 
M. Flack). After exercise the brachial pressure falls, while the 
phalangeal pressure remains higher owing to vaso-dilatation and 
for some time particularly if the subject is heated by exercise 


(Oliver). 


THE VENOUS PRESSURE 


Oliver presses the pad of his hemodynamometer onaselected vein _ 


of the back of the hand placed at heart’s apex level; next empties 
the blood by stroking it on past the next valve; then relaxing the 
pressure, notes the point when the vein just refills. The writer 
and M. Flack have tested the method by applying the armlet to the 
upper arm, and raising the pressure within it to say 60 mm. Hg. 
The pressure in the veins below the armlet must then rise to 
60 mm. Hg. Employing small bags in accordance with Oliver’s 
method to test the pressure in the superficial veins, they have found 
that they cannot be used accurately. A second armlet, however, 
can be so used. This is placed round the forearm, and the pressure 
raised above that in the veins, say to 70. The pressure in this 
is then relaxed till some selected vein above this armlet, which 
has been stroked empty up to the next valve, just refills; thus an 
accurate reading of the venous pressure is obtained. Employing 
the two armlets, they found the venous refilling pressure exactly 
corresponded to that in the upper armlet which was obstructing 
the venous return. A convincing proof of the general accuracy 
of the armlet method can be gained by this method thus—find 
the obliteration pressure for the brachial artery—say it is 150— 
lower the pressure to 145 so that blood can get through, and then 
find whether there is the same pressure, viz. 145, in the superficial 
veins. If so, it is clear the arterial reading is correct within 5 mm. 
They have done this in several cases, and in one where the 
arterial wall felt full of stiff sclerosed patches, and have found no 
evidence that the arterial wall influences the readings. 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 129 


Another method of Oliver’s is to place the hand, with the 
fingers extended, in the upright position and at the level of the 
heart’s apex, taking care that the veins are not compressed by 
clothes or the posture of the arm. He then observes the veins 

«on the back of the hand, and raising the hand until the veins just 
collapse, measures the vertical height of the veins above the apex 
of the heart. This measurement gives him the venous pressure in 
millimetres of blood. The method demands that the veins should 
be visibly distended, which may not be the case in a cool atmos- 
phere. The venous pressure varies with warm or cold atmosphere, 


- with posture and muscular effort, pressure of the arm against the 


body, pressure of clothes, &c., on the arm, and taking of food. 
When the veins of the hand are contracted down with cold, we 
cannot tell what positive pressure there is in the veins at the heart’s 
apex level by either of the above methods. The veins contract 
down to the thread of blood which they receive when the atmos- 
phere is cold, and dilate to hold a large volume of blood when 
brought into play as a part of the heat-losing mechanism of the 
body. The best demonstration of this is the network of super- 
ficial veins which become visible in a child in a hot bath, or a 
horse heated with work. 

If the hand be held at such a level that the veins are just 
collapsed, and then deep and prolonged expiratory efforts or a 
succession of coughs be made (Oliver), they will be seen to swell, 
and this is owing to raised arterial pressure and little if at all to 
obstructed venous outlet (T. Lewis). Prolonged inspirations, on 
the other hand, cause the visibly swollen veins to vanish. The 
influence of posture is seen on holding the hand and arm still 
in the dependent position, particularly in a warm atmosphere, 
or after exercise, or food. The veins fill under the hydrostatic 
pressure, and the colour of the hand becomes bluish owing to the 
lessened velocity of blood flow. A dull feeling of pressure arises 
which is uncomfortable and causes one to move the part. Con- 
traction of the muscles of the hand empties the blood in the 


_ capillaries and veins on past the valves, and so long as the hand 


4 


is at work no distension or discomfort arises. If the hand be 

kept quite still at the level of the heart’s apex little congestion 

arises. How much bodily movements further the circulation of 

_ the blood can be seen by alternately placing the hand dependent, 

and elevating it above the head, and observing how the hand 
I 


= 


130 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


changes from the flushed to the blanched state. The vaso-con- 
strictor control of the hand (for changes of posture) seems to be 
much less than that of the foot. If in a warm atmosphere foot and 
hand be lowered together from the heart’s apex level and be kept 
still in the dependent posture, it will be seen that congestion arises 
much less quickly in foot than in hand, and the pressure in the 
veins on the back of the hand reaches the full gravity effect much 
sooner than in the veins on the dorsum of the foot. It is the same 
with the face. The parts always exposed to the atmosphere 
congest quickest. The control of course varies with the effect of 
external temperature on vaso-dilatation, as may be seen by plung- 
ing the hand and foot in iced or in hot water and then repeating 
the observation. The pumping action of the movements of walk- 
ing may be observed very well on the veins of the back of the 
foot. Let the reader stand still with the feet bare, and observe _ 
the veins of his feet becoming prominent. If he bend to feel them 
he can estimate the high pressure within. Now let him take a 
few steps and observe the veins again. They are emptied by the 
movements, squeezed between skin and muscle, and feel soft for 
some little time until they fill again on standing still. Similarly 
the contractions of the muscles which occur with slight changes 
of posture empty the veins and prevent congestion when we sit 
working at a desk. It is the deficient flow or quality, not the 
pressure, of blood which leads to cedema, degeneration, distension 
of the venous wall, and varicose veins in those whose occupation 
requires them to stand for long periods of time. Some attention 
has been drawn to a curious hereditary oedema of the legs which ~ 
commences about the seventeenth year. It occurs in members 
of certain families on standing, and is prevented by bandaging. 
The venous pressure is raised, owing to vaso-dilatation, by warmth, 
rest, sleep or food, together with the pressure in the arterioles as 
measured in the distal phalanx, while the pressure in the large 
arteries falls. The arterial and venous pressures in the arm are 
both raised during muscular effort, owing to increased ventricular 
output, splanchnic constriction, and local dilatation. 


THE CAPILLARY PRESSURE 


As much light can be thrown upon the principles which govern 
the circulation of the blood, from the comparative structure of 
j : 


° 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE __ 131 


the vascular systems, the writer cites the following suggestive 
passages from the admirable Principles of Animal Histology of 
Dahlgren and Kepner, in which he underlines certain sentences 
which bear in particular on the arguments that follow. 

“The main blood channel system itself has many differentiated 
régions. The region of thin-walled capillaries and lacune, the 
strong-walled conducting vessels, the blood-forming organs, and 
the muscular pumping stations or hearts . . . most specific of these 
portions are the capillaries and lacuna, for it is here that the real 
; work of the blood is accomplished, the exchange of material with 
; the tissues. Here the walls of the vessels are thinnest or even 

apparently wanting. In this case the connective tissue cells that 
surround the channel, while not differentiated into definite channel 
walls, act in that capacity, so that we cannot say that retaining walls 
are altogether absent. The vessels of the periphery have in all cases . 
a larger total cross section than any other total cross section in 
the circuit. This results in the surface of contact between blood 
and tissue being large enough to effect necessary exchanges of 
materials as well as making the circuit slower to give requisite 
time for such exchanges. The smaller but more numerous branches 
of the periphery unite to form large channels that serve to conduct 
the blood to other portions of the periphery, or to and from the 
central pumping stations, or to the blood glands. These vessels 
and the veins, together with the vessels carrying blood back to 
the periphery, the arteries, act as the long-distance carriers of 
the circulatory system, and their walls are usually very strongly 
constructed. 

“The pumping region comprises one or more parts of the larger 
channel or channels that have acquired the power of rhythmic 
contraction. Sometimes this region occupies a considerable 
extent of the larger vessels. At other times it is found in a more 
{ specialised form, occupying only a short section of the tube, but 
very intensely developed. Such an organ is known as a heart. 
Both of the preceding conditions may be found together, as 
they are in the squid and other cephalopod molluscs where there 
_ __ are three or five separate hearts, and in addition the larger parts 
of the arteries are also constantly engaged in driving the blood on its 
course by wave-like pulsations. Other regions of the blood-channel 
_ system are found in which the walls are differentiated and in which 
_ the blood moves but slowly and sometimes almost comes to rest. 


132 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


These form the so-called blood glands, and in them the blood is 
renovated by the removal of some of its old parts and the addition 
of other new ones or both.” 

“ The internal tissue of a Turbellarian worm is a loose aggregate 
of several kinds of weakly differentiated cells, known as parenchyme. 
These cells do not touch each other at all points, but are connected by 
strands, and in consequence there may be easily seen between them a 
great many spaces, known as the intracellular spaces, which are united 
into a large connecting system that extends throughout the body. This 
system of spaces is filled with a fluid, and this fluid carries the digested 
food materials, the oxygen supply for internal cells, the combustion 
products, and in every other way acts as a simple blood. This is the 
undifferentiated and unorganised form of blood-vessel system, and a 
sort of circulation must inevitably take place as a result of the ordinary 
movements of the animal’s body. This grade of structure is to be | 
seen in a number of the lower and simpler animal forms and some- 
times as an accessory apparatus to several grades of complete blood- 
channel systems.” In the several typical forms studied by Dahlgren 
and Kepner, the walls of the blood channels show a strong analogy 
based on the physiological (which are here mechanical) needs of 
the vessels. The blood fluid must be confined to the channels, 
and this is usually done by the single inner layer of cells, the intima. 
In some forms the intima is formed not by the cells themselves 
but by a cuticle which is the product of these cells (lobster, &c.). 
‘The intima may alone confine the blood stream, or if the pressure 
is too great, it may be reinforced by the connective tissue cells 
that immediately surround it. These cells develop their connective 
tissue as fibrils or plates or webs with which they bind and hold the 
vessel intact when the blood presses on its walls. Again, these 
primitive mesoderm cells may develop into muscle cells that sur- 
round the channel and by their contractile strength cause it to 
pulsate and drive the blood on its course. The arrangement of 
these three classes of tissues to form the wall of the vessel falls, 
naturally, into layers, the so-called coats of the blood vessels. 
Each kind of coat usually has a particular position with reference to 
the lumen. This position, however, is sometimes changed in the several 
groups for no apparent reason, All these cells and the tissues that 
they form were probably not cells that were bound in the course 
of their development to become so specialised, but as far as can 
be told, they were such of the connective tissue cells as happened 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 133 


to be in the course of the developing blood channel as it pushed 
its way among them, and were developed in response to the needs of 
the vessel.” . 

The first capillaries in the area vascular of the developing 
; chick have their origin in the secretory activity of the cells which 
first form vacuoles and finally networks of capillary spaces. It is 
the functional activity of the cells which determines the rate of 
flow and pressure within, and finally the structure of the vessels 
and of the power of the heart. The increase in size of the lumen 
of a vessel, says Thoma, depends upon the rate of blood flow. 
Hence the dilatation of collateral pathways and establishment of 
efficient anastomoses which follows the ligation of a main artery. 
The growth in thickness of the vessel wall is dependent upon the 
diameter of the lumen of the vessels and the blood pressure. The 
tension in the circular direction of a tube is equal to the product of 
the pressure by the radius. The longitudinal tension is equal to 
half this product. The wider channels first formed in the area 
vasculosa thus develop into arteries ; collateral capillaries develop 
into arteries when a main artery is tied; and the thyroid vein 
develops the structure of an artery when the central end of the 
carotid is joined end to end on to it. 

Thoma states that at all points where the transverse sectional 
areas of the lumina of the arterial trunks has been investigated 
the sum of the sectional areas of the main trunks is equal to that 
of their branches. Thus the sectional area of the ascending aorta 
equals the sum of that of the two carotids, and subclavians and 
the descending aorta, together with such smaller-branches as were 
given off above the place of measurement. Similarly the sectional 
area of the abdominal aorta taken 2 cm. above its bifurcation 
is equal to that of the two common iliacs and the smaller 
branches. The same law holds good, Thoma says, in the case of 
the arterioles of the tongue or web of the frog. As, he says, the 
transverse section of the main artery is equal to the transverse 
section of all its branches, the average rate of flow is the same 
in all arteries, and the quantity flowing through the transverse 
section of any artery in a given time is proportional to the section 
of the artery. This law only holds good so long as disturbing 
influences due to vaso-dilatation and functional activity are in 
abeyance. 

That the ramification of the small arteries is homonomous 


134 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


is proved, says Thoma, by the microscopic observation of the axial 
stream. The stream of corpuscles moves so fast that it appears 
either completely homogeneous or faintly striated, both in the 
artery and its branches (excluding the capillaries), and thus must 
be flowing at the same rate, for the slightest diminution alters the 
homogeneity. When the tongue of a frog is drawn forward from 
the mouth of a curarised frog and pinned out for examination, 
there follows at first considerable deviation from homonomous 
ramification, owing to injury and local dilatation. Half-an-hour 
later the tongue is hyperemic, but the flow 1 is again of the same 
rate in all the arterial ramification. 

The generalisation follows on these observations of Thoma 
that the total cross sectional area of the arterial ramification 
measured at any place is the same,! and that the sudden increase 


occurs in the ramification of the capillaries, so that the rate of 


flow is lessened therein from 500 mm. to 3} mm. or less a second. 
This important generalisation, so opposed to accepted teaching, 
requires confirmation or refutation. 

The capillary system in the tissues of man is like the blood 
channel system of the Turbellarian worm, a vast system of tissue 
_ spaces lined by cells which confine the corpuscles, and by shrinking 
or swelling and by varying osmotic state regulate in part the 
outflow and inflow of lymph. These cells are capable of phagocytic 
action and of dividing and producing the primary type of leucocyte. 
The arteries deliver the blood to these spaces. and many or few of 
them are filled at any moment and in any tissue according as the 
arteries dilate and increase the supply, or the venous flow is im- 
peded by gravity or external pressure. It is absurd, therefore, to 
calculate the size of the capillary bed from the relative velocity in 
the aorta and in the capillaries of a transparent membrane. 

The cells both of the tissues and the capillaries swell or shrink 
under:the influence of the solutions which bathe them, and thus 
alter both the volume of the organ and the capacity of the vascular 
system which is included in it. The tissue cells adapt themselves 
slowly to altered osmotic conditions of the tissue fluids, and do 
not by any means come into equilibrium with them. With the 
return to normal physiological relations the recovery of the normal 
concentrations of the cellular contents is rapid and complete. 
The influence on the capillary circulation of osmotic and surface 

1 Supposing the state of constriction to be the same. . 


— - 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 


energy can be no less than that effected by the heart and vaso- 
motor system, and is, probably, as important in controlling the 
flow of blood. In the active conditions of life the contents of 
the capillaries are continually being emptied onwards by the 
contractions of the skeletal muscles, pressure against external 
bodies, and the influence of gravity in changes of posture. The 
capillary system is as a sponge squeezed and filled continually by 
the active motions of the body. The attempts which have been 
made to estimate capillary pressure have all been made on trans- 
parent membranes in the motionless animal fixed in the horizontal 
position, or on man’s skin with the part fixed, immobile during 
the observation. In the animal microscopic observations have 
been made by Roy and Brown on the frog’s mesentery, the 
capillaries being compressed by a sheet of transparent peritoneal 
De membrane which formed the base of a glass capsule, in which the 
f fluid pressure could be made greater or less. A pressure of 100 
7 to 250 mm. H,0 sufficed to stop the circulation in the capillaries 
under these conditions, while a pressure of 200 to 350 mm. H,O 
__ expelled the blood from the arterioles. When the heart was in- 
hibited the pressure sank to 0, rising again to 70 to 100 mm. H,O 
as the veins filled. The anemia so produced was followed by 
| hyperemia and increased pressure. 
+ Measurements of capillary pressure have been made on man 
by v. Kries, v. Recklinghausen, and others, the method being the 
finding of that pressure which just blanches the skin. V. Kries 
weighted a glass plate 4 sq. mm. big placed behind the finger nail. 
He found 0°25 grm. was the smallest difference which produced a 
visible effect. Now as 1 grm. equals the weight of 1 ¢.c. of H,O, 


135 


1 and Taam." 1 250 mm., the error of observation would be 
ian =62°5 mm. H,O, a very large one considering the smallness 


of the pressure measured. H. W. Recklinghausen places a small 
flaccid rubber bag on the skin, through the centre of which he has 
punched: a hole. He moistens skin and bag with glycerine, and — 
covers the hole with a glass slide, holding it so as to make an*air- 
_ tight junction between bag skin and glass, and then through a 
side tube blows air into the bag until the skin blanches. A mano- 
meter connected with the side tube gives the pressure. These 
methods necessitating the fication and immobility of the part, cannot 
be applied rapidly, and therefore do not give the capillary pressure 


136 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


under ordinary conditions. Moreover the pressure so applied is 
spent largely in deforming the horny convex plate of the epidermis, 
and in the case of v. Kries’ method the size of the horny layer 
which is depressed is much larger than the square glass plate. 
This can easily be seen by taking a set of broom bristles and fasten- 
ing them to matches in lengths of about an inch, and then finding 
the bristle which will just blanch the skin when pressed on it till 
it bends. It can be seen that the bristle depresses a horny plate 
selected, say, on the back of the hand, much larger than its own 
sectional area. If some of the horny layer be carefully removed 
with a razor (without producing hyperemia) a weaker bristle will 
effect the blanching. Observations of this kind show that these 
methods are too inaccurate to use as a measure of capillary pres- 
sure ; nor can they be used to give exact comparative differences of 


pressure under varying conditions, for the error cannot be taken ° 


the same all through. V. Kries found that the blanching pressure 
at the root of the nail did not give the full gravity effect on change 
of posture. Here the time of fixation in any posture is an im- 
portant and undetermined factor. 


Difference of Level of Finger 


from Top of Head in mm. Pressure in mm. H,0. 


0 328 
205 397 
490 513 
840 738 


Lewy has calculated the pressure required to overcome the re- 
sistance in the capillaries from the known facts concerning pressure, 
velocity, and the viscosity of the blood. The average length of the 
capillaries is 0'4 to 0°7 mm., their radius 0°0045 mm., and velocity 
0°5 to 0'9 mm. per second. There flows per second through a capillary 
a quantity varying from 7 x 0°5 x 0:0045? up to = x 0°9 x 0°0045? c.c. 
From the Poisemille formula—determined for flow in capillary glass 


tubes—Q = K x Ls where Q=the quantity per second, / the pressure 


difference between beginning and end of tube, d the diameter and / the 
_ length of the tube, and K a constant representing the coefficient of 
viscosity, from this formula Lewy calculates h to be equal to 10 to 
150 mm. of blood. The mean arterial pressure being taken as 115 mm. Hg 
or 1500 mm. of blood the fall of pressure in the capillaries is 45 of this 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 137 


at the least or ,'; at the greatest estimate. So wide a difference makes 
the calculation of little value except as an indication of the fact that 
there is little resistance in the capillaries. In making such estimates 
the velocity of flow in the capillaries is observed in their membranes 
with the animal motionless and in the horizontal posture. We have, 
moreover, no reason to think that the same laws hold good for glass 
tubes and the living capillaries. The cross section of a capillary 10p 
in diameter is some yz}y5 8q- mm., the outflow per second will be 
rztoo x $ if the velocity is 0°5 mm. per second, or 1 c.mm. in about 
six hours, or 1 c.c. in 250 days (Stewart). 


The writer has measured the capillary-venous pressure in the 
brain by finding the pressure which just overcomes the pressure 
_with which the brain bulges into a trephine hole. The “ brain 
pressure ” is due to the blood pressure in the capillaries and veins 
which is left over after the resistance in the arteries has been over- 
come, and distends the brain substance. When the circulation 
ceases the brain collapses away from the trephine hole and no 
longer exerts any pressure. The intra-cranial pressure cannot be 
greater than that in the pial veins, for otherwise these would be 
obliterated, and the pressure within them must then rise to the 
intra-cranial pressure. Observation can be made by screwing a 
glass tube into the trephine hole, the tube being filled with water 
and closed by a flaccid thin rubber membrane at the brain end, 
and connected to pressure bottle and manometer at the other end. 
An air bubble is introduced into the glass tube to mark the zero 
7 pressure. When the tube is put in place, the air bubble is pushed 
? outwards, and on raising the pressure bottle and pushing it back 
# to zero the “brain pressure” is balanced and the manometer 
indicates its amount. 

In the horizontal anwsthetised animal the “ brain pressure ” 
is about 100 mm. H,0, sinking even to 0 in the vertical head up 
position, and rising considerably in the vertical head down posture. 
The cay‘'lary pressure in the brain varies proportionately with the 
rise and fall ot vena cava pressure—time being granted for the 
_ capillaries to follow the alteration of venous pressure. The brain 
pressure does not follow in exact proportion alterations of aortic 
pressure, because between the capillaries and the aorta is placed 
the varying resistance of the arterioles. The cerebral venous 
_ pressure can be measured by inserting a tube into the torcular 

-Herophili, and the pressure of the cerebro-spinal fluid (the lymph 


‘. 


138 ‘THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


of the brain) by inserting a tube through the lamina of the axis. 
The brain pressure, the cerebral venous pressure, and the cerebro- 
spinal fluid pressure are one and the same. 

The writer has also measured the capillary-venous pressure in 
the eyeball by the same method, inserting a hollow needle into 
the aqueous. The globe formed by the corneo-sclera is analogous 
to the rigid case formed by the skull because its internal capacity 


is strictly limited, and cannot be increased. The intra-ocular 


pressure, like the intra-cranial, is purely circulatory in origin and 
represents the capillary-venous pressure within the eye. The venous 
sinus of the canal of Schlemm, according to Thomson Henderson, 
is formed of tributaries from the whole anterior portion of the 
uveal tract, and it thus offers the lowest pressure with which the 
aqueous humour comes into direct contact, just as the pial veins, 


where they pass into the sinuses, offer the lowest pressure to the © 


cerebro-spinal fluid. The intra-ocular fluid, just like the intra- 
cranial, transmits pressure equally in all directions, and therefore 


when its exit is diminished the tension rises, and the whole of - 


the elastic intra- vascular system tends to be converted into a 
rigid one. — 

Koster has proved experimentally the unyielding nature and 
rigidity of the globe under conditions of increased intra-ocular 
tension. If the internal pressure were raised from 19 to 70 mm. Hg, 
the increase in volume of the globe was only y955 of the original 
volume. 

In the globe of the eye the capillary-venous pressure is the intra- 


ocular pressure. The intra-ocular pressure in fact equals that in the © 


venous sinus of Schlemm’s canal, just as in the skull the pressure 
of the cerebro-spinal fluid is equal to that in the pial veins. 
Supposing the intra-ocular tension, owing to the increased 


secretion of the ciliary process, rise above the pressure within 


Schlemm’s canal, then the pressure of the aqueous will obliterate 
the wall of the sinus until the venous pressure rises within it to 


that in the aqueous. Similarly, supposing by increased secretion — 


of the choroidal fringes in the brain the pressure of the cerebro- 
spinal fluid rise above that in the pial veins, then these would be 


obliterated until the pressure of the blood within them rise to that 


of the cerebro-spinal fluid. Actual measurements show that the 
pressure of the cerebro-spinal fluid and of the cerebral venous 
pressure are the same. 


- tiedtItz=p.=z=z Ve 


—e ee = -. — 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE . 139 


If the arteries dilate in the brain or eyeball, room must be 
‘made by increased outflow of cerebro-spinal fluid—or aqueous—or 
by narrowing of the veins, or by both means. Probably each 
pulsatile expansion of the arteries helps to drive a little of the 
cerebro-spinal fluid or aqueous into the veins, and thus the effective 
- circulation of these tissue lymphs is maintained. Each pulsatile 
expansion of the arteries also causes a pulsatile expulsion of venous 
blood into the venous sinuses and thus helps the circulation. A 
rise of arterial pressure accompanied by expansion of the arteries 
of the brain or eye, cannot compress the veins so as to produce 
venous congestion or stasis, for the pressure transmitted through 
the walls of the arteries must always be less than the pressure 
transmitted directly through the blood within the vessels. The 
veins are narrowed to compensate for the expansion of the arteries 
- until the whole system becomes more like a rigid system, and the 
-___ velocity of flow is increased, less of the head of pressure being spent 
in overcoming the resistance in the arterioles, more going to the 
kinetic energy of flow. Experimental observations show that the 
- outflow is always increased by a rise of arterial pressure. 
In pathological states the intra-ocular or intra-cranial pressures 
are often increased. Such increase is entirely circulatory in origin. 
In inflammatory states the toxins produced by bacteria on the 
one hand dilate the blood-vessels, and on the other hand alter the 
metabolism of the tissues, and increasing those cell products which 
are crystalloidal in character, thus raise the osmotic pressure. The 
tissues swell, and the blood pressure rises, keeping pace with the 
* pressures of the tissue lymph. The swelling must be at the ex- 
pense of the blood supply of other surrounding parts, where there 
is no toxin to excite and no vaso-dilatation or edema. The 
anemia so produced may in its turn damage the tissue metabolism 
of these parts, and set up congestion and cedema therein, and thus 
a vicious circle may be established. Relief is obtained by opening 
or softening the confining capsule, and so allowing free flow of 
blood and free exudation of lymph through the inflamed part. 
The lymph brings with it neutralisers of the toxins, the phagocytes 
and their opsonins, the chemical repairers of disordered cell meta- 
bolism. If the products of tissue damage are successfully removed 
_ by the increased flow in the surrounding dilated vessels the whole 
mischief subsides, and the killed tissues are removed and replaced 
_ by growth of scar tissue. During inflammation of such an organ as 


140 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


pneumococcus (Carnegie Dickson). 


The author has borrowed these figures to show 


y capillaries. 


tance from tissue into blood-vessels may take place, 


congested vein and its tributar, 


bs 


great] 


how.large a transference of su 


cage? 
e" ¢ 2 xy 


Fic. 16.—Transverse section of marrow of femur of normal rabbit and of one infected with 


Note in the latter the 


—————— 
: 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 141 


~ the brain or bone marrow the amount of blood can be enormously 


increased by the transference of tissue fluid and tissue substance 
from the tissue cells into the blood channels. 

The way in which increased tension may arise can be illustrated 
by a consideration of the cause of glaucoma of the eye. 

In glaucoma the cribriform ligament is sclerosed, and by wall- 
ing in Schlemm’s canal prevents the free circulation of the aqueous 
humour which is necessary for the proper metabolism of the eye. 
When the cribriform ligament is sclerosed the aqueous has to get 
away mostly by the veins of the iris through the iris crypts, and 
thus it comes about that atropine induces an attack of glaucoma 
by dilating the pupil and closing the orifices of the crypts of the 


iris. Ividectomy, on the other hand, relieves glaucoma by opening 


up new channels for escape of the aqueous, as the surface of 
the iris never scars up, but remains open and unaltered after it 
has been cut (Thomson Henderson). 

The cedema of the tissues of the eye which occurs in glaucoma 
may be ascribed to the deficient circulation of aqueous, leading to 


altered metabolism and increase of osmotic pressure. The conges- 


tion of the blood vessels must be also set down to the irritant 
effect of the products of disordered metabolism. A rise of 
general vascular pressure may, it is said, precipitate an acute 
attack of glaucoma; probably by increasing the secretion of 
aqueous; this finds room at the expense of the veins which 
are narrowed, ¢.e. the absorbirig surface ; the excess of aqueous 
cannot escape again when the general pressure falls, and thus the 
tension of the eyeball remains heightened, the circulation in the 
eye lessened and the mischief increased. 

The conditions which hold good for the eye and brain also 
apply in part to the limbs enclosed by the skin, and to the other 
organs, such as the kidney, which are enclosed in capsules. The 
writer and M. Flack estimated the capillary pressure in man by 
pushing a hollow needle into the subcutaneous tissue of the arm 


_ or leg, and connecting this with a glass tube containing an air 


bubble, and the tube with an Hg manometer and a pressure bottle. 
The air bubble indicates when the pressure is sufficient to over- 
come the capillary pressure and drive the water in, and as the 
subject feels the smart of the water, he can give a confirmatory 
signal, The observations made in this way showed that a pressure 


of 10 to 20 mm. H,O was sufficient to overcome the capillary 


“ 


142 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


pressure in the arm placed at the heart’s apex level. In the leg 
at a level 380 mm. below the arm 90 mm. H,0 sufficed, giving 
a difference of 70 mm. H,O against a gravity difference of 
380 mm. H,O. On supplying the armlet and raising the pressure 
in it to 910 mm. H,0O, the capillary pressure in the forearm rose to 
45 mm. H,O, at a time when the venous pressure had risen to 
that in the armlet. On another occasion it was 57 mm. H,0O, 
and in the armlet and veins 910 mm. H,0. 

These observations only prove what any one can feel for him- - 
self by holding a limb motionless and dependent for some time 
and then estimating with his finger the pressure in the veins and 
the capillary areas of the skin. Every movement that occurs under 
the varying conditions of active life squeezes on the capillary and 
venous blood. The tissues and pressure sense organs are thus 
protected from any great increase of vascular pressure. After an - 
area of capillaries has been squeezed empty, the blood pressure 
for a.time must be nil within it. The blanching effect of clenching 
the fist demonstrates this. Thus in spite of the hydrostatic pressure 
due to gravity, equal to 140 mm. Hg in a man six feet high standing 
vertical, in spite of this the same bristle will blanch the capillaries 
of his feet and hand, so. long as he does not keep the parts for long 
immobile, but by contractions of his muscles every now and then 
expresses the blood from the capillaries onward past the venous 
valves. The vaso-motor system by constricting the arterioles— 
the arteries constrict of themselves to increased pressure—prevents 
over rapid filling of dependent parts, so that it takes some little 
time for the dependent and warm immobile hand to become greatly 
congested, and still longer the foot, while in a cold atmosphere, 
owing to vaso-constriction, congestion is only very slowly pro- 
duced by the dependent posture. 

So that while arterial and venous pressure in a relaxed limb 
exactly follow the variations of hydrostatic pressure on change of 
posture, the capillaries do not. Likewise on obstruction of the 
veins, while the venous pressure rapidly rises to the pressure of 
the obstructing armlet, the capillary pressure rises only very 
slowly. This fact which seems at first sight contrary to physical 
laws, in that the pressure is high in the artery which feeds and in 
the vein which drains but not in the capillary area which joins 
together the two, this fact is explained if we suppose that the 
capillary bed is a sponge work emptied by every movement, and 


4 
: 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 148 


taking time to fill up again, and that there are wider channels 
connecting the arteries and veins through which the pressure is 
transmitted to the veins. The existence of such wide channels is 
recognised by histologists. Inspection of the hand and foot when 
kept still, and held first in the dependent, and then in the horizontal 
position, when held in the dependent position and before and after 
contracting the muscles of the part (e.g. clenching the hand), teach 
the enormous importance of movement in maintaining the circula- 
tion of the blood." Muscular exercise, by increasing the combustion 
of food stuffs, and by _ ————E 
furthering the circula- — _— 4 
tion of the blood, and | 
so elimination of tissue 
waste, is the greatest 
source of vigorous bodily 
and mental health. 
Standing erect and 
with a leg relaxed the Nw 
arterial and venous pres- Fic. 17. rea pressure recorded in carotid 
sures in this leg become 2nd in femoral artery, each artery in turn being 


5 placed in the axis of rotation of the animal holder. 
higher than those inthe 4, horizontal ; F, D, vertical feet-down posture. 


arm by the column of The difference between the pressures in this posture 
was equal to the column of blood separating the 


blood separating the two two arteries. Note increased action of respiratory 
points of measurement. PU) 

In students hanging head dowhwards by their feet, M. Flack and 
the writer found the arterial pressure in the arm little altered from 
the normal taken in the horizontal posture. Thus—horizontal, 
brachial 126, post-tibial artery 126; standing, 140 and 204; head 
down, 116 and 42. The pressure in the circle of Willis was about 
20 mm. less than in the brachial in the standing, and about 
10 mm. more in the head-down posture. Now the column of 
blood above the point of observation is then in the six foot man 
almost equal to the normal arterial pressure taken in the hori- 
zontal position. The heart by lessened output and the vaso-motor 
mechanism by increased dilatation can compensate for this hydro- 
static pressure, and it is clear that the heart and brain are so_pro- 
tected from a greatly increased arterial pressure. In the standing 


1 Butchers say it is easy to bleed all the tissues of an ox if it is pole-axed after 
being driven off the road into the yard, but difficult if it has lain down for some 
hours. 


144 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


posture the column of blood in the vessels of the limbs can be 
broken up into segments by muscular action, and the blood per- 
mitted to circulate by alternate and appropriate contraction and 
relaxation of the muscles. A most important and hitherto almost 
unexplored reflex mechanism is here engaged, a mechanism in 
which the skeletal muscles and the muscular wall of the arteries 
each take a part, and in which sensory “receptors” are engaged, 
tuned in sensitivity to capillary blood pressure. 

To sum up then, the function of the heart is to drive the blood 
into the capillary bed, the function of the arterioles is to regulate 
the distribution and switch the current on to one or other part 
of the capillary bed and limit the pooling of the blood under the 
stress of gravity; the function of the skeletal muscles and res- 
piratory pump, aided by the valves in the veins, is to drive the 
blood back from the capillary bed to the heart, to prevent hypo- - 
static congestion, and to act as a pump to the vascular system 
of no less importance than the heart. The respiratory pump even ~ 
may drive the blood through the right heart and lungs. 


Tue ACTION OF THE RESPIRATORY PUMP 


The writer’s argument that the circulation is largely maintained 
by bodily movement and varies continuously with every change 
of posture and muscular contraction, and is liable to an extent 
which defies analysis in the living active animal, is borne out by 
recent researches of T. Lewis on the effect of respiration on the 
blood pressure of man. 

To determine this on man the sphygmograph can be used, but it 
must not be fixed by a band encircling the wrist, because this entails 
a serious fallacy due to the swelling of the volume of the arm. A 
sphygmograph applied with a band acts like a plethysmograph. 
T. Lewis fixed the sphygmograph by a suspension method and con- 
trolled the results with a sphygmomanometer. The effect of pure 
intercostal or pure diaphragmatic breathing in a trained subject 
he found to be as follows :— 


Suspended Suspended 

Inspiration. Inspiration. Expiration. Expiration. 
Thoracic . - \ = + + ~ 
Abdominal... + - - + 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 145 


If both abdomen and chest participate in the breathing, inter- 
mediate results will be obtained,—one effect will counteract the 
other; and if abdomen or chest do not participate to the same 
relative extent during different instants of a particular act, com- 
plex curves will result which are beyond analysis. When a patient 
is asked to take a deep breath, the breathing is often disorderly 


\ f\ 1) 


J I 
J L/ ay et a ys * fe pith) Jv MI YN IN WI ied nl 


— 


Insp. 
—BPLine _ 


D cwesr 


Fic. 18.—Influence of chest and abdominal breathing on the pulse (Lewis). 


in character, starting perhaps as intercostal and finishing as 
abdominal. 

In man a deep intercostal respiration, if not prolonged, yields a 
fall of pressure, and conversely a deep diaphragmatic inspiration 
yields a rise. But as the normal respiratory curves of blood 
pressure are of very complex origin, and the different factors in- 
volved vary widely, it is not possible to state what the effeet on 
blood pressure will be, unless the conditions and nature of the 
respiratory act are known. The ordinary statement in the text- 
books that inspiration raises and expiration lowers blood pressure 
is altogether unjustified by the records. The pressure almost 

K 


blk 7 


146 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM ; 


always falls when the patient is told to take a deep breath, and 
thus what has been termed the pulsus paradoxus and considered 
of pathological import, turns out to be quite a normal event. 


In animals, says Lewis, under deep anesthesia, the abdomen 


plays no part in the production of respiratory curves. The in- 
spiratory rise is due to the lessened pressure in the pericardium 
and consequent increased filling of the heart. It is abolished by 
allowing free access of atmospheric air to the pericardial sac. The 
varying intra-pleural pressure affects intimately the filling of the 
heart, while its influence on the resistance and capacity of the 
pulmonary vascular area is a matter of assumption rather than of 
experimental proof. We are at present, says Lewis, justified by 
experiment in ascribing the inspiratory rise in animals on intercostal 
breathing to the effect on the heart only. 


Tigerstedt found that ligation of the vessels of the left lung, . 


-out of thirty-one observations, produced in eighteen no noticeable 

effect on the output per second of the heart. In eleven cases it 
produced a decrease of 6 to 10 per cent., and in two cases a decrease 
of 18 to 20 per cent. In twenty-three cases the arterial pressure 
was unaltered by this operation, in seven it was decreased 
6 to 10 per cent., and in one case over 10 per cent. Thus the 
pulmonary circuit can be shut off to a very large extent—at least 
in the animal with artificial respiration—and the remainder 
suffice to deliver an unlessened quantity of blood to the heart. 
Tigerstedt’s observations also show that the arterial pressure may 
sometimes remain constant when the output of the heart varies 
considerably ; the explanation of this is that the arterial system 
contracts down on the blood that it contains. 

In Valsalva’s experiment—a deep expiration with the mouth 
and nose shut—the abdominal pressure rises very greatly, and 
this is the chief cause of the rise of arterial pressure which then 
occurs. If a stiff-walled rubber tube is used as a rectal sound, 
and is connected to a manometer, an estimate of intra-abdominal 
pressure may be obtained, and this can be compared with the 
expiratory pressure obtained by expiring against a mercurial 
manometer. In one case the rectal pressure rose to 94 mm. Hg, 
and the tracheal pressure to 87 mm. Hg. The same conditions 
occur in coughing. 


In deep abdominal breathing the rectal pressure may rise to- 


30 mm. Hg, and frequently shows a range of 20 mm. Hg, a fact 


4 ° 


1 
a ae a 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 147 


which shows what an effect such deep breathing has both on the 
return of venous blood and on the resistance in the splanchnic area. 
The action of abdominal breathing on the return of venous blood 
may be easily demonstrated by opening the thorax of a cat just 
above the diaphragm, and cutting a hole in the vena cava inferior. 
With each descent of the diaphragm the blood spurts out. The 
muscles of the abdomen and the levator ani support the abdominal 
pressure, and the avoidance of prolapse of the viscera depends on 
the proper exercise of these muscles and on the presence of an 
adequate amount of fat in the belly. 


EFFect OF MENTAL WorkK AND EMOTION 


In curarised animals excitation of the cortex cerebri in the 
motor areas of the limbs and trunk muscles raises the arterial] 


Fic. 19.—1, Volume of abdominal organs; 2, volume of arm ; 3, respiration ; 
+, pleasant; ,-— unpleasant taste. 


‘pressure, by causing contraction of the splanchnic area, and dilates 
the blood vessels of the limbs. Dilatation of the splanchnic area 


148 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


similarly is accompanied by constriction of the vessels of the 
limbs. The cortex discharges motor impulses and alters the 
circulation in accordance with the needs of the moving parts. 
E. Weber has recorded in man the volume of the arm by means 
of a plethysmograph, and the volume of the abdominal organs by 
a rectal sound with balloon attached to the end of it, the sound 


¥ic. 20.—1, Volume of abdominal organs; 2, volume of arm; 3, respiration. 


From — to +, suggestion to hypnotised subject of his execution. 


being connected to a strong tambour. He also has repeated 
Mosso’s experiment of placing a man in the horizontal posture on 
a long balanced board and seeing whether the head or feet end 
becomes heavier in different emotional states. Weber put the 
abdomen mostly on the foot side of the axis, instead of on the 
head side as Mosso, and balanced by weights on the head side. He 
found that mental work and painful emotions determine blood 
to the abdomen, while pleasurable ideas and those of active move- 
ment send blood from the abdomen into the peripheral parts. 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 149 


Pleasurable feelings are accompanied by relaxation of the muscles 
of the limbs and dilatation of the vessels of the limbs—relaxation 
in contrast to the strained taut condition of work. It is not the 
brain, as Mosso supposed, but the belly within which the blood 
collects during mental effort. The brain, limited in its expansion 
by the rigid cranial wall, contains a quantity of blood which can 
vary but little, except when the tissues of the brain actually lose 
or gain water, and so shrink or swell. Rise of arterial pressure 
produced by the contraction of the splanchnic area does not 
expand the brain so much as increase the proportion of arterial 
blood to venous blood within it and accelerate the velocity of 
flow. In muscular exercise the limbs receive more and the venous 


cistern in the abdomen contains less. The stagnation of the blood 


in the abdominal veins and liver during mental work and the 
sluggish circulation caused by the arm-chair posture must have a 
direct bearing on the irritable dyspepsia of brain-workers. Was 
not Thomas Carlyle cured of his by horse riding? Exercise 
sweeps the body clean of unoxidised food stuffs by the swirling 
current of blood and the greatly increased rate of metabolism. 
The breathing volume of a boxer after a three minutes bout may 
go up from the resting volume 9 to 40 litres per minute. How the 
muscles must squeeze the blood in his organs and drive it to the 
right heart ! 


THE FintRatTion HyPoruHEsis 


In the explanation of physiological and pathological processes 
capillary pressure has been since the teaching of Ludwig constantly 
evolved as a deus ex machina for producing filtration. Thus the 
glomerular capillaries have been generally supposed to filter water 
and salts into the renal capsules under the pressure of the blood, 
which is supposed to be higher than that of the urine in the renal 
tubule. Similarly in dealing with the formation of lymph, filtra- 


_ tion by capillary pressure is supposed to be an important factor, 
% a school of physiologists of whom Starling and Cohnstein in 


st have been chief exponents. Dropsy and cedema, such as 
occur in cardiac incompetence, have been attributed to filtration 
brought about by increased capillary pressure; an altered per- 
meability of the capillaries being admitted as an accessory patho- 


logical factor in the explanation of the fact that increased vascular 


ae 


150 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


pressure does not per se produce cedema in the healthy tissues. 
To show to what an extreme limit this view may be carried, the 
following example may be cited. Asher, a chief opponent of the 
mechanical theory of lymph formation, has sought to overthrow 
it by a number of ingenious experiments, of which the following is 
one. He injected into the vein of a dog a concentrated solution 
of sugar, and immediately killed the animal. The lymph flow from 
the thoracic duct was measured, and this, in spite of the animal 
being dead, increased from 4 to 37 ¢.c. per 10 minutes. In this 
case, says Asher, there can be no question of a filtration pressure 
because the animal is dead and the circulation at an end.» In an 
argument upholding the filtration hypothesis, Bainbridge suggests, 
however, that the sugar, by raising the osmotic pressure of the 
blood, drew water from the tissues into the blood, and thereby 


increased the volume and the mean (residual) hydrostatic pressure — 


in the vascular system. ‘‘ The capillary pressure after death will 
therefore,” he says, ‘be unusually high, and there will be an 
excessive transudation of lymph, so that on analysis, the 
experiment is seen to support, rather than to oppose, Starling’s 
views.” 

Now to make the mechanical theory possible we must suppose 
that the blood is driven through a system of rigid tubes with a 
sieve-like structure. If water be driven under pressure through a 
coil of hose which leaks, and the coil of hose be sunk in a tub of 
water, then it is. true water will filter through and the tub will 
overflow. But,in an organ of the body the conditions are quite 
otherwise, and an effective filtration pressure cannot exist because, 
as the writer has proved in the case of the brain, the blood in the 
capillaries, the tissue cells and lymph are at one and the same 
pressure, viz. the capillary-venous pressure. 

Let us take the example of the salivary gland. Asher has 
pointed out that while lymph flows in increased amount when the 
gland is thrown into secretory activity, from an atropinised gland 
no lymph flows when the chorda tympani is excited, although the 
arteries dilate, and the capillary pressure is raised. Here is an 
experiment, he says, which is against the filtration hypothesis, 
for capillary pressure is raised and yet lymph does not flow. The 
experiment may prove Asher’s chief contention, that it is functional 
activity with the consequent production of metabolites of high 
osmotic pressure which determines the flow of lymph, but is not 


4 
. 
n 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 151 


required to disprove the filtration hypothesis because the con- 
ditions are the same both in the resting and the excited gland, 
in as far as the whole of any lobule of the gland must be at the 
same pressure, and therefore no filtration pressure exists in the 
one state more than in the other. 

_ The gland is composed of a connective tissue framework, hold- 
ing together tubules full of secreting cells, whose protoplasm con- 
tains some 80 per cent. of water, the tissue spaces surrounding 
the tubules are full of lymph, the capillaries of blood, and the 
tubules of saliva. The wet films of protoplasm which form the 
walls of the tubules, the capillaries, and the lymphatics, may act 
as colloidal surfaces separating fluids of different chemical con- 
stitution, but cannot possibly act as rigid sieve-like structures. 
There cannot be a difference of hydrostatic pressure on either side 
of the films, as is required by the mechanical theory. Molecular 
not molar forces are here at play. The salivary cells are endowed 
by their colloidal structure with the power of linking up or setting 
free crystalloids brought to them in solution, and are thus the seat 
of the play of complex forces of surface and osmotic energy, and 
at the same time are the seat of chemical reaction, selective in 
_ character and depending on the ferments they contain—-the keys 
which fit the locks of chemical constitution. These living cells 
control the passage of fluid in one or other direction, and the 
mystery of the secretory process requires the same solution as in 
the case of the unicellular organism, and this solution at present 
is just as far from attainment in the one case as in the other. 

The comparative study of the structure of the nephridial 
tissues shows how far away from the truth are the mechanical 
theories of renal secretion which have held their ground in physio- 
logical text-books for the last fifty years. The writer has con- 
densed the following passages from the work of Dahlgren and 
Kepner. 

In the unicellular animals the excretory organ is formed by 
contracting vacuoles which form channels leading from the endo- 
plasm to the exterior of the cell. The fluid that the vacuoles 
throw out by their rhythmic expansion and contraction is drained 
from the cell and is charged with uric acid. The contractile 
vacuoles of Paramecium are two in number and permanent 
_ features of the cell. Their inner surface dips into the endoplasm 
___ and their outer surface opens through the ectoplasm to the exterior. 


152 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


Into each contractile vacuole a radiating series of drainage channels 
lead. The channels are filled as the contractile vacuole discharges 
its contents. In the higher animals tissues, selected for the secre- 
tion of urates, form the nephridia. In the lowest Metazoa, so far 
as is known, any surface cells may take on nephridial functions. The 
higher forms of nephridial tissues are usually mesodermal structures. 
These tissues are always epithelial; one face of the epithelium is 
directed towards the fluids from which waste products are being 
taken, the other face forms the surface of a retaining or conducting 
cavity. In the Ascidians the renal epitheliuin is a vestigial cuelomic 
epithelium. Into this blind space waste products are excreted 
and stored as solid particles. All other nephridial sacs or tubules 
deliver the waste products to the exterior through nephridial pores 
or ducts. In all the simpler forms where the nephridial tubules 
have a small lumen, the latter is intracellular. In invertebrates 
where the lumen becomes larger, and in all vertebrates it is inter- 
cellular. 

The fluids from which the waste products are taken may be 
intercellular fluid, coelomic fluid or blood. Intercellular fluid and 
ceelomic fluid when associated with nephridia bathe them on their 
proximal surfaces. The blood supply is effected in two ways. In 
a few types the nephridial tissues are merely bathed in the blood 
(Insecta). Blood is usually supplied to the nephridial tissues 
through the capillaries of a circulatory system. In the simplest 
tissues there is but an ordinary supply. In the vertebrates there 
is a general capillary supply as well as a terminal supply. The 
terminal capillary structure is a more or less distorted plexus 
which is supported upon a connective tissue framework at 
definite terminal regions of the nephridial tissues, forming a 
“ glomus.” \ 

We may rest assured that the nephridial tissues act in 
much the same way in every class of animal, viz. form vacuoles 
containing granules of excretory substances and expel the con- 
tents of these into tubules, Examination of the kidneys of 
hibernating and thirsting animals show evidence of secretory 
granules and vacuoles forming in the resting kidney. These 
disappear when diuresis is established. There is no question of 
filtration pressure in the excreting vacuole of Parameecium, none 
~ in the case of Insecta where the nephridia lie in a bath of blood, 
and consideration of the structural conditions which pertain in 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 153 


_ the mammalian kidney will show that there can be no question of 
a capillary pressure which can produce filtration therein. 

The formation of glands in the embryo displays the same pro- 
gressive evolution from the simple to the complex state, as is 
observed in ascending the animal scale. The most perfect and 
complex glands of the higher animals resemble in embryo the 
secretory organs of the lower animals. The arborescent ramifica- 
tions of the blood vessels accompany the ducts in their develop- 
ment, and in proportion as the development of a secreting plane 
surface into a cecum and ramified ceca proceeds, the vascular 
layer of the originally simple membrane spreads as a closely in- 
vesting network around them. The ramified secreting tubes, 
which, when the structure is simple as in Insecta and Crustacea 
and in the pancreas of the rabbit, lie free freely and unconnectedly, 
in proportion as their evolution is carried further acquire a common 
covering or capsule; and thus a solid organ is produced. The 
vascular conditions in the simple and the complex are the same. 
No one would be rash enough to suggest that filtration of fluid 
could occur from the capillaries of the rabbit’s pancreas through 
the ramified tubules exposed ia the mesentery in a thin sheet. 
The tubules and the capillaries here are obviously at one and the 
same pressure, that of the abdominal cavity, alike squeezed. by 
the respiratory muscles, and pulsed by the wave of blood which 
distends the abdominal arteries at each cardiac systole. There 
is barely a positive pressure in the capillaries, but this, aided by 
the rhythmic squeeze of the respiratory muscles, is sufficient to 
maintain the onward flow of blood. The gland cells when excited 
at times secrete their juice at a positive pressure, which with the 
help of the peristaltic wave of the muscular wall of the tubules 
drives the fluid into the intestine. Similarly in the case of the 
kidney, the blood in the capillary networks, the tissue lymph, and 
the urine in the tubules are all at one and the same pressure— 
the capillary-venous pressure. The whole kidney is expanded by 
_ each arterial pulse, and drops of urine may be squeezed thereby 
into the pelvis from the mouths of the tubules. The whole kidney 
is rhythmically squeezed by the respiratory muscles. The tubules 
are formed of watery cytoplasm, surrounded by lymph spaces full 
of fluid, and networks of capillaries full of blood. There is nothing 
of a rigid structure here, nothing of the nature of membranes which 
can separate fluid at one pressure in one system of tubes from 


154 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


fluid at another pressure in another system. If the capillary- 
venous pressure were higher than the pressure in the tubules, the 
latter would be obliterated until the pressures became the same, 
likewise if the pressure in the tubules were higher than that in 
the veins. If the ureter be obstructed so that the pressure rises 
within it to say 40 mm. Hg, the capillary-venous pressure and 
tension of the whole kidney rises to this amount, and it takes a 
pressure of 40 mm. Hg to drive water through a hollow needle 
into the kidney substance. If the renal vein be obstructed till 


the pressure in the renal venules rises to 40 mm. Hg, the same must . 


hold good, and the pressure of the urine in the renal tubules be- 
come the same. If the arterioles of the kidney be dilated the 
whole organ swells in its capsule, and the tension of the whole 
rises, the vascular system and the tubular system together ap- 


proximating towards the mgid condition, but both at the same © 


pressure, viz. the capillary-venous pressure. Under these con- 
ditions the velocity of blood flow is greatly increased, and if the 
kidney be enclosed in plaster of Paris, so that it cannot expand, 
it makes no jot of difference, because the arteries dilate at the 
expense of the veins, which are narrowed until a rigid system with 
a rigid flow is produced. When the blood reaches the glomerular 
capillaries, three courses are open to the fluid part, the capillary 
wall retaining the corpuscles. It may pass on by the efferent 


venules or by the lymphatics, or into the capsular ends of the’ 


renal tubules. Which course it takes depends on the play of 
such forces as surface tension, adsorption, and osmosis. Filtration 
has nothing to do with it, for the fluid pressure must be the same 
on either side of the wet films engaged, which are of a tenuity 
comparable to that of the bubbles of a soap lather. 

Confirmation of the above views may be drawn from the per- 
fusion experiments carried out by Sollmann on excised kidneys 
in spite of the fact that the flow he obtained from the ureter was 
attributed by him to filtration. He perfused 1 per cent. salt 
solution at arterial pressure through the renal artery, and collected 
and measured the outflow both from the renal vein and the ureter. 
He found the venous flow reached its maximum in fifteen minutes, 
while the ureter flow continued to increase for one to two hours, 
thus rising slowly to a maximum. As the ureter flow increased 
the venous flow declined slowly, and then. the two flows ran a 
parallel course. Subsequently undergoing minor oscillations—the 


a ~ 


—— ——— 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 155 


venous flow increasing slightly, and the ureter flow decreasing 
at first, and then increasing. On altering the arterial perfusion 
pressure the venous flow and ureter flow, the volume of the kidney, 
the maximal venous pressure and the maximal ureter pressure all 
varied in the same sense, but the maximal venous and ureter 
pressures were at a lower level than the injection pressure owing 
to leakage through capsular collaterals. If the perfusion pressure 
were made rhythmically intermittent the venous and ureter outflow 
were increased. Obstruction of the renal vein caused swelling of 
the kidney and almost complete cessation of the ureter flow. A 
graduated increase in the venous pressure, produced by raising the 
level of the venous outflow tube, diminished the venous and ureter 
flow and expanded the kidney, especially when the pressure rose 
above 40 to 60 mm. Hg. The venous and ureter outflow varied 
with the molecular concentration of the perfusion fluid. Hyper- 
tonic solutions caused lessened resistance in, and more flow from, 
the tubules and vessels, and hypertonic solutions the opposite. 
In volume a hypertonic solution produced at first a sharp fall, 
followed by a rise to original level, while a hypotonic solution gave 
a progressive diminution in renal volume. Many of these results 
are difficult to explain, but none are in favour of filtration. Vernon 
has shown that the fluid coming from the kidney under these 
conditions does not correspond to that sent into the artery. The 
kidney metabolism continues for days after death in a modified 
form. At first we see the perfused fluid found its way through 
the capillary-venous system, and only much more slowly made a 
passage through the renal tubules. At the start the capillaries 
were distended with the perfused fluid, the lumina of the capsules 
almost obliterated; the surface tension of the capillaries there- 
fore was high, that of the capsules low. The surface energy of 
the latter therefore was high. It may have been these conditions 
which led to the transference of the fluid from one side to the other 


of the wet film formed by the capillary and capsular epithelial 
_ cells. We may suppose with justice that the epithelium retains 


the corpuscles and the native colloidal material of the blood. -We 
know some colloids may pass. Sollmann found gum arabic, added 
to the perfused fluid passed through, and egg albumen passes into 
the urine of normal people if much be eaten. In the case of the 
renal tubules at the time when the capillaries become distended 


_ with fluid, the epithelium of the tubules contains stores of ex- 


5 Sa ae 
’ 


156 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


cretory products, some in colloidal linkage, some as crystalloidal 
substance. In these cells there comes about a complex play of the 
forces of osmosis and surface tension. Supposing the epithelium 
of the tubules swells owing to their surface energy being great in 
comparison with the capillaries where the surface tension is great, 
then the epithelium in its turn will have a higher surface tension 
than the lumina of the tubules, and this surface energy will cause 
the transference of liquid from vessel to tubule. We have in the 
kidney a gelatinous foam-like structure, the meshes of the foam 
containing fluid, and the meshes being formed of two sets of ramified 
channels, the one set leading to the venous and the other to the 
ureter outlet. Blood is driven. in pulses into the vascular mesh- 
work. By such forces as adsorption and surface tension the 
fluid part of the blood pervades the tubular meshwork, while the 
living cells concentrate, and extrude vacuoles filled with, urinary 
excretions. The mystery of the whole process is hidden from us, but 
we may be sure that an excretory cell in a bath of collecting fluid 
and provided with an excretory channel form the structural basis of 
the mechanism, and that the mechanical filtration theories may be 
relegated to the conceits of a science in its more primitive days. 
It has always been assumed by those who maintain the de- 
fence of the filtration theory that the capillary pressure must be 
raised not only when the general venous and arterial pressure rise 
together, but also when the general venous pressure is raised, so 
long as there is.no fall of arterial pressure compensating for this 
venous rise. No such assumption can be made in regard to a 
rise of arterial pressure because of the unknown factor—the re- 
sistance in the arterioles. Now the experiments of Martin Flack 
and the writer made on the arterial venous and capillary pressure 
of their own limbs show that the above assumptions are, at any 
rate in the case of the limbs, not valid. The capillary system is 
not filled to distension ; there is a large potential space which only 
gradually fills when the venous return is impeded, and thus the 
venous pressure—the veins being filled by broad paths of low 
resistance—-may rise greatly while the capillary pressure in large 
areas drained by these veins is scarcely altered. The gradual 
filling of the capillary system, while producing distension of the | 
. part, enormously increases the surface exposure of the blood fluid 
and the surface tension of the \capillary films, and these are pro- 
bably factors of the greatest importance in the formation of lymph, 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 157 


another factor being the alteration of tissue metabolism produced 
by the impeded blood flow with consequent want of oxygen, setting 
free of crystalloids from colloidal combination, and increase of 
waste products, leading to a rise of osmotic pressure and swelling 
of the tissues. 

- In the case of such an organ as the liver a great expansion 
takes place when the outflow from the vena cava inferior is impeded, 
or excess of blood or saline is injected intravenously. Here again 
increase of surface exposure and stagnation of flow are the factors 
at work which produce the greater outflow of lymph—not the 
increased capillary pressure. That the capillary pressure is in- 
creased in such capsulated organs as the kidney, salivary gland, 


liver, and brain when the outflow of venous blood (or secretion) 


is impeded is proved by the increased tension of the organ, but this 
increase must be uniform throughout each lobule, if not through- 
out the whole organ, and cannot act as a filtering agent. 


THE RESIDUAL VASCULAR PRESSURE IN THE DEAD ANIMAL 


The vascular system as a whole is not filled to distension. 
There is in the dead animal no uniform positive mean pressure 
throughout the vascular system. If there did exist in the system 
such a positive ‘mean hydrostatic pressure ” when the heart is 
arrested and the skeletal muscles are relaxed by death, it must be 
produced by some secretory power of the vascular epithelium or 
by the osmotic pressure of the blood. The blood must possess a 
greater power of holding water than the tissues, and the wet film 
of the capillary wall must be able to retain the water, so that the 
whole system is filled to distension. The shrinkage of the face 
in fainting, and how much more in death, shows the error of this 
conception. After death there is a residual pressure in the aorta 
owing to the resistance in the arterioles which contract and do 
not allow the last of the blood to leak through into the capillary- 
venous system. There is also 4 residual pressure in the vene cave 
produced by the influence of gravity and the post-mortem con- 
traction of the viscera which drives the blood from the capillary 
areas into the vene cave. These residual pressures are, however, 
unequal, and cannot be taken as representing a “ mean hydrostatic 
pressure” of the whole system. Thus the writer found in a © 


_ morphinised dog :— 


= 


158 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


Aorta. | Vena Cava. 
Mm. Hg. Mm. Hg. 
165 | 3 Normal, 
30 | 14 Heart arrested by 
| vagus. 
and in a morphinised and curarised dog :— 
= aa 7 
Aorta. | Vena Cava. | 
Mm. Hg. | Mm. Hg. ie 
80 | 3 Normal. 
| 14 | * 6 Heart arrested by 
vagus. 


In the uncurarised animal the convulsive movements of res- 
piration by compressing the abdominal vessels raise the vena cava 
and lessen the fall of aortic pressure. 

_ Similarly in Cohnheim’s classical experiments, when he injected 
oil into the pericardium to study the effect of impeding the filling 
of the heart, there resulted no equality of residual pressures in the 
aorta and vena cava. . 

It has been supposed that a general constriction of the arterioles, 
such as may be produced by the injection of adrenalin, would by 
reducing the capacity of the vascular system raise the “ mean 
hydrostatic pressure,” and therefore the capillary pressure. The 
writer produced by injecting adrenalin a rise of arterial pressure 
from 80 to 180 mm. Hg, and then arrested the circulation by 
clamping the ascending aorta. The vena cava pressure fell to the 
same residual pressure as when the aorta was clamped before the 
injection of the adrenalin. A rise of pressure of 1500 mm. of blood 
in the aorta may be accompanied only by a rise of 50 mm. in the 
vene cave, and this small rise is due not to diminution of the total 
capacity of the vascular system, but to a greater return to the 
vene cave of blood squeezed from the splanchnic area and the 
failure of the heart to empty itself in the face of the high resistance. 

It has been supposed that the intravenous injection of physio- 
logical saline, or the injection of concentrated sugar solution—this 
produces hydremia by drawing water out of the tissues into the 
blood—it has been supposed that either of these agents increases 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 159 


_ the flow of lymph by adding to the total volume of fluid in the 
- vascular system, and thus increasing the capillary pressure and 
’ filtration through the capillary wall. Now in a case of poly- 


cythwmia studied by the writer, the arterial, the venous, and the 
capillary pressure in the arm, held at heart’s apex level, were the 
same as in himself. There was not a sign of congestion, except a 
redness of the hands and face and fulness of the veins, and not a 
trace of cedema in this man. And yet he had a hemoglobin per 
cent. of 155, 33 times the normal oxygen capacity, 12,000,000 red 
corpuscles per c.mm. of blood, and a total blood volume 2} times 
the normal as determined by A. 8. Boycott. 

During the temporary arrest of the circulation in the dog the 
writer has injected rapidly 30 c.c. and more of physiological saline 
solution into the venz cave, and observed only a temporary and 
slight rise of pressure in the ven cave during the injection and 
no effect at all on the residual pressure in the arteries ; an experi- 
ment which shows without question that there is no such thing as 
a positive “mean hydrostatic pressure” in the vascular system, 
and that the pressure can be raised in one part witbout influencing . 
another, owing to the unfilled areas of capillaries, and the roomy 
capacity of the veins. 


Dog: Cannulex, in Aorta and Vena Cava Superior, connected with 
Manometers filled with 1 per cent. Sodium Citrate Solution. 


l 
mime, Beta Aortic enya Vena Cara | 
Mm. H,0. Mm. H,0. | | 
317 | 114 89 Pulmonary artery 
| occluded. wa 
3°18-19 114 204 Convulsive respira- 
tions. 
3:30 70 64 Heart poisoned by | 
chloroform | 
} 832 70 127 100 cc. saline in- | 
jected into femoral 
| vein. 
3°35 76 7 
3°45. | 63 56 


Tue Erect or Onsrructine THE Vena Cava 
Some remarkable experiments by C. Bolton may be cited here 


in connection with these experiments of the writer. Bolton 


160 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


studied the result of totally obstructing and of narrowing the 
ven cave in order to arrive at the share in the production of 
dropsy which increased capillary pressure had. He tried at first 
to produce cardiac deficiency by constriction of the pericardium by 
means of sutures, so as to prevent the proper diastolic expansion 
of the heart, but gave up this line of work as he found it very 
difficult to hit the right amount of constriction. The animals 
either died or recovered without symptoms. To obstruct the 
ven cave he made an incision one to two inches long parallel to 
the ribs in the third right intercostal space for the superior, and 
seventh space for the inferior cava. Artificial respiration was put 
on, the ribs drawn apart by retractors and the lung held aside by a 
spatula, and incomplete obstruction set up by encircling the vein 
with a short piece of soft rubber catheter of appropriate diameter. 

The thorax was then closed, after squeezing out the air, and the: 
animal allowed to recover. The venous pressures were measured 
in some of the animals after varying degrees of constriction had 
been established, the measurements being taken in the external 
iliac; at the lower end of the femoral above the ankle; in the 
splenic branch of the portal; and in the post-auricular branch of 
the external jugular. Complete occlusion of the superior cava 
caused the death of the animal in one to six days. There was 
caused considerable cedema of the mediastinum and exudation of 
venous fluid in the pericardial and pleural sacs. The animal 
refused food, wasted, and passed less urine. When the obstruction 
was made above the azygos vein, one anima! survived, efficient 
anastomoses becoming established by way of the internal mammary, 
azygos, veins of diaphragm and comes nervi phrenici. There was 
cedema and fluid in the pleura and pericardium until these 
anastomoses were established properly. Another animal died 
eighteen days after operation, and 145 c.c. of fluid were found in 
the right and 125 c.c. in the left pleural cavity. In the cases of 
partial obstruction Bolton found constriction to three-fifths of the 
normal size produces cedema and hydrothorax. 

Such obs*zuction only raises the venous pressure in the external 
jugular by 20 to 40 mm. of blood, and quite temporarily. Com- 
plete obstruction altered the arterial pressure very slightly and 
raised the pressure in the external jugular by 130 mm. of blood, 
but within an hour the pressure was normal again. The cedema 
and dropsy were produced next day long after the pressure had 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 161 


become normal again. Complete obstruction of the inferior cava 
caused death in a few hours (as was determined by R. Lower in 
the days of Charles II.). The arterial pressure at once fell to 
30 to 40 mm. Hg, and the venous pressure in the external iliac 
tose by 100 mm. of blood or more, but within an hour fell back to 
its old level. 

Partial constriction to more than three-fifths caused death. 
The diameter of the vena cava is about 5 mm., and on constriction 
to 3 mm. the animal survived, while on 2 to 24 mm. it might or 
might not die. The arterial pressure fell about 20 mm. Hg when 
the constriction was to 3 mm., and the pressure in the external 
iliac vein rose 20 to 30 mm. of blood, but this rise was quite tem- 
porary. Ascites resulted until proper anastomoses were established 
by way of the veins of the abdominal wall; but the first signs of 
ascites occurred long after the venous pressure had returned to 
normal. Constriction of the portal vein from its normal diameter 
4 mm. to 14 mm. gave the same results as constriction of the 
vena cava inferior. Constriction of both cave to 3 mm. produced 
dropsy of the pleure and peritoneum just as in a case of uncom- 
pensated heart disease. 

Finally Bolton observed that 130 c.c. physiological saline (an 
amount equal to from two-thirds to the whole blood quantum) 
might be slowly injected in the course of one and a half hours, the 
blood pressures remaining normal, and some ascites being produced 
meanwhile. 

In the experiments on the effect of complete obstruction of the 
inferior vena cava he records that while the pressure in the external 
iliac vein rose by 100 mm. of blood or more, that in the femoral 
vein above the ankle only rose by some 60 mm. _ The writer 
attributes this to the low pressure in the arteries, the derivation 
of the arterial blood through the lower resistance channels—the ab- 
dominal vessels—and consequent slow filling of the veins of the legs. 

Bolton’s observations show that the ascites and cedema occur 
when the general venous and arterial pressures are normal. They 
must be ascribed, therefore, to altered tissue metabolism, greater 
filling of and stasis in capillary areas, and consequent change in 
the conditions of surface and osmotic energy.! 


1 The work of B. Moore and his co-workers has shown how the osmotic pressure 
____ of the complex of colloids and crystalloids which forms the serum or tissue proteid 
is altered by slight changes in alkalinity, &c. See Bio-Chemical Journal, vol. iii. 
| - P- = 1908, 
L 


162 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


The mechanical theory as to the causation of cardiac dropsy 
has been summed up by Bainbridge as follows :— 

“First, in an uncompensated heart, there is a fall of arterial 
pressure, and a rise of venous pressures near the heart. There is 
also a fall of capillary pressure, in consequence of the fall of arterial 
pressure, in the kidneys, intestines, and peripheral parts of the 
body. The fall of capillary pressure lessens filtration, and for a 
time upsets the balance between filtration and absorption ; con- 
sequently an excess of fluid is absorbed by the blood vessels from 
the intestines and peripheral tissues. 

“Secondly, this continued absorption associated with the 
diminished urinary secretion, leads to hydremic plethora, and 
increases the mean systemic pressure. 

‘“‘ Thirdly, this hydreemic plethora raises the capillary pressure 


all over the body, and promotes increased filtration, the more so 


because the venous state of the blood damages the capillaries, 
and increases their permeability. A further subsidiary factor is 
the obstruction to the entrance of lymph into the great veins at 
the thoracic duct owing to the excessive venous pressure.” 

The writer controverts these views in each particular—the 
arterial pressure is not altered in uncompensated cases of heart 
disease unless death is imminent; neither is the venous pressure 
altered, nor the capillary pressure. 

Neither hydremic nor blood plethora raises the arterial venous 
or capillary pressures excepting during the period of intra-venous 
injection. Even if the capillary pressure were raised it would not 
cause filtration because the blood in the capillaries and the tissue 
lymph in any organ are and must be at one and the same hydro- 
static pressure. 


THE EFrFrect OF OBSTRUCTING THE BLOOD VESSELS 


While cessation of the blood flow in the “ higher level ” centres 
of the brain abolishes consciousness in a second or two, perma- 
nent recovery from a complete anemia may occur which has lasted 
some minutes, at the outside twenty minutes (Stewart and Guthrie). 
The heart quickly stops beating when the coronary arteries are 
closed, but can be recovered by transfusion hours after. Muscle 

1 A temporary recovery may occur after sixty minutes, Guthrie transplanted 


the head of a dog, and obtained reflex movements of the eyes, &c. ‘The circulation 
in the brain had been interrupted twenty-nine minutes. 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 163 


can stand a much longer anemia than the heart and viscera, and 
connected tissue and epithelia still longer. After the obstruction of 
arteries, beyond the possibility of an efficient current being set up 
by way of anastomotic pathways, the capillary-venous area of the 
| part affected fills by the slow inflow from surrounding areas, the 
plasma passes out owing to the altered osmotic conditions, and 
the red corpuscles heap together. Such congestion is seen in the 
tongue of the curarised frog after tying the artery on either side 
of the under surface. If the root of the ear of a rabbit be confined 
by a ligature for eight to ten hours, and the string be loosened, 
the ear swells and becomes very red, and the tissue lymph and 
white corpuscles increase in the tissue spaces. After ligation for 
twenty-four hours, small hemorrhages occur, the permeability of 
the capillaries being then seriously altered. 

The effect of back congestion has been studied in the frog’s 
tongue after tying the veins which on either side carry the blood 
from the tongue to the larger veins in the floor of the mouth 
(Cohnheim, Thoma). The. congestion can be observed micro- 
scopically as it spreads backwards. The veins and capillaries 
widen in the tongue, but scarcely so in the web of the foot where the 
surrounding tissue is inextensile. As the plasma escapes into the 
tissue spaces, the capillaries and veins become choked with red 
corpuscles. If a cannula is placed in the lymphatics on the outer 
side of the leg of a dog, and a hgature is drawn round the thigh so 
as to obstruct the veins, the lymph—before scarcely moving— 
begins to flow, and the foot may swell, gradually becoming 
cedematous. Division of the vaso-constrictor nerves increases the 
effect. This is well seen on obstructing the veins of the root of the 
rabbit’s ear and dividing the cervical sympathetic nerve. 

If the veins of the dog’s leg are entirely obstructed by injecting 
plaster of Paris into the vein on the dorsum of the foot—confining 
the thigh by a ligature during the injection—the leg next day is 
become cylindrical with cedema. The kidney swells after ligation 
of the renal vein to double or treble the size, and bloody extra- 
vasations appear in its substance. In the frog’s tongue the red 
cells may be seen escaping through bulgings which appear in the 
capillaries and small veins, that is when the venous obstruction 
is complete. In all these cases increased capillary pressure is not 

_ the prime cause of the phenomena. They result from stagnation, 
altered metabolism, and altered osmotic energy dnd surface energy 


164 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


of the capillary cells and tissues. The appearance of a part in a 
state of passive hyperemia is livid and bluish, swollen, pitted by 
pressure ; its temperature is lowered. The degree of swelling 
depends on the relative in and out flow of blood. It is little 
in hypostatic congestion because of the feeble arterial supply. 
Whether the tissues live or die depends on the maintenance of 
some flow by collateral paths. 

When a vein is slowly closed by a thrombus, collateral path- 
ways have time to enlarge. “Thrombosis of the common iliac 
vein or vena cava inferior produces oedema of the lower limbs 
and compensatory enlargement of the cutaneous vessels of the 
legs and abdominal wall. Thrombosis of the subclavian vein can 
be compensated by collateral paths through the internal mammary 
and intercostal veins, and even thrombosis of the innominate vein 
fails to produce cedema if the laryngeal descending veins remain 
open.” The effects depend on the rapidity with which the throm- 
bosis takes place, 7.e. on the relative damage of the tissues by 
deficient flow. If the inferior cava is thrombosed rapidly, general 
oedema occurs below the level, and blood and albumen appear in 
the urine if the renal veins are involved. Closure of the portal 
vein by a tumour or of its branches by cirrhosis of the liver, leads 
to congestion of the intestines, enlargement of the spleen, and 
ascites. Here again the results are due entirely to deficient flow 
and altered constitution of blood and tissue cells. The cranial 
sinuses are liable to thrombosis. They are wide, of irregular 
lumen, with Pacchionian granulations dipping into them, and in 
some cases bands crossing them. The current is forwarded by 
respiration but cannot be influenced directly by muscular con- 
tractions. Hence thrombosis occurs in marasmic states, with 
feeble respiration and deficient cardiac power. Sudden blockage 
of an artery by ligature or embolus is of little effect so long as there 
are adequate collateral anastomotic paths. These rapidly dilate, 
and while the blocked artery shrinks up the anastomotic capillary 
paths, where the flow is increased, develop the structure of arteries. 
Retinal, coronary, renal, splenic, and cerebral arteries beyond the 
circle of Willis behave as terminal arteries. Closure of these and 
of the superior mesenteric artery in spite of its collateral paths, — 
leads to stasis and necrosis. It seems to be possible for an embolus 
to be driven by the respiratory and muscular movements in a 
retrograde fashion down the large veins by coughing or expiratory 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 165 


effort, for after the injection of emulsions of white sand into the 
jugular or femoral vein, grains have been found in the veins of 
the face, liver, kidney, as well as in the venew cave. Thus the old 
Galenical doctrine that the blood is driven down the veins from the 
liver to the body has a grain of truth in it. 


PLETHORA 


When freshly defibrinated dog’s blood, warmed to body 
temperature, is injected into a dog by way of the jugular vein, the 
arterial pressure may rise with each injection some 20 to 30 mm. Hg, 
but quickly returns to its old level. After blood to the extent of 

3 to 4 per cent. of the body weight has been injected the arterial 
pressure may reach the height of 170 to 180 mm. Hg, but much 
beyond this it cannot be driven. If the transfusion be continued 
until more than 10 to 12 per cent. of the body weight has been 
introduced—t.e. more than twice the normal blood quantum— 
significant upward and downward variations of pressure occur, 
which presage cardiac failure. All the animals die in the course 
of a day or so who have received over 10 to 12 per cent. of their 
body weight. 

During each period of injection the heart being better filled 
responds with an ampler output and raises the arterial pressure. 
The pressure returns to its old level so soon as the injected blood is 
swallowed up by the capillary-venous reservoirs. 

Finally, when so much blood has been introduced that the 
venze cave and liver have become distended, the heart becomes 
over-loaded, and having to perform its systole in a dilated state 
and owing to its greater output against an increased resistance, 
begins to fail. The share of the vaso-motor system in the restitu- 
tion of the pressure in the early stages of the experiment is made 
manifest by dividing beforehand the spinal cord in the lower 
cervical region. The low arterial pressure, which obtains after 
such a lesion, is driven up by each injection until the normal arterial 
pressure is reached and restitution of pressure between the injec- 
tions then occurs. The falling back of the pressure after that point 
has been reached is due to the escape of the blood from the arteries 
into the capacious capillary-venous areas. 

It is in the small veins and capillaries and particularly in the 

___ abdominal organs that the excess of blood lodges. The pressure 


166 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


in the vene cave rises during each injection but sinks again owing 
to the extensibility and large potential capacity of the capillary- 
venous system. The liver holds a great deal and becomes large 
and firm to the touch, and when excised a great quantity of blood 
streams from it. The large abdominal veins and liver by receiving 
most of the blood injected protect the heart from over-distension, 
but if the transfusion into the jugular vein be made too rapidly, 
no time is given for the filling out of the liver and other capillary- 
venous reservoirs, and the heart becomes over-loaded and fails. 

Plethysmographic records of the heart showed Johansson and 
Tigerstedt that with a sufficiently slow transfusion there is an 
increase in the systolic output and no passive congestion of the 
heart occurs. With a more rapid rate of transfusion the heart 
fails to empty itself and expels a smaller amount of blood than | 
before. Hence the constancy of the arterial pressure during 
transfusion is to be ascribed largely to the heart’s action. 

Rabbit: period of transfusion of 20 per cent. of blood 
quantum marked by the vertical lines :— 


Output measured by Stromihr introduced into Ascending Aorta 


Arterial pressure . 78 | 88 99 97/114 131 134 133 140 
Output of heart per 18 | 32 53 39/| 31 33 32 38 36 
minute per kgm. 
of body weight 
144 147 148 146 148 147 148 149 147 
36 3606 «633806 (360 33244 45 OSC 


145 131 119 112 107 104 101 99 94 90 
66 66 66 61 59 59 59 54 50 51 


The following shows the effect of a larger rapid injection = 
50 per cent. of the blood quantum :— 


Arterial pressure . 78 | 89 120 130 144 158 160|151 146 136 

Minute volume . 33 | 49 80 71 51 35 32] 31 32 33 
131 

and so on to >, and then to | = the output rising as the arterial 


pressure fell and the heart recovered as the blood fluid found room 
in the capillary-venous system and tissue spaces. 


After an injection of 69 per cent. of the normal blood quantum _ 


in the period of fourteen minutes the heart worked well at first, 
and the arterial pressure rose from 50 to 125 mm. Hg. After 
twenty minutes the heart failed, the minute volume sank to half 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 167 


its original value, and the pressure fell from 117 to 37 mm. Hg. 
On, bleeding the rabbit 12 c.c. and then 20 c.c., and then again 
another undetermined amount, the pressure rose and reached 40 
mm. Hg some twenty minutes after the first signs of cardiac failure. 
At the end of another twenty minutes it had risen to 79 to 86 

It is possible that the cardiac failure may be due in part to 
other causes than the mere mechanical overloading of the heart. 
Some experiments made by Bier seem to show that defibrinated 
blood has a toxic influence, for he found that if he transfused the 
excised limb of a pig with pig’s blood, defibrinated and oxygenated, 
at arterial pressure, an active hyperemia ensued at first, and the 
- blood flowed from the veins in a full stream. Soon, however, the 
outflow diminished and became very small, and the limb turned 
deep blue in colour. Arterial blood transfused directly into the 
limb from another pig had no such effect ; the limb at each trans- 
fusion became hyperemic for a time and then returned to its 
natural colour. 

Worm-Miiller observed that bleeding an animal, to the extent 
only of that amount of blood which had been injected, killed it, 
if the transfusion had been a large one. The excess of blood fluid 
in part pooled in the expanded capillary-venous system and in 
the tissue spaces, in part excreted, could not be drawn on quickly 
enough to maintain an adequate supply to the heart. 

Much has been made of the increased resistance which is said 
to occur in plethora owing to transudation and excretion of the 
blood fluid and the concentration of the corpuscles. To the 
greater viscosity so produced the overstrain and failure of the 
heart has been attributed. The researches on surviving organs 
show on the whole the same proportion between viscosity and 
flow, as in a glass viscometer. If the viscosity is increased 
50 per cent. the flow will be about 50 per cent. slower. Such 
an alteration of viscosity is, however, of little significance in the 
vascular system, for a slight excitation of the vaso-motor nerves 
can alter the flow 100 per cent. or more. In the chewing muscles 
of the horse the flow may increase four times or more during work 
owing to vaso-dilatation and the furthering of the blood flow by 
the alternate relaxation and contraction of the muscles. A drug 
such as Yohinbim may in the first stage of its action lessen flow 
_ 50 per cent. and pressure 18 per cent., and subsequently increase 


168 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


flow 250 per cent. and pressure 11] per cent., so great is the 
effect of vaso-dilatation coupled with increased cardiac efficiency 
(Brodie and Miiller). 

In polycythemia, where the blood is more than doubled in 
quantity and the number of corpuscles twelve million instead of 
five, there are no signs of high pressure, hypertrophy of heart, &c. 
The heart continues to circulate about the normal amount, and 
the excess is stored away in the peripheral capillary-venous areas. 
Increased viscosity is compensated for easily by dilatation of the 
arterioles. The toxic conditions which arise in cases of poly- 
cythemia are probably due in part to the long time much of the 
blood must take to complete the circulation. Thrombosis also 
results in the cerebral veins. The viscosity of the blood may be 
increased nearly 100 per cent. by stasis—produced by confining it 
within a limb for twenty minutes by means of a rubber bandage. 
The increase is due to the passage of blood fluid into the tissues. 
In cholera and polycythemia the viscosity co-efficient has been 
observed to be as high as 20 in place of 4:8, the normal worth. 
A current of carbonic acid led through the blood, it is said, in- 
creases the viscosity 50 per cent. The maximal effect of hard 
work or Turkish baths and sweating is an increase of 20 to 30 
per cent. . 


HyDR2MIA 


Extraordinary amounts of physiological saline can be infused 
without harm—up to 60 to 70 per cent. of the body weight! No 
bad after effects follow the injection of threefold the fatal amount 
of blood. The arterial pressure is not notably altered, and the 
vena cava pressure, while rising by some 200 mm. of blood during 
the injection, soon returns again to the old level. The velocity of 
flow in the capillaries is greatly increased, as may be seen in the 
frog’s web, owing to the dilated arterioles and lessened viscosity. 
The output of the heart is immediately increased, and the heart 
shows no sign of failure. Thus Tigerstedt found on injecting into 
the rabbit 25 c.c. Ringer sol. (period of injection marked by 
vertical lines) :— 


Arterial pressure . 90| 96 99 97 | 8% 92 89 
Minute volume perkg. 51 | 64 78 100 | 95 122 122 


falling in a few minutes to . 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 169 


The greater part of the water passes rapidly out of the vascular 
system, the glands all actively secrete, watery urine is passed, the 
saliva drips, the guts become filled with watery solutions, the 
lymph flows in a continuous stream from the abdominal organs 
but not from the lymphatics of the limbs. After death the sub- 
cutaneous and intermuscular cellular tissues and the central nervous 
system and usually the thoracic organs are as dry as normal, while 
the abdominal organs are markedly oedematous and the peritoneal 
cavity contains fluid. Scalding, toxic agents, temporary anemia, 
by altering the vascular tone and conditions of surface and osmotic 
energy of a limb, render it cedematous when the state of hydraemia 
is produced. An hydremia maintained by daily injection has the 
same effect, so that ligation of the femoral vein, which normally 
does not produce cedema in the animal, in such case does. 

A number of toxic substances are known which have the effect 
of so altering the relations of osmotic and surface energy that 
hydremia causes cedema in the limbs. The cedema which occurs 
in renal disease is probably to be explained on these lines. The 
factors to consider are—a scanty output of urine, particularly of 
solid constituents, retention of salts and other waste products, 
with consequent alteration in osmotic conditions leading to the 
passage of fluid into the tissue spaces; excessive metabolism of 
the muscles resulting from the defect of renal functions ; an effort 
on the part of the tissues to-excrete into the body spaces waste 
substances (this is the normal process in the ascidian) when the 
nephridial tissue fails. Mellanby maintains that the creatin 
found in the muscles is a waste product stored therein with 
advancing life. 

The injection into the blood of a concentrated solution of a 
crystalloid such as sugar produces hydremia. Water is drawn 
from the tissue space into the blood vessels and to such an extent 
that the injection of 45 c.c. of 75 per cent. solution of dextrose may 
temporarily double or treble the volume of blood fluid in a dog, 
as measured by the alteration in number of red cells (Starling). 
The lymph flow which is greatly increased in quantity is ascribed 
to increased capillary pressure and filtration. In confirmation of 
this view Starling found that if he drew off an adequate amount of 


1 If a shrunken tissue cell border on a distended capillary, the surface tension of 


the capillary will be high and its energy low ; while the surface tension of the tissue 
_ ell will be low and its energy high—the one strives to shrink, the other to swell, 


= 


170 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


blood beforehand so as to compensate for the incoming of the 
tissue fluid, no noteworthy increase in the lymph flow occurred. 
Objections to the filtration hypothesis have already been fully 
stated ; it remains to suggest another explanation for Starling’s 
results. The following hypothesis, for the present, may be found 
adequate. 

The injection of the sugar increases the osmotic energy of the 
blood above that of the tissues, and draws water into the vascular 
system. The capillaries are better filled and the surface energy 
of the tissue spaces is increased in proportion as the surface tension 
is increased in the capillaries. Sugar passes through the permeable 
film formed by the capillary wall into the tissue spaces and cells, 
and thus the osmotic pressure of the blood falls. Surface energy 
then gets the upper hand and water passes from the distended 


capillaries into the tissue spaces. This occurs particularly in the 


highly vascularised abdominal organs where the flow of lymph is 
furthered by the respiratory pump. 


HYPERAMIA 


The blood supply of each organ is in relation to its functional 
activity, and thus while half of the whole blood quantum is esti- 
mated to be in the viscera, and the blood they contain may be 
equal to one-fifth of their weight, that within the locomotor organs 
in the resting state of the animal is said to be not more than 
2 to 3 per cent. of their weight. The capillaries of the skin are 
normally far from filled. The effect of an irritant on the vessels 
of the skin or conjunctive and on the mucous membrane of the 
lip demonstrates the striking difference in the capillary supply. 
Hypereemia may be active and arterial in origin, or passive and 
due to venous congestion. Active hyperemia may be caused by 
increased resistance in other vascular fields—the arterial supply 
to the brain is increased by constriction of the splanchnic area— 
or it may result from vaso-dilatation, and this in its turn may be 
produced by the vaso-motor nerves, or by physical or chemical 
agents acting locally, e.g. effect of heat and cupping on the skin, 
pilocarpine on the salivary glands and diuretics on the kidneys. 
It is suggested that the products of secretion cause vaso-dilatation 
in the salivary gland when the chorda is excited, saad as excretory 
products dilate the renal vessel. 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 171 


The arterial wall responds to increased tension by contraction, 

and to diminished tension by expansion, and thus tends auto- 

matically to compensate variations in aortic pressure and keep 

. the blood flow constant. That the vaso-motor centres have no 
share in this is shown by an experiment of Bier. He amputated 
the limb of a pig, leaving it connected with the body by an arterial 
cannula only, and found that an active hyperemia followed a 
temporary arrest of the arterial supply. 

The following is one of the most interesting experiments of Bier. 
He produced venous congestion of the arm by means of a bandage, 
then tightened the bandage so as to obliterate the artery, and 
lastly after five minutes loosened the bandage. The venous con- 

gestion was at once replaced by an active arterial hyperemia. 
He concludes then while arrest of the arterial flow is followed by 
active hyperemia, venosity of the blood causes arterial constriction. 
To confirm this conclusion Bier rendered bloodless the limb of a 
pig for ten minutes by means of an Esmarch bandage, and then 
loosened it. The limb responded by the usual active hyperemia. 
He next closed the windpipe of the animal, and noted that the 
limb first became blue and then paled. On opening the windpipe 
the active hyperemia returned, to be abolished once more on 
closing it. 

One more example of the influence of chemical state of the 
blood on the circulation is the following. Bier exposed the rabbit’s 
intestine in a bath of warm salt solution, and stopped the blood 
supply to two loops of it, one of which was empty and the other 
filled with milk at body temperature. On lessening the ligatures, 
the one filled with milk responded by an active hyperemia, the 
other not. Bier says the intestine of a fasting animal does not 

become hyperemic after a temporary anwmia, while that of a fed 
' one does, an observation which is of some interest to the surgeon. 
The stomach of the rabbit does not give the hyperemic reaction 
while that of the dog does, so that ligation of the chief arterial 
supply of the stomach causes necrosis in the one and not in the 
other animal. Man is like the rabbit in this respect. While 
obliteration of the mesenteric arteries for a length of only 3 cm. 
produces necrosis in the small intestine, 10 cm. is the minimum in 
the colon of the rabbit. 

Bier points out that every organ in functional activity is in a 
____ state of active hyperemia, so too where tissue growth is great as 


172 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


in the regeneration of feathers after moult. The procreative and 
reproductive functions too are accompanied by active hyperemia. 
Hypereemia is the natural reaction of the tissues to injury, foreign 
material and bacterial invasion, and this hyperemia is not to 
be combated, but regarded as the natural healing agent. Anti- 
phlogistic treatment, says Bier, may relieve the pain, but it retards 
the healing process, and may render it less complete. The treat- 
ment of Bier and of A. Wright is to promote the flow of plasma 
into the infected or inflamed tissues. Venous congestion or cupping 
are the methods employed by Bier. 

The knife of the surgeon employed in the opening of ab- 
scesses, &c., has the same influence in relieving tension and allow- 
ing the free transudation of plasma. Wright employs sodium 
citrate solutions as a drug or locally as an irrigant to lessen coagu-. 
lability and thus increase the permeation of the infected part. 
Chronic disease is chronic because the bacterial excitant does not 
provoke an active hyperemia; make this appear and healing 
follows. Animals infected with anthrax and streptococci have been 
protected by the induction of venous congestion in the infected 
part owing to the intensified “antitropic ” action of the plasma. 
The defence lies in the plasma, and hence while blood infection is 
rare local infection is of every-day occurrence. The plasma is 
turned on to the infected local area in full stream, and if nature 
fails to do this, the surgeon must aid the transudation. Salt 
tubbed into wounds of slaves after whipping by osmotic force has 
this effect, while hot fomentations produce hyperemia and soften 
the skin and so allow more transudation. 

Resorption is favoured by the hyperemia which follows venous 
congestion, especially if massage be employed. Potassium ferro- 
cyanide solution, introduced into a joint, appears thirty minutes 
later in the urine. When hyperemia of that joint is induced the 
salt appears in the urine in six to ten minutes. Lactose is a good 
test substance to inject because its presence in the urine can be 
detected by the polarimeter. The resorption of this is doubled or 
more in rate by exposure of the animal to hot air and made very 
slow by packing the limb in ice. When two limbs of an animal, 
with the bones fractured, are splinted, and “one limb surrounded — 
with an air bath at 38° C. and the other at 10° C., a callus forms 
in six to seven days in the limb which is kept warm, while the re- — 
generative process has just begun in the other. Hence the curative 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 173 


effect of poultices and stimulating solutions lies in the hyperemia 
and greater flow of plasma which they produce. The old ideas 
underlying the words “ derivants” and “ revulsants ” are entirely 
wrong. Mustard poultices and turpentine stoups do not draw 
the blood from the deeper diseased parts into the superficial, 
but produce hyperemia of both. The abdominal organs are ren- 
dered hyperemic by exposing the abdomen to a hot air bath. 
The intra-pleural temperature may be raised slightly (owing to 
hyperemia) by applying an irritant to the skin of the chest, and 
may be raised several degrees by applying hot poultices. The 
greater warmth of the part not only induces hyperemia but in- 
creases phagocytic and other actions. Herein we find the explana- 

tion of the success of these old-fashioned remedies in the treatment 
of fevers and inflammation. Bleeding likewise may have been 
effectual by drawing tissue lymph into the blood and so increasing 
the stock of bactericidal and opsonic substances. It is unlikely 
that our forefathers bled fever-patients for nothing excepting 
the fee. 


SHocK AND COLLAPSE 


When a large hutch rabbit is held for a few minutes in the 
vertical position with its limbs stretched out and head uppermost, 
it may become unconscious and die from cerebral anemia. The 
blood collects in the large flaecid abdomen, the animal not being 
able to return it to the heart by changing its posture. It struggles 
to maintain a circulation—cerebral anemia excites convulsions 
which squeeze the blood from the limbs, &c., into the heart—but the 
difficulty in face of the circulation is too great and the animal dies. 
A wild rabbit with taut abdomen is not affected in this way, neither 
is a cat or dog, but a goat is with its capacious belly. The wild 
rabbit can, however, be brought into like state by a dose of chloral, 
and so can the dog by chloroform poisoning or by bleeding. The 
emotional fainting of a man is.due to the inhibition of the nervous 
system—a neutralisation of all other afferent stimuli by one all- 
powerful one—the consequent sudden relaxation of muscular tone, 
collapse of the body and non-return of venous blood to the heart. 
The horizontal postute or compression of the abdomen immediately 
restores from syncope the rabbit or the man. The condition of 
shock which results from division of the spinal cord in the lower 
cervical region is recovered from and cannot be renewed by 


174 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


a subsequent section or total destruction of the spinal cord 
(Sherrington). It is caused by the sudden interruption of the 
nervous tonic influence which proceeds from the brain to the 
muscular system. The incessant inflow of visual and labyrinthine 
impulses no doubt maintain this tone ; just as the wires of a piano- 


tet tat ay tate Et tye yy Yt yy ty testi 


ah 
“ mV!) yin 
; Singita, ded ater dsb Mii bphttots 
if prep MRM HR iit aR MIAN 


x 


s 
5 
% 


Hg 


Fic. 21.—Carotid and superior vena cava pressures’ of dog. FD, animal 
turned into the vertical feet-down posture with the cannule in the axis of 
rotation. The arterial pressure fell in fifty minutes from 110 to 42 mm. Hg. 
From C to EX the animal was immersed in a bath which was deepened to the 
chin at D. Note the increased effect of respiration on the venous pressure 
after FD, and again after the bath. Note the fall of pressures at FD and 
the compensatory rise in arterial pressure, which gradually weakens. 


forte are ceaselessly kept humming by the noise of the world, so 
is the neuro-muscular system. Shock may be caused in man by 
any severe injury, and is generally attributed to paralysis of the 
vaso-motor centre. The centre is said to be exhausted by the 
violence of the sensory stimulation (Crile). Porter objects to this 
view, and says it is disproved by the fact that the centre responds 
to excitation of a sensory nerve in the usual way, and in an animal 


* 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 175 


in a condition of shock from injury raises the arterial pressure from 
its low level by an amount which is no less than in a normal animal. 
The centre, he says, is able to act quite efficiently. The fault 
may lie, however, in the sensory synapses. The centre fails to 
be excited by the usual weak excitation streaming into it, or the 
usual effect may be reversed—pressor turned into depressor reflexes, 
and so the arterial tone flags. 

sayliss has brought forward evidence that there exists a 


Fic. 22.—Inhibition of constrictors in Lovén reflex. Upper curves, volume of 
hind-limb ; lower curves, arterial pressure. First excitation, median nerve ; second, 
sixth lumbar posterior root (Bayliss). 


reciprocal innervation in the case of vaso-motor reflexes as in 
those affecting skeletal muscle. Under normal conditions the 
arterioles are in a state of moderate contraction or tone, which 
may continue even when the vessels are separated from connection 
with the nervous system. Such tone is a normal property of smooth 
muscle, and in the case of the vessels it seems to be kept up by 
the contractile reaction of the arterial wall to the distending force 
of the pulse, as well as, it is thought, by the internal secretion of 
the adrenal glands acting on the sympathetic nerve plexus in the 


176 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


wall (Elliot). This tone can be increased by vaso-constrictor 
and diminished by vaso-dilator impulses. These impulses may 
be continuous, resulting from a state of tonic excitation of the 
respective centres, which is partly of reflex origin and partly de- 
pends on the quality of the blood circulating through the centres, 
e.g. by the percentage of and CQ, in the blood. Bayliss finds 
that in depressor reflexes there 
is, along with inhibition of tone 
in the vaso-constrictor centres, 
an excitation of the vaso- 
dilator centres, and that in 
pressor reflexes along with ex- 
citation of constrictors there 
is under appropriate conditions 

inhibition of dilator tone. 

When an afferent nerve 
from any particular organ is 
excited there is produced along 
with the usual pressor reflex on 
the general blood pressure a 
vaso-dilatation in the organ 
itself. By this means the 
maximal supply of blood is 
sent to an active organ. In 
these local or Lovén reflexes 
as they are called, Bayliss 
finds evidence of both excita- 

_Fic. 23.—F, depressor; R, pressor tion of dilators and inhibition 
afferent nerves affecting arteriole muscle : 
through C.C., constrictor, and D.C., of constrictors. Chloral .and 
dilator centres, as shown by + and - signs ghloroform convert pressor into 
_ (Bayliss). 

depressor reflexes, acting not 

on the afferent neurone, but on some point in the reflex arc, 
probably the synapse. 

It is not possible to cause a great fall of arterial pressure merely 
by severe sensory excitation in an etherised and morphinised 
animal, The pressure falls lower the longer the animal is kept 
aneesthetised on the table exposed to cold, to chloral or chloroform, 
or to operations which expose large surfaces and lead to actual loss 
of blood fluid or to obstruction of venous return, vaso-dilatation, 
congestion and transudation. Opening the abdomen and exerting 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 177 


traction on the intestines is the most certain method of producing 
fall of pressure. The shock produced by severe injuries or operations 
is probably, therefore, of the same nature as collapse produced by 
bleeding. In man after a severe 
injury owing to the shock to the 
sensory synapses, there is loss of 
reflex tone, and relaxation of both 
skeletal and vascular muscle. The 
entire cessation of movement leads 
to the pooling of blood in the peri- 
pheral fields, and at the same time 
the injury may entail considerable 
~ loss of blood fluid. Toxic products 
of altered metabolism probably arise 
in the cooled and stagnant blood 
which secondarily poison the ner- 
vous system. Adrenalin and pitui- 
tary extract by constricting the 
arteries restore the blood pressure, 
the former for a brief, the latter 
for a much longer time (Mummery 
and Symes). 

Chloroform, says Bayliss, con- 
verts pressor into depressor reflexes 
in the rabbit by reversing the usual 
excitation of constrictors into inhibi- 
tion. Strychnine, he says, converts 
the inhibitory phase of all reflexes 
into excitation ; thus the depressor 
nerve produces a rise instead of the 
usual fall under full doses of strych- 
nine. The constrictor centre is ex- yg, 24. , constrictor; D, 
cited by the mechanism which dilator neurones; A, arterial 

: normally inhibits it. The first B, alleeat uaced ai tibery inhaese 
, effect of a small dose in the normal ing bulbar and spinal centres as 
animal is to increase the action of “"°Y™Y + and —signs (Bayliss). 
the vaso-constrictor centre on the viscera and dilate the cutaneous 
vessels. The synapses of the pressor fibres with the constrictor 
centre are the first to show paralysis as the dose is increased. 
Thus the usual pressor effect which follows excitation of a sensory 

- M 


178 THE 


nerve then becomes depressor. 


VASCULAR SYSTEM 


By increasing the cardiac power, 


and the action of the skeletal muscles, those of respiration in 


arterial 
E, after 


on 


cond dose ; 


D, of se 


ffect of excitation of anterior crural nerve 


4 


E 
t of first dose of strychnine ; 


). 


A, under ether; B, after chloral; C, effec 
Bayliss 


Fic. 25.—Antagonism of chloral and strychnine. 


strychnine 


pressure, 


sound and rational. method of 
are to fill the heart, increase 


signs of recovery. 


particular, strychnine may act 
favourably. 

Those who, like Crile, main- 
tain that strychnine is useless 
as a drug in shock can argue 
that the constrictor synapses are 
easily paralysed in this condition 
by the drug, and thus it may 
accentuate rather than alleviate 
the fall of arterial pressure, for 
it will then excite the vaso- 
dilators which supply all parts, 
and the limbs in particular. The 
cutaneous dilatation will favour 
the loss of body heat. Bandaging 
the limbs and particularly the 
abdomen partly restores the 
arterial pressure and _ cerebral 
circulation in a rabbit, which 
has been fixed in the vertical 
position, as it does in the dog 
chloroformed or collapsed from 
loss of blood. Crile has intro- 
duced a rubber pneumatic suit 
for confining the body in states 
of shock. An equable air pres- 
sure is kept up so that the 
arterial pressure measured with 
the sphygmometer is restored. 
Flaps are arranged in the dress 
which can be removed for the 
surgeon to operate. The pres- 
sure of the air can be reduced 
gradually as the patient shows 
This seems a 
treatment. The objects in view 
the resistance in the arterioles, 


and compress the capillary-venous areas of the limbs and trunk 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 179 


so that the cerebral, coronary, and pulmonary vessels are well 

Direct transfusion of human blood from artery to vein has been 
used by Crile in extreme cases of simple shock. It is unwise to 
use this in any state of toxemia, for hemalysins may then 
come into play. Injection of physiological saline is found a 
valuable aid. 

In collapse brought about by the Sr ae of bacterial toxins, 
it is said, the synapses of the vaso-motor centre are at fault, so 
that neither excitation of a sensory nerve nor asphyxia provoke 
any rise of arterial pressure, while compression of the abdomen or 
of the descending aorta provokes it, showing that the heart is not 
exhausted and can still respond with increase of energy to better 
filling, and increased resistance to its output (Romberg). 


Rabbit : 0°2 gm. Chloral intra-venously injected. 


| 


Excitant. Arterial Pressure. 

Mm. Hg. 

eae 565 

Sciatic faradised 1280 
Abdomen massaged 147-0 
Aorta compressed . 153-5 
Asphyxia : 139°5 
Sus 815 


Splanchnic nerves divided. 


ses | 340 

Sciatic faradised 34°5 
Abdomen 119°0 
Aorta compressed . 1320 
Asphyxia 27°5 
eee 33°0 

Chloral injected again. 

ds _ 20-0 

. Abdomen massaged . | 66:0 
Aorta compressed . 52-0 


23°0 


These figures show the relative effect of paralysis of vaso-motor 
nerves and of damaging the heart ka chloral, and with them the 


following may be contrasted : _ 


i 


180 THE VASCULAR SYSTEM 


Rabbit ; 1:2 c.m. Diphtheria Toxin injected. 
Collapse after Diphtheria Toxin. 


Excitant. Aortic Pressure. 

Mm. Hg. 

ie 39°0 

Sciatic faradised . a 47°0 
Abdomen compressed. 81:0 
Asphyxia ° “6 310 

Last stage of collapse 

Ms 175 

Abdomen compressed. 62-0 
Asphyxia : : : 18-0 


Mr. H. P. Dean informs the writer that in spinal anesthesia 
produced by Stovain (injected into the lumbar part of the spinal 
canal), the sensory anesthesia spreads upwards to a higher level 
than the motor paralysis, and the medullary centres are unaffected 
or almost so when the anesthesia has spread even to the head. 
The sensory synapses are the first to fail. The blood pressure 
falls some 20 mm. Hg only, showing that the visceral vaso- 
constrictor nerves are maintained in good state. To sum up 
then, the condition of shock or collapse is associated with cessation 
of the reflexes which maintain the body in a state of vascular 
tone and muscular activity. Hence the stagnation of the blood, 
fall of blood pressure, and loss of body heat. 


BIBLIOGRAPHY 


Carrel, Journ. Exper. Med., ix., p. 226, 1907. 
MacWilliam, Proc. Roy. Soc., lxx., p. 109, 1902. 
Lister, Brit. Med. Journ., 1899, 1, p. 924. 

W. Russell, Arterial Hypertonus, Sclerosis and Blood Pressure, 1907. 
G. Oliver, Studies in Blood Pressure, 1908. 

Roy and Adami, The Practitioner, xlv., p. 32. 

Howell and Brush, Bost. Med. Surg. Journ., cxiv., 146, 1901, 

C. J. Martin, Brit. Med. Journ., 1905, 1, p. 870. 

Erlanger, Amer. Journ. of Physiol., x., 1904. 

McCay, Lancet, 1907, i., p. 1484. 

Dahlgren and Kepner, The Principles of Histology, Macmillan, 1908. 
Thoma, Textbook of General Pathology, Trans. A. Bruce, 1896, 


AND BLOOD PRESSURE 181 


y 
Roy and Brown, Journ. of Physiol., 2, p. 323, 1879-80. 
V. Recklinghausen, Arch. f. Exp. Path. in Pharm., 55, 375, 1906. 
Lewy, Arch. f. d. ges. Physiol., 65, 447, 1897. 
Stewart, Manual of Physiology, 1895, p. 59. 
Leonard Hill, The Phys. and Path. of the Cerebral Circulation, London, 
1896. Cerebral Anewmia, Trans. Roy. Soc., Journ. of Physiol., xxvii., 
p- 337, 1901; and xxviii, p. 122, 1902. Article Circulation, Schifer’s 
Textbook of Physiology, vol. ii., 1900. 
T. Henderson, Ophthal. Soc. Trans., xxviii., 1908. The Ophthalmoscope, 
s Oct. 1908. 
| 7’. Lewis, Journ. of Physiol., xxxviii., p. 240, 1908. 
Tigerstedt, Ergeb. der Physiologie, vi., p. 269. 
E. Weber, Arch. f. Physiol., 1907, p. 300. 
Mosso, Die Furcht, 1889. 
Bainbridge, Practitioner, lxxv., p. 633, 1905. 
Sollmann, Amer. Journ, of Physiol., 1905, 13, p. 241. 
Bolton, Journ, Path. and Bact., 1903, p. 67. 
Stewart, Guthrie, dc., Journ. Exper. Med., viii., 289, 1906 ; x., 371-490, 
1908. 
Cohnheim, Vorlesungen iiber Allgem. Pathologie, 1882. 
Worm-Miiller, Transf. u. Plethora, Christiania, 1875. 
R. du Bois Raymond, Brodie, and F. Miiller, Arch. f. Physiol., 1907, 
Suppl. Bd., p. 37. 
Starling, Schifer’s Physiology, 1, 285, 1898. 
Bier, Hyperamie als Heilmittel, 1903, Leipzig. 
A. Wright, The Practitioner, Ixxx., p. 600, 1908. 
Sherrington, The Integrative Action of the Nervous System, 1906. 
Porter and Quinby, Amer. Journ. of Physiol., xx., p. 500, 1907-8. 
Bayliss, Proc. Roy. Soc., 80, p.339, 1908. 
Elliott, Journ. of Physiol., 32, 401, 1905. 
Crile, Blood Pressure in Surgery, 1903. 
Mummery and Symes, Brit. Med. Journ., Sept. 19, 1908. 
Romberg, Piissler, &c., Deutsch. Arch, f. Klin. Med., Bd. 64, p. 652, 1899 ; 
Bd. 77, p. 96, 1903. 


For a general list of recent literature consult Heinz. Handb. der exper. 
P: Path, u. Pharm., vol. 2, p. 283. 


THE MECHANISM OF RESPIRATION IN MAN 


By ARTHUR KEITH. 


THE following is a summary of the chief points dealt with in this 
article :— 

(1) The lung is composed of elements of varying degrees of 
extensibility ; hence the expansion of its parts are unequal 
during inspiration. 

(2) The infundibula or air sacs are the essential distensible — 
(inspiratory) elements of the lungs. The distensibility of any part 
of the lung will depend on the number and size of the infundibula 
in that part. 

(3) The bronchial musculature regulates the tension of the 
infundibular air and may regulate the distribution of air and 
blood throughout the lung. 

(4) The lungs do not expand equally in all directions, but 
execute a movement in certain definite directions during in- 
spiration. 

(5) The roots of the lungs are not fixed but undergo a res- 
piratory movement. 

(6) The great fissure of the lung is of functional significance. 
The upper lobe is chiefly expanded by a mechanism formed by 
the upper ribs, the lower lobe by a compound mechanism formed 
by the diaphragm and lower ribs. 

(7) Expiration is controlled by muscular action. 

(8) The extensibility and elasticity of the thorax are factors 
in producing expansion or compression of the lung only when 
the ribs pass into extreme inspiratory or expiratory positions. 

(9) Tue first pair of ribs and the manubrium sterni are parts 
of a single mechanism which may be described as the thoracic 
operculum. The sterno-manubrial joint is of functional im- 


. portance. 


(10) In observing and analysing the respiratory mowseiiaitl 
of the thorax it is advantageous to treat the costal cartilages 
aa) 


\ 
\ 
\ . 
\ 
\ 


RESPIRATION IN MAN 183 


and their musculature as an individual part of the respiratory 
mechanism. 

(11) The diaphragm acts as a true piston, moving the ab:lominal 
contents downwards and forwards, the direction depending on the 
type of respiration. The effect of its contraction depends on the 
action of its antagonists. 

, (12) That in describing the movements of the ribs it is neces- 
4 sary to recognise at least two types, one representative of the 
upper costal mechanism and the other of the lower or diaphrag- 
matic mechanism. The movements of the lower set are correlated 
with the action of the diaphragm; those of the upper set work 
independently of the diaphragm. 
(13) The floating ribs (eleventh, twelfth, and often the tenth) are 
functionally parts of the abdominal wall. 
(14) The articulations, muscles, movements, and conforma- 
tion of the ribs of the lower set differ widely from those of the 
. upper set. 

(15) The action of the intercostal muscles depends on the 
antagonists brought into use. Some intercostal spaces are 
widened and some diminished during inspiration, and the same 
is true during expiration. ; 

(16) The levatores costarum have no action on the ribs; they 
are purely spinal muscles. 


In this article the writer proposes to summarise certain recent 
papers dealing with the respiratory expansion and contraction of 
the human lungs. The evidence contained in these papers is 
derived from three sources: (1) From measurements of the res- 
piratory movements of the body wall by photography, by X-rays, 
by recording tambours, by direct measurements, or by tracings 

’ taken with strips of lead moulded on the body. When possible the 
| writer has selected those observations which deal with the normal 
| unconscious respiratory movements rather than records made on 
--—s conscious subjects taking exaggerated breaths. (2) From observa- 
tions on the anatomy of parts concerned in respiratory Move- 
| ments, for whatever theory be adopted of these movements, it 
must give a rational and complete explanation of the form and 
arrangement of the structures concerned in them. (3) From clinical 
observations made by means of percussion and auscultation. When 
the evidence from these three sources is summarised it is found 


184 THE MECHANISM OF 


to necessitate a considerable alteration in our current teaching 
of the mechanism of respiration. 


THe EXTENSIBILITY AND ELasticiry oF THE LuNG 


It is usually presumed that the lungs are equally extensile 
throughout, but an examination of their structure shows that this 
cannot beso. From an anatomical point of view the lung may be 
divided into three zones: (1) A root zone containing the bronchus, 
artery, and vein, and their main divisions, with lymphatic glands 
and vessels, and much fibrous tissue, all structures offering great 
resistance to a distending force. (2) An intermediate zone in which 
vascular and bronchial ramifications radiate towards the surface 
of the lung with pulmonary tissue implanted between the rays. 
It is a zone containing structures of varying degrees of extensi- 
bility, the veins being the least extensile and the pulmonary tissue 
the most. (3) An outer zone, estimated roughly at 25 to 30 mm. 
in depth, which expands much more freely and equally than the 
intermediate zone. Perhaps the subpleural stratum of the outer 
zone should be distinguished, for if a lung, which has been re- 
moved from the body, be gradually inflated, it will be found that 
the subpleural stratum is at first elevated at certain points into 
plateaux about 2 mm. above the surface of the lung, and from 
these elevated points the process of distension of the subpleural 
stratum spreads out in all directions. This at first appeared to 
be due to a more complete collapse of the subpleural air sacs, but 
microscopic sections show that in the collapsed condition these 
sacs are still as large as the deeper, so it may be concluded that, 
when distended, they are larger than the deeper sacs, and collapse 
to a greater degree. Seeing that the lung is intersected with 
radiating bronchio-vascular rays, of a much lower degree of 
extensibility than the pulmonary tissue between them, one must 
suppose that these rays during the inspiratory expansion of the 
lung must move apart so as to permit the pulmonary tissue lying 
between them to expand. A consideration of the anatomy of the — 
lung and of its movement during inspiration shows that the 
expansion of the lung is not the simple dilatation it was believed 
to be; its expansion is a regulated act resembling more the 
opening of a Japanese fan than the distension of a simple elastic 
sac. Among recent writers, Tendeloo is the only one to emphasise 


RESPIRATION IN MAN 185 


the varying degree of extensibility of the structures of the lung. 
In all mammalian lungs the veins and arteries occupy a definite 
and constant relationship to the bronchial ramifications—a relation- 
ship which must have a functional significance. Amongst the 
various means the writer employed to estimate the extensibility of 
| the several parts and structures of the lung was that of marking the 
| surface of the partly inflated organ with points placed at regular 
intervals, and then measuring the distances bctween these points 
when the lung was more fully inflated. The parts of the lung 
which were found to expand most were the central areas of the 
costal and of the diaphragmatic surfaces. The method was. 
abandoned because it was found that the expansion obtained by 
inflating the lung did not correspond to the expansion of the lung 
by means which imitated the normal action of the thoracic walls. 
Hutchinson discovered long ago that the form assumed by the 
thorax when the lungs are inflated after death, differs altogether 
from the inspiratory position of the thorax. His observation, 
however, has been forgotten, and models of lungs so inflated are at 
present the only ones on the market. Several anatomical papers 
have been published recently giving the inspiratory position of the 
apices of the lungs, that position being determined by artificial 
inflation of the lung. In the dead body the lung, when inflated, - 
expands in the direction of least resistance. 


On THE FUNCTION AND NATURE OF THE INFUNDIBULA 


Closely related to the extensibility of the various elements of the 
lung is the question of the function and nature of the infundibula. 
In the most valuable work recently published by Oppel the use of 
the term infundibulum is condemned. He prefers to follow Miller 
; in distinguishing the following terminal air spaces in the lung :— 
| (1) The terminal bronchiole, with its sphincter-like arrange- 
ment of musculature ; (2) the vestibule; (3) the atrium; (4) the 
air sacs; (5) the alveoli implanted on the walls of the air sacs. 
) Such an elaborate nomenclature obscures the functional nature 
; of the final pulmonary elements, for while the terminal bronchus 
is one functional element, and the alveoli another, the vestibule, 
the atrium, and air sacs combined form but one element, namely, 
the essential distensible air spaces of the lung (bellows part), and 
it is well to retain the term infundibulum to designate the com- 


186 THE MECHANISM OF 


plex space in which a terminal bronchiole ends. In the frog’s 
lung the central space represents the infundibular element of the 
mammalian lung; in reptiles it is represented by the thin walled 
posterior part of the lung; in birds it is represented by the air 
sacs ;*in the mammalian lung the distensible elément is scattered 
throughout the lung, whereas in all other vertebrate it forms a 
separate part. If out of a rubber balloon a model of the terminal 
bronchiole infundibulum and alveoli be made and inflated, it is 
the central or infundibular space which expands most, the alveoli 
implanted on its walls being widened but at the same time ren- 
-dered more shallow. The point which one seeks to emphasise 
is that it is not the alveoli but the infundibula that should be 
regarded as the essential expansile parts of the lung; the larger 
and more plentiful the infundibula in a part of the lung, the more 


readily will that part respond to any distending force. Oppel © 


gives the diameters of the infundibula in the apical part of the 
lung as ‘12 mm. at the third year and -45 mm. at the seventieth ; 
in the basal part of the lung as ‘38 mm. at the third year and -85 
mm. at the seventieth. They are largest in the subpleural zone 
and smallest in the root zone. One infers, therefore, that, when a 
breath is taken, the base expands more readily and to a greater 
extent than the apical part, and the subpleural more than the part 
at the root. When emphysema occurs it is the infundibula which 
first become hyperdistended, and the parts most liable to emphysema 
are those in which the infundibula have normally the greatest size. 


THE BRONCHIAL MUSCULATURE 


The older physiologists regarded the bronchial musculature as 
expiratory in function; even now most medical writers ascribe 
the reflex contraction of the lung (Abram’s reflex) which follows 
any stimulation of the chest wall to the action of:the bronchial 
musculature. It is more probable that the retraction of the lung 
is due to a reflex contraction of the musculature of the body wall. 
The action of the musculature of the bronchioles in regulating 
the tension of the air within the infundibula has not received the 


attention it deserves. The more the calibre of the bronchiole is — 


diminished during inspiration the greater must be the negative 
tension within the infundibula, a tension which must act on and 
distend the capillaries and various blood spaces embodied in the 


a 


RESPIRATION IN MAN 187 


walls of the infundibula. The bronchial musculature by diminish- 
ing or increasing the access to the infundibula in various parts 
of the lung may regulate the distribution of the indrawn air 
throughout the lung. By regulating the intra-alveolar pressure it 
may also influence the distribution of blood throughout the lung, 
and take the part of the vaso-motor mechanism which has not 
been proved to exist in the lung. 


Tue NEcEssIty OF RECOGNISING SuRFACES OF Direct EXPANSION 
AND SURFACES OF INDIRECT EXPANSION ON THE LUNG 


The lung is usually regarded not only as being equally dis- 
tensible in all its parts, but also of expanding equally in all 
directions during inspiration. This is far from being true of the 
mammalian lung. Of the five areas which one may distinguish 
on the surface of the human lung, three are in contact with 
stationary parts of the thoracic wall, and therefore cannot be 
directly expanded. These three pulmonary surfaces are—(1) The 
mediastinal, in contact with the pericardium and structures of the 
mediastinum ; (2) the dorsal surface, in contact with the spinal 
column and with the spinal segments of the ribs—those parts of 
the ribs to which the erector spine is attached; (3) the apical - 
_ surface, the pulmonary area lying in contact with Sibson’s fascia 
at the root of the neck. It is not strictly true to say that these 
three parts of the pleural wall are stationary, for the heart being 
conical in shape, with its base resting on the diaphragm, it is clear 
that the mediastinal surface of the lungs may expand inwards 
with the inspiratory descent of the heart; only the dorsal part 
of the apical surface is stationary, for the ventral or anterior part 
is elevated with the first rib, but the fact remains that during 

4 inspiration the apical resonance does not extend into the neck 
) but decreases (Colbeck), and by placing a tambour on the neck 
over the apex of the lung it is found that the apex descends when- 
ever the diaphragm is well in action, even if the first rib remains 
| stationary. The two surfaces of the lung which are directly~ex- 
| panded are the diaphragmatic and ventro-lateral or sterno-costal. 
Thus if the diaphragm and lower ribs are in action the whole lung 
is expanded in a downward and forward direction, the apical 
surface remaining stationary or descending until the structures 
of the neck become sufficiently tense to resist a further descent. 


188 THE MECHANISM OF 


The negative tension, in such an inspiration, falls first and most 
in the basal part of the lung; hence one finds very frequently a 
horizontal groove (Harrison’s sulcus) on each side of the thorax 
corresponding to the level of the domes of the diaphragm and 
to the zone of greatest negative pressure in those who have had 
the respiratory passage obstructed. Meltzer found the degree of 
negative pressure within the thorax of rabbits increased as the 
diaphragm was approached ; it is least along the stationary walls 
of the thorax. It is thus apparent that a certain degree of the 
expanding force transmitted by the thorax to the surfaces of direct 
expansion is lost as it passes through the lung to the surfaces of 
indirect expansion. Further, the extent to which any part of the 
lung is expanded depends on the distensibility of that part, being 
greatest in the superficial zone of the lung and least in the root 
zone. That the expansion of the lung does not take place instan- 
taneously and equally throughout all its parts is well substantiated 
by clinical observation. Huggard and Clive Riviere observed 
independently that if percussion and auscultation of the lung 
were carried out before a deep breath were taken, the apices of 
the lungs, especially in those who were regarded as being the 
subjects of a phthisical tendency, were less in action than the 
rest of the lungs; Lloyd Jones records that when a breath is 
taken the resonance of the anterior part of the apex of the lung 
increases much more than the posterior; Gerhardt noticed that 
when a pleural effusion was being absorbed the apex of the lung 
was the last part to regain its normal resonance. It is a well- 
known clinical fact that a localised consolidation is surrounded 
by an area of increased resonance, showing that the diminution 
of one part of the lung is not followed by an expansion of the 
whole lung but only of the part immediately adjacent. When it 
is remembered that those who lead sedentary lives use their lungs 
to only 10 per cent. or even less of the full pulmonary capacity, 
the importance of recognising that a thoracic movement only 
affects that part of the lung directly subject to the movement will 
become apparent. In those who have contracted a lazy habit of 
body it is possible for the parts of the lung which are the most 


remote from the surfaces of direct expansion and at the same — 


time of a low degree of distensibility, to pass into a condition of 
partial or almost complete disuse; such a part is that region of 
the apex where phthisis so frequently commences. 


RESPIRATION IN MAN 189 


RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS OF THE Roots OF THE LUNGS 


The root of the lung has hitherto been regarded as the most 
fixed part of the lung, the part from which the expansion of the 
lung takes place. Were this so it is manifest that those parts of 


x Verteb. Col.------- i a tar aeons 
Apex ; 


Root (inspir. — 


Pericard----@ 


lower Bord. lexp. : 
Lower Bord. (inspir, ce 


Crus (inspir) 


Crus (expir) 


D 


Fic. 1,—Mediastinal aspect of the right lung to show the respiratory movement 
of the root. The crus of the diaphragm is also indicated, and its attachment to the 
root of the lung through the pericardium. The arrows indicate the direction of the 
inspiratory movement of the various parts of the lung. 


the lung which lie between the root and the stationary walls of 
the thorax could undergo no expansion. The truth is that during 
a complete inspiration the whole lung, root included, undergoes 


a 


a definite movement. In the normal mixed inspiration, where 
diaphragm and ribs co-operate equally or almost equally, the 
lung expands in three directions—downwards, forwards, and out- 
wards, the root sharing in the combined moyements. When the 
abdominal breathing is well marked, the trachea can be felt de- 
scending in the neck as the epigastrium comes forwards. By the 
use of X-rays the heart, and therefore the roots of the lungs, for 
the heart is bound firmly to them, can be seen to follow the move- 
ments of the chest wall; with a thoracic breath, the heart follows 
the movement of the sternum; with a diaphragmatic breath, it 
descends with the diaphragm. The great muscular crura of the 
diaphragm, forming one-third of that muscle, can act directly on 
the roots of the lung through the pericardium and heart. In cases 
where the roots of the lungs are bound to the posterior or stationary 
wall of the thorax through adhesions set up by mediastinitis, © 
Wenckebach observed that both the respiratory and circulatory 
movements were abnormal in character. 


190 THE MECHANISM OF 


THE FUNCTIONAL SIGNIFICANCE OF THE: DIVISION OF THE 
Lunes Into LoBEs 


Tt is usually said that the division of the lungs into lobes has 
no functional significance. This opinion is founded on the fact 
that they may be only partially developed or completely oblite- 
rated by disease without altering the functional capacity of the 
lung. The obliteration of the pleural cavity by adhesions has so 
little apparent effect on the respiratory movements that their 
presence cannot be detected during life. In one case, where 
the lungs were completely adherent, Hutchinson found the vital 
capacity to be 680 cc. above the average amount. This method 
of reasoning is liable to lead one into great error, for there are 
many functional organs in the body which may be removed with- 
out any marked disturbance of the bodily economy. When the 
normal respiratory movements of the lung are fully understood 
it will be found that the great fissure, which divides the upper 
from the lower lobe, is functional in its significance. The upper 
lobe is normally expanded by one mechanism, the lower by 
another. When the lung is removed from the body and slightly 
inflated there will be seen marked out on the lateral and anterior 
aspects of the upper lobe—especially in the lungs of women— 4 


ale 


RESPIRATION IN MAN 191 


- the impressions of the first, second, third, fourth, and fifth ribs 


and costal cartilages. These impressions are marked in two ways : 


the part of the lung lying under the rib is less pigmented and is 


grooved ; the zones corresponding to the intercostal spaces are 
more pigmented and are elevated above the level of the costal 
zones. To obtain such results, for these marks cannot be post- 
mortem effects, the relationship of the upper lobes to the upper 
ribs must have been stationary during life; there could have 
been no gliding of the lung across the ribs and spaces during 
inspiration and expiration. But on the lower lobe, except for 
an occasional impression of the seventh costal cartilage, at the 
anterior angle, these costal impressions are absent; the pigment 
is evenly distributed, or if not, does not correspond to spaces. The 
inference one draws is that the lower lobe glides beneath the ribs 
during the respiratory movements ; there is not, as in the upper 
lobe, a constant relationship between ribs and spaces. But it 
must be noted, too, that the dorsal surface of the upper lobe does 
not show these costal impressions; here, too, there must be a 
downward and upward movement, one which I had inferred to 
take place before my attention was drawn to the costal markings 
as a guide to the respiratory movements of the lung. When 
dealing with the movements and mechanism of the ribs it will 
become apparent that the lower lobe and the dorsal part of the 
upper lobe are chiefly expanded by a diaphragmatic mechanism, 
and the upper lobe by the upper five ribs. It must not be sup- 
posed that these markings are to be found on the lungs of every 
individual; they are constant in the lungs of women, and their 
frequent absence on men’s lungs can be understood when one re- 
members how many there are that obtain their chief inspiratory 
expansion by a moderate use of the diaphragmatic mechanism 
alone. Pleuritic sounds and pleuritic pains are most intense 
over the lower lobe; Fowler and Pasteur have recorded cases 
of paralysis of the diaphragm where the collapse was confined to 
the lower lobe. The upper lobe is always relatively larger in 
women than in men, a result to be expected from their manner 
of breathing. It is true that pleuritic friction can frequently be 
detected over the upper lobe; this fact must certainly be taken 
into consideration, but it must be remembered that a localised 
pleurisy has a powerful reflex influence on the respiratory muscu- 


lature corresponding to that part, and it is therefore probable that 


192 THE MECHANISM OF " 4 


there is a grave disturbance of the normal respiratory movements 
in such cases. 


THE ACTION OF THE RESPIRATORY MUSCULATURE IN 
NorMAL EXPIRATION 


A consideration of the evidence now at our disposal makes it 
difficult to believe that normal expiration is merely the result of 
an elastic recoil. The recoil is evidently under muscular control. 
The antagonist of the diaphragm is the musculature of the belly 
wall; it is difficult to believe that the replacement of the dia- 
phragm and the abdominal viscera at the end of an inspiration 
is merely an elastic recoil of the abdominal musculature. One 
cannot deduce the elasticity of a living muscle from the study of 
the dead, but I may put on record here the following observation. — 
A piece of the human rectus abdominis 135 mm. long stretched 
to 180 mm. when a weight of 10 kilos was attached to it; on 
removing the weight it had retracted in five minutes to 150 mm. 
It is improbable that the muscles which are attached to the ribs 
are carried by the ribs as passive burdens during expiration. The 
reflex co-ordination which Sherrington has shown to exist between 
so many antagonistic groups of muscles, probably holds true also 
for the inspiratory and expiratory groups of muscles. Duchenne 
found that the abdominal musculature was brought into action 
when the phrenic nerve is stimulated. Spina and Mislawsky 
observed that stimulation of the apparent fibres in the phrenic 
had an inhibitory action on the abdominal musculature. Baglioni 
obtained an expiratory movement of the body wall when the dia- 
phragm itself was stimulated. At least one can feel the upper part 
of the rectus abdominis harden under the fingers during expiration, 
as if it were inaction. The observation of Treves, the Italian physio- 
logist, also points to expiration being a muscular act. He found 
that if an inspiration is arrested, it is followed by an expiration of 
normal amount. Mosso observed that the respiratory movement 
became thoracic in type when the horizontal position was 
assumed, a result which cannot be ascribed to an alteration in 
the elastic recoil of the lung. Mosso concluded that the assump- — 
tion of the thoracic type of respiration in the supine position is | 
brought about through a reflex action of the vagus, but in the 
writer’s opinion the change is due to an alteration in the 


° 


“a 
) 
4 
} 
j 


RESPIRATION IN MAN 193 


action of the expiratory muscles. The erect posture neces- 
sitates an increased action of the musculature of the belly wall 
in order. to balance the body, the ribs being thus more firmly 
fixed than in the supine position; with the assumption of the 
supine position the musculature of the belly wall is relieved of 
its postural function, thus lessening the strain on the ribs and 
allowing them to move more freely. If expiration were an elastic 
recoil uncontrolled by muscular action one would expect the thorax 
to diminish simultaneously in all its parts; this is not so, for ex- 
piration like inspiration is a definite movement commencing in the 
upper or lower part of the thorax and spreading gradually to the 
rest. Those who regard normal expiration as a result of the elastic 


recoil of the lungs cite as conclusive evidence of their opinion 


those cases of fracture of the spine or section of the spinal cord 
where the diaphragm is the sole respiratory muscle in action. In 
such cases the writer has observed that the patient lies on the 
back or turned somewhat on the left side; the epigastric move- 
ments are greater than normal; pressure of the hand applied to 
the epigastrium gives immediate distress and completely alters the 
type of respiratory movement, the lower ribs being then raised 
and the lower part of the chest expanded. Further, the writer 
observed that the diaphragm in such cases is kept in a condition 
of over-action; in one case the diaphragm worked some 25 to | 
30 mm. below its normal level, thus compressing the abdominal 
contents and obtaining a recoil from the weight and tension 
of the abdominal viscera. To what extent the tonus of the 
abdominal and thoracic musculature is lost in these cases has 
not been determined. Mosso has shown that tone varies indepen- 
dently of the contractibility, and that the abdominal musculature 
possess a high degree of tone. The parietal layer of the peritoneum 
is highly extensile and elastic to preserve its smoothness in the 
various states of movement. 


Tue RESPIRATORY VALUE OF THE ELASTICITY OF ~~ 
THE THORAX 


The degree of elasticity of the living thorax has to be esti- 
mated from experiments made on the dead. We now know, from 


observations made by the use of X-rays, that the diaphragm is 


N 


194 THE MECHANISM OF “s 


in a position of ultra-expiration after death, and we infer that if 
. the elasticity of the thorax is in antagonism to the elasticity of 

the lungs at the end of expiration, then the elastic recoil of the 

thorax in the dead is an exaggeration of that in the living. Sir 

Douglas Powell made observations on ten subjects; he noted the 

expansion of the thorax when the pleural cavity was punctured 

and the lungs allowed to collapse. In six of these there was no 

expansion of the thorax; in four the average expansion forwards 
of the chest wall was 2°3 mm. When it is remembered that the 

lateral or antero-posterior thoracic expansion varies from 1 to 2 

mm. in normal respiration, even in men with the thoracic type of 

breathing, it is evident that the elasticity of the thorax can play 

but a slight part in normal expiration. Any observation made on 

the dead body is vitiated by the rigor mortis and post-mortem 

changes of the muscles. I observed that when the muscles are 

removed from the thorax that the weight of the ribs and sternum 

was enough to cause the thorax to assume an expiratory position 

when the body was turned in the feet-down position, and to pass 

into the inspiratory position when the body was inverted. One 

must conclude from a study of the thorax of the dead that the rib 

movements are so free that elastic recoil of the thorax comes to 

be a factor in expiration and inspiration only towards the extreme | 
limits of respiratory movements. 

Recently the writer had reason to compare the elasticity and col- 
lapsibility of the thorax in living subjects with that of dead subjects. 
The matter has some importance in determining the best method 
for performing artificial respiration on the apparently drowned, © 
and also in selecting the position of patients in performing intra- 
thoracic operations. Elsberg, for instance, asserts that the pleura 
may be opened without collapse of the lung if the patient be 
placed in the prone position ; it is certainly possible to reduce the 
capacity of the thorax in adult human beings to a point when it 
is too small to contain the collapsed lungs, and hence a part is 
extruded as a hernia. The writer’s investigations show that the 
living and dead thorax react quite differently when compressed, 
the difference bemg due, in his opinion, to the reflex action of the 
respiratory musculature. The following table gives the results of 
his experiments ; the measurements for the living are the average 
for ten students varying from eighteen to twenty-four years 
of age; those given for the dead are the averages from five 


RESPIRATION IN MAN 195 


4 dissecting-room subjects varying in age from fifty to seventy- 
two years, in whom the compressibility of the thorax is expected 
to be greatly diminished. 


Results of Compression of the Thorax. Living. Dead, 
1. The front-to-back diameter of the thorax was 
] diminished in turning from the supine to 
the prone position , ; . ‘ . 174mm. 21°4 mm. 


2. The side-to-side diameter in the same experi- 
ment increased. ‘ ; ‘ ; - 221mm. 135 mn. 


3. The front-to-back diameter decreased on 
placing 10 kilos. on the thorax (subject 
supine) . ° P ‘ , ‘ z . 23°7mm, 29°6 mm. 


4. The transverse diameter increased in the same 
experiment . ° ° : : : . 56mm. 135 mm. 


5. The front-to-back diameter of the thorax de- 
creased on placing 10 kilos. on the thorax 
(subject prone). ‘ : , , - 128mm. 46mm. 


6. The transverse diameter increased in the same 
experiment . . : . ; ; - 38Omm. 8°6 mm. 


These experiments show that the compressibility of the thorax is — 
largely modified by the action of muscles during life, and that in 
the prone position a very great degree of the natural elasticity 
of the thorax is lost. 

Freund, whose observations are receiving active attention in 
Germany, after many years of neglect, is of opinion that the 
elastic torsion which occurs in the cartilage of the first rib during 

inspiration is one of the chief active forces in producing an 
7 expiratory recoil of the thorax ; he also regards emphysema as a 
result of loss of elasticity of the costal cartilages. His opinion 
concerning the expiratory recoil of the cartilage of the first rib 
will be referred to in dealing with the sterno-manubrial joint; as 
regards the loss of elasticity of the cartilages causing emphysema, 
it may be said that surgeons have put Freund’s belief into practice 
by section of the costal cartilages in such cases, but the results 
of such experiments have not yet been determined. Certainly 
calcification of the costal cartilages frequently occurs without 
emphysema. 


196 THE MECHANISM OF 


THe First Ris, MANUBRIO-STERNAL JOINT, AND THEIR 
RESPIRATORY SIGNIFICANCE 


The first rib has always been treated as merely one of the 
costal series. Its articulation to the spine, its ligaments, its 
muscles, its shape, its costal cartilage, its intimate union with 
the manubrium sterni, differ so markedly from the corresponding 
features of other ribs, that were only the anatomical evidence avail- 
able, one would conclude that it differed from all the others in 
its respiratory function. An examination of its movements and 
of the part it plays in expanding the lung shows this is so. The 
first pair of ribs and the manubrium sterni are bound intimately 


Manub.(exp.)—- ca sy 


AW OY 
\ Ags 


Fic. 2.—Diagram to show the respiratory movements of the first pair of ribs and 
manubrium sterni, and the effect of these movements on the expansion of the apex 
of the lung. : 


together by the broad and short first pair of costal cartilages, 
and form with the manubrium, a united structure which may be 
described as the lid or operculum of the thorax. Behind, this 
lid is articulated to the spinal column by a joint which is set more 
transversely and is wider in the extent of its attachment than 
any other of the costal arcs; in front the lid is articulated with 
the body of the sternum at the manubrio-sternal joint. The 
manubrio-sternal joint must be counted amongst the important 
respiratory joints. Anchylosis of this joint is rarely seen before 
the fiftieth year, and it is uncommon before the sixtieth. The 
respiratory movement which occurs at it varies with the indi- 
vidual, with the type of respiration, being greatest in. those with 


——— 


RESPIRATION IN MAN 197 


the thoracic type, and with the extent of the respiratory move- 
ment. Braune estimated its movement at 5° to 13°, Rothschild, 
who regards limitation in the movement of this joint as a cause 
of consumption, estimated the average movement (in full inspira- 
tion) to be 15°85° in the male and 12°85° in the female ; while the 
writer, who was unaware of Rothschild’s observations, found it 
varied from 1° to 16°. The degree of movement depends chiefly 
on the inspiratory behaviour of the body of the sternum, which 
is extremely variable. In some individuals the lower end of the 
sternum during the elevation of the thorax during inspiration may 
be drawn towards the spine or move forward to a less degree than 

the upper end of the body of the sternum; in such, the sterno- 
- manubrial movement is free. If, on the other hand, the lower 
end of the sternum moves more freely away from the spine than 
the upper end, the movement at the joint is less extensive. At 
the sterno-manubrial joint, the operculum or lid of the thorax 
articulates with the anterior thoracic wall. The prominence of this 
joint on the surface of the thorax is extremely variable—so many 
conditions may render it unduly prominent. Ludwig, the Parisian 
physician, is said to have regarded an undue prominence of the 
articulation (Angulus Ludovici) as an indication of phthisis ; but 
researches made by recent German writers have failed to trace - 
such a statement in Ludwig’s writings. Rothschild found that 
the sterno-manubrial movements were limited or absent in 
phthisical subjects, and ascribed the susceptibility of the apex to 
phthisis as due to an anchylosis or limitation of movement at this 
joint. In his opinion a free manubrio-sternal movement is neces- 
sary if the apex of the lung is to be properly expanded. Freund 
attributes the incomplete expansion of the apex in the phthisical 
action to a congenital shortening and ossification of the cartilage 
of the first rib. It is true that the necks of the first pair of ribs 
are so articulated to the spine that with the elevation of the 
manubrium sterni there is some degree of torsion of their cartilages, 
but the amount of the torsion is slight in extent because of the 
particularly loose manner in which the heads of those ribs are — 
bound to the first dorsal vertebra. The writer, unaware of the 
observations and theories of Freund and Rothschild, had concluded 
that the ossification of the first costal cartilage and limitation of 
the sterno-manubrial movements were consequences rather than 
_ causes of a limited expansion of the apices of the lungs. In 


198 THE MECHANISM OF | 


dealing with the significance of the division of the lungs into lobes, 
the direct influence of the diaphragm on the apices of the lungs 
has been already pointed out. The upward movement of the 
first pair of ribs and manubrium expand chiefly the anterior or 
ventro-lateral part of the apex of the lung; the movement has 
but an indirect influence on the dorsal part of the apex, espe- 
cially that part lying in front of the necks of the first and second 
pair of ribs. It is the dorsal part of the apex of the lung that 
is the common initial site of pulmonary tuberculosis. To secure 
a free expansion of that area of the lung a full diaphragmatic con- 
traction is much more effective than any movement of the upper 
thorax. 


THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS OF THE CosTAL CARTILAGES 


In man, and it is true of nearly all mammals, the costal carti- 
lages progressively increase in length and in their declivity to the 
sternum as one passes from the first downwards. The following are 
the lengths of the cartilages in millimetres of a well-built man 
from the first to the ninth ribs :—first, 25; second, 37; third, 50; 
fourth, 62; fifth, 75; sixth, 87; seventh, 112; eighth, 137; 
ninth, 160. Although the eighth and ninth cartilages do not 
directly reach the sternum, yet for functional purposes they really 
do through their close union with the seventh costal cartilage, 
and hence their length is estimated as if they did reach the 
sternum. The costal cartilage of the first rib descends to reach 
the sternum; that of the second normally lies horizontal and joins 
the sternum at a right angle; the third ascends to the sternum, 
and the degree of ascent becomes greater with each succeeding 
cartilage until, at the lower end of the sternum, the seventh pair 
of costal cartilages form together the subcostal angle which varies 
from 45° to 90°, 65° being a common size in well-formed adults. 
In observing the respiratory movements of any individual it is well 
to regard the costal cartilages as a separate part of the respiratory 
mechanism ; they have their own muscles ; during inspiration the 
interchondral muscles (anterior part of the internal intercostals) 
and the anterior digitations of the diaphragm raise them into a 
more horizontal position, increasing the transverse diameter of the 
thorax and the size of the subcostal angle. The cartilages are 
depressed and the subcostal angle decreased by the upper part of 


RESPIRATION IN MAN 199 


the transversalis and triangularis sterni; the rectus abdominis 


also depresses the cartilages, but its action, owing to its peculiar 


insertion, is really that of a depressor of the whole thorax. 
Measurements of the respiratory movements of the subcostal 
angle are useful in distinguishing between’ an inspiratory move- 
ment effected by an upward movement of the thorax as a whole, 
and one in which the upward movement is accompanied by a 
tilting upwards of the outer ends of the cartilages. For instance, 
during a forced inspiration, in three individuals, selected at random, 
the increase at the subcostal angle was 6°, 9°, and 30°. Unfor- 
tunately nearly all the observations published on the movements 
of this angle relate to forced inspiration ; in quiet inspiration the 


increase is about 1:5° for those with an abdominal type of respira- 


tion, and about half that amount in those with a thoracic type. 
The writer, from observations made by the aid of X-rays, has 
come to regard a free movement of the subcostal angle as an index 
of a free action of the diaphragm. 


Tue AcTION AND MOVEMENTS OF THE DIAPHRAGM 


In recent years anatomists have had an opportunity, thanks 
to the discovery of Rontgen, of correcting their inferences as to 
the action of the various parts of the diaphragm by direct obser- — 
vation on the living. When tested in this way the elaborate 
deductions of Hasse as to the internal respiratory movements have 
proved to be remarkably near the truth. He inferred that the 
central or pericardial part of the diaphragm must also participate 
in all respiratory movements, that the diaphragmatic movement 
as a whole must be in a forward as well as in a downward direc- 
tion, and that all diaphragmatic movements were accompanied 
by a definite movement of the abdominal viscera. The writer 
has shown that the diaphragm is made up of two parts which are 
different in origin, different in their nerve supply, and different in 
their action. These two parts are the spinal or crural part, the 
fibres of which arise from the spinal column and arcuate ligaments, 
and ascend to be inserted into the posterior or concave margin of 
the central aponeurosis; these fibres are normally from 125 to 
150 mm. long, and weigh about 60 grammes. The other part of 
the diaphragm—the sterno-costal—weighs about 96 gramines ; its 


several digitations vary in length, that from the ninth costal car- 


200 THE MECHANISM OF 


tilage being longest; the fibres pass backwards as they ascend. 
Thus while the spinal segment of the diaphragm tends to elongate 
the thorax in a vertical direction, the sterno-costal part pulls 
forwards and downwards the abdominal viscera, increasing the 
back-to-front diameter in the lower part of the thorax. The 
resultant movement of the diaphragm is one in a downward and 
forward direction ; the more the thorax is elevated the greater is 
the forward visceral movement; the more the downward move- 
ment, the more are the abdominal viscera depressed. The writer 
has observed in patients who were described clinically as neuras- 
thenic, that the spinal part of the diaphragm may act forcibly 
while the sterno-costal part is almost passive. Duchenne showed 
conclusively that if descent of the abdominal viscera is restrained 


the diaphragm spent its force in elevating the thorax. Dally | 


observed, and the observation has been frequently verified, that 
the curvature of the domes of the diaphragm is scarcely altered 
during even a profound inspiration. We have come to see that 
the diaphragm, rendered semi-solid by the abdominal viscera and 
having its circumferential zone kept constantly applied to the 
inner wall of the lower part of the thorax by the negative intra- 
thoracic pressure, acts as a true piston—a piston moving in a 
downward and forward direction, the lung expanding into the 
space it vacates. The action of the diaphragm depends on which 
group of muscles comes into play as its antagonists. If the 
abdominal contents are rendered fixed by the abdominal muscu- 
lature, the lower margin of the thorax moves towards the domes 
of the diaphragm ; if, on the other hand, the ribs are fixed by the 
intercostal muscles, and the abdominal musculature is reflexly 
relaxed, the domes of the diaphragm move towards the lower 
aperture of the thorax. In subjects of extreme visceroptosis 
Wenckebach observed that the diaphragm was thrown out of 
action by its visceral fulcrum being lost, and breathing was carried 
on by an elevation of the upper part of the thorax. Seeing how 
variable the action of the diaphragm is in the same individual, 
and how much it differs in its action from individual to individual, 
it is rather misleading to make any precise statement of the ampli- 
tude of the movements of its domes. In quiet breathing Dally 
found, by the use of the orthodiascope, that the mean descent 
of the right dome in 100 individuals was 12°5 mm.; the left 
dome, 12 mm.; the central part rather less than the left dome. 


RESPIRATION IN MAN 201 


Hultkranz estimated the average inspiratory descent of the whole 
diaphragm at 10°5, Guillemot at 15 to 18mm. About half an inch 
is its ordinary descent, the right dome moving rather more than 
the left, and the left more than the central part. From Hutchin- 
son’s, Dally’s, and my own observations the area of the diaphragm 
in contact with the lungs may be estimated at 250 sq. cm. A 
descent of 10 mm. all over gives an increase of 250 c.c. of thoracic 
space ; if one estimates an average quiet breath at 400 c.c. it will 
be seen that the diaphragmatic movement plays a larger part 
than the costal movement. Hultkranz estimated that in taking 
a breath of 490 c.c. 170 was the result of the diaphragmatic, and 
320 of the thoracic movement. Sewall and Pollard found that a 

larger breath could be taken by a thoracic inspiratory movement 
than by an abdominal one. R. T. Mackenzie has again verified 
Hutchinson’s observation that there is no relationship between 
the extent to which the chest can be expanded in circumference 
and the amount of breath that is thereby taken in. The thorax 
can be enlarged so that the abdominal viscera instead of air is 
drawn into the thoracic cavity. At the London Hospital there 
was an old acrobat who could draw in this way all his abdominal 
viscera up into the thorax, so that the abdominal aorta was felt 

_ pulsating under the fingers from the epigastrium downwards. - 
When the thorax is expanded beyond the extensibility of the lungs 
the abdominal viscera are drawn upwards to occupy the thoracic 
space; highly trained gymnasts at the army schools invariably 
obtain their great chest expansions in this way. . 


a 


THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS OF THE RIBS 


In describing the respiratory movements of the thorax most 
teachers restrict their description to a typical rib, in order to 
secure simplicity of description. Two movements are recognised, 
one round an axis corresponding to the spinal articulation of the 

_ rib, and another round the spino-sternal articulation. The first 
movement gives increase of the back-to-front diameter, the other 
of the side-to-side. Now, although in a general sense this de- 
scription is essentially true, yet when one comes to study the 
costal movements in the living and the arrangement of parts in 
the dead, it is found to afford a very imperfect explanation of 
_what is seen. The ribs vary in size, shape, inclination, articula- 


7 


tion, and action from the first to the twelfth; to describe the 
action and movement of each would cause the student to be lost 
in detail. It is enough, in the writer’s opinion, to-recognise that 
in the thorax there are two parts which are functionally inde- 
pendent ; these are (1) the upper mechanism connected with the 
expansion of the upper lobe of the lung (the part above the great 
fissure), consisting of the second, third, fourth, and fifth ribs, and 
their attached muscles; and (2) the lower mechanism, consisting 
of the sixth, seventh, eighth, ninth, and tenth ribs, and their 
attached muscles, designed for the expansion of the lower lobe. 
In this lower mechanism the diaphragm is the dominating part. 
The lower set of ribs is specially adapted to act with the diaphragm ; 
they are an intrinsic part of the diaphragmatic mechanism. The 
floating ribs, the eleventh and twelfth, and in 40 per cent. of bodies 
the tenth also belongs to this group, are, from a functional point — 
of view, essentially a part of the abdominal wall; their articula- 
tions and movements are peculiar. The peculiar shape and action 
of the first rib has already been described. In the writer’s opinion 
it is necessary, for a full analysis of the respiratory movements 
in health and in disease, to distinguish four parts in the costal 
series—(1) The first rib, part of the thoracic operculum ; (2) the 
upper costal series (second to fifth); (3) the lower costal | 
series (sixth to tenth), an intrinsic part of the diaphragmatic 
mechanism; (4) the floating series, functionally a part of the 
_ abdominal wall.. 


902 THE MECHANISM OF 


THe RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS OF THE LOWER CosTAL SERIES 


The muscles and movements of the lower series are totally 
different from those of the upper. The dominating muscle is the 
diaphragm ; the other muscles are so arranged in their attach- 
ments as to act either as synergic muscles (the ilio-costalis, the 
external intercostals, the interchondrals) or as antagonists (the 
external oblique, the internal oblique, the internal intercostals, 
and the transversalis). The ribs of the lower series are articulated _ 
to the spinal column in such a manner that the lateral and anterior 
part of each moves outwards more than the one above it during 
inspiration. The spinal part of each (50 to 70 mm. in length)— 
the part to which the erector spine is attached—rotates and 
actually moves forwards (sternal-wards) during inspiration. The — 


4 
* 


RESPIRATION IN MAN 203 


tubercle of the rib glides downwards and forwards on the flat 
upper facets on the transverse processes. The axis of the move- 
ment in the lower set does not correspond to the neck of the rib ; 
it corresponds to its spinal segment and is determined by the action 
of the muscles rather than by the shape of the articulations and 
ligaments. Anteriorly the diaphragmatic set of ribs is inserted 
to the lower end of the sternum by the strong peculiar complex 
formed by the costal cartilages of the sixth, seventh, eighth, and 
ninth ribs. As the diaphragm and external intercostals elevate 
the lower costal series, they also raise the sternum upwards through 
the cartilage complex. The ilio-costalis, rising from the iliac crest, 
steadies the spinal segments of the ribs, or during an energetic 


inspiration actually draws them downwards. The result of a 


movement of the lower ribs—or better, of the action of the 
diaphragmatic mechanism—is to increase the transverse and back- 
to-front diameter of the lower thorax and the vertical diameter of 
the whole cavity. 


- RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS OF THE FLoatiInG Riss 


As already mentioned, the eleventh and twelfth ribs are 
functionally parts of the belly wall. Sibson observed that the 
tenth and eleventh intercostal spaces widen during inspiration, 
and diminish during expiration, an observation which the writer 
has frequently verified. The opposite is true of the other spaces. 
The twelfth rib is not only controlled by the quadratus lumborum 
and erector spine muscles, but also by a strong and constant 
ligamentous membrane, the function of which has been entirely 
overlooked. This membrane, really an extension of the middle 
layer of the lumbar fascia, anchors the lower border of the twelfth 
rib to the transverse processes of the first and second lumbar ver- 
tebree ; it is continued up between the spinal parts of the lower 
five ribs. Thus the lower five ribs at and near their angles are 


_ bound to the spinal column in such a way as to strictly limit the 


upward movement of the spinal segments of the ribs. On the 
other hand, the lateral and anterior parts of the ribs are not so 
limited ; the whole arrangement is designed to secure the move- 
ments of the lower ribs round an axis, not corresponding to their 


necks, but to their spinal segments, the parts to sso the erector 
aa is attached. 


™ 


204 THE MECHANISM OF 


RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS OF THE Upper RIBS 


The upper ribs differ from the lower set (1) in their muscu- 
lature ; (2) in their articulation and ligaments; (3) in their shape 
and arrangement; (4) in their movements. Their movement is 
designed for the expansion of that part of the lung which lies 
above the great fissure. The musculature of these ribs is the inter- 
costal and interchondral. The first rib and its muscles provide a 
fulcrum towards which the upper set of ribs may be raised, while 
the lower set of ribs, fixed by the abdominal musculature, affords 
a fixed base towards which they may be depressed during expira- 
tion. Now the spaces between the ribs of the upper set are 
peculiarly wide on the anterior and lateral aspect of the chest— 
the area covered by the pectoral muscles and the upper part of 
the serratus magnus. It is in these spaces that most observations 
on the action of the intercostal muscles have been made. Duchenne 
noted that during life the musculature of these spaces was tense 
during inspiration and lax during expiration in the living, and 
inferred that both external and internal intercostals were in action 
during inspiration, and out of action during expiration. That was 
also Haller’s opinion. Rutherford and many others also drew 
that inference from experiments on the articulated thorax. Sibson, 
who was an accurate observer, also agreed that the internal inter- 
costals of the subpectoral region of the chest are inspiratory in 
function. Both internal and external muscles have the power 
of diminishing the intercostal spaces, and may therefore act as 
inspiratory or expiratory muscles according to whether they act 
from the first rib or from the sixth. One must receive with 
caution the inferences drawn from experiments which involve a 
wide disturbance of the circulatory and reflex mechanisms—such 
as those of Martin and Hartwell. They found the interchondral 
as well asthe internal intercostal muscles were expiratory, a con- 
clusion altogether at variance with the fact that these cartilages 
are elevated during inspiration. In other parts of the thorax, 
with the exception of the anterior and lateral aspect of the upper 
part of the thorax, the internal intercostals are normally a 
muscles. 

The spinal articulations of the ribs of the upper set differ from 
those of the lower set. The articulation on the tubercle is a con- 


ris 


RESPIRATION IN MAN 205 


vex ovoid facet, and fits into a corresponding hollow facet on the 
transverse process. unlike the flat facets of the lower rib. Each 
transverse process from above downwards is tilted a little more 
backwards than the one above, so that the angle at which the ribs 
are set to the spine increases as one passes down the series. The 
double articulation of each rib to the spine by its head and by its 
tubercle prevents any rotation of the rib round a sterno-spinal axis. 
There can be no “ bucket-handle”” movement. The axis on which 
the upper ribs move corresponds to their necks. The only part of 
the erector spine which can influence their movement is the acces- 
sorius. The series of slips included in this muscle rises from the 
lower set and is inserted into the upper set of ribs. It is 
physically possible for this muscle to act on the upper set during 
expiration. The muscles named levatores costarum increase in 
size from first to twelfth, being thus largest to the rib which is in 
least need of elevation ; they are so inserted to the ribs as to be 
unable to influence the movements of the ribs; they are not con- 
cerned in respiration but in lateral movements of the spine. 

One can see from the shape of the upper ribs that their 
mechanism is totally different to the lower set. The ribs of the 
upper set have a concave upper margin, the lower convex; the 
upper ribs are shaped like the blade of a sickle; each successive 
rib from second to fifth making a greater lateral and forward 
sweep than the rib above it. The ribs of the lower set form a 
vertical series, the one situated vertically above the other. The 
upper set is designed for the expansion of the conical upper lobe, 
the lower set for the expansion of the lower lobe which is a segment 
of a cylinder. 

Recently Dally has again directed attention to extension of 
the spine as a normal means of expanding the thorax. He has 
studied the spinal movements by means of the othodiascope. 
Hutchinson and Hasse have each demonstrated a normal in- 
spiratory extension of the spine. Extension of the spine causes 
an increase in all three diameters of the thorax. 

It would carry this article to an undue length were the writer 
to deal fully with other matters connected with the mechanism 
of respiration, such as its influence on the circulation (see Wencke- 

bach and T. Lewis); with the changes which set in soon after 
adult life and gradually increase until.old age (see Mehnert). He 
feels he has scarcely done justice in his review to the articles of 


~~ 
* 


206 THE MECHANISM OF 


du Bois Raymond, Boruttau, and Hart, the last named having 
compiled a most useful summary of German literature treating of 
the relationship of phthisis to the mechanism of respiration. 


BIBLIOGRAPHY 


Abrams, Lancet, Oct. 10, 1903, p. 1052. 

Du Bois Reymond, Mechanik der Atmung. Ergebnesse der Physiol. Bd.1 
1902, Abth. Bio-physik., p. 277. 

H. Boruttau, Die Atembegwegungen und ihre Innervation. Handbuch 
der Physiol. der Menschen, Nagel, Braunschweig, 1905, Bd. 1, Abth. 1, 
pp. 1-56. 

Braune, Der Sternelwinkel, Angulus Ludovici, in Anatomische und Klin- 
ische Beziehung. Archiv. fur Anat. und Physiol, Anat. Abth. 1888, p. 306. 

Colbeck and Pritchard, An Explanation of the Vulnerability of the Apices 
in Tuberculosis of the Lungs. Lancet, June 8, 1901. :; 

&. H. Colbeck, The Phenomena of Tidal Percussion at the Apices of the 
Lungs. Practitioner, March 1903. 

J. F. H. Dally, A Contribution to the Study of the Mechanism of Respira- 
tion. Proc. Roy. Soc., Feb. 6, 1908 ; Lancet, June 27, 1903, p. 1802. 

Duchenne, Physiologie des Movements, Paris, 1867. 

Elsberg, Zentralbl. fur Chirur., No. 10, 1908. 

W. A. Freund, Der Zusammenhang gewisses Lungen Krankenheiten mit 
Primaren Rippen Knorpelanomalien Erlanger, 1859, p. 127. (For references 
to Freund’s later papers see Hart.) 

K. Gregor, Die Entwickelung der Atemmechanik in Kindesalter. Anat. 
Anz., Bd. xxii., p. 119, 1902. 

C. Hart, Die Mechanische Disposition der Lungen spitzen zur tuberku- 
losen Phthise, Stuttgart, 1906, p. 267. 

C. Hasse, Die Formen des menschlichen Korpers und die Wort viccalae 
rungen bei der Atmung, Jena, 1888—90, parts i—ii. 

C. Hasse, Ueber die Atembemegungen des Menschlichen Korpers. Arch. 
f, Anat. u. Physiologie, 1901 and 1903. 

W. R. Huggard, Brit. Med. Journ., Oct. 14, 1905. 

J. Hutchinson, article on “‘ Thorax.” Todd’s Cyclopedia of Anatomy and 
Physiology, vol. iv., 1852. 

E. Lloyd Jones, The Physical Examination of the Upper Regions of the 
Chest. Brit. Med. Journ., Oct. 24, 1903. 

A, Keith, A Variation which occurs in the Manubrium Sterni of Higher 
Primates. Journ. Anat. and Phsyiol., vol. xxx., 1896, 

A. Keith, A Contribution to the Mechanism of Respirationin Man, Proe- 
Anat. Soc. of Gréat Brit. and Ir., May 1903. 

A. Keith, Why does Phthisis attack the Apex of the Lung? London | 
Hospital Gazette, Nov. 1903. 

A. Keith, The Nature and Anatomy of Enteroptosis. eee Marchi a 
1903, p. 634. 


—- 


* 


RESPIRATION IN MAN 207 


_ &, T. Mackenzie, The Relationship of the Thoracic Type to Lung Capacity. 
- Montreal Medical Journ., vol. xxxiii., 1904, p. 237. 

_ Martin and Hartwell, On the Action of the Intercostal Muscles. Journ. 
of Physiol., vol. ii., 1879, p. 24. 

EE, Mehnert, Ueber topographische Altersveranderungen des Atmungs- 
apparates, Jena, 1901. 

Meltzer, The Respiratory Changes of the Intrathoracic Pressure. Journ. 
of Physiol., vol. xiii., 1892. 

H, von Meyer, Der Mechanismus der Rippen. Archiv. fur Anat. und 
Entwickel, Leipzic, 1885, p. 253. 

A. Moso, Action des centres spinaun sur la Toncite des Muscles Respi- 
rateurs, Archiv. Ital. de Biol., t. xli., 1904, p. 111; also t. vii. p. 93. 

A. Mosso, Le Movements ‘yeapiratoires du Thorax et du diaphragme. 
Arch. Ital. de Biol., t. xl., 1903, p. 43. 

_ A. Oppel, Lehrbuch der mikroscopischen Anatomie der Wirbeltiere: part 
vi., Atmungs Appar., Jena, 1905, 

W. Pasteur, Massive Collapse of the Lung. Lancet, Nov. 7, 1508, p. 1351. 

Sir R. Douglas Powell, On some Effects of Lung Elasticity in Health and 
Disease. Medico-Chir. Soc., vol. lix., 1876, p. 165. 

A. Ransome, Observations in the Movement of the Chest. Journ. of Anat 
and|Physiol., vol. iv., 1870, p. 140. 

Clive Riviere, Lancet, June 8, 1907, p. 1603. 

Rothschild, Ueber die physiologische und pathologische Bedeutung des 
Sternelwinkels, XVII. Kongr. f. inn. Med., Karlsbad, 1899. (Hart gives full 
references to his later papers.) 

Rutherford, Note on the Action of the Intercostal Muscles. Journ. of Anat. 
and Physiol., vol. x., 1876, p. 608. 

Sewall aud Pollard, On the Relationship of Diaphragmatic and Costal 
Respiration to Phonation. Journ. of-Physiol., vol. xi., 1890, p. 159. 

F. Sibson, On the Mechanism of lespiration. Philosophical Trans., vol. 
exxxvi., 1846, p. 501. 

N. Ph. Tendeloo, Studien ueber die Ursachen der Lungen Krankenheiten, 
Wiesbaden, 1902, p. 480. 

K. F. Wenckebach, Ueber Pathologische Beziehungen Zwischen Atmung 
und Kneislauf beim Menschen. “Sammlung klinischer Vortrage, Neue Folge, 
Leipzig, 1907. 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


By M. 8. PEMBREY 


CHAPTER I 
INTRODUCTION 


ALTHOUGH muscular work hes always been one of the most im- 
portant factors in every-day life, it has not received sufficient 
attention from medical men. It has been the subject of much 


writing, numerous casual observations, but few scientific investi- 


gations. The practical importance of a knowledge of the influence 
of muscular work was never greater than it is at the present time. 
The migration from the land to the towns has deprived the youth 
of this country of many of the opportunities for healthy muscular 
exercise which their forefathers possessed. The increasing use 
of machinery in all occupations has reduced the demand for 
healthy muscular labour, and has thrown an extra strain upon the 
nervous system of the working man. The much-vaunted advances 
of medicine and surgery have prevented oftentimes the beneficent 
action of the law of the survival of the fit, and a morbid sentimen- 
tality under the guise of charity has pampered the degenerates of the 
country. The bad must be taken with the good, but this necessity 
does not make any thinking man satisfied with the conditions of 
modern civilisation. Its defects are well recognised, and for this 
reason the outlook is hopeful. The fear of a physical deterioration of 
the race found expression recently in the appointment of a depart- 
mental committee of inquiry, and serves at the present time as a 
strong argument in the hands of the advocates of compulsory 
military service. If degeneration is to be prevented, the public 
must recognise more fully the vanity of luxury and the benefits 
of healthy muscular exercise, whether it be as work or play, and 


must insist that the population is not weakened by immoral efforts _ 


to diminish the birth-rate and a healthy struggle for existence. 
Physiologists have often neglected the question of muscular 


* work or have devoted a vast amount of time and ingenuity to 
208 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK — 209 


experiments upon the isolated muscles of frogs. This has been 
reflected in the teaching and training of medical students. The 
underlying idea of the supporters of the school of muscle and 
nerve appears to be or to have been that a thorough investigation 
of the properties of two tissues would solve the problem of vital 
activity. The mystery is greater than ever it was. The day of 
that school of physiology is passing. A reaction set in some 
years ago, has not waned, but has steadily gained in strength. 
. Some extremists would maintain that the investigation of isolated 
muscles and nerves is strictly not physiology ; that a muscle and 
nerve under such abnormal conditions is pathological and patho- 
logical to a degree which does not obtain under the ordinary 
- conditions of life. It cannot, however, be denied that such ex- 
perimental work has its value, for all knowledge is useful if it be 
properly appraised. Muscle and nerve are not units of life; the 
unit is the living organism. Muscular activity under natural 
conditions is exhibited only by the organism, and under such 
conditions it should be studied. 

Athletes and the trainers of men and animals for sport have 
done much for the practical study of the physiology of muscular 
work. The practice of training is even at the present time ahead 
of the theory, and each year physiological investigations show the 
value of the methods introduced by athletes. Experience has 
been the guide of athletes, and experience is the result of nume- 
rous physiological experiments upon a large number of men. The » 
diversity and gradation of the muscular exercise involved in the 
numerous forms of sport render it suitable for all men, young and 
old, strong and weak. In these and many other respects sport is 
far superior to physical drill and gymnastic exercises, and as such 

. deserves even more recognition and study than it already receives. 
: The main purpose of this article is the physiology of muscular 
work in man. It will be necessary, however, to glance at some 
of the elementary facts concerning the structure and properties 
of muscle considered as a tissue. 


Tue Structure oF Muscie 


There are many interesting differences in the naked eye and 
microscopical structure of muscle, and these must be considered in 
relation to the various functions of the different kinds of muscular 
te) 


210 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


tissue. In muscle the power of contraction, which is present in 
the primitive cell, has been especially developed in the process of 
division of labour and differentiation of structure. Three kinds 
of muscle are recognised—voluntary, cardiac, and involuntary. 
Their minute structure, which has given rise to much con- 
troversy, is strictly a question of anatomy, and will not be 
discussed here. There are not only differences in the structure, 
composition, and properties of voluntary muscles in different 
animals, but also in the same animal. The best known example 
is the red and the white muscle of the rabbit (+). Simple inspec- 
tion of the muscles of a rabbit directly its skin is removed de- 
tects a marked contrast in the colour of the different muscles ; 
the masseters and some of the muscles of the hind limbs, such as 
the soleus and semi-membranosus, are red in colour, but most of 
them are pale. This difference is not due solely to variations in - 
the vascular supply, for even after the blood has been removed a 
contrast remains. The red muscle fibre contains hemoglobin and 
myohematin within its substance, and these pigments are pro- 
bably of some importance in the process of internal respiration. 
The capillary blood vessels have dilatations, which are not pre- 
sent in the case of the pale muscles. The red fibres are thin with 
nuclei in their substance as well as under the sarcolemma, and the 
transverse striation is less regular. Functional differences can be 
easily demonstrated. The red muscle contracts and relaxes slowly, 
in marked contrast to the rapid wave of contraction in white 
muscle ; red muscle is more easily tetanised, and does not pass into 
rigor mortis so rapidly. 


THE PHYSICAL AND CHEMICAL PROPERTIES OF MUSCLE 


One of the most important characteristics of muscle is its 
elasticity. A muscle fibre regains its original length after it has 
been stretched within certain limits; it possesses perfect elas- 
ticity and conforms to Hooke’s law; the successive increments in 
length produced by equal increments of weight are equal. It is 
only when the muscle has been extended beyond the limits to 
which it is exposed in the living body that its perfect elasticity 
is impaired (*). ie 

In the normal condition muscles are stretched between their 
points of attachment and by the action of antagonistic muscles ; 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK  2l1 


if a muscle be cut across its ends retract in the wound. The 
practical importance of this elasticity is found in the more prompt 
performance of work; the muscles are taut and have not to take in 
slack when they begin to contract. The extensibility of a muscle 
is greater during contraction than in the condition of rest. This 
is a further safeguard to the muscle in any sudden or vigorous 
contraction against a great resistance; the shock and strain are 
lessened and rupture of the muscle is prevented. It is more 
common to find bones fractured than muscles ruptured by violent 
contraction. Later it will be shown that in addition to this elas- 
ticity the muscles have a condition of tone or tonic contraction, 
which increases their efficiency in the performance of work. 

Muscle consists of 25 per cent. of solids and 75 per cent. of 
water ; twenty parts of the solids are proteins, and the remaining 
five parts are extractives and inorganic salts. From the muscle 
can be expressed a viscid alkaline juice which soon becomes 
acid and clots. The chief constituents of this muscle plasma 
are the proteins investigated and named by Halliburton (*) para- 
myosinogen and myosinogen; they correspond respectively to 
the myosin and myogen of Von Fiirth(*). These proteins in most 
respects resemble the globulins; para-myosinogen is coagulated 
by heat at 47°, myosinogen at 63°. The myosinogen gives rise to 
soluble myosin in the process of clotting, and this substance, which ~ 
is coagulated by heat at 40°, is a normal constituent of the muscle 
plasma of cold-blooded animals. 

The statements made concerning the proteins of muscle plasma 
may soon need revision, for in a recent preliminary communication 
Mellanby (°) has maintained that there is only one protein in muscle 
and that it is not a globulin. 

A comparison of the protein constituents of the different kinds 
of muscle brings out an interesting difference. Nucleo-protein is 
most abundant in plain muscle, and least abundant in volun- 
tary or striated muscle ; cardiac muscle occupies an intermediate 
position in this respect. In simple cells nucleo-protein is a typical 
constituent ; plain muscle is the least and voluntary muscle the 
most differentiated of the three kinds of muscle; thus changes 
in function and structure have been accompanied by a corre- 
sponding gradation in the amount of nucleo-protein. 

Another point of great interest has been discovered by a com- 
_ parison of the Peeaperwiares at which the different protein con- 


212 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK | 


stituents coagulate (*). When an excised voluntary muscle of a 
mammal is heated, it begins to shorten at 43°, shortens more at 
47°, and again when the temperature reaches 58°. The excita- 
bility of the muscle is destroyed when the shortening occurs at 
47°, and this corresponds with the temperature at which para- 
myosinogen is coagulated. The internal temperature of a healthy 
mammal is about 36° to 37°, and it is well known that life is 
endangered when owing to fever or some other cause the tem- 
perature of the body rises to 44°. Birds are also warm-blooded 
animals, but their internal temperature is several degrees above 
that of mammals; the temperature of a sparrow is 42°, that of a 
hen 41° to 43°. There must therefore be some difference in the 
heat-rigor of the proteins of their muscles; experiments on this 


point have proved that the coagulation of the protein does not 


occur until the temperature is raised to 53°. 

The temperatures at which coagulation begins are not rigidly 
fixed, for prolonged heating at a lower temperature will produce 
that change of state; it is necessary also to remember that the 
excised muscles are devoid of a circulation of blood, and no doubt 
are in a condition which is not the same chemically as the normal 
muscle. These criticisms do not invalidate the practical importance 
of heat-rigor in connection with the effects of hyperpyrexia and 
the pathological changes which occur in muscle during prolonged 
fever. It is necessary, however, to exercise care in any argument 
from the part to the whole body. 

The next constituents of muscle which must be considered are 
the extractives ; these can be divided into two classes, the non- 
nitrogenous and the nitrogenous. The former group is repre- 
sented by glycogen, dextrin and sugars, inosite, fat, and lactic acid. 

Glycogen or animal starch (C,H,,0,), is a polysaccharide which 
appears to be intimately associated with the source of muscular 
energy. The amount of glycogen which can be extracted from a 
muscle varies according to its activity, and this fact will explain 
the lack of agreement in the percentages found by different ob- 
servers. Resting muscle contains from 0°1 to 2°5 per cent. of 
glycogen. The amount not only varies in different animals, but 
also in different muscles of the same animal: the more active 
muscles contain less. By prolonged activity the glycogen. of the 
muscles can be removed, but it disappears from the liver first ; 
poisonous doses of strychnine which cause violent convulsions. 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 213 


bring about a loss of about 90 per cent. of the glycogen of the 

muscle. During starvation glycogen disappears from the liver 

sooner than from the muscles. These facts raise the question 

whether glycogen is supplied to the muscles by the blood-stream 

which has taken up glycogen from the liver or whether it can be 
formed by the activity of the muscles themselves. There is no 
conclusive evidence to show that the latter is the case, but it will 
be better to defer the consideration of this point until the sources 
of muscular energy are discussed in detail. 

Dextrin and Sugars.—Between these bodies and glycogen there 
is a close relationship. Glycogen appears to be the form in which 
carbohydrate is stored up, probably in loose combination with 

_ the proteins of the muscle ; dextrin, maltose, and glucose represent 
the stages through which the reserve material passes on its way 
to yield energy during combustion. The glycogen of an excised 
muscle rapidly decreases and the sugar at the same time increases. 
This conversion can be effected by the ferments which are found 
in muscle; there is an amylolytic enzyme and a maltase, and by 
their. action dextrin, maltose, and finally dextrose are formed. 
The relation of sugar to muscular activity will be considered 
later. 

Inosite is found in small quantities in the muscles. It was 
formerly known as muscle sugar, owing to the fact that it has the 
same molecular formula (C,H,;0,) as glucose. It is not, however, 
a sugar, but a crystallisable substance belonging to the aromatic 
series. Nothing appears to be known of its physiological im- 
portance, although it is found in other parts of the body besides 
muscle. 

| Fat is constantly present between the muscle fibres, but it is 

| uncertain whether it is a normal constituent of muscle; there is 
no definite evidence of its presence within the substance of the 
fibre. Leathes (*) found that the red muscles of the rabbit contain 
considerably more fat than the white muscles. 

Lactic acid, paralactic or sarcolactic acid, CH,(CH.OH)COOH. 
There has been a great conflict of opinion upon the question whether 

the normal muscle contains lactic acid, which is always present 

in dead muscle. Recently, however, the discrepancies have been 
explained by Hopkins and Fletcher (*) in an important research 
upon lactic acid in amphibian muscle ; by this work the knowledge 

_ of the conditions under which lactic acid is formed in muscle has 


9 « 


214 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


been greatly extended. Their results are given in the following 
summary taken from their paper :— 

‘Freshly excised resting muscle is found to yield very small 
quantities of lactic acid, and these small amounts are possibly not 
mote than can be accounted for by the unavoidable minimum of 
manipulation prior to extraction. 

‘« A large increase of the yield of lactic acid is found as the result 
of mechanical injury, of heating, and of chemical irritation. 

‘ Lactic acid is spontaneously developed under anaérobic con- 
ditions in excised muscles. During the survival periods of sub- 
sisting irritability, and not after, equal increments of acid arise 
in equal times. After complete loss of irritability the lactic acid 
yield remains stationary. 

“ Fatigue due to contractions of excised muscle is accompanied 
by an increase of lactic acid. The amount of acid attainable by — 
severe direct stimulation is found, with notable constancy, to be 
not more than about one-half of that reached in the production 
of full heat-rigor, or by the action of other destructive agencies 
than heat. 

‘“‘In an atmosphere of oxygen there is no survival development 
of lactic acid for long periods after excision. 

“ From a fatigued muscle, placed in oxygen, there is a disappear- 
ance of lactic acid already formed. 

“‘ This disappearance of lactic acid due to oxygen does not occur, 
or is masked, at supra-physiological temperatures. It is not found 
in muscle which has suffered mechanical injury; one essential 
condition for this effect of oxygen appears to be the maintenance 
of the normal architecture of the muscle. 

‘The amount of lactic acid produced in full heat-rigor is con- 
stant for similar muscles. This ‘acid maximum’ of heat-rigor is 
not affected by a previous appearance within the excised muscle 
of lactic acid due to fatigue, or by a previous disappearance of 
acid in the presence of oxygen, or by alternate appearances and 
disappearances several times repeated.” 

It will be necessary later to consider this work in its bearing 
upon the effects of muscular activity, upon hyperpneea and muscle 
soreness; at the present time only the chemical characteristics 
of muscle have to be discussed. 

The nitrogenous extractives of muscle form a large group; the 
chief ones are creatin, carnic acid, inosinic acid, carnosin and purin 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 215 


bodies. It is impossible to give a satisfactory account of their 
significance in the physiology of muscular work, but an attempt 
will be made to pick out the observations of most interest. 

Creatin is a substance which crystallises out when an aqueous 
extract of meat is allowed to evaporate. It has the formula 
C,H,N,0.. E. Mellanby(°) has found that it is present in 
different amounts in the muscles of various animals; as a general 
rule there is a greater quantity in the muscles of animals in an 
ascending scale of development from the cold-blooded to the 
warm-blooded. This is illustrated by the following table :-— 


Lamprey . . ‘25 percent. Hedgehog (winter) . . °2 per cent. 
Skate. ee a5 ‘ (summer) oh eee 
Cod . : 3 4 Rats (two months old) . 3 __s,, 
Frogs ‘ 2 EG: Ps Bullock . ; i 6. Bi gs 
Fowl , Par) eae Pig. c : : es Seer 
Guinea pig . ‘32, Rabbits . : : ee 


A further interesting difference was discovered by an analysis 
of the muscles of similar animals during several stages of de- 
velopment. 


Rabbits (foetal, 21 days) . ; : : . a trace. 
»  ‘@daysold . : ‘ : ; . ‘191 per cent. 
es: aes a a een... apes 
of AS yl ee ee. ae 
410. 2 ne ee ms lee 
» > ar oe en ee 
a a ee ee ae na 
gs fs mee Od oe en ca TRE 
» adult a ra OF ee 
Chick embryo, before 12thday. . . .  .~ no trace. 
" » Weight 671 grms. 12th day . : . a trace, 
s Ps a 119° 14th, : . 38 mgre. total. 
! ” ” » %W3 ,, 16th » °¢ . . 66 ” 
i. . =. $60. 10th eo S18 . 
ee ee ns |) ae 
Chick, weight 36-0 grms. 1 ig) after re but 
no food taken . P . 230 3 


In the embryo chick creatin is absent at a stage of develop- 
ment when muscle is present ; in hedgehogs the quantity of 
creatin in the muscles remains constant throughout the year, 
notwithstanding the great differences in the metabolism of these 
animals during the periods of hibernation and activity. 


216 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


Creatin is absent from invertebrate muscle, although morpho- 
logically and physiologically the cross striated muscle of these 
animals is identical with that of vertebrates. The closely related 
substance creatinin, C,H,N,O, is not present in muscle, and is 
not formed from creatin during prolonged work. Creatin has 
apparently no influence on muscular contraction or the passage of 
a nervous impulse into muscle. From a consideration of these and 
other facts, Mellanby suggests that creatinin is formed into creatin 
and-stored in the muscle, and that the development of the liver 
in the vertebrates may account for the presence of creatin in their 
muscles; the gland of the mid-gut of invertebrates, although it 
has been called a liver by some biologists, has no morphological or 
physiological connection with the liver of vertebrates. The urine 
of young children is almost free from creatinin, and chicks do not 
excrete creatinin until about a week after hatching, by which time — 
their muscles are saturated with creatin. 

The other nitrogenous extractives found in muscle are carnic 
acid, C,,H,,N,O,, combined with phosphorus to form phospho- 
carnic acid ; carnosin, C,H,,N,O,; iosinic acid, C,H, .N,PO.5 
small quantities of wrea, CO(NH,).; and of the following purine 
bodies—hypoxanthine or oxypurine, C,H,N,O ; xanthine or dioxy- 
purine, C,H,N,O, ; and uric acid or trioxypurine, C;H,N,O;. The 
significance of these substances in the metabolism of muscle is 
unknown. 


THE INORGANIC SALTS OF MUSCLE 


There are considerable variations in the quantity of the in- 
organic salts which are present in the muscles of different animals, 
but in all the samples analysed potassium and phosphorus are the 
most abundant constituents. The following results expressed in 
parts per 1000 were obtained by Bunge (1°) :— 


Lean Beef. Fat Beef. 


KO. . . » « #@5d 4°160 
NaO. « « «© Of 0811 
a 0-072 
MgO. «. iv oe » hee 0381 
BOD. \ oho cee cn hus Rae »> 
2 a eee eee 4580 
Gos CO Se eee 
BOR \: Nb 0:010 


s ; ‘ , ‘ otras 2°211 - 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 217 


In the above table the sulphur is represented in two divisions ; 
the first gives the sulphate which can be extracted by water, and 
the second the total sulphur after incineration. The difference 
is due to the large quantity of sulphur which is set free by the 
destruction of the proteins of the muscle. 

Interesting observations upon the distribution of potassium 
in muscle have been made by Macallum ("'), who precipitated the 
potassium as the hexanitrite of cobalt, sodium, and potassium. In 
striated muscle there is a condensation of the potassium in the 
dim bands, the rest of the fibre remaining free from the precipitate ; 
in involuntary muscle it is smaller in amount and is diffused 
throughout the cytoplasm, and in cardiac muscle it is distributed 
-as in voluntary muscle. Macallum suggests that the potassium 
is associated with the rapidity of contraction. 


THE PIGMENT OF MUSCLE 


The exact nature of the pigment, which is present in muscle 
after the blood has been washed out, has given rise to much con- 
troversy. MacMunn (*) found associated with hemoglobin in the 
muscles a pigment which gave a different spectrum ; this he named 
myohematin. He observed it in a compressed fresh muscle when 
it was held before the slit of a spectroscope, and on account of its 
capacity for oxidation and reduction he considered it to be a 
respiratory pigment. It was maintained on the other hand by 
Hoppe Seyler and his pupils that the pigment was either hemo- 
globin or a derivative produced by putrefaction. After the lapse 
of some years the subject has been reinvestigated by Mérner (?%), 
who confirms MacMunn’s view, and suggests that the pigment 
differs from hemoglobin in the combination of its hematin with 
some other protein or in a different linkage of the components. 


THE FERMENTS OF MUSCLE 


Modern investigations have demonstrated the great importance 
of unorganised ferments in physiological: processes (1*) ; the living 
cell may be compared to an organised ferment which produces 
unorganised ferments or enzymes. In voluntary muscles there 
have been found amylolytic, glycolytic, and peptic enzymes, or in 
_ other words, ferments which act upon glucose, glycogen, and protein 


218 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


respectively ; there are also present maltase and an oxidising 
ferment, or oxydase. These ferments doubtless play important 
parts in the exchange of material in the muscle, for in addition to 
their definite action there is the probability of a reversible action ; 
the breaking down process may, according to the condition of 
the tissue, be replaced by the building up or synthetic process. 


CHAPTER II 


Facts concerning the structure, and the physical, chemical, and 
physiological properties of muscle, have been given as an intro- 
duction to the physiology of muscular work. It is now necessary 


to study the effects of muscular exercise or work upon the living 


organism, especially man. 

The origin of muscular activity we cannot trace, nor can we 
think of its beginning in the offspring of any animal. Muscular 
activity is a specialised form of protoplasmic movement, and the 
capacity for such movement is an attribute of the minute male 
and female cells from the union of which the offspring arises. 
There has been a continuity of life from parent to offspring through 
countless generations, and this carries with it the potentiality of 
muscular movement. Heredity has a far-reaching influence upon 
muscular development and activity; even at birth the power of 
co-ordination possessed by some animals is remarkable. A guinea- 
pig is born with its eyes open and its body covered with fur, and it 
is able to run about: a calf within a few minutes of first seeing the 
light is able to walk and seeks its mother’s teats. These young 
animals do not learn to walk, but learn by practice to make perfect 
the power of walking which they possess at birth. A marked 
contrast is seen in the newly born rabbit, rat, and mouse; they 
are blind, naked, and helpless. A study of birds reveals a similar 
difference. Within an hour or two of hatching the chick is able 
to run about and peck up its food ; the pigeon, on the other hand, 
is for several days blind, naked, and helpless. The maturity or 
immaturity of different classes of animals at birth is related to the 
natural habits of the race and to adaptation. 


The condition of the newly born child is intermediate between — 


the extreme examples just given; its muscular development and 
power of co-ordination are considerable, and are especially seen 


i 
‘“ 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK — 219 


in its capacity to grasp with the hands and feet. Most interesting 
observations have been made upon this subject (15), which affords 
strong support for the Darwinian theory of evolution. Infants 
at the very beginning of their separate existence possess a remark- 
ably strong grip; they can hang by their hands from a horizontal 
bar and support their own weight, and the strongest may maintain 
their position for two minutes. Most adults would find it difficult 
to support themselves in this way for a quarter of the time. Infants 
possess also a power of grasping with the foot; the great toe is 
abducted and the foot is held more in the position of a hand. This 
power is gradually lost as the child grows up, and is only found 
greatly developed in the adult in those rare cases of congenital 
absence of the arms and hands. These prodigies may be seen at 
fairs exhibiting their skill in using their feet for all the purposes 
for which an ordinary man uses his hands. 

For these characteristics of the infant there appears to be 
no adequate explanation except the one suggested by Dr. Louis 
Robinson. He considers that they are examples of atavism ; that 
the infant of primitive man was carried in a manner similar to that 
seen in apes, the offspring clinging to the parent by grasping with 
its hands and feet the hair under the arms and over the pubes. 
Grasping their parents’ hair is well known to afford infants their 
earliest form of amusement. Dr. Robinson has also pointed out 
that his view would offer an explanation of the great development 
of hair over the pubes and in the axille at the time of puberty. 

It is well known that the foetus often makes active movements 
in the uterus; these movements are known as “ quickening with 
child,” and by one of those subtilties in which the legal mind 
delights have acquired some importance, for they entitle any preg- 
nant woman who is condemned to death to at least a temporary 
reprieve. In many cases, however, no active movements may be 
detected even in the last days of pregnancy. 

There is another striking example of muscular power and co- 
ordination in the infant. Within a few seconds of its birth it 
draws its first breath, and continues to ventilate its lungs by 
alternate contraction and relaxation of its respiratory muscles, 
although up to this time its respiration has been entirely carried 
out by the placenta. Ahlfeld (!*) maintains that this activity of 
_ the respiratory muscles is not suddenly assumed at birth, but 

_ that slight respiratory movements can be detected in the fcetus 


220 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


inside its mother’s womb during the last months of pregnancy. 
These movements he has studied both by palpation and by a 
graphic record; they resemble the respiratory movements of a 
newly born infant in their irregular rhythm and in periods of 
waxing and waning. The criticism that such respiratory move- 
ments would draw the amniotic fluid into the lungs is not a serious 
one, for the movements are slight, and even if the fluid were drawn 
_ further than the naso-pharynx it would be rapidly absorbed, and 
being sterile and isotonic, would not injure the foetus. It would 
appear, therefore, that all the muscles of the body are exercised 
during foetal life for the work which they have to perform after 
birth. It is impossible to say that muscular contraction appears 
at any definite time. 

Great advances have been made in the study of muscular 
movements in man and animals since the introduction of in- | 
stantaneous photography. Before that time too much stress had 
been laid upon the anatomical aspect of muscular movements. 
It is impossible to analyse movement in the living by an examina- 
tion of the points of origin and insertion of the different muscles 
acting upon the joints of dead subjects. Such imperfect know- 
ledge could only be partly corrected by careful observation of 
the natural movements; for the eye is not able to follow the 
real sequence of events; the impression given to the observer is a 
composite one of several movements. It is owing to this fact that 
many instantaneous photographs of men and animals in motion 
do not appear true to nature and are inartistic. The credit of 
first analysing movements by relays of instantaneous photographs 
is due to Muybridge; his work has been greatly extended by 
Marey and others. If such photographs are seen in rapid succs- 
sion the observer receives the impression that the objects are in 
motion ; this is due to the persistence of visual impressions, and 
is the principle involved in the bioscope. 

Another method introduced for the analysis of movements is 
the graphic one developed by Marey. It may be explained as an 
extension of the simple method of footprints. A man skilled in 
woodcraft can tell the nature and pace of an animal from its 
spoor or footprints, and by the examination of the fossils of such 
impressions geologists have been able to study the movements of 
animals which became extinct before the time of man. 

It would be outside the scope of this article to pursue this 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 221 


subject further, but it may not be amiss to point out how efficiently 
movements are carried out; the means are adapted to the end in 
view, and by practice the man or animal unconsciously learns to 
perform the movement in the most economical manner. “ Practice 
makes perfect”; natural movements cause less fatigue than con- 
ventional ones, and herein lies the explanation of the fact that 
the pace and style of one man may be quite unsuited for another. 
An adult experiences more fatigue when he adjusts his pace to 
that of a child than when ‘he walks the same distance at his usual 
speed. A child learns to crawl, walk and run, even if it receives 
no assistance from its mother ; healthy exercise is all that is needed, 
and the experience thus obtained is a far better guide than any 
system of teaching or drill. A movement which is suitable for one 
individual is unsuitable for another; some men have long legs, 
some short legs, and each walks most efficiently at his own pace. 
This is not sufficiently recognised by many trainers, but the re- 
cords of many a contest on land and water show that style’is a 
personal factor which can never be rigidly fixed. 

Muscular work is not a localised expenditure of energy; all 
parts of the body are involved in a greater or smaller degree ; it is 
useless to attempt to simplify the analysis of the process by experi- 
ments upon the isolated muscle. Such a preparation is artificial, 
and the issue is confounded. A recognition of the complexity 
of the changes involved in muscular work enables us to under- 
stand how far reaching are the effects which can be produced by 
exercise. Anatomical changes are produced in the body; not 
only do the muscles become larger, but the pull exerted by them 
causes changes in the size and shape of the bones. In certain 
cases such great changes are produced by the repeated contrac- 
tions of certain groups of muscles that the occupation of a man 
may be determined by an examination of his skeleton (1). 

- The law of the conservation of energy shows that without an 
adequate supply of energy in the form of food there can be no 
transformation into the energy of muscular work. The food must 
be adjusted to the work obtained from a man or beast, a truth 
which is constantly neglected with disastrous results. A man or 
horse works best when he is well fed, and feeds best when he is 
well worked. This is recognised by those who have extracted the 
_ greatest and most prolonged efforts from each. Napoleon is 
_ credited with the saying that “a soldier fights on his belly.” A 


~ 


‘ale 


222 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


man who takes enough active exercise does not overfeed and be- 
come corpulent, even if the supply of food is unlimited; his 
appetite is the expression of the needs of his body, and when these 
are satisfied the desire for food ceases. 

The digestion and assimilation of the food involve the activity 
of the alimentary canal and its various glands. Muscular work 
produces hunger, and “ hunger is the best sauce ” ; healthy exercise 
enables a man to enjoy and digest food which otherwise he could 
not eat. Pity for the labouring poor on account of the plainness 
of their fare is misplaced. 

The muscles are thrown into activity in response to various 
impulses, and in this way the whole of the nervous system, in- 
cluding the special senses, is influenced by muscular work. It is 
not a question of the discharge of motor impulses alone, but a 
general effect upon the whole system, so that a man overwrought | 
by sedentary mental work often finds the best restorative in 
muscular exercise. 

The respiratory system is thrown into greater activity to supply 
the increasing demand for the intake of oxygen and the discharge 
of carbon dioxide. The heart and the vaso-motor system must 
likewise respond with increased activity to maintain an adequate 
supply of blood and to adjust the flow to the different organs 
according to their relative needs. The sweat glands are stimu- 
lated by the increased heat of the body, and the kidneys will 
be influenced by the waste products of muscular activity and 
digestion. 

It may be said that it is unnecessary to lay so much stress upon 
the value of muscular activity. It is not so. The benefits of 
muscular work cannot be overestimated. It is a physiological 
need of a primitive kind, and cannot be eliminated by civilisation. 
If all men were well worked and well fed many of the great social 
problems of the present day would be solved. Physiological 
truths might be carried with advantage into the consideration of 
social questions. Would there be any danger to the race if those 
who did not work were not well fed? Do not those who have 
not to earn their bread show their recognition of the virtue of 
muscular work by their devotion to manly sports ? 

Muscular exercise is a necessary condition of a healthy exist- 
ence; it is difficult to find the man who has been injured by 
muscular work, it is easy to procure many who have. been ruined 


] 
: 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK — 223 


by the lack of it. The child or young animal which is not active 

for the pure love of activity is unwell. The belief that progress 

lies chiefly in mental training is less rampant than formerly. The 

| compulsory education of young children has increased the in- 

fectious diseases to which they are liable, has stunted the growth 
of their originality as well as their bodies, and has in many cases 
produced that mental instability which has revealed itself at a 
later stage of life in crime, insanity, or suicide. The sup- 
pression of the instinct to play has gone so far that it has become 
necessary to found societies for the purpose of teaching children 
how to play. Even the believers in compulsory education of young 
children have taken alarm, and think they can undo the harm by 
compulsory systems of monotonous drill, unnatural postures, and 
breathing exercises. The irony of it is that this kind of physical 
training is said to be based upon the teachings of physiology. It is 
a false physiology which does not recognise that natural exercise 
is the best, that instincts in healthy children ought not to be unduly. 
suppressed, and that heredity is more potent than systems of 
education. 


Types or MuscuLar EXERCISE 


Notwithstanding the numerous gradations of muscular exercise 
between the slightest and the most severe and prolonged exertion, 
it is convenient to recognise three types (78) : (1) Exercises of speed 
are those in which each individual effort is produced rapidly, but 
the maximum contraction of any single skeletal muscle is not 
reached ; (2) exercises of endurance are characterised, by long 
and continuous efforts of moderate strength; (3) exercises of 
strength are those in which the muscular exertion is very great 
for a brief period, such as the lifting of a heavy weight. In these 
different forms of exercise it can be shown that the effects produced 

| upon the body are not the same, and a different type of physique (?*) 
is found in men and animals who excel in any one of them. This 
is well seen in a comparison of a runner with a navvy or of a race- 
horse with a dray-horse. It is necessary to remember the im- 
portance of build and heredity in this respect, and to avoid an 
overestimate of training. 
The general effects of muscular activity have been sufficiently 
indicated ; its influence upon the different systems of the body 
must now be considered. 


294 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


Tue Errects or EXERCISE UPON THE MuscuLaR SysTEM 


The muscles of the body are in a condition of partial contraction 
even during apparent rest. This so-called tone is related to the 
motor and sensory nerves which supply the muscle. If these 
nerves be divided and do not regenerate the muscles will first 
waste or atrophy, owing to disuse, and then degenerate. De- 
struction of the motor cortex of the brain will cause atrophy of 
the muscles, but they do not degenerate, for through their motor 
and sensory nerves they are still under the influence of the spinal 
cord. 

The tone of the muscles is diminished by sleep ; the observant 
mother knows when her child has fallen to sleep in her arms by 
the relaxation of its body. A far greater decrease in tone is pro- - 
duced by the action of anesthetics; the arm of a patient drops 
in a limp and lifeless manner when he is deeply anzsthetised. 

From time to time the tone of the muscles varies in response 
to impulses from the skin and other parts of the body; it is in- 
creased by cold, diminished by heat, and herein appears to lie the 
chief explanation of the effects of different climates. The body is 
invigorated by a bracing atmosphere, it is depressed by a relaxing 
one. The tone of the muscles is accompanied by chemical changes 
and the production of heat ; in a stagnant moist and warm atmos- 
phere the body does not lose heat rapidly, and therefore in the 
maintenance of a constant internal temperature produces less heat 
by diminishing the activity of its muscles, and as far as possible 
favours the loss by a dilatation of the blood vessels of the skin. 
The opposite effects are produced by a cold dry atmosphere, or even 
by warm air if it be sufficiently dry and in motion. European 
children afford a good instance of these influences; they will not 
flourish in the hot moist districts of India. Two more examples 
will be sufficient. The value of a cold bath on getting up in the 
morning lies not in its cleansing properties so much as in the 
bracing or stimulating effect which it has upon the whole muscular 
system, including the muscles of the cutaneous arterioles. The 
contrast between the walk of a man on a cold frosty morning and 
on a damp warm day is known to every one who lives in this — 
country. 

The tonic contraction of the muscles may be so greatly increased 


———— 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 225 


that it passes into visible contractions of an involuntary nature ; 
shivering thus arises, and in most cases appears to be a protective 
response to the effects of cold. It is true that it may occur apart 
from external cold during nervous excitement, but even in this 
case observations may show that the temperature of the skin has 
been lowered by a sudden and vigorous contraction of its blood 
vessels. 

Voluntary contraction is the shortening of the muscles pro- 
duced by an effort of the will. This is only a general definition, 
for movements such as walking may be performed unconsciously. 
During ordinary movements the change of state is not confined to a 
single muscle ; others are also involved, it may be in active contrac- 
tion or in relaxation. The important researches of Sherrington * 
upon reciprocal innervation have shown how the muscles can in- 
fluence the activity of one another by means of their motor and 
sensory nerves: the contraction of one muscle brings about the 
relaxation of its antagonist. The muscle possesses in its muscle 
spindles a special sensory ending which is stimulated by the 
stretching produced by the contraction of its antagonistic muscle ; 
in this way not only can reflex effects be produced upon the muscle, 
but also upon the vaso-motor and respiratory mechanisms. 

A simple movement, such as removing the finger from a key 
at a given signal, may or may not, according to the individual’s 
type of reaction, be preceded by a movement of the opposite kind. 
This may be called the antagonistic form of reaction in contrast 
to the ordinary form in which the lifting of the finger is carried 
out at once (?°). 

Such a movement of the finger can only be repeated about eight 
to twelve times a second by the ordinary man, but a pianist by 
practice and by using muscles in relays can press the keys at a 
faster rate. In short, sharp, and rhythmical movements of the 
fingers the rate may be as rapid as forty a second. 

The nature of a voluntary contraction, whether it be a single 
twitch or a spasm, requires consideration. It has been studied 
in two ways, by a determination of the note of the sound produced 
by the contraction of the muscle, and by graphic records. The 
muscle note corresponds to forty vibrations a second, but the diffi- 
culty in such a determination by the ear lies in the fact that it 


1 Article Spinal Cord, ‘‘Textbook of Physiology,” edited by Schiifer, vol. ii., 


1900. 


P 


226 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


cannot perceive a note much lower than one of that frequency. 
It is probable that the muscle note really represents the overtone 
corresponding to the second octave of the vibration ten a second. 

The work of Horsley and Schifer (2) shows that the rate of 
contraction is determined chiefly by the rate of discharge of the 
nerve cells concerned in the movement. A muscle can contract 
at a faster rate, a nerve can conduct impulses more quickly than 
ten a second, if they be stimulated artificially. An interesting 
contrast is revealed by a comparison of the rates of contraction of 
a muscle when it is stimulated respectively through the cortex of 
the brain, the corona radiata, the spinal cord, and the motor nerve. 
In the last case only is the muscle thrown into such rapid contrac- 
tion that complete tetanus is produced. 

The rhythm of the muscular response (?*) to volitional impulses 


in man is about eight to thirteen per second, the number of waves © 


varying in different individuals and in the same individual under 
different conditions of work. A voluntary contraction is an 
incomplete tetanus, in which each component is a single con- 
traction prolonged long enough to produce imperfect fusion of 
the waves. 

The conditions which affect the activity of the muscles engaged 
in a simple movement can be studied by the apparatus introduced 
by Mosso, and called the ergograph. The movements of a finger 
or limb are transmitted by a system of levers to a writing point 
which marks a graphic record of the movement upon a piece of 
smoked paper fixed to a revolving drum. With this instru- 
ment many observations (7°) have been made to determine the 
influence upon the performance of work of practice, the rate of 
contraction, load, rest, hunger, mental activity, various foods and 
drugs. Many of the results, however, nwust be accepted with 
caution, for there is one source of fallacy which has not been suffi- 
ciently recognised by some investigators; \complications can be 
readily introduced during an experiment by the influence of sug- 
gestion upon the subjects of the research, and it is not possible to 
eliminate these entirely by employing as subjects men who have 
no special knowledge of, or interest in, the research. 

Ergographic studies have shown that general or local fatigue 
and hunger diminish, while previous practice, rest, sleep, and food 
increase the power of voluntary muscular contraction. These 
results are in accordance with the general experience of mankind, 


any 
int 

‘ 

: 


4 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK — 227 


and it is doubtful if a more exact measurement or analysis has 
been obtained by such experiments limited to a small group of 
muscles. 

The ergograph is a convenient instrument for demonstrations, 
but the conditions are so different from those of ordinary life that 
it is safer to rely upon experience and special experiments, such as 
those ! of Zuntz and Atwater, upon men marching or performing 
some other kind of general muscular work. 

Upon the physical and chemical changes which occur in an 
isolated muscle during contraction a vast amount of work has been 
done. Here it is only necessary to mention briefly those facts 
which have a bearing upon ordinary muscular work. The ex- 
tensibility of the muscle is increased during contraction, and its 
temperature is raised. Even in the resting condition the muscle 
is about 0°1° to 0°6° warmer than the blood which supplies it, and 
during contraction an increase of 1:15° has been observed in the 
muscles of a dog after the blood vessels had been ligatured. Under 
normal conditions, however, the rapid circulation of the blood 
distributes the heat so efficiently that the internal temperature of 
the body is only raised a degree or two by hard work.® 

The blood supply is greatly increased in the active muscle ; 
according to Chauveau and Kaufmann’s observations the flow 
is 4-5 times as rapid as during rest. The causes of this change 
will be considered later in relation to the effect of exercise upon 
the heart and circulation. . 

The chemical changes are shown by an increased absorption 
of oxygen and an increased discharge of carbon dioxide ; a muscle 
paralysed by section of its nerve uses 0°003 c.c. of oxygen for one 
gramme of its weight per minute, but this is raised to 0°006 or 
0-020 c.c. by tonic activity. The glycogen of the muscle de- 
creases, owing apparently to its combustion for the supply of 
energy. These and other similar questions, including the increased 
growth of muscle, must be discussed in a later portion of this 
article, which will deal with the general changes in metabolism 
during muscular work. 

1 See page 233. 

* See article by Burdon-Sanderson, “ Textbook of Physiology,” edited by Schafer, 
1900, vol. ii., p. 352; and one by v. Frey in Nagel’s Handbuch der Physiologie 
des Menschen, 1907, Bd. iv., s. 427. ; 

® See page 243. 

* Barcroft, Ergebnisse der Physiologie, vii. Jahrgang, 1908, p. 699. 


228 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


Tue Errects or Muscutar EXERCISE UPON THE HEART 
AND CIRCULATION 


There is little doubt that the heart is the organ which is most 
easily damaged by exercise unsuited to the weak and untrained. 
Trainers of both men and animals rightly attach the greatest im- 
portance to the condition of the heart, and a vast medical literature 
upon cardiac strain, the athlete’s heart, and the soldier’s heart 
bears witness to the importance of the subject to the medical man. 

It will be well therefore to indicate the most practical methods 
for the examination of the heart in relation to the effects of exercise. 
There is no question here of a consideration of a heart in a con- 
dition of disease, and nothing will be said about inspection, palpa- 
tion, percussion, and auscultation, which ate recognised methods 
in an ordinary clinical examination. In order to test the capacity’ 
of a heart to bear the strain of muscular work the most useful 
method appears to be a comparison of the rate of cardiac con- 
traction at rest, directly after moderate exercise of a few minutes’ 
duration, and again after periods of five, ten, or more minutes’ rest. 
The effect of exercise is an augmentation of the rate; during the 
rest which follows the work the beats rapidly decrease in the case 
of a strong well-trained heart, but slowly in the case of a weak 
and untrained one. The following example of such an observa- 
tion (24) will show the contrast :— 


| : Trained Man. Untrained Man. 
Fi Fi 
Rest, | Guereine, | Minutes | Rest, | Usb afteF| acinutes 
| 16 33 is | 47 38 | 24 
greta 14 30 14 21 40 24 
15 26 13 17 38 23 


The exercise lasted thirty seconds, and consisted in running 
down and up stairs. 

In some special cases examination of the heart by means of the 
Rontgen rays may elucidate certain questions, but the method is of 
limited application and is beset with sources of error. The same 
remarks apply to the cardiograph, the records of which may be 
so complicated as to be misleading. 

The pulse not only furnishes information on the rate and force 
of the beat of the heart, but also on the condition of the peripheral 
circulation. The sphygmograph affords a graphic record of the 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK — 229 


pulse wave, and in some cases extends the information which may 
be obtained by the hand alone. By palpation it is possible for the 
practised hand to gauge the pressure of the blood, but for accurate 
determinations the Riva Rocci sphygmometer is generally used. 

_ The circulation can be studied in several ways; by inspection 
the colour of the skin affords a guide to the flow of blood in the 
peripheral parts, and the information so obtained may be checked 
and extended by the use of flat bulb thermometers with which 
the temperature of the surface of the skin can be determined. 
The plethysmograph will measure the changes in the volume of 
a limb which are due to alterations in the supply of blood, and 
modifications of the sphygmometer, which indicate the pressures 
at which the arterioles, capillaries, and veins are blanched, will 
measure the pressure of the blood in those vessels. 

This is not the place for details of the practical use! of the 
various instruments which have been mentioned, but it will not 
be amiss to point out that the simplest are the best. The striving 
for exact results by the use of complicated pieces of apparatus 
often defeats the aim; it is far better to multiply simple obser- 
vations under different conditions than to spend much time and 
labour in obtaining a record with a complicated instrument, how- 
ever exact it may appear to be. For this warning there are two 
strong reasons. The effects of muscular exercises upon the heart 
and circulation quickly disappear when a healthy man takes rest ; 
the recovery begins at once, and during the seconds and minutes 
spent in the adjustment of apparatus great changes may occur. 
The second reason is a practical one. The medical man engaged 
in a busy practice has not the time, means, or experience necessary 
for the successful use of many scientific instruments, and it is far 
better that he should rely upon his own powers of observation by 
sight and touch. 

In the study of the effects of muscular exercise upon the heart 
and circulation it is always necessary to know the normal con- 
ditions of the subject during rest. The personal variations in 
different individuals are so great that data are deprived of a great 
_part of their value unless they combine the results of observations 
both before and after exercise. 

It is desirable that a writer should give the results of investi- 
gations conducted by himself or in conjunction with others. The 


1 See “Practical Physiology,” by Beddard, Edkins, Hill, Macleod, and 


) Pembrey, second edition, 1905, 


230 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


following table contains the results of observations! made by 
Captain L. E. L. Parker, R.A.M.C., and the author as part of the 
work of a committee appointed to report upon the physiological 
effects of food, training, and clothing on the soldier. 


Individual Variations in the Pulse during Rest. 


| Pulse. 
Number Average Average 
ae et iead of Days. | Time of Day. | ytaxi.| Mint rn ee of Mean 
, mum.) mum.) oF each | pr pees of each 
Day. Day. Day. 
ce afte : 60 10 9-10a.m. (110| 55 | 93 68 80 
men \3 6 2 2-3 P.M. 100 | 77! 99 85 92 
4 1 10-1 ae. »'|°90:| 72°), 90 72 78 
B{ ,° 4 1 2-3 P.M. (100 90) 100 90 93 
2 50 | 10 |10.30-11.30 a.m.) 92} 68| 87 72 80 . 
12 3 |10.30-11.30 a.m.| 88 | 60| 83 | 68 75 
2 {o°} 36 3 10-11 a.m. |104| 60| 99 65 78 
©.{ men} 4 20 5 10-llam. |104| 52| 965 59 7 
3 3 1 8-8.30 am. | 88) 76 | 38 76 82 
15 | 174 | 36 10] 52} 93 | 71 | 81 


* Including one of Group B. 


The next table gives the results obtained after marching; the 
figures are arranged so that the same groups of men may be dis- 
tinguished, but are not classified to indicate the influence of dis- 
tance, external temperature, clothing, and load. 


Individual Variations in the Pulse after Marching. 


Pulse. 
Number : 
Number of _ | Number a {| f 
Men. Orationa | of Days. ote Mien anes of| Minton of Mester 
7 * | each Day. | each Day. | each Day. 
Ad men}® 245 10 | 150 | 70 115 89 | 101 
8 1 | 112 88 108 93 101 
fe {s 50 10 | 160 72 123 100 112 | 
men | 4 12 3 114 72 105 85 95 r 
5 15 3 | 126 70 119 78 98 
Cf ne {é 20 5 126 80 112 88 101 
ments 3 1 138 84. 138 84 117 
p{ 6 f6 6 1 | 120 | 93 | 190 93 | 106 
359 | 34 | 160 | 70 118 87 104 


1 Second Report of Committee, 1908. 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK — 231 


The figures for the pulse in the last table are certainly somewhat 
under-estimated, for it was not possible to take all the observations 
immediately after the march; the rate of the pulse begins to de- 
crease directly the work is at an end, and this is especially the case 
in the well-trained man. 

These results represent the condition which probably obtains 
in active healthy men of the physique of the ordinary infantry 
soldier; the men were not picked men, and the data are not to 
be considered as rigidly exact, for it was not always possible to 
guard against the influence of nervousness in the first examination 
and of a brief rest directly after marching. 

The next table gives the results of observations in the pulse 


of healthy men before and after running :— 


Subject. Distance in | Pulse at Rest| Pulse just Pulse atine Rest ne wakes 
Miles. before Run. | after Run. Periods. | 
C. 1 68 164 98 (15 min.) 
. 128 (10 min.), 100 (30 min). 
2 70 164 82 (60 min.) 
B. {5 76 152 oes 
/( $ more = 144 112 (5 min.) | 
| 1 56 160 124 (1 min.) | 
120 (2 min.), 106 (33 min.) | 
2 66 a0 84 (77 min.), 74 (107 min.) | 
104 (15 min.), 88 (45 min.) | 
2 A 196 86 (75 min.) 
.. 70 _ 84 ee 
more one * 104 92 (5 min.) | 
76 120 
more in 140 100 (5 min. ye | 


The heart beats more quickly during muscular work. The 
object is clear, but the means whereby it is attained are complex. 
It has already been shown that a more rapid circulation is necessary 
to meet the various demands of muscular activity: the blood sup- 
plies these needs, and the heart is the pump which forces it to 
circulate through the various parts. The adjustment begins with 
the performance of work, and it is necessary to study the means 
whereby this is brought about. 

The heart and the arterioles are under the control of the nervous 
system ; by the action of the vagus nerve the beats become slower ; 
they are quickened by impulses passing down the sympathetic 
nerve. During rest the vagus nerve appears to exert a constant 
inhibitory action upon the heart, but directly impulses pass from 


* 
- 5 » 
® “4 f = 
™~ - i . 
< ——_ _s -, 


232 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


the motor cortex of the brain down to the muscles the inhibitory 
action is diminished and the accelerator nerve is stimulated. 
This is important, for it throws light upon the great influence of 
training upon the orderly contraction of the heart, and in relation 
' to that subject it will need further consideration later. In addi- 
tion to this nervous control, it would appear that other influences 
are at work. The products of muscular activity act upon the 
heart directly and indirectly through the nervous system; carbon 
dioxide is one of the most important of these metabolites, and it 
quickens the beat of the heart (**). Muscular work raises the 
pressure of carbon dioxide in the alveolar air of the lungs, as 
Haldane and Priestley! have shown; the breathing becomes 
more rapid or deeper for the maintenance of a constant level in 


the pressure of that gas in the alveolar air and presumably in the ~ 


blood of the lungs. Thus there is a mutual dependence of the 
heart and lungs upon one another, and an insight is afforded into 
the close association between the heart and lungs which is ex- 
pressed by a “ good wind.” Sarcolactic acid is another metabolite 
formed during muscular activity, and directly or indirectly may 
influence the heart. 

The temperature of the body rises during muscular activity, 


it may be as high as 102° F. in a healthy man, and it is known | 


that the heart responds to an increased temperature by a more 
rapid beat. In addition vaso-motor changes occur, more blood 
goes to the active muscles and more to the skin when the 
temperature rises, and in some cases at least a more rapid beat 
of the heart appears to be an effort of compensation for the fall 
of blood pressure which may arise from the dilatation of the vessels 
in those parts. 

The response of the heart to muscular work and to rest is so 
extremely rapid that it would appear to be due in the first place 
to nervous influences; the effects of metabolites and of the rise in 
the temperature of the body develop more slowly and persist for 
a considerable time after the work has ceased. 

The heart is a muscle, and resembles the skeletal muscles in its 
response to work. Gradually increasing work with good nutrition 
and periods of rest strengthens it. There is an increased growth, 
a physiological hypertrophy which is more or less proportional to 


the development of the skeletal muscles. The untrained muscle 


1 See page 240. 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 233 


is overstretched and injured by severe and prolonged exertion, 
but the damage is more easily done and less easily repaired in 
the case of the heart. The skeletal muscles become fatigued and 
painful during movement, and the enforced rest gives them time 
to recover; the heart cannot take such a profound rest, it must 
continue to beat, if life is to be maintained, and can rest only by 
beating less frequently and less vigorously. In the case of the 
untrained heart over-exertion produces palpitation, overdistension, 
| and dilatation. The duration of systole to diastole, that is, of 
| work to rest, may be less than 1:1 in the fatigued heart, whereas 
in the normal heart the proportion is 1 : 3. 

The increase in the rate of the heart beat is within certain 
narrow limits proportional to the work done. The pulse rate 
may be readily doubled by running down and up stairs for thirty 
seconds, but such vigorous exercise rapidly produces dyspnoea 
and cannot be maintained. The heart not only works more 
rapidly, but it has to pump the blood into the aorta at a pressure 
which may be one-half greater than the pressure at rest. Thus a 
healthy well-trained man who had performed such an exercise 
showed the following changes in the pulse and blood pressure ; 
the pulse was increased from 56 to 124, and the blood pressure in 
the brachial artery from 126 to 142 mm. Hg. A high blood pressure 
is not maintained, for during the continuation of exercise the 
cutaneous arterioles dilate. 

The limit of efficiency as regards the rate of the heart beat 
appears to be reached at about 160 per minute, but there are 
doubtless individual differences. 

The influence of marching with a load has been especially 
investigated by Zuntz and Schumberg (?*) ; they not only found an 
increase in the rate of the contraction of the heart but also evidence 
of engorgement of the liver and heart with blood. The frequency 
| of increased hepatic and cardiac dulness on percussion was 56, 70, 
| and 87 per cent., when the soldiers marched carrying loads of 
| 22, 27, and 31 kg. respectively. The average increase of cardiac 
dulness was 1 cm., that of the liver 2°3cm.; the highest figures were 
35 and 5cm. respectively. The cardiac dilatation involved the right 
ventricle on sixty-two occasions, and the left also on thirty-one 
occasions. The engorgement did not coincide with the increase in 
the rate of the pulse, but with the inereased breathing and the rise 
_ in the temperature of the body. In such experiments the belts and 


234 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


straps of the equipment as well as the load produce a hindrance 
to free breathing, but nevertheless the engorgement of the liver 
must be considered as a means of relieving the heart from still 
greater distension. The tendency to strain upon the heart is 
diminished by its increased beat, for the area of the inner surface 
of the ventricle exposed to the pressure of the blood is diminished 
during its contraction. 

After severe exercise the pulse shows an exaggerated dicrotic 
notch ; this has been observed in a considerable number of healthy 
men, and is no doubt a normal sign of the reaction to the high 
blood pressure during work. The following figure is a repro- 
duction of a tracing obtained from the radial artery of an athlete 
on the day following a severely contested game of Rugby football 
in a “ cup-tie ” match. 

The blood pressure is the force exerted by the blood upon the — 


MII SN 


The rate of the pulse was 72 per minute. 


walls of the blood vessels, and is dependent upon the pumping 
action of the heart and the resistance of the vessels, especially of 
the arterioles. The blood pressure will be raised by a more rapid 
or more powerful contraction of the heart, if the peripheral re- 
sistance remains the same or does not diminish in proportion to 
the increase in the work done by the heart. During muscular 
work it is known that the peripheral resistance varies; the vessels 
supplying the muscles are dilated, and, if the exercise is continued 
and the temperature of the body rises, the cutaneous arterioles 
expand. This statement applies to those forms of exercise or 
work in which there is no prolonged straining effort ; directly the 
muscles of a limb are rigidly contracted and the thorax is fixed 
in order that a better purchase may be given to the contracting 
muscles, as, for example} in lifting a heavy weight, or in wrestling, 
a great peripheral resistance may be introduced. During some 
efforts the pulse may disappear at the wrist owing to the com- 
pression of the arteries by the forcible contraction of the muscles 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK — 235 


and to the hindrances offered to the free passage of the blood 
through the heart. For these reasons it is necessary to consider 
the nature of the muscular exertion in any discussion of its effects 
upon the blood pressure. 

M‘Curdy (27) has drawn special attention to this question ; 
he recorded the blood pressure in seventy-seven experiments upon 
twenty-three men before, during, and two or three minutes after 
an exercise of strength or effort. The exercise consisted in the 
maximum lift for each man; the weight lifted varied from 118 to 
249 kilos., and the time of an average lift was five seconds. The 
following are the average values of all the measurements :— 


Blood Pressure in Mm. Hg. 


Before Lift. | During Lift. | T° aed ha na 


111 180 | 110 


The highest blood pressure before the lift was 127 mm. Hg., 
the lowest 93; the maximum during the lift was 210 and the 
minimum 146. The rise in blood pressure was not accompanied 
by any great change in the pulse rate ; some men showed an in- 
crease, others no change, and. others a decrease. Such exercises 
subject the heart and blood vessels to a great and sudden strain, 
and cannot be considered beneficial. A man must be carefully 
and progressively trained to lift great weights; otherwise there is 
a great danger of overstrain of the heart. 

These results may be compared with those obtained by 
Bowen (**), who investigated the changes in the beat of the heart 
and the blood pressure during bicycling. He found that there 
was a rapid rise in the blood pressure when the work began ; the 
maximum was reached in about four minutes, but during the con- 
tinuation of the exercise the pressure declined slowly. The extent 
of this fall depends upon the dilatation of the blood vessels of the 
skin, and this in turn depends upon the temperature of the body 
and its surroundings. This factor does not come into play during 
an effort such as a lift; the powerful and sustained contraction — 
of the muscles during the lifting of a weight nearly obliterates the 
lumen of the blood vessels, and thereby increases the peripheral 


236 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


resistance enormously ; the increase in the volume of the blood 
driven out of the heart raises the blood pressure quickly to a great 
height, because the blood already in the arteries is prevented from 
escaping. 

The changes in the blood pressure are effected very rapidly, 
as is shown by the following examples taken from a series of ob- 
servations (7°) upon two men, one well-trained, and the other un- 
trained. The exercise lasted only thirty seconds, and consisted in 
running down and up stairs. 


Trained Man. Untrained Man, 
past | Sambar [Pregame nan | getter [Fim ont 
110 mm. Hg 134 118 104 134 108 
122 =, 134 126 110 140 106 
122 -, 152 132 118 126 110 
126 142 130 116 148 103 
1168. 3, 136 129 122 148 116 


The blood pressure does not remain at a high level during the 
prolongation of exercise, but falls gradually and may become 
lower than it was before the beginning of the work. Such a con- 
dition has been observed during a march of seven miles; the fall 
in the blood pressure was associated with a dilatation of the 
cutaneous blood vessels, which was shown by the increased colour 
and temperature of the skin. 

The causes of the quickening of the beat beat during muscular 
work are to be sought, as already mentioned, in-nervous regulation, 
effected reflexly or directly by the products of muscular activity, 
and in the action of those metabolites and the rise of temperature 
upon the heart itself. It might be expected that the control of 
the blood vessels is similarly effected, for the heart is developed 
from blood vessels. The evidence from experiments upon the 
flow of blood through muscles is conflicting. Gaskell’s (2°) work 
led him to the conclusion that vaso-dilator fibres are stimulated 
reflexly when muscles contract, but Bayliss (#4) obtained only 
slight dilatation on stimulation of the dorsal nerve roots. Lactic 
_ acid was found by Gaskell to produce a dilatation of the arteries 
of a frog, but Osborne and Vincent could not obtain a similar 


result in mammals; carbon dioxide dilates the blood vessels in 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK ~— 237 


the muscles of frogs, as Bayliss has shown. There is no doubt 
about the close association of dilatation of the cutaneous vessels 
with a rise in the temperature of the body produced by muscular 
work. Further observations, however, are necessary to explain 
the means whereby muscular work influences the heart and 
circulation. 


INFLUENCE OF MuscuLAR WORK UPON THE RESPIRATION 


Even during rest the differences in the rate and depth of breath- 
ing of healthy men are considerable ; some men breathe slowly 
and deeply, others take rapid and shallow respirations. The 
“tidal air”? may vary from 100 to 1000 c.c., and the total volume 
of air breathed per minute shows a similar wide range, the deter- 
minations ranging from 3000 to 9000 c.c. These figures have 
been taken from the results of different observers ;! in addition 
may be given a series (**) recently made upon sixteen subjects, all 
of whom, with two exceptions, were medical students :— 


| 
Volume of Average 
Subject. Teen su bag gr pte eee | ger a cnee oe 
Months. inches. * Litres, | Pe? Minute. iacekl 
1 | 212 154 Blk, 6560 | 225 291 
2 | 18-4 132 57 7867 «19°34 406 
3 27°1 144 511 7660 | 165. 465 | 
4 18 115 5d 9234 =| Ql 441 | 
5 21 155 Bll 9°150 17°7 517 | 
6 148 91 4°8 6030 | 14 431 
7 22°9 173 6:24 9°753 10 981 
8 20°4 140 6:0 5113. | 17 300 
9 20 133 5°7 5008 =| sid 47 341 
10 19°5 129 56 | 6350 | 10 638 
11 21 131 56 5'168 | 19 271 
12 22°1 123 55 7570 | 194 392 
13 20 140-510 9-504 24 396 
14 21°6 126 57 5°560 13°5 416 | 
15 “| 19% 134 55 ° 7900 17 463 | 
16 219 126 56 5458 | 12°4 450 
Average} 205 | 134 | 57 7118? | 167 449? | 


1 See article Chemistry of Respiration, by M. S. Pembrey, in “ Textbook of 
Physiology,” edited by Schiifer, vol. i., 1898, p. 748. 
2 Measured at 15° moist. 


238 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


These results compare very well with those obtained by Haldane 
and Priestley (°°) upon fifteen men; the means of their results 
were :— 


vets | afta | SEER") ree, esi 
| | inches. | Mfoiat in Litres, | Pe? Minute. Moist. 
—————E——— 

154 5°11 8°55 | 152 | 604 


There is little doubt from the results shown in these two tables 
that many of the older estimates, which give much lower figures, 
are incorrect. 

The composition of the alveolar air is not the same in different 
adult subjects, for FitzGerald and Haldane (*4) found that the 
percentage of carbon dioxide varied from 5°86 to 4:29 in men 
and from 5:40 to 3:99 in women. From this the conclusion 
must be drawn that there is a personal variation in the 
response of the nervous system to the stimulating action of 
carbon dioxide. 

The effect of muscular work is always an increased respiratory 
exchange and increased ventilation of the lungs to meet the greater 
demand for the supply of oxygen and the removal of carbon dioxide. 
The breathing becomes quicker or deeper, or more often is affected 
in both ways; hyperpncea is produced and, if the work be severe, 
laboured breathing or dyspnoea may follow and give rise to so much 
discomfort that the man will pant and stop work to “recover his 
breath.” 

At rest the healthy man breathes through his nose; the in- 
spired air is warmed, moistened, and cleansed of foreign particles 
by passing over the moist surface of the nose and naso-pharynx. 
During vigorous exercise, however, mouth-breathing constantly 
replaces nasal breathing, for there is then less resistance to the entry 
and exit of the air, and the exposure of the moist vascular surface 
of the mouth assists in the cooling of the body. The man who 
resists the inclination to breathe through the mouth during active 
exercise throws an additional and unnecessary amount of work 
_ upon his respiratory muscles, and thereby increases his discomfort 
and distress. 

The increase in the ventilation of the lungs during exercise 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK ~— 239 


| may be shown by the following examples from experiments upon 
three healthy men :— 


Ventilation No. of 
Subject. | in Litres | per Respirntions. Ratag «eee Remarks. 

B. 1 18 60 _—_ At rest. 
15 31 124 —- After running ¢ of mile. 

| 9 29 70 At rest. 
| 12 22 84 | After running } of mile. 

C. 1 20 72 At rest. 
143 25 110 After running } of mile. 


The causes of hyperpnoea and dyspnoea have been the subject 
of many experiments and much controversy, and even at the 
present time it is impossible to maintain that the question is closed. 
It is generally admitted that the work of Haldane and Priestley (*) 
has shown that the factor which regulates the respiratory move- 
ments during rest is the pressure of the carbon dioxide in the 
blood, as gauged by the pressure of that gas in the pulmonary 
alveoli. The rate of breathing may be altered, but the subject 
of the experiment will also change the depth, so that the mean 
pressure of the carbon dioxide remains almost the same, as shown 
by the following results obtained by these authors :— 


Percentage of Carbon Dioxide 
in Alveolar Air. 
sues. | Sara 
At End of _ At End of Mean 
Inspiration | Expiration. ; 
9 5°59 5°87 5°73 
: J. 8. H. ; 19 5°56 | 5-70 5°63 
) 
9 533 | Csi '47 5°40 
J. 8. H. { 20 5°44 | 5°60 5°52 
: 105 595 | 674 6°35 
J.G. P. { 30 598 =| «= 6:05 6-02 


During muscular work Haldane and Priestley found that there 
was a slight but distinct rise in the percentage of carbon dioxide 
in the alveolar air. Their results are given in the next table. 


- 
- 
* 


240 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


Effects of Work on the Percentage of Carbon Dioxide in the 


Alveolar Air 
weir Gaeaiaiea | Alveolar CO, per Cent. | 
Subject. Foot-Pounds ane mae | mae | = 
r ie 
se niacaa Normal=1. Fastration: cptretion. 
_— : i ee eS 
i ee: 1880 rs 5'10 5:80 =| «545 
4140 ie 5°56 6°51 | 6-085 
4850 a 5:20 619 | 5695 
3380 ea 5-42 578 | 560 
3160 i) 5:79 5:88 | 5835 
2530 ae 5:48 6°15 5815 
Mean J. S. H. 3320 49 544 6°05 5°75 
Mean J. S. H. Rest 1 5°54 | 5:70 5°62 
Spare eae 2420 - 653 | 713 | 683 
1940 = 661 | 651 6°56 
2460 at 660 | 7-04 6°82 
2540 - 6:06 | 7:26 6.64 
Mean J. G. P. 2340 38 645 | 6-98 6-72 
Mean J. G. P. Rest 1 617 | 639 | 6-28 
Mean of J.S.H.| 2800 43 5945 D1 | «6-235 
and J. G. P. Rest 1 | 5855 | 6045 | 5:95 


From these data they conclude that the hyperpnoea of muscular 
work is due to a_rise in the pressure of carbon dioxide in the res- 
piratory centre, but it is necessary to bear in mind that they 
measured the pressure of carbon dioxide in the alveolar air, not 
in the blood. 

The history of the different views upon the causation of the 
hyperpnceea produced by muscular work illustrates extremely well 
those fluctuations between old and new theories which are so 
constantly observed in the biological sciences. Muscular activity 
increases the ventilation of the lungs, the intake of oxygen and the 
output of carbon dioxide. It was but natural, therefore, that the 
hyperpneea should be attributed to a deficiency of oxygen or to 
an excess of carbon dioxide in the blood due to the greatly in- 
creased metabolism. To these causes hyperpnoea was generally 
attributed until Geppert and Zuntz (**) found that muscular activity 
was accompanied by an increase in the oxygen and a decrease in 
the carbon dioxide of the blood. These observers considered that 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 241 


the hyperpnoea was produced by some product of muscular activity 
which was absorbed by the blood, and thus carried to the medulla 
oblongata, where it stimulated the respiratory centre. Loewy (%7) 
also maintained that carbon dioxide was not the factor, for he 
found that, whereas the rate of respiration was doubled by muscular 
work when the increase above the normal amount of carbon dioxide 
in the expired air was only 0°5 per cent., the same amount of 
_ dyspnoea could be produced during rest only by artificially raising 
the percentage of carbon dioxide to a much higher point, about 
5 per cent. 

The alkalinity of the blood is diminished during muscular 
activity, and this observation revived the question whether pro- 
ducts of activity stimulated the respiratory centre. Forty years 
ago Pfliiger (°°) suggested that lack of oxygen might act indirectly 
as a stimulus to respiration; products such as lactic acid un- 
oxidised owing to the deficiency of oxygen in the tissues might 

be the exciting agents. 

The work of Haldane and Priestley effected a return to the 
view that carbon dioxide was the chief factor which increased the 
respiration during muscular work. But quite recently the whole 
question of muscular hyperpnoea and dyspnoea has been reopened, 
and it is necessary to consider the possible action of such factors 
as lack of oxygen, metabolites such as lactic acid, increased tem- 
perature of the body and nervous impulses from various parts. 
The muscular work performed by Haldane and Priestley was 
slight, and their analyses of the alveolar air do not include the 
amount of oxygen. After vigorous exercise the alveolar air some- 
times shows a considerable rise in the percentage of carbon dioxide, 
and at other times a fall below the normal (*°). It becomes im- 
portant, therefore, to determine the respiratory quotient, the ratio 
of the carbon dioxide discharged to the oxygen absorbed. Such 
analyses show quotients as high as 1:2 during muscular dyspnea. 

It is necessary to consider whether the lack of oxygen produces 
dyspnoea directly or indirectly through metabolites which remain 
unoxidised owing to the lack of that gas. A man at rest breathes 
with practically the same frequency and depth whether he be 
breathing air or pure oxygen (*), and his response to an increase of 
carbon dioxide in his lungs appears to be unaffected by breathing 
oxygen (*'). Directly after exercise, however, breathing oxygen 
_ diminishes the dyspnoea, and enables the man to tolerate a higher 

; Q 


242 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


percentage of carbon dioxide. The following are examples of 
the results obtained :— 


Composition of Mixture in Spirometer at Breaking Point. 


Subject. | Started with Started with Pare 
Per Cent. Per Cent. 
Ear ee sb 3500, 
A. B 
After exercise { we vs ao 0. 


| 
| 
| 
| 


The complexity of muscular dyspncea is shown by the fact 
that it does not increase if the exercise be continued, but on the 
contrary decreases. This is well known to athletes. During the 
first quarter of a mile run the hyperpncea increases until panting” 
and distress are experienced, but if in spite of the discomfort the 
run be continued the difficulty of breathing disappears and the 
exercise can be maintained with comfort at the same or even at a 
greater pace. 
interesting phenomenon has not been thoroughly investigated, 
but it would appear that the distress is associated with a high 
respiratory quotient, with a relative deficiency of oxygen (4). 


Composition of Alveolar Air before and after the Advent of 


The runner has now 


6c“ 


got his second wind.” 


“ Second Wind.” 


This 


| Percentage Percentage co Pulse Rate 
Subject. | of Carbon of Rated E in 15 Remarks, 
Dioxide. | Oxygen. 0, Seconds. 
Pp: 5-11 ae ee 19 Rest. 

5:55 15-49 10 30 After 8 laps (} mile); 
panting ; pulse irre- 
gular. 

5°50 15°39 0°99 35 After 18 further laps; 
“second wind” at 
10th lap; pulse more 
regular ; sweating, f 

R. 5:27 14°32 0:79 ye das is 
7:36 14:03 1:06 After 6 laps. 
591 14-62 0°93 After 8 further laps; 
“second wind” ; sweat- . 
ing, 
B. 6:04 1448 | 093 19 Rest. 

8:13 13:17 1-04 38 After 8 laps, 

7°39 12°8 0:90 36 After 19 further laps ; 
“second wind” at 
16th lap; sweating. 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 243 


Other factors are probably involved in these conditions; the 
contraction of the heart is often irregular during the period of 
distress, and becomes regular when the “ second wind ” has arrived. 
A rise in the temperature of the body, vascular changes and sweat- 
ing, also accompany “second wind,” and further experiments 
are necessary to determine their significance in relation to the 
respiratory movements. 

Ryffel has found lactic acid in the sweat and urine after muscular 
exercise, but no pronouncement can be justly made for or against 
the theory that lactic acid stimulates the respiratory centre. 

It is known that a rise in the internal temperature (**) of the 
body quickens the respiratory movements; this is especially 

- marked in the dog; the rate of its respiration may be increased 
from 28 to 230 per minute when it is necessary for it to cool its 
body by the evaporation of moisture from its tongue and mouth (*). 

It is probable, moreover, that nervous impulses from various 
parts of the body, especially from the heart and lungs, may in- 
fluence the activity of the respiratory centre and produce such 
a co-ordination of the respiratory movements that an adequate 
supply of blood is maintained through the lungs. The sensory 
nerves of muscles may take part, for the respiratory movements 
are altered in type, rate, and depth by the nature of the exer- 
cise performed. During rowing a well-trained man adjusts his 
breathing to his stroke. ; 

The quantitative changes in the respiratory exchange during 
exercise will be considered later in connection with the exchange of 
material, of which they form an essential part. 


—— lO aS 


INFLUENCE oF MuscuLarR EXERCISE UPON THE TEMPERATURE 
OF THE Bopy - 


It is necessary here, equally with the other systems of the 
body, to consider the normal variations in the temperature of the 
body. One might maintain that it is more necessary, for on ther- 
mometers and temperature charts the point 98°4° (36-89°) has 
been marked as the normal ; this practice has had the unfortunate 
result of preventing the full recognition of the daily and personal 
variations in the temperature of man. Confusion is still greater 
when it is remembered that many physicians rely upon the tem- 
perature of the mouth, others upon that of the axilla, and a 


—? 


244 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


small number upon that of the rectum. It is easy to appreciate 


the reasons of delicacy which prevent the determination of the 


temperature in the rectum in the case of most patients, but there 
is no excuse for the constant neglect on the part of clinicians to 


mention in their reports the time and place where the temperature 


was taken. Accurate results can only be obtained by the deter- 
mination of the temperature in the rectum or stream of urine, 
for the cavity of the mouth is bounded by such thin walls, and 
is so readily cooled by exposure, breathing, and sweating, that 
it often does not indicate the true internal temperature of 
the body. 

The temperature of man shows a daily variation; it rises 
during the day, the time of activity, and falls during the night, 


the time of rest and sleep. The range is from 36-0° (96°8°) to 


37°8° (100°0°); these are average figures for the temperature of 
the rectum and urine, and do not include the absolute physio- 
logical range. 

The temperature is raised during muscular work (*); it may 
be as high as 38°9° (102°) in a healthy man. This truth is now 
generally accepted, although a few years ago, owing to observa- 
tions based upon temperatures taken in the mouth, it was generally 
denied or contested. The explanation of the disagreement is found 
in the fact that the temperature of the mouth may fall during the 
time that the temperature in the rectum and urine is raised. 

The heat of the body depends upon the production and loss of 
heat. During muscular work the production is greatly increased, 
owing to the vigorous combustion of material in the tissues, espe- 
cially in the active muscles. This combustion is indicated by 
the great rise in the discharge of carbon dioxide and in the ab- 
sorption of oxygen. The loss of heat also undergoes an augmenta- 
tion, otherwise the temperature of the body would steadily rise, 
and would soon reach a height incompatible with the performance 
of work and even with life itself. Indeed this does occur under 
special conditions, when a man is forced to work in a hot atmos- 
phere so laden with moisture that the cooling of his body by the 
evaporation of sweat is prevented. 

Under ordinary conditions the effect of muscular work is to 
cause a rise in the internal temperature ; the increased production 
of heat is not compensated by a corresponding loss. It appears 


that a rise of temperature, within certain narrow limits, is bene- 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 245 


ficial. The chemical changes associated with muscular work are 
probably facilitated by a temperature a degree or two above the 
temperature during complete rest, and it may be that the dilatation 
of the blood vessels of the skin relieves the heart from too great a 
blood pressure. Most men find that they can work more com- 


fortably and efficiently when they have “warmed up” to the 
work, and some maintain that they work better when they begin 


to sweat. The first half mile of a walk is not so well performed as 
the later portions. Such evidence, however, would not justify a 
statement that the improvement is solely due to a rise of tem- 
perature, for there are other factors to consider. Among these 


_ should be mentioned the adjustment of the heart and respiration, 


the increased flow of blood through the muscles, and probably an 
increased secretion of synovial fluid in the joints. This much, 
however, may be said. Muscular work both in the case of man 
and other warm-blooded animals is constantly accompanied by a 
rise in the internal temperature; the time of activity coincides 
with the rise in the daily variation of temperature, the time of 
rest and sleep with the fall to the minimum. Moreover, the daily 
variation in temperature is accompanied by corresponding changes 
in the pulse and the respiratory exchange. 

The following table will show the effect of various forms of 
exercise upon the temperature of man :— 


Temperature before Temperature after 
Exercise. ercise, 


Subject. Remarks. 


Urine. ‘Rectum, Mouth.| Urine. |Rectum. 


Mouth. 
Degrees. ees.| Degrees. Degrees. | Degrees. 
00| 3722| 37-67| 3645 3794| 38°00 Walk of about 4 miles, 
‘80| 37-22| 37-55) 36-32) 37-83| 37°94) Digging for 45 minutes. 
Sa ... | 36°60 | 38°35| 38°70 Work for about 2 hours 
in snow. 
P. | 376 | 372 | 376 | 37:°3.| 38:0 | 38:1 | Bicycle ride for 3} 
. miles; temperature 
of air in shade, 33°6 


d 
36°22) ... ... | 38°40 Pian iger of Simelihorn 
(2752 m.). 
37°53 | 36°67 | 37-72) 38-13 or seat ride for about 
20 miles. 
-36°78| .... | 39:00) Game of fives for 30 
36°22; ...° | 38°89 minutes, 


246 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK | 


In addition to the above data may be given the results of 
observations upon soldiers after marching : |— 


Number) Number | Number} yj. Mini- Average of | Averageof | Average of 
ts) of — mat | ants Maxima of Minima of Mean of 
Men. |Observations.| Days. | ™U™- ) mm | each Day. each Day. each Day. 
21 359 | 34 |102-4° | 98°8° 101°4° 100:2° 100°7° 
(39°11) (37°11) (38°56) (37°89) (38°17) 


In observations upon the temperature of the body it is necessary 
to determine both the deep and surface temperature, for there is 
no doubt that the greatest discomfort arises when the temperature 
of the skin is abnormal. It is in the skin that the sensations of 
heat and cold arise. A rise in both the deep and surface tem- 
perature shows that the body is warmed in all its parts, and dis- 
comfort is then experienced: if, on the other hand, the skin be 
kept cool by sweating or exposure, no discomfort and no bad 
effects are experienced by a healthy man whose internal tempera- 
ture has been raised a degree or two degrees by hard work. After 
exercise the temperature of the body falls, and the reaction is 
generally seen in a temperature below the normal for a day 
of rest. 

All men know that it is easier to work hard if the clothing be 
sufficiently light and loose to allow of the free evaporation of 
sweat. Work can only be performed efficiently under such con- 
ditions on a warm and damp day. When a labourer keeps on 
his coat he does not mean to do a fair day’s work, and there 
is very little doubt that he is being paid by the day and not 
by the piece. 

A man who is not forced to work finds in his sensations of 
heat a safeguard against overwork and heat-stroke; he works 
more slowly when he feels too hot, or, if he «wishes to continue to 
work hard, he increases his loss of heat by the removal of his 
coat or waistcoat and by turning up his sleeves. The evapora- 
tion of sweat is thus facilitated, as shown by the following 
comparative observations? upon soldiers after a march of seven 
miles :— ~ 2 


1 Committee on Physiological Effects of Food, Training, and Clothing on the 
Soldier, Second Report, 1908. ( 
* Ibid., Fourth Report, 1908, 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK = 247 


In in Rectal | Loss of Moisture from Increase in External 
Increase in Pulse.| “Temperature. | Body inGrammes. | W¢lghtofClothes| Tempera. 
Max.) Min. |Aver.| Max. | Min. Aver.| Max. Min. | Aver. | Max. Min. |Aver. Ra — 
——S ——— | —S EE 
52 | 16 | 28 |1°6° F.|0°6° 1:07] 1430 | 1000 | 1200 | 250! 0O|109|67°| 58° 
48 | 24] 41 /1°8 0°8 (15 2000 | 1200 | 1500 480) 90 | 254! 69 59 
| 


and load. 


* Drill order without jacket. 


+ Drill order with jacket. 


When the temperature of the air is high both by the dry and 
wet bulbs the evaporation of sweat must be greatly increased in 
order to cool the body. This is shown by a comparison of the 
results obtained upon the same men, when they performed the 
march on hot and cold days with the same clothing, equipment, 


Increase in Pulse. 


Number of Men. 


sei Min. 


me 


84 | 52 


24; 8 


- 
7 


.| Max. 


16 


2°3° F. 


Increase in Rectal 
Temperature. 


Min. | Aver. 


1°4° 
0'8 


| 


Loss of Molatare £ | Increase in External 
Body feagerntcaials Pee eee isp rr 
Max. | Min. | Aver. | Max.) Min. |Aver. nt c- 
: ea SS ee 
2390 | 1140 | 1816 | 640} 60 | 320! 79°| 67°5° 
555 3CO 419 | 40} OO] 27) 45 | 38 


—_ 


The limits of the regulation of temperature are passed when 

a man is obliged to work under unfavourable conditions of clothing 
in a very hot and moist atmosphere ; the temperature of his body 
rises to an abnormal height; the production of heat exceeds the 
loss, and a point is soon reached at which the metabolism becomes 
extravagant; more and more heat is produced, and heat-stroke (**) 
is the result. This condition is seen in soldiers forced to march 
carrying a load, and wearing unsuitable uniform and equipment 
in a hot and humid atmosphere. 


1 Committee on Physiological Effects of Food, Training, and ater: on the 
Soldier, cond Report, 1908, 


248 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


THE INFLUENCE OF MuscuLAR Work UPON THE EXCHANGE 
OF MATERIAL 


It has already been pointed out that the law of the conser- 
vation of energy applies to the performance of muscular work,! 
and data have been given to prove the great increase in the intake 
of oxygen, the output of carbon dioxide and the production of 
heat. The respiratory exchange represents only a portion of the 
total exchange which must be determined in any experiment upon 
the total income and output. The nitrogenous exchange must be 
estimated from the total nitrogen of the food ingested and of the 
urine and feeces excreted. In addition, the composition of the 
food in terms of carbohydrate and fat as well as of protein should 


be known, and also the amount of water taken in by, and discharged — 


from, the body. 

It is impossible in this article to consider this subject fully, 
but attention may be directed to the chief results of experiments. 
The nitrogenous output is dependent upon the intake of nitro- 
genous food, not upon the performance of muscular work; the 
intake of oxygen and the output of carbon dioxide are immediately 
raised by muscular exercise, and under normal conditions are pro- 
portional to the work done. It will be understood that the term 
work is here used in the sense of physiological work which may 
or may not be immediately apparent as physical work expressed 
in foot-tons or kilogrammetres. The first time a man performs 
work, to which he is unaccustomed, he expends more energy than 
he will do in performing the same work after. frequent practice. 
One of the most important effects of training is the economical 
performance of work. 

The fact that the nitrogenous output is not increased by work 
would suggest that the energy is supplied by carbohydrates and 
fats. It is necessary to give briefly the evidence upon these points. 
In the first place the experiments of Pettenkofer and Voit (*") show 
that: the excretion of nitrogen in the urine is the same whether a 
man does work or is at rest; the output of nitrogen is determined 
by the amount of that substance taken in the food. In their first 
series of experiments the mean excretion of nitrogen in the urine 
during hunger was 12°4 grms. during a day of rest and 11°8 grms. 


1 See Atwater, Hrgbenisse der Physiol., 3ter Jahrgang, Abtheilung 1, 1904, s. 497. 


, 


- —_— 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 249 
during a day of work, the time of labour being nine hours. In 


the series of experiments when the man received food the figures 


were 16°8 grms. during rest and 17:1 grms. during work. There 
is no increase in the discharge of nitrogen on the days following 
the work, provided that the man started with stores of energy 
in the form of glycogen and fat sufficient to prevent any demand 
upon the protein of his tissues to supply the energy required for 
the performance of the work. 

There is no doubt that the amount of food required by a man 
depends upon the muscular work which he performs. So much 
follows from our knowledge of the conservation of energy, but 


_ that law gives us no guidance as to the relative amounts of protein, 


carbohydrate, and fat which are required. To obtain evidence upon 
these points we must examine the diets of men whose occupations 
entail different amounts of manual labour. 

Atwater calculated the energy value of the dietaries of civilians 
living under different conditions, and upon these data based his 
well-known standards of diet. These are given in the table on 
the following page, together with other dietaries for the sake of 
comparison. 

Special importance must be attached to the dietaries of prisoners 
both military and civil, for in these cases it was impossible for 
the men to obtain extra food ; work was regularly performed, and 
medical inspection and the weights of the men at different times 
showed that their health was satisfactory. 

During the last two or three years special attention has been 
directed to the question of diet. Chittenden’s (*%) experiments 
and writings have given rise to much discussion, for he maintains 
that most men eat too much protein and impair their health by 
this extravagance. His contention is that the large amount of 


_ protein consumed overtaxes the kidneys in their work of excreting 


the waste products of digestion. For this he gives no evidence. 
On the other hand it is well known that the body is overcompen- 
sated and is able to adjust itself within certain limits to a wide 
range of work. This capacity is no doubt shared by the kidneys, 
for one kidney may for years efficiently remove the waste products 
which are normally discharged by two kidneys. 

It is impossible here to examine fully Chittenden’s experiments 
and views, but it may be well to emphasise again the importance 


__ of instinct and experience as guides to questions of food. Healthy 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


250 
| Protein. | Fat. ayorates Calories. Remarks, 
| | 
Grammes. Grammes. Grammes. 
No work 4 | Pe 2700 ae jac of 
. : s at and carbohydrate 
Light work E 110 3000 aoe. give bus 
Moderate work . .% 125 3500 they must be suffi- 
Hard work 3 150 | 4500 cient to make upwith 
; 8 | the protein the num- | , 
Very hard work | oe | 5500 ber of calories, The 
i standards are for 
| food actually eaten. 
Average food sup-| 133 | 115 424 3369 | Waste included, but 
pe gratis to four not extra food 
ritish regiments ! bought. 
British soldiersinde-| 127 — 65 497 3272 | Rations allowed. 
tention undergoing | 
sentences exceeding 
forty-two days with- 
out hard labour ! 
British soldiers in de- | 141 69 560 3614 | Rations allowed. 
tention undergoing 
sentences exceeding 
forty-two days with 
hard labour + 
Ordinary _ prisoners,| 135 35 536 3115 | Food supplied. 
Scotland,light work, 
mostly sedentary 
Convicts, Scotland,| 173 57 602 3707 | Food supplied. 
“hard labour,” so 
called ; 
Food supplied free, 91 48 406 =) Seamen and boys are 
seamen, Koyal Navy! '| also allowed 4d. a 
Food supplied free,| 107 69 406 2845 | day to buy the extra 
boys, Royal Navy1 food required. 
British army mini-} 138 105 528 3903 
mum war ration 
(South Africa) * 
Russian army war| 187 27 775 4891 
ration (Manchuria)! 
Japanese army war| 158 27 840 4343 
ration (Manchuria)! 
Members of two col-| 225 334 633 6812 | Food eaten. 
lege football teams, ; 
United States of 
America 


1 Third Report, 1908, Committee on Physiological Effects of Food, Training, 


and Clothing on the 


Soldier. 


fe 


ly 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 251: 


men and animals do not eat too much, if they perform an adequate 
amount of work. 

The increased consumption of food which is associated with 
muscular work generally involves an increase of protein, carbo- 
hydrate, and fat, as shown in the dietaries already given. There 
is need of more protein, even although it is agreed that the destruc- 
tion of that substance in the body ‘is not increased by muscular 
work: there is a retention of nitrogen during muscular work, and 
this nitrogen appears to be stored or built up in the body as protein. 
Bornstein ! found that he retained in eighteen days an amount of 
nitrogen which would correspond to 800 grms. of muscle. Muscles 
develop during work, and thus the protein of the tissues is increased. 

For the supply of energy during muscular work the carbohydrates 
and fats are increased. These foods stuffs can replace one another, 
for fat is formed from carbohydrate in the body, and there is evi- 
dence to show that fat may give rise to carbohydrate. The relative 
amounts of these articles of food selected by different men appear 
to depend upon individual tastes and the comparative cost. Fat 
is dearer than carbohydrate, but it is a concentrated food. 

The energy values of the different foods are most conveniently 
expressed in terms of the heat which they yield on combustion. 
One gramme of dry protein or of carbohydrate yields 4:1 calories, 
and an equal weight of fat yields 9:3 calories. The protein does 
not undergo complete combustion in the body; for this reason 
the physiological value given above is less than the physical value 
as determined by the calorimeter. For the supply of the energy 
needed during the performance of muscular work the different 
food substances can be substituted according to their equivalent 
quantities ; thus 100 grammes of fat, 211 grammes of protein, and 
230 grammes of carbohydrate are physiologically isodynamic. The 
- muscles obtain energy from each kind of food, but the carbohydrates 
and fats are the ones most readily used. The relative quantities 
will depend upon the diet ; carbohydrates form a large proportion 
of the food consumed by most working men. 

A man absorbs per kilogramme of his body weight and per 
hour about 0°29 grm. of oxygen and discharges about 0°33 grm. 
of carbon dioxide, when he is at rest; directly he performs work 
the respiratory exchange increases, thus a walk at the rate of about 
three miles an hour will raise the values four or five times. If 


a See Cohnheim, Ergebnisse der Physiol, Zweiter Jahrgang, Abtheilung 1, s. 621, 


SS = ~~ 7 “ — 


a 


the work be of an unusual kind the exchange is much greater the 
first time that it is performed, but by practice it may be reduced 
to two-thirds of its original value. This economical working of the 
muscles is one of the most striking results of training. The well- 
trained body is far more efficient than the best engine. 


252 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


INFLUENCE OF MuscuLaR WoRK UPON THE GLANDULAR 
; SysTEMS 


It is impossible to consider this subject fully within the limits 
fixed for this article, but attention may be directed to two im- 
portant glands, the kidney and the sweat glands. 

The sweat glands are thrown into activity by muscular work, 
and the evaporation of the sweat cools the body and removes waste — 
products. Figures + have been given to show how large an amount 
of water and heat may be removed from the body in this way. 
During work the production of heat is raised four or five times, 
but the increased loss of heat due to the exposure of the blood 
in the dilated vessels of the skin is insufficient to prevent the 
temperature of the body from rising to a dangerous height. The 
cooling of the body in such a case is effected by the evaporation 
of sweat and moisture from the respiratory tract. A very striking 
instance of the limitations and dangers incurred by a failure to 
sweat has been recorded by Zuntz and Tendlau (*). The man 
upon whom they made observations had no sweat glands, and 
directly his temperature rose to 39° on exposure to the sun or as 
the result of moderate work he breathed double as much air as 
during rest. His skin was flushed with blood, but these efforts of 
compensation were insufficient to prevent his temperature rising. 
He found that he could continue to work in summer only by 
frequently soaking his shirt in water; with this wet covering on 
his body he was able to supply a substitute for sweat. 

The great loss of moisture during sweating reduces the amount 
of water secreted by the kidneys, unless an adequate supply of 
water be taken by the mouth. The urine is concentrated, and on 
cooling throws down a Jeposit of urates. After vigorous exercise 
albumin is frequently found in the urine. This so-called “ func- 
tional albuminuria ” MES investigation on account of its 


1 Pp 247. 
\ age 


\ e 
\ . 
\ 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK ~ 253 


great medical importance. The majority of the best athletes 
examined have passed albumin in their urine after contests in sport, 
such as rowing, running, and football (°°). Is this albuminuria to 
be considered pathological or physiological? It is probably the 
expression not of disease but of disturbance in the supply of blood 
and oxygen to the kidneys during strenuous work. This is pro- 
bably the reason why most cases of albuminuria are found among 
the best cfews and teams; the men with the best physique and 
the best training are capable of the greatest exertion. During 
rest the albuminuria disappears. 
The subject demands attention, for there is no doubt that men 
have been rejected for life insurance on account of this albuminuria, 
and many have been treated as the subjects of serious disease of the 
kidneys. It is difficult to imagine how much worry and misery 
may have been caused by the failure to recognise that albuminuria 
is not necessarily a sign of renal disease. 


MuscutarR Work As A CAUSE oF FATIGUE 


Fatigue is a condition which it is very difficult te define or 
analyse. Excessive activity of any part of the body brings about 
a state of increasing inefficiency. The chief characteristic of 
fatigue is inefficiency, and a classification of fatigue might be based 
upon its apparent seat of origin. Fatigue due to muscular activity 
alone needs consideration in this article, but it must not be thought 
that the condition is one in which the muscles alone are involved ; 
it is impossible to exclude other parts of the body, for muscular 
activity affects all parts ; the body works as a whole. 

Muscular work produces fatigue, and its onset depends upon 
the nature of the work, its duration, and the external conditions 
-under which it is performed. An analysis of the sensations of 
this fatigue shows that there is a local fatigue, a muscular soreness 
and discomfort due to the excessive use of one group of muscles, 
and a general sense of fatigue which follows a more uniform activity 
of the muscular system, Local fatigue may quickly manifest itself 
when a special movement, to which the body is not accustomed, 
is performed. The muscles quickly tire and respond with weaker 
and less orderly contractions. The causes of this condition are 
not well known, but it would appear that lack of sufficient nutri- 
_ tion, including oxygen, is one of the most important ; waste pro- 


254 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


ducts such as lactic acid may be formed and may injuriously affect 
the activity of the muscle fibres or their nerve endings. It may 
be, as Hough (*') and Hill (5*) have suggested, that the muscular 
soreness experienced during work and for some hours afterwards 
is due to the waste products of activity, such as lactic acid. 

There is another kind of muscular soreness which is not ex- 
perienced at all during the time of exercise, but is felt the next 
day as an uncomfortable or even painful stiffness which gradually 
disappears during movement, but may be noticeable for two or 
three days. This form of soreness Hough thinks may be due to 
ruptures within the muscle fibres. It is difficult to obtain evidence 
upon this point, and it would appear more reasonable to attribute 
it to similar causes to those given for the first kind of muscular 
soreness. 

The onset of fatigue, both local and general, can be delayed — 
or largely prevented by progressive training. This may be due 
to the fact that the body by practice performs work more econo- 
mically and adjusts more readily the circulation of the blood to 
meet the general and local needs of nutrition. It may be that 
the body acquires an immunity to its waste products or increases 
its capacity for oxidising or otherwise rendering them inert. The 
‘receptive substance”? between the nerve and the muscle may 
be an important element in muscular fatigue. 

Weichardt (5%) maintains that there is a toxin which causes 
fatigue, that this toxic substance is not lactic acid, and that the 
living body produces an antagonistic substance, an antitoxin. 
The injection of blood, serum, or other fluids containing the toxin 
is said to produce in an animal all the signs of fatigue, unless it 
has been previously protected by a dose of the antitoxin. These 
observations need confirmation, but in support of the view other 
arguments have been advanced. The toxicity of the urine is 
increased by muscular exercise and the sweat secreted during 
hard work is toxic, whereas that poured out by a man at rest but 
exposed to a heated and moist atmosphere is free from harmful 
substances. It is well known that an untrained man experiences 
the day after hard and prolonged exercise a general feeling of 
lassitude and disorder; this has been attributed to an auto-intoxi- 
cation or poisoning by the products of the unusual muscular activity. 

Ryffel has found that even after moderate exercise lactic acid 
can be detected in the urine and sweat, but further observations 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK ~— 255 


are necessary to show the relationship of this substance to 
muscular fatigue. 

Undue stress is frequently laid upon the capacity of the heart 
to perform work. This is not peculiar to the heart, a similar 
condition is seen in the respiratory muscles. Their activity is 
rhythmic, work followed by rest; they have been progressively 
trained from birth and work efficiently and economically. By 
constant practice a corresponding capacity for work can be 
developed in the ordinary muscles of the body. When an 
unusual amount of work is thrown upon the heart and the 
respiratory muscles, signs of fatigue are observed in them similar 
to those shown by other muscles. 

The absence of fatigue in nerves need not be discussed here, 
for in the living body the nerve is a part of the nerve cell, and the 
nerve cells are without doubt subject to fatigue. How largely 
the nervous system is involved in fatigue is shown by the fact 
that a much greater amount of work can be performed, without 
the immediate experience of fatigue, when the subject is spurred 
on by nervous excitement. The fatigue, however, in these cases 
is only postponed, and the reaction is greater. The nervous ex- 
citement inhibits the sensations of fatigue, and under such con- 
ditions the body can be readily overtaxed. 

Nervous impressions may diminish the sensations of fatigue ; 
soldiers at the end of a long-march step out more briskly when 
the band strikes up a lively tune. The interaction between the 
nervous and muscular systems is of the closest nature. Good 
mental work cannot be done after a hard day’s sport, and study 
for a serious examination is incompatible with training for a race. 

Fatigue is a protective sensation, a warning that the body needs 
rest. .There is nothing to be gained by resisting it by the use of 
_ drugs. Work performed under such conditions is extravagant, and 
the price is paid later in a greater reaction and depression. The 
decrease in the general sensibility produced by fatigue renders a 
man unmindful of discomforts which under ordinary conditions 
would be intolerable; he can sleep profoundly and thus restore 
his energy. 

The rational method is to prevent as far as possible the con- 
ditions which are favourable to fatigue. Progressive training 
enables the body to work more economically, to adjust the supply 
_ of blood to the needs of the various parts, and to strengthen the 


256 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK 


body uniformly, so that no extra strain is thrown upon it by the 
early failure of any part, especially of the heart and lungs. This 
method has stood the test of experience, and is the only one recog- 
nised by good trainers of athletes and horses. 


BIBLIOGRAPHY 


+ Ranvier, Arch. de physiol. norm, et path., 1874. 

* Haycraft, Journal of Physiology, vol. xxxi., 1904, p. 392. 

% Halliburton, Journal of Physiology, vol. viii., 1887, p. 133. Biochem- 
istry of Muscle and Nerve, 1904. 

“ Von Fiirth, Ergebnisse der Physiol., 1902, i., Abt. 1, s. 110. 

5 Mellanby, Proc. Physiol. Soc., Journ. Physiol., 1908, vol. xxxvii., p. xxxiv. 

® Brodie and Richardson, Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc., 1899, B, vol. exci., p. 127. 
Vernon, Journ. of Physiol., 1899, vol. xxiv., p. 239. 

* Leathes, Problems in Animal Metabolism, 1906, p. 99 ; Journ. Physiol., 
1904, vol. xxxi., p. ii. 

8 Fletcher and Hopkins, Journ. Physiol., 1907, vol. xxxv., p. 247. 

® Mellunby, Journ. Physiol., 1908, vol. xxxvi., p. 445. 

10 Bunge, Zeitschr. f. physiol. Chem., 1885, Bd. ix., s. 60. 

4 Macallum, Journ. Physiol., 1905, vol. xxxii., p. 95. 

12, MacMunn, Proc. Physiol. Soc., Journ. Physiol., vol. v., 1884, p. xxiv. 
Phil. Trans., 1886, p. 267. Pete f. physiol. Chem., 1888, "Bd. 13, s. s. 497, 

13 Mérner, Jahresber. f, Thierchem., 1897, s. 456. 

14 Jacoby, Ergebnisse der Physiol., 1902, i, Abt. 1, s, 213. 

15 Robinson, Brit. Med, Journ., 1891, aoe li, p. 1226; Nineteenth 
Century, 1891, Buckman, ibid., 1894. Mumford, Brain, 1897, vol. xx., 

290. 

. 16 Ahlfeld, Woaischrite fiir Ludwig, Marburg (1890), s. 

1 Arbuthnot Lane, Guy’s Hospital Repoctas vol. xliii., oa p. 321; ibid., 
vol. xliv., 1887, p. 359. 

18° MS Curdy, Amer. Journ. Physiol., 1901, vol. v., p. 95. 

19 Tait Mackenzie, Journ. Anat. and Physiol., 1898, vol. xxxii., p. 468. 

20 W, G. Smith, Proc. Physiol. Soc., Journ. Physiol., 1900, vol. xxv, ; 
Mind, vol. xii., N.S., No. 45. 

*1 Horsley and Schifer, Journ. Physiol., 1886, vol. vii., p. 96. 

22 Schifer, Journ. Physiol., vol. vii., 1886, p. 111. Griffiths, ibid., vol. ix., 
1888, p. 39. Haycraft, ibid., vol. xi., 1890, p. 352. Fraser Harris, ibid., vol. 
xvii., 1894-1895, p. 315. 

* Warren Lombard, Journ. Physiol., vol. xiii, 1892, p. 1; vol, xiv., 
1893, p. 97. 

*4 Pembrey and Todd, Proc, Physiol. Soc., Journ, Physiol., vol. xxxvii., 1908. 

2 Henderson, Amer. Journ. Physiol., vol. xxi., 1908, p. 126. 

% Zuntz and Schwmberg, Physiologie des Marsches, Berlin, 1901. 

27 M‘Ourdy, Amer. Journ, Physiol., 1901, vol. v., p. 95, 

28 Bowen, Amer, Journ, Physiol., 1904, vol, xi., p. 59. 


- THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCULAR WORK ~— 257 


*® Pembrey and Todd, Proc, Physiol. Soc,, Journ, Physiol., vol. xxxvii., 1908. 
% Gaskell, Journ, Physiol., vol, i., pp. 108 and 262; Journ, Anat, and 
Physiol., vol, xi., 1877, p. 720. 
*t Bayliss, Ergebnisse der Physiol., v. Jahrgang, 1906, s, 331. 
_ * Schlesinger and Pembrey, Proc. Physiol. Soc., Journ, Physiol., vol. 
xxxvii., 1908. 
*% Haldane and Priestley, Journ. Physiol., vol. xxxii., 1905, p. 246. 
* FitzGerald and Haldane, Journ. Physiol., vol. xxxii., 1905, p. 486. 
% Haldane and Priestley, Journ. Physiol., vol. xxxii., 1905, p. 225. 
% Geppert and Zuntz, Arch. f, d. ges. Physiol. Bonn, 1888, Bd. xlii., s, 189. 
Loewy, ibid., s. 281; and 1890, Bd. xlvii., s. 601. 
%8 Pfliiger, ibid., 1868, Bd. 1, s. 61. 
3° Pembrey and Cook, Proc. Physiol. Soc., Journ. Physiol., vol. xxxvii., 
1908. Hill and Flack, Journ. Physiol., vol. xxxvii., 1908, p. 108. 
_ © Schlesinger and Pembrey, Proc. Physiol. Soc., Journ. Physiol., vol. 
Xxxvii., 1908. - 
“1 Pembrey and Cook, ibid., p. xli. 
*2 Pembrey and Cook, Proc, Physiol. Soc., Journ. Physiol., vol. xxxvii., 
1908, Oct. 17. 
* Boycott and Haldane, Proc. Physiol. Soc., Journ. Physiol., vol. xxxiii., 
1905-1906. 
 Richet, Compt. rend. Soc. de biol., Paris, 1887, p. 482. 
 Pembrey and Nicol, Journ. Physiol., vol. xxiii., 1898, p. 386. Pembrey, 
Arkle, Bolus, and Lecky, Guy’s Hospital Reports, vol. lvii., 1902, p. 283. In 
these papers the literature of the subject is considered. Pembrey, Brit. Med. 
Journ., vol. i., 1904, Feb. 27. Hill and Flack, Journ. Physiol., vol. xxxvi., 
) 1907-1908, p. 11. 
“© Haldane, Journ. Hyg., 1905, vol. v., p. 494. Rogers, Journ. Royal 
Army Medical Corps, 1908, vol. x.; p. 25. Sutton, Journ. Path. and Bact., 
1908, vol. xiii., p. 62. Pembrey, Guy’s Hospital Reports, 1902, vol. lvii., 
p. 261. 

“ Pettenkofer and Voit, Zeitsch. f. Biol., Bd. ii., 1866, s.537. Sce Tigerstedt 
Die Physiologie des Stoffwechsels, Handbuch der Physiologie des Menschen, 
von Nagel, Bd. i., 1906, s. 441. 

“8 Chittenden, Physiological Economy | in Nutrition, London, 1905. The 
Nutrition of Man, London, 1907. 
 Zuntz and Schumberg, Physiologie des Marsches, Berlin, 1901, s. 311. 
5° Collier, Brit. Med. Journ., 1907, vol. i., p. 4. Dunhill, ibid., p. 1031. 

51 Hough, Amer. Journ. Physiol., 1902, vol. vii., p. 76. 
*2 Hill and Fluck, Brit. Med. Journ., Aug. 22nd, 1908. 
58 Weichardt, Arch. f. Physiol., 1905, s. 219. 


SOME CHAPTERS ON THE PHYSIOLOGY OF 
NERVE 


By N. H. ALCOCK 


I. Introduction. II. The Nerves in the Living Animal. III. Fatigue. 
IV. Regeneration. V. The Theory of the Nervous Impulse. 


I. INTRODUCTION 


THE physiology of the peripheral nervous system has certain 
peculiarities which too often deter the would-be student from 
the study of the subject. The experimental facts are simple 
and easy to understand, and have been determined with great 
accuracy, yet the explanation is apt to be complex and lengthy 
to a degree unusual even in physiology, and the task of mastering 
the literature and understanding the various theories of what at 
first sight appears far removed from any practical application is 
one which is too often deferred indefinitely. It is a matter for 
regret that this should be so, for the study of living tissues cannot 
be separated into watertight compartments; no one subject can 
be neglected without hindrance to the rest. The processes under- 
lying the injury current of nerve are governed by the same 
laws that determine the secretion of the salivary glands, of the 
cells of the stomach or of the kidney, and it is only because 
so much interest has been aroused about the obvious and 
tangible results of the activity of these tissues that the under- 
lying causes have for the moment been lost sight of. As soon as 
one reflects as to the reason why a cell of the stomach sends 
HCl in one direction rather than another, one comes across 
problems that rest on the same physico-chemical basis as in the 
apparently simpler case of nerve. 

For those who demand an immediate practical advantage from ~ 
everything they read there is one comparatively minor considera- _ 
tion that may perhaps appeal. The charlatan and quack, ever 
with us, take advantage of the prevailing neglect of electro- | 

258 


' 
; 
| 
| 


THE PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE 259 


physiology ; it is hardly necessary to quote the instances of 
electropathic battery belts, the claim that nerve contracts on 
excitation (1), and the “records” of the electrical response in 
metals (*), to show that folly of a kind that one moment’s reflection 
should dissipate finds a ready acceptance with those who consider 
the experimental facts of animal electricity too dull for their 
enlightened minds. 

There is, however, one quite genuine difficulty in the study of 
our present subject that makes it less interesting than it will be 
in the future. None of the theories are in any way complete, and 
we come across many facts which appear to conflict with the best 


arranged of these explanations. All that can be done in such a 


case is to record these facts in the hope that later on the proper 
place may be found for them; the simple and easy receipt for the 
manufacture of hypotheses by the inclusion of favourable instances 
and the exclusion of everything that contradicts the theory is one 
that does not lead to a result on which much reliance can be 
placed. 


PRELIMINARY NOTES 


It is unnecessary to do more than briefly recapitulate the 
experimental facts that have been made out concerning excised 
medullated nerves that are still in a surviving condition, yet as 
they form the starting point of what follows it may not be 
out of place to give a short summary of the most important 
phenomena. 

After the nerves have been excised from the body they are 
best kept in an approximately isotonic solution of NaCl, to which 
a small quantity of glucose may with advantage be added. When 
the effect of the excision has passed off, such nerves are isoelectric, 
that is, they give no current in the galvanometer, and they have 
apparently (except in certain known particulars) resumed the con- 
dition they were in before they were removed from the body. 

Three main varieties of electrical phenomena directly connected 
with function can be observed in such nerves—(1) Current of 
injury; (2) current of action or negative variation; (3) electro- 
tonic currents; giving to each the usual name. Excellent reasons 
can be given for the disuse of each of these terms, but as their 
significance is perfectly clear, it seems better for our present 


_ purpose to use words with which the student is already familiar 


260 SOME CHAPTERS ON THE 


rather than introduce confusion by the employment of theoreti- 
cally more perfect but unknown expressions. 

The explanation of these terms is simple. If in an isoelectric 
nerve an injury be made at any point, that spot becomes electrically 


Qu Zn 
+ —_ 
LL NS SS 
() <— () 
FF Qurrent of injury. 


negative (zincative !) to the uninjured part, and this difference of 
potential gives rise to the current of injury (Fig. 1). 

If such an injured nerve be excited, the uninjured part also 
becomes electrically negative (zincative), and as this gives rise to 


ee 


Fig IL a a pa GIO PO Og pO gO pg OOD gnc 


a+ ——— —" pe 


Figl= eee Sa 
Ewcit: action. 


an opposing current to the current of injury, the latter appears to 
be diminished, and therefore the result is to produce a diminution 
or “ negative variation ” of the injury current (Fig. 2a). . i 
If an uninjured nerve be excited, the normal effect is that a 
wave of zincativity passes down the nerve, each point becoming — 


1 Like the zinc of a Daniell cell. 


PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE 261 


successively electrically negative (zincative) to the rest. This has 
been termed the current of action, and the negative variation is 
evidently only a particular case of this, namely, when the response 
from one spot is altered by the presence of an injury there (*), 
neglecting for the present certain possibilities to be considered 
later. 

These remarks can therefore be summarised thus :— 

(1) Any injured spot is electrically negative (zincative) to any 
uninjured. 

(2) Any active spot is as a rule electrically negative (zincative) 
to any inactive spot. © 
_-- Injury and excitation give, therefore, as a rule, an electrical 
response of the same sign. Whether this is the result of the same 
process or not is a matter that is not quite as simple as at first 


> —— 


Fig IL LOLOL A AL AD LA AL AL AL AL AL ALAA AAPA OPO” OPPO OL a oo 


q D q (perewe 
\ 
ee a er, 


sight might be supposed, though there is undoubtedly a close 
| connection between the two responses. 


| pete i" 
| 
| 


There is still another phenomenon to be noted in excised 
medullated nerve. If a constant current be passed through one 
part of the nerve, the extra-polar parts of the same nerve also give 
a current, the electronic current (Fig. 3). 

These various electric effects have been subjected to elaborate 
investigation and experiment, because they are the only direct 
unequivocal signs of nerve activity. The actual metabolism of 
nerve is at best very slight in amount. It is true that deprivation 

- of oxygen for some hours paralyses a nerve (* ® 7), that the result 
of excitation gives an effect similar to that of CO, (§), and that 
experiments to be noted later give some indication of fatigue, 
but there is no detectable change in the reaction of nerves_as a 
result of activity (unless certain experiments referred to later are 
to be interpreted in this sense), and there is no other effect such as 

_ that of heat produced or of mechanical shortening? that shows 


1 If a nerve is excited at one end by maximal induction shocks, and the 
; other end examined with suitable precautions under a microscope magnifying 200 
_ diameters, no shortening or movement of any kind can be detected. But Waller 


262 SOME CHAPTERS ON THE 


whether a nervous impulse is passing or not, so that the electrical 
signs of nerve activity have assumed a greater importance than 
is the case in the physiology of other organs of the body. Further, 
these effects, occurring in such a highly specialised structure as 
nerve, have seemed a suitable object for the attempted explana- 
tion of many problems of general physiology. It becomes then 
a matter of great interest to see how far these electrical effects 
observed on excised nerve represent the events occurring in nerve 
still in the body, especially as evidence has been brought forward 
to show that it may be possible, under exceptional circumstances, 
to have a nervous impulse without the corresponding electrical 
change (*). 


II. NERVES IN THE Livinc ANIMAL 


The first of the modern instances we shall quote is from the 
work of Gotch and Horsley (*). In this there is one series of 
experiments that bear on this question. In the cat and the 
monkey both the sciatic nerves and the cut end of the spinal cord 
gave unmistakable evidence of a negative variation which accom- 
panied the excitation due to the reflex discharges from the 
nervous centres when these were excited by absinthe or (more 
markedly) by strychnine. Negative variations produced in this 
manner corresponded exactly with those produced by the electri- 
cal excitation of the cortex and other parts, and except in so far 
as the nerve impulse might be altered centrally by the drug 
(Sherrington !), these variations represent the results of the 
normal excitation of the nerve fibre by the nerve cells. 

Bernstein (14) in 1898 observed a similar phenomenon. If a 
pithed frog be lightly strychninised, the muscles of the hind legs 
fall into tetanus whenever any stimulus reaches the spinal cord. 
These tetanic contractions are more or less synchronous, and if 
by a modification of Bernstein’s method one of the legs of the 
frog be connected with a myograph, and the sciatic nerve of the 
other be placed on electrodes, and connected with a galvanometer 
or electrometer, it can be easily seen that when there is a con- 
traction of the muscles there is a negative variation in the sciatic 
has shown that if strong induction shocks are \passed lengthwise through a nerve 
(or any other moist conductor), that the heating effect of the current produces 


either lengthening or shortening, according as evaporation or rise of temperature 
predominate. . 


‘iz 


; 


a 


PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE 263 


nerve. The reflex contraction can be called out by stimulating an 
afferent nerve by the induced current, or even by the impulses 
reaching the cord as a result of touching or pinching the animal. 


The E.M.F. of the negative variation is about *00025 volt, as 


determined by the capillary electrometer (!*). 

It is possible to take a step further and examine the negative 
variation in nerves which are conducting the impulses which 
naturally traverse them without the complication of any disturb- 
ing agency. Two nerves have been employed for experiments of 
this kind, the phrenic and the vagus. Both these conduct im- 
pulses which are repeated at each act of respiration, and so it is 
possible to arrange the rather elaborate apparatus required with 
some degree of certainty of obtaining the phenomenon often 
enough to be readily investigated. Reid and Macdonald (!%) ex- 
amined the phrenic ; Lewandowsky (14), Aleock and Seemann (!”), 
and Einthoven (!5) the vagus. The latter is by far the easier object 
to study, and allowing for the increasing delicacy of the instruments 
employed, the results agree very closely as far as the nervous 
phenomena are concerned. 

Lewandowsky, using the ordinary galvanometer, found that one 
negative variation occurred every time the lungs were artificially 
blown out. Alcock and Seemann, with the capillary electrometer, 
observed the same phenomenon. They found in addition that a 
negative variation occurred at each natural inspiration, and that 
an effect was also observed when the air was sucked out of the 
lungs. Einthoven, using his string galvanometer, by far the most 
sensitive instrument yet invented,’ was able to add certain very 
interesting details which are worthy of careful consideration. 

Einthoven’s results will be understood from an examination of 
Fig. 4. The upper line is the shadow of the “string” (a fibre 


_ 3 This galvanometer consists of a fibre of silvered quartz, about 2°54 in diameter, 
suspended in a powerful magnetic field (20,000 C.G.S. units). The fibre is illuminated 
by the light of the electric arc, and the shadow of the fibre, magnified 600 diameters, 
is thrown on a photographic plate. The instrument is thus a ‘‘ moving coil” 
galvanometer, with the coil reduced to its simplest expression. When a current 
of electricity passes down. the fibre, it is deflected, and 10-* amperes give a 
measurable deflection. P 

The instrument therefore measures current not voltage ; but if the resistance of 
the circuit is known, the voltage can be determined very simply by Ohm's law. In 
nearly all electro-physiological problems it is the E.M.F. that is of interest, as the 
resistance and consequently the current is varied by circumstances not bearing 


on the experiment. 


264 SOME CHAPTERS ON THE 


of quartz silvered on the surface), moving up and down according 
to the current traversing it; 


eee 228i 84 
a : = & the second line indicates the 
tit HS : = 6 respiration, inspiration mark- 
; £2 ing upwards. A slow wave 
Suerte sects =3 of negative variation occurs 
: ase *= at each inspiration (indicat- 
ae ing that an impulse passes 
sess aH if oe up the nerve at this time), a 
desea 2= conclusion arrived at on quite 
“© other grounds by those who 
Be: < HHH 23 have observed the pheno- 
: 38 menon of respiration. 
: : HY 5 There are many things of 
fps racesectetereeeesed Hy % interest to be observed in 
Bisse. soccer rssesee +] =£ studying this photograph. In 
Hh ce the first place, the curve is 
rH 2 3 Z a double one; superposed on 
gee a: +7 22 the long respiratory waves 
: HH 3 Bs are shorter ones, the result 
z HE gE ae of the contractions of the 
se: HH 42 2 heart. Secondly, to any one 
ieees HHH 22° used to the study of the 
: Hit] ~ = & negative variations recorded 
Suess sees ‘22. by the capillary electrometer 
at 22% two differences are most strik- 
: +H 5 72 ing—the E.M_F. of the current 
BES and the time taken by the 
; 222 wave. In Gotch’s (**) analysis 
Sih HERS T we of the electrometer curve the 
: gue E.M.F. of the negative varia- 
sists : #8 tion in the sciatic of the frog 
fesatetae. ts sass: /B8e2 is ‘03 volt and the time 
HTH "=". -Ol sec.; here the E.M.F. of 
fipseeticsceesttitess é Be the respiratory curve is 
aH = 5 
: EE tg 00005 volt and the dura- 
@4 S58 tion 5 secs. The question 
: ar & immediately presents itself— 
ae : sa 


Are these phenomena, which 
alike have been callect “‘ negative variation,” in reality the same ? 


PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE 265 


One possibility may be dismissed from the mind. As far as 
our present knowledge goes, these curves are not current escape 
or any error of the apparatus. They are abolished by ligature of 
the nerve, by touching the nerve with ammonia, and by “ electro- 
eution.” If the peripheral end of the other vagus is stimulated, 
the cardiac waves disappear, as the heart is stopped, while the 
respiratory waves continue, as can be seen in Fig. 4. In apnea, 
the cardiac continue and the others cease. 

Both the E.M.F. and the time relations of these curves agree 
with the older observations with the electrometer. Alcock and 
Seeman (loc. cit.) give the E.M.F. of the respiratory response 

as ‘00005 to -0002 volts, and the time as about 4 secs.; the 
maximum on prolonged inflation of the lungs as 00025 volt. 

What, then, is the explanation? The differences in potential 
may be due (and to some extent are certainly due) to the fact 
that only a few of the fibres of the nerve are in action at any 
one time, but how is the difference in time to be accounted for ? 
There are several possibilities. These long waves of small E.M.F. 
might be the algebraic sum of many short waves; and if we had 
the electrometer records alone to go by, these might be too slow 
to indicate the successive teeth of such a curve. But there is no 
trace of such a summation in the string galvanometer photograph, 
although the instrument is sensitive and quick enough to detect 
‘000005 volt lasting for the ;}; of a second. Further, this instru- 
ment is free from the peculiarity of the electrometer that shows 
the effect of equal alternate currents of short duration as a dis- 
placement of the meniscus in the direction of the sulphuric acid. 
The exact relation of these two phenomenon which we have called 
by the name of negative variation is therefore one of the pro- 
blems still awaiting solution, and we have only the suggestion 

- by Einthoven that possibly his curves are more nearly related to 
the electronic currents than to the short steep curves the result 
of single induction shocks, that Gotch has analysed in the sciatic 
of the frog. | ; 

Einthoven finally gives a very interesting photograph showing 
the variations in potential in the wninjured vagus as a result of 
the various nerve impulses passing both up and down the nerve. 
The exact analysis of the course is, however, so complex that we 
must await the result of future work before we understand the 


_ full meaning of the photograph. 


266 SOME CHAPTERS ON THE 


III. Faticue 


The apparent absence of fatigue in nerve, which is one of the 
earliest phenomenon that the observer meets with and the student 
hears discussed, is philosophically considered a very curious pro- 
perty of these structures, and has both a greater importance and 
a ‘closer connection with what has gone before than might at first 
sight be supposed. The fundamental experiments are known to 
every one. Medullated (17 1® 1%) and non-medullated (7°) nerves 
can be stimulated for hours, the impulse being blocked (by cold, 
ether vapour, the constant current and so forth), and when the 
block is removed the end organ responds to an amount apparently 
identical with that with which the observation began. As far as 
this class of experiment goes, no fatigue can be detected, and — 
even in isolated nerve, completely removed from any possibility 
of the natural circulation being maintained, Waller (*) has shown 
that fatigue is not found. 

Various explanations could be found of this result. The most 
obvious, that as no fatigue can be detected there is in reality none, 
would be, if true, a most remarkable phenomenon. It would 
mean that a nervous impulse was conducted along a. living 
structure, that connected with this was an electrical change of a 
measurable amount, and yet that no energy was made use of in 
the process, a phenomenon unlike any other occurring in the body. 

The next explanation, and the one that seems the most pro- 
bable, is that although there is a certain amount of fatigue, and 
a certain expenditure of energy, that the loss is very quickly made 
good, and the process of repair is so rapid? that even between 
induction shocks following one another at 500 per second (and none 
of the induction coils used for the first class of experiment ran 
at anything like this rate) there is time for the nerve to recover 
its initial state. 

There arose at one time a curious little controversy of which 
a short account may not be out of place. Waller (*) made the ~ 
suggestion that the rapid repair just referred to might be due to 
the medullary sheath acting as the storehouse of reserve material, 

1 Other examples of very rapid repair of tissues that admittedly show fatigue 
could be quoted. For example, the wing muscles of insects contract about 300 


times per second, and continue in action for hours at a time, yet no one imagines 
that they are incapable of fatigue on this account. 


PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE 267 


and therefore that non-medullated nerve not possessing this store 
should prove much more easily fatiguable. Miss Sowton (?"), using 
the olfactory nerve of the pike, stated that this was the case ; 
but she omitted to exclude the possibility of the so-called “ stimu- 


lation fatigue,” that is, the local injury at the spot where the 


exciting electrodes rested. Garten (**), in his classic researches on 
the same object (of which only very brief and incidental mention 
can be made here), excluded this possibility, and found that true 
fatigue of the conducted negative variation actually occurred, re- 
covery taking place after a suitable interval, even in excised nerve. 

The objection might be made that the olfactory nerve of the 
pike is not comparable to mammalian nerve, and certainly in the 
latter “stimulation fatigue” is more easily observable than that 
of the propagated change (**), but it is probable that the difference 
that exists is one of degree rather than of kind. It would be 
interesting to try the experiments quoted in the next section on 
non-medullated mammalian nerve ; the probability is that fatigue 
would be demonstrated even more readily than in the medullated 
variety. It is not unlikely, therefore, from these and other reasons 
to be presently referred to, that the structures known as sheath 
(including under this heading medullary sheath and neurilemma) 
act as stores of food material, and this does not exclude the idea 
of Boruttau, that the Cae substance may also function in 
this capacity. 

There is, however, a more indirect way of approaching the 
question that has led to very interesting results. If there is any 
expenditure of energy in transmitting a nerve impulse, a second 
impulse rapidly following the first should show some alteration. 
This is found to be the case. 

Gotch and Burch ('*) examined the electrical response in the 


_ sciatic of the frog, when this was excited by induction shocks. 


If these succeeded each other at longer interval than ,1, of a 
second (taking round numbers), two negative variations were 
observed ; if the interval were less than this, only the response 
due to the initial stimulus appeared, and the time short enough 
to abolish the second response was termed by these authors the 
“ critical interval.” They found that this varied with the intensity 
of the exciting stimulus and with the temperature of the nerve, 
becoming longer as the temperature grew less. Stated differently, 


_ this “ critical interval” is the “ refractory period ” of the nerve. 


268 SOME CHAPTERS ON THE “? 


Frohlich made a further advance, though before his results can 
be considered in this light it is necessary to make a very important 
assumption, viz. that the effect of anesthetics (ether, chloroform, 
&c.) is the same as that of cold, and that the lengthening of 
the “critical interval” or “refractory period” due to all these 
agents is merely a slowing down of the processes occurring in a 
normal nerve in the body. 

If this is granted, Fréhlich’s experiments formed a very interest- 
ing example of fatigue. He took a nerve which was in a definite 
state of anesthesia, and stimulated this with induction shocks 
which followed each other at an interval known to be a little 
longer than the “critical interval.’ The attached muscie served 
as the indicator. When the nerve was excited the muscle at first 
fell into tetanus. This is only what might be expected ; the suc- | 
cessive stimuli each gave rise to a separate impulse, and thesé 
fused at the muscle. But as the excitation continued, the tetanus 
very rapidly fell off, and the muscle ceased to contract, and 
Fréhlich’s explanation is that the successive impulses fatigued 
the nerve, so that the refractory period became longer, the rate 
of stimulation was now less than the critical interval, each exci- 
tation blocked the next, and so no impulse was propagated to 
the muscle. Tait and Gunn (74) have extended these observations, 
using Yohimbin lactate as the anesthetic. They find that this 
substance has in some respects the same action as other anzs- 
thetics, and they are able to confirm Froéhlich’s results and add 
some interesting details. There is little doubt, therefore, that 
under the action of reagents of this class nerve shows fatigue (and. 
in all probability Waller’s (2 26) experiments with proto-veratrine 
and aconitine come under this heading), and if the preliminary 
assumption be accepted, it could be deduced from these observa- 
tions that nerve fibres even in the body are fatiguable,’ and 
only do not show this property under ordinary conditions because 
the processes of repair are so rapidly carried out (?’). 


IV. REGENERATION OF NERVE FIBRES 


The effects of section of a nerve are known to every student, 
and there is a general agreement amongst those who have experi- 
mented on the subject as to the nature of the process. This may 

1 For a different method that also leads to this conclusion, see Thérner (?%). 


° 


De  E——— Ke SSrtci‘(i( ‘trl 


PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE 269 


be briefly summarised as consisting of the degeneration of that 
part of the nerve fibre which has been cut off from the appropriate 
nerve cell. Whether the “ neurone theory” is anatomically exact 


or no, as far as this particular physiological problem is concerned, 


the degeneration exactly follows the law. 

There is no such unanimity as to the events occurring subse- 
quently. Under certain circumstances, to be considered shortly, 
the degenerated part of the nerve fibre is regenerated, and the 
particular point on which controversy has arisen is the source of 
the new nerve fibres. 

There are three possible ways in which such fibres might 
arise. 

I. They could grow downwards from the central end of the 
divided nerve, as Waller originally described in 1850. 

II. They could be formed in situ from the cells in the peripheral 
portion, and then grow up towards the spinal cord. 

III. They could wander in from the central end of other cut 
fibres, to which they originally did not belong. 

Each of these possibilities must now be considered. 

I. While this source of the new nerve fibres is regarded by 


_ most observers as at least the usual one, it must be remembered 


that there are certain conditions which must be fulfilled before 
regeneration can take place in this manner. 

(a) The peripheral and central ends of the divided nerve must 
be brought into some sort of connection. This is usually done 
by apposition and suture of the two cut ends; but where there 
has been loss of nerve substance, and a gap is left, this can be filled 
by another nerve from the same individual, which is naturally the 
best method, or failing this, by the interposition of some other 
tissue. This can be either plain catgut, or a nerve from an- 
other animal, the results are about equally good in both cases 
(Kilvington *8), and Marinesco (**) has shown that an alien nerve is 
absorbed™s a foreign body and presents no advantages over catgut. 

(b) The negative side of the condition is interesting. If the 
cut end of the peripheral part be enclosed in a rubber cap or 
stitched to some part of the body where nerve fibres are scarce, 
such as the peritoneum (*), no regeneration occurs at ali. This is 
one of the arguments against the growth of the new fibres from 
the peripheral end, as we shall see presently. 

(c) The portion of the spinal cord from which the nerves 


7 


270 SOME CHAPTERS ON THE 


originated must be intact, for if this is excised no regeneration 
follows (Lugaro *), 

(d) In the case of the posterior nerve roots no regeneration 
takes place after the nerves have entered the spinal cord, and this 
corresponds with the anatomical fact that here the nerves lose 
their neurilemma. The inference is that the presence of this 
structure is therefore necessary in some way for the growth of 
new fibres. 

(e) There must be a fresh section of both ends of the nerve, 
for if a cut end is sutured to an uninjured longitudinal surface, 
no regeneration takes place (Kilvington 8), 

If these various conditions are fulfilled, the evidence that down- 
growth takes place from the central end is very good. 


Ramon y Cajal and Marinesco (**) figure the new fibres growing . 


downwards ; these are at first seen only in the neighbourhood of 
the suture, and gradually grow towards the periphery. The fibres 
are often quaintly twisted, and the bulbous end shows curious 
shapes which are probably the result of amceboid movements. 
The new fibre seems to seek the old path by some force of chemio- 
tactic attraction (*°). The medullary sheath is formed first at 
the central end, later at the periphery, so that the former is the 
oldest part of the new fibre. If a second cut is made, either 
central to the line of suture (Langley and Anderson *) or _peri- 
pheral to this (Halliburton), degeneration again takes place, but 
only in the peripheral part of the new fibres. 

Finally, the embryological evidence is in favour of the central 
origin of the fibres, though it must be remembered that this 
evidence is only available by making use of the assumption that 
the regeneration is carried out by the same process as began 
the growth in the embryo. Ross Harrison (*® #°) has verified 
on various species of Rana, the original observations of His, 
that all the parts of the nerve fibre grow outwards from 


the cells of the epiblast that form the neural ridge, and that — 


this is true not only for the axis cylinder, but also for the 
medullary sheath and neurilemma, in this latter particular con- 
troverting Bethe’s statements, referred to in the next section. 
Harrison actually observed under the microscope in parts of 
- embryos kept alive the outgrowth of new fibres from the’ neural 
crest. Each fibre had a swelling at the free end like that of a 


regenerating nerve, and this swelling showed amceboid movements. 
od 


al 


| 


PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE 271 


There was no growth of any structure such as neurilemma from 
any other part of the embryonic tissue, though the conditions 
were sufficiently good to permit of the development of striated 
muscle from mesoblastic cells. Harrison further localised the cells 
that gave rise to the anterior and posterior nerve roots; if the 
back of the neural crest were cut away the latter did not develop ; 
if His’s cells were excised the anterior nerve roots did not appear. 

II. Regeneration from the peripheral end. 

The evidence for this view would be much more satisfactory 
if the third possibility to which we have referred did not exist. 
Briefly, there are three points to be considered :— 

(a) The growth of new fibres when the central end of the cut 
nerve is no longer a possible source. 

(b) The rapid return of sensation after section. 

(c) Embryological and histological evidence. 

(b) Kennedy (*) and others look on the rapid return of sensa- 
tion (in a few days in some cases) as evidence of peripheral 
regeneration. But Head (* * 4) has shown that the measure- 
ment of returning sensation is by no means as simple as was at 
one time supposed, and when the various explanations in any 
given case have been disentangled there is very little left on which 
to found a theory of autogenetic regeneration. Head’s experi- 
ment on himself gave no support to this view, and here it was 
possible to make more precise observations than had hitherto been 
the case. 

(c) Bethe’s (47) embryological researches led him to the con- 
clusion that before the appearance of any trace of peripheral nerve 
fibres a band of spindle-shaped cells can be seen in the place where 
the nerve is to be found. These cells were supposed to combine 
to make a syncytium, producing by differentiation of their proto- 


- plasm the neuro-fibrils of the nerve fibres. Braus (*), as a result 


of his very ingenious transplantation experiments on tadpoles, came 
to a somewhat similar conclusion ; but as both these authors are 
directly opposed to Ross Harrison in their explanation of their 
observations, it is only possible to say that the experiments of 
the latter seem, with our present knowledge, the more worthy of 
credence. 

Mott and Haliburton (*4), and later Graham Kerr (**), have 
shown, however, that in the process of degeneration certain changes 


_ take place in the peripheral end of the nerve that simulate the 


272 SOME CHAPTERS ON THE 


growth of new fibres. The cells of the neurilemma multiply 
actively. They first act as phagocytes in removing the debris of 
the degenerated nerve, and then elongating, form themselves into 
long chains of cells ; but if no nerve fibres enter these chains, they 
never, as far as can be seen, form either medullary sheath or axis 
cylinder. So that although these neurilemmal cells doubtless have 
important nutritive and other functions (we have already referred 
to the suggestion that the failure of regeneration in the spinal 
cord is due to the absence of the neurilemma), yet by themselves 
these neurilemmal cells do not form new nerve fibres. 

III. The third possibility, that regeneration may occur from 
new fibres which wander into the cut peripheral end from an alien 
central end, is one that gives rise to many interesting questions. 

It has been known for a long time that intentional suture of . 
nerves originally distinct leads to regeneration, but even when 
actual suture is not performed, growth may take place if there 
are any cut nerves in the vicinity, and it is occurrences of this 
description that invalidate much of the evidence for autogenous 
growth, as has been explained in the previous section. 

There would appear to be degrees in the chemiotactic attrac- 
tion which determines the direction of the fibres; efferent somatic 
fibres unite most readily with their own kind, less readily with 
pre-ganglionic fibres, and apparently not at all with post-ganglionic 
fibres (5). Of the various possibilities of union thus summarised 
there is one which has attracted special attention, as it has an im- 
mediate bearing on surgical procedure. This is the particular case 
in which different efferent somatic fibres are made to unite with 
each other. Ballance (°°) was the first to make a practical applica- 
tion of the fact, although he did so under the impression that 
regeneration would occur from the peripheral end. He sutured 
part of the spinal accessory nerve to the peripheral end of the 
facial, and in his most successful case the patient recovered the 
use of the paralysed facial muscles, but with the drawback that 
there was a synchronous lifting of the shoulder. 

Kilvington (28) has studied the experimental conditions under 
which union of this class takes place, and two of the many 
interesting facts he observed may be noted. 

The optimum arrangement, as judged by the functional recovery 
of movement, is shown \in Fig. 5. Here the internal and external 
popliteal nerves are cut\across, the central end of the external 


PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE 273 


popliteal turned up out of the way, and the central end of the 
internal popliteal slit up lengthwise for a short distance. The 
peripheral ends of both nerves are then sutured to the two parts 
___ of the central internal popliteal. 

'  ——s'It is found that regeneration takes place, and after the 
appropriate interval the dog has the use of his hind limb with 
little or no apparent defect, and such delicate co-ordinated move- 
ments as the scratch reflex are performed to all appearance as 
£ well as in the normal animal. Kilvington infers that the fibres 
from the internal popliteal have grown downwards into both 


LP EP Le.) Er 


1 (8 U 


| 


IP E.P : IP EP 


Fic. 5.—Optimum arrangement for Fic, 6.—Arrangement giving ‘‘ axon 
restoration of function. J.P. Internal reflex.” 
and £.P. External Popliteal nerve. 


internal popliteal and external popliteal. If this inference is a 
just one, it follows that the muscles formerly supplied by the 
external popliteal (and from the corresponding cells in the anterior 
horn of the spinal cord) are now supplied from cells that formerly 
supplied the internal popliteal, and through the nerve trunk the 
antagonistic muscles. As the: co-ordination seems as good as 
before, the cells that have been changed over must have learned 
a new lesson, and this apparently applies, not only to the cells 
in the spinal cord, but also, as Kennedy (**) has shown, to the 
cells in the Rolandic cortex. 

The earlier experiments of Langley and Anderson and Kilving- 
ton, and the later of Kilvington and Osborne, provide another 
fact. If the connections at the time of suture were arranged as 

s 


274 SOME CHAPTERS ON THE "i 


in Fig. 6, that is, if the central end of the internal popliteal was 
simultaneously sutured to both external and internal popliteal 
together, then when regeneration has occurred three points are 
noted. (Langley and Anderson observed a similar phenomenon 
when the central end of the crural nerve is sutured to the in- | 
ternal saphenous and also connects with its own muscular branch.) 

(1) Co-ordination is nearly as good as in the normal animal. 

' (2) There are more new fibres in the two branches than in the 
central trunk. 

(3) Excitation of, e.g., the peripheral end of the regenerated 
external popliteal, gives a contraction of the muscles supplied by 
the internal popliteal; even when all connection with the spinal 
cord is removed by section some distance above the line of suture. 
This contraction is termed the axon reflex, following Langley’s 
terminology. . 

Some of the new fibres in the trunk must therefore have a 
double peripheral ending in the branches. The inference is that 
either the peripheral fibres have joined together, which is a 
process unknown elsewhere, or else that one central fibre has 
branched into two at the line of suture. The latter explanation 
is by far the most probable. The inference is that in this case 
antagonistic muscles must be to some extent at least innervated 
from the same anterior nerve cells. It is surprising, not that 
co-ordination fails in the finer details, but that it exists at all 
under these conditions. 


V. THEORIES OF NERVE ACTIVITY 


It is not possible to review, in the compass of a work of this 
kind, one-tenth of the literature that exists on this subject. All 
that can be done is to select such portions as seem worthy of con- 
sideration, not so much for their intrinsic worth as for the indica- 
tion they give as to the direction in which further researches will 
advance. 

From the physico-chemical standpoint all the phenomena of 
the electric currents in nerve (and in all tissues) must be due, as 
far as our present knowledge goes, to the movement of ions carry- 
ing charges of electricity. Current of injury, negative variation 
and electrotonic currents, must all ultimately have this explana- 
tion ; and as no other hypothesis seems at present at: all probable, ‘ 


Ve —— 


~ of Macallum ( 


—_— — 


PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE 275 


the work of the electro-physiologist has received an entirely new 
direction, namely, that of endeavouring to see how the laws of 
solutions and electrolytes can be applied to living tissues. The 
problem is by no means a simple one. The nature and composition 


‘of the ions has to be determined. They might be either inorganic 


electrolytes such as KCl, or more complex bodies such as occur in 
the products of digestion. They might be present in the tissues 
as such, or be formed by chemical action and then appear, and 
their distribution may be controlled by semi-permeable membranes, 
or by surfaces of separation which would act in a similar manner. 

Before considering the modern theories of nerve action it will 
be convenient to note briefly the result of the micro-chemical work 
51, 8, 54) which has greatly influenced the theoretical 
view of the subject in this country. 

Of these papers, two deal with the subjects with which we 
are now concerned, namely, the determination of (1) potassium 
and (2) chlorides in cells and nerve fibres. 

(1) To determine the potassium (51) fresh nerve fibres are placed 
in a solution of cobalt sodium hexanitrite (Co,Na,(No,),) ; where 
potassium is- present, a precipitate is formed. According to 
Gilbert (°*) its formula is (Co(No,), 3(K/Na)No, nH,O). The 
excess of the reagent is washed away with ice-cold water, and 
the precipitate blackened with ammonium sulphide. Macallum 
found a precipitate in the medullary sheath where the neuro- 
keratine network is supposed to exist, in Lanterman’s imbrica- 
tions, at the nodes of Ranvier, but not in the axis cylinder. Even 
at the nodes of Ranvier, where the precipitate surrounds this: 
structure, the axis cylinder itself is free. 

(2) The distribution of the chlorides (5 5) was investigated by 
the addition to the tissues of y'5 normal AgNo, solution con- 


_ taining 15 per cent. HNo,. The result is briefly the reverse 


picture of the potassium. Chlorides are absent from the medullary 
sheath. They are present all along the axis cylinder whenever 
the reagent can penetrate, either at a node of Ranvier or an 
injured spot. Lantermann’s imbrications are an exception: K 
and Cl are both found there. 

Taken as they stand, Macallum’s researches would show that 
in what is presumably the active part of the nerve, namely, the 
axis cylinder, chlorides were present in greater amount than in either 
the lymph outside or in the medullary sheath, while potassium was 


present in the latter but not in the axis cylinder. Macdonald 
has given a different explanation of these results, but it is not 
improbable that the original view is the more correct. 

We are now in a position to consider the bearing of these 
observations on the theory of the nervous impulse. All the 
modern workers agree in supposing that the dissolved electrolytes 
are the cause of the electric currents, but as to the details of the 
process each experimenter has a different view, and the more the 
details are considered the less probable any given theory appears, 
so that a very brief account here will be sufficient. 

In general, it is possible that the currents can be caused by 
any one of three different processes. 

I. Chemical cells, of which the familiar type is the Leclanché. 

II. Concentration cells, where the current is caused by the 
diffusion of ions with different velocities from a place of high 
concentration to one of lower. 

III. Fluid cells, where the current is caused by the interchange 
of the ions of two fluids separated by a membrane. 

I. The chemical-cell theories present more difficulties in the 
case of nerve than the other two, and in spite of the distinguished 
men who have been advocates on this side the general opinion is 
now rather adverse to this view. It is quite unlikely that there 
are no chemical changes in nerve. But the small amount of nerve 
metabolism, the difficulty of showing any change in this meta- 
bolism as a result of activity, and the apparent absence of any 
heat produced, are all observations which tell against the proba- 
bility that chemical activity is directly concerned with the nervous 
impulse. 

One series of observations, however, can be taken as supporting 
the chemical view, namely, those on the temperature coefficient. 
It has been observed that the rate of change of a physical process 
is less altered by variations in temperature than that of a chemical 
one—in other words, that the temperature coefficient given by the 
velocity at (T° + 10) 

velocity at TS 
case, more than this in the latter. 

Maxwell (°°) has for this reason carefully measured the velocity 
of the nervous impulse at different temperatures. He took the | 
pedal nerve of the giant slug (Ariolimax Columbianus) as the 
object, and although the observations are not easy even in this 


, 


276 SOME CHAPTERS ON THE 


equation , is usually less than 1:2 in the former 


PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE 277 


comparatively favourable object, it is probable that Maxwell’s 
mean value of 1°78 is sufficiently near the truth, and Keith Lucas (**) 
as a result of observations by a different method on the frog gives 
a mean value of 1°79. 

This is definitely higher than the usual physical value of 1:2, 
and might be taken as indicating a chemical basis of nerve con- 
duction if one could be certain (which is very far from being the 
case) that a tissue which contains loose combinations of electro- 
lytes with proteins would not have a similar coefficient. At any 
rate Keith Lucas shows that the value 1-78 to 2°01 is very nearly 
the same for conduction in both nerve and muscle, and markedly 
different from that of 3°26 to 3:6 for the refractory period in 
both tissues (5” 5), so that the underlying processes are probably 
different in the two cases. 

Maxwell makes the suggestion that if nervous impulse is a 
chemical process it is probably not a direct oxidation, as if it were 
the coefficient would be higher. This would agree with the absence 
of heat produced. But we can hardly pursue the subject further 
in the present state of our knowledge, though it presents interesting 
problems that further work may solve. 

II. Concentration Cells—Macdonald (5°) has adopted this theory 
of the causation of the nervous phenomena, and as he has based 
his views on a long series of ingenious experiments, they merit at 
least a careful examination, even if his conclusions are not accepted 
in every detail. Adopting the same plan as before, and separating 
experimental facts from the inferences therefrom, Macdonald’s © 
work may be summarised as follows:— __ 

(1) The specific resistance of nerve (sciatic of cat) is 180 ohms, 
approximately equal to ‘3 per cent. NaCl. 

(2) When a nerve is placed for a short time in a dilute 


‘solution of electrolytes, the injury current is increased ; in a strong 


solution it is diminished. In the particular case of chlorides this 
alteration follows the “ concentration law,” namely— 


E,, = Ee x log F 
where E,,=the final E.M.F. after immersion. 
E,=the initial E.M.F. before immersion. 
n=the concentration of the solution in gram-molecules per litre multiplied 
by the dissociation coefficient. 


(3) The inference is drawn from these and other considera- 


278 SOME CHAPTERS ON THE 


tions that a nerve fibre has a structure consisting of a “core” 


containing a concentrated solution of electrolytes, a semi-permeable 
membrane outside this, and outside the membrane a “sheath” 
containing dilute electrolytes. 


(4) From the concentration law E,=E, x log va calculation 
can be made of the concentration of electrolytes in the core. For 
when n=1, then as log 1=0, E, =0, therefore a solution of T 


(dissociated) MCl would cause the injury current to vanish, and 
would presumably be equal in E.M.F. to the solution of the 
electrolytes in the axis cylinder. If these consist of KCl, this 
concentration is nearly 10 per cent. 

(5) The conductivity of this solution in the axis cylinder can 
also be calculated, if this conductivity of the whole nerve is known, 
if the relative amounts of axis cylinder, sheath, and connection 
tissue are estimated, and if the conductivity of the sheath be 
supposed to be the same as that of the solution in which the nerve 
is placed for a short time. Macdonald gives the value from these 
data as equal to 2°5 per cent. KCl very approximately.? 

(6) Under the microscope the axis cylinder of a living tissue 
is clear and transparent.? At the injured end granules are seen, 
and these stain with neutral red. These red-stained granules 
gradually appear further and further down the nerve, and the 
rate of appearance varies as the concentration of the fluid bathing 
the fibres, being -17 x 10—° cm. per second for 2 NaCl, :30 x 10-° 


m 


for 55 NaCl. Macdonald infers that this process gives a graphic 


representation of the diffusion of the ions causing the injury 
current. Toluidine blue gives a rather different appearance which 
may represent the action current. 

(7) Macdonald agrees with Macallum in his picture of the 
distribution of chlorides in the axis cylinder, but disagrees as to 
the distribution of potassium. He declares that while potassium 


1 Some unpublished experiments indicate that this value may require revision. 
The strength of solution in which the sciatic nerves of the cat remain unaltered in 
weight is 1°16 per cent. NaCl (°°). 

2 All authors agree with this. If the fibre is fixed different appearances are — 
seen ; that usually accepted as the best shows longitudinal fibrils in.a clear or — 
granular matrix. Ashworth (*'), examining the giant nerve cells and fibres of Halla 
parthenopeia, where these appearances are very distinct, finds quite definite fibrils, 
arising from the nerve cell and continued into the nerve fibre. If the nerves in 
the animal resemble those in the frog, this observation supports the usual view. - 

e 


PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE 279 


is invisible under ordinary circumstances, the black precipitate is 
invariably obtained in the axis cylinder at every injured spot. 
Both neutral red and toluidine blue are readily precipitable by 
_KCl, and the granules stained by these dyes are probably caused 
by the diffusion of this electrolyte. 

(8) The theory deduced from these observations is that the axis 
cylinder is surrounded by an impervious wall except at Ranvier’s 
nodes, and is filled with a colloidal solution with potassium chlorides 
adsorbed on the surface of the colloid molecules. As the salt is 
attached in this manner it is supposed to be masked, so that it 
does not give the potassium reaction, and does not exert osmotic 
pressure, but can still conduct electricity. Injury (and to a less 
extent excitation) causes these colloid molecules to aggregate and 
become larger (*). Their surface is therefore diminished, and the 
electrolytes are set free into simple aqueous solution. From the 
nature of the precipitates by Macallum’s methods the electrolyte 
is inferred to be KCl, and the neutral red and toluidine blue 
precipitates confirm this. The concentration of salt is from the 
“concentration law” about 10 per cent. This theory is therefore 
a purely physical one, and has a close relationship to the theory 
propounded by Sutherland (*). Both these theories give an 
intelligible explanation of what has always been a difficulty, 
namely, the peculiar rate of transmission of the nervous 
impulse (& ®),1 

While this Macdonsld-Sutherland hypothesis would explain 
the observations just referred to, there are still many difficul- 
ties to be faced, some of which are of a formidable character. 
Potassium chloride is supposed to be masked in such a manner 
that it exerts no osmotic pressure, yet that the ions can move so 
as to conduct electricity. It is supposed to be present as KCl, 
yet Cl appears at once on the use of AgNO,, while the K is masked 
and only appears when there is an injury—a return by a back door 
to the “alteration-theory”” that had been abandoned. The rate 
of diffusion of the +K ion is -00066, of the —Cl ion is -00069, 

1 The most recent determinations (**) give the rate at 30 metres per second in the 
frog’s nerves at ordinary temperatures, 65 metres per second at 35° C. Helmholtz 
gave 33 metres per second for human nerve (which would make this process in 
man at 36° occur at the same rate as in the frog at 15° C.) But as Tigerstedt 
points out, the arm of this subject was cooled by a casing of plaster of Paris, and 


so this’ value is too low. Waller gives 50 metres per second ; Alcock as the mean 
of forty experiments on man gave 66 metres per second. 


so that to produce a current in the right direction diffusion must 
be supposed to be faster down the nerve than into the electrodes. 
Finally, as the difference in velocity of the two ions is so small, 
and as the E.M.F. is the difference of the diffusion into electrodes 
and nerve, a very high concentration is needed—according to the 
‘concentration law” equal to 10 per cent. KCl—yet the chemical 
analysis of the tissue (as far as the observations go) shows that 
even if all the K and Cl be supposed to be present in the axis 
cylinder and none elsewhere, there is not enough to give a con- 
centration of more than ‘5 per cent. at the outside. 

IIT. The theories based on fluid cells are at first sight more 
promising, especially as it has not yet been possible to give exact 
details which can be put to the test of direct experiment. 

The view of Brunings (*), perhaps the best instance of this 
theory, is that animal electricity in general, and nerve in par- 
ticular, is an example of a “diosmotic cell.” This is an arrange- 
ment by which two fluids, which may be of the same osmotic 
pressure but differ in composition, are separated by a membrane, 
which is impermeable to both fluids as a whole, but permeable 
to at least the kation of the fluid lying within the cell. Such an 
arrangement would give a current on fracture of the membrane, 
would not give heat externally, would show the same osmotic 
phenomenon as a nerve, and does not require a greater concen- 
tration of electrolytes than is met with elsewhere in the body.! 
The electrolytes are supposed to be “ preformed” ; they might be 
adsorbed on the surface of protein molecules or not. 

Alcock (**) has published some results on the electrical con- 
ductivity of nerve before and after chloroform and ether anesthesia 
which bear on the question, if the assumption is made that these 
agents produce a maximum injury. As both drugs cause an 
electrical effect of the same sign as the injury current and of the 
same order of magnitude this is not an unreasonable hypothesis. 
The result is that in nerve there is no alteration of conductivity 
(within 2 per cent.), while there is a marked diminution in the 
polarisation.? The inference from this is that chloroform does 


280 _ SOME CHAPTERS ON THE 


1 Alcock and Lynch (®) give the figures for chlorine in nerve fibres. Some 
unpublished results on the. potassium content support the view given above, but 


_ the experiments are still too\few in number to justify the pronouncement of a final 


opinion. 
* Waller (°’) had previously observed the alteration in polarisation. His‘ results P 
do not contradict those of Alcock, as Roaf and Alderson (**) suppose. 


PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE 281 


not in nerve set free electrolytes! and if the preliminary 
- assumption be granted, that there are none set free by an injury. 
This is quite consonant with the theory just stated, but presents 
some difficulty to the concentration cell theory. Macdonald’s 
invocation of “ pseudo polarisation,” and his supposition that the 
electrolytes in nerve are free to conduct electricity but not to 
exert osmotic pressure, is an explanation that seems to require 
a further experimental basis before it can be unreservedly 
accepted. 

It might be supposed that the location and properties of the 
membrane or surface of separation which divides the fluids would 
_ present some difficulties. Ostwald (7°) originally showed that such 
a membrane could exist, and that it might be permeable to either 
kations or anions, but not to both. .In the case of nerve the 
membrane would have to be permeable to kations alone, as stated 
above. Alcock’s (*) further observations on the action of chloro- 
form showed that while the results on nerve could be taken either 
way, the experiments on frog’s skin required for their explanation 
the presence of such membranes situated just below the outer 
surface, and that no simple diffusion process would be sufficient. 
So that as far as the membrane is concerned both the “‘ concentra- 
tion cell” and the “diosmotic”’ theories have some experimental 
justification. 

Here the subject must be left as far as the present occasion is 
concerned. For the future we may hope that further experiment 
will enable us to decide between the present series of conflicting . 
hypotheses. 

Although we have considered the matter solely from the stand- 
point of nerve, it is admitted that the same laws govern the life 
of every cell in the body, and the inquiry is really a fundamental 
one into the properties of living matter. To any one who takes 
an interest in anything beyond the routine work of examinations 
such inquiries cannot fail to be of the greatest importance, and it 
is only in physiology that it is necessary to defend the acquisition 
of knowledge for its own sake. 

* Moore and Roaf (**) and Roaf and Alderson (**) have advocated the contrary 


view as far as the action of CHCl, is concerned, but the discussion is too elaborate 
to be continued in this place. 


2 SOME CHAPTERS ON THE 


BIBLIOGRAPHY 


1 J. C. Bose, Comparative Electrophysiology, London, 1907. 

2 A. D. Waller, Proc. Physiol. Soc., J. Physiol., 37, xviii. 

3 J, C. Bose, Response in the Living and Non-Living, London, 1902. 

* F. Gotch, J. Physiol., 28, p. 32. 

5 H. von Baeyer, Verworn’s Zeitschrift, Bd. 2, H. 1, p. 169. 

° F. W. Fréhlich, Verworn’s Zeitschrift, Bd. 3, H. 1, p. 75. 

? K. H. Baas, Pfluger’s Archiv., Bd. 103, p. 276. 

8 A. D. Waller, Lectures on Animal Electricity, vol. i. 

® F. Gotch and v. Horsley, Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc., 182, 1891, p. 514. 

10 (, S. Sherrington, Integrative Action of the Central Nervous System, 


New York, 1906. 


1 Bernstein, Pfluger’s Archiv., Bd. 73, s. 376, 1898. 
2 N. H. Alcock and J. Seemann, Pfluger’s Archiv., Bd. 108, s. 426. 
8 W. Reid and J. S. Macdonald, J. Physiol., 23, p. 100. 


14 M. Lewandousky, Pfliiger’s Archiv., Bd. 73, p. 288; and Inaug. Dissert. 
Halle a. S., 1898. 


%° W. Einthoven, Pfliiger’s Archiv., Bd. 124, s. 246. 

6 F. Gotch and G. J. Burch, J. Physiol., 24, p. 410, 1889. 

™ Bernstein, Pfluger’s Archiv., Bd. 15, p. 289. 

8 Wedenski, Medisch. Zentralbl., 1884, p. 64. 

® Bowditch, Du Bois Reymond’s Archiv., 1890, p. 489. 

0 Brodie and Halliburton, J. Physiol., 28, p. 181, 1892. 

1 S.C. M. Sowton, Proc. Roy. Soc., vol. 66, p. 379. 

2 §. Garten, Beitrage zur Physiol. der Marklosen Nerven, Jena, 1903. 

*3 V. H. Alcock, Proc. Roy. Soc., 1904, vol. 73, p. 166; 1906, vol. 77, p. 


267; 1906, vol. 78, p. 159. 


4 J. Tait and Gunn, Quart. Journ. Exper. Physiol., i. p. 1. 

*% A.D. Waller, Proc. Physiol. Soc., J. Physiol., 25, p. i., and 36, p. xxx. 
*° A. D. Waller, Proc. Physiol. Soc., J. Physiol., 36, xxx. 

7 KH. Pfluger, Pfluger’s Archiv., Bd. 122, p. 593. 

*8 B, Kilvington, British Medical Journal, 1905, i. p. 935 ; 1905, ii. p. 625; 


1907, i. p. 988; 1908, i. p. 1414. 


*® W. Thorner, Verworn’s Zeitschrift, Bd. 8, H. 5, p. 530. 

82 Kilvington and Osborne, J. Physiol., 34, p. 267. 

33 Marinesco, J. f. Psychiat. u. Neurologie, vii. p. 141. 

%4 Halliburton, Mott, and Edmunds, Proc. Roy. Soc., B. 78, p. 259, 1906. 

35 Langley and Anderson, J. Physiol., 30, p. 439. 

3 Lugaro, Neurol. Zentralblatt, 25, p. 786. 

°° Harrison, Ross, Archiv. f. Mikros. Anat. u. Entw., 57, 1901. 

40 Harrison, Ross, Sitzungs b. d. N. Gesell. f. Nat. u. Heilk., 1904. 

“ Ballance and Stewart, The Healing of Nerves, 1902. 

“8 R. Kennedy, Phil. Trans., B. 188, 1897, p. 257; Brit. Med. Journ., 1904, 
\ 


ii, p. 729. 


“4 Head, Rivers, and Sherren, Brain, 28, p. 99, 1905. 
“* Head and Sherren, Brain, 28, p. 116, 1905. 


PHYSIOLOGY OF NERVE 283 


© Head and Thompson, Brain, 29, p. 537, 1906. 

“ Bethe, Alig. Anat. u. Physiol. der Nervensystems, 1903. 

“ Braus, Anat. Anzeiger, 26, p. 433, 1905. 

® Kerr, Graham, Roy. Soc. Edin. Trans., vol. 41, 1905, p. 121. 

® Ballance and Stewart, Brit. Med. Journ., 1903, May 2. 

5 A, Macallum, J. Physiol., 32, p. 95. 

- © Gilbert, Inaug. Dissert., Tubingen, 1898. 

53 Macallum and Menten, Proc. Roy. Soc., B. 77, p. 165, 1906. 

54 4. Macallum, Proc. Roy. Soc., 76, p. 217. 

% Mazwell, J. Biol. Chemistry, iii. 1907, p. 358. 

5° Keith Lucas, J. Physiol., vol. 37, 1908, p. 112. 

5? Bazett, J. Physiol., 36, p. 414, 1908. 

88 Woolley, J. Physiol., 37, p. 122, 1908. 

59 J, S. Macdonald, Proc, Roy. Soc., 67, p. 315, 1900, and p. 325, Lc. 
Thompson-Yates Lab. Reports, 4, p. 2, 1902. J. 8. Macdonald and 8. C. M. 
Sowton, ibid., Feb. 5, 1903. J. S. Macdonald, Proc. Physiol. Soc., J. Physiol., 
Dec. 17, 1904 ; March 18, 1905. Proc. Roy. Soc., 76 B., p. 322. Proc. Physiol. 
Soc., J. Physiol., June 17, 1905. Proc. Roy. Soc., 79, p. 12, 1906. 

© N. H. Alcock and G. Roche Lynch, J. Physiol., 36, p. 93. 

%1 J. H. Ashworth, Proc. Roy. Soc., B., vol. 80, p. 463. 

82 J. S. Macdonald, Science Progress, vol. 2, p. 482. 

6’ W. Sutherland, Am. Journal Physiol., 1906, vol. 17, pp. 266, 297. 
*« N. H. Alcock, Proc. Roy. Soc., 1903, vol. 72, p. 414. 

* yon Miram, Engelmann’s Archiv., 1906, p. 533. 

%® W. Brunings, Pfluger’s Archiv., Bd. 100, p. 367. 

* A, D, Waller, Proc. Physiol. Soc., J. Physiol., 38, p. vi. 

% B, Moore and H. E. Roaf, Proc. Roy. Soc., vol. 73, 1904, p. 382; B., 
vol. 77, 1906, p. 86; Deutsch..Med. Wochensch., 33, p. 1568; Zentralb. 
Physiol., 21, p. 477; Arch. Intern. Physiol., 5, p. 68. 

® H, E. Roaf and E. Alderson, Biochem. Journal, vol. ii. p. 412. 

 ()stwald, Zeitsch. f. Physik. Chemie, vol. 6, 1890. 


7 


RECENT RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALI- 
SATION AND ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE 
CEREBRUM 


By JOSEPH SHAW BOLTON 


INTRODUCTION 


THE experimental study of the functions of the cerebrum, after a 
period of activity lasting through three decades, reached its acme 
in an important research (Sherrington and Griinbaum), which 
resulted in the belated recognition of the histological investigation 
of Bevan Lewis and Henry Clarke (1878) on the cortical localisa- 
tion of the motor area of the brain. Of late years the histological 
has largely replaced the experimental method, and the study of 
cortical localisation, although still in its infancy, may now fairly 
claim to be regarded as a branch ot exact science. 
Owing to the far-reaching importance of the controversy regard- 
ing the neurone theory, the majority of the numerous recent 
publications on the anatomy and histology of the nervous system 
deal with the finer histology of nerve cells and fibres. Of 711 
contributions, for example, which appeared during the years 1905 
and 1906, and which were critically abstracted and reviewed by 
Edinger and Wallenberg in their last report, over 300 papers 
were concerned with this subject, and few of the remainder were 
of direct physiological significance. It is not the purpose of this 
article to deal with purely histological investigations which have 
no immediate bearing on cortical localisation, and therefore no 
reference will be made to such contributions beyond the remark 
that, of those by English writers, the papers of John Turner are . 
especially worthy of attention. 7. 
During the past eight years numerous publications have 
appeared on the subject of cortical localisation by the histological 
- method. In these several contributions the mode of evolution — 
and the functional significance of the different cell layers of the i 
cortex cerebri have been Scneiaanet from both the ontogenetic ‘ 


2 


RECENT RESEARCHES 285 


and the phylogenetic aspects; and the whole cortex cerebri in 

many orders of mammals (including man) has been mapped out 
into various histologically different regions. 

Of these regions, experimental or histo-pathological proof of 
the functions of the precisely-defined psychomotor and visuo- 
sensory areas is complete. In the case of the prefrontal region, 
which is the latest evolved, the most variable, and the most com- 
plexly constructed portion of the human cerebreum, histo-patho- 
logical evidence of its functional significance has’ been derived from 
the examination of cases of mental disease, which method consti- 
tutes an advance on the time-honoured study of cases of gross 
lesion of the brain. 

. Finally, with regard to the localisation of the language centres, 
important papers by Marie and by Monakow, and a lengthy volume 
by Moutier, have been published; and the existence of Broca’s 

: speech area has thereby been seriously threatened if not ren- 

| dered entirely doubtful. Articles on the psychological experiences 

| connected with the different parts of speech, on the language 

| mechanism and its psycho-physiology with regard to the functions 
of the cerebrum, and on the subject of sense-deprivation, have also 
been recently published. 

In the course of the present article, the lamination of the cortex 
cerebri and the functional significance of its several layers will 
first be considered. The subject of cortical localisation by means 
of the histological method will then be detailed. This description 
will be followed by remarks on the mode of evolution of the 
cerebral functions. The higher functions of the human brain will’ 
afterwards be dealt with, and in connection with this matter the 
psycho-physiology of the language mechanism will be discussed. 
The subject of sense-deprivation will next receive attention, as 

_ @ preliminary to the consideration of the recent researches on 
aphasia. The article will be concluded by references to certain 
matters of interest with regard to the functions of the cerebrum, 
and to a number of recent publications of importance, which 
could not be conveniently included in the general text. 


LAMINATION OF THE CorTEX CEREBRI 


i? Since the year 1872, when Meynert published a description of 
___ the structure of the cortex cerebri which, from the general aspect, 


286 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND {™ 


has required little modification, numerous classifications of the 
various layers of which it is composed have appeared. 

The various subdivisions which have been made by the several 
authors, owing to the different methods of preparation which have 
been employed, to the different aspects from which the subject 
has been studied, and to the different regions of the cortex cerebri 
which have been examined, have resulted in the employment 
of different numerals to designate the same or similar layers ; 
and thereby much confusion has resulted. This will be avoided 
during the following description by the employment of such 
terms as clearly indicate the cell layers which are under 
reference. 

Whilst from the aspect of cell form, few if any of the published 
descriptions equal in elaboration, and in probable accuracy, the — 
account given by Cajal in the numerous papers he has produced 
as the result of a systematic employment of various modifications 


_ of the method of Golgi, his classification is at present of histo- 


logical rather than of physiological interest. 

Another and more recent classification, that of Brodmann (1906), 
is more immediately useful, and, in consequence of the elaborate 
and prolonged investigations of the author on the subject of 
cortical localisation, deserves reference here. Brodmann divides 
the cortex cerebri into the following layers :— 

(1) A zonal layer, without cells (the equivalent of the tangential 
layer of Krause).. 

(2) A layer composed of the “ molecular” and “small pyra- 
midal ” layers of other writers. 

(3) A layer of medium and large pyramidal cells. 

(4) An internal granular or stellate layer. 

(5) A layer of ganglionic or deep pyramidal cells. 

(6) A layer of deep spindle-shaped or polymorphous cells. 

For the purposes, however, of this article, which deals with 
the subject from the functional rather than the structural aspect, 
the description has been adopted which was published by the 
present writer in 1900, and which is based on the mode of develop- 
ment of the several lamine of the cortex cerebri. This classifica- 
tion, which has been adopted, amongst others, by Mott and by 
Watson, is as follows :— 

(1) The superficial layer of nerve fibres or “ molecular” layer 
(outer fibre lamina). (Average prefrontal depth, -30054 mm.) 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM — 287 


(2) The layer of small, medium, and large pyramidal cells 
(outer cell lamina). (Average prefrontal depth, ‘83116 mm.) 

(3) The layer of granules (middle cell lamina). (Average pre- 
frontal depth, :22883 mm.) 

(4) The inner layer of nerve fibres or “ inner line of Baillarger,” 
containing large and frequently solitary cells (inner fibre lamina). 
(Average prefrontal depth, ‘23032 mm.) 

(5) The layer of polymorphic cells (imner cell lamina). 
(Average prefrontal depth, -30979 mm.) 

This five-layered type, though subject in different regions to 
structural modifications which probably possess in all cases a 
functional significance, is common to the whole cortex cerebri, 
with the exception of the hippocampus and the pyriform lobe, 
which parts belong to the archipallic, in contradistinction to neo- 
pallic cortex (Elliott Smith). In the psychomotor area, for example, 
the Betz cells lie in the inner fibre lamina, and the middle cell 
lamina is reduced in depth almost to vanishing point. Again, in . 
the visuo-sensory area, the middle cell lamina is hypertrophied 
and duplicated by the interposition of a special fibre layer, the 
line of Gennari.” 

The mode of development of this five-layered type of cortex will 
now be briefly described. 

In the foetus of four months lamination has not begun, and the 
cortex consists solely of a superficial indifferent layer and of a 
deeper layer of undifferentiated neuroblasts. The average depth 
of the former is 0°154665 mm., and of the latter 0°67758 mm., a 
total of 0°832245 mm., which is less than half the normal adult > 
general average depth of 1-90064 mm. | 

For simplicity of exposition, the process of development will 
be first deseribed in the case of each separate layer, and the results 
- will then be summarised. 


Prmary Cet, LAMIN® OF THE Cortex 


Lamina 2, Pyramidal Layer, Outer Cell Layer—The pyramidal 
layer is the last cell layer of the cortex to develop during the 
process of lamination. In a foetus of six months this layer is 
separable from the subjacent middle cell layer owing to the less 
differentiated condition of its cell elements, and it is at this period 
only one-fourth of the depth to which it attains in the adult. At 


ie Se o ’ ea 


288 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND . 


birth and in early infancy it is still little more than one-half of the 
adult depth. 

Lamina 3, Granule Layer, Middle Cell Layer.—The granule layer 
develops in the sixth month of foetal life, and at this period it is 
separable from the superjacent outer cell or pyramidal layer owing ~ 
to the more differentiated condition of its cell elements. At this 
period it is already one-half of the adult depth, and by the time 
of birth it has attained to a depth which is nearly three-fourths 
of this. 

Lamina 5, Polymorphic Layer, Inner Cell Layer—The poly- 
morphic layer is the first cell layer to appear, and it is separated 
off from the rest of the partially differentiated neuroblasts of the 
cortex by the development of the fourth or inner fibre lamina at 
the sixth month of foetal life. The polymorphic layer is already, — 
at the period referred to, about three-fourths of the adult depth, 
and it undergoes a slow further development until after birth. In 
a child of six weeks it has attained a depth which is within 18 per 
cent. of the adult normal. 


PRIMARY FisRE LAMINA OF THE CorRTEX 


Lamina 1, Superficial Layer, Outer Fibre Layer —At the fourth 
month of foetal life the cortex consists of this layer and of a 
deeper undifferentiated mass of neuroblasts. The layer under 
description is already about one-half of the adult depth, and it 
remains unchanged until the development of lamination in the 
sixth month. At birth, however, it has attained to a depth which 
is about two-thirds of the adult normal. It is probable that its 
further development to the normal adult depth occurs in associa- 
tion with that of the subjacent second, pyramidal, or outer cell 
layer. : 

ees 4, Inner Fibre Layer, Inner Line of Baillarger—This 
layer appears in the sixth month of foetal life, and almost at once 
attains to nearly the normal adult depth. The cleavage of the Vi 
partially differentiated neuroblasts of the cortex into an upper and = 
a lower portion by the development of this layer is the most striking i 
feature of the process of lamination. In view of what will be 4 
stated later concerning the functions carried on by the inner cell 
layer, this cleavage of the neuroblasts is an occurrence of the 


greatest significance. 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM 289 


Summary.—The inner cell layer, therefore, appears before the 
others, in consequence of a cleavage of the undifferentiated neuro- 
blasts into an upper and a lower portion, and it is almost at once 
of a depth which is about 75 per cent. of the adult normal. It 
remains at almost a stationary depth until after birth. Its depth 
in a child of six weeks is 82 per cent. of the adult normal. 

The middle cell layer is differentiated next in order, and is 
almost at once about one-half of the normal adult depth. It 
gradually increases in thickness, and at birth it has attained to a 
depth which is 75 per cent. of the adult normal. 

The outer cell layer is the last layer of the cortex to be 
differentiated, and at this time it is only one-fourth of the normal 
adult depth. It gradually increases in thickness, and in an infant 
of six weeks it has attained to a depth which is about 60 per cent. 
of the adult normal. 

In the adult human cerebrum, as will at once be seen from 
the measurements given on page 287, the outer cell lamina or 
pyramidal layer, in spite of its late evolution, becomes the promi- 
nent layer of the cortex, and in depth is greater than the depth 
of the combined layers 3, 4, and 5, which lie below it. The 
functional significance of the facts mentioned in this description 
will be referred to later. 


SS 


The above account of the mode of evolution of the cortex cerebri 
has recently been confirmed by G. A. Watson from the phylogenetic 
aspect. This writer, whilst hitherto paying especial attention to the 
insectivora, has carefully studied the cortex cerebri of many orders 
of mammals. He adopts the above classification of the cortical layers, 
but, for reasons which he details, employs a slightly different termi- 
nology, grouping the first and second lamine together under the term 
** supra-granular,” and the fourth and fifth together under the term 

_ *infra-granular.” He remarks :— 


“ Regarded from the developmental aspect—ontogenetic and phylo- 
genetic—the facts adduced support the thesis that the mammalian 
cerebral cortex (neopallium) is built up primarily on an infra-grandlar 
basis, 7.e. the infra-granular portion is the earliest to appear in the 
process of development, very quickly reaches maturity, and in the 
adult, especially if average size of component nerve cell is taken into 

consideration, presents remarkably little difference in absolute depth 
in one of the lowest mammals and in the highest. 
“The granular layer may be said to be the next addition to the 


. t 


Se x , 
ill Ms = , - 


290 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


cortex. Ontogenetically it appears shortly after the infra-granular 
portion of the cortex, and it reaches its maximum development in 
depth and definiteness in the projection spheres of the cerebrum. The 
last layer of the cortex to appear ontogenetically is the supra-granular 
(pyramidal). It is the slowest of all the layers to reach maturity. 
It is scarcely existent at all in certain regions in some of, if not all, 
the lowest mammals, and even at its best in the latter it reaches 
but a slight absolute depth as compared with its depth in practically 
every region of the neopallium in the human subject.” 


The above description of the mode of development of the cortex 
cerebri is illustrated in the following figure, which indicates, in 
graphic form, the five primary cell and fibre laminz of the cortex 
of foetuses of four and six months, a new-born child, a normal 
human adult, and a mole. It will be at once seen that the cortex 
of the human adult differs from that of the insectivore chiefly in 
the degree of development of the second, pyramidal, or outer cell - 
lamina. In spite of the human brain being one of the largest and 
that of the mole one of the smallest in the mammalian phylum, the 
actual average depths of the granular and infra-granular portions 
of the cortex (laminz 3, 4, and 5) are very similar in the two. 


It may here be remarked that Brodmann, in the course of his 
prolonged investigations into the structure of the cerebral cortex of 
the mammalia, has incidentally and independently (1906) made similar 
observations to those of Watson. He states that, during the ascent 
of the mammalian series, his layer 3, which is substantially the outer 
cell lamina or pyramidal layer of the writer and the supra-granular 
layer of Watson, increases in depth, whilst his layer 6, which is 
equivalent to the inner cell lamina 5 of the writer and the lower 
part of the infra-granular region of Watson, on the whole diminishes 
in depth. Brodmann also makes certain generalisations regarding 
the variability of the individual layers of the cortex cerebri, which, 
though they confirm the individual observations of several other 
writers, are worthy of reference here owing to the wide field covered 
by the investigations of this author. He remarks that his layer 4 
(granule layer of other writers) is the most variable; that his layers 3 
and 5 (medium and large pyramids, and infra-granular pyramidal- 
shaped cells) exhibit the greatest histological differentiation ; and that 
his layer 6 (polymorphic layer or inner cell lamina of the writer) 
exhibits the greatest variation in breadth. He further remarks that 
the psychomotor and striate (visuo-sensory) areas of the cortex cerebri 
are most highly developed in primates, 


‘daop 00} Jey Moutos poyuasasddr o1ojatat{} St UWUM[OO panty dy) Ut 

(xoq10o re[nUwis-vajut) yvd_popeys-A[ yep oy} andy oy} UT “FZ. OF ZF. “ZIA ‘pounsvout suautoeds oy} Ut yydap, 
outs ay} Jo AT[wotjowid o1OM PM] Wiogq-Mou PUB sHywWy ,SYJUOUL XIS OY} UT XoZIO0 IVTNURIT-eAyUT OY.—*9joAT 

“AP{Up popeys ("A puw *at) Avpnavis-eayuy $ yuwq yor soAvy ([eprumerdd) avueis-eidng —-uo0ppOgh aq’ 

Aq os[e ore a[OFY OY} JO X9z.100 at]} Jo aso} : UOjfog Meyg “¢ Aq (OGL ‘Me ‘TOA ‘Abopounayy fo svaryoup) ., BIFWOMLAG 
PUL VIZWOUTY Jo sisvg [VOLTFo]OYsSIF] OY, ,, WOIJ WOR} OL SasBO MOF 4SLY OY} UL S}UOMAIMSvaUL OTJOULOLOIW OY, 

‘QOL Of} pur Qlupe uvUNY [eULion oy} “(plM[> W1oq-MouU puB snywy SY}NOUT XIs pUe IMOJ) s}UOUTe WeUMY TeULIO 
Ur S1aAB[ [VOT}L09 [BAqa.ta0 oY} Jo sYydap oALjEfer ayy AozetUrxordde soyeysnt[[—(J061 “WOSsyeM, “VY “D) LT “OMT 


region 1s 


PW UL AMPY UeUMH [PULION, = “PITY WOT MON | “ETQADY SIUC O srqaoy SHqUOW & ‘sg . 
eee weusta: Foes 3 
WK H y 
SOMES, 
f—eaga etry 
oe § wns 


lamin of the cortex cereb 


The Visuo-sensory Area.—The lamination in this 


primary 


f the human cortex cerebri, will now be briefly detailed, 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM 291 
Certain facts regarding the lamination, the period and the mode 


of its evolution, and the functional significance of certain special 


$ 
as a preliminary to the consideration of the functional significance 


E 
> . re 
3 kiz L2P(n20 154 & 4 
; ‘S TRL erEy ~ weony -aImpy. _ “PID wg MOR “STO SPOS = “STI Syqoy & ; & ht 
2 PP OL uewny Ts0K ‘eyroury ueUNy TeULION S\ en - 


Ps a 
292 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND — 


specialised from the five-layered type already referred to, and - 
consists of the following layers :— 

1. The superficial layer of nerve fibres (outer fibre lamina). 

2. The layer of pyramidal cells (outer cell lamina). 

3a. The outer layer of granules. 

3b. The middle layer of nerve fibres or “line of | (middle cell 
Gennari ” containing solitary cells of Meynert. lamina). 

3c. The inner layer of granules. 

4. The inner layer of nerve fibres or “ inner line of Baillarger,” 
containing solitary cells of Meynert (nner fibre lamina). 

5. The layer of polymorphic cells (inner cell lamina). 

The specialisation of the visuo-sensory cortex consists, there- 
fore, in essentials, in a duplication of the third primary lamina of 
the cortex, and in the interposition, between the double layer, of . 
a layer composed of nerve fibres. Of this triple layer, layer 3a is 
an additional feature; layer 3b is an exaggeration of a thin fibre 
band, the “outer line of Baillarger,’ which in the adult cortex 
lies between the second and third primary lamine, but is only 
visible in sections prepared to show nerve fibres; and layer 3c is 
the original third primary lamina increased in depth. 

In congenital or long-standing blindness the depth of. layer 3b 
is decreased by nearly 50 per cent., and that of layer 3a is 
decreased by more than 10 per cent., owing to atrophy of the 
optic radiations. The other layers of the cortex are unchanged 
in depth by the existence of blindness. 

These facts prove that the cortical region under consideration 
is the projection centre for visual impressions or the visuo- 
sensory area. 

The cortex in this region rapidly attains to mature develop- 
ment. The second, pyramidal, or outer cell lamina thus develops 
much earlier than is the case in the visuo-psychic and prefrontal 
regions. For example, in infants of one and three months re- 
spectively, its depth is already 84 per cent. of the adult normal. 
A further important fact is that in the normal adult this cell layer 
of the visuo-sensory area\is only about five-ninths of its depth in 
the visuo-psychic and prefrontal regions of the cerebral cortex. 

The Visuo-psychic Region.—At the periphery of the visuo-sensory 
area, where this passes in each direction into the neighbouring 
cortex, termed by the writer “ visuo-psychic” (which word has 
since been adopted by Campbell and by Mott), an abrupt change 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM 293 


in lamination takes place, layer 3b, the line of Gennari, suddenly 
ceasing, and layers 3a and 3c, the two layers of granules, running 
into one and becoming layer 3 of the visuo-psychic region, which 


is thus of the ordinary five-layered type. 


Congenital or long-standing blindness causes no modification of 
the lamination of the visuo-psychic region, which fact proves that 
this region possesses no “ visuo-sensory ” functions. 

The cortex of this region reaches maturity later than the visuo- 
sensory, but earlier than the prefrontal. The second, pyramidal, 
or outer cell Jayer in infants of one and three months is respec- 
tively nearly two-thirds and more than three-quarters of the 
normal adult depth. This layer reaches to practically the same 
adult depth as in the prefrontal region. In cases of mental disease 
this layer does not vary in depth according to the degree of 
dementia existing in the patients, though a small and practically 
constant decrease in depth, which may be due either to sub- 
development or to retrogression, is evident in such cases. 

The Prefrontal Region.—The cortex of this region is late in 
reaching maturity. As hes been stated, the pyramidal or outer 
cell lamina is the last layer of the cortex to develop. Jt is visible 
owing to the undifferentiated condition of its constituent neuro- 
blasts in a foetus of six months, and is at this time only one- 
quarter of the normal adult.depth. In infants at birth and at 
the age of six weeks it is still less than two-thirds of the normal 
adult depth. 

It is the only cell layer of the cortex cerebri which varies - 
measurably in depth in “ normal” brains. 

In cases of mental disease it is under- -developed to different 
degrees, not only in idiots and imbeciles, in the severer grades of 
which its depth is only two-thirds of the adult normal, but also, 
and here to a lesser extent, in chronic and recurrent lunatics 
without dementia. The degree of its retrogression in demented 
patients varies directly, and to an equally marked degree as its 
subdevelopment in the case of amentia, with the amount of 
dementia existing in the respective cases. 


Of these three” regions of the cortex, therefore, the visuo- 
sensory area first reaches maturity. Though highly specialised, 
the cortex of this area is, however, not well developed, as the 


outer cell lamina or pyramidal layer remains at but five-ninths of 


* - F | 


294 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


the maximum adult depth. The visuo-psychic cortex attains to 
maturity later, but then becomes of the maximum adult depth. 

The prefrontal cortex is the last to reach maturity. The outer | 
cell lamina in this region varies in its degree of development 
according to the mental capacity of the individual and in its degree 

of retrogression according to the amount of mental decadence 
which exists. Further, it varies in measurable depth in “ normal ” 
individuals. 

All these variations regarding the degree of development occur 
in the pyramidal or outer cell layer, which is the last lamina to 
be evolved, and the most important feature of the human adult 
cortex cerebri. 

The inner cell lamina or polymorphic layer of the cortex, which 
is the first to be evolved, is, on the other hand, in all the three © 
regions referred to above, of extremely constant average depth. 
A very slight decrease in the average depth of this layer occurs, 
in the prefrontal region, in cases of mild cerebral and mental 
degeneracy (high-grade amentia), and in cases of chronic insanity 
with moderate dementia. A considerable decrease, on the other 
hand, exists, also in the prefrontal region, in more marked aments 
(whether normal foetuses and infants, or idiots and imbeciles) and 
in gross dements who are unable to carry on the ordinary animal 
functions, such as attending to their own wants, &c. 


FUNCTIONS OF THE DIFFERENT CELL LAMINZ OF 
THE CORTEX CEREBRI 


As has been stated, the cerebral cortex consists of three primary 
cell lamine (layers 2, 3, and 5), and of two primary fibre lamine 
(layers 1 and 4). The evidence above adduced regarding the 
ontogenetic and phylogenetic development of these lamin enables _ 
certain definite conclusions to be drawn with regard to their 
functional significance. 

The outer cell lamina (2, pyramidal), to which may be added 
the outer fibre lamina (1), is the prominent feature of the human 
cortex, and constitutes a “higher level” basis for the carrying 
on of the cerebral functions. It is the last cell layer of the cortex 
to be evolved and the first to undergo retrogression. In the 
visuo-sensory area (projection sphere) the outer cell lamina rapidly 
attains maturity, but is then only about five-ninths of its depth 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM = 295 


in the visuo-psychic and prefrontal regions of the cerebral cortex. 
In the visuo-psychic region this layer develops earlier than in the 
prefrontal region, but later than in the visuo-sensory area, and 
eventually attains to practically the same adult depth as in the 
former. In this region it is of practically constant adult depth, 
and does not vary in measurable depth according to the mental 
capacity of the individual. In the prefrontal region the outer 
cell lamina develops later than in the other regions referred to 
It is the only cell layer of the cortex cerebri which varies definitely 
in measurable depth in “normal” brains. It is under-developed to 
different degrees according to the mental capacity of the individual 
| in persons exhibiting various grades of mental subevolution ; and 
| it undergoes degrees of retrogression which correspond to the 
amount of dementia existing in cases which permanently suffer 
from diminution or loss of their mental powers. 

The second, pyramidal, or outer cell lamina of the human cerebral 
cortex, therefore, subserves the “‘ psychic” or associational functions 
of the cerebrum. These functions are pre-eminent in the prefrontal 
region (centre of higher association); they are less important in 
the visuo-psychic region (region of lower association) ; and they are 
of least importance in the visuo-sensory region (projection sphere). 
These three regions are therefore of different grades in the hierarchy 
of cerebral function. 


The results obtained by Watson during his histological investiga- 
tion of the cerebral cortex of the mammalia supply the complement 
from the phylogenetic aspect to the above (ontogenetic) conclusions. . 
Watson finds that in the insectivora the pyramidal or outer cell layer 
is in a rudimentary condition, though the lower layers of the cortex 
approximate in depth to that of these layers.in the normal human 
adult. Further, the pyramidal or outer cell layer is better developed 
 . in the rodents than in the insectivores; it is again better developed 
in the ungulates and in the carnivores than in the rodents; and it 
is strikingly more developed in the primates than in the carnivores. 
He therefore functionally correlates this layer with the educa- 
bility and general intelligence which appear in an increasing degree 
during the ascent of the mammalian scale. 
Watson remarks :— 
“The supra-granular (pyramidal) layer—which is, relatively to 
the infra-granular cortex, so poorly developed at birth—is slow in 
reaching maturity, and is, even at its best, in certain lower mammals, 


296 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


such as the insectivora, only of an insignificant absolute depth— 
subserves the higher associations, the capacity for which is shown by 
the educability of the animal. It has therefore to do with all those 
activities which it is obvious that the animal has acquired (or per- 
fected) by individual experience, and with all the possible modifica- 
tions of behaviour which may arise in relation to some novel situation, 
hence with what is usually described as indicating intelligent as apart 
from instinctive acts, the former being not merely accompanied but 
controlled by consciousness (Lloyd Morgan).” 


The middle cell lamina (3, granule) is developed after the fifth, 
polymorphic, or inner cell lamina, and before the second, pyramidal, 
or outer cell lamina. In the prefrontal region of a foetus of six 
months it has just become differentiated, by commencing specialisa- 
tion of its constituent cells, from the superjacent second or outer - 
cell lamina, and it is already one-half of the normal adult depth. 
In a child at birth it has become three-fourths of the normal adult 
depth. . 

In the visuo-sensory area the optic radiations end in the midst 
of a hypertrophied and duplicated third or granule layer. The 
duplication is due to the interposition in the midst of the hyper- 
trophied third or granule layer of a well-marked fibre band, the 
line of Gennari, which fibre band, as has already been stated, is 
a hypertrophy of the outer of the two horizontal interradiary fibre 
plexuses of the adult cortex, namely, the “ outer line of Baillarger.” . 
In old-standing or congenital optic atrophy, the outer (and addi- 
tional) of the granule layers is decreased in thickness by more than 
10 per cent., and the line of Gennari is decreased in thickness by 
nearly 50 per cent. . 

As has been pointed out by Watson, a hypertrophied third, 
granule, or middle cell lamina appears to be characteristic of the 
projection areas of the cerebrum. In the case of the visuo- 
sensory area (visual projection sphere), the third or granule layer 
first becomes definitely duplicated in the order primates, though 
slight indications of duplication occur in the higher carnivores. 

The third, granule, or middle cell lamina, therefore, primarily 
subserves the reception or immediate transformation of afferent im- 
pressions, whether these arrive directly from the lower sensory neurones — 
- or indirectly through other regions of the cerebrum. . 

‘The fifth, polymorphic, or inner cell lamina is the first cell lamina 
of the cortex cerebri to be differentiated during the process of 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM ~ 297 


lamination. In the prefrontal region of a foetus of six months it 
is separated off from the rest of the cortex by the fourth or inner 
fibre lamina, and is already within 29 per cent. of the normal 
adult depth. In a child of six weeks it has advanced to within 
18 per cent. of the normal adult depth. 

It is of extremely constant average adult depth. 

A very slight decrease in the depth of this layer exists in cases 
of high-grade amentia and of chronic insanity with moderate 
dementia. A considerable decrease, on the other hand, exists in 
more marked aments (whether foetuses and infants, or idiots 
and imbeciles), and in gross dements who are unable to carry on 
_ the ordinary animal functions, such as attending to their own 
wants, &c. 

The fifth, polymorphic, or inner cell lamina of the human cerebrum, 
therefore, in association with the fourth or inner fibre layer, subserves 
the lower voluntary and instinctive activities of the animal economy, 
and thus forms a lower level basis for the carrying on of cerebral 
function. 

Final proof of the last statement has, from the phylogenetic 
aspect, been afforded by the researches of Watson, who has shown 
that the “ infra-granular ” region contains the important cell layers 
of the lower mammalia, and is very little inferior in depth (see 
Fig. 1, page 291) to the normal adult human depth of the conjoined 
fourth and fifth lamine. ) 


Watson’s conclusions on this question are as follows :— 

“The infra-granular portion of the cortex (iv. and v.) (omitting 
the constituent cells which possess motor or analogous functions) is 
concerned especially with the associations necessary for the perform- 
ance of the instinctive activities, that i is, all those which are innate 
and require for their fulfilment no experience or education. These 
form the basis of many complex actions necessary for the preservation 
of the individual and the species, such as the seeking appropriate 
shelter and protection, the hunting for food—each after his own kind 
—and the quest of the opposite sex. Although these acts may be 
accompanied by consciousness, there is no evidence to show that this 
is ‘focal’ or that essentially they are controlled by consciousness 
(Lloyd Morgan). It is believed that lower mammals have provided in 
the infra-granular cortex (which is relatively so fully matured at birth 
in them as well as in man, and which in the adult, even in animals 


aS gd down in the mammalian scale, reaches such a great degree of 


298 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND — 


absolute development) a sufficient cerebral cortical mechanism for the 
performance of these lower associations. The actions which are the 
outcome of such associations are often complex, and as an instance 
of the class included under this heading the tunnelling of the mole 
may be mentioned. Such more or less stereotyped actions may show 
signs of improvement in their performance, firstly, as the result of 
perfection by use of an inherited mechanism, and secondly, as the 
result of the intermingling of activities for which it is concluded that 
the supra-granular layer is responsible. In the latter case, however, 
the actions would merge into those which are more properly described 
as habitual intelligent, or into the class of ‘incomplete instincts’ 
(Lloyd Morgan), or ‘ mixed instincts ’ (Romanes).” 


Watson finally remarks, on the subject of the functions of the 
“‘ supra-granular”’ (outer cell layer) and “ infra-granular” (inner 
fibre layer and inner cell layer) :— 

“In practical animal behaviour the two sets of processes are pro- 
bably more or less constantly interwoven, the higher activities (supra- 
granular layer) coming to the aid of the lower as far as the capability 
of the animal allows. In the case of the lower mammals (e.g. insecti- 
vora) the limits of this capability are comparatively soon reached, 
and correspondingly these mammals possess a relatively poor supra- 
granular layer. Many of these lower mammals have adopted a safe } 
mode of life, others have resorted to fecundity. With these, which 
may, for present purposes, be termed extraneous aids to survival, 
their essentially instinctive activities have been relatively sufficient 
to ensure their continued existence. There has, therefore, in these 
mammals, been little necessity for the development of a supra-granular 
layer, the infra-granular portion of the pallium providing most of the 
necessary cortical physical basis required for practical behaviour. 4 

“The infra-granular layers, with the reservation to which refer- 
ence has been made, thus constitute the earlier developed and more 
fundamental associational system of the cerebral cortex; the supra- 
granular layer, a higher and accessory system superadded, and of 
any considerable functional importance only in certain regions in 
lower mammals, such as the insectivora.” 


Before the question of cortical localisation receives attention, an 
important communication by Mott (1904), on the progressive evolu- 
tion of the structure and functions of the visual cortex in mammalia, 
calls for reference, as a prominent feature of the article is the con- 
sideration paid to the cell lamination of the visuo-sensory and visuo- 
psychic regions. The paper deals with the subject from not only 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM 299 


the histological but also the clinico-anatomical and experimental 
aspects, and contains a mass of interesting information. The author 
_ describes the visual cortex of the insectivora, rodents, marsupials, 
ungulates, carnivora, lemurs, and primates. He arrives at the 
following conclusions :— 


“There is a correlation of structure and function as exhibited by 
a progressive complexity of cell lamination of the visual cortex in 
mammalia from the insectivora to primates. The more the animal 
depends on vision as a directive faculty in its preservation the more 
complex is the structure. 

“The transition of uniocular panoramic to perfect binocular 
stereoscopic vision shows successive stages in the number of direct 
fibres until, in the primates, there is semidecussation and, as far as 
my observations go, this may be correlated with a progressive develop- 
ment in the layer of higher associational pyramidal cells lying above 
the layer of granules. 

“The progressive evolution of vision as a directive faculty is 
simultaneous with a motor adaptation, especially related to the mode 
of feeding and defence rather than to a particular species. 

“ Carnivorous animals, especially cats, therefore, have their eyes set 
forward, abundant direct fibres, and good binocular vision, to enable 
them to seize their rapidly moving prey with their teeth or paw. 

“ Better motor adaptation, as Sherrington has independently 
suggested from his flicker observations, then is probably the essential 
cause of the direct path of the optic fibres and binocular vision. 

“It is, however, in the primates that we have semidecussation 
of the optic fibres, a macula lutea, eye movements in all directions 
independent of head movements. Convergence and perfect binocular . 
stereoscopic vision associated with the hand, which, in the apes, be- 
comes the principal executive agent in the procuring of food, defence, 
and flight. Visual images are now always associated with impressions 
| of the exploring hand, and the ideas of form, substance, extension, 
~ and qualities of objects are the: complex of the visual and tactile 
kinesthetic images, and capable of endless variations. This we may 
connect with the appearance in the zoological series of an occipital 
lobe, a line of Gennari visible to the naked eye, a deep layer of 
pyramids with a double layer of granules in the visuo-sensory striate 
| area. But even more important than this is the appearance of a 

definite associational zone in which there is a much greater depth 
F of pyramidal cells, the third layer of which is characterised by very 
large pyramids serving as higher complex association neurones between 
the visual cortex and the auditory and tactile motor areas. As we 


re 
300 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


rise in the scale of primates this associational zone increases with 
the more perfect specialisation of the fore-limbs for manipulation 
and the erect posture, and this we may correlate with the increase 
in area of the associational or visuo-psychic zone, the increasing 
development of the parietal lobe, and the pushing back and infolding 
of the striate visuo-sensory cortex, so that in the highest types of 
man it comes to occupy the infolded calcarine region of the mesial 
strface, although some lower types still preserve the anthropoidal 
character. It is probable that the same causes give rise to the shifting 
forward of the anterior motor eye centres.” 

In this connection reference may be made to a paper by Wilfred 
Harris. This writer discusses the question of binocular and stereo- 
scopic vision in man and other vertebrates, with regard to the de- ~ 
cussation of the optic nerves, ocular movements, and the pupil light 
reflex. 


CorTICAL LOCALISATION BY MEANS OF THE HISTOLOGICAL 
METHOD 


The researches of Flechsig on the development of the human 
brain by a study of the process of myelination are so well known 
to neurologists that any description of them may seem superfluous. 

The conclusions at which he arrived are, however, of funda- 
mental importance, and form the origin, if not entirely the basis, 
of our present knowledge of cortical localisation by histological 
methods. Again, his generalisations in the gross have success- 
fully passed through a storm of discussion and dissent which is 
almost unsurpassed in the history of research. Lastly, even at 
the present time, except by neurologists, the researches of Flechsig 
are by no means as generally understood and appreciated as they 
merit. A short description of the main features of the investiga- 
tions of this writer is therefore perhaps not entirely out of place 
as an introduction to the subject under consideration. 

By studying the exact period of myelination of different parts 
of the cerebrum in the human fetus and infant, Flechsig was 
enabled to divide the cortex cerebri into the two great classes 
of “sensory centres”?\and “centres of association.” The former 
myelinate earlier than the latter, and possess a well-marked system 
of fibres of projection. ‘The latter myelinate later and are rich in 
long systems of fibres of association. The difference is really one 


of degree only, as Flechsig does not deny that fibres of projection 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM — 301 


exist in the association centres, and that association systems exist 
in the sensory spheres. 

In the whole cerebrum he described in 1898 no less than forty 
separate myelogenetic fields which develop at different periods. 
In 1901 he reduced these to thirty-six, and in 1903 he made no 
alteration in the number although he stated that later investi- 
gation might eventually cause him to do so. 

Of the sensory spheres he describes four, namely, for (1) bodily 
sensibility ; (2) visual; (3) auditory ; and (4) olfactory and gus- 
tatory sensations. (1) has eight separate myelogenetic fields, and 
each of the others has three each. 

Of the centres of association he originally described four, namely, 
the frontal, the parietal, the temporal, and the insular. Later he 
combined the temporal and parietal centres into one, the great 
posterior centre of association. In 1900, however, owing to his 
discovery of a centre of projection in the gyrus subangularis, he 

again separated these centres. 

In Fig. 2 these sensory spheres and centres of association are 
indicated. 

In the temporal and parietal centres of association there exist, 
according to Flechsig, peripheral zones, which develop earlier, and 
central zones, which develop later; the former adjoin the sensory 
centres, and are united to them by numerous arcuate fibres. 

In the frontal centre of association similar zones exist, but 
their disposition is much more complex. 

The insular centre of association, and also that in the precuneus, 
consist of peripheral zones only. Flechsig is of opinion that the 
peripheral zones may be intermediate types between the central 
territories of association and the sensory projection spheres. 

Hence, of the centres of association, the frontal exhibits the 

_ greatest complexity, the temporal and. parietal are intermediate in 
structure, and the insular and that in the precuneus are the least 
complex of the types. 


ee ee ee el _— — = 


Flechsig’s views on the functions of the central territories of the 
areas of association are as follows: “ The central territories of the 
zones of association (especially the middle of the angular gyrus, the 
third temporal convolution, and the anterior half of the second frontal 
convolution) are apparently the nodal points of the long systems of 
association, whilst the peripheral zones only feebly show these char- 


fa 


- 
‘ 


302 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


“The central territories are terminal territories; they are essen- 
tially characteristic of the human brain. Isolated destruction of * 
these is never accompanied by phenomena pointing to disturbance 


e sibility Project; 
a of = On Cenp,, 


UONeDosse 
U9 Ieju08y 


JO 


gustatory projection 
centre ; 7 


a/ - 
Centre of association 


any sensibility Projecti 
g0 - * 


void 


ye1gOSse 


uote! 
yo 243492 189 


_ Visual 
projection centre 


Insular centre 
of association 


projection 
: n 
Parietal and temporal oe 


céntres of association 


Fic. 2 (after Flechsig).—In these diagrams the ‘‘centres of projection” and 
‘centres of association” are mapped out according to Flechsig. The small dots 


are placed in the chief focus of each centre of projection. Around these chief foci » 
are the regions to which a smaller number of fibres of projection pass ; these are 4 
indicated by larger dots. These figures, which represent the different cortical | 


spheres indicated by Flechsig ‘as the result of his embryological studies, should be 
compared with Figs. 3 and 4, and 5 and 6, which illustrate the various areas into 
which the adult cerebral cortex has been subdivided by Campbell and Brodmann 
respectively. 


of motion or sensation. Motor phenomena, which occasionally 
accompany lesions of these territories, must be interpreted as actions 
a distance, 

“The central territories of the zones of association. are in more 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM 303 


or less direct relationship, each with several sensorial zones, some 
with all. 

“ After their bilateral removal intelligence is affected, and especi- 
ally is association of ideas interrupted. These central territories are 
thus probably of great importance for the exercise of intellectual 
activity, for the formation of mental images composed of several 
sensory qualities, for the accomplishment of arts, such as the naming 
of objects, speaking, &c. These functions are especially interfered 
with in affections of the posterior centres of association. Clinical 
observation establishes this fact, and thus justifies the legitimacy 
of our division of the cerebral cortex into sensory centres (centres of 
projection) and centres of association.” (Archiv de Neurologie, i., 
- 1900, pp. 337-338.) 


The views of Flechsig were severely criticised by Hitzig, who, 
4 however, is disposed to admit their general truth in a less positive 
form; and by Vogt, who directly contradicted many of Flechsig’s 
conclusions, and especially the thesis of the constancy and regu- 
larity of myelination of the different systems of fibres. He is 
inclined to look upon the majority of either early or late 
myelinated fibres as projection fibres, and he regards the method 
of Flechsig, though not valueless, as inferior to the more recent 
methods of Campbell and Brodmann and himself, namely, the 
. study of the cell and fibre architecture of the adult cerebrum. 
Monakow, in his criticism, drew attention to the small proportion 
which the projection fibres form of the total mass of cortical fibres 

| of any of the convolutions, and he thinks that it is not possible, 
however roughly it be done, to define the regions which are poor. 

in these fibres and those which are abundantly supplied with them. 

Sachs, whilst admitting that the sense centres are sharply defined 

off at an early period from the rest of the cortex, considered it 
impossible to prove later, when myelination has advanced, that 
medullated projection fibres do not pass to the centres of associa- 

tion. Bianchi also has for many years strongly combated the 
generalisations of Flechsig. He is not satisfied that myelination 

of the various bundles of cortical fibres follows a constant law or 

- occurs in a regular sequence. He denies that the phenomena of 
anatomical evolution need necessarily presuppose the existence 
of functional activity of corresponding grade. He remarks, for 
example, that there may be complete myelination of the supposed 
centre for reading in the brain of an imbecile who has never learned 


| 


S “ea 
304 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND — 


toread. He is of the opinion that the only region of the brain which 
can be regarded as purely associational in function is the prefrontal 
area, which is generally acknowledged to possess no fibres of pro- 
jection; and, excluding the language zone, he considers that the 
whole post- and infra-Rolandic portion of the cortex cerebri is 
perceptive in function. ‘One can symbolise the cerebral mantle 
as a state with a representative system—a parliament and govern- 
ment. The mantellar parliament would be constituted by the 
perceptive zones. . . . The central government would be repre- 
sented by the frontal lobes.” (Textbook, 1906, p. 172.) Bianchi, 
in other words, denies that the post- and infra-Rolandic parts of 
the cerebrum can be subdivided into areas of projection and zones 
of association. 

As a general statement it may be remarked that the conclusions 
of Flechsig, in their essential features, have been widely accepted, 
and that, whilst the projection areas, as has been already pointed 
out, probably occupy neither the identical position nor the same 
extent of cortex in the adult brain that they do in the foetus and 
infant, it may be assumed.that a great parieto-temporal centre 
of association exists posteriorly, and a more complex prefrontal 
centre of association anteriorly, the insular and precuneal centres 
being less complex in type, and probably of less importance, than 
either of the former. 

After this introduction to the question in its more general 
aspects, the subject of cortical localisation by histological methods 
will now be considered in detail. 

The first important paper on cortical localisation was that of 
Bevan Lewis and Henry Clarke, published in 1878, on the cortical 
localisation of the motor area of the brain. This communication, ~ 
which localised the motor area in front of the furrow of Rolando, 
attracted little attention owing to the fact that the conclusions 
contained in it were opposed to the results of the numerous 
physiological experiments which, during the last two decades of 
the nineteenth century, largely monopolised the field of inquiry 
into the functions of the cerebrum. The observations of Lewis 
and Clarke have, however, at last obtained complete if belated 
recognition in consequence of the experimental work of Sherrington 
and Griinbaum, recently confirmed by Oscar Vogt, and the his- 
’ tological researches of Campbell and of Brodmann. It is an 
interesting and in many respects a fortunate fact that the experi- 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM — 305 


mental method, which was responsible for the non-recognition of 
an important contribution to our knowledge, was also the method 
which first supplied evidence of its truth. 

With the exception of the elaborate cell and lamination studies 
| of Hammarberg, which were published posthumously in 1895, and 
, of a paper by Schlapp in 1898 on the cortex of the ape, no con- 
tributions of direct interest appeared until the year 1900, when 

the present writer communicated his paper on the exact histo- 
logical localisation of the visual area of the human cerebral 
cortex. 

In this paper the exact limits of the human “ visuo-sensory 
area’ were mapped out in six hemispheres derived from persons 
with normal vision and from cases of long-standing and congenital 
blindness, and the surrounding zone of cortex, the lamination of 
which was unaffected by the absence of vision, was described 
under the term “ visuo-psychic.” 

The limits given to the former area indicated the approximate 
truth of the opinion at this time held by Henschen and supported 
by the embryological researches of Flechsig and the clinico-patho- 
logical investigations of Seguin, Vialet, and others; and their 
correctness has since been confirmed by Campbell, Brodmann, 
Elliot Smith, and many other investigators. 

: Since this paper was published, numerous contributions have 
appeared on the subject of cortical localisation by the histological 
method. 


Of these the chief are by Brodmann (1902-1907), Campbell (1905), . 
W. Kolmer (1901), Hermanides and Képpen (1903), Képpen and 
Lowenstein (1905), Elliott Smith (1904-1907), O. Vogt (1906), Mott 
(1907), G. A. Watson (1907), and Mott and Kelley (1908). 


. In these papers the whole cortex’in many orders of mammals 
has been mapped out into various histologically different regions, 
but, except in the case of the psychomotor and visuo-sensory 
areas, experimental or histo-pathological proof of the function of 
these regions is not yet complete. 

The most elaborate of these researches are those of Brodmann 
and of Campbell. These authors have independently mapped out 
into histologically different areas the whole human cortex cerebri, 

as well as the cortex cerebri of many orders of mammals. Owing 
. U 


—— 


306 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


to the great importance of the subject, which, however, it must 
be remarked, is still in its infancy, the human maps of these 
investigators are reproduced here as Figs. 3, 4, 5, and 6. 

As might be expected in the case of such elaborate and totally 
independent investigations, and in view of the great difficulties 
involved in the cutting up, blocking, sectioning, orientating, and 
reproducing in diagram form of the cortex of entire human hemi- 
spheres, the maps given by Brodmann and by Campbell differ 
considerably in detail from one another. 

They agree with one another, on the other hand, much more 
than either of them resembles the maps of Flechsig (see Fig. 2). 
It must not be forgotten, however, that the diagrams of Flechsig 
indicate the projection areas of the foetus, which there is reason 
to suppose differ considerably in distribution from those existing 
in the adult. This difference is due to the increased development — 
of the later evolved associational spheres in the latter, which 
affects both the position and the relative size of the earlier evolved 
projection spheres. For example, Flechsig’s area for bodily sensi- 
bility is both pre- and post-Rolandic, although he states that the 
majority of the projection fibres pass behind the central fissure. 
In the adult it is at least probable that this projection area is 
entirely post-Rolandic; and it is known that the psychomotor 
area is pre-Rolandic. At the period when the latter area was 
considered to be both pre- and post-Rolandic, and to be sensori- 
motor in function, its supposed distribution closely resembled the 
foetal: bodily sensibility area of Flechsig; and the two were re- 
garded as identical. The maps of Brodmann and of Campbell 
must not therefore be regarded as impugning the general accuracy 
of the diagrams of Flechsig with regard to the projection areas of 
the foetus. 

It may be remarked that in only two regions are the maps of 
Brodmann and of Campbell in complete accord, namely, in the 
psychomotor or Betz cell area (4 of Brodmann and “ precentral ” 
of Campbell), and the visuo-sensory area (17 of Brodmann 
and “ visuo-sensory”” of Campbell). The former of these is the 
area mapped out by Lewis and Clarke (1878), and the latter is 
that mapped out by the writer (1900). The extent of the visuo- 
psychic region (18 of Brodmann), which was described by the 
present writer as surrounding the visuo-sensory area, but was not 
more closely defined owing to its somewhat indefinite limits, 


Ms 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM 307 


Whi . 
} oa WHS Z _ i 
vi » GN ZA : 
nwo AN < 
Visuo- ‘ 2S > . <et 
prychsc f y Z WY) hy Sos 
' i} ; errs, 4 ° 


Visuo- 


a 
ee oe 
= = =) 


= 


Fig. 4 (Campbell, 1905). 


308 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


2) 
44a ‘F32; 
RI GAR AACA 
88 = 


Fic. 6 (Brodmann, 1902-1907). 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM = 309 


is given so differently by Brodmann and by Campbell that it 
might appear that no advance had been made on his original 
description. A careful study of the maps of Brodmann and of 
Campbell in the light of the writer’s special knowledge of the 


histological characters of several portions of the cortex cerebri has, 


however, convinced him that the more elaborately detailed map 
of the former of these investigators is the more correct. In support 
of this statement he would refer to the recent paper of Gordon 
Holmes on the histology of the post-central gyrus, in which the 
findings of Brodmann are confirmed. —~ 

The writer is, however, of the opinion that, whilst further histo- 
logical research will undoubtedly enable certain other projection 
areas to be as precisely defined as have been the psychomotor 
and visuo-sensory areas (even if Brodmann’s findings in those 
respects should not be confirmed in their entirety), the exact 
differentiation of the remainder and greater portion of the grey 
mantle into equally precise areas will be attended with great 
difficulty owing to the probability that considerable differences 
exist in the case of different individuals. He nevertheless regards 
such precise differentiation as possible, and considers that light 
will in the future be thrown on the histo-pathology of amentia or 
cerebral sub-evolution by this means. For example, in the six 
occipital regions mapped out by the writer in the paper already 
referred to, the shape and apparent extent of the visuo-sensory 
area exhibit considerable individual differences quite apart from 
the questions of age and blindness. As such differences exist in 
a projection area, it is probable that more marked variations will 
occur in the case of the later specialised areas of different brains. 
Though fissuration and histological differentiation do not run hand 
in hand, the researches of Karplus on the fissuration of the human 
cerebrum with regard to family likeness are worthy of mention in 
this connection. 

Beyond a relatively gross. subdivision of the cerebral cortex 
into different areas, it is unlikely that the histological method 
will be of assistance, as cerebral function, even when relatively 
low in grade, consists of associational processes which involve 
many related regions of the cortex. Further, as is shown espe- 
cially by the study of sense deprivation, the maimed brains of 
patients suffering from this disability are capable, to a remarkable 
extent, of replacing the normal methods of association by dis- 


310 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


similar, though related, modes which in many instances fulfil their 
purpose in an admirable manner. 

Hence, the histological investigation of a given cerebrum is 
probably capable of affording a rough criterion of the anatomical 
basis which subserves, not the actual grade of functional activity, 
but the possible limits of educability of the organ. It may be re- 
marked that this mode of viewing the subject disposes of the chief 
objection urged by Bianchi against the doctrine of Flechsig. 

It may therefore be stated that.the exact limits of the psycho- 
motor or Betz cell area, and of the visuo-sensory area, are known 
beyond doubt, and that their functions have been proved by 
experimental or histo-pathological methods. As regards the less 
certainly defined visuo-psychic region, the associational, in contra- 
distinction to receptive, function of this area has been develop- 
mentally proved by facts stated earlier in this article. These 
from a different aspect, that of the evolution of cortical lamina- 
tion, confirm the doctrine of Flechsig with regard to centres of 
association and of projection. A similar statement may be made 
with regard to the well-known, but as yet not precisely defined, 
prefrontal region of the cortex cerebri (10 of Brodmann). Further, 
anatomical evidence points to the post-central gyrus or some part 
of it as the projection area for bodily sensibility, and the recent 
experiments of Oskar Vogt, who states that ablation of this gyrus 
in the monkey is followed by ataxy without palsy, have finally 
proved the truth of this view. There are anatomical grounds for 
considering that the projection spheres for hearing and for olfactory 
and gustatory sensations are more or less correctly located, but 
experimental or histo-pathological proof in these cases is not yet 
available. With regard to the numerous other areas mapped out 
by Brodmann and by Campbell, there is little doubt that future 
research will enable both their exact or variable limits to be 
determined, and their functions to be finally proved. 

It is ti: us possible to make the broad statement that the human 
cerebral co.tex, excluding for the moment the frontal lobes, is 
histologically differentiable into areas of projection connected with 
the various senses, each of which areas possesses a zone of cortex 
connected with or surrounding it, and into further areas which 
- occupy the remainder of the cortex. Reasoning by homology on 
the truths known with regard to the visuo-sensory and visuo- 
psychic regions, it is possible to state that the areas of projection 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM = 3il 


are chiefly concerned with the reception of afferent impressions of 
various kinds, and that the cortex in relation to each of these is 
especially concerned with associational functions or psychic pro- 
- cesses hall-marked by their several sensory areas. It may further 
be provisionally stated that a large part of the remainder of the 
cortex under consideration may probably be regarded as the 
physical basis of language (not words, but word-groupings), 7.¢. a 
mechanism for the symbolic integration of the various more or 
less complex products of cerebral association. This question will 
be considered later in connection with the subject of the higher 
functions of the human cerebrum. 

As regards the frontal lobes, but two regions call for especial 
remark here, namely, the psychomotor area and the prefrontal 
region. 

With regard to the psychomotor area (‘ precentral” of Campbell, 
4 of Brodmann), it is hardly necessary at the present time to 
indicate the various reasons which prove that this area is asso- 
ciational and not sensory in function, and that, in addition, it 
possesses a direct efferent connection with the motor groups of 
lower neurones. 

The cerebral cortex may, in fact, be regarded as a complex 
sensori-motor mechanism. Of this the non-frontal portion consists 
of sensory areas (centres of projection) and of zones of association 
of various types and grades of complexity. The frontal lobes, on 
the other hand, contain the efferent portion of the mechanism. 
The psychomotor area may be regarded as the lowest grade of 
this, and as concerned on the one hand with the integration - 
and on the other with the efferent transmission of the motor 
expression of the associationally elaborated results of sensorial 
stimulation. 

The prefrontal region (10 of Bradmann) is the most complex 
of the zones of association of Flechsig. It is the last portion of 
the cortex to be evolved and the first to undergo retrogression. 
The outer cell lamina or pyramidal layer of this region varies 
directly in depth according to the mental power of the individual, 
in the case of foetuses and infants, idiots and imbeciles, and chronic 
* and recurrent lunatics without dementia. In cases of dementia, 
degrees of retrogression occur which vary directly with the existing 
grade of dementia. Finally this layer of the cortex is the only 
layer which varies measurably in depth in normal individuals. On 


312 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


these grounds alone it is possible to assign to the prefrontal cortex 
the highest and latest evolved functions of voluntary attention 
and inhibition, and of selection and co-ordination of the various 
individually complex processes of cerebral association. The pre- 
frontal portion of the cortex is thus concerned in the performance 
of the highest grades of cerebral function, and also bears to the 
psychomotor area a similar relationship (though in the converse 
direction as regards action) to that borne by the posterior regions 
of association to the centres of projection. 

To endeavour to indicate the functions of the cortex of the 
intermediate portion of the frontal lobes would in the present 
state of knowledge be to enter the realm of conjecture. It may, 
however, be remarked that part of this associational zone un- 


doubtedly contains the physical basis of the motor aspect of the _ 


mechanism of language, which is the instrument for the symbolic 
integration of the various more or less complex processes of cerebral 
association. Until the recent publication of the investigations of 
Marie and of Monakow, Broca’s area was generally regarded, by 
neurologists at least, as the region concerned with one portion of 
this mechanism, namely, the “speech centre.”” Now, however, as 
will be indicated later, a much broader view must be taken of 
the complex mechanism of language, which possesses multifarious 
associational connections with probably the whole cerebrum. The 
mechanism of language must in fact be regarded, not merely as a 
series of cortical centres, one or more of which may be blotted out 
without more than local interference with the mental functions, 
but as the absolutely essential factor to the so performance 
of these. 

With reference to the respective functions of the two greatest 
centres of association, a large body of neurologists, notably Bastian, 
Hughlings Jackson, Mott, Schifer, and Flechsig himself, are of 
opinion that gross mental disabilities are more likely to occur in 
lesions of the posterior centre than of the anterior, whilst Won 
Hitzig, Ferrier, Bianchi, &c., hold the opposite view. 

From the purely neunclogioal aspect, especially when con- 
sidering the different varieties of “aphasia,” the former view 
had undoubtedly much in its favour. For example, until quite 
recently, cases of Wernicke’s (sensory) aphasia were generally 
considered to exhibit gross mental disability, whilst cases of Broca’s 
(motor) aphasia were supposed to be free from this symptom. The 


- 


, 
: 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM — 313 


latter option i, however, disputed in toto by Marie, who has been 
confirmed in his observations by numerous recent writers. 

Premising, however, that the posterior centres of association 
were concerned with processes of lower association alone, general 
mental disability would still be evident in cases of gross lesion of 
the hinder part of the hemispheres, as the patient would under 
these circumstances be unable in many cases to produce satis- 
factory evidence of general mental soundness. In many other 
cases, also, such an entire disturbance of perceptive and ideational 
processes as occurs, would be not unlikely to cause too great a 
strain on the higher associational functions, and to directly result 
in the development of symptoms of true mental alienation. This 
is the result which actually ensues in many cases of sense depriva- 
tion. It is further rendered probable by the fact that no less 
than: one in every 280 of the general population is at the present 
time suffering from mental alienation, and that the proportion of 
potential psychopaths is very much greater. That this explana- 
tion is correct is finally shown by the fact that the latest developed 
and most complex portion of the human cerebrum, namely, the 
prefrontal area, is, as has already been indicated, the region which 
is especially affected in the subjects of mental disease. 

Though it is not necessary to describe in detail the differences 
in degree of development and in structure which exist between 
the cerebrum of man and those of the anthropoid primates, it is 
desirable, in this connection, to indicate here briefly the chief of 
these. It consists, as may be seen by a study of the various 
publications already indicated, in the immense development of the 
anterior and posterior zones of association which has occurred in 
the human brain. Concurrently with this increase in the extent 
of the great zones of association there has developed in man the 
power of abstract thought and the employment for this purpose 
of highly complex articulate and written language. As the centres 
of lower association in the anthropoid primates differ from those 


of man in extent and complexity, so do the precepts of the former «* i 


differ from the highly complex lower psychic units of the latter. 
Equally does the rudimentary prefrontal region of the anthropoid 
primates, which imperfectly marshals the relatively simple lower 
psychic units of these animals, differ from the notable and still 
developing prefrontal lobe of man, the capacity of which, for 
co-ordinating the infinitely complex lower psychic units, which 


Gene ~~ 7 » i 


314 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


are compounded in the human mind, into harmonious series of 
concepts by means of voluntary attention and selection, is only 
limited by the degree of functional development of this lobe in any 
particular individual of the race. The lower associational centres 
of man, which represent the physical basis of the content of mind, 
are thus co-ordinate in development with the centre of higher 
association and co-ordination, which represents the physical basis 
of the capacity to voluntarily group into a harmonious and con- 
nected sequence the higher psychic units of the mind. 

Passing now from the highest to one of the lowest orders of 
mammals, namely, the insectivora, one may obtain histological 
evidence of equal importance with regard to the functions of the 
cerebrum. Watson, in his recent paper, has mapped out the 
minute brains of certain members of this order into histologically 
different regions. Certain of these areas he regards as centres ~ 
of projection, and another, on the dorsal and mesial aspect, he 
designates ,““ motor” from its histological structure. The: re- 
mainder of the cortex, from the embryonic appearance of the — 
cells contained in it, he designates “undifferentiated.” The 
conclusions of Watson with regard to his investigation are best 
expressed in his own words :— 


4 


“Tt seems necessary to assume that animals like the mole are 
possessed of some means of simple sensory association, otherwise it 
is probable that.the animal’s waking life would tend to be one of 
almost continuous sensory confusion. Such association is called up 
by the stimulation of one or other or more than one special sense, 
and passes more or less directly to a motor or efferent result. This 
appears to be the lowest grade of conscious neopallial association— 
t.e. of psychic function—and it doubtless persists in mammals much 
higher in the scale than the insectivora, and possibly in the highest, 
though obscured in them by the development of a higher grade or 
grades. It is not likely to exist as a random passing to and fro of 
impulses from this sensory area to that, but to lie in some fusion zone. 
Careful examination of the cortex of animals like the mole has failed 
to reveal any ‘attempt at the development of a ‘psychic’ zone in 
direct relation to any of the sensory projection spheres. The writer 
does not consider that on histological grounds one is justified in 
drawing close analogies between the areas he has termed ~“ undif- 
ferentiated’ in the mole, &c., and the areas of cortex surrounding 
the projection spheres in certain higher mammals. From the histo- 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM — 315 


logical aspect, therefore, the assumption that animals like the mole 
possess any special areas of association, however small, appears to be 
unjustified, and there is also, in view of the following remarks, some 
reason for believing that such areas are in them physiologically unneces- 
sary, in spite of the probable requirement by these animals of some 
physical basis for simple sensory association. 

“Dr. Bolton has suggested to the writer that in the mole and 
similar animals the fusion zone for simple sensory impressions, 
i.e. lowest grade of conscious association, lies in the area which has 
been designated ‘motor.’ Taking the mole, for example, the areas 
mapped out as kinesthetic, fifth sensory, visual, &c., are simple sensory 
reception spheres for the respective senses, whilst area 1’’ (motor) 
‘is the psychic equivalent of all these, and at the same time efferent. 
Area 1” (motor) “‘ would thus receive impulses from each or all of the 
sensory projection spheres and turn them into motor equivalents. 
It is a general area of simple sensory association with the origin of 
the efferent (motor) tracts included. This is as far as such an animal 
as the mole has got in the direction of higher association. The struc- 
ture of the cortex, however, even of this area, especially if regarded 
as being of an associational type, is of a relatively elementary nature. 

“The above suggestion does away with the apparent anomaly of 
__ the relatively great extent of area 1’’ (motor) “if this be looked upon 
as nothing more than a ‘ Betz cell’ region. It also coincides with 
the fact that this area ranks with the best developed portion of the 
cortex as regards supra-granular (pyramidal) layer which mammals 
such as the mole possess, poor though this is. 

“The writer’s views have been founded upon a study of the cortex 
cerebri, not only in adult mammals but also in foetal and young animals. 
He concludes that in the cerebral cortex of many—and probably of 
all—adult lower mammals there are areas, considerable in extent, which 
throughout life advance little—as regards complexity of their com- 
ponent nerve cells—beyond their condition in the foetal or very young 
animal. It is therefore on histological grounds presumed that such 
areas are of comparatively little functional value to the animal. 

“ He also considers it proved, as the result of the investigations 
of Bolton and himself, that the structure of the neopallium is founded 
upon an infra-granular basis. Further, it is suggested that in the 
earliest attempts at evolution of structure which come to be of any 
considerable functional value, the neopallium follows the plan of cor- 
tical architecture long previously in the phylogenetic scale laid down 
in the hippocampus, which plan in the latter situation has become 
fixed, and, as a plan, permanent. The earliest and lowest grade of 


316 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


neopallial representation is thus, as regards structure, a repetition 
of the hippocampal type—granular and infra-granular cortex. By 
the accrescence of a supra-granular layer of varying degrees of depth ~ 
and complexity of its component nerve cells, different grades of repre- 
sentation may be reached, and are reached to some extent in the 
same animal, even if this occupies a lowly place in the mammalian 
phylum, and to a greater extent the higher is the position in the scale . 
to which the animal belongs.” 


To summarise and correlate the mass of histological data which r 
bears on mammalian cortical localisation from the aspect of 
cerebral function would be beyond the scope of this article. Many ’ 
of the observations of the several authors differ greatly in detail, , 
as is naturally to be expected in the case of a subject still in its 7 
infancy, and some are contradictory. Further, the greater part. ‘ 
of the published work deals with histological observations rather q 
than with physiological deductions. It is thus not yet possible to ; 
properly correlate the results of the various investigations which 
deal with animal behaviour under different conditions, and the 
deductions which may be drawn from them with regard to the 
psychic processes which take place in the different members of 
the various natural orders, with the histological data which have ; 
hitherto been obtained with regard to the cerebra of the mammalia. ] 


‘ 
: 
EVOLUTION OF CEREBRAL FUNCTION q 
: 


The following broad statements, with regard to the mode of | 
evolution of cerebral function, are, however,. possible, if they be 
regarded in the light of a partially proved provisional hypothesis. 

The term “ neopallium” has been introduced by Elliott Smith 
to designate the cortex cerebri which is peculiar to mammals, and 
which consists of a variable area intercalated between the pyriform 
lobe and the hippocampus. This term is employed in contra- 
distinction to the “ archipallium” of the lower vertebrates. The 
neopallium increases in amount with the ascent of the mammalian 
~ scale, and in man constitutes almost the whole of the cortex — 
cerebri. 

The functions of the neopallium are the reception of sensorial 
stimuli, the conservation of associative memory, the performance 
of the higher psychic processes, and the evolution.of the com- — 


plicated motor phenomena, which depend on these, and which 
serve as the sole objective indications of the performance of 
psychic processes. 

The neopallium is thus the highest sensori-motor ganglion of 
the mammalia. 

The archipallium is built on a basis which consists primarily 
of but two cell lamin, which are homologous with the middle 
(granule) and inner (polymorphous) cell lamine of the neopallium 
(Turner, Watson). The neopallium is characterised by the ad- 
ditional development of an outer (pyramidal) cell lamina, which 
serves as the physical basis of “ psychic” in contradistinction to 
“instinctive” processes. This cell lamina is barely present in 
the neopallium of the lowest mammalia, and increases in depth 
with the ascent of the mammalian scale (Brodmann, Watson). 
It is earliest and also least developed in the projection areas, and 
is later developed and more highly evolved in the regions con- 
cerned with associational functions. The human brain is char- 
acterised by the evolution of a prefrontal region of notable size, 
and of great complexity of histological structure (Flechsig, Bolton, 
Turner, Watson). The outer cell lamina of this region varies 
measurably in depth in normal individuals, and in its degrees of 
evolution and retrogression varies in depth according to the mental 
capacity of the subjects of mental sub-evolution and dissolution. 

In the earlier evolution of-the neopallium, certain projection 
zones of simple structure exist, together with a rudimentary 
“ motor” area, the function of which is to fuse the products of 
the projection areas and turn them into motor equivalents. The 
remainder of the cortex is embryonic in structure (Watson), and 
is probably latent as regards functional activity. 

During the ascent of the mammalian scale, the projection and 

_ motor areas increase in complexity of structure (Brodmann, Elliott 
Smith, Mott, &c.), and advance in functional activity. Until, 
however, the carnivora are reached (Campbell), there is no indi- 
cation of the development of histologically differentiable associa- 
tional zones around or near the centres of projection. It is probable 


that, in agreement with the relatively embryonic structure of the 


test of the cortex, the functional activity of this is largely latent. 

By the primates are reached, well-developed and histologically 
differentiable zones of association exist in the neighbourhood of 
_ the projection areas, and similarly developed cortex exists in front 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM — 317 


i bow 


318 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND | 


of the psychomotor area. The remainder of the cortex has largely 
ceased to present embryonic features. 

The cerebrum of the higher primates is thus characterised by 
the evolution of regions of associational cortex, posteriorly be- 
tween the projection areas, and anteriorly in front of the psycho- 
motor area. The former is probably concerned with the higher 
elaboration of the “sensorial,” and the latter with that of the 
‘‘ motor” aspect of the cerebral functions. 

In man the area of cortex devoted to associational functions 
is enormously enlarged posteriorly, and is still more increased 
anteriorly by the evolution of a notable prefrontal lobe. Con- 
currently with this increase in the zones of association, language 


has been evolved, from both “ sensorial” and “ motor” aspects, as ~ 
a complex mechanism for the symbolic integration of the various” 


more or less individually complex products of cerebral associa- 
tion; and the higher psychic functions, of voluntary attention 
and inhibition, and of voluntary selection and co-ordination into 
orderly sequence of the processes of lower cerebral association, 
have been developed. 


THE HicHEeR FUNCTIONS OF THE HUMAN CEREBRUM 


The further consideration of the functions of the cerebrum, 
namely, the description, or attempted description, of those func- 
tions which are. peculiar to man, necessitates the passing of the 
_border-line between physiology and psychology. Hitherto the 
subject of aphasia has been regarded as a branch of the former 
science, and has fallen under the purview of the neurologist. 
Recent investigations, in particular those of Marie, have, however, 
shown that a wider view must be taken of the relationship of 
language to the psychic functions, and of the influence on 
these of the disabilities which are described under the term 

** aphasia.” 

As a preliminary to the further consideration of the functions 
of the cerebrum and to the description of these investigations, it 
is therefore necessary, for the benefit of such readers as are not 


versed in psychological terminology, to introduce here certain 


general observations and definitions. 
As has already been remarked, the cerebrum should be regan 


as a great sensori-motor ganglion, the only objective indications — 


7 
‘ 


hg 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM _ 319: 


of the functions of which are derived from the various motor 


exhibitions which are presented by its possessor. 

In the lower animals these are of two kinds, evidences of feeling 
or emotion, and evidences of intelligent (instinctive or acquired) 
activity ; and either of these may be partly indicated by means of 
articulatory exhibitions. 

The same evidences of cerebral activity are presented by man, 

in whom, however, the indications of emotion are more numerous 
and complex, and the evidences of psychomotor activity are in- 
finitely more elaborate. The latter, apart from the ordinary motor 
indications of intelligence, consist of gestures and of spoken and 
written language. 
_ Any discussion of the theory of the emotions would be out of 
place in this article, but it may be indicated in passing that they 
are probably evolved from the lower or instinctive plane of motor 
exhibition. This is suggested by the common observation that 
very young infants “ make faces” indicative of various emotions 
under the influence of gastro-intestinal irritation, long before they 
present evidence of intelligent motor activity, and still longer 
before they are able to articulate a word. Further, objective in- 
- dications of probably experienced emotions become well-developed 
in infants at a time when the ordinary motor indications of intelli- 
gence are still highly inco-ordinate. 

Of the indications of human psychic activity, the most im- 
portant, namely, language, will now be considered. 

The elements on which cerebral activity is based are the various 
sensory impressions which arrive at the cortex and which are con- 
served there as sensori-memorial images. These elements form 
the raw material of the psychic processes and are spoken of as 
sensations. To be understood, sensations require localisation in 
_ space and reference to the objects from which they arise. The 
cerebral process involved in the preliminary fusion of sensations 
is spoken of as perception. For example, the sound of a clock 
ticking results in the localisation of the sensation in a certain 
direction and to a particular object, the clock. Not only, however, 
does the process of perception involve the fusion of direct sen- 
sations, but it also necessitates the awakening and fusion with 
these of sensori-memorial images of former sensations. In the act 
of identifying an object, e.g. a locomotive, the various sensations 

arising from it awaken a variable number of sensori-memorial 


ee ‘ 5 = ‘ zy 


- 


320 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


images. Again, the sound of the word “horse ” awakens certain 
sensori-memorial images, which may be of any type, from the 
written word “ horse” to the last horse we have seen. 

The presentation of a sensation, therefore, results in a cerebral 
process which evolves the psychic product termed a percept. 
This cerebral process necessarily varies on each occasion on which 
it occurs. The word “ microscope,” for example, heard at different 
times, evokes numerous related but dissimilar sensori-memorial 
images. 

Perception, therefore, is a cerebral process which is-similarin 
nature but differs in detail on each occasion on which it occurs. 
It is thus incorrect to speak of a cerebral centre for percepts, 
which are psychic products that, except by accident, need never 
be identical even if the arousing sensation is the same. 

It may be remarked that perception can hardly be described as © 
the act of naming objects, for it is as often the reverse, namely, 
the act of applying one or more sensori-memorial images to a 
name; or the identifying of a sensation with a sensori-memorial _ 
image. The last corresponds to the crude perceptions of animals : 
the others to perceptions involving the employment of the symbolic 
mechanism of language. 

The next grade of complexity of cerebral processes which is 
rendered possible by the aid of language is the formation of a 
concept or general notion, e.g. the evolution of a general name 
such as animal, man, building. The psychic product is termed a 
concept, and the process of conception involves the revivification 
‘of numerous sensori-memorial images which present common 
points of similarity. 

The cerebral process involved becomes still more complex for 
the evolution of such “abstract concepts” as heaviness, beauty, 
religion. 

It is at once evident that the process of conception, whether 
it takes the form of generalisation from a series of sensori-memorial 
images to a general name, or of revivification of a series of sensori- 
memorial images in order that the meaning of a general name may 
be interpreted, is a very complex one; and necessarily differs in 
detail, in spite of a general similarity in nature, on each occasion 
on which it occurs. It is thus incorrect to speak of a cerebral 
centre for concepts. 

A physical basis for the several words which symbolise the 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM 321 


variable cerebral processes termed conception undoubtedly exists 
in the cortex cerebri. Common or abstract names, however, are 
not concepts, but are merely symbols which enable former active 
processes of conception to be recorded, and future acts of con- 
ception to be evolved. 

Proper, common, and abstract nouns may thus be compared in 
function to the algebraic symbols employed by mathematicians, 
whilst sensori-memorial images and crude sensations may be 
compared to the numerals of arithmetic. 

Language is, however, an infinitely more complex symbolic 
system than is that employed by mathematicians; and its 
“numerals,” 7.e. its sensori-memorial images and crude sensa- 
tions, are innumerable. 

As will be indicated later, the complete performance of the 
respective psychic processes, with the resulting reintegration of 
the particular percepts or concepts of which words are symbolic, 
does not necessarily occur during the mechanical employment 
of the language mechanism. Rowland, in fact, in her investiga- 
tion of the psychological experiences connected with the different 
parts of speech, distinguishes three stages of meaning—(1) A feeling 
. of familiarity with the word ; (2) a feeling that she would know how 
to use it; and (3) the unrolling of the images. The paper of this 
author, which includes a valuable bibliography, is well worthy of 
study in this connection. : 

The cerebrum possesses an almost infinite capacity for the 
development of symbolic systems. New languages may be ac- 
quired, and as if on the principle of “the more the merrier,” with 
each new acquisition the process of further acquirement appears 
to become easier. The numerous skilled psychomotor perform- 
ances which only compare with language in variety and complexity, 
eg. music, painting, sculpture, &c., may also be indicated as allied 
in nature to, though lower in grade than, the symbolic systems of 
language and mathematics. 

The language mechanism, like the routine systems and “red 
tape” of every-day life, whilst a good servant, tends to become a 
bad master, unless the cerebral processes necessary for the elucida- 
tion of the meaning of the words employed are continually being 
voluntarily performed. It is thus especially necessary, during 
the voluntary employment of written or spoken language for the 
evolution and reproduction of the highest psychic products, e.g. 

2 7 = 


“=n 


322 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


abstract thought, to continually revert to concrete examples. — 
Many results of the higher reasoning processes would in fact be 
quite unintelligible to the reader in the absence of concrete illus- 
tration, and, what is even more important, the author himself 
would not infrequently find a tortoise successful in its race with a 
hare. Philosophic disputes have as a rule depended largely on 
questions of definition, or on the employment of the same words 
under slightly different connotations. Forms of words are worse 
than useless unless their exact meaning is appreciated, and many 
discussions and disputes have arisen owing to the critic holding 
strictly to these, and thereby rendering it difficult or impossible 
for their originator to make his intentions quite clear and in- 
telligible. 

The necessarily imperfect employment of the language 
mechanism, owing to the occasional impossibility of satisfactorily 
discovering a form of words in which to clearly express the exact 
meaning of the writer or speaker, is in fact one of the most fruitful | 
sources of disputation. It is not intended to imply by this remark 
that such a person knows what he wishes to express. He is aware | 
that the phrases he employs do not express what he desires, and | 
at times his uncompleted cerebral processes may reach their goal 
through the suggestion by another individual of a suitable form ( 
of words. 

On the other hand, from the purely didactic aspect, a speaker . 
occasionally finds his only safe refuge in generalities which are 
capable of varied interpretation. Many a sermon is acceptable 
to, and is approved by, a congregation, which would be up in 
arms if the preacher produced concrete illustrations of his meaning, 
e.g. his views with regard to certain political questions, &c. 

The nature and mode of employment of the symbolic mechanism 
of language will now be briefly discussed. 

The whole of the higher intellectual processes are dependent 
on, and develop pari passu with, the evolution of language. Till 
of recent years the majority of, and even now many, individuals 
depend on the sense of hearing for the acquisition of the greater 
portion of their (human) psychic content, though persons who read 
and write perhaps gain an equal amount by means of the sense 
of sight, and the more intellectual members of the race probably 
acquire the greater part by means of the latter sense. It may, — 
however, be remarked that in some normal intelligent reading- 


° 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM 323 


_- and-writing individuals visual association, and in others auditory 


association, is the more natural method of acquisition. 

Language, therefore, as the instrument of thought, or even as 
its compeer, for the higher refinements of thought depend so 
entirely on, and draw so much of their inspiration from, the 
possession of a highly elaborate vocabulary, is of fundamental 
importance for the performance of the higher, as of the less 
complex, psychic functions. 

Language, according to the type of sensorial or sensori-motor 
avenues through which it is acquired and stored, and by means of 
which it is employed, possesses four chief physical bases in the 
cerebral cortex, namely, the auditory, visual, cheirographic, and 
articulatory. For the sake of simplicity no attempt is made to 
separate the kinesthetic from the purely motor divisions of the 
latter two, though, in fact, it may be regarded as certain that these 
are differently located. The kinesthetic divisions, are, however, 
those which are at present under reference. All these “ word- 
centres” naturally lie in or near the auditory, visual, and general 
sensory projection spheres of the cortex, as words merely constitute 
one variety of sensorial impression. It might therefore be sup- 
. posed that loss of any one of the four afferent avenues to these 
physical bases of language would not, owing to the commissural 
connections between the several spheres, be of serious import, 
apart from the non-reception of sensations through the absent 
channel. That such a view is incorrect can, however, readily be 
demonstrated. The spheres referred to, with their commissural 
connections and their afferent and efferent projection systems, 
merely form a convenient mechanism for the mechanical acquisi- 
tion and reproduction of language, which would be meaningless 
unless during the employment of its mechanism there occurred 
an active associational participation on the part of practically the 
whole mantle of the cerebrum. 

A word, per se, represents merely an auditory or visual sensation, 
or a cheirographic or articulatory kinesthetic impression, unless 
it is employed as a symbol on which to integrate the percept or 
concept which it signifies; and for this the cerebral mechanisms 
or associational systems connecting the different projection and 
sensori-memorial regions of the cortex are needed. 

Further, both these developed percepts and concepts, and 
also the associational processes involved in their formation, differ 


~ 
. + 


324 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


not fundamentally but in detail on every occasion on which they 
are evolved or employed. 

Words may arise into consciousness through any of the 
four language spheres. When, however, they are voluntarily and 
silently reproduced, 7.e. thought of, words are invariably awakened | 
through the articulatory word-centre under normal conditions. | 
Further, this reproduction requires a muscular effort, and cannot | 
take place without a definite articulatory attempt, usually, but 
not necessarily accompanied by a slight expiration. Again, words 
cannot be voluntarily repeated in thought by means of the cheiro- 
graphic centre if the hand is not actually moved, unless such hand- 
movements are replaced by slight movements of the head, or even 
of the lower jaw or the eyes, through the agency of their respective 
motor spheres. Such observations as have just been indicated 
are valuable because they render it probable that the important 
factors in the reproduction are not the muscular movements but 
the sensorial impulses derived from these ; otherwise there is no 
reason why such muscular movements should not be imagined 
without any attempt at their actual performance. If words should 
spontaneously arise in the visual or the auditory word-centre, the | 
condition is so abnormal as to constitute a hallucination, which 
the subject may or may not be able to distinguish from a true 
visual or auditory sensation. A homologous phenomenon to such 
a hallucination may be observed during the stimulation of the 
psychomotor area of a monkey which has recovered from anesthesia. . 
Such an animal regards the movement, say of the arm, with great 
surprise, and at once performs a voluntary and opposite movement, 
exactly as if the limb had been moved by an external agency. 

There is thus reason to consider that words invariably arise 
into consciousness by sensorial or sensori-memorial, and not by 
psychomotor, means ; and the observation that normally they are 
voluntarily awakened by the preliminary aid of the psychomotor 
area is of significance with regard to the thesis that the frontal 
lobes are concerned with psychic function from the higher asso- 
ciational and also the motor aspect, and the remainder of the 
cerebrum from the sensorial, sensori-memorial, and lower associa- — 
\ tional. 

However they arise into consciousness, words ‘naturally 
possess very different symbolic values. Articles, pronouns, pre- 
positions, conjunctions, interjections, and the simpler adjectives, 


7%, } \ in —  .- | 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM — 325 


adverbs and verbs, when thought of alone (articulatory word- 
centre), as a rule arouse little beyond their respective visual 
or auditory word-images, which, in themselves, are meaningless. 
Adjectives, adverbs, verbs, and abstract nouns, when thought of 
alone (articulatory word-centre), arouse first their respective visual 
or auditory word-images. These, however, are meaningless until 
by complex processes of association they are defined and illustrated 
through the sensori-memorial spheres attached to the various 
sensory or projection areas. Common or proper nouns, when 
thought of alone (articulatory word-centre), may first arouse their 
visual or auditory word-images, but they frequently at once awaken 
a whole series of processes of association, and thereby determine 
the reproduction of sensori-memorial images attached to one or 
more of the several sensory or projection areas. It may be re- 
marked that any such series of processes of association differs in 
detail on each occasion on which it is evolved. For example, the 
mental processes induced by the word “cat,” whether this be 
thought of (articulatory sphere) or be heard or seen (auditory or 
visual sphere), are different, not fundamentally but in detail, on 
each occasion on which they are aroused. This ever-varying 
perceptive content is consequent on the revivification of, and the 
modification of the complex relations of, the numerous existing 
sensori-memorial images, of which the word is symbolic, which 
are constantly taking place under the influence of even apparently 
unrelated afferent impressions. 

Hence, the auditory, visual, cheirographic, and articulatory word- 
centres merely signify the cortical regions in which lie the physical 
bases of mental algebraic symbols. These, unless they serve as in- 
citing agents from which spread, in different directions throughout 
the cerebrum, complex impulses of association, signify no more than 
_ unmeaning sounds, shapes, and musculo-kinesthetic sensations. _ 

Language is produced by the suitable co-ordination of the 
verbal content of the auditory and articulatory word-centres. It 
is originally acquired by imitation under the influence of audi- 
tory sensations, and in educated persons language is more highly 
evolved owing to education of the visual and cheirographic spheres. 
When, however, it has once been acquired, language (¢.e. functional 
activity of the several word-centres with their commissural systems) 
is not necessarily employed as the instrument of thought, although 
it has been primarily evolved for this purpose. 


4 


326 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


Examples are common in which the mechanism of language is” 
employed in a purely mechanical manner. Imbeciles can at times 
learn by rote long paragraphs, of the meaning of which they are 
ignorant. Children learn a large portion of their lessons in this 
way. Adults, even, may learn the Lord’s. Prayer backwards, or 
sentences in an unknown foreign tongue. Direct evidence of the 
purely mechanical nature of these performances is often afforded 
by the inability of the subjects to complete their feat, if they are 
stopped during its course, unless they start again at the commence- 
ment. Occasionally quite remarkable examples of mechanical 
memory and of mechanical employment of the word-centres are 
met with. From the former aspect may be mentioned the re- 
production of long verbal or musical compositions after a single 
reading or hearing, and from the latter the performances of “ cal-. 
culating boys.” An interesting article on the latter subject has 
recently been published by F. D. Mitchell. 

Examples of this mode of employment of the language 
mechanism may be readily drawn from every-day life. Many 
word-complexes, which are frequently repeated, e.g. daily prayers, 
are often gone through in a purely mechanical manner, whilst the 
individual reproducing them is perhaps thinking of something 
else. Again, it is appreciated by few that language, as normally 
employed, is very largely a purely reflex, or, at any rate, automatic 
function ; and that the significance of what is spoken is but feebly © 
appreciated by the speaker. In the majority of persons the word- 
vocabulary which is in common use is very limited, and the phrase 
vocabulary is both extremely limited, remarkably stereotyped, 
and in many instances quite automatically | employed. In edu- 
cated, and particularly in “well brought up” persons, on the 
other hand, the word and phrase vocabularies, though equally 
stereotyped) are much more extensive in range. 

The voluntary employment of the language mechanism is 
attended by greater executive difficulties than is the reflexly 
induced and automatically performed mode which has just been 
indicated ; and it is at times involuntarily incited, to the detri- 
ment of the performer, by emotional disturbances. For example, 
nervous persons, when in the presence of their real or imaginary, — 
social or intellectual, superiors, speak haltingly and from a limited 
vocabulary owing to the attempt to converse, not automatically, 
but to order. On the other hand, in the voluntary employment 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM = 327 


‘of written or spoken language for the evolution and reproduction 
of the highest psychic products, e.g. the production of an abstruse 
thesis, the language mechanism is made use of solely for the pur- 
pose for which it has been evolved, namely, as the instrument, 
and the important assistant, of thought. 

During the above observations an endeavour has been made 
to indicate that language, though so commonly employed in a 
largely automatic manner, and with but a feeble appreciation of 
its signification, is nevertheless in essence a symbolic mechanism 
for the integration of sensori-memorial images, and, though more 
complex, is analogous, as an instrument, to the symbolic system 
employed by mathematicians. 

By its use it is the servant, and the necessary servant, of 
thought ; by its abuse it becomes the compeer, or even the sup- 
planter, of thought. 

It is evident from the above considerations than any gross 
structural or functional derangement of the language mechanism 
must necessarily seriously impede, or even in certain cases pre- 
vent, the adequate performance of those complex processes of 
association which serve as the physical basis of the psychic 
functions. 

Apart from its general bearing on the higher functions of the 
cerebrum, this discussion of the language mechanism has served a 
further purpose, namely, the paving of the way to a clear under- 
standing of the far-reaching importance of the investigations of 
Marie on the subject of aphasia. 

With the latter object in view, another allied subject will now 
be considered, namely, the gross modifications of cerebral function 
which are the necessary consequences of congenital or acquired 
deprivation of the senses of hearing and sight. In cases of sense 
_ deprivation, the structural and functional maiming of the cerebrum 
concerns one or more of its earliest developed, most stable, and 
functionally lowest parts, namely, the centres of projection. Such 
lesions consequently present a simpler problem for study, in spite 
of their far-reaching influence on the functions of the cerebrum, 
than do the variable and often diffuse lesions which produce the 
complex symptomatology of aphasia. 

The senses of sight and hearing, especially the latter in un- 
educated individuals, are so necessary to, and play such an im- 
portant part in, both the evolution and the conservation of the 


_ 
328 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND — 


normal functions of the cerebrum, that deprivation of one or both 
of these senses in congenital or early cases grossly modifies, and in 
adult cases necessitates an entire readjustment of, the associa- 
tional processes which constitute the physical basis of the psychic 
functions. 

In cases of deafness or blindness in which the deprivation is 
congenital or is acquired in early life, the psychic functions are 
either very imperfectly evolved, or are performed in an entirely 
abnormal manner. In early or congenital deafness, the complex 
mechanism for the reception, storage, and reproduction of lan- 
guage, or the symbolic representation of the results of sensorial 
excitation and of psychic association, is incapable of evolution, 
unless the patients are laboriously educated through other avenues 
of sensation. It is hardly necessary to add that mutism is a. 
necessary consequence of this disability, though a considerable 
development of lip-language can often be induced by education. 
Such patients, in fact, unless educated by special methods, would 
necessarily possess mental functions relatively little removed from ~ 
those of the lower primates. On the other hand, the congenital 
or early blind can obtain a large and important part of their mental 
content by means of the sense of hearing, just as do ordinary un- 
educated (7.e. non-reading and non-writing) individuals. That 
the former can supplement their methods for the acquisition and 
communication of information by means of the deaf and dumb— 
alphabet, &c., and the latter by means of the tactile motor sense, 
does not affect the fundamental difference between them, which 
is based on the fact that a highly important part of the mental 
content is normally (in the uneducated) acquired by means of the 
sense of hearing and not by that of sight. 

In such cases deafness is therefore a more serious deprivation 
than blindness, as, for the evolution of the functional activity of 
the cerebrum, an entirely new development of associational spheres, 
to replace those normally employed for auditory and spoken lan- 
guage, has to be acquired. In the case of congenital or early 
acquired blindness, on the other hand, the complex sphere of 
language, with all its psychic components, can be employed in a 
perfectly normal manner, and almost exactly as it is brought into 
use in the case of persons who neither read nor write. 

Deprivation of sight or hearing, when occurring later in life, 
results, in the educated, in relatively less cerebral disability, and 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM = 329 


probably an approximately equal amount in the case of either 
of these senses. In the uneducated, loss of hearing produces 
greater cerebral disability than does loss of sight. 

In all cases of acquired deafness or blindness, however, the 
following results ensue. On the one hand, the patient suffers a 
permanent loss of one or both of the important avenues of special 
sensation, and on the other, all kinds and degrees of structural 
and functional impairment develop in the cerebrum in conse- 
quence of the deprivation. Not only does secondary atrophy of 
the particular afferent fibres to the cerebrum result, but the com- 
plex associational relations between the special projection area or 
areas and the rest of the cerebrum are seriously affected. The 
special sensori-memorial images dependent on the lost sense or 
senses pass more and more into the sphere of the permanently sub- 
conscious. The physical bases for the evolution of even the most 
elementary (already experienced) percepts require readjustment to 
the altered conditions. Finally, the mechanism for the development 
of new and the correction and continuation of already experienced 
percepts, which normally involves the majority of, if not all, the 
projection or sensory areas of the cerebrum, together with their 
related memorial spheres, becomes imperfect or “‘ maimed.” 

In all these types, therefore, both sensory and also extensive 
and grave associational deprivations exist; and the cerebrum, 
as a machine, is maimed not’only in its most stable and earliest 
acquired regions, namely, in one or more centres of projection 
or sensory areas, but also throughout its intricate, later evolved, 
and more important (from the psychic aspect) systems of lower 
association. 

Sense deprivation is, however, followed, in all, or nearly all, 
early and congenital cases, and in many acquired cases, by still 
more serious disturbances of the psychic functions than those 
already indicated. In the former, imperfect evolution of the 
higher functions of mind is usual if not invariable ; and in both 
types dissolution of the centre of higher association, with conse- 
quent dementia, is of common occurrence. In congenital cases 
involution of the centre of higher association, with resulting 
dementia, is eventually incited by the stress of prolonged sense 
deprivation and the consequent abnormal modes of psychic asso- 
ciation which result. In other words, the abnormally working 
psychic machine sooner or later breaks down. 


‘ 


330 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


In persons who acquire sense deprivation later in life, the 
mental stress involved on the one hand in the sense disability, 
and on the other in the more or less unsuccessful attempts to 
revive the related memories which tend to pass more and more 
into the permanently subconscious, or to replace the absence of 
these memories by the integration of percepts and concepts on 
am unusual sensori-memorial basis, often, or perhaps invariably, 
results in the development of irritability, or depression, or general 
emotional instability. In rare cases like one cited by Clouston, 
partial removal of the sense deprivation by operation (for cata- 
ract) may result in a return to normal psychic life. In the case, 
however, of individuals who possess higher cortical neurones of 
deficient durability, insanity followed by dementia ensues. 

It may finally be stated that cases of congenital or early 
acquired deafness are more liable to imperfect mental development, 
with which is associated mutism, than are cases of congenital or 
early acquired blindness. 

Further, both in the cases in which the sense deprivation is 
congenital or acquired early in life, and in those in which it is 
acquired after adult life has been reached, cerebral involution is 
more likely to occur in the case of the deaf than in that of the 
blind. This statement is supported by a series of cases which 
were recently published by the writer, for, of the ten in the series, 
three were deaf and dumb, two were deaf, four were almost or 
totally deaf and blind, and only one, a well-marked high-grade 
ament, who had been certified for thirty-seven years, was blind. 


RECENT RESEARCHES ON APHASIA 


The description of the language mechanism and the evidence 
of its fundamental importance to the performance of the psychic 
functions—especially that derived from the study of cases of 
sense deprivation—have been given at considerable length. Whilst 
the primary object of the writer has been the elucidation of the 
manner in which the higher functions of the cerebrum are per- 
formed, he has at the same time endeavoured to so present the 
subject of language as to prepare the reader, not merely for the 
appreciation of the gist of the recent researches on aphasia, but 
for the acceptance of their results as truths of great importance. 

The statements, that our views of aphasia require entire re- 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM 331 


vision, and that the time-honoured localisation of the motor speech 

centre in the cortex of the posterior portion of the third frontal 
gyrus (the speech area of Broca) is incorrect, are of so revolu- 
tionary a nature that the reader may well require more evidence 
of their truth than is afforded by the papers even of so renowned 
a neurologist as Pierre Marie. Such evidence as can be derived 
from the psycho-physiological study of the subject of language, 
and from the results of recent research on cortical localisation and 
cerebral function, the writer has endeavoured to produce. 
Before indicating the views of Marie, it is desirable to refer 
shortly to the theory of aphasia, advanced almost simultaneously 
by Monakow in 1906. This investigator throws doubt on the 
accepted localisation of the speech centre, and expresses the 
opinion that aphasic disorders are caused less by destruction of 
certain special regions of the cerebrum than by local losses of 
functional continuity between the various centres which together 
constitute the language mechanism. To this disturbance of 
functional continuity he applies the term diaschisis. He indicates 
that local affection of a connecting tract of fibres may cause tem- 
porary suspension of the functions of one part of the mechanism 
by removing from this the stimulus to action which normally 
proceeds from another part. On the other hand, relatively inde- 
pendent parts of the mechanism may pass into a state of abnormal 
activity from lack of the controlling impulses which normally 
proceed to them from other portions. Disturbances of local 
function may thus arise in consequence of lesions situated in. 
different parts of the brain. 

This theory is propounded by Monakow to explain the frequent 
discrepancies which exist between the clinical symptomatology 
and the anatomical lesions of cases of aphasia. 

He points out that many cases of aphasia occur in which search 
for the expected lesion gives a negative result ; that a still larger’ 
number of cases recover although the lesion persists and even 
becomes more extensive ; and that aphasic disorders often persist 
in cases in which the lesion is found to be situated beyond the 
limits of the speech area. He further remarks that “ sensory 
aphasia” may occur with lesion of Broca’s gyrus, and “ motor 
aphasia ” with lesion of the zone of Wernicke ; and that at times 
the symptomatology varies greatly in cases in which the situation 
and relations of the lesion are practically identical. 


332 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


The views of Monakow may thus be regarded as affording 
a plausible explanation of the contrary and negative evidence, 
with regard to the localisation of the speech-centre, which has 
been published ever since the time of Charcot. 

The articles of Pierre Marie, the first of which was published 
some months earlier than the paper of Monakow, treat the subject 
of aphasia in a more radical manner. 

Marie, as the result of a study of the subject during a period 
of ten years at the hospital at Bicétre, has expressed the opinion 
that the whole of the accepted doctrines regarding aphasia require 
reconsideration. 

He distinguishes in the orthodox manner between “ motor 
aphasia’ or the aphasia of Broca, “sensory aphasia” or the 
aphasia of Wernicke, and “ anarthria” or the subcortical motor 
aphasia of Déjérine. 

In the first of these conditions the patient is usually described 
as being unable to speak, although he understands what is said 
to him, and as a rule retains his intellectual powers unimpaired. 
In the second the patient is regarded as being able to speak in a 
more or less intelligible manner, but to have more or less marked 
impairment of intelligence. In the third, the only disability which 
exists is loss or affection of the power of articulate speech. 

In order to explain the views of Marie it is unnecessary to 
consider in detail the various more or less complicated types of 
symptomatology, which occur in cases of aphasia, and which are 
readily classed under one or other of the aphasias of Broca and of 
Wernicke. For such a description the reader is referred to the 
various classical publications. 

As the result of his investigations Marie considers that only 
one form of true aphasia exists, namely, that of Wernicke, and 
that the so-called “aphasia of Broca” is the “aphasia of Wer- 
nicke ” + “ anarthria.”” He considers that Broca’s gyrus is not 
concerned with the function of speech, that the “ area of Wernicke,” 
namely, the supra-marginal and angular gyri, and the hinder 
portions of the first and second temporo-sphenoidal gyri, is the 
site of the lesion which causes true aphasia, and that a lesion of 
the lenticular zone is the cause of anarthria. 

Marie thus limits the lesion of aphasia to the region of the 
cerebrum which has till of late been regarded as the seat of the 
visual and auditory word-centres. 


G 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM — 333 


Further, and this is the most important part of the doctrine, 

Marie urges that in true aphasia intellectual impairment is 
invariably present. “C'est quil y a chez les aphasiques 
quelque chose de bien plus important et de bien plus grave 
que la perte du sens des mots; il y a une diminution tres 
marquée dans la capacité intellectuelle en général.” He considers 
that the notion of intellectual impairment should dominate the 
doctrine of aphasia. 

He affirms that in all cases of the aphasias both of Broca and 
of Wernicke there is a greater or lesser amount of difficulty in 
the understanding of spoken language, and that evidence of definite 
diminution of the intellectual powers can invariably be obtained 
if the patients are properly studied. For this purpose he gives 
his subjects complicated instructions. Instead, for example, of 
telling the patient to cough, spit, put out his tongue, or shut his 
eyes, he gives certain instructions of which the following two are 
very commonly employed. “Of three pieces of paper of unequal 
size which are placed on this table, you will give me the largest, 
you will crumple up the medium one and throw it on the ground, 
as to the least you will put it in your pocket.” ... “ You will 
stand up, you will go and tap three times on the window with your 
finger, then you will return before this table, you will walk round 
your chair, and you will sit down.” He points out that, on 
superficial observation, aphasics may appear to possess normal 
intelligence, but that careful investigation readily enables their 
defective mental powers to be determined. He gives, as an 
illustration, a description of the blunders made by an aphasic who, 
prior to the onset of his infirmity, had been a really good cook, 
when he was provided with the necessary ingredients and instructed 
to prepare an “ ceuf sur le plat” or fried egg. His account of the 
incident is as follows :— , 

“Un de mes malades, atteint depuis des années d’une aphasie 
d’intensité moyenne qui ne l’empéche d’ailleurs pas de se méler a 
la vie commune, est un cuisinier, un bon cuisinier qui, sans aucun 
doute, savait bien son métier. Je lui demandai un jour de me 
faire un ‘ceuf sur le plat.’ Nous nous rendimes donc tous 4 la 
cuisine, avec la surveillante qui devait remplir les fonctions 
d’expert. La, devant le fourneau, on remit au malade les in- 
grédients necessaires: un plat, un ceuf, du beurre, du poivre et du 
sel et on lengagea & exercer ses talents. Cet homme hésite’ un 


334 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


moment, puis commet les solécismes suivants, qui nous sont au 
fur et & mesure signalés par notre surveillante, passablement 
scandalisée de voir un cuisinier se tirer aussi mal d’une épreuve 
qui, pour une simple ménagére, n’efit été qu’un jeu: il commence 
par casser son ceuf de fagon fort maladroite et le vide dans le plat 
sans aucune précaution pour éviter de crever le jaune, puis il met 
du beurre dans le plat, par-dessus l’ceuf, saupoudre de sel et de 
poivre et met le tout au four. C’était la une faute capitale et la 
surveillante nous fit remarquer qu'il avait fait l’inverse de ce qui 
devait étre fait, le beurre devant étre chauffé au préalable et l’ceuf 
jeté dedans. Inutile d’ajouter que le plat n’était absolument pas 
présentable, ce qui, d’ailleurs, ne parut pas émouvoir outre 
mesure notre malade. Ici encore il est bien évident qu'il ne 
sagissait pas d’un trouble du langage, mais d’une déchéance 
intellectuelle.” 

Further, Marie denies that aphasics are able to express their 
meaning, in the absence of speech, by means of descriptive mimicry. 
For example, he states that he has never yet come across a patient 
who was able in this way to indicate his occupation when asked 
to do so. 

He thus considers aphasia to be essentially characterised by 
an impairment of the intellectual powers, and he therefore regards 
the “area of Wernicke” as an intellectual and not a sensory | 
centre. His views are consequently in accord with the more ‘ 
recent results of the study of cortical localisation, for the area of . 
Wernicke is a zone of association and not an area of projection. ; 
The visual and auditory word-centres naturally lie in either the 
“sensory” or the “ psychic ” zones devoted to vision and hearing 
respectively, for there is no essential difference between the cortical 
projection of, and the conservation of the image of, a word, and 
of any other cause of sensorial stimulation. Marie, in fact, though 
he attributes word-deafness to defective comprehension and denies 
the existence of this as a pure symptom, admits the existence of 
word-blindness as a fairly pure symptom, associates it with hemia- 
nopsia, and locates its lesion in the general visual centres. In 
this connection the attention of the reader may be usefully drawn 
to the observations already made on the psycho-physiology of 
' the language mechanism in educated and uneducated persons. 
Language is learned through the sense of hearing, and it is only 
by education that it is further learned by the sense of sight. Word 

j « 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM = 335 


vision is thus an additional acquisition superadded to the mechanism 
of language, which under normal circumstances can carry on its 
functions in the absence of the capacity to read and write. It is 
therefore only to be expected that word-blindness should be found. 
clinically to be fairly common as a pure symptom, and should be 
less intimately associated with the general functions of the cerebrum 
than word-deafness, which disability necessarily seriously maims 
the normal, and earliest developed, mechanism for the acquisition 
and reproduction of articulate speech. 

It may be added that Marie does not .deny the existence of 
pure motor aphasia, but he terms this “ anarthria,” and locates 
_ its lesion in the lenticular zone, namely, “in the lenticular nucleus, 
in its neighbourhood, in the anterior part and the genu of the 
internal capsule, or, may be, in the external capsule.” That such 
a lesion may cause the subcortical motor aphasia of Déjérine is 
of course generally recognised, but Marie goes further and denies 
that pure motor aphasia can also occur in consequence of a lesion 
in Broca’s gyrus. 

It is impossible to summarise here the pathological evidence 
which is produced by Marie in support of his contention, but the 
general scope of his inquiry may be indicated. 

He states that an examination of the original specimens of 
Broca clearly shows that they do not afford adequate support to 
his localisation of the speech-centre in the posterior portion of 
the third frontal gyrus. He remarks that when Broca enunciated 
his doctrine in 1861 he was thirty-seven years of age, and was not 
yet a professor; and that Bouillaud was sixty-nine years of age, 
and had been professor of clinical medicine at la Charité for thirty 
years. He then suggests that Broca’s views gained acceptance 
owing to the still dominant influence of Bouillaud, who even in 
1825 had localised the faculty of articulate speech in the frontal 
lobes, which localisation Bouillaud regarded as confirmatory in 
this respect of the phrenological doctrines of Gall. Marie then 
traces the crystallisation of Broca’s views into the form of a dogma 
to the experimental investigations of Fritsch and Hitzig, of Ferrier 
and Yeo, &c. 

With regard to his own researches, Marie states that his doctrine 
of aphasia is based on the systematic examination, during a period 
of ten years, of nearly a hundred cases of aphasia, which included 
more than fifty autopsies. 


336 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


He adopts a twofold line of argument to prove that “la 
troisiéme circonvolution frontale gauche ne joue aucun réle spécial — 
dans la fonction du langage ” (p. 243). On the one hand he shows 
that cases exist, in right-handed individuals, in which local de- 
struction of the posterior part of the third frontal convolution is 
not followed by aphasia. One of Marie’s figures in illustration 
of such a case is reproduced opposite (Fig. 7). 

On the other hand he shows that characteristic examples of 
the aphasia of Broca occur in which the third left frontal convolu- 
tion is absolutely intact. In Fig. 8 is reproduced an illustration 
of such a case. 

The patient was a beautiful example of the aphasia of Broca. 
The gyrus of Broca is intact. The “aphasia of Broca” which 
was presented by the patient is here attributed to a lesion R-R1, 
which is the cause of the anarthria, and to a lesion R1-R11, which 
has given rise to the aphasia. 

The views of Marie, which in his first paper were indicated and 
illustrated, rather than supported by the complete evidence on 
which they are based, have naturally re-aroused interest in a 
subject which of late years has been taught in dogmatic form 
rather than investigated. One of his earliest critics was Déjérine, 
who bitterly opposed his doctrines with regard to both sensory and 
motor aphasia, and endeavoured to demonstrate that they are in 
complete opposition to the facts derived from clinical observation. 
Marie, in a subsequent article, replied in a masterly and restrained 
manner to the vehement criticisms of Déjérine, and, in a third, 
published an essay on the genesis and mode of evolution of the 
doctrine of Broca. 

In September 1907 an important discussion took place at the 
International Congress at Amsterdam, and many of the leading 
neurologists in the world expressed their views on aphasia. It 
is a significant fact that the speakers as a whole appeared to lay 
stress on the absence of definite knowledge regarding the exact 
lesions which produce the different types of symptomatology 
grouped under the term “aphasia,” rather than to discuss any 
particular types of lesion. 

The chief evidence on which the views of Marie are based has 
recently been published by his pupil Moutier in a bulky volume 
entitled L’Aphasie de Broca. In this work the whole question 
of aphasia is elaborately and fully discussed. After a historical 


. 
| 
; 


-o— 


Fic. 7. Fic. 8. 


Fic. 7 (Marie, 1906).—Horizontal section of the left Paar aaeg of Ber... 
The third frontal convolution is almost entirely destroyed by a softening. This 
patient showed neither any sign of Aphasia nor any anarthric trouble. 


Fic. 8 (Marie, 1906).—Horizontal section of the left hemisphere of Per. . . . 

The softening met with here is the type of deep softening of form B. It is 
indicated by the black line which, from R to R’, occupies the external capsule and 
the base of the white substance of the convolutions of the insula. At R’ the 
line of softening bifurcates and extends forward and inward towards the internal 
capsule ; posteriorly the softening crosses the temporo-parietal isthmus, and passes 
into the white substance of the temporo-occipital lobe R”, where it occupies the 
neighbourhood of the external wall of the oe horn of the lateral ventricle, and 
follows its contour closely ; it leaves this wall quite at the back of it and ends in 
the white substance of the occipital lobe. 

This patient was one of the finest cases of the Aphasia of Broca that I have had 
the opportunity of observing. Here the Aphasia of Broca ‘had been set up, owing 
to the ‘oles R R’ which had caused Anarthria, and to the lesion R’ R” which had 
caused Aphasia, 

na 


7. 


338 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


description of the genesis and mode of evolution of the doctrine 
of Broca, the following table (p. 96) is introduced— 


RELEVE TOTAL DES CAS AVEC AUTOPSIE (1861-1906) . : . 804 
Observations inutilisables . ‘ : : : : . ‘ . 201 
1. Insuffisantes . : : : : : ; : . 26 
2. Lésions trop étendues_ . ‘ ; , , ; . 175 

RESTENT 103 OBSERVATIONS AVEC DESTRUCTION LOCALISKE : 

1, Favorables a la 3° frontale (ou tenues pour telles) ‘ : . 19 
(a) Lésion corticale : ‘ : . ; ' ; we 
(b) Lésion sous-corticale . ; : : : , ramp & 

2. Contraires a la 3¢frontale  . , : : ; . 84 
(A) Il y aaphasie. Le pied de F3 eat intact : : . 57 


(B) Il n’y a pas d’aphasie. Le pied de F3 est détruit  . 27 
(a) Par traumatisme ; ; : ; 4 
(6) Partumeur . : : : , ; . (14 
(c) Par ramollissement. : : ‘ ; P 5 
(ad) Des deux cétés : 2 
(e) Chirurgicalement chez un droitier : 2 


In this table the author summarises all the cases, recorded 
up to 1906, in which the results of post-mortem examination are 
stated. It will be noted by the reader that, of the 304 recorded 
cases, but 103 possess the necessary details or exhibit sufficiently 
circumscribed lesions, for their utilisation as evidence for or against 
the doctrine of Broca. Of these 103 cases but 19 are in favour 
of, and no less than 84 are against, the localisation of the speech- 
centre in the posterior part of the third frontal convolution. 

The subject of aphasia is considered in detail from both 
anatomical and clinical aspects. A complete bibliography from 
1861 to 1907 is inserted. According to their relative importance, 
more or less lengthy details regarding the cases recorded in the 
several publications are inserted in a convenient form for reference. 
Half the volume is devoted to a description of the personal obser- 

‘vations of the author. The methods employed for the clinical 
examination of aphasics are described, and detailed records of 
44 cases are inserted. The book contains 175 illustrations, many 
of which are full-page plates. 

The bearing of the views of Marie on the subject of cortical 
localisation will now be considered. 

It must at once be confessed that the doctrines of Marie are 
destructive rather than constructive from the aspect of cortical 


————— rl 


- ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM — 339 


localisation. On the other hand, from the aspect of cerebral 
function, they are throughout constructive in tendency. From 
both points of view, however, they are in accord with the results 


of recent histological and psycho-physiological research. 


From the aspect of cortical localisation, Broca’s speech-centre 
is disrated by Marie. This is a conclusion for which modern 
histological investigations have already prepared the way. On 
referring to the maps of Brodmann and of Campbell, the reader 
will see that both these observers indicate a histological type 
of cortex (“intermediate precentral” of Campbell, and 44 of 
Brodmann) in the posterior third of the inferior frontal con- 
volution, which differs from that of the psychomotor area. 
Campbell, in fact, whose monograph appeared before the publi- 
cation of the papers of Marie, protested against the assumption 
that a lesion of the area of Broca is the essential factor in the 
production of motor aphasia on the ground that this region is 
histologically undifferentiable from, and is included in, his “ inter- 
mediate precentral” area. He cites two cases of destruction of 
Broca’s area in which permanent motor aphasia existed. In a 
third case, however, in which the area of destruction is even 
more extensive, the patient, though twelve years previously he had 
suffered from complete but transitory aphasia, exhibited no speech 
defect whatever during the whole of this period and right up to 
the time of his death. 

The more detailed map of Brodmann, it is true, indicates a 
special histological type (No. 44) in Broca’s area, but this type 
does not possess “ motor” characteristics, and is indeed separated 
from the psychomotor or Betz-cell region (No. 4) by an area (No. 6) 
which, though less in extent, is similar in distribution to the “ inter- 
mediate precentral” area of Campbell. If, as Campbell suggests, 
this last area is “ specially designed for the control of skilled move- 
ments of an associated kind,” his argument re motor aphasia is 
strengthened by the exclusion from this “ intermediate precentral ” 
region, by Brodmann, of Broca’s atea. 

The views of Marie will now be considered with regard to. their 
bearing on our knowledge of the functions of the cerebrum. Marie 
states that the essential feature of aphasia is the existence of 
intellectual impairment, although the patient may appear, from 
the aspect of every-day life, to possess normal mental faculties. 
To put this opinion into other words, Marie in effect states that 


340 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


the sufferer from aphasia is sane, but at the same time suffers 
from intellectual disability. 

As bas been remarked earlier in this article, the prefrontal _ 
region (No. 10 of Brodmann) is the highest zone of association, 
and insanity depends on sub-evolution or dissolution of this region. 
In the case of sub-evolution the patient may be permanently 
idiotic or imbecile, permanently or temporarily insane, or liable 
to the ‘onset of insanity: in the case of dissolution, according 
to its degree, the patient suffers from a corresponding grade of 
permanent dementia. 

On the other hand, it has been stated that the post- and infra- 
Rolandic regions of the cerebrum contain the sensory projection 
spheres and the various zones of association subserving the 
complex associational processes which occur with regard to the | 
projection spheres individually and collectively. A gross lesion 
situated in the “area of Wernicke,” 7.e. in the very midst of this 
region of association, must therefore seriously interfere with the 
performance of the cerebral functions of lower association, namely, 
with the evolution of percepts of various types and grades of 
complexity. 

Further, according to the distribution and depth of the lesion 
almost any grade and type of psycho-sensorial disability may be 
expected. The greater its proximity to the visual or auditory 
projection spheres, the greater must be the likelihood of relatively 
pure word blindness or deafness. For reasons already stated, 
the former of these disabilities can only occur in persons who 
have learned to read and write, and it is the more likely to exist 
in a pure form. The latter can occur in either educated or un- 
educated individuals, and in either case is unlikely to exist in a 
pure form (see pp. 334-5). 

On the other hand, a lesion, and especially a superficial one, 
located away from the projection spheres, would be expected to 
cause disabilities with regard to the more complicated processes 
of lower association, with resulting intellectual impairment. 

As will have been gathered from the remarks on the function 
of language, the complex mechanism which serves as its physical 
basis must necessarily be co-extensive in distribution with the 
projection spheres, and the whole region of lower assocviation. 
The visual and auditory word-centres lie in or near the visual and 
‘auditory projection spheres, as words seen and heard are merely 


) 
‘ 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM 341 


one variety of sensorial impression. On the other hand, the more 
or less complex cerebral processes which are needed for the evolu- 
tion of the percept symbolised by a certain word may involve the 


cortex in and near each of the several projection spheres. For 


example, the word “ mouse” at once sets on foot processes of 
association which pass to every projection sphere with the solitary 
exception of the gustatory, and even this may be reached in a 
person who has eaten a fried mouse in the hope of thereby re- 
covering from an attack of whooping cough. 

We may note a word, i.e. receive the sensorial impression 
produced by it; we may go a step further and recognise it as 
understandable ; but it is only when, by active, though not 
necessarily voluntary, processes of cerebral association, numerous 
sensori-memorial images of different orders have thereby been 
aroused into the field of consciousness, that the word acquires 
meaning. These processes of association are of widespread dis- 
tribution, and differ in detail, though they are similar in their 
general tendency, on each occasion on which they occur. In the 
normal brain these processes of association are directed and con- 
trolled, and suitable sensori-memorial neurone complexes are ac- 
cepted, whilst undesirable are rejected, by means of the centre of 
higher association. In dreamy states they follow certain natural 
laws, and their course depends solely on the existing state, as 
regards excitability, of the various associated groups of neurones. 
In the maimed brain of an aphasic, whether it be functioning 
in a controlled or in an involuntary manner, such processes 
of association are necessarily imperfectly and inadequately 
performed. 

The fairly common, rather than very occasional, determination 
of an “area of Wernicke,” and therefore of “ aphasia ” as a special 
symptomatology, like the determination of other special symp- 
tomatologies through local cerebral lesions, depends, in fact, on 
the distribution, and consequent liability to occlusion, of certain 
branches of the cerebral arteries. Had the arterial supply of the 
cerebrum been evolved in a different manner, it is conceivable 
that “aphasia,” as it occurs, would be unknown, for lesions due, 
eg. to tumours, traumatism, &c., would not have sufficed for its 
complete identification and description. 

“ Aphasia,” however, exists, as a complex of varying types of 
symptomatology ; and the study of these by Marie has produced 


342 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


a mass of evidence which confirms the correctness of the obser- 
vations with regard to cortical localisation and cerebral function 
which have been recorded during recent years. These observations 
are difficult to correlate with—in fact, they are in many respects 
opposed to—the hitherto current doctrines with regard to aphasia ; 
but they have received confirmation and derived illumination from 
the researches of Marie. 


The attention of the reader will finally be drawn to certain 
matters of interest with regard to the functions of the cerebrum, 
and to a number of recent publications, the consideration of which 
could not be conveniently introduced into the general text of this 
article without interference with its continuity. 

The first subject which will be briefly indicated is termed 
“alteration of personality.” This may be conveniently defined 
as a mental state in which the higher cerebral functions are 
exercised, not over psychic processes founded on such recently 
acquired time-related portions of the content of mind as con- 
stitute the normal personality, but over psychic processes founded 
on complex and time-related portions of the subconscious content 
of mind, which exhibit such abnormal prominence as to entirely 
replace for the time those recent experiences on which normal 
cerebral activity depends. In such cases not only one, but several 
such time-related portions of former experience, may separately 
and at different times acquire abnormal prominence, and thereby 
give rise to the phenomena of multiple personality. In the normal 
individual, on the other hand, the recent time-related personality 
cannot be voluntarily subordinated, and all that is possible in this 
direction is the occurrence of some degree of associational elabora- 
tion of former sensori-memorial images, which is always imperfect 
and often incorrect. To test the truth of this statement, the 
reader needs only to endeavour to recall, in a time-related manner, 
the events of yesterday. 

This “alteration of personality” or switching on of a former 
period of cerebral activity, with temporary obliteration of later 
experience, is common in hysteria, epilepsy, and hypnotic states. 
The phenomenon is of importance in that it proves that, whilst 
the exercise of the cerebral functions is an active process which 
derives its pabulum from both the past and the present, the whole 
of the psychic life is nevertheless recorded in the cerebrum in a@ 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM 343 


time-related manner. Thus, in certain pathological states of the 
cerebrum, not only may the clock of cerebral activity be put 
back, but the subject may reproduce in exact detail lengthy 
portions of former experience, and, stranger still, may start an 
entirely new time-related psychic life from the point of former 
experience to which he has returned. He may then, as if nothing 
had happened, return to the normal point of cerebral activity 
and recommence his ordinary time-related psychic life, with com- 
plete obliteration, as far as his conscious knowledge is concerned, 
of the experiences he has passed through whilst in the abnormal 
psychic state. It is possible, then, for a person to live two or 
more psychic lives, of which only one is normal, and in his normal 
waking state to be entirely unaware of the existence, apart from 
periods of time for which he cannot account, of his other, or 
numerous, “sub-egos.” Interesting cases of “ multiple person- 
ality ” which might seem incredible had they not been studied and 
recorded by competent and entirely trustworthy observers, have 
recently been published by Morton Prince and by Albert Wilson. 
Three less important cases have also been published by Lemaitre. 
Such cases differ somewhat from good examples of systematised 
delusional insanity, which are, however, of interest in this con- 
nection. In the latter the personality is altered, but this altera- 
tion is due, in the developed state, to the permanently abnormal 
prominence of certain time-related portions of what should be 
part of the subconscious content of mind. These particular time- 
related experiences serve as a basis on which develops a continually 
increasing aggregation of abnormal psychic units. In other words, 
in place of the normal gradually changing personality, a certain 
former personality remains as a permanent basis on which is built 
up a continually increasing abnormal psychic edifice. In such 
cases, when they become “ chronic,” it is probable that the greater 
part of the available psychic content consists of symbolic verbal 
groupings which have become relatively stable through frequent 
repetition ; and that the processes of cerebral association required 
for the reintegration of the former percepts and concepts which 
these verbal groupings symbolise, and for the revival of old sensori- 
memorial images, are markedly reduced. These symbolic verbal 
groupings continue throughout the life of the sufferer to entirely 
dominate what would otherwise be relatively normal processes of 
immediate cerebral activity, and in this, in effect though greater 


f 
: 


344 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


in degree, resemble the “opinions” of many of the one-idea-ed 
“cranks” in the outside world. 

It may be remarked that these various abnormal types of 
cerebral activity are at present chiefly of academic interest, apart 
from their obvious bearing on criminology. Our knowledge of the 
psycho-physiology of the brain is still in its infancy, and though 
it 4s at any rate certain that in such cases the cerebrum is not 
functioning in a normal manner during the phases of “ alteration 
of personality,” not only is no satisfactory explanation yet forth- 
coming, but it is even difficult to be certain in the complicated 
cases which of the several phases represents the “normal.” It 
is probable, however, that the subject will receive illumination 
when our present crude acquaintance with mental disease is 
replaced by the scientific knowledge which will undoubtedly 
follow its systematic study. 

Though the more difficult question of the time-related recording 
of psychic experience in the cerebrum, and of the influence of this 
on the normal processes of cerebral association, is at present an 
obscure one, the simpler problem of the manner in which the 
immediate functions of the cerebrum are performed derives much 
illumination from the study of insanity. It would be an un- 
warrantable digression to discuss here the various abnormal 
types of immediate cerebral activity which are met with during 
the clinical investigation of mental disease. One or two examples 
may, however, with advantage be introduced for illustrative 
purposes. 

In certain cases classed under the symptomatological group 
of “ mania,” the centre of higher association is in functional abey- 
ance, and the cerebrum acts as an uncontrolled sensori-psycho- 
motor machine. Instead of, as normally happens, but a small 
selected number of the ever-entering stream of afferent impres- 
sions being noticed, almost every visual and auditory stimulus in 
the neighbourhood of the patient is accepted. Processes of cerebral 
association incited\by these occur pell-mell, and find motor ex- 
pression in emotional disturbance, rapid movements, and a riotous 
display of words. It is impossible to obtain more than the 
momentary attention of the sufferer. This condition of uncon- 
- trolled sensori-psychomotor activity may last for many weeks, 
after which the patient becomes, at any rate for the time, sane. 
Such patients usually exhibit numerous signs of physical and 

e 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM = 345 


mental degeneracy ; but all grades exist, from individuals who 
may be regarded, in their normal condition, as “ sane,” to others, 
of greater cerebral degeneracy, who are never really so. In such 
cases the cerebrum, as regards its lower complexes, is “ racing ” ; 
and, in the more degenerate types, objective signs of generally 
aberrent and sub-normal cerebral activity are permanently in 
evidence. 

On the other hand, cases are common in which, from toxemia 
of the cortical neurones and consequent pathological conditions of 
these elements, all grades and types of temporary or permanent 
maiming of the processes of cerebral association are existent. 
Such patients exhibit what is described as ‘‘ mental confusion.” 
The earliest evolved and most stable parts, 7.e. the projection 
spheres, are the least affected, and hence the patients are able 
to receive sensory impressions in a more or less normal manner. 
The later evolved zones of lower association (and especially the 
latest evolved centre of higher association) are, however, more 
seriously affected, and hence the patients make frequent mistakes 
with regard to the identifying of sensations, suffer from halluci- 
nations, and may exhibit any type and grade of defective com- 
prehension and any symptom of “aphasia.” In such cases the 
cerebrum is temporarily or permanently maimed owing to lesion 
of its constituent elements. __ 

Though examples of abnormal cerebral activity of the immediate 
type might be multiplied to any extent, those given above will 
suffice to indicate that our knowledge of the higher functions of 
the cerebrum, though still in its early stages, does not depend 
solely on histological, neuro-pathological, and experimental data, 
but may derive illumination and illustration from the study of 
mental disease. 


A number of recent papers of interest will now be referred to. 

The mechanism and localisation of the psychic processes has 
formed the subject of a recent paper by Jendrassik. This writer 
denies the existence of an “ association centre,” on the ground that 
the connections between memory images are not actual paths but 
occur through a “‘ tuning” of associated images, with the result that 
if one of these is evoked the images in harmony with it are also aroused 
into consciousness. His theoretical discussion is rather unsatisfactory, 
but the deduction may be readily drawn that he considers association 
_ to be a process, and the psychic products, which are evolved as results 


346 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


of this, to have no fixed cerebral centres. His opinion thus approxi- 
mates to the views enunciated during the course of this article. 

Bianchi, on the other hand, considers that the cerebrum, apart 
from the frontal lobes, is the seat of centres for percepts, and that a 
centre for concepts exists in the prefrontal region. He argues that 
both percepts and concepts exist apart from words, though the evidence 
he adduces is chiefly or entirely in favour of the existence of words 
in the absence of percepts and concepts. He severely criticises the 
doctrine of Flechsig, chiefly on the ground that the phenomena of » 
anatomical evolution do not correspond with those of the development — 
of functional activity. For example, he remarks that the supposed 
centre for reading in an imbecile may be completely myelinated although 
the subject may never have learned to read. He states that histo- 
logical evidence is not in favour of the areas of projection possessing 
a simpler structure than the regions of association. He discusses the- 
complex nature of the processes of association which are necessary 
for the synthesis of a perception. He thus regards the portion of the 
cortex which is now differentiated into areas of projection or sensory 
areas and into posterior regions of association, as a series of perceptive 
zones ; and he is of the opinion that the only regions of the brain 
which can be regarded as associational are the prefrontal lobes, which 
are generally admitted to contain no projection fibres. He regards 
these lobes as the region of cerebral executive government, and con- 
siders that here the elaborated products of the perceptive zones, or 
mantellar parliament, are fused together. 

Instead of three anatomical grades in the hierarchy of cerebral 
function, namely, areas of projection, regions of lower association, 
and a region of higher association, Bianchi thus recognises but two, 
zones of perception and a centre for concepts.. Further, he regards 
percepts and concepts as entities with an anatomical basis apart from 
that for words. His opinion thus differs from the views elaborated 
during the course »f this article, namely, that percepts and concepts 
are merely psychc'ogical generalisations signifying the results of 
‘processes of cerebrai association which differ in detail, though they 
possess an underlying gen¢ral similarity, on each occasion on which 
they occur. Bianchi regards words, not as symbols for the integration 
of processes of cerebral axsociation without which they are meaningless, 
but as means for the communication of already existing percepts and 
concepts. Language, in the opinion of Bianchi, is thus merely a 
mechanism for the expression of thought, and not, as is the view of — 
the writer, a symbolic instrument without which it is impossible for 
the psychic functions to be adequately performed. 


| 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM 347 


Mills and Weisenburg have published a paper in which, after col- 
lating the clinical evidence in favour of the localisation of the higher 
psychic functions in the prefrontal lobe, they describe an interesting 
case of left prefrontal tumour. The patient, a physician, was affected 
with regard to his higher psychic functions, his judgment and powers 
of comparison, his grasp of work, his disposition, &c. 

Shepherd Ivory Franz has published a series of observations on 
the effect of experimental lesions of the frontal lobes in monkeys and 


cats. He finds that when these lobes are destroyed recently formed 


habits are lost ; and he indicates his reasons for concluding that the 
results are not due to shock. Unilateral lesions. are followed merely 
by a slowing of motor impulses. He concludes that the frontal lobes 


appear to be concerned in the performance of normal and daily associa- 


tional processes, and that by means of them we are able to learn and 
to form habits. R 

Mills and Weisenburg have recently produced clinical and patho- 
logical evidence indicating that the cortical areas for the representa- 
tion of movements, of sensibility, and of stereognosis are distinct from 
one another and are each divided into several sub-areas. 

Oskar Vogt has published an interesting article on the functional 
significance of the pre- and post-central gyri. He confirms from the 
experimental aspect the researches of Sherrington and Griinbaum with 
regard to the pre-Rolandic position of the excitable motor areas. He 
shows that the anterior limits of the excitable limb areas correspond 
accurately to those of Brodmann’s pre-central type (No. 4); and that 
the centres for movements of the head and eyes and for mastication 
correspond with the still more anterior histologically differentiated 
area of Brodmann (No. 6). In certain of the lower apes he has found 
that the excitable region does not extend as far back as the fissure 
of Rolando, and that the motor type of cortex in these has the same 
posterior limits. Further, by a study of the ‘effects of destructive 
lesions in eleven lower apes, he concludes that palsy without ataxy 


- follows destruction of the pre-central gyrus, and that considerable 


ataxy but no palsy results from destruction of the post-central 


Eugen Wehrli, in a paper dealing with lesions of the occipital 
region, denies that these afford proof of the exact cortical localisation 
of the visual area; and he is not disposed to regard local differences 
of histological structure as evidence in favour of the localisation of’ 
the visual area in a special region of the cortex. This paper well 
illustrates the critical attitude still held by many writers with regard 
to the recent researches on cortical localisation, and indicates. that 


348 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION AND 


the most elaborately detailed investigations, even when repeatedly 
confirmed, may still fail to gain general acceptance. 

The last investigation which will be referred to is not of direct 
physiological interest, but is nevertheless worthy of mention owing 
to its bearing on the subject of heredity. J. P. Karplus has recently 
published a work which deals with the fissuration of the human cere- 
brum from the aspect of family likeness. He has examined in a series 
of cases the cerebra of two or more members of a family, and also the 
brains of several members of families in the ape, dog, rat, and goat. 

He draws interesting conclusions with regard to the family likeness 
of the pattern of the fissures. He further finds that, whilst in the 
monkey the two hemispheres resemble one another, in man the original 
bilateral symmetry of fissuration is lost, and the sulci of the individual 
hemispheres possess a family permanence. 

Whilst it is true that the convolutional pattern of the brain does- 
not run hand in hand with, and in fact bears little relationship to, 
the various histologically differentiated areas into which the cerebral 
cortex has been divided, the truth that the former is susceptible to 
the impress of heredity is at least suggestive with regard to the possi- 
bility of this in the case of the latter. 


BIBLIOGRAPHY 


L. Bianchi, On the Teaching of Flechsig with regard to the Perceptive 
and the Associative Zones. Internat. Med. Congress of Madrid, 1903. Text- 
book of Psychiatry, translated by J. H. Macdonald, 1906. 

J. S. Bolton, The exact Histological Localisation of the Visual Area of 
the Human Cerebral Cortex. Phil. Trans., vol. 193, 1900. The Histological 
Basis of Amentia and Dementia. Archives of Neurol., vol. ii., 1903. The 
Functions of the Frontal Lobes. Brain, 1903. Amentia and Dementia: a 
Clinico-Pathological Study. Journ. Ment. Sci., 1905-8, chiefly April 1905, 
April 1906, and April and July 1908. 

K. Brodmann, Beitrage zur histologischen Lokalisation der Grosshirnrinde, 
‘Journ, fiir Psychol. und Neurol. Mitteilung I. Die Regio Rolandica. Bd. 2, 
1902-3. Mitteilung II. Der Calcarinatypus. Bd. 2, 1903. Mitteilung IIT. 
Die Rindenfelder der niedéren Affen. Bd. 4, 1905. Mitteilung IV. Der 
Riesenpyramidentypus und sein Verhalten zu den Furchen bei den Karni- 
voren. Bd. 4, 1905. Mitteilung V. Uber den allgemeinen Bauplan des 
Cortex pallii bei den Mammaliern und zwei homologe Rindenfelder im 
besonderen. Zugleich ein Beitrag zur Furchenlehre. Bd. 6, 1906. Mit- 
teilung VI. Die Cortex gliederung des Menschen. Bd. 10, 1907, 

A. W. Campbell, Histological Studies on the Localisation of Cerebral 
Function. Cambridge, 1905. 


ON THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBRUM 349 


J. Dé&erine, Liaphasie sensorielle. Sa localisation et sa physiologie patho- 
logique. La Presse médicale, 11 Juillet 1906. L’aphasie motrice. Sa 
localisation et sa physiologie pathologique. La Presse médicale, 18 Juillet 
1906. 

L. Edinger und A. Wallenberg, Bericht ueber die Leistungen auf dem 
gebiete der anatomie des centralnervensystems. Dritter Bericht, 1905 und 
1906. 

S. I. Franz, On the Functions of .the Cerebrum. The Frontal Lobes. 
Arch. of Psychology, March 1907. 

W. Harris, Binocular and Stereoscopic Vision in Man and other Verte- 
brates with regard to Decussation of Optic Nerves, Ocular Movements and 
Pupil Light Reflex. Brain, cv., 1904. 

Hermanides und Képpen, Uber die Furchen und iiber den Bau der Gross- 
hirnrinde bei den Lissencephalen, insbesondere iiber die Lokalisation des 
motorischen Zentrums und der Sehregion. Arch. f. Psychiatr., 37, 1903. 

Gordon Holmes, A Note on the Condition of the Post-Central Cortex in 
Tabes Dorsalis. Rev. of Neurol. and Psychiat., vol. vi., No. 1, 1908. 

Ernst Jendrassik, The Mechanism and Localisation of the Psychical 
Processes. Neurol. Centralb., xxvi., 1907, pp. 194 and 254, 

J.P. Karplus, Zur Kenntnis der variabilitat und vererbung am zentral- 
nervensystem des menschen und einiger siugetiere. Wien, 1907. 

W. Kolmer, Beitrag zur Kenntniss der “ motorischen” Hirnrindregion, 
Arch. f. mikr. Anat. u. Entwgesch., 57, 1901. 

Koppen und Lowenstein, Studien iiber den Zellenbau der Grosshirnrinde bei 
den Ungulaten und Karnivoren und iiber die Bedeutung einiger Furchen. 
Monatsch. f. Psychiat. u. Neurolog., 18, 1905. 

A, Lemaitre, Trois cas de dissociation mentale. Arch, de Psychol., 1907, 
p- 252. 

Bevan Lewis and Henry Clarke, The Cortical Localisation of the Motor 
Area of the Brain. Proc. Roy. Soc., No. 185, 1878. 

Pierre Marie, Revision de la question de l’aphasie. La Semaine médicale, 
1906, 23 Mai, 17 Octobre, and 28 Novembre. 

Millis and Weisenburg, Localisation of the Higher Psychic Functions. 
Journ, Am, Med. Assoc., Feb. 1906. The Subdivision of the Representation 
of Cutaneous and Muscular Sensibility and of Stereognosis in the Cerebral 
Cortex. Journ. of Nerv. and Ment. Dis., Oct. 1906, p. 617. 

F. D. Mitchell, Mathematical Prodigies. Am. Journ. of Psychol., Jan. 
1907. 

Von Monakow, Aphasia and Diaschisis. Neurol, Centralb., Noy. 16, 1906, 

1026. . 
ha F. W. Mott, The Progressive Evolution of the Structure and Functions 

of the Visual Cortex in Mammalia. (Bowman Lecture, 1904.) Arch. of 
Neurol., vol. iii., 1907. 

Mott and Halliburton, Localisation of Function in the Lemur’s Brain. 
Proc. Roy. Soc., 1907, B. vol. 80, p. 136. 

Mott and Kelley, Complete Survey of the Cell Lamination of the Cerebral 
Cortex of the Lemur. Proc. Roy. Soc., 1908, B. vol, 80, p. 488. 

F. Moutier, L’aphasie de Broca, Paris, 1908. 


~~ 


350 RESEARCHES ON CORTICAL LOCALISATION 


Morton Prince, The Dissociation of a Personality : a Biographical Study 
in Abnormal Psychology. New York, London, and Bombay, 1906. 

Eleanor H. Rowland, The Psychological Experiences connected with the 
different Parts of Speech. The Psychological Seige Monograph Supple- 
ment, January 1907. 

Elliott Smith, The Morphology of the Occipital Region of the Cerebral 
Hemispheres in Man and the Apes. Anat. Anz., 24,1904. Studies in the 
Morphology of the Human Brain, with Special Reference to the Egyptians, 
No. 1, The Occipital Region. Records of the Egyptian Gov. School of Med, 
2, 1904. A New Topographical Survey of the Human Cerebral Cortex, 
Journ. of Anat. and Phys., 41, 1907. 

John Turner, A Study of the Minute Structure of the Olfactory Lobe and 
Cornu Ammonis. Brain, 1906, p. 57. The Structure of Grey Matter. Brain, 
1907, p. 426. 

0. Vogt, Uber strukturelle Hirncentra, mit besonderer Beriicksichtigung 
der strukturellen Felder des Cortex pallii. Verhandl. der anatom. Gesellsch., 
1906, p. 74. Der Wert der myelogenetischen Felder der Grosshirnrinde. 
Anat. Anz., 29, 1906. 

G, A. Watson, The Mammalian Cerebral Cortex, with Special Reference 
to its Comparative Histology. I. Order Insectivora. Arch. of Neurology, 
vol, iii., 1907. « 

E. Wehrli, Ueber die anatomisch-histologische Grundlage der sog. 
Rindenblindheit und ueber die Lokalisation der corticalen Seesphere der 
Macula lutea und die Projection der Retina auf die Rinde des Occipital- 
Jappens. V. Graefe’s Archiv., lxii., 2, 1906. 

Albert Wilson, Education, Personality, and Crime. London, 1908. 


~~ ——__ 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 
By M. GREENWOOD, Juwr. 


PART I.—VISUAL ADAPTATION 


Ir one asked a number of students, taken at random, which de- 
partment of physiology seemed to them the least interesting, I 
am confident that a majority would award the palm of dulness 
to that subdivision which treats of the special sense mechanisms. 

It is, indeed, not difficult to understand such a result. Our 
knowledge of special sense physiology is at once very complete and 
very incomplete ; observations and experiments have accumulated 
to an enormous extent, yet we are far from the synthesis of these 
results which makes the inter-relationship of the various parts 
clear and enables the intelligent reader to perceive that the sub- 
ject is an harmonious system, not a mere collection of disconnected 
fragments. 

I think, therefore, the reader will more easily appreciate the 
scope and method of this branch of science if I choose two 
problems only, and examine them in detail, not so much because 
they are of practical importance, but as illustrations of the lines — 
upon which modern thought and experimental work seem to 
advance. : 

In the present section, I shall sketch the course of recent in- 


_ quiries into the phenomena of adaptation of the eye to various 


intensities of light, a subject which illustrates the practical methods 
of work ; in the later section I shall attempt to set out the principles 
of two modern theories of colour vision, and to show how far they 
may be regarded as really new contributions to science, and to 
what extent they are products of thought handed down to us from 
remote ages. Here, as in so much of modern science, we shall 
find that old and new are inextricably interwoven, that much of 
what is taken to be modern is so only in a geological sense. 

It has long been known that the nature of the response by the 

86 


Bs 
352 _ STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


eye to a stimulus largely depends upon whether, before the ex- 
periment, the subjects have rested in a dark room or been exposed 
to light—that is to say, whether there be a condition of light or 
dark adaptation. 

Many years ago, Aubert noticed that the response of the eye 
to feeble stimulation was increased by resting in the dark, and also 
laid down the rule that the threshold value of a stimulus in such 
cases varied inversely as the area of surface stimulated. This 
general statement received ample confirmation ; Charpentier (1), 
for instance, found that the central part of the ‘“‘ dark”? retina, 
although exhibiting an increased response as compared with the 
same region in the “light” eye, was far less sensitive than the 
periphery. This increased responsiveness was very marked in 
respect of the short waved regions of the spectrum, indeed it . 
is denied by many good observers that adaptation affects the 
response to red light at all (Parinaud and v. Kries), while nobody 
has demonstrated a large increase. | 

Of the many who have studied the changes of responsiveness 
in adaptation, all agree that they are far more prominent in the 
peripheral than in the central region, but much dispute has arisen = 
as to whether any part is absolutely unaffected. On the whole, : 
it may be said that no satisfactory proof is forthcoming that 7 
such a part exists. Experimentally, the problem is beset with ' 
difficulties, for it is hard to be certain of the exact limits of a very 
small stimulated field, and it is possible that the time required for 
adaptation is not the same in the fovea and the peripheral regions, 
being probably longer in the former (Tschermak °).. 

These changes in responsiveness immediately lead us to the 
consideration of Purkinje’s “ phenomenon,” an effect which may 
be described in the following terms. If one examines an ordinary 
spectrum, the brightest part of it seems to occupy the neighbour- 
hood of the yellow or orange-yellow ; if now its physical intensity 
be diminished—for instance, by moving the source of light farther 
away from the prism or grating—the maximum of brightness is. 
shifted towards the violet end. With the feeblest illumination 
which enables us to detect the spectral colours at all, the brightest 
part is at the junction of the green and blue. 

Hering (*) by means of a simple experiment has made it seem — 


1 For brevity, I shall speak of an eye which has been rested in darkness as a 
‘dark ” and one previously exposed to light as a ‘‘ light” eye. 


—— —— 


—— 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY = 353 


probable that these changes are due to adaptation. Two rooms 
which could be independently darkened are separated by a light- 
proof partition in which two slits are cut and covered with pig- 
mented glass. The amount of light transmitted by these slits 
can be varied independently with the aid of reflectors. So long as 
the room occupied by the observer is kept at a constant illumina- 
tion, diminishing the physical intensity of the light traversing the 
two slits, which are covered with blue and red glass respectively, 
does not change their relative intensities. If, however, the ob- 
servation room is darkened, the blue slit immediately appears 
brighter than the red one, even while the physical intensity of light 


passing through them is unaltered. This effect is enhanced by a 


prolonged stay in darkness, and is more noticeable in indirect 
(peripheral) than direct (central or foveal) vision. Burch, in a 
series of experiments which I shall discuss later, has obtained 
similar results, so that we may conclude that Purkinje’s “ pheno- 
menon” depends not on physical intensity but visual adaptation, 
and is a particular case of the general change already mentioned. 
These facts naturally incline us to examine more closely the 
differences between peripheral and central vision, and we shall 


’ find that all work tends to show that the former is more susceptible 


to adaptive changes and characterised by a heightened responsive- 
ness to feeble stimuli. 

Before going further, however, we must ask ourselves what: is 
meant by saying that different colours are equally or unequally 
bright. As a matter of fact, this question is more easily asked 
than answered. -A similar difficulty is experienced in attempting 
to ascertain what one means by saying that two notes of different 
pitch are unequally loud. If we attempt to define our meaning in 
terms of the physical properties of exciting stimuli, we become 


confused, and perhaps the only valid excuse for employing the 


expression is an empirical one. Ask a dozen normal persons to 
look at a spectrum in daylight, and they will agree that the yellow 
is the brightest part of it, meaning, I suppose, that the region, in 
question produces, somehow, a predominant effect in consciousness. 
This uniformity of results is a justification, perhaps the only justi- 
fication, of the method, and allows us to compare different colours 
with respect to brightness. 

Bearing in mind the empirical and sensational nature of the 
investigation, we can attain comparative results in numerous ways. 

; Z 


354 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY — | 


One of the best is the “ flicker” method which has been carefully 
studied in England by Haycraft (*) and Rivers (°). If a series of 
sectors are whirled round on a colour mixer, the rapidity necessary 
to produce a fused sensation depends upon the brightness of the 
sectors; hence with different sets of sectors equal in size, the 
velocity of rotation which just extinguishes the sensation of flicker 
will afford us a measure of brightness, this varying, within certain 
limits, inversely as the rapidity of rotation. Many other plans 
can be, and have been, adopted, some of which will be mentioned 
later. The above should give the reader some idea as to how 
researches can be arranged. 

Hering, in 1890, noted the relative darkening of red as one 
passes from central to peripheral vision ; he compared a pure red, 
a spectral mixture of red and blue green (656 wu+470 uu) and 
daylight. The converse was found to hold for spectral green 
(505 wu), but his results were not quite pure, since momentary 
dark adaptation occurred during the experiments (*). ‘Tschermak (°), 
who studied the whole question systematically in the “light ” eye, 
found with indirect vision a relative diminution in brightness for 
light of wave length between 693 and 525 wu, no change from 
525 to 516 wu, an increase from 516 to 466 uu. Similar changes 
were observable in “dark” eyes. 

Another line of research was to start with a large field of colour- 
less light, produced by mixing together complementaries, and then 
to diminish its size. It has been found that a colourless mixture 
of spectral red and bluish-green becomes, with such areal diminu- 
tion, redder and darker; if the change of size be an increase, it 
becomes greener and brighter. In fact, both for “light” and 
“dark” eyes, colourless matches valid for the periphery do not 
hold for the centrum and vice versa. Apart from adaptation, it 
_is interesting to see whether merely changing the physical in- 
tensities of the two mixtures renders the match invalid. It is 
agreed that mere intensity variation does not affect central matches, 
but there is much dispute as to the periphery. V. Kries and his 
fellow-workers have \brought forward experimental evidence on 
the affirmative side,\but difference of opinion exists. Thus, 
v. Kries (7) found the equation R+G=/Y, no longer valid on 
diminishing the intensities of both fields, the binary colour growing 
paler and brighter. We\ must remember that it is extremely 
difficult to avoid changes in the state of adaptation, and as these 

\ 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 355 


are especially important at the periphery, it is probable that 
some of the results have been vitiated by this circumstance 
(Tschermak ?). 

- In connection with the supposed relation between physical 
intensity and apparent brightness, an interesting set of observa- 
tions may be noted regarding what is called the “achromatic 
threshold of coloured light.” In studying Purkinje’s “ phenomenon” 
we found that if the intensity of a spectrum were steadily diminished, 
a point was reached at which the brightest region appeared to have 
been moved towards the violet. What, it may be asked, happens 
if the illumination be still further diminished ? Under normal 
circumstances we soon reach a point at which the whole spectrum 
appears colourless, differing, however, in brightness in the various 
regions. Reduction beyond this yields an intensity which is 
associated with no sensation at all. Hence it seemed necessary 
to distinguish between the absolute liminal intensity of a spectrum,’ 
that is, the least intensity corresponding to a colourless sensation, 
and the “ specific ” threshold or liminal value for which the spectral 
zones could be seen to differ in hue. 

A good many workers, including Purkinje, Helmholtz, Aubert, 
Landolt, &c., observed this phenomenon, and the actual absolute 
threshold values for various spectral lights and different parts of 
the retina have been studied. Charpentier (') found the “ photo- 
chromatic interval” (i.e. the difference between specific and abso- 
lute threshold intensities) least for red and greatest for blue light, 
and that the absolute thresholds were diminished by dark adapta- 
tion, while the specific liminal values were not affected. It also 
appeared that these results were more noticeable with peripheral 
than central vision. 

It was soon noticed, however, that the phenomena were in 
_ reality complex. Some workers (e.g. Parinaud and v. Kries) 
never obtained an achromatic value for red, and doubts arose as 
to the existence of such intervals at all. Finally, on comparing 
the brightness of central and peripheral lights, rendered colourless 
by a reduction of physical intensity, unexpected and puzzling 
results were obtained. _ 

Stegmann (°) performed the following experiments. Matches 
were made between a series of lights from 640 to 480 uu, and a 


1 The liminal value or threshold value of a stimulus is that intensity which 
corresponds to a “‘ just-noticeable ” sensation. , 


. 
356 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY " 


screen of blue-green or orange paper, illuminated by lamp light 
passing through an absorbing medium, the fixed illuminant being 
so chosen that no colour was discernible. The tested lights were 
arranged in the first series of experiments at an eccentricity of 
twenty degrees; in the second, at one of four degrees. In the 
first set of observations, Stegmann found that, after five to fifteen 
minutes’ dark adaptation, a diminution of 21 to 45 per cent. in 
orange intensity, and an increase of 23 to 50 per cent. in blue-green 
intensity were necessary in order to match the fixed standard ; 
that is to say, below the chromatic threshold, the peripheral 
responsiveness was increased with respect to the long waved light 
and diminished with regard to the short-waved light, 7.e. a change 
the exact converse of that associated with the ordinary Purkinje 
effect. V. Kries concluded that these results depended on the. 
feeble intensity of the stimulus employed, and that with light 
stronger but still below the chromatic threshold, the results agreed 
with the Purkinje effect, the discrepancy being therefore a conse- 
quence of physical intensity; this view has not, however, been 
generally accepted. Recently, G. J. Burch has published the 
results of some interesting experiments on achromatic thresholds (°). 
In the first place, Burch performed some qualitative experiments. 
A Bunsen burner was completely covered by a metal chimney so 
as to prevent any escape of light while not interfering with ventila- 
tion. By bringing the flame into contact with the metal chimney 
the latter could-be heated gradually to a point at which it became 
luminous. If the experiment were performed in a room with 
windows covered by ordinary blinds—that is to say, in a room 
from which light had not been absolutely’ excluded—the first 
appearance of light was a pearl-grey tint—the achromatic 
threshold—as had been previously stated. But when the room 
‘was changed to one without windows and absolutely dark, 
the first appearance of luminosity was not grey but dull red. 
On repeating this experiment, after spending a few minutes 
in a lighted room, the former grey appearance was once more 
obtained. 

Burch then made exact. measurements, using spectral red 
and violet, a Nicol prism and paper reflector (Fig. 1). He 
found that after a prolonged rest in total darkness, a period 
of two hours being necessary, no achromatic threshold existed 
for either light. 


Adaptation Values (Burch). 
A. During the period of increasing adaptation. 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 357 


I i I of 
or ee Minimum Visible | Minimum Visible | Ratio, V : R. 
Red, Blue- Violet. 
9 16°63 254°76 15°32 
21 11-14 5761 5:17 
60 9-4 5:39 2:24 


B. After spending two hours in the dark room. 


EEE ——— 
- 


__ Intensi 
—_—* Sis “Mintnum Visible | Mintmum Visible Ratio, V : R. 
(Mins. Red. Blue-Violet. 
120, 10 1-0 10 
122 | 1-20 1°63 1-34 
125 2:69 514 191 
127 5-04 12:44 2°47 
130 50-02 225°72 4-51 
C 
vy 
D 
B / is 
7 


le 


relative in 


A Raped pre) 


358 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


Burch, therefore, while confirming the statement that adapta- 
tion increases the stimulus value of blue-violet relatively to that | 
of red, inferred that the achromatic threshold had no existence, 
and that the positive results obtained by other workers were due 
to a compounding of the immediate stimulus with the effects of 
previous exposure to light—z.e. after-images. By careful observa- 
tions in a dark room he ascertained that diffused luminous effects— 
his “dazzle tints ”’—persist for as long as two and a half hours 
after entering the dark room, and that when these had disappeared 
no achromatic interval existed for his own eye. 

This evidence is sufficiently strong to render the existence of 
an absolute as distinct from a specific threshold for coloured light 
exceedingly doubtful in the case of foveal vision. On the other 
hand, it is not clear from Burch’s paper that special attention was, 
or could be, paid to the behaviour of the periphery ; so that we are 
not entitled to conclude from it that an achromatic interval does 
not exist when the peripheral retina is stimulated; indeed the 
careful researches of Tschermak and others, who worked with pro- 
longed dark adaptation (7), are opposed to such a belief. 

Burch’s results merely, I think, emphasise the view I am en- 
deavouring to develop that central and peripheral vision differ in q 
kind and degree. His work does not appear to be fundamentally | 
new, except in experimental technique, for Parinaud in 1898 | 
remarked in his most suggestive and valuable book: “At the 
fovea, on the contrary, a simple light of sufficient purity is per- . 
ceived primarily as a colour, whatever be the intensity of the light, | 
or whether the retina be or be not adapted ” (7°, p. 51). 

Without therefore going the length of asserting that an 
achromatic interval depends in some vague way on previous stimu- 
‘lation, we must be very cautious in interpreting results, such as 
Stegmann’s, obtained by this method. 

So far, then, direct experiment seems to have established the 
following points :— 

(1) The peripheral regions of the retina are relatively more 
sensitive than the fovea to light of moderate or short wave length. 

(2) Adaptation to darkness is characterised by an increase in 
responsiveness to short waved light, and this change is predomi- 
nantly, if not entirely, extra-foveal. 

The following tables illustrate these statements. The intensity 
values are arbitrary, the measurement of eccentricity 3 is in terms 
of the angle subtended at the nodal point. 


———— i — lS a 
‘ . 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 359 
A. Relative Stimulus Values of different Spectral Regions (Parinaud‘), 


Fraunhofer Lines. | Adapted Retina. | Unadapted Retina. | 
B | 
c = 
: y oa 
F 1 sho | 
i ue i | 


B. Increased Responsiveness of Peripheral Retina (Dark-adapted). 
Stimulus: A bluish-white object, 35 degrees in diameter (v. Kries," p. 171).' 


; 

: : Responsiveness Temporal Nasal 

CKrbitrary ap veto in Eooentricity in sal rh 

| 10 1-07 0°85 192 | 
1-78 1-22 1:06 2-28 
712 1:70 1°38 3:08 

| 16-02 2°3 1:92 4°22 

| 28°48 30 2-58 5°58 
44-50 3°75 3°33 | 7:08 
64-08 4-04 404 | 808 


1 Distances from the fovea centralis of any point on the retina can be con- 
veniently measured in terms of the angle subtended at the nodal point by a segment 
of the retina cut by a plane passing through the fovea, nodal point, and position of 


Cc 


Fie. 2. 


the object and bounded by a straight line through the nodal point from the object 
intersecting the retinal segment and by the principal axis. Z.g. if A be an object, 
istorlrra ran and CC a section of the retina, the position (eccentricity) of the 


Ra eto br to cage « 


360 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


We must next consider certain facts which seem to bear in- 
directly upon the problem under examination; of these the 
most important are connected with the condition of total colour- 
blindness. 

Total colour-blindness is almost always a congenital defect, 
and is characterised, apparently, by a complete absence of colour 
perception—in the ordinary sense. A person in this state may 
see a spectrum merely as a grey strip unequally bright in the 
different regions which seem to us of various colours. To describe 
the sensations of a second human being is, of course, an impossi- 
bility ; but perhaps we may say, for the sake of comparison and 
without pretending to any real precision, that a totally colour- 
blind man receives from a coloured print impressions similar to 
those excited in ourselves by an uncoloured one. The following | 
summary of observations by Hering (!”) will give the reader a 
tolerably clear impression of the facts. 

The subject was a man of twenty years, whose colour vision 
had always been abnormal. He said that he could read without 
difficulty, provided the light were not too intense, but that his 1 
eyes were readily fatigued by bright illumination. In twilight his 
vision was especially good, particularly if the light were very 
feeble. On examination, the following results were obtained. No 
objective changes were detected with the ophthalmoscope, nor was . 
any part of the retina insensitive; there was no scotoma. His 
power of distinguishing two spots unequally bright—physically— 
was much below that of a normal person, in bright light ; in a dark 
room, it was much superior. Working with a spectrum, it was | 
found that the area of red which produced any sensation was | 
much diminished ; there was shortening of the red end, and those 
parts which were effective seemed less bright than to a normal 
eye. The violet end, on the other hand, was not shortened, and it 
seemed relatively brighter than to the normal “light” eye, while 
the region of maximum brightness was in the neighbourhood 
of the Fraunhofer E and C lines. Brightness matches between 
coloured sectors and mixtures of black and white gave the results 
indicated in the table, which contains comparative values for 
the normal “light” eye. It is to be noted that the colour- 
blind’s matches were valid, 7.e. good matches, for a normal 
' “dark” eye. 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 361 


Hering’s Case of Total Colour-blindness. 


et ee ae qe Valency of a 
Coloured Circle. — Valency.1 the eloured Olrole tr 
White. | Black. | | a Normal “ Light” Eye. 
Degrees. | Degrees. | Degrees. Degrees. 
Bluish red . ‘ 130 | 3470 | 188 | 40 
Yellowish red. 55 . 3545 11-4 46 
Orange . «| 870 | 3230 | 43-4 159 
Yellow , . | 1366 | 223-5 | 1402 - 283 
Arsenic green . | 2280 1820 | 230°5 205 
Green . : .| 1520 | 2080 «1555 137 
Greenish blue .| 1095 | 2505 = 113-7 89 
Ultramarine blue 88-3 2717 | 928 | 34 
Violet . ‘ ‘ 475 | 3125 52:7 32 


Tests were also carried out by means of an ingenious polari- 
scopic method. At one end of a horizontal tube, blackened inside, 
a cork plate was fixed; the plate was perforated in the middle 
and a doubly refracting prism inserted. The other end of the 
tube was closed by a lid in which two equal and symmetrically 
placed semicircular openings were made. With this contrivance 
the ordinary and extraordinary images obtained by polarisation 
appeared to form a series of spherical surfaces when the tube was 
directed to a source of light—e.g. a piece of baryta paper stretched 
over a glass plate. Between the eye and the prism a small tele- 
scope and a Nicol prism were introduced, together with a graduated 
arc. The diaphragm of the telescope removed the lateral images, 
leaving only two magnified white circles, the halves of which could 
have their brightness altered in opposite directions by rotating 
the Nicol. In front of one opening in the tube a coloured glass 
- was placed, and the Nicol so arranged that for the normal “ dark ” 
or for the totally colour-blind eye, both halves appeared equally 
bright. - The next table gives the readings for two observers. Some 
of the variations may be explained by the fact that the normal- 
sighted person was unpractised in this sort of work. 

1 For methods of measuring white ‘‘ valency ” consult Hering (1*), p. 567, &c. 
For the present purpose the figures in the third column of the table may be regarded 


as a recalculation of the amounts of white in the sectors which match the coloured 
circles, so as to admit of comparison with the figures of the fourth column. 


1} 


. 
362 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


Precisely similar results were obtained in matching spectral 
colours. : 


Polariscopic Matches (Hering). 
Yellow Glass. Blue Glass. 


Vo | 
“Total Colour-bling. | Normal aoe Total Colour-blina, | Normal ree 


Degrees of Rotation. | Degrees of Rotation. || Degrees of Rotation. | Degrees of Rotation. 
22:3 22°9 18:2 18°35 


22°6 22°6 18-0 17°95 
22°3 23-0 18-0 18°15 
219 22°5 18°1 18°4 
22°3 22°7 17°8 18-9 
21:8 23°1 

22°1 21°8 


‘he analogy between normal vision under conditions of dark 
adaptation and the vision of the totally colour-blind will also be 
apparent from the next two tables. The first gives the intensity 
values of the different parts of the spectrum for a normal “ dark ” 
eye, as determined by Schaternikoff (1%), and the second Abney’s 
observations (14) on two other cases of total colour-blindness. The 
units of intensity in the two cases are not comparable, but it 
will be observed that the two maxima occur in the same 
part of the spectrum. 


“ Twilight” Values of a Spectrum (Schaternikof.). 


Wave Length. Intensity Value. | Wave Length. Intensity Value. 
In millionths of a In millionths of a 
mm. mm. 
670°8 18:0 529°3 2736-0 
651°8 36°5 §22°3 2532°3 
634-3 83:3 515°4 2219°3 
6181 216°9 508-7 19440 
603°1 423°2 502°2 1475°8 
589°3 881-7 490°0 10160 
577-1 14249 478°6 633-0 
566°4 2110°7 468:0 3645 
556°0 2609°7 458°7 208°8 
546-0 2899-0 451 1112 
537:2 3000'0 443°9 69°6 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 363 


Luminosity Values of two Cases of Total Colour-blindness (Abney). 
(No. 40 in Abney’s scale is close to the E line.) 


Scale of Spectrum. K. ba eoaearae P.'s Luminosity Value. 
56 25 
54 9-0 
52 16:0 70 
50 275 19-0 
48 42°5 39:0 
46 61-0 65:0 
44 82°5 85:0 
42 96°0 98:0 
40 100°0 99-0 
38 95°5 91:5 
36 87°5 90°0 
34 750 80:0 
32 615 65:0 
30 43-0 50°0 
28 37:0 36:0 
26 30-0 26°5 
24 24°0 19°5 
22 18°5 14:0 
20 14°5 10-0 
18 115 ee 
16 9-0 55 
14 70 
12 50 . | 
| | 


As we have already seen that direct evidence points to dark 
adaptation being chiefly an affair of the extra-foveal part of the © 
retina, it is reasonable to suppose that, in the totally colour-blind, 
the fovea centralis is relatively insensitive. It has indeed been 
asserted that a central scotoma, or totally insensitive area, is 
found in these cases, and much dispute has arisen on this point. 
Summarising the results of examination, we find that, out of 
eighteen cases investigated (Grunert '), in seven an absolute or 
relative central or para-central scotoma was definitely made out ; 
in the other eleven this was absent. The importance of a central 
scotoma has been greatly over-estimated for theoretical reasons, 
as we shall see later. In most cases, very imperfect fixation or 
actual nystagmus was observed. On reviewing the facts, it will, 
I think, be admitted that the agreement in type between the vision 
of the totally colour-blind and that of the normal “dark” eye 


364 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


is exceedingly close. The importance of these facts in forming 
conclusions as to the mechanism of visual processes will be pointed 
out when we have considered a few more experimental observa- 
tions regarding central and peripheral vision in the two phases of 
adaptation... 

A set of experiments, apparently of little practical import- 
ance, has proved very interesting with respect to the phenomena 
of adaptation. These deal with the effects which follow the 
application of luminous stimuli for very short intervals of 
time. 

Such experiments can be carried out in at least two ways. 
By a contrivance, similar to a photographic camera shutter, the eye 
can be stimulated for a very short time, or, the gaze being fixed, a 
source of light may be moved across the field of vision. For the - 
latter experiment, we may employ a rotating mirror and a pro- 
jection lantern, or a disc with a slit in it may be rotated in front of 
a source of light. If the length of the object be J and v be the 
velocity of movement, then J/v measures the time during which 
each retinal area is exposed to the action of the stimulus, and by 
a suitable choice of object and velocity this time becomes as short 
as we please. 

In principle the methods are alike, but the second is easier in 
practice, and the results obtained of special interest. 

The earliest observer seems to have been D’Arcy (}), who, in 
1765, measured the duration of the response produced by a glow- 
ing coal which was attached to the circumference of a wheel; the 
wheel was rotated faster and faster until a complete circle of light 
was seen. If a bright object be rotated against a dark back- 
ground, in the manner described, the whole sensory effect is some- 
what complicated, comprising under favourable circumstances the 
following six phases : — 

(1) A primary image; the immediate consequence of the 
stimulus, its first and strongest effect. As compared with the 
image due to a stationary illuminant it is more or less elongated 
into a streak of light. 

(2) Immediately following upon the primary image is a short 
dark streak. 

(3) After the dark streak we obtain a second phase of illumina- 
tion which, if the stimulus be coloured, appears complementarily 
tinged (this is often called, after its discoverer, the Purkinje after- 

; at. 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 365 


image). This phase, which is the most striking part of the whole 
experiment, produces the effect of a second bright object coming 
behind the first, so that its appearance was termed “ recurrent 
vision ” (Young, Davis), the “ ghost ” (Bidwell), or the “ satellite ” 
(Hamaker). 

(4) The end of the satellite is not sharply defined and is followed 
by another interval of darkness. 

(5) After this, the field once more brightens, and a faint bright 
or homoiochromatic phase is obtained (of the same colour as the 
original stimulus). If this part of the phenomenon be well de- 
veloped, it is found that with a velocity such that the satellite is 
distinct a bright haze fills the whole field, hence more than one 
- rotation should not be made. 

(6) Sixthly, and lastly, another dark interval occurs. 

It will be plain, even from this enumeration, that the phenomena 
under consideration are by no means simple; hence nobody will 
be surprised to learn that divergences, even flat contradictions, 
exist in the literature of the subject not merely as to the inter- 
pretation of these results, but even as to their bona-fide existence. 
I shall follow, for the most part, the lucid statement of the case 
which we owe to Professor J. von Kries(!"), indicating why I 
believe his results to be reliable. 

With respect to the experiment as a whole, we have three 
phases of illumination—the primary image, the secondary or 
satellite image, and the tertiary. In apparent brightness, these are 
arranged in the order of their appearance. With low illuminations, 
primary and secondary images alone are visible; with still lower 
intensities, the secondary also disappears. Further, the lengths 
of the images can be made to vary, the dark intervals being lost. 
Fixing our attention for a moment on the primary, we may note 
the following points. Very frequently it exhibits a striped appear- 
ance, similar to the well-known Charpentier “‘ bands” (1) seen on 
slow rotation of a white field. containing a black sector. Apart 
from this, it seems to the “light” eye uniformly bright.~ As, 
however, dark adaptation proceeds, we find that the primary 
image not only increases in extension and brightness, but with 
chromatic stimuli ceases to be uniform. Thus, with blue light, 
only the anterior border is deep blue, being followed by a white 
stripe. Macdougall (?*) finds this latter to commence at a distance 
corresponding, in his experiments, to a time interval of y's second. 


366 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


With other colours, except red, the same result is obtained, but. 
less distinctly. 
Although anticipating our theoretical summary, I must point out 
how strongly this suggests the activity of two distinct mechanisms 
with different latent periods. An analogous, but not identical, 
result is the old illusion of the “ fluttering hearts.” Small heart- 
shaped scraps of blue paper are pasted upon a red surface and the 
whole examined in dim light. On moving the sheet backwards 
and forwards, the blue scraps appear to lag behind the red back- 
ground. This phenomenon does not occur on stimulation of the 
fovea centralis. 
The secondary, ghost, or satellite, begins } to ¢ second after the 
commencement of the primary, and is, in general, complementary 
to it. This rule must, however, be modified in the following way. — 
If the primary be pure white, the secondary is bluish; indeed, the 
secondary is always modified in the direction of bluishness. Even 
with a feeble blue primary, the secondary may still be bluish. - It 
is only with a saturated blue primary that a secondary of a really 
complementary (yellow) hue is obtained. As regards brightness, 
the result depends on the adaptation value of the primary. Two 
lights of equal stimulus values for the “dark” eye give equally 
bright secondaries. Red, with its relatively low stimulating power 
as regards the “ dark ”’ eye, only gives a secondary when its physical 
brightness is great. 
As we should expect from its characteristics, the secondary is 
dependent on dark adaptation, increasing to a maximum of dis- 
tinctness as adaptation proceeds and then diminishing, although 
Macdougall has obtained it after prolonged dark adaptation. We 
thus see in the causative factors of the secondary image the pheno- 
mena we have already learned to associate with peripheral vision : 
(1) Relatively greater efficiency of short waves; (2) increased in- 
tensity with increasing dark adaptation. It only remains to add 
failure over the fovea centralis. “If one observes it in the form 
of an after-coming image and fixates carefully a luminous point in 
the track of ‘the object, one sees clearly that the satellite leaps 
over a small central area, while it follows without a break similar 
points of light moved over a paracentral region. Also, with 
stationary objects, momentarily illuminated, of suitable form and 
size, an analogous appearance can be demonstrated. Small objects 
which lie entirely within the foveal region do not exhibit the 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY = 367 


characteristic secondary illumination ; small lines passing through 
the fixation point exhibit a distinct interruption in the secondary 
image ” (v. Kries,'” p. 225). 
_ The great theoretical importance of this failure at the centrum 
has led to much, at times, acrimonious discussion. Hess (2°) ob- 
jected that v. Kries’ experiments were vitiated by the use of bright 
objects as fixation marks and too short intervals between successive 
stimulations. He showed that no interruption of the secondary 
image of a bright line occurred at the fovea, and obtained with 
chromatic stimuli similar results, except that for strong red light 
the secondary was not complementary but homoiochromatic. 
Hess, accordingly, was of opinion that no central difference exists 
with regard to the secondary image. 

These objections have been considered by v. Kries in an 
interesting and lucid article (*"). 

Failure of a central secondary can be demonstrated by rotating 
a screen with a slit in it before an ordinary projection lantern. 
The best results are obtained with an arrangement such that the 
object takes the form of a line 4° to 4° broad. If adaptation and 
physical brightness are so chosen that the secondary is distinctly 
separated from the primary, the latter’s failure over the fovea 
is clearly demonstrated. Experiments conducted without bright 
fixation points and with long intervals between the successive stimu- 
lations gave the same results.- V. Kries, however, found that the 
experiment failed if crossed lines were used or three small fields in 
a row. This seems to be due to psychological factors, especially 
the difficulty of concentrating the attention on any particular 
point. Thus in the line experiments of Hess, the failure to obtain 
a central interruption may be referred to an inability to fixate 
steadily any given point, so that the relations of the moving images 
are disturbed and a distinction becomes impossible. 

_ The results of v. Kries have been essentially confirmed by 
Macdougall (#*), and Hamaker (**) also failed to obtain foveal 
secondaries for blue and green stimuli. On the whole, the evidence 
in favour of v. Kries’ view is strong, and we may consider the third 
point suggesting the peripheral nature of the secondary image— 
its failure at the fovea—as established. 

Passing to the tertiary image, the following characteristics 
have been made out. The hue is best appreciated when one uses 
red light, and may, in this case, be very distinct. With increasing 


368 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY . 


dark adaptation, the tertiary gains in brightness and loses in 
chromatic value ; indeed, owing to the high adaptive powers of blue 
and green lights, when these are used at moderate intensities, the 
colouration of the tertiary image is only visible at the beginning 
of the experiment. 

There is some difference of opinion as to whether the tertiary 
can be obtained in direct vision ; since red light is the most suit- 
able stimulus for calling up this image, it is reasonable to suppose 
that it should be perceived by direct fixation. The suggestion is, 
therefore, that the customary form of the tertiary image is due to 
a chromatic element, unaffected by dark adaptation, and a bright- 
ness element which is so affected. 

We are almost in a position to attempt a sorting out of the 
various experimental facts described ; only one group remains 
for examination, namely, some researches into peripheral changes 
which accompany the retinal alterations, and are important as 
objective signs of the latter. 

Schirmer asserted that the pupil width and its reaction were 
related to the adaptive condition of the eye. With complete 
adaptation to a given grade of light, the pupil reaches after initial 
widening or narrowing a physiological mean position. Garten (?%) 
found that momentary illumination produces in the “light ” eye 
a weak sudden, in the “dark” eye a slow powerful contraction. 
Proceeding further on these lines, Sachs (?*) discovered that the 
pupillo-motor response to coloured lights followed closely their 
adaptation values. Abelsdorff (?*) confirmed these results, using 
the apparatus sketched (Fig. 3). One of the two lights serves as a 
standard ; the subject looking through the strong convex lens at the 
slit sees a bright point surrounded by a diffusion circle. If the light 
falling on the eye be changed the diffusion circle increases or 
diminishes in size. By rotation of a Nicol prism the new intensity 
can be increased or diminished and the intensity is found for which 
the diffusion circle produced by the standard light is not increased 
or diminished by changing to the tested colour. Naturally, the 
method does not yield very exact quantitative measurements ; the 
mean error is said to be about 7 per cent. The next table (see 
p- 370) gives some of Abelsdorff’s results. 

; The close agreement between the pupillo-motor values and 
those of apparent brightness justifies the method, and its importance 
lies in the fact that we can employ it in experiments on animals. 


—— a 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 369 


We have no direct means of investigating adaptive changes in any 
animals except man, but we can measure this pupillary response ; 
if we find it changing in the manner described, it is not an unfair 
inference to suppose that visual responsiveness may be similarly 
affected by adaptation. 

It has been found that intensities of red and blue which ap- 
peared equally bright to and exerted the same pupillo-motor effect 
upon a human “light” eye, did not produce identical changes 


pone 9 ae Triplex Lamp. 


Slit in Telescope. 
=~} Separating Prism. 


}- Oytinder of Blacked Paper 55 o.m.long. 


<—— Conve Lens. Focal Length 10 cm. 


) Unaccommodated Eye. 


Fie, 3.—Abelsdorff’s apparatus for studying the pupillo-motor response. 


_ in the pupils of the dove and the owl. For the former the red, 
for the latter the blue was the stronger stimulus. Indeed the 
pupillo-motor response to blue ‘in the owl’s eye was greater than 
in the case of a total-colour-blind (Abelsdorff). 

We have now completed a brief review of the experimental 
facts and are at liberty to consider their importance in our general 
conception of visual processes. 

First of all, is there any functional difference between the spot 
of distinctest vision, the fovea centralis retine, and the paracentral 
or peripheral regions of the retina ? 

= 2A 


370 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY — 
Pupillo-Motor Values. 


Comparison Lights. 
Wave Length. | Dark Adacholl 

rire 600 pn. Light Soe | ar’ nee tion. 

Mm. Mm. | Mm. 
640 6271 *3920 "2666 
620 8523 8376 | *5670 
600 9720 9822 ‘7260 
580 9536 ‘9090 | *8065 
560 8303 8739 *8865 
540 5518 6141 | 9200 
520 3333 | 2936 ‘5755 
500 1181 | ‘09141 "1612 


Brightness Values. 


(Readings obtained when the same subject adjusted the intensities of 
the lights until they seemed to be equally bright.) 


Comparison Lights. 
Wave Length. : ae ae ae: 
py. 600 pup. Light Pp i Dark Pep aa 
| Mm. Mm. Mm. 
640 | 5253 | *3518 2529 
620 | 8204 ‘7230 ‘5515 
600 | 9431 9090 8536 
580 ~ | 9431 ‘9090 8535 
560 ‘8811 ‘8613 "9540 
540 6259 6354 "9540 
520 *3700 *3189 ‘5750 
500 ‘1168 0944 i "1612 


In view of the long series of experiments bearing upon the 
Purkinje effect, the increase in brightness of the short waved 
spectral lights at the expense of the long waved vibrations, the 
reader is probably satisfied that this effect if not solely peripheral 
is at least mainly so. I do not wish to disregard those who haye 
obtained central adaptation, but even they admit the increase 
to be far less marked than the peripheral alterations; we may, 
without undue scepticism, entertain some doubts as to whether the 
positive results might not have been due to some slight eccentricity 
of fixation. However that may be, we are beyond question 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 371 


justified in assigning to the periphery a predominating share in 
the work of dark adaptation. 

We have also seen that these adaptive changes consist in a 
greatly increased responsiveness to light of short wave length, 
such appearing more intense than under “ light ” conditions. 

We have thus to account theoretically for a localised change in 
responsiveness with respect to certain forms of stimulation. 

The difference in histological structure between the fovea 
centralis and the surrounding area caused Schultze forty years 
ago (*4) to suggest a functional separation which he supported 
on evidence drawn from comparative anatomy. In his time, 
however, our experimental knowledge of adaptive changes was 
little advanced and his conception went unheeded. The first de- 
tailed investigation was due to H. Parinaud (”°), who has developed 
a theory of adaptation in a series of memoirs dating from 1881. 
Similar views have been carefully elaborated by Professor J. v. 
Kries of Freiburg and his colleagues and pupils in a large number 
of well-planned researches. 

Essentially the theories of Parinaud and v. Kries may be 
summarised quite simply. Two distinct visual mechanisms exist : 
one, subserving both chromatic and achromatic responsiveness, and 
represented in the retina by the cones; the other dealing with 
achromatic sensations alone, represented by the rods and visual 
purple. The former mechanisni is alone active in bright daylight 
and is unaffected by resting in the dark ; the latter is brought into 
play by shielding the eye from stimulation, being the sole or chief 
agency of twilight vision ; it is characterised by special responsive- 
ness to ethereal vibrations of short wave length. In view of the 
double nature of the mechanisms postulated, the theory has been 
christened the Duplicity Theory (Duplizititstheorie). Let us see 
_ how far the hypothesis covers the experimental observations I 
have enumerated. 

If the theory be true, we should expect (1) spectral maximum 
brightness to change in weak light in favour of the violet end ; 
(2) this change not to occur for images formed at the fovea 
centralis; (3) no achromatic threshold (vid. sup.) to be obtained 
for any light at the fovea or for red light anywhere. 

We have seen that each of these deductions is supported by 
good experimental evidence. 

Again, have we any forms of vision in which, apparently, the 


- 


372 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


basal mechanism is similar to that associated by the theory with 
twilight vision and uncomplicated by the existence of a second 
type of reaction? The subjects of total-colour-blindness appear 
to enable us to answer the question affirmatively. We found that 
the brightness judgments of these people agree well with those of 
normal men in a state of dark adaptation ; that there is evidence 
in such cases of diminished or absent foveal sensitivity, bad fixation, 
inferior acuteness of vision, nystagmus, central scotoma (some- 
times) and abnormally good vision in twilight. 

Conversely, are there any cases in which the hypothetical day- 
light mechanism alone reacts ? 

Parinaud (1°) has investigated several cases of “night-blindness ” 
or hemeralopia. He found that in such, vision was of the foveal 


type ; the colour sense was normal, but the spectrum shortened at. 


the violet end ; responsiveness to short waved light was abnormally 
low. The investigations of Messmer (7*) and others make it pro- 
bable, however, that the condition of night-blindness is not simple, 


different forms and degrees being classified under the same heading. ~ 


In some cases, dark adaptation is very slowly induced, but after 
a sufficiently long interval is normal in degree. In others some 
adaptation comes on in a normal time but is of inferior extent. 
It is important to bear these cautions in mind because night-blind- 
ness is markedly heritable and has been used, in the writer’s opinion 
illogically, as an argument in favour of the validity of the Mendelian 
theory of inheritance. For our present purpose, we must admit 
that night-blindness does not afford us so much information re- 
specting visual processes as the apparently opposite condition of 
total-colour-blindness. Provisionally, we may, perhaps, say that 
the latter condition is consistent with the activity of the hypothe- 
tical twilight mechanism functioning by itself, while some examples 
’ of the former peculiarity suggest that the daylight mechanism alone 
exists. But itis to be remembered that scarcely any theory, how- 
ever absurd, which has been advanced in explanation of a difficulty 
in sense physiology has failed to obtain the support of some patho- 
logical phenomena which have been tortured into a semblance of 
agreement with its postulates. 

The complex results in sensation which follow the application 
of short or moving stimuli to the retina have perhaps confused 
some readers; let us see whether our hypothesis i is capable of 
arranging them in an orderly manner. 


* 
ile i 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 373 


I have already pointed out that the peculiar striping of the 
primary image, the blue tailing off into white, suggests the interplay 
of two processes ; I also emphasised the dependence of the secondary 
image on adaptation and its (probable) absence at the fovea. We 
can perhaps sum up the effects in terms of our hypothesis thus :— 
The cone mechanism responds by two effects, the main part of 
the primary and the colour component in the tertiary. The rod 
apparatus responds in a threefold manner; it gives us the white 
tail of the primary, the whole of the secondary, and contributes, 
although slightly, to increasing the brightness of the tertiary. 
This way of putting the facts does, I think, clarify our ideas, but 
it certainly fails to remove all difficulties. Thus, if we are to 
regard our twilight mechanism as solely responsible for the 
secondary image, it is clear that we derive sensations of colour as 
well as sensations of luminosity without hue from that mechanism, 
since the secondary image is often coloured. Hence the mechanism 
cannot be identical with that of a totally colour-blind eye ; so that 
we must either give up the view that total-colour-blindness is a 
condition in which the rods and purple react as in a normal eye, 
or regard the secondary as due to something beyond rod stimula- 
tion. V. Kries is disposed to adopt the latter alternative, but in 
that case it is difficult to understand why the secondary is entirely 
peripheral, and therefore how it is that the cones can only respond 
with the rods in this case. 

This I believe to be the most serious difficulty in the way of an 
acceptance of the duplicity hypothesis. It is easy to evade the 
objection by transferring the production of secondary images to 
the brain (? consciousness), a course adopted by. Parinaud, but this 
is undesirable, and I think it better to leave the difficulty where it 
is. We can only say, in the stereotyped phrase of the embarrassed 


- physiologist, further work is necessary to clear up the point. 


We have so far examined the duplicity theory by the light of 
the chief experimental facts, but a little more evidence has to be 
considered. 

It has long been known that the relative numbers of rods and 
cones differ in various animals. Thus the rods are very large and 
almost exclusively present in the retine of nocturnal animals, such 
as owls, bats, and hedgehogs. In many other creatures, on the 
other hand, including most birds, cones predominate. It was 
indeed on the strength of this that Schultze advanced a theory 


374 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


essentially similar to the one we are considering. Kiihne subse- — 
quently showed (?7) that visual purple was present only in retine 
containing rods. He was not, however, able to extract this pig- 
ment from all rod-containing eyes, a notable exception being the 
bat ; recently, Trendelenburg (7°) has obtained abundance of visual 
purple from more than one species of bat. Experimentally, as we 
have seen, Abelsdorff has shown that the owl is specially sensitive 
to short waves and the dove relatively insensitive (tested by the 
pupillo-motor response). 

We all know that most nocturnal animals see badly in broad 
daylight, while such birds as the pigeon exhibit normally a marked 
degree of night-blindness. In Parinaud’s words :— 

“It is a matter of common observation that hens and pigeons 
see very imperfectly in artificial light and defend themselves with - 
difficulty against the hand that tries to seize them; that as soon 
as the sun goes down these animals seek their night shelter. The 
old adage, ‘To go to bed with the hens,’ meaning to go to bed © 
early, evidently having its origin in this fact ” (1°, p. 66). 

Biological investigation appears to show, therefore, a co- 
existence of rods and visual purple with vision of the twilight 

variety and of cones with optimal vision in daylight. It is easy 
~ to lay too much stress on this sort of evidence. Reasoning from 
analogy is dangerous and especially equivocal when dealing with 
sense physiology. Take the history of opinion regarding a bat’s 
vision. The objectors to the theory of duplicity pointed out that 
no visual purple had been extracted from the bat’s retina. Its 
partisans retorted that the bat relied on sensory mechanisms other 
than sight, possibly scent currents, as seems to be the case with 
moths. My friend Mr. Arthur Bacot points out to me that the 
.temarkably rapid darting movements of a bat in pursuit of prey, 
movements which are comparable in point of velocity with the 
swift’s flight, seem hardly compatible with the type of twilight 
vision we have agreed to associate with the rods, characterised as 
that is by poor acuity of vision. We are now aware that many 
species of bat possess visual purple. Hence, if the previous train 
of reasoning be at all correct, the value of an outfit of rods and 
visual purple to the twilight animals is rendered doubtful. In 
precisely the same way, the early roosting of diurnal birds may be 
due to causes other than a condition of night-blindness. 

It is necessary to dwell upon these points because in no de- — 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 375 


partment of physiology is there a greater tendency to advance 
equivocal evidence in favour of an hypothesis than in that con- 


cerned with the special senses. 


I can now sum up the case presented. I hope I have rendered 
it probable that— 

(1) There is a marked difference between central and peripheral 
vision, in regard to the phenomenon of darkness adaptation, the 
former being little if at all affected in the process. 

(2) These differences may be provisionally interpreted on the 
supposition that visual sensations are bound up with two distinct 
mechanisms : (a) That of the cones, with which chromatic sensitivity 
and achromatic sensations under daylight conditions are associated ; 
(b) that of the rods upon which depend achromatic sensations 
under conditions of dark adaptation. 

The objections to this view are neither few nor unimportant. 
It has not been proved that no central adaptation whatever occurs. 
The equations (colour matches) of totally colour-blind persons and 
those of the normal “dark” eye agree well but not absolutely. 
There is also a difficulty in interpreting the secondary image of 
recurrent vision. 

That the first of these objections (as well as the kindred one 
that a central scotoma does not exist in all cases of total-colour-— 
blindness) may be parried by supposing a trace of visual purple 
and a few scattered rods to be present in the fovea, isclear. Recent 
measurement by Fritsch (!7, p. 188) on a negro’s fovea gave an 
absolutely rod-free zone of only ‘2 mm., corresponding to an ~ 
angular distance of less than a degree ; we could not allow much 
weight to failures in the demonstration of such small adaptable 
areas, even supposing them to be absolutely rod-free. 

The difficulty regarding ‘‘total-colour-blinds” and normal 
“dark” equations is not formidable. The fact is that sufficient 
measurements have not been made to enable us to affirm that the 
differences are significant. The more serious question as to the real 
significance of the secondary image in recurrent vision has been 
already considered ; it may prove the crucial point in the theory. 
Accepting the above view of the réle of the visual purple and rods 
as an important element in the physiological processes of vision 
with low intensities of light, one is tempted to speculate as to the 
nature of their activity. Parinaud was of opinion that the process 
depended upon fluorescence of the retina mainly due to the presence 


376 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


of visual purple. This view is certainly incorrect. A bleached 
retina is more strongly fluorescent than one in which the visual 
purple is unreduced (Kiihne 2’), although the bleached substance 
itself may possess fluorescent properties, since Nagel and 
Himstedt (?8) observed that a bleached solution of visual purple 
was more strongly fluorescent than the solvent alone. 

_ If then we accept, as a working hypothesis, the view that the 
rods and visual purple form a link in the chain of processes by 
means of which certain forms of stimuli are, under particular 
conditions, associated with sensations, we must not, in the present 
state of the question, attempt to assign to them any precise 
physical or physiological share in the process. 

In conclusion, the reader need not suppose that the cones of 
the periphery are functionless, that in broad daylight peripheral . 
vision is at all of the type we have been studying. As a matter of 
fact, at the extreme periphery which is normally quite colour-blind, 
brightness values are altogether different from those of the “ dark ” 
eye, as is shown in the following table (v. Kries,!” p. 199) :— 


Na line=100 


Wave length. . 680 651 629 608 589 573 558 530 513 
Peripheral value : 
daylight . . 96 375 1775 101 100 796 522 285 146 
Peripheral value 
twilight . . 2? 34 140 35:5 100 256 351 321 198 


The study of this problem of visual adaptation illustrates well 
the patient and laborious experimental work necessary to de- 
monstrate even a limited range of phenomena, the complexity of 
results which appear at first simple and the necessity of caution 
in framing satisfactory hypotheses. It has been chosen to de- 
scribe because it is so instructive from these points of view. 


BIBLIOGRAPHY 


(This list is not, of course, complete. The papers marked with an asterisk 
contain extensive bibliographies, ) 


1 Charpentier, Arch, d’Opthalmologie, vol. iv., pp. 291-323. 

* A, Tschermak, Pfliiger’s Arch., vol. 70, pp. 297-328, A critical summary 
* of work up to 1902 will be found in * Die Helldunkeladaptation des Auges 
und die Funktion der Stabchen und Zapfen, by A. Tschermak, Ergebnisse 
der Physiologie, lst Jahrgang, 2nd part, pp. 695, &c. 


Po 
_ 
7 


— 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 377 


® Hering, Pfliig. Arch,, vol. 60, pp. 519-642. 

* Haycraft, Journal of Phy-iology, vol. 21, p. 126. 

® Rivers, Journ, of Physiol., vol. 22, p. 137. 

* Hering, Pfliig. Arch., vol. 47, », 417. 

_ 7 V. Kries and Nagel, Zeitschrift f. Psychol. u. Physiol. der Sinnesorgane, 
vol. 23, p. 161. 

* Tschermak, Ergeb. d. Physiol., 1st Jahrg., 2nd part, p. 731. 

* Burch, Proc. Roy. Soc., B, vol. 76, p. 199. See also Nagel and Schaefer, 
Zeits. f. Psy. u. Phys, d. Sinnes., vol. 34, p. 271; Loeser, ibid., vol. 36, p. 1. 

%” Parinaud, La Vision, Paris, 1898 (Doin), p. 51. 

u* J. v. Kries, Die Gesichtsempfindungen, Handb. d. Physiol. d. Mensch., 
herausgegeben v. W. Nagel, vol. 3, pp. 109-282. 

1 Hering, Pfliig. Arch., vol. 49, p. 563. 

1% Schaternikoff, Zeits, f. Psy. u. Phys. d. Sinnes., vol. 29, p, 255. See also 
Fujita, tbid., vol. 43 (pt. ii.), p. 243. 

* Abney, Proc, Roy. Soc,, vol. 66, p. 179. 

* * Crunert, Arch. f. Opthalmol., vol. 56, p. 182. Other papers on total- 
colour-blindness are :—Hering, Pfliig. Arch., vol. 49, p. 563, Hess, Zeits. f, 
Psy, u. Phys, d. Sinnes., vol. 29, p. 99. Hess and Hering, Pfliig. Arch., vol. 
71, p. 105. V. Hippel, Klinische Monatsblatter f. Augenheilkunde, vol. 36, 
p. 324. Nagel, Zeits. f. Psy. u. Phys. d. Sinnes., vol. 29, p. 118. Uhtoff, 
Zeits. f. Psy. u. Phys. d. Sinnes., vol. 20, p. 326; and vol. 27, p. 344. 

#® Quoted in * Helmholtz’s Handbuch d. Physiol. Optik., 2nd edit., p. 501. 

" V. Kries, Nagel’s Handb. d. Physiol., &c., vol. 3, pp. 220-226. 

8 Charpentier, Archives de Physiol., 1892, p. 541; also C. R. de l’Acad. 
des Sciences, vol. 113, p. 149. 

%” Macdougall, liritish Journal of Psychology, vol. 1, p. 78. 

® Hess, Zeits. f. Psy. u. Phys. d. Sinnes., vol. 27, p. 1. 

1 V. Kries, Zeits. f. Psy. u. Phys. d. Sinnes., vol. 29, p. 81. 

* Hamaker, Zeits. f. Psy. u. Phys. d. Sinnes., vol. 21, p. 1.. Other 
papers on recurrent vision are :—Bidwell, Proc. Roy. Soc., vol. 56, p. 132. 
Charpentier, C. R. de PAcad. d. Scien., vol. 113, p. 149. Hess, Pfliig. Arch., 
vol. 49, p. 190. Hess, Arch. f. Opthalmol., vol. 44, p. 445; vol. 51, p. 225. 
V. Kries, Zeits. f. Psy. u. Phys. d. Sinnes., vol. 12, p. 81; vol. 19, p. 175; 
vol. 25, p. 239. 

* Garten, Pfliig. Arch., vol. 68, p. 68. Sachs, Pfliig. Arch., vol. 52, p. 79. 


- Abelsdorff, Zeits. f. Psy. u. Pliys. d. Sinnes., vol. 22, pp. 81, 451; vol. 34, 


p. 111 (with Feitchenfeld). 

* Schultze, Arch, f. Mikrosk. Anatm., vol. 2, pp. 175-273. 

* Parinaud, Arch, Génér, de Med., 7th Series, vol. 7 (1881), pp. 403-414. 
C. R. de Acad. d. Sciences, vol. 93, p. 286. 

** Messmer, Zeits, f. Psy..u. Phys. d. Sinnes. (2te Abth. f. Sinnes- 
physiologie (1907), vol. 42, p. 83. 

* Kiihne, Hermann’s Handbuch der Phys., vol. 3, p. 235 (twenty-two 
other papers published, 1877-9). 

* Trendelenburg, Arch. f. Physiol., 1904, Suppl. Bnd., p. 228. 

*® Nagel and Himstedt, Festschr, d. Univ. Freiburg i. Br., 1902 (quoted 
in Nagel’s Handb., vol. 3, p. 96). . 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 
PART II 


Section [I.—HistToricAL INTRODUCTION 


Any one who examines the current text-books of physiology 
will find that in discussing colour vision great prominence is 
given to two theories, those of Helmholtz and Hering respectively. 
As the reader will also find it admitted that neither theory resumes © 
in an altogether satisfactory manner the data of observation and 
experiment, he may be tempted to ask why so much stress is put 
upon them. An obvious explanation is the authority attaching - 
to any utterances of men who have notably advanced our know- 
ledge in various fields. This cannot, however, be the sole reason ; 
physiologists not less distinguished than Helmholtz and Hering have 
before now promulgated theories which, being inconsistent with the 
subsequent results of investigation, have perished. The vitality 
of these particular theories of colour vision must therefore be 
referred to some.principle other than that of mere authority. 

The object of this essay is to show that these admittedly in- 
complete theories are important because each brings into prominence 
one aspect of the problem; the harmonising of these two aspects 
must occupy those who come after us and examine the question 
‘ by the light of fuller knowledge than we possess. 

With this end in view, it will be necessary, before considering 
the theories themselves and their relation to known facts, to 
examine briefly some opinions as to the nature of visual processes 
which were held long ago but have by no means lost all interest 
for us. Of these speculations, the most important are due to the 
Greek philosophers and men of science. 

In attempting to estimate the scientific value of the Greek — 
theories of vision, it is necessary to bear in mind certain limitations 

1 A valuable account of Greek Sense Physiology and Psychology will be found 


in “Greek Theories of Elementary Cognition from Alemzon to Aristotle,” by 
Professor J. I. Beare (Clarendon Press, bt: p- 354). 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY = 379 


which were imposed upon them by imperfect means of investiga- 
tion. Even with modern apparatus, it is not easy to obtain a pre- 
cise idea of the elaborate structures contained in the eye—as every 
student knows—hence workers unprovided with the simplest 
microscope knew almost nothing of what is now common know- 
ledge. Roughly, we may say that all the early theories agree in 
regarding the “ pupil” of the eye and the “image” within it as 
of primary importance. Again, the flash of light seen on pressing 
or rapidly moving the eye was held to prove the existence of an 
inherent or native “fire,” also of great significance. Thirdly, the 
presence of a watery substance within the eyeball had to be 
accounted for. The problem, as it presented itself to the earliest 
writers, was to assign: their proper shares in the visual act to the 
“ fire,” the “image,” and the “ water.” 

One of the earliest of the Greek writers on this subject was 
Alemeon of Cretona (fl. B.c. 500). Our knowledge of his views 
seems fragmentary; he thought that seeing is accomplished by 
rays passing from the ocular “ fire’ to the object, and that these 
returning to the eye, altered in some way, are reflected in the 
diaphanous “ water.” The “fire” is therefore the active element 
in vision. The hypothesis hardly appears consistent with itself, 
because the conception of a visual ray from the “ fire”? cannot 
readily be harmonised with a mirror-like action of the “‘ water.” 

Empedocles’ (circa B.c. 450) theory was more subtle and 
elaborate, although it is not easy to reconcile different statements 
attributed to him. 

According to the first doctrine enunciated by Empedocles, 
like perceives like. All bodies whatever are characterised by— 
(1) All are made up of four elements, earth, air, fire, and water. 
(2) All are permeated by minute passages or pores, and all give 
- off emanations which enter the pores. Thus, in perception, emana- 
tions from the object pass into the pores of the percipient organ.! 
But, that this passage may be effected, it is necessary that the 
emanations and the pores should correspond ; if the former are 
too large or too small for the latter, no perception can occur. 

Hence with the eye alone can we perceive emanations of colour 


1 Cf. Lucretius, De Rer. Nat., bk. ii., 833— 


* Noscere ut hine possis prius onmem efflare colorem 
Particulas, quam discedant ad semina rerum.” 
See also bk. iv., 72-86. : 


380 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


because these are “ symmetrical” with the pores of the eye alone. 
This correspondence is the basis of sense specificity. Further 
there is a symmetrical arrangement within the eye itself with 
respect to the different forms of stimulation. By means of the 
intra-ocular fire we perceive the emanations of fire—i.e. white— 
with the “‘ water” we see water—1.e. black—and so on. 

“With earth we see earth, with water we see water, with air 
we see the bright air; just as with love we (perceive) love, and 
with hate, baleful hate” (1). Empedocles is said to have regarded 
four colours, white, black, red, and green, as primaries (Stobeeus), 
but only examines white and black in detail. He also taught 
that rays issued from the visual “fire,” but how this process was 
associated with his general doctrine of pores and emanations is 
not certain. 

Democritus (? 460-357) agreed with Empedocles in postulating 
the entrance of particles from an object into pores contained in the 
perceiving structure and in the dictum that “like is perceived by 
like.” But he denied that there are four qualitatively distinct 
elements, and believed that all things are made up of homogeneous 
atoms moving in a vacuum and infinitely numerous. Vision is 
due to the mirroring of an object in the eye, the latter’s character F 
being somehow determined by its moist and porous nature. This 
part of Democritus’ theory was sharply criticised by Aristotle, 
who remarked : “ It is absurd also that it should not have occurred 
to him to doubt why the eye alone sees, but nothing else in which 
energies are apparent. That the sight is aqueous is true; yet it 
does not happen that it sees because it is aqueous but because it 
is diaphanous, which is also common to air” (2). 

Democritus seems to have been the first writer to attempt a 
detailed theory of colours, the simple ones being white, black, red, 
and green; his account, which is somewhat elaborate, has not 
played a sufficiently important part in the history of opinion to 
need further description, but it is well to remember that he (anti- 
cipating Berkeley!) held that colour had no objective reality. 
“ ... the ultimate\elements—the plenum and the vacwum—are 
destitute of all sensible qualities, while the things composed of 
them possess colour (as they do every sensible quality) owing 
merely to the order, figure, and position of the atoms, i.e. (a) to their 


1 Cf. ‘*The First Dialogue between Hylas and Philonous,” especially pp. 314- 
318 (Sampson's edition, vol. i.). Lucretius, loc. cit. 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY  38l 


order relatively to one another; (b) to their several shapes; and 
(c) to the position of each in its place. The subjective aspects— 
the qualities—of sensible objects are all due to these three things. 
Colour has no objective existence, since the colours of bodies are 
due to the position of the atoms in them ” (°). 

The views of Anaxagoras (B.c. 499-428) and Diogenes of 
Apollonia (5th cent. B.c.) may be passed over rapidly. Anaxagoras 
held, in opposition to his contemporaries, although the opposition 
is more formal than real, that perception is the result not of like 
operating upon like, but of the reaction between contrary and 
contrary. The “image ” is not reflected upon a part of like colour 
to the object but upon a different colour. Diogenes, who be- 
lieved that an all-pervading “air” was the ultimate agency in 
nature, has left no distinct theory of colour vision. 

One would naturally expect that Plato (429-347), to whom we 
now turn, would have powerfully contributed to the advancement 
of our knowledge of the physiological psychology of vision, but 
this is not the case. His account of the physical side of the 
problem is contained in a passage in the “ Timzus,” of which the 
following quotation gives an idea :— 

** And of the organs they first contrived the eyes to give light, 
fixing them by a cause on this wise. They contrived that as much 
of fire as would not have the power of burning, but would only 
give a gentle light, the light ‘of every-day life, should be formed 
into a body; and the pure fire which is within us and akin to 
this they made to flow through the eyes in a single entire and ~ 
smooth substance, at the same time compressing the centre of the 
eye so as to retain all the grosser element and only to allow this 
to be sifted through pure. When therefore the light of day sur- 
rounds the stream of vision, then like falls upon like, and there 
is a union, and one body is formed by natural affinity according 
to the direction of the eyes, wherever the light that falls from 
within meets that which comes from an external body. And 
everything being affected by likeness, whatever touches or is 
touched by the stream of vision, their motions are diffused. over 
the whole body and reach the soul, producing that perception 
which we call sight ” (*). 

In the genesis of colour, particles are discharged from external 
things and impinge upon the eye, some being larger, some smaller, 
and some equal in magnitude to the parts of the eye. All colours 


382 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


are compounded of four; white, black, bright, and red. Bright 
when mixed with red and white becomes golden-yellow; red 
blended with black and white yields violet. 

From statements in the “ Timzus” and “ Republic ” it would 
seem that Plato, unlike Democritus, believed in the objective 
existence of colour in things; but, as Helmholtz remarks (°), his 
views seem to have varied. In the “ Theetetus,” colour is con- 
sidered from an entirely different standpoint, as will be clear from { 
the next quotation :— 

‘We shall see that every colour, white, black, and every other 
colour, arises out of the eye meeting the appropriate motion, and 
that what we term the substance of each colour is neither the 
active nor the passive element, but something which passes between 
them and is peculiar to each percipient. . . .” 

‘““When the eye and the appropriate object meet together 
and give birth to whiteness and the sensation of whiteness which 
could not have been given by either of them going to any other _ | 
object ; while the sight is flowing from the eye, and whiteness 
from the colour-producing element, the eye becomes fulfilled with 
sight and sees, and becomes not sight but a seeing eye ; the object 
which combines in forming the colour is fulfilled with whiteness 
and becomes not whiteness but white ” (°). 

Aristotle’s (B.c. 384-322) theory of vision is of very great 
importance in the further development of the subject, and indeed 
still survives in a modified form. We must therefore examine it 
more closely. 

According to Aristotle, the object of sight is colour. Colour 
is at the surface of all visible objects, but, in order to be seen, 
requires the presence of light, which is the medium of vision.? 

Light again pre-supposes a diaphanous substrate which in its 
turn is the medium of light. Examples of this “‘ diaphanous ” are 
air, water, and many solids. The realisation or actualisation of 
this potential quality of being diaphanous is light, its absence 
darkness. When the former condition of actual light is estab- 
lished in the diaphanous medium, any coloured body sets up a 


——. 


1 Of. Lucretius, De Rer. Nat., bk. ii., 795— 


‘* Praeteria quoniam nequent sine luce colores 
Esse, neque in lucem existunt primordia rerum, 
Scire licet quam sint nullo velata colore.” 


The whole passage, from 730 to 833, is of much interest in this connection. 


~~ 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 383 


movement in it, between object and eye; this is the essential 
process in colour perception. 

The diaphanous substrate, upon which depends the existence 
of light and, a fortiori, colour, is not peculiar to the bodies called 
transparent or diaphanous, but is a species of universally diffused 
natural power ; it is not indeed capable of existence independently 
of “body” but subsists in varying degrees in all bodies. The 
colour of a body either forms its surface or is upon that surface, 
the latter opinion being the more exact since the indeterminate 
“diaphanous” of air and water exhibits colour, which, however, 
owing to the indeterminate boundary, is variable. This explains 
_ the changing hues of sea or sky. 

Bodies with a definite boundary have a fixed colour, so that 
one might again define colour as the surface limit of the “ dia- 
phanous ” in determinately bounded body. This definition is con- 
sistent with the first given, viz. that which stimulates the actualised 
“diaphanous”’ (light) between the object and the eye, but the 
latter is a definition in terms of vision and the medium of vision, 
the former in terms of the object as it exists apart from vision. 

Colour is a genus comprising seven species ; it is a quality and 
cannot therefore exist without a substrate. The seven species are 
white, black, golden-yellow, crimson, violet, leek-green, and deep 
blue. The colour genus (like all other genera of sensible qualities) 
consists of species lying between extremes ; outside these extremes 
there can be no colours, between them are specific boundaries. 
By subdividing the scale limited by the extremes, we cannot _ 
obtain an infinite number of distinct colours because a sensible 
quality is discrete not continuous. By dividing the substrate we 
do not arrive at any new colour, the halves of a white object are 
white. It is true that by sufficiently fine division no colour what- 
ever may be perceptible, but on reuniting these portions we again 
obtain white. The two limits are black and white; when one is 
actually existent the other is only potential. The transition from 
white to black is effected through the successive degrees which are 
the species of colour. The substratum, of which these are the 
qualities, is one, and is in strictness that which is changed; the 
colours alternate. 

Colour is not purely subjective. It is true that it depends 
upon the eye, but it also depends upon the object. Actual colour 
depends upon the possibilities of these two being realised together, 


384 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


but the coloured object existed in nature as a potential colour 
before the act of vision and apart from it. “It is light that at once 
transforms the potential colour to actuality and the potentially 
seeing to an actually seeing eye” (7). 

In the colour scale (as among the elements) there is a sort of 
opposition of positive and negative. White is the positive, black 
the negative. ‘ 

This is Aristotle’s general account of colour and of the “ dia- 
phanous”’ its vehicle—omitting his views of reflection which are 
not important from the physiological or psychological standpoint. 
He also treats of certain colours in detail. 

The presence of some fire-like element is the cause of light in 
the diaphanous, and in its absence we have darkness. In all de- 
terminately bounded bodies we may assume something analogous . 
with the presence and absence of this fiery element. Its absence 
means blackness, its presence whiteness. Therefore, in deter- 
minately bounded bodies, blackness is privation of whiteness. 
Thus, blackness and whiteness are contraries within one sensory _ 
province, that of colour, and from them all the other colours are 
to be explained. “The transition from white to black is possible 
through continuous degrees of privation; that from white to 
black is likewise possible by an ascending scale in the opposite 
direction. The various colours are species which fall between - 
the two contraries and are generated of certain combinations of 
these’ (8). For instance, in passing from white to black we first 
come to crimson. As the intervening stages in the passage mark 
relative extremes, change can start from any point.! 

With regard to the actual mode of origin of the intermediate 
colours, what is actually effected in the above-mentioned process ? 
Aristotle discusses and condemns the doctrine of atomic juxta- 
position and that of superposition, taking the view that a complete 
blending occurred. No individual part of the compound colour 
retains its primitive characters unmodified.? 

As specific illustrations of this theory, we may take red, which 
is produced by light streaming through black, and purple, distin- 
guished from crimson in possessing more of the dark ingredient. 


| 
| 


1 Goethe, of course, maintained the correctness of this theory in the Farben- 
lehre. (Cf. Goethe’s Theory of Colours, translated by Eastlake, London, 1840.) 

* Aristotle does not give this account in all his works. Vide Beare, op. cit., 
pp. 74-76. i 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 385 


Thus the sun shining through a fog is red; the feebler lamp ray 
sometimes appears purple. 

In this connection, Aristotle refers to positive and negative 
after-images. After looking at the sun and closing the eyes we see 
the object at first of the same colour as before ; this changes to 
crimson, then to purple, then black, and finally vanishes. This 
illustrates the genesis of colours from the blending of black and 
white. Simultaneous contrast is explained along these lines—the 
brightest rainbow appears in the darkest cloud, white wool has its 
colour intensified when placed next black wool, &c. 

Aristotle rejected altogether the theory of emanations and 
pores (Empedocles, &c.), while his conception of a vibratile move- 
‘ment imparted to the actualised “ diaphanous” may, perhaps, be 
regarded as a partial anticipation of the modern doctrine of a 
luminiferous ether. We cannot, however, push this comparison 
very far, since he maintained, in opposition to Empedocles, that 
light does not travel. It is not, I think, necessary to summarise 
the Aristotelian teaching in so far as it deals with structure, 
since, for the reasons already mentioned, it is of purely anti- 
quarian interest. The curious reader will find ample material, if 
he desires to pursue the matter further, in the works cited by 
Professor Beare. 

We can now consider for a moment the relations subsisting 
between Greek doctrines and the modern development of visual 
physiology and psychology. 

The reader will have noticed already that the opinions I have 
summarised are concerned both with the theory of visual sensations 
proper, and the nature and functions of the eye itself. Or, roughly, 
they are, in the modern terminology, partly psycho-physiological 
and partly anatomical or histological. 

Subsequent progress in these two departments has not been 
equal. Thus, while we are less ignorant than the Greeks regarding 
the structure of the eye and have framed formule more accurately 
descriptive of physical concepts, the development of physiological 
psychology has not been so great and is largely the work of recent 
times. The result has been that our present way of looking at and 
thinking about the structure of the eye and the physical changes 
associated with colour vision owes comparatively little to the 
Greeks, and may be said to date from the epoch-making discoveries 
of Newton. Theories of colour sensations, on the other hand, 

: 2B 


: 


may be traced through Hering to Goethe, and the latter almost 
explicitly founded his work on that of Aristotle. | 

It is true that the study of visual psychology received a consider- 
able impetus at the beginning of the eighteenth century from the 
work of Berkeley. But Berkeley’s writings are perhaps rather a 
contribution to epistemology than to physiological -psychology as 
we now understand it.t 

I shall therefore. pass to the development of our knowledge as 
to the actual process of stimulation, so far as experiment has 
tended to display it, and the theoretical developments this has 
received in modern times. We can then again take up the thread 
of the narrative treating of visual sensations and attempt an 
estimate of its modern outcome. Before leaving this pre- 
liminary sketch, however, it is well to point out one difficulty 
in framing an hypothesis of vision, which hampered the acutest 
thinker of the Greek period, but from which we have partially 
escaped. 

It will have been seen that practically all these philosophers 
either explicitly or implicitly adopted the postulate that like acts 
upon like. Anaxagoras might appear to be an exception, but a 
little thought convinces one that the exception is only formal. 

In adopting the literal converse of the proposition he too is com- 
mitted to the belief that there is some necessary connection in 
kind between the processes occurring within the eye—or the mind— 
and those supposed to exist outside of it. This idea pervades all 
the Greek speculations—thus, since the external medium is trans- 
parent, there must be some internal transparency ; since “ fire ” 
is visible, there must be some internal “fire” by which it is 
perceived, and so forth. In fact this “similarity hypothesis ” 
~ might be regarded as the most primitive of all forms of specula- 
tion. There is reason to believe that sympathetic or homeopathic 
magic, an obvious extension of the same idea, is culturally older 
than any religions, while most of the latter embody the conception 
in some more or less changed form. 

The real importance of what is called Miiller’s Law of Spétific 
Sense Energy, is that it contains an explicit denial of any necessary _ 


386 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


1 For Berkeley’s views on colour, see ‘‘The First Dialogue between Hylas and — 
Philonous”’ (vol. i., p. 314 e¢ seg.) ‘‘ Alciphoron,” Fourth Dialogue (vol. ii. — 
p 288, &c.). Cf. also ‘‘ An Essay towards a\New Theory of Vision” (vol. i p. 79 
et seg.). The references are to Sampson’s octien (Bell, 1898). 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 387 


qualitative connection or resemblance between the physical pro- 
cesses of stimulation and the psycho-physiological changes asso- 
ciated therewith in the sense organ and “consciousness.” For 
this reason, and although no definite proof of its applicability to 
all cases of sensory stimulation has been furnished, the law marked 
an important. step forward in the study of physiological psychology. 


Section II.—NorMau Visuaut STIMULI 


The physical basis of experimental work on colour stimulation 
is to be found in the conception of a simple or homogeneous light, 
and dates from Newton’s researches in prismatic analysis. By 
homogeneous light we understand ethereal vibrations all having 
the same wave length or vibration frequency (within assigned 
limits). Prisms and gratings allow us to filter such lights from a 
mixture and employ them for our experiments. A pure light is 
uniquely defined by its wave length, and any such light may possess 
any intensity. Unfortunately, the physical unit of intensity is not 
readily fixed. 

In any spectrum, the intensities of individual lights depend on 
the source of illumination and the method of analysis—z.e. on the 
extent of surface over which light of a given wave length is dis- 
persed. In an interference spectrum, dispersion is uniform; in a 
prismatic spectrum, on the other hand, it increases from red to 
violet, so that the short-waved light is relatively less intense than 
the long-waved. Spectra obtained by the two methods are not 
therefore directly comparable. 

By colour or light mixing we understand an arrangement by 
means of which two or more homogeneous lights fall upon the 
same retinal area. Numerous experimental methods have been 
devised for this purpose, and the results obtained ? enable us to 
formulate certain general statements respecting chromatic stimuli. 


1 The application by Krarup (H. Krarup, Physisch- opthalmologische Grenz- 
Leipzig, 1906) of Angstrém’s energy measurements is a step in the 
right direction. 

+ As this essay is chiefly concerned with the theories of colour vision, I assume 
the reader to be acquainted with the main experimental data and methods. Such 
knowledge can be obtained by consulting any good text-book, for instance Dr. 
Rivers’ article in the second volume of Schiifer’s Text-book, or Prof. v. Kries’ article 

in the third volume of Nagel’s Handbuch, 


388 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


These statements owe their present form to Grassmann, and can 
be summarised in the following way :— 

1. If in a mixture one component be continuously varied, the 
appearance of the mixture will likewise vary ; unequal lights mixed 
with equal lights produce unequal mixtures. 

2. On the other hand, lights which appear equal give, when 
mixed, equal mixtures. A corollary is that proportional increase 
of the intensity of each component does not destroy a match. 

Passing to the actual observations, we at once note that the 
effect of mixing spectral extremes is the production of a colour, 
purple, not present in the spectrum at all. It thus follows that 
any graphical representation of our results must take the form of a 
closed curve, since, passing from red to violet, we can either travel 
over the range of spectral colours or by way of purple. 

If we mix lights not belonging to the extreme ends of the range 
our results are quite different. The simplest cases are those of 
mixing colours of wave length not less than 540 uu. For instance, 
a red (670 wm) mixed with a yellow (580 uu) gives a pure colour 
of intermediate wave length; the greater the proportion of the 
long wave-lengthened component in the mixture, the nearer will the 
position of the mixed colour be to the red end, and convetsely.? 
The mixing relations for this part of the spectrum are therefore 
straightforward ; but the results obtained—i.e. that two simple 
lights when mixed give a colour matching that of a simple 
light the wave length of which is intermediate between those of 
its components—are only valid for a small part of the range. If 
we mix a blue-green (510 uu) with a blue (460 uu), the mixture, 
although resembling, perhaps closely, a pure intermediate, e.g. 
_ 490 up, does not match it perfectly. The mixed colour is paler, 
or as we say, “less saturated,” than any of the spectral colours. 
This is still more evident when we choose our components in such 
a fashion that the wave length of one is greater and that of the 
other less than 517 wu. If one constituent is taken a little nearer 
the red than 560 wm and the other diminished, in wave length, 
in each experiment, then, with suitable proportions, the mixtures 
pass from greenish yellow becoming paler and paler until we reach 


1 It is very important to remember that when we say that two colours are 
‘‘equal” or “match,” we only mean that viewed under apparently the same 
conditions they look alike, i.e. are followed by the same results in consciousness. 
Neglect of this truism has been a fertile source of misunderstanding. 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 389 


a combination which corresponds to a sensation of whiteness. As 
we tend to assign a unique position to white in our scale of 
sensations, it is customary to complete the mixing laws by the 
following addition :— 

3. Any light mixture whatever can be matched by a mixture of 
a définite homogeneous light (or a definite purple) and white light. 
That our results may be as general as possible, it is well to note 
that there is no necessity to accord a special place to white, 
although it is convenient in practice. 

We see then that the mixing experiments give us variations in 
colour tone and variations in whiteness, that is, two variables, so 


that our results ought to be expressible graphically by some plane 


figure. We have also seen that we can pass from red to violet 
through the spectral colours and back again to red through purple, 
so that our graph should be a closed figure. Finally in virtue of 
the fact that the position of any mixed colour depends directly 
upon the relative proportions of its components, we infer that the 
method of determining the position of the centre of inertia of 
masses might be adapted to the task of ascertaining the position 
of a mixed colour, given the nature and proportions of its 
constituents. 

If three colours, A, B, C, none of which can be mixed from the 
other two, be represented by three points in a plane, then, on 
assigning to them values in terms of any unit, the situations and 
quantitative values of their mixtures can be ascertained. Thus a 
colour mixed from a units of A and b units of B will lie on the 
line A B at the point at which the centre of gravity of two masses 
aA and bB (representing the proportions of each colour in the 
mixture) would be situated. 

In order to establish the correctness of this method it is 


necessary to prove that, given the experimental laws of colour- 


mixing, as above defined, this construction is valid in all possible 
cases—i.e. that the situation of a mixed colour, in a diagram, 
coincides with that of the centre of gravity of two equivalent 
masses when—({1) The two constituents can be mixed from the 
three chosen colours; (2) when one can and one cannot so be 
mixed ; (3) when neither can so be mixed. 

The proof is too long for insertion in this essay, but it is quite 
straightforward (*). We can show that the co-ordinates of the point 

= 1 Cf. on this point, J. v. Kries, Nagel’s Handb., vol. iii., p. 116. 


390 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


at which the mixture must be situated according to the mixing 
laws, are the co-ordinates of the centre of gravity of masses | 
situated at the points at which the components are represented. 

It is clear that a diagram constructed on these principles will 
vary in accordance with our choice of units and fixed points. Thus 
Newton chose white as a fixed point and arranged the simple 
colours at equal distances from it, so that his diagram was a 
circle. We should obtain this result except that the part of the 
curve passing from violet to red must be a straight line, since purple 
can only be mixed from these colours and therefore lies on the 
chord joining them, 

Recurring to the experimental facts previously mentioned, we 


Green 


a 


Fic. 4.—Colour Table. 


see that the most convenient form of diagram will be that 
figured (Fig. 4). From red to yellow the curve is a straight line 
since we found that mixtures of colours between these limits 
- matched pure spectral lights; we then get a sharp bend in the 
part of the diagram beyond green, representing the low saturation 
of mixtures from this region; finally we have the straight line 
through purple. 

Suppose now we select three colours in the spectral series, 
e.g. red, green, blue, then in accordance with our previous de- 
ductions, all mixtures of these are represented in the triangle 
RGB. This includes a good deal of our complete diagram, but 
not all of it. If we choose, instead of blue, violet, then the © 
triangle RGV comprises nearly the whole spectral diagram; in 
other words, nearly all colours can be matched by mixing three 
chosen lights in suitable proportions (vid. infra, p. 404). There is 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 391 


not, indeed, a complete super-position, because of the bend in 
the spectral diagram between green and blue, which means that 
mixtures of green and violet are less saturated than spectral 
cyanide blue; we can, however, generalise a little. 

If spectral greenish-blue be mixed with red in certain propor- 
tions, it matches a mixture of green and violet-— 


a.G Bl. +b.R.=c.Gr. +d.V. 


Hence 
a.G Bl. =c.Gr.+d.V. —b.R. 


or we have the unmixable colour in terms of our three chosen 


colours. 


It is to be remarked that colour equations, as they are termed, 
of the form 
a.R.+b.Gr. Cc.V.=d.R. 


seem to be a justifiable way of expressing the facts. Addition is 
uniform, the same result being always obtained when the same 
quantities are summed; it is commutative, the order of opera- 
tions does not affect the result ; it is associative and homogeneous. 
Analogies may be discerned in the process of colour mixing. 
Green + (mixed with) Red + Violet = Red + Green + Violet = (Red 
+ Violet) + Green. 

If we define subtraction in terms of arithmetical quantity, as 
uniform, non-commutative, and non-associative, similar analogies 
can be observed. This, however, is of little importance; the 
justification of using the symbol of addition in an arithmetical — 
sense is sufficient for our purposes. 

We can now consider some experimental points. Since practi- 
cally all chromatic stimuli may be expressed in terms of three, 
researches can be planned in the following way. A definite 
spectrum, e.g. the prismatic spectrum of an Auer gas lamp, and 
three lights of arbitrary but constant intensity, e.g. a red, a green, 
and a violet, are selected; each part of the spectrum is then 
matched by a mixture of all the three or of two, or by only one 
of them. In this way, Koenig and Dieterici investigated normal 
colour vision (1°). Details of the results need not detain us; all I 
wish to emphasise here is the general conclusion that the experi- 
mental facts of colour stimulation can be graphically represented 
by a plane figure and the possible forms of stimuli reduced to 


392 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


terms of three independent variables; that is to say, from the 
point of view of stimulations, normal colour vision is trichromatic. 
It is important to notice that a choice of variables is, from the 
theoretical standpoint, arbitrary ; indeed, if a table be constructed 
in terms of three primaries, A’, B’, C’, a second can be deduced 
in terms of another three, A”, B’, C’, because, in view of what 
has been said, we can evidently define any one of the new 
variables, e.g. A”, in terms of the old ones by a linear relation of | 

the form 
A” =a.A’+b.B’+c.C’. 


The process, in fact, merely involves a change of the co-ordinate 
axes. Of course, if we choose the three primaries so close together 
that we cannot experimentally reproduce all the spectral stimulus - 
values, our table will involve negative directions, but this is of no 
theoretical importance. 

Summarising the points dealt with in the last few pages, we 
find that— 

(1) Fixing our attention on stimuli alone, mixing results are 
functions of two variables and graphically expressible by means of 
a plane figure. . 

(2) Colour differences can be expressed in terms of three chosen 
stimuli, the choice being theoretically unrestricted, but in practice 
certain real stimuli are selected for convenience. | 

I must reiterate here the fact that all these statements purport . 
to be merely descriptive and to resume experimental observations | 
as briefly as possible. Colour diagrams and the assertion that 
colour vision is trichromatic are only short ways of expressing 
experimental results; they contain no hidden theoretical assump- 

“tions, and their truth, or falsehood, is purely a matter of observa- 
tion and necessary inference from such observations. It is essential 
to separate the theory I propose to discuss from the data which 
have given birth to it. 


Sreotion II{.—ParriaL CoLoUR-BLINDNESS 


We have seen that, from the purely experimental point of 
view, the characteristics of normal colour vision admit of relatively 
simple arrangement, that in fact all the results of stimulation can 
be expressed in terms of three different stimuli and no more; we — 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 393 


must next consider some types of vision which, although abnormal, 
throw light upon the normal mechanism. These are the commoner 
forms of congenital partial colour-blindness. 

_ The existence of abnormal colour perception in certain people 
has been recognised for more than two centuries (4) ;} but John 
Dalton, the great chemist, was the first who attracted much 
attention to the subject (1). 


Goethe in his Farbenlehre, which appeared in 1812, gives the ¢.4\° 


following description : (1*)— 

“* We will here first advert to a very remarkable state in which 
the vision of many persons is found to be. As it presents a 
deviation from the ordinary mode of seeing colours, it might fairly 
be classed under morbid impressions ; but as it is consistent with 
itself, as it often occurs, may extend to many members of a family 
and probably does not admit of cure, we may consider it as bor- 
dering only on the nosological cases and therefore place it first. 

“T was acquainted with two individuals not more than twenty 
years of age, who were thus affected. . . . They agreed with the 
rest of the world in denominating white, black, and grey in the 
usual manner. . . . They appeared to see yellow, red-yellow, and 
yellow-red like others. . . . But now a striking difference presented 
itself. If the carmine were passed thinly over the white saucer, 
they would compare the light colour thus produced to the colour of 
the sky and call it blue. If-a rose were shown them beside it, 
they would in like manner call it blue; and in all the trials that 
were made, it appeared that they could not distinguish light blue 
from rose colour. ; . 

“They confounded rose colour, blue, and violet on all occa- 
sions; these colours only appeared to them to be distinguished 
from each other by delicate shades of lighter, darker, intenser, or 
fainter appearance. 

_ “ Again they could not distinguish green from dark orange, 
nor, more especially, from a red-brown. 

“Tf any one accidentally conversing with these individuals, 
happened to question them about surrounding objects, their answers 
occasioned the greatest perplexity, and the interrogator began to 
fancy his own wits were out of order. With some method we 
may, however, approach to a nearer knowledge of the law of this 
deviation from the general law. 

“ These persons, as may be gathered from what has been stated, 


394 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY ; 


saw fewer colours than other people : hence arose the confusion of 
different colours. . . .” 

Since the time of Goethe a great deal of attention has been 
devoted to the subject and its literature has attained to formidable 
proportions. I only propose to consider that aspect of the subject 
which seems to be of theoretical interest. 

The most obvious distinction between the vision of partial 
colour-blinds and our own is their inability to perceive differences 
which are plain to us. As Goethe says, they see fewer colours 
than we do. This difficulty is of special interest because it is 
something definite ; it is easy to find out whether a person can 
distinguish between the effect produced on him by two stimuli 
which certainly affect us differently, while what the actual nature 
of the effect is, remains unknown. How, e.g., a certain green light . 
affects a colour-blind eye, by what sensation it is followed, we can 
only guess ; but we know that the effect cannot be distinguished 
by the subject from that produced by a certain physical intensity 
of red light. 

We have thus to deal with a condition in which the conscious 
responses to varied physical stimuli are fewer than in normal 
persons; the question is whether experimental results can be 
summarised in the simple manner that was possible in the case of 
normal vision. It will be found that the results appear capable of 
even simpler description. 

We concluded that, for most experimental purposes, normal 
colour stimulation is expressible in terms of three colours; the 
vision of partial colour-blinds is expressible in terms of two only. 
If we choose as our fixed lights a red and a blue, these, mixed in 
suitable proportions, match any part of the spectrum, as the latter 
appears to the partially colour-blind, and a mixture of the two 
also matches unanalysed daylight. In the sense in which normal 
colour vision is\trichromatic, this form is dichromatic. 

But, just as in normal persons the actual proportions of the 
constituents in mixtures equal to various spectral lights do not 
agree completely, we also find different types of dichromatic vision, 
distinguished by their respective mixing ratios. The two classes 


1 The question of so-called abnormal trichromatic systems cannot be discussed 
here. Consult Koenig, Zeits. f. phys. und psych. d. Sinnes., vol. 4, p. 317. Donders, 
Arch. f. Anat. u. Phys. (Physiol. Abth.), 1884, p. 518. Vv. Kries, Zeits. 7. Psy. Ue 
Physiol. d. Sinnesorg., vol. 19, p. 63. - 


fet 


Proportions of Standard Red and Blue in a Mixture of apparently the same Tint as 
various Spectral Colours in four cases of Partial Colour Blindness (v. Kries). 


ng pee ad tig Deuteranope (1). | Deuteranope (2). | Protanope (1). Protanope (2). 
millionths of a 
millim. Red. Blue. Red. Blue, Red. Blue. Red. Blue. 
670°8 330 | ... 344). 53| ... 49 
656-0 Mey 1) BOE) 91) |. 84 
642-0 790 | ... 950. 19-0)... 18-0 
628-0 1070| ... |1260 ... 38:0 | ... 385 
615-0 1470| ... | 1380) ... 63:0) ... | 63-0 
603-0 1510} ... | 1550)... 900... 84-0 
591-0 1370] ... | 1440) ... | 1000! ... | 1050 
5810 1240| ... |1290 ... | 1110 |... | 1130 
5710 | 1030] ... | 1080| ... | 1200] ... | 126-0 
561-0 820 |... 890 ... | 1080. ... | 1060 
552-0 640 |... 650... (20)... | 1010 
544°0 520 |... 560... 78:0 | ... 85°0 
536-0 410| 63 | 374, 60 | 650] ... 67:5 
525°0 26:0 | 12:0 | 21:0 103 | 383) 110 | 468] ... 
515-0 15°0| 28:0 | 137 21:6 | 206| 340 | 328] 13-0 
505°0 7-7| 360 75 | 32:2 98/350 | 17:2] 29°0 
496°0 37 | 48-0 41 463 48| 470 | 84/ 33:0 
488-0 16 | 62:0 19 | 58-0 2:2 | 57:0 53 | 49-0 
480°0 0-9 | 64:0 0-9 | 67-0 09 | 660 | 29)| 71:0 
469°0 03 | 700 03 656 03 | 670 | 10) 69-0 
| 460°8 | 67:0 686 .. | 540 | ... | 660 
= Fic. 5 
z = 
150 
ro TINA 
35 JAmNE 
130} 7 ‘ 
a3 120 \ 
n0 
ny 
24 /) N 
. fi f ALK 
” H \ IN 
3 5 ‘a X 
=. / N 
Ss ~ 
= ~ & 
10 44 SSO = a= 
2 BET SS 
©12345678 IWN RB HS 6 11 819 2A 
— = SF a = 
PSSPHSSPR RESTS # = $2? 8 
Wave Lengths of Spectrum. 


\) 


396 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


Amounts of Red (Lithiwm Line) required to match a Standard Yellow 
(Sodium Line) by 11 Protanopes and 9 Deuteranopes (v. Kries). 


Protanopes. Deuteranopes. | 
| 214-0 | 8652 
213-0 36°3 ! 
211°0 36°31! 
205°0 36°5? 
1960 38°4 
198-0 373 | 
210°0 37:0 | 
| 200-0 37'0 
210°0 37'8 
203-0 37°0 | 
225°0 369 
| a 38:0 


Ratio is 5: 1 (Approx.). 


which have been most studied are known as Protanopes and 
Deuteranopes. Their special characters will be described briefly. 

Experimentally, we proceed as in the examination of normal 
colour vision. A certain red and a certain blue are chosen; the 
whole spectrum matched and expressed in terms of the standard 
lights. The tables and diagram (p. 395) embody the results 
obtained by J. v. Kries (#4). 

An examination of the curves shows that the cases fall into 
two groups in respect of red values. The four sets of blue values, 
on the other hand, are not very dissimilar ; in fact, remembering 
that the physical differences of macular absorption would be most 
influential in that region of the spectrum where the blue values 
agree worst, we may regard the similarity as fairly close. 

Referring to the tables, we see that for matching spectral 
lights of wave length greater than 530 wu no blue at all is 
necessary, so that the forms of the red curves beyond this point 
are especially instructive. In one group the curve rises fairly 
sharply to a maximum in the neighbourhood of 571 up, and then 
falls sharply, suggesting a relatively low stimulus value for long- 
waved light ; in the other group the rise is more gradual, attaining 
* a maximum at 603 wu, while the curve does not fall to so low 
a point at the red end. 

1 One subject. 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 397 


Hence, within the region under examination, we may say with 
fair accuracy, that the four subjects are grouped into a pair 
relatively more sensitive to short-waved light and a pair relatively 
more sensitive to long-waved light. The former are called prota- 
nopes, the latter deuteranopes. 

But we have thus far only examined four cases, and must see 
if the results can be substantiated with the help of a larger number 
of subjects. If the conclusions just stated be valid, in matching 
yellow some partial colour-blinds will require enormously more 
red than others (protanopes) ; and this experiment, which can be 
readily performed, will test our conclusions. V. Kries made the 
test on twenty cases, using red (lithium line) and a fixed yellow 
(sodium line). As will be seen from the table (p. 396) the subjects 
do in fact fall into groups, one set (protanopes) requiring approxi- 
mately five times as much red as the other class (deuteranopes) 
to match the standard yellow. The classification is therefore a 
tolerably definite one. 

This relative insensibility to long-waved light explains the 
observation that in protanopes—who correspond to the class badly 
named “Red Blinds”—the red end of the spectrum appears 
shortened. The shortening is, however, of little interest from our 
present standpoint. 

It is to be remarked that individual differences occur within 
each class. In the two protanopic cases, from 552 uu onwards 
to violet, one subject constantly demanded more red in his mixture 
than the other. The subject requiring less red may be presumed 
to have a deeply pigmented macula, so that thé homogeneous light 
was weakened by selective absorption, because the diminution in 
red values agrees with the known increments of absorption as we 
pass towards the violet. The difference is of some importance in 
connection with an interesting peculiarity of dichromatic vision. 
Since daylight can be matched, for the dichromatic eye, by a 
mixture of red and blue, and since all spectral colours can be 
matched by mixing these standards, we should expect some spectral 
colour to match more or less closely unanalysed daylight. . The 
situation of such a colour is called the “ Neutral Point” of the 
spectrum. As for protanopes the stimulus value of lights falls off 
rapidly towards the red, their neutral point should be nearer the 
violet than that of deuteranopes, and this is generally the case. 
But, if there is a good deal of macular pigmentation, the mixed 


398 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 
light undergoes selective absorption, and the homogeneous match 
is nearer the red end. Thus the typical difference may be oblite- 
rated and a simple determination of the neutral point would not 
enable us to distinguish the two forms. 

A study of the neutral point, however, immediately brings to 
our notice that characteristic which has attracted the attention of 
practical men. Spectral light between 490 and 500 uu induces 
normally the sensation green; for the dichromatic it has the 
same effect as unanalysed daylight or a mixture of red and blue. 
Now this mixture contains much red and little blue, so that a 7 
normal person asked to name the simple colour and the mixed . 
colour which look alike to the dichromatic would probably call 
them green and red; so that we can say that both protanopes 
and deuteranopes confound red with green. Even here we observe 
a class difference. In matching a given bluish-green, the prota- 
nope, being relative insensitive to long waves, requires much red 
in the red-blue mixture; the deuteranope takes about the same 
amount of blue but much less red. Accordingly, a protanope will 
match a light bluish red (physically speaking) with a green that 
appears to us much darker, eg. a scarlet with olive green; a 
deuteranope matches a far bluer red with a green which we should 
take to be about equally bright. Hence, although both groups 
confuse certain reds with certain greens, the matches of one class 
are not usually valid for the other. 

These then being the chief characteristics of the two common 
forms of colour-blindness, we must endeavour to ascertain what 
relations their visual systems bear to those of normal persons. 
The mere fact that the systems are dichromatic would tell us com- 
paratively little; we should indeed conclude that, owing to the 
reduction of different stimulus forms, sensations of colour are less 
numerous for dichromatics than for ourselves, but they might 
be quite different, and what we seek is a relation between 
stimuli. . 

As early as 1837, Seebeck expressed the opinion that two lights 
or mixtures of lights which appeared equal to the normal eye, 
never appeared unequal when examined by a partial colour-blind. 
If this be true, it is of importance, because it would show that 
these people lack something possessed by a normal person, but — 
have nothing, no stimulus reaction, which is not shared by the 
normal eye; in other words, colour-blindness is purely an error of 


nt 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 399 


defect not excess. The validity of normal equations for the two 
groups of partial colour-blindness has been recently tested by 
v. Kries. His conclusions are clearly expressed in the following 

“One employs the frequently cited equations between a homo- 
geneous yellow and a mixture of red and yellowish-green (670°8 and 
550 uu). As all lights in this region are of equal stimulus value 
for the colour-blind, whatever be the ratio of red : yellowish-green, 
one can always give the homogeneous yellow an intensity such 
that the match is good either for a protanope or a deuteranope, 
but in general the matches of the one are not valid for the other. 
_ As we should expect, a strongly red mixture is for the protanope 
equivalent to a yellow of relatively feeble intensity ; a deuteranope 
finds in a match arranged by the protanope, the mixture too 
bright and the pure yellow too dark. The relation is reversed for 
strongly green mixtures. With extraordinary accuracy, however, 
we find that for the ratio of red : yellowish-green that has for the 
trichromatic an equal colour tone with the homogeneous yellow, 
both groups agree; trichromatic equations are valid for both 
protanopes and deuteranopes. Conversely, if we try to find an 
equation valid for both groups, we arrive precisely at the one 
valid for a normal person.” } 

Additional evidence is afforded by the following considerations. 
Suppose we prepare a table giving the mixing ratios for a normal 
person in matching spectral colours between 670°8 wu and 550 uu. 
The results are as follows :— 


Spectral Region. Proportions of Standard Colours. 
Wave Length. 670°8 pp. 552 wp. 
670'8 88°5 oe 
628-0 251-0 10 
6150 276°0 27 
603°0 270°0 49 
591-0 | 202-0 67 
581-0 123°0 76 
571-0 | 73°0 91 
561-0 21-0 80 
552-0 | aay 71 


1 Nagel’s Handb., vol. iii. p. 160. 


er'er 
* 


400 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY — 


We also find that a certain deuteranope requires 33 units of the 
standard 670°8 to match the spectral 670°8, and 64 units of the 
same standard to match the spectral 552. 

If deuteranopic vision be merely a reduction form, we ought to 
be able to calculate from these data the stimulus values of the 
intervening lights. Take as an example 591, which is matched 
by 202 units of 670°8 plus 67 units of 552. We must change from 
the arbitrary units of the normal system by dividing by 88°5 
and 71 respectively, and then express in the deuteranope’s intensity 
system by multiplying by 33 and 64. The stimulus value should 
be therefore : 202 x 33 + 88:5 + 67 x 64 -- 71 = 135 (approx.) units. 

This, then, is the stimulus value of 591 wm expressed in terms 
of the intensity values of 670°8 and 552 wu on the hypothesis 
that a match good for a trichromatic eye is valid for a dichromatic. 
The observed value was 137. In this way the following table was 
obtained : (!5)— 


Stimulus Values for Deuteranope and Protanope. 


Deuteranope. | Protanope. 
Wave — ee eee ee ere eee 
Length. 
Calculated. Observed. Calculated. Observed. 
| 
670 83 33 | 4:9 49 
628 106 107 | 28'8 38°5 
615 126 145 | 54°2 63°0 
603 145 151 86:0 84:0 
591 135 137 108-0 105°0 
581 114 124 | 117°0 113-0 
571 110 103 137°0, 126°0 
561 76 82 | 1110 106°0 
552 64 64 | 101°0 1010 
| 


A further, but less satisfactory test is afforded by the con- 
struction of a colour diagram or “ triangle” combining observa- 
tions made upon the two classes of colour-blinds. As this would 
involve the discussion of some rather technical details, and involves 
certain assumptions of questionable validity, it will not be further 
considered in this essay.* 

1 See the memoir by v. Kries citel in the last footnote, also Nagel’s Handb., — 
vol. iii, p. 162, and especially for the theory of a dichromatic ‘‘Fehlpunkt,” — 
Helmholtz, op. cit., pp. 363, &c. I am deeply indebted to Professor v. Kries for 
information respecting his work on dichromatic systems. 


\ . 


b 
| 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 401 


Summing up this rather difficult piece of work, I think the 
reader will agree that :— 

(1) The two commoner forms of partial colour-blindness are 
distinguished one from another by a different responsiveness to 
stimulation by long and medium wave-lengthened light. Protanopes 
are relatively insensitive to long waves, deuteranopes to moderately 
long waves. 

(2) Each is an example of dichromatic vision, using the term 
in a purely experimental sense. 

(3) Each may be regarded as, to some extent, a reduction form 
of normal vision, although, as we shall see later, this conclusion 
cannot be pushed very far. 

The forms of partial colour-blindness just described are of 
everyday occurrence, and their recognition of obvious practical 
importance ;' another type less common, and therefore less com- 
pletely studied, is that known as blue or blue-yellow blindness. 
This condition, unlike the last two, is not as a rule congenital, 
usually one-sided, and associated with definite pathological changes 
in the retina; it affects, generally, only a portion of the visual 
field. Koenig’s observations make it probable that this type also 
is dichromatic ;* two suitably chosen lights will match the entire 


1 For the practical tests employed in the detection of colour-blindness, the 
reader must consult the special treatises. It may be remarked that considerable 
difference of opinion exists among ophthalmologists as to the reliability of the test 
in common use. 

2 For the reader’s convenience, I give a list of the chief papers dealing with 
blue-yellow blindness. : 

(1) J. Stilling, Beitrige : z. Lehre vy. den Farbenempfindungen Klinische 
Monatsbl. f. Augenheilk. Jahrg. 13 (1875), 2nd Supp. p. 41, Jahrg. 14, 3rd Supp. 
p- 1. Stuttgart, 1875-6. 

(2) Cohn, Studien tiber angeborene Farbenblindheit, Breslau, 1879, pp. 139-148 
(five cases with good bibliography). 

(8) Donders, Annales d’Oculistique, 12th series, vol. 4, 1880, p. 212. 

(4) Holmgrén, Centralbl. f. Augenheil., 5th Jahrgang, 1881, p. 476. 

(5) Hermann, Ein Beitrag z. Casuistik d. Farbenblindheit. Inaugural Disserta- 
tion, Dorpat, 1882. (Not seen.) 

(6) V. Vintschgau, Pfliger’s Arch., vol. 48, p. 431. (A full account, without 
theoretical bias, of a case in which no pathological changes were observed and a 
precis of the observations contained in 1-5.) 

(7) V. Vintschgau, Pfl. Arch., vol. 57, p. 191. (Continuation of (6).) 

(8) Hering, Pfl. Arch., vol. 57, p. 308. (A study of v. Vintschgau’s case, which 
Hering regards as yellow-blue blindness combined with a weak red-green sense.) 

(9) Wundt's Philosophische Studien, vol. 8; p. 173, 1892. (Not seen.) 

(10) Koenig, Ueber Blaublindheit, Sitzungsbericht. d. Preuss. Akad. d. 
Wissenschaft. in Berlin, 29th Juli, 1897, xxxiv. p. 718 (Pathological). 

= . 2c 


402 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


spectrum. The neutral point is in the yellow, between 566 and 
570 up. 

At the risk of being tedious, I must emphasise the fact that the 
truth or falsehood of the foregoing account depends entirely upon 
the trustworthiness of the various observations. None of the 
statements rely on any hypothesis as to the mode of action of | 
the visual elements. . 


Section IV.—AFTER-IMAGES : ‘+ 


Considerations of space make it impossible to discuss, even 
in outline, the numerous interesting observations which have 
been made upon visual after-images. I will merely, for the | 
purpose of reminding the reader of facts with which he’ 
is doubtless familiar, tabulate the main results of modern . 
work. | 

(1) The distinction between “ positive ” and “ negative ” after- 
images is not absolute but relative, depending on the nature of the 
reacting stimulus. 

(2) An image-producing or “retuning” stimulus changes the 
stimulus value of a “reacting” (subsequently applied) light, but 
only in such a way that the sensation response following exposure 
to the “reacting” light is increased or diminished quantitatively. 
Colour equations do not lose their validity. 

(3) The latter statement is true in the case of foveal vision, but 

not for peripheral stimulation. 
| (4) We do not know the time relations of the retuning process, 
nor whether it proceeds uniformly or in pulses.} 


ee 


(11) Piper, Zeitsch. f. Psy. u. Phys. d. Sinnesorg., vol. 38, p. 155, 1905 (Patho- 
logical). 

(12) Levy, Arch. f. Opthalmologie, vol. 62, 1906, p. 464. 

(13) Collins und Nagel, Zeitsc. f. Psy. u. Phys. d. Sinnesorg., vol. 41, 2nd part, 
p« 74, 1906 (Pathological). 

(14) Schenck, Pfl. Arch., vol. 118, p. 161, 1907. (This memoir is mainly theo- 
retical ; it describes a case, okey er.) 

1 For full information, in addition to the treatises of Helmholtz and v. Kries, _ 
already frequently cited, see v. Tschermak, Uber das Verhéltnis von Gegenfarbe, 
Kompensationsfarbe und Kontrastfarbe, Pfl. Arch., 1907, vol. 117, p. 473. G. J. 
Burch, Proc. Roy. Soc. (1900), vol. 66, p. 204. Some amusing observations by 
Goethe will be found in his Furbenlehre (Eastlake’s translation, p. 953 


\ 


- 
, 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 403 


Secrion V.—THe Turee-CoLtour Hyporuesis or YounG 
AS Mopiriep By HELMHOLTz 


As we saw, in an earlier section, Newton’s researches demon- 
strating the (conceptually) complex nature of white light and the 
physical substratum of chromatic stimuli—as co-ordinated by the 
undulatory hypothesis—enable us to frame a coherent, if not 
necessarily final account of the physical elements involved in 
retinal stimulation. On the other hand, the specificity of physio- 
logical response, which finds a not quite accurate expression in 


‘Miiller’s law, releases us from some of the difficulties attendant 


upon the primitive theories of vision, the propounders of which 
were hampered by their acceptance of the dogma of physico- 
physiological identity. 

On so broad a basis, it is of course possible to erect 
many, more or less, substantial edifices. In a famous passage, 
R. L. Stevenson relates that he once overheard two persons 
arguing. ‘What I advance,” said one, “is true.” ‘ Yes,” 
replied the other, “but not the whole truth.” “Sir,” was the 
retort, “there is no such thing as the whole truth.” It is well 
to bear that saying in mind when one considers the theories of 
colour vision. 

In attempting to arrive at'an adequate interpretation of any 
experimental facts, more than one route is generally open. As a 
rule, it does not much matter which path we choose so long as we 
keep count of any assumptions made and avoid the introduction 
of unnecessary steps. Let us start, then, from the experimental 
laws of colour mixing, and see where we arrive by following the 
most obvious route. We saw that, for most experimental pur- 


_ poses, we could say that a sensation “produced by” a colour 


stimulus could be matched by a sensation due to a stimulus obtained 
by mixing not more than three lights. We saw that these three 
lights did not coincide accurately with any spectral colours, but 
that, if we admitted negative values into our colour equations, our 


_vision could be regarded as definitely trichromatic, even in terms 


of known stimuli. In order, however, to avoid this, let us so 
choose our stimuli that only positive values of each are employed. 
This means that they must be so chosen that the colour table is 


404 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


circumscribed by the lines joining their representatiye points in a 
plane, e.g. :— 


g' 


Red 


Fic. 6. 


This is our first assumption, and is little more than a generalisa- ' 
tion of experimental facts. 

Thus far, we have merely asserted that any stimulus, R’, say, 
can be expressed by the equation R’=2.R+y.G+z.V, where 
x, y, z are positive real quantities. But, our only measure of stimu- 
lation equality being corresponding sensation equality, we imply 
(and this is our second and most important assumption) that there 
is a definite relationship between the physiological excitatory pro- 
cesses which lead up, somehow, to sensations, and the stimulus 
magnitudes. 

This assumption is justified if we can show that it leads to the 
formulation of a satisfactory working hypothesis, and if we assume 
the simplest relationship consistent with the experimental facts. 
What, then, is the simplest relation we can suppose to subsist 
between the stimulatory and excitatory processes? Clearly that, 
‘just as stimuli may be reduced to terms of three independent 
variables, excitatory processes’ are represented by three inde- 
pendent variables. Thus, taking our previous example, any 
stimulus, R’=2.R +y.G +z.V, then A=f, (2, y, z), B=f, (a, y, 2), 
C=f, (x, y, z), and conversely, =F, (A, B, C), y=F, (A, B, C), 
z=F, (A, B, C). 

These latter expressions may be taken as “elements” or unit 
excitatory processes, or any linear functions of them may be so 
taken.! 

1 The proof of this statement, although simple, requires some knowledge ot 


analytical notation, and cannot therefore be reproduced here. See Helmholtz, 
Handb. d. Phys. Opt., 2nd edition, pp. 342-3. 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 405 


The conception is merely that three independent physiological] 
processes exist, each of which is defined by an equation connecting 
it with the three independent stimulus values which, as we have 
seen, determine the effect of any given stimulus. 

This statement represents the fundamental part of the hypo- 
thesis, first sketched by Thomas Young, and elaborated by 
Helmholtz. It is important to distinguish the essentials of the 
theory from its subsidiary parts. 

The effect produced by any chromatic stimulus is supposed to 
depend upon the resultant changes set up in three independent 
“* substances,” nothing whatever being postulated with respect to 
these substances except that the magnitude of change in each is a 
function of, i.e. depends on, the proportions of three independently 
variable stimuli in terms of which the given stimulus can be 
expressed. Conversely, any given stimulus value is a function 
of the independent activities of three visual “substances.” The 
nature of the substances, also the exact relation between them and 
the stimuli, are left open. For convenience of illustration, Young 
and Helmholtz assumed that the activity of each substance was 
associated with a single colour sensation, and chose red, green, and 
violet as “elementary sensations” from the present standpoint. 
“Substance ” A, when stimulated, was supposed to give rise to the 
sensation of red ; B, under similar conditions, to that of green ; and 
C to that of violet ; in this way the well-known “ valency curves ” 
of the text-books were obtained. The advantage of this method 
is that the theory seems more definite, but the disadvantage is 
entailed that if the illustration proves irreconcilable with facts of 
experiment, the reader omits to notice that what is found wanting 
is merely an illustration, not the basal theory. 

It is especially important to remember that the colour equations 
discussed in Section II. do not pretend to describe any direct 
relationship between the hypothetical “ substances” and stimulus 
magnitudes. . 

In precisely the same way, for the sake of illustration, Helm- 
holtz suggested the existence somewhere in the retino-cerébral 
apparatus of three sets of fibres, each corresponding to one of the 
hypothetical substances. 

This suggestion was less happy because its utility was not so 
great as, and the chance of misunderstanding greater than, in the 
previous case. The red, green, and violet fibres have reigned long 


406 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


and ingloriously over the student’s imagination: it is time to 
remember that they are pure abstractions, no more essential to 
the theory just sketched than the employment in algebra of the 
letter x to denote an unknown quantity is an essential procedure 
in that science. To avoid this source of misunderstanding, I shall 
use Prof. v. Kries’ term, Components, and speak of the theory as 
the “ Three Components Hypothesis.” 

Another mistake is illustrated by the following quotation from 
a paper by Miss Calkins : (*)— 

“From the point of view of a psychological analysis of our 
conscious sensations of colour, the postulates of this theory are 
not in accordance with the facts of observation ; for, even granting 
that violet is a simple fundamental colour sensation (which many 
observers regard as complex), it can hardly be denied that yellow . 
is just as well characterised and definite a sensation as red or 
green. Yellow looks yellow, and does not seem at all like a mixture 
of red and green, or indeed any other colour mixture.” 

Objections of this type are simply irrelevant. The theory isonly — 
an attempt to express physiological processes, of admittedly hypo- 
thetical character, in terms of experimental facts ;1 it has nothing. 
to do with a psychological analysis of sensations. Whether such 
analysis be possible may be doubted ; in any case it is and can be 
no part of our present inquiry, which is only concerned with visual 
sensations in so far as the latter are signs of the existence of a 
physiological reaction. 

Having reduced the hypothesis to its simplest terms, let us 
apply it to the facts reviewed in the former sections of this article. 
Abnormal trichromatic systems are satisfactorily described if we 
suppose one of the visual components to be defined in a peculiar 
way. Thus, if a component A is normally defined as f, (2, y, 2), 
in these cases it is some other function, /, (x, y, 2), say, of the 
stimulus values. 

Turning to dichromatic systems, we have seen that, from the 
experimental standpoint, they are reduction forms of a normal 
system. ‘Theoretically considered, the simplest reduction we could 
imagine would be the absence or ineffectiveness of one of the three 
normal components. Thus, if we take R, G, V (for the sake of 
clearness) as the normal components, in the absence of R. all 
sensations (sensation being used with the meaning above defined) — 


1 i.e. in terms of stimulus values. 
o 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 407 


are functions of G and V, a condition approximating to that of 
protanopia. If Gis absent, we get a form resembling deuteranopia. 
Under such conditions, not only would the relation between 
dichromatics and trichromatics be easily intelligible, but further 
we could determine, from observations on dichromatics, the com-. 
ponents of a normal system.1 A stimulus inoperative upon a 
dichromatic eye must act exclusively upon the missing component. 
We have learned how to find the situation of such a stimulus in 
the colour diagram (the ‘‘ Fehlpunkt”’), and the two such points 
obtained for the two systems of dichromatic vision determine the 
stimulus relations of two visual components of a trichromatic eye. 
All this depends, however, on the assumption that one com- 


ponent is absent and everything else unchanged in a dichromatic 


eye. Modern work renders it doubtful whether we may make 
this assumption. In 1885, when the second edition of his great 
treatise was being prepared, Helmholtz wrote as follows to Lord 
Rayleigh : (17) “I have never doubted that our colour system de- 
pended on three variables and no more. In regard to colour- 
blindness, the recent observations of Donders and of my assistant 
Dr. A. Koenig, show that this defect cannot be referred simply to 


‘the lack of one of the fundamental colours, but that two of the 


primaries (red and green) appear to acquire a more even distribu- 
bution in the spectrum, so that now one and now the other makes 
a more vigorous impression; in other words, the resulting curve 
approximates now more to the red and now to the normal green 
sensation. In addition to this we have every shade of lessened 
power of discrimination. Consequently different individuals require — 
very different mixtures of lithium and thallium light in order to 
make up sodium light.” 

But if we cannot regard dichromatic vision as differing from 
the normal merely in the absence of a component, we can, in 
terms of the fundamental hypothesis, assert that it depends upon 
a visual system made up of two variables defined by such expres- 
sions as & = dp, (A, B, C), and y=, (A ®, C). 

This way of looking at the matter is consistent and logical, but 
it is not free from objection. Thus we could not deduce from such 
an expression any definite statements respecting the components 
of normal systems; the interpretation of dichromatic systems is, 
necessarily, expressed in such highly general terms as to be hardly 
1 Or rather the stimuli which act exclusively upon such components. 


408 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


capable of direct verification. To put the matter in a nut-shell, 
the older conception of partial colour-blindness as due to the 
absence of a normal component is simple and, if true, practical, 
but is not, in fact, quite adequate; the more general form is 
adequate, but not very helpful. 

Next, what has the Component Hypothesis to say with regard 
to the phenomena of after-images ? 

A noteworthy feature of after-image experiments is, of course, 
that stimulation with a given light increases responsiveness to its 
complementary. It would appear, therefore, easy to imagine that 
activity of the three components, or any one or two of them, in 
a certain way diminishes their responsiveness in one direction, 
increasing it in an opposite direction. This amounts to supposing 
that we have a condition comparable with the state of the reflex _ 
arcs, so brilliantly described by Sherrington: the nervous path is 
occupied by one form of motor discharge, and this very occupancy 
paves the way for a discharge different, and even opposite in kind. 

To so highly general a statement as this, no objection will be 
found, but if we investigate details, difficulties arise. For instance, 
the apparent saturation of spectral colours is greatly enhanced by 
previously stimulating the eye with their complementaries. Helm- 
holtz accordingly supposed that all the spectral colours stimulate 
each visual component. But, if this be true, the simpler inter- 
pretation of colour-blindness once more fails. Observations of 
dichromatics suggest that lights having wave-lengths greater than 
550 wu do not affect the third component (the “blue” or 
“violet” component) at all, because no standard blue had to be 
mixed with the standard red in-order to effect a good match. To 
a normal eye, however, the saturation of spectral yellow (589 um) 
18 unquestionably enhanced by previous exposure to blue. Either 
spectral colours do not affect all three components, in which case 
the theory does not cover after-image effects, or the simpler 
explanation of partial colour-blindness must be abandoned. In 
view of what has already been said, the reader will perhaps agree 
that the second alternative is the more plausible, and conclude that 
after-images are adequately described at the cost of strengthening 
our suspicion that dichromatic and trichromatic systems cannot be 
co-ordinated in any simple manner. 

So far we have found that the Hypothesis of Three Components 
describes with sufficient clearness the facts of normal colour vision, — 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 409 


including the phenomena of after-images; that it also describes 
the facts of partial colour-blindness adequately enough, but in 
very general terms, the earlier direct explanation being insufficient. 
We have now to see whether any experimental evidence can be 
found pointing to the existence of independent visual components 
satisfying the above conditions, or conceivably capable of satis- 
fying them. 

Evidence of this kind is furnished by the experimental pro- 
duction of a peculiar form of colour-blindness by G. J. Burch. (!%) 
This observer exposed his eye to direct sunlight in the focus of 
a two-inch lens behind coloured glasses. A gelatine film stained 


with magenta and combined with a medium ruby glass was found 


to transmit a fairly pure red, three thicknesses of green glass 
were used for green, and a tank of cupric ammonia-sulphate for 
violet. Similar arrangements were made for the other hues, and 
in some experiments a large spectroscope was employed. Two 
minutes’ exposure was sufficient to produce the maximal effect in 
the case of red. 

After exposure to red light, the following effects were noted. 
Scarlet geraniums appeared black, calceolarias and sunflowers green, 
purple flowers, such as clematis, violet. Pink roses were sky blue. 
Fatiguing with violet light caused objects reflecting violet light 
to appear black, purples and reds seemed crimson. Green stimu- 
lation made the foliage appear reddish or bluish-grey. But, after 
these exposures, “the colour by which the eye has been dazzled, 
and to which it is now blind, tints all those objects which naturally 
reflect none of this.” The truth of this is illustrated by a simple 
experiment. (!*) Suppose the eye to be somewhat fatigued by green, 
as during a long country walk, if under these circumstances the 
eye be directed to a small red spot on a black surface, e.g. a 
geranium petal on the black cover of a book, and one walks 
quickly with it into a dark shed or barn, the colour of the petal 
changes from red through orange and yellow, becoming eventually 
perhaps whitish. On coming into the light again the red re- 
appears. 

These experiments suggest that stimulation with red, green, 
and violet alters responsiveness with respect to these stimuli alone, 
and the same is found in the case of blue. Orange stimulation, 
on the other hand, affects not only the appearance of the orange 
but that of the red and green as well. Both positive and negative 


410 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


effects pass off rapidly in the case of artificial red blindness (in 
ten minutes), more slowly after violet fatigue (in two hours). 

It is, 1 think, obvious that the state of affairs presented by 
these experiments is highly complex. We are dealing with a 
change of responsiveness, analogous to the retuning effects, as they 
are called, produced in the study of after-images, but of greater 
magnitude, and in less simple form. Take the experiment quoted 
as to the apparent hue of a geranium petal after green stimula- 
tion: this is an ordinary after-image effect, and differs in no way 
from the results obtained by other workers; in the case of 
exposure to stronger green light, the effect is similar. How does 
this, we may ask, differ from the experiment with intense orange 
light 2 Simply in the fact that “retuning” with orange, red 
and green both act like orange itself in inducing a negative image. — 
The conclusion that in this case the mechanism involved in the 
production of orange is compounded of a mechanism yielding a 
sensation of redness and a mechanism responding with a sensation 
of greenness is reasonable, and finds some confirmation in a recent ~ 
experiment carried out by Burch. (?°) 

We know that responsiveness to green is increased, relatively 
to that for red stimulation, by resting the eye in darkness ; hence, 
if orange or yellow depends upon a fusion of two physiological 
processes, one concerned with green, the other with red, then, 
under conditions of feeble illumination and dark adaptation, the 
yellow should appear greenish, because the mechanism responding 
by a sensation of greenness, is more active under these conditions 
than that associated in the same way with redness.* 

Burch found this actually to be the case ; “the sodium lines 
appeared pale green when of the minimum visible intensity.” 

These results do therefore support a contention that com- 
ponents in the sense of our theory may possibly have a physio- 
logical counterpart. I do not think, however, that the view that four 
components—a red, a green, a violet, and a blue—exist is proved 
by Burch’s experiments. To prove that any light acts upon only 
one component it would be necessary to show that after dazzling 
with, e.g., blue, any mixed light was altered by the subtraction 
of blue, and that any light not containing blue had that colour 
added to it. The facts that the condition is transitory, is perhaps 


1 The reader must forgive the apparent confusion between object and subject of 
vision which is rendered inevitable by the necessity of being concise.’ 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 4il 


attended with some risk, and almost certainly involves psychological 
complications, render exact observations difficult. Under the 
circumstances, it is perhaps best to say that, although the experi- 
ments are perfectly consistent with a component hypothesis of the 
type discussed, it would be rash, on the strength of them, to make 
any general statement as to the nature of the components from 
the physiological standpoint. Theoretically it does not matter 
whether we adopt three or four components ; the algebraical form 
of our theory would not be changed, but we should lose the 
practical advantages of considering normal colour vision to be, 
experimentally, trichromatic, which would be a serious objection. 

Before finally summarising the case presented, two matters 


“need attention. First, as to monochromatic vision or total colour- 


blindness. It has been asserted that such a condition cannot be 
described in terms of the Young-Helmholtz theory. As a matter 
of fact, the assertion is inaccurate ; symbolically we could cover 
the facts by supposing that the functions defining the variables 
are identical thus: A=f(z, y, z)=B=f(g, y, z)=C=f(z, y, 2), 
or, graphically, we can put it that the three valency curves coincide. 
In any case, the reader will probably see reason to think that 
monochromatic vision depends upon a mechanism entirely distinct 
from the precursors of normal foveal vision, and its treatment 
should be kept separate from that of the phenomena with which 
we are here concerned. 

In the second place, no aid has been made to Simul- 
taneous Contrast. The reason is that it seems doubtful whether 
the phenomena of simultaneous contrast are not of quite a special — 
kind. It is true that the original hypothesis of Helmholtz, which 
assumed that contrastive effects are dependent upon factors, purely 
psychological in nature, can hardly be maintained without some 
modification ; the subject is, however, so complex that its dis- 
cussion would not be intelligible in the space at our disposal. It 
must, however, be said that if subsequent work should compel us 
to assign a purely physiological basis to the facts of simultaneous 
contrast, it will probably be necessary to modify the theory of 
components in such a way that it will become somewhat more 
complicated than it is at present. 

Leaving this matter for future consideration, we can say that 
the Component Hypothesis associated with the names of Young 
and Helmholtz supposes—(1) That colour sensations depend upon 


‘ 


i 
412 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY : 


the activity of three independent physiological substances of un- 
known nature and situation. (2) That the relationship between 
these components and the complex of stimuli is expressible 
quantitatively by saying that the responsiveness of each component 
is measured by a real linear function of three standard stimuli. 
(3) The results of stimulating these components are unit sensa- 
tions in a purely physiological sense, not units of consciousness. 
(4) No spectral light acts upon only one hypothetical component. 

The theory describes with sufficient accuracy the main facts, 
and there is some direct experimental evidence—that of Burch— 
which is consistent with its truth. Further, the main objection 
to the hypothesis in its modern form is its highly general nature 
and want of direct applicability to the immediate objects of 
physiological and physical research. How far this is a real . 
objection may be a matter of discussion, but it at least in- 
clines one to examine those theories which are, in the colloquial 
phrase, less up in the clouds. Such an examination will now 
occupy us. 


Section VI.—HeErtina’s THEORY OF VISUAL SENSATIONS 


In the preceding section, I attempted to trace out the theo- 
retical consequences which it seemed possible to reconcile with 
the experimental facts of colour mixing. As I have pointed out, 
with wearisome frequency, perhaps, the whole process depended 
upon the observation that, in general, the effect of a given stimulus 
or of a combination of stimuli was constant, so that we might 
attribute to the stimulus a causal value. In other words, we have 
regarded the sensations as the signs or differentie of stimulation 
processes, and our theory, therefore, was in that sense not a theory 
of visual sensations at all but a theory of visual stimuli. 

If the estimate I formed of the value of this process be at all 
just, it would follow that its weakness lay rather in what was 
left unsaid than in any incorrectness or improbability in its positive — 
teaching. An objector, let us say the hypothetical man in the 
street, might perhaps express his criticism in the following terms : 
“You compel me,” he might say, “to examine a great many facts, 
- and force me to puzzle out some difficult quantitative reasoning, and 
at the end you leave me with a few highly general theorems which, 
you confess, only describe some of the phenomena. Whenever I — 


° 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 413 


clutched at a straw of definite specific statement, that straw was 
instantly snatched away again. Is it worth while going through 
so much for so little ? ” 

- I do not think such an objection at all unreasonable. It is 
good to be as simple as possible, provided we do not sacrifice truth 
in the process, and we find that deductions from stimulus values 
do not lead us to any very simple results. Let us see, therefore, 
what fortune attends us when we pursue our quest by a different 
path altogether; perhaps in this way we shall attain to some- 
thing more intelligible and practical. 

The other line of investigation practically resumes the problem 
-at the point at which the Greeks left it, and dates from the 
publication in 1810 of Goethe’s Farbenlehre. 

The comparatively slight influence that has been exercised by 
this work upon the development of modern physiological thought 
respecting the nature of visual processes, is due to causes well 
worth notice. The physical analysis of white light into mono- 
chromatic constituents by Newton had naturally attracted the 
chief, almost the exclusive, attention of those who occupied them- 
selves with the study of colour vision. Goethe, however, saw 
quite clearly that the difficulties of the problem were not to be 
overcome by vague references to physical experiments. He saw 
that the problem was one of sensations, and he therefore approached 
it from a sensational standpoint. Had he contented himself 
with this, with an analysis of sensations of colour, his work must 
have had an enormous influence; but he went further. The 
prevailing tendency to over-estimate the significance of the physi- 
cal side of the problem in visual theories led Goethe into the 
opposite error. He sustained the thesis that the Newtonian 
analysis was physically incorrect, and that the alleged physical 
analysis of white light was not in general possible. Consequently, 
much of his work is devoted to an attack upon the Newtonian 
hypothesis from the physical side. Since this attack failed, 
more valuable portions of Goethe’s book became involved in the 
discredit this produced. 

I can best give an idea cf the valuable parts by quoting a few 
significant passages. 

“With regard to the German terminology, it has the advan- 
tage of possessing four monosyllabic names no longer to be traced 
to their origin, viz. yellow (Gelb), blue, red, green. They represent 


414 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


the most general idea of colour to the imagination, without 
reference to any very specific modification. If we were to add 
two other qualifying terms to each of these four, as thus—red- 
yellow and yellow-red, red-blue and blue-red, yellow-green and 
green-yellow, blue-green and green-blue, we should express the 
gradations of the chromatic circle with sufficient distinctness ; and 
if we were to add the designations of light and dark, and again 
define, in some measure, the degree of purity or its opposite by 
the monosyllables black, white, grey, brown, we should have a 
tolerably sufficient range of expressions to describe the ordinary 
appearances presented to us, without troubling ourselves whether 
they were produced dynamically or atomically.” (?*) 

“Considered in a general point of view, colour is determined 
towards one of two sides. It thus presents a contrast which we . 
call a polarity, and which we may fitly designate by the expres- — 
sions plus and minus. 


Plus. Minus. 
“ Yellow. ; Blue. 
Action. Negation. 
Light. Shadow. 
Brightness. Darkness. 
Force. Weakness. 
Warmth. Coldness. 
Proximity. Distance. 
Repulsion. Attraction. 
Affinity with Acids. Affinity with Alkalis.” (2*) 


We see here formulated the conception of certain colour sensa- 
tions as occupying unique places in our sensational field, and 
presenting also, as it were, a species of sensational contrast one 
with the other. 

Not only is this conception present in the minds of those 
who seek to influence us through the colour sense (painters), but 
it is given effect to in their practice. | 

In a dialogue on colours by Ludovico Dolce, published in 1565, 
the following passage occurs : (7%) “He who wishes to combine 
colours that are agreeable to the eye, will put grey next dusky 
orange, yellow-green next rose colour, blue next orange, dark 
purple, black, next dark green, white next black, and white next 
flesh colour.” | . . 

Titian, according to his biographer Ridolfi, was fond of opposing — 


Ld 


“a vil: . —— 


a 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 415 
red and blue to his flesh tints, and Rubens contrasted a bright 


_ red with his “ still cooler flesh colour” (Eastlake). 


The point to mark is the general agreement that certain of our 
sensations of colour are really singled out from the whole group 
as presenting sharply-defined, special characters. All sensational 
theories are primarily concerned with the definition of these char- 
acters, and secondarily with an attempt to describe the data in 
terms of a physiological hypothesis. 

Of such attempts the views developed by Professor Ewald 
Hering and his pupils during the last five-and-thirty years are the 
most valuable results. Whatever may be our ultimate conclusion 
as to the validity of these theories, no one can doubt that they have 


greatly advanced our knowledge of visual physiology, and their 


study cannot be neglected by any one desirous of acquiring even a 
superficial idea of modern conceptions. 

According to Hering’s method of analysis our whole visual world 
can be resolved into six elementary sensation qualities—white, 
black, the toneless, and blue, yellow, green, red, the toned or 
bright (bunte) colours. 

If we consider the tone-free qualities, we can form a series of 
shades or graduations passing from intensest white to deepest 
black. If we attend to the toned colours, they can be arranged 
in a circle with four divisions. 

“Tf we choose in such a colour circle any colour as starting- 
point, for instance a red similar to that with which a spectrum 
usually begins at the long-waved end, we see the red colours 
arranged in one direction gradually becoming more yellowish, while 
the redness of the colours correspondingly diminishes, until finally, 
passing through orange and golden-yellow, we arrive at a yellow 
which contains no trace of the red which is still so apparent in 


_ the orange. To this yellow succeed other yellow colours which 


play more and more into the green (sulphur-yellow, canary-yellow) ; 
further on (as in sap-green) the yellowishness recedes more and 


1 In the work of Rembrandt, who is, I believe, regarded by those qualified to 
judge as the greatest exponent of contrastive effects, the most striking results seém to 
be rather light-dark contrasts, as in the Hague ‘‘ Anatomie,”’ than specific colour 
oppositions. For instance, in the (so-called) ‘‘ Nachtwache,”’ the yellow sunlight and 
yellow costume of one of the central figures, which produce such a magnificent 
brightening effect, are balanced by general shadow without any apparent use of the 

” This seems to apply also to the ‘*Staalmeesters.” A careful study 
of the chief master-works of painting from the standpoint of the visual physiologist 


- Lo Oneeelaiame 


»* 


- ~~ 5 fl / ie 


416 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


more behind the steadily increasing greenishness, until we finally 
reach a green which seems to be entirely free from’ yellow. To 
this succeed green colours which already play into blue (water- 
green); further on the bluishness of the colours becomes 
increasingly stronger, the greenishness weaker, until we finally 
reach a blue exhibiting no more greenishness at all. To this blue 
succeed blue colours of increasing reddishness and eorrespondingly 
diminishing bluishness (blue-violet, red-violet, purple-red), until 
the last trace of bluishness vanishes in a definite red.” (?4) 

If we define a pure green as a sensation free from admixture 
with blue and yellow, and the other three sensation qualities in 
the same manner, we see that our pure colours (from this point 
of view) can be arranged in two pairs, yellow and blue forming 
one and red and green the other. ‘he members of each pair can 
be placed opposite one another in a diagram, because we can only — 
pass from yellow to blue or from red to green by traversing the 
province of a member of the other pair, as just explained. There 
is no pure yellowish-blue or reddish-green sensation quality. 

But there is yet another contrast, from the standpoint of sen- 
sation quality, between yellow and red on the one hand, blue and 
green on the other. 

Somehow, in a manner difficult to express in words yet of 
universal experience, the two former colours are associated with 
a certain heightening and increased vividness of sensation-tone, 
while the two latter exercise a depressing or subduing influence. 
This finds its expression in the classification by artists of colours 
into warm and cold. Goethe has emphasised these points :— 

“We find from experience, agajn, that yellow excites a warm 
and agreeable impression. Hence in painting it belongs to the 
illumined and emphatic side. ‘ 

“This impression of warmth may be experienced in a very 
lively manner if we look at a landscape through a yellow glass, 
particularly on a grey winter’s day. The eye is gladdened, the 
heart expanded and cheered, a glow seems at once to breathe 
towards us.” (75) 

Of blue he says: “This colour has a peculiar and almost 
indescribable effect on the eye. Asa hue it is powerful, but it is 
on the negative side, and in its highest purity is, as it were, a | 
stimulating negation. Its appearance then is a kind of contra- 
diction between excitement and repose. 


iu i 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 417 


. Rooms which are hung with pure blue appear in some 
degree larger, but at the same time empty and cold. 

“The appearance of objects seen through a blue glass is gloomy 
and melancholy.” (**) 

- How far these results depend on mental factors, e.g. an asso- 
ciation of ideas, cannot be discussed in this place ; it is sufficient 
to recognise their existence. 

In the opinion of those whose theories we are examining, 
the facts are most satisfactorily described by saying that in 
any sensation-complex blue and green produce a darkening, 
and yellow or red a brightening effect; the toneless colours, 
black and white, also contribute respectively in a negative or 


positive sense to the sum total of effects. We have, there- 


fore, the brightness of a colour defined in strictly sensational 
terms. 

We have now arrived at the conception of six primary sensa- 
tion qualities arranged in three pairs—white-black, red-green, 
yellow-blue. The first member in each case increases, the second 
diminishes the subjective intensity or brightness of a sensation- 
complex of which it forms part." 

“The brightness or darkness of a toned (bunte) colour is, 
according to this view, the result of the inherent brightness or 
darkness (Higenhell und Eigendunkel) of its constituent pure 
colours, which as the pure constituents of that colour agreeably 
to their respective distinctness determine the quality of the colour. 
In any colour really existent for us is a definite inherent degree 
of brightness and darkness, and in accordance -with whether the 
brightness or the darkness be the more distinct, we call (the colour) 
bright or dark. 

“A toned colour may generally be regarded as made up of 


_ four primary components, two toned and two tone-free (black and 


white). Only in colours of the tone of a pure colour is one toned 
constituent present by itself. In any red-yellow colour, e.g. orange, 
we have accordingly to distinguish three bright, pure components 


_ (red, yellow, white), and one dark (black); but in any green-blue, 


three dark (green, blue, black) and one bright. The red-green 
and green-yellow colours would contain, however, two bright and 
two dark pure components. 


_1 Sensation-Complex is a term used merely to indicate the supposed multiplicity 


of infra-conscious representatives, not in a perceptual sense. 


= ' 2D 


7 


418 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


‘From what has been said the following rules can be deduced :— 

“Tf two colours of equal tone and equal purity differ in bright- 
ness this is due to a difference in their black-white components. 

“Two colours differing in tone may, notwithstanding equal 
degrees of purity and equality as regards their black-white com- | 
ponents, differ in brightness. 

‘With equality of conditions as to the black-white components, 

a yellow, a red, or a yellow-red colour is so much the brighter, 
a blue, a green, or a blue-green so much the darker, the more 
distinct the colour tone in comparison with the black-white con- 
stituent.” (77) 

This extract describes what is often called the theory of 
the “specific brightness” of colours. All that has gone before 
purports to be a faithful analysis of our visual sensations without 
reference to any hypothesis whatever. That this is a perfectly 
legitimate procedure I have already attempted to show: the next 
step is to translate these facts, or supposed facts, into terms of 
a physiological hypothesis. — 

Such a translation can readily be effected. 

It is supposed that somewhere in the retino-cerebral apparatus, 
in the infra-conscious sphere, four distinct substances exist. Each 
of these substances can undergo a building up or anabolic and a q 
breaking down or katabolic change. External stimuli will, depend- 
ing on their natures, induce either an anabolic or katabolic change 
in the substances, and these are associated with a definite colour 
sensation. The building up of the black-white substance corre- 
sponds to a sensation of blackness, its breaking down to a sensation 
of whiteness; anabolism of the red-green substance is associated 
with green colouration, its katabolism with red colouration ; 
similarly in the third substance yellow is katabolic in origin, blue 
anabolic. 

Before discussing these views in detail, I should like to clear 
up certain popular misunderstandings. Some opponents have 
asserted or suggested that the facts upon which the theory is 
based differ in some perverse way from those data which are 
ordinarily called facts of experiment. This is not the case. The 
facts the hypothesis attempts to describe are as legitimately 
objects of inquiry as any others within the purview of physiological 
science. | 

It is further to be noted that the four physiological “sub- — 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 419 


stances ” have just as much and just as little real existence as the 
three components of our other theory. It is idle to say that the 
postulated anabolic and katabolic processes are essentially unlike 
any chemical mechanisms with which we are acquainted. It is 
equally vain to object that stimulation processes in animal nature 
are, to all appearance, bound up with katabolic changes; this 
would be a valid objection if we tried to identify the hypothetical 
substances with any known retino-cerebral constituent. No such 
identification is attempted; the suggestion that Hering and his 
school identify the black-white substance with visual purple is 
altogether false. The fact is that this theory can only be judged 


on the grounds of scientific expediency, and in no other way. Does 


this method of presenting the facts give us a clearer insight into 
the phenomena of vision than the method based upon stimulus 
relations? This is the only question which requires to be 
answered. 

We notice at once that the hypothesis, in the form in which 
it has just been epitomised, offers two considerable advantages. 
Firstly, it deals with the immediate data of vision, the visual 
sensations of colour, directly, and not in terms of stimulation 
magnitudes. In this sense it might be called a primary hypo- 
thesis. In the second place it is essentially simple and easv of 
comprehension, which is a very strong point in its favour. 

But if we find, on testing this simple hypothesis with such 
facts of experiment as have previously been detailed, that it 
rapidly ceases to be simple ; if we find that its apparently direct 
grip upon the objects of investigation is perceptibly weakened ; in 
one word, if it ceases to be definite, then its characteristic advan- 
tages will have disappeared. 

Let us first of all attempt to express the facts of successive 


. contrast, after-images in terms of the hypothesis. After resting 


the eye on a white object, we should expect, under certain con- 
ditions, a positive, under others a negative after-effect. The 
experimental results agree perfectly with the theory. In the case 
of colour stimuli, we can understand why complementary reacting 
lights gain in saturation. For example, if the eye be stimulated 
with green, anabolism will occur in the red-green substance, an 
anabolism which will lead to the formation of a large quantity of 
“material.” If now red light stimulates the retina, it produces 


not only katabolism of the normal quantity of substance, but the 


420 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


new formed material also falls to pieces and the correlative 
sensation is greatly enhanced. All the simple phenomena of after- 
images are adequately described, but a detailed examination yields 
less promising results. 

V. Kries found that the responsiveness of the eye to mono- 
chromatic light was markedly altered by previous exposure to 
white. He found that the stimulus value of the red he employed 
was diminished in the ratio of about one to four by previous re- 
tuning with white.!| How can this come about if the black-white 
substance is independent of the red-green substance ? 

Hering’s own views on this matter deserve careful study.(?%) He 
objects that, in v. Kries’ experiments, the colour used as a reactor 
(180° blue, 180° white on a disc) was not sufficiently saturated, 
and brings forward the following experiment :— 

Two discs are arranged, A and B. A consists of a black centre 
surrounded by a white ring, B of a centre composed of 120° blue 
and 240° black, encircled by a ring containing 356° blue and 4° 
white. A point upon the internal margin of the white ring in A © 
is fixated for a given time, and the experimenter then turns his 
eye to an exactly corresponding point in B. Hering always found 
that the outer ring in B, under these conditions, looked more 
saturated than the centre. | 

As the term saturated is not used by Hering in the physical 
sense, the exact bearing of his objection to v. Kries’ experiment 
is not apparent. Presumably what is meant is that the “ blue- 
ness” of such a disc was not distinctly separable from its 
‘‘ whiteness,” with the result that the admitted change in the 
latter on retuning with white was mistaken for a change in the 
former. If this objection is admissible, it is, however, equally 
fair to retort that in Hering’s counter-experiment the difference 
between the amounts of blue in reacting and comparison fields 
was so great that an enormous diminution in responsiveness over 
-the retuned area would be necessary for the production of a good 
match between the centre and periphery of B. Put briefly, the con- 
tention is that after white retuning, a reacting blue and white can 
be made to match a pure blue in brightness by adding more white ; 
adding more blue will always make the reactor too saturated. 
Hering’s experiment does not seem to prove more than that a 


1 For an account of v. Kries’ work and some more recent observations of hi 
pupils, see Nagel’s Handb., Bad. iii., p. 210, &e. ; 


U a a "a 7 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 421 


diminution of about 66} per cent. in apparent intensity cannot 
be attained by retuning with the white he employed. That the 
apparent brightness was correspondingly reduced is indeed favour- 
able to his hypothesis, but it is to be noted that we have under 
these conditions the difficulties of specific brightness comparison 
in their most acute form. 

The discussion of conflicting experimental methods is, how- 
ever, always a thankless and mostly a dull task ; the reader’s special 
attention is accordingly directed to the following remarkable 
passage with which Hering concludes his memoir :— 

“To the change of state experienced by an element of the 


‘somatic visual field when acted upon by, e.g. blue light, with which 


the blue sensation is associated, the whole somatic visual field reacts 
by a change in the opposite direction which corresponds to the 
oppositely coloured or yellow sensation, and any light that now 
falls on the retina acts, in consequence of this chromatic retuning 
of the visual field, as if its yellow valency were increased and its 
blue valency correspondingly diminished. This retuning is maximal 
in the immediate vicinity of the element acted upon by the blue 
light, and diminishes with its distance from the same... . A 
white light falling on the neighbourhood of the region which has 
been stimulated with blue seems therefore more or less yellowish, 
but a white light which falls together with blue on the spot that 
has been stimulated with blue, seeing that it behaves as a more 
or less yellow-valent light, neutralises the blue valency of the blue 
light so much the more, the greater be the quantity of it mixed 
with the latter. This explains the striking fact that the chromatic 
quality of a saturated colour is so extremely quickly extinguished 
by increasing the amount of white mixed with it. . . .” 

“When v. Kries, therefore, supposed that, according to the 
theory of opposite colours (Gegenfarben), the same result would be 
obtained from a fatigued and an unfatigued area if the same 
quantity of blue light were allowed to fall on both, but in addition 
on the fatigued area a suitably chosen quantity of white light, he 
was in error. In such a case, the blue valency of the blue light 
at the unfatigued area is unaltered since no other light is mixed 
with it; over the fatigued area the blue valency of the blue light 
is partly neutralised by the admixture of white. Accordingly the 
blue at this latter area must appear less saturated than at the 


former. In fact a transitory equality in brightness and saturation 


' 


422. STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


between the two areas is only obtained when white is indeed mixed 
with blue for the fatigued area, but on the other hand, the blue 
light for the unfatigued area is suitably diminished. In general, for 
reasons already given, an equality in colour tone for blue can only 
be obtained under exceptional circumstances, when the character 
of the daylight is specially favourable and the tone of the blue 
just right.” (7°) 

It has been said by Professor William James that John Stuart 
Mill was in the habit of boldly asserting in general terms the 
truth of a philosophical theory derived from his father, and then 
admitting in detail the validity of all objections to it. A trace of 
this method can be discerned in the passage just quoted. The 
whole memoir is designed to show that white retuning does not 
affect colour valency. Experiments apparently in favour of the’ 
rival hypothesis are met by other experiments in support of this 
contention. We are led to suppose that white retuning and colour 
retuning are independent, and naturally infer that since white ~ 
retuning does not affect colour valency, colour retuning ought not 
to affect white valency. This, however, is specifically asserted not 
to be the case. 

It is not, of course, to be supposed that the two propositions 
are logically inconsistent, but the independence of the postulated 
physiological processes is no longer paralleled by the facts of 
sensational analysis. What is (from the standpoint of sensations) 
a pure white has a definite colour valency. Pure white (in terms 
of the physiological process in the black-white substance) is just 
as hypothetical as anything postulated by the theory of stimuli. 

It will, I think, be clear that in order to cover the facts of 
retuning, the sensational theory loses some of its original simplicity. 
It is interesting to notice that the theory becomes more difficult 
to follow by developing in a direction opposite to that followed 
by the stimulus theory; the latter became unsatisfactory, to — 
some, by tending to be too general, the former by multiplying its 
details. 

I have enlarged upon the question of retuning, a matter not 
perhaps of very special importance in itself, because, as I hope 
to have shown, the facts are not quite so easily described by the 
Hering theory as some of its supporters would lead one to 
imagine. These very facts have been claimed by eager adherents — 
of both schools as decisively favouring their respective beliefs. 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 423 


Perhaps the ingenuous reader will conclude that neither claim 
is admissible. 

Space and the reader’s patience do not admit of analysing in 
detail the further applications of the theory to visual phenomena. 
I will merely say a few words on the Hering theory of dichromatic 
systems. 

It was originally thought that the theory covered these facts 
extremely well, and even in so good a text-book as that of Professor 
Howell it is suggested that an advantage of the sensational 
theory is its better description of the facts of partial colour-blind- 
ness. In reality, it seems very difficult to bring the facts into line 
with the theory. 

It was once suggested that both types, protanopes and 
deuteranopes, depended on the non-existence of the red-green 
substance, the typical distinction being due to varying degrees 
of macular and lens pigmentation, combined with unequal respon- 
siveness of the blue-yellow substance. 

We have already seen that the characters of the two groups, 
protanopes and deuteranopes, cannot be due to differences in 
pigmentation, and the inconsistency of the colour equation, 
R+G= 652, is difficult to reconcile with this view. 

Tschermak, a prominent member of Hering’s school, seems 
definitely to abandon this attempt to describe dichromatic vision, (®°) 
and, so far as I know, an adequate expression of the facts in terms 
of Hering’s hypothesis is not yet forthcoming. This is not a 
fatal objection to the theory, because the sensational analysis of 
dichromatic vision is extremely difficult, owing to the fact, among 
others, that colour nomenclature is adapted to trichromatic vision, 
trichromatics forming the large majority of all civilised races. It 
must, however, be realised that this hypothesis does not seem likely 
to advance our knowledge of colour blindness further than that of 
three components. 

Our general conclusion, therefore, may perhaps be expressed 
in the following terms. 

The theory associated with the name of Hering is an attempt 
to arrive at a general conception of the visual processes by an 
analysis and comparison of sensations. In this way prominence 
is given to many interesting facts not specially or adequately 
considered by the theory founded on stimulus relations. If, 
however, we attempt to build up upon such data an hypothesis 


fe wi 


424 STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 


adequate to the task of describing all the experimental facts, we 
encounter difficulties not less formidable than those we found to 
be associated with the Young-Helmholtz theory. 

In attempting to meet their respective difficulties, the two 
theories become unsatisfactory in different ways. The stimulus 
hypothesis becomes too general, the sensational hypothesis too 
detailed. Examples have been described in the former’s treat- 
ment of dichromatic vision and the latter’s account of after- — 
images. 

Perhaps the most tempting inference is that these theories are 
in a sense complementary—that they both contain some measure 
of truth, surveying the vast complex of phenomena from very 
different points of view. Neither theory is wholly true nor yet 
wholly false ; nor does adhesion to the one imply total rejection ~ 
of its 3 cusible rival. 

I am only too conscious that, like Canning’s clerical friend, in 
being brief I have by no means escaped being tedious. My only 
excuse, and that but a poor one, is that if we are ever able to pro- 
pound a complete theory of the visual processes, this comprehensive 
formula will embrace both those I have described, and until that 
time comes a critical examination and comparison of both lines 
of thought will always be indispensable to those who wish to gain 
a real knowledge of visual physiology. In conclusion, the reader 
is most earnestly recommended to consult the original memoirs of 
Hering and the treatise of Helmholtz dealing with this subject. 
He will not, indeed, find them light reading—no really scientific 
books ate ; but he will be repaid by a grasp of the matter in hand 
such as can never be derived from text-books, however lucid, or 
summaries, however long. 


BIBLIOGRAPHY 


1 Beare, op. cit., p. 18. 

2 Aristotle, De Sensu et Sensilibus, Taylor’s Trans., vol. iii., p. 133, 
° Beare, op. cit., pp. 36-37, 

* Plato, Timeus, Jowett’s Translation, vol. ii., pp. 538-9. 

5 Helmholtz, Handb. d. Phys. Optik., 2nd edit., p. 248. 

° Plato, Thextetus, Jowett’s Trans., vol. iii., pp. 375, 378. 

7 Beare, op. cit., p. 64. 

* Beare, op. cit., p. 69. 

® Helmholtz, Handb, d. Phys. Opt., pp. 328-330. 


STUDIES IN SPECIAL SENSE PHYSIOLOGY 425 


” Koenig and Dieterici, Zeits, f. Psy. u. ‘Physiol. d. Sinnesorg., vol. 
iv., p. 241. 

™ Turberville, Phil. Trans., 1684. Huddart, Phil. Trans., 1777. Whisson, 
Phil. Trans., 1778. [Vide Koenig’s Bibliography appended to Helmholtz, 
Phy. Opt., 2nd ed., for full bibliography.] 

% J. Dalton, Literary and Philosophical Society of Manchester, 1794. 

% Goethe, Theory of Colours, translated by Eastlake, London, 1840 
(Murray), pp. 45-47. 

™ J. v. Kries, Ueber Farbensysteme Zeits. f. Psy. u. Phys. d. Sinnes., 
vol. 13, pp. 241-324 ; and Nagel’s Handb., vol. 3, pp. 153-5. The technic is 
partly described in Zeits. f. Psy. u. Physiol. d. Sinnesorg., vol. 12, pp. 4, &e. 
® J. v. Kries, Zeits. f. Psy. u. Phys. d. Sinnesorg., vol. 13, p. 241. 
© Miss Calkins, Arch. f. Physiologie, Supp. Bd., 1902, pp. 244 et seq. 
" Koenigsberger, Life of Helmholtz, Engl. Trans. by Miss Welly, Oxford, 


1907, p. 364. 


8 G. J. Burch, Phil. Trans., B., vol. 191, pp. 1-34. Proc. Roy. Soe., vol. 
66, pp. 216-219. 

* Burch, Proc. Roy. Soc., vol. 66, p. 219. 

*° Burch, Proc. Roy. Soc., B., 1905, p. 214. 

* Goethe, op. cit. (Eastlake’s trans.), pp. 243-4. 

*? Goethe, op. cit., p. 276. 

*% Tbid., Note C. 

* Ewald Hering, Grundziige der Lehre vom Lichtsinn, Leipzig, 1905, &c. 
(separate publication not yet complete), p. 41. 

*® Goethe, op. cit., p. 307. 

*° Thid., p. 310. 

*? Hering, op. cit., p. 61. 

** Hering, Ueber die von der Farbenempflindlichkeit unabhangige Aende- 
rung der Weissempflindlichkeit. Pfl. Arch., vol. 94 (1903), pp. 533, &c. 

*® Hering, op. cit., p. 552. ie 

%° Tschermak, Ergebn. d. Physiol., lst Jahrg., 1902, Ite Abth., s. 795, &c, 


INDEX 


ABDOMEN— 
Blood determined to, by emotion 
and mental work, 148, 149 
Blood pressure in, Valsalva’s ex- 
periment, 146 
Compression of, restoration from 
syncope by, 173 
Large, production of cerebral 
anemia by vertical suspension 
of animals with, 173 
Abdominal organs, volume of blood 
circulation of, recorded by rectal 
sound, 148 
Abram’s reflex, 186 
Acid— 
Acetic, properties of, 7 
Hippuric, 8 
Lactic— 
As a cause of fatigue, 254 
In muscle, 213 
Acids— 
Amido— 
Basie groups of, 7, 9 
Chemical characteristics of, 
8 
Compounds of protein mole- 
cules, 7 
Found in muscle, 216 
Achromatic threshold of coloured 


light, 355; Burch, apparatus for 
study of, 357 
Adrenalin— 


Constricting effect on arteries, 
177 


Injection of, effect on residual 
vascular pressure, 158 
Adsorption, examples of, 10 
Age, old, and mechanism of respira- 
tion, 205 ; 
Air— 
Alveolar— 
Carbon dioxide in, 240 
Composition of, and “second 
wind,”’ 242 
Breathed, total volume of, per 
minute, 237 
Exposure to, effect on venous 
pressure, 130 
Sacs, alveoli implanted on walls 
of, 185 
Spaces, terminal, in lung, 185 
Albuminuria, functional, 252 
Allorhythmia, induction of, 60, 62 


Alveoli implanted on walls of air 
sacs, 185 

Amido acids. See under Acids 

Anemia, 162-5 

Anemia, cerebral, production 
vertical suspension of animals with 
large abdomen, 173 


Anesthesia— 
Spinal,produced by strain, effects 
of, 180 
Use of chloroform in, 5 
Anesthetics, influence 


on nerve 
fatigue, 268 »° 
Anarthria of Déjérine, 332 
Anastomoses following ligation of 
arteries, 133 
Animals, supra-granular and infra- 
granular layers of cortex in, 298 
Atrium, terminal air space, 185 
Aorta— 
Contractility less than that of 
carotid, 118 
Residual pressure in, compared 
with that in vena cava, 157, 159 
Aphasia, 318 
Aphasia, recent researches in, 330-6 
Aqueous, deficient circulation of, in 
glaucoma, 141 
Arm, volume of blood circulation in, 
148 
Armlet— 
For measurement of arterial 
pressure, 121 
Readings by, failure in case of 
sclerosed arteries, 123, 124 
Readings by, obliteration me- 
thod compared with readings 
by radial hemodynamometer, 
124, 125, 126 
Use of, 142 
Use of, in recording venous 
pressure, 128 
Arterial origin of active hyperemia, 
170 
Arterial pulse, primary wave, “c 
wave in jugular synchronous with, 
102 
Arteries— 
Blood pressure in— 
Constant under alterations 
of output from ey 146 — 
Effect of exercise and food © 
on, 127, 128 . 


“ce ”» 


E ae of inhalation of 
Fall oh on, 128 
how produced, 176, 
Disaticedoeas by armlet, 121 
— investigations, 
12 
Raised by excitation of 
cortex cerebri, 147 
Restoration by constricting 
agents, 177 
Systolic, variations due to 
age, sex, and posture, 126, 
12 


Cerebral, 
139 
Contracted and relaxed, differ- 
ence between, depends on 
muscular element, 119, 121 

Contracted and sclerosed, failure 
of armlet readings, 123, 124 

Contracted, stretching of, 118, 
119 

Development of, 133 

Divided, insertion of lengths of 

' transplanted arteries or veins 
between, 112 

Elasticity and contractility of, 
116 

Embolism of, 164 

Expansion, increase in patho- 
logical states, 120 

esgr eball, pulsation expansion, 

9 

Ligation of, results, 133 

Maximal pulsation in, 122, 124 

Obliteration pressure by armlet 
method, 121, 124 

Obliteration readings to be taken 
during repose, 126 

ure in, determined by pos- 

ture, 413 

Proportion in size and area, 133 

_ Relaxation of, permanently pro- 
i by freezing and heating, 

Relaxed, extension produced by, 
first addition of weight, 118 


pulsatile expansion, 


Blood pressure in, 127 
Ramification homonomous, 


INDEX 


427 


Arteries (continued) 
Tortuosity of, 120 
Transfusion of blood from, to 


veins in cases of shock, 
179 

Union of, with divided ends and 
veins, 112 


Walls: of, contraction and ex- 
ansion of, in tension, 171 
peer es, 231, 234 
Constriction of, produced ex- 
perimentally, 158 
Function of, 144 
Muscle of, action of pressor and 
depressor afferent nerves on, 
176 
Tone of, 175, 176 
Ascidians, excretory organ in, 152 
Ascites, causes of, 149, 161 
Association, centres of functions of, 
312 
Athletes, training of, 208 
Atropine, induction of glaucoma by, 
141 
Auricle— 
Contraction of— 
As showing by intra-ceso- 
phageal hae 91, 92 
Indicated by * wave, 95 
Filing of— 
Influenced by 
ventricle, 105 
Possible factor in production 
of ‘‘v”’ wave, 103, 104 
Left, movements of, 68, 69 
Movements during cardiac cycle, 


systole of 


Of heart, 43 
Degree of see valley, 49 
Distribution of 
cells in, 46 
Stimulation of, result, 57 
Precedence of beat in right or 
left, 66 
Pressure in— 
““C ”’ wave, normal event of 
curve of, 97, 99 
Experiments on, 75 
Fall of, cause of ““y”’ de- 
pression, 107 
Pressure waves, 83 
Relaxation of, 105, 106 
Right, chief function, 67 
Stretching of — of, factor in 
production of “x” and By tae 
depressions, 107 
Tetanisation of, 95 
Auricle and ventricle in mammalian 
-heart; muscular connection be- 
tween, 39, 63. See also Auriculo- 
ventricular bundle 


ganglion ~ 


428 


Auricles— 
Contraction of both, graphic re- 
cords of, 73 
Function as reservoirs, 103, 106 
note 
Auricular— 
Canal, 44 
Of human heart, 38 
Cycle, relationship to 
cular, 82 
Ring, 38, 39 
Invaginated portion of, 39 
Stimulation of, result, 57 
Systole, 67, 69 
Auriculo-Ventricular (A-V) Bundle— 
Agent and means of passage of 
conduction of impulse, evi- 
dence for, 61 
‘** Antidromic ”’ conduction in, 66 
Description of, 39-43 
Isolation of, 36 
Microscopic appearances, 41 
Nerve-fibres in, 47 
Position of, 45 
Auriculo-Ventricular— 
Groove, 53, 67, 69 
Line movements of, 103, 106 
Node, 40, 59 
Node and nodal rhythm, 60 
Valves, position of opening, 81, 
107 
Axis-cylinder of nerve fibre, 278 


ventri- 


BACTERIAL Toxins, collapse due to, 179 
Bat, visual acuity in, 374 
Bidder’s ganglia, extirpation and 
stimulation of, 54, 55 
Bioplasm— 
Chemical nature of, 6 
Crystalloids kept in union with 
cell by, 26 
Birds— 
Infundibular element in wing of, 
186 
Vision in, 374 
Bleeding, explanation of success of, 
173 1 
Blindness— 
Blue or blue-yellow, 401 
Hemeralopia, 372 
In relation to the cortex, 292 
See also Colour Blindness 
Blood— 
Alkalinity of, during muscular 
activity, 241 
Carbohydrate material present 
in, 14 
Channel system, 131 
In Turbellarian worm, 132, 
134 ; undifferentiated and 
unorganised, 132, 134 


INDEX 


Blood (continued)— 
Circulation of. See Circulation 
vee toxic influence of, 
7 

Deficient flow, cause of varicose 
veins, 130 

Dependence of function of nerve 
cells on supply of, 115 

Deprivation of amount trans- 
fused, fatal effect of, 167 

Determined to abdomen by 
mental work, 148, 149 

Determined to peripheral parts 
by emotion and movements, 
148, 149 

Distribution of, throughout lung, 
187 

Flow of, how increased, 167, 168 

Glands, 132 

Influence of chemical state on 
circulation, 171 

Injection of, to excite callous 
formation, 116 

In peripheral fields, 177 

Osmotic energy of, 169, 170 

Quantity in brain, 149 

Serum, depression of freezing- 
point, 21, 28 ; 

Serum, percentages of chlorides 
and phosphates in, 24 

Stagnation of, in shock and 
collapse, 180 ; 

Supply, arterial, temporary 
arrest followed by active 
hyperemia, 171 

Supply of, to nephridial tissues, 
152 


Supply, plethora of, effects, 165 

Transfusion from artery to vein 
in cases of shock, 179 

Transfusion of, in excess, effects 
of, 165, 166 

Viscosity of, 167, 168 

Volume, ventricular, changes in, 
not indicated by curves of 
intra-ventricular pressure, 79 

See also Corpuscles 

Blood Pressure— 

Arterial, 123, 127, 146, 176 

Capillary, 149, 156 

Causes of increase in, 234 

Determined by various positions 
of holding limbs, 142, 143. 

Effect of mental work and 
‘emotion on, 147 

Effects of expiration, inspiration, 
and respiration on, 144, 146_ 

Fall of, in shock and collapse, 180 

Fall of, in spinal anesthesia, 180 — 

Intra-pleural effects of, 146 

_ Venous, 128 


Blood-vessels— 
Coats of, development, 132, 133 
Collateral, compensatory enlarge- 
ment following thrombosis, 164 
= of, in muscular work, 
236 
Structure of, how developed, 133 
See also Arteries, Capillaries, and 
Veins 
Body heat, loss of, in shock and 
collapse, 180 
Body, movements of, factor in main- 
tenance of circulation, 144 
Brain— 
Arteries in, pulsatile expansion, 
139 
Cortex— 
Cell laminz of, 287 
Development of, 289 
Evolution of cerebral func- 
tion, 316 
Excitation of, raises arterial 
pressure, 147 
Higher functions of, 318 
Histological method of local- 
isation, 300, 305 
Lamination of, 285, 291, 294 
Localisation of function, 
284-350 
Marie’s doctrines, 338 
Prefrontal region, 292, 310, 
312, 314 
Regions of, 292, 307 
Quantity of blood contained in, 
varies little, 149 
‘** Brain pressure,” definition of, 137 
** Brain-workers,”’ irritable dyspepsia 
of, 149 
Breathing. See Respiration 
Brightness values, 370 
Broca’s aphasia, 331 
Bronchiole, terminal, 185 
Bronchioles, musculature of, 186 
Bulbus cordis, 45 


Burch apparatus for study of achro- 
matic t holds, 357 

CatcrvuM salts, effect on heart muscle, 
‘3 

Callus— 


Rapid formation of— 
By injection of blood, 116 
_ Under action of warmth, 172 


Blood pressure in, 135, 156, 157 
Hypothesis of filtration by, 


149, 150 
Method of estimation, 135, 
141, 142 
ve tissue cells act as 
walls of, 131 


INDEX 429 


Capillaries (continued )— 
Distended, passage of water 
from, 170 
Excess of blood lodged in, 165 
Surface tension of, 169 note, 170 
Capillary system, nature of, 134 
Capillary-venous pressure, 150, 156 
In brain and eyeball, 138 
In kidney, 153, 154 
Carbohydrates, value of, 251 
Carbon-dioxide— 
Amounts of, effects of, 31 
Percentage of, in alveolar air, 238 
Carboxyl group in amido-acid, 7 
Cardiac cycle, human, pulse records 
in relation to events of, 72-111 
Cardiograms, time relations of ventri- 
cular movements dependent on, 77 
Cardiographic curves, uncertainty of, 
87 


Cardio-motor centre, 59 
Carotid artery— 
Contractility greater than that 
of aorta and pulmonary artery, 
118 
Union of peripheral end of 
external jugular vein to 
central end of, 112, 113 
Carotid, primary wave of synchronous 
with ‘‘c”’ wave of venous pulse, 
Carotid pulse, value as standard 
movement, 80 
Cat, transplantation of kidneys of, 
114, 115 
Cells— 
And fats, union between, 15 
Chemical, concentration and 
fluid, effect on nervous im- 
pulse, 276, 280 
Effect of osmotic pressure on, 4 
Electrolytes in, 25, 274 
Energy supplies of, | 
Equilibrium between ion and 
cell, 20 
Influence of drugs on, 32 
Lamine of the cortex cerebri, 
287; functions of, 294 
Living, colloid and crystalloid 
in, equilibrium of, 1-33 
Living, union between, 14 
Neurilemma, multiplication of, 
272 ‘ 
Osmotic pressure and, 25, 28 
Potassium and ions in, 17, 275 
Sodium and chlorides in, 17, 23, 
275 
Solubility of cell substance, 19 
Centres— 
Association, myelination of, 300 ; 
functions of, 312 


430 


Centres (continued )— 
For language, 285 
Sensory, myelination of, 300 
Word, 324 
Cerebro-spinal fluid, pressure of, 137 
Chemical cells, 276, 280 
Chemical stimuli for producing con- 
tractility of cardiac muscle, 48 
Chest, increased pressure in, factor 
in production of venous pulse, 106, 
107 
Chick embryo, creatin in, 215 
Chloral— 
Conversion of pressor into de- 
pressor reflexes by, 176 
Injurious action on heart, 179 
Intra-venous injection of, effects, 
179 
Chlorides in corpuscles and serum, 24 
Chlorides in nerve fibres and cells, 
275, 278 
Chloroform— 
Conversion of pressor into de- 
pressor reflexes by, 176 
Effect of, on the heart, 4 
Chloroform anesthesia and electrical! 
conductivity, 280 
Cholera, viscosity of blood in, 168 
Circulation of blood— 
Dependent on bodily movement, 


143 

Effects of muscular exercise on, 
228 

Effect of respiratory muscles on, 
153 

Means for determining volume 
of, 148 


Obstruction of, at two points 
during half of cardiac cycle, 
106 note 
Pulmonary obstruction of, effect 
on output from heart, 146 
Sluggish, in sedentary workers, 
149 
Therapeutic effects of accelera- 
tion, 149 
Varies with changes of posture 
and muscular contraction, 144 
Circulation and respiration, abnormal, 
due to adhesions following medias- 
tinitis, 190 
Circulatory system, comparative ana- 
tomy and histology of, 131 
Clothing, effect of unsuitable, 247 
Coagulation, protein, influence of 
temperature on, 212 
Ceelomic fluid, 152 
Collapse, 173-180 
: aused by injection of bacterial 
toxins, 179, 180 
Reflexes ceasing during, 180 


INDEX “Sa 


sass equilibrium of, in living onthe 
3 


Colloid union with crystalloid, 12 
Colour— 
Genesis of, 381 
Mixing of, 387 
Sensations— 
Mixing of, 354 
Purkinje’s phenomenon, 353 
Young—Helmholtz theory, 


403 
Three-colour hypothesis _—_ of 
Young, 403 
Toned, brightness and darkness 
of, 417 


Colour-blindness— 
Partial, 392, 395 
Total, 360, 375 
Hering’s case of, 361 
Luminosity values of, 363 
Colour-vision— 
Normal, 396 
Theories of, 378-425 
Ceaser blindness and deafness, 
328 


Congestion, production of, 162 
Connective tissue cells act as walls 
of capillaries, 131 
Contraction, muscular, determination 
of, 48, 51, 98, 226 
Corpuscles, chlorides. and phosphates 
in, 24 
Cortex cerebri. See Brain, Cortex 
Costal cartilages— 
Loss of elasticity, 195 
Respiratory movements of, 198 
Creatin in muscle, 169, 215 
Cribriform ligament, sclerosis of, 141 
Crystalloid— 
Chemical reaction of, 12 
ar of, in living cells, 
Cycling, heart beat and blood pressure 
during, 235 


DARKNESS, effect on vision, 353 
Deafness, causes of, 328 
Death, vascular residual pressure in 
animals after, 157 
Depressor reflexes, 176 
Deuteranopes, 396; stimulus values 
for, 400 
Dextrin in muscle, 213 
Dextrose, effect of, on heart muscle, 2 
Dextrose, injection into blood causing 
hydremia, 169 
Diaphragm— 
Action of, 199 
Piston-like, 200 
Variability, 200 
Antagonist of, 192 ~ 


l 
_ Descent of, 147 See also Respira- 
tion, abdominal 
Movements of, 199 
Muscular crura of, 190 
oe of, 193 been, 
aralysis of, collapse confined to 
lower pulmonary lobe, 191 
i tic pulmonary surface 
directly expanded, 187 
Diaschisis, 331 
Dichromatic systems of vision, 423 
Diet in relation to muscular work, 
249 
Diphtheria toxin, injection of, col- 
apse following, 180 
i , action of drugs upon, 32 


: Dog— 


Amputation and replantation of 
thigh in, 113 
Stomach of, subject to hyper- 
mic reaction, 171 
Dropsy— 
Cause of, 149, 161 
Cardiac, causation of, mechanical 
theory, 162 
Drugs— 
Aetions of, 5, 32 
Antagonistic action of, 20 
Effects of on cells, 32 
Dudgeon’s sphygmograph, 86 
Duplicity theory of vision, 371 
Dyspepsia, irritable, of brain-workers, 
149 


Dyspnea, causes of, 238, 241 


Ear, middle, new instrument for 
graphic records of, 77 

Einthoven’s electrocardiogram, 93 
note 

Einthoven’s string galvanometer, 76, 
77, 109 

Elasticity, muscular, 210 

Electric currents of nerves, 274-280 

Electrical conductivity and chloro- 
form anesthesia, 280 

Electrical —— in excised 


city, animal, a “ diosmotic 


Electrocardiogram— 
Einthoven’s, 93 note 
J curve shown simul- 
taneously with electrocardio- 
gram, 108, 109 a je 
experimental application 
to heart, 70 


of granules by, 279 
In cell and nutrient medium, 25 


INDEX 431 


Electrometer, capillary, for graphic 
records of heart sounds, 76 
Embolism, arterial, how compen- 
sated, 164 
Embolus, downward movement in 
large veins, 164, 165 
Emotion— 
Determines blood to abdomen, 
148, 149 
Effect on blood pressure, 147 
Evolution of, 319 
Emphysema— 
Cause of, 195 
Hyperdistention of infundibula 
in, 186 
Energy supplies of the cell, 1 
Energy value of foods, 251 
Equilibrium between cell and ion, 20 
Equilibrium of colloid and crystalloid 
in living cells, 1-33 
Equilibrium, osmotic of the cell, 28 
Ergograph of Mosso, 226 
Ether anesthesia and electrical con- 
ductivity, 280 
Evolution of cerebral function, 316 
Excitatory wave of heart impulse, 65, 


Excretory organ— 
In Ascidians, 152 
In unicellular animals, 151 
Exercise, muscular— 
Cardiac and circulatoryeffects, 228 
Effects of, 224 
Over-exhaustion from, 127, 128 
Physiology of, 208-257 
Therapeutic effects of accelerated 
circulation under, 149 
Types of, 223 
Expiration— 
Effect on blood-pressure, 145 
Normal, action of respiratory 
muscles in, 192 
Prolonged, effect on 
pressure, 129 
Extra-systole— 
Method of production, 52 
Ventricular, 74 
Eye— 
Adaptation of, to light, 351 
(@dema of tissues of, in glau- 
coma, 141 
Retina of, rods and cones of, 373 
Retina, sensitiveness of, to light, 
358 
Sensitivity to light, 420 
are of, response to, 352, 


Eyeball— 
oe in, pulsatile expansion, 

139 
Capillary-venous pressure in, 138 


' 


venous 


432 


FarnTINnG, cause of, 173 
Fat and cells, union between, 16 
Fat— 
Energy, value of, 251 
Present in muscle, 213 
Fatigue— 
Muscular— 
Causes of, 254 
Lactic acid, produced by, 
214 
In nerves, apparent absence of, 
266; influence of anesthetics 
on, 268 
Femur, amputation and replantation 
of, in dog, 113 
Ferments of muscle, 217 
Fibre lamine of the cortex cerebri, 
288 
Fibres. See Nerves 
Filtration hypothesis of capillary 
pressure, 149, 152, 156, 170 
Flechsig’s area for bodily sensibility. 
306 
“Flicker ’’ method in colour sensation, 
354 
Fluid cells, theories based on, 280 
Foetus— 
Cerebral cortex of, 296 
Movements of, 219 
Food— 
Comparative energy values of, 
251 
Conservation of energy and, 221 
Influence of, on output of 
nitrogen, 248 
Ingestion of, raises arterial pres- 
sure, 127 
Fovea centralis, sensitiveness of, to 
light, 358 
Freezing, permanent relaxation cf 
arteries by, 117 
Freezing point, depression of, of blood 
serum, 21, 28 
Frog, heart of, experiments on, 50, 70 
Infundibular element in lung of, 
186 


GALVANOMETER. See String galvano- 
meter, Einthoven’s, 76, 77 
Ganglion, sensori-motor, 317 
' Ganglion cells— 
Heart-beat, independent of in- 
vasion of, 58 
In vertebrate heart, 45, 47 
Gases, respiratory, pressure of, 30 
Glands, formation of, progressive 
evolution of, 153 
Glandular system, influence of mus- 
cular work on, 252 
; rere causes and treatment of, 


INDEX eS 


Glycogen in the cell, 15 
In muscle, 212 
a egies following hyperglycemia, 


Pee cell lamina of cortex cere- 
bri, 296 
Graphic records— 
Of heart sounds, 76 
Tnstrument for, 77 
Marey’s receiver for, 75 
Simultaneous from different pul- 
sating points, value of, 72 


H2MODYNAMOMETER, radial, maxi- 
mum pulsation method, 124, 126 
Hemoglobin— 
Attachment of ions to, 23 
Formed with oxygen,a dissociable 
compound, 11 
Hemorrhage, into connective tissues 
to be avoided in operation on 
kidneys. 116 
Harrison’s sulcus, 188 
Heart— 
Action of— 
Effect of transfusion of 
blood on, 166 
Increase by strychnine, 178 
Vagus nerve on, 54 
Adhesion to roots of lung, 190 
Automaticity of, 47, 49, 59, 64 
Block, 104, 106 
Definition of, 73 
Partial, 95 
Cardiac cycle, 72-111 
Effect of chloroform on, 4 
ee of muscular exercise on, 
28 
Excitability of, varies in different 
parts, 4 
Experimental action of drugs on 
different parts of, 58 
Function of, 144 
Human— 
Auricles of, 38, 43, 68, 91 
Sinus venosus in, how re- 
presented, 37 
Ventricles of, 44 
Injurious action of chloral on, 
179 
Mammalian, muscular connec- 
tion between auricle and 
ventricle in, 39. See also 
Auriculo-Ventricular Bundle 
Microscopical anatomy of, 34 
Movements of, 65, 76 
Output of blood from, 146 


Power of, how develo 133 

Some. Frere Raya of, 76° 
pec orm 0 

region of blood channel, a 


Hes leontinued)— 
Vertebrate— 


’ Morphology of, 36 
Nervous elements of, 45 
Primitive, chambers of, 36 
Wall, direction of movement of 
different parts of, 103 
See a'so Auricle and Ventricle 
Heart beat— 
And inorganic ions, 3 
Effect of oxygen pressure on, 4 
Experimental methods for pro- 
ucing cessation of, 48 
Independent of invasion of 
anglion cells, 58 
Influence of muscular work on, 236 
Revival after death, 58 
See also Pulse 


- Heart impulse— 


Conduction of, 64 

Excitatory wave of, 53, 56, 65, 70 

Muscular conduction, 60 

Site of origin, 59 

Heart muscle— 

Contraction of, 48, 51, 98 

Effect of dextrose on, 2 

Effect of sodium chloride on, 2 

Excitability of, 48 

Fibres of, 34 

Nature of, 34, 36 

Of invertebrates, how differing 
from that of vertebrates, 56 

Properties of, method of study 
during beat, 51 

Relaxation of, 52, 53 

Rhythmic stimulation, effect, 5 


Tetanisation, 51, 56 7 
Tonicity of, 52, 53 
Heat— 
Body, loss of in shock and 
collapse, 180 


Effect of muscular work on, 244 
Production of hyperemia by, 172 
ing to 50° C., permanent relaxa- 
tion of arteries produced by, 118 
Hemeralopia, 372 
Hens. See Leghorn hens 
ity and muscular activity, 218 
ngs theory of visual sensations, 


. acid, 8 : 
hle’s differential manometer, 76 
"s manometer, 86 


Hydremia, 168-170 


a of on tissues and viscera, 
Production of, 158, 162 


41 


Hydrostatic pressure— 
Compensation for, 143 
- Mean, 157, 159 
Variations of, 142 


INDEX 


433 


Hydrothorax caused by experimental 
obstruction of vene cave, 160 
Hyperemia, 170-173 
Active, origin and production of, 
170, 172 
Passive, 164 
Due to venous congestion, 
170 
Resorption favoured by, 172 
Procreative and _ reproductive 
functions accompanied by, 172 
Reaction to, in stomach of dog, 
rabbit, and man compared, 171 
Hyperpneea, causes of, 238, 240 


IMAGE, striping of, 373 
Images, after-, 402, 408 
Infants, grasping power in, 219 
Inflammation, processes of, relieved 
by flow of lymph, 139 
Infundibula— 
Function and nature of, 185 
Hyperdistension in emphysema, 
186 
Injuries, severe, production of shock 
by, 173 
Ink polygraph, Mackenzie’s, 86, 93 
Inosite in muscle, 213 
Insecta, nephridial tissues in, 152 
Insectivora, functions of brain in, 3l4 


Inspiration— 
Effect on blood pressure, 145, 146 
Prolonged, effect on venous 


pressure, 129 
Instinctive activities, localisation of, 
297 
Intellectual processes, 
upon language, 322 
Intercellular fluid, 152 
Intestines, hyperemia, after produc- 
tion of temporary anemia, 171 
Intra-cranial pressure increased by 
inflammatory processes, 137, 139 
Intra-ocular pressure increased by 
inflammatory processes, 139 
Tons— 
Cells rich in, 17 
Inorganic, and heart beat, 3 
Phosphatic, cells rich in, 17 
Presence of, in cells, 1 
Various, distribution of, in cell 
and fluid, 19, 23 d 
Iridectomy, relief of glaucoma by, 141 
Isoelectric nerves, 259 
Isotonic and isometric curves of con- 
tractility of cardiac muscle, 49 


Jornts, drugs introduced into, subse- 


~quentl pearing in urine, 172 
Lalas Bulb, curves and traces from, 
85 


2E 


dependent 


434 


Jugular pulse— 
** A” wave, delay in onset, 93 
*“C” wave, component part of, 
99 
*C” wave synchronous with 


primary wave of arterial pulse, 
102 
Curve shown simultaneously 


with electrocardiogram, 108 

See also Venous pulse 

Jugular vein— 

External peripheral end united 
to central end of carotid 
artery, 112, 113 

Pulsation in and tracings from, 
85 


KIpNEY— 

Capillary-venous 
153, 154 

Disease, cedema in, explanation 
of, 169 

Excised, perfusion experiments 
on, 154 

Functions of, 156 : 

Influence of protein on, 249 

(Edema, production of, 163 

Rhythmical squeezing by respi- 
ratory muscles, 153 é 

Transplantation of, experimental, 
114, 116 

Tubules of, 153, 155, 156 

See also  Excretory Organ; 
Nephridial tissues 


pressure in, 


Lasour, bodily, effect on viscosity 
of blood, 168 : 
Lactic acid, 213, 254 
Lactose, introduction into joint and 
appearance in urine; 172 
Language, 319 
As instrument of thought, 322 
Centres, localisation of, 285 
Leghorn hens, black and white, ex- 
change of ovaries in, 116 
Leg, cedema, production of, 163 
Legs, hereditary oedema of, 130 
Levatores costarum, 205 
Light— 
As stimulus to the eye, 352 
Coloured, achromatic threshold 
of, 355 
Mixing of, 387 
The medium of vision, 382 
Limbs— 
Determination of blood pressure 
by various positions of hold- 
ing, 142, 14 
(Edema in, 130, 163, 169 
Lipoids, cells surrounded by, 18 


INDEX 


Liver— 
Engorgement of, effect on heart, 
233 


Pulse, auricular, 100 
Loven reflexes, 176 


Lungs— 

a ry of, inspiratory position, 
85 

Area of increased resonance 
round localised consolidation 
in, 188 

Collapse of, 191, 194 > 

Division into, functional signi- — 
ficance of, 190 


Elasticity of, 184, 192, 193 
Extensibility of, 184, 188 
Inflation of, after death, 185 
Infundibula essential, expansile 
parts of, 186 
Lobes of, 190 
Male and female differences be- _ 
tween, 191 ‘ 
Prolonged ventilation of, effects 
of; 31 
Reflex contraction of, 186 
Roots of, 189, 190 
Sedentary habits produce disuse 
of parts of, 188 
Ventilation of, during exercise, 
238 
Zones of, 184, 186 
See also Circulation of blood, 
pulmonary 
Lymph— 
Flow of— 
From salivary gland, 150 
Production, 169, 170 
Formation of, 156, 157 
Mechanical theory of, 149 


MACKENZIE’S ink polygraph, 86, 93 
Mackenzie’s receiver, 86 
Mamma», arteries of, tortuosity, 120 
M os 
Functions of brain in, 295, 314 
Infundibular element in lung of, 
186 
Manometer, 105, 121, 135, 137, 141, 
146 
Alteration in tension of ventricle 
wall measured by, 49 
Hiirthle’s, 86 
Differential, 76 
Manubrio-sternal joint— ; 
Anchylosis, late occurrence in 
life, 196 
Limitation in movement as. 
cause of phthisis, 197 3 
Respiratory significance, 196 
Marasmus, extensibility of arteries. 
in, 120 ; 


nay, 7 variations in the pulse 

after, 230, 233 

Marey’s receiver for graphic records, 
5 


7 
Marie’s doctrines on cerebral localisa- 
tion, 338 


‘Mediastinal pulmonary surface, in- 


directly expanded, 187 
Mediastinitis, adhesions due to, caus- 
ing abnormal respiratory and cir- 
culatory movements, 190 
Medullary centres, effect of spinal 
anesthesia on, 180 
Mental disease— 
In relation to the cortex, 293 
Proportion of cases to general 
population, 313 
Mental disease and “alteration of 
personality,’’ 343 


Mental work— 


Determines blood to abdomen, 
148, 149 
Effect of, on blood pressure, 147 
Mesocardia, functions of, 65 
Metazoa, nephridial functions in, 152 
Microphone for graphic records of 
heart sounds, 76 
Mole, sensory association in the, 314 
Molecules, protein structure of, 7 
Molluses, circulatory system in, 131 
Mosso’s experiment, 148 
Motor aphasia, 331 
Movements— 
Active, send blood from abdomen 
to peripheral parts, 148, 149 
Analysis of, by Marey and Muy- 
bridge, 220 r 
Factor in maintenance of circu- 
lation, 144 
See also Exercise 


_ Muscearine, effect of on heart beat, 48 
uscle— 


Abdominal, effect of stimulation 
of phrenic nerve on, 192 

Cardiac, properties of, 47-53 

See also Heart muscle 

Chemical properties of, 210 

Contractility, means of, 49 

_ Contraction, voluntary, 225 

Intercostal action of, 204 , 

Nitrogenous extractives of, 214 

Of ventricle, function of longi- 
tudinal layer, 67, 68 

Protein constituents of,. 211 

Physical properties of, 210 


ry, action in normal 


INDEX 


435 


Muscle (continued )— 
Structure of, 209 
See also Respiratory muscles 
Muscular— 
Conduction of heart impulse, 60 
Exercise, See Exercise 
Fatigue. See Fatigue 
Work, physiology of, 208-257 
Musculi pectinale of auricle, 67 
Mustard poultices, hyperemic action 
of, 173 
Myogenic theory of excitatory wave, 
54, 56, 58, 63 
Myosinogen of Halliburton, 211 


Necrosis, due to closure of terminal 
arteries, 164 
Neopallium, 315, 317 
Nephridial tissues, 151, 152 
Supply of blood to, 152 
Nerve cells— 
Action of — 
Chloroform on, 5 
Strychnine on, 14 
Function dependent on supply 
of blood, 115 
Nerve-fibres— 
Axis-cylinder of, 278 
Central origin of, 270 
Chlorides and potassium in, 275 
In auriculo-ventricular bundle, 
47 
In vertebrate heart, 45, 46, 47 
No growth of, from peripheral 
ends, 269, 271 
Regeneration of, 268 
Nerves— 
Activity of, theories of, 274 
Afferent, pressor and depressor, 
action on muscle of arterioles, 
176 . 
Chemical concentration and fluid 
cells of, 276, 280 
Degeneration, chemical pheno- 
mena of, 16 
Electric currents in, 274 
Electrical conductivity of, 280 
Endings in vertebrate heart, 46 
Excised medullated eimai 
connected with, 26 
Fatigue in, apparent absence, 266 
Impulse, effect of chemical and 
other cells on, 276, 280; expen- 
diture of energy and, 267 
In living animal, 262 
Isoelectric, after excision, 259 
Physiology of, 258-283 
Nervous system, influence of on the 
heart, 231 
Neurogenic theory of excitatory 
wave, 54, 55 


‘ 


436 


Neurone theory, 269 

Neutral point of spectrum, 397 

Night-blindness, 372 

Nitrogen, excretion of, determination 
of, 248, 251 

Nitrogenous extractives of muscle, 
214 

Nodal rhythm, 60 


(pEMA— 
Causes of, 149, 160 
In limbs due to hydizemia, 169 
In renal disease, explanation of, 
169 
Occurring under normal venous 
and arterial pressure, 161 
Of legs, hereditary, 130 
Production of, 163 
(sophagus, tracings from within, 91, 
93, 97 
Oil, injection of, into pericardium, 
158 
Organs, transplantation of, from one 
animal to another, 112, 113 
Orthodiascope, 205 
Osmosis, 154, 156 
In capillary system, 134 
See also Blood, osmotic energy 
of 
Osmotic— 
Energy, 169 
Equilibrium of the cell, 28 
Pressure, 4, 6, 161 
Pressure and cells, 25, 29 
Ovaries, exchange of, in black and 
white Leghorn hens, 116 
Oxygen— 
Hemoglobin formed with, 11 fi 
Inhalation of, raises arterial 
pressure, 128 
Pressure of, effects of, 30 
Pressure, effect on heart beat, 4 


Pancreas of rabbit, 153 
Paralysis— 
Motor, in spinal 
limitations, 180 
Of constrictor synapses, 177 
Of diaphragm, 191 
Of vaso-motor centra, 174 
Of vaso-motor nerves,\ 179 
Parameecium, vacuoles of, 151, 152 
Para-myosinogen of Halliburton, 211 
Perception and sensation, 319 
Pericardium, injection of oil into, 
158 
Peripheral ends of nerves, regenera- 
tion from, 271 
Personality, alteration of, 342 
Phagocytosis, increase by warm and 
stimulating agencies, 173 


anesthesia, 


Phi 


Cy ae 
Pie Te 


INDEX 


Phosphates in corpuscles and serum, 
24 ¥ 


Phosphorus poisoning, extensibility 
of arteries in, 120 
Phrenic nerve, stimulation of, effect 
on abdominal musculature, 192 
Phthisis, production through limita- 
tion of manubrio-sternal joint, 197 
Physical deterioration, causes of, 
208, 222 
Physiological saline solution, injec- 
tions of, 158, 161, 168 
Pial veins, pressure in, 137 
Pigment of muscule, 217 
Pituitary extract, constricting effect 
on arteries restores blood pressure, 
177 
Plasma— 
‘* Antitropic ”’ action of, 172 
Flow of, 173 
Plethysmograph— 
Records by, 166 
Volume of blood-circulation in 
arm recorded by, 148 
Pleurisy, lower pulmonary lobe most 
affected in, 191 
Pocket sphygmometer with 
enclosed bag, value of, 126 
Poiseuille formula, 136 
Polarimeter, 172 
Polycythemia, viscosity of blood in, 
in, 168 
Polygraph, 86, 93 4 
Popliteal nerves, regeneration after 
suture, 273 
Portal vein, constriction of, 161 
Post-sphygmic interval, 79 
Posture— 
Effect on blood pressure, 129, 
143 
Effect on circulation, 144 
Potassium— 
Cells rich in, 17, 275 
Distribution of in axis-cylinder, 
278 
Ferro-cyanide solution introduc- 
tion into joint and appear- 
ance in urine, 
In muscle, 217 
Salts, effect on heart muscle, 3 
Poultices, hot, induction of hyper- 
zmia by, 173 
Presphygmic interval, 79, 100 
cure working estimate of, 80 
Pressor reflexes, conversion into de- 
pressor, 176 
ure— 
Aortic, and in vena cava, 157, 
159 
Atmospheric, effects of, 30 
Auricular, 75, 83, 97, 107 


large 


Pressure (continued) — 
Capi venous, 150, 156 
tic, 142, 157, 159 
Osmotic, 4, 6, 161 
Cells and, 25, 29 
Effects of chemical union, 10 


Respiratory gases, 30 


See also Arteries, Blood pressure, 


and Ventricle 
Prison diet, 249 
Procreative functions accompanied 
by hyperemia, 172 
Protanopes— 
Stimulus valves for, 400 
Proteins— 
Action of, upon blood serum, 22 
Bodies, 6 
And carbohydrate, union of, 15 
In food, value of, 249 
Of muscular plasma, 211 
Protoplasm, chemical nature of, 5 
Pseudo-arthrosis, excitation of callous 
~ pee in, by injection of blood, 
116 
Pulmonary artery, contractility less 
than that of carotid, 118 
Pulse— 
Human, advantages of study, 72 
Irregularities, significance not 
always pathological, 74 
Rate, effect of exercise upon, 228 
Study of from physiological ex- 
periments on animals, 73 
Variations in, in rest and exer- 
cise, 230 
* Youthful irregularity ”’ of, 74 
See also Jugular and Venots 
pulse 
Pulsus oxus, normal as well as 
pathological event, 146 
Pupil, contraction of, 368 
Pupillo-motor response, apparatus 
or, 369 
Purkinjé— 
Cells, 41 
Fibres, nature of, 39, 64, 98 
Phenomenon, 352 
dal— 
- Layer of the cortex, 287, 296 
Seat of psychic function, 295 | 


RasBsBirs— 
Creatin in muscles of, 215 
Pancreas of, 153 : 
Stomach of, not subject to 
_ hyperemia reaction, 171 

Radial artery, eerertion of sphyg- 

mometer to, 1 
Radial 


INDEX | 437 


Rectal sound, volume of blood circu- 
lation in abdominal organs re- 
corded by, 148 

um— 
Blood pressure in, use of, 146, 147 
Temperature in the, 244 

Reflexes— 

Conversion of inhibitory, 177 
See also Pressor, and Depressor 
reflexes 

Regeneration of nerve fibres, 268 

Relaxation, muscular, accompanying 
pleasurable feelings, 149 

Remak’s ganglia, 54 

Renal vein, flow from compared with 
ureteral flow, 154 

Reptiles, infundibular element in 
lung of, 186 

‘** Residual affinities ’ 
10, 13 

Residual vascular pressures in the 
dead animal, 157 

Respiration— 

Abdominal— 
Effect on blood pressure, 
144, 147 
Movements of roots of lung 
under, 190 
Artificial, determination of best 
method for, 194 
Diaphragmatic movements of 
roots of lung under, 190 
Effect of excess oxygen on, 31 
Human, mechanism of, 182-207 
Influence of muscular work on, 
237 
Pumping action of, 144-147 
Rate of, effect of muscular work 
on, 243 ; 
Thoracic— 
Effect on blood pressure, 144 
In supine position, how 
produced, 192, 193 
Movements of roots of lung 
under, 190 

Respiration and circulation, abnor- 
mal, due to adhesions following 
mediastinitis (Wenckebach), 190 

Respiratory movements in the foetus, 
219 

Respiratory muscles— 

Effect on circulation, 153 


’ 


or adsorption, 


Rhythmical squeezing of kidney. 


by, 153 
Retina, See Eye 
Rhythm, low, of conservation to 
hysiological stimuli, 52 
Rib, Bret, respiratory significance, 196 
Ribs— 
wieeding: respiratory movements, 


438 


Ribs (continuzd)— 
Respiratory movements of, 20] 
Upper— 
Mechanism different from 
that of lower, 205 
Respiratory movements, 204 
Spinal articulations, 204 
See also Costal cartilages 
Ringer’s solution, 115 
Action of, on nerves of heart, 58 
Effect on heart beat, 4 
Experimental injection of, 168 
Rods and cones of retina, 373 
Rubber bag for armlet, 121 
Rubber pneumatic suit for use in 
shock, 178 
Running— 
Composition of alveolar air in, 
242 
Variations in the pulse after, 231 


SALINE, physiological, placing heart 
in to produce cessation of beat, 48 
Salivary cells, 151 
Salivary gland, flow of lymph from, 
150, 151 
Salts— 
Inorganic— 
Of muscle, 216 
Present in cells, 17 
Sarcolemma and sarcoplasm, and 
sarcostyles of cardiac muscle fibres, 
35 
Schlemm’s canal— 
Venous sinus of, 138 
Walling in of, 141 
** Second wind ”’ and the composition 
of alveolar air, 242. 
Sedentary work, sluggish circulation 
due to, 149 
Semilunar valve, closure of, 104, 107 
Sensation— 
Head’s experiments in, 271 
Localisation and, 319 
Senses, special, physiology of, studies 
in, 351-425 
Sensibility, bodily, Flechsig’s area 
for, 306 . 
Sensory aphasia, of Wernicke, 33 
Sensory centres, 300 
Sensory “‘ receptors,’ 144 
Septum— 
Inter-auricular, 40 
Inter-ventricular, origin and 
mode of development, 44, 45 
Shock, 173-180 
Causes, 173, 174 
Methods of production, 174 
Reflexes ceasing during, 180 
Rubber pneumatic suit for con- 
fining body in, 178 


INDEX eae 


Shock (continued)— 
Simple, transfusign of human 
_— from artery to vein in, 
Sibson’s fascia, 187 
Sight. See Vision 
Sino-auricular groove, 54 
Sino-auricular node, 40, 59 
Sinus venosus, 44 
In human heart, 37 
Skeletal muscles, function of, 144 
Sleep. influence of, on muscular 
system, 224 
Sodium chloride, effect of, on heart 
muscle, 2 
Spectrum— 
Intensities of lights in, 387 
Neutral point of, 397 
“Twilight ”’ values of, 362 
And visual sensation, 355 
Sphygmogram, clinical information 
afforded by limited in value, 72 
Sphygmographs, 75 
Application by suspension me- 
thod, 144 
Dudgeon’s, 86 
Sphygmography, methods of, 75 
Sphygmomanometer, 144 
Sphygmometer— 
Bags, small, unenclosed, errors 
introduced by, 125 
oe eee to radial artery, 


(Hill’s) description of, 122 
See also Pocket Sphygmometer 
Spinal anesthesia. See Anesthesia, 
spinal 
Spinal cord, division of, shock re- 
sulting from, 173, 174 
Spine— 
Double articulation of upper 
ribs to, 205 
Extension of, as means of ex- 
panding thorax, 205 
Standard movement, value of, 77, 80 
Stannius ligature experiment, 48, 54, 
59 
Sterno-costal pulmonary surface di- 
rectly expanded, 187 
— inspiratory movements of, 
Stimulus— > 
Normal visual, 387 
Values for Deuteranope and 
Protanope, 400 
Stochiometric relationships, 12 
Stomach— 
Of dog subject to hyperemic 
reaction, 171 
Of rabbit and man, not subject 


| 


to hyperemic reaction, 171 | 


ae "I 


- Stovain, 
by stovain, 180 
as galvanometer, Einthoven’s, 
1 


hnine— . 
Action of, on nerve cells, 14 
Favourable action of, 177 


ugar— 
In the blood, 14 
jection into blood causing 
ydremia, 169, 170 
In muscle, 213 
Sweat, secretion of, and muscular 
work, 246, 252 
Sweating, effect on viscosity of blood, 
168 
Sympathetic nerve, how long active 
after death, 58 
| Syncitium— 
Definition of, 34, 36 
Heart muscle regarded as, 36 


8 7 a 
ey onstrictor, paralysis of, 177, 178 
Sensory, 175, 177 
In spinal anesthesia, 180 
Of vaso-motor centre, 179 
Syncope, restoration from, 173 
Systole, onset of, in man, not easy to 
fix, 76 


TnI terminalis, 66, 67 
Temperature— 
uence of muscular work on, 
243 
Influence on nervous impulse, 276 
Influence of, on protein coagula- 
tion, 212 
Tetanisation— 
Of auricle, 95 
Of heat muscle, 51, 56 - 
Thigh. See Femur 
Thorax— 
Compression of, results, 195 
Elasticity of, respiratory valve, 
193, 194 
Expansion of, 194 
By means of extension of 
spine, 205 
' Horizontal groove on each side 
of, 188 
aa or operculum of, 196, 197 
Wal of pressure in, 188 
of, adhesion of roots of 
ung to, 190 
Thrombors compensato enlarge- 
ment of collateral iekiwibedis 


passage of water from 


capillaries into, 170 


INDEX 
spinal anesthesia produced — 


439 


Torcular Herophili, 137 
Toxins. See Bacterial toxin; Diph- 
theria toxin 
Trachea, blood pressure in, rise of, 146 
Training. See Exercise 
Transfusion of blood, in excess, effects 
of, 165, 166 
Tricuspid regurgitation, ‘‘ v ” 
and, 102 and note 
Tuberculosis— 
Pulmonary— 
Initial site of, 198 
And mechanism of respira- 
tion, 206 
Turbellarian worm, 
system in, 132, 134 
Turkish baths, effect on viscosity of 
blood, 168 
Turpentine stoups, hyperemic action 
of, 173 


wave 


blood-channel 


UNICELLULAR 
organ in, 151 

Unions, chemical and physical, 12 

Ureter, flow from, compared with 
flow from renal vein, 154 

Urine, resorption into, of drugs intro- 
duced into joints, 172 

Uterus, arteries of, tortuosity, 120 


animals, excretory 


VACUOLES of Paramececium, 151, 152 
Vagus nerve— 
Action on heart, 48, 54 
How long active after death, 59 
Valsalva’s experiment, 146 
Varicose veins, due to deficient flow 
or quality of blood, 130 
Vascular pressure, residual, 
dead animal, 157 
Vascular system, experimental opera- 
tions on, 112 
Vaso-constrictor impulses, 176 
Vaso-dilatation of active organs, 176 
Vaso-dilator impulses, 176 
Vaso-motor centre— 
Paralysis of, 174 
Synapses of, 179 
Vaso-motor nerves— 
Excitation of, effect on blood 
flow, 167 
Paralysis of, 179 
Veins— & 
Abdominal and thoracic, citcu- 
latory function, 66, 67 
Blood pressure in, 128, 130 
o/-am structure of arteries, 


Large, downward movement of 
embolus in, 164, 165 

Over-engorgement, prevention, 
106 note 


in the 


440 


Veins (continued)— 

Preservation of normal situation 
and direction in operations on 
kidneys, 115, 116 

Small, excess of blood lodged in, 
165 

Transfusion of blood from ar- 
teries to, in cases Of shock, 179 

Transplanted portions of, in- 
sertion between divided ar- 
teries, 112 

Union of— 

Divided ends of, 112 
With arteries, 112 
See also Varicose Veins 
Vena cava— 

Inferior, obstruction, complete, 
effect on venous pressure, 161 

Residual pressure in, 157 

Obstruction of, effects, 159, 160 

Venous blood, effect of deep abdomi- 
nal respiration on, 147 

Venous congestion, value of, 172 

Venous pulse, 84 

a-c interval, 89, 93, 104, 109 

‘“ A ” wave, factors producing, 95 

“A”? wave, indication of auri- 
cular contraction, 95-97 

“A”? wave, factors producing, 
97, 99 

‘“C”? wave synchronous with 
primary wave of carotid, 89 


Depressions ey x: 99 #66 5. ead oe y 25 
87-91 | 

Elevations (Sa; “é Ce “ce ve 
waves), 87-91 

“Vv” wave, 89, 90, 102 

“XxX” and ‘“‘x’” depressions, 


factors producing, 105 

‘““y ”’ depression, factors pro- 
ducing, 90, °94, 107 

Ventricle— 

Beating, action of drugs on, 53 

Contractility of, 50 

Contraction of both, graphic re- 
cords of, 73 

Degree of excitability, 49 

Distribution of ganglion-cells in, 


4 
Effect of high intra-cardiac pres- | 


sure on action of, 70 
Of heart in man, 44 
Movements during cakdiac cycle, 
68 
Movements of, time relations, 77 
Muscle of, 67 
Pressure in— 
Curves of, 79 
Experiments on, 75 


Printed by BALLANTYNE, HANSON & Co, 
Edinburgh & London . 


INDEX 


Ventricle (continued)— 
Stimulation of, result, 57 
Systole of, 103, 105, 107 
Wall of, alteration in tension, 49 

Ventricle and auricle— 

Muscular connection between, 
39, 63 
See also 
Bundle 

Ventricular cycle— 

Relationship to auricular, 82 
And standard movements, 77 

Ventricular extra-systole, 74 

Ventricular systole, 69 

Ventro-lateral pulmonary 

See Sterno-costal 
Vestibule, terminal air space, 185 
Viscera, abdominal upward draught 

of, 201 

Visceroptosis, 200 

Vision— 

Colour, theories of, 351 
Dichromatic systems of, 423 
Effect of colour on, 353 

In birds, 374 

Progressive evolution of, 299 
‘* Recurrent,” 365 

Twilight, agency of, 371 

Sce also Colour vision 

Visual mechanisms, theory of, 371, 375 

Visual sensations, Hering’s Sheory of, 

412 
Visual stimuli, normal, 387 
Visuo-psychie region of the cortex 

cerebri, 292 
Visuo-sensory area of the cortex 

cerebri, 291, 296, 299, 306 


Auriculo-Ventricular 


surface. 


WarmtTH, rapid formation of callus 
under action of, 172 

Water, adsorption and chemical 
combination of, 10, 13 

Weight lifting, effect on blood pres- 
sure, 235 

Word-centres, 324 

Words, language and thought, 323 

Work, muscular, physiology of, 208- 
257 


YOHINBIM— 
Administration of— ; 
Effect on blood flow, nig as 
Effect on nerve fati 


Young-Helmholtz theory o nate ra 
sensations, 412 

Z1G-ZAG experiment of Engelmann, : 
60, 63 GaN 

Zincativity of injured nerve, 260 ~ ;, 


R. EDwarD ARNOLD’ § LisT 
MEDICAL BOOKS. 


Farther Advances in Physiology. 


EDITED By LEONARD HILL, M.B., F.R.S. 


With Contributions from Professor BENJAMIN MOORE, University 

1; MARTIN FLACK, B.A., M.B., Oxon.; LEONARD HILL, 
3B. Lecturer on Physiology, London Hospital ; Dr. THOMAS LEWIS: 
. ARTHUR KEITH, Curator of the Hunterian Museum, Royal College 
"Surgeons ; Dr. M.S. PEMBREY, Lecturer on Physiology, Guy's 
tal ; Dr. N. ALCOCK, Lecturer on Physiology, St. Mary’s Hospital ; 
OSEPH SHAW BOLTON ; MAJOR GREENWOOD, M.R.C.S., 
tor of Statistical Department, London Hospital. 


: 448 pages. With Diagrams and Index. Demy 8vo. 15s. net. 


Recent Advances in Physiology and Bio- 
a Chemistry. 
EDITED By LEONARD HILL, M.B., F.R.S. 


ConTRIBUTORS : 

BENJAMIN MOORE, M.A., D.Sc.; LEONARD HILL, M.B., F.R.S.; 
J. R. MACLEOD, M.B., Professor of Physiology, Western Reserve 

Jniversity, Cleveland, U. S.A.; late Demonstrator of Physiology, the 

5 ospital ; M.S. PEMBREY, MA., M.D.; and A. P, BEDDARD, 

M.A., M.D., Assistant Physician, late Demonstrator of Physiology, Guy's 

Hospital. 

xii+740 pages. With Diagrams. ae 8vo., cloth, 18s. net. 


Practical Physiology. 


y A. P. BEDDARD, M.A., res S. EDKINS, M.A., M. B. ; 
LEONARD HILL, M.B., F.R.S.; J. J. R. MACLEOD, M. B. ; 

and M. S. PEMBREY, M.A., M.D. 

SECOND EDITION. 

vi+503 pages. With Illustrations. Demy 8vo., cloth, 12s. 6d. net. 

From the LANCET. 

why can oes the student that, armed with the knowledge that 

sedulously following the scheme of work laid down 
im in Seder fashion in addition to his usual studies, he will be 
qu os the examination of patients and the diagnosis of 

se. e application of the facts that are given, if thoroughly 


d, will the work of his life i in the sick-room, and will dis- 


2 Myr. Edward Arnold's Medical Books 


Human Embryology and Morphology. 


By ARTHUR KEITH, M.D. ABERD., F.R.C.S. ENG., 


Lecturer on Anatomy, London Eipagital Medical College; formerly Hunterian Professor, 
Royal College of Surgeons, England, Examiner in Anatomy, University of 
Aberdeen, and in the Natural Science Tripos, University of Cambridge. 


SECOND EDITION. REVISED AND ENLARGED. 
xii + 402 pages, with 316 Illustrations in the text, of which 64 are new 
in this edition. Demy 8vo., cloth, 12s. 6d. net. 


Applied Physiology. 
A Handbook for Students of Medicine. 


By ROBERT HUTCHISON, M.D. Eprn., F.R.C.P., 
Physician to the London Hospital and Assistant Physician to the Hospital for Sick Children. 


Crown 8vo. Price 7s. 6d. net. 


Lectures on Diseases of Children, 
By ROBERT HUTCHISON, M.D. EDIN., F.R.C.P. 
NEW AND ENLARGED EDITION. 
xii+428 pages. With 64 Illustrations. Crown 8vo., cloth, 8s. 6d, net. 


Manual of Human Physiology. 


By LEONARD HILL, M.B., F.R.S., 


Lecturer on Physiology, London Hospital Medical School; Hunterian Professor, Royal 
College of Surgeons ; Examiner in Physiology for the Conjoint Board of the 
Royal Colleges of Physicians and Surgeons. 


xii+ 484 pages, with 173 Illustrations. Crown 8vo., cloth, 6s. 


The Physiological Action of Drugs. 
An Introduction to Practical Pharmacology. 
By M. S. PEMBREY, M.A., M.D., 
Lecturer on Physiology, Guy’s Hospital : 
AND C. D. F. PHILLIPS, M.D., LL.D., 


Examiner in Materia Medica and Therapeutics in the University of Aberdeen, 
late Examiner in the Universities of Edinburgh and Glasgow. 


With 68 Diagrams and Tracings. Demy 8vo., cloth, 4s. 6d. net. 


The Diagnosis of Nervous Diseases. 


By PURVES STUART, M.A., M.D., F.R.C.P., j 

Physician to Out-Patients at the Westminster Hopital ; Joint-Lecturer on Medicine in the 
Medical School ; Physician to the Royal National mee PTY Hospital ; 

Assistant Physician to the Italian Hospital. - 

New EDITION, REVISED AND ENLARGED. ae 

450 pages. With Illustrations from the Author’s Photographs, and 

; Coloured Plates. Demy 8vo., 15s, net. : . cen 


| Fe 


, = ' am 
sh i 
ed hy : ta 
. en 
~— 
; 


Mr. Edward Arnold's Medical Books 3 


A Guide to the 
Diseases of the Nose and Throat, 
and their Treatment. 


By CHARLES A. PARKER, F.R.C.S. EDIN., 
Surgeon to the Throat Hospital, Golden Square, W. 


xii+624 pages. With 254 Illustrations, Demy 8vo., cloth, 18s. net. 


Movable Kidney: its Pathology, Symptoms, 
and Treatment. 


By H. W. WILSON, M.B., B.S. LOND., F.R.C.S. ENG., 
Demonstrator of Anatomy, St. Bartholomew's Hospital ; 


AND 


-. C.M. HINDS HOWELL, M.A., M.B., M.R.C.P., LOND., 
Junior Demonstrator of Physiology, St. Bartholomew's Hospital. 


Demy 8vo., 4s. 6d. net. 


A Handbook of Skin Diseases and their 
Treatment. 


By ARTHUR WHITFIELD, M.D. LoOnND., F.R.C.P., 


Professor of eermenclony at King’s College ; Physician to the Skin Departments, 
King’s College and the Great orthern Central Hospitals. 


xii+ 320 pages. With Illustrations. Crown 8vo., 8s. 6d. net. 


A Manual of Pharmacology for Students. 


By WALTER E. DIXON, M.A. CanTAB., M.D., B.S., 
B.Sc. LOND., D. P.H. CAMB., 


Assistant to the Downing Professor of Medicine in the University of Cambridge ; ; 
Examiner in Pharmacology in the Universities of Cambridge and Glasgow. 


xii+451 pages. With Illustrations. Demy 8vo., cloth, 15s. net. 


The Laws of Health. 


| By D. NABARRO, M.D., B.Sc., D.P.H., 
Assistant Professor of Pathology and Bacteriology, University College, College, London. 


viilit+ 184 pages. With Illustrations. Crown 8vo., cloth, 1s. 6d. 


The Body at Work. 


By ALEX HILL, M.A., M.D., 
Recently Master of Downing ep Spey ; Author of “ An Introduction 


With Illustrations. Demy 8vo., 16s. net. 


4 Mr. Edward Arnolds Medical Books 


Food and the Principles of Dietetics. 


By ROBERT HUTCHISON, M.D. Epin., F.R.C.P., 


Physician to the London Hospital and Assistant-Physician to the Hospital pi Sick 
hildren, Great Ormond Street. 


xx +582 pages, with 3 Plates in Colour and _ 34 Illustrations in the 
text. Demy 8vo., red buckram, 16s. net. 


The Influence of Alcohol and other Drugs 
on Fatigue. 


By W. H. RIVERS, M.D., F.R.C.P., F.R.S., 4 
Lecturer in Physiological and Experimental perches at Cambridge University. 


Royal 8vo., 6s. net. 


The Chemical Investigation of 
Gastric and Intestinal Diseases by the Aid © 
of Test Meals. 


By VAUGHAN HARLEY, M.D., EDIN., M.R.C.P., F.C.S., 
Professor of Pathological Chemistry, University College, London ; 


AND FRANCIS GOODBODY, M.D., DueL., M R.G.P., 


Assistant-Professor of Pathological Chemistry, University College, London. 
Demy 8vo., 8s. 6d. net. 


BOOKS FOR NURSES. 


Surgical Nursing and the Principles of 
Surgery for Nurses. 
_ By RUSSELL HOWARD, M.B., M.S., F.R.C.S., 


Lecturer on Surgical Nursing to the aA Rh of the cpa Hospital. 
With Illustrations. Crown 8vo., 6s. 


Midwifery for Nurses. 


By HENRY RUSSELL ANDREWS, M.D., B.Sc., LonD., 
M.R.C.P. LOND., 
Examiner to the Central Midwives Board. 


New Edition. With Illustrations. Crown 8vo., 4s. 6d. net. 


A Text-Book of Nursing. 
By C. S. WEEKS-SHAW. 


Revised and largely Rewritten by W. RADFORD, Senior Resident — 
Medical Officer, Poplar Hospital. 


xiv+ 344 pages. With numerous Illustrations. 3s. 6d. 
ri t 
Lonpon: EDWARD ARNOLD, 41 & 43 Mappox St., Bonp St., W. 


. 
mat 
. 


ne VE hei 


hysiology 


Hill, Sir Leenacd Erskine 
Further advances in p 


, 
> un