Skip to main content

Full text of "Genius In The Making"

See other formats


136. 765 C31g (2) 




Keep Your Card in This Pocket 

Books will be issued only on presentation of proper 
library cards. 

Unless labeled otherwise, books may be retained 
for two weeks. Borrowers finding books marked, de- 
faced or mutilated are expected to report same at 
library desk; otherwise the last borrower will be held 
responsible for all imperfections discovered. 

The card holder is responsible for all books drawn 
on this card. 

Penalty for over-due books 2c a day plus cost of 
notices. 

Lost cards and change of residence must be re- 
ported promptly. 

Public Library 

Kansas City, Mo. 
Keep Your Card in This Pocket 




ECO., K. 0,, MO. 



D DDD1 D271flab 3 



r/ ,4 





AUGO 71W3 



McGRAW-HELL SERIES IN EDUCATION 
HAROLD BENJAMIN, CONSULTING EDIT'OB 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



McGraw-Hill 
Series in Education 

HAEOLD BENJAMIN 

CONSTH.TINQ EDITOB 

The Stanford University Education Faculty THE 
CHALLENGE OF EDUCATION 

Bowden and Melbo SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY OP 
EDUCATION 

Broom EDUCATIONAL MEASUREMENTS IN THE 
ELEMENTARY SCHOOL 

Brubacher MODERN PHILOSOPHIES OF EDUCA- 
TION 

Butts THE COLLEGE CHARTS ITS COURSE 
Carroll GENIUS IN THE MAKING 
Croxton SCIENCE IN THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL 
Grinnell INTERPRETING THE PUBLIC SCHOOLS 

Heck THE EDUCATION OF EXCEPTIONAL CHIL- 
DREN 

Horrall and Others LET'S Go TO SCHOOL 

Jones, Grizzell and Grinstead PRINCIPLES OF UNIT 
CONSTRUCTION 

McKown ACTIVITIES IN THE ELEMENTARY SCHOOL 

Newlon EDUCATION FOR DEMOCRACY IN OUR 
TIME 

Pringle THE JUNIOR HIGH SCHOOL 

/Sears CITY SCHOOL ADMINISTRATIVE CONTROLS 

Sorenson PSYCHOLOGY IN EDUCATION 

Thorpe PSYCHOLOGICAL FOUNDATIONS OF PER- 
SONALITY 

Updegraff and Others PRACTICE nsr PRESCHOOL 

EDUCATION 
Wert EDUCATIONAL STATISTICS 

Wilson, Stone and Dalrymple TEACHING THE 
NEW ARITHMETIC 

Winslow THE INTEGRATED SCHOOL ART PRO- 
GRAM 



GENIUS 
IN THE MAKING 



BY 
HERBERT A. ^CARROLL 

Formerly Assistant Professor of Educational 
Psychology, University of Minnesota 



FIRST EDITION 



McGRAW-HILL BOOK COMPANY, INC. 

NEW YOKE AND LONDON 
1940 



COPYRIGHT, 1940, BY THE 

BOOK COMPANY, INC. 



IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMEEICA 

All rights reserved. This look, or 

parts thereof, may not be reproduced 

in any form without permission of 

the publishers. 



To the Memory of 
PBOFESSOB LETA S. HOLLINGWORTH 



PREFACE 

Intellect is, without doubt, the greatest single endow- 
ment of the human race. It has made possible the most 
prized achievements of the past and will make possible 
the realization of the highest hopes for the future. It is 
the primary source of all progress. Granting the vast 
importance of intellect in the affairs of man, it surely 
follows that it is desirable to nurture it, especially where 
it is found to exist in high degree. Although all men may 
contribute to the march of civilization, the new roads are 
blazed by men of genius. 

During the last quarter of a century research workers 
have gathered considerable information concerning men- 
tally superior individuals. Most of this factual material 
relates to intellectually gifted children who are not in 
themselves geniuses but rather geniuses in the making. 
Genius, still an indefinable term, is something more than 
great intellectual capacity or even the summation of 
certain traits that can be quantitatively evaluated. That 
is why it can be truthfully said that not all gifted children 
will become eminent men. On the other hand, it is 
equally true that all who achieve eminence were gifted 
as children. 

In the following pages the author has attempted to 
present a picture of the intellectually gifted individual, 
especially as he appears in childhood. In discussing his 
mental, social, and physical characteristics and the 
educational adjustments necessary to his progress, full 
use is made of the results of research* However, being 
aware that the conclusions so far arrived at objectively 
do not tell the entire story, the author has not hesitated 

vii 



PREFACE 

to present a point of view, an interpretation of genius, 
that occasionally goes beyond statistical data. 

Since this volume is primarily concerned with a 
psychological description and interpretation of mental 
superiority, it has been necessary to restrict to a relatively 
small amount of space the discussion of educational 
adjustments, important though these are. Democracy 
can be expected to survive only where its system of 
education provides for the needs of all children, the 
bright as well as the average and dull. 

Anyone writing on the subject of intellectually gifted 
children today must draw heavily upon the work of two 
outstanding American psychologists, Prof. Lewis M. 
Terman and the late Prof. Leta S. Hollingworth. To 
them and to the many other writers in the field the 
author is much indebted. 

H. A. CARROLL. 

SOUTH BERWICK, MAINE, - 
May, 1940. 



Vlll 



CONTENTS 

PAGE 

PREFACE vii 

EDITOR'S INTRODTJCTIOK. . xi 

CSAPTEB 

I. IDENTIFICATION OF INTELLECTUALLY GIFTED CHILDREN ... 3 

Parents' Judgments , 6 

Teachers 7 Judgments 7 

Intelligence Tests 12 

II. RACIAL AND FAMILY BACKGROUND 20 

The Negro. 21 

The Indian 22 

Nationality Groups 23 

Families of Geniuses 25 

III. THE ROLE OF ENVIRONMENT 35 

Transmission of Mental Inferiority 38 

Transmission of Mental Superiority 38 

Mental Similarity of Twins 39 

Effect of Changing Environment 43 

Conclusions 52 

IV. PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 54 

Reasons for Belief in Inferior Physical Equipment 54 

Physical Characteristics 59 

Participation in Athletics 75 

Physical Health 78 

Mental Health 82 

V. SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 89 

Happiness 89 

Character Development of Gifted Children 94 

Character Traits of Eminent Men 95 

Play Activities 97 

Leadership 110 

VI. MENTAL CHARACTERISTICS AND ACHIEVEMENTS 114 

Mental Qualities 115 

Scholastic Attainments 126 

Progress Quotients 132 

ix 



CONTENTS 

CHAPTER . PAGE 

VII. CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 134 

Looking Backward 139 

Looking Forward 149 

Summary 159 

VIII. DETERMINERS OF EMINENCE 161 

Intellect Versus Achievement 161 

College Grades and Business Success 163 

Intrinsic Factors 166 

Extrinsic Factors 178 

IX. SPECIAL GIFTS 186 

Music . . 187 

Drawing 190 

Lightning Calculators 194 

Mechanical Ability 195 

Special Language Abilities 197 

Summary 205 

X. EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCBLEKATION 206 

Issues Involved '. . 206 

Acceleration 219 

XI. EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ENRICHMENT 243 

/totraclass Grouping 243 

Enrichment at Home 249 

Principles of Education of Gifted Children in Special Classes 253 
The Teacher 262 

XII. BIOGRAPHY OF A TYPICAL GIFTED CHILD . . ' 265 

Development during Infancy 266 

Preschool Period 271 

Elementary School Period 275 

At the End of Ten Years 292 

SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY 293 

INDEX. 297 



EDITOR'S INTRODUCTION 

In the stress and turmoil of a world which seems at 
times to be carrying a burden of ignorance and hatred 
greater than it can bear, the gifts of intelligence and good 
will are doubly precious. To envisage a solution of human 
problems in terms other than those based upon man's 
ability to learn and his willingness to help his fellows is 
to negate the principles of civilized education. To recog- 
nize outstanding ability and to develop it to its utmost is 
a chief task of this education. 

The present volume offers valuable aid to all parents 
and teachers who wish to perform this crucial task well. 
In clear and simple, but scholarly and comprehensive, 
fashion the book tells how intellectually gifted children 
may be identified, what they are like physically, socially, 
and mentally, and how they may be helped in childhood 
and youth to develop their great potentialities most 
effectively. 

The author is eminently qualified to perform this serv- 
ice. His long experience as a teacher in schools and uni- 
versities, his researches in the field of special intellectual 
gifts, and his thorough familiarity alike with psycho- 
logical studies and educational practices combine to 
give this book the precision of scholarship and the guid- 
ance of common sense which every good professional 
work needs. Behind these qualities, moreover, there is 
the greater quality of a generous and inquiring mind at 
work on a subject of absorbing interest. An appreciation 
of that quality may best be gained from the book itself. 

HAKOL.D BENJAMIN. 

UNIVERSITY OF MARYLAND, 
June, 1940. 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



CHAPTER I 

IDENTIFICATION OF INTELLECTUALLY 
GIFTED CHILDREN 

1. What is meant by the term "intellectually gifted"? 

2. What are the underlying causes of the errors made in subjective 
estimates of intelligence? 

3. What method of identifying intellectually gifted children is the 
best? 

It has long been customary to think of a "gift" as a 
specific, isolated aptitude in some artistic field music, 
painting, acting, writing, sculpture. These abilities, 
when sufficiently marked to be important, seem easily 
recognizable and familiar. The gift of high intelligence, 
on the other hand, is at present recognized and adjusted 
to with somewhat greater difficulty; yet it is probably the 
most important single attribute of man., and those 
possessing it in marked degree constitute the greatest 
asset of the human race. Great as may be any special, 
gift artistic or social it cannot go far without the gift 
of intellect as well. The more any man knows, the more 
he can do. 

Through the use of objective measuring instruments, 
it has been definitely established that intelligence is 
distributed over what is termed the normal-probability 
curve (see Fig. 1). 

At the extreme left of Fig. 1 stands the idiot, a human 
being whose intelligence is less than that of many 
animals. Directly above him is the imbecile/ who is capa- 
ble of simple learning but cannot master such intricate 
subjects as reading and arithmetic. Next in order is the 

3 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

moron, who is not nearly so numerous as he is popularly 
supposed to be. In fact, all three of these groups, usually 
classified together as " feeble-minded/' constitute only 
about 2 per cent of the total population. In terms of 
intelligence quotient 1 they range from to 70. 

Progressing upward by infinitesimal steps for human 
beings differ not in kind but in degree of intelligence 
through what is called for convenience the borderline and 
dull-normal groups, the great normal class, numbering 
60 per cent of the total population, is reached. These 




130 
I.Q. 

FIG. 1. Position and frequency of intellectually gifted children in relation to 
the genera/I population. 

average human beings range from 90 to 110 in terms of 
I.Q. As children they set the pace for grade school work; 
as adults they control the tempo of progress. Professor 
H. L. Hollingworth 2 has given a revealing picture of a 
hypothetical average man whose meager abilities should 
be kept in mind for reference and comparison in consider- 
ing the intellectual achievements of gifted individuals. 
The following passages are taken from Hollingworth/s 
description: 



intelligence quotient is arrived at by the following formula: 
M.A./C.A. = I.Q. M.A. stands for " mental age," which is determined by 
comparing the score which the child earns on an intelligence test with the 
test standards or norms. If his score is the same as that earned by the 
typical child of nine, then he is said to have a mental age of nine. If his 
C.A. (chronological age) is also nine, then he is of average intelligence. 
M.A./C.A. - 9/9 = 1.00 I.Q. (commonly expressed as 100 I.Q,). 

2 HOLLINGWORTH, H. L., "Mental Growth and Decline," pp. 276-279, 
D. Appleton-Century Company, Inc., New York, 1928. 

4 



IDENTIFICATION OF INTELLECTUALLY GIFTED CHILDREN 

Such an individual would leave school at the eighth grade, with a 
working knowledge of the "fundamentals," a smattering of local 
geography, a bit of history, and a few elementary facts of physiology, 
There would be no knowledge of a general kind hi such fields as 
literature, civics, science, politics. The individual would speak only 
his mother tongue, and would be intellectually unable to do satis- 
factory high-school work of the traditional kind. 

He might, if a male, through apprenticeship or after a short period 
of industrial training, become a plumber, a carpenter, a policeman, a 
mechanic. He would have a vocabulary of about 7,500 words, a little 
over half that of the ordinary high-school graduate. If a woman, she 
would be a competent housekeeper or plain nurse, or a mediocre or 
inferior clerical worker. . . . 

He can tell how many pencils can be bought for fifty cents if two 
pencils cost five cents, and how much seven feet of cloth will cost at 
fifteen cents a yard, if allowed one minute for each problem. But if 
told that hi a large box there are four small boxes, each containing 
four smaller ones, he is unable hi a reasonable time to tell the total 
number of boxes hi the collection. He can explain what a simple 
picture means, but is still unable to tell what more than half of 
the simple fables, which he is told, are supposed to teach, in a 
manner which suggests capacity for generalization from specific 
instances. . . . 

In spite of these meager endowments, it is the average man and 
the average woman that most often marry, become parents, are the 
autocratic dictators of a family of children, determine the results at 
popular elections. That they do not originate the measures for which 
they vote, invent the machinery they operate, or plan the curriculums 
they authorize, shows that, in spite of numbers, it is the contribution 
of the superior endowment that determines the course, although 
perhaps not the pace, of social development* 

If we look at the figure showing the distribution of 
intelligence, it will be seen that progress is continued 
from the normal group through the superior and very 
superior classes, with their steadily decreasing relative 
frequency, to the shaded area which represents those with 
intelligence quotients of 130 and above the 1 per cent 
of the population called "intellectually gifted. 7 ' There 
are approximately 250,000 such children in the United 

5 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

States. It is important to keep in mind that not all 
children identified as possessing potentialities for genius 
will actually achieve on a high level when they reach 
adult years. Intelligence is but one of the determiners of 
success. A man cannot achieve eminence without pos- 
sessing a great intellect, but he can possess a great 
intellect and yet fail to achieve eminence. 

There are a number of possible ways of identifying these 
children. Parents 7 judgment, teachers' judgment, intelli- 
gence tests, age-grade status, standardized achievement 
tests, and school marks constitute the usual criteria for 
selection. No one of these is completely valid, although 
the intelligence test is by far the best. Whenever possible, 
a combination of estimates should be used. 

PARENTS' JUDGMENTS 

The opinions of parents concerning the intelligence of 
their children, though usually biased, have some value 
because of parents' intimate and detailed knowledge of 
offspring. No teacher or psychologist can hope to have as 
complete a picture of a child as does his father or mother. 
This intimate knowledge is especially important for 
helping to evaluate the intelligence of preschool children. 
The psychologist, in testing the intelligence of the young 
child, frequently finds it difficult to gain his cooperation 
and so leans heavily upon the observations of the child's 
parents. If the parents themselves are equipped with 
psychological information, their statements may be of 
great value. Some of the most exact data on early mental 
development are contained in biographies of children 
written by a mother or father who reports with scientific 
objectivity on the activities of his child. 

In general, however, parents are notoriously poor 
judges of the intellectual capacity of their children. Their 
errors come from three sources: bias, inaccurate observa- 

6 



IDENTIFICATION OF INTELLECTUALLY GIFTED CHILDREN 

tion, and failure to keep in mind the total child popula- 
tion. Obviously parents are eager that their child shall 
be bright though not too bright and naturally they 
see everything that he does in a favorable light and find 
excuses for his failures. Their pardonable prejudice so 
affects their observation of the child's behavior that most 
baby books are filled with inaccurate statements. A 
meaningless sound uttered by a baby in vocal play is 
recorded as a word, a grimace from gas on the stomach is 
interpreted as a social smile, and the reciting of a memor- 
ized verse is noted as reading. The confidence of his 
parents is of course heartening and necessary to the 
child, but it unquestionably affects the validity of their 
judgment concerning his intelligence. 

A final, and perhaps equally important, source of error 
in the judgments of parents is their failure to keep in 
mind the wide intellectual differences between various 
socioeconomic groups. For example, a doctor and his 
wife, each of whom is intellectually gifted and whose 
friends are, in general, much superior in mentality to the 
population as a whole, are very likely to think of their 
child as "just an average boy," even though he is one in 
a hundred or even one in a thousand. An intellect that in 
a heterogeneous group would appear markedly superior 
in this select group shows to no advantage. In identifying 
gifted children, then, the opinions of parents should be 
used only with many reservations. 

TEACHERS' JUDGMENTS 

Any validity of the judgments of teachers concerning 
the intelligence of the pupils in their classes rests upon 
their professional training and their familiarity with the 
schoolwork of the child. Their opinions, in general, are of 
more value than those of parents even though they 
frequently disagree sharply among themselves. Most of 

7 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

the younger teachers of the present day have had some 
training in psychology, and even a slight contact with 
this science is likely to impress upon one the fact of the 
wide range of human abilities and of the difficulties 
involved in subjectively rating them. If a teacher with 
such training is asked to select the brightest children in 
her classes, she may be expected to try conscientiously 
to ignore irrelevant factors and to consider intellectual 
behavior only. 

Occasionally an experienced teacher with no knowledge 
at all of academic psychology can, by drawing upon the 
information which she has gathered through many years 
of work in teaching children ranging in intelligence from 
the moron to the genius, estimate with considerable 
accuracy the mental capacity of a certain child. If she 
takes into consideration as well the grade of schoolwork 
that the child has been doing, especially the scores which 
he may have earned on objective achievement tests, her 
chances of success are greatly improved. 

In general, however, teachers have shown a surprising 
lack of ability to identify intellectually gifted children. 
Varner reports in a study made some years ago that 
teachers can select only about one-fourth of the bright 
children in their grade. (It is interesting to note that 
Varner reports in the same study that teachers are much 
more successful in selecting dull children, being able to 
identify about half of them.) Professor Leta S. Holling- 
worth 1 tells of an excellent teacher, with five years of 
experience in the elementary schools, who was asked to 
list the five most intelligent of the forty pupils in her 
class, which she had been teaching for three months. It 
was found later by test that two of these were bright, two 
average, and one dull. In the same discussion Prof. 

1 HOLLINGWOBTH, L. S., "Gifted Children," pp. 46-48, The Macmillan 

Company, New York, 1926. 

8 



IDENTIFICATION OP INTELLECTUAIXY GIFTED CHILDREN 

Hollingworth refers to the teacher who recommended a 
child as "extremely intelligent because he can play the 
ukelele and sing." 

In 1921-1922 Prof. Lewis Terman of Leland Stanford 
University began the most elaborate study of gifted 
children that has been made. This mQmTmp.nf.al piece of 
research must be referred to frequently in any discussion 
of genius. For the present, reference will be made only 
to the relative success which Terman had with the 
several methods that he used in selecting the 1,000 gifted 
children whom he was to study. His criteria were four in 
number: (1) teachers' ratings; (2) age-grade status; (3) 
achievement tests; (4) intelligence tests. Teachers 7 
ratings were found to be the least valuable of the four. 
Only 15.7 per cent of those nominated by 6,000 teach- 
ers, each as the most intelligent in his class, finally 
qualified for the gifted group. Age-grade status was found 
to be a more reliable criterion, yielding 19.7 per cent of 
the final group. As Terman 1 says, "If one would identify 
the brightest child in a class of thirty to fifty pupils, it is 
better to consult the birth records in the class, register 
than to ask the teacher's opinion. 7 ' 

The underlying causes of errors in teachers 7 judgments 
are not difficult to locate. They can be grouped under 
three headings: (1) the inevitable intrusion of the per- 
sonal equation; (2) lack of standards as a basis for 
comparison; (3) f failure to consider the important factor 
of chronological age differences. 

Intrusion of Personal Equation 

One of the greatest difficulties with which the scientist 
is constantly faced is how to keep his own prejudices, 
his own likes and dislikes, from influencing the results of 

1 TERMAN, L. M., "Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. I, p. 33, 2nd ed., 
Stanford University Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1926. 

9 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Ms experiments. It is especially difficult to do this in a 
science such as psychology, economics, or sociology, 
which involves human beings. Even in the exact sciences 
the experimenter's desire to establish a certain postulate 
may influence his procedure and his conclusions unless he 
uses the greatest care. It is not surprising, then, that in 
so subjective a matter as rendering a judgment on the 
intelligence of a child the teacher should be influenced by 
such irrelevant factors as friendliness, physical beauty, 
willingness to work, cooperation, obedience, and loquac- 
ity. Since intelligence and curiosity are closely related 
traits, the truly gifted child is quite likely to ask many 
questions, some of them embarrassing in their difficulty. 
Many teachers respond antagonistically to these ques- 
tions, and, since it is a human failing to belittle those 
disliked, tend to underrate the intelligence of the ques- 
tioner. On the other hand, a friendly, cooperative child, 
who makes the teacher feel comfortable when he is with 
h'er, is likely to be thought brighter than he actually is. 
It is no wonder that the teacher, controlled as are most 
human beings by emotion rather than by intellect, is 
unable to judge clear-sightedly when .asked to identify 
the gifted children in her classes. 

Lack of Standards 

A difficulty which the teacher, in common with others, 
faces in rating an individual on any trait whatsoever is 
comparing that individual not with the members of a 
select group, but with a large heterogeneous group which 
is truly representative. This difficulty lies at the source of 
much of the fallacious thinking concerning geniuses, 
young and old. For example, it is frequently maintained 
that certain presidents of the United States were average 
or even below average in intellect. The man who makes 
that statement overlooks the fact that in his thinking he 

10 



IDENTIFICATION OF INTELLECTUALLY GIFTED CHILDREN 

is concerned only with a very select group made up of 
presidents and perhaps a few other leading American 
statesmen. If he allowed himself to consider seriously the 
mental equipment of the truly average man he would 
recognize the absurdity of his statement. To give another 
example, a football player who weighs a mere 165 pounds 
is referred to in the sports columns as small. He is small 
when compared with the average football player, but 
when compared with the average college man he is 
considerably above the median. 

With respect to intelligence the child is usually com- 
pared by his teacher with the members of his group. If he 
is a boy somewhat above average in intellectual capacity 
in a small country school attended by a group of rather 
dull children, he will appear to marked advantage. It is 
easy to see how his teacher might consider that she had a 
genius in her school, and that she would be very much 
surprised if an intelligence test indicated that he fell well 
below the top 1 per cent. On the other hand, a child of 
average intelligence in a school like the Horace Mann 
School in New York would appear to be dull because he 
would be competing with a group containing many 
children who were intellectually gifted. Wherever the 
individual is, he will inevitably be compared with those 
about him. The teacher, though less likely than others to 
err in this respect because of her greater experience with 
large numbers of children, is, nevertheless, affected to 
some extent by the great difficulty involved in attempt- 
ing to keep in mind a large unselected group of children 
while rating a single child. 

Chronological Age Factor 

A third source of errqr in teachers' judgments concern- 
ing the intelligence of children is the factor of chronologi- 
cal age. If, for example, a teacher is working with a group 

11 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

of ten-year-old cMldren of average intelligence and there 
is in that group an eight-year-old child who is doing as 
well as the others, the instructor may very easily overlook 
the difference of two years in age especially if the 
younger child happens to be as large as the others and 
consider that his intelligence is about the same as that 
of the older children. Actually, of course, his intellectual 
capacity would have to be very much greater to make 
it possible for him to do the same kind of work that the 
others are doing. 

An actual case was reported some time ago of two 
sisters, aged three and four. The father had brought them 
to a psychologist to have them tested, remarking that he 
was somewhat worried about the younger child because 
she did not seem to be nearly so bright as her sister. The 
psychologist, on testing them, found that the three- 
year-old girl was intellectually gifted while her four-year- 
old sister had a considerably lower I.Q. The father had 
been expecting the younger child to behave as intelli- 
gently as the older one, quite overlooking the year's 
difference in their chronological ages. It had been espe- 
cially easy for him to do this because the two children 
were of almost the same size. 

INTELLIGENCE TESTS 

The most satisfactory approach in evaluating the 
intelligence of a child is through an appropriate intel- 
ligence test. At the very beginning of any discussion of 
intelligence testing it is important to note that so far no 
way of measuring intellectual capacity directly has been 
devised and none will be until it is known what actually 
goes on in the nervous and glandular systems when an 
individual learns. A number of theories have been 
advanced, but in all honesty it must be admitted that 
concerning this important matter psychologists are as 

12 



IDENTIFICATION OF INTELLECTUALLY GIFTED CHILDREN 

completely ignorant as were medical men of bacteriology 

before the day of Pasteur. Lacking information on the 

physical basis of mental behavior, psychologists can only 

judge intelligence by obtaining ratings on observed 

^intellectual behavior. This indirect approach results in a 

^greater possibility of error than would be likely if a direct 

^attack could be made. Nevertheless, it has been fruitful, 

(^resulting in evaluations which have proved to be surpris- 

Mngly accurate. 

^ In measuring the intelligence of an individual, it is 
obviously necessary to determine what constitutes intel- 
lectual behavior. This is no easy task. Every individual 
frequently passes judgment on the brightness or dullness 
^ of his fellows. The criteria which he uses are interesting 
.and revealing. If, for example, a group of people in 
^informal discussion were asked to name what they 
^considered were cues to, or even absolute proof of, 
intellectual capacity, the following would almost cer- 
tainly be mentioned: sparkling eyes, being ready with an 
answer, long hair (genius), wearing glasses, pale skin, 
learning quickly, learning slowly but always remember- 
ing, reciting long poems in infancy, walking when very 
young, reading at the age of three or four, large head, 
being physically small, being physically large, shape of 
hands, shape of mouth, receding chin, pugnacious chin, 
very beautiful (lack of intelligence), sociability, mechani- 
cal ability, ability to see through a problem quickly, 
ability to use what he knows, common sense, date of 
birth, known intelligence of brothers and sisters, national- 
ity, sex, school marks, ability in music, ability in art, 
friendliness, obedience, shape of head, size of nose, and so 
on and on. 

These criteria have been listed at random, much as 
they might come from such a group. No attempt has 
been made at order or classification, the wholly irrelevant 

13 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

items being left to stand side by side with those which are 
relevant. The psychologist, in constructing an intelligence 
test, considers all such criteria and then eliminates those 
which previous investigations have shown to be without 
value in indicating intelligence. He knows, for example, 
that, though there is a slight positive relationship 
between physical size and mental capacity, such charac- 
teristics as size of head (excepting when the variation is 
extreme as in hydrocephalis and microcephalis), shape 
of mouth or nose or chin, and so on, reveal nothing 
concerning the intelligence of the individual. He knows 
that sparkling eyes are as frequently found in an institu- 
tion for feeble-minded children as in an opportunity 
class for gifted children. Although recognizing the great 
importance of specialized ability in mechanics, music, 
or art, he knows, from the large number of studies that 
have been made, that superiority in one of these fields 
by no means guarantees superiority in intellect. 

On the other hand, he considers that the ability to 
learn to read at the age of three or f our, to see through *a 
problem quickly, to apply knowledge, or to possess what 
is known as common sense indicates superior intelligence. 
Binet defines intelligence as " judgment or common 
sense, initiative, the ability to ad'apt oneself," Terman 
as "the ability to think in terms of abstract ideas/' 
Woodrow as "an acquiring capacity," Buckingham as 
"the ability to learn/' and Woodworth as the ability 
of an individual "to see the point of the problem set 
him, and to adapt what he has learned to this novel 
situation." 

Validity 

Having decided what constitutes intellectual behavior, 
the 'test maker devises and selects items for his test which 
will rate a child on these intellectual traits. Any item 

14 



IDENTIFICATION OF INTELLECTUALLY GIFTED CHILDREN 

which tests an unrelated ability, even in part, weakens 
the test. Since it is impossible, in measuring intelligence 
indirectly, to eliminate completely such disturbing 
factors as speed of handwriting, speed of reading (which 
may be determined by defective eyesight), and nervous 
tension, the score earned by a child on a mental test is not 
a perfect rating; it is merely the best rating that psycholo- 
gists at the present time know how to secure and is, for 
reasons that will be pointed out later, much superior to 
one subjectively given. 

No attempt at anything even approaching a complete 
analysis of test items will be made in this brief description 
of intelligence tests. It will be interesting, however, to 
note a few of the questions used, making special reference 
to the way in which they help the investigator to identify 
intellectually gifted children. For example, size of vocabu- 
lary is generally considered by psychologists as being the 
best single indication of intellectual capacity. This 
criterion is especially important in rating very young 
children, for the vocabulary of the intellectually inferior 
will always be small and that of the intellectually superior 
will nearly always be large. At the age of one year a child 
who is below average in intelligence will almost certainly 
be able to use no more than one word meaningfully. On 
the other hand, a very bright child will almost certainly 
have a vocabulary of several words perhaps a dozen 
or more. 

An interesting and valid test in the new, revised 
Stanford-Binet Tests the best of the existing instru- 
ments for measuring intelligence is the "plan of search." 
The psychologist presents to the child whom he is testing 
a drawing of a circle broken at one point only. He then 
tells him to imagine that the circle is a field and to show, 
by tracing his path with a pencil, how he would search 
for a purse of money lost in the field. 

15 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

In order to solve this problem the child must under- 
stand the directions, see the point of the problem, bring 
his experience to bear in working out a solution, and carry 
through the plan which he has considered to be the most 
promising. It has been found that an intellectually 
average thirteen-year-old child can pass this test. A dull 
child of the same age is greatly puzzled by it and may 
respond as did the thirteen-year-old boy who merely 
marked the paper at random (see Fig. 2). 





FIG. 2. "Plan of search" of a FIG, 3. " Plan of search " of a 

thirteen-year-old child with an I.Q. nine-year-old child with an I.Q. 
of 65. of 160. 

An intellectually gifted child of nine, four years 
younger than the boy who offered the plan shown in 
Fig. 2, solved the problem with insight and care in the 
manner shown in Fig. 3. 

The application of this single criterion leaves little 
doubt concerning the relative intellectual levels of these 
two children. When it is remembered that a good 
intelligence test utilizes not one but a large number of 
valid measures and applies them with statistical care, it 
appears reasonable to conclude that it is successful in 
at least roughly differentiating children according to 
their mental capacity. It is important, of course, that 
the individual items of an intelligence test be valid; that 
is, that each one really sample intellectual behavior. The 

16 



IDENTIFICATION OF INTELLECTUALLY GIFTED CHILDREN 

psychologist, in building his instrument, applies rigid 
objective tests to every part of it. He discards every 
question or problem that shows itself to be lacking in 
validity, keeping only those that have proved themselves 
as sound measures of mental ability. 

The most valid intelligence ratings for gifted children 
can be obtained between the ages of six and twelve. 
Scores earned by preschool children are frequently in 
error because of failure to meet one or both of the two 
assumptions underlying all intelligence testing: first, that 
the examinee cooperate fully; second, that the test items 
sample a body of information with which the examinee 
has had an opportunity to become familiar. Scores earned 
by older children are frequently in error because the 
brightest of these children reach the roof of the test; then- 
true abilities are not being evaluated. The selection of 
intellectually gifted children for purposes of classification 
or special instruction should be made, then, during the 
elementary school period, preferably during the first half 
of that period. 

Reliability 

The intelligence test is not only a more valid method of 
rating intelligence than any other, but it is also more 
reliable; that is, it measures consistently what it purports 
to measure. One of the serious difficulties involved in 
making subjective evaluations of intelligence has always 
been the considerable disagreement among those passing 
judgment. Investigations, for example, have revealed the 
fact that it is almost impossible to obtain a consensus of 
opinion among teachers with respect to the mentality 
of the pupils in their classes. Moreover, their individual 
judgments differ from year to year, or even from month 
to month. 

17 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Intelligence-test scores also vary somewhat from time 
to time and from test to test, but to no such marked 
degree as do subjective ratings. Personal judgments of 
intelligence, unsupported by data objectively gathered, 
rarely achieve a reliability coefficient as high as .50. 
Intelligence tests, on the other hand, possess coefficients 
of reliability considerably in excess of .50 the best of 
these, as for example the Stanford-Binet, rate over .90. 
A test should have a reliability coefficient of at least .85 
preferably higher if a score earned on it is to be used 
as a basis for individual prophecy and guidance. 

The best of the individual psychological examinations 
are the Stanford Revision of the Binet-Simon Scale, 
the Kuhlmann Revision of the Binet-Simon Scale, 
and the Minnesota Preschool Scale. There are a number 
of excellent group tests, although the coefficients of 
reliability for these instruments are seldom over .90. 
Among those used widely are the Haggerty Intelligence 
Examinations, the National Intelligence Test, the Ter- 
man Group Test of Mental Ability, and the Kuhlmann- 
Anderson Intelligence Test. 

Standards 

It was stated earlier in this discussion that one of the 
principal difficulties involved in subjectively rating 
intelligence is the lack of adequate standards. The 
individual who is passing judgment errs in his evaluation 
because he is forced to use as his criterion the minds of the 
people whom he knows. If he associates with persons of 
high mentality, he will think that a certain very bright 
child is merely average; if he associates with persons of 
low mentality, an average child will appear to him to be 
bright. The situation is similar to that which causes the 
rural visitor to find New York to be less spectacular than 
he expected; a twenty-story building in his home town 

13 



IDENTIFICATION OF INTELLECTUALLY GIFTED CHILDREN 

would be a skyscraper indeed, but in New York, among 
so many high buildings, even one of sixty stories seems 
small. 

The intelligence test does not completely eliminate the 
difficulties involved in making comparisons because it is 
impossible to obtain perfect standardization; it does, 
however, greatly improve upon subjective judgment in 
that it is accompanied by standard scores which represent 
the mental ratings of a large number of unselected 
children. Thousands of scores earned by children ranging 
from the idiot to the genius constitute the standard in 
some of the best intelligence tests. It is a far cry from this 
to the little group used for comparison by the layman in 
estimating the intelligence of a child. 



19 



CHAPTER II 
RACIAL AND FAMILY BACKGROUND 

1. Do races differ in their ability to produce men. of genius? 

2. Does great mental capacity run in families? 

3. From what occupational groups do intellectually gifted children 
come? 

In any discussion of the importance of race or nation- 
ality as a cause of high intelligence it should be made 
clear at the outset that the data gathered by anthro- 
pologists, biologists, and psychologists are, for the 
present, inadequate to justify final conclusions. The best 
that can be done until more exact evidence is obtained is 
to point out what these data indicate to be true. The 
biologist H. S. Jennings 1 adopts this attitude when he 
says: 

The diversity of genes in different races indisputably yields race 
differences in physical features in color, stature, structure, form, 
and the like. Among individuals belonging to the same race, diversity 
of genes produces differences in all these respects; and, moreover, 
produces differences in mentality; produces differences in power of 
adjustment to conditions met. It would be surprising if the same were 
not true for the differing genes of the differing races. 

Regardless of what ought to be so, according to the 
laws of inheritance, or what might be so, as a result of 
environmental conditions, it is an observable and tested 
fact that certain racial and nationality groups in the 
United States produce a smaller number of intellectually 
gifted children than do certain other racial and nation- 

1 JENNINGS, H. S., "The Biological Basis of Human Nature," p. 284, 
W. W. Norton & Company, Inc., New York, 1930. 

20 



RACIAL AND FAMILY BACKGROUND 



ality groups this incidence of course being based upon 
the relative frequency of the different groups in the total 
population. For instance, one would be absolutely certain 
of finding a higher percentage of children of superior 
mentality among those with a Scotch-Irish ancestry 
than among Negroes. 

THE NEGRO 

The mean I.Q. of American white children is 100. A 
number of surveys of the intelligence of American Negro 
children show average I.Q/s in the low 80's. Occasionally 
an average falls somewhat lower than that, as in a study 
by Paterson, who f oxind a mean of 75 for Negro children 
in a number of Tennessee schools. When performance 
rather than verbal tests are used the obtained measures 
of central tendency tend to run somewhat higher. North- 
ern Negroes on the whole excel Southern Negroes in 
intelligence test ratings. The scores which these two 
groups earned on the Army Alpha and Army Beta, when 
they were tested at the time of the World War, is 
striking: 





Alpha 


Beta 


Native-born white draft ... 


59 


43 


Foreign-born white draft 


47 


41 


Northern Negro draft 


39 


33 


Southern Negro draft 


12 


20 









Frank S. Freeman, 1 after a detailed and sympathetic 
examination of the psychological data which have been 
gathered on the American Negro, concludes that the 
present intellectual status of the American Negro is on 



, F. S., "Individual Differences," p. 167, Henry Holt & 
Company, New York, 1934. 

21 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

the whole appreciably inferior to that of the white 
population. 

Accepting a mean of 82 as being typical of the average 
mentality of the Negro and assuming that this is accom- 
panied by a standard deviation of 12, an LQ- of 130 
becomes four standard deviations above the mean. 
Statistically there is but one chance in 30,000 of a score 
falling above plus four standard deviations. In other 
words, JSegro child has butj^n ft flhflnce in 30 T 000 of 
being intellectually gifted. School experience bears out 
the validity ol this frequency. The white child, on the 
other hand, ^ 3QfyrTm-nnpa ij\ 30 T nnn of hftinff intftllftc* 
tjiflJJyjnfted. It seems that the cards of inheritance- 
and environment are stacked for the white child 
against the Negro by a ratio of 300 to 1. In line with this 
statement is the experience of Terman 1 in the selection 
of his group of California gifted children. He reports that 
Negroes, representing 2 per cent of the total of the com- 
bined population of Los Angeles, San Francisco, Oak- 
land, Alameda, and Berkeley, furnished 0.3 per cent of 
his gifted group; that is, two cases. Even these two cases 
were both part white. 

THE INDIAN 

The intelligence of the American Indian has been 
extensively studied, especially by Garth. The data in- 
variably indicate a relatively low level of the kind of 
mental ability measured by verbal intelligence tests. 
For instance, Garth, using national and Otis intelligence 
tests on 2,650 full-blooded Indians, obtained a mean 
I.Q. of 69. This extremely low average almost precludes 
the possibility of an Indian equaling in mental .capacity 
a white child who falls within the classification "intellec- 

1 TEHMAN, L. M., "Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. I, p. 56, 2d ed., 
Stanford University Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1926. 

22 



RACIAL AND FAMILY BACKGROUND 

tually gifted." The qualifying "almost" is used because 
once in a very great while there emerges an Indian with 
superior mentality. 

Garth 1 offers nurture and selection as the fundamental 
causes of the difference in intelligence between the 
Indian and the white. It is possible that these factors do 
exert a potent influence, although it is difficult to see 
that there has been a more conscious attempt to breed 
up the white race in America than there has been to 
breed up the red or black races. The important question 
of nurture will be taken up in considerable detail in the 
next chapter. 

NATIONALITY GROUPS 

The table on p. 24 compiled by Goodenough sum- 
marizes the results of a number of surveys of the mental 
ability of children representing various nationality 
groups in America. 

Terman, upon investigating the racial origin of his 
California group of intellectually gifted children, found 
approximately the same pattern as that seen in the 
preceding table. The percentage of representation of the 
different nationality groups among these gifted children 
is in many instances, however, devoid of exact meaning 
because of the difficulties involved in learning their 
relative incidence in the total population surveyed. 
Nevertheless, Terman 2 feels justified in offering the 
following conclusion: 

Data on racial origin indicate that, in comparison with the general 
population of the cities concerned, our gifted show a 100 per cent 
excess of Jewish blood; a 25 per cent excess of parents who are of 
native parentage; a probable excess of Scotch ancestry; and a very 
great deficiency of Latin and Negro ancestry. 

1 GABTH, T. R., "Race Psychology," p. 84, McGraw-Hill Book Com- 
pany, Inc., New York, 1931. 
3 TERMAN, op. cit., p. 82. 

23 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Perhaps the most striking fact in the above summary 
is the high frequency of Jewish children in Terman's 
gifted group. They appear about twice as often as 
would be expected, considering their known incidence in 

TABLE I. INTELLECTUAL ABILITY OF AMERICAN SCHOOL CHILDREN BY 

RACIAL STOCK* 



Nationality or racialVbock 


Reported by 


Mean I.Q. 


White American 


Dickson 


106 


White American 


Sheldon 


104 




Brown 


104 


German 


Brown 


102 




Brown 


102 




Murdock 


100 


Austrian 


Brown 


100 




Symonds 


99 




Pintner and Keller 


97 


French 


Brown 


95 


Jewish. 


Pintner and Keller 


95 




Pintner and Keller 


91 


T^innp . ..... 


Brown 


90 




Pintner and Keller 


89 


Negroes (Ohio) . . . .... 


Pintner and Keller 


88 


Portuguese 


Young 


86 




Pintner and Keller 


85 


Italian 


Pintner and Keller 


84 


Italian 


Dickson 


84 


Portuguese 


Dickson 


84 


Italian 


Young 


83 




Dickson 


75 


Negroes (Tennessee) 
Negroes (Arkansas) .... 


Paterson 
Jordan 


75 
71 









* GOODENOUGH, F. L., Racial Differences in the Intelligence of School Children, 
JT. Exper. Paychol., Vol. 9, p. 389, 1926. 

the total population. Terman's conclusions are borne out 
by the studies of Leta Hollingworth concerning intellec- 
tually gifted children in New York City and of Howard 
Gray in his investigation into the parentage of 154 gifted 
college students. 

24 



RACIAL AND FAMILY BACKGROUND 

That there are wide differences in mental capacity 
among the racial and nationality groups in this country 
is an indisputable fact. The causes of these differences, 
however, are highly debatable, with the evidence at pres- 
ent emphasizing the potency of heredity. 

FAMILIES OF GENIUSES 

There is a saying that "genius is 5 per cent inspiration 
and 95 per cent perspiration." Even Napoleon, an excep- 
tionally keen and objective self-analyst, gave as the most 
important single cause of his amazing success his ability 
to work hard for long hours. So convinced was he of this 
that instead of choosing some regal animal as his imperial 
symbol, he selected the busy bee. Perhaps Napoleon in 
common with other men was affected by an unconscious 
desire to believe that he became what he was by his own 
efforts instead of by means of an inherited capacity. The 
man who has done well likes to say, as does the politician 
whose plans have succeeded, "I did it." The man who 
has failed prefers to say, as does the politician whose 
plans have gone astray, "I am not responsible." 

This human weakness, this desire to pat one's ego, lies 
at the root of "the misconceptions concerning the role of 
heredity not only in genius but in mental inferiority and 
insanity as well. The parent of a gifted chilcj is quite 
likely to point to himself with pride, asserting without 
too much modesty that he is the cause of his child's 
brilliance either through the splendid inheritance which 
he has given the child or through the method of training 
which he has used. The parent of the feebleminded child, 
on the other hand, is almost certain to place the blame 
for the child's mental inferiority on some special incident 
outside his control, such as injury at the time of birth; 
and one rarely finds a person, who, having an insane 
relative, does not ascribe that insanity to a blow on the 

25 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

head or to a serious illness when the individual was 
young. There is little justification for being critical of this 
weakness, for human beings are in constant need of 
bolstering their self-respect. 

The truth concerning genius appears to be the reverse 
of the popular saying quoted at the beginning of this 
chapter. Family is definitely of^great importance in the 
creation of Mghmenj^JiijLJi is wel 



nigntality and hijh^achigvemeiit 
arejiot synonymousJiftr^g, fa** jj^Jj^ 

means guarantees^eat_succgg. 



_ 

A number of other influencing factors are too important 
for that to be true. On the other hand, great success can- 
not be achieved without a high degree of intelligence. 
Napoleon was right in emphasizing the importance of 
application, but wrong in underrating the importance of 
family stock. He was fond of saying that the Bonaparte 
family began on the Eighteenth Brumaire, the date of th.e 
coup d ? 6tat which made him first consul. As a matter of 
fact, the Bonaparte family had had a long and dis- 
tinguished history as the first family of Corsica. There is, 
of course, a great difference between being first in Corsica 
and first in the world, and it is this difference that has 
given rise to the popular belief that Napoleon sprang f ropa 
the masses. 

In the United States the belief has long b$en cherished 
that practically all great Americans were born to parents 
of average intelligence, who lived.in humble surroundings. 
Though this is the case in certain instances, a, far larger 
number who have achieved distinction came from families 
possessing a marked strain of intelligence Among. these 
are Washington, whose parents were members of suc- 
cessful, thrifty families of property and social, st^ndiijg; 
Emerson, who was descended from a long line of ministers 
of energetic Puritan stock and \vhose father was a^t one 
time pastor of the First Church of Boston and the author 

26 



RACIAL AND FAMILY BACKGROUND 

of a number of religious works; Jefferson, whose father 
was a colonel and a member of the Virginia House of 
Burgesses and whose mother came from a family which 
included warriors, churchmen, statesmen, and eminent 
scholars; and Longfellow, whose father was a man much 
honored for his ability in his profession of the law and for 
his sound good sense in public affairs. 

To be sure, such a selected list as the above illustrates 
rather than proves the point that genius runs in families. 
In this instance, however, careful studies, scientifically 
conducted, bear out the assertion; reference to a number 
of these follows. 

The most famous and most often-quoted study of a 
family of geniuses is that made by Winship and Daven- 
port of the descendants of Richard Edwards, an eminent 
lawyer, and his wife, Elizabeth Tuthill, noted for an 
exceptional mind. Their son, one of the founders of Yale 
University, fathered Jonathan Edwards, who married the 
brilliant Sarah Pierpont. Numbered^ among the descend- 
ants of these two, during the somewhat more than 200 
years following their marriage, are 12 college presidents, 
65 college professors, 60 physicians, 100 clergymen, 75 
Army ' officers, 60 prominent authors, 100 lawyers, 
30 judges, and a large number of high public officials, in- 
cluding two United States Senators, and one Vice- 
President of the United States. 

These data present an even more emphatic picture 
when compared with those concerning the descendants of 
a Revolutionary soldier, Martin Kallikak, and a feeble- 
minded girl. Of their 480 descendants, 143 were known to 
be feeble-minded, 36 illegitimate, 33 sexually immoral, 
24 'drunkards, and 3 criminals. 

Law of Ancestral -Inheritance 

The pioneer in the study of the families of geniuses 
was the English scholar, Galton. In one of his investi- 

27 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

gations he examined the biographies of 977 eminent men. 
He discovered that each of these had a far greater number 
of eminent relatives than would be expected on the basis 
of chance alone. In comparing the number of eminent 
relatives of a group of average men of similar size, he 
found a tremendous difference, namely 535 to 4, bringing 
him to the obvious conclusion that intellect does run in 
families. 

It is interesting to note that he found that 48 per cent 
of the eminent men whom he studied were the sons of 
men who were themselves eminent. This figure is strik- 
ingly similar to those arrived at in later studies. 

As a result of his extensive investigations Galton formu- 
lated a law which is called the "law of ancestral inherit- 
ance.' 7 According to his law a child inherits one-half of 
his mental capacity from his parents, one-fourth from his 
grandparents, one-eighth from his great-grandparents, 
one-sixteenth from his great-great-grandparents, and so 
on back through the generations. It is clear from this 
progression that the intellectual capacity of the parents 
has more influence upon the intellectual capacity of the 
child than does that of ancestors farther removed. The 
law, however, allows for the unexpected emergence of 
genius in a child whose parents and even grandparents 
are dull, for the trait may have been present in an an- 
cestor several times removed. 

Relatives of Gifted Children 

Terman, 1 in studying the relatives of a group of intel- 
lectually gifted children, uncovered facts corroborating 
the findings of Galton. Again and again, from whatever 
angle he approached the problem, he found that mentally 
superior children are much more likely to possess men- 
tally superior relatives than average children. For in- 

1 TERMAN, op. cit., Chap. V. 

28 



RACIAL AND FAMILY BACKGROUND 

stance, he found that 578 families were responsible for the* 
676 young geniuses in his group. Of these, seventy-three 
families yielded two subjects and nine yielded three or 
more. Terman points out that "the number of families 
with two subjects is more than 1,200 times the number 
chance would give." In other words, an intellectually 
gifted child is much more likely than an average child to 
have an intellectually gifted brother or sister. 

Leta Hollingworth, best known to the general public 
for her unusual work with a selected group of gifted 
children at Speyer School in New York City, found, as 
did Galton and Terman, that the siblings (brothers and 
sisters) of very bright children are themselves much above 
average. In a study with Cobb she tested the living 
siblings of a group of fifty-seven children who fell within 
the top 0.5 per cent of the population. The average LQ. 
of this very gifted group was 154. Cobb and Hollingworth 
found, upon testing the siblings, that they ranged in LQ. 
from 96 to 173, with an average of 129, far above the 
typical 100. 

There are two other interesting facts in connection 
with the relatives of the gifted children studied by Ter- 
man. It was found that of the sixty-two members of the 
Hall of Fame, fourteen were related to one or more 
children in the California group. In certain instances, the 
connection was close, as in the case of the two young 
geniuses who were in direct line from the grandparents of 
George Washington, the two who were directly descended 
from Roger Williams, and the child who was in the direct 
line of , John Adams. 

Twelve of Terman's group of gifted children had, at 
the time of selection, a parent or grandparent in " Who's 
Who in America." Of these, three were fathers, two 
mothers, and four grandparents. Terman points out that 
the chance of a man of forty-one years, the average age 

29 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

of the fathers of the gifted children whom he was study- 
ing, being in ''Who's Who 7 ' is about one in 2,000, yet 
three out of his group of 578 had been so honored. This is 
approximately ten times the number chance would give. 
The chance of a woman's being included in "Who's Who " 
is very much smaller, yet in this investigation it was 
found that two of the mothers had achieved this dis- 
tinction. Numbered among the relatives of these intel- 
lectually gifted children are such well-known persons as 
Champ Clark, Newell Dwight Hillis, Senator Hiram 
Johnson, F. B. McCormack, former Chancellor of the 
University of Pittsburgh, Dr. Albert Michaelson, famous 
physicist, 'and James Addison Young, former Supreme 
Court justice. 

Fathers of Eminent Men 

Some years ago a study was made of the 282 most 
eminent men who lived during the period from 1450 to 
1850. Data concerning these geniuses were gathered and 
analyzed with the greatest possible impartiality. The in- 
vestigation was made by Catherine M. Cox under the 
direction of Terman. Concerning the heredity of the 
geniuses which she studied, Cox presents the following 
statistics (see Table II). 

The Taussig scale referred to in the preceding table is a 
five-point rating system for socioeconomic standing. 
According to this classification, more than half of the 
world's most, eminent men for the period from 1450 to 
1850 were born to fathers who were members of the 
highest social class, and this despite the fact that this 
top class represented but a fraction of the total population 
approximately 3 per cent. The fathers of over 8 p^r 
cent of these eminent men belonged to the two highest 
social classes, leaving only 20 per cent as members of the 
skilled workmen and lower business, semiskilled, ajad 

30 



RACIAL AND FAMILY BACKGROUND 



TABLE II. OCCUPATIONAL STATUS OF THE FATHEBS AND MATEENAI/ 
GEANDFATHEBS or 282 EMINENT MEN* 



Taussig rating and classification 


Fathers 


Maternal grandfathers 


Fre- 
quency 


Per 

cent 


Fre- 
quency 


Per 
cent 
282 


Per cent 
of 184 
reported 


1. Professional and nobility. 
2. Semiprofessional, higher busi- 
ness, and gentry 


148 
81 

37 

11 
3 
2 


52.5 

28.7 

13.1 
3.9 
1.1 
0.7 


77 
65 

35 
3 

4 
98 


27.3 

23.0 

12.4 
1.1 
1.4 
34.7 


41.8 
35.3 

19.1 
1.6 

2.2 


3. Skilled workmen and lower 
business 


4, SfiTmslnllfid, 


R. TTnsVillP.H 


No record 


Total 


282 


100.0 


282 


99.9 


100.0 





* Cox, C. M., " Genetic Studies of Genius," VoL II, p. 37, Stanford University Press, 
Stanford University, Calif., 1926. 

unskilled groups. It is true that Miss Cox is considering 
in this study only those men who actually attained emi- 
nence. Obviously, in many instances, opportunity in the 
guise of family and money helped them in their rise to 
fame. She had no way of discovering how many men of 
equal intellect may have been born in humble surround- 
ings and kept from reaching the heights by an un- 
friendly environment. 

Another study of the ancestry of a group of eminent 
men is that made by Cattell of 885 American men of 
science. His conclusions correspond closely to those 
arrived at by Miss Cox. He found that the professional 
classes, which constituted only 3 per cent of the general 
population, produced nearly one-half of the eminent 
American scientists whom he was studying. On the 
otter hand, agriculture, which is commonly supposedly 
Americans to be the most fruitful source of eminence, 
contributed only slightly more than one-fifth of the men 

31 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

studied by Cattell, even though at the time the study was 
made agricultural workers represented more than two- 
fifths of the total population. In other words, the pro- 
fessional classes produced sixteen times as many eminent 
scientists as would be expected when their relative fre- 
quency in the general population is taken into consider- 
ation, while the farming classes did only half as well as 
they should have done on the basis of their numbers. 

An investigation by Clarke of the parentage of 666 
outstanding men of letters contains statistics that are 
striking in their similarity to CattelPs findings concerning 
the ancestry of American men of science. Clarke reports 
that the fathers of nearly half of the eminent men he 
studied belonged to the professional classes, somewhat 
less than one-fourth to the commercial classes, a slightly 
smaller number to the agricultural classes, and only 7 per 
cent to the mechanical, clerical, and unskilled groups. 

These studies are typical of the many investigations 
that have been made, and give weight to the statement 
that great men come from family lines which have 
already produced other men of high intelligence. How- 
ever, all of them deal with the fathers of adult geniuses, 
and, admittedly, parental influence plays an important 
role in individual achievement. A somewhat different 
approach to the problem is to investigate the parentage 
of children who have been identified by tests as being 
intellectually gifted. In trying to determine the role of 
family in the production of intellect, it is as important 
to consider geniuses in the making at it is to consider 
those who have actually achieved eminence. 

Fathers of Gifted Children 

Terman, 1 in his detailed study of a thousand bright 
children in California, presents data on their ancestry. 
1 TEBMAN, op. cit., Vol. I, p. 64. 

32 



RACIAL AND FAMILY BACKGROUND 

These children, all falling well within the top 1 per cent 
with respect to intellectual capacity and ranging in age 
from two to adolescence, were selected at random, no 
attention whatsoever being paid to any factor except 
mental ability. Data on the fathers of 560 of these gifted 
children were adequate to permit their grouping into the 
Taussig five-grade classification. 





Number 


Per cent 


Professional 


176 


31 4 


Semiprofessional and business 


280 


50 


Skilled labor 


66 


11 8 


Semiskilled labor to slightly skilled 
Common labor 


37 
1 


6.6 
2 









Terman did not have the figures on the relative fre- 
quency of the different groups in., the total population 
surveyed, but he was able to secure them for Los Angeles 
and San Francisco. In these two cities he found that the 
professional group made up but 2.9 per cent of the total; 
yet it was the source of almost one-third of the gifted 
children discovered there. In other words, fathers be- 
longing to the professional classes law, medicine, 
newspaper editors, and so on produced somewhat more 
than ten times as many intellectually superior children 
as would be expected considering their relatively small 
incidence in the general population. 

Concerning the contribution of the industrial group, 
Terman says; 

Only one man gives his occupation as laborer, which is 0.2 per cent 
of our fathers as compared with 15.0 per cent of the general popu- 
lation classified as laborer in the census report. Accordingly, fathers 
of gifted children yield only one seventy-seventh of their quota for 
this class. The man referred to was a farmer who had moved to 

33 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Berkeley and taken a position as laborer at the University of Cali- 
fornia in order that his children might attend college. 1 

It is a significant fact that, though the professional 
classes produced 31.4 per cent of the gifted children 
studied by Terman, the ratio is not so great as that found 
in the investigations of the ancestry of adult geniuses, 
where the figure consistently hovers around 50 per cent. 
This discrepancy is an indication that the mortality 
among promising children is much less in professional 
families than in the families of labor. A gifted child Jx>rn 
to jaJarWy^fe~imi^ than a gifted childjxxra 

to a laborer to have the wayntoTat^^emine^e made 
smooth for ^EfioT'Oire "of the^Tesjf6nsTBiir?ies^of a demo- 
cratic'" educational system is to salvage its vitally im- 
portant intellectual resources in the persons of these 
children who, though possessed of great mental ability, 
are kept from developing and utilizing it by insurmount- 
able environmental obstacles. 

1 Ibid., p. 63. 



34 



CHAPTER III 
THE ROLE OF ENVIRONMENT 

1. Does environment modify intellectual capacity? 

2. Is any environment ever wholly good or bad in itself? 

3. What do studies of twins indicate concern ing the relative 
potency of heredity and environment? 

Iu any discussion of the role of environment in the 
determination of the mental capacity of an individual, 
it should be kept in mind that the issue involved is never 
whether heredity or environment is all-powerful; the 
question is always one of the relative contribution of 
each. The forces of heredity and environment are so 
interwoven, so interdependent, as to make their sepa- 
ration, as impossible as unscrambling an egg. 

When a child is born into the world, heredity has 
determined that he shall be a human being, not a chim- 
panzee an obvious fact, but one that immediately 
places limits upon the possibilities of environmental 
influences. As a normal human being, he has legs and 
arms, ears with which to hear, and eyes with which to see. 
He has inherited his sex and the color of his skin and hair. 
Heredity has predisposed him to be tall or short, fat or 
slim, strong or weak. It has equipped him with the 
capacity to love, to fear, and to be angry. It has marked 
the limits of his intellectual powers. However, born a 
human being, the child's body may be so twisted and 
warped by disease that it loses much of its human shape; 
inheriting arms and legs, he may lose them in an auto- 
mobile accident or in a man-made war; given ears to 
hear with, disasters within his environment may deprive 

35 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

him of his hearing; given eyes to see with, much reading 
and study may weaken an ability which heredity had 
intended to be strong; inheriting a white skin, jaundice 
may make it yellow, or working in the sun, brown, or 
illness and confinement, gray; inheriting great intellec- 
tual capacity, an unsatisfactory home life or inadequate 
schooling may effectively prevent him from realizing his 
possibilities. Heredity, in the form of the nature of the 
physical mechanism which he possesses, has set the 
limits to his capacities, but the environment in which 
that mechanism functions modifies the course along 
which, and the extent to which, those capacities will be 
developed. 

Who can say which is more valuable: the innate 
ability to make sounds or the developed ability to use a 
language; the innate capacity to master mathematics, 
or the developed power to handle figures in everyday life 
situations. The teacher, unable to endow a child with 
great mental capacity, should devote her energies to 
teaching the child how to use whatever mental capacity 
he may have inherited. Giving the mind something to 
work with is as important as creating the mind. 

A second fact to be considered in any discussion of the 
relative potency of heredity and environment is that 
environment can never be considered by itself alone but 
must always be interpreted in terms of its interaction 
with the individual concerned. It should be a truism that, 
what is good environment for one child may be bad 
environment for another. If this statement is accepted, 
it follows that there is no such thing as a controlled envi- 
ronment which can be depended upon to bring posi- 
tive results to all. Moreover, a particular environment 
is not constant, in any absolute sense, fpr the. same 
individual, but affects him differently as he fluctuates in 
his responses. 

36 ' 



THE ROLE OF ENVIRONMENT 

It is, then, impossible to say what constitutes a good 
environment for those who are intellectually gifted. It 
may be that a relatively poor home would, for certain 
children, provide a more suitable atmosphere in which to 
grow toward adult eminence than would a wealthy, 
cultured home. There are many ways in which the poor 
home might furnish impetus. For instance, it might pro- 
vide the economic drive which has actuated the success of 
many individuals; or it might provide a background of 
understanding of the needs of the common people a 
solid foundation upon which to build a life of achieve- 
ment; or it might, because of an intense hatred of it, 
force the individual to great effort to overcome what were 
for him the handicaps of childhood. Probably no one 
would, if he could, consciously assign gifted children to an 
early life of poverty, but it is, nevertheless, a fact that 
many children born on a low socioeconomic level owe 
their later eminence partly to that fact. 

There have been a number of scientific investigations 
into the relative potency of heredity and environment. 
These are important, not because they succeed in proving 
that one or the other is the sole cause of the child's 
mental capacity nor because they can give the exact 
percentage of contribution of each, though some research 
workers claim to have thus neatly solved the problem. 
These studies are, in the main, descriptive and are 
important primarily for the light which they throw upon 
the complex interaction of heredity and environment in 
the production of genius. 

Investigations of the hereditary and environmental 
sources of mental capacity have followed four lines of 
attack: (1) the transmission of subnormal intelligence 
from parent to child; (2) the transmission of high 
intellectual capacity from parent to child; (3) the 
extent of similarity in mental traits between twins; (4) 

37 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

the constancy of intellectual status under changing 
environment. 

TRANSMISSION OF MENTAL INFERIORITY 

It has been definitely proved that feeble-mindedness 
runs in families. A leading authority in this field, Tred- 
gold, says that a defective germ plasm is responsible for 
80 per cent of all feeble-minded individuals. Percentages 
suggested by other investigators hover about that 
figure. For instance, Kuhlmann sets it at 75 per cent, 
while Leta Hollingworth places it at 90 per cent. There is 
little justification for accepting these percentages as 
anything more than estimates. 

Genealogical studies, like those of Goddard, show 
that the majority of subnormal children are born that 
way. It may be, however, that prenatal influences, which 
are, of course, environmental, have affected the men- 
tality of the developing embryo. Concerning such in- 
fluences little is known. There is, too, the question of the 
importance of the impact upon the infant of the presence 
in his family of other feeble-minded individuals and of the 
depressing effect upon him of the low socioeconomic 
status which is typical of the group possessing subnormal 
intelligence. That this low socioeconomic status could at 
most constitute merely a contributing cause becomes 
obvious when the fact is recalled that there are many 
feeble-minded children growing up in homes of wealth 
and culture. 

TRANSMISSION OF MENTAL SUPERIORITY 

Data on the more important studies of the trans- 
mission of mental superiority were given in Chap. II. 
These data show conclusively that genius runs in 
families. From this it does not follow that intellectually 
gifted children will be found only in homes where the 

38 



THE ROLE OF ENVIRONMENT 

parents possess superior minds. Rather it means that such 
children are more likely to be found in such homes. 
Obviously there are many exceptions. 

Although it has been definitely proved that intel- 
lectually gifted children and eminent adults tend to come 
from family lines possessing a marked strain of high 
mental ability, it does not follow that environment has 
not played an important part. It is quite possible that 
association with mentally superior individuals and direct 
contact with cultural surroundings, such as excellent 
home libraries, helps the child to earn a higher score on an 
achievement test or on an intelligence test. This is almost 
certain to be true if the child is in accord with Ms environ- 
ment. It is quite possible that the favorable early sur- 
roundings of those of CattelTs 885 American men of 
science who came from the professional classes had a 
great deal to do with their later success. Heredity had, 
to be sure, set a limit to the possibilities for achievement 
of the hundreds of thousands of boys born at the same 
time as were these children, who later became eminent 
in the field of science. Environment, however, in the 
guise of health, money, and opportunity, certainly 
modified the course which these children followed as 
they grew into manhood. Unquestionably there were 
many who, though having mental powers equal to those 
who actually attained eminence in science, were never 
heard of either because of an unfriendly environment 
or because of the possession of traits which were non- 
complementary to intellectual capacity. 

MENTAL SIMILARITY OF TWINS 

The question of likenesses between twins, always an 
interesting one to the layman, has become even more 
appealing siixce the birth of the Dionne quintuplets. The 
general public has followed the development of these five 

39 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

girls with avid curiosity. Concerning the mental ability 
of the quintuplets, the orthodox psychologist would 
expect two things : first, that, considering their hereditary 
background and in spite of their seemingly favorable 
environment, they would possess average or less-than- 
average intelligence; second, that, being of multiple 
birth and especially since there is a probability that 
they are identical, they would be approximately equal in 
mental power. A recent report by William E. Blatz, 
Professor of Child Psychology at the University of 
Toronto, fulfills these expectations. 

Language development is the best single indication of 
intellectual capacity in the young child. Professor Blatz 1 
points out in his book, "The Five Sisters/' that in this 
ability all five girls are retarded. At the age of two years 
each of the quintuplets had a vocabulary of less than ten 
words, a number which is typical for the average child of 
eighteen months. At three years of age each child had a 
vocabulary of approximately 110 words. This is some- 
what less than the typical vocabulary of the average 
child of two years. Professor Blatz, who gives his readers 
the impression of striving to present the quintuplets in 
the best possible light, offers as one contributing cause 
of this observed retardation the fact that the quintuplets 
were born two months prematurely. He also suggests 
that, since there were so many of them all of the same 
age, they did not find the use of language important, 
relying rather upon gestures. The first of these reasons 
is a plausible one; it does take from three to five years for 
a child born prematurely to overcome this handicap in 
time. The second observation by Professor Blatz appears 
to have less foundation. To be sure, the need for talking 
and the desire to talk have a direct effect upon the size of 

1 BLATZ, W. E., "The Five Sisters," pp. 34-37, William Morrow & 
Company, Inc., New York, 1938. 

40 



THE HOLE OF ENVIRONMENT 

a child's vocabulary, but it is difficult to believe tliat this 
urge would necessarily be less with quintuplets than it 
would be with an only child. 

Although the Dionne quintuplets are not intellectually 
gifted children, a statement of the results of Blatz's in- 
vestigation of the mental capacity of the group at the age 
of three years throws light upon the heredity-environ- 
ment question and so is pertinent to our present dis- 
cussion. Blatz measured four aspects of the development 
of these children: language, motor, adaptive, and per- 
sonal-social. From the data gathered, he concludes: 1 

If we were to combine the score on all four divisions, we would then 
get a composite picture of the mental development of the child. 
When we do this for the quintuplets, we find that although they 
start out below the standard for their age, they are gradually creeping 
up. The expectation is that in due course their performance will 
approximate that of the average child. . . . The chart shows the 
order in which the children are placed from the llth to the 35th 
month, with Yvonne at the top, Cecile and Annette about equal, 
Emilie next, and Marie last. This relationship is remarkably like that 
which will be described from the analysis of their physical character- 
istics, with Cecile in the center, Marie and Yvonne at opposite poles, 
Emilie more like Marie and Annette more like Yvonne. This may 
suggest that the fundamental basis of intelligence is structural and 
that the degree of intelligence, however difficult it is to determine, is 
an inherited characteristic. 

Blatz then goes on to say: 

Also it must be pointed out that the differences in the behavior of 
these children on various tests are relatively slight, but that these 
differences are fairly constant, as illustrated in the rank-order chart, 
which shows that Yvonne throughout the whole period of testing 
was first and Marie last and Emilie consistently fourth, with Annette 
and Cecile interchanging. 

There have been a number of studies made of twins. 
These agree that twins are more alike in every way, 

W., pp. 42-44. 

41 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

including mentality, than are unrelated children and 
that identical 1 twins are much more alike than fraternal 
twins. Heredity is presumably the cause of this greater 
similarity. However, as Carter 2 points out, it may be 
that, actually, nurture influences are somewhat more 
alike for identical than for fraternal twins, their identical 
inheritance making them more congenial, more inter- 
ested in the same things. But even identical twins do 
not have identical environments, although their similiar 
organisms do result in a likelihood of their reacting 
in much ihe same manner to similarly selected sets of 
stimuli. 

The question of what happens when identical twins 
are reared apart has challenged the attention of investi- 
gators. If such twins have been brought up in widely 
differing environments and yet are found to be more 
alike than unrelated individuals, heredity would appear 
to be more potent than environment. If, on the other 
hand, such twins after a period of years are found to 
be markedly divergent in intellectual capacity, then it 
would appear that environment appreciably depresses 
or increases one's intellectual level. 

Newman, * Freeman, and Holzinger studied nineteen 
pairs of identical twins reared in separate homes. These 
investigators found that identical twins reared apart 
differed somewhat more than do twins reared together. 
In one case a difference of 15 points in I.Q. was noticed. 
In general, however, it was found that the twins reared 

1 Identical or monozygotic twins are those which develop from the 
same fertilized egg cell; fraternal or dizygotic twins develop from two 
fertilized egg cells. 

2 CABTER, H. D., "Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National Society for 
the Study of Education," Part 1, p. 308, 1940. 

8 NEWMAN, H. H., F. N. FREEMAN, and K. J. HOLZINGER, "Twins: 
A Study of Heredity and Environment," University of Chicago Press, 
Chicago, 1937. 

4:2 



THE ROLE OF ENTIBONMEXT 

apart were amazingly alike in spite of wide differences in 
environment. 

Carter, 1 in summarizing the results of studies of 
twins during the past ten years, says : 

The whole array of twin-studies seems to suggest, to the writer 
at least, the futility and artificialty of the idea of untangling nature 
and nurture influences in the sense of ascertaining the percentage 
contributions of each in any general sense. The view that the idea of 
percentage contributions can have meaning only for specific mental 
traits, regarded under rather specific environmental conditions, seems 
to be implicit in much of the literature. The literature suggests that 
the hereditary determiners have a more pervasive influence than was 
heretofore believed; every increase in the body of data brings more 
evidence of hereditary influence. At the same time, the data indicate 
that further elucidation of the problems of operation of these heredi- 
tary factors is to be sought in study of the environment . . . The 
data show one thing clearly, that drastic differences in the educational 
and social environment are sometimes associated with moderate 
differences in the IQ's of identical twins reared apart. That only this 
can be said should bring caution into the writings of the most ardent 
environmentalists. 

EFFECT OF CHANGING ENVIRONMENT 

A fourth method of studying the relative contributions 
of heredity and environment is to observe changes in 
children taken from one environment and placed in 
another. At the time the change is made the intelligence 
of the children is either measured by actual tests or 
estimated on the basis of the known socioeconomic 
status of the parents. If, after a period of years in the 
new environment, a reliable change in mental level is 
observed, environment, since it has been definitely 
altered, can be pointed to as the cause of the change. 
Although no one of the following investigations is imme- 
diately concerned with intellectually gifted children, 
each does throw light upon the question as to whether or 

1 CABTBK, op. rit., pp. 248-249. 

43 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

not genius can be created by education or by excellent 
home surroundings. 

Barbara Burks studied 214 foster children who were 
placed in their new homes before they were one year of 
age. These foster homes represented a much higher social 
level than did those from which the children came. At the 
time the follow-up study was made, the children ranged 
in chronological age from five to fourteen. Miss Burks 1 
arrives at the following conclusions: first, home environ- 
ment can produce about 17 per cent of the variance in 
I.Q. ; second, the total contribution of heredity is prob- 
ably not far from 75 to 80 per cent; third, measurable 
environment one standard deviation above or below the 
mean of the population does not shift the I.Q. by more 
than six to nine points above or below the value it would 
have had under normal environmental conditions. In 
other words, nearly 70 per cent of school children have an 
actual I.Q. within six to nine points of that represented 
by their intelligence. 

Freeman, 2 worked with 671 children. The results of his 
three most significant investigations, out of the many 
that were made, follow. 

One hundred and twenty-five pairs of siblings were 
tested when they had reached an average age of 12 
years 8 months, after having been, separated from four to 
thirteen years. The. test indicated a correlation in 
intelligence between these siblings of .34, somewhat 
lower than the .50 which is usually found for siblings 
reared together in the same home. 

Seventy-four children of an average chronological age 
of eight years were given a Stanford-Binet intelligence 

1 BURKS, B,, "Twenty-seventh. Yearbook of the National Society for 
the Study of Education," Part I, p. 308, 1928. 

2 FREEMAN, F. N., "Twenty-seventh Year Book of the National 
Society for the Study of Education," Part I, Chap. IX, 1928. 

44 



THE ROLE OF ENVIRONMENT 



test and then placed in superior foster homes. When these 
children had attained an average age of 12 years 2 
months, they were retested. The average I.Q. of the 
first test was 91.2 and of the retest 93.7, indicating a gain 
of 2.5. Freeman, correcting the scores for age, publishes 
a difference between the mean LQ.'s of 7.5. 

Forty homes, in which there were both foster children 
and own children, were selected for study. It was found 
that the average I.Q. of the foster children was 95. 1 
1.7, and for own children 112.4 1.6. More complete 
data appear in Table III. 

TABLE III. COMPARISON OF PERIODS OF HOME INFLUENCE* 



Child 


Mean age entered 
home 


Mean age at test 


Mean time in home 


Qw'-ti . . . . . 


Birth 


10 years 3 months 


10 years 3 months 


Foster 


4 years 8 months 


11 years 5 months 


6 years 9 months 











* FEEEMAN, P. N., " Twenty-seventh Year Book of the National Society for the Study 
of Education," Part I, p. 137, 1928. 

Freeman concludes from his studies that an improve- 
ment in- environment produces a gain in intelligence. 

Influence of Nursery School Attendance 

A considerable majority of the investigations reported 
on in the 1940 Yearbook of the National Society for the 
Study of Education arrive at the conclusion that nursery 
school attendance or superior environment in the guise 
of excellent schools has little effect upon the intellectual 
status of the individual. For example, Reymert 1 and 
Hinton studied the case histories of 100 children who 
had been in the superior environment of Mooseheart 
for four years. These writers report that a comparison 

1 REYMEBT, M. L., and R. T. KENT-TON, " Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the 
National Society for the Study of Education," Part II, p. 266, 1940. 

45 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

of the entrance test scores of these children with the 
scores earned each succeeding year showed no significant 

gains in I.Q. 

Goodenough and Maurer, in an excellent study of 
nursery school children at the Institute of Child Welfare, 
University of Minnesota, find no appreciable difference in 
the LQ. changes between those children who had nursery 
school training and those who did not. Data on the 
changes which occurred during the interval between the 
first testing and a fourth, three years later, appear in 
Table IV. 

TABLE IV. CHANGES m MEAN I.Q. ON THE MINNESOTA PRESCHOOL 

TEST APTEB THREE YEAES OF NTOSERY-SCHOOL TRAINING COM- 

PAEED WITH CHANGES IN NON-NURSERY-SCHOOL CHILDREN 

AFTER AN INTERVAL OP THREE YEARS* 



Occupational group t 




Nursery 


Non-nursery 


I 


Cases 
Test 1 
Test 4 


5 
105.5 
121.5 


5 
113.5 
124.5 


n, in 


Cases 
Test 1 
Test 4 


$ 
109.5 
112.5 


8 
105.6 
106.9 


IV, V, VI 


Cases 
Test 1 
Test 4 


3 

117.5 
110.8 


2 
125.0 
122.5 


Total, I to VI 


Cases 
Test 1 


13 
109.8 


15 
110.8 




Test 4 


115.6 


114.8 



* GOODBNOUGH, F. &., and K. M. MAUHBB, "Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National 
Society for the Study of Education," Part II, p. 171, 1940. 
t Group I. Professional. 

Group II. Semiprofessional and managerial. 

Group III. Betail business, clerical, and skilled trades. 

Group IV. Semiskilled trades and minor clerical. 

Group V. Sightly skilled trades. 

Group VI. Unskilled occupations. Day labor. 

46 



THE ROLE OF ENVIRONMENT 

In a summarizing comment Goodenough 1 and Maurer 

say, 

None of the analyses that we have been able to make warrant the 
conclusion that attendance at the University of Minnesota Nursery 
School has any measurable effect whatever upon the mental develop- 
ment of children. Those who have had this training do no better on 
standardized intelligence tests than those who have not had it; they 
are neither more nor less advanced in school, and those who have 
attended longest and most regularly do not excel those whose period 
of enrollment was short and broken by frequent absences. 

Hildreth, in a report on the records of 54 children 
who had been adopted into superior homes and who had 
been given superior educational opportunities, including 
attendance at Lincoln School in New York, found that 
this superior environment had not succeeded in lifting 
the children above the average for the general popula- 
tion. The average LQ. of this group was found to be 
103.3 while for the general Lincoln School population 
it was 120.3. Hildreth 2 concludes with the statement, 

From the results of this study we may conclude that adopted 
children in a gifted school population tend to rate on intelligence tests 
more like the general country-wide population in mental ability than 
like the general population in the selected school population. So far as 
our records go, there is little evidence that attendance at such a 
school raises the average ability of these adopted children much above 
the general population level, or that continued attendance in the 
school is accompanied by an increase in intelligence as measured by 
individual tests. 

Starkweather 3 and Roberts, on the other hand, report 
an increase in LQ. as a result of nursery school training. 



., p. 76. 

2 HILDRETH, G., "Thiriy-iunth Yearbook of the National Society for 
the Study of Education," Part II, p. 184, 1940. 

3 STARKWEATHER, E. K., and K. E. ROBERTS, "Thirty-ninth Yearbook 
of the National Society for the Study of Education," Part II, p. 335, 
1940. 

47 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

In their investigation 107 nursery school children were 
given the Merrill-Palmer intelligence tests and 103 
children were given the Stanford-Binet intelligence 
tests upon entrance into the Merrill-Palmer Nursery 
School. These children were reexamined on the same 
tests after an interval of from 6 to 40 months. The 
authors found that : 

1. Children attending the Merrill-Palmer Nursery School gain in 
I.Q. and percentile as measured by Stanford-Binet and Merrill- 
Palmer retests. 

2. An inverse relationship exists between initial I.Q. or initial 
percentile levels and I.Q. gains or percentile gains. 

3. Varying lengths of nursery-school attendance show no relation- 
ship to I.Q. or to percentile changes. 

The Iowa Studies 

At the time of this writing a series of studies of the 
effect of a changed environment upon the I.Q. is being 
conducted at the University of Iowa. Preliminary reports 
indicate an amazing increase on intelligence-test ratings 
by children who have come under the influence of good 
foster homes or of excellent preschool educational train- 
ing. The mean increase found for certain groups is 
much greater than that ever reported elsewhere in psy- 
chological literature. An example of this is the thirty- 
point rise observed by Skeels. 

Skeels 1 presents data on seventy-three children, sixty- 
five of whom were illegitimate and were placed in foster 
homes before they were six months of age. Thirty-nine 
of the seventy-three own mothers were measured for 
intelligence; the test scores indicated an average I.Q. of 
83.8 with a standard deviation of 12.3. Only 10 per cent 
had LQ.'s above 100, while 38 per cent had I.Q.'s below 

1 SKEELS, H. M., Mental Development of Children in Foster Homes, 
J. Genet. Psychol Vol. 49, pp. 91-106, 1936. 

48 



THE ROLE OF ENVIRONMENT 

80. It was impossible to test the own fathers for intelli- 
gence, but their occupational status indicated an intellec- 
tual level approximating that of the own mothers. Forty- 
six per cent were day laborers and only 13 per cent were 
above the semiskilled group. Moreover, a large number 
of them had been on relief for some time. 

At the time the children were examined with the 
Stanford-Binet tests, their average chronological age 
was 24.4 months with a standard deviation of 14.2 
months. Their mean length of residence in foster homes 
was 21.9 months. Based on the known intelligence of the 
mothers and the estimated intelligence of the fathers, 
the expected mean I.Q. of these children would be about 
85; yet Skeels found it to be 115.3. Every child but one 
was found to be of average intelligence or better. Their 
intellectual level was superior even to that of their 
foster parents. 

Other studies at Iowa of the effects of a changed 
environment on the I.Q. offer conclusions in keeping with 
those presented in the Skeels article. For instance, Beth 
Wellman 1 publishes data showing that children attending 
the preschool laboratories at the University of Iowa 
make substantial and significant gains in I.Q. Moreover, 
she points out that under the favorable environment in 
which experimental classes are conducted, the dull 
children gain more and the bright children less than do 
those of average ability. 2 In other words, there is a 
slight tendency to reduce the spread in intelligence. 

Wellman 3 summarizes the main points of the pub- 
lished and unpublished studies made at the Iowa Child 

1 WELLMAN, B. L., The Effect of Pre-school Attendance on the IQ, 
J. Exper. Educ., Vol. 1, No. 2, pp. 48-69. 

2 WELLMAN, B. L., Growth in Intelligence under Differing School 
Environments, /. Exper. Educ., vol. 3, No. 2, December, 1934. 

* WELLMAN, B. L., "Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National Society 
for the Study of Education," Part II, p. 397, 1940. 

49 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Welfare Research Station on changes in intelligence 
associated with conditions of schooling as follows : 

1. Mental Growth during the Preschool Years 

1. The principal gains in IQ were made during preschool attendance 
and not during non-attendance. 

2. The gains were cumulative over the first two years of preschool 
attendance. 

3. Non-preschool children did not gain. 

4. Correlations between number of days 7 attendance during one 
preschool year and change in IQ approached zero. 

5. Cultural status of parents did not account for the changes in IQ. 

6. An appropriate educational program appeared to affect the IQ 
change of very superior children. 

7. Gains from preschool attendance appeared to be reflected in 
school achievement. 

8. Decreases in IQ were made by non-preschool children in an 
orphanage. 

9. Practice effects do not account for the changes. 

10. Coaching does not account for the gains. 

11. Tests at the preschool ages are fairly reliable and valid. 

2. Mental Growth during the Elementary-school Years 

1. The pattern of change varied with different elementary schools. 

2. Gains were made in the University School by non-preschool 
children; slightly less gain by preschool children. 

3. The peak of gain was reached in two years. 

4. Practice effects do not account for the gains. 

3. Mental Growth from Preschool to High School and College 

1. High-school intelligence-test scores were related to length of 
attendance in the University Schools. 

2. Preschool children made higher scores on the high-school test 
than did non-preschool children. 

3. College-entrance examination scores were related to length, of 
attendance in the University Schools. 

4. Preschool children made higher scores on the college-entrance 
examination than did non-preschool children. 

50 



THE ROLE OF ENVIRONMENT 

Criticisms by psychologists of the Iowa studies have 
been sharp. Leta Hollingworth 1 points out that Wellman 
"does not discuss these limitations of tests made at 
Iowa on preschool children. She does not take into 
consideration that tests standardized for preschool 
children are always brought out on less than a random 
sample. She uncritically accepts tests developed years 
ago for preschool ages as being tests standardized on the 
same samples of population as those now used during 
school years, which is not the case. Again, Wellman 
never considers the influence of negativism at all." 

Terman, 2 in an even more pointed criticism of the 
Iowa Studies, says, "The reader cannot fail to be 
impressed by the number of variables left uncontrolled 
in these investigations, by the faulty statistics employed, 
and even more by the extraordinary discrepancies 
between data presented and the conclusions drawn." 

It is too early to adequately evaluate the Iowa Studies. 
They must be continued for a number of years before 
well-founded conclusions can be drawn. It is interesting 
to note that even the Iowa workers, 3 although stressing 
the contributions which environment can make, admit 
that "the child can only be what he could have become." 
Stoddard and Wellman go on to say that "the lowa- 
Binet theory of intelligence simply permits a large 
amount of change in a child's brightness through 
environmental impingements on the organism." 4 

Undoubtedly it is possible through education to do 
a great deal more in the matter of developing intellect 

1 HOLLINGWOETH, L. S., "Thirty-ninth. Yearbook of the National 
Society for the Study of Education," Part I, p. 452, 1940. 

2 TERMAN, L. M., "Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National Society for 
the Study of Education," Part I, p. 461, 1940. 

3 STODDABD, G. D., and B. L. WELLMAN, "Thirty-nintt Yearbook of 
the National Society for the Study of Education," Part I, p. 431, 1940. 

4 Ibid., p. 436. 

51 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

than has been done in the past. A child may possess great 
potentialities but he needs the right training and the 
right physical, mental, and emotional environment if 
he is to achieve on the level for which his inheritance 
equipped him. 

CONCLUSIONS 

It is an accepted fact, then, that an improved environ- 
ment will be likely to effect a positive change in a score 
on an intelligence test. All studies, except those recently 
coming from Iowa, indicate, however, that this change 
is small. It is usually greater with preschool children 
than with school children. The fact that such changes 
do occur by no means proves that there has also occurred 
an actual change in the relative status of the mental 
capacity of the child. The LQ. does not express the exact 
limits of a child's ability. It is, rather, the closest approxi- 
mation that it is possible to obtain. As Terman 1 says : 

An obtained IQ is not only subject to chance errors resulting from 
inadequate samplings of abilities, but also to numerous constant 
errors, including practice effects, negativism or shyness, the personal 
equation of the examiner, and standardization errors in the test used. 
For these reasons an obtained IQ should never be taken as a final 
verdict, but only as a point of departure for further investigation of a 
subject. 

The task of teachers, and it is a very important one, 
is to help the child to do as well as he can. This principle 
applies to the intellectually gifted pupil as well as to 
those who are of average or subnormal mentality. There 
is a possibility that even at the University of Iowa the 
gifted child is not receiving the right kind of stimulation 
in sufficient amount, and that this is the reason why his 

1 TEBMANT, L. M., "Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National Society for 
the Study of Education," Part I, p. 466, 1940. 

52 



THE ROLE OF ENVIRONMENT 

I.Q. shows a less marked increase than that of the aver- 
age and below-average child. 

The studies reported in this chapter and in the one 
preceding add weight to the assertion that heredity and 
environment are interdependent. Every environmental 
situation must be interpreted in terms of the individual 
reacting to it, and his reaction will, in turn, be determined 
by the nature of his inherited bodily mechanism and 
of his earlier experiences. 



53 



CHAPTER IV 
PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 

1. What is the relationship between physical size and intellect? 

2. Do gifted children excel in athletic activities? 

3. Are geniuses likely to be mentally unbalanced? 

There are a number of generally held misconceptions 
concerning the physical characteristics and motor abili- 
ties of mentally superior children and adults. An exam- 
ination of a few of these, together with an attempt to 
explain why they are so insistently held even by well- 
informed people, should contribute toward a better 
understanding of the causes which have created a picture 
of the genius so at variance with the facts. In this picture 
he appears as undersized, with narrow, stooping shoul- 
ders, but with a large head and high forehead. He is 
especially if a girl homely and likely to possess at least 
one feature which is sufficiently out of proportion and 
peculiar to make him appear ludicrous. He is weak 
and inept in handling himself. He is poor in penmanship 
and art work, slow and awkward in manual training, and, 
of course, nonathletic. 

REASONS FOE BELIEF IN INFEKIOK PHYSICAL EQUIPMENT 

Data gathered in scientific investigations will b$ 
presented later in this chapter to show that actually the 
typical gifted child is not at all a slight, poorly coordi- 
nated, or in any way incongruous individual. Then why 
do most people honestly believe him to be so? There are 
four fundamental reasons, and at least two of them are so 

54 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 

deeply rooted in human nature that to extirpate them is 
impossible. The four are as follows: first, overlooking 
the chronological- and mental-age factors; second, gen- 
eralizing from too few instances; third, desiring to believe 
in a law of compensation; fourth, envy. 

Chronological- and Mental-age Factors 

The inability of others to keep his age in mind, has 
always handicapped the bright child and probably will 
continue to do so. For instance, there is the case of the 
boy with an I.Q. of 160 who entered the second grade in 
a large private school at the age of six. For weeks he 
underwent a barrage of criticism from teachers and 
classmates alike because he could not handle sweaters 
and buttons with the same facility as the other children 
in his grade. No one, not even his well-trained teachers, 
took cognizance of the fact that he was the youngest 
child in his class, the average age being something more 
than a year greater than his. It was especially easy to 
make this mistake since the boy, though the youngest, 
was also the largest, with respect to both height and 
weight. In this case, discrepancy between size and 
chronological age also added to the difficulty, for the 
child was expected, though only six, to do as well as or 
even better than his seven-year-old classmates since he 
appeared to be eight. 

The correlation between physical size and motor 
coordination is far from perfect. The child who at four 
is as large as the average seven-year-old is not likely to 
handle himself so well as one aged seven who is in size 
typical of his age group. A more clear-cut example would 
be a comparison of a 6-foot adolescent with a 6-foot man 
in his middle twenties. Though similar in height and 
weight the chances would be all against the boy's being 
equal to the man in bodily control. 

55 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

The brightest in a class is also likely to be the youngest. 
With that fact in mind it is easy to see why, when the 
valedictorian of a high school graduating class stands 
before his audience to deliver the valedictory speech, his 
listeners should note how small he appears among his 
classmates. It is but a step from that observation to the 
conclusion that all bright children are undersized. The 
boy's listeners have completely overlooked the fact that 
he is only sixteen while the average age of the adolescents 
around him is eighteen. Give him two more years in 
which to grow and he will quite surely be as large as if 
not slightly larger than they. 

It is equally easy to overlook the importance of the 
mental-age factor in evaluating the physical character- 
istics of a gifted child. High mentality carries with it 
an ability for self-criticism and for properly evaluating 
the various school activities which is far in advance of 
that possessed by children of similar age but of average 
mentality. This critical capacity often leads a superior 
child to concentrate on those things which he can do 
well and which he feels are important. If he is poor in 
penmanship and there is a slight negative correlation 
between intelligence and hand-writing the cause is not 
likely to be a deficiency in coordination but rather a 
realization by the child that excellent penmanship is 
merely a grace note in this day of dictaphones and type- 
writers. If he is not on the football team, the reason may 
not lie in an inability to play football but rather in his 
desire to devote his energy to preparing for a profession. 
If he is slow in manual training, it may not be because 
of an innate awkwardness, but because his mind' is 
occupied with some intellectual problem. 

The gifted child is always "old for his age." This fact 
should never be overlooked in passing judgment on 
his qualities ? physical or otherwise. 

56 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 

Hasty Generalizations 

A second cause of the illusion concerning the physical 
characteristics of mentally superior children is the 
universal tendency to generalize from too few instances 
and to permit such generalizations to be influenced by 
wishful thinking. If it seems reasonable that a genius 
should have an extremely large head and a very high 
forehead, then it is natural to look for three or four 
examples in support of that theory. The final step is to 
generalize from these instances to a conclusion which is 
identical with the belief held before the cases were 
selected. If it seems reasonable that a man who has read 
many books should be a physical weakling, then it is not 
hard to remember sickly or even bedridden intellectual 
giants. As a high school student the author was given the 
impression by his English teacher that Robert Louis 
Stevenson was a great writer because he was an invalid. 

This kind of reasoning is by no means limited to prob- 
lems relating to genius. It is a limitation coin in on to all 
men everywhere and works havoc to their thinking in 
any field. 

Theory of Compensation 

Most individuals possess the urge to endow the strong 
with compensating weaknesses and the weak with com- 
pensating powers. This desire has resulted in such widely 
held convictions as: morons and other feeble-minded 
individuals possess great strength; beautiful moving- 
picture actresses are of low mentality -and of lower 
morals; those who are gifted in the arts are extremely 
eccentric if not actually insane; the beautiful child will 
develop into an unprepossessing adult; the rapid learner 
forgets quickly and, conversely, the slow learner retains 
well; the rich are blackguards and the poor, honest men; 

57 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

a slow worker is painstaking and a fast worker careless; 
the good die young; the very bright child will grow into 
an average or below-average adult, and the average or 
dull child will be the successful man of tomorrow; a 
genius is also a fool. 

This fundamental motivation of innumerable widely 
held beliefs has exerted a considerable influence upon the 
prevalent opinion concerning the physical characteristics 
of intellectually gifted children. If, so the reasoning goes, 
the child has a superior mind, it is no more than right 
that he should have an inferior body. If he can read 
better, handle figures with greater facility, knows more 
geography and history than his classmates, and can 
discuss with an understanding far beyond his years 
problems of national and international importance, then 
surely he must possess startling weaknesses which will 
reduce Him to human proportions in the eyes of his 
fellows. Thus, if he happens to be small, or sickly, or 
homely, or lacking in social graces, the fact is noted with 
certain satisfaction. 

To the credit of mankind it should be pointed out that 
people are equally eager to supply the dull child with 
gifts. Their insistence on believing in compensation is, 
actually, a manifestation of essential kindness. 

Envy 

Another source of the illusion concerning precocious 
children is one not nearly so complimentary to human 
nature as the desire to believe in compensation. It is 
envy. The bright child will, from infancy on, find this 
feeling in the attitude of others toward him. To vitiate 
its effectiveness he must possess and make use of more 
charm and patience and understanding than will ever be 
required of his less gifted fellows. Again and again envy 
will cause individuals to point out and to emphasize his 

58 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 

faults. Envy will color not only the attitude of casual 
acquaintances but also that of his teachers, his playmates, 
his brothers and sisters, and even his own parents. Envy 
has helped to create, and will help to perpetuate, a false 
picture of him. 

PHYSICAL CHAEACTEEISTICS 

Since the early work of Galton, there have been a 
number of studies of the physical characteristics of 
intellectually gifted children and adults. There follows a 
presentation of the results of several significant investiga- 
tions. It will be seen from them that the true picture of 
the physical equipment of mentally superior individuals 
is much different from that which takes its form from the 
four sources just discussed. 

Height and Weight 

Galton, as a result of his pioneer studies in the nine- 
teenth century, concluded that there was a marked 
relationship between height or weight and intellect. He 1 

says: 

There is a prevelant belief that men of genius are unhealthy, puny 
beings all brain and no muscle weak-sighted, and generally of 
poor constitutions. I think most of my readers would be surprised at 
the stature and physical frames of the heroes of history, who fill my 
pages, if they could be assembled together in a hall, ... I do not 
deny that many men of extraordinary mental gifts have had wretched 
constitutions, but deny them to be an essential or even the usual 
accompaniment. , . . It is the second and third rate students who 
are usually weakly. A collection of living magnates in various 
branches of intellectual achievement is always a feast to my eyes; 
being, as they are, such massive, vigorous, capable-looking animals. 

Later investigations have indicated that Galton some- 
what overstated Ms case, but they have corroborated his 

1 GALTON, F., "Hereditary Genius," p. 321, Macmillan & Co., Ltd., 
London, 1869; reprinted 1925. 

59 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

assertion that a positive correlation does exist between 
physique and intellect, though that correlation is very 
low. Paterson, 1 after a critical evaluation of all the more 
important studies of height and weight in relation to 
intellect, concludes as follows: 

We may summarize the general trend of this critical review of the 
studies on the relation between height and weight and intelligence in 
normal children by stating that a slight positive correlation seems 
to exist between stature or weight and intelligence. The emphasis can 
be on either of two points : on the one hand, we would emphasize the 
fact that relationship, even though slight, is positive; on the other 
hand ... it is important to emphasize the fact that physical status 
and mental status are to a great extent independent of one another. 

In terms of coefficients of correlation the relationship 
between height or weight and intelligence is approxi- 
mately 0.15. Some studies have resulted in a higher figure 
and others in a lower, but this is typical. For example, 
Murdock and Sullivan 2 selected data on the height, 
weight, and intelligence of nearly 600 children in Hono- 
lulu who were of old American, British, German, or 
Scandinavian descent, making, as Paterson points out, a 
fairly homogeneous race group. The mental measure- 
ments were made by Catherine Murdock, a school 
psychologist, and the physical measurements by L. R. 
Sullivan, an anthropologist representing the American 
Museum of Natural History in New York. They reported 
a Pearson r of .16 .03 between weight and I.Q. for 
595 children and a Pearson r of .14 .03 between height 
and I.Q. for 597 children. These correlations are not high, 
but they become extremely significant when viewed in 

1 PATERSON, D. G., "Physique and Intellect," pp. 51-52, D. Appleton- 
Century Company, Inc., New York, 1930, 

2 MURDOCK, C., and L. H. SULLIVAN, A Contribution to the Study of 
Mental and Physical Measurements in Normal Children, Amer. Phys. 
Educ. Rev., Vol. 28, pp. 209-215, 276-280, 328-330, 1923. 

60 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 



the light of the generally held belief in the existence of a 
negative relationship between intellect and physical size. 
They indicate that a typical intellectually gifted child 
will be somewhat taller and weigh somewhat more than 
a typical child of average mentality, who in turn will be 
taller and weigh more than the typical child of low 
mentality. 

TABLE V. TABULATION SHOWING How HEIGHT (IN INCHES) Is DIS- 
TRIBUTED AMONG THREE GROUPS OF CHJLDEEN, NINE TO 
ELEVEN YEAES OLD 



Inches 


Group A, 
LQ. above 135, 
(median LQ. 
of 151) 


Group B, 
LQ. 90-110 
(median LQ. 
of 100) 


Group C, 
LQ. below 65 
(median LQ. 
of 43) 


59 


1 






58 








57 


3 






56 . 


4 


1 




55 


4 


1 


1 


54 


8 


2 


3 


53 


2 


3 


4 


52 


9 


% 8 


2 


51 


8 


10 


3 


50 


3 


7 


6 


49 


1 


8 


10 


48 


2 


5 


3 


47 




. . 


3 


46 


f m 


, f 


5 


45 


- 


- 


2 


44 








43 


. . 


. , 


1 


42 


. 


. . 


1 


41 








40 
Total 






1 


45 


45 


45 



* Adapted from HOLXINGWOBTH, Li S., "Gifted Children,*' p. 80, The Macmillan 
Company, New York, 1926. 

61 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



Supporting the preceding statement are the results of a 
study made by Hollingworth and Taylor who matched 
each of a group of forty-five intellectually gifted children, 
ranging in intelligence from 135 I.Q. to 190 I.Q., with a 
child rating between 90 I.Q. and 110 I.Q. and with 
another belonging in the feeble-minded group. Age, race, 
and sex were kept constant. The data concerning height 
appear in Table V and for weight in Table VI. 

The measurements in Table V when reduced to 
averages show that the medium height of the gifted was 
52.9 inches, that of the average 51.2 inches, and that of 
the feeble-minded 49.6 inches. Corresponding results with 
respect to weight appear in Table VI. 
TABLE VI. TABULATION SHOWING How WEIGHT (IN POUNDS) Is Dis- 

TEIBUTED AMONG THREE GROUPS OF CHILDREN, NlNE TO ELEVEN 

YEARS OLD* 





Group A, I.Q. above 


Group B, I.Q. 90- 


Group C, I.Q. below 


Pounds 


135 (median I.Q. 


100 (median I.Q. 


65 (median I.Q. 




151) 


100) 


43) 


115-110 


2 






110-105 








105-110 








100- 95 


2 






95- 90 


2 


1 




90- 85 


3 


1 




85- 80 


6 


2 


1 


80- 75 


5 


1 


2 


75- 70 


9 


6 


6 


70- 65 


4 


10 


3 


65- 60 


8 


9 


8 


60- 55 


3 


9 


13 


55- 50 


1 


5 


4 


50- 45 




1 


6 


45- 40 






2 


Total 


45 


45 


45 



* Adapted from HOLLINGWOBTH, "Gifted Children," p. 84. 

62 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 

A TL S. Public Health. Report contains statistics on 
the relative height and weight of children on different 
intellectual levels. The groups compared are not sharply 

TABLE VII. AVERAGE MEASUREMENTS or STANDING HEIGHT AND 

WEIGHT FOB THREE GROUPS OF CHZLDKEN CLASSIFIED ACCORDING 

TO I.Q., SEX, AND AGE* 



Age 


Boys, I.Q. 


Girls, I.Q. 


Under 90 


90-110 


110 up 


Under 90 


90-110 


110 up 



Number 



8 


16 


135 


86 


20 


130 


89 


9 


43 


127 


70 


34 


129 


60 


10 


34 


112 


61 


32 


111 


68 


11 


64 


93 


81 


48 


88 


75 


12 


46 


55 


122 


44 


76 


88 


13 


41 


52 


66 


17 


58 


52 


14 


22 


41 


33 


14 


41 


33 



Height, centimeters 



8 


121.94 


124.61 


126.60 


120.15 


123.86 


124.60 


9 


129.28 


129.92 


131 . 16 


125.26 


129.71 


130.58 


10 


132.88 


134.63 


135.67 


133.69 


133.12 


136.57 


11 


138.47 


138.71 


142.16 


138.52 


140.25 


141.67 


12 


143.13 


145.09 


146.32 


141.32 


145.72 


147.60 


13 


149.05 


149.04 


150.80 


147.82 


150.21 


152.87 


14 


150.95 


156.80 


156.97 


151.93 


153.88 


157.03 



Weight, kilograms 



8 


23.63 


25.35 


26.18 


23.08 


24.33 


24.36 


9 


24.48 


27.69 


28.53 


24.24 


27.34 


28.26 


10 


28.66 


30.15 


31.23 


29.61 


29.56 


31.41 


11 


32.39 


32.31 


34.96 


32.25 


34.79 


35.15 


12 


35.14 


37.43 


37.48 


34.43 


37.34 


38.70 


13 


39.21 


39.70 


41.03 


41.77 


41.58 


42.83 


14 


41.31 


45.54 


47.13 


43.31 


45.02 


48.07 



* Adapted from U. S. Public Health Report, VoL 44, No. 29, pp. 1774-1775, July 19, 
1929. 

63 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

differentiated as were the subjects in the Hollingworth- 
Taylor investigation. Nevertheless, an examination of the 
figures discloses an unmistakable trend to ward the greater 
size of the brighter children. These differences are small 
and not always present, but the tendency is significant 
(see Table VII). 

Gray, in a study of the undergraduate careers of a 
group of children who entered Columbia and Barnard 
colleges before their sixteenth birthday, gives data con- 
cerning their height and weight. This group was made 
up of 126 boys and 28 girls. The average age of the boys 
was 15 years 6 months and of the girls 15 years 8 months, 
which made them approximately three years younger 
than the average college freshman, who, as Gray points 
out, begins his higher education at the age of 18 years 
7 months. 

The height and weight of these gifted young college 
students were checked against the Baldwin- Wood norms 
for boys and girls of the same age and against Gray's 
control group of college students of the same sex and 
college year. Gray 1 reports that in the case of the boys 
the young group's average deviation from the norms for 
their ages was plus 1.96 points in weight and plus 0.77 
inches in height; in the case of the girls, the average 
deviation for the young group was plus 0.47 inches in 
height and plus 2.29 pounds in weight. When the gifted 
college students were compared with the control group, 
the boys were found to be 1.84 inches shorter and 14 
pounds lighter, and the girls 0.86 inches shorter and 
8.41 pounds lighter. The reason for the difference in favor 
of the control group lay in the fact that the gifted stu- 

1 GBAY, H. A., "Factors in the Undergraduate Careers of Young 
College Students," p. 42, Teachers College, Columbia University, New 
York, 1930. 

64 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 



4*1 

r 





I 



, 

a 



o 
p=c 

o 

| 
I 



i 

5 









OOC^O> 

IOUDOO 



OJb-t 

OOOOO 



OCOCCOi U3*-400t-00 



1>*OOO>O ?H( 



s .a 



65 



T-IOOCC 


OODJOO 


ON- ^1-1 

^ "tfiOO 


ON>i-t 

ooooc 


OO)CO 


^coocooo 


O5Or-tOO 

^u^OO 


OJ^<NW<*< 

i>ooco-i 
f-(1-H 


eo )** 


OCO*-(Ob- 


oo<?<a> 
>oooo 


oooi>.oo 

t>OOOSOr-l 
T-Hr-( 


otooo 


CO<NC<OON- 


OUSOO 
^tOOO 


^;*^H>00 
t*-GOCSOi-l 


j-tcioec 


t-OSOT-Cl 


oowooco 

Ti<iotOO 


OdOO OOOOO 

ot^csoo 


tOWOSCO 


ONO<NO 


0*OtOi-< 

o co cot- 


eob-o^oo 
oooooi-to 


cot^. ^^ 


ossooo 


eOt^ON 
Tj<-*O0 


>*ecOOJ 
1O0O 


r-co-^r-i 


OJbOOO 


OCDi-ifO 
rt*rt<30 


tONGST-ICO 
kQiQ*dC0<0 


OCOGDN. 


o eo^t-ec 


DOOOC<J 
-^^tOtO 


Ot^O>OrH 

0otO(ieo 


eo eo eo eo 


b-nrtt^ti-4 


^S2 


*t*Oi-iN 

iQkOtOOCO 


osoomua 


CJCOOfNO 


^OOi-i 
-tf^TMO 


WOQ'-<rH 

oo5ooeo 


ooeoo 


tOO-ICCO 


^2 


OOOr-ICq-* 

10*0000 


t^oooo 


fHOCO^IU^ 
r-HHrHi-tiH 



1 



:- IIBJS 






e_w 



_ 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

dents were more than two years younger than the control 
group. 

Terman, in his report on the California gifted group, 
presents figures showing that mentally superior children 
are superior in height and weight to unselected children. 
A summary of the data gathered by Bird T. Baldwin and 
his assistants appear in Table VIII. 

It is a striking fact that the gifted group is inferior in 
only one instance in the large number of comparisons 
appearing in Table VIII. The exception is that of the 
fifteen-year-old gifted boys, whose mean average weight 
was 0.7 pounds less than that of the boys in the Boas- 
Oakland group. In every comparison with every other 
age and sex group the California gifted children were 
superior. In the face of these figures it is impossible to 
maintain that the typical gifted child is undersized. 
Baldwin, 1 commenting on his findings with respect to 
these 594 bright children, says : 

Since the weight-height relationship of a child furnishes one of the 
best general criteria for its physical status, the average heights and 
weights for all the children included between the ages of 7 and 15 were 
first computed and the results compared with those of earlier writers 
on California children and on a few representative groups in other 
parts of the United States. The results . . . show . . . that this 
group, measured by the group average, is physically superior in both 
height and weight for age, although several children are small and 
some are considerably under weight. The Oakland children measured 
by Barnes in 1892 are considerably inferior to this group, although 
they were heavier and taller than similar groups of children from 
Boston, Worcester, Toronto, St. Louis, and Milwaukee. . . . The 
Oakland children later studied by Boas were superior to those studied 
by Barnes, but inferior to those included in this study. The Daven- 
port group represents a selection from the best residential district in 
the city. The Oak Park group is from one of the most favored social 
sections of Chicago. Faber's study, in 1923, was a group of California 

1 TEBMAN, L. M., " Genetic Studies of Genius/' Vol. I, p. 144, 2d ed., 
Stanford University Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1926. 

66 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 

children. The California gifted children excel them all in height and 
weight, for all ages included. They also excel the early Boas-Burk 
averages for the country at large, when approximately 90,000 children 
for height averages, and 68,000 for weight averages, between the ages 
of 5> and 18% years, were included. 



Constancy of Height and Weight Status 

If, as Galton and others have pointed out, adult 
geniuses are taller and heavier than ordinary men, and if, 
as the studies reported in the preceding pages show, 
intellectually gifted children are likewise taller and 
heavier than children of average mentality, then it would 
appear that the bright child maintains his superiority in 
size as he grows to maturity. Only a few studies have 
been made of the yearly increments in growth of gifted 
children. One of the best of these is that by Leta 
Hollingworth. 

Hollingworth 1 recorded the successive measurements, 
for a period of seven years, of each of a group of forty- 
seven intellectually gifted children. The mean LQ. of the 
group was 155, the lowest being 135. These children 
ranged in age from seven to nine, at the time the study 
was begun. Their heights were measured at, regular inter- 
vals of twelve months from January, 1923, to January, 
1929, inclusive. Each child was compared year after year 
with the norm appropriate to his age, race, and sex. 
A ratio was then given to indicate where the child stood 
in relation to those of average intelligence, 

At the time of the first measurements, Hollingworth 
found that forty-one of the forty-seven children were 
above their respective height norm. At the time of the 
seventh and final measurements, forty-two were above 

1 HOLLINGWORTH, L. S., Do Intellectually Gifted Children Grow 
toward Mediocrity in Stature? J. Genet. PsychoL, Vol. 37, pp. 345-350, 
1930. 

67 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



and five below the appropriate norm. There were some 
variations on the part of a few of the children, but in the 
main they demonstrated a striking consistency in the 
constancy with which they maintained their relative 
positions. Professor Hollingworth took 100 as a ratio to 



106 



105 



104 



: 103 



CfL 



102 



101 
100 



1929 



1923 1924 1925 1926 1927 1928 

Year 

FIG. 4. Mean ratio of stature of gifted to norms for age, sex, and race over a 

period of years. 

indicate exact correspondence to the norm. The results, 
condensed into a graph, appear in Fig. 4. 

Cranial Measurements 

There are a number of reasons for the mistaken 
impression that an intimate relationship exists between 
head size and intellect. If intellect is dependent upon the 
brain, then why should it not follow that the larger the 
brain the greater the intellect? Since a large brain re- 
quires a large skull to encase it, then why should it not 
be true that individuals with large heads are intellectual 
giants? The story is told of the new village minister who 
struggled, during his first two or three Sunday sermons, 
to impress the man with the large head, noble forehead, 
and Van Dyke beard who sat in the front pew. The 

68 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 

minister was somewhat taken aback, later, to find that 
this Charles-Evans-Hnghes-like member of his congre- 
gation was feeble-minded. 

The fact that the most startling type in the lower 
feeble-minded group is the microcephalic idiot has pro- 
vided the kind of proof that is eagerly seized upon, viz., 
the spectacular and arresting instance. With the picture 
of a microcephalic idiot in mind, one easily forgets the 
much larger number of idiots whose head size approxi- 
mates normality. 

The scientists who made the first quantitative studies 
of the relationship between head size and intellect 
found a slight positive correlation. They made the mis- 
take, however, (as Paterson points out in an excellent 
chapter on cranial measurements and intelligence in his 
book " Physique and Intellect") of overemphasizing the 
significance of these correlations and of high-lighting 
those which were considerable. 

The first study of this subject was published by 
Galton in 1888. He concluded that there is an appreciable 
relationship between scholastic success and head size. 
Murdock and Sullivan, in the study referred to earlier 
in this chapter, correlated the head diameter and LQ. 
of approximately six hundred children and found the 
relationship to be .22 .03. Reed and Mulligan 1 corre- 
lated the cranial capacity and scholastic performance of 
449 male students at Aberdeen University and found a 
coefficient of .08 .03. Sommerville, 2 in a study of 
Columbia University students, reports a coefficient of 
correlation for intelligence and head length of .10, for 
intelligence and head height '.09. 

1 REED, R. W., and J. H. MULLIGAN, Relation of Cranial Capacity to 
Intelligence, J. Royal Anthropological InsL, Vol. 53, pp. 322-332, 1923. 

2 SOMMERVILLE, R. C., Physical, Motor, and Sensory Traits, Arch. 
Psychol., Vol. 12, pp. 1-108, 1924. 

69 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



Other studies made during the past twenty years have 
produced corroborating data showing this tendency 
toward a slight positive correlation. Thus the U. S. 
Public Health Service presents the statistics found in 
Table IX concerning 2,707 children. 

TABLE IX. AVERAGE HEAD MEASTJEEMENTS FOB THEEE GBOTTPS or 
CHILDREN, CLASSIFIED ACCORDING TO I.Q., SEX, AND AGE* 



Age 


Boys 


Girls 


LQ. 

under 
90 


LQ. 
90-110 


LQ. 
110 or 
over 


LQ. 
under 
90 


LQ. 

90-110 


LQ. 

110 or 
over 



Cephalic index 



8 


80.00 


81.04 


80.20 


80.60 


80,56 


79.76 


9 .. . 


81 47 


80.68 


79.83 


81.35 


80.36 


79 47 


10 


81 32 


80.72 


80.48 


80.78 


80.88 


80.04 


11 


80 61 


81 27 


78 90 


80 77 


79 30 


79 79 


12 


80.65 


80.49 


79.58 


80.23 


79.89 


79 85 


13 


80.88 


80.29 


79.12 


80.06 


80.36 


79.71 


14 


80.05 


80 15 


79 58 


80.50 


79 10 


80 85 

















Head module, f cubic centimeters 



8 


15 17 


15 25 


15 35 


14 68 


14 86 


14 91 


9 


15.33 


15.29 


15.37 


14.78 


14 96 


15 01 


10.. . 


15.23 


15 38 


15 43 


14 92 


14 97 


15 08 


11 


15.50 


15.38 


15 39 


15 05 


15 12 


15 14 


12 .. 


15 49 


15 49 


15 62 


14 99 


15 14 


15 23 


13 


15.57 


15.58 


15.68 


15 07 


15 23 


15 34 


14 


15.51 


15.73 


15.87 


15.17 


15 34 


15 54 

















* Adapted from U. S. Public Health Kept., Vol. 44, No. 29, pp. 1774-1775, July 19, 
1929. 

t Module is average of length. 

In a study of intellectually gifted children, L. S. 
Hollingworth. compared the average size and shape of 
head of a group of forty-five mentally superior children 
with a group whose intelligence ranged between 90 and 
100 LQ. Each gifted child was matched with a control 

70 



PHYSIQUE AKD HEALTH 

child according to age, race, and sex. In commenting on 
the measurements which she obtained, Hollingworth 1 
says: 

The gifted have, therefore, larger heads than the ungifted, but only 
in accordance with their greater size in other respects. There is an 
interesting difference also in shape of head, between gifted and 
ungifted, in so far as shape is shown by cephalic index (which is the 
ratio found between width and length). There is no reliable difference 
between the two groups in width of head, as indicated by the probable 
error of the difference. There is, however, a reliably greater length 
of skull among the gifted. The gifted tend to be long-headed in com- 
parison with their ungifted contemporaries of the same age, race, and 
sex. 

Here again it must be pointed out that the overlapping in both 
size and shape of skull between gifted and ungifted is so extensive 
that intellect cannot be safely inferred from cranial dimensions in 
an individual case. The chances are more than even that a long- 
headed child will be very intelligent, but they fall far from certainty 
for an individual chosen at random. 

Hollingworth uncovers somewhat greater differences 
than have usually been found and emphasizes their 
importance. Her conclusions are not in agreement with 
those of Sorokin, who sees no significance in dolicho- 
cephaly, or long-headedness, or with MacDonald, who 
found a slight negative rather than a positive relation- 
ship. Paterson, 2 in his summary of the studies of cranial 
measurement and intelligence, says, "Head shape varies 
as a racial characteristic irrespective of the intellectual 
qualities exhibited by the several racial groups. Within a 
given racial strain, head shape appears to be indifferently 
related to intellect." 

It would appear, then, that intellectually gifted 
children have, on the average, slightly larger heads than 
children of lower intelligence. This difference, however, 

1 HOLLINGWORTH, op. cit., pp. 91-02. 
3 PATBBSON, op. dt. t p. 123. 

71 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

is extremely small and, when the slightly greater height 
and weight of the mentally superior child are taken into 
consideration, probably not at all out of proportion. 

Physical Beauty 

"Beautiful but dumb" is a phrase that has been 
current for a long time. It tersely expresses a tenaciously 
held belief. 

It is very difficult to investigate the relationship 
between physical beauty and intelligence, for beauty is 
the result of a composite of traits which cannot be ade- 
quately measured. Consequently, there have been very 
few scientific studies to test the assumption that the 
homely girl is bright and her beautiful cousin dull. To 
select instances like Shirley Temple, whose LQ. is well 
above the 130 customarily set as the minimum for intel- 
lectually gifted children, is to give an interesting example, 
but it proves nothing. One might reason along the line 
that, since there is a positive correlation of desirable 
traits and since the dull and feeble-minded as a group 
are likely to possess more physical stigmata than superior 
children, there must be a slight relationship between 
physical beauty and intellectual capacity. Probably such 
is the case, but there can be no finality attached to a 
conclusion readied in this manner. 

Almost the only carefully controlled investigation in 
this field has been made by Leta Hollingworth, 1 who 
compared the physical beauty of a small group of intel- 
lectually gifted children with a matched group of control 
children. There were thirteen boys and seven girls in each 
of the two groups. All were of the white race and between 
fourteen and fifteen years of age. The children were 
photographed under conditions as nearly identical as pos- 

1 HOLLINGWORTH, L. S., Comparative Beauty of Faces of Highly 
Intelligent Adolescents, Fed. Sem., Vol. 47, pp. 268-81 Dec., 1935. 

72 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 

sible. Two pictures were taken of each and the clearer 
one was given to the judges, who, ignorant of the mental- 
ity of the subjects, were asked to arrange them in order 
of merit. Twenty graduate students of education and 
their wives, between twenty-five and thirty-five years of 
age, and two older professors acted as judges. They di- 
vided the forty photographs into five piles of eight each, 
according to their judgment of the degree of beauty 
represented. Then each pile was arranged in order from 
one to eight. According to these judges, the faces of the 
intellectually gifted children were more attractive than 
the faces of the members of the control group, who were 
of average mentality. 

Conclusions 

Baldwin, cooperating with Terman in his study of the 
California group of gifted children, took 37 anthropo- 
metric measurements on each of 594 children between the 
ages of two and fifteen. Of these 312 were boys and 282 
girls. The characteristics measured were as follows: 

Height: 

Standing 

Sitting 

Stem length to vertex 

Stem length to sternal notch 
Head: 

Anterior-posterior diameter 

Transverse diameter 

Height 

Circumference 

Width of face 

Length of face 
Shoulders: 

Width 
Arms: 

Span 

Length from shoulder to elbow, right and left 
73 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Length from elbow to finger tip, right and left 

Width of wrist, proximal and distal, right and left 

Circumference of wrist, right and left 
Chest: 

Width 

Depth 

Circumference 
Hips: 

Width at ischia 

Width at trochanters 

Circumference 
Legs: 

Length 

Breathing capacity 
Grip: 

Right 

Left 
Weight, nude 

After an analysis of the 21,978 measurements obtained, 
Baldwin 1 was able to reach a number of conclusions : 

1. The gifted children deviate in a positive direction from the 
Baldwin weight, height, age, breathing standards for American-born 
children, but 62 to 73 per cent deviate not more than 10 per cent 
above or below these norms. 

2. A large proportion have broad shoulders and hips, strong 
muscles, and well-developed lungs. 

3. These children excel the children of a control group in Oak Park, 
HI., in four selected physical traits: arm span, width of shoulders, 
width of hips, and grip. 

4. Various types of cephalic indices are found within particular 
nationality groups represented by these children, but the majority 
of the children are of the meso cephalic type. 

5. The results of this investigation show that the gifted group 
is, as a whole, physically superior to the various groups used for 
comparison. 

The conclusion growing out of the data presented in 
this chapter, that gifted children as a group are superior 
to children of average mentality in their physical equip- 

1 TERMAN, op. tit., pp. 169-171. 

74 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 

ment, is significant even though the relationship between 
intelligence and physique is so slight as to preclude any 
possibility of prophesying that any one bright child will 
be physically superior or, conversely, that a physically 
superior child will be bright. The significance lies in the 
fact that it disproves the generally held belief that preco- 
cious children are physical weaklings. There are times 
when a coefficient of correlation even as low as .10 assumes 
considerable importance. This is one of those times. 

PABTICIPATION IN ATHLETICS 

The question of the degree to which intellectually 
gifted children participate in athletics has been scarcely 
touched upon by investigators. It is known that for un- 
selected groups there is little, if any, relationship between 
intelligence and athletic activity. Before presenting ob- 
jective data, it will be well to consider a few of the factors 
which influence the gifted child in making his decisions 
concerning competing for a place on one or more of his 
school teams. 

Intellectual curiosity is nearly always associated with 
high intelligence; consequently, the mentally superior 
child, eager to learn more of the exciting facts which he 
senses all about him, may feel that he would be losing 
precious hours if he were practicing and playing football 
or baseball. The less intelligent boy, with relatively little 
interest in abstract knowledge, is much more likely to 
find his attention spontaneously turned to sports of vari- 
ous kinds. If the gifted child, however, does decide to 
participate, the chances are somewhat better than even 
that he will excel the boy of average mentality who is of 
the same chronological age. 

The age of the gifted child is frequently an obstacle 
which he must overcome if he is to take part in sports. 
For example, if he is in high school, the chances are that 

75 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

he is from one to three years younger than the majority 
of the boys in his class. For a fourteen-year-old child to 
compete against those who are sixteen years old, either 
in football, where weight is important, or in basketball, 
where height is an asset, is very difficult. In order to 
participate successfully, his youth must be compensated 
for by his greater intelligence, by his being large for his 
age, or by a fortunate combination of the two. 

There are at least three points in favor of the superior 
child's being able to participate in school sports. In the 
first place he needs to spend so little time on the required 
academic work that he has plenty of leisure hours to do 
with as he pleases. Much studying at home in order to 
earn passing grades is not necessary for him, as it is 'for 
the average student. In the second place, if he is a 
typical gifted child, he is somewhat better equipped 
physically than boys of average intelligence. If he hap- 
pens to be markedly superior in this respect, he may feel 
almost as strong an urge to excel in athletics as to excel 
in scholarship. There have been a number of college boys 
who, like "Whizzer" White, of Colorado, have earned 
both a Phi Beta Kappa key and one or more college 
letters. Finally, he may enjoy using his intellect in sport. 
If two boys are equal physically but unequal mentally, 
the brighter of the two has a considerable advantage. 
A classic example in the world of professional sport is the 
two championship boxing matches of Dempsey and 
Tunney. For instance, in a football game much depends 
upon the mental alertness of the quarterback, who more 
often than not is a high-ranking student. He plans the 
plays and then helps his teammates to execute them an 
excellent bit of training for the leadership which, pre- 
sumably, he will be exercising in later life. 

Gray compared the athletic activities of 126 boys and 
28 girls who had entered Columbia and Barnard colleges 

76 



PHYSIQUE AIsD HEALTH 

TABLE X. ATHLETIC ACTIVITIES OF GIFTED GROUP COMPARED WITH 
CONTROL GROUP* 



Activity 



Young group 



Boys Girls 



Control group 



Boys Girls 



Interclass: 

Crew 9 

FootbaU 7 

Wrestling 4 

Baseballt 4 

Basketball 3 

Swimming 3 

Water polo 3 

Tennis 2 

Fencing 1 

Cross-country 1 

Varsity: 

Crew 3 

Water polo 3 

Baseball 2 

Cross-country 1 

Tennis 1 

Track 1 

Fencing 1 

Swimming 

Intramural: 

Track 4 

Fencing 2 

Tennis 1 

Swimming 

Dancing 

Total activities 56 

Students participating 30 

Won letters 2 



2 
2 



4 
5 

26 

17 

1 



35 

27 
1 



3 

2 
3 



2 

4 
18 
11 





* Gray, H. A., " Factors in the Undergraduate Careers of Young College Students," 
p. 47, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, 1930. 
t Indoor baseball for girls. 



77 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

before they were sixteen years of age with a control group 
matched for sex and college year. The resulting data 
appear in Table X. 

In discussing the data appearing in Table X, Gray 1 
points out: 

In inter-class activities, both boys and girls in the young groups 
engaged in more sports than did the members of the control group. 
The same is true for varsity and intramural sports, and in considering 
the totals it will be seen that a large number of each sex in the young 
groups participated in more athletic activities and won more recog- 
nition as measured by being awarded the college letter, than did the 
older students comprising the control group. 

PHYSICAL HEALTH 

The parents of a gifted child were approached one day 
by a sympathetic friend, who asked them, in all sincerity, 
if they were not worried because their child was so bright. 
"He won't live very long," she warned, "Precocious 
children never do." 

"Health" is an ever-changing variable and one not 
easy to evaluate in quantitative terms. However, though 
the evidence is inconclusive, it does cast considerable 
doubt over the point of .view that the genius dies young 
or is "sicklied o'er with the pale cast of thought"; it 
tends, rather, further to substantiate the thesis that de- 
sirable traits are correlated. It would appear reasonable 
that a group of mentally superior children coining from 
better-than-average stock, living in better-than-average 
environment, and possessing better-than-average phy- 
sique should also enjoy better-than-average health. 

Frequency of Diseases 

Mentally superior children are probably subjected to 
disease with about the same frequency as unselected 
children. Unfortunately there is no detailed and accurate 

1 GBAT, op. cit., p. 48. 

78 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 

comparison of the disease histories of a large group of 
intellectually gifted children with those of children of 
average mentality. Gray gathered information concern- 
ing the frequency of diseases suffered during precollege 
years by a small group of gifted college students. He 
compared these data with similar statistics on a control 
group (see Table XI). 

TABLE XI. FREQUENCY OP DISEASES CONTRACTED BEFORE COLLEGE 

ENTRY* 



Disease 


Young group 


Control group 


Boys 


Girls 


Boys 


Girls 


Indigestion 


12 
13 
10 
8 
4 
3 
2 


4 
11 
7 
2 


15 
12 
9 
11 
3 
1 


7 
15 
6 

2 
2 


Scarlet fever 


Constipation ... 


Diphtheria 


Typhoid t . . . 


"R.VlftlTmfl.tlRTn .-,-., n ,, r ., 


Pleurisy . 





* GBAT, "Factors in the Undergraduate Careers of Young College Students," p. 43. 

In five of the seven diseases listed in Table XI, the 
control group was slightly more affected than the gifted 
group. However, the number of individuals considered is 
so small that the difference noted cannot be considered 
very significant. 

Tennan summarized data supplied by parents con- 
cerning the frequency with which the California gifted 
children had suffered from infectious diseases. Unfortu- 
nately he did not make comparisons with a control group. 
He says: 1 

For both sexes the incidence of scarlet fever, diphtheria, and 
pneumonia seems high, but comparative data for the general popula- 



1 TBBMAN, op. dt., p. 190. 



79 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

tion of the cities are not available. Roughly, one in twelve has had 
scarlet fever; one in fifteen diphtheria; and one in twenty pneumonia. 
About a quarter of the cases of scarlet fever and diphtheria and half 
of the cases of pneumonia are described as having been severe or 
very severe. With an incidence so high, these diseases doubtless 
rob the world of many potential geniuses. At the same time, the 
frequency of severe cases among the superior children suggests that 
contagious diseases may not be as important a factor in the causation 
of mental defects as they are popularly believed to be. 

Tonsils and Adenoids 

There was a period some twenty years ago when the 
idea that a dull child could be made bright by removing 
his tonsils and adenoids swept through the schools. It 
was the kind of simple, direct, easily understood remedy 
that invariably appeals. Human beings are as eager for 
a short cut to superior intellectual capacity as they are 
for a short cut to wealth or popularity. As a result of the 
widely disseminated idea that tonsils and adenoids ob- 
structed the flow of mentation, the surgeons found their 
practice markedly increased; but there was no increase 
in the intellectual capacity of those operated upon. 

The fact that Terman found that his California gifted 
children were much more likely to have had their tonsils 
and adenoids removed than were unselected children 
is not a negation of the point of view expressed in the 
preceding paragraph. It is rather the result of their hav- 
ing come from homes on a higher socioeconomic level. 
In his investigation, Terman asked both the home and 
the school for information regarding the removal of 
tonsils and adenoids. Such information was supplied by 
the home for 550 of the gifted group and by the school 
for 511 of the gifted group and for 493 of the control 
group. Data gathered in this manner are by no means 
completely accurate, but they do indicate a trend (see 
Table XII). 

80 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 
TABLE XII. REMOVAL OP ADENOIDS AND TONSILS, PER CENT* 





Gifted 
boys 


Gifted 
girls 


AH 
gifted 


Con- 
trol 
boys 


Con- 
trol 
girls 


AH 
con- 
trol 


Adenoids removed: 
School blank 


44 


32 


39 


29 


18 


23 


Home blank 


54 


42 


49 








Tonsils removed: 
SnhnnT hlf,nlr . , r , . , 


48 


38 


44 


32 


18 


25 


Hnrnfi Manic 


54 


44 


49 























* Adapted from TEBMAN, " Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. I, p. 195. 

Since the gifted children have undergone tonsil and 
adenoid operations with much greater frequency than 
the control children, it would be expected that mouth 
breathing would be much less pronounced among them. 
Terman found in his study that this difference was very 
marked, certainly sufficient to show that a bright child 
is much less likely to be a mouth breather than a dull 
or average child. 

Hearing and Vision 

Wlien the California group of gifted children was 
compared with children in the control group, it was 
found that the former were somewhat less likely to suffer 
from defective hearing and somewhat more likely to 
suffer irom defective eyesight. The percentages, based on 
reports from the schools, were: for defects in hearing, 
2.3 for the gifted and 5.9 for the control, and for defects 
in vision, 20.3 for the gifted and 16 for the control. 
A possible cause of the gifted children's inferiority 
in vision is the strain placed on their eyes by exces- 
sive reading and writing, especially during preschool 
years. 

81 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Summarizing Statement 

A detailed presentation of the health data gathered 
in the California study can be found in Chaps. VIII and 
IX of Terman's "Genetic Studies of Genius/' Vol. I. 
These data were summed up by Doctors Moore and 
Bronson, who conducted the physical examinations. 
Dr. Moore's statement follows: 1 

In regard to a general comparison of this group with unselected 
children, it is my opinion that major and minor defects are much less 
common in the former. I do not have suitable figures on which to 
base a comparison as to the relative incidence of various defects, 
but I have a strong conviction that, other things being equal, there 
is a direct correlation between physical health and mentality in 
children when studied in groups. In my opinion the physical superi- 
ority of the gifted group is indicated by the higher average of nutri- 
tion and by superior stability, physical and mental. 

MENTAL HEALTH 

It is generally believed that highly intelligent indi- 
viduals tend to be neurotic and often actually insane. 
Before an attempt is made to see what light scientific 
investigations have thrown on this question, it might be 
well to examine subjectively the basis of this popular 
conviction. 

Probably the most important cause for the belief 
that geniuses are unstable is the one referred to earlier, 
viz., the urge to supply those who possess unusual gifts 
with compensating defects. It is comforting to believe 
that the great man is eccentric or even psychotic. The 
term " paranoiac " is hurled with abandon at Mussolini 
and Hitler. Whispering campaigns against presidential 
candidates invariably accompany every national election 

1 TEBMAN, Op. dt.j p. 251. 

82 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 

in America. The most improbable stories gain currency 
with amazing and disturbing ease when they appeal to 
the prejudices of a large number of people. 

Genius and neurasthenia are by no means incom- 
patible. Consequently, it is possible to select cases in 
support of the statement that geniuses are mentally 
abnormal. This was the procedure followed by Lombroso 
(who, as a scientist, should have known better), and 
Lombroso's conclusions still exert a tremendous influence 
on thought concerning the relationship of mental health 
to intellect. 

The desire to prove that neuroticism is widespread 
among those who are eminent often closes one's eyes to 
its frequency in individuals of average or below-average 
intelligence. Overemotional or temperamental behavior 
by an artist or a scholar is expected and emphasized; 
similar behavior by an ordinary person goes unremarked. 
For example, there is the case of a well-known novelist, 
who is unusually calm and well-balanced but who, simply 
because she is a writer, is generally believed to be 
temperamental and difficult to get along with. Sympathy 
is frequently expressed to her household help concerning 
the difficulties they must have in working for such an 
eccentric person. It so happened that at one time this 
novelist had in her employ an Irish housekeeper, whose 
intelligence was somewhat below average yet who was 
as temperamental as an opera star is supposed to be. 
A slight stimulus would throw her into a rage, and she 
frequently talked in all seriousness of seeing banshees 
and having incredible experiences. Nevertheless, in 
spite of this kind of behavior, because she was a house- 
keeper of ordinary intelligence, no one, except those 
who got in the way of one of her thrown dishes, ever 
thought of her as being temperamental or neurotic. To 
outsiders she represented normalcy. 

83 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

A second factor which has strengthened the belief 
in a relationship between intellect and neuroticism is that 
certain brilliant people, especially artists, musicians, 
actors, and writers, have decided that in self-preservation 
they must behave as their public expects them to behave. 
Added to this feeling there is probably also a sense of 
relief in finding that being uninhibited is socially desir- 
able. Human beings like a queen to look like a queen; 
in other words, to look as they think a queen ought to 
look. They also like a musician to be what they think a 
musician ought to be. For those who depend for success 
upon the public a certain amount of conformity is an 
absolute necessity. A musician who appears before his 
audience with the approved long hair, foreign name, 
dark, impenetrable features, exotic but slightly incon- 
gruous figure, and a nervous system so unstable that 
he makes a handsome show of temperament before 
the concert is over, has done much to insure his 
success. 

If a public personage is great enough, he does not 
feel it necessary to enhance his prestige by stage play. 
He can be at all times that exceedingly rare person him- 
self. The individual of truly first-rate talent is usually 
poised, natural, well-controlled. Surely a lack of inte- 
gration could hardly be considered an asset to anyone, 
On the other hand, there is a horde of second-rate mindk 
in politics, in medicine, in the academic world, and in 
the arts who think that they can fool the public into 
believing that they possess great gifts by behaving as 
they know the public expects those who have great 
gifts to behave. Greenwich Village was once full of these 
men and women, who had more ambition and more 
emotional drive than their intellects were able to match. 
The game is frequently a losing one, but altogether too 
often a professor of moderate attainments, by always 

84 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 

carrying an umbrella and being conscientiously absent- 
minded, a hack writer, by filling innumerable pages with 
meaningless and repetitious words, an artist of limited 
talent, by wearing freakish and ornate clothes, wins a 
public far larger than his undramatized abilities would 
ever have commanded. 

Stability of Gifted Children 

Although it is not possible at the present time to 
give the exact relationship between intellect and emo- 
tional stability, there are indications from the studies 
that have been made that, at least in childhood, the 
relationship is positive to a rather marked degree. 
Terman, in analyzing the responses of the school to 
the question "Is child especially nervous ?" found that the 
teachers replied in the affirmative for 13.3 per cent of the 
gifted and for 16.1 per cent of the control group. There 
were over 500 children in each of the two groups. When 
information on stuttering was asked for, the percentage 
of affirmative replies was 2.6 per cent for the gifted and 
3.4 per cent for the control children. The gifted children 
were found to be slightly more timid than the control 
children and manifested a slightly greater tendency to 
worry. 

Professor Hollingworth 1 concludes a discussion of the 
nervous stability of gifted children by saying: 

Investigators do not find a complete absence of the nervously 
unstable In large groups of gifted children. Nervous instability and 
superior intelligence are by no means totally incompatible; their 
incompatibility is but relative. The consensus of investigator's 
opinions is that there are fewer nervous children among the gifted 
than among unselected children, not that there are no gifted children 
who are nervous. 

1 HOLLINGWOKTH, L. S., "Gifted Children," pp. 130-131. 

85 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Jersild 1 also points out that there is a positive relation- 
ship between stability and intelligence, although he 
emphasizes the fact that it is far from perfect. 

Burdens Borne by Adult Geniuses 

Even assuming that it is true that intellectually 
gifted children are less neurotic than their mentally 
inferior playmates, it is possible that, when they reach 
adulthood, they may develop psychoneuroses and psy- 
choses. The brighter the individual the more problems 
he has. As a child and as an adult he is aware of com- 
plexities which never impinge upon the consciousness 
of the person of average mentality. For example, there 
is the case of the mentally superior child of six who wept 
upon hearing that Austria had been absorbed by Ger- 
many. The average child of six probably would not even 
have been cognizant of the existence of a nation called 
"Austria/' to say nothing of regretting its effacement 
from the map. 

The genius, young or old, conscious of the enormous 
problems which beset civilization and, at the same time, 
by virtue of the critical ability which usually accompanies 
high intellect, realizing how inadequately he is equipped 
to solve them, may falter after years of effort. Certainly 
the surgeon who must perform one delicate operation 
after another, the writer who attempts to interpret 
life truthfully, the conscientious businessman who feels 
a personal responsibility for the welfare of his employees, 
the musician who must night after night transmit the 
emotional values of the compositions which he is playing, 
the statesman who realizes that he carries the life of a 
people in his hand, certainly all these are under a strain 
seldom experienced by the man in the ranks. In many 

UEKSILB, A. T., "Child Psychology," p. 361, Prentice-Hall, Inc., 
New York, 1933. 

86 



PHYSIQUE AND HEALTH 

instances this strain works havoc with the nervous 
system. Lange-Eichbaum, 1 claims that from 12 to 13 
per cent of between three and four hundred geniuses 
whom he studied had been psychotic at least once 
during their lifetime. When he limited the number to 
seventy-eight of the very greatest names, he claims that 
more than one-third of them were psychotic at least 
once during their lifetime and that more than 83 per cent 
had been markedly psychopathic. 

Lange-Eichbaum does not adequately describe the 
methods by which he arrived at these conclusions. It 
would seem that these percentages are little more than 
his own estimates based to a too great degree upon 
selected cases. Nevertheless it is true that many out- 
standing geniuses of the past were mentally unbalanced. 
As Lange-Eichbaum points out, many did not become 
so until after the creation of their principal work. He 
says that this is true of Copernicus, Faraday, and others, 
who suffered in their later years from senile dementia. 
However, it is rather farfetched to claim that genius and 
insanity are related merely because a number of men of 
outstanding achievement have suffered from senile 
dementia, a disease of old age common at all mental 
levels. 

H. L. Hollingworth takes a point of view which is 
the opposite of that expressed by Lange-Eichbaum, 
but which represents the one held by a considerable 
majority of the psychologists who have studied the 
relationship between abstract intelligence and mental 
disease. The following quotation from Hollingworth 2 
will serve as a summary: 



, W., "The Problem of Genius" p. 112, The Mac- 
millan Company, New York, 1932. 

2 HOLLESTGWORTH, H. L., "Mental Growth and Decline," pp. 30, 31, 
D. Appleton-Century Company, Inc., New York, 1928. 

87 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Studies of individuals with psychoneurotic complaints show these 
individuals commonly to be of less than average intellectual com- 
petence. The degree of intelligence also exercises a determining 
influence on the nature of neurotic complaints. Thus Tendler, 
studying groups of civilian patients, found the median mental ages 
of the three groups, Neurasthenia, Psychasthenia, and Hysteria, to 
be respectively 11.0, 12,0, and 13.7 years, in comparison with an 
average of 14.0 to 15.0 years of mental age attributed to the normal 
adult. , . . Although individuals with any degree of intelligence 
may become involved in neurotic difficulties, the tendency is much 
greater in the cases of humble intelligence. 



88 



CHAPTER V 
SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 

1. Are intellectually superior individuals likely to be antisocial? 

2. What adjustments do gifted children make in their play associ- 
ations and activities? 

3. What effect does high intelligence have on school popularity 
and opportunities for leadership? 

It is difficult to know precisely what should be in- 
cluded in the term "social characteristics." Intelligence 
itself is an important section of the social-personality 
pattern; and certainly physical characteristics, especially 
beauty and bearing, cannot be left out. Actually, of 
course, no individual can be taken apart and put together 
again. Even though, for practical reasons, it is necessary 
to study him in his different aspects, it should always be 
kept in mind that his total personality is greater than 
the sum of his mental, physical, and social characteristics. 

Usually social intelligence is thought of in terms of 
getting along with people, of being happy and com- 
fortable with them, of being liked by them, of rarely 
coming into conflict with them. Theoretically a person 
with a high degree of social intelligence conforms suffi- 
ciently to the standards of his group and his kind. He is 
one with the crowd. 

HAPPINESS 

Assuming that to be socially acceptable one must 
conform, it is easy to see why the intellectually gifted 
child is faced from earliest years with social problems 
which the child of average intelligence is never called 

89 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

upon to meet. The intelligent individual, possessing 
imagination and insight, frequently sees where it would 
be more beneficial to his fellows and to himself to change 
rather than to conform, but his social sense warns him 
that, in self-defense, he must move carefully. Often he 
finds a way to bring about desirable innovations with- 
out disastrous consequences to himself. Many geniuses, 
however, are unable to hit upon happy solutions to the 
frequent clashes between what they know should be 
done and what they know would be socially approved 
and so lead baffled, unhappy, disillusioned lives. Fami- 
liarity with such cases as these caused the father of a 
gifted boy to write as follows : 

If an experimenting God were to grant me the power to determine 
the intelligence of a child of mine, I should not hesitate, guided by 
my interest in the child, to make bis mentality exactly average or, 
at the most, a little above average. Under no conditions should I 
make him intellectually gifted or precocious or a genius or whatever 
term it is appropriate to apply to a youngster whose mental endow- 
ment is far above that of the great majority of children. 

I should be obliged to make this choice if, as a natural father, I 
wished my child to be happy, because I know his chances for happi- 
ness, especially in so far as they depend upon the understanding and 
comradeship of Ms fellows, would decrease inexorably with the 
increase of his relative intellectual stature. If I were to be wholly 
selfish for him, I might even go so far as to prefer that he be feeble- 
minded rather than intellectually gifted, for the feeble-minded 
individual can at least find solace in the complacency of ignorance. 

I should hope, of course, that if God did not see fit to grant me the 
authority to select my child's intelligence quotient and should, during 
a brief lapse in EGs infinite kindness, put into my keeping an infant 
with the mental capacity to become a genius, I should be able to 
go down the years with that child in understanding and patience; 
but, since I am only a human being like other parents, the chances 
are too great that I should make the same mistakes other parents 
of bright children are making. That during his preschool years I 
should be forcing him to demonstrate his powers so that my friends 
and those not so friendly could see what a marvel I had produced. 

90 



SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 

That, as the child grew older, his questions, because I could not 
answer them, would irritate me; his intellect I should envy. That, 
when he reached adult years, I should find myself happier in his 
absence than in his presence and unable to prevent Ms knowing it. 

Yes, if I were given the power to determine my child's intelligence, 
I should be strongly, bitterly tempted, in my father's desire for his 
early and perhaps lifelong happiness, to endow him with an average 
mind. And yet I hope I should waver when the time came to make 
this choice. 

I hope I should hesitate long enough to weigh my child's personal 
happiness against the welfare of a civilization, his and mine, now 
rushing toward disaster because we lack intellects equal to controlling 
the institutions which we have built or to solving the complex prob- 
lems which we have created; a civilization standing perplexed and 
helpless, like a group of children who have built a pile of blocks so 
high that they cannot add another to reinforce the structure without 
causing its collapse. I hope I should be able to pity those who in their 
blindness imprison their potential leaders almost from birth, and to 
feel a new confidence in the possibility of a brighter future for the 
intellectual genius and so for the rest of us, and say, "AH right, God, 
since You have made me the offer, I will take for my child the best 
intellect you have in stock; and may he as well as the human race 
profit by my choice." 

Attitude of Immediate Family 

The extreme pessimism of this father is probably 
unjustified, yet it is true that many of the relationships 
and experiences which bring pleasure to the child of 
average intelligence are denied those who are brilliant. 
If the gifted child happens to come of parents of average 
or below-average intelligence, his chances are small of 
ever experiencing the satisfactions which come from 
having a community of interests with one's father and 
mother. He may even discover with dismay that his 
parents are envious of him and seek to minimize his 
gifts; or it may be that they are merely puzzled like a 
hen who finds that she has mothered a duckling. For 
example, there is the instance of a ship's steward who, 

91 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

in telling a passenger about his child, said regretfully 
that he no longer understood this boy, who had suddenly 
become very much interested in books. 

It is often true that not only the gifted child but his 
parents as well experience disappointments when they 
are separated by a wide gap in intellect. There is the 
story repeated often in America of an ambitious 
mother of average intelligence who, from the earliest 
years of her son's life, had longed for him to be a great 
man. Since he had the mental capacity and the other 
characteristics necessary to the attainment of eminence, 
he did reach a high place, but it was in the world of 
chemistry. Research articles and textbooks meant noth- 
ing to his mother, who could only have appreciated the 
significance of a fame which brought newspaper head- 
lines and cheering crowds. She died a pathetically 
frustrated and disappointed woman, finding comfort 
only in the stubborn conviction that some day her son 
might yet achieve. 

The relationship between the mentally superior child 
and his brothers and sisters is comparable to that which 
exists between his parents and himself. Eve Curie, 
in her biography of her mother, Mme Curie, tells of an 
experience which the four-year-old Marie had when 
she presumed one day to read aloud to her family from 
an elder sister's reader. The story as told by Eve Curie 1 
is as follows : 

One morning, while Bronya [an older sister] was faltering out a 
reading lesson to her parents, Manya [Marie] grew impatient, took 
the book from her hands, and read aloud the opening sentence. At 
first, flattered by the silence which surrounded her, she continued 
this fascinating game; but suddenly panic siezed her. One look at the 
stupefied faces of her parents, another at Bronya's sulky stare, a 

1 CURIE, K, "Madame Curie," p. 9, Doubleday, Doran & Company, 
New York, 1937. 

92 



SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 

few unintelligible stammers, an irrepressible sob and instead of 
the infant prodigy, there was only the baby of four, crying through 
her tears: 

"Beg pardon! Pardon! I didn't do it on purpose. It's not my 
fault it's not Bronya's fault! It's only because it was so easy!" 

Manya had suddenly conceived, with despair, that she might per- 
haps never be forgiven for having learned to read. 

Just as nature abhors a vacuum, so do human beings 
abhor the variant, the child or adult who is different. 
With this basic psychological fact in mind, it is not diffi- 
cult to see why there is a marked tendency for the family, 
parents and siblings, to unite against the precocious 
child. Instead of their being made to feel inferior by 
his superior mentality, he, as a result of ridicule and 
of sheer weight in numbers, is often forced into a realiza- 
tion that he is a "misfit. This family pressure is one of 
the two primary reasons for the prevalence of inferiority 
complexes among intellectually gifted children and 
adults. For example, there is the case of the very bright 
boy, born and reared on a farm in a family of average 
mentality, who was repeatedly told by his relatives that 
a college education would be necessary for him alone, 
out of all the family, because he would be unable to 
get along without it. This comment is not so absurd 
as it may at first seem, for it is a common human failing 
for a man to look kindly upon those whose interests and 
habits are similar to his own and honestly to think that 
there is something wrong with those who speak another 
language. Hence it was entirely natural that this boy, 
with an interest in books and abstract problems, should 
be regarded as an oddity and a probable failure by those 
who were concerned solely with the manual work and 
physical pleasures of the day. 

As the typical bright child grows older, he finds him- 
self becoming more and more ostracized by members of 

93 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

his family. He watches their growing envy and puzzled 
resentment with deep concern. Often he tries to make 
contact with them by doing the things they like to do, by 
appearing interested in the things they consider impor- 
tant. Occasionally he succeeds, but he is much more 
likely merely to arouse resentment by what they consider 
to be his patronizing attitude. He is no more responsible 
for the breadth of his mental power than for the breadth 
of his shoulders and yet by its possession he brings down 
upon himself the dislike of those whom he loves most. 
Considering the difficulties of the situation it is amazing 
that the results growing out of it are not more serious 
than they are. 

Fortunately for the mental health of the gifted child 
the happiness of an individual is by no means wholly, 
or even largely, dependent upon the sympathetic under- 
standing of others. The sources of happiness differ 
with individuals; frequently for gifted children the sheer 
joy of mental activity makes each day pleasant. 

CHARACTER DEVELOPMENT OF GIFTED CHILDREN 

In spite of the difficulties which the genius has in 
adjusting to a world made for average people, he rarely 
develops antisocial tendencies. This is fortunate for 
society, for the highly intelligent criminal is, of course, 
much more effective than his feeble-minded counterpart. 
Although conclusions reached in studies of the relation- 
ship between intelligence and delinquency are not always 
the same, they point to a negative relationship. 

Cyril Burt 1 studied the characteristics of 200 juvenile 
delinquents. He reported an average I.Q. of 89 for the 
group. Eighty-two per cent were below 100 I.Q. and 
18 per cent above. While 8 per cent fell below 70 I.Q. 

iBuRT, C., "The Young Delinquent," pp. 283-284, D. Appleton- 
Century Company, Inc., New York, 1928. 

94 



SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 

only 2.5 per cent rated above 115. The normal expecta- 
tion for the total population is a frequency of 11 per 
cent above 115 I.Q. Burt concluded that mental defec- 
tiveness is a notable factor in the production of crime. 
He pointed out that the presence of intellectually gifted 
children in a delinquent group is exceptional. 

The causes of juvenile delinquency and of adult 
criminal activity are too complex, too interrelated and 
interdependent, to be isolated and exactly evaluated, 
Superior mentality alone would not keep an individual 
from committing antisocial acts, but, as has been shown 
in previous chapters, superior mentality is likely to be 
part of a pattern made up of superior home life, superior 
economic opportunities, superior health, and so on. 
Coupled with these, perhaps caused by them, is super- 
iority in character. Terman 1 maintains, after a study of 
the character and personality traits of his California 
group, that the gifted child of nine years has reached a 
level of character development corresponding roughly 
to that of unselected children of fourteen years. 

CHARACTER TRAITS OF EMINENT MEN 

Catherine Cox 2 rated 67 character traits of 100 of 
the 301 most eminent individuals in the period from 
1450 to 1850. The traits measured may be grouped as 
follows, according to character elements which pre- 
dominate in each case: emotional, 14; emotional-social, 
5; social, 15; self (negative), 2; self (positive), 5; intel- 
lectual, 8; intellectual-social, 2; intellectual-emotional, 
2; intellectual activity, 2; activity (persistence of motive), 
4; physical activity, 6; strength or force of character, 1; 

1 TEBJSCAN, "Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. I, p. 516, 2d ed., Stanford 
University Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1926. 

*Cox, "Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. II, pp. 16&-180, Stanford 
University Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1926. 

95 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



balance, 1; Each of these sixty-seven character traits 
was rated on a seven-point scale ranging from +3 to 
3 with the midpoint at zero. Each step of the scale 
was defined as follows : 

Plus 3 denotes the possession of a very high degree of the quality 
as compared with the average. 

Plus 2 denotes the possession of a degree of the quality distinctly 
above the average. 

Plus 1 denotes the possession of a degree of the quality slightly 
above the average. 

denotes the possession of the average degree of the quality 
among youths in general. 

Minus 1 denotes the possession of somewhat less than the average. 

Minus 2 denotes the possession of distinctly less than the average. 

Minus 3 denotes the lowest degree of the quality as compared with 
the average. 

Miss Cox found that the average rating of the 100 
eminent men, for the combined 67 character traits, 
was 1.2, which is considerably above the average repre- 
sented by zero. She reports the average ratings for the 
trait groups to be as follows : 



Trait 


Average 
rating 


Number 
of traits 


Activity (persistence of motive) 


2.3 


4 


Intellectual activity 


2 


2 


Self (positive traits) .... . 


2 


5 


Strength or force of character 


2.0 


1 


Intellectual 


1.8 


8 


Social 


1.2 


15 


Tnt^11ftr*t.na1-8or:ial 


1 1 


2 


Balance 


1.0 


1 


Emotional-social 


1 


5 


TntelIfifltiiA,l-fimntioTml ,_ 


.9 


2 


Physical activity. 


6 


6 


Emotional, 


5 


14 


Self (negative traits) 


.1 


2 









96 



SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 

Miss Cox, then, studying the character traits of those 
individuals who actually became eminent, comes to a 
conclusion similar to that arrived at by Terman with 
respect to gifted children of the present day. Each group 
was found to be definitely above the average. Miss Cox, 
in commenting on the fact that the geniuses in her group 
possess not only great intellectual capacity but also 
marked strength of character and a considerable per- 
sistence of motive, says: 

Forcefulness or strength of character as a whole, persistence of 
motive, and the intellectual traits rate conspicuously high. The high 
scores on all traits containing the persistence of motive factor, and 
the intellective factor indicate that young geniuses possess these 
traits to an unusual degree. These and the summation trait of strength 
or force of character as a whole are the traits in which our subjects 
score the highest ratings. They appear to be peculiarly characteristic 
of young geniuses. The estimates on the self-traits and the persistence 
traits corroborate those on forcefulness or strength of character as a 
whole, emphasizing the presence of dynamic vigor of character and 
an innate assurance of superior ability in all of the members of the 
group. 

PLAY ACTIVITIES 

The intellectually gifted child has a number of adjust- 
ments to make with respect to his play activities. If 
he is eight years of age chronologically and physically 
but twelve or thirteen years of age mentally, the possi- 
bilities are great that the games which interest average 
eight-year-old children will appeal but little to him; 
yet he will not be welcomed into the games of twelve- 
year-old children, to whom he seems a baby. Moreover, 
while he can understand the games which appeal to 
those older than he, he is physically incapable of playing 
many of them. 

This problem is acute even on the high school level. 
For instance, there is the case of David Buxton, who 
entered high school when he was eleven years of age and 

97 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

became very much interested in football. He tried out 
for the team but was too small to fill even the position of 
quarterback. David, however, became so popular with 
the other boys on the squad that in his junior year they 
elected hrm manager. In this position he was a great 
help to the team, not only in handling their business 
affairs but also in assisting in working out plays. 

The play activities of mentally superior children 
have been studied sufficiently, during the past two or 
three decades, to justify a number of conclusions. 
Those which follow apply to the typical gifted child. 
There is, of course, much overlapping between a group 
of gifted children and a group of unselected children 
with respect to their play life, and this should not be 
overlooked. 

1. Gifted children usually play with those who are older than 
themselves. 

2. They are not so interested in competitive games as are other 
children of the same age. 

3. They are interested in games that require thinking. 

4. They possess more information about games than do average 
children. 

5. If boys, they are slightly more interested than control boys in 
games with a high masculinity index. 

6. They show a marked tendency toward having imaginary play- 
mates and an interest in elaborately constructed imaginary countries. 

7. They are somewhat more likely than average children to play 
alone. 

8. They find their companionship sought by others as frequently 
as do average children. 

9. If rating over 170 I.Q., they are less likely to make satisfactory 
play adjustments than those less bright. 

With Older Children 

The gifted child, faced with the problem of whether 
he shall be content with the simple games which children 
of his chronological age play or struggle to participate 

98 



SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 

in the games popular among those of the same mental 
age, usually compromises by playing with children who 
are from one to three years older than he. This question 
of play adjustment was raised by a group of seven gifted 
children, between nine and eleven years of age, possessing 
LQ.'s of 170 or above, who were in a special class in a 
school in New York City. The incitement to comment 
was a news story which reported a leading psychologist as 
saying that the child with an I.Q. above 150 finds himself 
an outcast among youths of his own age and develops a 
feeling of physical and social inferiority to the older 
children whom he demands as companions. 

Each of these seven unusually gifted children claimed 
that he was not unhappy in his play life but was as 
well adjusted in that respect as any other child. Two 
of the three girls present explained that their best 
friends were from one to two years older than they, 
but that this difference in chronological age presented no 
obstacle whatsoever. The four boys stated that they 
had no difficulty in playing with older children. Donald, 
for instance, maintained stoutly that, though he was 
only nine years old, thirteen- and fourteen-year-old boys 
did not object to his playing with them; that he preferred 
these older boys to those of his own age; that he enjoyed 
rough games, such as football. He claimed that his size 
was an asset rather than a handicap, for he could run 
under and around his bigger, clumsier playmates. 
Edward, usually very quiet, insisted with a similar 
warmth that his best friends were two to three years 
older than he but that the difference in age had no bad 
effects upon his relationship with them. Fred also claimed 
that his best friends were a few years older than he and 
that he was far from unhappy and lonely. Charles, 
too, seemed to feel that his social adjustments were 
satisfactory. 

99 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Terman considered the question of the age of the 
playmates of intellectually gifted children in his Califor- 
nia study, He sent questionnaires to the homes of his 
gifted group and to the schools attended by members 
of this group and of a control group as well. The question 
put was whether the child preferred playmates who were 
much older, older, same age, younger, or much younger. 
The data, in terms of percentages, appear in Table XIII. 

TABLE XIII. SCHOOL AND HOME REPOBTS ON AGE OF PLAYMATES* 

Per Cent 





Much 
older 


Older 


Same 
age 


Younger 


Much 

younger 


School blank control 


1.2 


7.9 


86.5 


3.8 


0.6 


School blank, gifted 


4.3 


20,8 


70.8 


3.3 


0.8 


Homo blftnt gifted 


4.2 


30.4 


61.2 


4.0 


0.2 















* Adapted from TBBMAN, " Genetic Studies of Genius," VoL I, p. 431* 

In commenting on the statistics in Table XIII Terman 
says: 1 

The school reports a much larger percentage of gifted than control 
children who prefer older playmates, and the home reports for the 
gifted agree fairly well with those from the school. This is probably 
due in part to the fact that the gifted child is usually associated in 
school with children a year or two older than himself, and in part to a 
tendency for mental ages to seek their level. 

Interests 

Although both gifted and unselected children are more 
interested in active, out-of-door games than in intel- 
lectual contests, the gifted show a relatively greater 
preference for games that require thinking, such as 
checkers and chess. Frequently mentally superior chil- 
dren delight in making up games of their own, working 
out elaborate sets of rules. For example, one gifted boy- 
had shown no interest in gymnasium activities until 

1 TEBMAN, op. cit., p. 431. 

100 



SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 

one day the instructor asked for someone to volunteer 
to plan a new game. This was an unexpected oppor- 
tunity for the child to exert a leadership in an activity 
which heretofore had meant nothing to him. In a few 
minutes, his fertile mind had created a game which, 
when it was explained to his classmates, met with their 
enthusiastic approval. The gifted boy, younger than they 
and in this instance not adept in athletics, had won their 
admiration. The gymnasium instructor had shown rare 
good sense. 

Terman, 1 in reporting on the play interests of un- 
selected children, says: 

(1) As compared with the control boys, gifted boys show much 
greater preference for jaekstraws, coasting, hiking, dancing, swim- 
ming, rowing, croquet, wrestling, racing or jumping, handball, 
soccer, tennis, dominoes, crokinole, parchesi, authors, guessing 
games, cards, checkers, and chess; and much less preference for 
rolling hoops, walking on stilts, flying kites, riding bicycle, garden 
work, shooting, riding horseback, hunting, ring around the rosy, 
farmer in the dell, drop the handkerchief, cat and mouse, anty over, 
jump the rope, fox and geese, volleyball, basketball, and playing 
house, 

(2) As compared with the control girls, gifted girls show much 
greater preference for jackstones, skating, hiking, dancing, fishing, 
swimming, sewing, using tools, shinny, wrestling, dominoes, parchesi, 
authors, guessing games, cards, puzzles, and chess; and much less 
preference for walking on stilts, riding bicycle, hunting, cooking, 
ring around the rosy, hopscotch, cat and mouse, anty over, dare base, 
fox and geese, baseball, racing or jumping, handball, volleyball, 
basketball, and charades. 

Masculinity of Gifted Boys' Games 

The causes which have actuated the belief that 
intellectually gifted children are inferior physically and 
socially have also given rise to the conviction that gifted 
boys are effeminate as well; but this picture of the sex 

., pp. 406-407. 

101 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



*i3 CO 

4 rH 







CO t^- O5 O 







rH 


j C5 1Q 




- 


- 


rH CO TJ* CO CO 




- 




S 2! rt 


CO 


O 




CO t* O <N 








s 

00 O O 

rH rH 


rH 


o 




rH rH 








00 rH 




o 




C-J ^H 1Q 1Q Tf rH 

rH rH 








S-H4 
O 

rH WD rH 


i 1 


o 


- 


ooe, *..* 








85 


rH 


o 




rH rH O* rH 








<N 00 

CO O 

O5 1C rH 
CO r-^ 


rH 







N co c t* oo i> 








*& OS 
00 ^* 

CO rH 




o 












oo oo 

CO -* 


O 






W C^ rH 








t^- rH 


rH 







CO C? Jt*" O5 OO 




rH 




CO rH 




o 




rH rH 






rH 


CO O 
CO O 

O* Tp rH 









Tt< CO T^ OO 








CO 00 
rH rH 

OS TH t-* 

rH rH 


OO 


o 




CO C<1 O OO *O CO 
rH -rt 


i-^ 






*' ^ 
















t>- TH 




















o 














































bD 




s= 













tf 














ll 



102 



SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 

characteristics of genius fits into the true pattern of his 
personality no better than does the popular picture of 
his physical size and strength. In his interests play 
and otherwise he is actually slightly more masculine 
than boys of average intelligence. 

Terman and his coworkers developed masculinity 
indices for a large number of play activities. Using these 
as a basis for judgment, a masculinity rating was com- 
puted for each child of the gifted and control groups. 
The results are similar to those obtained when the two 
groups are being compared as to physical traits. At 
every age, except thirteen, the gifted boys rate higher 
than the control boys in the masculinity of their play 
interests (see Table XIV). 

Imaginary Playmates and Countries 

Imaginary play is very common among young chil- 
dren and even more common among those who are 
gifted because these possess the more constantly creative 
minds. Occasionally the imaginary person or animal 
becomes almost as real to the child as though it were 
actually alive and in his presence, as in the case of the 
little girl of five who always insisted on taking her 
imaginary dog with her when she went for a walk. Often 
there were difficulties in getting the animal across the 
street and occasionally, to the distress of the child, he 
was nearly run over. There should be no concern over 
the vivid quality of a child's imagination, for at this 
early age the line between the real and the make-believe 
is indistinct. To be sure, if such illusions continue into 
adolescence they constitute an indication of abnormality. 

Jersild 1 and Markey, in a study of 400 ranging in 
age from five to twelve, describe in detail the day- 

IJEBSILD, A. T., "Child Psychology/' p. 274, Prentice-Hall, Inc., 
New York, 1933. 

103 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

dreams and imaginary companions experienced by these 
children. It is interesting to note that 19 per cent of 
this group mentioned having daydreams which involved 
a certain amount of self-glorification. It is possible that 
this constitutes one of the reasons why the gifted child 
is likely to give his imagination full sway. Unable to 
achieve and to lead on a level with his mental age, 
because he is so young in calendar years, he finds it 
pleasant to build for himself in imagination a position 
of prestige. No danger is involved if the self-delusion 
is not persisted in. However, if carried on into adult 
years, it may result in paranoia a psychosis which is 
more frequently found among those of superior mentality 
than among those of inferior mentality, and which has 
its beginnings in balked childhood ambitions coupled 
with early disillusion concerning the motives of others. 

Terman agrees with Jersild that a large portion of 
gifted children have imaginary playmates. There is 
need for comprehensive studies of this phenomenon, the 
conclusions of which might reveal many new facts 
concerning the mental process of gifted children. The 
writer, knowing a boy of six who had worked out a 
system which combined imaginary playmates, imaginary 
and real .countries, imaginary travels, and imaginary 
wars, asked him to dictate a description of what he 
called the "Lei Things." The child did so and later made 
corrections in the transcription. The following excerpts, 
rather remarkable for vocabulary and sentence structure, 
are taken from his narrative. 

THE LEL THINGS 

In the country of New South Wales, Australia, there was once a 
great bee-hive with a lot of bees. One was magic* It was a mother. 
On September 6, 897 A. D., she gave birth to a bee so queer that the 
others didn't think it was a bee. They decided it must be the first of 

104 



SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 

a new race, and, since tie was so small, they called him and his race 
"The Little Things.' * They named him Bydgojappebee. 

He grew up and married another one of his race (there were a lot 
of them being born now) and they had a child called Fernaxpo. He 
was the first Little Thing President from the time he was a day old. 
That could happen because the Little Things know more when they 
are born than human beings do. There was getting to be so many of 
them now that they began to call themselves, for a pet name, "The 
Lei Things." 

More and more came, and there was quite a crowd of them while 
Leev was president. 

By the time the next one, Djerra, was president, there was such a 
crowd that explorers began to go out looking for new countries where 
there would be more room and they could live peacefully. On these 
expeditions they found Norway, Sweden, and Finland. Djerra was 
the leader of the explorers. He died almost two years after he finished 
being president, for he got very tired out from leading all their 
expeditions. 

The next president of the Scandinavian Lei Things was Gangset. 
He was the Lei discoverer of America. His voyage was very important 
and I will tell you about it. In September, 1272, almost forty years 
after he had become president, he set out on a voyage to go around 
the Arctic. He had two ships when he started, the Elgraira and the 
Ddvor, but the Ddvor, the older of the two vessels, sank when it got 
into the Arctic because it was not strong enough to stand the pressure 
of the ice. 

Then all in the Elgraira went down and landed for a time on 
Bermuda, then went north and discovered Cape Cod. They stayed 
there for about a year and a half, and returned to Scandinavia in 
1279. America was wholly settled in 1286. 

In 1298 there came a great war in Australia between the Bees 
and the Lels. Lder Eve was one of the bravest generals of the 
Australian Lels. In that war the Bees were trying to drive out the 
Lels because there were so many there; the Bees didn't know what 
they might do to them. The Bees won. 

All the Australian Lels migrated to America. Broadway was their 
next president. He was president from 1500 (when Gangset died) 
to 1611 when he was assassinated. From the year 1558 to 1572 there 
was a war in the United States between the Bees and the Lels. This 
time the Lels won, 

105 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Broadway almost died in 1567 but not quite. In 1610 the men 
who voted against him planned to shoot him but their plans failed 
that year. The next year, 1611, in late December, they shot him. 

General Lder was the next president. In early February, 1621, he 
died of pneumonia. 

The next was George Narragansett. He died in the famous disaster 
of 1890. The famous disaster happened this way. NrPkSecchule, 
Elrev, Narragansett, Huna, Bartelot, Dyzico and Felomozo were 
on board "The Hourearlier," so called because it was supposed to 
go faster than most boats, but it certainly did not. When it was five 
leagues from Recife, Brazil, it began to sink. All the Captains had 
raisers then. These were big irons with which to turn the boat over 
and dump the water out and turn the boat back again. To do this the 
captain had to jump out into the water. Narragansett was so old 
that he was getting rather weak and when he went into the water, 
down he sank like a rock! NrPkSecchule and Elrev were the only 
ones to survive. NrPkSecchule had good luck in raising the ship, 
and these two sailed back to Recife. 

In 1891 he became president. NrPkSecchule was quite young when 
he became president. He was only twenty years old. 

In 1933 I was born, and a great excitement grew up among the 
Lei Things. I didn't begin to know NrPkSecchule until the middle of 
1938. When 1939 came along, we knew each other even better and 
were very good friends. I built a cave for NrPkSecchule, his officials, 
and all the lesser Lels who inhabited the neighborhood. Now they all 
live there. 

NrPkSeechule is my highest helping Lei. His greatest singer is 
Zaco, his greatest printer is Veranops, his greatest musical composer 
is Abbldox. The baby Lels usually eat dirt, ferns, leaves, and grass. 

How do you think that describes the Lels? All right? Well, this is 
the end. 

Besides the vivid imagination shown in the preceding 
paragraphs, certain characteristics of the interests of 
gifted children are manifest. For instance, although 
the boy was only six at the time he dictated this material, 
his handling of dates and of the element of time is far in 
advance of his age* He shows also an interest in biography 
in detailed facts concerning the lives of his imaginary 
characters. It so happens that this boy is also much inter- 

106 



SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 



ested in the biographies of great men who have actually 
lived. He has already read extensively into the lives of 
Galileo, Faraday, and others. 

The boy's fluency in dictation has been developed 
through much practice. As early as the age of four he 
dictated a travel narrative of several thousand words. He 
writes well, also, but his writing vocabulary is not yet 
so large as his speaking vocabulary. 

Solitary Games 

Gifted children are somewhat more likely than control 
children to play by themselves, though the difference 
is not considerable enough to be serious. The causes of 
this tendency are not hard to find. The gifted child, 
knowing more than he can do and possessing a creative 
imagination, is somewhat more likely than the average 
child to play solitary games in which he can make use of 
his vast amount of information concerning games and 
at the same time avoid the handicaps of physical size 
and youthfulness which so frequently keep him from 
thoroughly enjoying the typical competitive games of 
other children. The problems in play adjustments 
increase proportionately with the distance between 
chronological age and mental age. 

TABLE XV. EXTENT OP SOLITAKY PLAY OP GIFTED AND AVERAGE 
CHILDREN, PER CENT* 





Very 
much 


Average 
amount 


Little 


Control boys 


33 


55 


12 


Gifted boys 


20 


64 


16 


Control girls . . . 


33 


57 


10 


Gifted girls . . 


24 


64 


12 











* Adapted from TBRMAN, " Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. I, p. 430. 

107 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Yoder, in a study of the play interests of children 
who later became eminent, found that they were often 
of a solitary kind. However, as Leta Hollingworth points 
out, the persons studied by Yoder would probably all 
rate above 170 I.Q. Gifted children of a lower order are 
less likely to play alone. Terman, in reporting the 
extent of solitary play of gifted children as compared 
with control children, gives percentages based on data 
gathered on approximately 1200 cases (see Table XV). 

Witty, 1 in a genetic study of 50 gifted children begun 
in 1924-1925, says, "The children engage in the same 
number of play activities as the control group, though 
the gifted are somewhat more solitary and sedentary 
in their play." 

It seems safe to conclude that although solitary 
play is somewhat more frequent among gifted children 
than among average children, the incidence reaches 
serious proportions only among those possessing the very 
highest intellects. 

Companionship Sought 

If the gifted child is somewhat more likely than the 
average child to play alone, then it would be expected 
that his companionship would be sought a little less 
frequently. This appears to be the case, although again 
the difference between the two groups is extremely small. 
Terman, 2 in comparing his gifted children with a control 
group, found that the gifted boys' companionship was 
sought in 29 per cent of the cases while the control boys' 
companionship was sought in 32 per cent of the cases. 
Five per cent of the gifted boys were "rather avoided" 
as against 3 per cent of the control boys. Sixty-six 

1 WITTT, P., "Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National Society for the 
Study of Education/* Part II, p. 409, 1940. 
3 TEBMAN, op. cit. t p. 432. 

108 



SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 

per cent of the gifted and 65 per cent of the control boys 
found themselves neither sought nor avoided. A com- 
parison of the gifted girls and the control girls results 
in a similar picture. These differences are surprisingly 
small when one considers the intellectual barriers between 
gifted and average children. 

Greater Difficulties of the Extremely Gifted 

Although many children with LQ.'s above 170 succeed 
in making satisfactory play adjustments, the chances are 
great that they will not. Instead of participating in the 
activities of other children, they are much more likely 
either to stand on the sidelines to watch or to find solace 
in solitary play. It is difficult to know to what extent an 
extremely brilliant child should be forced into play that 
is typical of children of his own age. Certainly, in some 
instances, such forcing results in irreparable damage. 

There was the case of a five-year-old girl with an 
LQ. of 180, who preferred, during the first-grade play 
period, to observe other children rather than to join 
with them. She was not consciously scornful of the activi- 
ties of her classmates, but their pointless running, jump- 
ing, and dancing about seemed as unappealing to her 
as would the gyrations of a group of junior high school 
children to an intelligent adult. This girl interested 
herself in studying the personalities of her classmates; 
in observing the way they did things and what they said. 
Then, after school hours, she would write character 
sketches of them. When pressure was brought to bear by 
children and. teachers to make her conform with the 
others, she refused. She was not impressed by argument 
that she should do certain things because all the others 
did them. It is probably as impossible to make such a 
child socially typical as it is to make one of average 
intelligence into a genius. 

109 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Both Leta Hollingworth and Terman are in agreement 
with the point of view that children falling at the very 
top of the curve of distribution are likely, at least 
during their early years, to be socially maladjusted. 
Hollingworth says: 1 

When the intelligence quotient is extremely high, falling into the 
highest one hundredth of one per cent, the discrepancy between 
physical size and intelligence becomes so great as to render a satis- 
factory choice of playmates difficult. In these cases the child often 
falls back upon some form of solitary, intellectual play, such as 
intricate puzzles, mathematical calculations, reading, designing, 
chemical experimentation, radio, and the like. Attempts to interest 
extremely intelligent young children in the forms of play ordinarily 
enjoyed during early childhood are futile. 

The child with an I.Q. of 170 or above is confronted 
with problems of social adjustment of the greatest com- 
plexity. He needs all the sympathetic understanding 
that is possible for those about him to give. In future 
years he is likely to repay to society a thousandfold all 
that is done for him. 

LEADERSHIP 

There are a number of factors involved in the attain- 
ment of leadership or eminence. The more important of 
these, as they pertain to success in adult life, will be 
considered in a later chapter. Brief comment, however, 
is appropriate in the present discussion of the social 
characteristics of gifted children, for to be rewarded 
by being made a leader is the best evidence that a child 
possesses positive social traits. 

Intellectually gifted children are somewhat more likely 
than average children to become leaders. This is, of 
course, not solely the result of their superior intelligence, 

1 HOLLINGWORTH, L. S., "Gifted Children," p. 147, The Macmillan 
Company, New York, 1926. 

110 



SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 

but of their superior size, bearing, social sense, initiative, 
and social status as well. Terman found that, when his 
California group of gifted children was compared with 
a control group, 67 per cent of the gifted boys and 73 
per cent of the gifted girls were rated above the control 
mean for leadership. Yates, in a study of gifted high 
school seniors, found that 28 per cent showed genuine 
ability as leaders as against 12 per cent for average 
children. Brown followed the careers of 259 high school 
leaders over a period of 2J^ years. He found these leaders 
to be above the average in intelligence and scholarship. 

Finch and Carroll 1 made a study of leadership among 
pupils in the University High School at the University 
of Minnesota. They examined the records of three groups 
of students, each numbering sixty-six. The first group was 
made up of those who were of average intelligence for the 
University High School; the second, those who were 
below average; and the third, those who were above 
average. They found that over twice as many of the 
elective offices went to seniors in the superior group than 
to an equal number in the average group. Children in 
the below-average group were very much out of the 
running. 

One reason why gifted children become school leaders 
is that they have more time for extracurricular activities. 
The writer was interested, when attending the graduation 
exercises of a New England academy, to hear the 
principal say in his talk that a certain boy who was 
graduating as salutatorian of his class, with an average 
for the four years well up in the nineties, was a member 
of nearly every club in the school and president of two 
of them. In addition to this, he had won his letter in 
baseball and was a member of the debating team. This 

1 FESTCH, F. H., and H. A. CAKBOLL, Gifted Children as High School 
Leaders, /. Genet. Psychol, Vol. 41, pp. 476-481, 1932. 

Ill 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

boy further conformed to the typical picture of the gifted 
high school child by being two years younger than the 
average age of his classmates. The principal, with old- 
fashioned American philosophy, maintained that all 
these achievements had been possible through hard work 
and that any other boy in the school, by being equally 
persistent, could do as well. No reference was made to 
the factor of innate intelligence, yet a quick mind had 
made it possible for this boy to meet the school require- 
ments with casual ease and so make available time for 
other activities. 

Discrepancy in Intelligence between Leader and Followers 

Leta Hollingworth has recently expressed the opinion 
that no group would tolerate a leader who completely 
outclassed them in intelligence; they prefer one who is 
superior to themselves but not too superior. She says, 

There is a direct ratio between the intelligence of leader and that 
of the led. To be a leader of his contemporaries a child must be more 
intelligent, but not too much more intelligent, than those who are to 
be led. There are rare exceptions to this principle, but, generally 
speaking, a leadership pattern will not form or will break up when a 
discrepancy of more than about 30 points of IQ comes to exist 
between the leader and the led. 

This point of view appears to be a reasonable one. 
Children, like adults, tend to distrust the individual who 
uses words that they cannot understand, who thinks and 
talks about matters which are foreign to their interests. 
For example, there was the case of the boy with an I.Q. 
of 195 who was hooted down at an assembly of the upper 

1 HOLUNGWOETH, L. S., What We Know about the Early Selection 
and Training of Leaders, from a pamphlet "How Should a Democratic 
People Provide for the Selection and Training of Leaders in the Various 
Walks of Life?" pp. 16-17, Advanced School of Education, Teachers 
College, Columbia University, 1938. 

112 



SOCIAL CHARACTERISTICS 

grades when lie attempted to present a program of 
action. The very sentence structure which he used 
antagonized the other children. A boy with a much 
lower intelligence then took the floor and successfully 
explained what was to be done. 

Occasionally a child even one with an I.Q. of 195 
is able to adapt himself with humor and understanding 
to the limitations of average people. Such a one has the 
makings of the greatest of leaders. For example, Leta 
Hollingworth tells of an eleven-year-old boy with an 
I.Q. close to 180 who had decided to run for the office 
of class president in the senior high school in which he 
was a student. His classmates were approximately five 
years older than he. In a speech during the campaign, 
one of his rivals said tellingly, "Boys, we don't want a 
president in knee pants !" As soon as the applause had 
died away the eleven-year-old boy rose, and waving his 
hand casually in the direction of the full-length portrait 
of George Washington on the wall, said, "Fellows, 
kindly remember that when George got to be the 
father of our country, he was wearing knee pants/' 
Hollingworth reports that the gifted boy was elected. 



113 



CHAPTER VI 

MENTAL CHARACTERISTICS AND 
ACIHEVEMBNTS 

1. Are such traits as curiosity, alertness, and self-criticism charac- 
teristic of intellectually superior individuals? 

2. Do the scholastic attainments of gifted children approximate 
their mental status? 

3. Are gifted children retarded with respect to their grade place- 
ment? 

Studies of the childhood of eminent men reveal that 
in the great majority of cases these individuals stood 
out against the background of average children. They 
were recognized as being different, as possessing abilities 
which by comparison seemed almost nonexistent in their 
playmates. They were already marked for greatness. 

A visit to a school which has enrolled children of 
both high and low intelligence gives one a similar 
impression. To go from a room containing a group of 
ten-year-old morons to a room containing a group of 
ten-year-old gifted children is almost like journeying 
from one world to another. Even a visitor untrained in 
psychology or pedagogy could not help but realize that 
the gifted group was made of finer stuff. In fact this 
quick transit from a very dull group to a very bright 
group makes one doubt for a moment the psychological 
principle that individuals differ not in kind but in degree. 
However, the illusion of the difference in kind grows out 
of the contrast of the two extremes. The gifted group 
would not seem so different if they were being compared 
with superior children those with I.Q.'s between 110 

114 



MEXTAL CHARACTERISTICS AND ACHIEVEMENTS 

and 120 or 130. In the discussion, then, of the mental 
characteristics of intellectually gifted children it will 
be kept in -mind that they possess characteristics which 
are common to all, but possess them to such an extreme 
degree that they frequently appear as being unique 
traits. 

MENTAL QUALITIES 

Gifted children as a group are characterized by the 
following mental qualities: power, broad attention span, 
alertness, keen observation, curiosity, self-criticism, a 
sense of relative values, initiative, insight into relation- 
ships. 

Power 

Thorndike lists level, range, area, and speed as the 
attributes of intelligence. Of these, level the gradation 
of difficulty is probably the most important. Gifted 
children excel in their ability to perform difficult mental 
tasks. It is not at all unusual for one of them to be 
reading history at the age of six or beginning to explore 
the mazes of calculus at twelve or thirteen. Although 
in certain individual cases, one may feel that the gifted 
mind is superficial, usually one is impressed by its 
tremendous energy. A bright child, possessing this vital 
mental energy and most of them have it approaches 
a problem with the same steady, assured attitude that 
an athlete, conscious of his strength, enters a sporting 
contest. 

Attention Span 

Gifted children possess a wider attention span than 
average or below-average children. They are able to 
concentrate on one activity for a long period without a 

115 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

lagging of interest. This concentration is noticeable 
whether the bright child is building a house in his play- 
room, looking up material on the American Indian, or 
carrying on a discussion with his parents. For example, 
there was the gifted child of seven who was troubled by 
a statement in his history book to the effect that Chris- 
topher Columbus was the first to discover America. 
He thought the credit should have gone to the Vikings. 
This identification of his sympathies with the Vikings 
led him into extensive library research not only into the 
history of the Vikings but also into the early history of 
the Scandinavian countries. This interest and con- 
centration continued for several months. At the end of 
that time the child was better informed in this field than 
most adults. 

The attention span of the gifted child exists not only 
in the present but is likely to project itself into the past 
and future. Closely associated with the attention span is 
the time sense, which is usually present to a remarkable 
degree in gifted children. To the average child or adult, 
anything that happens before his lifetime is of little con- 
sequence. Prehistoric and early historic times are so com- 
pletely beyond his comprehension that he is quite likely 
to laugh them off. He does not really believe that there 
were such eras anyhow. The gifted child, on the other 
hand, is usually vitally interested in the past. He is not 
nonplussed by the phrase "a thousand years" or even 
"a million years." He wants to know what has happened 
and he struggles to see what has happened in relationship 
to what is now happening and to what will happen. 
His attention span is in direct contrast to those of the 
monkeys in Kipling's "Jungle Books." The following 
lines from that book might almost be a description of the 
inability to concentrate which is characteristic of children 
of ordinary intelligence : 

116 



MENTAL CHARACTERISTICS AND ACHIEVEMENTS 

Here we sit in a branchy row, 
Thinking of beautiful things we know; 
Dreaming of deeds that we mean to do, 
AH complete, in a minute or two 
Something noble and grand and good, 
Won by merely wishing we could. 
Now we're going to never mind, 
Brother, thy tail hangs down behind! 

The bright child is- interested in the future as well 
as in the past. Occasionally his concern with what has 
gone before and with what is coming later results in 
insufficient concentration on the present. This is one of 
the reasons why the gifted child and adult is some- 
times thought of as being absent-minded. He feels that 
there are more important matters requiring his attention 
than the chatter of his playmates or his teacher's over- 
simplified explanations of already familiar material. 
The bright child, interested in a wide sweep of events and 
in trying to weave them together into a meaningful 
pattern, has a number of delicate adjustments to make 
with respect to his behavior in an immediate situation. 
Most bright children make these adjustments extremely 
well. 

The intellectually gifted child, interested in the future, 
is concerned about such matters as death at a much 
earlier age than average children. Parents must use care 
in their explanations when this problem arises, for it is 
likely that their child's intellectual development is far 
ahead of his emotional development and he may become 
upset in his attempt to understand that all life ends 
inexorably in dissolution. The bright child looks forward 
at an earlier age than average children to his own per- 
sonal future, He is likely to decide upon his vocation 
and to plan his preparation for it with great care. With 
his knowledge of the past and of the present and with 

117 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

his interest in the future, he is likely to possess a rela- 
tively clear vision not only of what is the best program 
of action for himself, but also of what is best for society. 
Frequently he is wrong, for even the most brilliant 
intellects seem insufficient to organize and run a well- 
ordered world. 

Quickness 

Intellectually gifted children possess not only greater 
power than average or below-average children but 
also more alertness or quickness. The relationship 
between power and speed is high. A gifted child under- 
stands directions with comparatively little difficulty; 
the average child usually has to be told several times 
before he sees what it is that he is expected to do. Some 
high school teachers make it a practice to explain an 
assignment to a class from three to five times in order 
to be sure that it will be understood. Even then there 
is an excellent chance that some of the dull children 
will not know what she means. The brightest in the 
class is likely to understand it the first time. For example, 
there is the case of the bright girl of ten who was standing 
outside a door waiting for a friend to complete her music 
lesson. While she stood there she overheard the explana- 
tions of the teacher concerning the reading of notes. 
When the child came in for her own lesson and the teacher 
began a repetition of the explanation, the girl told her 
politely but firmly that it was not necessary for her to 
spend any time on that. She had heard them once. A 
little questioning proved that once was enough. 

Observing 

Closely related to alertness is the ability to observe 
and to remember details. The bright child, though 
he may occasionally appear to be absent-minded, actually 

118 



MENTAL CHARACTERISTICS AND ACHIEVEMENTS 

sees and hears a great deal more than the average child. 
What the neurological arrangement is that makes this 
possible is not known, but the fact remains. The bright 
child learns punctuation and spelling not so much from 
direct instruction as from indirect learning through 
reading. A dull child can go through the grades and 
through high school and, in spite of all the direct teaching 
and direct contact with books, still not know when to 
capitalize or how to spell a simple word. 

A housekeeper of a little less than average intelligence 
who has spent forty years buying groceries, seeing them 
in stores and in her kitchen, spells coffee with one / 
and sugar with two g's. A seven-year-old gifted girl 
in the home, with only a short period of time for observa- 
tion, can spell the name of every article in the kitchen 
perfectly. The difference between the woman and the 
girl lies in the difference which exists in their respective 
abilities to observe. Keen observation largely underlies 
the fact that approximately 50 per cent of intellectually 
gifted children learn to read before they attend school. 
The gifted child cannot easily be kept from seeing sign- 
boards, magazine advertisements, letters on cereal boxes, 
and so on. He asks a few questions, and he knows his 
letters; a few more questions, and he recognizes simple 
words; a few more questions, and he is reading, even 
though he may be only four years old. 

Curiosity 

The gifted child is nearly always characterized by 
an insatiable curiosity. He has a passion for knowing. 
If given an opportunity, he is likely to explore many 
subjects in the school curriculum merely for the satis- 
faction which he experiences in acquiring new facts. 
In these explorations he is somewhat more likely to 
select abstract subjects in preference to commercial 

119 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

and manual-training work. Even though only a second- 
or third-grade child, he may find considerable delight in 
looking up material on grammar, or fractions, or natural 
history. This boundless curiosity should be encouraged 
even though the parent or teacher may find it very 
difficult to supply the inquiring mind with personal 
answers or with references to source material. 

The writer together with all others within earshot 
was interested and amused to overhear a conversation 
between an obviously gifted boy of eight and his father, 
who were having lunch together in a public dining room. 
The boy was pushing a discussion of the French Revolu- 
tion and the subsequent Napoleonic era to a point 
which was embarrassing for his father. It was clear that 
the child had no intention of being discourteous or of 
testing his father's information; he was eager only to 
check up on what he had read and to have it explained. 
The father was an intelligent man but knew far less than 
his son about the historic period being discussed. Several 
times his statements concerning dates and geographical 
locations were critically questioned by the child. In every 
instance the child was right. The situation was not an 
easy or a pleasant one for the father, yet he showed 
no sign of impatience, but seemed, rather, to be pleased 
with the vitality and range of his child's interests and 
information. He was willing to help in any way that he 
could and frequently referred the boy to books which 
would give him some of the specific information which 
he was seeking. 

Terman made an objective study of the intellectual 
interests of his gifted California group and found, as 
would be expected, that they excelled average children 
by a wide margin. He says in a concluding statement: 1 

1 TEEMA.N, L. M., "Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. I, p. 482, 2d ed., 
Stanford University Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1926. 

120 



MENTAL CHARACTERISTICS AXD ACHIEVEMENTS 

In intellectual interest the mean score of the gifted children at 
most ages exceeds the mean of unselected children of corresponding 
age by approximately 1.4 times the S.D. of the latter. This is approxi- 
mately half as great a degree of superiority as obtains in the case of 
intelligence. Stated in another way, about 90 per cent of the gifted 
children equal or exceed the mean of unselected children in intellectual 
interest. 

Self-criticism 

The bright child with his wide range of information 
and his desire to know the facts is more likely than the 
average child to be critical of his own shortcomings. 
The possession of a high intellect makes it easier for him 
to know when he has made a mistake. For example, in 
giving the Stanford-Bluet intelligence test to a dull 
child, it is easy to follow the formula of saying after 
each answer, whether it is right or wrong, u That's 
fine " ; but in giving the test to a gifted child, it is neither 
easy nor wise, for usually he will know when he has not 
given the right answer and will lose his confidence in the 
examiner if praised for an erroneous solution. 

This ability to criticize one's self is present in intel- 
lectually gifted children at a very early age. Ruth, who 
was six years old and in the first grade, had become very 
much interested in her art work. She possessed no 
special talent in this direction but her satisfaction in 
conceiving and painting pictures had excited in her the 
ambition to become a great artist. She often referred to 
her paintings as the famous pictures by Ruth White. 
One day her mother took her to an art museum. With 
the care which is characteristic of superior children, 
Ruth studied a half dozen or so great pictures for over 
an hour. At the end of that time she announced that she 
was ready to go home, saying that she did not wish 
to look at any of the others. On the return ride the child 
was silent for a long time, then turned to her mother 

121 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

and said with finality, "Why did my teacher say my 
pictures were so good? They're not at all. 77 Ruth had no 
more illusions about becoming a famous painter. 

The gifted child not only properly evaluates his short- 
comings, but also, to the occasional discomfort of some 
of his adult associates, properly evaluates his assets. 
There has been and is much talk concerning whether 
or not bright children should know their intellectual 
level. The answer is that they know it anyhow regardless 
of camouflage devices used by teachers. The writer 
recalls talking with such a child a short time ago and, 
in an attempt to suggest to him that there were other 
children in his school who could do as well as he, was 
met with the matter-of-fact statement, "I have been 
carefully watching all the other children for a long time 
and I know that there isn't one who can do nearly as 
well as I can." This statement was made without 
egotism. It was as objective an observation as if the child 
had said that he was the tallest member of his class or 
could run the fastest. It would seem that in the education 
of gifted children it would be best for the teacher to start 
with a frank understanding with her pupils that they were 
mentally superior. To proceed on that assumption, 
rather than to be constantly debating it or even trying 
actually to disprove the existing fact, would not only 
gain the confidence of the superior child but would also 
help him to avoid the danger of thinking too much about 
his intellectual equipment. There is a vast difference in 
influence upon attitude between recognizing the fact 
of possession of superior intellectual ability and then 
forgetting about it and being constantly compared 
either favorably or unfavorably to other children. 

Seeing Relative Values 

Highly intelligent individuals are likely to see and 
understand the gradations in moral and social values. 

122 



MENTAL CHARACTERISTICS AND ACHIEVEMENTS 

Nothing to them is ever wholly white or wholly black, 
wholly right or wholly wrong. Possessing this ability, 
they are usually more generous and sympathetic toward 
the faults of others. They realize that any specific act 
is an inevitable outgrowth of a set of complex causes. 
The really great humanists are not found among bigots 
of limited intelligence but among those who have suffi- 
cient intellectual capacity to realize that all values are 
relative. 

Initiative 

Initiative or independence in thinking is characteristic 
of the minds of intellectually gifted children. That this 
is a desirable trait goes without saying. In school it 
manifests itself through a facility in working out highly 
individualistic approaches to problems in subject matter. 
Such creative approaches should be encouraged, but, 
if the teacher knows of a better method, then she should 
explain it to the gifted child and recommend that he 
substitute it for his own. If he can see that it is really a 
better one, he will make the change. 

For example, there was the gifted child in the second 
grade who had worked out devices which would increase 
his speed in addition. When he was asked to add 18 and 
8 he would, instead, add 18 and 10 and then subtract 2, 
for he had found that he could do this more quickly. 
He had found, also, that he could add a long column of 
figures more rapidly if he skipped about in the column 
adding the larger numbers first. His teacher, upon dis- 
covering this, instead of flatly ordering hrm to add his 
figures from the bottom up the procedure which she 
required of her class took pains to explain to this child 
that, although his method was a good one, there were two 
excellent reasons for accepting the one which she recom- 
mended : first, that by following a more ordered procedure 
he was less likely to make mistakes ; second, that it would 

123 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

give him needed practice in handling difficult combina- 
tions of numbers. 

Because of his ability to learn quickly and to create 
relatively satisfactory methods for study, the gifted 
child often shows an impatience with drill; yet drill is 
needed even for brilliant minds. In the teaching of 
gifted children the amount of drill can be reduced, but 
there is danger in too much reduction. A little mental 
discipline will do a great deal to steady the mental 
processes of the bright child. 

Ability to Generalize 

Perhaps the most important mental characteristic 
of the bright child is that of being able to see relation- 
ships, to make logical associations, to adapt abstract 
principles to concrete situations, to make mental trans- 
fers between situations with identical elements, to 
generalize. Actually all these statements mean very- 
much the same thing. Any one of them might constitute 
a working definition of intelligence. Thorndike and 
Gates 1 have stated the relationship between native 
ability and the ability to generalize as follows: 

The native ability of a pupil has a pronounced effect upon the 
degree of transfer. In most subjects, the brighter pupils, other things 
being equal, can make wider use of their acquisition than duller 
pupils. Brightness, indeed, means in a considerable measure sensi- 
tivity to the factors or principles which are common to many situa- 
tions. Not only do the bright pupils isolate the essential elements in a 
learning situation more quickly, but they also perceive more acutely 
the same elements in new settings. Transfer of experience therefore 
occurs more fully among bright than among dull individuals; it is in 
considerable degree determined by intelligence. 

The marked ability of the bright child to generalize 
obviously has a great many implications for his education. 

1 THORNDIKE, E. L., and A. I. Gates, "Elementary Principles in Edu- 
cation," p. 104, The Macmillan Company, New York, 1929. 

124 



MENTAL CHARACTERISTICS AND ACHIEVEMENTS 
TABLE XVI. A COMPARATIVE PHONETIC ANALYSIS* 



Subjects 


Number 
of sub- 
jects 


Mean 
LQ. 


<r 


Total words spelled 
incorrectly 


Number 


Per cent 
phonetic 


Per cent 
non- 
phonetic 


IV A: 
Bright 


36 
36 

35 

25 

29 
39 

100 
100 


126.4 
88.5 
-37.9 

110.5 
89.5 
-21.0 

140.6 
96.8 
-43.8 

124.9 
1.09 
92.0 
.87 
-32.9 
1.40 


8.8 
9.8 

11.3 
13.7 

12.3 
11.2 

16.3 
12,9 


2,473 

7,724 
+ 5,251 

2,031 
4,415 
4- 2,384 

1,498 
6,262 
-f 4,764 

6,002 
18,401 
+12,399 


68.9 
37.8 
-31.1 

65.5 
46.0 
-19.5 

74.7 
46.1 
-28.6 

69.2 
.401 
42.6 
.245 
-26.6 
.470 


31.1 
62.2 
4-31.1 

34.5 

54.0 
+19.5 

25.2 
53.9 

+28.7 

30.8 
.401 
57.4 
.245 
+26.6 
.470 


Dull 


Difference . . 


IV B i 
Bright 


Dull 


Difference . . 


VB: 
Bright 


DuH 


Difference 


Total: 
Bright 


Dull 


Difference 



* CABBOLI*, H. A.: " Generalization of Bright and Dull Children," p. 44, Teachers 
College, Contributions to Education, No. 439, Columbia University, New York, 1930. 

Because of this ability he is able to apply rules or to 
make rules of his own through observations of a body 
of facts. He is able to apply what he knows. Occasionally 
this ability gets him into trouble, for transfer may be 
negative as well as positive. The bright child frequently 
makes negative generalizations in reading, pronouncing 
words as it seems to him from their spelling they ought 
to be pronounced, or in spelling, through spelling words 
as they sound. Concerning the second example, Carroll 1 

I CABBOLL, H. A., "Generalization of Bright and Dull Children," 
p. 44, Teachers College Contributions to Education, No. 439, Columbia 
University, New York, 1930. 

125 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

made a study of the comparative amounts of negative 
transfer made by a group of bright children and a group 
of dull children. He found, as would be expected, that 
bright children were much more likely than dull children 
to make phonetic misspellings, while, on the other hand, 
the misspellings of the dull child were much more likely 
than those of the bright to be wholly illogical (see 
Table XVI). 

SCHOLASTIC ATTAINMENTS 

Variations among children are as great in achievement 
in school subjects as they are in intellectual capacity. 
Although the relationships between school achievement 
and intelligence is far from perfect, it is, nevertheless, 
sufficiently positive to make possible sound prophesies 
with respect to groups. Gifted children, by and large, 
will excel in all the school subjects. This statement, of 
course, is made in the light of the realization that there 
is overlapping that some gifted children will do poor 
school work and some average children, by dint of much 
application and, occasionally, through the possession 
of a special gift, will do superior work. 

The superiority in accomplishment of gifted children 
over average children is greater in the abstract subjects 
and less in the manual subjects. Terman, in his Cali- 
fornia study, asked teachers to rate the school work of 
over 500 intellectually gifted children and an equal 
number of control children. The ratings were made on 
the following 7-point scale. 

1 = very superior to average child of the same grade 

2 = superior to average child of the same grade 

3 = high average 

4 = average 

5 low average 

6 = inferior to average child of the same grade 

7 = very inferior to average child of the same grade 

126 



MENTAL CHARACTERISTICS AND ACHIEVEMENTS 



When rated on this scale the gifted and control 
children rank as follows: 

TABLE XVII. ORDEE OF SCHOOL SUBJECTS WITH RESPECT TO 

DIFFERENCE IN QUALITY OF WOEK OF GIFTED AINTD CONTROL 

GROUPS* 



Subject 


All 
gifted 


All 
control 


Difference 


1 T "Dfthating or ppft^lring , 


2.04 


3 88 


1 84 


2. U. S. History 


2.11 


3 92 


1 81 


3. Composition 


2 25 


3 93 


1 68 


4_ TjitfiTatiiTft , , . 


1.90 


3 56 


1 66 


5, Ancient history 


2.04 


3 60 


1 56 


fi. GrflTTiTinfrr. 


2.25 


3 76 


1 51 


7. General science 


2.35 


3 78 


1 43 


8. Geography . 


2 26 


3 67 


1 41 


9. Civics or citizenship 


2 12 


3 49 


1 37 


10. Reading 


1.92 


3 26 


1 34 


11. Arithmetic 


2.52 


3.80 


1.28 


12 Spelling 


2 13 


3 39 


1 26 


13. Dramatics 


2.40 


3.64 


1.24 


14. Nature study 


2.57 


3.66 


1.09 


15. Agriculture .... ., 


3.28 


3 96 


.69 


Ifx Singine? .. r -_. 


3.24 


3.89 


.65 


17. Folk dancing 


2.86 


3.45 


.59 


18 Cooking. . . 


3.06 


3 63 


.57 


19 Physiology or hygiene 


2.72 


3.28 


.56 


20 TuRtrmnftnt-Hl rmisift - -,-,-. 


2 89 


3 38 


.49 


21 Physical training. -,,,,., T 


3.25 


3.60 


.35 


22 Sewing 


3.11 


3.41 


.30 


23. Drawing 


3.62 


3.87 


.25 


24 Modeling 


3.48 


3.64 


.16 


25. Penmanship 


3.79 


3.92 


.13 


26 Games and sports 


3.41 


3.52 


.11 


27. IVfajHial trifling . T . - . , 


3.49 


3.60 


.11 


28. Painting 


3.71 


3.80 


.09 


29. Shot) work 


3.57 


3.50 


- .07 



* TEBMAN, "Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. I, p. 261. 



When 565 of the California gifted children were 
given the Stanford achievement battery, they earned 

127 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

scores considerably in excess of those earned by 1,800 
uaselected children who were tested in the derivation 
of the norms published for the battery (see Table 
XVIII). 

TABLE XVIII. MEAN SUBJECT QUOTIENTS FOR 565 GIFTED CHTXDBEN* 





Boys 


Girls 


TO 


151.6 


151.6 




146.2 


148.3 




145.3 


144.7 


Arithmetic 


138.5 


135.7 




140.2 


137.7 









* TBEMAN, "Genetic Studies of Genius," p. 291. 

It is interesting to note that, although the gifted 
children are definitely superior to the control children 
in language, reading, arithmetic, and spelling, their 
superiority in these school subjects is not so marked as 
their superiority in intelligence. The principal reason 
for this discrepancy is presumably their lack of oppor- 
tunity, since their average acceleration is only slightly 
more than one grade, for contact with the more difficult 
and advanced subject matter. 

This same group of gifted children was given a general 
information test and earned scores which were com- 
parable to their intelligence-test ratings. In fact, then- 
superiority was so marked that all but two of the 291 
gifted boys and five of the 242 gifted girls earned an 
information quotient above 120. Not a single gifted child 
fell as low as the average for the control group. 

Stedman has reported on the school attainments of 
a group of sixteen gifted children who had been placed 
in an opportunity room and given special instruction over 
a period of five years. The educational quotients earned 
by these children exceed those earned by the Terman 

128 



MENTAL CHARACTERISTICS AKD ACHIEVEMENTS 



group, presumably because needed adjustments in their 
school training had been made. It is significant, as Sted- 
man points out, that her pupils, although spending 
relatively little time on routine subject matter, not only 
invariably excelled the highest available norms for each 
subject, but also very nearly equaled what their mental 
ages would lead one to expect (see Table XIX). 

TABLE XIX. SCHOOL ATTAINMENT OF 16 GIFTED CHILDEEN IN AH 
OPPORTTJNITY CLASS* 













History 








Pupil 


I.Q. 


Read- 
ing 
quo- 


Arith- 
metic 
quo- 


Science 
quo- 
tient 


and 
litera- 
ture 


Lan- 
guage 
quo- 


Spell- 
ing 
quo- 


Edu- 
cation 
quo- 






tient 


tient 




quo- 


^ tient 


tient 


tient 












tient 








1 


140 


136 


107 


133 


139 


133 


120 


125 


2 


155 


142 


156 


147 


142 


142 


143 


148 


3 


155 


154 


167 


160 


154 


138 


144 


154 


4 


140 


142 


130 


154 


150 


143 


128 


138 


5 


214 


199 


206 


206 


198 


174 


207 


200 


6 


141 


141 


124 


145 


142 


128 


112 


131 


7 


120 


130 


108 


137 


143 


116 


100 


119 


8 


110 


122 


111 


125 


126 


109 


114 


117 


9 


140 


146 


148 


161 


158 


142 


132 


146 


10 


140 


139 


136 


138 


136 


140 


134 


137 


11 


138 


143 


145 


150 


146 


134 


140 


143 


12 


168 


163 


171 


178 


165 


154 


160 


165 


13 


129 


114 


100 


135 


140 


122 


97 


110 


14 


142 


138 


153 


160 


161 


140 


128 


144 


15 


146 


166 


140 


158 


168 


130 


135 


150 


16 


140 


138 


115 


152 


146 


127 


130 


131 


Average 


144 


144 


138 


153 


150 


136 


133 


141 



* Adapted from STEDMAN, L, M., "Education of Gifted Children," p. 107, World Book 
Company, Yonkers-on-Hudson, N. Y., 1924. 

The reading quotients of gifted children in the lower 
grades are likely to be somewhat higher than other 
subject quotients. This results in part from the existence 
of greater opportunities to learn how to read as compared 

129 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

with opportunities to become familiar with the content of 
such a subject as arithmetic. Occasionally the reading 
quotient of a gifted child exceeds his intelligence quotient. 
This is even more likely to occur if his interest in reading 
has been stimulated by his home associations. 

For example, Eugene Smith, who had been reared in 
such a home and who had learned to read at the age of 
three, earned, at the age of 8 years 1 month, an eighth- 
grade rating on the Gates Silent Reading Tests. His 
reading ages on the four types were: Type A, 12 years 
10 months; type B, 15 years 4 months; type C, 13 years 
9 months; type D, 13 years 9 months. Averaging the 
four tests results in a reading age of 13 years 11 months as 
contrasted with a mental age of 13 years. This boy at 
the time the tests were given was in the third grade 
" studying " a third-grade reader and working hard at 
devising methods of killing time. 

Witty 1 agrees that in the elementary school gifted 
children do their best work in reading and in language. 
He reports, however, that the 50 gifted children, whose 
school histories he has followed for a number of years, 

exceeded the norms for children of their ages in all subjects. . . . 
The sixth-grade group exceeded January averages for eighth-grade 
children in the composite results of the tests, and the composite score 
of the seventh-grade group exceeded ninth-grade standards. The 
children appear to have a knowledge of educational subject matter 
at least two years in excess of their grade placement. 

Extremely Gifted Compared with Less Gifted 

In the fall of 1938 the writer worked for some months 
with the group of intellectually gifted children at 
Speyer School in New York City. In the course of 
that work the American Council Cooperative General 

1 WITTY, P., "Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National Society for the 
Study of Education," Part II, p. 405, 1940. 

130 



MENTAL CHARACTERISTICS A^D ACHIEVEMENTS 

Achievement Tests were given to the six brightest chil- 
dren in the Speyer group, whose average LQ. was 180.3, 
and to a less gifted group of six, matched for chronologi- 
cal age and sex, whose average LQ. was 138.7. Data 
concerning these children, together with a record of 
the achievement-test scores which they earned, appear 
in Table XX. 

TABLE XX. COMPARISON OF SCORES MADE ON AMERICAN COUNCIL 
COOPERATIVE GENERAL ACHIEVEMENT TESTS BY Two GROUPS OF 
GIFTED CHILDREN 



Child 


Sex 


C.A. 


M.A. 


LQ. 


Social 
sciences 


Natural 
sciences 


Mathe- 
matics 


Score 


P.R. 


Score 


P.R. 


Score 


P.R. 



Extremely gifted group (mean LQ. 180) 



A 


M 


11-5 


22-2 


194 


61 


97.5 


82 


99 


42 


99 


B 


M 


11-0 


19-3 


175 


18 


61 


23 


25.5 


28 


95 


C 


M 


9-8 


16-5 


170 


22 


74 


37 


61 


14 


22 


D 


F 


9-4 


1S-8 


200 


11 


37 


39 


56 


25 


72.5 


E 


F 


10-6 


18-1 


172 


10 


32 


38 


61 


13 


17 


F 


F 


10-3 


17-6 


171 


12 


41.5 


18 


18 


9 




Average 




10-4 


18-8 


180.3 


22.3 


57.2 


39.5 


53.4 


21.8 


50.9 



Less gifted group (mean LQ. 139) 



A 


M 


11-8 


16-3 


139 


33 


90.5 


47 


76 


17 


34 


B 


M 


11-2 


15-8 


140 


13 


46 


40 


66.5 


9 




C 


M 


9-8 


13-6 


140 


7 


20 


20 


21 


14 


22 


D 


F 


9-10 


14-1 


143 


14 


46 


21 


21 


4 




E 


F 


10-9 


14-2 


132 


9 


27 


21 


21 


7 




F 


F 


10-1 


13-1 


138 


10 


32 


41 


66.5 


11 




Average 




10-6 


14-5 


138.7 


14.3 


43.6 


31.7 


45.3 


10,3 


9.3 



The average percentile rank, in terms of ninth-grade 
norms, for the two gifted groups on the three parts of 
the achievement battery is approximately 45. In other 

131 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

words, these ten-year-old children did about as well as 
average ninth-grade children, who are approximately 
four years older. The less gifted group, with an average 
mental age of 14 years 5 months, earned a mean test 
score comparable to their intellectual rating. The ex- 
tremely gifted group, however, although it consistently 
achieved higher average scores than the less gifted group, 
did not do so well as would be expected from its much 
higher average mental age. 

Leta Hollingworth, in an earlier study, compared 
a group with an average LQ. of 165 with a group with 
an average LQ. of 145, and found that the former con- 
sistently excelled the latter in school achievement. 
The differences between the two groups in the present 
study, though not great, are pertinent. Remembering 
that each unusually gifted child was matched with a 
less gifted child for chronological age and sex, it will be 
interesting to note certain comparative performances. 
On the social-studies test child D, a girl, was the only 
member of the less gifted group who excelled her "twin" 
in the very gifted group. On the mathematics test, no 
child in the less gifted group excelled the one with whom 
he was matched in the very gifted group. The two 
C's were tied. Child A of the very gifted group, a brilliant 
Jewish boy with an LQ. of 194, consistently topped the 
series of scores. This is as it should be, since A had a 
mental age of 22 years 2 months, the highest among the 
twelve children. 

PROGRESS QUOTIENTS 

Terman 1 mentions the fact that not a single child in 
his gifted group of 616 was retarded when retardation 
was defined as being in a grade lower than would be 
expected on the basis of his chronological age. As a 

1 TERMAN, op. ctt., p. 253. 

132 



MENTAL CHARACTERISTICS AND ACHIEVEMENTS 

matter of fact, on this basis four out of five gifted children 
are actually accelerated. However, when mental age 
is used as a criterion, the picture is entirely different. 
On this basis, all but six of Tennan's group of 616 were 
retarded. He found the average amount of actual 
acceleration to be a little more than one year, or one 
grade, which placed them on an educational level far 
below their scholastic attainments. 

Witty 1 found a similar situation existing among the 
gifted children whom he studied. He reports a mean 
progress quotient of 116 for this group as contrasted 
with a mean LQ. of 153 and a mean I.Q. of 136. 

This situation is a serious one and merits careful 
consideration. Should the gifted child be given full 
acceleration? Should he be placed with others of the same 
age, but have his school program enriched? Should he be 
segregated in an opportunity class where both accelera- 
tion and enrichment are possible? These questions are 
important ones and will be given attention in later 
chapters on educational adjustments. 

Y, op. tit., pp. 405-406. 



133 



CHAPTER VII 
CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 

1. Were eminent men and women mentally backward when they 
were children? 

2. Do intellectually precocious children regress toward mediocrity 
as they grow into adulthood? 

3. If constancy of development is an established principle, are there 
any exceptions? 

Thinking concerning the important question of the 
constancy of mental development has been considerably 
prejudiced by the frequent recurrence of two contradic- 
tory assertions: first, that the I.Q. of an individual 
remains exactly the same throughout his life; second, 
that the LQ. varies so markedly that it is possible for a 
child to be at one end of the distribution of intelligence 
at a certain age and at the other a few years later. No 
psychologist of standing would advocate either of these 
two extreme points of view, yet in many lay or student 
discussions one or the other is advanced as representing 
the thought of a group of scientists. Psychologists, 
rather, are concerned with the question of how much the 
I.Q. varies from year to year, fully realizing that it does 
change but fully realizing, also, that it changes within 
limits. The question of the extent of this change has a 
very important bearing upon any discussion of genius, 
for if it is great, then the whole program of the selection 
and education of intellectually gifted children for future 
leadership collapses, since this program rests on the 
assumption that those who are identified in their early 
years as being mentally superior will maintain their 

134 



CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 



relative position throughout life. In the following pages 
data that have been collected largely during the last 
twenty years will be examined to see if the conclusions 
of scientific investigations support the principle that 
mental growth proceeds at the rate at which it starts. 

Nemzek 1 analyzed a number of studies dealing with 
the constancy of the LQ. He considered not only inves- 
tigations in which the Stanford Revision of the Binet- 
Simon Scale was used, but also those which reported 
results from the use of group tests. He summarizes his 
findings in the following concluding remarks and table: 

The results from studies concerning the constancy of the IQ 
present a high degree of consistency. As one method of comparing 
the results of individual examination with those of group tests, the 
reliability coefficients found by correlating test and retest IQ's may 
be arranged into a frequency distribution. 

TABLE XXI. FREQUENCY DISTBIBUTION OF RELIABILITY COEFFICIENTS 
FOUND BY CORRELATING TEST AND RETEST I.Q.'s 



r's 


f (Stanford-Binet) 


/ (Group) 


.95-. 99 


5 


1 


.90-. 94 


15 


3 


.85-. 89 


20 


9 


.80-. 84 


23 


6 


.75-. 79 


12 


3 


.70-. 74 


9 


4 


.65-.69 


8 





.60-. 64 


3 


1 


.55-. 59 


1 





.50-.54 


1 





N 


97 


27 


Median 


.832 


.846 


3 


.889 


.885 


Q l 


,76 


.779 


Q 


.0645 


.053 



1 NEMZEK, C. M., The Constancy of the IQ, PsychoL Butt,, Vol. 30, 
p. 154, 1933. 

135 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

The preceding table presents correlations; hence, gives 
no information concerning changes in individual I.Q.'s. 
Theoretically, a single LQ. possesses a probable error of 
5, which means that the chances are fifty-fifty that the 
obtained measure lies within five points of the true 
measure. This, however, allows opportunity for consider- 
able variation; it is not at all uncommon to find that, on 
a retest, a child's LQ. is ten or fifteen points higher or 
lower than the one first obtained. Miller 1 , analyzing data 
gathered by Hirsch, who had reexamined annually for a 
period of six years a group of 343 elementary school 
children, found that 1736 of the 2400 obtained LQ. dif- 
ferences were less than ten points. However, of the 
remaining 434 differences, 67 were twenty points or more. 

From these figures two deductions can be drawn: first, 
that in the great majority of instances the LQ. remained 
approximately constant; second, that in a few cases there 
was a considerable change. These figures are based on 
the test scores of elementary school children. As has been 
pointed out before, present-day intelligence tests are 
best adapted for use with this group. 

R. L. Thorndike analyzed the Stanford-Binet retest 
records in the files of three well known New York private 
schools: Ethical Culture, Horace Mann, and Lincoln. 
Thorndike based his analysis upon those retests which 
had been given after intervals of at least two and one 
half years. No test record was used if the child was over 
fourteen years of age at the time of testing. In the case 
of a few of the records used the children were below five 
years of age at the time the initial test was given. This 
fact would tend to result in somewhat greater LQ. 
changes than would occur if all the tests had been given 
at a somewhat later age. Thorndike points out that the 

1 MILLER, W. S.: Variation of I.Q/s Obtained from Group Tests, J. 
Educ. Psychol, Vol. 24, pp. 468-474, 1933. 

136 



CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 



mean I.Q, for each of the three schools is approxi- 
mately 118. 

The amount of change in I.Q. between test and retest 
for 1167 children in these three superior schools is sum- 
marized in Table XXII. In two of these schools the 

TABLE XXII. DISTRIBUTION OF DIFFERENCES BETWEEN INITIAL AND 

RETEST I.Q.* 



Amount of change 
(difference) 


School 
A 


School 
B 


School 
C 


Total 


48 to 52 




2 




2 


43 to 47 




2 


I 


3 


38 to 42 


2 


4 


I 


7 


33 to 37 


1 


9 


1 


11 


28 to 32 


3 


7 


5 


15 


23 to 27 


3 


22 


20 


45 


18 to 22 


14 


31 


15 


60 


13 to 17 - 


26 


46 


33 


105 


8 to 12 


42 


56 


56 


154 


3 to 7 


48 


62 


69 


179 


2 to 2 


49 


55 


72 


176 


7 to 3 


47 


48 


71 


166 


12 to 8 


27 


31 


65 


123 


17 to 13 


20 


18 


42 


80 


22 to 18 


7 


5 


9 


21 


27 to - 23 


3 


2 


3 


8 


32 to 28 


1 


2 


1 


4 


37 to 33 


1 


2 


3 


6 


42 to 38 - - 






1 


1 


47 to 43 .... 






1 


1 














294 


404 


469 


1,167 




+1.40 


+6.17 


+0.65 


+2.77 


SD 


11.65 


13.75 


12.36 


12.89 


S D of the mean 


0.67 


0.68 


0.57 


0.38 













* THOBNTHKB, R. L,, "Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National Society for the Study 
of Education," Part II, p. 355, 1,940. 

average gain is much too small to be considered sig- 
nificant. In the third however, there is a mean differ- 

137 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

ence of 6 .17 points. Thorndike confesses his inability to 
explain so considerable an increase. 

As in the case of the Miller analysis the Thorndike 
data indicate that while, in general, the amount of 
change in I.Q. is small in a few instances it reaches 
considerable proportions. 

Psychologists who have studied the development of 
preschool children present conflicting evidence concern- 
ing their mental development as measured by tests. A 
tendency for these young children to earn higher scores 
after a period of time in a stimulating environment is 
pointed out in the Iowa studies. Shirley reports in "The 
First Two Years" that she finds much uneveness in 
development during the first eighteen months, but that 
after this period the children whom she studied were 
likely to hold their respective places, 

Gesell 1 after a ten year study of 30 children, many of 
whom were tested during the first year of life, feels 
justified in saying : 

In no instance did the course of growth prove whimsical or erratic. 
The behavior biographies give clear evidence of a high degree of 
latent predictability, even in infancy. 

Gesell, 2 by way of illustration, gives the following 
brief description of the intellectual development of a 
gifted boy. 

Child 0. P., at the age of three years was clinically adjudged to 
be distinctly superior because of the dynamic qualities of his per- 
formances, even though his initial intelligence score was not very 
extraordinary. On six annual examinations his drawing abilities 
proved equal to the average; his IC^s fluctuated widely: 115, 135, 
140, 130, 165, 160. Our clinical estimates of his capacity, however, 
remained consistently favorable and did not undergo corresponding 

1 GESELL, A., "Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National Society for the 
Study of Education," Part II, p. 149, 1940. 

2 Ibid. p. 152. 

138 



CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 

fluctuations. He passed his College Entrance Board examinations at 
age 16, after a superior record in preparatory school. He remains true 
to the superiority foreshadowed at age three. 

Anderson/ on the other hand, points out that intel- 
ligence tests are much more satisfactory when used upon 
elementary school children. He says, 

Infant tests, as at present constituted, measure very little, if at 
all, the function that is called 'intelligence 1 at later ages. Preschool 
intelligence tests, while they are instruments of some value and 
usefulness, measure only a portion of that function. Whether it will 
be possible to develop tests at these levels that will measure more of 
the function remains to be seen. 

LOOKING BACKWABD 

There are two ways of gathering facts on the problem 
as to whether or not genius maintains its relative place on 
the intellectual scale. The first is to look back into the 
early years of men and women of recognized eminence to 
see if they were mentally precocious children. The second 
is to follow the development of children who, at an early 
age, have been identified as being intellectually gifted to 
observe whether or not they grow into adult geniuses. 

As was pointed out earlier, it is commonly believed 
that most great men not only come from humble social 
and economic beginnings but also as children were 
mentally dull. It seems more just to have it that way 
rather than to believe that they were bright and success- 
ful all their lives. This point of view is aided and abetted 
by the difficulty which is experienced in comparing a 
certain eminent man with the populace as a whole. 
For instance, President Harding has frequently been 
referred to as an average man because he did not equal 
many other American presidents and statesmen; but 

1 ANDEESON, J. E., "Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National Society 
for the Study of Education," Part I, pp. 401-402, 194(X 

139 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

when compared to the actual average man such a one 
as would fit the Hollingworth description in the first 
chapter Harding is obviously superior. The following 
letter, written by a young man of grade school education 
whose intelligence is known by test to be slightly above 
average, will serve as a reminder of what the intellectual 
behavior of an average adult is really like: 

Dear John: 

Jim and I have about finished work on your automobile. 

Except a little more to be done on the fender. 

An hour's work or so which we would of done this morning but 
it's raining hard. 

And I have to go back to Stratton today. 

Jim will finish that when it clears up. 

And he'll send you his bill. 

Yours truely, 

Statements similar to those concerning Harding are 
commonly made of other eminent men of the present and 
of the past. For instance, it is frequently said that Grant, 
graduate at the middle of his class at West Point and a 
rather mediocre president, was a man of only average 
intelligence; yet to have been graduated from West 
Point at all required superior mentality. Hitler is com- 
monly offered as a man of average intellect who has 
attained tremendous power. It is perhaps true that 
Hitler does not have so good a mind as some of his con- 
temporaries, yet he is obviously far above average in this 
respect. Huey Long is a favorite example of a dullard who 
attained a high place, yet an examination of the intel- 
lectual behavior and school achievement of the boy Huey 
indicates an I.Q. of close to 200. There are, of course, 
wide differences in the intellectual capacities of men who 
have achieved eminence, but it is extremely unlikely that 
even one of them was below average, or even average, in 
intelligence as a child. 

140 



CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 

Helen Cohen 1 and Nancy Coryell have edited a volume 
containing reports on gifted high school children in the 
city of New York. Scattered through the book are com- 
ments on the school successes of certain outstanding 
graduates. Such instances do not constitute final proof 
of the constancy of achievement, but they are interest- 
ing examples of the "able man bright child" principle. 
The following items are taken at random from the 
section on English studies: 

Anna E. Bennet, who was graduated there in June 1931, wrote a 
number of remarkable poems for the school magazine and won the 
interschool poetry contest. She received a scholarship from Adelphi 
College where she is now a student. The poems she wrote have 
appeared in Harper's Magazine, The Lantern^ Poetry, and Voices. 

Esther Horowitz, who was editor-in-chief of the school magazine, 
has shown decided literary ability. Shortly after graduation she was 
engaged by the Mathilde Weil Literary Agency as critic of poetry. 
Contributions of hers have appeared in The New Yorker. 

While Alice Glasgow was in Hunter College, two of her plays, 
Onentcde and Scherzo, were produced and awarded prizes by the 
Century Theatre Club. After graduation, she was teacher-in-training 
in the Department of English in Washington Irving. Her first novel, 
The Twisted Tendril, a portrayal of the life of Guy Wilkes Booth, was 
published by Stokes in 1928. 

Claudette Colbert, undoubtedly the most widely known graduate 
of Washington Irving on the stage or screen, discovered herself in 
the course of her training on the boards of the Washington Irving 
theatre. Because of excellent class work in English this student 
(then known as Claudette Chauchoin) was recommended for and 
took part in LaunceLot George, a one-act play by a teacher of English 
in the school. Immediately following this, while still in the school, 
she appeared at the Provincetown Playhouse with Alice Rostetter 
in the fetter's play The Widow's Veil. This was Claudette's first 
professional appearance. In the last term she won a book, the class 
prize in Engjish. After graduation in 1920 and her stage success, 
Miss Colbert visited the school, by invitation, and talked to the 

1 COHEN, H. L., and N. G. COBYEIA, "Educating Superior Students," 
pp. 75, 81, 82, 83, American Book Company, New York, 

141 



GENIUS IX THE MAKING 

students in the auditorium. Her stage and screen successes are so 
numerous and so well known that it would be idle to list them. 

The Cox Compilation of Biographical Data 
The most complete body of information gathered on 
this question the intellectual status in childhood of men 
who later attained eminence is that of Catherine Morris 
Cox and her coworkers. Miss Cox made a careful and 
detailed study of all available biographical material on 
301 of the most eminent men and women in the world 
during the period between 1450 and 1850. On the basis of 
her data, the early mental behavior of these eminent 
individuals was compared with the standards of behavior 
of present-day children as determined by scientifically 
constructed intelligence tests. From this comparison, 
described at length in "Genetic Studies of Genius/ 7 
Vol. II, three experienced judges, Lewis M. Tennan, 
Maude A. Merrill, and Catherine Cox, made two I.Q. 
estimates for each of the 301 subjects. The first of these 
was based upon the mental behavior of the individual up 
to age seventeen; the second from age seventeen to age 
twenty-six. 

Miss Cox 1 presents three conclusions of considerable 
significance. 

Youtlis who achieve eminence, have in general (a) a heredity above 
the average and (&) superior advantages in early environment. 

Youths who achieve eminence are distinguished in childhood by 
behavior which indicates an unusually high IQ. 

Youths who achieve eminence are characterized not only by high 
intellectual traits, but also by persistence of motive and effort, 
confidence in their abilities, and great strength or force of character. 

Since the second of these conclusions bears directly 
upon the central problem of this chapter, Miss Cox's 2 
elaboration of the bare statement is presented in full. 



C. M., "Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol H, pp. 215-218, 
Stanford University Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1926. 
*Ibid., pp. 217-218. 

142 



CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 

A corrected estimate indicates that the true mean IQ for the group 
is not below 155 and probably at least as high as 165. The average of 
the obtained IQ estimates for a small group of cases more adequately 
reported than the others is 176 for the first 17 years of life. The 
corrected estimate, indicating a nearer approximation to a true IQ, 
is for the same group 184. It is probable that a number of the cases 
included among the SOI actually ranked in intelligence not far below 
the composition scores of several of their number and of these, many 
are well above the 200 IQ mark. Arnauld, Comte, Goethe, Grotius, 
Laplace, Leopardi, Michelangelo, Newton, Pascal, the younger 
Pitt, Sarpi, Schelling, Voltaire, and Wolsey probably rated at 
200 IQ or even higher. . . . 

The significant conclusion in the present study is derived from 
the evidence it presents that the extraordinary genius who achieves 
the highest eminence is also the gifted individual whom intelligence 
tests may discover in childhood. The converse of this proposition 
is yet to be proved. 

The biographical material summarized by Miss Cox 
constitutes a storehouse of factual material on the 
constancy of mental development and of achievement. 
From it to take a very few items it is learned that: 
Coleridge and Swift could read the Bible at the age of 
three, and von Humboldt could both read and write at 
the same age. Tasso was using words meaningfully when 
he was six months old and began the study of grammar 
at the age of three years. Victor Hugo had taught himself 
to read before he was six and learned writing and 
arithmetic almost as quickly. Longfellow was well into 
Latin grammar at the age of seven, and Scott had learned 
to read before he was four. At the age of eleven, Scott 
made the following translation from Latin: 

In awful ruins Aetna thunders nigh, 
And sends in pitchy whirlwinds to the sky 
Black clouds of smoke, which, still as they aspire, 
From then* dark sides there bursts the glowing fire; 
At other times huge balls of fire are toss'd, 
That lick the stars, and in the smoke are lost: 
Sometimes the mount, with vast convulsions torn, 
143 



GENIUS IX THE MAKING 

Emits huge rocks, which instantly are borne 
With loud explosions to the starry skies, 
The stones made liquid as the huge mass flies, 
Then back again with greater weight recoils, 
While Aetna thundering from, the bottom boils. 

George Sand had learned to write before she was five, 
but she never learned how to spell correctly. It was said 
of William Pitt that he grasped the meaning of a passage 
so readily that he never seemed to learn but only to 
recollect, Washington became a professional surveyor at 
fifteen, and had the responsible task of surveying the 
Fairfax estate when he was only sixteen. Newton was a 
silent, thoughtful boy who never played with other 
children, but devoted all his leisure time to making inven- 
tions. Charlotte Bronte and her sisters were authors while 
they were yet children. By the age of ten Charlotte was 
writing stories of 20,000 words. At the age of thirteen, 
she wrote this letter to a magazine editor: 

Sir, It is well known, that the Genii have declared that unless 
they perform certain arduous duties every year, of a mysterious 
nature, all the worlds in the firmament will be burnt up, and gathered 
together in one mighty 'globe, which will roll in solitary grandeur 
through the vast wilderness of space, inhabited only by the four 
high princes of the Genii, till time shall be succeeded by Eternity; 
and the impudence of this is only to be paralleled by another of their 
assertions, namely, that by their magic might they can reduce the 
world to a desert, the purest waters to streams of livid poison, and 
the clearest lakes to stagnant waters, the pestilential vapours of 
which shall slay all living creatures, except the blood-thirsty beast 
of the forest, and the ravenous bird of the rock; but that in the midst 
of this desolation the palace of the Chief Genii shall rise sparkling in 
the wilderness, and the horrible howl of their warcry shall spread 
over the land at morning, at noontide and night; but that they shall 
have their annual feast over the bones of the dead, and shall yearly 
rejoice with the joy of victors. I think, sir, that the horrible wicked- 
ness of this needs no remark, and therefore I haste to subscribe 
myself, &c, 

July 14, 1829. 

144 



CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 

The following description of the early behavior of 
Goethe indicates the kind of criteria upon which Miss 
Cox 1 and her cojudges based their estimates of the 
intellectual status of the eminent men whom they were 
studying. It serves the purpose, also, of bringing into 
bold relief the boyhood picture of the man of genius. 

Development to age 17. 

1. Interests. When Goethe was 4K a puppet theatre, presented by 
his grandmother, stimulated his dramatic sense, and at 6K he began 
to arrange and conduct plays on this miniature stage. Before he was 
8 he and some of his companions developed a passion for writing 
poetry. Young Goethe thought his works superior to the others; but 
when he found that his fellow " authors " had the same impression 
of their verses, he became discouraged and gave up writing for a 
time, only, however, to resume activity later, after praise had 
heartened him to try again. Intellectual or political discussions 
quickened the lad's thought, and the tales of his father's travels 
aroused in him the desire for wider horizons. In this stimulating 
intellectual atmosphere he enjoyed the balancing advantage of a 
first hand knowledge of the arts and crafts as practiced by his towns- 
men. At local fairs young Goethe was thrilled by the view of strange 
wares from many a far country; on his explorations of the city, 
history became a living tale. At the age of 9 he built an altar and 
developed a mystical religion of his own in the hope of approaching 
God directly and thus worshiping him without priestly intervention. 
Between 9^ and 12, he became deeply interested in French. He 
studied the language with the French commandant, quartered in his 
father's house; he read dramatic theory and criticism; and he visited 
the French theatre regularly on a pass presented by his grandfather, 
the mayor. In connection with his attendance at the play he learned, 
by visits behind the scenes, something of the contrast between the 
actors' lives and their professional attitudes. 

Dispatched to the university, Goethe (at 16) was, as always, full of 
literary and social interests. He read widely, attended the theatre, 
and discoursed with his friends. He also devoted considerable tune 
to the writing of verses in German, French, English, and Italian, 
attempting in his productions something more than the pseudo-poetry 
of the day. 

, pp. 694-698. 

145 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

2. Education, From the age of 3 until he was 6, Goethe attended a 
day nursery <>*" kindergarten, and here, according to tradition, he 
learned to read. His father had already begun to tell the little lad 
and his sister the history of the town. An ABC book was purchased 
for young Goethe (aged 4> 2 ') and a year later a catechism with 
Biblical quotations was presented to him. At this latter age, he 
attended a public school with his sister for several months. . . . 
Goethe's training in Latin began when he was 7 and within a few 
months he was writing free Latin compositions and learning military 
and legal phrases in the language. The corrections in his exercise 
books concern errors in orthography and the like rather than errors 
in language usage. One of his tutors, who was also a public school 
teacher, arranged prize literary competitions in which Goetbe com- 
peted successfully with boys in the regular school. A legal friend 
instructed the boy in international law, while other associates of his 
father and grandfather took an interest in his education along the 
lines in which they had specialized. When Goethe wag 9M, the 
French were quartered in Frankfort. The French commandant of 
the city was quartered in the Goethe house where he attracted an 
interesting group of people, especially artists, from whom Goethe 
learned something of their art. The prescribed preconfirmation 
instruction, which followed in regular course, was thoroughly dis- 
tasteful to the 12-year-old boy. More satisfying to him were a variety 
of studies carried on at this time including mathematics. English, 
drawing, piano playing and at length after much importunity, 
Hebrew also. At 14 his reading included law and Latin. From 14 to 
17 Ms scientific and pre-legal education progressed; Latin had by 
this time been thoroughly mastered. 

At 17 Goethe had completed a broad and liberal course of training: 
he was familiar with the poetry of the leading nations; his reading of 
German, French, Latin, and Hebrew literature had been extensive; he 
was conversant with the language and history of the principal coun- 
tries of Europe and he knew the political and legal history of Ger- 
many in minute detail; he had made progress in the study of theology 
and jurisprudence and the natural sciences; he knew something of 
drawing and music; he played the piano and the flute; and the artist 
Seekatz considered him a promising art student. At 16 he entered the 
University of Leipzig as a student of law and literature, for the elder 
Goethe intended that the brilliant career as a jurist which he had 
failed to achieve for himself should be realized by his son; but young 
Goethe wished to be a poet. 

146 



CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 

3. School standing and progress. "By means of a ready apprehen- 
sion, practice, and a good memory " Goethe soon outgrew the instruc- 
tions his father and his other teachers were able to give without ever 
requiring or receiving elementary drill in anything. Grammar which 
seemed to him a matter of arbitrary rules and exceptions, he despised, 
and he learned it only with some difficulty; Latin and geography, 
which he learned in verse, he thoroughly enjoyed and hence acquired 
readily enough. In rhetoric, composition, and the like he always 
excelled. Grammatical exercises, written when little Goethe was 7K 
and 8 show thorough and painstaking study and the ability to 
express the experiences and impressions of every-day life in natural 
and facile Latin form. When his penmanship was rated with that of 
other children in the town it stood, on an average, 9th in a group of 
SO. At 8J, Goethe began to translate exercises into Latin in imitation 
of the historian Justinian similar to those designed by his tutor, 
who was also assistant school rector, for the senior class in the 
Gymnasium. Thus in his 9th year the lad was competing with boys 
of 16 to 22 years. At 8 Goethe learned so readily that he was able to 
pick up Italian from overhearing his sister and her tutor, while he 
himself was studying his Latin lesson. His rapid progress in French 
reconciled his protesting father to his attendance at the theatre when 
the French classics were presented. A serious boy, and even at the 
age of 10 always the most industrious of them ail, he was annoyed 
by the trifling of his playmates. 

4. Friends and associates. In consequence of being made much of 
by his father's friends, Goethe became self-conscious and somewhat 
vain; but even his playmates admitted his actual superiority: "We 
were all his lackeys," later wrote one boy two years his senior. 
Goethe's intellectual endowment, his skill in narrating thrilling tales, 
the distinction of his bearing and manner made him a leader among 
his fellows. At the university, many of Goethe's associates were men 
of established reputation and ability, ten or more years his senior. 
Two girls had in turn won Goethe's passionate devotion before he 
was 17; the first when he was 14, the second when he was 16. 

5. Reading. When barely 6 years old Goethe began to examine 
and read the illustrated Orbis pidus, Merian's illustrated Bible (then 
and later a favorite), Gottfried's illustrated chronicles of universal 
history and Heidegger's Acerra pkUologica. A little later he was read- 
ing Robinson Crusoe, Rie Insel Feteenbnrg, and similar tales. At 8, 
he was already somewhat acquainted in his father's library with the 
older German poets of the 18th century, the best Latin and Italian 

147 



GENIUS IX THE MAKING 

poets, Roman antiquities, classic works on jurisprudence, books of 
travel, historical and philosophical treatises, and encyclopaedias of 
all kinds. A little later modern works, forbidden by his father, were 
secretly obtained and devoured. Evenings at home were spent in 
reading aloud in various edifying works. Bower's History of the Popes 
was one book so read and reread in its heavy entirety, for according 
to the elder Goethe's plan a work once begun had always to be 
finished. In his uncle's library the lad found a delightful translation 
of Homer with copperplate illustrations. Virgil came to hand a little 
later. While the French were in Frankfort, Goethe (between 9 and 12) 
read the works of the principal French dramatists: all of Racine and 
Moliere, and most of Corneille. In his 13th year he studied Hebrew 
and read much and long in the Bible; at 14 he read chiefly legal and 
philosophical books. But he was best satisfied by works in which 
poetry, religion, and philosophy were united. At 15 he read among 
other things the works of Wieland, French plays, and the dramas of 
Leasing. At 16 he was especially impressed by Lessing's Laokodn and 
Dodd's Beauties of Shakespeare. To supply the shortcomings in his 
prescribed training he had recourse to such encyclopaedic works as 
those of Bayle and Gessner. 

6. Production and achievement. The free compositions which 
Goethe wrote at 7} and 8 include three conversations that possess 
genuine creative quality and exhibit a remarkable ability in charac- 
terization. The "morning salutations" in German, Latin, and Greek, 
written before he was 9, express charming sentiments in artistic form; 
but Greek exercises of this period exhibit the faults of a beginner. 
At 9Ji Goethe had amassed a considerable collection of lessons and 
stories. At 10 he wrote a little play, hoping it might be staged; at 12 
he composed a story in the form of letters written in Latin, with a 
sprinkling of Greek, English, French, Italian, Yiddish, and German. 
In this year a series of controversial sermons so aroused his interest 
that he undertook to preserve them by dictation to his father's 
secretary, and with the help of a few notes he succeeded for several 
weeks in reconstructing what he had heard for the edification of his 
father. Gradually, however, interest in this undertaking waned and 
the report dwindled into a mere outline. 

7. Evidences of precocity. When the Christian world was stag- 
gered by the Lisbon earthquake, Goethe, aged 6, heard more than 
one sermon devoted to an explanation of this apparent contradiction 
of Providence. His comment was as follows: "After all, it is probably 
much ampler than they suppose. God knows that the immortal 

148 



CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 

soul can suffer BO harm through such a fate." Goethe's father early 
recognized his son's unusual ability, and friends of the family enthu- 
siastically mapped out careers suited to such rare talents. One wished 
to make him a courtier, another a diplomat, a third a jurist. Goethe's 
mother, when her son was 9, noting that he was distinguished from 
the other boys by his erect carriage, mentioned the fact to him in 
praise. He replied that he would later be distinguished from them in 
other ways. On another occasion he stated that he would never be 
satisfied with that which satisfied others. His more than common 
rapidity of development admitted him to confirmation at an unusually 
early age (12). Intellectual forwardness appeared in many serious 
discussions with his tutor. For example the 14-year-old was inter- 
ested to argue at length that there was no need for a separate study 
of philosophy, as religion and poetry covered the field. At 16, Goethe 
was familiar with the culture of Europe and his interests and tastes 
were those of a scholar and man of the world. 

LOOKING FOEWARD 

The typical intellectually gifted child, possessing at 
birth a much greater mental capacity and a slightly 
superior physical equipment than the child of average 
intelligence, progresses through early infancy at a 
relatively rapid pace. The rate of development varies, of 
course, with his various abilities. While it is very rapid 
for intellect, it may be only slightly above average for 
physique or social sense. The median for a group of gifted 
children, however, will nearly always exceed the median 
for a group of unselected children for any ability. 

There is an old New England saying, "Tali: before you 
go, born to sorrow and woe." Mentally superior children 
born to the sorrow and woe of carrying the major part 
of the burdens of civilization are likely to be able to use 
several words meaningfully before they can walk. The 
age at which the normal child learns to walk is, on the 
average, fourteen months, and the age at which he learns 
to talk that is, to use one or more words with meaning 
is approximately fifteen months. The feeble-minded 

149 



GENIUS IX THE MAKING 

child is approximately two years old before he learns to 
walk and over three before he learns to talk. The typical 
gifted child uses three words with understanding at the 
age of eleven months, and walks at thirteen months. In 
both walking and talking, bright children are superior, 
but their acceleration is relatively much greater in 
language than in physical movement. 

The extent to which gifted children maintain their 
mental status at or toward the top as they grow from 
infancy through childhood into maturity can be deter- 
mined only by long-range studies, The two most satis- 
factory efforts in this direction are the Hollingworth 
Growth Study of a group of intellectually gifted children 
in New York and the Terman Growth Study of a much 
larger group of such children in California. 

The Hollingworth Growth Study 

In 1922-1923 Prof. Leta S. Hollingworth, of Teachers 
College, Columbia, assisted by a committee from Public 
School 165, Manhattan, and Teachers College, initiated 
a project of selecting and following the development of a 
group of intellectually gifted children. One hundred and 
forty-eight such children, identified as having an I.Q. of 
at least 133, were chosen. Of this group fifty-six were 
taken as a randon sample whose growth would be care- 
fully and accurately followed for a period of ten years. 
Of these fifty-six only two were missing at the time the 
final test was given at the end of the decade. 

The follow-up studies which Prof. Hollingworth and 
her collaborators made definitely proved that t^e intel- 
lectual development of this group of gifted children had 
been strikingly constant. One of the earlier reports 
appeared in a monograph by Edna Lamson. 1 

1 LAMSON, E,, "A Study of Young Gifted Children in Senior High 
School/' p. 117, Teachers College, Contributions to Education, No. 424, 
Columbia University, 19SO. 

150 



CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 

Miss Lamson, working with the aforementioned group 
of fifty-six, presents data which show a slight increase 
in the average LQ. of the group during the first three 
years, when it was possible to test them with the 
Stanford Revision of the Binet-Simon Scale. The mean 
LQ. of the group on the first test was 153 1.7. One year 
later, the average LQ. for the same group was 156 1.1. 
One year after the second test was given, the average 
LQ. was found to be 157 1.2. The range also remained 
constant, no child, on any one of the three tests, falling 
below an LQ. of 135. The first-year range was 135-190, 
the second-year range 135-188, the third-year range 
137-188. Miss Lamson points out that the standard 
deviations for the three distributions were almost exactly 
the same. She also emphasizes the fact that all these 
children, who were in the top 1 per cent of the population 
at the beginning of the series of tests, were still in the top 
1 per cent at the end of the series. 

Miss Lamson, following the development of these 
children into and through high school from which they 
were graduated at an average chronological age of sixteen 
tested them with the Army Alpha examination in 1929, 
when the average chronological age of the group was 
fifteen. She found that they were in the top decile of the 
high school population when chronological age was held 
constant. Since high school students are a selected 
group, Lamson says that the scores which her group of 
gifted children earned on Army Alpha place them in the 
top 1 per cent for an unselected group. The median score 
earned by the gifted group on Army Alpha was fifteen 
points higher than the median obtained by a group of 
252 graduate students at Columbia University. 

Hollingworth and Kaunitz 1 report on the intellectual 
status of the Hollingworth group of gifted children 

1 HOI^INGWORTH, L. S., and R. M. KAJJOTTS, "The Gentile Status of 

151 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



approximately ten years after their identification. These 
children, now at an average age of 18 years 6 months, 

TABLE XXIII. PRESENTING TOTAL DATA otf AGE, SEX, AND TEST 

SCOEES OF EVERY FOUBTH CHILD OF 116 INTELLECTUALLY GIFTED 

CHILDREN RETESTED AT OR NEAR MATURITY* 



Subject 


Sex 


LQ. 

(S-B) 


Age 


Army 
Alpha 
score 


Form 
of 
Alpha 


Age 


Years 


Months 


Years 


Months 


4 


M 


178 


7 


7 


188 


5 


15 


10 


8 


F 


173 


7 


1 


181 


5 


16 


1 


12 


F 


170 


8 


1 


180 


8 


19 





16 


F 


167 


9 





197 


8 


19 


1 


20 


M 


162 


8 


8 


192 


8 


19 


3 


24 


M 


160 


5 


6 


188 


8 


15 


7 


28 


M 


157 


7 


2 


189 


5 


15 


9 


32 


F 


157 


9 


5 


189 


8 


19 


1 


36 


M 


156 


9 


9 


200 


8 


20 





40 


M 


154 


8 


6 


153 


8 


18 


11 


44 


M 


153 


10 


2 


177 


7 


18 


6 


48 


M 


152 


11 


4 


197 


7 


18 


10 


52 


M 


151 -f 


11 


4 


184 


7 


18 


11 


56 


F 


150 


9 





191 


8 


19 





60 


F 


147 


10 


6 


150 


7 


17 


4 


64 


F 


146 


7 


5 


153 


7 


16 


6 


68 


F 


145 


9 


4 


159 


8 


19 


7 


72 


F 


145 


8 


11 


192 


8 


19 


3 


76 


F 


144 


8 


10 


171 


8 


19 


4 


80 


M 


141 


8 


3 


160 


8 


17 


9 


84 


M 


141 


9 


8 


182 


8 


19 


11 


88 


M 


140 


8 


7 


189 


7 


18 


1 


92 


M 


139 


9 





183 


8 


19 


2 


96 


M 


138 


8 


3 


171 


7 


17 


3 


100 


M 


138 


8 


6 


167 


8 


18 


8 


104 


M 


136 


8 


4 


164 


7 


17 


4 


108 


M 


135 


6 


8 


163 


5 


15 


9 


112 


F 


133 


8 


1 


156 


7 


17 


3 


116 


M 


133 


9 


3 


151 


8 


18 


8 



* Adapted from HOLIINGWOBTH, L. S., and R- M. KATTNTTZ, Tlie Centile Status of 
Gifted Children at Maturity, J. Genet. Psychol. t vol. 45, p. 109, September, 1934. 

Gifted Children at Maturity," /. Genet. PsychoL, pp. 106-120, Vol. 45, 
September, 1934. 

152 



CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 

with no one less than 15, were too old to be given the 
Stanford-Binet test, so Hollingworth and Kaunitz chose 
Army Alpha even though, as the writers point out, the 
general-information section of this instrument is now at 
least partly obsolete and hence somewhat penalizes any 
group taking it at the present day. Data on each of the 
116 intellectually gifted children are summarized in table 
form by the writers. Table XXIII includes the reports 
on every fourth child of the 116. 

As Hollingworth and Kaunitz point out, an analysis 
of the scores earned by soldiers in the American army 
during the World War indicates an average of 62.9 points 
with the top centile earning 165 points or better* When 
Hollingworth and Kaunitz 1 compared the scores earned' 
by their 116 gifted subjects with those earned by the 
large group of male adults who were examined with the 
Army Alpha in 1917-1918, they found that 95 of the 116 
subjects, when tested as they approached maturity, 
reached the top centile. Nineteen fell at or about the 
ninety-seventh centile. Two, both girls, fell below the 
ninety-seventh centile. They point out that 82 per cent 
of the gifted group fell, at maturity, into the same centile 
of the white draft on Army Alpha which they occupied 
in childhood among school children on Stanford-Binet. 
The remaining 18 per cent very nearly reached this 
status. No individual regressed to average. 

A later report on the Hollingworth group was made by 
Lorge and Hollingworth, 2 In this study the investigators 
were concerned with the problem of the extent to which 
status on the CAVD scale at maturity is predictable 
from status on the Stanford-Binet in childhood. Eighteen 

1 HOLUNGWORTH and KATJNITZ, op. dt., p. 116. 

* LORGE, I., and L. S. HOLUNGWOBTH, Adult Status of Highly Intelli- 
gent Children, Fed. Sem., and J. Genet. PsyckaL, Vol. 49, pp. 215-226, 
1936. 

153 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

children who had been identified as being intellectually 
gifted when they were at or near eight years of age and 
who, ten years later, had been found to rate well toward 
the top on Army Alpha were now, thirteen years after the 
original test, given the CAYD, levels N to Q. Lorge and 
Hollingworth conclude that children possessing LQ.'s of 
140 and above fall within the upper quartile of the 
college-graduate population of the United States when 
they are at or near maturity. 

The data on the Hollingworth group, then, collected 
over a period of more than a decade, show definitely that 
these children, at least, have maintained their intellectual 
status through the years. There is little reason to believe 
that what has been found to be true of them is not typical 
of intellectually gifted children everywhere. 

The Terman Growth Study 

Terman's genetic study of a large group of intellectu- 
ally gifted children in California is, as has been mentioned 
earlier, the most comprehensive so far conducted. 
Terman and his coworkers gathered extensive data on 
the 643 cases in what he called the "Main Experimental 
Group/ 7 They also studied, although not so completely, 
approximately 300 other bright children. These subjects 
were selected in 1921-1922, and a full report of then- 
progress during the first six years that is, up to 1927- 
1928 was made in "Genetic Studies of Genius," VoL 
III. Included in this is a chapter 1 which presents data 
on the intellectual status of these gifted children six years 
after their selection. 

The average age of the regular group of 643 cases, at 
time of selection, was 9 years 11 months, with only 
68 cases under seven years of age. For this reason, a 



, L. M., " Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. Ill, Chap. Ill, 
Stanford University Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1930. 

154 



COXSTAXCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 



serious difficulty arose in 1927-1928 concerning the 
selection of adequate tests for reexamination purposes. 
The Stanford Revision of the Binet-Simon Scale could 
be used with the younger children only, because it is not 
difficult enough to test the mental capacity of superior 
adolescents. Terman and his assistants finally decided 
to use the Stanford-Binet for those children who, at the 
time of retesting were less than thirteen years of age, the 
Terman Group Test for those who were between thirteen 
and twenty, unless above college freshman standing, and 
the Thorndike College Entrance Examination for the 
small group of older children and those who were above 
the freshman year in college. 

TABLE XXIV. COMPABISON OF STAXFORD-BIXET I.Q.'s, 1921-1922 AND 

1927-1928* 







Boys 






Girls 






AH 






Mean 


S.D. 


N 


Mean 


S.D. 


N 


Mean 


S.D. 


N 


Regulars, 1921-1922 .... 


147 


8.4 


?7 


150 


11 8 


*>7 


148 


10 4 


">4 


Regulars, 1927-1927 


144 


14.5 


^7 


133 


14 


07 


139 


15 1 


54 


Diff eren ce 


3 


6 1 




17 


2 2 




9 


4 7 




S.D. of difference (approxi- 


2 






2 






2 






Regulars and outside Binets, 
1921-1922., 


146 


8.2 


3R 


149 


11.2 


35 


148 


10.0 


73 


Regulars and outside Binets, 
1927-1928 


143 


17 8t 


S8 


136 


15 4 


?= 


140 


17 


7S 


Difference 


3 


9.6 




13 


4.2 




8 


7.0 




S.D. of difference (approxi- 


2 






2 






1 



























* Adapted from TERMAN, "Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. Ill, p. 251. 
t The excessively high standard deviation is here due to one case, a boy who scored 
198 I.Q. We have reason to believe this boy had been coached before he took the retest. 

Contrary to the Hollingworth findings, Terman dis- 
covered that there was a tendency for the I.Q/s of those 
who were retested with the Stanford-Binet to decrease 
(see Table XXIV). 

155 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

It is important to note here that while the decrease of 
the boys' LQ.'s is very slight, that of the girls is consider- 
able. This sex difference consistently appears in reports 
on the constancy of the intellectual status of gifted 
children. 

As Terman points out, the distribution of changes in 
individual LQ.'s is of greater significance than changes in 
means. He sum in arizes these in the following table: 

TABLE XXV, SUMMARY OF I.Q. CHANGES, 1921-1922 TO 1927-1928* 





I.Q.'s 
lower in 
1927-1928 


LQ.'s 

higher in 
1927-1928 


All Stanford- 
Binet retests 
1927-1928 


Mean 
drop 


N 


Mean 
gain 


N 


Mean 
change 


S.D. 


N 


Regular boys 


13 

14 
16 

16 
15 


15 

24 
25 

30 

54 


12 

16 
1 

10 
14 


10 

12 
1 

4 
16 


12 

13 
16 

15 
14 


8.3 

11.1 
9,3 

9.8 
10.5 


27 

38 

26 f 

34f 
72f 


Regular and outside Binet 
bovs 


Regular girls 


Regular and outside Binet 
girls 


Total, boys and girls 





* Adapted from TBEMAN, " Genetic Studies of Genius," VoL III, p. 26. 

t One girl, whose original I.Q. was 192 and whose corrected 1928 I.Q. was 173, was 
not included in the tabulation, because she passed every test on the Stanf ord-Binet scale 
and hence was not adequately measured. 

In further explanation of Table XXV, Terman 1 says: 

Five of the twenty-seven regular boys and eight of the total thirty- 
eight boys lost as much as 15 points in IQ. Five of the regular boys 
and seven of the total thirty-eight boys gained as much as fifteen 
points each. None of the regular girls and only one of the entire 
thirty-four girls gained as much as fifteen each. There were several 
cases of extreme change, two regular boys and four regular girls each 
dropping 25 points or more. 



1 TBBMAN, op. cit., pp. 26-27. 



156 



CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 

A much larger number of gifted children, 399 to be 
exact, were examined with the Tennan Group Test. This 
instrument is by no means wholly adequate for measuring 
the mental capacity of highly intelligent individuals. As 
Terman points out, a child capable of earning a score of 
180 or better is under a handicap. An examination of the 
following table shows that a great majority of the scores 
earned by the 399 gifted children fall about 180. 

TABLE XXVI. DISTRIBUTION OF TEHRAN GSOUF TEST SCOKES, 1928, 
REGUI.AB GBOTJP* 





Ag 


e!3 


Ag 


e!4 


Ag 


e!5 


Ag 


e!6 


Ag 


Bl7 


Ag 


e!8 


Ag 


e!9 


Jtr oiiiu scores 


B 


G 


B 


G 


B 


G 


B 


G 


B 


G 


B 


G 


B 


G 


216-220 










1 












1 


1 






215 








1 


2 


1 


4 




5 






1 


1 




210 




m 


( f 


B 


6 


t 


5 




7 


5 


6 


3 


2 




205 


1 


. . 


5 


3 


8 


1 


8 


6 


8 


9 


4 


1 


1 




200 


3 


. . 


5 


5 


7 


5 


9 


6 


8 


4 


2 


4 


* . 


1 


195 


5 


2 


4 


5 


10 


8 


10 


3 


5 


5 


3 


2 




1 


190 


1 


. . 


5 


5 


5 


6 


9 


7 


5 


2 


. . 


2 






185 


2 


3 


4 


7 


6 


5 


2 


4 


f 


4 


2 








180 


2 


4 


3 


3 


5 


3 


3 


3 


2 


. . 




1 






175 


1 


2 


1 


1 


1 


3 


1 


f f 




1 


^ 


1 






170 


1 


. 


1 


2 


B 9 


1 


1 


. 


1 












165 


1 


3 






1 










1 










160 


1 


1 


1 


1 






















155 


1 


1 


. . 


2 


. 


. . 




1 














150 


1 


t f 


f ^ 


1 


1 




















145 


1 


1 


. , 


1 




. . 




1 














140 


1 




























135 






























130 




1 


























125 






























120 






























111-115 


1 




























Total. 


23 


18 


29 


37 


53 


33 


52 


38 


41 


31 


18 


16 


4 


3 

































Grand total , 399 



" TXBMAX, "Gmetfo Stadias of Genius," VoL III, p. 32. 

157 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Terman concludes from an analysis of the scores found 
by retesting gifted children with the Terman Group Test 
that the average is equivalent to an I.Q. of between 130 
and 135. This figure, although showing that the children 
still remain well toward the top with respect to intelli- 
gence, indicates that there has been a slight decrease in 
the average I.Q. In connection with the Terman Group 
Test scores, it is interesting to note also that the boys 
once more excelled the girls. This fact is consistent with 
the observations of Lincoln, in the Harvard Growth 
Study, and of Hollingworth in her work with the New 
York group of gifted children. 

Lincoln, 1 reporting on 109 children, 45 boys and 65 
girls, with initial I.Q.'s ranging from 119 to 145, says 
that, when his cases were reexamined after intervals 
ranging from five to eight years, the girls were found to 
have lost more in I.Q. than the boys. Lincoln points out 
that during this period 46.7 per cent of the boys gained 
in I.Q., the median gain being 8.27, while only 32.8 per 
cent of the girls gained, the median gain being 5.36, 
On the other hand, 51.1 per cent of the boys lost, the 
median loss being 8.25, while 62.5 per cent of the girls 
lost, the median loss being 11.54. The girls showed about 
14 per cent fewer gains and 11 per cent more losses than 
the boys. 

In the Holliagworth-Kaunitz study referred to earlier 
in this chapter, the authors, in their summaries, mention 
the fact that the girls regressed somewhat more fre- 
quently than the boys. This regression was not fully 
accounted for by the known sex difference between 
medians on Army Alpha. 

It would appear, then, although psychologists can offer 
no adequate explanation for it, that boys are more likely 

1 LINCOLN, E. A., A Study of Changes in the Intelligence Quotients 
of Superior Children, J. Educ. Res., Vol. 29, pp. 272-275, 1935. 

158 



CONSTANCY OF MENTAL DEVELOPMENT 

than girls to retain in later years a high I.Q. earlier 
evidenced. 

The oldest of the Terman group of gifted children were 
tested with the Thorndike College Entrance Examina- 
tion. The scores earned were then compared with those 
made by a group of students of typical college ability 
entering Stanford University in 1921-1922. This compari- 
son shows that only about 11 per cent of the scores 
earned by typical Stanford men exceeded the average 
earned by the gifted boys, and only 16 per cent of the 
scores earned by Stanford women exceeded the mean 
earned by the gifted girls. 

Witty, in his genetic study of 50 gifted children 
previously referred to, reports a marked constancy in 
intellectual status. At the time the study was begun in 
1924r-1925 the children ranged in I.Q. from 140 to 183 
with a mean of 153. At the time of the third study, in 
1935, the majority of the group were in college. Witty 1 
says that analyses of the scores made on group intel- 
ligence tests at that time show that 

all the group would fall among the upper 5 per cent of college stu- 
dents. These results confirm the observation that those who will be 
highly gifted in college and in high school can be identified with con- 
siderable accuracy by intelligence tests administered when they are 
still children in the elementary school. 

SUMMARY 

The conclusions of the above studies are in agreement 
that, in general, those individuals found to be intellec- 
tually gifted during the grade school period will be found 
in or near the top 1 per cent of the total population with 
respect to intelligence when they reach adolescence or 
adulthood; that there are some exceptions does not dis- 

1 WITTY, P., "Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National Society for the 
Study of Education," Part II, pp. 404-405, 1940. 

159 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

prove the rule. These exceptions may be in part the 
result of certain flaws in the measuring instrument or, 
what is more likely, of the intrusion of such factors as 
lack of effort or emotional blockings at the time of taking 
the test. It is also possible, as Terman points out, that 
in a few persons there exists an inherent change-of-rate 
factor which causes intellectual growth to proceed 
spasmodically rather than constantly. That this happens 
in physical growth has long been known. However, it is 
important to keep in mind that such a condition is the 
exception, not the rule, and that gifted children, like 
normal children, tend to develop steadily, maintaining 
their relative position on the intellectual scale. 



160 



CHAPTER VIII 
DETERMINERS OF EMINENCE 

1. Is there any relationship between college grades and later busi- 
ness success? 

2. What influence do such intrinsic factors as "drive" and "single- 
ness of purpose" have when achieving eminence? 

3. Are such extrinsic factors as " manner of death " and "period of 
activity" important in determining a man's place in posterity? 

Intellect is by no means tlie sole determiner of success 
or eminence; it is but one of the foundation stones upon 
which achievement is built. So interdependent are the 
stones in this foundation that it is hazardous even to say 
that intellect is the most important ; however, it is certain 
that without superior mental capacity, any considerable 
success is impossible. 

INTELLECT VEESTJS ACHIEVEMENT 

It is important to keep in mind this difference between 
intellect and achievement; a person frequently has the 
first without the second, but rarely the second without 
the first in high degree. This point of view is not anti- 
thetical to the law of constancy discussed in the preceding 
chapter. If an individual known to be intellectually gifted 
in childhood becomes a complete failure in adult years, 
the cause cannot be found in an appreciably lowered 
intellectual level, but lies rather in the absence of other 
determiners of success. To point to a Sidas is not to prove 
that all precocious children come to nought but merely to 
indicate what every student of genius knows, that many 
intellectually gifted children never fulfill their promise. A 
man may have great material wealth and squander it. 

161 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Possession never guarantees wise usage. If he has no 
wealth, however, he has nothing either to squander or to 
use wisely. All that can be said of intellectually gifted 
children is that they have the wealth, the potentialities 
for great achievement. Only time and circumstance will 
show how their wealth will be used. 

The disparity between intellectual capacity and actual 
achievement appears again and again from earliest in- 
fancy to adult years. For instance, the very bright child 
of a year and a half may, because of an intense interest 
in play or because of laziness, still have a vocabulary 
of only two or three words. Such a case would be most 
unusual but is possible and occasionally occurs. The fact 
that the child has such a small vocabulary is in itself 
not absolute proof that he is not gifted. On the other 
hand, if he does have a vocabulary far in excess of that of 
average children of the same age, then it is certain that he 
is gifted. A similar example can be taken from the field of 
reading. A child may be intellectually gifted and yet 
not have learned to read at the age of five or six. His 
inability to read is not in itself proof that he is incapable 
of doing so. On the other hand, if he reads fluently 
at the age of five, it is certain that he is mentally superior. 
A gifted adolescent may do poor work in high school, but 
this is not in itself sufficient proof that he is dull. His 
failure to achieve may be the result of a number of 
other causes. 

A book could be filled with instances of very bright 
children who have at different times in their school 
careers done very poor work. In many cases these 
children achieved on a high level after leaving the 
educational world behind them. Then there are other 
instances where the bright child did extremely well in 
school but failed in later life. For example, there is the 
story of two high school boys, one of whom had an LQ. 

162 



DETERMINED OF EMINENCE 

of 135 and the other an I.Q. of 160. In high school the boy 
with the lower I.Q. consistently excelled his brighter 
classmate. After being graduated from high school, they 
went on to college. The boy with the I.Q. of 135 was 
graduated at the head of his class, while the boy with the 
I.Q. of 160 barely passed his courses. The two then went 
into the same law school. Suddenly the brighter boy, 
though indolent by nature, realized that here was work in 
which he could be really interested; here was information 
that was going to be valuable to him in his profession. He 
settled down to work for the first time in his life, and 
was graduated from law school second in his class. The less 
intelligent boy, though working as hard as ever and con- 
tinuing to capitalize on a magnificent personality, found 
that he could not keep pace with his old high school friend, 
once this friend had decided to make full use of his greater 
mental capacity. Today each is a successful lawyer. 

Although there are many exceptions to the rule, then, 
there is in general a marked relationship between intelli- 
gence and school achievement. For elementary school 
children the coefficient of correlation is approximately 
.60; for high school children, .50 ; and for college students, 
.45. Any teacher knows that, by being familiar with 
the intellectual capacity of his pupils, he can prophesy 
with considerable accuracy where they will fall on their 
achievement tests. If he knows his students personally, 
his prophesies will be even more accurate, for he will have 
taken into consideration some of the factors other than 
intelligence which determine success in any field. 

COLLEGE GRADES AND BUSINESS SUCCESS 

Although the layman is quite willing to admit that 
there is a relationship between abstract intelligence and 
school achievement, he finds it much more difficult to 
accept the equally well established fact that there is 

163 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

a marked positive relationship between intelligence and 
later success, or between school achievement and later 
success. For instance, there is the case of the high school 
principal who was very proud of the fact that he had 
been graduated from college at a position well toward the 
bottom of Ms class. He maintained stoutly that to be 
number one or number two man in a graduating class was 
to court certain failure. It is interesting to note that this 
principal never went far in the educational system and is 
now in a small position in the hardware business, while 
the valedictorian of his college class became a well-known 
surgeon, 

Some years ago Donald S. Bridgman 1 published a 
report on tlie relationship between college grades and 
success attained in the American Telephone and Tele- 
graph Company by more than 4,000 college graduates 
who were employed by that company. Instead of work- 
ing with the usual letter grades Bridgman classified his 
subjects according to whether or not they were in the 
first tenth of their classes, or the first third, or the middle 
third, or the lowest third. He found that of the 3,806 
college graduates reported on in his studies, 14 per cent 
had come from the first tenth of their classes, 40 per cent 
from the first and middle thirds, and only about 20 per 
cent from the last third. Bridgman found a marked 
relationship to exist between scholarship position and 
salary earned from the Bell Telephone Company fifteen 
years later. 

He says: 

Fifteen years after graduation the median of the first tenth men 
is 20 per cent above that for the whole group, 25 years after gradua- 
tion it is 40 per cent and 30 years after graduation it is nearly 60 per 
cent. The median of the first third rises steadily but rather slowly 

1 BBIDOMAN, D. S., Success in College and Business, The Personnel 
Journal, Vol. 9, No. 1, June, 1930. 

164 



DETERMINERS OF EMINENCE 

and only approaches 20 per cent above the entire group median at 
25 years after graduation, and is only slightly above it at 30 years 
after. The median of the middle third is somewhat below the median 
of the whole group and that of the last third falls rather steadily 
until at 30 years after graduation it is only 80 per cent of that of the 
whole group. 

Continuing his study, Bridgman took a single group 
for whom he had complete data concerning scholarship, 
extracurricular achievement, degree of college earnings, 
and so on. This group numbered 1,310, all of whom had 
been four or more years out of college. He then studied 
the interrelation of these several factors, finding, as 
would be expected, that they were positive. In other 
words factors such as high scholarship, campus leader- 
ship, and early graduation tended to go together. Of 
these 1,310 men, 185 were in the first tenth in scholarship. 
Of these, 53 per cent were found to be in the first third in 
salary and only 18 per cent in the last third in salary. 
Forty-five per cent of the men in the first third in scholar- 
ship were in the first third in salary and 27 per cent in the 
last third, while those who were in the last third in 
scholarship were found to be, in nearly 50 per cent of the 
cases, in the last third in salary. It is important to note, 
however, that 22 per cent of these low-ranking students 
were in the first-third salary group. As always, the fact of 
overlapping must be recognized. 

In his concluding remarks, Bridgman says: 

Good scholarship, campus achievement, early graduation, in that 
order are significant indices of success in the Bell system . Not 
all the members of the groups which were highly selected by com- 
binations of these several factors do succeed. It is very evident that 
other elements are highly important. What all of these elements are, 
we have no way of knowing. They certainly include a man's ability 
to adjust himself to the environment of his home and of his business 
as distinct from his ability to adjust himself to the environment of 
college. 

165 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Thus, as Bridgman points out, there are a number of 
factors other than college achievement which affect 
success, just as there are a number of factors other than 
intellectual capacity which determine eminence. These 
latter may be roughly classified into two main groups: 
intrinsic and extrinsic. This division should be considered 
in the light of the realization that the individual is being 
constantly affected by his environment and that the 
environment, in turn, is different for every individual. 

INTRINSIC FACTORS 

The intrinsic factors are those which come primarily 
from within. Among the most important ones are: 

1. Ambition 

2. Drive 

3. Health 

4. Physical size and appearance 

5. Race 

6. Fluency in speaking or writing 

7. Singleness of purpose 

8. Ability to get along with people 

9. Character 

Ambition 

The desire to excel is usually found in a man who at- 
tains eminence. Marat once wrote that from earliest 
childhood he had been " devoured by the love of glory. 77 
Saint-Simon developed very early in life an intense 
desire to distinguish himself- O'Connell, Irish patriot, 
from the age of seven, felt that he would be a great man 
and on one occasion was said to have remarked, "I'll 
make a stir in the world yet." Thomas Chatterton, even 
before he was five, insisted on preeminence, ruling his 
playmates autocratically. 

The desire to excel can, of course, be carried to such an 
extreme that the individual having it becomes anathema 

166 



DETERMINERS OF EMINENCE 

to his associates. However, when it is equaled or exceeded 
by intellectual power and is tempered by kindness and 
understanding, it becomes a tremendous driving force. 
Few individuals have greatness thrust upon them. In 
most cases it is attained only after great obstacles have 
been surmounted. 

Drive 

Second in importance only to intelligence among the 
factors which make for great success is what is commonly 
called drive. An individual with drive experiences a 
compelling force which actuates him to prodigious labors. 
It is in this respect that drive differs from ambition; 
for to be ambitious is to wish for or to desire without 
necessarily pushing on to accomplish that desire. Drive 
is so characteristic of genius that it has given rise to 
the popular belief that a man can reach any goal if he 
will work hard enough. Certainly high achievement 
rarely comes to one who has not toiled more than his 
fellows. To be sure, if his intellect does not match his 
energy then Ms labors may avail him nothing, but if he 
has been endowed with superior mentality and then 
makes that mentality work to the highest degree possible, 
success will almost certainly be his reward. 

Frequently, very persistent individuals whose intelli- 
gence is not of the highest achieve more than those who 
excel them in mentality. Everyone knows of examples of 
two young men, seemingly equally equipped, starting 
out together in the same business or profession. After a 
few years one has achieved striking success while the 
other is still where he was at the beginning. It may 
be that the one who made progress was less brilliant than 
his friend, but through sheer endurance did what the 
other man, more dilatory, more complacent, failed to ac- 
complish. To be specific, there is the instance of two 

167 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



young university instructors, one brilliant but volatile, 
the other less gifted intellectually but willing to work 
fourteen or fifteen hours a day. The first of these two 
never progressed beyond the rank of instructor, while the 
second at the end of eight years was a full professor in a 
leading American university. The latter could not have 
achieved this by hard work alone; he had to have ade- 
quate mental equipment, but, almost as important, he 
had persistence as well. 

Cox, in investigating the relative importance of drive, 
makes a number of interesting comparisons between 
certain subgroups of the geniuses whom she studied. In 
one instance she made the following four classifications: 
the ten most eminent geniuses, the ten least eminent 
geniuses, the five with the highest I.Q., and the six with 
the lowest I.Q. 

TABLE XXVII. FOUB TYPICAL GROUPS OF YOUTHFUL GENIUSES* 



Last ten 


First ten 


Lowest LQ.'s 


Highest I.Q.'s 


Coleridge 


Napoleon 


Hogarth 


Goethe 


Murat 


Voltaire 


Cromwell 


Leibnitz 


Mazzini 


Baeon 


Cobden 


Pitt (the younger) 


Chatterton 


Goethe 


Murillo 


Schelling 


Danton 


Luther 


Murat 


Voltaire 


Chalmers 


Burke 


Ney 




Haydn 


Newton 






Bunsen 


Milton 






Lamennais 


Pitt 






Cobden 


Washington 







* Adapted from Cox, C. M., " Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. II, p. 181, Stanford 
University Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1926. 

Concerning the comparative persistence of these four 
groups, Cox 1 says : 

Whereas young geniuses who become eminent men are charac- 
terized by the possession to a very high degree of two general factors 

1 Cox, C. M,, "Genetic Studies of Genius," pp. 186-187, Stanford 
University Press, Stanford University, Calif., 

168 



DETERMINERS OF EMINENCE 

of personality, persistence of motive and general intelligence, the 
First Ten (the youths who become the most eminent of all) and the 
Highest IQ's (the youths who earliest gave indication of superior 
endowment) are, with respect to the possession of one of the general 
factors of personality, intelligence, significantly above the average of 
a group of typical geniuses. With respect to the other factor, per- 
sistence of motive, the Highest IQ's rate approximately at the same 
point as the Eminent Men while the First Ten rate significantly 
higher. It appears further that the Last Ten rate considerably lower 
than the First Ten in intellectual traits, but approximate the ratings 
of the latter in persistence traits; while the Lowest IQ's rate below 
the other subgroups on intellectual traits and also on persistence of 
motive. The Highest IQ's exceed the First Ten in desire to excel, but 
they are exceeded by them in perseverance in spite of obstacles and 
in tendency not to abandon tasks from mere changeability. 

The appearance within the group of "most eminent" men of indi- 
viduals who, according to the records, possessed in childhood, 
intelligence somewhat below the highest order, is explained by this 
conclusion : that high but not the highest intelligence, combined with 
the greatest degree of persistence, will achieve greater eminence than 
the highest degree of intelligence with somewhat less persistence. 

The sources of drive are varied, differing not only 
among individuals but also within individuals. Drive may 
appear, as Witty 1 points out, as a product of intense 
energy, of a desire to achieve, or of psychic and 
somatic infirmities. The intense energy is inherited with 
the individual's physical mechanism. It may be a direct 
result of glandular action, or it may be, as Crile suggests, 
a result of so simple a physical fact as the possession of an 
oversized coeliac ganglion. Whatever the cause, it is cer- 
tainly a known fact that many men are capable of an 
amazing amount of work. Edison at eighty-two was labor- 
ing sixteen hours a day. Theodore Roosevelt preached 
and practiced the doctrine of the strenuous life. Such 
dynamic force when combined with high intellect cannot 
fail "to make a stir." 

i WITTY, P. A., Exploitation of the Child of High Intelligence Quotient, 
Educ. Method, VoL 15, pp. 29&-304, March, 1936. 

169 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Impelling motivations to achievement frequently come 
from certain psychological deficiencies; personal frustra- 
tions may provide needed drive. A man may prefer hard 
work to leisure time in which to think about his unhappi- 
ness. The possible causes of such personal frustrations or 
unhappiness are of course multitudinous : the loss of one 
to whom he was devoted, a feeling of inferiority because 
of social position, need for money, an unhappy marriage, 
a desire to excel someone disliked, an inner conflict over 
some earlier failure, an urge to attain power to com- 
pensate for a feeling of inadequacy. In fact, a very good 
case could be made for the supposition that high achieve- 
ment is dependent primarily upon psychic infirmities. 
The well-integrated person is complacent. Perhaps the 
world needs fewer well-integrated people and so less 
complacency. Certainly, as Witty points out in the previ- 
ously mentioned article, there is considerable doubt as to 
the desirability of trying to make intellectually gifted 
children conform to the comfortable standards of their 
less brilliant fellows. It may well be that they should be en- 
couraged in developing an extreme form of individualism. 

Physical Defects and Health 

Closely allied with the point of view expressed in the 
preceding paragraph is the theory that physical defects 
constitute an important source of drive. This is the funda- 
mental philosophy of the psychoanalyst Adler. According 
to his reasoning, the Napoleonic era was a direct out- 
growth of the diminutive stature of the great French 
general, and the Kaiser's urge to power, which resulted 
in the World War, was engendered by Ms withered arm. 
It might likewise be argued that Franklin Roosevelt owes 
his high place to the fact that he suffered from infantile 
paralysis early in his political career. How much does a 
Steinmetz owe to his broken back, or a Milton to his 

170 



DETERMINERS OF EMINENCE 

blindness? Because of their dramatic quality, the impor- 
tance of such physical irregularities as sources of drive Is 
usually overemphasized. However, it should be remem- 
bered that an iniBrmity can be used as a stepping stone to 
success. 

Physical defects and poor health, however, are handi- 
caps which, in the great majority of cases, decrease rather 
than increase the chances for achieving eminence. It is 
obvious that an individual who possesses the physical 
energy which springs from good health has an advantage 
over his ailing contemporary. Other things being equal, 
the man of forty who has a heart which is functioning 
perfectly is much more likely to press on toward great 
successes than is the man who suffers from coronary 
thrombosis. Likewise, the man whose nervous system 
does not betray him in a crisis has an advantage over a 
rival whose nerves become so tense when difficulties 
arise that clear thinking is impossible. It is true that 
some men capitalize on deafness, but a great many more 
will find that it is a heavy weight to drag up the high 
road to success. A blind senator or a blind university 
professor is a dramatic figure worthy of the admiration 
which is accorded him; the same man, however, might 
have gone much farther if he had retained full use of his 
eyes. Surely no man would pray for poor health or for any 
other physical handicap in the belief that it was essential 
to success. For the most part men who have achieved 
eminence owe their achievements, in part, at least, to a 
magnificent physical equipment which made it possible 
for them to work long hours at top speed. 

Physical Size and Appearance 

The size of a person exerts considerable influence upon 
the opinions that others have of him and upon their 
attitude toward him. If he is extremely large, he excites 

171 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

others to ridicule or occasionally to pity. If lie is ex- 
tremely small, he excites others to pity and occasionally 
ridicule, with a dash of condescension. If he is only 
slightly below average in height and general physical 
equipment, the three attitudes just mentioned are 
modified accordingly. The most desirable status with 
respect to physical size is to be somewhat superior to the 
average but not too superior. The situation here is similar 
to that presented in connection with intellect. Admiration 
is likely to go to the one who deviates in a positive 
direction so long as he does not deviate too far. 

Physical size and appearance is of considerable impor- 
tance in leadership. It is not by chance that the average 
height of the world's leaders is definitely greater than that 
for the average. It is without question a determinant of 
success. For instance, there is the case of the college board 
of trustees which was considering two candidates for the 
position of president. The trustees eventually selected the 
man whom they said " looked like a president" even 
though the one whom they turned down had a better 
standing in the world of education. In business, the man 
who is large and well built is likely to carry more weight 
with his associates than another of equal ability who is 
small and quick in his movements. In politics a Jim 
Farley holds a considerable advantage over an opponent 
who is physically insignificant. Since stature is a de- 
terminer of success, it is fortunate that there is a cor- 
relation between stature and intellect, for stature then 
can serve as an additional aid to those who have the 
mental ability to exercise intelligent leadership. 

Race 

Racial and national antagonisms, whether instinctive 
or learned, play an important part not only in inter- 
national affairs but also in the affairs of individuals. 

172 



DETERMINERS OF EMINENCE 

ie inheritance of a black skin carries with it a social 
adicap that only a most fortunate and unusual com- 
lation of abilities and circumstances can surmount, 
-en though a Negro possesses an intellect of a high 
ler, he finds nearly all of the avenues to eminence 
her wholly or partly closed to him. 
Conditions are similar although somewhat less acute 
th such other groups as the Indian and the Jew. A 
xiing psychologist, interested in gifted children, was 
ee heard to say that he never attempted to obtain 
ancial assistance for needy Jewish children, regardless 
the degree of their mental superiority, because such 
investment was not a good risk. This may appear to 
a cold-blooded statement, yet it conforms to the facts, 
ajiy graduate schools refuse to permit the registration 
Jewish young people and many firms, both business 
d professional, close their doors against them. Preju- 
je against Jews is especially unfortunate in view of 
sir high level of intelligence. Perhaps their superior 
jntality has been one of the predisposing causes of the 
nost universal hatred of their race which has existed 
- centuries. 

However, this is not the place to discuss either the 
ises or the possible eradication of racial prejudices. 
Le fact to be faced is that such prejudices do exist, 
d that the color of a man's skin, the shape of his nose, 
d the texture of his hair profoundly influence his 
ssibilities for achievement. To say that intellect, or 
aracter, or drive, or even a combination of all three of 
3se will insure success is an oversimplification. 

Fluency in Speaking or Writing 

The ability to speak or to write well has an important 
aring on the attainment of eminence. The pages of 
story are filled with examples of men and women who 

173 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

found it easier to attain greatness because they were 
effectively articulate. The pamphlets of Voltaire and the 
innumerable speeches of Hitler have played a consider- 
able part not only in the careers of these men but also 
in the story of civilization. Unfortunately it is not 
only the most brilliant individual who is able to express 
himself fluently and well. Every teacher has had experi- 
ence with the dull child who is a good talker: It happens 
less frequently that such a child can express himself with 
equal glibness in writing. 

The two greatest American orators were Webster and 
Bryan. Webster possessed a mind which matched his 
oratorical ability, and he stands in history as one of the 

TABLE XXVIII. DISTRIBUTION OF 282 EMIJSTENT MEN ACCORDING TO 
THE FIELD IN WHICH EMINENCE WAS (PBIMABILY) ACHIEVED* 



I 

Field 


Eminent men 


Fre- 
quency 


Per 

cent 


1. Writers (PND)t 
Poets, 31 1 


52 
43 

43 

39 
27 
23 
22 
13 
11 
9 


18.0 
15.0 

15.0 

14.0 
10.0 
8.0 
8.0 
5.0 
4.0 
3.0 


Novelists, Dramatists, 21) 
2 Statesmen and politicians. 


3. Writers (EHCS)$ 
Essayists, Critics, Scholars, 31? 
Historians, 12) '" 
4 Scientists 


5 Soldiers 


6 Religious leaders 


7. Philosophers 


8 Artists 


9 Musicians 


10 Revolutionary statesmen , 


Total 


282 


100.0 





*Cox, "Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. II, p. 35. 

f Writers (PND) include the authors whose fame rests chiefly on imaginative works, 
i.e., the poets, novelists, and dramatists. 

t Writers (EHCS) include the essayists, historians, critics, and scholars. 

174 



DETERMINERS OF EMINENCE 

world's great. Bryan's mental equipment was consider- 
ably inferior to Ms ability as an orator. Nevertheless, 
through the sheer beauty and effectiveness of his speak- 
ing, he attained considerable success. 

The attainment of recognized eminence is partly 
dependent on the extent to which a man's contribution is 
available in written form. Of two equally able persons, he 
who publishes more will have the better chance of being 
remembered, for he has bequeathed to posterity concrete 
evidence of his ability. It is for this reason, that Cox 
found that writers constituted the largest single occupa- 
tional group among the geniuses whom she studied (see 
Table XX VIII). 

Likewise, the child who can write well has a distinct 
advantage over the one who cannot. This advantage is 
especially marked in schools where the essay form of 
examination is used. As a matter of fact, the importance 
of being articulate manifests itself from earliest infancy 
to old age. It is one of the most important determiners 
of success. 

Singleness of Purpose 

Occasionally an individual whose intelligence is not of 
the first order attains eminence because of his complete 
devotion to one idea or to a small group of closely related 
ideas. In fact, even a moron, if he applies all his mental 
energy toward the attainment of a single limited end, is 
able at times to perform astounding feats. Such a person 
is commonly called an idiot-savant. If a man with a 
superior mind applied himself with the same concentra- 
tion to a specific piece of work, he would achieve miracu- 
lous results. For instance, there was the case of the 
medical student whose intelligence was less than that of 
his classmates, though considerably above average. This 
student, upon graduating, decided to become a bone 

175 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

specialist; he limited his field with great care and devoted 
himself to it exclusively. As a result his name is now fa- 
mous in the annals of medicine. 

It is commonly believed that most geniuses possess 
this singleness of purpose or what is more commonly 
called a one-track mind. The possession of a one-track 
mmd has been, in certain instances, one of the important 
determiners of success. It is not, however, a necessary 
or even a customary corollary of eminence. Geniuses are 
broad rather than narrow in their interests. Perhaps, in 
popular thought the one-track mind has been confused 
with a marked ability to concentrate, this last being 
nearly always a characteristic of eminent men. A Henry 
Ford manifests the same enthusiastic interest in the col- 
lection of antiques that he does in the production of 
automobiles or a Paderewski in the problems of govern- 
ment as in playing a piano. Nevertheless, in spite of the 
fact that those who attain high places in the world are 
usually well-rounded individuals, it is important to keep 
in mind that, in certain exceptional cases, singleness of 
purpose, concentration on one small area, has resulted in 
great achievement. 

Social Intelligence 

Social sense, or the ability to get along with people, is 
perhaps a greater asset to the less important leaders than 
it is to those who achieve eminence. However, even to the 
outstanding genius it is no handicap to be well liked. 
Among eminent men there have been notable instances, 
as in the case of both Theodore and Franklin Roosevelt, 
in which a pleasing personality proved to be a great asset. 
While the ability to get along with people is important in 
politics and in business, it plays a minor role in the 
achievements of a scientist. In fact, because of the popu- 
lar picture of him as an unsocial, eccentric individual, 

176 



DETERMINERS OF EMINENCE 

sociability actually might be a handicap, for people like 
their heroes to fit a preconceived picture. Although many 
geniuses have unquestionably been lacking in sound 
social sense, in terms of central tendency they are a 
congenial group. 

Character 

It is not by chance that geniuses are usually men of 
good character; a high moral outlook exerts an important 
influence on the achievement of eminence. Pasteur is an 
excellent example of a great man who combined intellect 
and character in high degree. 

The character of Louis Pasteur and his philosophy of life all bear 
witness to the moral and social traits of a superior boy. Slowly they 
evolved in a pattern of life and work which clearly sets forth unusual 
devotion to an ideal the service to humanity. The books that 
Pasteur read, the letters he wrote, and the friends he chose all bear 
witness to the effective moral traits of his youth. His love of home and 
parents, interest in the welfare of his sisters, the meticulous care in 
following the advice of his worthy father are indices to the tractable- 
ness of his intellect and bear out our theory of reasonable submission 
to authority, which is usually seen in the gifted child. 

Pasteur, early in his life, habitually worshiped great men, and this 
reverence and devotion for the illustrious was a dominant element in 
his character. He believed that the spirit of jealousy and suspicion 
was alien to manhood at its best and should not be allowed to influ- 
ence judgment on a man's work. He was devoted to his friends, just 
and generous to his rivals, and patient under trying contradictions 
and vexatious oppositions. Sir William Osier has said of him: "In 
his growth the man kept pace with the scientist. Heart and head held 
even sway in his life. This story will reveal the true side of a great 
person hi whom filial piety, brotherly solicitude, generosity and self- 
sacrifice are combined with a rare devotion to country. His success 
was due to the untiring assiduity with which he worked, never spar- 
ing himself, never thinking of himself, but only of what might be 
accomplished to benefit humanity/* 1 

1 BENTLBY, J. E., "Superior Children," pp. 236-237, W. W. Norton <fe 
Company, Inc., New York, 1937. 

177 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

EXTRINSIC FACTORS 

There are a number of extrinsic factors those outside 
the individual and largely beyond his control which 
determine to a considerable extent the degree of fame he 
attains. Eminence, it should be remembered, is by no 
means an inevitable result of great mental ability or even 
of great mental ability plus such intrinsic factors as those 
previously discussed. Frequently environmental or chance 
conditions are the final determiners of whether a man 
shall be an outstanding success or remain unknown. The 
following list contains a few of the specific conditions 
which often play an important role: 

1. Economic status of family 

2. Size of home town 

3. Marriage 

4. Age and manner of death 

5. Strength of competition 

6. Nature of interests 

7. Period of activity 

Economic Status of Family 

The socioeconomic status of the parents of a gifted 
boy frequently exercises considerable influence upon his 
achievement as an adult. If the parents are living in 
poverty, they will be unable to send their boy to college 
or, it may be, even to high school. A great many boys 
with potentialities for greatness have found their way 
blocked by lack of funds. 

Examples without number could be given by way of 
illustration. For instance there was the case of a grade 
school child whose I.Q. was 165. She possessed not only a 
brilliant mind but also high social intelligence, being very 
popular among her classmates. Investigation of her home 
background revealed that her parents were living in 
poverty; they planned to take the girl out of school as 

178 



DETERMINERS OF EMINENCE 

soon as the law would permit and put her to work in her 
father's little store. This was done before she finished high 
school. Later she married a young man far beneath her 
intellectually and is now living in a poverty as hopeless 
as that of her parents. Her opportunity to succeed was 
cut off by lack of money. 

Then there is the story of the boy with an LQ. well 
within the top 1 per cent who was a descendant of a 
family which possessed a marked strain of intelligence. 
His immediate family, however, was so poor that it 
required state assistance. The boy had the opportunity 
to go through high school and finished as the top-ranking 
student in a class of over 300. Further education, how- 
ever, was impossible, and at the present time he is 
employed as a bellhop. 

A third case is that of a boy of a poor farm family who 
possessed not only the intelligence but also sufficient drive 
to cause T-nm to work his way through school. Since there 
was no high school in his home town, he went alone to a 
city some 50 miles away and found work which made it 
possible for him to continue his education. Toward the 
end of his third year, he reached the end of his financial 
resources. There was no possibility of help from his 
relatives. At this juncture a businessman in the city 
loaned "him $10. This seems a small amount, but it was 
the turning point between success and failure for this 
ambitious young genius. He continued with his high 
school course, was graduated at the head of his class, 
went on to college, working his way through, and there 
also was graduated at the head of his class. He then went 
on to medical school and later became an outstanding 
physician. 

Occasionally a great deal of money in a family will 
have the same deleterious effect upon a gifted child as 
does a lack of money. If the child feels that his economic 

179 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

future is taken care of by the family bank account, he is 
deprived of one of the sources of drive economic 
necessity. The most desirable economic condition is the 
possession by the parents of a moderate income; this 
makes it possible for the gifted child to continue his 
education as far as he likes and at the same time gives 
him the realization that he has a living to make. 

Size of Home Town 

A minor determiner of eminence is the size of town in 
which a man resides. Obviously, the smaller the town, the 
more limited the number of opportunities. Also a young 
man of genius, situated in a small village, would, in cer- 
tain occupations, find the scope of his activities seriously 
curtailed. A lawyer, no matter how brilliant his intellec- 
tual attainments, would find it extremely difficult to gain 
national attention if he were practicing in a town of 3,000 
people. A scientist on the faculty of a small college will not 
command the attention that goes to a man of similar 
achievements in a large university. A political leader in 
Presque Isle, Maine, has more difficulty in emerging as a 
national figure than a political leaderjLa New York. 
Occasionally, of course, a great man, on the building-a- 
better-mouse-trap theory, forces the world to come to his 
doorstep. Especially successful in this respect were 
William Allen White of Emporia, Kan., and the Mayo 
brothers of Rochester, Minn. 

Marriage 

Marriage involves a number of factors which may exert 
a critical influence on a man's possibilities for attaining 
eminence. Age at time of marriage, number of children, 
and, of course, the personality and influence of the wife 
any one of these may spell the difference between great- 
ness and mediocrity or even failure. For instance, there 

180 



DETERMINERS OF EMINENCE 

was the brilliant young theological student who married 
at the age of eighteen. Before he was twenty he was the 
father of a child and before he was thirty, the father of 
eight children. Instead of fulfilling the promise of his 
youth, this man, carrying family burdens which were 
much too heavy for his young shoulders, finally lost his 
grip and committed suicide. In this case marriage at 
twenty-eight instead of eighteen might well have meant 
the difference between recognized greatness and igno- 
minious self-annihilation. 

Marriage exercises an even more important effect upon 
a woman's possibilities for achieving any considerable 
success. A few women are able to follow a career while 
at the same time carrying family responsibilities. How- 
ever, in most instances one or the other usually the 
career has to be dropped. Even single women find it 
almost impossible to win an eminent place in a man- 
controlled world where the belief is still strong that 
woman's proper place is in the home* 

Age and Manner of Death 

A man may have all the potentialities for greatness, 
yet if an automobile, for instance, runs over him and 
kills him, he has no further chance for realizing on those 
potentialities. No one knows how many men of excep- 
tional talent have been cut off by death before they had 
time to achieve greatness. Who would have heard of 
Napoleon, if he had lost his life in battle at the age of 
twenty-one? Or what would the name of "George 
Washington" mean, if he had been killed with Braddock 
in the French and Indian War? Occasionally fate grants a 
man too much time, as in the case of Kipling, and the 
bright sunlight of his fame fades into the dim background 
of a long twilight. Sometimes, as in the case of von 
Hindenburg, a long life grants fresh opportunities for 

181 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

refurbishing a fame that has been tarnished. A great 
general, he suffered from being a leader of a losing cause, 
but survived the eclipse and became a much-beloved 
president of the German Republic. If he could have died 
at the height of his popularity, his position in the eyes of 
posterity would have been greater than it now is ; but he 
lived on into his dotage, and, while still president of 
Germany, became a cat's paw for the ambitious Hitler, 
into whose hands he placed the destinies of his country. 
The mere fact of chronological age certainly played a 
tremendously important part in the success of von Hin- 
denburg, in the history of Germany, and in the fate of 
the world. 

Fame by no means rests upon a carefully considered 
opinion by one's contemporaries or even by posterity. 
Frequently it is affected out of all proportion by some 
unusual characteristic of the individual or by some amaz- 
ing event. If a man attacks traditional values with bril- 
liant and extreme audacity, as did Jesus, Martin Luther, 
Galileo, and Charles Darwin, he impresses himself upon 
the popular mind with vividness and finality. It matters 
little if people hate him; perhaps they remember their 
hates longer than their loves. 

If a man's manner of death is spectacular, his chance 
for being remembered by posterity is considerably 
enhanced, Lincoln's position in the hearts of Americans 
was made secure by the fact that his assassination came 
so soon after the close of the Civil War. Had Wilson been 
shot before the making of the Treaty of Versailles, he 
would hold a greater place in popular affection than he 
now does. Napoleon, always interested in what succeed- 
ing generations would think of him, was fully aware of 
the importance of the way in which he spent his last days. 
He took every opportunity to dramatize himself as "an 
eagle chained to a rock." Occasionally even premature 

182 



DETERMINERS OF EMINENCE 

death is a determiner of eminence, providing, of course, 
that death does not come before the man has already 
achieved a relatively high place. The pity that is aroused 
by the death of a young man who has already accom- 
plished much and gives promise of accomplishing a great 
deal more is occasionally so poignant that it exerts a 
considerable effect upon the estimation of the man. Even 
suicide may increase a person's importance. However, the 
best way for a great man to die, if he wishes to insure his 
position in history, is either to have himself assassinated 
or to die, as Will Rogers did, in a spectacular accident. 

Competition 

The degree of competition which a man meets during 
the period of his greatest activity exerts some influence 
upon opinions concerning him. Ideally those in opposi- 
tion should be inferior, but not too inferior. To compete 
with another of equal ability is to divide the spoils. 

The best examples can be drawn from the realm of 
sport, where it is easy to make direct comparisons. For 
instance, in the case of Tilden and Johnston in tennis, 
Tilden's reputation as a great tennis player was helped 
considerably by the fact that he was able to defeat again 
and again a man who was almost but not quite so good as 
he. On the other hand, Johnston had no opportunity to 
gain first position because his career coincided with that 
of the greatest tennis player that the world has ever 
known. If Johnston had been at the peak of his powers a 
few years before or a few years alter, his reputation as a 
great athlete would have been definitely greater. A 
similar situation existed for a number of years in the case 
of Helen Wills Moody and Helen Jacobs. 

In the world of politics and science, a number of 
examples could be offered of the influence of competition 
upon a man's reputation. It was unfortunate for both 

183 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Disraeli and Gladstone that their careers were con- 
temporary. It is unfortunate, apparently, for Mussolini 
that his career and Hitler's coincide. Occasionally two 
scientists working in the same field make a discovery at 
approximately the same time. The public, finding it 
difficult to distribute honors, usually bestows the credit 
upon one and forgets the other. 

Nature of Interest 

It is easier for a man to attain world-wide recognition in 
some fields than in others. For instance, in literature one's 
abilities can be displayed before the world in a manner 
which all can understand. On the other hand, it is diffi- 
cult for a mathematician, equally gifted, to catch popular 
attention. Einstein appears to be an exception, although 
even in his case recognition by the masses has not been 
given so much to his great intellect as to his physical 
appearance and personal idiosyncrasies, which match so 
perfectly the picture of a genius which the average man 
enjoys and insists on preserving. 

A man must be a tremendous success in business before 
he is considered a genius, and even then most individuals 
would hesitate to apply the term. Henry Ford is as 
eminent in his field as Robert Frost is in his, yet he would 
not popularly be considered a genius. Two men of equal 
ability, each interested in politics, but one in the academic 
side and the other the practical side, would experience 
widely divergent opportunities for fame. 

Occasionally a man engages in a piece of work the 
results of which antagonize the masses. It is true, as was 
pointed out earlier, that if his conclusions antagonize 
them sufficiently, as in the case of Darwin, his fame is 
made even more secure. However, if his results are of the 
kind that can soon be brushed off and forgotten, his 

184 



DETERMINERS OF EMINENCE 

chances for being remembered are considerably less than 
those of one who is, like Edison, a benefactor of society. 

Period of Activity 

The time or period in which a potentially great man 
works is another determiner of success. It makes no 
difference how great a general's capacity for military 
leadership may be, he cannot become a Napoleon or a 
Wellington or a Lee unless there is a war. Obviously, of 
two men running for the position of president of the 
United States, the one who is elected is presented with a 
much greater opportunity for achievement than the one 
who is defeated. 

Periods of upheaval and war give certain groups of 
individuals opportunities for outstanding contributions. 
Periods of quiet and peace give other groups similar 
opportunities. In times of upheaval and war, generals and 
politicians flourish. In times of quiet and peace, scientists 
and the industrialists prosper. Artists and writers may be 
productive in any period, although they are more likely 
to create their greatest works during periods of calm. 

It is clear then, that intellectual capacity alone does 
not guarantee high achievement. Every characteristic of 
the individual, every environmental influence, plays its 
role. Occasionally one of these factors, seemingly un- 
important, proves to be the decisive determiner of success 
or failure. 



185 



CHAPTER IX 
SPECIAL GIFTS 

1. Does creative genius in the arts necessarily imply a high degree 
of abstract intelligence? 

2. Is the possession of a special gift likely to be recognized at an 
early age? 

3. Is there any relationship between sex and either creative or 
critical ability in the arts? 

Although great mental capacity is the most important 
single prerequisite to genius, there does occasionally 
appear an individual who possesses such an unusual 
special gift that he attains eminence in spite of a rela- 
tively low intelligence. This is not to say that it is possi- 
ble for a moron to become a great musical composer or for 
a person of less-than-average intelligence to become a 
great creative artist. It means rather that, in certain 
exceptional cases, unevenness of abilities may be so 
marked that a person succeeds on a very high level be- 
cause of one considerable talent even though in certain 
other abilities, such as intelligence, he may approach 
mediocrity. This is especially likely to be true of musicians 
and artists, for it is a known fact that the relationship 
between abstract intelligence and artistic ability or 
between abstract intelligence and musical ability is only 
slightly positive. Once in a great while it is true even in 
the field of mathematics, as in the case of the lightning 
calculator, and in language, as in the case of the child 
whose reading age is far in excess of his mental age. 

In the present chapter there will follow a discussion of 
five special gifts: music, drawing, arithmetical computa- 

186 



SPECIAL GIFTS 

tion, mechanics, language. The first two of these, music 
and drawing, appear much more frequently as specialized 
abilities than do the remaining three. In fact, it is doubt- 
ful if language should be included at all, for the correla- 
tion between language ability and intelligence is very 
high. However, there are a few cases of children and 
adults who show a facility either in reading or in the use 
of words for the writing of poetry or prose which so far 
exceeds what would be expected from their known mental 
age that it may be said to constitute a special gift. 

Music 

An interesting fact concerning the gift for music is that 
it manifests itself very early in life. This may be because 
ability in music depends very little upon experience. In 
nearly every instance, great musicians have been recog- 
nized early as musically gifted children. Mozart is an 
excellent example. By the time he was three years old, he 
had already demonstrated his unusual talent. Cox 1 in a 
biographical study, reports as follows on his early history: 

Wlien Mozart was 7 his first published work appeared four 
sonatas for piano and violin, one of which showed especially remark- 
able taste. Between the ages of 7 and 15 he composed works for piano- 
forte and violin, pianoforte concertos, masses and church music, 
18 symphonies, 2 operettas, and at the age of 14, an opera. When he 
was 12 his first operetta was performed. At 13 he received an appoint- 
ment as grand ducal concert master (without salary), and in his first 
year of office he composed 20 numbers. At the age of 14 his first 
Italian opera was presented with great success. During these years 
his musical genius was so prolific that his fingers ached with the work 
of committing his ideas to paper, At 16 he received his first salary. 
Mozart was a brilliant executive artist as well as a great creative 
genius. His first musical tour was undertaken at the age of 6, when he 
visited Munich and met there with a very favorable reception. By 
his 8th year two more successful tours had been made, the first to 

1 Cox, C. M., "Genetic Studies of Genius," VoL H, pp. 595-694, 
Stanford University Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1926. 

187 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Vienna and cities en route, the second through Germany. From bis 
8th to his 15th year Mozart visited Paris, London, The Hague, 
Amsterdam, and Vienna (for a second time). Two tours to Italy were 
carried out in his 16th and 17th years. Mozart's letters show the 
characteristics of an average pre-college student. 

When he was 3 or 4, Mozart began to invent musical ideas; im- 
pressed by seeing his sister play, he seated himself at the clavier and 
picked out thirds, to his great delight. Even at that early age he 
could retain musical passages that he had heard. On one occasion 
before his 5th birthday, he learned at half-past-nine at night and 
in half an hour a minuet and a trio, pieces requiring independence 
of the two hands and some musical comprehension. Before he was 6 
Mozart had begun to insist upon the presence of real connoisseurs 
whenever he played. His first concerto the little musician wrote down 
himself; the written composition was a daub of ink, but there was 
real order hi its music. The child exceeded expectations on his first 
musical tours, and he was received everywhere as a prodigy; he was 
an enthusiastic critic of the playing of others. He played charmingly, 
whether with one finger, with the keyboard covered, or in the usual 
way. At 7 his extraordinary sense of absolute pitch was discovered, 
as well as remarkable skill with the violin and the organ, which he 
had never been taught. His repertoire included the naming of any 
note played at a distance, improvising in any key on the harpsichord 
or organ, and transposing to any key. He could supply the accom- 
paniment to a singer without knowing the air in advance, or to a 
dictated melody with the use of the clavier. In his 10th year he was 
called by Tschudi, the instrument maker, "the most extraordinary 
performer in the world," and in the same year he was ;: investigated" 
in London by a lawyer who "reported proofs of the boy's decided 
inventive power." Compositions from the following year reveal 
remarkable ability in elaborating a theme. Locked up for a week by 
an incredulous archbishop, and required to prove his ability to write 
an oratorio without outside aid, Mozart (aged 11) achieved a brilliant 
triumph, a mature musical composition although written with 
blotted notes in a childish hand. 

It is interesting to note that Mozart's I.Q. is estimated 
to have been approximately 150. Biographical studies of 
other great musicians, such as Bach and Beethoven, 
indicate a like high intellectual level. It would appear that 
while eminence in musical composition, at least, is largely 

188 



SPECIAL GIFTS 

conditioned by intellectual capacity, a man may become 
a great performer and yet be of average mentality. Wlien 
large, unselected groups are measured for intellectual 
capacity and for musical ability, the coefficients of cor- 
relation hover about zero, usually being slightly above. 
The relationship is so low that it is impossible to predict 
musical ability from the results of general intelligence 
tests or intelligence from the results of musical ability 
tests. 

Scientific workers in the field of music have been handi- 
capped in their investigations by a lack of adequate 
measuring instruments. There is no objective test in the 
field of aesthetics which is equal in validity to the better 
tests of intelligence. The best of the music tests is the 
one devised by Seashore, who, recognizing that musical 
talent is a complex of many specific abilities, reduced 
musical sensitivity to what he considered to be the six 
basic elements: pitch, intensity, time, consonance, 
rhythm, and tonal memory. He then constructed a test of 
each of these elements and had it recorded on a phono- 
graph record. 

The results of the Seashore tests are largely negative ; 
that is, those who make low scores have very little chance 
of becoming successful musicians, but those making 
high scores are by no means sure of success. As in the case 
of abstract intelligence, other abilities than the musical 
gift alone are needed to ensure achievement. 

It is a mistake to assume that musical talent is a 
natural gift, the mere existence of which will inevitably 
bring achievement of a high order. Environmental factors 
are fully as potent in the case of the talented child as in 
the case of the intellectually gifted child. A man will not 
become either a great performer or a great composer 
without a thorough musical education or without long 
hours of arduous labor. There exists here the familiar 

189 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

situation: a child may be precocious in music and yet 
fail to achieve on a Mgh level in adult life because of cer- 
tain lacks in himself or in his environment ; but an indi- 
vidual will not achieve on a high level in music without 
having inherited the potentialities. 

DRAWING 

Ability in representative drawing or, for that matter, in 
all forms of drawing or art usually shows itself at an early 
age. It is interesting to note, however, that manifesta- 
tions of artistic ability do not appear so soon as do 
manifestations of musical ability. The early appearance 
of artistic talent in the great majority of celebrated artists 
indicates that it has been inherited. However, as in the 
case of music, other factors influence the extent to which 
it will be developed. 

A study of biographies of famous artists reveals many 
interesting facts concerning the relationship between 
later achievement and early tendencies. Rembrandt, 
Raphael, Murillo, and Rubens were successfully express- 
ing themselves through the artistic medium while they 
were still adolescents. Of Michelangelo, Cox 1 says : 

At school Michelangelo devoted most of his time to drawing, a 
pursuit not included in the curriculum and which his father tried to 
discourage as he did not wish a painter in the family. Michelangelo 
early sought the acquaintance of artists, and took every opportunity 
to converse with them. From his fourteenth to his sixteenth year, 
during the period of Lorenzo de' Medici's patronage, the youth 
devoted most of his time to the study and practice of drawing, 

painting, and sculpture At the age of thirteen Michelangelo 

was so proficient in drawing that he received a salary although he 
was then in the first year of his apprenticeship. His passion for his 
art was so strong that every available space became a sketch surface. 
It is written that he drew so well at this time that he caused wonder 
to all that saw it, and envy to the less generous. 

., pp. 543-544. 

190 



SPECIAL GIFTS 

Representative drawing, like music, shows little re- 
lationship to intelligence when large ? unselected groups 
are tested. However, as was pointed out with respect 
to music, it does not follow from this that high achieve- 
ment will result from the possession of artistic talent 
alone; it must be supplemented by abstract intelligence. 
Michelangelo was estimated to have an I.Q. of 160. It 
is doubtful if there has ever been a celebrated artist of 
average or below-average intelligence. 

Relationship between Intelligence and Art Appreciation 

In art, as in music, investigators have been handicapped 
by a lack of satisfactory measuring instruments. There 
does not exist at the present time an adequate test of 
creative ability. It has proved to be somewhat easier 
to evaluate the ability to appreciate or to judge. The 
two best tests constructed for this purpose are the Meier- 
Seashore Art Judgment Test and the McAdory Art Test. 
Even these, however, are If mi ted in their validity. Carroll 
and Eurich, using the two instruments together with cer- 
tain intelligence tests, studied the relationship between 
abstract intelligence and art appreciation. Their results 
coincide with the conclusions of earlier studies by Ayer 
and others. 

Carroll and Eurich 1 report a coefficient of correlation 
between Miller Analogies Intelligence Test and the Meier- 
Seashore Art Judgment Test of .26 .02 for 674 college 
students and a coefficient of correlation between the 
Analogies Test and the McAdory Art Test of .10 .05 
for 203 college students. Interested in the question of a 
possible difference in the ability to appreciate art be- 
tween mentally superior and mentally inferior children, 
they matched 43 intellectually gtfted children, with 43 

1 CABBOLL, H. A., and A. C. EUBICH, Abstract Intelligence and Art 
Appreciation, J. Educ. Psyck&l., Vol. 23, pp. 21^-220, 1932. 

191 



GEXIUS IN THE MAKING 



borderline children and tested them with the McAdory 
Art Test. The distribution of the scores obtained appears 
in Fig. 5. 

It will be seen from Fig. 5 that, although there was a 
difference of more than 60 points between the mean 
scores of the two groups, there was considerable over- 
lapping; actually 63 per cent of the borderline children 
(those with LQ.'s of approximately 70) exceeded the 
lowest score made by the gifted. Only 9 per cent of these 



15 



IO 

o 

c 

<l> 
3 

cr 

s 



lf\ 



65 80 <?5 



110 



170 185 200 



125 140 \J 
Score 

FIG. 5. Distribution of McAdory Art Test scores of 43 very dull and of 43 
very superior junior high-school children. 1 

very dull children exceeded the mean of the intellectually 
gifted group, and no dull child equaled the best among the 
gifted children. On the other hand, no bright child fell 
below the average for the dull group. It would appear 
from this study that abstract intelligence at the extremes 
affects art judgment ability. 

The effect of intelligence upon the accuracy of draw- 
ings, with respect to the correct inclusion of detail, is 
considerable. This would be expected since the more 

1 EURICH, A. C., and H. A. CA.EBOLL. "Educational Psychology," p. 
184, D. C. Heath & Company, Boston, 1935^ 

192 



SPECIAL GIFTS 

intelligent the child, the more he sees. As Goodenough 1 
says: 

The drawings of bright children are not always or necessarily more 
artistic than those of backward children, but they excel in such 
matters as the number of items shown, the correctness with which 
the parts have been assembled, the relative proportions of the dif- 
ferent parts, and in the control of eye and hand movements as shown 
by the regularity of the lines and the smoothness of their joinings. 

A comparable situation is found in music, where a 
brilliant child, though lacking in any real gift for music, 
may experience considerable pleasure and success in 
learning facts about music. He may even become a 
moderately good performer as an outgrowth of a purely 
intellectual interest. 

Sex Factor 

The role played by sex in art achievement is not as yet 
very well understood. Two seemingly contradictory 
observations have been made: first, that nearly all the 
great artists have been men; second, that girls as a group 
axe definitely superior to boys in artistic ability. It may 
well be that lack of opportunity has kept many artist- 
ically gifted women from achieving any marked success. 
Even today, when women are more free than ever before 
to have careers of their own, family responsibilities and 
social pressure block their progress. It may be that lack 
of opportunity is not wholly responsible, but that artist- 
ically gifted girls, like intellectually gifted girls, are much 
more likely than boys to retrogress toward mediocrity as 
they approach maturity; or it may be that the ability to 
create is more closely linked with the male sex hormones 
and the ability to appreciate more closely linked with the 
female sex hormones. It is interesting to note that boys 

1 GooDBNOtrGH, F. L., "Developmental Psychology," pp. 335-336, 
D. Appleton-Century Company, Inc., New York, 1934. 

193 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

who are gifted in art and music are much more likely to 
possess feminine traits than are intellectually gifted boys 
or unselected boys a fact that blurs the picture still 
more. It is possible, though not likely, that an element of 
homosexuality profoundly influences all achievements in 
aesthetics. 

LIGHTNING CALCULATORS 

Mathematical ability is made up of many subdivisions, 
some of which are more closely related to general intel- 
ligence than others. For example, the ability to handle 
symbols in a subject such as calculus requires more 
abstract intelligence than the ability to make arithmetical 
computations. However, even the relationship between 
performance in arithmetical computations and intel- 
ligence is very high; the fact that it is not perfect permits 
the occasional appearance of a lightning calculator of 
less-than-average intelligence. In such instances mathe- 
matical ability constitutes a special gift. 

As Binet pointed out a long time ago, the spectacular 
achievements of lightning calculators result from highly 
developed habits of computation, with multiplication 
being used as the basic operation. By a peculiar quirk of 
circumstance, the lightning calculator of low intelligence 
is interested almost solely in performing arithmetical 
feats. He concentrates every ounce of mental energy that 
he has upon mastering the mechanics of computation and 
of remembering combinations. Even a dull mind, by 
limiting his field of interest, can occasionally achieve 
astounding results. It would be a mistake, however, to 
assume that all lightning calculators are men of inferior 
mentality, for the gift has been possessed by such eminent 
individuals as Whately, Gauss, and Ampere. The fact 
that the gift appears at a very early age in Whately, 
Gauss, and Ampere at three indicates that* the ability 

194 



SPECIAL GIFTS 

is not wholly acquired but is based upon some specialized 
inherited mechanism. The dull child inheriting the talent 
(and it should be emphasized that the talent rarely ap- 
pears) grasps it as his one means of impressing others with 
his mental ability. The bright child, finding himself 
possessed of the gift, delights in using it as a tool for 
broader achievement. 

Leta Hollingworth 1 reports as follows on two lightning 
calculators who gave every indication of being of low 
intelligence: 

Jedediah Buxton (b. 1702) seems to be the first such calculator on 
record in modern accounts. He lived at Elmton, England, and 
"labored hard with a spade to support a family, but seems not to 
have shown even usual intelligence in regard to ordinary matters of 
life." In 1754, when he was taken to London, to be examined before 
the Royal Society, he went to see King Richard HI performed. 
"During the dance he fixed his attention upon the number of steps; 
he attended to Mr. Garriek only to count the words he uttered. At 
the conclusion of the play, they asked him how he liked it. ... He 
replied that such and such an actor went in and out so many times, 
and spoke so many words; another so many. . . . He returned to his 
village, and died poor and ignored." It is further stated that he could 
give an itemized account of all the free beer he had had from the age 
of twelve years. 

Another person who appears to have had a very special gift for 
calculation is Tom Fuller, "The Virginia Calculator" (b. 1710). 
He came from Africa as a slave when about fourteen years old. He is 
first recorded as a calculator at the age of seventy, when he mentally 
multiplied two numbers of nine figures each, and performed other 
remarkable arithmetical feats. He was totally illiterate, and no 
evidence of high general intelligence is given in the various anecdotes 
about his case, 

MECHANICAL ABIUTY 

Early in the testing movement, Thorndike subdivided 
intelligence into three classifications: abstract, social, 

1 HoLLiNGWORrH, L. S., " Gifted Children," pp. 211-212, Hie Mac- 
Tm'TTan Company, New York, 192S. 

195 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

and mechanical, the last named being defined as the 
ability to understand and to manipulate mechanical 
objects. During the years since the time that this classifi- 
cation was made, a number of investigations into the 
possible relationships existing among the three have been 
conducted, with the result that it has been established 
that those relationships are positive. The coefficient of 
correlation between mechanical intelligence and abstract 
intelligence is approximately .30. A correlation of that 
size indicates that, although there will be considerable 
overlapping in test scores when an unselected group is 
measured with a mechanical aptitude test and with an 
intelligence test, in terms of groups intellectually gifted 
children will do better and the intellectually inferior 
children less well in a situation requiring mechanical 
ability. 

To achieve greatness in the mechanical world, a man 
needs a high degree of abstract intelligence. Even the 
so-called chance invention requires that the inventor be 
able to observe and to make deductions. An apple falls on 
the head of Newton, and he gives to the world, as a result 
of the experience, a statement of the law of gravitation. 
The same apple falling on the head of a moron would 
suggest to him only an apple. 

In considering mechanical ability, it is important to 
make a distinction between understanding and manipula- 
tive skill, the former requiring much more intelligence 
than the latter. A dull boy might be able to take a clock 
apart and then reassemble it without having any appre- 
ciation of the principles involved. A bright child might be 
able to duplicate the performance, but in addition to that 
would be very much interested in the whys and where- 
fores. In fact, very often the bright child is so interested 
in the principles involved in the creation and functioning 
of a mechanical object that he cares but little about 

196. 



SPECIAL GIFTS 

watching it go. His mind is busy while his hands are idle. 
In the case of the dull child, the hands are busy while the 
mind is idle. 

Observations concerning the relative mechanical abili- 
ties of bright and dull children are often faulty because 
of failure to check both mental age and chronological age. 
Since there is such a low correlation between abstract 
intelligence and physical equipment, it follows that the 
child who is mentally accelerated is going to find that his 
manipulative ability has not kept pace with his mental 
development. On the other hand, the dull boy of ten 
with a mental age of six can use his hands relatively well. 
To be specific, if , in a fourth grade, there were a bright 
child of seven and a dull child of ten, it would appear to 
the casual observer that mechanical ability is negatively 
related to intelligence, for the dull child would presum- 
ably be able to handle tools better than his younger 
classmate. 

Education itself is a conditioning factor in the relation- 
ship between mechanical and abstract intelligence. 
Bright children are more likely to be interested in and to 
be taught subjects that are strictly verbal, while dull 
children, lacking both interest in and capacity for such 
abstract subjects, are encouraged to take up work in 
manual training or in similar fields, where special atten- 
tion will be given to developing dexterity. 

SPECIAL LANGUAGE ABILITIES 

The relationship between abstract intelligence and 
language ability, including both reading and writing, is 
extremely high. However, as in the case of the correlation 
between abstract intelligence and arithmetical ability, it 
is not perfect and so permits certain kxeeptionp to occur. 
Consequently, an occasional child is found to have a 
specific language ability or disability markedly-superior 

197 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

or inferior to his intellectual level. A superiority in this 
respect occurs much more rarely than an inferiority. 

Reading 

Examples of unusual reading ability among very dull 
or feeble-minded children are as scarce as the presence 
of lightning calculators among the same group. On the 
other hand, a fairly large number of intellectually superior 
children are deficient in reading or writing. 

Terman reports the case of a girl who was able to read 
simple material at the age of two. Since usually a child 
needs to reach a mental age of six before it is possible for 
him to read, this precocity would indicate an I.Q. of 
about 300. When she was tested, however, it was found 
that her I.Q. was only 150. Terman points out that 
the child had been given special instruction by her 
father, but it seems unlikely that this, in itself, would be 
sufficient to account for the unusual accomplishment. It 
is probable that she inherited a special talent for reading 
and that the personal instruction was an effective means 
of developing it. As the girl grew older she continued to 
excel in reading and in literature, a further indication that 
she had been endowed with a special ability. 

Goodenough tells a remarkable story of an imbecile 
boy with an I.Q. of only 25 who did not enter school until 
he was more than ten years old. Unlike most feeble- 
minded children, the boy talked a great deal and was 
intensely interested in stories. Concerning his reading, 
Goodenough 1 says: 

Because he was so greatly retarded it was not thought worth while 
at first to try to teach >nm to read. But one day, when the teacher 
was giving a word drill by means of "flash cards" to some of the 
more advanced children, she happened to glance at the corner where 
Arthur sat. There on the edge of his chair, all agog with excitement, 

1 GOODENOUGH, op. cit., pp. 417-418. 

198 



SPECIAL GIFTS 

hands on knees, body swaying backward and forward as each new 
card was shown, he was pronouncing the words along with the other 
children. The amazed teacher tried him out by himself and found 
that he could recognize and name quite a number of words without 
hesitation. From then on he was given a short lesson in reading each 
day and in three years time, that is, by the age of thirteen, he had 
learned to read easy stories well enough to give him much pleasure. 
To be sure his reading ability never exceeded that which an ordinary 
child accomplishes by the end of the second grade. If compared with 
the performance of the average boy of thirteen his reading would 
seem poor enough. But in comparison with what he could do along 
other lines, it was extraordinary. 

It is difficult to explain such a case of special reading 
ability. It may be the result of inheritance of the special 
gift or of the concentration upon this activity to the 
exclusion of nearly all others; perhaps it is a combination 
of both. It is again important to keep in mind that there 
is in reading a distinct difference between mechanics and 
understanding. The dull child who performs excellently 
at reading aloud will not be able to interpret what he has 
read on a level beyond his mental age. The situation is 
similar to that of the child who has memorized a poem 
which he can glibly recite without having the faintest 
idea what a line of it means. There was a certain dull 
high school boy who had come up through the grades 
bearing with him the reputation of being a wonderful 
reader. This boy could take a book like Ivanhoe and read 
aloud from it without mispronunciations and with ade- 
quate attention to punctuation marks, yet he was unable 
to pass his high school English courses or his other 
courses, either, for that matter because he could not 
understand what he had read. 

Writing Poetry 

Another form of specialized language ability is the 
writing of poetry. Poetic ability usually appears at an 

199 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

early age, although not at so early an age as music 
ability. Tennyson began writing poetry when he was 
eight. Macaulay, at seven, began a long poem, "Olans 
the Great/ 3 with the following lines: 

Day set on Cambria's hills supreme, 
And, Menai, on thy silver stream. 
The star of day had reached the West. 
Now in the main it sunk to rest. 
Shone great Eleindyn's castle tall: 
Shone every battery, every hall : 
Shone all fair Mona 7 s verdant plain; 
But chiefly shone the foaming main. 

Bryant, at the age of ten, was sufficiently gifted to 
write the following, called " After a Total Eclipse of the 
Sun 77 : 

How awfully sublime and grand to see 
The lamp of day wrapped in obscurity. 
To see the sun remove behind the moon, 
And nightly darkness shroud the day at noon; 
The birds no longer feel his genial ray, 
But cease to sing and sit upon the spray. 
A solemn gloom and stillness spreads around, 
Reigns in the air and broods o'er all the ground. 
Once-smiling Nature wears another face, 
The blooming meadow loses half its grace. 
All things are silent save the chilling breeze, 
That in low whispers rustles through the trees. 
The stars break forth and stud the azure sky, 
And larger planets meet the wondering eye. 

Now busy man leaves off his toil to gaze, 
And some are struck with horror and amaze, 
Others of noble feelings more refined 
Serenely view it with a tranquil mind. 
See God's bright image strikingly portrayed 
In each appearance which his power had made. 
(Fixed in their hearts cool Meditation sate, 
With upraised eye and thoughtful look sedate.) 
200 



SPECIAL GIFTS 

Now bursts the Sun from silence and from night, 
Though few his beams, they shed a welcome light; 
And Nature's choir, enlivened by his rays, 
Harmonious warble their Creator's praise. 
The shades of darkness feel his potent ray, 
Mine eye pursues them as they flee away; 
So from the greyhound flies the tim'rous hare, 
Swift as the dart divides the yielding air. 

Terman, in his work with gifted children, found a 
number who possessed remarkable poetic talent. The 
following was written by a girl only five years of age : 

THE PLACE I'D LIKE TO BE 

The place I'd like to be 
Is where the spreading tree 

Spreads its shade 

And is made 

By the gentle hand of God 
In the rich, black mud. 
And the brooklet ripples down 
To the other end of town, 
And the roses are in bloom, 
And the violets give perfume, 
And the blue grass waves like bushes, 
And in the brook, here, wave rushes, 
But instead a dingy town! 

A girl of seven \rrote this : 

MY PEAYER 

Oh, Master of fire! Oh, Lord of air, 
Oh, God of waters, hear my prayer! 
Oh, Lord of ground and of stirring trees, 
Oh, God of man and of pleasant breeze, 
Dear Father, let me happy be 
As happy as a growing tree! 

Obviously it takes a high degree of abstract intelli- 
gence to write such lines as appear in the preceding 
selections. The relationship between poetic ability and 

201 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

abstract intelligence is much higher than that found be- 
tween either musical ability or drawing ability and 
abstract intelligence. Terman, in an investigation of the 
writing of children, selected fourteen who showed the 
most merit. Their intelligence ratings appear in Table 
XXIX. 

TABLE XXIX. I.Q.'s OP 14 CALIFORNIA GIFTED CHILDREN 
TENTATIVELY SELECTED AS OF OUTSTANDING LITERARY ABILITY* 



Name (fictitious) 


I.Q. 


Corrected I.Q. 


Age when 
tested 




188 




7-10M 


Edith 


138 


148 


13- 


Ethel 


172 




8-10 




144 


152 


12- 3 




153 




10- 4 




134 


145 


13-10 




166 




8- 4 


Pauline 


143 


155 


12-10 


Ralph 


152 




6- 5 


Ruth 


141 


157 


13- 2 




170 


186 


11- 7 


Vivian 


140 




8- 6 


Walter . . - 


144 


148 


11- 7 


Wanda 


172 




9- 











TEBMAN, L. M-, "Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. Ill, p. 365, Stanford University 
Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1930. 

For purposes of comparison, Terman selected twenty- 
eight writers who had achieved eminence in English or 
American literature. An estimated I.Q. for each of these, 
based on the Cox biographical survey, appears in 
Table XXX. 

In 1925, Mearns published a number of examples of 
pupils' writings in a book called "Creative Youth." It 
was rather generally assumed at the time that any group 
of children, under wise encouragement and direction, 
could do equally well. The fact that Mearns was working 

202 



SPECIAL GIFTS 

TABLE XXX. ENGLISH AND AMERICAN MEN OF LETTERS FOB WHOM 
JUVENILIA WERE AVAILABLE* 

Estimated LQ. 
(Based on Data 

to Age 
Name Seventeen) 

Browning, Robert 165 

Bryant, William C 170 

Burns, Robert. 140 

Byron, Lord 160 

Coleridge, Samuel T 180 

Cowper, William 150 

Dryden, John 160 

Emerson, Ralph W 155 

Franklin, Benjamin , 160 

Freneau, Philip M 160 

Hamilton, Alexander 150 

Hawthorne, Nathaniel 150 

Holmes, Oliver W 155 

Jefferson, Thomas 160 

Johnson, Samuel 165 

Keats, John 165 

Longfellow, Henry W , 160 

Lowell, James R 165 

Macaulay, Thomas B 185 

Milton, John 155 

Poe, Edgar A 165 

Pope, Alexander 165 

SheUey, Percy B 165 

Tennyson, Alfred 160 

Thackeray, William M 145 

Thoreau, Henry D 155 

Whittier, John G 155 

Wordsworth, William 160 

* TEBMAN, "Genetic Studies of Genius, VoL IIL" p. 366. 

with a group of mentally superior children was given 
scant attention. A later report on the mental level of the 
children mentioned in "Creative Youth" showed that 
the I.Q/s ranged from 100 to 157. Since these children 
were at an age when the Stanford-Binet LQ. should be 
corrected, it is probable that the true quotients were ten 
or fifteen points higher. In general it can be said that 
intellectually gifted children can be taught to write 

203 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

excellent poetry and prose. However, this does not mean 
that in later years they will become eminent authors, for 
creative literary achievement rests upon a number of 
gifts, of which intellect and the ability to understand and 
to handle words are but two. A gifted poet will usually 
show promise not only in an interest in words but in 
such other contributing factors to creative ability as a 
vivid imagination, a sensitivity to beauty, and a reten- 
tive memory for detail. 

Tests 

There is at present no adequate objective test of 
creative ability in writing. It is likely that there never 
will be, for creative ability by its very nature is unique 
and cannot be evaluated according to set standards. It is 
even difficult to measure the ability to appreciate or to 
judge, although some progress has been made in this field. 
Psychologists, in attempting to measure critical ability 
in the field of literature, have to keep in mind that stand- 
ards of excellence change. The best that they can do is to 
compare the judgments of the child examined with a 
consensus of the opinions of a group of contemporary 
critics of established reputation. 

Abbott and Trabue have constructed a moderately 
adequate test of the ability to judge poetry. The test is 
low in reliability and its validity is open to question. 
Carroll has constructed a test of prose appreciation, the 
reliability of which is .70. This test depends for its valid- 
ity upon three criteria: source, expert opinion, the com- 
parative performances of groups on different educational 
levels. It is interesting to note that examinations with 
the Carroll Prose Appreciation Test show a decided sex 
difference in the ability to appreciate prose literature. 1 

1 CARROLL, H. A., Influence of the Sex Factor upon Appreciation of 
Literature, School and Society, Vol. 37; pp. 468-472, 1933. 

204 



SPECIAL GIFTS 

Similar differences are found witli the Abbott-Trabue 
Poetry Test. As was pointed out earlier, there appear to 
be like differences in art and music. Terman emphasizes 
the fact that the seven most gifted child writers in his 
group were all girls, yet opposed to that observation is 
the fact that practically all the eminent adult writers 
are men. This merely indicates once more the important 
part which factors other than inherited ability play in 
the attainment of eminence. 

STJMMABY 

Of the five gifts discussed in this chapter music, 
drawing, arithmetical calculation, mechanics, language 
only the first two, music and drawing, stand out dis- 
tinctly as special talents. The relationship between either 
of these and abstract intelligence is only slightly above 
zero. In each of the remaining three, the relationship is 
definitely higher. However, to attain eminence, even in 
music and drawing, it would appear that a high degree of 
abstract intelligence is needed. Most assuredly this is 
true in authorship. 

Musical talent appears at a very early age, frequently 
before three. Ability in drawing, depending somewhat 
more on experience, does not manifest itself until the 
child is older, usually just before or during early adoles- 
cence. Unusual ability in the use of language, especially in 
the writing of poetry, also appears at an early age, gen- 
erally before adolescence. Measuring instruments in 
these special fields are not nearly so satisfactory as 
those used for objectively evaluating intelligence. Noth- 
ing of a satisfactory nature has been done to test creative 
ability in music, art, or writing, but critical ability in 
each of these fields can now be measured with some degree 
of accuracy. 

205 



CHAPTER X 
EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

1. Is differentiation of education justifiable? 

2. What are the advantages and disadvantages of acceleration? 

3. What is the opinion of gifted children themselves concerning 
acceleration? 

Educators disagree concerning the advisability of 
making special provision for intellectually gifted children. 
Antithetical opinions exist among them concerning any 
method which may be proposed acceleration, enrich- 
ment, or grouping. There is the sharpest division, how- 
ever, concerning the plan of classification according to 
ability, especially when this involves putting gifted 
children in a group by themselves. 

ISSUES INVOLVED 

There are a number of questions which are raised 
whenever school adjustments for gifted children are 
proposed. The issues in the following list bear more di- 
rectly on the problem of segregation than on that of 
enrichment, although, in some respects, they apply to the 
latter method as well: 

1. Is differentiation democratic? 

2. Does differentiation violate the principle of respect for person- 
ality? 

3. Does differentiation make the bright child egotistical? 

4. Does differentiation prepare the child for actual life situations? 

5. Do dull children need those who are gifted as examples? 

6. Do gifted children need help? 

Democracy in Education 

Paradoxical as it may seem, there has developed in 
America, side by side with an enthusiasm for success 

206 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

stories, a cult of mediocrity. This is everywhere apparent 
in American social and political structure and, unfor- 
tunately, in the educational system. As De Tocqueville 
wrote in the last century, "A middling standard is fixed 
in America for human knowledge. All approach as near to 
it as they can; some as they rise, others as they descend." 
American schools are adjusted to meet the needs of the 
great average group. Direct effort, usually unsuccessful, 
has been made to raise the dull child to the standard 
considered desirable for the typical student, and indirect 
effort, in the guise of ignoring his needs, has been made to 
pull down the gifted child toward the middle. 

To canonize mediocrity at the expense of superiority 
is not sound democratic education. The writer remembers 
that, on taking up his duties in his first high school teach- 
ing position, he was instructed by the principal to devote 
his efforts to the training of average students, the princi- 
pal asserting that these were the ones who would at some 
future date bear the burdens of leadership. He made the 
further statement, heard often in educational circles, that 
the bright children needed no special attention because 
they could take care of themselves. 

A true democratic conception of education penalizes 
no group, whether bright or dull, but asserts that every 
child should be given an opportunity to develop to the full 
extent of his capacity. As Strayer has said, " Every pupil 
in the ideal school system is judged by the best which he 
can do and not by the median performance of a non- 
selected group." The same point of view is expressed by 
Harold Campbell, 1 superintendent of schools in New 
York, who says : 

The school that fails to offer opportunities for the child of unusual 
gif ts is fully as neglectful as the school that offers nothing to the child 

1 COHEN, H, L., and N. G. COBTELL, "Educating Superior Students," 
p. v, American Book Company, New York, 1935. 

207 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

of limited endowment. The school must be as zealous to do for the 
genius as for the dullard. There must be special education for the 
gifted as well as for the handicapped. 

The belief is held by those who feel that differentiating 
instruction is undemocratic that the creation of oppor- 
tunity classes for the gifted, or even the creation of an 
enriched program for them, constitutes special privilege. 
Those who hold this point of view concerning democracy 
in education interpret equality of opportunity as meaning 
identity of educational offerings. A broader conception of 
democracy would admit of the existence of individual 
differences and recognize that each child must receive the 
education best suited to his abilities and needs, in order 
that in later years he can better contribute to a demo- 
cratic society the service for which he is best qualified. 
To force upon all an education planned for average chil- 
dren, regardless of individual intellectual capacity, is to 
grant special privilege to the central group and to deny to 
the bright and the dull their rights. It is as undemocratic 
to require a high school freshman with an I.Q. of 75 to 
take algebra as it is to insist that a gifted child capable 
of reading "Treasure Island" must study a second-grade 
reader merely because he happens to be seven years old 
and has been placed by the educational machine in the 
second grade. 

A truly democratic system provides for the education 
of all children. It grants no special privilege to the middle 
group even though this happens to be the most numerous. 
It is wholly impartial, recognizing the needs of the moron 
as well as of the genius, or, more significantly, of the gen- 
ius as well as of the moron. A democracy needs not only to 
draw upon its man power of average intelligence but also 
to utilize the smaller contributions of the " hewers 
of wood and drawers of water" and the potentially 
greater contributions of its men and women of high intel- 

208 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

lect. No democratic society can afford to ignore the devel- 
opment of its greatest single possession its intellectual 
resources. 

Respect for Personality 

An argument advanced by Kilpatrick and others 
against grouping is that it violates the principle of respect 
for personality, the assumption being that the dull and 
even the average are being stigmatized by a recognition 
of the existence of individual differences and a resulting 
classification. The concern appears to be, unfortunately, 
not with the benefits to be gained by the gifted through 
grouping and differentiation of instruction but rather 
with the question of how the dull and average children 
will feel about it. This is a strange attitude. Children are 
segregated for almost any other purpose without objec- 
tion. The muscle boys can be put on a football team and 
given the benefit of a highly specialized training by a 
well-paid staff of coaches. The future purveyors of 
tc swing," or whatever it is that radios will be exhaling a 
decade from now, can be organized into an orchestra 
and given special instruction. Children with dramatic 
ability can be assigned parts in plays and experience the 
pleasure of public recognition of their talents. Even 
feeble-minded children stand a fair chance of being put 
into a special class, where instruction will be adjusted to 
their limited mentality. All these classifications can be 
made without the objections being raised that someone's 
personality is being violated; but if a superintendent or 
principal organizes his gifted children into a special group 
he may expect a roar of disapproval. 

The above-mentioned groupings should not be elimi- 
nated. It is indeed difficult to visualize a football coach 
giving all the boys in his school the same Mnd of football 
training and then sending a team, selected at random, 

209 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

against an opponent. Such a procedure would be unfair 
to everyone, especially to those boys who are good foot- 
ball players. It is not undemocratic to make use of the 
best available material in an athletic contest ; neither is it 
undemocratic to provide for the best available intellects. 
There even seems to be a danger in some of the so-called 
progressive schools in America that intellectual achieve- 
ment may be outmoded. Here it is considered admirable 
to be successful in such activities as art, or calisthenics, 
or emotional control, and neither the art teacher nor the 
gynasium instructor hesitates to praise those who do well; 
but in the classrooms the child who is gifted in reading, or 
in arithmetic, or in geography soon learns that to allow 
his ability to be detected is not socially desirable. In 
these fields, praise and rewards go, rather, to the mentally 
slow pupil who, as one teacher remarked, "need the 
encouragement. J ' 

Unsound though the point of view concerning the 
stigma attached to classification may be, its existence is, 
nevertheless, a fact which must be faced by any teacher or 
school administrator who is interested in providing suit- 
able education for all children. The present attitude being 
what it is, it seems desirable to camouflage differentiation 
as much as possible. For example, it is unwise to call a 
segregated group of superior children "a class for gifted 
children." It is better to use some such phrase as "op- 
portunity class/' 

Grouping within a heterogeneous class can usually be 
disguised very easily through the use of meaningless 
names and through complete flexibility, permitting the 
transfer of children from one group to another whenever 
such transfer seems wise. For instance, a teacher may 
divide her class, according to reading ability, into " blue- 
birds " and "robins," the "bluebirds," being the poor 
readers and the "robins" the good readers. No child's 

210 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

personality appears to be violated by this classification, 
the " bluebirds " never suspecting the basis for it and even 
the " robins" being misled by their mental pictures of the 
two birds. 

Egotism 

It is generally believed that the brilliant child is 
egotistical. This belief may spring from the feeling of 
average children and adults that if they were brilliant 
they would be aware of it and make sure that others 
knew about it. A similar situation is found in the mis- 
conception that men of wealth are forever conscious of 
their possessions or that successful writers and artists 
delight in reviewing their achievements. The person who 
has not won popular acclaim cannot be expected to 
understand how those who have achieved actually feel. 
During the first flush of success the experience is sweet, 
but the bitter-sweet stage comes quickly, and this is 
frequently followed by a period that is bitter without any 
sweetness. 

Gifted children, as a rule, are too busy taking stock of 
the world about them and of looking ahead to their future 
work to have time for self-congratulation. The average or 
below-average child, on the other hand, may compensate 
for his mental weakness by affecting a superior manner. 
Experienced teachers will testify that in general it is the 
dull rather than the bright child who is overassertive. 
Concerning this point, Leta Hollingworth 1 says: 

The conceit of the gifted need in any case give little concern, 
apparently. According to the repeated testimony of teachers, they 
are rated much above average children in modesty, whatever the 
circumstances under which they have been schooled. There appears 
to be a decided tendency among the very intelligent to compare 

1 HOLLINGWOBTH, L. S., " Gifted Children," p, 302, The Macmillan 
Company, New York, 1926. 

211 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

themselves with those above them, instead of with those below them, 
in any category of relative standing. One very gifted boy of ten years, 
asked to rate himself for achievement, wrote as follows: "I have not 
done much, when you think of Darwin and Newton and all the things 
they did. 7 ' 

The more one knows, the more keenly he recognizes his 
limitations. 

The statement is frequently made that, even though 
gifted children are not naturally conceited, to place them 
in an ability group would make them so ; yet this has not 
been the case, according to those who have had personal 
experience with such groups. Goddard 1 says flatly, 

The briefest and most conclusive answer to this objection is that 
it doesn't happen. Out of some 500 children who have been in the 
Cleveland special classes from one to four years, we have yet to hear 
of one who has been made conceited. 

Stedman 2 says, 

Among the many objections voiced to the plan of segregation was 
the suggestion that the opportunity to advance regardless of the 
progress of the other members of the class might result in developing 
selfish, self-centered, egotistical children. An experience of five years 
with the class has amply demonstrated the fallacy of this argument. 

Coy, 3 in a study of seventeen gifted children who had 
been placed in a special class, reports, 

It is often said that membership in a gifted class will make children 
conceited. We have practically no evidence of conceit except in the 
case of Number 11, who had acquired it before he came to the class. 

1 GODDABD, H. H., "School Training of Gifted Children," pp. 26-27, 
World Book Company, Yonkers-on-Hudson, N. Y., 1928. 

2 STEDMAN, L. M., "Education of Gifted Children," p. 17, World Book 
Company, Yonkers-on-Hudson, N. Y., 1924. 

3 COT, G. L., "Interests, Abilities, and Achievements of Gifted Chil- 
dren," p. 176, Teachers College Contributions to Education, No. 131, 
Columbia University, New York, 1923. 

212 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

If there were a tendency for mentally superior chil- 
dren to become egotistical, a heterogeneous class would 
provide a more favorable situation for the development of 
this characteristic than would a homogeneous class. In 
an unselected group, the gifted child is constantly being 
made conscious of the fact that he is brighter than his 
classmates. Problems which are easy for Kim are difficult 
for them; he succeeds where they fail. Put this child into 
an opportunity class, where he will be competing with 
other^children who possess minds as good or perhaps bet- 
ter than his, and his ego will tend to be deflated. For an 
individual who has lost sight of relative values through 
lack of competition there is nothing better than a contest 
with someone who can defeat him. A gifted boy who grad- 
uates as valedictorian of his class in a small high school 
may be tempted to believe that he will always be at the 
top, but, when he reaches college in the fall and finds him- 
self in competition with a number of other boys and girls 
who also were valedictorians of their classes, he is likely 
to awake to the fact that his standards of comparison 
were low. 

In conclusion, then, it would appear that gifted chil- 
dren as a group are somewhat less egotistical than other 
children of the same age. It appears also that a differentia- 
tion of educational procedures to meet their special needs 
eliminates one of the causes of an inflated self-esteem, 
viz., the heterogeneous class, in which there is little or no 
competition for the brilliant child. 

Preparation for Actual Life Situations 

An issue frequently raised concerning the grouping of 
gifted children for purposes of instruction is that the 
grouping creates an unreal situation, one which tbe child 
will not find in adult life where, so the argument runs, he 
will be associating with individuals of widely differing 

213 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

intelligence. This is another example of the curious 
habit that human beings have of failing to distinguish 
theory from facts. Theoretically, there are no groupings, 
no class distinctions, in democracy. Actually, adult 
society in America is made up of innumerable tight 
little groups. So long as individuals continue to be char- 
acterized by wide differences in every trait, groupings will 
exist. Even in a town of a few hundred people, those who 
are like-minded will seek one another's company. 
Grouping, of course, is not always on the basis of intellect, 
but in general those with able minds will seek intellectual 
companionship. Those with less able minds naturally and 
rightly look for their friends among those of like interests ; 
they would be bored and restless in the presence of an 
intellectual. Therefore, to force a bright child to find his 
friends and playmates among children of a wholly differ- 
ent intellectual level is to fail completely to train him for 
the situations in which he will find himself in later adult 
life. 

Individuals not only choose friends who are intel- 
lectually and socially congenial, but they also tend to 
choose vocations in which competition will be limited 
to men of approximately equal mental capacity. As 
Elsbeth Kroeber remarks, "Candidates for teaching 
licenses would find examinations easier if subway guards 
entered into competition with college graduates." It is 
especially fortunate for the subway guards that they 
prefer not only to compete with, but also to associate 
with, other subway guards. Certainly not even the educa- 
tional philosopher who objects to all groupings would care 
to teach a truly heterogeneous adult class made up of 
college students, a sprinkling of day laborers, and, per- 
haps, an imbecile or two to make the group really repre- 
sentative; yet this same philosopher, arguing before his 
homogeneous class, maintains that in the public schools 

214 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

the gifted child should be required to compete and to 
associate with an unselected group of children because 
this is the condition which he will find in adult life. 

The facts of the case are that in adult life the doctor 
associates pretty much with doctors, the college professor 
with other college professors, the baseball player with 
other baseball players, and the farmer with other farmers. 
Perhaps this is not an ideal situation, but it is a more 
comfortable and pleasing one than could possibly exist in 
a group of individuals chosen at random and forced to 
associate intimately with one another. Such a group, as 
social and political history clearly proves, is purely 
artificial and does not long endure. 

Gifted Children as Examples to Others 

It is believed by many that a heterogeneous class of 
students is desirable because it gives to the dull and 
average child an opportunity to imitate and to be stimu- 
lated by the intellectually gifted child. It is, however, 
very doubtful if the less intelligent children admire their 
superior classmates sufficiently to imitate them. It is 
more likely that the average child will feel somewhat 
uneasy in a class with a very bright child and will com- 
pensate for that uneasiness by being critical of him; or it 
may be that he will be discouraged by the ease with which 
the bright child solves problems that are so difficult for 
him, and will ask himself, " What's the use? John always 
gets the best grades. Why should I try?" 

It is well to think not only of the possibility of the 
gifted child's stimulating those who are less intelligent, 
but also of his being stimulated by competing with other 
children who are as bright as he a situation in which he 
would find himself were he in a segregated class. Even 
assuming that average children do need to have gifted 
children in their classes to serve as examples, it is doubt- 

215 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

ful wisdom to require the mental stultification of the 
gifted as a sacrifice to possible benefits which others are 
supposed to derive from his presence in the class. 

Gifted Children Need Help 

It is often maintained in educational circles as well as 
outside them that the gifted can take care of themselves. 
This, for instance, is the point of view usually expressed 
by philanthropic foundations when asked for funds with 
which to promote studies of the nature and needs of the 
bright. As a matter of fact, as Leta Hollingworth 1 points 
out, "not only the leaders of philanthropy today, but 
political, educational, and other kinds of leaders would 
give all to the burdens of society and nothing to the 
burden-bearers." 

There are two bases for this attitude toward the educa- 
tional needs of gifted children. The first is that most 
individuals envy and resent those who possess minds more 
brilliant than their own. This feeling is common among 
teachers perhaps because they are working in a field 
where the importance of mind is disproportionately 
magnified. 

Any high school teacher should hope and expect that 
there will be one or more children in her classes who excel 
her in intellect. Theoretically, she does expect exactly 
that, but emotionally she rebels against it, not because 
she is a teacher, not because she is more sensitive than 
others, but because she is a human being. The rebellion 
is natural and involuntary, and the expression of it, 
unfortunately, is seldom curbed. Any psychologist or 
teacher with an interest in bright children could tell 

1 HOLLINGWOKTH, L. S. f What We Know About the Early Selection 
and Training of Leaders, in a bulletin "How Should a Democratic People 
Provide for the Selection and Training of Leaders of the Various Walks 
of Life," Advanced School of Education, Teachers College, Columbia 
University, New York, 1938. 

216 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

stories without number of the humiliating treatment 
accorded by instructors to those who are intellectually 
superior. For instance, there is the case of the teacher 
who, upon being told that she had in her class the child 
with the highest intelligence in the school, informed the 
child publicly that she would now learn her place; that 
she might think she was bright but would find here that 
she was no better than the others. Then there is the story 
of the child of eight whose ears were soundly boxed by 
his teacher because he was getting the better of her in an 
argument concerning the relative size and position of the 
planets. It would be only a half-truth to state that this 
represents the attitude of all teachers toward precocious 
children, for there are many conscientious and under- 
standing instructors who suppress their spontaneous 
jealousy and experience an intense pleasure in contribut- 
ing to the development of genius. 

The second basis for the belief that gifted children 
should be left to rely upon their own resources is the 
honest, considered opinion that it is better for them and 
for society if they are left alone. Witty expresses this 
point of view when he voices his concern about the pub- 
licity which is often attached to the selection and special 
training of mentally superior children. He 1 says, 

Publicity centering about geniuses may result in stultifying their 
ability and developing undesirable attitudes in these and other 
promising children an unnecessary eventuality that would be 
ruinous for the children and socially detrimental. 

In the same article, however, he recognizes the fact 
that something ought to be done for gifted children when 
he says in his concluding statement that "the present 
social order is neglecting its richest asset, the gifted and 
precocious child. 7 ' 

1 WITTY, P. A., Exploitation of the Child of High Intelligence Quotient, 
Educ. Method, Vol. 15, pp. 298-304, March, 1936. 

217 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Although the fact is recognized that in certain instances 
gifted children may be harmed by too much attention, 
this hardly seems sufficient reason for leaving them to find 
their way by trial-and-error methods. To say that 
' ' Cream will always rise to the top " or ' ' You can't keep a 
good man down" is to refuse to face the facts. There are a 
great many factors, both intrinsic and extrinsic, as was 
pointed out in Chap. VIII, that "will keep a good man 
down"; and the obvious retort to the homely simile 
concerning cream is that if the housewife keeps stirring 
the cream into the milk it is likely to be submerged. The 
striking success of men like Lincoln and Garfield, who 
achieved on a remarkably high level despite innumerable 
obstacles, obscures the loss of many potential Lincolns 
and Garfields whose abilities have been crushed by the 
weight of an unfriendly environment. 

Intellectually gifted children constitute democracy's 
greatest wealth. There seems little reason to question 
Thorndike's statement that "all competent observers of 
the world's work and workers will agree that a very 
small number of men and women of great ability and 
good will account for a very large fraction of the world's 
progress." This being true, it would seem that public 
education should feel itself responsible for meeting the 
needs of these few. Certainly guidance, educational 
adjustments, and, frequently, financial help should be 
provided for those who have so much to contribute to 
society. Even without such help a great many gifted 
children will achieve success and a few may even com- 
pletely fulfill their promise; but if society is to realize 
fully on their possibilities, educators should see to it that 
all such children are given the best possible opportunity 
to develop their potentialities. Concerning this duty 
Thorndike 1 says: 

1 THOBNDIKB, E. L., The Distribution of Education, School Review, p. 
345, May, 1932. 

O1 Q 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

Doubtless great ability will often manage to get education outside 
of schools or to get along without it, but those who can do so much 
for the world with so little are the very ones who should be given 
more. In the wars we are incessantly waging against disease, misery, 
depravity, injustice, and ugliness, we should not provide our best 
marksmen with the poorest weapons nor ask our bravest to fight 
with their naked hands. 

While education cannot make a creative genius; 
it can do much to develop one who has inherited the 
capacity for great achievement. No form of education will 
increase the number of brilliant -minds, but a lack of the 
right kind of education can effectively decrease the num- 
ber of those who make great contributions to human 
society. 

ACCELEKATION 

For over half a century acceleration has been the most 
popular method of adjusting education to the superior 
learning abilities of the gifted child. Although full 
acceleration has rarely been effected, it has been, and is, 
a common experience for gifted children to be advanced 
one grade. Occasionally this has been stretched to two, 
but seldom to as many as three. When it is remembered 
that a child with an LQ. of 150 is capable of doing high 
school work at the age of ten, and one with an LQ. of 175 
at an even earlier age, it can be seen that the skipping of 
a single grade goes only a little way toward adapting the 
difficulty of the curriculum content to the mental ability 
of the gifted child./ 

During the last part of the nineteenth century, flexible 
promotion was the only method of adaptation. In spite 
of present-day enthusiasm for opportunity classes and 
enrichment in heterogeneous classes, rapid promotion is 
still the most popular procedure. It was first introduced 
by W. T. Harris in the city of St. Louis in 1868, 

219 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

when he put into operation a plan whereby pupils 
could be promoted at short intervals ; even in as brief 
a period as five weeks. In 1886 Supt. Shearer, of Elizabeth, 
N. J., introduced ability grouping. Each group, bright, 
average, or dull, was permitted to set its own pace in 
covering subject-matter essentials. Comparable to this 
was the Cambridge Double Track Plan, which made it 
possible for the brighter children to do the eight years' 
work in six, thus permitting an acceleration of two years. 
These plans, and many others which attained popularity, 
all stressed speed. 

Two other widely known plans which emphasize the 
importance of the timesaving element are the Winnetka 
Plan, developed by Supt. C. W. Washburn of Winnetka, 
111., and the Dalton Plan, used at Dalton, Mass. In each 
of these the importance of individual instruction is 
emphasized. Each permits the child to travel at his own 
rate, requiring only that he master the essentials of the 
curriculum. These plans, and many others which are 
variations of the same principle, indicate a recognition by 
school men of the necessity of taking into account 
individual differences in learning ability. Although no 
one of them adequately satisfies the needs of the intel- 
lectually gifted child, the contribution which these 
pioneers have made is great. 

Jt is entirely possible that acceleration, either partial 
or full, is the best means of adjusting the educational 
program to the superior child. It has not been and per- 
haps never can be scientifically established that gifted 
children should keep in step educationally with others of 
the same chronological age. The problem still has to be 
approached subjectively, and the considered opinion of 
one educator is perhaps as good as that of another. 
Possibly the opinions of gifted children themselves are as 
valuable as any.{ 

220 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

Advantages 

In Chap. I it was pointed out that the youngest child 
in an unselected class is usually an intellectually gifted 
child. It is true also that the youngest child in an un- 
selected class is usually among the top-ranking students 
in that class. In other words, the customary partial 
acceleration of gifted children does not appear to handi- 
cap them with respect to scholastic achievement. This is 
true not only in the elementary but also in the high school 
and in college. Gray, in a monograph on the undergrad- 
uate careers of young college students, quotes a number of 
opinions concerning the relationship between chrono- 
logical age and academic achievement. He refers to a 
statement made by Jones that studies at Northwestern 
University show younger students to be superior in 
academic achievement; to an address by former Pres. 
Lowell of Harvard University, in which he urged that 
students be allowed to enter college at an earlier age than 
now because the younger the boy, the more likely he is to 
be a good scholar; and to Bear, who found that the six- 
teen- and seventeen-year-old freshmen at Center College 
in 1925 led all their classmates in intelligence-test scores 
and scholastic achievement. Gray 1 concludes his survey of 
opinion with the statement : 

^he majority of educators who have expressed their views in the 
literature on the subject seem to favor the early admission of other- 
wise well-qualified students to institutions of higher learning, even 
though they are younger than those with whom they will have to 
associate. . . . Taken as a group, the younger students do achieve 
more scholastic success than average or over-age students^ 

There is little doubt, then, that the truly gifted child 
will be able to absorb an acceleration of one, two, or 

1 GRAY, H. A., "Some Factors in the Undergraduate Careers of College 
Students," p. 9, Teachers College Contributions to Education, No. 437, 
Columbia University, New York, 1930. 

221 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

three years without difficulty. Even an acceleration of 
as much as four years would not completely bridge the 
gap in mental age between a ten-year-old child with an 
LQ. of 160 and a typical high school freshman. 

An argument that is frequently advanced in favor of 
full acceleration is that it makes it possible for the bright 
child to compete with others of approximately the same 
mental age. (No group, of course, is ever truly homogene- 
ous in mental equipment; for that matter, no two indi- 
viduals are ever mentally identical, even though their 
chronological and mental ages may be the same.) A bright 
child, accelerated to a grade in which the subject material 
is sufficiently difficult to challenge him, will be less 
likely to develop bad mental habits than if he were 
required to mark time in a lower grade, where he would 
be competing with children of the same chronological 
age but where there would be little or nothing in the 
subject matter to interest his active mind. Gifted children 
respond to the latter situation in many different ways. 
They may become lazy; or they may develop a feeling 
of frustration, as in the case of David Brown, who at 
the age of six entered Grade 1. 

/ David could do fourth-grade work with ease, but the 
superintendent had an inflexible rule that six-year-old 
children entering school must begin at the beginning. 
For weeks David was obliged to sit quietly and listen 
while his classmates of ajerage mentality struggled with 
the task of reading "I see a cat" and attempted to 
remember from one day to the next that 2 and 1 make 3. 
One day, after months of this disillusioning experience, 
David came home and said, "Daddy, I'm caught in a 
trap." Many gifted children have experienced this feeling 
of being caught in a trap and as a result have developed 
an open antagonism toward formal education. It is 
possible that acceleration to a more stimulating grade 

222 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

level would do much toward eliminating both indolence 
and frustration 

An argument that is a very important one from the 
point of view of the superintendent or principal is that 
acceleration places no strain upon the administrative 
machinery of the school. All that is required is that the 
child be taken from one grade and placed in another. 
Moreover, this is a money-saving device, for the shorter 
the time a child stays in school, the less it costs to edu- 
cate him. These two reasons probably account in large 
part for the considerable popularity of this method 
among school executives. It has the added, pleasant 
feature of being satisfactory to the majority of parents. 
* Perhaps the most logical argument in favor of accelera- 
tion is that it permits the gifted individual to enter 
upon his life's work at an earlier age than would be the 
case if he traveled through school at the usual rate. The 
saving of from two to four years in time would be espe- 
cially valuable to the many brilliant students who wish 
to go to professional school after finishing college. For 
a young man to have his graduate degree and to be ready 
to enter upon his profession at the age of twenty-two 
instead of twenty-five might well constitute a consider- 
able advantage. A number of individuals who have been 
prominent in American life and letters were graduated 
at an early age, and it is possible that this fact exercised 
some influence upon their later eminence. For example, 
Emerson entered Harvard when he was only fourteen, 
Longfellow entered Bowdoin at fifteen, and Charles 
Evans Hughes was graduated from Brown at the age 
of nineteen. More recently, Berle was graduated from 
Harvard at the age of eighteen. " Who's Who in America" 
contains the n^mes of many men and women who seem 
to have made good use of the time saved through early 
graduation from 'college, f 

223 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

' On the other hand, it is true that many have found 
themselves, upon leaving college, seriously handicapped 
by immaturity. In this, as in all other situations con- 
cerning intellectually gifted children, much depends 
upon the individual. If he is accelerated socially and 
physically, the chances are excellent that he will not find 
early graduation from college a handicap. If, however, 
he is retarded either socially or physically, then early 
graduation is undesirable and may even prove to be 
disastrous. ^ 

As an example of the first situation, there is the case 
of the boy who entered college when he was fifteen. In 
physical development he appeared to be eighteen or 
nineteen. His social qualities were such that he was 
elected president of his freshmen class. He was graduated 
with a Phi Beta Kappa key at the age of nineteen and 
went on to Yale Law School, from which he was gradu- 
ated at the head of his class. He was immediately taken 
into a leading law firm and rapidly developed into a very 
successful attorney. If this young man had been under- 
sized or socially backward, the three years' acceleration 
might have been a handicap instead of an asset. 

Disadvantages 

Although ease of administration, timesaving, and other 
advantages of acceleration are sufficiently important to 
warrant serious consideration, they are overshadowed 
by the disadvantages which are likely to attend a too 
rapid progress through school. If a child is advanced 
according to a flexible promotion scheme wherein he is 
allowed to skip one or more grades, he misses important 
basic material. This is one of the reasons why so many 
gifted children do poor work in spelling or make errors 
in simple arithmetical computation. Most teachers of 

224 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

heterogeneous groups have little time to help an ac- 
celerated child fill the gaps in his scholastic information. 
The most serious disadvantage of acceleration, how- 
ever, is a social one, resulting from the discrepancy which 
exists between the child's intellectual maturity on the 
one hand and his physical and social maturity on the 
other. For instance, a ten-year-old boy who is accelerated 
five years mentally may be accelerated physically only 
one year and emotionally and socially but two years. 
The child is capable of doing tenth-grade work, but 
actually to place him in the tenth grade might result in 
numerous maladjustments. This boy of ten, although as 
large as the average eleven-year-old, would be unable to 
participate successfully in the athletic activities of fifteen- 
year-old children. He would have considerable difficulty, 
also, in adjusting to the social life of the school. The 
exciting new world of adolescence into which all his 
classmates had entered would still be closed to him. A 
ten-year-old child, he would be baffled by manifestations 
all about him of emotions and social interchanges which 
he could not experience. This disadvantage alone out- 
weighs all the advantages which can be enumerated in 
favor of full acceleration for intellectually gifted children. 
/ Terman, 1 taking the point of view that the amount of 
acceleration should always be determined by the charac- 
teristics of the individual pupil concerned, reports on 
a case of unwise acceleration as follows: 

ROGEK 

For a fifteen-year-old college boy who has been continually in 
boarding school from a very tender age, Roger presents an astonish- 
ing picture of profound childishness. . . . 

Roger was nearly eleven years old when first tested by us, and was 
finishing the seventh grade in an exclusive boarding school for boys. 

1 TERMAN, L. M., "Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. Ill, pp. 262-264, 
Stanford University Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1930. 

225 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

The Stanford-Biiiet test gave him at the age of 10-10 an I.Q. of 149 
(corrected, 152). A Stanford Achievement Test administered a few 
weeks later showed him to be advanced in most school subjects to 
the standards of grades several years beyond that in which he was 

located Roger skipped the eighth grade, and entered the ninth 

grade at the age of 11-3. His progress after that time was at the nor- 
mal rate. At 15-3 he entered the junior college department of the 
school that he had attended most of the time since the age of ten. 

The wisdom of placing a boy of Roger's type in the ninth grade 
when he was barely eleven seems to us at least questionable. When 
still in the seventh grade he was said by his teacher to be a solitary 
child, "decidedly less popular than the average," though his mother, 
in filling out the Home Information Blank, was inclined to be 
more liberal, assigning him an average rating upon popularity and 
leadership. . . . 

At the time of our recent follow-up, Roger's situation was rather 
pitiful. At an age when most boys are in their second year of high 
school, Roger was finishing his first year of junior college. He was 
thus three years accelerated. At this time he not only did not look 
or act like a college youth, but looked and acted several years younger 
than he really was. There were transparent attempts to assume the 
role of collegian, but with his round, childish face, short stature, and 
wistful expression, his stiff dancing-school bow when he acknowledged 
introductions, and bis naive conversation, the effect produced was 
that of a young child playing grown-up. He was courteous and re- 
sponsive, but ill at ease in the presence of the other members of the 
group with whom he took our follow-up tests. The fact of his preco- 
cious school advancement was evidently a source of both gratification 
and chagrin to him. At one instant he told the examiner with childlike 
relish that he was a freshman in junior college, ''though I won't be 
sixteen till this summer, and haven't done a bit of studying since I 
entered the college." A little later he was confessing that he found 
life very dull. "You get so sick of going to movies twice a week, 
playing bridge, and then doing the same thing all over again. There's 
nothing to do, and I'm left out of everything." And he added plain- 
tively, "I can't go out with girls the way the other fellows do because 
they would all say the girls were cradle-snatchers." . . . 

In our judgment, Roger has neither the maturity, the interests, 
nor the attitudes to make him a successful and happy college student 
at present. His case is one of the clearest in our entire gifted group 
of unwise haste in school advancement. 

226 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 



Opinions of Gifted Children 

Earlier reference has been made to the Speyer School 
group of gifted children with whom the writer worked 
for a time. In the course of that work two hours were 
given over to a parliamentary discussion of the following 
proposition, selected and phrased by the children them- 
selves: Resolved, That intellectually gifted children should 
be allowed to progress through school at their own rate of 
speed. Eight children, under the chairmanship of the 
writer, participated in this discussion. They were a re- 
markable group, since only one had an LQ. under 170. 
Their I.Q.'s and chronological ages, together with 
identifying but fictitious names, appear in Table XXXI. 

TABLE XXXI. AGES AND I.Q.'s OF EIGHT GIFTED CHILDREN 
PARTICIPATING IN A DEBATE ON ACCELERATION 



Name 


C.A., years 
and months 


LQ. 


Miles 


11-5 


190 


Eva 


9-5 


197 


Marv 


9-4 


184 


Ernest . 


11-1 


175 


Nina 


10-6 


172 


Ruth 


10-3 


171 


Donald 


9-8 


170 


Fred 


10-7 


159 









The discussion on acceleration by this gifted group ran 
for two hours with an intermission at the end of the first 
hour. A stenographer was present and took down in short- 
hand every word that was said. Her transcription appears 
in the following pages. This material not only reveals 
what gifted children themselves think about the desir- 
ability of progressing rapidly through school but also 

227 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

vividly displays the characteristics of the thinking of 
brilliant nine- and ten-year-old children. 

PABLIAMENTAET DISCUSSION 

Present: The Chairman, Miles, Eva, Ernest, Mary, Donald, Fred, 
Nina, Ruth, parents, and guests. 

(The children have some prepared comments. They discuss the 
number of words contained in their "papers" 86, 72, etc. They 
tease Miles, asking him whether he has "2,000 to 5,000" words pre- 
pared, but he replies that it is "nearer 200 to 500." Before his entrance 
they ask each other whether "the professor" is coming.) 

CHAIRMAN: Everyone will be allowed to talk as long as he likes. 
You will arise and address the Chair and wait for your turn. Be sure 
to address the Chair before you begin speaking. Stand while you talk. 
You may say anything you want to say that concerns the subject. 

Will Ernest open the discussion by presenting the proposition? 

ERNEST: The proposition is: Resolved, That intellectually gifted 
children should be allowed to progress through school at their own 
rate of speed. "Intellectually gif ted "' means all children of 130 I.Q. 
or more. " Own rate of speed" means they should be able to go ahead 
as fast as their I.Q.'s will allow them. 

I and the group I am working with think that all these children 
should be allowed to go through school as fast as they can, because 
if they are held back, they might develop a superiority complex. 

CHAIRMAN: Are there any comments on this? 

MILES: This is only a parliamentary discussion and "resolved" 
should not be used. 

CHAIRMAN: Are there any arguments? You may say anything you 
want to say. 

MART: Our opinions? 

CHAIRMAN: Or you may refer to material you have read. What is 
your point of view? 

MART: I rather think that Ernest is mixed up. I am arguing for 
moderate acceleration; it would be all right to go ahead one grade, 
one and a half grades, or even two grades. 

CHAIRMAN: Why? 

MART: Well, if you went ahead very far Well, I don't mean any 
special child, but there is a child named Betty who was skipped one 
grade and then skipped another grade and then she was skipped again. 
And she developed an inferiority complex and thought she was very 
very wonderful. They would lose their friends and nobody would like 

228 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

them and they wouldn't have any fun. Nobody would play with 
them. Nobody would care what happened to them, because they were 
stuck-up. 

MILES: I have an objection to make. The last thing that Mary 
brought up was that the child would lose his or her friends. This was 
brought out in an article recently in the Times. But in games of skill 
a child of high I.Q. could contend with older children because strength 
would not be at grips there but brain, and his brain would be as far 
developed as the normal or older children's. When it comes to games 
of brawn he could associate with children of his own age because he 
wouldn't be superior or inferior to them. 

MART: Just the same, children wouldn't want to let hum into their 
games because he would have an inferiority complex. Because any 
person if he gets stuck-up I am sure from experience and stories 
and all no one would want him. They can't expect to have friends 
if they do get inferiority complexes and affected. 

CHAIRMAN : You have made the point that older children wouldn't 
want to play with you if you were much younger and that they 
certainly wouldn't if you felt stuck-up. 

EENEST: I don't tnink anyone would get stuck-up unless they stay 
in the same grade, and then they really are better than anybody else. 

MARY: Some people do. 

ERNEST: Well, I think very few, less than one out of twenty-five. 

MART: I know about three. 

NINA: I know a lot of people stuck-up because they have been 
accelerated too far. One of my girl friends is bragging that she is in 
a higher class, j 

CHAIRMAN: Mary and Nina are saying very definitely that an 
accelerated child will become stuck-up and egotistic; Ernest says that 
if they are allowed to stay in the same class they will also become 
egotistical because they will be the brightest in the class./ 

MILES: I want to say that Mary has been using the wrong term. 
She has been defining inferiority complex as stuck-up while superior- 
ity complex is the word. 

If a child were held back in a class of the same chronological age, 
he would be doing work outside of school, because the class work 
will become boring to him. And as it became more so, he wouldn't 
bother taking part in the class discussions or class work and children 
would think that the child didn't know the answers. But the real 
reason would be because he knew only too well and they were too 
boring for him to answer. 

229 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

MARY: That may happen sometimes, but not very often. There 
was Ruth, another girl I know. She was put in another school 
Lincoln or Horace Mann, I thinkand she was very, very bright 
and sat in the first row in class and everything, and she always raised 
her hand, to show off. 

CHAIRMAN: Donald, do you have anything to say? 

DONALD: I will have to find something to argue against! 

RUTH: I have a friend who is bragging that whenever they have 
arithmetic or spelling or any other subjects she always gets ahead 
and is bored with it. One time I had one of my books home and she 
took the book and copied an example and worked it just as fast as I 
did, even though she is only in 4B. 

NINA: I don't think that what Miles said would happen often. 
They would try to study. If they weren't told that they were better, 
they would try to keep up with the children of their own age. 

CHAIRMAN: Let me say that if you want to bring in any quotations 
or any references from your reading, do so. 

ERNEST: Nina just said that she disagreed with Miles because she 
thought that a child would have to be told. He wouldn't, because if 
he tried to keep up, he would be able to do that much faster than 
the others. And then he would just sit around. If he was held back, 
he would get bored and would have to find something to do, so he 
would do something outside of school and get still farther ahead. 

EVA: Miles said if a child was held back, it would make him feel 
superior. But if he was left back, he would feel badly. 

FRED: When I saw a big boy bigger than Ernest very tall and 
old, marching with little kids, I said, "Look at that big gawk; he 
must be awfully dumb." They must be ashamed. 

CHAIRMAN: This concerns the question of dull children rather than 
bright children. We should probably keep to the question of superior 
children here today. 

MILES: First, I have an objection to make to the way Mary has 
been bringing in experience, because she is no expert on I.Q. and we 
do not know whether the child was really superior or only between 
normal and superior. 

Here, in this question of obtaining a superiority complex, according 
to Goddard, if the child is kept back and only allowed to go ahead at 
the pace of the regular public school child, he will develop a superior- 
ity complex and that should be seen from the fact that the child is 
able to do better and more work and can see differences from his own 
action and those of the others even if he doesn't know he is superior. 

230 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

DONALD: Eva said that if a child is left back, he wouldn't develop 
a superiority complex. He would, because he has had this work so 
much that it has become boring. 

MART (heatedly): Miles referred to experience. Why should he 
take himself for an example, then? 

MILES (excited): If you can point out one instance in this debate 
where I have used myself as an example I will give you a dollar for 
each one! 

(Children are quieted.) 

NINA (to Chairman) : At the beginning of this discussion, you said 
that we should address you as " Mr. Chairman," and no one has. 

CHAIRMAN : It would help if you did that, and if each stood as he 
spoke. It would be easier for us to follow. 

(The children act accordingly throughout the rest of the discussion 
reminding each other when necessary.) 

MART: Miles said that he would give a dollar for each one but 
this is not a debate , but a formal discussion! 

CHAIRMAN: We are concerned with the question given to us by 
Ernest. Very often political discussions degenerate into personalities, 
but we don't want that to happen here. 

ERNEST: Eva and Fred said that a child would develop a superiority 
complex by being left back, but we are talking about being held back 
to his chronological age. 

RUTH: I had a friend who was just promoted when I was being 
skipped. And she was supposed to have been skipped also, but the 
principal made her stay back, and so this girl started crying and got a 
great superiority complex. She started to brag about it, that it wasn't 
fair, I told her that I didn't think so either but that I couldn't do 
anything about it. 

CHAIRMAN: I don't want to direct this discussion too much, but I 
think we have talked about superiority enough. I know you have con- 
sidered many other possibilities, such as social difficulties, difficulties 
in making adult adjustments if graduated from college at 16, 17, or 18. 

RUTH: The child labor law says that children under 18 may not 
work. If I were accelerated, I could right now be in second year high 
school and then, if I went along steadily, by the time I would be 15, 1 
would be out of high school; college four years and I would be 18 and 
able to work. 

MILES: Here I have a big point to bring up in this discussion about 
adjustments. In the rehearsals we have had before, some of the chil- 
dren have stated that if a child were allowed to finish college much 

231 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

earlier, it would be hard financially. I want to bring out that if a child 
gets out of college at even 17, he will have all his time available for 
attempting to earn a living. He won't have college expenses any 
longer. He might not be able to go to work because of the child labor 
law, but he can have smaller jobs and be partially independent. Also 
if he wants to enter business, he will be gaining experience and then 
he can get a major job and hold it. By the time he is of age to support 
others, he will have had more experience in doing it and will have a 
better chance of obtaining a good job. 

CHAIRMAN: Miles has made an excellent argument. Some of you 
who are on the other side should try to reply to it. 

DONALD: It would be almost impossible to accelerate him seven 
or eight years. I think he could adjust himself, if he has the mental 
standard to do that. 

RUTH: Even if he was accelerated seven or eight years the child 
labor law is only for factories and so on. If a child wanted to be a 
lawyer, he could go right into law school after college or even if he 
wanted to go in a store, he could get some older person than he is to 
help him run the store. There would be no law against that. 

MART: I think if Nina or anybody wanted to be technical, they 
could get a better argument, but I didn't think that was allowed here. 

CHAIRMAN : We do want to give facts from authorities, although, in 
an informal discussion like this, it is a little more difficult to do than 
it would be in a formal debate. 

DONALD: If a child went into a profession, he would be glad to have 
the extra time. If he was kept back, he wouldn't have any time for it. 
If he was going into medical school or law school, each takes three or 
four more years. He would be glad to have the extra two or three 
years that you are arguing for. 

NINA: Two or three years moderate acceleration. 

CHAIRMAN: Would some one in your group explain what you mean 
by moderate acceleration? 

MART: Full acceleration would be three years or more . . . 

CHAIRMAN: May I say that Ernest defined full acceleration at the 
beginning by saying that it meant intellectually gifted children 
should be allowed to%b ahead at their own rate of speed. That is what 
that group is arguing for. 

MILES: I ask those against acceleration to refute this. Their main 
argument centers about financial and social difficulties and adult mal- 
adjustments. They are arguing that the child would not fit into the 
social world. Any person could see that if they were allowed to finish 
college, to get into a profession, and to gain experience, they would 

232 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

have a better source of income for the time when they get married. 
And I think a person would rather be married to one with a better 
source of income than to one kept in school. He would have more time 
to save up and also would be able to provide for a family. Here, with 
financial difficulties ruled out, social maladjustments are too, because 
he would be able to gain money from his experience and he would have 
money to get married on. 

FEED: You say that he will have more time afterwards. I say: what 
will he do in college? He won't have any fun there. Besides that, while 
he is in college, he could earn money enough to be able to pay for a lit- 
tle by having a little job; besides that, he could save up. 

RUTH: I object, because Fred said that he wouldn't have anything 
to do. Full acceleration would mean the child would be physically fit. 

NINA: He wouldn't be physically fit. Suppose he entered high school 
at ten, then, Dr. H. said when she was talking to us, he would have to 
have physical education, or a certain amount of it, and he wouldn't 
be fit for it. 

DONALD : There are many children now in high school not physically 
fit even though they are years older. If a person has the mental capac- 
ity, he could go in at any age. 

FEED : If you went to high school or college a few years younger than 
the others Well, did you ever see a nine-year-old wrestle with a 
twelve-year-old and come out on top? 

EENEST: I have seen many. 

FEED: You have no proof, no evidence, no witnesses! 

RUTH: Let's see yours 1 

FEED: I tried it. 

(The children are all very excited.) 

CHAIEMAN: I think the point is good but I don't think we should 
follow it up any further. 

NINA: I don't think we should take Ernest's statement; he has no 
proof. 

RUTH: If you think Ernest is lying 

(This sets off a new storm.) 

CHAIEMAN: We aren't going to have any more of this. The points 
made were very good, but we don't want to quarrel over them. 

MILES: Fred has no evidence himself, but the matter concerns 
intellectually gifted children. According to observations made by 
many authorities, they develop faster both mentally and physically 
more than other children. We can't say that they develop just as fast 
physically, but brain plays a big part in brawn. In boxing and 
wrestling a man can weigh more, but if he doesn't know where to 

233 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

strike, he will never come out on top, because the knowledge of the 
other man will tell him how to win the match. 

CHAIRMAN: Are there any further comments on this? 

RUTH: It was said before that, if he was playing football, the child 
who was fully accelerated wouldn't have fun. But full accelerations 
don't mean those who are very small. Mary is much taller than I am, 
although she is younger, so many people like her could go through 
high school. 

MART: There aren't many children that are so big. I think nine out 
of ten children are average size or smaller. 

NINA: I disagree with Miles because if the college had a wrestling 
match, the other person would have to know just as much about it as 
he would, because if he didn't know about it, how could he get in 
college? 

MILES: I don't want to quote personal experience, but it stands to 
reason that, in a good football team, someone behind the team would 
have to look up the plays because two teams would try to match each 
other by developing plays, going by trick plays that will win the game, 
and here an intellectually gifted child would be the best person to fit 
into it because he would be the best fitted to devise schemes, although 
he might not be physically fitted to play in the game. 

ERNEST: Even though an intellectually gifted child may be younger, 
he can play football with the others; you have seen many men as big 
as others being knocked out. 

NINA: Miles said that maybe he would figure out plays. Suppose he 
wants to play football? And, after college, he can't. 

ERNEST: Why not? 

FRED: If a child is too small, he can be tackled better. 

ERNEST: When he is out of college, he could play with those his own 
age and weight. Nina said he can't play when he is out of college. 

MILES: This statement about not being able to play after college: 
He could play with a team of his own age. There would be no restric- 
tion; it is just up to himself what he is going to do. 

I don't believe that many intellectually gifted children would want 
to be football players. They would be interested in chemistry, and so 
on. 

DONALD: What if he don't play football it would not ruin his life! 

NINA: Perhaps a little bit. I don't think there is so much enjoyment 
outside of college as on college teams the crowds, the cheering, and 
everything. 

CHAIRMAN: Let's turn to the social difficulties involved. Fred, 
you had some ideas on that subject? 

234 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

FRED : I think that if the gifted child is accelerated four years or 
more, he wouldn't be able to keep up with the others. 

CHAIRMAN: Why not? 

FRED : Well, a kid almost all the time has a girl all the others in 
college are the same age he wouldn't have any boy friends of his own 
age or any girl friends. 

DONALD: In the Minnesota Journal of Education it says: "Pupils so 
chosen are segregated for class work only and so may associate with 
children of their own chronological age." 

NINA: Suppose you want to go to dances? You are not invited be- 
cause you are too small. Can you picture somebody trying to dance 
with someone much smaller! 

MARY: It would be very uncomfortable. I saw a girl dancing with a 
dog - 

MILES: Who has ever seen anyone go out and dance with a dog! 
Here we are discussing dancing with human beings. That is irrelevant. 
In these social difficulties, what Donald brought up has something to 
do with it. He can't go back with children of his own age. We have 
often heard of child marriages. If a child gets married, it doesn't have 
to be a child marriage. 

NINA: I don't think that makes sense! 

RUTH : We had a maid who was just about eighteen but she was very 
small and she had a boy friend, she had a great big boy friend, and 
they went out to dances so frequently. 

CHAIRMAN: Some of you had more to say about full acceleration. 

DONALD: We are for acceleration, but not to a point that is 
harmful. 

CHAIRMAN: What do you mean by harmful? 

DONALD: As far as the child can take it. 

RUTH: To go ahead as far as he can mentally, but not to be harmed 
mentally. 

MILES: As far as a child can go, and tests will tell whether he knows 
what goes before, or not. 

FRED: All these are trying to explain is that they think a child 
should go ahead as far as he can, without missing anything he would 
need. 

CHAIRMAN: You must define your terms better. What is meant by 
full acceleration? 

DONALD: I have defined full acceleration. I say it is to a point where 
the child can take it. 

MART: I disagree, if he says that we are just for acceleration that 
way. If a child could go ahead, then just let him go. 

235 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

CHAIRMAN: As I understand it now, those on the affirmative are still 
in favor of full acceleration but want to guarantee to the bright child 
the right to cover all the needed material before he goes on into high 
school or college; they want him to have a chance to get fundamentals. 

MILES: If he has the required knowledge to pass the tests with 
high marks. 

RUTH: I think that acceleration would also mean that supposing 
a child would be accelerated five years in every subject except, let's 
say, geography, then I should think that he would be allowed to have 
the'books. I'm only in the fifth grade English and in the sixth year 
history, and so on. I think that's how a child should be, so that he 
would be able to go ahead in all his subjects. 

NINA: Last time I think it was Donald that asked the question of 
the negative team what a child is supposed to do, if he is only ac- 
celerated a few years, if he has finished the work earlier. I think he 
should be allowed to work on some subject that would ordinarily be 
out of school. 

DONALD: That would be developing a superiority complex to the 
children in his class. He would get so bright that he wouldn't play 
with them. 

MARY: Some children do study out of school. Lots take piano les- 
sons, dancing lessons, and all sorts of lessons. Frances does, Thelma 
does, Barbara, Jack, I, and others. Lots of children do it, so I can't 
see how anyone should get stuck-up about it. And if you take other 
lessons out of school, please tell me where you can get them if you 
don't have a tutor! 

CHAIRMAN: That is a good point to get enrichment at home. 

DONALD: He would develop a superiority complex. If he stayed out 
of school, they would be so far advanced in the other subjects, what 
would happen to him? 

NINA: I don't think he should stay out of school. Here we are hav- 
ing different subjects. At the other schools they don't study outside. 
I didn't. Maybe some do. We have units. There may be more than one 
bright child in the class and the others might try to keep up with 
them. 

MILES: In the point that Mary has brought out, I want to make a 
comeback to it. She has brought out music. I have Hebrew lessons out 
of school. She says something like enrichment. In half the class in most 
schools the children study these different things. But the point is that 
if you haven't a good ear for music and can't play, it doesn't mean that 
you have to sit down and sulk the rest of your life; there are things 
you will have to use all kinds of arithmetic; if you want to be a good 

236 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

salesman, you will have to know English, etc. Those are subjects 
which most people don't take out of school. The things you (Mary) 
brought out are not important unless you are becoming a maestro or 
something. 

R,UTH: There is not a child I don't think in the two Terman classes 
that don't have one special thing like music 

FRED (bursts out): I don't take any! 

RUTH (resuming) : That is, only one or two, out of fifty. They might 
not develop a superiority complex for music. But I know that I could 
read a lot in my father's library. Then if there was a child like that he 
would develop a superiority complex if he was allowed to go at modi- 
fied acceleration only. But if he went at full acceleration he could take 
those books and read them and go at his own rate of speed. 

(Donald and Fred have been drawing. Chairman comments on this. 
The boys stop.) 

MARY: Donald said that if they took lessons out of school, it would 
give them a superiority complex. But that can't happen in anything 
besides music and dancing and drawing and handicrafts and military 
things, because well, so far Donald or anybody hasn't been able to 
answer my question: Where could they get lessons in other things 
unless they had a private tutor? 

DONALD: Let me see now 

NINA: It may not be the right thing to do, but I start to disagree 
with Mary, because I know a lot of children whose mothers and fathers 
could help them. If a child is bright, he could help himself. This may 
not be a good thing to do, to oppose my own side, but it is true. 

CHAIRMAN: You have a good point there. 

FRED : To bring up Mary's point. I think if a boy was better than 
anyone else in dancing lessons or telling stories, he could just come out 
with all he knows, he'd think he was so smart. 

DONALD: I want to answer Mary's point. Where could he develop a 
superiority complex? There are arithmetic, history, and the other 
subjects. 

CHAIRMAN: Mary is suggesting a good substitute, enrichment in 
the home, through the home library, dancing lessons, music lessons, 
etc. I suggest that you four negative people stick to it. 

DONALD: What about poor children? What about the children in the 
South? Maybe they haven't a good book to their name! They don't 
have books. 

MARY: Don't I know! I've lived there! Mr. Chairman, there are 
libraries all over the world, even where there are poor children. And if 
they lived way off they could walk or hitch-hike to the libraries. 

237 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

FEED: We are talking about the Southern country boy and he 
hasn't any books or libraries. But he can go through the woods and 
learn a lot about nature. 

DONALD: Still he isn't advanced in his other school subjects. 

NINA: Sometimes children inherit some of their knowledge from 
their parents. And so if the parents didn't know enough to earn enough 
money to buy books, how can the child know enough to want 
books? 

CHAIRMAN: It is true that there is a relationship between size of 
library and intelligence of children ; you have hit upon something which 
Prof. Bagley pointed out a long time ago. 

MARY: He could study some kinds of arithmetic, addition and sub- 
traction, if his mother or someone explained to him. He could say: 
"There are four purple flowers (using vase on library for example) 
and four yellow flowers," and so on. Or if he said, ' 'If I have picked ten 
red flowers and three got crushed, how many would I have?" He 
could get a little arithmetic in. He could get English when people come 
to visit and when he went into towns or cities, because everyone does 
go in sometimes. He could get his own family history. 

CHAIRMAN: I don't think this point needs to be added to because 
she has made it very clearly: it is possible to learn outside of school. 

RUTH: I have a very good friend in the South who has never been in 
town. They haven't any schools down there. And also I object because 
well, I don't think we are going through with full acceleration, yet 
my mother teaches me some of the work in high school and my father 
knows enough to teach me something about law; I take music lessons 
and my parents try to teach me English; we have a maid that's 
German and I am learning German; and I am going to learn Russian 
and Hebrew; and French we have at school; and I am taking dancing, 
dramatic, and art lessons BO what more do I need for full acceleration! 

MILES: Mary says a child could learn by himself. From the reports 
of very eminent men, when a child is born he is no better than an ape. 
He has to be taught in order to learn. Otherwise he stays just as dumb 
as the day he was born. If you say he is going out in the woods and 
know things without a tutor, then you are mistaken: if he comes in 
contact with children who have had tutors he will have an inferiority 
complex. 

MARY: Well, first, Ruth was arguing for our side and now she is on 
the other side. Then, in answer to what Miles is saying, I didn't say 
that he would go out by himself and learn things; I said if Ms mother 
told him about different things, then he certainly could learn it, 
because if he didn't then someone else would turn up to teach him. 

238 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

FEED: About Ruth. First she said that she had a friend in the South 
without schools or cities. But then directly after that she brought up 
tutoring at home by the parents. This is just what the girl would get. 
Then, Miles says if a boy went out in the woods he wouldn't think 
anything of it. I think he would see flowers and colors and birds and 
would learn to appreciate them and love them. He would want to 
know who made them, and everything. 

DONALD: He wouldn't know enough to do it. 

NINA: In answer to Miles's statement about being born without 
knowledge : what about his parents? Are they deaf mutes or something? 
A child learns to talk, at least, from his parents. He wants to inquire 
about different things from his parents. 

EENEST: In answer to Fred. Well, instead of that, I think a boy 
would take it all for granted. He would probably see them every day 
and would probably take them for granted. 

CHAIRMAN: May I raise this question, Wouldn't the amount of 
learning vary among different children? We are speaking of intel- 
lectually gifted children. 

MILES: I would like to make some definite answers to Mary, Nina, 
and Fred. When a child opens his eyes, he is very young and he hasn't 
any intelligence. His intelligence is marked according to how much he 
has for his age. He may have a lot relatively, but, in relation to the 
intelligence of older people, he has very little and he wouldn't have 
enough to think why they were there. The second time he saw them he 
wouldn't think much of it either and as time went on he would be 
used to seeing it and he wouldn't be used to asking questions, because 
he would take it for granted because even when he was born they were 
there. 

Then, Fred stated that a child would get an education from his 
parents, that he would learn to speak, etc. Yes. He would learn the 
things that the parents through necessity had remembered, but you can 
ask plenty of school teachers in New York City and they don't 
remember things. For example. I asked one about square root and she 
didn't know. The other things his parents will have forgotten because 
they haven't had occasion to use it. 

FEED: Miles's first statement: He said that a child kept seeing these 
flowers and wouldn't notice them. I think there are so many things in 
the woods and so many things about them that each day he would 
learn a little bit more. 

MABY: Miles said that when a baby was born he might have, say, 
100 or 200 or 300 brain power but older people have more: but how do 
they get it without learning? 

239 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

CHAIRMAN: This may help to clarify the matter: the issue is between 
formal education in schools and the education which we get more or 
less indirectly through experience and through the casual teachings of 
other people. Miles is for formal education and Fred, for instance, is 
for indirect teaching. 

RUTH: I object. First, when Fred said that I was .only taking this 
friend and then showing that you could get languages and everything 
at home. That doesn't make a person brighter, because even if you go 
abroad and see things as, let's say, he is going in a store in France and 
didn't know arithmetic and gave the man a five-dollar bill he 
wouldn't know how much change he should get. Suppose the store 
keeper wanted to cheat him or was dumb. If he took my money, and I 
knew I deserved some change, I could point it out to him. 

CHAIRMAN: In order to give you time to prepare yourselves, let me 
say that I am going around the table and give each of you a chance to 
tell what he thinks should be done. We will have a moment longer for 
general discussion. 

EKNEST: I have another answer for Fred's statement. You live in 
the city and see houses every day do you think who built them ? 

RUTH: Would you if you weren't gifted? 

FEED: No, because most of them in my city are just the same. But 
the woods are always different. 

NINA: First of all, I would like to ask Miles to give Mary a dollar, 
because at the last discussion he said he would for every time that he 
took himself for an example and he took himself for an example just 
before, when he said he went to some New York teachers about square 
root! 

In answer to Miles again, I want to ask him did he ever wonder 
about houses when he first saw them? 

CHAIRMAN: We will take time for just one more reply and then give 
each a chance to give his position. 

MARY: Since some of my opponents said that they probably would 
take flowers and things for granted, some might take arithmetic for 
granted and just learn it because they have to and so even if they do 
go to school and get taught they don't take a great interest in arith- 
metic all the time. 

CHAIRMAN: I am going to ask each of you for your personal opinion. 
Ernest? 

ERNEST: I am still for the affirmative, that each child if he has a 
high I.Q. should be able to go ahead as far as he may, going by tests 
and results on tests. 

240 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ACCELERATION 

May I reply to Fred? You said that you love to go out in the woods 
well, that's because you do live in the city and there you won't find 
many trees and flowers, but one who sees them every day would love 
to come into the city just as you would to go into the country. 

NINA: What are we talking about now? About last Thursday or 
what? 

CHAIRMAN: Do you think an intellectually gifted child should be 
fnT1y accelerated? 

NINA: I don't believe they should, because suppose a child were 
eight and were put into high school, as someone already said, what 
fun would they have? They wouldn't be able to take part in any 
games, they wouldn't have the ability to. And if they went into col- 
lege at eleven, they wouldn't be able to do anything either. If they 
were put ahead one or two years even although he could go farther, I 
think if he weren't told that he could go ahead more he would try to 
study outside of school, and that's all right as long as you don't boast 
in school. 

DONALD: I am still for the affirmative. I want to bring up the A.Q., 
the Achievement Quotient. If the Achievement Quotient is also good 
enough that he can achieve in high school and college program I think 
it would be all right. 

RUTH: I am still for the affirmative because I think that if a child's 
got the ability even if he isn't tall he will feel he has and will hate the 
other children and all his surroundings if he is kept at modified 
acceleration. 

I have two more statements. First of all jokingly, maybe on 
Mondays, Wednesdays, and Fridays, on WHN at 7 o'clock there is a 
program and you will get an Emerson radio if you bring up a question. 
I think I will send in this question we have been discussing. 

And also, to the statement that Fred made that he loved to go into 
the woods. There are so many different kinds of things in the woods 
that the only reason he does is because he knows about everything 
and the rest he could pick up, but if he didn't know anything about it 
then there are so many different kinds of trees, leaves, and everything, 
that he wouldn't know anything. 

FRED: I am for modified acceleration. I think that a gifted child 
should be accelerated a few years because when a person is young, 
between the ages of four and seven, he grows very rapidly and age is a 
great difference, but when he gets older, ten or eleven, there isn't so 
much difference in height and unless he is very short or very tall there 
isn't much difference. So if he is accelerated a few years, he can make 

241 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

up to this in high school and will be just as big as the other boys on the 
teams. 

MARY: I am still for modified acceleration because I think it would 
be all right. Like in this school, you go ahead as fast as you can but you 
do the grades just the same. In the regular school, if you are skipped 
once and go on for a little while, you could be skipped again. But more 
than two years would be silly. If the child could take more than that 
the parents could teach them at home. My mother lived near Mexico 
and she is going to teach me Spanish. I am for different lessons outside 
of school, and in school if they don't have anything else to do; if it is 
a school like this, he could get books from the library upstairs and 
there would not be time to get bored, and I think there should be more 
schools like this one. 

MILES: I am the last speaker here and I would like to answer all the 
negative team's arguments, but first I want to rid myself of a debt. 
When I made that statement I used the teacher as an example, not me. 

In answer to Mary: The child would take it for granted, and I am 
going to ask her if she can remember what she saw the first time she 
opened her eyes and I will give you a dollar! 

Fred's statement was arguing for full acceleration. I believe that 
full acceleration will not hurt under the conditions we have set down. 

I believe that most of the negative-team arguments have been 
pushed down. I have answered the question of maladjustment. Others 
have answered the other arguments. And about growth and develop- 
ment, if a child were put ahead it has been answered that in games of 
brawn he could stay with children of his own age and in games of skill 
he could go with older children. If it is as Fred says, but I don't 
believe it is quite the way he says it, then to push him ahead entirely 
wouldn't be bad from the point of view of physical development: Fred 
says that a child doesn't grow ntuch when he gets older. I don't believe 
that any school would give any child a physical-development program 
that would tax him to the maximum; I think they provide for a 
margin of safety. 

(Discussion is officially closed here, but the children continue with 
animation as they return to their classes.) 



242 



CHAPTER XI 
EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ENRICHMENT 

1. What are the advantages and disadvantages of enrichment in a 
heterogeneous class? 

2. How may the home contribute to an enrichment program? 

3. What principles should govern the conduct of the special class? 

As the defects of acceleration, in terms of skipping 
grades, have become more widely recognized, various 
methods of enrichment of the program of studies for 
intellectually gifted children have increased in popularity. 
Those who favor a policy of enrichment feel that a broad 
and well-integrated body of knowledge is a much better 
foundation upon which to build a career than is a period 
of three or four years of time saved. 

Details concerning the several kinds of enrichment 
programs belong in a book on teaching methods rather 
than in one on the psychology of gifted children. Conse- 
quently, this chapter will deal in broad outline only with 
the problems involved in such a program. The most 
important question has to do with the relative merits of 
enrichment within the heterogeneous class and enrich- 
ment within the special class. Either procedure has much 
to recommend it. 

INTRACLASS GROUPING 

Grouping within the heterogeneous class liquidates 
many of the objections which were dlscu&sed at the begin- 
ning of the preceding chapter. Neither pupils nor parents 
are likely to feel that a child has been stigmatized because 
he has been asked, in company with a few other children, 

243 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

to give additional time to arithmetic drill or to feel that a 
bright child's head will be turned because he has been 
asked to make a special report. Intraclass groupings 
rarely excite resentment because the emphasis is placed 
upon subject achievement rather than upon intellectual 
capacity. It is much easier for a parent to admit that 
Mary is a poor reader than to grant that her I.Q. is only 
75. From the practical standpoint, this is an extremely 
important advantage of differentiated instruction within 
a class made up of children of varying intellectual levels, 
as opposed to a system which involves segregation. No 
theory, however good, will work in a democracy unless it 
has the support of the public. 

Flexibility 

Intraclass grouping is characterized by flexibility. 
Under this plan a child who has shown that he cannot do 
so much or so difficult work as the others in the group of 
which he was originally a member can be transferred to 
another little group without administrative red tape. 
Moreover, this plan makes grouping within the subject 
itself relatively easy. A certain amount of unevenness in 
achievement is characteristic of all children, a fact which 
makes classification according to subjects desirable. A 
child might be doing such excellent work in history that 
he would be placed in the rapid group and given an op- 
portunity to broaden his knowledge in that field. The 
same child might be doing relatively poor work in arith- 
metic and so be placed with a slow group which was giving 
special attention to drill. 

The typical gifted child is superior to the average in all 
subjects, but there are a number of individual cases of 
marked unevenness. For instance, it frequently happens 
that a bright child does poor work in handwriting or 
spelling; in a system of intraclass grouping, he would be 

244 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ENRICHMENT 

placed with, the slow group in these two subjects. His 
placement there would help to keep the other children 
from feeling that the groupings were arbitrary and prej- 
udiced. As an example of the sort of attitude that this 
kind of classification builds up, there is the reply of the 
fifth-grade boy who, when asked who was the brightest 
child in his class, said, "Well, I don't know. David knows 
the most about arithmetic, Martha is the best reader, and 
I think I know the most about history." Feeling that you 
"know the most about history" is perhaps a more 
desirable slant than feeling that you have the highest 
intelligence. 

Use in Rural Schools 

A strong argument in favor of enrichment within the 
heterogeneous class is that it is the only possible method 
in small school systems. The one-room country school, 
which has by no means gone out of existence, has had 
more than its share of criticism during the last two 
decades, but in this school the gifted child frequently 
had and has better opportunities for development 
than his city cousin. The degree of opportunity provided 
him depends to a considerable extent, of course, upon the 
vision of his teacher. The attitudes of many eminent 
individuals of today concerning the benefits which they 
derived from their years in the little red schoolhouse 
are not based wholly on nostalgia; in many cases they 
come from a realization that there they enjoyed a freedom 
in intellectual exploration which would have been denied 
them in the typical urban educational factory. 

Modification of Curriculum and Methods 

The disadvantages of intraclass grouping are many and 
serious. Under this plan it is difficult to modify either the 
curriculum or the methods of teaching. The subjects 

245 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

studied by a heterogeneous class are determined by 
tradition and by the needs and interests of average chil- 
dren. There is little or no opportunity, for instance, to 
introduce into the middle grades such subjects as biog- 
raphy and French as a means of broadening the scho- 
lastic and informational background of mentally superior 
children. There is little opportunity, also, for modification 
of methods, no matter how eager a teacher may be to help 
the bright children in her class. Knowing that the aver- 
age children need a large amount of drill, she feels forced 
to put the superior children through the process whether 
they need it or not. Knowing that detailed explanations, 
often repeated several times, are needed by average chil- 
dren, she is forced to make the bright child sit and listen 
attentively while what is already simple to him is being 
simplified. 

A favorite method of enrichment within the hetero- 
geneous class is that of asking the bright members of the 
group to do more work such as, in English, bringing in 
additional book reports or writing extra themes or stories. 
Some bright children, previously trained in laziness and 
observing that their more fortunate though less intelligent 
classmates have less to do, resent this as unfair. If these 
children were in a special class, where all were doing a 
considerable amount of work, this attitude would not be 
so generally found. The gifted child, like a contestant in 
an athletic event, desires and needs competition. One of 
the serious disadvantages of the heterogeneous class, inso- 
far as the gifted child is concerned, is that it provides little 
or no competition. 

The principal objection to enrichment for gifted chil- 
dren in the heterogeneous class is that in most cases it 
fails to work. Teachers are human beings with all the 
failings of human beings. Consequently, most of them 
prefer teaching procedures which require the least amount 

246 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ENRICHMENT 

of time. They find it much easier to treat the class as a 
unit than as a loosely organized group of subgroups and 
individuals. Mass education is infinitely simpler than 
individual education. Whenever there is time or thought 
for variants, the pressure from parents, school officials, 
and children is all in favor of more help for the dull. 
Teachers, even those with vision, are not keeping their 
bright children in at recess and after school for special 
help even though in many instances these children would 
welcome the additional guidance; rather, it is the children 
of low mentality, who, much against their will, profit 
by the extra attention. There is, the country over, very 
little actual enrichment for intellectually gifted children 
in the heterogeneous classes of the public schools. 

Teachers who do make provision for individual differ- 
ences in their classes do so in many and varied ways. 
Hymen Alpern, in a report appearing in "Educating 
Superior Students," gives the results of a questionnaire 
which was sent to the modern language department in 
each of the senior high schools of New York. One o| the 
questions in this questionnaire was as follows: "If your 
department is too small to warrant grouping of pupils by 
differentiated classes, how do you provide for the varying 
ability of students within each class?" Alpern 1 lists the 
following answers from those who made such provisions : 

Individual coaching is done by teachers and the brighter pupils. 
Teachers hold conferences with the pupils, their grade advisers, and 
their parents. While the abler students are preparing some project, 
the teacher gives intensive drills to the slower pupils. Teachers grade 
questions to suit the ability of individuals. Questions on new matter 
are asked of the abler pupils first. Additional work is assigned to 
bright pupils. Segregation into three groups rapid, average, and 
slow is practiced. Homework assignments are differentiated. Ad- 
justment of recitation devices is made giving slower pupils simpler 

1 COHEN, H. L., and N. G. COBYELL, "Educating Superior Students," 
pp. 215-216, American Book Company, New York, 1935. 

247 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

exercises to work out and sending poorer pupils to the board for 
review or very simple work. The project method is used, or individual 
instruction, or supervised study, or supplementary reading for bright 
pupils. Slower pupils are seated in the front of the room. Outside 
reading is assigned to brighter pupils. Brighter students do original 
work for the language clubs. "Make-up" classes are arranged for 
slow pupils. Bright pupils make up special notebooks, or look up 
illustrative material. Bright pupils contribute to the school foreign 
language paper. Bright pupils report to the class on special topics 
concerning which they have made independent investigation and 
reseazth. 

/In response to the question "In what respect does the 
work done with bright students differ from that with 
average students?" Alpern 1 summarizes the replies as 
follows : 

One school states that it makes no difference except in one special 
college preparatory class. In this class there is more oral a,nd aural 
training and sight reading. Three schools state that bright pupils 
are encouraged to activity in language clubs. Six schools state that 
the pace is quickened. Of these, five take less time for a given amount 
of work; one increases the amount of work done in a given time. 
One department states that the bright group does supplementary 
work during one regular class period a week while slower pupils 
are receiving drill and doing individual work. Six schools state that 
the bright pupils have an enriched course. Six schools state that the 
bright pupils do additional supplementary reading. Six schools state 
that the bright pupils are given more oral work. Two schools 
state that the bright pupils do more creative work. Seven schools state 
that the bright pupils do more reading (not supplementary). Four 
schools state that the bright pupils do more composition work. 
Three schools state that the department aims at more individualiza- 
tion with the bright student. Several departments report that the 
bright students are encouraged to correspond with students in foreign 
countries. 

. Osburn and Rohan 2 present a program for enrichment 
in "Enriching the Curriculum for Gifted Children." 

1 IUd, pp. 216-217. 

2 OsBTON, W. J., and B. J. ROHAN, "Enriching the Curriculum for 
Gifted Children," The Macmillan Company, New York, 1931. 

248 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ENRICHMENT 

These writers, believing that neither segregation nor 
acceleration is desirable, seek to broaden the school 
life of the gifted child by making it possible for him to 
participate in absorbing constructive activities. They do 
this through a variety of school clubs, which are open to 
high-ranking students. Osburn and Rohan feel that these 
clubs not only serve as a stimulus to scholastic endeavor 
but also provide opportunities for discovering and devel- 
oping individual aptitudes and for exploring vocational 
interests. 

ENRICHMENT AT HOME 

Since gifted children tend to come from a superior 
socioeconomic level, it is possible and in many instances 
desirable for them to acquire at home the needed enrich- 
ment of the school program. Instruction so received is not 
often direct, as in most cases parents do not have much 
time to devote to teaching their children; but a great 
deal can be done indirectly through conversation and 
reading. For example, there is the case of the gifted boy of 
ten who became interested in history. He read all the 
books in that field that were in the home library, discus- 
sing them at length with his parents. It is possible that 
this unhampered, undirected kind of enrichment is more 
valuable than a well-organized program would be. 

There have been a number of famous instances of men 
who received their early education at home. Frequently 
the requirements set up by the parents are of the most 
rigid kind. More often enrichment at home is largely a 
matter of encouragement and of making available oppor- 
tunities for contact with worth-while books and with 
cultivated people. 

The most famous instance of a successful program of 
home training is that of John Stuart Mill, great English 

249 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

philosopher. Cox 1 reports on Mill's early education as 
follows: 

Until he was 14, Mill was educated at home by his father. He began 
to learn Greek at 3; and from then to his 9th year he studied Greek 
classics, making daily reports of reading. At the same time under his 
father's direction he read innumerable historical works. At 7 he read 
Plato; at 8 he began the study of Latin. Before the end of the year he 
was busily reading the classical Latin writers. He did not neglect 
mathematics: at 8 his course included geometry and algebra; at 9 
conic sections, spherics, and Newton's arithmetic were added. In 
the latter he " performed all the problems without the book and most 
of them without any help from the book/' At 10 and 11 both mathe- 
matical and classical studies were continued; astronomy and me- 
chanical philosophy were also included. In fluxions, begun at 11, 
Mill was largely self-taught. One part of his course, the writing of 
English verse, he heartily disliked. At the age of 12, philosophy and 
logic, inculding argumentation, became important parts of the 
program. Daily debate with his father, who taught him above all 
things to accept no opinion unchallenged, was a most stimulating 
exercise. At 13 a complete course in political economy was under- 
taken with intensive supplementary reading. Young Mill attended 
also a course of lectures on chemistry at the royal military college. 
In spite of the wide variety of subjects and the early age at which he 
started to study them, Mill's education was not one of cram; com- 
plete understanding was made to precede, or at least to accompany, 
every forward step. Self-conceit of a sort was guarded against, for the 
boy never heard himself praised; moreover, he had no one with whom 
to compare himself except his father, and this comparison was 
always humbling to his own pretensions. His father's frequent request 
for the definition of words used accentuated young Mill's sense of 
ignorance. 

Instruction at home played an important part in the 
education of such men as Thomas Macaulay, Thomas 
Edison, Karl Witte, Louis Pasteur, and Charles Lind- 
bergh. Thomas Edison's teacher considered that the boy 
was dull, if not actually half-witted, and placed him at the 



C. M., "Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. II, pp. 707-708, 
Stanford University Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1926. 

250 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ENRICHMENT 

foot of his class. She complained that Thomas was forever 
asking questions that had nothing to do with the lesson. 
His first school report was so bad that his mother, a 
former teacher, decided to educate him at home. Under 
her sympathetic and understanding instruction, he 
soon demonstrated his great mental powers. Before he 
was twelve years old, he had read such books as Gibbons' 
"Decline and Fall of the Roman Empire/' Hume's 
"History of England/' and Burton's "Anatomy of 
Melancholy." When he was eleven years old, he became 
interested in chemistry and physics, in which latter field 
he was later to display remarkable genius. One stands 
aghast at the thought of what the world might have lost 
if Thomas Edison had been deprived of the rich education 
which he received at home. 

The help which Witte, Pasteur, and Lindbergh received 
was of a more indirect nature. The fathers of each 
of these men provided an intelligent and understanding 
companionship. In each case the father's position was 
such that contacts with people of culture were available 
to the growing boy. It is often overlooked, for instance, 
that Charles Lindbergh's father was a Congressman and 
that each year, from the time that Charles was five until 
he was fifteen, he spent some time in Washington, where 
he played with Quentin Roosevelt and made other similar 
friends and where he had the opportunity of meeting men 
distinguished in public service. 

Home training has also proved to be helpful to many 
gifted children of the present. Stedman 1 reports, after 
five years of personal experience in working with a group 
of young geniuses in a special class, that those children 
who came to her with a background of home teaching 
possessed a large fund of general information. She says 

i STEDMAN, L. M., "Education of Gifted Children/' Chap. XV, World 
Book Company, Yonkers-on-Hudson, N. Y., 1924. 

251 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

that the home-trained gifted child, even though he has 
had no formal school education, can at the age of nine or 
ten do sixth- or seventh-grade work in an opportunity 
room. Occasionally some of these children need special 
help in certain of the mechanical subjects such as hand- 
writing and, less frequently, arithmetic. This is to be 
expected since, as a group, they are more accelerated in 
content subjects than in tool subjects. Almost any gifted 
child, if given encouragement and if provided with oppor- 
tunities at home, will have amassed, at the age of nine or 
ten, an extraordinary amount of information and will 
have developed a facility in oral and written expression 
that is comparable to the typical high school student. 

Concerning the means used by parents to educate 
their children, Stedman 1 says: 

The method of teaching employed by the parents of our gifted 
children has usually been favorable to the development of individu- 
ality. "Let the child be free," "Loose him and let him go," is the 
essence of their educational philosophy. Teaching has been incidental, 
informal, and exceedingly effective because administered in response 
to a "felt need" on the part of the child. The mechanical processes 
have been taught through games originated by the parents and by 
the use of much concrete material. Self-activity and self-expression 
in some definite concrete way have been fostered. The gifted child 
usually has a hobby which he is permitted to indulge, such as writing 
books, painting, modeling, inventing toys or mechanical apparatus, 
and numberless other creative activities. These interests lead to 
investigation, wide reading, visiting libraries and museums, collecting 
data, using the research method, and attacking problems in a scientific 
manner. 

It must be kept in mind, however, that a large per- 
centage of intellectually gifted children come from homes 
which can contribute little to their cultural development. 
Children of such homes must depend upon the school to 
satisfy their intellectual needs. 

., pp. 184-185. 

252 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ENRICHMENT 

PRINCIPLES or EDUCATION OF GIFTED CHILDREN IN 
SPECIAL .CLASSES 

| The segregation of intellectually gifted children into a 
special class makes it possible to meet their specific 
educational needs more adequately than in a hetero- 
geneous classf This can be done through a far more 
complete adaptation of curriculum content and methods 
of instruction than is possible in any unselected group. 
The special class, too, makes possible a combination of 
acceleration and enrichment. The amount of acceleration, 
as was pointed out earlier, should always be dependent 
upon the characteristics of the individual child concerned. 
For the typical gifted child, two years is not excessive, 
but in certain instances no acceleration at all should be 
permitted and in others as much as three years can be 
absorbed. In a special class the amount can be determined 
upon and then carried through without any basic mater- 
ial's being skipped! At the time this acceleration is taking 
place, the child is benefiting from an enriched course of 
study in which a broad base rather than a high altitude is 
emphasized. 

In the adaptation of curriculum and methods to the 
abilities of gifted children, it is necessary to keep in mind 
that they differ from other children not in kind but in 
degree. Consequently, many of the principles underlying 
an enlightened education for average children are basic 
also to an education for mentally superior children. In 
the following discussion, emphasis is placed upon those 
fundamentals which are of greatest importance in the 
conduct of a special class. 

Needs and Interests of Children 

There has been going on for a long time in American 
education a conflict between those who maintain that 

253 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

education is preparation for life and those who maintain 
that education is life. At present the battle appears to 
have been won, insofar as theoretical discussion is con- 
cerned, by the latter school, but in actual practice the 
former is still far stronger. In this debate, as in most 
arguments, the man who stands in the middle, between 
two extremes, is most likely to be right. The middle-of- 
the-roader takes the stand that the curriculum should 
avoid unduly emphasizing either preparation for adult 
life or the interests of the immediate present, but should 
rather be made up of material which will be both utili- 
tarian and interesting which will on the one hand 
provide the child with information which will be basic to 
his adult activities and on the other be so selected as to 
have meaning for him. It is relatively easy to apply this 
principle to the education of gifted children, for they are 
more interested than average children in the kind of sub- 
ject matter which is usually considered as an important 
part of the preparation of the child for adult life. 
fin the education of gifted children, and of all others, for 
that matter, no subject should be included in the cur- 
riculum because of its supposed disciplinary value/* As 
Thorndike has said in this connection, " Whatever is 
valuable mainly as general training for the mind may be 
eliminated.! The education which enables great abilities 
to do their proper work will give sufficient general train- 
ing to their minds/ 7 If a gifted child happens to become 
interested in Latin, Greek, Hebrew, or Arabic, let him 
study it because of that interest and for the values which 
he may in later life derive from his knowledge of the lan- 
guage. Greek to a Greek professor is as utilitarian a tool 
as a noe is to a farmer. It should be kept in mind in the 
education of gifted children indeed in the education of 
all that no sharp line can be drawn between the utili- 
tarian and the cultural. 

254 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ENRICHMENT 

Rich Background of Associations 

I The special class provides an excellent opportunity 
for providing gifted children with a rich background of 
associations. Time which would in the typical hetero- 
geneous class be given to drill, or to doing nothing, can 
here be devoted to intellectual excursions of great interest 
and value. The gifted, more capable than the average 
in acquiring their experiences vicariously, will with a little 
direction cover an amazing amount of ground. In this 
process of collecting facts, so satisfying to the gifted 
child that frequently he reads the encyclopaedia just for 
the fun of it, care should be taken that some meaningful 
creative use be made of the information gathered./There 
are already too many people who possess the kind of 
intelligence which easily acquires and retains facts and 
yet are unable to integrate them. It is one thing for a child 
to know the name and date of a battle and quite another 
to have those facts woven into the pattern of his thinking. 
The following story will serve to illustrate an instance in 
which this transfer was made. 

A six-year-old boy with an LQ. of 170, who had al- 
ready read a good deal of history, was reporting at home 
one afternoon on a battle royal which had occurred at 
school that day. It appeared that one of the older boys 
had been maltreating a number of smaller children. On 
this day a gang from the primary grades banded together, 
attacked the bigger boy in a body, beat him soundly, and 
dragged him off. Said this gifted second-grader, who had 
participated in the fight, " We Waterlooed Danny/' This 
child's associative background waa functioning perfectly. 

f 
Standards of Achievement 

^fOne of the objections to leaving gifted children in 
heterogeneous classes is that in such classes the accepta- 

255 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

ble standards of achievement are necessarily low. In the 
special class high standards must be set and every effort 
made to see that the children reach themf Norms for the 
usual achievement tests have little value in gauging the 
accomplishments of highly gifted children beyond show- 
ing that they have mastered the regular work of the 
school up to a certain grade level. These tests reveal much 
concerning altitude of achievement but little concerning 
breadth. To evaluate properly the results of the enriched 
curriculum of the special class, tests which sample the 
contents of that curriculum must be used. 

It is an unwise procedure for any child to compare his 
school achievement with the achievement of his class- 
mates. It is much more unwise for a gifted than for an 
average child to do this. If he wishes to make compari- 
sons, let TIITYI do so with his own previous work; or, 
through his knowledge of biography, with the early 
achievement of men who became great. Such comparisons 
will tend to keep him modest and to spur him on to greater 
endeavors. 

Learning to Think 

tf A great deal has been said and written concerning the 
importance of teaching children how to think, f The 
results of attempts to instruct average and below-average 
children in the art of straight thinking are meager; as a 
matter of fact, they are none too positive with gifted 
children. Even the most intelligent of men arrive at their 
conclusions along pathways of emotion as well as of 
reason. Although intellectually gifted children and adults, 
even jurists and scientists, are by no means unemotional 
thinking machines, their conclusions are, nevertheless, 
based to a much greater degree upon processes of logic 
than are the conclusions arrived at by less intelligent 
individuals. 

256 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ENRICHMENT 

i Special-class children will be found eager to do their 
own thinking. /Although they should be encouraged in 
this self-reliance, they should also be made to realize that 
it is important, in doing one's own thinking on any prob- 
lem, to become familiar with the facts relating to that 
problem and to consider the opinions of authorities* Gifted 
children, having met with little competition from other 
children and often in conversation with adults found 
themselves the better informed, frequently tend to con- 
sider themselves as authorities. For instance, in connec- 
tion with the parliamentary discussion appearing in the 
preceding chapter, one of the boys remarked most con- 
fidently that some day he wanted to have a long talk with 
Prof. A on the education of gifted children, because he 
felt that the point of view of the professor, who is an 
authority in this field, was wrong, and he wanted to set 
him right. It was with some difficulty that the boy was 
convinced that before he argued the matter with Prof. 
A, or even with the members of the debating group, he 
should go to as many sources as possible to find out what 
research workers had learned and what leading educators 
thought concerning the question to be discussed. 

Development of Creative Resources 

Not all gifted children possess sufficient imagination to 
be truly creative. However, no child can be creative on a 
high level without being intellectually gifted. To illus- 
trate this point there is the case of the eleven-year-old 
girl who disliked school, was considered stupid by her 
teachers, and greatly preferred the out-of-doors, especially 
ranch life and horses. She developed a remarkable talent 
for sculpturing, filling her home with horses which she had 
made. One day she remarked that she could visualize 
every part, every muscle, of a horse's body. An intelli- 
gence test showed that she had an I.Q. of 145. Without a 

257 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

high level of intelligence she could not have visualized 
"every muscle in the horse's body." 

All subjects in the gifted child's curriculum can be 
made to serve as media for the development of his 
creative ability, with English probably being the most 
useful, since most gifted children are proficient in the use 
of language. Art and music are less promising, although 
the teacher should always stand ready to encourage the 
pupil who has a specialized talent. Creative imagination 
in any field is, by definition, original. The child who pos- 
sesses it should be encouraged in every way possible, 
but care should invariably be taken to leave him un- 
fettered by rigid requirements. 

Physical Exercise and Play 

I In "the education of gifted children emphasis should be 
placed upon physical exercise and play activities. These 
are especially needed by the bookish child of high mental- 
ity, the one who has become so entranced by the things of 
the mind that he has overlooked the importance of social 
development and of the need for a strong body. Physical 
exercise in the special room and play among the segre- 
gated children themselves should be a part of the daily 
program. Participation in the athletic and social activi- 
ties of the school as a whole should be required. The chil- 
dren in the special class have been segregated for purposes 
of instruction only; there is no reason why they should not 
participate in the normal school life of unselected children 

Training for Social Responsibilities 

Gifted children should learn early in life that the pos- 
session of superior mentality carries with it great re- 
sponsibilities. The human race has never been in greater 
need than it is today for men of superior intellect who are 
more eager to serve society than to serve themselves. Man 

258 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ENRICHMENT 

has built for himself, largely during the present century, 
great social and economic structures before which he now 
stands like the creator of Frankenstein before the colossal 
creature whom he had brought into being. The difficult 
problems which have followed in the wake of industrial- 
ized, urbanized life can be solved only by the best minds 
that can be mustered, and even they may not be equal to 
the task. Gifted children should be made conscious of 
these problems and given to understand that society looks 
to them for a solution. 

Drill 

One of the important modifications of methods in 
leaching a group of gifted children is the reduction of 
drill. This does not mean that drill should be eliminated, 
for even gifted children require a certain amount of 
repetition in mastering essentials. In the special class 
approximately one-half as much drill is needed as would 
be required for children of average mentality. This is a 
considerable saving both in time and in freedom from the 
boredom which a gifted child is likely to experience in a 
heterogeneous class. There are few situations so trying 
to an active, eager, questioning mind as to be forced to 
hear over and over a fact already known. 

Occasionally teachers or gifted children, somewhat too 
conscious of the impatience of their pupils to get on to 
difficult problems, have not required enough drill. As a 
result, the work of these children may be shot through 
with errors in such fields as arithmetical computation, 
spelling, and handwriting. For example, the Speyer group 
of gifted children, previously referred to, were unex- 
pectedly faced one day with the task of spelling causally 
with the following result: 

Ruth: causily 
Ernest: causely 

259 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

Donald: causilly 

Ernest: causuly 

Ruth: caussily illy 

Donald: tlly 

Nina: causelly 

Donald: causilly 

Ernest: causeley 

Donald: causilly 

Ruth: caussilly 

Miles: either causilly or causally 

Ernest: -ally! 

Miles: -ally 

New Subject Material 

{ In enriching the program of studies for the Speyer 
School group of gifted children, Leta Hollingworth 
added three new subjects: French, biography, and what 
she called "the evolution of common things." The study 
of biography is especially interesting and valuable for 
gifted children, who like to read about men and women 
who have achieved eminence and who like also to gather 
information about the social forces at work during the 
periods when the world's great figures were active. It 
contributes also to the social development of the child. 
Through knowing the lives of statesmen like Disraeli, 
Lincoln, Bismarck, Charlemagne, and Cardinal Richelieu, 
he understands more fully how to influence and direct 
others. By becoming familiar with the life stories of great 
scientists, he becomes conscious of the necessity of fre- 
quently making great personal sacrifices. It would be 
possible to build a complete curriculum for gifted children 
with the single subject, biography, as the core! Carlyle 
once said that "history is the essence of innumerable biog- 
raphies." This statement is true not only of history but 
also of art, literature, music, science, and all other fields 
of study./ 

' 260 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ENRICHMENT 

Emotional Education 

Leta Hollingworth, 1 in discussing a program of emo- 
tional education, lists five of the special problems of 
general conduct which most intelligent children face, and 
then considers ways of helping them to arrive at solu- 
tions. The five problems are as follows : (1) to find enough 
hard and interesting work at school, (2) to suffer fools 
gladly, (3) to keep from becoming negativistic toward 
authority, (4) to keep from becoming hermits, (5) to 
avoid the formation of habits of extreme chicanery. 

In connection with negativism toward authority, 
Hollingworth reports a conversation which she had with 
a ten-year-old boy, whose I.Q. was 165 and who had been 
sent to her as a school problem. 

What seems to be your main' problem in school? 

Several of them. 

Name one. 

Well, I will name the teachers. Oh, boy! It is bad enough when the 
pupils make mistakes, but when the teachers make mistakes, Oh, boy I 

Mention a few mistakes the teachers made. 

For instance I was sitting in 5 A and the teacher was teaching 5B. 
She was telling those children that the Germans discovered printing, 
that Gutenberg was the first discoverer of it, mind you. After a few 
minutes I couldn't stand it. I am not supposed to recite in that class, 
you see, but I got up. I said, "No; the Chinese invented, not dis- 
covered, printing, before the time of Gutenberg while the Germans 
were still barbarians." 

Then the teacher said, "Sit down. You are entirely too fresh." 
Later on she gave me a raking-over before the whole class. Oh, boy! 
What teaching! 

Professor Hollingworth granted that the teacher was 
in error, but urged the boy to remember that he must 

1 HOLLINGWORTH, L. S., "How Should a Democratic People Provide 
for the Selection and Training of Leaders in the Various Walks of life?," 
pp. 21-23, Advanced School of Education, Teachers College, Columbia 
University, 1938. 

261 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

learn to " suffer fools gladly." The boy, grasping in his 
resentment at the word "suffer," replied: 

Yes, that's it. That's what I say! Make ? em suffer. Roll a rock on 
'em. 

Summary 

The principles underlying the educational program of 
gifted children in a special class may be summarized as 
follows : 

1. A curriculum content based on the needs and interests of the 
children themselves 

2. An enriched associative background 

3. Insistence upon high standards of achievement 

4. Encouragement in independent thinking 

5. Instruction in scientific methods of attacking problems 

6. Provision of opportunities for doing creative work 

7. Requirement of participation in play and athletic activities 

8. A program of character training 

9. Training for coming social responsibilities 

10. Provision of opportunities for exploration 

11. Arrangement for increased use of libraries, museums, etc. 

12. Requirement of participation in the extracurricular activities 
of unselected children 

13. Reduction but not elimination of drill 

14. Inclusion in the program of studies of such subjects as French, 
biography, and argumentation 

15. A program of emotional education 

THE TEACHER 

The success of a special class for intellectually gifted 
children depends to a very large extent upon its teacher. 
The qualifications for leading a group of youthful 
geniuses include, of course, those which are necessary to 
good teaching everywhere, together with a few addition- 
al requirements which are needed to meet the exceptional 
situations inherent in the special class. 

262 



EDUCATIONAL ADJUSTMENTS: ENRICHMENT 

The teacher of gifted children should herself possess 
superior intelligence. She cannot be expected to have 
greater innate ability than the brightest child in her 
group, but surely she should excel the lowest in mental 
capacity. Being highly intelligent herself, she will pre- 
sumably possess a large fund of general information upon 
which she will be obliged to draw heavily in teaching a 
gifted group. She cannot be expected to know more than 
each and every child in her class, but she may be ex- 
pected to have a sufficiently wide range of knowledge to 
command the respect of the group. Anyone who has 
attempted to teach gifted children will realize how diffi- 
cult a requirement this is. 

The teacher of gifted children should be modest and 
yet confident. She should be interested in her pupils and 
enthusiastic about their projects. She should be as free 
from jealousy as is humanly possible, realizing at the out- 
set that it is not going to be a disgrace to discover that 
on many subjects her eight- or ten- or twelve-year-old 
charges are going to be better informed than she. This, 
too, is a difficult requirement and one rarely met even by 
teachers of special classes. 

Such a leader should be thoroughly trained in educa- 
tional psychology and in modern educational methods. 
She should have a thorough knowledge of individual 
differences and of the characteristics and problems of 
children of superior mentality. She should also have had 
teaching experience, preferably with normal children. 
She should, however, be open-minded and ready to make 
original approaches. Instruction in a special class can 
follow no set pattern. 

The qualifications of the teacher of intellectually 
gifted children in a special class may be summarized as 
follows : 

263 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

1. Intellectually gifted 

2. Well-informed 

3. Modest but confident 

4. Interested in children 

5. Free from jealousy 

6. Positive in personality 

7. In good mental and physical health 

8. Possessed of a sense of humor 

9. Open-minded 

10. Interested in social problems 

11. Thoroughly trained in pedagogical methods and in educational 
psychology, especially in the psychology of individual differences/ 

12. Experienced in teaching j 



264 



CHAPTER XII 

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A TYPICAL 
INTELLECTUALLY GIFTED CHILD 

Every individual is so uneven in his abilities that no 
one can be designated with scientific accuracy as "an 
average man" or as "a typical gifted child/' For prac- 
tical purposes, however, such descriptive phrases are 
acceptable. A feeble-minded child whose behavior, in 
general, conforms to that which is known to be char- 
acteristic of the majority of morons may be considered 
as being typical of that group. A man whose attainments 
approximate those described by Hollingworth earlier in 
this book may be looked upon as an average man. A 
child whose mental, physical, and social traits are similar 
to those which are known to be representative of preco- 
cious children may be considered as a typical gifted child. 

The biographical material which follows constitutes 
an attempt to construct a picture which conforms to 
established facts concerning the development of a gifted 
child during the first decade of his life. Robert Adams is 
not an actual child, but a hypothetical case; the story of 
his first ten years is, in a sense, a summary, a recapitula- 
tion of this book. However, all figures, tables, and quota- 
tions are authentic, being taken from case studies of 
gifted children whose I.Q.'s are approximately 150. It 
is especially important to keep this fact in mind with 
respect to the excerpts from the writings of gifted chil- 
dren which will be included in the following pages. These 
are in every instance reproduced exactly, with the excep- 

265 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

tion of proper names, which have been changed for 
obvious reasons. 

DEVELOPMENT DURING INFANCY 

Robert Adams was born on May 15, 1930. His parents 
were of Scotch-English descent. His father, who had been 
graduated from Harvard Law School in the top 10 per 
cent of his class, was a successful Boston attorney. His 
mother was a college graduate of moderate intellectual 
attainments. Robert's parents were not wealthy but had 
sufficient income to provide the cultural as well as the 
physical necessities of life. 

Robert was born in a hospital under normal conditions. 
His weight at birth was 7 pounds 14 ounces, nearly J 
pound more than the average weight of unselected boys 
at birth. His length was 19 inches, and the circumference 
of his head 12^ inches. The obstetrician in attendance 
pronounced the baby's condition to be excellent. At the 
end of a two weeks' stay in the hospital, he was taken 
home. 

The First Year 

By the end of the first month of his life Robert gave 
some indication of accelerated development. This was 
most noticeable with respect to motor ability. He had 
already performed such acts as rolling from stomach to 
side and from stomach to back. He had even lifted shoul- 
ders, chest, hips and legs from the table at one time with 
only his forearms and stomach resting on the table. 
He smiled occasionally, probably a manifestation of in- 
ternal contentment rather than of social understanding. 
When an adult stood near him and talked to him he 
watched intently and appeared to be pleased. When a 
colored rattle was moved vertically or horizontally or in 
a circle he followed it with his eyes. 

266 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

Early in the second month of his life Robert lifted his 
body completely off the bath table and rested on his 
forearms and knees. He hit a rattle which was held hang- 
ing about 4 inches from his face several successive times 
with his right hand. The movements appeared to be 
voluntary. At the age of about five weeks he could be 
depended upon to greet his mother with a social smile. 
During this second month he learned how to hold a spoon 
by the handle and would struggle toward a sitting posi- 
tion which, however, he was unable to attain. At the 
end of the second month he would intently regard his 
face in a mirror and occasionally smile at it; also, at 
the end of the second month, he was observed to carry 
a spoon to his mouth. 

At the end of the third month Robert's health was 
excellent, as indeed it had been since birth. He slept 
soundly for about fifteen hours out of each twenty-four. 
His sensorimotor development had reached a point 
which approximated that of an average child of between 
four and five months. He was distinctly aware of stran- 
gers at this age, crying if taken up by one. He watched 
individuals soberly, tending to concentrate on their 
eyes. 

At the age of four months Robert was able to sit 
alone and unsupported for brief periods of eight or ten 
seconds. He was able to pick up toys which were suffi- 
ciently near him, such as those on the tray of his high 
ch^ir. He was much more socially inclined than formerly, 
smiling and leaning toward any other child who came 
near. 

At the age of six months Robert's motor development 
was equal to that of the average eight-month-old child. 
In language he easily reached the eighth-month level, 
vocalizing in short, sharp syllables but not yet using 
words with meaning. His social behavior was not quite 

267 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

so advanced, being only a little in excess of what would 
be expected of a six-months-old baby. 

At the age of eleven months Robert was given the Kuhl- 
mann Intelligence Test and earned a mental age of 15.6 
months, which gave him a Kuhlmann I.Q. of 141. Tests 
given in later years showed this to be too low, for 
Robert's rating at an age when intelligence tests are most 




BHh 4 8 12 16 20 24 28 32 36 40 44 48 52 

Age, weeks 
FIG. 6. Weight chart of Robert's first year. 

dependable indicated an I.Q. of between 150 and 160. 
However, at the age of eleven months Robert passed all 
the tests on the six-month level, four of those on the 
twelve-month, three on the eighteen-month, and two on 
the two-year level. The child now used meaningfully two 
words, dada and bye. His health continued to be 
excellent. 

At the age of one year Robert had a vocabulary of 
twelve words, a somewhat larger number than is typical 
of an average child of eighteen months. The words which 

268 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

he used with full understanding were as follows: dada, 
mama, Nonna (for nursegirl), baby, bye-bye, bowwow, 
duck, flower, yes, no, boy, toes. Occasionally he put two 
of these words together in a phrase as " bye-bye dada." 
He could indicate on command his hair, ears, eyes, 
mouth, toes, and hands. At that age he had not yet learned 
to walk but could stand alone for a brief period. 

Robert's physical development during his first year, 
although by no means so accelerated as his mental 
development, was distinctly above average. Except for 
a brief period from his second to his tenth week, his 
weight at all times exceeded the norm for boys of his 
age (see Fig. 6). 

The Second Year 

Robert took three steps unassisted when he was thir- 
teen months of age. He continued to manifest marked 
facility in language, using the full sentence "I see you, 
dada" at the age of sixteen months. At the age of seven- 
teen months, he had a vocabulary of 74 words, which 
included the following: 

frog, flag, sand, shell, stone, water, bird, shoe, sock, suit, supper, 
sweater, wall, coat, milk, book, boat, horse, mouth, sunshine, towel, 
and soap. 

At the age of eighteen months he had added the follow- 
ing words: 

outdoors, gone, arm, foot, eold, door, fly, doctor, jar, hand, 
zipper, cereal, height, pajamas, apple, out, bear, parrot, truck, home, 
car, cauliflower, bowl, hair, chair, block, girl, shadow, spoon, joint, 
screw, barrel, button, telephone, jump, airplane, handkerchief, high 
chair, shelf, eggs, potato, paper, brush, belt, dress, bottle, button, 
world, rock, melon, light, cow, man, flashlight, pin, blanket, rain- 
drops, swing, bacon, hat, glasses, windmill, tree, blue, thank you. 

At the end of his second year Robert weighed 35 
pounds and was slightly less than 3 feet tall. He had 

269 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

maintained the physical superiority noted in infancy. 
His health had been very nearly perfect during the entire 
period, chicken pox constituting his only illness. He still 
manifested only slightly better-than-average sociability. 
Although he was cheerful and placid, he tended to be 
somewhat shy and cautious in his relations with stran- 
gers. In intellectual development he approximated that 
of the average three-year-old. He could enumerate 
objects in pictures without difficulty and could point 
upon request to various parts of his body. In language he 
showed a somewhat greater precocity than would be 
expected from his mental level. This special gift, so 
noticeable at the age of one year, when he had the 
unusual vocabulary of twelve words, had remained 
relatively constant. At the age of two years he could 
recognize all of the letters in the alphabet except 
q, x, and z, and spoke in phrases and sentences. 

On Robert's second birthday a record was made of 
everything that he said during a half-hour period. The 
boy was in his playroom among his books and toys at the 
time, which accounts for many of the bizarre references 
which appear in the following transcription. 

See geyser. Mountain. Another mountain. What's next the geyser? 
Waterfall. What's next to geyser? Boat. Eailroad tracks. Another 
waterfall. House. Another house. Grand canyon. Take a book out. 
Take a book out. Mummie take a big book up. Murnmie put the big 
book on the davenport. Mummie put the big book on the geyser. 
Bobby carry big book. Daddy take the green book out. See "B,." 
See "K." See "0." See another "0." See "B." What's there? 
Candle; balloons. What's there? Christmas tree; chu-chu train, 
'nother chu-chu train, tops, 'nother top, truck and another truck, 
and little boy. Lion. Little boys on wagons. Swing on gate. Horsie 
and another horsie; carriage; baby in carriage. Giraffe and zebra and 
monkeys and bears and lion. Stars. Daddy take other book out. 
Show letters. I see "A." I see "L." I see "E." I see "R." I see "V." 
Oh, ball! Play ball with daddy! Oh, my! See new truck. Bobby take 
truck down. Rooster make truck go. Pull truck. Daddy show clouds 

270 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

on the rug. Bobby carry truck downstairs. Make a truck go. Make a 
truck go. Make the rooster go. Flowers in picture, windows, other 
windows, doors, roof, chimney. Daddy pile blocks way up on top 
yellow block, now blue block, now orange block, now red block. Make 
a truck go. Make a rooster go. Engine, coal car, boxcar, caboose. 
Daddy make a top go. Mummie answer telephone. Doll in cradle. 
Put doll in cradle Dere's Tinkle. Daddy stop the auto, all go out, 
went walking along and walking along, came to window, looked in 
window and saw bunny. Daddy take the baby out. Put blanket on 
baby. Ball! Daddy pile blocks way up on top. Girls in shoe. Rooster; 
hen; chicken. See boat. Elephant, goat, dog, jaguar, horsie; now put 
the elephant in the dish. Make the truck go. Piggie and wolf. Daddy 
pile the blocks way up on top. Bout the outdoors again. Play ball 
with daddy. Bobby lay down side daddy on davenport and daddy 
tell stories. Put ball in truck and make truck go. Went outdoors. 
Wagon got all muddy. Put auto up on book and daddy tell about the 
pheasant. Daddy and Bobby looked at pheasant till auto came by 
and pheasant ran down over bank to river and daddy and Bobby 
couldn't see the pheasant more. Daddy got wood and daddy got 
paper and brought beside the fireplace and fire was hot. Wood was 
daddy's. Bobby played in living room and watched fire daddy made. 
Mummie smell flower. Put flower down on table. Make a truck go. 
Pull ball in truck and make the truck go. Daddy put the cows in 
barn. Bobby put the cow in the barn. Jump, jump in the barn. And 
all the cows. Daddy help the cow. Water, silo, tree, farmer. Put the 
cows in the barn, then the red cow. Daddy help the cow. Then the 
other red cow. Daddy help the cow. Daddy put the cow in the barn. 
Daddy put silo, farmer, cow, tree, another tree, another tree, another 
tree, another cow, another cow, another cow all back. 

PRESCHOOL PERIOD 

* During the next four years Robert continued to 
develop with striking constancy. In physical character- 
istics he maintained his superiority in height and weight, 
his weight being somewhat excessive for his height. With 
respect to motor coordination he was but little, if any, 
above average. This may have been the result of an 
inherited predisposition, his size, or his preoccupation 
with intellectual interests. Socially, especially during the 

271 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

latter part of the preschool period, Robert found it 
necessary to work out adjustments to problems which 
had arisen because of the difference between his mental 
age and that of his playmates. Their aimless movements 
and inability to concentrate on any game for more than a 
brief time disconcerted him. Although continuing to 
play with other children when opportunities arose, he 
took recourse in elaborately planned solitary games and 
in association with imaginary companions. This was in 
no sense abnormal, but rather a reasonable and adequate 
solution to some of his early social problems. 

Early Reading 

At the age of four Robert was given the Stanford 
Revision of the Binet-Simon Scale and earned an 
IQ. of 146. At this age Robert became interested in 
reading. He needed no teaching, but only someone to 
answer his questions. Earlier he had learned his letters 
from cereal packages and from advertisements which 
caught his eye. A considerable period elapsed between 
learning to identify letters and learning to read words, 
but shortly before his fourth birthday he began to ask 
questions concerning the signboards which he saw as he 
rode along with his parents on automobile trips. Because 
signboards were his primer, the first three words which he 
learned to read happened to be "Four Roses Whiskey. 77 

The boy 7 s library was well stocked, and in a short time 
he began reading simple books. Since he was especially 
gifted linguistically, he soon mastered these and went on 
to more difficult material. By the age of five, he had 
read the Gates-Huber and the Elson-Gray Readers as far 
as Book Three. At five, he became interested in pre- 
historic animals and read eight or ten children 7 s books in 
this field. He even attempted to gather information 
from adult books such as Wells' "Outline of History." 

272 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

Shortly before and for some time after his sixth birth- 
day, Robert's interests made a natural transition from 
prehistoric animals to geography. His parents, who never 
forced him but stood ready to encourage and to supply 
source material on whatever subject at the time inter- 
ested him, gave him a globe and two atlases. These were 
soon worn out and new ones had to be supplied. The child 
amused himself by drawing rough maps and by learning 
the location of all the countries in the world, their capitals 
and more important cities, their rivers, mountains, and 
climates. 

The study of prehistoric animals and of geography had 
aroused his interest in the various eras and he referred 
to them by name, easily and naturally, his sense of time, 
like that of most gifted children, being excellent. Rob- 
ert's ability to handle figures, however, was not nearly 
so marked as his sense of time or his facility in language. 
This was presumably due in part to the fact that his 
parents were giving him no formal training; to compute 
accurately requires drill. 

Achievement in School Subjects 

Although standardized achievement tests do not ade- 
quately sample the breadth of a gifted child's fund of 
knowledge, they do indicate where he stands in compari- 
son to other children with respect to his grasp of essential 
school information. During the summer after Robert 
was six, he was given " a Stanf ord-Binet Intelligence 
Test and several achievement tests from the Stanford 
battery. The results appear in Table XXXII. 

There are a number of facts of interest in Table 
XXXII. It can be seen that Robert's educational age 
almost exactly equaled his mental age in spite of the fact 
that he had never attended school. His grade level, at a 
chronological age of 6 years 3 months, was 3.8. This had 

273 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

been raised somewhat by his exceedingly high score on 
the geography test. In language usage, also, he was 
exceptionally superior, a natural outgrowth of growing 
up in a home of culture. He rated lowest in arithmetic 
computation, but even in this subject he proved that 
he was ready to begin third-grade work. All Robert's 
information had been gathered incidentally without 
formal training of any kind. The gifted mind learns 
without apparent effort. 

TABLE XXXII. ROBEBT'S RATING ON CERTAIN OF THE STANFORD 

ACHIEVEMENT TESTS 
C.A., 6 years 3 months, M.A., 9 years 6 montlis 



rp__j._ 


E.A. 


Grade 


lests 


9.4 


3.8 


Geography 


Yrs. Mos. 
10-10 


4.9 


Spelling 


10- 6 


4.6 


Language usage 


10- 7 


4.6 


Reading (average) 


9- 2 


3.6 


To note details 


10- 1 


4.4 


Word meaning 


10- 


4.15 


Word meaning 


9- 2 


3.45 


Understand directions 


9- 4 


3.8 


To predict outcomes . . 


9- 


3.5 


Paragraph rn finning . .... 


9- 2 


3.45 


Paragraph meaning 


8- 9 


3.35 


General significance 


8-10 


3.4 


Sentence reading 


8- 9 


3.35 


Word recognition . 


8- 9 


3.3 


Arithmetic reasoning 


9- 


3.3 


Arithmetic computation 


8- 7 


3.1 









During the summer of 1936, when Robert was six, 
his parents began to wonder what would happen when 
he went to school. Would the first-grade material be so 
boring to him that he would lose his intellectual inter- 
ests? Should an attempt be made to persuade the school 

274 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

principal to place him in a grade commensurate with 
his mental age? If this were done, would he be able to 
make the necessary social adjustments? 

ELEMENTARY SCHOOL PERIOD 

Robert's parents decided that they would make no 
effort to have Robert begin his school experience in an 
advanced grade. Consequently he began his work as a 
first-grade pupil. There was, of course, little for him to do 
insofar as subject matter was concerned, but he was 
tremendously interested in the social aspects of his new 
world. The interrelationships existing among the other 
pupils fascinated him. He was acquiring nothing new 
academically, but he was learning very valuable social 
lessons. 

The organization of the school was as interesting a 
problem to him as anything in mathematics could have 
been. He was eager to learn all the rules and to know 
just how everything was done. He was equally interested 
in observing various means by which children broke these 
rules with impunity or, if they were caught, in what 
manner they were punished. 

His knowledge of what should be done exceeded his 
ability to do it; his social intelligence was greater than 
his sociability. At first his classmates looked upon him as 
being a little odd, but, because he was naturally warm 
and eager, they soon learned to accept him. 

Dictation as a Medium of Expression 

During this period Robert was obtaining a valuable 
social education at school and an intellectual education 
at home. He had added to his interest in reading a delight 
in dictating stories. At the age of five he had dictated a 
series of travel narratives about a boy of three which 
eventually reached about 8,000 words in length. The 

275 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

speaking vocabulary of a young gifted child is considera- 
bly greater than his writing vocabulary. Moreover, his 
mind works so much more rapidly than his hands that 
he is likely to become impatient with the mechanical 
business of putting down thoughts on paper. Conse- 
quently, in many ways the material which a gifted child 
dictates is a better indication of his linguistic ability 
and of his capacity to think than is the material which 
he actually writes. 

A comparison of the results of the two methods of 
self-expression is interesting. When Robert was six-and- 
a-half years of age he wrote the following on a typewriter : 

STOKY OF THE PIGEON 

In Winter days the pigeons were quite cold. One day the leader of 
the pigeons was flying over the top of a bidding. He was swooping 
quite close to the bulding. In a moment he saw some sheltered 
lowered eaves where they could sleep during the cold Winter days. 
Then he told all the other pigeons about it. They shouted goody, 
goody we can lie there until Winter is over. After that they thought 
he was the best pigeon in the world. 

At about the time when Robert wrote the above brief 
story, he dictated a dozen or fifteen much longer ones. 
Of course, in the dictated material, the spelling and 
punctuation is that of the one dictated to; the words, 
however, are Robert's own, exactly as he spoke them. 
The following is a sample of one of the shorter, dictated 
stories: 

How SPECKLED HOBSIE CAME TO ME 

Back in the olden days of the Eocene Period there lived a family 
of three-toed horsies. All died but one called Speckled Horsie. He 
lived on through the Oligocene and Miocene Periods. In the ice age 
of the Pliocene Period he had quite a struggle to live but he dug a hole 
before -the Ice Age was at its height, gathered some food, and went 
down in it so he did not suffer very much. After the ice went back, he 
came out and lived on and on, on, on, on, on, on, on, on, on, on, on, 

276 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

on, until at last one time he was standing on the banks of the Atlantic 
Ocean near Chesapeake Bay's mouth when down swooped Santa 
Glaus on the night of the twenty-third of December, 1936. 

"Will you let me take you up to my observatory at the North 
Pole?" asked Santa. 

"I'd love to go up there," said the Little Horsie. 

So the next night, which was Christmas Eve, Santa Claus swooped 
down to Boston, taking Speckled Horsie as a present to a little boy 
who was living there. But in a letter the boy had left at the foot of 
his bed he said he wanted part of his presents to be left at his other 
home in Vermont. So Santa Claus took the Speckled Horsie to 
Vermont and left him there, 

On Christmas day the little boy found Speckled Horsie and has 
always kept him and loved him ever since, especially because he is 
so old. 

Accelerated and Retarded 

After the Christinas holidays of his first year in school, 
Robert, upon the recommendation of the principal, was 
accelerated to the second grade. The work here was 
almost as easy for him as it had been in the first grade. 
It did include, however, some materials in arithmetic 
upon which he found it necessary to drill. He was now 
accelerated a year and a half, taking chronological age 
as a reference point, but retarded about the same amount 
in terms of mental age. In physical size he slightly 
exceeded the average boy in the second grade; socially, 
he was somewhat younger. 

Continued Interest in Writing 

Robert maintained his interest in writing, continuing 
to do somewhat better than would be expected of a 
child with an I.Q. of 152. Shortly before his seventh 
birthday, he wrote the following letter to his father. 
(This was written, not dictated.) 

Dear Daddy, 

I liked your letter very much. I hope you will write me about the 
mines. 

277 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

I like the sun lamp quite a lot only it gets me very hot, and I am 
peeling from it. 

Mother bought me "Robinson Crusoe." I began it where his ship 
got wrecked and I liked that quite a lot. I liked also where the boat 
comes and rescues him. I think he should have gotten everything for 
himself instead of from the ship. 

I am perfectly well and could go to school but Mother has a little 
cold and Mary has a bad one so there is nobody to take me. I am 
looking after them, and I went downstairs to get the mail and up- 
stairs for the menu. 

Mother said you were starting April 27 so when does that mean 
you'll be back? 

With love, 
Bobby 

Robert's ability in art was slightly below that of 
average children of the same age. With the self-criticism 
which is typical of gifted children, he came home one 
day and remarked that he was glad that his teacher had 
taken the pupils' Easter paintings from the wall because 
his was the worst one there. 

Robert, like most children, was interested in keeping 
a diary. Gifted children differ from those who are 
average in that they begin their diaries at an earlier age, 
and keep them in somewhat greater detail. Robert 
began his before he was six and continued with it for 
about two years, at the end of which time his interest 
waned. The following is his entry for May 11, four days 
before his seventh birthday. 

I woke up this morning at 7:56H and got up to read at 8:11. 1 got 
up at 9:00 and got dressed. Then I had breakfast without Hot-Cross 
Buns. Then I looked at my Atlas until quarter of twelve. Yesterday 
night Mother and I made an agreement that I should only drink a 
quart of milk a day because I had been drinking over two quarts of 
milk. (But return to today's history) Then I went for a walk just 
after it rained. I watched the make-up vessels on the "Midlaw archi- 
pelago of rivers" in the gutters, and went home. Then we talked 
about Germany and had rest. I am going to write a list of the coun- 

278 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

tries' empires after I finish this. We read "Hiawatha" last night and 
read a lot of funny names, here are some of them, Gitche-Manito, 
the Mighty, (the great Spirit) The Ojibways (an Indian tribe) 
Mudjekewis (an Indian) Kaebyun (the west wind) Wabun (the east 
wind) Kabibanokka (the north wind) Shawandasee (the south wind) 
Mishe-Maukwa (the great bear of the Mountains) Shingabis (the 
diver) . 

Reading Interests 

During the summer after he was seven in May Robert 
lived a normal, active life, playing whenever possible 
with other children, especially with those who were some- 
what older. He continued to read and to write a great 
deal. His own library at this time numbered approxi- 
mately 300 books. In addition, he had the unrestricted 
use of his father's library. Robert kept in a bookcase in 
his bedroom the volumes which were his favorites. They 
constituted a catholic collection, ranging from the very 
youthful "Four Little Kittens" to Sir James Jean's 
"Through Space and Time." Of course this seven-year- 
old boy was incapable of reading and understanding all 
of "Through Space and Time." There were pages in it, 
however, which he could read and understand and which 
excited his imagination and curiosity. Robert had read 
many of these favorite books of his several times. Among 
the volumes which were given this repeated attention 
were "Science Stories, Book III," "The House at Pooh 
Corner," "The Bastable Children," "Heroes of Civiliza- 
tion," "Grammar Can Be Fun," "Homes and Habits of 
Wild Animals," "Robinson Crusoe," "New Pictorial 
Atlas of the World," "World Atlas," "Sonny Elephant," 
"The Earth for Sain," "Men Who Found America," 
"Wind in the Willows," "Famous Explorers," and "The 
Jungle Books." Of these the first of the two "Jungle 
Books" was named by Robert as being the book in his 
library which he liked best of all. The complete list of 

279 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

favorite books chosen from among several hundred by 
this gifted boy was as follows: 

"Aesop's Fables" 

"Alice in Wonderland" 

"A Modern Journey Put-together Book" 

"Animal Pets" 

"Art Stories, Books II and III," by William G. Whitford, Edna 
B. Liek, and William S. Gray 

"The Bastable Children," by E. Nesbit 

"Beasts of the Tarpits," by W. W. Robinson and Irene B. Robin- 
son 

"The Beginners' American History," by David Montgomery 

"Cinderella" 

"The Earth for Sam," by W. Maxwell Reed 

"Electricity Comes to Us," by Rose Wyler and Warren W. 
McSpadden 

"Fairy Grammar," by J. Harold Carpenter and Alice Hoben 

"Famous Explorers," by Jennie S. Kates 

"Four Little Kittens" 

"Four Little Bunnies" 

"Four Little Puppies" 

"Grammar Can Be Fun," by Munro Leaf 

Hammond's "Modern Atlas of the World" 

"Hans Andersen's Fairy Tales" 

"Heroes of Civilization," by Joseph Cottier and Hayna Jaffe 

"Homes and Habits of Wild Animals," by Karl Patterson Schmidt 

"The House at Pooh Corner," by A. A. Milne 

"The Junior Outline of History," by I. 0. Evans 

"Manners Can Be Fun," by Munro Leaf 

"Maple Sugar Time," by Royce S. Pitkin 

"Men who Found America," by Frederick Winthrop Hutchinson 

"Motor Days and Motor Ways" 

"New Pictoral Atlas of the World" 

"Old Rhymes for all Times," by Cicely Mary Barker 

"Robinson Crusoe" 

"The Rootabaga Stories," by Carl Sandburg 

"Safety Can Be Fun," by Munro Leaf 

"Science Stories, Book III," by Wilbur L. Beauchamp, Harriet 
M. Fogg, and William S. Gray 

"Seacoast Region of New Hampshire" 

280 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

"Sonny Elephant," by Madge A. Bingham 

"Story of Earthquakes and Volcanoes," by Gaylord Johnson 

"The Sun-egg," by Elsa Beskow 

"Sunshine and Rain," by Willard Frasier and Helen Dolman 

"Talking Leaves," by Julius King 

"Through Space and Time," by Sir James Jeans 

"Trains and Ships" 

"The Two Jungle Books," by Rudyard Kipling 

"Wait for William," by Marjorie Flack 

"Wind in the Willows," by Kenneth Graham 

"World Atlas" 

Intellectual Status at Age Seven Years Six Months 

When Robert was 7 years 6 months of age, he was given 
a Stanford-Binet intelligence test. The four I.Q.s ob- 
tained from previous examinations were: 141 (Kuhl- 
mann), 146 (S-B), 150 (S-B), and 152 (S-B). At the age 
of seven and a half Robert passed all the tests on the ten- 
year level of the Stanford-Binet Scale. 

RESPONSES TO TESTS ON TEN-TEAK LEVEL 

1. Vocabulary: Robert gave satisfactory definitions of twenty 
words. A few of these, verbatim, follow: 

a. puddle: a very little pool of water. 

b. envelope: Something you put a letter into and a stamp on. 
Really a paper folded. 

c. copper: What pennies are made of. A kind of metal. 

d. dungeon: A deep dark prison underground. 

e. nerve: Something in your body that carries messages back 
and forth. 

/ Mars: Mars is a planet. 

g. priceless: Something so costly that it can hardly be paid for. 

2. Absurdities: 

a. Answer: It couldn't be. If it were down hill all the way to 
the city, it would have to be up hill all the way back home. 

6. Answer: That isn't true. The more cars he had the slower he 
would go, because there would be more weight. 

c. Answer: She couldn't have killed herself. Somebody else 
would have had to cut her into eighteen pieces. 

d. Answer: I think it would be serious if 48 people were killed. 

281 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

3. Drawing Designs from Memory: Robert reproduced each of these 
correctly. 

4. Reading and Report: Although 35 seconds are allowed for the 
reading of the passage in this test, Robert read it without error in 
20 seconds. He remembered twelve of the specific details read. 

5. Comprehension: Robert gave satisfactory answers to all three 
of the questions included in this test. In his reply to &, he explained at 
length how he would prepare his men if he were going on an explor- 
ing trip, or how he would think out in detail a story that he was 
going to write. 

6. Naming Sixty Words: Robert named 82 words during the 
allotted 3 minutes, organizing them into related groups. 

Robert passed the first, second, fourth, sixth, and 
eighth tests on the twelve-year level and the second 
on the fourteen-year level. This gave him a mental age 
of 11 years 7 months and an I.Q. of 154. 

At the time when the above test was given Robert was 
in the third grade, accelerated in terms of chronological 
age, though considerably retarded in terms of mental 
age. His interest in observing the workings in the educa- 
tional machinery had waned and he was becoming 
impatient with the hours which he had to spend in wait- 
ing for the other children to do their work. Although he 
was still not so advanced in arithmetic as in other 
subjects, he was ahead of everyone in his grade and was 
usually the first to complete the day's assignment. Then 
he waited until the others finished or used the time in 
writing comments or in working out his own enrichment 
techniques. The following sample is an exact copy of one 
day's notation. The words in the left hand column are 
those which he listed as ones to be included in a paragraph 
which he then set himself to write. 

class In my class the children have 

children trouble with seven times four. Our 

trouble teacher has gone over it about 

times forty thousand times. I hope you 

282 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

teacher ' know the answer! Twenty-eight. 

forty When she goes around the class in 

thousand numberwork, when she comes to 

answer seven times four, the pupils always 

eight stop. No amount of going over can 

around make them remember. 1 hope some 

numberwork day they will be able to say it 

pupils quickly. 

stop 

amount 

going 

remember 

hope 

quickly 

Robert continued, however, to find at home satisfac- 
tions for his eager mind. Among his voluminous papers, 
this item on Copernicus was found : 

CHEEBFUL COPERNICUS 
1473^1543 

"What!" everybody cried "the earth not the center of the universe. 
We great men not the center of attraction. We won't believe it. You 
say we are of insignificant in size. You take us away from our shining 
center, you make us wabble around the sun, like a moth around a 
lamp. Outrageous!" (This was all about 1503 J 

Copernicus knew outrageous meant foolish! So he couldn't help 
laughing. "It is true" said Copernicus at last very cheerfully, almost 
laughing. "And" continued Copernicus "You will believe it in time 
when someone else as great as I will make you believe it. For I am 
great and shall not be forgotten." 

The quotation in the first paragraph of the preceding 
passage was written down as Robert remembered it from 
a book which he had read. The remaining material is 
his own comment. 

283 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

Ability in Music 

During the year following his seventh birthday Robert 
began taking lessons in piano. He progressed at a 
moderate rate of speed but showed no unusual talent. 
He was much more interested in composing music than 
in playing it. His compositions possessed some promise 



S? 



afegp 



SB* 



(Jf Li 


* 


I j j ; 








h 


*M ' 


\ 


^M~- 








"r 




TT 


f s f 


-]- 


4- 









) 


i 




t \ 








*- / I "> 









/ / 


a ( 


B? 






/ "ft 




A 




\ 


"i 


7 4 







p 




J 



t 



Fia. 7. Composition by Robert at seven years of age. 

but showed no indications of musical genius. In a period 
of three months he composed twenty-two songs. These 
like his stories were an outgrowth of intellectual energy 
and of an urge to create. One of Robert's compositions is 
reproduced in Fig. 7. It is included here because it indi- 
cates the kind of work that can be done by a seven- 
year-old gifted boy who is not especially talented in 
music. 

284 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

First Chapter of a Book 

At about the time of Ms eighth birthday, Robert 
decided that he would begin a book. He refused to write 
it in longhand, saying that it would take too long. He 
said that he had carefully planned the entire story and 
wanted to tell it in the quickest way possible. Conse- 
quently an adult member of his family consented to act 
as stenographer. 

It is true that dictated material does not possess the 
scientific accuracy that written material does. However, 
as was pointed out earlier, it does present a better picture 
of the richness of a gifted child's thoughts, for in dictating 
his mind can run free, unhampered by the necessity 
of coordinating the flow of ideas with the mechanics of 
writing. The phrasing in Robert's "The Adventures of 
Two Little Turtles" is everywhere his own. However, 
as he dictated, he was asked leading questions which 
tended to cause him to elaborate on his theme somewhat 
more than he would otherwise have done. The spelling 
and punctuation are, of course, those of the adult who 
wrote down what Robert dictated. The story eventually 
ran to 15,000 words. The first chapter follows: 

THE ADVENTURES OF Two LITTLE TURTLES 
Chapter One 
The Escape 

Once upon a time there were two turtles living with a little boy in 
Whitingham, Vermont. They lived in a bowl which was black and 
they had two rocks to rest on, one for each turtle. They were fed 
ants' eggs mixed with other things, and once in a while they were 
given lettuce. Lettuce was like candy to them because they hardly 
ever had it. 

One turtle was bigger than the other and the other turtle was littler 
than the other. Their shells were white. The bigger turtle had a crow 
painted on him and under the crow were the words Wizard of Oz. 

285 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

On the littler turtle's shell were painted two bunnies and beside them 
were the words Alky Oop. 

Before they had been put in the big black bowl they had been put 
in an aluminum pan. The little boy put them in the black bowl be- 
cause Alley Oop had jumped out of the aluminum pan. Before they 
were in the pan, they were in a Maxwell House coffee can, which got 
rusty. Before they were bought by the little boy, they were in a big 
glass bowl in a florist's shop, and that was all the little boy knew 
about them. 

One night about quarter past eleven Wizard of Oz said sleepily to 
Alley Oop, "Do you remember when we used to live in Mexico with 
our dear mother and father?" 

"I don't remember very clearly/' said Alley Oop, even more 
sleepily, "because I didn't have my eyes open when we came away. 
But I remember what you have told me about it." 

"I was just thinking about it," said Wizard of Oz, stretching. "It 
was very sad we had to come away. I wonder if we could ever get 
back again." 

"Oh, we never could, I don't believe," said Alley Oop. "We're 
very far from Mexico." 

"We might try," said Wizard of Oz, wide awake now. "Let 'me 
think. . . . I've seen people come in and go out of those holes over 
there. Why cannot we go out, too?" 

"That's an idea," said Alley Oop. "But how shall we get through? 
They're all closed now." 

"Well," said Wizard of Oz, "I don't know myself. But I was just 
thinking that if we could get out of this bowl, we might go over there 
and see if we could find a way to get through, and start back to 
Mexico." 

"But if we did get through," said Alley Oop, thoughtfully, "we'd 
need a map to find our way by along the roads, and we'd need to 
know how to keep from getting killed by those bangs we hear outside." 

"Yes, I had my eyes open before we left Mexico," said Wizard of 
Oz, "and mother showed me clearly how to make a map of the United 
States and Mexico. So I can draw us a map on this rock. Then we 
can listen to anything wise that the mother tells her little boy when- 
ever he is going out, and I can write the rules down for us. We'll see 
tomorrow night if we can get out of this bowl." 

"That is a fine idea," said Alley Oop, getting sleepy again, "I'm 
going down to the bottom of the bowl and get a nap before we 
start." 

286 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

"While you're doing that," said Wizard of Oz, "I'll make our 
map. You be sure to get a good sleep because we're going to have a 
very busy day tomorrow." 

So Wizard of Oz made the map. He showed the Atlantic Coast 
states, the southern end of Mississippi and Alabama, the north, east, 
and south of Texas, and all of Mexico, and the Gulf of California. 
On the western side of Mexico he put a big square, half in the Gulf 
and half on land, and beside that he wrote, "Where We Used to Live 
and Where We Want to Live Again." He did not draw any lines 
because he knew he and Alley Oop would not follow roads. They would 
have to go into the woods sometimes to find water, and they would 
travel where it was woody because turtles like that kind of country. 

By the time he finished the map, it was seven o'clock in the morn- 
ing, and Alley Oop woke up. 

"Good morning, Wizard of Oz," he said. "Have you made your 
map?" 

"Yes," Wizard of Oz answered. "Now you remember we are 
going to listen very carefully to what the mother says to the little 
boy when he goes out to play." 

All that forenoon they didn't hear any rules at all. Wizard of Oz 
was a little worried. But that afternoon the mother wanted the little 
boy to go down to his grandmother's on an errand. Wizard of Oz's 
ears were practically falling off he was listening so hard, hoping to 
hear some rules. At last he heard the mother say, "Be careful. Look 
out for the cars. Stop and look in all directions before you cross the 
road." 

"Thank goodness," said Wizard of Oz, and he wrote down on the 
rock which he had not used for his map these words in big letters: 

Look out for cars. Stop and look in all directions before crossing 
a road. t 

"Have you got any rules yet?" asked Alley Oop, who had got 
tired from listening so long and had been dozing on the far side of the 
bowl, 

"Yes, sleepyhead," said Wizard of Oz. "Now you wake up, and 
stay waked upl We've got a big business. I've got one rule at least. 
Now let me tell you that! 

"Look out for cars. Stop and look in all directions before crossing a 
road. 

"Now you come over here, and stay here, and listen for rules, while 
I eat, And don't fall asleep, either!" 

"I'll try very hard," said Alley Oop, good-naturedly. 

287 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

So Alley Oop listened. Whenever he felt sleepy, he shook the 
sleepiness out of him. But he didn't hear any rules. When Wizard of 
Oz finished eating, Alley Oop ate, and Wizard of Oz listened. But he 
didn't hear any rules. 

Now it was almost six o'clock. 

"Next, Alley Oop," said Wizard of Oz, "we'd better each eat a piece 
of lettuce and go down to the bottom of the bowl and sleep. I'll wake 
you up at ten o'clock. And try to wake up fast, when I call you, 
because we're leaving at eleven o'clock!" 

After a while, the clock struck ten, Wizard of Oz had been awake 
about five minutes and was just getting the sleepiness out of him 
when he heard Alley Oop say, "I'm awake! You needn't wake me 
up!" 

"What a smart Alley Oop!" said Wizard of Oz. "You woke up 
early, for you!" 

"I woke up of excitedness," said Alley Oop. 

Wizard of Oz said, "Well " 

"I shall be so glad to get out of this water," chattered Alley Oop. 
"They haven't changed it for a quite a long time, and it's dirty." 

"I think it's quite dirty, too," laughed Wizard of Oz, "but be a 
little patient. We'll be in fresh, clean waters on our journey. But now 
we've got to get ready. I've got to see if I can balance those two rocks 
on my shell." 

"But what can I take?" asked Alley Oop, all excited. "I can't go 
without carrying anything." 

"You're to take the food," replied Wizard of Oz. 

"But the food box isn't in the bowl," said Alley Oop. "How can I 
tell whether I can balance it?" 

"Be patient. Be patient. Be patient," said Wizard of Oz. "You ask 
me one question right after another. I can't answer them so fast. I 
have to think! But I can answer this one. As soon as I see if I can 
balance those two rocks, we're going out and get the food box. That 
will prove two things: first, that we can get out of the bowl, and, 
second, that we can carry our supplies." 

"All right, but let's hurry," said Alley Oop. 

Wizard of Oz turned around, and found the rocks. He lay down 
between the rocks in the bottom of the bowl. 

"Please push one of the rocks onto the front of my shell, close to 
my head," he said to Alley Oop. 

Alley Oop did, but it was quite hard work because Wizard of Oz 
was just about Alley Oop's size when he lay down. 

288 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

Then Wizard of Oz said, "Now please put the other rock on top of 
that." 

Alley Oop had to stretch very hard to get that rock on, but he 
did. 

"Now we'll see if I can walk with them on my shell," said Wizard 
of Oz. 

He got up on his legs slowly and carefully and took a short step, 
then a few others, and his steps grew a little longer as he got used 
to his load. At last he felt sure that he could balance it all the way to 
Mexico if he could get out of the bowl. 

"Now, Alley Oop/ 7 said Wizard of Oz, "You climb up and hang 
onto those rocks so they won't fall off. Push them right against my 
head. And I'll give you a ride." 

"Wonderful," said Alley Oop excitedly, so he climbed on. 

Then Wizard of Oz stretched, and stretched and stretched, very 
hard and carefully, and at last he got both his front paws on the side 
of the bowl. Even more carefully, he pulled his body with its heavy 
load up over the side of the bowl and hung on with his back feet, 
holding his load against the back of his head, until he got his front 
feet onto the shelf which the bowl stood on. 

"All right," he said then, panting. "You can get off now, Alley Oop. 
When I'm on a level the rocks will stay balanced." 

"With all this pulling and stretching and shoving and bouncing," 
said Alley Oop, "I've forgotten what we've come out for." 

"Well, if you aren't light-headed," said Wizard of Oz. "We've 
come out to go to Mexico! Now see if you can balance the food box, 
you little dummy!" 

Wizard of Oz took Alley Oop's paw and they started walking across 
the rug toward one of three holes which sometimes were open but now 
were closed. Wizard of Oz did not walk as fast as he could because he 
was leading Alley Oop, who could not go quite so fast. Finally they 
came to one of the closed holes. 

"I wonder if it pushes open," said Wizard of Oz. 

He pushed and pushed, but it did not open. 

"I wonder if you have to pull it," suggested Alley Oop. 

"That's an idea," said Wizard of Oz. "It isn't tight closed. I can 
get my paw in the crack and pull." 

And the first hard pull he gave swish! the door came open! 

"You showed us how to get out, Alley Oop," said Wizard of Oz. 
"That was pretty smart of you. I hope you'll make suggestions like 
that all the way to Mexico." 

289 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

But Alley Oop was too little to understand praise and did not ex- 
actly know what Wizard of Oz was talking about. So he tried to 
change the subject. 

"We're in a very big room," he said. 

"Not so terribly big," answered Wizard of Oz. "Wait until you see 
how big the world is ! 

Now they walked toward another crack. 

"We'll try pulling this/ 7 said Wizard of Oz. 

And he pulled very hard, and heard shk-shk-shk. Then the door 
came open. 

"Here's a big step, down," said Wizard of Oz. "We shall have to 
stretch for this one!" 

And now they were in the wide world. 

"It's very dark," said Alley Oop. 

"But there is light above your head," said Wizard of Oz. "Look 
up!" 

So Alley Oop looked up. 

He saw many lights. Nearly all were little and round with points on 
them. Those seemed to be winking their eyes at him. A few were round 
balls without points and these did not wink at him. Perhaps they 
didn't have any eyes. And there was one very large ball with no points 
but with zigzag marks all through it. That gave the most light of all. 

"They are funny lights," said Alley Oop, "but they are quite 
beautiful." 

"And they will light us all the way to Mexico," said Wizard of Oz. 

So the two little turtles set out into the wide world. 

The preceding story is sufficiently unusual to indicate 
a special gift. Most of the greatest contributions to civili- 
zation have been made by those individuals who, inherit- 
ing and developing a special ability, were able to make 
full use of it because it rested upon a base of high intelli- 
gence. The man with a special gift but with inferior 
mental capacity is likely to become either eccentric or 
neurotic; the man with superior mental capacity but 
without a special gift may scatter his energies or, per- 
haps, become a nonproductive scholar. Genius might well 
be defined as the possession of great mental capacity 
together with a special ability. 

290 



BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF A GIFTED CHILD 

In the Middle Grades 

Robert entered the fourth grade in the September after 
he was eight. His class was made up of unselected children. 
His teacher made a few tentative efforts at differentiation. 
Robert was encouraged in his writing and reading but 
otherwise was given little opportunity either for accelera- 
tion or for enrichment. This situation continued during 
the remaining two of the ten years of his life here being 
considered. Robert acquired almost no academic benefits 
from his years in the early grades. However, the social 
education which he received in school was invaluable. 
He learned how to get along with other children and how 
to lead by stratagem instead of by force. He learned the 
routine of institutional activities and came to understand 
how valuable it is to save personal strain by relaxing 
within the framework of a system. 

Association with other boys brought with it an interest 
in physical activities, including wrestling and fighting. 
This new interest was reflected in a few of the stories 
which Robert wrote during this period. These were not so 
ruminative, so introspective, as the earlier ones had been, 
but showed delight in rapid action. The following para- 
graphs are taken from one of these stories: 

So on he went to Big Horn Creek, where he found hoof marks that 
forded the creek. He followed these up the canyon, until the going 
was so rough he had to dismount. All of a sudden a roar sounded up 
the canyon. Down came a landslide right in front of him! Up the high 
wall of the canyon Jim jumped to a path barely a foot wide with a 
thousand foot drop on the left side, and the side of the mountain 
straight up on the right side. 

Suddenly a rock crashed down and wiped out the trail behind Jim. 
" What a pretty mess I'm in now! Guess I'll go on ahead." 

Jim walked on a ways till he came to a cave. As he walked in a 
voice ordered, " Reach fer the sky, Cowboy!" You can bet Jim 
reached for the sky, and said, "Well, what do you want? I suppose 

291 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

you're the guys that burned my place up. Well, that's what I call a 
darn neat job for a bunch of polecats like you. I'll sure skin you rats 
when I get away!" 

AT THE END OF TEN YEARS 

When Robert reached the end of the first decade of his 
life, he was, as he had always been, a strong, healthy boy 
with a positive, striking personality. His intellect cor- 
responded roughly to that of the average child of fifteen. 
He was original and resourceful and showed considerable 
ability in applying his intelligence to concrete life situa- 
tions. He was very ambitious and yet critical of himself, 
properly evaluating his defects as well as his assets. He 
had supreme confidence in his intellectual powers but was 
not arrogant. He wanted to be a leader and, although not 
the most popular boy in his class, was sufficiently well 
liked to be occasionally elected to a class office. He con- 
tinued to show an ability in writing which exceeded that 
which would be expected on the basis of his mental age. 

Robert was somewhat taller and heavier than other 
ten-year-old boys and in nearly all other respects was 
physically superior. However, he was already having 
some trouble with his eyes and the school nurse recom- 
mended that he wear glasses. Although now capable of 
doing tenth-grade work, he was in the fifth grade, where 
there was nothing to challenge him intellectually. He was, 
however, acquiring an excellent education at home 
through a broad reading program. 

During the first decade of his life Robert, inheriting 
great mental ability and a special gift in verbal expres- 
sion and surrounded during these formative years by a 
favorable home environment, had developed with marked 
constancy. There is every reason to believe that he will 
continue to maintain his present pace and in future years 
make an outstanding contribution to society. 

292 



SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY 

AI>AMS, F., and W. BROWN: "Teaching the Bright Pupil," Henry 
Holt & Company, New York, 1930, 249 pp. 

BAKER, H. J.: "Characteristic Differences in Bright and Dull Chil- 
dren," Public School Publishing Company, Bloomington, EL, 
1927. 

BENTLEY, J. E.: "Superior Children," W. W. Norton & Company, 
Inc., New York, 1937, 331 pp. 

BRIDGMAN, D. S.: Success in College and Business, The Person. /., 
Vol. 9, No. 1, June, 1930. 

BURKS, B. S. r D. W. JENSEN, and L. M. TERMAN : " Genetic Studies of 
Genius," Vol. Ill, Stanford University Press, Stanford Uni- 
versity, Calif., 1930, 508 pp. 

CARROLL, H. A.: "Generalization of Bright and Dull Children," 
Teachers College Contributions to Education, No. 439, Columbia 
University, New York, 1930, 54 pp. 

CATTELL, J. McK.: Families of American Men of Science, Popular 
Scientific Monthly, Vol. 86, pp. 504^515, 1915; Scientific Monthly, 
Vol. 4, pp. 248-262, 1917, Vol. 5, pp. 368-378, 1918. 

COHEN, H. L., and N. G. CORYELL: "Educating Superior Students," 
American Book Company, New York, 1935, 340 pp. 

Cox, C. M.: " Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. II, Stanford Univer- 
sity Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1926, 842 pp. 

COY, G. L.: "The Interests, Abilities, and Achievements of a Special 
Class for Gifted Children," Teachers College Contributions to 
Education, No. 131, Columbia University, New York, 1923, 
194 pp. 

DUFF, J. F.: Children of High Intelligence. A Follow-up Inquiry, 
Brit. J. PsychoL, Vol. 29, pp. 413-439, 1939. 

FINCH, F. H., and H. A. CARROLL: Gifted Children as High-school 
Leaders, J. Genet. PsychoL, Vol. 41, pp. 476-481, December, 1932. 

FREEMAN, F. S.: "Individual Differences," Henry Holt & Company, 
New York, 1934, 355 pp. 

GALTON, F.: "Hereditary Genius," The Macmillan Company, New 
York, 1914; original ed., Macmillan & Co., Ltd., London, 1869, 
379 pp. 

293 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 

GESELL, A.: "Infancy and Human Growth," The Macmillan Com- 
pany, New York, 1928, 418 pp. 
GODDAKO, H. EL: "School Training of Gifted Children," World 

Book Company, Yonkers-on-Hudson, N. Y., 1928, 226 pp. 
GRAY, H. A.: "Some Factors in the Undergraduate Careers of Young 

College Students, " Teachers College Contributions to Education, 

No. 437, Columbia University, New York, 66 pp. 
HOLLINGWORTH, L. S. i An Enrichment Curriculum for Rapid Learners 

at Public School, 500: Speyer School, Teachers College Record, 

Vol. 39, No. 4, pp. 296-306, January, 1938. 
HOLLINGWOBTH, L. S.: Comparative Beauty of the Faces of Highly 

Intelligent Adolescents, Fed. Bern, and J. Genet. PsychoL, Vol. 47, 

pp. 268-281, December, 1935. 
EOUJNGWORTH, L. S.: Do Intellectually Gifted Children Grow 

toward Mediocrity in Stature?, J. Genet. PsychoL, Vol. 37, pp. 

345-360, 1930. 
HOLLINGWORTH, L. S.: "Gifted Children," The Macmillan Company, 

New York, 1926, 374 pp. 
HOLLINGWORTH, L. S., and R. M. KAUNITZ: Centile Status of Gifted 

Children at Maturity, /. Genet. PsychoL, Vol. 45, pp. 106-120, 

September, 1934. 
LAMSON, E.: "A Study of Young Gifted Children in Senior High 

School," Teachers College Contributions to Education, No. 424, 

Columbia University, New York, 1930, 117 pp. 
LANGE-EICHBATJM, W.: "The Problem of Genius," The Macmillan 

Company, New York, 1932, 187 pp. 
LEHMAN, H. C., and P. A. Witty: The Play Behavior of Fifty Gifted 

Children, /. Educ. PsychoL, Vol. 18, pp. 259-265, 1927. 
LINCOLN, E. A.: Study of Changes in the Intelligence Quotients of 

Superior Children, /. Educ. Res., Vol. 29, pp. 272-275, December, 

1935. 
LOMBROSO, C.: "The Man of Genius," Charles Scribner's Sons, New 

York, 1891, 370 pp. 

LOBGE, L, and L. S. HOLLINGWORTH : Adult Status of Highly Intelli- 
gent Children, Ped. %n. and J. Genet. PsychoL, Vol. 49, pp. 

215-226, 1936. *" 

NEVILL, E. M.: Brilliant Children with Special Reference to Their 

Particular Difficulties, Brit. J. Educ. PsychoL Vol. 7, pp. 247-258, 

November, 1937. 
OSBURN, W. J., and B. J. ROHAN: "Enriching the Curriculum for 

Gifted Children," The Macmillan Company, New York, 1931, 

408 pp. 

294 



SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY 

PATEKSON, D. G.: "Physique and Intellect," D. Appleton-Century 
Company, Inc., New York, 1930, 304 pp. 

Report of the Evaluating Committee on the Education of Gifted 
Children in Secondary Schools: National Committee on Coordi- 
nation in Secondary Education, J. Educ. Soc., Vol. 13, pp. 112- 
119, October, 1939. 

STEDMAN, L. M.: " Education of Gifted Children," World Book 
Company, Yonkers-on-Hudson, N. Y., 1924, 192 pp. 

TERMAN, L. M. : Educational Suggestions from Follow-up Studies of 
Intellectually Gifted Children, J. Educ. Soc., Vol. 13, pp. 82-89, 
October, 1939. 

TERMAN, L. M. : " Genetic Studies of Genius," Vol. I, Stanford Univer- 
sity Press, Stanford University, Calif., 1926, 648 pp. 

Thirty-ninth Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of 
Education, Parts I and II: Intelligence: Its Nature and Nurture, 
Public School Publishing Company, Bloomington, HI., 1940. 

Twenty-third Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of 
Education, Part I: The Education of Gifted Children, Public 
School Publishing Company, Bloomington, HI., 1924. 

White House Conference. Special Education: The Handicapped and 
the Gifted. D. Appleton-Century Company, Inc., New York, 
1931. ^ 

WITTY, P. A.: "A Study of One Hundred Gifted Children," State 
Teachers College Studies in Education, Vol. I, No. 13, Emporia, 
Kansas, 1930, 44 pp. 

WITTY, P. A. : Exploitation of the Child of High Intelligence Quotient, 
Educ. Method, Vol. 15, pp. 298-304, March, 1936. 



295 



INDEX 



Abbott and Trabue, Poetry Test, 

204, 205 

Aberdeen University, 69 
Ability grouping, 220 
Abstract intelligence, 14, 195-197, 

205 
Acceleration, 206, 219-242, 249, 

277, 282 

advantages, 221-224 
disadvantages, 224-226 
full, 219, 222, 228-242 
opinions of gifted children, 227- 

242 

partial, 220, 228-242 
Achievement, constancy of, 141 
extracurricular, 165 
vs. intellect, 161-164 
and psychic infirmities, 170 
in school subjects, 273-275 
standards of, 255, 256, 262 
Achievement Quotient, 241 
Achievement tests, 6, 8, 9, 39, 130- 

132, 163 

Adams, John, 29 
Adenoids, 80, 81 
Adler, Alfred, 170 
Age-grade status, 6, 9 
Agricultural class, and eminence, 

31,32 
Alpern, Hymen, 247, 248 

questionnaire, 247 
Ambition, 166, 167 
American Council Cooperative 
General Achievement Tests, 
130-132 
American Indian, I.Q. of, 22, 23 



American Museum of Natural His- 
tory, 60 

American Telephone and Tele- 
graph Company, 164 

Ampere, Andr Marie, 194 

Ancestral inheritance, laws of, 27, 
28 

Anderson, J. R, 139 

Anthropologists, 20, 60 

Anthropometric tests, 73, 74 

Antisocial tendencies, 94, 95 

Army Alpha and Army Beta, tests, 
21, 151-154, 158 

Art, 57, 258, 260, 278 
(See also Drawing) 

Athletics, 75-78, 262 

Attention span, 115-118 

Average or normal group, 4, 5, 10, 
11, 47, 49, 61, 74, 75, 91, 139, 
140, 246-248, 256 
schools adjusted to, 207, 209, 
211, 220 



Bach, Johann Sebastian, 188 
Baldwin, Bird T., 66, 73, 74 
Baldwin- Wood norms, 64 
Barnard College, 64, 76, 77 
Barnes, E., 66 
Bear, E. H., 221 
Beauty, and intelligence, 72, 73 
Beethoven, Ludwig van, 188 
Bell Telephone Company, 164, 165 
Bennett, Anna E., 141 
Bentley, J. E., 177 
Berkeley, California, 22, 34 
Berle, A., 223 



297 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



Binet, Alfred, 14, 194 
Binet-Simon Scale, 18, 135, 151 

(See also Stanford-Binet 

Tests) 
Biographical sketch, intellectually 

gifted child, 265-292 
Biographical survey of genius (see 

Cox, Catherine Morris) 
Biography, 260, 262 
Biologists, 19 
Blatz, William E., 40, 41 
Boas, Franz, 65-67 
Bonaparte family, 26 
Borderline groups, 4 
Boys, gifted, 63, 64, 76, 78, 81, 101- 

103, 111, 152, 155-159, 193, 

194, 205, 206 

Bridgman, Donald S., 164-166 
Bright children, 125, 126, 192, 196, 
197, 207, 211, 244^246 

(See also Intellectually Gifted 

children) 

Bronson, Edith, 82 
Bronte, Charlotte, 144 
Brown, M., Ill 

Bryan, William Jennings, 174, 175 
Bryant, William Cullen, 200 
Buckingham, B. R,., 14 
Burk, F., 67 
Burks, Barbara, 44 
Burt, Cyril, 94, 95 
Business success, college grades 

and, 163-166 
Buxton, David, 97, 98 
Buxton, Jedediah, 195 



CAVD scale, 153, 154 

California, gifted children (see 

Terman, Lewis M.) 
California, University of, 34 
Cambridge Double Track Plan, 

220 

Campbell, Harold, 207 
Carlyle, Thomas, 260 



Carroll, H. A., Ill, 125, 191, 192, 
204 

Carroll Prose Appreciation Test, 
204 

Carter, H. D., 42, 43 

CatteU, J. Mc.K, 31, 32, 39, 293 

Center CoUege, 221 

Cephalic index, 71, 74 

Character, 94-97, 168-170, 177 

Chatterton, Thomas, 166 

Children, gifted (see Intellectually 
gifted children) 

Chronological age (C.A.), 4, 44, 49, 
75, 76, 97-99, 107, 132, 151, 
182, 197, 220-222, 235 
and academic achievement, 221 
factor in tests, 11, 12, 55, 56 

Clarke, E. L., 32 

Cobb, M., 29 

Cohen, Helen, 141, 207, 247 

Colbert, Claudette, 141 

Coleridge, Samuel Taylor, 143 

College-entrance tests, 50 

College grades, and business suc- 
cess, 163-166 

Columbia University, 64, 69, 76, 77 

Common sense, 13, 14 

Compensation, theory of, 55, 57, 58 

Competition, 183, 184 
needed by gifted children, 246 

Conformity, 89 

Copernicus, 87, 283 

Coryell, N. G., 141, 207, 247 

Cox, Catherine Morris, "Genetic 
Studies of Genius," 30, 31, 
95-97, 142-149, 168, 175, 187, 
190, 202, 250 

Coy, G. L., 212 

Cranial measurements, 68-72 

Creative ability, tests of, 204, 205 

Creative resources, development 
of, 257, 258, 262 

Crile, G. W., 169 

Crime, mental defectivenesa and, 
95 

Curie, Eve, 92, 93 



298 



INDEX 



Curie, Marie, 92, 93 
Curiosity, 119-121 
Curriculum, 245, 253-264 

D 

Dalton Plan, 220 
Darwin, Charles, 182, 184 
De Tocqueville, Alexis, 207 
Death, age and manner of, 181-183 
Democracy, in education, 206-209 

need to salvage gifted children, 

34 

Dempsey, Jack, 76 
Diaries, 278, 279 
Dictation as medium of expression, 

275-277, 285 
Differentiation, camouflaged, 210 

pf education, 206-213 
Dionne quintuplets, 39-41 
Diseases, frequency of, 78-80 
Disraeli, Benjamin, 184, 260 
Dolichocephaly, 71 
Drawing, 190-194, 205 

intelligence and, 191193 

sex factor, 193, 194 
Drill, 124, 244-248, 255, 259, 262 
Drive, 166-170 

importance of, 168, 169 

sources of, 169-171, 180 
Dull-normal groups, 4, 49, 58, 61, 
72, 118, 121, 125, 126, 192, 
195-198, 207, 209, 211, 220, 
246, 247, 256 

identification, 8 



E 



Economic necessity, a source of 

drive, 180 

Economic status of family, 178-180 
Edison, Thomas A., 169, 185, 250, 

251 
Education, as conditioning factor, 

197 
genius developed by, 219 



Edwards, Jonathan, 27 
Edwards, Richard, 27 
Egotism, 211-213 
Einstein, Albert, 184 
Elementary school, 163, 275 
intelligence tests, 136, 139, 159 
mental growth in, 50 
Emerson, Ralph Waldo, 26, 27, 223 
Eminence, factors, determining, 

166-185 

extrinsic, 178-185 
intrinsic, 166-177 
intelligence and, 161-166 
Eminent adults, character traits, 

95-97, 168-170 
distribution of, 174 
early years, 139-149 
environment, 39 
heredity, 30-32, 39 
I.Q., 142-149, 159, 168, 169, 203 
persistence and intelligence, 168- 

170 
(See also Cox, Catherine 

Morris) 

Emotional education, 261, 262 
Emotional stability, 85-88 
Enrichment, 206, 236, 237, 243- 

264 
within heterogeneous class, 243- 

247, 255, 259 
at home, 249-252 
intraclass grouping, 243-249 
within special class, 243, 246 
Environment, capacities modified 

by, 36 
changed, effect on I.Q., 48-53, 

138 
effect on I.Q., Iowa studies, 48- 

53, 138 

home, and I.Q., 44, 45 
interaction with individual, 36, 

53, 166 
role of, 35-53 
Envy, 55, 58, 59 
Ethical Culture School, 136, 137 
Eurich, A. C., 191, 192 



299 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



Extracurricular activities, 111,112, 
165, 262 



Faber, EL, 65-67 

Families, of geniuses, 25-34, 38, 39 
Winship and Davenport 

study, 27 
of gifted children, 91-94, 178- 

180 

Faraday, Michael, 87, 107 
Farley, J., 172 
Fathers, of eminent men, 30-32 

of gifted children, 32-34 
Feeble-mindedness, 3, 4, 14, 27, 62, 
69, 72, 149, 150, 198, 209, 265 
transmission of, 25, 38 
Finch, F. H., Ill 
Fluency in speaking or writing, 166, 

173-175 

FootbaU, 76, 97-99, 209, 210, 234 
Ford, Henry, 176, 184 
Foster homes, 44, 45, 48 
Fraternal twins, 42 
Freeman, F. N., 42, 44, 45 
Freeman, Frank S., 21, 22 
Fuller, Tom, "The Virginia Cal- 
culator," 195 

G 

Galileo, 107, 182 

Galton, F., 27-29, 59, 60, 67, 69, 

293 

Garth, T. R., 22, 23 
Gates, A. I., 124 
Gates Silent Reading Tests, 130 
Gauss, Karl Friedrich, 194 
Generalization, of bright and dull 

children, 5, 124-126 
Genes, and race differences, 20 
Genius, constancy of growth, 134- 

160 

definition, 290 
fallacies about, 10, 13 



Genius, four typical groups, 168 
heredity and environment, 36-53 
and mental health, 82-88 
Geniuses, burdens borne by, 86, 87 
families of, 25-34 
Winship and Davenport study, 

27 

Germ plasm, defective, 38 
Gesell, A., 138 
Gifted children (see Intellectually 

gifted children) 

Girls, gifted, 63-66, 76-78, 109, 
111, 152, 155-159, 193, 194, 
204, 205 

Gladstone, William E., 184 
Glasgow, Alice, 141 
Goddard, H. H., 38, 212, 294 
Goethe, Johann Wolfgang von, 

145-149 
Goodenough, F. L., 23, 24, 46, 47, 

193, 198 

Grant, Ulysses S., 140 
Gray, H. A., 24, 64, 76-79, 221 
Grouping, 206 
intraclass, 243-249 

H 

Haggerty Intelligence Examina- 
tions, 18 
Hall of Fame, 29 
Handwriting, 244, 252 
Happiness, 89-91 
Harding, Warren G., 139, 140 
Harris, W. T., 219, 220 
Harvard University, 221, 223 
Health, 54r-88 

factor in success, 78, 166, 170, 171 
Hearing, 81 
Height, 59-68 
Heredity, 25-53 
vs. environment, 41-53 
interdependence, 35, 36, 53 
in production of genius, 37 
relative potency, 37 
of geniuses, 30-34 



300 



INDEX 



Heterogeneous class, 213-215, 219, Individual instruction, 220 



225,243 

grouping within, 243, 244 
High school intelligence tests, 50 
High school students, gifted, 141, 

151 

Hildreth, G., 47 
Hindenburg, Paul von, 181, 182 
Hinton, R. I., 45, 46 
Hitler, Adolf, 140, 174, 182, 184 
Hobbies, 252 

Hollingworth, H. L., 4, 87, 88 
Hollingworth, Leta S., 8, 9, 108, 

265, 294 
gifted children, Speyer School, 

24, 29, 260 

studies of, 38, 61, 62, 67-73, 
110, 112, 113, 132, 140, 195, 
211, 216, 261 
growth study, 150-154 
Iowa studies, criticism, 51 
Holzinger, K. J., 42 
Home, enrichment at, 249-252, 

283, 292 

and I.Q., variance, 44, 45 
Home town, size of, 180 
Honolulu, 60 

Horace Mann School, 11, 136, 137 
Horowitz, Esther, 141 
Hughes, Charles Evans, 223 
Hugo, Victor, 143 
Humboldt, F. H. A. von, 143 



Identical twins, 42, 43 

changing environment, 43 , 
differing environment, 42, 43 

Idiot-savant, 175 

Idiots, 3, 69 

Illegitimacy, 48 

Imaginary playmates and coun- 
tries, 103-107, 272 

Imbeciles, 3 

Indians, American, 173 
I.Q. of, 22, 23 



Industrial group, 33, 34 
Infancy, development during, 266 
Infants, intelligence tests, 139 
Inferiority complexes, among 

gifted, 93 

Initiative, 14, 123, 124 
Insanity, 25, 26 
Institute of Child Welfare, 46 
Intellect, vs. achievement, 161- 
167 

and emotional stability, 85-88 

stature and, 172 

Intellectually gifted children, 3-6, 
9, 10 

anthropometric measurements, 
73-75 

athletics, 75-78, 262 

beauty, 72, 73 

biographical sketch, 265-292 

character development, 94, 95 

companionship sought, 108, 109 

cranial measurements, 68^72 

emotional education, 261, 262 

environment and, 37 

as examples, 215, 216 

extremely gifted vs. less gifted, 
130-132 

fathers of, 32-34 

greater difficulties of, 109, 110 

happiness, 89-91 

height and weight, 59-68 
constancy of status, 67, 68 

homes, 249-252 

identification, 6-19 

I.Q., 49, 52, 53, 60-73, 202 

intelligence ratings, age for, 17 

interests, 100, 101 

leadership, 110-113, 134, 172, 
216 

mental age, 107 

mental health, 82-85 

mental characteristics and 
achievements, 114-133 

need for help, 216-219 

need to salvage, 34 



301 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



Intellectually gif ted children, phys- 
ical beauty, 72, 73 
physical characteristics, 59-75 
physical exercise and play, 258, 

262 

physical health, 78-82 
physique, 54-68 
reasons for believing inferior, 

54r-59 

play activities, 97-108 
position and frequency, 3-6 
progress quotients, 132, 133 
relatives of, 28-34 
social responsibilities, training 

for, 258, 259, 262 
special classes, 252, 253-264 
stability, 85, 86 
stature, mean ratio to norms, 67, 

68 
writings of, 265, 275-292 

(See also Hollingworth, Leta 

S.; Terman, Lewis M.) 
Intelligence, abstract, 14, 195-197, 

205 
and language abilities, 197, 

198 

and poetic ability, 201, 202 
of American school children, 23- 

25 

art appreciation and, 191, 192 
constancy under changing en- 
vironment, 38 
definitions, 3, 14 
distribution of, 3-6 
gift of, 3 
height and weight, relationship 

with, 59-68 
importance of/ 3, 5 
mechanical, 196, 197, 205 
rating of, 9-19 

ratio between leader and led, 112 
social, 166, 176, 195 
subnormal, transmission, 37, 38 
transmission, 37-39 

(See also Intelligence tests) 



Intelligence quotient (I.Q.), of 

adopted children, 47 
changed environment and (Iowa 

studies), 47-53 
constancy of, 134^-160 
distribution of changes, 156-158 
of feeble-minded, 3, 4 
of gifted children, 49, 52, 53, 60- 

73, 202 

and home environment, 44, 45 
of identical twins, 42, 43 
of juvenile delinquents, 94, 95 
mean, American Indian, 22, 23 

American white children, 21 

negro, 21, 22 
men of letters, 203 
over 170, 98, 99, 108, 113, 143 
schools and, 45-53 
Intelligence tests,. 4-6, 8-14, 135- 

160, 196, 268, 272-274, 281, 

282 

Army, 21, 151-154, 158 
college, 50 
elementary school, 50, 136, 139, 

159 

high school, 50 
Honolulu, 60 
infant, 139 
Iowa studies, 48-52 
items, 15-17 
nursery schools, 45-48 
preschool, 6, 17, 50, 138, 139, 272 
reliability, 17, 18 
scores, 221 
standards, 18, 19 
vaEdity, 14r-17 

(See also Stanford-Biaet 

Tests) 

Intraclass grouping, 243-249 
disadvantages, 245-247 
flexibility, 244, 245 
modification of curriculum and 

methods, 245-249 
use in rural schools, 245 
Iowa, University of, Studies, 48- 

53, 138 



302 



INDEX 



Iowa Child Welfare Research. Lincoln School, New York City, 47, 



Station, 50 



Jacobs, Helen, 183 

Jefferson, Thomas, 27 

Jennings, BL S., 20 

Jersild, A. T., 86, 103 

Jesus, 182 

Jews, 23, 24, 173 

Johnston, W. M., 183 

Jones, A. L., 221 

Juvenile delinquents, I.Q,, 94, 95 



Kallikak, Martin, 27 
Kawnitz, E. M., 151-153, 158 
Kilpatrick, W. H., 209 
Kipling, Rudyard, 116, 117, 181 
Kroeber, Elsberth, 214 
Kuhlmann-Anderson Intelligence 
Test, 18, 38, 268, 281 



Lamson, Edna, 150, 151 
Lange-Eichbaum, W., 87 
Language, 41, 258, 270, 273, 274 
abilities, and abstract intelli- 
gence, 197, 198 
development, 40, 41 
Language clubs, 248 
Lathi ancestry, 23, 24 
Leadership, of intellectually gifted, 

101, 110-113, 134, 172, 216 
"Lei Things," 10^-106 
Leland Stanford University (see 

Stanford University) 
life situations, preparation for, 

213-215 
Lightning calculators, 186, 194, 

195, 198 

Lincoln, Abraham, 182, 218, 260 
Lincoln, E, A., 158 



136, 137 

Lindbergh, Charles A., 250, 251 
Long, Huey, 140 
Long-headedness, 71 
Longfellow, Henry Wadsworth, 27, 

143,223 

Lorge, L, 153, 154 
Los Angeles, 22, 33 
Lowell, Abbott Lawrence, 221 
Luther, Martin, 182 

M 

McAdory Art Test, 191, 192 

Macaulay, Thomas B., 200, 250 

MacDonald, A., 71 

" Make-up" classes, 248 

Marat, Jean Paul, 166 

Markey, F. V., 103 

Marriage, effect on success, 178, 

180 
Masculinity of gifted boys' games, 

101-103 

Mathematical ability, 194, 195, 205 
Maurer, K M., 46, 47 
Mayo brothers, 180 
Mearns, H., 202, 203 
Mechanical ability, 195-197, 205 
Meier-Seashore Art Judgment 

Test, 191 
Mental age (M.A.), 4, 55, 56, 100, 

107, 197, 282, 292 
Mental development, 43-53 
constancy of, 134^-160 
preschool to college, tests, 50 
Mental health, 82-85 
Mental inferiority, transmission of, 

37, 38 
Mental superiority, transmission 

of, 37-39 

Merrill, Maude A., 142 
Merrill-Palmer intelligence tests, 

48 

Mesocephalic type, 74 
Michelangelo Buonarroti, 190, 191 



303 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



Microcephalic idiots, 69 
MiU, John Stuart, 249, 250 
Miller, W. S., 136 
Milton, John, 170, 171 
Minnesota, University of, 46, 47, 

111 

Minnesota Pre-School Scale, 18 
Moody, Helen Wills, 183 
Moore, Dr. A. H. 82 
Mooseheart, 45 

Morons, 3, 4, 57, 114, 175, 208, 265 
Motor coordination, 55 
Mozart, Wolfgang A., 187, 188 
Mulligan, J. H., 69 
Murdock, K, 60 
Music, 14, 186-190, 193, 205, 258, 

260,284 

Music test, Seashore, 189 
Mussolini, Benito, 184 

N 

Napoleon, 25, 26, 120, 170, 182 
Nationality groups, cephalic indi- 
ces, 74 
I.Q., 23-25 

National Intelligence Test, 18 
National Society for the Study of 
Education, 1928 Yearbook, 44 
1940 Yearbook, 42, 45-47, 51, 

108, 130, 137, 139, 159 
Negativism, 51, 52 
Negroes, 21, 173 

I.Q., 21, 22 
Nemzek, C. M., 135 
Neurasthenia, and genius, 83-85 
Neurotic complaints, and genius, 

82-88 
and sub-average intelligence, 83, 

88 
New York City, 11, 18, 19, 24, 29, 

47, 60, 97, 207 
gifted high school children, 141, 

142 

Newman, H. H., 42 
Newton, Sir Isaac, 144, 196 



Normal or average group (see Aver- 
age group) 

Normal-probability curve, for dis- 
tribution of intelligence, 3-6 

Norms, mean ratio of stature to 
gifted, 67, 68 

Northwestern University, 221 

Nursery schools, 45-48 

Nurture, 23, 42, 43 



O 



Observation, 118, 119 
O'Connell, Daniel, 166 
Older children, play with, 98-100 
Opportunity classes, 129, 208, 210, 

213, 219, 252 
Osborn, W. J., 248, 249 
Osier, Sir William, 177 



Parents, judgment of children's in- 
telligence, 6, 7, 12 

Pasteur, Louis, 13, 177, 250, 251 

Paterson, D. G., 21, 60, 69, 71, 295 

Persistence, 168-170 

Personal equation, intrusion of, 9, 
10,52 

Personality, respect for, 209-211 

Phi Beta Kappa, 76, 224 

Phonetic analysis, comparative, 
125 

Physical beauty, 72, 73 

Physical exercise and play, 258, 
262 

Physical size and appearance, 166, 
171, 172 

Physique and health, 54-88 

Pierpont, Sarah, 27 

Pitt, William, 144 

Play activities, 258, 262 
of gifted children, 97-108 

Poetry, writing, 199-205 

Power, 115, 118 

Precocity, 93, 139, 148, 217, 270 



304 



INDEX 



Prenatal influences, 38 
Pre-Sehool Scale, Minnesota, 18 
Preschool children, 271, 272 

intelligence tests, 6, 17, 50, 138, 

139, 271, 272 
Iowa, 49, 50 
Minnesota, table, 46 
mental growth, 50-52 
Preschool laboratories, Iowa, 48-50 
Presidents of the United States, 

intelligence, 10, 11 
Professional classes, 31-34, 39 
Progress quotients, 132, 133 
Progressive schools, 210 
Project method, 248 
Promotion, rapid, 219 
Psychologists, 6, 12-17, 20, 60, 

134-138, 173, 216, 217 
Psychology, 8, 10 
educational, 263, 264 

Q 

Quickness, 118 
Quintuplets, Dionne, 39-41 

B 

Race, 166, 172 

and head shape, 71 

intelligence factor in, 20-34 
Reading, 92, 93, 119, 147, 148, 162, 
248, 250 

ability, 14, 15, 197-199 

early, 272, 273 

interests, of gifted children, 279- 
281 

program, 292 

quotient, 130 

special gift, 187 
Reed, R. W., 69 
Relative values, ability to see, 122, 

123 

Relatives of gifted children, 28-34 
Rembrandt van Rijn, 190 



Reymert, M. L,, 45, 46 
Roberts, K K, 47, 48 
Rohan, B. J., 248, 249 
Roosevelt, Franklin D., 170, 176 
Roosevelt, Theodore, 169, 176 
Rural schools, enrichment in, 245 



S 



St. Louis, 219, 220 

Saint-Simon, Claude Henri, Count 

de, 166 

San Francisco, 22, 33 
Sand, George, 144 
Scholastic attainments, 126-130 
School clubs, 249 
School work, of gifted children, 45- 

53, 126-128 
Scientists, American, heredity of, 

31, 32, 39 

Scotch ancestry, 23 
Scotch-Irish, 21 
Scott, Sir Walter, 143, 144 
"Search, plan of," 15-17 
Seashore, music test, 189 
Segregation, 244, 247, 249, 258 
Selection, 23 
Self-criticism, 121, 122 
Sex, in art achievement, 193, 194 

classification by, 63-66 
Shearer, Supt., 220 
Shirley, M. M., 138 
Siblings, 29, 44, 93 
Singleness of purpose, 166, 175, 

176 
Size and appearance, factors in 

success, 166, 171, 172 
Skeels, H. M., 48, 49 
Social characteristics, 89-113 
Social intelligence, 166, 176, 195 
Social responsibilities, of gifted 

children, 258, 259, 262 
Socioeconomic groups, 7, 30, 37, 38, 

43, 80, 178, 249 
Solitary games, 107-1 10, 272 
Sommerville, R. C., 69 



305 



GENIUS IN THE MAKING 



Special class, 251-264 

acceleration and enrichment, 253 
background of associations, 255, 

.262 

creative resources, 257 
needs and interests of children, 

253, 254 
standards of achievement, 255, 

262 

survey, 262 
teachers, 262-264 
Special gifts, 186-205, 270, 290 
Speed, 118 
Spelling, 259, 260 
Speyer School, New York City, 
group of gifted children, 24, 
29, 130-132, 259, 260 
debate by, 227-242 
Stability, of gifted children, 85, 86 
Standards, of intelligence rating, 

9-19 
Stanford Achievement Test, 127, 

225, 226 

Stanford-Binet Tests, revision 
Binet-Simon Scale, 15-18, 121, 
135, 151, 155, 272-274, 281 
retest, 44, 45, 48, 49, 136-138, 

151-158, 203, 226, 281 
Stanford University, 9, 65, 66, 159 
Starkweather, E. K, 47, 48 
Stature, 67, 68, 172 
Stedman, L. M., 129, 212, 251, 252 
Steimnetz, Charles P., 170 
Stevenson, Robert Louis, 57 
Stoddard, G. D., 51 
Strayer, G., 207 
Stuttering, 85 
Subjective ratings, intelligence, 15, 

17-19 
Subjects, school, mean quotients 

for gifted children, 128 
order of, 127 

Subnormal intelligence, transmis- 
sion jf, 37, 38 
Sullivafi, If H., 60 



Supplementary reading, 248, 250 
Swift, Jonathan, 143 



Tasso, Torquato, 143 
Taussig scale, 30, 33 
Taylor, G. A., 62 
Teachers, 246, 247 

of gifted children, 216, 217, 261- 

264 

qualifications, 263, 264 
intelligence ratings by, 6-9, 11, 

17, 52, 53 

of special classes, 261-264 
Teachers College, Columbia Uni- 
versity, 150 
Temple, Shirley, 72 
Ten-year level, tests on, 281, 282 
Tendler, A., 88 
Tennessee, 21 

Tennyson, Alfred, Lord, 200 
Terman, Lewis M., criticism of 

Iowa studies, 51, 52 
definition of intelligence, 14 
gifted children, study of, 9, 10, 
22-24, 28-34, 65, 66, 73, 74, 
79-82, 85, 95-97, 100-104, 
107-111, 120, 121, 126-128, 
132, 133, 198, 201, 202, 205, 
225, 226 

Growth Study, 150, 154-160 
I.Q. estimates of genius, 142 
Terman Group Test of Mental 

Ability, 18, 157-159 
Thinking, instruction in, 256, 257, 

262 

(See also Abstract intelligence) 
Thorndike, E. L., 124, 218, 254 
Thorndike, R. L., 136-138, 195 
Thorndike College Entrance Ex- 
amination, 155, 159 
Tilden, William T., 183 
Tonsils, 80, 81 
Tredgold, A. F., 38 
Tunney, Gene, 76 



306 



INDEX 



Tuthill, Elizabeth, 27 

Twins, different environments, 42 

fraternal, 42 

identical, 42, 43 

mental traits, similarity, 37, 39, 
41 

U 

United States Public Health Serv- 
ice, Report (1929), 63, 70 



Varner, G. F., 8 

Vikings, 116 

Vision, 15, 81 

Vocabulary, 162, 268-270, 276 

as criterion, 15 

Voltaire, Francois Marie Arouet 
de, 174 

W 

Washburn, C. W., 220 
Washington, George, 26, 29, 113, 

144 
Washington Irving High School, 

141, 142 
Webster, Daniel, 174, 175 



Weight, 59-68 
Wellman, Beth L., 49-51 
White, Byron ("Whizzer"), 76 
White, William Allen, 180 
"Who's Who in America," 29, 30, 

223 

Wilhelm II, Kaiser, 170 
Williams, Roger, 29 
Wilson, Woodrow, 182, 183 
Winnetka Plan, 220 
Winship and Davenport, study of 

inheritance, 27 
Witte, Karl, 250, 251 
Witty, P., 108, 130, 133, 159, 169, 

170, 217 

Woodrow, H., 14 
Woodworth, R. S., 14 
World War, 153, 170 

I.Q. army scores, 21 
Writing, 205, 277, 278, 285-290, 

292 

poetry, 19&-204 
tests of creative ability, 204, 205 



Yale University, 27 
Yates, D. H,, 111 
Yoder, A. H., 108 



307 



30320